You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/242345595

Psychology Students' Knowledge and Use of Mnemonics

Article  in  Teaching of Psychology · July 2013


DOI: 10.1177/0098628313487460

CITATIONS READS

30 9,265

4 authors, including:

Jennifer Mccabe
Goucher College
29 PUBLICATIONS   576 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The SAGE Encyclopedia of Abnormal and Clinical Psychology View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jennifer Mccabe on 14 July 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Teaching of Psychology
http://top.sagepub.com/

Psychology Students' Knowledge and Use of Mnemonics


Jennifer A. McCabe, Kelsey L. Osha, Jennifer A. Roche and Jonathan A. Susser
Teaching of Psychology 2013 40: 183 originally published online 10 May 2013
DOI: 10.1177/0098628313487460

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://top.sagepub.com/content/40/3/183

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Society for the Teaching of Psychology

Additional services and information for Teaching of Psychology can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://top.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://top.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - May 28, 2013

OnlineFirst Version of Record - May 10, 2013

What is This?

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
Topical Article
Teaching of Psychology
40(3) 183-192
Psychology Students’ Knowledge ª The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
and Use of Mnemonics DOI: 10.1177/0098628313487460
top.sagepub.com

Jennifer A. McCabe1, Kelsey L. Osha1,


Jennifer A. Roche1, and Jonathan A. Susser1

Abstract
Mnemonics are strategies that can enhance learning and memory of course material. An online survey examined psychology
students’ metacognitive awareness and self-reported behaviors regarding mnemonics. Results showed that most participants
could define mnemonics, but only a minority could describe the cognitive mechanisms involved. Participants were more familiar
with some mnemonics (acronyms and acrostics) compared to others (pegword); further, the most common sources of mnemonics
were those created by the students themselves and those provided by the instructors. Usefulness of mnemonics was rated at a
moderate level compared to other common study strategies. Finally, the ratings for mnemonics were positive and correlated with
independent measures of metacognition as well as psychology course experience. Findings are discussed in the context of increasing
mnemonics in psychology education.

Keywords
mnemonic devices, metacognition, self-regulation

Mnemonics are memory strategies that help meaningfully This topic connects to the literature on metacognition (and
organize and chunk to-be-learned information; as such, they more specifically, metamemory), one aspect of which is one’s
enhance encoding and facilitate retrieval. Examples include knowledge and beliefs about strategies that maximize memory
acronyms and acrostics (i.e., first-letter mnemonics), method (i.e., learning how to learn) and also the choice to control study
of loci, pegword method, keyword method, and a category of and deploy effective strategies in real-world learning situations
semantic-based mnemonic techniques including songs, stories, (i.e., self-regulated learning; Kornell & Bjork, 2007; see
and rhymes. Mnemonics are often used in education at the ini- Sperling, Howard, Staley, & DuBois, 2004 for a review). The
tial stage of knowledge acquisition. They may act in this early current study specifically aimed to assess psychology students’
stage as scaffolding for more permanent schematic knowledge metacognition about the benefits of using mnemonics as a strat-
that develops as education advances (Bellezza, 1996). Indeed, egy to help learn and remember college-level course material
many psychology courses, especially the introductory course, as well as the extent to which they apply mnemonics during
require the mastery of an entirely new ‘‘lexicon’’ before stu- studying.
dents can progress into the study of more complex concepts There is mixed evidence for the degree to which college stu-
(Balch, 2005; Carney & Levin, 1998). dents can accurately rate the effectiveness of various study stra-
Several of the mnemonics listed previously have been tegies (Karpicke, Butler, & Roediger, 2009; McCabe, 2011).
shown to benefit both short- and long-term memory in labora- Multiple studies have shown a metacognitive disconnect
tory studies (e.g., for the keyword technique, Carney & Levin, between self-rated and actual effectiveness for several strate-
2008) and in classroom settings (e.g., Bloom & Lamkin, 2006; gies classified as ‘‘desirable difficulties’’ (i.e., those that are
Carney, Levin, & Levin, 1994; Saber & Johnson, 2008; Van- initially challenging, error prone, and lead to slower learning
Voorhis, 2002). As such, mnemonics can be a useful pedagogi- but enhance memory in the long term; Bjork, 1994). That is,
cal tool to enhance course learning. However, the extent to participants show memory benefits from these strategies on
which undergraduates know about and apply mnemonic strate-
gies when choosing how to study for their courses remains a
1
relatively unexplored area of research (Soler & Ruiz, 1996; Department of Psychology, Goucher College, Baltimore, MD, USA
Stalder, 2005). Given the empirical evidence for mnemonics’
Corresponding Author:
effectiveness in enhancing memory for the course material, it Jennifer A. McCabe, Department of Psychology, Goucher College, 1021
is important for educators to understand how psychology Dulaney Valley Road, Baltimore, MD, 21204, USA.
students perceive and use mnemonics. Email: jennifer.mccabe@goucher.edu

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
184 Teaching of Psychology 40(3)

objective measures but provide low or inconsistent ratings for Method


perceived effectiveness (e.g., for the testing effect, Kornell &
Son, 2009; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006; for the spacing effect,
Participants
Kornell & Bjork, 2008; Susser & McCabe, 2012). However, Participants were 481 undergraduates who were currently
there is at least one ‘‘desirably difficult’’ memory strategy that enrolled in, or had taken, at least one college-level psychol-
shows relatively high metacognitive awareness: the generation ogy course. They were recruited from 64 institutions via Inter-
effect, or memory benefit for self-generated compared to net postings and social networking sites to complete an online
researcher-provided materials (Begg, Vinski, Frankovich, & survey. The mean age of the participants was 22.12 (standard
Holgate, 1991; McCabe, 2011). deviation [SD] ¼ 6.49; median ¼ 20), and the mean years of
Although the use of mnemonics has not been traditionally college completed was 2.57 (SD ¼ 1.27; median ¼ 2). The
classified as a desirable difficulty, one could argue that, espe- sample was 76.1% female and had a self-reported mean grade
cially in the case of self-created mnemonics (an instance of the point average (GPA) of 3.30 (SD ¼ 0.52). Regarding institu-
generation effect, e.g. Slamecka & Graf, 1978), there is a con- tion type, 22.2% attended a community college, 23.9%
siderable expenditure of effort necessary on the front end of the attended a 4-year college, and 53.4% attended a 4-year uni-
process, which should lead to memory improvement in the long versity. To preview the results, neither institution type nor
run. Relevant to the issue of metacognitive judgments, Balch years of college completed was related to any mnemonics-
(2005) showed that although psychology students were helped relevant variables.
more by keyword mnemonics than by paraphrasing and repeti- Specific to psychology, 31% of the sample reported major-
tion strategies on objective memory tests, they reported higher ing in psychology and 69% reported a major other than psy-
helpfulness ratings for mnemonics compared to repetition but chology. The mean number of psychology courses taken was
not compared to paraphrasing. Thus, learners may not always 2.85 (SD ¼ 2.92; median ¼ 1; range: 1–16). We further split
have the metacognitive sophistication to judge the most effective the sample into those who had taken an introductory psychol-
learning methods. ogy course only (Intro; 52.1%) and those who had taken at least
Our study was designed to provide a broad view of psychol- one psychology course beyond the introductory level (Introþ;
ogy students’ knowledge and use of mnemonics. One major 47.9%).
question, relevant to the previous discussion, was whether
students would show accurate metacognitive awareness of
mnemonics (similar to findings for the generation effect) or Materials and Procedures
whether they will show little knowledge of mnemonics and fail We invited potential participants via e-mail to participate in a
to rate mnemonics as an effective strategy (similar to findings web-based survey that took approximately 15 min to complete.
for the testing and spacing effects). In the invitation e-mail, they were given a link that directed
Understanding the student perspective on learning strategies them to the consent form and then the survey items.
such as mnemonics is an important first step for educators to The survey began with questions relating to knowledge
support and encourage the use of such effective techniques. about mnemonics. First, the participants were asked, ‘‘What
Likewise, learning about students’ beliefs regarding mnemo- is a MNEMONIC (or MNEMONIC DEVICE)?’’ in a free-
nics will increase knowledge in the realm of applied metacog- response format. Then, following the presentation of a standard
nition and metamemory. To this end, we developed an online definition of mnemonics, they were asked, ‘‘Why (and/or how)
survey to address the following research questions: do mnemonics help memory?’’ again using a free-response
format.
1. Are psychology students metacognitively aware of Next, the survey presented the name, definition, and exam-
mnemonics as a learning strategy? ple for five categories of mnemonics, each followed by Likert-
2. What kinds of mnemonics are psychology students type rating scales assessing level of familiarity, level of use,
most familiar with, most frequently used, and perceived and level of perceived helpfulness for that particular mnemonic
as most helpful? (see Table l). The mnemonic types described in the survey were
3. How does the perceived usefulness of mnemonics com- acronyms and acrostics (i.e., first-letter mnemonics), method of
pare with other commonly applied study strategies? loci, pegword method, keyword method, and a category of
4. What are the most common sources of mnemonics used semantic-based mnemonic techniques that we labeled songs/
for college courses? stories/rhymes.
5. What factors contribute to the decision to use mnemo- For acronyms and acrostics, the following information was
nics when studying? provided:
6. Which areas of psychology are judged as most relevant
to the use of mnemonics? Acronym definition: An abbreviation or word formed
7. From an individual differences perspective, what are from the initial letters or the letters of the successive
the relationships of mnemonic familiarity/use with (a) or major parts of a term.
demographics variables and (b) scales measuring meta- Example: ROY G BIV for the colors of the rainbow; red,
cognitive self-regulation and elaboration strategies? orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, violet.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
McCabe et al. 185

Acrostic Definition: A poem or series of lines in which Story mnemonic definition: Write a story with the to-be-
certain letters, usually the first in each line, form a learned words highlighted.
name, motto, or message when read in sequence. Rhyme mnemonic definition: Create a rhyme or poem
Example: Every Good Boy Does Fine, for the notes on the using the to-be-learned information.
lines of the treble clef; E, G, B, D, F. Rhyme example: To remember the illfated wives of
Henry VIII:
For Keyword mnemonics, the following information was Divorced, beheaded, died,
provided: Divorced, beheaded, survived

Keyword definition: A new word/concept is associated For each mnemonic type, the level of familiarity was
with a similar sounding, familiar word (i.e., keyword). assessed on a 5-point scale, with ‘‘1’’ indicating new to me and
The meaning of the original word is linked to the ‘‘5’’ indicating very familiar. The level of use was assessed on a
keyword through an interactive mental image. 5-point scale, with ‘‘1’’ indicating never use and ‘‘5’’ indicating
Example: To remember harpaxophobia, the fear of robbers, often use. Level of perceived helpfulness was assessed on a
use the keyword harp to create a mental image of robbers 5-point scale, with ‘‘1’’ indicating not at all helpful and ‘‘5’’
stealing a harp (Carney & Levin, 2008). indicating extremely helpful.
Next, the survey listed nine commonly used study strategies,
For method of loci, the following information was provided:
one of which was use of mnemonics; for each strategy,
Method of loci definition: Material is associated with the participants rated on a 5-point scale (‘‘1’’ ¼ not at all,
memorized spatial places to establish order and help ‘‘5’’ ¼ extremely) how useful they found each strategy. Table
in recollection. 2 contains the specific strategies included in the survey.
Example: One might remember a grocery list by imagin- Participants then responded to survey items measuring
ing each food item along the route one takes to school various aspects of general mnemonics use, on a scale from
or work. One may visualize a loaf of bread on the door- 1 (never) to 5 (very often): ‘‘On average, how often do you use
step, eggs cracked on the car window, and milk spilled mnemonics while studying for your courses?’’ and ‘‘How often
on the office desk. By taking a ‘‘mental walk’’ along have you encountered mnemonics in your college courses?’’
this path one can retrieve the information. We also included the question, ‘‘To what degree does your
current use of mnemonic devices reflect the extent to which you
For the pegword mnemonic, the following information was encountered them in your college courses?’’ on a scale from
provided: 1 (not at all) to 5 (completely).
The next question set asked participants where the mnemo-
Pegword mnemonic definition: Uses rhyming words to nics they use come from (i.e., their source), on a scale from
represent numbers or order by providing visual images 1 (never) to 5 (very often). Table 3 contains the full list of the
that can be associated with facts or events. sources included.
Example: First memorize the number/word pairs, then: The final set of items regarding general mnemonics use
assessed a variety of factors that may impact the likelihood
1. gun: Visualize the first item being fired from a gun. of choosing to use mnemonics in specific academic situations,
2. zoo: Visualize an association between the second thing such as, ‘‘There is a lot of terminology to learn for a course’’
and a zoo. and ‘‘The instructor of the course uses them.’’ These were rated
3. tree: Visualize the third item growing from a tree. on a 5-point scale, with 1 indicating not likely to use mnemo-
4. door: Visualize the fourth item associated with a door. nics, 3 (i.e., the neutral value) indicating equally likely to use
5. hive: Visualize the fifth item associated with a hive or or not use mnemonics, and 5 indicating very likely to use mne-
with bees. monics. Table 4 contains the complete list of factors included in
6. bricks: Visualize the sixth item associated with bricks. the survey.
7. heaven: Visualize the seventh item associated with Specific to the discipline of psychology, we asked partici-
heaven. pants, ‘‘For which of the following psychology topics do you
8. plate: Visualize the eighth item on a plate as if it is think mnemonics would be most helpful?’’ For this item, we
food. developed a list of 17 major psychology topics derived mostly
9. wine: Visualize a glass containing the ninth item. from consultations of Introductory Psychology textbook chap-
10. hen: Visualize the tenth item associated with a chicken. ter headings (see Table 5 for topics); participants could check
as many topics as they desired in order to answer this question.
For Song/story/rhyme mnemonics, the following informa- Next, the survey presented two subscales of the Motivated
tion was provided: Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ; Pintrich,
Song mnemonic definition: Write new lyrics to a well- Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1991, 1993; see also Duncan
known tune, or write an original song using to-be- & McKeachie, 2005), with the goal of measuring the use of
learned information. deeper processing learning strategies. The two scales used were

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
186 Teaching of Psychology 40(3)

elaboration (ELAB) and metacognitive self-regulation (MSR), the level of use, and perceived helpfulness. Means and standard
which in the current study had an average a level of .75, similar deviations are presented in Table 1, which is arranged in rough
to the average a of .78 reported in in Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, order from the most familiar/used/helpful mnemonics toward
and McKeachie (1993). Eighteen items (6 from ELAB and the top of the table to the least familiar/used/helpful mnemo-
12 from MSR) were included. Two sample items from the MSR nics toward the bottom.
scale are ‘‘If course readings are difficult to understand, We computed a repeated-measures analysis of variance
I change the way I read the material.’’ ‘‘When I study for this (ANOVA) on the familiarity ratings for the five mnemonic
class, I set goals for myself in order to direct my activities in types. Results showed a significant omnibus F-test, F(4,
each study period.’’ Two sample items from the ELAB scale 1920) ¼ 192.74, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .29. Using the Bonferroni
are ‘‘I try to relate ideas in this subject to those in other courses adjustment for multiple contrasts, we found that all pairwise
whenever possible.’’ ‘‘When reading for this class, I try to contrasts were significant (all ps < .003), except the contrast
relate the material to what I already know.’’ Each item was of method of loci to pegword (p ¼ .179). That is, the most
rated using a Likert-type scale from 1 (not at all true of me) highly rated category was acronym/acrostic (M ¼ 4.43, SD
to 7 (very true of me). ¼ 0.95), followed by song/story/rhyme (M ¼ 3.95, SD ¼
The culminating section of the survey consisted of demo- 1.13), and then the keyword method (M ¼ 3.75, SD ¼
graphics items, including the specific psychology courses that 1.27). The least familiar mnemonics were method of loci (M
participants were currently enrolled in and/or had already ¼ 2.91, SD ¼ 1.43) and pegword (M ¼ 2.72, SD ¼ 1.40; see
completed, along with years of college completed, major, GPA, Table 1).
institution, age, and sex. In a parallel analysis, we compared ratings of level of use for
the mnemonics, F(4, 1920) ¼ 168.83, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .26. After
the Bonferroni adjustment, the follow-up tests showed that all
Results pairwise contrasts were significant (all ps < .01), except the
contrast of method of loci to pegword (p ¼ .152). These results
Knowledge About Mnemonics mirror those of the familiarity ratings presented above. Again,
Two of the authors coded the free-response answers to the the acronym/acrostic category was rated highest (M ¼ 3.53,
question, ‘‘What is a MNEMONIC (or MNEMONIC DEV- SD ¼ 1.11), but in this analysis the keyword mnemonic was
ICE)?’’ using two categories: (1) an inaccurate definition or rated second highest (M ¼ 3.24, SD ¼ 1.27). The pegword
an answer of ‘‘Don’t know’’; and (2) a reasonably accurate def- method was rated the lowest (M ¼ 2.03, SD ¼ 1.13; see
inition. Coder agreement was near 100%; in the few cases of Table 1).
dispute, the coders discussed the answer and came to an agree- A final repeated measures ANOVA for ratings of helpful-
ment. Using this coding scheme, 81.1% of the participants ness was also significant, F(1, 1920) ¼ 218.96, p < .001, Zp2
provided a reasonably accurate definition and 18.9% did not. ¼ .31. Follow-up tests using the Bonferroni adjustment showed
We further assessed responses to this question with regard to psy- all pairwise contrasts were significant (all ps < .001), except
chology major status and also with regard to whether a cognition- method of loci to pegword (p ¼ 1.00) and keyword to song/
related psychology course had been taken. The chi-square analysis story/rhyme (p ¼ .286). Acronym/acrostic was rated to be most
was nonsignificant for major (p ¼ .106) and also for Intro versus helpful (M ¼ 4.07, SD ¼ 0.93), followed by keyword (M ¼
Intro þ courses (p ¼ .072) but was significant for cognition course 3.60, SD ¼ 1.08) and song/story/rhyme (M ¼ 3.46, SD ¼
completion, w2(1) ¼ 15.69, p < .001. Of the 59 students who had 1.16), then method of loci (M ¼ 2.57, SD ¼ 1.25) and pegword
taken a cognition course, 100% could provide an accurate definition, (M ¼ 2.50, SD ¼ 1.16; see Table 1).
whereas only 78.4% of the 422 students who had not taken a cogni-
tion course could accurately define mnemonics.
Using a similar coding procedure as described above, we
Mnemonics in the Context of Other Study Strategies
evaluated the survey question, ‘‘Why (and/or how) do mnemo- We asked participants to rate the usefulness of nine common
nics help memory?’’ We split the responses into 2 categories: study strategies, including the use of mnemonics, in order to
(1) no cognition-relevant terminology and (2) inclusion of evaluate how mnemonics compared to the perceived usefulness
cognition terminology (e.g., encoding, chunking, storage, of other strategies. Out of the nine strategies listed, using mne-
retrieval, elaboration). Only 8.9% of the participants included monic devices was numerically ranked in seventh place (see
cognition-relevant terms in their answers. This variable was not Table 2, which is arranged with the strategies listed in order
significantly related to psychology major status, Intro versus from most to least useful). Planned comparisons of mnemonics
Introþ, or having taken a cognition course (all ps > .05). ratings to each of the other study strategies, on the 5-point
scale, showed that using mnemonic devices was rated signifi-
cantly lower compared to rereading notes, t(480) ¼ 5.32, p <
Specific Mnemonics: Familiarity, Frequency of Use, and
.001, relating material to myself, t(480) ¼ 3.63, p < .001, doing
Helpfulness practice problems, t(480) ¼ 6.67, p < .001, self-testing, t(476)
For each of the five specific mnemonic strategies, we asked ¼ 6.41, p < .001, and studying the material over multiple ses-
participants to rate on a 5-point scale the level of familiarity, sions (i.e., spacing), t(480) ¼ 5.20, p < .001. Using mnemonic

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
McCabe et al. 187

Table 1. Means (Standard Deviations) for Five Types of Mnemonics Table 2. Ratings for Usefulness of Study Strategies.
on Ratings of Familiarity, Level of Use, and Helpfulness.
Study Strategy M SD
Mnemonic Familiarity Level of Use Helpfulness
Doing practice problems 4.23a 0.89
a a a
Acronym/acrostic 4.43 (0.95) 3.53 (1.11) 4.07 (0.93) Self-testing (practicing recall) 4.20a 0.91
Keyword 3.75b (1.27) 3.24b (1.27) 3.60b (1.08) Studying the same material over multiple 4.17a 1.00
Song/story/rhyme 3.95c (1.13) 2.98c (1.23) 3.46b (1.16) sessions (instead of in one session only)
Method of loci 2.91d (1.43) 2.19d (1.29) 2.57c (1.25) Rereading class notes 4.20a 0.90
Pegword 2.72d (1.40) 2.03d (1.13) 2.50c (1.16) Relating material to myself 4.07a 1.04
Making outlines or study guides 3.96 1.12
Notes. All variables measured on a 5-point rating scale. For familiarity: 1 ¼ new Using mnemonic devices 3.87 1.01
to me; 3 ¼ slightly familiar; 5 ¼ very familiar; for Level of Use: 1 ¼ never use; 3 ¼
Making and using flashcards 3.83 1.23
sometimes use; 5 ¼ often use; for helpfulness: 1 ¼ not at all helpful; 3 ¼ moderately
helpful; 5 ¼ extremely helpful. Different superscripts denote significantly differ-
Rereading textbook 3.53a 1.12
ent means within each rating scale, all ps < .01. Acronym/acrostic and keyword Notes. All variables measured on a 5-point rating scale, where 1 ¼ not at all useful,
familiarity means were the only ones significantly correlated with grade point 3 ¼ moderately useful, 5 ¼ extremely useful. a Mean rating significantly different
average (GPA; ps < .01). from using mnemonic devices, all ps < .001. Grade point average (GPA) was not
significantly correlated with any of the study strategies. SD ¼ standard deviation.
devices was rated significantly higher only compared to
rereading text, t(480) ¼ 5.09, p < .001.
Factors Involved in the Decision to Use Mnemonics
For eight factors, we asked participants to rate on a 5-point
Ratings of General Mnemonics Use scale how each would influence their decision to use or not
For the item asking how often participants use mnemonics to use mnemonics as a study strategy (see Table 4). We com-
while studying for their courses, the mean was 3.28 (SD ¼ pared the mean of each factor to the neutral value of ‘‘3’’ using
0.91; median ¼ 3) on a 5-point scale where ‘‘3’’ corresponded a series of one-sample t tests. All eight factors were rated
to sometimes and ‘‘4’’ corresponded to often. significantly higher than ‘‘3’’ (ps < .001), indicating that all the
Participants were also asked about how often they have factors were endorsed as encouraging mnemonics usage.
encountered mnemonics in their college courses; on a 5-point The most strongly endorsed factor was the instructor of the
scale where ‘‘2’’ corresponded to rarely and ‘‘3’’ corresponded course uses them (M ¼ 4.15, SD ¼ 1.01), followed by the
to sometimes, the mean was 2.97 (SD ¼ 0.88; median ¼ 3). course material requires rote memorization (M ¼ 4.05, SD ¼
As a final item in this category, we asked ‘‘To what degree 1.06) and there is a lot of new terminology to learn for a course
does your current use of mnemonic devices reflect the extent to (M ¼ 3.73, SD ¼ 1.12).
which you encountered them in your college courses?’’ On a
5-point scale where ‘‘2’’ corresponded to a little and ‘‘3’’ Mnemonics in Relation to Specific Psychology Topics
corresponded to somewhat, the mean was 2.72 (SD ¼ 0.94;
median ¼ 3). Given that all participants were currently enrolled in, or had
Additional analyses using these survey items are presented taken, at least one college-level psychology course, we
in the Individual Differences Analyses section. assessed for which specific psychology topics they felt mnemo-
nics would be most helpful (see Table 5). The most frequently
chosen topics were memory (74.2% of participants) and biolo-
gical concepts (68.4%), followed by psychological disorders
Sources of Mnemonics (55.9%) and learning (55.1%). The least frequently chosen
We asked participants about the sources from which the mne- topics were motivation (19.3%) and positive (12.5%).
monics they use were obtained. They rated the frequency of
getting their mnemonics from each of the five sources (see
Table 3). A repeated measures ANOVA using source type as
Individual Differences Analyses
the factor and ratings as the dependent variable was significant, To examine individual differences in relation to mnemonics,
F(4, 1856) ¼ 263.30, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .36. The source with high- we conducted Pearson correlations between mnemonics-
est ratings was I create my own mnemonics (M ¼ 3.57, SD ¼ relevant survey items and (1) general demographic variables;
1.20), followed by I use mnemonics provided by instructors (2) psychology-specific demographics variables; and (3) MSR
(M ¼ 3.19, SD ¼ 1.13), then I share mnemonics with my friends and ELAB scales (MSLQ; Pintrich et al., 1991). When one
(M ¼ 2.85, SD ¼ 1.32), I use mnemonics provided in a textbook variable was dichotomous, we conducted ANOVAs. Because
(M ¼ 2.73, SD ¼ 1.13), and finally I search for mnemonics there were a large number of analyses examined, we chose a
online (M ¼ 1.56, SD ¼ 0.89). Follow-up contrasts using the more conservative a level of .01 to evaluate statistical signifi-
Bonferroni correction showed that all contrasts were significant cance. Only significant findings are presented below.
(all ps < .001), except the comparison of textbook to friend Beginning with general demographics, GPA was signifi-
sources (p ¼ 1.00). cantly correlated only with acronym and acrostic familiarity

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
188 Teaching of Psychology 40(3)

Table 3. Ratings for Source From Which Mnemonics are Obtained. Table 5. Percentage of Participants Endorsing the Helpfulness of
Mnemonics in Various Psychology Topics.
Mnemonics Source M SD
Psychology Topic % Sample
I create my own mnemonics 3.57a 1.20
I use mnemonics provided by instructors 3.19b 1.13 Memory 74.2%
I share mnemonics with my friends 2.85c 1.32 Biological concepts (e.g., brain terms) 68.4%
I use mnemonics provided in a textbook 2.73c 1.13 Psychological disorders 55.9%
I search for mnemonics online 1.56d 0.89 Learning 55.1%
History of psychology (perspectives/schools 45.5%
Notes. All variables measured on a 5-point rating scale: 1 ¼ never, 2 ¼ rarely, 3 ¼ of thought/important people)
sometimes, 4 ¼ often, 5 ¼ very often. Different superscripts denote significantly Personality 45.3%
different means (ps < .001). SD ¼ standard deviation.
Research methods 42.0%
Development 37.4%
Table 4. Means and Standard Deviations for Ratings of Factors in the Language 35.8%
Decision to Use Mnemonics. Therapy 34.5%
Sensation and perception 33.5%
Factor M SD Statistics 28.3%
Intelligence 27.7%
The instructor of the course uses them 4.15 1.01 Social 27.2%
The course material requires rote memorization 4.05 1.06 Health 25.6%
There is a lot of new terminology to learn for a course 3.81 1.17 Motivation 19.3%
There is an overall large amount of material to learn for 3.73 1.12 Positive 12.5%
a course
The upcoming exam is more difficult than an average exam 3.71 1.14
My peers are using mnemonics to study for the course 3.62 1.13
There are complicated ideas or concepts to be learned 3.63 1.18 familiarity, F(1, 464) ¼ 11.36, p ¼ .001, Zp2 ¼ .02 (psychol-
I have extra time to prepare my study materials 3.24 1.16 ogy: M ¼ 4.23, SD ¼ 0.97; nonpsychology: M ¼ 3.83, SD ¼
1.17); and frequency of using mnemonics, F(1, 464) ¼ 10.12,
Notes. Ratings on a 5-point scale, 1 ¼ not likely to use mnemonics; 3 ¼ equally
likely to use or not use mnemonics; 5 ¼ very likely to use mnemonics. All means are p ¼ .002, Zp2 ¼ .02 (psychology: M ¼ 3.46, SD ¼ 1.02; non-
significantly higher than the neutral ‘‘3’’ response, ps < .001. SD ¼ standard psychology: M ¼ 3.19, SD ¼ 0.84).
deviation. A parallel set of ANCOVAs comparing participants who
had taken only an introductory psychology course to those with
ratings, r(320) ¼ .222, p < .001, and also keyword familiarity, more advanced psychology course experience revealed that
r(320) ¼ .151, p ¼ .007. Age only showed significant correla- those who had taken more psychology courses had higher
tions with acronym and acrostic familiarity, r(444) ¼ .156, means in the following variables: acronym and acrostic famil-
p ¼ .001, but in a negative direction. Thus, older students iarity, F(1, 465) ¼ 15.60, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .03 (Intro: M ¼ 4.31,
reported lower familiarity with this mnemonic technique. SD ¼ 1.03; Introþ: M ¼ 4.58, SD ¼ 0.80); acronym and acros-
Turning now to psychology-specific variables, we chose to tic level of use, F(1, 465) ¼ 10.25, p ¼ .001, Zp2 ¼ .02 (Intro:
run Pearson partial correlations, controlling for years of college M ¼ 3.39, SD ¼ 1.10; Introþ: M ¼ 3.68, SD ¼ 1.10); acronym
completed, and we ran ANOVAs with years of college com- and acrostic helpfulness, F(1, 465) ¼ 8.29, p ¼ .004, Zp2 ¼ .02
pleted as a covariate. These statistical control methods allowed (Intro: M ¼ 3.98, SD ¼ 0.95; Introþ: M ¼ 4.18, SD ¼ 0.88);
for a cleaner assessment of the psychology-specific variables, song/story/rhyme familiarity, F(1, 465) ¼ 13.25, p < .001,
separate from the potentially confounding factor of general col- Zp2 ¼ .03 (Intro: M ¼ 3.76, SD ¼ 1.15; Introþ: M ¼ 4.17,
lege experience. We report below only the analyses that were SD ¼ 1.06); mnemonics usefulness, F(1, 465) ¼ 11.39, p ¼
significant at the .01 level after controlling for years of college .001, Zp2 ¼ .02 (Intro: M ¼ 3.76, SD ¼ 1.05; Introþ: M ¼
completed. 4.01, SD ¼ 0.96); and mnemonics frequency of use, F(1,
The number of psychology courses taken was positively cor- 465) ¼ 17.73, p < .001, Zp2 ¼ .04 (Intro: M ¼ 3.14, SD ¼
related with several items: acronym and acrostic familiarity, 0.87; Introþ: M ¼ 3.42, SD ¼ 0.94).
r(313) ¼ .197, p < .001, song/story/rhyme familiarity, r(313) In the next set of analyses, we computed correlations
¼ .195, p < .001, frequency of using mnemonics, r(313) ¼ between mnemonics variables and the MSR and ELAB scales
.190, p ¼ .001, and frequency of encountering mnemonics in (MSLQ, Pintrich et al., 1991). To preview, numerous correla-
college-level courses, r(313) ¼ .149, p ¼ .008. tions were significant, even at our more conservative a level,
Analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) comparing psychol- and all were positive.
ogy versus other majors on the mnemonics survey items, con- We first present mnemonics variables that were correlated
trolling for college years completed as a covariate revealed a with both scales: acronym and acrostic familiarity, level of use,
significant advantage of psychology majors for the following and helpfulness (all ps < .001); keyword familiarity (p ¼ .002
variables: acronym and acrostic familiarity, F(1, 464) ¼ 6.74, MSR; p < .001 for ELAB), level of use (p ¼ .004 for MSR; p <
p ¼ .010, Zp2 ¼ .01 (psychology: M ¼ 4.58, SD ¼ 0.80; non- .001 for ELAB), and helpfulness (p ¼ .001 for MSR; p < .001
psychology: M ¼ 4.35, SD ¼ 1.01); song/story/rhyme for ELAB); method of loci familiarity (all ps < .001), level of

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
McCabe et al. 189

use (p ¼ .006 for MSR; p ¼ .004 for ELAB), and helpfulness mnemonics reflects the extent to which they have encountered
(p ¼ .001 for MSR; p < .001 for ELAB); song/story/rhyme them in their college courses. These ratings show that there is
level of use (p ¼ .006 for MSR; p < .001 for ELAB); mnemo- room for improvement with regard to the presentation of mnemo-
nics level of usefulness (ps < .001); mnemonics frequency of nics within college courses (e.g., Carney, Levin, & Levin, 1994)
use (ps < .001), mnemonics frequency of encounter (ps < and also the actual study behavior of using mnemonics (e.g.,
.001); and the degree to which current use of mnemonics Shimamura, 1984).
reflects past experiences (ps < .001).
The only mnemonics-relevant survey item that uniquely 2. What kinds of mnemonics are psychology students
correlated with MSR was pegword level of use (p ¼ .001). Two most familiar with, most frequently used and perceived
survey items were correlated only with ELAB: song/story/ as most helpful?
rhyme familiarity and helpfulness (ps < .001).
When we asked participants to provide ratings of familiar-
ity, frequency of use, and perceived helpfulness for five cate-
gories of specific mnemonic techniques, the results were
Discussion quite consistent across categories (see Table 1). Clearly, the
We undertook this online survey study to examine psychology most familiar, used, and helpful technique was the category
students’ knowledge, metacognitive beliefs, and reported use of first-letter mnemonics (acronyms and acrostics). This is con-
of mnemonics in relation to their college-level courses. We dis- sistent with empirical evidence for the benefits of first-letter
cuss summaries and implications of our findings in the follow- mnemonics (Bloom & Lamkin, 2006; Lakin, Giesler, Morris,
ing section, organized by our seven research questions. & Vosmik, 2007; Stalder, 2005; Stalder & Olson, 2011).
Although these mnemonics are popular and easy to use, some
1. Are psychology students metacognitively aware of evidence suggests they may not be as useful as other, more
mnemonics as a learning strategy? effortful, mnemonic techniques (e.g., Carlson, Zimmer, &
Glover, 1981; Pressley & Mullally, 1984).
We found that the majority (81.1%) of the participants could
The keyword technique and the song/story/rhyme category
provide a reasonably accurate definition of mnemonics, when
were the next two most highly rated mnemonics. Although little
polled in a free-response format. Thus, most undergraduates
empirical research has been published on song, story, and/or
seem to have basic knowledge of mnemonics as memory aides.
rhyme mnemonics (see VanVoorhis, 2002 for research on song
Interestingly, there was a significant relationship between
mnemonics in a statistics course), there is strong evidence of the
accuracy of answering this question and whether or not partici-
effectiveness of the keyword method, as applied to psychology-
pants had completed a cognition course; every participant with
relevant materials, from studies of both shorter and longer term
cognition course experience answered accurately, compared to
memory (e.g., Balch, 2005; Carney & Levin, 1998, 2008;
78.4% of those without this experience. This may suggest that
McCabe, in preparation). Thus, it is encouraging that psychology
students are learning about mnemonics in cognition-related
students rate keyword mnemonics as among the more familiar,
courses, and/or that those students choosing to take such
used, and helpful mnemonic types. However, there is certainly
courses are particularly interested in (and knowledgeable
an opportunity to further increase student knowledge and use of
about) memory strategies.
the keyword method. Educators may consider increasing specific
In an attempt to further probe psychology students’ knowl-
instruction on this technique, particularly for learning brain termi-
edge about mnemonics, we asked them more specifically why
nology (Balch, 2005; Carney & Levin, 1998; McCabe, in
(and/or how) mnemonics benefit memory. In their answers,
preparation).
we coded for the presence or absence of cognition-relevant ter-
At the lower end of the ratings (and not significantly differ-
minology (e.g., encoding, chunking, retrieval). Only a minority
ent from each other) were the method of loci and pegword tech-
(8.9%) of participants provided answers using these types of
niques. Empirical research on the effectiveness of these
terms, and, surprisingly, this was not related to psychology-
techniques, particularly in relation to psychology courses, is
specific experience, including taking a cognition course. Thus,
sparse. Overall, these appear to be less well-known and cer-
most psychology students do not have the knowledge needed to
tainly less researched techniques.
explain specific memory mechanisms or systems involved in
the use of mnemonics. Instructors, particularly those teaching 3. How does the perceived usefulness of mnemonics com-
units on human memory, may consider these findings when pare with other commonly applied study strategies?
deciding how much to teach (and test on) conceptual explana-
tions for why mnemonics work to help memory. Out of the nine study strategies, the mean rating of use-
We also included survey questions to gain additional informa- fulness for mnemonics was numerically ranked seventh (see
tion about how psychology students rate their use and level of Table 2). Five strategies, including doing practice problems
encounter with mnemonics. Ratings from these items suggest that and self-testing, were rated significantly more useful than using
participants are only sometimes using mnemonics when studying, mnemonics. Only one strategy, rereading textbook, was sig-
only sometimes have encountered mnemonics in their courses, nificantly lower than using mnemonics. Although these data
and somewhat feel that the degree of their current use of do imply that students view several study strategies as more

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
190 Teaching of Psychology 40(3)

effective than mnemonics, it is important to note that the mean The most endorsed factor was the instructor of the course uses
for mnemonics usefulness was 3.87 on a 5-point scale, where them, which we hope provides even stronger encouragement
‘‘3’’ indicated moderately useful. Thus, psychology students for instructors to include mnemonics in their courses. Also,
overall endorsed the use of mnemonics as useful but not as rated at a relatively high level were the factors of the course
strongly as several other commonly used study strategies. material requires rote memorization and there is a lot of new
These data do not present a clear picture with respect to the terminology to learn for the course. These latter factors seem
question of a metacognitive disconnect between mnemonics’ particularly pertinent to introductory psychology courses, in
effectiveness and perceived helpfulness. Despite high awareness which students are exposed to a wealth of novel information
of what mnemonic devices are, and empirical evidence of their and concepts that set the stage for more in-depth, advanced
effectiveness, the moderate levels of perceived usefulness and learning. Thus, introductory courses may be especially appro-
actual use of these strategies are at least somewhat consistent priate targets for the integration of mnemonics.
with other research, demonstrating a metacognitive disconnect
between individuals’ knowledge and utilization of ‘‘desirably 6. Which areas of psychology are judged as most relevant
difficult’’ study strategies (e.g., Susser & McCabe, 2012; but see to the use of mnemonics?
Begg et al., 1991). Yet, further research is needed to clarify, for a
broader variety of mnemonics, whether students’ perceptions of By far, the two most frequently chosen psychology topics that
effectiveness match actual effectiveness on memory tests. would be helped by the use of mnemonics were memory and
biological concepts (e.g., brain terms), followed by psychologi-
4. What are the most common sources of mnemonics used cal disorders and learning (see Table 5). On the one hand, it is
for college courses? possible that students feel there is a great deal of new terminol-
ogy to learn in these topics, a process that could be helped by
By far, the most frequently encountered source of mnemo-
mnemonics; alternatively, students may have already encoun-
nics for college courses was the student himself or herself; that
tered mnemonics most frequently in these particular areas. The
is, participants reported creating their own mnemonics for use
latter situation seems particularly likely for brain terminology,
in studying the course material. The second most frequently
which has been a topic used for empirical research on mnemo-
encountered source of mnemonics was the instructor of the
nics (Carney & Levin, 1998) and which may be particularly
course. Lower rated sources included friends, textbooks, and
amenable to self-created and/or instructor-provided mnemonics.
online searches (see Table 3).
The top ranking of memory as a mnemonic-friendly topic may be
It is interesting and encouraging that self-created mnemonics
less a choice related to the content of typical course content
were the most frequently used source, given the evidence of a
related to memory but more a choice indicating the close link
generation effect when comparing self-generated mnemonics
of mnemonics themselves to the study of memory.
to instructor-provided mnemonics (for acrostics, see Bloom &
Lamkin, 2006; for keywords, see McCabe, in preparation).
7. From an individual differences perspective, what are the
Self-created mnemonics are also likely to be more relevant to
relationships of mnemonic familiarity/use with (a) demo-
students’ self-schemas, thus enhancing their memorability due
graphics variables and (b) scales measuring metacogni-
to the self-reference effect (e.g., Rogers, Kuiper, & Kirker,
tive self-regulation and elaboration strategies?
1977), which itself is a specific instance of the well-
established depth of processing principle in memory (e.g., Craik
In addition to describing how the entire sample of partici-
& Lockhart, 1972).
pants responded to the survey questions, we also examined the
The fact that students rated instructors as the second ranked
data from an individual differences approach. To that end, we
source for mnemonics suggests that students do acquire mne-
conducted analyses to explore the relationships of mnemonics-
monics via classroom exposure and do seem to make use of
relevant survey items with demographic variables and also
instructor-provided mnemonics when studying. Furthermore,
independently validated scales of MSR and ELAB (MSLQ;
these data support the increased inclusion of mnemonics in
Pintrich et al., 1991).
course materials and assignments. Instructor-provided mnemo-
Starting with general demographic factors, there were no
nics have the advantage of being consistently accurate and of
clear patterns of correlations, except that GPA was positively
high quality and may be especially effective when provided
correlated with familiarity of two mnemonic strategies (acro-
to students in the context of particularly difficult course mate-
nym and acrostic, keyword), and age was inversely correlated
rial for which self-developed mnemonics would be less optimal
with acronym and acrostic familiarity. We were surprised by
(Bellezza, 1996).
the lack of GPA correlations with mnemonics-relevant and
5. What factors contribute to the decision to use mnemo- study-strategy variables, given that Balch (2001) reported a
nics when studying? significant correlation between elaborative encoding strategy
use and course performance. Regarding the age correlation,
Each of the eight factors included in the survey was rated we can only speculate that older (i.e., nontraditional) students
higher than the neutral point on the scale, in the direction of would have had fewer opportunities to encounter acronyms and
increasing the likelihood of using mnemonics (see Table 4). acrostics in college-level courses.

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
McCabe et al. 191

More interestingly, we found several significant relationships evidence for the effectiveness of mnemonics and the direct
of psychology-specific demographic variables with mnemonics- relevance of mnemonics to specific topics in the field of
relevant items, even after controlling for years of college com- psychology (most notably human learning, memory, and more
pleted. Students who had taken more psychology courses were generally, cognition), psychology educators may consider
also likely to provide higher ratings of familiarity for acronym increasing instruction in mnemonics. Our results contribute to
and acrostic and song/story/rhyme mnemonics. In addition, they this endeavor by showing that psychology students tend to
tended to report higher frequency of using mnemonics and, per- know the ‘‘what’’ but not the ‘‘how’’ of mnemonics, are more
haps most tellingly, a higher frequency of encountering mnemo- familiar with some mnemonic techniques than others, rate the
nics in their college courses. Similar results were found when we usefulness of mnemonics at a moderate level compared to other
compared psychology majors to nonmajors, in that majors study strategies, and are most likely to use mnemonics that are
reported higher ratings of the above variables, with the exception self-created or provided by their instructors.
of frequency of encountering mnemonics in courses, which was Research has shown that explicit instruction in memory
not different for majors versus nonmajors. improvement techniques can improve academic performance
A few more variables were shown to be important when and alter specific study behaviors (e.g., Fleming, 2002; Tuck-
comparing participants who had taken only an introductory man, 2003). Several researcher–educators in psychology have
psychology course with those who had taken coursework endorsed the inclusion of mnemonic techniques as an integrated
beyond the introductory level. In these comparisons, students part of course instruction (e.g., Carney et al., 1994; Shimamura,
with beyond introductory psychology coursework showed 1984). More broadly, training in mnemonics has the potential to
higher ratings for acronym and acrostic familiarity, level of impact students’ lives beyond the classroom, as it is a memory
use, and helpfulness; song/story/rhyme familiarity; mnemonics skill easily transferable to daily life. In light of the results pre-
usefulness, and mnemonics frequency of use. Overall, it sented here, and prior research on the topic, efforts to improve
appears that additional psychology course experience is a pos- students’ knowledge of mnemonics would support the more
itive predictor of various aspects of mnemonics use, suggesting widespread use of mnemonics as an effective learning strategy.
that students are either learning more about mnemonics as they
advance to higher level psychology courses and/or that their Author Notes’
metacognitive sophistication (i.e., awareness and choice to use Jennifer A. Roche is now at Maastricht University, Germany. Jonathan
mnemonics) is increasing with more advanced coursework. A. Susser is now at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Por-
Turning now to the correlations of mnemonics variables with tions of this research were presented at the 18th Annual APS-STP
the MSR and ELAB scales, we predicted that participants who Teaching Institute, at the 23rd Annual Association for Psychological
rated themselves as self-aware and strategic learners on these Science Convention, Washington, DC.
scales would also be more familiar with, and more likely to use,
mnemonics in college courses. Indeed, we found numerous sig- Declaration of Conflicting Interests
nificant positive correlations between one or, in most cases, both The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
of these scales and our mnemonics variables, including ratings of the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
acronym and acrostic, keyword, method of loci, and song/story/
rhyme mnemonics. Further, both scales were correlated with Funding
general mnemonics ratings of usefulness, frequency of use, fre- The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
quency of encounter in courses, and degree to which current use ship, and/or publication of this article.
of mnemonics reflects past course experiences. These findings
demonstrate a consistent connection between the constructs of References
metacognitive self-regulation (i.e., knowing when to switch to Balch, W. R. (2001). Study tips: How helpful do introductory psychol-
a different strategy, awareness of ongoing learning success or ogy students find them? Teaching of Psychology, 28, 272–274. doi:
failure) and elaboration (i.e., use of study strategies that make 10.1207/S15328023TOP2804_09
links among different sources of information), with both specific Balch, W. R. (2005). Elaborations of introductory psychology terms:
and general ratings of mnemonics. Although necessarily relying Effects on test performance and subjective ratings. Teaching of
on self-report, we feel these are important and validating results Psychology, 32, 29–34.
to show the overlap of mnemonics knowledge and use with more Begg, I., Vinski, E., Frankovich, L., & Holgate, B. (1991). Generating
broad constructs relevant to metacognition and metamemory makes words memorable, but so does effective reading. Memory &
(see McCabe, 2011; Susser & McCabe, 2012, for similar results Cognition, 19, 487–497. doi:10.3758/BF03199571
with regard to other study strategies). Bellezza, F. S. (1996). Mnemonic methods to enhance storage and
retrieval. In E. L., Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory (pp.
345–380). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Conclusion Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the
Taken together, these online survey results provide important training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe & A. Shimamura (Eds.),
information regarding psychology students’ real-world knowl- Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185–205). Cambridge,
edge and use of mnemonics. Given the established empirical MA: MIT Press. Retrieved from http://bjorklab.psych.ucla.edu

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013
192 Teaching of Psychology 40(3)

Bloom, C. M., & Lamkin, D. M. (2006). The Olympian struggle to Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J.
remember the cranial nerves: Mnemonics and student success. (1991). A manual for the use of the Motivated Strategies for Learn-
Teaching of Psychology, 33, 128–129. Retrieved from http:// ing Questionnaire (MSLQ). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michi-
teachpsych.org/top/index.php gan, National Center for Research to Improve Postsecondary
Carlson, L., Zimmer, J., & Glover, J. (1981). First-letter mnemonics: Teaching and Learning.
Dam (Don’t Aid Memory). The Journal of General Psychology, Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993).
104, 287–292. Retrieved from PsycINFO. Reliability and predictive validity of the Motivated Strategies for
Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (1998). Coming to terms with the key- Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological
word method in introductory psychology: A neuromnemonic Measurement, 53, 801–813. doi:10.1177/0013164493053003024
example. Teaching of Psychology, 25, 132–134. doi:10.1207/ Pressley, M., & Mullally, J. (1984). Alternative research paradigms in
s15328023top2502_15 the analysis of mnemonics. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (2008). Conquering mnemonophobia, ogy, 9, 48–60. doi:10.1016/0361-476X(84)90007-9
with help from three practical measures of memory and applica- Roediger, H. L. III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning:
tion. Teaching of Psychology, 35, 176–183. doi:10.1080/ Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological
00986280802186151 Science, 17, 249–255. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x
Carney, R. N., Levin, J. R., & Levin, M. E. (1994). Enhancing the psy- Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and
chology of memory by enhancing memory of psychology. Teaching the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and
of Psychology, 21, 171–174. doi:10.1207/s15328023top2103_12 Social Psychology, 35, 677–688. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.677
Craik, F., & Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework Saber, J. L., & Johnson, R. D. (2008). Don’t throw out the baby with
for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal the bathwater: Verbal repetition, mnemonics, and active learning.
Behavior, 11, 671–684. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X Journal of Marketing Education, 30, 207–216. doi:10.1177/
Duncan, T. G., & McKeachie, W. J. (2005). The making of the moti- 0273475308324630
vated strategies for learning questionnaire. Educational Psycholo- Shimamura, A. P. (1984). A guide for teaching mnemonic skills.
gist, 40, 117–128. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4002_6 Teaching of Psychology, 11, 162–166. Retrieved from PsycINFO
Fleming, V. M. (2002). Improving students’ exam performance by Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation
introducing study strategies and goal setting. Teaching of Psychol- of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
ogy, 29, 115–119. doi:10.1207/S15328023TOP2902_07 Learning and Memory, 4, 592–604. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.592
Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A., & Roediger, H. L. III. (2009). Metacogni- Soler, M. J., & Ruiz, J. C. (1996). The spontaneous use of memory
tive strategies in student learning: Do students practise retrieval aids at different educational levels. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
when they study on their own? Memory, 17, 471–479. doi:10. 10, 41–51. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199602)10:1<41:: AID-
1080/09658210802647009 ACP361>3.0.CO;2-1
Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2007). The promise and perils of self- Sperling, R. A., Howard, B. C., Staley, R., & DuBois, N. (2004). Metacog-
regulated study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 219–224. nition and self-regulated learning constructs. Educational Research
doi:10.3758/BF03194055 and Evaluation, 10, 117–139. doi:10.1076/edre.10.2.117.27905
Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Learning concepts and categories: Stalder, D. R. (2005). Learning and motivational benefits of acronym
Is spacing the ‘‘enemy of induction’’? Psychological Science, 19, use in introductory psychology. Teaching of Psychology, 32,
585–592. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02127.x 222–228. doi:10.1207/s15328023top3204_3
Kornell, N., & Son, L. K. (2009). Learners’ choices and beliefs about self- Stalder, D. R., & Olson (2011). T for two: Using mnemonics to teach
testing. Memory, 17, 493–501. doi:10.1080/09658210902832915 statistics. Teaching of Psychology, 38, 247–250. Retrieved from
Lakin, J. L., Giesler, R. B., Morris, K. A., & Vosmik, J. R. (2007). PsycINFO
HOMER as an acronym for the scientific method. Teaching of Susser, J. A., & McCabe, J. (2013). From the lab to the dorm room:
Psychology, 34, 94–96. Retrieved from PsycINFO Metacognitive awareness and use of spaced study. Instructional
McCabe, J. (2011). Metacognitive awareness of learning strategies in Science, 41(2), 345–363. doi: 10.1007/s11251-012-9231-8
undergraduates. Memory & Cognition, 39, 462–476. doi:10.3758/ Tuckman, B. W. (2003). The effect of learning and motivation
s13421-010-0035-2 strategies training on college students’ achievement. Journal of Col-
McCabe, J. A. (in preparation). Learning the brain in Introductory lege Student Development, 44, 430–437. doi:10.1353/csd.20030034
Psychology: Examining the generation effect for mnemonics and VanVoorhis, C. R. W. (2002). Stat jingles: To sing or not to sing.
examples. Manuscript in preparation. Teaching of Psychology, 29, 249–250. Retrieved from PsycINFO

Downloaded from top.sagepub.com at Society for the Teaching of Psychology on June 20, 2013

View publication stats

You might also like