You are on page 1of 17

878509

research-article2019
JTRXXX10.1177/0047287519878509Journal of Travel ResearchLunardo and Ponsignon

Empirical Research Article

Journal of Travel Research

Achieving Immersion in the Tourism


2020, Vol. 59(7) 1151­–1167
© The Author(s) 2019
Article reuse guidelines:
Experience: The Role of Autonomy, sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0047287519878509
https://doi.org/10.1177/0047287519878509

Temporal Dissociation, and Reactance journals.sagepub.com/home/jtr

Renaud Lunardo1 and Frédéric Ponsignon2

Abstract
In the past two decades, research in tourism has explored the shift to the experiential economy in which consumers
(visitors) seek memorable experiences. While prior studies converge toward the idea that immersion is a critical dimension
of the tourism experience, research on immersion remains scarce, particularly the conditions under which immersion is
achieved and its consequences on visitor evaluations. To fill this gap, this research builds on the experiential and flow
literature to propose a model where immersion derives from autonomy and leads to greater visitor satisfaction through
the underlying mechanism of temporal dissociation. Additionally, it is proposed that reactance moderates the relationship
between autonomy and temporal dissociation. Two empirical studies—a field study in an experiential wine museum and an
experiment involving a zoo visit—provide robust evidence of these effects, fostering theoretical progress on immersion and
subsequent temporal dissociation as important dimensions of the tourism experience.

Keywords
tourism experience, immersion, autonomy, temporal dissociation, visitor satisfaction, reactance

Introduction visitors are fully engrossed in the environment and absorbed


in their activities (Chang 2018)—has only recently been the
Following the emergence of the experience economy concept focus of tourism research (Lindberg, Hansen, and Eide 2014;
(Pine and Gilmore 1998), the consumer experience is now Vo Thanh and Kirova 2018). A consequence is that the fac-
widely recognized as the cornerstone of tourism (Tussyadiah tors that may induce feelings of immersion in a tourism set-
2014). The perception of tourists as active and emotional ting, and the mechanisms that explain why an immersive
actors looking for immersive experiences when visiting experience contributes to visitor satisfaction (Hansen and
museums, zoos, theme parks, heritage sites, or art galleries Mossberg 2013), remain unexplored. This lack of research
(Handler and Gable 1997; Hansen and Mossberg 2013) has on the effects of immersion is surprising given that visitor
led tourism managers to pay particular attention to the design satisfaction—likely an outcome of their immersion—drives
of their settings. Considerable resources are dedicated to various desirable behaviors (i.e., repurchasing and repatron-
designing complex, thematic, and refined environments age intention; Szymanski and Henard 2001).
(Bonn et al. 2007; Sherry 1998) that lead to an immersive, To fill this gap, this article builds on the experiential (Pine
enjoyable, and memorable escape from reality for visitors and Gilmore 1998; Carù and Cova 2006) and flow literature
(Morgan, Elbe, and de Esteban Curiel 2009; Otto and Richie streams (Csikszentmihalyi 1990) to propose that the degree
1996; Ritzer 1999). This trend is exemplified by the Burns of autonomy that visitors perceive during a visit explains
Birthplace Museum in Ayrshire, where visitors can put on 3D their propensity to immerse themselves, and their satisfac-
glasses and walk into a digital recreation of the Battle of tion with the experience. Although autonomy—or the
Bannockburn. Similarly, virtual reality and augmented reality freedom to choose or behave as wanted (Levav and Zhu
applications for smartphones increasingly allow immersive
visit of cities, castles, and museums. Another example is the 1
Marketing Department, KEDGE Business School, Talence, France
proliferation of IMAX theatres showcasing 3D immersive 2
Operations Management Department, KEDGE Business School, Talence,
cinematography making visitors feel part of the film, rather France
than just passive observers (Stuart 2006).
Corresponding Author:
Although recognized as a powerful emotional state by Frédéric Ponsignon, Operations Management Department, KEDGE
marketers (Carù and Cova 2007; Novak, Hoffman, and Yung Business School, 680 Cours de la Libération, Talence 33405, France.
2000), such immersion—or the psychological state in which Email: frederic.ponsignon@kedgebs.com
1152 Journal of Travel Research 59(7)

2009)—is a fundamental aspect of the motivation to engage context of a zoo. To provide external validity for our model
in tourism experiences in general (Smith 1994; Li, Chung, and results, our studies were thus performed in two tourism
and Kim 2018), and in museum experiences in particular settings (a wine museum and a zoo) using different research
(Silverman 1995), knowledge of its effects in tourism set- designs (a field study and an experiment), measures (short-
tings remains scarce (Caruana and Crane 2011). Considering ened and full-length scales), and samples (actual visitors vs.
this gap, this research examines whether and how such online participants). In so doing, this research allows a
autonomy can affect immersion and satisfaction. In addition, deeper investigation of the effects of autonomy and strength-
this article investigates the mechanisms that explain why ens the external validity. In addition, the experiment offers a
immersion may lead to an increase in visitor satisfaction. further contribution by showing that reactance moderates the
Although prior studies provide evidence of this direct effect effects of autonomy on the consequence of immersion, tem-
(Cuny, Fornerino, and Helme-Guizon 2015; Fornerino and poral dissociation, and more specifically, that only for highly
Helme-Guizon 2008), the mechanisms that explain how reactant visitors, autonomy leads to greater feelings of losing
immersion drives satisfaction remain unclear. Relying on the track of time.
temporal processing (Block 1990) literature, we propose that
the temporal dissociation phenomenon—that is, “the inabil-
ity to register the passage of time while engaged in interac- Theoretical Background
tion” (Agarwal and Karahanna 2000, p. 673)—explains why
immersion is likely to enhance visitor satisfaction.
Tourism Experience and Immersion
Further, we address the question of whether all visitors The emergence of the experience economy (Pine and
respond positively to the level of perceived autonomy. This Gilmore 1998) has triggered a shift in the marketing disci-
question is important since tourism providers sometimes pline, leading to a plethora of research dedicated to under-
struggle to find the right level of autonomy they should grant standing consumer experiences (e.g., Carù and Cova 2006;
visitors. For instance, Ponsignon, Durrieu, and Bouzdine- Holbrook 2006a, 2006b, 2006c; Gentile, Spiller, and Noci
Chameeva (2017) report that the designers of an experiential 2007). This shift has been beneficial to the tourism disci-
cultural organization gave visitors complete autonomy to pline, with more research specifically focused on how to
explore a 3,000-m2 space in any way they liked. While the design and manage the tourist experience (Quan and Wang
majority of visitors reported enjoying the freedom inherent 2004; Tussyadiah 2014; Yuan and Wu 2008). Prior studies
in the experience, some declared their unease and a frustrat- suggest that consumers value extraordinary and memorable
ing (e.g., “I fear I may have missed something interesting”) experiences and that tourism providers increasingly focus on
or confusing experience (e.g., “I do not know if I visited the the provision of such experiences (Oh, Fiore, and Jeoung
various thematic areas in the right order”). Research is there- 2007; Hosany and Witham 2010).
fore needed to provide a better understanding of the individ- The concept of immersion is central to the creation and
ual factors that can explain why visitors react either positively consumption of memorable tourism experiences (Lindberg
or negatively to autonomy, and the impact of autonomy on and Østergaard 2015; Carù and Cova 2006). When the envi-
the visitor experience. In this research, we propose reac- ronment is designed in such a way that visitors are encour-
tance—that is, a motivational state experienced in response aged to physically (e.g., touching, smelling, walking) or
to threatened freedom (Brehm and Brehm 1981; Wicklund mentally (i.e., thinking or watching) connect with a given
1974)—as such a variable. Building on reactance theory event or performance, they are able to immerse themselves in
(Brehm 1966), we posit that visitors differ in their responses the experience and become “physically (or virtually) a part of
to autonomy, with greater temporal dissociation emerging as the experience itself” (Pine and Gilmore 1999, p. 31). For
a response to autonomy only when people exhibit a high instance, McIntyre (2009, p. 165) shows that museums and
level of reactance. art galleries providing a mix of space types (e.g., contempla-
In what follows, we present a series of hypotheses tive and participative) enable visitors to immerse themselves
addressing the effect of immersion on visitor satisfaction that in a meaningful experience of learning and discovering. More
we test in two studies. The first study, performed in a leading generally, tourism settings that make visitors feel engrossed
experiential wine museum, shows that the higher the degree and deeply engaged with the activities at hand captivate visi-
of autonomy that visitors perceive, the greater the feelings of tors and are consensually viewed as highly immersive
immersion and satisfaction. Importantly, this study also iden- (Hosany and Witham 2010). The extent to which tourists or
tifies temporal dissociation as the underlying mechanism visitors feel immersed in the experience is particularly impor-
that explains why immersion increases satisfaction. Our sec- tant since immersive experiences tend to be more memorable
ond study replicates these findings in another context using than ordinary experiences (Tung and Ritchie 2011).
an experimental design. Drawing on Winer (1999, p. 350), Immersion thus represents a process whereby the indi-
who states that external validity involves “generalizing the vidual disconnects from the real world, a disconnection that
results to other settings and population beyond the current is either a sudden dive or a more gradual envelopment in the
research setting,” the second study was conducted in the experiential environment (Fornerino and Helme-Guizon
Lunardo and Ponsignon 1153

2008). In addition to this process-oriented perspective, Visitor autonomy is an important consideration in the
immersion also refers to a state characterized as “an intense design of tourism experiences (Caruana and Crane 2011). As
moment experienced by the consumer and the result of a par- tourists are often presented with a range of options, the expe-
tial or complete process of appropriation” (Carù and Cova rience of exploring a tourism site involves a series of deci-
2006, p. 60). Immersed consumers have all their senses fully sions of where to go and what to do or see next (Smith 1994).
engaged in the activity performed (Agarwal and Karahanna Reisinger (2013) notes that tourists tend to relish the oppor-
2000). They are described as detached and highly concen- tunity to choose what to do and how to do it by themselves.
trated on the activity at hand that becomes the focal point of Similarly, Bianchi (2016) shows that freedom of choice is a
their thoughts and feelings (Brown and Cairns 2004). key driver of satisfaction for the solo tourist. This effect is
Immersion is thus related but also conceptually distinct from due to the increased level of control that autonomous indi-
the flow construct. Flow considers a broad set of psychologi- viduals can exert over the activities performed to achieve
cal states as it includes affective and cognitive dimensions. their goals (Deci and Ryan 2000). In high-autonomy situa-
In particular, enjoyment is widely seen as a core dimension tions, visitors perceive a great sense of control over the situ-
of flow experiences (Drengner, Steffen, and Furchheim ation, as they make their own decisions to participate or not
2018). The focus of immersion is, however, centered on a in any given activity. Carù and Cova (2006) explain that con-
person’s concentration on the task at hand (i.e., a cognitive sumers who perceive a sufficient degree of control are able to
dimension). The immersion concept is hence more focused. familiarize themselves with the environment in which the
experience takes place. According to these authors, this
familiarity is a prerequisite for consumers to become
How Autonomy Can Prompt Immersion: An immersed. Drawing on human–computer interactions
Appropriation Perspective research, Ghani and Deshpande (1994) provide empirical
Since immersion occurs when the distance is minimal support for the notion that perceived autonomy influences
between the consumer and the context in which the experi- feelings of immersion. Their study shows that videogame
ence takes place (Carù and Cova 2006), the concept of dis- players are more likely to be captivated by games that give
tance is central in the immersion process. One way to reduce them full control over their actions.
distance and facilitate immersion is an “operation of appro- In a tourism context, autonomous visitors may familiarize
priation” that Carù and Cova (2006, p. 6) define as “the mark themselves with the range of activities or attractions avail-
of a fundamental psychological system of action on the con- able, which may put them in a position to select the ones they
text of experience in order to transform and personalize it.” feel most attracted to based on personal relevance or prefer-
More specifically, three major appropriation operations are ence. Such visitors are also able to spend the desired amount
often distinguished (Fischer and Atkin-Etienne 1997; of time in the attraction. The empirical study of Minkiewicz,
Mifsud, Cases, and N’Goala 2015). The first is “nesting,” a Evans, and Bridson (2013) conducted in museums supports
process whereby visitors try to control the place, such that this view, showing that when visitors are given the opportu-
they can feel at home. The second is “investigating,” an oper- nity to personalize their visit, they are able to immerse them-
ation during which visitors try to explore and identify new selves in the experience. We therefore posit that visitors are
products or activities to develop points of anchorage and more likely to focus intensely on an experience that they
control (signposts). By investigating, visitors aim to enhance have purposefully chosen. In contrast, visitors with limited
their knowledge of the experience setting and extend their autonomy may not invest sufficient time and effort to dive
territory. The third and last appropriation operation is “stamp- straight and fully into an experience that is imposed on them.
ing,” describing a meaning-making process whereby visitors Rather, they may be more easily distracted by other features
attribute a specific and personal meaning to an experience. of the environment, which may inhibit their ability to con-
In this set of operations that aim to facilitate the appro- centrate on the experience. As freedom may be needed for
priation of—and subsequent immersion in—the experience, visitors to engage in operations of appropriation, which facil-
consumers need a certain degree of freedom. In other words, itates immersion, we propose the following hypothesis:
since appropriation refers to the consumers’ ability to make
the place their own, it may require a certain degree of auton- Hypothesis 1: The greater the degree of autonomy per-
omy. Consumers need the freedom to choose how to behave ceived by the visitor, the greater the feeling of immersion.
in the focal environment in order to be able to act on that
environment, to personalize it, and make it feel like home. Immersion as a Disconnection That Contributes
Such freedom is consensually labeled as autonomy (e.g.,
Levav and Zhu 2009; Markus and Schwartz 2010). As the
to the Experience
belief that one can freely choose how to behave in a specific When immersing themselves in an event, performance, or
situation (Deci and Ryan 1985; White 1959), autonomy place, individuals disconnect from the real world (Carù and
refers to the extent to which people feel free to initiate their Cova 2006). Hence, visitors who are deeply immersed in an
own actions (Deci and Ryan 2000). experiential environment may forget about—and even lose
1154 Journal of Travel Research 59(7)

consciousness of—the real world, focusing on the experi- supports this reasoning. Additional theoretical evidence
ence taking place (Fornerino and Helme-Guizon 2008). Such from consumer culture research highlights the consumer’s
loss of consciousness is a key element in Csikszentmihalyi’s quest to experience immersion in thematic settings, rather
(1990) description of the flow experience where the autotelic than merely focusing on the quality and attributes of fin-
nature of the activity results in individuals losing track of ished products (Firat and Venkatesh 1995). However, the
time. A time distortion phenomenon might thus occur when question remains as to how visitors respond to the tempo-
people are immersed and lose consciousness of the external ral dissociation phenomenon that follows immersion, and
world. Such distortion refers to the notion of temporal dis- more specifically, whether such temporal dissociation is
sociation, or “the inability to register the passage of time beneficial to satisfaction. One answer can be drawn from
while engaged in interaction” (Agarwal and Karahanna the temporal processing literature and attentional models.
2000, p. 673). In the field of time perception in service settings, some
It is important to note that immersion is conceptually dis- authors—such as Lopez and Malhotra (1991) and Cameron
tinct from the temporal dissociation construct. Immersion et al. (2003)—find that respondents’ time estimates are
specifically relates to an attention state where the target is the negatively correlated with their reported liking of the ser-
activity (i.e., the person’s attention is totally focused on the vice setting. This suggests that when time is perceived as
activity). Temporal dissociation only relates to a person’s longer, satisfaction decreases. Therefore, by reducing the
time consciousness, that is, the (in)ability to register the pas- time perception, temporal dissociation may increase the
sage of time. Hence, the targets of the two concepts are dif- liking of the experience, or more broadly, visitor satisfac-
ferent: immersion is focused on an activity and temporal tion. We thus propose the following hypothesis:
dissociation relates to time consciousness, a lack of aware-
ness of time passing by. These two constructs have been Hypothesis 4: The greater the degree of temporal dissoci-
identified as uncorrelated in prior research, for instance, by ation perceived by the visitor, the greater the level of visi-
Agarwal and Karahanna (2000). tor satisfaction.
Previous research on temporal processing provides theo- Hypothesis 5: Immersion and temporal dissociation will
retical support for the notion of immersion as a source of serially mediate the indirect effect of autonomy on visitor
temporal dissociation. As visitors become immersed, they satisfaction.
focus entirely on the activity being performed. Their atten-
tion may therefore be directed toward some nontemporal The subsequent study is designed to test these five
processing, which involves thinking about things that are hypotheses.
unrelated to the passage of time during the target interval
(Bailey and Areni 2006). A decrease in attention to temporal
Study 1: The Field Study
information results in less encoded information on the pas-
sage of time (Block 1990), and as less temporal information The goal of study 1 was to test hypotheses 1−3 in the real
is encoded, the subjective experience of the interval reduces world, and thus provide evidence for the notion that visitor
and estimates of duration decrease. Therefore, as they immersion and temporal dissociation serially mediate the
become immersed, visitors may estimate the time as less effects of autonomy on satisfaction.
than the time actually spent during their immersion. They The field study is conducted in an experience-centric
may thus experience a phenomenon of temporal dissociation. wine museum. Research on museums has a long tradition in
Hence, we posit: the tourism literature. Scholars have explored a diversity of
phenomena, such as the museum image formation process
Hypothesis 2: The greater the degree of immersion per- (Moreno and Ritchie 2009), the impact of a museum on the
ceived by the visitor, the greater the feeling of temporal attractiveness of an area (Brida, Meleddu, and Pulina 2012),
dissociation. and their role as dark tourism sites (Cohen 2011). While a
Hypothesis 3: Immersion will positively mediate the indi- curatorial orientation focused on visitor information and
rect effect of visitor autonomy on temporal dissociation. object preservation is notable in early research endeavors
(Uriely 2005), a shift toward the visitor experience has been
Since immersion is often described as a key component of observed over the past three decades (Falk and Dierking
flow-type or optimal experiences (Csikszentmihalyi 1997), 2016). Accordingly, research focused on understanding visi-
and as flow refers to a state of particular enjoyment, there tor characteristics and expectations, and examining the
is reason to believe that immersion generates increased dimensions of the visitor experience (Antón, Camarero, and
satisfaction for visitors. Research showing that enjoyment Garrido 2018; McIntyre 2009; Rowley 1999) has grown sig-
can result from participating in immersive activities or nificantly (Sheng and Chen 2012). The present study thus
processes that offer a sense of escape from everyday falls within the realm of tourism research examining the con-
monotony (Mathwick, Malhotra, and Rigdon 2001) ditions under which visitors have a successful experience.
Lunardo and Ponsignon 1155

Procedure and Sample was included up-front to avoid any common method vari-
ance bias (Podsakoff et al. 2003). Then, visitors completed a
We selected as the setting for our field study a recently three-item measure of temporal dissociation adapted from
opened experience-centric wine museum (the ‘Permanent Agarwal and Karahanna (2000): “Time appeared to go by
Tour’ of La Cité du Vin1) that offers visitors a unique and very quickly,” “I lost track of time,” and “Time flew”; α =
multisensory adventure. The Permanent Tour welcomed .70. Visitors then rated their feelings of immersion using a
445,000 paying visitors in 2017, which makes it the most two-item scale adapted from Agarwal and Karahanna (2000):
popular tourist attraction in southwestern France. The expe- “I was able to block out most other distractions” and “I was
riential museum is situated in an open space consisting of 20 absorbed in what I am doing”; α = .80. Visitors then rated
individual themed areas that address a variety of wine-related how much autonomy they perceived having in the museum
topics. These modules collectively provide a good balance of on a two-item scale adapted from Song and Zinkhan (2008)
physical (e.g., pressing pear-shaped atomizers to release to fit the context of the current study: “I was able to choose
wine-related aromas) and mental activities (e.g., watching to which area of the museum to go” and “I could choose
diverse vine landscapes filmed from a helicopter on extra- freely what area I wanted to explore”; α = .73. All the state-
large mural screens) along the visitor journey. The museum ments were rated on Likert-type scales (1 = strongly dis-
setting is enclaved, thematic, and secure for visitors, making agree, 10 = strongly agree). The 1-10 scale was chosen by
it conducive to immersive experiences (Carù and Cova Cité du Vin’s management team.
2007), and therefore appropriate for this study (see Appendix To test for the convergent validity of the scales, we used
1 for a map of the permanent tour). As they needed assistance the procedure of Hair et al. (2005) where the variance
in designing and implementing a visitor satisfaction study, extracted from among a set of construct items must be higher
the Cité du Vin management team contacted the authors. than 0.5. All the multi-item scales showed convergent valid-
The field study took place over three consecutive days in ity (Table 1).
February 2018. Visitors were asked at the entrance of the To test the discriminant validity among the constructs, we
museum if they wanted to participate in a study about their used the procedure that Fornell and Larcker (1981) suggest.
experience, and those who agreed were invited to wear a In spite of some significant correlations among the con-
wristband so that the researchers would know exactly whom structs, the average variance extracted for each was higher
to interview once visitors exited the museum space. They than the squared correlation between these constructs and
were also asked to go to the top floor of the museum at the any other construct, supporting the discriminant validity of
end of the visit to respond to a questionnaire. Four students the measures (Table 2).
were hired, trained, and remunerated to facilitate the admin-
istration of the questionnaire, as attendance data suggested
that many customers visit the museum at the same time and Results
for a similar duration, leading to large numbers of visitors to To test hypotheses 1–3, we performed a mediation analysis
interview at the same time. Hence, six people—researchers using the Process macro and 5,000 bootstrap samples. Here,
included—administered the surveys using digital tablets to autonomy served as the independent variable, immersion as
minimize the risk of data entry and processing errors. In the mediating variable, and temporal dissociation as the
total, 394 customers agreed to participate and completed the dependent variable. The results show a significant indirect
survey. Data from customers under the age of 18 years were effect of autonomy on temporal dissociation (β = .07, 95%
removed. The final sample was thus composed of 384 visi- CI = .039, .114). More specifically, autonomy has a positive
tors. Regarding the sociodemographic characteristics, 204 effect on immersion (β = .25, p < .001), which then increases
visitors were women (53%), and the mean age was 47.15 temporal dissociation (β = .29, p < .001). Of note, auton-
(SD =15.85). The vast majority (90.6%) reported being first- omy has a direct positive effect on temporal dissociation (β
time visitors. = .11, p < .01; Figure 1). Overall, these results support
hypotheses 1–3, and the notion that immersion mediates the
effect of visitor autonomy on temporal dissociation.
Measures
Turning now to hypotheses 4 and 5, we performed a serial
All the measures used in the questionnaire were drawn from mediation (Process Macro, 5,000 bootstrap samples). Here,
prior research. Nevertheless, as the executives at the museum autonomy served as the independent variable, immersion and
explicitly asked that visitors not be bothered by a long ques- temporal dissociation as the mediating variables, and satis-
tionnaire, several multi-item scales were shortened. More faction as the dependent variable. The results show signifi-
specifically, visitors first rated their satisfaction with the visit cant serial mediation (β = .12, 95% CI = .004, .022), thus
using a three-item scale adapted from Machleit, Kellaris, and supporting the prediction that immersion and temporal dis-
Eroglu (1994): “I was satisfied with my experience in the sociation serially mediate the effect of autonomy on satisfac-
museum,” “I enjoyed visiting that museum,” and “Visiting tion. In this serial mediation, both immersion (β = .47, p <
that museum made me feel happy”; α = .85. This measure .001) and temporal dissociation (β = .15, p < .001) have a
1156 Journal of Travel Research 59(7)

Table 1.  The Measures Used in the Studies and Their Psychometric Properties (Studies 1 and 2).

Study 1 (Wine Museum) Study 2 (Zoo)

Measures Loadings Reliability Loadings Reliability


Satisfaction with the visit
  I would probably be (was) satisfied with my visit of the zoo/museum .871 .850 .944 .945
  I would probably enjoy(ed) visiting that zoo/museum .868 .961  
  Visiting that zoo/museum would make (made) me feel happy – .942  

Temporal dissociation
  Time would appear(ed) to go by very quickly .801 .702 .883 .875
  I would lose (lost) track of time .846 .875
  Time would fly (flew) .731 .916
  I would end up spending more time than I had planned – .755  
   
Immersion
  I would be (was) able to block out most other distractions .921 .807 .862 .807
  I would be (was) absorbed in what I am doing .917 .832  
  My attention would (did) not get diverted very easily – .861  

Autonomy
  I would be (was) able to choose which area of the zoo to go to .858 .728 .959 . 951
  I could freely choose what area I want to explore .854 .975
  I would have (had) absolute full control over where I could go – .930
   
Reactance
  I resist the attempts of others to influence me – – .696 .773
  It makes me angry when another person is held up as a model for – .714  
me to follow
  I become frustrated when I am unable to make free and – .841  
independent decisions
  I become angry when my freedom of choice is restricted – .829  

Note: The items slightly differed across studies 1 and 2, as they were adapted to fit the museum and zoo settings.

Table 2.  Squared Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations (Studies 1 and 2).

Mean
(SD) 1 2 3 4 5
Study 1 (Wine museum)  
  Satisfaction (1) 8.57 (1.28) .870 .609** .599** .285** –
  Temporal dissoc. (2) 7.79 (1.63) .165 .630 .439** .274** –
  Immersion (3) 8.44 (1.41) .358 .192 .840 .349** –
  Autonomy (4) 7.95 (1.97) .081 .075 .121 .729 –
   
Study 2 (Zoo)  
  Satisfaction (1) 5.32 (1.26) .900 .407** .419** .240** –.077
  Temporal dissoc. (2) 5.22 (1.10) .165 .738 .391** .146** .045
  Immersion (3) 4.88 (1.00) .175 .171 .725 .234** –.088
  Autonomy (4) 4.53 (1.78) .057 .075 .054 .911 .074
  Reactance (5) 4.67 (1.06) .005 .002 .007 .005 .597

Note: The squared correlations among constructs appear below the diagonal; the correlations appear above the diagonal. The average variance
extracted (AVE) for each construct is on the diagonal. Constructs are measured on a 10-point Likert-type scale in study 1, while 7-point measures are
used in study 2.
*
p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
Lunardo and Ponsignon 1157

βStudy1 = .29*
βStudy2 = .41***

Temporal
Immersion
βStudy1 = .25*** dissociaon βStudy1 = .15***
βStudy2 = .31*** βStudy2 = .60***

βStudy1 = .11** βStudy1 = .47***


βStudy2 = .19† βStudy2 = .27***

Autonomy Sasfacon

Figure 1.  The mediating effects of immersion and temporal dissociations in the autonomy–satisfaction relationship (studies 1 and 2).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, †p < .10.

positive effect on satisfaction. Autonomy has no significant alternatives such that whenever an alternative is threatened
effect (p > .10). Overall, these results support hypotheses 4 or eliminated, reactance occurs, and the motivation to pursue
and 5. the restricted freedom increases dramatically (Brehm 1966).
As research supports the notion that individuals value their
freedom of choice (Fitzsimons 2000; Moore and Fitzsimons
Discussion 2014), one could argue that such reactance is a common trait
Study 1 provides support for the notion that visitor immer- observed at a high level in every individual. However,
sion and temporal dissociation serially mediate the posi- research does not support this view, and rather shows that
tive effect of autonomy on satisfaction. Put differently, individuals vary in trait reactance, a personality trait that
visitors who feel they can freely choose how to behave and defines how much people value their freedom of choice and
live their experience reported greater feelings of immer- react negatively when such freedom is threatened. Research
sion than visitors who felt less autonomous, and because on trait reactance shows a positive association between
they were more immersed in the experience, they exhibited autonomy and trait reactance, where highly reactant individ-
greater loss of track of time and their satisfaction in turn uals routinely rebel against authority figures (Dowd et al.
increased. 1994). Such highly reactant individuals (vs. less reactant
Although we identify autonomy as an antecedent of individuals) are particularly motivated to re-establish the
immersion, our first study did not address the question of threatened freedom (Brehm 1966; Wicklund 1974).
whether all visitors react the same way to such autonomy. Hence, we suggest that in the context of experiential tour-
Extant theory posits that this may not be the case, as different ism settings, visitors who are highly reactant—and thus who
people seek and enjoy different kinds of experiences (Cohen value being able to visit a place or site autonomously—may
1979). This proposition has garnered empirical support in the exhibit stronger positive responses than those who do not
literature. In particular, in their qualitative study, Uriely, place such weight on autonomy. One potential response of
Yonay, and Simchai (2002) found that backpackers consti- interest for this research lies in temporal dissociation. This
tute a highly heterogeneous tourist population in terms of proposition builds on time perception research showing that
psychological attributes, such as their travel motivations and when people experience states of positive feelings, they tend
their perceptions of the travel experience. Similarly, Bianchi to underestimate how much time has elapsed. For instance,
(2016) suggests that researchers take into account personal people with arachnophobia reported longer estimates of a
factors or traits to explore and understand differences in indi- short interval spent observing a spider compared to those
vidual perceptions of the tourism experience. Since auton- who did not report such phobia (Watts and Sharrock 1984).
omy refers to the ability to choose how to behave (White Another experiment shows that people who make eye con-
1959; Deci and Ryan 2000), visitors—and more broadly tact with a person with a negative facial expression report
individuals—who value such freedom may react more posi- longer time estimates than those who make eye contact with
tively to autonomy. In this regard, research shows that one a person with a positive facial expression (Thayer and Schiff
particular construct tackles this value of freedom of choice: 1975). Considering these results showing that time estimates
reactance. Brehm (1966) defines reactance as a motivational are lower when people feel positive states, we propose that
state to reassert lost freedom of choice. Reactance theory the autonomy given to visitors with a high (vs. low) trait
explains human behavior in response to the perceived loss of reactance leads them to underestimate the time spent in the
freedom in an environment, and more specifically, assumes tourist setting due to the positive state that results from their
that individuals cherish their ability to choose from among autonomy. In other words, as such visitors may particularly
1158 Journal of Travel Research 59(7)

Figure 2.  The theoretical model.


Note: Full lines represent relationships that are hypothesized and tested; dotted lines represent relationships that are not hypothesized but nevertheless
tested through the mediation analyses.

value their autonomy, their positive feelings may lead to Cheng, and Hong 2017). The present study is positioned in
greater temporal dissociation. Hence, we propose: this visitor and experience-centric perspective.

Hypothesis 6: Customer reactance will moderate the Participants, Design, and Procedure
effect of autonomy on temporal dissociation. More spe-
cifically, for highly reactant customers, autonomy will A panelist recruited 375 US individuals (55.5% female,
lead to higher levels of temporal dissociation than for less MAge = 42.88, SD = 9.62, ranging from 28 to 65) online to
reactant customers. participate in this study. In terms of occupation, 57.6% are
employed, 16.8% are self-employed, 10.9% are unemployed,
The addition of this hypothesis leads to the following and 4.8% are retired. Students and other occupations repre-
complete theoretical model (Figure 2). sent 9.3% of the sample. Statistics provided by the US
Association of Zoos & Aquariums show that visitors are pri-
marily women/mothers, aged 25–35 years,1 suggesting that
our sample is reasonably representative of the target popula-
Study 2: The Zoo Experiment tion, and thus suitable for the study. To test the notion that
Beyond replicating the results observed in study 1, study 2 autonomy leads to increasing immersion and consequently
aims to examine whether all visitors react the same way to satisfaction, we manipulated autonomy as a between-sub-
autonomy. More specifically, study 2 explores reactance as jects factor (low vs. high). In both the low and high auton-
an individual trait able to explain when customers benefit omy conditions, the respondents were asked to read a
from autonomy. scenario about visiting a zoo, to project themselves in the
This experimental study is conducted in a zoo setting. situation described by the scenario, and to carefully inspect a
Zoos make an important contribution to the development of map of the zoo. However, as in previous studies (Chen and
the tourism sector by simultaneously playing the role of Sengupta 2014; Zhang et al. 2011), in the low-autonomy
tourism attractions and conservation organizations (Driml, condition the scenario constrained individual choice, as par-
Ballantyne, and Packer 2017). Tourism research on zoos ticipants were told they had to follow a compulsory path that
has explored a range of phenomena including the character- they could not deviate from (see Appendix 2 for the maps
istics of on-site and off-site conservation activities (Turley and scenarios). The respondents were presented with a map
1999), the financial impact of a new attraction (Driml, showing this compulsory path with green dots. Importantly,
Ballantyne, and Packer 2017), and the mission of zoos as to avoid any confounding effects, the compulsory path
sustainable eco-tourism destinations. Previous visitor-ori- included the most popular animals, exactly the same animals
ented research has focused on exploring visitor motivations as in the high-autonomy condition. In this latter condition,
(Klenosky and Saunders 2007), visitor preferences, and the scenario did not constrain individual choice, and no men-
animal attractiveness (Carr 2016). Recently, an experience- tion was made of any compulsory path either in the scenario
centric approach has begun to explore the dimensions of the or the map.
visitor experience, examining the relationship between After reading the scenario and inspecting the map, partici-
experience quality, visitor satisfaction, and loyalty (Wu, pants were asked to complete a questionnaire. We set a
Lunardo and Ponsignon 1159

quality check as a means to ensure the validity of the data. the independent samples. Manipulation of autonomy served
We formulated and inserted in the questionnaire an attention as the independent variable and the measure of visitor immer-
filter (addressing common method bias) as “if you read this sion as the dependent variable. The results show a significant
please check “strongly disagree.” Responses that did not difference of immersion across the conditions of autonomy,
meet this specification (n = 29) were removed from the data- the latter variable rated higher when autonomy is high
set. The measures were the same as in study 1, but adapted to (M = 5.04) compared to low (M = 4.73, t = −3.06, p < .01).
the context of this study (see Table 1). Also, unlike in study 1 This result supports hypothesis 1.
where some items had to be dropped, study 2 employs the Then, we performed a mediation analysis to test hypoth-
complete measurement scales. Specifically, and as in study eses 2 and 3 using the Process macro (model 4, 5,000 boot-
1, participants first rated their satisfaction with the visit using strap samples). Here, manipulation of autonomy served as
the same three-item scale (α = .94). They then completed the the independent variable, immersion as the mediating vari-
same measure of temporal dissociation used in study 1, but able, and temporal dissociation as the dependent variable. As
here using four items (α = .87). They then rated their immer- in study 1, the results show a significant indirect effect of
sion, using the three-item scale of Agarwal and Karahanna autonomy on temporal dissociation (95% CI = .049, .230).
(2000; α = .80). For manipulation check purposes, partici- More specifically, autonomy has a positive effect on immer-
pants rated how much autonomy they would perceive having sion (β = .31, p < .01), which then increases temporal dis-
in the zoo using three items from Song and Zinkham (2008) sociation (β = .41, p < .001). Hypotheses 2 and 3 are thus
(α = .95). All the statements were rated on Likert-type scales supported.
(1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Turning to the serial mediation, we followed the same
Finally, to test the predicted moderating role of trait reac- procedure as in study 1, but here with manipulation of auton-
tance, participants completed the Hong and Faedda (1996) omy as the independent variable. Again, a significant serial
reactance scale. The factorial analysis revealed the scale as mediation effect is found (95% CI = .030, .145), whereby
bidimensional, with two eigenvalues greater than 1 (Hair the aforementioned positive effects of autonomy and immer-
et al. 2005). After a close look at the factorial structure and sion are followed by a positive effect of temporal dissocia-
the items loading on each dimension, it appeared that the first tion on satisfaction (β = .60, p < .001). Of note, autonomy
dimension—composed of the four items “I resist the attempts only has a marginal effect on temporal dissociation (β = .19,
of others to influence me,” “It makes me angry when another p = .07), and no direct effect on satisfaction (β = –.00, p <
person is held up as a model for me to follow,” “I become .01). Immersion has a positive effect on satisfaction (β =
frustrated when I am unable to make free and independent .27, p < .001). Overall, these results support hypotheses 4
decisions,” and “I become angry when my freedom of choice and 5 and the notion that immersion and temporal dissocia-
is restricted”—was the most closely related to reactance and tion mediate the effect of visitor autonomy on satisfaction.
its essential notion of choice restriction. This four-item scale The last analysis pertained to the predicted moderating
proved reliable (α = .77; 7-point scale from “not at all” to role of reactance in the effect of autonomy on temporal dis-
“extremely”) and was thus retained for further analyses. sociation. To avoid any loss of statistical power, we chose
Here again, all the multi-item scales exhibited convergent not to dichotomize the reactance measure (Fitzsimons 2008)
and discriminant validity (Tables 1 and 2). and performed a floodlight analysis (Spiller et al. 2013)
using the Process macro and 5,000 bootstrap samples. Here,
the autonomy condition served as the independent variable,
Manipulation Check
temporal dissociation as the dependent variable, and the
To ensure that the manipulation of autonomy was success- measure of reactance as moderator. The results show no main
ful, we conducted a t-test for the independent samples with effects of autonomy and reactance (all p’s > .10), but as
the autonomy treatment condition as the independent vari- expected, their interaction is significant (β = .21, p < .05).
able and the measure of autonomy as the dependent vari- We plot this interaction in Figure 3, using the Johnson-
able. As expected, the result shows a significant difference Neyman technique for identifying regions in the range of the
of autonomy across conditions, with perceived autonomy moderator variable in which the effect of the independent
rated higher in the high-autonomy condition (M = 5.80) variable on the dependent variable is and is not significant
than in the low-autonomy condition (M = 3.31, t = −18.93, (Hayes and Matthes 2009). The Johnson-Neyman point for
p < .001). the reactance moderator occurs at the value 4.31. This value
indicates that a high level of autonomy results in signifi-
cantly higher levels of temporal dissociation for values of
Results
reactance greater than 4.31. These results thus support
Turning to the test of the notions that autonomy affects Hypothesis 6 and the notion that reactance moderates the
immersion and that immersion mediates the effect of auton- effect of autonomy on temporal dissociation, with highly
omy on temporal dissociation, we performed two analyses. reactant customer experiencing greater temporal dissociation
The first analysis tested hypothesis 1 and involved a t-test for when autonomous (vs. not).
1160 Journal of Travel Research 59(7)

Figure 3.  The moderating role of reactance in the autonomy–temporal dissociation relationship.

Discussion relates to extending the still-scarce literature on immersion


by demonstrating that a way to help visitors achieve an
Study 2 replicates the results of study 1 showing that auton- immersive state lies in autonomy. Immersion is shown to
omy leads to higher visitor satisfaction, since being autono- result from the visitor’s interaction with an enclosed, safe,
mous leads customers to more fully immerse and consequently thematic experiential context (Carù and Cova 2006), but its
lose track of time to a greater extent. Moreover, and of emergence as a response to autonomy remained unidentified.
importance, study 2 identifies reactance as an individual trait The identification of immersion as a result of high levels of
that explains why visitors react positively or negatively to autonomy is important for two reasons. First, immersive
autonomy. More specifically, our results show that when states are consensually seen as a required condition for
visitors are highly reactant—and thus do not appreciate hav- enjoyment and experiential value (Chung et al. 2017). We
ing their freedom constrained—being autonomous has a provide an explanation for why autonomy can help visitors
positive effect and leads to strong levels of temporal disso- derive higher value from their tourism experiences: provid-
ciation, while not being autonomous leads to lower percep- ing visitors with greater freedom of choice is beneficial to
tions of losing track of time. engaging in immersive states, which is helpful to better
understand the emergence of visitor value. Second, auton-
General Conclusion omy has been shown to exert positive effects on motivation
and learning (Levesque et al. 2004), but its ability to help
This research examines how the degree of perceived auton- visitors immerse themselves in the experience remained
omy facilitates visitor immersion in the experience, and how unexamined. Our research thus contributes by identifying
such immersion can affect their satisfaction with the visit. immersion as a novel outcome of autonomy.
Through a field study and an experiment, we first demon- Second, turning to temporal dissociation as an effect of
strate that visitors are more immersed and exhibit more satis- immersion, our research identifies trait reactance as a vari-
faction when they can experience their visit autonomously. able that helps distinguish between visitors who exhibit
Importantly, we identify temporal dissociation as the under- greater or lesser temporal dissociation as a reaction to their
lying mechanism that explains why being more immersed in freedom. This result helps to reconcile previous inconsistent
a tourism activity can lead to an increase in satisfaction. In findings in the literature on museum experiences, which sug-
other words, as immersion makes visitors lose track of time, gests either directing visitors and referring them to key activ-
such immersion fosters greater satisfaction in their experi- ities and exhibits (Goulding 2000), or enabling visitors to
ence. Further, we demonstrate the moderating role of trait create their own personal journeys (Minkiewicz, Evans, and
reactance in the effects of autonomy on immersion, whereby Bridson 2013). Our study shows under what conditions it is
highly reactant visitors experience more temporal dissocia- appropriate to provide or restrict the visitors’ freedom of
tion when they can visit the tourism setting autonomously. choice. Specifically, the results show that visitors who are
high in reactance—and thus enjoy being able to choose how
to behave—exhibit greater temporal dissociation as a
Theoretical Implications response to their autonomy than visitors who are low in reac-
The results we obtained in two different settings offer several tance. This latter group may be subject to the “tyranny of
important theoretical contributions. A first contribution choice” syndrome, also referred to as choice overload,
Lunardo and Ponsignon 1161

describing situations where freedom of choice is seen as a comes to freedom of choice. Visitors who embrace freedom
form of tyranny (Schwartz 2000): having to choose from relish the opportunity to create and personalize their own
among a variety of options is a demotivating factor for cer- experiences, which facilitates the achievement of immersive
tain individuals (Iyengar and Lepper 2002) who prefer hav- and fast-flying tourism experiences. On the contrary, visitors
ing their freedom of choice constrained. Reactance has been who do not like to make free and independent decisions find
shown to moderate a wide array of behaviors—from goal it difficult to immerse themselves in the experience and do
pursuit (Chartrand, Dalton, and Fitzsimons 2007) to choice not lose track of time. Such individuals are likely to value
of unhealthy food products (Fitzsimons and Lehmann recommendations and suggestions that help them explore a
2004)—but its identification as a moderating variable of the given site or destination. Following research demonstrating
effects of autonomy is new, showing that autonomy is bene- that individuals whose autonomy is threatened attempt to
ficial only for visitors who are highly reactant. restore their freedom, one important general recommenda-
In addition, this research contributes to the wide tourism tion for tourism sites lies in refraining from providing man-
literature that focuses on how visitor satisfaction is formed datory paths or routes, and allowing visitors to behave freely
(e.g., Heung and Cheng 2000; Hui, Wan, and Ho 2007). In within the site or attraction. Giving visitors full autonomy
the current work, we show that satisfaction is not directly over their experience should be the default configuration to
affected by immersion, but indirectly. More precisely, visitor minimize the risk of alienating highly reactant visitors.
satisfaction results from the feelings of immersion induced Moreover, we propose a straightforward, practical, and reli-
by autonomy, through the mediating effect of temporal dis- able way to identify visitors exhibiting different degrees of
sociation. By identifying this serial mediation linking auton- reactance. We recommend that managers of tourist attrac-
omy, immersion, temporal dissociation, and satisfaction, our tions ask visitors a single question to assess the extent to
research explains the emergence of satisfaction and the which they value and embrace freedom in general and, con-
sequence whereby autonomy can lead visitors to become sat- sequently, guide them toward the right kind of experience.
isfied with their experience. Furthermore, this sequence Such question, based on an item exhibiting high facial valid-
highlights the mediating role played by temporal dissocia- ity within the reactance measurement scale, could be formu-
tion in the relationship between immersion and satisfaction. lated as follows: “I dislike being told what to see or where to
The current research thus contributes to the growing litera- go when I visit a tourism destination/attraction.” Visitors
ture on immersion in tourism settings (e.g., Hosany and could be asked this question directly by an operator or indi-
Witham 2010; Oh, Fiore, and Jeoung 2007; Rasmi et al. rectly (e.g., online or self-service machine) when requesting
2014) by showing that temporal dissociation explains the information, booking places, or buying tickets. Based on the
positive effect of immersion on visitor satisfaction. response obtained, low reactant visitors would be offered the
opportunity to take a guided, directed, or recommended tour.
In this case, visitors would accept giving up some freedom if
Implications for Tourism Managers they have confidence in the attraction’s capacity to provide
The results of the studies presented in this article have impor- them with an optimized experience (e.g., visitors can make
tant implications for managers involved in the design and the most of their available time). By contrast, highly reactant
management of tourism experiences. Specifically, they visitors could be offered a fully autonomous experience.
address the important question of how to design an environ- Such a contingent approach would be beneficial for visitors
ment that enables visitors to immerse themselves in the expe- and tourism providers. Most visitors would be in a position
rience. The results suggest how tourism managers can to derive maximum value from their visit, as they would be
configure their operations to support the creation of immer- offered an experience that matches their personality traits
sive, fast-flying, and satisfying visitor experiences. The con- and preferences.
cept of autonomy appears to be central to facilitating the Finally, from an operations management perspective, the
emergence of feelings of immersion, timelessness, and posi- impact of autonomy on congestion levels and on the subse-
tive perceptions of the overall experience. Thus, we recom- quent visitor experience should not be underestimated.
mend that managers adopt autonomy (freedom of choice) as Autonomous visitors are likely to spend an extended period
a key principle in designing tourism experiences. This of time on site because of the serial relationship linking the
involves giving visitors sufficient freedom to explore a tour- concepts of autonomy, immersion, and temporal dissocia-
ism site or place without having to follow a predefined tion. When all visitors are given complete freedom to explore
sequence of activities or exhibits at a predetermined pace. a site, congestion issues are more likely to occur. This may in
Furthermore, the results provide guidance for managers turn have a negative impact on the visitor experience, since
who face the difficult design decision of either giving visi- perceptions of social density affect experience quality
tors complete autonomy to explore a given site or place, or (Goulding 2000). More specifically, visitors entering a con-
restrict their autonomy. Consider, for instance, the free- gested area may feel that their freedom is reduced as they are
choice learning strategy adopted by many museums. Our unable to see and do what they want when they want. This
study clearly shows that “no one size fits all visitors” when it issue is likely to be more salient in peak season when
1162 Journal of Travel Research 59(7)

attendance levels are high. On such occasions, the freedom between their autonomy and their motivations. For utilitarian
of some visitors may well interact with the freedom of fellow and thus task-oriented visitors, their motivation to reach a
visitors (Caruana and Crane 2011). It is therefore important specific goal might lead them to exhibit more positive
for tourism managers to consider the effects of visitor auton- responses to autonomy, which may help them in their quest.
omy on crowding levels. For instance, it may be relevant to Nevertheless, one may wonder if autonomy is always benefi-
propose several experiences with varying degrees of visitor cial for such utilitarian visitors, especially if they do not have
autonomy to ensure a smooth and regular flow of visitors and the required competence or knowledge to benefit from such
avoid excessive crowding levels. To illustrate, while visitors autonomy. Consider a visitor to a museum who aims to learn
taking a directed option are guided in the same direction, more about a specific topic, autonomy may here be benefi-
autonomous visitors are free to go where they want and cial only if the visitor has sufficient knowledge of the
explore the space at their own pace. In addition to offering a museum to visit it autonomously in such a way that this
tailored experience to all visitors, offering distinct types of autonomy enables him or her to optimize the visit. Further
visits could therefore reduce the risk of congestion and research is thus needed to explore the question of the effects
queues, a major source of dissatisfaction. of autonomy for tourists with specific motivations.
Furthermore, the recent boom in the use of personalized
recommendation systems in the form of digital platforms and
Limitations and Avenues for Future Research assistants is notable in the tourism sector. These smart tools
This study has some limitations that provide opportunities strive to support the creation of tailored visits by providing
for future research. First, we focus on visitor satisfaction as visitors with a recommended path or journey according to
the sole dependent variable. While a range of attitudinal and their personal preferences or fatigue levels, real-time crowd-
behavioral variables could have been included in the theo- ing levels, and peer ratings (Rodríguez-Díaz and Pulido-
retical model, our choice finds justification in the available Fernández 2018; Zheng, Liao, and Qin 2017). Although
evidence in the literature. Specifically, empirical research in intended to help visitors optimize their experiences, personal
the tourism literature broadly supports the existence of a planners reduce, at least partly, visitors’ autonomy. Since
direct and positive relationship between experience quality, they follow individualized recommendations prompting
satisfaction, and loyalty (e.g., Baker and Crompton 2000; attending specific sections in a predetermined order, visitors
Chen and Chen 2010; Chi and Qu 2008). This suggests that are less likely to visit the site in an exploratory manner. This
visitor satisfaction is a critical concept in tourism research suggests the need for scholars to investigate the relationship
that is central to understanding how visitors evaluate their between smart companions in tourism settings, consumers’
experience, and predicting visitor loyalty. A second limita- autonomy, and experience quality.
tion of the present research is that it does not take into Finally, it would be interesting to examine how first-time
account visitor motivations. Research shows that motiva- and non–first-time visitors differ in their responses to auton-
tions can deeply affect how people react to service environ- omy and the resulting impact on immersion. Such difference
ments (Kaltcheva and Weitz 2006), suggesting that autonomy may be observed as visitors who are familiar with a place are
in a tourism setting can differently affect satisfaction depend- more likely to wish to behave freely in any tourism setting.
ing on visitor motivations. Two main motivations are usually Previous research in the consumer behavior domain suggests
distinguished to explain the visit to a setting: utilitarian and that those who are familiar with their immediate consump-
recreational (Babin, Darden, and Griffin 1994). One question tion environment find it easier to fully access the experience
pertaining to motivations thus relates to the level of immer- and are more likely to become immersed in that experience
sion that visitors exhibit as a response to the interaction (Carù and Cova 2006).
Lunardo and Ponsignon 1163

Appendix 1: Map of the Permanent Tour of Cité du Vin (Study 1)

Source: https://www.laciteduvin.com/en/experience-la-cite-du-vin/the-permanent-tour.

Appendix 2: Maps and Scenarios of the decide to do something outdoors. You opt to visit a recently
Zoo (Study 2) opened zoo nearby to relieve the sense of boredom. The zoo
cares for a wide range of animals from various species. This
To manipulate the level of perceived autonomy, participants is the first time you visit this zoo. This 28-hectare zoo is
were randomly assigned to one of the following two scenar- home to more than 2,600 individual animals of 340 species.
ios and maps about a hypothetical experience visiting a zoo: To help you visit the zoo, you are invited to consult the map
that will be shown next. The map indicates the compulsory
path from which you cannot deviate. This path is designated
Low Autonomy Condition by green dots and includes the most popular animals (indi-
It’s just past noon on a Saturday, and none of your friends are cated by a bold pink-colored circle). Please take the time to
around. You find what’s on TV too dull to watch. You feel look at the map and then spend a couple of minutes trying to
very, very bored. Given that it’s a fine day weather-wise, you project yourself visiting the zoo.
1164 Journal of Travel Research 59(7)

High Autonomy Condition cares for a wide range of animals from various species. This
is the first time you visit this zoo. This 28-hectare zoo is
It’s just past noon on a Saturday, and none of your friends are home to more than 2,600 individual animals of 340 species.
around. You find what’s on TV too dull to watch. You feel To help you visit the zoo, you are invited to consult the map
very, very bored. Given that it’s a fine day weather-wise, you that will be shown next. Please take the time to look at the
decide to do something outdoors. You opt to visit a recently map and then spend a couple of minutes trying to project
opened zoo nearby to relieve the sense of boredom. The zoo yourself visiting the zoo.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests Bailey, N., and C. S. Areni. 2006. “When a Few Minutes Sound
Like a Lifetime: Does Atmospheric Music Expand or Contract
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
Perceived Time?” Journal of Retailing 82 (3): 189–202.
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Baker, D. A., and J. L. Crompton. 2000. “Quality, Satisfaction and
Behavioral intentions.” Annals of Tourism Research 27 (3):
Funding 785–804.
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author- Bianchi, C. 2016. “Solo Holiday Travellers: Motivators and Drivers
ship, and/or publication of this article. of Satisfaction and Dissatisfaction.” International Journal of
Tourism Research 18 (2): 197–208.
ORCID iD Block, R. A. 1990. “Models of Psychological Time?” In Cognitive
Models of Psychological Time, 1–35. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Frédéric Ponsignon https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0194-5236
Erlbaum.
Bonn, M. A., S. M. Joseph-Mathews, M. Dai, S. Hayes, and J. Cave.
Note 2007. “Heritage/Cultural Attraction Atmospherics: Creating
1. https://www.aza.org/partnerships-visitor-demographics. the Right Environment for the Heritage/Cultural Visitor.”
Journal of Travel Research 45 (February): 345–54.
Brehm, J. W. 1966. A Theory of Psychological Reactance. New
References York: Academic Press.
Agarwal, R., and E. Karahanna. 2000. “Time Flies When You’re Brehm, S. S., and J. W. Brehm. 1981. Psychological Reactance: A
Having Fun: Cognitive Absorption and Beliefs about Theory of Freedom and Control. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Information Technology Usage.” MIS Quarterly 24 (4): 665–94. Brida, J. G., M. Meleddu, and M. Pulina. 2012. “Understanding
Antón, C., C. Camarero, and M. J. Garrido. 2018. “Exploring Urban Tourism Attractiveness: The Case of the Archaeological
the Experience Value of Museum Visitors as a Co-Creation Ötzi Museum in Bolzano.” Journal of Travel Research 51 (6):
Process.” Current Issues in Tourism 21 (12): 1406–25. 730–41.
Babin, B. J., W. R. Darden, and M. Griffin. 1994. “Work and/or Brown, E., and P. Cairns. 2004. “A Grounded Investigation of
Fun: Measuring Hedonic and Utilitarian Shopping Value.” Game Immersion.” In CHI’04 Extended Abstracts on Human
Journal of Consumer Research 20 (March): 644–56. Factors in Computing Systems. ACM, 1297–300.
Lunardo and Ponsignon 1165

Cameron, M. A., J. Baker, M. Peterson, and K. Braunsberger. 2003. Driml, S., R. Ballantyne, and J. Packer. 2017. “How Long Does an
“The Effects of Music, Wait-Length Evaluation, and Mood on Economic Impact Last? Tracking the Impact of a New Giant
a Low-Cost Wait Experience.” Journal of Business Research Panda Attraction at an Australian Zoo.” Journal of Travel
56 (6): 421–30. Research 56 (5): 613–24.
Carr, N. 2016. “An Analysis of Zoo Visitors’ Favourite and Least Falk, J. H., and L. D. Dierking. 2016. The Museum Experience.
Favourite Animals.” Tourism Management Perspectives London: Routledge.
20:70–76. Firat, A. F., and A. Venkatesh. 1995. “Liberatory Postmodernism
Carù, A., and B. Cova. 2006. “How to Facilitate Immersion in a and the Reenchantment of Consumption.” Journal of Consumer
Consumption Experience: Appropriation Operations and Research 22 (December): 239–67.
Service Elements.” Journal of Consumer Behaviour 5 (1): 4–14. Fischer, G. N., and R. Atkin-Etienne. 1997. Individuals and
Carù, A., and B. Cova, eds. 2007. “Consumer Immersion in an Environment: A Psychosocial Approach to Workspace. Berlin,
Experiential Context.” In Consuming Experience, 34–47. Germany: De Gruyter.
London: Routledge. Fitzsimons, G. J. 2000. “Consumer Response to Stockouts.”
Caruana, R., and A. Crane. 2011. “Getting Away from It All: Journal of Consumer Research 27 (2): 249–66.
Exploring Freedom in Tourism.” Annals of Tourism Research Fitzsimons, G. J. 2008. “Death to Dichotomizing.” Journal of
38 (4): 1495–515. Consumer Research 35 (1): 5–8.
Chang, S. 2018. “Experience Economy in the Hospitality and Tourism Fitzsimons, G. J., and D. R. Lehmann. 2004. “Reactance to
Context.” Tourism Management Perspectives 27:83–90. Recommendations: When Unsolicited Advice Yields Contrary
Chartrand, T. L., A. N. Dalton, and G. J. Fitzsimons. 2007. Responses.” Marketing Science 23 (1): 82–94.
“Nonconscious Relationship Reactance: When Significant Fornell, C., and D. F. Larcker. 1981. “Evaluating Structural Equation
Others Prime Opposing Goals.” Journal of Experimental Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error.”
Social Psychology 43:719–26. Journal of Marketing Research 18:39–50.
Chen, C. F., and F. S. Chen. 2010. “Experience Quality, Perceived Fornerino, M., and A. Helme-Guizon. 2008. “Movie Consumption
Value, Satisfaction and Behavioral Intentions for Heritage Experience and Immersion: Impact on Satisfaction.” Recherche
Tourists.” Tourism Management 31 (1): 29–35. et Applications en Marketing 23 (3): 93–109.
Chen, F., and J. Sengupta. 2014. “Forced to Be Bad: The Positive Gentile, C., N. Spiller, and G. Noci. 2007. “How to Sustain
Impact of Low-Autonomy Vice Consumption on Consumer the Customer Experience: An Overview of Experience
Vitality.” Journal of Consumer Research 41 (4): 1089–107. Components that Co-create Value with the Consumer.”
Chi, C. G. Q., and H. Qu. 2008. “Examining the Structural European Management Journal 25 (5): 395–410.
Relationships of Destination Image, Tourist Satisfaction Ghani, J. A., and S. P. Deshpande. 1994. “Task Characteristics
and Destination Loyalty: An Integrated Approach.” Tourism and the Experience of Optimal Flow in Human–Computer
Management 29 (4): 624–36. Interaction.” Journal of Psychology 128 (4): 381–91.
Chung, N., H. Lee, J. Y. Kim, and C. Koo. 2017. “The Role of Goulding, C. 2000. “The Museum Environment and the Visitor
Augmented Reality for Experience-Influenced Environments: Experience.” European Journal of Marketing 34 (3/4): 261–
The Case of Cultural Heritage Tourism in Korea.” Journal of 78.
Travel Research 57 (5): 627–43. Hair, J. F., W. C. Black, B. J. Babin, R. E. Anderson, and R. L.
Cohen, E. 1979. “Rethinking the Sociology of Tourism.” Annals of Tatham. 2005. Multivariate Data Analysis. Englewood Cliffs,
Tourism Research 6 (1): 18–35. NJ: Prentice Hall.
Cohen, E. H. 2011. “Educational Dark Tourism at an In Populo Handler, R., and E. Gable. 1997. The New History in an Old
Site: The Holocaust Museum in Jerusalem.” Annals of Tourism Museum: Creating the Past at Colonial Williamsburg. Durham,
Research 38 (1): 193–209. NC: Duke University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1990. Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Hansen, A. H., and L. Mossberg. 2013. “Consumer Immersion:
Experience: Steps toward Enhancing the Quality of Life. New A Key to Extraordinary Experiences.” In Handbook on the
York: Harper & Row. Experience Economy, edited by J. Sundbo and F. Sørensen,
Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1997. Finding Flow. New York: Basic. 209–27. Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar.
Cuny, C., M. Fornerino, and A. Helme-Guizon. 2015. “Can Music Hayes, A. F., and J. Matthes. 2009. “Computational Procedures for
Improve E-behavioral Intentions by Enhancing Consumers’ Probing Interactions in OLS and Logistic Regression: SPSS
Immersion and Experience?” Information & Management and SAS Implementations.” Behavioral Research Methods 41
52:1025–34. (August): 924–36.
Deci, E. L., and R. M. Ryan. 1985. Intrinsic Motivation and Self- Heung, V. S., and L. Cheng. 2000. “Assessing Tourists’ Satisfaction
Determination in Human Behavior. New York: Plenum. with Shopping in the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region
Deci, E. L., and R. M. Ryan. 2000. “The ‘What’ and ‘Why’ of of China.” Journal of Travel Research 38 (May): 396–404.
Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Holbrook, M. B. 2006a. “The Consumption Experience—Something
Behavior.” Psychological Inquiry 11:227–68. New, Something Old, Something Borrowed, Something Sold:
Dowd, E. T., F. Wallbrown, D. Sanders, and J. M. Yesenosky. 1994. Part 1.” Journal of Macromarketing 26 (2): 259–66.
“Psychological Reactance and Its Relationship to Normal Holbrook, M. B. 2006b. “The Consumption Experience—Something
Personality Variables.” Cognitive Therapy and Research 18 New, Something Old, Something Borrowed, Something Sold:
(6): 601–12. Part 2.” Journal of Macromarketing 27 (1): 86–96.
Drengner, J., J. Steffen, and P. Furchheim. 2018. “Flow Revisited: Holbrook, M. B. 2006c. “The Consumption Experience—Something
Process Conceptualization and a Novel Application to Service New, Something Old, Something Borrowed, Something Sold:
Contexts.” Journal of Service Management 29 (4): 703–34. Part 3.” Journal of Macromarketing 27 (2): 173–83.
1166 Journal of Travel Research 59(7)

Hong, S.-M., and S. Faedda. 1996. “Refinement of the Moore, S. G., and G. J. Fitzsimons. 2014. “Yes, We Have No
Hong Psychological Reactance Scale.” Educational and Bananas: Consumer Responses to Restoration of Freedom.”
Psychological Measurement 56 (1): 173–82. Journal of Consumer Psychology 24 (4): 541–48.
Hosany, S., and M. Witham. 2010. “Dimensions of Cruisers’ Moreno, G. S., and J. B. Ritchie. 2009. “Understanding the Museum
Experiences, Satisfaction, and Intention to Recommend.” Image Formation Process: A Comparison of Residents and
Journal of Travel Research 49 (3): 351–64. Tourists.” Journal of Travel Research 47(4): 480–93.
Hui, T. K., D. Wan, and A. Ho. 2007. “Tourists’ Satisfaction, Morgan, M., J. Elbe, and J. de Esteban Curiel. 2009. “Has the
Recommendation and Revisiting Singapore.” Tourism Experience Economy Arrived? The Views of Destination
Management 28 (4): 965–75. Managers in Three Visitor-Dependent Areas.” International
Iyengar, S. S., and M. R. Lepper. 2000. “When Choice Is Journal of Tourism Research 11 (2): 201–16.
Demotivating: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing?” Novak, T. P., D. L. Hoffman, and Y. F. Yung. 2000. “Measuring the
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 (6): 995–1006. Customer Experience in Online Environments: A Structural
Kaltcheva, V. D., and B. A. Weitz. 2006. “When Should a Retailer Modeling Approach.” Marketing Science 19 (1): 22–42.
Create an Exciting Store Environment?” Journal of Marketing Oh, H., A. M. Fiore, and M. Jeoung. 2007. “Measuring Experience
70 (1): 107–18. Economy Concepts: Tourism Applications.” Journal of Travel
Klenosky, D. B., and C. D. Saunders. 2007. “Put Me in the Zoo! Research 46 (November): 119–32.
A Laddering Study of Zoo Visitor Motives.” Tourism Review Otto, J. E., and J. B. Ritchie. 1996. “The Service Experience in
International 11 (3): 317–27. Tourism.” Tourism Management 17 (3): 165–74.
Levav, J., and R. Zhu. 2009. “Seeking Freedom through Variety.” Pine, B. J., and J. H. Gilmore. 1998. “Welcome to the Experience
Journal of Consumer Research 36 (4): 600–10. Economy.” Harvard Business Review 76 (4): 97–105.
Levesque, C., A. N. Zuehlke, L. R. Stanek, and R. M. Ryan. 2004. Pine, B. J., and J. H. Gilmore. 1999. The Experience Economy:
“Autonomy and Competence in German and American University Work Is Theatre & Every Business a Stage. Boston, MA:
Students: A Comparative Study Based on Self-Determination Harvard Business Press.
Theory.” Journal of Educational Psychology 96 (1): 68–84. Podsakoff, P. M., S. B. MacKenzie, J. Y. Lee, and N. P. Podsakoff.
Li, J., Y. Chung, and W. G. Kim. 2018. “Freedom of Choice as 2003. “Common Method Biases in Behavioral Research:
a Critical Success Factor in Destination Marketing: Empirical A Critical Review of the Literature.” Journal of Applied
Evidence from a Far-East Gambling City.” Tourism and Psychology 88 (5): 879–903.
Hospitality Research 18 (3): 321–32. Ponsignon, F., F. Durrieu, and T. Bouzdine-Chameeva. 2017.
Lindberg, F., and P. Østergaard. 2015. “Extraordinary Consumer “Customer Experience Design: A Case Study in the Cultural
Experiences: Why Immersion and Transformation Cause Sector.” Journal of Service Management 28 (4): 763–87.
Trouble.” Journal of Consumer Behaviour 14 (4): 248–60. Quan, S., and N. Wang. 2004. “Towards a Structural Model of the
Lindberg, F., A. H. Hansen, and D. Eide. 2014. “A Multirelational Tourist Experience: An Illustration from Food Experiences in
Approach for Understanding Consumer Experiences within Tourism.” Tourism Management 25 (3): 297–305.
Tourism.” Journal of Hospitality Marketing & Management Rasmi, S., S. I. Ng, J. A. Lee, and G. N. Soutar. 2014. “Tourists’
23 (5): 487–512. Strategies: An Acculturation Approach.” Tourism Management
Lopez, L., and R. Malhotra. 1991. “Estimation of Time Intervals 40:311–20.
with Most Preferred and Least Preferred Music.” Psychological Reisinger, Y. 2013. Transformational Tourism: Tourist Perspectives.
Studies 36 (3): 203–9. Oxfordshire, UK: CABI.
Machleit, K. A., J. J. Kellaris, and S. A. Eroglu. 1994. “Human Ritzer, G. 1999. Enchanting a Disenchanted World. Thousand
versus Spatial Dimensions of Crowding Perceptions in Retail Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.
Environments: A Note on Their Measurement and Effect on Rodríguez-Díaz, B., and J. I. Pulido-Fernández. 2018. “Selecting
Shopper Satisfaction.” Marketing Letters 5 (2): 183–94. the Best Route in a Theme Park through Multi-objective
Markus, H. R., and B. Schwartz. 2010. “Does Choice Mean Programming.” Tourism Geographies 20 (5): 791–809.
Freedom and Well-Being?” Journal of Consumer Research 37 Rowley, J. 1999. “Measuring Total Customer Experience in
(2): 344–55. Museums.” International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality
Mathwick, C., N. Malhotra, and E. Rigdon. 2001. “Experiential Management 11 (6): 303–8.
Value: Conceptualization, Measurement and Application in Schwartz, B. 2000. “Self-Determination: The Tyranny of Freedom.”
the Catalog and Internet Shopping Environment.” Journal of American Psychologist 55 (1): 79–88.
Retailing 77 (1): 39–56. Sheng, C. W., and M. C. Chen. 2012. “A Study of Experience
McIntyre, C. 2009. “Museum and Art Gallery Experience Space Expectations of Museum Visitors.” Tourism Management 33
Characteristics: An Entertaining Show or a Contemplative (1): 53–60.
Bathe?” International Journal of Tourism Research 11 (2): Sherry, J. F. 1998. Servicescapes: The Concept of Place in
155–70. Contemporary Markets. Chicago: NTC Business Books.
Mifsud, M., A. S. Cases, and G. N’Goala. 2015. “Service Silverman, L. H. 1995. “Visitor Meaning-Making in Museums
Appropriation: How Do Customers Make the Service Their for a New Age.” Curator: The Museum Journal 38 (3): 161–
Own?” Journal of Service Management 26 (5): 706–25. 70.
Minkiewicz, J., J. Evans, and K. Bridson. 2013. “How Do Smith, S. L. 1994. “The Tourism Product.” Annals of Tourism
Consumers Co-create Their Experiences? An Exploration in Research 21 (3): 582–95.
the Heritage Sector.” Journal of Marketing Management 30 Song, J. H., and G. M. Zinkhan. 2008. “Determinants of Perceived
(1/2): 30–59. Web Site Interactivity.” Journal of Marketing 72 (2): 99–113.
Lunardo and Ponsignon 1167

Spiller, S. A., G. J. Fitzsimons, J. G. Lynch Jr., and G. H. Wicklund, R. A. 1974. Freedom and Reactance. Potomac, MD:
McClelland. 2013. “Spotlights, Floodlights, and the Magic Lawrence Erlbaum.
Number Zero: Simple Effects Tests in Moderated Regression.” Winer, R. S. 1999. “Experimentation in the 21st Century: The
Journal of Marketing Research 50:277–88. Importance of External Validity.” Journal of the Academy of
Stuart, F. I. 2006. “Designing and Executing Memorable Service Marketing Science 27 (3): 349–58.
Experiences: Lights, Camera, Experiment, Integrate, Action!” Wu, H. C., C. C. Cheng, and W. Hong. 2017. “An Assessment of
Business Horizons 49:149–59. Zoo Visitors’ Revisit Intentions.” Tourism Analysis 22 (3):
Szymanski, D. M., and D. H. Henard. 2001. “Customer Satisfaction: 361–75.
A Meta-analysis of the Empirical Evidence.” Journal of the Yuan, Y.-H., and C. Wu. 2008. “Relationship among Experiential
Academy of Marketing Science 29 (Winter): 16–35. Marketing, Experiential Value, and Customer Satisfaction.”
Thayer, S., and W. Schiff. 1975. “Eye-Contact, Facial Expression, Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research 32 (3):
and the Experience of Time.” Journal of Social Psychology 387–410.
95:117–24. Zhang, Y., J. Xu, Z. Jiang, and S. C. Huang. 2011. “Been There,
Tung, V. W. S., and J. B. Ritchie. 2011. “Exploring the Essence Done That: The Impact of Effort Investment on Goal Value
of Memorable Tourism Experiences.” Annals of Tourism and Consumer Motivation.” Journal of Consumer Research 38
Research 38 (4): 1367–86. (1): 78–93.
Turley, S. K. 1999. “Conservation and Tourism in the Traditional Zheng, W., Z. Liao, and J. Qin. 2017. “Using a Four-Step Heuristic
UK Zoo.” Journal of Tourism Studies 10 (2): 2–13. Algorithm to Design Personalized Day Tour Route within a
Tussyadiah, I. P. 2014. “Toward a Theoretical Foundation for Tourist Attraction.” Tourism Management 62:335–49.
Experience Design in Tourism.” Journal of Travel Research
53 (5): 543–64.
Uriely, N. 2005. “The Tourist Experience: Conceptual Author Biographies
Developments.” Annals of Tourism Research 32 (1): 199–216.
Renaud Lunardo is an Associate Professor of Marketing at
Uriely, N., Y. Yonay, and D. Simchai. 2002. “Backpacking
KEDGE Business School. His primary research interests focus on
Experiences: A Type and Form Analysis.” Annals of Tourism
the effects of marketing in-store stimuli (e.g., atmospherics like
Research 29 (2): 520–38.
ambient scents, music, colors…) on store perceptions and infer-
Vo Thanh, T., and V. Kirova. 2018. “Wine Tourism Experience:
ences of manipulative intent.
A Netnography Study.” Journal of Business Research
83:30–37. Frédéric Ponsignon is an Associate Professor of Operations
Watts, F. N., and R. Sharrock. 1984. “Fear and Time Estimation.” Management at KEDGE BS. His research primarily focuses
Perceptual and Motor Skills 59:597–98. on the design of service delivery systems, customer experi-
White, R. W. 1959. “Motivation Reconsidered: The Concept of ence management, quality management, and service recovery
Competence.” Psychological Review 66 (5): 297–333. management.

You might also like