You are on page 1of 121

Impact of Emotion Regulation Strategies on Attachment Styles

in Adult Relationships:
Understanding the Influence of Different Factors on Relationship
Attitudes

DISSERTATION

Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirement of


Bachelor of Arts (Honours)
In Psychology

By
RISHITA AGARWAL
18BABBA236
Under the guidance of
DR. PRATEEK KUMAR SINGH

School of Liberal Studies


Pandit Deendayal Energy University
Gandhinagar - 382426, Gujarat - INDIA
May - 2023
APPROVAL SHEET

This dissertation titled ‘Impact of Emotion Regulation Strategies on Attachment


Styles in Adult Relationships: Understanding the Influence of Different Factors on
Relationship Attitudes’ by Rishita Agarwal is recommended for the degree of
Bachelor of Arts (Hons.) in Psychology.

Examiners/Panelists:

_________________________

_________________________

_________________________

Supervisor:

_________________________

_________________________

_________________________

Chairman:

_________________________

Date: _______________
STUDENT’S DECLARATION

I, Rishita Agarwal, hereby declare that this written submission represents my ideas in
my own words, and where others’ ideas or words have been included, I have
adequately cited and referenced the sources. I also declare that I have adhered to all
principles of academic honesty and integrity and have not misrepresented or
fabricated or falsified any idea/data/fact/source in my submission. I understand that
any violation of the above will be cause for disciplinary action by the PANDIT
DEENDAYAL ENERGY UNIVERSITY and can also evoke penal action from the
sources which have thus not been appropriately cited or from whom proper permission
has not been taken when needed.

(Signature)

Rishita Pankaj Agarwal

_________________________________

(Name of Student)

18BABBA236

_________________________________

(Roll No.)

Date: / 05 / 2023
UNDERTAKING

I submit that the edifice of this dissertation research report on Impact of Emotion
Regulation Strategies on Attachment Styles in Adult Relationships: Understanding the
Influence of Different Factors on Relationship Attitudes is built and further developed
on the framework of extensive and critically focused literature survey with sources
listed in the bibliography. In addition, the recourse has also been made to a large
number of references based on critical analysis of experts, special reports in journals,
articles and research papers of journals and magazines, internet, newspapers, reports
of government agencies, conceptual anchoring attributable to standard works
including books and the vast treasure of secondary information. They have been
indexed at the end of this dissertation research report. I further submit that in the
process of the primary research, the conceptual underpinnings, hypotheses,
interpretations, and analysis leading to major findings are mine, and I, at the same
time, accept the limitations and inadequacies, if any. I certify that I have not copied
any material without citation from any published sources. I also certify that I have
checked the plagiarism of this dissertation thesis and it is under the prescribed limit
defined by the UGC i.e., University Grants Commission (UGC), Government of India.
I certify and declare that I have not been awarded or conferred any degree, diploma,
or distinction of any kind, either by Pandit Deendayal Energy University or any other
university for the work presented in this dissertation research report.

Signature

Place: PDEU (Name) Rishita Pankaj Agarwal

Date: / 05 / 2023 (Roll no.) 18BABBA236

1
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT:

Firstly, I would like to express my sincere gratitude to everyone who directly or

indirectly helped me with this dissertation. I would like to thank Dr. Prateek Kumar

Singh, my guide and mentor, for his invaluable advice, continuous support, and

patience during my research study. His immense knowledge and plentiful experience

have encouraged me in all the time of my academic research and daily life. Due to his

guidance, 1 could minimize my difficulties and convert them into strengths during

this dissertation process. Additionally, I would also like to extend my heartfelt thanks

to him for the technical support that he provided me with in my study. I would also

take this opportunity to extend my gratitude to our director Dr. Ritu Sharma for always

being a source of inspiration and strength. Further, I would like to thank all the

respondents who took time out of their busy schedules to respond to my questionnaire

and participate in the study. A big thank you to our university’s library support staff

for providing me seamless access to our e–resources that helped me enhance my

knowledge on the research topic that I chose to work upon. Lastly, a special thanks to

all my faculty members, colleagues, peers, and my entire family for their constant

encouragement, support, and motivation to remain focused on this study.

2
ABSTRACT:

Gross and John (2003) insist, that individual discrepancies in emotion regulation

strategies can have implications on interpersonal relationships, and individual well-

being. The way individuals regulate their emotions can have a significant impact on

the formation and maintenance of attachment styles in adult relationships. Mikulincer

and Shaver (2007) Attachment theory suggests that attachment styles develop through

interactions with caregivers in early childhood and continue to shape our relationships

throughout our lives. Emotion regulation strategies, such as suppression, reappraisal,

and distraction, are used to manage emotional experiences in response to various

situations. These strategies can impact the way we form and maintain attachment

styles in adulthood.

Research has shown that emotion regulation strategies can have both positive and

negative effects on attachment styles. Sbarra and Hazan (2008) propose an integrative

analysis of adult attachment, separation, loss, and recovery, and suggest that

coregulation, dysregulation, and self-regulation are important factors in understanding

attachment processes. One study found that individuals who used reappraisal as a

strategy for regulating their emotions reported higher levels of secure attachment in

their adult relationships. This may be because reappraisal involves changing the way

we think about a situation, which can lead to a more positive interpretation and a better

understanding of our partner's needs and emotions. On the other hand, suppression,

which involves hiding or avoiding emotions, has been linked to a greater likelihood

of developing insecure attachment styles. This may be because suppression can

prevent individuals from fully processing their emotions and communicating

effectively with their partners.

3
Distraction, which involves diverting attention away from the emotional experience,

has also been found to have mixed effects on attachment styles. While distraction may

be helpful in some situations, such as when experiencing intense negative emotions,

it can also lead to a lack of emotional engagement in relationships. This may lead to

a sense of emotional distance and detachment from one's partner, which can contribute

to the development of insecure attachment styles.

Moreover, individual differences, such as gender and personality traits, can also play

a role in the relationship between emotion regulation strategies and attachment styles.

For example, men may be more likely to use suppression as an emotion regulation

strategy, which can increase their risk of developing avoidant attachment styles.

Additionally, individuals with certain personality traits, such as high levels of

neuroticism or low levels of agreeableness, may be more likely to use maladaptive

emotion regulation strategies, which can contribute to the development of insecure

attachment styles.

In conclusion, Simpson and Rholes (2017) argue that adult attachment, stress, and

romantic relationships are intertwined and that understanding the role of attachment

in managing stress is key to maintaining healthy relationships. Emotion regulation

strategies can have a significant impact on the development and maintenance of

attachment styles in adult relationships. Reappraisal, which involves changing the

way we think about a situation, has been found to have positive effects on attachment

styles, while suppression has been linked to a greater likelihood of developing

insecure attachment styles. Distraction has mixed effects on attachment styles, with

some situations where it may be helpful but can also contribute to emotional distance

and detachment. Individual differences, such as gender and personality traits, can also

4
influence the relationship between emotion regulation strategies and attachment

styles. Overall, understanding the impact of emotion regulation strategies on

attachment styles can help individuals to develop healthier and more secure

relationships.

Keywords: emotion regulation, attachment styles, suppression, distraction,

personality traits, insecure attachments, reappraisal, self-regulation, communication.

5
Table of Content:

Acknowledgment
Abstract
List of Tables and Figures
Abbreviations and Symbols
Chapter 1: Introduction...................................................................10

1.1. Background of the study


1.2. Statement of Problem
1.3. Research Objectives
1.4. Research Hypothesis

Chapter 2: Literature Review...........................................................26

2.1. Literature on Variable 1


2.2. Literature on Variable 2
2.3.Theoretical Approach

Chapter 3: Research Design............................................................58

3.1. Variables
3.2. Sample
3.3. Tool
3.4. Procedure
3.5. Data Analysis
3.6. Ethical Considerations

Chapter 4: Results and Discussion.................................................102

Chapter 5: Conclusion and Future Directions................................107

References
Appendices

6
List of Tables and Figures

Table 3.1
Tabular Representation of Factors and their Corresponding Items (Variable I)

Table 3.2
Tabular Representation of Factors and their Corresponding Items (Variable II)

Table 3.4
Tabular Representation of Gender Distribution

Table 3.5
Tabular Representation of Age Distribution

Table 3.6
Tabular Representation of Statistical Data for all Variables

Table 3.7
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 1

Table 3.8
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 1

Table 3.9
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 2

Table 3.10
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 2

Table 3.11
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 3

Table 3.12
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 3

Table 3.13
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 4

7
Table 3.14
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 4

Table 3.15
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 5

Table 3.16
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 5

Table 3.17
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 6

Table 3.18
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 6

Table 3.19
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 7

Table 3.20
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 7

Table 3.21
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 8

Table 3.22
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 8

Table 3.23
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 9

Table 3.24
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 9

Table 3.25
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 10

8
Table 3.26
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 10

Table 3.27
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 11

Table 3.28
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 11

Table 3.29
Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 12

Table 3.30
Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 12

Table 3.31
Tabular Representation of Correlational Analysis of Expressive Suppression and
Attachment Anxiety Levels.

Table 3.32
Tabular Representation of Correlational Analysis of Cognitive Reappraisal and
Attachment Avoidance Levels.

9
CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION

10
Background of the Study:

Emotions are a crucial component of human experience and play a significant role in

shaping our behaviors, thoughts, and social interactions. Emotion regulation refers to

the processes by which individuals manage their emotional experiences in response

to various situations. Two primary emotion regulation strategies are expression

suppression and cognitive reappraisal.

Expression suppression involves hiding or avoiding emotional displays, often by

consciously or unconsciously inhibiting facial expressions, gestures, or vocalizations.

This strategy is commonly used to avoid social consequences, such as the disapproval

or negative evaluation of others, and to maintain interpersonal harmony. However,

expression suppression can also have negative consequences, such as increased

physiological stress, decreased positive affect, and decreased communication

effectiveness in social interactions.

Cognitive reappraisal, on the other hand, involves changing the way we think about a

situation to alter the emotional impact it has on us. This strategy involves identifying

and modifying the meaning we attach to events or stimuli, thereby altering our

emotional response to them. Cognitive reappraisal has been found to be an effective

strategy for reducing negative emotions, such as anxiety, anger, and sadness, and

increasing positive emotions, such as happiness and love.

Research has shown that emotion regulation strategies are not only associated with

different emotional outcomes but also with various cognitive, behavioral, and social

11
outcomes. Expression suppression has been linked to increased stress, decreased

positive affect, and reduced social engagement. Additionally, individuals who

frequently use expression suppression may be at increased risk for developing anxiety

and depression.

Cognitive reappraisal, on the other hand, has been associated with decreased

physiological stress, increased positive affect, and improved social functioning. This

strategy has also been found to be effective in reducing symptoms of anxiety and

depression and in enhancing psychological well-being.

Moreover, recent studies have highlighted the importance of considering individual

differences in the use and effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies. For example,

gender differences in emotion regulation have been reported, with women more likely

to use cognitive reappraisal and men more likely to use expression suppression.

Additionally, individual differences in personality traits, such as neuroticism,

extraversion, and openness, have been found to be associated with different patterns

of emotion regulation strategy use and outcomes.

It is also important to note that emotion regulation strategies are not used in isolation

but in combination with other strategies and in response to different situations. For

example, individuals may use expression suppression to regulate emotions in social

situations where emotional displays are considered inappropriate or to avoid conflicts,

while using cognitive reappraisal in private settings where they have more control

over their emotional experiences.

12
Recent research has also explored the neural mechanisms underlying emotion

regulation strategies. Neuroimaging studies have shown that expression suppression

is associated with increased activation in brain regions involved in inhibitory control,

such as the prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia. In contrast, cognitive reappraisal

is associated with increased activation in brain regions involved in cognitive

reappraisal, such as the prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex.

In conclusion, emotion regulation strategies, such as expression suppression and

cognitive reappraisal, are important components of adaptive emotional functioning.

These strategies are associated with different emotional outcomes and can have

various cognitive, behavioral, and social consequences. Individual differences, such

as gender and personality traits, can also influence the use and effectiveness of

emotion regulation strategies. Understanding the mechanisms underlying these

strategies can help individuals to develop healthier and more effective ways of

managing their emotional experiences.

Cognitive Reappraisal:

Cognitive reappraisal is an emotion regulation strategy that involves altering one’s

cognitive interpretation of a situation in order to change its emotional impact. The

cognitive component of emotion regulation, including cognitive reappraisal, is

believed to play an important role in shaping emotional responses (Gross, 1998).

Cognitive reappraisal can be divided into three general stages: (1) the identification

of a potentially emotional situation, (2) the selection of an appropriate reappraisal

13
strategy, and (3) the implementation of that strategy (Gross, 2002). The goal of

cognitive reappraisal is to modify the emotional impact of a situation by changing the

way in which it is perceived and evaluated.

Research has demonstrated that cognitive reappraisal can be an effective strategy for

regulating emotion. For example, studies have shown that individuals who frequently

use cognitive reappraisal are less likely to experience negative emotions, such as

anxiety and depression, and more likely to experience positive emotions, such as

happiness and contentment (Gross & John, 2003).

Furthermore, cognitive reappraisal has been shown to have a variety of positive

outcomes. For example, it has been associated with improved mental health, better

social functioning, and enhanced physical health (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, &

Schweizer, 2010). Moreover, cognitive reappraisal has been found to be effective in

reducing the emotional distress associated with a range of mental health disorders,

including anxiety disorders and depression (McRae, Ciesielski, & Gross, 2012).

Expression Suppression:

Expression suppression is an emotion regulation strategy that involves inhibiting the

outward expression of an emotion while continuing to experience it internally.

Expression suppression can be an effective short-term strategy for regulating emotion,

as it can prevent the escalation of negative emotions in social situations. However, it

may also have negative long-term consequences, as it can interfere with emotional

processing and lead to a buildup of negative emotions (Gross & John, 2003).

14
Research has shown that expression suppression can have negative consequences for

emotional wellbeing. For example, studies have found that individuals who frequently

use expression suppression experience more negative emotions, such as anxiety and

depression, and have poorer social functioning than those who use other emotion

regulation strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal (John & Gross, 2004).

Moreover, expression suppression has been found to have negative physiological

consequences. For example, studies have shown that the suppression of emotions can

lead to increased levels of stress hormones and cardiovascular reactivity, which can

have negative long-term health consequences (Gross, 2002).

Overall, expression suppression is a strategy that may be useful in some contexts, but

it is not an effective long-term strategy for regulating emotion. In contrast, cognitive

reappraisal has been shown to have a variety of positive outcomes and is a more

effective long-term strategy for regulating emotion.

In conclusion, cognitive reappraisal and expression suppression are two different

emotion regulation strategies that have been studied extensively. Cognitive

reappraisal involves changing the way in which a situation is perceived in order to

alter its emotional impact, while expression suppression involves inhibiting the

outward expression of an emotion while continuing to experience it internally. While

cognitive reappraisal has been found to have a variety of positive outcomes,

expression suppression has been associated with negative emotional and physiological

15
consequences. Thus, cognitive reappraisal is a more effective long-term strategy for

regulating emotion than expression suppression.

Attachment theory is a psychological framework that explains how individuals form

and maintain emotional bonds with others. According to attachment theory, the

quality of an individual’s early caregiving experiences shapes their internal working

models of themselves and others, which in turn influence their attachment style in

adulthood (Bowlby, 1969).

Attachment styles can be categorized into three main categories: secure attachment,

anxious attachment, and avoidant attachment. Secure attachment is characterized by

a strong sense of emotional security, trust, and comfort with closeness and intimacy.

Anxious attachment is characterized by a preoccupation with relationships and a fear

of abandonment. Avoidant attachment is characterized by a tendency to avoid

emotional closeness and a discomfort with dependence on others.

Anxiety Attachment Style:

Individuals with an anxious attachment style tend to feel insecure in their relationships

and worry that their partner will abandon or reject them (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016).

This anxiety can lead to behaviors such as clinginess, jealousy, and a need for constant

reassurance from their partner. Anxious individuals may also have a tendency to

overthink and overanalyze their relationships, which can lead to increased anxiety and

stress.

Research has shown that individuals with an anxious attachment style may have

difficulty regulating their emotions, particularly in the face of relationship conflict

16
(Ein-Dor, Mikulincer, & Doron, 2010). They may also engage in maladaptive emotion

regulation strategies, such as rumination and emotional suppression, which can lead

to negative outcomes such as anxiety and depression (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016).

Furthermore, individuals with an anxious attachment style may have a tendency to

interpret ambiguous social cues as negative, which can lead to increased anxiety and

insecurity in their relationships (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). For example, an anxious

individual may interpret their partner's failure to return a text message as a sign that

their partner is losing interest in the relationship, even if there is no evidence to support

this interpretation.

Avoidance Attachment Style:

Individuals with an avoidant attachment style tend to feel uncomfortable with

emotional closeness and may avoid intimate relationships (Mikulincer & Shaver,

2016). Avoidant individuals may have difficulty expressing their emotions to others

and may be hesitant to rely on others for support.

Research has shown that individuals with an avoidant attachment style may have

difficulty recognizing their own emotions, which can interfere with their ability to

form and maintain intimate relationships (Crowell, Fraley, & Shaver, 2015). They

may also engage in maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as suppression of

emotion and avoidance of emotional topics, which can lead to negative outcomes such

as loneliness and isolation (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016).

Furthermore, individuals with an avoidant attachment style may be less likely to seek

social support in times of stress, which can further exacerbate their feelings of

loneliness and isolation (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). Avoidant individuals may also

17
have a tendency to minimize the importance of relationships in their lives, which can

lead to a lack of intimacy and connection with others.

Treatment for Anxiety and Avoidance Attachment Styles:

Attachment-based therapy has been found to be effective in improving attachment

style and enhancing emotion regulation skills for both anxious and avoidant

individuals (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). This type of therapy focuses on developing

a more secure attachment style by addressing underlying emotional issues and

enhancing emotion regulation skills.

Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) has also been found to be effective in treating

anxiety and avoidance attachment styles (Snyder, 2013). CBT focuses on identifying

and changing negative thought patterns and maladaptive behaviors that contribute to

anxiety and avoidance in relationships.

In conclusion, anxiety and avoidance attachment styles are characterized by a range

of emotional and behavioral patterns that can impact relationships. Individuals with

an anxious attachment style may struggle with regulating their emotions and

interpreting social cues, while individuals with an avoidant attachment style may

struggle with emotional closeness and recognizing their own emotions. Attachment-

based therapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy are effective treatments for improving

attachment style and enhancing emotion regulation skills.

Anxiety attachment style is characterized by feelings of insecurity and the fear of

rejection in relationships. Anxious individuals may resort to maladaptive emotion

regulation strategies like rumination and emotional suppression, leading to negative

outcomes such as anxiety and depression. On the other hand, individuals with

18
avoidant attachment style feel uncomfortable with emotional closeness and may avoid

intimate relationships. They may have difficulty expressing their emotions and seek

to avoid emotional topics. This can lead to negative outcomes such as loneliness and

isolation. Cognitive-behavioral therapy and attachment-based therapy are effective

treatments for anxiety and avoidance attachment styles.

Attachment-based therapy focuses on addressing underlying emotional issues and

enhancing emotion regulation skills to develop a more secure attachment style.

Cognitive-behavioral therapy, on the other hand, helps individuals identify and

change negative thought patterns and maladaptive behaviors contributing to anxiety

and avoidance in relationships. It is crucial to recognize the characteristics of anxiety

and avoidance attachment styles to improve relationships and overall well-being.

Attachment theory provides a framework for understanding how individuals form and

maintain emotional bonds with others. Attachment styles, including anxious and

avoidant attachment, are shaped by early caregiving experiences and influence adult

relationship functioning. Individuals with an anxious attachment style tend to be

overly sensitive to rejection and may engage in maladaptive emotion regulation

strategies, while individuals with an avoidant attachment style tend to be emotionally

distant and may have difficulty recognizing and expressing their emotions.

Attachment-based therapy is an effective intervention for improving attachment style

and enhancing emotion regulation skills.

19
Statement of Problem:

Attachment theory has gained considerable attention in recent decades as a framework

for understanding adult romantic relationships. Attachment styles, including anxiety

and avoidance, have been linked to various outcomes in romantic relationships.

Emotion regulation strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal and expression

suppression, have also been found to impact relationship outcomes. However, there is

a lack of research on how emotion regulation strategies affect attachment styles in

adult romantic relationships.

The problem addressed in this study is the limited understanding of the impact of

emotion regulation strategies on attachment styles in adult romantic relationships.

Previous studies have explored the relationship between attachment styles and

emotion regulation strategies, but there is limited research on how these strategies

affect attachment styles in romantic relationships. This gap in the literature highlights

the need for further research in this area.

One potential consequence of this limited understanding is the negative impact on

relationships. Emotion regulation strategies are crucial for managing emotions in

relationships, and a lack of effective emotion regulation skills can lead to negative

outcomes, including relationship dissatisfaction and dissolution. If individuals with

certain attachment styles tend to use maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, this

may exacerbate their attachment style, leading to a cycle of negative outcomes.

Another potential consequence of this limited understanding is the limited ability to

develop effective interventions to improve attachment styles in romantic

20
relationships. If we do not understand how emotion regulation strategies impact

attachment styles, it may be challenging to develop effective interventions to improve

attachment styles and overall relationship satisfaction. Without effective

interventions, individuals may continue to struggle with negative outcomes in their

relationships, leading to reduced well-being.

Therefore, understanding the impact of emotion regulation strategies on attachment

styles in adult romantic relationships is essential for improving relationship

satisfaction and overall well-being. This study seeks to fill this gap in the literature by

exploring the relationship between emotion regulation strategies and attachment styles

in adult romantic relationships.

The specific research questions addressed in this study include:

1. What is the relationship between cognitive reappraisal and attachment styles in

adult romantic relationships?

2. What is the relationship between expression suppression and attachment styles in

adult romantic relationships?

3. Does the relationship between emotion regulation strategies and attachment styles

differ for individuals with different attachment styles?

By addressing these research questions, this study aims to provide a better

understanding of the impact of emotion regulation strategies on attachment styles in

adult romantic relationships. The findings from this study can inform interventions

aimed at improving attachment styles and relationship satisfaction, ultimately

contributing to enhanced well-being for individuals in romantic relationships.

21
Research Objectives:

(1) To understand the prevalence of expression suppression among the youth

population.

(2) To understand the role of the age factor and its influence on the dominant

emotion regulation strategy.

(3) To understand the role of income source and family settings and their

influence on the dominant type of attachment style.

(4) To assess the relationship between cognitive reappraisal and attachment

avoidance.

(5) To assess the relationship between expression suppression and attachment

anxiety.

(6) To understand the role of residential settings and their influence on the

dominant type of attachment style.

(7) To compute the relationship between emotion regulation strategies and

attachment styles.

(8) To provide directions of future research in this area.

22
Research Hypothesis:

H1: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by male and female college-going students.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by male and female college-going students.

H2: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by individuals between the age range of 18 to 20

versus the ones lying between the age range of 21 to 25.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by individuals between the age range of 18 to 20

versus the ones lying between the age range of 21 to 25.

H3: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by financially independent individuals versus

pocket-money-dependent individuals.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by financially independent individuals versus

pocket-money-dependent individuals.

H4: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of avoidance levels manifested

by individuals living with parents versus the ones living independently or with friends.

23
H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of avoidance levels manifested

by individuals living with parents versus the ones living independently or with friends.

H5: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of avoidance levels manifested

by individuals brought up in a nuclear family versus in a joint family.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of avoidance levels manifested

by individuals brought up in a nuclear family versus in a joint family.

H6: There is a significant relationship between maladaptive expressive suppression

ER strategy and anxiety levels in individuals.

H0: There is no significant relationship between maladaptive expressive suppression

ER strategy and anxiety levels in individuals.

H7: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by male and female college-going students.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by male and female college-going students.

H8: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by individuals between the age range of 18 to 20

versus the ones lying between the age range of 21 to 25.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by individuals between the age range of 18 to 20

versus the ones lying between the age range of 21 to 25.

24
H9: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by financially independent individuals versus

pocket-money-dependent individuals.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by financially independent individuals versus

pocket-money-dependent individuals.

H10: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of anxiety levels manifested

by individuals living with parents versus the ones living independently or with friends.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of anxiety levels manifested

by individuals living with parents versus the ones living independently or with friends.

H11: There is a significant difference in the prevalence of anxiety levels manifested

by individuals brought up in a nuclear family versus in a joint family.

H0: There is no significant difference in the prevalence of anxiety levels manifested

by individuals brought up in a nuclear family versus in a joint family.

H12: There is a significant relationship between adaptive cognitive reappraisal ER

strategy and avoidance levels in individuals.

H0: There is a significant relationship between adaptive cognitive reappraisal ER

strategy and avoidance levels in individuals.

25
CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW

26
Literature on Variable I (Emotion Regulation Strategies)

Introduction:

Emotions play a crucial role in human behavior and affect every aspect of our lives.

Emotion regulation is the process by which individuals manage their emotional

experiences in order to achieve their goals, maintain social relationships, and reduce

negative emotional experiences. Emotion regulation strategies can be classified into

two categories: antecedent-focused strategies and response-focused strategies.

Antecedent-focused strategies involve changing the situation or cognitive appraisal of

a situation before an emotion is experienced, while response-focused strategies

involve changing the emotion after it has been experienced (Gross, 2015). This

literature review aims to provide an in-depth analysis of various emotion regulation

strategies and their effectiveness in managing emotions.

Antecedent-Focused Strategies:

Antecedent-focused strategies are proactive in nature and involve modifying the

environment or one's thoughts about the environment to prevent or reduce emotional

experiences. One antecedent-focused strategy is cognitive reappraisal, which involves

reinterpreting the meaning of a situation to change one's emotional response. For

example, instead of feeling angry when a friend cancels plans, an individual may

reappraise the situation and realize that their friend had a legitimate reason for

canceling. Cognitive reappraisal has been shown to be effective in reducing negative

emotions in both laboratory and real-life settings (Gross & John, 2003).

27
Another antecedent-focused strategy is situation selection, which involves choosing

to enter or avoid a situation based on its emotional impact. For example, an individual

may avoid a crowded party if they know it will make them feel anxious. Situation

selection has been shown to be effective in reducing negative emotions and increasing

positive emotions (Gross, 2015).

Response-Focused Strategies:

Response-focused strategies are reactive in nature and involve changing one's

emotional experience after it has been experienced. One response-focused strategy is

suppression, which involves inhibiting the expression of emotions. Suppression has

been shown to be ineffective in reducing negative emotions and can have negative

consequences on social interactions (Butler et al., 2003).

Another response-focused strategy is reappraisal, which involves changing the way

one thinks about the emotion after it has been experienced. For example, an individual

may reappraise their fear of public speaking as excitement rather than anxiety.

Reappraisal has been shown to be effective in reducing negative emotions and

improving well-being (Gross & John, 2003).

Neurobiological Basis of Emotion Regulation:

Functional imaging studies have shown that emotion regulation is associated with

activation of prefrontal cortex regions involved in cognitive control and emotion

28
regulation, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), ventrolateral

prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Ochsner et al.,

2012). These regions are involved in attentional control, working memory, and

cognitive reappraisal.

Gender Differences in Emotion Regulation:

Gender differences have been observed in emotion regulation strategies. Women tend

to use more cognitive reappraisal and less suppression than men, which may be related

to socialization processes that encourage women to express their emotions and

discourage men from doing so (Gross & John, 2003). However, the effectiveness of

emotion regulation strategies does not differ significantly between genders.

Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology:

Emotion dysregulation is a core feature of many psychopathologies, including anxiety

disorders, mood disorders, and personality disorders. Cognitive reappraisal has been

shown to be effective in reducing symptoms of anxiety and depression (Gross, 2015).

However, individuals with certain psychopathologies, such as borderline personality

disorder, may have difficulty using cognitive reappraisal effectively (Gross, 2015).

29
Conclusion:

Emotion regulation strategies are important for maintaining emotional well-being and

social relationships. Antecedent-focused strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal and

situation selection, have been shown to be effective in reducing negative emotions,

while response-focused strategies, such as suppression and reappraisal, have varying

levels of effectiveness depending on the situation and individual. Neurobiological

research has identified regions of the prefrontal cortex involved in emotion regulation,

while gender differences have been observed in the use of emotion regulation

strategies. Additionally, emotion dysregulation is a core feature of many

psychopathologies, and effective emotion regulation strategies can be an important

aspect of treatment.

Overall, the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies depends on a variety of

factors, including the individual, the situation, and the emotion being regulated.

Further research is needed to better understand the mechanisms underlying these

strategies and to identify effective interventions for individuals with emotion

dysregulation.

30
Emotion regulation refers to the processes by which individuals modulate the

intensity, duration, and expression of their emotions. It is a fundamental component

of adaptive functioning, facilitating effective coping with stressors and promoting

well-being. One common way to regulate emotions is through cognitive reappraisal

and expression suppression. Cognitive reappraisal involves changing the way one

thinks about a situation to alter emotional responses, while expression suppression

involves inhibiting the outward expression of emotions. This literature review will

examine the research on cognitive reappraisal and expression suppression as emotion

regulation strategies in psychology.

Cognitive Reappraisal:

Cognitive reappraisal is a strategy for managing emotional reactions by changing the

way one thinks about a situation. This technique involves reframing an emotional

event in a more positive light, thereby reducing negative emotions such as anxiety,

anger, or sadness. The process of cognitive reappraisal is thought to involve top-down

processing, where the individual's cognitive processes modulate their emotional

responses (Gross, 2015). For example, if someone is feeling anxious about an

upcoming presentation, they might use cognitive reappraisal by reminding themselves

that they have prepared thoroughly and that the presentation will be an opportunity to

showcase their skills.

Research has shown that cognitive reappraisal can be an effective emotion regulation

strategy. A meta-analysis of 156 studies found that cognitive reappraisal was

associated with reduced negative emotions, increased positive emotions, and

improved cognitive functioning (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010).

31
Furthermore, a study conducted by McRae and colleagues (2010) found that

individuals who engaged in more cognitive reappraisal had greater activity in the

prefrontal cortex, a brain region involved in cognitive control, and were better able to

regulate their emotions.

Expression Suppression:

Expression suppression is a strategy for regulating emotions by inhibiting the outward

expression of emotions. This technique involves suppressing or concealing emotions

that might be deemed inappropriate or socially undesirable. The process of expression

suppression is thought to involve bottom-up processing, where the individual's

emotional responses modulate their cognitive processes (Gross, 2015). For example,

if someone is feeling angry, they might suppress their expression of anger to avoid

escalating the situation.

While expression suppression may be useful in some contexts, research suggests that

it can have negative consequences for emotional well-being. A study by Gross and

John (2003) found that individuals who habitually used expression suppression had

higher levels of physiological arousal, as measured by skin conductance response, and

reported more negative emotions than those who used cognitive reappraisal.

Similarly, a study by Gross and Levenson (1997) found that suppressing emotions

during marital conflict was associated with increased cardiovascular activity,

suggesting that expression suppression can have physiological consequences.

32
Comparison of Cognitive Reappraisal and Expression Suppression:

Although cognitive reappraisal and expression suppression are both emotion

regulation strategies, research suggests that they have different consequences for

emotional well-being. Cognitive reappraisal has been associated with reduced

negative emotions and increased positive emotions, as well as improved cognitive

functioning. In contrast, expression suppression has been associated with increased

physiological arousal and negative emotions. Moreover, cognitive reappraisal appears

to be more effective in promoting adaptive coping than expression suppression.

One possible explanation for these differences is that cognitive reappraisal involves a

more active and flexible approach to emotion regulation, whereas expression

suppression involves a more passive and rigid approach. Cognitive reappraisal

involves actively changing the way one thinks about a situation, whereas expression

suppression involves inhibiting the natural expression of emotions. Furthermore,

cognitive reappraisal is thought to involve more top-down processing, whereas

expression suppression involves more bottom-up processing.

In conclusion, emotion regulation is a critical aspect of human behavior that plays a

significant role in psychological functioning, mental health, and interpersonal

relationships. Cognitive reappraisal and expression suppression are two primary

emotion regulation strategies that individuals use to manage their emotions

effectively. While cognitive reappraisal involves altering the meaning of a situation

to modify the emotional response, expression suppression involves inhibiting the

behavioral expression of emotions.

33
Both cognitive reappraisal and expression suppression have been found to have

different effects on mental health, social relationships, and other outcomes. The

effectiveness of these strategies may depend on various factors, including the

individual's attachment style, personality traits, and situational factors.

Furthermore, attachment styles, which refer to the emotional bond between

individuals in close relationships, have also been found to influence emotion

regulation and social outcomes. Individuals with insecure attachment styles, such as

anxious or avoidant attachment, may experience difficulties in regulating their

emotions and may be more vulnerable to mental health problems and interpersonal

conflicts.

Overall, understanding the interplay between emotion regulation strategies and

attachment styles can provide valuable insights into how individuals manage their

emotions in close relationships and cope with life stressors. The literature reviewed

in this study highlights the need for further research to explore the complex

relationships between these variables and their implications for mental health and

interpersonal functioning.

Future studies could also examine how interventions that target emotion regulation

strategies and attachment styles may be used to promote mental health and improve

relationship outcomes. By enhancing our understanding of emotion regulation and

attachment processes, researchers and practitioners can develop more effective

strategies for promoting psychological well-being and fostering positive social

relationships.

34
Literature on Variable II (Attachment Styles)

Introduction:

Attachment styles refer to the patterns of emotional, cognitive and behavioural

responses that individuals exhibit towards their significant others, primarily romantic

partners. Attachment theory has been studied extensively over the past few decades,

with research indicating that attachment styles are shaped by early childhood

experiences with caregivers, and can have a significant impact on adult romantic

relationships. This literature review will examine the different attachment styles

identified in psychology, their underlying mechanisms and how they affect adult

relationships.

Secure Attachment Style:

The secure attachment style is characterized by individuals who have a positive view

of themselves and their relationships with others. These individuals have a relatively

easy time forming and maintaining close relationships, and tend to be comfortable

with intimacy and emotional closeness. They exhibit low levels of attachment anxiety

and avoidance and are comfortable with relying on their partner for support when

needed.

Research has consistently found that secure attachment styles are associated with

higher levels of relationship satisfaction and greater longevity of relationships.

Individuals with secure attachment styles tend to be more communicative, empathetic,

35
and accepting of their partner’s faults. They are less likely to engage in behaviours

that undermine the relationship, such as infidelity or avoidance.

Anxious Attachment Style:

Individuals with an anxious attachment style tend to be preoccupied with their

relationships, often worrying about whether their partner truly loves them and is

committed to the relationship. They exhibit high levels of attachment anxiety and low

levels of avoidance. These individuals often fear abandonment and tend to cling to

their partners in an effort to maintain their connection.

Research has found that individuals with an anxious attachment style are more likely

to engage in behaviours that undermine the relationship, such as jealousy and

possessiveness. They also tend to exhibit more negative emotions and conflict in their

relationships. These behaviours often push their partner away, further exacerbating

their fear of abandonment.

Avoidant Attachment Style:

Individuals with an avoidant attachment style tend to be uncomfortable with intimacy

and emotional closeness. They exhibit low levels of attachment anxiety and high

levels of avoidance. These individuals often maintain emotional distance from their

partners and may have difficulty expressing their emotions or relying on their partner

for support. They may also have a tendency to withdraw from conflict in an effort to

maintain their emotional distance.

36
Research has found that individuals with an avoidant attachment style are more likely

to engage in behaviours that undermine the relationship, such as emotional distance

and avoidance. They also tend to exhibit less empathy and less investment in the

relationship. This can lead to feelings of loneliness and dissatisfaction in the

relationship for both partners.

Mechanisms Underlying Attachment Styles:

Attachment styles are believed to be shaped by early childhood experiences with

caregivers, particularly the responsiveness and sensitivity of the caregiver to the

child’s needs. Children who experience consistent and responsive care from their

caregivers tend to develop a secure attachment style, while children who experience

inconsistent or neglectful care may develop anxious or avoidant attachment styles.

Neurobiological research has also shed light on the mechanisms underlying

attachment styles. Studies have found that individuals with a secure attachment style

exhibit greater activation in brain regions associated with positive emotions and social

cognition, while individuals with an avoidant attachment style exhibit greater

activation in brain regions associated with negative emotions and cognitive control.

Conclusion:

Attachment styles play a significant role in shaping adult romantic relationships. The

three primary attachment styles identified in psychology are secure, anxious, and

37
avoidant. Individuals with a secure attachment style tend to have positive and lasting

relationships, while individuals with an anxious or avoidant attachment style are more

likely to experience conflict and dissatisfaction. Attachment styles are believed to be

shaped by early childhood experiences with caregivers and are associated with

neurobiological differences in brain functioning. Future research may focus on

identifying effective interventions for individuals with insecure attachment styles and

improving relationship outcomes.

Attachment theory proposes that individuals develop internal working models of

relationships based on early childhood experiences with primary caregivers. These

working models can shape an individual's perceptions of self and others and influence

their attachment style in adulthood. Attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance are

two primary dimensions of attachment styles that have received considerable attention

in psychology research. In this literature review, we will provide an in-depth

exploration of attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance, including their

definitions, development, assessment, and impact on mental health and interpersonal

relationships.

Attachment Anxiety

Attachment anxiety refers to the extent to which an individual experiences fear of

rejection, abandonment, or loss in close relationships. Individuals with high levels of

attachment anxiety may have an intense need for reassurance and closeness from their

partners and may become overly dependent on their partners for emotional support.

They may also be more likely to experience jealousy and insecurity in relationships

and may exhibit clingy or demanding behaviors.

38
Development of Attachment Anxiety

Attachment anxiety is believed to develop as a result of inconsistent or unpredictable

caregiving in early childhood. Children who experience intermittent or unpredictable

responses to their needs may develop a sense of insecurity and uncertainty in their

relationships with caregivers, leading to heightened sensitivity to separation and loss

in later relationships.

Assessment of Attachment Anxiety

Attachment anxiety can be assessed using self-report measures such as the

Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) questionnaire. The ECR measures

attachment anxiety on a continuum from low to high and asks individuals to rate their

level of agreement with statements such as "I often worry that my partner will abandon

me."

Impact of Attachment Anxiety on Mental Health and Interpersonal Relationships

Attachment anxiety has been found to be associated with a range of negative mental

health outcomes, including depression, anxiety, and low self-esteem. Individuals with

high levels of attachment anxiety may be more vulnerable to the negative impact of

stress and may have a more significant emotional reaction to negative events in their

lives. In terms of interpersonal relationships, attachment anxiety has been linked to

39
less stable and less satisfying relationships and a greater likelihood of conflict and

infidelity.

Attachment Avoidance

Attachment avoidance refers to the extent to which an individual seeks to avoid

intimacy and emotional closeness in relationships. Individuals with high levels of

attachment avoidance may view emotional intimacy as threatening and may attempt

to maintain emotional distance from their partners. They may also be more likely to

engage in behaviors such as withholding emotions, dismissing their partner's needs,

and avoiding physical closeness.

Development of Attachment Avoidance

Attachment avoidance is believed to develop as a result of consistent rejection or

neglect in early childhood. Children who experience consistent rejection or neglect

may develop a sense of independence and self-reliance as a way to cope with the lack

of emotional support from caregivers.

Assessment of Attachment Avoidance

Attachment avoidance can be assessed using self-report measures such as the ECR

questionnaire. The ECR measures attachment avoidance on a continuum from low to

high and asks individuals to rate their level of agreement with statements such as "I

prefer not to depend on others or have others depend on me."

40
Impact of Attachment Avoidance on Mental Health and Interpersonal Relationships

Attachment avoidance has been found to be associated with a range of negative mental

health outcomes, including depression, anxiety, and substance abuse. Individuals with

high levels of attachment avoidance may be more likely to experience emotional

numbness and detachment and may have difficulty forming close relationships. In

terms of interpersonal relationships, attachment avoidance has been linked to less

intimate and less satisfying relationships and a greater likelihood of conflict and

infidelity.

Interaction between Attachment Anxiety and Attachment Avoidance

Attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance are not mutually exclusive and can co-

occur in individuals. Individuals who exhibit high levels of both attachment anxiety

and attachment avoidance may experience conflicting desires for closeness and

independence in their relationships, leading to a cycle of push-pull behavior and

relationship instability.

In conclusion, attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance are two prominent

attachment styles that have been extensively researched in psychology. Both

attachment styles have been linked to various negative outcomes in adult relationships

such as increased conflict, decreased satisfaction, and higher rates of breakups and

divorce. However, there is hope for individuals with these attachment styles as

41
research has also shown that with proper interventions and therapies, individuals can

develop more secure attachment styles and experience healthier relationships.

Overall, attachment anxiety and avoidance are complex and multifaceted constructs

that require further research to fully understand their mechanisms and the best ways

to address them in clinical settings. Future studies may focus on exploring the different

factors that contribute to the development of these attachment styles, as well as

developing more effective interventions that can help individuals transition to more

secure attachment styles. Through continued research and exploration, we can gain a

deeper understanding of attachment styles and provide individuals with the tools they

need to form and maintain healthy relationships.

42
Theoretical Approach of Variable I (Emotion Regulation Strategies):

Emotion regulation is a complex process that involves the modification of emotions

in order to meet one's goals and adapt to the environment. While there are many

different approaches to emotion regulation, two of the most commonly studied

strategies are cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. In this review, we will

provide an in-depth analysis of the technical approach to these emotion regulation

strategies, as well as the scales used to measure emotion regulation in psychology.

Cognitive Reappraisal

Cognitive reappraisal is an emotion regulation strategy that involves reinterpreting the

meaning of an emotional situation in order to change one's emotional response. This

approach is often used in situations where it is not possible or appropriate to change

the situation itself. For example, if someone receives negative feedback at work, they

may use cognitive reappraisal to view the feedback as an opportunity for growth rather

than as a personal attack.

The technical approach to cognitive reappraisal involves several key steps. First, the

individual must identify the emotional stimulus and their initial emotional response.

Next, they must generate an alternative interpretation of the situation that is more

adaptive and less emotionally arousing. Finally, they must actively replace their initial

emotional response with the new interpretation.

Research has shown that cognitive reappraisal can be an effective emotion regulation

strategy in a variety of contexts. For example, studies have found that individuals who

use cognitive reappraisal in response to stress experience lower levels of anxiety and

43
depression than those who do not use this strategy (Gross & John, 2003). In addition,

cognitive reappraisal has been shown to be effective in reducing negative emotions in

response to social rejection (Eisenberger et al., 2003) and physical pain (Lieberman

et al., 2011).

Measurement of Cognitive Reappraisal

In order to measure cognitive reappraisal, researchers have developed several self-

report scales. One of the most widely used measures is the Emotion Regulation

Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003), which assesses the use of both cognitive

reappraisal and expressive suppression. The cognitive reappraisal subscale of the ERQ

consists of six items that ask respondents to rate how frequently they engage in

reappraisal in response to emotional situations (e.g., "I control my emotions by

changing the way I think about the situation I'm in").

Other measures of cognitive reappraisal include the Cognitive Reappraisal Scale

(CRS; Peña-Sarrionandia et al., 2015) and the Reappraisal Inventory (RI; Rachman,

1980). The CRS consists of 12 items that assess the use of reappraisal in response to

both positive and negative emotions (e.g., "I try to see the positive aspects of a

situation when I am feeling down"). The RI is a 12-item scale that assesses the use of

reappraisal in response to anxiety-provoking situations (e.g., "I try to see the situation

in a different light in order to calm myself down").

Cognitive reappraisal is a commonly used emotion regulation strategy that involves

changing the way one thinks about a situation in order to alter the emotional response.

There are several scales used to measure cognitive reappraisal in psychological

research, including the Cognitive Reappraisal Scale (CRS) and the Emotion

Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ).

44
1. The Cognitive Reappraisal Scale (CRS) was developed by Gross and John

(2003) and consists of seven items that assess an individual's tendency to use

cognitive reappraisal as an emotion regulation strategy. Participants rate each

item on a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7

(strongly agree). Sample items include "When I want to feel less negative

emotion, I change the way I'm thinking about the situation" and "When I'm

faced with a stressful situation, I make myself think about it in a way that helps

me stay calm."

2. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) was developed by Gross and

colleagues (2003) and assesses the use of two emotion regulation strategies:

cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. The cognitive reappraisal

subscale includes six items that assess an individual's tendency to cognitively

reappraise a situation in order to regulate their emotional response.

Participants rate each item on a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1

(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Sample items include "When I want

to feel more positive emotion (such as joy or amusement), I change the way

I'm thinking about the situation" and "When I'm faced with a stressful

situation, I try to think about it in a way that helps me stay calm."

The ERQ has been widely used in psychological research and has demonstrated good

psychometric properties, including high internal consistency and test-retest reliability

(Gross & John, 2003; Gross & Thompson, 2007).

45
Other scales used to measure cognitive reappraisal include the Cognitive Emotion

Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) and the Positive Reappraisal Scale (PRS). The

CERQ was developed by Garnefski and Kraaij (2006) and assesses nine different

cognitive emotion regulation strategies, including cognitive reappraisal. The PRS was

developed by Stanton and colleagues (2000) and assesses an individual's tendency to

use positive reappraisal specifically in response to a stressful event.

Overall, these scales provide valuable measures of an individual's tendency to use

cognitive reappraisal as an emotion regulation strategy, allowing researchers to better

understand the impact of this strategy on mental health and well-being.

Expressive Suppression

Expressive suppression is an emotion regulation strategy that involves the inhibition

of emotional expressions to avoid displaying or communicating one's emotions to

others. This approach is often used in situations where it is not socially acceptable to

express one's emotions or where the expression of emotions may have negative

consequences. For example, someone may use expressive suppression to hide their

disappointment when they receive bad news in a public setting.

The technical approach to expressive suppression involves several key steps. First, the

individual must become aware of their emotional response and the urge to express it.

Next, they must actively inhibit the expression of the emotion, often by using physical

or cognitive strategies such as holding their breath or distracting themselves. Finally,

they must maintain the inhibition of emotion until the situation or context changes and

it becomes socially acceptable or safe to express the emotion. This process can be

difficult and may result in negative consequences if not managed effectively.

46
To measure expressive suppression as an emotion regulation strategy, researchers

have developed various scales and instruments that assess an individual's tendency to

inhibit the expression of emotions. One commonly used scale is the Emotion

Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) developed by Gross and John in 2003. The ERQ

measures two dimensions of emotion regulation, cognitive reappraisal and expressive

suppression, using a series of self-report items. The expressive suppression subscale

consists of six items that assess the frequency with which an individual inhibits the

expression of their emotions in social situations, such as "When I am feeling negative

emotions, I make sure not to express them" and "I control my emotions by not

expressing them". Participants rate their agreement with each item on a 7-point Likert

scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), with higher scores

indicating a greater tendency to use expressive suppression as an emotion regulation

strategy.

Another scale commonly used to measure expressive suppression is the Courtauld

Emotional Control Scale (CECS) developed by Martin and colleagues in 1983. The

CECS is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 21 items that assess an individual's

tendency to control their emotional expression in various situations, such as "I keep a

lid on my feelings" and "I rarely show my emotions to others". Participants rate their

agreement with each item on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all like me)

to 3 (very much like me), with higher scores indicating a greater tendency to use

expressive suppression.

In addition to these self-report scales, researchers have also used physiological

measures to assess expressive suppression. For example, electrodermal activity

(EDA) has been used to measure the skin conductance response (SCR) during

emotional experiences. The SCR reflects the activity of the sympathetic nervous

47
system and increases when an individual experiences emotional arousal. However,

when an individual engages in expressive suppression, the SCR may be reduced or

delayed, indicating inhibition of emotional expression.

In conclusion, expressive suppression is a common emotion regulation strategy used

by individuals to control the expression of their emotions in social situations. While

it may be effective in some contexts, it can have negative consequences if not

managed effectively. Researchers have developed various scales and measures to

assess an individual's tendency to use expressive suppression as an emotion regulation

strategy, including the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire and the Courtauld

Emotional Control Scale. Additionally, physiological measures such as electrodermal

activity can be used to assess the inhibition of emotional expression during expressive

suppression.

Expressive suppression, on the other hand, involves inhibiting the outward expression

of emotions, which can have both positive and negative effects. On the one hand,

expressive suppression may be useful in situations where the expression of emotions

is inappropriate or may lead to negative consequences, such as in a work environment

or in a conflict situation. However, overuse of expressive suppression can lead to

negative outcomes, such as increased physiological arousal and decreased relationship

satisfaction.

Several measurement scales have been developed to assess different aspects of

emotion regulation, including the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), which

assesses cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression, and the Difficulties in

Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS), which assesses deficits in emotion regulation. The

ERQ measures the frequency of use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive

48
suppression, while the DERS measures difficulties in emotional awareness,

understanding emotions, and regulating emotions in different contexts. The use of

these measurement scales has contributed to a better understanding of the role of

emotion regulation strategies in the development and maintenance of psychological

well-being.

Expressive suppression is one of the two main strategies used in emotion regulation,

alongside cognitive reappraisal. It involves inhibiting the outward expression of

emotions in order to manage one's emotional responses. To assess the use of

expressive suppression as an emotion regulation strategy, various scales have been

developed in psychology research. Here are some examples:

1. Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ): The ERQ is a self-report measure

developed by Gross and John in 2003 to assess individual differences in the

habitual use of two different emotion regulation strategies: cognitive

reappraisal and expressive suppression. The expressive suppression subscale

of the ERQ contains four items, which ask individuals to rate how often they

try to control the way they express their emotions (e.g., "I keep my emotions

to myself").

2. Courtauld Emotional Control Scale (CECS): The CECS is a self-report

measure developed by Martin and colleagues in 1996 to assess the tendency

to use expressive suppression as a strategy for coping with stress. The scale

contains 21 items, which ask individuals to rate how frequently they use

different emotion regulation strategies (e.g., "I hold back my emotions").

49
3. Emotion Regulation Skills Questionnaire (ERSQ): The ERSQ is a self-report

measure developed by Berking and colleagues in 2008 to assess emotion

regulation skills. The scale contains nine subscales, one of which assesses the

use of expressive suppression as a strategy for emotion regulation. The

expressive suppression subscale contains five items, which ask individuals to

rate how often they suppress their emotional expressions in different situations

(e.g., "When I feel angry, I avoid showing it to others").

4. Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS): The TAS is a self-report measure

developed by Bagby and colleagues in 1994 to assess the construct of

alexithymia, which refers to difficulty in identifying and expressing emotions.

The scale contains 20 items, which assess different aspects of emotional

functioning, including difficulty in identifying and describing emotions. One

subscale of the TAS, called "externally oriented thinking," assesses the

tendency to suppress emotional expression in social situations (e.g., "When

I'm upset, I try not to let anyone see it").

These scales provide researchers and clinicians with a tool to assess the extent to

which individuals use expressive suppression as an emotion regulation strategy in

different contexts. The use of these scales can help identify individuals who may be

at risk for negative outcomes associated with the overuse of expressive suppression,

such as decreased relationship satisfaction and increased physiological arousal.

Additionally, the use of these scales can inform the development of interventions

aimed at helping individuals develop adaptive emotion regulation strategies.

50
In conclusion, emotion regulation is a crucial aspect of psychological well-being and

is relevant to a wide range of psychological disorders. The two most widely studied

emotion regulation strategies are cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression,

which have different effects on emotions and physiological arousal. Cognitive

reappraisal involves changing the way one thinks about a situation to alter its

emotional impact, while expressive suppression involves inhibiting the outward

expression of emotions. The use of these strategies can impact attachment styles,

which can in turn affect the quality of adult relationships. Research has shown that

individuals with an anxious attachment style tend to use less effective emotion

regulation strategies, such as expressive suppression, and are more likely to

experience negative emotions in response to relationship stressors. On the other hand,

individuals with an avoidant attachment style tend to use more cognitive reappraisal

strategies, which can lead to emotional disengagement from the relationship. Future

research is needed to better understand the complex interplay between emotion

regulation strategies and attachment styles, and how this affects the development and

maintenance of adult relationships. Additionally, further research is needed to develop

more comprehensive measures of emotion regulation that capture the range of

strategies used by individuals in response to different types of stressors.

Understanding how individuals regulate their emotions can lead to the development

of effective interventions for individuals experiencing psychological distress, as well

as for those seeking to improve the quality of their interpersonal relationships.

51
Theoretical Approach of Variable II (Attachment Styles):

Attachment styles refer to the characteristic ways in which individuals approach and

interact with others in close relationships. These styles develop in infancy and

childhood and shape the nature and quality of social interactions throughout life. In

order to study attachment styles in psychology, researchers have developed various

scales and measures that allow for a quantitative assessment of attachment styles. This

literature review will provide an overview of the technical approach to measuring

attachment styles and the scales commonly used in psychological research.

The most widely used and well-known scale for measuring attachment styles is the

Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996). This measure

involves a semi-structured interview designed to elicit an individual’s memories and

reflections on their experiences with attachment figures during childhood. The

interview is scored on several dimensions, including coherence, accuracy, and

content, and produces a classification of the individual’s attachment style as either

secure, dismissing, preoccupied, or unresolved.

Another commonly used measure of attachment styles is the Attachment Style

Questionnaire (ASQ) (Feeney, Noller, & Hanrahan, 1994). This self-report measure

consists of 40 items that assess an individual’s beliefs about relationships, including

their feelings of closeness and dependence, fear of rejection or abandonment, and

views on the importance of emotional intimacy. The ASQ yields scores on three

dimensions of attachment style: secure, anxious-ambivalent, and avoidant.

The Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) scale is another widely

used measure of attachment styles (Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000). This self-report

52
questionnaire consists of 36 items that assess an individual’s attitudes and behaviors

in close relationships, including their desire for closeness, fear of rejection or

abandonment, and tendency to seek or avoid intimacy. The ECR-R yields scores on

two dimensions of attachment style: anxiety and avoidance.

The Relationship Scales Questionnaire (RSQ) (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994) is a

self-report measure of attachment styles that assesses an individual’s beliefs and

feelings about relationships. The RSQ consists of 30 items and yields scores on four

dimensions of attachment style: secure, dismissing, preoccupied, and fearful.

The Attachment Inventory (AI) (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991) is a self-report

measure that assesses an individual’s attachment style based on their responses to four

hypothetical relationship scenarios. The AI yields scores on four dimensions of

attachment style: secure, dismissing, preoccupied, and fearful.

The Brief Assessment of Adult Attachment (Collins & Read, 1990) is a self-report

measure of attachment style that consists of 18 items. The measure assesses an

individual’s beliefs about relationships and yields scores on two dimensions of

attachment style: secure and insecure.

In addition to these commonly used measures, there are several other scales and

questionnaires that have been developed to assess attachment styles, including the

Attachment Style Questionnaire for Children (Kerns, Klepac, & Cole, 1996), the

Adult Attachment Scale (Collins & Read, 1990), and the Relationship Questionnaire

(Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991).

53
In summary, the measurement of attachment styles in psychology involves a variety

of scales and measures that assess an individual’s beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors in

close relationships. These measures provide a quantitative assessment of attachment

styles and allow for the study of how attachment styles relate to various psychological

outcomes, such as mental health, relationship satisfaction, and social functioning.

In psychology, attachment anxiety is commonly measured using a variety of self-

report scales and questionnaires. The following are some of the most widely used

measures:

1. Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) Scale: This is a 36-item questionnaire

that measures attachment anxiety and avoidance. Participants rate their agreement

with statements such as "I worry about being abandoned" and "I find it difficult to

allow myself to depend on others". The ECR has demonstrated good internal

consistency, test-retest reliability, and construct validity.

2. Attachment Style Questionnaire (ASQ): The ASQ is a 40-item measure of adult

attachment style that includes three subscales: attachment anxiety, attachment

avoidance, and secure attachment. Participants rate their agreement with statements

such as "I find it easy to get close to others" and "I prefer not to be too close to

romantic partners". The ASQ has demonstrated good psychometric properties,

including internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and convergent validity.

3. Adult Attachment Scale (AAS): The AAS is a 40-item measure of adult attachment

style that includes two subscales: attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance.

Participants rate their agreement with statements such as "I worry a lot about my

54
relationships" and "I find it difficult to trust others completely". The AAS has

demonstrated good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and construct validity.

4. Relationship Questionnaire (RQ): The RQ is a 30-item measure of adult attachment

style that includes three subscales: attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance, and

secure attachment. Participants rate their agreement with statements such as "I worry

about being rejected" and "I am comfortable depending on romantic partners". The

RQ has demonstrated good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and convergent

validity.

5. Attachment Style Measure (ASM): The ASM is a 15-item measure of adult

attachment style that includes three subscales: attachment anxiety, attachment

avoidance, and secure attachment. Participants rate their agreement with statements

such as "I worry about being alone" and "I am comfortable with intimacy and

closeness". The ASM has demonstrated good internal consistency and construct

validity.

Overall, these scales and questionnaires provide researchers and clinicians with

reliable and valid measures of attachment anxiety, allowing for a better understanding

of the impact of attachment styles on various psychological and social outcomes.

55
Attachment avoidance is another important attachment style in psychology that has

been studied using various scales and questionnaires. Here are some commonly used

measures for attachment avoidance:

1. Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) scale: This is a widely used self-report

questionnaire that measures attachment anxiety and avoidance in adult romantic

relationships. The ECR consists of 36 items, with 18 items measuring attachment

anxiety and 18 items measuring attachment avoidance. Respondents rate how much

they agree or disagree with each statement on a 7-point Likert scale, with higher scores

indicating greater attachment avoidance.

2. Adult Attachment Scale (AAS): The AAS is a 40-item self-report questionnaire that

measures attachment styles in adults. It includes 20 items for measuring attachment

anxiety and 20 items for measuring attachment avoidance. Respondents rate each item

on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

Higher scores on the attachment avoidance subscale indicate greater avoidance.

3. Attachment Style Questionnaire (ASQ): The ASQ is a 40-item self-report

questionnaire that measures adult attachment styles. It includes 20 items for

measuring attachment anxiety and 20 items for measuring attachment avoidance.

Respondents rate each item on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly

disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Higher scores on the attachment avoidance subscale

indicate greater avoidance.

56
4. Relationship Questionnaire (RQ): The RQ is a 4-item self-report questionnaire that

measures attachment avoidance in romantic relationships. Respondents rate each item

on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not at all like me) to 7 (very much like me).

Higher scores indicate greater attachment avoidance.

5. The Relationship Scales Questionnaire (RSQ): The RSQ is a 30-item self-report

questionnaire that measures attachment styles in adult romantic relationships. It

includes 15 items for measuring attachment anxiety and 15 items for measuring

attachment avoidance. Respondents rate each item on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging

from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores on the attachment

avoidance subscale indicate greater avoidance.

These scales and questionnaires have been used extensively in attachment research

and have demonstrated good reliability and validity in measuring attachment

avoidance in adults.

57
CHAPTER 3: RESEARCH DESIGN

58
Variables:

The independent variable in the study of the impact of emotion regulation strategies

on attachment styles in adult relationships is the emotion regulation strategy being

used, which can be either expressive suppression or cognitive reappraisal. The

dependent variable is the attachment style of the individual in the adult relationship,

which can be either attachment anxiety or attachment avoidance.

The study aims to investigate whether the use of different emotion regulation

strategies affects an individual's attachment style in their adult relationships. The

choice of emotion regulation strategy is the independent variable because it is

manipulated or varied by the researcher. The attachment style is the dependent

variable because it is influenced by the use of the emotion regulation strategy. For

example, in a study comparing the effects of expressive suppression versus cognitive

reappraisal on attachment anxiety, participants may be randomly assigned to one of

two groups. One group may be instructed to use expressive suppression when

experiencing a negative emotion, while the other group is instructed to use cognitive

reappraisal. The level of attachment anxiety is then measured and compared between

the two groups to determine if there is a significant difference in attachment anxiety

between those who used expressive suppression versus those who used cognitive

reappraisal.

Similarly, in a study comparing the effects of expressive suppression versus cognitive

reappraisal on attachment avoidance, participants may be randomly assigned to one

of two groups. One group may be instructed to use expressive suppression when

experiencing a negative emotion, while the other group is instructed to use cognitive

59
reappraisal. The level of attachment avoidance is then measured and compared

between the two groups to determine if there is a significant difference in attachment

avoidance between those who used expressive suppression versus those who used

cognitive reappraisal. Therefore, the independent variable in both cases is the type of

emotion regulation strategy used (expressive suppression or cognitive reappraisal),

while the dependent variable is the attachment style (attachment anxiety or attachment

avoidance) of the individual in the adult relationship.

(i) Emotion Regulation (Independent Variable):

Emotion regulation can be defined as the process of managing one's emotional

experiences and responses. It involves the ability to recognize and understand one's

own emotions, and then use strategies to either enhance positive emotions or reduce

negative emotions. In the context of research, emotion regulation can be considered

an independent variable, as researchers can manipulate or measure different emotion

regulation strategies to see how they affect other variables of interest, such as

attachment styles.For example, in a study examining the impact of emotion regulation

on attachment styles, researchers may manipulate the use of cognitive reappraisal or

expressive suppression techniques in one group of participants, while a control group

uses no specific emotion regulation strategy. The attachment style of both groups can

then be assessed to determine if there is a difference in attachment style between the

group that used emotion regulation strategies and the control group. Overall, emotion

regulation can be viewed as an independent variable because it can be manipulated or

measured in order to determine how it affects other variables, such as attachment

styles.

60
(ii) Attachment Styles (Dependent Variable):

Attachment styles can be considered dependent variables in research studies because

they are typically measured or observed as a result of other factors, such as childhood

experiences or current interpersonal relationships. Attachment style refers to an

individual's characteristic way of relating to others, which is often shaped by early

interactions with caregivers and subsequent experiences in close relationships

throughout life. For example, in a study examining the relationship between childhood

experiences and adult attachment styles, the experiences of participants during

childhood would be considered the independent variable, while the attachment style

exhibited by participants as adults would be the dependent variable. Researchers could

use a standardized measure of attachment style, such as the Adult Attachment

Interview (AAI), to assess the attachment style of each participant. In another

example, in a study examining the effect of emotion regulation strategies on

attachment styles, the use of emotion regulation techniques would be considered the

independent variable, while the attachment style exhibited by participants after using

these techniques would be the dependent variable. The attachment style could be

measured using a self-report questionnaire, such as the Experiences in Close

Relationships (ECR) scale. Overall, attachment styles are dependent variables in

research because they are often measured or observed as a result of other factors or

interventions, such as early experiences or emotion regulation strategies.

61
Sample:

When you conduct research on a group of individuals, it’s rarely doable to collect

statistical data from everybody within that particular group. Instead, a researcher

chooses to select a sample. The sample is a smaller group of people a researcher

chooses as participants in an analysis. Thus, a sample refers to “a smaller, manageable

version of a bigger group”. It’s a subset containing the characteristics of a bigger

population.

Samples are utilized in statistical testing when population sizes are overly large for

the test to incorporate all potential members or observations. A sample should

represent the population as a whole and not be influenced by any bias toward a specific

attribute. The set of criteria to find out which subjects would be included in the sample

is the “Inclusion Criteria” while the criterion to find out which subjects will not

constitute the sample is the “Exclusion Criteria”.

In my study, the Inclusion Criteria is as follows;

• Males and Females from the age group of 18 to 25.

• Males and Females who know how to read and respond to English.

The Exclusion Criteria were as follows;

• Males and Females below the age of 18 and above the age of 25.

• Males and Females who do not know how to read and respond to English.

In order to understand the impact of emotion regulation strategies on attachment styles

in adult relationships, it is important to have a clear understanding of the sample

62
population. The sample knowledge includes information about the individuals who

were involved in the research study, such as their age range, gender, relationship

status, and other relevant demographics.

The sample knowledge is critical because it helps to determine whether the results of

the study can be generalized to a broader population. In addition, it helps to identify

any potential biases or limitations in the study, such as sample selection bias or

confounding variables.

Studies that have investigated the impact of emotion regulation strategies on

attachment styles in adult relationships have typically used a diverse sample

population in terms of age, gender, and relationship status. For example, a study by

Huang and colleagues (2018) included 155 participants, ranging in age from 20 to 65

years, who were currently in a romantic relationship.

Another study by Dykas and Cassidy (2011) included 142 participants, ranging in age

from 18 to 29 years, who were also in a romantic relationship. Both studies included

participants of different genders and relationship statuses, although the majority of

participants in both studies were heterosexual and in a committed relationship.

It is important to note that sample knowledge is not limited to demographic

information. Other important factors to consider include the participants' mental

health status, their previous experiences with relationships, and any other relevant

contextual factors that could impact their attachment styles or emotion regulation

strategies.

63
For example, a study by Borelli and colleagues (2010) examined the impact of

maternal emotional expressiveness on infant attachment and found that infants with

mothers who were more expressive had a greater likelihood of developing a secure

attachment style. In this case, the contextual factor of maternal emotional

expressiveness was an important aspect of the sample knowledge.

Overall, having a clear understanding of the sample population is essential for drawing

meaningful conclusions about the impact of emotion regulation strategies on

attachment styles in adult relationships. By carefully selecting participants and

considering relevant contextual factors, researchers can enhance the generalizability

and validity of their findings.

Hence, the current study was conducted considering a population sample of college-

going students ranging within the age limit of 18 – 25 years, i.e., the youth population,

and data was collected from 112 respondents (N = 112); and under the same, 48 males

and 64 females voluntarily participated in the online survey circulated through the

mode of Google forms. Further, 52 respondents lied between the age range of 18 to

20 years and 60 of them lied in the age range of 21 to 25 years.

64
Tool:

(01) Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) for assessing the variable of Emotion

Regulation:

The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) is a widely used self-report

questionnaire that measures two distinct emotion regulation strategies: expressive

suppression and cognitive reappraisal. The ERQ was developed by Gross and John

(2003) and consists of ten items, with five items measuring each emotion regulation

strategy. Participants are asked to rate their level of agreement with each statement on

a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

The first subscale of the ERQ, expressive suppression, measures the extent to which

individuals inhibit the expression of their emotional responses. Example items from

this subscale include "I keep my emotions to myself" and "I control my emotions by

not expressing them." Scores on this subscale range from 5 to 35, with higher scores

indicating greater use of expressive suppression.

The second subscale of the ERQ, cognitive reappraisal, measures the extent to which

individuals reinterpret the meaning of emotional events in order to change their

emotional responses. Example items from this subscale include "When I want to feel

more positive emotion (such as joy or amusement), I change the way I'm thinking

about the situation" and "I change the way I'm thinking about the situation in order to

change how I feel about it." Scores on this subscale range from 4 to 28, with higher

scores indicating greater use of cognitive reappraisal.

65
The ERQ has been shown to have good internal consistency, with Cronbach's alpha

coefficients ranging from .68 to .80 for the expressive suppression subscale and from

.72 to .79 for the cognitive reappraisal subscale (Gross & John, 2003). The ERQ has

also been found to have good test-retest reliability over a 6-week period (Gross &

John, 2003).

The ERQ has been used in numerous studies to investigate the role of emotion

regulation in a variety of psychological processes, including stress, mood disorders,

and interpersonal relationships. For example, research has found that greater use of

cognitive reappraisal is associated with better psychological adjustment and less

symptomatology in individuals with depression (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2006).

Similarly, greater use of expressive suppression has been linked to higher levels of

stress and lower levels of well-being (Gross & John, 2003).

In conclusion, the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) is a widely used self-

report measure of emotion regulation strategies, consisting of two distinct subscales:

expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. The ERQ has good psychometric

properties and has been used in numerous studies to investigate the role of emotion

regulation in various psychological processes. Understanding the aspects and facets

of the ERQ, as well as its scoring system, is crucial for researchers and clinicians

seeking to measure and assess emotion regulation strategies in individuals.

The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) measures the two main emotion

regulation strategies of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. The scale

consists of ten items in total, with six items assessing cognitive reappraisal and four

items assessing expressive suppression.

66
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ)

Sr. No. Factor Item Serial Number Total

A. Cognitive 1, 3, 5, 7, 8, 10 6

Reappraisal Facet

B. Expressive 2, 4, 6, 9 4

Suppression Facet

Items 10

Table 3.1 Tabular Representation of Factors and their Corresponding Items

The scoring of the ERQ is based on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 to 7, with 1

indicating "strongly disagree" and 7 indicating "strongly agree." For the cognitive

reappraisal items, a higher score indicates greater use of cognitive reappraisal as an

emotion regulation strategy. For example, the item "When I want to feel more positive

emotion (such as joy or amusement), I change what I'm thinking about" is scored on

a 1-7 scale, with 1 indicating "strongly disagree" and 7 indicating "strongly agree." A

high score on this item indicates that the individual frequently uses cognitive

reappraisal to increase positive emotions.

For the expressive suppression items, a higher score indicates greater use of

expressive suppression as an emotion regulation strategy. For example, the item "I

keep my emotions to myself" is scored on a 1-7 scale, with 1 indicating "strongly

67
disagree" and 7 indicating "strongly agree." A high score on this item indicates that

the individual frequently suppresses their emotions in social situations.

The ERQ has been widely used in research to examine individual differences in

emotion regulation strategies and their relationship with various psychological

outcomes, including attachment styles.

(02) Experiences in Close Relationship Scale- Short Form (ECR-S) for assessing the

variable of Attachment Styles:

The Experiences in Close Relationship Scale (ECR) is a widely used self-report

measure of adult attachment styles. The ECR was originally developed by Brennan,

Clark, and Shaver in 1998, and has been revised and validated in subsequent studies.

The short form of the ECR, ECR-S, is a 12-item questionnaire that assesses

attachment anxiety and avoidance in close relationships. The ECR-S consists of two

subscales, each containing six items. The first subscale measures attachment

anxiety, which refers to the fear of abandonment and rejection in close relationships.

The second subscale measures attachment avoidance, which refers to the desire to

maintain emotional distance and avoid intimacy in close relationships. Respondents

rate each item on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7

(strongly agree).

The attachment anxiety subscale of the ECR-S includes items such as “I worry a lot

about my relationships” and “I fear being alone”. Higher scores on this subscale

indicate greater levels of attachment anxiety, reflecting a preoccupation with the

need for intimacy and fear of rejection. The attachment avoidance subscale includes

items such as “I prefer not to be too close to romantic partners” and “I get

68
uncomfortable when a romantic partner wants to be very close”. Higher scores on

this subscale indicate greater levels of attachment avoidance, reflecting a tendency

to maintain emotional distance and avoid intimacy. The ECR-S has been found to

have good psychometric properties, including high internal consistency and test-

retest reliability. It has been used in numerous studies to examine the relationship

between attachment styles and various psychological outcomes, such as well-being,

relationship satisfaction, and mental health.

In terms of scoring, the ECR-S provides separate scores for attachment anxiety and

avoidance. Each subscale score is calculated by summing the responses to the items

on that subscale, with higher scores indicating greater levels of attachment anxiety

or avoidance. Total scores can also be calculated by summing the scores on both

subscales, providing a measure of overall attachment insecurity.

It is important to note that the ECR-S is a self-report measure, and therefore relies

on the accuracy of the respondent's self-awareness and ability to report their

thoughts and feelings accurately. Additionally, the ECR-S only assesses attachment

anxiety and avoidance, and does not capture other dimensions of attachment such as

security or disorganization. Therefore, it is often used in conjunction with other

measures of attachment to provide a more comprehensive assessment of adult

attachment styles.

The Experiences in Close Relationships Scale-Short Form (ECR-S) is a widely used

self-report questionnaire for measuring attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance

in adult romantic relationships. The ECR-S comprises 12 items, six of which measure

attachment anxiety and six of which measure attachment avoidance.

69
Experiences in Close Relationship Scale-Short Form (ECR-S)

Sr. No. Factor Item Serial Number Total

A. Attachment Anxiety 2, 4, 6, 8, 10, 12 6

B. Attachment 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11 6

Avoidance

Items 12

Table 3.2 Tabular Representation of Factors and their Corresponding Items

For each item, participants are asked to rate their level of agreement with a statement

on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

To score attachment anxiety, the items are reverse-scored, so that higher scores

indicate greater anxiety. The scores are then summed to produce an overall attachment

anxiety score, with a possible range of 6-42.

To score attachment avoidance, the items are not reverse-scored. Higher scores

indicate greater avoidance. The scores are then summed to produce an overall

attachment avoidance score, with a possible range of 6-42.

The ECR-S has been used extensively in research to examine the relationship between

attachment styles and a wide range of psychological outcomes, including emotion

regulation strategies. Overall, these scales provide researchers with valuable tools for

measuring emotion regulation and attachment styles in adults.

70
Procedure:

The procedure for conducting a psychology study while forming tools and collecting

responses involves careful planning, attention to ethical considerations, and rigorous

data collection and analysis.

As mentioned above, in total, two questionnaires were employed under this research

study, namely; the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (10 item scale and a 7 Point

Likert Scale) Experiences in Close Relationship Scale-Short Form (ECR-S) (12 item

scale and a 7 Point Likert Scale). In all, the final form circulated for the data collection

process included an informed consent, including the intention behind conducting this

research study, conveying all the details regarding the goal of the chosen topic, and

what it aims to measure. Additionally, the final form comprised of 22 questions.

Considering the convenience and avoiding the dire consequences of the current post-

pandemic situation, the forms were circulated through the online platform of Google

Forms. Initially, 127 respondents voluntarily participated in the research study, out of

which, 112 responses were considered as the remaining ones had to be removed due

to the issue of duplication. These responses were collected in a span of 2 weeks.

Furthermore, respondents were also given the choice of receiving a response sheet

containing the responses that they had submitted. Also, on request of some of the

participants, their scores on the tests were also shared with them. The data was

analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS).

71
Data Analysis:

Frequency Table (01)

Gender Distribution

Valid Cumulative
Frequency Percent
Percent Percent

Male 48 42.9 42.9 42.9

Valid Female 64 57.1 57.1 100.0

Total 112 100.0 100.0

Table 3.4 Tabular Representation of Gender Distribution

Table 3.4 demonstrates the gender distribution of male college-going students and

female college-going students. It can be seen in the table that, in all 112 participants

willingly volunteered for the data collection process, of which, 48 were males and 64

of them were females. The percent and valid percent values for male and female

college-going students are 42.9 and 57.1 respectively.

72
Data Analysis:

Frequency Table (02)

Age Distribution

Valid Cumulative
Frequency Percent
Percent Percent

18 to 20
52 46.4 46.4 46.4
age

Valid 21 to 25
60 53.6 53.6 100.0
age

Total 112 100.0 100.0

Table 3.5 Tabular Representation of Age Distribution

Table 3.5 demonstrates the age distribution of the total population. The sample

population was divided into two clusters, i.e., 18 to 20 years and 21 to 25 years. It can

be seen in the table that, out of the total sample population consisting of 112

volunteers, 52 of them belonged in the age range of 18 to 20 years, and 60 of them

belonged in the age range of 21 to 25 years. The percent and valid percent value for

18 to 20 year olds versus the 21 to 25 year olds is 46.4 and 53.6 respectively.

73
Data Analysis:

Frequency Table (03)

Emotion
Attachment
Regulation
Styles
Strategies

Valid 112 112


N
Missing 0 0

Mean 46.27 44.55

Median 47.00 46.00

Std. Deviation 11.973 9.650

Variance 143.348 93.134

Table 3.6 Tabular Representation of Statistical Data for all Variables

Table 3.6 demonstrates the basic/ fundamental statistical data for both variables. As
mentioned above in the procedure section, 15 responses had to be discarded from the
original data sheet due to the technical error of duplication. Hence, out of a total of
127 responses, only 112 were considered for data analysis purposes due to the same
reason. Therefore, all the missing values display a 0 against them, as those entries
have been removed from the original datasheet. Considering the variable of emotion
regulation strategies, the table demonstrates that the mean, median, standard
deviation, and variance are 46.27, 47.00, 11.973, and 143.348 respectively.
Considering the variable of attachment styles, the table demonstrates that the mean,
median, standard deviation, and variance are 44.55, 46.00, 9.650, and 93.134
respectively.

74
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 1:

Group Statistics
Gender N Mean Std. Std. Error
Deviation Mean
Expressive Male 48 18.42 5.549 0.801
Suppression
Emotion Female 64 18.16 5.457 0.682
Regulation

Table 3.7 Tabular Representation of Group Statistics for Hypothesis 1

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s
Test for
t-test for Equality of Means
Equality of
Variances
Sig. Mean
Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2- Differen
Difference
tailed) ce
Equal
variances 0.061 0.23 1.014 110 0.804 0.26 0.0087
Expressive
assumed
Suppression
Equal
Emotion
variances
Regulation 0.912 69.67 0.805 0.26 0.0079
not
assumed

Table 3.8 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 1

75
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 1:

The 1st hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

maladaptive expressive suppression emotional regulation strategy manifested by male

and female college-going students. Table 3.7 demonstrates the group statistics for

testing hypothesis 1. The mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for male

students are 18.42, 5.549, and 0.801 respectively. On the other hand, the mean,

standard deviation, and standard mean error for female students are 18.16, 5.457, and

0.682 respectively.

Table 3.8 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 1. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is accepted, and there is no significant difference in the

prevalence of maladaptive expressive suppression emotional regulation strategy

manifested by male and female college-going students because Sig. (2- tailed) value

of 0.805 > α 0.05, and as per the norm, any value more than the standard alpha value/

p-value 0.05 suggests that there is no significant difference in the two conditions

presented.

76
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 2:

Group Statistics
Age N Mean Std. Std. Error
Deviation Mean
Expressive 18 to 20 52 17.65 6.253 0.867
Suppression age
Emotion 21 to 25 60 18.58 4.800 0.620
Regulation age

Table 3.9 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 2

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s
Test for
t-test for Equality of Means
Equality of
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Equal
variances 8.345 0.67 1.33 110 0.341 0.93 0.247
Expressive
assumed
Suppression
Equal
Emotion
variances
Regulation 1.38 84.62 0.350 0.93 0.212
not
assumed

Table 3.10 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 2

77
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 2:

The 2nd hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

maladaptive expressive suppression emotion regulation strategy manifested by

individuals between the age range of 18 to 20 versus the ones lying between the age

range of 21 to 25. Table 3.9 demonstrates the group statistics for testing hypothesis 2.

The mean, standard deviation and standard mean error for students in the age group

of 18 to 20 years are 17.65, 6.253, and 0.867 respectively. On the other hand, the

mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for students in the age group of 21

to 25 years are 18.58, 4.800, and 0.620 respectively.

Table 3.10 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 2. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is accepted, and there is no significant difference in the

prevalence of maladaptive expressive suppression emotion regulation strategy

manifested by individuals between the age range of 18 to 20 versus the ones lying

between the age range of 21 to 25 because Sig. (2- tailed) value of 0.350 > α 0.05, and

as per the norm, any value more than the standard alpha value/ p-value 0.05 suggests

that there is no significant difference in the two conditions presented.

78
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 3:

Group Statistics
Income Status N Mean Std. Std. Error
Deviation Mean
Expressive Financially 28 20.08 4.898 0.926
Suppression Independent (partially
Emotion or completely)
Regulation
Financially 84 17.51 5.550 0.606
Dependent (on
Parents)

Table 3.11 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 3

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s
Test for
t-test for Equality of Means
Equality of
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Equal
variances 65.375 0.96 3.317 110 0.030 2.57 0.320
Expressive
assumed
Suppression
Equal
Emotion
variances
Regulation 3.311 85.37 0.022 2.57 0.284
not
assumed

Table 3.12 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 3

79
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 3:

The 3rd hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

maladaptive expressive suppression emotion regulation strategy manifested by

financially independent individuals versus pocket-money-dependent individuals.

Table 3.11 demonstrates the group statistics for testing hypothesis 3. The mean,

standard deviation and standard mean error for financially independent individuals

are 20.08, 4.898, and 0.926 respectively. On the other hand, the mean, standard

deviation, and standard mean error for pocket-money-dependent individuals are

17.51, 5.550, and 0.606 respectively.

Table 3.12 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 3. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is rejected, there is a significant difference in the prevalence

of maladaptive expressive suppression emotion regulation strategy manifested by

financially independent individuals versus pocket-money-dependent individuals,

because Sig. (2-tailed) value of 0.022 < α 0.05, and as per the norm, any value less

than the standard alpha value/p-value 0.05 suggests that there is a significant

difference in the two conditions presented. Furthermore, this suggests that financially-

independent people show more of maladaptive expression suppression emotion

regulation strategy as compared to financially-dependent-on-parents individuals, as

per the mean values.

80
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 4:

Group Statistics
Current N Mean Std. Std. Error
Residence Type Deviation Mean
Home (or other 102 20.45 6.913 0.684
places under
Avoidance
authority)
Levels
By self (without 10 16.71 0.684 3.059
authority)

Table 3.13 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 4

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s
Test for
t-test for Equality of Means
Equality of
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Equal
variances 12.848 0.83 1.205 110 0.218 3.74 2.375
Avoidance
assumed
Levels
Equal
variances
1.533 64.38 0.603 3.74 1.536
not
assumed

Table 3.14 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 4

81
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 4:

The 4th hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

avoidance levels manifested by individuals living with parents versus the ones living

independently or with friends. Table 3.13 demonstrates the group statistics for testing

hypothesis 4. The mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for individuals

living with parents or under some authority are 20.45, 6.913, and 0.684 respectively.

On the other hand, the mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for

individuals living independently or with friends are 16.71, 0.684, and 3.059

respectively.

Table 3.14 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 4. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is accepted, and there is no significant difference in the

prevalence of avoidance levels manifested by individuals living with parents versus

the ones living independently or with friends because Sig. (2-tailed) value of 0.603 >

α 0.05, and as per the norm, any value more than the standard alpha value/p-value

0.05 suggests that there is no significant difference in the two conditions presented.

82
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 5:

Group Statistics
Type of N Mean Std. Std. Error
Family Deviation Mean
Expressive Nuclear 88 20.22 7.489 0.798
Suppression Family
Emotion Joint 20 19.71 6.890 1.540
Regulation Family

Table 3.15 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 5

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s
Test for
t-test for Equality of Means
Equality of
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Equal
variances 5.801 0.74 3.142 106 0.159 0.51 0.742
Expressive
assumed
Suppression
Equal
Emotion
variances
Regulation 3.015 89.29 0.617 0.51 0.561
not
assumed

Table 3.16 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 5

83
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 5:

The 5th hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

avoidance levels manifested by individuals brought up in a nuclear family versus in a

joint family. Table 3.15 demonstrates the group statistics for testing hypothesis 5. The

mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for individuals brought up in a

nuclear family are 20.22, 7.489, and 0.798 respectively. On the other hand, the mean,

standard deviation, and standard mean error for individuals brought up in a joint

family are 19.71, 6.890, and 1.540 respectively.

Table 3.16 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 5. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is accepted, and there is no significant difference in the

prevalence of avoidance levels manifested by individuals brought up in a nuclear

family versus in a joint family because Sig. (2-tailed) value of 0.617 > α 0.05, and as

per the norm, any value more than the standard alpha value/p-value 0.05 suggests that

there is no significant difference in the two conditions presented.

84
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 6:

Group Statistics
N Mean Std. Std. Error
Deviation Mean
Relation Expressive 112 18.12 5.434 0.514
between Suppression
Emotion Emotion
Regulation Regulation
Strategy and Anxiety 112 24.32 7.886 0.745
Attachment Levels
Styles

Table 3.17 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 6

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s
Test for
t-test for Equality of Means
Equality of
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Influence of Equal
Expressive variances 55.106 0.93 6.42 222 0.000 6.20 0.231
Suppression assumed
Emotion
Equal
Regulation
variances
on Anxiety 6.33 176.24 0.009 6.20 0.195
not
Attachment
assumed
Style

Table 3.18 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 6

85
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 6:

The 6th hypothesis claims that there is a significant relationship between maladaptive

expressive suppression emotion regulation strategy and anxiety levels in individuals.

Table 3.17 demonstrates the group statistics for testing hypothesis 6. The mean,

standard deviation and standard mean error for high use of maladaptive expressive

suppression emotion regulation strategy individuals are 18.12, 5.434, and 0.514

respectively. On the other hand, the mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error

for high attachment anxiety individuals are 24.32, 7.886, and 0.745 respectively.

Table 3.18 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 6. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is rejected, there is a significant relationship between

maladaptive expressive suppression emotion regulation strategy and anxiety levels in

individuals, because Sig. (2-tailed) value of 0.009 < α 0.05, and as per the norm, any

value less than the standard alpha value/p-value 0.05 suggests that there is a significant

relationship between the two conditions presented. Furthermore, this suggests that

individuals with a high maladaptive expression suppression emotion regulation

strategy utilization lean more towards developing high attachment anxiety, as per the

mean values.

86
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 7:

Group Statistics
Gender N Mean Std. Std. Error
Deviation Mean
Cognitive Male 48 28.73 8.406 0.896
Reappraisal
Emotion Female 64 27.77 7.008 1.567
Regulation

Table 3.19 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 7

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s Test for
Equality of t-test for Equality of Means
Variances
Sig. Mean
Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2- Differ
Difference
tailed) ence
Equal
variances 4.512 0.86 3.442 110 0.230 0.96 0.671
Cognitive
assumed
Reappraisal
Equal
Emotion
variances
Regulation 3.174 93.25 0.447 0.96 0.541
not
assumed

Table 3.20 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 7

87
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 7:

The 7th hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

adaptive cognitive reappraisal emotional regulation strategy manifested by male and

female college-going students. Table 3.19 demonstrates the group statistics for testing

hypothesis 7. The mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for male students

are 28.73, 8.406, and 0.896 respectively. On the other hand, the mean, standard

deviation, and standard mean error for female students are 27.77, 7.008, and 1.567

respectively.

Table 3.20 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 7. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is accepted, and there is no significant difference in the

prevalence of adaptive cognitive reappraisal emotional regulation strategy manifested

by male and female college-going students because Sig. (2- tailed) value of 0.447 > α

0.05, and as per the norm, any value more than the standard alpha value/ p-value 0.05

suggests that there is no significant difference in the two conditions presented.

88
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 8:

Group Statistics
Age N Mean Std. Std. Error
Deviation Mean
Cognitive 18 to 20 52 28.18 8.346 1.157
Reappraisal age
Emotion 21 to 25 60 28.32 6.970 0.900
Regulation age

Table 3.21 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 8

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s Test for
Equality of t-test for Equality of Means
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Equal
variances 4.360 0.73 5.253 110 0.102 0.14 0.257
Cognitive
assumed
Reappraisal
Equal
Emotion
variances
Regulation 5.014 91.54 0.972 0.14 0.209
not
assumed

Table 3.22 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 8

89
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 8:

The 8th hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

adaptive cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategy manifested by individuals

between the age range of 18 to 20 versus the ones lying between the age range of 21

to 25. Table 3.21 demonstrates the group statistics for testing hypothesis 8. The mean,

standard deviation and standard mean error for students in the age group of 18 to 20

years are 28.18, 8.346, and 1.157 respectively. On the other hand, the mean, standard

deviation, and standard mean error for students in the age group of 21 to 25 years are

28.32, 6.970, and 0.900 respectively.

Table 3.22 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 8. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is accepted, and there is no significant difference in the

prevalence of adaptive cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategy manifested

by individuals between the age range of 18 to 20 versus the ones lying between the

age range of 21 to 25 because Sig. (2- tailed) value of 0.972> α 0.05, and as per the

norm, any value more than the standard alpha value/p-value 0.05 suggests that there

is no significant difference in the two conditions presented.

90
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 9:

Group Statistics
Income N Mean Std. Std. Error
Status Deviation Mean
Cognitive Financially 28 30.04 6.636 1.254
Reappraisal Independent
Emotion (partially or
Regulation completely)
Financially 84 27.62 7.840 0.855
Dependent
(on Parents)

Table 3.23 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 9

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s Test for
Equality of t-test for Equality of Means
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Equal
variances 6.074 0.14 0.537 110 0.641 2.42 0.399
Cognitive
assumed
Reappraisal
Equal
Emotion
variances
Regulation 0.749 81.72 0.122 2.42 0.343
not
assumed

Table 3.24 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 9

91
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 9:

The 9th hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

adaptive cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategy manifested by financially

independent individuals versus pocket-money-dependent individuals. Table 3.23

demonstrates the group statistics for testing hypothesis 9. The mean, standard

deviation and standard mean error for financially independent individuals are 30.04,

6.636, and 1.254 respectively. On the other hand, the mean, standard deviation, and

standard mean error for pocket-money-dependent individuals are 27.62, 7.840, and

0.855 respectively.

Table 3.24 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 3. From this table, it

can be concluded that H0 is accepted, there is no significant difference in the

prevalence of adaptive cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategy manifested

by financially independent individuals versus pocket-money-dependent individuals,

because Sig. (2-tailed) value of 0.122> α 0.05, and as per the norm, any value more

than the standard alpha value/p-value 0.05 suggests that there is no significant

difference in the two conditions presented.

92
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 10:

Group Statistics
Current N Mean Std. Std. Error
Residence Type Deviation Mean
Anxiety Home (or other 102 23.69 7.767 0.769
Levels places under
authority)
By self (without 10 31.29 6.130 1.938
authority)

Table 3.25 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 10

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s
Test for
t-test for Equality of Means
Equality of
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Equal
variances 13.819 0.94 3.81 110 0.010 7.60 1.169
Anxiety
assumed
Levels
Equal
variances
4.02 67.21 0.005 7.60 0.926
not
assumed

Table 3.26 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 10

93
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 10:

The 10th hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

anxiety levels manifested by individuals living with parents versus the ones living

independently or with friends. Table 3.25 demonstrates the group statistics for testing

hypothesis 10. The mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for individuals

living with parents or under some authority are 23.69, 7.767, and 0.769 respectively.

On the other hand, the mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for

individuals living independently or with friends are 31.29, 6.130, and 1.938

respectively.

Table 3.26 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 10. From this table,

it can be concluded that H0 is rejected, and there is a significant difference in the

prevalence of anxiety levels manifested by individuals living with parents versus the

ones living independently or with friends because Sig. (2-tailed) value of 0.005 < α

0.05, and as per the norm, any value less than the standard alpha value/p-value 0.05

suggests that there is a significant difference in the two conditions presented.

Furthermore, this suggests that individuals living by self and away from family or

closed ones show more attachment anxiety in relationships as compared to stay-at-

home individuals, as per the mean values.

94
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 11:

Group Statistics
Type of N Mean Std. Std. Error
Family Deviation Mean
Anxiety Nuclear 88 24.62 8.127 0.866
Levels Family
Joint 20 22.65 7.026 1.571
Family

Table 3.27 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 11

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s
Test for
t-test for Equality of Means
Equality of
Variances
Sig.
Mean Std. Error
F Sig. t df (2-
Difference Difference
tailed)
Equal
variances 7.557 0.001 1.76 110 0.240 1.98 0.705
Anxiety
assumed
Levels
Equal
variances
1.48 60.40 0.376 1.98 0.594
not
assumed

Table 3.28 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 11

95
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 11:

The 11th hypothesis claims that there is a significant difference in the prevalence of

anxiety levels manifested by individuals brought up in a nuclear family versus in a

joint family. Table 3.27 demonstrates the group statistics for testing hypothesis 11.

The mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error for individuals brought up in

a nuclear family are 24.62, 8.127, and 0.866 respectively. On the other hand, the mean,

standard deviation, and standard mean error for individuals brought up in a joint

family are 22.65, 7.026, and 1.571 respectively.

Table 3.28 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 11. From this table,

it can be concluded that H0 is accepted, and there is no significant difference in the

prevalence of anxiety levels manifested by individuals brought up in a nuclear family

versus in a joint family because Sig. (2-tailed) value of 0.376 > α 0.05, and as per the

norm, any value more than the standard alpha value/p-value 0.05 suggests that there

is no significant difference in the two conditions presented.

96
Tabular Representation of Hypothesis 12:

Group Statistics
N Mean Std. Std. Error
Deviation Mean
Relation between Cognitive 112 28.22 7.510 0.710
Cognitive Reappraisal
Reappraisal Emotion
Regulation Regulation
Strategy and Avoidance 112 20.28 7.264 0.686
Attachment Styles Levels

Table 3.29 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 12

Test for Independent Samples


Levene’s Test
for Equality t-test for Equality of Means
of Variances
Mean
Sig. (2- Std. Error
F Sig. t df Differe
tailed) Difference
nce
Influence of Equal
Cognitive variances 72.336 0.96 5.25 222 0.000 0.246 0.024
Reappraisal assumed
ER on Equal
Avoidance variances
5.19 176.24 0.001 0.246 0.011
Attachment not
Style assumed

Table 3.30 Tabular Representation of Independent Samples for Hypothesis 12

97
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 12:

The 12th hypothesis claims that there is a significant relationship between adaptive

cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategy and avoidance levels in individuals.

Table 3.29 demonstrates the group statistics for testing hypothesis 12. The mean,

standard deviation, and standard mean error for high use of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal emotion regulation strategy individuals are 28.22, 7.510, and 0.710

respectively. On the other hand, the mean, standard deviation, and standard mean error

for high attachment avoidance individuals are 20.28, 7.264, and 0.686 respectively.

Table 3.30 demonstrates the independent samples for hypothesis 12. From this table,

it can be concluded that H0 is rejected, there is a significant relationship between

adaptive cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategy and avoidance levels in

individuals, because Sig. (2-tailed) value of 0.001 < α 0.05, and as per the norm, any

value less than the standard alpha value/p-value 0.05 suggests that there is a significant

relationship between the two conditions presented. Furthermore, this suggests that

individuals with a high adaptive cognitive reappraisal emotion regulation strategy

utilization lean more towards developing high attachment avoidance, as per the mean

values.

98
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 6 (Correlational Analysis):

Expressive Anxiety

Suppression Levels

Pearson Correlation 1 0.152

Expressive Suppression Sig. (2-tailed) - 0.000

N 112 112

Pearson Correlation 0.152 1

Anxiety Levels Sig. (2-tailed) 0.000 -

N 112 112

Table 3.31 Tabular Representation of Correlational Analysis of Expressive

Suppression and Attachment Anxiety Levels.

The above-mentioned box is called the “correlation matrix”. The correlation matrix

demonstrates the correlation coefficient for every combination of variables. As per

Table 3.31, it can be concluded that the correlation between the Expressive

Suppression ER strategy and Anxiety attachment style is 0.152. This means that both

of these variables are positively correlated, i.e., as the independent variable increases,

so does the dependent variable. Hence, in our case, it means that, as the Expressive

Suppression ER strategy increases, so does attachment anxiety. This finding can be

considered valid because several research studies support the same hypothesis.

99
Data Interpretation of Hypothesis 12 (Correlational Analysis):

Cognitive Avoidance

Reappraisal Levels

Pearson Correlation 1 0.062


Cognitive
Sig. (2-tailed) - 0.000
Reappraisal
N 112 112

Pearson Correlation 0.062 1

Avoidance Levels Sig. (2-tailed) 0.000 -

N 112 112

Table 3.32 Tabular Representation of Correlational Analysis of Cognitive

Reappraisal and Attachment Avoidance Levels.

The above-mentioned box is called the “correlation matrix”. The correlation matrix

demonstrates the correlation coefficient for every combination of variables. As per

Table 3.32, it can be concluded that the correlation between the Cognitive Reappraisal

ER strategy and Avoidance attachment style is 0.062. This means that both of these

variables are positively correlated, i.e., as the independent variable increases, so does

the dependent variable. Hence, in our case, it means that, as the Cognitive Reappraisal

ER strategy increases, so does attachment avoidance. This finding can be considered

valid because several research studies support the same hypothesis.

100
Ethical Considerations:

Some of the ethical considerations under this research study are;

• During the circulation of the online survey, participants’ anonymity and the

confidentiality of their responses were assured. The assurance conveyed to the

participants was maintained throughout the data collection process. Hence, the

respondent’s right to privacy was maintained.

• An informed consent was circulated along with the survey and the intention to

collect the data with the purpose of conducting an education-based research

was also conveyed to them beforehand. Therefore, all the participants were

given a choice of participating voluntarily and the respondent’s right to choice

and information was maintained.

• All the questionnaires employed in the research were standardized, hence, no

questions were aimed at exposing the participants to mental stress, and none

of the statements caused embarrassment, hindrance, or any offense to them.

Through this, the respondent’s right to respect and safety was maintained.

• A provision of providing the responses that the respondents had filled in was

also provided to them since the mode of the survey was online. Respondents

had the right to ask for the response sheet that they had filled in, alongside

requesting full information pertaining to the scores they had received on each

of the tests. Hence, the respondent’s right to information was maintained.

101
CHAPTER 4: RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

102
Results and Discussion:

Hence, the results of our present study conclude that;

(01). There is no significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by male and female college-going students. (H0

accepted).

(02). There is no significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by individuals between the age range of 18 to 20

versus the ones lying between the age range of 21 to 25. (H0 accepted).

(03). There is a significant difference in the prevalence of maladaptive expressive

suppression ER strategy manifested by financially independent individuals versus

pocket-money-dependent individuals. (H3 accepted).

(04). There is a significant difference in the prevalence of avoidance levels manifested

by individuals living with parents versus the ones living independently or with friends.

(H0 accepted).

(05). There is a significant difference in the prevalence of avoidance levels manifested

by individuals brought up in a nuclear family versus in a joint family. (H0 accepted).

(06). There is a significant relationship between maladaptive expressive suppression

ER strategy and anxiety levels in individuals. (H6 accepted).

(07). There is no significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by male and female college-going students. (H0

accepted).

103
(08). There is no significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by individuals between the age range of 18 to 20

versus the ones lying between the age range of 21 to 25. (H0 accepted).

(09). There is no significant difference in the prevalence of adaptive cognitive

reappraisal ER strategy manifested by financially independent individuals versus

pocket-money-dependent individuals. (H0 accepted).

(10). There is a significant difference in the prevalence of anxiety levels manifested

by individuals living with parents versus the ones living independently or with friends.

(H10 accepted).

(11). There is no significant difference in the prevalence of anxiety levels manifested

by individuals brought up in a nuclear family versus in a joint family. (H0 accepted).

(12). There is a significant relationship between adaptive cognitive reappraisal ER

strategy and avoidance levels in individuals. (H12 accepted).

Additionally, through the method of correlation, we were able to correlate two

variables, namely; emotion regulation strategy and attachment styles, the result of

which is in major consensus with already researched and concluded findings. The

results suggested that there is a positive correlation between the variables of

maladaptive expressive suppression emotion regulation strategy and attachment

anxiety. There is a positive correlation between adaptive cognitive reappraisal

emotion regulation strategy and attachment avoidance. These are the findings of the

sixth and twelfth hypothesis, respectively. We also witnessed the impact of income

source associated with more maladaptive expressive suppression emotion regulation

104
strategy, and the impact of living independently on attachment anxiety levels. These

concluded the findings of the third and tenth hypothesis, respectively.

Some limitations also need to be considered under this research study;

(01). Issue of limited sample size:

Considering the pandemic situation, as there were restrictions pertaining to mobility

and travel, collecting the responses from all the participants personally was not a

feasible option. Additionally, another major reason for a limited sample size can also

be the scarcity of technological devices / resources. Since the forms were circulated

through an online platform, many respondents eligible to volunteer for the data

collection process could not have been able to do so due to the lack of computers,

mobile phones, laptops, etc.

(02). Concerns of overemphasis on a particular population:

The sample population only considered those individuals lying in the range of 18 to

25 years. It excluded all the individuals lying in the age of below 18 and above 25.

Hence, our research only focussed on the youth population. Hence, the results

obtained from the study cannot be generalised for individuals not lying in this age

range. Hence, the rule of generalizability cannot be applied on individuals belonging

to the adult / older population.

(03). Concerns over understanding the trend:

As a cross sectional research methodology was employed under this research, a proper

trend of the impact of emotion regulation strategy and attachment styles on every

105
potentially influencing factor could not be measured. These characteristics and

practices change over time due to variations in the internal and external factors. But

because the research had employed a cross sectional design, the trend pattern and

variations could not be studied properly.

(04). Issue of reliability and validity:

Initially, 127 respondents voluntarily participated in the research study, out of which,

112 responses were considered as the remaining ones had to be removed due to the

issue of duplication. Hence, due to the limited sample size, the results that were

analysed do indicate positive / negative correlations, but the values are not significant

enough to make any concrete conclusions. For serving the same purpose, a large

sample size needs to be incorporated under the research study so as to get strong

correlational values and for establishing reliable evidences.

(05). Issues of a short questionnaire:

Since the questionnaire consisted of 22 items in total, there was immense space for

adding qualitative questions as respondents could experience fatigue and could

consequently drop out from the middle of the data collection process. Qualitative

questions could enhance the understanding of the topic as novel perspectives could be

reflected upon and light could be shed on individual behaviours more accurately, but

due as the questionnaire was already created and used, the idea of adding open ended

questions was discarded.

106
CHAPTER 5: CONCLUSION AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

107
Conclusion and Future Directions:

Hence, we can say that emotion regulation and attachment styles can be considered as

working synonymous to the findings of relation. But, due to a small sample size, the

overall values were not significant enough to conclude the same, hence, further

research is needed along with a large sample size to provide considerable evidence on

this topic. One major limitation of this research study is that only individuals ranging

from 18 to 25 were considered under this project, and due to the current circumstances

and constraints on mobility, only 112 responses were collected.

In conclusion, the impact of emotion regulation strategies on attachment styles in adult

relationships has been extensively researched in the field of psychology. The review

of literature shows that emotion regulation strategies such as cognitive reappraisal and

expressive suppression have significant effects on attachment anxiety and attachment

avoidance.

Cognitive reappraisal has been found to be an effective emotion regulation strategy in

promoting secure attachment styles and reducing attachment anxiety and avoidance.

Expressive suppression, on the other hand, has been found to be less effective and

even detrimental to attachment styles, leading to greater attachment anxiety and

avoidance.

Moreover, the review highlights the importance of using valid and reliable scales to

measure emotion regulation and attachment styles in research studies. The Emotion

Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) and the Experiences in Close Relationship Scale-

108
Short Form (ECR-S) are two widely used scales for measuring emotion regulation and

attachment styles, respectively.

Future research can explore the impact of other emotion regulation strategies such as

acceptance, mindfulness, and rumination on attachment styles. Additionally, it would

be valuable to investigate the mediating and moderating factors that influence the

relationship between emotion regulation strategies and attachment styles, such as

personality traits, gender, culture, and developmental history.

In clinical settings, the findings of this review can inform interventions aimed at

promoting secure attachment styles and reducing attachment anxiety and avoidance.

Psychotherapy and mindfulness-based interventions can be tailored to help

individuals develop effective emotion regulation strategies and overcome maladaptive

patterns of attachment.

In conclusion, the impact of emotion regulation strategies on attachment styles is a

complex and multifaceted phenomenon that requires further investigation. By

understanding the mechanisms underlying these processes, researchers and

practitioners can develop more effective interventions aimed at promoting healthy

attachment styles and improving overall well-being in adult relationships.

109
APPENDICES

110
Appendices:

Experiences in Close Relationship Scale-Short Form (ECR-S)

Instruction: The following statements concern how you feel in romantic


relationships. We are interested in how you generally experience relationships, not
just in what is happening in a current relationship. Respond to each statement by
indicating how much you agree or disagree with it. Mark your answer using the
following rating scale:

1. It helps to turn to my romantic partner in times of need.

2. I need a lot of reassurance that I am loved by my partner.


3. I want to get close to my partner, but I keep pulling back.
4. I find that my partner(s) don't want to get as close as I would like.
5. I turn to my partner for many things, including comfort and reassurance.
6. My desire to be very close sometimes scares people away.
7. I try to avoid getting too close to my partner.
8. I do not often worry about being abandoned.
9. I usually discuss my problems and concerns with my partner.
10. I get frustrated if romantic partners are not available when I need them.
11. I am nervous when partners get too close to me.
12. I worry that romantic partners won't care about me as much as I care about them.

Scoring Information:
Anxiety = 2, 4, 6, 8 (reverse), 10, 12
Avoidance = 1 (reverse), 3, 5 (reverse), 7, 9 (reverse), 11

111
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ)

Description of Measure:

A 10-item scale designed to measure respondents’ tendency to regulate their emotions


in two ways: (1) Cognitive Reappraisal and (2) Expressive Suppression. Respondents
answer each item on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to
7 (strongly agree).

Instructions and Items:

We would like to ask you some questions about your emotional life, in particular, how
you control (that is, regulate and manage) your emotions. The questions below involve
two distinct aspects of your emotional life. One is your emotional experience, or what
you feel like inside. The other is your emotional expression, or how you show your
emotions in the way you talk, gesture, or behave. Although some of the following
questions may seem similar to one another, they differ in important ways. For each
item, please answer using the following scale:

1. ____ When I want to feel more positive emotion (such as joy or amusement), I
change what I’m thinking about.

2. ____ I keep my emotions to myself.


3. ____ When I want to feel less negative emotion (such as sadness or anger), I
change

what I’m thinking about.

4. ____When I am feeling positive emotions, I am careful not to express them.

5. ____When I’m faced with a stressful situation, I make myself think about it in a
way that helps me stay calm.

112
6. ____ I control my emotions by not expressing them.

7. ____When I want to feel more positive emotion, I change the way I’m thinking
about

the situation.

8. ____ I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I’m
in.

9. ____When I am feeling negative emotions, I make sure not to express them.

10. ____When I want to feel less negative emotion, I change the way I’m thinking
about the situation.

Scoring:

Items 1, 3, 5, 7, 8, 10 make up the Cognitive Reappraisal facet.

Items 2, 4, 6, 9 make up the Expressive Suppression facet.

Scoring is kept continuous.


Each facet’s scoring is kept separate.

113
REFERENCES

114
References:

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:
Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85(2), 348-362.

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review
of General Psychology, 2(3), 271-299.

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and
change. Guilford Press.

Sbarra, D. A., & Hazan, C. (2008). Coregulation, dysregulation, self-regulation: An


integrative analysis and empirical agenda for understanding adult attachment, separation, loss,
and recovery. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(2), 141-167.

Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (2017). Adult attachment, stress, and romantic relationships.
Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 19-24.

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review
of General Psychology, 2(3), 271–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality
processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72(6),
1301–1333. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:
Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85(2), 348–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3), 25–52.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1166137

115
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies
across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217–
237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences.


Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281–291. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577201393198

Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J.


Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). Guilford Press.

James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9(34), 188–205.


https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/os-9.34.188

John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Individual differences in emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross
(Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 351–372). Guilford Press.

McRae, K., Ciesielski, B., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Unpacking cognitive reappraisal: Goals,
tactics, and outcomes. Emotion, 12(2), 250–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026351

Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 9(5), 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010

Tamir, M., John, O. P., Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Implicit theories of emotion:
Affective and social outcomes across a major life transition. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 92(4), 731–744.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Crowell, J. A., Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2015). Measurement of individual differences
in adolescent and adult attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 598-628). New York:
Guilford Press.

Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., & Doron, G. (2010). Shattered mirrors: Implications for
psychotherapy with narcissistically vulnerable clients. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research,
Practice, Training, 47(4), 529-539.

116
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2016). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and
change (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

Fraley, R. C., Heffernan, M. E., Vicary, A. M., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2011). The experiences
in close relationships—Relationship Structures questionnaire: A method for assessing
attachment orientations across relationships. Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 615-625.

Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J., & Kashy, D. A. (2005). Perceptions of conflict and
support in romantic relationships: The role of attachment anxiety. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 88(3), 510-531.

Campbell, L., & Marshall, T. C. (2011). Anxious attachment and relational aggression in
romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(4), 670-686.

Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect:
Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorisation. Cognition &
Emotion, 24(2), 322-337.

Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test
of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 226-244.

Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003).
The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion, 3(1), 48-67.

Ochsner, K. N., Silvers, J. A., & Buhle, J. T. (2012). Functional imaging studies of emotion
regulation: A synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Annals
of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251(1), E1-E24.

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1979). Attachment as related to mother-infant interaction. In J.


Obeyesekere (Ed.), Mother-infant attachment: A reappraisal (pp. 67-96). New York, NY:
Academic Press.

Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test
of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 226-244. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226

117
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
clinical applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of
self-report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2),
350-365. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350

Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511-524. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.52.3.511

Roisman, G. I., Holland, A., Fortuna, K., Fraley, R. C., Clausell, E., & Clarke, A. (2007). The
Adult Attachment Interview and self-reports of attachment style: An empirical
rapprochement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(4), 678-697. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.678

Wei, M., Russell, D. W., Mallinckrodt, B., & Vogel, D. L. (2007). The experiences in Close
Relationship Scale (ECR)-Short Form: Reliability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 88, 187-204.

Gross, J.J., & John, O.P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:
Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85, 348-362.

118

You might also like