You are on page 1of 17

This article was downloaded by: [New York University]

On: 02 June 2015, At: 20:55


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Clinical and Experimental


Neuropsychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20

Know the risk, take the win: How executive


functions and probability processing influence
advantageous decision making under risk
conditions
ab a c c
Matthias Brand , Johannes Schiebener , Marie-Theres Pertl & Margarete Delazer
a
General Psychology: Cognition, University of Duisburg-Essen, Duisburg, Germany
b
Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Essen, Germany
c
Clinical Department of Neurology, Innsbruck Medical University, Innsbruck, Austria
Published online: 25 Sep 2014.
Click for updates

To cite this article: Matthias Brand, Johannes Schiebener, Marie-Theres Pertl & Margarete Delazer (2014) Know
the risk, take the win: How executive functions and probability processing influence advantageous decision
making under risk conditions, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 36:9, 914-929, DOI:
10.1080/13803395.2014.955783

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2014.955783

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors
make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability
for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions
and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of
the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of
information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands,
costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or
indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution
in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 2014
Vol. 36, No. 9, 914–929, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2014.955783

Know the risk, take the win: How executive functions and
probability processing influence advantageous decision
making under risk conditions
Matthias Brand1,2, Johannes Schiebener1, Marie-Theres Pertl3, and Margarete Delazer3
1
General Psychology: Cognition, University of Duisburg-Essen, Duisburg, Germany
2
Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Essen, Germany
3
Clinical Department of Neurology, Innsbruck Medical University, Innsbruck, Austria

(Received 5 March 2014; accepted 13 August 2014)


Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

Recent models on decision making under risk conditions have suggested that numerical abilities are important
ingredients of advantageous decision-making performance, but empirical evidence is still limited. The results of
our first study show that logical reasoning and basic mental calculation capacities predict ratio processing and
that ratio processing predicts decision making under risk. In the second study, logical reasoning together with
executive functions predicted probability processing (numeracy and probability knowledge), and probability
processing predicted decision making under risk. These findings suggest that increasing an individual’s under-
standing of ratios and probabilities should lead to more advantageous decisions under risk conditions.

Keywords: Decision making; Numerical abilities; Executive functions; Mental calculation; Ratio processing.

Neuropsychological studies suggest that patients other with respect to the expected value. This is a
with decision-making deficits have a significantly numerical quantity denoting the mathematical
poorer social outcome and a lower quality of life value of an option by summarizing the probabil-
than other neurological patients (Bar-On, Tranel, ities and the heights of potential outcomes in one
Denburg, & Bechara, 2003). Understanding the cog- number. The expected value is calculated by the
nitive mechanisms involved in decision making is following formula: (gain × winning probability) –
thus of major importance for understanding patients’ (loss × losing probability) (see e.g., Friedman &
difficulties and developing intervention methods. Savage, 1948; Keeney & Raiffa, 1993). In contrast,
Decision situations differ with respect to the in situations of ambiguous risk (from now on
predictability of their consequences and the expli- labeled as “decisions under ambiguity”), a decision
citness of probability-information for a positive or maker is not aware of the probabilities of the
negative outcome to occur. In decisions under various outcomes and/or cannot predict the possi-
explicit risk (from now on labeled as “decisions ble consequences (e.g., Bechara, Damasio, Tranel,
under risk”), the situation is exactly defined, and & Damasio, 1997; Brand, Labudda, &
the rules, probabilities, and possible positive or Markowitsch, 2006). However, the decision
negative consequences (e.g., gains or losses) are maker can use the feedback of previous decisions
given explicitly or are calculable. Therefore, the to learn which alternatives lead to more positive
decision maker can list all possible outcomes with and which to more negative consequences.
the related positive and negative consequences and In the present study, we focus on decision mak-
can contrast the different options against each ing under risk conditions and on specific cognitive

We thank Ida Gradl, Nina Blume, and Theresa Pfanner for data collection, Elisa Wegmann and Gerrit Stöckigt for helpful
comments on the manuscript, and Mirko Pawlikowski for programming the GDT–Double.
Address correspondence to: Matthias Brand, Department of General Psychology: Cognition, University of Duisburg-Essen,
Forsthausweg 2, 47057 Duisburg, Germany (E-mail: matthias.brand@uni-due.de).

© 2014 Taylor & Francis


PROBABILITY PROCESSING AND DECISION MAKING 915

functions that may support advantageous choices. decisions are guided by good executive functions.
In particular, we are interested in how numerical Furthermore, Brand et al. (2006) have suggested
abilities and executive functions support the deci- an effect of numerical abilities and probability
sion-making process. knowledge on decision making under risk condi-
Previous investigations have attributed a major tions. If the situation provides explicit numerical
role to executive functions and logical reasoning information, knowledge about numbers (e.g., that
in several instances of decision making depending 200 is greater than 100) and probabilities (e.g., that
on the structure and the complexity of the a winning chance of 50% is higher than a winning
decision situation (Brand, Recknor, Grabenhorst, chance of 10%) is activated and combined with the
& Bechara, 2007; Schiebener, Wegmann, situational information represented in working
Pawlikowski, & Brand, 2013; Schiebener, memory. Though it seems theoretically plausible
Zamarian, Delazer, & Brand, 2011). Decisions that numerical abilities have an impact on making
under risk conditions with explicit and stable rules advantageous decisions under risk, empirical evi-
have been shown to be correlated with subcompo- dence is still needed.
nents of executive functions, such as cognitive flex- Empirical studies in the domain of health care
ibility, set-shifting, reasoning, and monitoring (e.g., suggest that people with low numerical abilities
Bazanis et al., 2002; Brand, Fujiwara, et al., 2005; have unfavorable preconditions for making advan-
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

Brand, Kalbe, et al., 2005; Brand et al., 2004; tageous decisions. They show limited understand-
Delazer, Sinz, Zamarian, & Benke, 2007; Delazer ing of risks and benefits associated with a specific
et al., 2009; Drechsler, Rizzo, & Steinhausen, 2008; medical treatment (Weinfurt et al., 2003), have less
Euteneuer et al., 2009; Fond et al., 2013; Schiebener insight into the benefits of cancer screening
et al., 2011; Zamarian, Sinz, Bonatti, Gamboz, & (Schwartz, Woloshin, Black, & Welch, 1997),
Delazer, 2008). Executive functions are needed for have less access to treatment options, such as kid-
developing strategies or, alternatively, for retrieving ney transplantations (Abdel-Kader et al., 2010),
them from long-term memory. After developing or show lower abilities in self-management of chronic
retrieving strategies from long-term memory, they disease (Cavanaugh et al., 2008), and are more
have to be compared, evaluated, and selected strongly influenced by emotional framing (Peters
(Brand et al., 2006). Categorization and cognitive et al., 2006; Reyna, Nelson, Han, & Dieckmann,
flexibility play a major role in the recall and selec- 2009; Zamarian, Benke, Buchler, Wenter, &
tion of strategies. Problem solving is essential for Delazer, 2010). Thus, previous studies suggest
deciding which strategy seems to be the best. that low numerical abilities may significantly influ-
Working memory is required for representing the ence decision making in a real-world context.
features of the current decision situation as well as In everyday decision situations, for example
for representing the information retrieved from when comparing price reductions or mortgage
long-term memory, for combining these aspects, rates, individuals are confronted not only with
and for generating or initiating an advantageous basic quantitative measures and simple calculation
decision strategy. In brief, executive components, problems, but with more complex concepts.
such as categorization and cognitive flexibility, Typically, numerical information provided in the
would trigger and control decision strategies, while health context contain concepts such as propor-
working memory would make relevant information tions, probabilities, risks, or survival rates
available (Brand et al., 2006). Furthermore, when expressed in frequencies, percentages, or fractions
the decision situation provides feedback about the (Reyna et al., 2009). These concepts pose major
consequences of decisions consequences, executive problems to adults with low education, to older
functions are thought to be involved in using this individuals (Delazer, Kemmler, & Benke, 2013),
feedback for monitoring the success of the current and to patients with mild cognitive impairment
decision-making strategy and for revising the strat- (Pertl et al., 2014), but also to young and highly
egy, if necessary. This is called the cognitive feed- educated adults (Lipkus, Samsa, & Rimer, 2001)
back route in the model suggested by Brand et al. and even to health professionals (Estrada, Barnes,
(2006). However, the model also suggests an emo- Collins, & Byrd, 1999; Hoffrage, Lindsey,
tional route of feedback processing, in which emo- Hertwig, & Gigerenzer, 2000; Sheridan &
tional signals learned from processing feedback Pignone, 2002). A typical error in understanding
may bias towards advantageous decisions options ratio concepts consists in the so-called denomina-
intuitively (Brand et al., 2006). tor neglect when people focus their attention on the
Overall, the above summarized model of deci- nominator and neglect the denominator. For
sion making under risk conditions, as suggested by example, they consider only how often an event
Brand et al. (2006), predicts that advantageous may happen without paying attention to the
916 BRAND ET AL.

overall number of events (Garcia-Retamero, and number processing. It is reasonable to assume


Galesic, & Gigerenzer, 2010). The denominator that also numerical abilities—simple and complex
neglect may lead to misinterpretations, biases in ones—constitute an important factor for making
judgment, and in consequence to nonoptimal advantageous decisions under risk conditions, as
decisions. suggested in the model by Brand et al. (2006).
In sum, executive functions play a major role in With the current two studies, we aimed at assessing
decisions under risk when rules are explicit and the potential interaction between executive func-
expected outcomes calculable. It may also be tions and numerical abilities—in particular, ratio
assumed that numerical abilities are relevant in and probability processing—and decision making
decisions under risk. However, it is not clear how under risk conditions, when explicit information
numerical abilities and executive functions interact about the options’ contingencies are numerically
in order to facilitate advantageous decision mak- provided. The first study assesses the relations
ing. Hypothetically, executive functions not only between basic mental calculation abilities, logical
have a direct effect on decision making, but facil- reasoning, processing of ratios, and decision mak-
itate numerical abilities in general and processing ing under risk. Moreover, it tests the hypothesis that
probabilities specifically, which in turn promote the influence of basic calculation abilities and num-
advantageous decisions. In fact, previous studies ber-based logical reasoning, respectively, on deci-
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

reported close relations between numerical abilities sion making is mediated by the ability to process
and executive functions whenever tasks require ratios. The second study investigates relations
updating, inhibition, monitoring, or strategy selec- between logical reasoning and executive functions,
tion. Brain imaging studies also provide ample higher order probability processing (ratio proces-
evidence that the prefrontal cortex, which is asso- sing and complex probability knowledge), and deci-
ciated with executive functioning, supports arith- sions under risk. More specifically, we assume that
metic processing depending on task demands the influence of executive functions—logical reason-
(review in Arsalidou & Taylor, 2011). Working ing, set-shifting, and working memory—on deci-
memory is essential in memorizing and retrieving sions under risk is partly mediated by ratio
arithmetic facts from long-term memory as well as processing as well as by specific knowledge about
in all more complex arithmetic operations requir- probabilities. Results of these two studies will allow
ing the temporary storage of intermediate results. us to picture important cognitive components of
Arithmetic processing has been shown to rely on decision making under risk conditions more
task management and monitoring abilities precisely.
(Mantovan, Delazer, Ermani, & Denes, 1999; In order to test our hypotheses, we developed a
Semenza, Miceli, & Girelli, 1997), on shifting modified version of the Game of Dice Task (GDT;
between different operations, on inhibiting over- Brand, Fujiwara, et al., 2005). In the original GDT
learned associations in calculation (Hodzik & participants have to guess which number will
Lemaire, 2011; Zamarian, Semenza, Domahs, appear in the next throw of a die. They are con-
Benke, & Delazer, 2007), and on flexibility in fronted with different options and can explicitly
adapting numerical strategies to changing demands evaluate the chance to win and the risk to lose
(e.g., Hodzik & Lemaire, 2011; Lemaire & (for a detailed description see Method section).
Arnaud, 2008). Executive functions also play an The modification to the original GDT was moti-
essential role in conceptual knowledge, which pro- vated by two limitations of the task. First, contin-
vides understanding of arithmetic principles and gencies between winning probabilities and
allows one to make inferences and to relate differ- expected values are rather simple and may present
ent information involved in arithmetic—for exam- no challenge to mathematically skilled individuals.
ple, recognizing that 6 × 17 equals 17 × 6 or that 9 Thus, we increased the mathematical complexity of
× 8 equals 80 – 8 (Delazer et al., 2004). Finally, the task. Second, the original GDT does not allow
executive functions proved to have an effect on differentiating conservative, risk-avoiding behavior
performance in a numeracy task (Delazer et al., from optimal choices. That means that the most
2013), where subjects were asked to perform simple conservative choice is also the most advantageous
mathematical operations on risk information using choice, while in real life extremely conservative
percentages and ratios and to convert numerical decisions are not always the best. The modified
expressions between percentages, proportions, and version of the GDT overcomes both disadvan-
probabilities (based on Lipkus et al., 2001). tages, but retains the original task’s characteristics.
Overall, several investigations suggest that In brief, the GDT–Double is mathematically more
executive functions are involved in decision complex than the original version and allows dis-
making under risk conditions and in arithmetic tinguishing not only between risky and nonrisky
PROBABILITY PROCESSING AND DECISION MAKING 917

TABLE 1
Test batteries of studies 1 and 2

Domain Task Main variable Study 1 Study 2

Decision making under risk GDT Double Mean expected value x x


Basic calculation abilities Mental calculation task Correct responses x
Number-based logical reasoning VESPAR verbal series Correct responses x x
Ratio processing Numeracy task Correct responses x x
Probability knowledge Correct responses x
questionnaire

Executive functions MCST Nonperseverative errors x


TMT–B Time needed x
Digit span backward Longest span of x
correctly repeated
digits

Note. GDT = Game of Dice Task; VESPAR = Verbal and Spatial Reasoning Test; MCST = Modified Card Sorting Test; TMT = Trail
Making Test; x = included in the test battery.
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

choices, but also between optimal and conservative die. Prior to each throw, subjects have to guess
advantageous choices as well as between different which number of the die (1–6) will be thrown.
grades of disadvantageous risk taking. They can choose one certain number or a combi-
The test batteries of both studies can be found nation of two, three, or four numbers. Each alter-
in Table 1. The tasks were administered in a native is associated with a certain amount of
pseudorandomized order. money dependent upon the winning probability:
€1000 gain/loss for the choice of a single number
(winning probability 16.67%), €500 gain/loss for
STUDY 1
two numbers (winning probability 33.33%), €200
gain/loss for three numbers (winning probability
Method
50%), and €100 gain/loss for four numbers (win-
ning probability 66.67%). Participants win the cer-
Subjects
tain amount of money if their guess was correct
In this study, 190 participants were included. (the number they chose is thrown) and lose the
The participants were recruited by local advertise- same amount of money if their guess was not
ments. The assessment was done individually in a correct (when another number is thrown). When
quiet setting. Mean age of the sample was 35.91 choosing an alternative that includes two numbers,
years (SD = 15.73, range 18–85 years). The sample such as the numbers “3” and “4,” the related gain/
included 99 females and 91 males. Mean school loss is €500. In this case, participants win €500 if
education was 11.88 years (SD = 0.49). None of either the number “3” or the number “4” is
the participants had a history of neurological or thrown, and they lose €500 if one of the other
psychiatric diseases as determined by a screening numbers not chosen occurs (the 1, 2, 5, or 6). The
interview. The study was approved by the local same principle applies for the combinations of
ethics committee. three and four numbers (winning if one of the
numbers chosen is thrown and losing if one of the
numbers not chosen is thrown). Before starting the
Instruments
game, participants are explicitly briefed about the
The Game of Dice Task–Double (GDT– rules for winning and losing, and the alternatives
Double). We used a modified version of the are explained explicitly in a standardized compu-
Game of Dice Task (GDT) to assess decision mak- ter-based manner. The gains and losses linked to
ing under explicit risk conditions with advanced the different alternatives are permanently shown
requirements on probability processing and mental on the screen; explicit winning probabilities (in
calculation abilities. The GDT is a computer-based terms of percentages) are not shown or explained.
task (Brand, Fujiwara, et al., 2005), which has After the instructions, the participants are asked
been used in more than 50 studies on decision whether they understood the task’s rules, and they
making under risk, so far. The participants’ goal have the option to go back to the instructions in
in the GDT is to maximize the fictitious starting case they have any further questions with respect
capital of €1000 within 18 throws of a single virtual to the game’s rules. Subjects are also instructed
918 BRAND ET AL.

that the task contains 18 trials and that they can GDT, as has been done previously—for example,
continue playing even if they have a negative bal- the number of riskiest choices (choosing one single
ance. After each throw, the gain or loss is indicated number) and the number of safest choices (choosing
on the screen accompanied by a distinct sound (the a combination of four numbers). We also analyzed
jingle of a cash machine for a gain; a dull tone for the mean expected value of all 18 decisions for each
a loss). The current total capital and the number of subject. The expected value was calculated by the
the remaining trials are consistently displayed on formula: (gain × winning probability) – (loss ×
the screen. losing probability). The expected values of the dif-
In this study, we used a modified version of the ferent options with and without doubling the gain/
GDT. After each choice and before the die is loss can be found in Table 2. Here, it is obvious that
thrown, the participants are asked whether they in the GDT–Double, in contrast to the original
would like to double the gain and the loss. For GDT, the most conservative behavior (choosing
example, a participant has chosen one single num- the four numbers alternative) is not the most advan-
ber—for example, the “3.” Following his or her tageous one. The best behavior in the GDT–Double
decision, he or she is asked whether the amount of is to choose the four-number alternative and to
money that can be won or lost should be doubled double the potential gains/losses. The riskiest beha-
(yes/no answer). If the answer is “yes,” the poten- vior in the GDT–Double is choosing a one-number
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

tial gain, but also the potential loss, is doubled alternative and to double the gain/loss. This overall
(e.g., gain/loss €2000). If the answer is “no,” the behavior is best represented by the mean expected
original gain/loss (e.g., €1000) is played for. By value, and therefore this score is the main dependent
using the double option, the expected values of variable, which was used in the structural equation
the alternatives change, with the exception of the models.
three-numbers alternatives (expected value remains
zero, irrespective of doubling or not). The alterna- Mental calculation task. In order to asses basic
tives, winning probabilities, gains/losses, and calculation abilities, participants answered mental
expected values of the alternatives in the GDT– calculation problems, as done in the study by
Double are displayed in Table 2. Delazer et al. (2013). They are asked to solve 16
For analyzing GDT performance, the net score calculation problems altogether (4 addition, 4 sub-
was calculated by subtracting the number of high- traction, 4 multiplication, 4 division problems).
risk choices from the number of low-risk choices. Each problem is presented visually on a separate
Accordingly, a positive net score indicates low risk, page. Response time is maximum 45 s. For each
thus advantageous performance, and a negative net operation, the first problem is simple and has one-
score indicates high risk, thus disadvantageous digit operands for addition, subtraction, and multi-
behavior. This net score has been used in most of plication (for example, 4 + 3; 9 – 3, 6 × 6) and two
the recent studies on GDT performance (e.g., digits and one-digit operands for division (for exam-
Brand, Laier, Pawlikowski, & Markowitsch, 2009; ple 20:4). The following problems are of increasing
Brand et al., 2007; Brand & Schiebener, 2013; difficulty (for example, 48:12; 39:9; 260:13).
Labudda et al., 2009; Pabst, Brand, & Wolf, 2013;
Schiebener et al., 2013; Schiebener et al., 2011). In The Verbal and Spatial Reasoning Test
addition, we also analyzed further variables of the (VESPAR). We used the verbal series subtest of
the VESPAR (Langdon & Warrington, 1995,
TABLE 2 1997) to examine logical reasoning within the num-
GDT–Double options, winning probabilities, gains/losses, and ber domain. This subtest consists of 25 items. In
expected values each item, a line of three numbers is shown on the
left side of the paper. In the right column, four
GDT double Winning Gain/ Expected
options probability (%) loss € value numbers are shown. Participants have to decide
which of the numbers presented on the right side
1 single number 16.67 2000 –1333.33 logically completes the consecutive series of num-
doubled
bers shown on the left side. One example is: 2, 4, 6
1 single number 16.67 1000 –666.67
2 numbers doubled 33.33 1000 –333.33 followed by a blank on the left side and 8, 5, 7, 0
2 numbers 33.33 500 –166.67 on the right side. The correct answer would be “8.”
3 numbers doubled 50 400 0 In contrast to the original version, we used a time
3 numbers 50 200 0 limit of five minutes to complete the task. This was
4 numbers 66.67 100 33.33 done to increase the difficulty of the task, conse-
4 numbers doubled 66.67 200 66.67
quently leading to higher variance in task perfor-
Note. GDT = Game of Dice Task. mance in the sample of brain-healthy participants.
PROBABILITY PROCESSING AND DECISION MAKING 919

Numeracy task. The Numeracy Task (Delazer mean square error of approximation (RMSEA;
et al., 2013) includes 12 items similar to Lipkus “test of close fit”; a value below .08 with a signifi-
et al.’s (2001) expanded numeracy scale (7 items) cance value below .05 indicates acceptable fit).
and measures ratio processing. Participants are
asked to convert percentages to proportions or to
assess different proportions (“Which of the follow- Results
ing numbers represents the highest probability to
remain healthy? 80 in 100, 10 in 1000 or 9 in 10?”). Descriptive values and correlations
Two items require the addition or subtraction of a The descriptive values of the main GDT–Double
defined percentage, and one item tests the under- variables and the scores in the additional neuropsy-
standing of dosage instructions given in a very chological tests administered are shown in Table 3.
short package insert. All 12 items are presented in The bivariate correlations between the GDT–
a medical context, framed either as positive infor- Double variables and the neuropsychological test
mation (e.g., chance of recovery) or as negative scores are displayed in Table 4. There were no
information (e.g., risk of getting a disease). effects of gender on GDT–Double performance
Answers are open (7 items) or have to be chosen (t = 0.229, p = .819). Age was correlated with
in a multiple-choice design (5 items; 3 alternatives GDT–Double performance with a small effect
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

each). Items are presented on the computer screen; (r = –.180, p = .013).


answers are given orally. Response time is limited
to 45 s per item.
The structural equation model

Statistical analyses Although the variable “calculation” (total score


of the mental calculation task) was not signifi-
Statistical standard procedures were carried out cantly correlated with the mean expected value in
with SPSS 22.0 for Windows (IBM SPSS Statistics, the GDT–Double (but with the net score), we
released 2013). Pearson correlations were calcu- nevertheless calculated the structural equation
lated to test for zero-order relationships between model on manifest dimension, with “VESPAR ver-
two variables. To control the data for outliers, all bal series sum score” and “mental calculation task
variables were regressed on a random variable. All sum score” as predictor variables, “mean expected
correlations with the random variable were very value in the GDT–Double” as dependent variable,
low, rs < .079, and the regression model with the and “numeracy task sum score” as mediator.
main variables (VESPAR verbal series, mental cal- Given that this model was calculated on manifest
culation task, numeracy task, and mean expected level, the model fit was good, with RMSEA < .001,
value in the GDT–Double as predictors and the CFI = 1.000, TLI = 1.000, and SRMR < .001. The
random variable as dependent variable) was not χ2 test for the baseline model was significant, with
significant (R2 = .009, p = .789) indicating that χ2 = 113.56, p < .001. The direct effect from
there were no substantially influential outliers in VESPAR to mean expected value in the GDT–
any of the scores in the sample. Additionally, scat- Double was significant (β = .224, p = .004). Also,
terplots between the variables were controlled the direct effect from calculation to mean expected
visually. Again, no extreme outliers were found.
Therefore, the analyses were performed with all
subjects. The structural equation modeling (SEM) TABLE 3
analysis was computed with MPlus 6 (Muthén & Study 1: Description of the GDT variables and further neu-
Muthén, 2011). There were no missing data. ropsychological test scores
Before testing the full model, the fits of the latent Variable Mean SD
dimensions were also tested using confirmatory
factor analysis (CFA) in MPlus. For both SEM GDT net score 11.14 7.46
and CFA, maximum likelihood parameter estima- GDT 1 single number not doubled 0.45 0.98
GDT 1 single number doubled 0.34 0.99
tion was applied.
GDT 4 numbers not doubled 3.16 4.35
For the evaluation of model fits, we applied GDT 4 numbers doubled 5.06 5.41
standard criteria (Hu & Bentler, 1995, 1999). GDT Mean expected value –46.26 119.57
These are: the standardized root mean square resi- Mental calculation task (sum score) 14.93 1.48
dual (SRMR; values below .08 indicate good fit VESPAR verbal series (sum score) 17.04 3.31
Numeracy task (sum score) 10.23 1.85
with the data), comparative fit index and Tucker
Lewis index (CFI/TLI; values above .90 indicate a Note. GDT = Game of Dice Task; VESPAR = Verbal and
good fit, values above .95 an excellent fit), and root Spatial Reasoning Test.
920 BRAND ET AL.

TABLE 4
Study 1: Bivariate correlations between GDT variables and the scores in the additional neuropsychological tests administered

1
GDT
Mean EV 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

2 GDT net score .717***


3 GDT 1 single number not doubled –.714*** –.414***
4 GDT 1 single number doubled –.855*** –.756*** .441***
5 GDT 4 numbers not doubled .274*** .149* –.149* –.202**
6 GDT 4 numbers doubled .373*** .160* –.181* –.088 –.288**
7 Mental calculation task .048 .151* –.035 –.026 –.076 .116
8 VESPAR verbal series .293*** .307*** –.255*** –.255*** –.121 .184* .421***
9 Numeracy task .324*** .272*** –.256*** –.234** .030 .166* .502*** .490***

Note. GDT = Game of Dice Task; VESPAR = Verbal and Spatial Reasoning Test; EV = expected value. 1: Mean expected value
over the 18 trials of the GDT–Double.
p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001.
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

value in the GDT–Double was significant (β = –.206, had a history of neurological or psychiatric dis-
p = .008). The effects from VESPAR to numeracy eases as determined by a screening interview. The
(β = .338, p < .001), the effect from calculation to study was approved by the local ethics committee.
numeracy (β = .360, p < .001), and the effect from
numeracy to mean expected value in the GDT–
Double (β = .317, p < .001) were significant. The Instruments
indirect effect from VESPAR over numeracy to GDT–Double, VESPAR verbal series, and
mean expected value in the GDT–Double was numeracy task. We used the GDT–Double, the ver-
significant (β = .107, p = .001). Also, the indirect bal series of the VESPAR, and the numeracy task, as
effect from calculation over numeracy was signifi- described in the methods of Study 1. In addition to
cant (β = .114, p = .001). Overall, the direct and these tasks, we used the following instruments.
indirect effect resulted in a variance explanation of
15.8% (R2 = .158, p = .001) of GDT–Double perfor-
Modified Card Sorting Test (MCST). The
mance (mean expected value).
MCST (Nelson, 1976) assesses categorization,
When gender and age were included as additional
planning, and monitoring, as well as rule learning
predictors in the model, the model fit indices became
and set-shifting. In this computerized paradigm,
lower, indicating no model fit with the data with
participants are shown a deck of cards displaying
RMSEA = .193, CFI = .889, TLI = .502, and
a certain number (1–4) of different shapes (square,
SRMR = .046. This corresponds with the result
circle, triangle, or star) in a certain color (blue,
that there were no significant gender effects and
red, green, or yellow). The participants are asked
only small correlations between age and GDT–
to sort every card that appears on the screen to
Double performance on a bivariate level (see results
one of the four target cards (first line of the
in the Descriptive Values and Correlations section).
screen) without knowing the current sorting rule,
which can be the number of shapes, the shape
itself, or the color of the shapes. Visual and
STUDY 2 acoustic feedback is given to help participants
figure out the correct sorting rule. After six cor-
Method rectly sorted cards, participants are notified that
the rule has changed, and then they have to find
Subjects out the new sorting rule. The main measure of
In this study, 130 participants were included. MCST performance used in this study was the
The participants were recruited by advertisements, number of nonperseverative errors (incorrectly
Internet platforms, and e-mail lists. The assessment sorted cards not related to a relapse to the pre-
was done individually in a laboratory setting. vious sorting rule; fewer errors indicate better
Mean age of the sample was 28.81 years (SD = performance).
11.19, range 18–66 years). The sample included 85
females and 45 males. Mean school education was Trail Making Test–B (TMT–B). The TMT–B
11.25 years (SD = 1.84). None of the participants (Reitan, 1958; Reitan & Wolfson, 1995) measures
PROBABILITY PROCESSING AND DECISION MAKING 921

speed of processing with a focus on inhibitory variables. To control the data for outliers, all vari-
control and cognitive flexibility/set-shifting. We ables were regressed with a random variable. All
used the paper-and-pencil task, in which the parti- correlations with the random variable were very
cipants are confronted with a sheet showing low, rs < .096, and the regression model with the
encircled numbers (1–13) and letters (A–L). They main variables (VESPAR verbal series, MCST non-
are instructed to connect numbers and letters alter- perseverative errors, TMT–B, digit span backward,
nating in numerical, respectively alphabetical numeracy task sum score, sum score of the probabil-
order, starting with 1, proceeding to A, followed ity knowledge questionnaire, and mean expected
by 2, B, and so on, and ending with 13. The time value in the GDT–Double as predictors and the ran-
needed to fulfill the task was used as main measure dom variable as dependent variable) was not signifi-
(shorter times indicate better performances). cant (R2 = .075, p = .205), indicating that there were
no substantially influential outliers in any of the
Digit span backward. Working memory was scales in the sample. Additionally scatterplots
assessed with the subtest Digit Span Backward between the variables were controlled visually.
from the German version (Härting et al., 2000) of Again, no extreme outliers were found. Therefore,
the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (Wechsler, the analyses were performed with all subjects.
1987). In this subtest, digits are read out and The SEM analysis was computed with MPlus 6
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

have to be repeated by the participant in the (Muthén & Muthén, 2011) using the same methods
reverse order. The number of digits that have to and criteria as those described for study 1. There
be repeated will increase after two consecutive were no missing data.
trials, if at least one trial has been performed cor-
rectly. Here we used the longest span of correctly
repeated digits as the main measure. Results

Probability knowledge questionnaire. The prob- Descriptive values and correlations


ability knowledge questionnaire consists of eight
items assessing the knowledge and understanding The descriptive values of the main GDT–Double
of probabilities. All items are multiple-choice ques- variables and the scores in the additional neuropsy-
tions with four alternatives. The items are pre- chological tests administered are shown in Table 5.
sented on paper, and participants have to mark The bivariate correlations between the GDT–
the correct answer. Participants are instructed Double variables and the neuropsychological test
that they had to answer questions about probabil- scores are displayed in Table 6. There were no effects
ities. They are also informed that there are easier of gender on GDT–Double performance (t = 0.667,
questions as well as more difficult ones. p = .506). Age was not significantly correlated with
Furthermore, they are told that there is an overall GDT–Double performance, and again the correla-
time limit for answering the questionnaire (eight tion had a small effect, only (r = –.161, p = .068).
minutes). The participants are provided with a
calculator as well as an extra paper for auxiliary TABLE 5
calculation. For example, participants are asked Study 2: Description of the GDT variables and further neu-
what the chances are of heads coming up twice if ropsychological test scores
a fair coin is flipped twice: 0, .333, .5, or .25. Or: A Variable Mean SD
bowl is filled with 5 white and 5 black balls and
one by one the balls are drawn from the bowl, GDT net score 7.45 11.45
without putting them back in. On the first four GDT 1 single number not doubled 1.38 3.45
GDT 1 single number doubled 0.51 1.55
occasions a black ball was drawn from the bowl.
GDT 4 numbers not doubled 3.06 4.65
What is the possibility that a black ball is drawn on GDT 4 numbers doubled 3.46 4.33
the fifth draw? .024, .167, .004, .5. Performance in GDT mean expected value –112.07 205.80
the questionnaire was scored by the number of VESPAR verbal series (sum score) 18.11 2.88
correct answers (total score 0–8). MCST nonperseverative errors 5.68 4.87
TMT–B 64.65 22.25
Digit span backward 4.72 1.24
Statistical analyses Numeracy task (sum score) 9.45 2.05
Probability knowledge questionnaire 5.68 2.31
Statistical standard procedures were carried out (sum score)
with SPSS 22.0 for Windows (IBM SPSS Statistics, Note. GDT = Game of Dice Task; VESPAR = Verbal and
released 2013). Pearson correlations were calculated Spatial Reasoning Test; MCST = Modified Card Sorting Test;
to test for zero-order relationships between two TMT = Trail Making Test.
922 BRAND ET AL.

TABLE 6
Study 2: Bivariate correlations between GDT variables and the scores in the additional neuropsychological tests administered

1
GDT
Mean EV 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

2 GDT net score .852***


3 GDT 1 single number –.732*** –.646***
not doubled
4 GDT 1 single number –.712*** –.682*** .173*
doubled
5 GDT 4 numbers not .351*** .190* –.182* –.209*
doubled
6 GDT 4 numbers doubled .352*** .227** –.245** –.057 –.179*
7 VESPAR verbal series .275** .176* –.278** –.085 .049 .111
8 MCST nonperseverative –.201* –.131 .158 .086 .065 –.179* –.369***
errors
9 TMT–B –.203* –.166 .117 .190* –.077 –.017 –.380*** .232**
10 Digit span backward .070 .069 .028 –.149 –.016 .112 .147 .016 –.124
11 Numeracy task .430*** .382*** –.377*** –.207* .072 .285** .579*** –.241** –.198* .144
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

12 Probability knowledge .357*** .319*** –.282** –.242** –.011 .233** .460*** –.280** –.332*** .192* .579***
questionnaire

Note. GDT = Game of Dice Task; VESPAR = Verbal and Spatial Reasoning Test; MCST = Modified Card Sorting Test; TMT =
Trail Making Test; EV = expected value. 1: Mean expected value over the 18 trials of the GDT–Double.
p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001.

Latent dimensions of the proposed model in TABLE 7


confirmatory factor analysis Study 2: Coefficients of the manifest variables’ loadings on
the latent dimensions, tested with CFA in MPlus
In order to systematically test the proposed the-
Latent dimension Manifest variables β SE
oretical model, we first tested whether the latent
dimensions (executive functions and probability Executive VESPAR verbal series .870*** .074
processing) are acceptably represented by the functions
manifest variables. Therefore, CFA was performed MCST nonperseverative –.424*** .084
with these two latent dimensions. Overall, the CFA errors
TMT–B –.437*** .084
model had a good fit with the data. The RMSEA Digit span backward .183* .096
was .062, the CFI was .974, the TLI was .951, and Probability Numeracy task .820*** .062
the SRMR was .039. processing
The latent dimension “executive functions” was Probability knowledge .706*** .065
represented well by the scores in the four manifest questionnaire
variables. The only exception was “digit span Notes. VESPAR = Verbal and Spatial Reasoning Test;
backward,” although the loading was still signifi- MCST = Modified Card Sorting Test; TMT = Trail Making
cant. The second latent dimension “probability Test; CFA = confirmatory factor analysis.
*p ≤ .05. ***p ≤ .001.
processing” was also well represented by the scores
of the two manifest variables. Overall, the CFA
indicates that the latent dimensions are acceptably SRMR was .037. The χ2 test was not significant, χ2 =
represented by the manifest variables. All factor 14.06, p = .297. The χ2 test for the baseline model was
loadings and standard errors are shown in Table 7. significant with χ2 = 199.17, p < .001. In summary,
the data fit well with the proposed theoretical model.
Overall, 28.2% of the variance in the variable “mean
The full structural equation model
expected value in the GDT–Double” was signifi-
The proposed theoretical model with the mean cantly explained by the full SEM (R2 = .282, p =
expected value of the GDT–Double as dependent .003). The model and all direct and indirect effects
variable (on manifest level) and the predictor “execu- are shown in Figure 1.
tive functions” as latent variable and the mediator When adding gender and age as additional predic-
“probability processing” also as latent variable tors to the model, the model fit indices become
yielded a good fit with the data. The RMSEA was lower, indicating no model fit with the data with
.036, the CFI was .988, the TLI was .980, and the RMSEA = .105, CFI = .837, TLI = .762, and
PROBABILITY PROCESSING AND DECISION MAKING 923
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

Figure 1. Results of the structural equation model including factor loadings of the latent dimensions, β-weights, p-values, and residuals
(*p ≤ .05, ***p < .001). GDT = Game of Dice Task; VESPAR = Verbal and Spatial Reasoning Test; MCST = Modified Card Sorting
Test; TMT = Trail Making Test; EV = expected value.

SRMR = .103. Again, this corresponds with the result


that there were no significant gender effects and no
significant correlations between age and GDT–
Double performance on a bivariate level (see results
in Descriptive Values and Correlations section).

ADDITIONAL ANALYSES WITH THE


COMBINED SAMPLES OF STUDY 1 AND
STUDY 2

In order to better understand task performance in


the GDT–Double and to address the additional
goal of the studies—namely, to differentiate
between conservative and optimal performance in
the GDT–Double, we combined the two samples,
Figure 2. Relative proportion of the choices made in the GDT–
resulting in a total sample of n = 320 subjects (mean Double in the whole sample (n = 320, subjects from both studies
age = 33.02, SD = 14.47 years, 136 males). The combined). GDT = Game of Dice Task.
relative proportions of choosing the eight different
options provided in the GDT–Double (one number,
two numbers, three numbers, and four numbers,
each with the option to double the potential gain/ The GDT–Double mean net score in the total sam-
loss) are shown in Figure 2. Altogether, 42% of the ple was 9.64 (SD = 9.45), and the mean expected
choices in the GDT–Double represent safe choices value was –72.99 (SD = 163.23). There were no
with a positive expected value (choosing the four effects of gender (t = 0.853, p = .394) and no
numbers combination with or without doubling). correlations between GDT–Double performance
Among the risky choices with a negative expected and age (r = –.101, p = .073).
value (23% of all choices), the majority of decisions
were made for the alternative of two numbers
without doubling. Only 2% of all choices represent DISCUSSION
very risky and disadvantageous decisions (one sin-
gle number and doubling the potential gain/loss) The present study aimed at investigating the role of
with the lowest expected value of –1333.33. executive functions and numerical abilities in
924 BRAND ET AL.

decision making under explicit risk conditions. are a prerequisite to perform accurately.
Two studies were performed, the first focusing on However, low scores in the numeracy task are not
the effect of calculation abilities, logical reasoning, simply attributable to calculation difficulties, but
and ratio processing on decision making, the sec- more often reflect systematic misunderstandings of
ond on the impact of executive functions and prob- ratio concepts or of the decimal system. For exam-
ability processing—that is, ratio processing and ple, in previous studies subjects often had difficul-
probability knowledge. A further aim was to assess ties to convert a frequency measure (4 out of 1000)
healthy individuals’ decision performance in a into a percentage (“0.4 percent”), typically failing
modified version of the GDT allowing the fine- by a power of 10 (“4 percent”; Delazer et al., 2013;
grained differentiation between optimal, conserva- Pertl et al., 2014). Others showed the typical
tive, and risky choices. denominator neglect (Garcia-Retamero et al.,
First, results show that number-based logical 2010) and were unable to identify the largest of
reasoning and basic mental calculation capacities three different risks expressed as ratios (Delazer
predict ratio processing and that ratio processing et al., 2013; Lipkus et al., 2001). These results are
predicts decision making under risk. Secondly, in line with previous studies (Reyna & Brainerd,
number-based logical reasoning together with 2007; Reyna et al., 2009) showing that ratio con-
other executive functions predicts probability pro- cepts such as fraction, percentages, and propor-
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

cessing, and probability processing predicts deci- tions are especially difficult to understand and
sion making under risk. It should be noticed that pose difficulties not only to students, but also to
the amounts of variance explanation of decision- adults. As suggested by results of the present study,
making performance were moderate in both stu- people with high arithmetic competence in per-
dies (15.8% and 28.2%). These moderate variance forming mental calculation had also higher scores
explanations indicate that there are also further in the numeracy task requiring the understanding
factors influencing decision-making performance. of ratio concepts. This effect may be partly attrib-
Nevertheless, it makes sense that executive func- uted to calculation performance itself, as basic
tions, basic calculation capacities, and number- calculation is required in the numeracy task, but
based logical reasoning contribute to decision- also to the fact that people with higher calculation
making behavior in the GDT to a considerable abilities have more insight into ratio concepts.
extent and in a systematic manner. High competence in the numeracy task assessing
In sum, the results of the two studies converge ratio processing predicted better decision making
on the view that in decision making with explicit in a situation with exactly defined options, prob-
information about probabilities and possible out- abilities, and related consequences. Individuals
comes, the ability to process complex probability who have good understanding of ratio concepts
information acts as a mediator for the relationship as well as the competence to perform arithmetic
between more basic cognitive functions and deci- operations on these concepts frequently chose
sion-making performance. advantageous alternatives in the GDT–Double.
Calculation abilities predicted performance in These results are consistent with the theoretical
the numeracy task assessing the understanding model of decision making under risk proposed by
and manipulation of ratio concepts such as percen- Brand et al. (2006). This model assumes that basic
tages and proportions. The calculation task given knowledge about numbers and probabilities is a
in the present study includes simple arithmetic fundamental ingredient of making advantageous
facts (such as 6 × 6), but also more complex multi- decisions in situations that explicitly provide
digit problems that had to be performed mentally numerical information in terms of the amount of
(such as 32 × 4). Answering these calculation pro- potential gains and losses and/or in terms of (impli-
blems requires the retrieval of arithmetic fact cit or explicit) information about probabilities. The
knowledge from long-term memory as well as the present study is the first that supports this theore-
execution of mental calculation procedures or stra- tical assumption in an empirical study on
tegies (Dehaene & Cohen, 1995; Delazer, 2003; the GDT.
McCloskey, Caramazza, & Basili, 1985). The Calculation abilities, as measured in study 1,
more complex numeracy task (the mediator) showed not only an indirect positive effect on deci-
requires a basic understanding of ratio concepts, sion making mediated by ratio processing.
including proportions, risks, and percentages, as Surprisingly, there was also a direct negative effect.
well as the knowledge of when and how to perform Individuals performing well in mental calculation,
arithmetic operations on these ratio concepts. but not in ratio processing, performed poorly in the
Though the task probes a higher level of mathe- decision task. Seemingly, calculation abilities had a
matical understanding, basic calculation abilities paradoxical effect on decision making when they
PROBABILITY PROCESSING AND DECISION MAKING 925

were not accompanied by sufficient understanding of dependency (Brand, Roth-Bauer, Driessen, &
ratio concepts. We suggest future studies, to investi- Markowitsch, 2008), schizophrenia (Fond et al.,
gate whether this may be explained by the effect that 2013), and other groups (see Gleichgerrcht,
people with good calculation skills, but poor Ibanez, Roca, Torralva, & Manes, 2010). More
understanding of ratios, focus only on parts of the interestingly, executive functions have also been
given information (for example, on the different found as important moderators for the relationship
gains) without insight into the contingencies of between different individual characteristics and
advantageous choices. Future studies may also find decision making in the GDT. One example is the
a possible explanation for the paradoxical effect moderating effect of executive functions (e.g., mea-
when investigating the role of different types and sured by the MCST) on the relationship between
difficulties of calculation processes on decision age and decision making (Brand & Schiebener,
making. In the mental calculation task used in the 2013). It has been reported that only those older
current study there were easier and harder arithmetic adults with reduced executive functions show risky
problems. The easier ones may be solved by arith- decision making in the GDT, while those older
metic facts knowledge, while the harder ones require individuals with good executive functions perform
more complex calculations. One may assume that as well as young subjects on the GDT. This result
arithmetic facts knowledge and complex calculation is also emphasized by the nonsignificant correla-
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

abilities contribute differently to decision making tion between GDT–Double performance and age
under risk, which may explain the paradoxical in the current study (whole sample). Additionally,
findings. gender did not directly affect decision making in
A further result of the first study was that num- the GDT–Double. This is also consistent with pre-
ber-based logical reasoning directly predicts per- vious studies with even larger samples, in which no
formance on the GDT–Double, but that this effects of gender were found (e.g., Brand &
effect is partially mediated by the individuals’ abil- Schiebener, 2013). Another example is the interac-
ity to understand ratios. The result of the direct tion between executive functions and the applica-
effect of logical thinking fits well with a sum of tion of a certain decision strategy (Brand, Heinze,
previous investigations (Brand et al., 2009). Also, Labudda, & Markowitsch, 2008). Subjects with
the relationship between logical thinking and good executive functions (measured by the
understanding ratios is consistent with previous MCST) perform well on the GDT regardless of
findings. The interesting point of our current whether they use an intuitive or a calculative strat-
results is the partial mediation effect. This means egy, while subjects with lower executive function-
that logical thinking by itself, controlled for the ing perform well only if they use a calculative
mediation over ratio processing, explains variance strategy. Lastly, the study by Schiebener et al.
in GDT–Double performance. What does this (2011) demonstrated that executive functions (also
result tell us? We argue that independent from the measured by the MCST) interact with probability
ability to process ratios, logical thinking can processing in predicting GDT performance. As
increase the probability of individuals categorizing suggested by those results, good probability hand-
the decision options on the basis of the numerical ling may compensate for the effects of weak execu-
information. The second study yielded clear evi- tive functions in decisions under risk. Although the
dence that logical reasoning together with other study by Schiebener et al. (2011) contributes to a
executive functions predicts probability processing, better understanding of executive functions and
and probability processing predicts decision mak- probability processing in making advantageous
ing under risk. In previous studies, the impact of decisions, it has two important limitations and
executive functions on making advantageous deci- confounds: First, probability processing was
sions under risk conditions has been intensively assessed by another gambling task, the
addressed. Different executive subcomponents, Probability Associated Gambling Task (Zamarian
such as categorization, cognitive flexibility, logical et al., 2008), without assessing the participants’
reasoning, strategy application, and monitoring, explicit knowledge about probabilities or the abil-
have been found to correlate on a bivariate level ity to process and calculate probabilities correctly.
with performance in the GDT in various patient Secondly, the study by Schiebener et al. (2011) did
populations—for example, Morbus Parkinson not analyze potential processes that may lie behind
(Brand et al., 2004; Euteneuer et al., 2009), the relationships between executive functions,
Alzheimer’s disease (Delazer et al., 2007), temporal probability processing, and decision making.
lobe epilepsy (Labudda et al., 2009), Korsakoff’s These two limitations were explicitly addressed by
syndrome (Brand, Fujiwara, et al., 2005), patholo- our current study 2. Using structural equation
gical gambling (Brand, Kalbe, et al., 2005), opiate model on latent dimension for both the predictor
926 BRAND ET AL.

(executive functions) and the hypothesized media- better comprehension of numbers as well as ratio
tor (probability processing), we report that the concepts. They are more likely to manipulate num-
effect of executive functions on GDT performance bers and to derive meaning from numerical infor-
(in the GDT–Double version) is mediated by the mation, thus influencing decisions. Importantly,
ability to process complex probabilities correctly. the model by Lipkus and Peters (2009) assumes
The present results provide insight into the relation that the deliberate analytical mode is completed
between executive functions, probability proces- by intuitive processing, the two systems interacting
sing, and performance on the GDT–Double. and informing each other. Intuitive processing
They suggest that the interpretation of simple would also include basic approximate processing,
bivariate correlations indicating a direct relation- which constitutes the basis for more advanced
ship between executive functions and performance numerical operations (Lipkus & Peters, 2009).
on the GDT–Double may lead to an incomplete Differentiating between a deliberative and an intui-
and misrepresentative picture. tive system has a long tradition in psychological
Bringing together the model of decision making decision-making research (Epstein, Pacini, Denes-
under risk (Brand et al., 2006) and the results of Raj, & Heier, 1996; Kahneman, 2003; Reyna,
the current study, we may hypothesize the cogni- 2004; Stanovich & West, 2000; Tversky &
tive processes in decision making under explicit Kahneman, 1983). Consistent with those dual-pro-
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

risk conditions as follows. The person inspects the cess approaches, the neuropsychological model by
information provided (e.g., number of options, Brand et al. (2006) also suggests that decision mak-
possible gains, possible losses), extracts essential ing under risk can be done by an executive/calcula-
pieces of information, and keeps them in working tion path, but also by an emotionally driven
memory. Arithmetic fact knowledge, calculation feedback-based path. The model suggests that deci-
procedures, and strategies are employed to calcu- ders perform optimally when they rely on both
late the probabilities, related consequences, and pathways—that is, when they use the executive/
expected values of each of the available alterna- calculation path for selecting an advantageous
tives. In order to perform these operations cor- strategy and at the same time profit from feedback
rectly, an individual needs understanding of for monitoring its efficacy.
probabilities as well as the knowledge of how to The results of the current studies support parti-
perform arithmetic operations on them. Logical cularly the view that the deliberative/cognitive path
reasoning and other executive functions, such as to process ratios is guided primarily by executive
categorization, cognitive flexibility, and monitor- functions and that these cognitive processes result
ing, are important for extracting relevant informa- in advantageous decisions in situations that offer
tion (e.g., the gain in the single die option is explicit numerical information about the expected
€1000), retrieving knowledge about probabilities outcome.
from long-term memory (e.g., the chance of gues- An additional aim of the present research was to
sing a single die throw is 1/6), combining these assess whether healthy subjects decide advanta-
pieces of information into a mathematical problem geously in the modified version of the GDT. In
representation, selecting appropriate calculation the original GDT, the most conservative choice is
procedures and strategies to work on this problem also the most advantageous choice. That means
(e.g., multiply the possible gain with the chance to that advantageous choices may either reflect opti-
win and subtract the possible loss multiplied by the mal decision making or derive from risk aversive
risk to lose), and comparing different options. behavior (see also discussion in Sinz, Zamarian,
Executive functions are needed to retrieve knowl- Benke, Wenning, & Delazer, 2008). The GDT–
edge from long-term memory, to select procedures Double overcomes this weakness by the possibility
and strategies, and to organize and monitor the to double the stakes after each choice, but is iden-
single procedural steps, as well as to shift between tical to the original GDT in all other aspects. As
different problem situations. They are also needed shown by the present results, healthy individuals
in order to inhibit seemingly attractive, but risky chose the advantageous alternative (combination
choices. In particular, the present study adds evi- of 4 dices) in 42% of the cases. These advantageous
dence on the interaction of executive functions and decisions may be further differentiated into conser-
numerical abilities in decisions under risk. vative, risk-avoiding choices (41% of all advanta-
The present results are also compatible with the geous choices) and optimal choices (59%). The
Lipkus and Peters (2009) model of numeracy in present results suggest that healthy individuals
decision making in a real-world context. Highly show optimal decisions in a high proportion of
numerate individuals pay higher attention to num- instances. So obviously, choosing a combination
bers and show better computational abilities and a of four numbers in the original GDT may reflect
PROBABILITY PROCESSING AND DECISION MAKING 927

both optimal decision making and risk-avoiding Brand, M., Kalbe, E., Labudda, K., Fujiwara, E.,
behavior and does not necessarily indicate conser- Kessler, J., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2005). Decision-
making impairments in patients with pathological
vative behavior. Moreover, the results suggest that
gambling. Psychiatry Research, 133, 91–99.
the distinction between optimal and conservative Brand, M., Labudda, K., Kalbe, E., Hilker, R., Emmans,
behavior as provided by the GDT–Double D., Fuchs, G., … Markowitsch, H. J. (2004).
enhances the diagnostic validity of the original Decision-making impairments in patients with
GDT. While the modified version proved to be Parkinson’s disease. Behavioural Neurology, 15, 77–85.
Brand, M., Labudda, K., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2006).
useful in the assessment of healthy individuals
Neuropsychological correlates of decision-making in
with relatively high education, its application in ambiguous and risky situations. Neural Networks, 19,
neuropsychological research remains to be 1266–1276.
investigated. Brand, M., Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., & Markowitsch,
H. J. (2009). Decision making with and without feed-
back: The role of intelligence, strategies, executive
CONCLUSION functions, and cognitive styles. Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 984–998.
Brand, M., Recknor, E. C., Grabenhorst, F., & Bechara,
Decision making under risk conditions with expli- A. (2007). Decisions under ambiguity and decisions
cit numerical information about the consequences under risk: Correlations with executive functions and
and the probabilities is affected by the decider’s comparisons of two different gambling tasks with
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

level of executive functions and numerical abilities. implicit and explicit rules. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 86–99.
Number-based logical thinking influences decision Brand, M., Roth-Bauer, M., Driessen, M., &
making under risk directly, but this effect is also Markowitsch, H. J. (2008). Executive functions and
mediated by the ability to process ratios. Logical risky decision-making in patients with opiate depen-
reasoning in combination with other executive dence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 97, 64–72.
functions also affects more complex probability Brand, M., & Schiebener, J. (2013). Interactions of age
and cognitive functions in predicting decision making
processing, which then affects decision making under risky conditions over the life span. Journal of
under risk. These findings suggest that increasing Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35, 9–23.
an individual’s understanding of ratios and prob- Cavanaugh, K., Huizinga, M. M., Wallston, K. A.,
abilities should lead to more advantageous deci- Gebretsadik, T., Shintani, A., Davis, D., … Rothman,
sions under risk conditions. R. L. (2008). Association of numeracy and diabetes
control. Annals of Internal Medicine, 148, 737–746.
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomi-
cal and functional model of number processing.
REFERENCES Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83–120.
Delazer, M. (2003). Neuropsychological findings on con-
Abdel-Kader, K., Dew, M. A., Bhatnagar, M., ceptual knowledge in arithmetics. In A. Baroody &
Argyropoulos, C., Karpov, I., Switzer, G., & A. Dowker (Eds.), The development of arithmetic con-
Unruh, M. (2010). Numeracy skills in CKD: cepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise.
Correlates and outcomes. Clinical Journal of the Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
American Society of Nephrology, 5, 1566–1573. Delazer, M., Domahs, F., Lochy, A., Karner, E., Benke,
Arsalidou, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2+2 = 4? T., & Poewe, W. (2004). Number processing in basal
Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers ganglia dysfunction: A single case study.
and calculations. NeuroImage, 54, 2382–2393. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1050–1062.
Bar-On, R., Tranel, D., Denburg, N. L., & Bechara, A. Delazer, M., Kemmler, G., & Benke, T. (2013). Health
(2003). Exploring the neurological substrate of emo- numeracy and cognitive decline in advanced age.
tional and social intelligence. Brain, 126, 1790–1800. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section
Bazanis, E., Rogers, R. D., Dowson, J. H., Taylor, P., B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 20, 639–659.
Meux, C., Staley, C., … Sahakian, B. J. (2002). Delazer, M., Sinz, H., Zamarian, L., & Benke, T. (2007).
Neurocognitive deficits in decision-making and plan- Decision-making with explicit and stable rules in
ning of patients with DSM–III–R borderline person- mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 45,
ality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 32, 1395–1405. 1632–1641.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. Delazer, M., Sinz, H., Zamarian, L., Stockner, H., Seppi,
R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing K., Wenning, G. K., … Poewe, W. (2009). Decision
the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1295. making under risk and under ambiguity in Parkinson’s
Brand, M., Fujiwara, E., Borsutzky, S., Kalbe, E., disease. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1901–1908.
Kessler, J., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2005). Decision- Drechsler, R., Rizzo, P., & Steinhausen, H. C. (2008).
making deficits of Korsakoff patients in a new gam- Decision-making on an explicit risk-taking task in pre-
bling task with explicit rules: Associations with execu- adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
tive functions. Neuropsychology, 19, 267–277. der. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 201–209.
Brand, M., Heinze, K., Labudda, K., & Markowitsch, Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H.
H. J. (2008). The role of strategies in deciding advan- (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential
tageously in ambiguous and risky situations. and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of
Cognitive Processing, 9, 159–173. Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 390–405.
928 BRAND ET AL.

Estrada, C., Barnes, V., Collins, C., & Byrd, J. C. (1999). Lemaire, P., & Arnaud, L. (2008). Young and older
Health literacy and numeracy. Journal of the adults’ strategies in complex arithmetic. American
American Medical Association, 282, 527. Journal of Psychology, 121, 1–16.
Euteneuer, F., Schaefer, F., Stuermer, R., Boucsein, W., Lipkus, I. M., & Peters, E. (2009). Understanding the
Timmermann, L., Barbe, M. T., … Kalbe, E. (2009). role of numeracy in health: Proposed theoretical fra-
Dissociation of decision-making under ambiguity and mework and practical insights. Health Education &
decision-making under risk in patients with Behavior, 36, 1065–1081.
Parkinson’s disease: A neuropsychological and Lipkus, I. M., Samsa, G., & Rimer, B. K. (2001).
psychophysiological study. Neuropsychologia, 47, General performance on a numeracy scale among
2882–2890. highly educated samples. Medical Decision Making,
Fond, G., Bayard, S., Capdevielle, D., Del-Monte, J., 21, 37–44.
Mimoun, N., Macgregor, A., … Raffard, S. (2013). A Mantovan, M. C., Delazer, M., Ermani, M., & Denes,
further evaluation of decision-making under risk and G. (1999). The breakdown of calculation procedures
under ambiguity in schizophrenia. European Archives of in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 35, 21–38.
Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, 249–257. McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., & Basili, A. (1985).
Friedman, M., & Savage, L. J. (1948). The utility ana- Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and cal-
lysis of choices involving risk. Journal of Political culation: Evidence from dyscalculia. Brain and
Economy, 56, 279–304. Cognition, 4, 171–196.
Garcia-Retamero, R., Galesic, M., & Gigerenzer, G. Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (2011). Mplus. Los Angeles,
(2010). Do icon arrays help reduce denominator CA: Muthén & Muthén.
neglect? Medical Decision Making, 30, 672–684. Nelson, H. E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensi-
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

Gleichgerrcht, E., Ibanez, A., Roca, M., Torralva, T., & tive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex, 12, 313–324.
Manes, F. (2010). Decision making cognition in neu- Pabst, S., Brand, M., & Wolf, O. T. (2013). Stress effects
rodegenerative diseases. Nature Reviews Neurology, on framed decisions: There are differences for gains
6, 611–623. and losses. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7
Härting, C., Markowitsch, H. J., Neufeld, H., Calabrese, (Article 142), 1–10.
P., Deisinger, K., & Kessler, J. (2000). Wechsler Pertl, M., Benke, T., Zamarian, L., Martini, C., Bodner,
Gedächtnistest–revidierte Fassung: WMS–R; deutsche T., Karner, E., & Delazer, M. (2014). Do patients
Adaptation der revidierten Fassung der Wechsler with mild cognitive impairment understand numerical
Memory Scale [Revised Wechsler Memory Scale; health information? Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease,
German version]. Göttingen: Hogrefe. 40(3): 531–540. doi:10.3233/JAD-131895
Hodzik, S., & Lemaire, P. (2011). Inhibition and shifting Peters, E., Vastfjall, D., Slovic, P., Mert, C. K.,
capacities mediate adults’ age-related differences in Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy and
strategy selection and repertoire. Acta Psychologica, decision making. Psychological Science, 17, 407–413.
137, 335–344. Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trailmaking Test
Hoffrage, U., Lindsey, S., Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. as an indication of organic brain damage. Perceptual
(2000). Communicating statistical information. and Motor Skills, 8, 271–276.
Science, 290, 2261–2262. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1995). Category test and
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In trail making test as measures of frontal lobe func-
R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling con- tions. Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 50–56.
cepts issues and applications (pp. 76–99). London: Reyna, V. F. (2004). How people make decisions that
Sage. involve risk: A dual-processes approach. Current
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 60–66.
indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (2007). The importance
criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation of mathematics in health and human judgment:
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55. Numeracy, risk communication, and medical decision
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and making. Learning and Individual Differences, 17,
choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American 147–159.
Psychologist, 58, 697–720. Reyna, V. F., Nelson, W. L., Han, P. K., & Dieckmann,
Keeney, R. L., & Raiffa, H. (1993). Decisions with multi- N. F. (2009). How numeracy influences risk compre-
ple objectives: Preferences and value tradeoffs. New hension and medical decision making. Psychological
York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Bulletin, 135, 943–973.
Labudda, K., Frigge, K., Horstmann, S., Aengenendt, Schiebener, J., Wegmann, E., Pawlikowski, M., &
J., Woermann, F. G., Ebner, A., … Brand, M. Brand, M. (2013). Supporting decisions under risk:
(2009). Decision making in patients with temporal Explicit advice differentially affects people accord-
lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 47, 50–58. ing to their working memory performance and
Langdon, D. W., & Warrington, E. K. (1995). The executive functioning. Neuroscience of Decision
VESPAR: A verbal and spatial reasoning test. Hove: Making, 1, 9–18.
Lawrence Erlbaum. Schiebener, J., Zamarian, L., Delazer, M., & Brand, M.
Langdon, D. W., & Warrington, E. K. (1997). The (2011). Executive functions, categorization of prob-
abstraction of numerical relations: A role for the abilities and learning from feedback: What does
right hemisphere in arithmetic? Journal of the really matter for decision-making under explicit risk
International Neuropsychological Society, 3, conditions? Journal of Clinical and Experimental
260–268. Neuropsychology, 33, 1025–1039.
PROBABILITY PROCESSING AND DECISION MAKING 929

Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., Black, W. C., & Welch, Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised
H. G. (1997). The role of numeracy in understanding manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
the benefit of screening mammography. Annals of Corporation.
Internal Medicine, 127, 966–972. Weinfurt, K. P., Castel, L. D., Li, Y., Sulmasy, D. P.,
Semenza, C., Miceli, L., & Girelli, L. (1997). A deficit Balshem, A. M., Benson, A. B., … Meropol, N. J.
for arithmetical procedures: Lack of knowledge or (2003). The correlation between patient characteris-
lack of monitoring? Cortex, 33, 483–498. tics and expectations of benefit from Phase I clinical
Sheridan, S. L., & Pignone, M. (2002). Numeracy and trials. Cancer, 98, 166–175.
the medical students’ ability to interpret data. Zamarian, L., Benke, T., Buchler, M., Wenter, J., &
Effective Clinical Practice, 5, 35–40. Delazer, M. (2010). Information about medications
Sinz, H., Zamarian, L., Benke, T., Wenning, G. K., & may cause misunderstanding in older adults with
Delazer, M. (2008). Impact of ambiguity and risk on cognitive impairment. Journal of the Neurological
decision making in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Sciences, 298, 46–51.
Neuropsychologia, 46, 2043–2055. Zamarian, L., Semenza, C., Domahs, F., Benke, T., &
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual Delazer, M. (2007). Alzheimer’s disease and mild
differences in reasoning: Implications for the ration- cognitive impairment: Effects of shifting and interfer-
ality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645– ence in simple arithmetic. Journal of the Neurological
726. Sciences, 263, 79–88.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional ver- Zamarian, L., Sinz, H., Bonatti, E., Gamboz, N., &
sus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in Delazer, M. (2008). Normal aging affects decisions
probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, under ambiguity, but not decisions under risk.
Downloaded by [New York University] at 20:55 02 June 2015

293–315. Neuropsychology, 22, 645–657.

You might also like