You are on page 1of 8

Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2005) 902–909

Mobile phone use—Effects of handheld and


handsfree phones on driving performance
Jan E.B. Törnros ∗ , Anne K. Bolling
VTI, Olaus Magnus väg 35, S-58195 Linköping, Sweden

Received 3 March 2005; received in revised form 10 April 2005; accepted 11 April 2005

Abstract

The study was concerned with effects of handsfree and handheld mobile phone dialling and conversation in simulated driving. In the
main experiment dealing with conversation, 48 participants drove a distance of about 70 km on a route which led through urban and rural
environments. In the dialling experiment, the participants drove a distance of 15 km on a rural two-lane road. The experimental design was
mixed with phone mode as a between-subjects factor and phone use (yes/no) as a within-subjects factor.
Performance on a peripheral detection task (PDT) while driving was impaired by dialling and conversation for both phone modes, interpreted
as an increase in mental workload. Driving performance was impaired by dialling—lateral position deviation increased in a similar way for
both phone modes. Conversation had, however, opposite effects—lateral position deviation decreased in a similar way for both phone modes.
Driving speed decreased as an effect of dialling with the greatest effect for handsfree phone mode. Conversation also caused reduced speed,
but only for handheld phone mode. The effects on speed can be interpreted as a compensatory effort for the increased mental workload. In
spite of the compensatory behaviour, mental workload was still markedly increased by phone use.
Subjective effects of dialling and conversation were also analysed. Most participants reported a speed decrease but no effect on lateral
position deviation as an effect of dialling or conversation. In the conversation experiment, driving performance was rated better for handsfree
than for handheld mode. In the dialling experiment, no difference between the two phone modes appeared.
© 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Mobile phone; Handheld; Handsfree; Driving; Simulation

1. Introduction simulator study. According to mental effort ratings by the


participants, driving while using a handheld phone was
An issue that has been under considerable debate is the most difficult. The drivers slowed down when talking
the importance of telephone mode for the effects of tele- on the handheld phone compared to the control condition,
phoning while driving. Since the physical distraction may interpreted as a compensation for the increased workload.
be less for handsfree phones than for handheld phones, The standard deviation of speed and speed error measures
most international legislation against the use of a mobile indicated that the drivers had the poorest speed control when
phone while driving also concerns handheld phones only. using the handheld phone. Reaction times to warning signs at
However, both phone modes can cause cognitive dis- the roadside were slower for drivers using a handheld phone
traction which may have negative consequences (Rospa, compared to the control condition. The drivers also missed
2002). more target signs when they were using a phone compared to
Comparisons have been made between the two phone the control condition. No effect on lane keeping performance
modes. Burns et al. (2002) compared the effects of mobile was detected, however. Haigney et al. (2000) also found that
phone conversation (handheld or handsfree) in a driving conversation with a mobile phone (handheld or handsfree)
during driving was associated with reduced speed. The
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +46 13 204 000; fax: +46 13 141 436. lateral control of the car was impaired with a handheld phone
E-mail address: jan.tornros@vti.se (J.E.B. Törnros). (increased instances of driving off the carriageway) but not

0001-4575/$ – see front matter © 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.aap.2005.04.007
J.E.B. Törnros, A.K. Bolling / Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2005) 902–909 903

with a handsfree phone. Brookhuis et al. (1991) investigated 2. Method


the effects of using a mobile phone while driving on a quiet
rural road, a busy ring road and in a town. The phone was 2.1. General design
either handheld or handsfree. Conversation caused increased
mental workload according to self-reported effort ratings. It Two separate experiments were performed, the main one
also reduced the lateral position deviation, which the authors dealing with conversation and the other dealing with dialling.
interpreted as an effect of increased alertness. Dialling a The same individuals participated in both. The experimen-
telephone number, however, caused increased lateral position tal design was mixed—half the participant group was tested
deviation with the worst result for handheld phone. Matthews with and without handsfree phone use, and the other half was
et al. (2003) compared handheld and handsfree phones with tested with and without handheld phone use (for a complete
respect to subjective workload during conversation while description of the design and other details of the study, see
driving on a rural highway. Total subjective workload was Kircher et al., 2004).
higher for handheld than for handsfree phone. No difference
appeared for mental demand, whereas physical demand was 2.2. Participants
higher for handheld phone. Strayer et al. (2004) found that
conversation via handsfree or handheld phone mode delayed The participants in the conversation experiment were 48
the brake reactions of their participants in simulated driving. healthy volunteers, aged 24–54 years with mean age 34 years.
No difference was noted between the two phone modes. They had held a driving licence for 5–35 years. They had
Using an instrumented car driven on a motorway, Patten driven about 1000–70,000 km last year. The participant group
et al. (2004) studied the effects of using a mobile phone was randomly divided into two groups of equal size. One
while driving. Effects of conversation type (simple versus group used a handsfree phone and the other group used a
complex) and telephone mode (handheld versus handsfree) handheld phone. There were about as many male as female
were compared to baseline conditions. A peripheral detec- participants in both groups −12/12 in the handsfree group,
tion task (PDT) was employed to gauge mental workload. and 14/10 (more males) in the handheld group.
The results were that the PDT reaction times increased The majority of the 48 participants in the conversation
when conversing, but no benefit of handsfree units over experiment also participated in the dialling experiment (23
handheld units was found. The content of the conversation in the handsfree and 19 in the handheld part).
was far more important: the more difficult and complex the
conversation, the greater the increase of PDT reaction time.
2.3. Driving simulator
It was also found that the driving speed was reduced by
using a handheld mobile phone, whereas no speed reduction
The well validated VTI driving simulator II was used in the
was apparent for the handsfree mode. Conversation type
study. It is used to create realistic sensations in a laboratory
did not, however, seem to have any marked effects on
environment, and includes a cut-off vehicle cab, a comput-
driving speed.
erised vehicle model, a high-fidelity moving base system, a
It can be concluded that where the two modes have been
vibration table, a wide-angle visual system and an audio sys-
compared, either no difference is apparent or there is a
tem (Nilsson, 1993). The car body used in this experiment
tendency to suggest that the use of a handheld phone would
was a Volvo 850 with manual gear box.
interfere more with the driving task than a handsfree phone.
Another importing finding is that drivers tend to slow down
during conversation, maybe more so for the handheld mode, 2.4. Route
a result which could be interpreted as a compensation for the
increased workload. Dialling a phone number during driving Two separate routes were used in the study, a long one
may have more serious consequences than conversation. of varying complexity for the conversation experiment and
These issues need further study, and one of the major aims of a short one for the dialling experiment. The long route had
the present study is to compare the two modes with respect rural sections with varying speed limits (70 and 90 km/h) and
to effects on measures related to traffic safety. Driving urban sections with speed limit 50 km/h and with varying
behaviour as well as mental workload are important in this complexity. The route was driven through twice making a
respect. total driving distance of about 70 km.
It is further of interest to compare the two phone The dialling experiment used a rather straight two-lane
modes with regard to subjective effects. If the experience road with speed limit 110 km/h. There was sparse meeting
of the users is different from what is objectively true, traffic on the road. The driving distance was about 15 km.
there may be a need to inform the users about the true
effects of using the different modes of phones while 2.5. Phone task
driving.
The effects of conversation and the effects of dialling were The mobile phone used in the study was a Nokia 6310 with
studied in two separate experiments. a small screen showing still pictures in black and white. The
904 J.E.B. Törnros, A.K. Bolling / Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2005) 902–909

phone task in the conversation experiment was a demand- 1. RSME (rating scale mental effort), a self-reported mea-
ing paced serial addition task (Brookhuis et al., 1991). In sure of mental effort on a continuous unidimensional scale
the handsfree part the drivers used a handsfree mobile phone with eight defined points along the scale—from “abso-
(the telephone always remained in the holder attached to the lutely no effort” to “extreme effort” (Zijlstra and Van
dashboard and the driver conversed via loudspeakers) while Dorn, 1985).
driving in the simulator. Only for accepting and ending the 2. Influence of mobile phone use on speed and lateral posi-
call did the driver have to reach over to touch the corre- tion.
sponding buttons on the phone. In the handheld condition 3. Opinion about driving performance during phone use—a
the telephone had to be taken in the hand for use. While on simple scale with defined endpoints (“much worse than
stand-by, the telephone was placed in the holder, and when normal”, “much better than normal”) and the middlepoint
the phone rang the driver had to pick it up and hold it to his/her (“as normal”).
ear while talking. After finishing the call the telephone was
put back into the holder. When receiving a phone call, the par- It should be pointed our that the RSME ratings, made after
ticipant had to perform the paced serial addition task. Each the test drives, referred to the whole test drive irrespective
phone call lasted about 1 min. of whether the phone was used or not. Effects of using the
Dialling was done either in handsfree mode or in handheld phone (difference between using the phone and not using
mode. The driver was requested to make a phone call when the phone) could therefore not be analysed for the RSME
the word “Ring” in large yellow letters was projected in the measure. Comparisons based on RSME ratings could only be
centre of the simulator screen. The phone number was glued made between the two driver groups (handsfree, handheld).
to the telephone.
2.8. Procedure
2.6. Experimental design
Upon arriving at the driving simulator facility the partic-
ipant was introduced to the simulator. The participant was
In the conversation experiment, each participant received
presented a written instruction explaining the driving and
a total of 10 phone calls at pre-selected locations during the
phone tasks of the conversation experiment. The instruction
test drive. Comparisons were made with data collected at the
was to drive as s/he would do in real traffic under the same
same locations (the route was driven twice) where no phone
conditions. The conversation task was explained by the test
call was made, serving as a control condition. The effect of
leader and practised. The participant then entered the sim-
presentation order was controlled through counterbalancing.
ulator for a practice drive where the phone task was also
Data collected during conversation (excluding picking up the
practised. Then the main test drive took place. After the test
phone, starting and finishing the call, and replacing the phone
drive the participant left the simulator to fill in the question-
into the holder) was used in the analyses.
naire.
In the dialling experiment only one drive was made during
The dialling experiment followed immediately thereafter.
which the participant dialled a phone number three times.
The participant received a written instruction, followed
Comparisons were made between those occasions and the
by the test drive. After the test drive in the simulator the
part of the drive when the phone was not used, serving as a
participant filled in the questionnaire relating to the dialling
control condition.
session.

2.7. Effect measures 2.9. Significance tests


The following performance measures are analysed in the The effect of phone task (conversation and dialling) is
present article: analysed as two different experiments since the experimental
1. PDT (peripheral detection task): reaction time to detected conditions are not comparable. Behavioural data is analysed
stimuli and percentage missed PDT signals as a measure with variance analysis. Main effects of phone use (yes/no) and
of mental workload (van Winsum et al., 1999). The drivers phone mode (handsfree/handheld) are analysed. Where inter-
respond to visual stimuli presented off centre of their for- actions between use and mode appear simple main effects of
ward view by pressing a micro switch placed on the finger. phone use are analysed. The χ2 -test is used to analyse subjec-
If a driver reacted to the PDT stimulus within two seconds, tive effects of phone use on driving speed and lateral position
the reaction was scored as a “hit”, and reaction time was (after dichotomization). The Fisher exact test for independent
measured. Otherwise it was scored as a “miss”. samples is used to compare the two phone modes with respect
2. Standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP). to the results for the subjective effects of phone use on driving
3. Average driving speed. speed and lateral position. The Wilcoxon–Mann–Whitney
test for independent samples is used to analyse the results
The following subjective measures are analysed in the for RSME and for subjective driving performance. Pearson
present article: product-moment correlation coefficients between actual
J.E.B. Törnros, A.K. Bolling / Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2005) 902–909 905

and subjective effects of phone use, and the significance


levels attached to the correlation coefficients, are calculated.
A significance level of .05 is adopted for the significance
tests.

3. Results

3.1. Influence of phone use on performance on the


peripheral detection task (PDT) Fig. 3. Rated effort on RSME for conversation and for dialling (±quartile
deviation) (end points: 0 = absolutely no effort, 120 = extreme effort).
The effect of phone use (difference between phone and
control situation) on PDT reaction time is shown in Fig. 1. 3.2. Rated effort on RSME
In the conversation experiment, the PDT reaction time was
analysed as the mean for the locations where phone calls The drivers rated their experienced effort for the whole
took place compared to the mean for the corresponding route on the RSME scale, irrespective of whether the phone
control locations. A main effect of both types of tasks was used or not. The result is shown in Fig. 3. No differ-
appeared but there was no interaction between phone use and ence between phone modes appeared for either of the tasks.
phone mode in either of the tasks. It can be concluded that The average rating for the conversation task corresponded
PDT reaction time performance was impaired by both phone roughly to “rather much effort”–“considerable effort” and
tasks, irrespective of phone mode. The size of the effect was for the dialling task to “some effort” on the scale.
159 ms for the conversation task and 270 ms for the dialling
task. 3.3. Correlation between PDT performance and RSME
The effect of phone use on percentage missed PDT signals
is shown in Fig. 2. A main effect of phone use appeared Correlation coefficients between effort ratings and the
irrespective of task, but there was no interaction between effect of phone use on PDT performance (reaction time,
phone use and phone mode. It can be concluded that phone missed signals) were calculated for the both experiments
use caused an increased number of missed PDT signals for (conversation, dialling). No correlation coefficient was sig-
both phone modes. The size of the effect was 12.7% units nificant for either user group (handsfree, handheld).
for the conversation task and 24.3% units for the dialling
task. Thus, both phone modes had similar effects on PDT 3.4. Influence of phone use on lateral position deviation
performance.
The effect of phone use on the average standard deviation
of lateral position is shown in Fig. 4. A main effect of con-
versation appeared, but no interaction between conversation
and phone mode. The standard deviation of lateral position
decreased as an effect of conversation, irrespective of phone
mode. The size of the effect was 1.3 cm.
A main effect of dialling appeared, but no interaction
between dialling and phone mode. It can be concluded that
the standard deviation of lateral position increased as an effect
of dialling, irrespective of phone mode. The size of the effect
Fig. 1. Effect of conversation and dialling on PDT reaction time—difference was 6.5 cm.
between phone use and control condition (±S.D.).

Fig. 4. Effect of conversation and dialling on standard deviation of lat-


Fig. 2. Effect of conversation and dialling on percentage missed PDT eral position (SDLP)—difference between phone use and control condition
signals—difference between phone use and control condition (±S.D.). (±S.D.).
906 J.E.B. Törnros, A.K. Bolling / Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2005) 902–909

Fig. 5. Reported change in lateral position deviation (LPD) as a result of


conversation and dialling.
Fig. 7. Reported speed change as a result of conversation and dialling.

Thus, the effect of phone use was the opposite in the


two experiments. No difference appeared, however, between
handsfree and handheld phone use.
for both phone modes. The effect was greater for hands-
free mode; 3.7 km/h compared to 2.0 km/h for handheld
3.5. Influence of phone use on subjective lateral position mode.
deviation Thus, the speed reduction was greater for handheld
mode than for handsfree mode in the conversation experi-
The effect of phone use on subjective lateral position devi- ment, whereas the opposite effect appeared in the dialling
ation is shown in Fig. 5. In the conversation experiment 20 of experiment. It could be added that the speed levels were
the handsfree drivers and 17 of the handheld drivers answered quite normal for the traffic environments in the two experi-
the question in a way that can be classified either as increased ments.
lateral position deviation or no effect (reduced deviation was
not included as an alternative). The most common answer
3.7. Influence of phone use on subjective speed
was “no effect”. The answers to the question did not differ
for the two phone modes. The result in the dialling experi-
The effect of phone use on subjective speed is shown
ment was the same as in the conversation experiment (data
in Fig. 7. In the conversation experiment 22 of the hands-
from 16 handsfree and 13 handheld drivers).
free drivers and 18 of the handheld drivers answered
the question. The most common answer was “speed
3.6. Influence of phone use on average speed reduction”. The answers to the question did not differ
for the two phone modes. In the dialling experiment 22
The effect of phone use on speed is shown in Fig. 6. A of the handsfree drivers and 19 of the handheld drivers
main effect of conversation and an interaction between con- answered the question. The result was similar as in the
versation and phone mode were obtained. Analysis of simple conversation experiment with no difference between phone
main effects shows that average speed was not influenced modes.
by conversation for handsfree mode whereas it was reduced
by conversation for handheld mode. The size of the speed
reduction was 2.8 km/h. 3.8. Correlation between actual and subjective effect of
A main effect of dialling and an interaction between phone use on speed
dialling and phone mode appeared. Analysis of simple main
effects shows that average speed was reduced by dialling In the conversation experiment the correlation between
objective and subjective effect on driving speed is insignifi-
cant for both modes. Some individuals reported an effect of
phone conversation which was the opposite to the effect on
their driving behaviour. Seven of 30 persons who reported
reduced speed drove faster when using the phone, and three
of four persons who reported increased speed drove more
slowly when using the phone.
In the dialling experiment the result was similar. The cor-
relation between objective and subjective effects on driving
speed is also insignificant for both modes. Some individ-
uals reported an effect of dialling which was the opposite
to the real effect. Four of 26 persons who reported reduced
speed drove faster when dialling, and all four persons who
Fig. 6. Effect of conversation and dialling on average speed—difference reported increased speed actually drove more slowly when
between phone use and control condition (±S.D.). they dialled.
J.E.B. Törnros, A.K. Bolling / Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2005) 902–909 907

than for handsfree mode (Burns et al., 2002; Brookhuis et al.,


1991), or no difference between the two modes (Matthews et
al., 2003).
Lateral position deviation, used as the main driving perfor-
mance measure, was affected by dialling a phone number—it
increased for both phone modes. The result is interpreted as an
indication of reduced safety. The effect was of similar size for
the two phone modes, a result which deviates somewhat from
Brookhuis et al. (1991), who found a greater lateral position
deviation for the handheld mode as a result of dialling. The
Fig. 8. Rated driving performance for conversation and dialling (±quartile
result for the conversation experiment was, however, quite
deviation) (end points: 0 = much worse than normal, 100 = much better than
normal). the opposite. Lateral position deviation decreased to a small
extent for both modes. This finding deviates from Haigney
3.9. Rated driving performance et al. (2000) (who showed impaired lateral control only for
handheld phone) and from Burns et al. (2002), who did not
Fig. 8 shows the result of the ratings regarding driving demonstrate any effect of phone use on lane keeping per-
performance. In the conversation experiment the handsfree formance for either phone mode. A similar effect to what
drivers reported better driving performance than the hand- was found in the present experiment, decreased lateral posi-
held drivers. The average rating for handsfree drivers was tion deviation, was however demonstrated for car following
somewhat closer to “as normal” than to “much worse than in real traffic (Brookhuis et al., 1991). The interpretation,
normal”. The average rating for handheld drivers was some- suggested by Brookhuis et al. (1991), was that the decreased
what closer to “much worse than normal” than to “as normal”. lateral position deviation is an effect of increased driver alert-
In the dialling experiment there was no difference between ness. Alternative interpretations might be apt, however. One
the two phone modes. The average rating was closer to “as such attempt might be couched in terms of safety margins
normal” than to “much worse than normal”. in lateral position when driving. A “virtual safety margin”
can be seen as an invisible line to the left and to the right of
the vehicle, and the driver wants to keep the vehicle inside
4. Discussion this “tunnel”, where he or she feels safe. The driver accepts a
certain lateral position deviation as long as the vehicle does
The result of the study regarding driving behaviour is sum- not cross the safety margins. In a situation with increased
marised in Table 1. workload (for example high speed, a complex traffic envi-
The peripheral detection task was used as a measure of ronment or mobile phone conversation) the driver positions
mental workload. An increase in mental workload may inter- the safety margins closer to the vehicle, which means that the
fere with driving tasks that also make demands on the driver’s lateral position deviation will decrease since the driver wants
information processing abilities (Alm and Nilsson, 2001). to keep the car inside the margins. It could, in other words,
The handsfree and the handheld mode were rather equiv- be interpreted as a compensation for the increased workload.
alent in terms of increased workload caused by phone use Other explanations also seem plausible, however. It might be
according to the PDT results. This conclusion is valid both related to the steering becoming less prioritised during the
for dialling and for conversation. Handsfree mobile phone use phone conversation.
is commonly supposed to be more “user friendly” since the Driving speed, being related to crash risk and to the seri-
driver does not have to hold the phone in the hand. Conversely, ousness of a crash, is also relevant from a traffic safety
the results clearly show that the handsfree and the handheld perspective. The drivers reduced their speed when dialling.
mode were rather equivalent in terms of increased workload, The speed reduction was, however, greater for the hands-
a result that closely corresponds to the results found by Patten free mode, which should be interpreted in terms of different
et al. (2004) in real traffic. In other studies, the result for men- degrees of compensation. The reason for the results can be
tal effort ratings was somewhat mixed—higher for handheld supposed to lie in the fact that for dialling the number in

Table 1
The effects of phone use on driving behaviour (+: increase, −: decrease, 0: no effect)
Measure Dialling Conversation

Handsfree Handheld Handsfree Handheld


PDT reaction time + + + +
PDT missed signals + + + +
Lateral position deviation + + − −
Speed − − 0 −
908 J.E.B. Törnros, A.K. Bolling / Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2005) 902–909

handsfree mode the drivers had to look away from the road any safer than handheld phones. The effects of conversation
for a longer time period than in handheld mode. When ringing seem to be rather similar for the two phone modes. When
up in handheld mode the drivers grabbed the mobile phone dialling, the situation may even be the opposite one with the
from the holder (which hardly ever required the driver to handsfree phone less safe. These facts are not compatible to
look away from the road), and then held the phone close to what many national governments have legislated, not having
the upper part of the steering wheel while dialling. From this had sufficient knowledge and understanding of these issues.
position the mobile phone was much closer to the frontal view It can be discussed whether the effects found in the exper-
of the driver than in the handsfree mode. The driving speed iment are safety critical from a traffic perspective. The result
was also affected by phone conversation. The speed reducing for dialling certainly points in that direction and a recom-
effect of mobile phone conversation was, however, restricted mendation to use voice-activated handsfree phones instead
to handheld mode (even though a similar tendency appeared of manually controlled phones seems to be in place. As
for handsfree mode). Speed reduction caused by talking on to conversation, the PDT results indicate that the conver-
the phone while driving a car has been demonstrated in other sation was demanding in terms of mental workload, which
simulator studies as well: Haigney et al. (2000) showed the could be interpreted as the driver having less mental capacity
effect for both phone modes, whereas Burns et al. (2002) and left to attend to traffic during the conversation. This could
Patten et al. (2004) found a speed reducing effect for handheld have negative consequences in terms of reduced readiness
phones only. Since the speed reduction can be interpreted as to respond if a risky situation were suddenly to appear. It
an attempt to compensate for the increased workload caused can be concluded with reasonable certainty that the drivers
by the phone conversation, conversation via a handheld phone using handheld phones tried to compensate for the increased
seems to trigger a larger compensatory effect than a handsfree workload caused by the mobile phone conversation by speed
phone. The result is a bit intriguing since no difference in PDT reduction. To what extent the reduced speed would compen-
performance was apparent between the two phone modes. sate for the reduced readiness is unclear.
The drivers using a handheld phone may to some extent have When discussing safety effects of dialling versus con-
made the driving task less mentally loading by slowing down. versation, the time aspect should be considered. Although
This way, mental workload might have become rather simi- dialling may be associated with a higher risk than conversa-
lar to the workload in the handsfree condition. An alternative tion, it probably makes up a much smaller part of the total
explanation might be that drivers tend to underestimate the time of using a telephone compared to conversation. Differ-
risk associated with conversation when using a handsfree ences observed for the conversation phase, e.g. the lack of a
phone, and therefore do not compensate by reduced speed speed reduction when using a handsfree phone, may have a
as much as when they use a handheld telephone. The latter is much larger impact on the number of accidents than differ-
more intuitively associated with increased risk. ences during the dialling phase.
A strict comparison between dialling and conversation The present study concentrated on the effects of mobile
cannot be made, since the test situations were quite different, phone conversation and dialling on driving. Other aspects of
and the experimental control of the presentation order was not mobile phone use while driving still remain to be analysed
controlled, the dialling sessions always coming after the con- more in detail, such as distractions caused by starting or fin-
versation session. However, the impression is that the effects ishing a call, looking for a phone number to dial, mishaps like
of dialling might be more negative for traffic safety, since the dropping the phone, etc. A mobile phone with a screen show-
lateral control was negatively affected only by dialling, not ing black-and-white still images was used in the study. There
by conversation. The effect for dialling is probably related to are, however, newer and more advanced types of mobile
the visual/motor component which was lacking in the conver- phone on the market with the capacity to transmit moving
sation task. The fact that compensation in terms of reduced images in colour. The risk of interference with the driving
speed differed between the two experiments might be inter- task may well increase further with these new phones. This
preted in a similar way—speed reduction was apparent for issue would also require detailed study.
both phone modes during dialling with the largest effect for
handsfree mode, whereas such compensation was apparent
only for handheld mode during conversation.
The participants reported driving performance as being Acknowledgement
more influenced/impaired by conversing on the handheld
than on the handsfree phone, a result which hardly resem- This project was funded by the Swedish Road Adminis-
bles the results for actual driving performance. For dialling, tration (SRA).
the rated driving performance did not differ between the two
modes—the correspondence between subjective and objec-
tive effects regarding driving performance was better in this References
case.
The most obvious fact that ought to be transmitted to the Alm, H., Nilsson, L., 2001. The use of car phones and changes in driver
public is the fact that handsfree phones do not seem to be behaviour. Int. J. Vehicle Des. 26 (1), 4–11 (special issue).
J.E.B. Törnros, A.K. Bolling / Accident Analysis and Prevention 37 (2005) 902–909 909

Brookhuis, K., de Vries, G., de Waard, D., 1991. The effects of 37th Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomic Society,
mobile telephoning on driving performance. Accident Anal. Prev. 23, Seattle, USA, October 11–15.
309–316. Patten, C., Kircher, A., Östlund, J., Nilsson, L., 2004. Using mobile tele-
Burns, P.C., Parkes, A., Burton, S., Smith, R.K., Burch, D., 2002. How phones: cognitive workload and attention resource allocation. Accident
dangerous is driving with a mobile phone? Benchmarking the impair- Anal. Prev. 36, 341–350.
ment to alcohol. TRL Report TRL 547, TRL, Crowthorne, United Rospa (Royal Society for the Prevention of Accidents), 2002. The risk
Kingdom. of using a mobile phone while driving. On-line paper. Available at
Haigney, D.E., Taylor, R.G., Westerman, S.J., 2000. Concurrent mobile http://www.rospa.com/pdfs/road/mobiles/report.pdf.
(cellular) phone use and driving performance: task demand character- Strayer, D.L., Drews, F.A., Crouch, D.J., 2004. A comparison of the
istics and compensatory processes. Transport. Res., Part F, 113–121. cell phone driver and the drunk driver. On-line paper. Available at
Kircher, A., Vogel, K., Törnros, J., Bolling, A., Nilsson, L., Patten, http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract id=570222.
C., Malmström, T., Ceci, R., 2004. Mobile Telephone Simulator van Winsum, W., Martens, M.H., Herland, L., 1999. The effects of
Study. VTI Meddelande 969A. Swedish National Road and Trans- speech versus tactile driver support messages on workload, driver
port Research Institute, Linköping, Sweden. behaviour and user acceptance (TM-99-C043): TNO Human Factors,
Matthews, R., Legg, S., Charlton, S., 2003. The effect of cell phone The Netherlands.
type on drivers subjective workload during concurrent driving and Zijlstra, F., Van Dorn, L., 1985. The Construction of a Scale to Mea-
conversing. Accident Anal. Prev. 35 (4), 451–457. sure Perceived Effort. Delft University of Technology, The Nether-
Nilsson, L., 1993. Behavioural research in an advanced driving lands.
simulator—experiences of the VTI system. In: Paper Presented at the

You might also like