You are on page 1of 14

Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383

DOI 10.1007/s11121-011-0223-8

Contribution of Family Violence to the Intergenerational


Transmission of Externalizing Behavior
Miriam K. Ehrensaft & Patricia Cohen

Published online: 1 July 2011


# Society for Prevention Research 2011

Abstract Research finds that early antisocial behavior is a sion and hostile reactivity, and depressive mood in
risk for later intimate partner violence (IPV) perpetration and offspring. Implications for future research and prevention
victimization, and that children’s exposure to their parents’ are discussed.
IPV is a risk for subsequent behavior problems. This study
tests whether intimate violence (IPV) between partners Keywords Intimate partner violence . Family violence .
contributes independently to the intergenerational transmis- Psychopathology . Externalizing . Parenting
sion of antisocial behavior, using the Children in the
Community Study, a representative sample (N=821) followed
for over 25 years in 6 assessments. The present study
includes a subsample of parents (N=678) and their offspring Introduction
(N=396). We test the role of three mechanisms by which
IPV may influence child antisocial behavior—parental Three-generational studies consistently find evidence sup-
psychopathology, parenting practices, and child self- porting the intergenerational transmission of externalizing
regulation. Results suggest that IPV independently increased psychopathology across generations (Capaldi et al. 2003;
the risk for offspring externalizing problems, net of the Conger et al. 2003; Kim et al. 2009; Thornberry et al.
effects of parental history of antisocial behavior and family 2003). This familial transmission is critical from the
violence. IPV also increased the risk for parental post perspective of prevention, because of the serious additive
traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and alcohol use disorder sequellae of antisocial behavior over the life course (Moffitt
2 years later, but not for major depressive disorder. Alcohol et al. 2001; Patterson et al. 1989). These findings have lead
use disorder independently increased the risk for offspring to the pursuit of models explaining the propagation of
externalizing behavior, but IPV continued to predict off- antisocial behavior within families.
spring externalizing net of parental alcohol use. Parenting, A number of studies have begun to investigate the role
particularly low satisfaction with the child, was significantly of family conflict and violence as a major environmental
associated with both IPV and externalizing behavior, but stressor that may lead to increased risk for transmission of
did not mediate the effects of IPV on externalizing. IPV antisocial behavior (Ehrensaft, Cohen, et al. 2003; Capaldi
predicted higher levels of emotional expressivity, aggres- and Clark 1998; Jaffee et al. 2002; Yates et al. 2003).
Developmental psychologists are increasingly recognizing
the influence of child maltreatment and intimate partner
M. K. Ehrensaft (*) violence (IPV) on risk for new episodes of mental health
Dept of Psychology, John Jay College of Criminal Justice,
445 West 59th street,
disorders in those who are at familial risk, given their
New York, NY 10019, USA potential to impact physical health and to disrupt family
e-mail: mehrensaft@jjay.cuny.edu constellations, living conditions, and key support networks
(Ehrensaft 2005; Horwitz et al. 2001).
M. K. Ehrensaft : P. Cohen
Div. of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Columbia University
In fact, a wealth of studies documents the increased risk
College of Physicians & Surgeons, for externalizing psychopathology among the offspring of
New York, NY, USA men and women involved in IPV (Evans, Davies and
Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383 371

DiLillo 2008; Kitzman et al. 2003). The earliest studies impulsive and to use harsh punishment with their children
were limited by methodological problems, particularly (Osofsky 1998). These types of parenting practices have
reliance on samples of women and children in domestic been associated with elevated risk for internalizing and
violence shelters, and cross-sectional designs (Jouriles et al. externalizing problems in children (Chamberlain et al.
2001; Silva et al. 2000). Recent studies have addressed 1997). Limited evidence suggests that the quality of the
some of these flaws with epidemiological studies and parent–child relationship mediates the influence of IPV
longitudinal designs, finding evidence for the independent exposure on child behavior (Lieberman et al. 2005). On the
influence of exposure to IPV between parents and elevated other hand, these findings are based on a sample of
risk for mental health problems, substance abuse, and preschool children referred for either mental health prob-
juvenile crime (Ehrensaft, Cohen et al. 2006; Fergusson and lems or maternal experience of IPV, limiting generalizabil-
Horwood 1998; Wolfe et al. 2003), even accounting for ity to unreferred samples. Few longitudinal studies have
heritable risks (Jaffee et al. 2002; Yates et al. 2003). followed offspring past the preschool years.
Despite this clear identification of risk attributable to
IPV exposure, little remains known about the mechanisms IPV May Influence the Development of Emotional
by which the risk operates to influence subsequent and Behavioral Self-Regulation
psychopathology. Stress theory (Cicchetti and Walker
2001) provides a useful framework to conceptualize Finally, individual differences in self-regulation, such as
possible mechanisms by which IPV may influence the mood regulation, emotional expressivity, aggressive behav-
intergenerational transmission of psychopathology (Wolfe ior, and hostile reactivity, may be affected by exposure to
et al. 1985). That is, IPV, a known developmental outcome IPV. Patterns of emotional and behavioral regulation are
of early antisocial behavior (Ehrensaft et al. 2003), may learned and reinforced in the family, and may become
stress both parents and children on the level of parental stable with repetition (Gilliom et al. 2002). Studies of early
emotional distress, parent–child interactions (Jouriles et al. stress, such as child maltreatment, support its association
2009), and interference with the development of children’s with altered neurobiological processing, particularly the
stress reactivity (Kaufman and Charney 2001). Extant hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis functioning
literature points to three possible mechanisms that reflect (Kaufman et al. 1997). Abused children exhibit increased
this overarching stress framework: parental psychopatholo- anticipatory monitoring in response to hostile interpersonal
gy, parenting practices, and child self-regulation, reviewed interactions and greater difficulty regulating their arousal
presently. (Pollack et al. 2005). These neurobiological changes are
known to increase risk for depression (Kessler and Magee
IPV Increases Risk for Parental Psychopathology 1993) and reactive aggression (Lopez-Duran et al. 2009).

IPV may impact the risk for child psychopathology by Current Study and Hypotheses
increasing the risk for parental psychopathology. Although
family history of psychopathology increases vulnerability We sought to test intergenerational models linking family
to developing psychopathology, the risk increases with violence exposure with the risk for propagation of
environmental stressors (Kraemer et al. 1997). Domestic psychopathology across generations. Using the Children
violence has been associated with mental health symptoms, in the Community Study (CIC) sample—a prospective
particularly depression (Carlson et al. 2003; Levendosky longitudinal study of a representative sample of youth
and Graham-Bermann 2001; Stein and Kennedy 2001), and (Generation 2; G2), their parents (Generation 1; G1), and
anxiety (Carlson et al. 2003; Mertin and Mohr 2001). In their own offspring (Generation 3; G3), followed for over
prospective longitudinal studies, IPV increases the risk in 25 years—we tested five sets of hypotheses. First, we
young women for new episodes of mental health disorders, expected that adolescent conduct disorder (CD) measured
particularly major depressive disorder, post-traumatic prospectively in one generation (G2) would predict exter-
stress disorder (PTSD), and substance abuse disorders, nalizing behavior problems in the next (G3). Second, we
net of an earlier history of such disorders (Ehrensaft et al. expected that IPV reported by those youth (G2) in early
2006). adulthood would predict subsequent externalizing symp-
toms in their offspring in middle childhood (G3). We
IPV Increases Risk for Adverse Parenting Practices controlled for G2 parents’ prospectively measured child-
hood risks assumed to independently increase the risk for
Parenting practices represents a second possible mechanism externalizing behavior, including child abuse, exposure to
for the influence of IPV on child psychopathology. There is parental (G1) IPV, and an adolescent history of conduct
some evidence that victimized mothers tend to be more disorder. Third, we hypothesized that the association of
372 Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383

parental IPV and later child externalizing problems would assessments. Interviews were conducted in the home by
be accounted for by parental psychiatric disorders that are intensively trained and supervised lay personnel. Youths
known to be outcomes of partner violence (PTSD, and mothers were interviewed separately, and each
substance abuse, and depressive disorders). Fourth, we interviewer was blind to the other informant’s responses.
expected parenting practices would account for the influ- In 1999, a questionnaire on recent life changes, work
ence of IPV and parental psychopathology on later child history, aggressive behavior, intimate partner history, and
symptomatology. Finally, we expected that parental IPV partner violence was mailed to 815 participants, 582
would predict offspring preadolescent/adolescent self- (71%) of whom responded (detailed in Measures).
regulation (poorer mood regulation, higher aggression, Detailed descriptions of the sample characteristics, proce-
hostile reactivity, depressive mood, and emotional expres- dures and follow up are available in earlier reports (Cohen
sivity). Figure 1 outlines the hypotheses and the expected and Cohen 1996).
relationships between variables. G2 were 13.8 years (SD=2.6, range=9–19) in 1983
(Wave 2), 16.2 years (SD=2.8, range=11–22) in 1985–
1986 (Wave 3), 22.1 years (SD=2.7, range=17–28) in
Methods 1991–1993 (Wave 4), 31.0 years (SD=2.7, range=26–35)
in 1999 (Wave 5), and 33.48 years (SD=2.73 range=27–
Participants and Procedure 38) in 2001–2004 (Wave 6). At Wave 6, of 678 total
participants, 396 had children and completed measures of
The CIC cohort derives from a randomly selected sample offspring (G3) behavior and their parenting practices.
residing in two upstate New York counties in 1975 Women were about 5% more likely to participate in each
(Kogan et al. 1977). The area sampled for this study was adult follow-up, but differential sample attrition was
selected to be generally representative of the Northeastern unrelated to age, race, socioeconomic class, or with
United States on socioeconomic status (SES) and the adolescent or adult psychiatric disorders.
majority of demographic variables, but reflected the From 2000 to 2006, the 10–18 year old offspring (n=
sampled region with regard to high proportions of 190; median age=13.00 years, SD=2.40) of the G2 study
Catholic (54%) and Caucasian (91%) participants. From participants (i.e., Generation 3; G3) were invited to
1975 to 1983, 54 new families were recruited from urban participate in a telephone-based interview on opposite-sex
poverty areas to replace families lost to urban renewal. At relationship development, peer relations, and self-
first follow-up (1983), 821 parents (G1) and their regulation. Most were interviewed within 1–3 years after
offspring (G2) were interviewed on a range of health, the recruitment of their parents for the Wave 6 assessments
behavioral and environmental factors. The youths and (M=29 months; SD=13.7). Those who were recruited later
their mothers were assessed in three additional follow-up were younger, and were invited to participate once they
interviews (1985–1986, 1991–1993, and 2001–2004) turned 10. We successfully recruited 129 (68%) of age-
regarding demographic, psychiatric, and other psychoso- eligible G3 offspring. Aside from age, children who
cial factors. This sample was demographically representa- participated in the Teen Study did not differ significantly
tive of the sampled Northeast regions, with retention rates from the children who did not participate, on gender, SES,
of 95% until 1991–1993, and 84% by the 2001–2004 or ethnicity.

Fig. 1 Proposed conceptual G2


Childhood/ G2 Mean G2 Mean G3 Mean
model—parental IPV predicting Adolescence Age 31 Age 33 Age 13
offspring externalizing behaviors

G2 Adult
Disorders

G2 Conduct
Disorder

G3
G2 Childhood
IPV Exposure
G2 IPV Externalizing

G2 Childhood
Abuse G3 Self-
Regulation

G2 Parenting
of G3
Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383 373

The telephone-based, computerized format was selected heard as a child physical fights between their parents or
because the original study participants and their families between a parent and his or her partner (never, once, or two
were now living across multiple states, making in-person or more times). Respondents were counted as exposed to
interviews costly. Telephone assessments are commonly IPV as a child if they reported two or more incidents of IPV
used to measure sensitive personal information about on this scale, 14% (N=80) of G2. This retrospective
adolescents, including psychiatric symptomatology, antiso- measure was associated with prospective measures of
cial behavior, and substance abuse (Gould et al. 2004; ‘rough conflict resolution’ asked of G2’s mothers (i.e.,
Shaffer et al. 2000), as evidence suggests that adolescents G1) at the Wave 2, 3 and 4 assessments. Reliability and
are more likely to disclose sensitive information in validity of this measure are reported in detail in Ehrensaft,
computer- and telephone-based assessments than in face- Cohen, et al. (2003).
to-face interviews (Rohde et al. 1997; Supple et al. 1999).
Interviewers were laypersons extensively trained for the G2 Abuse and Neglect in Childhood
earlier protocols. The interview was designed and admin-
istered using Dialogix, a computer software tool designed Data on child abuse were obtained using a combination of
to support complex data entry of healthcare information retrospective self-reports obtained in 1991–1993 (Wave 4,
(Choi et al. 2005). Dialogix has been used to computerize M age 22 years) of all G2 aged 18 years or over (N=664),
and deploy a wide range of survey instruments used in large and official records from the New York State Central
epidemiological trials, Web-enabled decision trees, clinical Registry (full details in Cohen et al. 2001; Ehrensaft,
guidelines, and consumer-oriented decision support tools Cohen, et al. 2003). Consistent with prior work on child
(Choi et al. 2005). abuse and neglect on this sample, we used a combined
The Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric either/or measure of self-reported or official records of
Institute Institutional Review Boards approved all CIC study physical abuse (n=37), official records of neglect (n=14),
procedures. Written informed consent or assent was obtained and self-reported sexual abuse (n=19).
from all participants after the interview procedures were fully
explained. A National Institute of Health Certificate of G2 Childhood Psychiatric Disorders
Confidentiality was also secured for the study. Further details
about the study methodology is published (Cohen and Cohen Parent and youth versions of the Diagnostic Interview
1996; Johnson et al. 2001) and described on the study Web Schedule for Children (Costello et al. 1984) were admin-
site (http://nyspi.org/childcom). istered in 1983 (Wave 2), and 1985–1986 (Wave 3) for
disruptive behavior disorders (attention-deficit disorder
Measures [ADD], oppositional defiant disorder [ODD], and CD),
and for other disorders not included in this study. The
Table 1 lists the measures used in the study, in addition to current study includes only an assessment of CD and ODD.
sample items, scale reliabilities, and assessment period We combined mother (G1) and youth reports (G2), so that
information. Each measure is also detailed below. symptoms were considered present if endorsed by either the
parent or the child. This ‘or’ rule is based on empirical
Socioeconomic Status evidence that both the child and parent contribute unique
information to the diagnosis (Loeber et al. 1990).
Parental (G1) SES was assessed in 1975, 1983, and 1985– To improve on the specificity of the resulting diagnoses,
1986, as a standardized sum of: (a) maternal and paternal we created a scale for each syndrome based on all the
years of education, (b) maternal and paternal occupational relevant items, including associated impairment, and gave
status, and (c) family income category (Hollingshead and ‘severe’ diagnoses to children who scored at least two SD’s
Redlich 1958). In the present study, we employed the above the population mean. This approach generates better
1985–1986 score (Wave 3) which had more complete SES construct validity and prevalences consistent with clinical
data. Parental SES was chosen to represent G2’s childhood practice (Cohen et al. 1987, 1993; Piacentini et al. 1992).
disadvantage, which has been found to predict adult health We pooled diagnostic information from 1983 (Wave 2) and
and psychological distress even after controlling for adult 1985–1986 (Wave 3) interviews to produce an estimated
SES (Poulton et al. 2002). adolescent diagnosis (N=811; M age=14.9 years; SD=1.8).

G2 Childhood Exposure to IPV G2 Adult Psychiatric Disorders

A section of the 1999 crime questionnaire asked G2 We assessed Axis I psychiatric disorders at Wave 6
respondents (N=582, M age 31) whether they had seen or (N=671; M age 33) with the nonpatient version of the
374 Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383

Table 1 Measures used in the current study

Measure Respondent Wave Mean N α Sample items


age

Adolescent Psychological G1 & G2 2 & 3 15 811 N/A Followed DISC interview, e.g. “loses temper”;
Disorders (ODD, CD) “destructive”; “gets others in trouble,”
Child Abuse and Neglect G2 4 22 664 N/A “Did any older person (not a boyfriend or girlfriend)
(Retrospective) (≥18 Only) ever touch you or play with you sexually ?”
Exposure to Intimate Partner G2 VC 31 577 N/A “Did you see or hear as a child physical fights between
Violence (Retrospective) your parents or between your parent and your parent’s
partner?”
Intimate Partner Violence G2 VC 31 543 .89 “I kicked, bit, or hit my partner with a fist.”;
“My partner pushed, grabbed, or shoved me.”
Adult Psychological Disorders G2 6 33 671 N/A Followed DSM-IV criteria, e.g., “depressed mood 2+
(MDD, PTSD, SUD) weeks”; “loss of energy 2+ weeks,”
Inconsistency G2 6 33 395 .44 “It sometimes depends on my mood how strict I
am with my child.”
Satisfaction G2 6 33 395 .79 “I am satisfied with my child’s emotional or social
behavior.”
Authority Orientation G2 6 33 392 .62 “I expect my child to do what I say no matter what.”
Rules for the Child G2 6 33 393 .54 Rules for…“time for being in at night.”
Closeness G2 6 33 393 .81 “I frequently show my love for my child.”
Child’s Identification with Parent G2 6 33 392 .76 “How much does your child emulate you?”
Discipline G2 6 33 396 .63 “I sometimes reject my child if he/she does something
I do not like.”
Externalizing Behaviors (CBCL) G2 6 9 (G3) 256 .89 “Hangs around with others who get in trouble.”;
“Disobedient at home.”; “Threatens people.”
Berkeley Expressivity G3 TS 12.8 129 .75 “I experience my emotions very strongly.”;
Questionnaire “My body reacts very strongly to emotional situations.“
Early Adolescent Temperament G3 TS 12.8 129 .63 “When I’m angry, I throw or break things.”;
Questionnaire—Aggressive “I pick on people for no real reason.”
Early Adolescent Temperament G3 TS 12.8 129 .70 “It often takes very little for me to feel like crying.”;
Questionnaire—Depressive “My friends/other people my age seem to enjoy
themselves more than I do.”
Negative Mood Regulation G3 TS 12.8 129 .71 “I can do something to feel better.”;
“Doing something nice for someone else will cheer me up.”

G2 = Generation 2 Parents (N=821); G3 = Generation 3 Offspring (N=396)


Wave 1: 1975; Wave 2: 1983; Wave 3: 1985–1986; Wave 4: 1991–1993; Wave 6: 2001–2004
VC Violence & Crime Survey, Mailed in 1999
TS Teen Study (Generation 3, 10–18 year olds only), 2002–2007
MDD Major Depressive Disorder; PTSD Post Traumatic Stress Disorder; SUD Susbtance Use Disorder

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders G2 IPV


(First et al. 1996). This interview has strong reliability and
validity (Williams et al. 1992). Here, we focused on The questionnaire mailed in 1999 (N=582; M age 31) asked
diagnoses of PTSD, major depressive disorder, and sub- respondents whether they had a romantic partner during the
stance use disorders, because these disorders have been last 12 months, and, if so, to answer a series of questions
shown to be significant outcomes of IPV, particularly about violence toward and from a partner, derived from the
among women (Ehrensaft et al. 2006). We combined data Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus 1990). The scale is described
on alcohol use disorders (alcohol abuse, alcohol depen- in detail in earlier publications (Ehrensaft, Cohen, et al.
dence) and drug use disorders (abuse, drug dependence 2003, 2006). Participants who denied having a partner
disorders), given the low prevalence of each of these during the past year were excluded from the analyses.
disorders individually. The scale had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s
Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383 375

alpha=.89). Consistent with other studies of partner participants, CBCL forms were mailed to female partners of
violence in community samples (Magdol et al. 1998; the G2 participants. Of the 396 G2 parents that had children,
Straus 1990; Straus et al. 1980), when we collapsed across 256 completed the CBCL (65%). Analysis indicates parents
all levels of severity and frequency, 22% of the sample who did not complete the CBCL did not differ significantly
who reported having a partner (n=543) endorsed perpe- from the parents who did not complete the CBCL on age,
trating at least one act of physical violence toward their SES, or ethnicity. Female partners of male participants were
partner, and 19% endorsed being the victim of physical less likely to complete the CBCL than female participants
violence by their partner in the last 12 months. No gender (F(1,394)=23.5, p<.01).
differences were observed.
To examine more serious partner violence seen by G3 Adolescent Self Regulation
clinicians, we focused on models of risks for perpetrating
(n=34, 6%) and receiving (n=35, 6%) injury by the use of As part of the Teen and Preteen Study (n=129, M age 13,
any act during the past year. Injury was assessed by asking 2000–2006), G3 youth who were 10–18 years of age
participants, ‘Did you or a partner of yours receive the completed three measures of self-regulation.
following injuries in a fight with each other: cuts, bruises,
broken bone or sprains’ and ‘Did you or a partner of yours Negative Mood Regulation Negative mood regulation was
require medical attention for injuries received in an assessed with a subset of 12 items from the Negative Mood
argument between you’. Responses were recorded sepa- Regulation (NMR) scale (Catanzaro and Mearns 1990;
rately for injuries received by the respondent and the Cronbach’s alpha=.71). Item responses are based on a 5-
partner. point Likert scale, from 1 (‘strongly disagree’) to 5
(‘strongly agree’). This scale was modestly associated with
G2 Parenting Practices internal-external locus of control, and high scores were
associated with low self-report scores on the Beck
At the Wave 6 (N=678; M age 33) interview, G2 participants Depression Inventory. Psychometric studies of the scale
who reported having at least one child (n=396) were suggest that the NMR scale is not a proxy for general
asked 33 items assessing parental behaviors, that when negative mood; rather, there is evidence for the discriminant
high (e.g., inconsistent parental discipline) or when low validity of the scale from measures of depression. All of the
(e.g., parental affection), have been found to be indicative age-eligible G3 participants (n=129) completed the NMR.
of problematic parenting (e.g., Johnson et al. 2001). These
items were adapted from the Disorganizing Poverty Berkeley Expressivity Questionnaire The Berkeley Expres-
Interview and other validated measures of child-rearing sivity Questionnaire (BEQ; Gross and John 1997; Cronbach’s
behavior (Avgar et al. 1977; Schaefer 1965). For the alpha=.75), as originally developed, assesses three facets of
present study, we employed scaled scores of parental individual differences in emotional expressivity: negative
reports of inconsistency (Cronbach’s alpha=.44), satis- expressivity, positive expressivity, and impulse strength. For
faction with child (Cronbach’s alpha=.79), orientation to the purpose of the present study, only the impulse strength
authority with regards to the child (Cronbach’s alpha=.62), items (six in total) were included (‘I experience my emotions
closeness (Cronbach’s alpha=.81), child’s identification very strongly’; ‘I am sometimes unable to hide my feelings,
with the parent (Cronbach’s alpha=.76), rules for the even though I would like to’; ‘There have been times when I
child (Cronbach’s alpha=.54), and discipline (Cronbach’s have not been able to stop crying even though I tried to
alpha = .63). These child-rearing behavior assessment stop’; ‘My body reacts very strongly to emotional situations’;
items and subscales administered in the CIC are reliable ‘I sometimes cry during sad movies’; and ‘I am sometimes
and valid (Cohen and Cohen 1996; Johnson et al. 2001; unable to hide my feelings, even though I would like to’).
Kogan et al. 1977), and predict offspring risk for Although the original scale was designed with a 7-point
psychiatric symptoms and disorders (Cohen and Brook Likert scale, the scale was shortened to a 5-point scale for
1987; Johnson et al. 2001; Johnson et al. 2008). the present study, because the nature of the telephone-based
interview for adolescents required a parsimonious format
G3 Externalizing Behavior across several scales. In this way, item response choices were
identical for the BEQ and the NMR. All of the age-eligible
G3 behavior problems in middle childhood were assessed by G3 participants (n=129) completed the BEQ.
maternal report with the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL;
Achenbach 1991) externalizing problems scale during the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire (EATQ)—
Wave 6 G2 assessment, when G3 were a mean age of Aggressive and Depressive Behavior Scales This scale was
9 years. For G3 youth who were offspring of male G2 designed to assess temperament in adolescents aged 9–
376 Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383

15 years, but includes two behavioral subscales that were We first conducted descriptive analyses of the associa-
employed in the present study (Capaldi and Rothbart tions among variables. Second, we tested for multivariate
1992; Ellis and Rothbart 2001). The aggressive behavior associations, using hierarchical linear regressions, of
scale (Cronbach’s alpha=.63) measures hostile and ag- parents’ (G2) childhood risks (abuse, exposure to G1 IPV,
gressive actions, including person- and object-directed adolescent CD) and G2 adult IPV with the risk for offspring
physical violence, direct and indirect verbal aggression, and (G3) behavior problems. Next, we conducted hierarchical
hostile reactivity. The depressive mood scale (Cronbach’s linear regressions to test whether G2 adult psychopathology
alpha=.70) includes unpleasant affect and lowered mood, and parenting account for the association of G2 IPV and G3
and loss of enjoyment and interest in activities (Ellis and externalizing problems. Finally, we employed linear regres-
Rothbart 2001). All of the age-eligible G3 participants sion to test, in the sub-sample of 10–18 year old G3’s, the
(n=129) completed the EATQ. risk for poor self-regulation conferred by prior exposure to
G2 IPV.
Analytic Plan

Missing data was handled using the Expectation Maximi- Results


zation (EM) method in the SPSS Missing Values module to
impute missing data for G3 externalizing scores, resulting Descriptive Statistics
in a complete dataset (N=396). Expectation maximization
is an iterative procedure and consists of two steps, the first All variables were first examined for distribution and
of which involves computing the expected value of the standardized scores were used where indicated. Table 2
complete data log likelihood, while the second step shows the bivariate correlations among variables. G3
substitutes the expected values for the missing data externalizing was significantly correlated with G2 IPV, G2
obtained in the prior step to obtain new parameter estimates MDD and alcohol use disorder, G2 parenting practices, and
(Dempster et al. 1977). The EM method has been G3 aggression/hostility. G2 IPV was significantly correlat-
demonstrated to perform better than listwise and pairwise ed with G2 PTSD, alcohol use disorder, most parenting
deletion, and stochastic regression imputation in terms of practices, and three of the four G3 self-regulation measures.
parameter estimates and standard errors and performs Significant correlations were also obtained among the G2
similarly to multiple imputation in some cases (Newman parenting variables, and among most G3 self-regulation
2003). Analyses predicting G3 externalizing behaviors variables.
consisted of 396 participants, while analyses predicting The prevalence of G2 psychiatric disorders at age
Emotional or Self-Regulation consisted of a subset of 129 33 years was as follows: MDD, 12.2% (n=82); PTSD,
10–18 year old G3 offspring. 2.7% (n=18); any substance use disorder, 13.7% (n=92)

Table 2 Correlation matrix for all variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. G3 Externalizing 1 .35** .17** .07 .21** .28** .20** −.27** −.40** .06 .02 .20* .17+
2. G2 IPV .04 .11* .12** .10+ .16** −.09 −.19** .21* .06 .20* .19*
3. G2 MDD .23** .08* .01 .03 −.09+ −.14** .04 −.03 .22* .09
4. G2 PTSD .06 −.07 .11* −.02 .01 .09 −.03 .05 −.003
5. G2 Alcohol Use Disorder .10* .05 −.09+ −.14** .06 .08 .07 .13
6. Discipline .27** −.17** −.38** .10 −.10 .17* .02
7. Inconsistency −.14** −.20** −.04 .13 .04 −.08
8. Child Identification with Parent .44** .12 .15+ −.01 .03
9. Satisfaction with Child −.03 .08 −.14 −.16+
10. G3 Emotional Expressivity −.09 .18* .49**
11. G3 Negative Mood Regulation −.21* −.26**
12. G3 Aggression/Hostility .34**
13. G3 Depressive Mood 1

G2 Generation 2; G3 Generation 3; IPV Intimate Partner Violence; MDD Major Depressive Disorder, age 33 years; PTSD Post-Traumatic Stress
Disorder, age 33 years
*p<.05. **p<.01. + p<.10
Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383 377

(summing participants with alcohol dependence disorder, 387)=1.39, p=.17), and therefore was excluded from
2.7% (n=18); drug dependence disorder, 1.9% (n=13); further multivariate models.
alcohol abuse disorder, 8.9% (n=60) and drug abuse
disorder, 1.9% (n=13). Additionally, 11.5% (n=78) met IPV between G2 Parents and Risk for G3 Behavior
criteria for either alcohol abuse or dependence disorder, Problems
and 3.9% (n=26) met criteria for either drug abuse or drug
dependence disorder. Women had significantly higher Univariate linear regression analyses, controlling for SES,
odds of PTSD diagnosis (OR = 9.88, 95% CI = 2.96– child age, and child sex, indicated that G2 reports of
32.98) and MDD diagnosis OR=1.74, 95% CI=1.07– injurious IPV significantly predicted G3 externalizing
2.81, p < .02). Men had significantly higher odds of scores, accounting for 11% of the variance (B=9.52, SE
substance use disorder diagnosis (OR=3.34, 95% CI= (B)=1.36, Beta=.23, t (4, 391)=7.00, p<.001). Further, as
2.06–5.41, p<.001). shown in Table 4, hierarchical linear regression indicates
that G2 IPV significantly predicted G3 externalizing
Models of G2 Childhood Risks, IPV, and G3 Externalizing problems after controlling for G2’s exposure to IPV, G2’s
history of child abuse, G2’s history of adolescent CD, and
G2 Childhood Risks for Externalizing Problems in G3 demographic characteristics, accounting for 6% of the
variance (B=7.60, SE(B)=1.46, Beta=.27, t (7, 388)=
Univariate linear regression analyses indicated that G2 5.20. p<.001).
childhood physical abuse (B=7.27, SE(B)=2.10, Beta = .17, Similarly, after controlling for demographics, G2’s
t (4, 391)=3.47, p<.01), G2’s exposure to parental IPV (B= history of child abuse, and G2’s exposure to IPV, a
3.40, SE(B)=.95, Beta=.19, t (4, 391)=3.60, p<.001), and history of CD predicted G2 reports of injurious IPV
conduct disorder (B=6.52, SE(B)=1.40, Beta=.23, t (4, 391)= perpetration at M age 31 (B=.17, SE(B)=.05, Beta=.16, t
4.66, p<.001) predicted G3 externalizing symptoms, control- (6, 389)=3.44, p<.01). To test whether IPV perpetration
ling for SES, G3 age, and G3 sex. Sexual abuse and neglect mediated between G2 history of adolescent CD and G3
were not significantly related to G3 externalizing problems, externalizing behaviors, we conducted a hierarchical linear
and were thus not included in further analyses. As shown in regression controlling for demographics, G2’s history of
Table 3, the effects of child abuse (B=4.79, SE(B)=2.12, child abuse, G2’s exposure to IPV, and the proposed
Beta=.11, t (5, 390)=2.27, p=.02) and G2’s exposure to mediator. The addition of IPV to the model did not
IPV remained significant (B=2.22, SE(B)=.96, Beta=.12, significantly decrease the effect of CD; that is, it
t (6, 389)=2.31, p=.02) following introduction of G2’s maintained a significant effect on G3 externalizing
history of CD to the model. G2 adolescent ODD behaviors, net of all other variables in the model (B=
significantly predicted G3 externalizing symptoms (B= 3.66, SE(B) =1.43, Beta=.13, t (7, 388)=2.55, p=.01).
3.38, SE(B)=1.21, Beta = .14, t (4, 388)=2.79, p=.01), but However, IPV explained approximately 6% of the vari-
it did not predict G3 externalizing problems independent ance, while CD explained 3% of the variance in G3
of the effects of CD (B=1.75, SE(B)=1.26, Beta=.07, t (5, externalizing behaviors.

Table 3 Hierarchical linear regression analysis for parental (G2) childhood risks predicting offspring (g3) externalizing problems

Model 1: Demographics Model 2: Family violence Model 3: Adolescent CD

B SEB Beta B SEB Beta B SEB Beta

SES −.51 .32 −.08 −.24 .32 −.04 −.29 .32 −.05
Offspring Age .15 .09 .09 .12 .09 .07 .11 .09 .07
Offspring Sex 1.86 .74 .13** 1.49 .72 .10* 1.69 .71 .12*
G2 Childhood Physical Abuse 6.21 2.10 .15** 4.79 2.12 .11*
G2 Childhood IPV Exposure 2.95 .95 .16** 2.22 .96 .12**
G2 Adolescent CD 4.94 1.46 .17**
R2 Change Model .04** .05** .03**
F for R2 Change 5.65 10.98 11.46

Analyses control for all variables in prior steps. G3 Generation 3; G2 Generation 2; SES Socioeconomic Status; CD Conduct Disorder. IPV
Intimate Partner Violence
*p<.05. **p<.01
378 Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383

Table 4 Hierarchical linear regression of offspring (G3) externalizing problems on G2 childhood risks, IPV, adult alcohol use disorder, and
parenting practices

Model 1: Childhood risks Model 2: IPV Model 3: G2 Disorder Model 4: Parenting


age 33 age 33

B SEB Beta B SEB Beta B SEB Beta B SEB Beta

Child IPV Exposure 2.22 .96 .12* 1.40 .94 .08 1.72 .93 .09+ 1.07 .87 .06
G2 Childhood Physical Abuse 4.79 2.12 .11* 2.10 2.11 .05 2.62 2.07 .06 2.62 1.94 .06
G2 Adolescent CD 4.94 1.46 .17** 3.66 1.43 .13** 2.58 1.42 .09+ 2.75 1.33 .10*
G2 IPV Age 31 Years 7.60 1.46 .27** 7.87 1.43 .28** 6.55 1.34 .23**
G2 Alcohol Use Disorder Age 33 Years 4.90 1.15 .20** 3.78 1.08 .15**
Discipline of Child .41 .14 .13**
Satisfaction with Child −.68 .16 −.22**
Child Identification with Parent −.50 .16 −.15**
R2 Change Model .08** .06** .04* .12**
F for R2 Change 11.34 27.03 18.17 21.92

G2 Generation 2; G2 Generation 3; SES Socioeconomic Status; CD Conduct disorder; IPV Intimate Partner Violence.
All models control for SES, offspring age, and offspring sex.
*p<.05. **p<.01. + p<.10

When analyses were conducted separately by the sex of the at age 33 years was not significantly associated with IPV at
parent, G2 IPV predicted G3 externalizing net the effects of all age 31 years (OR=1.49, 95% CI=0.53–4.18, p>.10) and
other variables included in the model (B=7.20, SE(B)=1.79, was therefore excluded from additional analyses.
Beta=.26, t (7, 225)=4.02, p<.001) for G2 females.
Similarly, G2 CD predicted G3 externalizing net the G2 Psychopathology as Mechanism Linking G2 IPV
effects of all other variables included in the model (B= with G3 Externalizing Problems
5.33, SE(B)=2.25, Beta=.15, t (6, 226)=2.37, p=.02) for
G2 females. For G2 males, net the effects of all other To test the hypothesis that G2 psychopathology developed
variables included in the model, G2 IPV continued to after the experience of IPV would mediate the effects of
predict G3 externalizing (B=8.99, SE(B)=2.69, Beta=.30, IPV on child externalizing behavior, we first conducted a
t (7, 155)=3.35, p<.01), while G2 CD was no longer hierarchical linear regression of G3 externalizing problems
significant (B=3.32, SE(B)=1.82, Beta=.15, t (7, 155)= on each disorder, controlling for G3 age, sex, and SES, to
1.82, p=.07). Moderation analyses indicate that for G2 determine its independent association with G3 externalizing
females only, the interaction of G2 IPV and G2 CD scores.
significantly predicted G3 externalizing behaviors, net Using this approach, results suggest that this association was
the effects of all other variables in the model (B=10.21, not significant for PTSD, B=2.18, SE(B)=1.82, Beta=.06, t
SE(B)=5.19, Beta=.17, t (8, 224)=1.97, p=.05). (4, 391)=1.20, p=.23. As such, we did not pursue media-
tional models for PTSD.
Models of Psychopathology, IPV, and Externalizing Substance use disorder was associated with G3 exter-
nalizing problems (B=4.04, SE(B)=1.06, Beta = .19, t (4,
IPV between G2 Parents and Risk for G2 Psychopathology 391)=3.80, p<.001), and remained significant once we
controlled for the childhood risks described in Table 3 (B=
Hierarchical logistic regression of G2 PTSD at M age 3.57, SE(B)=1.04, Beta=.17, t (7, 388)=3.43, p<.001).
33 years on G2 IPV at M age 31 years, controlling for age Examination by type of substance disorder revealed that
and sex indicated that IPV significantly predicted the odds drug use disorders were unrelated to G3 externalizing. In
of PTSD (OR=4.45, 95% CI=1.14–17.32, p<.03). The contrast, alcohol use disorder predicted externalizing prob-
composite of any substance use disorder was also signifi- lems on a univariate level, B=4.76, SE(B) 1.22, Beta=.19,
cantly associated with IPV at M age 31 years (OR=4.77, t (4, 391)=3.92, p<.001, and after controlling for childhood
95% CI=2.01–11.33, p<.001), as was alcohol use disorder risks, B=4.62, SE(B)=1.19, Beta=.19, t (7, 388)=3.88,
(OR=4.08, 95% CI=1.63−10.20, p<.003), and drug use p<.001, and therefore was retained for subsequent multivar-
disorder (OR=5.88, 95% CI=1.69–20.39, p<.001). MDD iate models.
Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383 379

A hierarchical linear regression (Table 4), controlling for ducing parenting into the model, B=3.78, SE(B)=1.08,
demographic characteristics and childhood risks, followed Beta=.09, t (11, 384)=3.50, p<.001. However, parenting
by IPV and then alcohol abuse, indicated that a diagnosis of did not mediate the effects of IPV on G3 externalizing.
alcohol use disorder added independently to the prediction That is, IPV retained a significant, independent effect on
of G3 externalizing scores, but did not significantly reduce G3 externalizing, B=6.55, SE(B)=1.34, Beta=.23, t (11,
the influence of IPV on externalizing problems (B=7.87, SE 384)=4.89, p<.001.
(B)=1.43, Beta=.28, t (8, 387)=5.50, p<.001). That is, no
evidence of mediation was obtained. We lacked sufficient Models of G3 Adolescent Self-Regulation and IPV
statistical power to test for moderation effects of G2
psychopathology with IPV, and so did not pursue these Association of G2 Childhood Risks with G3
models. Self-Regulation

Models of G2 Parenting, IPV, and Externalizing Adolescent aggression/hostile reactivity was unrelated to
parental history of childhood exposure to IPV (B=.11,
Association of G2 IPV with Parenting of G3 SE(B)=.17, Beta=.07, t (5, 111)=.66, p > .10) and CD
(B=−.16, SE(B)=.29, Beta=−.05, t (5, 111)=−.58, p>.10).
Univariate linear regression models of G2 parenting at M These were therefore excluded from the multivariate
age 33 years on reports of IPV at M age 31 years found models described below.
significant associations with lower satisfaction, B=−1.47,
SE(B)=.43, Beta=−.16, t (4, 391)=−3.44, p<.01, and Association of G2 IPV with Subsequent G3 Adolescent
marginally with greater inconsistency, B=.69, SE(B)=.36, Self-Regulation Assessment
Beta=.10, t (4, 391)=1.91, p=.06. No associations were
found for identification with the parent, nor with rules, A hierarchical linear regression model indicated that G2
authority orientation, discipline, or closeness to the child IPV significantly predicted Emotional Expressivity, con-
(Beta’s ranged from <.01 to .09). trolling for age, sex and SES, B=.57, SE(B)=.29, Beta=.16,
t (4, 107)=2.00, p<.05. On the other hand, a similar
Association of G2 Parenting with G3 Externalizing hierarchical linear regression indicated that G3 Negative
Problems Mood Regulation was unrelated to G2 IPV, B=1.27, SE
(B)=2.36, Beta=.08, t (4, 107)=0.54, p>.10. The same
Parenting practices accounted for 18% of the variance in G3 analytic approach (controlling for age only, as SES and
externalizing, net SES, age, and sex. Independent effects were sex were not related to the dependent variable) indicated
found for lower satisfaction with the child, B=−.91, SE that G2 IPV predicted adolescent aggressive behavior and
(B)=.18, Beta=−.29, t (10, 385)=−5.08, p<.001; disci- hostile reactivity, B=.80, SE(B) =0.22, Beta=.34, t (1,
pline, B=.34, SE(B)=.16, Beta=.11, t (10, 385)=2.18, 107)=3.69, p<.001. However, controlling for age, sex and
p=.03; and child identification with the parent, B=−.44, SES, as these again were associated with the dependent
SE(B)=.18, Beta=−.14, t (10, 385)=−2.43 p=.02. These variable, the association of depressive mood approached
three parenting variables were retained in future multivar- but did not reach significance for IPV, B=.47, SE(B)=.31,
iate models given their significant association with G3 Beta=.14, t (3, 107)=1.52, p=.13.
externalizing behavior.

Parenting as a Mediator of the Association of G3 Discussion


Externalizing with G2 IPV
This prospective longitudinal study of men and women
To test the hypothesis that parenting would mediate the (G2) sought to investigate the mechanisms by which IPV
association of G2 IPV with G3 externalizing problems, we may contribute to the intergenerational transmission of
conducted a hierarchical linear regression, controlling for antisocial behavior. Taking advantage of a three-genera-
demographics and G2 childhood risks, followed by IPV, tional study design, analyses controlled for prospective
alcohol use disorder, and finally, parenting practices. As measures of parents’ own childhood risks for IPV, including
shown in Table 4, parenting accounted for 12% of the official records of childhood physical abuse, childhood
variance in externalizing scores, net of the effects of these exposure to violence between parents, and adolescent
other variables (F change in R2 =21.92, p<.001). history of CD. We expected that both parental history of
Moreover, Table 4 (see Model 4) shows the influence CD and parental IPV would subsequently predict offspring
of alcohol use disorder remained significant after intro- externalizing behaviors. We hypothesized that adult G2
380 Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383

PTSD, alcohol abuse disorder, and MDD would mediate As expected, parenting practices were significantly
the effects of IPV on G3 externalizing. We further expected related to IPV, including lower levels of parental satisfac-
that parenting practices would account for the influence tion with the child, and marginally, with higher levels of
of both parental psychopathology and IPV on G3 inconsistency. In multivariate models, we found that low
externalizing. Finally, we hypothesized that G3 IPV satisfaction with the child was a particularly important
childhood exposure would predict their subsequent outcome of IPV and also a risk for child externalizing
adolescent self-regulation. problems. Previous studies on links between IPV and
Overall, findings indicated that family violence exerts a parenting practices have focused more on discipline and
significant influence on the transmission of antisocial consistency (e.g., Jouriles et al. 2001), and our studies
behavior across generations. G2’s childhood history of support the influence of IPV on these parenting practices.
physical abuse, as measured by prospective official records Our findings also suggest that parents in abusive relation-
and self-reports, G2 adolescent CD, as well as their ships may experience low levels of satisfaction with their
childhood exposure to G1 IPV were each associated with child, and such parental rejection appears to increase the
G3 externalizing. In multivariate analyses, the effects of risk for externalizing problems in youth.
G2’s exposure to their parents’ (G1) IPV and G2’s CD Finally, we sought to investigate the influence of
independently predicted G3 externalizing behavior. Further, parental IPV on offspring self-regulation in a subsample
IPV between G2 as parents had a modest but significant of G3 adolescents. Emotional expressivity, but not mood
influence on G3 externalizing, above and beyond the regulation, was positively associated with their prior
effects of G2’s prior CD and G2’s own history of IPV exposure to IPV, as was a measure of aggression and
exposure. These findings suggest that IPV makes an hostile reactivity. It appears that IPV between parents
important, independent contribution to the intergenerational increases the risk for children’s difficulties with impulsive
transmission of antisocial behavior. Moreover, for females, emotionality and aggressive personality styles in adoles-
but not males, we obtained a significant interaction of their cence, in addition to its influence on externalizing behavior
prior CD with their IPV on offspring externalizing. That is, in middle childhood. These types of dysregulation are
for mothers, the risk conferred by CD on offspring known to be risks for perpetration of IPV in adults
externalizing is moderated by the presence of IPV. For (Ehrensaft, Cohen et al. 2006). Our findings indicate that
fathers, IPV and CD each exert independent, non-interactive these traits appear well before adult intimate relationships
effects on the risk for child externalizing. This suggests that are in place.
mothers’ IPV is at least as important as fathers’ as an
important intergenerational pathway to antisocial behavior. Strengths and Limitations
On the other hand, the results may reflect differences in the
validity of mothers’ and fathers’ IPV reporting. The strengths of this study are its prospective longitudinal
As expected, G2 (parent) IPV at age 31 years predicted design and inclusion of prospective measures of family
their subsequent PTSD and substance use disorder at age violence, psychopathology and parenting practices from
33 years. Surprisingly, IPV did not predict an increased risk two generations of informants. Moreover, this study is one
for MDD. Prior studies suggest that young women who of the few to have followed up a third generation into its
experience IPV are at significant risk for developing MDD adolescent years, extending prior work linking IPV,
in their mid-20s (Ehrensaft, Cohen, Chen and Johnson parenting and risk for antisocial behavior (Levendosky et
2006). It is possible that the current study did not find these al. 2003).
effects because MDD is no longer a risk at this later Limitations include the relatively small sample size of
developmental period; rather, the risk is limited to the the third generation, particularly the data on adolescent
period of the early twenties. temperament and self-regulation. Three-generational studies
In multivariate models predicting G3 externalizing are exceptionally difficult endeavors, principally because
behavior in middle childhood, we were surprised to find the sample has often migrated significantly by this time,
that parental (G2) alcohol use disorders did not mediate the and the researchers must establish a new alliance with the
influence of their earlier IPV on offspring externalizing. offspring of the study members who are well-invested in
Instead, IPV continued to exert an independent influence on the study after several decades of participation. A second
externalizing. Parenting, although not a mediator of IPV on limitation is the lack of data on partners of G2 study
risk for offspring externalizing, was a more potent predictor participants, such as their childhood antisocial behavior and
than parental psychopathology. This finding is consistent reports of IPV exposure. However, in view of high rates of
with other work supportive of the central role of parenting assortative partnering in individuals with childhood antiso-
practices in youth at familial risk for externalizing behavior cial behavior and high rates of concordance of IPV (Kim
(Conger et al. 2003; Ehrensaft, Wasserman, et al. 2003) and Capaldi 2004), we can assume that the risk for
Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383 381

antisocial behavior would also have been higher in the mediate the influence of IPV exposure on child externalizing
partners of our G2 participants who met criteria for CD in problems in middle childhood, but makes an independent
adolescence, as would IPV. Lastly, although the measures contribution. Parenting practices appear to make a greater
of G2 CD and G2 childhood abuse used multiple informants, independent contribution and account, in part, for the
other measures used in the study (e.g., parenting practices and influence of alcohol use disorders. Finally, differences in
self-regulation) were based on self-report. The study could aggressive behavior, hostile reactivity, and emotional ex-
have been strengthened by utilizing a multi-informant pressivity in adolescent offspring appear to relate to their
approach for all included measures. IPV exposure in middle childhood. Prevention should be
multimodal and adapt its focus to the child’s developmental
Prevention Implications period, targeting the parent–child relationship in early to
middle childhood, and strategies to reduce emotional expres-
Two sets of findings in the present study may inform the sivity, hostile reactivity, and aggression in adolescence.
design of preventive interventions for families experiencing
IPV. First, our findings indicate that parents who have Acknowledgments This study was funded in part by grants from the
experienced IPV are more likely to feel dissatisfied with Office of Child Development (OCD-CB-18; Dr. Leonard Kogan, P.I.),
The National Institute of Mental Health (MH-36971/MH-38916/MH-
their children, and in so doing may communicate rejection 49191/MH-60911; Dr. Patricia Cohen, P.I.; K08-MH-01913; Dr.
to their children. Thus, preventive efforts may be usefully Miriam Ehrensaft, P.I.), and the National Institute of Justice
focused on improving positive parental perceptions of the (1JCX0029; Dr. Patricia Cohen, P.I.).
child and efficacy in the role as parent. Our findings echo
those of others suggesting that parents in violent relation-
ships need help to strengthen fractured parent–child References
relationships. Although it is certainly possible that parental
dissatisfaction with the child accurately reflects that their
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-
child is more demanding behaviorally and emotionally 18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont,
during this high-stress period, changing parental cognitive Department of Psychiatry.
representations of the intentionality and function of misbe- Avgar, A., Bronfenbrenner, U., & Henderson, C. R. (1977).
Socialization practices of parents, teachers, and peers in Israel:
havior appears crucial. Improving parents’ sense of efficacy
Kibbutz, moshav, and city. Child Development, 48, 1219–1227.
in the role of parent seems equally critical. Capaldi, D. M., & Clark, S. (1998). Prospective family predictors of
Second, the present findings of problems in self- aggression toward female partners for at-risk young men.
regulation are a set of modifiable risks with important Developmental Psychology, 34, 1175–1188.
Capaldi, D. M., & Rothbart, M. K. (1992). Development and
developmental implications for prevention of future IPV in validation of an early adolescent temperament measure. Journal
exposed youth. Our findings highlight the importance of of Early Adolescence, 12, 153–173.
reducing aggressive, hostile, and verbally abusive interper- Capaldi, D. M., Pears, K. C., Patterson, G. R., & Owen, L. D. (2003).
sonal styles, ideally in the context of interventions that Continuity of parenting practices across generations in an at-risk
sample: A prospective comparison of direct and mediated associa-
target overall social cognitive errors and problem-solving
tions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 127–14.
skills (Lochman and Wells 2004). Carlson, B., McNutt, L., & Choi, D. (2003). Childhood and adult
From a developmental lens, these findings further abuse among women in primary health care: Effects on mental
highlight the importance of multimodal strategies to prevent health. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 924–941.
Catanzaro, S. J., & Mearns, J. (1990). Measuring generalized
the transmission of antisocial behavior in youth exposed to expectancies for negative mood regulation: Initial scale develop-
family violence, with a focus on parenting strategies in the ment and implications. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54,
school years, and an inclusion of direct social cognitive and 546–563.
behavioral problem solving strategies in adolescence. Chamberlain, P., Reid, J. B., Ray, J., Capaldi, D., & Fisher, P. (1997).
DSM-IV review for parent inadequate discipline (PID). In T. A.
Moreover, although our findings have focused on psycho-
Widiger (Ed.), DSM- IV Vol. 3 (pp. 569–629). Washington, DC:
logical mechanisms of risk for IPV on child psychopathol- American Psychiatric Association.
ogy, we also recognize the importance of addressing basic Choi, J., Jenkins, M. L., Cimino, J. J., White, T. M., & Bakken, S.
needs in these families, such as housing instability and (2005). Toward semantic interoperability in home health care:
Formally representing OASIS items for integration into a
poverty (Levendosky and Graham-Bermann 2001). concept-oriented terminology. Journal of American Medical
In summary, this study finds that IPV exposure contrib- Information Association, 12, 410–417.
utes significantly to the intergenerational transmission of Cicchetti, D., & Walker, E. F. (2001). Editorial: Stress and
antisocial behavior. The experience of IPV is related to development: Biological and psychological consequences. De-
velopment and Psychopathology, 13, 413–418.
parents’ subsequent risk for PTSD and substance use Cohen, P., & Brook, J. (1987). Family factors related to the
disorders, over reliance on discipline, and parental dissatis- persistence of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence.
faction with their child. Parental psychopathology does not Journal for the Study of Interpersonal Processes, 50, 332–345.
382 Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383

Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (1996). Life values and adolescent mental Gross, J., & John, O. L. (1997). Revealing feelings: Facets of
health. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. emotional expressivity in self-reports, peer ratings and behavior.
Cohen, P., Velez, C. N., Kohn, M., Schwab-Stone, M., & Johnson, J. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 435–448.
(1987). Child psychiatric diagnosis by computer algorithm: Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, F. C. (1958). Social class and mental
Theoretical issues and empirical tests. Journal of the American illness. New York: Wiley.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 631–638. Horwitz, A. V., Widom, C. S., McLaughlin, J., & White, H. R. (2001).
Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Kasen, S., Velez, C. N., Hartmark, C., Johnson, The impact of childhood abuse and neglect on adult mental
J., et al. (1993). An epidemiological study of disorders in late health: A prospective study. Journal of Health and Social
childhood and adolescence: Age- and gender-specific prevalence. Behavior, 42(2), 184–201.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 851–867. Jaffee, S. R., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., & Arseneault, L.
Cohen, P., Brown, J., & Smailes, E. (2001). Child abuse and neglect (2002). Influence of adult domestic violence on children’s
and the development of mental disorders in the general internalizing and externalizing problems: An environmentally
population. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 981–999. informative twin study. Journal of the American Academy of
Conger, R. D., Neppl, T., Kim, K. J., & Scaramella, L. (2003). Angry Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1095–1103.
and aggressive behavior across three generations: A prospective, Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., Smailes, E. M., & Brook, J. S.
longitudinal study of parents and children. Journal of Abnormal (2001). Association of maladaptive parental behavior with
Child Psychology, 31, 143–160. psychiatric disorder among parents and their offspring. Archives
Costello, E. J., Edelbrock, C. S., Duncan, M. K., & Kalas, R. (1984). of General Psychiatry, 58, 453–460.
Testing of the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., & Brook, J. S. (2008).
(DISC) in a clinic population: Final report to the Center for Psychiatric disorders in adolescence and early adulthood and
Epidemiological Studies, NIMH. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh. risk for child-rearing difficulties during middle adulthood.
Dempster, A. P., Laird, N. H., & Rubin, D. B. (1977). Maximum Journal of Family Issues, 29, 210–233.
likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of Jouriles, E. N., McDonald, R., Norwood, W. D., & Ezell, E. (2001).
the Royal Statistical Society, B39, 1–38. Issues and controversies in documenting the prevalence of
Ehrensaft, M. K. (2005). Interpersonal relationships and sex differ- children’s exposure to domestic violence. In S. A. Graham-
ences in the development of conduct problems. Clinical Child Bermann & J. L. Edleson (Eds.), Domestic violence in the lives
and Family Psychology Review, 8, 39–63. of children: The future of research, intervention, and social
Ehrensaft, M. K., Cohen, P., Brown, J., Smailes, E., Chen, H., & policy (pp. 12–34). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Johnson, J. G. (2003). Intergenerational transmission of partner Association.
violence: A 20-year prospective study. Journal of Consulting and Jouriles, E. N., McDonald, R., Rosenfield, D., Stephens, N., Corbitt-
Clinical Psychology, 71, 741–753. Shindler, D., & Miller, P. C. (2009). Reducing conduct problems
Ehrensaft, M. K., Wasserman, G. A., Verdeli, L., Greenwald, S., among children exposed to intimate partner violence: A
Miller, L. S., & Davies, M. (2003). Maternal antisocial behavior, randomized clinical trial examining effects of Project Support.
parenting practices, and behavior problems in boys at risk for Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 705–717.
antisocial behavior. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 12, 27– Kaufman, J., & Charney, D. (2001). Effects of early stress on brain
40. structure and function: Implications for understanding the
Ehrensaft, M. K., Cohen, P., Chen, H., & Johnson, J. G. (2006). relationship between child maltreatment and depression. Devel-
Development of personality disorder symptoms and the risk for opment and Psychopathology, 13, 451–471.
partner violence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 14, 474–483. Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Perel, J., Dahl, R. E., Moreci, P., Nelson,
Ehrensaft, M. K., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2006). Is domestic B., et al. (1997). The corticotropin-releasing hormone challenge
violence followed by an increased risk of psychiatric disorders in depressed abused, depressed nonabused, and normal control
among women but not among men?: A longitudinal cohort study. children. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 669–679.
The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1–7. Kessler, R. C., & Magee, W. J. (1993). Childhood adversities and
Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Revision of the early adult depression: Basic patterns of association in a US national
adolescent temperament questionnaire. Poster presented at the survey. Psychological Medicine, 23(3), 679–690.
Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop- Kim, H. K., & Capaldi, D. M. (2004). The association of antisocial
ment, Minneapolis. behavior and depressive symptoms between partners and risk for
Evans, S. E., Davies, C., & DiLillo, D. (2008). Exposure to domestic aggression in romantic relationships. Journal of Family Psychol-
violence: A meta-analysis of child and adolescent outcomes. ogy, 18, 82–96.
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13, 131–140. Kim, H., Capaldi, D. M., Pears, K. C., Kerr, D. C. R., & Owen, L. D.
Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1998). Exposure to interparental (2009). Intergenerational transmission of internalizing and
violence in childhood and psychosocial adjustment in young externalizing behaviors across three generations: Gender
adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 339–357. specific pathways. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 19,
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). 125–141.
The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Kitzmann, K. M., Gaylord, N. K., Holt, A. R., & Kenny, E. D. (2003).
Non-Patient Version (SCID-I/NP). New York: Biometrics Re- Child witnesses to domestic violence: A meta-analytic review.
search, New York State Psychiatric Institute. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 339–352.
Gilliom, M., Shaw, D. S., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. A., & Lukon, J. Kogan, L. S., Smith, J., & Jenkins, S. (1977). Ecological validity of
L. (2002). Anger regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys: indicator data as predictors of survey findings. Journal of Social
Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control. Service Research, 1, 117–132.
Developmental Psychology, 38, 222–235. Kraemer, H., Kazdin, A. E., Offord, D. R., Kessler, R. C., Jensen, P.
Gould, M. S., Velting, D., Kleinman, M., Lucas, C., Thomas, J. G., & S., & Kupfer, D. J. (1997). Coming to terms with the terms of
Chung, M. (2004). Teenagers’ attitudes about coping strategies risk. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 337–343.
and help-seeking behavior for suicidality. Journal of the Levendosky, A., & Graham-Bermann, S. (2001). Parenting in battered
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, women: The effects of domestic violence on women and their
1124–1133. children. Journal of Family Violence, 16, 171–192.
Prev Sci (2012) 13:370–383 383

Levendosky, A. A., Huth-Bocks, A. C., Shapiro, D. L., & Semel, M. Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1997). Comparability of
A. (2003). The impact of domestic violence on the maternal– telephone and face-to-face interviews in assessing axis I and II
child relationship and preschool age children’s functioning. disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1593–1598.
Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 275–287. Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children’s reports of parental behavior: An
Lieberman, A. F., Van Horn, P., & Ozer, E. J. (2005). Preschooler witnesses inventory. Child Development, 36, 413–424.
of marital violence: Predictors and mediators of child behavior Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone,
problems. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 385–396. M. E. (2000). NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children
Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2004). The Coping Power program Version IV: Description, differences from previous versions and
for preadolescent aggressive boys and their parents: Outcome reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American
effects at the one-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 28–38.
Clinical Psychology, 72, 571–578. Silva, R. R., Alpert, M., Munoz, D. M., Singh, S., Matzner, F., &
Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. Dummit, S. (2000). Stress and vulnerability to posttraumatic
(1990). Optimal informants on childhood disruptive disorders. stress disorder in children and adolescents. The American
Development and Psychopathology, 1, 317–337. Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1229–1235.
Lopez-Duran, N. L., Olson, S., Hajal, N. J., Felt, B. T., & Vazquez, D. Stein, M. B., & Kennedy, C. (2001). Major depressive and post
M. (2009). Hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis functioning in traumatic stress disorder comorbidity in female victims of
reactive and proactive aggression in children. Journal of intimate partner violence. Journal of Affective Disorders, 66,
Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 169–182. 133–138.
Magdol, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. (1998). Develop- Straus, M. A. (1990). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence:
mental antecedents of partner abuse: A prospective-longitudinal The Conflict Tactics (CT) Scales. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles
study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 375–389. (Eds.), Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and
Mertin, P., & Mohr, P. B. (2001). A follow-up study of PTSD, anxiety, adaptations to violence in 8,145 families (pp. 403–424). New
and depression in Australian victims of domestic violence. Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press.
Violence and Victims, 16, 645–654. Straus, M., Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Behind closed
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Rutter, M., & Silva, P. A. (2001). Sex doors: Violence in the American family. Garden City, NY:
differences in antisocial behavior: Conduct disorder, delinquen- Doubleday.
cy, and violence in the Dunedin longitudinal study. Cambridge, Supple, A. J., Aquilino, W. S., & Wright, D. L. (1999). Collecting
UK: Cambridge University Press. sensitive self-report data with laptop computers: Impact on the
Newman, D. A. (2003). Longitudinal modeling with randomly and response tendencies of adolescent in a home interview. Journal of
systematically missing data: A simulation of ad hoc, maximum Research on Adolescence, 9, 467–488.
likelihood, and multiple imputation techniques. Organizational Thornberry, T. P., Freeman-Gallant, A., Lizotte, A. J., Krohn, M. D.,
Research Methods, 6, 328–362. & Smith, C. A. (2003). Linked lives: The intergenerational
Osofsky, J. D. (1998). Children as invisible victims of domestic and transmission of antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child
community violence. In G. W. Holden, R. Geffner, & E. N. Jouriles Psychology, 31, 171–184.
(Eds.), Children exposed to marital violence: Theory, research, Williams, J., Gibbon, M., First, M., Spitzer, R., Davies, M., Borus, J.,
and applied issues (pp. 95–117). Washington, DC: American et al. (1992). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R,
Psychological Association. II: Multisite test-retest reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A 48, 124–130.
developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. The American Wolfe, D. A., Jaffe, P., Wilson, S. K., & Zak, L. (1985). Children of
Psychologist, 44, 329–335. battered women: The relation of child behavior to family
Piacentini, J., Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (1992). Combining discrepant violence and maternal stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
diagnostic information from multiple sources: Are complex Psychology, 53, 657–665.
algorithms better than simple ones? Journal of Abnormal Wolfe, D. A., Crooks, C. V., Lee, V., McIntyre-Smith, A., & Jaffe, P.
Psychology, 20, 51–63. G. (2003). The effects of children’s exposure to domestic
Pollack, S. D., Vardi, S., Putzer Bechner, A. M., & Curtin, J. J. (2005). violence: A meta-analysis and critique. Clinical Child and
Physically abused children’s regulation of attention in response Family Psychology Review, 6(3), 171–187.
to hostility. Child Development, 76, 968–977. Yates, T. M., Dodds, M. F., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2003).
Poulton, R., Caspi, A., Milne, B. J., Thomson, W. M., Taylor, A., Exposure to partner violence and child behavior problems: A
Sears, M. R., et al. (2002). Association between children’s prospective study controlling for child physical abuse and
experience of socioeconomic disadvantage and adult health: A neglect, child cognitive ability, socioeconomic status, and life
life-course study. The Lancet, 360(9346), 1640–1645. stress. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 199–218.

You might also like