You are on page 1of 11

J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660

DOI 10.1007/s10964-017-0677-5

EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

Continued Bullying Victimization in Adolescents: Maladaptive


Schemas as a Mediational Mechanism
Esther Calvete 1 Liria Fernández-González1 Joaquín M. González-Cabrera2
● ● ●

Manuel Gámez-Guadix3

Received: 13 April 2017 / Accepted: 17 April 2017 / Published online: 22 April 2017
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2017

Abstract Bullying victimization in adolescence is a sig- Keywords Bullying victimization Maladaptive schemas
● ●

nificant social problem that can become persistent over time Social anxiety Family emotional abuse

for some victims. However, there is an overall paucity of


research examining the factors that contribute to continued
bullying victimization. Schema therapy proposes a model Introduction
that can help us understand why bullying victimization can
be persistent for some victims. This study examines the role Bullying victimization is a severe problem in schools. A
of maladaptive schemas, the key concept in schema therapy, student is being bullied or victimized when he or she is
as a mechanism of continued bullying victimization. The exposed, repeatedly and over time, to negative actions on
hypothesis was that maladaptive schemas of rejection the part of one or more students (Olweus 2013). Devel-
mediate the predictive association between victimization in opmentally, bullying peaks during the middle school years
both the family and at school and future bullying victimi- and declines somewhat by the end of high school (Hymel
zation. Social anxiety was also considered, as previous and Swearer 2015; Kljakovic and Hunt 2016). As a con-
research suggests that it can increase the risk of victimiza- sequence of bullying victimization, victims can experience
tion. The participants were 1328 adolescents (45% female) adjustment difficulties, including depression and social
with a mean age of 15.05 years (SD = 1.37), who com- anxiety (e.g., Calvete et al. 2016b; Sigurdson et al. 2015;
pleted questionnaires at three time points with a 6-month Turner et al. 2013). Bullying victimization can become
interval between them. Time 2 maladaptive schemas of persistent over time for some adolescents. The results of a
rejection significantly mediated the predictive association recent meta-analysis indicated that chronic victimization
from Time 1 bullying victimization, family abuse and social ranges from 8 to 43%, depending on study characteristics,
anxiety to Time 3 bullying victimization. The findings and that it increases with age (Pouwels et al. 2016). The
pertaining to potentially malleable factors, such as mala- perpetuation of victimization seems to be similar in both
daptive schemas that maintain continued interpersonal vic- boys and girls (Benedini et al. 2016).
timization, have important implications for prevention and Chronic victimization can lead to more severe symptoms
treatment strategies with adolescents. of maladjustment, as compared to transient victimization
(Rueger et al. 2011). Therefore, it is necessary to identify
the factors that increase the risk of ongoing bullying victi-
mization for some adolescents. Although previous research
suggests that some characteristics of the victims, such as
* Esther Calvete social anxiety (Storch et al. 2005), can perpetuate the risk of
esther.calvete@deusto.es being a victim, there is an overall paucity of research
1
examining the factors that contribute to continued bullying
University of Deusto, Bilbao, Bizkaia, Spain
victimization. Schema therapy (Young et al. 2003) proposes
2
International University of la Rioja, Logroño, La Rioja, Spain a model that can help us understand why bullying victi-
3
Autonomous University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain mization can be persistent for some victims. In this study,
J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660 651

we examined the role of maladaptive schemas, the key first is schema surrender. When individuals surrender to a
concept in schema therapy, as a mechanism of continued schema, they accept that the content of the schema is true
bullying victimization. and act in ways that confirm the schema (Young et al.
2003). For example, an adolescent scoring high in the
Maladaptive Schemas and Continued Bullying schemas of rejection could more easily make hostile attri-
Victimization butions to the behavior of peers in ambiguous contexts and
react aggressively (Calvete and Orue 2012). This aggressive
Developmental models of psychopathology propose that reaction may, in turn, increase the likelihood of being
victimization experiences in childhood can threaten the rejected by peers. The second maladaptive coping style is
security of attachment relationships and lead to maladaptive schema avoidance, which involves cognitive, behavioral
schemas related to how victims feel about themselves and and emotional strategies to avoid the activation of the
how they perceive relationships with others (Hankin et al. schema. For example, an adolescent with the same schemas
2013). Maladaptive schemas are defined in schema therapy of rejection could avoid interacting with others in order to
as broad, dysfunctional and pervasive patterns, consisting of avoid being rejected. This isolation can lead other peers to
memories, emotions, cognitions and bodily sensations about interpret that he/she is unfriendly and arrogant, thus invo-
oneself and one’s relationships with others (Young et al. luntarily making the adolescent a target for abuse by others.
2003). Maladaptive schemas are relevant for developmental The last coping style is schema overcompensation, which
science because they are hypothesized to originate early in consists of behaviors and thoughts that seek to compensate
childhood and to be developed throughout one’s lifetime. for the limitations or weaknesses implicit in the schemas.
The construct of maladaptive schemas maintains the Following the same example, an adolescent endorsing the
cognitive processing component that is central to earlier schemas of rejection can abuse others to avoid being
definitions of cognitive schemas (Beck 1967), but it also rejected and abused by them (“get them before they get
includes several other components (i.e., feelings, bodily you”; Young et al. 2003). The adolescent’s own aggressive
sensations) and is more focused on thematic content behaviors toward peers would, in turn, increase the risk of
(Zeigler-Hill et al. 2011). The schema therapy model has becoming a victim of bullying. This is consistent with
proposed the existence of five maladaptive schema domains studies indicating that reactive aggression can increase the
or categories, according to the unmet emotional needs of the risk of victimization, particularly in bully-victims (Yang
child: rejection, impaired autonomy, other-directedness, and Salmivalli 2015). Thus, from the theory of schema
impaired limits and unrelenting standards. Among them, the therapy, it can be surmised that victimization in the school
maladaptive schemas of rejection are particularly relevant in and in the family can lead to the development of mala-
the context of victimization, as previous studies have found daptive schemas in the victims, and that these schemas in
that these schemas can be an outcome of victimization both turn increase the likelihood of future victimization.
in the family (Gay et al. 2013) and at school (Calvete 2014;
Calvete et al. 2015). Maladaptive schemas of rejection Maladaptive Schemas as a Mediational Mechanism
involve the expectation that one’s needs for acceptance and between Family Maltreatment and Bullying
respect will not be fulfilled. They include contents such as Victimization
defectiveness, which manifests as a feeling of unworthiness
and the belief that one is defective in some way, and mis- It has been suggested that victimization in one context (e.g.,
trust, which is expressed as the belief that others are abusers in the family) may increase the risk of victimization in other
who intentionally do harm. Girls usually score higher on contexts. For instance, a link has been found between
maladaptive schemas of rejection than boys (Calvete et al. maltreatment by parents during childhood and later peer
2013a). victimization (Benedini et al. 2016; Lereya et al. 2013). The
How can maladaptive schemas contribute to explaining identification of mechanisms that act as a bridge between
continued victimization? Schema therapy provides a theo- different forms of victimization is important because
retical model to explain why endorsing certain schemas can experiencing victimization across multiple contexts is rela-
contribute to generating new situations of victimization. As ted to severe negative outcomes (e.g., Turner et al. 2016).
indicated above, early negative experiences in the family However, little is known about the mechanisms explaining
and at school, such as victimization, are proposed to act as the link between victimization in the family and school
origins of maladaptive schemas. Once the schemas develop, settings. As maladaptive schemas have been found to
they tend to be perpetuated. Maladaptive schemas, although worsen as a consequence of victimization in both the family
very negative, manage to survive because of the human and at school (Calvete 2014; Gay et al. 2013), they may act
need for consistency. Specifically, schema therapy proposes not only as a mechanism for the continuity of bullying
three maladaptive coping styles to adapt to schemas. The victimization over time, but also as a mediating mechanism
652 J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660

that accounts for the predictive relationship between victi- defectiveness and emotional deprivation. Thus, we expected
mization in the family and bullying victimization. that maladaptive schemas of a different category (impaired
autonomy) would not mediate the predictive association
Social Anxiety, Bullying Victimization and Maladaptive between victimization in the past and future victimization.
Schemas We also included measures of social anxiety in the study,
as it has been found that social anxiety can be both a
Symptoms of social anxiety have been found to be asso- consequence of abuse in the family/at school (Bruce et al.
ciated with experiences of abuse in the school (Calvete 2012; Turner et al. 2013) and an antecedent of bullying
2014; Turner et al. 2013; Wichstrøm et al. 2013) and in the victimization (Wichstrøm et al. 2013), and that maladaptive
family setting (Bruce et al. 2012; Simon et al. 2009). In schemas are present in adolescents who are socially anxious
addition, social anxiety symptoms may contribute to mak- (Calvete et al. 2013b). Thus, we hypothesized that social
ing some adolescents more vulnerable to bullying victimi- anxiety symptoms could also act as a mediating mechanism
zation and, therefore, to continued victimization (Pabian between prior victimization in the family/at school and
and Vandebosch 2016; Storch et al. 2005). For instance, future bullying victimization, and that maladaptive schemas
symptoms of social anxiety could encourage bullies to would mediate the association between social anxiety and
choose them as victims, believing that they will have more future bullying victimization, thus contributing to prolonged
difficulty defending themselves. Thus, social anxiety could victimization over time.
not only be a consequence of victimization, it could also act Finally, gender differences were examined. Although
as a factor that increases the risk of future bullying victi- previous studies suggest that perpetuation of victimization
mization. Moreover, the role of social anxiety in bullying is similar in both boys and girls (Benedini et al. 2016) and
victimization could be stronger among girls because they that other cognitive styles lead to victimization in both boys
usually score higher than boys on social anxiety (Caballo and girls (Hamilton et al. 2013), the mediational paths could
et al. 2014). be different, as girls traditionally score higher than boys on
Importantly, several maladaptive schemas are present in maladaptive schemas of rejection (Calvete et al. 2013a, b)
adolescents with social anxiety symptoms (Calvete et al. and social anxiety (Caballo et al. 2014).
2013b). Previous research indicates that maladaptive sche- To evaluate the above hypotheses, we conducted a three-
mas mediate between experiences of bullying victimization wave longitudinal study with a large sample of adolescents
and the increase of social anxiety symptoms (Calvete 2014) who completed measures of bullying victimization, mala-
and that social anxiety accounts for the relationship between daptive schemas and social anxiety symptoms. In addition,
some maladaptive schemas and social functioning (Cohen family emotional abuse was assessed at the beginning of the
et al. 2015). Thus, both social anxiety and maladaptive study, and thus the study also contributes to expanding
schemas could be involved as mediational mechanisms of knowledge about the association between victimization in
continued bullying victimization. different contexts in adolescence, a developmental stage
when victimization experiences are associated with severe
negative outcomes (Turner et al. 2016).
The Current Study

Continued bullying victimization is associated with severe Method


negative consequences for victims (Rueger et al. 2011).
However, little research has been done on the factors that Participants
may contribute to chronic victimization. Although multiple
factors are involved in bullying in schools (e.g., attitudes The total sample consisted of 1460 adolescents who were
justifying aggression, exposure to violence, etc.), in this invited to participate in the study. They came from 51
study we focused on maladaptive schemas. Grounded on the classrooms at 8 educational centers in Bizkaia (Spain). The
assumptions of schema therapy (Young et al. 2003), we schools were randomly selected from the list of available
proposed that experiences of victimization in both the family schools in Bizkaia. Of the 1460 adolescents, 1311 adoles-
environment and at school can contribute to the development cents completed measures at T1 (participation rate:
of maladaptive schemas in adolescents, and that these sche- 89.79%), 1197 completed measures at T1 and T2 (partici-
mas, in turn, can lead to the increased likelihood of new pation rate: 81.99%), and 1001 completed measures during
experiences of victimization, thus contributing to the stability all three waves of the study (participation rate: 68.56%).
of victimization over time. We expected this hypothetical The attrition rate was the result of absences from school due
mediating mechanism to be specific to maladaptive schemas to illness and mistakes in the codes used to pair measures
of rejection, which include contents such as mistrust, over time. For this study, 1328 adolescents who completed
J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660 653

measures in at least two of the three waves of the study were schema categories, we only used 30 items to assess schemas
selected (597 girls, 729 boys, and 2 subjects who did not of rejection and schemas of impaired autonomy. The
indicate their gender). Participants were between 12 and 17 schemas of rejection included items with these contents:
years of age at the beginning of the study (M = 15.05; SD mistrust, emotional deprivation, defectiveness, and aban-
= 1.37). The socio-economic levels were determined donment. Mistrust describes the expectation that others will
according to the criteria recommended by the Spanish hurt, abuse or humiliate the individual (e.g., “I feel that
Society of Epidemiology (2000) for parental education and people will take advantage of me”). Emotional deprivation
income. The socio-economic levels showed the following involves the belief that others will not adequately meet
distribution: 6% low, 17.8% low-medium, 28.4% medium, one’s need for emotional support (e.g., “For much of my
24.1% high-medium, and 23.7% high. life, I haven’t felt that I am special to anyone”). Defective-
ness describes the feeling that one is defective, unwanted, or
Instruments invalid in significant aspects (e.g., “I feel that I’m not
lovable”). Abandonment refers to the perception that sig-
Bullying victimization nificant others will not continue to provide emotional sup-
port because they will abandon the individual in favor of
The 7-item victimization subscale of the Peer Relations someone better (e.g., “I need other people so much that I
Questionnaire for Children (PRQ; Rigby 1996) was used to worry about losing them”). The schemas of impaired
assess victimization by peers. One item described physical autonomy included contents related to vulnerability to
victimization (e.g., “They hit me or push me”), and the harm, which involves an exaggerated fear that a random
remaining items described emotional victimization (e.g., catastrophe could strike at any time and that one will be
“They pick on me at school”, “They exclude me from the unable to prevent it (e.g., “I worry about being attacked”),
group on purpose”). Participants were asked to indicate the and failure, which involves the belief that one has failed,
frequency with which this type of experience had occurred in will inevitably fail or is fundamentally inadequate as com-
the last 6 months on a 4-point response scale ranging from 1 pared to one’s peers in different areas of achievement (e.g.,
(never) to 4 (very often). The Spanish version was developed “I’m incompetent when it comes to achievement”). Partici-
by means of a translation and back-translation procedure, and pants rated each item on a 6-point scale ranging from 1
its structure was confirmed (Calvete 2014). Studies have (completely untrue of me) to 6 (describes me perfectly). The
shown this questionnaire to be an adequate measure of the Spanish version of the YSQ-3 has shown good psycho-
bullying experience, with good validity and reliability (e.g., metric properties, with confirmation of the factor structure
Bond et al. 2007). In the current study, the alpha coefficients and adequate alpha coefficients for the scales (Calvete et al.
were .84, .85, and .83 at T1, T2 and T3, respectively. 2013a). Alpha coefficients in this study were .89 and .91 for
the schemas of rejection, and .81 and .84 for the schemas of
Emotional abuse by parents impaired autonomy at T1 and T2, respectively.

A Spanish adaptation of the Psychological Abuse Scale of the


Conflict Tactics Scales-Parent-to-Child Version (CTS-PC; Social anxiety
Straus et al. 1998) was used to measure parent-to-child emo-
tional abuse. The scale is composed of 5 items that are asked The 18-item Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (SAS-A,
twice, once referring to the mother and once again referring to La Greca and López 1998) was used to assess symptoms of
the father (e.g., “At home, my mother/my father makes me feel social anxiety in terms of the fear of negative evaluation,
worthless”). In the Spanish version, participants were asked to social avoidance or distress specific to new situations and
indicate the frequency with which these experiences had general social avoidance. The items are described in the
occurred in the past on a 4-point response scale ranging from 1 form of self-statements (e.g., “I get nervous when I talk to
(never) to 4 (very often). The Spanish version was developed peers that I don’t know very well”) to which participants
by means of a translation and back-translation procedure and must respond according to a 5-point response scale ranging
has been shown to have adequate psychometric properties from 1 (not at all) to 5 (all the time). The SAS-A is an
(Calvete 2014). In this study, the alpha coefficient was .78. adaptation of the SAS-Revised (La Greca and Stone 1993),
and has demonstrated good reliability and validity (Inder-
Maladaptive schemas bitzen-Nolan and Walters 2000). The Spanish version of the
questionnaire has also shown good psychometric properties
The Young Schema Questionnaire-3 (YSQ-3; Young 2006) (Olivares et al. 2005). In the current study, the alpha coef-
was used to assess maladaptive schemas. Although the ficients were .89 and .92 for T1 and T2, respectively.
YSQ-3 consists of 90 items and assesses five maladaptive
654 J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660

Procedure Little (2013) were followed. First, the configural invariance,


which implies that the relations between each indicator and
The adolescents completed the questionnaires in their its construct have the same pattern of fixed and freed load-
classrooms at three different time points, which were spaced ings at each wave, was tested. Second, the configural model
6 months apart. They completed measures of bullying vic- was compared to a more restrictive model (a weak factorial
timization at all three times, measures of anxious automatic invariance model), which implies that factor loadings within
thoughts and maladaptive schemas at T1 and T2, and constructs across time points are equal. Third, the weak
measures of parental abuse at T1. In order to pair the factorial invariance model was compared to a more restrictive
questionnaires of T1, T2, and T3, a code known only by the model (a strong factorial invariance model), which implies
participant was used. Participation was voluntary, and the that each intercept is specified to be mathematically equal
adolescents were informed that their responses were con- over time. Next, we tested the hypothesized model, which
fidential and would only be read by the research team. A included autoregressive paths for bullying victimization from
trained psychologist guided the data collection in the T1 to T2 and from T2 to T3, and for maladaptive schemas
classrooms. The Ethics Committee of the University of and social anxiety from T1 to T2. In addition, the model
[masked name of the university] approved this study. included predictive paths from all variables measured at T1
Because there were no student names included on the sur- to all T2 and T3 variables, and from all T2 variables to T3
veys, the school staff chose to collect passive consent from bullying victimization. Age was included in the model to
parents. Thus, parents were informed and given the option control its associations with the rest of the variables.
of refusing to allow their child’s participation. No parent Lastly, we examined whether the mediational model was
refused to allow their child’s participation. invariant across male and female subsamples through a
multiple-group analysis. These analyses involved several
Data Analytic Plan steps. First, we estimated the model for boys and girls
separately. Second, we tested the configural invariance of
As participants in this study were nested in classrooms, we the model to demonstrate that the pattern of fixed and free
estimated the intra-class correlation coefficients (ICC) for all parameters was equivalent across subsamples. Third, we
the variables in the study. ICC values were small and ranged tested the weak factorial invariance, which involves speci-
from .028 to .046, which indicates that variability between fying factor loadings as equal in boys and girls. Finally, we
classrooms is small, and thus it is not necessary to include a tested the invariance of the paths linking the longitudinal
multilevel approach to the hypothesized model of the study. latent variables of the model.
To test the study hypotheses, Structural Equation Modeling The goodness of the model fit was evaluated using the
(SEM) was used, with LISREL 9.2, with Full Information comparative fit index (CFI), the non-normative fit index
Maximum Likelihood (FIML) to manage missing values. (NNFI) and the root mean square error of approximation
Three item-parcels were used as indicators of the latent (RMSEA). Generally, CFI and NNFI values of .90 or higher
variables of schemas and social anxiety. Two item-parcels reflect a good fit. RMSEA values lower than .06 indicate an
were used for bullying and family emotional abuse because excellent fit (Little 2013). We used ΔCFI to compare nested
these variables include few items. Items were assigned to models as change in χ2 is highly influenced by sample size.
parcels after conducting an exploratory factor analysis, with A value of ΔCFI smaller than or equal to .01 indicates that
all the items corresponding to a latent variable in such a way the null hypothesis of invariance should not be rejected
that factor loadings were balanced within parcels. To do (Cheung and Rensvold 2002).
this, we used a procedure described by Little et al. (2002).
For instance, for the schemas of rejection, using the load-
ings as a guide, we started by employing the three items
with the highest loadings to anchor the three parcels. The Results
three items with the next highest item-to-construct loadings
were added to the anchors in reverse order. The highest Descriptive Analyses
loaded item from among the anchor items was matched with
the lowest loaded item from among the second selections. If Around half of the adolescents had suffered at least one act
more items were available, the basic procedure was con- of bullying victimization in the last 6 months. Specifically,
tinued by placing lower loaded items with higher loaded the percentage of participants who had been victims of
parcels. The error terms of the same variable assessed on some type of bullying behavior was 57.7% at T1, 52.9% at
different occasions were treated as correlated. T2, and 48.1% at T3. The prevalence of bullying victimi-
We estimated the hypothesized model after examining the zation was slightly higher for boys than girls at all waves
invariance of the model over time. The steps proposed by (57.8 and 57.5% at T1; 55.0 and 50.3% at T2; and 48.9 and
J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660 655

47.4% at T3), although differences were only marginally autoregressive paths were statistically significant, pointing
significant at T2: χ2 (1, N = 1260) = 2.74, p = .098. to the stability of the measures over time. In addition, all T1
Table 1 displays the correlation coefficients among all variables (i.e., abuse in the family, bullying victimization,
the variables of the study, as well as the means and standard and social anxiety) predicted an increase in the schemas of
deviations for T1, T2, and T3. As can be seen, all the rejection at T2, and the schemas in turn predicted an
coefficients were statistically significant, and several of increase of bullying victimization at T3. In addition, the
them were particularly strong (r ≥ .6). The pattern of coef- schemas of rejection at T1 predicted an increase in bullying
ficients between T1 variables was very similar to the pattern victimization at T2 and both bullying victimization and
of coefficients between T2 variables. Adolescent girls schemas at T1 predicted an increase in social anxiety at T2
scored higher on social anxiety and maladaptive schemas at (Figure 1). The model fits the data well: FIML χ2(159, N =
both T1 and T2, whereas adolescent boys scored slightly 1328) = 440, p < .001, RMSEA = .043 (90% CI[.038,
higher on the frequency of bullying victimization at all three .048]), NNFI = .974, and CFI = .980. Next, the significance
waves (Table 2). There were no significant differences in of the mediational paths was examined via 5000 boot-
family abuse as a function of gender. strapping samples. The results indicated that the schemas of
rejection acted as a mediating variable between T1 family
Predictive Model abuse and T3 bullying victimization (0.231; 95% CI[0.067,
0.395]), between T1 bullying victimization and T3 bullying
Measurement invariance over time victimization (0.215; 95% CI[0.075, 0.355]), and between
T1 social anxiety and T3 bullying victimization (0.398;
In the first step, we found that the configural invariance 95% CI[0.114, 0.682]).
model fits the data well: FIML χ2(144, N = 1328) = 317, p
< .001, RMSEA = .035 (90% CI[.030, .041]), NNFI
= .982, and CFI = .988. In the second step, the invariance Alternative model
of factor loadings was tested. This constriction involved a
change in CFI of .002, which is therefore within the cut-off We tested an alternative model with the schemas of
proposed by Cheung and Rensvold (2002). Thus, the impaired autonomy. The findings indicated that these
invariance in the factor loadings over time was tenable. In schemas did not mediate the association between T1 and T3
the third step, we tested the strong factorial invariance of the bullying victimization. In the impaired autonomy model,
model. This constriction involved a change in CFI of .004, most of the paths were similar to those obtained for schemas
which suggested that the invariance in the intercepts over of rejection. However, the schemas of impaired autonomy
time was tenable. did not predict an increase in bullying victimization from
either T1 to T2 or from T2 to T3. Fit indexes were good:
Hypothesized mediational model FIML χ2(159, N = 1328) = 397, p < .001, RMSEA = .040
(90% CI[.035,.044]), NNFI = .975, and CFI = .981.
Building on the above model, we tested the hypothesized
structural model. The findings indicated that all

Table 1 Correlation coefficients between the study variables


1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Mean SD

1. T1 Family emotional abuse 1 0.48 0.49


2. T1 Bullying victimization .171** 1 1.81 2.49
3. T1 Schemas of rejection .310** .425** 1 2.32 0.82
4. T1 Social anxiety .157** .364** .671** 1 41.44 12.79
5. T1 Schemas of impaired autonomy .290** .310** .702** .474** 1 2.50 0.95
6. T2 Bullying victimization .154** .631** .353** .263** .239** 1 1.69 2.65
7. T2 Schemas of rejection .279** .389** .681** .550** .498** .446** 1 2.20 0.85
8. T2 Social anxiety .168** .331** .516** .597** .376** .380** .708** 1 40.50 13.25
9. T2 Schemas of impaired autonomy .282** .304** .540** .408** .659** .360** .755** .582** 1 2.38 0.99
10. T3 Bullying victimization .202** .530** .298** .189** .216** .594** .346** .307** .248** 1.49 2.40
T1 Time 1 (n = 1311), T2 Time 2 (n = 1197), T3 Time 3 (n = 1001)
**p < .001
656 J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660

Table 2 Sex differences in the


Girls Boys Differences
variables of the study
Mean SD Mean SD t p d

T1 Family emotional abuse 0.49 0.49 0.48 0.48 0.230 .818 0.02
T1 Bullying victimization 1.57 2.13 1.96 2.75 −2.291 .020 −0.16
T1 Schemas of rejection 2.41 0.80 2.22 0.83 3.478 .001 0.23
T1 Social anxiety 42.91 12.51 39.94 12.7 3.598 <.001 0.24
T1 Schemas of impaired autonomy 2.672 0.960 2.362 0.921 5.900 <.001 0.34
T2 Bullying victimization 1.39 2.15 1.88 2.94 −2.581 .010 −0.19
T2 Schemas of rejection 2.28 0.81 2.11 0.86 3.363 <.001 0.20
T2 Social anxiety 42.19 12.80 38.90 13.44 3.941 <.001 0.25
T2 Schemas of impaired autonomy 2.494 0.997 2.276 0.977 3.962 <.001 0.22
T3 Bullying victimization 1.30 1.99 1.58 2.58 −1.504 .133 −0.12
T1 Time 1 (n = 1311), T2 Time 2 (n = 1197), T3 Time 3 (n = 1001)

Fig. 1 Longitudinal paths for


continued bullying
victimization. Note: The values
represent standardized
coefficients. T1 Time 1,
T2 Time 2, T3 Time 3, *p < .05;
**p < .01; ***p < .001

Gender Differences for the Predictive Model was tenable, as CFI change was 0. Finally, we tested
whether longitudinal paths between latent variables were
We examined whether the hypothesized model was invar- equal across both subsamples. This imposition involved a
iant for boys and girls. The model was estimated separately ΔCFI of .002, which was acceptable according to the cri-
for boys (FIML χ2(159, N = 729) = 381, p < .001, RMSEA terion of Cheung and Rensvold (2002). Thus, although the
= .052 (90% CI[.046, .059]), NNFI = .963 and CFI = .972) path from family abuse to maladaptive schemas was sig-
and girls (FIML χ2(159, N = 597) = 266, p < .001, nificant only in girls, the general pattern of longitudinal
RMSEA = .039 (90% CI[.030, .047]), NNFI = .977 and paths was similar for both boys and girls.
CFI = .983). The path from family abuse to the schemas of
rejection was not significant in the sample of boys. We then
tested the configural invariance of the model to demonstrate Discussion
that the pattern of fixed and free parameters was equivalent
across subsamples. This model displayed good fit indexes: Bullying victimization can be a persistent problem over
FIML χ2(308, N = 1326) = 661, p < .001, RMSEA = .042 time for some adolescents with severe negative con-
(90% CI[.037, .046]), NNFI = .972 and CFI = .964. Next, sequences. The identification of the factors that contribute
we estimated a model in which factor loadings were con- to chronic victimization is of great relevance for interven-
stricted to be equal across both subsamples. This imposition tions. However, prior research to this end has been
J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660 657

relatively scarce. Although multiple factors may increase association of exposure to violence in the family with the
the risk of continued victimization (e.g., school context, perpetration of violence (e.g., Calvete et al. 2016a; Duke
perpetrator characteristics, exposure to environmental vio- et al. 2010). In contrast, longitudinal evidence for the per-
lence), this study focused on some characteristics of victims petuation of victimization from one context to another is
that can be modified. In particular, this study examined scarcer (see, for a recent review, Benedini et al. 2016). Our
whether maladaptive schemas act as a mediating mechan- study is one of the few providing support for the role of
ism between prior victimization at home and at school and victimization in the family as risk factor for future victi-
future bullying victimization. Given that previous research mization in school, indicating that this association is par-
indicates that social anxiety can be both an antecedent and a tially explained by the development of maladaptive
consequence of victimization (Wichstrøm et al. 2013) and schemas. The understanding of the mechanisms explaining
because maladaptive schemas are highly prevalent in ado- the link among different forms of victimization is of great
lescents with social anxiety (Calvete et al. 2013b), we also relevance, given that adolescence is a critical stage in which
examined maladaptive schemas as a mediating mechanism multiple victimizations can have deleterious consequences
between social anxiety and bullying victimization, as well (Turner et al. 2016). Moreover, these findings suggest that
as social anxiety symptoms as a potential mechanism of family interventions aimed at promoting appropriate par-
continued victimization. enting styles may help to prevent bullying victimization
In accordance with our hypothesis, the findings indicate during adolescence.
that maladaptive schemas play a significant role in con- Consistent with previous research (Calvete 2014; Turner
tinued bullying victimization over time, as they mediate the et al. 2013), bullying victimization predicted an increase in
predictive association from T1 to T3 bullying victimization. social anxiety from T1 to T2. However, in contrast with the
Moreover, this finding was specific to maladaptive schemas findings of other studies (Bruce et al. 2012; Simon et al.
of rejection, as the mediation was not replicated for the 2009), family abuse did not predict changes in social
schemas of impaired autonomy, which are less relevant for anxiety. In addition, contrary to our hypothesis, social
victimization. These findings are consistent with the results anxiety did not act as a mediating mechanism for the con-
of previous studies that indicate that bullying and cyber tinuity of bullying victimization over time, as the predictive
bullying deteriorate the victims’ cognitive styles (Calvete paths from social anxiety to bullying victimization between
et al. 2015; Gibb and Abela 2008; Padilla and Calvete T1 and T2 and between T2 and T3 were not statistically
2014). Moreover, the findings extend those of previous significant. Instead, in our study, T1 social anxiety indir-
studies by showing that maladaptive schemas can contribute ectly predicted the increase of bullying victimization at T3
to perpetuating victimization. Thus, adolescents who are through a worsening of maladaptive schemas of rejection.
rejected by peers and experience insults and humiliation can Thus, these maladaptive schemas are also one of the
develop cognitions and feelings that are characteristic of the mechanisms that explain why adolescents who are socially
maladaptive schemas of rejection (for example, feeling anxious are at higher risk of peer victimization.
defective and rejected and believing that others will inten- The findings of this study have developmental implica-
tionally harm them). These schemas, in turn, can influence tions. Maladaptive schemas can be the result of experiences
the victims’ behaviors. As reviewed in this manuscript, of victimization in childhood and adolescence that then
schema therapy proposes several maladaptive coping styles contribute to the exposure to new experiences of victimi-
(i.e., schema surrender, avoidance and overcompensation) zation. This process is important to understand the devel-
to adapt to schemas (Young et al. 2003). These maladaptive opment of psychopathology in adolescence. It is important
coping responses could have the undesired effect for victims to note that although effect sizes for the cross-lagged
of being chosen again as targets for new abusive acts. longitudinal associations were small, they are relevant
Future research should extend the current study by includ- because they represent the prediction of residual change in
ing measures of coping responses in order to elucidate the the outcomes beyond the variance explained by baseline
mechanisms through which the schemas contribute to levels of the variables.
maintain victimization. The findings indicate that age did not predict changes
Furthermore, consistent with our hypotheses, maladap- over time in any variable. However, a younger age was
tive schemas of rejection also mediated the predictive associated with less family emotional abuse at T1, but
association between emotional abuse in the family and later greater social anxiety and bullying victimization. Although
bullying victimization. Thus, maladaptive schemas seem to these associations were small, they are consistent with the
be one of the mechanisms involved in the transmission of results of previous research, which has evidenced a peak in
the risk of being a victim from the family context to the bullying behaviors during middle school (Hymel and
school context. This finding is relevant because previous Swearer 2015). Social anxiety has also been found to be
studies have obtained evidence mainly of the predictive greater during early adolescence (Parker et al. 2006). The
658 J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660

positive association between age and family abuse could be In addition, the findings have important implications for
explained by increased conflicts in child-to-parent rela- policy because maladaptive schemas can be modified
tionships during adolescence, as the adolescent strives for through interventions. The role that maladaptive schemas
greater autonomy. Overall, these findings suggest that pre- play in this process is relevant and opens up interesting
vention efforts should be implemented earlier in adoles- channels for prevention. Working on these schemas with
cence, when adolescents are more exposed to bullying and victims could help them reduce the risk of re-victimization.
its consequences, and before family victimization increases. Moreover, adolescents who develop maladaptive schemas
Gender differences in victimization were small for bul- may interpret ambiguous social situations as rejection,
lying victimization and null for emotional abuse in the threats or bullying (i.e., schema surrender). Interventions
family. Moreover, gender did not moderate the longitudinal should take into account this possibility. In addition, as the
associations between previous abuse and future victimiza- findings suggest that maladaptive schemas can contribute to
tion. This finding is consistent with the results of previous social anxiety symptoms and vice versa, intervention pro-
studies (Benedini et al. 2016; Kim et al. 2009; Tyler and grams should consider the interplay between these variables
Johnson 2006), which also found that the relationship to improve psychosocial adjustment during adolescence.
between abuse and revictimization was similar for boys and Moreover, because bullying victimization predicts an
girls. Although girls displayed higher scores on social increase in social anxiety, bullying victimization interven-
anxiety and disconnection and rejection schemas than boys, tions should help victims reduce anxiety.
the role of these variables in victimization was similar for Beyond interventions targeting specific symptomatology,
both boys and girls. prevention programs should favor the acceptance by the
The study has some limitations. The main limitation is group of each adolescent as she/he is (for example, more
that all measures were based exclusively on self-reports. introverted). This includes intervention in the social norms
Although self-reports are adequate to assess the variables in of the group to raise awareness and prevent aggression
this study, peer reports of victimization at school would against adolescents who are perceived to be weaker or just
have improved the validity of the measures. In addition, the different. Moreover, the results of this study should not be
measure of bullying victimization refers mainly to psy- interpreted as a causal attribution to the victims of the
chological abuse and only included only one item of phy- victimization they suffer. Bullying victimization should be
sical abuse. Although psychological abuse is the most considered the result of multiple factors (Cook et al. 2010).
prevalent form during adolescence, future studies should Bullies are responsible for the abuse, and the main pre-
examine whether the findings of the current study are dif- vention efforts should be directed at them. Addressing
ferent for physical and psychological abuse. Similarly, individual variables that increase perpetration, such as
future studies should assess whether these findings are psychopathic traits or moral disengagement (Orue and
generalizable to other types of family abuse, such as phy- Calvete 2016), could make a major contribution to the
sical abuse, since some studies indicate that the probability prevention of bullying. Our results are consistent with this
of being involved in bullying both as a victim and as a bully assumption.
is greater in adolescents who have experienced physical
abuse at home (e.g., Shields and Cicchetti 2001). Finally, Acknowledgements This research was supported by a grant from
we only measured family abuse at T1, and thus we could the Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (Spanish Government,
Ref. PSI2015-68426-R) and from the Basque Country (Ref. IT982-16
not examine whether maladaptive schemas, bullying victi-
and Ref. PI_2016_1_0023).
mization or social anxiety increased the risk of victimization
in the family. Author Contributions E.C. conceived of the study, participated in
its design, performed the analyses and prepared the initial draft of
introduction and discussion; L.F-G. completed part of the introduction
and discussion; J.G-C. participated in the review of the literature,
Conclusions prepared the tables of general statistics, and revised references; M.G-
G. completed part of the introduction, prepared the figure. All authors
The findings contribute to developmental science by eluci- read and approved the final manuscript.
dating some of the mechanisms involved in the perpetuation
of victimization in adolescence. As a result of victimization Compliance with Ethical Standards
in both the family and at school, adolescents can develop
maladaptive schemas that put them at a higher risk for new Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no compet-
ing interests.
episodes of victimization. This is important to understand
the development of psychopathology, as both maladaptive
Ethical Approval Participation was voluntary and participants were
schemas and victimization are important risk factors for informed that their responses were confidential and would only be read
psychological problems (Hankin et al. 2013). by the research team. The procedure always followed the standards of
J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660 659

the Declaration of Helsinki. The Ethics Committee of University of Cohen, J. N., Jensen, D., Dryman, M. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2015).
Deusto approved this study (no reference number provided). Enmeshment schema and quality of life deficits: The mediating
role of social anxiety. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29(1),
Informed Consent The school staff chose to collect passive consent 20–31. doi:10.1891/0889-8391.29.1.20.
from parents because there were no student names included on the Cook, C. R., Williams, K. R., Guerra, N. G., Kim, T. E., & Sadek, S.
surveys. Parents were informed and given the option of refusing to (2010). Predictors of bullying and victimization in childhood and
allow their child’s participation. No parent refused to allow their child’s adolescence: A meta-analytic investigation. School Psychology
participation. Quarterly, 25, 65–83.
Duke, N. N., Pettingell, S., McMorris, B. J., & Borowsky, I. W.
(2010). Adolescent violence perpetration: Associations with
References multiple types of adverse childhood experiences. Pediatrics, 125,
778–786. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0597.
Beck, A. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: Gay, L. E., Harding, H. G., Jackson, J. L., Burns, E. E., & Baker, B. D.
University of Pennsylvania Press. (2013). Attachment style and early maladaptive schemas as
Benedini, K. M., Fagan, A. A., & Gibson, C. L. (2016). The cycle of mediators of the relationship between childhood emotional abuse
victimization: The relationship between childhood maltreatment and intimate partner violence. Journal of Aggression, Maltreat-
and adolescent peer victimization. Child Abuse & Neglect, 59, ment & Trauma, 22(4), 408–424. doi:10.1080/10926771.2013.
111–121. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.08.003. 775982.
Bond, L., Wolfe, S., Tollit, M., Butler, H., & Patton, G. (2007). A Gibb, B. E., & Abela, J. R. Z. (2008). Emotional abuse, peer victi-
comparison of the Gatehouse bullying scale and the peer relations mization and the development of children´s negative inferential
questionnaire for students in secondary school. The Journal of styles and depressive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and
School Health, 77(2), 75–79. doi:10.1515/ijamh-2012-0106. Research, 32, 161–176. doi:10.1007/s10608-006-9106-x.
Bruce, L. C., Heimberg, R. G., Blanco, C., Schneier, F. R., & Lie- Hamilton, J. L., Stange, J. P., Shapero, B. G., Connolly, S. L.,
bowitz, M. R. (2012). Childhood maltreatment and social anxiety Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2013). Cognitive vulnerabilities
disorder: Implications for symptom severity and response to as predictors of stress generation in early adolescence: Pathway to
pharmacotherapy. Depression and Anxiety, 29(2), 131–138. depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41
doi:10.1002/da.20909. (7), 1027–1039. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9742-z.
Caballo, V. E., Salazar, I. C., Irurtia, M. J., Arias, B., Hofmann, S. G., Hankin, B. L., Snyder, H. R., & Gulley, L. D. (2013). Cognitive risks
Barinaga, E., & Vivas, E. (2014). Differences in social anxiety in developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.),
between men and women across 18 countries. Personality and Developmental psychopathology Wiley, Hoboken, NJ: John
Individual Differences, 64, 35–40. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.02. Wiley & Sons Inc.
013. Hymel, S., & Swearer, S. M. (2015). Four decades of research on
Calvete, E. (2014). Emotional abuse as a predictor of early maladap- school bullying: An introduction. American Psychologist, 70,
tive schemas in adolescents: Contributions to the development of 293–299. doi:10.1037/a0038928.
depressive and social anxiety symptoms. Child Abuse & Neglect, Inderbitzen-Nolan, H. M., & Walters, K. S. (2000). Social anxiety
38(4), 735–746. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.10.014. scale for adolescents: Normative data and further evidence of
Calvete, E., Fernández-González, L., Orue, I., & Little, T.D. (2016a). construct validity. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29,
Exposure to family violence and dating violence perpetration in 360–371. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP2903_7.
adolescents: Potential cognitive and emotional mechanisms. Kim, M. J., Tajima, E. A., Herrenkohl, T. I., & Huang, B. (2009).
Psychology of Violence, doi:10.1037/vio0000076. Early child maltreatment, runaway youths, and risk of delin-
Calvete, E., & Orue, I. (2012). Social information processing as a quency and victimization in adolescence: A mediational model.
mediator between cognitive schemas and aggressive behavior in Social Work Research, 33, 19–28. doi:10.1093/swr/33.1.19.
adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(1), Kljakovic, M., & Hunt, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of predictors of
105–117. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9546-y. bullying and victimisation in adolescence. Journal of Adoles-
Calvete, E., Orue, I., & Gamez-Guadix, M. (2016b). Do extraversion cence, 49, 134–145. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.03.002.
and neuroticism moderate the association between bullying vic- La Greca, A. M., & López, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adoles-
timization and internalizing symptoms? A three-wave long- cents: Linkage with peer relations and friendships. Journal of
itudinal study. Journal of School Psychology, 56, 1–11. doi:10. Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 83–94. doi:10.1023/A:
1016/j.jsp.2016.02.003. 1022684520514.
Calvete, E., Orue, I., & González-Diez, Z. (2013a). An examination of La Greca, A. M., & Stone, W. L. (1993). Social anxiety scale for
the structure and stability of early maladaptive schemas by means children-revised: Factor structure and concurrent validity. Journal
of the Young schema questionnaire-3. European Journal of of Clinical Child Psychology, 22(1), 17–27. doi:10.1207/
Psychological Assessment, 29, 283–290. doi:10.1027/1015- s15374424jccp2201_2.
5759/a000158. Lereya, S. T., Samara, M., & Wolke, D. (2013). Parenting behavior
Calvete, E., Orue, I., & Gámez-Guadix, M. (2015). Cyberbullying and the risk of becoming a victim and a bully/victim: A meta-
victimization and depression in adolescents: The mediating role analysis study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37, 1091–1108. doi:10.
of body image and cognitive schemas in a one-year prospective 1016/j.chiabu.2013.03.001.
study. European Journal on Criminal Policy and Research, 22, Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New
271–284. doi:10.1007/s10610-015-9292-8. York: Guilford Press.
Calvete, E., Orue, I., & Hankin, B. L. (2013b). Early maladaptive Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F.
schemas and social anxiety in adolescents: The mediating role of (2002). To parcel or not toparcel: Exploring the question,
anxious automatic thoughts. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27(3), weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151–173.
278–288. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.02.011. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1.
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit Olivares, J., Ruiz, J., Hidalgo, M. D., García-López, L. J., Rosa, A. I.,
indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation & Piqueras, J. A. (2005). Social anxiety scale for adolescents
Modeling, 9(2), 233–255. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5. (SAS-A): Psychometric properties in a Spanish-speaking
660 J Youth Adolescence (2018) 47:650–660

population. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psy- Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Moore, D. W., & Runyan,
chology, 5, 85–97. D. (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the parent-
Olweus, D. (2013). School bullying: Development and some important child conflict tactics scales: Development and psychometric data
challenges. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 751–780. for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse and
Orue, I. & Calvete, E. (2016). Psychopathic traits and moral disen- Neglect, 22, 249–270. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(97)00174-9.
gagement interact to predict bullying and cyberbullying among Turner, H. A., Shattuck, A., Finkelhor, D., & Hamby, S. (2016).
adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. doi:10.1177/ Polyvictimization and youth violence exposure across contexts.
0886260516660302 Journal of Adolescent Health, 58, 208–214. doi:10.1016/j.
Pabian, S., & Vandebosch, H. (2016). An investigation of short-term jadohealth.2015.09.021.
longitudinal associations between social anxiety and victimiza- Turner, M. G., Exum, M. L., Brame, R., & Holt, T. J. (2013). Bullying
tion and perpetration of traditional bullying and cyberbullying. victimization and adolescent mental health: General and typolo-
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45, 328–339. doi:10.1007/ gical effects across sex. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41(1),
s10964-015-0259-3. 53–59. doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2012.12.005.
Padilla, P., & Calvete, E. (2014). Cognitive vulnerabilities as media- Tyler, K. A., & Johnson, K. A. (2006). A longitudinal study of the
tors between emotional abuse and depressive symptoms. Journal effects of early abuse on later victimization among high-risk
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 743–753. doi:10.1007/ adolescents. Violence and Victims, 21, 287–306. doi:10.1891/
s10802-013-9828-7. vivi.21.3.287.
Parker, J. G., Rubin, K. H., Erath, S. A., Wojslawowicz, J. C., & Wichstrøm, L., Belsky, J., & Berg‐Nielsen, T. S. (2013). Preschool
Buskirk, A. A. (2006). Peer relationships, child development, and predictors of childhood anxiety disorders: A prospective com-
adjustment: A developmental psychopathology perspective. In D. munity study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54
Cicchetti, D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: (12), 1327–1336. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12116.
Theory and methods. 2nd edn. (Vol. 1, pp. 96–161). New York: Yang, A., & Salmivalli, C. (2015). Effectiveness of the KiVa anti-
Wiley. bullying programme on bully-victims, bullies and victims. Edu-
Pouwels, J. L., Souren, P. M., Lansu, T. A. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. cational Research, 57(1), 80–90. doi:10.1080/00131881.2014.
(2016). Stability of peer victimization: A meta-analysis of long- 983724.
itudinal research. Developmental Review, 40, 1–24. doi:10.1016/ Young, J. (2006). Young schema questionnaire-3. New York: Cog-
j.dr.2016.01.001. nitive Therapy Center.
Rigby, K. (1996). Manual for the peer relations questionnaire (The Young, J., Klosko, J. S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema therapy. A
PRQ). Underdale: University of South Australia. practitioner’s guide. New York: Guilford Press.
Rueger, S. Y., Malecki, C., & Demaray, M. K. (2011). Stability of peer Zeigler-Hill, V., Green, B. A., Arnau, R. C., Sisemore, T. B., & Myers,
victimization in early adolescence: Effects of timing and duration. E. M. (2011). Trouble ahead, trouble behind: Narcissism and
Journal of School Psychology, 49, 443–464. doi:10.1016/j.jsp. early maladaptive schemas. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
2011.04.005. Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 96–103. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Parental maltreatment and emotion 07.004.
dysregulation as risk factors for bullying and victimization in
middle childhood. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30,
349–363. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3003_7. Esther Calvete is a senior lecturer of Psychology at the University of
Sigurdson, J. F., Undheim, A. M., Wallander, J. L., Lydersen, S., & Deusto, Spain. Her major research interests include exposure to
Sund, A. M. (2015). The long-term effects of being bullied or a violence in the family and at school in adolescence.
bully in adolescence on externalizing and internalizing mental
health problems in adulthood. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
and Mental Health, 9, 1–13. doi:10.1186/s13034-015-0075-2. Liria Fernández-González is a postdoctoral researcher at the Faculty
Simon, N. M., Herlands, N. N., Marks, E. H., Mancini, C., Letamendi, of Psychology and Education of University of Deusto, Spain. Her
A., Li, Z., & Stein, M. B. (2009). Childhood maltreatment linked major research interests include dating violence and aggressive
to greater severity and poorer quality of life and function in social behavior in adolescents and youth.
anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 26(11), 1027–1032.
doi:10.1002/da.20604.
Spanish Society of Epidemiology and Family and Community Medi- Joaquín M. González-Cabrera is a lecturer of Psychology at the
cine. (2000). Una propuesta de medida de la clase social [A International University of La Rioja, Spain. His interest is on several
proposal to measure social class]. Atención Primaria, 25, forms of school violence, especially cyberbullying.
350–363.
Storch, E. A., Masia‐Warner, C., Crisp, H., & Klein, R. G. (2005).
Peer victimization and social anxiety in adolescence: A pro- Manuel Gámez-Guadix is a lecturer of Psychology at the
spective study. Aggressive Behavior, 31(5), 437–452. doi:10. Autonomous University of Madrid, Spain. His research interests
1002/ab.20093. include parenting and problem behaviors during adolescence.

You might also like