You are on page 1of 12

Augmentative and Alternative Communication

ISSN: 0743-4618 (Print) 1477-3848 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iaac20

Early cognitive skills as prerequisites to


augmentative and alternative communication use:
What are we waiting for?

Kathleen Kangas & Lyle Lloyd

To cite this article: Kathleen Kangas & Lyle Lloyd (1988) Early cognitive skills as prerequisites
to augmentative and alternative communication use: What are we waiting for?, Augmentative
and Alternative Communication, 4:4, 211-221

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07434618812331274817

Published online: 12 Jul 2009.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 202

View related articles

Citing articles: 21 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iaac20

Download by: [Universität Osnabrueck] Date: 07 February 2016, At: 14:53


0743-4618/88/0404-0211$2.00/0
AAC Augmentative and Alternative Communication
Copyright © 1988 by Williams & Wilkins

Early Cognitive Skills As Prerequisites to


Augmentative and Alternative Communication Use:
What Are We Waiting For?
Kathleen A. Kangas and Lyle L. Lloyd
Department of Audiology and Speech Sciences (K.A.K., L.L.L.) and Special Education (L.L.L.), Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907, USA

The observation of correlations between cognitive abilities and language and communication
abilities in normally developing children has led to the assumption that there are specific cognitive
prerequisites to the development of symbolic communication. For many individuals with severe
disabilities, this focus has led to a clinical decision to teach these prerequisite skills or to wait for
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

them to emerge before providing communication programs either for teaching speech or for
introducing augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) techniques. In this paper, literature
regarding cognitive prerequisites to communication development for nondisabled children and for
individuals with disabilities is reviewed. Issues related to providing communication teaching
programs are discussed, and specific intervention strategies are presented.
KEY WORDS: cognitive prerequisites, intervention, mental retardation

Cognitive prerequisites have been assumed to exist is the local homologies hypothesis which states that
for the development of symbolic communication Al- there is a simultaneous emergence of parallel cognition
though there are no convincing data to support a strict and language skills, but that the two do not depend on
cognitive prerequisites hypothesis, reliance on this as- each other for development. The interaction hypothesis
sumption has shaped the development of language and is that the influence between the two domains is mutual
communication intervention for individuals with severe and bidirectional with cognition supporting language
disabilities over the course of the last two decades. and language supporting cognition. The cognition-an-
Decisions to provide the services of a speech/language chored-in-language hypothesis states that cognitive
pathologist and to attempt augmentative and alterna- concepts are unstable until they can be anchored with
tive communication (AAC) approaches have been linguistic forms and, therefore, language supports cog-
based on these assumptions about cognitive prerequi- nition. The strong cognitive hypothesis proposes that
sites. The purpose of this paper is to examine some of cognition causes language to develop. That is, cogni-
the assumptions that have been made regarding cog- tion is necessary before linguistic development, and
nitive prerequisites to language use and the logic of the that linguistic development will follow from the cognitive
decisions which have followed from those assumptions. development. The weak cognitive hypothesis states
It will be argued that descriptive data regarding child that cognition is necessary but is not sufficient for
development have been overextended to a prescriptive language acquisition. In other words, language cannot
sequence of precommunication and communication in- progress without the necessary cognitive base; how-
tervention for individuals with disabilities. Furthermore, ever, achieving the appropriate cognitive levels does
a model of child development may not be a sufficient not necessarily mean that the expected language skills
framework for selecting goals of intervention for chil- will emerge. Rice and Kemper (1984) concluded that
dren and adults who experience severely disabling the relationship between cognition and language is very
conditions and who are functioning at early levels with complex, and no single model is adequate to charac-
respect to developmental norms. terize it. They suggest that language development is
analogous to the growth of a plant, in that it is an
Cognitive Hypotheses organic process with each element of the process
The relationship between cognition and language has dependent on the others, but also supporting and nour-
been the subject of much research and debate. Rice ishing the others.
(1983) and Rice and Kemper (1984) have reviewed
several explanations of the links between cognition Weak Cognitive Hypothesis
and language acquisition. They described six different Of greatest interest here is the weak cognitive hy-
models which seek to explain this relationship and pothesis, because this is the model which appears to
reviewed the literature in support of each. One model have had the greatest influence on programs and serv-
211
212 Kangas and Lloyd

ices for individuals with severe disabilities. Many au- Other investigators have presented correlational data
thors have concluded that the use of symbolic com- to support the cognitive hypothesis of language devel-
munication is directly dependent upon attainment of opment. Corrigan (1978) and Smolak (1982) reported
certain cognitive skills. Cromer (1974, 1976) has delin- a correlation between sensorimotor stage development
eated the weak cognitive hypothesis. While he accepts and the emergence of speech in young children. Smolak
the “importance of. . . cognitive processes in determin- (1982) also considered receptive language skills, and
ing various aspects of language acquisition” (Cromer, she reported only a weak relationship to cognitive
1976, p. 291) he also states that this explanation is development. Lowe (1975) and McCune-Nicolich and
not sufficient to explain language acquisition. He con- Bruskin (1982) found a relationship between the level
tends that “there may be some purely ‘linguistic’ proc- of representational play (e.g., feeding self, feeding a
esses which are also necessary in order to account for doll) and spoken language development for nondisabled
the encoding of thought into language” (Cromer, 1976, children. Kahn (1975) reported correlations between
p. 291). A similar position is described by Slobin (1973) four scales of sensorimotor development (i.e., object
who stated that a child “. . . must have both cognitive permanence, means-ends, causality, and imitation) and
and linguistic discovery procedures available . . .” (p. the presence of single word expressive speech for
186) in order to acquire language. In other words, while children with profound retardation. Lobato, Barrera,
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

cognitive development is necessary for language ac- and Feldman (1981) described a similar relationship
quisition, cognition alone is not sufficient for language between five scales of sensorimotor development (i.e.,
to develop. object permanence, means-ends, causality, vocal imi-
Bates, Benigni, Bretherton, Camaioni, and Volterra tation, and gestural imitation) and gestural communi-
(1977) also discussed cognitive prerequisites to the cation for children and adolescents with severe and
development of language. They concluded that Piaget’s profound retardation. Clink, Ryan, Sommers, and
sensorimotor stage 5 level of development was nec- Schrum (1986) found that children’s performance on
essary for the use of gestural performatives or “pre- sensorimotor scales of mean-ends and relating to ob-
verbal, intentional communication” (p. 251), and that jects was moderately correlated with their progress in
sensorimotor stage 6 development was necessary for language therapy as measured by a battery of speech
the use of words. and language tests. While these studies consistently
An examination of research to support this view that demonstrate some correlation between cognitive and
language development depends on the attainment of language measures, these correlations do not establish
specific cognitive skills reveals little empirical support. any causal link between the cognitive level and the
The conclusions are based mostly on data showing a language abilities. In fact, a careful review of these data
correlation between cognitive development and the seems to suggest that there are individuals who show
emergence of speech and not on experimental findings some use of referential language without showing the
which could demonstrate a cause and effect relation- cognitive level expected, as well as some individuals
ship. An early longitudinal study by Bates, Camaioni, who show the cognitive level of interest without show-
and Volterra (1975) was conducted to describe cogni- ing the emergence of referential speech.
tive and communication skill development from birth to Steckol and Leonard (1981) used an experimental
18 months. The data from this study form the basis for paradigm to investigate the relationship between sen-
their view of cognition supporting language (Bates, sorimotor cognitive development and expressive com-
1976, 1979; Bates et al., 1977). However, this study munication development. They taught means-ends
was based on only three children, and their ages and tasks and schemes for relating to objects (two scales
developmental levels at the beginning of the study were of sensorimotor behavior thought to be related to lan-
different to the degree that their developmental levels guage) to normally developing children who had not yet
overlapped during the course of the investigation. The begun to use words nor to use per-formative behaviors
findings do not reflect three children passing through (e.g., the nonverbal behavior of pointing to request an
the entire span of development studied, and reports of object). The children who were taught schemes for
specific skills at specific points in development are relating to objects demonstrated the greatest increase
based on observations of only child who was at that in pet-formative behaviors when compared to children
point of development. It is therefore difficult to gener- who were taught means-ends tasks or to those who
alize the observations of sequences of skill develop- were not taught either set of tasks. Steckol and Leon-
ment beyond the children studied. A later study by ard (1981) concluded that these data supported a hom-
Bates and her colleagues (Bates, 1979; Bates et al., ologue model of the relationship between sensorimotor
1977) involved 25 children with data recorded cross- development and communication development. That is,
sectionally in four monthly visits. Thus the data were the data suggested that the two areas were developing
less sensitive to sequential or concurrent skill devel- simultaneously, and that while teaching in the cognitive
opment. Even if the sequences of cognitive skills and task did assist communication development, it was not
language development were shown to be invariant, a prerequisite relationship. This conclusion was based
there would still be no convincing evidence of a causal on the observation that the sequence of acquired be-
relationship, because as the authors acknowledge, haviors was not invariant. That is, some children ad-
these data are correlational. vanced in per-formative behaviors and then advanced
Cognitive Prerequisites 213

on the schemes for relating to objects tasks, while a A different approach to communication programs for
prerequisite relationship would predict that the prog- severely disabled individuals is exemplified by curricula
ress in relating to objects tasks would always precede which focus on communicative behavior at a prelinguis-
progress in pet-formative behaviors. tic level (Klein et al., 1981; Sternberg, McNerney, &
Thus, in reviewing available literature, it seems that Pegnatore, 1985; Sternberg, Pegnatore, & Hill, 1983;
there is no clear evidence that language rests on any Stillman & Battle, 1984; Stremel-Campbell, Johnson-
well specified cognitive prerequisite skills. Other au- Darn, Guida, & Udell, 1984). In these programs, there
thors (Guess, 1980; Harris, 1982; Reichle & Karlan, are no prerequisite skills, either in cognitive nor in
1985, 1988; Rice, 1983; Rice & Kemper, 1984; Romski behavioral-task oriented domains. Early communicative
& Sevcik, 1988) have similarly noted an absence of behaviors which do not depend on linguistic symboli-
data to support the cognitive hypothesis of language zation are taught first, thus not excluding any individ-
development. Although it can be readily agreed that uals for failing to demonstrate sufficient skill to enter
there must be some relationship between cognition and the programs. None of these programs address the
language, it should be recognized that the nature of introduction of a formal AAC symbol system, and only
this relationship is not clearly understood. In concluding the Stremel-Campbell et al. (1984) program addresses
her review of the various models which have been a means to move from this prelinguistic level to the
proposed to explain this relationship, Rice (1983) stated beginning use of speech. However, since these pro-
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

that a “striking characteristic of the available literature grams emphasize meaningful communication at an
is the preliminary, tentative character of the available early level, they remain flexible enough to allow for
evidence and the associated arguments” (p. 354). exposure to any symbol set which might eventually be
of communicative use to the individual.
Cognitive Hypothesis: Impact on Intervention As noted above, in addition to the impact on early
speech teaching programs, the development of AAC
In spite of the lack of conclusive evidence to support for individuals with severe disabilities has also been
cognitive prerequisites to language, this assumption influenced by the assumption of cognitive prerequisites
has had an impact on the services for individuals with to symbolic communication. Chapman and Miller (1980)
severe disabilities. Language and communication have stated support for what has here been called the
teaching programs have explicitly or implicitly required weak cognitive hypothesis (i.e., that cognition is nec-
certain prerequisite skills. Several authors (Chapman & essary but not sufficient for language to emerge). They
Miller, 1980; Owens & House, 1984; Shane, 1981) have extend this view to the application of AAC approaches
recommended that the appropriate course of interven- when they state that it is only a child “with production
tion for an individual who has not achieved the cognitive delay relative to cognitive level, rather than to chrono-
prerequisites to speech is to teach these cognitive logical age, who is a candidate for a nonvocal system”
prerequisite skills first. These recommendations will be (page 163). Similarly, Shane (1981) has stated that a
discussed below. Although there will be no attempt cognitive development of at least sensorimotor stage 5
here to provide a comprehensive review of communi- or at least 18 months mental age is necessary in order
cation programs, some discussion of the general ap- to consider an AAC approach. He further states that
proaches to the notion of prerequisites is in order. there must be some evidence for the delay to be
Many speech and language programs, although re- peripheral, or motor based, before implementing an
ported to be appropriate for individuals with severe AAC technique. Owens and House (1984) appear to
disabilities, are in fact designed for individuals who have propose the most exclusionary criteria. They state that
surpassed the level that is of interest here. These the individual must show cognitive skills at sensorimo-
programs, such as the program by Stremel and Waryas tor stage 5 or at a developmental level of age 12 months
(1974) and that by Guess, Sailor, and Baer (1976), in in addition to demonstrating certain social/communi-
fact require the presence of speech as entry level cative skills and receptive language abilities before AAC
criteria, thus obviating the need to consider prerequi- approaches should be considered. Although they refer
sites to speech. Another approach has been taken by to these skills as correlates of expressive language,
some highly structured behavioral programs such as they recommend their use as prerequisite criteria. In
that by Kent (1974). While not requiring any specific proposing these requirements, these authors have not
cognitive prerequisite skills, this program requires cer- only accepted the cognitive hypothesis as the expla-
tain task behaviors such as visual attending and motor nation for the natural and normal emergence of lan-
imitation. The NonSLIP program (Carrier, 1976; Carrier guage in nondisabled children; they have also extended
& Peak, 1975) requires similar task skills, although this this hypothesis to a definitive restriction on the devel-
program is designed to teach the use of nonspeech opment of communication intervention programs for
symbols. These programs share an underlying assump- individuals with developmental delay. Chapman and
tion that there are certain prerequisite skills needed Miller (1980), Owens and House (1984) and Shane
before an individual can benefit from the specific com- (1981) have implied that if there is a cognitive delay
munication training program. There is no provision in which has contributed to the delayed language devel-
these programs for individuals who fail to demonstrate opment, then there is nothing that a speech and lan-
or to learn the required prerequisite tasks. guage clinician can do to improve the individual’s com-
214 Kangas and Lloyd

munication. This implication will be challenged in the same way as it does for nondisabled children. Clink et
arguments which follow. Reichle and Karlan (1985, al. (1986) reported that cognitive level and improvement
1988) in their review of decision rules for the implemen- in a language intervention program were only moder-
tation of AAC, reported that these rules are two-tiered ately correlated for children with language delay. Al-
systems, first requiring the establishment of some level though the specific reason for this phenomenon is not
of severity of speech and language impairment and known, it is clear that for a significantly large number
then requiring some prerequisite condition. They con- of individuals with developmental delay measures of
clude that this leaves many individuals “trapped in a ‘no cognitive abilities do not predict the development of
man’s land’ of having a sufficiently severe impairment language abilities to the same degree that they do for
but failing to have the prerequisites necessary for an nondisabled learners. This suggests that prevention of
augmentative system” (1985, p. 149). more severe communication deficits may be an appro-
priate goal for communication intervention for individ-
Application to Individuals with Disabilities uals with developmental delay.
As Karlan and Lloyd (in press) point out, Piaget’s
It has often been accepted without question that explanation of how the child develops through these
what holds true for the development of typical children cognitive stages relies heavily on the child’s interactions
with his environment. Indeed, the very name of the
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

must also be true for children who experience disabili-


ties. This assumption also must be carefully examined. developmental period which has been of greatest inter-
While information from normally developing children can est to the study of language development, the senso-
lead us into many productive areas for intervention and rimotor period, is derived from the observation that the
research, we need to constantly ask whether or not the child is using sensory input and motor skills to explore
same conditions and relationships will apply to other and understand the environment. For individuals with
populations. Karlan and Lloyd (in press) reviewed avail- disabilities, their early experience with the world is
able literature relating Piagetian assessment to learners altered by their disabilities. The difference for children
with disabilities. They concluded that, in general, learn- with physical disabilities is obvious; they are limited in
ers with disabilities appeared to progress through the their abilities to explore and to act on their surround-
stages of sensorimotor development in an ordinal fash- ings. Likewise, it is easy to see that children with
ion, much as nondisabled learners do. That is, they sensory impairments have an altered experience with
acquire skills at one stage of performance before be- the environment. What is not so obvious, however, is
coming able to succeed at the next stage within each how this will affect their early cognitive development.
behavioral area. However, Karlan and Lloyd (in press) Children who have cognitive deficits, even with good
also reported that there does not appear to be as much motor skills and sensory abilities, may also have an
congruence across behavioral areas for the learners altered experience with the world. These children may
with disabilities as there was for nondisabled children. not engage in the same exploration of their environ-
That is, the learners with disabilities appeared to show ment, they might not attend to and organize sensory
more variability by functioning at early stages in some perceptions in the same way, and their parents may
behavioral areas and later stages in other behavioral behave somewhat differently toward them. Thus, in
domains. They hypothesized that differences in expe- using a theory which relies heavily on experience, we
rience and task variables contributed to this reduced must be very cautious about extending our assump-
congruence for learners with disabilities. Karlan (1980) tions from normally developing children with normal
reported that he obtained widely different develop- experiences to learners with disabilities who may have
mental assessment results for individuals with severe very different experiential opportunities.
cognitive impairment when he compared their perform- The question of extending our assumptions becomes
ance using standard items or using individually pre- even more critical as we talk about older individuals
ferred items. Whatever the cause for variable perform- who still may not be showing the early cognitive skills
ance in individuals with disabilities, it clearly casts doubt we expect from young normally developing individuals.
on the practice of assigning a single stage description Karlan and Lloyd (in press) recommend caution in inter-
of development. Indicating that an individual is function- preting data from such assessments with adults. They
ing a sensorimotor stage 5 or stage 6 has little meaning noted the scarcity of data on Piagetian assessment
unless the specific measures, procedures, and nature tasks with older individuals and, in particular, they
of the materials used to determine that stage are also raised a question of test-retest reliability with this pop-
reported. ulation. Calculator (1988) also questioned the efficacy
Several researchers (Cardoso-Martins, Mervis, & of applying norms derived from child development to
Mervis, 1985; Cunningham, Glenn, Wilkinson, & Sloper, the communication assessment of adults with mental
1985; Smith & vonTelzchner, 1986) have reported that retardation.
there appears to be a significant delay of language Very little data are available to demonstrate the re-
abilities with respect to cognitive development for indi- lationship of cognitive measures to communication abil-
viduals who have Down syndrome. For at least this one ities for adults and adolescents with severe disabilities.
population of individuals it would seem that cognitive Snyder-McLean, Etter-Schroeder, and Rogers (1986)
development does not predict linguistic ability in the reported data from three groups of adolescents and
Cognitive Prerequisites 215

adults with severe disabilities. They found that the ferent than those correlated with learning spoken
communication level was a poor predictor of perform- words.
ance on sensorimotor assessment scales. Although in In summary, there are many reasons to question the
general there was some correlation, and the more earlier assumptions which led clinicians and teachers
verbal individuals usually scored higher than individuals to either wait for prerequisite skills to develop or to
who were nonverbal (meaning nonlinguistic in any sym- teach cognitive prerequisites before beginning to teach
bol system), there were many contradictory scores in the use of AAC techniques for individuals with severe
both directions. disabilities. There is considerable doubt as to the true
nature of the relationship between cognition and lin-
Application to Nonspeech Symbol Sets guistic development for normally developing children. It
has been suggested that even if that relationship were
Regardless of the individuals involved, we must also better understood, we must carefully examine the ex-
ask if the cognitive abilities related to the use of verbal/ tent to which that relationship would hold true for
vocal symbols (i.e., spoken words) are also necessary individuals who are disabled and for learning linguistic
for the use of other symbols. The symbols used in symbols other than speech. These issues support a
nonspeech communication methods are not a direct position that there are no empirically supported reasons
substitute for speech, and there are problems in draw- to wait for cognitive prerequisites to develop before
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

ing direct parallels between components of different beginning a communication program. But are there
symbol systems (Harris & Vanderheiden, 1980; Rom- compelling positive reasons for early intervention?
ski, Lloyd, & Sevcik, 1988; Romski & Sevcik, 1988;
Romski, Sevcik, & Joyner, 1984; Vanderheiden & Implications for Early Intervention
Lloyd, 1986). Each symbol system has its own char-
acteristics and has a different effect on overall interac- Questions of when to initiate language intervention
tion patterns. Several authors (Fristoe & Lloyd, 1979; have previously been asked with regard to initiating
Musselwhite & St. Louis, 1988; Silverman, 1980) have signing for very young deaf children. In summarizing
reported, for example, that for many individuals manual the arguments and research in this area, Wilbur (1987)
signs are easier to learn than spoken words, and they makes reference to information regarding the develop-
may not have the same cognitive demands. There are ment of the brain and the establishment of synaptic
several reports which indicate that children who are patterns. This development of the central nervous sys-
acquiring American Sign Language in an environment tem is most rapid up until the age of 5 or 6 years, and
where it is used in a natural communicative manner so the time from birth to age 6 is viewed as the most
obtain early language skills at younger ages than chil- critical for language learning. It should be stressed that
dren learning spoken language. According to these this argument refers to a physiological development
reports, first signs emerge as early as 8 months of age that is related to chronological age, and thus the same
and sign combinations occur as early as 10 months of critical period could be expected to be important for a
age (McIntyre, 1977; Schlesinger, 1978). Also, the total child who is cognitively delayed. This is consistent with
number of signs acquired by children learning signs Alpert’s (1980) report that for children with autism there
exceeds the average vocabulary of spoken words re- is a greater chance for improved social development if
ported for children learning speech, at least until the speech is present by age 5.
age of 2 years (Holmes & Holmes, 1980; McIntyre, Many authors have concluded that the use of AAC
1977; Orlansky & Bonvillian, 1984; Schlesinger, 1978). may have a facilitative effect on communication abilities.
This suggests that signs may not have the same cog- Reichle and Karlan (1985, 1988), Romski, Lloyd, and
nitive-developmental demands as spoken words. Sevcik (1988) Romski and Sevcik (1988), and Romski
Graphic symbols differ from speech and manual sign et al. (1984) suggest that AAC may facilitate compre-
in that they are fixed or static, and the user relies on hension skills. Fristoe and Lloyd (1979) conclude that
recognition memory to select a graphic symbol, not signing facilitates production of vocal/verbal reper-
recall of the symbol (Romski, Lloyd, & Sevcik, 1988; toires. They further report that teaching sign has no
Romski et al., 1984; Vanderheiden & Lloyd, 1986). adverse effect upon acquisition of vocal communica-
Reichle and Yoder (1985) demonstrated labeling skills tion. There are many reasons why the use of AAC
using a communication board could be taught to a child systems may have a facilitating effect on speech (Fris-
who had not reached the 12 month developmental level. toe & Lloyd, 1979; Musselwhite & St. Louis, 1988;
Romski, Sevcik, & Pate (1988) reported that their sub- Romski, Lloyd, & Sevcik, 1988; Silverman, 1980).
jects with severe mental retardation were able to learn Among these possible reasons are the reduced pres-
arbitrary graphic symbols to request specific food sure for verbal speech, the static nature of many of the
items. These subjects later learned additional symbols aided symbols used, and the development of input/
to request nonfood items and generalized the use of output modes other than the traditional auditory-vocal
the symbols to labeling, comprehension, and initiation. channel. Romski and Sevcik (1988) discussed the po-
Although the evidence is scant, these reports do sug- tential importance of augmented communicative input
gest that for some AAC symbol systems, the cognitive in the development of communication. We might also
skills correlated with learning the symbols may be dif- assume that successful communication will help to
216 Kangas and Lloyd

encourage the individual and to develop more appro- they believe no progress in language development will
priate interaction patterns. occur until the cognitive skills are demonstrated. In
Harris and Vanderheiden (1980) and Scheifelbusch addition to the stated cognitive hypothesis, there are
(1980) advocate early communication intervention for two assumptions implicit in this argument: that there is
children with severe disabilities. They point out that no progress unless the client advances in a develop-
communication begins at birth and many skills are mental sequence, and that the communicative needs
achieved before the child begins to use symbolic com- of the individual are comparable to the cognitive level
munication. There is an extensive body of literature that without reference to the chronological age.
describes normal child language development, including Let us first review the assumption that only advances
many sources which describe communication in a to higher developmental levels are to be viewed as
broader context than linguistic skills (Bloom & Lahey, progress. This notion assumes that once an individual
1978; Brown, 1973; Kearns, 1984; Lewis & Rosenblum, achieves a certain level, all of the skills that are com-
1977; Muma, 1986; Ochs & Schieffelen, 1979; Schie- parable to that level will be achieved. This idea has
felbusch & Bricker, 1979; Wood, 1976). Bates and her already been partly addressed by showing that there is
colleagues (Bates, 1979; Bates et al., 1977; Bates et a lesser congruence of skills across cognitive areas for
al., 1975) described four communicative behaviors that individuals with disabilities than for those who are non-
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

emerged between the ages of 9 and 13 months of age disabled. It may also be that individuals with disabilities
for the normally developing children they observed, and are more restricted in their applications of skills. As
which they believe are highly related to the later use of previously discussed, observations of infant behavior
symbolic, spoken communication. These behaviors tell us that from a very early age children engage in a
were Showing, Giving, Communicative Pointing, and variety of communication behaviors (Bates, 1976,
Ritual Requests. Bruner (1975a, 1975b) described the 1979; Bates et al., 1975; Bates et al., 1977; Bloom &
acquisition of formal language as the adoption of con- Lahey, 1978; Bruner, 1975b; Lewis & Rosenblum,
ventional means to express communication acts previ- 1977; Moerk, 1977; Ochs & Schieffelen, 1979; Schie-
ously developed within the contexts of joint attention felbusch & Bricker, 1979; Wood, 1976). For example,
and joint activity. These observations suggest that a child of 9 or 10 months old may use pointing gestures
within the domain of communicative development, we to request objects, to show objects or events, or to
can extend our attention to skills and behaviors which continue a showing off routine. These behaviors may
occur much earlier than the emergence of symbolic be seen before the child achieves sensorimotor stage
linguistic abilities. 5 or 6 level of development, the levels usually referred
Among the reasons cited by Harris and Vanderheiden to as prerequisite to language. It may be that the learner
(1980) for early intervention is to help the child to with disabilities who is at the same developmental level
develop physical and manipulative skills which the child may demonstrate the same pointing behavior, but may
might later use to access a communication device. use that behavior in a much more restricted set; for
There is a compelling logic here which might again be example, only to request a food which is already
related to what we know of normal child development. nearby. If the use of that skill can be expanded to a
If normally developing children use babbling and more frequent and more flexible usage, this in itself
thereby gain control of their speech articulators prior to may be an acceptable outcome of intervention. For a
producing those sounds for meaningful use, it seems child who is physically disabled, it may be appropriate
reasonable that a child with disabilities might benefit to develop a behavior other than pointing, such as eye-
from increasing his nonsymbolic and playlike use of gaze, to substitute for the directed point.
whatever motor patterns he might later access for The second assumption which is implicit in accepting
communication purposes. developmental goals in relation only to cognitive devel-
opment is that an individual’s communicative needs are
Implications for Goals of Intervention comparable to those of a normally developing child of
the same cognitive level. Based on this logic, if an adult
Authors who support some form of the cognitive with severe cognitive delay displays only the commu-
hypothesis of language development make an assump- nication behaviors of a normally developing infant or
tion that normal language development is the most toddler, that adult might not be considered for speech
appropriate reference for the development of interven- and language intervention nor the possibility of AAC
tion programs. These authors frequently recommend techniques. However, the needs of an adult with severe
that if cognitive prerequisite skills are not present, disabilities are very different than those of a young
communication intervention programs should be de- child. When a child is very young, we expect the ma-
layed or should be directed toward the goal of teaching jority of his communication to be with family and highly
or facilitating the prerequisite skills (Chapman & Miller, familiar care givers, we are very tolerant of interrup-
1980; Owens & House, 1984; Shane, 1981). The rea- tions, we accept unintelligible or idiosyncratic words,
soning seems to be that there are prerequisite skills and we have little concern for the overall independence
necessary for the development of truly symbolic, rule of the child. However, for an adult with developmental
based language. Since these authors believe the lan- delay, our goals are quite different. We attempt to
guage development is based on the cognitive skills, establish the highest possible level of independence
Cognitive Prerequisites 217

(Bender & Valletutti, 1985; Brown, Branston, Hamre- Once having established a small set of movement sig-
Nietupski, Pumpian, Certo, & Gruenewald, 1979; Snell, nals, it might be expected that the learner could pro-
1983; Valletutti & Sims-Tucker, 1984). With recent gress to acquiring more conventional gestures.
emphasis on preventing or discontinuing residential 2. Use of a signal to request “more” (Klein et al.,
placements and the growth of a variety of community 1981). The first request taught in this program is a
living arrangements for individuals with disabilities signal for “more.” A high-preference item or activity is
(Amary, 1980; Gollay, Freedman, Wyngaarden, & presented, and subsequently withdrawn or interrupted.
Kurtz, 1978), intervention for communication skills must The interventionist first pauses to allow the learner to
place greater emphasis on socially acceptable behavior respond and then, if necessary, prompts the desired
and on independent functioning with known and un- signal. The signal is reinforced by reintroducing the
known communicative partners. For example, we may object or restarting the activity.
wish to see the individual be capable of asking for This approach is somewhat similar to the develop-
assistance to locate an item in the grocery store, or to ment of movement signals described above. It also
tell a work supervisor that the work is finished, or to shares features with Keogh & Reichle’s (1985) program
request the proper shoe size at a bowling alley. The (described below) in that a single response is trained
appropriate selection and teaching of AAC approaches and used across several items or activities. In their
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

will facilitate these goals for some individuals without discussion, Klein et al. (1981) stress the flexibility of
advancing the developmental level of either the cogni- this approach with respect to selecting the signal to be
tive or the language abilities. taught, as virtually any behavior that the individual can
control could be used as a signal.
Specific Goals and Intervention Strategies 3. Use of a generalized symbol to represent “Want”
(Keogh & Reichle, 1985). This program is designed to
There are several communication intervention pro- teach the use of a single generalized symbol to repre-
grams and descriptions of specific teaching procedures sent a request in a contextually appropriate situation.
which would be appropriate for individuals who dem- In the presence of a preferred item, the interventionist
onstrate limited cognitive skills. Some of these pro- prompts the selection of the Want symbol, and rein-
grams and procedures are described below. This listing forces the symbol selection with the desired item.
is not exhaustive of the interventions which have been Although Keogh & Reichle (1985) described this pro-
described in the literature, nor is it complete with re- gram only for use with graphic symbols, it could easily
spect to the possible intervention approaches. The be adapted for the teaching of manual signs or ges-
suggestions listed below have been selected as appro- tures. They recommended that this program be insti-
priate for individuals with limited cognitive abilities, and tuted concurrently with the program to teach labeling,
as representative of the range of teaching approaches described below.
which are available. This listing should not be miscon- 4. Use of specific object names to label salient items
strued as a sequence or hierarchy of skills to be taught. (Keogh & Reichle, 1985). This program teaches the
The most effective communication intervention will usu- learner to use object names as a means of expressing
ally be developed by selecting or adapting several of a description or comment communicative function. The
these suggestions, and providing an individual with authors recommend teaching the labeling behavior con-
many different opportunities to develop functional com- currently with the above described teaching of a gen-
munication. eralized request function.
1. Development of movement patterns to signal spe- Keogh and Reichle (1985) stress the importance of
cific activities (Sternberg, McNerney, & Pegnatore, teaching separate vocabulary items for different com-
1985; Sternberg, Pegnatore, & Hill, 1983; Stillman & municative functions in the early stages of communi-
Battle, 1984). This three stage program begins with cation intervention. The object names are later com-
movement resonance, where the learner and the inter- bined with the generalized request to form specific
ventionist are physically touching and move together. requests for specific objects (i.e., Want + object name).
At this stage, and throughout the program, the inter- 5. Use of Lexigrams on a computer keyboard to
ventionist pauses frequently and allows the learner an request specific items (Romski, Sevcik, & Pate, 1988).
opportunity to signal in some way that the activity In the presence of a communicative need (i.e., food to
should begin again. From this, coactive movement is be obtained), the learner is prompted to select the
developed, where the two continue to do the same symbol representing that food item on a computer-
activity at the same time, but separated in space. The based keyboard. Prompts used include pauses, spoken
third stage is imitative movement, where the two imitate directions, gestures, and physical prompts. A correct
each other and movement signals for the activities are response is reinforced with the requested item. A re-
continued. quest for a single item is taught to criterion before the
One advantage of this approach, is that it is not next item and symbol are introduced.
necessary to establish visual abilities in order to begin Romski, Sevcik, & Pate (1988) stressed that the
the resonance phase, and in fact the original program structured teaching methods were the most important
was developed by VanDijk (cited in Stillman & Battle, decision in the design of the intervention program.
1984) for use with students who were deaf and blind. Although they used arbitrary graphic symbols, Lexi-
218 Kangas and Lloyd

grams, this teaching approach could be used with any 9. Use of various communicative functions (Stremel-
symbol set, either graphic or manual. Additionally, al- Campbell et al., 1984, Area VI Communication Func-
though the program described used a computer key- tions, Programs No. 1 through No. 10, pp. 373-391).
board and a computer activated vending system to This program includes descriptions of procedures to
dispense the food in the first stages of the program, establish 10 different communicative functions that may
the basic approach could be used without the computer be expressed without the use of language symbols.
equipment. As with Keogh and Reichle’s (1985) ap- The first five are recommended to be taught first, and
proach, this program teaches object names. However, they are Protesting & Rejecting; Request for Objects;
this program teaches names in a request paradigm, Request for Action; Naming Objects for Attention, Con-
whereas Keogh and Reichle recommended introducing firmation, or Reaction; and Call for Attention. When
object names in a labeling task. these are relatively well established, another set of five
6. Selection of graphic symbols paired with synthetic are introduced, and these are Offering Objects, Repe-
speech output to request items (Romski, 1987; Romski, tition, Requests for Permission, Asking Questions, and
Sevcik, & Washburn, 1987). Romski and her colleagues Answering Questions. These communicative functions
(Romski, 1987; Romski, Sevcik, & Washburn, 1987) are integrated throughout the many individual programs
paired synthetic speech output with the selection of the included in the total curriculum.
graphic symbols to request specific items in a commu- The concurrent teaching of several different com-
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

nication program similar to that described above (Rom- municative skills is a strength of this program. Keogh
ski, Sevcik, & Pate, 1988). They reported that the and Reichle (1985) also stressed concurrently teaching
synthetic speech output appeared to facilitate the sym- different responses for different functions; however,
bol learning by their subjects with severe mental retar- they described only two communicative functions. By
dation. They speculated that the highly consistent na- focusing on 5 or 10 of the communicative functions as
ture of the synthetic speech may have contributed to recommended by Stremel-Campbell et al. (1984), inter-
the subjects’ learning. ventionists will find many opportunities to integrate
Although this technique has only recently been de- communication teaching into a wide variety of activities.
scribed in the literature (Romski & Sevcik, 1988), the 10. Response to touch cues (Stremel-Campbell et al.,
use of voice output communication aids by individuals 1984, Area IV Receptive Communication, Program No.
with severe mental retardation shows the potential to 5, pp. 205-207). Physical touch cues are paired with
be an effective approach to communication interven- specific spoken directions to improve the receptive
tion. communication for direction following. Physical assist-
7. Indication of a choice from an array of two or more ance is initially provided in order to establish pairing of
(Klein et al., 1981). The learner is first presented with a the behavior with the touch cue, and pauses and re-
single choice, a highly preferred item. When a signal duced physical assistance are used to establish com-
has been established to represent that choice, the array pliance with the direction paired with cue in the absence
changes to a two choice system, first containing one of other physical assistance.
highly preferred and one nonpreferred item, and later One important contribution of this program is the
containing two highly preferred items. focus on receptive communication. If the selected di-
This program is somewhat similar to Romski, Sevcik, rections and cues are in the context of functional daily
and Pate’s (1988) program which began with a request activities, then the program will directly increase the
for a single item. These programs differ in that Klein et learner’s active participation in those activities. Al-
al. (1981) primarily discussed nonsymbolic means of though the examples cited by Stremel-Campbell et al.
indicating a choice, such as pointing or eye gazing at (1984) appear to be most appropriate for individuals
the desired item. who have relatively intact motor skills, the same tech-
8. Indication of protest or rejection (Stremel-Campbell niques could be used with individuals who experience
et al., 1984, Area V Expressive Language, Program severe physical impairment. For example, one might
No. 3, pp. 271-272). Physical assistance is provided in increase the extent to which the learner participates in
order to teach the learner to push away unwanted a dressing routine by teaching the person to begin to
items. This requires an interventionist who can antici- roll onto one side when given a specific spoken direction
pate what the individual does not want, or one who is and touch cue.
sensitive to the learner’s indications of displeasure. 11. Development and use of communication in natural
The development of the ability to protest or reject settings (Fey, 1986; Halle, 1982; Hart, 1985; Hart &
items and activities can be a very powerful skill for an Risley, 1975, 1986; Hart & Rogers-Warren, 1978;
individual and can be taught concurrently with request- MacDonald, 1985; Warren & Rogers-Warren, 1985).
ing. This ability may help to prevent or reduce negative There are several teaching strategies which may be
behaviors such as tantrums or self-abuse (Donnellan, loosely grouped under the heading of naturalistic teach-
Mirenda, Mesaros, & Fassbender, 1984). Once the ing strategies. These include such areas as environ-
behavior of physically pushing away the unwanted item mental arrangements to increase the opportunity and
is established, the action might be abbreviated to a desire to communicate, appropriate cueing techniques
simple gesture of the hand raised. to elicit the appropriate communication behavior, highly
Cognitive Prerequisites 219

responsive consequences for communicating, and rec- to the development of symbolic communication. The
ommendations for working with parents, caretakers, assumption that such skills are prerequisite to language
and teachers. learning has impacted on the treatment programs for
These methods may be an effective adjunct to many learners with severe disabilities and has often led to
of the specific communication programs described delaying or denying the provision of AAC methods. It
above. The naturalistic methods may be employed to appears that there are not sufficient data to support
establish new behaviors or to facilitate generalization the view that certain skills, such as attainment of Pi-
of behaviors taught in a more structured setting. A aget’s sensorimotor stage 5 or stage 6, are necessary
critical feature in the implementation of this approach for the development of language in normally developing
is that all people who interact with the individual with children. Furthermore, caution is suggested when ap-
disabilities (e.g., parents, teachers, caregivers, and plying conclusions from the study of normally develop-
work supervisors) are involved in the development of ing children to the treatment of individuals with disabil-
communication. ities. It has been shown that there are not sufficient
Whatever the communication program selected, it is reasons for delaying the start of communication pro-
necessary to carefully differentiate the outward topog- grams for individuals with severe disabilities and, in
raphy of the communication from the individual’s use fact, there are some compelling reasons for beginning
and understanding of the message. Even when using communication intervention at a young age even if
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

a communication technique that involves language certain cognitive skills have not been attained.
symbols (e.g., operation of a tape loop which has a
recorded message to request attention, or pointing to Acknowledgments
a graphic symbol that represents an item), the individual The authors wish to thank members of the Purdue
may still lack the understanding of the symbols and University Augmentative and Alternative Communica-
therefore be restricted in their use. For example, a tape tion Research Group for their comments on an earlier
loop with a message that calls for attention could be draft of this paper. Preparation of this manuscript was
used in a symbolic manner each time the user desires supported in part by Grant No. G008630079 from the
some specific form of attention. The symbolic nature of Office of Special Education and Rehabilitation Services
the use would be suggested by the AAC user’s atten- and by a David Ross research fellowship at Purdue
tion to whether or not a listener is available, and by the University.
use of the tape loop in novel situations where attention Address reprint requests to: Kathleen A. Kangas,
or help is necessary. Alternatively, however, it might be M.S.P.A., Doctoral Student, Purdue University, Depart-
activated as part of a well established routine, such as ment of Audiology and Speech Sciences, Heavilon Hall,
going into the bedroom, facing the bed, activating the West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA.
call signal, and moving with assistance from a wheel-
chair to the bed. In this case, a communication partner
might over-interpret the usage as symbolic even when REFERENCES
the person requiring assistance does not understand
Alpert, C. (1980). Procedures for determining the optimal nonspeech
the nature of the message. The individual’s response, mode with the autistic child. In R.C. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Nonspeech
if attention and help are not immediately forthcoming, language and communication: Analysis and intervention (pp. 389-
might provide a clue as to the actual understanding. Or 420). Baltimore: University Park Press.
the tape could be activated simply as a response which Amary, I. B. (1980). The rights of the mentally retarded-develop-
mentally disabled to treatment and education. Springfield, IL:
has a rich history of reinforcement, but which is not
Charles C. Thomas.
associated with a specific need for attention nor with Bates, E. (1976). Language and context, the acquisition of pragmatics.
the presence of a potential listener. While any of these New York: Academic Press.
uses may be appropriate at some point in an interven- Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols, cognition and commu-
tion program, it is important to recognize the extent to nication in infancy. New York: Academic Press. With L. Benigni, I.
Bretherton, L. Camaioni, & V. Volterra.
which the AAC user understands the symbolic nature Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
of the messages in order to determine the most effec- (1977). From gesture to the first word: On cognitive and social
tive next step in a program sequence. prerequisites. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Interaction,
Throughout this discussion of specific strategies for conversation, and the development of language (pp. 247-307).
intervention, the emphasis has been on functional com- New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of
munication. These strategies are designed to give the performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 247-
individual some immediate power over the environment 307.
and to provide a means of taking a more active role in Bender, M., & Valletutti, P. J. (1985). Teaching the moderately and
daily activities. The selection of these recommended severely handicapped, Vol. I (2nd ed). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language
approaches to intervention is based on the belief that disorders. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
all individuals are ready to communicate. Brown, L., Branston, M. B., Hamre-Nietupski, S., Pupian, I., Certo,
Summary N., & Gruenewald, L. (1979). A strategy for developing chronolog-
ical-age-appropriate and functional curricular content for severely
In summary, questions have been raised as to the handicapped adolescents and young adults. The Journal of Special
validity of accepting any strict cognitive prerequisites Education, 13, 81-90.
220 Kangas and Lloyd

Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- Hart, B. (1985). Naturalistic language training techniques. In S. F.
sity Press. Warren, & A. K. Rogers-Warren (Eds.), Teaching functional lan-
Bruner, J. S. (1975a). Entry info early language: A spiral curriculum. guage : Generalization and maintenance of language skills (pp. 63-
The Charles Gittins Memorial Lecture at University College of 88). Baltimore: University Park Press.
Swansea, March 13. Morriston, Swansea: Crown Printers. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1975). Incidental teaching of language in the
Bruner, J. S. (1975b). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of preschool. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8, 411-420.
Child Language, 2, 1-19. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1986). Incidental strategies. In R. L. Schie-
Calculator, S. N. (1988). Exploring the language of adults with mental felbusch (Ed.), Language competence: Assessment and interven-
retardation. In S. N. Calculator, & J. L. Bedrosian (Eds.), Commu- tion (pp. 213-225). San Diego: College-Hill Press.
nication assessment and intervention for adults with mental retar- Hart, B., & Rogers-Warren, A. (1978). A milieu approach to language
dation (pp. 95-106). Boston: College-Hill Press. teaching. In R. L. Schiefelbush (Ed.), Language intervention strat-
Cardoso-Martins, C., Mervis, C., & Mervis, C. (1985). Early vocabu- egies (pp. 193-235). Baltimore: University Park Press.
lary acquisition by children with Down syndrome. American Journal Holmes, K. M., & Holmes, D. W. (1980). Signed and spoken language
of Mental Deficiency, 90, 255-265. development in a hearing child of hearing parents. Sign Language
Carrier, J. (1976). Application of a nonspeech language system with Studies, 28, 239-254.
the severely language handicapped. In L. Lloyd (Ed.), Communi- Kahn, J. (1975). Relationship of Piaget’s sensorimotor period to
cation assessment and intervention strategies (pp. 523-547). Bal- language acquisition of profoundly retarded children. American
timore: University Park Press. Journal of Mental Deficiency, 79, 640-643.
Carrier, J., & Peak, T. (1975). Non-SLIP (non-speech language initia- Karlan, G. R. (1980). The effects of preference for objects and
tion program). Lawrence, KS: H & H Enterprises, Inc. repeated measures upon the assessed level of object permanence
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

Chapman, R., & Miller, J. (1980). Analyzing language and communi- and means/end ability in severely handicapped students. Journal
cation in the child. In R. C. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Nonspeech lan- of the Association for the Severely Handicapped, 5, 174-193.
guage and communication: Acquisition and intervention (pp. 159- Karlan, G. R., & Lloyd, L. L. (in press). Communication intervention
196). Baltimore: University Park Press. for the severely handicapped. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Clink, P., Ryan, C., Sommers, R., & Schrum, D. (1986, November). Kearns, J. T. (1984). Using language: The structures of speech acts.
Sensorimotor stages and severely handicapped children’s lan- Albany: State University of New York Press.
guage performances. Paper presented at the ASHA Annual Con- Kent, L. (1974). Language acquisition program for the severely re-
vention, Detroit. tarded. Champaign, IL: Research Press.
Corrigan, R. (1978). Language development as related to stage 6 Keogh, W. J., & Reichle, J. (1985). Communication intervention for
object permanence development. Journal of Child Language, 5, the “difficult-to-teach” severely handicapped. In S. F. Warren, & A.
173-189. K. Rogers-Warren (Eds.), Teaching functional language: Generali-
Cromer, R. (1974). The development of language and cognition: The zation and maintenance of language skills (pp. 157-194). Baltimore:
cognitive hypothesis. In B. Foss (Ed.), New perspectives in child University Park Press.
development (pp. 184-252). Harmondsworth, Middlesex, England: Klein, M. D., Wulz, S. V., Hall, M. K., Waldo, L. J., Carpenter, S. A.,
Penguin Books Ltd. Lathan, D. A., Meyers, S. P., Fox, T., & Marshall, A. M. (1981).
Cromer, R. (1976). The cognitive hypothesis of language acquisition Comprehensive communication curriculum guide. Kansas Early
and its implications for child language deficiency. In D. Morehead, Childhood Institute (ECI Document No. 902) Haworth Hall, Univer-
& A. Morehead (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. sity of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045.
283-333). Baltimore: University Park Press. Lewis, M., & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.) (1977). Interaction, conversation
Cunningham, C., Glenn, S., Wilkinson, P., & Sloper, P. (1985). Mental and the development of language. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
ability, symbolic play and receptive and expressive language of Lobato, D., Barrera, R. D., & Feldman, R. S. (1981). Sensorimotor
young children with Down’s syndrome. The Journal of Child Psy- functioning and prelinguistic communication of severely and pro-
chology Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 26, 255-265. foundly retarded individuals. American Journal of Mental Deficiency,
Donnellan, A. M., Mirenda, P. L., Mesaros, R. A., & Fassbender, L. 85, 489-496.
L., (1984). Analyzing the communicative functions of aberrant Lowe, M. (1975). Trends in the development of representational play
behavior. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Hand- in infants from one to three years: An observational study. Journal
icaps, 9, 201-212. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 33-47.
Fey, M. E. (1986). Language intervention with young children. Boston: MacDonald, J. D. (1985). Language through conversation: A model
Little, Brown & Company. for intervention with language-delayed persons. In S. F. Warren, &
Fristoe, M., & Lloyd, L. (1979). Nonspeech communication. In N. R. A. K. Rogers-Warren (Eds.), Teaching functional language: Gen-
Ellis (Ed.), Handbook of mental deficiency: Psychological theory eralization and maintenance of language skills (pp. 89-122). Austin,
and research (2nd ed.) (pp. 401-430). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence TX: Pro-Ed.
Erlbaum Assoc. McCune-Nicolich, L., & Bruskin, C. (1982). Combinatorial competency
Gollay, E., Freedman, R., Wyngaarden, M., & Kurtz, N. R. (1978). in symbolic play and language. In D. J. Pepler, & K. H. Rublin
Coming back: The community experiences of deinstitutionalized (Eds.), The play of children: Current theory and research. New
mentally retarded people. Cambridge, MA: Abt Books. York: S. Karger.
Guess, D. (1980). Methods in communication instruction for severely McIntyre, M. L. (1977). The acquisition of American Sign Language
handicapped persons. In W. Sailor, B. Wilcox, & L. Brown (Eds.), hand configurations. Sign Language Studies, 16, 247-266.
Methods of instruction for severely handicapped students (pp. 195- Moerk, E. L. (1977). Pragmatic and semantic aspects of early lan-
225). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. guage development. Baltimore: University Park Press.
Guess, D., Sailor, W., & Baer, D. M. (1976). Functional speech and Muma, J. R. (1986). Language acquisition, a functionalistic perspec-
language training for the severely handicapped, Part I : Persons and tive. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
things. Lawrence, KS: H & H Enterprises. Musselwhite, C., & St. Louis, K. (1988). Communication programming
Halle, J. W. (1982). Teaching functional language to the handicapped: for persons with severe handicaps : Vocal and non-vocal strategies.
An integrative model of natural environment teaching techniques. Boston: College Hill Press.
Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 7, Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (1979). Developmental pragmatics. New
29-37. York: Academic Press.
Harris D., & Vanderheiden, G. (1980). Enhancing the development of Orlansky, M., & Bonvillian, J. (1984). The role of iconicity in early sign
communication interaction. In R. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Nonspeech language acquisition. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
language and communication: Analysis and intervention (pp. 227- 49, 287-292.
257). Baltimore: University Park Press. Owens, R. E., Jr., & House, L. I. (1984). Decision-making processes
Harris, P. L. (1982). Cognitive prerequisites to language? British in augmentative communication. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Journal of Psychology, 73, 187-195. Disorders, 49, 18-25.
221

Reichle, J., & Karlan, G. (1985). The selection of an augmentative Silverman, F. (1980). Communication for the speechless, Englewood
system in communication intervention: a critique of decision rules. Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 10, Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of
146-156. grammar. In D. I. Slobin, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Studies of Child
Reichle, J., & Karlan, G. (1988). Selecting augmentative communi- language development (pp. 175-208). New York: Holt, Rinehart
cation interventions: A critique of candidacy criteria and a proposed and Winston.
alternative. In R. L. Schiefelbusch, & L. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language Smith, L., & vonTelzchner, S. (1986). Communicative, sensorimotor,
perspectives: Acquisition, retardation, and intervention (2nd ed., and language skills of young children with Down syndrome. Amer-
pp. 321-329). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. ican Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 57-66.
Reichle, J., & Yoder, D. (1985). Communication board use in severely Smolak, L. (1982). Cognitive precursors of receptive vs. expressive
handicapped learners. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in language. Journal of Child Language, 9, 13-22.
the Schools, 16, 146-157. Snell, M. E., Ed. (1983). Systematic Instruction of the Moderately and
Rice, M. (1983). Contemporary accounts of the cognition/language Severely Handicapped (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.
relationship: Implications for speech-language clinicians. Journal of Snyder-McLean, L., Etter-Schroeder, R., & Rogers, N. (1986). Issues
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 347-359. in Piagetian cognitive assessment of severely/profoundly retarded
Rice, M., & Kemper, S. (1984). Child Language and Cognition. Balti- individuals. Paper presented at the ASHA Annual Convention,
more: University Park Press. Detroit.
Romski, M. A. (1987, August). Augmentative and alternative com- Steckol, K. F., & Leonard, L. B. (1981). Sensorimotor development
munication systems : Considerations for individuals with severe and the use of prelinguistic performatives. Journal of Speech and
intellectual disabilities. Presentation at the think tank, Augmenta- Hearing Research, 24, 262-268.
tive and Alternative Communication: State of the Art and Science, Sternberg, L., McNerney, C. D., & Pegnatore, L. (1985). Developing
Downloaded by [Universität Osnabrueck] at 14:53 07 February 2016

Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN. co-active imitative behaviors with profoundly mentally handicapped
Romski, M. A., Lloyd, L. L., & Sevcik, R. A. (1988). Augmentative students. Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded Vol. 20,
and alternative communication issues. In R. L. Schiefelbusch, & L. 260-267.
L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives: Acquisition, retardation Sternberg, L., Pegnatore, L., & Hill, C. (1983). Establishing interactive
and intervention (2nd ed., pp. 343-366). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. communication behaviors with profoundly mentally handicapped
Romski, M. A., & Sevcik, R. A. (1988). Augmentative and alternative students. The Association for the Severely Handicapped Journal,
communication systems: Considerations for individuals with severe 8, 39-46.
intellectual disabilities. Augmentative and Alternative Communica- Stillman, R. D., & Battle, C. W. (1984). Developing prelanguage
tion, 4, 83-93. communication in the severely handicapped: An interpretation of
Romski, M. A., Sevcik, R. A., & Joyner, S. E. (1984). Nonspeech the Van Dijk method. Seminars in Speech and Language, 5, 159-
communication systems: Implications for language intervention 170.
with mentally retarded children. Topics in Language Disorders, 5, Stremel, K., & Waryas, C. (1974). A behavioral-psycholinguistic ap-
66-81. proach to language training. American Speech and Hearing Mono-
Romski, M. A., Sevcik, R. A., & Pate, J. L. (1988). The establishment graphs, 18, 96-124.
of symbolic communication in persons with severe mental retar- Stremel-Campbell, K., Johnson-Dorn, N., Guida, J. C., & Dell, T.
dation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 97-107. (1984). Communication curriculum. Teaching Research Integration
Romski, M. A., Sevcik, R. A., & Washburn, D. (1987, May). Micro- Project for Children and Youth with Severe Handicaps, Todd Hall,
computer communication system implementation in homes and Monmouth, OR, 97361.
classrooms of nonspeaking youngsters with retardation. Paper Valletutti, P. J., & Sims-Tucker, B. M. (Eds.) (1984). Severely &
presented at the annual meeting of the American Association on profoundly handicapped students: Their nature and needs. Balti-
Mental Deficiency, Los Angeles. more: Paul H. Brookes.
Schiefelbusch, R. L. (1980). Speech, language, and communication Vanderheiden, G. C., & Lloyd, L. L. (1986). Nonspeech modes and
disorders of the multiple handicapped. Paper presented at the systems. In S. W. Blackstone (Ed.), Augmentative communication:
International Association of Logopedics, Washington, DC. An introduction (pp. 49-161). Rockville, MD: American Speech-
Schiefelbusch, R. L., & Bricker, D. D. (Eds.) (1979). Early language: Language-Hearing Association.
Acquisition and intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press. Warren, S. F., & Rogers-Warren, A. K. (1985). Teaching functional
Schlesinger, H. S. (1978). The acquisition of signed and spoken language: An introduction. In S. F. Warren, & A. K. Rogers-Warren
language. In L. S. Liben, (Ed.), Deaf children: Developmental per- (Eds.), Teaching functional language: Generalization and mainte-
spectives (pp. 69-85). New York: Academic Press. nance of language skills (pp. 3-23). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Shane, H. (1981). Decision making in early augmentative communi- Wilbur, R. (1987). American sign language: Linguistic and applied
cation system use. In R. Schiefelbusch,& D. Bricker (Eds.), Early dimensions (2nd ed.). Boston: College-Hill Press.
language: Acquisition and intervention (pp. 389-425). Baltimore: Wood, B. (1976). Children and communication: Verbal and nonverbal
University Park Press. language development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Augmentative and Alternative Communication Fellowship


for
Master’s, Doctoral or Postdoctoral Studies

The president of the Prentke Romich company is pleased to announce a new annual fellowship program for
advanced studies in the area of augmentative and alternative communication. The Prentke Romich AAC Fellowship
provides support for MS-thesis option, PhD, or postdoctoral research studies at Purdue University. The fellowship is
open to individuals of any nationality. Although the fellow would be pursuing studies in either audiology and speech
sciences or in special education, individuals from any discipline applicable to the area of augmentative and alternative
communication are eligible. In addition to coursework and research activities, the fellow will work closely with the
editor of AAC to gain a practical knowledge of manuscript review and processing. For application materials and
further information write: Lyle L. Lloyd, Ph.D., Editor, Augmentative and AIternative Communication, Purdue
University, South Campus Courts— Building E, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907, USA.

You might also like