You are on page 1of 16

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

ISSN: 1380-3395 (Print) 1744-411X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20

Cognitive Decline Following Stroke: A


Comprehensive Study of Cognitive Decline
Following Stroke*

Jacqueline Hochstenbach , Theo Mulder , Jacques van Limbeek , Rogier


Donders & Henny Schoonderwaldt

To cite this article: Jacqueline Hochstenbach , Theo Mulder , Jacques van Limbeek ,
Rogier Donders & Henny Schoonderwaldt (1998) Cognitive Decline Following Stroke:
A Comprehensive Study of Cognitive Decline Following Stroke*, Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 20:4, 503-517, DOI: 10.1076/jcen.20.4.503.1471

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.20.4.503.1471

Published online: 09 Aug 2010.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1136

View related articles

Citing articles: 16 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ncen20
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1380-3395/98/2004-503$12.00
1998, Vol. 20, No. 4, pp. 503-517 © Swets & Zeitlinger

Cognitive Decline Following Stroke:


A Comprehensive Study of Cognitive Decline
Following Stroke*
Jacqueline Hochstenbach1,2, Theo Mulder1,2, Jacques van Limbeek1,3, Rogier Donders4, and Henny
Schoonderwaldt2
1Sint Maartenskliniek-Research, Nijmegen, The Netherlands, 2Neurological Institute, University of Nijmegen,
3
Sint Maartenskliniek-Rehabilitation Centre, Nijmegen, and 4Centre for Biostatistics, University of Utrecht,
The Netherlands

ABSTRACT

General insight into the frequency and gravity of cognitive dysfunctions following stroke and its influenc-
ing factors is still lacking. With an extensive neuropsychological battery 229 patients who had suffered a
stroke were assessed. More than 70% of the patients showed a marked slowness of information processing,
whereas at least 40% of all patients had difficulty with memory, visuospatial and constructive tasks, lan-
guage skills, and arithmetic. A significant effect was found for side and type of stroke, gender, and the
presence of aphasia. No significant effect was found for cortical versus subcortical lesions, having one
versus multiple strokes, having lowered consciousness on admission, the presence of risk factors, a paresis
of the hand, or the interval between the stroke and the neuropsychological assessment.

Although stroke is one of the most prominent Limbeek, & Schoonderwaldt, 1996). However,
neurological disorders, often leading to serious to date studies concerning an overall view of the
and long-lasting sensorimotor and behavioral cognitive consequences following stroke are still
disabilities, a general insight into the gravity and rare. The majority of publications on the neuro-
frequency of cognitive impairments is still lack- psychological consequences of stroke focus on
ing. Such an insight, however, is urgently specific cognitive impairments such as neglect,
needed, first since it is well known that cogni- aphasia, or apraxia. The few studies which con-
tive dysfunctions may hinder functional recov- cern a more general analysis, however, employ
ery and rehabilitation (Allen, 1984; Feigenson, mostly only short global screening tests, such as
McDowell, Meese, McCarthy, & Greenberg, the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE;
1977; Feigenson, McCarthy, Greenberg, & Galasko et al., 1990) and its variants (Babikian,
Feigenson, 1977; Galski, Bruno, Zorowitz, & Wolfe, Linn, Knoefel, & Albert, 1990; Down-
Walker, 1993; Novack, Haban, Graham, & hill, & Robinson, 1994; Ebrahim, Nouri, &
Satterfield, 1987; Sundet, Finset, & Reinvang, Barer, 1985; Friedman, 1991; Kappelle, Adams,
1988; Tatemichi et al., 1994) and second, be- Heffner, & Tomer, 1994), the Neurobehavioral
cause this knowledge is needed for the improve- Cognitive Status Examination (NCSE; Galski et
ment of long-term disability-oriented care pro- al., 1993; Mysiw, Beehan, & Gatens, 1989) or
grams (Hochstenbach, Donders, Mulder, van the CAMCOG, which is part of the Cambridge

*
This work has been supported by a grant from the Health Research and Development Council, number 281706.
The helpful comments of Patricia Anderson are gratefully acknowledged.
Address correspondence to: Jacqueline Hochstenbach, Sint Maartenskliniek-Research, P.O.Box 9011, 6500 GM
Nijmegen, The Netherlands. E-mail: jh_aro@universal.nl
Accepted for publication June 18, 1998.
504 JACQUELINE HOCHSTENBACH ET AL.

Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly degree of impairment is open to debate (Dvir,
(CAMDEX; Kwa et al., 1996). The value of 1997), the 6-point scale was recoded to only distin-
these studies is limited, because little insight can guish between normal and affected muscle
strength. The number of previous strokes as well
be gained about the diversity and complexity of
as a series of risk factors were scored. Stroke char-
the dysfunctions (Wade, 1993). acteristics were determined by the CT scans. This
The aim of the present paper is twofold: (1) to resulted in a group of 229 patients (140 men and
present an explorative, descriptive overview of 89 women) with a mean age of 55.9 years (SD =
the frequency and severity of cognitive changes 11.19) and a mean educational level of 4.1 years
which may take place after stroke, and (2) to (SD = 1.5) (see Appendix). The mean interval be-
detemine whether a relationship exists between tween stroke and the neuropsychological assess-
ment was 72.2 days (SD = 49.7, range 5–293
the gravity of the dysfunctions and variables days). Further clinical data of these patients are
such as anatomic location, type of vascular le- presented in Table 1.
sions, number of strokes, gender, aphasia, and Because not all neuropsychological tests have
lowered consciousness on admission. norms adequately stratified for age, a group of
healthy controls (n = 33) was included as a refer-
ence sample. The control group consisted of volun-
METHOD teers from the community (n = 18) and spouses of
stroke patients (n = 15). There were 12 males and
21 females. Mean age was 52.4 years (range 25-
Participants 73), with a mean level of education of 4.4 years
The protocol was approved by the Medical Ethics (SD = 1.5). None of the controls suffered from any
Board of each participating hospital, and all partic- neurological or psychiatric illness. Five controls
ipants gave their informed consent for the study. In were under medical treatment at the time of assess-
total 229 patients participated in the present study. ment: one was being treated by an orthopedic sur-
They were recruited from patients consecutively geon; two by a lung specialist; and two had re-
admitted between September 1992 and August cently visited both an internist and an ophthalmol-
1995 to three different hospitals in the Nijmegen ogist.
area: a university hospital, a rehabilitation centre,
and a regional hospital. Univariate analyses of Neuropsychological Assessment
variance were used to investigate potential con- The following neuropsychological ‘‘domains’’
founding by hospital: none of the analyses showed were assessed: orientation, memory, attention and
significant differences. Patients were between 18 concentration, visuospatial and visuoconstructive
and 70 years of age, with the diagnosis of a stroke functions, language, and arithmetic.
confirmed by computed tomography of the brain. For detailed information concerning the admin-
By imposing an age limit of 70 years, the effects of istered tests, the reader is referred to Lezak (1995).
aging on cognitive functioning have been reduced. In the present paper only a global description of
Patients were excluded when they suffered from the tasks is given.
any other major physical illnesses (life-threaten- Orientation was determined by questions about
ing, neurological, or disabling diseases) or mental time, length of time, place, and person.
disorders (under psychiatric treatment in the last Memory performance was evaluated using the
10 years), or when a reliable assessment of neuro- Dutch version of the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learn-
psychological functions was not possible (e.g., in ing Test (AVLT; Rey, 1964), the Wechsler Adult
patients with a persistent impairment in conscious- Intelligence Scale (WAIS; Stinissen, Willems,
ness like drowsiness). Aphasic patients were only Coetsier, & Hulsman, 1970; Wechsler, 1955) In-
excluded if they were not able to respond ade- formation and Digit Span subtests, and the
quately to ‘yes’ or ‘no’ questions. Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (RBMT;
Sensory deficits, reflex pathology, or lowered Wilson, Cockburn, & Baddeley, 1985; Wilson,
consciousness were scored on basis of the medical Cockburn, Baddely, & Hiorns, 1989). From the
reports, all at the time of admission in the acute AVLT the scores for immediate recall, delayed
phase. The Medical Research Council scale (MRC; recall, and recognition were used. WAIS Informa-
Wade, 1992, p. 53) was used to determine the de- tion subtest was used for examining (the reproduc-
gree of strength of the hands, arms, and legs. Be- tion of) general knowledge. The Digit Span sub-
cause the interrater reliability of the MRC scale is test, which comprises two different tests (digits
low and the interpretation of the scores indicating forward and digits backward), reflects the memory
COGNITIVE DECLINE FOLLOWING STROKE 505

Table 1. Clinical Data for 229 Stroke Patientsa.

n %
Stroke description
Type
Infarct 199 (86.9%)
Hemorrhage 130 (13.1%)
Side
Left 114 (49.8%)
Right 103 (45%)
Both 110 (4.4%)
Not applicable 112 (0.9%)
Brain
Cortical 137 (59.8%)
Subcortical 182 (35.8%)
Both 118 (3.5%)
SAHb 112 (0.9%)
Previous stroke 136 (15.7%)
Affected sensory system 198 (42.8%)
Affected reflexes 134 (58.7%)
Lowered consciousness 142 (19.3%)
Paresis
Arm 195 (85.2%)
Hand 192 (83.9%)
Leg 184 (80.3%)
Facialis 146 (63.8%)
Risk factors
Heart disease 152 (22.3%)
Atrial Fibrillation 114 (25.9%)
Myocardial infarction 122 (40.7%)
Other 118 (33.4%)
Vascular disease 170 (30.6%)
Carotic stenosis 131 (43.7%)
Hypercholesterolemia 111 (15.5%)
Arteriosclerosis 116 (8.5%)
AV malformation 115 (7.0%)
Other 118 (25.3%)
Diabetes mellitus 123 (10%)
Hypertension 168 (29.7%)
Contraception 110 (4.4%)
Obesity 112 (5.2%)
Smoking 170 (30.6%)

Note. SAH = Subarchnoid Hemorrhage; AV malformation = Arterio-Venous Malformation.

span of immediate verbal recall. The RBMT, sustained, focused attention, and directed visual
which consists of ecologically relevant tasks such shifting. For testing visual inattention a letter can-
as remembering a newspaper article, a route, a cellation task, which according to Halligan and
name, or an appointment, provides information Robertson (1994) is one of the most sensitive mea-
about the effects of impaired memory on activities sures of neglect, was used, as well as one photo-
of daily life. graph from the Picture Scanning subtest of the
Attention was investigated with the Trail Mak- Behavioural Inattention Task (BIT; Wilson, Cock-
ing Test, Parts A and B (TMT-A, TMT-B; Reitan, burn, & Halligan, 1987). The scoring of the TMT-
1958) which involves motor speed and agility, and A and TMT-B was slightly modified in order to
the WAIS Digit Symbol subtest which involves control for differences in response times and cor-
506 JACQUELINE HOCHSTENBACH ET AL.

rections made by the patient. Therefore, the num- RESULTS


ber of correct connections made by the patients
was scored in addition to the time score. Table 2 gives a general overview of the tests
Visuospatial and visuoconstructive functions
employed as well as the mean scores and the
were assessed by the WAIS Block Design subtest,
the Bobertag (Bobertag, 1914; Karpov, Luria, & standard deviations for the patients and the con-
Yarbuss, 1968), a structured clock test (with dif- trols. The stroke patients as a group have lower
ferent levels of difficulty), a clock-drawing task scores indicating poorer performance than con-
(Freedman, 1991), the copying task of the BIT, and trols. On almost all tests the performance of the
by Money’s Road Map Test (Money, 1976) for stroke patients differs from the performance of
left-right orientation. The Bobertag shows a draw- the controls. The only exceptions are the WAIS
ing of a complex social situation for which the par-
ticipant has to construct a logical story. The struc-
Information subtest, the immediate recall of a
tured clock test consists of two parts: in the first story, the number of correct connections of
part the patient has to set the time in five circles TMT-A, the picture scanning task, the clock
(clocks). In the second part the time has to be read test, word comprehension, and recognizing
from 20 clocks which have a decreasing level of money. On these tests the scores differ only
difficulty. The first 5 clocks have only the two minimally from the scores of the controls.
hands, the next 5 clocks have markings for the Table 2 gives only a general overview of the
quarter hour, the following 5 clocks have markings
for every 5 minutes, and finally 5 digital clocks mean scores, and therefore no insight is given
were shown. For aphasics, a multiple choice ver- into the number of patients performing less well
sion was available. and also no answer is given to the question
Language was evaluated with the Similarities whether there is a relevant problem or not. In
subtest from the WAIS and several subtests from Table 3 the percentage of patients with a prob-
the Dutch Aphasia Society (SAN; Deelman, lem are listed for each test. The percentages are
Liebrand, Koning-Haanstra, & van den Burg,
expressed in terms of statistical relevance as
1987), concerning word comprehension, sentence
comprehension, verbal fluency (in the category well as in terms of clinical relevance. For statis-
‘animals’), and naming. The Similarities subtest is tical relevance, the scores of the patients were
a test of verbal concept formation and provides a standardized into z scores by using the mean and
sensitive measure of abstract thinking. When pa- the standard deviation of the control group. A
tients named a difference instead of a similarity, standardized score of –2 (or 2 in those cases
they were given another chance instead of record- where a higher score indicates a worse perfor-
ing an incorrect response. Different aspects of
mance), was taken as a cutoff point; that is, test
writing were assessed as well. Only the results of
the task about writing-on-command have been results which differed by more than 2 SDs from
used in the present article as the other tasks (spon- the results of the controls were seen as clearly
taneous writing and copying) were highly corre- pathological. However, a clinical criterion was
lated with this test (respectively, r = .88 and r = also employed, that is, the 15th percentile score
.95). In order to decide whether writing was af- of the control group. Thus, all patients whose
fected or not, qualitative aspects such as scrib- scores fall below the 15th percentile of the con-
bling, spatial disorders, agrammatisms, spelling,
and other linguistic errors were taken into account. trol group (or above the 85th percentile score in
Arithmetic skills were assessed by different as- cases where a higher test score indicates a worse
pects of handling money: recognizing, counting, performance) are classified as patients with a
and arithmetic. deficit on that test.
Testing length was tailored to the individual From the percentages of dysfunction in the
abilities of the patients. This generally resulted in 15th percentile, it can be inferred that slowness
three testing sessions to complete the assessment,
of information processing forms the main prob-
in order to avoid fatigue interference.
lem, that is to say, about 70% of the patients
showed difficulties in performing adequately
under conditions of time pressure (as can be de-
rived from the time scores on Tests 13, 15, 16,
17, 21 in Table 3). Even when using the more
COGNITIVE DECLINE FOLLOWING STROKE 507

Table 2. Performance of Stroke Patients and Controls on Neuropsychological Tests.

Patients Controls

Measures n M SD n M SD
a
Orientation (0–4) 216 .36 .78 33 0 .00
Memory
AVLT immediate recall (0–75) 194 32.53 10.98 33 43.79 12.04
AVLT delayed recall (0–15) 191 6.08 3.34 33 9.76 3.43
AVLT recognition (0–15) 186 26.99 3.04 32 28.47 3.13
WAIS Information (0–22) 207 9.77 5.26 33 11.24 5.08
WAIS Digit Span (0–24) 213 9.02 3.56 32 11.72 2.40
Forward (0–12) 199 4.87 2.18 32 5.78 1.86
Backward (0–12) 199 4.10 1.97 32 5.94 1.41
RBMT profile score (0–24) 204 17.67 4.33 33 20.76 2.77
Story immediate recall (0–21) 205 6.65 3.82 33 7.65 3.48
Story delayed recall (0–21) 205 4.81 3.58 33 6.48 3.83
Attention
TMT-A correct (0–24) 222 23.59 2.02 33 23.97 .17
TMT-A time in s 223 86.34 55.84 33 39.03 14.82
TMT-B correct (0–24) 184 17.28 7.93 33 22.06 6.06
TMT-B time in s 185 203.28 133.09 33 84.21 31.96
WAIS Digit Symbol (0–115) 211 27.36 15.20 33 46.73 14.17
Letter cancellation
Time 220 166.75 99.16 32 88.03 27.36
Omissions left 221 2.57 4.40 33 .24 .50
Omissions right 221 1.47 2.85 33 .33 .85
Picture scanning (0–3) 222 2.49 .82 33 2.55 .56
Visuospatial/constructive
WAIS Block Design (0–26)
With time limit 225 7.51 5.49 33 17.39 5.78
No time limit 165 11.36 6.36 33 20.03 6.52
Bobertag (– 20–35) 200 14.48 7.07 33 18.06 6.36
Clock test (0–20) 222 15.65 9.11 32 18.19 1.23
Clock drawing (0–10) 223 7.82 2.42 32 9.75 .62
Copying (0–19) 222 11.73 3.87 33 15.67 2.31
Money’s Road Map
Time 197 129.4 79.97 32 79.19 41.27
Defaults (0–32) 197 7.65 5.96 33 4.85 5.14
Language and arithmetic
WAIS Similarities (0–26) 199 11.25 6.40 32 15.75 5.36
Word comprehension (0–18) 222 17.75 1.09 33 18 .00
Sentence comprehension (0–45) 222 42.36 4.31 33 44.64 .90
Verbal fluency (0–54) 219 21.52 10.52 33 33.58 6.42
Naming (0–18) 218 16.5 4.00 33 18 .00
Writing (0–1) 213 .33 .47 33 .03 .17
Recognizing money (0–12) 223 11.78 1.03 33 12 .00
Counting money (0–10) 223 8.85 2.13 33 9.94 .35
Arithmetic with money (0–20) 212 14.25 6.00 33 17.82 2.72

Note. AVLT = Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; RBMT = Rivermead
Behavioural Memory Test; TMT = Trail Making Test.
a
Numbers in parentheses indicate range of possible scores.
508 JACQUELINE HOCHSTENBACH ET AL.

Table 3. Percent of Stroke Patients Falling below 2 Standard Deviations or the 15th Percentilea.

Measures 2 SD 15th percentile


Orientation
1. Totalb –d –
Memory
2. AVLT Immediate recall 13.9 31.4
3. AVLT delayed recall 20.9 47.5
4. AVLT recognition 11 26.7
5. WAIS Information 4.4 20.3
6. WAIS Digit Span 34.7 50.2
7. Forward 14.1 34.6
8. Backward 40 58.5
9. RBMT 31.9 42.6
10. Story immediate recall 8.3 34.6
11. Story delayed recallc – 38.3
Attention
12. TMT-A correctb –e –
13. TMT-A time 50.2 68.2
14. TMT-B correct 25 56.2
15. TMT-B time 56.4 71.4
16. WAIS Digit Symbol 29.9 69.7
Letter cancellation
17. Time 51.4 72.3
18. Omissions left 35 49.5
19. Omissions right 26.2 43.9
20. Picture scanning 10 17.7
Visuospatial/constructive
WAIS Block Design
21. With time limit 46.7 80.2
22. No time limit 23.6 75.2
23. Bobertag 14 34.2
24. Clock test 46 50.5
25. Clock drawing 58.7 62.3
26. Copying 49.1 62.6
Money’s Road Map
27. Time 25.4 42.6
28. Defaults 15.2 34
Language and arithmetic
29. WAIS Similarities 25.1 41.1
30. Word comprehensionb –f –
31. Sentence comprehension 39 41
32. Verbal fluency 49.3 67.3
33. Namingb –g –
34. Writingb –h –
35. Recognizing moneyb –i –
36. Counting moneyb –j –
37. Arithmetic 30 46.1

Note. AVLT = Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; RBMT = Rivermead
Behavioural Memory Test; TMT = Trail Making Test.
a
Based on standardized residual scores.
b
For these tests almost all controls had a perfect score (see also Table 2), so it was not possible to determine
either correct SD or percentile scores.
c
Due to large variation in the scores of the control group it was not possible to obtain a score that would be 2 SD
lower than the mean of the group
d
22.2% of the patient group had a less than perfect score.
e
13.1% of the patient group had a less than perfect score.
f
10.4% of the patient group had a less than perfect score.
g
22.5% of the patient group had a less than perfect score.
h
32.9% of the patient group had a less than perfect score.
i
8.5% of the patient group had a less than perfect score
j
33.6% of the patient group had a less than perfect score.
COGNITIVE DECLINE FOLLOWING STROKE 509

rigorous statistical cutoff criterion, more than ‘‘verbal response’’ tests were the AVLT,
50% of the patients showed impairments in this RBMT, TMT-A, TMT-B, Bobertag, Money’s
cognitive domain. Furthermore, the data show Road Map, WAIS Similarities, sentence com-
clear attentional deficits. Left-sided neglect was prehension, verbal fluency, and arithmetic with
found in 50% (Test 18 in Table 3), and even money. The TMT is included in the ‘‘verbal re-
right-sided neglect was prominent (Test 19). sponse’’ tests because knowing the alphabet is a
From the figures for other cognitive processes, it sine qua non for the completion of the test. The
can be concluded that at least 40% of all patients ‘‘nonverbal response’’ tests were WAIS Digit
had difficulty with visuospatial and constructive Span, letter cancellation, WAIS Block Design,
tasks (Tests 21–28) and with language skills and clock drawing, copying, and WAIS Digit Sym-
arithmetic. In the language domain especially bol.
verbal fluency (Test 32) seems to cause diffi- The MANOVA procedure from SPSS was
culty. The least affected domain seems to be used to analyze the influence of a number of
memory, but even there at least 30% showed a variables. In both the analyses the educational
significantly poorer performance than the con- level and the age of the patients were used as
trols. Furthermore, results show that long-term covariates. Although educational level is scored
memory seems to be more affected than does on an ordinal scale, we chose to use it here as if
short-term memory (Test 3 vs. 2, 11 vs. 10) and it was an interval one. There were 11 indepen-
that recognition is least affected (Test 4). The dent variables:(1) gender, (2) type of stroke (in-
RBMT (Test 9) shows that 42.6% had difficulty farct or hemorrhage), (3) side of stroke (left or
remembering everyday information. right), (4) cortical or subcortical stroke, (5) the
interval between the stroke and the neuropsy-
Influence of Clinically Relevant Variables chological assessment (NPA), (6) single or mul-
Only the tests on which the stroke patients tiple strokes, (7) level of consciousness on ad-
performed less well than the controls were mission in the hospital, (8) inclusion in high risk
taken as a starting point for further statistical group (heart disease, vascular disease, diabetes,
analysis. Orientation and writing were excluded hypertension, smoking), (9) presence or absence
because of a ‘‘ceiling effect’’, caused by the fact of paresis of the hand (based on the scores on
that the scores obtained by the patient group muscle strength: affected or not), (10) presence
were close to the perfect score. The test results of receptive aphasia, or (11) an expressive apha-
that were analyzed further were: the scores on sia. The variable ‘‘receptive aphasia’’ was based
the AVLT immediate and delayed recall, WAIS on the scores for sentence comprehension,
Digit Span, RBMT, TMT-A, TMT-B, WAIS which were dichotomized; the variable ‘‘expres-
Digit Symbol, letter cancellation, WAIS Block sive aphasia’’ was based on the scores on verbal
Design (with time limit), Bobertag, clock draw- fluency. These last two independent variables
ing, copying, Money’s Road Map, WAIS Simi- were used only in the analysis of the results ob-
larities, sentence comprehension, verbal fluency, tained with the nonverbal response tests. The
and arithmetic with money. The results were interval between the stroke and the time of as-
divided into two data clusters: a cluster derived sessment (variable 5) was divided into four cate-
from tests where a verbal response was needed, gories: 3 to 37 days (n = 57), 37 to 70 days (n =
and a cluster derived from tests where such a 57), 70 to 95 days (n = 57), and more than 95
verbal response was not required. This was done days (n = 58).
because a joint analysis of all tests would lead to The effect of these independent variables was
the undesirable effect that aphasic patients tested in two multivariate analyses, one for the
would be dropped from the analysis because tests which involved verbal responding, and one
they were not able to complete the tests requir- for the nonverbal response tests. Because the
ing a verbal response. Moreover, this grouping large number of possible interactions would
of tests allows us to study the influence of apha- clearly hinder the adequate interpretation of the
sia on the ‘‘nonverbal response’’ tests. The results, only the main effects will be examined.
510 JACQUELINE HOCHSTENBACH ET AL.

The residual variance was used as the error DISCUSSION


term.
Table 4 shows the significant results of the The main purpose of the present article is to give
multivariate analysis of the verbal response tests a comprehensive overview of the gravity and
(vr), whereas Table 5 shows the results of the frequency of cognitive changes following
nonverbal response tests (nvr). Those variables stroke. The data clearly show that stroke has a
that proved to be significant in the univariate massive effect on many cognitive processes.
tests are reported as well, together with the Tatemichi et al. (1994), who defined failure by
means of the different patient groups. a cutoff score in the fifth percentile (comparable
The analysis of the vr tests shows that only to 1.65 SD, which is less rigorous than the statis-
gender and the side of the stroke had a signifi- tical criterium chosen in this study), had basi-
cant multivariate effect. The analysis for gender cally the same results, although the percentages
indicates that women performed better on mem- of dysfunction in their study were lower. What-
ory tasks, but that men performed better than ever the cutoff, the present data show that the
women on visuospatial tasks. Performance was effects of stroke upon cognitive functioning lead
better after right-sided lesions than after left- to far-reaching and dramatic changes, with a
sided lesions with the exceptions of WAIS general slowing in information processing and
Block Design and the letter cancellation. The attentional deficits being the most prominent.
analysis of the nvr tests showed a significant The question now becomes relevant whether the
effect for the type and the side of stroke. For the reduced performance on other tasks can be seen
vr tests no significant difference was found for as a consequence of this general slowing. Al-
the type of stroke (F (14, 92) = .92, p = .543). though it seems plausible to assume that slow-
The results of the nvr tests further show a better ness of information processing as a nonspecific
performance for three tests after an infarction deficit will have some effect on other test re-
compared to the performance following a hem- sults, it cannot explain the full range of deterio-
orrhage. Furthermore, no significant effect of ration, as the present data reveal that, even with-
gender on the results of the nvr-tests was found out time limits, patients with a stroke still have
(F (8, 135) = 1.48, p = .171). The data also show considerable problems performing adequately
that aphasia had a disruptive effect on test per- on a number of tests. Not only the effect of
formance, even on tests where verbal responding slowness of information processing, but the ef-
played a minor role. fect of motor problems on neuropsychological
No significant effect on neuropsychological test performances, should be further elaborated.
test scores was found for cortical versus subcor- The fact that we found no effect of hand paresis
tical lesions (vr: F (14, 92) = 1.76, p = .058; nvr: does not mean that there is no such effect. Per-
F (8, 135) = 1.04, p = .413), the interval between haps it is a consequence of the relatively crude
stroke and NPA (vr: F (42, 272) = 1.26, p = measure (Dvir, 1997) and the use of a more sen-
.145; nvr: F (24, 401) = 1.12, p = .314), single sitive measure like the Purdue Pegboard might
versus multiple strokes (vr: F (14, 92) = 1.48, p indicate that some of the results are reflective of
= .131; nvr: F (8, 135) = .53, p = .836), lowered motor disability required in task performance.
consciousness on admission (vr: F (14, 92) = The distinction we made between tests requir-
.81, p = .657; nvr: F (8, 135) = .97, p = .460), ing a verbal response and tests requiring no such
the presence of risk factors (vr: F (14, 92) = response may seem a bit arbitrary since in fact
1.02, p = .443; nvr: F (8,135) = .95, p = .480), or each test requires some involvement of language
a paresis of the hand (vr: F (14, 92) = .95, p = (e.g., understanding the instructions). However,
.505; nvr: F (8, 135) = 1.35, p = .226). although this division has its shortcomings, it
does offer the opportunity to analyze the results
of aphasic patients who otherwise would have
been omitted from the sample because of unob-
tainable data. Hence, the data of an important
Table 4. MANOVA Results for Verbal Response Tests, with Univariate Analyses to Indicate Significant Difference

Independent n F df p Univariate F df p M n M n
variables
Gender 119 4.50 14, 92 .000 1, 105 Male 71 Female 48

AVLT immediate recall 15.62 .000 31.08 37.69


AVLT delayed recall 14.29 .000 5.48 7.56
Money’s Road Map time 5.17 .025 106.31 137.46
Money’s Road Map de- 10.30 .002 5.11 8.25
faults

COGNITIVE DECLINE FOLLOWING STROKE


Side of the stroke 119 2.94 14, 92 .001 1, 105 Left 46 rRght 73

RBMT profile score 6.52 .012 17.17 19.14


AVLT immediate recall 15.90 .000 29.09 36.68
AVLT delayed recall 6.82 .010 5.24 7.00
WAIS Similarities 5.13 .026 11.41 13.48

Note. AVLT = Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; RBMT = Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.

511
512
Table 5. MANOVA Results for Nonverbal Response Tests, with Univariate Analyses to Indicate Significant Difference.

Independent n F df p Univariate F df p M n M n
variables

Type 158 2.46 8, 135 .016 1, 142 Infarct 138 Bleed- 20


ing
WAIS Block Design w.t.l. 10.47 .002 8.22 5.00
copying 10.44 .002 12.18 9.65
WAIS Digit Symbol 11.87 .001 27.44 20.25

Side of the 158 3.92 8, 135 .000 1, 142 Left 73 Right 85


stroke

JACQUELINE HOCHSTENBACH ET AL.


WAIS Digit Span 14.57 .000 7.62 10.12
letter cancellation o.l. 14.24 .000 1.37 3.65
WAIS Block Design w.t.l. 12.85 .000 8.82 6.94

Receptive 158 3.92 8, 135 .000 1, 142 Yes 92 No 66


aphasia
letter cancellation o.l. 13.78 .000 3.47 1.39
latter cancellation o.r. 8.57 .004 1.99 .44
WAIS Block Design w.t.l. 16.72 .000 6.09 10.21
clock drawing 13.61 .000 7.33 9.05
copying 8.27 .005 10.96 13.12
WAIS Digit Symbol 11.06 .001 22.67 31.91

Expressive 158 2.96 8, 135 .004 1, 142 Yes 75 No 83


aphasia
letter cancellation o.r. 8.28 .005 2.12 .64
WAIS Digit Symbol 22.14 .000 20.61 31.88

Note. WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; w.t.l = with time limit; o.l. = omissions left; o.r. = omissions right; WAIS.
COGNITIVE DECLINE FOLLOWING STROKE 513

subgroup of patients could be analyzed, showing mental processes should be considered as the
an overall effect of aphasia on the test results. combined work of both hemispheres.
The rather general character of the cognitive During the last two decades a growing inter-
decline needs some further discussion. Indeed, est can be observed in the relationship between
at first sight it may seem remarkable that the subcortical structures and cognitive processes.
relation between type of stroke, number and lo- Research in degenerative diseases like Parkin-
cation of strokes on the one hand, and the fre- son’s and Huntington’s disease have shown that
quency and gravity of cognitive disorders, on subcortical structures are involved in a number
the other hand, is so weak. However, as early as of cognitive processes. To our knowledge, little
1966 Luria indicated that higher mental func- or no research has been devoted to the differ-
tions are the result of a complex and dynamic ence between the effect of subcortical versus
interaction between a number of brain structures cortical strokes on cognitive functioning. In the
united in functional systems and that these struc- present study no significant difference between
tures make their own contribution to the dy- subcortical and cortical strokes was found.
namic whole. A cognitive function, therefore, However, more studies are needed to determine
should not be understood in terms of being a the robustness of these findings.
direct property of a specialized group of cells, We wanted to determine whether relevant
located at a certain ‘‘spot’’ in the brain, but as variables could be isolated with a predictive
the end result of dynamic neural networks, ‘‘power’’ for cognitive functioning. Most of
territorially scattered throughout the brain (for a these independent variables have been distilled
recent discussion see Alexandrov & Jarvilehto, from the literature on functional recovery. It has
1993). This is an important argument, still rele- been shown, for example, that functional out-
vant for modern neuropsychology as it under- come following infarction is better than after
scores the dynamic and integrated character of hemorrhage (Abu-Zeid, Choi, Hsu, & Maini,
the cerebral cortex. Many of the results of the 1978; Allen, 1984; Bamford, Sandercock, Den-
present study can be explained in light of these nis, Burn, & Warlow, 1990; Bonita, Ford, &
arguments. Comparable results have been re- Stewart, 1988; Foulkes, Wolf, Price, Mohr, &
ported by Hom and Reitan (1990) who came to Hier, 1988). De Haan, Limburg, van der
the same conclusion as Luria: Neuropsychologi- Meulen, Jacobs, and Aaronson (1995), however,
cal functioning is dependent on the overall gen- found no such difference for quality of life when
eral integrity of the brain (see also Lezak, 1995). comparing patients with infratentorial infarc-
Table 5 shows that aphasia has a far-reaching tions with those with hemorrhages, whereas
impact on general cognitive functioning. In light Mori, Sadoshima, Ibayashi, Iino, and Fujishima
of the above-mentioned arguments on the inte- (1994) found that the type of stroke did not in-
grated character of the brain, these results are fluence the degree of cognitive dysfunctioning.
less striking. In accordance with Luria (1966), The present study indicated that only the results
language should be considered as the means by of 3 of the 22 (sub)tests showed a significant
which all other mental functions can be articu- difference in favor of the infarction, which
lated; no single complex form of human mental raises the question whether it is correct to con-
activity can take place without the direct or indi- clude that, in general, cognitive functioning af-
rect participation of speech. Speech activity, ter infarction is better than following hemor-
therefore, should not be limited to the processes rhage.
related to verbal communication, but should be Desmond, Tatemichi, Paik, and Stern (1993)
seen as a ‘‘higher regulator of human behav- have shown that the presence of vascular risk
ior’’, enabling us to make abstractions and gen- factors (like diabetes mellitus and hypercholes-
eralizations, or to encompass relationships and terolemia) in a stroke-free cohort of elderly per-
associations between objects and events extend- sons leads to cognitive decline. Furthermore, it
ing far beyond the limits of direct sensory per- has been shown that vascular risk factors are
ception. Hence, the ‘‘linguistic organization’’ of negative predictors for survival and functional
514 JACQUELINE HOCHSTENBACH ET AL.

recovery after stroke (Anderson, Jamrozik, REFERENCES


Broadhurst, & Stewart-Wynne, 1994; Lai, Alter,
Friday, & Sobel, 1995; Olsson, Viitanen, Abu-Zeid, H.A.H., Choi, N.W., Hsu, P.H., & Maini,
Asplund, Eriksson, & Hagg, 1990; Sacco, Wolf, K.K. (1978). Prognostic factors in the survival of
1,484 stroke cases observed for 30 to 48 months. I.
Kannel, & McNamara, 1982). Although for
Diagnostic types and descriptive variables. Ar-
functional or sensorimotor recovery these risk chives of Neurology, 35, 121-125.
factors may play an important role, the present Alexandrov, Y., & Jarvilehto, T. (1993). Activity ver-
study indicates that for the cognitive outcome sus reactivity in psychology and neurophysiology.
these risk factors have no significant influence. Ecological Psychology, 5, 85-103.
Allen, C.M.C. (1984). Predicting outcome of acute
Although several studies (Anderson, Boure- stroke: A prognostic score. Journal of Neurology,
strom, Greenberg, & Hildyard, 1974; Boure- Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 47, 475-480.
strom, 1976; Jongbloed, 1986; Katz, Ford, Anderson, T.P., Bourestom, N.C., Greenberg, F.R., &
Chinn, & Newill, 1966; Rankin, 1957) reported Hildyard, V.G. (1974). Predictive factors in stroke
that for functional recovery a previous stroke rehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Rehabilitation, 55, 545-553.
was an adverse prognostic indicator, this finding Anderson, C.S., Jamrozik, K.D., Broadhurst, R.J., &
could not be confirmed for cognitive function- Stewart-Wynne, E.G. (1994). Predicting survival
ing. Finally, in studies about recovery it has also for 1 year among different subtypes of stroke: Re-
been shown that during the first few months sults from the Perth Community Stroke Study.
most patients with stroke show considerable re- Stroke, 25, 1935-1944.
Babikian, V.L., Wolfe, N., Linn, R., Knoefel, J.E., &
covery of function, although the exact extent Albert, M.L. (1990). Cognitive changes in patients
and duration of this recovery is less certain with multiple cerebral infarcts. Stroke, 21, 1013-
(Feigenson, McDowell, et al., 1977; Feigenson, 1018.
McCarthy, et al., 1977, Ferrucci et al., 1993; Bamford, J., Sandercock, P., Dennis, M., Burn, J., &
Kotila, Waltimo, Niemi, Laaksonen, & Lem- Warlow, C. (1990). A prospective study of acute
cerebrovascular disease in the community: The
pinen, 1984; Ogden, Mee, & Henning, 1993; Oxfordshire community stroke project-1981–1986.
Wade, Wood, & Langton Hewer, 1985, 1988). 2. Incidence, case fatality rates and overall out-
In the present study, however, no significant come at one year of cerebral infarction, primary
difference in cognitive functioning between pa- intracerebral and subarachnois haemorrhage. Jour-
tients who were assessed at different poststroke nal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
53,16-22.
intervals was found. Nevertheless, caution Bobertag, O. (1914). Uber intelligenzprüfungen. Leip-
should be taken, because it might well be that zig: Ambrosius Barth.
the cognitive impairment observed in this study Bonita, R., Ford, F.A., & Stewart, A.W. (1988). Pre-
would not be present to this extent, especially in dicting survival after stroke: A three-year follow-
those patients who were tested soon after their up. Stroke, 19, 669-673.
Bourestom, N.C. (1967). Predictors of long-term re-
stroke. Moreover, it would be interesting to ex- covery in cerebrovascular disease. Archives of
plore the extent of recovery at a 1- or 2-year fol- Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 48, 415-
low-up. 419.
In summary, the results of this study show an Brooks, D.N. (1975). Memory and head injury. Jour-
extensive general decline in cognitive function- nal of Nervous and Mental Disease , 155, 350-355.
Deelman, B.G., Liebrand, W.B.G., Koning-Haanstra,
ing following stroke. The implication of the re- M., & van den Burg, W. (1987). Een afasietest
sults for neurological rehabilitation are clear: voor auditief taalbegrip en mondeling taalgebruik
Focusing too much on specific problems can [An aphasia test for auditive language comprehen-
lead to the underestimation of the general effects sion and verbal language use]. Lisse: Swets &
Zeitlinger.
of a brain lesion, which may hinder the design of
Desmond, D.W., Tatemichi, T.K., Paik, M., & Stern
an optimal rehabilitation process. Y. (1993). Risk factors for cerebrovascular disease
as correlates of cognitive function in a stroke-free
cohort. Archives of Neurology, 50, 162-166.
Downhill, D.E., & Robinson, R.G. (1994). Longitudi-
nal assessment of depression and cognitive impair-
COGNITIVE DECLINE FOLLOWING STROKE 515

ment following stroke. Neurosurgical Clinics of Kappelle, L.J., Adams, H.P., Heffner, M.L., & Tomer,
North America, 182, 425-431. J.C. (1994). Prognosis of young adults with isch-
Dvir, Z. (1997). Grade 4 in manual muscle testing: emic stroke. Stroke, 25, 1360-1365.
The problem with submaximal strength assess- Karpov, B.A., Luria, A.R., & Yarbuss, A.L. (1968).
ment. Clinical Rehabilitation, 11, 36-41. Disturbances of the structure of active perception
Ebrahim, S., Nouri, F., & Barer, D. (1985). Cognitive in lesions of the posterior and anterior regions of
impairment after stroke. Age and Ageing, 14, 345- the brain. Neuropsychologia, 6, 157-158.
348. Katz, S., Ford, A.B., Chinn, A.B., & Newill, V.A.
Feigenson, J.S., McDowell, F.H., Meese, P., McCar- (1966). Prognosis after strokes: Long-term course
thy, M.L., & Greenberg, S.D. (1977). Factors in- of 159 patients. Medicine, 45, 236-246.
fluencing outcome and length of stay in a stroke Kotila, M., Waltimo, O., Niemi, M.L., Laaksonen, R.,
rehabilitation unit. Part 1. Analysis of 248 un- & Lempinen, M. (1984). The profile of recovery
screened patients – Medical and functional prog- from stroke and factors influencing outcome.
nostic indicators. Stroke , 8, 651-656. Stroke, 15, 1039-1044.
Feigenson, J.S., McCarthy, M.L., Greenberg, S.D, & Kwa, V.I.H., Limburg, M., Voogel, A.J., Teunisse, S.,
Feigenson, W.D. (1977). Factors influencing out- Derix, M.M.A., & Hijdra, A. (1996). Feasibility of
come and length of stay in a stroke rehabilitation cognitive screening of patients with ischaemic
unit. Part 2. Comparison of 318 screened and 248 stroke using the CAMCOG: A hospital-based
unscreened patients. Stroke, 8, 657-663. study. Journal of Neurology, 243, 405-409.
Ferrucci, L., Bandinelli, S., Guralnik, J.M., Lamponi, Lai, S.M., Alter, M., Friday, G., & Sobel, E. (1995).
M., Bertini, C., Falchini, M., & Baroni, A. (1993). Prognosis for survival after an initial stroke.
Recovery of functional status after stroke: A post Stroke, 26, 2011-2015.
rehabilitation follow-up study. Stroke, 24, 200-205. Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment.
Foulkes, M.A., Wolf, P.A., Price, T.R., Mohr, J.P., & New York: Oxford University Press.
Hier, D.B. (1988). The stroke data bank: Design, Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man.
methods, and baseline characteristics. Stroke, 19, New York: Basic Books.
547-554. Money, J. (1976). A standardized Road Map Test of
Friedman, P.J. (1991). Clock drawing in acute stroke. direction sense. Manual. San Rafael: Academic
Age and Ageing, 20, 140-145. Therapy Publications.
Galasko, D., Klauer, M.R., Hofstetter, R., Salmon, Mori, S., Sadoshima, S., Ibayashi, S., Iino, K., &
D.P., Lasker, B., & Thal, L.J. (1990). The Mini- Fujishima, M. (1994). Relation of cerebral blood
Mental State Examination in the early diagnosis of flow to motor and cognitive functions in chronic
Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 7, 49- stroke patients. Stroke, 25, 309-317.
52. Mysiw, W.J., Beegan, J.G., & Gatens, P.F. (1989).
Galski, T., Bruno, R.L., Zorowitz, R., & Walker, J. Prospective cognitive assessment of stroke patients
(1993). Predicting length of stay, functional out- before inpatient rehabilitation. The relationship of
come, and aftercare in the rehabilitation of stroke the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination
patients. The dominant role of higher-order cogni- to functional improvement. American Journal of
tion. Stroke, 24, 1794-1800. Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 68, 168-
Haan, R. de, Limburg, M., Meulen, J. van der, Jacobs, 171.
H., & Aaronson, R. (1995). Quality of life after Novack, T.A., Haban, G., Graham, K., & Satterfield,
stroke: Impact of stroke type and lesion location. W.T. (1987). Prediction of stroke rehabilitation
Stroke, 26, 402-408. outcome from psychologic screening. Archives of
Halligan, P.W., & Robertson, I.H. (1994). The assess- Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 68, 729-
ment of unilateral neglect. In J.R.Crawford, D.M. 734.
Parker, & W.W. McKinlay (Eds.), A handbook of Ogden, J.A., Mee, E.W., & Henning, M. (1993). A
neuropsychological assessment (pp. 151-177). prospective study of impairment of cognition and
Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum. memory after subarachnoid hemorrhage. Neurosur-
Hochstenbach, J., Donders, A.R.T., Mulder, Th., gery, 33, 572-587.
Limbeek, J. van, Schoonderwaldt, H. (1996). The Olsson, T., Viitanen, M., Asplund, K., Eriksson, S., &
long-term outcome after stroke: A disability-ori- Hagg, E. (1990). Prognosis after stroke in diabetic
ented approach. International Journal of Rehabili- patients: a controlled prospective study. Diabeto-
tation Research, 19, 189-200. logia, 33, 244-249.
Hom, J., & Reitan, R.M. (1990). Generalized cogni- Rankin, J. (1957). Cerebral vascular accidents in pa-
tive function after stroke. Journal of Clinical and tients over the age of 60: Prognosis. Scottish Medi-
Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 644-654. cal Journal, 2, 200-215.
Jongbloed, L. (1986). Prediction of function after Reitan, R.M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making
stroke: A critical review. Stroke, 17, 765-776. Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Per-
ceptual Motor Skills, 8, 271-276.
516 JACQUELINE HOCHSTENBACH ET AL.

Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie. months. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. Psychiatry, 48, 7-13.
Sacco, R.L., Wolf, P.A., Kannel, W.B., & McNamara, Wade, D.T., Wood, V.A., & Langton Hewer, R.
P.M. (1982). Survival and recurrence after stroke. (1988). Recovery of function soon after stroke: A
Stroke, 13, 290-295. study of visual neglect, attention span and verbal
Stinissen, J., Willems, P.J., Coetsier, P., & Hulsman, recall. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
W.L.L. (1970). Handleiding bij de Nederlands- Psychiatry, 51, 10-13.
talige bewerking van de Wechsler Adult Intelli- Wade, D.T. (1992). Measurement in neurological re-
gence Scale (WAIS) [Manual for the Dutch transla- habilitation. New York: Oxford University Press.
tion of the WAIS]. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeit- Wechsler, D. (1955). Wechsler Adult Intelligence
linger. Scale. Manual. New York: Psychological Corp.
Sundet, K., Finset, A., & Reinvang, I. (1988). Neuro- Wilson, B.A., Cockburn, J., & Baddeley, A.D. (1985).
psychological predictors in stroke rehabilitation. The Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy- Titchfield, Fareham Hants: Thames Valley Test
chology, 10, 363-379. Company.
Tatemichi, T.K., Desmond, D.W., Stern, Y., Paik, M., Wilson, B.A., Cockburn, J., & Halligan, P. (1987).
Sano, M., & Bagiella, E. (1994). Cognitive impair- Behavioural Inattention Test. Titchfield, Fareham
ment after stroke: Frequency, patterns, and rela- Hants: Thames Valley Test Company.
tionship to functional abilities. Journal of Neurol- Wilson, B.A., Cockburn, J., Baddeley, A.D., & Hiorns
ogy, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 202-207. RW. (1989). The development and validation of a
Wade, D.T. (1993). Measurement in neurological re- test battery for detecting and monitoring everyday
habilitation. Current Opinion in Neurology, 6, 778- memory problems. Journal of Clinical and Experi-
784. mental Neuropsychology, 11, 855-870.
Wade, D.T., Wood, V.A., & Langton Hewer, R.
(1985). Recovery after stroke-The first three
COGNITIVE DECLINE FOLLOWING STROKE 517

APPENDIX

Dutch Educational System.

Level Years of education


1 (< 6 years elementary school)
2 6
3 8
4 9
5 10-11
6 12-18
7 (university) > 18

You might also like