You are on page 1of 17

Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychology of Sport & Exercise


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport

Review

Self-determination theory based instructional interventions and


motivational regulations in organized physical activity: A systematic review
and multivariate meta-analysis
Mika Manninen a, *, Rod Dishman b, Yongju Hwang b, Eric Magrum c, Yangyang Deng b,
Sami Yli-Piipari b
a
Dublin City University, School of Sport and Human Performance, Dublin, Ireland
b
University of Georgia, Department of Kinesiology, Athens, GA, USA
c
Stockton University, School of Health Sciences, Galloway, NJ, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This study aimed to provide a quantitative synthesis of the effect of Self-determination theory (SDT) based
Motivation instructional interventions on the motivational regulations of participants in organized physical activity. We
Teaching conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis on experimental studies conducted before December 2021. The
Coaching
search using the online databases PsychINFO, PsychARTICLES, ERIC, SportDISCUS, and ProQuest Dissertations
Physical education
Self-determination theory
and Theses, and Google Scholar and other supplementary search strategies yielded 7774 articles, with 38 articles
(142 effects and 12,457 participants) meeting the inclusion criteria. The articles were analyzed using a meta-
analytic multivariate model. The study showed that SDT-based instruction had a positive heterogeneous small
effect on intrinsic motivation (g = 0.29; CI 95% [0.17, 0.41]) and identified regulation (g = 0.23; CI 95% [0.10,
0.35]) and a negative heterogeneous, small effect on external regulation (g = − 0.16; CI 95% [-0.31, − 0.00]) and
amotivation (g = − 0.14; CI 95% [-0.28, − 0.01]). SDT-based instruction did not have an effect on integrated
regulation (g = 0.08; CI 95% [-0.11, 0.28]) nor introjected regulation (g = 0.03; CI 95% [-0.7, 0.13]). Univariate
categorical moderator analyses highlighted multiple variables that impacted the size of the effects on the out­
comes, including type of intervention and control group, length of study, age of participants, and study quality.
Findings from the moderator analyses challenge the practical implications of SDT-based instructional in­
terventions in improving motivation in organized physical activity. High-quality experimental trials using careful
and precise conceptualizations of need-supportive behaviors and strategies would benefit the discipline.

Motivation, generally speaking, impacts the direction, strength, and 1. Self-determination theory
persistence of human behavior (Pintrich, 2003). As it relates to physical
activity, motivation has been shown to predict performance (Cerasoli, In psychology, the construct of motivation can be viewed from
Nicklin, & Ford, 2014; Charbonneau, Barling, & Kelloway, 2001), several perspectives (see Pintrich, 2003 for a review). This study utilized
physical activity behavior (Howard, Bureau, Guay, Chong, & Ryan, the widely used self-determination theory (SDT) framework (Deci &
2021; Owen, Smith, Lubans, Ng, & Lonsdale, 2014; Standage, Gillison, Ryan, 1985, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2017) to determine the effect of theory
Ntoumanis, & Treasure, 2012; Teixeira, Carraça, Markland, Silva, & based interventions on participants’ motivation in organized physical
Ryan, 2012), and both positive and negative affective outcomes (Cox, activity. SDT is one of the most prevalent contemporary social-cognitive
Smith, & Williams, 2008; Vasconcellos et al., 2019). Given the signifi­ motivational theories in the area of physical activity, sport, and exercise.
cance of motivation, several theory-based interventions have been SDT theorizes how social contexts may support or thwart human func­
developed in order to influence motivation. However, in physical ac­ tioning based on the social environments’ ability to satisfy participants
tivity contexts, the aggregated causal effects of some of these widely basic psychological needs. Specifically, SDT suggests that humans have
adopted interventions have not been investigated. three basic psychological needs, namely autonomy (i.e., engaging in a

* Corresponding author. Dublin City University, School of Health and Human Performance, Dublin 9, Ireland.
E-mail address: mika.manninen@dcu.ie (M. Manninen).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2022.102248
Received 11 February 2022; Received in revised form 26 May 2022; Accepted 22 June 2022
Available online 2 July 2022
1469-0292/© 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

behavior with a full sense of volition), competence (i.e., the experience participant motivation has been explored most extensively in education
of mastery and efficacy), and relatedness (i.e., the need to feel connected (e.g., Deci, Spiegel, Ryan, Koestner, & Kauffman, 1982; Skinner & Bel­
to other people in a meaningful way) (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000). Ac­ mont, 1993).
cording to the theory, these basic needs are considered essential nutri­ To date, SDT-based instructional interventions have been conducted
ents for individuals’ adjustment, integrity, and growth (Ryan, 1995; from multiple perspectives. A prominent line of this research has
Vansteenkiste, Ryan, & Soenens, 2020). Further, the theory postulates explicitly focused on autonomy-supportive teaching and instruction
that need-supportive social environments improve human’s internal highlighting, for example, the importance of teachers providing choice
motivational sources and well-being. In juxtaposition, need-depriving and meaningful rationales for students (e.g., Reeve & Jang, 2006). While
(disregard for the needs) and need-thwarting (active undermining of most of the SDT-based interventions have reported to use
the needs) social environments impact human’s external motivational autonomy-supportive techniques, in reality, these interventions have
sources with maladaptive consequences such as passivity and ill-being possibly incorporated strategies that might influence also relatedness
(Deci & Ryan, 1985; Vansteenkiste et al., 2020). Thus, the social envi­ and competence (e.g., Cheon, Reeve, & Moon, 2012). For example, a
ronments described above occupy opposite ends of a continuum that set commonly used instructional strategy in autonomy-supportive in­
the stage for human motivation. terventions is acknowledging and accepting students’ negative affect,
SDT suggests the extent of need-support, -deprivation, and -thwart­ which probably affects also students’ relatedness satisfaction. Further,
ing impact the quality of motivation (i.e., the relative quantity of some of these studies mention nurturing inner motivational resources as
different behavioral regulations) (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Ryan & Deci, an autonomy-supportive instructional strategy, which in itself is some­
2017). SDT distinguishes the quality of motivation to six types that can times explained to include a wide range of specific instructional stra­
be placed on a continuum ranging from low to high self-determination tegies such as offering students optimal challenges or asking students
(Ryan & Deci, 2017). This theoretical postulation is, apart from a few curiosity-inducing questions (Cheon & Reeve, 2015).
exceptions, supported by correlational meta-analytic findings on the Contrasting the broader autonomy-supportive interventions, some
continuum structure (Howard, Gagné, Morin, & Forest, 2017). The SDT-based interventions have focused on influencing participants’
continuum has two polar ends representing intrinsic motivation on one satisfaction for only one need, such as relatedness by such techniques as
end and amotivation on the other. The intermediate signposts on the friendly communication and support (Sparks, Lonsdale, Dimmock, &
continuum represent the other four types of motivation, all of which will Jackson, 2017). In the most precise and focused interventions, only one
be explained hereafter. specific instructional element, such as provision of positive feedback
According to SDT, the most self-determined type of motivation is (“Well done, great shot!”), is manipulated (e.g., Fransen, Boen, Van­
intrinsic and is characterized by participating in behaviors due to mo­ steenkiste, Mertens, & Vande Broek, 2017). Finally, a group of studies
tivations relating to the inherent satisfaction and interest in the behavior have examined the social agents’ support explicitly for all three needs
(Deci & Ryan, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2017). The next four motivation types focusing on the broad spectrum of techniques such as offering
on the continuum are forms of extrinsic motivation. The most constructive and informational feedback (competence-support or
self-determined type of extrinsic motivation is integrated regulation, structure) and expressing interest in the students (relatedness-support or
which represents reasons for behaviors which are an essential part of a involvement) along with autonomy-support (see Stroet, Opdenakker, &
person’s identity. Identified regulation, the third most self-determined Minnaer, 2013 for review). Adding to the variability of the SDT-based
type of motivation reflects personally important and valued causes of interventions, the comparison/control conditions also vary between
participating in activities (e.g., health). Following these autonomous SDT-based intervention studies. Broadly speaking, the study participants
types of motivation in the continuum are introjected and external reg­ in the comparison conditions follow either as usual/traditional
ulations, which are considered to be controlling in nature. Introjected instructional practices (e.g., Ulstad, Halvari, Sørebø, & Deci, 2018) or
regulation characterizes behavior that is motivated by the desire to seek instruction that systematically reduces need support (e.g, Leptokaridou,
acceptance from others while external regulation illustrates being Vlachopoulos, & Papaioannou, 2016).
motivated by behavior that is performed to comply with externally Recognizing the differences in operationalizing SDT-based in­
administered reward/punishment contingencies. Amotivation is placed terventions, recent efforts have been taken to organize all known need-
last and on the opposite side of the continuum from intrinsic motivation supportive techniques into comprehensive and systematic taxonomies
as it describes a state that is characterized by a total lack of motivation based on their most apparent target needs (Gillison, Rouse, Standage,
and intention. Sebire, & Ryan, 2018; Teixeira et al., 2019). However, there are some
Central to the SDT is the concept of internalization. Internalization limitations with these classifications as they are not strictly based on
signifies the process in which less self-determined behavioral regula­ experimental quantitative evidence about the techniques’ effect on
tions become more self-determined. According to the theory, this participants’ need satisfaction but rather on qualitative assessments of
development can be supported by the satisfaction of the basic psycho­ researchers in the field.
logical needs (Ryan & Connell, 1989). In general, the theory posits that Besides differences in utilizing different need-supportive strategies
the tendency for internalization is natural for humans and based on the and comparison conditions, SDT-based interventions differ in terms of
process of assimilating external values into personal values (Ryan, the type of intervention delivery. In one approach, teachers, instructors,
1993). However, based on the social environment and satisfaction of the or coaches are trained by a research team on SDT, its application, and
basic needs, internalization can lead to different degrees of possible ways to provide need-support without manipulating the
self-determination (i.e., different quality of motivation) (Deci & Ryan, instructional dynamics directly (e.g., Cheon et al., 2012; Langan, Toner,
2000; Ryan, 1993) and further to different behavioral, cognitive, and Blake, & Lonsdale, 2015; Ntoumanis, Thøgersen-Ntoumani, Quested, &
affective consequences (Vallerand, 1997; 2000). Hancox, 2017). Typically, this type of training consists of three work­
shop sessions with variable durations measured in hours (e.g., Cheon,
1.1. SDT-based interventions in organized physical activity Reeve, & Song, 2016; Cheon & Reeve, 2015). In another intervention
approach, the intervention interaction between the social agent and the
In organized social environments (e.g., physical education; PE), a participants is more controlled and predetermined (even scripted), and
social agent (e.g., coach, teacher, or instructor) is able to intentionally the focus is not on the professional development of the social agents (e.
influence the social environment. And as a result, the instructor may g., Fransen, Boen, Vansteenkiste, Mertens, & Vande Broek, 2018).
support (or influence) participants need satisfaction as well as the Overall, the relationship between adolescents’ perceptions of need-
internalization process through various instructional and communica­ supportive teaching and motivation has been shown to be positive by
tional techniques. The link between these instructional behaviors and narratively synthesizing the evidence of 71 education studies (Stroet,

2
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Opdenakker, & Minnaert, 2013). In PE, interventions aiming to improve conducted on need-supportive strategies in education and health, to our
teachers’ autonomy-support have also shown to positively influence knowledge, two meta-analyses have been conducted explicitly targeting
students’ autonomous motivation (Cheon et al., 2012; Fin, the physical activity domain (Lochbaum & Jean-Noel, 2015; Vascon­
Moreno-Murcia, León, Baretta, & Júnior, 2019). In exercise settings, the cellos et al., 2019). However, these quantitative syntheses have not
data indicates trainer/coach interventions are effective in improving examined causality. Lastly, there are meta-analyses conducted in phys­
exercise instructors’ need-supportive instructional behaviors. However, ical activity that have examined other relationships, such as correlations
in terms of adult participants’ motivation, ceiling and floor effects have between the motivational regulations, in the SDT-framework (e.g.,
been reported to be a factor (Ntoumanis, Thøgersen-Ntoumani, Quested, Chatzisarantis, Hagger, Biddle, Smith, & Wang, 2003).
& Hancox, 2017). Lastly, youth (adolescent) coaches’ positive feedback The meta-analysis of Lochbaum and Jean-Noel (2015; Nstudies = 39;
during a single practice has been shown to increase basketball players’ Nparticipants = 23,554), aggregated correlational data on
intrinsic motivation (Fransen et al., 2018). autonomy-supportive teaching in PE and detected large and significant
positive correlations between perceived autonomy support and intrinsic
1.2. Associations of motivational regulations in organized physical motivation (r = 0.54) and identified regulation (r = 0.50) and small
activity sized correlations between perceived autonomy support and introjected
regulation (r = 0.20), external regulation (r = − 0.15), and amotivation
The meta-analytic findings by Vasconcellos et al. (2019) show that (r = − 0.19). Further, the correlational meta-analysis in the PE context
intrinsic motivation, identified regulation, and introjected regulation by Vasconcellos et al. (2019; Nstudies = 252; Nparticipants = na) determined
have a positive relationship with students’ desirable affective (e.g., that teachers’ relative need-support correlated positively with students’
persistence), behavioral (e.g., physical activity outside and during PE intrinsic motivation (r = 0.46), identified regulation (r = 0.48) and
lessons), and cognitive outcomes in PE (e.g., concentration). In contrast, introjected regulation (r = 0.19) and negatively with students’ external
external regulation and amotivation have shown to be negatively asso­ regulation (r = − 0.07) and amotivation (r = − 0.24).
ciated with these outcomes and positively linked to maladaptive out­ In general education, systematic reviews have supported the central
comes (e.g., lack of engagement) (Aelterman et al., 2012; Vasconcellos assumptions of the SDT and have corroborated the benefits of need-
et al., 2019). Together the data supports a conceptual continuum that supportive instruction on student motivation (Lazowski & Hulleman,
highlights motivations that are more or less self-determined and are 2016; Stroet et al., 2013). Specifically, a narrative review by Stroet et al.
anchored at the ends by amotivation and intrinsic motivation. Gener­ (2013; Nstudies = 71; Nparticipants = na) on the effects of need-supportive
ally, as students score themselves closer to intrinsically motivated teaching on adolescents’ motivation concluded on a positive relation­
desirable outcomes persist. Conversely, scores closer in proximity to ship between perceptions of need-supportive teaching and students’
amotivation along the scale demonstrate less desirable outcomes. motivation measured by students’ self-reports. Lazowski and Hulleman
In sport, autonomous forms of motivation have shown to be associ­ (2016; Nstudies = 74 [11 SDT-centered interventions]; Nparticipants = 38,
ated with positive affective outcomes, such as effort (Pope & Wilson, 377 in total), in turn, showed SDT-based motivational interventions to
2012), increased enjoyment (Rottensteiner, Happonen, & Konttinen, improve various student outcomes (self-report measures, performance,
2015), and reduced drop-out intentions (Gillet, Berjot, Vallerand, & and behavioral indicators) by a medium effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.70).
Amoura, 2012). Similarly, the data demonstrates students’ intrinsic In the health care context (excluding PE and sport studies), corre­
motivation and identified regulation to be positively linked to greater lational meta-analytic findings by Ng et al. (2012; Nstudies = 188; Nparti­
training participation (Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, & Briere, 2001). On cipants = na) demonstrated a positive link between autonomy-supportive
the contrary, findings from the same study suggest that students amo­ health care climate and intrinsic motivation (r = 0.42) and identified
tivation and external regulation correlate with weak sport participation regulation (r = 0.36). In contrast, the associations between
adherence. autonomy-supportive health care climate and introjected regulation (r =
Similarly, meta-analytic findings indicate that the intention to ex­ 0.09) and external regulation (r = − 0.02) were minuscule and, in
ercise is positively associated with introjected and identified regulation amotivation, negative (r = − 0.27). Second, the meta-analytic path
as well as intrinsic motivation (Howard et al., 2021). Moreover, analysis of SDT constructs in health behavior by Hagger and Chatzi­
although some mixed correlational findings have been reported (see sarantis (2009; Neffects = 18; Nparticipants = 4036) showed a positive
McDonough & Crocker, 2007; Werman-Josefsson, Lindwall, & Ivarsson, relationship between perceived autonomy-support and self-determined
2015), intrinsic motivation, integrated regulation, and identified regu­ motivation (r = .32). In addition, in a recent meta-analysis by Gillison
lation have usually been linked to increased physical activity (Daley & et al. (2018; Nstudies = 74 [58 effects for autonomous motivation]; Npar­
Duda, 2006; Dishman, Mciver, Dowda, & Pate, 2018; Duncan, Hall, ticipants = na) examining SDT-centered health intervention studies
Wilson, & Jenny, 2010; Owen et al., 2014; Wilson, Rodgers, Fraser, & showed that SDT-interventions impacted autonomous motivation to­
Murray, 2004). In contrast, meta-analytic findings have shown ward health behavior change on average by 0.41 standard deviations
controlled motivation and amotivation to be negatively associated with (Hedge’s g). An additional finding was that out of all the examined
children’s and adolescents’ physical activity (Howard et al., 2021; Owen need-supportive strategies (e.g., interpersonal involvement, acknowl­
et al., 2014). edging participant’s perspective, and provision of structure), only the
In regards to affective outcomes in exercise settings, intrinsic moti­ provision of rationale was detected to increase autonomous motivation
vation, and identified regulation were found to be related to higher (Gillison et al., 2018). Lastly, the meta-analysis by Ntoumanis et al.
physical self-esteem among female exercisers (Wilson & Rodgers, 2002). (2020; Nstudies = 73; [k = 37, 18, and 14 for autonomous- and controlled
The same constructs have also shown to be positively linked to physical motivation, and amotivation, respectively]; Nparticipants = 30,088 in
self-worth (Thøgersen-Ntoumani & Ntoumanis, 2006), whereas amoti­ total), concluded that SDT-based interventions in the health context
vation and external regulation were found to have an inverse relation­ improve autonomous motivation (g = 0.30) measured at the end of the
ship with these constructs. Finally, intrinsic motivation, identified intervention but have no effect on participants controlled motivation
regulation, and introjected regulation have shown to have a positive nor amotivation.
reciprocal relationship with the development of exercise identity Our review of the current literature exposed several gaps in knowl­
(Ntoumanis et al., 2018). edge that warrant this meta-analytic review. Most importantly, the
meta-analyses by Lochbaum and Jean-Noel (2015) and Vasconcellos
1.3. Prior meta-analyses on need-support and motivation et al. (2019) focus on correlational data preventing conclusions of causal
relationships between need-supportive instruction and motivational
Although several systematic reviews and meta-analyses have been regulations in organized physical activity. Second, although the recent

3
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

meta-analyses based on experimental studies by Gillison et al. (2018) 2.2. Study selection
and Ntoumanis et al. (2020) included physical activity studies, they did
not include physical activity setting moderators in their analyses. More The inclusion criteria were 1) peer-reviewed research publications
importantly, the focus of these reviews was more broadly on health from academic journals, unpublished articles, conference presentation,
studies and/or in summative scores of motivational regulations as out­ and dissertations 2) intervention studies (quasi-experimental and RCTs)
comes. The use of summative scores (e.g., controlled motivation score using control and intervention groups with pre- and post-measures, 3)
consisting of introjected and external regulation) have been criticized in PE, sport, or exercise setting, 4) motivational interventions based on
the light of recent meta-analytic findings, which suggest these scores to SDT, 5) at least one motivational regulation (amotivation, external
lead to loss of critical information and reduction in predictive validity regulation, introjected regulation, identified regulation, integrated
(Howard, Gagné, Morin, & Forest, 2017). regulation, and intrinsic regulation) as outcome measures of the inter­
Contextually, organized physical activity differs from the general vention, and 6) conducted before December 2021. Exclusion criteria
education and health contexts where most of the quantitative syntheses were 1) other than teacher, coach, or exercise instructor conducting the
are done. For example, physical activity is almost by definition visible intervention (e.g., peer).
and, as such, more open for other people’s evaluations compared to The search with all the different search strategies identified 7774
classroom activities or one-to-one health counseling sessions. This study studies, of which the first, third, fourth, and the last author initially fully
also seeks to explain effect size differences between studies by meta reviewed 185 papers. Out of these papers, only three inclusion/exclu­
regression analyses targeting important study and intervention design sion decisions were initially in conflict between two authors. All the
features. For example, in addition to the various designs of SDT-based conflicts were resolved by discussion. In addition, the authors deemed
interventions, the motivational context of mandatory PE lessons dif­ 15 articles eligible if additional information could be attained from the
fers quite significantly from sport and exercise settings in which people authors. Lastly, the authors of five already included studies were con­
take part in their leisure time. Finally, focusing only on experimentally- tacted to seek further information on the unreported scores relating to
oriented research allows for the establishment of causality, whereas motivational regulations (e.g., only composite scores reported). In the
correlational and qualitative approaches are limited in this respect. To case of two studies with incomplete reporting of standard deviations or
this end, the purpose of this meta-analysis was to find the existing SDT- errors and no response from authors, we estimated the standard de­
based instructional experimental studies and to focus on the effect of viations from the most similar other study (Cheon et al., 2012). In total,
SDT-based instruction on different qualities of participant motivation in 38 articles were carried to the quantitative analysis. The complete
organized physical activity as theorized by SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985). flowchart representing the study selection process is provided in
Specifically, this study aimed to address two broad research questions: Figure 1.
What is the effect of SDT-based instructional interventions on par­
2.3. Effect size calculation
ticipants’ motivational regulations in organized physical activity?
Are there important study design features that moderate the effect of For all the outcomes we focused primarily on the standardized mean
the SDT-based instructional interventions on the motivational change differences (Hedge’s g) between the intervention and control
regulations? conditions. First, we computed the standardized mean change for the
treatment and control conditions using the pre-test standard deviations
and a bias correction factor (Becker, 1988). Next, we computed the
2. Method
difference in the standardized mean changes by subtracting the stan­
dardized mean change of the intervention conditions from the stan­
2.1. Literature search
dardized mean change of the control conditions (Morris, 2008). The
corresponding sampling variances were computed by summing up the
The meta-analysis was not pre-registed but was conducted according
sampling variances of the two conditions. Besides Hedge’s g effect sizes,
to the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and
we computed raw mean change and raw mean change difference effect
Meta-analyses) statement guidelines (Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, & Alt­
sizes (%) using the Percent of Maximum Possible (POMP) scores for an
man, 2009). The articles used in this meta-analysis were conducted
alternative interpretation of the results (Cohen, Cohen, Aiken, & West,
before December 2021. The research articles were searched from Psy­
1999).
chINFO, PsychARTICLES, ERIC, SportDISCUS, and ProQuest Disserta­
The studies that had multiple measurement points, the time point
tions and Theses databases as well as Google Scholar. The authors of the
closest to 10 weeks (the average time for the studies with only two
study gathered the articles from these databases using different combi­
measurement points), was used as the post-measurement point. In the
nations of the keywords: autonomy, competence, relatedness, need, sup­
studies reporting only a total number of participants, an even split of
port*, structure, involvement, self-determination, motivation, teach*,
participants was assumed to the different conditions (k = 2). An increase
coach*, instruct*, strateg*, physical, education, sport, exercise, intervention,
in the behavioral regulations resulted in a positive effect size. Cohen’s
training, workshop, treatment, experiment, comparison. The full search
(1988) widely used criteria of small (0.2), medium (0.5), and large (0.8)
strings are listed in the supplemental file.
were used to assess the size of the effects.
All the found articles were scanned by the title, and the articles that
The reliability of all the derived datapoints was assessed with
seemed to fit the inclusion criteria were investigated by reading the
Cohen’s kappa and intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC). The initial
abstract. All duplicate publications were removed. Besides the database
agreement on the continuous data which was used to compute the effect
search, the authors reviewed the reference lists of the retrieved articles
sizes (means, standard deviations, and participants numbers) was very
and the tables of the found review articles. In the final phase of the
high (two-way mixed effect model and “single rater” unit: 0.99 [0.99,
search, unpublished research was searched by contacting individually
0.99], p < .001, rough percentage agreement = 97.3%). Before the
the first authors of the articles that were included in the qualitative
analyses, a rater agreement was achieved by locating and resolving
analysis. The authors were contacted a maximum of three times with a
dissimilarities.
one-week time interval between the contact efforts. If the email address
of the author was not working or it was not publicly available, the pri­
2.4. Selection and coding of the moderators
vate message function of the Research Gate website was used as the
method of contact.
An a priori moderator selection based on reason and relevant litera­
ture were conducted to explain the expected variation in the effect sizes

4
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Figure 1. Flowchart of the study selection.

(Rosenthal & DiMatteo, 2001). A total number of eight moderators were 2.5. Statistical analysis
categorized as setting (PE or exercise/sport), age (under 18 or 18 and
over), intervention scope (only one need supported or two or three needs A multivariate maximum likelihood random effects model (Berkey,
supported), intervention type (intervention was delivered by trained Hoaglin, Antczak-Bouckoms, Mosteller, & Colditz, 1998) was fitted to
“real life” professionals or intervention was delivered by researchers the data using R (version 4.0.0) (R Core Team, 2018) and the Metafor
through a scripted plan), control condition (as usual or manipulated), package (Viechtbauer, 2010). The adopted meta-analytic multivariate
intervention length (i.e., participants exposure to the intervention: approach handles the non-independence of the effect sizes by including
maximum two weeks, six to nine weeks, or more than 10 weeks), and a variance-covariance matrix in the model. The between outcome cor­
quality measured by the MERSQI instrument and by an adapted quality relations required for the variance-covariance matrix were derived from
instrument developed for the study (see Supplemental file for more the original studies or from the meta-analysis by Howard, Gagné, Morin,
detailed descriptions). and Forest (2017). In the model, random effects were added for each
The agreement of moderator and quality coding between the raters outcome within each study allowing the outcomes to be correlated and
was initially high (Cohen’s kappa, [2, 450] = 0.83, z = 22.7, p < .001, to have different variances. Parameters of tau2 and I2 were used to
95% CI = [0.79, 0.88]; rough percentage agreement = 90%). The large examine the between-study heterogeneity of the effects (Higgins,
majority of the initial differences in coding were due to dissimilarities in Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003; Jackson, White, & Riley, 2012).
determining the scope and the type of intervention. Particularly, the Further, as the Q statistic for heterogeneity cannot be applied to
authors had initial disparities in how many basic needs the intervention multivariate models, a likelihood ratio test examining the effect of tau2
content covered and whether the intervention was indirect via profes­ on all the outcomes was used as an indicator of significant between study
sional training or executed according to a clearly defined plan (e.g., a heterogeneity. The between study heterogeneity of the effect sizes was
script). The differences in coding were reconciled via a discussion before indicated if likelihood ratio test (χ 2) reached a significance level of p <
the analyses. .05, and the sampling error contributed to the observed variance less
than 75% (Hedges & Olkins, 1985; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). The I2

5
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

values, which represent the heterogeneity of the studies, were catego­ For integrated regulation, the outcomes of five studies ranged from
rized as low (25%), moderate (50%), and high (75%). The precision of − 0.27 to 0.16 with 60% of the studies having positive estimates. The
effect sizes were indicated by 95% CIs. standardized mean change difference based on the multivariate model
The moderators were used in a linear regression analysis as univar­ was 0.08 (CI 95% [-0.11, 0.28], z = 0.85, p = .276), and hence, statis­
iate independent variables to explain the possible heterogeneous effects tically not significant. The mean estimate was strongly heterogeneous
of the outcomes. Interactions of the moderators were not tested because (χ 2(1) = 5.46, p = .0195, tau2 = 0.122, I2 = 76.45%).
of inadequate numbers of effects for each outcome (Deeks, Higgins, & The observed mean change differences from 28 studies for identified
Altman, 2011). regulation ranged between − 0.13 and 2.98. Out of the 28 studies, in
An extension of the Egger’s test (Egger, Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 71% the estimates were positive. On average, the standardized mean
1997) using the standard error of the observed outcomes as a predictor change difference with the multivariate model was 0.23 (CI 95% [0.10,
in a multivariate model and a visual examination of the contour 0.35]) differing from zero (z = 3.52, p < .001). The Likelihood ratio test
enhanced funnel plots were used to detect publication bias. The exis­ indicated the true standardized mean change differences to be hetero­
tence of outlier and influential studies and effects were determined using geneous (χ 2(1) = 14.27, p < .001, tau2 = 0.111, I2 = 91.9%).
Cook’s distances and by analyzing the distribution of studentized re­ The slight majority of standardized mean change differences for
siduals (Viechtbauer & Cheung, 2010). The data set and R-code used for introjected regulation (23 studies) were negative (57%) with the mean
the analyses can be found on the project OSF website: https://osf. estimates ranging from − 1.00 to 1.48. The mean estimate was 0.03 (CI
io/k6cqr/. 95% [-0.07, 0.13]), and it was not significantly different from zero (z =
0.63, p = .526). The true standardized mean change differences seemed
3. Results to be heterogeneous (χ 2(1) = 17.67, p < .001, tau2 = 0.043, I2 =
81.41%).
In total, 142 effects from 38 studies were derived for six outcomes: For external regulation, 56% of the estimates coming from 25 studies
intrinsic motivation (k = 35), integrated regulation (k = 5), identified were negative ranging from − 2.40 to 0.33. Based on the multivariate
regulation (k = 28), introjected regulation (k = 23), external regulation model, the mean estimate was − 0.16 (CI 95% [-0.31, − 0.00]) and,
(k = 25), and amotivation (k = 26). The total amount of participants was hence, significantly different from zero (z = -2.02, p = .044). The true
12,457 (55% females, with two studies not reporting gender distribu­ standardized mean change differences appeared to be heterogeneous (χ 2
tion). In the 33 studies with the detailed ages of the participants, the (1) = 51.31, p < .001, tau2 = 0.144, I2 = 93.33%).
weighted mean age was 15.4 years. The other five studies provided in­ The estimate for the standardized mean change difference of amo­
formation only on the grade level of the participants (e.g., college). The tivation was − 0.14 (CI 95% [-0.28, − 0.01]). The individual outcomes of
setting for 27 studies was PE, for five studies sports, and for six studies, a 26 studies ranged between − 3.02 and 0.17 with 73% of the effects were
fitness class. All studies except for two applied a cluster randomization negative. The mean estimate was significantly different from zero (z =
design with existing groups. Twenty-six of the studies implemented an -2.10, p = .036), but the Likelihood ratio test suggested the true out­
as usual (or traditional) control condition, whereas 12 manipulated the comes to be heterogeneous (χ 2(1) = 27.06, p < .001, tau2 = 0.126, I2 =
control condition in some way (e.g., systematically limiting choice or 92.93%).
providing minimum need support). The implementation of the in­ The individual effect sizes and their CIs and the standardized mean
terventions was conducted by teachers (k = 24), researchers (k = 2), change differences according to a meta-analytic multivariate model as
instructors (k = 6), coaches (k = 4), and teaching assistants (k = 1). The well as independendent two-level random effects models are displayed
interventions used various need-supportive behaviors and strategies, in Figures 2-4. Further, standardized mean changes from pre-to post-test
which were either taught and made known to the intervention con­ for both conditions, intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) between the
ductors via workshops (k = 23), or carried out according to a specific six outcomes, and the heterogeneity statistics for all the six outcomes are
and systematic plan (k = 15). Twenty of the studies implementing presented in Table 2. Lastly, for a different interpretation of the results,
workshops reported the amount of the workshops held and the amount Percent of maximum possible (POMP) scores of the six outcomes and
of total workshop hours. The amount of workshops ranged from 1 to 5 mean the estimates computed with those scores are displayed in Table 3.
with an average of 2.9 while the amount of total hours ranged from 3 to
40 with an average of 10.7 h. Seven of the studies reported supporting 3.2. Moderator analyses
one need (competence, autonomy, or relatedness), 15 two needs (au­
tonomy and competence), and 16 all the three psychological needs. The Based on the significant likelihood ratio tests and high values of I2 for
most frequently used behavioral regulation scales were Perceived Locus all the outcomes, there was a strong reason to believe that variability in
of Causality in PE scale (Goudas, Biddle, & Fox, 1994; PLOC in PE) (k = need-supportive interventions was not only due to a sampling error of
16), Behavioral Regulations in Exercise Questionnaire − 2 (Markland & the independent studies. Thus, the between-study variances were pur­
Tobin, 2004; BREQ-2) (k = 4), and Self-Regulation Questionnaire (Ryan sued to identify using univariate regression analyses in the meta-analytic
& Connell, 1989; SRQ) (k = 4). Six of the interventions lasted between multivariate model.
one session and two weeks, 15 between six and nine weeks, and 17 more Out of the eight moderators, five moderated the mean estimates at
than 10 weeks. Twenty of the studies were conducted in Europe, nine in least on one motivational outcome. First, the interventions that were
Asia, four in Australia, three in the U.S., and one in Brazil and Mexico. conducted by researchers according to a specific plan compared to in­
The full details of the independent studies are listed in Table 1 and in terventions in which professionals (e.g., teachers) were trained to be
supplemental Table 4. need supportive in workshops had stronger effects on identified regu­
lation (z = 2.40, p = .017), external regulation (z = − 2.39, p = .017),
3.1. Effects of need-supportive instruction and amotivation (z = − 2.65, p = .008). Second, the studies that
manipulated the control group (e.g., limiting participant choice)
The observed outcomes of 35 studies for intrinsic motivation ranged compared to studies with an as usual control group had larger effects on
from − 0.18 to 2.61, with 83% of the effects being positive. Based on the intrinsic motivation (z = 2.11, p = .036), identified regulation (z = 2.94,
multivariate model, the standardized mean change difference between p = .003), external regulation (z = − 3.04, p = .002), and amotivation (z
the intervention and control conditions for intrinsic motivation was 0.29 = − 3.48, p = .001). Third, the studies that lasted between six and nine
(CI 95% [0.17, 0.41], z = 4.64, p < .001). The standardized mean change weeks had a stronger effect on intrinsic motivation compared to studies
difference differed significantly from zero but was strongly heteroge­ lasting two weeks or less (z = 2.12, p = .034). In contrast, studies lasting
neous (χ 2(1) = 54.97, p < .001, tau2 = 0.103, I2 = 91%). two weeks or less had a stronger effect on external regulation compared

6
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Table 1
Study features.
Authors n (sex) Age Country Setting Outcomes Scale Design Conductor Support Intervention Control Length

Abula, 2018 258 19.9 China PE (Col.) No IN PLOC- TL CR Teachers ASIP T-3(NA) As usual 3 m.
(53% F) PE (2)
Amado, 2017 921 13.17 Mexico PE No IN QMDCE CL CR Teachers NS (3) T - 3(15h) As usual 4m.
(46%F)
Barkoukis, 2021 256 13.5 Greece PE No IN, PLOC- SL CT Teachers ASIP T-3(4.5h) As usual 10 w.
(53%F) AM PE (2)
Behzadnia, 2017 28 21 Iran PE (Col.) No IN, SRQ-L R Teacher AS (2) P Controlling 14 w.
(100% AM
M)
Chang, 2016 126 13.7 Taiwan PE No IN SDM SL CR Teachers AS (1) P No choices 6 w.
(48% F)
Cheon, 2010 28 College S-Korea Fitness No IN SRQ GL CR Instructor AS (3) P Controlling 6 w.
(100% class
M)
Cheon, 2012 1430 MS & S-Korea PE AM PLOC SL CR Teachers ASIP T-3(5h) As usual 2 m.
(47% F) HS PE (2)
Cheon, 2015 598 MS & S-Korea PE AM AI SL CR Teachers ASIP T-3(4h) As usual 2 m.
(54% F) HS (2)
Cheon, 2016 1017 MS & S-Korea PE AM AI + SL CR Teachers ASIP T-3(7h) As usual 2 m.
(57% F) HS PRS (2)
Edmunds, 2008 56 21.3 UK Fitness All BREQ-2 CL CR Instructor AS (3) P Min 9 w.
(100% class support
F)
Fin, 2019 61 12.9 Brazil PE No IN PLOC- CL CR Teachers AS (3) T-NA(40h) As usual 8 m.
(54% F) PE
Franco, 2017 53 13.4 Spain PE IM PLOC- CL CR Teachers NS (3) T-5(10h) As usual 3 m.
(43% F) PE
Fransen, 2018 60 14.9 Belgium Club sport IM PLOC- GL CR Researcher CS (1) P No 1 ses.
(100% PE feedback
M)
Gillison, 2013 190 13.7 UK PE No IN SIMS CL CR Researcher AS (2) P Controlling 1 ses.
(M&F)
González-Cutre, 2018 88 14.7 Spain Extracur. All PLOC- SL CR Teachers NS (3) P As usual 2 m.
(59% F) PE PE
Ha, 2020 667 14.4 HK PE IM, ID PLOC CL CR Teachers NS (3) T − 2(7h) As usual 4 w.*
(31%M)
Langan, 2015 87 15.2 Ireland Club sport All BRSQ GL CR Coaches NS (3) T- As usual 12 w.
(100% (biweekly)
M)
Leptokaridou, 2016 54 11, 12 Greece PE No IN PLOC- CLCR Teacher AS (2) P Lack of AS 9 w.
(46% F) y PE
Leyton, 2017 95 59.1 Spain Fitness No IN BREQ-2 CL CR NA NS (3) P Traditional 12 w.
(100% class
F)
Lonsdale, 2013 132 13.6 Australia PE No IN, IJ SIMS CL CR Teachers AS (1) P As usual 1 ses.
(51%
M)
Lonsdale, 2019 1255 12.9 Australia PE No IN PLOC SL CR Teachers NS (3) T 3(9h)* As usual 7.5 m.
(55%M)
Manninen, Deng, 57 20.6 USA PE (Col.) All BREQ-3 GL CR Teacher NS (3) P No support 2 w.
Hwang, Waller, & (75% F)
Yli-Piipari, 2020
Mertens, 2018 84 16 Belgium Club sport IM PLOC- GL CR Coach CS (1) P No 1 ses.
(100% PE feedback
M)
Moreno-Murcia & 145 10.4 Spain PE IM PLOC- CL CR Teachers AS (2) T-NA(12h) As usual 4 m.
Sánchez-Latorre, (51% F) PE
2016
Moustaka, 2012 35 44.3 Greece Fitness No IN BREQ-2 CL CR Instructor AS (2) P Lack of AS 8 w.
(100% class
F)
Ntoumanis, 2017 317 39.9 Australia Fitness No IN BREQ-2 GL CR Instructors NS (3) T − 3(9h) As usual 11w.
(79% F) class
Prusak, 2004 1110 MS USA PE No IJ SIMS TL CR Teachers AS (1) P No choices 2 w.
(100%
F)
Pulido, 2017 109 13.7 Spain Club sport No IN SMQ GL CR Coaches NS (3) T-2(12h) Traditional 6 w.
(100%
M)
Sánchez-Oliva et al., 836 12.8 Spain PE IM, ID, CMEF TL CR Teachers NS (3) T-3(15h) As usual 3 m.
2017 (49% F) AM
Schneider, 2019 502 14.5 Finland PE No IN PLOC TL CR Teachers ASIP T-6(12h) As usual 1 m.
(56%M) (2)
Sebire, 2016 10 UK BREQ SL CR TAs AS (3) T-5(25h) Traditional 20 w.
(continued on next page)

7
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Table 1 (continued )
Authors n (sex) Age Country Setting Outcomes Scale Design Conductor Support Intervention Control Length

490 Extracur. No IN,


(45%M) PE AM
Sfandyari, 2020 30 15.1 Iran PE/ IM SMS R Instructor AS (1) P No choice 8 w.
(100% Fitness
M)
Sparks, 2017 383 13.2 Australia PE No IN PLOC- SL CR Teachers RS (1) T-1(3h) As usual 3 m.
(72% F) PE
Sullivan (2005) 107 11, 12 USA Youth IM, AM SMS GL CR Coaches NS (3) T-1(4.5h) As usual 4 m*
(51%M) y sport
Tilga, 2019 190 13.7 Estonia PE IM, ID PLOC TL CR Teachers ASIPw T-4(4h) As usual 9 w.
(45%M) (2)
Tilga, 2020 329 13.2 Estonia PE No PLOC TL CR Teachers AS (2) T-1(8h) As usual 5 w.
(53%M)
Ulstad, 2018 425 14 Norway PE No IN, SRQ SL CR Teachers ASIP T-3(8h) As usual 12 m.
(M&F) AM (2)
Zazo, 2018 49 45.1 Spain Fitness All BREQ-3 GL CR Instructor ASIP T-1(NA) As usual 9 m.
(100% class (2)
F)

Note. AI = Amotivation Inventory-PE; BREQ = Behavioural Regulations in Exercise Questionnaire; BRSQ = Behavioral Regulations in Sports Questionnaire; CMEF =
Questionnaire of Motivation in PE; PLOC = Perceived Locus of Causality (in PE); PRS = Problematic Relationship Scale; QMDCE = Questionnaire on Motivation in
Dance and Corporal Expression; SDM = Self-determined Motivation in PE; SIMS = Situational Motivation Scale; SMS = Sport Motivation Scale Questionnaire; SMQ =
Sport Motivation Questionnaire; SRQ = Self-Regulation Questionnaire; CL = Classroom level; CR = Cluster Randomization; GL = Group level; R = Randomized; SL =
School level; TL = Teacher level; AS = Autonomy Support; ASIP = Autonomy Supportive Intervention Program (w = web); CS = Competence Support; NS = Need
Support; RS = Relatedness Support; P = Planned; T = Trained.

to studies lasting more than ten weeks (z = 2.11, p = .035). Fourth, evidence on autonomy-supportive teaching and motivational regula­
studies with participants older than 18-years had stronger effects tions (Lochbaum & Jean-Noel, 2015; Vasconcellos et al., 2019).
intrinsic motivation compared to studies with underage participants (z Considering that only five included studies assessed integrated regula­
= 2.41, p = .016). Lastly, the study quality scores moderated the effects. tion, any definitive conclusions regarding integrated regulation are
The lowest quality studies measured by the MERSQI-instrument had beyond the findings of this study. However, due to the inability of
stronger effects on intrinsic motivation compared to the intermediate (z psychometric scales measuring motivation to distinguish integrated
= − 2.47, p = .014) and highest quality studies (z = − 2.34, p = .019) and regulation from intrinsic motivation and identified regulation (Howard,
on amotivation compared to the intermediate (z = 2.25, p = .025) and Gagné, Morin, & Forest, 2017), the null effect might be due to a small
highest quality studies (z = 2.21, p = .027). The applied quality scores number of analyzed effects. Alternatively, it might be that longer and
moderated the effects only on introjected regulation with lowest quality more foundational interventions (i.e., interventions beyond instruc­
studies having stronger effects compared to intermediate (z = − 2.65, p tional practices) are needed to change individuals’ integrated regulation
= .008) and highest quality studies (z = − 2.73, p = .006). The full de­ as it ties so strongly with people’s identity.
tails of the moderator effects are displayed in Table 4. It is mostly unknown why SDT-based interventions do not seem to
impact introjected regulation in organized physical activity. It may be
4. Discussion that participants perceive organized physical activity inherently con­
trolling, thus undermining the effect of need-supportive instruction. On
SDT stipulates that socio-environmental factors, such as teachers’ or the other hand, two of the reviewed interventions resulted in substantial
coaches’ instructional strategies and behaviors, affect human motiva­ positive effects whereas two studies derived large negative effects on
tion through the satisfaction of three basic needs of competence, au­ introjected regulation corroborating the previous mixed findings
tonomy, and relatedness (Deci & Ryan, 2000). As instructional strategies (Teixeira et al., 2012). These variable effects may in part be explained by
and behaviors that nurture need-satisfaction are a) not clearly estab­ the fact that introjected regulation reflects a mix of reasons for behavior
lished (see Gillison et al., 2018), b) vary from study to study, and c) may as it is connected with both the pursuit of desired outcomes (e.g.,
be culturally determined (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Taylor & Lonsdale, self-esteem) as well as negative ones (e.g., pride) (Howard, Gagné,
2010), the findings of this study do not result in absolute certainty on the Morin, & Forest, 2017). Lastly, this study did not corroborate the find­
applicability of SDT-based intervention studies in organized physical ings that have shown introjected regulation to increase across time
activity. The results, however, do advance our knowledge on how cur­ (Rodgers, Hall, Duncan, Pearson, & Milne, 2010).
rent SDT-based interventions influence participants’ motivational reg­ The practical significance of the findings of this study was under­
ulations towards organized physical activity in different contexts. mined by the considerable heterogeneity of the effect sizes across all
The results of this study suggest that SDT-based instructional in­ outcomes, as demonstrated by the previous meta-analyses (Gillison
terventions have a small heterogeneous effect on improving partici­ et al., 2018; Su & Reeve, 2011). However, the initial large magnitude of
pants’ intrinsic motivation and identified regulation (autonomous forms the heterogeneity was unexpected. A closer examination of the
of motivation). The results also suggest that SDT-based instructional need-supportive instruction strategies indicated that SDT-based in­
interventions slightly reduce participants’ external regulation and struction was conceptualized in numerous different ways, which may
amotivation. The mean estimates are somewhat encouraging and in line partly explain the demonstrated heterogeneity. For example, five of the
with the theorization of the SDT and the previous studies [e.g., the meta- analyzed studies (Abula et al., 2018; Cheon et al., 2012; Cheon & Reeve,
analysis by Vasconcellos et al. (2019)] and the systematic review by 2015; Cheon et al., 2016; Ulstad et al., 2018) utilized an
Stroet el al. (2013). In addition, this meta-analysis showed SDT-based autonomy-supportive intervention program that conceptualized
interventions to have no statistically significant effect on integrated need-supportive (or autonomy-supportive) strategies into six specific
and introjected regulations. While not fully supporting the central categories: taking the students’ perspective, vitalizing students’ inner
theorization of the SDT, the mean estimates are mostly in line with the motivational resources, provision of explanatory rationales, acknowl­
findings of the previous meta-analysis summarizing the correlational edgement and acceptance of negative affect, use of informational and

8
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Figure 2. Forest plots for Intrinsic Motivation and Integrated Regulation.

non-pressuring language and patience (Reeve, 2016). On the other hand, different from one another for at least one outcome. Specifically, dif­
the study by Sánchez-Oliva et al. (2017) focused their need-supportive ferences for a) type of intervention (trained or planned) on identified
intervention program specifically to target all the basic needs of SDT regulation, external regulation, and amotivation, b) the type of the
and thus, at least according to procedure descriptions, provided control group (as usual or manipulated) on intrinsic motivation, iden­
considerable support to the need for relatedness by behaviors, such as tified regulation, external regulation, and amotivation, c) the length of
being helpful and friendly, which are not part of the the study (maximum two weeks, from six to nine weeks, and more than
autonomy-supportive intervention program protocol. Moreover, adding ten weeks) on intrinsic motivation and external regulation, d) the age of
to the variation of the intervention procedures, the study by Edmunds, the participants (under or over 18-year old) on intrinsic motivation, and
Ntoumanis, and Duda (2008) conceptualized need-supportive strategies e) the total quality of the studies (low, medium or high) for intrinsic
based on autonomy-support, provision of structure, and interpersonal motivation, introjected regulation, and amotivation were detected. On
involvement, which differs slightly from the conceptualization of one hand, these moderating effects partly corroborate the central pos­
Sánchez-Oliva and colleagues (2017). tulations of the SDT (Deci & Ryan, 2000) and on the other challenge the
Out of the eight moderators, some categories within the type of practical implications of SDT-based interventions in PA settings. Spe­
intervention, the type of the control group, the length of the interven­ cifically, it seems that the effect of need-supportive instruction on
tion, the age of the participants, and the study quality were statistically identified regulation, external regulation, and amotivation arises only

9
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Figure 3. Forest plots for Identified and Introjected Regulation.

when compared to the conditions that are manipulated (e.g., a lack of intrinsic motivation and identified regulation. It might be that one ses­
autonomy-support) but not when compared to the as-usual instructional sion or even two weeks of instruction with a need-supportive style is not
conditions. The logical conclusion of this finding is that many teachers, enough to result in changes in participant motivation. Further,
instructors, and coaches know how to motivate in their work quite SDT-based interventions seem to lose some of their impact if the inter­
effectively without explicit training. Moreover, the results suggest that vention continues more than ten weeks. This reduction may be due to
the effect of SDT-instruction workshops on these three outcomes is the ceiling effect, that is when motivational values cannot continue
non-existent. Altogether, the practical significance of SDT-based in­ increasing due to already achieved maximum values. Alternatively, over
struction decreases in the light of these findings, at least in terms of time, people may get accustomed to the new instructional style, which
participants’ motivation. However, the finding supports the previous may weaken the effect of the motivational intervention. Moreover, this
correlational evidence suggesting that need-depriving and -thwarting study corroborates the findings of Gillison et al. (2018), showing that the
environments should be avoided in PA settings (Bartholomew, Ntou­ benefits of SDT-based instruction on intrinsic motivation are greater for
manis, Ryan, Bosch, & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, 2011; Haerens, Aelterman, participants who are older than 18 years compared to under-aged par­
Vansteenkiste, Soenens, & Van Petegem, 2015). Regarding the duration ticipants. It might be that the specific need-supportive instruction stra­
of the instructional period, it appears that the most optimal length of the tegies are, at least to some extent, age-specific and that underage
need-supportive instruction exposure is six to 9 weeks as it relates to participants’ need satisfaction mechanisms are different from those of

10
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Figure 4. Forest plots for External Regulation and Amotivation.

adults. Also, the adult participants were either college students or influential studies. The following sensitivity analysis excluding these
middle-aged adults taking part in voluntary fitness classes. Compared to studies from the analyses, reduced the heterogeneity between the
mandatory PE classes (the most prevalent context for under 18-year-­ studies quite considerably. Thus, much of the initial heterogeneity in the
olds), the adults, thus, operated in a very different need-supportive effects was due to these studies with outlying effects sizes, especially for
environment regardless the instructional characteristics. Lastly, the identified regulation and amotivation. As all these studies were small,
lowest quality studies yielded bigger effects on intrinsic motivation, planned, had a long duration, and manipulated the control group
introjected regulation, and amotivation compared to the medium and (except for one study), the univariate moderator analyses were not able
high quality studies. As the lowest quality of studies typically had small to detect their effect but support the overall conclusions of the moder­
sample sizes, described study protocols vaguely, and did not conduct ator analyses.
fidelity checks and randomization procedures, the result might be linked Although the large heterogeneity and moderator analyses challenge
with the general tendency of less-rigorous studies to yield bigger effects the practical implications of some of the initial results, most of the
(Shrout & Rodgers, 2018). detected standardized mean change differences in the sensitivity ana­
The visual interpretation of the funnel plots and the testing of Cook’s lyses remained significant for intrinsic motivation, identified regulation,
distances and studentized residuals indicated that five relatively small and amotivation. All in all, the sensitivity analyses suggest that the effect
studies with many very large effects were potential outliers and/or of SDT-based interventions on intrinsic motivation, identified

11
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Table 2
True effect ICC:s between the motivational regulations, standardized mean change estimates, and heterogeneity values.
Outcomes 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Intrinsic -
2. Integrated 0.928 -
3. Identified 0.791 0.672 -
4. Introjected 0.699 0.911 0.424 -
5. External − 0.643 − 0.626 − 0.766 − 0.423 -
6. Amotivation − 0.732 − 0.758 − 0.923 − 0.653 0.765 -
Control SMC (Hedge’s g) − 0.129 [-0.261, − 0.006 [-0.131, − 0.158 [-0.257, − 0.026 [-0.106, 0.029 [-0.092, 0.15] 0.108 [-0.003, 0.219]
0.004] 0.112] − 0.058]** 0.055]
Intervention SMC 0.186 [0.058, 0.315] 0.099 [-0.041, 0.074 [-0.042, 0.189] 0.019 [-0.061, − 0.104 [-0.202, − 0.017 [-0.112,
(Hedge’s g) ** 0.239] 0.098] − 0.006]* 0.077]
SMCD (Hedge’s g) 0.287 [0.166, 0.408] 0.083 [-0.11, 0.276] 0.226 [0.10, 0.352]*** 0.031 [-0.065, − 0.155 [-0.306, − 0.144 [-0.278,
*** 0.127] − 0.004]* − 0.01]*
Tau2 (SMCD) 0.103 0.122 0.111 0.043 0.144 0.126
I2 (SMCD) 91% 76.45% 91.9% 81.41% 93.33% 92.93%

Note. SMC = Standardized mean change (pre-post), SMCD = Standardized mean change difference. *p < .05., **p < .01., ***p < .001.

regulation, and amotivation is only small. On the other hand, the outlier 4.2. Future studies
studies suggested that the changes in participant motivation can be quite
dramatic if a) SDT-based interventions are contrasted with instructional Relating to the heterogeneity of the studies, there is a need for well-
styles that lack in or thwart need-support (e.g., controlling instruction), designed studies using more detailed descriptions of the experimental
b) the instructor-participant relationship is between six and fourteen procedures, manipulation checks, and the study characteristics. Clearer
weeks long, and c) the intervention is designed according to a specific conceptualizations of the study characteristics would allow a more
plan. It is also noteworthy that in all excluded studies, the same teacher careful and reliable examination of the moderating effects and would be
or instructor interacted as the only person teaching both the control and valuable in determining which contexts and in which way conducted
intervention group. SDT-based instruction is the most effective.
In educational settings, true randomization is usually not attainable
4.1. Limitations as demonstrated by this review; cluster randomization is regarded to be
the most practical design to apply. However, in exercise settings, designs
The results of this meta-analysis are not free from limitations. The that are more ambitious could be deployed more easily. Moreover,
relatively small number of effects (at most 35 for intrinsic motivation) different age groups, school-levels, and physical activity settings should
and the large initial heterogeneity of the effects limit the interpretation be examined more thoroughly in the future. It would also be interesting
and generalizability of the findings, as well as the statistical power to to see how culture influences the effect of SDT-based instruction on
detect moderator effects. The results of the analyses are also bound to motivation. SDT argues that there are no cultural differences in the
the methodological features of the included studies, which typically satisfaction of the basic psychological needs (Deci & Ryan, 2000). In the
lacked a randomized allocation to the conditions and rigorous control of PE setting, the findings of the recent meta-analysis by Vasconcellos et al.
confounding factors. (2019) partially support this idea. However, people from different cul­
Although the small number of studies and the small power to detect tures may respond differently to the same instruction if it carries a
moderating effects were anticipated, the non-significance of some of the different cultural meaning (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Markus &
moderators was unexpected. Like already stated, the high heterogeneity Kitayama, 2003). In addition, the as usual style (the most used control
of the effect sizes added to the notion SDT-based interventions have been condition in this review) is probably very different across cultures (e.g.,
conducted and operationalized in very different ways (Gillison et al., the use of teacher- and student-led instruction) (Cothran et al., 2005). As
2018; Stroet et al., 2013), and that the interplay between the different the nature of the control condition affected the size of the effect on
need-supportive schemes and different contextual factors and pop­ external regulation and amotivation, it might be reasonable to anticipate
ulations are not well understood. In addition, although a great deal of variable effects across different cultures with different normative
attention was devoted to coding the intervention characteristics practices.
correctly, the used strategies and the scope of the intervention was at Based on the number of studies included in this review, it can be
times, hard to interpret and conclude from the article descriptions. concluded that more research on the impact of SDT-based instruction
Hence, there is a possibility that the coding of the moderators (in some and sport is in demand. In fact, for this review, only three studies were
cases) does not reflect actual differences in the nature of the conducted in sport settings, and only one used a long-term intervention
interventions. design. We suggest that sport interventions intervening with coaching
Other moderators might have been selected a priori. However, behaviors could be highly important as a strong correlational link be­
because of the small number of effects and the increased risk for a type 1 tween need-thwarting coaching strategies and detrimental outcomes
error, we did not pursue additional univariate moderator analyses. In have been established (Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Ryan, & Thøger­
addition, all moderators were selected carefully. sen-Ntoumani, 2011; Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Ryan, & Thøgersen-N­
The modified Egger’s tests and funnel plots suggested that there is a toumani, 2011). Furthermore, including sport settings and, for example,
possibility for publication bias with the retrieved studies. However, coaching training programs would offer a completely new venue and
caution regarding the publication bias results should be taken as the opportunity for longer-term need-supportive interventions.
number of analyzed studies was relatively small, and the heterogeneity In this analysis, we decided to include only one post measurement
among effects high (Sterne, Gavaghan, & Egger, 2000). In addition, as time point from each study, although some of the studies had several
the detected outlying studies were removed, there seemed to be no issue measuring points of motivational regulations, even including follow-up
with publication bias. In addition, alleviating the issue, the search measurements for maintenance of motivation. With additional studies
strategy included unpublished research as well as dissertations and an and adoption of more complex multivariate analyses, the moderating
extensive contacting of the authors known to do research with SDT. effect of the duration of the intervention on the motivational regulations
could be estimated with better precision. Additionally, we suggest that

12
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

findings of meta-analyses in this discipline could be presented also as

Note: POMP effect sizes are computed without the study by Sullivan (2005) which reported only change scores. SMC = Standardized mean change; SMCD = Standardized mean change difference. *p < .05., **p < .01.,
POMP SMCD [CI.lb, CI.ub]
POMP scores to provide clarity of the overall motivational levels of
participants. For example, our analyses of POMP scores illustrate that

− 3.24 [-6.38, − 0.09]*


− 3.04 [-5.81, − 0.28]*
5.54 [2.97, 8.11]***
5.48 [0.55, 10.41]*
4.07 [1.52, 6.61]**
participants in organized physical activity display relatively high levels

1.01 [-0.92, 2.95]


of intrinsic motivation and identified regulation and low levels of
amotivation.
For this meta-analysis, we included only studies that had outcome
measures for separate motivational regulations postulated by SDT. This
approach left us to reduce the number included studies excluding studies
that reported motivational outcomes as weighted composite scores, such
as the relative autonomy index, self-determination index, and autono­

− 2.21 [-4.36, − 0.06]*


mous motivation. We suggest that future research should always report

− 0.90 [-2.80, 1.01]


the information also on the separate motivational regulations as they are
3.99 [0.51, 7.48]*
4.54 [0.07, 9.01]*
1.30 [-0.89, 3.48]
1.23 [-0.71, 3.17]

theoretically different from one another and have been theorized to have
Intervention

different implications for behavioral, affective, and cognitive outcomes


(Vallerand, 1997). Suppressing information can sometimes be justified,
but for example, a relative autonomy index combines all the different
motivational constructs into one variable limiting the interpretation of
the effects of interventions considerably (Howard, Gagné, Morin, &
POMP SMC [CI.lb, CI.ub]

Forest, 2017). Furthermore, as only five included studies reported scores


− 2.20 [-5.46, 1.07]
− 0.63 [-4.37, 3.11]
− 2.46 [-5.14, 0.21]

on integrated regulation, conclusions on the effect of SDT-based in­


0.05 [-2.05, 2.14]
0.37 [-2.46, 3.20]
1.67 [-1.26, 4.61]

struction on integrated regulation are difficult to make. Future research


should also measure this regulation as it is postulated in SDT to be one of
the six distinct motivational regulations with its implications for human
Control

behavior. However, the current scales measuring this construct might


need to be revised (Howard, Gagné, Morin, & Forest, 2017).
POMP as raw means at pre- and post-post, standardized mean change from pre-to post-test, and standardized mean change difference.

Lastly, we propose that future research efforts should use standard­


ized need-supportive strategies and behaviors. The meta-analysis by
[75.30, 81.70]
[48.77, 63.39]
[73.13, 78.46]
[49.44, 60.96]
[35.74, 50.23]
[25.15, 35.40]

Gillison et al. (2018) and the classification study by Teixeira et al. (2019)
are a good start to this direction and clearly shows the abundance of
behaviors and strategies that could be defined as need-supportive. This
Intervention

is potentially a problem since interventions utilizing several strategies


78.50
56.08
75.79
55.20
42.98
30.28

and behaviors are not able to pinpoint the importance of single strate­
gies or their interaction with a certain another strategy. This leaves the
efficacy of SDT-based instruction somewhat vague, open for too much
interpretation, and the importance of specific strategies over others
PostPOMP(CI.lb, CI.ub)

impossible to determine. Promising, however, is that 30 of the 38


76.97]
78.43]
75.83]
59.22]
53.71]
39.56]

analyzed studies were published in 2015 or after. This indicates a clear


positive trend of research in this area.
73.23 [69.48,
64.93 [51.43,
72.77 [69.71,
53.86 [48.50,
46.56 [39.42,
33.96 [28.37,
Control

5. Conclusion

This meta-analysis suggests that SDT-based instructional in­


terventions have a positive but heterogeneous effect on some of the
participants’ motivational regulations in organized physical activity. In
[70.92, 78.27]
[46.40, 57.33]
[70.98, 76.91]
[49.40, 59.64]
[37.07, 50.85]
[24.74, 33.76]

detail, the observed effect is small on intrinsic motivation, identified


regulation, external regulation, and amotivation. The practical impli­
Intervention

cations of the results should be considered with caution for three reasons
highlighted by our moderator analyses. First, as usual instruction seems
74.60
51.87
73.94
54.52
43.96
29.25

to be almost equally effective in motivating participants as SDT-based


instruction. Second, workshops in which teachers, coaches, or in­
structors are trained in SDT-based instruction are less effective at
improving motivation compared to more controlled and prescriptive
PrePOMP(CI.lb, CI.ub)

75.53 [71.64, 79.42]


57.99 [49.95, 66.03]
75.20 [72.32, 78.09]
52.34 [46.60, 58.07]
43.34 [36.13, 50.55]
29.81 [25.18, 34.45]

interventions. Finally, lower quality SDT-based intervention studies


yield the biggest effects on participant motivation. More well-designed
research in different contexts would help to understand the variable
effects of SDT-based instruction on motivational regulations of partici­
Control

pants in organized physical activity.

Funding

This research was not supported by any specific grant from any
6. Amotivation
4. Introjected
2. Integrated

funding agency in the public, private, or not-for-profit sector.


***p < .001.
3. Identified

5. External
1. Intrinsic
Outcomes
Table 3

13
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Table 4
Univariate Moderator analyses for all the outcomes.
Effect moderator Intrinsic Integrated Identified Introjected External Amotivation

k g (95% CI) k g (95% CI) k g (95% CI) k g (95% CI) k g (95% CI) k g(95% CI)

Setting
PE 24 .23 (.10, .36) 2 .11 (− .22, .44) 20 .19 (.05, .32) 15 .05 (− .07, .17) 17 − .18 (− .35, 17 − .12 (− .26, .03)
− .01)
Exercise & Sport 11 .42 (.20, .65) 3 .12 (− .19, .43) 8 .27 (.03, .51) 8 − .07 (− .28, .14) 8 − .12 (− .41, .17) 9 − .21 (− .46, .05)
Scope of need support
1 supported 7 .19 (− .09, .46) 0 NA 4 .13 (− .18, .44) 2 .17 (− .09, .42) 4 − .19 (− .53, .16) 4 − .19 (− .50, .14)
2 supported 12 .40 (.20, .59) 1 .17 (− .34, .68) 10 .35 (.15, .55) 9 .08 (− .07, .22) 9 − .30 (− .54, 9 − .29 (− .50, − .08)
− .06)
3 supported 16 .22 (.05, .39) 4 − .02 (− .26, 14 .15 (− .04, .33) 12 − .03 (− .15, .09) 12 − .01 (− .21, .20) 13 − .01 (− .20, .18)
.22)
Type of intervention
Trained 20 .22 (.07, .37) 2 .06 (− .18, .31) 16 .12 (− .02, 13 .05 (− .08, .18) 13 − .03 (− .20, .14)a 15 − .03 (− .17, .10)a
.25)a
Planned 15 .42 (.22, .63) 3 .09 (− .19, .37) 12 .41 (.21, .61)b 10 − .04 (− .23, .16) 12 − .37(-59, − .15)b 11 − .37(-57, − .16)b
Type of control
As usual 23 .22 (.08, .35)a 3 .09 (− .12, .31) 19 .12 (− .01, 15 .04 (− .09, .15) 16 − .03 (− .18, .12)a 18 − .04 (− .16, .08)a
.25)a
Manipulated 12 .51 (.28, .74)b 2 .14 (− .19, .47) 9 .52 (.29, .75)b 8 − .03 (− .25, .20) 9 − .47(-.71, − .23)b 8 − .48(-.69, − .26)b
Length of the study
Max 2 weeks 6 .05 (− .26, 1 − .02 (− .48, 4 .16 (− .21, .54) 2 − .02 (− .29, .25) 4 − 42 (− .72, 4 − .21 (− .54, .13)
.37)a .44) − .12)a
6–9 weeks 13 .46 (.25, .67)b 2 − .05 (− .32, 11 .39 (.15, .62) 9 − .07 (− .23, .09) 9 − 17 (− .39, .04)ab 11 − 29 (− .50, − .08)
.22)
> 10 weeks 16 .22 (.04, .41)ab 2 .06 (− .17, .28) 13 .11 (− .10, .33) 12 .07 (− .04, .19) 12 − .04 (− .22, .14)b 11 − .03 (− .22, .17)
Age, y
<18 26 .21 (.09, .33)a 2 .14 (− .08, .36) 29 .16 (.03, .30) 14 .04 (− .07, .15) 16 − .18 (− .35, .00) 18 − .13 (− .28, .02)
>18 9 .55 (.31, .80)b 3 .22 (− .11, .55) 9 .44 (.19, .70) 9 .06 (− .14, .25) 9 − .16 (− .45, .14) 8 − 23 (− .51, .06)
MERSQI
Low 10 .56 (.33, .79)a 1 .30 (− .22, .82) 9 .44 (.20, .69) 9 .14 (− .04, .32) 9 − .36 (− .64, 9 − .40 (− .64,
− .08) − .16)a
Medium 16 .21 (.05, .37)b 4 .01 (− .21, .24) 13 .14 (− .04, .32) 9 .00 (− .14, .14) 11 − .06 (− .27, .16) 10 − .06 (− .24, .12)b
High 9 .20 (− .01, 0 NA 6 .19 (− .03, .41) 5 − .03 (− .21, .16) 5 − .17 (− .46, .12) 7 − .04 (− .25, .16)b
.40)b
Quality Applied
Low 13 .45(.26, .64) 0 NA 10 .35 (.14, .56) 8 .21 (.07, .36)a 8 − .24 (− .51, .03) 9 − .30 (− .52, − .07)
Medium 16 .20 (.04, .37) 4 − .06 (− .28, 14 .14 (− .03, .32) 12 − .04 (− .15, 13 − .08 (− .29, .13) 13 − .06 (− .24, .12)
.15) .08)b
High 6 .13 (− .16, .42) 1 .01 (− .49, .51) 4 .21 (− .12, .54) 3 − .15 (− .36, 4 − .19 (− .59, .21) 4 − .05 (− .39, .29)
.07)b
Quality Total
Low 10 .55 (.33, .76)a 1 .29 (− .21, .80) 9 .43 (.19, .67) 8 .27 (.13, .42)a 8 − .29 (− .58, 8 − .44 (− .67,
− .00) − .20)a
b
Medium 16 .21 (.05, .37) 3 − .09 (− .30, 12 .15 (− .04, .33) 9 − .08 (− .20, 10 − .06 (− .29, .17) 10 − .00 (− .19, .18)b
.13) .03)b
High 9 .15 (− .05, 1 .09 (− .38, .56) 7 .16 (− .06, .38) 6 − .02 (− .15, 7 − .18 (− .45, .09) 8 − .07 (− .28, .13)b
.35)b .11)b
Total 35 .29 (.17, .41) 5 .08 (-.11, .28) 28 .23 (.10, .35) 23 .03 (-.07, .13) 25 ¡.16 (-.31, -.00) 26 ¡.14 (-.28, -.01)

Note. ab = within a categorical moderator, mean estimates without a common superscript differ (p < .05)

Declaration of competing interest school and its effect on the both genders. PLoS One, 12. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174393.
* Barkoukis, V., Chatzisarantis, N., & Hagger, M. S. (2021). Effects of a school-based
None. intervention on motivation for out-of-school physical activity participation. Research
Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 92(3), 477–491.
Appendix A. Supplementary data Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., Bosch, J. A., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C.
(2011). Self-determination theory and diminished functioning: The role of
interpersonal control and psychological need thwarting. Personality and Social
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi. Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1459–1473. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211413125
org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2022.102248. Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2011).
Psychological need thwarting in the sport context: Assessing the darker side of
athletic experience. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33, 75–102. https://doi.
References org/10.1123/jsep.33.1.75
Becker, B. J. (1988). Synthesizing standardized mean-change measures. British Journal of
* Abula, K., Beckmann, J., He, Z., Cheong, C., Lu, F., & Gröpel, P. (2018). Autonomy Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 41(2), 257–278.
support in physical education promotes autonomous motivation towards * Behzadnia, B., Mohammadzadeh, H., & Ahmadi, M. (2017). Autonomy-supportive
leisure-time physical activity: Evidence from a sample of Chinese college students. behaviors promote autonomous motivation, knowledge structures, motor skills
Health Promotion International, 102, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1093/heapro/day102. learning and performance in physical education. Current Psychology. https://doi.
Aelterman, N., Vansteenkiste, M., Van Keer, H., Van den Berghe, L., De Meyer, J., & org/10.1007/s12144-017-9727-0.
Haerens, L. (2012). Students’ objectively measured physical activity levels and Berkey, C. S., Hoaglin, D. C., Antczak-Bouckoms, A., Mosteller, F., & Colditz, G. A.
engagement as a function of between-class and between-student differences in (1998). Meta-analysis of multiple outcomes by regression with random effects.
motivation toward physical education. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 34, Statistics in Medicine, 17, 2537–2550.
457–480. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.34.4.457 Cerasoli, C. P., Nicklin, J. M., & Ford, M. T. (2014). Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic
* Amado, D., Sánchez-Miguel, P. A., & Molero, P. (2017). Creativity associated with the incentives jointly predict performance: A 40-year meta-analysis. Psychological
application of a motivational intervention programme for the teaching of dance at Bulletin, 140, 980–1008. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035661

14
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Chang, Y. K., Chen, S., Tu, K. W., & Chi, L. K. (2016). Effect of autonomy support on self- self-determination theory perspective. Health Psychology Review, 13, 110–130.
determined motivation in elementary physical education. Journal of Sports Science https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2018.1534071
and Medicine, 15(3), 460–466. * Gillison, F. B., Standage, M., & Skevington, S. M. (2013). The effects of manipulating
* Charbonneau, D., Barling, J., & Kelloway, E. K. (2001). Transformational leadership goal content and autonomy support climate on outcomes of a physical education
and sports performance: The mediating role of intrinsic motivation. Journal of fitness class. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14, 342–352. https://doi.
Applied Social Psychology, 31, 1521–1534. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.11.011.
org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb02686.x. * González-Cutre, D., Sierra, A. C., Beltrán-Carrillo, V. J., Peláez-Pérez, M., & Cervelló, E.
Chatzisarantis, N. L., Hagger, M. S., Biddle, S. J., Smith, B., & Wang, J. C. (2003). A meta- (2018). A school-based motivational intervention to promote physical activity from
analysis of perceived locus of causality in exercise, sport, and physical education a self-determination theory perspective. The Journal of Educational Research, 111,
contexts. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 25, 284–306. https://doi.org/ 320–330. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2016.1255871.
10.1123/jsep.25.3.284 Goudas, M., Biddle, S., & Fox, K. (1994). Perceived locus of causality, goal orientations,
* Cheon, S. H., & Moon, I. S. (2010). Implementing an autonomy-supportive fitness and perceived competence in school physical education classes. British Journal of
program to facilitate students’ autonomy and engagement. Korean Journal of Sport Educational Psychology, 64, 453–463. https://doi.org/10.1123/pes.6.2.159
Psychology, 21(1), 175–195. Haerens, L., Aelterman, N., Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., & Van Petegem, S. (2015). Do
* Cheon, S. H., & Reeve, J. (2015). A classroom-based intervention to help teachers perceived autonomy-supportive and controlling teaching relate to physical
decrease students’ amotivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 40, 99–111. education students’ motivational experiences through unique pathways?
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.06.004. Distinguishing between the bright and dark side of motivation. Psychology of Sport
* Cheon, S. H., Reeve, J., & Moon, I. S. (2012). Experimentally based, longitudinally and Exercise, 16, 26–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.08.013
designed, teacher-focused intervention to help physical education teachers be more Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2009). Integrating the theory of planned
autonomy supportive toward their students. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, behaviour and self-determination theory in health behaviour: A meta-analysis.
34, 365–396. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.34.3.365. British Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 275–302. https://doi.org/10.1348/
* Cheon, S. H., Reeve, J., & Song, Y. G. (2016). A teacher-focused intervention to 135910708X373959
decrease PE students’ amotivation by increasing need satisfaction and decreasing * Ha, A. S., Lonsdale, C., Lubans, D. R., & Ng, J. Y. (2020). Increasing students’ activity in
need frustration. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 38, 217–235. https://doi. physical education: Results of the self-determined exercise and learning for FITness
org/10.1123/jsep.2015-0236. trial. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 52, 696–704. https://doi.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York, NY: org/10.1249/mss.0000000000002172.
Routledge Academic. Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic press.
Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1999). The problem of units and the Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring
circumstance for POMP. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 34, 315–346. https://doi. inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327, 557–560. https://doi.org/10.1136/
org/10.1207/S15327906MBR3403_2 bmj.327.7414.557
Core Team, R. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Howard, J. L., Bureau, J., Guay, F., Chong, J. X., & Ryan, R. M. (2021). Student
Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-pro motivation and associated outcomes: A meta-analysis from self-determination
ject.org/. theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Cothran, D. J., Kulinna, P. H., Banville, D., Choi, E., Amade-Escot, C., MacPhail, A., … 1745691620966789
Kirk, D. (2005). A cross-cultural investigation of the use of teaching styles. Research Howard, J. L., Gagné, M., & Bureau, J. S. (2017). Testing a continuum structure of self-
Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 76, 193–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/ determined motivation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 1346.
02701367.2005.10599280 https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000125
Cox, A. E., Smith, A. L., & Williams, L. (2008). Change in physical education motivation Howard, J. L., Gagné, M., Morin, A. J., & Forest, J. (2017). Using bifactor exploratory
and physical activity behavior during middle school. Journal of Adolescent Health, 43, structural equation modeling to test for a continuum structure of motivation. Journal
506–513. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.04.020 of Management, 44, 2638–2664. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206316645653
Daley, A. J., & Duda, J. L. (2006). Self-determination, stage of readiness to change for Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural
exercise, and frequency of physical activity in young people. European Journal of perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
Sport Science, 6, 231–243. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461390601012637 349. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514 .76.3.349
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human Jackson, D., White, I. R., & Riley, R. D. (2012). Quantifying the impact of between-study
behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-2271-7 heterogeneity in multivariate meta-analyses. Statistics in Medicine, 31(29),
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ’what’ and ’why’ of goal pursuits: Human needs 3805–3820. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.5453
and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. https:// * Langan, E., Toner, J., Blake, C., & Lonsdale, C. (2015). Testing the effects of a
doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01 self-determination theory-based intervention with youth gaelic football coaches on
Deci, E. L., Spiegel, N. H., Ryan, R. M., Koestner, R., & Kauffman, M. (1982). Effects of athlete motivation and burnout. The Sport Psychologist, 29, 293–301. https://doi.
performance standards on teaching styles: Behavior of controlling teachers. Journal org/10.1123/tsp.2013-0107.
of Educational Psychology, 74, 852. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.74.6.852 Lazowski, R. A., & Hulleman, C. S. (2016). Motivation interventions in education: A
Deeks, J. J., Higgins, J. P. T., & Altman, D. G. (2011). Chapter 9: Analysing data and meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research, 86, 602–640, 10.3102%
undertaking meta-analyses. In J. P. T. Higgins, & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane Handbook 2F0034654315617832.
for systematic Reviews of interventions version 5.1.0. The Cochrane Collaboration. * Leptokaridou, E. T., Vlachopoulos, S. P., & Papaioannou, A. G. (2016). Experimental
https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470712184.ch9, 2011. longitudinal test of the influence of autonomy-supportive teaching on motivation for
Dishman, R. K., Mciver, K. L., Dowda, M., & Pate, R. R. (2018). Declining physical participation in elementary school physical education. Educational Psychology, 36,
activity and motivation from middle school to high school. Medicine & Science in 1138–1159. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2014.950195.
Sports & Exercise, 50, 1206–1215. https://doi.org/10.1249/ * Leyton, M., Batista, M., Lobato, S., Aspano, M. I., & Jimenez, R. (2017). Application of
MSS.0000000000001542 two intervention programs in order to optimize motivation and to improve eating
Duncan, L. R., Hall, C. R., Wilson, P. M., & Jenny, O. (2010). Exercise motivation: A habits in adult and elderly women. Journal of Human Kinetics, 59, 131. https://doi.
cross-sectional analysis examining its relationships with frequency, intensity, and org/10.1515/hukin-2017-0153.
duration of exercise. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. London, UK: Sage
7, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-7-7 Publications.
* Edmunds, J., Ntoumanis, N., & Duda, J. L. (2008). Testing a self-determination Lochbaum, M., & Jean-Noel, J. (2015). Perceived autonomy-support instruction and
theory-based teaching style intervention in the exercise domain. European Journal of student outcomes in physical education and leisure-time: A meta-analytic review of
Social Psychology, 38, 375–388. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.463. correlates. Revista Internacional de Ciencias del Deporte, 12, 29–47. https://doi.org/
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis 10.5232/ricyde2016.04302
detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315, 629–634. https://doi.org/10.1136/ * Lonsdale, C., Lester, A., Owen, K. B., White, R. L., Peralta, L., Kirwan, M., … Lubans,
bmj.315.7109.629 D. R. (2019). An internet-supported school physical activity intervention in low
* Fin, G., Moreno-Murcia, J. A., León, J., Baretta, E., & Júnior, R. J. N. (2019). socioeconomic status communities: Results from the activity and motivation in
Interpersonal autonomy support style and its consequences in physical education physical education (AMPED) cluster randomised controlled trial. British Journal of
classes. PLoS One, 14. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216609. Sports Medicine, 53, 341–347. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2017-097904.
* Franco, E., & Coterón, J. (2017). The effects of a Physical education intervention to * Lonsdale, C., Rosenkranz, R. R., Sanders, T., Peralta, L. R., Bennie, A., Jackson, B., …
support the satisfaction of basic psychological needs on the motivation and Lubans, D. R. (2013). A cluster randomized controlled trial of strategies to increase
intentions to be physically active. Journal of Human Kinetics, 59, 5–15. https://doi. adolescents’ physical activity and motivation in physical education: Results of the
org/10.1515/hukin-2017-0143. Motivating Active Learning in Physical Education (MALP) trial. Preventive Medicine,
* Fransen, K., Boen, F., Vansteenkiste, M., Mertens, N., & Vande Broek, G. (2018). The 57, 696–702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.09.003.
power of competence support: The impact of coaches and athlete leaders on intrinsic * Manninen, M., Deng, Y., Hwang, Y., Waller, S., & Yli-Piipari, S. (2020). Psychological
motivation and performance. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 28, need-supportive instruction improves novel skill performance, intrinsic motivation,
725–745. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.12950. and enjoyment: a cluster-randomised study. International Journal of Sport and Exercise
Gillet, N., Berjot, S., Vallerand, R. J., & Amoura, S. (2012). The role of autonomy support Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2020.1826999.
and motivation in the prediction of interest and drop-out intentions in sport and Markland, D., & Tobin, V. (2004). A modification to the behavioural regulation in
education settings. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34, 278–286. https://doi.org/ exercise questionnaire to include an assessment of amotivation. Journal of Sport &
10.1080/01973533.2012.674754 Exercise Psychology, 26, 191–196. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.26.2.191
Gillison, F. B., Rouse, P., Standage, M., Sebire, S. J., & Ryan, R. M. (2018). A meta- Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Models of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the
analysis of techniques to promote motivation for health behaviour change from a construction of action. In V. Murphy-Berman, & J. J. Berman (Eds.), Nebraska

15
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

symposium on motivation, 9 pp. 1–57). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Cross- Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes.
cultural differences in perspectives on the self. Journal of Personality, 63, 397–427. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1995.
McDonough, M. H., & Crocker, P. R. (2007). Testing self-determined motivation as a tb00501.x
mediator of the relationship between psychological needs and affective and Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization:
behavioral outcomes. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 29, 645–663. https:// Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social
doi.org/10.1123/jsep.29.5.645 Psychology, 57, 749–761. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.749
* Mertens, N., Boen, F., Vande Broek, G., Vansteenkiste, M., & Fransen, K. (2018). An Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in
experiment on the impact of coaches’ and athlete leaders’ competence support on motivation development and wellness. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
athletes’ motivation and performance. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in * Sánchez-Oliva, D., Pulido-González, J. J., Leo, F. M., González-Ponce, I., &
Sports, 28, 2734–2750. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13273. García-Calvo, T. (2017). Effects of an intervention with teachers in the physical
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & Prisma Group.. (2009). Preferred education context: A self-determination theory approach. PLoS One, 12. https://doi.
reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189986.
PLoS Medicine, 6, Article e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. * Schneider, J., Polet, J., Hassandra, M., Lintunen, T., Laukkanen, A., Hankonen, N., …
pmed.1000097 Hagger, M. S. (2020). Using physical education to promote leisure-time physical activity
* Moreno-Murcia, J. A., & Sánchez-Latorre, F. (2016). The effects of autonomy support in in lower secondary school students: The PETALS Trial. Research Square. Preprint.
physical education classes. Revista Internacional De Ciencias Del Deporte, 12, 79–89. * Sebire, S. J., Edwards, M. J., Fox, K. R., Davies, B., Banfield, K., Wood, L., & Jago, R.
https://doi.org/10.5232/ricyde2016.04305. (2016). Delivery and receipt of a self-determination-theory-based extracurricular
Morris, S. B. (2008). Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. physical activity intervention: Exploring theoretical fidelity in action 3:30. Journal of
Organizational Research Methods, 11, 364–386. https://doi: 10.1177/10944281 Sport & Exercise Psychology, 38, 381–395. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2015-0217.
06291059. * Sfandyari, B., Ghorbani, S., Rezaeeshirazi, R., & Noohpisheh, S. (2020). The
* Moustaka, F. C., Vlachopoulos, S. P., Kabitsis, C., & Theodorakis, Y. (2012). Effects of effectiveness of an autonomy-based exercise training on intrinsic motivation,
an autonomy-supportive exercise instructing style on exercise motivation, physical activity intention, and health-related fitness of sedentary students in middle
psychological well-being, and exercise attendance in middle-age women. Journal of school. International Journal of School Health, 7, 40–47. https://doi.
Physical Activity and Health, 9, 138–150. https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.9.1.138. org/10.30476/INTJSH.2020.84678.1046.
Ng, J. Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Duda, J. L., & Shrout, P. E., & Rodgers, J. L. (2018). Psychology, science, and knowledge construction:
Williams, G. C. (2012). Self-determination theory applied to health contexts: A meta- Broadening perspectives from the replication crisis. Annual Review of Psychology, 69,
analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 325–340, 10.1177% 487–510. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011845
2F1745691612447309. Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects
Ntoumanis, N., Ng, J. Y., Prestwich, A., Quested, E., Hancox, J. E., Thøgersen- of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of
Ntoumani, C., … Williams, G. C. (2020). A meta-analysis of self-determination Educational Psychology, 85, 571. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.4.571
theory-informed intervention studies in the health domain: Effects on motivation, * Sparks, C., Lonsdale, C., Dimmock, J., & Jackson, B. (2017). An intervention to improve
health behavior, physical, and psychological health. Health Psychology Review, 1–31. teachers’ interpersonally involving instructional practices in high school physical
https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2020.1718529 education: Implications for student relatedness support and in-class experiences.
Ntoumanis, N., Stenling, A., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Vlachopoulos, S., Lindwall, M., … Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 39, 120–133. https://doi.
Tsakonitis, C. (2018). Longitudinal associations between exercise identity and org/10.1123/jsep.2016-0198.
exercise motivation: A multilevel growth curve model approach. Scandinavian Standage, M., Gillison, F. B., Ntoumanis, N., & Treasure, D. C. (2012). Predicting
Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 28, 746–753. https://doi.org/10.1111/ students’ physical activity and health-related well-being: A prospective cross-domain
sms.12951 investigation of motivation across school physical education and exercise settings.
* Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Quested, E., & Hancox, J. (2017). The effects Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 34, 37–60. https://doi.org/10.1123/
of training group exercise class instructors to adopt a motivationally adaptive jsep.34.1.37
communication style. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 27, Sterne, J. A., Gavaghan, D., & Egger, M. (2000). Publication and related bias in meta-
1026–1034. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.12713. analysis: Power of statistical tests and prevalence in the literature. Journal of Clinical
Owen, K. B., Smith, J., Lubans, D. R., Ng, J. Y., & Lonsdale, C. (2014). Self-determined Epidemiology, 53, 1119–1129. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0895-4356(00)00242-0
motivation and physical activity in children and adolescents: A systematic review Stroet, K., Opdenakker, M. C., & Minnaert, A. (2013). Effects of need supportive teaching
and meta-analysis. Preventive Medicine, 67, 270–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. on early adolescents’ motivation and engagement: A review of the literature.
ypmed.2014.07.033 Educational Research Review, 9, 65–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Briere, N. M. (2001). Associations edurev.2012.11.003
among perceived autonomy support, forms of self-regulation, and persistence: A * Sullivan, G. S. (2005). The effects of a coaching education workshop on the self-regulated
prospective study. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 279–306. https://doi.org/10.1023/A: motivation of 6th grade male and female basketball players. The Ohio State University:
1014805132406 Doctoral Dissertation.
Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on the role of student Su, Y. L., & Reeve, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of intervention
motivation in learning and teaching contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, programs designed to support autonomy. Educational Psychology Review, 23,
667. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.667 159–188. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9142-7
Pope, J. P., & Wilson, P. M. (2012). Understanding motivational processes in university Taylor, I. M., & Lonsdale, C. (2010). Cultural differences in the relationships among
rugby players: A preliminary test of the hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic autonomy support, psychological need satisfaction, subjective vitality, and effort in
motivation at the contextual level. International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching, British and Chinese physical education. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32,
7, 89–107, 10.1260%2F1747-9541.7.1.89. 655–673. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.32.5.655
* Prusak, K. A., Treasure, D. C., Darst, P. W., & Pangrazi, R. P. (2004). The effects of Teixeira, P. J., Carraça, E. V., Markland, D., Silva, M. N., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Exercise,
choice on the motivation of adolescent girls in physical education. Journal of physical activity, and self-determination theory: A systematic review. International
Teaching in Physical Education, 23, 19–29. https://doi.org/10.1123/jtpe.23.1.19. Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9, 78. https://doi.org/10.1186/
Pulido, J. J., Sánchez-Oliva, D., Leo, F. M., Matos, S., & García-Calvo, T. (2017). Effects of 1479-5868-9-78
an interpersonal style intervention for coaches on young soccer players’ Teixeira, P. J., Marques, M. M., Silva, M. N., Brunet, J., Duda, J., Haerens, L., &
motivational processes. Journal of Human Kinetics, 59, 107. https://doi.org/10.1515/ Hagger, M. S. (2019). Classification of techniques used in self-determination theory-
hukin-2017-0151 based interventions in health contexts: An expert consensus study. Preprint.
Reeve, J. (2016). Autonomy-supportive teaching: What it is, how to do it. In Available in: https://osf.io/f65jy.
J. C. K. Wang, W. C. Liu, & R. M. Ryan’s (Eds.), Building autonomous learners: Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., & Ntoumanis, N. (2006). The role of self-determined
Perspectives from research and practice using self-determination theory (pp. 129–152). motivation in the understanding of exercise-related behaviours, cognitions and
New York, NY: Springer. Chap. 5. physical self-evaluations. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 393–404. https://doi.org/
Reeve, J., & Jang, H. (2006). What teachers say and do to support students’ autonomy 10.1080/02640410500131670
during a learning activity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 209. https://doi. * Tilga, H., Hein, V., & Koka, A. (2019). Effects of a web-based intervention for PE
org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.209 teachers on students’ perceptions of teacher behaviors, psychological needs, and
Rodgers, W. M., Hall, C. R., Duncan, L. R., Pearson, E., & Milne, M. I. (2010). Becoming a intrinsic motivation. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 126, 559–580. https://doi.
regular exerciser: Examining change in behavioural regulations among exercise org/10.1177/0031512519840150.
initiates. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11, 378–386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. * Tilga, H., Kalajas-Tilga, H., Hein, V., Raudsepp, L., & Koka, A. (2020). Effects of a brief
psychsport.2010.04.007 one-day autonomy-supportive intervention on improving basic psychological needs,
Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in motivation, and behaviours of physical activity among middle-school students: A
quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 59–82. multidimensional approach. International Journal of Sport Psychology.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59 * Ulstad, S. O., Halvari, H., Sørebø, Ø., & Deci, E. L. (2018). Motivational predictors of
Rottensteiner, C., Happonen, L., & Konttinen, N. (2015). The interplay of autonomous learning strategies, participation, exertion, and performance in physical education: A
and controlled motivation in youth team sports. International Journal of Sport randomized controlled trial. Motivation and Emotion, 42, 497–512. https://doi.
Psychology, 46, 225–243. https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP.2015.46.225 org/10.1007/s11031-018-9694-2.
Ryan, R. M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic motivation, autonomy and the Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic
self in psychological development. In J. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp.
motivation: Developmental perspectives on motivation, 40 pp. 1–56). Lincoln, NE: 271–359). New York: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)
University of Nebraska Press. 60019-2.

16
M. Manninen et al. Psychology of Sport & Exercise 62 (2022) 102248

Vallerand, R. J. (2000). Deci and Ryan’s self-determination theory: A view from the International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 12, 67. https://doi.
hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, org/10.1186/s129660150226-0
312–318. Wilson, P. M., & Rodgers, W. M. (2002). The relationship between exercise motives and
Vansteenkiste, M., Ryan, R. M., & Soenens, B. (2020). Basic psychological need theory: physical self-esteem in female exercise participants: An application of self-
Advancements, critical themes, and future directions. Motivation and Emotion, 44, determination theory. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 7, 30–43. https://
1–31. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-019-09818-1 doi.org/10.1111/j.17519861.2002.tb00074.x
Vasconcellos, D., Parker, P. D., Hilland, T., Cinelli, R., Owen, K. B., Kapsal, N., … Wilson, P. M., Rodgers, W. M., Fraser, S. N., & Murray, T. C. (2004). Relationships
Lonsdale, C. (2019). Self-determination theory applied to physical education: A between exercise regulations and motivational consequences in university students.
systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology. https://doi. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 75, 81–91. https://doi.org/10.1080/
org/10.1037/edu0000420 02701367.2004.10609136
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the Metafor package. * Zazo, R., Peruyero, F., & Murcia, J. A. M. (2018). Autonomy support in the aquatic
Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03 motivational healthy program through the SDT. Motricidade, 14, 95–106.
Viechtbauer, W., & Cheung, M. W. (2010). Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta- https://doi.org/10.6063/motricidade.13864.
analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 1, 112–125. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.11 An asterisk indicates the studies used in the meta-analysis.
Werman-Josefsson, K., Lindwall, M., & Ivarsson, A. (2015). Need satisfaction,
motivational regulations and exercise: Moderation and mediation effects.

17

You might also like