You are on page 1of 21

SECTION D Clinical Neurosciences

43 Neuropsychology
Benjamin D. Hill, Justin J.F. O’Rourke, Leigh Beglinger, Jane S. Paulsen

CHAPTER OUTLINE impairments associated with major neurological disorders


are discussed.
GOALS OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION GOALS OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
Test Administration When neural damage is present or cognitive changes are
Test Interpretation observed, a neuropsychological evaluation is appropriate. The
BRIEF MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION prominent neuropsychologist Arthur Benton (1975) best
Montreal Cognitive Assessment described neuropsychology as “a refinement of clinical neuro-
Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status—Modified logical observation [that] serves the function of enhancing
Mini-Mental State Examination clinical observation [and] is closely allied to clinical neuro-
logical evaluation and in fact can be considered to be a special
Modified Mini-Mental State Examination
form of it” (p. 68). Neuropsychological assessment aims to
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF extend the neurological exam by: (1) providing important
NEUROLOGICAL DISEASE information for differential diagnosis and prognosis; (2) iden-
Mild Cognitive Impairment tifying the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral deficits of
Alzheimer Disease disease or injury and characterizing their severity; (3) guiding
Vascular Dementia treatment by using test results to select effective rehabilitation
Mixed Dementia strategies, determining functional capacity and decision-
making abilities for level-of-care decisions, driving and work
Frontotemporal Dementia
capacity, assessing medication cognitive side effects, and estab-
Parkinson Disease with Dementia and Dementia with lishing candidacy for surgical procedures; and (4) monitoring
Lewy Bodies cognitive changes and treatment effectiveness across time.
Huntington Disease Neuropsychological assessment is also frequently used in
Multiple Sclerosis forensic settings and for neuroscience research, but discussion
Epilepsy on these topics is beyond the clinical focus of this chapter
Traumatic Brain Injury (Schoenberg et al., 2011).
Before the advent of neuroimaging in the 1970s and 1980s,
one of the main goals of neuropsychology was lesion localiza-
tion. Today, neuropsychology has shifted toward differential
diagnosis when lesions may not be evident or in conditions
Neuropsychology is the scientific study of neural correlates for with no clear biomarkers. For example, neuropsychologists
cognition and behavior, with a specific clinical interest in assist in the early identification of various dementias since
patients presenting with a range of medical, neurological, and they are primarily diagnosed based on patterns of clear cogni-
psychiatric illnesses. Neuropsychologists are specialized clini- tive declines and behavioral disturbances (see Table 43.1 for
cians who receive extended fellowship training (with available a comparison of cortical versus subcortical dementias as an
board certification) in functional neuroanatomy, neurobiol- example). Neuropsychological testing is also useful for diag-
ogy, psychopharmacology, neurological illness or injury, nosing “non-neurological” conditions that can affect cognitive
neuroimaging, psychometric and statistical principles of neu- functioning or masquerade as neurocognitive disease, such as
rocognitive measures, and clinical psychology. Neuropsycho- dementia of depression or somatoform disorders. Exaggerated
logical evaluation refines neuroimaging and neurological and manufactured symptoms can also be clearly identified
examinations by operating from a biopsychosocial framework through the use of stand-alone and embedded measures of
to determine the extent to which cognition and behavior are valid test performances and symptom reports.
affected by brain dysfunction. Neuropsychologists aim to Another goal of neuropsychology is to accurately describe
characterize and objectively quantify abilities ranging from cognitive deficits and their severity. Even when the cause of
simple sensory and motor functions to complex “higher cognitive dysfunction is clear (e.g., traumatic brain injury)
cognitive abilities” that include cognitive processing speed, or lesions are evident on imaging, the cognitive and behavio-
attention, language, visuoperception/construction, memory, ral manifestations of neural damage can be heterogeneous.
executive functioning (behavioral, cognitive, and motivational The interaction between symptom onset, etiology, and patient
aspects), and emotional/personality functioning. characteristics results in a wide range of individual variability
In this chapter, we begin by explaining the goals and in cognitive deficits. For instance, the neuropsychological
utility of neuropsychology and describing the neuropsycho- profiles of stroke and tumor patients can be very dissimilar
logical evaluation. Guidelines are then suggested for brief even after matching for lesion location (tumor patients
cognitive screenings that may be useful for neurologists in show notably less severe language deficits in the left hemi-
clinical settings. Finally, the typical patterns of cognitive sphere, presumably due to the acute versus chronic etiologies;
511
512 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

TABLE 43.1 Neuropsychological Characteristics of Cortical versus make legal, financial, and healthcare decisions, provide super-
Subcortical Dementia Using Alzheimer Disease and Huntington vision, live independently, and return to work (Demakis,
Disease as Examples 2012). Neurocognitive assessments may also be used to guide
Huntington disease
treatment plans by identifying cognitive deficits for specific
Alzheimer disease (subcortical rehabilitation strategies. For example, patients with behavioral
(cortical dementia) dementia) disinhibition and poor emotional regulation due to lesions in
the orbitofrontal cortex can be targeted for behavioral modi-
LEARNING AND MEMORY
fication strategies and training in self-monitoring (Sohlberg
Episodic memory Impaired encoding/ Impaired information and Turkstra, 2011). Neuropsychological assessments are also
consolidation retrieval useful for evaluating patients’ candidacy for certain surgical
Poor delayed recall and Recognition memory procedures. Neurosurgeons considering a temporal lobectomy
recognition memory is better than for refractory epilepsy often call on neuropsychologists to
delayed recall
conduct Wada testing to localize language, memory, or motor
Retrograde Severe, temporally Mild, nongraded, functioning in order to minimize postoperative cognitive
amnesia graded, retrograde retrograde losses. Neuropsychological assessment is also used prior to the
amnesia amnesia placement of a deep brain stimulator to help predict post-
Priming Impaired Preserved surgical outcomes.
Implicit Preserved Impaired
Lastly, neurocognitive testing is useful for monitoring treat-
procedural/ ment effectiveness and patients’ recovery from acquired brain
motor learning injuries. For instance, neuropsychologists use their expertise to
determine whether a coma patient has progressed into a veg-
Implicit cognitive Preserved Impaired
etative or minimally conscious state (Giacino and Whyte,
skill learning
2005). Accurate monitoring is vital given the differences in
Attention/ Relatively preserved Poor auditory and clinical outcomes for each level of consciousness and the
concentration visual attention danger of making erroneous decisions regarding the with-
Processing Relatively intact Very slow drawal of treatment. Treatment effectiveness can also be moni-
speed tored using repeat assessments to determine whether medical,
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING
pharmacological, and rehabilitation interventions are having
their desired cognitive effects. Monitoring treatment effective-
Set shifting Better able to shift Difficulty with ness leads to more efficient utilization of resources by updat-
focus perseveration ing treatment plans as necessary.
Working memory Mild deficits in ability Early notable deficits
to manipulate in phonological
information, but loop, visuospatial NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION
preserved sketchpad, and Depending on the referral question and clinical setting, neu-
phonological loop ability to
ropsychological assessments can range from quick bedside
and visuospatial manipulate
sketchpad information
assessments to extended evaluations that include formal
standardized testing and a comprehensive clinical interview.
LANGUAGE AND SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE A complete neuropsychological interview covers the onset and
Speech Preserved Dysarthric and slow course of the patient’s cognitive and mood problems, current
functional capacity, developmental background, medical and
Fluency More impaired Severe and equal
semantic fluency impairment in
psychiatric history, family medical history, academic perform-
than phonemic phonemic and ance, vocational achievements, and social background. Infor-
fluency semantic fluency mation obtained from collateral sources such as caregivers or
spouses about the patient’s medical and psychosocial history
Naming Impaired; more Relatively preserved;
can also be critical because many patients lack insight into
semantic errors (e.g., more perceptual
calling a lion “an errors (e.g., calling
their deficits. Besides gathering patient reported information,
animal”) a bucket “a cup”) the goals of the neuropsychological interview are to develop
hypotheses about the patient’s cognitive status and to estab-
Structure of Tend to focus on Able to focus on lish rapport that will elicit their best performance on testing.
semantic concrete perceptual abstract
Behavioral observations made during the interview and testing
knowledge information conceptual
knowledge are also an important source of information that can influence
test selection and interpretation. After a clinical interview is
Adapted from Salmon, D.P., Filoteo, J.V., 2007. Neuropsychology of completed, the following cognitive domains are assessed:
cortical versus subcortical dementia syndromes. Semin Neurol 27, sensory, motor, intellectual functioning, processing speed,
7–21.
attention, language, visuoperception/construction, memory,
executive functioning (behavioral, cognitive, and motivational
aspects), functional capacity, and emotional/personality
functioning.
Anderson et al., 1990). Repeated neuropsychological evalua-
tions are also useful for monitoring the decline of neurode-
generative diseases over time given the potential for varying
Test Administration
degrees of disease progression across patients. Two major approaches to neuropsychological evaluation cur-
In addition to offering information regarding the diagnosis rently dominate the field: the fixed battery approach and the
and clinical manifestation of neuroanatomical dysfunction, flexible battery approach (Barr, 2008).
neuropsychological assessment is unique in its ability to guide The fixed battery approach requires that the same tests are
treatment needs. Neuropsychologists are capable of utilizing administered to every patient in a standardized manner.
objective test data to thoroughly assess patients’ abilities to One example of a fixed battery is the Halstead–Reitan battery
Neuropsychology 513

functioning. Test interpretation requires an understanding


BOX 43.1 Heaton Adaptation of Halstead–Reitan of test validity and reliability, sensitivity and specificity, 43
Neuropsychological Test Battery likelihood ratios, and score distributions to avoid over- or
underdiagnosing cognitive deficits. Substantial intraindividual
Tactual performance test
differences exist in cognitive abilities and a certain number of
Finger oscillation test
poor test performances is common among the general popula-
Category test
tion. Cognitive test performances are also impacted by extra-
Seashore rhythm test
neurological factors such as the number of tests administered,
Speech sounds perception test
where cut scores are placed, the probability of certain test
Aphasia screening test
scores occurring, and the demographic characteristics of the
Sensory-perceptual examination
patient (Iverson and Brooks, 2011). Proper test interpretation
Grip strength test
requires that all of these variables are considered and that
Tactile form recognition test
conclusions are based on recognizable patterns of test results
Wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised
rather than the interpretation of test scores in isolation.
Wechsler memory scale—revised
Neuropsychological test interpretation is also dependent
Adapted from Heaton, R.K., Grant, I., Matthews, C.G., 1991. on an understanding of the scientific and theoretical concepts
Comprehensive Norms for Expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: that underlie cognitive tests. No cognitive test measures a
Demographic Corrections, Research Findings, and Clinical Applications. single isolated aspect of cognitive functioning. Most neuropsy-
Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, Florida. chological tests engage multiple cognitive abilities simultane-
ously. To illustrate, verbal memory tests (e.g., word list memory
tasks) assess memory functioning but they are also dependent
on the patient’s attention, processing speed, and executive
(Box 43.1), for which comprehensive norms have been pub- functioning. Therefore, an impaired score on a verbal memory
lished by Heaton and colleagues (Heaton et al., 1991). An task does not necessarily indicate a primary memory impair-
advantage to the fixed battery approach is that the information ment. It is the neuropsychologist’s task to determine which
gathered is comprehensive and systematically assesses multi- cognitive deficits are actually causing impaired test perform-
ple domains of cognitive functioning. Additionally, if repeated ances by analyzing the patient’s overall pattern of results
assessments are available, test scores can be directly compared across the test battery and by comparing the neuropsychologi-
with baseline information, and tests are well validated and cal profile to known patterns of disease. If a score on a verbal
normed. Drawbacks of the fixed battery approach include its memory test does reflect a primary memory deficit, then the
length (up to 8 hours), because it may be too long for some neuropsychologist determines whether the impairment is due
patients to tolerate and is difficult to afford with the limited to a deficit in encoding, storage, or retrieval since the type of
reimbursement schedules in managed care. Furthermore, an memory impairment may be indicative of different disease
extended assessment may not be necessary to address the refer- processes or lesion locations. Neuropsychologists use a similar
ral question. method of analysis when assessing performances in other cog-
In contrast to the fixed battery approach, the flexible battery nitive domains.
(or hypothesis-driven) approach allows neuropsychologists to Test interpretation also requires the integration of neu-
develop a test battery based on the referral question, patient’s ropsychological test scores with findings from the clinical
history, and clinical interview. In the flexible battery approach, interview, the patient’s history, the neurological examination,
a brief set of basic tests is initially administered, and addi- neurophysiology and neuroimaging data, and relevant lit-
tional tests of more specific abilities are used to conduct erature. Raw test scores must be compared to an appropriate
in-depth follow-up assessments based on each particular reference standard. Several reference standards are used in
patient’s needs. For example, clinicians using the Iowa–Benton interpreting neuropsychological test scores, including the use
method (Tranel, 2008) specifically tailor testing to each patient of normative data, cut scores, and comparisons with an indi-
based on their presenting concern by administering the appro- vidual’s own prior testing results.
priate portions of a core battery, which are then followed up Inferences about individual patients’ neuropsychological
with tests that assess suspected impairments in more detail test scores are often derived by comparing test scores to nor-
(Fig. 43.1). Considerations when selecting tests include age, mative data that are typically collected by test developers as a
primary language, level of education, ethnicity/cultural factors, standardization sample. Normative data are useful for account-
reading level, expected level of global cognitive impairment ing for variables that are likely to influence test performance
(to avoid ceiling or floor effects in testing), and physical dis- (e.g., demographic factors) so that accurate and appropriate
abilities (Smith et al., 2008). Although this approach is more conclusions are drawn. Confounding variables are accounted
tailored to the individual needs of the patient (and is therefore for by stratifying test scores according to gender, age, and/or
briefer), it can be less comprehensive than the fixed battery level of education. An individual’s raw score is compared
approach. Most neuropsychologists’ approaches fall some- with the distribution of scores from his or her peer group to
where between the use of a set battery and a completely indi- determine where it falls within the range of expected perform-
vidualized examination. ances. Figure 43.2 and Table 43.2 show a normal distribution
and interpretive guidelines for use in neuropsychological
interpretation. The usefulness of normative data depends
Test Interpretation strongly on the size and representativeness of the standardiza-
The interpretation of cognitive test results is central to the role tion sample. Clinical interpretation can also be greatly affected
of the neuropsychologist and differentiates neuropsychology by the goodness-of-fit between the individual patient and the
from all other disciplines. Accurate interpretation of neuropsy- standardization sample. Furthermore, it is important to use
chological test results depends on a comprehensive under- the most recent norms available, because cohort effects may
standing of the neuroanatomical correlates of cognition, lead to differences between current patients and those from
neurological disease processes, and psychometric testing prin- whom data were collected years ago. When appropriate norms
ciples. One cannot simply administer a test, look at the score, are not available, there is a danger of over- or underdiagnosis
and declare that the score indicates intact/impaired cognitive of cognitive impairment.
514 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

CORE BATTERY FOLLOW-UP TESTS

Wechsler memory scale-III


Memory
Iowa famous faces test

• Interview Category fluency test


• Orientation to time, person, and Language
place Boston naming test
• Recall of recent presidents
Judgement of
• Information subtest (WAIS-III) line orientation
Perception and
• Complex figure test attention
Hooper visual
• Auditory verbal learning test organization test

• Draw a clock
Draw a house, flower, bicycle
• Arithmetic subtest (WAIS-III)
Visuoconstruction
• Block design subtest (WAIS-III) Three-dimensional block
construction
• Digit span subtest (WAIS-III)

• Similarities (WAIS-III) Grooved pegboard test


Psychomotor and
• Trail-making test psychosensory
Line cancellation test
• Digit symbol subtest (WAIS-III)

• Controlled oral word association Wisconsin card sorting test


Executive
• Benton visual retention test functions
Stroop color-word test
• Benton facial discrimination test

• Picture arrangement subtest Minnesota multiphasic


(WAIS-III) personality inventory-2
Personality and
• Geschwind-Oldfield handedness affect
questionnaire Iowa rating scales of
personality change
• Beck depression inventory-II
Test of memory and
malingering
Symptom validity
testing
Rey 15 item test

Fig. 43.1 Example of a flexible battery approach. (Adapted from Tranel, D., 2008. Theories of clinical neuropsychology and brain–behavior
relationships, in: Morgan, J.E., Ricker, J.H. (Eds), Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. Taylor & Francis, New York, pp. 25–37.)

Another approach to test interpretation is through the use (e.g., AD versus mild cognitive impairment versus healthy).
of cut scores. Tests that rely on cut scores often measure per- Test validation studies commonly use sensitivity and specifi-
formances with low base rates or deficits very few healthy city data with base rate information to calculate likelihood
people demonstrate. Some tests are fairly straightforward in ratios and positive predictive values for individual tests. Like-
their capability to measure abilities that are largely intact in lihood ratios and positive predictive values are differing
normal subjects but impaired in disordered patients. For expressions of the probability that a patient has a particular
example, most people are able to bisect a line without diffi- condition given his or her test score. Smith et al. (2008) put
culty, but patients with left-sided visuospatial neglect typi- these concepts another way by saying, “the positive predictive
cally identify the midpoint of the line to be to the right of value allows for statements such as: ‘Based on the patient
center. Other tests, however, are more complex and require having earned a score of y on test z, the probability that this
more sophisticated analyses to develop valid cut scores. Smith patient has the condition of interest is x’ ” (p. 47). A common
et al. (2008) provide an excellent explanation for how cut example of the application of cut scores can be found in the
scores are useful individual statistics that allow inferences use of screening instruments to quickly identify potential
about which diagnostic group a patient is likely to belong to impairments.
Neuropsychology 515

43

Standard deviations –4 –3 –2 –1 0 +1 +2 +3 +4
Cumulative 0.1% 2.3% 15.9% 50% 84.1% 97.7% 99.9%
percentages
Percentiles 1 5 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 95 99
Z scores –4.0 –3.0 –2.0 –1.0 0 +1.0 +2.0 +3.0 +4.0
T scores 20 30 40 50 60 70 80
Standard nine 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
(stanines)
Percentage 4% 7% 12% 17% 20% 17% 12% 7% 4%
in stanine
Fig. 43.2 The normal curve and its relationship to derived scores.

TABLE 43.2 Descriptive Terms Associated with Performance within treatment recommendations, is communicated to the referring
Various Ranges of the Normal Distribution physician and the patient. Some form of written report is
typical in neuropsychological evaluations, and these tend to
Standard
deviation
vary in length and level of detail (e.g., <1 to 15 pages). A
score (i.e. Percentile common structure for neuropsychological report includes sec-
Qualitative terms z-score) T-score rank tions summarizing the patient interview, collateral interview,
medications, medical history, social background, behavioral
Severely impaired <−2.20 <29 <2
observations, neuropsychological battery, neuropsychological
Moderately impaired −2.20 to −1.60 29–34 2–5 test results and interpretation, final diagnostic impressions,
Mildly impaired −1.59 to −1.33 35–37 6–9 and treatment recommendations.
Below average −1.32 to −0.68 38–42 10–24
Average −0.67 to +0.67 43–57 25–75
BRIEF MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION
Above average +0.68 to +1.59 58–66 76–94
Before referring a patient for a neuropsychological evaluation,
the neurologist typically has either clinical or historical evi-
Superior +1.60 to +2.20 67–72 95–98 dence of cognitive concerns. This might come from patient
Very superior >+2.20 >72 >98 self-report or collateral report, an informal mental status
examination, or a brief objective screening measure of mental
Note: The patient’s educational history and premorbid level of
status. Although many mental status examinations are con-
functioning should be taken into consideration in applying any
qualitative label.
ducted in a nonstandard manner, neurologists are encouraged
to use formal cognitive screening measures to develop a stand-
ardized method of mental status examination so comparisons
across time and patients can be reliably made. The purpose of
The comparison of current performance with past test cognitive screening measures is to determine the need for a
scores is another important component of test interpretation, more extended evaluation of neuropsychological functioning.
especially if cognitive decline is suspected. Rarely, however, do Given the limited scope of cognitive screening measures and
individuals have previous test data available for these com- the psychometric considerations noted earlier, it is not recom-
parisons. When no previous test scores are available, evidence mended that cognitive screening measures are routinely used
of the patient’s premorbid intellectual functioning is esti- as a final summation of a patient’s neuropsychological status.
mated. Several techniques are available for estimating premor- Scores from cognitive screeners must be considered in con-
bid intellect, including regression equations that utilize junction with clinical observation and judgment to determine
demographic variables as predictors of IQ (e.g., the Barona whether a referral for neuropsychological evaluation is neces-
formula; Barona et al., 1984), irregular-word reading tests that sary, since many patients may pass a cognitive screen but still
are correlated with IQ (e.g., the North American Adult Reading have suspected deficits that warrant more sensitive neuropsy-
Test; Blair and Spreen, 1989), and “hold” subtests from intel- chological testing. A few suggested objective screening meas-
ligence measures that are frequently used as proxies for pre- ures of cognitive functioning that may be useful for neurologists
morbid functioning (e.g., see Lezak et al., 2012, for review). to administer are briefly described in the following.
Most contemporary neuropsychologists use a combination of
these strategies, either formally (e.g., Oklahoma Premorbid
Intelligence Estimate-3, Schoenberg et al., 2002; Test of Pre-
Montreal Cognitive Assessment
morbid Functioning, Pearson, 2009) or informally. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was originally
Ultimately, feedback about the results of the neuropsycho- developed as a screening tool to correct the shortcomings of
logical evaluation, along with diagnostic impressions and the widely used Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; see
516 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

description in self-titled section), which demonstrated an place (see Figure 43.3 for example). Including more cognitive
insensitivity to mild cognitive impairment (Nasreddine et al., domains reduces the likelihood that impairments or disorders
2005). The MoCA also improved upon the MMSE by probing will be overlooked (e.g., executive dysfunction, a hallmark
more cognitive domains, including executive functioning, symptom of vascular dementia). The total score ranges from
immediate and delayed memory, visuospatial abilities, atten- 0 to 30 points, and a cut score of 26 has demonstrated very
tion, working memory, language, and orientation to time and good specificity (by correctly identifying 87% of healthy

Fig. 43.3 Montreal Cognitive Assessment. (Reprinted with permission from Nasreddine, Z.S., Phillips, N.A., Bedirian, V., et al., 2005. The
Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 53, 695–699.)
Neuropsychology 517

TABLE 43.3 Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score by age and education
43
Years of education
<12 12 >12 Total by age
Age group (years) NO. MEAD (SD) MEDIAN NO. MEAN (SD) MEDIAN NO. MEAN (SD) MEDIAN NO. MEAN (SD) MEDIAN

<35 20 22.80 (3.38) 23 65 24.46 (3.49) 25 122 25.93 (2.48) 26 207 25.16 (3.08) 26
30–40 37 22.84 (3.18) 23 106 23.99 (2.93) 24 264 25.81 (2.64) 26 408 25.07 (2.95) 25
35–45 55 22.11 (3.33) 23 177 23.02 (3.67) 24 355 25.38 (3.05) 26 588 24.37 (3.51) 25
40–50 77 21.36 (3.73) 22 227 22.26 (3.94) 23 418 25.09 (3.16) 26 723 23.80 (3.80) 24
45–55 77 20.75 (3.80) 21 216 21.87 (3.95) 22 461 24.70 (3.24) 25 755 23.48 (3.84) 24
50–60 62 19.94 (4.34) 20 172 22.25 (3.46) 22 424 24.34 (3.38) 25 659 23.37 (3.78) 24
55–65 60 19.60 (4.14) 20 143 21.58 (3.93) 22 369 24.43 (3.31) 25 573 23.20 (3.96) 23
60–70 57 19.30 (3.79) 19 113 20.89 (4.50) 21 246 24.32 (3.04) 25 418 22.69 (4.12) 23
65–75 38 18.37 (3.87) 19 67 20.57 (4.79) 21 122 24.00 (3.35) 24 228 22.05 (4.48) 23
70–80 14 16.07 (3.17) 17 23 20.35 (4.91) 20 42 23.06 (3.47) 24 79 21.32 (4.78) 22
Total by education 230 20.55 (4.04) 21 608 22.34 (3.97) 23 1,306 24.81 (3.20) 25 2,148 23.65 (3.84) 24
Adapted from Rossetti, H.C., Lacritz, L.H., Cullum, C.M. & Weiner, M.F., 2011. Normative data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a
population-based sample. Neurology 77, 1272–5

participants) and excellent sensitivity when differentiating 2003) such as amnestic MCI (Fig. 43.4). Age-, education-,
mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (90%) and AD (100%) from gender-, and race-corrected normative data are available
healthy comparisons. More importantly, the positive predic- (Hogervorst et al., 2004).
tive value of the MoCA was 89% for both MCI and AD. Since
its inception as a screening measure for MCI, other studies
have found the MoCA to outperform the MMSE in screening Mini-Mental State Examination
for general cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease (PD) One of the most widely used mental status examinations is
(Hoops et al., 2009; Nazem et al., 2009), vascular dementia the MMSE (Folstein et al., 1975), a 30-point standardized
after acute stroke (Dong et al., 2010), and HD (Videnovic screening tool for assessing orientation, attention, short-
et al., 2010) as a measure sensitive to early stages of different term recall, naming, repetition, simple verbal and written
types of dementia. commands, writing, and construction (Fig. 43.5). The MMSE
Although the MoCA has demonstrated its utility as a cogni- has been used in a variety of settings (e.g., community, institu-
tive screener, there are a few caveats worth noting. First, some tions, general hospital, specialty clinics), with many different
studies have demonstrated that its reliability is notably low in neurological and psychiatric conditions (e.g., dementia, stroke,
nonclinical populations (Bernstein et al., 2011), which indi- depression), across age ranges, and with different cultural and
cates that it should primarily be used only to detect suspected ethnic subgroups. Demographic variables such as age and edu-
cognitive impairment in clinical patients. Additionally, the cation have been shown to systematically influence MMSE
original cut score of 26 used to identify impairment was devel- scores, so normative data or cut scores should account for
oped without fully accounting for other variables that affect these variables. One example of appropriate norms comes
test performance (e.g., age, education, gender, and race) and from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area study (Crum et al.,
the score has also been shown to identify a high number of 1993); these are presented in Table 43.4. Whereas many intact
false positives in certain populations. Rossetti and colleagues individuals achieve total scores near 30, a cut score of 23 on
(2011) attempted to correct these problems by conducting a the MMSE has been shown to have adequate sensitivity and
normative study of the MoCA in an ethnically diverse sample specificity (86% and 91%, respectively) for detecting dementia
of healthy participants as presented in Table 43.3. They found in community samples (Cullen et al., 2005). However, when
that 66% of their sample fell below the cut score of 26, indi- working with highly educated patients (i.e., ≥16 years of
cating “impairment,” and that many of the MoCA items had formal education) a cut score of 27 is recommended (O’Bryant
high failure rates. et al., 2008).
The MoCA is free to clinicians (http://www.mocatest.org) Despite its widespread use, the MMSE has some drawbacks.
and has been translated into 31 different languages and First, it only assesses a limited number of cognitive functions.
dialects. Second, a potential threat to the test’s internal validity is the
nonstandardized administration of some of the items. Exam-
Telephone Interview for Cognitive ples of these frequent adaptations of the MMSE include the
use of nonorthogonal (i.e., semantically related) word stimuli
Status—Modified for registration and recall, nonstandard scoring of serial 7’s,
The Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status—Modified and nonstandard inclusion of spelling world backward.
(mTICS) is a relatively brief screening instrument designed to Another drawback of the MMSE is that it has “ceiling effects”
quickly and accurately assess cognition over the telephone, that can miss cognitive impairments in high-functioning indi-
although it can also be used in face-to-face settings. This viduals. The MMSE also has difficulty differentiating individu-
13-item measure is heavily weighted toward immediate and als with MCI from controls and those with dementia ( Mitchell,
delayed free recall, which might make it particularly useful in 2009). Finally, because this test relies on a single total score,
detecting mild impairments (Duff et al., 2009; Lines et al., partial administration of the measure (e.g., due to sensory
518 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

Modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (mTICS) impairments of the patient) provides no information about
cognitive status. This same limitation is true for the MoCA.
1. What is your name? /2

2. What is your telephone number? /2


Modified Mini-Mental State Examination
3. What is today’s date (month, date, year, season, day)? (5 points maximum,
1 point per correct response) Some of the criticisms of the MMSE led to the development
Month: /1 of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS) (Teng
Date: /1 and Chui, 1987), a 15-item extension of the MMSE that
Year: /1 assesses orientation (self, time, place), attention (simple
Season: /1 and complex), memory (recall and recognition), language
Day: /1 /5
(naming, verbal fluency, repetition, following commands,
4. I’m going to read you a list of 10 words. Please listen carefully and try to writing), construction, and executive functioning (similari-
remember them. When I am done, tell me as many as you can in any order. ties). It remains relatively brief to administer (10 minutes),
Ready? (10 points maximum, 1 point per correctly recalled word)
and age- and education-corrected normative data are available
Cabin (Tschanz et al., 2002). Regression-based prediction formulas
Pipe for the 3MS allow for more accurate assessments of change
Elephant across time (Tombaugh, 2005). The broader scoring range (0
Chest
to 100) has been shown to be more sensitive than that of the
MMSE in identifying dementia (McDowell et al., 1997;
Silk
Tschanz et al., 2002) and other cognitive disorders (Bland and
Theatre
Newman, 2001) in large community samples. A cut score for
Watch
cognitive impairment is typically 77 (Bland and Newman,
Whip 2001; McDowell et al., 1997), and a change of 5 points over
Pillow the course of 5 to 10 years indicates the presence of clinically
Giant meaningful decline (Andrew and Rockwood, 2008).
TOTAL /10
5. Please count backwards from 20 to 1. /2 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF
20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
6. Please take 7 away from 100. Now continue to take away 7 from what you
NEUROLOGICAL DISEASE
have left over until I ask you to stop. (5 points maximum, 1 point per correct
response) In this section we briefly address the neurocognitive sequelae
93: /1 of some of the major neurological disorders. Although many
86: /1 of these disorders have psychiatric characteristics as well, these
79: /1 will only be briefly discussed. Please see Chapter 9, Behavior
72: /1 and Personality Disturbances, for a more comprehensive dis-
65: /1 /5 cussion on the psychiatric aspects of neurological disorders.
7. What do people usually use to cut paper? /2

8. What is the prickly green plant found in the desert? /2 Mild Cognitive Impairment
9. Who is president of the United States now? /2 A major focus of dementia research has been detecting pro-
10. Who is the vice president of the United States now? /2 dromes of cognitive impairment associated with neurodegen-
erative diseases, with a particular emphasis on characterizing
11. What word is opposite of east? /2
the incipient stages of Alzheimer disease. The diagnostic term
12. Please say this: “Methodist Episcopal” /2 mild cognitive impairment was initially defined by Petersen
Correct response (circle one): YES NO and colleagues (1999) as the presence of subjective memory
13. Please tap your finger 5 times on the part of the phone you speak into. complaints, a measured deficit in a cognitive domain of
(2 points if they tap 5 times, 1 point if they tap more or less than 5 times) /2 approximately 1.5 standard deviations below normative
14. Please repeat the list of 10 words I read earlier. means, otherwise intact cognition, no functional impair-
Cabin ments, and the absence of dementia.
Pipe
This became known as the Petersen criteria and requires
the utilization of neuropsychological tests to quantify the
Elephant
deficit using normative data. While MCI originally focused on
Chest
memory impairment, the concept has evolved to include other
Silk cognitive domains. Currently, MCI is typically categorized as
Theatre either amnestic or nonamnestic with some also specifying
Watch whether the cognitive impairment is single- or multi-domain.
Whip Both amnestic MCI and nonamnestic MCI are considered to
Pillow
be risk factors for Alzheimer disease with nonamnestic MCI
being viewed as a risk factor for other dementias as well. The
Giant
new DSM-V (APA, 2013) diagnosis of mild neurocognitive
TOTAL
/10 disorder is essentially MCI (Petersen et al., 2009). Amnestic
TOTAL: /50 MCI is associated with smaller hippocampal and larger infe-
rior lateral ventricle volumes similar to those of Alzheimer
Fig. 43.4 Modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status. (Data neuropathology (England et al., 2014) and approximately half
from Welsh, K.A., Breitner, J.C.S., Magruder-Habib, K.M., 1993. of these cases will convert to Alzheimer dementia (Ferman
Detection of dementia in the elderly using telephone screening of et al., 2013). However, MCI as a diagnostic concept is extremely
cognitive status. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 6, heterogeneous, in terms of both cognitive profile and neu-
103–110.) ropathology (Stephan et al., 2012). Other MCI subtypes are
Neuropsychology 519

1. “What is the year? 7. Read and obey /1 43


season?
date?
day of week? CLOSE YOUR EYES
month?
/5
8. Copy design /1
2. “Where are we? state?
county? (Must have 10 angles and
town? intersect)
hospital?
floor?
/5

3. Repeat 3 words:
(use list) 9. Write a sentence: /1
(Must have subject and verb make sense)
“Here are three words I want you to
remember.” Repeat up to 6 times.
# words on 1st trial
# repetitions
/3
4. Serial 7’s: 93 10. Repeat the following:
86
79 “no ifs, ands, or buts” /1
72
65 11. Follow a 3-stage command:
a. take a paper in your
/5
right hand
“Start with 100 and count backward b. fold it in half
by 7. For example: What is 100–7? c. put it on the floor
Keep subtracting 7. Stop after five /3
responses.”
“Now I will give you some directions to
Spell “world” backward: follow. Please listen carefully and do as I say.
D Take this paper in your right hand, fold it in
L half, and put it on the floor.”
R
O
W
/5

5. Recall 3 words:
(Set used in #3)

/3

6. Name a: Pencil
Watch TOTAL
/2 MAX = 30

Fig. 43.5 Mini-Mental State Examination. (Reprinted with permission from Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., McHugh, P.R., 1975. Mini-Mental
State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12, 189–198.)

hypothesized to progress to their own dementia outcomes. For whether severity of MCI symptoms does not predict which
example, 20% of nonamnestic MCI cases with impairment in individuals will convert to dementia (Guo et al., 2013),
either attention or visuospatial, or both domains have been though impaired olfaction increases the risk of conversion by
reported to convert to Lewy body dementia (Ferman et al., four times (Conti et al., 2013). Using preclinical staging of AD
2013). Multi-domain MCI (e.g., deficits in executive functions adds to the predictive validity of who demonstrates conver-
and processing speed) might be indicative of eventual vascular sion (Vos et al., 2013). Having a copy of APOE ε 4 also
dementia. Neuropsychological testing is essential for obtain- increases the risk of developing MCI (Brainerd et al., 2013).
ing the necessary information to differentiate MCI subtypes Individuals with MCI also experience global cognitive decline
and track their progression into the various forms of at over twice the rate of what is observed in nonimpaired
dementia. individuals (Wilson et al., 2010). For cases that progress to
MCI has attracted so much attention because of its prog- dementia, the average time they would be considered to have
nostic value, with a 10-fold increase in the rate of dementia MCI is 5.5 years, though they may not be diagnosed early in
for individuals with MCI (Petersen et al., 1999). Patients of the process (Wilson et al., 2012). Since not everyone with MCI
memory disorder clinics with MCI progress to dementia at a progresses to dementia, several predictors of the progression
rate of 10% to 15% per year (Farias et al., 2009), and com- from MCI have been identified. Bilingualism has been reported
munity dwelling adults at an annual rate of 6% to 10% per to be a protective factor resulting in later onset of MCI (Bia-
year (Petersen et al., 2009), but up to 25% may revert back to lystok et al., 2014) as are more advanced education and
normal cognitive baseline at follow-up. It is controversial healthy lifestyle behaviors (Lojo-Seoane et al., 2014). Despite
520

TABLE 43.4 Mini-Mental State Examination Score by Age and Educational Level, Number of Participants, Mean, Standard Deviation, and Selected Percentiles
Age (years) 18–24 25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 45–49 50–54 55–59 60–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84 ≥85 Total
EDUCATIONAL LEVEL
0–4 yr 17 23 41 33 36 28 34 49 88 126 139 112 105 61 892
Mean 22 25 25 23 23 23 23 22 23 22 22 21 20 19 22
SD 2.9 2.0 2.4 2.5 2.6 3.7 2.6 2.7 1.9 1.9 1.7 2.0 2.2 2.9 2.3
5–8 yr 94 83 74 101 100 121 154 208 310 633 533 437 241 134 3223
Mean 27 27 26 26 27 26 27 26 26 26 26 25 25 23 26
SD 2.7 2.5 1.8 2.8 1.8 2.5 2.4 2.9 2.3 1.7 1.8 2.1 1.9 3.3 22
9–12 yr or H.S. diploma 1326 958 822 668 489 423 462 525 626 814 550 315 163 99 8240
Mean 29 29 29 28 28 28 28 28 28 28 27 27 25 26 28
SD 2.2 1.3 1.3 1.8 1.9 2.4 2.2 2.2 1.7 1.4 1.6 1.5 2.3 2.0 1.9
College experience 783 1012 989 641 354+ 259 220 231 270 358 255 181 96 52 5701
PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

Mean 29 29 29 29 29 29 29 29 29 29 28 28 27 27 29
SD 1.3 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.7 1.6 1.9 1.5 1.3 1.0 1.6 1.6 0.9 1.3 1.3
Total 2220 2076 1926 1443 979 831 870 1013 1294 1931 1477 1045 605 346 18,056
Mean 29 29 29 29 28 28 28 28 28 27 27 26 25 24 28
SD 2.0 1.3 1.3 1.8 2.0 2.5 2.4 2.5 2.0 1.6 1.8 2.1 2.2 2.9 2.0
Data from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area household surveys in New Haven, CT; Baltimore, MD; St Louis, MO; Durham, NC; and Los Angeles, CA, between 1980 and 1984. The data are weighted
based on the 1980 U.S. population census by age, sex, and race. Adapted from Crum, R.M., Anthony, J.C., Bassett, S.S., et al., 1993. JAMA 269, 2386–2391.
Neuropsychology 521

this growing research, no standard for predicting the MCI-to- presented early in lists (Salmon and Bondi, 2009). Recogni-
dementia conversion has yet been established. tion memory is also likely to be significantly impaired and 43
intrusion errors are common (e.g., adding extra words to
delayed recall trials on word-list memory tasks). Other cog-
Alzheimer Disease nitive deficits are seen in a number of other domains includ-
Alzheimer disease-related dementia is the most common type, ing language functions (e.g., paraphasias, naming), semantic
with prevalence rates of 11% in those of 65 years and older knowledge, visuospatial abilities, and executive functions such
(Hebert et al., 2013) and 68% in memory disorder clinics as motor planning (Weintraub et al., 2012). Impaired aware-
(Paulino Ramirez Diaz et al., 2005). Definitive diagnosis ness of their own cognitive deficits, known as anosognosia or
requires postmortem neuropathological examination of brain impaired metacognition, is also common (Rosen et al., 2014).
tissue for the hallmark signs of plaques and neurofibrillary Even though impaired memory is a cardinal feature of AD, it
tangles in the hippocampal and entorhinal regions (Braak and is important to keep in mind that other neurodegenerative
Braak, 1991); however, premortem diagnostic criteria are dementia syndromes can also result in memory decline and
widely employed in clinical and research settings. Recently, that a nonmemory cognitive deficit may be the primary cogni-
DSM-V (APA, 2013) has changed the terminology for diagno- tive presentation of AD, possibly reflecting posterior cortical
sis of dementia in AD to major neurocognitive disorder due atrophy. Global cognitive functioning in AD declines at a rate
to Alzheimer disease with specified criteria being evidence of nearly four times faster than what is observed in cognitively
significant cognitive decline from a previously higher level of intact individuals of the same age (Wilson et al., 2010). Given
functioning and the cognitive impairments interfering with the pattern of deficits in AD, it is not surprising that measures
everyday functioning. The diagnosis is considered probable of semantic fluency, delayed free recall, and global cognitive
Alzheimer disease if there is objective evidence of decline in status demonstrate the highest levels of sensitivity and specifi-
memory and one other cognitive domain, insidious onset and city for detecting patients with early AD (Salmon et al., 2002).
gradual progression of symptoms, and no evidence of other Although memory deficits are usually glaring compared to
etiology. Evidence of a genetic mutation associated with Alzhei- other deficits in AD, general and progressive cognitive decline
mer disease would also result in probable Alzheimer disease is common and shows an eightfold increase in rate of decline
diagnosis. However, autosomal dominant cases such as APP, approximately 3 years prior to death (Yu et al., 2013). As
PSEN1, or PSEN2 are rare and account for less than 1% of dementia progresses to moderate stages, learning and memory
Alzheimer disease patients (Storandt et al., 2014). For research performances are likely to produce floor effects on many
purposes, the National Institute of Neurological and Com- standardized neuropsychological measures, and more pro-
municative Disorders and Stroke Alzheimer’s Disease and found deficits are apparent in other cognitive domains. Defi-
Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) criteria for cits in praxis also begin to develop. These pervasive declines
probable AD are the most commonly used. The National Insti- in late AD often make formal neuropsychological testing
tutes of Aging–Alzheimer’s Association workgroups (McKhann unnecessary or impossible.
et al., 2011) have recommended diagnosing probable Alzhei-
mer disease dementia when there is a change in cognition
that results in functional impairment and there is either an
Vascular Dementia
amnestic presentation or a nonamnestic one in the domains Cerebrovascular disease frequently leads to cognitive impair-
of language, visuospatial, or executive function with no evi- ment and vascular factors have garnered significant attention
dence of other likely etiologies. The workgroups recommend as a modifiable risk factor for dementia. So-called silent brain
a diagnosis of possible Alzheimer disease dementia if there is infarcts, or asymptomatic vascular pathology, are found in up
an atypical course such as a lack of evidence of progressive to 35% of individuals with vascular disease (Slark et al., 2012)
onset or mixed presentation that suggests comorbid etiologies. and are known to double the risk of vascular dementia (VaD)
They also recommend including the incorporation of biomar- or major neurocognitive disorder due to vascular disease, as
kers, neuroimaging, and genetic findings into the diagnostic described in the DSM-V. Extracerebellar lacunar, large vessel,
criteria but particularly emphasize the importance of measur- or strategically placed infarcts and hemorrhage lead to varying
able cognitive decline, especially for MCI. The presence of degrees of cognitive impairment, and small vessel disease is
biomarkers without cognitive decline is considered a preclini- the most frequently observed vascular pathology that leads to
cal AD stage. However, the presence of biomarkers without cognitive decline. Recently, the term vascular cognitive impair-
evidence of early cognitive decline is thought to convey lower ment (VCI) has been introduced to encompass all cognitive
risk for dementia than if cognitive deficits are present (Jack changes attributable to cerebrovascular pathology (i.e., from
et al., 2013). Formal neuropsychological assessment for meas- mild cognitive impairment due to vascular events to VaD;
uring AD related declines has been demonstrated to have Rincon and Wright, 2013; Sachdev et al., 2014). Cognitive
better sensitivity than MRI for tracking disease progression changes can occur abruptly and in a stepwise pattern with
(Schmand et al., 2014). The brief bedside exams mentioned coinciding cerebrovascular accidents (CVAs), or they may fluc-
earlier in this chapter also have some degree of utility in detect- tuate or remain static. Neuroimaging is likely to detect lesions
ing cognitive changes, but their comparatively limited scope, that are a result of a CVA and enhance diagnostic certainty, but
low sensitivity, and susceptibility to ceiling effects make them imaging is not required to accurately identify dementia with
better suited for screening purposes. a vascular etiology. Neurological evidence of cerebrovascular
Complaints about “memory problems” are what often lead pathology (e.g., history of strokes, sensorimotor changes con-
to a clinical evaluation of possible AD and cognitive decline sistent with stroke) in combination with cognitive changes is
begins 7.5 years prior to diagnosis on average (Wilson et al., considered indicative of VaD and the estimated prevalence is
2012). Consistent with patient report and behavioral observa- 8–12% (Jellinger, 2013).
tions of rapid forgetting of new information, performances The clinical presentation of VaD varies depending on the
across a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological assess- number of infarcts, severity of neural damage following stroke,
ment measures are likely to identify stark memory deficits for and location of the CVA. Although many VaD patients may
both verbally (e.g., lists, stories, paired associates) and visually acknowledge “memory problems,” which may lead to suspi-
(e.g., concrete or abstract figures) mediated information, cions of AD, further questioning reveals that these complaints
including an atypical lack of a primacy effect for information are quite different from those typically seen in AD. If VaD
522 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

results from a discrete stroke, then the primary cognitive impaired than similarly staged AD patients (Dong et al.,
impairment will likely be functionally related to the area of 2013). While this may result from interactive effects, there is
infarct, resulting in focal deficits and a heterogeneous presen- evidence that vascular and amyloid burden combine inde-
tation for VaD. Many patients do not experience a notable pendently to impact cognitive impairment (Park et al., 2013).
stroke preceding the onset of VaD, and may not present to
memory disorder clinics because their primary symptoms are
often dysexecutive in nature (e.g., the patient “just can’t figure
Frontotemporal Dementia
things out anymore”) with accompanying apathy and/or Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is another common type of
depression being common (Weintraub et al., 2012). Particu- dementia and is in fact the second most common in those
larly in the frontal regions, increasing lacunae are associated younger than age 65 (Arvanitakis, 2010). It is less common
with a higher probability of depression, apathy, atypical in older adults and is frequently misdiagnosed as AD (Beber
behaviors, and other neuropsychiatric symptoms (Kim et al., and Chaves, 2013). The term frontotemporal lobar degeneration
2013). Impaired executive functioning is also strongly related (FTLD) is also frequently used in connection with FTD but
to the presence of metabolic syndrome, a significant risk factor usually refers to the pathological aspects of FTD and not the
for VaD (Falkowski et al., 2014). clinical entity (Hales and Hu, 2013). According to DSM-V
Classic dementia symptoms might be reported (e.g., diffi- (APA, 2013) criteria, FTD has multiple clinical manifestations
culty remembering names, appointments, medications) but that include behavioral and language variants. Behavioral
changes in instrumental activities of daily living that require observations, neuropsychological testing, and neuroimaging
complex organizational and problem-solving skills (such as (e.g., frontal or temporal lobe atrophy on computed tomogra-
managing finances or following directions) are likely more phy [CT] or magnetic resonance imaging [MRI], hypoperfusion
prominent in a patient with VaD than in one with AD. Depres- or hypometabolism on single-photon emission tomography
sion and apathy are hallmark symptoms and there is evidence [SPECT] or positron emission tomography [PET]) are used in
that vascular factors are etiologically linked to late life depres- combination to establish a clinically probable diagnosis of
sion (Taylor et al., 2013). Upon first inspection, VaD may FTD. A definitive diagnosis can be made with histopathologi-
initially be cognitively indistinguishable from other demen- cal evidence through biopsy or postmortem examination. The
tias in many ways. However, a detailed neuropsychological latest consensus clinical diagnostic criteria (Rascovsky et al.,
examination and behavioral observations are likely to reveal 2011) define possible FTD as the presence of progressive
a unique pattern of deficits in VaD referred to as the subcorti- behavioral and/or cognitive deterioration and three of the
cal profile. VaD patients tend to have better long-term verbal following: behavioral disinhibition, apathy or inertia, loss of
memory than their AD counterparts, worse executive func- empathy, perseverative or compulsive behaviors, hyperorality
tioning, and slowed processing speed, particularly on motor or dietary change, and neuropsychological testing demonstrat-
tasks. Impaired complex attention is also prominent (Benedet ing executive impairments with intact memory and visuospa-
et al., 2012) and VaD tends to have a rapid initial onset. Free tial abilities. Probable FTD requires that the patient meet
recall might occasionally appear similar in VaD and in AD, criteria for possible FTD and also has evidence of functional
but patients with VaD significantly outperform their AD coun- decline and neuroimaging consistent with frontal and anterior
terparts on tests of recognition memory (Tierney et al., 2001). temporal atrophy or dysfunction.
Better recognition memory suggests encoding processes are Although cognitive deficits are clearly evident on formal
relatively intact in VaD patients compared to AD patients, testing, the earliest and most common complaints for the
but information retrieval is deficient. This retrieval deficit behavioral variant of FTD are related to changes in personality
appears to be quite common in subcortical dementias, with and behavior. Increases in impulsivity, poor judgment, stere-
which VaD appears to have several components in common. otypic behaviors, lack of hygiene, and loss of appropriate
Patients with VaD also perform more poorly on phonemic social behaviors are all prominent. Euphoria and disinhibi-
fluency tasks relative to semantic fluency tasks, which suggests tion are particularly predictive of FTD compared to other
greater executive dysfunction due to disruption of the frontal dementias (Perri et al., 2014) while apathy is a nonspecific
subcortical circuitry rather than degeneration in the temporal symptom that can occur in all dementias. A formerly mild-
lobes. mannered and conscientious patient who develops the behav-
ioral variant of FTD may present as overly frank, crass, and
uncaring. The patient is usually indifferent to their behavior,
Mixed Dementia but spouses and adult children are usually embarrassed by his
More common than AD and VaD in their pure forms is mixed or her conduct. Many of these behavioral issues may have
dementia. While mixed dementia typically refers to a patient underlying cognitive components. Impaired metacognition,
having both AD and cerebrovascular pathology, it concep- or a deficit in monitoring and appreciating behavior, is typical
tually can be any case which involves the coexistence of mul- and much more severe than is seen in AD (Rosen et al., 2014).
tiple neuropathologies (e.g., amyloid, small vessel ischemic Social impairments likely reflect an inability to cognitively
changes, Lewy bodies). For patients diagnosed with VaD, 30% recognize emotions or perspectives in others. Deficits on cog-
may have insignificant AD neuropathology as well (Lee et al., nitive testing typically involve impaired executive functions
2011) and these patients tend to be older than pure VaD cases. such as mental flexibility, planning deficits, slowed word/
Across dementia autopsy studies, 21%–80% of cases had design generation, and multiple response errors. Phonemic
mixed AD and vascular pathology (Jellinger, 2013; Wilson fluency also tends to be more impaired than semantic fluency.
et al., 2012). In older patients (>90 years), having both AD As the disease progresses, even simple go/no go tasks are dif-
and vascular pathology at autopsy was more likely if they had ficult. Despite the array of cognitive deficits in FTD, memory
been diagnosed with mixed dementia rather than AD alone and visuospatial functioning tend to be well preserved, and
(James et al., 2012). The vast majority of mixed dementia some suggest that a lack of deficits in these domains might be
cases are AD and vascular pathology but 5% of cases are AD one of the best ways to differentiate FTD from other diseases
and Lewy body disease. Persons with multiple pathologies are such as AD (Weintraub et al., 2012).
three times more likely to develop dementia than others with The language variants of FTD are different clinical entities
only one identifiable diagnosis (Schneider et al., 2007). There than the behavioral variant (Harciarek and Cosentino, 2013).
is evidence that mixed dementia cases are more globally Confrontation naming, word finding, speech production,
Neuropsychology 523

comprehension, and/or syntax all gradually worsen over the declines can be detected early in the course of PDD (Tröster,
course of disease. Memory functioning and visuospatial skills 2008). 43
are relatively spared. The proposed DSM-V diagnostic criteria The neuropsychological profiles of PDD and DLB are
suggest that the language variant of FTD be divided into four similar. There is considerable heterogeneity in the presenta-
subtypes: primary progressive aphasia (PPA), semantic variant, tion and progression of cognitive deficits, with some patients
nonfluent/agrammatic variant, and logopenic/phonologic displaying only minor cognitive slowing. Rapidly fluctuating
variant (APA, 2013). All of these variants except possibly the attention and level of alertness is a signature symptom (Lee
semantic variant involve some aspect of anomia or impaired et al., 2012) along with abnormal REM-sleep behaviors and
object naming that is worse than AD (Reilly et al., 2011) but anosmia. Overall, the neuropsychological profile of PDD
have otherwise unique neuropsychological profiles. PPA is includes bradyphrenia and significant executive and visuospa-
characterized primarily by the insidious and gradual decom- tial impairments (Ballard et al., 2013). Executive dysfunction—
position of expressive language functions such as word finding, including impairments in decision-making and planning—may
object naming, grammar, and speech production that also be among the earliest signs of cognitive decline in PDD. Lan-
affects comprehension later in the disease process (Weintraub guage problems such as decreased verbal fluency, poor com-
et al., 2012). The semantic variant of FTD involves impaired prehension, and difficulty producing complex sentences can
single-word comprehension, object and/or person knowledge, occur frequently. Regardless of domain, patients with Parkin-
and dyslexia in the presence of spared single-word repetition, son disease generally perform better on neuropsychological
motor speech production, and syntax. These patients may testing than Alzheimer patients. It has been noted that the
appear to have intact expressive and comprehension language visuospatial deficits common in Parkinson disease may
functions but are unable to define vocabulary words or con- account for some impairments observed on neuropsychologi-
cepts. Nonfluent/agrammatic FTD results in impaired motor cal testing (Toner et al., 2012). Psychiatric manifestations,
speech production, grammatical errors, and comprehension including mood disturbances, anxiety, apathy, and psychosis,
of complex sentences, with intact single-word comprehension are also common in PDD (Ballard et al., 2013). Similar psy-
and object knowledge. Lastly, the logopenic/phonologic chiatric problems are also observed in patients with DLB, with
variant presents with deficits in spontaneous word retrieval the most striking symptom in these patients being well-formed
and repetition of sentences and phrases, while single-word visual hallucinations (Weintraub et al., 2012). These halluci-
comprehension, object knowledge, and motor speech and nations are typically not frightening to the patient. They can
grammar production are spared. Neuropsychological testing is be any visual object but often take human or animal form.
ideally suited for specifically characterizing deficits and parsing Severity of visuospatial deficits predicts likelihood of visual
out the complexity of FTD variants and their specific cognitive hallucinations (Perri et al., 2014). Importantly, both PDD and
profiles. DLB patients have strong adverse reactions to antipsychotic
medications that make their use contraindicated in this group.
Parkinson Disease with Dementia and Dementia In addition to characterizing the cognitive sequelae of PD
and the effects of pharmacological treatment, neuropsycholo-
with Lewy Bodies gists are often called upon to evaluate candidates for deep
Parkinson disease with dementia (PDD) and dementia with brain stimulation (DBS) of the subthalamic nucleus. DBS in
Lewy bodies (DLB) have been collectively classified as Lewy this region is known to result in potential global cognitive
body dementias, given their shared α-synuclein pathology (Lim decline (Witt et al., 2013) in preoperatively intact patients and
et al., 2013). While it is believed that all patients with Parkin- may be contraindicated in PDD or DLB patients who are
son disease would eventually develop a Lewy body dementia already cognitively impaired. Furthermore, older PD patients
with sufficient time, the incidence in patients with Parkinson with MCI prior to DBS may be at greater risk for postoperative
disease is only 12% for DLB and 8.5% for PDD (Savica et al, declines (Halpern et al., 2009).
2013). The Lewy body dementias are more common in women
under 80 years and significantly more common in men over
80 years. DLB accounts for 4.2% of all dementia cases in com-
Huntington Disease
munity settings and 7.5% of dementia diagnoses in specialty The diagnosis of Huntington disease (HD) is dependent upon
clinics (Vann Jones and O’Brien, 2014). DLB is believed to be the manifestation of an unequivocal extrapyramidal move-
commonly misdiagnosed, making the actual incidence rate ment disorder, although cognitive (Paulsen et al., 2014) and
likely higher. Also, there is significant comorbidity with AD as behavioral deficits can be the most debilitating aspect of the
autopsy studies have reported that 27% of cases of likely AD disease, place the greatest burden on HD families (Nehl and
also had Lewy bodies present (Wilson et al., 2012). Incidence Paulsen, 2004), and can be present in the absence of motor
peaks at 70–79 years and decreases after that (Savica et al., dysfunction (Paulsen and Long, 2014) (see Fig. 43.6 and
2013), making both PDD and DLB two of the young demen- Fig 43.7). HD is characterized by insidious and progressive
tias (along with FTD) that do not increase in prevalence with cognitive, emotional-behavioral, and motor symptoms. The
advanced age. The neuropsychological findings in the two symptom presentation for any given individual can include
disorders are largely similar, so they are often distinguished one or all of the HD domains. Cognitive symptoms can
by differences in the onset of signs and symptoms (Weintraub include forgetfulness, inattention, executive dysfunction,
et al., 2012). The cognitive symptoms of PDD often develop slowed psychomotor speed, negative emotion recognition,
at least 2 years after the onset of motor signs (e.g., bradyki- and deficits in time discrimination and production. Emotional-
nesia, gait instability, tremor, rigidity), while the cognitive behavioral symptoms may include apathy, depression, aggres-
symptoms in DLB tend to precede motor features or occur sion, disinhibition, anxiety, and obsessive and perseverative
within 1 year of the manifestation of motor signs (Gealogo, behaviors. In response to findings illustrating the prominence
2013). Motor signs develop more quickly in DLB when com- of cognitive outcomes in HD, Brooks and Dunnett (2013)
pared to the insidious onset of PDD signs and symptoms. reviewed animal models of basal ganglia cognitive impair-
Although the time course of cognitive symptoms and motor ments suggesting that translation of cognitive outcomes may
signs in DLB and PDD may serve as a helpful heuristic, caution provide readouts that better track disease for interventional
should be used when differentiating these dementias from studies. Additionally, readers are referred to new reviews that
one another based on onset alone, since subtle cognitive provide excellent characterization of the human cognitive
524 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

60

55

Symbol Digit Modalities Test


Group
50
Control
Low
Medium
45 High

40

35
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Year

330
320
310
300
290
Speeded Tapping

Group
280
Control
270 Low
Medium
260
High
250
240
230
220
210
0 1 2 3 4 5 6
Year
Fig. 43.6 The upper graph shows the fitted curves (based on a linear mixed effects regression fitted model) of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test
by year for progression groups (Control, Low probability of near-future diagnosis, Medium probability, High probability). The lower graph shows
the fitted curves for speeded tapping. (From Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, Paulsen, J.S., Smith, M.M., Long, J.D.,
PREDICT-HD Investigators and Coordinators of the Huntington Study Group, 84, 1233–1239, 2013, with permission from BMJ Publishing
Group Ltd.)

profile of HD (Dumas et al., 2013; O’Callaghan et al., 2014). cognitive profile of HD is characterized by bradyphrenia,
Efforts to determine the best candidate biomarkers are impairments in executive function, attention, working
ongoing. memory, emotion recognition, smell identification, and the
In a large study of over 1000 research participants with the perception and production of time (Paulsen et al., 2013, 2014;
gene expansion for HD, longitudinal cognitive declines were Rowe et al., 2010). One author proposed that poor attention
evident in every test examined (19 tests), with the earliest in HD may be due to an inability to automatize task perform-
declines in the Symbol Digit Modality Test, the Smell Identi- ance, which results in the diversion of cognitive resources
fication Test, the Stroop Word Identification Test, and the Trail to tasks that are normally automatic in healthy people
Making Test evident in a subgroup decades before motor diag- (Thompson et al., 2010). This premise is consistent with the
nosis (Paulsen et al., 2013, 2014). All cognitive measures well-known deficit HD patients have with procedural learning
examined showed significant changes in the subgroup with and memory (Holl et al., 2012) as well as automated behavior
the greatest proximity to motor diagnosis, whereas nine of such as the Stroop reading task. Memory problems are also
ten cognitive measures showed significant change in the inter- frequently reported by patients with HD and their families.
mediate group (7 to 15 years from motor diagnosis). The Objective deficits in learning and memory have been widely
Neuropsychology 525

10 TMS 43
9
8
7
6
CSF
5
4 DYS
3
SD

2 O-C
1
0
–1
–2
SDMT
–3 SMELL
–4 LOW MEDIUM HIGH TFC
PUTAMEN
–5
100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600
CAPD

Fig. 43.7 Data-derived model of premanifest Huntington disease. Scaled fitted spline curves of key variables plotted over CAPD (CAG-
corrected age). Blue colors indicate variables that decrease and red colors indicate variables that increase. Vertical axis is in standard deviation
(SD) units. TMS, Total motor score; CSF, cerebral spinal fluid; DYS, dysrhythmia; O-C, obsessive–compulsive; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities
Test; SMELL, University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT); TFC, total functional capacity. (From Paulsen, J.S., Long, J.D., Johnson,
H.J., et al., the PREDICT-HD Investigators and Coordinators of the Huntington Study Group, 2014, Clinical and biomarker changes in premanifest
Huntington disease show trial feasibility: a decade of the PREDICT-HD study. Front Aging Neurosci 6, 78.)

reported, with these patients displaying a typical subcortical depending on the location of sclerotic plaques (e.g., subcorti-
profile of reduced learning with difficulty in retrieval and cal white matter versus spinal cord). Furthermore, the cogni-
improvement with recognition cues. Additionally, emotion tive sequelae of MS are complicated by the fact that deficits
recognition declines regardless of modality (i.e., facial expres- vary across the different subtypes of MS, with progressive sub-
sion, verbal intonation), especially for fear, anger, and disgust types performing worse over time (Huijbregts et al., 2006).
(Brune et al., 2011; Eddy et al., 2011; 2012; Johnson et al., Although deficits vary, the typical cognitive profile of MS has
2007; Snowden et al., 2008). The cognitive processing retarda- been described as a subcortical one, similar to PD or HD,
tion is a sensitive (but nonspecific) indicator of dysfunction given the deficits outlined above. Depression and other distur-
in most diseases of the basal ganglia and some recent authors bances of emotional functioning occur in nearly half of these
have attempted to disintegrate cognitive processing speed, patients (Siegert and Abernethy, 2005). Psychiatric distur-
motor slowing, and executive abilities (Maroof et al., 2011; bances have also been linked with additional cognitive disa-
O’Rourke et al., 2011). Similarly, efforts have been made to bility (Feinstein, 2004). Depression in MS has also been found
separate the syntax deficits from working memory in HD to be related to cognitive deficits, particularly speeded atten-
(Sambin et al., 2012) to determine whether difficulties in lan- tion and executive functioning (Arnett et al., 2002). Recent
guage processing may be secondary to word retrieval, working updates in MS have indicated that subgroups may exist in MS
memory, or a direct syntactical role of the striatum. and that new measures may be beneficial to treatment assess-
ment, but neuropsychological assessment has not yet proven
effective (Filippi and Rocca, 2013; Rudick, 2012).
Multiple Sclerosis
Estimates of the prevalence of cognitive disability in multiple
sclerosis (MS) range between 40% and 50% in community-
Epilepsy
based samples (Amato et al., 2006; Jonsson et al., 2006) to Epilepsy, or seizure disorder, is well known to result in signifi-
more than 55% in clinical settings (Feinstein, 2004). Slowed cant psychiatric and cognitive comorbidities. Traditionally,
processing speed is perhaps the most prominent neuropsycho- these comorbid issues were viewed as epiphenomena of epi-
logical deficit in MS (Parmenter et al., 2007; Rao et al., 1991). lepsy but the emerging view is that there is a bidirectional
A variety of other cognitive declines observed in MS include relationship (Helmstaedter et al., 2014). New evidence sug-
working memory, cognitive flexibility, inhibition, problem gests that neuropsychological deficits in epilepsy are likely
solving, attention, verbal fluency, and spatial reasoning. Con- related to a variety of factors such as the cumulative effect on
frontation naming and verbal comprehension are generally the brain of seizure disorder over time or interictal activity, the
not affected in MS, and dementia is uncommon (Rao et al., results of which have been called epileptic encephalopathies.
1991). Consistent correlations between brain lesion magni- Many of the antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) are also known to have
tude and severity of cognitive dysfunction have also been negative cognitive side effects. Neuropsychological impair-
reported (Benedict et al., 2002). ments in epilepsy are generally underappreciated. The cogni-
A distinct cognitive profile is difficult to establish in MS, tive effects of epilepsy are unique for each patient, because
since individual differences in the microscopic and macro- there is no single cognitive profile associated with any
scopic pathology of the disease lead to tremendous variability epilepsy subtype (e.g., partial seizures, complex partial,
526 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

primary generalized). This is largely due to the fact that epi- language functioning given the deleterious effects these
lepsy is not a single disease process but rather any number of areas can have on quality of life impairments, but executive
etiologies affecting different cortical substrates that result in functioning, attention, cognitive efficiency, and visuospatial
seizures (Korczyn et al., 2013). Earlier onset of seizures is abilities are also examined thoroughly. Wada testing is still
associated with increased risk of general cognitive decline and considered the gold standard for determining how the non-
significant discrepancies in intellectual functions (van Iterson epileptic hemisphere will function in isolation but fMRI is
et al., 2014). Cognitive deficits also vary according to the loca- gaining acceptance as an alternative for language mapping
tion of the seizure foci, frequency of seizures, duration of (Barnett et al., 2014). Other indications include early seizure
seizure disorder, age of onset, and antiepileptic drug effects onset (due to the increased potential for cortical reorganiza-
(Jokeit and Schacher, 2004; Lee and Clason, 2008). Regardless tion) and left-handedness. Ultimately, neuropsychological
of whether a seizure is part of a single idiopathic event or a assessment in Wada testing can help reduce the potential
chronic epileptic disorder, the abnormal electrical activity pro- for postsurgical disconnection syndromes, aphasia, and
duced during the ictal period is usually associated with cogni- functional loss due to unexpectedly poor contralateral
tive and psychiatric changes. In chronic conditions, residual compensation.
interictal alterations in cognition and mood also occur. Given In addition to the cognitive aspects of epilepsy, psychiatric
the heterogeneity of cognitive deficits in epilepsy, neuropsy- and behavioral disorders are prevalent (McCagh et al., 2009),
chological testing is particularly indicated to provide an accu- though they tend to not be severe. Depression is the most
rate characterization of patients’ unique cognitive deficits. common mood disorder, with 42% of epilepsy patients report-
Memory deficits are one of the most common clinical ing symptoms (Helmstaedter and Witt, 2012). Rates of depres-
manifestations of cognitive impairment in epilepsy, likely due sion do not differ between TLE and FLE, though TLE patients
to the high incidence of seizures originating in the medial may be more neurotic and introverted. Symptoms of depres-
temporal lobes. There is general support for left temporal sion also appear to be associated with the future onset of
lobe epilepsy (TLE) resulting in verbal memory impairment focal seizures in some patients. TLE patients with comorbid
(Brown et al., 2014), but there are mixed findings regarding depression or interictal psychosis have more complicated
right TLE specifically resulting in visual memory impairments. courses and neuropathological differences than TLE patients
There is evidence that left TLE results in greater disruption of without psychiatric comorbidities (Kandratavicius et al., 2012).
cortical connective networks than right TLE and this may Increased anxiety in patients does not appear to be directly
support greater memory deficits in left TLE than right TLE associated with the neuropathology of epilepsy, but rather the
(Zulfi et al., 2014). In addition to memory, general intellectual unpredictability of seizures, social consequences of epilepsy,
functioning can be impaired. More specifically, executive func- and societal stigmatization (Mensah et al., 2007). There is also
tions and visuoconstructional abilities can be mildly impaired the phenomenon known as forced normalization where the
in both left and right TLE compared to healthy individuals, successful pharmacological management of seizures results
with earlier age of onset being associated with worse deficits, in onset of psychotic symptoms. It is hypothesized that the
but right TLE was specifically impaired for psychomotor speed uncontrolled seizures acted as a type of electroconvulsive
and higher level cognitive estimation abilities as well (Parente therapy to suppress a pre-existing psychotic condition.
et al., 2013) if the focus of seizure activity is in the language- A complicating issue in identifying cognitive and behavio-
dominant hemisphere. Postictal language recovery is also ral deficits associated with epilepsy is the independent effect
affected by seizure origin and generalization in frontal lobe antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) commonly have on neuropsycho-
epilepsy patients. Patients with seizures originating in the left logical functioning (Carreno et al., 2008). Memory, executive
frontal lobe and spreading to the language-dominant tempo- functioning, and processing are the most frequently affected
ral lobe have a slower recovery than their counterparts with by both classic and new AEDs, with older drugs such as topira-
either right frontal lobe seizures or seizures confined to the mate having worse cognitive side effects than newer medica-
frontal lobe only (Goldberg-Stern et al., 2004). Executive tions (Helmstaedter and Witt, 2013). The varying effects these
functioning is primarily affected in frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE), medications have on cognition must be weighed against the
especially if seizures originate in the left frontal lobe. However, benefits of minimizing the frequency and severity of seizures.
executive dysfunction may not be specific to FLE, and TLE An emerging area where neuropsychology is significantly con-
patients can perform similarly on executive measures (Longo tributing is the diagnosis and treatment of psychogenic
et al., 2013). It should be noted that many forms of epilepsy nonepileptic seizure disorder (PNES). PNES is a conversion
are thought to have little to no progressive effect on cognitive disorder in which the patient has the behavioral manifesta-
functions such as benign rolandic, absence, and juvenile myo- tions of a seizure without any accompany epileptiform activ-
clonic epilepsies, to name just a few (Avanzini et al., 2013). ity. The typical PNES case is a young adult female with lower
Neuropsychological evaluation is an integral part of neu- intellectual functioning, a history of trauma, and possibly
rosurgical treatments for pharmaco-resistant epilepsy (Helm- another psychogenic disorder that tends to experience psycho-
staedter, 2004). Preoperative evaluations and Wada testing genic seizures when under stress. At least 10% of these patients
provide useful information about the localization and later- also have EEG diagnosed seizure disorder. While the gold
alization of seizure-induced cognitive impairment. Assess- standard for diagnosing PNES is video electroencephalogra-
ment of patients’ cognitive reserve can be useful for predicting phy, this is not always available and neuropsychological
postoperative outcomes (e.g., better memory performance at testing is recommended as part of a staged comprehensive
baseline usually means greater memory loss after resection). diagnostic process for PNES (LaFrance et al., 2013).
Postoperative testing is also useful for treatment planning and
quality control following resection, and ultimately refines the
effectiveness of surgical procedures. It should be noted that
Traumatic Brain Injury
the evidence is mixed whether resective surgery significantly Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is one of the most common
improves cognitive functioning (Hallböök et al., 2013; Smith forms of neurological damage in the United States, resulting
et al., 2014). Neuropsychological test batteries for surgery in more than 53,000 deaths a year, particularly in young males
candidates are usually very extensive. Given the frequency of (Coronado et al., 2011). In cases of moderate to severe
temporal lobectomy surgery relative to other procedures, injury, impairments in several cognitive domains can occur,
most batteries are heavily focused on assessing memory and depending on what caused the injury (i.e., blunt force
Neuropsychology 527

versus projectile), the severity of the injury, site of the lesion, TABLE 43.5 Common Neurocognitive Sequelae of Moderate to
premorbid cognitive and personality factors, and treatment Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 43
received after the injury. Advances in knowledge regarding the
Cognitive domain Clinical manifestation of impairment
various cognitive sequelae of TBI have allowed for better diag-
nostic clarity. Beyond the established methods for staging the Attention Difficulty with sustained attention
severity of TBI (e.g., length of loss of consciousness or post- Poor concentration
traumatic amnesia, Glasgow Coma Scale [GCS]), neuropsy- Psychomotor impersistence
chological testing can be used to increase diagnostic accuracy Memory Problems with acquiring and retaining new
by measuring the classic cognitive deficits that result from verbal or nonverbal information
head injury. Problems in retrieving verbal and nonverbal
Neuropsychological testing is frequently indicated in TBI, memories
because the range of deficits following parenchymal damage Speed of information Slowed sensorimotor skills and information
can vary significantly. Damage to the frontal cortex is common processing processing
due to the morphology of the skull and frequency of anterior Executive functioning Problems in convergent and divergent
impacts during falls and motor vehicle accidents. For example, reasoning
a circumscribed TBI involving the ventromedial prefrontal Poor judgment
cortex can produce changes in social cognition and emotion Difficulty planning
recognition that result in behavioral issues (Spikman et al., Problems in self-monitoring and self-
2013) associated with misinterpretation of social cues or correcting behavior
lowered frustration tolerance. There is also evidence of a Awareness of Difficulty recognizing deficits
general pattern of executive impairment following TBI result- symptoms Unrealistic expectations concerning the
ing from disruption of white matter structural networks in the recovery of functions
brain (Caeyenberghs et al., 2014). This combination of social, Problems related to poor treatment
behavioral, and executive dysfunction is often called the compliance
frontal lobe syndrome and is common in TBI. A less severe Language and Problems in word comprehension
TBI might only lead to subtle deficits in attention or process- communication Impaired reading, spelling, and writing ability
ing speed (Rackley et al., 2012). Lingering fatigue is also a Tendency to become fragmented in free
common comorbidity (Zgaljardic et al., 2014). Despite indi- speech
vidual clinical variability, several common neurobehavioral Integrative functions Problems in adequate or time-efficient
sequelae of moderate to severe TBI can be identified (Table execution of various perceptual-motor-
43.5). Cognitive recovery is protracted for moderate to severe spatial-sequential tasks
TBIs, with most improvements occurring in the first year but
measurable recovery of cognitive functioning still occurring
several years after the injury. However, even after a prolonged
length of time, individuals do not necessarily return to prein-
jury levels. neuropsychological assessment is recommended to assist with
In addition to moderate and severe TBI, the cognitive management, and that while acute rest is recommended fol-
sequelae of concussion or mild TBI (mTBI) have become a lowing mTBI, there is limited research support for the efficacy
major focus of research and public education campaigns with of this intervention. Comparing TBI to mTBI, executive func-
particular emphasis on sports-related concussion and blast- tioning, attention, processing speed, and memory are the most
related mTBIs sustained by soldiers in Iraq and Afghanistan. common deficits while mTBI typically results in no lingering
For mTBI, there are generally two categories: uncomplicated deficits (Jamora et al., 2012; Vanderploeg et al., 2014; Winardi
with a recognized change in mental status generally less than et al., 2014). The vast majority of mTBI cases will make a full
20 minutes and complicated where there is an acute change recovery. Some individuals report a constellation of symptoms
in mental status and visible structural abnormalities on neu- following mTBI that include headaches, fatigue, irritability,
roimaging. While complicated mTBI has traditionally been depression, and poor concentration. This is commonly
believed to result in worse outcomes, research has demon- referred to as the postconcussion syndrome (PCS) and is asso-
strated there is no real difference between complicated and ciated with pre-existing mood and anxiety symptoms, as well
uncomplicated mTBI on measures of cognitive functioning as mood changes surrounding the injury, and negative beliefs
and self-reported symptoms at a month post-injury (Iverson regarding cognitive effects of TBI (Kit et al., 2014; Lange et al.,
et al., 2012a; Lange et al., 2012). Additionally, though clinical 2014). Treatment for patients with persisting PCS frequently
lore stipulates that there is increasing neurologic damage with involves education and setting factual expectations about the
a history of multiple concussions, there is mixed evidence normal course of recovery from their injury and prognosis,
supporting this proposition (Iverson et al, 2012b), though a with earlier education leading to generally better outcomes.
history of more severe TBI may negatively affect health out-
comes (Dams-O’Connor et al., 2013). The latest consensus REFERENCES
statement on treatment of concussion (McCrory et al., 2013) The complete reference list is available online at https://expertconsult
states that 90% of adult mTBIs resolve within 10 days, that .inkling.com/.
Neuropsychology 527.e1

Caeyenberghs, K., Leemans, A., Leunissen, I., et al., 2014. Altered


REFERENCES structural networks and executive deficits in traumatic brain injury 43
Amato, M.P., Zipoli, V., Portaccio, E., 2006. Multiple sclerosis-related patients. Brain Struct. Funct. 219 (1), 193–209.
cognitive changes: a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal Carreno, M., Donaire, A., Sanchez-Carpintero, R., 2008. Cognitive
studies. J. Neurol. Sci. 245, 41–46. disorders associated with epilepsy: diagnosis and treatment. Neu-
American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2013. Diagnostic and Statisti- rologist 14 (6 Suppl. 1), S26–S34.
cal Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth ed. American Psychiatric Pub- Conti, M.Z., Vicini-Chilovi, B., Riva, M., et al., 2013. Odor identifica-
lishing, Arlington, VA. tion deficit predicts clinical conversion from mild cognitive impair-
Anderson, S.W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., 1990. Neuropsychological ment to dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Clin.
impairments associated with lesions caused by tumor or stroke. Neuropsychol. 28 (5), 391–399.
Arch. Neurol. 47, 397–405. Coronado, V.G., Xu, L., Basavaraju, S.V., et al., Centers for Disease
Andrew, M.K., Rockwood, K., 2008. A five-point change in Modified Control and Prevention (CDC), 2011. Surveillance for traumatic
Mini-Mental State Examination was clinically meaningful in brain injury-related deaths—United States, 1997-2007. MMWR Sur-
community-dwelling elderly people. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 61, veill. Summ. 60 (5), 1–32.
827–831. Crum, R.M., Anthony, J.C., Bassett, S.S., et al., 1993. Population-based
Arnett, P.A., Higginson, C.I., Voss, W.D., et al., 2002. Relationship norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educa-
between coping, cognitive dysfunction, and depression in multiple tional level. JAMA 296, 2386–2391.
sclerosis. Clin. Neuropsychol. 16, 341–355. Cullen, B., Fahy, S., Cunningham, C.J., et al., 2005. Screening for
Arvanitakis, Z., 2010. Update on frontotemporal dementia. Neurolo- dementia in an Irish community sample using MMSE: a compari-
gist 16, 16–22. son of norm-adjusted versus fixed cut-points. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychia-
Avanzini, G., Depaulis, A., Tassinari, A., de Curtis, M., 2013. Do sei- try 20, 371–376.
zures and epileptic activity worsen epilepsy and deteriorate cogni- Dams-O’Connor, K., Spielman, L., Singh, A., et al., 2013. The impact
tive function? Epilepsia 54 (Suppl. 8), 14–21. of previous traumatic brain injury on health and functioning: a
Ballard, C., Aarsland, D., Francis, P., Corbett, A., 2013. Neuropsychi- TRACK-TBI study. J. Neurotrauma 30 (24), 2014–2020.
atric symptoms in patients with dementias associated with cortical Demakis, G.J., 2012. Civil Capacities in Clinical Neuropsychology
Lewy bodies: pathophysiology, clinical features, and pharmacologi- Research Findings and Practical Applications. National Academy of
cal management. Drugs Aging 30, 603–611. Neuropsychology series on evidence-based practices. Oxford Uni-
Barnett, A., Marty-Dugas, J., McAndrews, M.P., 2014. Advantages of versity Press, New York.
sentence-level fMRI language tasks in presurgical language mapping Dong, Y., Sharma, V., Chan, B.P., 2010. The Montreal Cognitive Assess-
for temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 32, 114–120. ment (MoCA) is superior to the Mini-Mental State Examination
Barona, A., Reynolds, C., Chastain, R., 1984. A demographically based (MMSE) for the detection of vascular cognitive impairment after
index of premorbid intelligence for the WAIS-R. J. Clin. Consult. acute stroke. J. Neurol. Sci. 299, 15–18.
Psychol. 52, 885–887. Dong, Y.H., Gan, D., Tay, S., et al., 2013. Patterns of neuropsychologi-
Barr, W.B., 2008. Historical development of the neuropsychological cal impairment in Alzheimer’s disease and mixed dementia. J.
test battery. In: Morgan, J.E., Ricker, J.H. (Eds.), Textbook of Clinical Neurol. Sci. 333 (1–2), 5–8.
Neuropsychology. Taylor & Francis, New York, pp. 3–17. Duff, K., Beglinger, L.J., Adams, W.H., 2009. Validation of the modi-
Beber, B.C., Chaves, M.L.F., 2013. Evaluation of patients with behav- fied telephone interview for cognitive status in amnestic mild cogni-
ioral and cognitive complaints: misdiagnosis in frontotemporal tive impairment and intact elders. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 23,
dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Neuropsychol. 7 (1), 38–43.
60–65. Dumas, E.M., van den Bogaard, S.J., Middelkoop, H.A., Roos, R.A.,
Benedet, M.J., Montz, R., Delgado-Bolton, R.C., 2012. Alzheimer’s 2013. A review of cognition in Huntington’s disease. Front Biosci.
disease and vascular dementia: neuropsychological differentiation (Schol. Ed.) 5, 1–18.
in clinical practice. Clin. Gerontol. 35, 88–104. Eddy, C.M., Mitchell, I.J., Beck, S.R., et al., 2011. Altered subjective fear
Benedict, R.H.B., Bakshi, R., Simon, J., et al., 2002. Frontal cortex responses in Huntington’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 17,
atrophy predicts cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. J. Neu- 386–389.
ropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 14, 44–51. Eddy, C.M., Sira Mahalingappa, S., Rickards, H.E., 2012. Is Hunting-
Benton, A.L., 1975. Neuropsychological assessment. In: Tower, D.B. ton’s disease associated with deficits in theory of mind? Acta Neurol.
(Ed.), The Nervous System, vol. 2. The Clinical Neurosciences. Scand. 126 (6), 376–383.
Raven Press, New York. England, H.B., Gillis, M.M., Hampstead, B.M., 2014. RBANS memory
Bernstein, I.H., Lacrtiz, L., Barlow, C.E., et al., 2011. Psychometric indices are related to medial temporal lobe volumetrics in healthy
evaluation of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in three older adults and those with mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Clin.
diverse samples. Clin. Neuropsychol. 25 (1), 119–126. Neuropsychol. 29, 322–328.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Binns, M.A., et al., 2014. Effects of bilin- Falkowski, J., Atchison, T., DeButte-Smith, M., et al., 2014. Executive
gualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: evi- functioning and the metabolic syndrome: A Project FRONTIER
dence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology 28 (2), Study. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 29 (1), 47–53.
290–304. Farias, S.T., Mungas, D., Reed, B.R., et al., 2009. Progression of mild
Blair, J.R., Spreen, O., 1989. Predicting premorbid IQ: a revision cognitive impairment to dementia in clinic- vs community-based
of the National Adult Reading Test. Clin. Neuropsychol. 3, cohorts. Arch. Neurol. 66, 1151–1157.
129–136. Feinstein, A., 2004. The neuropsychiatry of multiple sclerosis. Can. J.
Bland, R.C., Newman, S.C., 2001. Mild dementia or cognitive impair- Psychiatry 49, 157–163.
ment: the Modified Mini-Mental State examination (3MS) as a Ferman, T.J., Smith, G.E., Kantarci, K., et al., 2013. Nonamnestic mild
screen for dementia. Can. J. Psychiatry 46, 506–510. cognitive impairment progresses to dementia with Lewy bodies.
Braak, H., Braak, E., 1991. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer- Neurology 81, 2032–2038.
related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. Filippi, M., Rocca, M.A., 2013. Multiple sclerosis: new measures to
Brainerd, C.J., Reyna, V.F., Petersen, R.C., et al., 2013. The apolipopro- monitor the disease. Lancet Neurol. 12 (1), 12–13.
tein E genotype predicts longitudinal transitions to mild cognitive Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., McHugh, P.R., 1975. Mini-Mental State:
impairment but not to Alzheimer’s dementia: findings from a A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the
nationally representative study. Neuropsychology 27 (1), 86–94. clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198.
Brooks, S.P., Dunnett, S.B., 2013. Cognitive deficits in animal models Gealogo, G.A., 2013. Dementia with Lewy bodies: A comprehensive
of basal ganglia disorders. Brain Res. Bull. 92, 29–40. review for nurses. J. Neurosci. Nurs. 45 (6), 347–359.
Brown, F.C., Westerveld, M., Langfitt, J.T., et al., 2014. Influence of Giacino, J., Whyte, J., 2005. The vegetative and minimally conscious
anxiety on memory performance in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epi- states: current knowledge and remaining questions. J. Head Trauma
lepsy Behav. 31, 19–24. Rehabil. 20, 30–50.
Brune, M., Blank, K., Witthaus, H., Saft, C., 2011. “Theory of mind” Goldberg-Stern, H., Gadoth, N., Cahill, W., et al., 2004. Language
is impaired in Huntington’s disease. Mov. Disord. 26, 671– dysfunction after frontal lobe partial seizures. Neurology 62,
678. 1637–1638.
527.e2 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

Guo, L.H., Alexopoulos, P., Eisele, T., et al., 2013. The National Insti- Johnson, S.A., Stout, J.C., Solomon, A.C., et al., 2007. Beyond disgust:
tute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association research criteria for mild impaired recognition of negative emotions prior to diagnosis in
cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: predicting the Huntington’s disease. Brain 130 (Pt 7), 1732–1744.
outcome. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 263, 325–333. Jokeit, H., Schacher, M., 2004. Neuropsychological aspects of type of
Hales, C.M., Hu, W.T., 2013. From frontotemporal lobar degeneration epilepsy and etiological factors in adults. Epilepsy Behav. 5 (Suppl.
pathology to frontotemporal lobar degeneration biomarkers. Int. 1), S14–S20.
Rev. Psychiatry 25 (2), 210–220. Jonsson, A., Andresen, J., Storr, L., et al., 2006. Cognitive impairment
Hallböök, T., Tideman, P., Rosen, I., et al., 2013. Epilepsy surgery in in newly diagnosed multiple sclerosis patients: a 4-year follow-up
children with drug-resistant epilepsy, a long-term follow-up. Acta study. J. Neurol. Sci. 245, 77–85.
Neurol. Scand. 128 (6), 414–421. Kandratavicius, L., Hallak, J.E., Young, L.T., et al., 2012. Differential
Halpern, C.H., Rick, J.H., Danish, S.F., et al., 2009. Cognition follow- aberrant sprouting in temporal lobe epilepsy with psychiatric
ing bilateral deep brain stimulation surgery of the subthalamic co-morbidities. Psychiatry Res. 195 (3), 144–150.
nucleus for Parkinson’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, Kim, H.J., Kang, S.J., Kim, C., et al., 2013. The effects of small vessel
443–451. disease and amyloid burden on neuropsychiatric symptoms: a study
Harciarek, M., Cosentino, S., 2013. Language, executive function and among patients with subcortical vascular cognitive impairments.
social cognition in the diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia syn- Neurobiol. Aging 34 (7), 1913–1920.
dromes. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 25 (2), 178–196. Kit, K.A., Mateer, C.A., Tuokko, H.A., Spencer-Rodgers, J., 2014. Influ-
Heaton, R.K., Grant, I., Matthews, C.G., 1991. Comprehensive Norms ence of negative stereotypes and beliefs on neuropsychological test
for Expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographic Corrections, performance in a traumatic brain injury population. J. Int. Neu-
Research Findings, and Clinical Applications. Psychological Assess- ropsychol. Soc. 20 (2), 157–167.
ment Resources, Odessa, FL. Korczyn, A.D., Schachter, S.C., Brodie, M.J., et al., 2013. Epilepsy,
Hebert, L.E., Weuve, J., Scherr, P.A., Evans, D.A., 2013. Alzheimer cognition, and neuropsychiatry (Epilepsy, Brain, and Mind, part 2).
disease in the United States (2010–2050) estimated using the 2010 Epilepsy Behav. 28 (2), 283–302.
census. Neurology 80, 1778–1783. LaFrance, W.C. Jr., Baker, G.A., Duncan, R., et al., 2013. Minimum
Helmstaedter, C., 2004. Neuropsychological aspects of epilepsy requirements for the diagnosis of psychogenic nonepileptic sei-
surgery. Epilepsy Behav. 5 (Suppl. 1), S45–S55. zures: a staged approach: a report from the International League
Helmstaedter, C., Aldenkamp, A.P., Baker, G.A., et al., 2014. Disentan- against Epilepsy Nonepileptic Seizures Task Force. Epilepsia 54 (11),
gling the relationship between epilepsy and its behavioral 2005–2018.
comorbidities—the need for prospective studies in new-onset epi- Lange, R.T., Brickell, T.A., French, L.M., et al., 2012. Neuropsychologi-
lepsies. Epilepsy Behav. 31, 43–47. cal outcome from uncomplicated mild, complicated mild, and
Helmstaedter, C., Witt, J.A., 2012. Multifactorial etiology of interictal moderate traumatic brain injury in US military personnel. Arch.
behavior in frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 53 (10), Clin. Neuropsychol. 27 (5), 480–494.
1765–1773. Lange, R.T., Brickell, T.A., Kennedy, J.E., et al., 2014. Factors influenc-
Helmstaedter, C., Witt, J.A., 2013. The longer-term cognitive effects of ing postconcussion and posttraumatic stress symptom reporting
adjunctive antiepileptic treatment with lacosamide in comparison following military-related concurrent polytrauma and traumatic
with lamotrigine and topiramate in a naturalistic outpatient setting. brain injury. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 29 (4), 329–347.
Epilepsy Behav. 26 (2), 182–187. Lee, D.R., Taylor, J.P., Thomas, A.J., 2012. Assessment of cognitive
Hogervorst, E., Bandelow, S., Hart, J. Jr., et al., 2004. Telephone fluctuation in dementia: a systematic review of the literature. Int. J.
word-list recall tested in the rural aging and memory study: two Geriatr. Psychiatry 27, 989–998.
parallel versions for the TICS-M. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 19, Lee, G.P., Clason, C.L., 2008. Classification of seizure disorder
875–880. and syndromes, and neuropsychological impairment in adults
Holl, A.K., Wilkinson, L., Tabrizi, S.J., et al., 2012. Probabilistic with epilepsy. In: Morgan, J.E., Ricker, J.H. (Eds.), Textbook of
classification learning with corrective feedback is selectively Clinical Neuropsychology. Taylor & Francis, New York, pp.
impaired in early Huntington’s disease—evidence for the role of the 437–465.
striatum in learning with feedback. Neuropsychologia 50, Lee, J.H., Kim, S.H., Kim, G.H., et al., 2011. Identification of pure
2176–2186. subcortical vascular dementia using 11C-Pittsburgh compound B.
Hoops, S., Nazem, S., Siderowf, A.D., et al., 2009. Validity of the Neurology 77, 18–25.
MoCA and MMSE in the detection of MCI and dementia in Parkin- Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B., Bigler, E.D., Tranel, D., 2012. The prac-
son disease. Neurology 73, 1738–1745. tice of neuropsychological assessment. In: Lezak, M.D., Howieson,
Huijbregts, S.C., Kalkers, N.F., de Sonneville, L.M., et al., 2006. Cogni- D.B., Loring, D.W. (Eds.), Neuropsychological Assessment, fourth
tive impairment and decline in different MS subtypes. J. Neurol. Sci. ed. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 3–14.
245, 187–194. Lim, X., Yeo, J.M., Green, A., Pal, S., 2013. The diagnostic utility of
Iverson, G.L., Brooks, B.L., 2011. Improving accuracy for identifying cerebrospinal fluid alpha-synuclein analysis in dementia with Lewy
cognitive impairment. In: Schoenberg, M.R., Scott, J.G. (Eds.), The bodies—a systematic review and meta-analysis. Parkinsonism Relat.
Little Black Book of Neuropsychology: A Syndrome-Based Approach. Disord. 19 (10), 851–858.
Springer, New York. Lines, C.R., McCarroll, K.A., Lipton, R.B., et al., 2003. Telephone
Iverson, G.L., Echemendia, R.J., Lamarre, A.K., et al., 2012b. Possible screening for amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 60,
lingering effects of multiple past concussions. Rehabil. Res. Pract. 261–266.
2012, 316575. Lojo-Seoane, C., Facal, D., Guàrdia-Olmos, J., Juncos-Rabadán, O.,
Iverson, G.L., Lange, R.T., Waljas, M., et al., 2012a. Outcome from 2014. Structural model for estimating the influence of cognitive
complicated versus uncomplicated mild traumatic brain injury. reserve on cognitive performance in adults with subjective memory
Rehabil. Res. Pract. 2012, 415740. complaints. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 29 (3), 245–255.
Jack, C., Knopman, D.S., Jagust, W.J., et al., 2013. Tracking pathophysi- Longo, C.A., Kerr, E.N., Smith, M.L., 2013. Executive functioning in
ological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical children with intractable frontal lobe or temporal lobe epilepsy.
model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 12, 207–216. Epilepsy Behav. 26 (1), 102–108.
James, B.D., Bennett, D.A., Boyle, P.A., et al., 2012. Dementia from Maroof, D.A., Gross, A.L., Brandt, J., 2011. Modeling longitudinal
Alzheimer disease and mixed pathologies in the oldest old. JAMA change in motor and cognitive processing speed in presymptomatic
307 (17), 1798–1800. Huntington’s disease. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 33 (8),
Jamora, C.W., Young, A., Ruff, R.M., 2012. Comparison of subjective 901–909.
cognitive complaints with neuropsychological tests in individuals McCagh, J., Fisk, J.E., Baker, G.A., 2009. Epilepsy, psychosocial and
with mild vs more severe traumatic brain injuries. Brain Inj. 26 (1), cognitive functioning. Epilepsy Res. 86, 1–14.
36–47. McCrory, P., Meeuwisse, W.H., Aubry, M., et al., 2013. Consensus state-
Jellinger, K.A., 2013. Pathology and pathogenesis of vascular ment on concussion in sport: the 4th International Conference on
cognitive impairment—a critical update. Front Aging Neurosci. Concussion in Sport, Zurich, November 2012. J. Athl. Train. 48 (4),
5, 17. 554–575.
Neuropsychology 527.e3

McDowell, I., Kristjansson, B., Hill, G.B., et al., 1997. Community Rackley, C., Allen, D.N., Fuhrman, L.J., Mayfield, J., 2012. Generaliz-
screening for dementia: the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) and ability of WISC-IV index and subtest score profiles in children with 43
Modified Mini-Mental State Exam (3MS) compared. J. Clin. Epide- traumatic brain injury. Child. Neuropsychol. 18 (5), 512–519.
miol. 50, 377–383. Rao, S.M., Leo, G.J., Bernardin, L., et al., 1991. Cognitive dysfunction
McKhann, G.M., Knopman, D.S., Chertkow, H., et al., 2011. The diag- in multiple sclerosis I. Frequency, patterns, and prediction. Neurol-
nosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations ogy 41, 685–691.
from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association Rascovsky, K., Hodges, J.R., Knopman, D., et al., 2011. Sensitivity of
workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotem-
Alzheimer’s Dement. 7, 263–269. poral dementia. Brain 134, 2456–2477.
Mensah, S.A., Beavis, J.M., Thapar, A.K., et al., 2007. A community Reilly, J.P., Antonucci, J.E., Grossman, S.M., Grossman, M., 2011.
study of the presence of anxiety disorder in people with epilepsy. Anomia as a marker of distinct semantic memory impairments in
Epilepsy Behav. 11, 118–124. Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neuropsychology 25
Mitchell, A.J., 2009. A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini- (4), 413–426.
mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild Rincon, F., Wright, C.B., 2013. Vascular cognitive impairment. Curr.
cognitive impairment. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 411–431. Opin. Neurol. 26, 29–36.
Nasreddine, Z.S., Phillips, N.A., Bedirian, V., et al., 2005. The Mon- Rosen, H.J., Alcantar, O., Zakrzewski, J., et al., 2014. Metacognition in
treal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s
cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. disease. Neuropsychology 28 (3), 436–447.
Nazem, S., Siderowf, A.D., Duda, J.E., et al., 2009. Montreal cognitive Rossetti, H., Lacritz, L.H., Cullum, C.M., et al., 2011. Normative data
assessment performance in patients with Parkinson’s disease with for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a population
“normal” global cognition according to mini-mental state examina- based sample. Neurology 77, 1272–1275.
tion score. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 57, 304–308. Rowe, K.C., Paulsen, J.S., Langbehn, D.R., et al., 2010. Self-paced
Nehl, C., Paulsen, J.S., 2004. Cognitive and psychiatric aspects of timing detects and tracks change in prodromal Huntington disease.
Huntington disease contribute to functional capacity. J. Nerv. Ment. Neuropsychology 24, 435–442.
Dis. 192, 72–74. Rudick, R.A., 2012. The elusive biomarker for personalized medicine
O’Bryant, S.E., Humphreys, J.D., Smith, G.E., et al., 2008. Detecting in multiple sclerosis: the search continues. Neurology 79 (6),
dementia with the mini-mental state examination in highly edu- 498–499.
cated individuals. Arch. Neurol. 65, 963–967. Sachdev, P., Kalaria, R., O’Brien, J., et al., 2014. Diagnostic criteria for
O’Callaghan, C., Bertoux, M., Hornberger, M., 2014. Beyond and vascular cognitive disorders: A VASCOG statement. Alzheimer Dis.
below the cortex: the contribution of striatal dysfunction to cogni- Assoc. Disord. 28 (3), 206–218.
tion and behaviour in neurodegeneration. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Salmon, D.P., Bondi, M.W., 2009. Neuropsychological assessment of
Psychiatry 85 (4), 371–378. dementia. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 257–282.
O’Rourke, J.J., Beglinger, L.J., Smith, M.M., et al., 2011. The Trail Salmon, D.P., Thomas, R.G., Pay, M.M., et al., 2002. Alzheimer’s
Making Test in prodromal Huntington disease: contributions of disease can be accurately diagnosed in very mildly impaired indi-
disease progression to test performance. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. viduals. Neurology 59, 1022–1028.
33, 567–579. Sambin, S., Teichmann, M., De Diego Balaguer, R., et al., 2012. The
Parente, A., Manfredi, V., Villani, F., et al., 2013. Investigating higher- role of the striatum in sentence processing: disentangling syntax
order cognitive functions in temporal lobe epilepsy: cognitive esti- from working memory in Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia
mation. Epilepsy Behav. 29 (2), 330–336. 50 (11), 2625–2635.
Park, J.H., Seo, S.W., Kim, C., et al., 2013. Effects of cerebrovascular Savica, R., Grossardt, B.R., Bower, J.H., et al., 2013. Incidence of
disease and amyloid beta burden on cognition in subjects with dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson disease dementia. JAMA
subcortical vascular cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 35 (1), Neurol. 70 (11), 1396–1402.
254–260. Schmand, B., Rienstra, A., Tamminga, H., et al., 2014. Responsiveness
Parmenter, B.A., Shucard, J.L., Shucard, D.W., 2007. Information of magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological assessment
processing deficits in multiple sclerosis: a matter of complexity. J. in memory clinic patients. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 40, 409–418.
Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 417–423. Schneider, J.A., Arvanitakis, Z., Bang, W., et al., 2007. Mixed brain
Paulino Ramirez Diaz, S., Gil Gregorio, P., Manuel Ribera Casado, J., pathologies account for most dementia cases in community-
et al., 2005. The need for a consensus in the use of assessment tools dwelling older persons. Neurology 69, 2197–2204.
for Alzheimer’s disease: the Feasibility Study (assessment tools for Schoenberg, M.R., Scott, J.G., Duff, K., et al., 2002. Estimation of
dementia in Alzheimer Centres across Europe), a European Alzhe- WAIS-III intelligence from combined performance and demo-
imer’s Disease Consortium’s (EADC) survey. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychia- graphic variables: development of the OPIE-3. Clin. Neuropsychol.
try 20, 744–748. 16, 426–437.
Paulsen, J.S., Long, J.D., Johnson, H.J., et al., Predict-HD Investigators Schoenberg, M.R., Scott, J.G., 2011. The neuropsychology referral and
and Coordinators of the Huntington Study Group, 2014. Clinical answering the referral question. In: Schoenberg, M.R., Scott, J.G.
and biomarker changes in premanifest Huntington Disease show (Eds.), The Little Black Book of Neuropsychology: A Syndrome-
trial feasibility: a decade of the PREDICT-HD study. Front Aging Based Approach. Springer, New York.
Neurosci. l6, 78. Siegert, R.J., Abernethy, D.A., 2005. Depression in multiple sclerosis:
Paulsen, J.S., Long, D.L., 2014. Onset of Huntington disease: Can it a review. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 469–475.
be purely cognitive? Mov. Disord. 29 (11), 1342–1350. Slark, J., Bentley, P., Sharma, P., 2012. Silent brain infarction in the
Paulsen, J.S., Smith, M.M., Long, J.D., Predict-HD Investigators and presence of systemic vascular disease. JRSM Cardiovasc. Dis. 1 (3),
Coordinators of the Huntington Study Group, 2013. Cognitive 1–9.
decline in prodromal Huntington Disease: implications for clinical Smith, G.E., Ivnik, R.J., Lucas, J., 2008. Assessment techniques: tests,
trials. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 84, 1233–1239. test batteries, norms, and methodological approaches. In: Morgan,
Pearson, 2009. Advanced clinical solutions for WAIS-IV and WMS-IV: J.E., Ricker, J.H. (Eds.), Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology.
Administration and scoring manual. Pearson Assessment, San Taylor & Francis, New York, pp. 38–57.
Antonio. Smith, M.L., Olds, J., Snyder, T., et al., 2014. A follow-up study of
Perri, R., Monaco, M., Fadda, L., et al., 2014. Neuropsychological cor- cognitive function in young adults who had resective epilepsy
relates of behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease, frontal variant surgery in childhood. Epilepsy Behav. 32, 79–83.
of frontotemporal, subcortical vascular, and Lewy body dementias: a Snowden, J.S., Austin, N.A., Sembi, S., et al., 2008. Emotion recogni-
comparative study. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 39 (3), 669–677. tion in Huntington’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neu-
Petersen, R., Smith, G., Waring, S., et al., 1999. Mild cognitive impair- ropsychologia 46, 2638–2649.
ment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56, Sohlberg, M.M., Turkstra, L., 2011. Optimizing Cognitive Rehabilita-
303–308. tion Effective Instructional Methods. Guilford Press, New York.
Petersen, R.C., Roberts, R.O., Knopman, D.S., et al., 2009. Mild cogni- Spikman, J.M., Milders, M.V., Visser-Keizer, A.C., et al., 2013. Deficits
tive impairment: ten years later. Arch. Neurol. 66, 1447–1455. in facial emotion recognition indicate behavioral changes and
527.e4 PART II Neurological Investigations and Related Clinical Neurosciences

impaired self-awareness after moderate to severe traumatic brain van Iterson, L., Zijlstra, B.J., Augustijn, P.B., et al., 2014. Duration of
injury. PLoS ONE 8 (6), e65581. epilepsy and cognitive development in children: a longitudinal
Stephan, B.C., Matthews, F.E., Hunter, S., et al., Medical Research study. Neuropsychology 28 (2), 212–221.
Council Cognitive Function Ageing Study, 2012. Neuropathological Vanderploeg, R.D., Donnell, A.J., Belanger, H.G., Curtiss, G., 2014.
profile of mild cognitive impairment from a population perspective. Consolidation deficits in traumatic brain injury: the core and resid-
Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 26, 205–212. ual verbal memory defect. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 36 (1),
Storandt, M., Balota, D.A., Aschenbrenner, A.J., Morris, J.C., 2014. 58–73.
Clinical and psychological characteristics of the initial cohort of the Vann Jones, S.A., O’Brien, J.T., 2014. The prevalence and incidence of
dominantly inherited Alzheimer network (DIAN). Neuropsychol- dementia with Lewy bodies: a systematic review of population and
ogy 28 (1), 19–29. clinical studies. Psychol. Med. 44 (4), 673–683.
Taylor, W.D., Aizenstein, H.J., Alexopoulos, G.S., 2013. The vascular Videnovic, A., Bernard, B., Fan, W., et al., 2010. The Montreal Cogni-
depression hypothesis: mechanisms linking vascular disease with tive Assessment as a screening tool for cognitive dysfunction in
depression. Mol. Psychiatry 18 (9), 963–974. Huntington’s disease. Mov. Disord. 25, 401–404.
Teng, E.L., Chui, H.C., 1987. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) Vos, S.J., Xiong, C., Visser, P.J., et al., 2013. Preclinical Alzheimer’s
examination. J. Clin. Psychiatry 48, 314–318. disease and its outcome: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurol.
Thompson, J.C., Poliakoff, E., Sollom, A.C., et al., 2010. Automaticity 12 (10), 957–965.
and attention in Huntington’s disease: when two hands are not Weintraub, S., Wicklund, A.H., Salmon, D.P., 2012. The neuropsycho-
better than one. Neuropsychologia 48, 171–178. logical profile of Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect.
Tierney, M.C., Black, S.E., Szalai, J.P., et al., 2001. Recognition memory Med. 2, a006171.
and verbal fluency differentiate probable Alzheimer disease from Wilson, R.S., Aggarwal, N.T., Barnes, L.L., et al., 2010. Cognitive
subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Arch. Neurol. 58, decline in incident Alzheimer disease in a community population.
1654–1659. Neurology 74, 951–955.
Tombaugh, T.N., 2005. Test-retest reliable coefficients and 5-year Wilson, R.S., Segawa, E., Boyle, P.A., et al., 2012. The natural history
change scores for the MMSE and 3MS. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychol. Aging 27 (4),
20, 485–503. 1008–1017.
Toner, C.K., Reese, B.E., Neargarder, S., et al., 2012. Vision-fair neu- Winardi, W., Kwan, A.L., Wang, T.L., et al., 2014. Predictors of memory
ropsychological assessment in normal aging, Parkinson’s disease and processing speed dysfunctions after traumatic brain injury.
and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychol. Aging 27 (3), 785–790. BioMed. Res. Int. 2014, 129796.
Tranel, D., 2008. Theories of clinical neuropsychology and brain- Witt, K., Granert, O., Daniels, C., et al., 2013. Relation of lead trajec-
behavior relationships. In: Morgan, J.E., Ricker, J.H. (Eds.), Text- tory and electrode position to neuropsychological outcomes of
book of Clinical Neuropsychology. Taylor & Francis, New York, pp. subthalamic neurostimulation in Parkinson’s disease: results from
25–37. a randomized trial. Brain 136, 2109–2119.
Tröster, A.I., 2008. Neuropsychological characteristics of dementia Yu, L., Boyle, P., Schneider, J.A., et al., 2013. APOE ε4, Alzheimer’s
with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease with dementia: differen- disease pathology, cerebrovascular disease, and cognitive change
tiation, early detection, and implications for mild cognitive impair- over the years prior to death. Psychol. Aging 28 (4), 1015–1023.
ment” and biomarkers. Neuropsychol. Rev. 18, 103–119. Zgaljardic, D.J., Durham, W.J., Mossberg, K.A., et al., 2014. Neuropsy-
Tschanz, J.T., Welsh-Bohmer, K.A., Plassman, B.L., et al., 2002. An chological and physiological correlates of fatigue following trau-
adaptation of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination: analy- matic brain injury. Brain Inj. 28 (4), 389–397.
sis of demographic influences and normative data: the Cache Zulfi, H., Lenartowicz, A., Yeh, H.J., et al., 2014. Functional connectiv-
County study. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 15, ity of hippocampal networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia
28–38. 55 (1), 137–145.

You might also like