You are on page 1of 31

ARTICLE IN PRESS

G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

1 Review

2 Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans:


3 Focus on the Trier Social Stress Test
4 Q1 Andrew P. Allen a,b , Paul J. Kennedy a,b , John F. Cryan a,c , Timothy G. Dinan a,b ,
5 Gerard Clarke a,b,∗
a
6 Alimentary Pharmabiotic Centre, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
b
7 Department of Psychiatry, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
c
8 Department of Anatomy and Neuroscience, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
9

10 a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t
11
12 Article history: Validated biological and psychological markers of acute stress in humans are an important tool in trans-
13 Received 1 August 2013 lational research. The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), involving public interview and mental arithmetic
14 Received in revised form 1 November 2013 performance, is among the most popular methods of inducing acute stress in experimental settings,
15 Accepted 6 November 2013
and reliably increases hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activation. However, although much research
16 has focused on HPA axis activity, the TSST also affects the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system, the
17 Keywords:
immune system, cardiovascular outputs, gastric function and cognition. We critically assess the utility
18 Stress
of different biological and psychological markers, with guidance for future research, and discuss fac-
19 Cortisol
20 ACTH
tors which can moderate TSST effects. We outline the effects of the TSST in stress-related disorders, and
21 Serotonin if these responses can be abrogated by pharmacological and psychological treatments. Modified TSST
22 HPA axis protocols are discussed, and the TSST is compared to alternative methods of inducing acute stress. Our
23 Sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system analysis suggests that multiple readouts are necessary to derive maximum information; this strategy
24 Brain–gut axis will enhance our understanding of the psychobiology of stress and provide the means to assess novel
25 Heart rate therapeutic agents.
26 Immune system © 2013 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
27 Genetics

28 Contents

29 1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
30 1.1. General introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
31 1.2. The Trier Social Stress Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
32 2. Biomarkers and psychological effects of the TSST . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
33 2.1. Biomarkers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
34 2.1.1. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
35 2.1.2. Immune system effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
36 2.1.3. Sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
37 2.1.4. Cardiovascular effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
38 2.1.5. Brain–gut axis effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
39 2.1.6. Biomarkers: summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
40 2.2. Psychological effects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
41 2.2.1. Subjective effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
42 2.2.2. Cognitive effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
43 2.2.3. Neurobiological effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
44 2.2.4. Psychological effects: summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
45 3. Procedural considerations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
46 3.1. Biomarkers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
47 3.1.1. HPA axis measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

∗ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychiatry, 1.15 Biosciences Institute, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland. Tel.: +353 21 4901408.
E-mail address: G.Clarke@ucc.ie (G. Clarke).

0149-7634/$ – see front matter © 2013 Published by Elsevier Ltd.


http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

2 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

3.1.2. Immune system measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00


3.1.3. Sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
3.1.4. Cardiovascular measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
3.1.5. Brain–gut axis measures. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
3.1.6. Procedural considerations in biomarkers: summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
3.2. Psychological effects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
3.2.1. Subjective measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
3.2.2. Cognitive measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
3.2.3. Procedural considerations in psychological effects: summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4. Factors moderating the effects of the TSST . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.1. Gender and age . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.1.1. Age and gender differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.1.2. Sex hormones and menstrual cycle . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.2. Genetics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.3. Personality and individual differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.3.1. Personality factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.3.2. Stressor appraisal . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.4. Social factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.4.1. The interview panel . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.4.2. Social support and social status . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.4.3. Cultural factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
4.5. Factors moderating the effects of the TSST: summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5. TSST response and stress-related psychiatric disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.1. Depression . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.2. Anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.3. Other stress-related psychiatric disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.4. Factors moderating TSST response in stress-related disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.5. Treatment for stress-related psychiatric disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.5.1. Psychological treatment for stress-related psychiatric disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.5.2. Pharmacological treatment for stress-related psychiatric disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.5.3. Dietary interventions for stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
5.6. TSST and stress-related psychiatric disorders: summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
6. Modified versions of the TSST . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
6.1. Repeated measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
6.2. Control TSST . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
6.3. Group assessment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
6.4. Children and adolescents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
6.5. Virtual/imagined TSST committee . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
6.6. Hybrid versions of the TSST . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
6.7. Modified versions of the TSST: summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.1. Comparison with other methods of inducing stress and measuring HPA axis activity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.1.1. Pharmacological stimulation of the HPA axis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.1.2. The cold pressor task . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.1.3. The CO2 challenge test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.1.4. The cortisol awakening response . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.1.5. Cognitive tasks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.1.6. Noise . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.1.7. Comparison with other methods of inducing stress and measuring HPA axis activity: summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.2. Future research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
7.3. Summary and conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

48 1. Introduction et al., 2007; Starcke and Brand, 2012). Although acute stress is 60

important for responding to threatening situations, chronic stress is 61

49 1.1. General introduction damaging to health (McEwen, 2007). Over time, stressful life events 62

can induce chronic stimulation of the HPA axis, leading to depres- 63

50 Exposure to a situation perceived as threatening or excessively sion (Checkley, 1992; Juruena et al., 2011), and poorer prognosis 64

51 demanding (a stressor) leads to the release of chemicals which can for cancer and heart disease (Maddock and Pariante, 2001). Early- 65

52 help cope with the stressor (e.g. Joels and Baram, 2009; Schwabe life stress is associated with disorders such as depression (Batten 66

53 et al., 2012). Acute stress can alter numerous biological functions, et al., 2004; Dinan, 2005), post-traumatic stress disorder (Koenen 67

54 such as the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Foley and et al., 2007), and irritable bowel syndrome (Mayer et al., 2001), 68

55 Kirschbaum, 2010), the immune system (Steptoe et al., 2007), the and chronic work stress can exacerbate anxiety and depression 69

56 autonomic nervous system (Xhyheri et al., 2012), and the enteric (Smith, 2000). Moreover, chronic stress will result in changes in 70

57 nervous system (Ziegler, 2012). Psychologically, acute stress is a subsequent reactivity to an acute stressor (e.g. Chatkoff et al., 2010; 71

58 subjectively negative experience (but see Jamieson et al., 2013), and Low et al., 2009; Roth et al., 2012). Given the contrast between 72

59 can have positive as well as negative effects on cognition (Lupien acute and chronic stress, it is of interest to examine the effects 73

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 3

74 of acute stress in the context of disorders associated with chronic research contexts. Finally, the TSST is compared to different meth- 138

75 stress. ods of studying acute stress, and suggestions for future research are 139

76 Preclinical methods for inducing stress are well established offered (Section 7). 140

77 (Buynitsky and Mostofsky, 2009) and preclinical research has pro-


78 vided insight into the effects of stress, the possible role of factors 2. Biomarkers and psychological effects of the TSST 141

79 such as neurotransmitters (e.g. Browne et al., 2011; Cryan et al.,


80 2004; Julio-Pieper et al., 2012) and the interaction between the 2.1. Biomarkers 142

81 central and enteric nervous systems (Cryan and Dinan, 2012). There
82 is now growing recognition that preclinical efforts to gain greater 2.1.1. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis effects 143

83 insight into the effects of stress and stress mechanisms need to be In response to stress, the release of corticotropin-releasing fac- 144

84 bolstered by translationally relevant studies in human populations. tor from the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus leads to 145

85 Accordingly, a stressor which can reliably produce a psychophysi- the release of ACTH from the pituitary gland, which in turn leads 146

86 ological stress response in humans under controlled conditions is to the secretion of cortisol from the adrenal gland (see Fig. 2). 147

87 of immense value to this field. Glucocorticoids such as cortisol can affect the activity of miner- 148

alocorticoid receptors as well as glucocorticoid receptors, and the 149

88 1.2. The Trier Social Stress Test interaction between the two types of receptor may play a key 150

role in mediating glucocorticoid feedback inhibition (Pariante and 151

89 Social stressors are amongst the most reliable forms of stress Lightman, 2008). A significant increase in HPA axis activity follow- 152

90 in humans and other species (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004; ing exposure to the TSST is well established. 153

91 Tamashiro et al., 2005). The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) is an acute
92 stress protocol used to experimentally study the stress response 2.1.1.1. ACTH and cortisol. Numerous studies have indicated 154

93 in human subjects. It was designed by researchers at the Univer- increases in ACTH and cortisol (e.g. Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2007; 155

94 sity of Trier and first outlined in the early nineties (Kirschbaum Gerra et al., 2001; Kirschbaum et al., 1994; Munro et al., 2005), with 156

95 et al., 1992, 1993). The procedure runs as follows (Kudielka et al., studies observing an increase in salivary, plasma and serum corti- 157

96 2007): after a rest period on entering the laboratory, which helps sol (e.g. Gaab et al., 2002, 2005a; Jezova et al., 2004; Kirschbaum 158

97 to establish a clear baseline (Balodis et al., 2010), participants et al., 1993, 1999). Peaks in salivary cortisol levels commonly occur 159

98 are introduced to a role-playing scenario. They have to prepare a 10 min after cessation of stress exposure (e.g. Gaab et al., 2002; Het 160

99 speech to convince a panel that they are the perfect candidate for et al., 2009; Petrowski et al., 2010; Rimmele et al., 2007, 2009), 161

100 a job, which they must then present to a panel of assessors, fol- although peaks have also been observed 5 min (Childs et al., 2006), 162

101 lowed by a mental arithmetic task (serial subtraction). Following and 20 min (Rohleder et al., 2001) post-stress. Cortisol has also been 163

102 these tasks participants rest and post-stressor measures are taken; found to increase 14–20 min after the onset of stress anticipation 164

103 ideally, multiple time points should be used to chart changes in (Engert et al., 2013) and cortisol levels recovered more steeply in 165

104 biological and psychological effects (see Fig. 1 for a detailed out- participants who had experienced higher anticipatory stress (Juster 166

105 line of the typical procedure). Whereas public speaking and mental et al., 2012). Taken together, these findings suggest that multiple 167

106 arithmetic tasks in isolation induce small or inconsistent effects, time point sampling is necessary to fully capture the stress-induced 168

107 the TSST employs a combination of elements (public speaking, cortisol response. 169

108 mental arithmetic, anticipation, social evaluation) to produce mod- A minority of participants have a HPA axis response that does 170

109 erate stress in a majority of participants (Kirschbaum et al., 1993) not fall over repeated exposure (Kirschbaum et al., 1995b) and a 171

110 and reliable responses in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis minority may even experience sensitisation (Wüst et al., 2005). In 172

111 (HPA axis) in particular (Kudielka et al., 2007), with cortisol and the majority of participants who experience increased cortisol, this 173

112 adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) being most frequently stud- response habituates to repeated TSST administration over repeated 174

113 ied. Characteristics of the TSST which may explain its effectiveness sessions (Gerra et al., 2001; Mischler et al., 2005; Schommer et al., 175

114 come from a meta-analysis which has indicated that stressor tasks 2003; von Känel et al., 2006) (see Section 6.1 for further discussion 176

115 associated with social-evaluative threat and are uncontrollable (i.e. of repeated exposure to the TSST). 177

116 the participants’ behaviour does not have a clear effect on the out-
117 come of the task) are particularly effective in inducing a HPA axis 2.1.1.2. Vasopressin. Vasopressin is involved in the regulation of 178

118 response (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004). Het et al. (2009) have the HPA axis and may also be altered in stress-related disor- 179

119 highlighted that there are two types of research in which the test is ders such as depression (Scott and Dinan, 1998). For example, 180

120 used; research investigating the effects of stress on outcomes rel- the response of the pituitary gland’s vasopressin V3 receptors to 181

121 evant to the stress process (e.g. neuroendocrine or psychological arginine vasopressin stimulation is increased in depression (Dinan 182

122 variables), as well as research on how certain factors (e.g. gender and Scott, 2005). Exogenous vasopressin enhanced the cortisol 183

123 or psychiatric disorders) may moderate the stress response. response to the TSST, although vasopressin did not have this effect 184

124 Stress is associated with a number of physiological effects (see for a low-stress control or bike ergometry (Shalev et al., 2011). How- 185

125 Fig. 2). The following review discusses the effects of the TSST on key ever, the TSST did not affect plasma vasopressin in lactating women 186

126 biomarkers of stress (e.g. HPA axis activity, immune activation), as (Heinrichs et al., 2001). 187

127 well as its psychological effects (cognitive performance and subjec-


128 tive effects) (Section 2). Procedural considerations for biomarkers 2.1.1.3. DHEA and DHEA-S. The androgen precur- 188

129 and psychological effects are than discussed (Section 3), followed sors dehydroepiandrosterone/DHEA and its metabolite 189

130 by factors which can moderate TSST effects (Section 4). The effects dehydroepiandrosterone-sulphate/DHEA-S increase in response 190

131 of the TSST in participants with stress-related psychiatric disor- to the TSST in healthy participants, and this response is positively 191

132 ders (e.g. anxiety disorders, major depressive) are then discussed related to that of cortisol (Izawa et al., 2008; Lennartsson et al., 192

133 (Section 5). Modifications of the TSST are outlined, as well as how 2012b). Higher levels of work stress have been associated with a 193

134 these modified versions may impact on biological and psychologi- lower DHEA-S response to the TSST, although this effect was not 194

135 cal outcomes (Section 6). Minor modifications to the TSST have been evident for DHEA (Lennartsson et al., 2013). Participants with low 195

136 made to investigate specific research questions; this review focuses baseline DHEA-S have shown a stronger cognitive response to a 196

137 on modifications which may be useful for a number of different social rejection version of the TSST (Akinola and Mendes, 2008). 197

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

4 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Fig. 1. Typical Trier Social Stress Test procedure. (A). Key procedural stages: The baseline rest and stress procedures are carried out in separate rooms (depicted as Room A and
Room B). The baseline rest phase takes place in Room A. Following task instructions, participants then complete the stress procedure in Room B. The content of the speech
varies between laboratories but most commonly involves participants imagining they have applied for their ideal or dream job, and have to convince a committee why they
are the best candidates for the job. Importantly, for the stress procedure to be successful, committee members are pre-trained to be completely impartial and provide no
encouragement verbally or non-verbally. They are also typically introduced as experts in behavioural analysis and at identifying non-verbal signs of stress. Participants are
told to continue speaking if they stop early. If the participant stops for 20 s before the 5 min is up, they are told to continue, and if another pause occurs the panel ask a series of
prepared questions. Following the speech task participants are then given a surprise mental arithmetic task; they must serially subtract a two-digit number from a four-digit
number (e.g. 17 from 2023), speaking aloud. If they make an error they are required to start again. Following the stress procedure, participants return to Room A for the
recovery period. (B). Example sampling schedule: The baseline rest period varies depending on the type of samples which will be collected. If salivary cortisol is the key measure
then a 30 min period is required to allow hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity to normalise, as the process of attending the laboratory session itself will likely cause an
increase in HPA-axis activity in many participants. If blood is to be collected via an indwelling catheter, a baseline rest period of 45–60 min is recommended (Kudielka et al.,
2007). Samples/measures are collected immediately pre- and post-stress. Following the stress procedure, repeated samples are collected at 10–15 min intervals. The length
of the recovery period will depend upon the nature and number of measures being taken, although a period of 60 min is frequently used. Typical time points for sampling
are indicated by numbers in red. (C). Measures: A number of physiological and psychological parameters may be measured throughout the procedure via saliva (e.g. cortisol,
␣-amylase), blood (e.g. plasma/serum cortisol, cytokines, noradrenaline), psychophysiological tests (galvanic skin response, heart rate, blood pressure), cognitive measures
(e.g. attention, memory, executive function) and psychometric tests (e.g. stress, anxiety, perceived control).

198 Although DHEA and cortisol are both secreted by the adrenal influence cytokine production (Sapolsky et al., 2000). Indeed, the 218

199 cortex, cortisol is secreted from the zona fasciculata, while DHEA TSST response of the transcription factor nuclear factor kappa B, 219

200 is secreted from the zona reticularis (Nguyen and Conley, 2008). which induces inflammatory genes, has been associated with cor- 220

201 DHEA and cortisol responses can diverge – for example, DHEA tisol responses (Wolf et al., 2009). 221

202 does not show the post-awakening increase evident in cortisol Following the TSST, increases have been observed in lym- 222

203 (Hucklebridge et al., 2005). However, the findings from TSST phocytes, monocytes (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2002b, 2007), 223

204 research suggest that DHEA and cortisol responses do not diverge neurophil, basophil (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2002b) granulocytes, 224

205 in response to psychosocial stress (Lennartsson et al., 2012b). T cells, T helper cells and natural killer cells, but not B cells (Buske- 225

Kirschbaum et al., 2007) and higher monocytes have been observed 226

206 2.1.2. Immune system effects 1 h post-TSST (Rohleder et al., 2002). The effect on monocytes was 227

207 Cell types involved in the immune system include lymphocytes not replicated in a later study, although the TSST did increase leuko- 228

208 (the principal effector cells of the immune system; comprising T cytes (Gaab et al., 2005a). Changes in cortisol and ACTH in response 229

209 cells and B cells), cytokines, neurophils, basophils granulocytes to the TSST correlated positively with changes in granulocyte and 230

210 and T helper cells. These cells can be mobilised to fight infection natural killer cells and correlated negatively with changes in B cells 231

211 or modulate inflammation (e.g. Guo et al., 2002; Salazar-Mather (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2007). Although the TSST increased sali- 232

212 et al., 1996) but in addition to these functions the immune systems vary cortisol in healthy undergraduates, there was no effect on 233

213 are sensitive to stress (for reviews, see Kemeny and Schedlowski, C-reactive protein, and a non-significant increase in salivary lev- 234

214 2007; Pruett, 2003). Furthermore, HPA activity and the immune els of secretory immunoglobulin-A (Campisi et al., 2012), although 235

215 systems are linked; pro-inflammatory cytokines can activate the an increase in light chain immunoglobulin-A has been observed 236

216 HPA axis (Turnbull and Rivier, 1999) and alter glucocorticoid recep- elsewhere (Trueba et al., 2012). Levels of the pro-inflammatory 237

217 tor translocation (Miller et al., 1999), and glucocorticoids can cytokine interleukin-6 (IL-6) increase in response to the TSST in 238

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 5

Fig. 2. Psychophysiological reaction to stress. The hypothalamus and other regions of the central nervous system interact in response to stress and anxiety. Strong anticipatory
anxiety increases activation in the pregenual ACC, but moderate anxiety reduces activation (Straube et al., 2009). Recent life stress is associated with reduced mPFC size
(Ansell et al., 2012), and acute stress can alter reward processing in the OFC (Porcelli et al., 2012). The amygdala, which contains corticotropin-releasing factor receptors
(Jochman et al., 2005), is sensitised by stress (van Marle et al., 2009), and is involved in the formation of stress-related memories in the hippocampus (Kim et al., 2001). The
hippocampus itself is involved in inhibitory regulation of the HPA axis (Herman et al., 2005). Stress precipitates a chain of events in the HPA axis and immune systems: (A).
The hypothalamus activates the pituitary gland through the secretion of corticotropin-releasing factor. (B). The pituitary gland activates the adrenal gland through secretion
of ACTH. (C). The adrenal gland secretes cortisol. The negative feedback mechanism of cortisol (Gibbison et al., 2013; Posener et al., 1997) is reduced by chronic exposure to
stress, maintaining excessive secretion of cortisol. (D). Glucocorticoid receptors are activated by cortisol from the adrenal glands. The structures involved in the function of
neurotransmitters such as noradrenaline, dopamine and serotonin contain GC receptors (Czyrak and Chocyk, 2001; Laaris et al., 1995; Li et al., 2011). Excessive stimulation
of GC receptors impairs the function of these neurotransmitters. (E). Altered neurotransmitter function affects the CNS, visible in cognitive function (see Section 2.2.2 for
a discussion of cognitive effects of the TSST). (F). Heightened cortisol secretion leads to a disrupted response in the immune system (Corwin et al., 2013; Sapolsky et al.,
2000). (G). Immune responses lead to immunotransmitter activation in the CNS (Shanahan, 1999); pro-inflammatory cytokines can enter the CNS, where they interact with a
cytokine network, and in cases of chronic stress this can enhance the risk of neuropsychiatric disorder (Capuron and Miller, 2011). CNS, central nervous system; ACC, anterior
cingulate cortex; MPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; GC, glucocorticoid; 5-HT, serotonin; NA, noradrenaline; DA, dopamine

239 healthy adults (Slavich et al., 2010); salivary IL-6 can increase by with higher heart rate during the mental arithmetic task and lower 254

240 50% in response to the TSST and remain elevated 20 min post- cortisol response (Izawa et al., 2013b). 255

241 stressor (Izawa et al., 2013b). Lipopolysaccharide (LPS)-stimulated


242 IL-6 production has also been found to increase immediately post- 2.1.3. Sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system effects 256

243 TSST, returning to baseline levels 1 h later (Bellingrath et al., 2013). The sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system releases the cate- 257

244 Although the cortisol response habituated to repeated TSST admin- cholamines adrenaline and noradrenaline, activating a “fight or 258

245 istration over three weekly sessions in healthy men, IL-6 did not flight” response (Wetherell et al., 2006). Adrenaline increases fol- 259

246 (von Känel et al., 2006). IL-2 has been found to be higher post- lowing the TSST (Gold et al., 2004; Jezova et al., 2004). Furthermore, 260

247 TSST for teachers with poor coping strategies (Bellingrath et al., the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system shows less habituation 261

248 2010). Compared to a control condition (reading a newspaper), than the HPA axis to repeated exposure to the TSST (three sessions 262

249 plasma IL-1␤ has been raised by the TSST, although TNF-␣, IL-2, with four week intervals between them) (Schommer et al., 2003). 263

250 IL-12, interferon-␥ (IFN-␥) and IL-6 did not change significantly Adrenaline response did not change across sessions; noradrenaline 264

251 (Yamakawa et al., 2009). The interaction between the immune sys- showed less of an increase for the third session, although this may 265

252 tem, sympathetic nervous system and the HPA axis is evident in have been due to elevated levels at the beginning. Sympathetic- 266

253 findings that higher IL-6 elevation following the TSST is associated adrenal-medullary activity can also be tracked using the enzyme 267

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

6 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

268 ␣-amylase (for a review, see Nater and Rohleder, 2009) and gal- et al., 2012). Increases in leptin (a protein which regulates sati- 332

269 vanic skin response, a measure of activity of sweat glands which ety) post-TSST have been associated with reduced intake of foods 333

270 are innervated by the sympathetic nervous system (GSR; El-Sheikh high in sugar and fat for healthy women (Tomiyama et al., 2012), 334

271 et al., 2008; Graeff et al., 2003). The TSST induces increased salivary although the TSST did not induce a significant change in leptin. 335

272 ␣-amylase (sAA), with peaks observed immediately post-stress Stress response to the TSST can also affect feeding behaviour in 336

273 (Gordis et al., 2006; Het et al., 2009) and increased GSR (Jezova et al., those suffering from an eating disorder (see Section 5.3). Given 337

274 2004; Rohrmann et al., 1999). Although the TSST increases sAA as the reliable increase in HPA axis activity by the TSST, it is ideal for 338

275 well as plasma adrenaline and noradrenaline, these responses are investigating the impact of stress on brain–gut axis functioning, in 339

276 not correlated, suggesting that although sAA and catecholamines relation to both stress related functional gastrointestinal disorders 340

277 can both be used to assess sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system such as IBS, and with regard to modulating hunger and satiety. 341

278 activity, these measures are not redundant (Nater et al., 2006).
2.1.6. Biomarkers: summary 342

279 2.1.4. Cardiovascular effects The TSST reliably induces increased HPA axis activity (with 343

280 In contrast with the peak in cortisol which occurs post- extensive evidence for heightened ACTH and cortisol secretion 344

281 stress, the TSST raises heart rate throughout stress exposure, and post-stress, as well as recent evidence for an associated increase 345

282 heart rate returns to pre-stress levels within approximately 5 min in DHEA and DHEA-S) as well as sympathetic-adrenal-medullary 346

283 (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2002a; Childs et al., 2006). Peaks in activity (evident in enhanced adrenaline and sAA). There is also evi- 347

284 heart rate have been observed at the beginning of the stressor dence that the TSST can alter cardiovascular activity (e.g. increased 348

285 (Rimmele et al., 2007), as well as 1 min (Rimmele et al., 2009), heart rate and systolic blood pressure) and immune system activ- 349

286 15 min (Yamakawa et al., 2009) and 30 min (Jezova et al., 2004) ity (e.g. by increasing activation of the pro-inflammatory cytokine 350

287 into the stressor. Heart rate variability can also provide insight into IL-6). Effects of the TSST on the brain–gut axis are an interesting 351

288 the activity of the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous sys- avenue for TSST research, with emerging evidence for altered feed- 352

289 tems. Heart rate variability measures include power in the high ing behaviour, although there is a lack of research using the TSST 353

290 frequency range (HF; 0.15–0.4 Hz), which is indicative of parasym- to examine the effects of acute stress on factors such as abdominal 354

291 pathetic tone, power in the low frequency range (LF; 0.04–0.15 Hz) discomfort and urgency. 355

292 which has been suggested as a marker of sympathetic tone, and the
293 ratio of HF to LF, which is indicative of an interaction between sym- 2.2. Psychological effects 356

294 pathetic and vagal activity (for a review, see Xhyheri et al., 2012).
295 Although the TSST consistently increases heart rate, heart rate vari- 2.2.1. Subjective effects 357

296 ability (high frequency band variance) did not differ between a Although exposure to stress can lead to different subjective 358

297 TSST and a control condition (reading a newspaper aloud) (Rohleder effects depending upon appraisal of the stressor (see Section 4.3), 359

298 et al., 2006), nor did the TSST affect cardiac vagal tone, as assessed stress is generally a subjectively negative experience. The TSST 360

299 via respiratory sinus arrhythmia (Altemus et al., 2001). In contrast, increases reported stress (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2002b; Sugaya 361

300 another study indicated that a number of heart variability measures et al., 2012), and anxiety (Jezova et al., 2004; Rimmele et al., 362

301 changed significantly between baseline and post-TSST, including 2007, 2009; Rohrmann et al., 1999; von Dawans et al., 2011), and 363

302 HF, LF and HF/LF (Lackschewitz et al., 2008). The TSST has increased reduces reported calmness (Childs et al., 2006; Kirschbaum et al., 364

303 both systolic blood pressure (SBP; i.e. highest reading) and diastolic 1999). Administration of the TSST also leads to higher wakeful- 365

304 blood pressure (DBP; i.e. lowest reading) (Jezova et al., 2004). Other ness (Kirschbaum et al., 1999), although the TSST did not increase 366

305 studies have indicated that SBP, but not DBP, increased in response reported alertness elsewhere (Gray et al., 2012; Sketchley-Kaye 367

306 to the TSST (Campisi et al., 2012; Gerra et al., 2001). A measure of et al., 2011). Broader measures of mood have also been used; the 368

307 mean arterial blood pressure which combines SBP and DBP recov- TSST worsened negative mood as assessed by the positive and neg- 369

308 ered more steeply in participants experiencing higher anticipatory ative affect scale (PANAS) (Firk and Markus, 2009; Yim et al., 2010) 370

309 stress (Juster et al., 2012). as well as on the multidimensional mood questionnaire (Rimmele 371

et al., 2007, 2009) and on a visual analogue scale (Childs et al., 2006). 372

310 2.1.5. Brain–gut axis effects


311 In addition to modulating activity in the central and auto- 2.2.2. Cognitive effects 373

312 nomic nervous systems, stress and associated HPA axis activity can The endocrine, immune and central nervous systems are closely 374

313 affect the enteric nervous system; corticotropin-releasing factor connected (Leonard and Song, 1999); stress hormones can easily 375

314 signalling pathways in the gut alter gastrointestinal motility (Liu cross the blood–brain barrier, and can affect learning and memory 376

315 and Wood, 2012). The gastrointestinal tract may in turn modu- (Buchanan and Lovallo, 2001; Lupien et al., 2007; Takahashi et al., 377

316 late affect and even higher cognitive function (Mayer, 2011). Acute 2004), and TSST exposure can increase plasma lactate (Kubera et al., 378

317 psychological stress induced by dichotomous listening increases 2012), which can be used as an alternative to glucose as a source of 379

318 gut-specific autonomic innervation, assessed using rectal mucosal energy for the brain (it should be noted that sodium lactate can be 380

319 blood flow (Murray et al., 2004). Similarly, stress induced by men- used as a pharmacological means of panic induction, e.g. Plag et al., 381

320 tal arithmetic and a Stroop task increases rectal sensitivity in both 2012). The endocrine effects of the TSST can lead to changes in cog- 382

321 irritable bowel syndrome patients and healthy controls (Posserud nition; participants with a higher cortisol increase from the TSST 383

322 et al., 2004). It is thus plausible that the TSST can exacerbate symp- performed better at a declarative memory task requiring free recall 384

323 toms associated with irritable bowel syndrome, such as abdominal of words (Domes et al., 2002; Nater et al., 2007). In contrast to this 385

324 pain and urgency, although gastrointestinal effects have not rou- enhancing effect, other research has indicated no effect on declar- 386

325 tinely been assessed in research employing the TSST. ative memory (Hidalgo et al., 2012), impaired delayed memory of 387

326 The TSST can also affect eating behaviour; in children, a higher cards in children (Quesada et al., 2012), and impaired declarative 388

327 cortisol response to stress was associated with greater caloric memory for a word list in response to the TSST and oral adminis- 389

328 intake in the absence of hunger (Francis et al., 2013). In contrast, tration of hydrocortisone (Kirschbaum et al., 1996). The TSST led to 390

329 lower cortisol reactivity to the TSST has been associated with higher greater false recognition of semantically related distracter words 391

330 calorie consumption post-stress, with this effect more evident in on a recall task (Payne et al., 2002) although such an effect was 392

331 those with high scores on a scale of emotional eating (van Strien not found elsewhere (Beato et al., 2013). High cortisol responders 393

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 7

394 to the TSST have shown worse delayed recall for emotional and if the positive and negative feedback conditions are regarded as a 460

395 emotionally neutral pictures, whereas those who did not show a challenge and a threat respectively; this can allow for differentia- 461

396 cortisol response had improved memory for emotionally negative tion between “positive stress” and “negative stress”. 462

397 pictures (Buchanan and Tranel, 2008). TSST-induced stress led to


398 reduced memory for emotionally neutral words, but to unimpaired 2.2.3. Neurobiological effects 463

399 or even enhanced recall of emotional words (Jelici et al., 2004; Cortisol responders to the Montreal Imaging Stress Test (MIST; 464

400 Smeets et al., 2006), although the TSST reduced recall of positive Dedovic et al., 2005) which involves a mathematical challenge with 465

401 words in particular (Domes et al., 2004). Participants exposed to feedback, based on the Trier Mental Challenge Test (Pruessner et al., 466

402 the TSST have shown enhanced memory of stressor-relevant mate- 1999), have higher hippocampal deactivation during stress, as well 467

403 rial (Smeets et al., 2009; Wiemers et al., 2013a), and TSST stress led as more errors on a visual recognition task (Khalili-Mahani et al., 468

404 to better recognition of central over peripheral event information 2010). Furthermore, the cortisol responders showed hippocam- 469

405 (Echterhoff and Wolf, 2012). The evidence thus suggests that the pal deactivation prior to the onset of the stressor, suggesting that 470

406 cognitive effects of the TSST may be moderated by the emotional the anticipation of the stressor may have similar effects on this 471

407 valence and relevance of the stimuli used. brain area. In cortisol responders, the MIST has led to broader 472

408 The TSST has also impaired working memory for neutral stimuli deactivation in limbic system structures including the hypothala- 473

409 in high cortisol responders (using a digit span task, requiring par- mus and medio-orbitofrontal cortex as well as the hippocampus 474

410 ticipants to recall a series of digits either in the order presented (Lederbogen et al., 2011; Pruessner et al., 2008) and Pruessner 475

411 or in reverse order) (Elzinga and Roelofs, 2005), although only et al. suggested that differing brain activities may be related to 476

412 when adrenergic activity was not normalised post-stressor. Work- fear response, such as increased amygdala activity rather than 477

413 ing memory (assessed using an n-back task) was impaired by the decreased hippocampus activity; using a task like the TSST or MIST 478

414 TSST following administration of the mineralocorticoid antagonist in conjunction with fear induction may shed further light on this. 479

415 spironolactone, which increased cortisol availability (Cornelisse Adolescents with higher cortisol response to the TSST-C (see Sec- 480

416 et al., 2011a). The TSST led to slower reaction time and fewer cor- tion 6.3) have also shown reduced response in the left hippocampus 481

417 rect responses for n-back tasks completed 10 min after exposure to to fearful faces (Liu et al., 2012). An event-related MRI has also been 482

418 the stressor (Schoofs et al., 2008), and larger cortisol increases were developed to examine specific effects of social evaluative threat 483

419 associated with slower response times. The n-back tasks required separately from challenging arithmetic; cortisol responders to the 484

420 participants to attend to a series of single digits and state if the MIST showed deactivation in the limbic system to negative social 485

421 currently presented stimulus was the same as the stimulus pre- evaluation specifically, with increased bilateral activation in the 486

422 sented two or three trials earlier, requiring participants to hold prefrontal cortex for the maths challenge (Dedovic et al., 2009a). 487

423 and continuously update information in working memory. Schoofs Structural imaging is also of interest; greater hippocampal volume 488

424 et al. (2008) also found that the stress-induced impairment was has been found to be associated with a stronger cortisol increase in 489

425 attenuated towards the end of the task. Performance on a reading response to the TSST, as well as impaired declarative memory per- 490

426 span task was also impaired by the TSST (Luethi et al., 2008). In formance, but only pre-TSST (Pruessner et al., 2007). Future studies 491

427 contrast to this impairing effect on reading span, a positive correla- utilising brain imaging-based psychosocial stress tasks such as the 492

428 tion was evident between TSST-induced cortisol levels and both MIST will be of great importance in understanding the interactions 493

429 encoding and maintenance for a change-detection task (Stauble between psychosocial stress, cognition and emotion at the neuro- 494

430 et al., 2013). Stauble et al. argued that such a positive effect may be biological level (see King and Liberzon, 2009, for further discussion 495

431 obscured by tests which do not examine different stages of memory of neuroendocrine measurement in the context of imaging). Func- 496

432 processing. The TSST has been found to improve spatial memory tional connectivity analysis could also be of interest; processing of 497

433 for map routes (Luethi et al., 2008), but did not improve spatial emotional faces has been shown to be differentially modulated by 498

434 memory on a virtual water Morris maze (Klopp et al., 2012). The the TSST according to the visual hemifield in which face stimuli are 499

435 TSST slowed reaction time for a go/no-go task using neutral stimuli presented, indicating functional asymmetry and suggesting possi- 500

436 (Scholz et al., 2009); it would be of interest to see if such an effect ble interhemispheric transfer of information (Brüne et al., 2012), 501

437 may be moderated by emotional valence in verbal stimuli, similar although functional imaging was not used in this study. 502

438 to the effects shown in verbal recall. Indeed, the TSST may affect
439 processing of emotional stimuli at an early, automatic stage; corti- 2.2.4. Psychological effects: summary 503

440 sol response has been associated with attentional shifting on trials The TSST heightens self-reported stress and anxiety, as well 504

441 with masked angry faces (Ellenbogen et al., 2010). Another manner as negative mood broadly. Attention and memory responses to 505

442 in which cognition and emotion can interact in producing a given the TSST may depend upon the emotional valence of the stim- 506

443 response to the TSST is that rumination about the stressor may lead uli used. TSST exposure has been found to modulate numerous 507

444 to enhanced mood effects, which can manifest on a physiological forms of complex cognition. The TSST and adaptations for brain 508

445 level (Brosschot, 2010). imaging have implicated a number of brain regions involved in the 509

446 The TSST can also moderate more complex cognition, includ- acute stress response, particularly the hippocampus. Further inves- 510

447 ing task switching (Plessow et al., 2012a), dual task performance tigation into the specific roles of these regions should be a focus 511

448 (Plessow et al., 2012b), cognitive flexibility (Plessow et al., 2011), of future research. However, the complexity of the relationship 512

449 creativity (Akinola and Mendes, 2008), choice between present and between stress, associated biomarkers and cognition will neces- 513

450 future rewards (Kimura et al., 2013), answering questions requir- sitate extensive research on the subject. 514

451 ing estimation (Kassam et al., 2009), strategising (Leder et al., 2013)
452 and decision making involving risk (Pabst et al., 2013a,b; Van den 3. Procedural considerations 515

453 Bos et al., 2009), as well as a measure of risk-taking behaviour in


454 adolescents (Reynolds et al., 2013). Two of these studies (Akinola 3.1. Biomarkers 516

455 and Mendes, 2008; Kassam et al., 2009) used a modification of


456 the TSST whereby participants either received positive or nega- 3.1.1. HPA axis measures 517

457 tive feedback from the panel, rather than a cold, unencouraging TSST responses in ACTH, salivary and total plasma cortisol were 518

458 response. A measure of stress appraisal such as the transactional comparable for when testing occurred at different times of day 519

459 stress questionnaire (see Section 4.3.2) can be used to ascertain between 0900 and 1900 h, although basal levels were higher in the 520

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

8 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

521 morning (Kudielka et al., 2004c). However, the authors also point salivary glands may control the secretion of proteins differently 586

522 out that they could not control for chronotype, i.e. the waking hours (Bishop and Gleeson, 2009; Brandtzaeg, 2007). When comparing 587

523 preferred by an individual. If the TSST is conducted early in the day, studies by different authors, it should be noted that the assays used 588

524 waking times should be taken into account to control for the cortisol to assess cytokines have not been standardised across laboratories 589

525 awakening response (Pruessner et al., 1997b). The time window for (Kennedy et al., 2012). Furthermore, research assessing saliva and 590

526 conducting the TSST in the late afternoon is broader than during the plasma samples has indicated a lack of correspondence in results: 591

527 morning (Kudielka et al., 2007). Thus, although past research has although 16 out of 27 assessed cytokines were correlated for saliva 592

528 tested in the morning (e.g. von Känel et al., 2006) or both the morn- collected by passive drool and saliva collected by filter paper placed 593

529 ing and the evening (e.g. Het et al., 2009; Kirschbaum et al., 1993), a under the tongue, cytokines assessed in plasma from venipuncture 594

530 large number of studies have restricted testing to afternoon hours blood samples showed only 3 correlations with passive drool saliva 595

531 (e.g. Bellingrath et al., 2010; Kirschbaum et al., 1996, 1999; Pace (IL-6, IFN-␥ and macrophage inflammatory protein-1␣) and none 596

532 et al., 2006; Petrowski et al., 2012; Young et al., 2000). In general, with filter paper (Williamson et al., 2012). High-sensitivity assays of 597

533 whatever the method of stimulation it is easier to elevate cortisol C-reactive protein have also indicated a moderate-to-strong corre- 598

534 when it is relatively low in the afternoon than when it is high in the lation between salivary and serum measures (Ouellet-Morin et al., 599

535 morning. 2011). The association between salivary and blood measures may 600

536 Foley and Kirschbaum (2010) argue that the acute stress effects thus not be as reliable for many cytokines as it is for cortisol. IL-6 has 601

537 of the TSST have been best characterised using salivary cortisol. been activated by the TSST in a number of studies, although there is 602

538 Although plasma, serum and salivary cortisol can be increased currently no commercially available kit for assessing salivary IL-6, 603

539 by the TSST, it has been argued that the assessment of salivary so measures between laboratories will vary considerably, whereas 604

540 instead of plasma cortisol may account for the difference in findings commercial kits are available for salivary C-reactive protein. Pas- 605

541 between studies, such as conflicting evidence regarding moderat- sive drool is recommended as a method of collection, given that 606

542 ing effects of the menstrual cycle (Kelly et al., 2008; Kirschbaum cotton salivettes may interfere with assays for IL-6 (Minetto et al., 607

543 et al., 1999). However, there is a high correlation between salivary 2007). An alternative to saliva sampling is to measure cytokines 608

544 cortisol and unbound cortisol in plasma and serum, which persists via fluid derived from serum transport into the mouth via oral 609

545 in response to a number of challenges that alter HPA axis activ- mucosa, known as gingivial crevicular fluid or oral mucosal transu- 610

546 ity, such as ACTH stimulation and dexamethasone suppression test date (Nishanian et al., 1998); this method has indicated heightened 611

547 (DST; see Section 7.1) (Hellhammer et al., 2009). Salivary cortisol IL-6 responses to the TSST in men (Moons et al., 2010). However, 612

548 should thus be an adequate measure of bioavailable cortisol. oral mucosal transudate has its own shortcomings; IL-6 measured 613

549 It is also important to bear in mind that, due to factors such as in oral mucosal transudate has been found to correlate only mod- 614

550 cortisol pulsatility, which leads to variation in cortisol levels over estly with IL-6 levels in plasma (Fernandez-Botran et al., 2011). 615

551 shorter periods of time than the broader, diurnal pattern (Young With regard to time of day, IL-6 levels peaked at awakening and 616

552 et al., 2004a), single time point cortisol measurements can be highly declined over the morning, but then rose again at night, while CRP 617

553 variable. Furthermore, researchers cannot chart the progress of the levels were elevated at awakening but did not change substantially 618

554 cortisol response with only a single post-stress measure (Ramsay between 10.00 and 1.00 (Izawa et al., 2013a). 619

555 and Lewis, 2003). Consequently, every effort must be made to


556 ensure that samples are taken at multiple time points and with
3.1.3. Sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system measures 620
557 due regard for the factors which can adversely influence results (see
sAA is useful as a less invasive means of testing activity of the 621
558 Inder et al., 2012, for a review of practical issues related to collec-
sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system than adrenaline or nora- 622
559 tion and analysis of salivary cortisol). Nevertheless, salivary cortisol
drenaline (Rohleder et al., 2004); this is also the case for GSR, which 623
560 is a reliable index of HPA axis reactivity during the TSST once the
can be measured continuously throughout the TSST speech task. 624
561 appropriate controls are in place. Moreover, this measure has been
Research has indicated that sAA is not confounded with flow rate 625
562 shown to be responsive to anxiolytic agents (Fries et al., 2006), sug-
of saliva (Rohleder et al., 2006) and that SAA response to the TSST 626
563 gesting it is of use in assessing pharmaceutical interventions that
predicts plasma noradrenaline response (Thoma et al., 2012). How- 627
564 can modulate the acute stress response.
ever, whereas the HPA axis is particularly activated in response to 628

stressful situations associated with factors such as unpredictabil- 629


565 3.1.2. Immune system measures
ity and social-evaluative threat (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004), the 630
566 The lack of the use of control groups in studies on stress
sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system responds to pleasurable 631
567 and inflammatory factors such as cytokines has been criticised,
stimuli as well as those involved in stress (Clow, 2001). Conse- 632
568 particularly since not only the experimental stressor but also diur-
quently, care must be taken in interpreting results. It has been 633
569 nal variation and blood sampling processes may have effects on
shown that the TSST’s effect on a ratio of sAA to cortisol has a 634
570 cytokines (Izawa et al., 2013a; Steptoe et al., 2007). Given that a
stronger association with measures of chronic stress and depres- 635
571 control version of the TSST is available (see Section 6.2), this could
sion than for cortisol or sAA alone (Ali and Pruessner, 2012); this 636
572 be remedied in future research. The timing of post-stressor mea-
may offer a measure that tracks the effects of long-term stress more 637
573 surement is also relevant; as the effect of mental stress on cytokines
reliably. 638
574 may occur indirectly, through mechanisms such as lymphocytes or
575 endothelial cells, the effect may not actually be manifest until hours
576 after the occurrence of stress (Steptoe et al., 2001); the typical time 3.1.4. Cardiovascular measures 639

577 course of the TSST may thus not detect all effects. It is thus advisable Similar to GSR, heart rate and heart rate variability can be contin- 640

578 to extend the length of the post-stress sampling period when mea- uously measured throughout testing, including during the stressor. 641

579 suring these factors (Steptoe et al. measured cytokines at 45 min This confers these measures with an advantage over measures 642

580 and 2 h post-stress). which are less practical to take during the speech and arithmetic 643

581 Although saliva samples are non-invasive, blood measures of task (e.g. blood and saliva samples). However, care should be taken 644

582 cytokines are a more accepted method than salivary measures. in interpreting heart rate changes, as heart rate may be less specific 645

583 There are a number of other methodological issues to consider in to stressful stimuli than HPA axis activity (Gordis et al., 2006). To 646

584 using salivary measures of cytokines. The assessment of salivary this end, it is important to also assess subjective mood effects in 647

585 cytokines may require assessment of salivary flow rate, as different response to the TSST.

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 9

Table 1
Advantages of different biomarkers of TSST response.

Biomarker Non-invasive Can be measured Specifically Similar results Consistently


continuously during responsive to for blood/saliva responsive to
speech task stress repeated TSST
√ √
Plasma cortisol X X X
√ √ √
Salivary cortisol X X
Blood cytokines X X X IL-6, IFN-␥, CRP IL-6
ACTH X X X n/a X

Salivary cytokines X X IL-6, IFN-␥, CRP n/a
√ √ √
Heart rate X n/a
√ √
Galvanic skin response X n/a n/a

Adrenaline X X X n/a

Noradrenaline X X X n/a

sAA X X X n/a

648 3.1.5. Brain–gut axis measures findings that a response to the TSST characterised by anger, com- 696

649 The lack of assessment of gastrointestinal responses to the TSST pared to a response characterised by fear, can lead to a greater 697

650 is unfortunate, as questionnaires assessing symptoms of irritable increase in cortisol, but not proinflammatory cytokines (Moons 698

651 bowel syndrome have been developed which can be used to assess et al., 2010). In contrast, an anger response to a version of the MIST 699

652 gastrointestinal problems (see Bijkerk et al., 2003, for a discussion). (which was modified to induce anger) was associated with reduced 700

653 For example, visual analogue scales from the irritable bowel sever- cortisol response (Kazén et al., 2012). Biomarker responses to the 701

654 ity scoring system (IBS-SSS; Francis et al., 1997) can be used to TSST can thus be moderated by what type of mood the participant 702

655 quickly assess current levels of bloating, urgency and abdominal experiences during the stressor. In terms of what type of scale to 703

656 pain. use in measuring subjective responses to the TSST, although visual 704

analogue scales and Likert scales have comparable reliability (van 705

657 3.1.6. Procedural considerations in biomarkers: summary Laerhoven et al., 2004) and validity (Davey et al., 2007), visual ana- 706

658 In protocol design one needs to consider factors such as ease of logue scales may be more sensitive to subtle effects, given their 707

659 sampling (whether sampling is invasive and/or can be performed continuous nature. 708

660 without interrupting the psychosocial stressor), although other fac-


661 tors may also influence ease of sampling (e.g. with heart rate/GSR
662 participants may have to avoid moving their hands to avoid move- 3.2.2. Cognitive measures 709
663 ment artefacts). One also needs to consider medium of sample (e.g. The relationship between acute stress and cognition may not 710
664 blood or saliva), whether the biomarker is specifically responsive always be simply linear; both high and low levels of circulating glu- 711
665 to psychosocial stress, and level of habituation following repeated cocorticoids can impair memory performance compared to more 712
666 exposures. Given the extensive literature indicating that salivary moderate levels (Lupien et al., 2002). It may thus be of interest to 713
667 cortisol is increased by the TSST, combined with the advantages of compare cortisol non-responders to the TSST, those with a moder- 714
668 this biomarker outlined in Table 1, salivary cortisol will be the most ate response and those with a high response. It should be noted that 715
669 useful stress biomarker for much research employing the TSST. the linearity of the cognitive effects of arousal states such as stress 716

may depend on factors such as difficulty of the cognitive tasks (e.g. 717

670 3.2. Psychological effects Hanoch and Vitouch, 2004) or strategies employed during cognitive 718

tasks in response to increasing stress (e.g. Fisher, 1986; Matthews 719

671 3.2.1. Subjective measures et al., 2010), indeed, there is evidence that spatial learning strate- 720

672 Subjective and physiological measures are frequently included gies are altered in response to stress increases following the TSST 721

673 in TSST research; a re-analysis of previous studies indicated a pos- (Schwabe et al., 2007). In employing repeated measures of cognitive 722

674 itive relationship between sAA and negative affect, but a negative performance care should be taken to avoid practice effects. 723

675 correlation between cortisol and negative affect; the latter finding As well as the TSST affecting cognitive performance, cogni- 724

676 suggests cortisol may have a protective effect on subjective effects tive ability seems to be associated with the endocrine response 725

677 of stress (Het et al., 2012). However, another analysis of previous to the TSST: participants with higher executive functioning (as 726

678 research which assessed both psychological and physiological TSST measured with a letter fluency task) measured before the TSST 727

679 responses found that only 25% of the analysed studies indicated a have been shown to have a lower cortisol and reported anxiety 728

680 significant correlation between cortisol and perceived emotional response to the TSST (Hendrawan et al., 2012), although lower 729

681 stress (Campbell and Ehlert, 2012). This may be due to HPA axis performance on a nonverbal memory task predicted lower corti- 730

682 responses to the TSST lagging behind the subjective psychological sol reactivity to the TSST in adolescents (Slattery et al., 2013); this 731

683 response (Schlotz et al., 2008). An analysis of data from nine differ- difference in findings may be due to the longer time lag between 732

684 ent studies indicated that subjective mood rated during the TSST TSST stress and cognitive assessment in the latter study. The tim- 733

685 (between the public speech and the mental arithmetic task) is more ings of recall and stress exposure should also be considered; two 734

686 highly correlated with cortisol and heart rate responses, although studies indicated that immediate retrieval of emotional stimuli 735

687 the magnitude of the covariation was still small (Hellhammer and studied post-TSST reduced a positive effect of the TSST on long- 736

688 Schubert, 2012). It will thus be useful to collect mood data between term emotional memory consolidation (Wolf, 2012). Wolf has thus 737

689 the public speaking and mental arithmetic task where this is prac- suggested that although post-learning stress may be associated 738

690 tical. with enhanced consolidation, post-stress learning may interfere 739

691 It is worthwhile collecting subjective data in addition to phys- with consolidation of emotional memory. If using fMRI methods, 740

692 iological measures, as a given level of arousal may be interpreted it should be borne in mind that undergoing scanning can be stress- 741

693 differently by different participants (e.g. one participant may feel ful in itself; fMRI scanning has been shown to increase subjective 742

694 a positive excitement during the procedure whereas another may nervousness as well as sAA, with cortisol rising in a minority of 743

695 feel upset). The relevance of collecting mood data is underlined by participants (Muehlhan et al., 2011).

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

10 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

744 3.2.3. Procedural considerations in psychological effects: the TSST may be attenuated (Dedovic et al., 2009b). Potential gen- 806

745 summary erational differences will need to be monitored in future research 807

746 Subjective psychological data should be collected in addition using the TSST, and modifications may need to be made to account 808

747 to biomarkers to aid in interpretation of the physiological stress for such generational differences. A difference in perception of the 809

748 response, and ideally should be collected between the interview psychosocial stressor is likely also true of participants who are 810

749 and mental arithmetic task as well as pre- and post-stressor. Indi- generally less used to a university environment or interview per- 811

750 vidual differences in cognitive ability and practice effects should formance than students. For example, in a middle-aged sample, 812

751 be considered in investigating the interaction between acute stress lower educational attainment was associated with a higher cortisol 813

752 and cognition, as well as the possibility that effects may be non- response to the TSST (Fiocco et al., 2007). It may thus be desirable to 814

753 linear. have an acclimatisation period to the testing environment, before 815

exposing participants to the stressful speech task. 816

754 4. Factors moderating the effects of the TSST 4.1.2. Sex hormones and menstrual cycle 817

One explanation for gender moderating either cortisol effects 818

755 4.1. Gender and age or subjective effects but not both is that female sex steroids are 819

interacting with TSST effects (Kudielka et al., 2007). Women in 820

756 4.1.1. Age and gender differences the follicular stage and those taking oral contraceptives showed 821

757 Researchers have conducted TSST research on a broad age range a smaller response in salivary cortisol to the TSST than women in 822

758 of participants. Children (mean age = 12.1 years) and younger adults luteal phase or men (Kirschbaum et al., 1999), and women taking 823

759 (mean age = 23.5) show greater heart increases in response to the oral contraceptives compared to women in the luteal phase have 824

760 TSST than older adults (mean age = 67.3) (Kudielka et al., 2004b). indicated greater glucocorticoid sensitivity of pro-inflammatory 825

761 Younger men have shown a greater ACTH response than older men, cytokine production (Rohleder et al., 2003). Women but not men 826

762 and older men a higher salivary cortisol response than older women displayed a negative declarative memory effect of a TSST-induced 827

763 (Kudielka et al., 2004a). Men had a higher cortisol response to the rise in cortisol (Almela et al., 2011a), but a negative relationship 828

764 TSST than women in samples of young to middle-aged participants between peak cortisol and declarative memory has more specifi- 829

765 (e.g. Eisenberger et al., 2007; Uhart et al., 2006). Kelly et al. (2008) cally been observed in the luteal phase only in women (Espin et al., 830

766 found that women reported a greater increase in irritability and fear 2013). The effects of the TSST on sex hormones may be moderated 831

767 immediately following the TSST, although gender did not moder- by age: in men and women aged 30–50, levels of sex steroids (estra- 832

768 ate any changes in heart rate or plasma cortisol. There is evidence diol, testosterone, androstenedione, as well as sex hormone binding 833

769 that glucocorticoid sensitivity to IL-6 production may be moder- globulin) were increased by the TSST, and across sexes there were 834

770 ated by gender (Rohleder et al., 2001) although a gender effect was contrasting associations between changes in sex hormones and 835

771 not shown for IL-6 in oral mucosal transudate (Slavich et al., 2010). changes in HPA axis and sympathetic-adrenal-medullary axis activ- 836

772 Young men have increased glucocorticoid sensitivity to IL-6 pro- ity (e.g. changes in testosterone were associated with changes in 837

773 duction post-TSST than older men (Rohleder et al., 2002). Gender cortisol, ACTH and heart rate in women but with changes in heart 838

774 has not moderated sAA response to the TSST, although there is evi- rate only in men) (Lennartsson et al., 2012a). However, in a younger 839

775 dence for higher sAA in older participants (Almela et al., 2011b; mixed-sex sample (mean age in the mid-twenties), testosterone 840

776 Strahler et al., 2010). Medial prefrontal cortex damage has been and estradiol were not affected by the TSST (Schoofs and Wolf, 841

777 associated with heightened TSST cortisol response in women but an 2011). When employing the TSST in a sample of older women, 842

778 attenuated response in men (Buchanan et al., 2010). The TSST has estradiol can lead to higher increases in negative mood (Dumas 843

779 enhanced working memory and memory for emotionally arous- et al., 2012) as well as reduction in attention and psychomotor 844

780 ing over neutral pictures in males only (Cornelisse et al., 2011b). performance speed (Newhouse et al., 2010). 845

781 There is evidence that the TSST can enhance classical conditioning Given the broad moderating effect of gender, many studies eval- 846

782 for negative stimuli in males (Luethi et al., 2008); TSST exposure uating the effects of the TSST have recruited only male or female 847

783 has also enhanced fear conditioning in males, but inhibited such participants. In the case of the recruiting females only, the stage of 848

784 conditioning in females (Jackson et al., 2006). the menstrual cycle should be controlled which presents its own 849

785 Lupien et al. (2007) make the point that, for older participants, logistical challenges, especially where multiple assessments are 850

786 the testing environment itself may impose some of the character- required. 851

787 istics associated with Trier stress (e.g. novelty, unpredictability) to


788 a greater extent than younger populations. Indeed, younger people 4.2. Genetics 852

789 are more likely to be more familiar with being filmed (e.g. through
790 their use of social media); consequently the filming of the partici- The examination of genetic factors involved in acute stress has 853

791 pant may not induce as much stress in younger people. At the same been an exciting development in TSST research. FK506 binding 854

792 time, although they may be more familiar with interview perfor- protein 5 and G-protein coupled type-1 CRH receptor, which are 855

793 mance and with being filmed, young participants who are earlier encoded by the genes FKBP506 and CRHR1 respectively, have been 856

794 in their career paths may have a greater emotional investment in implicated in the HPA axis response (e.g. Lee et al., 2011; Tyrka 857

795 performing well in an interview scenario, as their future success et al., 2009). The CRHR1 polymorphisms rs7209436, rs110402, and 858

796 will hinge on their ability to perform well in interviews. The inter- rs242924 were associated with the cortisol response, while the 859

797 action between perceived performance and stress is thus likely FKBP506 polymorphism rs3800372 was associated with cortisol 860

798 to be stronger in such participants. Furthermore, younger partic- response in males only (Mahon et al., 2013). Gender interactions 861

799 ipants may find the mental arithmetic task more stressful than have also been shown for the Val66Met polymorphism of the BDNF 862

800 older participants, due to generational differences in reliance upon gene, with males val/val homozygotes showing a greater cortisol 863

801 technology for performing calculations. The point has also been response than val/met heterozygotes, with the opposite trend in 864

802 raised that gender socialisation may play a major role in gender females (Shalev et al., 2009). Polymorphisms of the glucocorticoid 865

803 differences in the psychosocial stress response; if the socialisation receptor gene (NR3C1) have been associated with HPA axis response 866

804 of children is becoming less pronounced in its gender differenti- to the TSST in males, with carriers of the A to G transition in the 867

805 ation over time, the moderating effect of gender on responses to 3 untranslated region at exon 9␤ showing a higher HPA response 868

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 11

869 (Kumsta et al., 2007) and BclI (a polymorphism located in intron B) not indicated elsewhere (Schommer et al., 1999). Moderation of 933

870 GG carriers showing an attenuated response (Kumsta et al., 2007; HPA axis effects by trait anxiety has also been equivocal (Jezova 934

871 Wüst et al., 2004). Participants homozygous for the mineralocor- et al., 2004; Takahashi et al., 2005a). Perfectionism has been asso- 935

872 ticoid receptor haplotype MR-2C/MRI180 showed higher cortisol, ciated with a greater increase in salivary cortisol (but not adrenaline 936

873 ACTH and heart rate responses to the TSST (van Leeuwen et al., or noradrenaline) in response to the TSST (Wirtz et al., 2007a). 937

874 2011). Although personality factors may offer limited explanatory power 938

875 A polymorphism of the 5-HT transporter gene predicted cor- for responses to a single TSST, associations between personality and 939

876 tisol responses to the TSST; those with the short/short genotype cortisol responses may be evident after repeated exposure to the 940

877 showed the strongest response to the stressor (Way and Taylor, TSST (Kudielka et al., 2009). 941

878 2010). Research using the mental arithmetic component of the


879 TSST in adolescent girls has shown a similar trend (Gotlib et al., 4.3.2. Stressor appraisal 942

880 2008), and children with both secure attachment and long/long As different people interpret psychosocial stressors in differ- 943

881 alleles showed lower galvanic skin response in response to the TSST ent ways, it is plausible that appraisal might account for why 944

882 (Gilissen et al., 2008). However, an effect of 5-HT transporter geno- some people are less stressed by the TSST. The transactional 945

883 type was not observed elsewhere for the TSST (Wüst et al., 2009) stress questionnaire, also known as primary appraisal secondary 946

884 or a modified social stress test (Bouma et al., 2010); this lack of an appraisal scale or PASA, assesses items based on the theoreti- 947

885 effect may be due to a moderating role of the dopamine D4 recep- cal constructs of primary appraisal (threat and challenge) and 948

886 tor gene (Armbruster et al., 2009; Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010). secondary appraisal (control expectancy and perception of one’s 949

887 Research with children has shown that carriers of the Met allele ability) (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). Perception of one’s ability is 950

888 of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met polymor- re-formulation of the concept of efficacy expectations, taken from 951

889 phism have a higher cortisol response (Armbruster et al., 2012), Bandura (1977). The PASA has been found to explain 22% of the 952

890 while heart rate response is moderated by the COMT polymor- variance in baseline-corrected cortisol following TSST (Gaab et al., 953

891 phism and sAA response is moderated by the 5-HT transporter gene 2005b). This suggests that these factors concerning the appraisal 954

892 (Mueller et al., 2012). In response to the TSST for groups (see Section of the situation may be more useful than measures of personal- 955

893 6.3), T-allele carriers on the neuropeptide S receptor gene poly- ity traits in predicting cortisol responses. Associations between the 956

894 morphism rs324981, which increases neuropeptide potency at the transactional stress questionnaire and cortisol response to the TSST 957

895 neuropeptide S receptor, showed greater increases in cortisol and have been shown elsewhere (Het et al., 2009) as well as associa- 958

896 subjective stress (Kumsta et al., 2013). tions between the transactional stress questionnaire and immune 959

897 Genetics can also moderate the cognitive effects of the TSST: biomarker responses to the TSST; higher control expectancy pre- 960

898 Met homozygotes on the COMT Val158Met polymorphism per- dicted lower tumour necrosis factor-␣ (TNF-␣) and IL-6 expression 961

899 formed worse on an n-back task (Buckert et al., 2012). The COMT in response to the TSST, while lower challenge predicted lower IL- 962

900 Val158Met polymorphism affects dopamine availability in the pre- 6 expression (Wirtz et al., 2007b). It is worth noting that the PASA 963

901 frontal cortex (Sesack et al., 1998; Weinshilboum et al., 1999); assesses appraisal of the stressor before it occurs, as opposed to 964

902 genetic factors may thus underscore the effects of serotonin, retrospectively reporting on the effects of a stressor. 965

903 dopamine and the interaction between the two on acute physi-
904 ological stress responses. Similar mechanisms also seem evident 4.4. Social factors 966

905 for noradrenaline: carriers of a functional deletion variant of the


906 ADRAA2B gene encoding the ␣2B-adrenergic receptor showed 4.4.1. The interview panel 967

907 impaired performance on a face recognition task; this was evident Panel behaviour may moderate the response to the TSST; criti- 968

908 for neutral faces under stress and for negatively emotional faces in cal versus supportive committees have shown differing effects on 969

909 the control condition (Li et al., 2013). cognitive performance (Akinola and Mendes, 2008; Kassam et al., 970

910 Given findings indicating gene–environment interactions in 2009), although a critical committee did not induce a larger cor- 971

911 response to the TSST (Gilissen et al., 2008), an increasing amount of tisol response than a supportive one (Taylor et al., 2010; Way 972

912 research is examining how epigenetic processes moderate the psy- and Taylor, 2010). However, performing the TSST speech task 973

913 chophysiology of the stress response (e.g. Lam et al., 2012; Mathews in the presence of an inattentive audience led to lower cortisol 974

914 et al., 2011) as well as stress-related disorders such as irritable and reported self-consciousness than presenting to an evaluative 975

915 bowel syndrome (Dinan et al., 2010). Epigenetic processes in acute audience (Dickerson et al., 2008). Compared to participants who 976

916 psychosocial stress have been shown using the TSST; DNA methy- performed in front of a panel, those who completed the TSST tasks 977

917 lation of the OXTR1 target sequence of the oxytocin receptor gene alone had lower heart rate during the tasks and lower salivary corti- 978

918 (located in the region of OXTR exon 3) increased between pre- and sol at post-stress and recovery (Gruenewald et al., 2004). A measure 979

919 post-stress, but decreased at a 90-min follow-up. However, the tar- of stress appraisal such as the transactional stress questionnaire 980

920 get region of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene (located (Gaab et al., 2005b) can be used to ascertain if the positive feed- 981

921 around the 3 end of brain-derived neurotrophic factor exon 6) back, negative feedback and no feedback conditions are regarded 982

922 did not change (Unternaehrer et al., 2012). Epigenetic factors may as a challenge and a threat respectively; this can allow for differen- 983

923 also be predictive of HPA axis response to the TSST: glucocorticoid tiation between “positive stress” and “negative stress”. The gender 984

924 receptor exon 1F methylation is predictive of total cortisol output of the panel is also relevant; young men and women only showed a 985

925 in females (Edelman et al., 2012). cortisol increase when the panel member were of the opposite sex, 986

and this effect was only observed in women in the follicular phase 987

926 4.3. Personality and individual differences of the menstrual cycle (Duchesne et al., 2012). 988

927 4.3.1. Personality factors 4.4.2. Social support and social status 989

928 Participants vary in their stress response to the TSST. However, The social nature of the TSST is further indicated by findings 990

929 “classic” measures of personality have shown equivocal results: of a moderating effect of social support (Foley and Kirschbaum, 991

930 lower extraversion in men and higher neuroticism in women, as 2010). Social support attenuates the cortisol response to the TSST 992

931 well as lower openness regardless of sex, led to a blunted cor- (Ditzen et al., 2008; Eisenberger et al., 2007), although the effects of 993

932 tisol response (Oswald et al., 2006), although such effects were social support from a stranger, boy/girlfriend and no social support 994

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

12 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

995 are moderated by the gender of the participant (Kirschbaum et al., with depression (Dockray et al., 2009). Consistent with this find- 1055

996 1995a). In children, social support by a friendly dog was found to ing, cortisol responses to the TSST have been found to be larger in 1056

997 be more effective in attenuating the cortisol response to the TSST- size and longer in duration for adolescents with major depressive 1057

998 C than a toy dog or support from a friendly human (Beetz et al., disorder compared to healthy controls (Rao et al., 2008). A pos- 1058

999 2012). Oxytocin has been found to enhance the attenuation of HPA sible explanatory factor is trait rumination, which was associated 1059

1000 axis response by social support, suggesting a possible mechanism with slower cortisol recovery in depressed adolescents (Stewart 1060

1001 for the positive effect of social support (Heinrichs et al., 2003). A et al., 2013). In young adult females, those with a current major 1061

1002 higher degree of dispositional social trust is also associated with an depressive episode showed slower recovery in both salivary cor- 1062

1003 attenuated cortisol increase in men (Takahashi et al., 2005b). tisol and negative affect than participants who were at risk for 1063

1004 Lower levels of subjective social status were associated with depression (Dienes et al., 2013). Older patients with depression 1064

1005 higher IL-6 responses at 45 min and 2 h post-baseline, and predicted (mean age = 62.3) have shown reduced cortisol response to the 1065

1006 perception of the TSST as more threatening and less manageable TSST (Taylor et al., 2006), and young adults with major depression 1066

1007 (Derry et al., 2013). Despite this perception of the TSST as more (mean age = 33) did not differ from age-matched controls in corti- 1067

1008 threatening for those of lower subjective social status, such partic- sol response, though they had lower ␤-endorphin response (Young 1068

1009 ipants have shown a lower cortisol response to the TSST in another et al., 2000). Pro-inflammatory cytokines are increased in chronic 1069

1010 study (Gruenewald et al., 2006). stress and depression (Leonard and Song, 2007) and inflammatory 1070

responses may have a role in the pathophysiology of depression 1071

1011 4.4.3. Cultural factors (Raison et al., 2006); in response to the TSST, IL-6 activation is 1072

1012 The TSST has been used to induce a stress response in various increased in participants with higher levels of depressive symp- 1073

1013 regions; for example, a HPA axis response has been induced by the toms (Fagundes et al., 2013) and in male patients with current 1074

1014 TSST in research conducted in Germany (e.g. Rimmele et al., 2007), major depression and early-life stress (Pace et al., 2006). Partici- 1075

1015 the USA (e.g. Childs et al., 2006), the UK (e.g. Gray et al., 2012), Japan pants who were remitted from depression for at least six months 1076

1016 (e.g. Yamakawa et al., 2009), and other countries. Nonetheless, it is did not show a difference in TSST response compared to healthy 1077

1017 possible that a public speech task may have differing psychological controls with no history of depression (Lange et al., 2013). 1078

1018 effects in different cultures. Unfortunately, there has been a rela- Research should report the nature of participants’ depressive 1079

1019 tive lack of cross-cultural research examining if culture moderates state; melancholic depression is associated with higher basal HPA 1080

1020 the effects of the TSST on psychological and biological markers of axis activity and less immune activation than depression without 1081

1021 the acute stress response. An exception is a study which found that melancholic features (Kaestner et al., 2005), and atypical depres- 1082

1022 stress has been attenuated more by implicit than explicit social sup- sion is associated with suppressed activity in the sympathetic 1083

1023 port in Asian Americans compared to European Americans (Taylor nervous system (Carney et al., 2005; Veith et al., 1994) and HPA 1084

1024 et al., 2007). axis (O’Keane et al., 2012). Furthermore, it has been suggested 1085

that the manifestation of HPA axis dysfunction in depression may 1086

1025 4.5. Factors moderating the effects of the TSST: summary alter as the symptoms associated with depression change over time 1087

(O’Keane et al., 2012). Participants at risk for suicide (those with a 1088

1026 Age and gender are perhaps the most widely investigated mod- first degree relative who completed suicide, but without current 1089

1027 erating factors of TSST response. Women have a lower cortisol psychopathology themselves) did not show improvement on mea- 1090

1028 response to the TSST, but this response can be altered by the men- sures of cognitive inhibition following the TSST (McGirr et al., 2010). 1091

1029 strual cycle, which should be controlled for in research employing Indeed, changes in the HPA axis and immune system induced by 1092

1030 the TSST. sAA response to the TSST is higher in older participants, chronic stress lead to damage in the prefrontal cortex, hippocam- 1093

1031 although heart rate response may be lower. The effects of cortisol pus and amygdala, critical brain regions associated with learning 1094

1032 on learning and memory may be more positive in males than in and memory, as well as anxiety and depression (Leonard and Myint, 1095

1033 females. The age of participants may alter their perception of the 2009), and hippocampal damage has been shown to abolish the HPA 1096

1034 stressor, and adaptations are necessary for child participants (see axis response to the TSST (Buchanan et al., 2009). Future research 1097

1035 Section 6.3). should further examine cognition following acute stress in partici- 1098

1036 Other factors which can moderate the stress response to the pants with current depression. 1099

1037 TSST include genetic factors underpinning glucocorticoid and min-


1038 eralocorticoid receptor function (see DeRijk and de Kloet, 2008, for 5.2. Anxiety 1100

1039 a review of corticoid receptor genetics and stress), as well as the


1040 genetics underlying monoaminergic function, primary appraisal In adolescents with generalised anxiety disorder, but not in 1101

1041 and secondary appraisal of the TSST (which is more predictive of healthy controls, noradrenaline and growth hormone increased 1102

1042 stress response than more general measures of personality traits), and prolactin and ␤-endorphin levels decreased following test- 1103

1043 social support, social status, interview panel members’ gender and ing (using a Stoop task and a mental arithmetic task as well as 1104

1044 behaviour towards the participant. Cultural factors may also mod- TSST) (Gerra et al., 2000). Children with social phobia have been 1105

1045 erate the stress response to the TSST, although further research on shown to have a greater subjective response than controls to the 1106

1046 the topic is required. TSST for children, and higher heart rate throughout the experimen- 1107

tal session (Krämer et al., 2012). Social phobia is also associated 1108

1047 5. TSST response and stress-related psychiatric disorders with slower HR recovery and reduced respiratory sinus arrhyth- 1109

mia reactivity (Schmitz et al., 2011), although social phobia did not 1110

1048 5.1. Depression increase cortisol or sAA responses (Krämer et al., 2012). The TSST 1111

increased cortisol and sAA in adults with social phobia (van Veen 1112

1049 A meta-analysis of the effects of various stressors in clinical et al., 2009), although this study lacked a healthy control group. 1113

1050 depression has indicated that baseline cortisol levels are similar Adults with social phobia are also more likely to experience mod- 1114

1051 to healthy controls, but cortisol levels are generally elevated dur- erate to severe depersonalisation (a sense that the self is not real) 1115

1052 ing recovery (Burke et al., 2005), and research in healthy children and derealisation (a sense that the world is unreal) than healthy 1116

1053 and adolescents has indicated that cortisol reactivity is positively controls in response to the TSST, and this was related to subsequent 1117

1054 associated with behaviours and emotional problems associated post-event processing and safety behaviours (Hoyer et al., 2013). 1118

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 13

1119 Patients with panic disorder have been shown to have an atten- participants with moderate to severe maltreatment in childhood 1181

1120 uated response to the TSST in cortisol but not heart rate (Petrowski experienced a greater IL-6 increase in response to the TSST than 1182

1121 et al., 2010, 2013). PTSD is associated with greater subjective controls (Carpenter et al., 2010). In contrast, maltreatment in child- 1183

1122 response to the TSST but in contrast to panic disorder it was not hood was associated with blunted ACTH and cortisol response to 1184

1123 associated with altered cortisol reactivity (Simeon et al., 2007a). the TSST in adult participants without current psychopathology 1185

1124 Comorbid lifetime major depression within the PTSD group was (Carpenter et al., 2007) and a blunted cortisol response in girls 1186

1125 not associated with greater TSST cortisol response. There is other aged 12–16 (MacMillan et al., 2009). Early life stress can also affect 1187

1126 evidence that comorbid anxiety and depression may have different cognition; both emotional abuse and physical neglect in childhood 1188

1127 effects than either disorder in isolation. Compared to participants were associated with impaired spatial working memory in healthy 1189

1128 with “pure” depression or anxiety disorders, those with comorbid adults (Majer et al., 2010). Severity of depression seems to play a 1190

1129 anxiety and depression had a heightened ACTH response to the role in the interaction between early life stress and depression; 1191

1130 TSST (Young et al., 2004b). A negative correlation between ACTH moderate/severe depression was associated with a blunted cor- 1192

1131 and noradrenergic activity has been observed in healthy controls tisol response to the TSST regardless of childhood maltreatment, 1193

1132 and participants with “pure” anxiety, whereas this relationship did whereas a history of maltreatment led to a more prolonged cor- 1194

1133 not hold for either those with “pure” depression or comorbid anx- tisol response in those with milder depression (Harkness et al., 1195

1134 iety and depression (Young et al., 2005). 2011). It has been suggested that a dampened HPA axis response 1196

could reflect resilience rather than risk for future psychopathol- 1197

1135 5.3. Other stress-related psychiatric disorders ogy (Carpenter et al., 2007). However, longitudinal research will 1198

be necessary to show if psychophysiological responses to acute 1199

1136 Stressful life events are associated with the onset and exacerba- psychosocial stress are predictive of resilience or susceptibility to 1200

1137 tion of chronic gastrointestinal disorders (Bradesi and Mayer, 2009; psychiatric disorders. Recent traumatic events can also affect TSST 1201

1138 Mayer, 2000) and participants with higher levels of stress were responses; cortisol reactivity to the TSST was lower in male par- 1202

1139 more likely to report functional gastrointestinal disorders (Suarez ticipants with exposure to violence as a victim or witness in the 1203

1140 et al., 2010). In line with findings that the TSST can alter gastroin- previous two years, but without a history of maltreatment (Peckins 1204

1141 testinal effects, adults with irritable bowel syndrome and without et al., 2012). 1205

1142 psychiatric comorbidity have shown an attenuated increase in Gender may moderate TSST effects within a depressed sample; 1206

1143 cortisol in response to the TSST compared to healthy controls females with chronic major depressive disorder exhibited a greater 1207

1144 (Suárez-Hitz et al., 2012). However, results from our laboratory cortisol response to the TSST than female controls, whereas in males 1208

1145 indicate that cortisol levels in irritable bowel syndrome remain depression was associated with reduced cortisol peak percentage 1209

1146 significantly elevated up to 1 h following exposure to the TSST in change (Chopra et al., 2009). In contrast, remitted depression (com- 1210

1147 comparison to healthy control participants (Kennedy et al., 2013). pared to no depressive history) attenuated the cortisol response in 1211

1148 Heart rate, cortisol and blood pressure responses to the TSST-C (see females, but did not affect cortisol response in males (Bagley et al., 1212

1149 Section 6.3) were higher for children with recurrent abdominal pain 2011). 1213

1150 than controls, although these effects were non-significant (Dorn


1151 et al., 2003).
1152 The gastric peptide and cortisol secretagogue ghrelin has an
1153 increased response to the TSST in participants with bulimia ner- 5.5. Treatment for stress-related psychiatric disorders 1214

1154 vosa (Monteleone et al., 2012), although stress-induced changes in


1155 ghrelin do not acutely moderate changes in desire to binge (Rouach 5.5.1. Psychological treatment for stress-related psychiatric 1215

1156 et al., 2007). HPA axis response to the TSST is correlated with disorders 1216

1157 increased desire to binge in participants with binge eating disorder, The TSST has been used to examine the effects of psychological 1217

1158 and participants with binge eating disorder have a blunted corti- interventions for stress-related disorders. A higher level of practice 1218

1159 sol response (Rosenberg et al., 2012). In obese participants, TSST of compassion meditation reduces IL-6 responses as well subjective 1219

1160 stress was associated with higher initial rate of eating (chocolate distress in the TSST, but not cortisol (Pace et al., 2009). Consis- 1220

1161 pudding) and reduced deceleration of eating towards the end of eat- tent with findings that trait mindfulness can attenuate cortisol and 1221

1162 ing, but only for those participants with binge eating disorder, even subjective anxiety response to the TSST (Brown et al., 2012), state 1222

1163 though those with binge eating disorder did not report greater sub- anxiety reaction to the TSST has been attenuated by mindfulness- 1223

1164 jective stress (Schulz and Laessle, 2012). Despite an altered ghrelin based stress reduction (MBSR) in participants with generalised 1224

1165 response, participants with bulimia nervosa did not have a higher anxiety disorder (Hoge et al., 2013), and by mindfulness-based cog- 1225

1166 cortisol response (Monteleone et al., 2012). nitive therapy in participants with partially remitted depression 1226

1167 Participants at high risk for psychosis showed an attenuated (Britton et al., 2012); this effect may be due in part to reduced 1227

1168 response to the TSST; this group also indicated higher levels of rumination (Van Vugt et al., 2012); trait rumination has also been 1228

1169 depression and chronic stress, which may explain the altered acute shown to predict the size and duration of the TSST cortisol response 1229

1170 stress response (Pruessner et al., 2013). (Zoccola et al., 2010). In a community sample, MBSR was compared 1230

to an active control group who received an intervention which 1231

1171 5.4. Factors moderating TSST response in stress-related disorders was similar to MBSR but avoided components which are specific 1232

to mindfulness – MBSR did not have differing effects on subjective 1233

1172 The effects of depression may interact with childhood mal- or cortisol response to TSST, but led to a reduction in inflammatory 1234

1173 treatment in how participants respond to the TSST. The TSST led effects (assessed with topical capsaican) (Rosenkranz et al., 2013). 1235

1174 to higher cortisol and heart rate levels in women with both a Contemplative/emotional training has been found to accelerate 1236

1175 major depressive disorder and sexual or physical abuse in child- autonomic recovery from the TSST, as well as reducing reactivity 1237

1176 hood (Heim et al., 2000). It also increased ACTH for those with for those who practiced more (Kemeny et al., 2012). Healthy partic- 1238

1177 both problems or just abuse, although ACTH was not higher than ipants have shown reduced cortisol response and stress appraisal 1239

1178 controls for those with major depressive disorder only. Early life on the TSST following cognitive-behavioural stress management 1240

1179 adversity and chronic stress predicted enhanced adrenal response training (Gaab et al., 2003), and a stress management and resilience 1241

1180 to the TSST in depressed adolescents (Rao et al., 2008), and adult programme reduced stress, heightened perceived control and 1242

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

14 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

1243 ␣-amylase recovery in stressed participants without a psychiatric specific mechanisms underlying such effects should be elucidated 1305

1244 disorder (Rose et al., 2013). in future research and novel therapeutics should be tested for 1306

efficacy in normalising the stress response. There is also emerg- 1307

1245 5.5.2. Pharmacological treatment for stress-related psychiatric ing evidence that dietary interventions may alter TSST response, 1308

1246 disorders although there is a paucity of research in this area using clinical 1309

1247 Research using the TSST and similar paradigms has also inves- samples. 1310

1248 tigated the effects of pharmacological agents under conditions of


1249 acute stress. Research using simulated public speaking (Guimarães
1250 et al., 1987; McNair et al., 1982) has indicated that benzodiazepines 6. Modified versions of the TSST 1311

1251 can reduce anxiety at pre-stress (Graeff et al., 2003). Pre-treatment


1252 with the benzodiazepine alprazolam blunts the ACTH and cortisol 6.1. Repeated measures 1312

1253 response to the TSST in healthy participants, but not state anx-
1254 iety, heart rate, adrenaline or noradrenaline (Fries et al., 2006), Researchers may wish to expose participants to the TSST on 1313

1255 which suggests that HPA axis responses may be more sensitive more than one occasion; for example, pre- and post-treatment 1314

1256 to detecting the effects of anti-anxiety drugs. Fries et al. (2006) when examining the effects of treatment for depression. However, 1315

1257 suggest that GABA binding may be a plausible mechanism for this where psychophysiological responses habituate to repeated expo- 1316

1258 effect; the T1521C polymorphism in the GABAA ␣6 receptor sub- sure to the TSST, this makes it difficult to separate the effects of 1317

1259 unit gene has been shown to attenuate ACTH, cortisol and blood treatment from habituation. Previous studies have exposed partic- 1318

1260 pressure responses to the TSST (Uhart et al., 2004). Administration ipants repeatedly to the TSST. Pruessner et al. (1997a) aggregated 1319

1261 of lamotrigine, a glutamate antagonist used to treat bipolar disor- data from multiple visits; although it is also possible that data 1320

1262 der, reduced cortisol, growth hormone and diastolic blood pressure aggregation reduces the level of noise in the data created by cortisol 1321

1263 response to the TSST in healthy volunteers (Makatsori et al., 2004). pulsatility (Young et al., 2004a), Pruessner et al. suggest that exclu- 1322

1264 The SSRI (selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor) paroxetine has sion of the first day’s cortisol response should have greatly reduced 1323

1265 been found to significantly reduce blood pressure during a modified a masking effect of novelty on cortisol response to other stressful 1324

1266 version of the TSST (Golding et al., 2005), although heart rate was aspects of their study. Nonetheless, as novelty is arguably a use- 1325

1267 not affected. Dysfunctional serotonergic function is associated with ful factor in inducing stress, “masking” this aspect of a stressful 1326

1268 dysfunctional HPA axis function and immune activity in depression experience should alter the nature of the stressor, and under- 1327

1269 (Leonard, 2006), and treatment with interferon-␣ can alter sero- mine the extent to which it is stressful. In one study involving 1328

1270 tonin metabolism as well as neuroendocrine activity (Capuron and repeated TSST exposure, some degree of novelty was maintained 1329

1271 Miller, 2004). SSRI’s may thus be relevant in studying immune and by making changes to the job applied for in the verbal part of 1330

1272 HPA axis response to acute stress in people with depression. Nora- task and the numbers involved in the arithmetic task for each 1331

1273 drenaline reuptake inhibitors also alter changes in the immune and TSST session (Kirschbaum et al., 1995b). For about one third of 1332

1274 endocrine systems induced by chronic stress (Leonard, 2001); the the participants, cortisol was markedly increased for all five days 1333

1275 effects of such substances in the acute stress response is thus also of participation (a Monday–Thursday plus the following Monday), 1334

1276 of interest. suggesting that at least some people do not quickly habituate to 1335

this stressor. Conversely, a minority of participants (16%) show 1336

1277 5.5.3. Dietary interventions for stress sensitisation to the TSST (Wüst et al., 2005). Vital exhaustion (a psy- 1337

1278 Dietary interventions for stress may also be of interest; both chological state of exhaustion comprising fatigue, irritability and 1338

1279 omega 3 phosphatidylserine (Hellhammer et al., 2012) and fer- demoralisation) reduced habituation to repeated exposure to the 1339

1280 tilised egg powder (Schult et al., 2010) have been found to restore TSST (Kudielka et al., 2006). Mean cortisol response was low for 1340

1281 HPA axis responses to the TSST which had been blunted in partici- all TSST exposures in participants with high levels of vital exhaus- 1341

1282 pants with high levels of chronic stress. Phospholipids did not alter tion. 1342

1283 the HPA axis response to the TSST, although participants receiv- Others have similarly modified the speech and arithmetic com- 1343

1284 ing a higher dose showed better memory performance post-TSST ponents between repeated TSST assessments and utilised longer 1344

1285 in participants aged 41–51 (Schubert et al., 2011). It may thus be inter-session intervals, such as one week (Gerra et al., 2001; 1345

1286 of interest to evaluate if other dietary changes can normalise the Mischler et al., 2005; von Känel et al., 2006) and four weeks 1346

1287 stress response to the TSST in depressed or anxious participants. (Schommer et al., 2003). The results of these studies indicated 1347

that although HPA axis outcomes (ACTH, salivary and serum corti- 1348

1288 5.6. TSST and stress-related psychiatric disorders: summary sol) habituated over two to three sessions, heart rate responses 1349

remained relatively stable. The lack of habituation in heart rate 1350

1289 Responses to the TSST in cases of stress-related disorders are may be due to a consistent level of general arousal elicited by 1351

1290 summarised in Table 2. There is evidence that major depression can repeated performances of the speech task despite a decline in the 1352

1291 lead to lengthened cortisol response to the TSST, and the inflam- severity of stress. Neither adrenaline nor noradrenaline response 1353

1292 matory response has also been shown to be altered in depression. habituated (Gerra et al., 2001; Schommer et al., 2003), nor did IL-6 1354

1293 Although panic disorder and PTSD are both considered anxiety dis- response habituate (von Känel et al., 2006), although the evidence 1355

1294 orders, they did not indicate the same moderating effect on HPA on blood pressure is less clear (Gerra et al., 2001; von Känel et al., 1356

1295 axis activity. Comparability may be reduced by the significant age 2006). 1357

1296 differences in the samples of some studies; child- versus adult- It thus seems that although a number of psychophysiological 1358

1297 onset depression have been associated with differences in HPA axis factors do not rapidly habituate to the TSST, for a majority of partic- 1359

1298 and immune measures (Kaufman et al., 2001). Response to the TSST ipants HPA axis responses to the TSST are attenuated after the first 1360

1299 in stress-related disorders may be moderated by gender and early visit. Greater procedural differences, e.g. speech content, serial sub- 1361

1300 life stress; major stressors in early life should be assessed. Other traction, can reduce the level of habituation (Foley and Kirschbaum, 1362

1301 stress-related disorders such as binge eating and irritable bowel 2010). Longer periods between TSST exposure may also reduce 1363

1302 syndrome may lead to perturbed stress responses. habituation; salivary cortisol response was attenuated with a 24 h 1364

1303 Both pharmacological and psychotherapeutic interventions for period between TSST performances but not after a 10 week inter- 1365

1304 stress-related disorders can moderate the TSST response. The val (Petrowski et al., 2012). Animal models of stress habituation 1366

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
NBR 1866 1–31
G Model
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress

Table 2
Responses to TSST in stress-related disorders.

Disorder Paper Sample Age Outcomes assessed Effect of TSST and psychiatric Substance
disorder use/pre-treatment

Anxiety disorders: Generalised Gerra et al. (2000) N = 40: (all male) 20 with Mean = 12.4, SD = 0.6 Plasma cort, adrenaline, Noradrenaline, testosterone, GH, No
anxiety disorder (GAD) GAD, 20 HC noradrenaline, ACTH, ␤-endorphin, HR, SBP, increase more in GAD.
GH, prolactin, testosterone, HR, BP Prolactin, ␤-endorphin, decrease
(pre- and post-stress) more in GAD. Non-significant
differences for cort & ACTH

Anxiety disorders: Social Hoyer et al. (2013) N = 89: 55 with SP SP: mean = 26.5, SD = 6, HC: Saliva and plasma cort (8 Higher No
phobia (male = 21, female = 24), 34 mean = 25.3, SD = 6.4 timepoints), HR, PASA (following depersonalisation/derealisation in
HC (male = 18, female = 16) speech preparation), SP, which was associated with
depersonalisation/derealisation, higher primary appraisal, safety
stress, safety behaviours behaviours and post-event

ARTICLE IN PRESS
(immediately post-stress), processing. Cort and HR not

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
post-event processing (one week reported
post-stress)
Krämer et al. N = 81: 41 with SP SP: mean = 10.1, SD = 1.4, Saliva cort, sAA (baseline, Heightened increase in anxiety in No
(2012) (male = 20, female = 21), 40 HC: mean = 10, SD = 1.1, immediately post-stress, response to TSST and higher HR
HC (male = 20, female = 20) range = 8–12 post-stress +10, 20, 30 min), HR, throughout experimental session
subjective anxiety (baseline, in SP
post-preparation, between TSST
tasks, immediately post-stress,
post-stress +10, 20, 30 min)
Schmitz et al. N = 56: 30 with SP SP: mean = 10.1, SD = 1.2, HR, GSR, respiratory sinus Reduced respiratory sinus No
(2011) (male = 15, female = 15), 26 HC: mean = 10, SD = 1.1, arrhythmia, subjective anxiety arrhythmia activity compared to
HC (male = 15, female = 11) range = 8–12 (timing same as Krämer et al., HC, slower HR recovery
2012)
van Veen et al. N = 18 (female = 8, Mean = 39.1 Saliva cort, sAA (stressor onset Higher saliva cort and Responders to
(2009) male = 10), SP, no lifetime −60, 10, +10, 20, 30, 40, 50, 90, alpha-amylase. No HC group 20–60 mg
comorbidity 120, 180, 240, 360, 420) citalopram. Use of
psychotropic
medication
stopped 2 weeks
pre-test.
Tryptophan
depletion or
control
administered

Anxiety disorders: Panic Petrowski et al. N = 34: 34 with PD PD: mean = 35, SD = 12, HC: Saliva cort (at stressor −10, +1, 10, PD leads to lower cort response, no PD participants
disorder (2010) (female = 23, male = 11), 34 mean = 35.3, SD = 12.2 20, 30, 40, 50), HR moderation for HR. Second TSST taking medication
HC (female = 23, male = 11), exposure = lower cort response for
HC, no habituation for HR
Petrowski et al. N = 64: 32 with PD PD: mean = 32.9, SD = 11.2, Saliva and plasma cort and ACTH PD leads to lower cort response, no No
(2013) (female = 20, male = 12), 32 HC: mean = 31.2, SD = 11.1 (stressor onset −15, 1 min, end of moderation for HR. Trend for
HC (female = 20, male = 12) stressor +1, 10, 20, 30, 45, 60 min), higher response in subjective
HR (3 min periods pre-stress, anxiety
speech task, arithmetic task,
post-stress) subjective anxiety
(pre- and post-stress)

Anxiety disorders: Simeon et al. N = 139: 35 PTSD PTSD: mean = 41.5, Plasma cort (post-anticipation, PTSD and DD led to higher TSST compared to
Post-traumatic stress disorder (2007a,b) (female = 19, male = 16), 46 SD = 11.5, DD: mean = 31.2, post-stressor, post-stress +30, subjective response, but did not DST
dissociative disorder SD = 10.3, HC: mean = 32.8, +60 min), urinary cort, subjective moderate plasma cort response
(female = 26, male = 20), 58 SD = 10.8 stress (post-stressor)
HC (female = 26, male = 32)

15
16

NBR 1866 1–31


G Model
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress

Table 2 (Continued)

Disorder Paper Sample Age Outcomes assessed Effect of TSST and psychiatric Substance
disorder use/pre-treatment

Depression Chopra et al. (2009) N = 54: 26 chronic MD MD: mean = 43.3, SD = 5.7, Saliva cort, subjective stress Females: greater cort response in 18 MD taking
(female = 12, male = 14), 28 HC: mean = 39.4, SD = 6 post-stressor (stressor onset -35, MD. Males: lower peak percent psychotropic
HC (female = 14, male = 14) 20, immediately prior to onset, cort change in MD. Trend for higher medication, 16
+20, 40, 60, 80 min) subjective stress in MD generally taking
antidepressants

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Dienes et al. (2013) N = 57 (all female) 15 MD, MD: mean = 18.4, SD = 0.9, Saliva cort, positive and negative MD group had higher net increase 2 MD participants

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
20 at risk, 22 HC At risk: mean = 18.7, affect (baseline, immediately, 10, in cort than at risk, slower recovery taking
SD = 0.8, HC: mean = 18.9, 25, 40 min post-stress) in negative affect fluoxetine/amphetamine
SD = 1.2
Harkness et al. N = 71: 30 no/mild No/mild depression: Saliva cort (baseline, Higher cort in mild/moderate 8 participants
(2011) depression (female = 20, mean = 14.7, SD = 2.3, post-preparation, immediately depression with childhood taking
male = 10), 16 moderate Moderate: mean = 15.7, post-stress, post-stress +1, 2 h) maltreatment. Moderate/severe anti-depressant
(female = 9, male = 7), 25 SD = 1.6, Severe: mean = 16, depression: blunted cort regardless medication
severe (female = 19, SD = 2 of child maltreatment
male = 6)
Heim et al. (2000) N = 49 (all female) 13 with MD + CA: mean = 32, No Plasma cort, ACTH, HR (stressor Higher ACTH for CA group (+15, No
both current MD and CA, MD, CA: mean = 30, MD, no onset −15, 0, +15, 30, 45, 60, 75, 30 min). Higher cort for MD + CA
14 CA and no MD, 10 MD CA: mean = 34.6, HC: 90 min) group (+30, 45, 60 min). Trend for
and no CA, 12 HC mean = 29 higher HR in MD + CA group
(+15 min)
Pace et al. (2006) N = 28 (all male) physically MD: mean = 29.9, SD = 9.6, Plasma IL-6, NF-␬B DNA-binding, Higher IL-6 (baseline and 90 min), No
healthy, 14 with MD and HC: mean = 29.4, SD = 8 natural killer (NK) cells (baseline at higher increase in NF-␬B
early-life stress, 14 HC stress onset −1 min, baseline +30, DNA-binding for MD group. No
60, 75, 90 min) group difference in NK cells
Rao et al. (2008) N = 55: 30 with MD MD: mean = 15.1, SD = 1.4, Saliva cort (baseline: 30 min Higher and more prolonged cort No
(female = 17, male = 13), 25 HC: mean = 15, SD = 1.5 intervals for 2 h, post-stress: response for MD group
HC (female = 13, male = 12) immediately post-stress plus
10 min intervals for 60 min)
Stewart et al. N = 64: 31 with current MD: M = 16, SD = 1.6, HC: Saliva cort (baseline, Lower cort response in depression 9 depressed
(2013) depression (female = 25, mean = 14.6, SD = 2 post-preparation, immediately participants taking
male = 6), 33 HC post-stress, 25, 65 min post-stress) psychotropic
(female = 22, male = 11) medicine
Taylor et al. (2006) N = 68 (high cardiovascular MD: mean = 62.3, SD = 6.4, Saliva cort, HR, BP, subjective MD had lower cortisol response Excluded if taking
disease risk) 48 with MD HC: mean = 62.5, SD = 6 emotions (PANAS), RSA, C-reactive and lower RSA during stress medications
(female = 32, male = 16), 20 protein (pre-baseline, pre- and affecting HPA axis
HC (female = 8, male = 12) post-anticipation, post-stressor,
10, 20, 30, 45, 60 min into recovery)
Young et al. (2000) N = 20: 10 with MD MD: mean = 33, SD = 11, Plasma cort, ␤-endorphin (stressor MD leads to lower ␤-endorphin Treatment
(female = 4, male = 6), 10 HC: mean = 33.9, SD = 11.5 onset −50, 20, 10, 0, +5, 10, 15, 20, response, higher negative mood postponed until
HC, Axis I comorbidity 25, 30, 35, 40, 55, 70) subjective post-stress, no moderation for cort end of study
excluded (female = 4, mood (end of TSST, 20 min later)
male = 6)
NBR 1866 1–31
G Model
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress

Table 2 (Continued)

Disorder Paper Sample Age Outcomes assessed Effect of TSST and psychiatric Substance
disorder use/pre-treatment

Anxiety and depression Young et al. N = 96: 15 pure MD Pure MD: mean = 28.7, Blood cort and ACTH (10 min Comorbid group showed greater MD and anxiety
(2004a,b) (female = 7, male = 8), 15 SD = 7.7, Pure anxiety: intervals from 30 min before stress ACTH response. All anxiety and untreated
pure anxiety (12 SP, 3 PD, 1 mean = 25.4, SD = 8.5, onset to 1 h post-stress) subjective depression group had higher
PTSD; female = 9, male = 7), Comorbid: M = 23.2, anxiety (immediately pre-stress, subjective anxiety at each
18 comorbid MD and SD = 5.5, HC: M = 26, post-stress, 20 min post-stress), measurement
anxiety (13 SP, 5 = PD, SD = 7.1 HR, BP (30 min and immediately

ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 = PTSD; female = 14, pre-stress, end of stress)

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx
male = 4;, 48 HC
(female = 29, male = 19)
Young et al. (2005) N = 90: 13 pure MD Pure MD: mean = 28.2, Blood cort and ACTH (10 min Negative correlation between GH MD and anxiety
(female = 7, male = 6), 15 SD = 7.7, Pure anxiety: intervals from 30 min before stress and ACTH in HC and pure anxiety, untreated
pure anxiety (12 SP, 3 PD, 1 mean = 25.4, SD = 8.5, onset to 1 h post-stress). GH but not in pure MD and comorbid
PTSD; female = 8, male = 7), Comorbid: M = 23.2, (separate day to blood; clonidine anxiety and MD
17 comorbid MD and SD = 5.5, HC: M = 25.8, infusion −30, 15, 1 min, end of
anxiety (13 SP, 4 = PD, SD = 7.1 infusion 15, 30, 60, 90, 120 min)
2 = PTSD; female = 13,
male = 4), 45 HC
(female = 26, male = 19)

Eating disorders Monteleone et al. N = 20 (all female) 10 with BN: mean = 24.1, Salivary cort, ghrelin (baseline, Higher ghrelin response in BN No
(2012) BN, 10 HC range = 20–28, HC: immediately post-stress, end of
mean = 25.3, stress +15, 30, 50 min)
range = 21–29.5
Rosenberg et al. N = 24: 8 obese with BED BED: mean = 49.8, SD = 4.9, Serum cort (stress onset −30, 0, Blunted cort response in BED group No
(2012) (female = 6, male = 2), 8 Non-BED obese: +15, 35, 55, 75 min), HR, BP,
obese without BED mean = 50.3, SD = 5.7, HC: subjective stress, anxiety
(female = 5, male = 3), 8 mean = 32.6, SD = 3.55 (immediately pre- and post-TSST)
normal weight controls
(female = 5, male = 3)

Irritable bowel syndrome Suárez-Hitz et al. N = 77 (all female) 57 with IBS: mean = 29.3, Saliva cort, plasma ACTH (baseline, Blunted increase in cort and lower No
(2012) IBS (34 = no Axis I range = 20–51, HC: post-preparation, immediately ACTH at recovery for IBS without
comorbidity, 16 = major mean = 29, range = 23–43 post-stress, post-stress +20, 30, 40, psychiatric comorbidity compared
depression/dysthymia, 60 min) subjective stress (baseline, to HC. Higher PASA for IBS group
3 = generalised anxiety, post-preparation, immediately and higher subjective stress for IBS
5 = PD, 4 = social phobia, post-stress) PASA group with comorbidity
3 = specific phobia), 20 HC (post-preparation)

Ultra-high risk for psychosis Pruessner et al. N = 42: 21 with UHR UHR: mean = 20.8, Saliva cort, HR, BP (stress onset Lower cort, lower SBP for UHR 6 UHR patients
(2013) (female = 9, male = 12), 21 range = 16.4–27.2, HC: −45, 30, 15, 1, +1, 10, 20, 40, taking
HC (female = 9, male = 12) age = 20.8, range 15.9–27.4) 60 min), subjective stress antidepressant
(immediately post-stress) medication

Key: BED, binge eating disorder; BN, bulimia nervosa; BP, blood pressure; CA, childhood abuse; cort, cortisol; DD, dissociative disorder; GH, growth hormone; GSR, galvanic skin response; HC, healthy controls; HR, heart rate; IBS,
irritable bowel syndrome; MD, major depression; PD, panic disorder; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; SBP, systolic blood pressure; SP, social phobia; UHR, ultra-high risk for psychosis.

17
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

18 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

1367 support these suggestions; habituation in prolactin response can Researchers have designed a version of the TSST for use with 1428

1368 be attenuated by exposure to differing types of stressor (Jean Kant larger groups of participants (TSST-G; von Dawans et al., 2011), 1429

1369 et al., 1985), and habituation in adrenaline and corticosterone and tested it against a less stressful control condition for grouped 1430

1370 response can be attenuated by longer inter-stressor intervals (De participants. While standing in a line with five other participants, 1431

1371 Boer et al., 1990). participants speak for 2 min each on their suitability for a relevant 1432

job (mobile walls are used to restrict eye contact between partic- 1433

ipants). In a control condition, participants read a scientific text 1434


1372 6.2. Control TSST
rather than preparing and performing a speech. Unlike the con- 1435

trol condition in Het et al. (2009), control participants are required 1436
1373 A non-stress control condition is required for research using
to speak in front of the panel. However, they simply read aloud 1437
1374 the TSST aims to examine the effects of stress per se, rather than
the extract simultaneously in front of the panel, followed by serial 1438
1375 the difference between groups in terms of their response to stress.
addition of numbers. Video cameras are present in the testing room 1439
1376 Although some TSST studies have included a control group that
for the experimental condition but not the control condition. Sali- 1440
1377 simply involves sitting quietly, a control TSST has been designed
vary cortisol increased in response to the TSST-G, and not in the 1441
1378 which aims to be as similar as possible to the typical TSST with the
control group; heart rate increased under both conditions, but to 1442
1379 exception that it should not be stress-inducing (Het et al., 2009).
a greater extent in the stress condition. Participants in the TSST-G 1443
1380 Participants are required to talk about a film, novel or recent holiday
group reported higher post-test stress, strain, challenge and lower 1444
1381 trip (the topic is thus less relevant to self-presentation and self-
controllability than the control group. The serial position in which 1445
1382 esteem than in the experimental TSST). Social-evaluative threat is
the participants were called did not have a significant effect. The 1446
1383 reduced, as participants do not speak in front of a committee and
TSST-G lasts longer than the TSST overall, although the free speech 1447
1384 they are not led to believe that they are being recorded. Salivary
task for any given participant is shorter. Childs et al. (2006) used 1448
1385 cortisol concentrations were found to increase in the TSST condi-
a free speech task that was equally long for grouped and individ- 1449
1386 tion but fall in the control TSST, and participants in the control
ual participants, although this may be impractical in larger groups. 1450
1387 TSST reported less feelings of threat, challenge, as well as higher
Where group tests last longer there may be a greater component 1451
1388 control expectancy (suggesting lower amounts of uncontrollabil-
of anticipation compared to individual participants, particularly for 1452
1389 ity) and self-concept of own ability. A second experiment using
grouped participants who speak after the rest of the group. In com- 1453
1390 a crossover design and communication via intercom for the con-
parison, those participants who speak first in a group will have a 1454
1391 trol TSST also indicated higher cortisol, threat, challenge and lower
longer recovery period. 1455
1392 self-concept of self ability in the TSST condition than in the con-
1393 trol, regardless of condition order. Another control condition for the
1394 TSST has involved describing a favourite book or film to an assis- 6.4. Children and adolescents 1456
1395 tant followed by performing a Solitaire game without being taped
1396 or evaluated, and has been associated with lower anxiety, negative Researchers have also developed a TSST for children (the TSST- 1457
1397 mood, salivary cortisol, salivary ␣-amylase heart rate, blood pres- C) (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 1997). The verbal component requires 1458
1398 sure than the TSST (Childs and de Wit, 2010; Childs et al., 2011; children to finish a story in as exciting a manner as possible, and 1459
1399 Hamidovic et al., 2010). to perform this task better than the other children taking part. The 1460
1400 More recently, a friendly TSST (f-TSST) has been designed arithmetic component is graded based on age group (e.g. subtract- 1461
1401 (Wiemers et al., 2013b); unlike the procedure by Het et al. (2009), ing 7 from 758 for children aged 9–11 years). The committee give 1462
1402 it involves speaking in front of a panel. The f-TSST requires partic- positive feedback, and the children are encouraged to continue if 1463
1403 ipants to write about their education, career aspirations, hobbies, they finish before the 5 min are up. The TSST-C has also been used 1464
1404 and favourite book or film before being interviewed without being with adolescents aged 10–14 years (Gordis et al., 2006). Salivary 1465
1405 filmed. The panel smile and nod to indicate encouragement, and cortisol was increased by the TSST-C, as was sAA. sAA has also been 1466
1406 avoid pauses in the interview by promptly asking follow-up ques- increased by a version of the TSST-C which used academic topics for 1467
1407 tions. The f-TSST did not increase HPA axis activity or negative the speech task (Stroud et al., 2005, as cited in Granger et al., 2006). 1468
1408 affect, although it did increase salivary ␣-amylase. However, more A modified version of the TSST which can be used with both chil- 1469
1409 encouraging panels have elsewhere shown comparable HPA axis dren and adults has been developed (TSST-M; Yim et al., 2010). It 1470
1410 effects to the classic, unencouraging TSST panels (Taylor et al., requires participants to give a presentation that they are to imagine 1471
1411 2010). Control conditions have also been designed for group assess- they are giving to a new class; they are to talk about their person- 1472
1412 ment (see Section 6.3). ality (including one positive and one negative aspect thereof) and 1473

why they would be liked by the class. The TSST-M increased sali- 1474

1413 6.3. Group assessment vary cortisol and led to higher negative affect and lower positive 1475

affect; the increase in cortisol applied to 9–10 year olds as well 1476

1414 Group assessment for the TSST is desirable, as it can reduce the as 11–12 year olds. However, the authors note that the test may 1477

1415 time commitment of confederates (Childs et al., 2006), although not be suitable for a wide age range of children. Greater cortisol 1478

1416 this may not always be practical where sample collection is slower response to the TSST-M was associated with enhanced recall of the 1479

1417 (e.g. if collecting blood samples). Childs et al. (2006) compared par- test two weeks later for children, adolescents and adults (Quas et al., 1480

1418 ticipants tested individually and those tested in groups of two to 2012). In a later study this effect was again evident for 7–8 year olds, 1481

1419 three, with grouped participants completing the verbal and mental although in those aged 12–14 higher arousal was associated with 1482

1420 arithmetic tasks one after the other. Similar to repeated measures both fewer correct and fewer incorrect responses, suggesting that 1483

1421 Trier designs, each participant was given a different starting num- stress might reduce willingness to respond in the adolescent group 1484

1422 ber for serial subtraction to avoid learning effects. Participants (Quas et al., 2013). An advantage of the TSST-M is that the person- 1485

1423 tested in groups did not experience a significantly different change ality speech may be more consistently stressful across participants 1486

1424 in salivary cortisol or reported anxiety compared to participants than the job speech from the original TSST, since the job application 1487

1425 tested one at a time. However, peak heart rate was higher for context allows participants to focus on the specifics of the qualifi- 1488

1426 grouped participants, with heart rate increasing more dramatically cations they hold, and thus avoid having to explicitly address their 1489

1427 for the first 5 min of the stressor. own personality and likability. 1490

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 19

1491 6.5. Virtual/imagined TSST committee negative affect, sAA and cortisol. Smeets et al. also developed a con- 1554

trol Maastricht Acute Stress Test which requires hand immersion in 1555

1492 Similar to grouped assessment, a virtual reality version of a lukewarm water and a simple counting task; this did not increase 1556

1493 committee could greatly reduce the labour required of the TSST sAA or salivary cortisol levels. 1557

1494 committee. However, such a methodology may be less stressful


1495 than performing for a panel “in the flesh”; neither a speech task
6.7. Modified versions of the TSST: summary 1558
1496 within an environment generated by a virtual reality helmet nor
1497 a math task outside the virtual reality environment induced sig-
Numerous adaptations have been made to the “classic” TSST 1559
1498 nificant changes in anxiety or cortisol, although both increased
to meet the requirements of researchers. Repeated versions of 1560
1499 heart rate (Kotlyar et al., 2008b). Performing for an imagined audi-
the TSST have generally led to habituation in HPA axis responses, 1561
1500 ence behind a one-way mirror or for a virtual reality audience
although longer periods between tests seem to attenuate this effect. 1562
1501 increased cortisol compared to a control group, although perform-
A control TSST without an interview committee has been shown 1563
1502 ing in front of a live panel led to a greater response than the virtual
not to increase HPA axis response, although control versions with 1564
1503 reality or imaginary panel (Kelly et al., 2007). A one-way mirror
a friendly or encouraging panel have shown mixed effects. The 1565
1504 has been used in social phobia research, perhaps from concern
TSST has been shown to be effective when administered to groups, 1566
1505 that the classic TSST would be too stressful to complete for many
and a control version for groups is also available. This may free up 1567
1506 participants with this condition. The Leiden Public Speaking Task
resources which could be used to perform a control Trier as well 1568
1507 involves speaking in front of a pre-recorded audience; it has been
as the stressful version. However, virtual committees may require 1569
1508 shown to induce a substantial cortisol response in 55% of partici-
further development and evaluation before they can be treated as 1570
1509 pants in an adolescent sample (Westenberg et al., 2009), with the
a reliable means of inducing psychosocial stress. The TSST for chil- 1571
1510 response being stronger in adolescents than in children (Sumter
dren provides an age-appropriate psychosocial stressor, although 1572
1511 et al., 2010). A virtual reality version of the TSST which uses immer-
the TSST-M has potential for use in both children and adults, allow- 1573
1512 sive computer-generated rooms (Jönsson et al., 2010) rather than
ing for more direct examination of the effects of maturation on the 1574
1513 the head-mounted displays has led to a cortisol response substan-
stress response. Combining the TSST with pharmacological chal- 1575
1514 tially greater than that observed in the virtual reality condition of
lenges and cold pressor tasks can potentially allow for a more 1576
1515 Kelly et al. (2007), although given the small sample size this should
detailed examination of psychosocial stress effects on human psy- 1577
1516 be interpreted with caution. Virtual reality methodology could have
chophysiology. 1578
1517 the added advantage of holding the responses of the panel more
1518 constant (Westenberg et al., 2009).
7. Discussion 1579

1519 6.6. Hybrid versions of the TSST


7.1. Comparison with other methods of inducing stress and 1580

1520 The DST (see Section 7.1) and the TSST have been combined, measuring HPA axis activity 1581

1521 with dexamethasone taken at bedtime and exposure to the TSST


1522 occurring the following morning. The participants who took dex- 7.1.1. Pharmacological stimulation of the HPA axis 1582

1523 amethasone showed typical suppression of cortisol compared to The correlation between salivary cortisol and unbound corti- 1583

1524 a placebo group, but also displayed increased heart rate through- sol in plasma and serum persists across the TSST, DST and ACTH 1584

1525 out the protocol, including during exposure to the TSST (sAA and stimulation (Hellhammer et al., 2009). The DST involves the admin- 1585

1526 blood pressure did not differ between dexamethasone conditions). istration of 1 mg of dexamethasone at 23.00 and examination of 1586

1527 The authors suggest that this methodology allows for the examina- HPA axis activity at 16.00 and 23.00 the following day; as healthy 1587

1528 tion of interactions between the sympathetic nervous system and controls will have suppressed HPA axis activity for at least 24 h after 1588

1529 the HPA axis (Andrews et al., 2012). Conversely, the TSST has been overnight administration, samples have also been taken at 08.00, 1589

1530 combined with the administration of propranolol, which reduces although this time point was not as efficient for detecting abnormal 1590

1531 sympathetic nervous system responses to stress; heightened cor- results in melancholic depression (Carroll, 1982). ACTH stimulation 1591

1532 tisol responses have been shown in participants who received can be performed through a short Synacthen test, which typically 1592

1533 propranolol instead of placebo, suggesting that one system may involves the administration of 0.25 mg ACTH1–24 , although a lower 1593

1534 compensate where the activity of the other is reduced (Andrews dose of 1 ␮g may be more useful in a clinical context (Abdu et al., 1594

1535 and Pruessner, 2013). However, propranolol does not alter IL-6 1999), and assessing HPA axis activity at similar time points to the 1595

1536 response to the TSST (von Känel et al., 2008). TSST (e.g. Blair et al., 2013; Clark et al., 1998). The TSST has an effect 1596

1537 The cold pressor task is a means of inducing stress by sub- on cortisol levels that is similar in magnitude to the effect of injec- 1597

1538 merging the hand in cold water for 1–2 min (see Section 7.1 for tion of 1 ␮g/kg human corticotropin releasing hormone (hCRH), 1598

1539 further discussion). The socially evaluated cold pressor task takes although administration of 0.25 mg of ACTH1–24 led to a larger 1599

1540 the cold pressor task and adds a psychosocial element to it by film- and more sustained increase in salivary cortisol (Schmidt-Reinwald 1600

1541 ing the participant, telling them that this footage will be analysed, et al., 1999). 1601

1542 and having an experimenter watch the participant during hand Both the TSST and DST administration are sensitive to higher 1602

1543 immersion (Schwabe et al., 2008). Consistent with the finding that HPA axis responsivity in depression. The DST has been found to 1603

1544 social evaluative stress is particularly effective at increasing HPA be less likely to lead to reduced cortisol in depressed participants 1604

1545 axis activity (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004), the socially evaluated compared to controls (Lowy et al., 1984), and patients with depres- 1605

1546 cold pressor task induces a greater cortisol response than the orig- sion who had higher cortisol levels following DST also had higher 1606

1547 inal cold pressor task. The Maastricht Acute Stress Test (Smeets catecholamine levels (Barnes et al., 1983; Sora et al., 1986). There 1607

1548 et al., 2012) combines aspects of the socially evaluated cold pressor is evidence that the DST normalises during treatment for depres- 1608

1549 task and the TSST. This comprises a preparation period, followed by sion (Carroll et al., 1976), although this was the not case for some 1609

1550 immersion of a hand in ice-cold water, combined with mental arith- patients, who may have lower levels of ACTH (Yerevanian et al., 1610

1551 metic. The Maastricht Acute Stress Test has been shown to induce 1983). Depressed patients with abnormal response to the DST were 1611

1552 a higher cortisol response than either the original or socially eval- more likely to commit suicide in a 15-year follow-up (Coryell and 1612

1553 uated cold pressor tasks, and it has similar effects to the TSST on Schlesser, 2001). The HPA axis response to the TSST may also 1613

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

20 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

1614 have predictive validity; cortisol reactivity to a control version 7.1.4. The cortisol awakening response 1675

1615 of the TSST was shown to predict depressive symptoms at a six- The cortisol awakening response leads to an increase in cortisol 1676

1616 month follow-up (Morris et al., 2012). Similar to the standard dose release of 50–75% and peaks approximately 30 min after waking 1677

1617 DST, there is evidence that the HPA axis response to a low-dose (Pruessner et al., 1997b). The TSST has been found to have an 1678

1618 (0.5 mg) DST normalises in non-suppressors following remission effect on cortisol levels that is similar in magnitude to the cortisol 1679

1619 (Sarai and Matsunaga, 1986). PTSD did not alter HPA response to awakening response (Schmidt-Reinwald et al., 1999). The corti- 1680

1620 a low-dose DST in adults (Simeon et al., 2007a) or adolescents sol awakening response is a useful measure of HPA axis activity 1681

1621 (Lipschitz et al., 2003). In contrast, greater suppression of corti- for large studies, as participants can collect saliva samples while 1682

1622 sol in response to a low-dose DST occurred in women who had at home (Kudielka and Wüst, 2010). In ambulatory studies, about 1683

1623 experienced severe childhood sexual abuse (Stein et al., 1997), and 15–25% of participants do not show this response (Dockray et al., 1684

1624 older trauma survivors with PTSD (Yehuda et al., 2002). Similar to 2008; Wüst et al., 2000). However, given observed non-compliance 1685

1625 the cortisol response to the TSST, the low-dose DST differentiated in ambulatory studies and consistent presence of the cortisol awak- 1686

1626 childhood abuse survivors with depression from non-depressed ening response in sleep laboratory studies, healthy young adults 1687

1627 survivors and healthy controls, although the standard DST did not may generally show this response (Kudielka and Wüst, 2010). The 1688

1628 (Newport et al., 2004). Combining administration of dexametha- cortisol awakening response can nonetheless be moderated by fac- 1689

1629 sone the night before the day of assessment with administration of tors such as gender and health status (Fries et al., 2009). Awakening 1690

1630 CRH on the day of assessment has been found to have greater sen- has also been associated with a fall in secretory immunoglobu- 1691

1631 sitivity to major depression than the classic DST (DEX/CRH; Heuser lin A (Hucklebridge et al., 1998) as well as increased heart rate 1692

1632 et al., 1994). Although the level of sensitivity of this DEX/CRH test and low frequency/high frequency ratio, although there was not 1693

1633 has subsequently been challenged (Schüle et al., 2009), depressed a clear association between the cortisol awakening response and 1694

1634 patients show higher cortisol and ACTH following the DEX/CRH the changes in heart rate or heart rate variability (Stalder et al., 1695

1635 (Heuser et al., 1994; Ising et al., 2007; von Bardeleben and Holsboer, 2011); there is thus scope for further research on the effects of 1696

1636 1989), and higher cortisol response to the DEX/CRH test is asso- awakening on immune and cardiovascular factors as well as HPA 1697

1637 ciated with increased risk of relapse in patients with remitted axis response. The cortisol awakening response is also affected by 1698

1638 depression (Zobel et al., 1999, 2001). Similarly, men who had expe- stress-related disorders; it is higher in unmedicated participants 1699

1639 rienced childhood abuse had higher cortisol and ACTH following with current major depressive disorder (Bhagwagar et al., 2005), 1700

1640 administration of corticotrophin-releasing factor and dexametha- as well as in men with depressive symptoms but without clini- 1701

1641 sone (Heim et al., 2008). In contrast to the pro-inflammatory effect cal depression (Pruessner et al., 2003) and recovered depression 1702

1642 of the TSST, DEX/CRH reduces plasma IL-6 and TNF-␣ in depressed patients (Bhagwagar et al., 2003). 1703

1643 patients (Schuld et al., 2001).


7.1.5. Cognitive tasks 1704

Many studies use cognitive tasks to assess the psychophysiolog- 1705

ical effects of stress. For example, the Stroop test has been shown 1706
1644 7.1.2. The cold pressor task
to produce subjective stress effects, as well as changes in heart rate 1707
1645 Although the cold pressor task induces subjective stress
and heart rate variability (Delaney and Brodie, 2000) and inhibi- 1708
1646 (Schwabe et al., 2008) and distress (Kotlyar et al., 2008a), the TSST
tion of gastric activity (Huerta-Franco et al., 2012). The defined 1709
1647 leads to higher plasma cortisol and ACTH than the cold pressor task
intensity stress simulator, which requires simultaneous perfor- 1710
1648 (McRae et al., 2006). Similar to DEX/CRH challenge, the cold pres-
mance of multiple cognitive tasks, such as mental arithmetic and 1711
1649 sor task has been found to reduce the pro-inflammatory cytokine
Stroop test, leads to an increase in cortisol, although this may be 1712
1650 response via TNF-␣, IL-1␤, MCP-1 and IL-12 (Cruz-Almeida et al.,
restricted to the afternoon (Scholey et al., 2009), higher subjective 1713
1651 2012). The cold pressor task has increased heart rate and adrenaline
anxiety (Kennedy et al., 2006) and down-regulation of secretory 1714
1652 in some studies (Grassi et al., 2008; Kotlyar et al., 2008a) although
immunoglobulin A when workload is increased (Wetherell and 1715
1653 other research has not shown a significant effect on heart rate
Sidgreaves, 2005). An advantage of using computer-based cognitive 1716
1654 (Weise et al., 1993) or adrenaline (Dyson et al., 2006). An advan-
tasks is that the effects of stress on cognition can be studied using 1717
1655 tage of cold pressor task is that it is shorter than the TSST (stress
precise measures of cognitive performance, and since the cognitive 1718
1656 exposure only lasts 3 min) and only requires the presence of one
task is itself the stressor, current stress is studied, rather than post- 1719
1657 experimenter. However, as the cold pressor task is a method of
stress effects, as is the case of the TSST followed by cognitive tasks. 1720
1658 inducing pain as well as being a stressor (and a psychosocial stressor
Tasks on which the difficulty of the task can be altered precisely 1721
1659 in the case of the socially evaluated cold pressor task), conceptual
are advantageous in terms of controlling the intensity of the stres- 1722
1660 differences between the two should be borne in mind.
sor. However, comparing different types of psychological stressor, 1723

tests combining public speaking with a cognitive task (including 1724

the TSST) proved to induce larger cortisol responses than cognitive 1725
1661 7.1.3. The CO2 challenge test tasks or public speaking alone (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004). This 1726
1662 The CO2 challenge test, which involves inhaling carbon dioxide- difference was not attributable to task duration, suggesting that the 1727
1663 enriched air, has been found to induce panic attacks in people with nature of the stressor may be the key factor. 1728
1664 panic disorder, but is also an effective physiological stressor. The
1665 CO2 challenge can induce a modest but significant increase in sub- 7.1.6. Noise 1729
1666 jective anxiety, noradrenaline and systolic blood pressure, as well Noise is frequently used as a stressor in research, in field studies 1730
1667 as a reduction in heart rate, although no effect has been detected as well as in experimental work; noise has been found to increase 1731
1668 on adrenaline or immune factors (for a review, see Vickers et al., sAA (Wagner et al., 2010), noradrenaline secretion (Babisch et al., 1732
1669 2012). Plasma and serum cortisol appear to increase following the 2001), heart rate variability (Schnell et al., 2013), and to impair cog- 1733
1670 CO2 challenge, but salivary cortisol does not (e.g. Kaye et al., 2004; nitive performance (Bengtsson et al., 2004; Jahncke et al., 2011), 1734
1671 Loft et al., 2007; Wetherell et al., 2006). It has been observed that and increase fatigue (Jahncke et al., 2011), although noise may 1735
1672 70% of participants can be classified as salivary cortisol responders enhance performance under conditions of low alertness (Smith, 1736
1673 or non-responders across testing sessions, although a majority of 2012). Although evening cortisol has been found to correlate with 1737
1674 these were non-responders (Wetherell et al., 2006). noise exposure (Fouladi et al., 2012) research has not indicated a 1738

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 21

1739 clear effect of noise on cortisol or subjective stress (e.g. Bengtsson neuroendocrine responses (Yim et al., 2010). Although the Mon- 1802

1740 et al., 2004; Ljungberg and Neely, 2007). A novel paradigm called treal Imaging Stress Test induces stress in participants while being 1803

1741 the Mannheim Multicomponent Stress Test (Reinhardt et al., 2012) scanned by fMRI, there is a lack of a full TSST to be used in com- 1804

1742 combines noise stress with the added stressors of cognitive per- bination with fMRI. Nonetheless, the existence of virtual reality 1805

1743 formance, emotional induction and the threat of loss of money. versions of the TSST may allow for such a version to be designed 1806

1744 Participants perform serial addition while being presented with in the future. Imaging techniques may be useful for teasing apart 1807

1745 pictures which induce negative emotions and increasing decibels the cognitive effects of psychosocial stress, although careful con- 1808

1746 of white noise. They are also informed that incorrect answers result trol conditions (e.g. reading an extract aloud to a virtual committee, 1809

1747 in reduced payment. The Mannheim Multicomponent Stress Test similar to von Dawans et al., 2011) may be necessary to delineate 1810

1748 increased subjective stress, heart rate and cortisol activity. Aspects the brain areas affected by psychosocial stress and those involved 1811

1749 of this task, such as threatened reduction in payment, could easily in the performance of mental arithmetic and speech tasks. 1812

1750 be adapted to the TSST. As mentioned above, TSST research employing a repeated mea- 1813

sures design could induce more consistent stress responses over 1814

1751 7.1.7. Comparison with other methods of inducing stress and multiple testing sessions by varying the stressors to a greater 1815

1752 measuring HPA axis activity: summary extent (Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010). In a design with three vis- 1816

1753 Unlike the DST, the TSST does not require any pharmacologi- its, this could be carried out by using the job interview speech 1817

1754 cal treatment, and unlike the cold pressor task it does not expose from the classic TSST during one testing session, the personality 1818

1755 the participant to physical discomfort/pain. However, in the case of speech from the TSST-M during another session and the storytelling 1819

1756 very severe depression or social anxiety, the TSST may be more dif- speech task from the TSST-C for the third. Having each partic- 1820

1757 ficult to complete than tests such as the DST, given the performance ipant perform for different panel members during each session 1821

1758 requirements placed on the research participant for the speech and should also lessen habituation. Alternatively, researchers may wish 1822

1759 mental arithmetic tasks. The use of the interview panel can make to pre-select those high-responding participants who do not habit- 1823

1760 the TSST more labour-intensive than other methods such as the uate to repeated stress observed previously (e.g. Kirschbaum et al., 1824

1761 cold pressor task, although the amount of work required may be 1995b). A repeated measures design for the TSST which can reli- 1825

1762 reduced by the use of modified protocols such as group assessment ably induce a stress response in the HPA axis over multiple visits 1826

1763 or virtual committees. Table 3 summarises these methods of stress will be most useful for intervention studies (e.g. those investigat- 1827

1764 induction and their relative advantages and disadvantages. ing coping strategies for acute stressors or experiments on changes 1828

in diet). A repeated measures version of the TSST for children and 1829

1765 7.2. Future research adolescents may be useful in longitudinal research; the longer time 1830

lags between testing sessions should mean that habituation is less 1831

1766 It has been suggested that analysis of participants’ performance likely. 1832

1767 during the TSST could generate informative data, which may be In line with the reduction in HPA axis response to stress after 1833

1768 linked to the physiological response to the TSST (Williams et al., administration of the serotonin precursor tryptophan (Firk and 1834

1769 2004). Cognitive performance has typically been assessed follow- Markus, 2009) and enhanced sAA response following depletion of 1835

1770 ing the TSST, although performance on the mental arithmetic task tryptophan (van Veen et al., 2009), further research could estab- 1836

1771 itself has been measured; this did not indicate a significant corre- lish if SSRI’s can attenuate the HPA axis response to the TSST. As 1837

1772 lation between the lowest number reached and salivary cortisol in serotonin can act directly on the adrenal gland (Dinan, 1996), sero- 1838

1773 healthy volunteers (Simeon et al., 2007b). It is possible that per- tonin may play a role in HPA axis responses to the TSST. However, 1839

1774 formance on the maths tasks may affect other psychophysiological research using simulated public speaking has not found an effect 1840

1775 factors; given the transient effect of TSST stress on heart rate in on anxiety of tryptophan depletion, a method which reduces avail- 1841

1776 particular, such a method could be useful for assessing if heart able tryptophan (a serotonin precursor) by administering amino 1842

1777 rate changes are associated with performance on the maths task. acids which compete for access through the blood–brain barrier 1843

1778 Quality of performance on the speech task may be more difficult (Monteiro-dos-Santos et al., 2000; Shansis et al., 2000). A problem 1844

1779 to assess reliably than mathematical performance, although future with measuring tryptophan alone when studying serotonin effects 1845

1780 research could use a method such as the consensual assessment is that the large majority of tryptophan is catabolised along the 1846

1781 technique (Amabile, 1982) with experienced judges. Furthermore, kynurenine pathway without being converted to serotonin, and this 1847

1782 non-verbal behaviour during the TSST has been analysed using an alternative metabolic pathway also generates neuroactive metabo- 1848

1783 ethological scoring system developed by Troisi (1999); assertive lites (Bender, 1986; Van Donkelaar et al., 2011); measurements of 1849

1784 behaviours were associated with higher cortisol before and lower tryptophan/kynurenine ratios and downstream metabolites (e.g. 1850

1785 cortisol after the speech task, while submissive behaviours were kynurenic acid) may thus be useful. Serotonin effects may be mod- 1851

1786 associated with lower noradrenaline throughout the procedure ulated by individual differences at a genetic level; those with the 1852

1787 (Makatsori et al., 2004). Research using the same ethological scor- short/short genotype of the serotonin transporter gene have the 1853

1788 ing system has revealed that displacement behaviours (activities strongest cortisol response to the TSST (Gotlib et al., 2008; Way 1854

1789 such as face scratching or lip biting) are associated with reduced and Taylor, 2010) and clearer reduction by tryptophan adminis- 1855

1790 stress, but only in males (Mohiyeddini et al., 2013a), that displace- tration in HPA axis response to the TSST (Cerit et al., 2013), as 1856

1791 ment behaviour in men can mediate the relationship between state well as higher serum IL-6 and IL-10 at baseline and during stressor 1857

1792 anxiety and stress (Mohiyeddini and Semple, 2013), and that higher (Fredericks et al., 2010). Micro RNAs (mi-RNAs) are a potential ther- 1858

1793 levels of displacement behaviour can actually increase stress in apeutic target for the treatment of anxiety and depression effectors 1859

1794 females with high public self-consciousness (Mohiyeddini et al., (O’Connor et al., 2011); preclinical research has indicated that both 1860

1795 2013b). The lack of measurement of performance and behaviour on acute and chronic stress can produce region-specific changes in 1861

1796 the speech and mental arithmetic tasks during most TSST research miRNA levels (Meerson et al., 2010; Rinaldi et al., 2010). Genetic 1862

1797 ignores a large potential source of individual variability. research employing miRNAs combined with the TSST may give 1863

1798 More work needs to be conducted on modified versions of the insight into genetic factors underpinning sustained response to 1864

1799 TSST. Direct comparison of different versions of the TSST (e.g. TSST acute psychosocial stress. Early-life stress may be crucial in this 1865

1800 and TSST-M) within a single study would be useful in evaluating research area, not only because it can predispose individual to 1866

1801 the key aspects of the test that induce different psychological and stress-related disorders, but also because early-life stress has been 1867

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

22 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Table 3
Comparison of different methods of measuring stress/HPA axis responses in humans.

Method Description Associated effects Advantages Disadvantages

Trier social stress test Participants perform a public ↑ HPA axis activity Induces reliable, moderate Labour intensive
speech task and a mental ↑ Pro-inflammatory increase of HPA axis HPA axis habituates to
arithmetic task in front of an response activity current methods of
unresponsive audience ↑ Cardiovascular arousal Psychosocial nature allows repeated exposure
↑ Sympathetic-adrenal- for flexibility in design (e.g.
medullary system activity modifications such as
↑ Subjective stress positive versus negative
Variable effects on feedback)
cognitive performance
More research required on
gastrointestinal effects

Dexamethasone 1 mg of dexamethasone is ↓ Cortisol secretion Useful biomarker for Pharmacological


suppression test administered at night and ↑ Catecholamines (in melancholic episodes intervention
cortisol examined during the depression) Useful for assessing clinical Requires administration of
following day DEX/CRH: ↓ recovery dexamethasone and
Pro-inflammatory response within-participant assessment on different
Low dose: Lower HPA axis days
response in depression
with childhood trauma and
older trauma survivors
with PTSD

CO2 challenge Participants take maximal vital ↑ Plasma/serum cortisol Only one experimenter Inconsistent HPA axis
capacity inhalation of carbon ↑ Systolic BP required effects
dioxide-enriched air ↓ Heart rate Useful for panic disorder Need to exclude vulnerable
↑ Subjective anxiety research participants (e.g. those
with cardiovascular or
respiratory disorders)

Cold pressor test Participants immerse their ↑ HPA axis activity (smaller Only one experimenter HPA axis activation smaller
hand in ice-cold water than TSST) required than TSST
↓ Pro-inflammatory A social-evaluative Can be painful for
response element can be added participants
↑ Subjective stress

Cortisol awakening Samples collected to measure ↑ HPA axis activity Established as suitable for Reliance on participant
response HPA axis response to ↑ sIga study in field settings compliance in collecting
awakening in the morning ↑ Cardiovascular activity In non-clinical samples, samples if not conducted in
this response may nearly sleep laboratory
always be present

Cognitive tasks A variety of cognitive tasks are Variable, depending on Only one experimenter Unlikely to induce HPA axis
used to induce performance tasks employed required effects as large as those
stress, such as multitasking Cognitive performance induced by the TSST
frameworks or Stroop test measures intrinsic to task
Flexibility in altering stress
intensity

Noise Exposure to loud, unpleasant ↑ sAA Established as suitable for Unlikely to induce HPA axis
sound; can be manipulated in ↑ Noradrenaline study in field settings or subjective effects as
the laboratory or assessed in ↑ Heart rate variability consistently as the TSST
field setting

Mannheim Requires mathematical ↑ HPA axis activity Cognitive performance Less established than older
multicomponent performance under conditions ↑ Cardiovascular arousal measure intrinsic to task measures; further research
stress test of noise, imagery-based ↑ Subjective stress Flexibility in altering stress required to ascertain
negative emotional induction intensity effects
and threat of reduced payment Care needs to taken not to
if performance is poor confound stress with other
states induced by
emotional induction

1868 shown to interact with the short allele of the 5-HTTLPR in TSST effects on multiple biomarkers (see Fig. 3) and other measures 1878

1869 response in young adults (Mueller et al., 2011). have their own advantages, such as consistently responsiveness to 1879

repeated exposure to the TSST or the ability to measure continu- 1880

1870 7.3. Summary and conclusions ously during the mental arithmetic and speech tasks (see Table 1). 1881

The TSST also consistently induces subjective stress, although this 1882

1871 The TSST has become one of the most widely used means of will not necessarily be correlated with physiological outcomes, and 1883

1872 experimentally inducing acute psychosocial stress. Salivary corti- further work remains to be done on the cognitive effects of the 1884

1873 sol is a particularly useful marker of the stress response using this TSST. Given the relative advantages and disadvantages between 1885

1874 method, due to extensive evidence for its response to the TSST, measures, the possibility of interactions between them and the 1886

1875 the fact that as a marker of HPA axis activity it is selectively acti- likelihood that the timing of their effects will differ, it is impor- 1887

1876 vated by stress, rather than arousal in general, as well as the ease of tant to collect multiple biomarker outputs, as well as subjective 1888

1877 collection of saliva compared to blood. However, the TSST induces measures. 1889

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 23

Fig. 3. Psychophysiological effects of the TSST. The TSST increases ACTH (e.g. Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2007; Munro et al., 2005) and salivary, serum, and plasma cortisol
(e.g. Gaab et al., 2002; Kirschbaum et al., 1993, 1999), as well as DHEA and DHEA-S (Lennartsson et al., 2012b). At a subjective and behavioural level, the TSST induces
heightened subjective stress (e.g. Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2002b; Sugaya et al., 2012), and can alter memory performance, although this latter effect can be moderated
by stimulus valence (e.g. Domes et al., 2004; Jelici et al., 2004). Complex cognition has been shown to be moderated by panel behaviour (e.g. Akinola and Mendes, 2008;
Kassam et al., 2009). The variable effects of the TSST on cognition suggest that further research using the TSST may provide insight into the complex relationship between
acute stress and cognition. The TSST increases heart rate (e.g. Jezova et al., 2004; Rimmele et al., 2007, 2009) and systolic blood pressure (e.g. Gerra et al., 2001; Jezova
et al., 2004), although variable results have been observed for heart rate variability (e.g. Lackschewitz et al., 2008; Rohleder et al., 2006). The TSST also increases adrenaline
(Gold et al., 2004; Jezova et al., 2004), noradrenaline (Schommer et al., 2003), salivary ␣-amylase (Gordis et al., 2006; Het et al., 2009) and galvanic skin response (Jezova
et al., 2004; Rohrmann et al., 1999). The HPA axis effects of the TSST can in turn lead to a heightened pro-inflammatory response, visible in the activity of cytokines such
as IL-6 (e.g. Slavich et al., 2010; von Känel et al., 2006), IL-2 (Bellingrath et al., 2010) and IL-1␤ (Yamakawa et al., 2009). There is preliminary evidence to suggest that this
stress response may also alter gastrointestinal activity, as ghrelin response has been altered in bulimia nervosa (Monteleone et al., 2012) and HPA axis responses have been
altered in irritable bowel syndrome (Kennedy et al., 2013; Suárez-Hitz et al., 2012). However, further research on the effects of the TSST on the brain–gut axis is required.
CNS, central nervous system; HRV, heart rate variability; HPA, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal; ACTH, adrenocorticotropic hormone; DHEA, dehydroepiandrosterone; SAM,
sympathetic-adrenal-medullary; sAA, saliva ␣-amylase; GSR, galvanic skin response.

1890 Anxiety disorders may moderate TSST effects, although this can consistently high HPA axis responses in a majority of participants. 1905

1891 vary depending on the specific anxiety disorder. The research on Grouped assessment and the use of virtual committees may reduce 1906

1892 TSST effects in depression is equivocal, perhaps due to age differ- the time commitment of confederates. The current evidence sug- 1907

1893 ences between samples in different studies. There is preliminary gests that while grouped assessment is successful in inducing 1908

1894 evidence that psychological and pharmacological therapies can stress, virtual committees may need further development before 1909

1895 alter stress responses to the TSST, although HPA axis activity should they can be treated as being as reliable as live committees. Such 1910

1896 be investigated further in this area. Genetic research as well as methods could be very useful in using the TSST to further the study 1911

1897 studies investigating tryptophan loading and SSRI use suggest that of acute stress within cognitive neuroscience. In addition to the 1912

1898 serotonin may play a role in the modulation of stress by individual TSST, a number of new paradigms are emerging which combine 1913

1899 differences and in stress-related disorders. different types of stressor in order to induce a psychophysiologi- 1914

1900 Although the “classic” TSST involves testing adult participants cal stress response, such as the Maastricht Acute Stress Test and the 1915

1901 individually using a live committee, modifications are frequently Mannheim Multicomponent Stress Test; future research will estab- 1916

1902 made to the TSST. Control versions of the TSST allow for a more com- lish the usefulness of these new methodologies in furthering the 1917

1903 plete assessment of psychosocial stress effects, although repeated study of acute stress. Until they are adequately validated, the TSST 1918

1904 measures versions may require further modification to induce remains one of the most important tools available to researchers 1919

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

24 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

1920 in this area and its continued use in conjunction with a more con- Bender, D.A., 1986. The relative importance of dietary tryptophan and performed 1997

1921 sidered selection of biomarkers from those evaluated above will be nicotinic acid and nicotinamide as precursors of nicotinamide nucleotide coen- 1998
zymes. In: Bender, D.A., Joseph, M.H., Kochen, W., Steinhart, H.W. (Eds.), Progress 1999
1922 key to further advances. in Tryptophan and Serotonin Research. de Gruyter, Berlin, pp. 159–164. 2000
Bengtsson, J., Persson Waye, K., Kjellberg, A., 2004. Evaluations of effects due to 2001
low-frequency noise in a low demanding work situation. J. Sound Vibrat. 278, 2002
1923 Acknowledgements 83–99. 2003
Bhagwagar, Z., Hafizi, S., Cowen, P.J., 2003. Increase in concentration of waking 2004
1924 The Alimentary Pharmabiotic Centre is a research centre funded salivary cortisol in recovered patients with depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 2005
1890–1891. 2006
1925 by Science Foundation Ireland (SFI), through the Irish Govern- Bhagwagar, Z., Hafizi, S., Cowen, P.J., 2005. Increased salivary cortisol after waking 2007
1926 ment’s National Development Plan (NDP). The authors and their in depression. Psychopharmacol. 182, 54–57. 2008
1927 work were supported by SFI (Grant Nos. 12/RC/2273, 02/CE/B124 Bijkerk, C.J., de Wit, N.J., Muris, J.W.M., Jones, R.H., Knottnerus, J.A., Hoes, A.W., 2003. 2009
Outcome measures in irritable bowel syndrome: comparison of psychometric 2010
1928 and 07/CE/B1368). G.C., J.F.C. and T.D. are also funded by the Irish
and methodological characteristics. Am. J. Gastroenterol. 98, 122–127. 2011
1929 Health Research Board (HRB) Health Research Awards (Grant No. Bishop, N.C., Gleeson, M., 2009. Acute and chronic effects of exercise on markers of 2012
1930 HRA POR/2011/23). mucosal immunity. Front. Biosci. 14, 4444–4456. 2013
Blair, J., Lancaster, G., Titman, A., Peak, M., Newlands, P., Collingwood, C., Chesters, 2014
C., Moorcroft, T., Wallin, N., Hawcutt, D., Gardner, C., Didi, M., Lacy, D., Couriel, 2015
1931 References J., 2013. Early morning salivary cortisol and cortisone, and adrenal responses 2016
to a simplified low-dose short Synacthen test in children with asthma. Clin. 2017

1932 Abdu, T.A.M., Elhadd, T.A., Neary, R., Clayton, R.N., 1999. Comparison of the low dose Endocrinol., http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cen.12302. 2018

1933 short synacthen test (1 ␮g), the conventional dose short synacthen test (250 ␮g), Bouma, E., Riese, H., Nederhof, E., Ormel, J., Oldehinkel, A., 2010. No replication 2019

1934 and the insulin tolerance test for assessment of the hypothalamo-pituitary- of genotype effect of 5-HTTLPR on cortisol response to social stress in larger 2020

1935 adrenal axis in patients with pituitary disease. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 84, adolescent sample. Biol. Psychiatry 68, e33–e34. 2021

1936 838–843. Bradesi, S., Mayer, E.A., 2009. Experimental models of stress and pain: do they help 2022

1937 Akinola, M., Mendes, W.B., 2008. The dark side of creativity: biological vulnerability to develop new therapies? Dig. Dis. 27 (Suppl. 1), 55–67. 2023

1938 and negative emotions lead to greater artistic creativity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. Brandtzaeg, P.E.R., 2007. Do salivary antibodies reliably reflect both mucosal and 2024

1939 34, 1677–1686. systemic immunity? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1098, 288–311. 2025

1940 Ali, N., Pruessner, J.C., 2012. The salivary alpha amylase over cortisol ratio as a marker Britton, W.B., Shahar, B., Szepsenwol, O., Jacobs, W.J., 2012. Mindfulness-based cog- 2026

1941 to assess dysregulations of the stress systems. Physiol. Behav. 106, 65–72. nitive therapy improves emotional reactivity to social stress: results from a 2027

1942 Almela, M., Hidalgo, V., Villada, C., Espín, L., Gómez-Amor, J., Salvador, A., 2011a. randomized controlled trial. Behav. Ther. 43, 365–380. 2028

1943 The impact of cortisol reactivity to acute stress on memory: sex differences in Brosschot, J.F., 2010. Markers of chronic stress: prolonged physiological activation 2029

1944 middle-aged people. Stress 14, 117–127. and (un) conscious perseverative cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 46. 2030

1945 Almela, M., Hidalgo, V., Villada, C., van der Meij, L., Espín, L., Gómez-Amor, J., Salvador, Brown, K.W., Weinstein, N., Creswell, J.D., 2012. Trait mindfulness modulates 2031

1946 A., 2011b. Salivary alpha-amylase response to acute psychosocial stress: the neuroendocrine and affective responses to social evaluative threat. Psychoneu- 2032

1947 impact of age. Biol. Psychol. 87, 421–429. roendocrinology 37, 2037–2041. 2033

1948 Altemus, M., Redwine, L.S., Leong, Y., Frye, C.A., Porges, S.W., Carter, C.S., 2001. Browne, C.A., Clarke, G., Dinan, T.G., Cryan, J.F., 2011. Differential stress-induced 2034

1949 Responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in postpartum women. Psychosom. alterations in tryptophan hydroxylase activity and serotonin turnover in two 2035

1950 Med. 63, 814–821. inbred mouse strains. Neuropharmacology 60, 683–691. 2036

1951 Amabile, T.M., 1982. Social psychology of creativity: a consensual assessment tech- Brüne, M., Nadolny, N., Güntürkün, O., Wolf, O.T., 2012. Stress induces a functional 2037

1952 nique. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 43, 997–1013. asymmetry in an emotional attention task. Cogn. Emot. 27, 558–566. 2038

1953 Andrews, J., Pruessner, J.C., 2013. The combined propranolol/TSST paradigm—a new Buchanan, T.W., Lovallo, W.R., 2001. Enhanced memory for emotional material fol- 2039

1954 method for psychoneuroendocrinology. PLoS ONE 8, e57567. lowing stress-level cortisol treatment in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2040

1955 Andrews, J., D’Aguiar, C., Pruessner, J.C., 2012. The combined dexamethasone/TSST 26, 307–317. 2041

1956 paradigm—a new method for psychoneuroendocrinology. PLoS ONE 7, e38994. Buchanan, T.W., Tranel, D., 2008. Stress and emotional memory retrieval: effects of 2042

1957 Ansell, E.B., Rando, K., Tuit, K., Guarnaccia, J., Sinha, R., 2012. Cumulative adversity sex and cortisol response. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 89, 134. 2043

1958 and smaller gray matter volume in medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and Buchanan, T.W., Tranel, D., Kirschbaum, C., 2009. Hippocampal damage abolishes 2044

1959 insula regions. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 57–64. the cortisol response to psychosocial stress in humans. Horm. Behav. 56, 2045

1960 Armbruster, D., Mueller, A., Moser, D., Lesch, K., Brocke, B., Kirschbaum, C., 2009. 44–50. 2046

1961 Interaction effect of D4 dopamine receptor gene and serotonin transporter pro- Buchanan, T.W., Driscoll, D., Mowrer, S.M., Sollers Iii, J.J., Thayer, J.F., Kirschbaum, 2047

1962 moter polymorphism on the cortisol stress response. Behav. Neurosci. 123, C., Tranel, D., 2010. Medial prefrontal cortex damage affects physiological and 2048

1963 1288. psychological stress responses differently in men and women. Psychoneuroen- 2049

1964 Armbruster, D., Mueller, A., Strobel, A., Lesch, K., Brocke, B., Kirschbaum, C., 2012. docrinology 35, 56–66. 2050

1965 Children under stress—COMT genotype and stressful life events predict cortisol Buckert, M., Kudielka, B.M., Reuter, M., Fiebach, C.J., 2012. The COMT Val158Met 2051

1966 increase in an acute social stress paradigm. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 15, polymorphism modulates working memory performance under acute stress. 2052

1967 1229–1239. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1810–1821. 2053

1968 Babisch, W., Fromme, H., Beyer, A., Ising, H., 2001. Increased catecholamine levels Burke, H.M., Davis, M.C., Otte, C., Mohr, D.C., 2005. Depression and cortisol 2054

1969 in urine in subjects exposed to road traffic noise: the role of stress hormones in responses to psychological stress: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2055

1970 noise research. Environ. Int. 26, 475–481. 30, 846–856. 2056

1971 Bagley, S.L., Weaver, T.L., Buchanan, T.W., 2011. Sex differences in physiological Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Geiben, A., Höllig, H., Morschhäuser, E., Hellhammer, D., 2057

1972 and affective responses to stress in remitted depression. Physiol. Behav. 104, 2002a. Altered responsiveness of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and 2058

1973 180–186. the sympathetic adrenomedullary system to stress in patients with atopic der- 2059

1974 Balodis, I.M., Wynne-Edwards, K.E., Olmstead, M.C., 2010. The other side of the curve: matitis. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 87, 4245–4251. 2060

1975 examining the relationship between pre-stressor physiological responses and Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Geirens, A., Höllig, H., Hellhammer, D.H., 2002b. Stress- 2061

1976 stress reactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1363–1373. induced immunomodulation is altered in patients with atopic dermatitis. J. 2062

1977 Bandura, A., 1977. Social Learning Theory. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Neuroimmunol. 129, 161–167. 2063

1978 Barnes, R.F., Veith, R.C., Borson, S., Verhey, J., Raskind, M.A., Halter, J.B., 1983. Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Jobst, S., Wustmans, A., Kirschbaum, C., Rauh, W., Hellham- 2064

1979 High levels of plasma catecholamines in dexamethasone-resistant depressed mer, D.H., 1997. Attenuated free cortisol response to psychosocial stress in 2065

1980 patients. Am. J. Psychiatry 140, 1623–1625. children with atopic dermatitis. Psychosom. Med. 59, 419–426. 2066

1981 Batten, S.V., Aslan, M., Maciejewski, P.K., Mazure, C.M., 2004. Childhood maltreat- Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Kern, S., Ebrecht, M., Hellhammer, D.H., 2007. Altered dis- 2067

1982 ment as a risk factor for adult cardiovascular disease and depression. J. Clin. tribution of leukocyte subsets and cytokine production in response to acute 2068

1983 Psychiatry 65, 249–254. psychosocial stress in patients with psoriasis vulgaris. Brain Behav. Immun. 21, 2069

1984 Beato, M.S., Cadavid, S., Pulido, R.F., Pinho, M.S., 2013. No effect of stress on false 92–99. 2070

1985 recognition. Psicothema 25, 25–30. Buynitsky, T., Mostofsky, D.I., 2009. Restraint stress in biobehavioral research: recent 2071

1986 Beetz, A., Julius, H., Turner, D., Kotrschal, K., 2012. Effects of social support by a dog developments. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 1089–1098. 2072

1987 on stress modulation in male children with insecure attachment. Front. Psychol. Campbell, J., Ehlert, U., 2012. Acute psychosocial stress: does the emotional stress 2073

1988 3, 352. response correspond with physiological responses? Psychoneuroendocrinology 2074

1989 Bellingrath, S., Rohleder, N., Kudielka, B.M., 2010. Healthy working school teachers 37, 1111–1134. 2075

1990 with high effort-reward-imbalance and overcommitment show increased pro- Campisi, J., Bravo, Y., Cole, J., Gobeil, K., 2012. Acute psychosocial stress differentially 2076

1991 inflammatory immune activity and a dampened innate immune defence. Brain influences salivary endocrine and immune measures in undergraduate students. 2077

1992 Behav. Immun. 24, 1332–1339. Physiol. Behav. 107, 317–321. 2078

1993 Bellingrath, S., Rohleder, N., Kudielka, B.M., 2013. Effort-reward-imbalance in Capuron, L., Miller, A.H., 2004. Cytokines and psychopathology: lessons from 2079

1994 healthy teachers is associated with higher LPS-stimulated production and interferon-␣. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 819–824. 2080

1995 lower glucocorticoid sensitivity of interleukin-6 in vitro. Biol. Psychol. 92, Capuron, L., Miller, A.H., 2011. Immune system to brain signaling: neuropsychophar- 2081

1996 403–409. macological implications. Pharmacol. Ther. 130, 226–238. 2082

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 25

2083 Carney, R.M., Freedland, K.E., Veith, R.C., 2005. Depression, the autonomic nervous Derry, H.M., Fagundes, C.P., Andridge, R., Glaser, R., Malarkey, W.B., Kiecolt-Glaser, 2169
2084 system, and coronary heart disease. Psychosom. Med. 67, S29–S33. J.K., 2013. Lower subjective social status exaggerates interleukin-6 responses 2170
2085 Carpenter, L.L., Carvalho, J.P., Tyrka, A.R., Wier, L.M., Mello, A.F., Mello, M.F., Ander- to a laboratory stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ 2171
2086 son, G.M., Wilkinson, C.W., Price, L.H., 2007. Decreased adrenocorticotropic j.psyneuen.2013.06.026. 2172
2087 hormone and cortisol responses to stress in healthy adults reporting significant Dickerson, S.S., Kemeny, M.E., 2004. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a the- 2173
2088 childhood maltreatment. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 1080–1087. oretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol. Bull. 130, 2174
2089 Carpenter, L.L., Gawuga, C.E., Tyrka, A.R., Lee, J.K., Anderson, G.M., Price, L.H., 2010. 355–391. 2175
2090 Association between plasma IL-6 response to acute stress and early-life adver- Dickerson, S.S., Mycek, P.J., Zaldivar, F., 2008. Negative social evaluation, but not mere 2176
2091 sity in healthy adults. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 2617–2623. social presence, elicits cortisol responses to a laboratory stressor task. Health 2177
2092 Carroll, B.J., 1982. The dexamethasone suppression test for melancholia. Br. J. Psy- Psychol. 27, 116–121. 2178
2093 chiatry 140, 292–304. Dienes, K.A., Hazel, N.A., Hammen, C.L., 2013. Cortisol secretion in depressed, and 2179
2094 Carroll, B.J., Curtis, G.C., Mendels, J., 1976. Neuroendocrine regulation in depres- at-risk adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 927–940. 2180
2095 sion: II. Discrimination of depressed from nondepressed patients. Arch. Gen. Dinan, T.G., 1996. Serotonin and the regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal 2181
2096 Psychiatry 33, 1051. axis function. Life Sci. 58, 1683–1694. 2182
2097 Cerit, H., Jans, L.A.W., Van der Does, W., 2013. The effect of tryptophan on the cortisol Dinan, T.G., 2005. Stress: the shared common component in major mental illnesses. 2183
2098 response to social stress is modulated by the 5-HTTLPR genotype. Psychoneu- Eur. Psychiatry 20 (Suppl. 3), S326–S328. 2184
2099 roendocrinology 38, 201–208. Dinan, T.G., Scott, L.V., 2005. Anatomy of melancholia: focus on hypothalamic- 2185
2100 Chatkoff, D.K., Maier, K.J., Klein, C., 2010. Nonlinear associations between chronic pituitary-adrenal axis overactivity and the role of vasopressin. J. Anat. 207, 2186
2101 stress and cardiovascular reactivity and recovery. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 77, 259–264. 2187
2102 150–156. Dinan, T.G., Cryan, J., Shanahan, F., Keeling, P.N., Quigley, E.M., 2010. IBS: an epige- 2188
2103 Checkley, S., 1992. Neuroendocrine mechanisms and the precipitation of depression netic perspective. Nat. Rev. Gastroenterol. Hepatol. 7, 465–471. 2189
2104 by life events. Br. J. Psychiatry 160, 7–17. Ditzen, B., Schmidt, S., Strauss, B., Nater, U.M., Ehlert, U., Heinrichs, M., 2008. Adult 2190
2105 Childs, E., de Wit, H., 2010. Effects of acute psychosocial stress on cigarette craving attachment and social support interact to reduce psychological but not cortisol 2191
2106 and smoking. Nicotine Tobacco Res. 12, 449–453. responses to stress. J. Psychosom. Res. 64, 479–486. 2192
2107 Childs, E., Vicini, L.M., De Wit, H., 2006. Responses to the Trier Social Stress Dockray, S., Bhattacharyya, M.R., Molloy, G.J., Steptoe, A., 2008. The cortisol awaken- 2193
2108 Test (TSST) in single versus grouped participants. Psychophysiology 43, ing response in relation to objective and subjective measures of waking in the 2194
2109 366–371. morning. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 77. 2195
2110 Childs, E., O’Connor, S., de Wit, H., 2011. Bidirectional interactions between acute Dockray, S., Susman, E.J., Dorn, L.D., 2009. Depression, cortisol reactivity, and obesity 2196
2111 psychosocial stress and acute intravenous alcohol in healthy men. Alcohol.: Clin. in childhood and adolescence. J. Adolesc. Health 45, 344–350. 2197
2112 Exp. Res. 35, 1794–1803. Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Reichwald, U., Hautzinger, M., 2002. Hypothalamic- 2198
2113 Chopra, K.K., Ravindran, A., Kennedy, S.H., Mackenzie, B., Matthews, S., Anisman, pituitary-adrenal axis reactivity to psychological stress and memory in 2199
2114 H., Bagby, R.M., Farvolden, P., Levitan, R.D., 2009. Sex differences in hormonal middle-aged women: high responders exhibit enhanced declarative memory 2200
2115 responses to a social stressor in chronic major depression. Psychoneuroen- performance. Psychoneuroendocrinology 27, 843–853. 2201
2116 docrinology 34, 1235–1241. Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Rimmele, U., Reichwald, U., Hautzinger, M., 2004. Acute 2202
2117 Clark, P., Neylon, I., Raggatt, P., Sheppard, M., Stewart, P., 1998. Defining the normal stress impairs recognition for positive words—association with stress-induced 2203
2118 cortisol response to the short Synacthen test: implications for the investigation cortisol secretion. Stress 7, 173–181. 2204
2119 of hypothalamic-pituitary disorders. Clin. Endocrinol. 49, 287–292. Dorn, L.D., Campo, J.C., Thato, S., Dahl, R.E., Lewin, D., Chandra, R., Di Lorenzo, C., 2205
2120 Clow, A., 2001. The physiology of stress. In: Jones, F., Bright, J. (Eds.), Stress: Myth, 2003. Psychological comorbidity and stress reactivity in children and adoles- 2206
2121 Theory and Research. Pearson, Essex, UK, pp. 47–61. cents with recurrent abdominal pain and anxiety disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child 2207
2122 Cornelisse, S., Joels, M., Smeets, T., 2011a. A randomized trial on mineralocorticoid Adolesc. Psychiatry 42, 66–75. 2208
2123 receptor blockade in men: effects on stress responses, selective attention, and Duchesne, A., Tessera, E., Dedovic, K., Engert, V., Pruessner, J.C., 2012. Effects of panel 2209
2124 memory. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 2720–2728. sex composition on the physiological stress responses to psychosocial stress in 2210
2125 Cornelisse, S., van Stegeren, A.H., Joëls, M., 2011b. Implications of psychosocial healthy young men and women. Biol. Psychol. 89, 99–106. 2211
2126 stress on memory formation in a typical male versus female student sample. Dumas, J.A., Albert, K.M., Naylor, M.R., Sites, C.K., Benkelfat, C., Newhouse, P.A., 2012. 2212
2127 Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 569–578. The effects of age and estrogen on stress responsivity in older women. Am. J. 2213
2128 Corwin, E.J., Guo, Y., Pajer, K., Lowe, N., McCarthy, D., Schmiege, S., Weber, M., Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 734–743. 2214
2129 Pace, T., Stafford, B., 2013. Immune dysregulation and glucocorticoid resis- Dyson, K.S., Shoemaker, J.K., Hughson, R.L., 2006. Effect of acute sympathetic nervous 2215
2130 tance in minority and low income pregnant women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, system activation on flow-mediated dilation of brachial artery. Am. J. Physiol. 2216
2131 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.02.015. Heart Circ. Physiol. 290, H1446–H1453. 2217
2132 Coryell, W., Schlesser, M., 2001. The dexamethasone suppression test and suicide Echterhoff, G., Wolf, O.T., 2012. The stressed eyewitness: the interaction of the- 2218
2133 prediction. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 748–753. matic arousal and post-event stress in memory for central and peripheral event 2219
2134 Cruz-Almeida, Y., King, C.D., Wallet, S.M., Riley, J.L., 2012. Immune biomarker information. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6, 57. 2220
2135 response depends on choice of experimental pain stimulus in healthy adults: Edelman, S., Shalev, I., Uzefovsky, F., Israel, S., Knafo, A., Kremer, I., Mankuta, D., Kaitz, 2221
2136 a preliminary study. Pain Res. Treat. 2012. M., Ebstein, R.P., 2012. Epigenetic and genetic factors predict women’s salivary 2222
2137 Cryan, J.F., Dinan, T.G., 2012. Mind-altering microorganisms: the impact of the gut cortisol following a threat to the social self. PLoS ONE 7, e48597. 2223
2138 microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 701–712. Eisenberger, N.I., Taylor, S.E., Gable, S.L., Hilmert, C.J., Lieberman, M.D., 2007. Neural 2224
2139 Cryan, J.F., Kelly, P.H., Chaperon, F., Gentsch, C., Mombereau, C., Lingen- pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. 2225
2140 hoehl, K., Froestl, W., Bettler, B., Kaupmann, K., Spooren, W.P., 2004. Neuroimage 35, 1601–1612. 2226
2141 Behavioral characterization of the novel GABAB receptor-positive modu- El-Sheikh, M., Erath, S.A., Buckhalt, J.A., Granger, D.A., Mize, J., 2008. Cortisol and chil- 2227
2142 lator GS39783 (N,N -dicyclopentyl-2-methylsulfanyl-5-nitro-pyrimidine-4,6- dren’s adjustment: the moderating role of sympathetic nervous system activity. 2228
2143 diamine): anxiolytic-like activity without side effects associated with baclofen J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 36, 601–611. 2229
2144 or benzodiazepines. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 310, 952–963. Ellenbogen, M., Carson, R., Pishva, R., 2010. Automatic emotional information pro- 2230
2145 Czyrak, A., Chocyk, A., 2001. Search for the presence of glucocorticoid receptors in cessing and the cortisol response to acute psychosocial stress. Cogn. Affect. 2231
2146 dopaminergic neurons of rat ventral tegmenal area and substantia nigra. Pol. J. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 71–82. 2232
2147 Pharmacol. 53, 681–684. Elzinga, B.M., Roelofs, K., 2005. Cortisol-induced impairments of working memory 2233
2148 Davey, H.M., Barratt, A.L., Butow, P.N., Deeks, J.J., 2007. A one-item question with require acute sympathetic activation. Behav. Neurosci. 119, 98–103. 2234
2149 a Likert or Visual Analogue Scale adequately measured current anxiety. J. Clin. Engert, V., Efanov, S.I., Duchesne, A., Vogel, S., Corbo, V., Pruessner, J.C., 2013. Dif- 2235
2150 Epidemiol. 60, 356–360. ferentiating anticipatory from reactive cortisol responses to psychosocial stress. Q3 2236
2151 De Boer, S.F., Koopmans, S.J., Slangen, J.L., Van Der Gugten, J., 1990. Plasma Psychoneuroendocrinology 38. 2237
2152 catecholamine, corticosterone and glucose responses to repeated Espin, L., Almela, M., Hidalgo, V., Villada, C., Salvador, A., Gomez-Amor, J., 2013. Acute 2238
2153 stress in rats: Effect of interstressor interval length. Physiol. Behav. 47, pre-learning stress and declarative memory: impact of sex, cortisol response and 2239
2154 1117–1124. menstrual cycle phase. Horm. Behav. 63, 759–765. 2240
2155 Dedovic, K., Renwick, R., Mahani, N.K., Engert, V., Lupien, S.J., Pruessner, J.C., 2005. Fagundes, C.P., Glaser, R., Hwang, B.S., Malarkey, W.B., Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K., 2013. 2241
2156 The Montreal Imaging Stress Task: using functional imaging to investigate the Depressive symptoms enhance stress-induced inflammatory responses. Brain 2242
2157 effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. J. Behav. Immun. 31, 172–176. 2243
2158 Psychiatry Neurosci. 30, 319–325. Fernandez-Botran, R., Miller, J.J., Burns, V.E., Newton, T.L., 2011. Correlations among 2244
2159 Dedovic, K., Rexroth, M., Wolff, E., Duchesne, A., Scherling, C., Beaudry, T., Lue, S.D., inflammatory markers in plasma, saliva and oral mucosal transudate in post- 2245
2160 Lord, C., Engert, V., Pruessner, J.C., 2009a. Neural correlates of processing stress- menopausal women with past intimate partner violence. Brain Behav. Immun. 2246
2161 ful information: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Res. 1293, 49–60. 25, 314–321. 2247
2162 Dedovic, K., Wadiwalla, M., Engert, V., Pruessner, J.C., 2009b. The role of sex and Fiocco, A.J., Joober, R., Lupien, S.J., 2007. Education modulates cortisol reactivity to 2248
2163 gender socialization in stress reactivity. Dev. Psychol. 45, 45–55. the Trier Social Stress Test in middle-aged adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2249
2164 Delaney, J.P.A., Brodie, D.A., 2000. Effects of short-term psychological stress on the 32, 1158–1163. 2250
2165 time and frequency domains of heart-rate variability. Percept. Motor Skills 91, Firk, C., Markus, C.R., 2009. Mood and cortisol responses following tryptophan-rich 2251
2166 515–524. hydrolyzed protein and acute stress in healthy subjects with high and low cog- 2252
2167 DeRijk, R.H., de Kloet, E.R., 2008. Corticosteroid receptor polymorphisms: determi- nitive reactivity to depression. Clin. Nutr. 28, 266–271. 2253
2168 nants of vulnerability and resilience. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 583, 303–311. Fisher, S., 1986. Stress and Strategy. Taylor & Francis, London. 2254

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

26 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

2255 Foley, P., Kirschbaum, C., 2010. Human hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis Hanoch, Y., Vitouch, O., 2004. When less is more: information, emotional arousal and 2341
2256 responses to acute psychosocial stress in laboratory settings. Neurosci. Biobehav. the ecological reframing of the Yerkes-Dodson law. Theory Psychol. 14, 427–452. 2342
2257 Rev. 35, 91–96. Harkness, K.L., Stewart, J.G., Wynne-Edwards, K.E., 2011. Cortisol reactivity to social 2343
2258 Fouladi, D., Nassiri, P., Monazzam, E., Farahani, S., Hassanzadeh, G., Hoseini, M., 2012. stress in adolescents: role of depression severity and child maltreatment. Psy- 2344
2259 Industrial noise exposure and salivary cortisol in blue collar industrial workers. choneuroendocrinology 36, 173–181. 2345
2260 Noise Health 14, 184. Heim, C., Newport, D.J., Heit, S., Graham, Y.P., Wilcox, M., Bonsall, R., Miller, A.H., 2346
2261 Francis, C., Morris, J., Whorwell, P., 1997. The irritable bowel severity scoring sys- Nemeroff, C.B., 2000. Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in 2347
2262 tem: a simple method of monitoring irritable bowel syndrome and its progress. women after sexual and physical abuse in childhood. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 284, 2348
2263 Aliment. Pharmacol. Ther. 11, 395–402. 592–597. 2349
2264 Francis, L.A., Granger, D.A., Susman, E.J., 2013. Adrenocortical regulation, eating in Heim, C., Mletzko, T., Purselle, D., Musselman, D.L., Nemeroff, C.B., 2008. The 2350
2265 the absence of hunger and BMI in young children. Appetite 64, 32–38. dexamethasone/corticotropin-releasing factor test in men with major depres- 2351
2266 Fredericks, C.A., Drabant, E.M., Edge, M.D., Tillie, J.M., Hallmayer, J., Ramel, W., Kuo, sion: role of childhood trauma. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 398–405. 2352
2267 J.R., Mackey, S., Gross, J.J., Dhabhar, F.S., 2010. Healthy young women with sero- Hellhammer, J., Schubert, M., 2012. The physiological response to Trier Social Stress 2353
2268 tonin transporter SS polymorphism show pro-inflammatory bias under resting Test relates to subjective measures of stress during but not before or after the 2354
2269 and stress conditions. Brain Behav. Immun. 24, 350. test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 119–124. 2355
2270 Fries, E., Hellhammer, D.H., Hellhammer, J., 2006. Attenuation of the hypothala- Heinrichs, M., Meinlschmidt, G., Neumann, I., Wagner, S., Kirschbaum, C., Ehlert, U., 2356
2271 mic-pituitary-adrenal axis responsivity to the Trier Social Stress Test by the Hellhammer, D.H., 2001. Effects of suckling on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal 2357
2272 benzodiazepine alprazolam. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 1278–1288. axis responses to psychosocial stress in postpartum lactating women. J. Clin. 2358
2273 Fries, E., Dettenborn, L., Kirschbaum, C., 2009. The cortisol awakening response Endocrinol. Metab. 86, 4798–4804. 2359
2274 (CAR): facts and future directions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 72, 67–73. Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., Ehlert, U., 2003. Social support and 2360
2275 Gaab, J., Hüster, D., Peisen, R., Engert, V., Heitz, V., Schad, T., Schürmeyer, T.H., Ehlert, oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial 2361
2276 U., 2002. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis reactivity in chronic fatigue syn- stress. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1389–1398. 2362
2277 drome and health under psychological, physiological, and pharmacological Hellhammer, D.H., Wüst, S., Kudielka, B.M., 2009. Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in 2363
2278 stimulation. Psychosom. Med. 64, 951–962. stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 163–171. 2364
2279 Gaab, J., Blattler, N., Menzi, T., Pabst, B., Stoyer, S., Ehlert, U., 2003. Randomized con- Hellhammer, J., Hero, T., Franz, N., Contreras, C., Schubert, M., 2012. Omega-3 fatty 2365
2280 trolled evaluation of the effects of cognitive-behavioral stress management on acids administered in phosphatidylserine improved certain aspects of high 2366
2281 cortisol responses to acute stress in healthy subjects. Psychoneuroendocrinol- chronic stress in men. Nutrition Research 32, 241–250. 2367
2282 ogy 28, 767–779. Hendrawan, D., Yamakawa, K., Kimura, M., Murakami, H., Ohira, H., 2012. Execu- 2368
2283 Gaab, J., Rohleder, N., Heitz, V., Engert, V., Schad, T., Schürmeyer, T.H., Ehlert, U., tive functioning performance predicts subjective and physiological acute stress 2369
2284 2005a. Stress-induced changes in LPS-induced pro-inflammatory cytokine pro- reactivity: preliminary results. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 84, 277–283. 2370
2285 duction in chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 599–610. Herman, J.P., Ostrander, M.M., Mueller, N.K., Figueiredo, H., 2005. Limbic system 2371
2286 Gaab, J., Rohleder, N., Nater, U., Ehlert, U., 2005b. Psychological determinants of the mechanisms of stress regulation: hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. 2372
2287 cortisol stress response: the role of anticipatory cognitive appraisal. Psychoneu- Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 29, 1201–1213. 2373
2288 roendocrinology 30, 599–610. Het, S., Rohleder, N., Schoofs, D., Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, O., 2009. Neuroendocrine and 2374
2289 Gerra, G., Zaimovic, A., Zambelli, U., Timpano, M., Reali, N., Bernasconi, S., Brambilla, psychometric evaluation of a placebo version of the ‘Trier Social Stress Test’. 2375
2290 F., 2000. Neuroendocrine responses to psychological stress in adolescents with Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1075–1086. 2376
2291 anxiety disorder. Neuropsychobiology 42, 82–92. Het, S., Schoofs, D., Rohleder, N., Wolf, O.T., 2012. Stress-induced cortisol level ele- 2377
2292 Gerra, G., Zaimovic, A., Mascetti, G., Gardini, S., Zambelli, U., Timpano, M., Raggi, vations are associated with reduced negative affect after stress: indications for 2378
2293 M., Brambilla, F., 2001. Neuroendocrine responses to experimentally-induced a mood-buffering cortisol effect. Psychosom. Med. 74, 23–32. 2379
2294 psychological stress in healthy humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 91–107. Heuser, I., Yassouridis, A., Holsboer, F., 1994. The combined dexamethasone/CRH 2380
2295 Gibbison, B., Angelini, G.D., Lightman, S.L., 2013. Dynamic output and control of the test: a refined laboratory test for psychiatric disorders. J. Psychiatr. Res. 28, 2381
2296 hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in critical illness and major surgery. Br. J. 341–356. 2382
2297 Anaesth., http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/bja/aet077. Hidalgo, V., Villada, C., Almela, M., Espín, L., Gómez-Amor, J., Salvador, A., 2012. 2383
2298 Gilissen, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., van Ijzendoorn, M.H., Linting, M., 2008. Enhancing effects of acute psychosocial stress on priming of non-declarative 2384
2299 Electrodermal reactivity during the Trier Social Stress Test for children: memory in healthy young adults. Stress 15, 329–338. 2385
2300 Interaction between the serotonin transporter polymorphism and children’s Hoge, E.A., Bui, E., Marques, L., Metcalf, C.A., Morris, L.K., Robinaugh, D.J., Wor- 2386
2301 attachment representation. Dev. Psychobiol. 50, 615–625. thington, J.J., Pollack, M.H., Simon, N.M., 2013. Randomized controlled trial of Q4 2387
2302 Gold, S.M., Zakowski, S.G., Valdimarsdottir, H.B., Bovbjerg, D.H., 2004. Higher Beck mindfulness meditation for generalized anxiety disorder: effects on anxiety and 2388
2303 depression scores predict delayed epinephrine recovery after acute psycholog- stress reactivity. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2389
2304 ical stress independent of baseline levels of stress and mood. Biol. Psychol. 67, Hoyer, J., Braeuer, D., Crawcour, S., Klumbies, E., Kirschbaum, C., 2013. Depersonal- 2390
2305 261–273. ization/derealization during acute social stress in social phobia. J. Anxiety Disord. 2391
2306 Golding, M., Kotlyar, M., Carson, S.W., Hoyler, S., Lazarus, C., Davidson, C., Guzzo, 27, 178–187. 2392
2307 J., Sontz, E., Garbutt, J., 2005. Effects of paroxetine on cardiovascular response Hucklebridge, F., Clow, A., Evans, P., 1998. The relationship between salivary secre- 2393
2308 to mental stress in subjects with a history of coronary artery disease and no tory immunoglobulin A and cortisol: neuroendocrine response to awakening 2394
2309 psychiatric diagnoses. Psychopharmacology 182, 321–326. and the diurnal cycle. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 31, 69–76. 2395
2310 Gordis, E.B., Granger, D.A., Susman, E.J., Trickett, P.K., 2006. Asymmetry between Hucklebridge, F., Hussain, T., Evans, P., Clow, A., 2005. The diurnal patterns of the 2396
2311 salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase reactivity to stress: relation to aggressive adrenal steroids cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) in relation to 2397
2312 behavior in adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 976–987. awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 51–57. 2398
2313 Gotlib, I.H., Joormann, J., Minor, K.L., Hallmayer, J., 2008. HPA-axis reactivity: a mech- Huerta-Franco, M.R., Vargas-Luna, M., Montes-Frausto, J.B., Morales-Mata, I., 2399
2314 anism underlying the associations among 5-HTTLPR, stress, and depression. Biol. Ramirez-Padilla, L., 2012. Effect of psychological stress on gastric motility 2400
2315 Psychiatry 63, 847. assessed by electrical bio-impedance. World J. Gastroenterol. 18, 5027. 2401
2316 Graeff, F.G., Parente, A., Del-Ben, C.M., Guimarães, F.S., 2003. Pharmacology of human Inder, W.J., Dimeski, G., Russell, A., 2012. Measurement of salivary cortisol in 2402
2317 experimental anxiety. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 36, 421–432. 2012—laboratory techniques and clinical indications. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 77, 2403
2318 Granger, D.A., Kivlighan, K.T., Blair, C., El-Sheikh, M., Mize, J., Lisonbee, J.A., Buckhalt, 645–651. 2404
2319 J.A., Stroud, L.R., Handwerger, K., Schwartz, E.B., 2006. Integrating the measure- Ising, M., Horstmann, S., Kloiber, S., Lucae, S., Binder, E.B., Kern, N., Künzel, H.E., 2405
2320 ment of salivary ␣-amylase into studies of child health, development, and social Pfennig, A., Uhr, M., Holsboer, F., 2007. Combined dexamethasone/corticotropin 2406
2321 relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 23, 267–290. releasing hormone test predicts treatment response in major depression—a 2407
2322 Grassi, G., Seravalle, G., Bolla, G., Quarti-Trevano, F., Dell’Oro, R., Arenare, F., Mancia, potential biomarker? Biol. Psychiatry 62, 47–54. 2408
2323 G., 2008. Heart rate as a sympathetic marker during acute adrenergic challenge. Izawa, S., Sugaya, N., Shirotsuki, K., Yamada, K.C., Ogawa, N., Ouchi, Y., Nagano, 2409
2324 J. Hypertens. 26, 70–75. Y., Suzuki, K., Nomura, S., 2008. Salivary dehydroepiandrosterone secretion in 2410
2325 Gray, G., Miles, C., Wilson, N., Jenks, R., Cox, M., Johnson, A.J., 2012. The contrasting response to acute psychosocial stress and its correlations with biological and 2411
2326 physiological and subjective effects of chewing gum on social stress. Appetite psychological changes. Biol. Psychol. 79, 294–298. 2412
2327 58, 554–558. Izawa, S., Miki, K., Liu, X., Ogawa, N., 2013a. The diurnal patterns of salivary 2413
2328 Gruenewald, T.L., Kemeny, M.E., Aziz, N., Fahey, J.L., 2004. Acute threat to the interleukin-6 and C-reactive protein in healthy young adults. Brain Behav. 2414
2329 social self: shame, social self-esteem, and cortisol activity. Psychosom. Med. 66, Immun. 27, 38–41. 2415
2330 915–924. Izawa, S., Sugaya, N., Kimura, K., Ogawa, N., Yamada, K.C., Shirotsuki, K., Mikami, I., 2416
2331 Gruenewald, T.L., Kemeny, M.E., Aziz, N., 2006. Subjective social status moderates Hirata, K., Nagano, Y., Nomura, S., 2013b. An increase in salivary interleukin-6 2417
2332 cortisol responses to social threat. Brain Behav. Immun. 20, 410–419. level following acute psychosocial stress and its biological correlates in healthy 2418
2333 Guimarães, F.S., Zuardi, A., Graeff, F., 1987. Effect of chlorimipramine and maprotiline young adults. Biol. Psychol. 94, 249–254. 2419
2334 on experimental anxiety in humans. J. Psychopharm. 1, 184–192. Jackson, E.D., Payne, J.D., Nadel, L., Jacobs, W.J., 2006. Stress differentially mod- 2420
2335 Guo, R., Riedemann, N.C., Laudes, I.J., Sarma, V.J., Kunkel, R.G., Dilley, K.A., Paulauskis, ulates fear conditioning in healthy men and women. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 2421
2336 J.D., Ward, P.A., 2002. Altered neutrophil trafficking during sepsis. J. Immunol. 516–522. 2422
2337 169, 307–314. Jahncke, H., Hygge, S., Halin, N., Green, A.M., Dimberg, K., 2011. Open-plan office 2423
2338 Hamidovic, A., Childs, E., Conrad, M., King, A., de Wit, H., 2010. Stress-induced noise: cognitive performance and restoration. J. Environ. Psychol. 31, 373–382. 2424
2339 changes in mood and cortisol release predict mood effects of amphetamine. Jamieson, J.P., Mendes, W.B., Nock, M.K., 2013. Improving acute stress responses: 2425
2340 Drug Alcohol Depend. 109, 175–180. the power of reappraisal. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 51–56. 2426

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 27

2427 Jean Kant, G., Eggleston, T., Landman-Roberts, L., Kenion, C.C., Driver, G.C., Meyerhoff, Kirschbaum, C., Klauer, T., Filipp, S.H., Hellhammer, D.H., 1995. Sex-specific effects of 2513
2428 J.L., 1985. Habituation to repeated stress is stressor specific. Pharmacol. Biochem. social support on cortisol and subjective responses to acute psychological stress. 2514
2429 Behav. 22, 631–634. Psychosom. Med. 57, 23–31. 2515
2430 Jelici, M., Geraerts, E., Merckelbach, H., Guerrieri, R., 2004. Acute stress enhances Kirschbaum, C., Prussner, J.C., Stone, A.A., Federenko, I., Gaab, J., Lintz, D., Schom- 2516
2431 memory for emotional words, but impairs memory for neutral words. Int. J. mer, N., Hellhammer, D.H., 1995. Persistent high cortisol responses to repeated 2517
2432 Neurosci. 114, 1343–1351. psychological stress in a subpopulation of healthy men. Psychosom. Med. 57, 2518
2433 Jezova, D., Makatsori, A., Duncko, R., Moncek, F., Jakubek, M., 2004. High trait anxiety 468–474. 2519
2434 in healthy subjects is associated with low neuroendocrine activity during psy- Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, O.T., May, M., Wippich, W., Hellhammer, D.H., 1996. Stress- 2520
2435 chosocial stress. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 28, 1331–1336. and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired 2521
2436 Jochman, K.A., Newman, S.M., Kalin, N.H., Bakshi, V.P., 2005. Corticotropin-releasing declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Sci. 58, 1475–1483. 2522
2437 factor-1 receptors in the basolateral amygdala mediate stress-induced anorexia. Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B.M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N.C., Hellhammer, D.H., 1999. 2523
2438 Behav. Neurosci. 119, 1448. Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity 2524
2439 Joels, M., Baram, T.Z., 2009. The neuro-symphony of stress. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychosom. Med. 61, 154–162. 2525
2440 459–466. Klopp, C., Garcia, C., Schulman, A.H., Ward, C.P., Tartar, J.L., 2012. Acute social stress 2526
2441 Jönsson, P., Wallergård, M., Österberg, K., Hansen, Å.M., Johansson, G., Karlson, B., increases biochemical and self report markers of stress without altering spatial 2527
2442 2010. Cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity and habituation to a virtual reality learning in humans. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 33, 425–430. 2528
2443 version of the Trier Social Stress Test: a pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology Koenen, K.C., Moffitt, T.E., Poulton, R., Martin, J., Caspi, A., 2007. Early childhood fac- 2529
2444 35, 1397–1403. tors associated with the development of post-traumatic stress disorder: results 2530
2445 Julio-Pieper, M., O’Mahony, C.M., Clarke, G., Bravo, J.A., Dinan, T.G., Cryan, J.F., from a longitudinal birth cohort. Psychol. Med. 37, 181–192. 2531
2446 2012. Chronic stress-induced alterations in mouse colonic 5-HT and defecation Kotlyar, M., al’Absi, M., Brauer, L.H., Grant, J.E., Fong, E., Kim, S.W., 2008a. Naltrex- 2532
2447 responses are strain dependent. Stress 15, 218–226. one effect on physiological and subjective response to a cold pressor task. Biol. 2533
2448 Juruena, M., Baes, C.V.W., de Carvalho Tofoli, S.M., Martins, C.M.S., 2011. Early life Psychol. 77, 233–236. 2534
2449 stress, HPA axis, and depression. Psychol. Neurosci. 4, 229–234. Kotlyar, M., Donahue, C., Thuras, P., Kushner, M.G., O’Gorman, N., Smith, E.A., Adson, 2535
2450 Juster, R., Perna, A., Marin, M., Sindi, S., Lupien, S.J., 2012. Timing is everything: D.E., 2008b. Physiological response to a speech stressor presented in a virtual 2536
2451 anticipatory stress dynamics among cortisol and blood pressure reactivity and reality environment. Psychophysiology 45, 1034–1037. 2537
2452 recovery in healthy adults. Stress 15, 569–577. Krämer, M., Seefeldt, W.L., Heinrichs, N., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schmitz, J., Wolf, O.T., 2538
2453 Kaestner, F., Hettich, M., Peters, M., Sibrowski, W., Hetzel, G., Ponath, G., Arolt, V., Blechert, J., 2012. Subjective, autonomic, and endocrine reactivity during social 2539
2454 Cassens, U., Rothermundt, M., 2005. Different activation patterns of proinflam- stress in children with social phobia. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 40, 95–104. 2540
2455 matory cytokines in melancholic and non-melancholic major depression are Kubera, B., Hubold, C., Otte, S., Lindenberg, A.S., Zeiß, I., Krause, R., Steinkamp, M., 2541
2456 associated with HPA axis activity. J. Affect. Disord. 87, 305–311. Klement, J., Entringer, S., Pellerin, L., Peters, A., 2012. Rise in plasma lactate con- 2542
2457 Kassam, K.S., Koslov, K., Mendes, W.B., 2009. Decisions under distress stress profiles centrations with psychosocial stress: a possible sign of cerebral energy demand. 2543
2458 influence anchoring and adjustment. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1394–1399. Obesity Facts 5, 384–392. 2544
2459 Kaufman, J., Martin, A., King, R.A., Charney, D., 2001. Are child-, adolescent-, Kudielka, B.M., Wüst, S., 2010. Human models in acute and chronic stress: assess- 2545
2460 and adult-onset depression one and the same disorder? Biol. Psychiatry 49, ing determinants of individual hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis activity and 2546
2461 980–1001. reactivity. Stress 13, 1–14. 2547
2462 Kaye, J., Buchanan, F., Kendrick, A., Johnson, P., Lowry, C., Bailey, J., Nutt, D., Light- Kudielka, B., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Hellhammer, D., Kirschbaum, C., 2004a. HPA axis 2548
2463 man, S., 2004. Acute carbon dioxide exposure in healthy adults: evaluation responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in healthy elderly adults, younger 2549
2464 of a novel means of investigating the stress response. J. Neuroendocrinol. 16, adults, and children: impact of age and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 2550
2465 256–264. 83–98. 2551
2466 Kazén, M., Kuenne, T., Frankenberg, H., Quirin, M., 2012. Inverse relation between Kudielka, B.M., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Hellhammer, D.H., Kirschbaum, C., 2004b. 2552
2467 cortisol and anger and their relation to performance and explicit memory. Biol. Differential heart rate reactivity and recovery after psychosocial stress (TSST) 2553
2468 Psychol. 91, 28–35. in healthy children, younger adults, and elderly adults: the impact of age and 2554
2469 Kelly, O., Matheson, K., Martinez, A., Merali, Z., Anisman, H., 2007. Psychosocial stress gender. Int. J. Behav. Med. 11, 116–121. 2555
2470 evoked by a virtual audience: relation to neuroendocrine activity. CyberPsychol. Kudielka, B.M., Schommer, N.C., Hellhammer, D.H., Kirschbaum, C., 2004c. Acute 2556
2471 Behav. 10, 655–662. HPA axis responses, heart rate, and mood changes to psychosocial stress (TSST) 2557
2472 Kelly, M.M., Tyrka, A.R., Anderson, G.M., Price, L.H., Carpenter, L.L., 2008. Sex differ- in humans at different times of day. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 983–992. 2558
2473 ences in emotional and physiological responses to the Trier Social Stress Test. J. Kudielka, B.M., von Känel, R., Preckel, D., Zgraggen, L., Mischler, K., Fischer, J.E., 2006. 2559
2474 Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 39, 87–98. Exhaustion is associated with reduced habituation of free cortisol responses to 2560
2475 Kemeny, M.E., Schedlowski, M., 2007. Understanding the interaction between psy- repeated acute psychosocial stress. Biol. Psychol. 72, 147–153. 2561
2476 chosocial stress and immune-related diseases: A stepwise progression. Brain Kudielka, B.M., Hellhammer, D.H., Kirschbaum, C., 2007. Ten years of research with 2562
2477 Behav. Immun. 21, 1009–1018. the Trier Social Stress Test—revisited. In: Harmon-Jones, E., Vinkielman, P. (Eds.), 2563
2478 Kemeny, M.E., Foltz, C., Cavanagh, J.F., Cullen, M., Giese-Davis, J., Jennings, P., Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of 2564
2479 Rosenberg, E.L., Gillath, O., Shaver, P.R., Wallace, B.A., Ekman, P., 2012. Contem- Social Behavior. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 56–83. 2565
2480 plative/emotion training reduces negative emotional behavior and promotes Kudielka, B.M., Hellhammer, D.H., Wüst, S., 2009. Why do we respond so differently? 2566
2481 prosocial responses. Emotion 12, 338–350. Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psy- 2567
2482 Kennedy, D.O., Little, W., Haskell, C.F., Scholey, A.B., 2006. Anxiolytic effects of a com- choneuroendocrinology 34, 2–18. 2568
2483 bination of Melissa ofcinalis and Valeriana ofcinalis during laboratory induced Kumsta, R., Entringer, S., Koper, J.W., van Rossum, E.F., Hellhammer, D.H., Wüst, S., 2569
2484 stress. Phytother. Res. 20, 96–102. 2007. Sex specific associations between common glucocorticoid receptor gene 2570
2485 Kennedy, P.J., Clarke, G., Quigley, E.M.M., Groeger, J.A., Dinan, T.G., Cryan, J.F., 2012. variants and hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis responses to psychosocial 2571
2486 Gut memories: towards a cognitive neurobiology of irritable bowel syndrome. stress. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 863–869. 2572
2487 Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 310–340. Kumsta, R., Chen, F.S., Pape, H.-C., Heinrichs, M., 2013. Neuropeptide S receptor gene 2573
2488 Kennedy, P.J., Clarke, G., Cryan, J.F., Dinan, T.G., 2013. Sustained hypothalamic- is associated with cortisol responses to social stress in humans. Biol. Psychol. 93, 2574
2489 pituitary-adrenal-axis response to acute psychosocial stress in irritable bowel 304–307. 2575
2490 syndrome. In: 43rd Annual Meeting of the International Society of Psychoneu- Laaris, N., Haj-dahmane, S., Hamon, M., Lanfumey, L., 1995. Glucocorticoid receptor- 2576
2491 roendocrinology, Leiden, The Netherlands. mediated inhibition by corticosterone of 5-HT1a autoreceptor functioning in the 2577
2492 Khalili-Mahani, N., Dedovic, K., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., Pruessner, J.C., 2010. Hip- rat dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuropharmacology 34, 1201–1210. 2578
2493 pocampal activation during a cognitive task is associated with subsequent Lackschewitz, H., Hüther, G., Kröner-Herwig, B., 2008. Physiological and psycho- 2579
2494 neuroendocrine and cognitive responses to psychological stress. Hippocampus logical stress responses in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder 2580
2495 20, 323–334. (ADHD). Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 612–624. 2581
2496 Kim, J.J., Lee, H.J., Han, J., Packard, M.G., 2001. Amygdala is critical for stress-induced Lam, L.L., Emberly, E., Fraser, H.B., Neumann, S.M., Chen, E., Miller, G.E., Kobor, M.S., 2582
2497 modulation of hippocampal long-term potentiation and learning. J. Neurosci. 2012. Factors underlying variable DNA methylation in a human community 2583
2498 21, 5222–5228. cohort. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 17253–17260. 2584
2499 Kimura, K., Izawa, S., Sugaya, N., Ogawa, N., Yamada, K.C., Shirotsuki, K., Mikami, Lange, C., Zschucke, E., Ising, M., Uhr, M., Bermpohl, F., Adli, M., 2013. Evidence 2585
2500 I., Hirata, K., Nagano, Y., Hasegawa, T., 2013. The biological effects of for a normal HPA axis response to psychosocial stress in patients remit- 2586
2501 acute psychosocial stress on delay discounting. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, ted from depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ 2587
2502 2300–2308. j.psyneuen.2013.06.033. 2588
2503 King, A.P., Liberzon, I., 2009. Assessing the neuroendocrine stress response in the Lazarus, R.S., Folkman, S., 1984. Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. Springer Publishing 2589
2504 functional neuroimaging context. Neuroimage 47, 1116–1124. Company, New York. 2590
2505 Kirschbaum, C., Wüst, S., Hellhammer, D., 1992. Consistent sex differences in cortisol Leder, J., Häusser, J.A., Mojzisch, A., 2013. Stress and strategic decision-making in 2591
2506 responses to psychological stress. Psychosom. Med. 54, 648–657. the beauty contest game. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1503–1511. 2592
2507 Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.M., Hellhammer, D.H., 1993. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’—a Lederbogen, F., Kirsch, P., Haddad, L., Streit, F., Tost, H., Schuch, P., Wüst, S., Pruessner, 2593
2508 tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. J.C., Rietschel, M., Deuschle, M., 2011. City living and urban upbringing affect 2594
2509 Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–81. neural social stress processing in humans. Nature 474, 498–501. 2595
2510 Kirschbaum, C., Scherer, G., Strasburger, C., 1994. Pituitary and adrenal hormone Lee, R.S., Tamashiro, K.L., Yang, X., Purcell, R.H., Huo, Y., Rongione, M., Potash, J.B., 2596
2511 responses to pharmacological, physical, and psychological stimulation in habit- Wand, G.S., 2011. A measure of glucocorticoid load provided by DNA methylation 2597
2512 ual smokers and nonsmokers. J. Mol. Med. 72, 804–810. of Fkbp5 in mice. Psychopharmacology 218, 303–312. 2598

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

28 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

2599 Lennartsson, A.-K., Kushnir, M.M., Bergquist, J., Billig, H., Jonsdottir, I.H., 2012a. Sex McEwen, B.S., 2007. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central 2685
2600 steroid levels temporarily increase in response to acute psychosocial stress in role of the brain. Physiol. Rev. 87, 873–904. 2686
2601 healthy men and women. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 84, 246–253. McGirr, A., Diaconu, G., Berlim, M.T., Pruessner, J.C., Sablé, R., Cabot, S., Turecki, 2687
2602 Lennartsson, A., Kushnir, M.M., Bergquist, J., Jonsdottir, I.H., 2012. DHEA and DHEA-S G., 2010. Dysregulation of the sympathetic nervous system, hypothalamic- 2688
2603 response to acute psychosocial stress in healthy men and women. Biol. Psychol. pituitary-adrenal axis and executive function in individuals at risk for suicide. J. 2689
2604 90, 143–149. Psychiatry Neurosci. 35, 399. 2690
2605 Lennartsson, A.-K., Theorell, T., Kushnir, M.M., Bergquist, J., Jonsdottir, I.H., 2013. McNair, D.M., Frankenthaler, L.M., Czerlinsky, T., White, T.W., Sasson, S., Fisher, S., 2691
2606 Perceived stress at work is associated with attenuated DHEA-S response during 1982. Simulated public speaking as a model of clinical anxiety. Psychopharma- 2692
2607 acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1650–1657. cology 77, 7–10. 2693
2608 Leonard, B.E., 2001. Stress, norepinephrine and depression. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. McRae, A.L., Saladin, M.E., Brady, K.T., Upadhyaya, H., Back, S.E., Timmerman, M.A., 2694
2609 26, S11. 2006. Stress reactivity: biological and subjective responses to the cold pressor 2695
2610 Leonard, B.E., 2006. HPA and immune axes in stress: involvement of the serotonergic and Trier Social stressors. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 21, 377–385. 2696
2611 system. Neuroimmunomodulation 13, 268–276. Meerson, A., Cacheaux, L., Goosens, K., Sapolsky, R., Soreq, H., Kaufer, D., 2010. 2697
2612 Leonard, B.E., Song, C., 1999. Stress, depression, and the role of cytokines. In: Dantzer, Changes in brain microRNAs contribute to cholinergic stress reactions. J. Mol. 2698
2613 R., Wollman, E., Yirmiya, R. (Eds.), Cytokines, Stress, and Depression. Springer, Neurosci. 40, 47–55. 2699
2614 US, pp. 251–265. Miller, A., Pariante, C., Pearce, B., 1999. Effects of cytokines on glucocorticoid recep- 2700
2615 Leonard, B.E., Song, C., 2007. Cytokines, stress, and depression. In: McEwen, B., de tor expression and function. In: Dantzer, R., Wollman, E., Yirmiya, R. (Eds.), 2701
2616 Kloet, E.R., Rubin, R., Chrousos, G., Steptoe, A., Rose, N., Craig, I., Feuerstein, G. Cytokines, Stress, and Depression. Springer, US, pp. 107–116. 2702
2617 (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Stress. , 2nd ed. Academic Press, New York, pp. 705–711. Minetto, M.A., Gazzoni, M., Lanfranco, F., Baldi, M., Saba, L., Pedrola, R., Komi, P.V., 2703
2618 Leonard, B.E., Myint, A., 2009. The psychoneuroimmunology of depression. Hum. Rainoldi, A., 2007. Influence of the sample collection method on salivary inter- 2704
2619 Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 24, 165–175. leukin-6 levels in resting and post-exercise conditions. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 101, 2705
2620 Li, M., Han, F., Shi, Y., 2011. Expression of locus coeruleus mineralocorticoid receptor 249–256. 2706
2621 and glucocorticoid receptor in rats under single-prolonged stress. Neurol. Sci. Mischler, K., Fischer, J.E., Zgraggen, L., Kudielka, B.M., Preckel, D., von Känel, R., 2707
2622 32, 625–631. 2005. The effect of repeated acute mental stress on habituation and recovery 2708
2623 Li, S., Weerda, R., Guenzel, F., Wolf, O.T., Thiel, C.M., 2013. ADRA2B genotype mod- responses in hemoconcentration and blood cells in healthy men. Life Sci. 77, 2709
2624 ulates effects of acute psychosocial stress on emotional memory retrieval in 1166–1179. 2710
2625 healthy young men. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 103, 11–18. Mohiyeddini, C., Semple, S., 2013. Displacement behaviour regulates the experience 2711
2626 Lipschitz, D.S., Rasmusson, A.M., Yehuda, R., Wang, S., Anyan, W., Gueoguieva, R., of stress in men. Stress 16, 163–171. 2712
2627 Grilo, C.M., Fehon, D.C., Southwick, S.M., 2003. Salivary cortisol responses to Mohiyeddini, C., Bauer, S., Semple, S., 2013a. Displacement behaviour is associated 2713
2628 dexamethasone in adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Am. Acad. with reduced stress levels among men but not women. PLoS ONE 8, e56355. 2714
2629 Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 42, 1310–1317. Mohiyeddini, C., Bauer, S., Semple, S., 2013b. Public self-consciousness moderates 2715
2630 Liu, S., Wood, J., 2012. Neurobiology of corticotropin-releasing factor in the enteric the link between displacement behaviour and experience of stress in women. 2716
2631 nervous system during stress. Front. Gastrointest. Res. 30, 115–123. Stress 16, 384–392. 2717
2632 Liu, J., Chaplin, T.M., Wang, F., Sinha, R., Mayes, L.C., Blumberg, H.P., 2012. Stress Monteiro-dos-Santos, P., Graeff, F., Dos-Santos, J., Ribeiro, R., Guimarães, F.S., Zuardi, 2718
2633 reactivity and corticolimbic response to emotional faces in adolescents. J. Am. A., 2000. Effects of tryptophan depletion on anxiety induced by simulated public 2719
2634 Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 304–312. speaking. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 33, 581–587. 2720
2635 Ljungberg, J.K., Neely, G., 2007. Stress, subjective experience and cognitive perfor- Monteleone, P., Tortorella, A., Scognamiglio, P., Serino, I., Monteleone, A.M., Maj, M., 2721
2636 mance during exposure to noise and vibration. J. Environ. Psychol. 27, 44–54. 2012. The acute salivary ghrelin response to a psychosocial stress is enhanced in 2722
2637 Loft, P., Thomas, M.G., Petrie, K.J., Booth, R.J., Miles, J., Vedhara, K., 2007. Examination symptomatic patients with bulimia nervosa: a pilot study. Neuropsychobiology 2723
2638 stress results in altered cardiovascular responses to acute challenge and lower 66, 230–236. 2724
2639 cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 367–375. Moons, W.G., Eisenberger, N.I., Taylor, S.E., 2010. Anger and fear responses to stress 2725
2640 Low, C.A., Salomon, K., Matthews, K.A., 2009. Chronic life stress, cardiovascular reac- have different biological profiles. Brain Behav. Immun. 24, 215–219. 2726
2641 tivity, and subclinical cardiovascular disease in adolescents. Psychosom. Med. Morris, M.C., Rao, U., Garber, J., 2012. Cortisol responses to psychosocial stress 2727
2642 71, 927–931. predict depression trajectories: social-evaluative threat and prior depressive 2728
2643 Lowy, M.T., Reder, A., Antel, J., Meltzer, H., 1984. Glucocorticoid resistance in episodes as moderators. J. Affect. Disord. 143, 223–230. 2729
2644 depression: the dexamethasone suppression test and lymphocyte sensitivity Muehlhan, M., Lueken, U., Wittchen, H.-U., Kirschbaum, C., 2011. The scanner as 2730
2645 to dexamethasone. Am. J. Psychiatry 141, 1365–1370. a stressor: evidence from subjective and neuroendocrine stress parameters in 2731
2646 Luethi, M., Meier, B., Sandi, C., 2008. Stress effects on working memory, explicit the time course of a functional magnetic resonance imaging session. Int. J. Psy- 2732
2647 memory, and implicit memory for neutral and emotional stimuli in healthy men. chophysiol. 79, 118–126. 2733
2648 Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 5. Mueller, A., Armbruster, D., Moser, D.A., Canli, T., Lesch, K., Brocke, B., Kirschbaum, 2734
2649 Lupien, S.J., Wilkinson, C.W., Brière, S., Ménard, C., Ng Ying Kin, N.M.K., Nair, N.P.V., C., 2011. Interaction of serotonin transporter gene-linked polymorphic region 2735
2650 2002. The modulatory effects of corticosteroids on cognition: studies in young and stressful life events predicts cortisol stress response. Neuropsychopharma- 2736
2651 human populations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 27, 401–416. cology 36, 1332–1339. 2737
2652 Lupien, S.J., Maheu, F., Tu, M., Fiocco, A., Schramek, T.E., 2007. The effects of stress Mueller, A., Strahler, J., Armbruster, D., Lesch, K., Brocke, B., Kirschbaum, C., 2012. 2738
2653 and stress hormones on human cognition: implications for the field of brain and Genetic contributions to acute autonomic stress responsiveness in children. Int. 2739
2654 cognition. Brain Cogn. 65, 209–237. J. Psychophysiol. 83, 302–308. 2740
2655 MacMillan, H.L., Georgiades, K., Duku, E.K., Shea, A., Steiner, M., Niec, A., Tanaka, M., Munro, C.A., Oswald, L.M., Weerts, E.M., McCaul, M.E., Wand, G.S., 2005. Hormone 2741
2656 Gensey, S., Spree, S., Vella, E., 2009. Cortisol response to stress in female youths responses to social stress in abstinent alcohol-dependent subjects and social 2742
2657 exposed to childhood maltreatment: results of the youth mood project. Biol. drinkers with no history of alcohol dependence. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 29, 2743
2658 Psychiatry 66, 62–68. 1133–1138. 2744
2659 Maddock, C., Pariante, C.M., 2001. How does stress affect you? An overview of stress, Murray, C., Flynn, J., Ratcliffe, L., Jacyna, M.R., Kamm, M.A., Emmanuel, A.V., 2004. 2745
2660 immunity, depression and disease. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 10, 153–162. Effect of acute physical and psychological stress on gut autonomic innervation 2746
2661 Mahon, P.B., Zandi, P.P., Potash, J.B., Nestadt, G., Wand, G.S., 2013. Genetic associa- in irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology 127, 1695–1703. 2747
2662 tion of FKBP5 and CRHR1 with cortisol response to acute psychosocial stress in Nater, U., Rohleder, N., 2009. Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker 2748
2663 healthy adults. Psychopharmacology 227, 231–241. for the sympathetic nervous system: current state of research. Psychoneuroen- 2749
2664 Majer, M., Nater, U., Lin, J., Capuron, L., Reeves, W., 2010. Association of childhood docrinology 34, 486–496. 2750
2665 trauma with cognitive function in healthy adults: a pilot study. BMC Neurol. 10, Nater, U.M., La Marca, R., Florin, L., Moses, A., Langhans, W., Koller, M.M., 2751
2666 61. Ehlert, U., 2006. Stress-induced changes in human salivary alpha-amylase 2752
2667 Makatsori, A., Duncko, R., Moncek, F., Loder, I., Katina, S., Jezova, D., 2004. Modula- activity—associations with adrenergic activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 2753
2668 tion of neuroendocrine response and non-verbal behavior during psychosocial 49–58. 2754
2669 stress in healthy volunteers by the glutamate release-inhibiting drug lamotrig- Nater, U.M., Moor, C., Okere, U., Stallkamp, R., Martin, M., Ehlert, U., Kliegel, 2755
2670 ine. Neuroendocrinology 79, 34–42. M., 2007. Performance on a declarative memory task is better in high than 2756
2671 Matthews, G., Warm, J., Reinerman, L., Langheim, L., Saxby, D., 2010. Task engage- low cortisol responders to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 2757
2672 ment, attention, and executive control. In: Gruszka, A., Matthews, G., Szymura, 758–763. 2758
2673 B. (Eds.), Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition. Springer, New York, Newhouse, P.A., Dumas, J., Wilkins, H., Coderre, E., Sites, C.K., Naylor, M., Benkelfat, 2759
2674 pp. 205–230. C., Young, S.N., 2010. Estrogen treatment impairs cognitive performance follow- 2760
2675 Mathews, H.L., Konley, T., Kosik, K.L., Krukowski, K., Eddy, J., Albuquerque, K., ing psychosocial stress and monoamine depletion in postmenopausal women. 2761
2676 Janusek, L.W., 2011. Epigenetic patterns associated with the immune dys- Menopause (New York, NY) 17, 860. 2762
2677 regulation that accompanies psychosocial distress. Brain Behav. Immun. 25, Newport, D.J., Heim, C., Bonsall, R., Miller, A.H., Nemeroff, C.B., 2004. Pituitary- 2763
2678 830–839. adrenal responses to standard and low-dose dexamethasone suppression tests 2764
2679 Mayer, E.A., 2000. The neurobiology of stress and gastrointestinal disease. Gut 47, in adult survivors of child abuse. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 10–20. 2765
2680 861–869. Nguyen, A.D., Conley, A.J., 2008. Adrenal adrogens in human and nonhuman pri- 2766
2681 Mayer, E.A., 2011. Gut feelings: the emerging biology of gut–brain communication. mates: production, zonation and regulation. Endocr. Dev. 13, 33–54. 2767
2682 Nat Rev Neurosci 12, 453–466. Nishanian, P., Aziz, N., Chung, J., Detels, R., Fahey, J.L., 1998. Oral fluids as an alter- 2768
2683 Mayer, E.A., Naliboff, B.D., Chang, L., Coutinho, S.V., 2001. V. Stress and irritable bowel native to serum for measurement of markers of immune activation. Clin. Diagn. 2769
2684 syndrome. Am. J. Physiol. Gastrointest. Liver Physiol. 280, G519–G524. Lab. Immunol. 5, 507–512. 2770

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 29

2771 O’Connor, R., Dinan, T., Cryan, J., 2011. Little things on which happiness depends: Pruett, S.B., 2003. Stress and the immune system. Pathophysiology 9, 133–153. 2857
2772 microRNAs as novel therapeutic targets for the treatment of anxiety and depres- Quas, J.A., Yim, I.S., Rush, E., Sumaroka, M., 2012. Hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis 2858
2773 sion. Mol. Psychiatry 17, 359–376. and sympathetic activation: joint predictors of memory in children, adolescents, 2859
2774 O’Keane, V., Frodl, T., Dinan, T.G., 2012. A review of atypical depression in relation and adults. Biol. Psychol. 89, 335–341. 2860
2775 to the course of depression and changes in HPA axis organization. Psychoneu- Quas, J.A., Rush, E.B., Yim, I.S., Nikolayev, M., 2013. Effects of stress on 2861
2776 roendocrinology 37, 1589–1599. memory in children and adolescents: testing causal connections. Memory., 2862
2777 Oswald, L.M., Zandi, P., Nestadt, G., Potash, J.B., Kalaydjian, A.E., Wand, G.S., 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.809766. 2863
2778 Relationship between cortisol responses to stress and personality. Neuropsy- Quesada, A.A., Wiemers, U.S., Schoofs, D., Wolf, O.T., 2012. Psychosocial stress 2864
2779 chopharmacology 31, 1583–1591. exposure impairs memory retrieval in children. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 2865
2780 Ouellet-Morin, I., Danese, A., Williams, B., Arseneault, L., 2011. Validation of a high- 125–136. 2866
2781 sensitivity assay for C-reactive protein in human saliva. Brain Behav. Immun. Raison, C.L., Capuron, L., Miller, A.H., 2006. Cytokines sing the blues: inflammation 2867
2782 25, 640–646. and the pathogenesis of depression. Trends Immunol. 27, 24–31. 2868
2783 Pabst, S., Brand, M., Wolf, O.T., 2013a. Stress and decision making: a few minutes Ramsay, D., Lewis, M., 2003. Reactivity and regulation in cortisol and behavioral 2869
2784 make all the difference. Behav. Brain Res. 250, 39–45. responses to stress. Child Dev. 74, 456–464. 2870
2785 Pabst, S., Schoofs, D., Pawlikowski, M., Brand, M., Wolf, O.T., 2013b. Paradoxical Rao, U., Hammen, C., Ortiz, L.R., Chen, L.A., Poland, R.E., 2008. Effects of early 2871
2786 effects of stress and an executive task on decisions under risk. Behav. Neurosci. and recent adverse experiences on adrenal response to psychosocial stress in 2872
2787 127, 369–379. depressed adolescents. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 521–526. 2873
2788 Pace, T., Mletzko, T., Alagbe, O., Musselman, D., Nemeroff, C., Miller, A., Heim, C., 2006. Reinhardt, T., Schmahl, C., Wüst, S., Bohus, M., 2012. Salivary cortisol, heart rate, 2874
2789 Increased stress-induced inflammatory responses in male patients with major electrodermal activity and subjective stress responses to the Mannheim Multi- 2875
2790 depression and increased early life stress. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 1630–1633. component Stress Test (MMST). Psychiatry Res. 198, 106–111. 2876
2791 Pace, T.W.W., Negi, L.T., Adame, D.D., Cole, S.P., Sivilli, T.I., Brown, T.D., Issa, Reynolds, E.K., Schreiber, W.M., Geisel, K., MacPherson, L., Ernst, M., Lejuez, C.W., 2877
2792 M.J., Raison, C.L., 2009. Effect of compassion meditation on neuroendocrine, 2013. Influence of social stress on risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J. Anxiety 2878
2793 innate immune and behavioral responses to psychosocial stress. Psychoneu- Disord. 27, 272–277. 2879
2794 roendocrinology 34, 87–98. Rimmele, U., Zellweger, B.C., Marti, B., Seiler, R., Mohiyeddini, C., Ehlert, U., Hein- 2880
2795 Pariante, C.M., Lightman, S.L., 2008. The HPA axis in major depression: classical richs, M., 2007. Trained men show lower cortisol, heart rate and psychological 2881
2796 theories and new developments. Trends Neurosci. 31, 464–468. responses to psychosocial stress compared with untrained men. Psychoneu- 2882
2797 Payne, J.D., Nadel, L., Allen, J.J.B., Thomas, K.G.F., Jacobs, W.J., 2002. The effects of roendocrinology 32, 627–635. 2883
2798 experimentally induced stress on false recognition. Memory 10, 1–6. Rimmele, U., Seiler, R., Marti, B., Wirtz, P.H., Ehlert, U., Heinrichs, M., 2009. The level 2884
2799 Peckins, M.K., Dockray, S., Eckenrode, J.L., Heaton, J., Susman, E.J., 2012. The longi- of physical activity affects adrenal and cardiovascular reactivity to psychosocial 2885
2800 tudinal impact of exposure to violence on cortisol reactivity in adolescents. J. stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 190–198. 2886
2801 Adolesc. Health 51, 366–372. Rinaldi, A., Vincenti, S., De Vito, F., Bozzoni, I., Oliverio, A., Presutti, C., Fragapane, P., 2887
2802 Petrowski, K., Herold, U., Joraschky, P., Wittchen, H.U., Kirschbaum, C., 2010. A strik- Mele, A., 2010. Stress induces region specific alterations in microRNAs expres- 2888
2803 ing pattern of cortisol non-responsiveness to psychosocial stress in patients sion in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 208, 265–269. 2889
2804 with panic disorder with concurrent normal cortisol awakening responses. Psy- Rohleder, N., Schommer, N.C., Hellhammer, D.H., Engel, R., Kirschbaum, C., 2001. 2890
2805 choneuroendocrinology 35, 414–421. Sex differences in glucocorticoid sensitivity of proinflammatory cytokine pro- 2891
Q5
2806 Petrowski, K., Wintermann, G., Siepmann, M., 2012. Cortisol response to repeated duction after psychosocial stress. Psychosom. Med. 63, 966–972. 2892
2807 psychosocial stress. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback, 1–5. Rohleder, N., Kudielka, B.M., Hellhammer, D.H., Wolf, J.M., Kirschbaum, C., 2002. 2893
2808 Petrowski, K., Wintermann, G., Schaarschmidt, M., Bornstein, S.R., Kirschbaum, C., Age and sex steroid-related changes in glucocorticoid sensitivity of pro- 2894
2809 2013. Blunted salivary and plasma cortisol response in patients with panic dis- inflammatory cytokine production after psychosocial stress. J. Neuroimmunol. 2895
2810 order under psychosocial stress. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 88, 35–39. 126, 69–77. 2896
2811 Plag, J., Gaudlitz, K., Zschucke, E., Yassouridis, A., Pyrkosch, L., Wittmann, A., Hols- Rohleder, N., Wolf, J., Piel, M., Kirschbaum, C., 2003. Impact of oral contraceptive 2897
2812 boer, F., Ströhle, A., 2012. Distinct panicogenic activity of sodium lactate and use on glucocorticoid sensitivity of pro-inflammatory cytokine production after 2898
2813 cholecystokinin tetrapeptide in patients with panic disorder. Curr. Pharm. Des. psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 261–273. 2899
2814 18, 5619–5626. Rohleder, N., Nater, U.M., Wolf, J.M., Ehlert, U., Kirschbaum, C., 2004. Psychosocial 2900
2815 Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., Goschke, T., 2011. Inflexibly focused under stress-induced activation of salivary alpha-amylase: an indicator of sympathetic 2901
2816 stress: acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the activity? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1032, 258–263. 2902
2817 expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. Rohleder, N., Wolf, J.M., Maldonado, E.F., Kirschbaum, C., 2006. The psychosocial 2903
2818 J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3218–3227. stress-induced increase in salivary alpha-amylase is independent of saliva flow 2904
2819 Plessow, F., Kiesel, A., Kirschbaum, C., 2012a. The stressed prefrontal cortex and rate. Psychophysiology 43, 645–652. 2905
2820 goal-directed behaviour: acute psychosocial stress impairs the flexible imple- Rohrmann, S., Hennig, J., Netter, P., 1999. Changing psychobiological stress reactions 2906
2821 mentation of task goals. Exp. Brain Res. 216, 397–408. by manipulating cognitive processes. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 33, 149–161. 2907
2822 Plessow, F., Schade, S., Kirschbaum, C., Fischer, R., 2012b. Better not to deal with two Rose, R.D., Buckey Jr., J.C., Zbozinek, T.D., Motivala, S.J., Glenn, D.E., Cartreine, J.A., 2908
2823 tasks at the same time when stressed? Acute psychosocial stress reduces task Craske, M.G., 2013. A randomized controlled trial of a self-guided, multime- 2909
2824 shielding in dual-task performance. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 557–570. dia, stress management and resilience training program. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 2910
2825 Porcelli, A.J., Lewis, A.H., Delgado, M.R., 2012. Acute stress influences neural circuits 106–112. 2911
2826 of reward processing. Front. Neurosci. 6, 157. Rosenberg, N., Bloch, M., Ben Avi, I., Rouach, V., Schreiber, S., Stern, N., Greenman, 2912
2827 Posener, J.A., Schildkraut, J.J., Wilfams, G.H., Schatzberg, A.F., 1997. Cortisol feedback Y., 2012. Cortisol response and desire to binge following psychological stress: 2913
2828 effects on plasma corticotropin levels in healthy subjects. Psychoneuroen- comparison between obese subjects with and without binge eating disorder. 2914
2829 docrinology 22, 169–176. Psychiatry Res. 208, 156–161. 2915
2830 Posserud, I., Agerforz, P., Ekman, R., Björnsson, E.S., Abrahamsson, H., Simrén, M., Rosenkranz, M.A., Davidson, R.J., MacCoon, D.G., Sheridan, J.F., Kalin, N.H., Lutz, A., 2916
2831 2004. Altered visceral perceptual and neuroendocrine response in patients with 2013. A comparison of mindfulness-based stress reduction and an active control 2917
2832 irritable bowel syndrome during mental stress. Gut 53, 1102–1108. in modulation of neurogenic inflammation. Brain Behav. Immun. 27, 174–184. 2918
2833 Pruessner, J.C., Gaab, J., Hellhammer, D.H., Lintz, D., Schommer, N., Kirschbaum, Roth, M.K., Bingham, B., Shah, A., Joshi, A., Frazer, A., Strong, R., Morilak, D.A., 2012. 2919
2834 C., 1997. Increasing correlations between personality traits and cortisol Effects of chronic plus acute prolonged stress on measures of coping style, anx- 2920
2835 stress responses obtained by data aggregation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 22, iety, and evoked HPA-axis reactivity. Neuropharmacology 63, 1118–1126. 2921
2836 615–625. Rouach, V., Bloch, M., Rosenberg, N., Gilad, S., Limor, R., Stern, N., Greenman, Y., 2007. 2922
2837 Pruessner, J.C., Wolf, O.T., Hellhammer, D.H., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., von Auer, K., The acute ghrelin response to a psychological stress challenge does not predict 2923
2838 Jobst, S., Kaspers, F., Kirschbaum, C., 1997. Free cortisol levels after awakening: the post-stress urge to eat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 693–702. 2924
2839 a reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Salazar-Mather, T.P., Ishikawa, R., Biron, C.A., 1996. NK cell trafficking and cytokine 2925
2840 Sci. 61, 2539–2549. expression in splenic compartments after IFN induction and viral infection. J. 2926
2841 Pruessner, J.C., Hellhammer, D.H., Kirschbaum, C., 1999. Low self-esteem, induced Immunol. 157, 3054–3064. 2927
2842 failure and the adrenocortical stress response. Pers. Individ. Diff. 27, 477–489. Sapolsky, R.M., Romero, L.M., Munck, A.U., 2000. How do glucocorticoids influence 2928
2843 Pruessner, M., Hellhammer, D.H., Pruessner, J.C., Lupien, S.J., 2003. Self-reported stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and prepar- 2929
2844 depressive symptoms and stress levels in healthy young men: associations with ative actions. Endocr. Rev. 21, 55–89. 2930
2845 the cortisol response to awakening. Psychosom. Med 65, 92–99. Sarai, M., Matsunaga, H., 1986. Low-dose (0.5-mg) dexamethasone suppression test 2931
2846 Pruessner, M., Pruessner, J.C., Hellhammer, D.H., Bruce Pike, G., Lupien, S.J., 2007. in depressive patients. Biol. Psychiatry 21, 744–750. 2932
2847 The associations among hippocampal volume, cortisol reactivity, and memory Schlotz, W., Kumsta, R., Layes, I., Entringer, S., Jones, A., Wüst, S., 2008. Covariance 2933
2848 performance in healthy young men. Psychiatry Res.: Neuroimaging 155, 1–10. between psychological and endocrine responses to pharmacological hallenge 2934
2849 Pruessner, J.C., Dedovic, K., Khalili-Mahani, N., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., Buss, C., and psychosocial stress: a question of timing. Psychosom. Med. 70, 787–796. 2935
2850 Renwick, R., Dagher, A., Meaney, M.J., Lupien, S., 2008. Deactivation of the lim- Schmidt-Reinwald, A., Pruessner, J., Hellhammer, D., Federenko, I., Rohleder, N., 2936
2851 bic system during acute psychosocial stress: evidence from positron emission Schürmeyer, T., Kirschbaum, C., 1999. The cortisol response to awakening in 2937
2852 tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biol. Psychia- relation to different challenge tests and a 12-h cortisol rhythm. Life Sci. 64, 2938
2853 try 63, 234–240. 1653–1660. 2939
2854 Pruessner, M., Béchard-Evans, L., Boekestyn, L., Iyer, S.N., Pruessner, J.C., Malla, A.K., Schmitz, J., Krämer, M., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Heinrichs, N., Blechert, J., 2011. Restricted 2940
2855 2013. Attenuated cortisol response to acute psychosocial stress in individuals at autonomic flexibility in children with social phobia. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2941
2856 ultra-high risk for psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 146, 79–86. 52, 1203–1211. 2942

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

30 A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

2943 Schnell, I., Potchter, O., Epstein, Y., Yaakov, Y., Hermesh, H., Brenner, S., Tirosh, Smith, A., 2012. An update on noise and performance: comment on Szalma and 3029
2944 E., 2013. The effects of exposure to environmental factors on heart rate vari- Hancock. Psychol. Bull. 138, 1262–1268. 3030
2945 ability: an ecological perspective. Environ. Pollut., http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Sora, I., Nishimon, K., Otsuki, S., 1986. Dexamethasone suppression test and nora- 3031
2946 j.envpol.2013.02.005. drenergic function in affective and schizophrenic disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 21, 3032
2947 Scholey, A., Haskell, C., Robertson, B., Kennedy, D., Milne, A., Wetherell, M., 2009. 621–631. 3033
2948 Chewing gum alleviates negative mood and reduces cortisol during acute labo- Stalder, T., Evans, P., Hucklebridge, F., Clow, A., 2011. Associations between the cor- 3034
2949 ratory psychological stress. Physiol. Behav. 97, 304–312. tisol awakening response and heart rate variability. Psychoneuroendocrinology 3035
2950 Scholz, U., La Marca, R., Nater, U.M., Aberle, I., Ehlert, U., Hornung, R., Martin, M., 36, 454–462. 3036
2951 Kliegel, M., 2009. Go no-go performance under psychosocial stress: beneficial Starcke, K., Brand, M., 2012. Decision making under stress: a selective review. Neu- 3037
2952 effects of implementation intentions. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 91, 89–92. rosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1228–1248. 3038
2953 Schommer, N., Kudielka, B., Hellhammer, D., Kirschbaum, C., 1999. No evidence for Stauble, M.R., Thompson, L.A., Morgan, G., 2013. Increases in cortisol are positively 3039
2954 a close relationship between personality traits and circadian cortisol rhythm or associated with gains in encoding and maintenance working memory perfor- 3040
2955 a single cortisol stress response. Psychol. Rep. 84, 840–842. mance in young men. Stress 16, 402–410. 3041
2956 Schommer, N.C., Hellhammer, D.H., Kirschbaum, C., 2003. Dissociation between Stein, M.B., Yehuda, R., Koverola, C., Hanna, C., 1997. Enhanced dexamethasone sup- 3042
2957 reactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic- pression of plasma cortisol in adult women traumatized by childhood sexual 3043
2958 adrenal-medullary system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosom. Med. 65, abuse. Biol. Psychiatry 42, 680–686. 3044
2959 450–460. Steptoe, A., Willemsen, G., Owen, N., Flower, L., Mohamed-Ali, V., 2001. Acute mental 3045
2960 Schoofs, D., Wolf, O.T., 2011. Are salivary gonadal steroid concentrations influenced stress elicits delayed increases in circulating inflammatory cytokine levels. Clin. 3046
2961 by acute psychosocial stress? A study using the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). Sci. 101, 185–192. 3047
2962 Int. J. Psychophysiol. 80, 36–43. Steptoe, A., Hamer, M., Chida, Y., 2007. The effects of acute psychological stress on 3048
2963 Schoofs, D., Preuß, D., Wolf, O.T., 2008. Psychosocial stress induces working memory circulating inflammatory factors in humans: a review and meta-analysis. Brain 3049
2964 impairments in an n-back paradigm. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 643–653. Behav. Immun. 21, 901–912. 3050
2965 Schubert, M., Contreras, C., Franz, N., Hellhammer, J., 2011. Milk-based phospho- Stewart, J.G., Mazurka, R., Bond, L., Wynne-Edwards, K.E., Harkness, K.L., 2013. Rumi- 3051
2966 lipids increase morning cortisol availability and improve memory in chronically nation and impaired cortisol recovery following a social stressor in adolescent 3052
2967 stressed men. Nutr. Res. 31, 413–420. depression. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol., 1–12. 3053
2968 Schuld, A., Kraus, T., Haack, M., Hinze-Selch, D., Zobel, A.W., Holsboer, F., Pollmächer, Strahler, J., Mueller, A., Rosenloecher, F., Kirschbaum, C., Rohleder, N., 2010. Salivary 3054
2969 T., 2001. Effects of dexamethasone on cytokine plasma levels and white blood ␣-amylase stress reactivity across different age groups. Psychophysiology 47, 3055
2970 cell counts in depressed patients. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 65–76. 587–595. 3056
2971 Schüle, C., Baghai, T.C., Eser, D., Häfner, S., Born, C., Herrmann, S., Rupprecht, R., 2009. Straube, T., Schmidt, S., Weiss, T., Mentzel, H., Miltner, W.H.R., 2009. Dynamic acti- 3057
2972 The combined dexamethasone/CRH Test (DEX/CRH test) and prediction of acute vation of the anterior cingulate cortex during anticipatory anxiety. Neuroimage 3058
2973 treatment response in major depression. PLoS ONE 4, e4324. 44, 975–981. 3059
2974 Schult, J., Hero, T., Hellhammer, J., 2010. Effects of powdered fertilized eggs on the Suárez-Hitz, K.A., Otto, B., Bidlingmaier, M., Schwizer, W., Fried, M., Ehlert, U., 2012. 3060
2975 stress response. Clin. Nutr. 29, 255–260. Altered psychobiological responsiveness in women with irritable bowel syn- 3061
2976 Schulz, S., Laessle, R.G., 2012. Stress-induced laboratory eating behavior in obese drome. Psychosom. Med. 74, 221–231. 3062
2977 women with binge eating disorder. Appetite 58, 457–461. Suarez, K., Mayer, C., Ehlert, U., Nater, U.M., 2010. Psychological stress and 3063
2978 Schwabe, L., Oitzl, M.S., Philippsen, C., Richter, S., Bohringer, A., Wippich, W., self-reported functional gastrointestinal disorders. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 198, 3064
2979 Schachinger, H., 2007. Stress modulates the use of spatial versus stimulus- 226–229. 3065
2980 response learning strategies in humans. Learn. Mem. 14, 109–116. Sugaya, N., Izawa, S., Kimura, K., Ogawa, N., Yamada, C.K., Shirotsuki, K., Mikami, 3066
2981 Schwabe, L., Haddad, L., Schachinger, H., 2008. HPA axis activation by a socially I., Hirata, K., Nagano, Y., Nomura, S., 2012. Adrenal hormone response and 3067
2982 evaluated cold-pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 890–895. psychophysiological correlates under psychosocial stress in individuals with 3068
2983 Schwabe, L., Joëls, M., Roozendaal, B., Wolf, O.T., Oitzl, M.S., 2012. Stress effects on irritable bowel syndrome. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 84, 39–44. 3069
2984 memory: an update and integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1740–1749. Sumter, S.R., Bokhorst, C.L., Miers, A.C., Van Pelt, J., Westenberg, P.M., 2010. Age and 3070
2985 Scott, L.V., Dinan, T.G., 1998. Vasopressin and the regulation of hypothalamic- puberty differences in stress responses during a public speaking task: do ado- 3071
2986 pituitary-adrenal axis function: implications for the pathophysiology of lescents grow more sensitive to social evaluation? Psychoneuroendocrinology 3072
2987 depression. Life Sci. 62, 1985–1998. 35, 1510–1516. 3073
2988 Sesack, S.R., Hawrylak, V.A., Matus, C., Guido, M.A., Levey, A.I., 1998. Dopamine axon Takahashi, T., Ikeda, K., Ishikawa, M., Tsukasaki, T., Nakama, D., Tanida, S., Kameda, 3074
2989 varicosities in the prelimbic division of the rat prefrontal cortex exhibit sparse T., 2004. Social stress-induced cortisol elevation acutely impairs social memory 3075
2990 immunoreactivity for the dopamine transporter. J. Neurosci. 18, 2697–2708. in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 363, 125–130. 3076
2991 Shalev, I., Lerer, E., Israel, S., Uzefovsky, F., Gritsenko, I., Mankuta, D., Ebstein, R.P., Takahashi, T., Ikeda, K., Ishikawa, M., Kitamura, N., Tsukasaki, T., Nakama, D., Kameda, 3077
2992 Kaitz, M., 2009. BDNF Val66Met polymorphism is associated with HPA axis reac- T., 2005a. Anxiety, reactivity, and social stress-induced cortisol elevation in 3078
2993 tivity to psychological stress characterized by genotype and gender interactions. humans. Neuroendocrinol. Lett. 26, 351–354. 3079
2994 Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 382–388. Takahashi, T., Ikeda, K., Ishikawa, M., Kitamura, N., Tsukasaki, T., Nakama, D., Kameda, 3080
2995 Shalev, I., Israel, S., Uzefovsky, F., Gritsenko, I., Kaitz, M., Ebstein, R.P., 2011. T., 2005b. Interpersonal trust and social stress-induced cortisol elevation. Neu- 3081
2996 Vasopressin needs an audience: neuropeptide elicited stress responses are con- roreport 16, 197. 3082
2997 tingent upon perceived social evaluative threats. Horm. Behav. 60, 121–127. Tamashiro, K.L.K., Nguyen, M.M.N., Sakai, R.R., 2005. Social stress: from rodents to 3083
2998 Shanahan, F., 1999. Brain–gut axis and mucosal immunity: a perspective on mucosal primates. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 26, 27–40. 3084
2999 psychoneuroimmunology. Semin. Gastrointest. Dis., 8–13. Taylor, C.B., Conrad, A., Wilhelm, F.H., Neri, E., DeLorenzo, A., Kramer, M.A., Giese- 3085
3000 Shansis, F.M., Busnello, J.V., Quevedo, J., Forster, L., Young, S., Izquierdo, I., Kapczin- Davis, J., Roth, W.T., Oka, R., Cooke, J.P., 2006. Psychophysiological and cortisol 3086
3001 ski, F., 2000. Behavioural effects of acute tryptophan depletion in healthy male responses to psychological stress in depressed and nondepressed older men 3087
3002 volunteers. J. Psychopharm. 14, 157–163. and women with elevated cardiovascular disease risk. Psychosom. Med. 68, 3088
3003 Simeon, D., Knutelska, M., Yehuda, R., Putnam, F., Schmeidler, J., Smith, L.M., 2007a. 538–546. 3089
3004 Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function in dissociative disorders, PTSD, Taylor, S.E., Welch, W.T., Kim, H.S., Sherman, D.K., 2007. Cultural differences in 3090
3005 and healthy volunteers. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 966. the impact of social support on psychological and biological stress responses. 3091
3006 Simeon, D., Yehuda, R., Cunill, R., Knutelska, M., Putnam, F.W., Smith, L.M., 2007b. Psychol. Sci. 18, 831–837. 3092
3007 Factors associated with resilience in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology Taylor, S.E., Seeman, T.E., Eisenberger, N.I., Kozanian, T.A., Moore, A.N., Moons, W.G., 3093
3008 32, 1149–1152. 2010. Effects of a supportive or an unsupportive audience on biological and 3094
3009 Sketchley-Kaye, K., Jenks, R., Miles, C., Johnson, A.J., 2011. Chewing gum modifies psychological responses to stress. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 47–56. 3095
3010 state anxiety and alertness under conditions of social stress. Nutr. Neurosci. 14, Thoma, M.V., Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, J.M., Rohleder, N., 2012. Acute stress responses 3096
3011 237–242. in salivary alpha-amylase predict increases of plasma norepinephrine. Biol. Psy- 3097
3012 Slattery, M.J., Grieve, A.J., Ames, M.E., Armstrong, J.M., Essex, M.J., 2013. Neurocog- chol. 91, 342–348. 3098
3013 nitive function and state cognitive stress appraisal predict cortisol reactivity to Tomiyama, A.J., Schamarek, I., Lustig, R.H., Kirschbaum, C., Puterman, E., Havel, P.J., 3099
3014 an acute psychosocial stressor in adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, Epel, E.S., 2012. Leptin concentrations in response to acute stress predict subse- 3100
3015 1318–1327. quent intake of comfort foods. Physiol. Behav. 107, 34–39. 3101
3016 Slavich, G.M., Way, B.M., Eisenberger, N.I., Taylor, S.E., 2010. Neural sensitivity to Troisi, A., 1999. Ethological research in clinical psychiatry: the study of nonverbal 3102
3017 social rejection is associated with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proc. behavior during interviews. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 905–913. 3103
3018 Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 14817–14822. Trueba, A.F., Mizrachi, D., Auchus, R.J., Vogel, P.D., Ritz, T., 2012. Effects of psy- 3104
3019 Smeets, T., Cornelisse, S., Quaedflieg, C.W., Meyer, T., Jelicic, M., Merckelbach, chosocial stress on the pattern of salivary protein release. Physiol. Behav. 105, 3105
3020 H., 2012. Introducing the Maastricht Acute Stress Test (MAST): a quick and 841–849. 3106
3021 non-invasive approach to elicit robust autonomic and glucocorticoid stress Turnbull, A.V., Rivier, C.L., 1999. Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal 3107
3022 responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1998–2008. axis by cytokines: actions and mechanisms of action. Physiol. Rev. 79, 1–71. 3108
3023 Smeets, T., Jelicic, M., Merckelbach, H., 2006. The effect of acute stress on memory Tyrka, A.R., Price, L.H., Gelernter, J., Schepker, C., Anderson, G.M., Carpenter, L.L., 3109
3024 depends on word valence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 62, 30–37. 2009. Interaction of childhood maltreatment with the corticotropin-releasing 3110
3025 Smeets, T., Wolf, O.T., Giesbrecht, T., Sijstermans, K., Telgen, S., Joëls, M., 2009. Stress hormone receptor gene: effects on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis reac- 3111
3026 selectively and lastingly promotes learning of context-related high arousing tivity. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 681–685. 3112
3027 information. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1152–1161. Uhart, M., McCaul, M., Oswald, L., Choi, L., Wand, G., 2004. GABRA6 gene polymor- 3113
3028 Smith, A., 2000. The scale of perceived occupational stress. Occup. Med. 50, 294–298. phism and an attenuated stress response. Mol. Psychiatry 9, 998–1006. 3114

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005
ARTICLE IN PRESS
G Model
NBR 1866 1–31

A.P. Allen et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xxx (2013) xxx–xxx 31

3115 Uhart, M., Chong, R.Y., Oswald, L., Lin, P.I., Wand, G.S., 2006. Gender differences in Wiemers, U.S., Sauvage, M.M., Schoofs, D., Hamacher-Dang, T.C., Wolf, O.T., 2013a. 3190
3116 hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinol- What we remember from a stressful episode. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 3191
3117 ogy 31, 642–652. 2268–2277. 3192
3118 Unternaehrer, E., Luers, P., Mill, J., Dempster, E., Meyer, A.H., Staehli, S., Lieb, R., Hell- Wiemers, U.S., Schoofs, D., Wolf, O.T., 2013b. A friendly version of the Trier Social 3193
3119 hammer, D.H., Meinlschmidt, G., 2012. Dynamic changes in DNA methylation Stress Test does not activate the HPA axis in healthy men and women. Stress 16, 3194
3120 of stress-associated genes (OXTR, BDNF) after acute psychosocial stress. Transl. 254–260. 3195
3121 Psychiatry 2, e150. Williams, R.A., Hagerty, B.M., Brooks, G., 2004. Trier Social Stress Test: a method for 3196
3122 Van den Bos, R., Harteveld, M., Stoop, H., 2009. Stress and decision-making in use in nursing research. Nurs. Res. 53, 277–280. 3197
3123 humans: performance is related to cortisol reactivity, albeit differently in men Williamson, S., Munro, C., Pickler, R., Grap, M.J., Elswick, R., 2012. Comparison of 3198
3124 and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1449–1458. biomarkers in blood and saliva in healthy adults. Nurs. Res. Pract. 2012. 3199
3125 Van Donkelaar, E., Blokland, A., Ferrington, L., Kelly, P., Steinbusch, H., Prickaerts, Wirtz, P.H., Elsenbruch, S., Emini, L., Rüdisüli, K., Groessbauer, S., Ehlert, U., 2007a. 3200
3126 J., 2011. Mechanism of acute tryptophan depletion: is it only serotonin? Mol. Perfectionism and the cortisol response to psychosocial stress in men. Psycho- 3201
3127 Psychiatry 16, 695–713. som. Med. 69, 249–255. 3202
3128 van Laerhoven, H., van der Zaag-Loonen, H., Derkx, B., 2004. A comparison of Likert Wirtz, P.H., von Känel, R., Emini, L., Suter, T., Fontana, A., Ehlert, U., 2007b. Varia- 3203
3129 scale and visual analogue scales as response options in children’s questionnaires. tions in anticipatory cognitive stress appraisal and differential proinflammatory 3204
3130 Acta Paediatr. 93, 830–835. cytokine expression in response to acute stress. Brain Behav. Immun. 21, 3205
3131 van Leeuwen, N., Bellingrath, S., de Kloet, E.R., Zitman, F.G., DeRijk, R.H., Kudielka, 851–859. 3206
3132 B.M., Wüst, S., 2011. Human mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) gene haplo- Wolf, O.T., 2012. Immediate recall influences the effects of pre-encoding stress on 3207
3133 types modulate MR expression and transactivation: Implication for the stress emotional episodic long-term memory consolidation in healthy young men. 3208
3134 response. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 699–709. Stress 15, 272–280. 3209
3135 van Marle, H.J., Hermans, E.J., Qin, S., Fernández, G., 2009. From specificity to sensitiv- Wolf, J.M., Rohleder, N., Bierhaus, A., Nawroth, P.P., Kirschbaum, C., 2009. Deter- 3210
3136 ity: how acute stress affects amygdala processing of biologically salient stimuli. minants of the NF-␬B response to acute psychosocial stress in humans. Brain 3211
3137 Biol. Psychiatry 66, 649–655. Behav. Immun. 23, 742–749. 3212
3138 van Strien, T., Roelofs, K., de Weerth, C., 2012. Cortisol reactivity and distress-induced Wüst, S., Wolf, J., Hellhammer, D.H., Federenko, I., Schommer, N., Kirschbaum, C., 3213
3139 emotional eating. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 677–684. 2000. The cortisol awakening response-normal values and confounds. Noise 3214
3140 van Veen, J.F., van Vliet, I.M., de Rijk, R.H., van Pelt, J., Mertens, B., Fekkes, D., Zit- Health 2, 79. 3215
3141 man, F.G., 2009. Tryptophan depletion affects the autonomic stress response in Wüst, S., van Rossum, E.F.C., Federenko, I.S., Koper, J.W., Kumsta, R., Hellham- 3216
3142 generalized social anxiety disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1590–1594. mer, D.H., 2004. Common polymorphisms in the glucocorticoid receptor gene 3217
3143 Van Vugt, M.K., Hitchcock, P., Shahar, B., Britton, W., 2012. The effects of mindfulness- are associated with adrenocortical responses to psychosocial stress. J. Clin. 3218
3144 based cognitive therapy on affective memory recall dynamics in depression: a Endocrinol. Metab. 89, 565–573. 3219
3145 mechanistic model of rumination. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 257. Wüst, S., Federenko, I.S., van Rossum, E.F.C., Koper, J.W., Hellhammer, D.H., 2005. 3220
3146 Veith, R.C., Lewis, N., Linares, O.A., Barnes, R.F., Raskind, M.A., Villacres, E.C., Mur- Habituation of cortisol responses to repeated psychosocial stress—further 3221
3147 burg, M.M., Ashleigh, E.A., Castillo, S., Peskind, E.R., 1994. Sympathetic nervous characterization and impact of genetic factors. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 3222
3148 system activity in major depression: basal and desipramine-induced alterations 199–211. 3223
3149 in plasma norepinephrine kinetics. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 411. Wüst, S., Kumsta, R., Treutlein, J., Frank, J., Entringer, S., Schulze, T.G., Rietschel, 3224
3150 Vickers, K., Jafarpour, S., Mofidi, A., Rafat, B., Woznica, A., 2012. The 35% carbon M., 2009. Sex-specific association between the 5-HTT gene-linked polymorphic 3225
3151 dioxide test in stress and panic research: overview of effects and integration of region and basal cortisol secretion. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 972–982. 3226
3152 findings. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 153–164. Xhyheri, B., Manfrini, O., Mazzolini, M., Pizzi, C., Bugiardini, R., 2012. Heart rate 3227
3153 von Bardeleben, U., Holsboer, F., 1989. Cortisol response to a combined variability today. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 55, 321–331. 3228
3154 dexamethasone-human corticotrophin-releasing hormone challenge in Yamakawa, K., Matsunaga, M., Isowa, T., Kimura, K., Kasugai, K., Yoneda, M., Kaneko, 3229
3155 patients with depression. J. Neuroendocrinol. 1, 485–488. H., Ohira, H., 2009. Transient responses of inflammatory cytokines in acute 3230
3156 von Dawans, B., Kirschbaum, C., Heinrichs, M., 2011. The Trier Social Stress Test for stress. Biol. Psychol. 82, 25–32. 3231
3157 Groups (TSST-G): a new research tool for controlled simultaneous social stress Yehuda, R., Halligan, S.L., Grossman, R., Golier, J., Wong, C., 2002. The cortisol and 3232
3158 exposure in a group format. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 514–522. glucocorticoid receptor response to low dose dexamethasone administration in 3233
3159 von Känel, R., Kudielka, B.M., Preckel, D., Hanebuth, D., Fischer, J.E., 2006. Delayed aging combat veterans and holocaust survivors with and without posttraumatic 3234
3160 response and lack of habituation in plasma interleukin-6 to acute mental stress stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 393–403. 3235
3161 in men. Brain Behav. Immun. 20, 40. Yerevanian, B.I., Woolf, P.D., Iker, H.P., 1983. Plasma ACTH levels in depression before 3236
3162 von Känel, R., Kudielka, B.M., Metzenthin, P., Helfricht, S., Preckel, D., Haeberli, and after recovery: relationship to the dexamethasone suppression test. Psychi- 3237
3163 A., Stutz, M., Fischer, J.E., 2008. Aspirin, but not propranolol, attenuates the atry Res. 10, 175–181. 3238
3164 acute stress-induced increase in circulating levels of interleukin-6: a ran- Yim, I.S., Quas, J.A., Cahill, L., Hayakawa, C.M., 2010. Children’s and adults’ salivary 3239
3165 domized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Brain Behav. Immun. 22, cortisol responses to an identical psychosocial laboratory stressor. Psychoneu- 3240
3166 150–157. roendocrinology 35, 241–248. 3241
3167 Wagner, J., Cik, M., Marth, E., Santner, B.I., Gallasch, E., Lackner, A., Raggam, R.B., 2010. Young, E.A., Lopez, J.F., Murphy-Weinberg, V., Watson, S.J., Akil, H., 2000. Hormonal 3242
3168 Feasibility of testing three salivary stress biomarkers in relation to naturalistic evidence for altered responsiveness to social stress in major depression. Neu- 3243
3169 traffic noise exposure. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 213, 153–155. ropsychopharmacology 23, 411–418. 3244
3170 Way, B.M., Taylor, S.E., 2010. The serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism Young, E.A., Abelson, J., Lightman, S.L., 2004a. Cortisol pulsatility and its role in stress 3245
3171 is associated with cortisol response to psychosocial stress. Biol. Psychiatry 67, regulation and health. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25, 69–76. 3246
3172 487–492. Young, E.A., Abelson, J.L., Cameron, O.G., 2004b. Effect of comorbid anxiety disorders 3247
3173 Weinshilboum, R.M., Otterness, D.M., Szumlanski, C.L., 1999. Methylation pharma- on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis response to a social stressor in major 3248
3174 cogenetics: catechol O-methyltransferase, thiopurine methyltransferase, and depression. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 113–120. 3249
3175 histamine N-methyltransferase. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 39, 19–52. Young, E.A., Abelson, J.L., Cameron, O.G., 2005. Interaction of brain noradrenergic 3250
3176 Weise, F., Laude, D., Girard, A., Zitoun, P., Siché, J., 1993. Effects of the cold pressor system and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in man. Psychoneu- 3251
3177 test on short-term fluctuations of finger arterial blood pressure and heart rate roendocrinology 30, 807–814. 3252
3178 in normal subjects. Clin. Auton. Res. 3, 303–310. Ziegler, M.G., 2012. Psychological stress and the autonomic nervous system. In: 3253
3179 Westenberg, P.M., Bokhorst, C.L., Miers, A.C., Sumter, S.R., Kallen, V.L., van Pelt, J., David, R., Italo, B., Geoffrey, B., Phillip, A.L., Julian, F.R.P. (Eds.), Primer on the 3254
3180 Blöte, A.W., 2009. A prepared speech in front of a pre-recorded audience: subjec- Autonomic Nervous System. , 3rd ed. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 291–293. 3255
3181 tive, physiological, and neuroendocrine responses to the Leiden Public Speaking Zobel, A.W., Yassouridis, A., Frieboes, R., Holsboer, F., 1999. Prediction of medium- 3256
3182 Task. Biol. Psychol. 82, 116–124. term outcome by cortisol response to the combined dexamethasone-CRH test 3257
3183 Wetherell, M.A., Sidgreaves, M.C., 2005. Secretory immunoglobulin-A reactivity fol- in patients with remitted depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 949–951. 3258
3184 lowing increases in workload intensity using the Defined Intensity Stressor Zobel, A.W., Nickel, T., Sonntag, A., Uhr, M., Holsboer, F., Ising, M., 2001. Cortisol 3259
3185 Simulation (DISS). Stress Health 21, 99–106. response in the combined dexamethasone/CRH test as predictor of relapse in 3260
3186 Wetherell, M.A., Crown, A.L., Lightman, S.L., Miles, J.N.V., Kaye, J., Vedhara, patients with remitted depression: a prospective study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 35, 3261
3187 K., 2006. The four-dimensional stress test: psychological, sympathetic- 83–94. 3262
3188 adrenal-medullary, parasympathetic and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal Zoccola, P.M., Quas, J., Yim, I.S., 2010. Salivary cortisol responses to a psychosocial 3263
3189 responses following inhalation of 35% CO2 . Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, laboratory stressor and later verbal recall of the stressor: the role of trait and 3264
736–747. state rumination. Stress 13, 435–443. 3265

Please cite this article in press as: Allen, A.P., et al., Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005

You might also like