You are on page 1of 6

THE BEHAVIOR ANALYST TODAY VOLUME NUMBER 6, ISSUE NUMBER 4, 2005

DISCOVERY OF MOTOR DEVELOPMENT: A TRIBUTE TO


ESTHER THELEN

G.J.P SAVELSBERGH

HUMAN MOVEMENT SCIENCES, VRIJ UNIVERSITY

In this tribute to Esther Thelen’s legacy, it is discussed how she brought concepts of new theoretical perspectives into the
domain of motor development. As a consequence of this a rejuvenalisation of motor development took place in the mid 1980’s.
The study of motor development skills became a testing ground not only for the task at hand, but also for other areas of
development.
Key words: Thelen, infant motor development, stereotypy
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

“The (re) discovery of motor development” is part of ments, and Dynamic field Theory. In the latter she worked
a title used by Esther Thelen (1989) for a discussion pa- towards an encompassing dynamic theory for the de-
per in Developmental Psychology. It reflects why she velopment of action, perception, and cognition.
became famous in the world of developmental psychol-
ogy during the early 1980s. She put the field of infants’ A NEW KID ON THE DEVELOPMENTAL
motor development in infants back on the scientific BLOCK: THE MUSCLE-FAT RATIO STUDY
map as an important topic of study both in its own The experiments conducted by Thelen in the early
right and as a window into other, less accessible do- 1980s (Thelen & Fisher, 1982, Thelen et al., 1983; 1984)
mains of development. Her achievements are relevant can be considered as the starting point of a major intel-
for any academic interested in teaching and research in lectual shift in motor development. In these experiments,
the areas of movement co-ordination, movement control she challenged the, then-prevailing view on motor devel-
and skill acquisition. Esther Stillman Thelen passed away opment as being determined by neural maturation. Be-
at the age of 63 on December 29th, 2004 at a hospital fore discussing these experiments, we go back into his-
in Bloomington, Indiana. tory.
Esther was trained as ethologist and started her In the first two years an infant acquires different a
career by carefully documenting repetitive movement range of goal-directed motor behaviors, such as reach-
stereotypy’s, first in grooming wasps (masters degree, ing, grasping, sitting, crawling and walking. These so-
1973) and, later in developing infants (doctoral de- called milestones are well known since the work of Gesell
gree, 1977). This early work resulted in a number of (1929), McGraw (1935) and Shirley (1933). The descrip-
well-received publications and the beginning of what tion of these milestones resulted in a view of motor de-
would be continuous funding by NSF and NIH. In a velopment as a rather rigid and gradual unfolding of pos-
comparatively short career, spanning only 25 years, tures and movements, that was mainly attributed to the
Esther’s impact on the field is reflected in three books, general process of maturation of the central nervous sys-
an SRCD monograph, and over 120 scientific articles tem. From a neural-maturation perspective the develop-
and book chapters. She served on the editorial boards ment of movement co-ordination is regarded as a gradual
of 15 scientific journals, gave countless invited lec- unfolding of predetermined patterns (from cephalo-to-
tures and was elected president of the International caudal and central-to-distal sequences) in the central ner-
Society on Infant Studies (1996-1998) and president vous system and an increasing cortical control over lower
of the Society for Research in Child Development. reflexes. This view was still dominant among scientist
Esther’s influence and significance for scientific work way into the 1960s and 1970s, the so called heydays of
in development will be measurable beyond these out- the information-processing approach. Esther challenged
puts for many years to come. this perspective with her experiments. In one of the key
In this tribute to her legacy, we will comment on some papers, ‘The relationship between physical growth and a
of Esther’s impact and contribution to the field of devel- newborn reflex’ published in Infant Behavior and Devel-
opmental science. We will start with her early classic opment, she and her co-workers illustrate instead that the
muscle-fat study and end with her A-not-B error experi- complex interplay between infants’ bodies, their environ-
243
THE BEHAVIOR ANALYST TODAY VOLUME NUMBER 6, ISSUE NUMBER 4, 2005
ment, and earlier experiences determine the course of the body midline increased with age. Furthermore, the
developmental changes. They found that in 8-week-old majority of the midline crossings were part of two-handed
infants held upright, the stepping movements observed reaches for the large ball and occurred at, or after onset
at a younger age disappeared. However, when the infants of bimanual reaching. Together, this strongly suggest that
were lying supine, they performed kicking movements the development of crossing the body midline emerges
that were kinematically similar to the earlier observed in the context of bimanual reaching. It is concluded that
stepping movements. Moreover, when the same infants the need to grasp a large ball positioned contralaterally
were held upright in water the stepping pattern also re- with two hands induces midline crossing. Hence, the de-
emerged. If the disappearance of the stepping reflex is velopment of midline crossings is not exclusively depen-
due to cortical inhibition, as the traditional explanation dent on organismic constraints (e.g., the maturation of
would have it, why would the cortex inhibit movements hemispheric connections), but on their interaction with
in the upright posture but not in the supine posture or in environmental constraints (e.g., object size). Similar to
the water? Thelen explained this disappearance of new- Thelen’s work, no single factor had priority.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

born stepping movements as a consequence of the dis- The experiments conducted by Thelen in the early
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

proportionate growth of leg muscles and fat tissues 1980s can be considered as the starting point of a major
(Thelen et al., 1984). Specifically during this period of intellectual shift in motor development. Although their
development infants acquire fat at a greater rate than primary intention in conducting the research in the origi-
muscle mass, which leads to relatively less muscle force. nal paper was to provide empirical evidence to provide
Thus, the occurrence of stepping movements (task) is a an alternative explanation for the results of training on
consequence of the interaction between organismic con- infant stepping reported by Zelazo (1983). The impact
straints (body proportions) and environmental constraints of the articles however, is also (and perhaps mainly) due
(orientation to the gravity vector), and not uniquely de- to the change in theoretical thinking about motor control
termined by neuro-maturational constraints. No single and particularly motor development. Concepts stemming
factor has priority, a theme that would resurface through- from new perspectives had begun to achieve popularity
out her work. in the scientific community, but empirical data support-
In the earlier eighties, ecological psychology, coordi- ive of these new ideas was lacking, especially in motor
native structure theory and dynamic systems theory were development. Thelen et al.’s article provided some of
introduced (Gibson, 1987; Kelso, 1995; Kugler, Kelso, the earliest support for these new emerging theoretical
& Turvey, 1982). The research described in the article of perspectives and became one of the major catalysts re-
Thelen et al. (1984) can be considered as one of the first sponsible for the rejuvenation of the study of motor de-
experimental studies to test the theoretical concepts pro- velopment.
vided by these new avenues of theoretical thinking.
Twenty years later, studies of motor development have ESTABLISHING AND EXTENDING A REPUTA-
become a major testing ground for examining the devel- TION: THE DYNAMIC SYSTEMS APPROACH TO
opmental implications of these new theoretical perspec- DEVELOPMENT
tives (some examples from our Amsterdam group: Inspired by the work of Bernstein (1967), Gibson’s
Savelsbergh & Van der Kamp, 1994, Kawai, Savelsbergh (1979, 1988) and Turvey (1990) and dynamic systems
& Wimmers, 1999; Van Hof, Van der Kamp & theory by Kelso (Kelso 1995; Kugler, Kelso, & Turvey,
Savelsbergh, 2002). For example, the Van Hof et al. 1982; Kugler & Turvey, 1987), Esther started to use mo-
(2002) experiment examined how the development of tor development as a vehicle to provide empirical evi-
crossing the midline is interwoven with the development dence for (i) developmental processes are nonlinear, (ii)
of bimanual reaching. Previously, it was held that the action and perception form an inseparable loop and (iii)
development of midline crossing is uniquely determined variability in development is functional.
by maturation of the hemispheric specialisation (Provine For instance, Ester was inspired by Nikolai Bernstein
& Westerman, 1979) or the maturation of spinal tracts (1967), a Russian physiologist, who formulated one of
(Morange & Bloch, 1996). Van Hof et al. observed in- the central issues in understanding the development of
fants longitudinally at 12, 18 and 26 weeks of age while motor co-ordination: the ‘degrees of freedom’ problem.
reaching for two balls (3 and 8 cm in diameter) at three The degrees of freedom problem refers to the possible
positions (ipsilateral, midline and contralateral). With age, movements of all the components (e.g., muscles, tendons,
the infants increasingly adapted the number of hands used joints etc.) of the motor apparatus of the human body.
to the size of the object. The number of reaches crossing Bernstein realised that the non-linear nature of the inter-

244
THE BEHAVIOR ANALYST TODAY VOLUME NUMBER 6, ISSUE NUMBER 4, 2005
actions among these different components of the human behavioral unit. A collective variable (order parameter)
body makes their separate regulation impossible and in- is the parameter that captures the observed behavior (co-
ferred that to be able to control all these components, or ordination pattern), while a control parameter is the pa-
degrees of freedom, these movements have to be co- rameter that leads the system through different co-ordi-
ordinated. Co-ordination, therefore, is the process of nation patterns. Within this approach, the behavioral pat-
mastering the redundant degrees of freedom into a con- tern is regarded as a stable collective state attained by
trollable system. It is this idea of non-linearity what comes the system under certain constraints (boundary condi-
back in many papers of Esther (e.g. Thelen et al, 1989; tions) and informational settings (Zanone, Kelso, & Jeka,
Thelen & Smith, 1994). 1993). When the control parameter passes through a criti-
A couple that had a considerable influence on Esther’s cal point, a co-ordination pattern that was stable becomes
thinking is Eleanor and James Gibson’s (1979, 1988). unstable causing a sudden discrete transition to a quali-
The Gibson’s ecological psychology approach to percep- tatively different, stable co-ordination pattern. Such a
tion is also known as the direct perception perspective. change appears without any prescription from outside,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The word ‘direct’ refers to the fact that objects, places but is acquired by the system self, i.e., through self-orga-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and events in the environment can be perceived without nization. Esther Thelen’s group provided empirical evi-
the need of cognitive mediation to make perception mean- dence (among others, e.g. Van Geert, 1999; Van der Maas,
ingful, such as in the information-processing approach. 1993) that developmental processes are not smooth and
Information in the environment is not static in time and monotonic, but are better characterized by phenomenon
space, but specifies events, places and objects. The child such as discontinuities, transitions, instabilities, and re-
has to learn to pick up and select the appropriate infor- gressions (Thelen, 1995; Thelen & Smith, 1994, 1995;
mation by exploration. The concept of exploration plays Thelen & Ulrich, 1991). These phenomena are charac-
a key role in many publications by Esther. She advocates teristic of non-linear dynamical processes and scientists
that through active and directed exploration of the envi- from this perspective often search for control and order
ronment the child learns to detect relevant information, parameters. The identification of rate-limiting factors can
and to couple the information to movements. For instance, help to identify these control parameters (like relative
this is nicely shown by Thelen in her earlier ethologically muscle force as result of the ratio of fat/muscle tissue by
orientated studies (1979) where she studied the occur- Thelen et al., 1984).
rence of rhythmical behaviors like kicking, rocking, arm Within this theoretical perspective, Esther’s carried
waving and banging in natural settings. She showed out a string of ingenious cross-sectional and longitu-
that these behaviors occurred most frequently around tran- dinal experiments. The development of infants’ leg
sitions where new behaviors emerged. Rocking on hands movements as a research paradigm dominated in the
and feet, for example, emerged in development just be- 1980s, with careful tracking of infants’ kicking, step-
fore the onset of crawling. This rocking might represent ping, and walking patterns. In the 1990s, she extended
the infant’s exploration of his action capabilities and the her research program from cyclic leg movements to-
(proprioceptive) information it produces. This eventually wards perceptually guided, goal-directed arm move-
leads to a new stable action pattern (crawling). Through ments in reaching and grasping (for instance; Thelen
exploration the infant discovers how perception and ac- et al., 1993). An important concept in these studies
tion are coupled. was variability. When the data are grouped with respect
A third source of inspiration, and maybe her major to the transition point an increase in variability is found
source, is the dynamic systems theory (Scott Kelso, 1995). around the transition point. Thelen and Smith (1994) sug-
The aim of the dynamic systems approach is to charac- gested that variability around a transition is a prerequi-
terize spatio-temporal and functional patterns of motor site in order to jump to a qualitatively new level of be-
behavior in terms of their stability properties by formal- havior. In their view, variability is highly functional: it
izing the time-evolution of relevant variables into dynami- provides the infant/child with possibilities for explora-
cal equations of motion. Stationary, stable states or pat- tion, that is, the opportunity to discover new qualitatively
terns of activity, as well as abrupt transitions between different solutions.
different states accompanied with loss of stability (in-
duced by changes in external conditions), have been suc- EMBODIED COGNITION
cessfully modeled in this way (Kelso, 1995). The per- Esther’s most recent work was directed towards
spective portrays co-ordination as a process that con- embodied cognition, showing how the complex inter-
strains the potentially free variables of a system into a actions between looking, remembering, and acting

245
THE BEHAVIOR ANALYST TODAY VOLUME NUMBER 6, ISSUE NUMBER 4, 2005
shape infants’ perseverative errors in the A not B search eral times in succession. This means that no single
task. With this work Esther challenged one of the most element has causal priority.
highly researched phenomena in the study of infant In collaboration with Gregor Schöner, a powerful
cognitive development: Piaget’s theory of object per- mathematical model of A-not-B (i.e. the dynamical
manence with this work. In the traditional work field theory) was offered and tested (Thelen, Schöner,
(Piaget, 1954), Piaget described how his 9.5 months - Scheier, & Smith, 2001). The field model simulates
old child, Laurent, made reaches for his hidden watch. the decision of infants to reach to location A or B and
The watch was hidden several times under a garment the level of inclination to reach in any direction is
at location A and Laurent searched for it. Subsequently, constantly changing over time. This constantly chang-
the watch was hidden at a nearer and highly similar ing set of inclinations (i.e. the movement planning
location B while Laurent watched. The infant made a field) depends on a number of factors, including (1)
curious reaching ‘error’; after a delay between hiding the current state of the field, (2) the task input, cap-
and reaching, Laurent searched for the watch at loca- turing the perceptual cues that specify the possible
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion A, the location where it was first found. Piaget hiding locations, (3) the selective input, capturing the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

concluded that it is a universal phenomenon that is hiding event or the waving of the lid, (4) the memory
due to limitations in Laurent’s concept of an object. of previous reaches become an input to the next trial,
The explanation focuses attention on Laurent’s inter- so in absence of other cues the system is likely to stick
nal cognitive machinery, the way he thinks about the to previous choices (5) interactions between the loca-
world. tions make sure that a highly activated location will
Following the work of Piaget the hide and seek task exert a strong inhibitory influence over the other point,
has been repeated countless times, with myriad varia- so one location wins out.
tions. The error turned out was found to be a robust This model can reproduce the A not B error. Be-
phenomenon in its standard form, but highly complex fore there is any object hidden there is activation at
in its context dependency. Each of the highlighted both A and B. As the experimenter directs attention to
components seems to be crucial to the occurrence of A by for example hiding the toy, it produces high acti-
the error (see for a review Smith, Thelen, Titzer, vation at A. When the activation peak crosses a thresh-
McLin, & Smith, 1999). After decades of research no old the infant reaches to A. Each time the infant reaches
cognitive theory could explain the full pattern of ex- to A, the memory becomes an input and the activation
perimental results, neither a consensus on the nature level at A builds. After several A trials the experimenter
of the error nor on its meaning was attained. If the A draws attention to location B. But as that cue decays,
not B error is a measure of the infant’s object con- the lingering memories of location A begin to domi-
cept, how can it be that infants have a more mature nate the field and over time the inclination shifts back
object representation when seemingly small task con- to location A. Experimenters can make the error come
ditions are altered? and go by manipulating the inputs to the fields (Smith
Thelen and Smith challenged the notion that the and Thelen, 2003). For example the A not B error in
error reflects the infant’s immature understanding of 10 month old infants diminishes when the number of
the concept of an object. They focused on the infant’s prior reaches to A decreases, when the two hiding lo-
reaching activity and not on the contents of the infant’s cations are made more distinctive and, when the hid-
mind (Smith and Thelen, 1993; Thelen and Smith, ing event at B is more attractive (i.e. hiding cookies
1994). A critical experiment was performed to dem- instead of toys). Younger infants are not able to main-
onstrate, for example, that the same patterns of be- tain stable peaks. In the A not B task they are domi-
havior are evident when waving a continually in-view nant on B trials through their motor memory of reaches
object at location A or B, instead of hiding a toy (Smith to A. Older infants maintain a stable peak after the
et al., 1999). So, the error is not about what infants cue at B so they reach to B despite delay and motor
have and don’t have as enduring object concepts but memory. Clearly the development of keeping the rel-
what they are doing and have done. From a dynamic evant, but no longer visual, cues in memory is a criti-
perspective on goal-directed reaching, perseveration cal developmental achievement. But also developmen-
in the A not B task is a continuous, self-organizing tal changes in the perceptual differentiation of the tar-
process which involves multiple interactions includ- gets and attention processes may contribute to a shift
ing visual input, looking behavior, posture, and the from perseveration to non-perseveration. The conse-
memory dynamics of repeating the same action sev- quences are that the division between what is concep-

246
THE BEHAVIOR ANALYST TODAY VOLUME NUMBER 6, ISSUE NUMBER 4, 2005
tual and what is perceptual-motor may be very hard to Kugler, P.N., & Turvey, M.T. (1987). Information, natural law, and
draw. the self-assembly of rhythmic movements. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
The dynamic field theory predicts that the dynam- Kugler, P.N., Kelso, J.A.S., & Turvey, M.T. (1982). On the control
ics that create the error in infants are general processes and coordination of naturally developing systems. In J.A.S. Kelso
involved in goal-directed reaching at all ages. Indeed, & J.E. Clark (Eds.), The development of movement control and co-
even toddlers make perseverative errors in the A not ordination (pp. 5-78). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
McGraw, M. (1935). Growth: A study of Johnny and Jimmy. New
B task when the A and B locations are not marked by
york: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
lids but instead are two locations in a homogeneous Morange, F. & Bloch, H. (1996). Laterlization of the approach
task space such as a sandbox (Spencer, Smith, and movement and the prehension movement in infants from 4 to 7
Thelen, 2001). The model captures both the robust- months. Early Development and Parenting, 5, 81 – 92
ness of the task and its exquisite context-sensitivity. Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York:
basic Books.
This is in itself a great achievement, but the model Provine, R. R. & Westerman, J. A. (1979). Crossing the midline:
also provided a number of novel predictions, which Limits of early eye-hand behavior. Child Development, 50, 437
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

were born out in further experiments (for instance: – 441.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Schutte, Spencer & Schöner, 2003; Spencer & Schöner, Savelsbergh, G. J. P., & van der Kamp, J. (1994). The effect of
body orientation to gravity on early infant reaching. Journal of
2003). Esther’s principle of multicausality demands a
Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 510-528.
rethinking of what is meant by knowledge and devel- Schutte, A.R., Spencer, J.P., & Schöner, G. (2003). Testing the dynamic
opment. The model accomplishes infant behavior in field theory: Working memory for locations becomes more spatially
the A not B task without invoking constructs of object precise over development. Child Development, 74, 1393-1417.
representation or other knowledge structures. Rather Shirley, M.M. (1931). The first two years: A study of twenty-five ba-
bies. Vol. 1. Postural and locomotor development. Minneapolis:
the infants’ behavior of knowing or not-knowing is University of Minnesota Press.
emergent from the dynamics of the reaching task it- Smith, L. B., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). Knowing in
self, including the history of previous actions. With the Context of Acting: The task Dynamics of the A not B error.
her work on embodied cognition, Thelen showed that Psychological Review, 106(2), 235-260.
Smith L. B., & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a dynamic system.
knowing cannot be separated from perceiving, acting,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 343-348.
and remembering. Spencer, J.P. & Schöner, G. (2003). Bridging the representational gap
In this tribute we provide a taste of Esther’s Thelen in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental
impact and contribution to the field of developmental Science, 6, 392-412.
science. Her works towards an encompassing dynamic Spencer, J. P., Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2001) Tests of a dynamic
systems account of the A not B error: the influence of prior experi-
theory for the development of action, perception, and ence on the spatial memory abilities of 2-years-old. Child Develop-
cognition will be a source of inspiration for several ment, 72, 1327-1346.
research groups over the world. She left us, undoubt- Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants.
edly, with a vibrant, healthy state of current research Animal Behaviour, 27, 699-715.
Thelen, E. (1989). The (RE) discovery of motor development: Learn-
on motor development. Esther’s influence and contri-
ing new things from on old field. Developmental Psychology, 25,
butions to scientific work and academic life will be 946-949.
measurable beyond these outputs for many years to Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development: A new synthesis. American
come. Psychologist, 50, 79-95
Thelen, E., & Fisher, D.M.. (1982). Newborn stepping: An explana-
tion for disappearing reflex. Developmental Psychology, 15, 447-
REFERENCES 453
Bernstein, N.A. (1967). The co-ordination and regulation of move- Thelen, E., & Smith. L. (1995). A dynamic systems approach to de-
ments. Oxford: Pergamon Press. velopment of cognition and action . Cambridge: MIT Press.
Gesell, A. (1929). Maturation and infant behavior pattern. Psycho- Thelen E, & Smith LB. (1994). A Dynamic systems approach to the
logical Review, 36, 307-319 development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press
Gibson, J.J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Thelen , E., Fisher, D., & Ridley Johson, R. (1984) The relationship
Boston: Houghton Mifflin between physical growth and a newbone reflex. Infant Behavior &
Gibson, E.J. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of per- Development, 7, 479-493.
ceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. In, M.R. Thelen, E., Ridley Johson, R. & Fisher, D., (1983). Shifting patterns
Rosenzweig & L.W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology of bilateral coordination and lateral dominance in the leg move-
(pp.1-41). Palo Alto, California: Annual review, Inc. ments of young children. Developmental Psychobiology, 16, 29-
Kawai, M., Savelsbergh, G. J. P., & Wimmers, R. H. (1999). New- 46.
borns spontaneous arm movements are influenced by the environ- Thelen E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., Smith, L. B. (2001). The dynamics
ment. Early Human Development, 54, 15-27. of embodiment: a field theory of infant perseverative reaching.
Kelso, J.A.S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns. The self-organization of Behavioral Brain Sciences, 24, 1-86.
Brain and Behavior. Cambridge: MIT press. Thelen, E., Corbetta, D., Kamm, K., Spencer, J.P., Schneider, K., &

247
THE BEHAVIOR ANALYST TODAY VOLUME NUMBER 6, ISSUE NUMBER 4, 2005
Zernicke, R.F. (1993). The transition to reaching: mapping inten-
tion and intrinsic dynamics. Child Development, 64, 1058-1098.
Thelen, E. & Ulrich, B (1991). Hidden Skills: A dynamic systems
analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 56, (1, Serial No.
223)
Turvey, M.T. (1990). Coordination. American Psychologist, 45, 938-
953.
Van Geert, P.C.L. (1999). Dynamic systems of development. Change
between complexity and chaos. New York: Harvest Wheatsheaf
Van der Maas, H.L.J. (1993). Catastrophe analysis of stepwise cogni-
tive development. Academic thesis, University of Amsterdam.
Van Hof, P, van der Kamp, J., & Savelsbergh, G.J.P. (2002) The Rela-
tion of Unimanual and Bimanual Reaching to Crossing the Mid-
line. Child Development, 73, 1353-1362.
Wimmers, R.H., Savelsbergh, G.J.P., Kamp, van der, J., & Hartelman,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

P. (1998). A cusp catastrophe model as a model for transition in the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

development of prehension. Developmental Psychobiology , 32, 23-


35
Zanone, P.G., Kelso, J.A.S., & Jeka, J.J. (1993). Concepts and meth-
ods for a dynamical approach to behavioral coordination and change.
In G.J.P Savelsbergh (Ed.), The development of coordination in in-
fancy (pp. 89-136). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Zelazo, P. R. (1983). The development of walling: New findings
and old assumptions. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15,99-t37.

Author Information:
G.J.P Savelsbergh, Institute for Fundamental
and Clinical Human Movement Sciences,
Faculty of Human Movement Sciences, Free
University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands,
Institute for Biophysical and Clinical Research
into Human Movement, Department for
Exercise & Sport Sciences, Manchester
Metropolitan University, United Kingdom.

Human Movement Sciences, Vrij University


Van der Boechorststraat 9, 1081 BT
Amsterdam Netherlands
Tel.:+31-20-5988461
Fax: +31-20-598
Email: gsavelsbergh@fbw.vu.nl

248

You might also like