You are on page 1of 5

Current Biology

Magazine

being linked to immunity, stress Primer in posterior parietal cortex is founded


responses, basal metabolic rate and primarily on neurophysiological
behavioural traits. This means that
pale-to-dark colour variation along
Posterior parietal recordings from animal models, starting
in the 1970s in non-human primates,
climatic gradients could simply be a cortex followed by a smaller number of rodent
side-effect of selection on other traits. studies beginning in the 1980s. In
Indeed, owls provide support for this addition to neurophysiology, a number
idea. Many owls have two morphs, a Jonathan R. Whitlock of labs now use genetically encoded
pale grey one which is generally found calcium indicators to image the activity
at higher latitudes and a darker reddish The posterior parietal cortex, along with of hundreds to thousands of neurons at
morph more common closer to the temporal and prefrontal cortices, is one a time in behaving animals. Long before
equator. In tawny owls (Figure 2), the grey of the three major associative regions the development of modern recording
morph survives better in harsh winters in the cortex of the mammalian brain. techniques, however, the first insights
and the converse is true in snow-poor It is situated between the visual cortex into posterior parietal function came from
winters. This could explain not only at the caudal pole of the brain and the clinical observations of human patients
the geographic distribution of grey somatosensory cortex just behind the recovering from stroke or head injuries.
and rufous morphs but also temporal central sulcus. Technically, any cortex In this Primer I will discuss some of the
patterns: warming conditions have led covered by the parietal bone is referred more illuminating (and bizarre) clinical
to an increase in reddish-morph tawny to as ‘parietal cortex’, but the posterior cases before returning to the modern
owls. Grey morphs also have fewer sector, formally referred to as posterior state of the field.
blood parasites and mount stronger parietal cortex, is indeed its own
immune responses than rufous morph functional section of cortex, consisting Clinical deficits: spatial coding and
individuals. Links between melanin- of Brodmann’s areas 5, 7, 39, and 40 embodiment
based coloration and behavioural in humans, areas 5 and 7 in macaques, One of the earliest characterizations of
and physiological traits suggest that and area 7 in rodents (Figure 1). Whereas behavioral deficits following damage
pleiotropic effects could underlie Gloger’s the anterior parietal cortex in humans to posterior parietal cortex came from
rule. However, pleiotropic links have been comprises primary somatosensory areas, the Austro-Hungarian physician Rezsö
mainly documented for MC1R, which is the posterior parietal cortex has several Bálint. In 1909, he published a study
not the only locus causing variation in higher-order functions. It is referred to as detailing the symptoms of patients with
melanin-based colour. Whether similar an ‘associative’ cortical region because it bilateral stroke damage to the posterior
pleiotropic links exist for other loci is not is neither strictly sensory nor motor, but parietal cortex and parieto-visual
known. Understanding the genes and combines inputs from a number of brain border areas. His patients presented
metabolic processes underlying melanin areas including somatosensory, auditory, three major common symptoms:
colouration opens up new avenues to visual, motor, cingulate and prefrontal simultagnosia, the inability to perceive
assess the mechanisms behind Gloger’s cortices, and it integrates proprioceptive more than one item in the visual field;
rule. Until then, we will have to agree with and vestibular signals from subcortical oculomotor apraxia, difficulty in making
Ernst Mayr in that: “The precise selective areas. targeted eye movements; and optic
factors responsible for Gloger’s rule are By virtue of its vast connectivity ataxia, the inability to make visually-
still a mystery.” (Figure 2), different portions of posterior guided arm and hand movements. In
parietal cortex participate in multiple the case of optic ataxia, for example, a
Where can I find out more? cognitive processes including, but not patient could look directly at an object
Burtt Jr, E.H., and Ichida, J.M. (2004). Gloger’s rule, limited to, sensorimotor integration, in front of them, name it, but not be able
feather-degrading bacteria, and color variation
among song sparrows. Condor 106, 681–686. spatial attention, spatial navigation, to grasp it. This trio of deficits, referred
Caro, T. (2005). The adaptive significance of coloration decision making, working memory, to as ‘Bálint’s syndrome’, provided the
in mammals. BioScience, 55, 125–136.
Ducrest, A.L., Keller, L., and Roulin, A. (2008). early motor planning, as well as first major clues that posterior parietal
Pleiotropy in the melanocortin system, coloration more complex behaviors such as cortex was critical to the construction
and behavioural syndromes. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23,
502–510. pantomiming the use of objects. It also of a map of peripersonal space and the
Galván, I., and Solano, F. (2016). Bird integumentary mediates some abstract and symbolic coordination of actions in it.
melanins: Biosynthesis, forms, function and
evolution. Internat. J. Mol. Sci. 17, 520.
cognitive capacities, including the Contemporaneous work by British
Koski, M.H., and Ashman, T.L. (2015). Floral representation of real and imagined neurologists Henry Head and Gordon
pigmentation patterns provide an example of spatial relationships, as well as Holmes found that damage to parietal
Gloger’s rule in plants. Nat. Plants 1, 14007.
Rensch, B. (1929). Das Prinzip geographischer numerical quantity and mathematical cortex was often associated with a
Rassenkreise und das Problem der Artbildung. abilities. Though each of these functions profound lack of awareness of bodily
Gebrüder Bornträger, Berlin.
Roulin, A. (2014). Melanin-based colour polymorphism currently comprises a proper subfield posture or the position of limbs, leading
responding to climate change. Glob. Change Biol. in neuroscience (and therefore cannot them to propose the concept of ‘body
20, 3344–3350.
Zink, R.M., and Remsen Jr., J.V. (1986). Evolutionary be discussed at length here), it was schema’. Body schema, according
processes and patterns of geographic variation in not always clear that posterior parietal to them, represent an individual’s
birds. Curr. Ornithol. 4, 1–69. cortex performed such a diverse continuous awareness of how his/her
School of Biological Sciences, Monash panoply of cognitive feats. body and its parts are configured in
University, 3800 Clayton, Victoria, Australia. Our current understanding of the three-dimensional space, providing a
E-mail: kaspar.delhey@monash.edu many types of neural representations “standard against which all subsequent

Current Biology 27, R681–R701, July 24, 2017 © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. R691
Current Biology

Magazine

Human Both classical and modern-day clinical


observations support the interpretation
that our sense of corporeal awareness
Macaque and self-localization arise from an
amalgamation of co-registered sensory
signals: we feel like we are in our own
Rat
bodies because our brains tell us so, and
we remain unaware of this fact unless
the process of sensory integration is
perturbed.

Posterior parietal cortex and


Primary visual Superior PPC Somatosensory *Brains not to scale navigation
Secondary visual Inferior PPC Primary motor The role of posterior parietal cortex in
Current Biology representing bodily position and spatial
orientation is not limited to peripersonal
Figure 1. Topography of posterior parietal cortex relative to other cortical areas. space, but includes movement over
Lateral view of human, macaque and rat brains, showing the organization of visual, posterior pari- larger spatial scales during navigation.
etal, somatosensory and primary motor areas of cortex. The ordering of cortical areas is the same
for all mammals, with the visual areas furthest posterior, posterior parietal cortex lying between
Spatial navigation is a complex behavior
visual and somatosensory areas, and primary motor areas in front of somatosensory cortex. that involves the interaction of multiple
brain systems, and though posterior
changes of posture are measured before Modern day clinical investigations parietal cortex likely contributes in
they enter consciousness... every new continue to underscore the importance multiple ways, several lines of evidence
posture or movement is recorded on this of posterior parietal cortex in generating point toward a role in formulating
plastic schema”. the sense of embodiment. Several navigational routes. One of the key early
The essential role of posterior parietal insights have been gleaned through insights into this came from a study
cortex in generating body image was studying the effects of evaluative brain by Eleanor Maguire and others, who
later confirmed and expanded upon stimulation in open-skulled epileptic measured levels of brain activity using
in Macdonald Critchley’s definitive patients prior to surgery. In a fascinating functional magnetic resonance imaging
clinical monograph, The Parietal Lobes, study published by Blanke et al. in 2002, (fMRI) while subjects navigated through
published in 1953. In it, Critchley neurosurgeons applied focal electrical a virtual town. They found that several
discussed not only patients with stimulation to different sectors of cortex, cortical and sub-cortical areas showed
disturbances in body image and various including the ventral-most part of heightened activation over the course of
motor apraxias, but a broader spectrum parietal cortex, near the occipital border various tasks, with inferior and medial
of neurological disorders including in the right hemisphere. Much to the parietal areas showing the highest
sensory disturbances, deficiencies in surprise of the surgeons, stimulating this activation when subjects computed
symbolic thought, mathematical abilities part of the brain induced an out of body sequences of turns and movements
and visuospatial attention. A few patients experience in one female patient who, to reach a goal. The hippocampus,
exhibited autotopagnosia, or the inability upon stimulation, reported “I see myself on the other hand, was engaged
when instructed to correctly locate a lying in bed, from above, but I only see during allocentric, or map-like, spatial
body part, while others showed a total my legs and lower trunk”. Subsequent processing and navigation.
loss of awareness of body parts, often stimulation induced similar feelings of A subsequent study in Maguire’s lab
the fingers. In rare cases, patients were ‘lightness’ and ‘floating’ near the ceiling imaged the brains of London taxi drivers
unaware that they were paralysed on the of the room, two meters above the bed. while they took pretend passengers
left or right half of the body, while others In 2006, Blanke’s team published to specific destinations in a ‘virtual
exhibited hemispatial neglect, ignoring another study in which they applied London’. The taxi drivers then watched
either their left or right visual hemifields stimulation to the junction of the left videos of their performance after the
altogether. temporal and parietal lobes, which task and gave post-hoc reports of what
Together, these studies crystallized the caused that patient to report the they were thinking at various stages
notion that posterior parietal cortex plays presence of a ‘shadow person’ hovering of each journey. In line with previous
a critical role generating and guiding just behind her, mimicking her body findings, it was found that medial
spatial awareness, as well as one’s sense positions and movements. When she parietal cortex was activated during
of orientation, limb location and how leaned forward and grasped her knees, ‘movement’ planning in the immediate
their statuses co-vary during movement. for example, she sensed that he leaned environment, such as changing lanes,
The neural representation of this last forward as if to embrace her around the while lateral parietal areas were most
feature — the position of body parts waist. active when drivers performed extended
relative to one another — was precisely In both studies, the locations of the route planning beyond the present
measured during neurophysiological stimulations were in the very ventral location.
recordings in monkeys and termed ‘gain parietal region, which is a site of These functional imaging studies
modulation’ more than 70 years after the massive confluence for visual, tactile, also resonate with a parallel line of work
work of Head and Holmes. proprioceptive and vestibular signals. investigating parietal-damaged patients’

R692 Current Biology 27, R681–R701, July 24, 2017


Current Biology

Magazine

abilities to perform ‘mental space


travel’ through familiar remembered
landscapes. In this work, from the lab
of Morris Moscovitch, it was found that Orbitofrontal
Orrbiito
O itof
tto
offro
fronta
rontta
al
long-term residents of Toronto, Canada Frontal
Fronta
Fro
F rro
ront
onnta
nttta
al
who had suffered prior posterior parietal motor
otttor
mot
mo
m o orr areas
o are
a
arre
reaas
s
cortical damage had no problems
recalling a detailed image of the city, but
could not navigate mentally between
known locations. Their subjective Somatosensory
S
Soomatosensory
omatosenso
ma
atosen
tossens
ensory
ry Striatum
Sttriatum
riatum
experience of imagined navigation
was described as “impoverished and
disembodied” relative to controls.
Together, these and other studies Retrosplenial
Retrosple
Retrosplenia
Retrossplen
plenial
have led to the interpretation that Thalamus
Th
haala
amus
mus
halamus
posterior parietal cortex is critically
involved in transforming world-based
spatial information from landmarks into PPC
C
Pre-subiculum
Prre-su
e-ssu
ubbiiculu
bicu
re-subiculum umm
first-person (egocentric) movement Auditory
Auditor y cortex
ccor
or tex
sequences required to reach a goal.
The work in humans is paralleled
marvelously by multiple recording studies
in animal models, including rats and Superior
Sup
Sup rior colliculus
erior
e
perior collic
ccollicu
olliculus
lus
macaque monkeys. Clear evidence of Visual
Vis
Visu
uaal areas
ar
a reas
are as
route mapping in the rat posterior parietal
Current Biology
Biol
cortex was reported in a 2006 paper by
Douglas Nitz, in which parietal cells were
recorded while freely behaving animals Figure 2. Cortical and sub-cortical connections of posterior parietal cortex.
Shown in schematic form, a given neuron in posterior parietal cortex can receive input and send
traversed irregular multi-part tracks
output to a large number of areas in different systems of the brain. The diversity of connec-
(Figure 3A). It was found that many of tions speaks to the variety of behaviors in which posterior parietal cortex participates, including
the posterior parietal neurons fired only decision-making, spatial attention, working memory, movement planning, navigation, as well as
when a particular movement, such as processing visual, somatosensory and auditory signals.
a left turn, was made at a certain point
along the journey, and that the movement First-person route-mapping functions a function at the interface between
correlates of the cells changed depending have also been described in the medial egocentric and allocentric frames of
on which path the animals ran. The firing parietal region in monkeys. In a 2006 reference.
fields were the same whether the lights study by Sato et al., neurons in the
were on or off, indicating that the cells medial parietal region were recorded in Gain modulation in posterior parietal
followed the animals’ internally generated macaques while they used a joystick cortex
sense of ‘route’ as opposed to visual to navigate through a virtual house to The co-registration of signals across
landmarks in the room. reach instructed end-locations. More reference frames in general, whether
Further evidence of first-person route than 75% of the neurons that were active they are body-based or world-based,
mapping came from experiments by my in the task appeared to encode certain is a cardinal feature of neural coding
colleagues and I (Whitlock et al. 2012), ‘movements’, such as a right turn, made in posterior parietal cortex. Take, for
in which we found that the ‘route’ to at specific locations, for example, before example, swatting a fly buzzing just
which posterior parietal cells were tuned the stairs. Similar to the findings in rats, above your head: when you first hear
did not even have to be physical: simply the firing fields in this task were specific the sound of the fly, you move your
having rats run in north–south sequences to the different routes the animals eyes and head up to spot it, and
in an open arena was sufficient to took, and pharmacological inactivation without hesitation you can swing at it
elicit the same firing patterns recorded of the medial parietal region caused with your hand. In order for the sensory
in posterior parietal cortex when the the animals to become lost during information regarding the fly’s location
animals ran in a real maze consisting of navigational trials. to be of service to your hand, visual
north–south alleys. This is not to say that And much as in monkeys, lesions and auditory signals must first be
posterior parietal cortex does not make of posterior parietal cortex in rats and transformed into reference frames that
use of external landmarks when they mice result in navigational deficits, often are intelligible to your motor system. This
are available — on the contrary, a recent in selecting the correct trajectory to process of coordinate transformation
paper by Wilber et al. (2014) reports that reach a goal. Thus, studies in humans, occurs over several steps, including the
subsets of posterior parietal cells in rats monkeys and rats point to a key role for co-registration of the location of the fly
encode movement types as well as the posterior parietal cortex in constructing image on your retina with where your
direction of goal locations relative to the first-person route maps which can be eyes are in their orbits, which is in turn
animal’s heading. calibrated against external landmarks, co-registered with head position relative

Current Biology 27, R681–R701, July 24, 2017 R693


Current Biology

Magazine

has been reported during a variety of


A B
Visual + spatially guided behaviors, including
auditory movements of the eyes, head, arm,
cues hand, whole body and even the locus
Choose Choose of one’s attention. Previous work
left right specifically demonstrated that cells
in the parietal reach region encode
the end goals of hand movements
relative to eye position (Figure 3C) — a
computation which would indeed come
in handy when trying to swat away a
flying pest. In light of these and many
other electrophysiological studies,
the neural mechanisms underlying
spatially targeted action and attention
have become much clearer in recent
decades, providing an increasingly
detailed framework for understanding
C the symptomologies described in both
Fixate Cue for reach Reach early and ongoing clinical studies.

Imitation and mirror neurons


Perhaps one of the most unusual
features of posterior parietal cortex
is its involvement in coordinating not
only actions in first-person, but in
imitating observed actions of others.
To borrow the earlier parlance of Henry
Head, posterior parietal cortex enables
Neural
firing one to map their own ‘body schema’
Time Time Time onto that of another individual, and
Current Biology parietal cortical damage can impair
this ability in a condition referred to as
Figure 3. Behavioral paradigms for studying posterior parietal cortex. visuo-imitative apraxia. The inability to
Common animal models used for studying posterior parietal cortex include monkeys, rats and imitate observed movements is part
mice, though posterior parietal cortex anatomy and physiology have been studied in several of the larger family of motor apraxias
species, including cats, bats, ferrets, pro-simian galagos, and new-world monkeys. Different
observed following parietal damage,
species bring different advantages depending on the experimental question. (A) Rodents are
a popular model for studying spatial navigation since neural activity can be recorded in freely- and has been best documented as
behaving subjects as they move about different environments. Route-tracking functions were deficits in mimicking hand positions
described in posterior parietal cortex in rats running irregular paths as shown. (B) Rats and mice and hand gestures. For example,
have also been studied in evidence accumulation and decision-making tasks. Here, instructional patients with left parietal damage have
auditory and visual stimuli are delivered at a certain frequency while the rat keeps its nose in a been reported as having difficulty in
center port; based on the frequency, the animal must decide to orient left or right to get a liquid
pantomiming the use of various objects,
reward. (C) Studies in head-fixed primates have provided the foundation for understanding the
behavioral neurophysiology of posterior parietal cortex. Shown here is an example of a visually- such as hammering a nail, whether they
instructed reaching task, in which the animal moves its hand to where a stimulus was flashed were instructed to do so verbally or by
on the screen (middle). The hypothetical neuron (spikes shown below) fires maximally when the visual demonstration. Such patients
hand moves to the target directly above where they eyes are fixated. It illustrates the findings could still use a real hammer and nail,
of Batista et al., which showed that neural activity in the parietal cortex encoded reach-goals in and they could identify such behaviors
eye-centered coordinates.
performed by someone else, but they
lacked the ability conjure up the motor
to your shoulders and the horizon, which As pointed out earlier, the concept program using sensory or verbal input.
is co-registered with your shoulder of gain modulation was evident already Conversely, functional imaging work by
position relative to your torso, and so at the turn of the 20th century, but it Frey and Gerry (2006) revealed that the
on. Each step is an example of a neural was not recorded in the brain until brains of normal individuals showed
computation termed ‘gain modulation’, the 1980s, when Richard Andersen heightened activation in the inferior
in which the coding of one variable and Vernon Mountcastle showed parietal cortices and ventral pre-motor
(the image of the fly on your retina) is that the coding of a visual stimulus cortices when passively viewing hand–
multiplied by an independent variable in the monkey parietal area 7a varied object interactions, and that the level of
(the orbital position of your eyes relative dramatically depending on where activation was larger when viewing with
to your head), leading to a single signal the animal’s eyes were fixated. This the intention to imitate the movements
encoding both features simultaneously. elegant form of sensory interweaving afterward.

R694 Current Biology 27, R681–R701, July 24, 2017


Current Biology

Magazine

At the cellular level, sensory-motor posterior parietal cortex clearly of the light-sensitive neurons of the posterior
parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 3, 532–548.
matching has been recorded directly in participates in a manifold of cognitive Arzy, S., Seeck, M., Ortigue, S., Spinelli, L., and
the form of ‘mirror’ neurons, which are functions, and just a few have been Blanke, O. (2006). Induction of an illusory shadow
person. Nature 443, 287.
neurons that fire whether a particular touched upon here. While the present Batista, A.P., Buneo, C.A., Snyder, L.H., and Andersen,
action is made or merely observed. discussion was centered around the R.A. (1999). Reach plans in eye-centered
Mirror neurons were discovered in the theme of body schema and neural coordinates. Science 285, 257–260.
Balint, R. (1909). Seelenlähmung des “Schauens,”
monkey pre-motor cortex in the lab of coding in first-person, the parietal optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der
Giacomo Rizzolatti, where it was found cortex also plays a major role in Aufmerksamkeit. Eur. Neurol. 25, 51–66, 67–81.
Behrmann, M., Geng, J.J., and Shomstein, S.
that neurons that fired when the animal shaping how we see the world ‘out (2004). Parietal cortex and attention. Curr. Opin.
grabbed a piece of food also fired when there’. For example, the deficiencies Neurobiol. 14, 212–217.
the monkey simply watched one of the in spatial attention seen after parietal Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., and Seeck, M.
(2002). Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions.
experimenters do the same. Since then, damage extend well beyond corporeal Nature 419, 269–270.
mirror neurons have been described in awareness, and include features and Carandini, M., and Churchland, A.K. (2013). Probing
other areas of the cortical motor system, images in the outside world. perceptual decisions in rodents. Nat. Neurosci.
16, 824–831.
including the inferior parietal lobe and Nowhere is this more clearly Ciaramelli, E., Rosenbaum, R.S., Solcz, S-, Levine, B.,
primary motor cortex, as well as other evidenced than in patients with and Moscovitch, M. (2010). Mental space travel:
damage to posterior parietal cortex prevents
areas which process emotions and facial hemispatial neglect who, when asked egocentric navigation and re-experiencing of
expressions. As far as parietal cortex is to copy a picture of a clock or house, remote spatial memories. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn.
concerned, a cleverly designed study will only copy the right half of the Mem. Cogn. 36, 619–634.
Critchley, M. (1953). The Parietal Lobes (New York, NY:
by Fogassi et al. (2005) recorded from picture. Fascinating neurophysiological Hafner Press).
neurons in the inferior parietal lobe of recordings have also shown that Duhamel, J.-R., Colby, C.L., and Goldberg, M.E.
(1992). The updating of the representation of
macaques while they grasped pieces the locus of visual receptive fields in visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye
of food to either eat or place in a cup parietal area LIP will pre-emptively movements. Science 255, 90–92.
by their mouth. Subsets of cells were shift in the direction of an impending Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P.F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S.,
Chersi, F., and Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal
modulated by the end-goal of the action, eye movement, again illustrating the lobe: from action organization to intention
firing only when grasping was followed quintessential role for posterior parietal understanding. Science 308, 662–667.
Frey, S.H., and Gerry, V.E. (2006). Modulaiton of neural
by eating or placing the food in a cup. neurons in linking together the ‘inside’ activity during observational learning of actions
Incredibly, the authors recorded from and ‘outside’ worlds. Other major topics and their sequential orders. J. Neurosci. 26,
mirror neurons that showed similar goal- of study regarding posterior parietal 13194–13201.
Head, H., and Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory
specificity for observed actions — that cortex include selective attention, disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 34,
is, the cells encoded what the monkey evidence accumulation, decision making 102–254.
Maguire, E.A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J.G., Frackowiak,
expected the demonstrator to do with and working memory, and entire reviews R.S., Firth, C.D., and O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing
the grasped good. on those topics are listed below (see where and getting there: a human navigation
There are few examples in the Further reading). network. Science 280, 921–924.
Niessen, E., Fink, G.R., and Weiss, P.H. (2014).
field that provide such a direct As modern experimental tools Apraxia, pantomime and the parietal cortex.
window on the intersection of motor advance and enable increasingly Neuroimage Clin. 5, 42–52.
Nitz, D.A. (2006). Tracking route progression in the
neurophysiology and social cognition. sophisticated questions, the field posterior parietal cortex. Neuron 49, 747–756.
Outside of monkeys, mirror neurons will continue to dissect the functions Sato, N., Sakata, H., Tanaka, Y.L., and Taira, M. (2006).
have been recorded in humans prior to of different cell types, microcircuits, Navigation-associated medial parietal neurons
in monkeys. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 103,
neurosurgery, and very elegant work and anatomical connections that link 17001–17006.
has directly recorded changes in the posterior parietal cortex with other Spiers, H.J., and Maguire, E.A. (2006). Thoughts,
behavior, and brain dynamics during navigation in
‘mirror’ properties of pre-motor neurons areas. For example, what are the inputs the real world. NeuroImage 31, 1826–1840.
in juvenile birds when they learned a new that a mirror cell receives that make Vallentin, D., Kosche, G., Lipkind, D., and Long,
song. While it has not yet been causally it a mirror cell, but not the cell next to M.A. (2016). Inhibition protects acquired song
segments during vocal learning in zebra finches.
demonstrated that mirror neurons it? As recording techniques advance Science 351, 267–271.
enable imitative learning in mammals, we also stand to gain deeper insights Whitlock, J.R., Sutherland, R.J., Witter, M.P.,
Moser, M.B., and Moser, E.I. (2008). Navigating
the unique coding properties of the cells into network-level computations from hippocampus to parietal cortex. Proc. Natl.
would provide a logical mechanism for implemented in posterior parietal Acad. Sci. USA 105, 14755–14762.
teaching a ‘blind’ motor system new cortex when solving tasks with different Whitlock, J.R., Pfuhl, G., Dagslott, N., Moser, M.B.,
and Moser, E.I. (2012). Functional split between
behaviors using visual or other sensory cognitive or behavioral demands. If we parietal and entorhinal cortices in rats. Neuron 73,
information. What remains a matter of neuroscientists are successful, common 789–802.
Wilber, A.A., Clark, B.J., Forster, T.C., Tatsuno, M.,
great debate, however, is the broader computational principles will begin to and McNaughton, B.L. (2014). Interaction of
role of the mirror neuron system in emerge which link parietal functions egocentric and world-centered reference frames
understanding the conceptual meaning that were previously taken as unrelated, in the rat posterior parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 34,
5431–5446.
of observed behaviors, and other and perhaps those computations will
processes such as emotional cognition be surprisingly similar across mice,
and social awareness. monkeys and humans. Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience,
Centre for Neural Computation, Egil and Pauline
Braathen and Fred Kavli Centre for Cortical
Conclusions FURTHER READING
Microcircuits, NTNU, Norwegian University of
As evidenced by the diversity of the Andersen, R.A., and Mountcastle, V.B. (1983). The Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway.
literature and sub-areas of research, influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability E-mail: whitlock@ntnu.no

Current Biology 27, R681–R701, July 24, 2017 R695

You might also like