You are on page 1of 13

A synthesis model of creative thinking in musical composition

DAVID COLLINS,
Doncaster College, England
Introduction

With the exception of research carried out by Bamberger (1977, 1978, 1991), studies
of the music composition process have used predominantly positivistic approaches, in
experimental, laboratory, non real-world scenarios for data collection. The aim of this paper is
to describe the cognitive processes, in real-time, of a composer in the act of writing music by
assuming an alternative qualitative stance, using a constructivist, naturalistic case-study
methodology.

Background

Webster (1992) outlines a literature model, or organisational scheme, which draws a


clear distinction between those studies which have focused mainly upon the assessment of
musical product (with some consideration of general processes, or approaches to
composition), and those primarily concerned with the musical process, which is the main
concern of this paper. Product-based studies, not including innumerable post-hoc analyses of
compositions, which "often centre on the nature of musical characteristics as a clue to mental
processes", (Webster, op.cit: 270) are frequently underpinned by pedagogical considerations
(e.g. Doig, 1941; Loane, 1984; Kratus, 1985; Swanwick & Tillman, 1986; Bunting 1987; van
Ernst, 1993; Kennedy, 1999). Process-based studies on the other hand, "focus upon
observable behaviours or reported thought processes during the creative act" (Webster, op.cit
:70).
The number of empirical studies in this area of music cognition is very limited in
comparison to studies, for example, in music perception. This imbalance was originally
pointed out by Sloboda (Sloboda,1985), and a decade later, the same author indicated that the
situation had undergone little change: "While there are now several thousand empirical
studies on music perception, there are still fewer than ten serious direct studies of the
compositional process, involving in total, fewer than 20 composers." (Sloboda, 1995:3), a
situation which continues.
Additionally, most empirical studies in musical composition, have focused on one
isolated parameter of music, namely melody (e.g. Bamberger, 1977, 1978; Bamberger &
Schön, 1977, 1980; Flohr, 1985; Swanwick & Tillman, 1986; Davidson & Welsh, 1988;
Conant, 1988; Scripp, Meyaard & Davidson, 1988; Kratus, 1989; Moore, 1989; Kratus, 1994;
Wilson & Wales, 1995; Younker & Smith, 1996). This paper suggests that the tracking of
musical processes which does not adopt a deconstructed focus upon individual sound
parameters, may yield significant findings
Furthermore, composers themselves speak of process-related issues such as:
inspiration and the origin of ideas, the activity of developing ideas, the reflexivity which
occurs between the artist and the emerging art-work, the notion of problem-solving and
wrestling with emerging musical material, or the need for compositional frameworks or
'scaffolds', but it is the case that such accounts relating to these issues remain anecdotal and
vague.
Much of the literature (empirical, hypothetical and composers accounts) suggests that
the process of music composition proceeds in stages over time, with evidence of some
recursivity between these stages, but the overall picture is one which describes only overtly
linear, macro-level planning and implementation, i.e. general routes and working strategies
which could be used by composers, together with hypotheses of a possible synthesis of
convergent/divergent thinking (e.g. Bennett, 1976; Aranosian, 1981; Sloboda, 1985; Webster,
1987, 1989; Emmerson, 1989; Roozendall, 1993).
The opportunities for producing empirical traces of the compositional process using
the computer was discussed by Minsky & Laske,in 1992, and has been used by others for
short, melodically-based, constrained tasks ( Kratus, 1989, 1994) in step-time ( Scripp,
Meyaard & Davidson, 1988; Wilson & Wales, 1995) or by using only graphical notation
(Folkestad, 1996), This paper outlines the use of computer-based techniques which:

a) enable the accurate time-based mapping of compositional processes over longer


periods
b) allow the study the of the compositional process in real-time in a non-experimental
context
c) enhances the collection and analysis of multiple-parameter music data in digital form

Method

As highlighted above, the aim of this paper is to describe some of the cognitive
processes, in real-time, of a composer in the act of writing music; it assumes an qualitative
stance, with a constructivist, naturalistic idiographic case-study methodology.
A professional composer (regularly involved in writing music to commission, with his
own computer-based studio) was chosen for this in-depth study. He had not received any
formal training in music nor was a fluent instrumentalist; he relied heavily upon music
technology. He had received a recent commission which acted as the basis of this study; apart
from a completion space of a year, there were no other constraints as to length of piece, genre,
instrumentation etc.
The data was collected using the following methods:

1. MIDI save-as files: This technique built upon that used by Folkestad (1996) The
particular software chosen for this MIDI save-as protocol was a commercial
package, 'E-magic Logic Audio v.3.6'. This software allows music data (MIDI) to
be stored and edited using a variety of graphical displays: arrange window, score
window, list, matrix etc. Each save-as was made when the composer felt that he
had added something significantly new or made a significant change to the
ongoing composition. Each file included its own date and time stamp.
1. Verbal protocols: The composer was asked to retrospectively report on his
processes of composition (plans, immediately following the compositional activity
on audio tape).
1. Verification sessions: This informal interviewing strategy informed issues such as
linking MIDI data files with the correct verbal protocol, explaining vague or
inconclusive comments in the verbal protocol etc.
1. Interviews: Semi-structured interviews were carried out with the composer in order
to verify emerging data from the structural mapping and verbal protocols. These
were carried out at the initial stages of the composition, and once the composition
had been completed.

Data analysis

Data was analysed in the following ways:

1. Real-time mapping- to present a the sequence of MIDI save-as files (either as


individual screen-shots of the 'arrange' window or by conventional music notation)
that had been collected over the period of the composition, verified and illustrated
with the corresponding verbal protocol. This allowed insight into the developing
composition, specifically in terms of statements made by the composer relating to
reflection and planning. In essence, it represented a diary of sound and verbal data.
This data was also interleaved with extracts from the verification sessions.
1. . Thematic analysis-the verbal protocol data was analysed in order to identify
categories and themes (Erlandson et al, 1993).
1. 3. Structural mapping-which acted as a global, graphically presented overview of
macro-events (in this case, thematic sections) in the form of individual 'folders' in
the sequencing software arrange window. Such sections encompassed all necessary
parameters, not melody alone, and acted not as an illustration of thematic
development over time, but thematic placement over time.

Findings

Real-time mapping, thematic analysis of the verbal protocol and structural mapping
indicated the generation of an ongoing set of sub-goals and sub-problems where, the
composer-as-problem solver was making a tangible time-based product without a clearly
defined or prescribed end-state. Moments of 'illumination', where the composer 'saw' the
broader picture, occurred at particular times, that is, after 1) putting themes in place and 2)
carrying out small-scale editing; but these moments often occurred after moving backwards
and forwards within the composition rather than in a linear (or 'logical') sequence. These
insight-moments, relating to gestalt restructuring and reformulating (Wertheimer, 1945;
Duncker, 1945) occurred several times throughout the evolving composition in
contextualised, 'work-in-progress' solution spaces (as opposed to the stage theorists de-
contextualised single moment of 'illumination'). Within each solution space there also
occurred various strategies of problem solving: a) general, b) specific and c) 'insightful'. At
times, particular specific solutions were verbalised but deferred to a later stage in the process.
Moments of insight acted as a 'ladder up' to better grasp the overall structural view of the
evolving composition, but reflected emerging systems theory, these were just a part of a
process which involved an extraordinary degree of small-scale changes. Furthermore, the
composer was seen to be involved in reflexively connecting with the emerging composition in
order to help galvanise his creative thought, which may be termed an iterative feedback
process (Bamberger,1978; Kratus, 1989).
The findings also indicated that a form of simultaneity, or the simultaneous handling
of multiple operations, occurred in the form of a) those deferred solutions which may have
arisen concurrently with other solutions within a particular solution space but not
implemented until the composer reached a secondary or later solution space, and b) in the
form of nested restructuring- the reformulation of problems which occur close to the event of
a previous restructuring process, and in fact 'enter' in real-time the solution space occupied by
the initial restructuring process.

Proposed conceptual model

As a result of the findings a conceptual model is proposed. This model represents a


synthesis of stage process models and Gestalt theory with the spirit of Gruber's emerging
systems theory (Gruber,1980; Gruber & Davis, 1988). Like Wallas's stage theory
(Wallas,1926), it views the creative process as a series of steps over time. However, these do
not have clear boundaries; the stages of preparation, incubation, illumination and verification
may well be subsumed into richly context-driven solution spaces in the model where, as the
composer is actively involved in shaping his material, s/he moves recursively back and forth
across the stages.
The stages of generated solution spaces are cumulative or additive, rather than
discrete, and activate a series of goals and sub-goals, which in turn lead to a proliferation of
sub-problems which require addressing before moving forwards. (I propose the term 'solution
space' as opposed to Newell and Simon's information-processing 'problem space' (Newell &
Simon.1972). Music composition is seen not as a means-end problem-solving activity, but
rather a dynamic, expressive, self-authenticated solution-generating activity).
Within the ‘solution-space’, solutions themselves may be conjectured, implemented or
deferred. At non-predictable times, a gestalt restructuring of the problem may occur, which
either involves the composer in reformulating the givens (i.e. the initial ideas/themes and
associated generated sub-goals and sub-problems preceding the restructuring), or
reformulating the goals (where the initial postulated end-state of the composition is
abandoned in order to assimilate new material in the restructuring process).
The model acknowledges that in the construction of a piece of music, the process will
involve the application of both general and specific solutions to the proliferating problems and
sub-problems, but additionally, it also encompasses the emerging systems theorists many
moments of insight rather than a single moment which can occur in creative/imaginative work
( Gruber & Davis, 1988).
The observed simultaneity in problem-solving strategies lend weight to Aranosians'
hypothetical model of concurrent streams of thought in the music composition process
(Aranosian, 1981).The explanation of creative thinking (especially in the domain of artistic
creativity) by the stage theorists, given the current data, is inadequate to explain the
"transaction between finding and making" (Bamberger & Schön, 1977 side-stepping, jumping
back and forth in the composition, dealing with large-scale elements then small scale elements
This recursive working with materials is not uncommon; interaction and turn-taking
between local and global structures in the making process is evident in the literature e.g.
Bamberger (1977) , Davidson & Welsh ( 1988), Colley, Banton & Down (1992) and Kratus
(1989), Roozendall (1993).

Conclusions

At present, despite a number of studies on gestalt theory and musical /auditory


perception (e.g. Tenney & Polansky, 1980;Terhardt,1987; Shepard, 1999) there are no
examples in the literature which relate to gestalt problem-solving and musical composition.
This study therefore builds upon previous theories of composition and represents an original
contribution to the literature. The study has implications for both future educational software
design, and curriculum development and teaching strategies. In line with a qualitative
approach, the study aimed to further understand and explore the compositional process, and
offers no necessarily generalisable findings. However, in order to build upon the findings it is
suggested that future research should explore whether the proposed conceptual model can be
applied in a variety of compositional situations.
Address for correspondence:
Northern Centre for Performing Arts
Doncaster College
High Melton
DONCASTER DN5 7SZ
David.Collins@don.ac.uk
References

Aranosian,C.M. 1981 Musical creativity: the stream of consciousness in composition,


improvisation and education.Imagination,Cognition and Personality
1(1): 67-88

Bamberger, J 1978 Intuitive and formal musical knowing; parables of cognitive


dissonance. IN: S.S.MADEJA (ed.) The arts; cognition and basic
skills. (St.Louis: CEMREL, Inc.)

Bamberger, J. 1977 In search of a tune. IN: D.PERKINS & B.LEONDAR (eds.) The
Arts and Cognition. (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press)

Bamberger, J. 1991 The mind behind the musical ear: how children develop
musical intelligence. (Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press)

Bamberger, J. & 1980 The figural-formal transaction. DSRE working paper no 1


Schön, D. (Cambridge,Mass: MIT): 191-226

Bennett,S 1976 The process of musical creation: interviews with eight composers.
Journal of Research in Music Education 24:3-13

Bunting,R. 1987 Composing music: case studies in the teaching and learning
process. British Journal of Music Education 4(1): 25-32

Colley,A., 1992 An expert-novice comparison in musical composition. Psychology


Banton,L., & of Music 20:124-137
Down,J.
Conant,B. 1988 A study of cognitive processes of children creating music in a
computer learning environment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

Davidson,L. & 1988 From collections to structure: the developmental path of tonal
Welsh, P. thinking IN:J.A.SLOBODA (ed.) Generative processes in music;
the psychology of performance, improvisation &
composition.(Oxford Science Publications)

Doig, D 1941 Creative music: 1. Music composed for a given text. Journal of
Educational Research 35(4): 263-275

Duncker, K. 1945 On problem solving. Psychological monographs, 58: 1-110

Emmerson, S. 1989 Composing strategies and pedagogy.Contemporary Music Review


3:133-144

Erlandson, D.A. et 1993 Doing naturalistic enquiry. ( London: Sage Publications)


al .

Flohr, J.W. 1985 Young children's improvisations:emerging creative thought.


Creative Child & Adult Quarterly 10(2): 79-85

Folkestad,G. 1996 Computer based creative music making: young people's music in
the digital age. Göteborg Studies in Educational Sciences 104 (Acta
Universitatis Gothoburgensis, Sweden)

Gruber, H.E. 1980 Darwin on man: a psychological study of scientific creativity.


(Chicago: University of Chicago Press)
Gruber,H.E. & 1988 Inching our way up Mount Olympus: the evolving systems
Davis,S. approach to creative thinking. IN: R.J. STERNBERG (ed.) The
nature of creativity; contemporary psychological perspectives.
(Cambridge University Press)

Gruber,H.E. & 1988 Inching our way up Mount Olympus: the evolving systems
Davis,S. approach to creative thinking. IN: R.J. STERNBERG (ed.) The
nature of creativity; contemporary psychological perspectives.
(Cambridge University Press)

Kennedy, M.A. 1999 Where does the music come from? A comparison case-study of the
compositional processes of a high-school and a collegiate
composer. British Journal of Music Education,16(2): 157-177.

Kratus, J. 1985 Rhythm, melody, motive and phrase characteristics of original


songs by children aged 5 to 13 Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Northwestern University, Evanston.

Kratus, J. 1989 A time analysis of the compositional processes used by children


ages 7 to 11 Journal of Research in Music Education 37(1): 5-20

Kratus, J. 1994 Relationships among children's music audiation and their


compositional processes and products. Journal of Research in
Music Education 42 (2):115-130

Loane,B. 1984 Thinking about children's compositions. British Journal Of Music


Education (3): 205-231

Minsky,M. & 1992 Foreword: a conversation with Marvin Minsky. IN: M.BALABAN,
Laske, O. K.EBCIOGLU, O.LASKE.(eds.) Understanding music with AI:
perspectives on music cognition. (Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press)
Moore, B. 1989 Musical thinking processes. IN:E.BOARDMAN (ed.) Dimensions
of musical thinking. (Reston:MENC)

Newell,A. & 1972 Human problem solving. (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall)
Simon, H.A.

Roozendall,R. 1993 Psychological analysis of musical composition: composition as


design.Contemporary Music Review,9 (1&2):311-324

Scripp,L, 1988 Discerning musical development: using computers to discover what


Meyaard,J.& we know. Journal of Aesthetic Education 22(1):75-88
Davidson,L.

Scripp,L, 1988 Discerning musical development: using computers to discover what


Meyaard,J.& we know. Journal of Aesthetic Education 22(1):75-88
Davidson,L.

Shephard, R. 1999 Cognitive psychology & music. IN: R.P.COOK et al (eds.) Music,
cognition and computerized sound: an introduction to
psychoacoustics. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press)

Sloboda, J. 1985 The musical mind. (Oxford University Press)

Sloboda, J. 1995 Do psychologists have anything useful to say about composition?


Paper presented at the Third European Conference of Music
Analysis, Montpellier, France, 16-19 February. Courtesy of the
author.

Swanwick,K.& 1986 The sequence of musical development: a study of childrens'


Tillman, J. composition. British Journal of Music Education (3): 305-338

Tenny, J. & 1980 Temporal gestalt perception in music. Journal of Music Theory
Polansky, L. 24(3): 205-241
Terhardt, E. 1987 Gestalt principles and music perception. IN: W.A. YOST, C.S.
WATSON (eds.) Perception of complex auditory stimuli.
(Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum)

VanErnst,B. 1993 A study of the learning and teaching processes of non-naive music
students engaged in composition. Research studies in Music
Education 1:22-39

Wallas, G. 1926 The art of thought. (London;Watts)

Webster, P.R. 1987 Conceptual bases for creative thinking in music. IN: J.C.PEERY et
al. (eds.) Music and child development. (New York:Springer-
Verlag)

Webster, P.R. 1989 Creative thinking in music; the assessment question. . IN:
J.W.RICHMOND (ed.) The proceedings of the Suncoast music
education forum on creativity. (Tampa:University of S.Florida):
40-74

Webster, P.R. 1992 Research on creative thinking in music: the assessment issue.
IN:R.COLWELL (ed.)Handbook of Research on Music
Teaching and Learning. Music Educators National Conference
(New York: Schirmer)

Wertheimer, M. 1945 Productive thinking. (London: Tavistock Publications)

Wilson, S.J.& 1995 An exploration of children's musical compositions. Journal of


Wales, R.J. Research in Music Education 43(2):94-111

Wilson, S.J.& 1995 An exploration of children's musical compositions. Journal of


Wales, R.J. Research in Music Education 43(2):94-111
Younker,B.A. & 1996 Comparing and modelling musical thought processes of expert and
Smith,W.H. novice composers. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music
Education 128:25-35

You might also like