You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/264554420

Optimal hunting conditions drive circalunar behavior of a diurnal carnivore

Article  in  Behavioral Ecology · August 2014


DOI: 10.1093/beheco/aru122

CITATIONS READS
48 2,259

4 authors:

Femke Broekhuis Steffen Grünewälder


Wageningen University & Research University College London
59 PUBLICATIONS   1,444 CITATIONS    24 PUBLICATIONS   385 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

J. Weldon Mcnutt David Macdonald

137 PUBLICATIONS   4,342 CITATIONS   


University of Oxford
1,510 PUBLICATIONS   63,252 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Hwange Big Cat Project View project

Efficacy of rodenticides View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Femke Broekhuis on 26 October 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Behavioral Ecology Advance Access published August 5, 2014

Behavioral The official journal of the

Ecology ISBE
International Society for Behavioral Ecology

Behavioral Ecology (2014), 00(00), 1–8. doi:10.1093/beheco/aru122

Original Article

Optimal hunting conditions drive circalunar


behavior of a diurnal carnivore
Femke Broekhuis,a,b Steffen Grünewälder,c,d John W. McNutt,b and David W. Macdonalda
aDepartment of Zoology, Wildlife Conservation Research Unit, University of Oxford, Recanati-Kaplan Centre,

Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ at Bodleian Library on August 9, 2014


Tubney House Abingdon Road, Tubney, Oxfordshire OX13 5QL, UK, bBotswana Predator Conservation Trust,
Private Bag 13, Maun, Botswana, cCentre for Computational Statistics and Machine Learning, Graham
Knight Computer Science Department, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK,
and dDepartment of Computer Science, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK
Received 2 January 2014; revised 13 June 2014; accepted 15 June 2014.

Foraging requirements and predation risk shape activity patterns and temporal behavior patterns widely across taxa. Although this
has been extensively studied in small mammals, the influence of predation and prey acquisition on the activity and behavior of large
carnivores has received little attention. The diurnal activity described as typical for cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) has been explained in
terms of their avoidance of antagonistic interactions with other larger predators. However, a recent study revealed that cheetahs are
frequently active at night, especially during periods of full moon. Being both predator and “prey” in an environment with comparatively
high densities of larger and competitively dominant nocturnal predator species, we investigated whether cheetah nocturnal behav-
ior could be explained by favorable conditions for 1)  predator avoidance or 2) prey acquisition. We used a data set of continuously
recorded behavior created using machine-learning techniques on behavioral data collected in the field to transform recorded 2D activ-
ity values from radio-collars into 3 distinct behavioral states (feeding, moving, and resting). We found that 32.5% of cheetah feeding
behavior occurred at night and that, in the dry season, nocturnal feeding behavior was positively correlated with moonlight intensity.
Our results suggest that nocturnal and circalunar behavior of cheetahs is driven by optimal hunting conditions, outweighing the risks
of encountering other predators. Using novel methodology, the results provide new insights into the temporal distribution of behavior,
contributing to our understanding of the importance of moonlight and season on the behavior patterns of diurnal species.
Key words:  circalunar behavior, diurnal carnivore, machine-learning, moonlight, optimal hunting, time budgets.

Introduction et al. 1997). Despite this, activity patterns are not completely rigid
Foraging requirements and predation risk shape activity patterns and subtle changes to temporal patterns of animal behavior are
and temporal patterns of behavior widely across taxa (Schoener expected to reflect adaptive, if not optimal, cost–benefit responses
1974; Kronfeld-Schor and Dayan 2003). For example, Fenn and to variation in both resource acquisition and the risk of competi-
Macdonald (1995) showed that nocturnal Norwegian rats (Rattus tion or predation (Daan and Aschoff 1975; Lima and Bednekoff
norvegicus) shifted to diurnal activity to escape predation by a noc- 1999; Kronfeld-Schor and Dayan 2003; Kotler et  al. 2010). In a
turnal predator (Vulpes vulpes), and Harrington et al. (2009) showed recent study, Prugh and Golden (2014) found that nocturnal species
a similar shift by mink (Neovison vison) in response to the presence that depend primarily on vision are more likely to be active during
of competitively dominant otters (Lutra lutra) and polecats (Mustela periods of increased moonlight because it increases foraging effi-
putorius). A  complete switch in activity is rare however as differ- ciency and the ability to detect predators. Numerous studies have
ent levels of light availability require anatomical, physiological, focused on the influence of moonlight on the behavior, activity,
and behavioral adaptations (Daan and Aschoff 1975; Kronfeld- and interactions of nocturnal species (e.g., Brown and Kotler 2004;
Schor and Dayan 2003). As a result, patterns of activity are often Griffin et al. 2005; Penteriani et al. 2013; Prugh and Golden 2014).
entrained by day–night cycles, underpinned by hormone physiol- However, the role of moonlight on the behavior of diurnal species,
ogy, which leads to species either being active during the day (diur- particularly those that are both predator and “prey”, is still poorly
nal) or active at night (nocturnal; Daan and Aschoff 1975; Klein understood. Similar to the circalunar behavior of nocturnal spe-
cies, diurnal species that rely on vision could likewise adjust their
nocturnal activity to optimize predator avoidance, increase forag-
Address correspondence to F. Broekhuis. E-mail: femke.broekhuis@gmail.com. ing efficiency, or both.

© The Author 2014. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of


the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. For
permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com
Page 2 of 8 Behavioral Ecology

Cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) are predominantly diurnal, a trait that Methods


is rare among the Felidae (Gittleman 1986). It is believed that this
Study area
is an evolutionary adaptation to avoid risks associated with larger,
more competitive nocturnal carnivores, in particular lions (Panthera The study was conducted in the Okavango Delta, a permanent
leo) and spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta; Durant 1998; Hayward and inland delta in Northern Botswana. The core study site (19°31′S,
Slotow 2009). These 3 species compete over similar prey resources 23°37′E; elevation ca. 950 m) encompasses an area of approxi-
(Mills and Biggs 1993; Hayward and Kerley 2008) but cheetah have mately 2720 km2, including the Southern part of the Moremi
a competitive disadvantage in that they are morphologically smaller Game Reserve and the adjacent Wildlife Management Areas (for
and predominantly solitary (Caro 1994). Cheetahs are therefore not details, see McNutt 1996). The area is characterized by 2 distinct
only a predator but also “prey” as they fall victim to lions and spot- seasons: a cool, dry winter (April to October) and a hot, wet sum-
ted hyaenas through predation and kleptoparasitism (Laurenson mer (November to March) with a mean annual rainfall of 450–
1994; Hunter et al. 2007b). Despite this, a recent study has shown 600 mm/year (Mendelson et  al. 2010). Daylight varies from the
cheetahs are more active at night than previously described and shortest day to the longest day by 2 h and 15 min (05h40–19h15
that nocturnal activity correlates with moonlight (Cozzi et al. 2012). local time on 21 June, to 04h30–20h20 21 December; http://
This raises an interesting question: Why would this diurnal species aa.usno.navy.mil/data/).
be active at night, particularly at times of increased moonlight?
The nocturnal activity patterns of cheetahs could simply reflect

Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ at Bodleian Library on August 9, 2014


the increased ability to detect and avoid equally active predators as Cheetah data
previous studies have shown that cheetahs move away from lions and Between October 2008 and July 2011, Global Positioning System
spotted hyaenas when in close proximity (Durant 2000; Broekhuis (GPS) radio-collars (VECTRONIC Aerospace GmbH, Germany)
et al. 2013). Alternatively, Cozzi et al. (2012) hypothesized that this were fitted on all known adult cheetahs in the study area (n  =  6).
circalunar activity may be explained by benefits in terms of prey The GPS radio-collars were equipped with 2 single-axis accelerom-
capture: that with increased moonlight, hunting success increases, eters providing quantitative activity data for both the lateral (y axis)
offsetting risks of encountering lions or spotted hyaenas. However, and the anterior–posterior (x axis) movement. The animals’ accel-
behavioral data at night are difficult to come by, especially for elu- eration on these 2 axes was continuously recorded 4 times/s and
sive and wide-ranging carnivores, and Cozzi et al. (2012) were there- cumulative scores were logged every 5 min for the duration of the
fore unable to distinguish between these 2 hypotheses. study. Collars recorded activity continuously for a mean of 332 days
We circumvent these difficulties by using a novel machine learn- (range: 282–381 d). During the time when the collars were active,
ing technique to transform the activity data set used by Cozzi et al. each cheetah was found at least once every 2 weeks to collect behav-
(2012) into distinct behavioral states: resting, moving, and feeding. ioral data. Behavioral data were then recorded continuously for a
This data set was obtained using direct behavioral observations minimum of 1 h, resulting in 188 h of direct observations. During
to quantify and transform continuous cumulative activity scores each observation, 3 fundamental behavior categories were recorded:
recorded by data-loggers fitted on cheetahs. This enabled reliable feeding, moving, and resting. Each time the focal animal changed
categorization of cheetah behavior at 5-min intervals resulting in a its behavioral state, the exact time was recorded using a handheld
unique, continuous, and fine-scale behavioral data set. Importantly, GPS device (Garmin eTrex HC; Garmin, Olathe, KS). This time-
it allowed for behavioral data collection during times when chee- stamp allowed for the precise synchronization with the activity data
tahs could not be directly monitored. In addition, data on several recorded by the collars (see Data processing for more details). To
individuals could be collected simultaneously over a relatively long minimize the impact on cheetah behavior, cheetahs were followed
period allowing for the investigation of longer term patterns such at a minimum distance of 30 m and observations were conducted
as those influenced by the lunar cycle and season. Using these data, during twilight and daylight hours so that the cheetahs would not be
we investigated the diel composition of feeding, moving, and rest- disturbed by the necessary use of artificial lighting.
ing behavior of cheetahs. In compliance with Botswana law, all immobilizations for
We hypothesize that if the nocturnal activity described by Cozzi deployment/removal of radio-collars were performed by a
et al. (2012) is a response to interactions with predators and com- Botswana registered veterinarian. Cheetahs were free-darted and
petitors, cheetah would not feed at night because this is when they immobilized using a combination of ketamine (2–2.5 mg/kg) and
are most vulnerable to antagonistic encounters with their nocturnal medetomidine (0.07  mg/kg), remotely administered by a Dan-
counterparts (Hunter et al. 2007b). However, if this nocturnal activ- Inject CO2 rifle (Dan-Inject, Denmark), and reversed with atipa-
ity is related to optimal hunting conditions, then we would expect a mezol (0.3 mg/mL; following Kock et al. 2006). Sedation time was
relative increase in hunting success as evidenced by nocturnal feed- kept to a minimum, typically less than 1 h. After immobilization,
ing behavior. Being visual hunters, we would expect cheetah noc- all cheetahs recovered fully, showing no signs of distress, and no
turnal feeding behavior to be positively correlated with moonlight apparent side effects were observed on both the short and long
intensity. If this is the case, then we would expect there to be more term. Radio-collars were fitted on adults only and weighed 300 g,
feeding events during the dry season when cloud cover does not 0.67 ± 0.10% (mean ± standard deviation [SD]) of an adult’s
obscure moonlight. Behavioral adaptation to risk and hunting con- body weight (mean ± SD  =  45.5 ± 7.0 kg; range  =  38–57 kg). On
ditions can be investigated even further. If all else is equal, then we completion of the study, all radio-collars were removed. The ani-
would expect no detectable differences in activity characteristics of mal handling protocols used conformed to the standards of the
feeding bouts (e.g., frequency and duration) irrespective of time of American Society of Mammalogists (Sikes and Gannon 2011) and
day or presence of moonlight. were approved by both the Zoology Ethical Review Committee, a
Continuous and contemporaneous data sets of cheetah behavior subsidiary of Oxford Universities Animal Care and Ethical Review
in this study population provide a unique opportunity to determine (ACER) Committee (License CER-FB2008) and by the Botswana
the adaptive value of nocturnal behavior in a diurnal species. Department of Wildlife and National Parks (permit EWT 8/36/4).
Broekhuis et al. • Circalunar behavior of a diurnal carnivore Page 3 of 8

Data processing This predictive data set was used for all subsequent analyses using
a 2-stage process to investigate the temporal distribution of cheetah
To investigate patterns of behavior over several moon cycles and
behavior. First, the entire data set was used to investigate the pro-
seasons, a large number of data points are required, something that
portion of time cheetahs spent in each behavioral state. The sec-
is not always achievable with purely observational data. We there-
ond part of the analysis focused on the feeding behavior of cheetahs
fore used a novel technique enabling us to gather large amounts
because this is when cheetahs are least vigilant and most at risk of
of behavioral data over a relatively short space of time by trans-
kleptoparasitism and predation by lions and spotted hyaenas (Lima
forming the activity data from the GPS radio-collars into distinct
1987; Hunter et al. 2007b). Feeding bouts consisted of one or more
behavioral states.
feeding states, defined as a continuous block of feeding data points
Based on the time-stamp, behavioral observations recorded in the
not separated by mobile behavior (Figure 1). The proportion of time
field were used to label the activity data collected by the activity sen-
cheetahs spent feeding is a function of both the feeding frequency
sors in the GPS collars. This labeled data set was then used to classify
and the length of feeding bouts. Both these components were ana-
the remaining, unlabelled, activity data by means of machine-learn-
lyzed in relation to the diel phase (day vs. night), the seasons, and
ing techniques (Bishop 2008). The technique we used was a com-
the proportion of visible moon (which is a proxy for the amount of
bined Support Vector Machine (SVM) and Hidden Markov Model
moonlight). Only feeding bouts that took place exclusively during
(HMM) approach. The SVM is a common method for classification
the day or night were considered for further analysis.
(Shawe-Taylor and Cristianini 2004), splitting the activity data from

Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ at Bodleian Library on August 9, 2014


the collars into 3 different behavioral categories: resting, moving, Predictor variables
and feeding. However, because the classifier does not take the tem-
poral sequence of the data into account, we smoothed the classifica- To determine whether day–night cycles and light availability
tion using a HMM approach, thereby minimizing misclassifications. influenced cheetah behavior, data on the time of the transition
This trained classifier was then applied to each of the 5-min activity between day and night and data on the phases of the moon cycle
values resulting in a full sequence of predicted cheetah behavior for were obtained from the United States Naval Observatory website
the entire time that the collars were operational (see Grünewälder (http://aa.usno.navy.mil/data/). Discrimination between day and
et al. 2012 for full details). The performance of the algorithm was night was based on the astronomical twilight, which is when the sun
evaluated using a leave-one-out cross validation approach (see is 18° below the horizon. Season was included as a variable because
Table  1 for results). This is a standard approach for performance cloud cover during the wet season is likely to influence moonlight
evaluation and works in the following way: the SVM was trained on
all but one data set (defined as a block of continuous observations),
and the performance of this learned SVM was then measured on
the omitted data set. This process was then repeated for each of the
data sets and the performance was averaged over all data sets. The
behavior categories were characterized by specific activity values
(mean ± SD—Resting: X  =  5.78 ± 2.10, Y  =  4.28 ± 1.94; Mobile:
X = 117.77 ± 16.37, Y = 116.87 ± 25.67; Feeding: X = 74.98 ± 26.40,
Y = 53.68 ± 17.77). Because of the similarity between activity values
for feeding and mobile behavior, some misclassification occurred.
However, misclassified behaviors are likely randomly distributed Figure 1
over the entire data set (i.e., not biased toward time of day, season, Definition of feeding point, feeding state, and feedings bout. F  =  feeding
or moonlight) and therefore should not affect the overall results. and R = resting. Each compartment represents a 5-min interval.

Table 1
Number of labeled data points and the cross-validation of the combined SVM and HMM method that was used to translate the
activity data obtained from data-loggers, fitted to 6 free-ranging cheetahs, into 3 behavioral categories: feeding, mobile, and resting
Individuals M1 M2 F1 F2 F3 F4

Number of labeled data points Feeding 100 46 58 42 31 47


Mobile 99 76 61 97 81 83
Resting 153 268 122 139 171 317
Total 352 390 241 278 283 447
True positives Overall 94% 92% 91% 94% 84% 90%
Feeding 93% 52% 95% 100% 23% 66%
Mobile 97% 87% 74% 91% 75% 69%
Resting 93% 100% 98% 95% 99% 99%
False positives Feeding 2% 2% 5% 0% 6% 0%
Mobile 9% 24% 0% 7% 21% 2%
Resting 7% 5% 16% 6% 16% 13%
Number of predicted data points Feeding 4089 4504 2790 2948 1862 2409
Mobile 22 511 16 274 13 072 7306 9413 10 752
Resting 87 702 74 150 83 328 68 179 68 590 92 852
Total 114 302 94 928 100 233 78 434 79 865 106 013

The true positives are the percentages with which the 3 different behaviors were detected correctly, whereas the false positives are the percentages with which
behaviors were falsely classified as one of the other behaviors.
Page 4 of 8 Behavioral Ecology

intensity at night and could therefore play a role in determining the (Z  =  0.152, P  =  0.879; Figure  2). These results suggest that chee-
temporal distribution of cheetah behavior. There were therefore 3 tahs actively shift their feeding behavior, and thus hunting behavior,
predictor variables: diel phase (categorical with 2 levels: night and to the night during the dry season.
day), season (categorical with 2 levels: wet and dry), and moonlight Moonlight intensity had no influence on the proportion of time
intensity (a continuous variable rendering the proportion of the cheetahs spent feeding (Figure 3a) but did influence the proportion
moon that was visible where 0  =  new moon and 1  =  full moon). of time for which cheetahs were mobile (Figure 3b). Higher levels
As both lions and spotted hyaenas are predominately nocturnal of moonlight were linked to a significant increase in mobile behav-
(Kruuk 1972; Schaller 1972; Hayward and Slotow 2009), the diel ior at night (t124  =  4.724, P ≤ 0.001) and a significant decrease
phase (day vs. night) was used as a proxy for relative risk. in mobile behavior during the day (t124  =  3.308, P  =  0.001). The
inverse was therefore true for the proportion of time cheetahs spent
Statistical analysis resting: at night, cheetahs were more likely to rest when there was
less light present (t124  =  −4.988, P ≤ 0.001) and more likely to be
The proportion of time cheetahs spent feeding, mobile, or resting
resting during the day (t124 = 2.781, P = 0.006; Figure 3c).
in relation to the diel phase (day vs. night), season, and the moon
cycle were analyzed separately using Generalized Linear Models Feeding frequency and length
(GLM) with a binomial error structure (Warton and Hui 2010).
The frequency of feeding bouts was similarly analyzed using GLMs Of the 910 feeding bouts, 12% took place at night, 14% in the
crepuscular transitions (i.e., dawn and dusk), and 73% took

Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ at Bodleian Library on August 9, 2014


with a binomial error structure. The lengths of feeding bouts were
entered as a dependent variable in a linear model after being log place during the day. Feeding bouts at night lasted on average
transformed to meet model assumptions. Individual cheetahs were 62.29 ± 3.54 min and were significantly shorter than those dur-
entered as a fixed blocking effect because of the small number of ing the day (97.15 ± 3.55 min; Z  =  6.048, P ≤ 0.001; Figure  4).
individuals (Bates 2010). To analyze the proportion of time that Nocturnal feeding bouts were more frequent in the dry season than
cheetahs spent in each behavioral state, 14 a priori candidate mod- in the wet season (Z = −3.235, P ≤ 0.001; Figure 5). During the dry
els were created for each of the behavioral states, including each season, the night time frequency of feeding events increased sig-
of the main effects (diel phase, season, and moonlight intensity) nificantly with increased moonlight (3-way interaction: Z = −2.275,
and their interaction terms. Models were ranked using Akaike P = 0.023). However, moonlight intensity had no significant effect
Information Criterion corrected for small sample size. If one model on feeding length (Z = 1.277, P = 0.202). Season had a significant
was clearly dominant (wi > 0.9), this was used, otherwise parameter effect on both the length and the frequency of feeding bouts (feed-
estimates were averaged across the models correcting for model ing length: Z  =  1.972, P  =  0.048; feeding frequency: Z  =  11.349,
weights (wi; Burnham and Anderson 2002). Models and model P ≤ 0.001; Figures 4 and 5). In the wet season, cheetahs fed more
comparison statistics can be found in the Supplementary Appendix. frequently but feeding bouts where shorter (97.35 ± 2.52 min) com-
Statistical analyses were performed using the statistical software pared with the dry season when cheetah fed less often but for lon-
R 2.14.2 (R Development Core Team 2012). Values are given as ger (104.66 ± 4.34 min; Figure 4).
mean ± 95% confidence interval.
Discussion
Results Because animals are most at risk of costly encounters when feed-
ing, not only because vigilance is reduced (Lima 1987), but also
Temporal distribution of cheetah behavior because food resources attract kleptoparasites (Hunter et al. 2007a),
Cheetahs spent the majority of their time resting (84.5 ± 2.78%) we hypothesized that cheetahs would not feed at night if noc-
and only spent a small portion of their time feeding (3.0 ± 3.2%) or turnal behavior was determined by predator avoidance. On the
mobile (12.5 ± 2.78%; Figure  2). Although cheetahs are predomi- other hand, we hypothesized that cheetahs would feed at night
nantly diurnal, the results show that 45.2% of their overall mobile if nocturnal behavior was driven by optimal hunting conditions.
and 32.5% of their overall feeding behavior occurred at night We found that there were a high number of nocturnal feeding
(mobile: Z  =  9.797, P ≤ 0.001; feeding: Z  =  5.138, P ≤ 0.001). events. Furthermore, an increase in nocturnal activity was offset by
Nonetheless, cheetahs still spent a greater portion of their time rest- decreased activity during the day, indicating an active shift toward
ing during the night than during the day (night: 94.2 ± 2.6%; day: nocturnal behavior. Thus, our results indicate that the noctur-
78.6 ± 2.3%; Z = 10.711, P ≤ 0.001; Figure 2). nal behavior of predominantly diurnal cheetahs in our study can
The temporal distribution of cheetah behavior varied with be explained by optimal hunting conditions rather than predator
season. Cheetahs spent more time feeding during the wet season avoidance.
compared with the dry season (Z  =  2.424, P  =  0.015), and there Cheetahs are visual hunters, thus we expected that the observed
was a significant interaction between the diel phase and season nocturnal feeding behavior would be positively correlated with
(Z = 3.459, P ≤ 0.001), suggesting the day–night difference was not moonlight intensity. Our results support this hypothesis, but only
the same in the wet and dry seasons. Cheetahs spent less time feed- during the dry season. This seasonal difference further illustrates
ing during the day in the dry season compared with the wet season the importance of moonlight: cheetahs were more mobile and fed
(daydry: 3.4 ± 1.0%; daywet: 4.2 ± 0.5%; t124 = 2.238, P = 0.027) and more at night during the dry season compared with the wet sea-
more time feeding at night in the dry season compared with the son. During the wet season, the moon is frequently obscured by the
wet season (nightdry: 1.2 ± 0.0%; nightwet: 0.4 ± 0.3; t124 = −3.384, P presence of clouds and thus the light conditions would be similar to
≤ 0.001). Similarly, cheetahs were more mobile at night in the dry those during new moon in the dry season when cheetahs are barely
season compared with the wet season (nightdry: 3.1 ± 2.4%; nightwet: active at night. These results suggest that predators, especially those
0.5 ± 0.2%; t124 = −2.770, P ≤ 0.001), whereas the amount of time that are visual hunters and use speed rather than ambush tech-
cheetahs were mobile during the day did not differ between seasons niques, benefit from increased moonlight (Prugh and Golden 2014).
Broekhuis et al. • Circalunar behavior of a diurnal carnivore Page 5 of 8

Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ at Bodleian Library on August 9, 2014


Figure 2
Proportion of time cheetah in the Okavango Delta, Botswana (n = 6) spent feeding (black), mobile (dark gray), and resting (light gray) in relation to time of
day for the (a) dry and (b) wet season. The bar above the individual figures depicts the day–night length (day = gray, night = black) based on the astronomical
twilight on the solstices.

For example, the hunting efficiency of both short-eared owls (Asio variation of risk. Rather, it is likely that avoidance of predators is
flammeus) and wolves (Canis lupus) was shown to increase with moon- influenced by finer scale behavioral adaptations, resulting in partial,
light (Clarke 1983; Theuerkauf et  al. 2003). Although these are rather than complete, temporal segregation (Carothers and Jaksić
examples of nocturnal species, the same could be true for diurnal 1984; Kronfeld-Schor and Dayan 2003). For instance, direct cues,
cursorial predators. During moonlit nights, cheetahs would benefit such as calls of lions and spotted hyaenas, rather than indirect ones
from increased visibility, whereas the darkness would allow them to such as day–night cycles may influence behavioral decisions (Kotler
approach more closely to their prey. This may increase the rate of et al. 1991; Durant 2000). Indeed, this was the conclusion of Kotler
successful hunts and/or decrease the chase distance and therefore et  al. (1991) who showed that great Egyptian sand gerbils (Gerbillus
energy expenditure, similar to what has been found in wild dogs pyramidum) had a stronger response to the direct risk of owl predation
(Lycaon pictus; Rasmussen et al. 2008). Furthermore, during moonlit rather than the indirect risk associated with increased moonlight at
nights, impala (Aepyceros melampus), cheetahs’ primary prey (Purchase night. Similarly, cheetahs respond to the direct risk of encountering
and du Toit 2000; Hayward et  al. 2006), tend to move into open other predators rather than the indirect risk (Broekhuis et al. 2013).
areas (Jarman and Jarman 1973). This may benefit cheetahs, which It is therefore plausible that, in general, the benefits of hunting at
are a high-speed, open habitat, cursorial predator. night when lions and spotted hyaenas are active outweighs the risk
Although hunting conditions may be optimal for cheetahs at of negative encounters, but that hunting and feeding behavior ceases
night when moonlight is present, this nocturnal behavior also sug- when there is a direct risk of encountering predators (Durant 2000).
gests that cheetah behavior is not strictly shaped by the temporal Accordingly, the present results indicate that feeding behavior itself
Page 6 of 8 Behavioral Ecology

Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ at Bodleian Library on August 9, 2014


Figure 4
The length of cheetah feeding bouts at (a) different times of day and in (b)
different seasons.

Figure 3
Relationship between the moonlight intensity at night (0 = no moon, 1 = full
moon) and the proportion of time cheetah spent (a) feeding, (b) moving, and Figure 5
(c) resting. Fitted lines are displayed ± 95% confidence intervals. Frequency of cheetah feeding bouts (adjusted for time) in relation to the
time of day and season.
could be influenced by the increased likelihood of encountering noc-
turnal predators. Nocturnal feeding bouts were significantly shorter may be explained by impalas calving at the beginning of the wet
than those during the day, implying either a higher rate of kleptopar- season: during the wet season, young impalas make up 50% of the
asitism or that cheetahs left their kill sooner. Kleptoparasitism is ener- total prey consumed by cheetahs (n = 38), whereas no young impa-
getically costly (Gorman et al. 1998) and would counterbalance the las were taken during the dry season (n = 20; Broekhuis F, unpub-
possible energy gained by more efficient hunts at night. It is there- lished data). Therefore, with the presence of smaller prey, cheetahs
fore possible that cheetahs either selected for smaller prey at night or may feed more frequently but for shorter periods of time.
that they tried to spend less time on a kill when they were in a risky There are other factors that could govern cheetahs’ nocturnal
situation. Indeed, Hunter et al. (2007b) found that cheetahs left kills behavior. First, temperature has been shown to influence chee-
earlier when they were in an environment associated with more risk. tah activity (Cozzi et  al. 2012). Because night time temperatures
We also found that feeding length and frequency varied accord- are significantly lower than day time temperatures, it is possible
ing to season with cheetahs feeding more frequently but for shorter that cheetahs hunt at night to avoid high day time temperatures.
periods during the wet season compared with the dry season. This However, if this were the case in the current study, we should have
Broekhuis et al. • Circalunar behavior of a diurnal carnivore Page 7 of 8

found that night time feeding was more frequent in the hot, wet discussions in the early stages of this research, and M.  Valeix, P.  Johnson,
season, whereas we found the opposite. Second, previous studies N. Elliot, G. Cozzi, and 2 anonymous reviewers for commenting on previ-
ous drafts of the manuscript.
have found that diurnal species shift their activity to the night as
a result of human activity during the day (Marnewick et al. 2006; Handling editor: Shinichi Nakagawa
Rasmussen and Macdonald 2012). This is unlikely to be the case in
our study which was conducted in a wildlife area (Moremi Game
Reserve and the surrounding Wildlife Management Areas) where
References
anthropogenic pressures are minimal (Boggs 2000). We therefore
conclude that cheetahs naturally choose to be active at night rather Bates DM. 2010. lme4: mixed-effects modeling with R. Springer [cited
than being driven to do so by external factors. Although we real- 2014 April 25]. Available from: http://lme4 r-forge r-project org/book.
Bishop CM. 2008. Pattern recognition and machine learning. New York:
ize that our sample size is small, these results may be characteristic Springer.
of cheetah behavior more broadly. Indeed, studies elsewhere have Bissett C, Bernard RTF. 2007. Habitat selection and feeding ecology of the
noted similar nocturnal hunting activity by cheetahs (Stander 1990; cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) in thicket vegetation: is the cheetah a savanna
Bissett and Bernard 2007). It is plausible that this nocturnal behav- specialist? J Zool. 271:310–317.
Boggs LP. 2000. Community power, participation, conflict and develop-
ior has not been quantified before because cheetahs are generally ment choice: community wildlife conservation in the Okavango region
recognized as being diurnal (Hilborn et  al. 2012). Therefore, data of Northern Botswana. London: International Institute for Environment
collection for cheetahs is typically biased toward daylight hours and Development.

Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ at Bodleian Library on August 9, 2014


(e.g., Durant 1998; Hunter et  al. 2007b). As a result, the com- Broekhuis F, Cozzi G, Valeix M, McNutt JW, Macdonald DW. 2013. Risk
paratively less frequent, nocturnal foraging and feeding behavior avoidance in sympatric large carnivores: reactive or predictive? J Anim
Ecol. 82:1098–1105.
may not have been documented in such detail because of lack of Brown JS, Kotler BP. 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of
sampling techniques. Traditional methods of behavioral data col- predation. Ecol Lett. 7:999–1014.
lection, especially direct observation, make it difficult to constantly Burnham KP, Anderson DR. 2002. Model selection and multimodel
monitor animals, thus introducing a bias. Detailed studies on the inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
timing of behavior of elusive, far ranging species such as chee- Carbone C, Mace GM, Roberts SC, Macdonald DW. 1999. Energetic con-
tahs that live at low densities or in remote areas that are difficult straints on the diet of terrestrial carnivores. Nature. 402:286–288.
to access have therefore been limited. Studies that have managed Caro TM. 1994. Cheetahs of the Serengeti: group living in an asocial spe-
to follow cheetahs continuously, including during the night, have cies. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
done so for comparatively short time intervals (14 d; Mills and Carothers JH, Jaksić FM. 1984. Time as a niche difference: the role of
interference competition. Oikos. 42:403–406.
Biggs 1993; Hayward and Slotow 2009) and were therefore unable Clarke JA. 1983. Moonlight’s influence on predator/prey interactions
to detect trends associated with long-term cycles such as the moon between short-eared owls (Asio flammeus) and deermice (Peromyscus manicu-
and season. The development of data-loggers, that automatically latus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 13:205–209.
record and store animal activity data, has made it possible to inves- Cozzi G, Broekhuis F, McNutt JW, Turnbull LA, Macdonald DW,
Schmid B. 2012. Fear of the dark or dinner by moonlight? Reduced
tigate behavioral data, which was previously difficult or impossible temporal partitioning among Africa’s large carnivores. Ecology.
to obtain. 93:2590–2599.
In summary, our results suggest that the nocturnal behavior of Daan S, Aschoff J. 1975. Circadian rhythms of locomotor activity in captive
cheetahs can be explained in terms of optimal hunting conditions. birds and mammals: their variations with season and latitude. Oecologia.
In addition, cheetahs are more flexible and adaptive in their behav- 18:269–316.
Durant SM. 1998. Competition refuges and coexistence: an example from
ior than previously believed. The same could therefore be true for Serengeti carnivores. J Anim Ecol. 67:370–386.
other diurnal predators, such as African wild dogs. The results pre- Durant SM. 2000. Living with the enemy: avoidance of hyenas and lions by
sented here illustrate that by taking longer term cycles (e.g., lunar cheetahs in the Serengeti. Behav Ecol. 11:624–632.
phase and season) into account, variations in animal behavior may Fenn MGP, Macdonald DW. 1995. Use of middens by red foxes: risk
reverses rhythms of rats. J Mammal. 76:130–136.
be detected. This study thereby sheds new light on cheetah behav- Gittleman JL. 1986. Carnivore brain size, behavioral ecology, and phylog-
ior and contributes to our understanding of the importance of eny. J Mammal. 67:23–36.
moonlight and season on the behavior patterns of diurnal species. Gorman ML, Mills MG, Raath JP, Speakman JR. 1998. High hunting
Finally, our methodology and inclusion of lunar and seasonal vari- costs make African wild dogs vulnerable to kleptoparasitism by hyaenas.
ables, could be incorporated into studies on energetic budgets and Nature. 391:479.
Griffin PC, Griffin SC, Waroquiers C, Mills LS. 2005. Mortality by moon-
optimal diet models (e.g., Carbone et al. 1999), because our results light: predation risk and the snowshoe hare. Behav Ecol. 16:938–944.
suggest that feeding events and frequencies vary over time. Grünewälder S, Broekhuis F, Macdonald DW, Wilson AM, McNutt JW,
Shawe-Taylor J, Hailes S. 2012. Movement activity based classification
of animal behaviour with an application to data from cheetah (Acinonyx
Supplementary Material jubatus). PLoS ONE. 7:e49120.
Harrington LA, Harrington AL, Yamaguchi N, Thom MD, Ferreras P,
Supplementary material can be found at http://www.beheco. Windham TR, Macdonald DW. 2009. The impact of native competitors
oxfordjournals.org/. on an alien invasive: temporal niche shifts to avoid interspecific aggres-
sion? Ecology. 90:1207–1216.
Hayward MW, Hofmeyr M, O’Brien J, Kerley GIH. 2006. Prey preferences
Funding of the cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) (Felidae: Carnivora): morphological limi-
tations or the need to capture rapidly consumable prey before kleptopar-
Tom Kaplan Prize Scholarship; Wilderness Wildlife Trust.
asites arrive? J Zool. 270:615–627.
Hayward MW, Kerley GIH. 2008. Prey preferences and dietary overlap
We thank the Botswana Ministry of Environment, Wildlife, and Tourism amongst Africa’s large predators. S Afr J Wildl Res. 38:93–108.
for permission to conduct this research under permit number EWT 8/36/4 Hayward MW, Slotow R. 2009. Temporal partitioning of activity in large
IV. We thank the Botswana Predator Conservation Trust for logistical sup- African carnivores: tests of multiple hypotheses. S Afr J Wildl Res.
port, A. Kusler for her help in the field, A. Wilson and S. Hailes for fruitful 39:109–125.
Page 8 of 8 Behavioral Ecology

Hilborn A, Pettorelli N, Orme CDL, Durant SM. 2012. Stalk and chase: Mendelson J, Vanderpost C, Ramberg L. 2010. Okavango Delta: floods of
how hunt stages affect hunting success in Serengeti cheetah. Anim Behav. life. Olympia Windhoek (Namibia): Raison cc.
84:701–706. Mills MGL, Biggs HC. 1993. Prey apportionment and related ecological
Hunter JS, Durant SM, Caro TM. 2007a. Patterns of scavenger arrival at relationships between large carnivores in Kruger National Park. Zool
cheetah kills in Serengeti National Park Tanzania. Afr J Ecol. 45:275–281. Sympos. 65:253–268.
Hunter JS, Durant SM, Caro TM. 2007b. To flee or not to flee: predator Penteriani V, Kuparinen A, Mar Delgado M, Palomares F, López-Bao J,
avoidance by cheetahs at kills. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 61:1033–1042. Fedriani J, Calzada J, Moreno S, Villafuerte R, Campioni L, et al. 2013.
Jarman MV, Jarman PJ. 1973. Daily activity of impala. E Afr Wildl J. Responses of a top and a meso predator and their prey to moon phases.
11:75–92. Oecologia. 1–14.
Klein DC, Coon SL, Roseboom PH, Weller J, Bernard M, Gastel JA, Zatz Prugh LR, Golden CD. 2014. Does moonlight increase predation risk?
M, Iuvone M, Rodriguez IR, Bégay V. 1997. The melatonin rhythm-gen- Meta-analysis reveals divergent responses of nocturnal mammals to lunar
erating enzyme: molecular regulation of serotonin N-acetyltransferase in cycles. J Anim Ecol. 83:504–514.
the pineal gland. Recent Progr Horm Res. 52:307–358. Purchase GK, du Toit JT. 2000. The use of space and prey by cheetahs in
Kock M, Meltzer D, Burroughs R. 2006. Chemical and physical restraint Matusadona National Park, Zimbabwe. S Afr J Wildl Res. 30:139–144.
of wild animals: a training and field manual for African species. R Development Core Team. 2012. R: a language and environment for
Johannesburg (South Africa): International Wildlife Veterinary Services. statistical computing. Vienna (Austria): R Foundation for Statistical
Kotler BP, Brown JS, Hasson O. 1991. Factors affecting gerbil foraging Computing.
behavior and rates of owl predation. Ecology. 72:2249–2260. Rasmussen GSA, Gusset M, Courchamp F, Macdonald DW. 2008. Achilles’
Kotler BP, Brown J, Mukherjee S, Berger-Tal O, Bouskila A. 2010. Heel of Sociality Revealed by Energetic Poverty Trap in Cursorial
Moonlight avoidance in gerbils reveals a sophisticated interplay among Hunters. Am Nat. 172:508–518.
time allocation, vigilance and state-dependent foraging. Proc R Soc Lond Rasmussen GSA, Macdonald DW. 2012. Masking of the zeitgeber: African

Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ at Bodleian Library on August 9, 2014


B Biol Sci. 277:1469–1474. wild dogs mitigate persecution by balancing time. J Zool. 286:232–242.
Kronfeld-Schor N, Dayan T. 2003. Partitioning of time as an ecological Schaller GB. 1972. The Serengeti lion: a study of predator-prey relations.
resource. Ann Rev Ecol System. 34:153–181. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kruuk H. 1972. The spotted hyena: a study of predation and social behav- Schoener T. 1974. Resource partitioning in ecological communities.
iour. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Science. 185:27–39.
Laurenson MK. 1994. High juvenile mortality in cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) Shawe-Taylor J, Cristianini N. 2004. Kernel methods for pattern analysis.
and its consequences for maternal care. J Zool. 234:387–408. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lima SL. 1987. Vigilance while feeding and its relation to the risk of preda- Sikes RS, Gannon WL. 2011. Guidelines of the American Society of
tion. J Theor Biol. 124:303–316. Mammalogists for the use of wild mammals in research. J Mammal.
Lima SL, Bednekoff PA. 1999. Temporal variation in danger drives anti- 92:235–253.
predator behavior: the predation risk allocation hypothesis. Am Nat. Stander PE. 1990. Notes on foraging habits of cheetah. S Afr Tydskr
153:649–659. Natuurnav. 20:130–132.
Marnewick KA, Bothma JdP, Verdoorn GH. 2006. Using camera-trapping Theuerkauf J, Jędrzejewski W, Schmidt K, Okarma H, Ruczyński I,
to investigate the use of a tree as a scent-marking post by cheetahs in the Śnieżko S, Gula R. 2003. Daily patterns and duration of wolf activity in
Thabazimbi district. S Afr J Wildl Res. 36:139–145. the Białowieża Forest, Poland. J Mammal. 84:243–253.
McNutt JW. 1996. Sex-biased dispersal in African wild dogs, Lycaon pictus. Warton DI, Hui FKC. 2010. The arcsine is asinine: the analysis of propor-
Anim Behav. 52:1067–1077. tions in ecology. Ecology. 92:3–10.

View publication stats

You might also like