You are on page 1of 18

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/301283647

Supporting Second Language Writing Using Multimodal Feedback

Article  in  Foreign Language Annals · March 2016


DOI: 10.1111/flan.12183

CITATIONS READS

98 978

2 authors:

Idoia Elola Ana Oskoz


Texas Tech University University of Maryland, Baltimore County
44 PUBLICATIONS   1,016 CITATIONS    47 PUBLICATIONS   1,252 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Effects of collaborative writing and peer feedback on subsequent written productions using technological correction tools and not using them View project

Heritage Learner Writing Project View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Idoia Elola on 15 August 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


58 SPRING 2016

Supporting Second Language


Writing Using Multimodal
Feedback
Idoia Elola
Texas Tech University
Ana Oskoz1
University of Maryland, Baltimore County

Abstract: The educational use of computer-based feedback in the classroom is


becoming widespread. However, less is known about (1) the extent to which tools
influence how instructors provide written and oral comments, and (2) whether receiving
oral or written feedback influences the nature of learners’ revisions. This case study,
which expands existing research on computer-mediated feedback, examines how four
Spanish learners enrolled in a Spanish advanced writing course received multimodal
feedback while working on the different drafts of a narrative essay. The instructor
provided written feedback via Microsoft Word and oral feedback using screencast
software. Results indicate that the tool used affected the quantity and quality of the
instructor’s comments. When using the screencast software, the instructor provided
additional and lengthier comments on content, structure, and organization; the instruc-
tor was more explicit on form when using the coding system in Word. Although learners
revised similarly regardless of the tool being used, they tended to prefer the oral feedback
for global aspects, such as content, structure, and organization, and the written feedback
for form. However, learners agreed that no matter the mode and the tool, both ap-
proaches to feedback helped them improve their writing skills.

Key words: instructor feedback, L2 writing, learner beliefs, revisions, written and oral
feedback

Introduction
Although the role that feedback plays in acquiring a second language (L2) is not
yet completely recognized (Truscott & Hsu, 2008), learners expect to receive it
and expect instructors to provide it. In addition, recent studies in L2 contexts

1
Authors are listed in alphabetical order and contributed equally to the manuscript.
Idoia Elola (PhD, University of Iowa) is Associate Professor of Spanish and
Applied Linguistics & Second Language Studies, Texas Tech University, Lubbock.
Ana Oskoz (PhD, University of Iowa) is Associate Professor of Spanish, University
of Maryland, Baltimore County.
Foreign Language Annals, Vol. 49, Iss. 1, pp. 58–74. © 2016 by American Council on the Teaching of Foreign
Languages.
DOI: 10.1111/flan.12183
Foreign Language Annals  VOL. 49, NO. 1 59

confirmed that feedback supports linguistic (as opposed to automated) feedback studies
learning not only from draft to draft (Ferris, analyzed types of feedback, types of
1999, 2006), but also when new pieces of errors, and learners’ immediate responses
writing have been completed (Ellis, Sheen, (Fernandez-Garcıa & Martınez-Arbelaiz,
Murakami, & Takashima, 2008; Sheen, 2002), as well as the impact of negative
2010). However, the provision of feedback feedback within chat sessions (Morris,
is a complex endeavor: Instructors need to 2005). Some studies showed that the slower
decide if revisions are needed for content or pace of asynchronous and even synchro-
organization, the types of error to highlight, nous computer-mediated communication,
how explicit feedback should be, and the often provided by some learners in telecol-
way in which to offer it. laborative encounters (Sauro, 2009; Vinagre
The integration of technology has ex- & Mu~ noz, 2011; Ware & O’Dowd, 2008),
panded the way in which instructors can give other learners more opportunity to
provide feedback to their L2 learners, focus on form (Hampel & Hauck, 2006;
from sophisticated programs developed by Vinagre & Lera, 2008).
testing services (Lavolette, Polio, & Kahng, Similarly, sociocultural theory consid-
2015) to screencasting software (Ducate & ers feedback to be essential; however, given
Arnold, 2012) to the Track Changes tool that feedback is regarded as a dialogic
(Ene & Upton, 2014; Ferris, 2012; Ho & process between instructors and students
Savignon, 2007; Tuzi, 2004). All of these (Lantolf, 2006), its main strength is its po-
tools give instructors the potential to effi- tential to scaffold the learner to a higher
ciently provide oral or written feedback, for level if pitched at the learner’s current pro-
example, in the form of error coding sys- ficiency level. Nassaji and Swain’s (2000)
tems, written comments, or a video that can case study, for example, found that their
be watched as many times as needed. Korean learner was able to understand feed-
Within the framework of activity theory, back better when the feedback was based on
this case study explored how the medium the zone of proximal development (ZPD).
(video [screencast] software vs. Word) and Oskoz (2009) also found that learner-
mode (oral or written) in which the instruc- provided feedback in written online
tor provided feedback to four fourth-year synchronous interactions helped fellow
learners mediated the production of a final learners advance if it was within their
narrative essay. ZPD. More recently, Storch and Wiggles-
worth (2010), who examined the efficacy
of reformulations and coding symbols,
Literature Review noted that uptake and retention appeared
When framed by interactionist and socio- to be affected not only by linguistics cues,
cultural theories, the improvement of such as the type of error, but also by affec-
writing has been regarded as evidence of tive factors, including learners’ attitudes,
language acquisition (Manch on, 2011) beliefs, and goals.
and of learning (Bitchener, 2012). The Hence, both interactionist and socio-
interactionist theory sees the provision cultural theories see feedback as an
of feedback as an essential instructional important element in teacher-learner and
practice—it provides negative evidence, peer-to-peer interactions, not only empha-
which raises awareness of learning gaps sizing the relationship between expert and
and encourages the reorganization of learn- novice, but also as a way to raise awareness
ers’ linguistic mental processes. Perhaps and achieve higher levels of linguistic
because of the emphasis on oral or “chat” knowledge, thereby enabling learners to
communication in the initial wave of close linguistic gaps and move to the next
widespread classroom technology, the first level (Lantolf & Thorne, 2007). The role of
generation of human-computer-mediated technology in supporting L2 writing has
60 SPRING 2016

been studied in terms of content develop- written text, common tools and artifacts
ment (Elola & Oskoz, 2010), the use of such as Word (for written feedback) and
reference tools and collaboration (Kessler, screencast software (for oral feedback)
2009), and the development of new genres may influence the way foreign language
(Crystal, 2006); however, only recently has learners envision the object and how they
research examined the effect of different develop it.
feedback tools on L2 learners’ performance
(Ducate & Arnold, 2012; Ene & Upton,
2014; Tuzi, 2004). The lack of research Feedback in the L2 Classroom
on technology-mediated feedback is sur- Research regarding feedback in the L2 class-
prising given that, as already suggested by room has often examined who is providing
sociocultural theory, higher forms of mental the feedback (instructor, peer, or a combi-
activity such as planning and monitoring nation of both) and whether feedback fo-
are mental processes that are mediated by cuses on the product (summative feedback)
psychological or semiotic tools, such as or the process (linking feedback to new
numbers or language, or with physical tools drafts or pieces of writing). This extensive
and artifacts (Vygotsky, 1978). The rele- research has examined (1) the degree of
vance of tools is emphasized in activity the- explicitness (direct or indirect) of the feed-
ory, which considers tools and artifacts to back provided (Bitchener, 2008; Chandler,
be “integral and inseparable components 2003; Ferris & Roberts, 2001; Khatib
of human functioning” (Engestr€ om, 1999, & Bijani, 2012; Truscott & Hsu, 2008);
p. 29) and where the object is seen and (2) the influence, or lack thereof, of the
manipulated within the limitations set by syntactic form of the comment (questions)
the instrument (Engestr€ om, 1991). Tools and its pragmatic shape (suggestions or di-
are both enabling and limiting, because rectives) (Conrad & Goldstein, 1999; Elola,
they can empower the learner toward a 2005; Ferris, 1997, 2001); (3) the mode in
desired outcome, such as revising an essay, which feedback is presented, either orally or
but they can also restrict learners’ actions in written form (Haneda, 2004; Sheen,
according to the limitations of the tools in 2010); and (4) the medium in which feed-
use (Kaptelinin, Kuutti, & Bannon, 1995; back is provided—for instance, via pen and
Kuutti, 1996). In fact, the integration of a paper, in a student-teacher conference, or
new tool or artifact into a well-rehearsed using a technological tool such as Word
activity, such as receiving instructor feed- or software such as screencast (Ducate &
back in a form other than comments written Arnold, 2012; Tuzi, 2004).
on paper, may unsettle learners’ ideas of In terms of the effectiveness of direct
how the L2 classroom should function feedback (when the correct target language
(Basharina, 2007). Even when, for example, form with or without linguistic explanation
an instructor provided specific guidelines is provided) and indirect feedback (when
for the steps to be carried out and provided the instructor signals the error by under-
tools to complete the writing task, Blin and lining, circling, or enumerating the number
Appel (2011) found that learners deviated of errors in a specific sentence, leaving it up
from expected procedures in applying the to the learner to figure out how to fix the
tools and developed their own set of actions error), results are still inconclusive. While
to accomplish the writing task. In contrast, some studies reported an advantage for in-
in Oskoz and Elola’s (2014) study, learners’ direct feedback at the intermediate profi-
interactions with the tools and artifacts me- ciency level, other studies (Lalande, 1982;
diated their evolving perceptions of a task Robb, Ross, & Shortreed, 1986; Semke,
and helped them focus on different aspects 1984) did not find significant differences
of the object (an essay) in a sophisticated between the two feedback types. In Farro-
way. Thus, in the case of a technology-aided khi and Sattarpour’s (2012) study with
Foreign Language Annals  VOL. 49, NO. 1 61

high-proficiency learners, direct feedback than feedback that challenged the writers’
provided evidence of accuracy gains in ideas. Conrad and Goldstein (1999), how-
new pieces of writing, suggesting that direct ever, did not find that the comment form
feedback may provide the best results for L2 had any influence on how effective learners
acquisition (Ellis et al., 2008). Yet, research- revise.
ers have also understood that indirect feed- Studies that have examined feedback
back engages learners in problem-solving via the mode offered (written vs. oral) or
and reflection on their existing knowledge, feedback using different types of technology
which is more likely to foster long-term media are still quite scarce. Despite the
acquisition and greater written accuracy growing interest in the use of electronic
(Bitchener & Knoch, 2010) and may be a feedback in learners’ online written discus-
valuable practice activity for learners who sions (Sauro, 2009; Ware & O’Dowd,
are advanced enough to self-correct (Chan- 2008), few studies have focused on elec-
dler, 2003). Indirect feedback, however, can tronic feedback on students’ writing
(1) be confusing if the error codes are unfa- (Ducate & Arnold, 2012; Ene & Upton,
miliar (Bitchener & Knoch, 2010; Ferris & 2014; Tuzi, 2004). Focusing exclusively
Roberts, 2001) and the errors themselves on Word, Ene and Upton (2014) found
are complex (Bitchener & Knoch, 2010), that most instructors’ feedback focused on
and (2) make error revision more difficult content, followed by grammar and organi-
(Chandler, 2003). Rather than considering zation, then vocabulary and mechanics;
one type of feedback better than the other, however, their English for Academic Pur-
Ferris (2010) suggested that both direct and poses learners tended to focus more on
indirect feedback may deliver different but grammar in the context of either explicit
complementary results. In composition or implicit feedback. When comparing
classes where L2 writers are invited to edit oral to electronic feedback, Tuzi (2004)
and revise their texts, indirect feedback is an found that electronic feedback was never
attractive choice because writers can draw the primary stimulus for his freshman com-
on their linguistic knowledge when at- position class L2 learners’ revisions; how-
tempting to correct errors that have been ever, it was well used by learners to add
flagged. Furthermore, previous research on information or increase the impact of a
L2 writing found that indirect feedback can particular section. In Ducate and Arnold’s
“engage student writers in guided-problem- (2012) study, which compared written
solving” (Ferris, 2010, p. 190), thus encour- e-feedback to oral e-feedback, they found
aging them to take more responsibility for that use of screencast was associated with a
their own learning, which is perhaps more slightly higher success rate than Word, per-
valuable in the long term (Ferris, Liu, haps because, in many cases, the informa-
Sinha, & Senna, 2013). In the case of tion was encoded by both the instructor’s
lower-proficiency writers, however, indi- marks and the oral comments. Despite these
rect feedback is generally less preferred be- conflicting results, these studies suggest
cause these learners have a more limited potential benefits of providing computer-
linguistic range to draw from. Other reasons mediated feedback that could merit further
are related to the form of feedback com- exploration.
ments. Ferris (1997, 2001) noted, for exam-
ple, that comments formed as questions or
additions, or those related to problems of Learner Perceptions
logic, were less successful than comments Given that learners’ beliefs and attitudes
about clarification. Elola (2005) pointed out are “a significant contributory factor in the
that some functions of feedback, such as [language] learning process and ultimate
those related to elaboration, exemplifica- success” (Bernat & Gvozdenko, 2005, intro-
tion, or organization, were more successful duction, para. 2), learners’ perceptions
62 SPRING 2016

cannot be ignored when addressing the role Learners’ perceptions of the use of com-
of feedback in the foreign language class- puter-mediated feedback are still largely un-
room (Ducate & Arnold, 2012). Although explored. In terms of medium, when Tuzi
results have been inconclusive, prior re- (2004) compared oral face-to-face feedback
search on student perceptions has generally to electronic feedback, he found that although
revealed that learners value instructor feed- learners tended to prefer oral feedback, they
back and prefer it to other forms of feedback, made more revisions after receiving electronic
such as peer evaluation and self-evaluation feedback. Similarly, when comparing oral vs.
(Saito, 1994; Yang, Badger, & Yu, 2006; written computer-mediated feedback, Ducate
Zhang, 1995). Because of their preoccupa- and Arnold (2012) found that learners pre-
tion with improving the accuracy of their ferred the oral screencast method. Although
written texts, learners expect to have all their there were no statistical differences in the
errors marked by the instructor (Komura, success rate of error correction between
1999; Rennie, 2000). Furthermore, learners both types of feedback, learners felt the in-
value and expect feedback on their errors structor cared more about their writing and
(Chandler, 2003; Ferris, 1999; Hedgcock could better understand the feedback that was
& Lefkowitz, 1996, 2012; Hyland, 2001) provided when using the screencast option.
in relation to form-focused (language-re- Ducate and Arnold concluded that, despite
lated) errors (Ferris, 1995) or comments not having any marked cognitive effect, the
on weaknesses in content, structure, or or- screencast software had a positive influence
ganization (Hedgcock & Lefkowitz, 1994; on their learners and could be a motivational
Hyland, 2001; Leki, 1991; Oladejo, 1993; force in the L2 classroom.
Saito, 1994). However, Lee (2007), who The present case study aimed to expand
conducted a study with high- and low- existing research on computer-mediated
proficiency learners, noted that the low- feedback. Although the educational use of
proficiency learners appeared to develop a computer-based feedback in the classroom
less positive attitude toward error feedback is becoming widespread and popular, less is
than did their high-proficiency counterparts, known about how the mode (oral vs. writ-
requesting less focus on errors in instructor ten) as well as the use of Screencast-O-Matic
feedback. Nevertheless, studies exploring software (“screencast” from now on) and
different degrees of explicitness have indi- Microsoft Word features (“Word” from
cated that learners prefer direct error feed- now on) influence the manner in which
back because indirect feedback does not instructors provide, and learners receive,
provide them with sufficient information feedback. In particular, this study posed
to resolve more complex errors, such as the following questions:
idiosyncratic or syntactic errors (Radecki
& Swales, 1988). Other studies, however,  To what extent does using Word or
have shown that learners favor indirect error screencast to provide written or oral feed-
feedback because it gives them clues and, back influence how the instructor pro-
thus, a more active role in the revision pro- vides feedback to students?
cess (Arndt, 1993; Chandler, 2003; Ferris &  To what extent does receiving oral or
Roberts, 2001; Hyland, 2001; Saito, 1994). written feedback using Word or screen-
These results illustrate that, regardless of the cast influence students’ revisions?
learner’s proficiency and the instructor’s
feedback practices, learners’ individual dif-
ferences can also influence expectations and
reactions to instructor feedback (Lee, 2007).
Methodology
This suggests that feedback provision may Participants and Setting
have to be individualized to learners’ specific The study took place at a mid-sized univer-
needs. sity on the East Coast of the United States.
Foreign Language Annals  VOL. 49, NO. 1 63

Of the eight students enrolled in a required took about 25 minutes to produce 12–15
three-credit capstone advanced Spanish minutes of screencast (video) commentary.
writing course, four agreed to complete Two of the learners, Julie and Laura,
questionnaires and participate in inter- received the instructor’s comments on their
views. These four participants, whose age first draft via Word and via screencast on
ranged from 19 to 21, had completed at least their second draft; in contrast, Allan and
five courses in Spanish at the third- or Sancho received comments via screencast
fourth-year level, two of which focused on on their first draft and Word on their second
the development of grammar and writing draft. Given that the learners were in an
conventions. All of the participants were advanced Spanish language class, feedback
learners of Spanish as a second language in both treatments was given in Spanish.
whose first language was English. The class Regardless of the mode of delivery,
met one day a week for two and a half hours. both sets of learners received indirect, un-
To maintain confidentiality, each learner focused feedback based on their individual
was assigned a pseudonym. The instructor, needs as observed in their essays rather than
a native speaker of Spanish, had been focused feedback designed to address a pre-
teaching the class for 8 years and was determined particular problem in the writ-
very familiar with the writing curriculum ing of all the learners, such as the selection
at this level. of the imperfect or preterit that may have
just been studied in class and with which
learners might have ongoing issues. Indirect
Procedures feedback was chosen because it (1) engages
During the semester, learners completed learners in problem solving and reflection
two expository essays, two argumentative that may be more likely to foster linguistic
essays, and two narrative essays. The last improvement and accuracy in writing
narrative essay was selected for analysis be- (Bitchener & Knoch, 2010), and (2) is a
cause learners were already accustomed to viable practice for learners who are ad-
receiving feedback using Word and vanced enough to self-correct (Chandler,
screencast. 2003). When using Word, the instructor
For each essay, learners made revisions provided written comments on content,
triggered by the instructor’s feedback on the structure, and organization as well as
two drafts, then submitted the final version labeled errors of form using error codes,
of the essay. Only the second draft and final e.g., (arg) agreement—for incorrect sub-
version received a grade, based on content, ject-verb, article-noun, or noun-adjective
structure, organization, vocabulary, editing agreement; (prep) prepositions; (art) ar-
(punctuation and spelling), and grammati- ticles; (conj) conjunctions; (ort) spelling
cal accuracy. When using Word, the in- and punctuation; (rel) relatives; (asp) verb
structor provided feedback on both global aspect; (mod) verb mood; (per) periphrasis;
issues in the form of written comments on (refl) reflexive verb; (tiem) verb tense; (eng)
content, structure, and organization and on English; (voc) vocabulary; and (???) not
local errors using abbreviations in parenthe- understandable.
ses for grammar, vocabulary, and editing). When using screencast, the instructor
When using the screencast software, the first provided comments on content, struc-
instructor also offered oral comments on ture, and organization so as to emphasize
issues of content, structure, and organiza- that structure and content are fundamental
tion and on local errors. Regardless of the aspects of effective written communication;
method by which feedback was provided, the instructor then enumerated the number
the instructor first read each essay. She then and type of errors in each sentence, address-
spent approximately 20 minutes providing ing, e.g., whether an error had occurred in
written feedback using Word. In contrast, it the use of a verb (tense, aspect, mood) or
64 SPRING 2016

subject-verb agreement. The instructor of- What were the advantages and the disadvan-
ten hovered with the cursor in the area tages in relation to feedback and revision
where the error was or indicated whether using Word and screencast?), twelve semi-
the error occurred at the beginning, middle, directed questions in which the learners
or end of a sentence. chose from a given pool of answers, and
four ranking questions (e.g., learners ranked
their preferred type of feedback from a given
Data Collection and Analysis pool of answers). A month after finishing the
To understand the kinds of revisions that semester, learners also participated in indi-
learners made under each feedback condi- vidual semi-structured interviews that were
tion, the researchers counted the type guided by a set of open-ended questions
of errors and analyzed the instructor’s that addressed the perceived benefits and
recorded comments provided via the drawbacks of using Word and screencast to
screencast software and Word using the address content, structure, organization,
comments/coding error rubric. They then grammar, vocabulary, and editing errors.
compared the initial draft containing the
instructor’s comments to learners’ revised
versions and subsequently compared the Results
second drafts featuring the instructor’s sec- The study first focused on the extent to which
ond set of comments to the final draft sub- providing written or oral feedback using
mitted by the learners. Each error that was Word or screencast influenced how the in-
noted in the first and second drafts was structor provided comments to students.
coded and then marked as successfully re-
vised, unsuccessfully corrected, or ignored.
An error was coded as successfully revised if Feedback on Draft 1: Analyses of Text
the error was corrected according to the and Instructor’s Feedback
given error code. The error was coded as Table 1 illustrates that there were not clear
an unsuccessful revision if the learner’s differences in the amount of feedback that
modified changes were still inaccurate or was offered across the two different modal-
if the feedback had simply been ignored. ities. Yet, although learners still received
Feedback on content, structure, and orga- numerous oral comments on form, oral
nization was also categorized in two ways: feedback was more frequent in the areas
(1) how feedback was provided (i.e., sug- of content, structure, and organization
gestions, statements, commands, questions, whereas written feedback occurred more
disagreement, or praise), and (2) the ex- frequently on form.
pected revision action (deletion, addition, In addition to the differences in where
change, or clarification) in order to deter- feedback was focused, the manner of the
mine whether mode or medium changed instructor’s feedback (i.e., linguistic func-
how the feedback was expressed by the tions such as suggestions, statements, ques-
instructor or acted on by the learners. tions, commands) and the type of learners’
Both researchers participated in coding expected revisions, such as addition/dele-
the data. An initial interrater reliability of tion, also differed (Table 2). It is noticeable
94% and discrepancies were discussed until that the instructor used praise and state-
final agreement was reached. ments with all four learners. Other com-
In addition to completing the narrative ments were conveyed using different
essay, learners completed a pre-questionnaire manners of feedback: suggestions with
designed to obtain background information Laura, Sancho, and Allan; commands
(e.g., students’ origin, first language, courses with Julie, Sancho, and Allan; questions
taken in Spanish) and a post-questionnaire with Laura; and disagreement with Julie.
comprising four open-ended questions (e.g., The medium (screencast or Word) through
Foreign Language Annals  VOL. 49, NO. 1 65

TABLE 1
Amount of Feedback Provided on Content, Structure, Organization, and
Form Errors Using Screencast and Word
First Draft Second Draft
Oral Written Oral Written
(Screencast) (Word) (Screencast) (Word)
Number of Number of Number of Number of
Comments Comments Comments Comments
Allan and Julie and Laura Julie and Laura Allan and
Sancho Sancho

Content 15 9 11 5
Structure 11 2 4 1
Organization 2 0 3 0
Form 32 54 57 54
Total 60 65 75 60

which comments were offered did not ap- In addition to differences in the types of
pear to affect the manner in which the in- errors that were noted, two additional qual-
structor provided the feedback. Similarly, itative differences stood out: (1) the length
the instructor’s most frequent feedback in of feedback (longer versions in video), and
both modes was to request clarification in (2) differences in the degree of explicitness
aspects of the narrative. in feedback on form. For example, Julie and

TABLE 2
Manner of Instructor’s Feedback and Learners’ Expected Revisions
First Draft Second Draft
Oral Written Oral Written
(Screencast) (Word) (Screencast) (Word)
Sancho Allan Laura Julie Laura Julie Sancho Allan

Manner of feedback
Suggestions 31% 7% 29% 10% 12.5% 67%
Statements 23% 46% 43% 25% 40% 33%
Questions 14% 12.5%
Command 38% 7% 25% 20% 25%
Disagreement 25%
Praise 8% 40% 14% 25% 30% 50% 33% 67%
Change expected
Addition 8% 17%
Clarification 25% 66% 83% 33% 14%
Change 50% 33% 67% 86% 50% 100%
Delete 17% 50% 100%
66 SPRING 2016

Sancho, whose narrative essays resembled careful with this. [. . .] What I suggest is
descriptive accounts rather than narratives, that you concentrate on one of these
received the same type of feedback address- two stories: the story of how you go to
ing the genre and structure of the essay. Nigeria every year, or the story of how
However, the length of the feedback varied you left, of how your family decided to
depending on whether the instructor was leave Nigeria [pointing in the text] and
using Word (Laura) or screencast (Sancho). come to the United States, perhaps be-
In the case of Laura, the instructor’s written cause of the education system, to give
feedback in Word addressed the problems you a better life. And remember that
concisely and cohesively in 130 words:1 you need to have the four elements of
narration, which are the background,
In your story we see how, despite you
plot, conflict, and denouement.2
never having being abroad, you have
experienced multiculturalism through Comparisons of the number of words,
swimming. What is not clear are the the number of topics addressed, and the
different stages in the narration. To detail in which each concern (e.g., same
some extent, I can see the background genre structure problem) was addressed
and the plot, but where is the conflict demonstrated that the instructor gave
(remember, when Cinderella needs to more detailed feedback on narrative issues
run away from the palace) and the end- using screencast than using Word, includ-
ing (when the prince puts the shoe on ing longer explanations on content and
her)? Perhaps, you could elaborate on structure that referred not only to problems
whether it was strange to share with in the current version of the essay but also to
people from another culture some previous comments provided.
event or action that could have created A closer examination of feedback on
tension with your university room- form showed that errors were more clearly
mate. Remember, this is a narration in indicated in Word; errors were distinctly
which you have license to make up facts marked using the error coding system,
that help you create the tension you and the type of error could thus be easily
need to create an interesting story. counted. In the screencast mode, however,
the instructor was less explicit with her
In contrast, when addressing the same
indirect feedback, limiting herself to saying,
kind of key revisions, the instructor’s oral
for example, “Hay dos errores de concordan-
feedback to Sancho was more elaborate,
cia en esta frase” [“There are two agreement
consisting of 367 words:
errors in this sentence”] (to Laura) and
sometimes marking with the cursor where
This essay is more a reflection than a
the error was or noting that there were
narration, which was what I was asking
similar problems in other parts of the essay
for; to make it into a narration, you
(to Allan):
could focus on two components. One
of the main topics that you have here is One of the problems that you have is
that there are many people who migrate with verb tenses, and I am going to ask
to the United States, and it seems that you to go back to the textbook, to chap-
the topic of education [marking in the ter 4, where it explains complex narra-
text] is very important for you. You say tion and [I am going to ask you] to look
that the United States has an education at lesson 19 and read lesson 19 closely,
system that is better than [in] other in which it explains [passing pages in
countries, but be careful with this the book] the use of the participle and
idea because it has been shown in the the perfect tense. This makes it clear
last few years that the education system which are the tenses that you need to
is not as good as it used to be. So be use for the diverse temporal jumps in
Foreign Language Annals  VOL. 49, NO. 1 67

the past; when to use the pluperfect in individuals’ needs at this stage of the com-
the indicative and the present perfect in position, rather than to the medium (Word,
the subjunctive; when we use the pret- screencast) through which feedback was
erit or the imperfect instead of the plu- offered.
perfect in the indicative or when we use When comparing the instructor’s use of
the pluperfect in the indicative instead Word and screencast to provide feedback on
of the preterit or imperfect. This is the first and the second draft, the research-
something I want you to focus on. ers noted that the instructor consistently
used these tools in similar ways. For exam-
ple, in both the first and second drafts,
Feedback on Draft 2: Analyses of the feedback on form differed in the degree of
Texts and the Instructor’s Feedback explicitness and immediacy: With Word,
When analyzing draft 2 and comparing it to the error could be clearly identified by loca-
draft 1 (see Table 1), the researchers noted a tion (i.e., giving a sense of immediacy)
decrease in the amount of feedback related and type (e.g., subject-verb agreement),
to content and structure: Evidently, many whereas when using screencast, the error
concerns from the first draft had been was flagged but not always clearly located;
addressed. However, once again, more the instructor also mentioned grammatical
frequent comments were offered in the rules more frequently. In addition, the
oral mode (screencast) than in writing length of the instructor’s feedback on con-
(Word). An interesting point is that feed- tent and structure on both drafts was longer
back on organization was only given in the with screencast, and comments on organi-
video (in both drafts) and never occurred in zation were the only type of feedback that
the written feedback. Regarding form, feed- appeared only in the video.
back was equally present in both mediums,
reflecting the ongoing need for learners to
work on linguistic aspects of the task. Learners’ Revisions
Regarding feedback manner and ex- With regard to learners’ revisions, as shown
pected changes in the second draft, there in Table 3, on the first draft, all learners
were some differences in how the feedback responded to comments on content and
was provided (Table 2). Learners all re- structure regardless of the mode in which
ceived more praise than in the first draft. they received the feedback. Learners also
Laura, Julie, and Sancho received sugges- corrected similar percentages of form errors
tions for more changes, while only Laura whether using Word’s Track Changes/
and Julie received commands—in Julie’s coding or screencast. Similarly, in the sec-
case, to delete information, and in Laura’s ond draft, learners also corrected all issues
case, to change it. Laura and Allan received of content and most issues of structure,
feedback in the form of statements, and Julie regardless of the mode in which they re-
was the only one who received questions. ceived the feedback.
The instructor’s choice of linguistic func-
tion used to convey her comments seemed
to be related to the type of expected solu- Learners’ Perceptions
tion, such as deletion and change. With Data from the questionnaires3 and the semi-
regard to the instructor’s expected correc- structured interviews provided a deeper un-
tions, change, clarification, and deletion derstanding of learners’ perceptions about
were again the most common (see Table 2). the use of oral and written feedback using
The instructor did not ask for additions, as screencast or Word. Regarding the medium,
they would have been less relevant at this learners’ questionnaire responses and com-
stage of the composition. The anticipated ments during the interviews revealed that
modifications seemed to be related to receiving feedback via screencast (orally)
68 SPRING 2016

TABLE 3
Percentages of Corrected Content, Structure, and Form Issues in First and
Second Draft
First Draft Second Draft
Oral Written Oral Written
(Screencast) (Word) (Screencast) (Word)
# of Feedback # of Feedback # of Feedback # of Feedback
Corrections Corrections Corrections Corrections
Allan Sancho Julie Laura Julie Laura Allan Sancho

Content 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100%


Structure 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 0% NA 100%
Organization 100% 100% NA NA NA 100% NA NA
Form 79% NA NA 63% 100% 33% 50% 62.5%

was initially a challenge. Allan commented, frequent.” Clearly, learners perceived a


“[Students] are used to receiving papers qualitative difference in the two modes of
with corrections marked on them so that’s feedback. In terms of the quantity of com-
what you are familiar with.” In her inter- ments, learners pointed out differences in
view, Laura noted, the amount and specificity of the comments
that they received in each medium. When
The first time the instructor sent us the
talking about the written comments that
video, I didn’t know how to approach it;
they had received (Word), learners felt
I didn’t know what to do with it. . . : I
that because of the more limited space of-
just kind of . . . like sat and stared at the
fered by the tracked changes, they
screen for a while and like would listen
“wouldn’t be 100% sure of the suggestion
to it and I was, like, I still don’t know
. . . because sometimes I still didn’t quite
what I am supposed to be doing . . . so I
know what I should be doing” (Allan, inter-
had to go back and watch it again . . .
view) and that the instructor “had more
aah . . . and that was kind of frustrating.
space” (Julie, interview) in screencast.
In addition, in her interview, Julie With regard to the quality of com-
pointed out that “the video would have ments, when discussing content, structure,
taken hours if you went through it and and organization, learners felt that the
picked out each and every grammatical printed version allowed for less elaboration
error.” “because there was such a small amount of
Regarding the tone of comments, learn- space, the instructor would give one or two
ers sometimes noted that written comments options, like you could do this or this”
could seem “very impersonal and rigid” (Julie, interview). In addition, given the
(Sancho, questionnaire). In contrast, with lack of space, the comments were not al-
screencast, learners felt “like [they were] ways clear and “not everything made sense”
having a conversation with the professor” (Laura, interview). However, via screencast,
(Sancho, questionnaire). Julie also noted Laura felt that the instructor was able to
that “with the written feedback, every elaborate and provide more information.
once in a while the instructor would point In his interview, Allan mentioned that
out, like, ‘This is good—I like this conclu- “[the video feedback] was a little more in
sion statement for this paragraph,’ or some- depth because it was easier [for the instruc-
thing like that . . . but . . . it wasn’t as tor] to provide more information speaking
Foreign Language Annals  VOL. 49, NO. 1 69

than having to type it all out,” and Laura (Laura, interview). When they talked about
also commented that “for content and stuff grammar, learners commented how the in-
like that,” it provided “a much more broad structor, using the cursor, highlighted “the
sense of where I could fix things . . . and how sentence where the grammatical or vocabu-
I could go back and do that.” lary error was” (Julie, interview), which
On the other hand, when commenting helped her find the errors, and Allan noted
on form, learners quickly picked up that the that “the instructor had a little cursor she
instructor could offer more direct help would move towards you.” However, Laura,
when providing comments on form using perhaps overwhelmed by the exertion of
Word. Learners welcomed the coding sys- having to listen for the errors and having
tem because “[the instructor] would put the to mark them down on her composition,
little grammar codes like ASP . . . next to was not aware that the instructor was using
the words and [they] would all be in red, the cursor to help the students.
nice and colorful” (Julie, interview), which Despite the difficulty in pinpointing
told them “exactly where [their] errors their grammatical errors in screencast,
were” (Allan, interview). Learners also felt learners mentioned some other interesting
that they addressed the form comments bet- benefits of this medium. In her interview,
ter when they were provided in Word be- Julie pointed out that listening to the videos
cause, for example, “I was able to print out provided meaningful opportunities to de-
the recommendations and look at it side by velop her listening skills: “We were focused
side with what I was thinking of for my new so much on writing, but that was good
version” (Julie, interview). In fact, Laura listening practice, too.” Laura (interview)
(interview) also mentioned that when she also pointed out that listening to the in-
received the comments in Word, “it made structor read the sentences out loud helped
[her] go back and look into where else [she] her understand her errors, saying, “When I
had made that mistake, even if it wasn’t heard the instructor read things on the vid-
necessarily highlighted for [her].” Even eos, I was like . . . yeah, there has got to be a
when feeling discouraged by seeing all the better word for that than what I’ve chosen,”
coding errors on their papers (“when I saw which then encouraged her to look up dif-
all of the little red marks I would just be ferent vocabulary.
shocked”), learners seemed to agree that Overall, results suggested that in gen-
being “able to go through [the errors] and eral, frequency of feedback was related to
just highlight things” while revising made learners’ needs and the instructor’s ease of
the correction process easier. commenting on content, structure, and or-
However, when using screencast, learn- ganization via screencast or Word. How-
ers felt that “with the video . . . the error was ever, the explicitness and the quality of
indicated generally, but it’s not exact” the feedback differed greatly based on the
(Allan, interview). Learners were not always medium (Word and screencast) and mode
able to find the error, for example, “because (written and oral). Learners’ perceptions of
in some sentences I would look at it and I these differences also revealed the influence
was like, I don’t know, I think maybe, of the tools on how they saw their own
maybe it’s this one, maybe it’s this one, I performance. Moreover, when comparing
am not sure though” (Laura, interview). both mediums, the learners all preferred
Laura, who acknowledged that her “gram- “written comments for grammar issues”
mar wasn’t very good,” found that the video (Allan, interview) and agreed that Word
required her to do many things at the same was better “for more specific things, like
time (i.e., writing, listening, and thinking): grammar” (Laura, interview). Perhaps Julie
“I had to put a sheet of paper alongside my said it best in the questionnaire when
printed essay so I could write notes that she pointed out that they would ideally
weren’t just specific points on that essay” like to receive “content comments in video
70 SPRING 2016

format and grammar commentary in written were also clearly related to the mode and the
format.” Yet despite learners’ beliefs that medium used (Ducate & Arnold, 2012).
Word was better for error correction, results Once more, while both tools allowed the
showed a better rate of corrections when the instructor to provide feedback in a useful
instructor used screencast to address lin- fashion, the limitations of each tool re-
guistic errors. stricted her actions (Kaptelinin et al.,
1995). Finally, results show that the fre-
quency of feedback, the choice of feedback
Discussion tool, the manner in which feedback was
It has previously been shown that tools, given (suggestions, statements, or com-
with their various affordances and limita- mands), and the type of revision expected
tions, can be used by learners or instructors (e.g., modifying an inappropriate structure
to manipulate and mediate a product for the narrative genre) were all influenced
(Engestr€ om, 1991, 1999). First, this study by the instructor’s perception of the learn-
set out to explore to what extent two tools, ers’ individual needs and personalities and
i.e., Screencast-O-Matic and Word, influ- further highlight the critical need for in-
enced the nature of the instructor’s written structors to investigate and understand
and oral feedback. The findings suggest that learners’ strengths and differences (Lee,
overall, when providing feedback on con- 2007).
tent, structure, and organization, the in- In addition, the study sought to better
structor provided more frequent and understand learners’ reactions to the types
lengthier comments, elaborated more freely of feedback they received. Results show that
on her comments, and could move the cur- learners made similar numbers of changes
sor back and forth via screencast to high- regardless of whether they received written
light different sections of the essay, thus or oral feedback. These findings, which may
giving the impression of a more holistic seem to contradict Tuzi’s (2004) study—
commentary. In contrast, although poten- where learners made more revisions when
tially the instructor could have written as they received electronic feedback than
much as she wanted when using Word, the when they received oral feedback—may
medium itself, with its comment boxes in be explained by the particular tools that
the sidebar, did not encourage frequent or were employed. While Tuzi’s learners could
lengthy responses; remarks offered in this not go back to the oral conversations with
medium tended to be more concise. The the instructor, learners in this study could
findings suggest that the oral and written listen to the instructor’s comments in the
feedback provided by the instructor was video as many times as they wished. There-
manipulated by the limitations set by the fore, as in Ducate and Arnold’s (2012)
medium (Engestr€ om, 1991). study, these learners did not show a signifi-
Furthermore, the tool used also af- cant qualitative difference in the number of
fected the degree of explicitness provided remedial changes they made based on the
by comments on linguistic elements. When method of feedback they had received. Al-
the instructor used screencast, although she though learners tended to prefer the oral
identified the type of error, she was less feedback for global aspects such as content,
explicit about where to find it. She did, structure, and organization, and the written
however, suggest strategies that helped feedback for form, they all agreed that both
learners fix their own errors. When the feedback mediums helped them improve
instructor used Word, on the other hand, their writing skills.
although she did not explicitly correct er- The fact that learners were generally
rors, using the coding system, she indicated successful in utilizing the indirect feedback,
both the type and location of the error. even when provided with different degrees
Therefore, degrees of feedback explicitness of explicitness, supports Bitchener and
Foreign Language Annals  VOL. 49, NO. 1 71

Knoch’s (2010) suggestion that this type of knowledge benefited from receiving the
feedback can engage learners in a reflective written feedback while they also acknowl-
problem-solving activity to improve their edged the advantages of more lengthy oral
writing and accuracy. Except for one explanations. Therefore, using diverse me-
learner, who acknowledged her struggle diums and modes can help educators meet
with grammar and who felt that listening learners’ diverse needs in a way that a single
to the instructor’s comments made the task medium may not.
more difficult, the indirect feedback pro- While these findings should be inter-
vided in both mediums was seen as a valu- preted with caution due to the limited num-
able exercise for learners who were ber of advanced-level participants and the
advanced enough to self-correct, corrobo- focus on a single written genre (narration),
rating previous work (Chandler, 2003). In they also highlight the ways in which two
fact, learners in this study, despite the in- popular modes of feedback delivery can be
structor giving less explicit feedback on used as well as learners’ perceptions of the
form via screencast than via Word, tended efficacy of both types. Future studies could
to make more corrections after receiving focus on additional genres, collect data from
video feedback. This may be explained by a larger number of participants from a vari-
the strategies that the instructor used with ety of course and proficiency levels, delve
screencast, such as asking learners to refer more deeply into learners’ need for explicit
to particular pages of the book and provid- correction—particularly at lower profi-
ing longer explanations for language errors. ciency levels—and address differences be-
Although some learners expressed a view tween treatable and untreatable errors as a
that playing the video was a time-consum- way to identify which degree of explicitness
ing way to focus on form, they actually in feedback most effectively results in reme-
preferred overall to receive feedback via dial action.
the screencast software than via Word,
also corroborating previous findings (Du-
cate & Arnold, 2012). In particular, partic- Conclusion
ipants reported that screencast offered some The emergence, increasing use, affordances,
of the features of an actual conversation and limitations of providing feedback in
with the instructor, they felt engaged in diverse, technology-driven, and multimodal
a dialogue with the instructor, and they ways have not been widely explored. Re-
underscored that hearing the instructor’s sults of this study suggest that both Word
voice made the feedback more personal and screencast offer effective approaches to
and therefore motivational than written engaging learners in improving their writ-
feedback. ing. Accessing both mediums allowed the
Perhaps, as Julie mentioned, the ideal instructor to offer different kinds of feed-
type of feedback would combine both tools. back (direct, indirect), formulate that feed-
Learners would benefit from the immediacy back in different ways (suggestions,
of the coding system using Word—al- statements, commands), refer learners to
though this reduces opportunities for anal- additional resources, engage them in what
ysis and self-directed learning—while also they viewed as a more personalized but
enjoying the benefits offered by more infinitely repeatable coaching session, and
lengthy and deeper explanations on con- target the type of revisions that were needed
tent, structure, and organization from hear- (using addition and deletion) based on her
ing the instructor’s remarks. Indeed, using knowledge of her learners’ personalities and
both mediums may also be useful to address individual needs. Thus, rather than lan-
learners’ different proficiency levels. As seen guage educators viewing these approaches
in this study, those learners who acknowl- as two competing technology-based me-
edged having a lower level of grammatical diums, the findings demonstrate that they
72 SPRING 2016

should be viewed as complementary and Conrad, S. M., & Goldstein, L. M. (1999). ESL
can effectively be used in tandem. student revision after teacher-written com-
ments: Text, context, and individuals. Journal
of Second Language Writing, 8, 147–179.
Notes Crystal, D. (2006). Language and the Internet.
1. All instructor feedback was provided in Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Spanish but is presented in English to Ducate, L., & Arnold, D. (2012). Computer-
streamline the article. mediated feedback: Effectiveness and stu-
2. Two paragraphs were omitted to stream- dents’ perceptions of screen-casting software
line the instructor’s lengthy comments. vs the comment function. In G. Kessler, A.
Oskoz, & I. Elola (Eds.), Technology across
3. Due to the small number of participants, writing contexts and tasks (CALICO Mono-
no patterns emerged from either the graph Series Vol. 10, pp. 31–55). San Marcos,
semi-directed questions or the ranking TX: CALICO.
questions. Ellis, R., Sheen, Y., Murakami, M., & Taka-
shima, H. (2008). The effects of focused and
unfocused written corrective feedback in an
English as a foreign language context. System,
References 36, 353–371.
Arndt, V. (1993). Response to writing: Using Elola, I. (2005). The complexity of revision in
feedback to inform the writing process. In M. the foreign language classroom and the creation
Brock & L. Walters (Eds.), Teaching composi- of a FL revision model (Unpublished doctoral
tion around the Pacific Rim: Politics and pedagogy dissertation). University of Iowa, Iowa City.
(pp. 90–116). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual
Elola, I., & Oskoz, A. (2010). Collaborative
Matters.
writing: Fostering foreign language and writ-
Basharina, O. K. (2007). An activity theory ing conventions development. Language
perspective on student-report contradictions Learning & Technology, 14, 30–49.
in international telecollaboration. Language
Ene, E., & Upton, T. A. (2014). Learner up-
Learning and Technology, 11, 82–103.
take of teacher electronic feedback in ESL
Bernat, E., & Gvozdenko, I. (2005). Beliefs composition. System, 46, 80–95.
about language learning: Current knowledge,
Engestr€ om, Y. (1991). Developmental work
pedagogical implications, and new research
research: Reconstructing expertise through
directions. TESL-EJ, 9 [Electronic version].
expansive learning. In M. I. Nurminen &
Retrieved June 15, 2015, from http://tesl-ej.
G. R. S. Weir (Eds.), Human jobs and computer
org/ej33/a1.html
interfaces (pp. 265–290). Amsterdam: Elsevier
Bitchener, J. (2008). Evidence in support of Science Publishers.
written corrective feedback. Journal of Second
Engestr€ om, Y. (1999). Innovative learning in
Language Writing, 17, 102–118.
work teams: Analyzing cycles of knowledge
Bitchener, J. (2012). A reflection on “the lan- creation in practice. In Y. Engestr€
om, R. Miet-
guage learning potential” of written CF. Jour- tinen, & R. Punam€aki (Eds.), Perspectives on
nal of Second Language Writing, 21, 348–363. activity theory (pp. 377–404). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Bitchener, J., & Knoch, U. (2010). Raising
linguistic accuracy level of advanced L2 writ- Farrokhi, F., & Sattarpour, S. (2012). The
ers with written corrective feedback. Journal of effects of direct written corrective feedback
Second Language Writing, 19, 207–217. on improvement of grammatical accuracy of
high-proficient L2 learners. World Journal of
Blin, F., & Appel, C. (2011). Computer sup-
Education, 2, 49–57.
ported collaborative writing in practice: An
activity theoretical study. CALICO Journal, Fernandez-Garcıa, M., & Martınez-Arbelaiz,
28, 473–497. A. (2002). Negotiation of meaning in nonna-
tive speaker-nonnative speaker synchronous
Chandler, J. (2003). The efficacy of various
discussions. CALICO Journal, 19, 279–294.
kinds of error feedback for improvement in
the accuracy and fluency of L2 student writ- Ferris, D. R. (1995). Student reactions to
ing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 12, teacher response in multiple-draft composi-
267–296. tion classrooms. TESOL Quarterly, 29, 33–53.
Foreign Language Annals  VOL. 49, NO. 1 73

Ferris, D. R. (1997). The influence of teacher development in heritage learning potential


commentary on student revision. TESOL of L2 writing. In R. M. Manchon (Ed.), Learn-
Quarterly, 31, 315–339. ing-to-write and writing-to-learn in an addi-
tional language (pp. 209–236). Amsterdam:
Ferris, D. R. (1999). The case for grammar
John Benjamins.
correction in L2 writing classes: A response to
Truscott (1996). Journal of Second Language Ho, M., & Savignon, S. (2007). Face-to-face
Writing, 8, 1–10. and computer-mediated peer review in EFL
writing. CALICO Journal, 24, 269–290.
Ferris, D. R. (2001). Teaching writing for
academic purposes. In J. Flowerdew & M. Hyland, F. (2001). Providing effective sup-
Peacock (Eds.), Research perspectives on En- port: Investigating feedback to distance lan-
glish for academic purposes (pp. 298–314). guage learners. Open Learning, 16, 233–247.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Kaptelinin, V., Kuutti, K., & Bannon, L.
Ferris, D. R. (2006). Does error feedback help (1995). Activity theory: Basic concepts and
student writers? New evidence on the short- applications. In B. Blumenthal, J. Gornostaev,
and long-term effects of written error correc- & C. Unger (Eds.), Human-computer interac-
tion. In K. Hyland & F. Hyland (Eds.), Feed- tion (pp. 189–201). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
back in second language writing (pp. 81–104).
Kessler, G. (2009). Student-initiated attention
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
to form in autonomous wiki-based collabora-
Ferris, D. R. (2010). Second language writing tive writing. Language Learning & Technology,
research and written corrective feedback in 13, 79–95.
SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
Khatib, M., & Bijani, H. (2012). Evaluating
32, 181–201.
the effectiveness of various types of error feed-
Ferris, D. R. (2012). Technology and correc- back on students’ L2 writing quality. Iranian
tive feedback for L2 writers: Principles, prac- EFI Journal, 8, 102–116.
tices, and problems. In G. Kessler, A. Oskoz,
Komura, K. (1999). Student response to error
& I. Elola (Eds.), Technology across writing
correction in ESL classrooms (Unpublished
contexts and tasks (CALICO Monograph Series
master’s thesis). California State University,
Vol. 10, pp. 7–29). San Marcos, TX: CALICO.
Sacramento.
Ferris, D. R., Liu, H., Sinha, A., & Senna, M.
Kuutti, K. (1996). Activity theory as a potential
(2013). Written corrective feedback for indi-
framework for human-computer interaction re-
vidual L2 writers. Journal of Second Language
search. In B. A. Nardi (Ed.), Context and con-
Writing, 22, 307–329.
sciousness: Activity theory and human-computer
Ferris, D. R., & Roberts, B. (2001). Error interaction (pp. 17–44). Cambridge, MA: MIT
feedback in L2 writing classes: How explicit Press.
does it need to be? Journal of Second Language
Lalande, J. F. (1982). Reducing composition
Writing, 10, 161–184.
errors: An experiment. Modern Language Jour-
Hampel, R., & Hauck, M. (2006). Computer- nal, 66, 140–149.
mediated language learning: Making meaning
Lantolf, J. P. (2006). Sociocultural theory and
in multimodal virtual learning spaces. The
second language learning: State of the art. Stud-
JALT CALL Journal, 2, 3–18.
ies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 67–109.
Haneda, M. (2004). The joint construction of
Lantolf, J. P., & Thorne, S. (2007). Sociocul-
meaning in writing conferences. Applied Lin-
tural theory. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams
guistics, 25, 178–219.
(Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition:
Hedgcock, J., & Lefkowitz, N. (1994). Feed- An introduction (pp. 201–224). Mahwah, NJ:
back on feedback: Assessing learner receptiv- Lawrence Erlbaum.
ity in second language writing. Journal of
Lavolette, E., Polio, C., & Kahng, J. (2015).
Second Language Writing, 3, 141–163.
The accuracy of computer-assisted feedback
Hedgcock, J., & Lefkowitz, N. (1996). Some and students’ responses to it. Language Learn-
input on input: Two analyses of student re- ing & Technology, 19, 50–68. Retrieved
sponse to expert response in L2 writing. Mod- June 15, 2015, from http://llt.msu.edu/
ern Language Journal, 80, 287–308. issues/june2015/lavolettepoliokahng.pdf
Hedgcock, J., & Lefkowitz, N. (2012). Explor- Lee, L. (2007). Fostering second language
ing the learning potential of writing oral communication through constructivist
74 SPRING 2016

interaction in desktop videoconferencing. Sauro, S. (2009). Computer-mediated correc-


Foreign Language Annals, 40, 635–649. tive feedback and the development of L2
grammar. Language Learning & Technology,
Leki, I. (1991). The preferences of ESL
13, 96–120.
students for error correction in college-level
writing classes. Foreign Language Annals, 24, Semke, H. (1984). The effects of the red pen.
203–218. Foreign Language Annals, 17, 195–202.
Manch on, R. M. (Ed.). (2011). Learning-to- Sheen, Y. (2010). The role of oral and written
write and writing-to-learn in an additional lan- corrective feedback in SLA. Studies in Second
guage. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Language Acquisition, 32, 169–179.
Morris, F. (2005). Child-to-child interaction Storch, N., & Wigglesworth, G. (2010).
and corrective feedback in a computer-medi- Learners’ processing, uptake, and retention
ated L2 class. Language Learning & Technol- of corrective feedback on writing: Case stud-
ogy, 9, 29–45. Retrieved July 1, 2015, from ies. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32,
http://llt.msu.edu/vol9num1/morris/default. 303–334.
html
Truscott, J., & Hsu, A. Y.-P. (2008). Error
Nassaji, H., & Swain, M. (2000). A correction, revision, and learning. Journal of
Vygotskyian perspective on corrective feed- Second Language Writing, 17, 292–305.
back in L2: The effect of random versus nego-
Tuzi, F. (2004). The impact of e-feedback on
tiated help on the learning of English articles.
the revisions of L2 writers in an academic
Language Awareness, 9, 34–51.
writing course. Computers and Composition,
Oladejo, J. A. (1993). Error correction in ESL: 21, 217–235.
Learners’ preferences. TESL Canada Journal,
Vinagre, M., & Lera, M. (2008). The role of
10, 71–89.
error correction in online exchanges. In F.
Oskoz, A. (2009). Learners’ feedback in online Zhang & B. Barber (Eds.), Handbook of re-
chats: What does it reveal about students search on computer-enhanced language acquisi-
learning? CALICO Journal, 27, 48–68. tion and learning (pp. 326–341). Hershey, PA:
IGI Publishing.
Oskoz, A., & Elola, I. (2014). Promoting for-
eign language collaborative writing through Vinagre, M., & Mu~ noz, B. (2011). Computer-
the use of Web 2.0 tools. In M. Gonzalez- mediated corrective feedback and language
Lloret & L. Ortega (Eds.), Technology and accuracy in telecollaborative exchanges. Lan-
tasks: Exploring technology-mediated TBLT guage Learning & Technology, 15, 72–103.
(pp. 115–147). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cam-
Radecki, P., & Swales, J. (1988). ESL student bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
reaction to written comment on their work.
Ware, P. D., & O’Dowd, R. (2008). Peer feed-
System, 16, 355–365.
back on language form in telecollaboration.
Rennie, C. E. (2000). Student preferences re- Language Learning & Technology, 12, 43–63.
garding grammar instruction and feedback in
Yang, M., Badger, R., & Yu, Z. (2006).
university-level ESL courses (Unpublished
A comparative study of peer and teacher feed-
master’s thesis). California State University,
back in Chinese EFL writing class. Journal of
Sacramento.
Second Language Writing, 15, 179–200.
Robb, T., Ross, S., & Shortreed, I., (1986).
Zhang, S. (1995). Reexamining the affective
Salience of feedback on error and its effect
advantage of peer feedback in the ESL writing
on EFL writing quality. TESOL Quarterly,
class. Journal of Second Language Writing, 4,
20, 83–93.
209–222.
Saito, H. (1994). Teachers’ practices and
students’ preferences for feedback on second
language writing: A case study of adult Submitted September 11, 2015
ESL learners. TESL Canada Journal, 11,
46–70. Accepted December 18, 2015

View publication stats

You might also like