You are on page 1of 9

Theory Into Practice, 50:12–19, 2011

Copyright © The College of Education and Human Ecology, The Ohio State University
ISSN: 0040-5841 print/1543-0421 online
DOI: 10.1080/00405841.2011.534912

Asha K. Jitendra
Jon R. Star

Meeting the Needs of Students


With Learning Disabilities in
Inclusive Mathematics Classrooms:
The Role of Schema-Based
Instruction on Mathematical
Problem-Solving

This article discusses schema-based instruction performance of students with learning disabili-
(SBI) as an alternative to traditional instruction ties (LD). In the authors’ most recent research
for enhancing the mathematical problem solving and developmental efforts, they designed SBI
to meet the needs of middle school students
with LD in inclusive mathematics classrooms
Asha K. Jitendra is a professor of Educational Psychol-
ogy at the University of Minnesota; Jon R. Star is an by addressing the research literatures in spe-
assistant professor of Education at Harvard University. cial education, cognitive psychology, and math-
This article is based on research supported by ematics education. This innovative instructional
the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department approach encourages students to look beyond
of Education, through Grant # R305K060075. The surface features of word problems to grasp the
opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not underlying mathematical structure of ratio and
represent views of the U.S. Department of Education. proportion problems. In addition, SBI introduces
Correspondence should be addressed to Professor students to multiple strategies for solving ratio
Asha K. Jitendra, 245 Education Sciences Building, 56 and proportion problems and encourages the
East River Road, University of Minnesota, Minneapo-
selection of appropriate strategies.
lis, MN 55455-0364. E-mail: jiten001@umn.edu

12
Jitendra and Star Schema-Based Instruction for Mathematical Problem-Solving

A SIGNIFICANT NUMBER OF STUDENTS


(about 6% to 7%) in the United States
demonstrate poor achievement in mathematics
are designed to address these deficits and scaf-
fold student learning. A body of literature is
available that indicates the importance of prac-
(Swanson & Olga, 2006). Although mathematics tices to improve student learning in mathematics
achievement has improved over the years, the that include (a) systematic, explicit instruction;
achievement rates of students with disabilities in (b) student think-alouds; (c) visual representa-
particular remain considerably lower than those tions; (d) peer assisted learning opportunities in
for other students. This disparity suggests that which students focus on problem details, observe
more needs to be done to make school mathemat- or are guided by models of proficient students’
ics instruction accessible to all students. Further, problem solving; and (e) formative assessments
educational equity is one of several principles to provide feedback to teachers and students
articulated in the National Council of Teachers (Baker, Gersten, & Lee, 2002; Kroesbergen &
of Mathematics Standards (NCTM, 2000) and Van Luit, 2003; National Mathematics Advisory
is based on the fundamental notion that all Panel, 2008). Schema-based instruction (SBI),
students, “regardless of personal characteristics, an alternative to traditional instruction, integrates
backgrounds, or physical challenges” (p. 12) many of these features to improve mathemati-
should have access to a curriculum that is chal- cal problem-solving outcomes for learners with
lenging. The literature includes several studies of diverse needs, including students with LD (see
equity that have involved low-income, minority Jitendra, 2007).
students (e.g., Fennema, Franke, Carpenter, &
Carey, 1993; Moses, 2001; Silver, Smith, & Nel-
son, 1995), who have benefited from challenging Mathematical Problem Solving and
(reform-based) mathematics programs. However, Traditional Instruction
the beneficial effect of reform-based mathematics
programs for students with learning disabilities Students struggling in mathematics lag behind
(LD) is not well documented. Preliminary evi- their peers academically and continue to experi-
dence suggests that these students do not respond ence difficulties in mathematical problem solving
adequately to core instruction in reform-based regardless of motivation. One plausible reason
mathematics classrooms and require considerable is that problem solving is not well addressed in
accommodations to participate in small group ac- many mathematics textbooks. Most textbooks do
tivities and whole class discussions (e.g., Baxter, not provide opportunities to discriminate among
Woodward, & Olson, 2001; Baxter, Woodward, problems that require different solutions, because
Voorhies, & Wong, 2002; Woodward & Baxter, all problems presented on a page can be solved
1997). using the same procedure (e.g., multiplication).
Most notably, students with LD have difficulty A second concern is the use of superficial cues
with basic mathematical concepts and skills that such as key words (e.g., in all suggests addition,
negatively impact their ability to solve novel left suggests subtraction, share suggest division;
problems. Improving these students’ problem- Lester, Garofalo, & Kroll, 1989) that “send a ter-
solving skills has proved to be a significant ribly wrong message about doing math” (Van de
challenge for several reasons. Students with LD Walle, 2004, p. 152). The reliance on keywords to
typically have deficits in attention, memory, select an operation or a solution procedure (e.g.,
background knowledge, vocabulary, language cross multiply) can lead to systematic errors.
processes, strategy knowledge and use, visual- For example, in the problem, During a vacation
spatial processing, and self-regulation (Baker, to Rome with her family, Chaya took some
Simmons, & Kame’enui, 1995; Geary, 2003), photographs with her digital camera. When she
which presumably have a deleterious effect on returned home, she deleted 9 of the photographs.
one or more domains (e.g., mathematics, read- Now, she has 72 photographs left of her trip
ing). As such, it is critical that interventions to Rome. How many photographs did she have

13
Current Perspectives on Learning Disabilities and ADHD

in her camera initially?, students may focus on instruction that special education promulgates for
the keyword, left, and subtract even though the students with disabilities (Kroesbergen & Van
solution requires addition as the operation. As a Luit, 2003), it draws largely from schema theory
result of applying the key word method, students of cognitive psychology. As such, it addresses
struggling in mathematics have persistent diffi- some of the concerns with traditional problem
culties in mathematical problem solving despite solving instruction primarily by going beyond
plenty of practice. The keyword method does surface features of word problems to identify the
not emphasize the meaning and structure of the problem schema (semantic structure) and analyze
problem and thus may not help students to reason underlying mathematical relationships that are
and make sense of problem situations, which is critical to successful problem solving (Marshall,
crucial in solving novel problems (Ben-Zeev & 1995). Change, group, compare, restate, and vary
Star, 2001). problems represent the set of schemata in the
Third, a general heuristic approach based on domain of arithmetic word problems (Marshall,
George Pólya’s (1990) four-step problem solving 1995). These schemata are categorized into ei-
model (i.e., understand the problem, devise a ther additive or multiplicative structures. Change,
plan, carry out the plan, and look back and group, and compare problems belong to the
reflect) used commonly in mathematics textbooks additive field, because the solution operation is
is problematic for students struggling in math- either addition or subtraction. In contrast, restate
ematics. One concern is that general heuristics (i.e., multiplicative compare) and vary (i.e., equal
do not reliably lead to improvements in stu- groups, proportion) problems belong to the mul-
dents’ word problem solving performance (Lesh tiplicative field, because the solution operation
& Zawojewski, 2007; Schoenfeld, 1992). The is either multiplication or division (Christou &
strategies in the devise a plan step are too general Philippou, 1999).
to support the learning of students struggling in Change problems involve situations in which
mathematics. For example, a common strategy, “there is a permanent change over time in the
to draw a diagram, may not necessarily gen- value of one variable” (Kalyuga, 2006, p. 5).
erate a representation that depicts the relations The change schema usually begins with an initial
between critical elements (e.g., part-to-part or quantity and a direct or implied action causes
part-to-whole comparisons) in the problem es- either an increase or decrease in that quantity.
sential for successful problem solving (Hegarty In the problem, A squirrel made a pile of nuts. It
& Kozhevnikov, 1999). Further, the characteriza- carried away 15 nuts up to its nest. Now there are
tions of strategies in the general method are “de- 38 nuts in the pile. How many nuts were in the
scriptive rather than prescriptive” (Schoenfeld, pile at the beginning? the unknown quantity (the
1992, p. 353) and may not provide the necessary initial number of nuts in the pile) was changed,
detail for individuals who are not already familiar and the resulting quantity is specified. The group
with the strategies to implement them. schema involves a number of smaller groups
combining to form a new larger group, with an
emphasis on the part–part–whole relation. The
Word Problem Solving and SBI group problem, A new baseball bat costs $50. A
new baseball cap costs $10. How much would it
Our instructional approach (SBI), which is cost to buy the baseball bat and cap? requires
intermediate in generality between key word understanding that the unknown quantity (cost
and general heuristic methods, was designed to of the baseball bat and cap) comprises two parts,
address the research literatures from multiple dis- baseball bat ($50) and cap ($10). The compare
ciplines (special education, cognitive psychology, schema involves a situation that compares two
and mathematics education) to meet the diverse distinct, disjoint sets (compared and referent) and
needs of students struggling in mathematics. Al- the relation between the two sets is emphasized.
though SBI incorporates systematic and explicit In the problem, At the park, there were 8 children

14
Jitendra and Star Schema-Based Instruction for Mathematical Problem-Solving

on the slide and some children on the swings. ing, other connected pieces of information will
There were 5 more children on the slide than be activated. SBI also borrows from cognitive
on the swings. How many children were on the psychology the use of metacognitive strategy
swings? the two things compared are the number knowledge (e.g., self-monitoring) to facilitate
of children on slides (8) and children on swings reflecting on the problem-solving processes for
(the unknown quantity), and the relation between “comprehending problem statements, organizing
the two sets is a difference of 5. information or data, planning solution attempts,
The restate schema involves situations in executing plans, and checking results” (Cold-
which a relation exists between two things (e.g., berg & Bush, 2003, p. 168). Further, flexible
twice as much as, nth times) and there is a use of well-articulated problem-solving strategies
“restatement of this relationship using values (e.g., unit rate, equivalent fractions) in SBI is
different from those involved in the initial re- based on the mathematics education literature
lation statement” (Kalyuga, 2006, p. 6). This that supports comparing and contrasting mul-
schema represents ratio-type situations such as tiple solution methods (Rittle-Johnson & Star,
the following problem: Orange juice concentrate 2007; Star & Rittle-Johnson, 2009). In light of
is mixed with water in the ratio of 1:3. How working memory deficits evidenced by students
many cans of orange juice need to be mixed with disabilities, SBI explicitly teaches a small
with 6 cans of water? This problem type involves but adequate number of strategies to scaffold
a known relation between two things (1:6 or student learning by providing explicit instruc-
6 times as much), and the same relation applies to tion.
two different values describing the same things: Successful problem solving entails both prob-
unknown amount of orange juice and 6 cans of lem representation (modeling the problem situa-
water, which should be 6 times more than the tion) and problem solution. Expert performance
amount of orange juice concentrate expressed in is characterized by the ability to translate and
cans. So, 1:3 D x: 6, or 2 cans of orange juice integrate information in the problem into a co-
is needed to mix 6 cans of water. In contrast, herent mental representation that mediates prob-
the vary schema represents a situation in which lem solution (Mayer, 1999; Mayer & Hegarty,
“a systematic relationship exists between the 1996). For students with LD, teaching them to
amounts of two different things. If the amount represent the situation described in the problem
of one thing varies (decreases or increases), the using schematic diagrams is critical to reduce
amount of the second thing changes in a fixed working memory resources. As such, SBI focuses
way. These situations could be redescribed as students’ attention on the problem schema (e.g.,
involving ‘IF : : : THEN’ relationship” (Kalyuga, proportion) and helps them represent the relations
2006, p. 6). The vary problem, Yuri can eat 3 between the different elements described in the
hot dogs in 2 minutes. At this rate, how many hot text using schematic diagrams (e.g., Hegarty
dogs can he eat in 6 minutes? describes a relation & Kozhevnikov, 1999; Janvier, 1987; Willis &
between hot dogs eaten and the time taken. If Fuson, 1988). A schematic diagram depicts the
the amount of time increases (three times, in this spatial relations between objects in the problem
instance), then the number of hot dogs eaten will text (Hegarty & Kozhevnikov, 1999) and differs
also increase by three times. from pictorial representations of problems that
These schemata are hierarchically organized, include concrete, but irrelevant details, which
cognitive structures that are acquired and stored “are superfluous to solution of the math problem”
in long-term memory. Because multiple ele- (Edens & Potter, 2006, p. 186). Because the
ments of information are chunked into a single difficulty of the problem may be a function of the
schema, a distinctive feature of schema-based difficulty in understanding the problem situation,
knowledge structures held in long-term mem- the nature of representations in our SBI model
ory is that when one piece of information is not only focus on the problem schema (e.g.,
retrieved from memory during problem solv- proportion) maintained in long-term memory, but

15
Current Perspectives on Learning Disabilities and ADHD

also emphasize information (nonmathematical) boys to students) as in the ratio problem. At


contained in the situation model, as described the same time, the proportion problem is similar
by Van Dijk and Kintsch (1983). The situation to ratio word problems in that they compare
model “is a temporary structure stored in work- two ratios that are equivalent and require solv-
ing memory [and] : : : corresponds to a level ing for the unknown quantity in one of the
of representation that specifies the agents, the ratios.
actions, and the relationships between the events For Step 2, the teacher demonstrates how to
in everyday contexts” (Thevenot, Devidal, Bar- organize information using the schematic dia-
rouillet, & Fayol, 2007, p. 44). In the following gram (see Figure 1). This step includes self-
is a description of how SBI, in conjunction with instructions to read the problem to identify criti-
self-questioning to monitor the learning process, cal information in the problem to represent using
can be used to help students solve word problems the schematic diagram. For example, questions
(e.g., proportion). such as, “What does this problem compare?”
(cans of icing and cupcakes) and “Does the
problem describe a specific ratio between cans
Teaching Problem Solving With SBI of icing and cupcakes?” (yes, both the statement
and question in the problem describe the number
The SBI intervention described here is based of cans of icing and cupcakes) are used to make
on our work with middle school students with sense of the problem situation. Specifically, the
learning difficulties (Jitendra, et al., 2009). A questioning makes public the two quantities (cans
four-step strategy (FOPS; F–find the problem of icing and cupcakes) that form a ratio, which
type, O–organize the information in the problem are first represented in the diagram followed by
using the diagram, P–plan to solve the problem, writing the quantities and units for each of the
S–solve the problem) helps anchor students’ two ratios that are given and an “x” for what
learning of the problem solving process. The must be solved in the diagram. The teacher
teacher uses a checklist based on the strategy then analyzes the problem situation using the
steps to scaffold the cognitive processes as he/she completed diagram as follows: If 2 cans of icing
thinks aloud to solve word problems (e.g., pro- cover 24 cupcakes, then 3 cans of icing will cover
portion). For example, consider the following x cupcakes (see Figure 1).
problem: Toshi used exactly 2 cans of icing to Step 3 involves translating the information
cover 24 cupcakes. How many cupcakes can in the diagram into a math equation. From the
she ice with 3 cans of icing? Using Step 1 of proportion diagram, the ratio presented in the
the strategy, the teacher identifies the problem if statement (2 cans of icing to 24 cupcakes or
2
as a proportion problem by reading, retelling, 24 ) and the ratio presented in the then statement
and examining information in the problem to (3 cans of icing to x cupcakes, or x3 ) are set up
recognize it as a proportion problem via self-
instructions (e.g., Is there a if–then statement
of equality between two ratios? Does the if
statement describe a rate/ratio between two quan-
tities and the then statement involve either an
increase or decrease in the two quantities, but
with the same ratio?). In addition, the teacher
makes the connection between previously solved
problems by noting that this problem is dif-
ferent from the ratio problems solved earlier,
because this problem compares quantities with
different units (e.g., cans of icing, cupcakes)
rather than the same units (boys to girls or Figure 1. Schematic diagram for proportion problem.

16
Jitendra and Star Schema-Based Instruction for Mathematical Problem-Solving

as follows: 2 cans of icing


( 24 cupcakes ). When I simplify both ratios,
x
? 2 cans of icing
x 1 can
I get 12 cupcakes (notice the 12 again–1 can
3 cans of icing ?
y 24 cupcakes D x cupcakes
? ?
1
y of icing per 12 cupcakes, or 12 can of icing
for every cupcake), which tells me that they are
During this step, students learn to discrimi- equivalent.
nate between instances when a particular solu-
tion strategy (e.g., cross-multiplication, equiva- In sum, SBI encourages student think-alouds
lent fractions, unit rate) is appropriate based on to monitor and direct problem-solving behavior
the numbers in the problem. In this instance, along the following dimensions: (a) problem
students learn that the unit rate strategy would be comprehension (e.g., “Did I read and retell the
the easiest to apply. This strategy is represented problem to understand what is given and what
by vertical arrows, which show that one reasons must be solved?” “Why is this a proportion
up or down to analyze the relationship between problem?” “How is this problem similar to or
the two quantities. To solve for x, the teacher different from ones I already solved?”), (b) prob-
2
explores by starting with the given ratio ( 24 ) lem representation (e.g., “What diagram can help
and reasoning from the top to the bottom in the me adequately represent information in the prob-
2
given ratio 24 , because x is in the denominator lem to show the relation between quantities?”),
in the second ratio. The teacher models using (c) planning (e.g., “How can I set up the math
self-instructions by asking, “2 multiplied by what equation?” “What solution strategy can I use to
number equals 24?” The answer is 12 (because solve this problem?”), and (d) problem solution
2 times 12 D 24), so you take this multiplier and (e.g., “Does the answer make sense?” “How can
use it on the other ratio. Because you multiplied I verify the solution?”).
2 by 12 to get 24, you multiply 3 by 12 to get Several studies have shown the benefits of SBI
x. So, 3  12 D 36. That is, 36 cupcakes can be for improving the problem-solving performance
iced by 3 cans of icing. Teacher questions such of many different groups of students, including
as, “Does anyone know what the 12 that we got students with LD, attention deficit hyperactivity
as a multiplier means?” serve to emphasize that disorder, as well as English language learners
12 is the unit rate of cupcakes per can of icing. and nondisabled students struggling in mathe-
Finally, Step 4 has the students solve the matics (e.g., Fuchs, Seethaler, Powell, Fuchs,
problem using the solution strategy identified Hamlett, Fletcher, 2008; Jitendra et al., 2007;
in Step 3, justify the derived solutions using Jitendra et al., 2009; Xin, Jitendra, & Deatline-
the schema features as anchors for explanations Buchman, 2005). For example, in a recent study
and elaborations, and check the accuracy of not we conducted with seventh-graders in a middle
only the computation but also the representation. school, students received 10 days of instruction
The following provides an illustration of the from their regular classroom mathematics teacher
mathematical reasoning to make sense of the on ratio and proportion topics (e.g., ratios, rates,
answer: solving proportions, scale drawings; see Jitendra
et al., 2009). Instruction took place during the
Let’s see : : : if 2 cans of icing cover 24 cup-
regularly scheduled mathematics class for 40 min
cakes, then 3 cans of icing cover 36 cupcakes.
daily, 5 days per week. Students in the control
This seems right, because 3 cans should cover
more than 2 cans. So, the answer 36 cupcakes, group received instruction from their teachers
which is more than 24 cupcakes, seems right. who followed normal instructional procedures
I will also check the answer by taking the as outlined in the district-adopted mathematics
2 cans of icing
ratio in the then situation ( 24 cupcakes D textbook; SBI students received instruction using
our instructional materials that replaced regular
3 cans of icing
x cupcakes ) and seeing if the value of this instruction on ratios and proportions. Results
ratio is equal to the ratio in the if situation suggested that students in the SBI group outper-

17
Current Perspectives on Learning Disabilities and ADHD

formed students in a control group on a problem- versity of Oregon, National Center to Improve the
solving test both at posttest and on a delayed Tools of Educators.
posttest administered four months later. In addi- Baxter, J., Woodward, J., Voorhies, J., & Wong, J.
tion, we found a similar pattern of findings for (2002). We talk about it, but do they get it?
students with disabilities (SBI: n D 10; control: Learning Disabilities: Research and Practice, 17,
173–185.
n D 5) at posttest. Use of SBI had a large
Baxter, J., Woodward, J., & Olson, D. (2001). Effects
impact on students’ ratio and proportion problem
of reform-based mathematics instruction in five
solving. The average percent improvement from third-grade classrooms. Elementary School Jour-
pretest to posttest on the PS test was 24% for nal, 101, 529–548.
SBI students compared to only 2% improvement Ben-Zeev, T., & Star, J. R. (2001). Spurious corre-
for the control students. lations in mathematical thinking. Cognition and
Instruction, 19, 253–275.
Christou, C., & Philippou, G. (1999). Role of schemas
in one-step word problems. Educational Research
Summary
and Evaluation, 5, 269–289.
Coldberg, P., & Bush, W. (2003). Using metacognitive
In sum, students with disabilities can suc- skills to improve third graders’ math problem solv-
cessfully learn problem-solving skills when in- ing. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics,
struction is designed to promote understanding. 5(10), 29–48.
Moving beyond conventional problem solving Edens, K., & Potter, E. (2006). How students “unpack”
procedures to developing deep understanding of the structure of a word problem: Graphic represen-
the mathematical problem structure and fostering tations and problem solving. School Science and
flexible solution strategies helped SBI students Mathematics, 108, 184–196.
improve their problem solving performance. The Fennema, E., Franke, M., Carpenter, T., & Carey, D.
schematic diagrams in SBI may have provided (1993). Using children’s mathematical knowledge
in instruction. American Educational Research
a “level of concreteness and support to help un-
Journal, 30, 555–583.
derstand key concepts” necessary for successful
Fuchs, L. S., Seethaler, P. M., Powell, S. R., Fuchs,
problem solving (Gersten, 2005, p. 203). Other D., Hamlett, C. L., & Fletcher, J. M. (2008).
factors contributing to these students’ successful Effects of preventative tutoring on the mathematical
performance may be attributed to the emphasis problem solving of third-grade students with math
in SBI on metacognitive strategy knowledge to and reading difficulties. Exceptional Children, 74,
monitor problem-solving processes as well as ex- 155–173.
plicit and systematic instruction (e.g., consistent, Geary, D. C. (2003). Learning disabilities and arith-
logical, and complete explanations). As such, SBI metic: Problem-solving differences and cognitive
represents a promising approach for teachers to deficits. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S.
meet the provisions of the No Child Left Behind Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities
Act with regard to closing the achievement gap (pp. 199–212). New York: Guilford.
Gersten, R. (2005). Behind the scenes of an interven-
for students with disabilities.
tion research study. Learning Disabilities Research
and Practice, 20, 200–212.
Hegarty, M., & Kozhevnikov, M. (1999). Types
References of visual–spatial representations and mathematical
problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychol-
Baker, S., Gersten, R., & Lee, D. (2002). A synthesis ogy, 91, 684–689.
of empirical research on teaching mathematics to Janvier, C. (1987). Problems of representation in the
low-achieving students. Elementary School Jour- teaching and learning of mathematics. Hillsdale,
nal, 103, 51–73. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baker, S. K., Simmons, D. C., & Kame’enui, E. J. Jitendra, A. K. (2007). Solving math word problems:
(1995). Vocabulary acquisition: Synthesis of the Teaching students with learning disabilities using
research. (Tech. Rep. No. 13). Eugene, OR: Uni- schema-based instruction. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

18
Jitendra and Star Schema-Based Instruction for Mathematical Problem-Solving

Jitendra, A. K., Griffin, C., Haria, P., Leh, J., Adams, Pólya, G. (1990). How to solve it. London: Penguin.
A., & Kaduvetoor, A. (2007). A comparison of sin- (Original work published 1945)
gle and multiple strategy instruction on third grade Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does com-
students’ mathematical problem solving. Journal of paring solution methods facilitate conceptual and
Educational Psychology, 99, 115–127. procedural knowledge? An experimental study on
Jitendra, A. K., Star, J., Starosta, K., Leh, J., Sood, learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational
S., Caskie, G., et al. (2009). Improving stu- Psychology, 99, 561–574.
dents’ learning of ratio and proportion problem Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think math-
solving: The role of schema-based instruction. ematically: Problem solving, metacognition, and
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 250– sense making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.),
264. Handbook of research on mathematics teaching
Kalyuga, S. (2006). Rapid cognitive assessment of and learning (pp. 334–370). New York: McMillan.
learners’ knowledge structures. Learning and In- Silver, E., Smith, M., & Nelson, B. (1995). The
struction, 16, 1–11. QUASAR project: Equity concerns meet mathe-
Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2003). matics education reform in the middle school. In
Mathematics intervention for children with special W. Secada, E. Fennema, & L. Adajain (Eds.),
educational needs. Remedial and Special Educa- New directions for equity in mathematics education
tion, 24, 97–114. (pp. 9–56). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lesh, R., & Zawojewski, J. (2007). Problem solving Star, J. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2009). It pays to
and modeling. In F. K. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second compare: An experimental study on computational
handbook of research on mathematics teaching and estimation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol-
learning (pp. 763–804). Charlotte, NC: Information ogy, 102, 408–426.
Age. Swanson, H. L., & Olga, J. (2006). Math disabilities:
Lester, F. K., Garofalo, J., & Kroll, D. L. (1989). Self- A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Review
confidence, interest, beliefs, and metacognition: of Educational Research, 76, 249–274.
Key influences on problem-solving behavior. In Thevenot, C., Devidal, M., Barrouillet, P., & Fayol,
D. B. McLeaod & V. M. Adams (Eds.), Affect and M. (2007). Why does placing the question before
mathematical problem solving: A new perspective an arithmetic word problem improve performance?
(pp. 75–88). New York: Springer-Verlag. A situation model account. Quarterly Journal of
Marshall, S. P. (1995). Schemas in problem solving. Experimental Psychology, 60, 43–56.
New York: Cambridge University Press. Van de Walle, J. A. (2004). Elementary and mid-
Mayer, R. E. (1999). The promise of educational dle school mathematics: Teaching developmentally
psychology Vol. I: Learning in the content areas. (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education.
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice Hall. Van Dijk, T., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of
Mayer, R. E. & Hegarty, M. (1996). The process discourse comprehension. New York: Academic
of understanding mathematics problems. In R. J. Press.
Sternberg & T. Ben-Zeev (Eds.), The nature of Willis, G. B., & Fuson, K. C. (1988). Teaching children
mathematical thinking (pp. 29–53). Mahwah, NJ: to use schematic drawings to solve addition and
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. subtraction word problems. Journal of Educational
Moses, R. (2001). Radical equations. Boston: Beacon Psychology, 80, 191–201.
Press. Woodward, J., & Baxter, J. (1997). The effects of
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). an innovative approach to mathematics on aca-
Principles and standards for school mathematics. demically low achieving students in mainstreamed
Reston, VA: Author. settings. Exceptional Children, 63, 373–388.
National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). Foun- Xin, Y. P., Jitendra, A. K., & Deatline-Buchman, A.
dations for success: The Final Report of the Na- (2005). Effects of mathematical word problem solv-
tional Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington, ing instruction on students with learning problems.
DC: U.S. Department of Education. Journal of Special Education, 39, 181–192.

19
Copyright of Theory Into Practice is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like