You are on page 1of 9

International Journal of Psychology

International Journal of Psychology, 2021


DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12796

Clinical symptoms of anxiety disorders as predictors


of political attitudes: A prospective cohort study
Vilja Helminen1 , Marko Elovainio1,2 , and Markus Jokela1
1 Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki,
Finland
2 National Institute for Health and Welfare, Finland

C onservative political ideologies have been suggested to correlate with elevated sensitivity to threat. However, it
is unclear whether the associations between threat sensitivity and political attitudes can be observed with clinical
measures of mental health. We examined how anxiety disorders predicted attitudes on several political issues. Participants
were 7253 individuals from the 1958 British Birth Cohort study. Symptoms of generalised anxiety disorder, phobia
and panic were assessed in a clinical interview at age 44, and opinions about political issues were self-reported by
the participants 6 years later. Anxiety symptoms were associated with higher concerns about economic inequality,
preservation of the environment, distrust in politics and lower work ethic. No associations were observed with racist
or authoritarian attitudes, or support for traditional family values. We also assessed how political attitudes at ages 33 and
42 predicted anxiety disorder symptoms at age 44, revealing a possible bidirectional association between concern for
economic inequality and anxiety disorder symptoms. These findings do not support an association between conservative
political attitudes and elevated threat sensitivity. Rather, elevated anxiety may increase concerns about social inequality
and the environment.
Keywords: Political attitudes; Threat; NCDS; Fear; Anxiety.

According to the ‘motivated social cognition’ theory Political ideology is often represented as liberal-
of political ideology, differences in political ideology conservative or left–right continuum. The liberal or
and attitudes partly reflect psychological differences in left side is generally associated with such things as
sensitivity to threat (Altemeyer, 2006; Duckitt, 2001). In equal opportunity, more just distribution of wealth and
particular, conservative beliefs have been suggested to the rights of minorities. The conservative or right side,
defend an individual against psychological threats by ful- by contrast, is associated with free enterprise, capital-
filling the individual’s heightened needs for security and ism, authoritarianism and traditional values. Elevated
certainty (Jost et al., 2003; Jost et al., 2007). Conservative threat sensitivity has been associated with conser-
people are suggested to be more sensitive to threatening vative political ideology in many different research
and ambiguous stimuli than people with liberal attitudes, designs (Jost et al., 2003; Onraet et al., 2013). Most
and this may help to explain why they find conservative studies have focused only on conservatism, which
political views more appealing. However, it is unclear (1) has often been assessed with self-identification on
whether the associations between threat sensitivity and a one-dimensional liberal-conservative continuum
ideology extend to other sociopolitical attitudes besides (e.g., Jost et al., 2007), right-wing authoritarianism
conservatism, and (2) whether these associations can be (e.g., Duckitt & Fisher, 2003), or party affiliation and
observed also with clinical measures of threat sensitivity, support for conservative or liberal political candidates
that is, measures of anxiety disorder. (e.g., Ogilvie et al., 2008).

Correspondence should be addressed to Vilja Helminen, University of Helsinki, P.O. Box 9, 00014, Helsinki, Finland. (E-mail:
vilja.helminen@helsinki.fi)
The analyses in this work are based wholly or in part on analysis of data from the National Child Development Study (NCDS). The data was deposited
at the UK Data Archive by the Centre for Longitudinal Studies at the Institute of Education, University of London. NCDS is funded by the Economic
and Social Research Council (ESRC).
Vilja Helminen: Conceptualisation, methodology, formal analysis, writing—original draft.
Marko Elovainio: Methodology, writing—review and editing.
Markus Jokela: Conceptualisation, methodology, writing—review and editing, supervision.

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
2 HELMINEN, ELOVAINIO, JOKELA

To test whether threat sensitivity is broadly related seven different domains of political attitudes assessed
to different aspects of conservative beliefs, it is neces- 6 years later. We also examined the reverse temporal
sary to examine specific subscales of political attitudes direction, that is, whether political attitudes assessed
instead of broad measures of ideology; it is possible that two and 9 years earlier predicted the presence of anxiety
the associations between threat sensitivity and conser- symptoms in the clinical interview.
vatism are driven by specific domains of political atti- Based on the theory of motivated social cognition and
tudes. For example, threat sensitivity has been linked to previous studies, it would be expected that anxiety dis-
attitudes towards out-groups, including negative attitudes order symptoms are associated with those attitudes that
towards immigrants and Muslims (e.g., Golec de Zavala are usually considered to reflect right-wing or conser-
et al., 2010; Van de Vyver et al., 2016) and opposition to vative political ideology, or at the very least with atti-
affirmative action, less support for multiculturalism, and tudes concerning out-groups if not conservative attitudes
the promotion of equal rights (e.g., Renfro et al., 2006; more broadly. Alternative hypotheses are also plausible:
Verkuyten, 2009). anxiety proneness might be related to generalised worry
While conservatives tend to hold more negative atti- over different life domains, in which case the associations
tudes towards out-groups than liberals (Jost et al., 2009), could be observed also in attitudes unrelated to conserva-
the general attitude towards outgroups is not a defining tive beliefs. We assume that the associations would be as
feature of conservative political ideology which encom- strong, or even stronger, with the clinical measures of fear
passes a broader set of political positions. It is therefore and anxiety than with the previously used measurement
important to examine whether threat sensitivity is system- instruments of threat, as clinical measures assess extreme
atically related to multiple political attitudes related to the tendency to experience fear and anxiety.
conservative/liberal axis, or whether the associations are
related to more narrow domains of political attitudes.
It is also important to broaden the measures of threat METHODS
sensitivity. Threat sensitivity may represent reactions of
fear and anxiety. Fear occurs when the threat is imminent Sample
and the fear response usually dissipates quickly. Anxi-
ety is a sustained response to uncertain threats (Sylvers Participants were from the National Child Development
et al., 2011). Study (NCDS; Power & Elliott, 2006), which follows
There are individual differences in how easily and how 18,558 participants from Great Britain born in one partic-
strongly these threat responses arise (Sylvers et al., 2011). ular week in 1958. Since the first survey, there have been
Extreme reactivity can develop into clinical anxiety disor- nine follow-up surveys at the ages of 7, 11, 16, 23, 33, 42,
ders, such as generalised anxiety disorder (GAD), panic 46, 50 and 55. An additional biomedical survey for 12,069
disorder, specific phobia, or social anxiety. These disor- of the cohort members in 2002–2003 at age 44 also cov-
ders manifest as excessive fear and worry either in relation ered biomedical risk factors and current health condition,
to specific situations or in a more free-floating manner, as including mental health. The present study utilises data
well as somatic symptoms such as physiological arousal, from the 8th follow-up survey conducted in 2008–2009
muscle tension, fatigue and sleep disturbances (Sylvers at age 50, the biomedical survey conducted in 2002–2003
et al., 2011). Anxiety disorders can be divided into at age 44, as well as the 5th and 6th follow-up surveys
two sub-dimensions of distress (sometimes also called conducted in 1991 at age 33 and in 1999–2000 at age
anxious-misery) and fear, so that phobias are more closely 42, respectively (University of London, Institute of Edu-
related to fear, GAD to distress and panic disorder equally cation, Centre for Longitudinal Studies, 2008a, 2008b,
to both depending on the specific symptom profile (e.g., 2009, 2012).
Eaton et al., 2013; Vaidyanathan et al., 2009). The current sample comprised those cohort mem-
The evidence on clinical measures and political atti- bers selected for the additional biomedical survey. The
tudes is very limited. Hatemi et al. (2013) found that selection was based on the response to previous three
recent phobic fear and lifetime social phobia correlated follow-up surveys leaving 12,069 participants out of the
with conservative self-identification and more negative original 18,558 cohort members. A total of 9377 partic-
out-group attitudes, suggesting that clinical symptoms ipants answered the biomedical survey. Compared to the
related to threat sensitivity may be associated with more surviving cohort, those with behavioural problems, lower
conservative political orientation. social class and lower cognitive ability in childhood are
The purpose of the present study was to extend previ- somewhat under-represented in the biomedical survey
ous research by using a broader assessment of political (Atherton et al., 2008).
attitudes and a clinical assessment of threat sensitivity. A total of 1182 of those who answered the biomedical
We used data from a large longitudinal cohort study to survey had not participated in the 8th follow-up survey,
examine whether symptoms of anxiety and fear assessed and were therefore excluded from the analyses. Further
in a clinical interview were longitudinally associated with 942 had not answered some of the relevant mental health,

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.
ANXIETY DISORDERS PREDICTING ATTITUDES 3

political attitude, or personality questions and were categories: two points or more were considered clinical
excluded, leaving the final sample to be 7253 (48.1% levels of symptoms (Lewis et al., 1992).
men) for the analyses examining how clinical levels of
fear and anxiety at age 44 predict political attitudes at age
Political attitudes
50. Of these 7253 participants, 626 had not responded
to some of the relevant political attitude questions at age Political attitudes were assessed at ages 50, 42 and
33 or 42 and were excluded from the analyses examining 33 with 21 statements covering, among others, attitudes
how earlier political attitudes predict clinical levels of towards environmental issues, the severity of punishments
fear and anxiety at age 44, leaving the final sample for and marriage (e.g., ‘Schools should teach children to obey
these analyses to be 5823 (52.3% men). authority’). Participants indicated to what extent they
We examined attrition using logistic regression to see agreed with the statements on a scale from 1 (strongly
if participants’ gender or GAD, phobia and panic symp- agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). For the purposes of this
toms predicted non-participation in the 8th follow-up sur- study, items were reverse coded for more intuitive inter-
vey. Assessed separately, panic, phobia, GAD (ps < .05) pretation.
and gender (p < .001) all predicted attrition statistically
significantly. Controlling for other predictors, only panic
was not a significant predictor of attrition. The odds ratios Level of education
showed one-point increase in participant’s GAD, panic, or For the analyses examining how anxiety disorder
phobia score increasing the risk of non-response in the 8th symptoms at age 44 predict later political attitudes partic-
follow-up survey 1.1-fold.
ipants’ level of education was categorised based on infor-
Ethical approval for the NCDS was obtained from the
mation from the previous follow-up surveys as follows:
London Multi-centre Research Ethics Committee for sur-
none (0), bad O-levels or CSE 2–5 (1), good O-levels or 1
veys at age 50 and 42, South-east Multi-centre Research
A-level (2), 2 or more A-levels (3), sub-degree (4), degree
Ethics Committee for the biomedical survey at age 44,
(5) and higher degree (6). For the analyses examining how
and internally for the survey at age 33 which predates the
attitudes at age 33 predict later anxiety disorder symp-
establishment of Multi-centre Research Ethics Commit-
toms, we used participants’ level of education at age 33
tees (University of London, Institute of Education, Centre
categorised as follows: none (0), CSE 2–5 (1), O-levels
for Longitudinal Studies, 2014). Study was performed in
(2), A-levels (3), professional qualifications (4), degree
accordance with the ethical standards as set forth in the
level or higher (5).
1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.
Written informed consent was obtained from all indi-
vidual adult participants of the biomedical survey at age Statistical analyses
44, and all participants were given information about and
the opportunity to opt out of the surveys at ages 33, 42 and The underlying structure of the attitude variables mea-
50 (University of London, Institute of Education, Centre sured at age 50 was first examined with exploratory
for Longitudinal Studies, 2014). factor analysis using maximum likelihood extraction
and oblimin rotation. The resulting attitude factors were
calculated as mean scores (not weighted by factor load-
Measures
ings) in further analyses. The associations of anxiety,
GAD, phobia and panic phobia and panic scores with later political attitudes were
estimated with linear regression. First, we used the mean
GAD, phobia and panic disorder symptoms were score of the three anxiety variables to predict the attitude
assessed at age 44 using the CIS-R (Lewis et al., 1992), a scales. We then examined each of the three subscales sep-
widely used standardised interview for assessing common arately. In model 1, the mean anxiety disorder symptom
mental health disorders. Each symptom category com- score was regressed on the attitude summary scores. In
prised four questions about the symptoms experienced model 2, the dummy variables indicating the presence of
past week (e.g., ‘On how many of the past seven days clinical levels of each GAD, panic and phobia symptoms
have you felt generally anxious/nervous/tense?’). A score were regressed on the attitude summary scores. Both,
ranging from 0 to 4 was calculated for GAD, phobia and models 1 and 2 were tested by running seven separate
panic symptoms separately, based on the reported severity linear regressions with the anxiety and fear variables as
and frequency of symptoms, with higher scores indicat- predictors together with gender and level of education
ing more severe and frequent symptoms. A mean score as control variables. Regression results are reported as
consisting of GAD, phobia and panic symptoms was also unstandardised regression coefficients (B) and standard-
calculated to assess overall anxiety disorder symptoms. ised regression coefficients (β). No power analysis was
For GAD, phobia and panic symptoms we used a cut-off conducted before analysing the data. All analyses were
of two points out of four to divide participants into two conducted using R version 3.6.1. Package psych was used

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.
4 HELMINEN, ELOVAINIO, JOKELA

p < .001; phobia: B = 0.18, p < .001), and environmental


concern (GAD: B = 0.07, p = .041; phobia: B = 0.11,
p = .016). Phobia also predicted higher distrust in politics
(B = 0.12, p = .021), and lower work ethic (B = −0.10,
p = .026).
Tables 4 and 5 shows how political attitudes at age
33 and 42 predicted anxiety disorder symptoms at age
44. Of the attitudes at age 42, concern about economic
inequality and authoritarian attitudes predicted higher
likelihood of GAD and panic disorder; higher scores on
family values predicted lower likelihood of GAD and
panic; and concern about economic inequality predicted
higher likelihood of phobias. Of the attitudes at age 33,
concern about economic inequality and work ethic were
associated with higher likelihood of GAD; and concern
about economic inequality was associated with higher
Figure 1. Scree plot showing eigenvalues for each factor of political
attitudes at the age of 50.
likelihood of phobia.

for the exploratory factor analysis. The analysis code is DISCUSSION


available upon request.
We examined whether clinically assessed anxiety disor-
ders (generalised anxiety disorder, phobia and panic dis-
RESULTS order) were longitudinally related to political attitudes
and vice versa. Higher scores on anxiety symptoms were
We retained seven factors in the exploratory factor analy- associated with higher concerns about economic inequal-
sis based on the results of parallel analysis and the exam- ity, distrust in politics, environmental concern, as well as
ination of the scree plot (Figure 1). Table 1 shows the lower work ethic. These associations were observed par-
factor loadings after rotation. Together, the seven fac- ticularly with symptoms of generalised anxiety disorder
tors explained 46.90% of the total variance and the com- (GAD) and phobia, whereas symptoms of panic disor-
munalities ranged between .06 and .77. We interpreted der did not have independent associations with political
the factors to capture attitudes concerning (1) economic attitudes. Most of these associations were small in magni-
inequality, (2) distrust in politics, (3) racism, (4) attitudes tude (B < 0.20). Higher concern about inequality assessed
concerning authority, (5) protecting the environment, (6) years before the clinical interview also predicted higher
family values and (7) work ethic. The solution is simi- likelihood of GAD and phobia symptoms, suggesting a
lar to the ones obtained in previous studies (e.g., Cheng possible bidirectional association.
et al., 2012). The findings suggest that the previously reported asso-
The factor loadings of one item (‘All women should ciations between threat sensitivity and political ideology
have the right to choose abortion’) was remarkably low, (e.g., Jost et al., 2003; Onraet et al., 2013) may not gen-
with the highest loading being −0.23, so this item was not eralise to clinical measures of anxiety disorders or may
included in any of the attitude scores. One item (‘Politi- be limited to only some political and cultural contexts. In
cians are in politics for self, not community benefit’) had fact, the current associations between anxiety disorders
similar loadings on factors of inequality and distrust in and political attitudes were opposite to the hypothesis of
politics. We included the item in distrust in politics, as the conservatism and heightened threat sensitivity, because
item content fitted the interpretation of the factors better. concerns about inequality and the environment tend to
The association between the mean anxiety disorder be associated with liberal and left-wing political ideol-
symptom score and political attitudes (model 1), and ogy (e.g., Cheng et al., 2012; Poortinga et al., 2011), and
GAD, phobia and panic symptoms and political atti- these attitudes were associated with higher scores on anx-
tudes (model 2) are shown in Tables 2 and 3. The mean iety disorders. Lower work ethic and distrust in politics
symptom score predicted higher concern about inequal- were also associated with anxiety disorders. Lower work
ity (unstandardised B = 0.17, p < .001), distrust in poli- ethic would probably be related to more liberal than con-
tics (B = 0.09, p < .001) and concern for the environment servative attitudes, whereas distrust in politics is more
(B = 0.07, p < .001) and lower work ethic (B = −0.08, ambiguous—insofar as it reflects a distrust towards gov-
p < .001). ernmental decision making, it might represent more con-
Of the specific factors, GAD and phobia predicted servative than liberal attitudes on the conservative–liberal
higher concern for economic inequality (GAD: B = 0.14, axis.

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.
ANXIETY DISORDERS PREDICTING ATTITUDES 5

TABLE 1
Factor analysis of political attitude items at the age of 50

Loadings
Item F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6 F7 h2

Ordinary working people do not get their fair share of 0.53 0.01 0.05 0.17 0.10 −0.01 −0.00 0.37
the nation’s wealth
There is one law for the rich and one for the poor 0.89 −0.02 0.00 −0.02 −0.01 −0.02 0.01 0.77
Management will always try to get the better of 0.45 0.11 −0.09 0.06 0.02 0.04 0.03 0.33
employees if it gets the chance
Politicians are mainly in politics for their own benefit 0.38 0.33 −0.04 0.06 −0.11 0.04 0.01 0.46
and not for the benefit of the community
None of the political parties would do anything to 0.06 0.72 −0.02 0.01 −0.03 −0.01 −0.01 0.59
benefit me
It does not really make much difference which political −0.04 0.81 0.03 0.02 0.02 −0.01 0.01 0.63
party is in power in Britain
I would not mind working with people from other races 0.01 0.01 0.81 0.00 0.00 0.02 0.04 0.66
I would not want a person from another race to be my 0.04 −0.01 −0.78 −0.02 0.01 0.04 0.03 0.62
boss
I would not mind if a family from another race moved in 0.06 0.01 0.70 −0.09 0.02 0.02 −0.02 0.50
next door to me
People who break the law should be given stiffer 0.01 −0.01 0.01 0.81 −0.02 0.01 −0.00 0.66
sentences
School should teach children to obey authority −0.01 −0.03 −0.01 0.54 0.07 0.10 0.06 0.34
For some crimes the death penalty is the most 0.04 0.09 −0.11 0.61 −0.04 −0.05 −0.02 0.49
appropriate sentence
Problems in the environment are not as serious as 0.04 0.14 −0.03 0.04 −0.58 0.11 −0.03 0.41
people claim
We should tackle problems in the environment even if 0.03 −0.04 0.04 0.00 0.74 0.03 −0.02 0.57
this means slower economic growth
Preserving the environment is more important than any 0.01 0.19 −0.07 −0.00 0.62 0.04 0.00 0.40
other political issue today
Marriage is for life −0.04 −0.04 0.14 0.12 −0.01 0.52 0.02 0.31
Couples who have children should not separate −0.01 −0.01 −0.05 −0.02 0.00 0.75 0.00 0.56
All women should have the right to choose to have an 0.04 −0.03 0.08 0.10 0.04 −0.23 0.03 0.06
abortion if they wish
Having almost any job is better than being unemployed −0.08 0.02 0.09 0.16 0.01 0.05 0.46 0.28
If I did not like a job I’d pack it in, even if there was no 0.06 0.04 −0.02 −0.01 0.02 0.12 −0.63 0.39
other job to go to
Once you have got a job it’s important to hang on to it 0.10 0.04 −0.05 −0.04 0.01 0.0 0.66 0.49
even if you do not really like it
Eigenvalues 1.60 1.51 1.86 1.54 1.31 0.96 1.08
% of variance 7.6 7.2 8.9 7.3 6.2 4.6 5.1
α 0.74 0.75 0.80 0.69 0.66 0.46 0.60

Note. Loadings ≥ |0.30| are in bold.

Based on the theory of motivated cognition (Jost & Najman, 1987). At least two recent studies (Bakker
et al., 2003), we would have expected the anxiety symp- et al., 2020; Osmundsen et al., 2019) also found no
toms to predict more racist attitudes, more authoritarian support for the link between conservatism and threat
leanings and more support for traditional family values, reactivity using psychophysiological measures, which
but these attitudes were not associated with anxiety disor- casts further doubt on the motivated social cognition
der symptoms. Our results are in contrast to the study by explanation of conservatism.
Hatemi et al. (2013) in which clinically assessed social It is also worth noting that there is evidence indicat-
phobia was associated with more negative out-group atti- ing that the association between threat sensitivity and
tudes. Our lack of specific measure of social phobia, or political orientation is a far more complex phenomenon
differences in the measurement of racism and out-group than the theory of motivated cognition suggests. A recent
attitudes, might explain the differing results. However, cross-cultural study by Brandt et al. (2021) found that the
our findings are in line with the few studies that have way feelings of threat affect political orientation might
concluded that anxiety might not lead to more conser- depend on the type of threat and content of political
vative political attitudes (e.g., Huddy et al., 2005; Ray beliefs under study, and there also seems to be significant

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.
6 HELMINEN, ELOVAINIO, JOKELA

TABLE 2 TABLE 4
Mean anxiety disorder score at the age of 44 predicting political Political attitudes at the ages of 42 and 33 predicting anxiety
attitudes at the age of 50 disorders at the age of 44

Anxiety disorders Mean anxiety score


Predicted attitude B SE 𝛽 p B SE β p

Inequality 0.17 0.03 0.08 <.001 Age 42


Distrust in politics 0.09 0.03 0.04 <.001 Inequality 0.04 0.01 0.09 <.001
Racism 0.04 0.02 0.02 .057 Trust in politics 0.00 0.01 −0.01 .601
Authority 0.02 0.02 0.01 .516 Racism −0.02 0.01 −0.03 .036
Environment 0.07 0.02 0.04 <.001 Authority 0.02 0.01 0.04 .014
Family values −0.04 0.03 −0.02 .152 Environment 0.02 0.01 0.04 .002
Work ethic −0.08 0.02 −0.04 <.001 Family values −0.02 0.01 −0.05 <.001
Work ethic 0.00 0.01 0.00 .897
Note: Values are coefficients from seven separate linear regression
Age 33
models, adjusted for gender and education. Values are unstandardised
Inequality 0.03 0.01 0.08 <.001
regression coefficients (B) and standardised regression coefficients (β),
Trust in politics −0.00 0.01 −0.01 .575
SE is the standard error of the unstandardised coefficient.
Racism −0.00 0.01 −0.01 .653
Authority 0.00 0.01 0.00 .888
variation between countries in how these associations Environment 0.02 0.01 0.03 .029
Family values 0.00 0.01 0.00 .995
manifest. In our study, the political attitudes at age 50
Work ethic 0.01 0.01 0.02 .137
were measured in 2008–2009 when economic issues
were very salient in the wake of the global financial crisis Note: Values are coefficients from two separate linear regression models
and economic recession in Great Britain. This might have in which political attitudes were used to predict later anxiety symptoms.
Analysis at age 33 are adjusted for gender and education. Values are
amplified the relevance of perceived economic inequal- unstandardized regression coefficients (B) and standardised regression
ity and would be in line with the findings of Brandt coefficients (β), SE is the standard error of the unstandardized coeffi-
et al. (2021) that economic threats are generally asso- cient.
ciated with more left-leaning attitudes in the economic
domain. Regarding study limitations, the CIS-R queried the
Most notably, in the recent study by Brandt participants about their anxiety symptoms experienced in
et al. (2021), the associations between different types of the past week, which might have weighted the assess-
threats and different political beliefs were not consistent ment of anxiety symptoms towards short-term symptoms
between countries, with the threat being associated with and not long-term dispositions. However, many measures
more right-wing beliefs in some countries and more of mental health that assess symptoms in past weeks or
left-wing beliefs in others. Especially in this light, our months show considerable rank-order stability between
findings might indeed reflect a phenomenon constrained individuals (e.g., GHQ), so clinical measures do not mea-
to a specific country, period of time, or political context, sure only transient psychological states but also more
rather than more generalisable associations between persistent trait-like dispositions. Fourth, the observational
threat sensitivity and political attitudes. This makes our study design does not allow us to make causal conclu-
sample the most pressing limitation of our study, as it sions. Indeed, our findings suggest that the associations
included only a specific birth cohort from Great Britain. between anxiety disorders and political attitudes may be

TABLE 3
Symptoms of generalised anxiety disorder (GAD), panic disorder, and phobia at the age of 44 predicting political attitudes at the age of
50

GAD Panic Phobia


Predicted attitude B SE 𝛽 p B SE 𝛽 p B SE 𝛽 p

Inequality 0.14 0.04 0.04 <.001 0.02 0.08 0.00 .764 0.18 0.05 0.05 <.001
Distrust in politics 0.07 0.04 0.02 .085 0.06 0.08 0.01 .472 0.12 0.05 0.03 .021
Racism 0.04 0.04 0.01 .288 −0.03 0.07 −0.01 .672 0.05 0.04 0.01 .235
Authority 0.01 0.04 0.00 .779 0.05 0.08 0.01 .549 0.03 0.05 0.01 .492
Environment 0.07 0.04 0.02 .041 −0.11 0.07 −0.02 .128 0.11 0.04 0.03 .016
Family values −0.05 0.04 −0.01 .217 0.02 0.09 0.00 .829 −0.08 0.05 −0.02 .133
Work ethic −0.05 0.04 −0.02 .167 −0.08 0.08 −0.01 .303 −0.10 0.05 −0.03 .026

Note: Values are coefficients from seven separate linear regression models, adjusted for gender and education. Anxiety disorder symptoms are coded
0 (not presenting clinical levels of symptoms), 1 (presenting clinical levels of symptoms). Values are unstandardised regression coefficients (B) and
standardised regression coefficients (𝛽), SE is the standard error of the unstandardised coefficient.

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.
ANXIETY DISORDERS PREDICTING ATTITUDES 7

TABLE 5
Political attitudes at the ages of 42 and 33 predicting anxiety disorders at the age of 44

GAD Panic Phobia


OR 95% CI p OR 95% CI p OR 95% CI p

Age 42
Inequality 1.32 [1.13, 1.54] <.001 1.54 [1.12, 2.14] <.001 1.58 [1.31, 1.92] <.001
Trust in politics 0.90 [0.77, 1.05] .179 1.09 [0.81, 1.49] .566 1.06 [0.88, 1.26] .551
Racism 0.87 [0.73, 1.03] .105 0.84 [0.59, 1.18] .319 0.95 [0.78, 1.15] .593
Authority 1.18 [1.01, 1.39] .037 1.46 [1.05, 2.06] .026 1.17 [0.97, 1.43] .099
Environment 1.18 [0.98, 1.41] .073 0.86 [0.60, 1.22] .395 1.8 [0.95, 1.46] .126
Family values 0.80 [0.70, 0.91] <.001 0.74 [0.57, 0.96] .026 0.88 [0.75, 1.02] .097
Work ethic 1.08 [0.94, 1.26] .284 0.80 [0.60, 1.06] .116 0.99 [0.84, 1.18] .935
Age 33
Inequality 1.40 [1.21, 1.63] <.001 1.34 [0.99, 1.84] .061 1.41 [1.18, 1.69] <.001
Trust in politics 0.92 [0.79, 1.08] .328 0.95 [0.69, 1.31] .750 0.98 [0.81, 1.18] .824
Racism 0.93 [0.78, 1.11] .431 1.14 [0.80, 1.59] .449 1.10 [0.90, 1.34] .335
Authority 1.02 [0.87, 1.19] .821 1.19 [0.85, 1.66] .314 0.95 [0.79, 1.15] .616
Environment 1.18 [0.99, 1.40] .073 1.13 [0.79, 1.63] .505 1.00 [0.81, 1.24] 1.00
Family values 0.98 [0.86, 1.13] .808 0.80 [0.60, 1.07] .129 1.07 [0.90, 1.26] .436
Work ethic 1.24 [1.07, 1.44] .005 0.95 [0.71, 1.27] .705 0.93 [0.78, 1.10] .404

Note: Values are odds ratios from six separate logistic regression models in which political attitudes were used to predict later symptoms of GAD,
panic disorder and phobia. Analyses at age 33 were adjusted for gender and education. Anxiety disorder symptoms are coded 0 (not presenting clinical
levels of symptoms), 1 (presenting clinical levels of symptoms).

bidirectional, and at least one previous study (Hatemi previously mentioned, there is also evidence suggesting
et al., 2013) found evidence of a genetic correlation. Thus, that the way feelings of threat affect political orientation
it is yet unclear whether anxiety disorders cause differ- differs significantly across countries. As the majority of
ences in political attitudes, or whether they represent the the previous studies regarding the association between
result of common causes. threat and political orientation have been conducted in
Together the current findings do not provide evidence the United States or western Europe, little is yet known
to support the theory of motivated social cognition (Jost about how the cultural and political context might affect
et al., 2003; Jost et al., 2007) according to which more this phenomenon. Some studies have identified factors
conservative attitudes would reflect heightened threat sen- such as ideological constraint, the economic conditions
sitivity. Clinically assessed symptoms of anxiety disor- and the ideological status quo as important determinants
ders were not associated with conservative political atti- of how threat is associated with political orientation (e.g.,
tudes in the British cohort under examination. Rather, they Malka et al., 2014), but at least one large cross-cultural
were either unrelated to beliefs that correlate with conser- study failed to find significant support for any consistent
vative vs. liberal ideology (e.g., racist and authoritarian effects of country characteristics (Brandt et al., 2021).
attitudes and traditional family values) or they predicted This highlights the urgent need for both, studies using
more liberal or left-leaning attitudes of concerns over eco- more diverse cross-cultural samples, as well as a larger
nomic inequality and preserving the environment, as well and more diverse body of research examining this phe-
as lower work ethic and higher distrust in politics. nomenon in more detail while taking into account the
Our analysis emphasises the need for multiple clin- specific political and cultural settings.
ical and non-clinical measures of threat sensitivity,
and more detailed measures of political attitudes than
Manuscript received March 2021
the conservative–liberal axis, to better evaluate the
Revised manuscript accepted July 2021
psychological underpinnings of political attitudes and
ideologies. Although the association between threat and
political orientation has been examined in a significant REFERENCES
number of studies, there are multiple aspects that have not
Altemeyer, B. (2006). The Authoritarians. http://
been given enough attention. As there is recent evidence theauthoritarians.org/Downloads/TheAuthoritarians.pdf
that the association depends on the type of threat as well Atherton, K., Fuller, E., Shepherd, P., Strachan, D. P., & Power,
as the type of political beliefs (Brandt et al., 2021), future C. (2008). Loss and representativeness in a biomedical sur-
studies could examine if there are differences in how vey at age 45 years: 1958 British birth cohort. Journal of
susceptible individuals are to different types of threat, and Epidemiology & Community Health, 62(3), 216–223. https://
how this in turn might affect their political leanings. As doi.org/10.1136/jech.2006.058966

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.
8 HELMINEN, ELOVAINIO, JOKELA

Bakker, B. N., Schumacher, G., Gothreau, C., & Arceneaux, K. Ogilvie, D. M., Cohen, F., & Solomon, S. (2008). The undesired
(2020). Conservatives and liberals have similar physiological self: Deadly connotations. Journal of Research in Person-
responses to threats. Nature Human Behavior, 4, 613–621. ality, 42(3), 564–576. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.07
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0823-z .012
Brandt, M. J., Turner-Zwinkels, F. M., Karapirinler, B., Van Onraet, E., Van Hiel, A., Dhont, K., & Pattyn, S. (2013). Internal
Leeuwen, F., Bender, M., van Osch, Y., & Adams, B. (2021). and external threat in relationship with right-wing attitudes.
The association between threat and politics depends on the Journal of Personality, 81(3), 233–248. https://doi.org/10
type of threat, the political domain, and the country. Per- .1111/jopy.12011
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 47(2), 324–343. Osmundsen, M., Hendry, D., Laustsen, L., Smith, K., &
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167220946187 Petersen, M. (2019). The psychophysiology of political
Cheng, H., Bynner, J., Wiggins, R., & Schoon, I. (2012). The ideology: Replications, reanalysis and recommendations.
measurement and evaluation of social attitudes in two British https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/49hfg
cohort studies. Social Indicators Research, 107(2), 351–371. Poortinga, W., Spence, A., Whitmarsh, L., Capstick, S., &
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-011-9852-3 Pidgeon, N. F. (2011). Uncertain climate: An investigation
Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual-process cognitive-motivational the- into public scepticism about anthropogenic climate change.
ory of ideology and prejudice. Advances in Experimen- Global Environmental Change, 21(3), 1015–1024. https://
tal Social Psychology, 33, 41–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/ doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2011.03.001
s0065-2601(01)80004-6 Power, C., & Elliott, J. (2006). Cohort profile: 1958 British birth
Duckitt, J., & Fisher, K. (2003). The impact of social threat cohort (National Child Development Study). International
on worldview and ideological attitudes. Political Psychology, Journal of Epidemiology, 35(1), 34–41. https://doi.org/10
24(1), 199–222. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00322 .1093/ije/dyi183
Eaton, N. R., Krueger, R. F., Keyes, K. M., Wall, M., & Grant, Ray, J. J., & Najman, J. M. (1987). Neoconservatism, men-
B. F. (2013). The structure and predictive validity of the tal health and attitude to death. Personality and Individual
internalizing disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Differences, 8(2), 277–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-
122(1), 86–92. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029598 8869(87)90187-5
Golec de Zavala, A., Cisłak, A., & Wesołowska, E. (2010). Polit- Renfro, C. L., Duran, A., Stephan, W. G., & Clason, D. L.
ical conservatism, need for cognitive closure and intergroup (2006). The role of threat in attitudes toward affirmative
hostility. Political Psychology, 31(4), 521–541. https://doi action and its beneficiaries. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00767.x chology, 36(1), 41–74. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0021-9029
Hatemi, P. K., McDermott, R., Eaves, L. J., Kendler, K. S., & .2006.00003.x
Neale, M. C. (2013). Fear as a disposition and an emotional Sylvers, P., Lilienfeld, S. O., & LaPrairie, J. L. (2011). Dif-
state: A genetic and environmental approach to out-group ferences between trait fear and trait anxiety: Implications
political preferences. American Journal of Political Science, for psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1),
57(2), 279–293. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12016 122–137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.004
Huddy, L., Feldman, S., Taber, C., & Lahav, G. (2005). Threat, University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longi-
anxiety, and support of antiterrorism policies. American tudinal Studies. (2008a). National Child Development Study:
Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 593–608. https://doi.org/ Sweep 5, 1991. [data collection] (2nd ed.). City University,
10.1111/j.1540-5907.2005.00144.x Social Statistics Research Unit [original data producer(s)].
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ide- SN: 5567. http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-5567-1
ology: Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longi-
Review of Psychology, 60, 307–337. tudinal Studies. (2008b). National Child Development Study:
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. Sweep 6, 1999–2000. [data collection] (2nd ed.). Joint Centre
(2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. for Longitudinal Research [original data producer(s)]. SN:
Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375. https://doi.org/10 5578. http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-5578-1
.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339 University of London, Institute of Education. Centre for Longi-
Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., tudinal Studies. (2009). National Child Development Study:
Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Are needs to man- Biomedical data, 2002–2004: Special licence access. [data
age uncertainty and threat associated with political conser- collection]. UK Data Service. SN: 5594. https://doi.org/10
vatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psy- .5255/UKDA-SN-5594-2
chology Bulletin, 33(7), 989–1007. https://doi.org/10.1177/ University of London, Institute of Education. Centre for Longi-
0146167207301028 tudinal Studies. (2012). National Child Development Study:
Lewis, G., Pelosi, A. J., Araya, R., & Dunn, G. (1992). Sweep 8, 2008–2009. [data collection] (3rd ed.). UK Data
Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: A stan- Service. SN: 6137. http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-6137-
dardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psycho- 2
logical Medicine, 22(2), 465–486. https://doi.org/10.1017/ University of London. Institute of Education. Centre for Longi-
S0033291700030415 tudinal Studies (2014). NCDS Ethical Review and Consent.
Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Inzlicht, M., & Lelkes, Y. (2014). Do https://cls.ucl.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/NCDS-
needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic Ethical-review-and-Consent-2014.pdf
conservatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal of Person- Vaidyanathan, U., Patrick, C. J., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2009). Link-
ality and Social Psychology, 106(6), 1031–1051. https://doi ing dimensional models of internalizing psychopathology
.org/10.1037/a0036170 to neurobiological systems: Affect-modulated startle as an

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.
ANXIETY DISORDERS PREDICTING ATTITUDES 9

indicator of fear and distress disorders and affiliated traits. prejudice. Psychological Science, 27(2), 169–177. https://
Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 909–942. https://doi.org/10 doi.org/10.1177/0956797615615584
.1037/a0017222 Verkuyten, M. (2009). Support for multiculturalism and minor-
Van de Vyver, J., Houston, D. M., Abrams, D., & Vasiljevic, ity rights: The role of national identification and out-group
M. (2016). Boosting belligerence: How the July 7, 2005, threat. Social Justice Research, 22(1), 31–52. https://doi.org/
London bombings affected Liberals’ moral foundations and 10.1007/s11211-008-0087-7

© 2021 The Authors. International Journal of Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Union of Psychological Science.

You might also like