You are on page 1of 13

The Use of Social Stories to

Promote Independent Behaviors


in Novel Events for Children
with PDD-NOS

Michelle L. Ivey, L. Juane Heflin, and Paul Alberto

This study investigated a previously undocumented use for Social Stories (Gray, 1994, ficulty due to inappropriate social behav-
2000): preparation for a novel event. Three children with pervasive developmental ior in these settings (Howlin, 1998). The
disorder—not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) participated in novel activities within the reliance on routine and ritual may help
routine setting of their weekly speech–language therapy sessions. During intervention, the child with autism exercise some con-
Social Stories written for the novel activities were used to prepare the participants for trol in an environment that is generally
the upcoming events. Independent participation during the novel events was calcu-
overstimulating and unpredictable (Klin-
lated using event recording of the presence of five different skills within each session.
ger & Dawson, 1995; Quill, 1998; Schu-
The effectiveness of the strategy was documented through a withdrawal design with
counterbalanced presentation of novel events. Results indicated that the use of Social ler, 1995).
Stories produced a 15% to 30% increase in participation during novel events for all Children with ASD are a heteroge-
three participants. neous group. They show different
strengths and deficits, and each child has
different needs (Committee on Educa-
tional Interventions, 2001; Quill, 1998).

A
utism spectrum disorder (ASD) is on sameness (American Psychiatric Asso- Because there are a multitude of strate-
an umbrella term used to en- ciation, 2000). Individuals with ASD may gies for addressing the needs of these
compass pervasive developmen- be observed to be rigid in expectations children, the following information in
tal disorders (Waltz, 1999). The Diag- for events; they may become anxious this introduction will review what inves-
nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental when they are expected to incorporate tigators currently know regarding affect-
Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; American Psy- change into a routine, and those with ing change within routines and enhanc-
chiatric Association, 2000) defines per- ASD may react negatively to even small ing social skills in a variety of situations
vasive developmental disorders as exist- changes in their environment. Such neg- for children with ASD. One particular
ing in five subcategories, all of which are ative reactions occur on a continuum. At method, the use of Social Stories, as de-
diagnosed through qualitative impair- the mild end of the continuum, a child scribed by Gray (1994, 2000) and Gray
ments in reciprocal socialization, verbal with ASD may attempt to manipulate a and Garand (1993), will be proposed as
and nonverbal communication, and re- situation to fit into an anticipated routine a means of developing flexibility within a
stricted and repetitive behaviors. or show mild distress at having a change routine, while considering the language
Of interest in this study is the diag- in routine. Severe responses may involve and interactional needs of children with
nostic category related to restrictive and large-scale reactions that result in physi- ASD.
repetitive behaviors. Individuals with cal stress and aggressive behaviors (Amer- Researchers have considered reliance
ASD typically exhibit restricted patterns ican Psychiatric Association, 2000, p. 71; on routine characteristic of children with
of behavior (Wing, 2001), which are ev- Howlin, 1998). A child’s resistance and ASD. Ferrara and Hill (1980) observed
idenced by reliance on precise routines, apprehension may lead to substantial prob- children with autism having more engage-
preoccupation with parts of objects, lim- lems in acquiring new skills that require ment and play in predictable situations
ited ranges of interests, and an insistence change in established routines and to dif- and becoming increasingly withdrawn in

FOCUS ON AUTISM AND OTHER DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES


VOLUME 19, NUMBER 3, FALL 2004
Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015
PAGES 164–176
VOLUME 19, NUMBER 3, FALL 2004
165

unpredictable and novel situations. Mc- support children with ASD in transition- application of the social skill to only the
Clannahan and Krantz (1999) suggested ing from one activity to the next within situation depicted, because children with
building flexibility into a child’s daily rou- their environments (MacDuff, Krantz, & ASD have a tendency to be rigid in their
tine by making small, systematic changes McClannahan, 1993; Schopler, Mesibov, expectations of events. Some students
to a detailed picture schedule. They sug- & Hearsey, 1995). Dettmer, Simpson, will respond to stories written on a single
gest that making changes in the schedule Myles, and Ganz (2000) used a variety of page, while others may respond to book-
may require effort at first, but may be- visual cues, including picture schedules, style stories with one concept and a very
come increasingly simpler as the child visual timers, and a finished box to de- general picture or icon on each page. The
learns to adapt to new activities within his crease latency and increase independence story should be able to be generalized to
or her day. during transitions for children with ASD. other similar situations for the targeted
The introduction of a novel event is Quill (1997) identified several potential skill or concept. Therefore, using terms
another situation that may alter a rou- means of supplementing language-based such as usually will allow for some flexi-
tine. However, no specific research has instruction with visual supports to en- bility in the interpretation of the story.
been conducted in the area of novel hance treatment programs for children Using words such as always or every may
events in relation to children with ASD with ASD. Furthermore, video priming lead to problems in a situation with an
or any disability. There is a small body of was used to prepare children for an up- unanticipated change or an irregular oc-
literature investigating novel events for coming change in activities. Priming, a currence. The story should be as clear
typically developing children. The re- procedure of previewing future events to and specific as possible, as in the follow-
search concludes that children’s expecta- make them more predictable, was done ing example: “I sit at my desk until my
tions during novel events will rely on re- prior to each targeted transition. Results work is finished. I know that I am fin-
call of the general information for similar indicated a decrease in disruptive behav- ished because I have answered all of the
events and that memory for events is bet- ior and some generalization of successful questions on my page.” When writing a
ter than memory for objects (Fivush, transitions into other settings (Schreib- Social Story, the author should not make
Hudson, & Nelson, 1984; Jones, Swift, man, Whalen, & Stahmer, 2000). assumptions regarding the reader’s pref-
& Johnson, 1988). Given that individu- Social Stories, as described by Gray erences or opinions, but the author can
als with ASD have difficulty generalizing (1994, 2000) and Gray and Garand make statements regarding the reactions
information (Siegel, 1996), the research (1993), have been researched as a visual and feelings of other people involved in
conducted with typically developing chil- and auditory means for introducing en- the story. Gray (1994) recommended
dren has limited applicability. vironmental information in a format that that individuals’ cognitive functioning
Transitions are another possible form is understandable to an individual with levels be at or above the level of moder-
of novel events. A transition between ac- ASD. Social Stories explain social con- ate intellectual disability when using So-
tivities is somewhat similar to a change in cepts and situations, describe necessary cial Story techniques. In addition, the
routine, because a child has to stop an ac- components, and suggest expected re- story should be written at an individual’s
tivity and move on to doing something sponses. The story can be used to answer comprehension level. Stories can be used
different. Transitions, however, can be questions individuals with ASD do not with individuals of a variety of ages.
routine or novel. For example, a routine ask or do not ask correctly, thereby help- Social Stories are generally short and
transition occurs when a class regularly ing them gain information. Social Stories contain four main sentence types, used
goes to music on Thursdays after atten- are not a form of social skill instruction; most frequently, and two other sentence
dance is taken. A novel transition occurs rather, they are a medium for explaining types, used less frequently (Gray, 1994,
when, after attendance on a Thursday, what is happening and expected within 2000; Gray & Garand, 1993). Descrip-
for example, the class goes into the room environmental settings. A byproduct of tive sentences answer the wh questions:
across the hall to watch a video as a spe- better understanding of the environment when a situation will occur, what is hap-
cial event. Several empirical studies re- is an increase in socially acceptable be- pening, why it is happening, and who is
garding transitions for children with havior from the individual with ASD. included in the situation. Perspective sen-
ASD give insight into means of prepar- Gray (1994, 2000) and Gray and tences show the reaction and feelings of
ing children for novel events within their Garand (1993) described the content others. Affirmative sentences describe
routines. Schmit, Alper, Raschke, and and structure of Social Stories. Stories are common associations with a particular
Ryndak (2000) concluded that a combi- individually written to address the needs situation. These sentences highlight con-
nation of visual and verbal cueing re- of a particular student. Generally, they cepts such as laws and rules, provide re-
duced the number of tantrums and in- are written in first person with present or assurance, and emphasize important
creased independence in the classroom, future tense verbs. Illustrations, particu- points. Cooperative sentences describe
an outcome that transferred to the stu- larly for younger children, may be in- the roles of others within a situation. Di-
dent’s home environment. Other re- cluded. However, for some children, rective sentences tell the individual what
searchers have used picture schedules to drawings are restrictive and may limit the the desired responses is. Finally, a control

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


FOCUS ON AUTISM AND OTHER DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES
166

sentence, which is frequently written by propriate sharing, poor eating manners, routine setting would lead to indepen-
the individual, provides a strategy for re- and verbal assaults (Kuoch & Mirenda, dent behavior during the actual event for
calling and using the information in the 2003; Kuttler, Myles, & Carlson, 1998; children with ASD.
Social Story. Because the purpose of the Lorimer, Simpson, Myles, & Ganz,
story is to describe the target situation, 2002; Norris & Dattilo, 1999). Further-
not to direct the child’s behavior (Gray, more, Swaggart et al. (1995) and Rowe Method
1994, 2000), stories generally are written (1999) observed a decrease in undesir-
with a ratio of zero or one directive or able behavior, such as shouting and phys- Participants
control sentence to every two to five de- ical assault, and an increase in desirable
scriptive, perspective, affirmative, or co- behavior, such as greetings and eating Participants in the study were three boys
operative sentences. The ratio ensures lunch. Thiemann and Goldstein (2001) with a diagnosis of PDD-NOS. Diag-
the emphasis of the story is on descrip- used Social Stories as part of a treatment noses were made by an independent psy-
tive and assuring qualities, not on direct- package to increase social communica- chologist, developmental pediatrician, or
ing or controlling the individual. tion skills for children with ASD. Finally, neurologist. According to Gray (1994),
Presentation of the story is systematic some researchers have used unique ap- individuals using Social Stories should
and designed to minimize potentially proaches in presentation of Social Stories have intellectual abilities of moderate im-
distracting interactions. The presenter to children with autism. Individualized pairment or better, as determined by IQ
should sit beside and slightly behind the stories have been presented via computer scores greater than 55. Only one of the
student while reading the story. An au- program (Hagiwara & Myles, 1999) and participants had been given an IQ test.
diocassette or even a video presentation incorporated as lyrics in songs (M. Other test results, which were provided
of the story further reduces person-to- Brownell, 2002). The researchers found by the parents or acquired by the first au-
person interaction. Frequency of reading some increase of skill level for the tar- thor, are presented here to help illustrate
and fading the story vary for each child geted tasks. Despite considerable meth- the participants’ current levels of func-
(Gray, 1994, 2000; Gray & Garand, odological concerns, the current research tioning. The children’s ages ranged from
1993). on Social Stories appears promising. 5 to 7 years old. The first author was fa-
Social Stories are theorized to benefit The research thus far about Social miliar to the participants because she was
students with autism for a variety of rea- Stories has focused on describing a situ- their speech–language pathologist, and
sons. First, they provide access to infor- ation and appropriate behaviors, and ex- the children attended weekly therapy ses-
mation regarding concepts and social be- plaining simple, understandable steps for sions with her. These three participants
havior, which may be difficult for the certain goals. To date, none of the stud- represent all of the children on the first
student to obtain otherwise. Second, ies have addressed Gray’s (1994) other author’s caseload at the inception of the
they clarify potentially confusing situa- suggested use for Social Stories: teaching study who were similar in language func-
tions. Third, they have minimal teacher– or preparing for a novel event. The chal- tioning and had histories of difficulty
student interaction and can be used in- lenge in conducting such research is that adapting to novel situations.
dependently by students following an once an event is experienced for baseline
introduction of the story (Gray, 1994; purposes, it is no longer novel for inves- Ron. Ron, a Caucasian boy, was 7
Gray & Garand, 1993). tigation of the treatment. Previous re- years 5 months old at the initiation of the
Social Stories may serve several pur- search on novel events with typically de- study. He participated in partial integra-
poses within the child’s environment. veloping children (Fivush et al., 1994; tion and partial special education in his
Gray (1994) suggested three distinct Jones et al., 1988) used analysis of sur- public school program. His academic
purposes for Social Story use: (a) de- vey responses following the novel events work was based in a first-grade curricu-
scribing a situation and appropriate be- to determine participants’ memory and lum. He was reading and spelling at age-
haviors, (b) explaining simple, under- coding of events without application of expected levels, but he disliked fine-
standable steps for certain goals, and empirical designs. The current study motor tasks such as writing. Ron had
(c) teaching new routines and anticipated used a variety of novel events, grouped difficulty with concepts such as relative
actions. Research has been conducted on into distinct categories. The categories size and quantity, which led to difficulty
the first two purposes, but not the third. are repeated in the various phases. Due with mathematics. Ron was competent in
The study results show at least an emer- to the characteristic difficulty with gen- a variety of self-help skills, such as inde-
gence of the desired social behavior in eralization exhibited by children with pendent dressing, toileting, and feeding.
participants with autism or pervasive de- ASD (Quill, 2000; Siegel, 1996), the He was able to participate in structured
velopmental disorder—not otherwise repetition of the category of event did and unstructured play.
specified (PDD-NOS). not necessarily prohibit novelty of each Ron’s receptive and expressive vocab-
Researchers have successfully used So- individual activity. The purpose of this ularies were within normal limits for his
cial Stories to decrease socially unaccept- study was to determine if the use of a So- age. He used appropriate grammatical
able behavior, including tantrums, inap- cial Story prior to a novel event within a structures. On the Test of Language

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


VOLUME 19, NUMBER 3, FALL 2004
167

Development–Primary (Newcomer & orate. He used limited spontaneous com- range on the Linguistic Concepts, Sen-
Hammill, 1997), Ron received a Spoken ments. Adam would ask for permission tence Structure, Recalling Sentences in
Language Quotient of 91 (M = 100 ± and for objects, but did not inquire Context, and Word Structure subtests.
15). Ron had difficulty with figurative about the state or needs of others. He On the Expressive One-Word Picture Vo-
language, making inferences, and consid- was not able to tell a sequenced story. On cabulary Test, he received a standard
ering multiple meanings of words. On the the Test of Early Language Development– score of 93. Finally, on the Test for Au-
Test of Language Competence–Expanded Third Edition (Hresko, Reid, & Ham- ditory Comprehension of Language–
Edition (Wiig & Secord, 1989), Ron’s mill, 1999), he received a Spoken Lan- Third Edition (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1999),
overall composite score was 75 (M = 100 guage Quotient of 91 (M = 100 ± 15). he received a quotient of 85 (M = 100 ±
± 15). On the Test of Auditory-Perceptual On the Bracken Basic Concept Scale– 15).
Skills–Revised (Gardner, 1997), Ron re- Revised (Bracken, 1998), he received a Hal lived in a two-parent home in an
ceived an overall Auditory Processing classification of Average in all areas ex- upper middle class area. His mother had
standard score of 80 (M = 100 ± 15). cept Time/Sequence. On the Expressive a bachelor’s degree; his father had an ad-
Ron had poor conversational skills as ev- One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (R. vanced degree. He did not have any sib-
idenced by inappropriate topic changes Brownell, 2000), he received a standard lings. Hal responded well to structured
and insertions, decreased elaboration, score of 103 (M = 100 ± 15). environmental supports such as visual
and poor initiation. He used a visual Adam lived in a two-parent, upper schedules and markers to indicate when
schedule within his speech–language middle class family. His mother had com- he completed an activity.
pathology sessions. pleted two years of college; his father had Prior to selection for the study, parents
Ron lived in a two-parent, upper mid- an advanced degree. He had one older were asked to rate their children on a
dle class home. Both of his parents had brother. Throughout the study, Adam five-point scale on resistance to change
earned bachelor’s degrees, and he had participated in the Samonas (Spectral Ac- within routine events, the existence of
two brothers, one older and one tivated Music of Optimal Natural Struc- problems generated by the resistance,
younger. He was on a gluten- and casein- ture; Steinbach, 1994) listening program and anticipated difficulty in their ability
free diet throughout the entire interven- under the supervision of an occupational to implement the procedure. Parents
tion. therapist. confirmed that their children experi-
enced difficulty with change in routine
Adam. Adam, a Caucasian male, was Hal. Hal, an African American boy, situations. On a scale of 1 (nonexistent)
age 5 years, 1 month when the interven- was 5 years 8 months old when the study to 5 ( frequent), two parents indicated
tion began. Adam attended a special began. He participated in an integrated that their child’s level of resistance to
needs preschool program through the kindergarten class with special education change was a 4. The third parent rated
public schools. He exhibited prereading supports throughout the week. Hal was her son at 5. When asked to rate the ex-
skills in the areas of letter and sound independent in self-help skills. He was tent of problems that the resistance to
recognition. He did not yet rhyme. On reading beginning sight words and en- novelty created in daily functioning, two
the Wechsler Preschool Primary Scale of joyed writing and drawing. There were parents said 5 and one said 4. Parents re-
Intelligence–Third Edition (Wechsler, no academic or psychological test results ported observing difficulties such as
2002), he received a Verbal Score of 90, available for Hal. tantrums (including screaming, crying,
Performance Score of 97, and a Full Hal had a mild to moderate language and hitting), resistance to going some-
Scale Score of 92 (M = 100 ± 15). How- disorder. His receptive language deficits where new (saying “No, no!,” not mov-
ever, on the Vineland Adaptive Behavior included difficulty processing complex ing, or pulling away from the adult), anx-
Scales (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, information and understanding temporal ious or apprehensive body language, and
1985), he exhibited relatively lower concepts. He used delayed echolalia as a use of echolalic or rehearsed comfort
scores in adaptive living skills (Compos- way to express his thoughts or comment phrases.
ite Score 68, M = 100 ± 15). He had on something familiar in his environ-
deficits in pragmatic communication, ment. Hal rarely elaborated on a topic
Setting
toileting, shoe tying, interpersonal inter- within a conversational setting, and
actions and awareness, and sensory inte- requested desired objects, but not infor- Although settings varied across the dif-
gration, and had low muscle tone. mation, from others. Hal did not com- ferent novel events targeted at a given
Adam presented with a mild receptive plete the Clinical Evaluation of Lan- session, all activities occurred on the
language disorder. He had difficulties guage Fundamentals–Preschool (Wiig, campus of a children’s hospital in a met-
understanding complex information and Secord, & Semel, 1992) during an an- ropolitan area. Activities that did not in-
inferring from contextual cues. Adam nual assessment of his language. He re- volve making a purchase or a novel set-
had greater difficulty with expressive lan- ceived scores within normal range on the ting were conducted in the first author’s
guage. He was able to answer questions Basic Concepts and Formulating Labels treatment room in an outpatient speech–
appropriately; however, he did not elab- subtests. He had scores below normal language pathology clinic on the hospital

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


FOCUS ON AUTISM AND OTHER DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES
168

campus. This was the typical location for complex. Finally, the novel activities con- within a speech–language pathology ses-
the children’s therapy sessions. The chil- sisted of experiences within the therapy sion. Table 1 contains a list of the specific
dren were familiar with and comfortable sessions that were quite different from target skills by task classification and an
in this room. There were generally two typical events, including having a birth- example for each task.
sessions per 1-hour therapy appointment. day celebration and making a video. Five skills were targeted in each ses-
There were four different activities in sion, resulting in a possibility of a child
each event classification, for a total of 16 receiving five points per session (one
Design
novel events. point per skill). A child received credit for
A reversal (ABAB) design (Alberto & The event categories and activities using the skill if he completed it inde-
Troutman, 2003) was used to analyze were designed to be generally similar in pendently or with one prompt. The ex-
participants’ participation in the novel complexity, duration, and functionality ception to this criterion was the skill of
events without Social Stories (baseline) to allow for comparison of all sessions remaining on task. Due to the age of the
and with Social Stories (intervention). and phases. Barlow and Hersen (1973) children, a child received credit for re-
Within each phase, four types of novel supported similar phase lengths to ensure maining on task if he received two or
events were targeted: setting changes, that observed changes are due to the im- fewer prompts to remain on task. All of
novel toys presented by an unfamiliar plementation of a treatment variable. the necessary skills and vocabulary, with
person, purchases, and novel activities The specific activities were counterbal- the exception of remaining on task, were
occurring within the session. These clas- anced across phases and participants. In- addressed explicitly in Social Stories and
sifications of events were chosen because dividuals with ASD frequently have dif- then reiterated in vivo within the ses-
they represent several skills that children ficulty with generalization (Quill, 2000; sions.
encounter as part of home, school, and Siegel, 1996). Therefore, the children On-task behavior was observed when
community life. The order of activities were not expected to have mastery of the the child was engaged in an activity re-
within the events was counterbalanced various skills that were repeated in all of lated to the task, a discussion regarding
for each participant. the novel contexts. the task, or a social discussion with an-
Some of the novel settings included other person during extended transition
leaving the therapy room and going to a times such as walking to a location or
Data Collection
fish pond on the hospital campus, and waiting for the next step. Individual
going to the top floor of the parking deck Five behaviors in four categories were novel events lasted at least 10 minutes.
to see the view. The novel toys were all targeted for each of the novel routines. Data for on-task behavior were collected
recently purchased and had multiple Data on the occurrence or nonoccur- during the first 10 minutes of the event
steps for assembly and use. The first au- rence of specific behaviors were collected if it lasted longer than the targeted time.
thor confirmed with the parents that during all target sessions via event Two prompts were allowed for on-task
their child had not played with the toy recording on a checklist. The four be- because this was deemed typical accord-
previously. The directions for using the havior categories were chosen for the fol- ing to the children’s ages.
novel toys were presented by an unfamil- lowing reasons: (a) they represent what is Two key tasks were identified for each
iar person because the participants were observed when a child is participating in activity. They generally required using
accustomed to listening to directions an event, (b) they are common deficits necessary materials, following directions
from the first author. Examples of pur- for children with ASD, (c) they could be to continue the activity, or following the
chase locations included a gift store and observed in all of the activities, and rules of a game. The child was requested
a convenience store, both in the hospital (d) they reflect skills frequently targeted to do tasks by the adult leading the in-

TABLE 1
Examples of Target Skills for Each Novel Event Category
Skill Setting change Purchase Novel toy Novel event

Example Memorial fountain Fund-raiser purchase Kerplunk Make a video


Attention Remain on task Remain on task Remain on task Remain on task
Key Task 1 Comment Choose a snack Push sticks through tube Follow a script
Key Task 2 Read a sign Give money Twist the tube Say, “cut”
Vocabulary Memorial Delicious Kerplunk Rehearsal
Request Directions Ask what adult wants Marbles Ask how to turn camera on

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


VOLUME 19, NUMBER 3, FALL 2004
169

teraction, who was either an unfamiliar pendently or with minimal assistance. sulting stories met the requirements of
person (novel toy sessions) or the first au- Minimal assistance was defined as one face validity.
thor (all other sessions). For example, rephrasing of the stimulus cue that an The actual storybooks were made on
when looking at a fountain in the novel item or material was necessary. black 81⁄2" × 11" construction paper,
setting situation, the child was asked to As dependent variables, the five be- folded in half horizontally. The text was
“read” a sign for important information. haviors were clearly defined and objec- printed on white paper in 16-point Arial
In the cafeteria for the purchase situa- tively observed. Therefore, it was appro- and mounted on the upper half of the
tion, the child was instructed to give priate for the first author to collect the page. Digital photographs of key infor-
money to the cashier. A child was given data, although she was not blind to the mation and black-and-white line draw-
credit for using the targeted key skill if it purpose of the study. ings, using the Boardmaker software
was performed independently or with (King, 1995), of general information were
minimal assistance. Minimal assistance placed on the lower half of each page.
Interobserver Reliability
was defined as cueing by gesture, model- For example, there were photos of the
ing the behavior, or indirectly referenc- The second author collected interob- toys and their pieces, the various settings,
ing the necessary behavior. A key task was server reliability data on the five behav- such as the gift shop, and the pillows
scored as not occurring if partial or full iors for 20% of the sessions across all where the children were asked to sit to
physical assistance was needed, or if the phases. These sessions were videotaped watch their videos. Icons were used in-
child had the target skill done for him. and the second observer was able to view stead of pictures for any illustrations in
The target vocabulary word for each them independently. The video camera which the person or purchase item could
session was modeled at least five times in was placed in an inconspicuous place change (such as a cashier). Icons were
the natural context of the event for all within the treatment room of the first au- also used instead of pictures for nonspe-
baseline and intervention phases. The thor or in an observation room with a cific concepts such as filthy and sorting.
target word was also used and defined in speaker behind one-way glass. Partici- Generally there was one picture per page,
each of the Social Stories. To receive pants were not aware of the video process but some pages had more than one illus-
credit for the vocabulary skill, the child although parents had provided consent tration to help clarify the text. The sto-
had to use the target word appropriately for the taping. The videotapes were ries were laminated then folded together
in the context of the event. Immediate erased following the interobserver relia- and secured with pipe cleaners running
echolalia, without regard to context, did bility procedure. The second observer through the middle of the book. All
not count as using the target word. was trained in the operational definitions three children used the same set of
Finally, during each session necessary and use of the data collection form. In- books. As an example, the text of the
information or a necessary item was terobserver reliability was determined by story for the birthday party is presented
withheld from the child to determine if calculating the scores for each target ses- in Figure 1.
he would make a request. The child was sion and counting the number of agree-
cued that an item or information was ments between the two observers divided
Procedure
necessary, but was not explicitly told to by the number of agreements plus dis-
request it. For example, in the cafeteria, agreements multiplied by 100. Mean in- Approximately 1 week prior to the actual
the adult said that she was hungry for a terobserver reliability across all phases event, the first author met individually
snack. The child was expected to request was 89%. with one parent of each child for train-
information to learn what she wanted to ing. Parent training included an intro-
eat. During the novel games, essential duction or review of Social Story theory
Materials
pieces from the toys were withheld. The and instructions on presenting the Social
child was expected to inquire about the Following Gray (1994) and Gray and Story. The parents had varying degrees of
missing pieces. When making a video, Garand’s (1993) criteria, the first author prior knowledge of Social Stories; there-
the child was told to turn on the camera, wrote a Social Story for each of the novel fore, the content of the introduction was
but not told how to do so. Strategies for routines used in the study. The first and controlled with a training checklist. The
successfully completing the requests second author collaborated on the revi- use of the checklist assured that all of the
were provided in the Social Stories. The sions for the final stories. In addition, a necessary information was reviewed with
stories contained phrasing that suggested third person, also familiar with Social each parent. A copy of the checklist is in-
the children could ask for help if they Stories, read 11 of the 16 of the stories cluded as Appendix A. The parents were
needed information or assistance. To be (69%) before they were used in the in- provided with phone numbers to contact
scored as occurring, the child was ex- tervention. The third reader agreed that the first author with any questions re-
pected to request the item or informa- the stories adequately explained the garding presentation of the Social Sto-
tion to continue the event. A child was novel events, were written with a reas- ries.
given credit for requesting materials or suring quality, and contained the proper During intervention phases, each
assistance if he made the request inde- sentence-type ratio. Therefore, the re- week parents were given two Social Story

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


FOCUS ON AUTISM AND OTHER DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES
170

The next time I go to see Michelle, it will be a very special day. It will be Michelle’s birthday. Everyone has a birthday one time a year.
Usually people like to have a party to celebrate. We will have a party for Michelle’s birthday.

To get ready for the party, we will decorate the room. We will tape streamers on the table. Streamers are long papers that have pretty
colors and hang down toward the floor. We might have a balloon at Michelle’s party. We can tape the balloon to the table by the long
streamers.

Next we will decorate Michelle’s cake. The cake will already be made. We’ll decorate it using frosting and sprinkles. We can use the
frosting to write her name, initials, or just “happy birthday” on the cake. We can use sprinkles to put pretty colors on the cake.

Next we need to set the table for eating the cake and drinking juice. We will need a plate, cup, and napkin for each person at the
party. There will be only two people at Michelle’s party, Michelle and me. I can put a plate, cup, and napkin on the table for each of us.

After the table is set, it will be time for the party. Michelle will sing “Happy Birthday.” Then Michelle will cut the cake, and we can eat
and drink. We can talk about the party while we’re eating and drinking.

We will have fun until the party is over. I will take the long streamers off of the table and put them in a bag. Then we will clean up
everything else. It will be time to do something else.

FIGURE 1. Full text of the birthday party Social Story, separated by pages. Key tasks included setting plates on the table and
decorating the cake. The vocabulary word was streamers. The boys needed to request tape.

books for the following week’s target ac- The investigation lasted for 12 weeks ing for attention was consistently main-
tivities. Parents were instructed to intro- for Adam and Hal and 11 weeks for Ron, tained for 10 minutes. Finally, parents
duce the stories and read them to their due to absences. The only change in the were provided with a daily checklist to
child one time per day for 5 days prior to procedure occurred during the cafeteria use at home to document when they read
the events. Parents were responsible for trip for Ron. The cafeteria was closed at the stories to their child. There was space
choosing a time of day when the child the time of the session; therefore, mak- on the parent form for writing questions
was calm and receptive to listening to a ing a purchase was not possible. A vend- or comments that they or the child had
story. None of the children had sufficient ing machine area was substituted for the during the reading.
skills for independent story reading. The cafeteria. Ron spontaneously used the
parents were instructed to answer any target vocabulary word, cashier, when he
questions the children asked sponta- asked, “Will there be a cashier at the Results
neously. Parents were provided with a vending machine?” Inserting money into
record form to document the reading the machine was substituted for giving it Figure 2 depicts the boys’ participation
times, questions, and responses. Parents to a cashier; the rest of the activity was in the various novel events introduced
were asked to keep the stories available to completed as designed. during the routine speech–language
the child throughout the day. Finally, therapy sessions. Each phase consisted of
parents read the Social Stories to their four sessions, which are represented on
Treatment Fidelity
child just prior to going to therapy. the horizontal axis. Below the session
On the days of the sessions, the par- Treatment fidelity was maintained number is a letter indicating the type of
ticipants worked on specific speech– throughout the intervention. All of the novel event. The vertical axis indicates
language pathology activities as always; target skills were predetermined and the number of participation skills the
however, there were one or two novel maintained on a list. This list was used as boys used within each activity. The max-
events in the therapy time. There was a a reference prior to each session and tar- imum number of participation skills for
period of familiar activity between two get skills were transferred to the data col- each session was five.
target activities, allowing the child to lection form. Operational definitions for During the initial baseline phase, Ron
relax, if necessary. During target activi- each skill were created and reviewed fre- exhibited a mean of 2.75 points out of
ties, the first author used event-recording quently to determine occurrence or non- a possible 5.0 points (55%). In the first
data collection based on a predetermined occurrence of a given skill. For consis- Social Story intervention phase, Ron’s
checklist of target behaviors. Start times tency, the examiner modeled the target mean number of observed participation
and specific target skills that had been vocabulary word at least five times per ac- skills increased to 4.25 (85%), a 30% in-
previously operationally defined were on tivity for sessions with and without Social crease in the number of skills observed
the data recording page. Skills were Stories. This was tallied on the data col- with Social Story preparation. In the with-
scored as 0 (if not demonstrated) or 1 (if lection form. Start times were also noted drawal (second baseline) phase, Ron’s
demonstrated according to criteria). on the data collection form so that tim- mean number of skills returned to the

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


171

FIGURE 2. Number of targeted skills observed during therapy activities without and with
Social Story preparation. The type of activity is coded as follows: S = setting change, P =
purchase activity, T = novel toy, E = novel event.

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


FOCUS ON AUTISM AND OTHER DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES
172

preintervention level of 2.75 (55%), a Of primary importance is teaching the he liked the books and would read them
30% decline. The second introduction of child with ASD how and when to use their again. He also said they did help him play
Social Stories again increased his use of communication and interaction skills by games. When asked to tell the evaluator
targeted skills. His mean score was 4.25 reading and understanding social rules and about the books, he reported, “They
out of 5.0 points (85%), again a 30% in- conventions, as well as by following the were great. I want some more books.”
crease with Social Stories. rules of social communication and play Ron’s mother reported that Ron asked
Adam initially had a mean of 2.75 schemes inherent in play interactions. for stories to help with other recent so-
points out of 5.0 possible points (55%). (p. 215) cial situations at home.
His scores increased 30% to 4.25 (85%) Adam gave an emphatic “yes” when
with the addition of Social Stories. The use of Social Stories to assist in es- asked if he liked the books. He agreed
Adam’s mean score decreased by 35% to tablishing new routines and changes in that they helped him play games and do
2.5 (50%) when the stories were re- routine addresses both of these areas. activities, and that he would like to read
moved. With the reintroduction, his The technique provided support for un- them again sometime. He did not have
mean increased to 4.0 (80%), a gain of derstanding, accepting, and working in a any further comments regarding Social
30% with the Social Stories. changed environment. Stories.
During the initial baseline phase, Hal Following the intervention, parents Hal also agreed that he liked the books
exhibited a mean of 2.25 points of the were asked to complete a social validity and would like to read them again some-
possible 5.0 points (45%). His perfor- instrument (Appendix B). Results were day. He did not respond to the question
mance increased to 3.0 (60%) with the used to establish information regarding about whether the books helped him
introduction of Social Stories, a 15% im- ease of presentation of the Social Stories, play games. Hal’s mother observed that
provement. Hal’s participation decreased likeliness of using the intervention in he attended to these books more than
10% to 2.0 (40%), when the stories were novel routines, and parents’ perceived ef- regular books, possibly because they per-
withdrawn. Finally, Hal had a mean of ficacy of the intervention. Two of the tained directly to him. Hal’s mother also
3.5 points (70%) during the second in- parents indicated that they felt the Social observed an increase in Hal’s anticipa-
tervention phase, a 20% increase from Stories were highly effective and applica- tion and excitement when he was getting
the withdrawal condition. ble to a variety of situations. They also ready for therapy sessions.
All three boys evidenced an improve- agreed that the stories would have lasting
ment in the percentage of participation improvements in their children’s func-
tioning. These two parents reported that
Discussion
skills observed from the baseline through
the initial Social Story phase. The range they were already using the stories in The purpose of this study was to investi-
of increase was from 15% to 30%. Subse- other situations. Both parents sponta- gate the extent to which participation in
quently, all three evidenced a decrease in neously commented that they noticed novel situations within familiar routines
the percentage of skills observed when that their sons exhibited decreased anxi- increased with the use of Social Stories
the Social Story intervention was discon- ety and disruptive behaviors with the use for three boys with ASD. All three boys
tinued. The range of decrease was be- of Social Stories. The other parent felt evidenced an increase in targeted partic-
tween 10% and 35%. Finally, all three the stories were quite effective and highly ipation skills when they were prepared
participants evidenced an increase in the useful in a variety of situations. She be- for the novel events with Social Stories.
mean percentage of skills observed with lieved, however, that there would be only The range of the increase was between
the reintroduction of Social Story prepa- a fair amount of lasting change and that 15% and 30% from baseline measures.
ration. These data demonstrate a func- the strategy might not be as effective in The results of this study confirm the
tional relationship between use of Social the home. This third parent did not at- benefit of using Social Stories with chil-
Stories and subsequent increases in par- tempt to use Social Stories indepen- dren with ASD as reported by others
ticipation for novel situations in a famil- dently. The presence of two enthusiastic (Dettmer et al., 2000; Hagiwara & Myles,
iar environment. parents and one more reticent parent is 1999; Kuoch & Mirenda, 2003; Kuttler
substantiated by Smith (2001), who re- et al., 1998; Norris & Dattilo, 1999; Rowe,
ported that a majority of people who im- 1999; Swaggart et al., 1995). In addi-
Social Validity plemented Social Stories with children tion, these results extend current knowl-
and adolescents with ASD and other dis- edge because they confirm the benefit of
Harris (1994) explained that “people abilities found the stories to be beneficial, including Social Stories in preparation
with autism need to learn to tolerate easy to use, and pleasurable. for a novel event. This study also differed
change if they are to function effectively After the study concluded, each child from many of the previous studies in that
in the world, and learning to manage the was asked if he liked reading the books, photographs of the specific aspects of
stress of change is therefore an important if the books helped him to play the games each event were included in the stories.
lesson in childhood” (p. 84). Prizant, and do activities, and if he would like to This was done to help make the novel as-
Wetherby, and Rydell (2000) stated, read the stories again. Ron indicated that pects of each event more familiar.

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


VOLUME 19, NUMBER 3, FALL 2004
173

The functional relationship between Reading Social Stories daily for 5 days session setting; they were not necessarily
Social Story preparation and increased prior to introducing a novel event al- novel to the individual child. Although
participation in novel activities may be lowed the children opportunities to com- the activities were designed to be similar,
due to the decrease in the novelty of the ment or ask questions regarding the if a child had a prior experience with the
actual events. With prior preparation and upcoming event. The parents recorded event, it may have affected his perfor-
introduction to the activity, materials, vo- several questions the children asked dur- mance. The creation of four activities per
cabulary, and expectations, the children ing the readings, such as “Can I say, category of novel event, the creation of
in the study were able to focus more on ‘Cut’?” and “What are streamers?” The discrete expectations within the event,
participating in the event and less on parents recorded their children’s com- and the use of a counterbalanced design
comprehending and processing new in- ments that they were excited about the helped to control for prior experience.
formation. This directly relates to Gray’s upcoming session (e.g., “Tomorrow I When investigating a truly novel event,
(1994) description of Social Stories as a can go to Michelle’s!”). They indicated such as an airplane ride or attending a
means for the individual to understand that they liked the stories (e.g., “That theater performance, there could be no
social situations and expected responses. was a good story.” “I want to read the baseline data for comparison.
In addition, the current results are simi- books, Mommy.”). They commented Another limitation was the predeter-
lar to those of Ferrara and Hill (1980), about the content of the stories (e.g., “I mined number of sessions per phase,
who found increases in engagement and think the frogs are pretend.” “The stairs rather than allowing data to reach levels
play within familiar environments. won’t scare you.”). Finally, one partici- of stability before moving to the next
Although the boys made substantial pant encountered a story about an activ- phase. This was done for two reasons:
gains in participation with the introduc- ity that he did not want to do. When (a) because children with ASD frequently
tion of Social Stories, there were still er- reading the story about previewing and have scattered performance, stability
rors or omission of skills. During the watching the video, Hal told his mother, could be difficult to obtain, and (b) the
intervention phases, the most frequent “I don’t love it. I don’t like a preview. I arrangement allowed for representation
errors were decreased attention and a want to play Mr. Potato Head.” Creating of each type of activity in each phase.
failure to ask for information, particularly this forum for interaction and discussion However, the limited number of sessions
failing to ask what the first author wanted between a child with ASD and his parent may have restricted the natural path of
to eat in the purchase situation. Atten- is an important goal in communication the results.
tion to task is generally a deficit area for therapy. Discussing the concepts of fu- The next limitation is the small num-
children with ASD (Quill, 2000). At- ture events and time sequences in a func- ber of targeted skills available for analysis
tending to the activity was not explicitly tional format is also beneficial. The va- in each session. To keep the various ses-
mentioned in the text of the stories as riety and extent of these child–parent sions as homogenous as possible, a lim-
other target skills were. Rather it was an interactions are exciting byproducts of ited number of skills were targeted in
anticipated outcome of increased prepa- this intervention. each. The children actually participated
ration. Although decreased attention was The effectiveness of Social Story use in in many more aspects of the activities
a frequent error type, overall demonstra- novel routines for children with ASD is than were charted on the data forms.
tion of the skill across participants was beneficial for generalization to many Finally the use of an ABAB design
better with than without Social Stories. events and situations. This method could could have resulted in some carryover
Overall, there was no credit given for the be generalized to school field trips, test- effect as was observed in the work by
key task of attention in only 6 of the total ing schedules, or fire drills. It could apply Kuoch and Mirenda (2003). To help
sessions with Social Stories as compared at home to babysitters or weekend plans. control for carryover, none of the actual
to 10 of the sessions without stories, a In the community, stories may help with activities were repeated for any one par-
40% increase in participant attention with change from routine expectations. The ticipant. Although there were only four
Social Stories. During purchasing ses- use of the stories allows the children with classifications of activities, there were
sions, requesting information errors were ASD increased opportunity for participa- four distinctly different activities within
observed with all three children. During tion, learning, enjoyment, and interac- each classification. In addition, any carry-
many of the trials, the boys offered the tions. over effect was not substantial, because
researcher something that they chose. all three participants dropped to near-
When this item was refused, the child got baseline levels during the second baseline
Limitations
another item and offered it. This was a phase.
consistent strategy for all three partici- There were limitations in this study that
pants. Therefore, requesting another’s should be addressed. First, as previously
Future Investigations
preference may be a skill that needs to be mentioned, using novel events in re-
taught with focused intervention tech- search created a dilemma in designing The benefits of Social Stories are becom-
niques, rather than indirectly in the So- this study. The events that were used were ing increasingly evident. The addition of
cial Story. novel to the speech–language pathology this information regarding using Social

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


FOCUS ON AUTISM AND OTHER DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES
174

Stories in novel events within familiar set- Bracken, B. (1998). Bracken basic concept Hresko, W., Reid, D. K., & Hammill, D.
tings is the beginning of another path of scale–Revised. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt (1999). Test of early language development–
investigation. As previously mentioned, Assessment. Third edition. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
further research could investigate the Brownell, M. (2002). Musically adapted So- Jones, D., Swift, D., & Johnson, M. (1988).
cial Stories to modify behaviors in students Nondeliberate memory for a novel event
benefits of Social Stories in truly novel
with autism: Four case studies. Journal of among preschoolers. Developmental Psy-
situations. In addition, reading the story
Music Therapy, 39, 117–144. chology, 24, 641–645.
for 5 days before the event was an ar- King, D. (1995). Boardmaker [Computer
Brownell, R. (Ed.). (2000). Expressive one-
bitrary number. Reading the story just software]. Solana, Beach, CA: Mayer John-
word picture vocabulary test. San Antonio,
prior to the event is customary. Further son.
TX: Harcourt Assessment.
investigation may consider the optimum Klinger, L., & Dawson, G. (1995). A fresh
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Test for audi-
amount of time a Social Story is needed look at categorization abilities in persons
tory comprehension of language–Third edi-
prior to a novel experience. This study tion. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
with autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov
was completed with only three partici- (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism
Committee on Educational Interventions for
pants. Replication with more participants (pp. 119–136). New York: Plenum Press.
Children with Autism, Division of Behav-
and participants with different skill levels Kuoch, H., & Mirenda, P. (2003). Social
ioral and Social Sciences and Education,
Story interventions for young children with
will be beneficial in understanding the National Research Council. (2001). Edu-
autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism
best use of Social Stories in novel events. cating children with autism. Washington,
and Other Developmental Disorders, 18,
Finally, empirical research needs to be DC: National Academy Press.
219–227.
done to explore whether preparation for Dettmer, S., Simpson, R., Myles, B., & Ganz, Kuttler, S., Myles, B., & Carlson, J. (1998).
a specific novel event supports general- J. (2000). The use of visual supports to fa- The use of Social Stories to reduce precur-
ization to other novel events, similar and cilitate transitions of students with autism. sors to tantrum behaviors in a student with
dissimilar. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental autism. Focus on Autism and Other Devel-
Disabilities, 15, 163–169. opmental Disabilities, 13, 176–182.
Ferrara, C., & Hill, S. (1980). The respon- Lorimer, P., Simpson, R., Myles, B., & Ganz,
ABOUT THE AUTHORS siveness of autistic children to the pre- J. (2002). The use of Social Stories as a pre-
dictability of social and nonsocial toys. ventative behavioral intervention in a home
Michelle L. Ivey, MA, CCC-SLP, is a speech–
Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- setting with a child with autism. Journal of
language pathologist and current doctoral stu-
orders, 10, 51–57. Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 53–60.
dent in special education. Her interests include
Fivush, R., Hudson, J., & Nelson, K. (1984). MacDuff, G., Krantz, P., & McClannahan, L.
improving language and social communica-
Children’s long-term memory for a novel (1993). Teaching children with autism to
tion, especially for children with autism spec-
event: An exploratory study. Merrill- use photographic activity schedules: Main-
trum disorders. L. Juane Heflin, PhD, is an
Palmer Quarterly, 30, 303–316. tenance and generalization of complex re-
associate professor who coordinates the personnel
Gardner, M. (1997). Test of auditory percep- sponse chains. Journal of Applied Behavior
preparation program at Georgia State Univer-
tual skills–Revised. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Analysis, 26, 89–97.
sity that emphasizes autism spectrum disorders.
Gray, C. (1994). The Social Story kit. In McClannahan, L., & Krantz, P. (1999). Ac-
Paul Alberto, PhD, is a professor of special ed-
C. Gray (Ed.), The new Social Story book tivity schedules for children with autism:
ucation at Georgia State University. His cur-
(pp. 215–224). Arlington, TX: Future Ho- Teaching independent behavior. Bethesda,
rent interests include community intensive
rizons. MD: Woodbine House.
instruction and literacy for students with mod-
Newcomer, P., & Hammill, D. (1997). Test of
erate and severe disabilities. Address: Michelle Gray, C. (2000). Writing Social Stories with
language development: Primary–Third edi-
L. Ivey, Georgia State University, Educational Carol Gray. Arlington, TX: Future Hori-
tion. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Psychology and Special Education, MSC zons.
Norris, C., & Dattilo, J. (1999). Evaluating
6A0820, 33 Gilmer St., Atlanta, GA 30303- Gray, C., & Garand, J. (1993). Social Stories:
effects of a Social Story intervention on a
3086. Improving responses of students with
young girl with autism. Focus on Autism
autism with accurate social information.
and Other Developmental Disabilities 14,
Focus on Autistic Behavior, 8, 1–10.
REFERENCES 180–186.
Hagiwara, T., & Myles, B. (1999). A multi- Prizant, B., Wetherby, A., & Rydell, P.
Alberto, P., & Troutman, A. (2003). Applied media Social Story intervention: Teaching (2000). Communication intervention is-
behavior analysis for teachers (6th ed.). skills to children with autism. Focus on sues for young children with autism spec-
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice Autism and Other Developmental Disabili- trum disorders. In A. Wetherby & B.
Hall. ties, 14, 82–95. Prizant (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders: A
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Harris, S. (1994). Siblings of children with transactional developmental perspective (pp.
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental autism: A guide for families. Bethesda, MD: 193–224). Baltimore: Brookes.
disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washing- Woodbine House. Quill, K. (1997). Instructional considerations
ton, DC: Author. Howlin, P. (1998). Practitioner review: Psy- for young children with autism: The ratio-
Barlow, D., & Hersen, M. (1973). Single-case chological and educational treatments for nale for visually cued instruction. Journal of
experimental designs. Archives of General autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27,
Psychiatry, 29, 319–325. chiatry, 39, 307–322. 697–713.

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


VOLUME 19, NUMBER 3, FALL 2004
175

Quill, K. (1998). Environmental supports to with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior In- havioral skills to children with autism. Focus
enhance social communication. Seminars terventions, 2, 3–11. on Autistic Behavior, 10, 1–16.
in Speech and Language, 19, 407–423. Schuler, A. (1995). Thinking in autism: Dif- Thiemann, S., & Goldstein, H. (2001). Social
Quill, K. (2000). Do-watch-listen-say: Social ferences in learning and development. In K. Stories, written text cues, and video feed-
and communication intervention for chil- Quill (Ed.), Teaching children with autism: back: Effects on social communication on
dren with autism. Baltimore: Brookes. Strategies to enhance communication and so- children with autism. Journal of Applied Be-
Rowe, C. (1999). Do Social Stories benefit cialization (pp. 11–32). New York: Delmar. havior Analysis, 34, 415–446.
children with autism in mainstream primary Siegel, B. (1996). The world of the autistic Waltz, M. (1999). Pervasive developmental
schools? British Journal of Special Educa- child: Understanding and treating autistic disorders: Finding a diagnosis and getting
tion, 26, 12–14. spectrum disorders. New York: Oxford Uni- help. Sebastopol, CA: O’Reilly.
versity Press.
Schmit, J., Alper, S., Raschke, D., & Ryndak, Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler preschool and
Smith, C. (2001). Using Social Stories to en-
D. (2000). Effects of using a photographic primary scale of intelligence–Third edition.
hance behaviour in children with autistic
cueing package during routine school tran- San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
spectrum difficulties. Educational Psychol-
sitions with a child who has autism. Mental Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1989). Test of lan-
ogy in Practice, 17, 337–345.
Retardation, 38, 131–137. guage competence–Expanded edition. San
Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D.
Schopler, E., Mesibov, G., & Hearsey, K. (1985). Vineland adaptive behavior scales. Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
(1995). Structured teaching in the TEAACH Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Ser- Wiig, E., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (1992). Clin-
system. In E. Schopler & E. Mesibov (Eds.), vice. ical evaluation of language fundamentals–
Learning and cognition in autism (pp. 243– Steinbach, I. (1994). Samonas sound therapy. Preschool. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt As-
286). New York: Plenum Press. Kellinghusen, Germany: Teachau Verlag. sessment.
Schreibman, L., Whalen, C., & Stahmer, A. Swaggart, B., Gagnon, E., Bock, S., Earles, Wing, L. (2001). The autistic spectrum: A
(2000). The use of video priming to reduce T., Quinn, C., Myles, B., et al. (1995). parents’ guide to understanding and helping
disruptive transition behavior in children Using Social Stories to teach social and be- your child. Berkeley, CA: Ulysses Press.

APPENDIX A
Parent Training Checklist

Parent Name Date

Theory
1. Social Stories help the child understand why.
2. Social Stories answer questions the children don’t ask or don’t ask well.
3. The purpose is to describe, not direct.
4. Suggested uses for Social Stories include describing expected behavior, preparing for a novel event, and preparing for a new
routine.
5. Goal of the project is to see an increase in participation in a novel routine when the child has the benefit of the Social Story.
Also hoping for a decrease in resistance to change.

Procedure
1. I will make the books for the study.
2. Parent to read the story starting in 2 days (________), so it begins 5 days before the next therapy session.
3. Read the story daily at a time when child is able to attend to a story.
4. Answer any questions the child asks and record the question on the record.
5. Check the record page each day after you read the story.
6. Read it one more time just before coming to therapy.
7. Return the record page to me at the next session.

Comments and questions:

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015


FOCUS ON AUTISM AND OTHER DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES
176

APPENDIX B
Social Validity Instrument

Please read each item carefully and circle the number that best describes your response. Use the following scale to determine
how each statement reflects your opinion.
1 2 3 4 5
Not at all Just a little Somewhat Quite a bit Exactly

1. I believe Social Stories are useful for my child.


1 2 3 4 5
2. This intervention would be appropriate for a variety of situations.
1 2 3 4 5
3. This intervention would produce a lasting improvement in my child’s functioning.
1 2 3 4 5
4. I am likely to use this intervention again.
1 2 3 4 5
5. I would suggest this intervention to others.
1 2 3 4 5

Please write any comments that you have on the use of Social Stories.

Thank you for your time and participation in this project.

Downloaded from foa.sagepub.com at VITERBO UNIV on January 16, 2015

You might also like