You are on page 1of 18

Development and Psychopathology, 12 (2000), 695–712

Copyright  2000 Cambridge University Press


Printed in the United States of America

The role of early experience in shaping


behavioral and brain development and its
implications for social policy

GERALDINE DAWSON, SHARON B. ASHMAN, AND LESLIE J. CARVER


University of Washington

Abstract
This article provides a targeted review of the scientific literature on the effects of experience on early brain and
behavioral development and later outcome as it pertains to risk for some forms of child psychopathology. It is
argued that ample evidence exists indicating that the prenatal and early postnatal years likely represent a sensitive
period with respect to the effects of stress on the developing nervous system and behavioral outcome, and with
respect to the long-term beneficial effects of early interventions on brain and behavioral development for some
genetically based disorders, such as phenylketonuria and autism. Moreover, evidence suggests that parental mental
health during the first years of life has a significant influence on early brain activity and behavior, and long-term
behavioral outcome. It is concluded that, although prevention and early intervention efforts should not exclusively
focus on the earliest years of development, such efforts should begin during this period. By directing such efforts
toward promoting optimal prenatal and infant–toddler development, the long-term negative consequences of factors
that have their greatest influences during early development and which set the stage for future development can be
minimized or avoided entirely. Several recommendations for public policy and future research pertaining to the
effects of early experience on child outcome are offered.

In the past few decades, efforts at promoting from birth to 3 years; the Family and Medical
optimal development in children have increas- Leave Act enacted in 1993, which recognized
ingly focused on the earliest years of life, the importance of parents spending time with
including the prenatal period, infancy, and their young infants after birth before returning
toddlerhood. Broad-based policy changes re- to work; and Early Head Start created in
flecting a focus on early development have 1994, which provided education and childcare
included PL 99-457 enacted in 1986, which to at-risk children from birth to 3 years. The
provided services to handicapped children rationale behind these efforts has been based
largely on an increased understanding of nor-
mal infant behavior and development and of
The writing of this article and the research on the effects the negative influences of factors such as poor
of maternal depression on infant brain activity reported nutrition, drug exposure, abuse and neglect,
herein were supported by grants from the National Insti- and parent psychopathology and stress on
tute of Mental Health (MH47117) and the National Insti-
tutes of Child Health and Human Development, and early development and later outcome (Nelson,
Deafness and Communication Disorders (POIHD34565). in press).
Lara Embry, Karin Frey, Lili Lengua, Charles Nelson, The idea that early experience is important
and Joanna Self provided helpful critiques of early ver- for promoting optimal long-term outcomes for
sions of this paper. children has been supported by studies of be-
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: G.
Dawson, Department of Psychology and Center on Hu- havioral outcomes and early intervention in
man Development and Disability, Box 357920, Univer- various at-risk and developmentally disabled
sity of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195. populations. Examples of such research in-

695
696 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

clude studies of (a) the effects of prenatal nu- tion discussed the importance of the new
trition and exposure to toxic substances on brain science for understanding children’s de-
brain and behavioral development (Morgan & velopment and for public policy. As a result,
Gibson, 1991; Sonderegger, 1992; Spohr, leaders in several states enacted policy
Williams, & Steinhausen, 1993); (b) the ef- changes based on what they viewed as the
fects of maternal depression during pregnancy new research on brain development. Numer-
and infancy on later cognitive and emotional ous policy initiatives and exploratory panels
functioning in children (Goodman & Gotlib, emerged to address the issue of early experi-
1999); and (c) the effects of early interven- ence and brain development in several states
tions for improving cognitive, social, and aca- (Groginsky, Christian, & McConnell, 1998).
demic functioning in children from economi- In 1997, the Federal Early Childhood Devel-
cally and socially disadvantaged backgrounds opment Act was introduced, stressing the im-
(Campbell & Ramey, 1994) and in children portance of early experience on brain devel-
with genetic disorders that affect brain devel- opment and outcome for children.
opment, such as autism and phenylketonuria The recent public and political enthusiasm
(PKU; Dawson & Osterling, 1997; Wappner, over the effects of early environment on brain
Cho, Kronmal, Schuett, & Seashore, 1999). development subsequently led to a backlash
At the same time that research and policy among some scientists who were concerned
have increasingly stressed the importance of that the public and policymakers have over-
early experience in the development of chil- simplified a complex issue. In fact, the public
dren, new techniques for studying infant be- hype over the implications of brain science
havior and brain activity have been devel- and the resulting recent critiques and contro-
oped, allowing developmental scientists to versy may have had the unfortunate effect of
begin to learn more about relations between a waning of interest in early childhood, with
biology and behavior in infants and children a loss of policies with the potential to benefit
(reviewed in Dawson & Fischer, 1994). In the children (Thompson & Nelson, in press).
1990s, as these intriguing studies were pub- Nevertheless, scientists such as Bruer (1999),
lished and discussed in the popular media, Greenough (1997), and others have raised im-
public interest in early brain development and portant questions regarding what is actually
behavior began to swell. The effects of early known about the effects of early experience
experience on the developing brain, in partic- on brain development. In his recent book,
ular, became of interest after a White House Bruer (1999) argues that the media have been
conference on the topic was held in the spring misguided in promoting the myth that the first
of 1997. The White House conference coin- 3 years of a baby’s life can determine a
cided with a national media campaign focused child’s long-range outcome. He further de-
on early development, which included broad- scribes what he refers to as “three neuroscien-
casts on “Good Morning America” and fea- tific strands” that taken together form the
ture articles in Newsweek. Concurrently, pub- “myth of the first three years.” In this article,
lic policy documents on the relation between we will discuss each of these strands and what
brain science and early development were can be scientifically concluded about their im-
published by major foundations interested in portance for understanding the effects of early
promoting the health and welfare of children, experience on later outcome. We will cover
including Starting Points, published by the in some detail what is known about the devel-
Carnegie Corporation, and Rethinking the opment and underlying mechanisms involved
Brain: New Insights Into Early Development, in synaptic formation and efficiency. We will,
published in conjunction with the White in addition, describe the growing body of evi-
House conference. These documents were dence demonstrating that exposure to early
covered in the popular media and read by pol- maladaptive experiences, such as prenatal and
icymakers and politicians interested in public postnatal stress and parental psychopathology,
health and child development. Keynote speak- does indeed have long-term implications for
ers at the 1997 National Governor’s Associa- brain and behavioral development. The impli-
Role of experience 697

cations of the research pertaining to the ef- aptic formation and pruning and behavioral
fects of early experience on brain and behav- development. First, it is known that some dis-
ior for public policy will be considered in the orders that involve mental retardation, such as
concluding section of this paper. Fragile X, are associated with abnormal syn-
aptic pruning, specifically with excess syn-
Synaptogenesis and the Remarkable apses (Comery, Harris, Willems, Oostra, Ir-
Period of Early Brain Growth win, Weiler, & Greenough, 1997). While
The first neuroscientific strand that Bruer de- these disorders are caused by genetic abnor-
scribes pertains to the fact that the brain de- malities rather than faulty early experiences,
velops rapidly during the prenatal and early it is noteworthy that there does appear to be a
postnatal months. Remarkably, early in a relation between synaptic density and intelli-
child’s life the brain produces trillions more gence, with higher numbers of synapses asso-
synapses than are found in adulthood. During ciated with lower intelligence.
the first 3 years of life, synapses form at a Second, it appears that increased early en-
phenomenal rate. In humans, it appears that vironmental stimulation does not necessarily
rapid synapse formation begins during the lead to increases in synaptic formation during
first few postnatal months and reaches highest the period of rapid synaptic proliferation and
densities at about 3 months for sensory cortex pruning, at least for the visual system in non-
and about 2–3.5 years for frontal cortex (Hut- human primates (Bourgeois, Jastreboff, &
tenlocher, 1994; Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, Rakic, 1989). It is clear, however, that experi-
1997; Rakic, Bourgeois, & Goldman–Rakic, ence in some instances can alter the synaptic
1994). Furthermore, this period of peak syn- formation and organization in the cortex (see
aptic density coincides in time with the emer- Kolb, Forgie, Gibb, Gorny, & Rowntree,
gence of important cognitive skills, including 1998, for a review of this rich set of data).
early manifestations of working memory (Di- Animals raised in complex, enriched environ-
amond & Goldman–Rakic, 1989). For exam- ments have more synapses in certain parts of
ple, a study by Bell and Fox (1994) showed their brains compared to animals raised in
that infants who are able to achieve early nonenriched environments (Greenough, Black,
frontal tasks involving working memory dis- & Wallace, 1987). Furthermore, changes in
played increased electrical brain activity over synaptic organization as a result of experience
the frontal scalp region. Put simply, neurosci- are correlated with changes in behavioral per-
ence and developmental science have shown formance in both animals and humans (Kolb
that early life is a time of truly remarkable et al., 1998). For example, Jacobs and
growth in terms of both brain and behavior. Scheibel (1993) and Jacobs, Schall, and
However, as Bruer points out, there is still rel- Scheibel (1993) reported a relation between
atively little known about how early experi- extent of dendritic arborization (which would
ences, such as parenting, actually affect brain act to increase the number of synapses) and
development, such as the rate or patterns of the amount of education received. Interest-
synaptic formation and pruning, or even about ingly, research has shown that the effect of
the relation between changes in regional syn- enriched experience on synaptic number and
aptic densities and specific cognitive skills organization is highly dependent upon the
(e.g., peak synaptic densities in the frontal re- age. Enriched experience during early postna-
gion and the emergence of early frontal lobe tal life appears to lead to a decrease in spine
skills). Although it is intriguing that certain density on the neuron but to have no effect on
biological events such as rapid synaptic pro- dendritic length (Kolb et al., 1998).
liferation and the emergence of certain cogni- Science is still addressing the question of
tive skills co-occur in development, such a co- precisely how experiences affect the pattern
occurrence does not prove that there is any of selective elimination and responsiveness of
causal relation between these two processes. neural networks formed during early develop-
There are, however, several things that are ment, as opposed to the sheer quantity of syn-
known about the relation between early syn- apses, which appears, in large part, to be
698 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

under genetic control. It is known that experi- ute to memory. A recent report described the
ence can and does play a role in selecting and formation of new dendritic spines following
establishing preferentially active synapses. LTP (Toni, Buchs, Nikonenko, Bron, & Mul-
Selective amplification of specific neural ler, 1999). This morphological synaptic
groups occurs via a competitive process as a change may also lead to changes in synapse
result of the frequency and intensity of stimu- responsiveness.
lation (Edelman, 1987; Hebb, 1949). Once a Consistent with the idea that LTP underlies
specific pattern of neuronal groups is selected the formation of memories, a number of stud-
in a mapped area, exposure to the same or ies have shown that blocking LTP through re-
similar stimuli is likely to preferentially acti- ceptor antagonists or genetic manipulations
vate previously selected neuronal groups. In leads to impaired memory performance in ex-
this way, it is hypothesized, developing neural perimental animals (Lu, Jia, Janus, Hender-
patterns become stabilized and less suscepti- son, Gerlai, Wojtowicz, & Roder, 1997; May-
ble to change over time (Edelman, 1989). ford, Bach, Huang, Wang, Hawkins, &
Such maps are believed to involve cortical Kandel, 1996). In addition, enhancing LTP by
sensory and motor maps as well as the brain manipulating second-messenger cascades in-
regions with which these cortical areas inter- volved in LTP is correlated with improved
act, such as limbic regions. Research is shed- memory performance (Barad, Bourtchou-
ding light on the mechanisms involved in se- ladze, Winder, Golan, & Kandel, 1998). In
lecting and establishing preferentially active addition to memory formation, some research
neural patterns. Repeated and synchronous ac- suggests that components of LTP that are in-
tivation of neighboring neurons strengthens volved in gene regulation are important for
the connections between them, and allows consolidation of memory traces (Bourtchou-
them to fire more efficiently. ladze, Frenguelli, Blendy, Cioffi, Schutz, &
Decades ago, Hebb (1949) suggested that Silva, 1994). Thus, although there are some
either morphological or metabolic changes reports that eliminating or decreasing LTP
might underlie changes in synaptic efficiency. does not affect memory (Meiri, Sun, Segal, &
Much attention has been focused on long- Alkon, 1998), most research suggests that
term potentiation (LTP), which may be a met- they are related.
abolic mechanism for memory formation Although it is becoming increasingly clear
(Chen & Tonegawa, 1997; Malenka & Nicoll, that LTP is likely involved in memory forma-
1999). In LTP, high-frequency activation of tion, there are a number of aspects of the
glutamate receptors results in a long-lasting physiological basis of memory that still must
strengthening of the synaptic connection such be understood. LTP is most frequently studied
that for intervals of up to hours the synapse in the hippocampus, a structure thought to be
responds more readily and efficiently to acti- involved in the formation of memory traces
vation. In LTP, non-N-methyl-D-aspartate (Chen & Tonegawa, 1997; Malenka & Nicoll,
(NMDA) glutamate receptors are activated, 1999). It has yet to be determined, however,
depolarizing the postsynaptic membrane and how memory traces are stored in long-term
allowing for activation of NMDA glutamate memory and what role, if any, synaptic plas-
receptors. Most likely, both postsynaptic and ticity plays in the storage process. There is
presynaptic mechanisms are involved in this some suggestion that, in addition to memory,
process. Removal of the magnesium block LTP may be involved in activity-dependent
that typically prevents activation of the formation of neural circuitry, certainly in sen-
NMDA receptor allows calcium to enter the sory systems (Chen & Tonegawa, 1997;
postsynaptic cell, beginning a cascade of sec- Katz & Shatz, 1996). One hypothesis yet to
ond-messenger events that ultimately leads to be tested is that experience-dependent mecha-
long-lasting changes in synaptic strength. In nisms such as LTP might also be involved in
addition to the metabolic changes that are in- building associative networks that form the
volved in LTP, structural changes have been basis for memories as well.
identified and such changes may also contrib- In summary, ample scientific evidence in-
Role of experience 699

dicates that experiences can influence the for- development, such as PKU and autism, have
mation, strength, and efficiency of synapses. shown that optimal long-term outcome in
Research is revealing the mechanisms in- terms of IQ and adaptive behavior critically
volved in experience-dependent changes in depends on intervention during the early years
synaptic efficiency. However, it is unknown of life. For PKU, intervention must begin
to what extent the early years of life, because when the infant is less than 21 days old (Wap-
of the tremendous changes in the sheer num- pner et al., 1999). It has been demonstrated
ber of synapses that take place during this that children with autism who receive inten-
time, represent a particularly sensitive period sive interventions by 2–3 years of age fare
for selecting and establishing preferentially substantially better than those who receive in-
active patterns of neural networks that are less tervention after this age (see Dawson & Os-
susceptible to change later in life. terling, 1997, for review of early intervention
studies in autism). Moreover, recent research
has shown that relatively common variations
Sensitive Periods and Long-Term
in early experience related to stress and mal-
Effect of Early Experience
adaptive parenting can have long-term effects
This brings us to the second and third neuros- on development. In the next sections, we will
cientific strands that, according to Bruer, form review in detail what is known about the in-
the “myth of the first three years.” These per- fluences of early stress and maternal depres-
tain to the related notions that (a) the early sion on development. It will be concluded that
years of development represent a sensitive pe- early stress and maternal depression represent
riod for brain and behavioral development and relatively common deviations in early experi-
(b) early experiences can have long-term in- ence that appear to negatively influence both
fluences on brain and behavioral develop- brain and behavioral development.
ment. To begin, it is known that there are sen-
sitive periods in brain development (Knudsen,
Effects of early stress
1999); that is, there are periods in develop-
ment during which specific types of experi- Numerous studies, most involving animals
ence are needed for the brain to develop but some in humans, have documented the
normally. The term “experience-expectant” de- negative effects of early exposure to stress
velopment has been used to refer to develop- and social deprivation on both brain and be-
mental processes that involve a readiness of the havioral development, and on long-term out-
brain to receive specific types of information come. Prenatal stress, for example, has been
from the environment (Greenough et al., 1987). shown to have numerous and varied effects
This readiness occurs during sensitive periods on the subsequent development of offspring.
in which specific types of information are reli- The function of the hypothalamic–pituitary–
ably present for most members of the species. adrenal (HPA) axis, the major hormonal stress
This information can be visual, social, affec- response system, is affected in the offspring
tive, and cognitive. It has been argued that the of rats exposed to high levels of stress hor-
overproliferation of synapses during early post- mones either by injection or by exposure to
natal life reflects the brain’s readiness to re- stress-inducing events (e.g., handling, re-
ceive expected information during this period, straint, light, heat, cold). Offspring of preg-
with the subset of synapses being selectively nant rats injected with adrenocorticotropin
retained depending on experience (Black, hormone (ACTH) showed increased basal
Jones, Nelson, & Greenough, 1998). levels of corticosterone and decreased levels
Studies have clearly demonstrated sensi- in response to stress (Fameli, Kitraki, & Styli-
tive periods for the development of language anopoulou, 1994). In these offspring, the HPA
and visual systems (Levay, Wiesel, & Hubel, system was hyperaroused during baseline
1980; Newport, 1990). Also, studies of chil- conditions and was overwhelmed and unable
dren at risk for cognitive and social impair- to respond during stressful conditions. This
ment due to genetic disorders that affect brain result suggests that the systems that typically
700 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

regulate the activity of the HPA system do not prenatal stress on regulation of the HPA axis
function normally in cases of prenatal stress. and on brain development seem to be stronger
The HPA axis is regulated by a complex sys- for females than for males (e.g., Fride &
tem involving Type I glucocorticoid receptors Weinstock, 1989; Kinsley, Mann, & Bridges,
in the hippocampus and Type II receptors 1989). The maternal behavior of rats that were
throughout the brain (Weinstock, 1997). Dis- themselves stressed prenatally clearly differs
regulation of this feedback system can lead to from that of nonprenatally stressed dams
increased response to stressors, as well as an (Weinstock, Fride, & Hertzberg, 1988). This
inability to adapt to ongoing stress (Wein- raises the interesting possibility of multigen-
stock, 1997). erational effects of prenatal stress, an issue
In nonhuman primates, prenatal stress is that has yet to be well examined in the litera-
associated with increased basal levels of ture. In summary, there is evidence to suggest
ACTH in plasma, as well as increased ACTH that prenatal stress results in long-term
levels in response to stress situations of vary- changes in both brain and behavior in ani-
ing degree in the offspring (Clarke, Wittwer, mals.
Abbott, & Schneider, 1994). Behavioral ef- In comparison to the animal literature, less
fects have also been reported in infant mon- is known about the effects of prenatal stress
keys born to mothers that were stressed dur- in humans. The majority of research in the
ing pregnancy. Monkeys that were exposed to area is retrospective, such as studies in which
repeated unpredictable noise in a darkened parents respond to questionnaires about their
room gave birth to infants that exhibited in- pregnancy history (Lobel, 1994). Children
creased behaviors associated with disturbance whose mothers’ were exposed to a variety of
(clinging to another monkey, clinging to an stressors during pregnancy were found to
artificial surrogate monkey, or self-directed have less optimal outcomes, including emo-
motor activity) and decreased motor and ex- tion regulation problems and motor delays
ploratory behaviors (locomotion, play, explo- (Meier, 1985; Stott, 1973). The prospective
ration of the environment, and climbing) com- studies that have been done have primarily fo-
pared to infant monkeys born to nonstressed cused on very basic indicators of health and
mothers (Schneider, 1992a). Changes in cog- brain development such as birth weight, head
nitive ability have also been noted in prena- size, and risk for prematurity. Although these
tally stressed rats and monkeys. In rats, per- are likely important factors in the future cog-
formance on a delayed alteration task was nitive and social development of the child,
impaired in the offspring of females exposed there is little direct evidence from studies in-
to the presence of a cat during the gestational vestigating outcomes in children exposed to
period (Lordi, Protais, Mellier, & Caston, stress in prenatal or early life. Indirect links
1997). In prenatally stressed rhesus monkeys, between head size and HPA responsiveness
the development of object permanence was (Ramsay & Lewis, 1995) suggest that prenatal
delayed compared to controls (Schneider, stress may be related to later HPA responsive-
1992b). ness (Glover, 1997), but the relation between
The mechanism responsible for these be- stress and head size is not made directly in
havioral changes may lie in brain develop- the Ramsay and Lewis study. Thus, a relation
ment changes that occur as a result of prenatal between stress response and prenatal stress in
stress. In rats exposed to prenatal stress, mild the human is hypothetical at best: infants pre-
stressors resulted in an increase in acetylcho- natally exposed to stress have small heads,
line in the hippocampus (Day, Koehl, De- and infants with small heads have abnormal
roche, Le Moal, & Maccari, 1998). In addi- stress responses. Direct measures of the activ-
tion, decreases in levels of dopamine and ity of the HPA axis are required to determine
increases in levels of serotonin were discov- precisely the effects on the system of prenatal
ered in the brains of adult rats exposed to stress in the developing human before it can
stress hormones prenatally (Fameli et al., concluded that results are truly parallel to
1994). those found in nonhuman primates and rats.
Interestingly, in many studies the effects of Studies of animals and humans also have
Role of experience 701

demonstrated that early social deprivation and tions as well as changes in CRF neural sys-
exposure to high levels of stress during the tems. Similarly, Plotsky and Meaney (1993)
early postnatal period are related to poor so- found that maternal separation during the first
cial outcomes and alterations in psychobio- 2 weeks of life resulted in elevated stress re-
logical functioning. Exposure to stress during sponses in adult rats.
the postnatal period is associated with in- Studies of nonhuman primates have dem-
creased levels of glucocorticoids, and gluco- onstrated similar long-term consequences of
corticoids have been shown to have harmful early life stress (Capitanio, Rasmussen, Sny-
effects on the developing brain. Evidence now der, Laudenslager, & Reite, 1986; Coplan,
indicates that maternal behavior is important Andrews, Rosenblum, Owens, Friedman,
for regulation of the stress response. In rats, Gorman, & Nemeroff, 1996). Coplan et al.
maternal separation stimulates HPA axis (1996) exposed bonnet macaques to either
activity and leads to elevated corticosterone predictable or unpredictable rearing condi-
levels, as well as changes in, corticotropin re- tions by varying the amount and predictability
leasing factor (CRF) concentrations and re- of foraging that mothers needed to do to ac-
ceptor number (Kuhn, Pauk, & Schanberg, quire food. Years later, compared to primates
1990; Kuhn & Schanberg, 1998; Pihoker, exposed to predictable high foraging or pre-
Owens, Kuhn, Schanberg, & Nemeroff, dictable low foraging conditions, primates
1993). Maternal behaviors, such as feeding exposed to variable foraging conditions ex-
and stroking, can weaken this adrenocortical hibited elevated cerebrospinal fluid concentra-
response to maternal separation. Suchecki, tions of CRF. Capitanio et al. (1986) reported
Rosenfeld, and Levine (1993) have shown that monkeys who had experienced maternal
that, in maternally deprived rat pups, feeding separations as infants displayed more dis-
inhibits corticosterone secretion, while strok- turbed behavior as adults.
ing suppresses ACTH secretion. Moreover, What is known about the influence of early
the attenuating effects of tactile stimulation postnatal stress on human development? Re-
appear to be specific to maternal stimulation searchers have investigated how the mother–
(Stanton & Levine, 1990). infant relationship affects cortisol levels by
Similar research with nonhuman primates exploring the relationship between attachment
has shown that plasma cortisol increases fol- security and cortisol reactivity. Nachmias,
lowing maternal separation as a function of its Gunnar, Mangelsdorf, Parritz, and Buss
duration (Levine & Wiener, 1988). In nonhu- (1996) reported that a secure attachment rela-
man primates, the presence of a familiar so- tionship buffers the cortisol responses of be-
cial group can serve to reduce the endocrine haviorally inhibited 18-month-olds to a novel
effects of maternal separation. Monkeys who situation. Only the inhibited toddlers who
were separated from their mothers and then were also insecurely attached to their mothers
isolated displayed a more profound increase exhibited significant elevations in salivary
in plasma cortisol levels and a slower return cortisol. Gunnar, Brodersen, Nachmias, Buss,
to baseline. It has been hypothesized that the and Rigatuso (1996) found that attachment se-
lower stress response in the presence of a fa- curity moderates the cortisol response of 15-
miliar social group reflects the continued month-olds to inoculation distress. Fearful
availability of a stable, predictable social en- toddlers who were also insecurely attached
vironment (Levine & Wiener, 1988). exhibited elevated cortisol levels. These data
Importantly, animal studies involving both suggest that, especially for temperamentally
rodents and nonhuman primates indicate that inhibited or fearful children, sensitive, respon-
the effects of maternal separation can persist sive caretaking may be an important factor in
into adulthood (Anisman, Zaharia, Meaney, & the modulation of the HPA axis.
Merali, 1998; Ladd, Owens, & Nemeroff, One of the most profound, long-term natu-
1996; Plotsky & Meaney, 1993). Ladd et al. ralistic studies of early deprivation has oc-
(1996) found that adult rats who had been ma- curred with Romanian orphanage children.
ternally deprived showed increased baseline These children experienced significant depri-
and stress-induced plasma ACTH concentra- vation until their adoption into homes in Eu-
702 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

rope, Canada, and the United States. About Lynch, & Fielding, 1993; Grunebaum,
6 years after adoption, Gunnar and Chisholm Cohler, Kaufman, & Gallant, 1978; Hammen,
(1999) examined the effects of early institu- Burge, Burney, & Adrian, 1990; Orvaschel,
tional rearing on cortisol levels and attach- Welsh–Allis, & Weijai, 1988; Redding, Har-
ment quality. They found that children who mon, & Morgan, 1990). By early childhood,
had experienced 8 or more months of institu- children of depressed mothers have a 29%
tional rearing had significantly higher salivary chance of developing an emotional or behav-
cortisol compared to sex, age, and socioeco- ioral disorder, compared to an 8% chance for
nomic status matched controls and compared children of medically ill parents (Hammen et
to children who had experienced 4 or fewer al., 1990). Later, we will review evidence that
months of institutional life. Also, there was a maternal depression is associated not only
significant positive correlation between eve- with risk for maladaptive behavior but also
ning cortisol levels and time in institution, with risk for alterations in brain activity.
suggesting that the longer exposure to depri- The behavioral disturbances associated
vation is related to greater effects on the HPA with maternal depression are evident even in
axis. Moreover, securely attached institution- young infants. Infants of depressed mothers
alized children were more likely to show nor- tend to be withdrawn and less active and to
mal cortisol levels. display lower levels of positive affect (Field,
1986, 1992; Field, Healy, Goldstein, Perry,
Bendall, Schanberg, Zimmerman, & Kuhn,
Effects of maternal depression
1988; Field, Healy, Goldstein, & Guthertz,
Bruer argues that examples involving such 1990). They also have difficulty sustaining
unusual and severe deprivation as occurs in their attention and exhibit poor mastery moti-
the studies of children in orphanages cited vation (Radke–Yarrow, Cummings, Kuczyn-
above do not really bear upon the concerns ski, & Chapman, 1985; Zahn–Waxler, Cum-
that typical parents might have about the ap- mings, McKnew, & Radke–Yarrow, 1984).
propriate kinds of emotional stimulation and Cognitive and language delays have been ob-
responsiveness they should provide their chil- served by 1 year of age (Radke–Yarrow et al.,
dren. But, in fact, even less extreme variations 1985; Zahn–Waxler et al., 1984).
in early parenting related to the parent’s own Thus, it appears that maternal depression
emotional well-being appear to have signifi- may have adverse consequences on a young
cant long-term influence on children’s emo- infant’s early development and increase the
tional development, and possibly on their risk for affective or behavioral disorder in
brain development as well. Evidence for this childhood (Hammen et al., 1990). The biolog-
comes from studies of the effects of maternal ical and social mechanisms mediating these
depression, which is a fairly common risk fac- effects are not yet understood. One proposed
tor for children, even for those children who mechanism involves the influence of the
grow up in otherwise advantaged environ- mother’s depression on her ability to interact
ments. Children of mothers experiencing af- with her infant in a way that facilitates emo-
fective disorders, including depression and bi- tional and cognitive development. Numerous
polar disorder, during the early postnatal studies have shown that a mother experienc-
years are at risk for developing problems in ing depression is less likely to interact in an
self-regulation, peer relationships, and sleep adaptive way with her young infant. De-
regulation, and for having behavioral prob- pressed mothers often find it difficult to pro-
lems, academic difficulties, and affective dis- vide contingent responses and optimal levels
orders (Alpern & Lyons–Ruth, 1993; Coghill, of stimulation, and tend to show less positive
Caplan, Alexandra, Robson, & Kumar, 1986; and more negative affect when interacting
Denham, Zahn–Waxler, Cummings, & Ian- with their infants (Cohn, Matias, Tronick,
notti, 1991; Downey & Coyne, 1990; Erick- Connell, & Lyons–Ruth, 1986; Cohn & Tron-
son, Sroufe, & Egeland, 1985; Ghodsian, Zaj- ick, 1989; Field et al., 1988, 1990; Field,
icek, & Wolkind, 1984; Goodman, Brogan, Sandberg, Garcia, Vega–Lahr, Goldstein, &
Role of experience 703

Guy, 1985). During mother–infant interac- emotion and attention regulation and that poor
tions, there is less frequent sharing of positive regulatory skills contribute to several different
emotional states and more frequent matching types of psychopathology (Dawson, 1994).
of negative emotional states (Field et al., Furthermore, there is increasing evidence that
1990). parents play a central role in facilitating the
When the child reaches toddlerhood, it has development of self-regulatory skills in young
been found that toddlers of depressed mothers infants and children (Cicchetti, Ganiban, &
are less likely to maintain interactions and Barnett, 1991; Malatesta & Haviland, 1982;
display less coordinated interactions. Toddlers Tronick & Gianino, 1986; Tronick, 1989). In
of depressed mothers have also been found to the case of maternal depression, it is likely
display fewer positive affiliative behaviors to- that the inability to provide adequate positive
ward their mothers and higher levels of hos- emotion and regulatory input affects not only
tile and aggressive behavior (Dawson, Frey, the infant’s behavioral regulation abilities but
Self, Panagiotides, Hessl, Yamada, & Rinaldi, also the brain systems that underlie these abil-
1999). Depressed mothers also are less likely ities (Dawson, 1994).
to repair interrupted interactions (Jameson, Although the studies described above have
Gelfand, Kulcsar, & Teti, 1997). When inter- focused on the role of the depressed mother’s
acting with their preschool-aged children, de- behavior, other mechanisms mediating the ef-
pressed mothers have been found to use a less fects of maternal depression on children must
positive tone, display more negative affect, be considered. Research has shown, for exam-
express more anxiety, and provide fewer di- ple, that neonates born to depressed mothers
rect commands (Cox, Puckering, Pound, & tend to be less active, less socially responsive,
Mills, 1987; Kochanska, Kuczynski, & Ma- and fussier than those born to nondepressed
guire, 1989; Radke–Yarrow, Nottelmann, mothers (Field et al., 1985; Whiffen & Gott-
Belmont, & Welsh, 1993). lib, 1989; Zuckerman, Als, Bauchner, Par-
To explain the effect of maternal depres- ker, & Cabral, 1990). Prenatal exposure to
sion on children’s behavior, some investiga- maternal depression may be associated with a
tors have suggested that children may be imi- less than optimal intrauterine environment.
tating the mother’s depressed behavior. The contribution of genetic factors that in-
Others have hypothesized that children of de- crease risk for depression is another important
pressed mothers may fail to develop adequate factor, although it is unlikely that genetic fac-
means for regulating arousal and negative af- tors alone can account for the high risk for
fect because of the mother’s difficulty in pro- emotional and behavioral disturbances found
viding appropriate levels of stimulation, re- in children of depressed mothers (Moldin,
sponsiveness, and arousal modulation. Later, Reich, & Rice, 1991; Todd, Neuman, Geller,
in toddlerhood, these mothers may have diffi- Fox, & Hickok, 1993).
culty helping the young child to develop ade- There is some evidence that the first few
quate self-regulatory strategies, leading to years of life may represent a time of increased
maladaptive affective, attentional, social, and vulnerability for enduring effects of maternal
cognitive behavior. Early in the toddler years, depression (Alpern & Lyons–Ruth, 1993).
a major achievement is the ability to inhibit Wolkind, Zajicek–Coleman, and Ghodsian
inappropriate responses and the use of self- (1980) reported that maternal depression oc-
generated mental images to guide appropriate curring when the infants were 14 months old
behavior. For example, Piaget’s A not B task, was predictive of later behavioral problems
believed to be an early prefrontal task that re- during the preschool years, even when the de-
quires both inhibition of a prepotent response pression was absent by the time the children
and working memory to guide behavior, is were in preschool. Coghill and colleagues
achieved some time in the 2nd year of life (1986) found that maternal depression during
(Diamond & Goldman–Rakic, 1989). It has the infant’s 1st year of life was predictive of
been hypothesized that inhibitory skills medi- lower cognitive ability at 4 years of age, re-
ated by the frontal lobe play a central role in gardless of mother’s depression status when
704 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

the child was 4 years old. Similarly, Alpern tively structured conditions. Thus, it does not
and Lyons–Ruth (1993) found that infants of appear that the differences in brain activity
mothers who were depressed when their in- were simply the result of differences in the
fants were 18–24 months but not depressed infants’ patterns of emotional expression dur-
when their children were 5 years were never- ing EEG recording.
theless at increased risk for anxiety symptoms Dawson and her coworkers also examined
at age 5 years. the relation between individual differences in
As stated earlier, children of depressed frontal brain activity and infant behavior,
mothers are not only a higher risk for cogni- which were observed in naturalistic situations
tive, social, and affective disturbances; they involving mother–infant play outside the psy-
also exhibit changes in brain activity early in chophysiology laboratory. Based on previous
life. Dawson and colleagues have demon- theoretical and experimental work linking the
strated that 13- to 15-month-old infants of de- left frontal region with approach behaviors
pressed mothers show reduced electrical brain and the right frontal region with withdrawal
activity over the left frontal scalp region, as behaviors (Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Sen-
compared to infants of nondepressed mothers ulis, & Freisen, 1990; Fox, 1991), it was pre-
(Dawson, Grofer–Klinger, Panagiotides, Hill, dicted that reduced left frontal activity would
& Spieker, 1992; Dawson, Frey, Panagiotides, be related to lower frequencies of positive ap-
Osterling, & Hessl, 1997). This finding has proach behaviors in the infants. In addition,
been demonstrated in two independent sam- based on a previous finding from our labora-
ples, one involving teenaged mothers with tory that infants of depressed mothers also
multiple risk factors and another involving show increased generalized frontal activation
middle-income, primarily married, adult during the expression of negative emotions
mothers who were carefully screened for a va- (Dawson, Panagiotides, Grofer–Klinger, &
riety of other risk factors that could poten- Spieker, 1997), it was predicted that increased
tially influence infant behavior and brain ac- levels of generalized frontal activation would
tivity (e.g., other major psychiatric disorders, be related to increased levels of intense nega-
substance abuse, contact with Child Protective tive affect.
Services, and prenatal and birth difficulties). In this study (Dawson, Frey, Self, et al.,
In the latter longitudinal study involving 159 1999), it was found that infants of depressed
mothers and their infants, Dawson and col- mothers were less affectionate and less likely
leagues (1997) found that infants’ atypical to touch their mothers during free play. They
frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) patterns also had more difficulty quietly occupying
were linearly related to the severity of mater- themselves while their mothers filled out a
nal depression. Interestingly, infant frontal questionnaire, and thus appeared to have more
EEG patterns were not found to be related to difficulty with self-regulation. Reduced left
mothers’ levels of anxiety or hostility, even frontal activity was correlated with lower lev-
though depressed mothers reported signifi- els of positive approach behaviors (affection
cantly higher levels of these symptoms (Daw- toward mother during free play) and higher
son et al., 1997). It was also found that the frequencies of negative bids for attention
pattern of atypical frontal electrical brain ac- (grabbing mother’s clipboard or pen while she
tivity in infants of depressed mothers general- attempted to fill out questionnaire). In addi-
ized to a range of situations in which we re- tion, increased generalized frontal brain activ-
corded brain activity, including a “nonsocial” ity was associated with higher levels of nega-
baseline condition and a standardized playful tive affect, hostility, tantrums, and aggression.
condition carried out with a familiar experi- Thus, the behaviors that were found to be as-
menter (Dawson, Frey, Panagiotides, Ya- sociated with frontal brain activity appear to
mada, Hessl, & Osterling, 1999). Importantly, reflect the dimensions of positive approach
infants of depressed and nondepressed moth- and affiliative behaviors and regulation of
ers were not found to differ in terms of their negative emotions. These findings suggest
affective behavior during these brief, rela- that maternal depression is associated with al-
Role of experience 705

terations in infant brain activity and that atyp- evaluate assumptions at the intersection of
ical frontal brain activity is associated with science, policy, and clinical practice. First, the
lower levels of approach behaviors and in- current state of scientific findings on the issue
creased intensity of negative affect or diffi- must be understood. Second, reasonable
culty in regulating negative affect. hypotheses can be generated as logical exten-
Dawson (1999) also found that mothers ex- sions of these scientific findings. Finally, un-
periencing depression displayed higher levels warranted assertions need to be assessed.
of insensitive behaviors when interacting with Such an analysis can then lead to well-justi-
their infants. Insensitive behaviors broadly in- fied policy recommendations. Let us consider
cluded behaviors that reflected a difficulty in such an analysis and its implications for social
sensitively following the cues of the infant, policy recommendations.
including behaviors such as poking, tickling, Regarding the current state of the science,
responding to infant’s bid for attention by critics agree that from the perspective of es-
withdrawing from or rejecting solicited physi- tablished scientific knowledge our under-
cal contact, and unsolicited holding. Further- standing of the neurobiology of the effects of
more, path analyses indicated that mother’s early experience on developmental outcome is
insensitivity with her infant mediated the rela- still in its infancy (Bruer, 1999; Nelson,
tion between maternal depression and infant 1999a, 1999b; Shonkoff, in press). Neverthe-
frontal EEG activity (Dawson, 1999). These less, a number of reasonable assertions and
results provide support for the hypothesis that hypotheses can be generated from what is
the behavior of a depressed mother toward her known based on both brain and behavioral
infant—in particular, the degree of maternal science. First, it is clear that the prenatal and
insensitivity—can influence patterns of infant early postnatal years are a time during which
frontal brain activity. Specifically, high levels remarkable growth in multiple domains of
of maternal insensitivity appear to be associ- functioning (e.g., social, linguistic, cognitive,
ated with lower levels of infant left frontal motor) occurs in terms of both brain and be-
electrical brain activity. These results, taken havioral development. Second, ample evi-
together with previous studies on the effects dence exists indicating that these years likely
of maternal depression on infant behavior and represent a sensitive period with respect to the
brain activity, suggest that relatively common effects of nutrition, exposure to toxins, and
variations in early parenting that are related to possibly stress. Third, this period also seems
a parent’s emotional well-being are associated to represent a sensitive period with respect to
with alterations in infant brain activity and be- the long-term beneficial effects of early inter-
havior. Other studies have shown that mater- vention on brain and behavioral development
nal depression early in life is a risk factor for for some genetically based disorders, such as
later psychopathology in childhood (Good- PKU and autism. Fourth, evidence suggests
man & Gotlib, 1999). The question of that parental mental health during the first
whether altered brain activity early in life years of life is a significant factor with respect
constitutes a risk factor for later psychopath- to early brain activity and behavior and long-
ology is as yet unanswered and is currently term behavioral outcome. This paper has fo-
being addressed in a longitudinal study being cused on early stress and maternal depression,
conducted by Dawson and colleagues. but similar evidence exists with respect to the
influence of other forms of maladaptive par-
enting, such as maltreatment (DeBellis, Kes-
Implications for Social Policy and
havan, Clark, Casey, Giedd, Boring, Frus-
Future Research
taci, & Ryan, 1999; Pollak, Cicchetti,
What can we say about the effects of early Klorman, & Brumaghim, 1997).
experience on brain development and later Understanding the factors that determine
outcome and their implications for social pol- social and emotional outcome in humans is a
icy? Shonkoff (in press) outlined three levels complex task that requires integrating infor-
of analysis that should be applied to critically mation about the genetic, physical, and social
706 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

environment. Outcome is variable because of 1999). As Cicchetti and Cannon (1999) point
the nondeterministic, probabilistic nature of out, our hopes of finding new avenues for in-
development. As Cicchetti and Cannon (1999) tervention rest on discovery of the mecha-
state, “Epigenesis is viewed as probabilistic nisms that constrain plasticity and change.
rather than predetermined or preformational, Early development is a period that is com-
with the bidirectional and transactional nature prised of many fundamental “experience ex-
of genetic, neural, behavioral, and environ- pectant” processes. Unfortunately, there are
mental influence over the life course captur- many conditions, such as autism, in which ge-
ing the essence of probabilistic epigensis” (p. netic or acquired brain abnormalities preclude
377). Although early experiences are impor- having a normal experience of an otherwise
tant for later outcome, this does not imply that normal environment. As described by Black
the effects of any one type of early experience et al. (1998), neuropathology can result in
are necessarily causal or permanent. Systems
theory principles such as the notion that mul- . . . a developmental “groove” that will carry devel-
tiple pathways can converge on a common opment forward. Therapeutic manipulations may
outcome (equifinality) and vice versa (multifi- perturb this trajectory initially, but development
nality) are well accepted by developmental will fall back into the existing track if the canaliza-
psychopathologists and must be incorporated tion is sufficiently entrenched. However, if the per-
turbation is large enough, then the trajectory can
into any theory about the effects of early ex-
be shifted out of one equilibrium into another, and
perience on later outcome (Cicchetti & Ro-
from there brain development will proceed down a
gosch, 1996). In developmental psychopathol- different pathway. As development proceeds, canal-
ogy, the term “pathway” is used to refer to ization generally deepens—developmental trajec-
the cumulative influence of an individual’s tories progressively become more difficult to redi-
specific genetic makeup and experiences that rect by experience. This perspective supports the
together operate in a complex and probabilis- common therapeutic strategies that early interven-
tic fashion to influence outcome (Gottlieb, tion is better than late and that more intervention
1991, 1992). An experience involving a de- may work better than less. (pp. 46–47)
pressed mother most likely is moderated by a
host of other factors, including genetic fac- The substantial effect of early intervention
tors, other significant relationships, the child’s has been dramatically demonstrated in the
intelligence and schooling, peers, and so on. case of autism. If very intensive behavioral
Each individual has a unique combination of intervention is commenced by 2 years of age,
factors influencing outcome. Thus, in most a substantial number of children with autism
cases it is not reasonable to consider one type show a remarkable improvement in this con-
of experience as having a permanent and spe- dition (see Dawson & Osterling, 1997).
cific effect on later outcome. On the other Furthermore, there are many common con-
hand, the term “pathway” implies that, at any ditions, such as exposure to stress and mater-
point in an individual’s development, that in- nal depression, in which the variation in early,
dividual has, indeed, developed along a spe- expected environment falls outside the norm,
cific pathway involving a complex set of risk and these conditions do appear to have an in-
and protective factors, and to alter this trajec- fluence on long-term outcome. Because we
tory one must intervene through planned ther- have the knowledge of many early factors that
apeutic activities that focus on changing be- promote optimal development, it is imperative
havioral and neural patterns of conditioning, that we translate that knowledge into social
through insight and self-will, or through ser- policy so that society can benefit from the sci-
endipitous advantageous experiences (a posi- entific knowledge that has been gained. In the
tive role model or mentor). However it oc- case of our understanding of the importance
curs, real energy is required; it rarely, if ever, of early experience on brain and behavioral
happens spontaneously or easily. Fortunately, development, evidence from the neurosci-
inherent plasticity appears to be a characteris- ences and the behavioral sciences offers a
tic of at least some brain structures (Johnson, firm base for identifying optimal conditions
Role of experience 707

for children’s development. Public under- tervention programs for pregnant women
standing of the importance of such conditions suffering from alcohol or drug addiction
is a critical first step that must be followed by are needed.
the design and implementation of policy and
intervention efforts. There is much more we 3. Similarly, a brief assessment of mother’s
do as a society to provide an optimal environ- mental health should be included as part of
ment in the first years of life and beyond. Pol- the regular well-baby checkups, along with
icy should focus on primary prevention of ad- educational materials related to infant nutri-
verse outcomes by reducing known risk tion and behavioral development. Contin-
factors related to early environment and expe- ued efforts to improve identification of psy-
rience so that such factors can be thwarted be- chopathology in infants during well-baby
fore they have any influence on outcome, as checkups should also be supported.
well as on secondary prevention by identify- 4. Increased efforts to train health care work-
ing environmental factors that are modifiable ers in how to detect and respond when par-
moderators or mediators of outcome so that ents are experiencing mental health prob-
we can mitigate the negative influence of such lems are needed.
risk factors on children’s development and
outcome. Specifically, we believe that the fol- 5. Communities struggling with poverty and
lowing policy recommendations are well jus- the host of associated problems that are as-
tified based on the current state of knowledge sociated with poverty (stress, violence,
regarding the effects of early experience on poor nutrition, parental psychopathology,
brain and behavioral development. impoverished educational opportunities)
should be a direct and substantial focus of
programs related to optimal prenatal and
1. In general, although prevention and early early postnatal environments because chil-
intervention efforts should not exclusively dren growing up in such communities are
focus on the earliest years of development, most likely to be exposed to multiple risk
it is clear from the research discussed factors that are associated with poor long-
above that such efforts should begin as term outcome.
early as possible. By directing such efforts
toward promoting optimal prenatal and in- 6. Because the prenatal and early postnatal
fant–toddler development, the long-term years represent a sensitive period with re-
negative consequences of factors that have spect to the long-term beneficial effects of
their greatest influences during early devel- early intervention on brain and behavioral
opment and which set the stage for future development for some genetically based
development can be minimized or avoided disorders, such as autism, increased efforts
entirely. at early identification of such disorders is
needed. Greater public awareness and edu-
2. Prevention efforts that promote healthy pre- cation of health care providers with respect
natal development should focus on increas- how to detect developmental disorders dur-
ing public awareness, outreach efforts, and ing infancy and toddlerhood and how to fa-
intervention programs related to improving cilitate access to appropriate interventions
prenatal nutrition, reducing fetal exposure are needed.
to toxins (alcohol, drugs), and minimizing
maternal stress and psychopathology dur- 7. Efforts aimed at early identification, pre-
ing pregnancy. In light of findings that pre- vention, and remediation of developmental
natal depression and stress can influence disorders should be enhanced. To provide
fetal and neonatal behavior, prenatal one example, autism has become recog-
screening should include a brief assessment nized as a relatively prevalent (1 in 500–
of maternal mental health so that appro- 1000 persons) developmental disorder that
priate medical or psychological interven- can and should be detected during infancy
tions can begin at this time. Moreover, in- in most cases (Osterling & Dawson, 1994,
708 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

1999). Unfortunately, autism is not typi- tribution of parenting factors to the devel-
cally diagnosed in children until around the opment of self-regulatory brain mecha-
age of 3–4 years (Siegel, Pliner, Eschler, & nisms that have been hypothesized to relate
Elliot, 1988). Pediatricians and other health to affective and attentional disorders is still
care professionals need to be aware of how poorly understood.
to assess early symptoms of autism in in-
fants and toddlers so that appropriate 3. It needs to be determined whether psycho-
screening and referral can occur. Moreover, biological measures such as neuropsycho-
given the dramatic impact of early interven- logical performance, cortisol levels, auto-
tion in autism, funding for early behavioral nomic responses, and brain electrical
intervention programs for infants and tod- activity will be useful in identifying chil-
dlers at risk for autism is clearly justified dren at risk for psychopathology.
and cost effective. 4. Continued research on the neurobiological
8. In general, financial, emotional, and practi- bases of different types of child psycho-
cal support needs to be provided to families pathology will be important for understand-
who are struggling to access appropriate in- ing the etiology, nature, and treatment of
tervention for their children who have been such disorders. Such research will lay the
identified as having a developmental disor- foundation for identifying phenotypic
der, chronic illness, or mental health prob- markers for childhood disorders, which will
lem. Denial of such support to families by be essential for early identification, design-
insurance companies and government agen- ing interventions, and discovering genetic
cies is shortsighted on the part of these mechanisms.
health care systems in light of what is
known about the critical impact of early in- 5. There is also a need to develop neurobio-
tervention on long-term outcome. logical frameworks for interventions for
children at risk for psychopathology be-
Future research should focus on increasing cause of genetic or environmental factors.
our understanding of the multiple influences For example, the use of neurocognitive and
on children’s development and outcome, and psychophysiological assessments that target
thus will depend upon multidisciplinary, lon- specific brain regions, such as prefrontal
gitudinal approaches that integrate biological functioning, may allow us to design indi-
and environmental factors. Among the impor- vidualized treatment approaches based on
tant issues that need to be addressed in future neurobiological frameworks.
research are the following: 6. Similarly, measures of response to inter-
ventions should include neuropsychological
1. Given that different biological systems de- and psychophysiological indices of brain
velop on different timetables, it needs to be function to determine whether and how
determined whether and how specific envi- specific brain systems may be influenced
ronmental events, such as exposure to by interventions.
stress and maternal depression, selectively
affect the development of specific neurobi- 7. The degree of plasticity in neural systems
ological systems depending on the timing as it relates to risk for child psychopathol-
and duration of the exposure to such events ogy needs to be understood. For example,
and whether and how timing influences risk issues related to timing, duration, and type
for psychopathology. In particular, studies of early interventions that may influence
that address these issues in humans are outcome in children at risk for psychopath-
needed. ology need to be explored.
2. The role of the environment in promoting
optimal brain function in children needs to In conclusion, technological advances in
be better understood. For example, the con- brain research continue to make this an excit-
Role of experience 709

ing time with respect to our ability to under- ing of brain and behavioral development but
stand the neural bases of normal and abnor- rather the translation of that knowledge into
mal development. As science continues to meaningful policy and action. This responsi-
move this effort forward at a remarkably fast bility, shared by the general public, scientists,
pace, it has become apparent that our most practitioners, and policymakers, should be a
difficult challenge will not be the understand- central focus of our efforts.

References
Alpern, L., & Lyons–Ruth, K. (1993). Preschool children mammalian brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience,
at social risk: Chronicity and timing of maternal de- 1997, 157–184.
pressive symptoms and child behavior at school and Cicchetti, D., & Cannon, T. D. (1999). Neurodevelop-
at home. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 371– mental processes in the ontogenesis and epigenesis of
387. psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology,
Anisman, H., Zaharia, M. D., Meaney, M. J., & Merali, 11, 375–393.
Z. (1998). Do early-life events permanently alter be- Cicchetti, D., Ganiban, J., & Barnett, D. (1991). Contri-
havioral and hormonal responses to stressors? Inter- butions from the study of high risk populations to un-
national Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16, derstanding the development of emotion regulation.
149–164. In K. Dodge & J. Garber (Eds.), The development of
Barad, M., Bourtchouladze, R., Winder, D. G., Golan, emotion regulation (pp. 15–48). New York: Cam-
H., & Kandel, E. R. (1998). Rolipram, a type IV-spe- bridge University Press.
cific phosphodiesterase inhibitor, facilitates the estab- Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and
lishment of long-lasting long-term potentiation and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. De-
improves memory. Proceedings of the National Acad- velopmental Psychopathology, 8, 597–600.
emy of Sciences, USA, 95, 15020–15025. Clarke, A. S., Wittwer, D. J., Abbott, D. H., & Schneider,
Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (1994). Brain development M. L. (1994). Long-term effects of prenatal stress on
over the first year of life: Relations between EEG fre- HPA axis activity in juvenile rhesus monkeys. Devel-
quency and coherence and cognitive and affective be- opmental Psychobiology, 27, 257–269.
haviors. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human Coghill, S. R., Caplan, H. L., Alexandra, H., Robson,
behavior and the developing brain (pp. 314–345). K., & Kumar, R. (1986). Impact of maternal postnatal
New York: Guilford Press. depression on cognitive development of young chil-
Black, J. E., Jones, T. A., Nelson, C. A., & Greenough, dren. British Medical Journal, 292, 1165–1167.
W. T. (1998). Neuronal plasticity and the developing Cohn, J. F., Matias, R., Tronick, E. Z., Connell, D., &
brain. In N. E. Alessi, J. T. Coyle, S. I. Harrison, & Lyons–Ruth, D. (1986). Face-to-face interactions of
S. Eith (Eds.), Handbook of child and adolescent psy- depressed mothers and their infants. In E. Z. Tron-
chiatry: Vol 6. Basic psychiatric science and treat- ick & T. Field (Eds.), Maternal depression and infant
ment (pp. 31–53). New York: Wiley. disturbance (pp. 31–45). San Francisco: Jossey–Bass.
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Specificity of in-
Bourgeois, J. P., Jastreboff, P., & Rakic, P. (1989). Syn-
fants’ response to mothers’ affective behavior. Jour-
aptogenesis in the visual cortex of normal and pre-
nal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
term monkeys: Evidence of the intrinsic regulation of
Psychiatry, 28, 242–248.
synaptic overproduction. Proceedings of the National
Comery, T. A., Harris, J. B., Willems, P. J., Oostra,
Academy of Sciences, 86, 4297–4301.
B. A., Irwin, S. A., Weiler, I. J., & Greenough, W. T.
Bourtchouladze, R., Frenguelli, B., Blendy, J., Cioffi, D., (1997). Abnormal dendritic spines in Fragile X
Schutz, G., & Silva, A. J. (1994). Deficient long-term knockout mice: Maturation and pruning deficits. Pro-
memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94,
CAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell, 79, 5401–5404.
59–68. Coplan, J. D., Andrews, M. W., Rosenblum, L. A., Ow-
Bruer, J. T. (1999). The myth of the first three years: A ens, M. J., Friedman, S., Gorman, J. M., & Nemeroff,
new understanding of early brain development and C. B. (1996). Persistent elevations of cerebrospinal
lifelong learning. New York: Free Press. fluid concentrations of corticotropin-releasing factor
Campbell, F. A., & Ramey, C. T. (1994). Effects of early in adult nonhuman primates exposed to early-life
intervention on intellectual and academic achieve- stressors: Implications for the pathophysiology of
ment: A follow-up study of children from low-income mood and anxiety disorders. Proceedings of the Na-
families. Child Development, 65, 684–698. tional Academy of Sciences, 93, 1619–1623.
Capitanio, J. P., Rasmussen, K. L. R., Snyder, D. S., Cox, A. D., Puckering, C., Pound, A., & Mills, M.
Laudenslager, M., & Reite, M. (1986). Long-term fol- (1987). The impact of maternal depression in young
low-up of previously separated pigtail macaques: children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
Group and individual differences in response to novel try, 28, 917–928.
situations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C., Senulis, R., &
try, 27, 531–538. Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach–withdrawal and ce-
Chen, C., & Tonegawa, S. (1997). Molecular genetic rebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain
analysis of synaptic plasticity, activity-dependent physiology I. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
neural development, learning, and memory in the chology, 58, 330–341.
710 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

Dawson, G. (1994). Frontal electroencephalographic cor- logical theory of consciousness. New York: Basic
relates of individual differences in emotional expres- Books.
sion in infants: A brain systems perspective on emo- Erickson, M., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (1985). The
tion. Monographs of the Society for Research In Child relationship between quality of attachment and behav-
Development, 59(2/3), 135–151. ior problems in preschool in a high-risk sample.
Dawson, G. (1999). The effects of maternal depression Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De-
on children’s emotional and psychobiological devel- velopment, 50(1/2), 147–166.
opment. Paper presented at a 1999 National Institutes Fameli, M., Kitraki, E., & Stylianopoulou, F. (1994). Ef-
of Health Conference on Parenting, Bethesda, MD. fects of hyperactivity of the maternal hypothalamic–
Dawson, G., & Fischer, K. W. (Eds.). (1994). Human be- pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis during pregnancy on the
havior and the developing brain. New York: Guilford development of the HPA axis and brain monoamines
Press. of the offspring. International Journal of Develop-
Dawson, G., Frey, K., Panagiotides, H., Osterling, J., & mental Neuroscience, 12, 651–659.
Hessl, D. (1997). Infants of depressed mothers exhibit Field, T. (1986). Models for reactive and chronic depres-
atypical frontal brain activity: A replication and ex- sion in infancy. In E. Z. Tronick & T. Field (Eds.),
tension of previous findings. Journal of Child Psy- Maternal depression and infant disturbance (pp. 47–
chology and Psychiatry, 38, 179–186. 60). San Francisco: Jossey–Bass.
Dawson, G., Frey, K., Panagiotides, H., Yamada, E., Field, T. (1992). Infants of depressed mothers. Develop-
Hessl, D., & Osterling, J. (1999). Infants of depressed ment and Psychopathology, 4, 49–66.
mothers exhibit atypical frontal electrical brain activ- Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990).
ity during interactions with mother and with a famil- Behavior–state matching and synchrony in mother–
iar, nondepressed adult. Child Development, 70, infant interactions of non-depressed versus depressed
1058–1066. dyads. Developmental Psychology, 26, 7–14.
Dawson, G., Frey, K., Self, J., Panagiotides, H., Hessl, Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Bendall, D.,
D., Yamada, E., & Rinaldi, J. (1999). Frontal electri- Schanberg, S., Zimmerman, E., & Kuhn, C. (1988).
cal brain activity in infants of depressed mothers: Re- Infants of depressed mothers show “depressed” be-
lation to variations in infant behavior. Development havior even with non-depressed adults. Child Devel-
and Psychopathology, 11, 589–605. opment, 59, 1569–1579.
Dawson, G., Grofer–Klinger, L., Panagiotides, H., Hill, Field, T., Sandberg, D., Garcia, R., Vega–Lahr, N., Gold-
D., & Spieker, S. (1992). Frontal lobe activity and stein, S., & Guy, L. (1985). Prenatal problems, post-
affective behavior of infants of mothers with depres- partum depression, and early mother–infant interac-
sive symptoms. Child Development, 63, 725–737. tions, Developmental Psychology, 12, 1152–1156.
Dawson, G., & Osterling, J. (1997). Early intervention Fox, N. A. (1991). If it’s not left, it’s right: Electroen-
in autism: Effectiveness and common elements of cephalograph asymmetry and the development of
current approaches. In Guralnick, M. J. (Ed.), The ef- emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 863–872.
fectiveness of early intervention: Second generation Fride, E., & Weinstock, M. (1989). Alterations in behav-
research (pp. 307–326). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. ioral and striatal dopamine asymmetries induced by
Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., Grofer–Klinger, L., & prenatal stress. Pharmacological Biochemical Behav-
Spieker, S. (1997). Infants of depressed and nonde- ior, 32, 425–430.
pressed mothers exhibit differences in frontal brain Ghodsian, M., Zajicek, E., & Wolkind, S. (1984). A lon-
electrical activity during the expression of negative gitudinal study of maternal depression and child be-
emotions. Developmental Psychology, 33, 650–656. havior problems. Journal of Child Psychology and
Day, J. C., Koehl, M., Deroche, V., Le Moal, M., & Mac- Psychiatry, 25, 91–109.
cari, S. (1998). Prenatal stress enhances stress- and Glover, V. (1997). Maternal stress or anxiety in preg-
corticotropin-releasing factor-induced stimulation of nancy and emotional development of the child [Edito-
hippocampal acetylcholine release in adult rats. Jour- rial]. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 105–
nal of Neuroscience, 18, 1886–1892. 106.
DeBellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Clark, D. B., Casey, Goodman, S. H., Brogan, D., Lynch, M. E., & Fielding,
B. J., Giedd, J. N., Boring, A. M., Frustaci, K., & B. (1993). Social and emotional competence in chil-
Ryan, N. D. (1999). A. E. Bennett Research Award. dren of depressed mothers. Child Development, 64,
Developmental traumatology. Part II: Brain develop- 516–531.
ment. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1271–1284. Goodman, S. H., & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). Risk for psycho-
Denham, S. A., Zahn–Waxler, C., Cummings, E. M., & pathology in the children of depressed mothers: A de-
Iannotti, R. J. (1991). Social competence in young velopmental model for understanding mechanisms of
children’s peer relations: Patterns of development and transmission. Psychological Review, 106, 458–490.
change. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavior
22, 29–44. development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27,
Diamond, A., & Goldman–Rakic, P. (1989). Comparison 4–13.
of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget’s A- Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual development and evolu-
not-B task: Evidence for dependence on dorsolateral tion: The genesis of novel behavior. New York: Ox-
prefrontal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 74, ford University Press.
24–40. Greenough, W. T. (1997). We can’t just focus on ages
Downey, G., & Coyne, J. (1990). Children of depressed zero to three. APA Monitor, 28, 19.
parents: An integrative review. Psychological Bulle- Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987).
tin, 108, 50–76. Experience and brain development. Child Develop-
Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory ment, 58, 539–559.
of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books. Groginsky, S., Christian, S., & McConnell, L. (1998).
Edelman, G. M. (1989). The remembered present: A bio- Early childhood initiatives in the states: Translating
Role of experience 711

research into policy. In State Legislative Report (Vol. maternal separation: Mechanisms and mediators. In-
23). Denver, CO: National Conference of State Legis- ternational Journal of Developmental Neuroscience,
latures. 16, 261–270.
Grunebaum, H. U., Cohler, B. J., Kaufman, C., & Gal- Ladd, C. O., Owens, M. J., & Nemeroff, C. B. (1996).
lant, D. H. (1978). Children of depressed and schizo- Persistent changes in corticotropin-releasing factor
phrenic mothers. Child Psychiatry and Human Devel- neuronal systems induced by maternal deprivation.
opment, 8, 219–228. Endocrinology, 137, 1212–1218.
Gunnar, M. R., & Chisholm, K. C. (1999, April). Effects Levay, S., Wiesal, T. N., & Hubel, D. H. (1980). The
of early institutional rearing and attachment quality development of ocular dominancy columns in normal
on salivary cortisol levels in adopted Romanian chil- and visually deprived monkeys. Journal of Compara-
dren. Poster session presented at the biennial meeting tive Neurology, 19, 11–51.
of the Society for Research in Child Development, Levine, S., & Wiener, S. G. (1988). Psychoendocrine as-
Albuquerque, NM. pects of mother–infant relationships in nonhuman pri-
Gunnar, M. R., Broderson, L., Nachmias, M., Buss, K., & mates. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 13, 143–154.
Rigatuso, J. (1996). Stress reactivity and attachment Lobel, M. (1994). Conceptualizations, measurement, and
security. Developmental Psychobiology, 29, 191–204. effects of prenatal maternal stress on birth outcomes.
Hammen, C., Burge, D., Burney, E., & Adrian, C. (1990). Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 17, 225–272.
Longitudinal study of diagnoses in children of women Lordi, B., Protais, P., Mellier, D., & Caston, J. (1997).
with unipolar and bipolar affective disorder. Archives Acute stress in pregnant rats: Effects on growth rate,
of General Psychiatry, 47, 1112–1117. learning, and memory capabilities of the offspring.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neu- Physiology & Behavior, 62, 1087–1092.
ropsychological theory. New York: Wiley. Lu, Y., Jia, Z., Janus, C., Henderson, J., Gerlai, R., Woj-
Huttenlocher, P. R. (1994). Synaptogenesis in human ce- towicz, J. M., & Roder, J. C. (1997). Mice lacking
rebral cortex. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Hu- metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 show impaired
man behavior and the developing brain (pp. 137– learning and reduced CA2 long-term potentiation
152). New York: Guilford. (LTP) but normal CA 3LTP. The Journal of Neuro-
Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional science, 17, 5196–5205.
differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cor- Malatesta, C. Z., & Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning dis-
tex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387, 167– play rules: The socialization of emotion expression in
178. infancy. Child Development, 53, 991–1003.
Jacobs, B., Schall, M., & Scheibel, A. B. (1993). A quan- Malenka, R. C., & Nicoll, R.A. (1999). Long-term poten-
titative dendritic analysis of Wernicke’s area: Gender, tiation—A decade of progress? Science, 285, 1870–
hemispheric, and environmental factors. Journal of 1874.
Comparative Neurology, 237, 97–111. Mayford, M., Bach, M. E., Huang, Y., Wang, L., Haw-
Jacobs, B., & Scheibel, A. B. (1993). A quantitative den- kins, R. D., & Kandel, E. R. (1996). Control of mem-
dritic analysis of Wernicke’s area: Lifespan changes. ory formation through regulated expression of a
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 237, 83–96. CaMK11 Transgene. Science, 274, 1678–1683.
Jameson, P. B., Gelfand, D. M., Kulcsar, E., & Teti, Meier, A. (1985). Child psychiatric sequelae of maternal
D. M. (1997). Mother–toddler interaction patterns as- war stress. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 72, 505–
sociated with maternal depression. Development and 511.
Psychopathology, 9, 537–550. Meiri, N., Sun, M., Segal, Z., & Alkon, D. L. (1998).
Johnson, M. H. (1999). Cortical plasticity in normal and
Memory and long-term potentiation (LTP) dissoci-
abnormal development: Evidence and working
ated: Normal spatial memory despite CA1 LTP elimi-
hypotheses. Development and Psychopathology, 11,
nation with Kv1.4 antisense. Proceedings of the Na-
419–437.
tional Academy of Science, USA, 95, 15037–15042.
Katz, L. D., & Shatz, C. J. (1996). Synaptic activity and
Moldin, S. O., Reich, T., & Rice, J. P. (1991). Current
the construction of cortical circuits. Science, 274,
perspectives on the genetics of unipolar depression.
1133–1138.
Kinsley, C. H., Mann, P. E., & Bridges, R. S. (1989). Behavior Genetics, 21, 211–242.
Alterations in stress-induced prolactin release in adult Morgan, G., & Gibson, K. R. (1991). Nutritional and en-
female and male rats exposed to stress, in utero. Phys- vironmental interactions in brain development. In
iological Behavior, 45, 1073–1076. K. R. Gibson & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Brain matura-
Knudsen, E. I. (1999). Early experience and critical peri- tion and cognitive development (pp. 91–106). New
ods. Developmental Psychology, 26, 398–408. York: Aldine de Bruyter.
Kochanska, G., Kuczynski, L., & Maguire, M. (1989). Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf, S., Parritz,
Impact of diagnosed depression and self-reported R. H., & Buss, K. (1996). Behavioral inhibition and
mood on mothers’ control strategies: A longitudinal stress reactivity: The moderating role of attachment
study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, security. Child Development, 67, 508–522.
493–511. Nelson, C. A. (1999a). Neural plasticity and human de-
Kolb, B., Forgie, M., Gibb, R., Gorney, G., & Rowntree, velopment. Current Directions in Psychological Sci-
S. (1998). Age, experience and the changing brain. ence, 8, 42–45.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 123– Nelson, C. A. (1999b). How important are the first 3
159. years of life? Applied Developmental Science, 3,
Kuhn, C. M., Pauk, J., & Schanberg, S. M. (1990). Endo- 235–238.
crine responses to mother infant separation in devel- Nelson, C. A. (Ed.). (in press). Minnesota Symposia on
oping rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 23, 395– Child Psychology: Vol. 31. The effects of adversity on
410. neurobehavioral development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
Kuhn, C. M., & Schanberg, S. M. (1998). Responses to baum.
712 G. Dawson, S. B. Ashman, and L. J. Carver

Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on lan- Sonderegger, T. B. (Ed.). (1992). Perinatal substance
guage learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28. abuse. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University
Orvaschel, H., Welsh–Allis, G., & Weijai, Y. (1988). Press.
Psychopathology in children of parents with recurrent Spohr, H. L., Williams, J., & Steinhausen, H. C. (1993).
depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Prenatal alcohol exposure and long-term develop-
16, 17–28. mental consequences. Lancet, 341, 907–910.
Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of Stanton, M. E., & Levine, S. (1990). Inhibition of infant
children with autism: A study of first birthday home glucocorticoid stress response: Specific role of mater-
video tapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental nal cues. Developmental Psychobiology, 23, 411–
Disorders, 24, 247–257. 426.
Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1999). Early identification Stott, D. H. (1973). Follow-up study from birth of the
of one-year-old with autism versus mental retarda- effects of prenatal stresses Developmental Medicine
tion. Poster presented at the 1999 meeting of the Soci- and Child Neurology, 15, 770–787.
ety of Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, Suchecki, D., Rosenfeld, P., & Levine, S. (1993). Mater-
NM. nal regulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal
Pihoker, C., Owens, M. J., Kuhn, C. M., Schanberg, axis in the infant rat: The role of feeding and stroking.
S. M., & Nemeroff, C. B. (1993). Maternal separation Journal of Brain Research, 75, 185–192.
in neonatal rats elicits activation of the hypothala- Thompson, R. A., & Nelson, C. A. (in press). Develop-
mic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A putative role for mental science and the media: Early brain develop-
corticotropin-releasing factor. Psychoneuroendocrin- ment. American Psychologist.
ology, 18, 485–493. Todd, R. D., Neuman, R., Geller, B., Fox L. W., &
Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (1993). Early, postnatal Hickok, J. (1993). Genetic studies of affective disor-
experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releas- ders: Should we be starting with childhood onset pro-
ing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF con- bands? Journal of the American Academy of Child &
tent and stress-induced release in adult rats. Molecu- Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1164–1171.
lar Brain Research, 18, 195–200. Toni, N., Buchs, P. A., Nikonenko, I., Bron, C. R., &
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Klorman, R., & Brumaghim, Muller, D. (1999). LTP promotes formation of multi-
J. T. (1997). Cognitive brain event-related potentials ple spine synapses between a single axon terminal
and emotion processing in maltreated children. Child and a dendrite. Nature, 402, 421–425.
Development, 68, 773–787. Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communi-
Radke–Yarrow, M., Cummings, E. M., Kuczynski, L., & cation in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.
Chapman, J. (1985). Patterns of attachment in two- Tronick, E. Z., & Gianino, A. F. (1986). The transmission
and three-year-olds in normal families and families of maternal disturbances to the infant. In E. Z. Tron-
with parental depression. Child Development, 56, ick & T. Field (Eds.), Maternal depression and infant
884–893. disturbance (pp. 5–11). San Francisco: Jossey–Bass.
Radke–Yarrow, M., Nottelmann, E., Belmont, B., & Wappner, R., Cho, S., Kronmal, R. A., Schuett, V., &
Welsh, J. D. (1993). Affective interactions of de- Seashore, J. R. (1999). Management of phenylketonu-
pressed and nondepressed mothers and their children. ria for optimal outcome: A review of guidelines for
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 683–695. phenylketonuria management and a report of surveys
Rakic, P., Bourgeois, J. P., & Goldman–Rakic, P. S. of parents, patients, and clinic directors. Pediatrics,
(1994). Synaptic development of the cerebral cortex: 104, 4–9.
Implications for learning, memory, and mental illness. Weinstock, M. (1997). Does prenatal stress impair coping
Progress in brain research, 102, 227–243. and regulation of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal
Ramsay, D. S., & Lewis, M. (1995). The effects of birth axis? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 21,
condition on infants’ cortisol response to stress. Pedi- 1–10.
atrics, 95, 546–549. Weinstock, M., Fride, E., & Hertzberg, R. (1988). Prena-
Redding, R. E., Harmon, R. J., & Morgan, G. A. (1990). tal stress effects on functional development of the off-
Relationships between maternal depression and in- spring. In G. J. Boer, M. G. P. Feenstra, M. Mirmiran,
fants’ mastery behaviors. Infant Behavior and Devel- D. F. Swaab, & F. Van Haaren (Eds.), Progress in
opment, 13, 391–395. brain research (pp. 319–331). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Schneider, M. L. (1992a). Prenatal stress exposure alters Whiffen, V. E., & Gottlib, I. M. (1989). Infant of postpar-
postnatal behavioral expression under conditions of tum depressed mothers: Temperament and cognitive
novelty challenge in rhesus monkey infants. Develop- status. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 274–
mental Psychobiology, 25, 529–540. 279.
Schneider, M. L. (1992b). Delayed object permanence Wolkind, S. N., Zajicek–Coleman, E., & Ghodsian, M.
development in prenatally stressed rhesus monkey in- (1980). Continuities in maternal depression. Interna-
fants (Macaca mulatta). Occupational Therapy Jour- tional Journal of Family Psychiatry, 1, 167–182.
nal of Research, 12, 96–110. Zahn–Waxler, C., Cummings, E. M., McKnew, D. H., &
Shonkoff, J. P. (in press). Science, policy, and practice: Radke–Yarrow, J. (1984). Problem behaviors and
Three cultures in search of a shared mission. Child peer interactions of young children with a manic–de-
Development. pressive parent. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141,
Siegel, B., Pliner, C., Eschler, J., & Elliot, G. R. (1988). 236–240.
How children with autism are diagnosed: Difficulties Zuckerman, B., Als, H., Bauchner, H., Parker, S., & Ca-
in identification of children with multiple develop- bral, H. (1990). Maternal depressive symptoms during
mental delays. Developmental and Behavioral Pediat- pregnancy and newborn irritability. Developmental
rics, 9, 199–204. and Behavioral Pediatrics, 11, 190–194.

You might also like