You are on page 1of 9

Chronobiology International, 28(3): 229–237, (2011)

Copyright © Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.


ISSN 0742-0528 print/1525-6073 online
DOI: 10.3109/07420528.2011.553696

The Circadian Energy Scale (CIRENS): Two Simple Questions for a Reliable
Chronotype Measurement Based on Energy

Gustavo L. Ottoni,1 Eduardo Antoniolli,1 and Diogo R. Lara2


1
Departamento de Bioquímica, Instituto de Ciências Básicas da Saúde, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre,
Brazil, 2Faculdade de Biociências, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14

This study presents the Circadian Energy Scale (CIRENS), a very short and simple chronotype measurement tool based
on energy. The CIRENS consists of two introspective questions about the usual energy level (very low, low, moderate,
high, or very high, scored 1 to 5) in the morning and in the evening. The difference between energy level scores (−4
to 4) felt by respondents in the evening and morning defines the chronotype score and classification. A concurrent
validity analysis of the CIRENS with the widely used Horne and Östberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire
(MEQ) was conducted using a sample of 225 college students, and with MSFsc, a sleep-based chronotype
assessment tool based on the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire (MCTQ), using a sample of 34,530 subjects (18–83
yrs, 27% males). This large sample was collected in a Web survey for behavioral correlates of the CIRENS with
variables previously associated with chronotype differences. The correlation of the CIRENS chronotype score was r =
−.70 with the MEQ and r = .32 with the MSFsc. CIRENS chronotype scores declined with age and were not affected
by sex. Both CIRENS and MSFsc chronotype scores were related to differences in tobacco, caffeine, and cola soft-
For personal use only.

drink consumption (all higher in evening types). The CIRENS provides a simple chronotype index and a measure of
absolute energy throughout the day and seems to be a reliable chronotype assessment tool that may be useful
both clinically and for large-scale studies. (Author correspondence: diogorlara@gmail.com)
Keywords: Chronotype, Circadian rhythm, Morningness-eveningness, Scale validation

INTRODUCTION
Recently, to enable a better understanding of the
The most widely used tool for chronotype assessment is genetic basis of the temporal organization in humans,
the Horne and Östberg Morningness-Eveningness Scale Roenneberg et al. (2003) developed the Munich Chrono-
(MEQ) (Horne & Ostberg, 1976). The main objective of type Questionnaire (MCTQ). This questionnaire enables
the MEQ is to identify how much the respondent a quantitative and a qualitative assessment of chrono-
prefers a particular time of the day to be active and not type. The quantitative evaluation focuses on sleep-time
the time that he/she actually is active. Subsequent characteristics, determination of the mid-sleep time
studies have found a skewed distribution towards morn- (midpoint between the sleep-onset and wake-up times)
ingness as a function of age (Taillard et al., 1999, 2001), as the phase reference point for sleep, and takes the influ-
which could be due an inappropriate classification. ence of social stimuli into account in the chronotype
Taillard et al. (2004) validated this scale in an older determination. In a correlation study between the MEQ
sample (30–49 yrs of age) of workers, suggesting new and the MCTQ, the mid-sleep on free days (MSF) was
cutoffs for this population. Despite its widespread use, the MCTQ sleep-time parameter with higher correlation
the MEQ has some potential limitations: the use of with the MEQ chronotype classification (r = −.73) (Zavada
hypothetical situations, comparison of subject’s prefer- et al., 2005). In a larger study, the same group demon-
ences to the habits of others, scores that need adjustment strated the need for MSF adjustment for sleep-debt
according to age groups (Taillard et al., 2004), hetero- accumulated during the workweek (MSFsc), and for age
geneity among intermediate types (Martynhak et al., and sex (MSFsasc) for a more accurate estimation and
2010), and its length (19 multiple-choice questions), standardization of chronotyping. In this same study,
that may limit its widespread application in clinical MCTQ correlations with MEQ were r = −.74 for MSF, −.66
settings and large epidemiological studies. for MSFsc, and −.59 for MSFsasc (Roenneberg et al., 2007).

Submitted September 1, 2010, Returned for revision December 14, 2010, Accepted December 31, 2010
Address correspondence to Diogo R. Lara, Faculdade de Biociências–PUCRS, Av. Ipiranga, 6681–Pd12A, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil 90619-900.
Tel.: +55 51 81219187; Fax: +55 51 33203612; E-mail: diogorlara@gmail.com


 G. L. Ottoni et al.

Morning and evening types differ in several sleep par- conformed to international ethical standards (Portaluppi
ameters. Evening types usually sleep and wake up later et al., 2010), and the study was approved by the
(Andrade et al., 1992; Ishihara et al., 1987), have more ir- Institutional Review Board of Hospital São Lucas from
regular sleeping and wakening times (Ishihara et al., Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul.
1987), and have shorter workday and longer free-day The study consisted of two different samples.
sleep durations (Andrade et al., 1992; Ishihara et al., College sample: 225 college students, 18–35 yrs old
1992; Roenneberg et al., 2007, 2003). Regarding age, (mean ± SD: 20.9 ± 2.9 yrs), with 81 (36%) being males
morning preference is more frequent up to 10–12 yrs of (20.9 ± 2.9 yrs) and 144 (64%) females (20.9 ± 2.9 yrs)
age, followed by a shift towards evening preference who answered pen-and-paper versions of the Circadian
in different cultures, which is completed around 17 and Energy Scale (CIRENS) and of the Brazilian version for
21 yrs of age, respectively, for women and men the Horne and Östberg Morningness-Eveningness
(Carskadon et al., 1993; Roenneberg et al., 2004; Tonetti Questionnaire (MEQ) (Andrade et al., 1992).
et al., 2008). In later adulthood, again a morning Internet sample: The data here presented are part of a
preference is observed (Carrier et al., 1997; Robilliard large Web-based survey to study temperament, psychia-
et al., 2002; Roenneberg et al., 2004). Especially tric disorders, and psychobiological measures. This
between adolescence and menopause, perhaps due to research Web site appeared in a newspaper article and
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14

sex hormones, women show significantly more morning- was broadcasted once on a national TV news program
ness than men (Adan & Natale, 2002; Lehnkering & in Brazil. For this study, the sample consisted of 34,530
Siegmund, 2007; Randler, 2007). Morever, consumption subjects from the general population, 18–83 yrs old
of alcohol, coffee, caffeinated soft drinks, and cigarettes (32.2 ± 11.0 yrs), with 9,388 (27.2%) being males (32.7 ±
are significantly higher in late chronotypes (Adan, 1994; 11.4 yrs) and 25,142 (72.8%) females (32.1 ± 10.8 yrs)
Taillard et al., 1999; Wittmann et al., 2006). who answered the CIRENS and questions concerning
Although chronotype refers to the sleep-wake rhythm, sleep-onset time and wake-up time; age and sex;
most studies have focused on sleep parameters, with tobacco, coffee, tea, and cola soft drinks intake; and
lower emphasis on introspective energy levels during psychiatric diagnosis by Internet (www.temperamento.
the wake phase. Moreover, very short scales to assess com.br). This sample represented 82.2% of the total
For personal use only.

the circadian rhythm are lacking and would be sample (41,970 subjects) who had correct answers for all
useful for large-scale studies (Di Milia & Bohle, 2009). eight built-in validity questions throughout the system to
In order to easily and rapidly assess an energy-based test attention (five questions asking the volunteer to mark
circadian typology in the different spans of the day, we a specific answer and one at the end asking about concen-
created the Circadian Energy Scale (CIRENS). The tration level during the tests), reliability (“how seriously
CIRENS consists of two simple questions asking respon- did you answer to these scales”), and sincerity (“how
dents to introspect about their energy levels in the sincere were you answering these scales”) of answers.
morning and in the evening, with an optional question
about how they feel in the afternoon. Respondents use Circadian Energy Scale (CIRENS)
a 5-point scale, ranging between very low (1) to very The CIRENS was applied as follows.
high (5). The difference between the scale values in the In general, how is your energy level:
morning and in the evening results in a single score cor-
responding to chronotype; negative difference values are (1) in the morning? ( ) very low ( ) low ( ) moderate ( )
defined as morning types and positive difference values high ( ) very high
evening types. This study aims to explore the data on (2) in the evening? ( ) very low ( ) low ( ) moderate ( )
the CIRENS, to perform a concurrent validity analysis high ( ) very high
with the MEQ in a sample of college students, and to
evaluate how the CIRENS chronotype score correlates Optional question:
with the following demographic and behavioral par-
ameters in a large Web-based survey: age, sex, sleep- (3) in the afternoon? ( ) very low ( ) low ( ) moderate
onset time, wake-up time, and tobacco, coffee, tea, and ( ) high ( ) very high
cola soft drink consumption.
The answers are rated from 1 (very low) to 5 (very
high). The sum of the morning and evening scores was
METHODS
considered as the total energy score, ranging from 2 to
Participants 10. With the inclusion of the optional third question,
All participants gave their informed consent before com- this score ranges from 3 to 15.
pleting the instruments. This form was elaborated to Chronotype (morningness-eveningness) classification
fulfill the requirements of the National Health Council was determined by subtracting the morning from the
of Brazil (Resolution 196/1996). Their participation evening energy score. Therefore, in the dimensional
was voluntary and could be canceled at any moment evaluation, the CIRENS chronotype score ranges from
without justification. The experimental protocol −4 (most marked morning preference) to 4 (most
Chronobiology International
CIRENS: An Energy-Based Chronotype Assessment Tool 

marked evening preference). In the categorical classifi- considered if p < .05. The SPSS 18.0 software was used
cation, subjects are considered as morning-type (≤− 2), for all analyses.
neither-type (≥− 1 and ≤1), or evening-type (≥2). These
cut-offs were chosen based on the distribution of the
RESULTS
CIRENS scores; they corresponded roughly to the lower
and higher quintiles. Energy scores for the different times of day (morning,
afternoon, evening) and the total energy scores were nor-
Behavioral Parameters mally distributed both in the college sample (Figure 1A)
The following sleep parameters were assessed with the and Internet sample (Figure 1B). Figure 2A–C show the
respective questions from the Basic Nordic Sleep Ques- normal distribution of MEQ (for the college sample)
tionnaire (Partinen & Gislason, 1995): (1) bedtime on and CIRENS chronotype scores for the college sample
workdays, (2) wake-up time on workdays, (3) sleep- and Internet sample, respectively.
onset latency on workdays, (4) bedtime on free days, The MEQ scores (sum of the score of the 19 questions)
(5) wake-up time on free days, and (6) sleep-onset and the CIRENS chronotype scores (morning minus
latency on free days. The sleep-onset time was derived night energy score) were negatively correlated, −.70 ( p
from the sum of the sleep-onset latency to the bedtime. < .001; Figure 3A); unlike the MEQ, high CIRENS chron-
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14

Total sleep duration was calculated as the interval otype scores indicate evening preference. Figure 3B
between sleep-onset time and wake-up time. From the shows the CIRENS energy scores throughout the day ac-
sleep-onset time and the wake-up time on free days, cording to the MEQ chronotypes. The MEQ morning
the mid-sleep on free days (MSF) was calculated and types evidenced their highest energy score in the
then corrected for compensatory sleep on free days morning, the score decreasing in a gradual fashion
(MSFsc), as in the MCTQ (Roenneberg et al., 2003, 2004). during the day (F = 32.1, df = 3, 190, p < .001). The oppo-
Subjects were asked about their cigarette smoking site pattern was observed among the evening types,
status (nonsmoker, currently smoker, quitter, and especially the definitely evening types (F = 18.6, df = 3,
chipper, i.e., occasional user) and their coffee, tea, and 190, p < .001). The neither types had the most stable
cola soft drinks intake pattern. According to the energy distribution throughout the day, with a slightly
For personal use only.

amount of psychostimulant intake, subjects were strati- higher energy score in the morning than in the evening
fied in categories: (i) for coffee (one regular 100 mL cup (F = 4.11, df = 3, 190, p = .007).
of brewed or instant coffee as used in Brazil, or one
espresso): none, low (≤100 mL/day), moderate (>100– Behavioral Correlates
300 mL/day), high (>300–500 mL/day), and very high We investigated if the CIRENS was associated with some
(>500 mL/day); (ii) for tea: none, low (≤0.5 L/wk), mod- variables in which differences between chronotypes have
erate (>0.5–2 L/wk), high (>2–5 L/wk), and very high been reported.
(>5 L/wk); and (iii) for cola soft drinks: none, very low Differently from the MEQ and other chronotype
(≤1 L/wk), low (>1–2 L/wk), moderate (>2–4 L/wk), assessment questionnaires (e.g., MCTQ), the CIRENS
high (>4–10 L/wk), and very high (>10 L/wk). does not specifically assess sleep parameters, but only
Subjects from the Internet sample were also ques- energy levels. However, subjects with low CIRENS chron-
tioned about having ever received a psychiatric diagnosis otype scores (morning preference) slept and woke up 2 to
by a mental health professional, current intake of psycho- 3 h earlier than those with high scores (evening prefer-
tropic medication, educational level, marital status, and ence) (data for work and free days shown as means and
personal income, since these also potentially influence 95% confidence intervals [CIs] in Figure 4; p < .001 for
chronotype. all). As expected, compared to workdays, on free days
subjects reported falling asleep later (mean ± SD: 0.89 ±
Statistical Analysis 1.14 h) and waking-up later (1.89 ± 1.70 h); therefore,
The concurrent validity analysis between CIRENS and their sleep duration was longer (1.0 ± 1.6 h) on free
MEQ was accomplished using the Pearson correlation days. The CIRENS chronotype score was also correlated
coefficient. Comparisons of CIRENS scores with demo- with mid-sleep time on free days (MSF, r = .43, p < .001)
graphic (age and sex) and behavioral parameters (sleep, and with the MSF corrected for compensatory sleep on
substance use) were performed with multivariate analy- free days (MSFsc, r = .32, p < .001). There were no signifi-
sis of variance with Bonferroni confidence interval ad- cant differences in sleep duration between CIRENS
justment. When applicable, the following parameters chronotypes, either on work or free days ( p > .05 for all),
were used as covariates in multivariate analyses: age, as shown in Figure 4.
sex, educational level, personal income, marital status, Morning energy level increased (Figure 5A) and
coffee, tea, and cola soft drinks intake, smoking status, evening level decreased with age (Figure 5C), whereas
psychiatric diagnosis (yes/no), and psychotropic drug afternoon and whole-day energy levels were not signifi-
intake (yes/no). To reach statistical significance, the cantly affected by age (Figure 5B and D). Regarding
criterion probability for a Type I error was 5% ( p < .05) chronotypes, younger subjects (18–19 yrs old) had
in every analysis. Thus, statistical significance was the strongest evening preference, whereas morning
© Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.
 G. L. Ottoni et al.
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14

FIGURE 1. Frequency distribution of the CIRENS energy score for the different times-of-day and of the CIRENS total energy score in the
college (A) and in the Internet (B) samples. The energy scores range from 1 (very low) to 5 (very high). The sum of the morning and
For personal use only.

evening scores corresponds to the total energy score (from 2 to 10).

preference increased steadily up to 70 yrs of age and relationship between circadian preference and tea con-
older (Figure 5E). For all times of the day, the energy sumption with both the CIRENS and MSFsc scores,
score was significantly higher for males than females except for the latter MSFsc chronotype when comparing
(morning: F = 78.45; afternoon: F = 315.86; evening: the very high consumers with the nonconsumers. The
F = 176.24; whole day: F = 467.15; df = 1, 34518, and most striking differences were found among cola soft
p < .001 for all). The influence of sex on dimensional drinks consumers, with both circadian scores increasing
chronotype was not significant ( p > .05; Figure 5E). The in a gradual fashion from the no/very low to very high
correlation between the sum of morning and evening drinkers.
scores (total energy score using only two questions)
and the sum of the three times of the day scorings was
DISCUSSION
strong (r = .89, p < .001).
Figure 6 presents the association of the CIRENS and In contrast to other scales, the CIRENS assesses chrono-
MSFsc chronotype scores with cigarette smoking type considering energy, without taking into account
(Figure 6A) and with intake of coffee (Figure 6B), tea sleep parameters. Despite this difference, the CIRENS
(Figure 6C), and cola soft drinks (Figure 6D). Nonsmo- and MEQ chronotype scores were correlated (r = −.70),
kers had significantly lower CIRENS and MSFsc scores with the evening chronotypes as identified by the
(morning preference), whereas current smokers had sig- CIRENS having much later bedtime and wake-up times
nificantly higher scores (evening preference). Quitters than morning-types. The results pertaining to the
and occasional smokers (chippers) had intermediate CIRENS energy score for different MEQ chronotypes
CIRENS scores, significantly higher than nonsmokers (Figure 3B) are similar to the findings of Natale and
and lower than smokers, although they were not statisti- Cicogna (2002), who showed that subjective alertness
cally different. The differences of the MSFsc scores ac- throughout the day varies substantially according to
cording to smoking status were more robust, with MEQ chronotype. Moreover, despite a lower correlation
chippers having similar scores as current smokers. Re- between the CIRENS and MSFsc chronotype scores
garding coffee, the nondrinkers showed an intermediate (.32), they related similarly to the behavioral parameters
CIRENS score between low and very high consumers. evaluated (Figure 6). Thus, the CIRENS chronotype score
Among coffee consumers, the CIRENS score increased seems to reflect circadian preference and sleep habits.
(from morning to evening preference) in a gradual Factor analysis has identified that MEQ is constituted
fashion from low- to very-high-dose use. This trend was by two factors related to sleep parameters and one to
weaker with MSFsc scores. In contrast, there was no time of greatest efficiency (Adan & Natale, 2002),
Chronobiology International
CIRENS: An Energy-Based Chronotype Assessment Tool 
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14
For personal use only.

FIGURE 2. Frequency distribution of the MEQ dimensional scores and chronotypes in the college sample (A) and frequency distribution of
the CIRENS dimensional scores and chronotypes in the college (B) and in the Internet (C) sample. The MEQ score ranges from 16 to 86.
MEQ chronotype classification: evening-type (16–41), neither-type (42–58), and morning-type (59–86). The CIRENS chronotype score
ranges from −4 to 4. CIRENS chronotype classification: morning-type (−4, −3, and −2), neither-type (−1, 0, 1), and evening-type (2, 3,
and 4).

FIGURE 3. Correlation between the CIRENS chronotype score and the MEQ score (A) and CIRENS energy scores according to MEQ chron-
otypes (B). (A) The CIRENS chronotype score ranges from −4 to 4 (extremely morning to extremely evening), and the MEQ score ranges from
16 to 86 (extremely evening to extremely morning). (B) The CIRENS energy score ranges from 1 (very low) to 5 (very high). Results are shown
as means and 95% confidence intervals of CIRENS energy scores. MEQ chronotypes are definitely morning type (DM; 70–86), moderately
morning type (MM; 59–69), neither type (N; 42–58), moderately evening type (ME; 31-41), and definitely evening type (DE; 16-30). DM and
MM types were merged because only one subject was DM type.

© Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.


 G. L. Ottoni et al.
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14

FIGURE 4. Association of the CIRENS chronotype score with sleep-onset time and wake-up time both on work days and on free days. The
CIRENS chronotype score ranges from −4 to 4 (extremely morning to extremely evening). Results are shown as means and 95% confidence
intervals. Some 95% confidence interval bars are not visible because they are smaller than the symbol representing the mean. Sleep-onset
time and wake-up time are presented as clock time.
For personal use only.

FIGURE 5. Association of morning (A), afternoon (B), evening (C), total (D), and circadian (E) energy scores for males and females with age.
The morning, afternoon, and evening scores range from 1 (very low) to 5 (very high); the total energy score ranges from 2 to 10; the circadian
energy score ranges from −4 to 4 (extremely morning to extremely evening). Results are shown as means. The sample consisted of 9388 males
and 25,142 females. Age is presented in yrs.

Chronobiology International
CIRENS: An Energy-Based Chronotype Assessment Tool 

similar to our energy concept. A dimensional chronotype but possibly the perception of someone’s energy level
assessment focused on the sleep factors is provided by is the same on work and free days, with different ways
the MSFsc of the MCTQ, which shows correlations of to adapt depending on the context.
−.66 with the MEQ score (Roenneberg et al., 2007) and Energy score was age dependent in the morning and
.32 with the CIRENS score. The lower correlation in the evening, but not in the afternoon and throughout
between the CIRENS and MSFsc suggests that they do the day. Since the CIRENS chronotype score considers
not assess the same circadian characteristics. Thus, the difference between evening and morning energy, it
some individuals who go to bed and wake up late may was also dependent on age. As observed with the MEQ
report morning preference in terms of energy, although (Carrier et al., 1997; Ishihara et al., 1992) and MCTQ
usually this is not the case. More importantly, intermedi- (Roenneberg et al., 2004, 2007), the CIRENS chronotype
ate types regarding sleep parameters may show a clear detected the same pattern of progressively higher
energetic preference for a specific time of the day. morning preference from late adolescence to late adult-
However, despite this lower correlation between the hood. Interestingly, men showed higher energy scores
CIRENS and MSFsc chronotype scores, which is exclu- than women at all times of day and, therefore, total
sively based on sleep, they seem to relate similarly to energy score, especially before 40–45 yrs of age. Although
the behavioral characteristics evaluated, especially cola not addressed in this study, this difference in energy
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14

drink intake (Figure 6). levels may be associated with more inhibited tempera-
As expected, subjects slept longer on free days than on ments and higher risk of depression and anxiety in
workdays, particularly because of later wake-up times. women (McLean & Anderson, 2009). The CIRENS chron-
Previous studies have reported the increased sleep on otypes showed no relevant sex differences, in contrast to
free days compared to workdays to be more evident in the commonly reported morning preference for women
evening types (Andrade et al., 1992; Roenneberg et al., using measures heavily based on sleep parameters,
2003, 2007). This was not observed with the CIRENS, especially between puberty and menopause (Adan &
which does not address work and free days separately, Natale, 2002; Lehnkering & Siegmund, 2007; Randler,
For personal use only.

FIGURE 6. Association of the CIRENS and MSFsc chronotype scores with cigarette smoking status (A), and consumption of coffee (B), tea
(C), and cola soft drinks (D). The CIRENS chronotype score ranges from −4 to 4 (extremely morning to extremely evening), and the MSFsc
score is shown as clock time. Results are shown as means and 95% confidence intervals.

© Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.


 G. L. Ottoni et al.

2007; Roenneberg et al., 2007). However, our results on studies (e.g., genetics), the design or analysis should take
absolute energy scores also point to a similar energy into account that age may weakly affect the results, since
level between males and females from 40–50 yrs of age the mean variation was around 1 point (out of 9 points)
onwards. between 18 and 60 yrs of age. Age-adjustments are more
Regarding the association of psychostimulant con- critical for the MEQ, also limiting its use in large-scale
sumption and chronotype, higher use was related to studies. As limitations, the CIRENS relies on a subjective
more evening preference, except for tea consumption. perception rather than more objective measures (such as
These results are in agreement with data on the MCTQ time), and the CIRENS has a limited score range com-
by Wittman et al. (2006), who reported a positive corre- pared to other scales, which makes it less discriminative
lation between both cigarette smoking and intake of for small variations and changes. Of note, unlike other
caffeinated drinks and evening preference and lack of scales, sex is not a relevant variable for chronotype
correlation between tea intake and chronotype. For measurement with the CIRENS.
coffee intake, they reported a small negative association Some limitations of our study warrant consideration.
with evening preference, but we found a nonlinear First, the correlation of the CIRENS with the MEQ is
relationship between coffee and chronotype: low coffee based on a relatively small sample of college students,
intake was associated with more morning preference with a narrow age range and probably healthier profile
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14

than no intake, but among coffee users, evening prefer- than the general population. Second, an independent
ence increased with higher doses. Accordingly, Adan sample collected by the Internet was used to evaluate
(1994) reported a positive association between evening demographic and behavioral correlates of the CIRENS
type and nicotine, coffee, and cola consumption. The scores.
most striking result in our study was the association In conclusion, the CIRENS is an introspective, energy-
with cola soft drinks, with an almost 1-point difference based chronotype assessment tool correlated with the
between intake extremes. This magnitude is similar to MEQ, but without the need for several adjustments
the chronotype change from 18 to 40 yrs of age. One required for sleep-based chronotype measures. CIRENS
simple interpretation of these results is that evening chronotype scores were clearly related to sleep par-
types consume more caffeine to improve their adaptation ameters and other variables classically linked to chrono-
For personal use only.

during the day. However, this would not explain why the type. Thus, the CIRENS is a simple, very short, and
association was stronger with cola drinks, which contain reliable tool to study the role of circadian energy and
less caffeine than coffee (Barone & Roberts, 1996), and is absolute energy levels as important parameters of the
not readily applicable for the cigarette data. An alterna- temporal organization in human behavior. This profile
tive explanation is that evening types have a personality makes it particularly suitable for use in large-scale
profile that is more associated with drug use, in studies and clinical settings. Further studies are war-
general, due to a pattern of more immediate gratification. ranted to confirm the validity and utility of CIRENS.
Indeed, evening-type subjects have higher novelty-
seeking, lower harm avoidance, persistence, and self-
directedness according to Cloninger’s psychobiological ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
model (Adan et al., 2010), and higher conscientiousness This study was supported by the Conselho Nacional de
(self-discipline and deliberation) than morning types Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) and
(Tonetti et al., 2009). by FINEP research grant “Rede Instituto Brasileiro de
Compared to other chronotype scales, the CIRENS has Neurociência (IBN-Net)” no. 01.06.0842-00. PRONEX-
advantages and limitations. The main advantage is that FAPERGS. CNPq provided scholarships for D.R.L.
only two simple questions (the morning and evening
questions) and the derived chronotype index were
clearly sufficient to identify chronotypes both dimension- Declaration of Interest: The authors report no conflicts
ally and categorically. Also, their sum provides an index of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the
of total energy level, which is not provided by other content and writing of the paper.
scales. Being so short, this is particularly useful for
large-scale studies that evaluate many variables simul- REFERENCES
taneously. In comparison, the MEQ has 19 questions
Adan A. (1994). Chronotype and personality factors in the daily con-
and does not provide an absolute measure of energy
sumption of alcohol and psychostimulants. Addiction 89:455–462.
or efficiency throughout the day. Other advantages of Adan A, Natale V. (2002). Gender differences in morningness-evening-
approaching chronotype from a subjective energy ness preference. Chronobiol. Int. 19:709–720.
perspective are that adjustments for being employed, Adan A, Lachica J, Caci H, Natale V. (2010). Circadian typology and
work/free days, and other daily variations, as well as job temperament and character personality dimensions. Chronobiol.
Int. 27:181–193.
type (e.g., shiftwork), child care, and routine participation
Andrade MM, Benedito-Silva AA, Menna-Barreto L. (1992). Corre-
in morning activities during the weekend (e.g., religious lations between morningness-eveningness character, sleep habits
meetings), and culture-related variations, which heavily and temperature rhythm in adolescents. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res.
influence sleep parameters, are not necessary. For some 25:835–839.

Chronobiology International
CIRENS: An Energy-Based Chronotype Assessment Tool 

Barone JJ, Roberts HR. (1996). Caffeine consumption. Food Chem. Robilliard DL, Archer SN, Arendt J, Lockley SW, Hack LM, English J,
Toxicol. 34:119–129. Leger D, Smits MG, Williams A, Skene DJ, Von Schantz M.
Carrier J, Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ. (1997). Sleep and morning- (2002). The 3111 Clock gene polymorphism is not associated with
ness–eveningness in the ‘middle’ years of life (20–59 y). J. Sleep. Res. sleep and circadian rhythmicity in phenotypically characterized
6:230–237. human subjects. J. Sleep Res. 11:305–312.
Carskadon MA, Vieira C, Acebo C. (1993). Association between puberty Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. (2003). Life between clocks—
and delayed phase preference. Sleep 16:258–262. daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J. Biol. Rhythms
Di Milia L, Bohle P. (2009). Morningness or morning affect? A short 18:80–90.
composite scale of morningness. Chronobiol. Int. 26:494–509. Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller PP, Ricken J, Havel M, Guth A,
Horne JA, Östberg O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to Merrow M. (2004). A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr. Biol.
determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. 14:1038–1039.
Int. J. Chronobiol. 4:97–110. Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K,
Ishihara K, Miyasita A, Inugami M, Fukuda K, Miyata Y. (1987). Differ- Gordijn M, Merrow M. (2007). Epidemiology of human circadian
ences in sleep-wake habits and EEG sleep variables between clock. Sleep Med. Rev. 11:429–438.
active morning and evening subjects. Sleep 10:330–342. Taillard J, Philip P, Bioulac B. (1999). Morningness/eveningness and
Ishihara K, Miyake S, Miyasita A, Miyata Y. (1992). Morningness- the need for sleep. J. Sleep Res. 8:291–295.
eveningness preference and sleep habits in Japanese office
Taillard J, Philip P, Chastang JF, Diefenbach K, Bioulac B. (2001). Is
workers of different ages. Chronobiologia 19:9–16.
self-reported morbidity related to the circadian clock? J. Biol.
Lehnkering H, Siegmund R. (2007). Influence of chronotype, season,
Rhythms 16:183–190.
Chronobiol Int Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Michigan University on 11/03/14

and sex of subject on sleep behavior of young adults. Chronobiol.


Taillard J, Philip P, Chastang JF, Bioulac B. (2004). Validation of
Int. 24:875–888.
Horne and Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire in
Martynhak BJ, Louzada FM, Pedrazzoli M, Araujo JF. (2010). Does
a middle-aged population of French workers. J. Biol. Rhythms
the chronotype classification need to be updated? Preliminary
19:76–86.
findings. Chronobiol. Int. 27:1329–1334.
Tonetti L, Fabbri M, Natale V. (2008). Sex difference in sleep-time
McLean CP, Anderson ER. (2009). Brave men and timid women?
preference and sleep need: a cross-sectional survey among
A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clin.
Psychol. Rev. 29:496–505. Italian pre-adolescents, adolescents, and adults. Chronobiol. Int.
Natale N, Cicogna P. (2002). Morningness-eveningness dimension: is it 25:745–759.
really a continuum? Pers. Individ. Differ. 32:809–816 Tonetti L, Fabbri M, Natale V. (2009). Relationship between circadian
Partinen M, Gislason T. (1995). Basic Nordic Sleep Questionnaire typology and big five personality domains. Chronobiol Int.
(BNSQ): a quantitated measure of subjective sleep complaints. 26:337–347.
For personal use only.

J. Sleep Res. 4:150–155. Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T. (2006). Social jetlag:
Portaluppi F, Smolensky MH, Touitou Y. (2010). Ethics and methods misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol. Int.
for animal and human biological rhythm research. Chronobiol. 23:497–509.
Int. 27:1911–1929. Zavada A, Gordijn MC, Beersma DG, Daan S, Roenneberg T. (2005).
Randler C. (2007). Gender differences in morningness-eveningness as- Comparison of the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire with the
sessed by self-report questionnaires: a meta-analysis. Pers. Individ. Horne-Östberg’s Morningness-Eveningness Score. Chronobiol.
Differ. 43:1667–1675. Int. 22:267–278.

© Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.

You might also like