You are on page 1of 19

Promoting Social Bonding

Strategies for Helping the Lonely and Socially Isolated


Karen S. Rook University of California, Irvine

ABSTRACT: The process of establishing and main- Stephenson, 1981) said that they never or only rarely
taining satisfactory social bonds is quite difficult for visited friends outside of their own neighborhood.1
many people. This article considers how interpersonal How should these figures be interpreted? Most
ties may be facilitated among the lonely and socially discussions of such data emphasize the majority of
isolated. A general framework for conceptualizing respondents who report ample social ties (Fischer,
solutions to loneliness is presented, and specific 1976, 1982; Seeman, 1975). Although it is important
interventions are discussed. Limitations of person- not to overstate the prevalence of social isolation
centered interventions are examined, and alternative and loneliness, it is also important not to minimize
interventions aimed at modifying features of social the existence of a significant minority whose social
settings are suggested. Interventions that may be lives are seriously deficient. Periods of loneliness and
helpful even when it is not possible to improve feelings of social awkwardness or alienation from
deficient social relations are also considered. Ethical others are probably experienced by most people over
issues raised by interventions for loneliness and the course of their lives, but for some these feelings
cultural values that may constrain their effectiveness persist or are sufficiently severe to make them vul-
are discussed. nerable to emotional disorders and impaired physical
health. Research has pointed to possible links be-
tween loneliness and depression (Bragg, 1979; Ho-
Social bonds have long been considered essential to rowitz, French, & Anderson, 1982), alcohol use
psychological well-being (Durkheim, 1897/1951; (Bell, 1956; Nerviano & Gross, 1976), adolescent
Paris, 1934; Murray, 1938). Yet for many people the delinquency (Brennan & Auslander, 1979), aggres-
process of establishing and maintaining satisfactory siveness (Sermat, 1980), physical illness (Lynch,
social bonds is quite difficult. Cross-national surveys 1977), and suicide (Diamant & Windholz, 1981;
indicate that loneliness is a painful and common Trout, 1980). Sociological analyses similarly have
problem. Estimates of loneliness in these surveys found social isolation to be associated with psycho-
range from 11% to 26% (Peplau, Russell, & Heim, pathology, particularly schizophrenia (Jaco, 1954;
1979). An average 40% of respondents in several Kohn & Clausen, 1955), and mortality due to
large-scale surveys described themselves as currently suicide and alcoholism (Collette, Webb, & Smith,
shy, and the vast majority of this group reported 1979; Gove & Hughes, 1980). A large literature on
that shyness was a major personal problem (Pilkonis social support suggests that those who lack others
& Zimbardo, 1979). Surveys that have focused on
objective indicators of social involvement rather
than respondents' self-labels suggest that many people An earlier version of this article was prepared for the workshop,
Preventive Interventions to Reduce the Harmful Consequences
lack important social ties. The quality of life surveys of Severe and Persistent Loneliness, which was sponsored by the
conducted by the Institute for Social Research Office of Prevention, NIMH, in cooperation with the Department
(Campbell, 1981), for example, found that one in of Psychology, UCLA, February 1982, Santa Barbara, California.
six Americans did not have a friend to whom they The following individuals are gratefully acknowledged for their
contributions to the preparation of this article: Mark Baldassare,
could confide personal problems, and this figure Sharon Biegen, Stephen Goldston, Raymond Novaco, Anne Peplau,
increased to one in five when city dwellers were Dan Perlman, Brian Vandenberg, and Carol Whalen.
considered separately (pp. 104-106). Although most Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed
respondents knew people to whom they could turn to Karen S. Rook, Program in Social Ecology, University of
for help in times of trouble, 5% knew no one California, Irvine, California 92717.
(Campbell, 1981), and 19% reported that they did
not have "many very good friends" (Campbell, ' This figure was computed from published frequency tables
for the General Social Survey (Davis et al., 1981). Of the 6,012
Converse, & Rodgers, 1976, p. 358). Similarly, an people who were interviewed about their social lives during this
average 19% of those interviewed for the General period, 1,157 (or 19.24%) indicated that they visited friends
Social Survey from 1972 to 1980 (Davis, Smith, & outside of their neighborhood only "once a year" or "never."

December 1984 « American Psychologist 1389


Copyright 1984 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
Vol. 39, No. 12. 1389-1407
to turn to for emotional or material support are to question nostalgic beliefs in a more socially
prone to stress-related illnesses and psychiatric dis- cohesive past. Nonetheless, the lingering doubt re-
orders (reviewed in Cohen & McKay, 1984; Heller mains that the process of successful social bonding
& Swindle, 1983; House, 1981; Kessler & McLeod, is somehow more haphazard or ephemeral than it
in press; Mitchell, Billings, & Moos, 1983). Although need be.
much of this research has been correlational in This article addresses the question of how in-
nature and thus is subject to alternative interpreta- terpersonal ties may be facilitated among the lonely
tions, the results of several longitudinal studies point and socially isolated. Until recently, loneliness has
to a causally prior role of deficient social relations been neglected as a topic for research (Perlman &
in the etiology of mental health problems (Pearlin, Peplau, 1981; Weiss, 1973), and as a result, relatively
Lieberman, Menaghan, & Mullan, 1981; Turner, little work has considered how to alleviate or prevent
1981; Williams, Ware, & Donald, 1981). Moreover, loneliness. Systematic comparisons of different in-
increased mortality has been linked to social isolation terventions are therefore not possible. Instead, the
in two carefully controlled prospective studies (Berk- goals of this article are to suggest a general framework
man & Syme, 1979; House, Robbins, & Metzner, for conceptualizing ways to help the lonely and to
1982). illustrate diverse intervention strategies. Existing ap-
In light of this research, persistent loneliness proaches to reducing loneliness are reviewed, and
and social isolation represent potential risk factors additional approaches are proposed. Ethical issues
for emotional and physical disorders. Some of the raised by interventions for loneliness and cultural
many people who describe themselves as lonely or values that may constrain their effectiveness are also
shy in surveys, or who appear relatively isolated on examined.
objective indices of social involvement, are likely to
experience impaired functioning because of deficien- Definitional Issues
cies in their social lives. This was indeed the judgment A discussion of what is meant by loneliness must
of the Office of Prevention, National Institute of precede any consideration of interventions. Many
Mental Health, when it convened a conference in definitions of loneliness have been offered (see Peplau
early 1982 to consider ways to prevent the adverse & Perlman, 1982), but a consensus has yet to
consequences of severe and persistent loneliness. A emerge. Indeed, some have argued that there are
similar conclusion was reached by the President's distinct types of loneliness (e.g., Weiss, 1973). Despite
Commission on Mental Health in 1978 in targeting the lack of definitional consensus, these different
the development of programs to foster natural sup- approaches share several common elements that
port systems as a national priority for mental health provide the basis for the definition offered in this
professionals (President's Commission on Mental article.
Health, 1978). First, loneliness is nearly always an aversive
Concern about the consequences of social iso- experience. Although voluntary isolation or solitude
lation and loneliness is also evident in social critics' may have positive consequences (Larson, Csikszent-
suggestions that social bonding has somehow gone mihalyi, & Graef, 1982; Suedfeld, 1982), unwanted
awry in contemporary society. The growing rate of isolation is usually distressing and is associated with
divorce, increases in the number of people who are sadness, anxiety, anger, self-deprecation, boredom,
living alone, and the magnitude of the singles industry and feelings of marginality (Rubenstein, Shaver, &
are frequently cited as evidence of the failure of Peplau, 1979; Weiss, 1973). These affective reactions
traditional institutions to foster the development of overlap with those associated with depression, but
enduring social bonds (Gordon, 1976; Slater, 1970). the negativity of loneliness is more circumscribed
The view that contemporary social relations are than that of depression (Bragg, 1979; Horowitz,
superficial or transitory is widely held and is variously French, & Anderson, 1982).
attributed to greater geographic mobility (Packard, Second, loneliness is not synonymous with
1972), weakening of the family (Bronfenbrenner, being alone, nor can being with others be assumed
1974; Conger, 1981) and other traditional commit- to offer protection from feelings of loneliness (Peplau
ments (Yankelovich, 1981), as well as a marked & Perlman, 1982). Loneliness occurs in the absence
increase in narcissism (Lasch, 1979). Pilisuk (1982) of social relationships as well as within ongoing
summarized this view: "The very social currents of relationships, such as "empty shell marriages" (e.g.,
careerism, autonomy, mobility, privacy and achieve- Levinger, 1979). Recognizing that loneliness does
ment that tend to disrupt our traditional roots and not covary directly with the amount of social contact,
ties also make difficult the continuity of new bonds" researchers have argued that cognitive processes
(p. 27). Empirical analyses contradict the popular determine whether or not the individual feels lonely.
belief that urbanization has undermined the quality Specifically, loneliness is hypothesized to occur when
of our social ties (Fischer, 1982), and it is prudent a person's existing social relationships are discrepant

1390 December 1984 • American Psychologist


from the person's expected or desired relationships partner for some desired activity. Such transient
(Gordon, 1976; Peplau & Caldwell, 1978). The lonely moods usually vanish fairly quickly and do
discrepancy model of loneliness has stimulated con- not warrant professional attention. This article is
siderable research but needs refinement in order to concerned with persistent loneliness that arises when
predict when loneliness rather than some other people have suffered disrupted social lives or when
reaction will be evoked by a dissatisfying interper- they have never established satisfying social relation-
sonal situation (Rook, 1983). For example, when ships.
does a person's perception that a marital relationship In sum, loneliness is defined as an enduring
falls short of his or her expectations result in lone- condition of emotional distress that arises when a
liness rather than resentment or problem-solving person feels estranged from, misunderstood, or re-
attempts? Two types of dissatisfying interpersonal jected by others and/or lacks appropriate social
situations seem particularly likely to engender feelings partners for desired activities, particularly activities
of loneliness—situations in which the individual that provide a sense of social integration and oppor-
feels estranged from, misunderstood, or rejected by tunities for emotional intimacy. From this perspec-
others (Fromm-Reichmann, 1959; Leiderman, 1969) tive, people who do not experience distress because
and those in which the individual must forego of an absence of social ties would not be considered
desired activities for lack of appropriate social part- lonely and, accordingly, would not be considered
ners (Derlega & Margulis, 1982). Thus, for some appropriate candidates for loneliness interventions.
people a disturbing and persistent sense of separate- This view respects the preferences of those who
ness from others is the core of loneliness, whereas choose to limit or refrain from social contact. Yet
for others loneliness arises from the repeated disap- clinical theorists have cautioned that some isolated
pointment of having to forego activities that depend people deny or fail to recognize loneliness, instead
upon the participation of another person. These two reporting somatic problems or vague feelings of
meanings of loneliness are not mutually exclusive, dissatisfaction (e.g., Peplau, 1955). Because people
however. Loneliness is apt to be most debilitating who are lonely tend to be viewed as social failures
when people lack social companions and additionally (Gordon, 1976; Parmelee & Werner, 1978), it is
feel misunderstood by or different from others. likely that even those who are well aware of their
Weiss (1969, 1973) offered an analysis of social loneliness may be hesitant to discuss their experience
needs that provides a basis for further specifying the (Fromm-Reichmann, 1959). Thus, service providers
types of companionship deficits that are most likely must be sensitive to the issue of stigma as they seek
to cause loneliness. He argued that people require to define and contact appropriate target groups for
both a sense of social integration and opportunities loneliness interventions.
for emotional intimacy to avoid loneliness. Moreover,
because social relationships become specialized to
Approaches to Intervention
perform certain functions, multiple relationships are Interventions focused on the problem of loneliness
necessary to meet these different social needs. Spe- could have three very general goals. The most obvious
cifically, ties to a social group (such as a network of goal would be to help lonely individuals establish
friends, a club, or a neighborhood organization) are satisfying interpersonal ties. Possible strategies might
needed to provide a sense of social integration, be to help lonely people improve how they relate to
whereas an attachment figure (such as a spouse or others or to identify new opportunities for them to
intimate partner) is needed to provide emotional have social contact. Other interventions might strive
intimacy. Deficits of either category of companion- not to alleviate loneliness but, rather, to prevent
ship cause loneliness. In Weiss's view, a person who loneliness from evolving into or contributing to
lacks ties to a social group cannot compensate for more serious problems, such as depression, suicide,
this deficit by acquiring an attachment figure, nor or drug abuse. For example, a severely lonely widow
can a person who lacks an attachment figure com- might be helped to identify enjoyable activities that
pensate by acquiring more group ties. This analysis she could engage in on her own, or a lonely adolescent
helps to explain why someone who has an extensive might be taught drug-free strategies for managing
network of friends but lacks a romantic partner the anxiety caused by feelings of isolation. Thus,
might complain of loneliness and, conversely, why even when it is not possible to improve the individ-
someone who has a romantic partner but no friends ual's social relations, service providers may perform
might also feel lonely. a valuable role in helping the individual cope with
A final element that is important in a compre- loneliness. A third goal of loneliness interventions
hensive definition of loneliness is a temporal dimen- might be to prevent loneliness from occurring rather
sion (Young, 1982). Many people undoubtedly ex- than to help people who are already lonely. Preventive
perience occasional feelings of separateness from interventions could focus on groups known to be at
others or periodic disappointment over lacking a high risk for loneliness or on the design of social

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1391


settings, such as schools and workplaces, to facilitate them, although they were not differentially evaluated
friendship formation. Educational programs could by nonlonely students.
provide information about how to make friends or Most of this research on the social behavior of
where to meet others with similar interests. lonely people has been conducted with college stu-
The following discussion of interventions is dents, thus limiting the extent to which the findings
organized according to these three broad goals. can be generalized. Moreover, it is not clear whether
Although few interventions have been developed the differences observed represent causes or conse-
specifically for loneliness, some elements of inter- quences of loneliness (Jones, 1982). Nonetheless, the
ventions for related problems such as shyness, dating apparent importance of personality dynamics and
anxiety, and depression may prove useful in efforts social skill deficits has prompted some researchers
to help the lonely. Therefore, interventions that to argue that loneliness represents an enduring trait
appear relevant to the problem of loneliness, as well for a subset of lonely people (Gerson & Perlman,
as those developed specifically for loneliness, are 1979; Shaver, Furman, & Buhrmester, in press).
discussed in the following sections. Such "trait-lonely" individuals are apt to remain
lonely even when they have access to a social network
Facilitating Social Bonding (Shaver et al., in press). Moreover, they appear to
be sadly resigned to further loneliness and engage in
Many factors undoubtedly contribute to the diffi- passive, ineffective coping responses, such as watching
culties that some poeple experience in establishing television, overeating, taking tranquilizers, and over-
and maintaining interpersonal bonds. Discussions of sleeping (Rubenstein & Shaver, 1982).
the causes of loneliness often distinguish between Horowitz (1983) offered an insightful analysis
characteristics of the lonely person and situational that links these ineffective social behaviors and coping
constraints (Peplau & Perlman, 1979; Weiss, 1973), responses to efforts to protect fragile self-esteem.
Social inhibition or deficient social skills may underlie Studies have repeatedly found loneliness to be as-
loneliness for some people. For others loneliness sociated with low feelings of self-worth (Peplau,
may result from limited opportunities for social Miceli, & Morasch, 1982), self-consciousness in
contact. social interaction (Cheek & Busch, 1981), and self-
blame for social failures (Anderson, Horowitz, &
Changing Characteristics of the Lonely Person French, 1983). Because social interaction is a cen-
Although it is appropriate to guard against the trally important source of feedback about the self
tendency to explain loneliness in person-centered (Altman & Taylor, 1973), some people with low self-
terms (Perlman & Peplau, in press), empirical evi- esteem adopt dysfunctional interpersonal strategies
dence suggests that personal characteristics predispose out of a desire to minimize the risks of negative
some people to loneliness. Studies have found that feedback. Horowitz (1983) described three dysfunc-
lonely college students have greater difficulty than tional strategies that may serve this self-protective
nonlonely students in introducing themselves to function—excessive compliance with the wishes of
others, making phone calls to initiate social contact, others, rebellion, and withdrawal. By trying to please
participating in groups, enjoying themselves at par- others, excessively compliant individuals hope to
ties, asserting themselves, and taking social risks avoid criticism and social rejection. Rebellion, in
(reviewed in Horowitz et al., 1982; Jones, 1982; contrast, invalidates others as a source of negative
Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980). Laboratory stud- feedback. Horowitz noted that some people rebel
ies of dyadic interaction suggest that lonely college internally rather than overtly, which may help to
students are less skilled at self-disclosure (Chelune, explain the cynical and mistrusting attitudes that
Sulton, & Williams, 1980; Solano, Batten, & Parish, have been found to characterize some lonely people.
1982) and less responsive to others (Jones, Hobbs, Withdrawal from interpersonal situations removes
& Hockenbury, 1982). In addition to these behavioral the threat of criticism or rejection by eliminating
differences, research suggests that lonely people may social contact altogether. Interestingly, these inter-
approach social encounters with greater cynicism personal strategies correspond closely to three dys-
and interpersonal mistrust than nonlonely people functional interpersonal orientations identified by
(Brennan & Auslander, 1979; Jones, Freemon, & Bowlby (1977a, 1977b)—compulsive care-giving,
Goswick, 1981; Moore & Sermat, 1974). Lonely emotional detachment, and compulsive self-reliance.
college students rated others more negatively and It is important to note that these strategies do
expressed less desire for continued contact with not serve to enhance self-esteem; rather, they protect
others in laboratory studies of acquaintanceship, an already fragile self-concept from further negative
suggesting a possible pattern of "rejecting others feedback. Loneliness is the cost of such protection
first" (Jones, 1982). Lonely students also evaluated because the individual is likely either to be isolated
themselves critically and expected others to reject or to form problematic or superficial relationships.

1392 December 1984 • American Psychologist


Thus, those who work with the lonely should be point for many lonely people, it may leave them
aware that the motivation to avoid threatening feed- unprepared for handling the transition to greater
back about the self may at times be stronger than intimacy. This is particularly important in light of
the motivation to establish social relationships. research indicating that as relationships become
Further research is needed to identify and trace deeper, criticism and attempts at control increase
the origins of individual characteristics that predis- (Rands & Levinger, 1979). In order to enhance their
pose people to loneliness (cf. Hansson, Jones, & prospects for maintaining satisfying relationships,
Carpenter, in press). Progress made to date on this lonely people may need to learn how to deal with
important topic emphasizes the role of social skill criticism and conflict (Rook & Peplau, 1982). In
deficits, low self-esteem, and dysfunctional interper- addition, social skills researchers have tended not to
sonal beliefs and strategies in causing or sustaining distinguish between clients whose interpersonal
loneliness. The interventions reviewed in this section problems reflect a history of social avoidance and
address these factors. those whose problems reflect a history of social
Improving social skills. Social skills training rejection, even though this distinction would have
programs were developed initially to help people important implications for the design of effective
deal with the problem of heterosexual dating anxiety programs. Conger and Keane (1981) noted, for
but have since been adapted for a variety of problems, example, that different skill deficits underly the
including social withdrawal and loneliness. These interpersonal problems of children who are shy or
programs draw heavily upon such behavioral tech- withdrawn as compared with children who are ag-
niques as modeling, role playing, performance feed- gressive or disruptive. Social skills interventions
back (sometimes using videotape methods), and should be tailored accordingly.
homework assignments. The specific skills to be Modifying dysfunctional beliefs. A number of
taught vary and have been the subject of some cognitively oriented therapies have been developed
controversy (e.g., McFall, 1982), but commonly in- to help clients recognize and correct self-defeating
clude initiating conversations, speaking fluently on thought patterns (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979;
the telephone, giving and receiving compliments, Mahoney, 1974; Meichenbaum, 1977; Novaco,
handling periods of silence, enhancing physical at- 1975). Young (1982) recently developed a cognitive-
tractiveness, nonverbal methods of communication, behavioral therapy specifically for loneliness. A crit-
and approaches to physical intimacy. Training is ical feature of the cognitive-behavioral model is
usually conducted in groups and is intended to be teaching clients to identify automatic thoughts and
time limited, often lasting 10 to 12 weeks. to regard them as hypotheses to be tested rather
Social skills training programs have been dem- than facts. For example, in new social situations
onstrated to be effective in helping participants anxious lonely clients may automatically think "I'll
acquire new interpersonal skills when compared make a fool of myself" and, as a result, may try to
with waiting-list and no-treatment control groups, avoid such situations. The therapist would encourage
although they have not been shown to be superior the client to test this thought empirically by inquiring
to more traditional therapeutic approaches (Twen- how many times the client actually has behaved
tyman & Zimering, 1979). Jones, Hobbs, and Hock- foolishly in the past, by probing the basis for the
enbury (1982) evaluated a social skills intervention client's belief that others are constantly judging his
developed specifically for the lonely. A group of or her behavior, by having the client keep a record
lonely college students was taught to increase personal of "foolish behavior" for a specific period, and so
attention (e.g., topic continuation, questions, refer- forth. This process of testing automatic thoughts is
ences to the other person) to their partners in a intended to help clients discover inconsistencies in
series of dyadic interaction tasks. Training consisted their assumptions and alternative interpretations that
of modeling, practice interaction, and feedback. may have been overlooked. Behavioral homework
Compared with two control groups of lonely students assignments of graded difficulty are also used to
(a no-treatment group and a practice-interaction- promote transfer of learning and to help lonely
only group), these students reported significantly clients gradually overcome their social inhibitions.
less loneliness, self-consciousness, and shyness at the In a useful extension of the general cognitive-
end of the study. Social skills training has also been behavioral approach, Young outlined six hierarchical
adapted for work with shy people (Pilkonis, Heape, stages in establishing intimate relationships and ar-
& Klein, 1980; Pilkonis & Zimbardo, 1979), although gued that lonely clients should master each stage
published evaluation studies are not available. before attempting successive stages. For example,
Most social skills training programs appear to learning to spend enjoyable time alone and engaging
emphasize skills needed to initiate social contact in activities with casual friends must be mastered
(see Monti, Corriveau, & Curran, 1982, for an before attempting to establish an intimate relation-
exception). Although this is a necessary starting ship, although Young cautioned that seeking intimate

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1393


ties may not be an appropriate goal for ail lonely able to form friendships and intimate attachments
clients. These distinctions encourage practitioners to after therapy. That is, modifying characteristics of
view treatment of loneliness as a developmental the lonely person should remove the major obstacle
process. to satisfactory social bonding. This view assumes
Modifying interpersonal orientations. Other that a large pool of potential companions exists to
systems of therapy make use of the client-therapist be readily accessed by the lonely once they have
relationship itself to modify dysfunctional interper- been equipped with the requisite intrapsychic and
sonal orientations. Proponents of client-centered behavioral assets. Although such a view may be
therapies (Rogers, 1961, 1973, pp. 119-121), for technically correct, it overlooks external constraints
example, attribute loneliness to clients' beliefs that that sustain loneliness and the objective difficulties
they will be rejected by others unless their behavior of meeting others in situations conducive to rela-
conforms rigidly to social norms. That is, the real tionship formation. Lack of time or money to
self is believed to be unlovable. Unlike the cognitive-participate in social activities and distance from
behavioral approach, which might seek to help others represent very basic constraints. The degree
lonely clients overcome this belief through critical of match between a person and his or her social
evaluation or carefully designed behavioral assign- environment additionally affects the likelihood of
ments, client-centered therapies emphasize the de- meeting others with similar interests, values, and
velopment of trust within the client-therapist rela- backgrounds. People who are different from those
tionship. By establishing a relationship in which around them have fewer opportunities to initiate
clients feel safe enough to express themselves without relationships. Blau's (1961) classic study of friend-
threat of censure, their fear of rejection is thought ships in old age illustrated, for example, that the
to be reduced. Thus, lonely clients' interpersonal social lives of widows were largely determined by
orientations and self-perceptions are changed by the prevalence of other widows in the local area. In
actually providing them with new social experiences areas where widowhood was uncommon, being wid-
that disconfirm their expectations. owed was associated with having fewer social ties.
Psychodynamic approaches assume that over Outside of a limited number of special contexts
the course of therapy clients come to manifest their that may facilitate acquaintanceship, such as college
problematic interpersonal orientations in the rela- dormitories, clubs, or community organizations, the
tionship with the therapist (e.g., Sullivan, 1953). opportunities to meet and interact with similar
Analysis of this resulting transference relationship others are probably more haphazard and generally
provides a vehicle through which clients can be limited than is commonly assumed. Moreover, social
made aware of (and presumably gain control over) interaction in such centrally important contexts as
unconscious processes that contribute to their inter- work and school is often structured in a way that
personal difficulties. For example, a client who alien-encourages competition or wariness of others rather
ates others through excessive efforts to be consideratethan friendliness.
may be helped to recognize such actions as an Interventions aimed at changing personal char-
implicit demand for approval and affection (e.g., acteristics of lonely people, therefore, will not inev-
Horney, 1937). Moreover, among clients whose lone- itably be accompanied by changes in their interper-
liness reflects intense social anxiety and avoidance sonal relationships. This is not intended to minimize
of relationships, the relationship with a reliable and the potential benefits of such interventions but rather
trusted therapist represents a new and beneficial to suggest that a rationale exists for considering
interpersonal experience in its own right (Greenson, interventions aimed at increasing the lonely person's
1967; Leiderman, 1969). opportunities for social contact. The President's
Few detailed analyses exist of the psychody- Commission on Mental Health (1978) took the
namic processes believed to contribute to loneliness position that it is a societal responsibility to provide
and the therapeutic methods considered most effec- mechanisms for social integration and stated that
tive in working with the lonely (Leiderman, 1969; "a healthy society provides opportunities for people
Satran, 1978). A systematic review of psychodynamic to be connected in [meaningful] ways . . . and
theory and research that specifically addresses the provides special help for those unable to avail them-
etiology and treatment of loneliness, as distinct from selves of such opportunities" (p. 144).
depression and anxiety, would be quite valuable. Relatively little work has directly addressed this
possibility, but several promising approaches are
discussed in this section. Interventions that seek to
Providing New Opportunities for Social Contact
restructure existing opportunities for social contact
The approaches discussed thus far differ substantially in order to promote friendly interaction are distin-
in their theoretical orientations, but they share an guished from those that seek to create new oppor-
implicit expectation that lonely people should be tunities for social contact.

1394 December 1984 • American Psychologist


Restructuring existing social opportunities. The (Sommer, Herrick, & Sommer, 1981; Sommer &
development of social ties is often fostered when Homer, 1981) argued that modern supermarkets are
people, sometimes out of necessity rather than choice, needlessly alienating environments that could be
work together to accomplish shared goals. The ben- altered to encourage friendly exchanges between
efits of such cooperative interdependence were dem- shoppers. Shopping malls, urban parks, and sports
onstrated in an innovative intervention for school facilities have been cited as public settings that could
children developed by Aronson and his associates be designed to facilitate social contact (e.g., Ulrich
(Aronson, Blaney, Stephan, Sikes, & Snapp, 1978). & Addoms, 1981). Within specific institutional set-
Aronson et al. argued that elementary schools are tings, such as nursing homes, work has begun to
often competitive, unfriendly places. Concerned identify environmental factors that promote social
especially about the impact of such an environment interaction (reviewed in Kasl & Rosenfield, 1980;
on minority children, Aronson et al. suggested that Lawton, 1980).
"it would be valuable if the basic process could be Increasing social opportunities through network
changed so that children could learn to like and building. When existing opportunities for social
trust each other not as an extracurricular activity contact are quite limited or not easily modified, it
but in the course of learning their reading, writing, may be necessary to consider ways to create new
and arithmetic" (p. 23). To this end, he and his opportunities for contact. Pilisuk and Minkler (1980)
colleagues developed a new instructional method, described several interesting projects that sought to
the "jigsaw classroom" technique, in which children establish supportive social networks for isolated se-
formed small learning groups. In the groups, each nior citizens. One project took place in inner-city,
child was given a different piece of information single-room occupancy hotels that housed many
about the lesson. In order to master the material, elderly persons. Physical disability, poverty, and fear
each child had to learn from other children and, in of crime prevented most of these inner-city residents
turn, teach them about his or her own part of the from venturing beyond their hotel rooms, and as a
assignment. Thus, cooperation was the rewarded result their social contacts were extremely limited.
pattern. Evaluation studies indicated that children Public health nurses set up stations in the lobbies of
in the jigsaw classrooms showed greater liking for these hotels and offered free blood pressure checkups
each other and greater self-esteem than children in as an initial means of contact. Over the course of
traditional classrooms (Blaney, Stephan, Rosenfield, several months they were able to identify shared
Aronson, & Sikes, 1977). Although the intervention interests among residents, which provided a basis
was not designed to reduce loneliness, a consequence for linking residents in dyads or larger groupings.
of the restructuring of classroom instruction was to After a year of such informal interaction, residents
reduce social isolation and increase friendliness. formed their own Senior Activities Club, which
Such cooperative task interdependence appears came to function as an independent, active support
to have been a feature of a successful low-income group.
housing project for the elderly in San Francisco An important feature of this project is that it
(Hochschild, 1973). Hochschild described how work made creative use of an indirect approach—provision
roles spontaneously evolved in this retirement resi- of free blood pressure checkups—to initiate contact
dence. These roles involved primarily interdependent with an isolated population. Had the project coor-
volunteer activities, such as construction of yarn dinators turned to a more direct approach, such as
dolls for an orphanage, fund raising for a local advertising for support groups, they would most
women's club, and visits to nearby nursing homes. likely have met with failure. Pilisuk and Minkler
This work was neither required nor reimbursed but (1980) observed that "where the fact of belonging
served an important function in structuring residents' and interacting is in itself foreign . . . special diffi-
social relations and contributing to the "web of culties [arise] in establishing trust levels that enable
sociability" (p. 40), supportive interpersonal linkages to develop"
Features of the physical environment may also (p. 106).
serve to foster or inhibit social interaction (Stokols, The development of support networks was an
1983). For example, structural characteristics of unexpected byproduct of a program aimed at im-
elementary school classes, such as class size and proving the nutritional status of the low-income
classroom organization, have been found to affect elderly. In 1975 an organization called the Senior
children's friendliness and popularity (Hallinan, Gleaners was formed to salvage and distribute to
1979). Tenant gardening programs established in the needy elderly food that normally would have
low-income housing projects in New York, Chicago, been wasted by food producers and suppliers. Mem-
and Philadelphia resulted in increased social inter- bers of the Senior Gleaners organization were pri-
action among residents as well as lower rates of marily isolated, poor elderly whose initial incentive
vandalism (Lewis, 1976). Sommer and his colleagues for participation was access to food. A high level of

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1395


cooperative interaction was required of members to by enhancing personal coping. One approach pro-
organize trips to produce fields and to coordinate vides emotional support during the transitional pe-
the distribution of food collected. Over time members riod following a major social loss. The second ap-
formed informal support networks, although this proach attempts to help lonely individuals develop
had been neither expected nor explicitly facilitated enjoyable activities in which they can engage inde-
by the project directors. pendently.
This process was referred to by Pilisuk and
Minkler as "unintentional network building," and it Coping With Social Loss: Transition Programs
is worth considering more generally the implications Loneliness that results from the loss of a significant
of intentional versus unintentional network building social relationship raises different intervention issues
for lonely clients. A common feature of the examples than loneliness that results from never having at-
discussed in this section, in addition to the feature tained such a relationship. Weiss (1976) developed
of cooperative task interdependence, is that partici- two programs tailored to the problem of loss of a
pants of the programs were not self-consciously spouse. Seminars for the Separated is a program
focused on establishing friendships with each other. that was designed to alleviate feelings of marginality,
This stands in sharp contrast to many profit-oriented confusion, and self-doubt that follow marital sepa-
singles programs, such as "people mixers" or dating ration. It typically involves 8 to 10 weekly group
services, in which the sole purpose of interaction is meetings of men and women who have recently
to find a partner. It seems likely that the self- separated from their spouses. Meetings begin with a
consciousness felt by participants in such programs short lecture given by a staff member, followed by
would doom many of their encounters to failure. small group discussions. The lectures cover such
Shyness researchers have argued that self-conscious- topics as the emotional reactions accompanying
ness inhibits the interactions of shy people and that separation, the impact of separation on relationships
strategies for distracting them from their self-con- with children, and issues of starting to date again.
sciousness would have value (Pilkonis & Zimbardo, Meetings usually conclude with an opportunity for
1979). Lonely people might similarly benefit from participants to socialize over refreshments.
social interactions structured in ways that distract Several benefits have been attributed to the
them from their self-consciousness and that do not program. First, the information provided through
appear contrived. Studies of the relative effectiveness the lectures reportedly helps participants to under-
of established organizations with goals not directly stand puzzling and sometimes disturbing emotional
related to friendship (for example, churches, the reactions. Weiss gave the example of a woman who
Sierra Club, adult education classes) versus the was quite disturbed by her impulse to attempt a
"singles business" (for example, singles bars, dating reunion with her former husband and who reported
services, singles apartment complexes) would be relief when she learned through a lecture that such
useful in this regard. feelings are common and reflect the persistence of
attachment feelings despite hostility toward the for-
Enhanced Coping With Loneliness mer spouse. Contact with similar, sympathetic others
Interventions that attempt to increase the lonely is believed to reduce participants' feelings of mar-
person's prospects for achieving satisfying interper- ginality and to provide a useful opportunity to
sonal ties would seem to provide the most direct practice socializing with members of the opposite
antidote to loneliness, but this approach is not sex. Finally, Weiss stressed the group's value in
always possible or appropriate. Newly divorced or simply getting members moving again and initiating
recently bereaved people may experience severe steps to improve their lives.
loneliness but may not be ready to establish new In attempting to generalize this group treatment
relationships. Similarly, the geographic isolation that approach to the recently widowed (Seminar for the
causes the rural widow to be lonely or the chronic Bereaved), Weiss encountered unanticipated prob-
disability that keeps some people housebound may lems. Explanations of the nature of separation dis-
be insurmountable obstacles to social involvement. tress were comforting to the newly separated, but
In such instances, a more appropriate goal of inter- comparable explanations of grief were experienced
vention would be to help people cope with their by the bereaved as painful. In contrast to the sepa-
feelings of loneliness. Rather than attempting to rated, the bereaved were more ambivalent about
improve the lonely person's interpersonal ties, the overcoming their distress, because their pain testified
goal would be to prevent loneliness from contributing to the intensity of their feelings for the deceased
to more serious problems such as depression or spouse. Weiss reported that the bereaved were also
alcoholism. less self-doubting than the separated and were quick
This section distinguishes two types of inter- to resent clumsy or awkward attempts to help. As a
ventions aimed at reducing the stress of loneliness result, the bereaved were less tolerant of fellow group

1396 December 1984 • American Psychologist


members whose mode of participation in the group of individuals who are both lonely and depressed.
differed from their own (e.g., who were more talk- Finally, if encouraging lonely individuals to develop
ative), suggesting a need for screening of potential enjoyable solitary activities seems like a "last resort,"
group members to ensure compatibility. Finally, it should be remembered that social relations entail
whereas contact between the sexes appeared to have personal costs as well as rewards (Homans, 1974;
been helpful to the separated, the mourning process Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). Establishing a love rela-
of widows and widowers differed sufficiently so that tionship is not always the panacea that some people
having both sexes together in one group was detri- expect (Lederer & Jackson, 1968). Moreover, Weiss
mental. (1973) has cautioned that the "campaign . . . for
These two programs have been described in an attachment figure" (p. 234) is difficult and fraught
some detail to illustrate the functions of short-term with risks, including the risks of social embarrass-
interventions for those whose social ties have been ment and of making hasty or troublesome partner
disrupted and to underscore the need for tailoring choices.
such interventions to the specific population served. An emphasis on enhancing solitary skills has
Empirical evaluations document the effectiveness of been recommended as an adjunct to therapy with
comparable professionally initiated interventions for lonely clients (Shaul, 1982; Young, 1982) and may
the bereaved (Raphael, 1977; Vachon, Lyall, Rogers, also be valuable in cases in which social isolation is
Freedman-Letofsky, & Freeman, 1980), as well as imposed by external constraints that cannot readily
for the elderly whose personal networks have been be changed, such as distance from others or physical
disrupted through residential relocation (Kowalski, disability. The stigma associated with being alone
1981), children whose parents have divorced (Wall- (Fromm-Reichmann, 1959; Parmelee & Werner,
erstein & Kelly, 1977), and individuals who have 1978), however, may make some lonely individuals
effectively lost a family member through a person- reluctant to focus on solitary activities. In such
ality-altering illness such as Alzheimer's disease (La- cases, it may be necessary to encourage the individual
zarus, Stafford, Cooper, Cohler, & Dysken, 1981). It to find enjoyable activities that bolster self-esteem
should be noted, however, that not all participants and that do not publicly identify him or her as being
benefit equally from such programs, as indicated without a partner, such as mastering a daytime
both by dropout rates and by analyses of individual hobby rather than attending a movie alone on a
change scores (cf. Gottlieb, 1983). For example, the Saturday evening.
ability to benefit from support programs has been
linked to the quality of participants' existing social Preventing Loneliness
networks (e.g., •Vachon, Lyall, Rogers, Freedman- Most discussions of prevention distinguish between
Letofsky, & Freeman, 1980). Thus, an important secondary and primary prevention (Cowen, 1977;
task for loneliness researchers is to evaluate which Goldston, 1977). Secondary prevention of loneliness
lonely individuals are the best candidates for such would involve the design of interventions tailored
programs. In addition, information is needed about to groups known to be at high risk for chronic or
the extent to which lonely people turn to or benefit severe loneliness. Primary prevention, in contrast,
from lay-initiated self-help groups. would most likely involve dissemination of educa-
Coping With Aloneness: Solitary Skills tional materials to a community or specific popula-
tion.
For some people the emotional distress associated
with loneliness is exacerbated by their inability to Identification of High-Risk Groups
spend satisfying time by themselves. Young (1982) Demographic factors such as age, marital status,
suggested that many lonely people are actually afraid gender, and income may predispose some groups of
of being alone and that, paradoxically, once they people to greater loneliness. Survey data indicate
overcome their fears of aloneness they can often that loneliness is higher among the young, the un-
initiate friendships more easily. Helping lonely in- married, the unemployed, women, and low-income
dividuals increase their repertoire of rewarding sol- groups (reviewed in Perlman & Peplau, in press).
itary activities may be useful for several reasons. These group differences point to possible structural
First, it relieves them of dependence on others to causes of loneliness. Among the permanently poor,
experience satisfaction and thus may increase their for example, "the routine affronts from a noxious
sense of personal control (Rook & Peplau, 1982). and sparse environment" not only threaten health
Second, research supports the usefulness of increasing and well-being but also contribute to the breakdown
pleasurable activities, including solitary activities, in of supportive social ties (Pilisuk, 1982, p. 28). Some
the treatment of depression (Fuchs & Rehn, 1977; of the demographic patterns of loneliness suggested
Lewinsohn, Biglan, & Zeiss, 1976). This strategy by survey data, however, may reflect differential
may be especially valuable for improving the morale willingness to admit personal problems (Perlman &

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1397


Peplau, in press). The surprising tendency for older Educational Approaches
adults to describe themselves as less lonely than
An interesting example of an educational interven-
middle-aged and younger adults has been attributed
tion intended to foster the formation of supportive
to self-report biases as well as to more modest social
social ties is the Friends Can Be Good Medicine
aspirations (Peplau, Bikson, Rook, & Goodchilds,
project, undertaken in 1982 by the California De-
1982). Experimental evidence suggests that lonely
partment of Mental Health, Mental Health Promo-
men are evaluated more negatively by others than
tion Branch. The objectives of the program were
lonely women, which may make men more reluctant
"to communicate the critical importance of sup-
to admit loneliness (Borys, Perlman, & Goldenberg,
portive personal relationships to California residents
1982). Extrapolation from survey evidence of group
and to stimulate community activities which connect
differences to recommendations for preventive inter-
people and provide opportunities to enhance personal
ventions must therefore be made with considerable
relationships in everyday life" (California Depart-
caution.
ment of Mental Health, 1982, p. 4). To accomplish
Obstacles to social participation make some
these objectives, public service announcements em-
groups needlessly vulnerable to isolation. Elderly
phasizing the value of friends were broadcast on
individuals, for example, often lack transportation
radio and television, and pamphlets were distributed
that would make it possible for them to visit friends
through schools, libraries, community groups, and
(Perlman, Gerson, & Spinner, 1978). Snow and
municipal offices. The pamphlets covered such topics
Gordon (1980) observed that many communities
as how social support enhances health, the ingredients
have a minibus service that will provide free rides of friendship, how to assess the quality of friendships,
to senior citizens for medical purposes but not for
and how to enrich existing relationships.
social purposes. They argued that this policy is
As with many educational outreach efforts, it
shortsighted and that providing safe transportation is not clear that these messages reached their intended
for an older person to visit a friend may contribute
audience. That is, people who already had satisfactory
as much to health and well-being as providing
social ties or who were most competent to develop
transportation for visits to physicians.
such ties may have been most likely to respond to
Much attention has also focused recently on
the educational messages. Evaluations of similar
the difficulties encountered by people who function
prevention programs have found that those who
as caretakers of chronically disabled family members.
take advantage of such programs are often the least
Such caretaking is often a full-time responsibility,
needy (e.g., Gottlieb & Hall, 1980). In addition, a
and although these individuals provide valuable social
major emphasis of the Friends Can Be Good Med-
support for the disabled person, their own social
icine project was to make people aware of the
lives often suffer. In fact, evidence suggests that the
importance of friends and to motivate them to
strain associated with caring for disabled relatives
pursue friendships. Yet it is doubtful that lack of
makes the caretakers themselves vulnerable to health motivation accounts for the loneliness and social
and emotional problems (Cantor, 1983; Fengler & isolation experienced by many people.2 Although
Goodrich, 1979). The phrase "supporting the sup- such messages may be useful in prompting some
porters" has evolved to refer to the idea that resources people to reevaluate their personal priorities, others
should be made available to ease the burden of
may require more detailed information about how
caretaking. Respite care, in which a professional or
to make friends or where to seek professional help
paraprofessional is sent to care for the disabled
if their previous attempts have been unsuccessful.
person for short periods, represents a relatively
For example, the Chicago Daily News ran a 3-part
simple intervention that would enable these family
series on loneliness just before the Christinas holidays
caretakers to maintain their social ties. in 1977 that provided information about a loneliness
Most caretakers are middle-aged and elderly hotline and community agencies that offered profes-
women (Shanas, 1979; Sussman, 1976), and their sional help to the lonely. Interestingly, the newspaper
caretaking role is likely to increase in the future as also published information about volunteer oppor-
greater emphasis is placed on preventing institution- tunities with local hospitals and charitable organi-
alization. Moreover, the frail elderly population is
increasing at a faster rate than other segments of the
elderly population, suggesting that increasing num- 2
An evaluation of the project indicated that roughly 70% of
bers of women will be called on to care for chronically respondents surveyed at baseline espoused the importance of
ill parents or spouses. These groups represent some- friendship (Hersey & Klibanoff, 1982, p. 8, Table 1). A 1-year
what "hidden" populations at risk for loneliness and follow-up indicated that those who were exposed to the educational
materials were somewhat more likely than those not exposed to
concomitant problems. Provision of services to re- say that friendships were personally quite important, although the
move obstacles to social network participation would two groups did not differ in their reported intentions to increase
thus seem to be a priority for community planning. their friendship activities (p. 9, Table 2).

1398 December 1984 » American Psychologist


rations on the assumption that such volunteer activity of older adults (Arling, 1976; Beckman, 1981; Lee,
could reduce loneliness (Mark, 1977). 1979). Nor does an empty parental nest (Borland,
1982) or voluntary childlessness (Macklin, 1980)
Role of the Family appear to increase loneliness and dissatisfaction. The
A very basic question from the standpoint of pre- degree to which family interaction protects family
vention is what role families play in alleviating or members against loneliness appears to depend upon
exacerbating loneliness. This question is particularly such factors as the nature of their role relationships
important in light of social changes that appear to and their stage in the life cycle.
be altering the structure and functions of contem- Such findings admittedly offer only indirect
porary families. These changes include increased evidence on the ability of family involvement to
divorce rates and concomitant increases in the num- prevent loneliness. Critics might also argue that
ber of single-parent and reconstituted households, family life in the past consistently deterred loneliness
increases in the number of dual-career families and by providing intimacy and companionship but that
other nontraditional family forms, extension of the the changes affecting contemporary families have
postparental (or empty-nest) stage of the life cycle, eroded such benefits. Available evidence tends not
postponement of childbearing, and increases in the to support this view. Recent analyses by historians
number of voluntarily childless marriages (Bronfen- indicate that American families in the 18th and
brenner, 1976; Macklin, 1980). What these changes 19th centuries were far less nurturant and harmo-
signify for the American social fabric is hotly con- nious than is typically assumed (Davis, 1979; Demos,
tested (e.g., Genovese, 1984; Hess, 1984) and is too 1976). Moreover, empirical comparisons of interac-
complex to consider here, This section focuses on a tion patterns within different kinds of families do
narrower question: Are these changes likely to affect not reveal that nontraditional families have distinct
the incidence of loneliness within families? To answer liabilities; e.g., neither single-parent nor dual-career
this question, it is necessary to consider the various families appear to be disadvantaged relative to more
ways that families may function to prevent or cause traditional families on such measures as the amount
loneliness. Although the diversity of contemporary of time spent with children at home or type of
family structures and life-styles makes generalizations activities engaged in (Medrich et al,, 1982). When
difficult, three potential effects of family life seem nontraditional families, such as female-headed fam-
particularly important. ilies, do exhibit signs of greater strain than traditional
First, family life may directly reduce loneliness families, the difference is as likely to reflect harsh
by providing opportunities for social interaction. economic realities as lack of commitment to the
For example, family activities may provide the sense parenting role or other psychological factors (Levitan
of social integration, or involvement in a socially & Belous, 1984). Although further research is needed
cohesive group, that Weiss (1973) postulated to be to clarify how family interaction affects loneliness,
a necessary antidote to loneliness. On the other these results suggest caution in assuming that the
hand, outside of the marital relationship, families quality of affectional bonds in modern families has
may be ill-equipped to provide emotional intimacy, deteriorated as compared to an idealized past.
which Weiss also postulated to be necessary to A second way in which families may affect the
prevent loneliness. Indeed, it may be difficult for a incidence of loneliness is through parental socializa-
single social group to provide both a sense of group tion of children's interpersonal skills and self-confi-
solidarity and opportunities for emotional intimacy dence. As indicated earlier, low self-esteem appears
(cf. Shaver & Buhrmester, 1983). Although empirical to be a centrally important factor in the onset and
work bearing directly on this issue is sparse, available persistence of loneliness. That such low self-esteem
evidence suggests that family interaction does not may originate in family treatment is suggested by
uniformly protect family members from feelings of evidence that lonely adolescents (Brennan & Auslan-
loneliness. For example, married people are less der, 1979) and college students (Sarason, Levine,
lonely than unmarried people (Perlman & Peplau, Basham, & Sarason, 1983) describe their parents as
in press), but loneliness is quite high among adoles- rejecting and unsupportive. Although these findings
cents (Brennah & Auslander, 1979; Parlee, 1979). may reflect distorted perceptions of parental treat-
Time use studies indicate that television viewing is ment, there seems little reason to doubt the value
the major context in which parents and children of encouraging parents to help their children develop
spend time together (Medrich, Roizen, Rubin, & healthy self-esteem.
Buckley, 1982), and it is not clear that this activity Parents may additionally foster effective inter-
does more than ensure close physical proximity personal attitudes and skills through modeling and
(Himmelweit, 1958). Frequent interaction between direct teaching. For example, research suggests that
adult children and their parents does not appear to parents who are socially withdrawn and anxious
reduce loneliness or substantially enhance the morale tend to rear children who are similarly withdrawn

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1399


and anxious (Filsinger & Lamke, 1983). Interpersonal because loneliness has many causes and manifesta-
cynicism and mistrust have been found to charac- tions. These approaches vary in their intended out-
terize lonely adolescents (Brennan & Auslander, 1979) comes and target populations. Moreover, some in-
and college students (Jones, 1982). What role, if terventions have been developed specifically to deal
any, do family experiences have in engendering these with loneliness, whereas others have been developed
attitudes? Interestingly, Greenberg and Nay (1982) for different problems but nonetheless may be useful
found that college students whose parents had di- in working with the lonely. The interventions also
vorced were more likely to endorse the termination differ in their focus on the lonely individual, on
of relationships as a solution to interpersonal prob- specific groups, or on social settings. Table 1 sum-
lems than students who had lost a parent through marizes the approaches discussed in terms of these
death or who were from intact families, although distinguishing dimensions. It should be noted that
the three groups did not differ on measures of not all of the cells defined by this table represent
interpersonal skill. In addition, parental divorce has plausible approaches to the problem of loneliness.
been found to be a stronger predictor of adult For example, prevention of loneliness at the individ-
loneliness than parental death (Rubenstein, Shaver, ual level or environmental approaches for enhancing
& Peplau, 1979), further suggesting that the effects coping with loneliness would seem likely to have
of parental divorce may persist into adulthood. limited appeal. It is also important to emphasize
When their parents' marriages deteriorate and ulti- that the approaches listed in Table 1 are not mutually
mately end, children often experience an over- exclusive. An intervention aimed at network building,
whelming sense of loss, anxiety about further aban- for example, might incorporate components of social
donment and social rejection, intense feelings of skills training programs or pleasurable activities
guilt, and fears of being different from peers (Gottlieb, scheduling.
in press; Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980). Early interven- Evaluations of the effectiveness of these inter-
tion with children of divorce (Felner, Farber, & ventions are sorely needed. With the exception of
Primavera, 1980; Gottlieb, in press; Wallerstein & social skills approaches and some bereavement pro-
Kelly, 1977) may help them cope with these reactions grams, few published evaluation studies exist of
and thereby reduce their vulnerability to loneliness. interventions developed specifically for loneliness.
Further analyses are needed of child-rearing practices Moreover, evaluation studies of interventions devel-
and family events that affect children's acquisition oped for related problems rarely include ratings of
of interpersonal attitudes and skills. loneliness or the quality of social ties as outcome
A final way in which families may affect the measures.
incidence of loneliness is through supporting or
hindering children's friendship choices. Parents are Ethical Issues
often ambivalent about their children's friends Interventions that seek to change people's social
(Medrich et al., 1982). Lonely people remember relations are not without risks (Snow & Gordon,
their parents as having been critical of their friends 1980). For example, programs designed to help
in childhood (Perlman, 1984). Loneliness is apt to socially isolated school children could be stigmatiz-
be reduced and interpersonal confidence enhanced ing. Oden and Asher (1977) urged service providers
if the family affords a supportive base from which to exercise caution in implementing such programs,
children can explore the formation of relationships for instance, by inviting nonisolated as well as
outside of the family. isolated children to participate. In addition, some
In sum, the family may function as a direct programs have made children both more aggressive
source of interpersonal involvement, as a context and more cooperative, and such potential negative
for the acquisition of interpersonal skills and atti- effects should be monitored (Conger & Keane, 1981).
tudes, and as a secure base from which to establish Friendly visitor programs aimed at reducing
peer relationships. Three questions require further the isolation of housebound or institutionalized el-
research. First, which of these functions, or other derly and disabled persons (e.g., Milligan & Bennett,
functions yet to be identified, is most effective in 1978) provide an example of a well-intentioned
preventing loneliness? Second, what factors inhibit intervention that may backfire if not carefully con-
the ability of contemporary families to perform each ducted. In a typical program, a volunteer visits the
of these functions? Finally, what should be done to shut-in person on a regular basis ranging from once
reduce the incidence of loneliness attributable to or twice a week to once a month. In effect, loneliness
ineffective family functioning? is countered in these programs by providing a friend.
The visits continue for an indefinite period, often
Overview of Intervention Approaches determined by the volunteer's other commitments.
It is apparent from this review that many strategies Expectations created on the recipient's part and the
exist for helping lonely people, which is appropriate manner in which termination of the volunteer's

1400 December 1984 • American Psychologist


Table 1
Overview of Interventions for Loneliness
Individual approaches Group approaches Environmental approaches

Goal of Specific to Relevant to Specific to Relevant to Specific to Relevant to


intervention loneliness loneliness loneliness loneliness loneliness loneliness

Facilitate Cognitive- Client-centered Social skills Social skills Network Restructuring


social behavior therapies training for training for building social settings
bonding therapy for Psychodynamic lonely college dating (intentional (e.g., school
loneliness therapies students and anxiety and unintentional) classroom
socially isolated Shyness modification)
children groups
Enhance Improving Support Self-help
coping solitary groups for groups
with skills bereavement,
loneliness (pleasurable divorce, and
activities other social
scheduling) losses
Prevent Early intervention Community Removing
loneliness with high-risk awareness obstacles
groups (e.g., and educational to social
children of programs contact
divorce)

visits is handled appear to be crucial. Some programs that directly attempt to provide social contact. Ther-
make use of college student volunteers who receive apists who see lonely clients in individual therapy
course credit for taking part in the program for a may need to be particularly sensitive to the extent
college quarter or semester. It is not clear that the to which they function as surrogates for the rela-
individuals visited are always well informed of the tionships lacking in their clients' lives and to the
time-limited nature of the program or that the impact of terminating therapy. These studies illus-
students are prepared to handle reactions evoked by trate, moreover, that the long-term effects of loneli-
pending termination of their visits. ness interventions may differ markedly from short-
A pair of studies by Schulz and his colleagues term effects.
(Schulz, 1976; Schulz & Hanusa, 1978) demonstrated It is also important to remember that most
these risks. In the initial study, residents of a retire- lonely people do not consult professionals about
ment home who were given control over the fre- their loneliness (Rook & Peplau, 1982), and many
quency and duration of visits made by college would not welcome professional assistance. Grounds
student volunteers scored higher on measures of for intervention therefore must be carefully estab-
health and psychological well-being than residents lished. Outreach efforts to contact vulnerable pop-
who were visited on a random basis or residents ulations may raise ethical issues. Some widow-to-
who were not visited. However, follow-up data col- widow support groups, for example, contact pro-
lected 24, 30, and 42 months after completion of spective participants by reading the obituaries and
the study indicated that the initial gains had been making unsolicited telephone calls. It is plausible
lost and that, in fact, the groups that had previously that some widows might find this intrusive or might
benefited from the visiting program now showed be offended by the implication that they need a
precipitous declines on the same measures. The special program to help them cope. Practitioners
residents who had not been visited by the college should strive to make high-risk groups aware of
students were the healthiest and best adjusted at available services without violating their privacy or
follow-up. The researchers attributed these deleteri- labeling them as needy.
ous effects of the visiting program to expectations Interventions designed to prevent loneliness raise
that had been raised and then abruptly violated similar issues, particularly because many interven-
when the study ended. Schulz and Hanusa (1978) tions are likely to be aimed at children. For example,
suggested that the declines "might have been avoided those who offer programs for children of divorce
had we provided substitute . . . events" (p. 1199). must weigh potential benefits of preventing future
This research underscores the risks that inhere attachment problems against possible risks of exac-
in some loneliness interventions, particularly those erbating the impact of the divorce and inadvertently

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1401


labeling the children as deviant from their peers. ineffective by participants even though their social
Service providers must be sensitive to how children relations improve at a general level. Service providers
interpret invitations to participate in such programs should be cautious about the extent to which they
and whether children are capable of making informed share this cultural bias.
decisions about their participation independent of Finally, Flanders (1979) suggested that programs
pressure from parents or teachers. to alleviate loneliness are at odds with the profit
The concept of unintentional network building, motives of the "loneliness industry" and with busi-
discussed earlier as a promising approach to the ness interests that thrive on the consumer behavior
problem of loneliness, also raises the issue of in- of lonely people. He noted, for example, that many
formed consent. Programs that bring lonely or iso- businesses in shopping malls count heavily on Sat-
lated people together for a manifest purpose (e.g., urday and Sunday business, a practice that is scorned
receipt of health care services, volunteer activities) by Europeans. In Europe it is customary for busi-
that differs from the true purpose (building social nesses to close on Saturday afternoon, presumably
networks) may be more successful than programs in because weekend time is considered best spent in
which the goal of promoting social ties is made family and leisure pursuits.
quite explicit. This potential advantage is offset, These different perspectives suggest that the
however, by the fact that participants of such pro- effectiveness of interventions to reduce loneliness is
grams are denied fully informed consent. Advocates likely to be diminished by conflicting cultural values
of outreach efforts must guard against potential and practices. Achievement of large-scale reduction
abuses of interventions intended to be benevolently in social isolation, therefore, may require attitude
protective of the lonely and socially isolated. change and educational efforts. As a first step, such
Cultural Values efforts might seek to loosen our cultural preoccu-
pation with romantic relationships (Brain, 1976)
Cultural values in this country may present imped- and to ascribe greater value to other forms of
iments to the alleviation of loneliness as a widespread relationships, particularly friendships. Increased
problem. Traditional cultural emphases on individ- public awareness of the situational and structural as
ualism and privacy, for example, have been cited as opposed to personal causes of loneliness might serve
obstacles to community acceptance of interventions to debunk the myth of "the lonely loser." Public
designed to promote social bonding (Heller, 1979). interest in the subject of loneliness appears to be
"The institutionalization of social support programs quite strong currently (Rubenstein & Shaver, 1980),
would require a shift in this value toward the suggesting that social scientists' contributions to
usefulness of interpersonal support as a key ingre- greater understanding of loneliness would be well-
dient in the maintenance of psychological well- received.
being" (Heller, 1979, p. 375).
Moreover, cultural values dictate that certain Conclusion
forms of interpersonal involvement are preferable This article has considered a variety of strategies for
to others. Gordon (1976) argued that we live in a helping the lonely and socially isolated. Several
"couple culture" where success is measured not themes from this discussion warrant highlighting.
only in terms of material possessions but also in First, the dominant research paradigm to date
terms of achieving certain types of relationships. has been to locate the sources of and solutions to
For the young adolescent this may mean having a loneliness in the lonely individual. Greater attention
"best friend"; for the teenage girl it may entail must be given to structural variables and features of
having a "boyfriend." For adult Americans, social social settings that inhibit the development of sup-
status has traditionally been attached to marriage. portive social ties. Although some of these factors
Stein (1976) discussed the "cultural imperative" of may not be amenable to change, many others rep-
marriage and cited such novels as Sheila Levine Is resent plausible foci of interventions. For example,
Dead and Living in New York by Gail Parent (1972) restructuring group interactions to encourage coop-
and Looking for Mr. Goodbar by Judith Rossner erative interdependence could complement more
(1975) as reinforcing the cultural stereotype that traditional social programming in such contexts as
unmarried people are failures in the marriage market. schools, workplaces, senior citizen centers, and nurs-
Because of social pressures and rewards associated ing homes. Supportive interpersonal linkages have
with certain roles, individuals who fail to measure been fostered even in severely isolated populations
up to cultural expectations may feel dissatisfied and without recourse to individually oriented therapies
lonely, even if they derive important gratifications (e.g., Pilisuk & Minkler, 1980). This is not intended
from other types of relationships. Thus, loneliness to minimize the usefulness of efforts to identify
interventions that do not culminate in the formation personal characteristics that predispose some people
of culturally sanctioned social bonds may be judged to loneliness but rather to urge analyses of the

1402 December 1984 « American Psychologist


ecological roots of loneliness. At a minimum, it transitions (Rook & Peplau, 1982) and may some-
would seem important for mental health profession- times serve as an impetus to growth (Suedfeld,
als to guard against the tendencies to blame people 1982). The goal of loneliness interventions, accord-
for their loneliness and to assume that abundant ingly, is not to eliminate aloneness but to provide
social contexts exist that are conducive to the for- greater options for those who seek to improve their
mation of friendships. social relations and particularly to offer hope to
Second, although many lonely people believe those whose aloneness is unwanted and prolonged.
that establishing a primary relationship (e.g., love
REFERENCES
relationship) would provide the antidote to their
loneliness, interventions that focus on alternative Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The
goals may also be beneficial. Establishing friendships development of interpersonal relationships. New York: Irvington.
may be a more appropriate initial goal for some Anderson, C. A., Horowitz, L. M., & French, R. D. (1983).
lonely people than seeking to establish love relation- Attributional style of lonely and depressed people. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 127-136.
ships. Helping lonely individuals develop a repertoire Arling, G. (1976). The elderly widow and her family, neighbors
of enjoyable solitary activities may also be useful and friends. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 757-767.
even though it does not directly affect their social Aronson, E., Blaney, N., Stephan, C., Sikes, J., & Snapp, M.
ties. Such interventions may enhance psychological (1978). The jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
well-being even if they do not remedy major defi- Beck, A. T, Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).
Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.
ciencies in the lonely person's social network. Beckman, L. J. (1981). Effects of social interaction and children's
Third, further research is needed to inform relative inputs on older women's psychological well-being. Journal
intervention efforts. Epidemiological studies are of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 1075-1086.
needed to clarify our understanding of who is at Bell, R. G. (1956). Alcohol and loneliness. Journal of Social
Therapy, 2, 171-181.
greatest risk for severe and persistent loneliness. Berkman, L. F., & Syme, S. L. (1979). Social networks, host
Longitudinal studies would be helpful in this regard resistance, and mortality: A nine-year follow-up of Alameda
to identify temporal sequences that may link lone- County residents. American Journal of Epidemiology, 109, 186-
liness to depression and other disorders. Such re- 204.
search is particularly important in an era of dimin- Blaney, N. X, Stephan, C., Rosenfield, D., Aronson, E., & Sikes,
J. (1977). Interdependence in the classroom: A field study.
ishing funds for mental health services to aid in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 121-128.
targeting interventions to those groups most in need. Blau, Z. (1961). Structural constraints on friendship in old age.
In addition, controlled evaluations of the effectiveness American Sociological Review, 26, 429-439,
of existing loneliness should be conducted. Careful Borland, D. C. (1982). A cohort analysis approach to the empty-
nest syndrome among three ethnic groups of women: A theo-
consideration must be given to the outcomes assessed retical position. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 44, 117-
in evaluation studies. It may be advisable, for ex- 129.
ample, to supplement self-reports of loneliness with Borys, S., Perlman, D., & Goldenberg, S. (1982, May). Sex
measures assessing the extent and quality of inter- differences in loneliness. Paper presented at the annual meeting
personal contacts. Moreover, researchers should be of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Minneapolis, MN.
Bowlby, J. (1977a). The making and breaking of affectional bonds.
alert to the possibility of unintended consequences I. Aetiology and psychopathology in the light of attachment
of their interventions. As noted earlier, some social theory. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 201-210,
skills interventions appear to have increased aggres- Bowlby, J. (1977b). The making and breaking of affectional bonds,
siveness in children (Kirby & Toler, 1970), and an II. Some principles of psychotherapy. British Journal of Psy-
chiatry, 130, 421-431.
intervention with the elderly had a long-term detri- Bragg, M. E. (1979). A comparative study of loneliness and
mental impact that was not evident in the initial depression (Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los
evaluation (Schulz & Hamisa, 1978). These examples Angeles, 1979). Dissertation Abstracts International, 39, 79-
speak to the need to incorporate measures of possible 13710.
Brain, R. (1976). Friends and lovers. New York: Basic Books,
negative outcomes and to include long-term follow- Brennan, T., & Auslander, N. (1979). Adolescent loneliness: An
up in the design of evaluation studies. exploratory study 'of social and psychological predispositions
Finally, it is important that efforts to alleviate and theory (Vol. 1). Washington, DC: National Institute of
loneliness not be construed as endorsing an ethic of Mental Health, Juvenile Problems Division. (ERIC Document
sociability. Most individuals strive for an optimal Reproduction Service No. ED 194822)
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). The origins of alienation. Scientific
level of social contact that balances the need for American, 321, 53-61.
intimacy and the need for privacy. For some people Bronfenbrenner, U. (1976). Who cares for America's children? In
this optimal level is achieved with very infrequent V. C. Vaughan & T. B. Brazelton (Eds.), The family—can it be
social contact. It is easy to exaggerate the benefits saved? (pp. 3-32). Chicago: Yearbook Medical Publishers.
of social ties and to overlook their very real con- California Department of Mental Health, Mental Health Promotion
Branch. (1982). Friends can be good medicine: Resource guide,
straints and limitations. It is easy to forget, moreover, Sacramento, CA: Author.
that loneliness is not inevitably bad. Loneliness may Campbell, A. (1981). The sense of well-being in America. New
be an unavoidable aspect of certain developmental York: McGraw-Hill.

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1403


Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The Genovese, R. G. (Ed.). (1984). Families and change (pp. 3-10).
quality of American life. New York: Russell Sage. New York: Praeger.
Cantor, M. H. (1983). Strain among caregivers: A study of Gerson, A. C., & Perlman, D. (1979). Loneliness and expressive
experience in the United States. Gerontologist, 23, 597-604. communication. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 258-266.
Cheek, J. M., & Busch, C. M. (1981). The influence of shyness Goldston, S. E. (1977). Denning primary prevention. In G. W.
on loneliness in a new situation. Personality and Social Psy- Albee & J. M. Joffee (Eds.), Primary prevention of psychopa-
chology Bulletin, 7, 572-577 thology (Vol. 1, pp. 18-41). Hanover, NH: University Press of
Chelune, G. J., Sultan, F. E., & Williams, C. L. (1980). Loneliness, New England.
self-disclosure, and interpersonal effectiveness. Journal of Coun- Gordon, S. (1976). Lonely in America. New York: Simon &
seling Psychology, 27, 462-468. Schuster.
Cohen, S., & McKay, G, (1984). Social support, stress and the Gottlieb, B. H. (1983). Social support strategies: Guidelines for
buffering hypothesis: A theoretical analysis. In A. Baum, J. E. mental health practice. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Singer, & S. E. Taylor (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and Gottlieb, B. H. (in press). Theory into practice: Issues that surface
health (Vol. 4, pp. 253-267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. in planning interventions which mobilize support. In I. G.
Collette, J., Webb, S. D., & Smith, D. L. (1979). Suicide, Sarason Si B. R. Sarason (Eds,), Social support: Theory, research
alcoholism and types of social integration: Clarification of a and application. The Hague: Martinus Nijhof.
theoretical legacy. Sociology and Social Research, 63, 699-721. Gottlieb, B. H., & Hall, A. (1980). Social networks and the
Conger, J. C., & Keane, S. P. (1981). Social skills intervention in utilization of preventive mental health services. In R. H. Price,
the treatment of isolated or withdrawn children. Psychological R. F. Ketterer, B. C. Bader, & J. Monahan (Eds.), Prevention in
Bulletin, 90, 478-495. mental health (pp. 167-194). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Conger, J. J. (1981). Freedom and commitment: Families, youth, Gove, W. R., & Hughes, M. (1980). Re-examining the ecological
and social change. American Psychologist, 36, 1475-1484. fallacy: A study in which aggregate data are critical in investi-
Cowen, E. L. (1977). Baby steps toward primary prevention. gating the pathological effects of living alone. Social Forces, 58,
American Journal of Community Psychology, 5, 1-22. 1157-1177.
Davis, D. B. (1979). The American family and boundaries in Greenberg, E., & Nay, W. R. (1982). The intergenerational trans-
historical perspective. In D. Reiss & H. A. Hoffman (Eds.), The mission of marital instability reconsidered. Journal of Marriage
American family: Dying or developing? (pp. 13-33). New York: and the Family, 44, 335-347.
Plenum. Greenson, R. R. (1967). The technique and practice of psycho-
Davis, J. A., Smith, T. W., & Stephenson, C. B. (1981). General analysis. New York: International Universities Press.
social survey, cumulative file, 1972-1980. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter- Hallinan, M. T. (1979). Structural effects on children's friendships
University Consortium for Political and Social Research. and cliques. Social Psychology Quarterly, 1, 43-54.
Demos, J. (1976). Myths and realities in the history of American Hansson, R. Q, Jones, W., & Carpenter, B. N. (in press). Relational
family life. In H. Grunebaum & J. Christ (Eds.), Contemporary competence and social support. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of
marriage: Structure, dynamics, and therapy (pp. 9-31). Boston: personality and social psychology. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Little, Brown. Heller, K. (1979). The effects of social support: Prevention and
Derlega, V. J., & Margulis, S. T. (1982). Why loneliness occurs: treatment implications. In A. P, Goldstein & F. H. Kanfer
The interrelationship of social-psychological and privacy con- (Eds.), Maximizing treatment gains: Transfer enhancement in
cepts. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A psychotherapy (pp. 353-382). New "York: Academic Press.
sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 152- Heller, K., & Swindle, R. W. (1983). Social networks, perceived
165). New York: Wiley-Interscience. social support and coping with stress. In R. D. Felner, C. A.
Diamant, L., & Windholz, G. (1981). Loneliness in college Jason, J. Moritsugu, & S. S. Farber (Eds.), Preventive psychology:
students: Some therapeutic considerations. Journal of College Theory, research and practice in community intervention (pp.
Student Personnel, 22, 515-522. 87-103). New York: Permagon Press.
Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide: A study in sociology (J. A. Spaulding Hersey, J. C., & Klibanoff, L. S. (1982). Executive summary:
& G. Simpson, Trans.). New York: Free Press. (Original work Friends can be good medicine project. Sacramento, CA: Cali-
published 1897) fornia Department of Mental Health.
Paris, R. L. (1934). Cultural isolation and the schizophrenic Hess, B. B. (1984). Protecting the American family: Public policy,
personality. American Journal of Sociology, 40, 156-164. family, and the New Right. In R. G. Genovese (Ed.), Families
Felner, R. D., Farber, S. S., & Primavera, J. (1980). Children of and change (pp. 11-21). New York: Praeger.
divorce, stressful life events and transitions: A framework for Himmelweit, H. (1958). Television and the child: An empirical
preventive efforts. In R. H. Price, R. F. Ketterer, B. C. Bader, study of the effect of television on the young. London: Oxford
& J. Monahan (Eds.), Prevention in mental health: Research, University Press.
policy and practice (pp. 81-108). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Hochschild, A. (1973). The unexpected community. Berkeley, CA:
Fengler, A., & Goodrich, N. (1979). Wives of elderly disabled University of California Press.
men: The hidden patients. Gerontologist, 19, 175-183. Homans, G. C. (1974). Social behavior (2nd ed.). New York:
Filsinger, E. E., & Lamke, L. K. (1983). The lineage transmission Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
of interpersonal competence. Journal of Marriage and the Horney, K. (1937). The neurotic personality of our time. New
Family, 45, 75-80. York: Norton.
Fischer, C. S. (1976). The urban experience. New York; Harcourt Horowitz, L. M. (1983). The toll of loneliness: Manifestations,
Brace Jovanovich. mechanisms, and means of prevention. (Technical Advisory
Fischer, C. S. (1982). To dwell among friends. Chicago: University Report) Washington, DC: National Institute of Mental Health,
of Chicago Press. Office of Prevention.
Flanders, J. P. (1979, May). Paradigms to study and control Horowitz, L. M., French, R. S., & Anderson, C. A. (1982). The
loneliness. Paper presented at the UCLA research conference prototype of a lonely person. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman
on loneliness, Los Angeles, CA. (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and
Fromm-Reichmann, F. (1959), Loneliness. Psychiatry, 22, 1-15. therapy (pp. 183-205). New York: Wiley-Interscience.
Fuchs, C. Z., & Rehn, L. P. (1977). A self-control behavior House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading, MA:
therapy program for depression. Journal of Consulting and Addison-Wesley.
Clinical Psychology, 45, 206-215. House, J. S., Robbins, C., & Metzner, H. L. (1982). The association

1404 December 1984 • American Psychologist


of social relationships and activities with mortality: Prospective McFall, R. (1982). A review and reformulation of the concept of
evidence from the Tecumseh Community Health Study. Amer- social skills. Behavioral Assessment, 4, 1-34.
ican Journal of Epidemiology, 116, 123-140. Medrich, E. A., Roizen, J., Rubin, V., & Buckley, S. (1982). The
Jaco, E. G. (1954). The social isolation hypothesis and schizo- serious business of growing up: A study of children's lives
phrenia. American Sociological Review, 19, 567-577. outside school. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Jones, W. (1982). Loneliness and social behavior. In L. A. Peplau Meichenbaum, D. (1977). Cognitive behavior modification: An
& D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current integrative approach. New York: Plenum Press.
theory, research and therapy (pp. 238-252). New York: Wiley- Milligan, M. A., & Bennett, R. (1978). Assessment of mental
Interscience. health and social problems during multiple friendly visits: The
Jones, W. H., Freemon, J. R., & Goswick, R. A. (1981). The development and evaluation of a friendly visiting program for
persistence of loneliness: Self and other determinants. Journal isolated elderly. International Journal of Aging and Human
of Personality, 49, 27-48. Development. 8, 43-45.
Jones, W. H., Hobbs, S. A., & Hockenbury, D. (1982). Loneliness Mitchell, R. E., Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1983). Social
and social skill deficits. Journal of Personality and Social support and well-being: Implications for prevention programs.
Psychology 42, 682-689. Journal of Primary Prevention, 3, 77-98.
Kasl, S. V., & Rosenfield, S. (1980). The residential environment Monti, P. M., Corriveau, D. P., & Curran, J. P. (1982). Social
and its impact on the mental health of the aged. In J. E. Birren skills training for psychiatric patients: Treatment and outcome.
& R. B. Sloane (Eds.), Handbook of mental health and aging In J. P. Curran & P. M. Monti (Eds.), Social skills training
(pp. 468-498). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. (pp. 185-223). New York: Guilford.
Kessler, R. C., & McLeod, J. (in press). Social support and mental Moore, J. A., & Sermat, V. (1974). Relationship between self-
health in community samples. In S. Cohen & L, Syme (Eds.), actualization and self-reported loneliness. Canadian Counsellor,
Social support and health. New York: Academic Press. 8(3), 194-196.
Kirby, F. D., & Toler, H. C. (1970). Modification of pre-school Murray, H. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford
isolate behavior: A case study. Journal of Applied Behavior University Press.
Analysis, 3, 309-314. Nerviano, N. J., & Gross, W. F, (1976). Loneliness and locus of
Kohn, M. L., & Clausen, J. A. (1955). Social isolation and control for alcoholic males: Validity against Murray need and
schizophrenia. American Sociological Review, 20, 265-273. Cattell trait dimensions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32,
Kowalski, N. C. (1981). Institutional relocation: Current programs 479-484.
and applied approaches. Gerontologist, 21, 512-519. Novaco, R. (1975). Anger control: The development and evaluation
Larson, Ri, Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Graef, R. (1982). Time alone of an experimental treatment. Lexington, MA: Heath & Co.
in daily experience: Loneliness or renewal? In L. A. Peplau & Oden, S., & Asher, S. R. (1977). Coaching children in social skills
D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, for friendship-making. Child Development, 48, 495-506.
research and therapy (pp. 40-53). New York: Wiley-Interscience. Packard, V. (1972). A nation of strangers. New York: David
Lasch, C. (1979). The culture of narcissism: American life in an McKay.
age of diminishing expectations. New York: Norton. Parent, G. (1972). Sheila Levine is dead and living in New York.
Lawton, M. P. (1980). Environment and aging. Belmont, CA: New York: Putnam.
Wadsworth. Parlec, M. B. (1979). The friendship bond. Psychology Today,
Lazarus, C. W., Stafford, B., Cooper, K., Cohler, B., & Dysken, 113, 43-54.
M. (1981). A pilot study of an Alzheimer patients' relatives Parmelee, P., & Werner, C. (1978). Lonely losers: Stereotypes of
discussion group. Gerontologist, 21, 353-358. single dwellers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4,
Lederer, W. J., & Jackson, D. D. (1968). The mirages of marriage. 292-295.
New York: Norton. Pearlin, L. I., Lieberman, M. A., Menaghan, E. G., & Mullan,
Lee, G. R. (1979). Children and the elderly: Interaction and J. T. (1981). The stress process. Journal of Health and Social
morale. Research on Aging, 1, 335-360. Behavior, 22, 337-356.
Leiderman, P. H, (1969). Loneliness: A psychodynamic interpre- Peplau, H. E. (1955). Loneliness. American Journal of Nursing,
tation. International Psychiatry Clinic, 6(2), 155-174. 55(12), 1476-1481.
Levinger, G. A. (1979). A social psychological perspective on Peplau, L. A., Bikson, T. K., Rook, K. S., & Goodchilds, J. D.
marital dissolution. In G. Levinger & O. C. Moles (Eds.), (1982). Being old and living alone. In L. A. Peplau & D.
Divorce and separation (pp. 37-60). New York: Basic Books. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory,
Levitan, S. A., & Belous, R. S. (1984). Working wives and mothers: research and therapy (pp. 327-347). New \brk: Wiley-Intersci-
What happens to family life? In R. G. Genovese (Ed.), Families ence.
and change (pp. 110-117). New York: Praeger. Peplau, L. A., & Caldwell, M. A. (1978). Loneliness: A cognitive
Lewinsohn, P. M., Biglan, A,, & Zeiss, A. M. (1976). Behavioral analysis. Essence, 2, 207-220.
treatment of depression. In P. O. Davison (Ed.), The behavioral Peplau, L. A., Miceli, M., & Morasch, B. (1982). Loneliness and
management of anxiety, depression and pain (pp. 91-145). New self-evaluation. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness:
York: Brunner/Mazel. A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 135-
Lewis, C. A. (1976). People/plant proxemics: A concept for 151). New York: Wiley-Interscience.
humane design. In P. Suedfeld & J. Russell (Eds.), The behavioral Peplau, L. A., & Perlman, D. (1979). Blueprint for a social
basis of design: Proceedings of the 7th International Environ- psychological theory of loneliness. In M. Cook & G. Wilson
mental Design Research Association Conference (pp. 102-107). (Eds.), Love and attraction (pp. 101-110). Oxford, England:
Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross. Pergamon.
Lynch, J. J. (1977). The broken heart: The medical consequences Peplau, L. A., & Perlman, D. (1982). Perspectives on loneliness.
of loneliness in America. New York: Basic Books. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook
Macklin, E. D. (1980). Nontraditional family: A decade of research. of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 1-18). New York:
'Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42, 905-922. Wiley-Interscience.
Mahoney, M. J. (1974). Cognition and behavior modification. Peplau, L. A., Russell, D., & Heim, M. (1979). The experience
Cambridge, MA: Ballinger. of loneliness. In I. H. Frieze, D. Bar-Tal, & J. S. Carroll (Eds.),
Mark, N. (1977, December 19-21). Loneliness can kill. Chicago New approaches to social problems: Applications of attribution
Daily Ne\vs, pp. 8-10. theory (pp. 53-78). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1405


Perlman, D. (1984, August). Loneliness and social support in physical and psychological well-being of the institutionalized
developmental perspective. Paper presented at the annual meeting aged. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 563-
of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. 573.
Perlman, D., Gerson, A. C, & Spinner, B. (1978). Loneliness Schulz, R., & Hanusa, B. H. (1978). Long-term effects of control
among senior citizens: An empirical report. Essence, 2, 239- and predictability-enhancing interventions: Findings and ethical
248. issues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1194-
Perlman, D., & Peplau, L. A. (1981). Toward a social psychology 1201.
of loneliness. In S. Duck & R. Oilmour (Eds.), Personal Seeman, M. (1975). Alienation studies. Annual Review of Sociology,
relationships 3: Personal relationships in disorder (pp. 31-56). 1, 91-123.
London: Academic Press. Sermat, V. (1980). Some situational and personality correlates of
Perlman, D., & Peplau, L. A. (in press). Loneliness research: A loneliness. In J. Hartog, J. R. Audy, & Y. A. Cohen (Eds.), The
survey of empirical findings. In L. A. Peplau & S. E. Goldston anatomy of loneliness. New \brk: International Universities
(Eds.), Preventing the harmful consequences of severe and per- Press.
sistent loneliness. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Shanas, E. (1979). The family as a social support system in old
Office. age. Gerontologist, 19, 169-174.
Pilisuk, M. (1982). Delivery of social support: The social inocu- Shaul, S. (1982, August). The effectiveness of two counseling
lation. American Journal of Ortltopsychiatry, 52, 20-31. strategies for reducing loneliness. Paper presented at the annual
Pilisuk, M., & Minkler, M. (1980). Supportive networks: Life ties meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington,
for the elderly. Journal of Social Issues, 36, 95-116. DC.
Pilkonis, P. A., Heape, C., & Klein, R. H. (1980). Treating shyness Shaver, P., & Buhrmester, D. (1983). Loneliness, sex-role orientation
and other relationship difficulties in psychiatric outpatients. and group life: A social needs perspective. In P. B. Baulus (Ed.),
Communication Education, 29, 250-255. Basic group processes (pp. 259-288). New York: Springer-
Pilkonis, P. A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1979). The personal and Verlag.
social dynamics of shyness. In C. E. Izard (Ed.), Emotions in Shaver, P., Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (in press). Aspects of
personality and psychopatho/ogy (pp. 133-160). New York: a life transition: Network changes, social skills, and loneliness.
Plenum Press. In S. Duck & D. Perlman (Eds.), The Sage series in personal
President's Commission on Mental Health. (1978, February). relationships (Vol. 1). London: Sage.
Report of the Task Panel on Community Support Systems. Slater, P. (1970). The pursuit of loneliness: American culture at
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. the breaking point. Boston: Beacon Press.
Rands, M., & Levinger, G. (1979). Implicit theories of relationship: Snow, D. L., & Gordon, J. B. (1980). Social network analysis and
An intergenerational study. Journal of Personality and Social intervention with the elderly. Gerontologist, 20, 463-467.
Psychology, 37, 645-661. Solano, C. H., Batten, P. G., & Parish, E. A. (1982). Loneliness
Raphael, B. (1977). Preventive intervention with the recently and patterns of self-disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social
bereaved. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 1450-1454. Psychology, 43, 524-531.
Rogers, C. R. (1961). The loneliness of contemporary man as Sommer, R., Herrick, J., & Sommer, T. R. (1981). The behavioral
seen in "The case of Ellen West." Annals of Psychotherapy, 3, ecology of supermarkets and farmer's markets. Journal of
22-27. Environmental Psychology, 1, 13-19.
Rogers, C. R. (1973). Carl Rogers on encounter groups. New York: Sommer, R., & Horner, M. (1981). Social interaction in co-ops
Harper & Row. and supermarkets. Communities, 20, 15-18.
Rook, K. S. (1983). Social support, loneliness, and social isolation. Stein, P. J. (1976). Single. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
(Technical Advisory Report) Washington, DC: National Institute Stokols, D. (1983). The environmental context of behavior. In D.
of Mental Health, Office of Prevention. Perlman & C. Cozby (Eds.), Social psychology (pp. 443-472).
Rook, K. S., & Peplau, L. A. (1982). Perspectives on helping the New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
lonely. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A Suedfeld, P. (1982). Aloneness as a healing experience. In L. A.
sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 351- Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of
378). New York: Wiley-Interscience. current theory, research and therapy (pp. 54-67). New York:
Rossner, J. (1975). Looking for Mr. Goodbar. New York: Simon Wiley-Interscience.
& Schuster. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New
York: Norton.
Rubenstein, C. M., & Shaver, P. (1980). Loneliness in two
Sussman, M. B. (1976). The family life of old people. In R.
Northeastern cities. In J. Hartog, J. R. Audy, & Y. A. Cohen
Binstock & E. Shanas (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social
(Eds.), The anatomy of loneliness (pp. 319-337). New York:
sciences (pp. 218-243). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
International Universities Press.
Thibaut, J. W, & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of
Rubenstein, C. M., & Shaver, P. (1982). The experience of groups. New York: Wiley.
loneliness. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A Trout, D. L. (1980). The role of social isolation in suicide. Suicide
sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 206- and Life-Threatening Behavior, 10, 10-23.
223). New York: Wiley-Interscience. Turner, R. J. (1981). Social support as a contingency in psycho-
Rubenstein, C, Shaver, P., & Peplau, L. A. (1979). Loneliness. logical well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22,
Human Nature, 2, 59-65. 357-367.
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised Twentyman, C. T, & Zimering, R. T. (1979). Behavioral training
UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity. of social skills: A critical review. In M. Hersen, R. M. Eisler, &
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472-480. P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in behavior modification (Vol. 7,
Sarason, I. G., Levine, H. M., Basham, R. B., & Sarason, B. R. pp. 319-399). New York: Academic Press.
(1983). Assessing social support: The social support question- Ulrich, R. S., & Addoms, D. L. (1981). Psychological and recre-
naire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 127- ational benefits of a residential park. Journal of Leisure Research,
139. 13, 43-65.
Satran, G. (1978). Notes on loneliness. Journal of the American Vachon, M. I., Lyall, W., Rogers, J., Freedman-Letofsky, K., &
Academy of Psychoanalysis, 6, 281-300. Freeman, S. (1980). A controlled study of a self-help intervention
Schulz, R. (1976). Effects of control and predictability on the for widows. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1380-1384.

1406 December 1984 = American Psychologist


WaUerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. (1977). Divorce counseling: A & M. Killilea (Eds.), Support systems and mutual help: Multi-
community service for families in the midst of divorce. American disciplinary explorations (pp. 213-232). New York: Grune &
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 47, 4-22. Stratton.
Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. (1980). The effects of parental Williams, A. W., Ware, J. E., & Donald, C. A, (1981). A model
divorce: Experiences of the child in later latency. In J. Hartog, of mental health, life events, and social supports applicable to
J, R, Audy, & Y. A. Cohen (Eds.), The anatomy of loneliness general populations. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22,
(pp. 148-169). New York: International Universities Press. 324-336.
Weiss, R. S. (1969). The fund of sociability. Transaction/Society, Yankelovich, D. (1981). New rules: Searching for self-fulfillment
6, 36-43. in a world turned upside down. New York: Random House.
Weiss, R. S. (Ed.). (1973). Loneliness: The experience of emotional Young, J. E. (1982). Loneliness, depression and cognitive therapy:
and social isolation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Theory and application. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.),
Weiss, R. S. (1976). Transition states and other stressful situations: Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy
Their nature and programs for their management. In G. Caplan (pp. 379-405). New York: Wiley-Interscience.

December 1984 • American Psychologist 1407

You might also like