You are on page 1of 8

anales de psicología, 2013, vol. 29, nº 1 (enero), 293-300 © Copyright 2013: Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Murcia.

Murcia (España)
http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.29.1.138231 ISSN edición impresa: 0212-9728. ISSN edición web (http://revistas.um.es/analesps): 1695-2294

Frontal variant of Alzheimer's disease and typical Alzheimer's disease:


A comparative study
Bernardino Fernández-Calvo1*, Francisco Ramos2 Virginia Menezes de Lucena3
1 Department of Psychology. CCHLA. Federal University of Paraiba, Brazil
2 Department of Personality, Assessment and Psychological Treatments. University of Salamanca, Spain
3 Department of Internal Medicine. CCM. Federal University of Paraiba, Brazil

Título: La variante frontal de la enfermedad de Alzheimer y la enfermedad Abstract: Clinical heterogeneity is one of the characteristics of Alzheimer's
de Alzheimer típica: un estudio comparativo. disease (AD). Hence, the atypical frontal or dysexecutive presentation is
Resumen: Una de las características de la Enfermedad de Alzheimer (EA) becoming increasingly well-known, although the underlying factors are still
es su heterogeneidad clínica. Así, la presentación atípica frontal o disejecu- unknown. In this study, the neuropsychological performance of two
tiva es cada vez más conocida, aunque los factores subyacentes se descono- groups of patients with AD (frontal variant--ADfv--and typical--TAD)
cen. En este estudio se comparó el rendimiento neuropsicológico de dos were compared. The ADfv group (n = 13) was selected due to the exist-
grupos de pacientes con EA (variante frontal -EAvf- y típica -EAT-). El ence of frontal hypoperfusion on a simple photon emission computer to-
grupo EAvf (n = 13) fue seleccionado por la existencia de una hipocapta- mography (SPECT). The results revealed that the ADfv group displayed a
ción frontal. Los resultados revelaron que el grupo EAvf manifestó un tras- severe dysexecutive disorder, more severe neuropsychiatric symptomatolo-
torno disejecutivo grave, una sintomatología neuropsiquiátrica más severa gy (disinhibition and apathy), more functional impairment, and it generated
(desinhibición y apatía), mayor deterioro funcional y generó mayor sobre- a higher caregiver overload than the TAD group without frontal impair-
carga en el cuidador que el grupo EAT sin afectación frontal (n = 47). A ment (n = 47). Despite the facts that the ADfv group’s performance was
pesar de que el grupo EAvf rindió más bajo en todos los test neuropsicoló- poorer in all the neuropsychological tests, significant group differences
gicos, solo se encontraron diferencias significativas entre ambos grupos en were only found in the processing speed and visuoconstruction tasks. Lo-
las tareas de velocidad de procesamiento y visuoconstrucción. El análisis de gistic regression analysis revealed that the processing speed and mental
regresión logística reveló que las puntuaciones de velocidad de procesa- flexibility scores significantly predicted a diagnosis of ADfv. The existence
miento y flexibilidad mental predicen significativamente el diagnostico de of the grasp reflex, anosognosia, and the absence of apolipoprotein E epsi-
EAvf. La existencia de reflejo de graspin, anosognosia y la no posesión del lon 4 allele (APOE e4) were also more prevalent in the ADfv group. This
APOE e4 también fue más prevalente en el grupo EAvf. Este grupo group had a predominance of males and it was more likely to have a posi-
mostró una predominancia de varones y fue más propenso a tener una his- tive family history of AD. To conclude, the study suggests that ADfv rep-
toria familiar positiva para la EA. Para concluir, el estudio sugiere que la resents a subtype of AD that seems to have different clinical, neuropsycho-
EAvf representa un subtipo de EA que parece tener características clínicas, logical, and genetic characteristics from TAD.
neuropsicológicas y genéticas diferentes a la EAT. Key words: Frontal variant Alzheimer’s disease; executive functions;
Palabras clave: Variante frontal de la enfermedad de Alzheimer; funciones anosognosia; Apolipoprotein E; neuropsychological deficits; neuropsychi-
ejecutivas; anosognosia; Apolipoprotein E; déficits neuropsicológicos; tras- atric disorders.
tornos neuropsiquiátricos.

Introduction This heterogeneity of AD was revealed in the results of a


series of neuropathological (Galton et al., 2000; Kanne,
The typical presentation of Alzheimer's disease (AD) implies Balota, Storandt, McKeel & Morris, 1998), neuropsychologi-
a deterioration of episodic memory, with less severe deficits cal (Becker, Huff, Nebes, Holland & Boller, 1988; Stopford
in attention, semantic memory, and visuospatial skills (Perri, et al., 2008), and neuroimaging (Grady et al., 1990) studies
Watson & Hodges, 2000), which is related to initial that established the existence of three subtypes or atypical
neuropathological lesions in the medial temporal lobe presentations of AD: visual agnosic, aphasic and apraxic
(entorhinal cortex and hippocampal formation), subsequent- (Wallin & Blennow, 1996). These subtypes are closely relat-
ly extending to other regions associated with the neocortex ed to the location, degree, and type of neuropathology pre-
(Braak & Braak, 1991). sent in each one (Galton et al., 2000; Kanne et al., 1998;
However, some authors suggest that impairment of the Lambon et al., 2003).
executive functions (EF) (Lafleche & Albert, 1995) and the In the last decade, another subtype of AD has been dif-
neuropsychiatric alterations (Mega, Lee, Dinov, Mishkin, ferentiated and referenced. Some neuroimaging studies con-
Toga & Cummings, 2000) can also be early characteristics of firm the presence of a subgroup of patients with early AD
AD, with high prognostic value (Chen, Sultzer, Hinkin, who display frontal and temporal-parietal hypometabolism
Mahler & Cummings, 1998; Palmer et al., 2011). In this con- with neuropsychiatric (Chase, Burrows & Mohr, 1987;
text, other profiles of cognitive impairment (Grady et al., Grady et al., 1990; Perani et al., 1988) and dysexecutive dis-
1988; Stopford, Snowden, Thompson & Neary, 2008), with orders (Mann, Mohr, Gearing & Chase, 1992). Johnson,
a different evolution (Galton, Patterson, Xuereb & Hodges, Head, Kim, Starr & Cotman (1999) detected a subtype of
2000; Lambon, Patterson, Graham, Dawson & Hodges, patients, which they called a frontal variant of AD (ADfv),
2003) have been described. which correlates neuropathologically with a higher number
of neurofibrillary tangles (NTFs) in the frontal lobe, and ex-
cessive dysexecutive deficit (e.g., Trail Making Test-A
* Dirección para correspondencia [Correspondence address]:
Dr. Bernardino Fernández Calvo. Departamento de Psicologia. Univer-
[TMT-A] and FAS verbal fluency test [FAS]) in comparison
sidade Federal da Paraíba, Cidade Universitária. Castelo Branco. João to other patients with typical AD (TAD). Subsequently, this
Pessoa, 58059-900, PB Brasil. E-mail: bfcalvo@usal.es executive subgroup of AD has also been reported in a series

- 293 -
294 Bernardino Fernández-Calvo et al.

of clinical cases (Jeong, 2003; Larner, 2006), clinical- hypothesize that both groups of participants will achieve a
anatomo-pathological descriptions (Alladi et al., 2007; similar neuropsychological performance in other non-
Habek, Hajnsek, Zarkovic, Chudy & Mubrin, 2010; Taylor, executive tests. However, we expect to find group differ-
Probst, Miserez, Monsch & Tolnay, 2008), and neuropsy- ences in the frequency of the APOE e4 allele. Specifically,
chological studies (Back-Madruga et al., 2002; Binetti, Magni, the ADfv group will present a lower frequency of the APOE
Padovani, Cappa, Bianchetti & Trabucchi, 1996; Woodward e4 allele.
et al., 2010a).
With regard to patients with TAD, neuropsychological Method
findings show that people affected with ADfv are slower to
process information (Back-Madruga et al., 2002), their func- Participants
tional performance is poorer (Back-Madruga et al., 2002;
Swanberg, Tractenberg, Mohs, Thal & Cummings, 2004; The 84 participants in the study, 60 patients and 24 cog-
Woodward et al., 2010a), their global impairment progresses nitively healthy participants (CH), were selected from the
faster (Woodward et al., 2010a), and they generate more Neurology Section of the University Hospital of Salamanca
caregiver overload (Back-Madruga et al., 2002). It is im- and from the family caregivers association of patients with
portant to note that these signs do not correlate with the se- Alzheimer’s disease (AFA) of Salamanca, respectively. The
verity of the dementia. Moreover, these patients do not usu- data of the patients were collected consecutively from July
ally present positive family history of dementia, and their to December 2002. In contrast, the data of the CH group
symptoms are clinically (Woodward et al., 2010b), but not were collected from January to April of 2003. The CHs and
neuropsychologically, more similar (Woodward et al., 2010a) the person in charge of each patient gave their informed
to frontotemporal dementia (FTD). consent to participate in the study.
However, the neuropsychological identification of this The patients underwent a general clinical, neurological,
ADfv group has not yet been confirmed by means of neu- and neuropsychiatric examination, as well as the following
roimaging tests, so these clinical-neuropsychological results complementary studies: hemogram, Homocysteine, general
should be taken with precaution. Therefore, some of these biochemistry,T4-TSH, vitamin B12 and folic acid, luetic se-
neuropsychological findings may not coincide with the neu- rology, APOE, a neuroimaging study: computed tomogra-
ropsychological data contributed by publications of clinical- phy (CT) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and simple
neuropathological descriptions of patients with ADfv photon emission computer tomography (SPECT), using the
(Habek et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2008). In this sense, more radiopharmaceutic 99mTc-hexamethylpropyleneamine
research is needed of people affected with ADfv, defined oxime (Tc99m-HMPAO). The assessment of each brain
through neuroimaging studies that confirm the SPECT was carried out visually by a radiologist who was
neuroanatomic connection of the executive deficits, in order blind to the patients' clinical diagnosis, and who classified
to analyze these and other neuropsychological characteris- the perfusion deficits according to the criteria proposed by
tics, or features of another nature (e.g., genetic--the presence Holman et al. (Holman, Johnson, Gerada, Carvalho & Satlin,
of apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele (APOE e4). A better de- 1992).
scription of ADfv could help to differentiate it from TAD The diagnosis of AD was made according to the criteria
and FTD. In this sense, some authors note that certain pa- of the National Institute of Neurologic, Communicative
tients affected with neuropathologically confirmed AD, and Disorders and Stroke - Alzheimer's Disease and Related
who present executive deficits and neuropsychiatric disor- Disorders Association (McKhann, Drachman, Folstein,
ders in addition to amnesia, are diagnosed with FTD (Alladi Katzman, Price & Stadlan, 1984). The diagnosis was sup-
et al., 2007; Brun, 1987) or AD (Balasa et al., 2011) at the ported by a neuropsychological assessment using screening
onset of the clinical picture; a diagnosis that is maintained tests and a neuropsychological battery developed to assess
until their deaths despite their manifesting, in the case of diverse cognitive functions (memory, language, praxis,
FTD, clear mnestic disorders a few years after the diagnosis visuospatial function, attention, and executive functions),
(Alladi et al., 2007). Whereas other works suggest that, in the which has been shown to be efficacious to diagnose demen-
atypical presentations of AD, there is a low prevalence of tia (Contador, Fernandez-Calvo, Cacho, Ramos & Hernán-
APOE e4 allele (Snowden et al., 2007) and that AD patients dez-Martín, 2009). Only patients who met the criteria of
who are not APOE 4 carriers present greater executive def- "probable" Alzheimer-type dementia and who had a mild
icits (Wolk et al., 2010). degree of dementia (CDR-1) according to the Clinical De-
Thus, this investigation attempts to compare the neuro- mentia Rating (Hughes, Berg, Danziger, Coben & Martin,
psychological performance of the ADfv group, defined on 1982) were included. All the patients were exempt from
the basis of frontal hypoperfusion, and the TAD group signs of focal deficit in the neurological exploration--less
(without frontal hypoperfusion), also analyzing the relation than four points on the Hachinski scale (Hachinski, et al.,
between these two groups of patients with the APOE e4 al- 1975)--and from focal lesions in the CT or the MRI of the
lele. First, we hypothesize that the ADfv group will display brain. None of them met the criteria for major depression
more executive deficits than the TAD group. Second, we according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

anales de psicología, 2013, vol. 29, nº 1 (enero)


Frontal variant of Alzheimer’s disease and typical Alzheimer’s disease: A comparative study 295

Disorders, 4th edition (American Psychiatric Association, nández, Llinàs, López-Pousa & López, 1999); Functional Al-
1994). terations: Interview for Deterioration in Daily Living Activi-
The patients with AD were classified according to the ties in Dementia (IDDD; Böhm, Peña-Casanova, Aguilar,
result of the brain SPECT as: frontal variant AD (ADfv, n = Hernández, Sol & Blesa, 1998); Caregiver burden: Zarit Burden
13) or typical AD (TAD, n = 47). The pattern of Interview (ZBI; Izal & Montorio, 1994).
hypoperfusion found in the patients was: normal (3), frontal
(13), unilateral temporal (14), and bilateral temporoparietal Statistical analyses
(30).
Lastly, during the clinical interview, we confirmed the The statistical analysis of the data was carried out with
level of awareness of their own cognitive deficits, using a the software of the Statistical Package for Social Sciences
semi-quantitative scale developed by Reed, Jagust & Coulter (SPSS) version 17. The χ2 contingency test was used for di-
(1993), the presence of positive family history of AD, de- chotomous variables. Group differences in the continuous
fined as having at least one biological parent with AD, ex- variables were analyzed with nonparametric tests (the Mann-
trapyramidal signs (rigidity, tremor, bradykinesia), and grasp Whitney U or the Kruskal-Wallis H), as the groups had a
reflex. In order to guarantee the diagnosis, each patient with small and unequal n. Subsequently, multiple comparisons
AD was followed up for at least five years to confirm the were examined with Dunn's test. Lastly, logistic regression
AD diagnosis. analysis was used to determine the scores of the neuropsy-
Control participants. The 24 CH participants were healthy chological tests that were capable of predicting the presence
older Spanish-speaking people, who lived independently, or absence of ADfv. The level of significance was p < .05.
were not institutionalized, and who maintained their daily
activities, both basic and instrumental, integrally. People Results
who presented a history of psychiatric or neurological dis-
ease, or alcoholism, or who were under psychopharmacolog- Table 1 shows the participants' sociodemographic and clini-
ical treatment and who had a score equal to or higher than cal characteristics. In comparison to the TAD group, the
27 in the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE] were ex- ADfv group was made up predominantly of males, was
cluded from the sample (Folstein, Folstein & McHugh, more likely to have a positive family history of AD, display-
1975). All the participants from the CH group performed ing significantly higher frequency of anosognosia, grasp re-
the same psychometric and neuropsychological tests and in flex, and a lower proportion of the APOE e4 allele. Like-
the same sequence as the group of patients. wise, the ADfv group presented a significant increase in
functional and neuropsychiatric impairment and generated
Measures greater caregiver overload, despite the fact that both groups
of patients were balanced in their degree of impairment and
All the participants were assessed by means of a broad years of evolution of symptomatology (TAD, range 1 to 4.5
neuropsychological battery that explored diverse cognitive years; ADfv, range 1.5 to 4.0 years).
functions, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and functional capac- The executive functioning of the ADfv group was also
ity: Processing speed: Trail Making Test-part A (TMT-A; significantly lower in all five executive tests administered
Strauss, Sherman & Spreen, 2006); Digit Symbol Substitu- (Table 2). In view of these differences, we decided to deter-
tion subtests (DSST) of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence mine the number of patients of each group with significantly
Scale-revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981); Attention: Forward deteriorated scores in the executive tests, which consisted of
digit span subtests (FDS) of the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981); displaying < 2SDs from the CH group's mean in each meas-
A-Random Letter Test of Auditory Vigilance (A-Test; Strub ure, after standardizing by age and schooling.
& Black, 2000); Working memory: Backward digit span sub- A greater number of patients from the ADfv group had
tests (BDS) of the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981); Verbal memory: significantly worse scores in the TMT-B-, Stroop C, and
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-RA (HVLT-RA; Benedict, FAS measures. In two other executive measures, there were
Schretlen, Groninger & Brandt, 1998); Visual memory: Ben- also a higher number of ADfv patients who performed
ton Visual Retention Test (BVRT; Benton, 1981); Rey- worse than the patients from the TAD group, although the
Osterreith Complex Figure (ROCF), delayed reproduction difference was marginal. Moreover, 11 of these 13 patients
(Rey, 1987); Semantic memory: animal fluency test (AF; Strauss, from the ADfv group presented significantly worse scores in
et al., 2006); Visuoconstruction: Clock drawing test copy condi- at least 4 out of the 5 executive tests administered. In con-
tion (CDT-C; Cacho et al., 2005); copy of the ROCF (Rey, trast, none of the TAD patients obtained deteriorated scores
1987); Executive functioning: TMT-B (Strauss, et al., 2006), in more than two executive tests (Table 3). Additionally, the
Stroop test (part C) (Strauss et al., 2006), FAS (Strauss et al., ADfv group employed worse production strategies when
2006); Similarities subtest (S) and Comprehension subtest copying the ROCF than the TAD group (χ2 = 20.4, p
(C) of the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981); Performance strategies <.001).
of the ROCF (Rey, 1987); Neuropsychiatric Alteration : Neuro-
psychiatric Inventory (NPI; Vilalta-Franch, Lozano, Her-

anales de psicología, 2013, vol. 29, nº 1 (enero)


296 Bernardino Fernández-Calvo et al.

Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the participants.


Patients CH
Statistic p value†
TAD (N = 47) ADfv (N = 13) N = 24
Age; years 75.9 (5.5) 72.8 (7.6) 72.8 (4.6) H (2) = 4.2 ns‡
Sex (M/F) 43/57 69/31 45/54 χ2 (2) = 2.9 ns
Education; years 8.7 (4.2) 9.2 (3.3) 9.6(3.7) H (2) = 0.1 ns
Evolution; years 3.05 (2.1) 2.4 (1.5) ______ U = 268.0 ns‡
FH; n (%) 19 (40%) 9 (69%) ______ χ2 (1) = 3.3 .002
AG; n (%) 21 (45%) 12 (92%) ______ χ2 (1) = 9.3 .02
APOE e 4; n (%) 25 (53%) 2 (15%) ______ χ2 (1) = 5.3 ns
Presence ESP; n (%) 9 (20%) 3 (23%) ______ χ2 (1) = 0.1 .001
Presence GR; n (%) 1 (2%) 8 (62%) ______ χ2 (1) = 28.1 a, b
MMSE 22.8 (1.2) 22.5 (2.1) 28.3 (1.3) H(2) = 46.8* a, b
CDT-CM 6.2 (1.8) 5.4 (2.1) 8.9 (1.1) H(2) = 31.0* a, b, c
NPI 10.8 (4.6) 19.9 (9.2) 0.7 (2.5) H(2) = 12.3* a, b, c
IDDD 45.2 (4.5) 55.6 (6.8) 33.3 (0.4) H(2) = 54.3* .04
ZBI 20.2 (10.0) 28.2 (12.3) _______ U = 195.0* ns‡
Note. TAD = traditional Alzheimer's disease; ADfv = Alzheimer's disease frontal variant; CH = healthy control subjects; FH = family history of AD; AG =
anosognosia; EPS = extrapyramidal signs; GR = grasp reflex; MMSE = Mini Mental State Examination; CDT-CM = clock drawing test command condition;
NPI = neuropsychiatric inventory; IDDD = interview for deterioration in daily living activities in dementia; ZBI = Zarit Burden Interview; The Standard de-
viation in brackets; χ2 = Chi square statistic; H Kruskal-Wallis statistic; U Mann-Whitney Statistic; a: CH and AD differences; b: CH and ADfv differences; c:
TAD and ADfv differences; *p < .001 between all the groups; † indicates the significance of p-value according to Dunn's test; ‡ indicates marginal difference.

Table 2. Comparison of means of the participants in the executive tests


Patients CH
H (2) Comparisons†
TAD (N =47) Adfv (N =13) (N = 24)
TMT-B; s 302.6 (152.4) 739.3 (429.0) 195.2 (72.2) 44.0* HC >TAD>ADfv
Stroop C; s 66.3 (45.9) 100.4 (40.3) 32.3 (6.6) 41.3* HC >TAD> ADfv
S 8.1 (3.7) 3.9 (1.8) 13.6 (4.2) 31.4* HC >TAD> ADfv
C 13.1 (3.9) 9.6 (5.4) 19.3 (3.5) 28.8* HC >TAD> ADfv
FAS 21.5 (11.3) 14.8 (16.1) 32.0 (13.1) 14.3* HC >TAD> ADfv
Note. TAD = traditional Alzheimer's disease; ADfv = Alzheimer's disease frontal variant; CH = healthy control subjects; TMT-B = Trail Making Test, part B;
S = Similarities of the WAIS-R; C = Comprehension of the WAIS-R; FAS = FAS verbal fluency test; The standard deviation is in brackets; s = seconds; H
Kruskal-Wallis statistic; *p < .001 among all the groups; † indicates the significance of p-value according to Dunn's test; ‡ indicates marginal difference.

Table 3. Frequency of deterioration in the executive tests


Executive scales TAD (N = 47) ADfv (N = 13)
χ2 p value
Patients below the cut-off point (%) Patients below the cut-off point (%)
TMT-B 18 (39%) 13 (100%) 7.9 .01
Stroop C 25(53%) 13 (100%) 8.9 .04
S 17 (36%) 11 (85%) 4.1 .05
C 8 (18%) 11 (85%) 2.3 .08
FAS 10(21%) 12 (92%) 11.4 .01
Note. TAD = traditional Alzheimer's disease; ADfv = Alzheimer's disease frontal variant; TMT-B = Trail Making Test, part B; S = Similarities of the WAIS-
R; C = Comprehension of the WAIS-R; FAS = FAS verbal fluency test.

The patients from the ADfv group displayed more apa- Table 4. Comparison of means of patients in the NPI subscales.
thy, disinhibition, and worse processing speed and NPI Subscale TAD (N = 47) ADfv (N = 17) p value
visuoconstruction than the TAD group (see Tables 4 and 5). Delusions 0.7 (0.9) 0.5 (0.9) .20
Hallucinations 0.1 (0.3) 0.2 (0.4) .60
It is important to note that the performance of the ADfv
Agitation 0.9 (0.9) 1.2 (1.5) .80
group was lower in all the cognitive functions analyzed. In Depression 0.8 (1.1) 1.4 (1.7) .33
fact, in some of them, we found marginal significance, such Anxiety 1.4 (1.7) 0.9 (1.1) .11
as the case of immediate and delayed visual memory. Euphoria 0.7 (1.2) 0.9 (1.1) .10
Apathy 1.9 (1.7) 5.8(3.0) .001
Logistic Regression Analysis Disinhibition 0.5 (0. 8) 3.9 (3. 8) .001
Irritability/Lability 1.0 (1.2) 1.5(2.2) .70
In the logistic regression analysis, we entered the scores Aberrant motor behaviors 1.1 (1.7) 1.9(2.2) .13
of the tests that were lower in the ADfv patients than in the Sleep disorders 0.9 (2.2) 1.1 (2.8) .45
Eating disorders 0.8 (1.7) 0.9 (0.9) .15
TAD patients (TMT-A, DSST, CDT-C, ROCF-DR, TMT- Note. NPI = neuropsychiatric inventory; TAD = traditional Alzheimer's dis-
B, Stroop- C, S, C, FAS). The model was significant (χ2 = ease; ADfv = Alzheimer's disease frontal variant; The standard deviation is
in brackets.

anales de psicología, 2013, vol. 29, nº 1 (enero)


Frontal variant of Alzheimer’s disease and typical Alzheimer’s disease: A comparative study 297

13.3, p < .001); the TMT-B tests [Exp(B) = .97, CI: (.95– also include the clinical sphere. Our results show that
.99), p = .02], FAS [Exp(B) = .78, CI: (.64–.94), p = .01], and dysexecutive impairment is more pronounced and homoge-
TMT-A [Exp(B) = .96, CI: (.95–.99), p = .01] significantly neous in the ADfv subgroup than in the TAD group, to the
predicted the presence of ADfv, correctly classifying 82% of extent that approximately 77% of the participants of the
the patients (69% ADfv and 85% AD). ADfv group displayed severe impairment in the five execu-
tive measures analyzed in the study. The other 13% showed
Discussion impairment in at least three executive tests. In contrast, 47%
of the patients of the TAD group scored within the normal
This work reveals the existence of a subgroup of AD with range in all the tests, whereas the rest of the patients showed
frontal hypoperfusion that differs from the rest of the pa- impairment in just one measure, and there was more hetero-
tients with AD in a series of neuropsychological and genetic geneity in the deteriorated test.
characteristics. Moreover, we found that these differences

Table 5. Participants' performance in the non-executive neuropsychological tests.


TAD (N = 47) ADfv (N = 13) CH (N = 24) H (2) Comparisons†
TMT-A; s 112.1 (70.4) 345.2 (84.6) 64.2 (26.3) 22.6* ADfv<TAD<HC
Processing speed
DSST 8.6 (4.9) 5.1 (6.9) 27.9 (8.2) 48.3* ADfv<TAD<HC
FDS 4.7 (0.8) 4.0 (1.5) 5.7 (1.0) 24.0* ADfv=TAD<HC
Attention
A-Test; n°e 3.4 (2.0) 4.0 (3.1) 0.7 (1.3) 12.4* ADfv=TAD<HC
Working memory BDS 3.5 (0.9) 3.0 (1.3) 4.9 (1.1) 13.7* ADfv=TAD<HC
VFR 11.2 (3.8) 8.4 (2.6) 22.8 (3.2) 39.9* ADfv=TAD<HC
VFR´ 0.6 (0.5) 0.4 (0.7) 8.3 (2.5) 62.7* ADfv=TAD <HC
Verbal memory VGR 2.9 (1.6) 1.8 (2.4) 8.9 (2.7) 38.9* ADfv=TAD <HC
VR 3.4 (2.0) 2.4 (1.5) 9.3 (2.1) 36.8* ADfv=TAD <HC
BVRT 6.6 (2.4) 5.0 (2.3) 9.8 (0.6) 36.0* ADfv=TAD <HC‡
Semantic memory AF 7.9 (2.7) 6.2 (3.7) 19.9 (4.2) 52.0* ADfv= TAD <HC
CDT-C 6.8 (1.8) 4.9 (2.0) 9.6 (0.6) 12.6* ADfv<TAD =HC
Visuoconstruction
ROCF-C 15.2 (10.2) 10.1 (7.6) 32.7 (3.1) 28.4* ADfv<TAD =HC
Note. TAD = traditional Alzheimer's disease; ADfv = Alzheimer's disease frontal variant; CH = healthy control subjects; TMT-A = Trail Making Test part A;
DSST = Digit Symbol Substitution subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-revised (WAIS-R); FDS = Forward Digit Span subtest of the WAIS-R;
A-Test = A-Random Letter Test of Auditory Vigilance; BDS = Backward Digit Span subtest of the WAIS-R; VFR = verbal immediate free recall of the
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-RA (HVLT-RA); VFR´ = verbal delayed free recall of the HVLT-RA; VGR = verbal Guided recall of the HVLT-RA; VR =
Verbal recognition of the HVLT-RA; BVRT = Benton visual retention test; ROCF-DR = Delayed reproduction of the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure; AF=
animal fluency test; CDT-C = Clock drawing test copy condition; ROCF-C = Copy of the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure; The standard deviation is in
brackets; s = seconds; nr e = total number of errors; * p < .001 among all the groups; † indicates the significance of p-value according to Dunn's test; ‡ indi-
cates marginal difference.

Therefore, we can consider that the existence of a TAD. To our knowledge, this investigation is the first work
dysexecutive disorder (< 2 SD) at the onset of the disease that combined neuroimaging and a neuropsychological
seems characteristic of a subgroup of patients with atypical method to define and analyze the ADfv group.
dysexecutive presentation of AD. Other neuropathological Furthermore, the ADfv group presents an increase in the
and neuropsychological studies (Back-Madruga et al., 2002; neuropsychiatric symptomatology, greater functional im-
Stopford et al., 2008; Woodward et al., 2010a; Woodward et pairment, and it generates greater caregiver overload than
al., 2010b) have observed this type of atypical presentation the TAD group. On the one hand, these results confirm the
with early impairment of the frontal lobe in AD. For exam- findings of other neuropsychological studies (Back-Madruga
ple, Stopford et al. (2008) found that 22% of the patients et al., 2002; Woodward et al., 2010a), clinical (Larner, 2006;
studied presented impairment in a nonamnestic single do- Woodward et al., 2010b) and/or neuropathological (Habek
main (language, visuospatial and praxis), and the et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2008); on the other hand, they
dysexecutive presentation was the most frequent (9%). The show the relation between dysexecutive disorder and the
patients of this group were more prone to display frontal presence of functional and behavioral impairments in early
hypoperfusion than the other patients. In contrast, patients AD (Chen et al., 1998), verifying that the increase in the
with aphasic presentation did not display frontal alterations neuropsychiatric symptoms of AD (e.g., disinhibition) is sig-
in the SPECT. Other neuroimaging studies (Grady et al., nificantly related to greater impairment of the frontal lobe
1988; Perani et al., 1988) also detected the presence of a (Bruen, McGeown, Shanks & Venneri, 2008; Mega et al.,
subgroup of AD with frontal hypoperfusion that coexists 2000) and to caregiver overload (Mohamed, Rosenheck,
with bilateral temporal and parietal dysfunction, but they did Lyketsos & Schneider, 2010). In this sense, caregivers of
not use a neuropsychological criterion to analyze the degree other kinds of non-Alzheimer dementias show higher rates
of impairment of the executive deficits nor did they carry of overload than caregivers of patients with AD (Ricci et al.,
out a comparative study with another type of patients with 2009).

anales de psicología, 2013, vol. 29, nº 1 (enero)


298 Bernardino Fernández-Calvo et al.

However, the performance of the ADfv group in other the noncarriers of APOE e4 allele present greater impair-
neuropsychological skills is similar to that of the TAD ment of the executive functions (Wolk et al., 2010). In any
group, except for their greater impairment in processing event, it is noted that other atypical presentations of AD
speed and visuoconstruction, with worse planning strategies. have also been associated with a low prevalence of APOE
This result partially coincides with that of Back-Madruga et e4, which suggests that this gene may not be very relevant as
al. (2002) and corroborates the neuropsychological descrip- a risk factor for atypical phenotypes (Snowden et al., 2007).
tions carried out in the series of clinical cases with this vari- This study also has some limitations. First, the
ant of AD (Jeong et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 1999; Taylor et dysexecutive disorder presented by the patients with AD
al., 2008). may be secondary to white matter hyperintensities in the
Lastly, our ADfv group presented a higher frequency of fronto-subcortical circuits. However, this seems improbable;
grasp reflex and anosognosia, and lower presence of the we included the criterion of scoring ≤ 4 on a Hachinski is-
APOE e4 allele. The existence of anosognosia in the ADfv chemic scale, which reduces the significant presence of vas-
group has also been described in the work of Taylor et al. cular disease. Second, it is possible that the participants of
(2008). Moreover, Starkstein, Jorge, Mizrahi, Adrian & Rob- the ADfv group presented frontotemporal dementia (FTD)
inson (2007) suggest that the people affected by AD with instead of AD, because of their notable neuropsychiatric and
greater frontal impairment may develop anosognosia even at dysexecutive disorder. Nevertheless, this seems improbable
initial stages of the disease, as our results indicate. Amanzio for two reasons. On the one hand, the mean age at the onset
et al. (2011) consider that the impairment in cognitive flexi- of the disease in the ADfv group was relatively higher (72
bility may be a prerequisite for anosognosia in AD. In our years) than the age generally observed in FTD (onset before
study, cognitive flexibility, measured with the TMT-B and 65 years). On the other hand, the ADfv group showed a
the FAS, is the executive component that undergoes the visuospatial deficit; a skill that is relatively well preserved in
greatest alteration in the ADfv group, and less in the TAD the initial phase of frontotemporal dementia (Hutchinson &
group, and it significantly predicts, along with the TMT-A Mathias, 2007). Lastly, there was no neuropathological con-
score, the diagnosis in 69% of the cases of ADfv and it ex- firmation of the clinical diagnoses presented in this study.
cluded the diagnosis in 85% of the cases of TAD.
However, we consider that the existence of greater apa- Conclusions
thy and disinhibition in the ADfv group has to do with the
neuroanatomic relation (e.g., dorsolateral frontal and anteri- Despite these limitations, the results of the study show the
or cingulate cortex) that seems to exist between these symp- clinical heterogeneity of early AD by detecting a subgroup of
toms, executive functioning and anosognosia (Amanzio et AD, called ADfv, with phenotypical clinical and neuropsy-
al., 2011; Starkstein, Brockman, Bruce & Petracca, 2010). In chological characteristics that are different from those of
this sense, the present results confirm the opinion of some TAD; characteristics that, in the absence of neuropa-
authors who state that flexible thinking and the ability to in- thological confirmation, seem to be associated with a variety
hibit a response seem to be essential skills to become aware of risk factors and genes, and not merely with APOE. Alt-
of the cognitive deficits in the daily life of patients with AD hough ADfv is a rare atypical presentation (Wallin &
(Amanzio et al., 2011). Likewise, the most severe Blennow, 1996), it is necessary to carry out other studies in
visuoconstructive deficits of patients with focal brain dam- order to advance in the phenotypical and neuropathological
age are related to anosognosia and the impairment of the description of this variant of AD. This information may help
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Rinaldi, Piras & Pizzamiglio, to homogenize patients for pharmacological investigation
2010). In fact, Fink, et al. (1999) indicate that these struc- and facilitate clinical practice, improving the differential and
tures are essential to monitor the congruence between one's prognostic diagnosis among the diverse clinical phenotypes
expectations and what is actually done when carrying out of AD and other non-Alzheimer dementias (e.g., fronto-
goal-directed actions. It is feasible that an impairment of temporal dementia).
these frontal structures, which generates incapacity to detect
the incoherence between a foreseen action and the action Acknowledgements.- We thank all the participants for their time
performed when copying or reproducing models (Rinaldi et and commitment to the study. The authors also thank Dr. Israel
al., 2010), may have caused more severe visuoconstructive Contador for his very helpful comments on this paper.
deficits in the ADfv group, compared to the TAD group.
Some authors relate the absence of the APOE e4 allele Funding support.- The study was supported in part by Velum
foundation.
in AD to a greater atrophy in the frontoparietal structures
(e.g., dorsofrontal-parietal atrophy; Wolk et al., 2010). Thus,

anales de psicología, 2013, vol. 29, nº 1 (enero)


Frontal variant of Alzheimer’s disease and typical Alzheimer’s disease: A comparative study 299

References
Alladi, S., Xuereb, J., Bak, T., Nestor, P., Knibb, J., Patterson, K., & Hodges, Habek, M., Hajnsek, S., Zarkovic, K., Chudy, D., & Mubrin, Z. (2010).
J. R. (2007). Focal cortical presentations of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, Frontal variant of Alzheimer's disease: clinico-CSF-pathological correla-
130, 2636-2645. tion. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 37(1), 118-120.
Amanzio, M., Torta, D. M. E., Sacco, K., Cauda, F., D’ Agata, F., Duca, S., . Hachinski, V. C., Iliff, L. D., Zinhla, E., DuBoulay, G. H., McAllister, V.,
. . Geminiani, G. C. (2011). Unawareness of deficits in Alzheimer’s dis- Marshall, L., . . . Symon, L. (1975). Cerebral Blood flow in dementia.
ease: role of the cingulate cortex. Brain, 134, 1061-1076. Archives of Neurology, 32, 632-637.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual Holman, B.L., Johnson, K.A., Gerada, B., Carvalho, P.A., & Satlin, A.,
of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American (1992). The scintigraphic appearance of Alzheimer’s disease: A prospec-
Psychiatric Press. tive study using Technetium-99m-HMPAO SPECT. Journal of Nuclear
Back-Madruga, C., Boone, K. B, Briere, J., Cummings, J., McPherson, S., Medicine 33 (2), 181-185.
Fairbanks, L., & Thompson, E. (2002). Functional ability in executive Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Cohen, L. A., & Martin, R. L.
variant Alzheimer's disease and typical Alzheimer's disease. The Clinical (1982). A new clinical scale for staging of dementia. British Journal of Psy-
Neuropsychologist, 16, 331-340. chiatry, 140, 566-572
Balasa, M., Gelpi, E., Antonell, A., Rey, M. J., Sánchez-Valle, R., Molinuevo, Hutchinson, A. & Mathias, J. L. (2007). Neuropsychological deficits in
J. L., & Lladó, A. (2011). Clinical features and APOE genotype of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analytic re-
pathologically proven early-onset Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 76(20), view. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78, 917-928.
1720-1725. Izal, M., & Montorio, I. (1994). Evaluación del medio y del cuidador del
Becker, J. T., Huff, F. J., Nebes, R. D., Holland, A. & Boller, F., (1988). demente. En T. Del Ser y J. Peña Casanova (eds.). Evaluación neuropsicoló-
Neuropsychological function in Alzheimer's disease: pattern of impair- gica y funcional de la demencia, (pp. 201-212). Barcelona: Prous Science.
ment and rates of progression. Archives of Neurology, 45, 263-268. Jeong, Y., Han, D. H., Yi, H. A., Cho, S. S., Chin, J., Kang, S. J., . . . Na, D.
Benton, A. L. (1981). El Test de Retención Visual. Ediciones TEA. Madrid. L. (2003). Neuropsychological and Neuroimaging Findings of Frontal
Binetti, G., Magni, E., Padovani, A., Cappa, S. F., Bianchetti, A., & Variant of Alzheimer's Disease. Journal of the Korean Neurological Associa-
Trabucchi, M. (1996). Executive dysfunction in early Alzheimer’s dis- tion, 21(1), 32-40.
ease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 60, 91-93. Johnson, J. K., Head, E., Kim, R., Starr, A., & Cotman, C. W. (1999). Clini-
Böhm, P., Peña-Casanova, J., Aguilar, M., Hernández, G., Sol, J. M., & cal and pathological evidence for a frontal variant of Alzheimer’s dis-
Blesa, R. (1998). Clinical validity and utility of the Interview for Deteri- ease. Archives of Neurology, 56, 1233-1239.
oration of Daily Living in Dementia for Spanish-speaking communities. Kanne, S. M., Balota, D. A., Storandt, M., McKeel, Jr., D. W., & Morris, J.
International Psychogeriatric, 10, 261-270. C. (1998). Relating anatomy to function in Alzheimer's disease: neuro-
Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer- psychological profiles predict regional neuropathology 5 years later.
related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82, 239-259. Neurology, 50, 979-985.
Bruen, P. D., McGeown, W. J., Shanks, M. F., & Venneri, A. (2008). Neuro- Lafleche, G., & Albert, M. S. (1995). Executive function deficits in mild
anatomical correlates of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s dis- Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 9, 313-320.
ease. Brain. 131, 2455-2463. Lambon, M. A., Patterson, K., Graham, N., Dawson, K., & Hodges, J. R.
Brun, A. (1987). Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. I. Neu- (2003). Homogeneity and heterogeneity in mild cognitive impairment
ropathology. Archives of Gerontology Geriatric, 6, 193-208. and Alzheimer's disease: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study of 55
Cacho, J. L., García-García, R., Arcaya, J., Gay, F. J., Guerrero, A. L., & cases. Brain, 126, 2350-2362.
Gómez, J. C. (1996). El test del reloj en ancianos sanos. Revista de Neuro- Larner, A. J. (2006). Frontal variant Alzheimer’s disease: A reappraisal. Clini-
logía, 24, 1525-1528. cal Neurology and Neurosurgery, 108(7), 705-708.
Chase, T. N., Burrows, G. H., & Mohr, E. (1987). Cortical glucose utiliza- Mann, U. M., Mohr, E., Gearing, M., & Chase, T. (1992). Heterogeneity in
tion patterns in primary degenerative dementias of the anterior and pos- Alzheimer’s disease: Progression rate segregated by distinct neuropsy-
terior type. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 6, 289-292. chological and cerebral metabolic profiles. Journal of Neurology, Neurosur-
Chen, S. T., Sultzer, D. L., Hinkin, C. H., Mahler, M. E. & Cummings, J. L. gery, and Psychiatry, 55, 956-959.
(1998). Executive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: Association with McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., &
neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional impairment. Journal of Neu- Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report
ropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 10, 426-43. of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of the De-
Contador, I., Fernández-Calvo, B., Cacho, J., Ramos, F., & Hernández- partment of Health and Human Services task force on Alzheimer’s dis-
Martín, L. (2009). La depresión en la demencia tipo Alzheimer: ¿Existe ease. Neurology, 34, 939-944.
algún efecto sobre la cognición?. Revista de neurología, 49(10), 505-510. Mega, M. S., Lee L., Dinov, I. D., Mishkin, F., Toga, A. W., & Cummings, J.
Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., Frith, C. D., Driver, J., Fracko- L. (2000). Cerebral correlates of psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer's
wiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). The neural consequences of conflict disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 69(2), 167-171.
between intention and the senses. Brain, 122, 497–512. Mohamed, S., Rosenheck, R., Lyketsos, C.G., & Schneider, L.S. (2010).
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: Caregiver burden in Alzheimer disease: Cross-sectional and longitudinal
A practical method for grading the state of patients for the clinician. patient correlates. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 917-927.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. Palmer, K., Lupo, F., Perri, R., Salamone, G., Fadda, L., Caltagirone, C., . . .
Galton, C. J., Patterson, K., Xuereb, J. H., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). Atypical Cravello, L. (2011). Predicting disease progression in Alzheimer's dis-
and typical presentations of Alzheimer’s disease: A clinical, neuropsy- ease: the role of neuropsychiatric syndromes on functional and cogni-
chological, neuroimaging and pathological study of 13 cases. Brain, 123, tive decline. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 24(1), 35-345.
484-498. Perani, D., Di Piero, V., Vallar, G., Cappa, S., Messa, C., Bottini, G., . . .
Grady, C. L, Haxby, J. V, Horwitz, B., Sundaram, M., Berg, G., Schapiro, Fazio, F. (1998). Technetium-99m HM-PAO-SPECT study of regional
M.B., . . . Rapoport, S.I. (1988). Longitudinal study of the early neuro- cerebral perfusion in early Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Nuclear Medicine,
psychological and cerebral metabolic changes in dementia of the Alz- 29, 1507-1514.
heimer type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 576- Reed, B. R., Jagust, W. J., & Coulter, L. (1993). Anosognosia in Alzheimer’s
596. disease: Relationships to depression, cognitive function, and cerebral
Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Schapiro, M. B., Gonzalez-Aviles, A., Kumar, A., perfusion. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 15(2), 231-244.
Ball, M. J., . . . Rapoport, S. I. (1990). Subgroups in dementia of the Rey A (1987). Test de copia de la figura compleja. Madrid: TEA.
Alzheimer type identified using positron emission tomography. Journal Ricci, M., Guidoni, S. V., Sepe-Monti, M., Bomboi, G., Antonini, G., Blun-
of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 2, 373-384. do, C., & Giubilei, F. (2009). Clinical findings, functional abilities and
caregiver distress in the early stage of dementia with Lewy bodies

anales de psicología, 2013, vol. 29, nº 1 (enero)


300 Bernardino Fernández-Calvo et al.

(DLB) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, Vilalta-Franch, J., Lozano-Gallego, M., Hernández-Ferrándiz, M., Llinàs-
49(2), e101-e104. Reglà, J., López-Pousa, S., & López, O.L. (1999). El inventario neurop-
Rinaldi, M. C., Piras, F., & Pizzamiglio, L. (2010). Lack of awareness for spa- siquiátrico: propiedades psicométricas de su adaptación al castellano.
tial and verbal constructive apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 48(6), 1574-1582. Revista de Neurología, 29, 15-19.
Snowden, J. S., Stopford, C. L., Julien, C. L., Thompson, J. C., Davidson, Y., Wallin, A., & Blennow, K. (1996). Clinical subgroups of the Alzheimer syn-
Gibbons, L., . . . Mann, D. M. A. (2007). Cognitive phenotypes in Alz- drome. Acta neurologica Scandinavica Supplementum, 165, 51-57.
heimer's disease and genetic risk. Cortex, 43(7), 835-845. Perry, R. J., Watson, P., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). The nature and staging of
Starkstein, S.E., Brockman, S., Bruce, D., & Petracca, G. (2010). attention dysfunction in early (minimal and mild) Alzheimer's disease:
Anosognosia Is a Significant Predictor of Apathy in Alzheimer’s dis- Relationship to episodic and semantic memory impairment.
ease. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 378-383. Neuropsychologia 38, 252-271.
Starkstein, S. E., Jorge, R., Mizrahi, R., Adrian, J., & Robinson, R. G. (2007). Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised. The Psychological
Insight and danger in Alzheimer’s disease. European journal of neurology, Corporation, New York.
14(4), 455-460. Wolk, D. A., Dickerson, B. C., Weiner, M., Aiello, M., Aisen, P., Albert, M.
Stopford, C. L., Snowden, J. S., Thompson, J. C., & Neary, D. (2008). Vari- S., . . . Wilks, K. L. (2010). Apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype has dis-
ability in cognitive presentation of Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 44(2), sociable effects on memory and attentional-executive network function
185-195. in Alzheimer disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of neuro- United States of America, 107(22), 10256-10261.
psychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. (3rd. ed.). NY. Ox- Woodward, M., Brodaty, H., Boundy, K., Ames, D., Blanch, G., Balshaw,
ford University Press. R., and PRIME Study Group (2010a). Does executive impairment
Strub, R. L & Black, F. W. (2000). The Mental Status Examination in Neurology. define a frontal variant of Alzheimer’s disease?. International
F.A. Davis Company. Psychogeriatrics, 22, 1280-1290.
Swanberg, M. M., Tractenberg, R. E., Mohs, R., Thal, L. J. & Cummings, J. Woodward, M., Jacova, C., Black, S. E., Kertesz, A., Mackenzie, I. R., &
L., (2004). Executive dysfunction in Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neu- Feldman, H. (2010b). Differentiating the frontal variant of Alzheimer’s
rology, 61, 556-560. disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(7), 732-738.
Taylor, K. I., Probst, A., Miserez, A. R., Monsch, A. U., & Tolnay, M.
(2008). Clinical course of neuropathologically confirmed frontal-variant (Article received: 17-10-2011, reviewed: 10-1-2012, accepted: 18-1-2012)
Alzheimer’s disease. Nature Clinical Practice Neurology 4, 226-232.

anales de psicología, 2013, vol. 29, nº 1 (enero)

You might also like