You are on page 1of 182

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/266864498

Is Congruence Dead? An Examination of the Correlation Between Holland's


Congruence and Job Satisfaction Using Improved Methodology

Article

CITATIONS READS

7 306

11 authors, including:

Shannon Bowles Roy Tunick


University of Georgia West Virginia University
2 PUBLICATIONS   39 CITATIONS    34 PUBLICATIONS   232 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Donald Eggerth
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
48 PUBLICATIONS   1,274 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

using mmpi-2 and pictogram in usa and russia differentiating schizophrenia and normals View project

exploring Holland codes and O Net compatibility View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Roy Tunick on 28 April 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Is Congruence Dead? An Examination of the Correlation Between
Holland’s Congruence and Job Satisfaction Using Improved Methodology

Shannon Marie Bowles

Dissertation submitted to the


College of Human Resources and Education
at West Virginia University
in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy
In
Counseling Psychology

Roy H. Tunick, Ed.D., Committee Chair


Jennifer R. Adams, Ph.D.
Donald E. Eggerth, Ph.D.
Ed E. Jacobs, Ph.D.
Richard T. Walls, Ph.D.

Department of Counseling, Rehabilitation Counseling, and Counseling Psychology

Morgantown, West Virginia


2008

Keywords: Congruence, Job Satisfaction, John L. Holland, Methodology, RIASEC

Copyright 2008 Shannon M. Bowles


ABSTRACT

Is Congruence Dead? An Examination of the Correlation Between


Holland’s Congruence and Job Satisfaction Using Improved Methodology

Shannon Marie Bowles

Job satisfaction is considered to be the most important and frequently examined outcome
variable in congruence research. The assumption that congruence results in job satisfaction is
fundamental to Holland’s theory; however, the empirical evidence predominantly indicates that
the congruence – job satisfaction correlation is, at best, equivocal. There is a movement within
vocational psychology that the congruence construct is “dead” as a viable theoretical concept and
that the field should abandon Holland’s theory. This idea does not exist without significant
argument, however.

It has been recognized widely that congruence has not fared better in the empirical research
literature because methodological limitations have led to unimpressive and ambiguous findings.
This study addressed the common methodological problems associated with congruence research
and provided a rationale for identifying and comparing measurement alternatives. This study
compared two methods for generating Holland summary codes and two indexes for calculating
congruence and investigated three aspects of job satisfaction.

Significant findings included support for a weak correlation between congruence and overall job
satisfaction and a weak correlation between congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction; however,
none of the significant relationships found in this study were in the predicted direction. It is
discussed that the congruence construct should not be considered as dead, but instead should be
reconceptualized if it is to remain a viable concept in the research and practice of vocational
psychology. It is suggested here that contemporary views of congruence should reflect the social
and economic shifts that have influenced the changing nature of the meaning of work.
Is Congruence Dead? iii

What doesn't kill us makes us stronger.

~ Friedrich Nietzsche

Many, many times over the past eight years, I have wondered whether

my name had been changed to Sisyphus, and no one let me in on the

ruse. If Nietzsche’s adage is true, by now I should be the greatest

superhero or Greek mythological character the world ever has known. I

have not yet discovered the full extent of my special powers. I have

learned, though, that I don’t have the ability to shimmy down to the

ground from a second-story balcony, but I can develop influenza and

bronchitis at precisely the worst moment in time.


Is Congruence Dead? iv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Dr. Ed E. Jacobs
As you are the Coordinator of the Counselor Education program, you had virtually no contact
with me and had no firsthand knowledge of my academic work. When changes in the Counseling
Psychology program necessitated that I reconstitute my dissertation committee, you readily
agreed to assume a vacancy with no questions asked. I greatly appreciate your apparent faith in
me and willingness to help me at a time when I most needed it.

Dr. Jennifer R. Adams


You also had no idea of me and agreed to assume a place on my committee, and for that, I am
grateful also for your leap of faith. Having also previously not experienced my academic work,
you had no way of knowing of my bad habit of “overwriting.” What I thank you for most is your
guidance in editing and in the procedures of scientific writing and your implicit permission to not
feel compelled to explain everything in minute detail. It has been a challenge for me but also a
very valuable learning experience.

Dr. Richard T. Walls


My professional preference for the practice of psychotherapy also means that statistics are not an
area of strength for me. Thank you for your guidance in helping me to determine how to translate
my research questions into testable procedures. I also appreciate that, in general, you are a very
kind person. Your words of encouragement have meant much to me.

Dr. Roy H. Tunick


I suppose that I am considered to be a statistical outlier in terms of the number of years it has
taken me to complete my degree. As my advisor, thank you for not giving up on me a long time
ago. Your advocacy and support for me in the program made it possible for me to finally
complete my degree and get about the business of being a psychologist.

Dr. Donald E. Eggerth


For reasons that still are unknown to me, you decided to “take me under your wing” very early
on in my doctoral career. I believe it was my very first semester, in fact. Since that time, your
personal and professional mentorship have been invaluable, and I would not have made it
through this process without your support and understanding. Thank you for your patience and
sense of humor.

Dr. Neil R. Snyder


You know better than anyone that, in this process, I quite literally almost died. In the past 2 ½
years, I have been knocked down seemingly at every turn. You always were there to pick me up.
You have selflessly given of your strength, energy, and encouragement. You believed in me
when I did not believe in myself, and I do credit you with saving my life. I am honored to call
you a mentor and a friend.
Is Congruence Dead? v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Abstract ii

Dedication iii

Acknowledgments iv

Table of Contents v

List of Tables xvii

List of Figures xix

CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION 1

Introduction to Chapter One 1

From Parsons to Person–Environment Fit 1

Holland’s Theory of Vocational Personality Types and Model Work Environments 3

Primary Assumptions of Holland’s Theory 3

Primary Assumption One: Vocational Personality Types 3

Primary Assumption Two: Model Work Environments 4

Primary Assumption Three: Person-Environment Fit 4

Primary Assumption Four: Outcomes of Person - Environment Fit 4

Rationale for the Research Study 5

Statement of the Research Problem 6

Purpose of the Research Study 7

Sampling Issues 7

Assessment of Vocational Personality 7

Assessment of Work Environment 8

Calculation of Congruence 8
Is Congruence Dead? vi

Job Satisfaction Measurement 9

General Research Questions 9

General Research Question Set 1: Measuring Congruence – Job Satisfaction


Correlations with Traditionally-Derived Codes and the Modified C Index 10

Question 1a 10

Question 1b 10

Question 1c 10

General Research Question Set 2: Measuring Congruence – Job Satisfaction


Correlations with Traditionally-Derived Codes and the Substitution C Index 10

Question 2a 10

Question 2b 11

Question 2c 11

General Research Question Set 3: Measuring Congruence – Job Satisfaction


Correlations with Decision-Derived Codes and the Modified C Index 11

Question 3a 11

Question 3b 11

Question 3c 11

General Research Question Set 4: Measuring Congruence – Job Satisfaction


Correlations with Decision-Derived Codes and the Substitution C Index 12

Question 4a 12

Question 4b 12

Question 4c 12

General Research Question Set 5: Comparing Job Satisfaction Correlation Values 12

Question 5a 12

Question 5b 12
Is Congruence Dead? vii

Question 5c 13

General Research Question 6: Comparing the Overall Job Satisfaction Correlation


to Established Meta-Analytic Values 13

Summary of Chapter One 13

CHAPTER TWO: LITERATURE REVIEW 15

Introduction to Chapter Two 15

Sampling Issues 16

College and University Student Samples 16

Single Occupation Samples 17

Vocational Personality 17

Vocational Personality: Types 17

Realistic Vocational Personality Type 17

Investigative Vocational Personality Type 18

Artistic Vocational Personality Type 18

Social Vocational Personality Type 18

Enterprising Vocational Personality Type 18

Conventional Vocational Personality Type 19

Vocational Personality: Instruments 19

The Self-Directed Search 19

The Vocational Preference Inventory 19

The Strong Interest Inventory 20

Vocational Personality: Limitations 20

Self-Directed Search 20

Construct Validity 20
Is Congruence Dead? viii

Scoring 21

Vocational Preference Inventory 23

Strong Interest Inventory 23

Scoring Options and Cost 23

Administration Time 24

Vocational Personality: Alternatives 24

The Strong Interest Explorer 24

Decision-Derived Rules for Assigning Holland Summary Codes. 25

Work Environment 25

Model Work Environment: Types 25

Realistic Model Work Environment 26

Investigative Model Work Environment 26

Artistic Model Work Environment 26

Social Model Work Environment 26

Enterprising Model Work Environment 27

Conventional Model Work Environment 27

Work Environment: Instruments 27

The Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes 27

The Occupations Finder 28

The Environmental Assessment Technique 28

The Position Classification Inventory 28

The Occupational Information Network 29

Work Environment: Limitations 30


Is Congruence Dead? ix

The Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes and The Occupations Finder 30

The Environmental Assessment Technique 30

The Position Classification Inventory 31

The Occupational Information Network 31

Compatibility of Holland Code Classification Systems 32

Work Environment: Instrument Alternatives 32

Strong Interest Inventory General Occupational Theme Codes 32

Measurement of Congruence 33

Secondary Assumptions of Holland’s Theory 33

The Hexagonal Model of RIASEC Types 33

Consistency 35

Measurement of Congruence: Congruence Indexes 36

The First Letter Hexagonal Distance Index 37

The Compatibility Index 37

The K-P Index 38

The M Index 38

The Sb Index 39

The C Index 39

Measurement of Congruence: Congruence Index Limitations 40

Failure to Incorporate the Circumplex Assumption 41

Limitation to Three-Letter Holland Summary Codes 41

Measurement of Congruence: Congruence Index Alternatives 41

The Modified C Index 41


Is Congruence Dead? x

Support for the Modified C Index 42

The Substitution C Index 43

Support for the Substitution C Index 43

Job Satisfaction 43

Job Satisfaction: Instruments 44

The Hoppock Job Satisfaction Blank 44

The Job Descriptive Index and the Job in General Scale 44

The Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire 45

One-Item Measures of Overall Job Satisfaction 45

Measures Developed by the Research Authors 46

Measurement of Job Satisfaction: Instrument Limitations 46

Measures of Unknown Reliability and Validity 46

One-Item Measures 47

Measurement of Overall Job Satisfaction 47

Measurement of Job Satisfaction Instrument Alternative 47

Short-Form Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire 47

Summary of Chapter Two 48

CHAPTER THREE: METHODOLOGY 50

Introduction to Chapter Three 50

General Sample Characteristics 50

Measures 50

Strong Interest Explorer 50

The Short-Form Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire 52


Is Congruence Dead? xi

Participant Recruitment 53

Data Evaluation 54

Instrument Scoring 54

Statistical Analysis of Data 59

Demographic Analysis 59

MSQ Score Analysis 59

Congruence Score Analysis 59

General Research Question Analysis 60

Summary of Chapter Three 60

CHAPTER FOUR: DATA ANALYSIS 61

Introduction to Chapter Four 61

Demographic analysis 61

Demographic Analysis for All Data 61

Demographic Analysis by Occupational Group 62

Demographic Analysis by Primary Holland Type 63

MSQ Score Analysis 65

MSQ Score Analysis for All Data 65

MSQ Score Analysis by Occupational Group 66

MSQ Score Analysis by Primary Holland Type 66

Congruence Score Analysis 68

Congruence Score Analysis for All Data 68

Congruence Score Analysis by Occupational Group 69

Congruence Score Analysis by Primary Holland Type 70


Is Congruence Dead? xii

General Research Question Analysis 72

Question 1a 72

All Data 73

Occupational Groups 73

Primary Holland Type 73

Question 1b 74

All Data 74

Occupational Groups 74

Primary Holland Type 74

Question 1c 76

All Data 76

Occupational Groups 77

Primary Holland Type 77

Question 2a 77

All Data 77

Occupational Groups 78

Primary Holland Type 78

Question 2b 78

All Data 78

Occupational Groups 79

Primary Holland Type 79

Question 2c 79

All Data 79
Is Congruence Dead? xiii

Occupational Groups 79

Primary Holland Type 80

Question 3a 80

All Data 80

Occupational Groups 80

Primary Holland Type 81

Question 3b 81

All Data 81

Occupational Groups 81

Primary Holland Type 81

Question 3c 82

All Data 82

Occupational Groups 82

Primary Holland Type 82

Question 4a 82

All Data 83

Occupational Groups 83

Primary Holland Type 83

Question 4b 83

All Data 83

Occupational Groups 84

Primary Holland Type 84

Question 4c 84
Is Congruence Dead? xiv

All Data 84

Occupational Groups 86

Primary Holland Type 86

Question 5a 86

All Data 86

Occupational Groups 86

Primary Holland Type 87

Question 5b 88

All Data 88

Occupational Groups 88

Primary Holland Type 88

Question 5c 89

All Data 89

Occupational Groups 89

Primary Holland Type 89

Question 6 90

Correlation between Congruence and Overall Job Satisfaction 90

Summary of Chapter Four 90

CHAPTER FIVE: DISCUSSION 91

Review of Results 92

Demographic Analysis 93

Recommendations for Further Research 93

Mean Job Satisfaction Scores 94


Is Congruence Dead? xv

Recommendations for Further Research 94

Mean Congruence Scores 95

Recommendations for Further Research 95

Congruence – Job Satisfaction Correlations 96

Recommendations for Further Research 96

Traditionally-Derived and Decision-Derived Holland Summary Codes 96

Recommendations for Further Research 98

The Modfied C Index and the Substitution C Index 98

Recommendations for Further Research 99

Is Congruence Dead? 100

Research Limitations 101

REFERENCES 104

APPENDICES 127

Appendix A: Compatibility Index 127

Appendix B: K-P Index 128

Appendix C: M Index 130

Appendix D: Sb Index 131

Appendix E: C Index 135

Appendix F: The Modified C Index 136

Appendix G: The Substitution C Index 141

Appendix H: Steps for the Selection of Occupational Groups 145

Appendix I: Strong Interest Explorer Directions and Items 146

Appendix J: Strong Interest Explorer Items by RIASEC Type and Scale 148
Is Congruence Dead? xvi

Appendix K: Short-Form Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire Directions and Items 151

Appendix L: Professional Organizations and Businesses Corresponding to the


Occupational Groups 154

Appendix M: Introductory Cover Letter Mailed to Potential Participants 155

Appendix N: Consent for Participation in a Study of Interests and Job Satisfaction 156

Appendix O: Reminder Letter Mailed to Participants 160

Appendix P: Rules for Resolving Tied SIE Scores 161

Appendix Q: Rules for Generating Decision Derived Holland Codes 162


Is Congruence Dead? xvii

LIST OF TABLES

1. Qualitatively Different Self-Directed Search Profile Scores with the Same Summary
Code 22

2. Self-Directed Search Profile Scores with Tied Scores 23

3. Equal, Alternate, and Opposite Segments of Holland’s Hexagonal Model 35

4. Holland’s Hexagonal Model According to Levels of Consistency 36

5. Occupational Groups Included in the Research Sample 51

6. Occupational Group Usable Date Response Rates 54

7. Traditionally-Derived Codes and Decision-Derived Codes According to Primary


Holland Type 56

8. Occupational Group Hit Rates Between Summary Code Primary Types and GOT
Code Primary Types 57

9. Holland Type Hit Rates Between Summary Code Primary Types and GOT Code
Primary Types 57

10. Primary Holland Types by Coding Method 58

11. Primary Holland Types Changes after Conversion from Traditionally-Derived Codes
to Decision-Derived Codes 58

12. Demographic Data for All Cases 62

13. Demographic Data According to Occupational Group 64

14. Demographic Data According to Primary Holland Type 65

15. MSQ Data for All Cases 66

16. MSQ Data According to Occupational Group 67

17. MSQ Data According to Primary Holland Type 68

18. Congruence Score Data for All Cases 69

19. Congruence Score Data by Occupational Group 71

20. Congruence Score Data by Primary Holland Type 72


Is Congruence Dead? xviii

21. Job Satisfaction Correlations with Congruence Scores for All Data 73

22. Occupational Group Job Satisfaction Correlations with Traditionally-Derived


Congruence Scores 75

23. Holland Type Job Satisfaction Correlations with Traditionally-Derived Congruence


Scores 76

24. Occupational Group Job Satisfaction Correlations with Decision-Derived


Congruence Scores 85

25. Holland Type Job Satisfaction Correlations with Decision-Derived Congruence


Scores 87

26. Comparisons of Congruence – Overall Job Satisfaction Correlations 90

B1. K-P Index Hexagonal Segment Distance Proportion Values 129

C1. M Index Two-Letter Codes Weights 130

C2. M Index Three-Letter Code Weights 130

D1. Sb Index Distances 132

E1. Distances between Holland Types Used in the Calculation of the C Index 135
Is Congruence Dead? xix

LIST OF FIGURES

1. Hexagonal Model of Holland Types 34


Is Congruence Dead? 1

CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION

INTRODUCTION TO CHAPTER ONE

The person-environment (P-E) fit model is discussed as the dominant theoretical

framework in vocational psychology. John L. Holland’s theory of vocational personality types

and model work environments is identified as the most widely applied P-E fit approach within

the field. Holland’s primary assumptions are described, and salient theoretical concepts are

reviewed. The relationship between Holland’s congruence construct and job satisfaction is

presented as the primary focus.

It is discussed that the relationship between congruence and job satisfaction is

equivocally empirically supported in the vocational psychology literature. The evidence

supporting the argument that Holland’s congruence is “dead” as a viable theoretical construct is

reviewed. The counterargument that the relationship between congruence and job satisfaction is

viable but has been obscured by methodological problems is reviewed.

It is argued that many studies examining the relationship between congruence and job

satisfaction are flawed because common elements of the typical congruence – job satisfaction

research framework have not been well operationalized. Common methodological limitations are

discussed, and an alternative approach with concomitant general research questions is presented.

FROM PARSONS TO PERSON–ENVIRONMENT FIT

Frank Parsons (1909) often is credited as the “father” of vocational guidance in the

United States (Heppner, 2000). He developed a rational, three-part process to follow when

selecting an occupation:

In the wise choice of a vocation there are three broad factors: (1) a clear

understanding of yourself, your aptitudes, abilities, interests, ambitions,


Is Congruence Dead? 2

resources, limitations, and their causes; (2) a knowledge of the requirements and

conditions of success, advantages and disadvantages, compensation,

opportunities, and prospects in different lines of work; (3) true reasoning on these

two groups of facts. (Parsons, 1909, p. 5)

The introduction of this model planted the seeds of modern vocational psychology and marked

the beginning of an organized, theoretical approach to vocational guidance (Brown, 2002;

Williamson, 1966).

The trait-and-factor model that emerged and grew out of Parsons’ method similarly is

based on the idea that occupational choice is a straightforward reasoning process that involves

matching individual traits to commensurate job requirements (Walsh, 1999). The trait-and-factor

model further specifies that positive job outcomes occur when there is a good match, more

commonly referred to as fit, between the individual’s traits and the requirements of the job (Betz,

Fitzgerald, & Hill, 1989).

Within the relatively short history of vocational psychology, the trait-and-factor approach

now has been replaced by person-environment (P-E) fit theories. Eggerth (2004) concisely sums

up P-E fit theory by stating, “the crux of P-E fit models is the ability to make meaningful

predictions about outcome based upon the quality of fit between the characteristics of a person

and of an environment” (p. 93). Swanson (1996) and Chartrand (1991) distinguish the trait-and-

factor approach from P-E fit by explaining that P-E fit incorporates trait-and-factor ideas but

more broadly defines vocational behavior and its salient concepts. The P-E fit approach, with its

intuitive appeal and solid theoretical framework, has grown to become the model of inquiry “par

excellence” (Dawis, 2000) for the field of vocational psychology.


Is Congruence Dead? 3

HOLLAND’S THEORY OF VOCATIONAL PERSONALITY TYPES AND

MODEL WORK ENVIRONMENTS

John L. Holland’s (1959,1966, 1973, 1985a, 1992, 1997) theory of vocational personality

types and model work environments is thought to be the most widely applied P-E fit approach in

vocational psychology. The theory is cited widely for its parsimony, definition of constructs, and

relationships among its principles. It generally is considered to be the most respected and

influential career theory within the field (Campbell & Borgen, 1999; Gottfredson, 1999; Griffin

& Hesketh, 2005).

Holland’s emphasis on the role of personality makes the theory distinctive from other

theories in vocational psychology (Osipow, 1994). According to the theory, an individual’s

occupational choices represent an extension of personality and an effort to apply broad personal

behavioral styles within the context of the work environment (Hogan & Blake, 1999; Holland,

1999; Walsh & Osipow, 1986).

PRIMARY ASSUMPTIONS OF HOLLAND’S THEORY

The essence of Holland’s (1997) theory can be found in four primary assumptions that

correspond to the basic aspects of P-E fit. These assumptions explain (1) vocational personality

types, (2) model work environments, (3) person-environment fit, and (4) outcomes of person-

environment fit.

Primary Assumption One: Vocational Personality Types

Holland (1997) states that, “in our culture, most persons can be categorized as one of six

personality types: Realistic, Investigative, Artistic, Social, Enterprising, or Conventional” (p. 2).

The acronym RIASEC typically is used as a shorthand to refer to this typology.


Is Congruence Dead? 4

Primary Assumption Two: Model Work Environments

According to Holland (1997), work environments cannot be separated from the

individuals who occupy them. From this tenet follows there are six model work environments

that are commensurate with the six vocational personality types. These environments also are

named as Realistic, Investigative, Artistic, Social, Enterprising, and Conventional.

Primary Assumption Three: Person–Environment Fit

Holland (1997) indicates that people are drawn to environments that allow them to use

their skills and abilities and to express their attitudes and values. This assertion specifically refers

to the idea that individuals tend to seek out occupational environments that are compatible to

their vocational personality types within the RIASEC scheme.

The term congruence commonly is used to imply the degree of fit, match, or similarity

between the individual and the requirements of the individual’s chosen work environment

(Edwards, 1994; Gothard, 1985). In Holland’s model, an individual is said to be congruent when

the vocational personality type is identical or highly similar to the work environment.

Conversely, an individual is incongruent when the vocational personality type and the work

environment are inconsistent.

Primary Assumption Four: Outcomes of Person–Environment Fit

Holland’s (1997) theory specifies that individuals will succeed in congruent work

environments because those environments, by definition, provide the kinds of opportunities and

rewards that are important. Congruence has been shown empirically to be positively correlated

with a variety of dependent outcomes, including vocational stability (e.g., Oleski & Subich,

1996; Villwock, Schnitzen, & Carbonari, 1976), occupational performance and productivity
Is Congruence Dead? 5

(e.g., Fritzsche, Powell, & Hoffman, 1999; Richards, 1993), and personal adjustment (e.g.,

Eagan & Walsh, 1995; Lachterman & Meir, 2004).

Job satisfaction is the most frequently examined outcome variable in congruence

research. Congruent individuals are expected to experience a higher level of job satisfaction than

those individuals who are incongruent (Carson & Mowsesian, 1993; Jagger, Neukrug, &

McAuliffe, 1992; Mount & Muchinsky, 1978; Smart, Elton, & McLaughlin, 1986; Wiggins,

1984; Wiggins, Lederer, Salkowe, & Rys, 1983). The assumption that congruence results in job

satisfaction is fundamental to Holland’s theory (Gottfredson & Holland, 1990).

RATIONALE FOR THE RESEARCH STUDY

The empirical evidence predominantly indicates that the congruence – job satisfaction

correlation is, at best, equivocal. While there are many studies that provide support for a positive

correlation between congruence and job satisfaction (e.g., Burgner, 1994; Smart, Elton, &

McLaughlin, 1986), there are many more studies that have failed to validate this relationship or

have produced mixed results (e.g., Cook, 1996; Harris, Mortizen, Robitschek, Imhoff, & Lynch,

2001; Meir, Keinan, & Segal, 1986; Tokar & Subich, 1997).

The results of three meta-analyses further support that congruence is not meaningfully

related to job satisfaction within Holland’s theory. After an analysis of 53 congruence studies,

Assouline and Meir (1987) report the weighted mean correlation between congruence and job

satisfaction to be a modest .21. The analysis conducted by Tranberg, Slane, and Ekeberg (1993)

found a comparably low mean correlation of .20. Tsabari, Tziner, and Meir (2005) performed a

meta-analysis of 53 samples and found the mean correlation between congruence and job

satisfaction to be weak at .166. Tsabari, et al. then corrected for sampling and measurement

errors that have been identified as ones that can obscure significant meta-analytic results
Is Congruence Dead? 6

(Holland, 1987; Meir, 1995). The modified analysis revealed an even lower mean congruence

correlation value of .158.

In the wake of these and related findings, there is a movement within vocational

psychology that the field should “move away” from Holland’s theory (Hesketh, 2000; Schwartz,

1992). Tinsley (2006), for example, states that the theory stands as an exception to the generally

positive performance of other P-E fit models and indicates there is no evidence that congruence

is a valid predictor of any meaningful work outcomes. Subich (1992) similarly advises that

Holland’s fundamental assumption of the ability to predict job satisfaction from congruence may

be too broadly stated. Even Holland himself has acknowledged the popular opinion within

vocational psychology that the congruence construct is “dead” as a viable theoretical concept

(Feller, Honaker, & Zagzebski, 2001; Holland, 1996). This is not to say that Holland has agreed

with this view.

In fact, the idea that Holland’s theory should be abandoned does not exist without

significant argument. The apparently dubious relationship between Holland’s congruence and

job satisfaction is a controversial issue within vocational psychology (Spokane, Luchetta, &

Richwine, 2002; Swanson & Gore, 2000). An alternative point of view within the field maintains

that congruence has not fared better in the empirical research literature because methodological

limitations have led to unimpressive and ambiguous findings (Chartrand, Strong, & Weitzman,

1995; Eggerth, Bowles, Tunick & Andrew, 2005; Fassinger, 2005; Furnham, 2001; Meir, 1995;

Phillips & Jome, 2005; Prediger & Vansickle, 1992; Swanson, 1992).

STATEMENT OF THE RESEARCH PROBLEM

Holland’s theory is expected to continue to dominate vocational behavior research

(Brown, 2003; Hansen, 1984; Lowman & Carson, 2000). This means that the continued vitality
Is Congruence Dead? 7

of the congruence construct depends upon the evolution of appropriate methodology (Prediger,

2000; Spokane, Meir, & Catalano, 2000). Many researchers have neither acknowledged the

methodological problems associated with congruence research nor implemented viable empirical

alternatives (Edwards, 1991).

PURPOSE OF THE RESEARCH STUDY

Chartrand and Walsh (1999) point out that congruence – job satisfaction correlation

research requires four methodological components: (1) the assessment of vocational personality,

(2) the assessment of the work environment, (3) the calculation of congruence, and (4) the

correlation of congruence to a job satisfaction outcome measure. This study addresses sampling

issues and common methodological problems associated with each of the four aspects outlined

by Chartrand and Walsh. This research additionally provides a rationale for identifying and

comparing measurement alternatives.

Sampling Issues

This study first addresses two sampling issues that have plagued congruence research.

Previous research has relied heavily on the sampling of college students and members

representing a single occupation or occupational group. The correction of previous errors is

described in Chapter Three through this study’s use of a sample of employed adults that is

representative of the entire RIASEC scheme.

Assessment of Vocational Personality

The Self-Directed Search (SDS; Holland, Fritzsche, & Powell, 1994), the Vocational

Preference Inventory (VPI; Holland, 1985b), and the Strong Interest Inventory (SII; Donnay,

Morris, Schaubhut, & Thompson, 2005) are discussed as the most common instruments used to
Is Congruence Dead? 8

measure vocational personality. Questionable construct validity, problems incurred with scoring

and coding procedures, and practical concerns are reviewed as they relate to these instruments.

The Strong Interest Explorer (SIE; Chartrand, 2001) is identified as the measure of

vocational personality chosen for this study. Research conducted by Dik, Hu, and Hansen (2007)

that describes an alternative method for generating Holland codes is reviewed. This research

generates traditionally-derived summary codes and decision-derived summary codes. It is

examined whether the congruence - job satisfaction correlation is dependent upon the summary

coding method that is applied.

Assessment of Work Environment

The Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes (DHOC; Gottfredson & Holland, 1996),

the Occupations Finder (OF; Holland, 1994), the Environmental Assessment Technique (EAT;

Astin, 1963; Astin & Holland, 1961), the Position Classification Inventory (PCI; Gottfredson &

Holland, 1991), and the Occupational Information Network (O*NET; National O*NET

Consortium, n.d.) are identified as popular methods for assessing the Holland work environment.

Conceptual and practical problems are identified and discussed for each of these techniques.

The General Occupational Theme (GOT) codes of the SII are described. It is identified

that this research uses GOT codes to assess the work environment as they represent an

application of the RIASEC scheme that incorporates methods to limit potential measurement

error.

Calculation of Congruence

Six mathematical congruence indexes are identified and described: (1) the First Letter

Hexagonal Distance index (FLHD; Holland, 1973), (2) the Compatibility index (CI; Wiggins &

Moody, 1981; Moody, 1983), (3) the K-P index (Kwak & Pulvino, 1982), (4) the M index
Is Congruence Dead? 9

(Iachan, 1984, 1990), (5) the Sb Index (Gati, 1985), and (6) the C index (Brown & Gore, 1994).

The limitations of the use of these indexes in the calculation of congruence are discussed. The

modified C index, developed by Eggerth and Andrew (2006) in order to address these

limitations, is discussed. The substitution C index, developed by Gore and Brown (2006) as an

alternative to modified C index, is discussed as well. Both indexes are used in this research to

generate congruence scores. It is examined whether the congruence - job satisfaction correlation

is dependent upon the index that is used to calculate congruence scores.

Job Satisfaction Measurement

The most frequently used job satisfaction assessment techniques are identified as the

Hoppock Job Satisfaction Blank (HJSB; Hoppock, 1935), the Job Descriptive Index (JDI;

Baltzer, et al., 1997; Baltzer & Smith, 1990; Smith, Kendall, & Hulin, 1969), the Job in General

scale (JIG; Ironson, Smith, Brannick, Gibson, & Paul, 1989), (3) the Minnesota Satisfaction

Questionnaire (MSQ; Weiss, Dawis, England, & Lofquist, 1967) (4) one-item measures of

overall job satisfaction, and (5) measures developed by the research authors. Limitations related

to valid and reliable measurement are reviewed. The importance of examining intrinsic,

extrinsic, and overall aspects of job satisfaction is discussed, and the short-form MSQ is

identified as the measure used here.

GENERAL RESEARCH QUESTIONS

In order to examine the congruence – job satisfaction correlation while comparing two

methods for generating Holland codes and two indexes for calculating congruence and

investigating three aspects of job satisfaction, the following research questions are investigated.

It is recognized that other inferential procedures may be applied.


Is Congruence Dead? 10

General Research Question Set 1: Measuring Congruence – Job Satisfaction Correlations with

Traditionally-Derived Codes and the Modified C Index

Question 1a

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

Question 1b

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and extrinsic job

satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

Question 1c

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction

when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the Modified C

index is used to calculate congruence scores?

General Research Question Set 2: Measuring Congruence – Job Satisfaction Correlations with

Traditionally-Derived Codes and the Substitution C Index

Question 2a

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores?


Is Congruence Dead? 11

Question 2b

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and extrinsic job

satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

Question 2c

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction

when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the Substitution

C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

General Research Question Set 3: Measuring Congruence – Job Satisfaction Correlations with

Decision-Derived Codes and the Modified C Index

Question 3a

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

Question 3b

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and extrinsic job

satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

Question 3c

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction

when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the Modified C

index is used to calculate congruence scores?


Is Congruence Dead? 12

General Research Question Set 4: Measuring Congruence – Job Satisfaction Correlations with

Decision-Derived Codes and the Substitution C Index

Question 4a

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Substitution C index is used to measure congruence?

Question 4b

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and extrinsic job

satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Substitution C index is used to measure congruence?

Question 4c

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction

when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the Substitution C

index is used to measure congruence?

General Research Question Set 5: Comparing Job Satisfaction Correlation Values

Question 5a

Are there significant differences in the intrinsic job satisfaction correlation values

identified in Questions 1a, 2a, 3a, and 4a?

Question 5b

Are there significant differences in the extrinsic job satisfaction correlation values

identified in Questions 1b, 2b, 3b, and 4b?


Is Congruence Dead? 13

Question 5c

Are there significant differences in the overall job satisfaction correlation values

identified in Questions 1c, 2c, 3c, and 4c?

General Research Question 6: Comparing the Overall Job Satisfaction Correlation to Established

Meta-Analytic Values

Do any of the overall job satisfaction correlation values identified in Questions 1c, 2c, 3c,

and 4c meet or significantly exceed the meta-analytic correlations that were reported by

Assouline & Meir (1987), Tranberg, et al. (1993), and Tsabari, et al. (2005)?

SUMMARY OF CHAPTER ONE

The person-environment (P-E) fit model was discussed as the dominant theoretical

framework in vocational psychology. John L. Holland’s theory of vocational personality types

and model work environments was identified as the most widely applied P-E fit approach within

the field. Holland’s primary assumptions were described, and salient theoretical concepts were

reviewed. The relationship between Holland’s congruence construct and job satisfaction was

presented as the primary focus.

It was discussed that the relationship between congruence and job satisfaction is

equivocally empirically supported in the vocational psychology literature. The evidence

supporting the argument that Holland’s congruence is “dead” as a viable theoretical construct

was reviewed. The counterargument that the relationship between congruence and job

satisfaction is viable but has been obscured by methodological problems was reviewed.

It was argued that many studies examining the relationship between congruence and job

satisfaction are flawed because common elements of the typical congruence – job satisfaction

research framework have not been well operationalized. Common methodological limitations
Is Congruence Dead? 14

were discussed, and an alternative approach with concomitant general research questions was

presented.
Is Congruence Dead? 15

CHAPTER TWO: LITERATURE REVIEW

INTRODUCTION TO CHAPTER TWO

Chapter Two details the methodological limitations characteristic of congruence – job

satisfaction correlation research and discusses appropriate measurement alternatives. The

rationale for the comparison of two Holland summary coding methods and two congruence

indexes and the investigation of three aspects of job satisfaction is presented.

The six vocational personality types are outlined. The Self-Directed Search, the

Vocational Preference Inventory, and the Strong Interest Inventory are discussed as the major

vocational personality assessments that are used in congruence research. Construct validity,

traditionally-derived scoring issues, and practical concerns are described. The Strong Interest

Explorer is presented as the measurement option used in this research. Research on guidelines

for developing decision-derived Holland summary codes is described. It is discussed that the

research compares traditionally-derived codes with decision-derived codes that are generated

with a cut standard score method.

The six work environments are described. The Dictionary of Holland Occupational

Codes, the Occupations Finder, the Environmental Assessment Technique, the Position

Classification Inventory, and the Occupational Information Network are discussed as popular

work environment assessment methods. Conceptual and practical problems and a misassumption

regarding the interchangeability of these measures are presented. The SII General Occupational

Theme codes are identified as the work environment assessment approach that is used in this

research.

Holland’s circumplex model and secondary theoretical assumption of conistency are

explained. The First Letter Hexagonal Distance index, the Compatibility index, the K-P index,
Is Congruence Dead? 16

the M index, the Sb Index, and the C index are identified as well-known congruence indexes. The

methodological weaknesses of these indexes are discussed. The modified C index and the

substitution C index are identified and presented as alternative congruence measures that are

compared in this research.

Job satisfaction is identified as an affective state that results from the overall assessment

of job experiences. The Hoppock Job Satisfaction Blank, the Job Descriptive Index and the Job

in General scale, the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire, one-item measures of overall job

satisfaction, and measures developed by the research authors are discussed as frequently used job

satisfaction assessment techniques. Validity and reliability issues associated with these methods

are described. It is explained that this research uses the short-form MSQ to measure intrinsic,

extrinsic, and overall job satisfaction.

SAMPLING ISSUES

College and University Student Samples

Congruence correlation research has relied heavily on the use of college and university

student samples (Hackett, Lent, & Greenhaus, 1991; Smart, 1976; Spokane, 1985; Thomas &

Robbins, 1979; Tinsley, 2000). Educational satisfaction, in particular, has been investigated often

as a dependent outcome of congruence (e.g., Allen, 1996; Frantz & Walsh, 1972; Morrow, 1971;

Nafziger, Holland, & Gottfredson, 1975; Shaw, 2005; Spokane, 1979; Spokane & Derby, 1979;

Walsh, 1974; Walsh & Lewis, 1972).The prevalence of this type of research has resulted in a

disproportionate focus on college and university students and reflects an assumption that

educational satisfaction can be considered as a proxy for job satisfaction. The investigation of

congruence in this manner is misguided as it fails to reflect Holland’s assumption that working
Is Congruence Dead? 17

adults seek out congruent work environments (Rounds, McKenna, Hubert, & Day, 2000;

Tsabari, et al., 2005).

Single Occupation Samples

Much of the congruence – job satisfaction correlation research has examined samples that

represent a single occupation or occupational group (e.g., Aranya, Barak, & Amernic, 1981;

Clark, 1991; Coddington, 1998; Finley, 1989; Furnham, Toop, Lewis, & Fisher, 1995; Hoile,

2000; Portscheller, 1992; Wiggins, 1984). As Holland uses the RIASEC scheme to define six

vocational personalities and six corresponding work environments, it is doubtful whether this

kind of research can be used to make appropriate statements regarding congruence. The use of

single-sample methodology may be better considered as an examination of a particular

occupational group or Holland type rather than as an investigation of the congruence – job

satisfaction relationship that is within the scope of the theory.

VOCATIONAL PERSONALITY

Vocational Personality: Types

Each personality type within the RIASEC scheme is an ideal model of behavior that is

defined by a characteristic personal repertoire of attributes, attitudes, and behaviors. Holland

states that one tends to most resemble one particular type. The theory specifies, though, that

individuals implement a range of behaviors and can resemble to a degree more than one, and in

most cases all, of the six personality types.

Realistic Vocational Personality Type

Realistic individuals tend to be interested more in dealing with things than with people or

ideas. They may prefer that which is tangible and concrete to that which is abstract and
Is Congruence Dead? 18

subjective. They are inclined to be oriented toward developing physical strength, honing motor

coordination, and engaging in activities that require mechanical and manual abilities.

Investigative Vocational Personality Type

Investigative individuals prefer to engage in activities that involve organizing and

understanding ideas. They tend to be task-oriented, analytical, and enjoy pursuing academic

activities. They often possess strong verbal, scientific, and mathematical skills.

Artistic Vocational Personality Type

Artistic individuals tend to enjoy activities that require self-expression and encourage

independence and subjectivity. They may prefer aesthetic endeavors that involve the use of

imaginative, introspective, and creative skills, and they tend to possess artistic and musical

ability. They may prefer to avoid highly controlled environments that require compliance to pre-

existing standards.

Social Vocational Personality Type

Social individuals seek situations where they can engage in social interactions and

exercise their interpersonal abilities. They tend to be sensitive to the needs of others and have a

strong interest in helping and understanding others. They may prefer teaching activities where

they can use their verbal and social skills.

Enterprising Vocational Personality Type

Enterprising individuals typically are verbally skilled, extroverted, and confident.

They tend to enjoy adventurous activities and value economic and political pursuits. They may

prefer roles that involve power and status and may enjoy organizing, directing, and persuading

others.
Is Congruence Dead? 19

Conventional Vocational Personality Type

Conventional individuals enjoy pursuing structured activities. They may prefer clear

guidelines and have the ability to adapt to the structure and demands of a particular situation.

They tend to be organized and practical, have numerical and clerical ability, and pursue business

and economic endeavors.

Vocational Personality: Instruments

The Self-Directed Search

The Self-Directed Search (SDS; Holland, et. al, 1994) is one of the most widely used

inventories in all of vocational interest research. The assessment booklet is an ipsative measure,

designed to be self-administered and self-scored. It consists of four primary sections, listings of

(1) 66 Activities, (2) 66 Competencies, (3) 84 Occupations, and (4) 12 Self-Estimates of abilities.

Each of these components represents each of the six dimensions in the RIASEC typology.

Responses regarding interest in Activities (“Like”, “Dislike”) and Occupations (“Yes”, “No”)

combined with responses regarding Competencies (“Yes”, “No”) and Self-Estimates of abilities

(scales ranging from 1 to 7) yield a raw score for each of the six types.

The Vocational Preference Inventory

The Vocational Preference Inventory (VPI; Holland, 1985b) first was developed in 1953

and currently is in its seventh edition. The full-scale VPI consists of 160 occupational titles that

represent each of the six dimensions in the RIASEC typology. Ipsative responses regarding level

of interest (“Yes”, “No”, “Undecided”) in the occupational titles yield a raw score for each type.

In addition to the six scales representing the RIASEC typology, the VPI contains five

supplemental scales: (1) Self-control, (2) Masculinity/Femininity, (3) Status, (4) Infrequency,

and (5) Acquiescence.


Is Congruence Dead? 20

The Strong Interest Inventory

The Strong Interest Inventory (SII; Donnay, et. al, 2005) has long been combined with

Holland’s RIASEC typology (Campbell & Holland, 1972). It consists of 291 items each with a

corresponding 5-point Likert-type response scale (“Strongly Like”, “Like”, “Indifferent”,

“Dislike”, “Strongly Dislike”). The SII is a computer-scored assessment that yields standardized

T-scores based on extensive normative data in the areas of six General Occupational Themes

(GOT), 30 Basic Interest Scales (BIS), 122 pairs of Occupational Scales (OS), and 5 Personality

Style Scales (PSS). The GOT and BIS components of the SII are the particular sections that are

commensurate with the RIASEC scheme and yield standard scores for each of the six types.

Vocational Personality: Limitations

Self-Directed Search

Construct Validity

The four sections of the SDS are approximately equally weighted and therefore confound

an individual’s vocational personality results with information regarding competencies (Lowman

& Williams, 1987). Swanson (1993) points out that interests, skills, and abilities are related, yet

distinct, constructs and highlights that having skill or ability in a certain area does not necessarily

equate with also having an interest in that area.

The incorporation of self-estimates into SDS scores assumes that individuals are able to

accurately approximate their own skills. There is evidence to indicate that this may not be the

case (Mabe & West, 1982; Poh, 1996). Kelso, Holland, and Gottfredson (1977) administered the

SDS and the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery (ASVAB; Bayroff & Fuchs, 1970) to

a group of female preparatory school students. It was predicted that the SDS Self-Estimates and

Competencies scales would correspond with the commensurate scales of the ASVAB; however,
Is Congruence Dead? 21

the findings indicated that the SDS and the ASVAB scores were only moderately correlated.

Hodgson and Cramer (1977) investigated a group students who had been administered the

Differential Aptitude Tests (DAT; Bennett, Seashore, & Wesman, 1959). The students

completed the Self-Estimates portion of the SDS, and the Math Ability, Clerical Ability, and

Mechanical Ability scales of the SDS were compared with the commensurate, same-named DAT

scales. The overall findings did not support the use of self-estimates when assessing skills and

abilities. Lowman and Williams (1987) studied a group of college women in order to compare

self-estimates to objective measures of abilities. Each woman was administered the SDS and a

battery of 10 objective measures differentially corresponding to the six Holland types. Support

for the validity of the self-estimated abilities was mixed. The authors conclude that “it remains

open to debate what precisely the SDS is measuring” (p. 11).

Scoring

The scoring procedure of the SDS is designed to highlight what is termed here as an

individual’s traditionally-derived summary code, the three types within the RIASEC scheme that

correspond to the three highest SDS raw scores. Traditionally-derived summary codes have

grown to become standard shorthand in Holland’s theory. Despite this, the use of the

traditionally-derived approach creates both theoretical and practical difficulties and is

methodologically insensitive (Gati, 1987; Meir, 1993).

The rigid implementation of the traditional coding procedure does not take into

consideration the distances between scores or the overall magnitude of the scores (Arnold, 2004).

This permits qualitatively different profiles to be identified with the same code. The SDS

summary codes of Persons A, B, and C in Table 1 all would be reported as RAS even though the

scores illustrate three distinct patterns. Person A has Realistic interests that appear to be well-
Is Congruence Dead? 22

defined, and it is debatable whether the addition of Artistic and Social to the summary code of

Person A adds relevant information. Person B could be characterized as RAS, but all of the

scores are clustered close together. The flat and low profile of Person C demonstrates little

meaningful similarity to any of the six types.

Table 1

Qualitatively Different Self-Directed Search Profile Scores with the Same Summary Code

Realistic Investigative Artistic Social Enterprising Conventional

Person A 20 5 8 7 4 4

Person B 15 11 14 13 10 12

Person C 6 2 5 3 0 1

Formal methods for handling SDS tie scores have not been established; moreover,

procedures used for breaking ties often is not reported in congruence research (Spokane, et al.,

2000; Strahan & Severinghaus, 1992). The three highest SDS scores of Person D in Table 2 are

Social, Enterprising, and Conventional. As the Enterprising and Conventional scores are tied, it

must be decided whether the summary code should be SEC or SCE. The SDS profile of Person E

shows that Social and Enterprising and Artistic and Conventional scores are tied. It must be

decided whether the first two letters of the summary code should be SE or ES and whether the

third letter should be A or C. Finally, the highest scores for Person F appear in a three-way-tie

and potentially could be ordered to create six different summary codes.


Is Congruence Dead? 23

Table 2

Self-Directed Search Profile Scores with Tied Scores

Realistic Investigative Artistic Social Enterprising Conventional

Person D 13 12 15 25 20 20

Person E 8 11 14 17 17 14

Person F 13 19 22 28 28 28

Vocational Preference Inventory

The VPI is a more construct valid measure of vocational interests when compared to the

SDS. The VPI does yield RIASEC raw data that is comparable to that of the SDS. The VPI thus

incurs the same scoring issues that already have been outlined for the SDS.

Strong Interest Inventory

The SII is one of the most respected and technologically sound vocational interest

inventories that currently is available (Borgen & Harmon, 1996; Hansen, 2000). Extensive

studies support its validity and indicate that the SII best fits the RIASEC model when compared

with the SDS and the VPI (Hubert & Arabie, 1987; Tracey & Rounds, 1993). Despite this, the

SII is not often used in congruence – job satisfaction correlation research. Despite the empirical

superiority of the SII, there are practical concerns associated with the use of this instrument.

Scoring Options and Cost

The SII can be purchased with options for either mail-in scoring or internet scoring. The

cost of the SII can be expensive, and in fact, could be prohibitive for the independent researcher

or for an organization with a limited research budget. Moreover, the mail-in scoring option has

the potential to hinder efficient research as one must wait for the instrument to be scored and
Is Congruence Dead? 24

returned before data can be analyzed. While the internet option provides a faster alternative, this

option also would require adequate access to a computer with internet capabilities. Sufficient

access to up-to-date equipment and office space is not always an uncomplicated matter in the

research process.

Administration Time

Manual information indicates that an individual should be able to complete the SII in an

average time of 35 to 40 minutes. Today, in the 21st century, when time is considered more than

ever to be a premium commodity, many potential research participants may not be willing to take

the SII. Researchers have the responsibility of weighing their instrument choices against

potential respondent burden (Sharp & Frankel, 1983), and subject willingness to participate is as

much a practical consideration as protection of research participants is an ethical consideration.

Vocational Personality: Alternatives

The Strong Interest Explorer

The SII has been used to develop the Strong Interest Explorer (SIE; Chartrand, 2001).

The SIE differs from the SII in that it is a simplified alternative. The SIE is designed to be

quickly self-administered and self-scored and can be purchased at prices that are more affordable

than those of the SII. The SIE is a relatively new and lesser-known instrument. There exists no

known published research with the SIE outside of its development and the initial establishment

of validity and reliability (Research Department, 2002). The SIE warrants empirical study. This

instrument may provide a more practical, and yet still highly valid, measurement option in

congruence research.
Is Congruence Dead? 25

Decision-Derived Rules for Assigning Holland Summary Codes

Dik, et al. (2007) point out that research on congruence – job satisfaction correlations

across methods of assigning Holland codes has been “virtually non-existent.” In an effort to

address this gap in the literature, the authors used gender-normed standard scores based on the

GOT scales of the 1985 version of the SII (Hansen & Campbell, 1985) to develop and compare

the utility of four sets of Holland code assignment methods.

All of the coding methods were sensitive both to the elevation of scores and to the

magnitude of the differences between the scores. Two coding assignment approaches used

decision rules based on a mean T score cut score process. Two techniques used decision rules

based on an individual’s highest GOT score relative to the standard deviation of difference

scores. Data analysis indicated that the largest mean congruence value was associated with the

cut score method anchored by a mean T score of 60. It is this method that is used here to

generate the decision-derived summary codes that are compared to the traditionally-derived

codes.

WORK ENVIRONMENT

Model Work Environment: Types

In Holland’s theory, work environments are seen as a function of the individuals who

occupy them. “The people make the place” (Schneider, 1987) as individuals tend to create work

environments where certain characteristics dominate. Work environments in turn become

settings that elicit, develop, and reward the behaviors of the individuals who resemble the

prevailing environmental type. These factors combine such that work environments and

personality types mirror one another.


Is Congruence Dead? 26

Realistic Model Work Environment

Realistic work environments encourage the development of technical competency and

psychomotor skills. They encourage achievement in skilled trades and mechanical occupations

and provide opportunities to build or repair with the use of tools or machines. Examples of

Realistic occupations include automobile mechanic, construction worker, emergency medical

technician, and bus driver.

Investigative Model Work Environment

Investigative work environments encourage the application of analytical ability in the

observation and investigation of physical, biological, and cultural phenomena. They provide

opportunities to work independently on conceptual tasks and tend to support perseverance in

solving problems. Examples of Investigative occupations include astronomer, chemist, medical

technologist, mathematician, anthropologist, and veterinarian.

Artistic Model Work Environment

Artistic work environments encourage the development of creativity, originality,

imagination, artistic ability, and verbal skills. They stimulate artistic, musical, and literary

achievement in unstructured and flexible environments. Examples of Artistic occupations

include composer, poet, singer, book editor, wedding consultant, actor, interior designer,

architect, and illustrator.

Social Model Work Environment

Social work environments promote social, interpersonal, and teaching skills and the

ability to understand and empathize with others. They stimulate performance in occupations

where individuals can demonstrate a desire to inform, train, develop, enlighten or improve the
Is Congruence Dead? 27

lives of others. Examples of Social occupations include family counselor, clergy member,

secondary school teacher, registered nurse, and physical therapist.

Enterprising Model Work Environment

Enterprising work environments encourage the development of verbal and leadership

skills and promote achievement in managing others and in purchasing and selling. Enterprising

work environments provide the opportunity for individuals to meet organizational, project, and

personal goals by persuading others. Examples of Enterprising occupations include hotel

manager, real estate agent, flight attendant, buyer, urban planner, and estate planner.

Conventional Model Work Environment

Conventional work environments encourage the development of efficiency, data

management, mathematical skills, and attention to detail. They provide opportunities for

individuals to manage procedures, oversee files, write reports, and operate business machines in

order to attain organizational goals. Examples of Conventional occupations include bookkeeper,

accountant, bank teller, payroll clerk, and credit analyst.

Work Environment: Instruments

The Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes

In the development of the Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes (DHOC,

Gottfredson & Holland, 1996), Holland and his colleagues rationally derived three-letter

summary codes for occupations primarily by examining job analysis data gathered by the United

States Department of Labor Employment and Training Administration (1972, 1974, 1977). A

classification algorithm was developed and validated and since has been used to generate

Holland codes for many federal classification systems including the Dictionary of Occupational

Titles (DOT; United States Department of Labor, 1991), the Occupational Outlook Handbook
Is Congruence Dead? 28

(OOH; Bureau of Labor Statistics, 1996), the Standard Occupational Classification manual

(SOC; Office of Federal Statistical Policy and Standards, 1980), and the Guide for Occupational

Exploration (GOE; Harrington & O’Shea, 1984; United States Department of Labor, 1979).

The Occupations Finder

The Occupations Finder (OF, Holland, 1994) exists today as an abridged version of the

DHOC. It lists selected occupational titles along with their corresponding Holland summary

codes. The OF most frequently is seen as a career counseling companion tool that is sold with

SDS product assessment packages.

The Environmental Assessment Technique

The Environmental Assessment Technique (EAT; Astin, 1963; Astin & Holland, 1961)

first was developed as a method for conducting research on the impact of college environments

on students. Major areas of study at 411 colleges and universities were examined, and each field

was assigned a primary Holland type that best matched its academic requirements. It was

assumed that students selected majors that matched their own primary Holland type. The Holland

type of the institutions thus was classified according to the proportion of students in each major.

Holland (1997) discusses the current application of the EAT as a very similar process of taking a

census of the primary types that exist in a particular environment.

The Position Classification Inventory

The use of the Position Classification Inventory (PCI; Gottfredson & Holland, 1991) for

the assessment of work environments is analogous to the use of the SDS for the assessment of

vocational personality. The PCI is a subjective job analysis inventory that tabulates raw scores to

yield a three-letter Holland code. The inventory consists of a total of 84 items that evaluate the

six Holland types as they apply to the work environment. The items are arranged into seven
Is Congruence Dead? 29

questions, each with 12 occupational aspects that evaluate the position. The questions are

presented according to (1) position requirements, (2) skills and abilities used in the position, (3)

outlook/perspective demand of the position, (4) personal style/values expressed in the position,

(5) personal characteristics required in the position, (6) abilities/skills/talents needed in the

position, and (7) frequency of certain activities in the position. Items are answered based on

whether the respondent thinks the item “Often”, “Sometimes”, or “Seldom/Never” describes the

position being evaluated.

The Occupational Information Network

The Occupational Information Network (O*NET, National O*NET Consortium, n.d.) is

an interactive World Wide Web database application that is a resource for information on a

variety of occupations. The O*NET replaces the DOT and all other federal occupational

classification systems. Occupational Interest Profiles (OIPs) were established for the occupations

in the database through the use of expert ratings of three trained judges (Rounds, Smith, Hubert,

Lewis, & Rivkin, 1999). Each OIP consists of six standard scores that correspond to the RIASEC

types. A proportion cut-off score method was applied in order to create a “reasonable

distribution” of one-, two-, and three-letter Holland summary codes. The O*NET taxonomy was

updated in 2000 in order to be compatible with the Standard Occupational Classification (SOC)

system (Levine, Nottingham, Paige, & Lewis, 2000) and recently was updated again in 2006

(National Center for O*NET Development, 2006). The O*NET has maintained the OIP

information throughout these database revisions.


Is Congruence Dead? 30

Work Environment: Instrument Limitations

The Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes and The Occupations Finder

The accuracy of the classification algorithm that was used to generate the three letter

Holland codes that now make-up the DHOC and the OF has been questioned. There is evidence

to suggest that the algorithm may lead to arbitrary code assignments (Rounds, et al, 1999).

Gottfredson and Holland (1996) acknowledge that the information used to create the codes now

is dated, sometimes was unreliable, and was incomplete for some occupations. The algorithm

was not validated for second-letter and third–letter agreement, and additional data suggest that

even first-letter assignments may be inaccurate. Using a sample of 289 occupations, Gottfredson

and Holland conducted a “quality control check” by comparing first-letter code agreement based

on their own judgment and based on the algorithm. They found overall hit rate of 77.4%. If one

applied Meehl and Rosen’s (1955) argument of antecedent probability and arbitrarily assigned all

occupational titles as Realistic, one would have been correct 82% of the time. This is a 4.6%

improvement over the cross-validation rate that actually was observed.

The Environmental Assessment Technique

A cursory review of the literature indicates that the EAT has been the subject of little

research outside of Holland’s own work with college environments. A PsycINFO search using

Environmental Assessment Technique as a keyword resulted in merely 14 hits. Among these,

only two studies assessed the work environment and also were authored by someone other than

Holland himself (Jaskolski, 1997; Wigington & Apostal, 1973). Moreover, the EAT is not an

actual measurement, but a concept that relies on Holland’s second primary assumption. The

implementation of the EAT actually requires the use of a vocational personality measurement in

order to take a census of the Holland types in a given work environment. The use of the EAT
Is Congruence Dead? 31

therefore can be only as valid as the vocational personality instrument used, and the limitations

associated with the most popular methods such as the SDS and the VPI already have been

reviewed. Finally, as the EAT originally was developed as an assessment technique for an entire

environment, the use of the EAT in congruence research seems to be theoretically incompatible

with Holland’s intent to more narrowly focus on the level of the occupation or occupational

group.

The Position Classification Inventory

As the use of the PCI is analogous to the use of the SDS, the PCI incurs construct validity

and scoring limitations similar to the ones previously identified with the SDS. The PCI asks

respondents to report on work aspects that includes skills and abilities. Summary codes derived

from the PCI therefore are not a pure reflection of the vocational interests associated with the

environment that is being measured. With no formally established methods for handling tied raw

scores, the kinds of SDS scoring limitations that were illustrated in Table 1 and Table 2 are

similarly characteristic of the PCI. Spokane, et al. (2002) additionally point out that users of the

PCI should remain aware that this method for classifying work environments relies on subjective

judgment of a particular position rather than on a broader, objective analysis of an occupation or

occupational group.

The Occupational Information Network

A comparison of the O*NET-SOC 2000 database to the O*NET-SOC 2006 database

indicates that changes were made to the manner in which Holland summary codes are reported.

In some instances, codes were extended, shortened, or reordered. An inquiry via electronic mail

with the National Center for O*NET Development customer service department resulted in an

explanation that code lengths were modified to include Holland types that have an OIP standard
Is Congruence Dead? 32

score of at least 50. Codes were reordered to reflect the alphabetical ordering of types with tied

OIP scores (personal communication, January 31, 2007). An inspection of the O*NET-SOC

2006 database resulted in the identification of cases where these updated decision rules were

inconsistently applied. Moreover, as the O*NET is scheduled for major updates twice annually

(Data Publication Schedule, n.d.), it is apparent that it was developed to be a continuously

reviewed interactive internet application rather than as a stable research tool.

Compatibility of Holland Code Classification Systems

There generally exists the assumption that Holland work environment classification

systems should agree. As these methods theoretically generate the same work environment data,

one might rely on the concept of convergent validity (Campbell & Fiske, 1959) and simply

assume that these systems indeed are compatible (Eggerth, et. al, 2005). Research suggests that

this is far from the case. Eggerth, et al. investigated DHOC, SII, and O*NET agreement rates by

comparing the same or very similar occupational titles. Three-way first-letter agreement was

60.21%. Three-way first- and second-letter agreement dropped to 15.71%. The first, second- and

third-letter percentage agreement rate among all three of the taxonomies was found to be only

2.62%. In a study with similar results, Lent and Lopez (1996) used both the DHOC and the EAT

to generate work environment Holland codes for two different samples. The correlational data

indicated that congruence levels were the lowest when the same congruence index was used

between the DHOC and the EAT.

Work Environment: Instrument Alternatives

Strong Interest Inventory General Occupational Theme Codes

Dik, et al. (2007) used the O*NET, the DHOC, and the GOTS of the SII to generate three

sets of Holland work environment codes. Pairwise comparisons indicated that the mean
Is Congruence Dead? 33

congruence values associated with the GOTs were higher that those of both the O*NET and the

DHOC. The authors conclude that the GOT codes should serve as the “codes of choice” when

assigning work environment codes in congruence research. A perusal of the Strong Interest

Inventory manual (Donnay, et al., 2005; pp. 114-120) demonstrates that the GOT codes are

developed with a methodology that reflects the basic tenets of Holland’s theory of working

adults, is an empirically-derived and standardized application of the RIASEC scheme, and limits

potential error.

MEASUREMENT OF CONGRUENCE

Secondary Assumptions of Holland’s Theory

The Hexagonal Model of RIASEC Types

The psychological similarities and differences among the RIASEC types can be

represented graphically by an equilateral hexagon that plots the six types in a clockwise circular

order (see Figure 1). The hexagonal model is essential in understanding the congruence

construct.

Each of the six types appears at one point on the perimeter of the hexagon in a manner

that is intended to reflect a predictable ordering of the types and the magnitude of the

relationships among them. The hexagonal distances among the types are inversely proportional

to the theorized relationships between them. The types that appear closest together in the

hexagon are considered to be psychologically related, and types that are further removed from

one another are considered to be psychologically different (Tracey & Rounds, 1996, 1997).
Is Congruence Dead? 34

Figure 1

Hexagonal Model of Holland Types

Holland’s hexagon has been discussed in the literature as a model that meets the

assumptions of a circumplex (Arnold, 2004; Rounds, Tracey, & Hubert, 1992; Tinsley, 2000).

Guttman (1954) defines a circumplex as a circular order of components where adjacent

components around the circle are equally correlated, alternate components are equally correlated,

and opposite components are equally correlated. Correlations between the adjacent components

are greater than the correlations between the alternate components, and the correlations between

alternate components are greater than correlations between opposite components. In Holland’s

model of RIASEC types as it applies to the definition of a circumplex, equal segments lie

adjacent on the hexagon (e.g., R-I), alternate segments are separated by an intermediate type

(e.g., R-A), and opposite segments are separated by two intervening types (e.g., R-S). Table 3

illustrates all segments of the hexagon as they are considered respectively to be equal, alternate,

and opposite.
Is Congruence Dead? 35

Table 3

Equal, Alternate, and Opposite Segments of Holland’s Hexagonal Model

Equal Segments R-I, R-C, I-R, I-A, A-I, A-S, S-A, S-E, E-S, E-C, C-E, C-R

Alternate Segments R-A, R-E, I-S, IC, A-R, A-E, S-I. S-C, E-A, E-R, C-S, C-I

Opposite Segments R-S, I-E, A-C, S-R, E-I, C-A

If the model were plotted to scale and drawn to reflect the empirically-derived

correlations among the types, the figure would resemble a misshapen polygon rather than an

equilateral hexagon. Most vocational psychologists within the field agree that evidence supports

the roughly hexagonal circumplex arrangement of Holland’s RIASEC types (Bobele, Alston,

Wakefield, & Schnitzen, 1975; Borgen & Donnay, 1996; Cole, Whitney, & Holland, 1971;

Dawis, 1992; Edwards & Whitney, 1972; Holland, Whitney, Cole, & Richards, 1969; Khan,

Alvi, & Kirkwood, 1990; Prediger, 1982, Rounds, 1995; Spokane & Cruza-Guet, 2005; Toenjes

and Borgen, (1974; Wakefield & Doughtie, 1973). The hexagon generally is accepted as an

adequate “approximation of reality” (Prediger, 2000) and as close enough to lend strength to the

veracity of Holland’s congruence concept.

Consistency

Holland uses the term consistency to refer to the fact that there are differing degrees of

relatedness among the RIASEC types. Consistency is determined according to the properties of

the circumplex model. RIASEC types that are identical (e.g., I-I) of course represent maximum

consistency. Adjacent types on the perimeter of the hexagon (e.g., I-A) reflect the next highest

level of consistency. Alternate types on the perimeter of the hexagon (e.g., I-S) represent

moderate consistency. Types that lie opposite each other on the hexagon (e.g., I-E) are
Is Congruence Dead? 36

considered to reflect the lowest degree of consistency among the six types (i.e., inconsistency).

Table 4, which is directly comparable to Table 3, illustrates the segments of the hexagon

according to their level of consistency.

Table 4

Holland’s Hexagonal Model According to Levels of Consistency

Most Consistent (Identical) R-R I-I, A-A, S-S, E-E, C-C

Consistent (Equal/Adjacent) R-I, R-C, I-R, I-A, A-I, A-S, S-A, S-E, E-S,

E-C, C-E, C-R

Moderately Consistent (Alternate) R-A, R-E, I-S, IC, A-R, A-E, S-I. S-C, E-A,

E-R, C-S, C-I

Inconsistent (Opposite) R-S, I-E, A-C, S-R, E-I, C-A

Measurement of Congruence: Congruence Indexes

The earliest studies of congruence involved simply determining whether the individual’s

primary type matched the work environment’s primary type (Holland, 1963). This first-letter

agreement index takes a dichotomous approach in the assessment of congruence, assuming that

congruence either is or is not present. The contemporary research paradigm places congruence

on a continuum, measuring the degree of fit between vocational personality and work

environment.

Quite a few mathematical indexes have been developed for the purpose of measuring

congruence (e.g., Brown & Gore, 1994; Gati, 1985; Grotevant, Cooper, & Kramer, 1986; Healy

& Mourton, 1983; Holland, 1973; Iachan 1984, 1990; Kwak & Pulvino, 1982; Moody, 1983,

Robbins, Thomas, Harvey, & Kandefer, 1978; Swaney & Prediger, 1985; Wiggins & Moody,
Is Congruence Dead? 37

1981; Wolfe & Betz, 1981; Zener & Schnuelle, 1972). Six of these indexes have been selected

for discussion here as they appear to be the ones most well-known and most often used in

congruence research: (1) the First Letter Hexagonal Distance index (Holland), (2) the

Compatibility index (Wiggins & Moody), (3) the K-P index (Kwak & Pulvino), (4) the M index

(Iachan), (5) the Sb Index (Gati), and (6) the C index (Brown & Gore).

First Letter Hexagonal Distance Index

The First Letter Hexagonal Distance (FLHD; Holland, 1973) index uses the hexagonal

model and degrees of consistency (see Table 4) to determine congruence values. There are a total

of four possible levels of congruence. The highest level of congruence signifies that the primary

vocational personality type exactly matches the primary work environment type (e.g., C – C).

The second highest level of congruence is represented when the primary vocational personality

type and the primary work environment type are adjacent (e.g., S – E). A third, and lesser, level

of congruence occurs when the primary vocational personality type appears alternate on the

hexagon to the primary work environment type (e.g., R – A). The primary vocational personality

type is considered to be incongruent with the primary work environment type when the types lie

opposite each other on the hexagon (e.g., I – E). The FLHD index commonly is operationalized

on an integer scale ranging from 1 to 4, where higher values designate a greater degree of

congruence.

Compatibility Index

The Compatibility Index (CI; Wiggins & Moody, 1981; Moody, 1983) initially was

developed by Wiggins and Weslander (1979) as an expansion of the Z-S index (Zener &

Schnuelle, 1972) and was intended as a measure of compatibility between married couples.

Wiggins and Moody (1981) then broadened the use of the CI to vocational purposes. In
Is Congruence Dead? 38

congruence research, a CI score is derived by assessing the degree of fit between any two three-

letter Holland codes using a 9-point scale that is weighted according to the relative positioning of

the types. Decision rules are used to assign congruence values that range from 0 to 8, where

higher scores are associated with greater congruence (see Appendix A).

K-P Index

Kwak and Pulvino (1982) developed the K-P index for the assessment of congruence

across all three letters of Holland summary codes. The formula for the index reflects the relative

importance of type positioning, using rationally-derived weights of 4, 2, and 1, respectively. The

first-letter position is calculated as twice as important as the second-letter position, which is

calculated as twice as important as the third-letter position. The K-P index also incorporates the

circumplex assumption in its formula through the use of empirically-determined, hexagonal

segment distance proportion values that originally were derived by Holland, et al. (1969). K-P

index congruence scores range from 0 to 1, where values closer to 1 represent greater congruence

(see Appendix B).

M Index

The K-P index prompted Iachan (1984, 1990) to create the M index as a method that

could be used for a wider range of purposes. The M index can derive congruence values between

two two-letter summary codes or between two three-letter summary codes. Congruence scores

are calculated by summing rationally-derived weights of pairs of matching types within two

Holland codes. Numerical weights are assigned based on the importance of the relative

positioning of pairs of matched types. For example, a match between types in the first-letter

position is considered to be more important (i.e., more congruent) than a match between types in

the first-letter and third-letter positions. Three-letter code comparison congruence scores range
Is Congruence Dead? 39

from 0 to 28, and two-letter code comparison congruence scores range from 0 to 6, where higher

values represent greater congruence (see Appendix C).

Sb Index

The Sb index (Gati, 1985) incorporates the use of a 0 – 1 vector approach and subjective

judgment in order to address potential problems related to tied profile scores and qualitative

issues among profiles (e.g., spiked scores, flat profiles). The index takes into account the relative

number of and distances between types that are both common and unique to each code. The

index assumes certain distance values ranging between 1 and 3 based on the principles of

consistency. These values reflect that more proximal types are associated with lower values. The

Sb index incorporates these distances in order to assign higher values to pairs of codes that have

relatively more shared types and relatively fewer unshared types, or fewer unshared types that

are relatively distant. Scores can range from 0 to 5, where higher values represent greater

congruence (see Appendix D).

C Index

Brown and Gore (1994) assessed the strengths and limitations of 9 well-known

congruence indexes (i.e., Gati, 1985; Healy & Mourton, 1983; Holland, 1963; Iachan, 1984;

Kwak & Pulvino, 1982; Robbins, et al., 1978; Wiggins & Moody, 1981; Wolfe & Betz, 1981;

Zener & Schnuelle, 1972). Only the K – P index was determined to be a congruence index that

both incorporates the circumplex assumption and is sensitive enough to discriminate between

identical but out-of-order codes. The shape of the K – P score distributions was found to be

positively skewed, and the index was noted as being difficult to calculate, however. Brown and

Gore developed the C index to retain the advantages of the K-P index as well as to symmetrically

distribute scores and to simplify computation.


Is Congruence Dead? 40

The formula for the C index uses the weights 3, 2, and 1 to calculate the first-letter

position as 1.5 times as important as the second-letter position, which is calculated as twice as

important as the third-letter position. A congruence value is obtained by multiplying the weights

by rationally-derived hexagonal distances between sequential pairs of types. Identical matches

are assigned a distance value equal to 3. Adjacent segments are assigned a distance value equal

to 2, alternate segments are assigned a distance value equal to 1, and opposite segments are

assigned a distance value of 0. The values then are summed. C index scores can range from 0 to

18, where a higher value represents a greater level of congruence (see Appendix E).

Measurement of Congruence: Congruence Index Limitations

A review of congruence indexes illustrates that these measures have been developed with

increasing mathematical precision. Despite this, the computation of congruence remains an area

of great concern. Research illustrates the index considered to be the best measure of the construct

remains debatable (Miller, 1992; Osipow, 1987; Osipow & Fitzgerald, 1996; Rounds, et al.,

2000). Camp and Chartrand (1992) evaluated 13 congruence indexes and conclude that none of

them is “completely satisfactory.” In a similar study, Young, Tokar, and Subich (1998) found no

significant relationships among the possible combinations of 11 congruence indexes and two job

satisfaction measures. While Assouline and Meir (1987) and Meir (1995) indicate that

congruence correlations are higher when more complex congruence indexes are used, Tsabari, et

al. (2005) showed that more sophisticated indexes were associated with weaker mean

correlations. These seemingly ambiguous findings become clearer when it is considered that all

of the most well-known congruence indexes fail to incorporate the circumplex assumption and/or

are limited to the use of three-letter Holland summary codes.


Is Congruence Dead? 41

Failure to Incorporate the Circumplex Assumption

It has been noted frequently that the indexes considered to best operationalize congruence

are the ones that integrate the circumplex assumption. Camp and Chartrand (1992) assert that

congruence indexes that do not include the circumplex assumption should not be used in

Holland-type congruence studies. Tracey and Rounds (1992) similarly state that important

information inherent in Holland’s theory is lost when congruence indexes ignore hexagonal

properties. Moreover, Gati (1989) indicates congruence studies that do not take into account the

circumplex assumption may blur meaningful correlations.

Limitation to Three-Letter Holland Summary Codes

Congruence indexes that incorporate three-letter codes in their calculations cannot be

used in research with methods such as the SII and the O*NET make use of summary codes that

vary in length. Moreover, congruence indexes that include three-letter codes force the use of

measures such as the SDS, the VPI, the DHOC, the OF, and the PCI. Given the concerns

associated with these measures that already have been discussed, congruence indexes that use

three-letter codes necessarily limit and potentially may compromise valid research from the very

beginning of the process.

Measurement of Congruence: Index Alternatives

The Modified C Index

Despite the fact that Brown and Gore’s C index is limited to the use of three-letter

Holland codes, it has been the congruence index that most often is recommended for use in

research (Holland, 1997; Tinsley, 2000). Eggerth, et al. (2005) note work needs to be done to

design a congruence index that both reflects the mathematical precision of the C index and has
Is Congruence Dead? 42

the ability to compare Holland codes of differing lengths. In response to this, Eggerth and

Andrew (2006) developed the modified C index.

The modified C index is based on the same rationally derived weights, hexagonal

distance values, and scoring range that are used by the C index. It assumes that codes of unequal

lengths are, in fact, equivalent in their quantity of descriptive information. Relatively shorter

codes merely reflect a greater “concentration” of information where the types that are included in

the code hold the information of the “missing” types. Eggerth and Andrew (2006) provide

equations for six possible combinations of code comparisons: (1) three letters x three letters, (2)

two letters x two letters, (3) one letter x one letter, (4) three letters x one letter, (5) two letters x

one letter, and (6) three letters x two letters (see Appendix F).

Support for the Modified C Index

Preliminary research on the modified C index supports the efficacy of its use. Dik and

Hansen (2004) compared mean congruence scores using both the C index and the modified C

index. Statistical analyses suggested that the modified C index was associated with slightly, yet

significantly, higher mean congruence scores. Dik and Hansen also examined the performance of

the modified C index, the C index, and the K-P index by comparing correlations between

congruence and overall job satisfaction and between congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction.

The correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction statistically was highest when

measured by the modified C index. The correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction also was highest in favor of the modified C index, but those results did not reach

statistical significance. Dik and Hansen conclude that the modified C index should serve as the

“congruence index of choice” when conducting research on Holland’s theory.


Is Congruence Dead? 43

The Substitution C Index

Despite the theoretical and empirical support the modified C index has received, Gore

and Brown (2006) do not endorse its use, indicating that the modified C index is unnecessarily

computationally complex. Gore and Brown suggest a different index that is easier to calculate

while remaining logically sound. This alternative extends shorter codes to the three-letter length

by using the type in the first-letter position as a substitute for any “missing” types in the code.

The C index (Brown & Gore, 1994) formula then can be applied to compute congruence scores

to cases that are commensurate with those of Eggerth and Andrew (2006) (see Appendix G).

Support for the Substitution C Index

Gore and Brown (2006) compare scores derived by the modified C index and by the

substitution C index to show that the two congruence indexes yield very similar, and in some

cases identical, congruence values. Eggerth (2006) responds to this by pointing out that the

substitution C index assigns exaggerated influence to the first-letter position and is not consistent

with what Brown and Gore (1994) originally proposed. Eggerth moreover indicates that the

modified C index takes into account the contribution of types in the second-letter position,

asserting that the modified C index thus is more logically consistent with the C index. Eggerth

concludes by indicating “ultimately the question of whether the greater mathematical rigor of

Eggerth and Andrew will outweigh the computational ease of Gore and Brown is one that must

be answered empirically” (p. 290). This research therefore uses both the modified C index and

the substitution C index to generate congruence scores.

JOB SATISFACTION

It has been noted extensively that job satisfaction is a complex construct that evades

definition as a singular term (Herr, Cramer, & Niles, 2004; Herzberg, Mausner, Peterson, &
Is Congruence Dead? 44

Capwell, 1957; Locke, 1969; Strong, 1958; Wanous & Lawler, 1972; Warr, 1991). There does

seem to be general agreement, though, that job satisfaction is an affective state that results from

an individual’s overall assessment of job experiences (Dawis, 1984; Fritzsche & Parrish, 2005;

Kalleberg, 1977; Locke, 1983). Given that the construct of job satisfaction is quite broad, many

methods have been developed for its measurement. The five most frequently used techniques

will be detailed here: (1) the Hoppock Job Satisfaction Blank, (2) the Job Descriptive Index and

the Job in General scale, (3) the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire, (4) one-item measures of

overall job satisfaction, and (5) measures developed by the research authors.

Job Satisfaction: Instruments

The Hoppock Job Satisfaction Blank

The Hoppock Job Satisfaction Blank (HJSB; Hoppock, 1935) is a global (i.e., overall)

measure of job satisfaction that determine an individual’s likes and dislikes for a job. Four

questions assess (1) how much an individual likes the job, (2) how much of the time the

individual feels satisfied with the job, (3) how the individual feels about changing jobs, and (4)

how the individual compares self to others regarding liking or disliking the job. Each item is

scored according to a 7-point Likert-type scale (e.g., I hate it – I love it, All of the time – Never).

The HJSB yields total scores that range between 4 and 28 where higher scores indicate a greater

feeling of job satisfaction.

The Job Descriptive Index and the Job in General Scale

The Job Descriptive Index originally was developed by Smith, et al., (1969). The JDI was

revised in 1990 and again in 1997 (Baltzer, et al., 1997; Baltzer & Smith, 1990). The current

version of the instrument provides measures of satisfaction on five scales: (1) Work on Present

Job, (2) Present Pay, (3) Opportunities for Promotion, (4) Supervision, and (5) Co-Workers.
Is Congruence Dead? 45

Instrument items consist of a list of adjectives or short phrases pertaining to the various scales.

Respondents indicate whether the adjective or phrase describes their job and are provided with

the response choices Y (i.e., yes), ? (i.e., undecided), and N (i.e., no). Positive items are scored as

3, 1, and 0, respectively, and negative items are scored as 0, 1, and 3, respectively. In 1989,

Ironson, et al. developed the Job in General scale to supplement the JDI scales. The JIG scale is a

measure of overall job satisfaction and is included in the 1997 revision of the JDI (Baltzer, et al.,

1997). JIG items and scoring procedures are similar to the JDI, yielding total scores that range

between 0 and 54 where higher scores indicate a greater feeling of job satisfaction.

The Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire

The Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (MSQ; Weiss, et al. 1967) measures the degree

to which an individual perceives that a job satisfies vocational needs and values. Both a long-

form MSQ and a short-form MSQ are available. The long-form consists of 120 items that

measure job satisfaction according to a 20-item general job satisfaction scale and 20 scales each

containing five items. The more popular short-form MSQ is composed of 20 items that are taken

from the long-form. Each item is scored according to a 5-point Likert-type scale where response

choices are Very Dissatisfied (i.e., 1), Dissatisfied (i.e., 2), Neither (i.e., 3), Satisfied (i.e., 4), and

Very Satisfied (i.e., 5). The short-form MSQ yields a general job satisfaction score that ranges

between 20 and 100 where higher scores indicate a greater feeling of job satisfaction. The short-

form additionally breaks down into an intrinsic job satisfaction scale, where scores range

between 12 and 60, and an extrinsic job satisfaction scale, where scores range between 6 and 30.

One-Item Measures of Overall Job Satisfaction

The use of one-item overall job satisfaction measures is popular in congruence – job

satisfaction research. These items typically are assessed on a Likert-type scale and include
Is Congruence Dead? 46

questions such as “How satisfied are you with your vocational choice?” (Aranya, et al., 1981),

“I am very satisfied with my current job.” (Heesacker, Elliott, & Howe, 1988), “To what extent

are you satisfied with your crew your place of work / educational institution / residential unit?”

(Meir, et al., 1986; Meir, Hadas, & Noyfeld, 1997).

Measures Developed by the Research Authors

Measures developed for specific congruence – job satisfaction studies are as varied as the

researchers who have designed them. LaBarbera (2005), for example, developed a 14-item

survey for a sample of Physician Assistants to evaluate factors such as demographics and

attitudes towards overall career as well as specialty choice. Meir and colleagues developed a 20-

item survey that has been used and modified in a series of studies that examine different

occupational samples. This instrument also assesses both occupational and job-specific

satisfaction (Meir & Green-Eppel, 1999; Meir & Navon, 1992; Meir & Segal-Halevi, 2001;

Meir, Tziner, & Glazner, 1997; Meir & Yaari, 1988).

Measurement of Job Satisfaction: Instrument Limitations

Measures of Unknown Reliability and Validity

Hulin and Judge (2003) and Betz and Fitzgerald (1987) point out that much of the

congruence - job satisfaction research has been based on “homegrown” and “homemade”

measures of satisfaction. Locke (1983) similarly indicates that it is common for congruence

research to use arbitrarily chosen operational definitions of job satisfaction where the definition

of satisfaction is not part of a predetermined methodology, but instead is defined by the measure

that happens to be used. Specific congruence research examples that reflect this include Swaney

and Prediger (1985), who discuss that a reliability estimate was not available for their job
Is Congruence Dead? 47

satisfaction measure, and Heesacker, et al. (1988), who state that reliability and validity data for

their job satisfaction instrument was not available.

One-Item Measures

Betz and Fitzgerald (1987) assert that the common use of one-item measures of job

satisfaction is an “unfortunate practice”. Single-item measures have been shown to be associated

with low reliability and restricted research findings (Quinn, Staines, & McCullough, 1974;

Rounds, et al., 2000). Much congruence – job satisfaction studies that use one-item measures of

job satisfaction cite the meta-analysis conducted by Wanous, Reichers, and Hudy (1997) where

single-item measures were found to be valid. Wanous, et al. also point out that there are good

reasons to prefer job satisfaction scales to single items.

Measurement of Overall Job Satisfaction

The use of global measures is perhaps the largest criticism of job satisfaction in

congruence research. It has been noted widely that job satisfaction is a broad construct with

various components. It is best assessed with a multifaceted approach as overall satisfaction

measures confound the contributions of particular job satisfaction features (Dawis, 1984, 1991;

Edwards, 1991; Hansen, 2005; Kahn; 1972). Moreover, even though the division of job

satisfaction into intrinsic and extrinsic components is well-supported by research and leads to

more precise congruence information, it is infrequently found in the research (Betz, et al.; 1989;

Elton & Smart, 1988).

Measurement of Job Satisfaction: Instrument Alternative

Short-Form Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire

The short-form MSQ (Weiss, et al. 1967) has been extensively researched and is

recognized for its internal consistency and construct validity (Hulin & Judge, 2003; Prediger,
Is Congruence Dead? 48

2000). Morris (2003) conducted a meta-analysis of studies evaluating the congruence – job

satisfaction relationship and found the mean effect size was larger among studies where the MSQ

was used to measure job satisfaction. Factor analysis additionally supports the division of the

MSQ into its intrinsic and extrinsic scales (Weiss, et al. 1967). This research therefore uses the

short-form MSQ to measure the intrinsic, extrinsic, and overall components of job satisfaction.

SUMMARY OF CHAPTER TWO

Chapter Two detailed the methodological limitations characteristic of congruence – job

satisfaction correlation research and discussed appropriate measurement alternatives. The

rationale for the comparison of two Holland summary coding methods and two congruence

indexes and the investigation of three aspects of job satisfaction was presented.

The six vocational personality types were outlined. The Self-Directed Search, the

Vocational Preference Inventory, and the Strong Interest Inventory were discussed as the major

vocational personality assessments that were used in congruence research. Construct validity,

traditionally-derived scoring issues, and practical concerns were described. The Strong Interest

Explorer was presented as the measurement option used in this research. Research on guidelines

for developing decision-derived Holland summary codes was described. It was identified that the

research compares traditionally-derived codes with decision-derived codes that are generated

with a cut standard score method.

The six work environments were described. The Dictionary of Holland Occupational

Codes, the Occupations Finder, the Environmental Assessment Technique, the Position

Classification Inventory, and the Occupational Information Network were discussed as popular

work environment assessment methods. Conceptual and practical problems and a misassumption

regarding the interchangeability of these measures were presented. The SII General Occupational
Is Congruence Dead? 49

Theme (GOT) codes were identified as the work environment assessment approach that was used

in this research.

Holland’s circumplex model and consistency secondary theoretical assumptions were

explained. The First Letter Hexagonal Distance index, the Compatibility index, the K-P index,

the M index, the Sb Index, and the C index were identified as well-known congruence indexes.

The methodological weaknesses of these indexes were discussed. The modified C index and the

substitution C index were identified and presented as alternative congruence measures that are

compared in this research.

Job satisfaction was identified as an affective state that results from the overall

assessment of job experiences. The Hoppock Job Satisfaction Blank, the Job Descriptive Index

and the Job in General scale, the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire, one-item measures of

overall job satisfaction, and measures developed by the research authors were discussed as

frequently used job satisfaction assessment techniques. Validity and reliability issues associated

with these methods were described. It was explained that this research uses the short-form

Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire to measure intrinsic, extrinsic, and overall job satisfaction.
Is Congruence Dead? 50

CHAPTER THREE: METHODOLOGY

INTRODUCTION TO CHAPTER THREE

Chapter Three presents the methodological components that were used to implement this

study. General research sample characteristics first are explained. The instruments used in this

research then are described. Details of participant recruitment and data gathering are discussed.

Methods of data evaluation and the statistical analysis of data are reviewed.

GENERAL SAMPLE CHARACTERISTICS

A total of twelve occupational groups, 2 per primary Holland type, were pre-selected for

examination in this study according to the Strong Interest Inventory General Occupational

Theme codes (Donnay, et al., 2005; Harmon, Hansen, Borgen, & Hammer, 1994). The groups

were selected in a manner that served not only as the measure of work environment, but also as

an accounting for the fact that GOT codes are gender-normed. The standardized scores that were

generated with the SIE data are based on a mixed-gender sample. The 12 occupational groups

therefore were chosen in order to reflect pairs of corresponding male and female GOT codes that

are as equal as practically possible. The five steps taken to select the groups are detailed in

Appendix H. A listing of the occupational groups examined in this study is presented in Table 5.

MEASURES

Strong Interest Explorer

The Strong Interest Explorer (SIE; Chartrand, 2001) is made-up of a total of 140 items.

Sixty items were drawn from the 1994 SII (Harmon, et al., 1994), 33 items were generated from

the 2002 research on the SII, 35 items were written specifically for the SIE by the CPP research

staff, and 12 items were modified from either 1994 SII items or 2002 SII research items

(Research Department, 2002).


Is Congruence Dead? 51

Table 5

Occupational Groups Included in the Research Sample

Holland Type SII Occupational Group GOT Code

Realistic Forester RI (female)


RI (male)

Engineer RI (female)
RI (male)

Investigative Medical Technologist IRC (female)


IRC (male)

Psychologist IA (female)
IA (male)

Artistic Attorney A (female)


A (male)

Librarian A (female)
A (male)

Social Licensed Practical Nurse SCE (female)


SCE (male)

Social Worker SA (female)


SA (male)

Enterprising Florist EAC (female)


EAC (male)

Real Estate Agents E (female)


E (male)

Conventional Accountant CE (female)


CE (male)

Banker CE (female)
CE (male)
Is Congruence Dead? 52

Items include job titles, occupationally- related activities, and school subjects that are endorsed

based on whether the respondent “likes an item” (see Appendix I). The selected items are

summed and yield raw scores for 14 basic interest areas each containing 10 items. The areas are

commensurate with the RIASEC typology so that raw scores then can be combined to generate a

Holland summary code (see Appendix J).

The SIE was administered to samples of college students, high school students, and

employed adults in order to establish initial reliability and validity estimates. In most cases, all

basic interest scales across the three groups had internal consistency alpha levels of at least .80.

Validity was evaluated with the employed adults only. Members of this group were administered

a supplemental, self-expressed interests questionnaire in addition to the SIE. Results indicated a

significant and positive mean correlation of .55 between self-expressed interests and the SIE

scales. More detailed information regarding SIE reliability and validity can be found in Research

Department (2002).

The Short-Form Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire

The 20 Likert-type items of the short-form MSQ (Weiss, et al., 1967) represent the ones

that have been found to be the most highly correlated with their respective long-form scales. The

instrument additionally includes short-answer type demographic questions (see Appendix K).

Norms for the short-form MSQ were developed with data from 7 occupational samples of

varying sizes. Median reliability coefficients obtained for the three scales across all occupational

samples were .90 for General Satisfaction (range .87 to .92), .86 for Intrinsic Satisfaction (range

.84 to .91), and .80 for Extrinsic Satisfaction (range .77 to .82). Additional information regarding

short-form MSQ reliability and validity can be found in Lofquist and Dawis (1969) and in Weiss,

Dawis, England, and Lofquist (1966).


Is Congruence Dead? 53

PARTICIPANT RECRUITMENT

The participants for this study were recruited largely through their association with

commensurate professional organizations and convenience sampling methods (see Appendix L).

Membership lists that were posted on the internet and considered to be public information were

identified. Individuals on these lists were contacted based on a random number generator

approach. Among the businesses that were identified, all employees were invited to participate in

this study. Potential participants were mailed packets containing an introductory cover letter

reflecting research interest in their particular occupational group (see Appendix M), two copies

of the Consent for Participation in a Study of Work Interests and Job Satisfaction form (see

Appendix N), a short-form MSQ, a copy of the SIE, and a self-addressed, stamped return

envelope. Most participants who did not return their packets were mailed at least two follow-up,

reminder letters at two to three week intervals (see Appendix O).

The professional organization lists and businesses were sampled and data packets were

mailed to potential participants until usable data from at least 15 participants per occupational

group, 30 participants per RIASEC primary type, were received. Research information was

received from all 48 continuous states except South Dakota. The overall response rate for valid

data received was approximately 25.80%, with the percentage per occupational group ranging

between 18.75% (Accountants, Bankers) and 36.67% (Medical Technologists) (see Table 6).

Complete data were analyzed for a total of 180 participants. If the usable data per occupational

group exceeded 15 subjects, “extra” cases were eliminated from analysis in a manner that

minimized occurrence of missing data.


Is Congruence Dead? 54

Table 6

Occupational Group Usable Date Response Rates

Occupational Group Response Rate

Accountant 18.75%

Attorney 20.0%

Banker 18.75%

Engineer 26.03%

Florist 25.0%

Forester 35.71%

Librarian 32.73%

Licensed Practical Nurse 24.59%

Medical Technologist 36.67%

Psychologist 26.56%

Real Estate Agents 24.66%

Social Worker 25.42%

DATA EVALUATION

Instrument Scoring

The MSQ was hand scored according to technical manual guidelines. In order to generate

complete scale scores for the intrinsic, extrinsic, and overall job satisfaction scales, missing data

on MSQ items were assigned a score of 3 (i.e., Neither). This procedure was implemented as the

majority of missing data reflected respondents marking “NA” by the item and indicating that the
Is Congruence Dead? 55

item was not salient to them (e.g., a self-employed participant with no direct supervisor). This

method was used for a total of 43 missing values.

The SIE was hand scored according to instrument guidelines. SIE traditionally-derived

summary codes were calculated according to the Holland types that corresponded to the three

highest raw scores. A list of rules for resolving tied scores was created and followed according to

the secondary assumption of consistency (see Appendix P). Tied scores were resolved among

traditionally-derived codes in 90 cases.

In order to generate SIE decision-derived codes, it was necessary to first create

standardized scores for each Holland type using a z-score to T-score transformation method.

Standardized scores then were used to generate decision-derived codes based on Dik et al.’s

(2007) standardized cut score method anchored by a mean T-score of 60. Rules that were used

for generating decision-derived codes are presented in Appendix Q. Rules for breaking ties

among decision-derived codes were resolved according to the same consistency guidelines that

are presented in Appendix P. Tied scores were resolved among decision-derived codes in 14

cases.

Participants’ traditionally-derived codes and decision-derived codes are presented

according to primary Holland type in Table 7. Traditionally-derived codes and decision-derived

codes generated identical overall hit rates of 38.33% when the primary Holland types of the

participants’ summary codes were compared to the primary Holland types of the GOT codes.

An analysis of traditionally-derived codes by occupational group generated a range of hit

rates between 20.0% (Florist, Psychologist) and 86.67% (Social Worker). An analysis of

decision-derived codes by occupational group generated a range of hit rates between 20.0%

(Florists) and 66.67% (Social Workers) (see Table 8). An analysis of traditionally-derived codes
Is Congruence Dead? 56

by primary Holland type generated a range of hit rates between 23.33% (Enterprising) and

76.67% (Social). An analysis of decision-derived codes by primary Holland type generated a

range of hit rates between 33.33% (Enterprising) and 40.0% (Artistic, Social) (see Table 9).

Table 7

Traditionally-Derived Codes and Decision-Derived Codes According to Primary Holland Type

Traditionally-Derived Decision-Derived
Summary Codes Summary Codes
R I A S E C R I A S E C
Realistic
Engineer 5 4 6 5 4 1 5
Forester 8 2 2 3 6 2 1 1 5
Investigative
Medical Technologist 3 5 2 4 1 3 6 3 2 1
Psychologist 2 3 1 9 1 5 3 6
Artistic
Attorney 2 4 3 6 1 5 1 8
Librarian 1 5 7 2 7 3 1 4
Social
Licensed Practical Nurse 2 1 10 2 2 1 3 4 5
Social Worker 1 1 13 1 3 10 1
Enterprising
Florist 3 1 5 2 3 1 1 2 5 2 3 2
Real Estate Agent 3 2 2 4 4 3 2 1 1 7 1
Conventional
Accountant 4 5 1 5 2 6 1 6
Banker 1 3 7 4 2 2 4 2 5

Bold indicates hits between summary code primary types and GOT code primary types

An examination of summary coding methods indicates that the conversion from

traditionally-derived codes to decision-derived codes resulted in some changes among the

frequencies of primary Holland types (see Table 10). There was an increase in the frequency of

Investigative, Artistic, Enterprising, and Conventional primary types ranging between 5.0%
Is Congruence Dead? 57

(Investigative) and 6.11% (Enterprising) while there was a 4.44% decrease in Realistic primary

Holland types and a 17.78% decrease in Social primary Holland types (see Table 11).

Table 8

Occupational Group Hit Rates Between Summary Code Primary Types and GOT Code Primary
Types

Traditionally-Derived Decision-Derived
Summary Codes Summary Codes
Accountant 33.33% 40.0%
Attorney 26.67% 33.33%
Banker 26.67% 33.33%
Engineer 33.33% 33.33%
Florist 20.0% 20.0%
Forester 53.33% 40.0%
Librarian 33.33% 46.67%
Licensed Practical Nurse 66.67% 26.67%
Medical Technologist 33.33% 40.0%
Psychologist 20.0% 33.33%
Real Estate Agent 26.67% 46.67%
Social Worker 86.67% 66.67%

Table 9

Holland Type Hit Rates Between Summary Code Primary Types and GOT Code Primary Types

Traditionally-Derived Decision-Derived
Summary Codes Summary Codes
Realistic 43.33% 36.67%
Investigative 26.67% 36.67%
Artistic 30.0% 40.0%
Social 76.67% 40.0%
Enterprising 23.33% 33.33%
Conventional 30.0% 36.67%
Is Congruence Dead? 58

Table 10

Primary Holland Types by Coding Method

Traditionally-Derived Codes Decision-Derived Codes

Realistic n = 35 (19.44%) n = 27 (15.0%)

Investigative n = 21 (11.67%) n = 30 (16.67%)

Artistic n = 21 (11.67%) n = 31 (17.22%)

Social n = 65 (36.11%) n = 33 (18.33%)

Enterprising n = 14 (7.78%) n = 25 (13.89%)

Conventional n = 24 (13.33%) n = 34 (18.89%)

Total n = 180 (100%) n = 180 (100%)

Table 11

Primary Holland Types Changes after Conversion from Traditionally-Derived Codes to


Decision-Derived Codes

Realistic
4.44%
Investigative
5.0%
Artistic
5.55%
Social
17.78%
Enterprising
6.11%
Conventional
5.56%
Is Congruence Dead? 59

Statistical Analysis of Data

Study data were evaluated according to all cases, and results additionally were analyzed

by occupational group and by primary Holland type. All data analyses were conducted according

to the descriptive and inferential statistics that were appropriate for addressing the study’s

research questions.

Demographic Analysis

An analysis of overall demographic data was performed. Gender was assessed through a

frequency count procedure. Means, standard deviations, and values ranges for the variables Age,

Years of Education, Years on Present Job, Years in Occupation, and Hours Worked per week

were calculated. An analysis for significant differences in gender among occupational groups

and primary Holland types was conducted with the Chi Square Test of Independence. An

analysis for significant differences among the other demographic variables was conducted with a

one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA). Tukey's HSD post hoc test was conducted when

ANOVAs indicated significant differences.

MSQ Score Analysis

MSQ mean scores were analyzed according to intrinsic, extrinsic, and overall job

satisfaction for all data, for occupational groups, and for primary Holland types. An analysis for

significant mean MSQ score differences was conducted with a one-way Analysis of Variance

(ANOVA). Tukey's HSD post hoc test was performed when ANOVAs indicated significant

differences.

Congruence Score Analysis

Congruence mean scores were analyzed according to the modified C index, the

substitution C index, traditionally-derived Holland codes, and decision-derived Holland codes


Is Congruence Dead? 60

for all data, for occupational groups, and for primary Holland types. An analysis for significant

mean congruence score differences was conducted with a one-way Analysis of Variance

(ANOVA). Tukey's HSD post hoc test was performed when ANOVAs indicated significant

differences.

General Research Question Analysis

General research questions were analyzed according to all data, for occupational groups,

and for primary Holland types. General Research Question Sets 1, 2, 3, and 4 used the one-tailed,

Pearson Product Moment Correlation statistic to assess the specified relationships between

congruence and job satisfaction. General Research Question Set 5 and General Research

Question 6 used the Fisher’s r-to-z’ Transformation to assess significant differences among the

correlations found in General Research Question Sets 1, 2, 3, and 4.

SUMMARY OF CHAPTER THREE

Chapter Three presented the methodological components that were used to implement

this study. General research sample characteristics first were explained. The instruments used in

this research then were described. Details of participant recruitment and data gathering were

discussed. Methods of data evaluation and the statistical analysis of data were reviewed.
Is Congruence Dead? 61

CHAPTER FOUR: DATA ANALYSIS

INTRODUCTION TO CHAPTER FOUR

Chapter Four presents the research data analyzed according to all cases, occupational

groups, and primary Holland types. Demographic information first is evaluated. MSQ score and

congruence score data are analyzed and discussed. An analysis of this study’s general research

questions then is presented.

DEMOGRAPHIC ANALYSIS

Demographic data were obtained via MSQ short-answer questions. This information

includes the variables Gender, Age, Years of Education, Time on Present Job, Years in

Occupation, and Hours Worked Per Week. Data are presented according to all cases (see Table

12) and are further analyzed according to occupational group (see Table 13) and primary Holland

type (see Table 14).

Demographic Analysis for All Data

Demographic categories with missing cases were calculated according to the number of

cases that were reported. Mean data for all available cases indicated that, overall, the sample was

fairly evenly divided between men and women (Females = 96, Males = 84). Participants tended

to be of middle-age (M = 50.31, SD = 11.26), but with a range in ages between 24 years and 73

years. Overall, participants were college-educated or the equivalent (M = 16.80, SD = 2.57).

Mean data indicated that participants tended to be experienced in their occupations (M = 22.56,

SD = 11.65), but the range was wide, between 1 year and 50.17 years. Data indicated that

participants tended also to be experienced in their current positions (M = 13.06, SD = 10.85), the

range here also was wide, between 1 month and 42 years. Mean data indicated that participants
Is Congruence Dead? 62

predominantly worked full-time (M = 43.51, SD = 12.79), but again the range was quite wide,

between 5 and 80 hours per week.

Table 12

Demographic Data for All Cases

Gender Age Years of Years on Years in Hours


Education Present Job Occupation Worked
Per Week
Females 96 M = 50.31 b M = 16.80 M = 13.06 M = 22.56 a M = 43.51 c
Males 84 SD = 11.26 SD = 2.57 SD = 10.85 SD = 11.65 SD = 12.79

a
1 missing case
b
2 missing cases
c
7 missing cases

Demographic Analysis by Occupational Group

A Chi Square Test of Independence indicated a relationship between occupational group

and gender, x2 (11, n = 180) = 19.68, p < .05. The most significant differences occurred among

engineers, librarians, and licensed practical nurses. No engineers in the sample were female.

Only 1 librarian was male, and 13 licensed practical nurses were female.

An ANOVA for Age indicated significant differences, F(11, 166) = 3.01, p < .01. The

Tukey HSD revealed that Bankers were significantly younger than Accountants, Engineers,

Licensed Practical Nurses, Social Workers, and Medical Technologists.

An ANOVA for Years of Education indicated significant differences, F(11, 168) = 25.14,

p < .0001. The Tukey HSD revealed that Licensed Practical Nurses reported spending

significantly less time in school than all other occupational groups except Bankers and Florists.

As a group, Bankers, Florists, Medical Technologists and Real Estate Agents reported fewer

years of education when compared to Librarians, Social Workers, Foresters, Attorneys, and
Is Congruence Dead? 63

Psychologists. Engineers reported less time in school than Foresters, Attorneys, and

Psychologists.

An ANOVA for Time on Present Job indicated significant differences, F(11, 168) = 2.17,

p < .05. The Tukey HSD revealed that Florists and Accountants reported more time on their

present jobs when compared to Bankers.

An ANOVA for Years in Occupation indicated significant differences, F(11, 167) = 5.98,

p < .0001. The Tukey HSD revealed that Bankers reported less time spent in their occupations

when compared to Florists, Accountants, Licensed Practical Nurses, Engineers, and Medical

Technologists. Real Estate Agents reported less time spent in their occupations when compared

to Accountants, Licensed Practical Nurses, Engineers, and Medical Technologists. Psychologists

reported less time spent in their occupations when compared to Licensed Practical Nurses,

Engineers, and Medical Technologists.

An ANOVA for Hours Worked Per Week indicated significant differences, F(11, 161) =

2.45, p < .01. The Tukey HSD revealed that Florists reported working significantly more hours

when compared to Social Workers.

Demographic Analysis by Primary Holland Type

A Chi Square Test of Independence indicated a relationship between primary Holland

type and gender, x2 (5, n = 180) = 11.07, p < .05. The significant differences occurred among the

Realistic (Engineer, Forester) and Social (Licensed Practical Nurse, Social Worker) types.

Realistic types predominantly were male while Social types predominantly were female.

An ANOVA for Age indicated significant differences, F(5, 172) = 2.44, p < .05. The

Tukey HSD revealed that Social types (Licensed Practical Nurse, Social Worker) were

significantly older when compared with Conventional types (Accountant, Banker).


Is Congruence Dead? 64

Table 13

Demographic Data According to Occupational Group

Occupational Gender Age Years of Time on Years in Hours


Group Education Present Job Occupation Worked
Per Week
Accountant Females 6 M = 52.73 M = 16.80 M = 19.82 M = 27.17 a M = 39.64 a
Males 9 SD = 8.54 SD = 1.08 SD = 10.87 SD = 8.97 SD = 12.12
Attorney Females 5 M = 48.27 M = 19.13 M = 11.42 M = 20.26 M = 47.40
Males 10 SD = 12.58 SD = 0.35 SD = 9.28 SD = 12.58 SD = 16.94
Banker Females 10 M = 39.37 M = 14.87 M = 5.71 M = 13.79 M = 48.27
Males 5 SD = 13.07 SD = 2.47 SD = 7.49 SD = 12.94 SD =9.61
Engineer Females 0 M = 53.40 M = 16.67 M = 13.24 M = 28.98 M = 49.0
Males 15 SD = 8.68 SD = 1.05 SD = 9.67 SD = 8.31 SD = 6.03
Florist Females 8 M = 50.20 M = 14.90 M = 18.65 M = 26.22 M = 51.83
Males 7 SD = 7.83 SD = 1.73 SD = 13.58 SD = 9.93 SD = 11.79
Forester Females 4 M = 45.60 M = 18.73 M = 11.42 M = 17.14 M = 40.37
Males 11 SD = 9.62 SD = 1.98 SD = 9.57 SD = 8.01 SD = 3.31
Librarian Females 14 M = 49.87 M = 17.80 M = 9.68 M = 19.48 M = 45.0
Males 1 SD = 10.23 SD = 2.51 SD = 8.69 SD = 7.65 SD = 7.73
Licensed Females 13 M = 54.57 a M = 13.20 M = 13.46 M = 28.60 M = 43.62 b
Practical Nurse Males 2 SD = 12.31 SD = 0.77 SD = 13.73 SD = 14.34 SD = 11.27
Medical Females 10 M = 56.40 M = 15.33 M = 14.24 M = 33.06 M = 37.10
Technologist Males 5 SD = 8.27 SD = 1.63 SD = 11.87 SD = 10.21 SD = 14.25
Psychologist Females 7 M = 49.20 M = 20.30 M = 9.10 M = 16.22 M = 40.54 c
Males 8 SD = 10.75 SD = 1.07 SD = 7.4 SD = 8.09 SD = 15.21
Real Estate Females 8 M = 49.13 M = 15.53 M = 13.95 M = 14.57 M = 42.14 a
Agent Males 7 SD = 13.15 SD = 2.26 SD = 9.82 SD = 9.58 SD = 17.26
Social Worker Females 11 M = 55.79 a M = 18.33 M = 15.99 M = 25.56 M = 36.37
Males 4 SD = 10.48 SD = 0.72 SD = 11.52 SD = 8.95 SD = 13.99
a
1 missing case
b
2 missing cases
c
3 missing cases

An ANOVA for Years of Education indicated significant differences, F(5, 174) = 10.54,

p < .0001. The Tukey HSD revealed that Enterprising (Florist, Real Estate Agent), Social

(Licensed Practical Nurse, Social Worker), and Conventional types (Accountant, Banker)

reported spending less time in school than the Realistic (Engineer, Forester), Investigative

(Medical Technologist, Psychologist), and Artistic types (Attorney, Librarian).


Is Congruence Dead? 65

When the data were collapsed into Holland type, ANOVAS did not indicate significant

differences for Time on Present Job, Years in Occupation, or Hours Worked Per Week.

Table 14

Demographic Data According to Primary Holland Type

Holland Type Gender Age Years of Time on Years in Hours


Education Present Job Occupation Worked
Per Week
Realistic Females 4 M = 49.50 M = 17.70 M = 12.34 M = 23.06 M= 44.68
Males 26 SD = 9.83 SD = 1.88 SD = 9.50 SD = 10.03 SD = 6.49
Investigative Females 17 M = 52.80 M = 17.82 M= 11.67 M = 24.64 M = 38.63 c
Males 13 SD = 10.11 SD = 2.86 SD = 10.07 SD = 12.46 SD = 14.50
Artistic Females 19 M = 49.07 M = 18.47 M = 10.55 M = 19.87 M = 46.20
Males 11 SD = 11.30 SD = 1.88 SD = 8.88 SD = 10.24 SD = 13.00
Social Females 24 M = 55.18 b M = 15.77 M = 14.73 M = 27.08 M = 39.73 b
Males 6 SD = 11.24 SD = 2.71 SD = 12.52 SD = 11.85 SD = 13.09
Enterprising Females 16 M = 49.67 M = 15.22 M = 16.30 M = 20.39 M = 47.16 a
Males 14 SD = 10.65 SD = 2.00 SD = 11.88 SD = 11.27 SD = 15.23
Conventional Females 16 M = 46.0 M = 15.83 M = 12.76 M = 20.25 a M = 44.10 a
Males 14 SD = 12.82 SD = 2.19 SD = 11.64 SD = 12.94 SD = 11.56
a
1 missing case
b
2 missing cases
c
3 missing cases

MSQ SCORE ANALYSIS

MSQ mean scores were analyzed according to intrinsic, extrinsic, and overall job

satisfaction for all data, for occupational groups, and for primary Holland types. Data are

presented according to all cases (see Table 15) and are further analyzed according to

occupational group (see Table 16) and primary Holland type (see Table 17).

MSQ Score Analysis for All Data

Mean MSQ data for all cases indicated that, overall, the sample reported moderately high

levels of intrinsic (M = 50.51, SD = 6.99), extrinsic (M = 20.58, SD = 11.26), and overall (M

=78.88, SD = 12.35) job satisfaction. Value ranges were large; intrinsic satisfaction scores
Is Congruence Dead? 66

ranged from 12 to 60, extrinsic satisfaction scores ranged from 6 to 30, and overall satisfaction

scores ranged from 22 to 99.

Table 15

MSQ Data for All Cases

Intrinsic Extrinsic Overall


Satisfaction Satisfaction Satisfaction
M = 50.51 M = 20.58 M = 78.88
SD = 6.99 SD = 5.42 SD = 12.35

MSQ Score Analysis by Occupational Group

An ANOVA did not indicate significant differences in intrinsic job satisfaction scores

among occupational groups.

An ANOVA for extrinsic job satisfaction indicated significant differences, F(11, 168) =

2.10, p < .05. The Tukey HSD revealed that Licensed Practical Nurses and Medical

Technologists reported lower levels of extrinsic satisfaction when compared to Real Estate

Agents.

An ANOVA for overall job satisfaction indicated significant differences, F(11, 168) =

2.04, p < .05. The Tukey HSD revealed that Licensed Practical Nurses reported lower levels of

overall job satisfaction when compared to Real Estate Agents.

MSQ Score Analysis by Primary Holland Type

An ANOVA did not indicate significant differences in intrinsic job satisfaction scores

among Holland types.

An ANOVA for extrinsic job satisfaction indicated significant differences, F(5, 174) =

2.84, p < .05. The Tukey HSD revealed that Social types (Licensed Practical Nurse, Social
Is Congruence Dead? 67

Worker) reported lower levels of extrinsic satisfaction when compared to Enterprising types

(Florist, Real Estate Agent).

When the data were collapsed into Holland type, an ANOVA did not indicate significant

differences in overall job satisfaction.

Table 16

MSQ Data According to Occupational Group

Occupational Intrinsic Extrinsic Overall


Group Satisfaction Satisfaction Satisfaction
Accountant M = 51.0 M = 21.93 M = 81.13
SD = 5.61 SD = 3.92 SD = 9.90
Attorney M = 49.93 M = 19.73 M = 77.13
SD =6.12 SD = 5.26 SD = 10.77
Banker M = 47.27 M = 20.60 M = 75.47
SD =5.43 SD = 4.80 SD = 9.16
Engineer M = 51.07 M = 21.2 M = 80.07
SD =7.09 SD = 5.95 SD = 12.96
Florist M = 52.07 M = 21.20 M = 80.73
SD =11.33 SD = 4.89 SD = 16.55
Forester M = 47.80 M = 19.33 M = 74.33
SD =6.88 SD = 6.53 SD = 13.06
Librarian M = 54.73 M = 22.53 M = 85.47
SD =4.49 SD = 5.24 SD = 9.82
Licensed M = 47.33 M = 17.87 M = 72.13
Practical Nurse SD =11.23 SD = 6.98 SD = 18.76
Medical M = 49.67 M = 18.47 M = 75.47
Technologist SD =5.12 SD = 6.56 SD = 12.51
Psychologist M = 50.47 M = 20.33 M = 79.20
SD =4.72 SD = 4.69 SD = 9.32
Real Estate M = 53.60 M = 24.93 M = 87.13
Agent SD =4.29 SD = 2.79 SD = 6.15
Social Worker M = 51.27 M = 18.93 M = 78.33
SD =4.80 SD = 3.58 SD = 8.94
Is Congruence Dead? 68

Table 17

MSQ Data According to Primary Holland Type

Holland Type Intrinsic Extrinsic Overall


Satisfaction Satisfaction Satisfaction
Realistic M = 49.43 M = 20.27 M = 77.20
SD = 7.06 SD = 6.21 SD = 13.11
Investigative M = 50.07 M = 19.40 M= 77.33
SD = 4.86 SD = 5.68 SD = 11.01
Artistic M = 52.33 M = 21.13 M = 81.30
SD = 5.81 SD = 5.35 SD = 10.98
Social M = 49.30 M = 18.40 M = 75.23
SD = 8.72 SD = 5.47 SD = 14.78
Enterprising M = 52.83 M = 23.07 M = 89.93
SD = 8.45 SD = 4.34 SD = 12.70
Conventional M = 49.13 M = 21.27 M = 78.3
SD = 5.75 SD = 4.36 SD = 9.80

CONGRUENCE SCORE ANALYSIS

Congruence mean scores were analyzed according to the modified C index, the

substitution C index, traditionally-derived Holland codes, and decision-derived Holland codes.

Data are presented according to all cases (see Table 18), for occupational groups (see Table 19),

and for primary Holland types (see Table 20).

Congruence Score Analysis for All Data

Mean congruence score data for all cases indicated that, overall, the two congruence

indices and the two methods of congruence generated very similar values and score ranges.

While the data indicated that decision-derived Holland codes are associated with slightly higher

mean congruence scores, an ANOVA indicated that there are no significant differences among

any of the congruence values.

An analysis of the ranges of values indicated that 0 was the lowest value associated with

the decision-derived codes while 1 was the lowest value associated with the traditionally-derived
Is Congruence Dead? 69

codes. This means that it was possible for congruence to be mathematically absent when using

the decision derived method. In contrast, the traditionally-derived method always detected some

degree of congruence.

Table 18

Congruence Score Data for All Cases

Modified C Index Substitution C Index

Traditionally-Derived M = 10.95 M = 10.90


Holland Codes SD = 3.60 SD = 3.60
Score Range 1.4 - 18 Score Range 1 – 18
Decision-Derived M = 11.20 M = 11.21
Holland Codes SD = 4.56 SD = 4.62
Score Range 0 - 18 Score Range 0 - 18

Congruence Score Analysis by Occupational Group

An ANOVA examining congruence means as measured by the modified C index and

traditionally-derived Holland codes indicated significant differences, F(11, 168) = 2.17, p < .05.

The Tukey HSD revealed congruence scores for Social Workers were higher when compared to

Florists, Medical Technologists, and Licensed Practical Nurses.

An ANOVA examining congruence means as measured by the substitution C index and

traditionally-derived Holland codes indicated significant differences, F(11, 168) = 1.99, p < .05.

The Tukey HSD revealed congruence scores for Social Workers were higher when compared to

Florists and Medical Technologists.

An ANOVA examining congruence means as measured by the modified C index and

decision-derived Holland codes indicated significant differences, F(11, 168) = 2.13, p < .05.

Even though a significant F value was obtained, the Tukey HSD did not detect where the source

of the significant differences.


Is Congruence Dead? 70

An ANOVA examining congruence means as measured by the substitution C index and

decision-derived Holland codes indicated significant differences, F(11, 168) = 2.02, p < .05.

Even though a significant F value was obtained, the Tukey HSD did not detect the source of the

significant differences.

Congruence Score Analysis by Primary Holland Type

When the data were collapsed into Holland type, an ANOVA did not indicate significant

differences in congruence means as measured by the modified C index and traditionally-derived

Holland codes.

When the data were collapsed into Holland type, an ANOVA did not indicate significant

differences in congruence means as measured by the substitution C index and traditionally-

derived Holland codes.

When the data were collapsed into Holland type, an ANOVA did not indicate significant

differences in congruence means as measured by the modified C index and decision-derived

Holland codes.

When the data were collapsed into Holland type, an ANOVA did not indicate significant

differences in congruence means as measured by the substitution C index and decision-derived

Holland codes.
Is Congruence Dead? 71

Table 19

Congruence Score Data by Occupational Group

Occupational Group Modified C Index Substitution C Index


Traditionally-Derived Accountant M = 10.75, SD = 3.09 M = 10.67, SD = 3.20
Holland Codes
Attorney* M = 10.87, SD =3.07 M = 10.87, SD =3.07

Banker M = 10.33, SD =3.93 M = 10.2, SD = 4.04

Engineer M = 11.19, SD = 4.10 M = 11.27, SD = 4.28

Florist* M = 9.40, SD =3.11 M = 9.40, SD =3.11

Forester M = 12.54, SD =3.48 M = 12.27, SD = 3.41

Librarian* M = 10.53, SD = 2.85 M = 10.53, SD = 2.85

Licensed Practical Nurse* M = 10.00, SD = 4.77 M = 10.00, SD = 4.77

Medical Technologist* M = 9.93, SD =2.91 M = 9.93, SD =2.91

Psychologist M = 11.25, SD =3.15 M = 11.07, SD = 3.13

Real Estate Agent* M = 10.40, SD = 3.62 M = 10.40, SD = 3.62


Social Worker M = 14.36, SD =3.10 M = 14.2, SD = 2.96
Decision –Derived Accountant M = 10.40, SD = 5.34 M = 10.67, SD = 5.32
Holland Codes
Attorney M = 10.01, SD = 5.60 M = 10.07, SD = 5.63

Banker M = 11.13, SD = 4.27 M = 11.13, SD = 4.29

Engineer M = 11.95, SD = 3.91 M = 12.27, SD = 3.84

Florist M = 9.08, SD = 3.47 M = 9.00, SD = 3.61

Forester M = 13.49, SD = 3.53 M = 13.47, SD = 3.56

Librarian M = 9.31, SD = 6.02 M = 9.47, SD = 6.15

Licensed Practical Nurse M = 9.00, SD = 3.63 M = 9.00, SD = 3.64

Medical Technologist M = 11.56, SD = 3.05 M = 11.53, SD = 3.06

Psychologist M = 12.16, SD = 3.23 M = 12.13, SD = 3.48

Real Estate Agent M = 12.51, SD = 5.69 M = 12.40, SD = 5.81


Social Worker M = 13.81, SD = 3.81 M = 14.00, SD = 4.05

* Modified C Index and substitution C index calculations are computationally equivalent.


Is Congruence Dead? 72

Table 20

Congruence Score Data by Primary Holland Type

Occupational Group Modified C Index Substitution C Index


Traditionally-Derived Realistic M = 11.82, SD = 3.79 M = 11.77 , SD = 3.84
Holland Codes
Investigative M = 10.59, SD = 3.05 M = 10.50, SD = 3.03

Artistic* M = 10.70, SD = 2.91 M = 10.70 , SD = 2.91

Social M = 12.18 , SD = 4.53 M = 12.10 , SD = 4.44

Enterprising* M = 9.90 , SD = 3.35 M = 9.90, SD = 3.36


Conventional M = 10.54, SD = 3.48 M = 10.43, SD = 3.59
Decision –Derived Realistic M = 12.72, SD = 3.74 M = 12.87, SD = 3.69
Holland Codes
Investigative M = 11.86, SD = 3.11 M = 11.83, SD = 3.24

Artistic M = 9.66, SD = 5.72 M = 9.77, SD = 5.81

Social M = 11.41, SD = 4.40 M = 11.50 , SD = 4.56

Enterprising M = 10.79, SD = 4.94 M = 10.70, SD = 5.06

Conventional M = 10.77, SD = 4.76 M = 10.90, SD = 4.76

* Modified C Index and substitution C index calculations are computationally equivalent.

GENERAL RESEARCH QUESTION ANALYSIS

General research questions were analyzed according to all data, for occupational groups,

and for primary Holland types. Tables indicating relevant data are indicated with each question.

Question 1a

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores?


Is Congruence Dead? 73

All Data

An analysis of all data indicates there is no significant correlation between congruence

and intrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational

personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see Table 21).

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data

do indicate a strong negative correlation among Medical Technologists, r(13) = - .643, p < .01

(see Table 22).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data

do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Investigative types (Medical Technologist,

Psychologist), r(28) = - .380, p < .05 (See Table 23).

Table 21

Job Satisfaction Correlations with Congruence Scores for All Data

Modified C Index Substitution C Index


Traditionally Decision Traditionally Decision
Derived Derived Derived Derived
Intrinsic -.083 (α = .135) -.025 (α = .369) -.075 (α = .159) -.024 (α = .375)
Job Satisfaction
Extrinsic -.176 (p < .01)* -.019 (α = .403) -.167 (p < .05)* -.030 (α = .346)
Job Satisfaction
Overall -.131 (p < .05)* -.009 (α = .454) -.123 (α = .051) -.013 (α = .429)
Job Satisfaction
Is Congruence Dead? 74

Question 1b

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and extrinsic job

satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicates there is no positive correlation between congruence and

extrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational

personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data do

indicate a weak negative correlation between congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction, r(178) =

- .176, p < .01 (see Table 21).

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data

do indicate a strong negative correlation among Accountants r(13) = - .609, p < .01 and a

moderate negative correlation among Medical Technologists, r(13) = - .555, p < .05 (see Table

22).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data

do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Investigative types (Medical Technologist,

Psychologist), r(28) = - .360, p < .05 (See Table 23).


Is Congruence Dead? 75

Table 22

Occupational Group Job Satisfaction Correlations with Traditionally-Derived Congruence


Scores

Modified C Index Substitution C Index


Intrinsic Job Satisfaction Accountant -.238 (α = .196) -.279 (α = .157)
Attorney .258 (α = .177) .258 (α = .177)
Banker .176 (α = .266) .209 (α = .227)
Engineer .064 (α = .411) .086 (α = .380)
Florist -.021 (α = .470) -.021 (α = .470)
Forester -.384 (α = .079) -.363 (α = .092)
Librarian .213 (α = .223) .213 (α = .223)
Licensed Practical Nurse -.228 (α = .207) -.228 (α = .207)
Medical Technologist* -.643 (p < .01) -.643 (p < .01)
Psychologist -.179 (α = .261) -.157 (α = .288)
Real Estate Agent -.145 (α = .303) -.145 (α = .303)
Social Worker .171 (α = .272) .172 (α = .270)
Extrinsic Job Accountant -.609 (p < .01) -.629 (p < .01)
Satisfaction Attorney -.025 (α = .465) -.025 (α = .465)
Banker -.044 (α = .438) -.054 (α = .424)
Engineer -.024 (α = .466) .020 (α = .472)
Florist -.363 (α = .092) -.363 (α = .092)
Forester -.188 (α = .251) -.152 (α = .295)
Librarian .281 (α = .155) .281 (α = .155)
Licensed Practical Nurse -.395 (α = .072) -.395 (α = .072)
Medical Technologist* -.555 (p < .05) -.555 (p < .05)
Psychologist -.248 (α = .187) -.216 (α = .220)
Real Estate Agent .335 (α = .111) .335 (α = .111)
Social Worker .220 (α = .215) .224 (α = .211)
Overall Job Satisfaction Accountant -.428 (α = .056) -.461 (p <.05)
Attorney .128 (α = .325) .128 (α = .325)
Banker .020(α = .472) .036 (α = .449)
Engineer .043 (α = .440) .079 (α = .389)
Florist -.142 (α = .307) -.142 (α = .307)
Forester -.307 (α = .133) -.278 (α = .158)
Librarian .297 (α = .141) .297 (α = .141)
Licensed Practical Nurse -.319 (α = .123) -.319 (α = .123)
Medical Technologist* -.573 (p < .05) -.573 (p < .05)
Psychologist -.218 (α = .218) -.192 (α = .247)
Real Estate Agent .142 (α = .307) .142 (α = .307)
Social Worker .152 (α = .295) .157 (α =. 289)
* Medical Technologist correlation calculations are computationally equivalent.
Is Congruence Dead? 76

Table 23

Holland Type Job Satisfaction Correlations with Traditionally-Derived Congruence Scores

Modified C Index Substitution C Index


Intrinsic Job Satisfaction Realistic -.172 (α = .182) -.136 (α = .237)
Investigative -.380 (p < .05) -.375 (p < .05)
Artistic .191 (α = .155) .191 (α = .155)
Social -.006 (α = .488) -.011 (α = .476)
Enterprising -.037 (α = .423) -.037 (α = .423)
Conventional .010 (α = .478) .012 (α = .475)

Extrinsic Job Satisfaction Realistic -.125 (α = .255) -.078 (α = .341)


Investigative -.360 (p < .05) -.355 (p < .05)
Artistic .102 (α = .295) .102 (α = .295)
Social -.160 (α = .199) -.166 (α = .190)
Enterprising -.004 (α = .491) -.004 (α = .491)
Conventional -.252 (α = .090) -.265 (α = .078)

Overall Job Satisfaction Realistic -.151 (α = .213) -.106 (α = .289)


Investigative -.354 (p < .05) -.351 (p < .05)
Artistic .167 (α = .189) .167 (α = .189)
Social -.071 (α = .355) -.076 (α = .344)
Enterprising -.008 (α = .483) -.008 (α = .483)
Conventional -.158 (α =. 203) -.163 (α = .195)

Question 1c

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction

when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the Modified C

index is used to calculate congruence scores?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicates there is no positive correlation between congruence and

overall job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational

personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data do
Is Congruence Dead? 77

indicate a weak negative correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction, r(178) = -

.131, p < .05 (see Table 21).

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data

do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Medical Technologists, r(13) = - .573, p < .05

(see Table 22).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data

do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Investigative types (Medical Technologist,

Psychologist), r(28) = - .354, p < .05 (See Table 23).

Question 2a

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicates there is no significant correlation between congruence

and intrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational

personality and the substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see Table 21).
Is Congruence Dead? 78

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The

data do indicate a strong negative correlation among Medical Technologists, r(13) = - .643, p <

.01 (see Table 22).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicate there is no positive correlation between

congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The

data do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Investigative types (Medical

Technologist, Psychologist), r(28) = - .375, p < .05 (see Table 23).

Question 2b

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and extrinsic job

satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicate there is no positive correlation between congruence and

extrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational

personality and the substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The data do

indicate a weak negative correlation between congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction, r(178) =

- .167, p < .05 (see Table 21).


Is Congruence Dead? 79

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The

data do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Medical Technologists, r(13) = - .555, p

< .05 and a strong negative correlation among Accountants r(13) = - .629, p < .05 (see Table 22).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicate there is no positive correlation between

congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The

data do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Investigative types (Medical

Technologist, Psychologist) r(28) = - .355, p < .05 (see Table 23).

Question 2c

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction

when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the substitution

C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

All Data

The is no significant correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction when

traditionally-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the substitution C

index is used to calculate congruence scores (see Table 21).

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure
Is Congruence Dead? 80

vocational personality and the substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The

data do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Accountants, r(13) = - .461, p < .05, and

among Medical Technologists, r(13) = - .573, p < .05 (see Table 22).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicates there is no positive correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the substitution C index is used to calculate congruence scores. The

data do indicate a moderate negative correlation among Investigative types (Medical

Technologist, Psychologist) r(28) = - .351, p < .05 (see Table 23).

Question 3a

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicates there is no significant correlation between congruence

and intrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational

personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see Table 21).

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates there is no significant correlation

between congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to

measure vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores

(see Table 24).


Is Congruence Dead? 81

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicates there is no significant correlation between

congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see

Table 25).

Question 3b

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and extrinsic job

satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicate there is no significant correlation between congruence and

extrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality

and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see Table 21).

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates that there is no significant correlation

between congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to

measure vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores

(see Table 24).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicate that there is no significant correlation between

congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see

Table 25).
Is Congruence Dead? 82

Question 3c

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction

when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the Modified C

index is used to calculate congruence scores?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicate that there is no significant correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see table

21).

Occupational Groups

An analysis of all data indicate that there is no significant correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see table

24).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicate that there is no significant correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Modified C index is used to calculate congruence scores (see table

25).

Question 4a

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and intrinsic job

satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Substitution C index is used to measure congruence?


Is Congruence Dead? 83

All Data

An analysis of all data indicates that there is no significant correlation between

congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see Table

21).

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicates there is no significant correlation

between congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to

measure vocational personality and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see

Table 24).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicates that there is no significant correlation between

congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see Table

25).

Question 4b

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and extrinsic job

satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the

Substitution C index is used to measure congruence?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicate there is no significant correlation between congruence and

extrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality

and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see Table 21).
Is Congruence Dead? 84

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicate there is no significant correlation

between congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to

measure vocational personality and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see

Table 24).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicate there is no significant correlation between

congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see Table

25).

Question 4c

Is there a significant positive correlation between congruence and overall job satisfaction

when decision-derived codes are used to measure vocational personality and the Substitution C

index is used to measure congruence?

All Data

An analysis of all data indicate that there is no significant correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see Table

21).
Is Congruence Dead? 85

Table 24

Occupational Group Job Satisfaction Correlations with Decision-Derived Congruence Scores

Modified C Index Substitution C Index


Overall Job Accountant -.375 (α = .084) -.365 (α = .091)
Satisfaction Attorney -.068 (α = .404) -.063 (α = .412)
Banker -.124 (α = .330) -.098 (α = .364)
Engineer .140 (α = .309) .103 (α = .358)
Florist .109 (α = .349) .101 (α = .361)
Forester .368 (α = .089) .350 (α = .101)
Librarian .124 (α = .330) .101 (α = .360)
Licensed Practical Nurse -.199 (α = .238) -.192 (α = .246)
Medical Technologist -.252 (α = .182) -.247 (α = .187)
Psychologist .013 (α = .482) -.010 (α = .486)
Real Estate Agent .090 (α = .375) .122 (α = .332)
Social Worker .201 (α = .237) .173 (α = .268)
Intrinsic Job Accountant -.318 (α = .124) -.311 (α = .130)
Satisfaction Attorney .027 (α = .462) .025 (α = .465)
Banker -.067 (α = .406) -.032 (α = .455)
Engineer .213 (α = .223) .180 (α = .260)
Florist .161 (α = .284) .149 (α = .299)
Forester .324 (α = .119) .296 (α = .142)
Librarian -.161 (α = .283) -.171 (α = .271)
Licensed Practical Nurse -.183 (α = .257) -.183 (α = .257)
Medical Technologist -.361 (α = .093) -.356 (α = .096)
Psychologist .033 (α = .453) .009 (α = .487)
Real Estate Agent -.125 (α = .329) -.113 (α = .344)
Social Worker .221 (α = .215) .202 (α = .235)
Extrinsic Job Accountant -.386 (α = .077) -.374 (α = .085)
Satisfaction Attorney -.210 (α = .226) -.195 (α = .244)
Banker -.134 (α = .317) -.122 (α = .333)
Engineer -.041 (α = .443) -.074 (α = .396)
Florist -.004 (α = .495) -.012 (α = .483)
Forester .306 (α = .134) .297 (α = .141)
Librarian .261 (α = .174) .242 (α = .192)
Licensed Practical Nurse -.140 (α = .310) -.124 (α = .330)
Medical Technologist -.216 (α = .220) -.209 (α = .228)
Psychologist -.030 (α = .457) -.047 (α = .434)
Real Estate Agent .204 (α = .233) .248 (α = .186)
Social Worker .234 (α = .201) .197 (α =. 241)
Is Congruence Dead? 86

Occupational Groups

An analysis of occupational group data indicate there is no significant correlation

between congruence and overall job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to

measure vocational personality and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see

Table 24).

Primary Holland Types

An analysis of Holland type data indicate that there is no significant correlation between

congruence and overall job satisfaction when decision-derived codes are used to measure

vocational personality and the Substitution C index is used to measure congruence (see Table

25).

Question 5a

Are there significant differences in the intrinsic job satisfaction correlation values

identified in Questions 1a, 2a, 3a, and 4a?

All Data

Questions 1a, 2a, 3a, and 4a did not indicate any significant correlations between

congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction.

Occupational Groups

Questions 1a and 2a indicated a strong negative correlation between congruence and

intrinsic job satisfaction among Medical Technologists when traditionally-derived codes were

used with the modified C index and the substitution C index. These calculations were based on

identical congruence scores. No significant difference exists between the two correlations.
Is Congruence Dead? 87

Primary Holland Types

Questions 1a and 2a indicated a moderate negative correlation between congruence and

intrinsic job satisfaction among Investigative (Medical Technologist, Psychologist) types when

traditionally-derived codes were used with the modified C index and the substitution C index. A

Fisher’s r to z’ Transformation indicated no difference between the two correlations (z = -0.021).

Table 25

Holland Type Job Satisfaction Correlations with Decision-Derived Congruence Scores

Modified C Index Substitution C Index


Overall Job Satisfaction Realistic .190 (α = .157) .176 (α = .175)
Investigative -.114 (α = .275) -.116 (α = .270)
Artistic .000 (α = .499) -.003 (α = .494)
Social .069 (α = .359) .070 (α = .357)
Enterprising .163 (α = .195) .166 (α = .191)
Conventional -.278 (α = .068) -.254 (α = .088)

Intrinsic Job Satisfaction Realistic .202 (α = .142) .186 (α = .162)


Investigative -.156 (α = .205) -.159 (α = .200)
Artistic -.076 (α = .345) -.077 (α = .342)
Social .084 (α = .329) .085 (α = .328)
Enterprising .070 (α = .356) .068 (α = .360)
Conventional -.221 (α = .120) -.194 (α = .153)

Extrinsic Job Realistic .095 (α = .309) .084 (α = .329)


Satisfaction
Investigative -.114(α = .274) -.114 (α = .274)
Artistic .015 (α = .468) .018 (α = .463)
Social .048 (α = .401) .050 (α = .397)
Enterprising .225 (α = .116) .233 (α = .108)
Conventional -.264 (α = .080) -.248 (α = .093)
Is Congruence Dead? 88

Question 5b

Are there significant differences in the extrinsic job satisfaction correlation values

identified in Questions 1b, 2b, 3b, and 4b?

All Data

Questions 1b and 2b indicated a weak, negative correlation between congruence and

extrinsic job satisfaction when traditionally-derived codes were used with the modified C index

and the substitution C index. A Fisher’s r to z’ Transformation indicated no difference between

the two correlations (z = -0.087).

Occupational Groups

Questions 1b and 2b indicated a strong, negative correlation between extrinsic job

satisfaction and congruence among Accountants when traditionally-derived codes were used

with the modified C index and the substitution C index. A Fisher’s r to z’ Transformation

indicated no difference between the two correlations (z = 0.119).

Questions 1b and 2b indicated a moderate, negative correlation between extrinsic job

satisfaction and congruence among Medical Technologists when traditionally-derived codes

were used with the modified C index and the substitution C index. These calculations were based

on identical congruence scores. No significant difference exists between the two correlations.

Primary Holland Types

Questions 1b and 2b indicated a moderate, negative correlation between congruence and

extrinsic job satisfaction among Investigative types (Medical Technologist, Psychologist) when

traditionally-derived codes were used with the modified C index and the substitution C index. A

Fisher’s r to z’ Transformation indicated no difference between the two correlations (z = -0.021).


Is Congruence Dead? 89

Question 5c

Are there significant differences in the overall job satisfaction correlation values

identified in Questions 1c, 2c, 3c, and 4c?

All Data

Only Question 1c indicated a weak negative correlation between congruence and overall

job satisfaction for all data.

Occupational Groups

Question 1c indicated a moderate, negative correlation between congruence and overall

job satisfaction among Accountants when traditionally derived codes were used with the

substitution C index. Question 2c did not indicate a significant correlation between congruence

and overall job satisfaction among Accountants when traditionally derived codes were used with

the modified C index.

Questions 1c and 2c indicated a moderate, negative correlation between congruence and

overall job satisfaction among Medical Technologists when traditionally derived codes were

used with the modified C index and the substitution C index. These calculations were based on

identical congruence scores. No significant difference exists between the two correlations.

Primary Holland Types

Questions 1c and 2c indicated a moderate, negative correlation between congruence and

overall job satisfaction among Investigative types (Medical Technologist, Psychologist) when

traditionally derived codes were used with the modified C index and the substitution C index. A

Fisher’s r to z’ Transformation indicated no difference between the two correlations (z = -0.013).


Is Congruence Dead? 90

Question 6

Do any of the overall job satisfaction correlation values identified in Questions 1c, 2c, 3c,

and 4c meet or significantly exceed the meta-analytic correlations that were reported by

Assouline & Meir (1987), Tranberg, et al. (1993), and Tsabari, et al. (2005)?

Correlation between Congruence and Overall Job Satisfaction

Only Question 1c identified a significant correlation between congruence and overall job

satisfaction, a negative correlation value of r = -.131, when traditionally-derived codes were used

with the Modified C index. Fisher’s r to z’ Transformations indicated that this correlation value

is significantly lower than the values obtained by Assouline & Meir (1987), Tranberg, et al.

(1993), and Tsabari, et al. (2005) (see Table 26).

Table 26

Comparisons of Congruence – Overall Job Satisfaction Correlations

Assouline & Meir (1987) Tranberg, et al. (1993) Tsabari, et al. (2005)
r =.21 (N = 9,041) r =.20 (N = 11,104) r = .158 (N = 5,805)

r = -.131 (N = 180) z = -4.545 z = -4.415 z = -3.815


(p < 0.001) (p < 0.001) (p < 0.001)

SUMMARY OF CHAPTER FOUR

Chapter Four presented the research data analyzed according to all cases, occupational

groups, and primary Holland types. Demographic information first was evaluated. MSQ score

and congruence score data were analyzed and discussed. An analysis of this study’s general

research questions then was presented.


Is Congruence Dead? 91

CHAPTER FIVE: DISCUSSION

Job satisfaction is considered to be the most important outcome variable in congruence

research. Even though the assumption that there is a positive correlation between congruence and

job satisfaction is fundamental to Holland’s theory, the empirical evidence to support this

relationship is, at best, equivocal. While there is popular opinion within the vocational

psychology field that the congruence construct is “dead” as a viable theoretical concept, an

alternative point of view maintains that congruence has not fared better in the research literature

because methodological limitations have led to unimpressive and ambiguous findings. Many

researchers have neither acknowledged the methodological problems associated with congruence

research nor implemented viable empirical alternatives. The purpose of this study was to address

the common problems associated with congruence – job satisfaction research and to provide a

rationale for identifying and comparing measurement alternatives.

Sampling issues were addressed through the use of a sample of employed adults that was

representative of the entire RIASEC scheme. The Strong Interest Explorer (SIE; Chartrand,

2001) was chosen as the measure of vocational personality as it is based upon the psychometric

strength of the Strong Interest Inventory (SII; Donnay, et. al, 2005) but heretofore had not been

used in empirical research. The SIE was scored in a manner to compare a traditionally-derived

summary coding method to a decision-derived summary coding method. It was examined

whether the congruence - job satisfaction correlation was dependent upon the summary coding

method that was applied. The General Occupational Theme (GOT) codes of the SII were

identified to assess the work environment as they represent an application of the RIASEC

scheme that incorporates methods to limit potential measurement error. The modified C index

(Eggerth & Andrew, 2006) and the substitution C index (Gore & Brown, 2006) were used in this
Is Congruence Dead? 92

research to generate congruence scores. It was examined whether the congruence - job

satisfaction correlation was dependent upon the index that was used to calculate congruence. The

Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire (MSQ; Weiss, et. al, 1967) was selected as a valid and

reliable measure to investigate facets of intrinsic, extrinsic, and overall job satisfaction.

This research supports the use of a sample composed of working adults from multiple

occupational groups that is representative of the entire RIASEC scheme. This approach allowed

for a more specific identification of significant findings and more thorough analysis of the data.

Research results indicated that, overall, the traditionally-derived coding method performed better

than the decision-derived coding method. The data showed equivocal findings when the

modified C index and the substitution C index were compared. Job satisfaction correlation data

supported the use of a multifaceted measure.

The most noteworthy findings of this research are that significant, yet weak, correlations

were detected between congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction and between congruence and

overall job satisfaction. These relationships were not in the predicted direction. In fact, none of

the significant relationships detected in this study were in the predicted direction. In addressing

the question of whether the congruence construct is “dead”, it may be interpreted that the concept

is changing to reflect the social and economic shifts that have influenced the changing nature of

the meaning of work. This research suggests that congruence should be reconceptualized if it is

to remain a viable concept in the research and practice of vocational psychology.

REVIEW OF RESULTS

The use of a sample composed of multiple occupational groups that is representative of

the entire RIASEC scheme allowed for an analysis of the study data according to all cases and

also by occupational group and by primary Holland type. Demographic data, mean job
Is Congruence Dead? 93

satisfaction scores, mean congruence scores, and congruence – job satisfaction correlations were

able to be examined more thoroughly. If the sample had been composed of only one

occupational group or one primary type, an analysis would not have identified important

information regarding differences among occupational groups and among primary Holland types.

Demographic Analysis

An analysis of the demographic data by occupational group indicated significant

differences among occupations on all demographic variables. Those differences on three

variables disappeared when the data were analyzed according to primary Holland type. While it

was not within the scope of this research to examine the relationship between demographic

variables and the congruence – job satisfaction correlation, the sampling method used here did

permit the gathering and analysis of that data in order to provide areas of inquiry for further

research.

Recommendations for Further Research

An interesting finding among the overall demographic data for this sample is that all

variables except Years of Education indicated quite a range of values. Years on Present Job, for

example, yielded an overall mean value of 13.06. With a standard deviation of 10.85,

approximately 68% of the sample reported values between 2.21 years and 23.91 years. Among

occupational groups, mean reported Years on Present Job values ranged from 5.71 years for

Bankers and 19.82 years for Accountants, the two occupational groups that were selected to

represent the Conventional type. Further study may be designed in order to examine

demographic variables in order to assess whether there are significant relationships with the

congruence – job satisfaction correlation. Additional research may uncover information that

provides a clearer picture of the demographic factors that affect these variables. An analysis of
Is Congruence Dead? 94

Years on Present Job, for example, may indicate a meaningful relationship between job tenure

and job satisfaction.

Mean Job Satisfaction Scores

When MSQ scores were analyzed separately, the data for all cases indicated that the

sample tended to report moderately high levels of intrinsic, extrinsic, and overall job satisfaction

regardless of congruence. While an analysis of MSQ scores by occupational group did not show

differences in intrinsic satisfaction scores, some significant differences did appear among the

groups for extrinsic and overall job satisfaction. When the data were collapsed into primary

Holland types, the differences in overall job satisfaction disappeared. The analysis continued to

show mean extrinsic score differences among two of the primary types.

Recommendations for Further Research

The analysis of this study’s MSQ information suggests interesting areas for further

inquiry. When the data were collapsed into primary Holland type, there remained a significant

difference in extrinsic satisfaction between the Social and Enterprising types. Additional

congruence – job satisfaction studies should continue to examine a range of occupational groups

and primary Holland types as relationships between congruence and job satisfaction that are

particular to occupational groups or primary Holland types may be uncovered. For example,

Social occupations such as Social Worker, Licensed Practical Nurse, Elementary School

Teacher, and High School Counselor stereotypically are the ones that tend to be associated

systematically with fewer opportunities for extrinsic satisfaction (e.g., poorer working

conditions, lower pay and benefits, etc.). It could be that congruence – job satisfaction studies

always or almost always will find comparably lower satisfaction scores among Social types.
Is Congruence Dead? 95

Given the limited variability in this study’s MSQ scores, it additionally is recommended

that further research pay particular attention to the potential effects of range restriction among

the data.

Mean Congruence Scores

When mean congruence scores were analyzed separately, an examination of data for all

cases indicated that the decision-derived codes were associated with slightly higher congruence

means, but these differences were not found to be significant. A further examination of mean

congruence scores by occupational group showed significant differences among some of the

occupations when the traditionally-derived codes were used to measure congruence. These

differences disappeared when the data were collapsed into primary Holland type.

Recommendations for Further Research

Similar to the measurement of job satisfaction, additional congruence – job satisfaction

studies should continue to examine a range of occupational groups and primary Holland types in

order to uncover whether there are particular relationships between congruence and job

satisfaction that provide more information to explain the correlation. It may be found that certain

occupations necessarily require higher levels of congruence for successful employment. For

example, while it is difficult to imagine someone who is employed as a musician who does not

also have a high level of Artistic interest, it may be plausible for a retail sales representative to be

successfully employed with only moderate Enterprising interests. It additionally is recommended

that congruence – job satisfaction studies continue to measure congruence in a variety of ways.

The comparison of several methods more readily allows for an analysis regarding whether results

can be considered to be valid or perhaps are more a product of measurement error.


Is Congruence Dead? 96

Congruence – Job Satisfaction Correlations

When all data were analyzed, weak, negative correlations were found between

congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction and between congruence and overall job satisfaction.

An overall relationship between congruence and intrinsic satisfaction was not detected. A further

analysis of the data by occupational group indicated moderate to strong significant negative

relationships between congruence and overall job satisfaction and between congruence and

extrinsic job satisfaction for two of the occupational groups and a strong negative correlation

between congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction for just one of the occupational groups. When

data were collapsed into primary Holland type, a moderate, negative correlation was found

between all facets of job satisfaction and only one primary type.

Recommendations for Further Research

Overall, this study’s analysis of facets of job satisfaction revealed relationships that

would have been hidden had only an overall measure of job satisfaction been used. For example,

while an analysis of all data indicated a weak, negative correlation between congruence and job

satisfaction, a further analysis of intrinsic and extrinsic aspects suggests that it appears to be the

correlation between extrinsic satisfaction and congruence that is contributing more to the

significance of the correlation with overall job satisfaction. It is recommended that congruence –

job satisfaction research continue to measure facets of job satisfaction in order to obtain a clearer

picture of the job satisfaction construct.

Traditionally-Derived and Decision-Derived Holland Summary Codes

Congruence score comparisons of overall data indicated that the decision-derived codes

were associated with slightly higher congruence means. These mean differences were not found

to be significant when compared to traditionally-derived codes. In fact, an analysis of the data by


Is Congruence Dead? 97

occupational group showed that decision-derived codes were associated with lower congruence

means approximately 50% of the time. An analysis of the data by primary Holland type indicated

that decision-derived codes were associated with lower congruence means approximately 30% of

the time. Statistical procedures examining the differences among congruence means when the

decision-derived codes were used did indicate significant differences among means by both

occupational group and by Holland type. Post hoc testing suggested that none of the pairwise

comparisons showed a level of variance large enough to indicate the source of the significant

differences. Moreover, the use of decision-derived codes did not result in any significant

correlations between congruence and job satisfaction.

Statistical procedures examining differences among congruence means when the

traditionally-derived codes were used did indicate significant differences among means by

occupational group, and post hoc testing did detect the source of significant differences.

Moreover, the data indicated that it was only the use of traditionally-derived codes that was

associated with significant correlations between congruence and job satisfaction. This result was

illustrated when correlation data were analyzed according to congruence index and facet of job

satisfaction and by occupational group and primary Holland type.

The better performance of the traditionally-derived codes is surprising given the

methodological limitations that are associated with this approach to coding and Dik, et al.’s

(2007) and Dik & Hansen’s (2004) empirical support of decision-derived codes. There are

several reasons that may explain this finding. This study did correct for previous methodological

limitations by implementing tie-breaking guidelines based on Holland’s assumptions of

consistency. It also should be noted that Dik, et al.’s (2007) research compared four decision-
Is Congruence Dead? 98

derived methods of generating Holland codes without also examining traditionally-derived

codes.

Recommendations for Further Research

It is recommended that congruence research continue to make methodological

refinements in the measurement of vocational personality. Further investigation of the

appropriateness of the SIE as a research instrument is warranted. It is recommended that the SIE

be measured directly against the SII in order to make comparisons regarding the compatibility of

Holland summary codes between the two instruments. Moreover, it is suggested that the SIE

continue to be used in order to generate further standardization data that can be used in the

development of decision-derived coding methods. Finally, this study also indicates support for

additional work regarding the implementation of a summary coding method that is theoretically

based on Holland’s hexagonal assumptions.

The Modified C Index and the Substitution C Index

This study’s findings on the modified C index and the substitution C index are somewhat

equivocal. Overall, the indexes generated almost identical congruence means regardless of

summary coding method. When congruence – job satisfaction correlations were analyzed for all

data, neither index was associated with a significant correlation between congruence and

intrinsic job satisfaction. Both indexes measured a weak negative correlation between

congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction. While the substitution C index correlation was lower,

it was not found to be significantly different from the modified C index correlation. The

modified C index did measure a weak significant negative correlation between congruence and

overall job satisfaction while the substitution C index was not associated with a significant
Is Congruence Dead? 99

correlation at the p = .05 level. The substitution C index correlation was significant at the p = .06

level.

A further analysis of congruence means and congruence – job satisfaction correlations by

occupational group and by Holland type yielded comparable results regarding the similar

performance of the modified C index and the substitution C index. The indexes detected negative

correlations between congruence and facets of job satisfaction for the same occupational groups

and primary Holland types. In cases where the correlations were not identical due to

computationally equivalent results, there were no significant differences. The substitution C

index did measure a moderately significant negative correlation between congruence and overall

job satisfaction among Accountants, while the modified C index did not. The modified C index

correlation was significant at the p = .06 level.

Recommendations for Further Research

Given that this research represents the first empirical study to compare the modified C

index and the substitution C index, further research needs to be conducted before definitive

statements can be made regarding the relative efficacy of these two indexes. As the modified C

index and the substitution C index are computationally equivalent in many cases, it is suggested

that further research focus on where the scores of these two indexes diverge. Additional study

should compare, in particular, the calculation of congruence scores in the cases of one letter x

two letters, two letters x two letters, and two letters x three letters. This may provide a clearer

picture of the differences between the modified C index and substitution C index and permit

recommendations for their use based on the types of congruence calculations are to be

performed.
Is Congruence Dead? 100

IS CONGRUENCE DEAD?

On the whole when all data were analyzed, this study did find a weak, yet significant,

relationship between congruence and overall job satisfaction. The correlation did not reach the

level of significance that was obtained in the meta-analyses by Assouline and Meir (1987),

Tranberg, et al. (1993), or Tsabari, et al. (2005). What is noteworthy is that the correlation found

here between congruence and overall job satisfaction was not in the predicted direction, and

instead was negative. Moreover, the data suggested that it could be a weak, yet significant,

negative relationship between congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction that especially

contributed to this finding. The relationship between congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction

was not found to be significant. What does this mean given Holland’s theory assumes that there

is a positive correlation between congruence and job satisfaction?

The finding of increasing levels of extrinsic satisfaction as levels of congruence decrease

makes logical sense. The concept of congruence as it is reflected in vocational personality

assessments and Holland summary codes tends to be one that emphasizes aspects of intrinsic

satisfaction. If congruence is low, it also is likely that intrinsic job satisfaction will be low.

Individuals must achieve some sense of job satisfaction in order to sustain their employment,

which means an increasing reliance on extrinsic factors for job satisfaction.

The negative correlation between congruence and extrinsic job satisfaction with no

significant corresponding correlation between congruence and intrinsic job satisfaction may

suggest an increasing importance of extrinsic factors over intrinsic factors. This finding makes

sense when one considers contemporary social, economic, technological, legal, and political

shifts that have influenced the contemporary meaning of work.


Is Congruence Dead? 101

We live in a time where the organization of work is changing at “whirlwind speed”

(National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, 1999). The workforce has experienced

widespread restructuring and downsizing. Employment practices increasingly depend upon

flexible staffing, temporary workers, and contract-supplied labor. Increases in involuntary job

displacement are associated with decreased job stability and delayed retirement. Moreover,

increases in workload, work roles, and work hours now are common experiences (National

Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, 2002).

A series of surveys reported by NIOSH (1999) indicated that 26% to 40% of workers

reported that their jobs were very stressful. It may be that intensifying individual and situational

job stressors are resulting in decreased opportunities to experience the internal motivations such

as autonomy and competence that are necessary for the facilitation of intrinsic job satisfaction

(Ryan & Deci, 2000). Individuals consequently may tend increasingly to seek employment based

on factors of extrinsic satisfaction.

When one considers that the meaning of work has changed significantly and rapidly, it

seems only logical that we might see changes in the nature of the correlation between

congruence and job satisfaction. Therefore, in addressing the issue of whether congruence is

“dead”, the answer may be “no” – as long as we are able to conceptualize that the relationship

between congruence and job satisfaction necessarily must be fluid and reflect changes over time.

We cannot expect congruence to exist in a vacuum if the construct is to continue to be useful to

the research and practice of vocational psychology.

RESEARCH LIMITATIONS

Given that the findings of this study contradict over 40 years of research on Holland’s

congruence construct, it is especially important to acknowledge the methodological limitations


Is Congruence Dead? 102

that may be impacting the results. First of all, the selection of occupational groups from the SII

necessarily represents a restriction in the occupational groups that could be sampled. Moreover,

the comparison of gender-normed GOT codes to ones from a mixed-gender sample further

reduces the generalizability of the results that were obtained here.

Dik, et al. (2007) generated standardized scores for their decision-based rules using years

of SII research data. The use of the SIE in this study necessitated the creation of a standardized

sample that was tested against itself. The conversion from traditionally-derived codes to decision

derived codes further highlighted reasons to suggest that, generally, the use of the SIE may have

been problematic. The basic interest areas that are part of the Social and Realistic scales create a

possible total on each of these scales of 30 points. The basic interest areas that are part of the

Investigative, Artistic, Enterprising, and Conventional scales create a possible total on each of

these scales of 20 points. Given that over half of the traditionally-derived codes reflected Social

and Realistic primary Holland types, the predominance of these types simply may have been a

function of the fact that there were more opportunities to score highly on these scales. The

subsequent decrease among Realistic and Social primary Holland types and increase among

Investigative, Artistic, Enterprising, Conventional types primary Holland types that was

associated with the decision-derived codes further calls into question the validity of the SIE.

It is acknowledged that the method of data gathering that was used here is a limitation of

the study. Participants were individuals who happened to be members of certain professional

organizations with public information membership lists or who were sampled at appropriate

businesses for the sake of convenience. Moreover, participants represented a group of individuals

who all volunteered for this study and who, by their participation, implicitly expressed an interest

in a study about job satisfaction. Analysis and interpretation of the results must be tempered with
Is Congruence Dead? 103

the knowledge that the data, to some degree, reflect those individuals who self-selected to

compose the sample.


Is Congruence Dead? 104

REFERENCES

Allen, M. L. (1996). Dimensions of educational satisfaction and academic achievement among

music therapy majors. Journal of Music Therapy, 33(2), 147 – 160.

Aranya, N., Barak, A., & Amernic, J. (1981). A test of Holland’s theory in a population of

accountants. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 19(1), 15 – 24.

Arnold, J. (2004). The congruence problem in John Holland’s theory of vocational decisions.

Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 77(1), 95 – 113.

Assouline, M., & Meir, E. I. (1987). Meta-analysis of the relationship between congruence and

well-being measures. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 31(3), 319 – 332.

Astin, A. W. (1963). Further validation of the environmental assessment technique. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 54(4), 217 – 226.

Astin, A. W., & Holland, J. L. (1961). The environmental assessment technique: A way to

measure college environments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 52(6), 308 – 316.

Baltzer, W. K., Kihm, J. A., Smith, P. C., Irwin, J. L., Bachiochi, P. D., Robie, C., et al. (1997).

Users' manual for the Job Descriptive Index (JDI; 1997 Revision) and the Job In General

scales. Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green State University.

Baltzer, W. K., & Smith, P. C. (1990). User’s manual: Job Descriptive Index (JDI). Bowling

Green, OH: Bowling Green State University.

Bayroff, A. G., & Fuchs, E. F. (1970). The Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery.

Arlington, VA: United States Army Behavior and Systems Research Laboratory.

Bennett, G. K., Seashore, H. G., & Wesman, A. G. (1959). Manual for the Differential Aptitude

Tests (3rd ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation.

Betz, N. E., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1987). The career psychology of women. Orlando, FL:
Is Congruence Dead? 105

Academic Press.

Betz, N. E., Fitzgerald, L. F., & Hill, R. E. (1989). Trait-factor theories: Traditional cornerstone

of career theory. In M.B. Arthur, D. T. Hall, & B. S. Lawrence (Eds.), Handbook of

career theory (pp. 26 – 40). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Bobele, R. M., Alston, H. L., Wakefield, J. A., & Schnitzen, J. P. (1975). Relationships among

Holland’s personality types measured by an adjective checklist. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 7(2), 201 – 206.

Borgen, F. H., & Donnay, D. A. C. (1996). Slicing the vocational interest pie one more time:

Comment on Tracey and Rounds. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 48(1), 42 – 52.

Borgen, F. H., & Harmon, L. W. (1996). Linking interest assessment and personality theory: An

example of convergence between practice and theory. In M. L. Savickas & W. B. Walsh

(Eds.), Handbook of career counseling theory and practice (pp. 251 – 266). Palo Alto,

CA: Davies – Black.

Brown, D. (2002). Introduction to theories of career development and choice: Origins, evolution,

and current efforts. In D. Brown & Associates (Eds.), Career choice and development

(4th ed., pp. 3 – 23). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Brown, D. (2003). Career information, career counseling, and career development (8th ed.).

Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Brown, S. D., & Gore, P. A. (1994). An evaluation of interest congruence indices: Distribution

characteristics and measurement properties. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45(3), 310 –

327.

Bureau of Labor Statistics. (1996). Occupational outlook handbook. Washington, DC: United

States Government Printing Office.


Is Congruence Dead? 106

Burgner, L. D. (1994). The Myers-Briggs and Holland typologies as correlates of job

satisfaction. Dissertation Abstracts International, 56 (03), 1732B. (UMI No. 9524227)

Camp, C. C., & Chartrand, J. M. (1992). A comparison and evaluation of interest congruence

indices. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 41(2), 162 – 182.

Campbell, D. P., & Borgen, F. H. (1999). Holland’s theory and the development of interest

inventories. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 55(1), 86 – 101.

Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the

multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56(2), 546 – 553.

Campbell, D. P., & Holland, J. L. (1972). A merger in vocational interest research: Applying

Holland’s theory to Strong’s data. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 2(4), 353 – 376.

Carson, A. D., & Mowsesian, R. (1993). Moderators of the prediction of job satisfaction from

congruence: A test of Holland’s theory. Journal of Career Assessment, 1(2), 130 – 144.

Chartrand, J. M. (1991). The evolution of trait-and-factor career counseling: A person x

environment fit approach. Journal of Counseling and Development, 69(6), 518 – 524.

Chartrand, J. (2001). Strong Interest Explorer. Palo Alto CA: CPP.

Chartrand, J. M., Strong, S. R., & Weitzman, L. M. (1995). The interactional perspective in

vocational psychology: Paradigms, theories, and research practices. In W. B. Walsh & S.

H. Osipow (Eds.), Handbook of vocational psychology: Theory, research, and practice

(2nd ed., pp. 35 – 65). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Chartrand, J., & Walsh, W. B. (1999). What should we expect from congruence? Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 55(1), 136 – 146.

Clark, D. A. (1991). Teacher job satisfaction and personality-environment congruence: An

examination of Holland's theory of vocations. Dissertation Abstracts International, 52


Is Congruence Dead? 107

(06), 1956A. (UMI No. 9123412)

Coddington, T. M. (1998). The role of expectations, personality, and spirituality in the

tenure of Catholic Campus ministers: A three year study. Dissertation Abstracts

International, 59 (09), 5149B. (UMI No. 9905443)

Cole, N. S., Whitney, D. R., & Holland, J. L. (1971). A spatial configuration of occupations.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 1(1), 1 – 9.

Cook, K. W. (1996). The relationship of Holland's concept of congruence to work-related

outcomes. Dissertation Abstracts International, 57 (07), 4758B. (UMI No.

9637024)

(Data Publication Schedule, n.d.) Retrieved January 16, 2007, from

http://www.onetcenter.org/dataPublication.html

Dawis, R. V. (1984). Job satisfaction: Worker aspirations, attitudes, and behavior. In N. C.

Gysbers and Associates (Eds.), Designing careers: Counseling to enhance education,

work, and leisure (pp. 275 – 301). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

Dawis, R. V. (1991). Vocational interests, values, and preferences. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M.

Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology: Vol. 2 (2nd ed., pp.

833 – 871). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Dawis, R. V. (1992). The structure(s) of occupations: Beyond RIASEC. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 40(2), 171 – 178.

Dawis, R. V. (2000). P-E fit as paradigm: Comment on Tinsley (2000). Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 56(2), 180 – 183.

Dik, B. J., & Hansen, J. C. (2004, July). Increasingly precise congruence measurement: The
Is Congruence Dead? 108

modified C index at work. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the American

Psychological Association, Honolulu, HI.

Dik, B. J., Hu, R. S. C., & Hansen, J. C. (2007). An empirical test of the Modified C index and

SII, O*NET, and DHOC occupational code classifications. Journal of Career

Assessment, 15(3), 279 – 300.

Donnay, D. A. C., Morris, M. L., Schaubhut, N. A., & Thompson, R. C. (2005). Strong Interest

Inventory manual: Research, development, and strategies for interpretation (revised ed.).

Mountain View, CA: CPP.

Eagan, A. E., & Walsh, W. B. (1995). Person-environment congruence and coping strategies.

The Career Development Quarterly, 43(3), 246 – 256.

Edwards, J. R. (1991). Person-job fit: A conceptual integration, literature review, and

methodological critique. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review

of industrial and organizational psychology (vol. 6, pp. 283 – 357). New York: Wiley.

Edwards, J. R. (1994). The study of congruence in organizational behavior research: Critique and

a proposed alternative. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58(1)

51 – 100.

Edwards, K. J., & Whitney, D. R. (1972). Structural analysis of Holland’s personality types

using factor and configural analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19(2), 136 – 145.

Eggerth, D. E. (2004). Applying the Bradley-Terry-Luce method to P-E fit. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 64(1), 92 – 107.

Eggerth, D. E. (2006). The complicated pig speaks: A reply to Gore and Brown and Tinsley.

Journal of Career Assessment, 14(2), 289 – 291.

Eggerth, D. E., & Andrew, M. E. (2006). Modifying the C index for use with Holland codes of
Is Congruence Dead? 109

unequal length. Journal of Career Assessment, 14(2), 267 – 275.

Eggerth, D. E., Bowles, S. M., Tunick, R. H., & Andrew, M. E. (2005). Convergent validity of

O*NET Holland code classifications. Journal of Career Assessment, 13(2), 150 – 168.

Elton, C. F., & Smart, J. C. (1988). Extrinsic job satisfaction and person – environment

congruence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32(2), 226 – 238.

Fassinger, R. E. (2005). Theoretical issues in the study of women’s career development:

Building bridges in a brave new world. In W. B. Walsh & M. L. Savickas (Eds.),

Handbook of vocational psychology: Theory, research, and practice (3rd ed., pp. 85 –

124). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Feller, R. W., Honaker, S. L., & Zagzebski, L. M. (2001). Theoretical voices directing the career

development journey: Holland, Harris-Bowlsbey, and Krumboltz. The Career

Development Quarterly, 49(3), 212 – 224.

Finley, R. L. (1989). The relationship of personal and environmental factors to job satisfaction

among counseling professionals: A test of Holland's theory. Dissertation Abstracts

International, 50 (06), 2608B. (UMI No. 8920242)

Fritzsche, B. A., & Parrish, T. J. (2005). Theories and research on job satisfaction. In S. D.

Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Career development and counseling: Putting theory and

research to work (pp. 180 – 202). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

Fritzsche, B. A., Powell, A. B., & Hoffman, R. (1999). Person-environment congruence as a

predictor of customer service performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54(1), 59 –

70.

Furnham, A. (2001). Vocational preference and P-O fit: Reflections on Holland’s theory of

vocational choice. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 50(1), 5 – 29.


Is Congruence Dead? 110

Furnham, A., Toop, A., Lewis, C., & Fisher, A. (1995). P-E fit and job satisfaction: A failure to

support Holland’s theory in three British samples. Personality and Individual

Differences, 19(5), 677 – 690.

Gati, I. (1985). Description of alternative measures of the concepts of vocational interest:

Crystallization, congruence, and coherence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 27(1),

37 – 55.

Gati, I. (1987). Description and validation of a procedure for the interpretation of an interest

inventory score profile. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 34(2), 141 – 148.

Gati, I. (1989). Person - environment fit research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 35(2), 181 – 193.

Gore, P. A., & Brown, S. D. (2006). Simpler may still be better: A reply to Eggerth and

Andrew. Journal of Career Assessment, 14(2), 276 – 282.

Gothard, W. P. (1985). Vocational guidance: Theory and practice. Dover, NH: Croom Helm.

Gottfredson, G. D. (1999). John L. Holland’s contributions to vocational psychology: A review

and evaluation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 55(1), 15 – 40.

Gottfredson, G. D., & Holland, J. L. (1990). A longitudinal test of the influence of congruence:

Job satisfaction, competency utilization, and counterproductive behavior. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 37(4), 389 – 398.

Gottfredson, G. D., & Holland, J. L. (1991). The Position Classification Inventory professional

manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Gottfredson, G. D., & Holland, J. L. (1996). Dictionary of Holland occupational codes (3rd ed.).

Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Griffin, B., & Hesketh, B. (2005). Counseling for work adjustment. In S. D. Brown & R. W.
Is Congruence Dead? 111

Lent (Eds.), Career development and counseling: Putting theory and research to work

(pp. 483 – 505). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

Grotevant, H. D., Cooper, C. R., & Kramer, K. (1986). Exploration as a predictor of congruence

in adolescents’ career choices. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 29(2), 201 – 215.

Guttman, L. (1954). A new approach to factor analysis: The radex. In P. F. Lazarsfeld (Ed.),

Mathematical thinking in the social sciences (pp. 258 – 348). Glencoe, IL: The Free

Press.

Hackett, G., Lent, R. W., & Greenhaus, J. H. (1991). Advances in vocational theory and

research: A 20-year retrospective. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 38(1), 3 – 38.

Hansen, J. C. (1984). The measurement of vocational interests: Issues and future directions. In S.

D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (pp. 99 – 136). New

York: John Wiley.

Hansen, J. C. (2000). Strong Interest Inventory. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of

psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 490 – 491). Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Hansen, J. C. (2005). Assessment of interests. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Career

development and counseling: Putting theory and research to work (pp. 281 – 304).

Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

Hansen, J. C., & Campbell, D. P. (1985). Manual for the Strong-Campbell Interest Inventory.

Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Harmon, L. W., Hansen, J. C., Borgen, F. H., & Hammer, A. L. (1994). Strong Interest

Inventory: Applications and technical guide. Palo Alto, CA: CPP.

Harrington, T. F., & O’Shea, A. J. (Eds.). (1984). Guide for occupational exploration (2nd
Is Congruence Dead? 112

ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. (Distributed for National Forum

Foundation; originally published in 1979 by the Employment and Training Administration,

United States Department of Labor).

Harris, J. I., Mortizen, S. K., Robitschek, C., Imhoff, A., & Lynch, J. L. A. (2001). The

comparative contributions of congruence and social support in career outcomes. Career

Development Quarterly, 49(4), 314 – 323.

Healy, C. C., & Mourton, D. L. (1983). Derivatives of the Self-Directed Search: Potential

clinical and evaluative issues. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23(3), 318-328.

Heesacker, M., Elliott, T. R., & Howe, L. A. (1988). Does the Holland code predict job

satisfaction and productivity in clothing factory workers? Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 35(2), 144 – 148.

Herr, E. L., Cramer, S. H., & Niles, S. G. (2004). Career guidance and counseling through the

lifespan: Systematic approaches (6th ed.). Boston: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.

Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., Peterson, R. O., & Capwell, D. F. (1957). Job attitudes: Review of

research and opinion. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychological Service of Pittsburgh.

Hesketh, B. (2000). The next millennium of “fit” research: Comments on “The congruence

myth: An analysis of the efficacy of the person-environment fit model” by H.E.A.

Tinsley. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56(2), 191 – 196.

Hodgson, M. L., & Cramer, S. H. (1977). The relationship between selected self-estimated and

measured abilities in adolescents. Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance, 10(2), 98 –

104.

Hogan, R., & Blake, R. (1999). John Holland’s vocational typology and personality theory.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 55(1), 41 – 56.


Is Congruence Dead? 113

Hoile, P. C. (2000). Improving person-environment fit: Increasing job satisfaction by matching

employees' preferred levels of arousal to job complexity within Holland's model of

occupational types. Dissertation Abstracts International, 61 (05), 2804B. (UMI No.

9974231)

Holland, J. L. (1963). Explorations of a theory of vocational choice and achievement: II. A four-

year prediction study. Psychological Reports, 12(2) (Mono Suppl No. 4-V12), 547 – 594.

Holland, J. L. (1966). The psychology of vocational choice: A theory of personality types and

model environments. Waltham, MA: Blaisdell Publishing.

Holland, J. L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of careers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:

Prentice-Hall.

Holland, J. L. (1985a). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and

work environments. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Holland, J. L. (1985b). Manual for the Vocational Preference Inventory. Odessa, FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources.

Holland, J. L. (1987). Some speculation about the investigation of person-environment

transactions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 31(3), 337 – 340.

Holland, J. L. (1992). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work

environments (2nd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

Holland, J. L. (1994). The occupations finder. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Holland, J. L. (1996). Integrating career theory and practice: The current situation and some

potential remedies. In M. L. Savickas & W. B. Walsh (Eds.), Handbook of career

counseling theory and practice (pp. 1 – 11). Palo Alto, CA: Davies – Black.

Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work
Is Congruence Dead? 114

environments (3rd ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Holland, J. L. (1999). Why interest inventories are also personality inventories. In M. L.

Savickas & A. R. Spokane (Eds.), Vocational interests: Meaning, measurement, and

counseling use (pp. 87 – 101). Palo Alto, CA: Davies – Black.

Holland, J. L., Fritzsche, B. A., & Powell, A. B. (1994). The Self-Directed Search technical

manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Holland, J. L., Whitney, D. R., Cole, N. S., & Richards, J. M (1969). An empirical occupational

classification derived from a theory of personality and intended for practice and research

(ACT Research Report No. 29). Iowa City, IA: American College Testing Program.

Hoppock, R. (1935). Job satisfaction. New York: Harper & Brothers.

Hubert, L., & Arabie, P. (1987). Evaluating order hypotheses within proximity matrices.

Psychological Bulletin, 102(1), 172 – 178.

Hulin, C. L., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Job attitudes. In W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen, & R. J.

Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Industrial and organizational psychology

(Vol. 12, pp. 255 – 276). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

Iachan, R. (1984). A measure of agreement for use with the Holland classification system.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 24(2), 133 – 141.

Iachan, R. (1990). Some extensions of the Iachan congruence index. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 36(2), 176 – 180.

Ironson, G. H., Smith, P. C., Brannick, M. T., Gibson, W. M. & Paul, K. B. (1989). Construction

of a job in general scale: A comparison of global, composite and specific measures.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(2), 193 – 200.

Jagger, L., Neukrug, E., & McAuliffe, G. (1992). Congruence between personality traits and
Is Congruence Dead? 115

chosen occupation as a predictor of job satisfaction for people with disabilities.

Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 36(1), 53 – 60.

Kahn, R. L. (1972). The meaning of work: Interpretation and proposals for measurement. In A.

Campbell & P. E. Converse (Eds.), The human meaning of social change (pp. 159 – 203).

New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Kalleberg, A. L. (1977). Work values and job rewards: A theory of job satisfaction. American

Sociological Review, 42(1), 124 – 143.

Kelso, G. I., Holland, J. L., & Gottfredson, G. D. (1977). The relation of self-reported

competencies to aptitude test scores. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 10(1), 99 – 103.

Khan, S. B., Alvi, S. A., & Kirkwood, K. J. (1990). Validity of Holland’s model: A confirmatory

factor analysis. Canadian Journal of Counselling, 24(3), 178 – 185.

Kwak, J. C., & Pulvino, C. J. (1982). A mathematical model for comparing Holland’s personality

and environmental codes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 21(2), 231 – 241.

LaBarbera, D. M. (2005). Physician assistant Self-Directed Search Holland codes. Journal of

Career Assessment, 13(3), 337 – 346.

Lachterman, B. L., & Meir, E. I. (2004). The impact of work setting congruence on well-being.

Journal of Career Assessment, 12(2), 150 – 168.

Lent, E. B., & Lopez, F. G. (1996). Congruence from many angles: Relations of multiple

congruence indices to job satisfaction among adult workers. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 49(1), 24 – 37.

Levine, J., Nottingham, J., Paige, B., & Lewis, P. (2000). Transitioning O*NET to the Standard

Occupational Classification. Raleigh, NC: National O*NET Consortium.

Locke, E. A. (1969). What is job satisfaction? Organizational Behavior and Human


Is Congruence Dead? 116

Performance, 4(4), 309 – 336.

Locke, E. A. (1983). The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.),

Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1297 – 1349). New York:

John Wiley.

Lofquist, L. H., & Dawis, R. V. (1969). Adjustment to work: A psychological view of man’s

problems in a work-oriented society. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Lowman, R. L., & Carson, A. D. (2000). Integrating assessment data into career counseling. In

D. A. Luzzo (Ed.), Career counseling of college students: An empirical guide to

strategies that work (pp. 121 – 136). Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Lowman, R. L., & Williams, R. E. (1987). Validity of self-ratings of abilities and competencies.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 31(1), 1 – 13.

Mabe, P. A., & West, S. G. (1982). Validity of self-evaluation of ability: A review and meta-

analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67(3), 280 – 296.

Meehl, P. E., & Rosen, A. (1955). Antecedent probability and the efficiency of psychometric

signs, patterns, or cutting scores. Psychological Bulletin, 52(3), 194 – 216.

Meir, E. I. (1993). Conceptual elaboration of Holland’s theory on personality-occupation

congruence. Man and Work, 4(1 – 2), 70 – 59.

Meir, E. I. (1995). Elaboration of the relation between interest congruence and satisfaction.

Journal of Career Assessment, 3(3), 341 – 346.

Meir, E. I., & Green-Eppel, T. (1999). Congruence, skill utilization, and group importance as

predictors of well being in army reserve units. Journal of Career Assessment, 7(4), 429 –

438.
Is Congruence Dead? 117

Meir, E. I., Hadas, C., & Noyfeld, M. (1997). Person-environment fit in small army units.

Journal of Career Assessment, 5(1), 21 – 29.

Meir, E. I., Keinan, G., & Segal, Z. (1986). Group importance as a mediator between

personality-environment congruence and satisfaction. Journal of Vocational Behavior,

28(1), 60 – 69.

Meir, E. I., & Navon, M. (1992). A longitudinal examination of congruence hypotheses. Journal

of Vocational Behavior, 41(1), 35 – 47.

Meir, E. I., & Segal-Halevi, A. (2001). Environmental congruence, group importance, and well

being among paratroopers. Journal of Career Assessment, 9(2), 169 – 183.

Meir, E. I., Tziner, A, & Glazner, Y. (1997). Environmental congruence, group importance, and

job satisfaction. Journal of Career Assessment, 5(3), 343 – 353.

Meir, E. I., & Yaari, Y. (1988). The relationship between congruent specialty choice within

occupations and satisfaction. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 33(1), 99 – 117.

Miller, M. J. (1992). Correlations among three measures of congruence. Measurement and

Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 25(3), 113 – 120.

Minor, C. W. (1992). Career development theories and models. In D. H. Montross & C.

J.Shinkman (Eds.), Career development: Theory and practice (pp. 7 – 34). Springfield,

IL: Charles C Thomas.

Moody, A. (1983). The Compatibility Index: Beyond high point codes. American Mental Health

Counselors Association Journal, 5(2), 85 – 89.

Morris, M. A. (2003). A meta-analytic investigation of vocational interest-based job fit, and its

relationship to job satisfaction, performance, and turnover. Dissertation Abstracts

International, 64(05), 2428B. (UMI No. 3089788)


Is Congruence Dead? 118

Morrow, J. M. (1971). A test of Holland’s theory of vocational choice. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 18(5), 422 – 425.

Mount, M. K., & Muchinsky, P. M. (1978). Person-environment congruence and employee job

satisfaction: A test of Holland’s theory. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 13(1), 84 – 100.

Nafziger, D. H, Holland, J. L., & Gottfredson, G. D. (1975). Student-college congruency as a

predictor of satisfaction. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22(2), 132 – 139.

National Center for O*NET Development. (2006). Updating the O*NET-SOC taxonomy:

Summary and Implementation. Raleigh, NC: Author.

National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. (1999). Stress…at work (DHHS

Publication No. 99-101). Cincinnati, OH: Author.

National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. (2002, April). The changing organization

of work and the safety and health of working people: Knowledge gaps and research

directions (DHHS Publication No. 2002-116). Cincinnati, OH: Author.

National O*NET Consortium, Employment and Training Administration, United States

Department of Labor. (n.d.). Occupational Information Network O*NET Online

[Interactive World Wide Web application]. Retrieved December 18, 2006 from

http://online.onetcenter.org/

Office of Federal Statistical Policy and Standards, United States Department of Commerce.

(1980). Standard occupational classification manual. Washington, DC: United States

Government Printing Office.

Oleski, D., & Subich, L. M. (1996). Congruence and career change in employed adults. Journal

of Vocational Behavior, 49(3), 221 – 229.

Osipow, S. H. (1987). Applying person-environment theory to vocational behavior. Journal of


Is Congruence Dead? 119

Vocational Behavior, 31(3), 333 –336.

Osipow, S. H. (1994). Moving career theory into the twenty-first century. In M. L. Savickas & R.

W. Lent (Eds.), Convergence in career development theories: Implications for science

and practice (pp. 217 – 224). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Osipow, S. H., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1996). Theories of career development (4th ed.). Boston:

Allyn & Bacon.

Parsons, F. (1909). Choosing a vocation. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

Phillips, S. D., & Jome, L. M. (2005). Vocational choices: What do we know? What do we need

to know? In W. B. Walsh & M. L. Savickas (Eds.), Handbook of vocational psychology:

Theory, research, and practice (3rd ed., pp. 127 – 153). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Poh, S. H. (1996, November). Perceived and demonstrated aptitudes: Same or different? Paper

presented at the joint conference of the Educational Research Association of Singapore

and the Australian Association for Research in Education, Singapore Polytechnic,

Singapore.

Portscheller, M. K. (1992). Job satisfaction of CAOs of continuing higher education, Holland's

congruency construct, and selected variables. Dissertation Abstracts International,

53(06), 1755 - 1756A. (UMI No. 9227172)

Prediger, D. J. (1982). Dimensions underlying Holland’s hexagon: Missing link between

interests and occupations? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 21(3), 259 – 287.

Prediger, D. J. (2000). Holland's hexagon is alive and well--though somewhat out of shape:

Response to Tinsley. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56(2), 197 – 204.

Prediger, D. J., & Vansickle, T. R. (1992). Who claims Holland’s hexagon is perfect? Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 40(2), 210 – 219.


Is Congruence Dead? 120

Quinn, R. P., Staines, G. L., & McCullough, M. R. (1974). Job satisfaction: Is there a trend?

[Manpower Research Monograph No. 30]. Washington, DC: United States Department of

Labor Manpower Administration.

Research Department. (2002). Instrument development: Reliability and validity of the Strong

Interest Explorer (Technical Rep. No. 2002-2). White Bear Lake, MN: CPP.

Richards, J. M. (1993). Career development: A ten-year longitudinal study in population

scientists. Journal of Career Assessment, 1(2), 181 – 192.

Robbins, P. I., Thomas, L. E., Harvey, D. W., & Kandefer, C. (1978). Career change and

congruence of personality type: An examination of DOT-derived work environment

designations. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 13(1), 15 – 25.

Rounds, J. (1995). Vocational interests: Evaluating structural hypotheses. In D. Lubinski & R. V.

Dawis (Eds.), Assessing individual differences in human behavior: New concepts,

methods, and findings (pp. 177 – 232). Palo Alto, CA: Davies-Black.

Rounds, J., McKenna, M. C., Hubert, L., & Day, S. X. (2000). Tinsley on Holland: A misshapen

argument. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56(2), 205 – 215.

Rounds, J. Smith, T., Hubert, L., Lewis, P., & Rivkin, D. (1999). Development of Occupational

Interest Profiles for O*NET. Raleigh, NC: National Center for O*NET Development.

Rounds, J., Tracey, T. J., & Hubert, L. (1992). Methods for evaluating vocational interest

structural hypotheses. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 40(2), 239 – 259.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic

motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68 – 78.

Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40(3), 437 – 453.

Schwartz, R. H. (1992). Is Holland’s theory worthy of so much attention, or should vocational


Is Congruence Dead? 121

psychology move on? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 40(2), 179 – 187.

Sharp, L. M., & Frankel, J. (1983). Respondent burden: A test of some common assumptions.

Public Opinion Quarterly, 47(1), 36 – 53.

Shaw, T. A. (2005). Occupational personality of funeral service education students: Congruence

and variable interaction. Dissertation Abstracts International, 67 (01), 160A. (UMI No.

3204672)

Smart, J. C. (1976). Distinctive career orientations of Holland personality types. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 8(3), 313 – 319.

Smart, J. C., Elton, C. F., & McLaughlin, G. W. (1986). Person-environment congruence and job

satisfaction. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 29(2) 216 – 225.

Smith, P. C., Kendall, L. M, & Hulin, C. L. (1969). The measurement of satisfaction in work and

retirement. Chicago: Rand McNally.

Spokane, A. R. (1979). Validity of the Holland categories for college women and men. Journal

of College Student Personnel, 20(4), 335 – 340.

Spokane, A. R. (1985). A review of research on person-environment congruence in Holland’s

theory of careers. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 26(3), 306 – 343.

Spokane, A. R., & Cruza-Guet, M. C. (2005). Holland’s theory of vocational personalities in

work environments. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Career development and

counseling: Putting theory and research to work (pp. 24 – 41). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

Spokane, A. R., & Derby, D. P. (1979). Congruence, personality pattern, and satisfaction in

college women. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 15(1), 36 – 42.

Spokane, A. R., Luchetta, E. J., & Richwine, M. H. (2002). Holland’s theory of personalities in
Is Congruence Dead? 122

work environments. In D. Brown (Ed.), Career choice and development (4th ed., pp. 373

– 426). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Spokane, A. R., Meir, E. I., & Catalano, M. (2000). Person-environment congruence and

Holland's theory: A review and reconsideration. Journal of Vocational Behavior,

57(2), 137 – 187.

Strahan, R. F, & Severinghaus, J. B. (1992). Dealing with ties in Holland-type consistency

measures. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 40(2), 260 – 267.

Strong, E. K. (1958). Satisfactions and interests. American Psychologist, 13(8), 449 – 456.

Subich, L. M. (1992). Holland’s theory: “Pushing the envelope”. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 40(2), 201 – 206.

Swaney, K., & Prediger, D. (1985). The relationship between interest-occupation congruence

and job satisfaction. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 26(1), 13-24.

Swanson, J. L. (1992). In search of structural validity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 40(2),

229 – 238.

Swanson, J. L. (1993). Integrated assessment of vocational interests and self-rated skills and

abilities. Journal of Career Assessment, 1(1), 50 – 65.

Swanson, J. L. (1996). The theory Is the practice: Trait and factor/person-environment fit

counseling. In M. L. Savickas & W. B. Walsh (Eds.), Handbook of career counseling

theory and practice (pp. 93 – 108). Palo Alto, CA: Davies – Black Publishing.

Swanson, J. L., & Gore, P. A. (2000). Advances in vocational psychology and research. In S. D.

Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (3rd ed., pp. 233 – 269).

New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Thomas, L. E., & Robbins, P. I. (1979). Personality and work environment congruence of
Is Congruence Dead? 123

mid-life career changers. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 52(3), 177 – 183.

Tinsley, H.E.A. (2000). The congruence myth: An analysis of the efficacy of the person-

environment fit model. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56(2), 147 – 179.

Tinsley, H. E. A. (2006). A pig in a suit is still a pig: A comment on “Modifying the C index for

use with Holland codes of unequal length.” Journal of Career Assessment, 14(2), 283 –

288.

Toenjes, C. M., & Borgen, F. H. (1974). Validity generalization of Holland’s hexagonal model.

Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance, 7(2), 79 – 85.

Tokar, D. M., & Subich, L. M. (1997). Relative contributions of congruence and personality

dimensions to job satisfaction. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 50(3), 482 – 491.

Tracey, T. J., & Rounds, J. (1992). Evaluating the RIASEC circumplex using high-point codes.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 41(3), 295 – 311.

Tracey, T. J., & Rounds, J. (1993). Evaluating Holland’s and Gati’s vocational interest models:

A structural meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 229 – 246.

Tracey, T. J. G., & Rounds, J. (1996). The spherical representation of vocational interests.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 48(1), 3 – 41.

Tracey, T. J. G., & Rounds, J. (1997). Circular structure of vocational interests. In R. Plutchik &

H. R. Conte (Eds.), Circumplex models of personality and emotions (pp. 183 – 201).

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Tranberg, M., Slane, S., & Ekeberg, S. E. (1993). The relation between interest congruence and

satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 42(3), 253 – 264.

Tsabari, O., Tziner, A., & Meir, E. I. (2005). Updated meta-analysis on the relationship between

congruence and satisfaction. Journal of Career Assessment, 13(2), 216 – 232.


Is Congruence Dead? 124

United States Department of Labor Employment and Training Administration. (1972). Handbook

for analyzing jobs. Washington, DC: United States Government Printing Office.

United States Department of Labor Employment and Training Administration. (1974). Definition

writer’s manual. Washington, DC: United States Government Printing Office.

United States Department of Labor Employment and Training Administration. (1977).

Dictionary of occupational titles (4th ed.). Washington, DC: United States Government

Printing Office.

United States Department of Labor Employment and Training Administration. (1979). Guide for

occupational exploration. Washington, DC: United States Government Printing Office.

United States Department of Labor Employment and Training Administration. (1991).

Dictionary of occupational titles (4th ed., revised). Washington, DC: United States

Government Printing Office.

Villwock, J. D., Schnitzen, J. P., & Carbonari, J. P. (1976). Holland’s personality constructs as

predictors of stability of choice. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 9(1), 77 – 85.

Wakefield, J. A., & Doughtie, E. B. (1973). The geometric relationship between Holland’s

personality typology and the Vocational Preference Inventory. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 20(6), 513 – 518.

Walsh, W. B. (1974). Consistent occupational preferences and personality. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 4(2), 145 – 153.


Is Congruence Dead? 125

Walsh, W. B. (1999). What we know and need to know: A few comments. In M. L. Savickas &

A. R. Spokane (Eds.), Vocational interests: Meaning, measurement, and counseling use

(pp. 371 – 382). Palo Alto, CA: Davies – Black.

Walsh, W. B., & Lewis, R. O. (1972). Consistent, inconsistent, and undecided career preferences

and personality. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 2(3), 309 – 316.

Walsh, W. B., & Osipow, S. H. (1986). Introduction: The assessment of interests. In W. B.

Walsh & S. H. Osipow (Eds.), Advances in vocational psychology: The assessment of

interests (vol. 1, pp. vii – ix). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Wanous, J. P., & Lawler, E. E. (1972). Measurement and meaning of job satisfaction. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 56(2), 95 – 105.

Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A. E., & Hudy, M. J. (1997). Overall job satisfaction: How good are

single-item measures? Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(2), 247 – 252.

Warr, P. (1991). Mental health, well-being and job satisfaction. In B. Hesketh & A. Adams

(Eds.), Psychological perspectives on occupational health and rehabilitation (pp. 143 –

165). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Weiss, D. J., Dawis, R. V., England, G. W., & Lofquist, L. H. (1966). Instrumentation for the

Theory of Work Adjustment. Minnesota Studies in Vocational Rehabilitation, XXI.

Weiss, D.J., Dawis, R. V., England, G. W., & Lofquist, L. H. (1967). Manual for the Minnesota

Satisfaction Questionnaire. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota.

Wiggins, J. D. (1984). Personality-environmental factors related to job satisfaction of school

counselors. Vocational Guidance Quarterly, 33(2), 169 – 177.

Wiggins, J. D., & Moody, A. (1981). Compatibility index descriptors. Dover, DE: Training

Associates Limited.
Is Congruence Dead? 126

Wiggins, J. D., Lederer, D. A., Salkowe, A., & Rys, G. S. (1983). Job satisfaction related to

tested congruence and differentiation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23(1), 112 – 121.

Wiggins, J. D., & Weslander, D. L. (1979). Tested personality typologies and marital

compatibility. American Mental Health Counselors Association Journal, 1(1), 44 – 52.

Wigington, J. H., & Apostal, R. A. (1973). Personality differences among men in selected Air

Force specialties. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 20(5), 454 – 458.

Williamson, E. G. (1966). An historical perspective of the vocational guidance movement. In H.

J. Peters & J. C. Hansen (Eds.), Vocational guidance and career development: Selected

readings (pp. 229 – 238). New York: MacMillan.

Wolfe, L. K., & Betz, N. E. (1981). Traditionality of choice and sex-role identification as

predictors of the congruence of occupational choice in college women. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 18(1), 43 – 55.

Young, G., Tokar, D. M., & Subich, L. M. (1998). Congruence revisited: Do 11 indices

differentially predict job satisfaction and is the relation moderated by person and situation

variables? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 52(2), 208 – 223.

Zener, T. B., & Schnuelle, L. (1972, February). An evaluation of the Self-Directed Search: A

guide to educational and vocational planning (Report No. 124). Baltimore, MD: The

Johns Hopkins University Center for Social Organization of Schools.


Is Congruence Dead? 127

Appendix A

Compatibility Index

Scale

Score CI Decision Rule


8 The letters and ordering of both codes match exactly (e.g., ASE – ASE).

7 The first letters match. The second and third letters of one code are reversed in the
other code (e.g. ASE – AES), or the first and second letters of both codes match
in the same order (e.g., ASE – ASI).

6 All three letters of both codes match, but the first letters are not the same (e.g.,
ASE – SEA).

5 The first letters match, with the second or third letter of one code matching the
third letter of the other code (e.g., ASE – AIS), or the first and second letters
match in reverse order (e.g., ASE – SAI), or the first and second letters of one
code match the first and third letters of the other code in reverse order (e.g., ASE
– SIA).

4 The first and second or third letters of one code match any two letters of the other
code in any order (e.g., ASE – SIA), or the first letter of one code matches the
first letter of the other code (e.g., ASE – AIR).

3 The second and third letters of one code are found in the other code in any order
(e.g., ASE – EIS), or the first letter of one code matches the second letter of the
other code (e.g., ASE – RAC).

2 The first letter of one code matches the third letter of the other code (e.g., ASE –
RCA), or the second letter of one code matches the second or third letter of the
other code (e.g., ASE – IRS).

1 The third letters of both codes match (e.g., ASE – ICE).

0 No letters match in either code (e.g., ASE – CRI).

Calculation

CAE – CRI

The types in the first-letter positions match each other (i.e., C). The second and third letters of
the codes (i.e., A, E, R, I) are unique and do not match in any combination. The CI calculation of
CAE – CRI therefore equals a congruence score of 4.
Is Congruence Dead? 128

Appendix B

K – P Index

Formula

X = (W1 + W2 + W3)-1 (W1AD + W2BE + W3CF)

Variable Values

X= K-P index congruence value

W1 = Weight assigned to the first-letter position

W2 = Weight assigned to the second-letter position

W3 = Weight assigned to the third-letter position

A= Type in the first-letter position of code 1

B= Type in the second-letter position of code 1

C= Type in the third-letter position of code 1

D= Type in the first-letter position of code 2

E= Type in the second-letter position of code 2

F= Type in the third-letter position of code 2

AD = Value of the hexagonal segment distance between the type in the first-letter position of
code 1 and the type in the first-letter position of code 2

BE = Value of the hexagonal segment distance between the type in the second-letter position
of code 1 and the type in the second-letter position of code 2

CF = Value of the hexagonal segment distance between the type in the third-letter position of
code 1 and the type in the third-letter position of code 2
Is Congruence Dead? 129

Table B1
K – P Index Hexagonal Segment Distance Proportion Values

Hexagonal Distance Hexagonal Distance Hexagonal Distance


Segment Proportion Segment Proportion Segment Proportion
Value Value Value
A-A 1.0 E-A .35 R-A .16
A-C .11 E-C .68 R-C .36
A-E .35 E-E 1.0 R-E .30
A-I .34 E-I .16 R-I .46
A-R .16 E-R .30 R-R 1.0
A-S .42 E-S .54 R-S .21
C-A .11 I-A .34 S-A .42
C-C 1.0 I-C .16 S-C .38
C-E .68 I-E .16 S-E .54
C-I .16 I-I 1.0 S-I .30
C-R .36 I-R .46 S-R .21
C-S .38 I-S .30 S-S 1.0

Calculation

SEC – SCE

W1 = 4 D=S
W2 = 2 E=C
W3 = 1 F=E
A=S AD = 1.0
B=E BE = .68
C=C CF = .68

X = (4 + 2 + 1) -1 [(4)SS + (2)EC + (1)CE]

X = (1/7)[(4)1.0 + (2).68 + (1).68]

X = (1/7) [4 + 1.36 + .68]

X = (1/7) (6.04)

X = .8629
Is Congruence Dead? 130

Appendix C

M Index

Tables

Table C1
M Index Two-Letter Code Weights

First Letter Second Letter Other Letters


First Letter 5 2 0
Second Letter 2 1 0

Table C2
M Three-Letter Code Weights

First Letter Second Letter Third Letter Other Letters


First Letter 22 10 4 0
Second Letter 10 5 2 0
Third Letter 4 2 1 0

Calculation

REC – CIR

• The type in the first-letter position of the first code matches the type in the third-letter
position of the second code (i.e., R). This combination has a weighted value of 4.

• The type in the third-letter position of the first code matches the type in the first-letter
position of the second code (i.e., C). This combination has a weighted value of 4.

• There are no other pairs of matching types within the two codes.

M = R1R3 + C3C1

M=4+4

M=8

REC – CIR = 8
Is Congruence Dead? 131

Appendix D

Sb Index

Step 1

Determine which summary codes will be examined. For example, RIASEC profile scores
may be 14, 12, 3, 0, 5, and 5, respectively. As the scores for R and I are relatively higher than the
scores for A, S, E, and C, it seems most reasonable to consider this code to be RI. Here, for
purposes of the example, RI will be compared to the code IEA.

Step 2

Assign vector values. Each summary code is assigned values to the types that appear in
the respective codes. A value of 0 is assigned to the types that do not appear in the codes, and a
value of 1 is assigned to the types that do appear in the codes. Additionally, in this example, here
the code RI has been assigned as code x and the code IEA has been assigned as code y in order to
facilitate later calculations:

Vector for Code RI (code x) Vector for Code IEA code (y)
R = 1 R = 0
I = 1 I = 1
A = 0 A = 1
S = 0 S = 0
E = 0 E = 1
C = 0 C = 0

Formula

Sb = αC + L – D

Variable Values

α= Weighting parameter equal to the relative number of shared types between codes

C= Relative number of types common to both codes defined as

K .

K+I+J

K= Number of common types between codes (i.e., the number of types


common to both profiles that have a vector value of 1).

I= Number of unshared types in code x (i.e., the number of types that


have a vector value of 1 in code x and a vector value of 0 in code y).
Is Congruence Dead? 132

J= Number of unshared types in code y (i.e., the number of types that


have a vector value of 1 in code y and a vector value of 0 in code x).

L= Positive constant added to the Sb index in order to prevent negative values. Equal to the
value of the largest possible distance between two types.

D= Mean distances between the unshared types defined as

ΣiΣjd(xi,yj) + βΣiΣkd(xiyk) + γΣjΣkd(xkyj)


(I x J) + K x (I + J)

β= Weighting parameter

γ= Weighting parameter

ΣiΣjd(xi,yj) = Sum of the distances between the unshared types in code x and the
unshared types in code y.

ΣiΣkd(xiyk) = Sum of the distances between the unshared types in code x and the
shared types in code y.

γΣjΣkd(xkyj) = Sum of the distances between the shared types in code x and the
unshared types in code y.

Table D1
Sb Index Distances

Holland Type R I A S E C
R --- --- --- --- --- ---

I 1 --- --- --- --- ---

A 2 1 --- --- --- ---

S 3 2 1 --- --- ---

E 2 3 2 1 --- ---

C 1 2 3 2 1 ---
Is Congruence Dead? 133

Calculation

RI – IEA

α=1

C=¼

K=1

I=1

J=2

C= 1 . = 1/4
1+1+2

L=3

D=1
β=1

γ=1

ΣiΣjd(xi,yj) = 4

ΣiΣkd(xiyk) = 1

γΣjΣkd(xkyj) = 4

D = 4 + (1)(1) + (1)(4) D=4+1+4 D= 9 D=1


(1 x 2) + 1 x (1 + 2) (2 + 1) x 3 9
Is Congruence Dead? 134

Sb = αC + L – D

Sb = (1)¼ + 3 - 1

Sb = ¼ + 2

Sb = 2.25
Is congruence dead? 135

Appendix E

The C Index

Formula

C = (3)(Xi) + (2)(Xi) + (1)(Xi)

Variable Values

Xi = Hexagonal distances between pairs of Holland types

Table E1
Distances between Holland Types Used in the Calculation of the C Index

Holland Type R I A S E C
R 3 --- --- --- --- ---
I 2 3 --- --- --- ---
A 1 2 3 --- --- ---
S 0 1 2 3 -- --
E 1 0 1 2 3 --
C 2 1 0 1 2 3

Calculation

AEI – CAE

Xi

A–C=0

E–A=1

I–E=0

C = (3)(AC) + (2)EA) + (1)(IE)

C = (3)(0) + (2)(1) + (1)(0)

C=0+2+0

C=2
Is congruence dead? 136

Appendix F

The Modified C Index

Case 1: Three Letters x Three Letters

Formula

C3x3 = 3 ( X1Y1) + 2 ( X2Y2) + 1 ( X3Y3)

Example

The modified C index and the C index are computationally equivalent in this case.
Mathematical modification of the C index is not necessary. The two indexes yield identical
scores.
Case 2: Two Letters x Two Letters

Formula

In this case, the expression

Ccase 2 = 3 ( X1Y1) + 2 ( X2Y2)

represents the comparison of two two-letter codes according to the modified C index model.
Calculation using the same values that are used for the C3x3 index could equal only a maximum
value of 15, however. Eggerth and Andrew (2006) explain that perfect congruence between two-
letter profiles should count for the same as perfect congruence between three-letter profiles (i.e.,
a value of 18). A multiplicative scaling constant of 18/15 is added to this formula in order to put
the C2x2 index on the same metric with the C3x3 index:

C2x2 = 18/15 [3 ( X1Y1) + 2 ( X2Y2)]

Example

AE – CA

Variable Values

X1Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between A and C is 0.

X2Y2 = Hexagonal distance value between E and A is 1.


Is congruence dead? 137

Calculation

C2x2 = 18/15 [3 (AC) + 2 (EA)]

C2x2 = 18/15 [3(0) + 2(1)]

C2x2 = 18/15 (0 + 2)

C2x2 = 18/15 (2)

C2x2 = 2.4

Case 3: One Letter x One Letter

Formula

Similar to Case 2, the expression

Ccase 3 = 3 ( X1Y1)

could have a maximum value of only 9 using the same values that are used for the C3x3 index. A
multiplicative scaling constant of 18/9 is added, which reduces to a value of 2:

C1x1 = 2 [3 ( X1Y1)]

which can be reduced further to

C1x1 = 6 ( X1Y1)

Example

A–A

Variable Values

X1Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between A and A is 3.

Calculation

C1x1 = 6 (AA)

C1x1 = 6 (3)

C1x1 = 18
Is congruence dead? 138

Case 4: Three Letters x One Letter

Formula

In this case, the type that makes-up the one-letter code “stands for” the second- and third-
letter positions so that it can be compared to a three-letter summary code:

C3x1 = 3 ( X1Y1) + 2 ( X2Y1) + 1 ( X3Y1)

Example

S - SER

Variable Values

X1Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between S and S is 3.

X2Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between S and E is 2.

X3Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between S and R is 0.

Calculation

C3x1 = 3 (SS) + 2 (SE) + 1 (SR)

C3x1 = 3 (3) + 2 (2) + 1 (0)

C3x1 = 9 + 4 + 0

C3x1 = 13

Case 5: Two Letters x One Letter

Formula

The C2x1 index combines the rationales of the C2x2 index and the C3x1 index. A
multiplicative scaling constant of 18/15 is added, and the type that makes-up the one-letter code
“stands for” the second-letter position:

C2x1 = 18/15 [3 (X1Y1) + 2 (X2Y1)]

Example

CR – C
Is congruence dead? 139

Variable Values

X1Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between C and C is 3.

X2Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between R and C is 2.

Calculation

C2x1 = 18/15 [3 (CC) + 2 (RC)]

C2x1 = 18/15 [3 (3) + 2 (2)]

C2x1 = 18/15 (9 + 4)

C2x1 = 18/15 (13)

C2x1 = 15.6

Case 6: Three Letters x Two Letters

Formula

This case necessitates the use of a weighted composite of the types that compose the two-
letter summary code. This value accounts for the “missing” third-letter position in the two-letter
code. As the C index uses the weights 3 and 2 for the first and second code letter positions, the
modified C index weighting ratio of the first-letter type to the second-letter type is 3:2:

WC = 1 {1/5 [3 (X3Y1) + 2 (X3Y2)]}

The substitution of the WC value into the third-letter position results in the formula:

C3x2 = 3 (X1Y1) + 2 (X2Y2) + 1 {1/5 [3 (X3Y1) + 2 (X3Y2)]}

Example

IA – CAI

Variable Values

X1Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between I and C is 1.

X2Y2 = Hexagonal distance value between A and A is 3.

X3Y1 = Hexagonal distance value between I and I is 3.

X3Y2 = Hexagonal distance value between I and A is 2.


Is congruence dead? 140

Calculation

C3x2 = 3 (IC) + 2 (AA) + 1 {1/5 [3 (II) + 2 (IA)]}

C3x2 = 3 (1) + 2 (3) + 1 {1/5 [3 (3) + 2 (2)]}

C3x2 = 3 + 6 + 1 [(1/5) (9 + 4)]

C3x2 = 3 + 6 + 1 [(1/5) (13)]

C3x2 = 3 + 6 + (1) (2.6)

C3x2 = 3 + 6 + 2.6

C3x2 = 11.6
Is congruence dead? 141

Appendix G

The Substitution C Index

Case 1: Three Letters x Three Letters

Formula

C3x3 = 3 ( Xi) + 2 ( Xi) + 1 ( Xi)

Example

The C index and the substitution C index are computationally equivalent in this case. The
two indexes yield identical scores.

Case 2: Two Letters x Two Letters

Example

AE – CA

A is substituted in the third-letter position in code AE. C is substituted in the third-letter


position in code CA:

AEA – CAC

Variable Values

Xi = Hexagonal distance value between A and C is 0.

Hexagonal distance value between E and A is 1.

Calculation

C = (3) (AC) + (2) (EA) + (1) (AC)

C = (3) (0) + (2) (1) + (1) (0)

C=0+2+0

C=2
Is congruence dead? 142

Case 3: One Letter x One Letter

Example

A–A

In both codes, A is substituted in the second-letter and third-letter positions:

AAA – AAA

Variable Values

Xi = Hexagonal distance value between A and A is 3.

Calculation

C = (3)(AA) + (2)(AA) + (1)(AA)

C = (3)(3) + (2)(3) + (1)(3)

C=9+6+3

C = 18

Case 4: Three Letters x One Letter

Example

S - SER

S is substituted in the second-letter and third-letter positions in code S:

SSS – SER

Variable Values

Xi = Hexagonal distance value between S and S is 3.

Hexagonal distance value between S and E is 2.

Hexagonal distance value between S and R is 0.


Is congruence dead? 143

Calculation

C = (3)(SS) + (2)(SE) + (1)(SR)

C = (3)(3) + (2)(2) + (1)(0)

C=9+4+0

C = 13

Case 5: Two Letters x One Letter

Example

CR – C

C is substituted for the third-letter position of code CR. C is substituted for the second-
letter and third-letter positions of code C:

CRC – CCC

Variable Values

Xi = Hexagonal distance value between C and C is 3.

Hexagonal distance value between R and C is 2.

Calculation

C = (3)(CC) + (2)(RC) + (1)(CC)

C = (3)(3) + (2)(2) + (1)(3)

C=9+4+3

C = 16

Case 6: Three Letters x Two Letters

Example

IA – CAI

I is substituted for the third-letter position of code IA:

IAI – CAI
Is congruence dead? 144

Variable Values

Xi = Hexagonal distance value between I and C is 1.

Hexagonal distance value between A and A is 3.

Hexagonal distance value between I and I is 3.

Calculation

C = (3)(IC) + (2)(AA) + (1)(II)

C = (3)(1) + (2)(3) + (1)(3)

C=3+6+3

C = 12
Is congruence dead? 145

Appendix H

Steps for Selection of Occupational Groups

Step 1

GOT codes were sorted according to their primary type. This step resulted in the
following breakdown: Realistic = 35, Investigative = 46, Artistic = 45, Social = 40, Enterprising
= 44, and Conventional = 34.

Step 2

GOT codes were arranged according to whether the female and male codes of the
occupation had the same primary type. This step resulted in the elimination of 20 codes and the
following breakdown according to the remaining codes: Realistic = 28, Investigative = 42,
Artistic = 43, Social = 38, Enterprising = 43, and Conventional = 30.

Step 3

The GOT codes were organized according to whether the female and male codes of the
occupation were identical. This step resulted in the elimination of 104 codes and the following
breakdown according to the remaining 120 identical GOT codes: Realistic = 10, Investigative =
16, Artistic = 30, Social = 18, Enterprising = 24, and Conventional = 22.

Step 4

Pairs of male and female GOT standard scores were compared in order to determine
which pairs of codes were most similar according to mean scores. Pairs of codes where there was
at least one instance of a more than 5 point difference (i.e., more than half of a standard
deviation) between types were identified and eliminated. The scales for Engineering Technician,
Urban and Regional Planner, Editor, ESL Instructor, Rehabilitation Counselor, Top Executive,
Retail Sales Representative, Financial Analyst, and Financial Manager were eliminated at this
point in the study as these are new occupational groups, and appropriate gender-normed GOT
mean scores are not provided in the updated (i.e., 2005) technical manual. This step resulted in
the elimination of 20 codes and the following breakdown according to the remaining 50 pairs of
male and female identical GOT codes: Realistic = 6, Investigative = 16, Artistic = 24, Social =
16, Enterprising = 20, and Conventional = 18.

Step 5

The selection of the final 12 occupational groups was based on further minimizing
differences between pairs of gender-normed mean GOT scores. Realistic considerations
regarding occupational group accessibility also were considered.
Is congruence dead? 146

Appendix I

Strong Interest Explorer Directions and Items

Directions
The next three pages list many different jobs, school subjects, and activities. If you like an item,
place an X in the box next to the item. If you do not like an item, leave the box empty. It does not
matter how many or how few items you like. Do not stop to total your score.

When considering a job, school subject, or activity, don’t worry about whether you would be
good at it or concern yourself about not being trained for it. This is not a test of your abilities.
And don’t think about how much money you would make or whether you could get ahead. There
are no right or wrong answers. Give the first answer that comes to mind and work quickly, but
consider each item.

Items

Accounting Biologist
Algebra Chemistry
Creating a budget Dentist
Geometry Determining the cause of a disease
Making statistical charts Examining blood samples under a microscope
Mathematician Medical doctor
Mathematics Paramedic
Statistics Performing scientific experiments
Using a calculator Watching an open heart operation
Using math to solve problems X-ray technician
Drama Author of novels
Actor / actress Editing a paper
Art English composition
Artist Journalism
Cartoonist Literature
Illustrator Making a speech
Musician Newspaper reporter
Performing in a musical Poet
Sculptor Writing
Singing in a choir Writing a one-act play
Bilingual teacher Counseling distressed individuals
Creating harmony among ethnic groups Day-care worker
Cultural relations Helping others overcome their difficulties
Ethnic studies Providing spiritual counseling
Helping immigrants adapt to a new culture School counselor
Improving racial understanding School nurse
Learning about cultural differences Serving as a foster parent
Learning about gender differences Social worker
Working for cultural diversity Special education teacher
Is congruence dead? 147

Working in international relations Volunteering for community service


Being a teaching assistant Debating a political opponent
College professor Discussing politics
Education Government
High school teacher Governor of a state
Instructing people in a new method Judge
Leading a discussion group Lawyer
Preparing lesson plans Political science
School principal Politician
Teaching Providing legal advice to a clients
Training new employees Persuading a jury
Advertising executive Managing business / office work
Business management Office manager
Displaying merchandise in a store Project manager
Earning an income based on commission Planning and organizing a detailed event
Hotel manager Scheduling tasks for a project
Marketing Tracking inventory
Sales Using an electronic organizer
Sales manager Using computer spreadsheets
Tracking a company’s profits and losses Word processing
Trading stocks Working with new office equipment
Computer programming Agriculture
Computer science Training animals
Constructing a website Working outdoors
Fixing computer hardware problems Environmental science
Installing computer software Fish and wildlife management
Managing a computer database Forester
Networking computer systems Landscape designer
Providing technical support Nature study
Setting up a new computer Planting natural grasses in a park reserve
Using computer-aided design software Raising flowers and vegetables
Architectural drafting Examining a crime scene
Building contractor Firefighter
Building projects Learning the proper use of firearms
Carpenter Military training
Civil Engineer Police officer
Designing bridges and roads Private investigator
Industrial arts Providing security for a concert
Mechanical drafting Rescuing climbers on a mountain
Reading a blueprint Secret service agent
Woodworking Security guard
Is congruence dead? 148

Appendix J

Strong Interest Explorer Items by RIASEC Type and Scale

REALISTIC INVESTIGATIVE
Outdoor, Environment, Plants, and Animals Working with Numbers
Agriculture Accounting
Training animals Algebra
Working outdoors Creating a budget
Environmental science Geometry
Fish and wildlife management Making statistical charts
Forester Mathematician
Landscape designer Mathematics
Nature study Statistics
Planting natural grasses in a park reserve Using a calculator
Raising flowers and vegetables Using math to solve problems

Construction and Engineering Health and Science


Architectural drafting Biologist
Building contractor Chemistry
Building projects Dentist
Carpenter Determining the cause of a disease
Civil Engineer Examining blood samples under a microscope
Designing bridges and roads Medical doctor
Industrial arts Paramedic
Mechanical drafting Performing scientific experiments
Reading a blueprint Watching an open heart operation
Woodworking X-ray technician

Protective Services
Examining a crime scene
Firefighter
Learning the proper use of firearms
Military training
Police officer
Private investigator
Providing security for a concert
Rescuing climbers on a mountain
Secret service agent
Security guard
Is congruence dead? 149

ARTISTIC SOCIAL
Music and Arts Cultural Relations
Drama Bilingual teacher
Actor / actress Creating harmony among ethnic groups
Art Cultural relations
Artist Ethnic studies
Cartoonist Helping immigrants adapt to a new culture
Illustrator Improving racial understanding
Musician Learning about cultural differences
Performing in a musical Learning about gender differences
Sculptor Working for cultural diversity
Singing in a choir Working in international relations

Writing and Mass Communications Helping Others


Author of novels Counseling distressed individuals
Editing a paper Day-care worker
English composition Helping others overcome their difficulties
Journalism Providing spiritual counseling
Literature School counselor
Making a speech School nurse
Newspaper reporter Serving as a foster parent
Poet Social worker
Writing Special education teacher
Writing a one-act play Volunteering for community service

Teaching and Training


Being a teaching assistant
College professor
Education
High school teacher
Instructing people in a new method
Leading a discussion group
Preparing lesson plans
School principal
Teaching
Training new employees
Is congruence dead? 150

ENTERPRISING CONVENTIONAL
Law and Politics Office and Project Management
Debating a political opponent Managing business / office work
Discussing politics Office manager
Government Project manager
Governor of a state Planning and organizing a detailed event
Judge Scheduling tasks for a project
Lawyer Tracking inventory
Political science Using an electronic organizer
Politician Using computer spreadsheets
Providing legal advice to a client Word processing
Persuading a jury Working with new office equipment

Business, Sales, and Marketing Working with Computers


Advertising executive Computer programming Computer Programming
Business management Computer science
Displaying merchandise in a store Constructing a website
Earning an income based on commission Fixing computer hardware problems
Hotel manager Installing computer software
Marketing Managing a computer database
Sales Networking computer systems
Sales manager Providing technical support
Tracking a company’s profits and losses Setting up a new computer
Trading stocks Using computer-aided design software
Is congruence dead? 151

Appendix K

Short-Form Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire Directions and Items

Excerpt from Directions

Read each statement carefully. Decide how satisfied you feel about the aspect of your job
described by the statement. Keeping the statement in mind:

- if you feel that your job gives you more than you expected, check the box under “Very Sat.”
(Very Satisfied);

- if you feel that your job gives you what you expected, check the box under “Sat.” (Satisfied);

- if you cannot make up your mind whether or not the job gives you what you expected, check
the box under “N” (Neither Satisfied nor Dissatisfied);

- if you feel that your job gives you less than you expected, check the box under “Dissat.”
Dissatisfied);

- if you feel that your job gives you much less than you expected, check the box under “Very
Dissat.” (Very Dissatisfied).

Items by Facet Satisfaction Scale and MSQ Long-Form Scale

On my present job, this is how I feel about. . . Very Very


Dissat. Dissat. N Sat. Sat.

I = Intrinsic Satisfaction Scale


E = Extrinsic Satisfaction Scale
G = General Satisfaction Scale

1. Being able to keep busy all the time


(I, G; Activity)

2. The chance to work alone on the job


(I, G; Independence)

3. The chance to do different things from time to time


(I, G; Variety)

4. The chance to be “somebody” in the community


(I, G; Social Status)

5. The way my boss handles his/her workers


(E, G; Supervision-human relations)
Is congruence dead? 152

6. The competence of my supervisor in making decisions


(E, G; Supervision-technical)

7. Being able to do things that don’t go against my conscience


(I, G; Moral values)

8. The way my job provides for steady employment


(I, G; Security)

9. The chance to do things for other people


(I, G; Social Service)

10. The chance to tell people what to do


(I, G; Authority)

11. The chance to do something that makes use of my abilities


(I, G; Ability utilization)

12. The way company policies are put into practice


(E, G; Company policies and practices)

13. My pay and the amount of work I do


(E, G; Compensation)

14. The chances for advancement on this job


(E, G; Advancement)

15. The freedom to use my own judgment


(I, G; Responsibility)

16. The chance to try my own methods of doing the job


(I, G; Creativity)

17. The working conditions


(G; Working conditions)

18. The way my co-workers get along with each other


(G; Co-workers)

19. The praise I get for doing a good job


(E; Recognition)

20. The feeling of accomplishment I get from the job


(I; Achievement)
Is congruence dead? 153

Short-Answer Items

Today’s Date ____________________

Check One: male female

When were you born? _____________

Circle the number of years of schooling you completed:


45678 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
Grade School High School College Graduate or
Professional School

What is your present job called? ______________________________________________

What do you do on your present job? __________________________________________

How long have you been on your present job? ____________ years ____________ months

What would you call your occupation, your usual line of work? _____________________

How long have you been in this line of work? _____________ years ____________ months
Is congruence dead? 154

Appendix L

Professional Organizations and Businesses Corresponding to the Occupational Groups

Table L1

Occupational Group Professional Organizations and Businesess


Accountant Directory of Certified Public Accountants
Kennett & Kennett P.C., Roanoke, VA
Attorney American Bar Association Lawyer Locator
Virginia Lawyers Directory
Cranwell, Moore & Emick, PLC, Roanoke, VA
Banker National Bankers Association
SunTrust Banks, Inc., Roanoke, VA
Bank of America, Roanoke, VA
First Market Bank, Roanoke, VA
Engineer National Society of Professional Engineers
NIOSH, Cincinnati, OH
Florist American Academy of Floriculture
Creative Occasions Florals & Fine Gifts, Roanoke, VA
Flowers & Things, Inc., Roanoke, VA
Kroger, Roanoke, VA
Forester USDA Forest Service

Librarian Public Library Directory

Licensed Practical Nurse National Federation of Licensed Practical Nurses


State of Georgia professional licenses
Carilion Family Medicine, Troutville, VA
Asthma & Allergy Center, Roanoke, VA
Medical Technologist American Medical Technologist state societies

Psychologist APA Help Center Psychologist Locator

Real Estate Agents National Association of Realtors®


Old Colony Company Realtors®, Morgantown, WV
Howard Hanna Premier Properties, Morgantown, WV
Social Worker NASW Register of Clinical Social Workers
Dr. Lewis Weber & Associates, Charlottesville, VA
Wellspring Family Services, Morgantown, WV
Is congruence dead? 155

Appendix M

Introductory Cover Letter Mailed to Potential Participants

[Address]

[Date]

Dear [Addressee]:

I am a doctoral candidate in the Department of Counseling, Rehabilitation Counseling, and Counseling


Psychology in the College of Human Resources and Education at West Virginia University in
Morgantown, WV. Currently I am completing my dissertation research under the supervision of my
faculty advisor, Roy H. Tunick, Ed.D., CRC.

I am writing to ask you to participate in my study. I am interested in finding out whether [occupational
group name] like you experience higher levels of job satisfaction when their interests match their job
duties.

In the envelope you received, you should find the following contents:

• Two copies of the form Consent for Participation in a Study of Work Interests and Job
Satisfaction.

• Two questionnaires: the Strong Interest Explorer, and the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire.

• A pre-paid, return envelope.

Please review the Consent for Participation in a Study of Work Interests and Job Satisfaction form. If you
decide that you would like to participate in this study, please initial and sign one copy of the form and
send it back to me in the return envelope. Keep the other copy and this letter for your records.

Complete the two questionnaires that are inside the envelope. You may take them in any order you wish.
Please DO NOT put your name on any part of the materials or the envelope. DO NOT score the
questionnaires.

Please put a signed copy of the Consent for Participation in a Study of Work Interests and Job
Satisfaction form and the two completed questionnaires back in the pre-paid envelope, seal it, and return
it to me in the mail.

Thank you for your considering to participate in my research.

Sincerely,

Shannon M. Bowles, M.Ed., NCC


Is congruence dead? 156

Appendix N
Is congruence dead? 157
Is congruence dead? 158
Is congruence dead? 159
Is congruence dead? 160

Appendix O

Reminder Letter Mailed to Participants

[Address]

[Date]

Dear [Addressee],

I have yet to receive your questionnaire materials regarding whether [occupational group name]
like you experience higher levels of job satisfaction when their interests march their job duties. If
you already have sent them, thank you, and please disregard this letter. If you have not yet sent
them back to me, please consider whether you would like to participate in my dissertation
research. Remember that your decision to participate is completely voluntary, and there is no
penalty for not participating.

Sincerely,

Shannon M. Bowles, M.Ed., NCC


Is congruence dead? 161

Appendix P

Rules for Resolving Tied SIE Scores

Tie for First-Letter and Second-Letter Positions

The first-letter and second-letter positions are ordered according to the type that is most
consistent with the type in the third-letter position.

Tie for Second-Letter and Third Letter Positions

The second-letter and third-letter positions are ordered according to the type that is most
consistent with the type in the first-letter position.

Tie for Third-Letter Position

The third-letter position is ordered according to the type that is most consistent with the
type in the second-letter position.

Ties of Equal Consistency

Ties of equal consistency are ordered according to their consecutive appearance in the
RIASEC order. While it is recognized that this method may seem arbitrary, some decision
guideline needs to be in place in the event that ties of equal consistency are incurred.

Ties among More Than Two Positions

Ties among more than two positions are arranged according to which order represents the
highest possible degree of consistency between types.

Ties of Equal Consistency among More Than Two Positions

Ties of equal consistency among more than two positions are ordered according to their
consecutive appearance in the RIASEC order.

Ties that Cannot Otherwise be Resolved

In the event that ties cannot be resolved with the above consistency and RIASEC order
guidelines, decide ties based on the ordering that is most consistent with the occupational group
GOT code.
Is congruence dead? 162

Appendix Q

Rules for Generating Decision-Derived Holland Codes

Decision Step 1

Identify the T-scores with a value of 60 or higher. Assign the summary code types
according to descending T-score values.

Decision Step 2

If no T-scores are at least 60, identify the T-scores with values between 55 and 59.
Assign the summary code types according to descending T-score values.

Decision Step 3

If no T-scores are at least 55, identify the T-scores with values between 50 and 54.
Assign the summary code types according to descending T-score values.

Decision Step 4

If more that three T-scores can be identified in the previous steps, assign the summary
code according to only the three highest values.

Decision Step 5

If no T-scores are at least 50, identify the T-score with the highest value. Assign the
summary code according to the type corresponding with the highest value.

Digitally signed by John H. Hagen


DN: cn=John H. Hagen, o=West Virginia
University Libraries, ou=Acquisitions Department,
email=John.Hagen@mail.wvu.edu, c=US
Reason: I am approving this document
Date: 2008.04.17 11:40:12 -04'00'

View publication stats

You might also like