You are on page 1of 9

Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Development of a Relational Rumination Questionnaire


Svenja Senkans a,⁎, Troy E. McEwan a, Jason Skues b, James R.P. Ogloff a
a
Swinburne University of Technology and Forensicare, Centre for Forensic Behavioural Science, 505 Hoddle Street, Clifton Hill VIC 3068, Australia
b
Swinburne University of Technology, Department of Psychological Sciences, Mail H99, PO Box 218, Hawthorn VIC 3122, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Rumination about romantic relationships has been implicated in interpersonal problems generally, and intimate
Received 19 August 2015 partner violence and stalking of former romantic partners specifically. While various scales exist to measure
Received in revised form 10 October 2015 depressive, angry, or general rumination, no existing scale comprehensively assesses rumination on romantic re-
Accepted 12 October 2015
lationships. This paper describes the development and validation of the Relational Rumination Questionnaire
Available online 24 October 2015
(RelRQ). The RelRQ was developed and tested across two studies involving university students and members
Keywords:
of the general population. Study 1 (n = 578) used exploratory factor analyses to develop an 18-item RelRQ
Rumination from a larger item pool. The derived three-factor structure: 1) romantic preoccupation rumination; 2) relation-
Romantic relationships ship uncertainty rumination; and 3) break-up rumination was confirmed in Study 2 (n = 525), and the scale
Adult attachment was revised to a 16-item version. Total RelRQ and subscale scores showed high internal consistency, good
Stalking test–retest reliability, and expected correlations with related constructs such as insecure attachment, anger
Intimate partner violence rumination, and negative affect. Results indicate that the RelRQ can be used in future studies to test if relational
Scale rumination is associated with maladaptive relational outcomes such as intimate partner violence and stalking.
Exploratory factor analysis (EFA)
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)

1. Introduction interpersonal transgressions and what these mean for the self
(McCullough, Bono, & Root, 2007; Sukhodolsky, Golub, & Cromwell,
Success in romantic relationships is thought to be a culturally 2001; Wade, Vogel, Liao, & Goldman, 2008). Another commonly
prescribed life goal (DePaulo & Morris, 2006) and a salient developmen- recognised feature of ruminative thinking is its repetitive nature; it is
tal task in emerging adulthood (Roisman, Masten, Coatsworth, & also experienced as difficult to control, and difficult to disengage from
Tellegen, 2004). Accordingly, unrequited love, conflict in romantic (Ehring et al., 2011; Ingram, 1990). Rumination is perceived as unpro-
relationships, relational uncertainty, and relationship dissolution and ductive and non-instrumental, while nevertheless occupying significant
reconciliation are major concerns for many people (e.g., Afifi & mental capacity (Ehring et al., 2011; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991).
Reichert, 1996; Aron, Aron, & Allen, 1998; Boelen & van den Hout, While ruminative thinking can occur in response to a particular neg-
2010). Various authors have highlighted that ruminative thinking ative life event or relational transgression and so can be state-like
about such issues can influence adjustment when relationship goals (Kirkegaard Thomsen, 2006; Wade et al., 2008), research has demon-
are frustrated (e.g., Boelen & Reijntjes, 2009; Cupach, Spitzberg, strated that ruminative thinking can also be characterised as a trait-
Bolingbroke, & Tellitocci, 2011; Cupach, Spitzberg, & Carson, 2000; like response style (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). A ruminative response
Saffrey & Ehrenberg, 2007; Sotelo & Babcock, 2013). style is thought to be a characteristic way of thinking that involves
Rumination is characterised as a form of negative self-focused focussing on a problem (including negative emotional states), while
attention that is observed across various types of psychopathology and inhibiting actions or thoughts that may either distract from the problem
problematic behaviour (Ehring et al., 2011; Ingram, 1990). Although or contribute to a solution (Ehring et al., 2011; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991).
the content of ruminative thinking may differ greatly, it tends to focus The evidence to date suggests that rumination has a negative feedback
on issues that are self-relevant (Ingram, 1990) in internal domains relationship with affect, meaning that rumination may be caused by
(i.e., self, mood), external domains (i.e., events related to the self), or negative emotional states, maintain such states, or both (for a review
both (Kirkegaard Thomsen, 2006). For example, depressive rumination see Kirkegaard Thomsen, 2006). This is one reason why rumination
is internal and self-degrading (Ingram, 1990; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991), has been implicated in a wide variety of negative psychological out-
whereas angry rumination typically focuses on external events of comes, including depression, anger, jealousy, and anxiety (e.g., Aldao,
Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010; Carson & Cupach, 2000;
⁎ Corresponding author.
Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991; Sukhodolsky et al., 2001).
E-mail addresses: ssenkans@swin.edu.au (S. Senkans), tmcewan@swin.edu.au Some authors have pointed to the need to further investigate rumi-
(T.E. McEwan), jskues@swin.edu.au (J. Skues), jogloff@swin.edu.au (J.R.P. Ogloff). nation on different themes (e.g., Kirkegaard Thomsen, 2006). In the

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.10.032
0191-8869/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
28 S. Senkans et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35

specific area of romantic relationships, there has been some interest in example, a person who ruminates about their current relationship
how rumination may be involved in emotional maladjustment in differ- may also ruminate about an ex-partner, while someone who recently
ent relational contexts (e.g., Reynolds, Searight, & Ratwik, 2014; Saffrey experienced a breakup may ruminate about this but also about acquir-
& Ehrenberg, 2007; Tran & Joormann, 2015). More specifically, rumina- ing a new relationship. This is consistent with the attachment literature,
tive thinking is thought to be associated with maladjustment at all three which suggests that a pervasive concern surrounding all types of
stages of a relationship: during unsuccessful relationship pursuit, during relationship problems is an important cognitive aspect of anxious
romantic relationships, and after relationship dissolution. attachment (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007).
Unrequited love is a concern for many (Aron et al., 1998; Baumeister, While relationship rumination would appear to be a useful topic for
Wotman, & Stillwell, 1993), but the way individuals think about further research, at present there are no measures of the construct that
unreciprocated love and/or sexual attraction is thought to influence have been carefully validated. Existing scales are either highly specific
adjustment to frustration of relationship goals. For instance, some indi- (e.g., Facebook rumination after a breakup; Tran & Joormann, 2015)
viduals may ruminate about why the other person was not interested and/or were not subjected to comprehensive validity and reliability
and the implications this has for their sense of self. This is thought to analyses (e.g., Cupach et al., 2011; Davis et al., 2003; Saffrey &
lead to romantic goal preoccupation associated with negative affect, Ehrenberg, 2007; Spitzberg et al., 2014; Tran & Joormann, 2015). Only
relationship-focused ruminative thoughts, and behaviours intended to Spitzberg et al. (2014) reported the internal structure of their rumina-
attain relational goals (Yanowitz, 2006). This may be particularly true tion measure using exploratory principle component analysis, however
for those who link the goal of attaining a romantic relationship to their measure only included rumination about breakups. While a vali-
higher-order goals such as life happiness (Cupach et al., 2000; Martin, dated scale exists to measure romantic preoccupation, it is lengthy
Tesser, & McIntosh, 1993). Very little research has investigated these and also includes emotional (e.g., “I do not feel depressed when I
theoretical ideas empirically. think about my lack of romantic relationships”) and behavioural
Research has also suggested that thinking about established (e.g., “I tend to scan my social environment for potential romantic rela-
relationships in a ruminative way can have negative implications for re- tionships”) components of romantic goal preoccupation (Yanowitz,
lationship functioning. For example, in a series of longitudinal studies, 2006). No existing scale specifically measures rumination about
McCullough et al. (2007) found that rumination was negatively associ- ongoing relationships. It could be argued that a measure of anxious
ated with forgiveness of relationship transgressions. Recent couple- attachment may suffice to capture relationship rumination, however;
based research has identified a vicious cycle of passive dyadic coping, attachment research and theories often conflate cognitive, affective,
of which rumination by one partner and withdrawal by the other are and behavioural domains of attachment (Holmes, 2000), and can there-
interdependent components (D. B. King & DeLongis, 2014). Rumination fore not measure the construct of relationship rumination as relevant to
may also be associated with aggressive behaviours in a romantic different relationship contexts.
relationship context. The few studies that have investigated rumination Given the shortcomings in existing measures, the aim of the current
in intimate partner violence (IPV) found that increased levels of rumina- research was to develop and validate a brief, multi-faceted, self-report
tive thinking were positively associated with IPV perpetration (Sotelo & measure of romantic relationship rumination. The aim of Study 1 was
Babcock, 2013; Watkins, DiLillo, & Maldonado, 2015). to establish the factor structure of the newly developed measure using
Attachment theorists have also highlighted the link between rumi- exploratory factor analysis (EFA). The aim of Study 2 was to replicate
native thinking and interpersonal problems. Rumination is one of a the measure's factor structure using confirmatory factor analysis
number of self-defeating strategies thought to maintain a “self-amplify- (CFA), and to evaluate test-retest reliability and convergent validity.
ing cycle of distress”, which renders threats to the attachment system
cognitively accessible long after they have dissipated (Mikulincer & 2. Study 1 — exploratory factor analysis
Shaver, 2008, p. 520). From this perspective, relational rumination is
one aspect of anxious attachment. There is also some preliminary The aims of Study 1 were a) to explore the factor structure of the
evidence that rumination may mediate the link between insecure attach- newly developed RelRQ; b) evaluate internal consistency (Cronbach's
ment and a number of poor interpersonal outcomes (e.g., Burnette, Davis, alpha) and preliminary convergent validity; and c) revise and shorten
Green, Worthington, & Bradfield, 2009; Chung, 2014; Reynolds et al., the RelRQ. This study was approved by the Monash University Human
2014), although no studies have specifically examined the link between Research Ethics Committee. Informed consent was obtained from all
rumination about relationships and anxious attachment. participants.
In addition to contributing to maladjustment during relationship
pursuit and dissatisfaction during relationships, rumination has been 2.1. Sample
linked to maladjustment after relationship dissolution (Davis, Shaver,
& Vernon, 2003; Saffrey & Ehrenberg, 2007; Tran & Joormann, 2015), in- The sample consisted of 578 participants including volunteers who
cluding post-separation stalking. Cupach et al. (2000) suggested that ru- answered advertisements on an Australian university news website
mination on relational goals and preoccupation with the ex-partner (n = 258) and participants recruited by a market research company
may be a central contributing factor in ex-partner stalking and relational (n = 320). The former group received no compensation for completing
intrusion (see also: Cupach et al., 2011). Many studies have indicated the online survey while those recruited by the market research compa-
that general rumination, anger rumination, and partner preoccupation ny were compensated according to the company's policies with an
are all associated with engaging in post-relationship relational intrusion amount unknown to the researchers. Participants were between 18
(Cupach et al., 2011; De Smet, Uzieblo, Loeys, Buysse, & Onraedt, 2015; and 80 years old (M = 38.44, SD = 15.82). Two-hundred and twelve
Marquez, 2013; Spitzberg, Cupach, Hannawa, & Crowley, 2014). participants were male (36.7%), 363 female (62.8), and three (0.5%)
Most of the research conducted on rumination seems to suggest that did not specify their gender. Most identified as Australian (64.7%, n =
it is most commonly conceptualised as a trait variable (with the excep- 368) including 1% (n = 6) who were Aboriginal or Torres Strait Islander;
tion of Wade et al., 2008). It can be assumed that rumination on 13.9% (n = 81) identified as Asian; 13.6% (n = 79) European; 1.4% (n =
relationship attainment would be most salient while single, while rumi- 8) New Zealand; 0.7% (n = 4) Middle Eastern/North African; 0.5% (n =
nation about an ongoing relationship or a breakup would be most sa- 3) South or East African; and 0.2% (n = 1) North American. Region of or-
lient while in a relationship or after a breakup, respectively. However, igin was unspecified for 4.8% (n = 28). Eighty-five percent (n = 492)
people who have a trait-like tendency to ruminate on relationships reported English as their first language.
may do so irrespective of their relationship status, meaning that differ- Thirty-five percent (n = 202) were single at the time of participation;
ent types of relationship rumination would be highly correlated. For the remaining 65% (n = 376) had partners (dating, in a relationship,
S. Senkans et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35 29

engaged, married). Most participants (78.4%, n = 453) reported previous relational rumination. Model 2 was a two-factor model comprised of
relationships. The majority (72.8%, n = 421) had experienced a breakup rumination about relationship (re)acquisition and relationship uncer-
before, 17.1% (n = 99) had not and 10.3% (n = 58) did not specify. tainty. Model 3 encompassed three factors based on the literature
reviewed above: romantic preoccupation, relationship uncertainty,
2.2. Measures and rumination about breakup(s).
It was hypothesised that there would be significant and large
2.2.1. Relational Rumination Questionnaire (RelRQ) correlations between the PTQ and the RelRQ. We also tested some hy-
Possible “topics” for relationship rumination were identified from potheses regarding the factor structure of the RelRQ and demographic
the extant attachment, close relationships, and social cognition litera- variables, which are further detailed in Section 2.5.4 following presenta-
tures: loneliness, romantic preoccupation (i.e., establishing a romantic tion of the factor structure identified using EFA.
relationship), abandonment/rejection, jealousy, and breakup. Fifty
items were developed measuring these topics (10 per topic) and admin- 2.4. Analyses
istered online in random order. Thirty-eight items were generated inde-
pendently by the first and second author, while remaining items were All analyses were conducted using MPlus Version 7.11 (Muthén &
adapted from published scales. All items, including adapted items, Muthén, 2013) unless otherwise specified. An EFA was performed on
reflected central aspects of ruminative thinking such as repetitiveness the full 50-item pool to test the goodness-of-fit of the three candidate
(e.g., “I repeatedly think about …”), difficult to control (e.g., “The thought models to identify the optimal model in the set (Preacher, Zhang, Kim,
that … plagues my mind”), or unproductiveness (e.g., “I contemplate … & Mels, 2013). Item responses were not normally distributed and the re-
without arriving at any conclusions”). Two loneliness items were based sponse scale used for the RelRQ is ordinal. Thus, the appropriate estima-
on content from Russel, Peplau, and Cutrona's (1980) Revised UCLA tion method used in all analyses was WLSMV (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-
Loneliness Scale. Five romantic preoccupation items were adapted 2012). This estimation method outperforms other robust estimation
from the scale described in Yanowitz's (2006) thesis. Wording for two methods for skewed, ordinal variables (Li, 2014). Based on the literature
abandonment/rejection items was adapted from measures of anxious at- review, we expected the factors to be highly correlated and selected the
tachment (Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000) and three items in the jeal- default oblique rotation method (GEOMIN). The χ2-test indicates the
ousy domain were adapted from existing scales of cognitive jealousy or exact goodness-of-fit of a model (Barrett, 2007) but often rejects
anxious jealousy (Buunk, 1997; Elphinston, Feeney, & Noller, 2011). models that show acceptable to good approximate fit (Bentler, 2007;
Items were piloted in two focus groups of university students (total Goffin, 2007; Markland, 2007; Miles & Shevlin, 2007; Steiger, 2007).
n = 10) to assess if they were understandable. This resulted in slight Thus approximate fit indices were also used for model assessments. Ac-
modifications (mostly rewording) of 41.6% of the item pool. Each item cording to Hu and Bentler (1999), a Comparative Fit Index (CFI; Bentler,
was rated on a five point Likert-type scale from almost never to almost 1990) above .95, a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA)
always. under .06 and a Standardised Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) under
Participants in the validation sample read the following instructions .08 suggest good approximate fit of the model and the observed data. In
prior to completing the questionnaire: “In this questionnaire you will be the CFA, using WLSMV, MPlus computes the Weighted Root Mean
asked to describe how frequently you typically think about negative Square Residual (WRMR) instead of the SRMR. A WRMR under .90
feelings and experiences in relationships. Please read the following indicates good fit (Yu, 2002).
statements carefully then rate how often (almost never, rarely, some-
times, often, almost always) the statement applies to you. There are 2.5. Results
no right or wrong answers as everyone's thinking and emotions are dif-
ferent. We are interested in how you generally experience relationship 2.5.1. Exploratory factor analyses
problems, not just what is happening in a current relationship. If you've All χ2 tests were significant, indicating that none of the three models
never had a romantic relationship or never experienced certain was an exact fit to the data. However, based on the indices shown in
relationship events, please answer according to how you imagine you Table 1, a three factor model was the best approximate fit to the data
would be in a relationship. If you are currently in a relationship but for both the full item pool. Table 1 also shows fit indices of the revised
you are answering a question relating to your thoughts when you are 18-item version that is subsequently described in more detail.
single, please try to recall your usual thoughts/actions when you are The GEOMIN rotated loadings were examined to interpret the factor
single before answering.” structure and the three factors were labelled romantic preoccupation
rumination (RP), relationship uncertainty rumination (RU), and break-
2.2.2. Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ) up rumination (BU). Fifteen items loaded on the RP factor, including
The PTQ (Ehring et al., 2011) was used to assess convergent validity
in a sub-sample of 556 participants (22 participants completed the pro-
Table 1
tocol prior to the addition of the PTQ). The 15-item PTQ is a measure of
Model Fit Information for exploratory factor analyses.
general ruminative thinking style and comprises statements such as
“Thoughts intrude into my mind”, which are scored on a 5-point Model χ2 df RMSEA SRMR CFI
Likert-type scale ranging from 0 = never to 4 = almost always. The 1 6665.20 1175 .091 [.089, .093] .087 .897
PTQ has reported internal consistency of α = .94 to .95 and test–retest p b .001
reliability of rtt = .69. In the current study, a 4 point scale ranging from 2 4467.73 1126 .072 [.070, .075] .058 .937
p b .001
0 = almost never to 4 = almost always was used and item 3 was
3a 2886.77 1078 .054 [.052, .057] .041 .966
changed from “I can't stop dwelling on them” to “I can't stop dwelling p b .001
on my thoughts” as the PTQ and RelRQ items were administered in a 3b 430.84 102 .075 [0.068, 0.083] .024 .984
combined, randomised item pool. In the current sample internal consis- p b .001
tency of the PTQ was α = .96. Note: Model 1 = single common factor model of full item pool; Model 2 = two factor
model (relationship uncertainty and relationship (re)acquisition) of full item pool;
2.3. Hypotheses Model 3a = three factor model (romantic preoccupation, relationship uncertainty, and
breakup rumination) of full item pool; Model 3b = three factor model of 18-item revised
version. χ2 = WLSMV Chi-Square test; df = degrees of freedom; RMSEA = Root Mean
Multiple models for the RelRQ were hypothesised a priori. Model 1 Square Error of Approximation; numbers in brackets are 95% confidence intervals of
was a one-factor model with all items loading on a single factor: RMSEA; SRMR = Standardised Root Mean Square Residual; CFI = Comparative Fit Index.
30 S. Senkans et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35

Table 2
Obliquely rotated relationship rumination questionnaire factor loadings.

Item F1 F2 F3

2 Thoughts about how to find a partner plague my mind. .92


7 I keep on wondering why my friends have romantic relationships and I don't.a .87
12 I constantly compare myself to others to understand why they have romantic relationships and I don't.a .86
16 I think about how to find a romantic relationship to avoid ending up alone. .86
1 I think of strategies to get into a romantic relationship over and over again.a .83
10 Thoughts about why I am not in relationship pop into my head without me wanting them to. .76 .16
4 The thought of my partner sleeping with somebody else crosses my mind.b .97
5 Thoughts about my partner cheating on me stress me out. .95
17 I imagine my partner cheating on me even though I don't want to. .93
11 I get caught up in imagining scenarios in which my partner would cheat on me. .88
13 Nagging doubts about my partner's faithfulness pop up in my mind. .88
8 I keep thinking that other people are interested in my partner. .74
3 I have repetitive thoughts about losing my partner. .72 .11
18 I wish I could stop thinking about my ex-partner(s), but I can't. −.12 .99
9 I think about how I should have prevented the break-up with an ex-partner. .87
6 I go over and over the reasons why my relationship(s) with my ex-partner(s) ended. .84
15 Thoughts about my ex-partner(s) distract me from other things I should be doing. .82
14 I think over and over again about how to re-establish the relationship with my ex-partner. .10 .78

Note: F1 = romantic preoccupation rumination (RP); F2 = relationship uncertainty rumination (RU); F3 = breakup rumination (BU). All loadings significant at p b .05.
Bold indicates primary loadings.
a
Adapted from Yanowitz (2006).
b
Adapted from Buunk (1997).

nine of the originally developed romantic preoccupation items and six singles would score higher on the RP factor. We expected these results
loneliness items. Sixteen items loaded on the RU factor, including six as the trait tendency to ruminate on relationship should be most
original rejection/abandonment items and all jealousy items. Eight of expressed as relevant to the participant's current relationship status.
the break-up items loaded on the BU factor. The remaining 11 items We did not have any directional hypotheses about BU factor scores. De-
were removed because the factor matrix (item-factor correlations) scriptive statistics, Spearman's ρ correlations, and t-tests computed
indicated that they were moderately correlated with all three factors. with SPSS 22.0 (IBM Corp., 2013) were used to assess the relationship
Factor loadings, item-factor correlations, variances, means, α-if- between RelRQ total and factor scores and age, gender, and relationship
item-deleted, and item content were investigated to decide which status.
other items to remove from the scale. Based on this information, anoth- These hypotheses were partly supported. As expected, RelRQ re-
er 21 items were deleted for psychometric and/or content reasons. The sponses varied between genders. Women scored significantly higher
revised RelRQ included six romantic preoccupation items on Factor 1, on the total RelRQ (M = 33.80, SD = 13.70) and RU (M = 13.14,
six jealousy items and one rejection/abandonment item on Factor 2, SD = 6.33) than men (RelRQ: M = 31.46, SD = 13.00,
and five break-up rumination items on Factor 3. Some loneliness t(568) = − 2.01, p b .05, d = 0.18; RU: M = 11.91, SD = 5.42,
items had high loadings on F1, but all were considered redundant and t(497.266) = −1.23, p b .05, d = 0.21), but with small effects (Cohen,
were removed from the revised RelRQ. Interestingly, many of the rejec- 1988). There were no significant gender differences on the RP and BU
tion/abandonment items showed moderate to high correlations with all subscales. The second hypothesis was not confirmed. Single individuals
factors, suggesting that they measured a broader underlying construct had significantly higher total scores and higher scores on all three sub-
such as attachment anxiety, and so were removed from the RelRQ. scales (RelRQ: M = 38.16, SD = 14.28, t(353.87) = 6.77, p b .05; RP:
M = 14.40, SD = 5.56, t(353.60) = 9.43, p b .05; RU: M = 13.40,
2.5.2. Exploratory factor analyses — revised SD = 6.08, t(576) = 2.12, p b .05; BU: M = 10.34, SD = 5.00,
Another EFA was conducted on the revised scale. As shown in t(319.77) = 6.36, p b .05) than partnered people (RelRQ: M = 30.11,
Table 1, the three-factor model showed good fit using the revised SD = 12.11; RP: M = 10.04, SD = 4.71; RU: M = 12.28, SD = 9.95;
measure. As evident in Table 2, the EFA resulted in a meaningful and BU: M = 7.78, SD = 3.68). This effect was large for the RP subscale
interpretable factor structure.1 (d = 0.85), moderate for RelRQ total (d = 0.61) and BU (d = 0.58),
and small for RU (d = 0.14; Cohen, 1988). While singles differed
2.5.3. Correlations between PTQ and RelRQ on all scales, the strongest effect was found for the RP scale, in the
The revisions resulted in an 18-item measure with three subscales. hypothesised direction. This subscale reflects issues that should be
Correlations between the factors were significant and substantial as il- theoretically more relevant to single people, providing some support
lustrated in Table 3. As expected, the correlation between PTQ and
RelRQ was significant and large. Table 3
Inter-correlations between RelRQ total score, RelRQ subscales, and PTQ.

2.5.4. RelRQ and demographics RelRQ RelRQ-RP RelRQ-RU RelRQ-BU PTQ


It was hypothesised that women would score higher on the RelRQ RELRQ –
total than men given the robust finding in the rumination literature RelRQ-RP .86 [.84, .88]a –
that women achieve higher scores on rumination measures RelRQ-RU .86 [.83, .88]a .57 [.50, .63]a –
(e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema, 2001). Exploratory subscale analyses were RelRQ-BU .82 [.78, .85]a .63 [.57, .68]a .62 [.56, .67]a –
PTQ .72 [.68; .76]b .62 [.56, .67]b .61 [.55, .67]b .54 [.47, .60]b –
also conducted to test the hypotheses that there would be no difference
on overall RelRQ between single and partnered individuals; partnered Note: Numbers in brackets are 95% confidence intervals of Spearman's ρ correlation coef-
ficients. All coefficients are significant at p b .01 (2-tailed). RelRQ = Relational Rumination
individuals would score higher on the RU factor than singles; and
Questionnaire; RelRQ-RP = Relational Rumination Questionnaire Romantic Preoccupa-
tion subscale; RelRQ-RU = Relational Rumination Questionnaire Relationship uncertainty
1
The factor structure held in additional analyses excluding participants who never had subscale; RelRQ-BU = Relational Rumination Questionnaire Breakup subscale.
a
a romantic relationship or never experienced the breakup of a romantic relationship. The- n = 578.
b
se results are available on request. n = 556.
S. Senkans et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35 31

for the external validity for the RelRQ and its subscales. Age was indicated by modification indices) and one item (#3) was deleted,
moderately inversely correlated with RelRQ (rs = −.31, p b .001). with the resulting 17-item model showing slightly better fit. A second
item (#12) was deleted based on misfit and redundancy, resulting in
2.6. Summary the final 16-item measure. The revised 16-item, three-factor model
was the best-fitting model in the training set (see Table 4). The model
Study 1 showed the RelRQ to be a reliable 18-item measure of was successfully cross-validated in the validation sample (see Table 4;
relational rumination. While the identified exploratory model was not Model 3b). Table 5 shows the final revision of the RelRQ and
an exact fit for the data, a three-factor solution (romantic preoccupation standardised factor loadings in both the validation and training samples.
rumination, relationship uncertainty rumination, and break-up rumina-
tion) was the optimal model in the set showing adequate approximate 3.3. Convergent validity
fit. The factors correlated highly with each other. In addition, the RelRQ
and its subscales correlated meaningfully with a measure of general 3.3.1. Hypotheses
rumination. A second study was conducted in an independent sample It was hypothesised that: a) The RelRQ would positively correlate
to confirm the factor structure of the RelRQ, replicate results regarding with anxious and avoidant attachment, although more strongly with
demographic differences, and examine test–retest reliability and the former; b) The RelRQ would be negatively related to positive affect
convergent validity of the RelRQ. and positively related to negative affect; c) The RelRQ would be
positively associated with state and anger rumination; d) The RelRQ
3. Study 2 — confirmatory factor analysis and construct validity would be positively related to emotional suppression, negatively related
to emotional reappraisal, and positively associated with emotion dys-
The aims of Study 2 were a) to conduct confirmatory factor analyses regulation; e) The RelRQ would be negatively correlated with positive
(CFAs) on the revised RelRQ; b) to establish test–retest reliability; and relationship functioning.
c) to test convergent validity of the final version of the RelRQ. This It was further hypothesised that Study 1 results regarding the RelRQ
study was approved by the Swinburne University Human Research and demographics would be replicated: Women would score higher on
Ethics Committee. Informed consent was obtained from all participants. the total RelRQ and the RU subscale than men; and singles would score
The survey was hosted online using the platform Surveymonkey™ and significantly higher on the RelRQ and all its subscales than partnered
consisted of a demographic questionnaire followed by 12 scales that participants.
were counter-balanced (total of 192 items). Eight scales were used in
the current study.
3.3.2. Measures
All measures were administered in random order with item order
3.1. Sample
randomised within each questionnaire.
Course credits were used to compensate 525 undergraduate
students for participating. Participants were between 18–62 years old 3.3.2.1. RelRQ. The 16-item RelRQ was used in all analyses. Cronbach's α
(M = 29.29, SD = 9.94) and 80.5% (n = 422) of the sample were female. values were: total scale = .91, RP = .90, RU = .92 and BU = .91. The in-
Eighty-nine percent (n = 470) identified as exclusively or predomi- structions detailed in Section 2.2.1 were provided to participants.
nantly heterosexual, 3.8% (n = 20) as bisexual, and 4.4% (n = 23) as
exclusively or predominantly homosexual. Twelve (2.1%) did not want 3.3.2.2. Attachment. The Revised Experiences in Close Relationships
to specify or ticked “other”. Reflecting the ethnic diversity of the (ECR-R; Fraley et al., 2000) measures avoidant (models of others) and
Australian tertiary student population, 66.6% (n = 351) of participants anxious (models of self) attachment orientations using two 18 item
considered themselves Australian (2 identifying themselves as Aborigi- scales rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to
nal Australian and 89 identifying a combined ethnicity such as Chinese 6 (strongly agree). Sibley, Fischer, and Liu (2005) identified two inter-
Australian or Italian Australian); 15.4% (n = 81) various European nally consistent factors in the ECR-R: anxious attachment (α = .93)
ethnicities; 6.7% (n = 35) various Asian ethnicities; 2.1% (n = 11) a and avoidant attachment (α = .94) that were moderately strongly cor-
Southern or Eastern African ethnicity; 1.5% (n = 8) various North related (r = .48, p b .001). The ECR-R has been shown to be test–retest
African or Middle Eastern ethnicities; 1.1% (n = 6) New Zealander; 1% reliable (rtt = .90–.92) over a three-week period (Sibley et al., 2005).
(n = 5) a North or South American ethnicity; and, 0.6% (n = 3) Pacific We used the instructions detailed in Sibley et al. (2005), specifying
Islander. Five percent (n = 24) did not specify. Most participants had that participants think about their experiences in romantic relationships
completed secondary school (n = 501, 95.6%), 37% had already com- when responding. In the current sample, internal consistency was α =
pleted a diploma or certificate, 14% another Bachelor degree and 3% .94 for anxious attachment and α = .95 for avoidant attachment.
had postgraduate qualifications. Ninety two percent of participants
spoke English as their first language.
Table 4
Sixty-two percent (n = 325) were in a relationship at the time of Model Fit Information for confirmatory factor analyses.
participating. The majority (n = 443; 84.5%) had experienced the
χ2 df RMSEA WRMR CFI
breakup of a romantic relationship in the past, 47 (9%) had not, and
6.5% (n = 34) had never been in a romantic relationship. A subsample 1 302.40 132 .070 [.060, .080] 1.021 .981
of 65 participants completed the RelRQ again after 4 weeks to establish p b .001
2 196.33 116 .051 [.039, .063] .811 .991
test–retest reliability.
p b .001
3a 152.93 101 .044 [.029, .058] .724 .994
3.2. Confirmatory factor analysis p b .001
3b 144.00 101 .040 [.024, .055] .649 .994
p b .001
The sample was randomly divided into a training sample (n = 263),
used to initially test the three factor model, and a validation sample to Note: Model 1 = three factor model (RP, RU, BU) 18 items — training set; Model 2 = three
confirm the factor structure after possible modification of the model factor model (RP, RU, BU) 17 items — training set; Model 3a = three factor model (RP, RU,
BU) 16 items — training set; Model 3b = three factor model of 16-items — validation set.
(n = 262). As shown in Table 4, neither model showed exact fit, but χ2 = WLSMV Chi-Square test; df = degrees of freedom; RMSEA = Root Mean Square
Model 3a showed acceptable approximate fit. To increase model fit Error of Approximation; numbers in brackets are 95% confidence intervals of RMSEA;
and parsimony, residuals were examined for items that had misfit (as WRMR = Weighted Root Mean Square Residual; CFI = Comparative Fit Index.
32 S. Senkans et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35

Table 5
Factor loadings of RelRQ in training and validation data sets.

Item # Item Training data set Validation data set

F1 F2 F3 F1 F2 F3

2 Thoughts about how to find a partner plague my mind. .90 .89


16 I think about how to find a romantic relationship to avoid ending up alone. .86 .83
7 I keep on wondering why my friends have romantic relationships and I don't. .85 .85
10 Thoughts about why I am not in relationship pop into my head without me wanting them to. .85 .90
1 I think of strategies to get into a romantic relationship over and over again. .83 .83
17 I imagine my partner cheating on me even though I don't want to. .92 .91
13 Nagging doubts about my partner's faithfulness pop up in my mind. .91 .90
5 Thoughts about my partner cheating on me stress me out. .90 .84
11 I get caught up in imagining scenarios in which my partner would cheat on me. .88 .86
4 The thought of my partner sleeping with somebody else crosses my mind. .86 .83
8 I keep thinking that other people are interested in my partner. .78 .73
6 I go over and over the reasons why my relationship(s) with my ex-partner(s) ended. .90 .90
9 I think about how I should have prevented the break-up with an ex-partner. .88 .86
14 I think over and over again about how to re-establish the relationship with my ex-partner. .87 .83
15 Thoughts about my ex-partner(s) distract me from other things I should be doing. .86 .87
18 I wish I could stop thinking about my ex-partner(s), but I can't. .86 .85

Note: F1 = romantic preoccupation rumination (RP); F2 = relationship uncertainty rumination (RU); F3 = breakup rumination (BU). All loadings significant at p b .001.

Avoidant and anxious attachment were moderately correlated (r = .38, (Newhill et al., 2004). Newhill et al. (2004) found a ceiling effect
p b .001). suggesting that the scale is appropriate for use in a non-clinical sample.
In the current sample α = .92.
3.3.2.3. Positive and negative affectivity. The short form of the Positive and
Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS-SF; Mackinnon et al., 1999) measures 3.3.2.7. Anger Rumination Scale. The Anger Rumination Scale (ARS;
the dispositional tendency to experience positive (PA) and negative af- Sukhodolsky et al., 2001) uses 19 items to examine the degree to
fect (NA). In the development sample, internal consistency was α = .79 which individuals tend to focus on angry moods. Participants are
for positive affect, and α = .85 for negative affect. Test–retest reliability asked to respond to items on a 4 point Likert-type scale ranging from
was not established. In the current sample internal consistency was 1 (Almost never) to 4 (Almost always). The ARS consists of four subscales:
α = .80 for positive affect, and α = .85 for negative affect. Angry Afterthoughts, Thoughts of Revenge, Angry Memories, and
Understanding of Causes. The total scale is highly reliable (α = .93),
3.3.2.4. Inclusion of Other in the Self. Aron, Aron, and Smollan's (1992) stable across a month (rtt = .77), and has established convergent and
single-item pictorial Inclusion of Other in the Self scale (IOS) was used discriminant validity (Sukhodolsky et al., 2001). In the current sample
to assess healthy, positive relationship functioning. Participants select the ARS exhibited α = .93.
which of seven Venn diagrams with different degrees of overlap
between two circles (creating an interval scale) best represents their 3.3.2.8. Rumination about Interpersonal Offence Scale (RIO). Wade et al.
view of themselves in relation to their partner in a romantic relation- (2008) developed a measure of ruminative responses to situations in
ship. The IOS is test–retest reliable (rtt = .83) over a period of 2 weeks which participants recall a specific upsetting interpersonal experience
and has demonstrated convergent validity with related measures. that occurred within the last 7 days. They are then asked to rate state-
The higher IOS the higher is relationship satisfaction, intimacy, and ments in relation to this experience, e.g., “I can't stop thinking about
commitment (Aron et al., 1992). how I was wronged by this person” on a 5-point Likert scale ranging
from (from 1, strongly disagree to 5, strongly agree). Internal consistency
3.3.2.5. Emotion Regulation Questionnaire — 9. The Emotion Regulation was high (α = .91–.92) and test–retest reliability was acceptable (rtt =
Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003) is a 10-item, self-report mea- .51, over 10 weeks). Convergent validity was also supported in Wade
sure of trait emotion expression/suppression (ERQ-S; four items) and et al.’s (2008) study. In the current sample α = .86.
reappraisal (ERQ-R; six items). Participants endorse items on a 7-point
Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Spaapen, 3.3.3. Analyses
Waters, Brummer, Stopa, and Bucks (2014) report validation of the SPSS 22 (IBM Corp., 2013) was used for all analyses with Spearman's
ERQ in representative Australian and UK samples, identifying a nine ρ correlations used to establish test–retest reliability and convergent va-
item version as preferred, which was used in this study. Reliability of lidity. Differences regarding RelRQ responses by demographic charac-
the suppression (Australian: α = .78; United Kingdom: α = .74) and teristics were assessed using t-tests to compare group mean differences.
reappraisal (Australian: α = .76; United Kingdom: α = .80) scales
was adequate. Gross and John (2003) reported rtt = .69 across 3 months 3.3.4. Results
in their original validation sample. In the current sample we found α = The total RelRQ and all subscales showed good test–retest reliability
.82 for suppression and α = .87 for reappraisal. over a period of 4 weeks (RelRQ: rtt = .71, p b .01; RP: rtt = .74, p b .01;
RU: rtt = .68, p b .01; BU: rtt = .71, p b .01). All hypotheses regarding
3.3.2.6. Emotion Dysregulation Measure. Newhill, Mulvey, and Pilkonis convergent validity were supported (see Table 6), with a large effect
(2004) developed the 13-item scale General Emotional Dysregulation size observed between attachment anxiety and relationship rumina-
Measure (GEDM) measuring general emotional arousal and dysregula- tion, and moderate effect sizes between relationship rumination and
tion of negative affect in a sample of a 100 individuals diagnosed with other forms of ruminative thinking (ARS and RIO), negative affect, and
a DSM-IV Cluster B personality disorder. Participants endorse state- avoidant attachment.
ments such as: “In general, I have a hard time handling my emotions” In contrast to our hypotheses and previous findings, there were no
on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 gender differences in RelRQ total or subscale scores in this sample. How-
(strongly agree).The GEDM is reliable (α = .82), stable over 3 weeks ever, single individuals scored significantly higher on the RelRQ overall,
(rtt = .81), and demonstrates convergent and discriminant validity and all three subscales (RelRQ: M = 37.73, SD = 12.14, t(355.70) =
S. Senkans et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35 33

Table 6
Convergent validity of the RelRQ with other measures.

RelRQ RelRQ-RP RelRQ-RU RelRQ-BU

ECR-R Anx .72 [.67, .76]⁎⁎ .57 [.50, .63]⁎⁎ .59 [.53, .65]⁎⁎ .52 [.45, .58]⁎⁎
ECR-R Av .31 [.23, .38]⁎⁎ .29 [.22, .36]⁎⁎ .18 [.10, .27]⁎⁎ .29 [.21, .37]⁎⁎
PA −.20 [−.28, −.12]⁎⁎ −.15 [−.23, −.07]⁎⁎ −.15 [−.24, −.06]⁎⁎ −.15 [−.24, −.06]⁎⁎
NA .43 [34, .49]⁎⁎ .34 [.26, .41]⁎⁎ .37 [.30, .44]⁎⁎ .29 [.20, .37]⁎⁎
IOS −.19 [−.27, −.11]⁎⁎ −.23 [−.31, −.14]⁎⁎ −.03 [−.13, .05] −.21 [−.29, −.12]⁎⁎
GEDM .29 [.20, .37]⁎⁎ .23 [.14, .32]⁎⁎ .25 [.17, .33]⁎⁎ .20 [.10, .28]⁎⁎
ERQ-R −.18 [−.26, −.09]⁎⁎ −.15 [−.23, −.06]⁎⁎ −.13 [−.21, −.05]⁎⁎ −.13 [−.22, −.04]⁎⁎
ERQ-S .15 [.07, .23]⁎⁎ .14 [.05, .22]⁎⁎ .12 [.04, .20]⁎⁎ .11 [.03, .19]⁎
ARS .45 [.37, .52]⁎⁎ .33 [.25, .41]⁎⁎ .38 [.30, .46]⁎⁎ .35 [.27, .43]⁎⁎
RIO .33 [.25, .41]⁎⁎ .27 [.19, .35]⁎⁎ .29 [.20, .37]⁎⁎ .25 [.17, .34]⁎⁎

Note: numbers in brackets are 95% confidence intervals of Spearman's ρ correlation coefficients. RelRQ = Relational Rumination Questionnaire; RelRQ-RP = Relational Rumination Ques-
tionnaire Romantic Preoccupation subscale; RelRQ-RU = Relational Rumination Questionnaire Relationship uncertainty subscale; RelRQ-BU = Relational Rumination Questionnaire
Breakup subscale. ECR-R Anx = Experiences in Close Relationships Anxious Attachment; ECR-R Av = Experiences in Close Relationships Avoidant Attachment; PA = PANAS Positive Af-
fect; NA = PANAS NA; IOS = Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale; GEDM = General Emotion Dysregulation Measure; ERQ-R = Emotion Regulation Questionnaire — 9 reappraisal; ERQ-S =
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire — 9 suppression; ARS = Anger Rumination Scale; RIO = Rumination about Interpersonal Offence Scale.
⁎ p b .05.
⁎⁎ p b .01 (2-tailed).

9.07, p b .05; RP: M = 13.26, SD = 5.15, t(320.62) = 11.87, p b .05; RU: et al., 2004). These explanations could not be tested in this study and re-
M = 12.79, SD = 5.17, t(521) = 2.47, p b .05; BU: M = 11.68, SD = 5.14, quire further research.
t(348.57) = 7.34, p b .05) than partnered people (RelRQ: M = 28.43, Single participants in both studies obtained higher scores on the
SD = 9.97; RP: M = 8.29, SD = 3.69; RU: M = 11.63, SD = 5.26; BU: RelRQ, with the greatest differences on the romantic preoccupation sub-
M = 8.51, SD = 4.11). Echoing Study 1, the effect sizes for these mean scale, as predicted. Singles also scored higher on the relationship uncer-
differences was large for the total RelRQ (d = 0.84), and the RP subscale tainty scale, although the effect size suggested that this difference was
(d = 1.11), moderate for the BU subscale (d = 0.68), and small for the not particularly meaningful. Single participants also scored more highly
RU subscale (d = 0.22; Cohen, 1988). Age was again inversely correlat- on the BU subscale, with a moderate to large effect size. It is possible that
ed with RelRQ (rs = −.19, p b .001). these findings reflect underlying beliefs that finding a partner is a neces-
sary condition to leading a fulfilled, happy life. This in turn may be asso-
ciated with viewing singlehood and oneself in a negative light (DePaulo
3.4. Summary
& Morris, 2006, p. 253) and rumination about previous breakups and
how to attain a romantic relationship (Martin et al., 1993).
The RelRQ's factor structure was confirmed. The RelRQ was test–re-
Cupach et al. (2000) propose that linking of a romantic relationship
test reliable over a period of 4 weeks and internally consistent. Almost
to abstract goals such as life happiness can increase rumination on
all hypotheses regarding convergent validity were supported. In Study
romantic relationships and associated negative affect (Martin et al.,
2, the RelRQ was gender-invariant. Single participants scored signifi-
1993). This in turn is thought to drive persistent pursuit of desirable po-
cantly higher on the RelRQ and its subscales than individuals who
tential partners or ex-partners, or even stalking (Cupach et al., 2011;
were in a relationship, with the strongest group difference observed
Cupach et al., 2000). Such hypotheses could be more rigorously tested
for RP subscale scores.
using the newly-developed RelRQ with its focus on rumination on
pre- and post-relational romantic concerns separate to rumination on
4. General discussion current relationships.
The RelRQ also has potential uses in research examining psycholog-
A three-factor, 16-item measure of relational rumination was devel- ical mechanisms associated with IPV. While angry rumination has
oped. The factor structure reflected the related themes of rumination been shown to relate to IPV (Sotelo & Babcock, 2013; Watkins et al.,
about beginning a relationship, rumination about an established 2015), the RelRQ could add considerable value to such research by mea-
relationship, and rumination about ex-partner(s) and previous suring the highly relevant construct of ruminative relationship jealousy
breakups. This factor structure was robust across independent samples. and uncertainty. One potential cause of IPV may be difficulties managing
The RelRQ demonstrated good internal consistency, test–retest reliabil- jealousy or other negative emotions (Langhinrichsen-Rohling, 2010;
ity, and construct validity. Correlations between the RelRQ and attach- Langhinrichsen-Rohling, McCullars, & Misra, 2012), which may result
ment insecurity, general and other specific forms of rumination, and in aggressive interpersonal responses to jealousy-arousing situations
negative affect showed strong to moderate associations with RelRQ, (Carson & Cupach, 2000). It may be that a combination of relationship-
consistent with the rumination literature and strongly supporting the related rumination and angry rumination specific to the relationship
RelRQs' validity. Correlations with positive affect and measures of partner is relevant to IPV, in conjunction with difficulties with effective
emotion regulation were small, though still in the expected direction. emotional regulation. The RelRQ showed moderate correlations with
There were some possible demographic differences in RelRQ negative affect, small to moderate correlations with emotion dysregula-
responses. Women scored higher on the RU subscale and the RelRQ tion measures, and small correlations with a measure of effective emo-
overall than men in Study 1, although the size of the difference was tion regulation strategies. This provides preliminary evidence that
small and there were no such differences in Study 2. This could be be- relationship rumination is linked to problems with maintaining ideal
cause Study 2 participants were younger university students, a popula- levels of emotional arousal (Aldao et al., 2010). The association between
tion in which both genders may be equally concerned about relational trait-like rumination, state-based rumination on particular relationship
goals. Age was inversely correlated with relational rumination in both transgressions (Wade et al., 2008), and emotional dysregulation could
studies. This might be explained by older individuals being involved in be a fruitful area for future research.
more stable, more committed relationships (e.g., V. King & Scott, The RelRQ was strongly correlated with a measure of general rumi-
2005), or by younger people being more concerned about meeting so- nation, and moderately correlated with anger rumination and state ru-
cially prescribed development of romantic competence (e.g., Roisman mination after a specific interpersonal transgression. Perhaps some
34 S. Senkans et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35

individuals have a generally ruminative response style that colours not Aron, A., Aron, E.N., & Allen, J. (1998). Motivations for unreciprocated love. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(8), 787–796. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167298248001.
only their relational goals and concerns, but also their responses to in- Barrett, P. (2007). Structural equation modelling: Adjudging model fit. Personality and
terpersonal transgressions and negative emotional states. The associa- Individual Differences, 42(5), 815–824. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.018.
tion between general ‘content free’ rumination (Ehring et al., 2011) Baumeister, R.F., Wotman, S.R., & Stillwell, A.M. (1993). Unrequited love: On heartbreak,
anger, guilt, scriptlessness, and humiliation. Journal of Personality and Social
and rumination on different content warrants further research. Psychology, 64(3), 377–394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.377.
As expected, the RelRQ was strongly positively correlated with anx- Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin,
ious attachment and less strongly with avoidant attachment. The RelRQ 107(2), 238–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238.
Bentler, P.M. (2007). On tests and indices for evaluating structural models. Personality and
can add considerably to existing attachment measures which tend to
Individual Differences, 42(5), 825–829. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.024.
conflate cognitive (e.g., “I worry a lot about my relationships”), affective Boelen, P.A., & Reijntjes, A. (2009). Negative cognitions in emotional problems following
(e.g., “It makes me mad that I don't get the affection and support that I romantic relationship break-ups. Stress and Health, 25(1), 11–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1002/smi.1219.
need from my partner”), and behavioural components (“I usually
Boelen, P.A., & van den Hout, M.A. (2010). Inclusion of other in the self and breakup-
discuss my problems and concerns with my partner”; Fraley et al., related grief following relationship dissolution. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 15(6),
2000, p. 361) of attachment (Holmes, 2000). The RelRQ may be able 534–547. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15325024.2010.519274.
to more clearly measure one cognitive aspects of attachment, relation- Burnette, J.L., Davis, D.E., Green, J.D., Worthington, E.L., Jr., & Bradfield, E. (2009). Insecure
attachment and depressive symptoms: The mediating role of rumination, empathy,
ship rumination. and forgiveness. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(3), 276–280. http://dx.doi.
It is also possible that rumination is not merely an aspect of anxious org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.016.
attachment but that a third variable explains the relationship. For exam- Buunk, B.P. (1997). Personality, birth order and attachment styles as related to various
types of jealousy. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(6), 997–1006. http://dx.
ple, deficits in self-regulation may underlie insecure attachment, rumi- doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00136-0.
nation, and relational maladjustment, including aggression towards Carson, C.L., & Cupach, W.R. (2000). Fueling the flames of the green-eyed monster: the
partners (Finkel, DeWall, Slotter, Oaten, & Foshee, 2009). Among related role of ruminative thought in reaction to romantic jealousy. Western Journal of
Communication, 64(3), 308–329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10570310009374678.
executive functions, self-regulation includes maintaining optimal levels Chung, M. -S. (2014). Pathways between attachment and marital satisfaction: The medi-
of emotional, motivational, and cognitive arousal (Diamond, 2013). ating roles of rumination, empathy, and forgiveness. Personality and Individual
Whereas insecure attachment is associated with ineffective self- Differences, 70(0), 246–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.032.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ, USA:
regulation in the relational domain overall, relational rumination can Erlbaum.
be seen as a specific failure to maintain optimal cognitive arousal. Cupach, W.R., Spitzberg, B.H., & Carson, C.L. (2000). Toward a theory of obsessive relation-
The only construct that did not evidence the predicted relationship al intrusion and stalking. In S. Duck, & K. Dindia (Eds.), Communication and personal
relationships (pp. 131–146). New York, NY, USA: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
with all subscales of RelRQ was healthy relationship functioning (IOS).
Cupach, W.R., Spitzberg, B.H., Bolingbroke, C.M., & Tellitocci, B.S. (2011). Persistence of at-
While total RelRQ, RP, and BU showed the predicted inverse correlation, tempts to reconcile a terminated romantic relationship: A partial test of relational
there was no correlation between RU and IOS. An explanation may be goal pursuit theory. Communication Reports, 24(2), 99–115. http://dx.doi.org/10.
that increased closeness is sometimes associated with more anxiety 1080/08934215.2011.613737.
Davis, D., Shaver, P.R., & Vernon, M.L. (2003). Physical, emotional, and behavioral reac-
about losing the partner, generating relationship uncertainty rumina- tions to breaking up: The roles of gender, age, emotional involvement, and attach-
tion. It is also possible that this single item measure did not sufficiently ment style. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 871–884. http://dx.doi.
measure relationship adjustment. Further investigation of the associa- org/10.1177/0146167203029007006.
De Smet, O., Uzieblo, K., Loeys, T., Buysse, A., & Onraedt, T. (2015). Unwanted pursuit be-
tion between RelRQ scores and relational adjustment is warranted. havior after breakup: Occurrence, risk factors, and gender differences. Journal of
Family Violence, 1–15http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10896-015-9687-9.
4.1. Limitations and conclusion DePaulo, B.M., & Morris, W.L. (2006). The unrecognized stereotyping and discrimination
against singles. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(5), 251–254. http://dx.
doi.org/10.2307/20183125.
This study is limited by convenience sampling, with the predomi- Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168.
nantly young, educated, and female sample potentially impacting the http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.
Ehring, T., Zetsche, U., Weidacker, K., Wahl, K., Schönfeld, S., & Ehlers, A. (2011). The Per-
generalizability of findings. Moreover, no adjustments were made for severative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent mea-
social desirability, which may affect responses on measures of negative sure of repetitive negative thinking. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
characteristics. Despite these limitations, the RelRQ appears to be a reli- Psychiatry, 42(2), 225–232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.003.
Elphinston, R.A., Feeney, J.A., & Noller, P. (2011). Measuring romantic jealousy: Validation
able and valid measure of different aspects of rumination about rela- of the multidimensional jealousy scale in Australian samples. Australian Journal of
tionships in English-speaking, industrialised societies. We recommend Psychology, 63(4), 243–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1742-9536.2011.00026.x.
that the full scale is administered, regardless of participants' dating his- Finkel, E.J., DeWall, C.N., Slotter, E.B., Oaten, M., & Foshee, V.A. (2009). Self-regulatory fail-
ure and intimate partner violence perpetration. Journal of Personality and Social
tory or current relationship status. Although the subscales are strongly Psychology, 97(3), 483–499. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015433.
correlated, it may be appropriate in some cases to report only RP scale Fraley, R.C., Waller, N.G., & Brennan, K.A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of self-
results for those who have never experienced a relationship, and only report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
78(2), 350–365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350.
RP and RU results for those who have never experienced a break-up.
Goffin, R.D. (2007). Assessing the adequacy of structural equation models: Golden rules
The RelRQ shares some similar content with existing face-valid scales, and editorial policies. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(5), 831–839. http://
however unlike those scales it is multi-faceted and was comprehensive- dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.019.
ly validated. The factor structure was evident in three independent Gross, J.J., & John, O.P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:
Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
samples, the measure has sound test–retest reliability, and it shows Psychology, 85(2), 348–362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.
meaningful correlations with related constructs. The RelRQ has consid- Holmes, J.G. (2000). Social relationships: The nature and function of relational schemas.
erable potential to add to the body of knowledge about the role and European Journal of Social Psychology, 30(4), 447–495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
1099-0992(200007/08)30:4b447::aid-ejsp10N3.0.co;2-q.
impact of romantic relationship rumination on individual functioning. Hu, L. -T., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure anal-
ysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A
References Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.
IBM Corp. (2013). IBM SPSS statistics for Windows, version 22.0. Armonk, NY, USA: IBM Corp.
Afifi, W.A., & Reichert, T. (1996). Understanding the role of uncertainty in jealousy expe- Ingram, R.E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual
rience and expression. Communication Reports, 9(2), 93–103. http://dx.doi.org/10. model. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 156–176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.
1080/08934219609367642. 107.2.156.
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies King, D.B., & DeLongis, A. (2014). When couples disconnect: Rumination and withdrawal
across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), as maladaptive responses to everyday stress. Journal of Family Psychology, 28(4),
217–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004. 460–469. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037160.
Aron, A., Aron, E.N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the King, V., & Scott, M.E. (2005). A comparison of cohabiting relationships among older and
structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, younger adults. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67(2), 271–285. http://dx.doi.org/10.
63(4), 596–612. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596. 1111/j.0022-2445.2005.00115.x.
S. Senkans et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 27–35 35

Kirkegaard Thomsen, D. (2006). The association between rumination and negative affect: Multivariate Behavioral Research, 48(1), 28–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
A review. Cognition & Emotion, 20(8), 1216–1235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 00273171.2012.710386.
02699930500473533. Reynolds, S., Searight, H.R., & Ratwik, S. (2014). Adult attachment styles and rumination
Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J. (2010). Controversies involving gender and intimate partner in the context of intimate relationships. North American Journal of Psychology, 16(3),
violence in the United States. Sex Roles, 62(3–4), 179–193. http://dx.doi.org/10. 495–506.
1007/s11199-009-9628-2. Roisman, G.I., Masten, A.S., Coatsworth, J.D., & Tellegen, A. (2004). Salient and emerging
Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., McCullars, A., & Misra, T.A. (2012). Motivations for men and developmental tasks in the transition to adulthood. Child Development, 75(1),
women's intimate partner violence perpetration: A comprehensive review. Partner 123–133. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3696570.
Abuse, 3(4), 429–468. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/1946-6560.3.4.429. Russel, D., Peplau, L.A., & Cutrona, C.E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale:
Li, C. -H. (2014). The performance of MLR, USLMV, and WLSMV estimation in structural regres- Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social
sion models with ordinal variables. East Lansing, MI, USA: Michigan State University Psychology, 39(3), 472–480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472.
(Retrieved from http://gradworks.umi.com/36/20/3620406.html ProQuest database). Saffrey, C., & Ehrenberg, M. (2007). When thinking hurts: Attachment, rumination, and
Mackinnon, A., Jorm, A.F., Christensen, H., Korten, A.E., Jacomb, P.A., & Rodgers, B. (1999). postrelationship adjustment. Personal Relationships, 14(3), 351–368. http://dx.doi.
A short form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: Evaluation of factorial va- org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00160.x.
lidity and invariance across demographic variables in a community sample. Sibley, C.G., Fischer, R., & Liu, J.H. (2005). Reliability and validity of the revised experiences
Personality and Individual Differences, 27(3), 405–416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ in close relationships (ECR-R) self-report measure of adult romantic attachment.
S0191-8869(98)00251-7. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(11), 1524–1536. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Markland, D. (2007). The golden rule is that there are no golden rules: A commentary on 1177/0146167205276865.
Paul Barrett's recommendations for reporting model fit in structural equation model- Sotelo, J.M., & Babcock, J.C. (2013). BIS/BAS variables as moderators of the rumination-
ling. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(5), 851–858. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ intimate partner violence link. Journal of Family Violence, 28(3), 233–242. http://dx.
j.paid.2006.09.023. doi.org/10.1007/s10896-013-9500-6.
Marquez, A. (2013). Emotional, social, and cognitive correlates of stalking and intrusive ha- Spaapen, D.L., Waters, F., Brummer, L., Stopa, L., & Bucks, R.S. (2014). The Emotion
rassment. (Doctoral dissertation) Nebraska, USA: University of Nebraska-Lincoln (Re- Regulation Questionnaire: Validation of the ERQ-9 in two community samples.
trieved from http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychdiss/56/). Psychological Assessment, 26(1), 46–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034474.
Martin, L.L., Tesser, A., & McIntosh, W.D. (1993). Wanting but not having: The effects of Spitzberg, B.H., Cupach, W.R., Hannawa, A.F., & Crowley, J.P. (2014). A preliminary test of a
unattained goals on thoughts and feelings. In D.M. Wegner, & J.W. Pennebaker relational goal pursuit theory of obsessive relational intrusion and stalking. Studies in
(Eds.), Handbook of mental control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA: Prentice Hall. Communication Sciences, 14(1), 29–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.scoms.2014.03.
McCullough, M.E., Bono, G., & Root, L.M. (2007). Rumination, emotion, and forgiveness: 007.
Three longitudinal studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(3), Steiger, J.H. (2007). Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural
490–505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.490. equation modeling. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(5), 893–898. http://dx.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P.R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.017.
change. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press. Sukhodolsky, D.G., Golub, A., & Cromwell, E.N. (2001). Development and validation of the
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P.R. (2008). Adult attachment and affect regulation. In J. Cassidy, anger rumination scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 31(5), 689–700. http://
& P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (2nd ed.). New York, NY, USA: The dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00171-9.
Guilford Press. Tran, T.B., & Joormann, J. (2015). The role of Facebook use in mediating the relation be-
Miles, J., & Shevlin, M. (2007). A time and a place for incremental fit indices. Personality and tween rumination and adjustment after a relationship breakup. Computers in
Individual Differences, 42(5), 869–874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.022. Human Behavior, 49, 56–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.050.
Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O. (1998-2012). Mplus user's guide (7th ed.) (Retrieved from Wade, N.G., Vogel, D.L., Liao, K.Y.H., & Goldman, D.B. (2008). Measuring state-specific ru-
www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/Mplus%20user%20guide%20Ver_7_r6_ mination: Development of the Rumination About an Interpersonal Offense Scale.
web.pdf). Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55(3), 419–426. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O. (2013, June). MPlus 7.11. Los Angeles, CA, USA: Muthén & 0167.55.3.419.
Muthén. Watkins, L.E., DiLillo, D., & Maldonado, R.C. (2015). The interactive effects of emotion reg-
Newhill, C.E., Mulvey, E.P., & Pilkonis, P.A. (2004). Initial development of a measure of emo- ulation and alcohol intoxication on lab-based intimate partner aggression. Psychology
tional dysregulation for individuals with cluster B personality disorders. Research on of Addictive Behaviorshttp://dx.doi.org/10.1037/adb0000074.
Social Work Practice, 14(6), 443–449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1049731504267332. Yanowitz, J.L. (2006). Romantic goal preoccupation: Scale validation and experimental
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of evidence of its effects on the processing of social information. (Doctoral dissertation)
depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582. http://dx.doi. Minneapolis, MN, USA: University of Minnesota (Retrieved from http://search.
org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569. proquest.com/docview/305304781 ProQuest database).
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Gender differences in depression. Current Directions in Yu, C. -Y. (2002). Evaluation of model fit indices for latent variable models with categorical
Psychological Science, 10(5), 173–176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00142. and continuous outcomes. (Doctoral dissertation) Los Angeles, CA, USA: University
Preacher, K.J., Zhang, G., Kim, C., & Mels, G. (2013). Choosing the optimal number of California (Retrieved from http://www.statmodel.com/download/Yudissertation.
of factors in exploratory factor analysis: A model selection perspective. pdf MPlus website).

You might also like