You are on page 1of 11

E D I T O R I A L

THE WORLDWIDE BURDEN OF INFANT MENTAL AND EMOTIONAL DISORDER:


REPORT OF THE TASK FORCE OF THE WORLD ASSOCIATION FOR INFANT MENTAL
HEALTH

KARLEN LYONS-RUTH
Harvard Medical School, Cambridge, MA

JODY TODD MANLY


University of Rochester, Rochester, NY

KAI VON KLITZING


University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany

TUULA TAMMINEN
University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland

ROBERT EMDE
University of Colorado School of Medicine, Denver, Colorado

HIRAM FITZGERALD
Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI

CAMPBELL PAUL
Royal Children’s Hospital Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia

MIRI KEREN
Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel

ASTRID BERG
Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, South Africa

MAREE FOLEY
Child, Family and Organisational Consultancy, Geneva, Switzerland

HISAKO WATANABE
Watanabe Clinic, Yokohama, Japan

ABSTRACT: Children worldwide experience mental and emotional disorders. Mental disorders occurring among young children, especially infants
(birth –3 years), often go unrecognized. Prevalence rates are difficult to determine because of lack of awareness and difficulty assessing and diagnosing

We acknowledge the invaluable assistance of Mallika Rajamani in the preparation of this article. We have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Direct correspondence to: Karlen Lyons-Ruth, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Cambridge Hospital, 1493 Cambridge Street, Cambridge,
MA 02139; e-mail: klruth@hms.harvard.edu.

INFANT MENTAL HEALTH JOURNAL, Vol. 38(6), 695–705 (2017)


C 2017 Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health

View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com.


DOI: 10.1002/imhj.21674

695
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
696 • K. Lyons-Ruth et al.

young children. Existing data, however, suggest that rates of disorders in young children are comparable to those of older children and adolescents
(von Klitzing, Dohnert, Kroll, & Grube, 2015). The lack of widespread recognition of disorders of infancy is particularly concerning due to the unique
positioning of infancy as foundational in the developmental process. Both the brain and behavior are in vulnerable states of development across the first
3 years of life, with potential for enduring deviations to occur in response to early trauma and deprivation. Intervention approaches for young children
require sensitivity to their developmental needs within their families. The primacy of infancy as a time of unique foundational risks for disorder, the
impact of trauma and violence on young children’s development, the impact of family disruption on children’s attachment, and existing literature on
prevalence rates of early disorders are discussed. Finally, global priorities for addressing these disorders of infancy are highlighted to support prevention
and intervention actions that may alleviate suffering among our youngest world citizens.
Keywords: Infant mental health, infant mental disorders, prevalence of infant disorder, burden of disease, early intervention

RESUMEN: Los niños en el mundo entero experimentan trastornos mentales y emocionales. Los trastornos mentales que ocurren en niños pequeños,
especialmente infantes (entre el nacimiento y los 3 años de edad), a menudo pasan sin ser reconocidos. Las tasas de prevalencia son difı́ciles de
determinar a causa de la falta de conocimiento y la dificultad de evaluar y diagnosticar niños pequeños. Sin embargo, la información existente sugiere
que las tasas de trastornos en niños pequeños son comparables con aquellas de niños mayores y adolescentes. La falta de un amplio reconocimiento
de los trastornos de infancia es particularmente preocupante debido a la posición clave que ocupa la infancia como pilar fundamental en el proceso
de desarrollo. Tanto el cerebro como la conducta se encuentran en vulnerables etapas de desarrollo a lo largo de los tres primeros años de vida con la
potencialidad de sufrir desviaciones que ocurren como respuesta al temprano trauma y a las privaciones. Los acercamientos de intervención para niños
pequeños requieren una sensibilidad hacia sus necesidades de desarrollo dentro de sus familias. Se discuten la primacı́a de la infancia como una etapa
de riesgos fundamentales para los trastornos, el impacto que el trauma y la violencia tienen sobre el desarrollo de los niños pequeños, el impacto que las
rupturas familiares ejercen sobre la afectividad de los niños, y la literatura existente sobre las tasas de prevalencia de tempranos trastornos. Finalmente
se subrayan las prioridades globales para prestarle atención a estos trastornos de infancia con el fin de apoyar acciones de prevención e intervención
que pudieran aliviar el sufrimiento entre nuestros más jóvenes ciudadanos del mundo.
Palabras claves: salud mental infantil, trastornos mentales de los infantes, prevalencia del trastorno infantil, carga de la enfermedad, intervención
temprana

RÉSUMÉ: Dans le monde entier les enfants font l’expérience de troubles mentaux et émotionnels. Les troubles mentaux chez très jeunes enfants,
surtout les très jeunes enfants (de la naissance à l’âge de 3 ans), ne sont pas toujours visibles ou diagnostiqués. Les taux de prévalence sont difficiles à
determiner du fait d’un manque de connaissances et de la difficulté à évaluer et à diagnostiquer les jeunes enfants. Les données qui existent, cependant,
suggèrent que les taux de troubles chez les jeunes enfants sont comparables à ceux que l’on voit chez les enfants plus âgés et les adolescents. Le
manque de reconnaissance généralisée de troubles chez l’enfant en bas âge est particulièrement préoccupant du fait du positionnement unique de la
petite enfance comme étant fondamental dans le processus de développement. Le cerveau et le comportement se trouvent tous deux dans des états
de développement au fil des trois premières années de la vie, pouvant voir s’installer des déviations durables en réaction à un trauma précoce et des
privations. Les approaches d’intervention pour les jeunes enfants exigent une sensibilité à leurs besoins dévelopmentaux au sein de leurs familles. La
primauté de la petite enfance en tant que moment de risques fondamentaux de trouble, l’impact du trauma et de la violence sur le développement des
jeunes enfants, l’impact des perturbations familiales sur l’attachement des enfants, et les recherches sur les taux de prévalence de troubles précoces
sont tous discutés dans cet article. Enfin les priorités globales pour faire face à ces troubles de la petite enfance sont mises en valeur afin de soutenir des
actions de prévention et d’intervention pouvant soulager la souffrance chez nos jeunes citoyens du monde.
Mots clés: santé mentale du nourrisson, troubles mentaux de la petite enfance, prévalence des troubles chez la petite enfance, fardeau de la maladie,
intervention

ZUSAMMENFASSUNG: Weltweit erfahren Kinder psychische und emotionale Störungen. Psychische Störungen bei Kleinkindern, insbesondere bei
Säuglingen (von der Geburt bis zum Alter von 3 Jahren), bleiben oft unerkannt. Die Prävalenzraten sind aufgrund des fehlenden Bewusstseins und der
Schwierigkeit, kleine Kinder zu beurteilen und zu diagnostizieren, schwer zu bestimmen. Die vorliegenden Daten deuten jedoch darauf hin, dass die
Häufigkeit der Erkrankungen bei Kleinkindern mit der bei älteren Kindern und Jugendlichen vergleichbar ist. Der Mangel einer allgemein verbreiteten
Beachtung von Störungen im Säuglingsalter ist besonders besorgniserregend, da das Säuglingsalter das Fundament für den weiteren Entwicklungsprozess
bildet. Sowohl Gehirn als auch Verhalten befinden sich in den ersten drei Lebensjahren in einem vulnerablen Entwicklungsstadium, mit dem Potenzial,
dass anhaltende Abweichungen als Reaktion auf frühe Traumata und Deprivation auftreten können. Interventionsansätze für Kleinkinder erfordern eine
Sensibilität für ihre Entwicklungsbedürfnisse innerhalb ihrer Familien. Die Vorrangstellung des Säuglingsalters als eine Zeit einzigartiger grundlegender
Risiken für Störungen, die Auswirkungen von Traumata und Gewalt auf die Entwicklung von Kleinkindern, die Auswirkungen von Zerrüttung in der
Familie auf die Bindung von Kindern und die existierende Literatur über Prävalenzraten früher Störungen werden diskutiert. Schließlich werden globale
Prioritäten hervorgehoben, die diese Störungen im Säuglingsalter adressieren, um Präventions- und Interventionsmaßnahmen zu unterstützen, die das
Leid unserer jüngsten Weltbürger lindern können.

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Burden of Infant Mental Health • 697

Stichwörter: Psychische Gesundheit von Säuglingen und Kleinkindern, psychische Störungen von Säuglingen und Kleinkindern, Prävalenz von
Störungen bei Säuglingen, Krankheitslast, Frühintervention

dd:dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd (dddd3ddd) dddddddd


dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
ddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
ddddddddddddddddd3dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddddddd
dd d dd: dddddd, dddddd, ddddddddd, ddddburden, dddd

: (3)
, , , , 
, , , 

, ,   ,  

 
dd d : , , , , 

* * *

Worldwide, 10 to 20% of children and adolescents experience and deprivation (Fox, Levitt, & Nelson, 2010; Shonkoff, Garner,
mental disorders (World Health Organization, 2003). If untreated, Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health,
these conditions severely influence children’s development, their Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care,
educational attainments, and their potential to live fulfilling and and Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 2012).
productive lives. While the global burden of mental health dis- In times of so many human crises worldwide caused by wars,
orders in childhood and adolescence is currently the subject of violence, global forced displacements, and natural disasters, very
investigation, costs associated with mental disorders in infancy young children are particularly impacted by insecurity and threats
in particular (birth–3 years) have remained largely invisible. In- to well-being and do not have the cognitive or verbal capacity to
deed, service providers who work with young children, as well understand such events. These kinds of early negative experiences
as caregivers themselves, increasingly recognize the needs of in- can lead to anxiety, depression, posttraumatic stress, and other
fants and their families. However, at the level of service-delivery challenges and disorders in infants and young children. Adverse
systems and social policy, the concept of mental disorders in in- early experiences may be especially harmful if they occur in the
fancy is not widely recognized. This lack of widespread recogni- first 3 years of life, during formative periods for brain development,
tion of disorders of infancy is particularly concerning due to the leading to lifelong consequences. Infancy, more than any other
unique positioning of infancy at the beginning of the developmen- developmental period, also is a time of heightened vulnerability to
tal process. Both the brain and behavior are in vulnerable states disruptions in attachment relationships. All of these challenges can
of development across the first 3 years of life, with the poten- lead to disorders in infancy, place stress on parents, and can put
tial for enduring deviations to occur in response to early trauma further development of physical and mental health at severe risk.

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
698 • K. Lyons-Ruth et al.

As noted in the World Health Organization’s (2016) global plan of system function (Belsky & de Haan, 2011; Blair, 2010). Thus,
action on violence and INSPIRE publication, threats to children’s the infant needs to be treated within the caregiving relationship,
well-being can have enduring developmental consequences across in contrast to later modes of mental health intervention that most
multiple domains of functioning, leading to long-term economic often focus on the child alone. Many young children with mental
and public health impacts. health disorders do not receive treatment, however, sometimes with
the misconception that infants are too young to need treatment or
that they will grow out of their symptoms, despite evidence that
WHY IS INFANCY UNIQUE?
psychiatric symptoms that emerge early in life often persist into
How is the developmental period of infancy unique, and why does later phases of development.
it require separate consideration? First, infancy is a time of par- There are a number of evidence-based interventions based on
ticularly rapid brain and behavioral development, constituting a randomized controlled trials that are effective in treating infants
sensitive period when the organism’s development is particularly with mental health problems and disrupted infant–caregiver rela-
open to and affected by the prevailing environment. Thus, traumatic tionships (Bernard et al., 2012; Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2006;
environments in infancy have heightened potential to lead to de- Fisher, Gunnar, Dozier, Bruce, & Pears, 2006; Smyke, Zeanah,
viant adaptations with long-term negative consequences. Atypical Fox, Nelson, & Guthrie, 2010; Tereno et al., 2017; Toth, Rogosch,
developmental trajectories can be identified in the first 6 months Manly, & Cicchetti, 2006). Some of these programs are preven-
of life that predict higher risk for later symptomatology (Cote tive and focus on promoting positive birth outcomes, parent–child
et al., 2009), and, when severe behavior problems are evident in relationships, parenting practices, and parent mental health (Olds,
the first few years of life, there is moderate to strong continuity in Sadler, & Kitzman, 2007; Tamminen & Puura, 2015; Toth, Pe-
symptoms that place these children at risk for long-term problems, trenko, Gravener-Davis, & Handley, 2015). Other models are tar-
especially in the context of family dysfunction (S.B. Campbell, geted to address specific types of disorders or high-risk groups.
Shaw, & Gilliom, 2000). Outcomes of these intervention approaches have included im-
Second, due to the infant’s immaturity, the features of dis- provements in security of parent–child attachment relationships,
order in infancy are somewhat different than those in the later reductions in child behavior problems, reductions in child and par-
years, requiring separate diagnostic descriptors. Because disorders ent symptoms of traumatic stress and depression, improvements
of infancy are not described separately in the current versions of in maternal sensitivity, improvements in cortisol regulation, and
the International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision (ICD- reductions in failed foster care placements (Tamminen & Puura,
10; World Health Organization, 1993) and the Diagnostic and 2015; Toth et al., 2015; von Klitzing, et al., 2015). The key points
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5; are:
American Psychiatric Association, 2000), a manual for disorders
• Adverse early experiences may be especially harmful if they
of infancy more fitted to the presentations of very young children
has been developed, termed the Diagnostic Classification: Zero to occur in the first 3 years of life, during formative periods
Five (DC:0–5) (ZERO TO THREE, 2016); formerly the Diagnos- for brain development, leading to enduring consequences.
tic Classification: Zero to Three (DC:0–3) (ZERO TO THREE, • Due to the infant’s immaturity, the features of symptoms
2005). Because of the greater correspondence of these diagnostic and disorders in infancy are somewhat different from those
descriptions to what is actually observed in early childhood, this in the later years, requiring separate diagnostic descriptors.
manual is in use among many infant mental health clinicians. To • During infancy, stressors on caregivers have particularly
convey a sense of the many disordered presentations that clinicians immediate consequences for the infant’s developing stress
encounter in treating very young children, the DC:0–5 diagnostic response systems and overall development, as compared to
listing is shown in Appendix A. later ages.
Third, the infant’s immature systems place him or her in a • Early dependence on caregivers requires different modes of
unique state of dependence upon the care of attentive adults. Care-
mental health intervention that involve both the infant and
givers constitute the essence of the infant’s world and are essential
the caregiver.
for the infant’s capacity to regulate emotional and physiological
• Evidence-based intervention approaches have demonstrated
states. During infancy, stresses on caregivers have more immedi-
ate consequences for the infant’s own developing stress response success in improving outcomes for children and families.
systems and overall development than they do at later ages.
Finally, this early dependence on caregivers requires different
TRAUMA AND VIOLENCE AFFECTING INFANTS AND
modes of mental health intervention that involve both the infant and
TODDLERS
the caregiver. The caregiver’s presence and attentiveness to infant
cues is a critical regulator of the infant’s development. A number A consideration of the global burden of infant mental and emotional
of controlled studies have confirmed that the quality of caregiver disorder must, in part, consider the costs to society. But just as
regulation in these formative years has widespread effects on the importantly, we must acknowledge the burden of suffering for the
expression of genes that control aspects of brain growth and stress individual. We can readily imagine and feel deep empathy for the

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Burden of Infant Mental Health • 699

suffering of infants who are physically or emotionally abused, who of Pediatrics, 2000) and results in emotional and behavioral prob-
experience extremely deprived care, and who get caught up in the lems as well (Perry, 2004).
traumas of war, violence, and loss. Suffering linked to environmental stress is more likely to
A recent Centers for Disease Control and Prevention report etch itself into the being of the infant in the form of a mental or
has highlighted that over 1 billion children worldwide have expe- emotional problem when regulatory caregiving relationships also
rienced violence in the past year (Hillis, Mercy, Amobi, & Kress, are seriously impaired. This is clearly the case in abuse and neglect
2016). According to the National Child Traumatic Stress Net- situations, but also is likely to be a factor in conditions of war, fam-
work, hundreds of thousands of young children and adolescents in ily dislocation, and natural disasters, when parents themselves also
the United States alone are exposed to severely traumatic events are suffering. Developmental studies have further documented the
each year, including maltreatment (neglect, physical, sexual, or adverse effects of early abuse on later parenting behavior, linking
emotional abuse), exposure to intimate partner violence, commu- early maltreatment to intergenerational cycles of disorder (Cic-
nity violence, life-threatening illness or injury, or sudden loss. chetti & Valentino, 2006). Thus, exposure to trauma in infancy is
Among children under 3 years of age, more than 1 million (i.e., likely to affect formative developmental processes that impair the
1,650,000) were the subject of abuse and neglect reports to U.S. foundation of future growth and may set up more negative devel-
child protection authorities in 2014 (U.S. Department of Health opmental trajectories that have lifelong and even intergenerational
and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, consequences (Bowers & Yehuda, 2016).
& Children’s Bureau, 2016). In low- and middle-income coun- In one common example of infant suffering and resulting dis-
tries, the United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF; 2014) found order, consider a stressed father, unable to find work, who engages
that 7 to 8% of parents of 2- to 4-year-old children reported that in bouts of heavy drinking followed by rages that escalate into
the child was either beaten with an object or beaten as hard as violence toward his partner and often spill over toward the infant
one could during the past month. Children under the age of 3 or toddler who is crying and trying to cling to his mother. When
years also were more likely to die as a result of abuse and ne- the father is home, the infant becomes increasingly fear-ridden and
glect than were older children, with 71% of fatalities, or more than frozen into hypervigilant attention, has trouble falling and staying
1,000 children per year in the United States alone, dying from asleep, and becomes increasingly aggressive and dysregulated in
maltreatment (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, his behavior. Here, we see an infant who is suffering emotion-
2016). ally in this fraught family context and whose distress is leading
Exposure to trauma can affect multiple aspects of health and to disorders marked by disturbances in mood and behavior and
development, leading to emotional, behavioral, and learning dif- impairment in functioning. More chronic situations of abuse or
ficulties as well as related (and often sustained) health, substance neglect also are often marked by delays in reaching developmental
abuse, and mental health problems. Bogat, DeJonghe, Levendosky, milestones.
Davidson, and von Eye (2006) found that 44% of infants exposed
to intimate partner violence had at least one trauma symptom. Re-
FAMILY DISRUPTION AFFECTING INFANTS AND TODDLERS
sults from the Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) studies have
demonstrated that trauma early in development has been associ- Severe economic stress, war, and natural disasters also lead to in-
ated with increased risk for a range of psychological and phys- creased parental death and family disruption, increasing the burden
ical diagnoses in adulthood, including alcoholism, drug abuse, on the world’s care systems for infants. UNICEF has estimated that
depression, suicide attempts, teen pregnancy, ischemic heart dis- some 2.2 million children around the world live in institutional set-
ease, cancer, chronic lung disease, and liver disease (F. Campbell tings, but this figure is considered to be an underestimate due to
et al., 2014; Felitti et al., 1998). Epigenetic studies examining underreporting and lack of data in many places (World Health Or-
changes in gene expression have found that maltreatment early in ganization & Fundação Calouste Gulbenkian, 2015). While quality
childhood is associated with higher methylation of key genes as- of care varies enormously in institutional settings, many institutions
sociated with stress response, and methylation changes may place are understaffed and poorly equipped. Estimates also have shown
maltreated children at risk for various cancers, cardiovascular dis- that up to two thirds of children in institutions have a disability and
ease, changes in immune functioning, and psychiatric disorders that a significant number of these children have psychosocial dis-
(Cicchetti, Hetzel, Rogosch, Handley, & Toth, 2016). Controlled abilities that include developmental delay or intellectual disabili-
animal studies have concluded that exposure to early life adversity ties (World Health Organization & Fundação Calouste Gulbenkian,
programs stress response systems to have an abnormal response 2015).
to subsequent stressors, altering trajectories of brain development According to the European Offices of UNICEF and the Of-
and creating maltreatment-associated structural or functional dif- fice of the High Commissioner for Human Rights, the region of
ferences in multiple brain regions (Teicher & Samson, 2016). Thus, Eastern Europe and Central Asia has the highest number of chil-
complex childhood trauma, and the chronic stress it engenders, ad- dren growing up in institutional settings—more than 1 in every
versely affects brain development in areas involved in cognition, 100 children. The number of children in institutions in the 20 coun-
attention, memory, and emotional regulation (American Academy tries in this region is the highest in the world—more than 600,000
children (World Health Organization & Fundação Calouste

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
700 • K. Lyons-Ruth et al.

Gulbenkian, 2015). In 2003, the World Health Organization Re- Thus, their continued costs to the child and to society in later years
gional Office for Europe carried out a survey of 33 European can be clearly assigned to the conditions of rearing in infancy.
countries (excluding Russian-speaking countries) to map the num- While large-scale studies are not available, RAD and DAD are
ber and characteristics of children younger than 3 years old in estimated to have a prevalence of about 1% (von Klitzing et al.,
institutional care. Although the figures varied greatly between the 2015). However, their joint prevalence rises to 25 to 40% among
countries, 12 countries had institutionalized between 1 and 10 children in foster care (Boris et al., 2004; C.H. Zeanah et al., 2004)
young children per 10,000 under 3 years of age, seven countries and to up to 60 to 70% among children in orphanages (Tizard &
had institutionalized between 11 and 30 children per 10,000 under Rees, 1975; C.H. Zeanah, Smyke, & Dumitrescu, 2002).
3 years of age, and eight countries had institutionalized between
31 and 60 children per 10,000 under 3 years of age (Browne, 2009;
MENTAL AND EMOTIONAL DISORDER AMONG INFANTS
UNICEF, 2009).
AND TODDLERS
Furthermore, the number of children in institutions is stable or
rising in several European Union member states, both new and old. While the suffering of infants in many contexts worldwide is clear,
Despite efforts for change, the rate in this region has been almost it is challenging to estimate the worldwide societal burden of the
stagnant since 2000. In 12 countries, however, the rate of children smaller set of conditions that are defined as psychiatric disorders
in institutional care increased between 2000 and 2007, and in of infancy. As noted earlier, primary diagnostic systems (American
16 of the countries of this region, well over 28,000 of the children Psychiatric Association, 2000; World Health Organization, 1993)
placed in institutional care were under 3 years of age (UNICEF were not devised with developmental sensitivity as to how symp-
Office of the High Commissioner for Human Rights Regional toms might manifest differently in very young children. In addition,
Office for Europe, 2011). Observations in understaffed and poorly the developmentally specific DC:0–5 criteria have not yet achieved
equipped institutions have produced images in which infants are universal acceptance as an improved alternative to the more global
sitting emotionless and apathetic, crying helplessly, or repetitively DSM-5 and ICD-10 descriptions of childhood disorders.
rocking and waving their hands in an attempt to find some stimula- Epidemiological studies have revealed a 16 to 18% preva-
tion in a barren environment. When compared to the responses of lence of mental disorders among children aged 1 to 5 years, with
home-reared infants, 97% of infants in institutional care exhibited somewhat more than half (8–9%) being severely affected (von
highly anomalous responses to their caregivers when stressed and Klitzing et al., 2015). This rate is quite similar to the 10 to 20%
in need of comfort (P.D. Zeanah, Stafford, & Zeanah, 2005). incidence among older children and adolescents estimated by the
These images have catalyzed a number of studies supporting World Health Organization, as noted earlier.
the description of two disorders of infancy linked to very substan- Specifically, in surveys of children aged 1 to 3 years, preva-
dard care. Both of these disorders are understood as damaging lence of deviant behavior or behavioral and emotional syndromes
adaptations to an environment that cannot meet the human infant’s ranged from 7.3% (Richman, Stevenson, & Graham, 1975) to
basic developmental need for emotional comfort and regulation 12 to 16% (Briggs-Gowan, Carter, Skuban, & Horwitz, 2001).
within a stable caregiving relationship. One disorder, reactive at- When only severe cases were included (Lavigne et al., 1998; Lavi-
tachment disorder (RAD), describes the apathetic infant mentioned gne et al., 1996), similar rates were obtained: 7.1% among 2-year-
earlier who, even when an orphanage worker comes to feed or care olds and 14.0% among 3-year-olds. These data include all child
for the baby, makes no attempt to smile, babble, or seek contact psychiatric diagnoses except general and pervasive developmental
and comfort. In a second and very different disorder linked to in- disorders. In the most recent study of toddlers aged 18 months
stitutional care, disinhibited attachment disorder (DAD) (DSM-5: in Denmark, the prevalence of Axis I (see Appendix A, B) diag-
Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder), the infant may indis- noses of a primary child psychiatric syndrome remained similar, at
criminately seek contact and comfort from anyone who passes by, 16% using the ICD-10 and at 18% using the DC:0–3 (Skovgaard
even from unfamiliar visitors. In home situations of extreme ne- et al., 2007). For all diagnostic categories investigated, there was
glect, disinhibited attachment behavior toward strangers also has considerably higher prevalence in the community as compared
been shown. to the incidence of disorders diagnosed at hospitals in the first
Even more ominously, a number of studies have found that 3 years of life (Skovgaard et al., 2007) (for prevalence of par-
even after transfer to good care, disinhibited attachment behaviors ticular diagnostic categories, see Appendix C). Reviews of the
may persist into adolescence and beyond (Kreppner et al., 2007). literature pertaining to each diagnostic classification in early child-
Notably, both RAD and DAD emerge in the first 2 years of life and hood are available in Lyons-Ruth, Zeanah, Benoit, Madigan, and
do not have new onset after the age of 2. Thus, the first 2 years of Mills-Koonce (2014).
life may constitute a vulnerable period in which very poor early Note that all of the aforementioned epidemiologic studies
care has enduring effects on the brain and behavior over the life were conducted in stable and developed countries with strong
span and continues to exact costs at least into adolescence. These economies and peaceful living conditions. Based on these rates,
extended costs must be considered as part of the overall burden of among the 643,386,753 children internationally between 0 to 4
infant disorder because these disorders have onset only in infancy years old, at least 102,941,880 young children would be diagnosed
and are preventable by providing early environments of good care. with a mental or emotional disorder. These must be regarded as

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Burden of Infant Mental Health • 701

lower bounds on the rates of disorder among very young chil- signs of mental health problems are identified, increased access to
dren, and we would expect to see significantly higher rates in evidence-based prevention and intervention services should be a
contexts of extreme poverty, war, and family displacement and priority.
trauma (Grantham-McGregor et al., 2007; Tomlinson, Bornstein,
Marlow, & Swartz, 2014).
GLOBAL PRIORITIES FOR ADDRESSING THE MENTAL AND
DISTURBANCES IN CAREGIVING AFFECTING INFANTS AND EMOTIONAL DISORDERS OF INFANCY
TODDLERS In conclusion, during pregnancy and infancy, critical neurobiolog-
Disturbances in caregiver–infant interaction are particularly im- ical systems are being organized to adapt to the prevailing envi-
portant to infant mental health because developmental studies have ronment. Infants are exposed to the full range of human trauma,
consistently shown the importance of the caregivers’ attentive reg- including violence, disasters, and loss, and their successful adap-
ulation of infant stress for the infant’s adequate health and de- tation depends heavily on the soothing and containing regulation
velopment (Madigan, Brumariu, Villani, Atkinson, & Lyons-Ruth, of their caregivers. Although information on prevalence of mental
2016). Infants engage in powerful and formative emotional com- and emotional disorders is limited, rates appear to be similar to
munications with caregivers from the first months of life, and by those among older children. However, effective treatments differ
3 months of age, infants actively engage their caregivers in cycles from those of older children in that they often need to involve both
of mutual communication and interaction. Further, by 9 months of the infant and the caregiver.
age, infants actively seek out caregivers’ signals of safety or fear to Failure to take preventive or therapeutic action is likely to
guide their behavior in new situations. By the end of the first year, result in several deleterious consequences, including (a) increased
infants show wariness toward unfamiliar people and selectively individual suffering because of lifelong enhanced risk of mental
seek out a few familiar caregivers for comfort and security when and/or somatic disorders; (b) heightened societal costs (e.g., in
upset or alarmed. healthcare, social service, and legal systems); and (c) enhanced
Although the majority of infants across countries and cultural risk of social disintegration, lack of social cooperation, and vio-
groups develop secure attachment relationships with their care- lence. The risk of social disintegration is heightened in part because
givers, a sizeable minority of infants have marked difficulty seeking the development of empathy and cooperative social relationships
comfort and contact with the caregiver to regulate their states of fear are based on experiences of healthy relationships during infancy. In
and distress. In meta-analyses of 6,000 infants, such disturbances in contrast, promoting positive mental well-being by preventing risks,
the caregiver–infant relationship, termed disorganized attachment supporting parental sensitivity, and increasing resilience in infants
behavior, were found in 15% of infants in the general population and caregivers can yield lifelong positive trajectories. Recent de-
(van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1999); velopmental science has identified a range of effective prevention
this figure rises to 48% among maltreated infants and toddlers and intervention strategies that can prevent maladaptation and fa-
(Cyr, Euser, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2010). cilitate healing after adversity to support positive development for
Disorganized attachment behaviors in infancy further predicted young children and their families.
elevated rates of emotional and behavioral disturbances in the in- The Nobel Laureate economist James Heckman, in his 2006
fancy, toddlerhood, preschool, middle childhood, and adolescent analyses of rates of return relative to investments in human capital
periods in several meta-analyses (Fearon, Bakersman-Kranenberg, by age, highlighted the gains that can be realized by investing in
van IJzendoorn, Lapsley, & Roisman, 2010; Madigan, et al., 2016; children during the early years of life. He emphasized that these op-
van Ijzendoorn et al., 1999). Although father–child attachment portunities can pay large dividends, not only in future productivity
research is less extensive than are mother–child studies, fathers but also in terms of equity and social justice. Addressing the needs
clearly play an important role in infants’ development, and father of young children is not only advantageous as an international
absence may place an additional stress on children’s adaptation and priority—it is imperative. In summary, these priorities are:
family functioning (Grossmann et al., 2002). Thus, assessment of • Priority on global education regarding the signs of disorder
the caregiver–child relationship is particularly important in studies in infancy and toddlerhood.
of mental health in infancy. • Priority on enhancing the availability of treatment for infants
Because of the centrality of these caregiver–child relation-
and their caregivers.
ships, it is essential that staff in child-serving organizations across
• Priority on developing reliable information regarding in-
disciplines receive adequate training in principles of infant men-
tal health and in supporting parents of infants and young children fant and toddler mental health in developing and war-torn
who are experiencing stress and major mental health challenges. countries.
This training can include raising awareness of the developmental
REFERENCES
needs of infants and toddlers and the risk factors that impinge on
adaptive development, as well as early identification of symptoms American Academy of Pediatrics. (2000). Fellows Survey. Elk Grove
of distress in young children and their family systems. Once early Village, IL: AP.

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
702 • K. Lyons-Ruth et al.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical man- & D. Cohen (Eds.). Development and Psychopathology (Vol. 3, 2nd
ual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychi- ed., pp. 129–201). New York: Wiley.
atric Association. Cote, S.M., Boivin, M., Liu, X., Nagin, D.S., Zoccolillo, M., &
Belsky, J. & de Haan, M. (2011). Annual research review: Parenting and Tremblay, R.E. (2009). Depression and anxiety symptoms: On-
children’s brain development: the end of the beginning. Journal of set, developmental course and risk factors during early childhood.
child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(4), 409–428. https://doi.org/ Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(10), 1201–1208.
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02281.x https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02099.x
Bernard, K., Dozier, M., Bick, J., Lewis-Morrarty, E., Lindhiem, Cyr, C., Euser, E.M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van IJzendoorn,
O., & Carlson, E. (2012). Enhancing attachment organiza- M.H. (2010). Attachment security and disorganization in mal-
tion among maltreated children: Results of a randomized clin- treating and high-risk families: A series of meta-analyses. De-
ical trial. Child Development, 83(2), 623–636. https://doi.org/ velopment and Psychopathology, 22(1), 87–108. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01712.x 10.1017/s0954579409990289
Blair, C. (2010). Stress and the development of self-regulation Fearon, R.P., Bakersman-Kranenberg, M.J., van IJzendoorn, M.H., Laps-
in context. Child Development Perspectives, 4(3), 181–188. ley, A., & Roisman, G.I. (2010). The significance of insecure attach-
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00145.x ment and disorganizarion in the development of children’s external-
Bogat, G.A., DeJonghe, E., Levendosky, A.A., Davidson, W.S., & von izing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Development, 81(2),
Eye, A. (2006). Trauma symptoms among infants exposed to in- 435–456. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01405.x
timate partner violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30(2), 109–125. Felitti, V.J., Anda, R.F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D.F., Spitz, A.M.,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.09.002 Edwards, V. et al. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and house-
Boris, N.W., Hinshaw-Fuselier, S.S., Smyke, A.T., Scheeringa, hold dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The
M.S., Heller, S.S., & Zeanah, C.H. (2004). Comparing crite- Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of
ria for attachment disorders: Establishing reliability and valid- Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245–258. Retrieved May 10, 2016 from
ity in high-risk samples. Journal of the American Academy of http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9635069
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(5), 568–577. https://doi.org/ Fisher, P.A., Gunnar, M.R., Dozier, M., Bruce, J., & Pears, K.C. (2006).
10.1097/00004583-200405000-00010 Effects of therapeutic interventions for foster children on behav-
Bowers, M.E., & Yehuda, R. (2016). Intergenerational transmission ioral problems, caregiver attachment, and stress regulatory neural
of stress in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(1), 232–244. systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 215–
https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.247 225. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.023
Briggs-Gowan, M.J., Carter, A.S., Skuban, E.M., & Horwitz, S.M. (2001). Fox, S.E., Levitt, P., & Nelson, C.A., III. (2010). How the tim-
Prevalence of social-emotional and behavioral problems in a com- ing and quality of early experiences influence the develop-
munity sample of 1- and 2-year-old children. Journal of the Amer- ment of brain architecture. Child Development, 81(1), 28–40.
ican Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(7), 811–819. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01380.x
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200107000-00016 Grantham-McGregor, S., Cheung, Y.B., Cueto, S., Glewwe, P., Richter,
Browne, K. (2009). The risk of harm to young children in institutional L., & Strupp, B. (2007). Developmental potential in the first 5 years
care. London: Save the Children. for children in developing countries. The Lancet, 369(9555), 60–70.
Campbell, F., Conti, G., Heckman, J.J., Moon, S.H., Pinto, R., Pun- https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(07)60032-4
gello, E., & Pan, Y. (2014). Early childhood investments substantially Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K.E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H.,
boost adult health. Science, 343(6178), 1478–1485. https://doi.org/ Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2002). The unique-
10.1126/science.1248429 ness of the child–father attachment relationship: Fathers’ sen-
Campbell, S.B., Shaw, D.S., & Gilliom, M. (2000). Early externalizing sitive and challenging play as a pivotal variable in a 16-
behavior problems: Toddlers and preschoolers at risk for later mal- year longitudinal study. Social Development, 11(3), 301–337.
adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 467–488. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00202
Cicchetti, D., Hetzel, S., Rogosch, F.A., Handley, E.D., & Toth, Heckman, J.J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of invest-
S.L. (2016). An investigation of child maltreatment and epige- ing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312(5782), 1900–1902.
netic mechanisms of mental and physical health risk. Develop- https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1128898
ment and Psychopathology, 28(4, Pt. 2), 1305–1317. https://doi.org/ Hillis, S., Mercy, J., Amobi, A., & Kress, H. (2016). Global prevalence
10.1017/s0954579416000869 of past-year violence against children: A systematic review and
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F.A., & Toth, S.L. (2006). Fostering secure minimum estimates. Pediatrics, 137(3), e20154079. https://doi.org/
attachment in infants in maltreating families through preven- 10.1542/peds.2015-4079
tive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 18(3), 623– Kreppner, J.M., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Colvert, E.,
649. Groothues, C. et al. (2007). Normality and impairment following
Cicchetti, D., & Valentino, K. (2006). An ecological-transactional per- profound early institutional deprivation: A longitudinal follow-up
spective on child maltreatment: Failure of the average expectable into early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 931–946.
environment and its influence on child development. In D. Cicchetti https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.93

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Burden of Infant Mental Health • 703

Lavigne, J.V., Arend, R., Rosenbaum, D., Binns, H.J., Christoffel, K.K., Teicher, M.H., & Samson, J.A. (2016). Annual Research Review: En-
& Gibbons, R.D. (1998). Psychiatric disorders with onset in the during neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect.
preschool years: I. Stability of diagnoses. Journal of the American Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(3), 241–266.
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 37(12), 1246–1254. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12507
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199812000-00007 Tereno, S., Madigan, S., Lyons-Ruth, K., Plamondon, A., Atkinson, L.,
Lavigne, J.V., Gibbons, R.D., Christoffel, K.K., Arend, R., Rosenbaum, Guedeney, N. et al. (2017). Assessing a change mechanism in a
D., Binns, H. et al. (1996). Prevalence rates and correlates of psy- randomized home-visiting trial: Reducing disrupted maternal com-
chiatric disorders among preschool children. Journal of the Amer- munication decreases infant disorganization. Development and Psy-
ican Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(2), 204–214. chopathology, 29, 637–649. https://doi.org/S0954579417000232
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199602000-00014 Tizard, B., & Rees, J. (1975). The effect of early institutional rearing on
Lyons-Ruth, K., Zeanah, C.H., Benoit, D., Madigan, S., & Mills-Koonce, the behaviour problems and affectional relationships of four-year-
R. (2014). Disorder and risk for disorder in infancy and toddlerhood. old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16(1),
In E. Mash & R.A. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (3rd ed., 61–73.
pp. 673–736). New York: Guilford Press. Tomlinson, M., Bornstein, M.H., Marlow, M., & Swartz, L. (2014). Im-
Madigan, S., Brumariu, L.E., Villani, V., Atkinson, L., & Lyons-Ruth, balances in the knowledge about infant mental health in rich and
K. (2016). Representational and questionnaire measures of at- poor countries: Too little progress in bridging the gap. Infant Mental
tachment: A meta-analysis of relations to child internalizing and Health Journal, 35(6), 624–629. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21462
externalizing problems. Psychological Bulletin, 142(4), 367–399. Toth, S.L., Petrenko, C.L.M., Gravener-Davis, J.A., & Handley,
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000029 E.D. (2015). Advances in prevention science: A developmental
Meiser-Stedman, R., Smith, P., Glucksman, E., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T. psychopathology perspective. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Develop-
(2008). The posttraumatic stress disorder diagnosis in preschool- mental psychopathology (Vol. 4, pp. 815–873). Hoboken, NJ:
and elementary school-age children exposed to motor vehicle Wiley.
accidents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(10), 1326–1337. Toth, S.L., Rogosch, F.A., Manly, J.T., & Cicchetti, D. (2006). The
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081282 efficacy of toddler-parent psychotherapy to reorganize attachment
Olds, D.L., Sadler, L., & Kitzman, H. (2007). Programs for parents in the young offspring of mothers with major depressive disor-
of infants and toddlers: Recent evidence from randomized trials. der: A randomized preventive trial. Journal of Consulting and
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3–4), 355–391. Clinical Psychology, 74(6), 1006–1016. https://doi.org/10.1037/
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01702.x 0022-006x.74.6.1006
Perry, B.D. (2004). Maltreated children: Experience, brain development, United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF) Office of the High Com-
and the next generation. New York: Norton. missioner for Human Rights Regional Office for Europe. (2011).
Richman, N., Stevenson, J.E., & Graham, P.J. (1975). Prevalence of be- End placing children under three years in institutions: A call to ac-
haviour problems in 3-year-old children: An epidemiological study tion. Retrieved May 10, 2016 from www.unicef.org/ceecis/UNICEF_
in a London borough. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, ECD_What_Parliaments_V4.pdf
16(4), 277–287. United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF). (2009). Progress for children:
Shonkoff, J.P., Garner, A.S., The Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of A report card on child protection (No. 8). Retrieved May 10, 2016
Child and Family Health, Committee on Early Childhood, Adop- from http://www.unicef.org/ceecis/UNICEF_A_call_to_action_
tion, and Dependent Care, and Section on Developmental and Be- En_Web%281%29.pdf
havioral Pediatrics. (2012). The lifelong effects of early child- United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF). (2014). Early child-
hood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics, 129(1), e232–e246. hood development: A statistical snapshot. Building better brains
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2663 and sustainable outcomes for children. Retrieved May 10, 2016
Skovgaard, A.M., Houmann, T., Christiansen, E., Landorph, S., Jorgensen, from https://www.unicef.org/earlychildhood/files/ECD_Brochure_
T., Olsen, E. M. et al. (2007). The prevalence of mental health prob- FINAL_LR.pdf
lems in children 1(1/2) years of age—The Copenhagen Child Cohort U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Chil-
2000. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(1), 62–70. dren and Families, & Children’s Bureau. (2016). Childhood maltreat-
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01659.x ment 2014. Retrieved May 10, 2016 from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/
Smyke, A.T., Zeanah, C.H., Fox, N.A., Nelson, C.A., & Guthrie, D. cb/resource/child-maltreatment-2014
(2010). Placement in foster care enhances quality of attachment van IJzendoorn, M.H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.
among young institutionalized children. Child Development, 81(1), (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis
212–223. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01390.x of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psy-
Tamminen, T., & Puura, K. (2015). Infant/early years mental health. In chopathology, 11(2), 225–249.
A. Thapar, D.S. Pine, J.F. Leckman, S. Scott, M.J. Snowling, & E. von Klitzing, K., Dohnert, M., Kroll, M., & Grube, M. (2015). Men-
Taylor (Eds.), Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry (pp. 79–92). tal disorders in early childhood. Deutsches Arzteblatt International,
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 112(21–22), 375–386. https://doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2015.0375

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
704 • K. Lyons-Ruth et al.

World Health Organization. (1993). ICD-10, the ICD-10 classification of Anxiety Disorders
mental and behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research.
Geneva, Switzerland: Author. ◦ Separation Anxiety Disorder
World Health Organization. (2003). Caring for children and adolescents ◦ Social Anxiety Disorder (Social Phobia)
with mental disorders: Setting WHO directions. Geneva, Switzer- ◦ Generalized Anxiety Disorder
land: Author.
◦ Selective Mutism
World Health Organization. (2016). INSPIRE: Seven strategies for ending
violence against children. Geneva, Switzerland: Author. ◦ Inhibition to Novelty Disorder
World Health Organization & Fundação Calouste Gulbenkian. (2015). ◦ Other Anxiety Disorder
Promoting rights and community living for children with psychoso-
Mood Disorders
cial disabilities. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Zeanah, C.H., Scheeringa, M., Boris, N.W., Heller, S.S., Smyke, A.T., ◦ Depressive Disorder of Early Childhood
& Trapani, J. (2004). Reactive attachment disorder in maltreated ◦ Disorder of Dysregulated Anger and Aggression of Early
toddlers. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28(8), 877–888. https://doi.org/
Childhood
10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.010
◦ Other Mood Disorder
Zeanah, C.H., Smyke, A.T., & Dumitrescu, A. (2002). Attachment
disturbances in young children. II: Indiscriminate behavior and Obsessive Compulsive and Related Disorders
institutional care. Journal of the American Academy of Child
& Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(8), 983–989. https://doi.org/10.1097/ ◦ Obsessive Compulsive Disorder
00004583-200208000-00017 ◦ Tourette’s Disorder
Zeanah, P.D., Stafford, B., & Zeanah, C.H. (2005). Clinical interventions
◦ Motor or Vocal Tic Disorder
to enhance infant mental health: A selective review. Los Angeles:
UCLA Center for Healthier Children, Families and Communities. ◦ Trichotillomania
ZERO TO THREE. (2005). DC:0–3R: Diagnostic classification of mental ◦ Skin Picking Disorder
health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood ◦ Other Obsessive Compulsive and Related Disorders
(Rev. ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
ZERO TO THREE. (2016). DC:0–5 Diagnostic classification of mental Sleep, Eating, and Crying Disorders
health and developemental disorders of infancy and early childhood
◦ Sleep Disorders
(Rev. ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
 Sleep Onset Disorder
 Night Waking Disorder
APPENDIX A  Partial-Arousal Sleep Disorder
 Nightmare Disorder of Early Childhood
Infant Mental Health Disorders from the Diagnostic Classification
of Mental Health and Developmental Disorders of Infancy and ◦ Eating Disorders of Infancy
Early Childhood (ZERO TO THREE, 2016)  Overeating Disorder
 Undereating Disorder
Axis I: Clinical Disorders  Atypical Eating Disorder
Neurodevelopmental Disorders
◦ Crying Disorder of Infancy
◦ Autism Spectrum Disorder  Excessive Crying Disorder
◦ Early Atypical Autism Spectrum Disorder  Other Disorder of Sleep, Eating, or Crying
◦ Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Trauma, Stress, and Deprivation Disorders
◦ Overactivity Disorder of Toddlerhood
◦ Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
◦ Global Developmental Delay
◦ Adjustment Disorder
◦ Developmental Language Disorder
◦ Complicated Grief Disorder of Early Childhood
◦ Developmental Coordination Disorder
◦ Reactive Attachment Disorder
◦ Other Neurodevelopmental Disorder
◦ Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder
Sensory Processing Disorders ◦ Other Trauma, Stress, and Deprivation Related Disorder

◦ Sensory Over-Responsivity Disorder Relationship-Specific Disorders


◦ Sensory Under-Responsivity Disorder Note. Existing epidemiological data are based on the previous
◦ Other Sensory Processing Disorder 2005 version of the classification system. The new DC:0–5 criteria

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
10970355, 2017, 6, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/imhj.21674 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [27/06/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Burden of Infant Mental Health • 705

outlined here were released in 2016, but no prevalence data are yet F95.9 – Tic disorder, unspecified
available for this new version. F98: Other Behavioural and Emotional Disorders With Onset
Usually Occurring in Childhood and Adolescence
APPENDIX B F98.0 – Nonorganic enuresis
F98.1 – Nonorganic encopresis
International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Revision: F98.2 – Feeding disorder of infancy and childhood
Behavioural and Emotional Disorders With Onset Usually F98.3 – Pica of infancy and childhood
Occurring in Childhood and Adolescence (F90–F98) F98.4 – Stereotyped movement disorders
F90: Hyperkinetic Disorders F98.5 – Stuttering [stammering]
F90.0 – Disturbance of activity and attention F98.6 – Cluttering
F90.1 – Hyperkinetic conduct disorder F98.8 – Other specified behavioural and emotional disor-
F90.8 – Other hyperkinetic disorders ders associated with onset usually occurring in
F90.9 – Hyperkinetic disorder, unspecified childhood and adolescence
F98.9 – Behavioural and emotional disorders associated
F91: Conduct Disorders
with onset usually occurring in childhood and
F91.0 – Conduct disorder confined to the family
adolescence, unspecified
context
F91.1 – Unsocialized conduct disorder
F91.2 – Socialized conduct disorder APPENDIX C
F91.3 – Oppositional defiant disorder
Rates of Specific Disorders Reported by Skovgaard et al. (2007) in
F91.8 – Other conduct disorders
a Representative Sample of 18-Month-Old Danish Children
F91.9 – Conduct disorder, unspecified
F92: Mixed Disorders of Conduct and Emotion The most frequent child diagnosis was the DC:0–3 diagnosis of
F92.0 – Depressive conduct disorder regulatory disorder, which was diagnosed in 7.1%.
F92.8 – Other mixed disorders of conduct and emotion The prevalence of diagnosed feeding disorders in 18-month-
F92.9 – Mixed disorder of conduct and emotion, unspec- old infants was 2.4%, using DC:0–3 criteria. However, estimates of
ified the incidence and prevalence rates of severe feeding disorders, also
termed failure to thrive (FTT), depends on the demographics of
F93: Emotional Disorders With Onset Specific to Childhood
the population studied, with higher rates occurring in economically
F93.0 – Separation anxiety disorder of childhood
disadvantaged rural and urban areas. In the United States, FTT is
F93.1 – Phobic anxiety disorder of childhood
estimated to affect 10% of those living below the poverty level
F93.2 – Social anxiety disorder of childhood
in rural and urban areas, 20% of infants born prematurely, 1 to
F93.3 – Sibling rivalry disorder
5% of infants under age 2 years admitted to hospitals, and up
F93.9 – Other childhood emotional disorders
to 30% of infants seen in emergency room and ambulatory care
F93.9 – Childhood emotional disorder, unspecified
settings (Lyons-Ruth, Zeanah, Benoit, Madigan, & Mills-Koonce,
F94: Disorders of Social Functioning With Onset Specific to 2014).
Childhood and Adolescence Skovgaard et al. (2007) also found some type of affective
F94.0 – Elective mutism disorder (depressive mood, anxiety, or rage lasting at least 2 weeks)
F94.1 – Reactive attachment disorder of childhood in 2.8% of infants. However, among young children exposed to
F94.2 – Disinhibited attachment disorder of childhood a specific traumatic event, e.g., a motor vehicle accident, Meiser-
F94.8 – Other childhood disorders of social functioning Stedman, Smith, Glucksman, Yule, and Dalgleish (2008) found that
F94.9 – Childhood disorder of social functioning, unspec- of the exposed children, 6.5% met posttraumatic stress disorder-
ified Alcoholics Anonymous (PTSD-AA) criteria at 2 to 4 weeks after
F95: Tic Disorders the trauma and 10% at 6 months following the trauma.
F95.0 – Transient tic disorder Using ICD-10 criteria, 2.8% had developmental disorders,
F95.1 – Chronic motor or vocal tic disorder which were convergent with the diagnoses of multisystem devel-
F95.2 – Combined vocal and multiple motor tic disorder opmental disorder (MSDD) in DC:0–3.
[de la Tourette’s syndrome] Using ICD-10 criteria, 2.4% had attention deficit hyperactiv-
F95.8 – Other tic disorders ity disorder (ADHD).

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.

You might also like