You are on page 1of 25

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1997, 68, 1–25 NUMBER 1 (JULY)

INCREASING THE VARIABILITY OF RESPONSE


SEQUENCES IN PIGEONS BY ADJUSTING
THE FREQUENCY OF SWITCHING
BETWEEN TWO KEYS
A RMANDO M ACHADO
INDIANA UNIVERSIT Y

Three experiments compared the amounts of behavioral variability generated with two reinforcement
rules. In Experiments 1 and 2 pigeons received food whenever they generated a sequence of eight
pecks, distributed over two keys, provided that the sequence contained a certain number of change-
overs between the keys. Although no variability was required—the birds could obtain all reinforcers
by repeating the same sequence—the pigeons emitted a large number of different sequences. In
Experiment 3 pigeons received food whenever they generated a sequence that had not occurred
during the last 25 trials. After prolonged training, the birds showed more sequence variability than
in the first two experiments. The analysis of the internal structure of the response sequences revealed
that, in general, (a) the location of the first peck was highly stereotyped; (b) as the trial advanced,
the probability of switching to the initially preferred key decreased whereas the probability of switch-
ing to the other key increased; and (c) a first-order Markov chain model with transition probabilities
given by a logistic function accounted well for the internal structure of the birds’ response sequences.
These findings suggest that, to a large extent, the variability of response sequences is an indirect
effect of adjustments in changeover frequency.
Key words: behavioral variability, switching behavior, nonstationary Markov chain model, response
sequence, key peck, pigeon

When a class of responses is reinforced, the sponse topographies (e.g., Blough, 1966; Bry-
distributions of force, duration, latency, lo- ant & Church, 1974; Hest, Haaren, & Van de
cation, and topography across the members Poll, 1989; Machado, 1989, 1992, 1993; Mor-
of the class typically become less variable (for gan & Neuringer, 1990; Morris, 1987, 1989;
a review see, e.g., Boulanger, Ingebos, Lahak, Neuringer, 1991; Schoenfeld, Harris, & Farm-
Machado, & Richelle, 1987). But reinforce- er, 1966; Shimp, 1967). Taken together, the
ment may also promote and maintain varia- two sets of studies suggest the following gen-
tion within and between response classes. For eralization: When reinforcement is contin-
example, when porpoises received food for gent on response variation, response variabil-
generating behavior that experienced train- ity increases; when reinforcement is not
ers had not seen before, they produced new contingent on response variation, response
and highly variable behavior (Pryor, Haag, & variability decreases.
O’Reilly, 1969). Similarly, when pigeons re- However, it is still unclear how animals
ceived food for generating sequences of eight come to behave variably when reinforcement
left or right choices, provided that such se- depends on response variability. According to
quences had not occurred during the last 25 one viewpoint, animals are directly sensitive
trials, they produced high degrees of se- to the variability requirements of the task.
quence variability (Page & Neuringer, 1985). Thus Page and Neuringer (1985) suggested
Similar results have been reported with dif- that when pigeons receive food for generat-
ferent procedures, animal species, and re- ing novel response sequences, an internal
variability generator is activated and its out-
Parts of this paper were presented at the Winter Con- put is tuned to the degree of variability de-
ference on Animal Learning, Winter Park, Colorado,
1996. I am grateful to Kathy Webster and Jennifer Burin
manded by the schedule. In the same vein,
for their help running the experiments, and to Francisco Stokes (1995) suggested that rats in a new
Silva, Munire Cevik, and Richard Keen for many helpful task learn new classes of responses and how
comments on early versions of the paper. much variability to sustain within each class.
Correspondence concerning this article should be sent
to Armando Machado, Department of Psychology, Indi-
A second viewpoint suggests that, at least in
ana University, Bloomington, Indiana 47405 (E-mail: some of the studies mentioned above (e.g.,
amachado@indiana.edu). Bryant & Church, 1974; Hest et al., 1989; Ma-

1
2 ARMANDO MACHADO

chado, 1989, 1992, 1993; Morgan & Neurin- pecks or lever presses, are similar response
ger, 1990; Page & Neuringer, 1985), response events and consequently some generalization
variability may have been a derivative of more between them is likely to occur. Second, if a
fundamental processes. The nature of these changeover after the first response (e.g.,
processes and how they might engender re- LRRRRRRR) is easy to learn, a changeover
sponse variability as one of their by-products after seven responses (e.g., LLLLLLLR) is
is best illustrated by an example. not, because the stimulus control function of
In Page and Neuringer’s (1985) study, pi- number of responses, or elapsed time, shows
geons received food whenever they emitted increased variance as number or time increas-
an eight-peck sequence that had not oc- es (for a review see, e.g., Gallistel, 1990).
curred during the last 25 trials. This rein- Third, the occasional reinforcement of se-
forcement rule instantiates two contingen- quences that contain different numbers of
cies, one on sequence variability and the changeovers may reduce the tendency to al-
other on the behavior of switching between ways produce three or four changeovers per
the two keys. The contingency on switching sequence. I refer to these reasons collectively
takes place because reinforced sequences are as limitations of stimulus control.
more likely to contain an intermediate num- In summar y, the approaches outlined
ber of changeovers (i.e., three or four) than above differ in how they conceptualize the re-
too many (e.g., seven) or too few (e.g., zero). lations between sequence variability and
This bitonic relation between the number of switching or changeover behavior. One ap-
changeovers per sequence and the probabil- proach implies that in Page and Neuringer’s
ity of reinforcement holds because there are (1985) and similar studies the animals
more eight-peck sequences with three or four learned to vary their behavior and, as a con-
changeovers than with any other number. sequence, most of their sequences contained
Hence, the schedule used by Page and Neu- three or four changeovers; the other implies
ringer not only reinforces sequence variabil- that the animals learned to generate sequenc-
ity but also could shape the total number of es with three or four changeovers and, as a
changeovers per sequence towards interme- consequence of stimulus control limitations,
diate values. they also generated a high degree of se-
But how could response variability emerge quence variability. In the former case, vari-
from the simple effects of the schedule on ability is fundamental, whereas the pattern of
changeover frequency? The question is per- changeovers is a derivative; in the latter case,
tinent because the relation between the fre- the frequency of switching and limitations of
quency of switching and sequence variability stimulus control are fundamental, whereas
is asymmetric: To generate variable sequences variability is a derivative.
of choices one must switch between the re- The two viewpoints are not mutually exclu-
sponse alternatives, but the converse is not sive. For example, pigeons and rats may be
necessarily true, because one can switch be- sensitive to both the variability requirements
tween the alternatives without generating of the task, particularly when the response se-
variable sequences. However, the hypothesis I quences are short and easily discriminable
entertain here states that, for reasons identi- from one another, and the switching require-
fied below, the birds in Page and Neuringer’s ments, particularly when the sequences are
task may have been unable to reproduce long. In other words, pigeons and rats may
eight-peck sequences with three or four discriminate that reinforcers are more likely
changeovers (the sequences most likely to to follow different sequences as well as se-
yield food) without simultaneously varying quences that contain an intermediate num-
the location of the changeovers within the se- ber of changeovers. If the preceding view-
quence and consequently without varying point is correct, then only empirical analyses
their response sequences. can reveal how much each process contrib-
Several reasons may explain why, in the ab- utes to a specific outcome. The present set of
sence of external cues, pigeons and rats do experiments initiated these analyses.
not switch at the exact same locations in a In Experiments 1 and 2 I asked how much
sequence of eight choices. First, the compo- sequence variability is generated when rein-
nents of the sequence, left and right key forcement depends exclusively on the num-
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 3

ber of changeovers in an eight-peck se- First, when pigeons and rats are trained to
quence. In both experiments, all reinforcers vary their sequences of choices and then re-
could be obtained by emitting only one se- ceive the same amount of food without any
quence, which means that response variabil- variability constraint (yoked control proce-
ity, although permitted, was not required. My dure), their initially variable behavior gradu-
prediction was that because of the limitations ally converges to the homogeneous patterns
of stimulus control mentioned above, se- ‘‘all choices on the left’’ or ‘‘all choices on
quence variability would be substantial, par- the right’’ (Hunziker, Saldana, & Neuringer,
ticularly when the distribution of switches was 1996; Machado, 1989, 1992, 1993; Page &
shaped towards the intermediate values of Neuringer, 1985). Second, in my previous
three or four. In order to assess the signifi- studies on behavioral variability, I observed
cance of the amounts of behavioral variability that during the first session of variability
obtained in the first two experiments, Exper- training pigeons lose most of the available re-
iment 3 reproduced the conditions that in inforcers because they frequently repeat
Page and Neuringer’s (1985) study generated these two homogeneous patterns. Third, even
the highest degree of sequence variability. Ex- when behavior is variable, it is not uncom-
periment 3 provided a baseline against which mon to see that the patterns with zero or one
the results from Experiments 1 and 2 could changeover outnumber the patterns with
be compared. more changeovers (e.g., Hunziker et al.,
Another goal of the present study was to 1996, Figure 4). Not surprisingly, perhaps,
characterize the serial structure of behavior the patterns with zero or one changeover
in schedules that promote response variation. have greater unconditional strength and are
How sequences of behavior are internally or- more sensitive to reinforcement than other
ganized remains a relatively neglected topic patterns. Hence, the first experiment was ar-
of research, presumably because experiment- ranged to investigate how much sequence
ers have been concerned mainly with global variability would be generated if all but these
effects (e.g., does variability increase when re- patterns were reinforced. If stimulus control
inforcement depends on variability? How is is reduced as the birds switch more frequently
the degree of behavioral variability affected between the keys, then sequence variability
by the type of reinforcement schedule?). Yet, should be substantial even if all reinforcers
the analysis of the serial order of behavior could be obtained by repeating only one se-
should help us to identify the processes that quence.
are responsible for maintaining behavior in a
more or less variable condition. For example, M ETHOD
if one finds that behavior, although highly Subjects
variable, departs systematically from random-
ness (i.e., it contains some structure), then Five experimentally naive pigeons (Columba
the type of structure will provide clues to the livia) and 2 with previous experience in an
principles of serial order in behavior. Hence, autoshaping study participated in the exper-
the second part of this study analyzes the in- iment. The birds were housed in individual
ternal structure of the response sequences home cages, with water and grit continuously
generated in switching-based (Experiment 2) available, but with no dark-light cycle in ef-
and variability-based (Experiment 3) sched- fect. Throughout the experiment the birds
ules of reinforcement. were maintained at 80% of their free-feeding
body weights.

EXPERIMENT 1 Apparatus
In the first experiment, reinforcement was A standard experimental chamber for pi-
contingent on minimal amounts of switching. geons from Med Associatest was used. The
Specifically, pigeons received food whenever front aluminum panel contained three keys
they generated eight-peck sequences with at centered on the wall, 2 cm in diameter, 22
least one (Group 1) or two (Group 2) cm above the floor, and 8 cm apart, center to
changeovers between two response keys. center. The keys could be illuminated from
Three reasons motivated the experiment. behind with red light. Because the right key
4 ARMANDO MACHADO

was not used during the present experiments periment lasted at least 15 sessions and until
it was covered with black tape. Directly below the proportion of sequences that were rein-
the center key and 4 cm from the floor was forced showed no consistent trend for five
a hopper opening (6 cm by 7 cm). The bird consecutive sessions. The total number of ses-
had access to mixed grain when the hopper sions varied across birds from 15 to 25.
was raised and illuminated with a 7.5-W white
light. On the back wall of the chamber, an- R ESULTS AND D ISCUSSION
other 7.5-W houselight provided general il- Most sessions except the first ended before
lumination. An outer box equipped with a the 60th trial. Therefore, to make the present
ventilating fan enclosed the experimental results directly comparable to those of Exper-
chamber. All events were controlled by a per- iment 3 and to previous studies, only the data
sonal computer. from the first 50 trials of each session were
analyzed. In addition, because there were no
Procedure significant differences between the two ex-
The birds learned to peck the keys through perimental groups, the results are presented
a modified autoshaping procedure. After vari- without reference to the group membership
able intertrial intervals (M 5 60 s) during of each bird. Unless otherwise stated, all anal-
which the houselight was on, one of the two yses are based on the last three sessions of
keys was randomly selected and lit. If no peck training.
occurred for 6 s, the lit key was turned off The top panel of Figure 1 shows each bird’s
and food was presented for 3 s; a peck at the average proportion of reinforced sequences.
lit key provided food immediately. During For all birds except 10798, more than 95% of
food presentations, the keylights and the the sequences were reinforced, which implies
houselight were turned off and the hopper that the number of changeovers per se-
light was lit. After three or four sessions, all quence was at least equal to the minimum
birds pecked the keys reliably. required by the reinforcement schedule. The
During the experiment proper, each daily middle panel shows the averages of the num-
session was divided into trials, and each trial ber of changeovers per sequence. Clearly, all
began with the illumination of the houselight birds switched significantly more frequently
and both keylights. A peck at either key than the minimum required to obtain food.
turned both keylights off and initiated a 0.4-s Moreover, for 6 birds the average number of
interpeck interval. Pecks during this period changeovers was above the value predicted by
reset the timer for the interval but had no random responding. Hence, not reinforcing
other scheduled consequences. After 0.4 s the patterns with zero changeovers, or with
without a peck, both keys were illuminated zero and one changeovers, was sufficient to
again and the procedure was repeated for a generate a high frequency of switching be-
total of eight pecks. After the eighth peck, tween the keys.
either a 3-s timeout, during which all lights The bottom panel of Figure 1 shows the
were off, or 3 s of access to food followed; average proportion of different sequences
then, a new trial began. Sessions ended after generated by each bird. A proportion of .6,
100 trials or 50 reinforcers, whichever oc- for example, means that during the first 50
curred first. trials the bird emitted 30 different sequences
The 7 birds were assigned randomly to two on average. All birds generated a substantial
groups, with the constraint that the two ex- number of different sequences (M 5 34.5)
perienced birds had to be in different groups. even though only one was required to obtain
In Group 1 (Birds 5291, 1782, 10798, and food. However, the amount of sequence vari-
10405), all eight-peck sequences with more ability was consistently below the lower limit
than zero changeovers (i.e., all sequences ex- of the 95% confidence interval predicted by
cept LLLLLLLL and RRRRRRRR) were re- random responding (the Appendix derives
inforced. In Group 2 (Birds 2186, 25433, and the predictions for random responding).
5269) all sequences containing more than Figure 2 shows that the high frequency of
one changeover were reinforced (of the total switching observed at the end of training was
256 eight-peck sequences, two contained zero a true schedule effect. As predicted, during
changeovers and 14 contained one). The ex- the first session the birds tended to generate
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 5

In conclusion, when reinforcement de-


pended on minimal amounts of switching per
eight-peck sequence, the birds switched sub-
stantially more frequently than required and
earned most of the available reinforcers. With
the increased frequency of switching came a
substantial amount of sequence variability, de-
spite the fact that variability was not required
to obtain food. The switching distributions
also suggest that sequence variability was not
more substantial because the birds switched
too much; as the number of changeovers in-
creases from four to seven, the number of
different sequences is reduced from 70 to
two.

EXPERIMENT 2
The purpose of Experiment 2 was to repro-
duce more closely the contingencies on
switching behavior that are embedded in
most variability-inducing schedules. Thus, in
Page and Neuringer’s (1985) and similar
studies, sequences with zero or seven change-
overs are differentially extinguished when
they occur frequently because only four dif-
ferent sequences can be produced with that
number of changeovers. For similar reasons,
sequences with one and six changeovers may
be reinforced less often than sequences with
two and five changeovers. More generally, the
reinforcement rule in variability-inducing
schedules should shape the distribution of
switches towards intermediate values. When
the birds emit sequences predominantly with
three or four changeovers they are able to
generate a substantial number of different se-
quences and collect most of the available re-
inforcers.
Fig. 1. Top: the horizontal lines show the proportions
An earlier experiment by Br yant and
of reinforced sequences predicted by random respond- Church (1974) also shows how an adequate
ing for Groups 1 (.99) and 2 (.94). Middle: the horizontal shaping of switching behavior may generate
line shows the value of 3.5 switches per sequence pre- high levels of response variability. In a two-
dicted by random responding. Bottom: the solid and dot- choice situation, one group of rats received
ted lines show the average proportion of different se-
quences and the 95% confidence interval predicted by food with a probability of .75 every time they
random responding. The dark bars show averages across switched levers and with a probability of .25
birds. All data come from the first 50 trials of the last ever y time they repeated the preceding
three sessions of Experiment 1. choice. In two other groups these probabili-
ties were .5/.5 and 1/0, respectively. The au-
thors found that the rats in the .5/.5 group
sequences with few or no changeovers, but by tended to persevere on one lever, those in the
the last day all distributions had shifted con- 1/0 group alternated frequently, and those in
siderably to the right. The average curves in the .75/.25 group generated random-like be-
the bottom right panel summarize the effect. havior. These results suggest that response
6 ARMANDO MACHADO

Fig. 2. Relative frequency distributions of the number of switches per sequence for each pigeon. The filled and
open circles correspond to the first and last sessions of training, respectively.

variability may be increased if the reinforce- per eight-peck sequence and not on variabil-
ment contingencies favor to some extent the ity. If sequence variability is indeed a by-prod-
rat’s nonpreferred response alternative uct of switching, then it should increase as
(switching). the number of sequences with three and four
Hence, in Experiment 2 I attempted to switches increases.
shape a binomial distribution of switches cen-
tered around 3.5, the distribution predicted M ETHOD
by random responding. Sequences with too
few or too many changeovers were less likely Subjects and Apparatus
to be reinforced than were sequences with an
intermediate number of changeovers. Hence, The birds, housing conditions, and exper-
as in Experiment 1, reinforcement depended imental chamber were the same as in Exper-
exclusively on the number of changeovers iment 1.
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 7

Procedure
All procedural details remained the same
except that the probabilities of reinforcement
varied with the number of changeovers per se-
quence according to a binomial distribution:
Sequences with zero or seven changeovers
were reinforced with a probability of .03, se-
quences with one or six were reinforced with
a probability of .2, sequences with two or five
were reinforced with a probability of .6, and
sequences with three or four were reinforced
with a probability of 1. These values are pro-
portional to the total number of sequences
that can be generated with a given number of
switches. Thus, a total of 2, 14, 42, and 70 se-
quences are possible with 0, 1, 2, and 3
changeovers, respectively. With the probability
of reinforcement equal to 1 for sequences with
three changeovers, the remaining values were
automatically set. The probabilities of rein-
forcement following the sequences with 4, 5,
6, and 7 changeovers preserved the symmetry
of the binomial distribution.
Each session ended after 50 trials, and the
experiment lasted 20 sessions for Bird 1782
and 25 sessions for the remaining birds. Al-
though the number of reinforced sequences
stabilized before 20 or 25 sessions, the addi-
tional sessions were conducted to collect the
amount of data necessary to analyze the in-
ternal structure of the sequences.
R ESULTS AND D ISCUSSION

Preliminary analyses of the results revealed


no significant differences between the data
from the last three and the last six sessions.
For this reason, the data from the last six ses-
sions of training are reported. The top panel
of Figure 3 shows that 5 birds collected al-
Fig. 3. Top: average proportion of reinforced se-
most as many reinforcers as those predicted quences for each bird. The horizontal line shows the pro-
by random responding (birds’ average 5 portion predicted by random responding. Middle: aver-
0.74; random 5 0.77), but 2 birds earned less age number of switches per sequence. Bottom: average
than 35% of the total reinforcers. Given this proportion of different sequences. The data are from the
last six sessions of Experiment 2. The dark bars show the
large difference between the two sets of birds, group medians.
the median of all scores was selected to char-
acterize the birds’ performance. The middle
panel of Figure 3 shows that, compared to for Birds 10798 and 5269 switching decreased
Experiment 1, the average frequency of markedly to average values less than 1.5. In-
switching for all birds (except 2543) was re- terestingly, the behavior of these 2 pigeons
duced. For Birds 10405, 1782, 5291, and 2186 changed gradually, in one case after nine ses-
the average was between three and four sions during which the average switching fre-
switches, the optimal values; for Bird 2543 the quency was 2.5 (Bird 10798), and in another
average remained high at 4.3 switches, and case after eight sessions during which the av-
8 ARMANDO MACHADO

erage was 3.5 (Bird 5269). Both birds showed served in some earlier studies (e.g., Bryant &
the terminal performance for more than 10 Church, 1974; Hest et al., 1989; Machado,
consecutive sessions. 1989, 1992, 1993; Morgan & Neuringer, 1990;
The bottom panel of Figure 3 shows the Page & Neuringer, 1985) may have been due
proportion of different sequences generated largely to the effects of reinforcement on
by each bird. As one might have guessed from switching, not to the reinforcement of vari-
the preceding results, Birds 10798 and 5269 ability per se. The results presented in Fig-
generated less than 40% of different sequenc- ures 1 and 3 show that simply by adjusting
es (in Experiment 1 both had generated the frequency of switching between the two
more than 60%); for Birds 2543 and 2186 the keys, as many as 30 to 35 different sequences
proportion of different sequences decreased may be generated during 50 trials. To assess
by about .1, for Birds 10405 and 1782 it in- the significance of these values, Experiment
creased by about .05, and for Bird 5291 it re- 3 reproduced the conditions that in Page and
mained constant. Overall, variability de- Neuringer’s (1985) study generated the high-
creased from Experiment 1 to Experiment 2 est degree of sequence variability. If the
(the median proportion of different sequenc- amount of variability observed when the re-
es, for example, decreased from .69 to .61). inforcement rule is defined in terms of
In conclusion, at the end of Experiment 1 switching frequency (Experiments 1 and 2)
the birds were switching more often than pre- matches the amount of variability observed
dicted by random responding, and presum- when the reinforcement rule is defined in
ably for that reason the variability of their re- terms of sequence variability (Experiment 3),
sponse sequences was not more substantial. then one is more inclined to believe that se-
Experiment 2 was implemented to increase quence variability is not directly reinforced,
sequence variability by reshaping the distri- or, equivalently, that sequence variability is a
bution of switches to intermediate values. by-product of other processes. On the other
The results show that 5 pigeons generated se- hand, if the results show that when variations
quences with an average number of switches are explicitly reinforced, sequence variability
close to 3.5, the value predicted by random is greater than when switching is reinforced,
responding, but, contrary to my prediction, then the difference between the amounts of
their behavior did not become more variable. variability observed under the two conditions
For 2 other birds, the frequency of switching will help to quantify the specific effects of the
decreased so much that most reinforcers contingencies of reinforcement on response
were lost, and the variability of their response variation.
sequences was greatly reduced. The reasons Experiment 3 also provided the data that
for the maladaptive behavior of these birds allowed a comparison of the structure of the
remain unclear. response sequences observed in switching-
In one respect, however, Experiment 2 cor- based and variability-based schedules of re-
roborates the findings of Experiment 1, inforcement. If that structure proves to be
namely, that a substantial amount of se- very similar in the two tasks, there is reason
quence variability may be achieved by rein- to believe that the same behavioral processes
forcing the behavior of switching. In fact, the underlie both types of performance.
birds that switched three or four times per M ETHOD
sequence on the average generated more
than 30 different sequences for 20 or more Subjects and Apparatus
consecutive sessions, even though only one Five experimentally naive pigeons (Columba
sequence was required to obtain food. I will livia) participated in the experiment. The
return to the results of Experiment 2 in a sub- housing conditions remained as in Experi-
sequent section concerned with the analysis ment 1. Two experimental chambers were
of the internal structure of the response se- used, the one used in Experiments 1 and 2
quences. and another identical one. Birds 9393 and
5320 were studied in the new chamber.
EXPERIMENT 3 Procedure
The first two experiments were predicated All birds learned to peck the keys using the
on the assumption that the variability ob- autoshaping training described in Experiment
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 9

1. The experiment proper followed an ABA R ESULTS AND D ISCUSSION


design. During the A, or VAR, phases, rein-
Figure 4 shows that during the first VAR
forcement followed each eight-peck sequence
condition the proportions of reinforced trials
that had not occurred during the last 25 trials.
ranged from .50 to .69, with an average of .60.
For example, if the bird produced the se-
The self-yoked condition forced the same val-
quence LLRLLLRR on Trial 35, it received
ues during NoVAR. During the second VAR
food if that sequence had not occurred on Tri-
condition, more trials ended with food and
als 10 to 34. If the sequence repeated any of
the proportions ranged from .61 to .78, with
the last 25 sequences, a timeout occurred. an average of .72. Despite the improvement
Starting with the second session, the last se- in performance in the last phase, the pro-
quences of the previous session were used to portion of reinforced trials was always signif-
decide whether to reinforce the first sequenc- icantly below the value expected from ran-
es of the new session. Thus, the first sequence dom performance.
of each session was compared with Sequences Figure 4 also shows that when reinforce-
50, 49, . . ., 26 of the previous session; the sec- ment required response variation, switching
ond sequence was compared with the first one was more frequent than when reinforcement
and with Sequences 50, 49, . . ., 27 of the pre- required no variation. On the other hand,
vious session, and so on. During the very first the differences between the two VAR condi-
session the computer generated 25 sequences tions were not large (average difference, .24;
randomly to simulate the previous session. range, 2.16 to 1.57). In all conditions, the
Phase B, or NoVAR, was a self-yoked con- average number of changeovers per sequence
dition wherein variability was permitted but was always below the value predicted by ran-
not required to obtain food (Page & Neurin- dom responding.
ger, 1985). Specifically, if the first VAR phase Figure 4 also shows the proportion of dif-
lasted for, say, 20 sessions, then the first ses- ferent sequences generated by each bird.
sion of NoVAR reproduced the order of re- Three features of the data are noteworthy.
inforced and unreinforced trials of Session First, sequence variability was more substan-
15. That is to say, if during Session 15 rein- tial when variability was required (VAR) than
forcement occurred on Trials 1, 3, and 5 and when it was simply permitted (NoVAR). This
a timeout occurred on Trials 2, 4, and 6, then result agrees with previous findings (e.g., Ma-
the same sequence of outcomes occurred on chado, 1989; Page & Neuringer, 1985). Sec-
the first session of NoVAR, regardless of the ond, variability increased from the first to the
bird’s pattern of behavior. The second session second VAR phase, with the size of the effect
of NoVAR reproduced the sequence of out- varying from slight (Birds 10097 and 5320)
comes of Session 16, the third session those to substantial (Bird 9393) (average differ-
of Session 17, and so on, with the seventh ence, .11; range, .03 to .24). Third, the levels
session of NoVAR reproducing again the se- of variability during the VAR phases (.64 and
quence of outcomes of Session 15. Thus, each .74) were always below the values predicted
session during NoVAR reproduced the se- by random responding.
quence of outcomes of one of the last six ses- Experiment 3 implemented the reinforce-
sions of the preceding VAR phase. After the ment rule that in Page and Neuringer’s
NoVAR phase, the birds were reexposed to (1985) study generated the highest degree of
the VAR condition. sequence variability, but it did not yield the
Each phase lasted until the proportions of same outcome. In fact, Page and Neuringer
reinforced and different sequences showed obtained an average of 85% of different se-
no consistent trend for five consecutive ses- quences, whereas Experiment 3 obtained
sions. This criterion yielded 57 to 61 sessions only 64% (first VAR) and 74% (second VAR
during the first VAR phase, 11 to 19 sessions phase). One reason for this difference may
during NoVAR, and 12 to 29 sessions during be that in Page and Neuringer’s study the
the second VAR phase. All other procedural high variability requirement—each sequence
details remained as in Experiment 2. Data had to differ from the last 25—was intro-
analyses are based on the last six sessions of duced only after the birds had experienced
each phase. less stringent requirements (initially each se-
10 ARMANDO MACHADO

quence had to differ from the last 5, then 10,


and then 15 preceding sequences). The hy-
pothesis that a gradual increase of the vari-
ability requirement leads to more variation
than a nongradual increase remains to be
tested.
The degrees of variability observed in Ex-
periment 3 (64% and 74%) were close to the
degree observed in Experiment 1 (69%) and
were slightly greater than the median value
obtained in Experiment 2 (61%). However,
the fact that in Experiment 3 the average
number of switches per sequence was lower
than in the first two experiments suggests that
the similarity in molar measures of variation
(e.g., proportion of different sequences) may
hide differences in the underlying behavioral
processes. Whether this was indeed the case
can only be ascertained by means of a more
refined analysis of the serial structure of be-
havior.

THE INTERNAL STRUCTURE


OF RESPONSE SEQUENCES
Several questions point to the importance
of analyzing the serial structure of the re-
sponse sequences: Why was behavior not
more variable in Experiments 2 and 3? How
did performance differ between these exper-
iments? Given that most birds produced 30
or more different sequences during 50 trials,
what characterizes those sequences? More
generally, what behavioral process generated
the sequences? The approach I followed to
answer these and related questions consisted
of hypothesizing a simple process of response
generation, deriving its predictions concern-
ing the internal structure of the sequences,
and comparing these predictions against the
Fig. 4. The average proportion of reinforced se-
quences (top), the average number of switches (middle), data. If a simple process was rejected, then a
and the average number of different sequences (bottom) slightly more complex one was studied next.
obtained in Experiment 3. For each bird, the left and The analysis is restricted to the last six ses-
right open bars show the results from the first and last sions of Experiment 2 and the last six sessions
VAR phases, respectively; the dark bar shows the data
from the NoVAR phase. The three rightmost columns
of the first VAR condition of Experiment 3,
show averages across birds. All data come from the last because the relatively large number of ses-
six sessions of each phase. The horizontal lines show the sions in these two cases increases the chances
average (solid) and the 95% confidence interval (dotted) that the molecular processes that determine
predicted by random responding. the serial organization of the sequences had
reached a steady state (behavior may be sta-
ble at the molar level after a few sessions but
may still undergo molecular changes).
The simplest process of response genera-
tion is the Bernoulli process—the bird pecks
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 11

the right key with probability p and the left where a is a rate parameter and b is one of
key with the complementary probability 1 2 p’s two fixed points (i.e., when p 5 b, no fur-
p. When p 5 .5 this process corresponds to ther changes in p take place; p 5 0 is the
random responding. None of the data sets other fixed point). A similar equation, possi-
presented in Figures 1 to 4 support this mod- bly with different parameters, holds for q(n).
el because, among other things, a Bernoulli Next, I approximated this difference equa-
process would always generate fewer than 3.5 tion by its continuous version,
changeovers per sequence on the average,
dp
whereas some data sets clearly show a higher 5 ap(b 2 p)
number (see middle panels of Figures 1 and dt
3, and Figure 2).

1 2
As a variation of the above process, one p
5 gp 1 2 , g 5 ab, (2)
might let the probability p vary within the se- b
quence. That is, on the first peck of the se-
quence the bird could choose the right key and assumed that the eight pecks occurred at
with probability p1, on the second peck with times t 5 0, 1, . . ., 7.
probability p2, and so on, with p1 not neces- The bottom panels of Figure 5 plot Equa-
sarily equal to p2. This nonstationary Ber- tion 2. Depending on the sign of g, two cases
noulli process could try to capture the com- may occur. If g . 0, then dp/dt . 0 for p ,
bined effect of variables such as the b and therefore p will increase (see direction
increasing proximity to reinforcement as the of arrows along the p axis); if p . b, then dp/
sequence progresses and the effect of the pre- dt , 0 and p will decrease. Regardless of its
ceding pecks on the location of subsequent initial value, p will approach b, the only stable
pecks. Although plausible, a nonstationary equilibrium. On the other hand, if g , 0
Bernoulli process can also be rejected be- (right panel), b is an unstable equilibrium
cause, as I show below, it fails to predict the and, for p , b, p will approach 0 (the other
switching probability profiles generated by possibility, p . b, would eventually yield p .
the birds. 1, which cannot occur if p is a probability).
A third candidate is a first-order Markov The solution of Equation 2 is the logistic
chain with stationary transition probabilities. function
Here, the location of the next peck is influ- b
enced only by the last peck. That is, the bird’s p(t) 5 , (3)
b 2 p(0)
last choice defines its current state (left or 11 exp(2gt)
right), and the probabilities of choosing the p(0)
left or right keys on the next peck depend on where p(0) is the initial probability of pecking
the state. Again, the profiles of switching the right key (see Figure 5, top). In summary,
probability presented below were inconsistent the model implies that (a) the two changeover
this model. probabilities are the fundamental response
Finally, I arrived at a process that describes units in the situation (in this regard, the mod-
the data well: a nonstationary first-order Mar- el is similar to Myerson and Miezin’s, 1980,
kov chain. The model is illustrated in Figure kinetic model of choice), (b) the two types of
5. The changeover probabilities during the changeover, from the left to the right and
next choice depend on the previous choice from the right to the left keys, follow indepen-
(hence a first-order Markov chain) and how dent courses, and (c) as the trial proceeds, the
far in the sequence the bird is (hence non- changeover probabilities change according to
stationary transition probabilities). The heart a logistic function. In what follows, the model
of the model is the description of how the is compared against the data from Experi-
two changeover probabilities, from the left to ments 2 and 3.
the right key, p(n), and from the right to the
left key, q(n), change with peck number n. I Response Sequences in Experiment 2
assumed that p(n) changes with n according
Figure 6 provides the empirical justification
to the equation
of the model. The data points are the switching
Dp probabilities observed in Experiment 2 as a
5 ap(b 2 p), (1)
Dn function of position in the sequence. There was
12 ARMANDO MACHADO

Fig. 5. Top: First-order Markov chain model with nonstationary transition probabilities. After food or a timeout,
the bird pecks the right and left keys with probabilities p(0) and q(0), respectively, where p(0) 5 1 2 q(0). Subsequent
choice probabilities are given by p(n) and q(n), where n is the number of preceding pecks. For example, if the
second peck was on the left key, the probability of pecking the right key on the third choice would equal p(2); if the
second peck was on the right, the probability of pecking the right key on the third choice would equal 1 2 q(2).
Bottom: plot of Equation 3. The growth parameter g is positive on the left panel and negative on the right.

a remarkable consistency across birds. First, for first peck emerges from this analysis as one
all birds the two switching probability curves of the organizing elements of response se-
are clearly different, which means that the di- quences, presumably because the event pre-
rection of switching matters. Second, the ten- ceding it is markedly different from the
dency to switch to one key increased as the trial events preceding subsequent pecks.
advanced, whereas the tendency to switch to The solid lines in Figure 6 show the fits of
the other key either decreased (for 6 birds) or Equation 3. To satisfy the constraint that q(0)
stayed constant (Bird 2543). Third, Birds 10798 5 1 2 p(0), p(0) and q(0) were set equal to
and 5269 (top panels) fit the same general de- their observed values (i.e., they were not
scription even though they lost most of the treated as free parameters). Parameters g and
available reinforcers because they rarely b were estimated by a nonlinear least squares
switched between the keys. algorithm. Table 1 shows the obtained param-
Another noteworthy feature of the data is eter values. In general the model fitted the
that the birds that varied their sequences the data well. The only exceptions came from the
most all showed a strong stereotypy on their birds that switched so little, particularly late
first choice (first data points away from .5), in the sequence, that some probabilities were
but the birds that varied their sequences the estimated from very small numbers. In fact,
least showed greater variability during their the two most discordant points for Bird 10798
first choice (first data points close to .5). The were not used to fit the model.
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 13

Fig. 6. Probability of switching to the left key (open circles) and to the right key (filled circles) as a function of
position in the sequence. The first data point of each curve corresponds to the probability of starting the sequence
on the corresponding key. The data are from the last six sessions of Experiment 2. The solid lines show fits based
on Equation 3.
14 ARMANDO MACHADO

Table 1 switching within the sequence, whereas all birds


Parameter values used to fit Equation 3 to the data of showed systematic deviations from a constant
Experiments 2 and 3. probability.
Bird g b The preceding fits were all local fits be-
cause each data point was derived from one
Experiment 2 or two consecutive pecks. Figures 8 and 9 il-
10405 p(n) 0.000690 .00284 lustrate the fit of the model to statistics de-
q(n) 0.654 .994
rived from more pecks. Figure 8 shows the
2543 p(n) 0.160 1.00 observed and the predicted distributions of
q(n) 0.908 .756 the number of switches per sequence. Not
1782 p(n) 0.938 .853 surprisingly, two distinct patterns can be ob-
q(n 0.00708 .0387 served. The top two panels show a distribu-
2186 p(n) 1.21 .997 tion with a mode at one and a high frequency
q(n) 0.325 .303 of sequences with zero switches. The remain-
5269 p(n) 0.196 1.00 ing panels show more symmetric distribu-
q(n) 20.815 1.00 tions. Interestingly, 4 birds (10405, 2186,
5291 p(n) 3.01 .813 1782, and 5291) showed modes at three and
q(n) 2.06 .332 five, even though the maximum probability
10798 p(n) 0.376 1.00 of reinforcement occurred after sequences
q(n) 22.14 1.00 with three and four switches. The relatively
Experiment 3 high frequency of sequences with an odd
10097 p(n) 1.44 .518 number of switches occurred because these
q(n) 0.304 .489 birds tended to start and end their sequences
10770 p(n) 0.000710 .000790 on different keys. (The filled circles in Figure
q(n) 0.452 1.00 7 clearly show that the probability of pecking
5320 p(n) 1.97 .252 the right key was well below .5 on the first
q(n) 1.89 .973 choice but was well above .5 on the last one.)
5841 p(n) 21.35 .382 The lower right panel of Figure 8 shows that
q(n) 1.11 .620 the 5 birds that maintained a high degree of
9393 p(n) 0.732 .845 sequence variability switched slightly more
q(n) 20.759 .855 than random responding would predict. In
general, the model fits all switching distribu-
tions reasonably well.
If Equation 3 accurately describes the pro- Figure 9 highlights the response structure
cess underlying the birds’ behavior, then it at the beginning of the trial in terms of run-
should fit not only the changeover probability length distributions. Again, the behavior of
curves displayed in Figure 6 but other proper- Birds 10798 and 5269 differed from the be-
ties of the response sequences as well. Further- havior of the remaining birds; their run-
more, these additional properties should be fit length frequency curves decreased monoton-
with the same parameter values. Figure 7 shows ically from zero to seven, and the curve for
that the model fit well the unconditional prob- the left key increased because many of these
abilities of pecking the two keys and the overall birds’ sequences consisted of pecking the left
probability of switching as a function of posi- key eight consecutive times. For the other
tion in the sequence. Figure 7 also shows why birds the two distributions had different
a nonstationary Bernoulli process or a first-or- shapes; one peaked at zero because of the
der Markov chain with stationary transition strong preference for the opposite key during
probabilities does not fit the data. In fact, if one the first choice; the other peaked at either
tries to predict the switching probabilities on one (Bird 5291) or three (Birds 2543, 10405,
the basis of the probabilities of pecking the left 2186, and 1782), which suggests some ten-
and right keys, the observed values are under- dency to perseverate early in the sequence.
estimated systematically. This fact suffices to re- As the solid lines show, the model fits all data
ject the nonstationary Bernoulli process. Simi- sets reasonably well.
larly, a first-order Markov chain also fails In summary, when exposed to a schedule
because it predicts a constant probability of that differentially reinforced sequences with
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 15

Fig. 7. Probability of right or left key pecks (filled circles) and overall probability of switching (open circles) as a
function of position in the sequence. Switching is defined as either a left-right or a right-left sequence of pecks. The
solid lines show the predictions of the model. The data are from the last six sessions of Experiment 2.

an intermediate number of changeovers, the ferred key. By adjusting the rates at which the
majority of the birds adapted to the schedule switching probabilities changed along the tri-
by (a) starting most of the sequences on one al, 5 birds managed to generate most of their
key, (b) increasing the probability of switch- sequences with the optimal or close to the
ing to the other key as the trial advanced, and optimal number of changeovers. In the pro-
(c) decreasing or maintaining constant the cess they also generated a large number of
probability of switching to the initially pre- different sequences.
16 ARMANDO MACHADO

Fig. 8. Observed (circles) and predicted (lines) distributions of the number of switches per sequence. The bottom
right panel shows the average of all birds except 10798 and 5269 (filled circles), the corresponding average predicted
by the model (solid line), and the binomial distribution predicted by random responding (open circles). The data
are from the last six sessions of Experiment 2.

Response Sequences in Experiment 3 variability. However, if the model fails, then


To what extent does the previous model ac- one is forced to conclude that different pro-
count for the data when sequence variability cesses are at play when switching and vari-
is explicitly reinforced? If the same account ability are the targets of reinforcement. A
holds, then it seems plausible that the same third alternative is possible: The same model
behavioral processes could have been en- may hold but its parameters, and hence the
gaged by the two schedules and, therefore, shape of the curves, are systematically differ-
that switching adjustments and limitations of ent in the two experiments. In this case one
stimulus control may play a large role in what should conclude that the behavioral process-
Page and Neuringer (1985) called operant es are the same but that the specifics of each
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 17

Fig. 9. Observed (circles and squares) and predicted (solid lines) distributions of run lengths at the beginning
of a sequence. Circles and squares are for runs of left and right key pecks, respectively. A run length of three on the
left key, for example, includes all the sequences that start with the pattern LLLR. The data are from the last six
sessions of Experiment 2.
18 ARMANDO MACHADO

Fig. 10. Probability of switching to the left key (open circles) and to the right key (filled circles) as a function of
position in the sequence. The first data point of each curve corresponds to the probability of starting the sequence
on the corresponding key. The data are from the last six sessions of the first VAR phase of Experiment 3. The solid
lines show fits based on Equation 3.

schedule lead these processes into different .5). Afterwards the probability of switching to
directions. the initially preferred key decreased while the
Figure 10 shows the switching probabilities other changeover probability increased. Bird
when sequence variability was explicitly rein- 5841 is the exception because both probabil-
forced, and Table 1 lists the parameters for ities decreased. [For this bird, goodness of fit
each curve fit to the data. For 4 birds, the is reduced markedly if p(0) and q(0) equal
shape of the curves is similar to that observed the observed values. Hence, I treated p(0) as
in Experiment 2. That is, the birds showed a a free parameter and then set q(0) 5 1 2
marked preference for one of the keys on p(0).]
their first choice (first data points away from Figures 11, 12, and 13 test whether the
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 19

Fig. 11. Probability of right or left key pecks (filled circles) and overall probability of switching (open circles) as
a function of position in the sequence. The solid lines show the predictions of the model. The data are from the last
six sessions of the first VAR phase of Experiment 3.

model could fit additional properties of the peaks in Experiment 2 occurred at three and
response sequences using the same parame- five. On the other hand, the average curve in
ter values. Figure 11 shows that the model fit Figure 12 indicates that when variability was
well the overall probabilities of switching and explicitly reinforced, the birds switched less
the unconditional probabilities of pecking often than expected from random respond-
the two keys; Figure 12 shows that it fit the ing, whereas in Experiment 2 (see Figure 8)
switching distributions well, and Figure 13 the birds switched slightly more often than
shows the distributions of run length. Figures predicted by random choices. Figure 13 re-
12 and 13 also reveal additional similarities veals that in Experiment 3 the distributions
and differences between the data from Ex- of run length for the right and left keys dif-
periments 2 and 3. For example, 3 of the 5 fered for all birds. The strong preference for
birds (10070, 5320, and 9393) tended to re- one key during the first choice yielded the
peat sequences with an odd number of fast-dropping curves in the figure. The curves
switches, particularly with one and three (see for the other key were bitonic in two cases,
Figure 12). This result was also observed in with a mode at one, or monotonic but with a
Experiment 2 (see Figure 8), except that the rather slow rate of decay with increasing run
20 ARMANDO MACHADO

Fig. 12. Observed (circles) and predicted (lines) distributions of the number of switches per sequence. The
bottom right panel shows the average of all birds (filled circles), the corresponding average predicted by the model
(solid line), and the binomial distribution predicted by random responding (open circles). The data are from the
last six sessions of the first VAR phase of Experiment 3.

length. These results are qualitatively similar GENERAL DISCUSSION


to those observed in Experiment 2.
The experimental study of a behavioral
In summary, when reinforcement depend-
phenomenon tends to proceed along two av-
ed on sequence variability, the pigeons adapt- enues: the analysis of the conditions that are
ed by (a) starting most of their sequences on necessary and sufficient to generate the phe-
one key; as the trial advanced, (b) the prob- nomenon, that is, the analysis of its causes,
ability of switching to the other key generally and the synthesis of the phenomenon as a
increased, whereas (c) the probability of way to test our understanding of its causes.
switching to the initially preferred key gen- Quite often investigators are able to synthe-
erally decreased. However, for 1 bird the two size a behavioral phenomenon without clear-
changeover probabilities decreased during ly understanding its causes. Such is the case
the trial. The model used to fit the data ob- of behavioral variability, because although
tained when switching was explicitly shaped several studies have shown that response vari-
(Experiment 2) also provided a good descrip- ation can be increased by making reinforce-
tion of the data obtained when variability was ment contingent on variation, the causes of
explicitly reinforced (Experiment 3). this phenomenon and how they are interre-
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 21

Fig. 13. Observed (circles and squares) and predicted (lines) distributions of run lengths at the beginning of a
sequence. Circles and squares are for runs of left and right key pecks, respectively. The data are from the last six
sessions of the first VAR phase of Experiment 3.

lated remain unclear. The experiments re- values of three or four. Although only one se-
ported here attempted to fill this gap. quence was required to obtain all reinforcers
in both experiments, most birds generated
Molar Results more than 30 different sequences in the first
The first two experiments investigated the 50 trials. Experiment 3 showed that when se-
role of switching in promoting the variability of quence variability was explicitly reinforced, the
response sequences. Experiment 1 showed that birds varied their behavior more than when the
when reinforcement followed sequences that same pattern of reinforcers and nonreinforcers
contained one or more changeovers between was delivered regardless of sequence variability
the keys, sequence variability was substantial. (Machado, 1989; Page & Neuringer, 1985).
Similarly, Experiment 2 showed that 5 of 7 Furthermore, at the end of the experiment, the
birds maintained a relatively high degree of se- birds generated a higher number of different
quence variability when the reinforcement sequences, but a lower average number of
schedule shaped the number of switches per switches per sequence, than in the first two ex-
eight-peck sequence towards the intermediate periments.
22 ARMANDO MACHADO

The molar results from the three experi- and RRRRRRRR also increased in Experiment
ments suggest the following interpretation. 3 but not in Experiments 1 and 2. This dynam-
Initially the birds tended to repeat sequences ic property may explain why the average num-
with few or no changeovers between the keys ber of switches per sequence was lower in the
(see Figure 2). If the schedule differentially last experiment (compare the middle panels
extinguished these sequences, then the birds of Figures 1, 3, and 4). The general point is
increased their frequency of switching (see that we cannot rule out the hypothesis that
the middle panels of Figures 1, 3, and 4) and such differences underlay the differences in
generated a relatively large number of differ- the degree of sequence variability across the
ent sequences (see the bottom panels of Fig- three experiments. The hypothesis could be
ures 1 and 3). This large amount of sequence tested by shaping the distribution of switches
variability does not seem to be due to any di- to different values (two, three, etc.) and mea-
rect effects of reinforcement on variability suring the resulting degree of sequence vari-
but to limitations of stimulus control. For ex- ability. If the hypothesis proves to be true,
ample, when the sequence RLLRRRRL is re- then switching behavior, not variability, is the
inforced, the bird may not be able to repeat operant.
that sequence exactly because it will not be
able to switch precisely after the first, third, The Internal Structure of
and seventh pecks. The bird’s difficulty in re- Response Sequences
peating sequences with an intermediate num- The similarity between the internal struc-
ber of changeovers may stem from the simi- ture of the response sequences observed in
larity of the two responses, the increasing Experiments 2 and 3 further suggests that the
variance in the stimulus control function of two types of schedules engaged the same pro-
the number of previous pecks and the time cesses. In both cases the sequences were char-
since the beginning of the sequence, or the acterized by three major attributes: the loca-
occasional reinforcement of sequences with tion of the first peck, the probability of
much higher or much lower number of switching in one direction, and the probabil-
switches. ity of switching in the opposite direction. Spe-
When variability was explicitly targeted, the cifically, the location of the first peck tended
birds generated slightly more different se- to be highly stereotyped in most birds, even
quences than when reinforcement depended though in Experiment 2 the stereotypy of the
only on switching. The reasons for this small first peck did not affect the outcome of the
difference remain unclear. On the one hand, trial, whereas in Experiment 3 such strong
it could be due to a direct effect on response stereotypy reduced the chances of reinforce-
variability of the reinforcement rule used in ment. Also, the probability of switching to the
Experiment 3. For example, the birds in that initially preferred key decreased with the
experiment may have learned after prolonged number of pecks within the sequence, where-
training that reinforcement followed only rel- as the probability of switching in the opposite
atively new sequences, that is, sequences that direction generally increased. However, a few
had not been emitted recently. If so, then vari- birds in both experiments did not fit the pre-
ability is indeed an operant, as Neuringer and ceding description, because they showed ei-
his collaborators have argued. On the other ther increased variability on their first choice
hand, it is also possible that the differences in or a decrease of both changeover probabili-
the degree of variability were due to differ- ties as the trial advanced.
ences in how the two types of schedules inter- The behavior of all birds was well described
acted with the switching behavior of the bird. by a first-order Markov chain with nonstation-
That is to say, although Experiments 1 and 2 ary transition probabilities. The good fit of
attempted to reproduce the contingencies on the model further emphasizes the following
switching that are embedded in variability-in- points. First, because the two changeover
ducing schedules, some additional contingen- probability functions p(n) and q(n) were gen-
cies may have been omitted. For example, erally different, a Bernoulli process cannot
when the number of switches per sequence in- account for the birds’ behavior. Second, be-
creased, the probability of reinforcement for cause p(n) and q(n) increased or decreased
the two homogeneous patterns LLLLLLLL in an orderly fashion with peck number, n, a
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 23

first-order Markov chain with stationary tran- the other hand, the model does suggest more
sition probabilities can also be ruled out. effective ways to increase response variability.
Third, even though alternative models can- For example, the reinforcement rule should
not be ruled out at this stage, the ability of not be based on a composite, such as the total
the changeover model to predict a variety of number of changeovers per sequence, that ig-
other properties of behavior—the proportion nores the direction of the changeover. In oth-
of right key pecks, the overall switching pro- er words, the schedule should treat the two
portions, the distribution of the number of changeovers independently, and should ex-
switches per sequence, and the run lengths plicitly attempt to equalize the probabilities
at the beginning of the sequences—is a pow- p(n) and q(n). Furthermore, to the extent
erful indicator that the model captured the that these changeover probabilities can adjust
major features of the process that structured in a period of 10 s or less (the typical dura-
the birds’ behavior in the three experiments. tion of an eight-peck sequence), the schedule
Equation 2 states that the rate of change of should also consider the serial order of the
the probability of switching is affected by two changeovers. To implement this require-
factors, an intrinsic growth rate, g, and a max- ment, the schedule could give more weight
imum switching probability b. For most birds to the first and the last pecks of the sequence
g was positive and b was close to one for one and less weight to the middle ones (Machado
response and close to zero for the other re- & Cevik, 1997).
sponse. Together with the information that
p(0) was generally far from .5, these param- Explaining Random-Like Behavior
eters indicate that the discriminative stimulus The use of a stochastic model to account for
functions of the two keys differed. The birds behavioral variability may initially look suspi-
tended to perseverate on one key for the first cious. After all, if the model assumes random
few pecks and then switch to the other key. variables p and q, then does it not explain re-
Afterwards, whenever they returned to the sponse variability by fiat? The question raises
initially preferred key, they almost invariably the more general issue of how we should deal
switched back on the next choice. This strat- theoretically with highly variable, random-like
egy assured that switching was not so frequent behavior. For example, assume that in one ex-
that it lowered the probability of reinforce- periment a pigeon behaved in a way that is
ment and, in addition, that switching was con- practically indistinguishable from random re-
tiguous with the reinforcer (see Figure 7, sponding (i.e., an overall proportion of right
open circles). key pecks equal to .5, changeover probabilities
For other birds, g was negative and p (or always at .5, a binomial distribution of the
q) decreased monotonically to zero. This number of switches centered at 3.5, a geomet-
strategy leads to a concentration of switching ric distribution of run lengths, etc.). What
early in the sequence (cf. Figure 7, Birds would constitute an adequate explanation of
10798 and 5269, and Figure 11, Birds 5841 its behavior? One possibility is to say that the
and 9393) and therefore it is potentially un- bird did not learn to respond randomly; rath-
stable—reducing the contiguity between er, it unlearned to respond in specific ways.
switching and the reinforcer could lead to For example, its bias for one key during the
even less frequent switching late in the se- first choice was eliminated, the strong tenden-
quence. However, as switching frequency de- cy to switch in one direction was weakened,
creases so does reinforcement rate, which and the like. According to this point of view,
may evoke some switching. The net effect of random-like responding is what is left when all
these two forces, one tending to decrease sources of bias are eliminated (Machado,
switching frequency and the other tending to 1993; Neuringer, 1986). In terms of the mod-
increase it, is likely to depend on each bird’s el, random responding would be approached
delay-of-reinforcement gradient and resis- as the parameter g goes to 0 and p(0) goes to
tance to extinction. .5. This negative definition of randomness im-
The generality of the changeover model mediately suggests that random-like respond-
cannot be fully assessed because no study of ing may be very difficult, if not impossible, to
sequence variability has analyzed in detail the obtain, and, perhaps more important, that our
internal structure of the animals’ choices. On theories should not attempt to explain ran-
24 ARMANDO MACHADO

dom-like behavior per se, but rather the type ule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52,
and amount of structure still present in the 155–166.
Machado, A. (1992). Behavioral variability and frequen-
animal’s performance. In other words, the cy-dependent selection. Journal of the Experimental
theoretical approach suggested here considers Analysis of Behavior, 58, 241–263.
random-like behavior as a limit condition not Machado, A. (1993). Learning variable and stereotypical
to be directly explained. Instead, our theories sequences of responses: Some data and a new model.
Behavioral Processes, 30, 103–130.
should focus on the types of environmental Machado, A., & Cevik, M. (1997). The discrimination of
conditions that generate particular types of be- relative frequency by pigeons. Journal of the Experimen-
havioral structures (e.g., the alternation be- tal Analysis of Behavior, 67, 11–41.
tween the two keys), the conditions that de- Morgan, L., & Neuringer, A. (1990). Behavioral variabil-
termine the strength of those structures (the ity as a function of response topography and rein-
forcement contingency. Animal Learning & Behavior,
rate or probability of alternation) and, by ex- 18, 257–263.
trapolation, the conditions that may strongly Morris, C. J. (1987). The operant conditioning of re-
reduce those structures (variability-inducing sponse variability: Free-operant versus discrete-re-
schedules) and thereby approach random-like sponse procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
of Behavior, 47, 273–277.
performance. Morris, C. J. (1989). The effects of lag value on the op-
In conclusion, the present study shows that erant control of response variability under free-oper-
when reinforcement depends on the behavior ant and discrete-response procedures. The Psychologi-
of switching between two keys, pigeons auto- cal Record, 39, 263–270.
matically vary their sequences of choices. The Myerson, J., & Miezin, F. M. (1980). The kinetics of
choice: An operant systems analysis. Psychological Re-
experiments also suggest that reinforcing se- view, 87, 160–174.
quence variability explicitly is slightly more ef- Neuringer, A. (1986). Can people behave ‘‘randomly’’?:
fective at generating response variation than is The role of feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
reinforcing switching only. The molecular General, 115, 62–75.
Neuringer, A. (1991). Operant variability and repetition
analyses of the birds’ performance revealed as functions of interresponse time. Journal of Experi-
three organizing elements of their response mental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 3–12.
sequences: the location of the first peck and Page, S., & Neuringer, A. (1985). Variability is an oper-
the two probabilities of a changeover. A first- ant. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior
Processes, 11, 429–452.
order Markov chain model with transition Pryor, K. W., Haag, R., & O’Reilly, J. (1969). The creative
probabilities given by a logistic function de- porpoise: Training for novel behavior. Journal of the
scribed the birds’ behavior well. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 653–661.
Schoenfeld, W. N., Harris, A. H., & Farmer, J. (1966).
Conditioning response variability. Psychological Reports,
REFERENCES 19, 551–557.
Shimp, C. P. (1967). Reinforcement of least-frequent se-
Blough, D. S. (1966). The reinforcement of least-fre- quences of choices. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
quent interresponse times. Journal of the Experimental of Behavior, 10, 57–65.
Analysis of Behavior, 9, 581–591. Stokes, P. (1995). Learned variability. Animal Learning &
Boulanger, B., Ingebos, A., Lahak, M., Machado, A., & Behavior, 23, 164–176.
Richelle, M. (1987). Variabilité comportementale et
conditionnement opérant chez l’animal [Behavioral Received January 5, 1997
variability and operant conditioning in animals]. Final acceptance April 10, 1997
L’Année Psychologique, 87, 417–434.
Bryant, D., & Church, R. M. (1974). The determinants
of random choice. Animal Learning & Behavior, 2, 245–
248.
Gallistel, R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cam- APPENDIX
bridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hest, A. N., Haaren, F. V., & Van De Poll, N. E. (1989). To obtain the probability distribution of
Operant conditioning of response variability in male the number of different sequences predicted
and female Wistar rats. Physiology and Behavior, 45,
551–555. by random responding, let p(N,k) denote the
Hunziker, M. H. L., Saldana, R. L., & Neuringer, A. probability of the event ‘‘k different sequenc-
(1996). Behavioral variability in SHR and WKY rats as es are produced during the first N trials.’’
a function of rearing environment and reinforcement This event can occur in one of two mutually
contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be-
havior, 65, 129–143. exclusive ways. During the first N 2 1 trials,
Machado, A. (1989). Operant conditioning of behavior- the bird either produced k 2 1 different se-
al variability using a percentile reinforcement sched- quences and then produced a new sequence,
SWITCHING AND VARIABILITY 25

or it produced k different sequences and (256 in the experiments reported here) and
then repeated one of them on the last trial. N $ 1. The boundary conditions of the pre-
The probabilities of these two events are giv- ceding equation are
en by the right side of the following equation: P(1,1) 5 1
M 2 (k 2 1) P(N,k) 5 0 for k 5 0 or k . N.
P(N,k) 5 P(N 2 1,k 2 1)
M Once the probabilities p(N,k) are comput-
k ed recursively, the average and the 95% con-
1 P(N 2 1,k) , fidence interval of the number of different
M
sequences can be obtained (see bottom panel
where M is the total number of sequences of Figure 1).

You might also like