You are on page 1of 10

Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Aggression and Violent Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aggviobeh

Neuropsychological functioning in child sexual abusers: A systematic review T


a,⁎ b,c
Daniel Turner , Martin Rettenberger
a
Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Mainz, Untere Zahlbacher Straße 8, 55131 Mainz, Germany
b
Department of Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg-University Mainz (JGU), Binger Straße 14-16, 55122 Mainz, Germany
c
Centre for Criminology (Kriminologische Zentralstelle – KrimZ), Viktoriastraße 35, 65189 Wiesbaden, Germany

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Impairments in neuropsychological functioning are emphasized among the core components in etiological
Executive function models about sexual offending against children. In order to distinguish those neuropsychological deficits that can
Inhibition specifically be found in child sexual abusers, the present systematic review summarizes all studies published
Set-shifting between 1980 and 2019 comparing neuropsychological functions of child sexual abusers with different control
Pedophilia
groups. The databases PubMed, Google scholar, and PsycInfo were used to find relevant studies. After applying
Sex offender
Neuropsychology
certain inclusion criteria (e.g., at least one comparison group), 25 studies were identified as being relevant for
the present review. Most evidence exists about the assumption that child sexual abusers have more deficits in set-
switching, inhibition, and verbal functioning compared to healthy controls. When comparing child sexual
abusers to non-sexual offenders or sexual offenders against adults, no distinct differences were found. Single
studies indicate that pedophilic child sexual abusers tend to have superior abstraction and planning abilities
compared to non-pedophilic child sexual abusers. Impairments in executive functioning could lead to more self-
regulatory deficits thereby increasing the likelihood of committing sexual offences. The findings of the present
review indicate that classifying child abusers based on their motivation to offend might help to dissolve some of
the existing empirical inconsistencies.

1. Introduction They suggested that there are three main executive functions: inhibi-
tion, updating, and shifting (Friedman et al., 2008; Friedman & Miyake,
Deficits in neuropsychological functioning are highly relevant in 2004; Miyake et al., 2000). Inhibition (synonym: response inhibition,
theories about the development of sexually aggressive behaviors (Tost interference inhibition, behavioral inhibition) refers to the ability to
et al., 2004; Turner et al., 2018; Ward & Beech, 2006). The term suppress a prepotent, activated or already ongoing action or impulse
‘neuropsychological functioning’ covers a wide range of cognitive that interferes with reaching a higher-level goal (Miyake et al., 2000;
abilities that are under the control of specific brain structures (Morgan Stahl et al., 2014). Updating describes the ability to continuously
& Ricker, 2016), including speed of information processing, attention, monitor the information stored in one's working memory, to adapt the
language, memory, and executive functions. Until today, executive working memory contents according to changing circumstances, and to
functions have received the most attention when it comes to the re- shield the working memory contents from distraction (Hofmann,
lationship with criminal behaviors (Ogilvie, Stewart, Chan, & Shum, Schmeichel, & Baddeley, 2012; Kane, Bleckley, Conway, & Engle, 2001;
2011). Miyake et al., 2000). Finally, shifting (synonym: set-shifting, task-
Executive functioning is a multifaceted neuropsychological con- switching, cognitive flexibility) describes the ability to shift between
struct referring to a set of top-down mental processes enabling an in- multiple tasks or mental sets subserving the same goal or the ability to
dividual to self-regulate and self-direct behavior towards a certain goal switch between multiple equivalent goals (Hofmann et al., 2012;
(Diamond, 2013; Snyder, Miyake, & Hankin, 2015; Suchy, 2009). Al- Miyake et al., 2000; Monsell, 2003).
though there is no unifying theory about executive functioning the Executive functions are to a large part genetically determined
approach proposed by Miyake et al. (2000) is among the most cited. (Barnes, Dean, Nandam, O'Connell, & Bellgrove, 2011; Friedman et al.,

Abbreviations: CSA, child sexual abusers; HC, healthy controls; NSO, non-sexual offenders; COWAT, Controlled Word Association Test; MFFT, Matching Familiar
Figures Test; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; CBT, Corsi Block Tapping test; RNTB, Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery;
LNNTB, Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Test Battery; CANTAB, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery

Corresponding author at: Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Mainz, Untere Zahlbacher Straße 8, 55131 Mainz, Germany.
E-mail address: daniel.turner@unimedizin-mainz.de (D. Turner).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2020.101405
Received 22 January 2019; Received in revised form 31 May 2019; Accepted 23 March 2020
Available online 20 April 2020
1359-1789/ © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

2008; Lee et al., 2012). However, an individual's executive functioning followed by legal consequences resulting in a further disinhibition
can also strongly be influenced by psychiatric conditions, for example, within the action selection and control system.
affective disorders or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Konrad Neuropsychological dysfunctions are not only seen as a central part
& Eickhoff, 2010; Snyder, 2013), specific personality traits like im- in the etiology of sexual offending in general but also in the develop-
pulsivity or hostility (Coolidge, Thede, & Jang, 2004), or traumatic ment of paraphilic sexual interests (Flor-Henry, 1987; Tost et al., 2004).
childhood experiences such as physical, emotional or sexual abuse The starting point of these theoretical assumptions was the finding that
(Gould et al., 2012; Navalta, Polcari, Webster, Boghossian, & Teicher, individuals suffering from bilateral lesions of the medial temporal lobe
2006). Due to their outstanding importance for goal-directed behaviors, (Klüver-Bucy syndrome) are frequently characterized by hypersexual
already more than 50 years ago researchers have discovered the sig- behaviors or the tendency to seek sexual stimulation from unusual or
nificance of neuropsychological and executive dysfunctions for the ex- inappropriate objects (e.g., children; Marlowe, Mancall, & Thomas,
planation of sexual offending (see, for example, Lang, 1993, for a more 1975; Ott, 1995). Building upon these findings, early neuropsycholo-
detailed description). In the meantime, a considerable amount of re- gical studies found that paraphilic sexual offenders performed sig-
search addressing neuropsychological functioning in sexual offenders nificantly worse compared to HC on neuropsychological tests tapping
has accumulated. into left frontal and temporal brain functioning (e.g., tests of verbal
learning or set-shifting; Hucker et al., 1986; Yeudall & Fromm-Auch,
1.1. The current state of research concerning neuropsychological 1979, 1982). Furthermore, sexual offenders showed significant EEG
functioning of sexual offenders alterations especially over left hemispheric and interhemispheric re-
gions (Flor-Henry, 1987; Flor-Henry, Lang, Koles, & Frenzel, 1991).
The current state of research is quite ambiguous and previous re- Combining these findings Flor-Henry (1987; see also Flor-Henry et al.,
searchers have criticized that most studies used relatively hetero- 1991) hypothesized that a pathological neural organization of the left
geneous experimental and control groups (Adjorlolo & Egbenya, 2016; (dominant) hemisphere leads to sexually deviant representations and
Joyal, Beaulieu-Plante, & de Chantérac, 2014). Summarizing the main due to disturbed interhemispheric connections only these sexually de-
results of previous reviews, it can be noted that sexual offenders in viant representations are able to induce an orgasm (Flor-Henry, 1987).
general seem to have significant deficits in neuropsychological func- Some years later, Tost et al. (2004) described a pedophilic disorder
tioning compared to healthy controls (HCs) and non-sexual offenders as a reward deficiency syndrome due to its phenomenological overlap
(NSOs; e.g., Adjorlolo & Egbenya, 2016; Joyal et al., 2014; Joyal, Black, with compulsive-impulsive spectrum disorders. They further hypothe-
& Dassylva, 2007). sized that—just like in other disorders from the compulsive-impulsive
Impairments could be found in most neuropsychological functions spectrum (e.g., pathological gambling, binge eating disorder, or tri-
such as verbal abilities, working memory, cognitive flexibility, atten- chotillomania)—there exists a specific rewarding content of interest,
tion and concentration, visual/spatial abilities, abstract reasoning, re- i.e., sexual activity with children, which is the consequence of deficient
sponse and behavioral inhibition, and planning (Adjorlolo & Egbenya, striato-thalamo-cortical processing loops (Tost et al., 2004). To test this
2016). Sexual offenders against adults and sexual offenders against hypothesis Tost et al. (2004) assessed the neuropsychological func-
children have not explicitly been compared yet. Based on these findings tioning of four pedophilic male subjects with a neuropsychological test
different etiological theories and models acknowledging the relevance battery tapping into all cognitive domains relevant for their model and
of neuropsychological dysfunctions in the development of sexually of- found that the pedophilic men were severely impaired in most of the
fensive behaviors have been published (e.g., Cohen et al., 2002; Flor- proposed domains (up to five standard deviations below the mean of
Henry, 1987; Tost et al., 2004; Ward & Beech, 2006). the normative sample).
However, none of these early studies compared the performance of
1.2. Theoretical approaches concerning sexually offensive behaviors pedophilic and non-pedophilic participants and thus it remains unclear
whether the impairments were due to a pedophilic disorder or rather
In their Integrated Theory of Sexual Offending Ward and Beech (2006, due to the fact that all included individuals were sexual offenders. This
2016) described sexual offending as a result of certain clinical symp- is all the more important because deficits in neuropsychological func-
toms or state factors, for example, deviant sexual arousal, emotional tioning have been found in different offender populations, including
problems, social difficulties, or cognitive distortions. These clinical sexual offenders against children, sexual offenders against adults, and
symptoms, in turn, are generated through the ongoing interaction of non-sexual violent offenders (Ogilvie et al., 2011). To find out whether
biological factors (e.g., genetic inheritance and brain development), or not sexual offenders against children differ from other offenders
environmental factors (e.g., social, cultural, and personal circum- concerning their neuropsychological profile it is necessary to compare
stances), and neuropsychological functioning. According to Ward and the neuropsychological functioning of different offender subgroups,
Beech (2006, 2016) there are three interlocking neuropsychological which was the starting point of the current review.
systems: a motivational/emotional system, a perception and memory
system, and an action selection and control system. 1.3. Present study
The motivational/emotional system is responsible for the creation
of goals and values and to adjust the motivational state to changing The present systematic review aimed at improving our under-
environmental circumstances and is comparable to the concept of set- standing of the neuropsychological functioning of sexual offenders
shifting. The perception and memory system's responsibility is to pro- against children and tries to answer the following questions:
cess sensory information and to create representations of objects and
events and to have these representations readily available for working 1.) Can sexual offenders against children be differentiated from non-
memory. The perception and memory system is comparable to the offending individuals or other offenders (e.g., non-sexual offenders,
concept of updating. Finally, the action selection and control system is sexual offenders against adults) according to their neuropsycholo-
responsible for planning, implementing, and evaluating action plans, gical profile?
and to control behavior, thoughts, and emotions; this system is com- 2.) Are there any differences in the neuropsychological profiles be-
parable to the concept of inhibition (Ward & Beech, 2006, 2016). Ac- tween pedophilic and non-pedophilic sexual offenders against
cording to this theory the commission of a sexual offence could lead to children?
the maintenance or escalation of offence-supportive neuropsychological 3.) Are there any differences in the neuropsychological profiles be-
deficits (Ward & Beech, 2006). For example, a first undetected sexual tween intra- and extra-familial sexual offenders against children?
offence could lead to the perception that sexual offending is not 4.) Are there any differences in the neuropsychological profiles

2
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

Fig. 1. PRISMA Flow Diagram for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses.

between offending and non-offending pedophilic individuals? have command of English and German language, only studies that were
conducted in these languages could be included in the systematic re-
view process; however, all of the identified studies were in English
2. Methods
language.
2.1. Data search
3. Results
The present systematic review was conducted according to the
PRISMA checklist for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (Moher, Altogether we identified 25 relevant studies, which were included in
Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, & The PRISMA Group, 2009). To identify our systematic review. Eight of these studies (Habermeyer et al., 2013;
relevant publications, we performed a systematic search of the scientific Kärgel et al., 2017; Langevin & Curnoe, 2008; Massau et al., 2017;
literature in PubMed, Google scholar, and PsycInfo using the following Perley-Robertson, Helmus, Derkzen, & Serin, 2016; Rosburg et al.,
combinations of keywords: pedophilia OR paraphilia OR sex offender 2018; Suchy, Eastvold, Strassberg, & Franchow, 2014; Turner et al.,
OR child molester AND neuropsychology OR neuropsychological OR 2018) were neither included in the most recent systematic review by
executive function OR brain function. Only journal articles that were Adjorlolo and Egbenya (2016) nor in the meta-analysis of Joyal et al.
published between 1980 and 1st of May 2019 were considered for in- (2014). All included studies were case control studies comparing neu-
clusion in the present review. Furthermore, the reference lists of all ropsychological functioning in sexual offenders against children (child
pertinent papers as well as the reference lists of previous reviews and sexual abusers; CSA) with either sexual offenders against adults, non-
meta-analyses (e.g., Adjorlolo & Egbenya, 2016; Blanchard, Cantor, & sexual offenders (NSO), or healthy controls (HC). In the following we
Robichaud, 2006; Joyal et al., 2007; Joyal et al., 2014; Lang, 1993) will provide a detailed textual analysis of the included studies. Each
were checked for additional studies not identified through the search section begins with a short summary of the main results, followed by a
engines (see Fig. 1). Relevant studies were selected based on the fol- more detailed study description. Because most studies had multiple
lowing inclusion criteria: 1.) published in a scientific journal, 2.) con- control groups and in order to avoid redundancies detailed study de-
tain a subgroup of sexual offenders against children whose results were scriptions are only given once although one study might fall under
reported separately, 3.) contain a comparison group, 4.) report on at different subheadings. Furthermore, Table 1 provides a targeted
least one neuropsychological outcome other than IQ, and 5.) English or synthesis of the findings concerning the most researched neu-
German language. Because the authors of the current systematic review ropsychological functions.

3
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

Table 1
Overview about the findings concerning the most frequently studied neuropsychological functions.
Set-switching Abracen, O'Carroll, & Ladha, 1991; Becerra-García & Egan, 2014; Cohen et al., 2002;
Set-switching was evaluated in 12 studies. All except of one study used the Trail- Cohen, Nesci, Steinfeld, Haeri, & Galynker, 2010; Eastvold, Suchy, & Strassberg,
Making-Test B or the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. In general, CSA showed more 2011; Joyal et al., 2007; Kruger & Schiffer, 2011; Langevin, Wortzman, Dickey,
deficits in set-switching than HC. However, it seems as if this difference is mainly Wright, & Handy, 1988; Langevin, Wortzman, Wright, & Handy, 1989; Massau et al.,
driven by non-pedophilic CSA. One study even found a superior set-switching 2017; Schiffer & Vonlaufen, 2011; Young, Justice, & Erdberg, 2012.
performance in pedophilic CSA compared to HC and non-offending pedophilic men.
Compared to NSO and sexual offenders against adults the findings were again less
clear. An equal number of studies could show performance deficits in CSA,
irrespective of whether or not they were diagnosed with a pedophilic disorder.
Inhibition Cohen et al., 2002; Cohen et al., 2010; Eastvold et al., 2011; Habermeyer et al., 2013;
Altogether, 10 studies evaluated inhibition performance in CSA. Most frequently the Joyal et al., 2007; Massau et al., 2017; Perley-Robertson et al., 2016; Schiffer &
Stroop Task, different versions of the Go/No-Go Task or of the Stop-Signal-Task were Vonlaufen, 2011; Suchy, Whittaker, Strassberg, & Eastvold, 2009; Turner et al.,
used to assess inhibition. While the Stroop Task measures interference inhibition, or 2018.
the ability to selectively attend goal-relevant stimuli and neglect distracting stimuli,
the Go/No-Go Task and the Stop-Signal-Task assess response inhibition, or the ability
to suppress an activated or already ongoing action (Stahl et al., 2014; Turner,
Sebastian, & Tüscher, 2017). CSA showed clearly more deficits in interference
inhibition compared to HC. Concerning response inhibition the findings of previous
studies are less clear, with some studies finding a worse performance in CSA
compared to HC, while others did not report any differences. Deficits in response
inhibition seem to be especially pronounced when sexual stimuli are used as Go- or
No-Go cues. More studies found deficits in inhibition in CSA compared to NSO,
however, a not negligible number of studies did not find any differences.
Attention Becerra-García & Egan, 2014; Cohen et al., 2002; Eastvold et al., 2011; Kruger &
Attention was assessed in eight studies altogether. Most frequently the Trail-Making- Schiffer, 2011; Langevin et al., 1988; Langevin et al., 1989; Schiffer & Vonlaufen,
Test A was applyed for the assessment of attention (six studies altogether). Only two 2011; Young et al., 2012
of the included studies found a weaker attention performance in CSA, in one study
compare to HC and in the other compared to NSO. Interestingly, in both studies this
accounted for intra-familial CSA only.
Planning Cantor et al., 2004; Cohen et al., 2002; Cohen et al., 2010; Eastvold et al., 2011;
Planning abilities were assessed in seven studies, always with the differing tests (the Massau et al., 2017; Schiffer & Vonlaufen, 2011; Valliant, Gauthier, Pottier, &
Tower of London Task was used in two studies). Two studies found a superior Kosmyna, 2000.
performance of pedophilic CSA compared to non-pedophilic CSA and opiate addicts.
No differences were found in comparison to HC, NSO and sexual offenders against
adults.
Verbal functioning Abracen et al., 1991; Cantor et al., 2004; Cohen et al., 2002; Cohen et al., 2010; Joyal
All in all, six studies assessed verbal functioning in terms of verbal memory or verbal et al., 2007; Schiffer & Vonlaufen, 2011.
fluency. While verbal fluency was in all cases evaluated using the Controlled Word
Association Task or comparable tests, verbal memory was always assessed with
different tasks. Again, compared to HC, CSA showed clear deficits in verbal fluency as
well as in verbal memory. No performance deficits were found compared to NSO.
While one study found impairments in CSA compared to sexual offenders against
adults, another study did not find any differences between the two groups.
Abstraction Eastvold et al., 2011; Langevin et al., 1988; Langevin et al., 1989; Young et al., 2012.
In total, four studies assessed abstraction performance. Three studies used the
Category Test of the RNTB. Two of the studies applying the Category Test found
performance deficits in CSA compared to NSO, while the third study reported about a
superior performance of CSA compared to sexual offenders against adults.
Working memory Becerra-García & Egan, 2014; Cantor et al., 2004; Eastvold et al., 2011; Massau et al.,
Four studies evaluated working memory. Of these three used the Digit Span Task. It 2017.
was found that CSA had poorer working memory capacity than HC. One study found
performance deficits in CSA compared to sexual offenders against adults, while one
study did not find any differences between CSA and NSO.

3.1. Differences between sexual offenders against children and healthy (LNNTB) for the assessment of neuropsychological functioning in 36
controls sexual offenders and 31 HC. The LNNTB is a standardized set of 14
different tests evaluating learning and cognitive skills. The submeasures
A total of 15 studies compared the neuropsychological functioning assess the following neuropsychological functions: motor performance,
between CSA and HC (Abracen et al., 1991; Becerra-García & Egan, rhythm, tactile and visual perception, receptive speech, expressive
2014; Cohen et al., 2002; Cohen et al., 2010; Habermeyer et al., 2013; speech, writing, reading, arithmetic, memory, intellectual processes,
Kärgel et al., 2017; Kruger & Schiffer, 2011; Massau et al., 2017; Plante, pathognomonic, left hemisphere function, and right hemisphere func-
Manuel, & Bryant, 1996; Rosburg et al., 2018; Schiffer & Vonlaufen, tion. The sexual offenders were further divided into sexual offenders
2011; Scott, Cole, McKay, Golden, & Liggett, 1984; Suchy et al., 2009; against adults and CSA; however, the authors did not report how many
Turner et al., 2018; Valliant et al., 2000). Thereby, CSA showed more individuals were assigned to each group. All sexual offenders were re-
impairments in inhibition (six out of ten studies), set-switching (four cruited from a state psychiatric facility. The whole sexual offender
out of seven studies), processing speed (four out of four studies), and group performed significantly worse on most submeasures of the
verbal functioning (three out of five studies). Furthermore, four studies LNNTB compared to the HC. Furthermore, CSA performed significantly
did not find any differences between CSA and HC on any of the included worse on all subtests compared to sexual offenders against adults.
neuropsychological measures. As the first and only so far, Plante et al. (1996) compared 80 Roman
The first study, which was conducted within the time period re- Catholic priests who had sexually abused children with 80 priests
levant for the present review was published by Scott et al. (1984). The without a history of sexually abusing minors. All participants were re-
authors used the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Test Battery cruited from a private psychiatric hospital specialized on the treatment

4
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

of clergy. Participants from the non-abusive group were mainly treated did not find any differences between CSA and NSO and one study even
because of affective, adjustment, or eating disorders. Neuropsycholo- found some performance advantages—in abstraction and problem-sol-
gical functioning of all participants was assessed using the Halstead- ving—in CSA compared to NSO.
Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery (RNTB). The RNTB is a com- In this context, Abracen et al. (1991) compared the performance in
prehensive neuropsychological test battery measuring, for example, set- set-switching (TMT–B), abstract reasoning (Coloured Progressive Ma-
switching, tactual performance, and motor speed but also provides a trices), and verbal memory (Williams' Verbal Learning Test) between
composite measure for overall neuropsychological functioning. On that CSA (n = 12) and non-violent NSO (n = 12) from one penitentiary with
composite score the two groups did not differ. Subtest performance was the performance of non-professional prison and hospital employees
not reported. (n = 13). Seven CSA had sexually abused male children only, while five
Cohen et al. (2002) compared the performance in verbal functioning had sexually abused female children only. None of the CSA was diag-
(Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised [WAIS-R] vocabulary test), nosed with a pedophilic disorder. CSA were significantly older than
set-switching (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [WCST] and Trail-Making- participants from the other two groups. Age, education, and current or
Test B [TMT-B]), visual attention (TMT-A), planning and impulse previous alcohol dependence were significantly correlated with one of
control (Gambling Task), interference inhibition (Stroop Test), and the neuropsychological measures and were thus inserted as covariates
verbal fluency (Controlled Oral Word Association Test [COWAT]) be- in all subsequent analyses. After controlling for these variables no dif-
tween 22 CSA and 24 HC. All CSA were diagnosed with a non-exclusive ferences were found between the three groups on any of the neu-
pedophilic disorder oriented towards female children based on the ropsychological measures. Furthermore, no differences in neu-
DSM-IV diagnostic criteria. They were all either on parole or probation ropsychological functioning were found when comparing CSA with one
following conviction for a contact sexual offence. No CSA was diag- victim with those who had multiple victims.
nosed with an axis I psychiatric disorder. No differences in demo- Langevin et al. (1988) compared the neuropsychological func-
graphic variables were found between the two groups. The CSA showed tioning of 91 intra-familial CSA with 36 non-violent NSO on different
a significantly worse performance in verbal functioning; however, when subtests from the RNTB. All participants were assessed at a local re-
controlling for education this difference disappeared. On all other tests search institution prior to their court trial. The intra-familial CSA were
the groups did not differ significantly. significantly older, less educated, and had lower IQ scores than the
With the intention to replicate and extend their previous findings NSO. Although the two groups differed concerning these variables the
Cohen et al. (2010) compared 51 CSA with 53 male and female opiate authors did not control for their influence in the subsequent analyses. It
addicts and 84 male and female HC. Again, all CSA were male, were was found that intra-familial CSA had a significantly poorer perfor-
diagnosed with a pedophilic disorder based on DSM-IV, and were re- mance in auditory attention and concentration, set-switching, psycho-
cruited from an outpatient facility. The opiate addicts had at least a motor speed, and abstraction than NSO. Furthermore, higher levels of
two-year history of opiate dependence; however, they had been ab- violence during the index offence and a non-biological relationship
stinent from illicit substances for at least six months at the time the between offender and victim were associated with more neuropsycho-
study was conducted. HC were recruited from the general population. logical deficits.
CSA and opiate addicts were significantly older and significantly less In another study using parts of the above sample Langevin and
educated than HC. Neuropsychological functions included verbal flu- Curnoe (2008) evaluated performance in the RNTB in 1180 sexual of-
ency (COWAT), reflection impulsivity and planning (Porteus Mazes), fenders including 849 CSA in comparison to 113 NSO of whom some
interference inhibition (Stroop Test), and set-switching (TMT–B, were convicted for a violent offence. In contrast to the previous studies
WCST). Because level of education was significantly correlated with of Langevin et al. (1988, 1989) only the global impairment score of the
neuropsychological functioning, it was entered as a covariate in all RNTB was analyzed and reported. It was found that sexual offenders did
analyses. In sum, CSA showed a significantly worse performance in set- not differ from the NSO concerning their overall impairment score.
switching and interference inhibition compared to HC and had a slower However, when comparing sexual offender subgroups, CSA were more
processing speed compared to HC and opiate addicts. On the other side, impaired than sexual offenders against adults. In order to identify the
CSA had superior planning abilities at least compared to the opiate influence of demographic and clinical variables on neuropsychological
addicts. performance, the authors calculated a stepwise logistic regression
Habermeyer et al. (2013) assessed response inhibition abilities using analysis showing that age, IQ, learning disorders, and endocrine ab-
a Go/No-Go task in 11 outpatient CSA and compared their performance normalities had a significant influence on neuropsychological perfor-
with seven HC from the general population. All CSA were diagnosed mance across all participants.
with a pedophilic disorder based on DSM-IV-TR. Five CSA were con-
victed because of a sexual offence against children involving direct 3.3. Differences between sexual offenders against children and sexual
physical contact with the victim(s), while six CSA were convicted of offenders against adults
internet child pornography-related offences. CSA showed significantly
slower reaction times on No-Go trials and had significantly lower d’ Seven studies could be identified, which compared the neu-
scores (= hit rate minus false alarm rate), indicating a significantly ropsychological performance between CSA and sexual offenders against
worse response inhibition performance than the HC. adults (Cantor et al., 2004; Joyal et al., 2007; Langevin et al., 1989;
Langevin & Curnoe, 2008; Perley-Robertson et al., 2016; Valliant et al.,
3.2. Differences between sexual offenders against children and non-sexual 2000; Young et al., 2012). While two studies found more neu-
offenders ropsychological deficits in CSA compared to sexual offenders against
adults, including impairments in verbal functioning, working memory,
In total, 10 studies could be identified concerning studies comparing immediate and delayed recall, and visuospatial memory, one study
CSA to non-sexual offenders (NSO) (Abracen et al., 1991; Eastvold found a superior performance in attention, set-switching, memory for
et al., 2011; Hucker et al., 1986; Langevin et al., 1988; Langevin et al., shapes, and abstraction in CSA. All other studies did not find any dif-
1989; Langevin & Curnoe, 2008; Perley-Robertson et al., 2016; Schiffer ferences between CSA and sexual offenders against adults.
& Vonlaufen, 2011; Suchy et al., 2014; Valliant et al., 2000). Impaired Cantor et al. (2004) compared 47 pedophilic CSA with 158 hebe-
neuropsychological functions found by most studies were inhibition philic CSA and 93 sexual offenders against adults using the WAIS-R.
(three out of five studies) and processing speed (two out of three stu- CSA were identified as pedophilic (sexual preference for children below
dies). Furthermore, CSA showed at least some impairments in attention, the age of 13) or hebephilic (sexual preference for children aged 13 to
set-switching, abstraction, verbal processing, and planning. Two studies 17 years) based on penile plethysmography (PPG) or self-report. All

5
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

participants were outpatients at the time of data collection. CSA had as well as NSO, the pedophilic CSA showed a significantly worse per-
significantly lower full-scale IQ scores than HC. Pedophilic CSA showed formance concerning visual-perceptual speed and speed of visual-motor
poorer verbal concept formation than sexual offenders against adults. integration and made more errors in the Modified Visual Inspection
Furthermore, pedophilic and hebephilic CSA showed poorer func- Time Task assessing visual-perceptual processing speed independent of
tioning in semantic knowledge, working memory, and in immediate response speed. No differences were found in motor speed between the
and delayed recall concerning verbal and visuospatial memory than three groups.
sexual offenders against adults. In a third study from the working group around Yana Suchy,
Neuropsychological performance of 12 CSA and eight sexual of- Eastvold et al. (2011) compared 30 pedophilic CSA, 30 non-pedophilic
fenders against adults on interference inhibition (Stroop Test), verbal CSA, and 29 NSO on different executive functioning tasks, including
fluency (COWAT), verbal memory (California Verbal Learning Test), set-switching, inhibition, abstraction, working memory, verbal fluency,
set-switching (WCST, TMT–B), and visuospatial integration (Rey- planning, and attention. All participants were in a residential treatment
Osterrieth Complex Figure Task) was assessed by Joyal et al. (2007). All program. Classification into pedophilic or non-pedophilic was based on
participants were from different forensic psychiatric hospitals and all self-report or PPG. The NSO had significantly lower IQ scores than the
participants had an extensive history of sexual offending (i.e., at least two CSA groups, while they did not differ concerning age or level of
one sexual re-offence). The groups did not differ concerning IQ or any education. Nevertheless, age and education were entered as covariates
of the included neuropsychological measures. in all analyses, while IQ was not. In sum, the authors found that both
Like other researchers, Young et al. (2012) used different subtests of CSA groups performed significantly worse on inhibition but had a sig-
the RNTB to compare neuropsychological functioning of 15 CSA and 45 nificantly superior performance in abstraction compared to NSO. Fur-
sexual offenders against adults. The two comparison groups did not thermore, pedophilic CSA performed significantly better than non-
differ concerning age or years of education and they were all placed in pedophilic CSA in planning.
one US prison at the time of study. CSA showed a superior performance Schiffer and Vonlaufen (2011) assessed attention and psychomotor
in auditory attention, set-switching, memory for shapes and abstraction speed (TMT-A), set-switching (TMT–B, WCST), verbal fluency (Re-
compared to sexual offenders against adults. gensburger Word Fluency Test), visuospatial and verbal memory
In a more recent study that was not included in previous systematic (Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, WMS-R), response inhibition (Go/
reviews, Perley-Robertson et al. (2016) evaluated response inhibition No-Go Task), and planning and problem-solving (Tower of London) in
abilities of 37 CSA, nine sexual offenders against adults, and 75 NSO 15 CSA diagnosed with a pedophilic disorder, 15 CSA without a ped-
using a Go/Stop impulsivity paradigm. All participants were from three ophilic disorder, 16 NSO, and 17 HC. The NSO were convicted for
different federal prisons in Canada. Although descriptively CSA were on homicide or grievous bodily harm and were recruited, just like the CSA,
average more than 14 years older than sexual offenders against adults from three different penitentiaries. The groups did not differ concerning
and more than 10 years older than NSO, it was not reported whether or age and level of education. CSA with and without a pedophilic disorder
not these differences reached the commonly used level of significance. showed a significantly worse performance in set-switching, verbal flu-
Independently of this issue, the three groups did not differ in their re- ency, and verbal memory compared to HC. Furthermore, both CSA
sponse inhibition performances. groups were significantly impaired in response inhibition in comparison
to HC and NSO. No other comparisons reached the pre-determined level
3.4. Differences between CSA subgroups of significance.
In a recent study 63 CSA were compared with 63 HC from the
3.4.1. Differences between pedophilic and non-pedophilic CSA general population on tasks assessing response inhibition (Go/No-Go
Six studies compared the neuropsychological functioning of CSA Task) and decision-making (Iowa Gambling Task, Game of Dice Task;
with a pedophilic disorder with CSA without a pedophilic disorder Turner et al., 2018). In contrast to previous studies applying the Go/No-
(Eastvold et al., 2011; Massau et al., 2017; Schiffer & Vonlaufen, 2011; Go Task computer-generated images of nude children and adults as Go-
Suchy et al., 2009; Suchy et al., 2014; Turner et al., 2018). Suchy et al. or No-Go cues were used. These sexual stimuli were also used within
(2009) compared the neuropsychological functioning of 20 pedophilic the Iowa Gambling Task, while the Game of Dice Task was performed in
CSA, 20 non-pedophilic CSA, and 20 HC. While the CSA were from its original version. Because the two comparison groups differed in
three different residential treatment sites, the HC were recruited from terms of age and years of education, all subsequent analyses controlled
the general population. In order to be classified as pedophilic the in- for these two variables. The CSA group had significantly more deficits
dividual CSA had to either acknowledge his sexual interest in pre- in response inhibition, irrespective whether child or adult images were
pubescent children, had to show greater arousal to prepubescent chil- used as Go- or No-Go cues. In the Iowa Gambling Task, CSA with more
dren than to adults using PPG, or had to have a score of four or five in intense pedophilic sexual interests, measured with the SSPI, showed
the Screening Scale for Pedophilic Interests (SSPI, Seto & Lalumiére, more impairments in decision-making because they chose images of
2001). The three groups did not differ concerning age, level of educa- children more regularly, irrespective whether child images were con-
tion, and IQ. The following neuropsychological functions were assessed: sidered as advantageous or disadvantageous stimuli. No differences
semantic knowledge (WAIS), interference inhibition (Stroop Test), fig- were found in decision-making using the original version of the Game
ural fluency (Ruff Figural Fluency Test), general executive functioning of Dice Task.
(Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale), auditory and visual memory (Wechsler
Memory Scale [WMS]), motor speed (Simple Choice Reaction Time 3.4.2. Differences between pedophilic CSA and pedophilic men who have
Task), and processing speed (Complex Choice Reaction Time Task). The not sexually offended against children
pedophilic as well as the non-pedophilic CSA performed worse on Also using a Go/No-Go task Kärgel et al. (2017) compared 40 CSA
general executive functioning compared to the HC. Furthermore, the diagnosed with a pedophilic disorder based on DSM-IV-TR, 37 in-
pedophilic CSA had a slower processing speed than the non-pedophilic dividuals with a pedophilic disorder who have not yet been convicted
CSA and the HC, while the non-pedophilic CSA performed more poorly because of a sexual offence against children, and 40 male HC from the
on semantic knowledge than the HC. general population. Pedophilic CSA were recruited from the commu-
In a follow-up study, Suchy et al. (2014) compared 20 pedophilic nity, outpatient and inpatient settings. Pedophilic individuals without a
CSA with 20 non-pedophilic CSA and 20 NSO. All participants came previous sexual offence were also recruited from the community or
from different residential treatment facilities. Pedophilia was again from outpatient treatment facilities. The three groups did not differ on
assessed based on self-report or PPG. The groups did not differ con- any demographic characteristics. The authors found that pedophilic
cerning age, level of education, or IQ. Compared to non-pedophilic CSA CSA showed more deficits in response inhibition because they made

6
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

more false responses in No-Go trials compared to pedophilic individuals during the study period. NSO were significantly younger than CSA,
who were not yet convicted for a sexual offence against children. while no differences occurred concerning the level of education. Age
Using participants from the same sample as Kärgel et al. (2017), was controlled for in all subsequent analyses. The CSA as a whole group
Massau et al. (2017) compared the neuropsychological performance had a significantly higher overall impairment score on both the RNTB
measured with the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated and LNNTB. No differences occurred between the CSA subgroups,
Battery (CANTAB) of 45 pedophilic CSA, 45 pedophilic individuals scores in RNTB and LNNTB submeasures were not reported.
without a previous sexual offence, 19 non-pedophilic CSA, and 49 HC Kruger and Schiffer (2011) compared nine pedophilic CSA attracted
from the general population. The CANTAB is a computerized neu- towards female children and 11 pedophilic CSA attracted towards male
ropsychological test system measuring different domains of executive children with 14 heterosexual and 14 homosexual HC from the general
functioning. For this study the following subtests were taken from the population. CSA were recruited from two high-security forensic treat-
CANTAB: response inhibition (Stop Signal Task [SST]), information ment facilities. CSA and HC were matched concerning age, sexual or-
sampling (Information Sampling Task), planning (Stocking of Cam- ientation, handedness, socioeconomic status, and education. The fol-
bridge Task), set-switching (Intra/Extradimensional Set Shift Task), and lowing neuropsychological functions were assessed: set-switching
working memory (Spatial Working Memory Task). While the compar- (WCST), visuospatial memory (Corsi-Block Tapping Test [CBT]), and
ison groups did not differ concerning age, intelligence, handedness, and alertness and concentration (d2 Test of Attention). CSA showed a worse
sexual orientation, the HC were significantly better educated than both alertness and concentration performance compared to HC. Further-
the pedophilic and non-pedophilic CSA. Both CSA groups had a sig- more, CSA made significantly more total errors in the WCST re-
nificantly worse response inhibition performance than pedophilic in- presenting a worse set-switching ability. However, on the more difficile
dividuals who had not been convicted for a sexual offence and HC. outcome measures of the WCST the two groups did not differ, limiting
Furthermore, CSA without a pedophilic disorder performed sig- the generalizability of the results.
nificantly worse in the working memory task than HC. Finally, it was Rosburg et al. (2018) used a Go/No-Go task to assess response in-
found that pedophilic CSA had superior set-switching abilities com- hibition abilities in 21 contact CSA, 19 child pornography offenders,
pared to the HC and the pedophilic individuals without a previous and 21 HC. In this sample, the contact but not the child pornography
sexual offence. offenders were diagnosed with a pedophilic disorder. While the CSA
were significantly older compared to the HC, no differences were found
3.4.3. Differences between intra-familial and extra-familial CSA concerning the level of education. Across all outcome measures there
In a Spanish study published by Becerra-García and Egan (2014), 21 were no differences between CSA and HC, suggesting that CSA and HC
intra-familial CSA, 11 extra-familial CSA, and 28 HC were compared on did not differ concerning their response inhibition abilities.
set-switching (TMT–B), attention and psychomotor speed (TMT-A), as
well as on the short-term working memory performance (Digit Span 4. Discussion
Task). All CSA were incarcerated and were convicted because of a
contact sexual offence including a child or adolescent below the age of The present systematic review is the first one summarizing relevant
16 years. The groups did not differ concerning age or level of education. research on the neuropsychological functioning of CSA and comparing
Both CSA groups had significantly worse attention and psychomotor their performance to HC, NSO, and sexual offenders against adults.
speed and lower set-switching abilities than the HC. Furthermore, the Furthermore, the performance between different CSA subgroups was
intra-familial CSA had a significantly lower working memory capacity compared. In general, the current state of research is quite hetero-
than extra-familial CSA and HC. geneous making it difficult to find scientifically sound conclusions.
Langevin et al. (1989) assessed 160 extra-familial CSA, 123 intra- However, some noteworthy insights could be obtained and will be
familial CSA, 108 sexual offenders against adults, and 36 non-violent discussed in the following sections.
NSO. The sexual offenders either faced charges of sexual assault, had Supported by a sufficient number of studies is the finding that CSA
been convicted for a sexual offence, or were involved in a post-prison show more neuropsychological impairments than HCs. In line with
treatment program for sexual offenders. Furthermore, some CSA were previous reviews on the neuropsychological functioning of sexual of-
also evaluated using PPG, in order to determine the presence of pedo- fenders in general, among the assessed executive functions impairments
philic sexual interests. Neuropsychological measures consisted of in inhibition and set-shifting were found most frequently in CSA
subtests taken from the RNTB. Pedophilic CSA had significantly lower (Adjorlolo & Egbenya, 2016; Joyal et al., 2014). CSA who are relatively
IQ scores and lower overall neuropsychological impairment scores than low in inhibition might be unable to control the impulse to sexually
the NSO group. Furthermore, pedophilic CSA attracted towards female abuse a child although the individual's long-term goal is the desistance
children were more impaired on abstraction and language related dif- from sexual offending. The deficits in set-shifting could contribute to an
ficulties compared to the homosexual and bisexual CSA as well as non- excessive fixation on a specific goal like, for example, the goal to abuse
sexual offenders. children (Cohen et al., 2010). Furthermore, a CSA with deficient set-
Valliant et al. (2000) assessed planning abilities (Porteus Mazes) in switching abilities might not be able to shift between different pre-
11 CSA with extra-familial victims, nine intra-familial CSA, 14 sexual vention strategies or cannot adopt his strategies to changing circum-
offenders against adults, 20 NSO as well as in 20 HC. At the time of stances and environments.
study all offenders were placed in one state prison. The HC had sig- There is growing recognition that successful self-regulation is based
nificantly higher IQ scores than all four offender groups, while the latter on the effective interplay between an individual's executive functions
did not differ concerning their IQ scores. Despite these significant dif- (Beaver, Wright, & Delisi, 2007; Hofmann et al., 2012; Ratchford &
ferences, IQ was not entered as a covariate in further comparisons. The Beaver, 2009; Wikström & Treiber, 2007). Previous research has shown
data analyses showed that the three sexual offender groups had sig- that impairments in executive functioning are associated with a wide
nificantly impaired planning abilities compared to NSO and HC. range of social problems including delinquent behavior (Denson,
Pedersen, Friese, Hahm, & Roberts, 2011; Ogilvie et al., 2011) and risky
3.4.4. Differences between CSA with male or female victims sexual behaviors (Ariely & Loewenstein, 2006; Carrier Emond, Gagnon,
Hucker et al. (1986) assessed 15 pedophilic CSA oriented towards Nolet, Cyr, & Rouleau, 2018) which come along with impaired self-
female children, 14 pedophilic CSA orientated towards male children, regulation. Furthermore, a decreased self-regulation capacity is among
10 pedophilic CSA oriented towards children of both genders, and 14 relevant risk factors for sexual but also for violent and general re-
non-violent NSO with the RNTB and the LNNTB. Pedophilic sexual in- cidivism in sexual offenders (Etzler, Eher, & Rettenberger, 2018;
terests were assessed using PPG and all participants were outpatients Hanson & Morton-Bourgon, 2005; Mann, Hanson, & Thornton, 2010).

7
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

Thus, it can be suggested that executive dysfunctions, on the one hand, unable to change these strategies. These persons might possess well-
and sexual delinquency, on the other hand, might on a behavioral level developed inhibition abilities but less-developed set-shifting skills and
be linked through self-regulation failures. However, the question re- thus are not able to adopt the appropriate strategies to desist from of-
mains whether these executive functioning impairments increase the fending. The approach-automatic pathway is characterized by the de-
tendency to sexually offend against children in particular or to show sire to sexually offend but offending happens in an impulsive and
socially dysfunctional behavior in general (including all kinds of de- poorly planned way. Non-pedophilic CSA with low inhibition and low
linquent behaviors) or both. planning skills could be prototypical for this pathway. Finally, the ap-
Some studies found more neuropsychological deficits in CSA com- proach-explicit pathway describes sexual offenders whose offences are
pared to NSO which underscores the former suggestion, although there not impulsive but carefully planned. These CSA are probably driven by
were also single studies not finding any differences between these two strong pedophilic sexual interests and most likely possess well-devel-
groups. Again, impairments in inhibition were found most frequently oped neuropsychological functions helping them to execute a highly
(e.g., Eastvold et al., 2011; Schiffer & Vonlaufen, 2011). Because sexual sophisticated offence. These hypotheses are not yet scientifically proven
offences against children are among the most devastating offences it is but could provide the starting point for future research about the cur-
conceivable that the internal barriers that have to be overcome are rently existing discrepancies in neuropsychological research in CSA.
higher than for other offences, supporting the suggestion of a stronger The present systematic review has shown that distinct neu-
disinhibition in CSA compared to NSO (Finkelhor, 1984). However, this ropsychological deficits can be identified in CSA, some even over and
also accounts for sexual offences against adults, explaining why most above the impairments found in general offenders. However, it has to
studies did not find any differences between sexual offenders against be concluded that the current state of research is far away from being
adults and CSA. Furthermore, sexual offences committed by non-ped- consistent. Methodological differences could be responsible for some of
ophilic CSA are usually seen as more impulsive and less planned than these inconsistencies. While some studies assessed CSA who were cur-
those of pedophilic CSA (Eher, Neuwirth, Fruehwald, & Frottier, 2003). rently incarcerated, others assessed CSA prior to their actual trial, from
Thus, in non-pedophilic CSA the deficits in inhibition could also be seen outpatient settings, or from the general population. Furthermore, while
as a result of a general behavioral disinhibition increasing all kinds of some studies assessed CSA with a pedophilic disorder, other studies
delinquent behavior. assessed CSA without a pedophilic disorder or mixed experimental
In pedophilic CSA the reported impairments in inhibition provide groups. Even the determination whether or not CSA were considered as
some evidence that the pedophilic disorder shares some similarities pedophilic differed between studies. Finally, not all studies controlled
with disorders from the compulsive-impulsive spectrum which are also for the influence of relevant demographic or clinical variables that
characterized by concurrent behavioral disinhibition (Tost et al., 2004). differed between the study populations like, for example, IQ, age, level
In this case, however, the neuropsychological deficits should be espe- of education, or previous alcohol or drugs use. Despite all these in-
cially pronounced in sexual situations as these include the specific re- consistencies it seems obvious that deficits in neuropsychological
warding content of interest. First evidence for this suggestion was functioning are a relevant aspect regarding the explanation of sexual
provided by a recent study showing that performance in a neu- offending against children.
ropsychological test assessing impulsive decision-making and using Although large parts of the variance in neuropsychological func-
stimuli showing nude children was more impaired in those individuals tioning is determined by the individual genetic make-up, it is possible
with more intense pedophilic sexual interests (Turner et al., 2018). to improve neuropsychological functioning through psychotherapeutic
In the meantime, two studies have been published comparing of- treatment (e.g., cognitive-behavioral therapy; Anastopoulos et al.,
fending and non-offending pedophilic men (Kärgel et al., 2017; Massau 2018; DeVito, Kiluk, Nich, Mouratidis, & Carroll, 2018), neurophysio-
et al., 2017). In line with the above made suggestions that sexual of- logical treatments (e.g., neurofeedback, transcranial magnetic stimu-
fending is the consequence of reduced self-regulation abilities caused by lation, or transcranial direct current stimulation; Gill, Shah-Basak, &
dysfunctional executive functioning both studies found a worse in- Hamilton, 2015; Gruzelier, 2014; Hosseini, Oritchard-Berman, Sosa,
hibition performance in pedophilic CSA than in pedophilic non-of- Ceja, & Kesler, 2016; Luber & Lisbany, 2014), or through environmental
fending individuals. enrichment (e.g., increased physical activity, education or occupation;
This line of argument is only reasonable, however, if we assume that Colcombe & Kramer, 2003; Hillman, Erickson, & Kramer, 2008;
it is a CSA's primary self-regulatory goal to desist from offending. Petrosini et al., 2009). Effective treatment programs addressing the
However, there are different pathways of sexual offending (Ward & enhancement of neuropsychological functioning will not only decrease
Hudson, 1998): While in some CSA the sexual offence is indeed the the risk of reoffending but also the risk of other maladaptive psycho-
consequence of self-regulatory failures, in others it is the result of a social outcomes such as poor physical health, poor quality of life, and
careful and systematic planning process (Kingston, Yates, & Firestone, poor school and job success (Bailey, 2007; Borella, Carretti, &
2012; Marshall, 1996; Ward, Louden, Hudson, & Marshall, 1995; Yates Pelegrina, 2010; Crescioni et al., 2011; Miller, Barnes, & Beaver, 2011).
& Kingston, 2006). For example, men who explicitly take on a job that This points towards the outstanding relevance of a precise under-
puts them in close contact with children, in order to increase the pos- standing and assessment of the neuropsychological functioning in CSA
sibility to abuse these children, show more sophisticated grooming and other sexual offender subgroups.
strategies and greater planning before executing the actual offence and
thus probably possess well-developed executive functions (Turner et al., Acknowledgments
2016; Turner, Rettenberger, Lohmann, Eher, & Briken, 2013; Turner &
Briken, 2015). None.
Ward and colleagues have proposed four different offence pathways
and it can be hypothesized that each pathway is accompanied by Funding
neuropsychological—or more specifically—certain executive func-
tioning deficits (Ward & Gannon, 2006; Ward & Hudson, 1998). In- This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
dividuals following the avoidant-passive pathway have a desire to desist agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
from sexual offending but are not able to utilize the relevant self-reg-
ulation abilities. It can be assumed that these individuals show a gen- References
eral impairment in executive functions. The avoidant-active pathway
describes individuals who actively try to desist from sexual offending; Abracen, J., O’Carroll, R., & Ladha, N. (1991). Neuropsychological dysfunction in sex
however, they use ineffective or counterproductive strategies and are offenders? Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 2, 167–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/

8
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

09585189108407646. control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology:


Adjorlolo, S., & Egbenya, D. L. (2016). Executive functioning profiles of adult and juvenile General, 133, 101–135. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101.
male sexual offenders: A systematic review. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., Defries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K.
Psychology, 27, 349–375. https://doi.org/10.1080/14789949.2016.1141431. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in
Anastopoulos, A. D., King, K. A., Besecker, L. H., O’Rourke, S. R., Bray, A. C., & Supple, A. origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 201–225. https://doi.org/10.
J. (2018). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for college students with ADHD: Temporal 1037/0096-3445.137.2.201.Individual.
stability of improvements in functioning following active treatment. Journal of Gill, J., Shah-Basak, P. P., & Hamilton, R. (2015). It’s the thought that counts: Examining
Attention Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054717749932 epub ahead of the task-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive
print. function. Brain Stimulation, 8, 253–259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.018.
Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2006). The heat of the moment: The effect of sexual arousal Gould, F., Clarke, J., Heim, C., Harvey, P. D., Majer, M., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2012). The
on sexual decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 87–98. https:// effects of child abuse and neglect on cognitive functioning in adulthood. Journal of
doi.org/10.1002/bdm.501. Psychiatric Research, 46, 500–506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.005.
Bailey, C. E. (2007). Cognitive accuracy and intelligent executive function in the brain Gruzelier, J. H. (2014). EEG-neurofeedback for optimising performance. I: A review of
and in business. Annalys of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1118, 122–141. https:// cognitive and affective outcome in healthy participants. Neuroscience and
doi.org/10.1196/annals.1412.011. Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 124–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.
Barnes, J. J. M., Dean, A. J., Nandam, L. S., O’Connell, R. G., & Bellgrove, M. A. (2011). 015.
The molecular genetics of executive function: Role of monoamine system genes. Habermeyer, B., Esposito, F., Händel, N., Lemoine, P., Kuhl, H. C., Klarhöfer, M., & Graf,
Biological Psychiatry, 69, e127–e143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12. M. (2013). Response inhibition in pedophilia: An fMRI pilot study.
040. Neuropsychobiology, 68, 228–237. https://doi.org/10.1159/000355295.
Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., & Delisi, M. (2007). Self-control as an executive function: Hanson, R. K., & Morton-Bourgon, K. E. (2005). The characteristics of persistent sexual
Reformulating Gottfredson and Hirschi’s parental socialization thesis. Criminal Justice offenders: A meta-analysis of recidivism studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
and Behavior, 34, 1345–1361. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854807302049. Psychology, 73, 1154–1163. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.6.1154.
Becerra-García, J. A., & Egan, V. (2014). Neurocognitive functioning and subtypes of Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart:
child molesters: Poorer working memory differentiates incestuous from non-in- Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 58. https://
cestuous offenders. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 21, 585–590. https://doi.org/10. doi.org/10.1038/nrn2298.
1080/13218719.2013.873974. Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-
Blanchard, R., Cantor, J. M., & Robichaud, L. K. (2006). Biological factors in the devel- regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.
opment of sexual deviance and aggression in males. In H. E. Barbaree, & W. L. 2012.01.006.
Marshall (Eds.). The juvenile sex offender (pp. 77–104). New York: The Guilford Press. Hosseini, S. M. H., Oritchard-Berman, M., Sosa, N., Ceja, A., & Kesler, S. R. (2016). Task-
Borella, E., Carretti, B., & Pelegrina, S. (2010). The specific role of inhibition in reading based neurofeedback training: A novel approach toward training executive functions.
comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, NeuroImage, 134, 153–159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.035.
43(6), 541–552. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219410371676. Hucker, S., Langevin, R., Wortzman, G., Bain, J., Handy, L., Chambers, J., & Wright, S.
Cantor, J. M., Blanchard, R., Christensen, B. K., Dickey, R., Klassen, P. E., Beckstead, A. L., (1986). Neuropsychological impairment in pedophiles. Canadian Journal of
... Kuban, M. E. (2004). Intelligence, memory, and handedness in pedophilia. Behavioural Science, 18, 440–448. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0079965.
Neuropsychology, 18, 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.3. Joyal, C. C., Beaulieu-Plante, J., & de Chantérac, A. (2014). The neuropsychology of sex
Carrier Emond, F., Gagnon, J., Nolet, K., Cyr, G., & Rouleau, J. L. (2018). What money offenders: A meta-analysis. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 26,
can’t buy: Different patterns in decision making about sex and money predict past 149–177. https://doi.org/10.1177/1079063213482842.
sexual coercion perpetration. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47, 429–441. https://doi. Joyal, C. C., Black, D. N., & Dassylva, B. (2007). The neuropsychology and neurology of
org/10.1007/s10508-017-1116-0. sexual. Sexual Abuse: Journal of Research and Treatment, 19, 155–173. https://doi.org/
Cohen, L. J., Nesci, C., Steinfeld, M., Haeri, S., & Galynker, I. (2010). Investigating the 10.1007/s11194-007-9045-4.
relationship between sexual and chemical addictions by comparing executive func- Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-
tion in pedophiles, opiate addicts and healthy controls. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
16, 405–412. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.pra.0000390759.04581.7c.Investigating. General, 130, 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.169.
Cohen, L. J., Nikiforov, K., Gans, S., Poznansky, O., McGeoch, P., Weaver, C., ... Galynker, Kärgel, C., Massau, C., Weiß, S., Walter, M., Borchardt, V., Krueger, T. H. C., ... Schiffer, B.
I. (2002). Heterosexual male perpetrators of childhood sexual abuse: A preliminary (2017). Evidence for superior neurobiological and behavioral inhibitory control
neuropsychiatric model. Psychiatric Quarterly, 73, 313–336. https://doi.org/10.1023/ abilities in non-offending as compared to offending pedophiles. Human Brain
A:1020416101092. Mapping, 38, 1092–1104. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23443.
Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older Kingston, D. A., Yates, P. M., & Firestone, P. (2012). The self-regulation model of sexual
adults. Psychological Science, 1, 125–130. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1- offending: Relationship to risk and need. Law and Human Behavior, 36, 215–224.
01430. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093960.
Coolidge, F. L., Thede, L. L., & Jang, K. L. (2004). Are personality disorders psychological Konrad, K., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). Is the ADHD brain wired differently? A review on
manifestations of executive function deficits? Bivariate heritability evidence from a structural and functional connectivity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
twin study. Behavior Genetics, 34, 75–84. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:BEGE. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 904–916. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21058.
0000009486.97375.53. Kruger, T. H. C., & Schiffer, B. (2011). Neurocognitive and personality factors in homo-
Crescioni, A. W., Ehrlinger, J., Alquist, J. L., Conlon, K. E., Baumeister, R. F., and heterosexual pedophiles and controls. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 8, 1650–1659.
Schatschneider, C., & Dutton, G. R. (2011). High trait self-control predicts positive https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01564.x.
health behaviors and success in weight loss. Journal of Health Psychology, 16, Lang, R. A. (1993). Neuropsychologcal deficits in sexual offenders: Implications for
750–759. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105310390247. treatment. Sexual and Marital Therapy, 8, 181–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Denson, T. F., Pedersen, W. C., Friese, M., Hahm, A., & Roberts, L. (2011). Understanding 02674659308408193.
impulsive aggression: Angry rumination and reduced self-control capacity are me- Langevin, R., & Curnoe, S. (2008). Assessing neuropsychological impairment among sex
chanisms underlying the provocation-aggression relationship. Personality and Social offenders and paraphilics. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 8, 150–173. https://
Psychology Bulletin, 37, 850–862. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211401420. doi.org/10.1080/15228930801963960.
DeVito, E. E., Kiluk, B. D., Nich, C., Mouratidis, M., & Carroll, K. M. (2018). Drug Stroop: Langevin, R., Wortzman, G., Dickey, R., Wright, P., & Handy, L. (1988).
Mechanisms of response to computerized cognitive behavioral therapy for cocaine Neuropsychological impairment in incest offenders. Annals of Sex Research, 1,
dependence in a randomized clinical trial. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 183, 401–415. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00878106.
162–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.10.022. Langevin, R., Wortzman, G., Wright, P., & Handy, L. (1989). Studies of brain damage and
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive function. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. dysfunction in sex offenders. Annals of Sex Research, 2, 163–179. https://doi.org/10.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. 1007/BF00851321.
Eastvold, A., Suchy, Y., & Strassberg, D. (2011). Executive function profiles of pedophilic Lee, T., Mosing, M. A., Henry, J. D., Trollor, J. N., Ames, D., Martin, N. G., & Sachdev, P.
and nonpedophilic child molesters. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, S. (2012). Genetic influences on four measures of executive functions and their
17, 295–307. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710001669. covariation with general cognitive ability: The older Australian twins study. Behavior
Eher, R., Neuwirth, W., Fruehwald, S., & Frottier, P. (2003). Sexualization and lifestyle Genetics, 42, 528–538. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-012-9526-1.
impulsivity: Clinically valid discriminators in sexual offenders. International Journal Luber, B., & Lisbany, S. H. (2014). Enhancement of human cognitive performance using
of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 47(4), 452–467. https://doi.org/10. transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). NeuroImage, 85, 961–970. https://doi.org/
1177/0306624X03253844. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.007.
Etzler, S., Eher, R., & Rettenberger, M. (2018). Dynamic risk assessment of sexual of- Mann, R. E., Hanson, R. K., & Thornton, D. (2010). Assessing risk for sexual recidivism:
fenders: Validity and dimensional structure of the Stable-2007. Assessment. Advance Some proposals on the nature of psychologically meaningful risk factors. Sexual
online publication.. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191118754705. Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 22, 191–217. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and research. New York, NY: The Free 1079063210366039.
Press. Marlowe, W. B., Mancall, E. L., & Thomas, J. J. (1975). Complete Klüver-Bucy syndrome
Flor-Henry, P. (1987). Cerebral aspects of sexual deviation. In G. Wilson (Ed.). Variant in man. Cortex, 11, 53–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(75)80020-7.
sexuality. Research and theory (pp. 49–83). New York, NY: Routledge. Marshall, W. L. (1996). Assessment, treatment, and theorizing about sex offenders:
Flor-Henry, P., Lang, R. A., Koles, Z. J., & Frenzel, R. R. (1991). Quantitative EEG studies Developments during the past twenty years and future directions. Criminal Justice and
of pedophilia. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 10, 253–258. https://doi.org/ Behaviour, 23, 162–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854896023001011.
10.1016/0167-8760(91)90036-W. Massau, C., Tenbergen, G., Kärgel, C., Weiß, S., Gerwinn, H., Pohl, A., ... Schiffer, B.
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference (2017). Executive functioning in pedophilia and child sexual offending. Journal of the

9
D. Turner and M. Rettenberger Aggression and Violent Behavior 54 (2020) 101405

International Neuropsychological Society, 23, 460–470. https://doi.org/10.1017/ 10.1007/s12160-009-9097-4.


S1355617717000315. Suchy, Y., Eastvold, A. D., Strassberg, D. S., & Franchow, E. I. (2014). Understanding
Miller, H. V., Barnes, J. C., & Beaver, K. M. (2011). Self-control and health outcomes in a processing speed weaknesses among pedophilic child Molesters: Response style vs.
nationally representative sample. American Journal of Health Behavior, 35, 15–27. neuropathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123, 273–285. https://doi.org/10.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2010.00667.x. 1037/a0035812.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. Suchy, Y., Whittaker, J. W., Strassberg, D. S., & Eastvold, A. (2009). Neurocognitive
(2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to differences between pedophilic and nonpedophilic child molesters. Journal of the
complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 248–257. https://doi.org/10.1017/
49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. S1355617709090353.
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D., & The PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred Tost, H., Vollmert, C., Brassen, S., Schmitt, A., Dressing, H., & Braus, D. F. (2004).
reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analysis: The PRISMA statement. Pedophilia: Neuropsychological evidence encouraging a brain network perspective.
PLoS Medicine, 6(7), e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed1000097. Medical Hypotheses, 63, 528–531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2004.03.004.
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 134–140. https://doi. Turner, D., & Briken, P. (2015). Child sexual abusers working with
org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7. children—Characteristics and risk factors. Sexual Offender Treatment, 10(1), 1–12.
Morgan, J. E., & Ricker, J. H. (2016). Textbook of clinical neuropsychology. New York: Retrieved from http://www.sexual-offender-treatment.org/index.php?id=138&
Taylor & Francis. type=123.
Navalta, C. P., Polcari, A., Webster, D. M., Boghossian, A., & Teicher, M. H. (2006). Effects Turner, D., Rettenberger, M., Lohmann, L., Eher, R., & Briken, P. (2013). Pedophilic
of childhood sexual abuse on neuropsychological and cognitive function in college sexual interests and psychopathy in child sexual abusers working with children. Child
women. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 18, 45–53. https:// Abuse & Neglect, 38, 326–335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.07.019.
doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.18.1.45. Turner, D., Rettenberger, M., Yoon, D., Klein, V., Eher, R., & Briken, P. (2016). Risk
Ogilvie, J. M., Stewart, A. L., Chan, R. C. K., & Shum, D. (2011). Neuropsychological Assessment in Child Sexual Abusers Working With Children. Sexual Abuse, 28(6),
measures of executive function and antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Criminology, 572–596. https://doi.org/10.1177/1079063214564390.
49, 1063–1107. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00252.x. Turner, D., Sebastian, A., & Tüscher, O. (2017). Impulsivity and Cluster B Personality
Ott, B. R. (1995). Leuprolide treatment of sexual aggression in a patient with dementia Disorders. Current Psychiatry Reports, 19, 15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-017-
and the Klüver-Bucy syndrome. Clinical Neuropharmacology, 18, 443–447. https:// 0768-8.
doi.org/10.1097/00002826-199510000-00007. Turner, D., Laier, C., Brand, M., Bockshammer, T., Welsch, R., & Rettenberger, M. (2018).
Perley-Robertson, B., Helmus, L. M., Derkzen, D., & Serin, R. C. (2016). Do sex offenders Response inhibition and impulsive decision-making in sexual offenders against chil-
against adults, sex offenders against children, and non-sex offenders differ in im- dren. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127, 471–481. https://doi.org/10.1037/
pulsivity? Sexual Offender Treatment, 11, 1–21. Retrieved from http://www.sexual- abn0000359.
offender-treatment.org/index.php?id=156&type=123. Valliant, P., Gauthier, T., Pottier, D., & Kosmyna, R. (2000). Moral reasoning, inter-
Petrosini, L., De Bartolo, P., Foti, F., Gelfo, F., Cutuli, D., Leggio, M. G., & Mandolesi, L. personal skills, and cognition of rapists, child molesters, and incest offenders.
(2009). On whether the environmental enrichment may provide cognitive and brain Psychological Reports, 86, 67–75. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2000.86.1.67.
reserves. Brain Research Reviews, 61, 221–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Ward, T., & Beech, A. (2006). An integrated theory of sexual offending. Aggression and
brainresrev.2009.07.002. Violent Behavior, 11, 44–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2005.05.002.
Plante, T. G., Manuel, G., & Bryant, C. (1996). Personality and cognitive functioning Ward, T., & Beech, A. R. (2016). The integrated theory of sexual offending - revised. A
among hospitalized sexual offending Roman Catholic priests. Pastoral Psychology, 45, multifield perspective. In D. P. Boer (Ed.). The Wiley handbook on the theories, as-
129–139. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02260019. sessment and treatment of sexual offending (pp. 123–137). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
Ratchford, M., & Beaver, K. M. (2009). Neuropsychological deficits, low self-control, and Sons.
delinquent involvement: Toward a biosocial explanation of delinquency. Criminal Ward, T., & Gannon, T. A. (2006). Rehabilitation, etiology, and self-regulation: The
Justice and Behavior, 36, 147–162. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854808325967. comprehensive good lives model of treatment for sexual offenders. Aggression and
Rosburg, T., Deuring, G., Boillat, C., Lemoine, P., Falkenstein, M., Graf, M., & Mager, R. Violent Behavior, 11, 77–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2005.06.001.
(2018). Inhibition and attentional control in pedophilic child sexual offenders – An Ward, T., & Hudson, S. M. (1998). A model of the relapse process in sexual offenders.
event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 129, 1990–1998. https://doi. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 700–725. https://doi.org/10.1177/
org/10.1016/j.clinph.2018.06.029. 088626098013006003.
Schiffer, B., & Vonlaufen, C. (2011). Executive dysfunctions in pedophilic and non- Ward, T., Louden, K., Hudson, S. M., & Marshall, W. L. (1995). A descriptive model of the
pedophilic child molesters. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 8, 1975–1984. https://doi. offense chain for child molesters. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 452–472.
org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2010.02140.x. https://doi.org/10.1177/088626095010004005.
Scott, M., Cole, J., McKay, S., Golden, C., & Liggett, K. (1984). Neuropsychological Wikström, P. O. H., & Treiber, K. (2007). The role of self-control in crime causation:
Performance of Sexual Assaulters and Pedophiles. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 29(4), Beyond Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime. European Journal of
1114–1118. https://doi.org/10.1520/JFS11778J. Criminology, 4, 237–264. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477370807074858.
Seto, M. C., & Lalumiére, M. L. (2001). A Brief Screening Scale to Identify Pedophilic Yates, P. M., & Kingston, D. A. (2006). The self-regulation model of sexual offending: The
Interests Among Child Molesters. Sex Abuse, 13, 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1023/ relationship between offence pathways and static and dynamic sexual offence risk.
A:1009510328588. Sexual Abuse: Journal of Research and Treatment, 18, 259–270. https://doi.org/10.
Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on 1007/s11194-006-9018-z.
neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Yeudall, L., & Fromm-Auch, D. (1979). Neuropsychological impairments in various psy-
Psychological Bulletin, 139, 81–132. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028727. chopathological populations. In J. Gruzelier, & P. Flor-Henry (Eds.). Hemisphere
Snyder, H. R., Miyake, A., & Hankin, B. L. (2015). Advancing understanding of executive asymmetries of function in psychopathology. Oxford, England: Elsevier/North-Holland
function impairments and psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical and Biomedical Press.
cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015. Yeudall, L., & Fromm-Auch, D. (1982). Neuropsychological impairments of persistent
00328. delinquency. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 170, 257–265.
Stahl, C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., Nuszbaum, M., Tüscher, O., Lieb, K., & Klauer, K. C. Young, M. H., Justice, J., & Erdberg, P. (2012). A comparison of rape and molest offenders
(2014). Behavioral components of impulsivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. in prison psychiatric treatment. International Journal of Offender Therapy and
General, 143, 850–886. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033981. Comparative Criminology, 56, 1103–1123. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Suchy, Y. (2009). Executive functioning: Overview, assessment, and research issues for 0306624X11417361.
non-neuropsychologists. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 106–116. https://doi.org/

10

You might also like