You are on page 1of 4

T H E B E H A V I O R A N A L Y S T T O D A Y V O L U M E 4 , I S S U E 1 , 2 0 0 3

THE MOLAR VIEW OF BEHAVIOR AND ITS USEFULNESS IN BEHAVIOR


ANALYSIS
William M. Baum
University of California, Davis

The molar view of behavior contrasts with the older, molecular view. The difference is
paradigmatic, not theoretical. No experiment can decide between them, because they interpret all
the same phenomena, but in different terms. The molecular view relies on the concepts of discrete,
momentary events and contiguity between them, whereas the molar view relies on the concepts of
temporally extended patterns of activity and correlations. When dealing with phenomena such as
avoidance, rule-governed behavior, and choice, the molar view has the advantage that it requires no
appeal to hypothetical constructs. The molecular view always appeals to hypothetical constructs to
provide immediate reinforcers and stimuli when none are apparent. As a result, the explanations
offered by the molar view are straightforward and concrete, whereas those offered by the molecular
view are awkward and implausible. The usefulness of the molar view for applied behavior analysis
lies in the flexibility and conceptual power it provides for talking about behavior and contingencies
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

over time.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The molar view of behavior is relatively momentary events and causes when none are
new. Although its origins may be traced back apparent, whereas the molar view relies on
earlier, its first partial articulation was by Baum extended activities and extended causation,
and Rachlin (1969), in a paper called “Choice as avoiding postulation of hypothetical constructs.
time allocation.” It was presented more fully in Replacing the concept of momentary
a paper by Baum (1973), “The correlation-based response with the concept of extended activity
law of effect.” Rachlin (1994) offered a book- requires one to become familiar with thinking in
length presentation, and Baum (2002) elaborated more continuous terms—that is, in terms of
on his 1973 paper in another paper, “From extended patterns that cannot be seen at a
molecular to molar: A paradigm shift in moment in time. A familiar example is the
behavior analysis” and some papers in-between concept of probability. An unbiased coin, when
(Baum, 1995a; 1997). flipped, comes up heads with a probability of
The molar view contrasts with an older view that .50. What does this mean? On any particular
behavior analysis inherited from nineteenth- flip, the coin comes up heads or tails; nothing
century psychology. I call this older view more can be observed. Only for a long series of
“molecular,” because it is based on the notion flips can one observe the probability of .50. If
explanations of behavior may be constructed by one says that on a particular flip the probability
thinking of small discrete units being joined is .50, all one means is that in a long series of
together into larger units, like the joining such flips about half would show heads. The
together of atoms into molecules in chemistry. same is true of response rate. At any particular
The difference between the molecular moment, an activity (lever pressing) is occurring
and molar views of behavior is paradigmatic, not or not. One can only observe the response rate
theoretical. No data, no experiment can decide over some substantial time period. A response
between the two views, because no matter what that occurs 60 times per minute cannot occur 60
behavioral phenomenon one chooses, a times per minute at a moment.
proponent of either view is able to construct an Although Skinner advocated the use of
account of it. The difference between the two response rate as a dependent variable, he was a
lies in the concepts each brings to bear in such molecularist. In his well-known paper on
an account. The molecular view relies on superstition, Skinner (1948) proposed a
momentary events and momentary causation, “snapshot” view of reinforcement, in which
which leads to postulating hypothetical delivery of a reinforcer strengthens whatever
behavior happens to be occurring at the moment.
Author’s Note:
The molecularity of his approach is perhaps
Correspondence concerning this article may be addressed to:
William M. Baum, 611 Mason, #504, San Francisco,
nowhere clearer than in a short piece he wrote
CA 94108 or 415-345-0050. Email address: called “Farewell, My Lovely!” in which he
wbaum@sbcglobal.net deplored the absence of cumulative records in
the pages of JEAB and extolled the virtues of

78
B A U M

being able to observe “molecular,” moment-to- The likeliest way to overcome problem drinking
moment changes in behavior (Skinner, 1976). A is with local reinforcers for abstinent behavior.
cumulative recorder, however, is an averaging Thus, the molar view, like the molecular view,
machine; it only produces smooth curves says that, in practice, the one who would shape
because the chart moves slowly and the pen behavior needs to be swift with the reinforcers.
moves in small steps. At any particular moment, The molecular view has one point in its
either a response is occurring or not. The local favor: It coincides with a prejudice toward
changes in response rate are changes from one immediate causes. The notion that the events
interval to another. If, however, one were to fit that affect behavior occur either immediately
a truly continuous curve to a cumulative record, before or immediately after a response lends
then one might think of momentary rate as the simplicity to analysis. One knows just where to
slope of the curve at a particular point. This, look for the antecedents and consequences that
however, requires abstracting the continuous control the response. That simplicity, however,
function. comes at a high price: the necessity of inventing
In the molecular view, each response is immediate antecedents and consequences when
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

taken as a concrete particular (i.e., the basic none are apparent. Perhaps the best example is
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

observation), and response rate is a “derived” explaining avoidance.


measure (i.e., an abstraction) summarizing To explain avoidance, in which success
behavior over a period of time. The molar view means that nothing happens following a
turns this distinction around, making the response, molecularists turn to two-factor
extended pattern the concrete particular and the theory. Since a reinforcer must follow the
momentary response the abstraction. A response avoidance response, even if none is apparent,
rate or activity exists as a pattern through time. one has to be invented. Suppose that the
Any attempt to infer activity at a moment stimulus preceding the response becomes a
depends on abstraction, as in the example of the Pavlovian conditional stimulus, eliciting “fear.”
cumulative record. In fact, no behavior can be Then, when the response turns off the stimulus,
observed at a moment, because even the the reduction in fear reinforces the response.
simplest unit of behavior—lever press, key peck, Avoidance responding occurs, however, even if
button push—takes up time and must unfold no stimulus precedes or is terminated by the
from beginning to end before it can be recorded activity (Herrnstein & Hineline, 1966;
with certainty (for further discussion, see Baum, Herrnstein, 1969). Having already invented the
1997; 2002). Because every activity takes up fear-reduction reinforcer, the molecularist now
time, the concept of behavior at a moment is an also invents the stimulus. Dinsmoor (2001), for
abstraction, an inference made after the fact. example, argued that response-produced stimuli,
Although it has little use for momentary paired with a lower frequency of electric shock
events, the molar view supports analysis in more than their absence, become safety signals. The
and less extended time frames (Baum, 1995; cost of maintaining the molecular view here is
1997; 2002). That patterns take up time in no that one must appeal to hypothetical reinforcers
way precludes them from being brief. A and stimuli when none are observable. The
pigeon’s key peck, for example, is an extended result is a theory that cannot be refuted.
pattern that takes a fraction of a second. The molar view of avoidance is
Analysis may be as local or as extended as suits arguably simpler, but requires one to think in
one’s purpose. When trying to change behavior, terms of temporally extended patterns.
one should make sure that reinforcers are closely Avoidance activity is acquired and maintained
coordinated with the activity one is trying to because when that activity is present the rate of
increase. The molecularist insists reinforcers noxious events is lower than when it is absent.
must immediately follow the responses they are People avoid sensitive topics in conversation to
to strengthen; the molarist says reinforcers lower the likelihood of embarrassment to
should coincide closely with the activity to be themselves and others. People buy insurance to
increased. Such local relations often have lower the likelihood of financial hardship.
powerful effects, sometimes to our grief, when Much apparently dysfunctional behavior may be
they override more extended relations (Rachlin, understood as avoidance. If working and failing
2000). Each additional drink might seem would be too hard an outcome, one may avoid it
harmless, but in the long run they add up to ruin. by being ill.

79
T H E B E H A V I O R A N A L Y S T T O D A Y V O L U M E 4 , I S S U E 1 , 2 0 0 3

Another example of paying a high price to view, such a pattern constitutes a concrete
retain a molecular view is in accounting for rule- particular. The molecular view, focusing on a
governed behavior. Rules present a problem for moment, immediately moves to hypothetical
the molecular view because they are invariably constructs. Each alternative has a certain
associated with behavior that has important strength, unobservable at the moment but
consequences in the long run (Baum, 1994; existing at the moment. The extended pattern of
1995). Since long-delayed effects must be allocation is thought to reveal the relative
ineffective to the molecularist, if rule-governed strengths of the alternatives. If a pigeon pecks
behavior is maintained, some immediate twice as often at the left key than the right key,
(effective) consequences must be found. Why the strength of left pecking is considered twice
would someone eat vegetables instead of candy that of right pecking. If a child spends twice as
when no one else is present to observe? Why much time disrupting classroom activities as the
would someone save a piece of trash until a trash child spends doing schoolwork, the strength of
can appears, when it might have been dropped disrupting is twice that of remaining on task. In
on the street with impunity? Mallott (2001), in a the molar view, no hypothetical strength enters
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

paper about moral and legal control, provides in, because these patterns of allocation are what
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the molecularist’s answer: thoughts and self- the science is about.


punishment. He argues, “For moral control to Even if the molar view seems to allow
work, society must have established a special, such phenomena as avoidance, rule-governed
learned aversive condition—the thought of the behavior, and choice to be understood more
wrath of one’s God or the thought of the wrath readily, the question arises as to whether the
of one’s parents. And those thoughts must be molar view has any implications for applied
aversive, even when no one is looking” (p. 4). behavior analysis. It makes for the same sort of
Again the molecular view leads directly into the rule of thumb as the molecular view when one is
realm of the hypothetical and unverifiable. trying to change behavior: reinforcement must
be frequent and quick. Beyond this, however, I
The molar view of rule-governed think the molar view might have some
behavior allows that any contingency, no matter advantages for applications. First, it offers
how extended, may control behavior, even flexibility in thinking about goals and
though more local contingencies may be more treatments. No need arises to define some
powerful than more extended ones (Baum, 1994; artificial discrete response for reinforcement.
1995; Rachlin, 1994; 1995; 2000). Rules exist, One needs only to make sure that reinforcers
however, because extended contingencies are accompany appropriate activity. For example,
weak. A rule is a discriminative stimulus in school settings applied behavior analysts
produced by one person that induces in another already often talk about time on task as a
person behavior that is reinforced socially in the reinforceable activity. The molar view allows
short run (and reinforced in some major way in this kind of flexible thinking about
the long run). The behavior may come under the reinforcement of activities to be extended
control of the long-term contingency—for indefinitely. Second, it frees one to think about
example, the relationship between diet and time spent instead of response rate. Without
health. Although people often say that then the artificial discrete responses, activities like
rule has been “internalized,” from the molar reading, playing, grooming, and the like can be
point of view, it actually is further externalized, measured by timing them. Time spent should be
because the control is exerted by a more no harder to measure than counting responses
extended contingency. In looking at rule- and often will be less ambiguous, because one
governed behavior this way, the molar view may be able start and stop timing more easily
introduces no hypothetical events and no new than decide whether exactly the right response
terms. occurred. Once applied behavior analysts grow
Perhaps the strongest area of application accustomed to the molar way of talking, they
of the molar view is to choice, the allocation of will find it more congenial for communicating
behavior among alternatives. At any moment, with one another about behavior and
behavior is assigned to only one alternative. contingencies, because it is more flexible and
Over time, however, one sees a pattern of more concrete.
allocation among alternatives. In the molar

80
B A U M

In conclusion, two points might be Baum, W. M., & Rachlin, H. C. (1969). Choice as time allocation.
made. First, although the molecular view was Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 861-
874.
useful early in the development of behavior
Dinsmoor, J. A. (2001). Stimuli inevitably generated by behavior
analysis, the science has outgrown it, and the
that avoids electric shock are inherently reinforcing. Journal
molar view supplies the conceptual power of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 311-333.
required for the new developments. Second, the Herrnstein, R. J. (1969). Method and theory in the study of
molar view may be recommended for the avoidance. Psychological Review, 76, 49-69.
flexibility and power that it allows both applied Herrnstein, R. J., & Hineline, P. N. (1966). Negative reinforcement
and basic researchers in talking about behavior as shock-frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental
and contingencies. Analysis of Behavior, 9, 421-430.
Mallott, R. W. (2001). Moral and legal control. Behavioral
References Development Bulletin, 1, 1-7.
Baum, W. M. (1973). The correlation-based law of effect. Journal Rachlin, H. (1994). Behavior and mind: The roots of modern
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 20, 137-153. psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Baum, W. M. (1994). Understanding behaviorism: Science, Rachlin, H. (1995). Self-control: Beyond commitment. Behavioral
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavior, and culture. New York: HarperCollins. and Brain Sciences, 18, 109-159.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Baum, W. M. (1995a). Introduction to molar behavior analysis. Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA:
Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 21, 7-25. Harvard University Press.
Baum, W. M. (1995b). Rules, culture, and fitness. The Behavior Skinner, B. F. (1948). "Superstition" in the pigeon. Journal of
Analyst, 18, 1-21. Experimental Psychology, 38, 168-172.
Baum, W. M. (1997). The trouble with time. In L. J. Hayes & P. Skinner, B. F. (1976). Farewell, my lovely! Journal of the
M. Ghezzi (Eds.), Investigations in behavioral epistemology Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 218.
(pp. 47-59). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Baum, W. M. (2002). From molecular to molar: A paradigm shift
in behavior analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 78, 95-116.

81

You might also like