You are on page 1of 838

Handbook of

Research on Student Engagement


Sandra L. Christenson • Amy L. Reschly
Cathy Wylie
Editors

Handbook of
Research on Student
Engagement
Editors
Sandra L. Christenson Amy L. Reschly
Department of Educational Psychology Department of Educational Psychology
University of Minnesota and Instructional Technology
Minneapolis, MN, USA University of Georgia
Athens, GA, USA
Cathy Wylie
New Zealand Council
for Educational Research
Wellington, New Zealand

ISBN 978-1-4614-2017-0 e-ISBN 978-1-4614-2018-7


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7
Springer New York Dordrecht Heidelberg London

Library of Congress Control Number: 2011944509

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012


All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the
written permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring
Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or
scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval,
electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now
known or hereafter developed is forbidden.
The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if
they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not
they are subject to proprietary rights.

Printed on acid-free paper

Springer is part of Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com)


Preface

Who are engaged students? And why are students engaged? What are the
antecedents and outcomes of engaged students and engaging contexts? How
do engaging contexts (schools, families, peers) affect students and, in turn,
student outcomes? What is the relationship between engagement, learning,
achievement, and other long-term outcomes, such as high school completion
and college attendance? What conditions foster reengagement of students
who are no longer invested in learning or school? Questions such as these
have captured the interest and curiosity of international researchers from a
range of disciplines, including educational psychology, developmental psy-
chology, public health, and teacher education for the past two decades.
Active research on student engagement has occurred primarily in the past
25 years, advancing with an article in 1985 by Mosher and McGowan. There
are questions and unresolved issues related to engagement, which this vol-
ume explores; however, there is also general consensus regarding a number of
facets of engagement theory and research, such as:
• Student engagement is considered the primary theoretical model for under-
standing dropout and promoting school completion, defined as graduation
from high school with sufficient academic and social skills to partake in
postsecondary educational options and/or the world of work (Christenson
et al., 2008; Finn, 2006; Reschly & Christenson, 2006b).
• Engaged students do more than attend or perform academically; they also
put forth effort, persist, self-regulate their behavior toward goals, chal-
lenge themselves to exceed, and enjoy challenges and learning (Klem &
Connell, 2004; National Research Council and the Institute of Medicine
[NRC and IoM], 2004).
• Student engagement, irrespective of the specificity of its definition, is gen-
erally associated positively with desired academic, social, and emotional
learning outcomes (Klem & Connell, 2004).
• Engagement is a multidimensional construct – one that requires an under-
standing of affective connections within the academic environment (e.g.,
positive adult-student and peer relationships) and active student behavior
(e.g., attendance, participation, effort, prosocial behavior) (Appleton,
Christenson, & Furlong, 2008; Newmann, Wehlage, & Lamborn, 1992).
• The role of context cannot be ignored. Engagement is not conceptualized
as an attribute of the student but rather as an alterable state of being that is
highly influenced by the capacity of school, family, and peers to provide

v
vi Preface

consistent expectations and supports for learning (Reschly & Christenson,


2006a, 2006b). Engagement is an active image (Wylie, 2009) depicting
effortful learning through interaction with the teacher and the classroom
learning opportunities. In short, both the individual and context matter.
• Student engagement reinforces the notion that effective instruction explic-
itly considers and programs for the role of student motivation on learning
outcomes (NRC and IoM, 2004; Russell, Ainley, & Frydenberg, 2005).
• The increase in student engagement measures with adequate psychometric
properties has cemented the power and value of student engagement as a
useful variable for data-driven decision-making efforts in schools
(Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, 2006; Betts, Appleton, Reschly,
Christenson, & Huebner, 2010; Darr, 2009; Fredricks et al., 2011).
• There is an emerging intervention database that suggests evidence-based
or promising strategies for educators to employ to enhance student engage-
ment (Christenson et al., 2008).
This volume seeks to address a number of the “unknowns” that character-
ize theory and research on student engagement. These unknowns, or in some
cases controversies in the field, affect the advancement of research on student
engagement and, consequently, our knowledge base for improving student
learning outcomes. We offer the following:
• Some researchers consider student engagement a “metaconstruct” or an
organizing framework – one that integrates such areas as belonging,
behavioral participation, motivation, self-efficacy, school connectedness,
and so forth (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004), while others disagree,
believing that engagement must have clearly defined boundaries (Finn &
Kasza, 2009).
• Although researchers have reached consensus that student engagement is
multidimensional, agreement on the multidimensionality differs from
agreement on the number and types of engagement dimensions, which
ranges from two to four. It may be that consensus only will be achieved
with respect that student engagement is multidimensional, and, if so,
researchers will need to define clearly their conceptualization in each
study.
• Other methodological considerations (e.g., selection of informants, valid-
ity of self-report, common agreement of items within dimensions, devel-
opment of instruments with strong psychometric properties) must be
addressed if the construct and application of student engagement to prac-
tice will be advanced.
• The relationship between and/or differentiation of engagement and moti-
vation is subject to debate (Appleton et al., 2006, 2008). What is the rela-
tionship between these two constructs? Are motivation and engagement
separate? Can one be motivated but not actively engaged in a task or goal
accomplishment?
Recently, there has been a proliferation of definitions of student engage-
ment. Definitions of the terms of engagement, student engagement, school
engagement, engagement in schoolwork, and academic engagement have
Preface vii

been offered. These conceptualizations vary further along a number of other


dimensions, such as participation, behavior, action, emotion, investment,
motivation, and so on (see Appleton et al., 2008). Some studies have consid-
ered engagement as a process, while others conceptualize it as an outcome
(Appleton et al., 2008; Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, & Kinderman, 2008). We
contend that establishing construct validity for student engagement requires
common agreement regarding what comprises the engagement construct – or
what engagement is and what it is not. It demands an understanding of
whether engagement is the outcome, a process to other desired outcomes, or
plays a dual role. The constancy of the construct across researchers – in con-
ceptualization and measurement – is a worthy endeavor, one with practical,
scientific, and policy implications.
To date, conceptual clarity and methodological rigor (e.g., use of psycho-
metrically sound measures) have not been achieved; they are considered a
prerequisite to advance the emerging construct of student engagement and its
usefulness in interventions and school programs. A particular concern
addressed in this volume is the apparent overlap and confusion of engage-
ment with motivation-to-learn variables. We designed this handbook as a way
to create a dialogue among engagement and motivational researchers. To do
so, we invited authors to cover their research topic and to respond to the fol-
lowing questions:
• What is your definition of engagement and motivation? How do you dif-
ferentiate the two?
• What overarching framework or theory do you use to study/explain
engagement or motivation?
• What is the role of context in explaining engagement or motivation?
• Focusing on the emerging construct of student engagement, what are nec-
essary advances in theory, research, and practice to propel this construct
forward?
The 34 chapters were placed into one of these 5 parts: (1) What Is
Student Engagement? (2) Engagement as Linked to Motivational Variables,
(3) Engagement and Contextual Influences, (4) Student Engagement:
Determinants and Student Outcomes, and (5) Measurement Issues,
Instruments, and Approaches. We also solicited an expert commentary for
each of the above parts, for a total of 39 chapters. As coeditors, we are
grateful to both the chapter and commentary authors.
Engagement is thought to be especially important for apathetic and dis-
couraged learners (Brophy, 2004) and those at high risk for dropping out, but
the primary appeal of the engagement construct is that it is relevant for all
students. The universal appeal of engagement is underscored by high school
reform efforts that explicitly address students’ motivation to learn and engage-
ment with school (NRC and IoM, 2004). Thus, student engagement underlies
school reform – or what we seek to engender for all students through the
school environment, teaching, and coursework. In addition, indicators of
engagement may be used for screening and early detection of disengagement;
these indicators provide links to intervention targets to reengage students at
school and with learning.
viii Preface

Establishing construct validity for student engagement is integral to its


utility in classrooms and the value of future scientific studies. The authors in
this volume provided definitions for student engagement, offered their per-
spective on engagement and motivation, underscored the role of contextual
influences, and proposed a range of future research directions. It is our hope,
as coeditors, that this comprehensive volume stimulates the quality of student
engagement research and advances the field. Let the dialogue begin.

Minneapolis, MN, USA Sandra L. Christenson


Athens, GA, USA Amy L. Reschly
Wellington, New Zealand Cathy Wylie

References
Appleton, J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. (2006). Measuring cognitive and
psychological engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement Instrument. Journal
of School Psychology, 44, 427–445.
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. (2008). Student engagement with
school: Critical conceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology in
the Schools, 45, 369–386.
Betts, J., Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., Christenson, S. L., & Huebner, E. S. (2010). A
study of the reliability and construct validity of the Student Engagement Instrument
across multiple grades. School Psychology Quarterly, 25, 84–93.
Brophy, J. (2004). Motivating students to learn (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., Appleton, J. J., Berman, S., Spanjers, D., & Varro, P.
(2008). Best practices in fostering student engagement. In A. Thomas, & J. Grimes
(Eds.), Best practices in school psychology (5th ed.). Bethesda, MD: National
Association of School Psychologists.
Darr, C. (2009). The me and my school survey. In J. Morton (Ed.), Engaging young people
in learning: Why does it matter and what can we do?: Conference proceedings (pp.
85–100). Wellington, New Zealand: New Zealand Council for Educational Research.
Finn, J. D. (2006). The adult lives of at-risk students: The roles of attainment and engage-
ment in high school (NCES 2006–328). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education,
National Center for Education Statistics.
Finn, J. D., & Kasza, K. A. (2009). Disengagement from School. In Engaging young peo-
ple in learning: Why does it matter and what can we do? (pp. 4–35). Wellington, New
Zealand: New Zealand Council for Educational Research.
Fredricks, J., McColskey, W., Meli, J., Mordica, J., Montrosse, B., & Mooney, K. (2011).
Measuring student engagement in upper elementary through high school: A description
of 21 instruments (Issues & Answers Report, REL 2011–No. 098). Washington, DC:
U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for
Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Regional Educational Laboratory
Southeast. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/edlabs
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of
the concept, state of the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59–109.
Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships matter: Linking teacher support to
student engagement and achievement. Journal of School Health, 74(7), 262–273.
Mosher, R., & McGowan, B. (1985). Assessing student engagement in secondary schools:
Alternative conceptions, strategies of assessing, and instruments. University of
Wisconsin, Research and Development Center (ERIC Document Reproduction Service
No. ED 272812).
National Research Council and the Institute of Medicine [NRC and IoM] (2004). Engaging
schools: Fostering high school students’ motivation to learn. Washington, DC: The
National Academies Press.
Preface ix

Newmann, F. M., Wehlage, G. G., & Lamborn, S. D. (1992). The significance and sources
of student engagement. In F. M. Newmann (Ed.), Student engagement and achievement
in American secondary schools (pp. 11–39). New York: Teachers College Press.
Reschly, A., & Christenson, S. L. (2006a). Prediction of dropout among students with mild
disabilities: A case for the inclusion of student engagement variables. Remedial and
Special Education, 27, 276–292.
Reschly, A., & Christenson, S. L. (2006b). Promoting school completion. In G. Bear, & K.
Minke (Eds.), Children’s needs III: Understanding and addressing the developmental
needs of children. Bethesda, MD: National Association of School Psychologists.
Russell, V. J., Ainley, M., & Frydenberg, E. (2005). Schooling issues digest: Student moti-
vation and engagement. Retrieved November 9, 2005, from http://www.dest.gov.au/
sectors/school education/publications resources/schooling issues digest/schooling
issues digest motivation engagement.htm
Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kinderman, T. (2008). Engagement and disaffec-
tion in the classroom: Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of Educational
Psychology, 100, 765–781.
Wylie, C. (2009). Introduction. J. Morton (Ed.), Engaging young people in learning: Why
does it matter and what can we do?: Conference proceedings (pp. 1–3). Wellington,
New Zealand: New Zealand Council for Educational Research.
About the Editors

Sandra L. Christenson is the Birkmaier professor of Educational Leadership,


professor of Educational Psychology, and faculty member in the School
Psychology Program at the University of Minnesota. Her research focuses on
interventions that enhance engagement at school and with learning for mar-
ginalized students with and without disabilities. She is particularly interested
in the identification of contextual factors that facilitate student engagement
and increase the probability for student success in school and the identifica-
tion of the effect of family-school partnership variables.
Amy L. Reschly is an associate professor and training director in the School
Psychology Program, Department of Educational Psychology & Instructional
Technology, at the University of Georgia. Her areas of expertise include
engagement and dropout prevention, working with families and schools to
promote student success, and curriculum-based measurement (CBM) and
problem solving.
Cathy Wylie is a chief researcher with the New Zealand Council for
Educational Research. She has led the longitudinal Competent Learners proj-
ect since 1993. Her research interests include the interaction between experi-
ences and student development and identifying policies that best support
school capability to provide engaging and productive learning. Her research
into policy includes the impact on schools and students of New Zealand
national policies, including the shift to self-managed schools since 1989.

xi
Contents

Part I What Is Student Engagement?

1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness:


Evolution and Future Directions
of the Engagement Construct .................................................... 3
Amy L. Reschly and Sandra L. Christenson
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement,
Coping, and Everyday Resilience .............................................. 21
Ellen A. Skinner and Jennifer R. Pitzer
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods ............................ 45
Duhita Mahatmya, Brenda J. Lohman,
Jennifer L. Matjasko, and Amy Feldman Farb
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement........................................... 65
Gary E. Bingham and Lynn Okagaki
5 Student Engagement: What Is It?
Why Does It Matter? .................................................................. 97
Jeremy D. Finn and Kayla S. Zimmer
6 Part I Commentary: So What Is Student
Engagement Anyway? ................................................................ 133
Jacquelynne Eccles and Ming-Te Wang

Part II Engagement as Linked to Motivational Variables

7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective


on Student Engagement.............................................................. 149
Johnmarshall Reeve
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization
of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate ....................... 173
Eric M. Anderman and Helen Patrick
9 School Identification ................................................................... 193
Kristin E. Voelkl

xiii
xiv Contents

10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner ........................................ 219


Dale H. Schunk and Carol A. Mullen
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student
Engagement: Theoretical Foundations
and Applications.......................................................................... 237
Timothy J. Cleary and Barry J. Zimmerman
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement ........................ 259
Reinhard Pekrun and Lisa Linnenbrink-Garcia
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement
in Classroom Activities ............................................................... 283
Mary Ainley
14 Part II Commentary: Motivation and Engagement:
Conceptual, Operational, and Empirical Clarity..................... 303
Andrew J. Martin

Part III Engagement and Contextual Influences

15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation


and Student Engagement ........................................................... 315
Janine Bempechat and David J. Shernoff
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement:
Toward a Home-School Partnership Model ............................. 343
Jacquelyn N. Raftery, Wendy S. Grolnick,
and Elizabeth S. Flamm
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement:
Conceptualizing, Measuring, and Improving
the Capacity of Classroom Interactions .................................... 365
Robert C. Pianta, Bridget K. Hamre, and Joseph P. Allen
18 The Role of Peer Relationships in Student Academic
and Extracurricular Engagement ............................................. 387
Jaana Juvonen, Guadalupe Espinoza, and Casey Knifsend
19 Understanding Student Engagement
with a Contextual Model ............................................................ 403
Shui-fong Lam, Bernard P.H. Wong,
Hongfei Yang, and Yi Liu
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass:
Educational Practices Supporting Students’ Need
for Autonomy ............................................................................... 421
Avi Assor
21 The Engaging Nature of Teaching
for Competency Development .................................................... 441
Rosemary Hipkins
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement .................. 457
Sharon L. Nichols and Heather S. Dawson
Contents xv

23 Part III Commentary: Socio-Cultural Contexts,


Social Competence, and Engagement at School ....................... 479
Kathryn Wentzel

Part IV Student Engagement: Determinants and Student Outcomes

24 The Relationship Between Engagement


and High School Dropout ........................................................... 491
Russell W. Rumberger and Susan Rotermund
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement........................ 515
Marcia H. Davis and James M. McPartland
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement,
Motivation, and Resilience in Students ..................................... 541
Robert Brooks, Suzanne Brooks, and Sam Goldstein
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement
with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors ................................. 563
Amy-Jane Griffiths, Elena Lilles, Michael J. Furlong,
and Jennifer Sidhwa
28 Trajectories and Patterns of Student Engagement:
Evidence from a Longitudinal Study ........................................ 585
Cathy Wylie and Edith Hodgen
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement
and Achievement in Reading ..................................................... 601
John T. Guthrie, Allan Wigfield, and Wei You
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective
on Student Engagement.............................................................. 635
Christopher A. Wolters and Daniel J. Taylor
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic
Engaged Time .............................................................................. 653
Maribeth Gettinger and Martha J. Walter
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System:
Identity, Learning Power and Authentic Enquiry ................... 675
Ruth Deakin Crick
33 Part IV Commentary: Outcomes of Engagement
and Engagement as an Outcome: Some Consensus,
Divergences, and Unanswered Questions ................................. 695
Michel Janosz

Part V Measurement Issues, Instruments, and Approaches

34 Measuring Student Engagement: The Development


of a Scale for Formative Use....................................................... 707
Charles W. Darr
xvi Contents

35 Systems Consultation: Developing


the Assessment-to-Intervention Link
with the Student Engagement Instrument................................ 725
James J. Appleton
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data:
Understanding, Measuring, and Strengthening
Student Engagement ................................................................... 743
Ethan Yazzie-Mintz and Kim McCormick
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement:
A Comparative Analysis of Various Methods
and Student Self-report Instruments ........................................ 763
Jennifer A. Fredricks and Wendy McColskey
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student
Engagement: Advancing the Construct Through
Statistical Modeling .................................................................... 783
Joseph Betts
39 Part V Commentary: Possible New Directions
in the Measurement of Student Engagement ........................... 805
Karen M. Samuelsen

Epilogue ............................................................................................... 813

Index ..................................................................................................... 819


Contributors

Mary Ainley is an associate professor in Psychological Sciences at the


University of Melbourne, Australia. Her experience and research interests are
in developmental and educational psychology. Most recently, these interests
have been applied to investigating the experience of interest and exploring the
psychological processes that are involved when students engage with (and
disengage from) achievement tasks. This has resulted in the development of
software for tracking students’ choices and affective responses as they are
tackling specific reading and problem-solving tasks. The goal of this research
is to understand how to support positive educational experiences for students
of all ages.
Joseph P. Allen is professor of psychology at the University of Virginia and
director of the clinical psychology doctoral program in the Department of
Psychology. His interests include adolescent and young adult social develop-
ment and interventions to enhance behavioral and academic outcomes for
adolescents.
Eric M. Anderman is a professor in the School of Educational Policy and
Leadership at the Ohio State University. His research examines academic
motivation. His studies have focused specifically on school transitions, pre-
vention of risky behaviors, and academic cheating.
James J. Appleton is employed within the Research and Evaluation Office of
Gwinnett County Public Schools, GA (a district of around 160,000 students).
His publications include the area of student engagement with school, a pathway
for dropping out, and he presents and consults nationally on this topic. Dr.
Appleton has research and evaluation experience in district advisement and
graduation coach programs, school initiatives, accountability metrics, and
nested data. He has work experience in research and consultation within large
urban and small rural school districts. Dr. Appleton has also served as mentor
and researcher within the Check & Connect school completion intervention. He
has codeveloped the Student Engagement Instrument (SEI).
Avi Assor is professor and former head of the Educational and School
Psychology Program in Ben Gurion University, Israel. His research focuses
on processes affecting children’s autonomous internalization of values and
practices that affect students’ and teachers’ motivation. In addition, he is
involved in the development and assessment of school reforms aimed at

xvii
xviii Contributors

enhancing students’ and teachers’ basic psychological needs, intrinsic


motivation, and caring for others. He has published in the Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, Child Development, the Journal of
Educational Psychology, the Journal of Personality, the Journal of Educational
Administration, and Learning and Instruction.
Janine Bempechat is an associate professor in the Department of Human
Development at Wheelock College. Her research interests include the achieve-
ment motivation of low-income students, school and family influences in
achievement motivation, and ethnic and cultural influences in the develop-
ment of achievement beliefs.
Joseph Betts is the director of research and principal research scientist at
Riverside Publishing. He also serves as the senior research consultant for the
Center for Cultural Diversity & Minority Education. Joe has participated in
the development of computerized adaptive tests and worked as a practicing
psychologist. In addition to student engagement, his research interests involve
the applications of latent variable models to various educational research and
assessment issues. The study of children’s early literacy and mathematics
developmental trajectories has been an area of research for a number of years,
and he has recently developed growth norms for use in progress monitoring
in the areas of reading, mathematics, and early literacy.
Gary E. Bingham is an assistant professor in the Department of Early
Childhood Education in the College of Education at Georgia State University.
He received his Ph.D. from Purdue University with an emphasis in early
childhood education and child development. His research examines how chil-
dren’s early experiences within multiple contexts (i.e., home environment
and early childhood educational settings) impact their language, literacy, and
social development. As a result of this focus, much of his research seeks to
understand the connection of culture to family and educational processes,
particularly in relation to the engagement and achievement patterns of ethni-
cally and economically diverse children and youth.
Robert Brooks is an assistant clinical professor of psychology at Harvard
Medical School and former director of the Department of Psychology at
McLean Hospital. He is the author or coauthor of 14 books and dozens of
chapters and peer-reviewed articles. His interests pertain to topics of resil-
ience, motivation, school climate, and the mindset and strategies of effective
students, educators, and other professionals.
Suzanne Brooks is a clinical and school psychologist. In addition to working
as a school psychologist in the Weston, MA Public Schools, she also has a
part-time private practice in which she sees children, adolescents, and fami-
lies. She has a special interest in assessing and treating students with learning
and behavior problems and collaborating with teachers to develop effective
classroom interventions for these youngsters.
Timothy J. Cleary is an associate professor in the Department of Educational
Psychology and the training director of the School Psychology Program at the
University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee. His research interests include developing
Contributors xix

alternative assessment tools targeting student motivation and self-regulation,


developing and evaluating efficacy of self-regulation interventions in math and
science, and examining trends in school psychology and education.
Ruth Deakin Crick is a reader in education at the Graduate School of
Education in the University of Bristol and conjoint professor of education at
the University of Newcastle in Australia. Her research interests include the
theory and practice of learning power, pedagogies for deep engagement, sys-
tems learning and leadership, and approaches to engagement and social enter-
prise which integrate research, policy, practice, and enterprise.
Charles W. Darr is a senior researcher at the New Zealand Council for
Educational Research and manager of the Assessment Design and Reporting
Team. Charles’ research interests include the measurement of student engage-
ment, the development of assessment tools across the curriculum, and the
application of computer technologies to administer assessments, and report
and analyze assessment data.
Marcia H. Davis is an associate research scientist at the Center for Social
Organization of Schools, Johns Hopkins University. She holds a doctorate in
educational psychology from the University of Maryland and a master’s
degree in educational statistics. Her research interests include reading com-
prehension measurement, reading motivation, and school engagement as it
relates to dropout prevention.
Heather S. Dawson is a Ph.D. candidate in the Department of Educational
Policy and Leadership at the Ohio State University. Her research interests
include examining the relationship between education policy and teacher and
student motivation.
Dr. Jacquelynne Eccles (McKeachie/Pintrich Distinguished University pro-
fessor of psychology at the University of Michigan) received her Ph.D. from
UCLA in 1973. She was chair of the MacArthur Foundation Network on
Successful Pathways through Middle Childhood, chair of the NAS Committee
on After School Programs for Youth, and president of both the Society for
Research on Adolescence and Division 35 of APA. She is currently editor of
Developmental Psychology and is past editor of the Journal of Research on
Adolescence. Her awards include the Spencer Foundation Fellowship for
Outstanding Young Scholar in Educational Research, the Sarah Goddard
Power Award for Outstanding Service from the University of Michigan, the
APS Cattell Fellows Award for Outstanding Applied Work in Psychology,
SPSSI’s Kurt Lewin Award for outstanding research, the Distinguished
Career Awards from SRA and Division 15 APA, and the University of
Michigan Faculty Recognition Award for Outstanding Scholarship. Her
research ranges from gender-role socialization, teacher expectancies, and
classroom influences on student motivation to social development in the fam-
ily and school context.
Guadalupe Espinoza is doctoral student in developmental psychology at the
University of California, Los Angeles. Her research interests include examin-
ing how social contexts impact school functioning and peer relationships (e.g.,
xx Contributors

bullying) within the school and online context, particularly among Latino
adolescents.
Amy Feldman Farb is an evaluation specialist with the Office of Adolescent
Health in the US Department of Health and Human Services. She manages
federal evaluations of teen pregnancy prevention programs and development
of performance measures and participates in an ongoing evidence review.
Prior to this position, Dr. Farb was a research scientist with the US Department
of Education, where she managed a contract conducting methodological
investigations to advance the field of education research and a technical peer
review process for the Regional Educational Laboratory program.
Jeremy D. Finn is professor of education (quantitative methods) at the State
University of New York at Buffalo. His research interests include issues of
educational equity that are affected by school policies and practices: class
size reduction, educational risk and resilience, gender differences in behavior
and performance, and student disengagement and dropping out. He has con-
ducted a number of long-term studies of student attitudes and performance.
Elizabeth S. Flamm is a doctoral student in the Clark University Clinical
Psychology Program. Her research explores the social and environmental
contexts that facilitate motivation, achievement, and well-being in children
and adolescents.
Jennifer A. Fredricks is an associate professor of Human Development at
Connecticut College where she teaches courses in adolescence and child and
family policy. Her research interests include school engagement, motivation,
organized activity participation, youth sports, and adolescent development.
Michael J. Furlong is a professor in the Counseling, Clinical, and School
Psychology Department at the University of California, Santa Barbara. He is
the director of the Center for School-Based Youth Development and the edi-
tor of the Journal of School Violence.
Maribeth Gettinger is a professor in the School Psychology Program,
Department of Educational Psychology, at the University of Wisconsin,
Madison. Her research focuses on classroom environments that support aca-
demic success and evidence-based practices to promote skill development
among high-risk children.
Sam Goldstein is an assistant clinical professor at the University of Utah
School of Medicine. He is the clinical director of the Neurology, Learning
and Behavior Center in Salt Lake City, Utah. Dr. Goldstein has authored 32
books and dozens of chapters and peer-reviewed research articles. He also
serves as editor in chief of the Journal of Attention Disorders and coeditor in
chief of Encyclopedia of Child Development.
Amy-Jane Griffiths is the assistant director at The Help Group’s residential
treatment center for adolescents with social, emotional, and behavioral chal-
lenges. Her research interests include empirically supported interventions for
children engaging in high-risk behaviors, system level change, and positive
school adaptation.
Contributors xxi

Wendy S. Grolnick is a professor of psychology in the Frances L. Hiatt


School of Psychology at Clark University. Her research, which has been sup-
ported by NIMH, The William T. Grant Foundation, and the Spencer
Foundation, has focused on the effects of home and school environments on
children’s motivation as well as factors affecting the environments that par-
ents and teachers create for their children.
John T. Guthrie is the Jean Mullan Professor of Literacy Emeritus in the
Department of Human Development at the University of Maryland. He cur-
rently directs an NICHD grant on adolescent motivation and achievement in
reading, with an emphasis on ethnic variations. The project includes a reading
intervention for all seventh-grade students in one school district.
Bridget K. Hamre is associate director of the Center for Advanced Study of
Teaching and Learning at the University of Virginia. Her areas of expertise
include large-scale assessment of teacher-student relationships and classroom
processes that promote positive academic and social development for chil-
dren and adolescents.
Rosemary Hipkins is a chief researcher at the New Zealand Council for
Educational Research. She is interested in educating for competencies and
the challenges of transforming curriculum, teaching, and assessment prac-
tices to meet “twenty-first century” learning needs. Her expertise is in high
school education and science education.
Edith Hodgen is the chief statistician at the New Zealand Council for
Educational Research, undertaking the analysis for the longitudinal Competent
Learners project and for a range of research and evaluation studies, with a
focus on analyzing changes over time.
Michel Janosz is director of the School Environment Research Group and
professor at the School of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal. His
research interests concern the etiologies and prevention of school dropout
and violence and the specific role of school environments. His current work
focuses upon (1) the efficacy of school-level interventions and the effective-
ness of large-scale government initiatives to improve school success in disad-
vantaged populations, (2) the relationship between school climate and
practices and student adjustment, and (3) the bidirectional links between
internalizing/externalizing problems and school adjustment.
Jaana Juvonen is a professor in the Department of Psychology at the
University of California, Los Angeles. She conducts research on school-
based peer relationships and school adjustment. Much of her work focuses on
bullying in middle schools.
Casey Knifsend is a doctoral student in Developmental Psychology at the
University of California, Los Angeles. Her research interests include examin-
ing social identity development through ascribed (e.g., ethnicity) and achieved
(e.g., extracurricular activities) group memberships and, specifically, how
multiple social identities contribute to adolescent adjustment and intergroup
attitudes.
xxii Contributors

Shui-fong Lam is an associate professor in the Department of Psychology at


the University of Hong Kong. She is the director of a professional training
program for educational psychologists. Her research interests include achieve-
ment motivation, parenting, school engagement, instructional strategies, and
school-based prevention programs.
Elena Lilles is a doctoral candidate student at the University of California,
Santa Barbara. Her research interests include early intervention with children
ages 0–5 to promote academic readiness, social-emotional well-being, and
school success.
Lisa Linnenbrink-Garcia is an assistant professor of Developmental
Psychology in the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, with a sec-
ondary appointment in Education, at Duke University. Her research focuses
on the development of achievement motivation in school settings and the
interplay among achievement motivation, emotions, and learning, especially
in the domains of science and mathematics.
Yi Liu is an associate researcher in the Urban Community and Mental Health
Education Office at the Yunnan Health Education Institute. She is also a
social worker providing services to students and patients. Her research inter-
ests include student mental health education in impoverished minority areas,
tobacco control, and quitting smoking.
Brenda J. Lohman is the director of Graduate Education and an associate
professor in the Department of Human Development and Family Studies at
Iowa State University. Utilizing a multidisciplinary framework, her research
interests focus on the successful academic, psychological, social, and sexual
adjustment of adolescents, especially those from economically disadvantaged
minority families and communities.
Duhita Mahatmya is an assistant professor in the New Century College at
George Mason University. Anchored in the ecological theory of human devel-
opment and a positive youth development framework, her research empha-
sizes the multicontextual effects of the family, school, and neighborhood on
adolescent engagement and well-being.
Andrew J. Martin is a professorial research fellow in the Faculty of Education
and Social Work at the University of Sydney. His research interests include
motivation, engagement, achievement, and quantitative research methods.
Jennifer L. Matjasko is a an adjunct assistant professor at the University of
Texas at Austin and a behavioral scientist in the Division of Violence
Prevention at the National Center for Injury Prevention and Control located
at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. She received her Ph.D. in
Public Policy from the University of Chicago. Her research interests focus on
the development of at-risk adolescents and the factors that promote their
health and well-being. Her research emphasizes the use of ecological, life-
course, and person-centered approaches in understanding the relationship
between individual, family, school, and community factors and adolescent
functioning in order to inform prevention, intervention, and policy efforts
targeted toward at-risk youth.
Contributors xxiii

Wendy McColskey is a program director with the SERVE Center at the


University of North Carolina at Greensboro. Her research interests include
student engagement and motivation, formative and classroom assessment
processes, and use of research and evaluation by educators.
Kim McCormick is a doctoral student in the Department of Counseling and
Educational Psychology at the Indiana University School of Education in
Bloomington, Indiana. Her research interests include student engagement
and gifted and talented education.
James M. McPartland is research professor of sociology and codirector of
the Center for Social Organization of Schools, Johns Hopkins University. His
research specialties include high school reform and scale-up processes,
effects of school formal and informal organization on student outcomes, and
adolescent literacy innovations and impacts.
Carol A. Mullen is professor and chair of the Department of Educational
Leadership and Cultural Foundations at the University of North Carolina at
Greensboro. Her research interests include mentoring, leadership, and social
justice in higher education and K–12 settings.
Sharon L. Nichols is an associate professor in the Department of Educational
Psychology at the University of Texas at San Antonio. Her research interests
include the intended and unintended consequences of high-stakes testing
accountability on teachers and teaching and the ways in which test-related
practices connect to students’ motivational development.
Lynn Okagaki is Dean of the College of Education and Human Development
at the University of Delaware. Her research focuses on academic achieve-
ment as influenced by culture and family values. From 2002–2005, she served
as the first Deputy Director for Science in the U.S. Department of Education’s
Institute of Education Sciences. In December 2005, she was appointed for a
six-year term as Commissioner for Education Research at the Institute of
Education Sciences. Prior to 2002, she held appointments at Purdue University,
Yale University, and the University of Houston. Dr. Okagaki received her
bachelor of science degree in applied behavioral sciences from the University
of California Davis and her doctoral degree in developmental psychology
from Cornell University.
Helen Patrick is a professor in the Department of Educational Studies at
Purdue University. Her research interests involve student motivation, includ-
ing classroom processes and contexts that support motivation and
engagement.
Reinhard Pekrun holds the chair for Personality and Educational Psychology
in the Department of Psychology at the University of Munich. His research
interests pertain to achievement emotion and motivation, students’ person-
ality development, and educational assessment and evaluation, including
international large-scale assessments of student achievement.
Robert C. Pianta is Novartis US Foundation professor of education and dean
of the Curry School of Education at the University of Virginia. His interests
xxiv Contributors

include theory, assessment, and intervention focused on teacher-student inter-


actions and relationships, as well as policies that support increased attention
to interactions in the classroom.
Jennifer R. Pitzer is a graduate student in the Department of Psychology at
Portland State University. Her research interests include the influence of
teacher support (e.g., warmth, structure, and autonomy support) on student
reengagement, everyday resilience, and coping.
Jacquelyn N. Raftery is a graduate student in the Department of Psychology
at Clark University. Her research interests include the development of coping
with a special focus on how social contexts facilitate more adaptive coping.
Johnmarshall Reeve is a WCU (World Class University) professor in the
Department of Education at Korea University in Seoul, Korea. Professor
Reeve’s research interests center on the empirical study of all aspects of human
motivation and emotion, though he particularly emphasizes student motiva-
tion, student engagement, and teachers’ motivating styles toward students.
Susan Rotermund is a research associate at MPR Associates in Berkeley,
CA. Her research interests include student engagement, school climate, and
how these factors can be altered to reduce dropouts. She uses a variety of
quantitative methodologies for her research, including factor analysis, struc-
tural equation modeling, and latent class analysis.
Russell W. Rumberger is the vice provost for Education Partnerships,
University of California Office of the President, and professor of Education in
the Gevirtz Graduate School of Education at UC Santa Barbara. His research
interests include education and work; the schooling of disadvantaged students,
particularly school dropouts and linguistic minority students; school effective-
ness; and education policy. He recently completed a book on high school drop-
outs published by Harvard University Press in 2011.
Karen M. Samuelsen is an assistant professor in the Department of
Educational Psychology and Instructional Technology at the University of
Georgia. She holds a doctorate in measurement, statistics, and evaluation
from the University of Maryland. Dr. Samuelsen currently teaches graduate
courses in measurement and statistics and serves as the director on a federally
funded research project examining the efficacy of a conversational pedagogy
for English language learners. Her research interests are in the fields of dif-
ferential item functioning, latent mixture models, and validity. Before getting
her Ph.D., Dr. Samuelsen was a high school science teacher with a certifica-
tion in physics.
Dale H. Schunk is dean in the School of Education and professor of Teacher
Education and Higher Education at the University of North Carolina at
Greensboro. His research interests include social cognitive learning, moti-
vation, and self-regulation.
David J. Shernoff is an associate professor in the Department of Leadership,
Educational Psychology, and Foundations at Northern Illinois University. His
Contributors xxv

research interests include the motivation and engagement of adolescents in


school and after-school contexts, mentoring, early career development, and
positive psychology.
Jennifer Sidhwa is a graduate student in the Educational Leadership and
Organizations program at the Gevirtz Graduate School of Education,
University of California, Santa Barbara. Her research interests focus on factors
that promote positive organizational climate.
Ellen A. Skinner is a professor of developmental science and education in
the Department of Psychology at Portland State University. Her research
focuses on the dynamics of motivational development and resilience during
childhood and adolescence and examines how school contexts and students’
self-system processes shape the development of constructive engagement,
coping, and (eventually) the emergence of a sense of purpose and ownership
for one’s own progress in school and beyond. In the last several years, she has
become very interested in the development of teacher engagement and
coping.
Daniel J. Taylor is a doctoral student in the Department of Educational
Psychology at the University of Houston. His research interests include stu-
dent motivation and academic achievement. His most recent empirical work
examines motivational factors involved in the effects of stereotype threat in
college populations.
Kristin E. Voelkl is an associate professor in the Department of Adolescence
Education at Canisius College in Buffalo, New York. Her research interests
include students’ attitudes toward school and adolescent misbehavior includ-
ing school-related substance use, academic dishonesty, and aggression in
school. She continues to research engagement in school as it relates to other
forms of risky and health-related behavior.
Martha J. Walter is a graduate student in the Department of Educational
Psychology at the University of Wisconsin, Madison. Her current research
interests include response to intervention, universal screening, and the social
and emotional development of preschool children.
Ming-Te Wang is a research scientist in the Institute for Social Research at the
University of Michigan and assistant professor of Applied Developmental
Psychology at the University of Pittsburgh. He received his doctoral degree in
Human Development and Psychology from Harvard University. His research has
focused on the impact of school/classroom climate on adolescents’ motivational
beliefs and engagement and the effects of multiple ecological systems on the
behavioral, social, and emotional development of youth from diverse socioeco-
nomic and cultural backgrounds. His work emphasizes the interplay of develop-
mental processes across both academic and social domains in adolescence and
situates these processes within family, school, and community contexts.
Kathryn Wentzel is a professor in the Department of Human Development at
the University of Maryland, College Park. Her research interests focus on par-
ents, peers, and teachers as motivators of adolescents’ classroom behavior and
xxvi Contributors

academic accomplishments. She has published over 100 articles and book chap-
ters based on this work and has coedited books on achievement motivation. She
is currently coeditor of the Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology.
Dr. Wentzel is past vice president of Division E (Counseling and Human
Development, AERA) and a fellow of the American Psychological Association,
Division 15, and of the American Educational Research Association.
Allan Wigfield is professor and chair of the Department of Human
Development at the University of Maryland. His research interests include
the development and socialization of children’s achievement motivation in
different areas, and the effects of different kinds of reading comprehension
instructional practice on children’s reading motivation and achievement.
Christopher A. Wolters received his Ph.D. in Education and Psychology
(1996) from the University of Michigan in Ann Arbor. He is now a professor in
the Department of Educational Psychology at the University of Houston. He
teaches master’s- and doctoral-level courses that focus on theories of learning,
motivation, and self-regulated learning. His research interests focus on the
interface of motivational, cognitive, and metacognitive processes and their rela-
tion to students’ learning and achievement, especially during adolescence.
Bernard P.H. Wong is a Ph.D. candidate in the Department of Psychology at
the University of Hong Kong. His research interests include student engage-
ment and disengagement in school, social-cognitive and identity development
during adolescence, school-based intervention, and program evaluation.
Hongfei Yang is an associate professor in the Department of Psychology and
Behavioral Sciences at the University of Zhejiang. He is a graduate advisor of
counseling psychology. His research interests include counseling outcome
assessment, perfectionism, mental health education, Chinese culture, and
mental health.
Ethan Yazzie-Mintz was director of the High School Survey of Student
Engagement and assistant research scientist at Indiana University from 2005
through 2011. Prior to coming to Indiana University, he worked in a number
of large urban school districts – including Boston and New York – as a leader-
ship trainer, data analysis consultant, and curriculum consultant. His research
centers on student engagement, educational leadership and policy, and arts
(specifically drama) and education. Dr. Yazzie-Mintz is coeditor of The
Complex World of Teaching: Perspectives from Theory and Practice and is
currently director of the First Light Education Project and an independent
consultant based in Denver, Colorado.
Wei You is a doctoral student in the Department of Curriculum & Instruction
at the University of Maryland. Her research interests include reading motiva-
tion and achievement, the interplay between reading engagement and reading
comprehension, and measurement issues in reading research.
Kayla S. Zimmer is a lecturer in Elementary Education at St. Bonaventure
University and a doctoral candidate at the State University of New York at
Contributors xxvii

Buffalo. Her research interests include student-generated questions and how


preservice teachers can be prepared to create the supportive environment that
facilitates question generation and engagement.
Barry J. Zimmerman is a distinguished professor of Educational Psychology
and head of Learning, Development, and Instruction area at the Graduate
School and University Center of the City University of New York. He has
devoted his career to investigating social cognitive and self-regulatory pro-
cesses of children and youth in diverse areas of academic functioning, such as
mathematics, writing, and science, as well as in sport and health functioning.
Part I
What Is Student Engagement?
Jingle, Jangle,1 and Conceptual
Haziness2: Evolution and Future 1
Directions of the Engagement
Construct

Amy L. Reschly and Sandra L. Christenson

Abstract
This chapter serves as an introduction to the history and study of student
engagement. We describe the evolution of the construct of engagement
and disciplinary differences in theories and use of the engagement con-
struct. We highlight how our work on engagement, arising out of dropout
intervention, has changed over the last decade. In addition, we delineate
current issues in the study of engagement. The chapter ends with a discus-
sion of future directions to advance the theoretical and applied use of stu-
dent engagement to enhance outcomes for youth.

ment (see Epilogue, this volume) are explicitly


Introduction linked to academic tasks and activities.
However, engagement has long been viewed as
The roots of interest in engagement are, at least in
more than academic engaged time. From the earliest
part, driven by the desire to enhance student learn-
review to include the term engagement (Mosher &
ing. There is a long history of research on the rel-
McGowan, 1985), to the publication of seminal
evance of academic engaged time for improving
theory about the underpinnings of school dropout
student achievement (Fisher & Berliner, 1985).
and completion (Finn, 1989), to more recent con-
Indeed, many current definitions of student engage-
ceptualizations (see Epilogue, this volume), engage-
ment is viewed as multidimensional, involving
aspects of students’ emotion, behavior (participa-
A.L. Reschly (*)
tion, academic learning time), and cognition
Department of Educational Psychology
& Instructional Technology, University of Georgia, (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004). In other
325N Aderhold Hall, Athens, GA 30602, USA words, academic engaged time is important but
e-mail: Reschly@uga.edu not enough to accomplish the goals of schooling—
S.L. Christenson student learning across academic, social-emotional,
Department of Educational Psychology, and behavioral domains. Student engagement
University of Minnesota, 250 Educational Sciences
is the glue, or mediator, that links important
Building 56 East River Road, Minneapolis,
MN 55455, USA contexts—home, school, peers, and community—
e-mail: Chris002@umn.edu to students and, in turn, to outcomes of interest.

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 3


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_1, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
4 A.L. Reschly and S.L. Christenson

Our work in engagement began with direct the measurement of student engagement. Finally,
intervention provided to students at elevated risk we proposed directions for research to advance
for dropping out of high school. Through this the theoretical and applied use of student engage-
work, the importance of the social context of ment to enhance outcomes for youth.
schooling (e.g., relationships with teachers and
peers, belonging) and the need to foster students’
personal investment became apparent. Our goal Student Engagement
shifted from dropout prevention to school com- and High School Completion
pletion. The successful completion of high school
is much more than simply staying in school, and The Participation–Identification Model, postu-
thus, much more than the dropout problem—it lated by Finn (1989), was a seminal theory
involves meeting the defined academic, social, addressing critical variables of student engage-
and behavioral standards of schooling to succeed ment and the process of both dropout and com-
in school and ensure access to and success in post- pletion. According to the theory, dropout and
secondary enrollment options (Christenson, completion are conceptualized, respectively, as
Sinclair, Lehr, & Godber, 2001; Reschly & ongoing processes of participation ® school
Christenson, 2006b). When the goal is promoting success ® identification (completion) or of non-
school completion, the major foci of interventions participation ® poor school performance ®
are strategies that help students acquire skills to emotional withdrawal (dropout). In other words,
meet the demands and expectations of the school dropout and completion are not events but long-
environment, create relationships with adults and term processes of disengagement or engagement
students to facilitate their active participation in with school. In this view, engagement is com-
learning and school, and engage in future-oriented prised of behavior (participation) and affect, in
thinking that links skill acquisition to postsecon- the form of belonging and valuing school. Further,
dary enrollment success. As will be illustrated in this theory highlighted the importance of student
this chapter and volume, engagement not only development prior to school entry in that there
drives learning but also predicts school success. are differences in students’ skills, attitudes, and
The purpose of this chapter is to describe the behaviors (preparation) at the time of the com-
evolution of the construct of engagement, with mencement of formal schooling that affect the
roots in the dropout prevention literature, to more likelihood of successful participation, success,
recent conceptualizations of engagement as the and identification with school.
basis of high school reform and school-based There is a great deal of research that supports
interventions to enhance student outcomes across the importance of the main components of this
academic, social, behavioral, and emotional theory. For example, developmental pathways to
domains. In this endeavor, we first detail how dropout and completion have been delineated
our own theorizing and measurement of student from early childhood (Evans & DiBenedetto,
engagement has changed with time and our 1990; Garnier, Stein, & Jacobs, 1997; Jimerson,
experiences with implementing a dropout pre- Egeland, Sroufe, & Carlson, 2000). In addition,
vention program. Next, we delineate several several studies have reported long-term positive
issues that have arisen in the study of engage- effects of intensive early childhood programs on
ment, including the number of types and defini- academic achievement and high school graduation
tions of engagement; the differentiation of (Reynolds, 2001; Schweinhart & Weikart, 1999),
indicators from facilitators of engagement; which presumably affect long-term engagement
whether engagement is understood as a process by increasing students’ readiness in terms of aca-
or outcome and conceptualized on a single con- demic skills and behavior, thereby facilitating the
tinuum or two continua of engagement and dis- participation–success–identification cycle. Other
engagement/disaffection; the differentiation of research has shown dropout and completion may
the constructs of motivation and engagement; be predicted fairly accurately from data such as
and technical and related conceptual issues in attendance, behavior, academic performance, and
1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness… 5

attachment to school in grades 1–3 (e.g., completion after achievement, grade retention,
Alexander, Entwisle, & Horsey, 1997; Barrington and socioeconomic status were statistically con-
& Hendricks, 1989; Ensminger & Slusarcick, trolled, for another population of students with
1992) and later elementary school. For example, high rates of dropout (i.e., those with high-
Balfanz, Herzog, and Mac Iver (2007) were able incidence disabilities).
to identify 60% of high school dropouts from
sixth-grade attendance, misbehavior, and course
failures. Similarly, robust conclusions can be Engagement and Dropout Prevention
drawn from the literature on classroom (e.g., Finn
& Cox, 1992) and extracurricular participation The number of students who leave US high schools
(Feldman & Matjasko, 2005) and with respect to prematurely as dropouts has been of concern for
the importance of belonging and identification several decades. Interestingly, despite publicity
(see Voelkl, 2012). Finally, student engagement in and great interest in preventing dropout, many
high school has also been found to be related to commonly used interventions have been found to
postsecondary outcomes (Finn, 2006). have negligible effects (Christenson et al., 2001;
Among the most compelling evidence are Dynarski & Gleason, 2002; c.f., What Works
those studies that differentiate student outcomes, Clearinghouse, ND). Of those interventions that
within groups of students considered to be at risk appear to be most promising, many of these may
given certain demographics, based on their be characterized as addressing students’ engage-
engagement (attendance, participation, belong- ment at school and with learning. Thus, in addi-
ing) with school. Using a national, longitudinal tion to the prominent role ascribed student
dataset, Finn and Rock (1997) identified minority engagement in theory underlying student comple-
students from low-income backgrounds, two tion and dropout, student engagement is the cor-
powerful demographic risk factors for poor out- nerstone of our most promising dropout prevention
comes in the USA, and classified them into three and intervention efforts (Christenson et al., 2008).
groups: resilient completers (higher achieving One important difference, however, between
students, on track to graduate), nonresilient com- theory (Finn, 1989) and direct intervention is the
pleters (likely to graduate but with poorer aca- importance of contexts—families, schools, peers,
demic performance), and noncompleters and communities—as targets of intervention to
(dropouts). The groups differed in expected ways engender or enhance students’ engagement
according to a number of other demographic and (Christenson et al., 2008; Reschly & Christenson,
social variables (e.g., percent of students living 2006b). Dozens of predictors of dropout and
with both parents, family income, amount of completion have been identified (Hammond,
schooling parents expected students to complete). Linton, Smink, & Drew, 2007; Reschly &
After controlling for socioeconomic status and Christenson, 2006b; Rosenthal, 1998; Rumberger
family structure, the three groups of students dif- & Lim, 2008). Some predictors are inherent to
fered significantly in terms of teacher-rated and the individual students and families (e.g., race/
student self-reported engagement (e.g., atten- ethnicity) or are impractical or too difficult to
dance, working hard, paying attention in class, address in even the most comprehensive dropout
preparing for class/school, behavior problems). prevention program (e.g., socioeconomic status,
In other words, within a group of students who single parents). However, a number of variables
were demographically at risk for poor school out- are alterable, represent behaviors or indicators
comes, engagement variables significantly differ- that directly impact student behavior, prepara-
entiated those who were academically successful tion, and success in school, and may be found in
from lower achieving school completers and high external contexts, such as families and schools.
school dropouts. In a similar vein, Reschly and Categorizations of variables (e.g., nonschool cor-
Christenson (2006a) found that student engage- relates, status and alterable, proximal and distal)
ment variables, as measured in the eighth grade, predictive of dropout and completion may be
were predictive of high school dropout and found in Table 1.1.
Table 1.1 Categorizations of variables predictive of high school dropout and completion
Status variables
Nonschool correlates (Rosenthal, 1998) (Reschly & Christenson, 2006b)
SES Family process Dropout
Minority group status Those with parental involvement and Low SES
Gender monitoring less likely to drop out Reside in southeastern and
Males slightly more likely to drop out western regions of the USA
Student involvement with education
Community characteristics Students with disabilities
Dropouts have lower aspirations and
Dropout more likely in urban areas, English language learners
achievement, less participation, etc.
Southeastern and Western USA, poorer From Native American,
Social conformity vs. autonomy Hispanic, or Black racial/ethnic
communities, single-parent families, Dropouts have a higher need for
nonwhite communities, communities with backgrounds
autonomy, less conformity and less
high rates of foreign-born individuals accepting of authority, lower church
Household stress involvement, etc.
Several stress variables related to dropout Social deviance
(e.g., single parenting, substance abuse, Dropouts more likely to be deviant
mobility, neighborhood violence) (e.g., substance abuse, conduct
Taking adult roles disorder, runaway)
Teen pregnancy, employment, other Personality
adult responsibilities Dropouts have lower self-esteem
Social support for staying in school and confidence, more impulsive,
Valuing education by parents and peers difficulty communicating, etc.
reduces likelihood of dropout (e.g.,
parental expectations and achievement)

Alterable variables (Reschly & Christenson, 2006b)


By context
Protective Risk
Student Complete homework High rates of absences
Come to class prepared Behavior problems
High locus of control Poor academic performance
Good self-concept Grade retention
Expectations for school completion Working
Family Academic support (e.g., help with Low educational expectations
homework) and motivational Mobility
support (e.g., high expectations, Permissive parenting styles
talk to children about school) for
learning
Parental monitoring
School Orderly school environments Weak adult authority
Committed, caring teachers Large school size (>1,000 students)
Fair discipline policies High pupil–teacher ratios
Few caring relationships between staff and students
Poor or uninteresting curricula
Low expectations and high rates of truancy

Pull (Jordan, McPartland,


Push & Lara, 1999) Proximal Distal (Rumberger, 1995)
Conditions or events in Events or conditions outside e.g., school attendance e.g., family background,
the school environment of school that pull kids away and behavior early school experiences
that push kids out (e.g., (e.g., caring for a family member,
disciplinary policies, having to get a job).
grade retention)

Functional risk
Demographic risk variables (Christenson, 2008)
SES, disability or English learner, Attendance, behavior,
status, race/ethnicity, etc. academic performance,
credits earned, low levels
of participation, etc.
Sources: Rosenthal (1998); Reschly and Christenson (2006b); Jordan et al. (1999); Rumberger (1995); Christenson (2008)
1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness… 7

These various categorizations of variables pre-


dictive of dropout and completion clearly indicate
Check & Connect
the importance of contexts. Some variables are
The notion of alterable variables drawn from
more useful than others for determining which
Finn’s (1989) Participation–Identification Model
students are at greatest risk for disengagement
was applied to the development of Check &
and dropout and monitoring the effects of inter-
Connect (Christenson & Reschly, 2010). Check
vention. Christenson’s (2008) distinction between
& Connect is a structured mentoring intervention
demographic and functional risk builds on stu-
to promote student success and engagement at
dent engagement research that showed within
school and with learning, through relationship
demographically high-risk groups of students
building and systematic use of data to design per-
engagement variables differentiate those who
sonalized “connect” interventions. A targeted
graduate from those who do not (e.g., Finn &
intervention intended to complement universal
Rock, 1997; Reschly & Christenson, 2006a) and
intervention initiatives of schools and districts,
further identifies those variables that are sensitive
Check & Connect is designed to promote student
to small changes in student performance and may
engagement through relationship building, prob-
be used to monitor the effects of intervention.
lem solving, and persistence for marginalized
From a practical standpoint, it is not feasible to
students. The intervention has four components:
implement individually focused intervention to
(1) a mentor who works with students and fami-
all students within a demographically higher risk
lies for a minimum of 2 years; (2) regular checks,
category, for example, in the USA, to all Hispanic
utilizing data schools collect, on school adjust-
students, or all students identified as receiving
ment, behavior, and educational progress of the
free or reduced lunch (a broad, widely used indi-
student; (3) timely interventions, driven by data,
cator of low socioeconomic status), and more
to reestablish and maintain the student’s connec-
importantly, not every student within one of these
tion to school and learning and to enhance the
demographically high-risk groups needs inter-
student’s social and academic competencies; and
vention. Thus, functional risk—attendance,
(4) a partnership with families.
behavior, low levels of involvement, and few
Since its inception in 1995, a seminal assump-
relationships at schools—differentiates those
tion of the Check & Connect student engagement
who would benefit from interventions from those
intervention is that students are placed at risk when
who do not need additional support or services.
the learning environment does not respond to their
Developmental changes and progression across
needs (we differentiate this from the common
levels of schooling necessitate that indicators of
phrase of “at-risk” students). Four theoretical per-
functional risk are further differentiated by ele-
spectives undergird Check & Connect: systems-
mentary and middle/high school levels
ecological, resilience, cognitive–behavioral, and
(Christenson & Reschly, 2010).
autonomous motivation (Connell & Wellborn,
Interest in both dropout prevention and stu-
1991; Ryan & Deci, 2002). With respect to sys-
dent engagement has grown steadily from the
tems theory, it is assumed that students benefit
publication of Finn’s seminal theory in the late
from congruent and consistent messages about
1980s. The applied nature of engagement as a
expectations and goals across home and school
basis of dropout prevention and the importance
and within the school setting and that to promote
of contexts as targets of intervention have led to
student success, the needs of students must be
expansion from the two-factor Participation–
considered in the context of their family, class-
Identification Model. Indeed, our own theorizing
room, peers, and community.
about the nature of engagement and our attempts
Drawing on resilience theory, the mentor is
to measure this construct have been heavily influ-
the significant person with a long-term relation-
enced by our dropout prevention work with
ship with youth. However, mentors recognize
Check & Connect.
8 A.L. Reschly and S.L. Christenson

that context matters—what parents and teachers control student differences for a 5-year gradua-
do to support the student toward school comple- tion rate was significant and moderate (ES = .53).
tion makes a difference. They draw upon com- Christenson (2009) concluded that Check &
munity resources as much as feasible, avoiding Connect improves indicators of students’ aca-
“reinventing the wheel.” Finally, irrespective of demic (e.g., credits earned, homework comple-
family circumstances (e.g., status variables), tion) and behavioral engagement (attendance,
mentors believe in students’ capability to change ratings of social skills) and demonstrates the
their trajectory when standards for performance necessity of sustained intervention for students
are paired with persistent support. Mentors con- who are disengaged from learning.
sider student perspectives and personal goals, Consistent with the experimental studies, two
and they use problem solving to maintain a focus other longitudinal studies using a pre-post mea-
on developing self-determined, self-directed, and surement design revealed similar positive results
self-regulated learners, avoiding dependence on (reduced rates of truancy, out-of-school suspen-
the mentor. We have always said that mentors sions, and course failures and increased rates of
“fuel the academic motivation” of students by attendance) for students both with and without
underscoring autonomous motivation and pro- disabilities who attended elementary and secondary
viding instrumental support—facilitating prob- schools in suburban settings (Lehr, Sinclair, &
lem solving toward students’ personal goals; Christenson, 2004; Sinclair & Kaibel, 2002).
giving regular, systematic, informed feedback in Also, closer relationships between Check &
a nonjudgmental way; encouraging the student to Connect intervention staff and students were
self-observe, self-evaluate, and self-reflect on associated with improved engagement (Anderson,
progress toward goals; emphasizing the impor- Christenson, Sinclair, & Lehr, 2004). Specifically,
tance of effort, persistence, and trying again to the mentor perspective of the relationship pre-
help the student self-regulate their motivation; dicted teacher-rated academic engagement, while
and making a long-term commitment, at least the student perspective of the relationship
2 years, to be a persistent source of support for approached significance as a predictor of teacher-
the student and family. rated academic engagement. Lastly, our findings
suggest that Check & Connect is working to
actively engage students and their families with
Research Findings school and with learning (Lehr et al., 2004). For
example, 87% of parents of students in grades
Check & Connect has met the evidence-based K–8 (N = 147) who took part in Check & Connect
standards of the US Department of Education’s for 2 years were rated by different teachers as
What Works Clearinghouse (2006) for students more supportive of their children’s education
with disabilities who were at risk of disengage- (defined as parental follow-through, communica-
ment and dropout. Specifically, two experimental tion with school, and homework completion).
studies with secondary students with disabilities Furthermore, teachers’ perceptions (N = 123) of
(Ns of 94 and 144) revealed that Check & Connect student behavior were positive—90% indicated
youth were more likely to be enrolled in school students in grades K–8 were showing improve-
(i.e., attendance), never interrupted their schooling, ment in homework completion, interest in school,
have persisted in school with no periods of and attendance. Teachers’ observations of stu-
15 days or more absences, were less likely to dents who received 2 years of sustained interven-
have dropped out, and were more likely be on tion were very positive. Teachers rated these
track to graduate within 5 years, be enrolled in an students significantly more likely to be eager to
educational program (alternative, GED), or to learn, follow school rules, think ahead about con-
have completed high school in 2 years than stu- sequences, get along with others, show respect
dents in the control group (Sinclair, Christenson, for others’ rights and feelings, and persist when
Evelo, & Hurley, 1998; Sinclair, Christenson, & challenged by difficult tasks, all critical compe-
Thurlow, 2005). The effect size for treatment and tencies and habits of learning success.
1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness… 9

In sum, analysis of Check & Connect program values and goals (I want to), and social connect-
impact data consistently yields positive results: edness (I belong) around schooling and learning,
reduced rates of truancy, tardiness, suspensions, we created the SEI to measure affective (per-
course failures, and dropout and increased rates of ceived connection to others) and cognitive
attendance. In particular, treatment–control differ- engagement (perceived relevance and motivation
ences in critical student engagement variables such to learn). Coincidently, our Check & Connect
as participation (attendance), behavior (social skills intervention—within the dropout arena—drew
ratings), academics (credits earned), and, ulti- upon similar theoretical underpinnings (intrinsic
mately, graduation rates have been demonstrated motivation, expectations, and supports) as the
for middle and high school students with learning Committee on Increasing High School Students’
and behavioral disabilities (Sinclair et al., 1998, Engagement and Motivation to Learn (National
2005). Our current research projects are focused on Research Council, 2004).
(a) assessing the efficacy of Check & Connect At this point, we posit that the theory of stu-
with a general education population in three large dent engagement that undergirds Check &
urban school districts and (b) modifying Check & Connect is multidimensional; the four subtypes
Connect for use in community college settings or (academic, behavioral, cognitive, affective) serve
assisting secondary school students with college as a heuristic for designing interventions that
readiness information and support. Beginning with maximize the person–environment fit for students
the 2010 school year, Check & Connect has been who are disengaged, alienated, or marginalized
supported by the core budget of the Minneapolis from learning. Inherent in our theorizing on stu-
Public Schools (MPS); hence, sustainability of an dent engagement is the belief that students are
intervention that was developed with MPS during able to accurately report on their own engagement
1990–1995 has been achieved. and environments and, further, that their perspec-
tives are integral to the selection, implementation,
and monitoring of interventions. In our view,
Measuring Student Engagement engagement can be considered an outcome and a
process. In Check & Connect, we want students
Important to the ability to examine outcomes of to improve on all subtypes of engagement (out-
Check & Connect with different populations and comes). However, we recognize that student
under different conditions in our engagement engagement is also a mediator between the con-
research has been the development of the Student textual influences (facilitators) and our desired
Engagement Instrument (SEI) (Appleton, learning outcomes across academic, social, and
Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, 2006; Betts, emotional domains. We speculate that cognitive
Appleton, Reschly, Christenson, & Huebner, and affective engagement are potentially media-
2010). As noted previously, the dependent vari- tors of academic and behavioral engagement
ables for the research on Check & Connect imple- (Reschly & Christenson, 2006a), or in other words,
mentations have been indicators of academic or engaging or disengaging students cognitively and
behavioral engagement (e.g., credits earned, affectively precedes changes in students’ behavior
extracurricular participation, homework comple- and academic engagement. As others have pos-
tion, attendance). Ongoing comments from the ited (Furrer, Skinner, Marchand, & Kindermann,
Check & Connect high school students with emo- 2006), we hypothesize that there are Matthew
tional disabilities (Sinclair et al., 2005) assisted Effects (Ceci & Papierno, 2005) between con-
us in broadening our indicators and theory of stu- texts and engagement wherein as students are
dent engagement. These students quite regularly engaged, contexts provide feedback and support
and persistently reported in various ways to their that promote ever greater engagement (Reschly,
mentors that they “can’t do the schoolwork,” 2010). A representation of this model may be
“won’t try anymore,” and that “no one likes them found in Fig. 1.1. Inherent in our model is the
anyway.” In response to our students’ questioning belief that the student perspective is essential for
of their personal competence and control (I can), change in student learning and behavior.
10 A.L. Reschly and S.L. Christenson

Context Indicators of Student Engagement Selected Proximal Selected Distal


Learning Outcomes Outcomes

Family Academic High School


Academic and motivational Grades (GPA) Graduation
support for learning Affective Behavioral Performance on
Goals and expectations (student perception) Attendance standardized tests
Monitoring/supervision Belonging/ (absences, skips, Passing Basic
Learning resources in the Identification tardies) Skills Tests
home with school Participation
School (classroom, Social
Peers connectedness extracurricular) Social awareness

Post secondary
Educational expectations Behavioral Relationship

Employment
Shared common school values incidents (office skills with peers

Education
Attendance referrals, and adults
Academic beliefs and efforts suspensions, Responsible
Aspiration for learning detentions) decision-making

School Academic Emotional


School Relational climate Cognitive Time on task Self-awareness of
(peers, teachers) (student perception) Credit hours feelings
Instruction and Curriculum Self-regulation toward Emotion
*Programming; curricula; Relevance of graduation (high regulation Productive
quality of instruction school to future school) Conflict Citizenry
*Goal structure (task vs. aspirations Homework resolution skills
ability) Value of completion rate
*Clear and appropriate learning (goal- and accuracy
expectations setting) Class grades
Support (number of
*Mental health support and failing grades)
service
*Academic support
Management
*Disciplinary climate
*Authority
*Opportunities for student
participation

Community
Service learning

Fig. 1.1 Model of associations between context, engagement, and student outcomes

The academic lens maintained for Check & student achievement and behavior for all students
Connect students by mentors is paramount. They (Fredricks et al., 2004). Engagement was also
employ a problem-solving orientation with the given a central role in high school reform efforts
student, teacher, and family to foster a person– (National Research Council, 2004).
environment fit for the student at school and with Underscoring the predominant view in the
learning; provide persistent, ongoing support to dropout prevention arena, Fredricks et al. (2004)
enhance the student’s capacity to meet the aca- also conceptualized engagement as a multidi-
demic, social, and behavioral standards of the mensional construct that is amenable to the
school environment; and aim to improve stu- effects of intervention and highly affected by
dents’ academic, social, and emotional learning contexts—teachers, families, etc. The authors’
outcomes by fostering the academic, behavioral, extensive review of the literature was organized
cognitive, and affective engagement of students. around three types of engagement: behavioral,
cognitive, and emotional. Behavioral engage-
ment is defined by participation in academic,
Expansion of Engagement: social, or extracurricular activities. Emotional
Metaconstruct and School Reform engagement is comprised of affect (positive and
negative) in interactions with teachers, peers,
While engagement theory and intervention were schoolwork, and the school. The definition of
expanding within the dropout prevention litera- cognitive engagement was rooted in personal
ture, a more general interest in engagement was investment, self-regulation, and striving for mas-
burgeoning as a central construct to understanding tery (Fredricks et al., 2004). Unique to the
1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness… 11

Fredricks et al. conceptualization of engagement, there is little agreement about the definition of
however, was the elevation of engagement to the each subtype of engagement. Block (2000)
level of a metaconstruct that brings together many applied traditional psychology terms of jingle
previously separate lines of research and may (Thorndike, 1904) and jangle (Kelly, 1927) to
subsume the construct of motivation as a form of describe the confusing way terms and concepts
engagement. were used in personality psychology. Engagement
There are essentially three schools of thought currently suffers from a similar problem wherein
on student engagement: one arising from the the same term is used to refer to different things
dropout prevention theory and intervention area, (jingle) and different terms are used for the same
another from a more general school reform per- construct (jangle). For example, Finn (2006)
spective (i.e., National Research Council, 2004), classified perceived relevance, or utility, of school
and the third arising out of the motivational lit- as affective engagement, whereas we would char-
erature (e.g., Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, & acterize it as cognitive engagement (Appleton
Kinderman, 2008; Skinner, Kinderman, & Furrer, et al., 2006; Christenson et al., 2008) and others
2009). Overlapping with these schools of thought as motivation (Wylie & Hodgen, 2012). Or, we
are subdisciplines within the academic field of described student-perceived relationships with
psychology. Some scholars, ourselves included, teachers and peers and feelings of belonging as
are educational psychologists whose interests are affective engagement (Appleton et al., 2006),
more educationally focused and applied in orien- while Yazzie-Mintz and colleagues from the High
tation, while others are developmental psycholo- School Survey of Student Engagement include
gists with more theoretical interests in motivation interaction with others and connection to commu-
and basic (i.e., theory-testing) research. It is per- nity in the scale Social/Behavioral/Participatory
haps not surprising that given the relatively recent Engagement and feelings of connection to others
ascendance of the engagement construct and as emotional engagement (Yazzie-Mintz &
somewhat varying origins of interest in engage- McCormick, 2012). Selected engagement theo-
ment, there are a number of issues that have yet ries and respective indicators may be found in
to be resolved, including those related to theory, Table 1.2.
definitions, and measurement. These issues are
explicated in the paragraphs that follow.
Process or Outcome

Types and Definitions In Connell and Wellborn’s (1991) influential


model of self-system processes, engagement
The study of engagement is hindered by lack of was viewed as a mediator between context, indi-
consensus in both the number of subtypes and viduals’ needs for autonomy, competence, and
definitions of student engagement. Two-, three-, relatedness, and outcomes (context–self-action
and four-subtype models are prevalent in the lit- [engagement/disaffection]–outcomes). The notion
erature. There is agreement that at a minimum, that engagement connects contexts and student
engagement is comprised of participatory behavior outcomes is prevalent in other engagement theo-
and some affective component. Other scholars ries (Appleton et al., 2006; Skinner et al., 2008).
add cognitive engagement (Appleton et al., 2006; When one considers developmental changes and
Christenson & Anderson, 2002; Fredricks et al., the passage of time in addition to this mediating
2004) and/or bifurcate behavioral engagement role, questions about engagement as process or
into two subtypes: academic (e.g., time on task) outcome arise. Phrased differently, how can
and behavioral (participation) (Appleton et al., engagement be both a mediator and an outcome?
2006; Christenson et al., 2008). The answer lies in how long range one’s view is:
Although a number of authors, many in this a semester? A year? Over several years? Given
volume included, ascribe to the three-part typology, the conclusion that dropout and completion are
12 A.L. Reschly and S.L. Christenson

Table 1.2 Comparisons of prominent engagement models on key dimensions


Continuum/
Number of types Definitions/indicators continuaa
Finn (1989) 2 Participation Measured as
Respond to requirements continuum
Class-related initiative
Extracurricular activities
Decision-making
Identification
Belonging
Valuing
Appleton, 4 Academic Continuum
Christenson, Time on task, credit accrual, homework completion
and colleaguesb Behavioral
Attendance, participation, preparation for class/school
Cognitive
Value/relevance, self-regulation, goal setting
Affective
Belonging, identification with school
Skinner and 4 (2 Engagement, Behavioral engagement Continua
colleaguesc 2 Disaffection) Action initiation, effort, persistence, intensity, attention,
absorption, involvement
Behavioral disaffection
Passivity, giving up, withdrawal, inattentive, unprepared,
distracted, mentally disengaged
Emotional engagement
Enthusiasm, interest, enjoyment, satisfaction, pride, vitality, zest
Emotional disaffection
Boredom, disinterest, frustration, sadness, worry/anxiety, shame,
self-blame
Martin (2007) 4 Higher order Adaptive cognition Continua
factors, 11 total Valuing, mastery orientation, self-efficacy
Adaptive behavior
Persistence, planning, task management
Maladaptive behavior
Disengagement, self-handicapping
Impeding/maladaptive cognition
Uncertain control, failure avoidance
a
Continuum refers to a single dimension of engagement (ranging from high to low); continua refers to the separation of
engagement and disengagement/disaffection into two dimensions (each ranging from high to low)
b
Appleton et al. (2006) and Christenson et al. (2008)
c
Skinner et al. (2008, 2009)

long-term processes of engagement and disen- as high school graduation and postsecondary
gagement from school over several years, it is enrollment.
appropriate that engagement is both process
(mediator) and outcome. For example, an out-
come at one-time point, such as attendance or Continuum or Continua
skipping classes, may be considered important
process variables (i.e., indicators of engage- Another difference among various scholars’ con-
ment) for another later outcome, such as grades ceptualizations and measures of engagement is
or credits earned, which in turn are process whether engagement is viewed and measured on
variables for even longer range outcomes, such a single continuum or whether engagement and
1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness… 13

disengagement, often referred to as disaffection, Sinclair, Christenson, Lehr & Anderson, 2003).
are separate continua. A parallel may be found in From an intervention perspective, we posited that
the field of positive psychology where it is argued it is important to assess both indicators and facili-
that mental health and happiness are distinct from tators of this construct. Theoretically, it is our
mental illness (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, contention that cognitive and affective engage-
2000). Thus, health and happiness are more than ment are inherently individual internal processes
the absence of psychopathology. The parallel for and, thus, students are the most accurate source
engagement may be that having low engagement is of information about these forms of engagement.
different from being disengaged/disaffected. In our Skinner and colleagues, however, have argued
own work, we have measured engagement on a for the separation of indicators and facilitators, a
single continuum ranging from low to high whereas position echoed by other scholars (Lam, Wong,
other scholars measure both engagement and disaf- Yang, & Liu, 2012), in order to more fully exam-
fection (Martin, 2007; Skinner et al., 2008). ine the effects of context on engagement (Skinner
Another parallel may be drawn from the et al., 2008). We value different explications and
research within positive psychology. There is believe productive scholarly debate is necessary
some question as to whether there is a threshold to propel the field forward. These different view-
on the benefits of happiness. Research generally points may align with the aforementioned disci-
finds that most people report being above the plinary differences between educational and
neutral point in happiness (Oishi, Diener, & developmental psychology and purpose of mea-
Lucas, 2007); we find similar results in our sur- surement (theory testing vs. prevention/interven-
vey work on student engagement (Appleton, tion). Or, these dissimilar viewpoints represent
Reschly, & Martin, 2012). Research finds that differences in views on how context affects
those who are happiest are most successful in engagement and outcomes. For example, in the
terms of relationships and volunteer work, while self-system process model (Connell & Wellborn,
those with somewhat lower levels of happiness 1991), engagement is a bridge between context
are most successful in other domains, such as and outcomes (context–self-action–outcome).
income and education (Oishi et al., 2007). How does one separate objective measures of
Questions that may be raised for engagement context, such as mastery goal orientation or
include: Is there a point at which greater engage- school climate, from students’ internal processes
ment no longer confers additional benefits? Or at of these?
which one is so highly engaged it is no longer A final, related point concerns the source of
adaptive but maladaptive? Continuing in this engagement data. It is our belief that students can
vein, how much engagement is needed to pro- accurately report on their school experiences, and
duce corresponding changes in desired out- in fact, their reports are likely more accurate, or at
comes—academic, social, and behavioral? a minimum an important addition to, the informa-
tion obtained from other sources (peers, adults,
etc.). Do parents and teachers know if students
Indicators and Facilitators feel like they belong at school? Or whether a stu-
dent thinks a class is relevant to his/her future?
Scholarly debate has also touched on whether it is Perhaps it is necessary to separate parent or
important to distinguish facilitators of engage- teacher reports regarding contextual facilitators of
ment from true indicators of the engagement con- engagement from those collected from students.
struct (Skinner et al., 2008). Based on our dropout We believe the student perspective is critical to
intervention work, we originally proposed the understanding the person–environment fit and to
distinction between indicators and facilitators to efforts to enhance student engagement. In fact,
guide screening and monitoring practices for tar- the seminal nature of the student perspective is
geted students (indicators) and direct attention to reflected in our framework of engagement (see
contexts that are logical foci of intervention Fig. 1.1). Teacher–student relationships as per-
efforts (facilitators; Christenson et al., 2008; ceived and reported by the student (an indicator of
14 A.L. Reschly and S.L. Christenson

affective engagement) are conceptually distinct currently exist for student engagement. Indeed,
from teacher–student relationships as reported by motivation has a long, rich history and extensive
the teacher or others (facilitators of students’ literature. Conversely, engagement is the prover-
engagement). Hence, we posit that the source of bial new kid on the block, with the first appear-
the informant must be considered and is critical ance of the term engagement occurring in the
for understanding the distinction between facilita- 1980s (Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, 2008).
tors and indicators. Considering the source of the A number of authors in this volume endorsed
informant maintains the independence between the three-type model of engagement: emotional/
facilitators and indicators that is deemed essential affective, cognitive, and behavioral (see Epilogue,
in measurement (Skinner et al., 2008). Differences this volume). Although there are jingle/jangle
in the perspectives of the informants, we would problems among each of these types, the greatest
surmise, influence the teacher–student interaction confusion arises over what we have classified as
that often shapes student engagement. more internal forms: cognitive engagement and
affective engagement and between each of these
and motivation. In congruence with the engage-
Engagement and Motivation ment as a metaconstruct idea (Fredricks et al.,
2004), it is possible that cognitive engagement
A prominent, lingering issue in the study of and motivation are in fact very similar or even that
engagement is the relationship between engage- same subconstruct, as evidenced by the use of tra-
ment and motivation. Some scholars use the ditional motivational concepts like self-regulation
terms engagement and motivation interchange- in definitions of cognitive engagement (Wolters &
ably (e.g., Martin, 2007; National Research Taylor, 2012). The distinction between cognitive
Council, 2004); others have proposed that the engagement and motivation is even murkier when
metaconstruct of student engagement subsumes one considers the differentiation of motivation
motivation (Fredricks et al., 2004), while others and engagement that is espoused by many schol-
ascribe to the position that engagement and moti- ars (see Epilogue, this volume). In this view, moti-
vation are distinct, related constructs wherein vation is considered to be intent and engagement
motivation represents intention and engagement as action. Thus, engagement is defined by an
is action (see Epilogue, this volume). observable, action-oriented subtype (behavioral)
For intervention work, it may be unimportant and two internal ones (cognitive and affective
to differentiate these constructs. Indeed, Martin’s engagement) but then is differentiated from moti-
work with the Motivation–Engagement Scale vation as engagement being action (observable
and affiliated workbook intervention (Martin, behavior), motivation as intent (internal).
2006, 2008) follow this course. To this end, it is In our writings on engagement, we have fre-
generally accepted that both motivation and quently acknowledged that there are both theo-
engagement are influenced by context, that there retical and measurement issues relative to
are individual differences in how students respond motivation and engagement (e.g., Appleton et al.,
to the environment, and that these constructs are 2006, 2008; Reschly, 2010). And, as described
linked to important student outcomes. However, earlier in this chapter, Check & Connect has moti-
theoretical advancement of the constructs of vational underpinnings. We have argued that
engagement and motivation requires that the engagement and motivation are separate, but
association between the two be clearly specified related constructs, wherein motivation is neces-
and tested. sary but not sufficient for engagement (Appleton
One issue in the theoretical differentiation of et al.). In one of our first studies of engagement,
motivation and engagement is the time of exis- we used items representing boredom and per-
tence and extensiveness of the scholarly literature. ceived utility of education to one’s future from
Fredricks et al. (2004) noted that motivation has the NELS:88 longitudinal dataset as indicators of
more elaborate and differentiated definitions than cognitive engagement (Reschly & Christenson,
1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness… 15

2006a). In the SEI, cognitive engagement is engagement measures has also differed according
measured by the scales: control and relevance of to whether data were collected via active or pas-
school work, future aspirations and goals, and sive parental consent. Elsewhere, we have specu-
extrinsic motivation (Appleton et al.). Clearly, lated that studies requiring active parental consent
there is overlap between our operationalization of may be subject to a positive engagement bias
cognitive engagement and some motivational because those children whose parents are less
concepts (e.g., intrinsic/extrinsic motivation, likely to return forms may also be students at risk
goal-setting). Our model conceives of engage- for disengaging and dropping out of school
ment as a multidimensional construct. Our opera- (Reschly et al., 2008). Methodologies also differ
tionalization of cognitive engagement may be as to whether students complete surveys anony-
characterized as motivation to learn as evidenced mously or whether the data are confidential from
by the internal processes such as perceived rele- teachers but maintained with identifying student
vance to future, goal setting, and so on. This inter- information and linked over time.
nal motivation to learn is inherently individual In summary, efforts to measure student
and influenced by the motivational contexts (e.g., engagement are in the earliest stages. Advances
classroom goal structures, school climate and in measurement are needed to clarify the theoreti-
messages regarding effort and ability, peer group cal issues in the engagement construct, from dif-
norms relative to academic behavior). A goal of ferentiating engagement and motivation to the
this volume was to elucidate scholars’ thinking on utility of conceptualizing engagement on a single
motivation and engagement (see Prologue/ continuum or as continua of engagement and dis-
Epilogue, this volume). It is clear that the theo- engagement/disaffection. These and other direc-
retical differentiation of the constructs of engage- tions for future research are addressed in the next
ment and motivation continues to be an important section.
area to be addressed in future scholarly work.

Future Directions
Measurement
The issues described in this chapter, and through-
Advances in theory require concomitant advances out this volume (Epilogue), indicate that excite-
in measurement technology. At the time we began ment about the engagement construct should be
the development of our instrument, the SEI, there tempered with knowledge of these issues and the
were few comprehensive measures of engage- limitations of our current knowledge base.
ment based in any theoretical model. Of those However, enthusiasm about engagement as a
available, some were created from larger, exist- basis of theory for conceptualizing important
ing surveys to represent aspects of certain types processes and outcomes and interventions to
of engagement, and jangling (i.e., items jumping enhance these outcomes support the need for
from subtype to subtype, depending on author) continued scholarship and research. Issues, such
was common. A recent review of instruments as the number of types of engagement, lack of
(Fredricks et al., 2011) provides evidence that the consensus regarding definitions, and differentiat-
number of instruments intended to measure, in ing constructs of motivation and engagement, are
the scale’s entirety or via subscales, student clear targets of future research efforts. These, and
engagement has grown dramatically. The avail- other directions, are described in the following
able measures differ in terms of the source of data paragraphs.
(student self-report; teacher-report; observation Block (2000) delineated how jingle/jangle
instruments), how many types of engagement are problems limit the conclusions that can be reached
measured, and whether designed to measure through research. Thus, addressing jingle/
engagement generally or with reference to a spe- jangle problems in theory and measurement is a
cific subject area (Fredricks et al.). Research with logical step for moving forward. In addition,
16 A.L. Reschly and S.L. Christenson

differentiating the purpose of measurement (e.g., of measuring disengagement as well as engage-


theory testing vs. linking to intervention) may ment (Reschly et al., 2012), something we will
facilitate greater conceptual clarity with the consider in our ongoing examinations and poten-
engagement construct. Indeed, measurement tial revision of the SEI (Appleton et al., 2006;
technologies are needed to answer important Betts et al., 2010). Research is needed to explore
questions about engagement. Of currently avail- the combination of engagement and disengage-
able measures, a number of authors have pro- ment/disaffection that yields the most psycho-
vided correlational evidence of associations metrically sound measure of the construct.
between engagement and achievement (Fredricks Another direction for research is to examine
et al., 2011); however, there are few studies of whether there are cultural differences in the con-
convergent and divergent validity among mea- struct of student engagement. Differences may be
sures of engagement and between measures of found in the relative importance of contextual
engagement and motivation. variables (e.g., influence of peers, classroom/
Currently, there are few longitudinal studies school structures, family involvement or support)
that utilize a comprehensive, theory-based mea- or the construct may be different across youth in
sure of student engagement. This type of research different countries. A small study we conducted
is needed to investigate developmental changes using the SEI and Martin’s MES (previously only
in the engagement construct. Currently, the examined with Australian students) indicated that
Motivation–Engagement Scale (MES) has been there may be differences in how US and Australian
examined cross-sectionally with students from students respond to these measures (Reschly
elementary through college age (Martin, 2009). et al., 2012).
Similarly, we have recently piloted elementary- Our knowledge base regarding the outcomes
and college-age versions of the SEI (Carter, of engagement interventions is limited. Some
Reschly, Lovelace, Appleton, & Thompson, interventions focus on changing students to func-
2012; Waldrop & Reschly, 2011, respectively). tion more effectively in the environment, others
Longitudinal engagement research will also on curriculum and pedagogy, and still others on
facilitate the examination of (a) trajectories of the school environment with the goal of person-
engagement and the relationship between engage- alizing education and enhancing relationships
ment and student outcomes over time, as well as among students and between students and staff.
(b) the importance of different subtypes of As engagement is increasingly conceptualized as
engagement to these outcomes. We have pro- an organizing heuristic for interventions deliv-
posed that there is a hierarchy of types of engage- ered at universal-level, for all students, as well as
ment (Reschly & Christenson, 2006a) such that those who are at elevated risk for poor outcomes
affective and cognitive changes (greater belong- (targeted or intensive; Christenson et al., 2008),
ing, better relationships with teachers and peers) more work is needed to evaluate interventions to
precede the observable, behavioral changes in determine what works, for whom, and under what
important variables such as attendance and conditions. Particularly concerning is the notion
behavior. Initial research appears to support this of sustained intervention for students disengaged
contention (Lam et al., 2012; Skinner et al., 2008; from learning or on the trajectory for school
Wylie & Hodgen, 2012). dropout. Specifically, for how long and with what
Future research is also needed to address intensity must intervention support be provided
whether engagement is best conceptualized on a to achieve positive learning outcomes? In addi-
single continuum or as two continua of engage- tion, improved measurement methodology is
ment and disengagement/disaffection. In the needed to link assessment to intervention. Our
examination of their measure of engagement, work with Check & Connect underscored the
Skinner et al. (2009) found engagement and dis- importance of systematic monitoring of student
affection to be distinct subscales. Our research data so that interventions could be delivered at
suggests that there may be some additive benefits the first sign of disengagement.
1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness… 17

In summary, student engagement is a burgeon- Nystedt (Eds.), Developmental science and the holis-
ing construct. It is viewed as a basis of theory and tic approach (pp. 155–164). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carter, C., Reschly, A. L., Lovelace, M. D., Appleton, J. J.,
interventions related to high school dropout, high & Thompson, D. (2012). Measuring student engage-
school reform, and as a necessary element for ment among elementary students: Pilot of the Elementary
improving student outcomes. The construct of Student Engagement Instrument. Manuscript under
engagement has evolved rapidly in the last review.
Ceci, S. J., & Papierno, P. B. (2005). The rhetoric and
10 years. Although there are numerous concep- reality of gap closing: When the “have-nots” gain but
tual and measurement issues, scholarship around the “haves” gain even more. American Psychologist,
engagement is robust. It continues to be a fruitful 60, 149–160.
and important area of research. Christenson, S. L. (2008, January 22). Engaging students
with school: The essential dimension of dropout pre-
vention programs. [Webinar]. National Dropout
Authors’ Notes Prevention Center for Students with Disabilities.
1. The jingle/jangle distinction was used to describe per- Christenson, S. L. (2009). The relevance of engagement
sonality psychology by Block (2000). for students at-risk of educational failure: Findings
2. Conceptual haziness used to characterize the construct and lessons from Check & Connect research. In J.
of engagement by Appleton et al. (2008). Morton (Ed.), Engaging young people in learning:
Why does it matter and what can we do?: Conference
proceedings (pp. 36–84). Wellington, New Zealand:
NZCER Press.
References Christenson, S. L., & Anderson, A. R. (2002).
Commentary: The centrality of the learning context
for students’ academic enabler skills. School
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Horsey, C. S. (1997).
Psychology Review, 31, 378–393.
From first grade forward: Early foundations of high
Christenson, S. L., & Reschly, A. L. (2010). Check &
school dropouts. Sociology of Education, 70, 87–107.
Connect: Enhancing school completion through stu-
Anderson, A. R., Christenson, S. L., Sinclair, M. F., &
dent engagement. In E. Doll & J. Charvat (Eds.),
Lehr, C. (2004). Check & Connect: The importance of
Handbook of prevention science. Mahwah, NJ:
relationships for promoting engagement with school.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Journal of School Psychology, 42(2), 95–113.
Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., Appleton, J. J., Berman,
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. S., Spanjers, D., & Varro, P. (2008). Best practices in
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- fostering student engagement. In A. Thomas & J.
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. (5th ed.). Bethesda, MD: National Association of
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, School Psychologists.
A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological Christenson, S. L., Sinclair, M. F., Lehr, C. A., & Godber, Y.
engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement (2001). Promoting successful school completion:
Instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44, Critical conceptual and methodological guidelines.
427–445. School Psychology Quarterly, 16, 468–484.
Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., & Martin, C. (2012). Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence,
Research to practice: Measuring and reporting stu- autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of
dent engagement. Manuscript under review. self system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe
Balfanz, R., Herzog, L., & Mac Iver, D. J. (2007). (Eds.), Self processes and development: The Minnesota
Preventing student disengagement and keeping stu- symposia on child psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 43–77).
dents on the graduation path in urban middle-grades Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.
schools: Early identification and effective interven- Dynarski, M., & Gleason, P. (2002). How can we help?
tions. Educational Psychologist, 42, 223–235. What we have learned from recent federal dropout
Barrington, B. L., & Hendricks, B. (1989). Differentiating prevention evaluations. Journal of Education for
characteristics of high school graduates, dropouts, and Students Placed At-Risk, 7, 43–69.
nongraduates. The Journal of Educational Research, Ensminger, M. E., & Slusarcick, A. L. (1992). Paths to high
89, 309–319. school graduation or dropout: A longitudinal study of a
Betts, J., Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., Christenson, S. L., first-grade cohort. Sociology of Education, 65, 95–113.
& Huebner, E. S. (2010). A study of the reliability and Evans, I., & DiBenedetto, A. (1990). Pathways to school
construct validity of the School Engagement dropout: A conceptual model for early prevention.
Instrument across multiple grades. School Psychology Special Services in School, 6, 63–80.
Quarterly, 25, 84–93. Feldman, A. F., & Matjasko, J. L. (2005). The role of
Block, J. (2000). Three tasks for personality psychology. school-based extracurricular activities in adolescent
In L. R. Bergman, R. B. Cairns, L.-G. Nilsson, & L. development: A comprehensive review and future
18 A.L. Reschly and S.L. Christenson

directions. Review of Educational Research, 75(2), (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement
159–210. (pp. 403–419). New York: Springer.
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Lehr, C. A., Sinclair, M. F., & Christenson, S. L. (2004).
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. Addressing student engagement and truancy preven-
Finn, J. D. (2006). The adult lives of at-risk students: tion during the elementary years: A replication study
The roles of attainment and engagement in high of the Check & Connect model. Journal of Education
school (NCES 2006–328). Washington, DC: National for Students Placed At-Risk, 9(3), 279–301.
Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Martin, A. J. (2006). Enhancing student motivation and
Education. engagement: The effects of a multidimensional inter-
Finn, J. D., & Cox, D. (1992). Participation and with- vention. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33,
drawal among fourth-grade pupils. American 239–269.
Educational Research Journal, 29, 141–162. Martin, A. J. (2007). Examining a multidimensional
Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success model of student motivation and engagement using a
among students at risk for school failure. Journal of construct validation approach. British Journal of
Applied Psychology, 82(2), 221–234. Educational Psychology, 77, 413–440.
Fisher, C. W., & Berliner, D. C. (1985). Perspectives on Martin, A. J. (2008). Motivation and engagement scale
instructional time. New York: Longman. and workbook: Testing and administration guidelines.
Fredricks, J., McColskey, W., Meli, J., Mordica, J., Lifelong Achievement Group, New South Wales,
Montrosse, B., & Mooney, K. (2011). Measuring stu- Australia.
dent engagement in upper elementary through high Martin, A. J. (2009). Motivation and engagement across the
school: A description of 21 instruments (Issues & academic life span: A developmental construct validity
Answers Report, REL 2011–No. 098). Washington, study of elementary school, high school, and university/
DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of college students. Educational and Psychological
Education Sciences, National Center for Education Measurement, 69, 794–824.
Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Regional Mosher, R., & McGowan, B. (1985). Assessing student
Educational Laboratory Southeast. Retrieved from engagement in secondary schools: Alternative con-
http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/edlabs. ceptions, strategies of assessing, and instruments.
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). University of Wisconsin, Research and Development
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of Center. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, ED 272812).
59–109. National Research Council and the Institute of Medicine.
Furrer, C. J., Skinner, E., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering high school
T. A. (2006, March). Engagement vs. disaffection as students’ motivation to learn. Washington, DC: The
central constructs in the dynamics of motivational National Academies Press.
development. Paper presented at the annual meeting of Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2007). The opti-
the Society for Research on Adolescence, San mum level of well-being: Can people be too happy?
Francisco, CA. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4),
Garnier, H., Stein, J., & Jacobs, J. (1997). The process of 346–360.
dropping out of high school: A 19-year perspective. Reschly, A. L., Betts, J., & Appleton, J. J. (2012). Student
American Educational Research Journal, 34(2), Engagement Instrument: Evidence of convergent and
395–419. divergent validity across measures of engagement and
motivation. Manuscript under review.
Hammond, C., Linton, D., Smink, J., & Drew, S. (2007).
Reschly, A. (2010). Reading and school completion:
Dropout risk factors and exemplary programs: A tech-
Critical connections and Matthew effects. Reading
nical report. Clemson, SC: National Dropout
and Writing Quarterly, 26, 1–23.
Prevention Center, Communities in Schools, Inc.
Reschly, A. L., Huebner, E. S., Appleton, J. J., &
Jimerson, S. R., Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A., & Carlson, E.
Antaramian, S. (2008). Engagement as flourishing:
(2000). A prospective longitudinal study of high
The role of positive emotions and coping in student
school dropouts: Examining multiple predictors across
engagement at school and with learning. Psychology
development. Journal of School Psychology, 38(6),
in the Schools, 45(5), 419–431.
525–549.
Reschly, A., & Christenson, S. L. (2006a). Prediction of
Jordan, W. J., McPartland, J. M., & Lara, J. (1999). dropout among students with mild disabilities: A case
Rethinking the causes of high school dropout. The for the inclusion of student engagement variables.
Prevention Researcher, 6, 1–4. Remedial and Special Education, 27, 276–292.
Kelley, T. L. (1927). Interpretation of educational mea- Reschly, A., & Christenson, S. L. (2006b). Promoting
surements. Yonkers, NY: World Book. school completion. In G. Bear & K. Minke (Eds.),
Lam, S., Wong, B. P. H., Yang, H., & Liu, Y. (2012). Children’s needs III: Understanding and addressing
Understanding student engagement with a contextual the developmental needs of children. Bethesda, MD:
model. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly & C. Wylie National Association of School Psychologists.
1 Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness… 19

Reynolds, A. J. (2001). Press release: Long-term effects of Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of
CPC program. Retrieved October 3, 2006, from http:// Educational Psychology, 100, 765–781.
www.waisman.wisc.edu/cls/PRESS01.PDF. Skinner, E. A., Kinderman, T. A., & Furrer, C. J. (2009).
Rosenthal, B. S. (1998). Non-school correlates of drop- A motivational perspective on engagement and disaf-
out: An integrative review of the literature. Children fection: Conceptualization and assessment of chil-
and Youth Services Review, 20, 413–433. dren’s behavioral and emotional participation in
Rumberger, R. W. (1995). Dropping out of middle school: academic activities in the classroom. Educational and
A multilevel analysis of students and schools. American Psychological Measurement, 69, 493–525.
Educational Research Journal, 32, 583–625. Thorndike, E. L. (1904). An introduction to the theory of
Rumberger, R. W., & Lim, S. A. (2008). Why students mental and social measurements. New York: Teachers
drop out of school: A review of 25 years of research College, Columbia University.
(California Dropout Research Project Report #15). Voelkl, K. E. (2012). School identification. In S. L.
University of California Santa Barbara. Christenson, A. L. Reschly & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). An overview of self- of research on student engagement (pp. 193–218).
determination theory: An organismic-dialectical per- New York: Springer.
spective. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook Waldrop, D., & Reschly, A. L. (2011, February).
of self-determination research (pp. 3–33). Rochester, Examining student engagement and motivation among
NY: University of Rochester Press. college students. Accepted presentation at the annual
Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D. P. (1999, September). The meeting of the National Association of School
advantages of High/Scope: Helping children lead suc- Psychologists. San Francisco, CA.
cessful lives. Educational Leadership, 57(1), 77–78. What Works Clearinghouse. (2006). Dropout prevention:
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Check & Connect. Institute of Education Sciences,
Positive psychology: An introduction. American Department of Education. Downloaded August 8, 2011,
Psychologist, 55, 5–14. from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/WWC_Check_
Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Evelo, D., & Hurley, C. Connect_092106.pdf.
(1998). Dropout prevention for high-risk youth with What Works Clearinghouse, Institute of Education
disabilities: Efficacy of a sustained school engagement Sciences, Department of Education. http://ies.ed.gov/
procedure. Exceptional Children, 65, 7–21. ncee/wwc/reports/advancedss.aspx.
Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Lehr, C. A., & Anderson, Wolters, C. A., & Taylor, D. J. (2012). A self-regulated
A. R. (2003). Facilitating 890 student engagement: learning perspective on student engagement. In S. L.
Lessons learned from check & connect longitudinal Christenson, A. L. Reschly & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook
studies. The California School Psychologist, 8, of research on student engagement (pp. 635–651). New
29–42. York: Springer.
Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., & Thurlow, M. L. Wylie, C., & Hodgen, E. (2012). Trajectories and patterns of
(2005). Promoting school completion of urban sec- student engagement: Evidence from a longitudinal
ondary youth with emotional or behavioral disabili- study. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly & C. Wylie
ties. Exceptional Children, 71, 465–482. (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement
Sinclair, M. F., & Kaibel, C. (2002). 2002 Dakota county: (pp. 585–599). New York: Springer.
Secondary Check & Connect program. Program eval- Yazzie-Mintz, E., & McCormick, K. (2012). Finding the
uation 2002 final summary report. Minneapolis, MN: humanity in the data: Understanding, measuring and
University of Minnesota, Institute on Community strengthening student engagement. In S. L.
Integration. Christenson, A. L. Reschly & C. Wylie (Eds.),
Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kinderman, T. Handbook of research on student engagement
(2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom: (pp. 743–761). New York: Springer.
Developmental Dynamics
of Student Engagement, Coping, 2
and Everyday Resilience

Ellen A. Skinner and Jennifer R. Pitzer

Abstract
The goal of this chapter is to present a perspective on student engagement
with academic work that emphasizes its role in organizing the daily
school experiences of children and youth as well as their cumulative
learning, long-term achievement, and eventual academic success. A model
grounded in self-determination theory, and organized around student
engagement and disaffection with learning activities, seems to offer
promise to the study of academic development by specifying the dynamic
cycles of context, self, action, and outcomes that are self-stablizing or
self-amplifying, and may underlie trajectories of motivation across many
school years. The study of ongoing engagement can be enriched by the
incorporation of concepts of everyday resilience, focusing on what hap-
pens when students make mistakes and encounter difficulties and failures
in school. The same personal and interpersonal resources that promote
engagement may shape students’ reactions to challenges and obstacles,
with academic coping an especially important bridge back to reengage-
ment. Future research can examine how these motivational dynamics
contribute to the development of durable academic assets, such as self-
regulated learning and proactive coping, and an academic identity that
allows students eventually to take ownership for their own learning and
success in school.

The last two decades have witnessed an explosion a malleable state that can be shaped by schools
of interest in the construct of academic engage- and a robust predictor of students’ learning,
ment, based on evidence that engagement is both grades, achievement test scores, retention, and
graduation (Appleton, Christenson & Furlong,
2008; Finn, 1993; Fredricks, Blumenfeld, &
E.A. Skinner, Ph.D. (*) • J.R. Pitzer, MS
Department of Psychology, Portland State University, Paris, 2004; Furlong & Christenson, 2008;
Portland, OR, USA Jimerson, Campos, & Grief, 2003; Klem &
e-mail: ellen.skinner@pdx.edu; jenniferpitzer@gmail.com Connell, 2004; Newmann, Wehlage, & Lamborn,

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 21


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_2, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
22 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

1992; National Research Council [NRC], 2004; includes an action component that shares core
Sinclair, Christenson, Lehr, & Anderson, 2003). features with engagement (Skinner et al., 2009a;
As enthusiasm for the notion of engagement has Wigfield, Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-
grown, however, so too has an appreciation for Kean, 2006).
the complexity of the construct (Appleton et al., Our motivational conceptualization is located
2008; Fredricks et al., 2004). Engagement not within a multilevel model of positive youth devel-
only has an intuitively appealing holistic mean- opment and resilience, which recognizes engage-
ing that focuses on the quality of a student’s ment with school and other prosocial institutions
involvement with school, but it also incorporates as a protective factor and a positive force in the
multiple distinguishable features, such as behav- lives of children and youth, especially those who
ioral, emotional, cognitive, and psychological are at risk for underachievement and dropout.
engagement. Definitions differ about whether to Engagement has been studied on at least four
include the opposite of engagement; some do, nested levels, as shown in Fig. 2.1. At the most
using labels such as disengagement, disaffection, general level, engagement refers to the involve-
alienation, or burnout (Miceli & Castelfranchi, ment of children and youth in school as a proso-
2000; Salmela-Aro, Kiuru, Leskinen, & Nurmi, cial institution, along with other institutions, such
2009; Vallerand et al., 1993). Conceptualizations as church, youth groups, and community organi-
disagree about the components that should be zations. This kind of engagement promotes posi-
incorporated into the construct proper—some tive youth development and protects children
include academic outcomes such as grades and from risks that emerge during early adolescence,
performance, whereas others include a student’s such as delinquency, gang involvement, substance
feelings of bonding, academic identity, or posi- use, and unsafe sexual activity (e.g., Morrison,
tive relationships with teachers and classmates. Robertson, Laurie & Kelly, 2002). At the second
As the popularity of engagement grows, it has level, engagement with school refers to the
become increasingly important for researchers to involvement of children and youth in school
clarify their conceptualizations, both the defini- activities, including academics, sports, band, stu-
tion of engagement itself and the larger assump- dent government, and extracurricular pursuits.
tions and models explaining how it operates. This kind of engagement promotes students’ com-
In our work, we view engagement as the out- pletion and graduation from high school, and pro-
ward manifestation of motivation (Skinner, tects against absenteeism and dropout.
Kindermann, Connell, & Wellborn, 2009a). At Nested within the classroom is the kind of
their heart, theories of motivation are most fun- engagement we are most interested in: student
damentally concerned with the psychological engagement with academic work, which we
processes that underlie energy, purpose, and define as constructive, enthusiastic, willing,
durability of human action (Deci, 1992a). emotionally positive, and cognitively focused
Engagement’s characteristic effort, exertion, participation with learning activities in school
vigor, intensity, vitality, zest, and enthusiasm are (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Skinner, Kindermann,
markers of energy; its interest, focus, and con- Connell, et al., 2009a; Skinner, Kindermann, &
centration are outward expressions of purpose or Furrer, 2009b). This kind of engagement is criti-
direction; and its absorption, determination, and cal for three reasons. First, it is a necessary con-
persistence are signs of durability. Motivation dition for students to learn. Only if students
refers to the underlying sources of energy, pur- participate in academic activities with both
pose, and durability, whereas engagement refers “hands-on” and “heads-on” will the time they
to their visible manifestation. That is why con- spend in classrooms result in the acquisition of
structs of engagement and disaffection have knowledge and skills. No matter how many extra-
always been central to theories of motivation. In curriculars students undertake or how attached
fact, every model of motivation in the field today they are to school, they will not learn or achieve
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 23

Fig. 2.1 A multilevel perspective on engagement with school that highlights student engagement with learning activities
as central to an understanding of the development of motivational dynamics

unless they are constructively engaged with the processes” that ecological models (e.g.,
academic work of the classroom. Engagement is Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998) posit are the
the active verb between the curriculum and actual primary engines of development. As a result,
learning. Engagement depicts the “proximal engagement is the direct (and only) pathway to
24 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

cumulative learning, long-term achievement, and that engagement is central to feed-forward and
eventual academic success. feedback loops that shape educational pathways.
Second, engagement shapes students’ every- Third, we explain how these cycles of engage-
day experiences in school, both psychologically ment may influence the development of every-
and socially. High-quality engagement and its day academic resilience, and specifically, how
resultant learning and scholastic success lead stu- children and youth cope with challenges and
dents to feel more academically competent and setbacks in school. We also speculate how these
connected, and elicit more positive interactions dynamics may cumulatively shape the develop-
and support from teachers. Moreover, engaged ment of important but elusive personal assets
students are allowed entry into friendships and and social resources at multiple points in a stu-
peer groups with more engaged classmates. In dent’s academic career. In the final section, we
contrast, disengaged students tend to perform explore some important implications for educa-
poorly in school and so feel marginalized, resentful, tional practice.
and ineffective. Teachers respond to such stu-
dents with less support and more coercion, and
disaffected students are more likely to join disen-
gaged peer groups and become friends with other Motivational Model of Context, Self,
disaffected students. Hence, students’ classroom Action, and Outcomes
engagement plays an important role in the quality
of their daily experiences while they are attending Engagement is the action component of our
school. model of motivational development (Connell &
Third, engagement is a critical contributor to Wellborn, 1991; Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000;
students’ academic development. Engagement is Skinner & Wellborn, 1994). In this context,
a part of the process of everyday academic resil- “action” refers to goal-directed emotion-infused
ience, and an energetic resource that helps stu- behaviors, reflecting the idea that actions are the
dents cope more adaptively with daily stressors, natural unit of analysis for conceptualizing trans-
challenges, and setbacks in school. From epi- actions between people and their social and phys-
sodes of effective coping may come the develop- ical contexts (Boesch, 1976; Brandtstädter, 1998;
ment of durable long-term motivational mindsets Chapman, 1984). Hence, engagement refers to
and skill sets, such as an autonomous learning energized, directed, and sustained action, or the
style or mastery orientation, self-regulated learning, observable qualities of students’ actual interac-
a positive academic identity, and eventually tions with academic tasks.
ownership for one’s own progress in high school As a result, as depicted in Fig. 2.2, the motiva-
(and beyond). Therefore, engagement can be tional conceptualization of engagement includes
seen as a key player in the development of aca- not only behavior but also emotion and cognitive
demic assets that takes place across the school orientation: the behavioral dimension of engage-
year and over the arc of a student’s entire educa- ment includes effort, intensity, persistence, deter-
tional career. mination, and perseverance in the face of
obstacles and difficulties; emotional or affective
engagement includes enthusiasm, enjoyment,
Purpose of the Chapter fun, and satisfaction; and cognitive engagement
encompasses attention, concentration, focus,
This chapter is structured around these themes, absorption, “heads-on” participation, and a will-
which we refer to collectively as the dynamics ingness to go beyond what is required. This
of motivational development. First, we provide conceptualization also includes the opposite of
our conceptualization of engagement and engagement, referred to as disaffection or burnout.
explain the larger motivational model that Motivational conceptualizations of disaffection
depicts its functioning. We then review evidence comprise the ways in which students withdraw
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 25

Engagement Disaffection
Action initiation Passivity, Procrastination
Behavior
Effort, Exertion Giving up
Initiation
Working hard Restlessness
Ongoing participation
Attempts Half-hearted
Re-engagement
Persistence Unfocused, Inattentive
Intensity Distracted
Focus, Attention Mentally withdrawn
Concentration Burned out, Exhausted
Absorption Unprepared
Involvement Absent
Enthusiasm Boredom
Emotion
Interest Disinterest
Initiation
Enjoyment Frustration/anger
Ongoing participation
Satisfaction Sadness
Re-engagement
Pride Worry/anxiety
Vitality Shame
Zest Self-blame
Purposeful Aimless
Cognitive Orientation
Approach Helpless
Initiation
Goal strivings Resigned
Ongoing participation
Strategy search Unwilling
Re-engagement
Willing participation Opposition
Preference for challenge Avoidance
Mastery Apathy
Follow-through, care Hopeless
Thoroughness Pressured

Fig. 2.2 A motivational conceptualization of engagement and disaffection in the classroom

from learning tasks, including physical with- Indicators Versus Facilitators


drawal of effort, such as lack of exertion, pas- of Engagement
sivity, merely going through the motions, or
exhaustion as well as their mental counterparts, In order to study how it functions, indicators of
such as lack of concentration, apathy, inattention, engagement must be distinguished from facilita-
or amotivation. Emotional reactions are critical tors of engagement (Sinclair et al., 2003). In gen-
components of disaffection because patterns of eral, indicators are markers or descriptive parts
action differ depending on whether lack of par- inside a target construct, whereas facilitators are
ticipation is based on boredom, anxiety, shame, explanatory causal factors, outside the target
sadness, or frustration. construct, that have the potential to influence the
26 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

target. For example, if a target of study is weight personal and social. Personal facilitators are
loss, then indicators of weight loss include pounds students’ self-perceptions or self-system pro-
on a scale, dimensions of the body, and the body cesses which refer to durable appraisals of mul-
mass index. Potential facilitators of weight loss tiple features of the self, such as self-efficacy or a
include a healthy diet and exercise. It is an empir- sense of belongingness in school. Social facilita-
ical question whether a particular pattern of eat- tors, also referred to as social contexts, are inter-
ing and exercise actually produces any weight personal interactions with important social
loss, and even if they are highly correlated, it partners, such as teachers, peers, and parents, and
does not mean that diet is part of weight loss. In include their quality and nature, such as whether
fact, it is essential to conceptually distinguish they are warm, dependable, or controlling.
them and to measure them separately, in order to Explanatory research and intervention efforts
determine whether the potential facilitators can require a clear demarcation between indicators
actually influence indicators of the target. Both and facilitators. If, for example, theories hold that
indicators and facilitators can be distinguished supportive interactions with teachers are an indi-
from the outcomes of engagement, which refer to cator of engagement itself, as opposed to a facili-
the results that engagement itself can produce. In tator that potentially contributes to engagement,
the weight loss example, outcomes or effects of research that combines these factors into a “meta-
weight loss might include lowered blood pressure construct” can never investigate whether teacher
or increased energy. It is an empirical question support influences student engagement. In order
whether weight loss can influence these out- to empirically explore whether interpersonal fac-
comes, however, and even if weight loss and out- tors and self-perceptions shape the development
comes are highly correlated, lowered blood of engagement and disaffection, it is essential to
pressure is not an indicator of weight loss. conceptualize and measure facilitators separately
Maintaining the distinctions among indica- from indicators.
tors, facilitators, and outcomes of engagement
can add clarity to conceptualizations and improve
studies of engagement. In the motivational model, Sources of Engagement:
indicators of engagement must be action compo- Self-determination Theory
nents, and so in addition to the behavioral, emo-
tional, and cognitive features of action described Many important facilitators and outcomes of
previously, we would accept as indicators of engagement have been integrated into a model
engagement other observable student interactions of positive motivational development grounded
with academic activities, such as on-task behav- in self-determination theory, called the Self-
ior or homework completion. In contrast, aca- System Model of Motivational Development
demic performance (grades on tests or homework, (SSMMD; Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Deci, Connell
semester grades, achievement test scores) would & Ryan, 1985; Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000; Deci,
not be indicators of engagement. They are poten- Vallerand, Pelletier & Ryan, 1991; Reeve, 2002;
tial outcomes. Any studies that measure engage- Ryan, Connell & Deci, 1985; Skinner &
ment by combining, for example, GPA with Wellborn, 1994). This model is rooted in organ-
on-task behavior, are confusing because they do ismic assumptions about intrinsic motivation,
not allow the examination of whether more on- asserting that “people are innately curious,
task behavior (an indicator) produces a higher interested creatures who possess a natural love
GPA (an outcome). of learning and who desire to internalize the
In work on engagement, the greatest confu- knowledge, customs, and values that surround
sion is between indicators and facilitators of them” (Niemiec & Ryan, 2009, p. 133). The core
engagement. Many conceptualizations and mea- idea is that humans come with basic needs, and
sures combine them. In the motivational model, when these needs are met by social contexts
we distinguish two kinds of potential facilitators: or activities, people will engage constructively
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 27

with them. When these needs are thwarted, Perceptions of self-efficacy, ability, academic
people become disaffected, that is, they withdraw, competence, and control are robust predictors of
escape, or act out. student engagement and eventual learning, aca-
The model posits three fundamental psycho- demic performance, and achievement (see
logical needs that are based in physiology and are Bandura, 1997; Dweck, 1999; Harter, 1982;
evolutionarily adaptive: the needs for relatedness, Skinner, 1995, 1996; Skinner, Zimmer-Gembeck,
competence, and autonomy. School contexts influ- & Connell, 1998; Stipek, 2002a; Weiner, 2005;
ence engagement by supporting (or undermining) Wigfield et al., 2006).
students’ experiences of themselves as related in Autonomy refers to the need to express one’s
school, as competent to succeed, and as autono- authentic self and to experience that self as the
mous or self-determined learners. From these source of action, and is hypothesized to under-
experiences, children cumulatively construct lie processes of self-determination (Deci &
views of themselves, referred to as self-system Ryan, 1985, 2000, 2002a). For autonomy, self-
processes (Connell & Wellborn, 1991). These system processes have been studied as auton-
beliefs are not fleeting self-perceptions; they are omy or goal orientations (Deci & Ryan, 1985,
durable convictions that shape apparent reality 1991; Dweck, 1991; Kuhl, 1987; Ryan &
and so guide action. Relatedness refers to the need Connell, 1989) and contain views about the self
to experience oneself as connected to other people, as motivated for self-determined or intrinsic
as belonging; it is hypothesized to underlie pro- reasons (or for extrinsic reasons). Students with
cesses of attachment (Ainsworth, 1979; Bowlby, a greater sense of autonomy in school also show
1969/1973; Bretherton, 1985; Crittenden, 1990) higher levels of classroom engagement, enjoy-
and has been studied across the lifespan as the ment, persistence, achievement, and learning (e.g.,
“need to belong” (Baldwin, 1992; Baumeister & Deci & Ryan, 2002b; Grolnick & Ryan, 1987;
Leary, 1995). Although relatedness is a relatively Hardre & Reeve, 2003; Miserandino, 1996; Otis,
recent addition to research in the academic domain, Grouzet, & Pelletier, 2005; Patrick, Skinner, &
studies find links between a sense of belonging in Connell, 1993; Vallerand, Fortier, & Guay, 1997;
school and multiple indicators of motivation, Vasalampi, Salmela-Aro, & Nurmi, 2009).
engagement, and adjustment (e.g., Anderman,
1999; Battistich, Solomon, Kim, Watson, &
Schnaps, 1995; Booker, 2006; Eccles & Midgley, Schools, Teachers, Peers, Parents,
1989; Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Goodenow, 1993; and the Social Context
Kuperminc, Blatt, Shahar, Henrich, & Leadbetter,
2004; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1992, 1997; Roeser, Although all children and youth come with the
Midgley, & Urdan, 1996; Ryan, Stiller, & Lynch, needs for relatedness, competence, and autonomy,
1994; Wentzel, 1997, 1998, 1999). they act on the motivations provided by these
Competence refers to the need to experience needs in social contexts, like schools, that are dif-
oneself as effective in one’s interactions with the ferentially responsive to them. The motivational
social and physical environments (Elliot & model emphasizes the importance of supportive
Dweck, 2005; Harter, 1978; Koestner & interactions with teachers, peers, and parents,
McClelland, 1990; White, 1959) and is hypothe- and intrinsically interesting academic work.
sized to underlie processes of control (Bandura,
1997; Peterson, Maier, & Seligman, 1993; Teachers Shape Engagement
Seligman, 1975). For competence, self-system According to the model, three important qualities
processes have been studied as perceptions of of student-teacher interactions are pedagogical
control (Bandura, 1997; Dweck, 1991; Heckhausen caring (which supports experiences of related-
& Schultz, 1995; Skinner, 1996; Weisz, 1986); ness), optimal structure (which facilitates compe-
these are perhaps the most frequently studied aca- tence), and autonomy support (which promotes
demic self-perceptions (Wigfield et al., 2006). self-determined motivation). Research validates
28 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

the notion that all three are important in shaping & Jiao, 1999; Berndt & Keefe, 1995; Berndt,
motivation and engagement in the classroom Laychak & Park, 1990). Moreover, studies of nat-
(Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Murray & Greenberg, urally occurring peer groups also suggest that
2000; Pianta, 1999, 2006; Ryan & Stiller, 1991; peers influence students’ motivation, behavior, and
Stipek, 2002b; Wentzel, 1998, 2009; Wigfield achievement in school (e.g., Cairns, Neckerman &
et al., 2006). Early work showed that properly Cairns, 1989; Chen, Chang & He, 2003; Estell,
structured classrooms promote student motivation Farmer, Cairns & Cairns, 2002; Gest, Rulison,
(e.g., Ames & Ames, 1985; Rosenholtz & Wilson, Davidson & Welsh, 2008; Kindermann, 1993,
1980). Subsequently, the quality of student-teacher 2007; Kindermann, McCollam & Gibson, 1996;
relationships, in the form of caring supportive alli- Kindermann & Skinner, 2009, in press).
ances, was emphasized as a predictor of motiva-
tion and achievement (Birch & Ladd, 1997, 1998; Parents Shape Engagement
Goodenow, 1993; Murray & Murray, 2004; Ryan Following up on the large body of work demon-
& Powelson, 1991). Recently, autonomy support- strating a connection between parenting practices
ive instruction (giving choices, making learning and school achievement, studies are accumulating
relevant) has been linked to engagement (Deci & which suggest that one pathway through which
Ryan 2002b Guthrie & Davis, 2003; Reeve, Jang, parenting has an impact on children’s school per-
Carrell, Jeon & Barch, 2004). formance is by shaping children’s classroom
The model focuses on all three facets of engagement, intrinsic motivation, preference for
teacher support: warmth, provision of structure, challenge, valuing and commitment to school,
and autonomy support, all of which have been and enthusiasm, enjoyment, and interest in school-
shown to contribute to students’ positive self- work (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Epstein &
perceptions as well as to classroom engagement Sanders, 2002; Ginsberg & Bronstein, 1993;
(e.g., Skinner & Belmont, 1993). Close and caring Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 1994; Grolnick
relationships with teachers and other adults in & Ryan, 1989, 1992; Grolnick, Ryan, & Deci,
school have been shown to be an important 1991; Grolnick & Slowiaczek, 1994; Jeynes,
predictor of student engagement across race, eth- 2007; Pomerantz, Grolnick, & Price, 2005;
nicity, and class (e.g., Brewster & Bowen, 2004; Reynolds & Clements, 2005; Steinberg, Elmen, &
Connell, Halpern-Felsher, Clifford, Crichlow, & Mounts, 1989; Wigfield et al., 2006). Longitudinal
Usinger, 1995; Connell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; studies of the motivational mediators between
Garcia-Reid, Reid & Peterson, 2002; Wooley & authoritative parenting and children’s school per-
Bowen, 2007). formance are especially informative (e.g.,
Steinberg et al., 1989) as are studies that examine
Peers Shape Engagement parents’ use of specific motivational practices
In addition to teachers, peers and parents also (e.g., Gottfried, Marcoulides, Gottfried, & Oliver,
influence student motivation and engagement 2009; Grolnick & Slowiaczek, 1994).
(Wentzel, 1998). Although many studies high-
light negative developmental influences from The Nature of Academic Work
friends, in recent years, an increasing number Especially important determinants of motivation
show that children’s friendships in school can and engagement are the academic tasks students
also exert positive effects on academic develop- undertake in the classroom (Newmann, King, &
ment (e.g., Altermatt & Pomeranz, 2003; Hallinan Carmichael, 2007; Newmann et al., 1992;
& Williams, 1990; Kandel, 1978; Ladd, 1990; Wigfield et al., 2006). Because learning activities
Ladd, Kochenderfer & Coleman, 1997; Ryan, are the “interaction partners” with which students
2001; Wentzel, McNamara-Barry, & Caldwell, engage, their qualities influence the nature of the
2004; for a review, see Bukowski, Motzoi & interaction. Hence, active participation, engage-
Meyer, 2009), especially school motivation and ment, and effort are promoted by tasks that are
achievement (e.g., Berndt, 2004; Berndt, Hawkins hands-on, heads-on, project-based, relevant,
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 29

progressive, and integrated across subject matter, critical mechanism through which motivational
or in other words, intrinsically motivating, inher- processes contribute to learning and achievement.
ently interesting, and fun (Deci, 1992b, 1998;
Renninger, 2000). Authentic work is a term used
to characterize “tasks that are considered mean- Reciprocal Feedback Effects
ingful, valuable, significant, and worthy of one’s of Engagement
effort, in contrast to those considered nonsen-
sical, useless, contrived, trivial, and therefore As can be seen in Fig. 2.3, engagement not only
unworthy of effort” (Newmann et al., 1992, p. 23). contributes to students’ subsequent learning and
By connecting to the “real world” beyond school, performance, but it has a reciprocal connection to
such tasks offer students a sense of purpose and teachers, parents, and peers. The key idea is that
ownership (Newmann et al., 2007). students’ motivation, as expressed through their
engagement, is salient to their social partners and
so has an impact on the way that others respond
to them. Most of the research that links motiva-
Motivational Dynamics tional support (from teachers, parents, or peers)
of Engagement and Disaffection to student engagement is correlational and cross-
sectional, and is typically interpreted as reflecting
The motivational model is depicted graphically in the feed-forward effects of social partners on stu-
Fig. 2.3. According to the model, school contexts dents’ motivation. However, a few experimental
differentially provide children and youth with and longitudinal studies have been conducted
opportunities to fulfill their fundamental psycho- which show that adults respond to children dif-
logical needs (through provision of warmth/ ferentially depending on their on-task, engaged,
involvement, structure, and autonomy support). or disruptive behaviors, and that children join or
Based on these experiences, students construct are allowed entry into friendships and peer groups
self-system processes which are organized around based on their engagement in school.
relatedness, competence, and autonomy. These
self-system processes in turn provide a motiva- Effects of Engagement on Teachers
tional basis for their patterns of engagement Only a few studies have explicitly investigated
versus disaffection with learning activities. whether students’ engagement shapes how
Constructive engagement is considered to be a teachers subsequently respond to them (Furrer &

Teacher Parent Peer


CONTEXT SELF ACTION OUTCOMES

Warmth
vs. Relatedness
Rejection Engagement
vs.
Disaffection
Structure Learning
vs. Competence and
Chaos Achievement
COPING

Autonomy
Support Adaptive
vs. Autonomy Maladaptive
Coercion

Fig. 2.3 A dynamic model of motivational development organized around student engagement and disaffection
30 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

Skinner, 2009; Pelletier & Vallerand, 1996). For selection effects, or how children enter and leave
example, kindergarteners who were more behav- friendship and peer relationships. The key idea is
iorally engaged in the classroom tended to that children select and are selected by other
develop closer relationships with their teachers children based in part on their engagement in
over time than did those who were less engaged school, with more engaged children and youth
(Ladd, Birch, & Buhs, 1999). Similarly, elemen- joining peer and friendship groups with more
tary school students (in grades 3 through 5) with engaged peers, and more disaffected children and
higher behavioral engagement in the fall experi- youth joining groups of more disaffected peers.
enced increases in teacher support over the school Evidence comes from cross-sectional studies
year, and students with higher emotional engage- showing that students’ own levels of engagement
ment experienced increases in teacher autonomy are correlated with those of their friendship
granting as the year progressed (Furrer & Skinner, networks and peer groups (Kindermann &
2009; Skinner & Belmont, 1993). In the same Skinner, in press), and longitudinal studies which
vein, two observational studies, one of middle show that despite high turnover in actual members
schoolers (Altermatt, Jovanovic, & Perry, 1998) over a school year, there is relatively high sta-
and one of junior high and high schoolers (Fiedler, bility in the motivational composition (average
1975), revealed that students who showed more levels of engagement) of children’s peer groups
participation in class elicited greater teacher (Kindermann, 1993, 2007). Taken together, this
responsiveness. work suggests that children who are more engaged
join peer and friendship networks of other chil-
Effects of Engagement on Parents dren who are likewise more engaged in school.
A growing body of research also examines chil-
dren’s effects on their parents (Bell, 1968, 1979;
Patterson, 1982). A portion of this research looks Cycles of Engagement and Disaffection
directly at parental reactions to children who are
resistant, unresponsive, uncooperative, or off- Motivational dynamics involve the feed-forward
task (or who are perceived to be so), and suggests and feedback causal effects among context, self,
that parents respond to such children by with- action, and outcomes, which result in feedback
drawing their involvement or becoming more loops or “cycles” of engagement. Supportive
controlling (power assertive and coercive; interactions with teachers, parents, and peers
Anderson, Lytton, & Romney, 1996; Grolnick & contribute to positive self-perceptions, which
Apostoleris, 2002; Patterson, 1982). Especially promote student engagement with interesting
interesting are the few experimental studies in and meaningful academic activities—which facil-
which child behavior was manipulated or itates learning and the development of compe-
assigned. In one study, children ages 9–11 were tence. High-quality engagement and achievement
trained as confederates to be difficult, uncoopera- in turn bolster students’ positive self-perceptions,
tive, and disinterested (versus easy, cooperative, elicit further teacher and parent support, and
and interested); mothers who were trying to teach allow children to join networks of engaged peers
children anagrams were more controlling with and friends. In contrast, unsupportive interper-
the “difficult” children (Jelsma, 1982). Taken sonal interactions or perceptions of the self as
together, these studies suggest that students’ aca- unwelcome, incompetent, or pressured in school
demic engagement is likely to shape how adults, lead to disaffection—which undermines learning
both teachers and parents, respond to them. and achievement. Disaffection and failure in turn
undercut students’ sense of self, can result in
Effects of Engagement on Peers withdrawal of support or increasing coercion
In research on the effects of children’s friendships from teachers and parents, and lead children
and peer groups on their academic performance, to join more disengaged friendship and peer
a few studies examine what are referred to as groups.
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 31

These feedback loops are self-amplifying, Although it can be tempting to interpret such
forming virtuous or vicious cycles that magnify high cross-time correlations as evidence that
initial individual differences across time, making engagement is a fixed motivational trait, research
motivationally “rich” students richer, and motiva- on the dynamics of engagement contradict this
tionally “poor” students poorer. Studies examin- conclusion. Taken together, studies demonstrate
ing engagement at multiple time points have that engagement is a malleable state, open to con-
empirically captured some of these dynamics, textual conditions, that can be shaped by interper-
some involving motivational resources, such as sonal and task characteristics. Dynamic stability
perceived control (e.g., Schmitz & Skinner, is continually recreated by the feedback loops
1993), achievement (e.g., Gottfried, Marcoulides, between students’ engaged and disaffected actions,
Gottfried, Oliver, & Guerin, 2007), or teacher on the one hand, and their facilitators and out-
support (e.g., Altermatt et al., 1998; Fiedler, comes, on the other, including the context created
1975; Skinner & Belmont, 1993), and some by teachers, parents, peers, and the nature of aca-
involving multiple components (e.g., Skinner, demic work, students’ self-perceptions, and their
Furrer, Marchand, & Kindermann, 2008; Skinner performance outcomes. It is the thousands of epi-
et al., 1998). Although other kinds of cycles are sodes of engaged participation or disaffected
theoretically possible, all the dynamics that have withdrawal that organize these feedback loops,
been documented so far have turned out to be which is why engagement is a sensitive indicator
self-amplifying or self-stabilizing, in that they of the state of the whole motivational system.
magnify or verify the pattern of individual differ-
ences present in the initial conditions.

Trajectories of Engagement Engagement and the Development


These dynamics may be responsible for the high of Coping and Everyday Resilience
stability of engagement and disaffection, and
may underlie interindividual differences in tra- Cycles of ongoing engagement also create a moti-
jectories of motivation over a student’s school vational context that may shape how students deal
career. Although there is an overall normative with everyday difficulties, challenges, and obsta-
decline in engagement across school years cles in school. As studied under the name “every-
(Wigfield et al., 2006), research also documents a day resilience” or “academic buoyancy” (Martin
high level of interindividual stability. That is, & Marsh, 2006, 2008a, 2008b, 2009), these pro-
children’s levels of engagement at the beginning cesses refer to resources students can access to
of the school year are highly correlated with their help them bounce back from setbacks and fail-
levels at the end of the school year (e.g., Skinner ures, and allow them to constructively reengage
& Belmont, 1993); engagement during one grade with challenging academic tasks after running
is highly correlated with engagement in neigh- into obstacles or problems. Academic buoyancy
boring grades (e.g., Gottfried, Fleming, & refers to “students’ ability to successfully deal
Gottfried, 2001); and children’s engagement in with academic setbacks and challenges that are
the early elementary school years is highly cor- typical of the ordinary course of school life (e.g.,
related with their engagement in middle school poor grades, competing deadlines, exam pres-
(e.g., Gottfried et al., 2007; Skinner et al., 1998) sure, difficult schoolwork)” (Martin & Marsh,
and high school (Gottfried et al., 2001; Marks, 2008a, p. 72). The motivational model suggests
2000; Otis et al., 2005). In fact, in the few studies that both interpersonal resources, such as teacher
comparing such relations, interindividual stability warmth or peer engagement, and personal
seems to increase as students move through resources, such as a sense of competence, relat-
junior high and high school (Gottfried, 1990; edness, and autonomy, are assets that can support
Gottfried et al., 2001). everyday resilience and reengagement.
32 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

Academic Coping as a Mechanism Emergence of Academic Resources


of Everyday Resilience for Resilience

A primary process of resilience in school is cop- Over time, ongoing engagement, constructive
ing, which describes how students deal with chal- coping, and reengagement following failures and
lenges, threats, and failures in their daily setbacks may work together to shape children’s
experiences with academic tasks (Skinner & academic development. The central idea is that
Wellborn, 1994, 1997). Work on coping is distin- these cycles of engagement and coping, over
guished by its focus on what children and youth months or years, give rise to the development of
actually do in their real-life encounters with qualitatively different mindsets and skill sets at
stressful events. These reactions can be classified different ages. For example, early research on
into families of coping, such as problem-solving, participation-identification models of engage-
support seeking, or escape (Skinner, Edge, ment argued that positive patterns of engagement
Altman, & Sherwood, 2003). Many of these ways lead to a sense of belonging in school and valuing
of coping have been studied individually, but of school-related goals (Finn, 1989). And reviews
when considered as a profile or repertoire of ways of coping show that (compared with younger
of coping, it is possible to examine how they children) older children are able to use more
work together cumulatively as a series of adap- complex cognitive coping strategies and to more
tive (or maladaptive) responses to problems and flexibly match the demands of the stressor to the
difficulties with schoolwork or other stressful family of coping (Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner,
events in school. 2011).
A developmental model has identified a dozen Although educators and parents stress how
families of coping (Skinner et al., 2003), some important it is for students to take responsibility
of which promote reengagement (e.g., problem- or ownership for their own academic progress,
solving or help seeking) and some of which lead very little is known about how and at what ages
to giving up (e.g., helplessness or social isola- specific qualitativly new resources emerge dur-
tion) or getting in trouble (e.g., delegation or ing a student’s scholastic career. It is clear that
opposition). Help seeking seems to be an espe- some qualitative growth must be taking place, in
cially adaptive strategy for dealing with prob- that kindergarten and first-grade students do not
lems (Newman, 1994, 2000). In fact, it is the have the means to form a complex academic
most common all-purpose strategy used by chil- identity, use sophisticated cognitive strategies,
dren (Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, 2011) and a or flexibly regulate their own learning.
common way of coping even for adolescents and Researchers have begun to identify some of the
adults (Skinner et al., 2003). One reason it is so cognitive and meta-cognitive abilities students
adaptive is that interactions with competent and need to become more proactive, self-reliant, and
supportive social partners (like teachers) can autonomous in their own learning (Otis et al.,
help students reengage with difficult material 2005; Schunk & Zimmerman, 2007) and in their
and eventually develop strategies like problem- adaptive help seeking (Newman, 2002), but little
solving and self-reliance that they can then research examines the effects of these underly-
employ in dealing with (or preventing) subse- ing processes on students’ development.
quent stressors (Nelson-Le Gall, Gumerman, & Early adolescence seems to be a key develop-
Scott-Jones, 1983). Unfortunately, over the mental period for students to construct an iden-
same age range that children and adolescents tity as academically capable, socially integrated,
show declines in motivation, they also evince and committed to learning (Roeser, Peck, &
declines in the use of help seeking (Marchand & Nasir, 2006; Wentzel, 1991), but it is possible
Skinner, 2007; Newman, 2002; Ryan, Patrick, that qualitative changes in academic resources
& Shim, 2005). occur at other points as well, for example, during
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 33

the five to seven shift (Sameroff & Haith, 1996) test scores cannot be considered a success if they
or the third-grade shift. One indicator of a transi- come at the cost of undermining engagement
tion might be steeper rates of normative decline and increasing student disaffection. The good
in engagement, signaling a window of opportu- news is that constructive engagement, when
nity as well as of vulnerability. A noticeable trend combined with a challenging curriculum and
in findings from the study of all such forms of authentic learning activities, creates opportuni-
potential academic development is that these ties for increased learning and so is a direct
desirable attributes are quite rare even in older pathway to better performance. It is important to
academically successful students (Miserandino, include the entire complex construct of engage-
1996). Future research can examine how positive ment in target outcomes. Teachers and parents
motivational dynamics may contribute to the can easily focus on only the behavioral compo-
development of self-regulated learning and pro- nent—on-task behavior—and lose track of emo-
active coping, and an academic identity that tion, cognition, and orientation, as embodied, for
allows students to eventually take ownership for example, by enthusiasm, interest, excitement, will-
their own learning and success in school. ingness, preference for challenge, and “heads-on”
participation. Although behavioral engagement
seems to be the primary driver of actual perfor-
mance, emotion is likely the fuel for the kind of
Educational Implications behavioral and cognitive engagement that leads to
for Promoting Engagement, high-quality learning (Skinner et al., 2008).
Coping, and Everyday Resilience
Tracking Engagement
The motivational model of engagement and dis- Additional good news is that the action compo-
affection inspired by self-determination theory nent of student engagement with academic work
has several important implications for the struc- is directly observable, and so teachers can track it
turing of learning environments (see also Deci, at the classroom level (Reeve et al., 2004) or at
Connell, & Ryan, 1985; Niemiec & Ryan, 2009; the level of individual students (Skinner et al.,
Reeve, 2002) and comprehensive school reform 2009a, 2009b). The positive and significant cor-
(Connell, Klem, Lacher, Leiderman, & Moore, relations between teachers’ ratings of engage-
2009; Deci, 2009). The most important is the ment and both student ratings and observers’
core assumption that all students come with a reports indicate that teachers seem to do this
wellspring of intrinsic motivation that does not spontaneously and accurately, suggesting that
have to be acquired and cannot be lost. However, student engagement is a source of information
steady declines in students’ intrinsic motivation available to teachers in designing and delivering
and engagement signal that schools are not nur- their lesson plans. Student engagement with
turing this precious energetic resource (Eccles learning activities is a marker of the whole moti-
et al., 1993; Wigfield et al., 2006). We highlight vational system and so provides teachers a diag-
three important antidotes (see Fig. 2.4). nostic window into other important motivational
processes that are not directly observable, such as
students’ self-system processes of belonging,
Focus on Engagement and Disaffection competence, or value (Furrer, Kelly, & Skinner,
2003). Researchers and interventionists who want
The motivational model encourages schools and to support students’ motivation and learning can
teachers, when formulating their target out- also take advantage of engagement as a key sum-
comes, to insist on a dual focus on learning and mary marker of the quality of students’ school
engagement. High grades or high achievement experiences.
34 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

Focus on Engagement and Disaffection


1. Adopt as a central goal the promotion of engagement in academic work, tracking
especially student orientation, emotional, and cognitive engagement, as expressed
through student enthusiasm, interest, excitement, willingness, preference for
challenge, and “heads-on” participation.
2. Use student disaffection as a diagnostic tool signaling that a student needs more
warmth, involvement, structure, and/or autonomy support. View students’
misunderstandings and failures as opportunities for students to learn something new
about the subject and about how to cope more constructively.
3. Provide academic tasks that are authentic, challenging, relevant to students’
experiences and concerns, hands-on, project-based, integrated across subject areas,
and that allow students some freedom to choose their own direction and to work
closely in cooperative groups over long periods of time.
Focus on the Social Learning Environment
1. Promote students’ intrinsic motivation, by offering challenging and fun learning
activities, allowing and encouraging students to discover and follow their own
interests and goals, and providing clear instruction and feedback about how to reach
them.
2. Meet students’ needs for relatedness, competence, and autonomy: Foster caring
relationships (warmth and involvement), provide challenging learning activities with
high expectations and clear feedback (optimal structure), and explain the relevance
and importance of activities and rules while soliciting input from students and
respecting their opinions (autonomy support).
3. Promote classroom goals that focus on mastery, by creating a climate that
emphasizes hard work, sustained effort, self-improvement, deep understanding, and
the recognition that “mistakes,” “setbacks,” and “failures” can be interesting detours
and good information about next steps.
Focus on Teachers
1. Model your own engagement in teaching, by showing your enthusiasm, hard work,
careful thought, and excitement about a subject area. Model constructive coping in
the classroom. Admit mistakes and tell stories of your own past failures and
struggles.
2. View student amotivation as a fascinating challenge, a puzzle to be solved, and an
opportunity to learn more about teaching and more about coping successfully with
challenging students.
3. Remember that teachers have their own needs for relatedness, competence, and
autonomy, and when they are met, it provides opportunities for more constructive
engagement and coping, everyday resilience, vigor, vitality, and the development of
teaching expertise.

Fig. 2.4 Educational practices that promote the development of engagement, coping, and everyday resilience

Coping with Student Disaffection their support or increasing coercion. In other


and Failure words, teachers typically respond in ways that
Just because teachers are accurate monitors of are likely to further undermine students’ engage-
engagement and disaffection does not mean that ment, making matters worse. Little research exam-
they always respond to students’ motivation in ines the mindsets or contextual conditions that
the optimal fashion. In fact, as described previ- would allow teachers to react to disengaged stu-
ously, the feedback loops from student engage- dents with increased warmth, involvement, and
ment to teacher support found in several studies autonomy support. Perhaps teachers could respond
suggest that teachers typically react to students’ more positively if they could see student disaffec-
disaffection in the classroom by withdrawing tion, not as a personal insult to them or a character
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 35

flaw in the student, but as a handy diagnostic tool in science and English class during middle school,
signaling times when a student is encountering and a radio program covering the Red Scare of
resistance and need more support. It might be the 1920s performed in high school. Ten, twenty,
likewise helpful if teachers could see students’ thirty years later, these experiences evoke smiles
misunderstandings and failures, not as shortcom- and detailed indelible memories of wholehearted
ings of teacher or student, but as opportunities for engagement. Our research group is currently
students to learn something new about the subject studying the effects of garden-based science
and about how to deal more constructively with education programs for at-risk middle school
challenging learning tasks. students—and finding that the holistic, authen-
tic, cooperative, fun, environmentally friendly
The Nature of Academic Work activities of gardening promote both students’
For educators and researchers interested in class- engagement and their achievement (Ratcliffe,
room engagement, it is evident that the primary Goldberg, Rogers, & Merrigan, 2010; Skinner,
interaction partners for students, if they are to Chi, & the LEAG, 2012).
learn, are the academic tasks that we require them
to undertake as part of the curriculum in schools.
The nature of these learning activities is a defini- Focus on the Social Learning
tive determinant of students’ intrinsic interest and Environment
can make much easier (or much harder) the job of
the teacher in facilitating motivation. Curricula Formal classroom curricula are essential, of
and academic tasks will naturally arouse intrinsic course, but so too are the informal or tacit curri-
motivation the more they are authentic, challeng- cula—answers to the questions: What are we
ing, relevant to students’ experiences and con- doing here? What is the purpose of school?
cerns, hands-on or project-based, integrated across Although it seems obvious—we are here to
subject areas and into students’ real lives, and learn—research on goal orientations over the last
reflect students’ own interests and goals—in other 25 years eloquently demonstrates that teachers
words, are fun and interesting (Deci & Ryan, and schools seem to be consistently communicat-
1985; Newmann et al., 1992). Complex learning ing to students, especially as they grow older, that
environments, which include project-based curri- schools have an agenda that is not fully aligned
cula, integrated across subject matter, that allow with learning and mastery (e.g., Ames, 1992;
students some freedom to chose their direction Midgley & Edelin, 1998; Roeser et al., 1996).
and to work closely in cooperative groups over Although questions remain about the exact mean-
long periods of time, awaken and sustain students’ ing of achievement goal constructs (Hulleman,
natural curiosity and love of learning. Schrager, Bodman, & Harachiewicz, 2010), it is
In general, these are the learning environments clear that engagement, joy, high-quality concep-
provided by high-quality preschools and gra- tual learning, creativity, and constructive coping
duate schools, two levels of schooling at which are all undermined by the external and internal
intrinsic motivation and engagement flourish. pressures created by a focus on performance and
Unfortunately, they are not the norm for the grades grades, the evaluation of fixed abilities, and the
in between. However, simply ask any adults about shame and embarrassment of mistakes and fail-
their favorite memories of school (as we recently ures (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Dweck,
did in our research group) and you will find that 1991; Hulleman et al., 2010; Pintrich, 2003).
they nevertheless do appear as individual unfor- A complement to curricula designed to tap intrin-
gettable experiences. We heard enthusiastic tales sic motivation is the establishment of a classroom
of an opera written and performed by third grad- climate focused on mastery, that is, hard work,
ers, the creation of an Egyptian museum in ele- sustained effort, self-improvement, deep under-
mentary school, a Japanese tea house in sixth standing, the unshakable conviction that everyone
grade, a CSI-type investigation of a “dead” body can excel, and the recognition that “mistakes,”
36 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

“setbacks,” and “failures” can not only be inter- College, 2000). Teachers’ hard work and careful
esting detours but are also informative about next thought can communicate the importance and
steps in one’s own thinking and progress. value of knowledge and skills. Perhaps most
important are the ways in which teachers model
Teacher-Student Interactions how to deal with roadblocks, confusion, and
as Facilitators of Engagement mistakes. Teachers can demonstrate constructive
The nature of the interactions teachers have with coping through such simple (and challenging)
their students can shape student engagement in means as admitting that they do not know some-
the classroom in at least two ways. The first is by thing or that their own current understandings
promoting students’ intrinsic motivation: by can sometimes be contradictory and uncertain,
offering challenging and fun learning activities, and then taking the time to straighten them out or
allowing and encouraging students to discover to find out more, by identifying areas of confu-
and follow their own interests and goals, and sion and consulting resources or experts.
providing clear instruction and feedback about Constructive coping can also involve telling sto-
how to reach them. The second is by creating ries of one’s own past failures and mistakes, as
classroom contexts that support the development inspiration for students who are currently strug-
of increasingly more self-determined reasons for gling. Compared to the effects of parents
accomplishing the parts of learning that are not (Bradley, 2007; Power, 2004), much less research
intrinsically fun. All worthwhile tasks involve a examines how teachers can promote the devel-
mix of inspiration and perspiration, and self- opment of constructive coping and everyday
determination theory posits that activities that resilience in their students, making this a fruitful
are extrinsically motivated can nevertheless be area for research.
completed autonomously if students identify
with their value and relevance (Ryan, 1995; Teacher Motivation and Engagement
Ryan & Connell, 1989). Students are more likely The motivational model holds that teachers have
to internalize autonomous reasons for complet- the same needs as students and so provides a
ing extrinsically motivated tasks in school when useful lens through which to hypothesize about
they learn from teachers who display the three the effects of students’ motivational problems on
features of motivational support described pre- teachers. If teachers experience low student
viously: when teachers foster caring relation- motivation as an obstacle to their teaching and
ships (warmth and involvement), provide lesson plans, then it thwarts teacher autonomy. If
challenging learning activities with high expec- it is perceived as a signal that teachers are bad at
tations and clear feedback (optimal structure), teaching, then it undercuts teachers’ sense of
and explain the relevance and importance of competence. If it is seen as evidence that stu-
activities and rules while soliciting input from dents don’t like the teacher, it can undermine
students and respecting their opinions (auton- teachers’ feelings of relatedness. According to
omy support) (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Deci the motivational model, any of these interpreta-
& Ryan, 2000). tions should lead teachers to become disaffected
from the target students, and could produce the
withdrawal, hostility, or coercion found in stud-
Focus on Teacher Motivation, ies of the reciprocal effects of student motivation
Engagement, Coping, and Resilience on teacher behaviors. If, however, in contrast,
teachers can see student amotivation as a fasci-
Teachers can facilitate students’ engagement and nating challenge, an interesting puzzle which
constructive coping directly through their own they are confident they can solve, then the bore-
actions and modeling in the classroom. Teachers’ dom, passivity, or disruptive behavior students
enthusiasm and excitement about a subject can show in class can be opportunities for teachers to
be contagious (Patrick, Hisley, Kempler, & learn more about teaching and more about how
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 37

to cope successfully with challenging students motivational model it inspires offer an alternative
(Hakanen, Bakker, & Schaufeli, 2006; Martin & vision (Connell et al., 2009; Deci, 2009).
Marsh, 2008b). In the current chapter, we have attempted to
show how a motivational model grounded in self-
determination theory can be used as a framework
Teachers Within the Larger School to both clarify and enrich the study of student
Context engagement. We suggest that, within a multilevel
Student engagement is a precious energetic perspective on engagement, student constructive
resource, not only for students, but also for teach- participation in academic work enjoys a privi-
ers’ own enjoyment and engagement in teaching. leged status as the focus of research on engage-
When students are trying hard, taking on chal- ment because it is the only gateway to learning
lenges, seeking and providing help, and making and scholastic development. We have empha-
strides in their learning, teachers remember why sized the importance of distinguishing indicators
they decided to become teachers in the first place. of engagement from its facilitators, and along
The research on reciprocal effects suggests that with many other researchers, we favor indicators
teacher and student engagement can create a vir- of engagement as an action construct that capture
tuous circle—one that supports both partners its behavioral, cognitive, and emotional facets.
(and by implication the whole classroom) in self- We have suggested sets of important social and
stabilizing cycles of hard work, joy, and learning, personal facilitators that highlight the nature of
as well as increasing feelings of connectedness academic work, and include many of the self-
to each other as a learning community, compe- system processes studied in research on motiva-
tence in learning and teaching, and autonomy tion today. Facilitators also take into account a
toward the activities and enterprise of schooling. range of interpersonal relationships that can sat-
Comprehensive school reforms based on self- isfy or undermine students’ needs for relatedness,
determination theory have the goal of creating competence, and autonomy, including interac-
such vibrant self-renewing communities, and tions with parents, friends, and peer group mem-
highlight the larger contextual supports that need bers, but emphasizing as fundamental students’
to be in place to create and sustain them (Connell relationships with their teachers.
et al., 2009; Deci, 2009). The episodes of students’ daily lives in school,
which are shaped by their engagement and disaf-
fection, have only recently become the focus of
research on the development of motivational
Conclusion dynamics. However, such dynamics hold promise
for helping to explain the durability of students’
For many schools and teachers, the creation and motivation across the school year and for identi-
continual renegotiation of an intrinsically moti- fying underlying processes that contribute to
vating curriculum and a supportive classroom interindividual trajectories of motivation across
climate may appear to require too much work and multiple years. We have suggested directions for
coordination among teachers, and to produce too future research that can examine the role that
uncertain a path to the achievement test scores cycles of engagement may play in the emergence
upon which evaluations and accountability of of everyday resilience and constructive coping.
teachers and schools are now based (Ryan & Taken together, these ideas may provide tools to
Brown, 2005). However, the downward spirals of help researchers explore and educators nurture the
student and teacher engagement, the draining long-term development of valuable (but rare) aca-
away of students’ intrinsic motivation, and the demic assets, such as self-regulated and autono-
rates of student dropout and teacher burnout, are mous learning, and an academic identity and sense
all reminders of the costs associated with the cur- of purpose that allow students to take ownership
rent situation. Self-determination theory and the for their own progress in school and beyond.
38 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

Berndt, T. J., Laychak, A. E., & Park, K. (1990). Friends’


References influence on adolescents’ academic achievement moti-
vation: An experimental study. Journal of Educational
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1979). Infant–mother attachment. Psychology, 82, 664–670.
American Psychologist, 34, 932–937. Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher–child
Altermatt, E. R., Jovanovic, J., & Perry, M. (1998). Bias relationship and children’s early school. Journal of
or responsivity? Sex and achievement level effects on School Psychology, 35, 61–79.
teachers’ classroom questioning practices. Journal of Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Children’s interper-
Educational Psychology, 90, 516–527. sonal behaviors and the teacher–child relationship.
Altermatt, E. R., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2003). The devel- Developmental Psychology, 34, 934–946.
opment of competence-related and motivational Boesch, E. E. (1976). Psychopathologie des alltags
beliefs: An investigation of similarity and influence [Everyday psychopathology]. Bern, Switzerland:
among friends. Journal of Educational Psychology, Huber.
95, 111–123. Booker, K. C. (2006). School belonging and the African-
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and stu- American adolescent: What do we know and where
dent motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, should we go? The High School Journal, 89, 1–7.
84, 261–271. Bowlby, J. (1969/1973). Attachment and loss (Vols. 1 and 2).
Ames, C., & Ames, R. (1985). Research on motivation in New York: Basic Books.
education (The classroom milieu, Vol. 2). San Diego, Bradley, R. H. (2007). Parenting in the breach: How
CA: Academic. parents help children cope with developmentally
Anderman, L. H. (1999). Classroom goal orientation, challenging circumstances. Parenting: Science and
school belonging, and social goals as predictors of stu- Practice, 7, 99–148.
dents’ positive and negative affect following transition Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human
to middle school. Journal of Research and Development development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R. M.
in Education, 32, 89–103. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology:
Anderson, K. E., Lytton, H., & Romney, D. (1996). Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development
Mothers’ interactions with normal and conduct disor- (pp. 807–863). New York: Wiley.
dered boys: Who affects whom? Developmental Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and
Psychology, 22, 604–609. prospect. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. Child Development, 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209),
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- 276–297.
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Brewster, A. B., & Bowen, G. L. (2004). Teacher support
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. and the school engagement of Latino middle and high
Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schemas and the pro- school students at risk of school failure. Child and
cessing of social information. Psychological Bulletin, Adolescent Social Work Journal, 21, 47–67.
112, 461–484. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. of developmental processes. In W. Damon & R. Lerner
New York: Freeman. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1:
Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Kim, D., Watson, M., & Theoretical models of human development (5th ed.,
Schnaps, K. (1995). Schools as communities, poverty pp. 993–1028). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
levels of student populations, and students’ attitudes Bukowski, W. M., Motzoi, C., & Meyer, F. (2009).
and performance: A multilevel analysis. American Friendship as process, function, and outcome. In K. H.
Educational Research Journal, 32(3), 627–658. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.),
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and
belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fun- groups (pp. 217–231). New York: Guilford.
damental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, Cairns, R. B., Neckerman, H. J., & Cairns, B. D. (1989).
117, 497–529. Social networks and shadows of synchrony. In G. R.
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of Adams, R. Montemayor, & T. P. Gullota (Eds.),
effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Biology of adolescent behavior and development
Review, 75, 81–95. (pp. 275–305). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Bell, R. Q. (1979). Parent, child, and reciprocal influ- Chapman, M. (1984). Intentional action as a paradigm for
ences. The American Psychologist, 34, 821–826. developmental psychology: A symposium. Human
Berndt, T. J. (2004). Children’s friendships: Shifts over a Development, 27, 113–114.
half-century in perspectives on their development and Chen, X., Chang, L., & He, Y. (2003). The peer group as
effects. Merill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 206–223. a context: Mediating and moderating effects on rela-
Berndt, T. J., Hawkins, J. A., & Jiao, Z. (1999). Influences tions between academic achievement and social func-
of friends and friendships on adjustment to junior high tioning in Chinese children. Child Development, 74,
school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 13–41. 710–727.
Berndt, T. J., & Keefe, K. (1995). Friends’ influences on Connell, J. P., Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Clifford, E.,
adolescents’ adjustment to school. Child Development, Crichlow, W., & Usinger, P. (1995). Hanging in there:
66, 1312–1329. Behavioral, psychological, and contextual factors
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 39

affecting whether African-American adolescents stay Impact of psychological factors on education


in high school. Journal of Adolescent Research, 10, (pp. 61–87). San Diego, CA: Academic.
41–63. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (Eds.). (2002b). The handbook of
Connell, J. P., Klem, A. M., Lacher, T., Leiderman, S., & self-determination research. Rochester, NY: University
Moore, w., with Deci, E. L. (2009). First Things First: of Rochester Press.
Theory, research, and practice. Toms River, NJ: Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R.
Institute for Research and Reform in Education. M. (1991). Motivation and education: The self-deter-
Available online: http://www.irre.orglpublications. mination perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26,
Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994). 642–650.
Educational risk and resilience in African American Dweck, C. S. (1991). Self-theories and goals: Their role in
youth: Context, self, and action outcomes in school. motivation, personality, and development. In R. A.
Child Development, 65, 493–506. Dienstbier (Ed.), Perspectives on motivation: Nebraska
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, symposium on motivation (Vol. 38). Lincoln, NE:
autonomy and relatedness: A motivational analysis of University of Nebraska Press.
self-system processes. In M. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motiva-
(Eds.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology: tion, personality, and development. Philadelphia:
Vol. 23. Self processes in development (pp. 43–77). Psychology Press.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage-environment fit:
Crittenden, P. M. (1990). Internal representational models Developmentally appropriate classrooms for early
of attachment relationships. Infant Mental Health adolescents. In R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research
Journal, 11, 259–277. on motivation in education: Goals and cognitions
Deci, E. L. (1992a). On the nature and function of motiva- (Vol. 3, pp. 13–44). New York: Academic.
tional theories. Psychological Science, 3, 167–171. Eccles., J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M.,
Deci, E. L. (1992b). The relation of interest to the motiva- Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development
tion of behavior: A self-determination theory perspec- during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment
tive. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), fit on adolescents’ experiences in schools and families.
The role of interest in learning and development American Psychologist, 48, 90–101.
(pp. 43–70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Elliot, A. J., & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of
Deci, E. L. (1998). The relation of interest to motivation competence and motivation. New York: Guilford.
and human needs: The self-determination theory view- Epstein, J. L., & Sanders, M. G. (2002). Family, school,
point. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, K. A. Renninger, & and community partnerships. In M. Bornstein (Ed.),
J. Baumert (Eds.), Interest and learning (pp. 146–162). Handbook of parenting (Practical issues in parenting
Kiel, Germany: Institute for Science Education. 2nd ed., Vol. 5, pp. 407–438). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Deci, E. L. (2009). Large-scale school reform as viewed Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D.
from the self-determination theory perspective. Theory (2002). Social relations and academic achievement in
and Research in Education, 7, 244–253. inner-city early elementary classrooms. International
Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). A moti- Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 518–528.
vational analysis of self-determination and self-regu- Fiedler, M. L. (1975). Bidirectionality of influence in
lation in the classroom. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), classroom interactions. Journal of Educational
Research on motivation in education: Vol. 2: The Psychology, 87, 735–744.
classroom milieu (pp. 13–52). San Diego, CA: Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
Academic. Educational Research, 59, 117–142.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta- Finn, J. D. (1993). School engagement and students at
analytic review of experiments examining the effects of risk. Washington, DC: National Center of Educational
extrinsic motivation on intrinsic rewards. Psychological Statistics.
Bulletin, 125, 627–668. Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
and self-determination in human behavior. New York: the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1),
Plenum Press. 59–109.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational Furlong, M. J., & Christenson, S. L. (2008). Engaging
approach to self: Integration in personality. In R. students at school with learning: A relevant construct
Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: for all students. Psychology in the Schools, 45,
Vol. 38. Perspectives on motivation (pp. 237–288). 365–368.
Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Furrer, C., Kelly, G., & Skinner, E. (2003, April). Can
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” teachers use children’s emotions in the classroom to
of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determina- diagnose and treat underlying motivational problems?
tion of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. Poster presented at the biennial meetings of the Society
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002a). The paradox of for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.
achievement: The harder you push, the worse it gets. Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness
In J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement: as a factor in children’s academic engagement and
40 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1992). Parental resources
148–162. and the developing child in school. In M. E. Procidano
Furrer, C. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2009, April). Reciprocal & C. B. F. Fisher (Eds.), Contemporary families:
effects of student engagement in the classroom on A handbook for school professionals (pp. 275–291).
changes in teacher support over the school year. New York: Teachers College Press.
Poster at the Society for Research in Child Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner
Development, Denver, CO. resources for school achievement: Motivational medi-
Garcia-Reid, P., Reid, R., & Peterson, N. (2002). School ators of children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal
engagement among Latino youth in an urban middle of Educational Psychology, 83, 508–517.
school context: Valuing the role of social support. Grolnick, W. S., & Slowiaczek, J. L. (1994). Parental
Education and Urban Society, 37, 257–275. involvement in children’s schooling: A multidimen-
Gest, S. D., Rulison, K. L., Davidson, A. J., & Welsh, J. A. sional conceptualization and motivational model.
(2008). A reputation for success (or failure): The asso- Child Development, 65, 237–252.
ciation of peer academic reputations with academic Guthrie, J. T., & Davis, M. H. (2003). Motivating strug-
self-concept, effort, and performance across the upper gling readers in middle school through an engagement
elementary grades. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), model of classroom practice. Reading & Writing
625–636. Quarterly, 19, 59–85.
Ginsberg, G. S., & Bronstein, P. (1993). Family factors Hakanen, J. J., Bakker, A. B., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2006).
related to children’s intrinsic/extrinsic motivational Burnout and work engagement among teachers.
orientation and academic performance. Child Develop- Journal of School Psychology, 43, 495–513.
ment, 64, 1461–1474. Hallinan, M. T., & Williams, R. A. (1990). Students’ char-
Goodenow, C. (1993). Classroom belonging among early acteristics and the peer-influence process. Sociology of
adolescent students: Relationships to motivation and Education, 63, 122–132.
achievement. Journal of Early Adolescence, 13, Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher-child
21–43. relationships and the trajectory of children’s school
Gottfried, A. E. (1990). Academic intrinsic motivation in outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development,
young elementary school children. Journal of 72, 625–638.
Educational Psychology, 82, 525–538. Hardre, P. L., & Reeve, J. (2003). A motivational model of
Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994). students’ intentions to persist in, versus drop out of,
Role of parental motivational practices in children’s high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,
academic intrinsic motivation and achievement. 347–356.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 104–113. Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered:
Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Toward a developmental model. Human Development,
Continuity of academic intrinsic motivation from 21, 36–64.
childhood through late adolescence: A longitudinal Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for
study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 3–13. children. Child Development, 53, 89–97.
Gottfried, A. E., Marcoulides, G. A., Gottfried, A. W., & Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory
Oliver, P. H. (2009). A latent curve model of parental of control. Psychological Review, 102, 284–304.
motivational practices and developmental decline in Hulleman, C. S., Schrager, S. M., Bodman, S. M., &
math and science academic intrinsic motivation. Harachiewicz, J. M. (2010). A meta-analytic review of
Journal of Educational Psychology, 3, 729–739. achievement goal measures: Different labels for the
Gottfried, A. E., Marcoulides, G. A., Gottfried, A. W., same constructs or different constructs with similar
Oliver, P. H., & Guerin, D. W. (2007). Multivariate labels? Psychological Bulletin, 136, 422–449.
latent change modeling of developmental decline in Jelsma, B. M. (1982). Adult control behaviors: The inter-
academic intrinsic math motivation and achievement: action between orientation toward control in women
Childhood through adolescence. International Journal and activity level of children. Unpublished doctoral
of Behavioral Development, 31, 317–327. Dissertation, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY.
Grolnick, W. S., & Apostoleris, N. H. (2002). What makes Jeynes, W. H. (2007). The relationship between parental
parents controlling? In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan involvement and urban secondary school student
(Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Urban
(pp. 161–181). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Education, 42, 82–110.
Press. Jimerson, S. J., Campos, E., & Greif, J. L. (2003). Towards
Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in chil- an understanding of definitions and measures of school
dren’s learning: An experimental and individual dif- engagement and related terms. The California School
ference investigation. Journal of Personality and Psychologist, 8, 7–27.
Social Psychology, 52, 890–898. Kandel, D. B. (1978). Homophily, selection, and social-
Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles asso- ization in adolescent friendships. The American
ciated with children’s self-regulation and competence: Journal of Sociology, 84, 427–436.
A social contextual perspective. Journal of Educational Kindermann, T. A. (1993). Natural peer groups as contexts
Psychology, 81, 143–154. for individual development: The case of children’s
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 41

motivation in school. Developmental Psychology, 29, Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Children’s relation-
970–977. ships with adults and peers: An examination of
Kindermann, T. A. (2007). Effects of naturally existing elementary and junior high school students. Journal of
peer groups on changes in academic engagement in a School Psychology, 35, 81–99.
cohort of sixth graders. Child Development, 78, Marchand, G., & Skinner, E. A. (2007). Motivational
1186–1203. dynamics of children’s academic help-seeking and
Kindermann, T. A., McCollam, T. L., & Gibson, E. (1996). concealment. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Peer group influences on children’s developing school 99(1), 65–82.
motivation. In K. Wentzel & J. Juvonen (Eds.), Social Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional
motivation: Understanding children’s school adjust- activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high
ment (pp. 279–312). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. school years. American Educational Research Journal,
Kindermann, T. A., & Skinner, E. A. (2009). How do 37, 153–184.
naturally existing peer groups shape children’s aca- Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2006). Academic resil-
demic development during sixth grade? European ience and its psychological and educational correlates:
Journal of Psychological Science, 3, 31–43. A construct validity approach. Psychology in the
Kindermann, T. A., & Skinner, E. A. (in press). Will the Schools, 43, 267–282.
real peer group please stand up? A “tensegrity” Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2008a). Academic buoy-
approach to examining the synergistic influences of ancy: Towards an understanding of students’ everyday
peer groups and friendship networks on academic academic resilience. Journal of School Psychology,
development. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Series Eds.), 46, 53–83.
Adolescents and education, A. Ryan & G. Ladd (Vol. Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2008b). Workplace and
Eds.), Peer relationships and adjustment at school. academic buoyancy: Psychometric assessment and
New York: Information Age Publishing. construct validity amongst school personnel and stu-
Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships mat- dents. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 26,
ter: Linking teacher support to student engagement 168–184.
and achievement. The Journal of School Health, 74, Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2009). Academic resil-
262–273. ience and academic buoyancy: Multidimensional and
Koestner, R., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Perspectives hierarchical conceptual framing of causes, correlates
on competence motivation. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), and cognate constructs. Oxford Review of Education,
Handbook of personality: Theory and research 35, 353–370.
(pp. 527–548). New York: Guilford. Miceli, M., & Castelfranchi, C. (2000). Nature and mech-
Kuhl, J. (1987). Action control: The maintenance of moti- anisms of loss of motivation. Review of General
vational states. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Psychology, 4(3), 238–263.
Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 279–291). Midgley, C., & Edelin, K. C. (1998). Middle school
New York: Springer. reform and early adolescent well-being: The good
Kuperminc, G. P., Blatt, S. J., Shahar, G., Henrich, C., & news and the bad. Educational Psychologist, 33,
Leadbetter, B. J. (2004). Cultural equivalence and cul- 195–206.
tural variance in longitudinal associations of young Miserandino, M. (1996). Children who do well in school:
adolescent self-definition and interpersonal related- Individual differences in perceived competence and
ness to psychological and school adjustment. Journal autonomy in above-average children. Journal of
of Youth and Adolescence, 33(1), 13–30. Educational Psychology, 88, 203–214.
Ladd, G. W. (1990). Having friends, keeping friends, Morrison, G. M., Robertson, L., Laurie, B., & Kelly, J.
making friends, and being liked by peers in the class- (2002). Protective factors related to antisocial behav-
room: Predictors of children’s early school adjust- ior trajectories. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58,
ment? Child Development, 61, 1081–1100. 277–290.
Ladd, G. W., Birch, S. H., & Buhs, E. S. (1999). Children’s Murray, C., & Greenberg, M. T. (2000). Children’s rela-
social and scholastic lives in kindergarten: Related tionship with teachers and bonds with school. Journal
spheres of influence? Child Development, 70, of School Psychology, 38, 423–445.
1373–1400. Murray, C., & Murray, K. M. (2004). Child level corre-
Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C. lates of teacher-student relationships: An examination
(1997). Classroom peer acceptance, friendship, and of demographic characteristics, academic orientations,
victimization: Distinct relational systems that contrib- and behavioral orientations. Psychology in the Schools,
ute uniquely to children’s school adjustment? Child 41(7), 751–762.
Development, 68, 1181–1197. National Research Council. (2004). Engaging schools:
Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1992). Maltreated chil- Fostering high school students’ motivation to learn.
dren’s reports of relatedness to their teachers. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
In R. C. Pianta (Ed.), New directions for child devel- Nelson-Le Gall, S., Gumerman, R. A., & Scott-Jones, D.
opment: No. 57. Beyond the parent: The role of other (1983). Instrumental help-seeking and everyday
adults in children’s lives (pp. 81–107). San Francisco: problem-solving: A developmental perspective. In B.
Jossey-Bass. DePaulo, A. Nadler, & J. Fisher (Eds.), New directions
42 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

in helping: Help-seeking (Vol. 2, pp. 265–284). Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook of classroom management:
New York: Academic. Research, practice, and contemporary issues
Newman, R. S. (1994). Adaptive help seeking: A strategy (pp. 85–710). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
of self-regulated learning. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective
Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-regulation of learning and of the role of student motivation in learning and teach-
performance: Issues and educational applications ing contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,
(pp. 283–301). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 667–686.
Newman, R. S. (2000). Social influences on the develop- Pomerantz, E. M., Grolnick, W. S., & Price, C. E. (2005).
ment of children’s adaptive help seeking: The role of The role of parents in how children approach achieve-
parents, teachers, and peers. Developmental Review, ment: A dynamic process perspective. In A. J. Elliot &
20(3), 350–404. C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and
Newman, R. S. (2002). What do I need to do to succeed… motivation (pp. 259–278). New York: Guilford.
when I don’t understand what I am doing!? Power, T. G. (2004). Stress and coping in childhood: The
Developmental influences on students’ adaptive help- parents’ role. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4,
seeking. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), 271–317.
Development of achievement motivation (pp. 285–306). Ratcliffe, M. M., Goldberg, J., Rogers, B., & Merrigan, K.
San Diego, CA: Academic. (2010). A model of garden-based education in school
Newmann, F. M., King, M. B., & Carmichael, D. L. settings: Development of a conceptual framework to
(2007). Authentic instruction and assessment: improve children’s academic achievement, ecoliteracy,
Common standards for rigor and relevance in teach- health and wellness while enhancing schools, commu-
ing academic subjects. Des Moines, IA: Iowa nities, and bioregions. Unpublished manuscript,
Department of Education. Friedman School of Nutrition Science and Policy,
Newmann, F., Wehlage, G. G., & Lamborn, S. D. (1992). Tufts University, Medford, MA.
The significance and sources of student engagement. Reeve, J. (2002). Self-determination theory applied to
In F. Newmann (Ed.), Student engagement and educational settings. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan
achievement in American secondary schools (Eds.), The handbook of self-determination research
(pp. 11–39). New York: Teachers College Press. (pp. 183–203). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester
Niemiec, C. P., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). Autonomy, compe- Press.
tence, and relatedness in the classroom: Applying self- Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J.
determination theory to educational practice. Theory (2004). Enhancing students’ engagement by increas-
and Research in Education, 7(2), 133–144. ing teachers’ autonomy support. Motivation and
Otis, N., Grouzet, F. M. E., & Pelletier, L. G. (2005). Emotion, 28, 147–169.
Latent motivational change in an academic setting: Renninger, K. A. (2000). Individual interest and its impli-
A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational cations for understanding intrinsic motivation. In
Psychology, 97, 170–183. C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and
Patrick, B. C., Hisley, J., Kempler, T., & College, G. extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motiva-
(2000). “What’s everybody so excited about?”: The tion and performance (pp. 373–404). San Diego, CA:
effects of teacher enthusiasm on student intrinsic moti- Academic.
vation and vitality. The Journal of Experimental Reynolds, A. J., & Clements, M. (2005). Parental involve-
Education, 68, 217–236. ment and children’s school success. In E. N. Patrikakou,
Patrick, B. C., Skinner, E. A., & Connell, J. P. (1993). R. P. Weisberg, S. Redding, & H. J. Walberg (Eds.),
What motivates children’s behavior and emotion? The School-family partnerships for children’s success.
joint effects of perceived control and autonomy in the New York: Teachers College Press.
academic domain. Journal of Personality and Social Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996).
Psychology, 65(4), 781–791. Perceptions of the school psychological environment
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family processes. and early adolescents’ psychological and behavioral
Eugene, OR: Castalia Press. functioning in school: The mediating role of goals and
Pelletier, L. G., & Vallerand, R. J. (1996). Supervisors’ belonging. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88,
beliefs and subordinates’ intrinsic motivation: A behav- 408–422.
ioral confirmation analysis. Journal of Personality and Roeser, R. W., Peck, S. C., & Nasir, N. S. (2006). Self and
Social Psychology, 71, 331–340. identity processes in school motivation, learning, and
Peterson, C., Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1993). achievement. In P. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.),
Learned helplessness: A theory for the age of personal Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed.,
control. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 391–424). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Pianta, R. C. (1999). Enhancing relationships between Rosenholtz, S. J., & Wilson, B. (1980). The effect of
children and teachers. Washington, DC: American classroom structure on shared perceptions of ability.
Psychologist Association. American Educational Research Journal, 17, 75–82.
Pianta, R. C. (2006). Classroom management and rela- Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilita-
tionships between children and teachers: Implications tion of integrative processes. Journal of Personality,
for research and practice. In C. M. Evertson & C. S. 63, 397–427.
2 Developmental Dynamics of Student Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience 43

Ryan, A. M. (2001). The peer group as a context for the Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control.
development of young adolescents’ motivation and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
achievement. Child Development, 72, 1135–1150. 549–570.
Ryan, R. M., & Brown, K. W. (2005). Legislating compe- Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the
tence: High-stakes testing policies and their relations classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and
with psychological theories and research. In A. J. student engagement across the school year. Journal of
Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581.
and motivation (pp. 354–372). New York: Guilford. Skinner, E. A., Chi, U., & the Learning-Gardens
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of Educational Assessment Group (2012). Intrinsic moti-
causality and internalization: Examining reasons for vation and engagement as “active ingredients” in gar-
acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and den-based education: Examining models and measures
Social Psychology, 57, 749–761. derived from self-determination theory. Journal of
Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Deci, E. L. (1985). A Environmental Education, 43(1), 16–36.
motivational analysis of self-determination and self- Skinner, E., Edge, K., Altman, J., & Sherwood, H. (2003).
regulation in education. In C. Ames & R. E. Ames Searching for the structure of coping: A review and
(Eds.), Research on motivation in education: The critique of category systems for classifying ways of
classroom milieu (pp. 13–51). New York: Academic. coping. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 216–269.
Ryan, A. M., Patrick, H., & Shim, S. (2005). Differential Skinner, E. A., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann,
profiles of students identified by their teacher as hav- T. (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the class-
ing avoidant, appropriate, or dependent help-seeking room: Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal
tendencies in the classroom. Journal of Educational of Educational Psychology, 100, 765–781.
Psychology, 97(2), 275–285. Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & Wellborn,
Ryan, R. M., & Powelson, C. L. (1991). Autonomy and J. G. (2009a). Engagement as an organizational construct
relatedness as fundamental to motivation and educa- in the dynamics of motivational development. In K.
tion. The Journal of Experimental Education, 60, Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motiva-
49–66. tion at school (pp. 223–245). Malwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ryan, R. M., & Stiller, J. (1991). The social contexts of Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., & Furrer, C. (2009b).
internalization: Parent and teacher influences on A motivational perspective on engagement and disaf-
autonomy, motivation, and learning. In M. L. Maehr & fection: Conceptualization and assessment of chil-
P. L. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and dren’s behavioral and emotional participation in
achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 115–149). Greenwich, CT: academic activities in the classroom. Educational and
JAI Press. Psychological Measurement, 69, 493–525.
Ryan, R. M., Stiller, J. D., & Lynch, J. H. (1994). Skinner, E. A., & Wellborn, J. G. (1994). Coping during
Representations and relationships to teachers, parents, childhood and adolescence: A motivational perspective.
and friends as predictors of academic motivation and In D. Featherman, R. Lerner, & M. Perlmutter (Eds.),
self-esteem. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 12,
226–249. pp. 91–133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Salmela-Aro, K., Kiuru, N., Leskinen, E., & Nurmi, J.-E. Skinner, E. A., & Wellborn, J. G. (1997). Children’s cop-
(2009). School-Burnout Inventory. European Journal ing in the academic domain. In S. A. Wolchik & I. N.
of Psychological Assessment, 25, 48–57. Sandler (Eds.), Handbook of children’s coping with
Sameroff, A. J., & Haith, M. M. (Eds.). (1996). The five to common stressors: Linking theory and intervention
seven year shift: The age of reason and responsibility. (pp. 387–422). New York: Plenum Press.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Skinner, E. A., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Connell, J. P.
Schmitz, B., & Skinner, E. (1993). Perceived control, (1998). Individual differences and the development of
effort, and academic performance: Interindividual, perceived control. Monographs of the Society for
intraindividual, and multivariate time-series analyses. Research in Child Development, 63 (nos. 2 and 3)
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(6), whole no. 254, pp. 1–220.
1010–1028. Steinberg, L., Elmen, J. D., & Mounts, N. S. (1989).
Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (Eds.). (2007). Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and
Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory, academic success in adolescents. Child Development,
research, and practice. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence 60, 1424–1436.
Erlbaum Associates. Stipek, D. J. (2002a). Good instruction is motivating. In
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of
development, and death. San Francisco: Freeman. achievement motivation (pp. 309–332). San Diego,
Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Lehr, C. A., & CA: Academic.
Anderson, A. R. (2003). Facilitating student learning Stipek, D. J. (2002b). Motivation to learn: From theory to
and engagement: Lessons learned from Check & practice (4th ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn &
Connect longitudinal studies. The California School Bacon.
Psychologist, 8, 29–41. Vallerand, R. J., Fortier, M. S., & Guay, F. (1997). Self-
Skinner, E. A. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, and determination and persistence in a real-life setting:
coping. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Toward a motivational model of high school dropout.
44 E.A. Skinner and J.R. Pitzer

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, peers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2),
72(1161), 1176. 202–209.
Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., Blais, M. R., Brière, N. Wentzel, K. R. (1999). Social-motivational processes and
M., Senécal, C. B., & Vallières, E. F. (1993). On the interpersonal relationships: Implications for under-
assessment of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in standing motivation at school. Journal of Educational
education: Evidence on the concurrent and construct Psychology, 91, 76–97.
validity of the Academic Motivation Scale. Wentzel, K. (2009). Students’ relationships with teachers
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53, as motivation contexts. In K. Wentzel & A. Wigfield
159–172. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation in school (pp. 301–322).
Vasalampi, K., Salmela-Aro, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2009). Malwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Adolescents’ self-concordance, school engagement, Wentzel, K. R., McNamara-Barry, C., & Caldwell, K. A.
and burnout predict their educational trajectories. (2004). Friendships in middle school: Influences on
European Psychologist, 14, 1–11. motivation and school adjustment. Journal of
Weiner, B. (2005). Motivation from an attributional per- Educational Psychology, 96, 195–203.
spective and the social psychology of perceived com- White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The con-
petence. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), cept of competence. Psychological Review, 66,
Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 73–84). 297–333.
New York: Guilford. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R., &
Weisz, J. R. (1986). Understanding the developing under- Davis-Kean, P. (2006). Development of achievement
standing of control. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota motivation. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg
symposium on child psychology: Vol. 18. Social cogni- (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, 6th Ed. Vol.
tion (pp. 219–278). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development
Wentzel, K. R. (1991). Social competence at school: (pp. 933–1002). New York: John Wiley.
Relation between social responsibility and academic Wooley, M. E., & Bowen, G. L. (2007). In the context of
achievement. Review of Educational Research, 61, risk: Supportive adults and the school engagement of
1–24. middle-school students. Family Relations, 56, 92–104.
Wentzel, K. R. (1997). Student motivation in middle Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2011). The
school: The role of perceived pedagogical caring. development of coping across childhood and adoles-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 411–419. cence: An integrative review and critique of research.
Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social relationships and motivation International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35,
in middle school: The role of parents, teachers, and 1–17.
Engagement Across Developmental
Periods 3
Duhita Mahatmya, Brenda J. Lohman,
Jennifer L. Matjasko, and Amy Feldman Farb

Abstract
The goal of this chapter is to provide a cohesive developmental framework
and foundation for which to understand student engagement across early
childhood, middle childhood, and adolescence. Guided by the bioecologi-
cal theory of human development and the person-environment fit perspec-
tive, this chapter extends Finn’s participation-identification model of
engagement by mapping student engagement within a larger developmen-
tal sequence. This chapter discusses student engagement within specific
developmental periods that are tied to the developmental tasks, opportuni-
ties, and challenges unique to early childhood, middle childhood, and ado-
lescence. Student engagement is found to be a nuanced developmental
outcome, and the differences may be a result of the maturation of biologi-
cal, cognitive, and socioemotional developmental tasks and the changing
contextual landscape for the children and adolescents. Recommendations
for future research as well as policy implications are also discussed.

Chapter Aim and Overview


D. Mahatmya, Ph.D. (*) Research consistently shows that student engage-
New Century College, George Mason University,
Fairfax, VA, USA
ment plays a critical role in the development
e-mail: dmahatmy@gmu.edu of positive outcomes in children and adoles-
B.J. Lohman, Ph.D.
cents such as increasing academic achievement
Human Development and Family Studies, (Carbonaro, 1998; Eccles, 2004; Manke,
Iowa State University, Ames, IA, USA McGuire, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin, 1995;
e-mail: blohman@iastate.edu Portes, 2000) and facilitating the development of
J.L. Matjasko, Ph.D. new social competencies (Karcher, Kuperminc,
University of Texas, Austin, TX, USA Portwood, Sipe, & Taylor, 2006; Parra, Dubois,
e-mail: jmatjasko@cdc.gov
Neville, Pugh-Lilly, & Povinelli, 2002). While it
A.F. Farb, Ph.D. is important to consider the factors that shape
Office of Adolescent Health,
US Department of Health and Human Services,
student engagement and its potential conse-
Washington, DC, USA quences, we argue that understanding student
e-mail: amy.farb@hhs.gov engagement within the context of the individual’s

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 45


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_3, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
46 D. Mahatmya et al.

developmental history is also important. The goal skills children first gain through participation in
of this chapter is to provide a cohesive develop- peer play, a developmental task of early childhood,
mental framework and foundation for which to and further cultivate during middle childhood
understand student engagement across early and adolescence, may promote his/her student
childhood, middle childhood, and adolescence. engagement in school-related activities. Children
For the purposes of this chapter, we limit our dis- and adolescents who are more engaged may sub-
cussion on engagement to school-related activi- sequently increase their likelihood of success-
ties. School-related activities comprise both fully reaching a developmental task. Not only
schoolwork (e.g., engagement on academic-spe- does this suggest that student engagement is more
cific tasks both within and outside of school) and likely to happen if children’s and adolescents’
nonacademic school-related activities (e.g., extra- school experiences are framed within the devel-
curricular activities). opmental tasks fitting the general developmental
Moreover, this chapter highlights the impor- period, but also student engagement can
tance of accounting for changes in developmen- strengthen the accomplishment of developmental
tal tasks (defined in the following sections) and tasks. Based on this, the two main questions that
how they codevelop with student engagement guide this chapter are:
over the childhood and adolescent years. Indeed, 1. How do developmental tasks encourage or
while much of the student engagement literature discourage the development of student
has focused on defining the specific components engagement?
of student engagement, mainly behavioral, cog- 2. How can student engagement strengthen the
nitive, and emotional engagement (Appleton, acquisition of developmental tasks?
Christenson, & Furlong, 2008; Marks, 2000; To further understand the reciprocal processes,
Rose-Krasnor, 2009), the research examining the we first define student engagement. Second, we
interplay of developmental tasks and the devel- briefly describe the prominent developmental
opment of student engagement is limited. Echoing tasks of early childhood, middle childhood, and
Finn’s (1989) statement about school dropouts adolescence, and the links between these devel-
initiated by a “chain of events” (Finn, 1989, p. 119), opmental tasks and student engagement. Next,
we conceptualize the interplay of developmental we detail our overarching theoretical framework
tasks and the development of student engagement that stresses the importance of understanding the
as a reciprocal process that also occurs over a emergence of the developmental tasks to enhance
period of time. In this way, we adapt Finn’s original our knowledge of the development of student
argument for the participation-identification engagement. The bulk of this chapter addresses
model of engagement (Finn, 1989; Reschly, student engagement across these developmental
2010) by imparting concepts from the develop- periods and how the key behavioral, cognitive,
mental literature to identify the direct and indirect and socioemotional developmental tasks in child-
effects of developmental tasks on student hood and adolescence are related. It must be
engagement and vice versa. noted that the majority of the work assessing stu-
Indeed, childhood and adolescence is a time dent engagement discussed here draws from lit-
of rapid growth signified by key developmental erature that is nonexperimental in nature. As a
tasks that capture overt biological and physiolog- result, we cannot make any causal statements
ical changes, significant cognitive advancements, about the link between developmental periods
emotional maturation, as well as new social rela- and student engagement. Where appropriate, we
tionships. The specific manifestation of the devel- note findings from experimental research and
opmental tasks within each developmental period, those that are nationally representative or longi-
however, will likely vary across individuals and tudinal in nature. Throughout these sections, we
contexts, and these manifestations can in turn be discuss the importance of understanding growth
linked to the developing child or adolescent’s and development and how it codevelops with stu-
student engagement. For instance, the social dent engagement. Finally, we conclude with brief
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 47

remarks summarizing future research directions and are determined by the extent to which the
in this area and the importance of family, school, individual values and identifies with the activities
and community partnerships for enhancing stu- and whether they believe the activities are rele-
dent engagement across developmental periods. vant to their future. We expand the latter state-
ment and argue that the components of student
engagement cannot be fully observed without the
Defining Student Engagement appropriate developmental foundation via the
attainment of developmental tasks, which are
In defining student engagement, prior research described in the next section.
has identified three distinct dimensions to the
construct (e.g., Appleton et al., 2008; Fredricks,
Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004), mainly, behavioral, Developmental Tasks of Childhood
cognitive, and emotional engagement. According and Adolescence
to Fredricks and colleagues (2004) and Blumenfeld
and colleagues (2005): Developmental tasks describe the main changes
1. Behavioral engagement draws on the idea of and challenges that occur during a certain devel-
participation; it includes involvement in aca- opmental period. Generally, they represent any
demic and social or extracurricular activities. number of things from physical milestones to
It is usually defined in three ways. The first societal expectations for individuals based on
entails positive conduct, as well as the absence age. Beginning in early childhood (birth to
of disruptive behaviors such as skipping school. 6 years), children are increasingly faced with new
The second definition concerns involvement in and complex socialization forces that influence
learning and academic tasks and includes their behavioral, cognitive, and emotional devel-
behaviors such as effort, persistence, concen- opment. For instance, as noted above, one main
tration, attention, asking questions, etc. A third developmental task during early childhood is
definition involves participation in school- participation in peer play (Newman & Newman,
related activities such as athletics or school 2009). Through the process of learning rules and
governance. playing cooperatively with others, children begin
2. Cognitive engagement draws on the idea of to form meaningful friendships and mental repre-
investment; it incorporates thoughtfulness and sentations of ways of participating in groups.
willingness to exert the effort necessary to Entry into the formal school setting also brings
comprehend complex ideas and master diffi- new opportunities, new information, and new
cult skills. interactions with teachers and peers that can fos-
3. Emotional engagement encompasses positive ter or inhibit the child’s maturation.
and negative reactions to teachers, classmates, Moving into middle childhood (6–12 years),
academics, and school, and is presumed to create there is continued growth in intellectual capaci-
ties to an institution and influence willingness to ties, mastery, competence, and steady physical
do the work. It refers to students’ affective development. During this time, children are learn-
reactions in the classroom, including interest, ing the fundamental skills and values that are
boredom, happiness, sadness, and anxiety. associated with their particular environment,
Appleton and colleagues (2008) build on this which increasingly involves the school environ-
characterization by further operationalizing the ment. As children become adolescents (12–18 years),
three engagement constructs. For example, atten- academic expectations increase in complexity and
dance, suspensions, voluntary classroom partici- responsibility; youths are expected to learn and to
pation, and extracurricular activity participation follow the rules and laws that govern conduct in
are part of behavioral engagement (Appleton adult society, and they begin to learn about respon-
et al., 2008). They also claim that both cognitive sible dating and romantic social conduct in their
and emotional engagement are not easily observed community and culture. Adolescence is also a
48 D. Mahatmya et al.

period marked by increased exposure to environ- the development of his/her prefrontal cortex and
ments outside of the family, and a large develop- limbic system, which inform higher order reason-
mental task is achieving a psychological sense ing capabilities. The opposite may be true as well,
of autonomy from one’s parents (Newman & where cognitive development can be improved
Newman, 2009). Many parents also consider it by being more engaged. It is not until adoles-
important for a child to contribute to the family or cence that youth begin to have greater self-reflec-
community through chores or good deeds, or at tion, become more deliberate and focused, and
least not to destroy and to harm others or com- are able to hypothesize and think about several
munity property. strategies or outcomes for these hypotheses
Acceptable performances in these tasks repre- simultaneously rather than focusing on just one
sent important milestones in the eyes of the stake- domain or issue at a time (Keating, 2004). Thus,
holders for positive child development, including the ability to become cognitively engaged with
parents, teachers, other community members, and school is greater during adolescence compared to
children themselves. Failing in these domains by both early and middle childhood. Increased cog-
not meeting expectations may have consequences nitive student engagement may not only show
for children’s current and future opportunities, benefits for academic achievement, but also the
peer reputation, social support, self-esteem, fam- continued maturation of cognitive and socioemo-
ily relationships, and, of particular relevance of tional developmental tasks.
this chapter, student engagement. With regard to emotional engagement, deficits
in the development of the limbic system or social
competencies can hinder a child’s or adolescent’s
Linking Developmental Tasks ability to have affective connections to other peo-
and Engagement ple or contexts. As mentioned, peer play begins
in early childhood, becomes more purposeful in
By acknowledging the larger context of positive middle childhood, and then continues to change
development, we can further our understanding in composition and importance during adoles-
of student engagement. For instance, a child or cence. The continued growth and maturation of
adolescent may be having problems behaviorally these behavioral, cognitive, and socioemotional
engaging in school-related activities if he/she competencies, paired with the accumulation of
lacks necessary motor or social skills to partici- learning experiences in and out of the classroom,
pate. Social skills may be obtained through par- make it important to understand the links among
ticipation in peer play, a main developmental task the developmental tasks and student engagement
in early childhood (as noted above) that contin- across developmental periods.
ues to grow in middle childhood. During middle Moreover, it is important to understand the
childhood, friendships become based on who multidimensionality of student engagement
plays together, likes the same activities, shares because research has shown that engagement
common interests, enjoys each other’s company, helps to mediate the relationship between involve-
and counts on each other for help. In addition, ment in school-related activities and healthy
children are introduced to the concept of group developmental outcomes (Bartko, 2005; Weiss,
cooperation through organized activities and Little, & Blumenfeld, 2005). Indeed, Blumenfeld
team play, which enhances their abilities to ana- and colleagues (2005) go so far as to claim that
lyze and manage social relationships, such as student engagement is necessary to prepare chil-
group cooperation. These social skills can in turn dren and adolescents for the transition into adult-
influence a child’s likelihood to be engaged, and hood. Furthermore, supporting our argument for
a child that is more engaged may increase his/her the codevelopment of developmental tasks and
likelihood of successfully reaching a develop- student engagement, research has found that
mental task related to friendship formation. different behavioral, cognitive, and emotional
Likewise, a child’s or adolescent’s ability to patterns and psychological states are linked to dif-
become cognitively engaged may be restricted by ferent developmental outcomes across individuals
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 49

such as mood, internalizing and externalizing development and student engagement. First,
behaviors, and motivation (Blumenfeld et al., Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological theory asserts that
2005; Larson, 2000; Shernoff, 2010). development is a function of the interaction
With regard to motivation in particular, some between the developing person and his/her envi-
scholars have emphasized that student motiva- ronments. Bronfenbrenner and Morris (1998)
tion and engagement are separate constructs, defined those interactions as proximal processes
while others have argued that motivation is a and posited that they are the primary vehicle for
necessary but not a sufficient condition for development. Couched within those proximal pro-
engagement (Blumenfeld, Kempler, & Krajcik, cesses are two other considerations: the individual’s
2006). Connell’s process model of motivation context and characteristics. An individual’s con-
outlines the process through which motivation text reflects the idea that development is situated
influences student engagement (Connell, within a set of overlapping and multifaceted envi-
Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Connell & Wellborn, ronmental systems such as the home, school,
1991). The model states that the perceived social neighborhood, and larger sociohistorical context
context influences students’ perceived autonomy that also interact to shape development. For chil-
and relatedness. This perceived autonomy and dren and adolescents in particular, the family and
relatedness then leads to student behavioral, the school environments are central developmen-
cognitive, and emotional engagement. Similarly, tal contexts and have been shown to be signifi-
Blumenfeld and colleagues (2006) stated that cantly related to student engagement (Lohman,
motivation is a precursor to cognitive engage- Kaura, & Newman, 2007; Roeser & Eccles, 1998;
ment and achievement. We assume that motiva- Steinberg, Bradford, & Dornbusch, 1996).
tion is a precursor to all three types of engagement. Second, an individual’s characteristics can
Therefore, it is an implicit part of our definition determine whether these proximal processes
of student engagement. occur and how an individual experiences his/her
This suggests that in order to adequately contexts. For example, Finn (1989) discussed
capture the multidimensional construct of student how race, socioeconomic status, school ability
engagement, it is necessary to observe the extent and performance, and autonomy (an important
to which children and adolescents are involved developmental task realized in adolescence) are
with their schoolwork and extracurricular activi- often reasons given for a school dropout. In this
ties while also assessing whether one believes way, the student’s demographic and academic
that the activities are relevant to their current characteristics influenced his/her experience of
and future goals. In addition, capacities for school and subsequent likelihood of dropping
and expressions of student engagement will out. Likewise, and of particular interest in this
vary by developmental periods. In discussing chapter, certain developmental tasks may interact
the developmental-stage-specific forms of stu- with the individual’s motivation for, or experi-
dent engagement during early childhood, middle ences in, school-related activities to influence the
childhood, and adolescence, we rely on the development of student engagement. Child and
bioecological theory of human development adolescent characteristics may interact with the
and the person-environment fit perspective. A family and school contexts in determining stu-
description of both theories follows. dent engagement as well. Student engagement
itself can also be seen as a personal characteristic
that may contribute to the attainment of develop-
Theoretical Considerations mental tasks.
Integrating concepts from the person-environ-
The bioecological theory of human development ment fit perspective (Eccles, Midgefield, &
(Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998) and the person- Wigfield, 1993), we highlight that one size does
environment fit perspective (Eccles, 2004; Gutman not fit all in terms of the optimal organization of
& Eccles, 2007) put forward a way to integrate developmental tasks and ecologies that promote
the extant literature on child and adolescent student engagement and vice versa. According to
50 D. Mahatmya et al.

person-environment fit, processes and character- are associated with ongoing activity involvement
istics within one context may be coupled with and different levels of participation across each
congruent or divergent processes and characteris- of the activity dimensions (Eccles, 2005).
tics in another to shape an individual’s develop- To that end, we argue for a more comprehen-
ment (Catalano, Berglund, Ryan, Lonczak, & sive and integrative developmental-contextual
Hawkins, 2004; Eccles, 2004; Larson, 2000; approach. There is a need for research that
Lerner, Brentano, Dowling, & Anderson, 2002; explores the individual and contextual factors
Lerner & Castellino, 2002). With respect to stu- that are predictive of student engagement and
dent engagement, this may mean synchrony that examines whether these facets of engage-
across the values and practices espoused by fami- ment are more or less important depending on the
lies and schools to encourage engagement. characteristics of the child/adolescent or the eco-
Indeed, while Bronfenbrenner originally sug- logical context in which they live. In considering
gested that contextual levels overlap with each the developmental correlates and manifestation
other and tend to be consistent within a society of student engagement specifically, longitudinal
(Epstein, 1983; Miller, 2002), researchers have studies that adjust for some of the individual,
noted that this is not always the case as contexts family, and neighborhood factors associated with
may also vary in their degree of embeddedness these facets of engagement can help to disentan-
with one another and are sometimes even at odds gle the extent to which findings are a function of
with each other (Sternberg & Grigorenko, 2001). involvement in organized contexts and how much
Thus, a child’s or an adolescent’s developmental they reflect self-selection effects (Larson, 2000).
course may be dependent on whether contexts are From there, researchers, educators, and profes-
in synchrony or in dissynchrony (Bronfenbrenner sionals can determine the optimal developmental
& Ceci, 1994; Mahoney & Bergman, 2002; correlates to, and of, student engagement.
Mahoney & Magnusson, 2001). We argue that
congruence or synchrony across environments
may help foster student engagement, while The Developmental Context
dissynchrony, incongruence or a mismatch in of Student Engagement
environments, may hinder student engagement
(Goodenow, 1995; Lohman et al., 2007). Guided by Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological theory
Researchers can explore this overlap by taking of human development and a person-environment
an ecological approach (i.e., including multiple fit framework, what follows is a discussion of stu-
ecological contexts) in inquiries regarding student dent engagement within the specific developmen-
engagement during the child and adolescent tal periods that are tied to the specific developmental
years. Indeed, a handful of studies have begun to tasks, opportunities, and challenges of early child-
address the individual, family, and contextual hood, middle childhood, and adolescence.
factors that are associated with participation and
lack of participation in organized activities
(Dearing et al., 2009; Mahoney, Vandell, Early Childhood
Simpkins, & Zarrett, 2009; Persson, Kerr, &
Stattin, 2007). However, most of the work has Much of the context for student engagement
focused on demographic characteristics that are research centers on early childhood education
associated with activity participation (see and intervention programs such as Head Start
Anderson, Funk, Elliot, & Smith, 2003; Bohnert, (Barnett, 1995), of which Bronfenbrenner was an
Fredricks, & Randall, 2010; Denault & Poulin, early proponent (Bronfenbrenner, 1975). The
2009; Fletcher, Elder, & Mekos, 2000, for excep- emphasis is on providing effective learning
tions); therefore, it is not clear from the extant opportunities to develop the building blocks for
literature if the factors that are related to who ini- cognitive and linguistic development, literacy,
tially participates are the same as the factors that and social competencies (Bierman et al., 2008;
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 51

McWilliam & Casey, 2008; Ramey & Ramey, Behavioral Engagement


2004), which parallel the important developmental The behavioral component of engagement
tasks of this period. To that end, the early child- has been an area of emphasis in the early child-
hood engagement literature is more concerned hood literature (Coolahan, Fantuzzo, Mendez,
with the developmental markers associated with & McDermott, 2000; Fantuzzo & McWayne,
a child’s school readiness potential rather than 2002; McWilliam & Bailey, 1992; McWilliam
student engagement itself (Blair, 2002; Hair, & Casey, 2008) given that children’s behav-
Halle, & Terry-Human, 2006; Kagan, 1990; ioral problems in the classroom are often cited
McCormick et al., 2006; Ramey & Ramey, 2004). as a risk factor for poor school readiness and
Ramey and Ramey, however, argued that a child’s long-term academic performance (Coolahan
school readiness in early childhood can influence et al., 2000; Fantuzzo & McWayne, 2002;
his/her future student engagement. Kuperschmidt, Bryant, & Willoughby, 2000;
Indeed, research has found that participation Raver, 2002). This further demonstrates the
in early childhood education programs such as bioecological idea of how a child’s characteris-
Head Start promotes cognitive, behavioral, and tics can influence his/her interaction with the
socioemotional competencies that influence later environment, in this case, the classroom. Thus,
well-being and academic achievement (Barnett, care should be taken to provide children with
1995; Fantuzzo & McWayne, 2002; Hair et al., the space and resources needed to maintain a
2006; Luo, Hughes, Liew, & Kwok, 2009; level of focused attention and constructive
McCormick et al., 2006). The success of early behaviors (McWilliam & Casey, 2008). Staying
childhood programs comes from structured on task and the ability to follow rules and direc-
curricula that emphasize strategic learning inter- tions in the classroom become ways to define
actions, positive teacher-student relationships, positive student engagement in early childhood
and brain development, with the overall objective (Bierman et al., 2008; Liew, McTigue, Barrois,
of promoting school readiness (Barnett, 1995; & Hughes, 2008; Luo et al., 2009). Moreover,
Bierman et al., 2008; Currie, 2001; Ramey & the extent to which a child has a self-theory, a
Ramey, 2004). The early childhood education developmental task of early childhood, may
literature defines school readiness as the acquisi- influence the degree to which a child can fol-
tion of basic behavioral, cognitive, and socioe- low rules and directions in the classroom
motional skills needed to meet school demands (Wigfield & Karpathian, 1991).
in reading, writing, and math (Kagan, 1990; Another behavioral component to student
Ramey & Ramey, 2004). In this way, the main engagement in early childhood, and a develop-
developmental tasks of early childhood are mental task, is peer play as noted above
framed within the school context. For example, (Coolahan et al., 2000; Fantuzzo & McWayne,
school readiness is generally measured via the 2002). Peer play captures the child’s interac-
child’s behaviors in the classroom, which focuses tion with his/her peer group and carrying
the discussion of student engagement on the abil- out shared activities (Fantuzzo & McWayne,
ity to follow classroom rules, perform tasks, or 2002). Research has shown that peer play can
engage in cooperative participation with class- be an antecedent to long-term school success
mates (Bierman et al., 2008; Luo et al., 2009; (Coolahan et al., 2000; Fantuzzo & McWayne,
McWilliam & Bailey, 1992; McWilliam & Casey, 2002) as well as self-regulation (Bierman et al.,
2008), which reflect the developmental tasks of 2008). Specifically, interactive play is associ-
moral development and peer play. A further dis- ated with active engagement in classroom
cussion of how the developmental tasks of early learning activities, prosocial classroom behav-
childhood map onto concepts of student engage- ior such as helping and sharing, a motivation to
ment follows; additional early childhood educa- learn, task persistence, and autonomy (Bierman
tion literature not specific to engagement will be et al., 2008; Coolahan et al., 2000; Fantuzzo &
used to supplement the discussion. McWayne, 2002).
52 D. Mahatmya et al.

Cognitive Engagement Likewise, the added influence of interacting with


A child’s ability to follow rules and instructions nonfamilial adults can contribute to the child’s
and otherwise be behaviorally engaged can be emotional maturation more generally and shows
influenced by, and also influence, the child’s cog- the importance for synchrony across contexts, or
nitive development. The research on early child- person-environment fit. In other words, the mul-
hood cognitive development finds that children at tiple opportunities for positive interactions can
this stage begin to transition from externally to have multiplicative effects for encouraging the
internally regulated actions (Kochanska, Coy, & child’s engagement.
Murray, 2001; Kochanska & Knaack, 2003). This
is defined as self-regulation and effortful control Future Research
whereby the child learns how to control and Research in early childhood education demon-
inhibit his/her own emotions and behaviors (Liew strates that participating in early childhood pro-
et al., 2008; Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; grams can improve a child’s school readiness,
Kochanska et al., 2001), and may also reflect the which has important implications for a child’s
child’s acquisition of a personal self-theory, future academic success by providing another
which, as previously described, is an important context that encourages positive development.
developmental task in early childhood. The extent Indeed, the results from research show children
to which a child has an internal sense of control who participate in early childhood programs
and can self-regulate his/her behaviors has been make gains in vocabulary and math, behavioral,
shown to influence that child’s engagement in a and social skills (Barnett, 1995; Bierman et al.,
learning environment, specifically the child’s 2008). Beyond those findings, early childhood
ability to participate in classroom activities, con- education research has yet to investigate the
trol attention, and stay on task (Bierman et al., relationship between school readiness and stu-
2008). Again, student engagement in school dur- dent engagement specifically (Blair, 2002). For
ing early childhood is often measured by the instance, research is needed to examine how stu-
child’s classroom behaviors and is a function of dent engagement might vary by school readiness
his/her interaction with the school context. levels, which in turn may be influenced by
whether the child possesses certain developmen-
Emotional Engagement tal tasks and the contextual factors associated
A child’s emotional engagement has implica- with engagement. Thus, early childhood research-
tions for school readiness and academic achieve- ers should work to integrate the research in early
ment (Bierman et al., 2008; Liew et al., 2008; childhood education and other developmental
Raver, 2002). Particularly in early childhood, research into a cohesive framework that captures
children are beginning to interact with persons all three components of student engagement.
other than their parents such as peers and teach- Moreover, engagement research in the early
ers. Having positive interactions with multiple childhood literature should expand its scope to
people, such as parents and other caring indi- include nonschool/learning environments in
viduals, can help promote learning and build a understanding the developmental precursors to
warm and responsive social context (Ramey & student engagement in early childhood educa-
Ramey, 2004). These interactions in turn can tion. As the theoretical considerations we pro-
encourage a sense of belonging and liking in pose suggest, it is important to take an ecological
school and the development of social-emotional approach to the study of developmental out-
competencies, which has been shown to decrease comes, student engagement included. Currently,
off-task and aggressive behavior and increase most of the literature has focused specifically on
prosocial classroom and task engagement the preschool, kindergarten, and special educa-
(Bierman et al., 2008; Raver, 2002). Teachers tion environments (e.g., Mahoney & Wheeden,
especially can help nurture interest in school 1999; Malmskog & McDonnell, 1999; McWilliam
and learning activities (Bierman et al., 2008). & Casey, 2008).
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 53

Middle Childhood are characterized as being attentive in class,


responsive to rules and instructions, and initiate
During this time, children continue to transition action (Finn, 1989; Luo et al., 2009). Indeed, the
into more formal schooling and learning environ- classroom becomes an important learning envi-
ments, and there is a concurrent increase in the lit- ronment for youth in middle childhood, and the
erature on student engagement, especially as it extent to which children actively participate and
pertains to academic achievement and school are involved in classroom tasks and activities has
adjustment (Ripke, Huston, & Casey, 2006; been argued as a prerequisite for achievement
Simpkins, Fredricks, Davis-Kean, & Eccles, 2006; and engagement (Finn, Folger, & Cox, 1991;
Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, & Kinderman, 2008). Ladd, Birch, & Buhs, 1999; Skinner et al., 2008).
The increase in literature, however, is limited to This demonstrates the bioecological argument
research within the school context. Indeed, during that active interactions with a person’s environ-
middle childhood, the classroom becomes the most ment drive development. Moreover, the contin-
salient learning environment, with additional learn- ued maturation of developmental tasks during
ing experiences provided through after-school middle childhood may facilitate or hinder engage-
activities. The accumulation of these experiences ment; developmental tasks may facilitate the
is said to contribute to the development of student development of student engagement if the child
engagement in middle childhood (Ripke et al., has successfully reached the task, whereas diffi-
2006; Rose-Krasnor, 2009; Simpkins et al., 2006). culties in coping with new developmental tasks can
As with development in general (according to hinder the development of engagement. As the
the bioecological theory), engagement in middle main propositions of the person-environment fit
childhood has been defined as a function of perspective suggest, the extent to which the child’s
individual student characteristics and learning contexts fit his/her developmental needs can also
experiences (Marks, 2000), and sustained inter- contribute to the expression of engagement.
actions between the student and activity context Beyond the classroom environment, after-
(Appleton et al., 2008; Rose-Krasnor, 2009). school activities offer another context and oppor-
Moreover, student engagement during middle tunity for students to become engaged
childhood increases in importance as the role of behaviorally (Simpkins et al., 2006; Vandell,
parents and teachers in promoting classroom and Pierce, & Dadisman, 2005). Behavioral engage-
participation in extracurricular activities begins ment in after-school activities is defined as the
to wane (Ripke et al., 2006; Simpkins et al., 2006; child’s attendance and involvement in the activi-
Skinner et al., 2008). Engagement is also at its ties (Morris & Kalil, 2006; Rose-Krasnor, 2009;
peak during middle childhood while children are Vandell et al., 2005). Middle childhood is often
in elementary school (Marks, 2000), perhaps when children begin to become involved in
paralleling the development of children’s abili- activities outside of school; participating in
ties to manage group cooperation (i.e., team activities such as sports, and arts and music les-
play). In this way, learning to manage group work sons has been shown to promote psychosocial
may foster engagement, and being engaged may and academic outcomes for children (Dumais,
facilitate team play. In addition, research on 2006; Ripke et al., 2006). Research has also
engagement in middle childhood begins to dif- shown that positive experiences outside of the
ferentiate between behavioral, cognitive, and classroom can supplement and benefit the child’s
emotional engagement. The definitions and influ- engagement in the classroom (Luo et al., 2009;
ences of these three types of engagement are dis- Rose-Krasnor, 2009). Hence, synchrony among
cussed in further detail in the following sections. school-related activities can positively influence
the development of student engagement.
Behavioral Engagement Moreover, the literature suggests that a child’s
Results taken from teacher’s reports of students’ behavioral engagement is a precursor to skill
behaviors show that behaviorally engaged students development, positive social interactions, and
54 D. Mahatmya et al.

emotional engagement (Morris & Kalil, 2006; belonging to his/her school, values learning and
Rose-Krasnor, 2009). shows excitement toward classroom and after-
school activities (Finn, 1989; Luo et al., 2009;
Cognitive Engagement Rose-Krasnor, 2009). During middle childhood, the
While behavioral engagement reflects a child’s student-teacher relationship and the child’s rela-
attendance and participation with an activity, cog- tionship with friends contribute to the child’s
nitive engagement captures the child’s knowledge social skill development, which demonstrate how
and beliefs about the activity and self (Appleton the accumulation of positive interactions across
et al., 2008; Ripke et al., 2006; Rose-Krasnor, multiple people and contexts can reinforce both
2009; Simpkins et al., 2006). The key develop- positive development and student engagement.
mental tasks in middle childhood include the Indeed, Ladd et al. (1999) found that stressful
development of concrete operational reasoning, teacher and peer relationships negatively influ-
skill learning, and self-evaluation (Newman & enced classroom engagement, which was defined
Newman, 2009); thus, children continue to develop by participation in classroom activities and aca-
their self-regulatory skills that encourage self-per- demic achievement. A warm and supportive stu-
ceptions of competence and intrinsic motivation dent-teacher relationship has been shown to
(Appleton et al., 2008; Ripke et al., 2006; Simpkins facilitate gains in achievement (Birch & Ladd,
et al., 2006; Skinner et al., 2008). Having positive 1997; Hughes et al., 2008; Skinner et al., 2008)
self-perceptions and self-efficacy beliefs has been with elementary school students reporting greater
linked to academic achievement as well as future classroom support than in middle and high school
activity participation (Appleton et al., 2008; (Marks, 2000). Peer validation has been shown
Simpkins et al., 2006). In middle childhood, stu- to improve school living and engagement, with
dents with high cognitive engagement are char- engagement defined as classroom involvement
acterized as having high self-efficacy beliefs and behaviors (Ladd et al., 1999).
and being mastery oriented (Luo et al., 2009).
Subsequently, children demonstrating high cogni- Future Research
tive engagement are more likely to sustain their Extant research has identified middle childhood
engagement in school and activities over time as the prime developmental period to cultivate
(Ripke et al., 2006; Rose-Krasnor, 2009). In this student engagement given that children become
way, cognitive engagement is an individual char- increasingly involved in relationships outside of
acteristic that facilitates interactions within the the home and move into formal schooling. As
school context and that encourages engagement. such, the literature’s focus on the school context
The child’s engagement in activities also is justifiable because it is through these increased
becomes more self-directed compared to early school experiences that children gain opportuni-
childhood; as children develop a greater sense of ties to develop their academic engagement.
self-efficacy, the role of parents’ and teachers’ However, we cannot neglect the potential influ-
demands on classroom and activity engagement ence that other contexts, such as the home and
wane (Hughes, Luo, Kwok, & Loyd, 2008; Skinner neighborhood, still have on a child’s develop-
et al., 2008). In other words, while parents and ment and student engagement. Indeed, related
teachers may still introduce children to activities research on adolescents has shown that having
and promote activity participation, children can positive bonds with one’s parents, peers, and
begin to develop their own beliefs and interests teachers lays the foundation for supportive learning
toward the activity, which drives their future environments, which in turn increase academic
engagement (Ripke et al., 2006). achievement and social skill development (Eccles,
2004; Libbey, 2004). Thus, to expand our under-
Emotional Engagement standing of the multidimensionality of student
A child’s emotional engagement is represented engagement, similar research on the implications
by the extent to which the child feels a sense of of person-environment fit on engagement during
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 55

the middle childhood and elementary school student engagement during adolescence (Libbey,
years is needed; longitudinal research on early 2004; Mullis, Rathge & Mullis, 2003). Overall,
and middle childhood can enhance the discussion compared to the literature on student engagement
on student engagement, especially when consid- in early and middle childhood, research on stu-
ering student engagement within a developmen- dent engagement during adolescence has clearly
tal and ecological context. delineated between behavioral, cognitive, and
emotional engagement.

Adolescence Behavioral Engagement


For adolescents, behavioral engagement is con-
Given that engagement research started off as a sistently defined as time on task, study behaviors,
model for understanding dropout (Finn, 1989), school and class attendance, and participation in
there are a multitude of studies that cover the class discussions. Often, teacher reports of stu-
adolescent years—the time when youth have the dent behaviors are used to gauge behavioral
opportunity to dropout. Moreover, Eccles et al. engagement. In addition, official school atten-
(1993) showed a decline in student engagement dance records and adolescent self-reports are also
during the transition to junior high school. They widely used in the literature. Most of the research
documented that these changes in engagement on adolescent behavioral engagement has focused
are a function of poor person-environment fit on student truancy and dropout, which Blumenfeld
through decreased opportunities for autonomy and et al. (2005) argued reflects the disengaged stu-
relatedness at critical point in development when dent. Many disengaged students are dissatisfied
both aspects are important in explaining healthy with school, are disruptive in the classroom, have
developmental outcomes (Eccles et al., 1993). parents that are more controlling, and have more
After this period of early adolescence, Janosz, family conflict (Corville-Smith, Ryan, Adams, &
Archambault, Morizot, and Pagani (2008) found Dalicandro, 1998). With regard to the family’s
that student engagement tends to be stable for influence, Leone and Richards (1989) found that
many over the course of adolescence and that adolescents who completed their homework with
many display moderate to high levels of behav- their parents had higher achievement scores.
ioral, cognitive, and emotional engagement, albeit Shumow and Miller (2001) also found that paren-
lower than in the middle schooling years. tal assistance with homework was positively
Developmentally, during adolescence, indi- associated with measures of school engagement.
viduals experience rapid physical maturation as Beyond the family, peers, teachers, and extracur-
well as rapid development of cognitive skills. ricular activities can influence the development
Youth begin to have greater self-reflection, of student engagement during adolescence. As
become more deliberate and focused, and are described in the developmental section, two key
able to hypothesize and think about several strat- developmental tasks during adolescence is the
egies or outcomes for these hypotheses simulta- increasing salience and influence of platonic and
neously rather than focusing on just one domain romantic peer relationships. For instance, several
or issue at a time (Keating, 2004). Thus, the abil- studies suggest that peers are particularly influen-
ity to become cognitively engaged with school is tial on adolescents’ day-to-day school activities
greater during adolescence compared to both such as doing homework and the effort put forth
early and middle childhood. Peers also become during class (Midgely & Urdan, 1995; Steinberg
even more salient compared to prior develop- et al., 1996). Klem and Connell (2004) found that
mental periods, and Ryan (2001) demonstrated middle-school student attendance was higher
that peers significantly predicted changes in aca- when their teachers created caring, well-struc-
demic performance over time. Experiences with tured classroom environments. Extracurricular
peers coupled with the family, classroom, and and after-school activities provide another way
school context are important determinants of for adolescents to be behaviorally engaged with
56 D. Mahatmya et al.

the school context outside of the classroom envi- Steinberg, Lamborn, Dornbusch, and Darling
ronment (Feldman & Matjasko, 2005). This fur- (1992) found a positive relationship between
ther serves to illustrate the importance of authoritative parenting and cognitive engage-
recognizing how individual interactions and char- ment. Ryan and Patrick (2001) found that stu-
acteristics within one context (e.g., school, after- dents’ perceptions of teacher support were a
school) may be coupled with congruent or significant predictor of cognitive engagement
divergent processes in another (e.g., home) to during middle school. Ryan and Patrick (2001)
drive an individual toward engagement. found that peer group characteristics were sig-
nificantly related to achievement orientation (i.e.,
Cognitive Engagement intrinsic motivation) and that peers significantly
Cognitive engagement is defined as attention to predicted decreases in achievement orientation
task, task mastery, and preference for challenging over time. However, Goodenow and Grady (1993)
tasks. During adolescence, youth have developed found that peer academic values were less impor-
the self-regulatory skills necessary for the self- tant than feelings of school belonging in explain-
perceptions of competence and intrinsic motiva- ing adolescent academic motivation. In an
tion, and abstract thinking. Furthermore, as experimental study conducted on a sample of col-
students move from elementary to middle school, lege students, Patrick, Tisley, and Kempler (2000)
their desire for easy work increases. However, found that teacher enthusiasm was related to
the standards-based educational context in the higher intrinsic motivation scores. Thus, echoing
USA tends to foster extrinsic motivation, which the theoretical considerations we described, stu-
can create dissonance between the adolescent’s dents’ cognitive engagement is situated within a
developmental characteristics and the learning set of overlapping environmental systems that
environments in which they participate. Indeed, interact to shape development.
as students progress from elementary through
high school, their self-worth increasingly depends Emotional Engagement
more on their ability to achieve competitively Emotions such as fear, anxiety, boredom, or
(Harari & Covington, 1981). Extrinsic rewards enthusiasm about a school-related task have
for learning, such as good grades and perfor- been considered in investigations of emotional
mance on standardized tests, are symbols of suc- engagement in academic tasks. Along with
cess that maintain one’s self-worth. The increased behavioral and cognitive engagement, emotional
emphasis on competition and evaluation of stu- engagement also tends to decrease upon the tran-
dent performance from elementary through high sition to adolescence (Eccles et al., 1993).
school (Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 2001) Caraway, Tucker, Reinke, and Hall (2003) inves-
may, in part, contribute to the documented decline tigated the relationship between fear of failure
in students’ intrinsic motivation from elementary and academic engagement. They found that fear
through middle school (Lepper, Corpus, & of failure significantly predicted a decrease in
Iyengar, 2005) and preference for challenge, GPA. In addition, test anxiety was negatively
curiosity, interest, and mastery from elementary related to grades, but it was not significantly
school to high school (Harter & Jackson, 1992). related to student engagement or attendance.
Despite this decline in intrinsic motivation McNeely, Nonnemaker, and Blum (2002) found
over the course of childhood and adolescence, that adolescents who report higher levels of
certain contextual conditions are related to higher school connectedness had higher grades and
levels of intrinsic motivation. Gottfried, Fleming, were less likely to skip school. Furthermore, cer-
and Gottfried (1998) found that a cognitively tain schools were more likely to have students
stimulating home environment (e.g., access to who reported higher levels of school connected-
hobbies, books, trips to museums) was signifi- ness. Smaller schools and those with less harsh
cantly related to academic intrinsic motivation disciplinary policies tended to have students
over the course of childhood and adolescence. who reported feeling connected to their schools.
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 57

In a study using experiential sampling methods,


Shernoff (2010) investigated whether the qual-
Conclusion
ity of experience in after-school programs
At the onset of this chapter, we offered two main
mediated the relationship between program par-
guiding questions, essentially how developmen-
ticipation and academic achievement. He found
tal tasks influence student engagement and vice
that feelings of challenge and importance while
versa. These two questions capture the idea that
participating in after-school programs were
human development and the development of stu-
positively related to academic achievement
dent engagement can and most likely occur in
(Shernoff 2010).
tandem. All things considered, there are potential
Knollmann and Wild (2007) explored whether
connections between research on child and ado-
the relationship between parental support for
lescent development and the development of stu-
autonomy and emotional engagement with
dent engagement. The connections, however, are
homework varied by adolescent cognitive
not always transparent, and thus this chapter
engagement (i.e., intrinsic vs. extrinsic motiva-
aimed to present one interpretation of the two
tion). Even though autonomy is a key develop-
streams of theory and research.
mental task of adolescence, Knollmann and Wild
As discussed in this chapter, theoretically, a
found that extrinsically motivated students
combination of the bioecological theory of human
reported more negative affect under autonomy-
development (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998),
supportive conditions while the opposite was
person-environment fit perspective (Eccles et al.,
true for intrinsically motivated adolescents. This
1993), and the participation-identification model
suggests that cognitive engagement moderates
(Finn, 1989; Reschly, 2010) can create a more
the influence of family factors on emotional
comprehensive picture of student engagement,
engagement. In this, we once again see the inter-
its correlates, and its consequences. Where the
play between the individual’s developmental
participation-identification model excels in out-
characteristics and the manifestation of student
lining the development of student engagement, it
engagement.
does not map out how student engagement itself
occurs within a larger developmental sequence.
Future Research
The student engagement literature does point out
While a large amount of research on student
that engagement changes as students progress
engagement during adolescence exists, there are
through school (Finn, 1989) because of changing
some notable gaps. First, longitudinal work is
opportunities for engagement due to changing
needed that links all three aspects of student
contexts. Furthermore, it is important to include a
engagement during adolescence with early and
discussion of developmental tasks because chil-
middle childhood measures of engagement.
dren and adolescent may face challenges in suc-
Making such links will allow us to understand
cessfully reaching those tasks, which may cause
the important precursors of adolescent student
some youth to be ill-equipped to reach their full
engagement and the potential reciprocal pro-
potential for student engagement. The opposite
cesses that exist between developmental tasks
may be true as well, where changes and chal-
and student engagement. Furthermore, research
lenges in student engagement influence success-
on the specific forms of behavioral engagement
ful developmental transitions.
is needed. We know relatively little about time
The emphasis on the proximal processes, con-
on task, disruptive classroom behavior, and par-
texts, and individual characteristics that contrib-
ticipation in classroom discussions during ado-
ute to human development in the bioecological
lescence. In addition, studies that use experiential
theory and person-environment fit perspective
sampling methodology will continue to docu-
further help to enhance our understanding of the
ment the links between behaviors, cognitions,
developmental context of student engagement.
and emotions around school-related tasks during
First, the bioecological theory recognizes that
adolescence.
58 D. Mahatmya et al.

development is shaped by interactions between the developmental processes that occur across
people, their characteristics, and their contexts multiple contexts such as the school, home, and
(Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998). Applying this neighborhood to not only encourage healthy
to the student engagement research discussed in human development, but the development of stu-
this chapter, we see that the manifestation of dent engagement as well. According to bioeco-
behavioral, cognitive, and emotional engagement logical theory, it is important to account for
at the different developmental periods is often a multiple contexts in understanding student
result of the individual’s own capacities and his/ engagement. Additional research is needed on the
her participation in the family, and especially, family context and how parents support or detract
school contexts. As Finn (1989) mentioned as from the development of student engagement.
well, the contexts are themselves important as Furthermore, the person-environment fit perspec-
they provide the opportunities for children and tive calls attention to whether specific contexts fit
adolescents to be engaged. More generally, the with the developmental needs of children and
contexts provide the social and structural adolescents, whether these contexts are in syn-
resources that can mold healthy development. chrony with each other, and the changing nature
Additionally, it is important to have congruence and consequences of contextual synchrony/
between the person and their contexts. As dissynchrony across early childhood, middle
espoused by the person-environment fit perspec- childhood, and adolescence. Methodologically,
tive (Eccles et al., 1993), having synchrony across we recommend further development of observa-
healthy environments fosters healthy develop- tional (Pittman, Merita, Tolman, Yohalem, &
ment and in the same vein can facilitate the devel- Ferber, 2003) and survey measures (Bohnert
opment of student engagement. As discussed in et al., 2010; Lippman & Rivers, 2008) of individ-
this chapter, behavioral, cognitive, and emotional ual-level behavioral, emotional, and cognitive
engagement are conceptually and methodologi- student engagement, and that these measures be
cally distinct at each developmental period. These integrated into the multifaceted and longitudinal
differences may be a result of the maturation of studies of student educational attainment and stu-
developmental tasks and the changing contextual dent activity involvement.
landscape for the children and adolescents. Taken
together, student engagement is a nuanced devel-
opmental outcome. Application and Policy Implications

There is growing recognition among educators


Future Research and policymakers that student engagement inside
and outside (i.e., civic engagement; not discussed
To further understand the differences in student in this chapter) of school settings is important for
engagement across developmental periods, devel- the positive growth and development of America’s
opmental and engagement research should focus young children. In addition, a new report finds
on growing the empirical evidence for the ecolo- that student engagement may be particularly
gies and interplay of developmental tasks and the important for older adolescents who are prepar-
development of student engagement. Indeed, as ing for the roles of adult life (Deschenes et al.,
discussed in this chapter, the research on student 2010); yet the extant literature on the develop-
engagement in early and middle childhood is mental precursors of student engagement or how
especially lacking, and a majority of the research student engagement may manifest across devel-
on student engagement has focused on just the opmental periods is limited, resulting in poten-
school context. While the school context does tially discontinuous developmental transitions
become increasingly salient in the lives of chil- into adult roles (Sherrod & Lauckhard, 2009).
dren and adolescents, it is necessary to understand Again, there is an important interplay between
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 59

the development of engagement and human Bierman, K. L., Domitrovich, C. E., Nix, R. L., Gest, S. D.,
development more generally that needs to be Welsh, J. A., Greenberg, M. T., & Gill, S. (2008).
Promoting academic and socioal-emotional school
recognized. readiness: The head start REDI program. Child
To facilitate the many transitions children and Development, 79, 1802–1817.
adolescents face, family, school, and community Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher-child
initiatives that promote student engagement may relationship and children’s early school adjustment.
Journal of School Psychology, 35, 61–79.
be crucial for the growth and development of stu- Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition
dent engagement. Moreover, we argue for the and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of
potential importance of creating integrative mul- children’s functioning at school entry. American
ticontextual partnerships that enhance student Psychologist, 57, 111–127.
Blumenfeld, P. C., Kempler, T. M., & Krajcik, J. S. (2006).
engagement across developmental periods. Motivation and cognitive engagement in learning
Indeed, the bioecological theory and person- environments. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge
environment fit perspective paired with the devel- handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 475–488).
opmental and engagement research reviewed in New York: Cambridge University Press.
Blumenfeld, P. C., Modell, J., Bartko, W. T., Secada, W.,
this chapter suggest that what might be the most Fredricks, J., Friedel, J., & Parks, A. (2005). School
optimal for successful student engagement and engagement of inner city students during middle child-
human development is consistency through the hood. In C. R. Cooper, C. Garcia-Coll, W. T. Bartko,
developmental periods in providing adequate H. M. Davis, & C. Chatman (Eds.), Developmental
pathways through middle childhood: Rethinking diver-
resources to address the developmental and edu- sity and contexts as recourses (pp. 145–170). Mahwah,
cational challenges in childhood and adolescence. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
As we began this chapter saying, the development Bohnert, A., Fredricks, J., & Randall, E. (2010).
of student engagement must be understood within Capturing unique dimensions of youth organized
activity involvement: Theoretical and methodological
the context of the individual’s developmental considerations. Review of Educational Research,
history. The two are not separate outcomes, Online First, May 2010.
rather they complement each other; both involve Bronfenbrenner, U. (1975). Is early intervention effec-
a sequence of events, and by recognizing that tive? In E. Stuening & M. Guttentag (Eds.), Handbook
of evaluation research (Vol. 2, pp. 519–603). Beverly
these sequences occur simultaneously, educators, Hills, CA: Sage.
researchers, policy makers, and other profession- Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture
als can build environments that promote positive reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bio-
development in multiple domains. ecological model. Psychological Review, 101,
568–586.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology
of developmental processes. In W. Damon (Series Ed.)
& R. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology:
References Vol. 1. Theory (5th ed., pp. 993–1028). New York:
John Wiley.
Anderson, J. C., Funk, J. B., Elliot, R., & Smith, P. H. Caraway, K., Tucker, C. M., Reinke, W. M., & Hall, C.
(2003). Parental support and pressure and children’s (2003). Self-efficacy, goal orientation, and fear of fail-
extracurricular activities: Relationships with amount ure as predictors of school engagement with high
of involvement and affective experience of participa- school students. Psychology in the Schools, 40,
tion. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 417–427.
24, 241–257. Carbonaro, W. J. (1998). A little help from my friend’s
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. parents: Social closure and educational outcomes.
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- Sociology of Education, 71, 295–313.
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Catalano, R. F., Berglund, M. L., Ryan, J. A. M., Lonczack,
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. H. S., & Hawkins, J. D. (2004). Positive youth devel-
Barnett, S. (1995). Long-term effects of early childhood opment in the United States: Research findings on
programs on cognitive and school outcomes. The evaluations of positive youth development programs.
Future of Children, 5, 25–50. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and
Bartko, T. W. (2005). The ABCS of engagement in out-of- Social Science, 591, 98–124.
school programs. New Directions for Youth Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994).
Development, 105, 109–120. Educational risk and resilience in African-American
60 D. Mahatmya et al.

youth: Context, self, action, and outcomes in school. Epstein, J. (1983). Selecting friends in contrasting sec-
Child Development, 65, 493–506. ondary school environments. In J. Epstein &
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. (1991). Competence, auton- N. Karweit (Eds.), Friends in school. New York:
omy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of self- Academic.
system processes. In M. Gunnar & A. Sroufe (Eds.), Fantuzzo, J., & McWayne, C. (2002). The relationship
Minnesota symposium on child development (Vol. 22, between peer-play interactions in the family context
pp. 43–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and dimensions of school readiness for low-income
Coolahan, K., Fantuzzo, J., Mendez, J., & McDermott, P. preschool children. Journal of Educational Psychology,
(2000). Preschool peer interactions and readiness to 94, 79–87.
learn: Relationships between classroom peer play and Feldman, A. F., & Matjasko, J. L. (2005). The role of
learning behaviors and conduct. Journal of Educational school-based extracurricular activities in adolescent
Psychology, 92, 458–465. development: A comprehensive review and future
Corville-Smith, J., Ryan, B. A., Adams, G. R., & directions. Review of Educational Research, 75,
Dalicandro, T. (1998). Distinguishing absentee stu- 159–210.
dents from regular attenders: The combined influence Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
of personal, family, and school factors. Journal of Educational Research, 59, 117–142.
Youth and Adolescence, 27, 629–640. Finn, J. D., Folger, J., & Cox, D. (1991). Measuring partici-
Currie, J. (2001). Early childhood education programs. pation among elementary grade students. Educational
Journal of Economic Perspectives, 15, 213–238. and Psychological Measurement, 51, 393–402.
Dearing, E., Wimer, C., Simpkins, S., Lund, T., Bouffard, Fletcher, A. C., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Mekos, D. (2000).
W., Caronongan, P., et al. (2009). Do neighborhood Parental influences on adolescent involvement in com-
and home contexts help explain why low-income chil- munity activities. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
dren miss opportunities to participate in activities out- 10, 29–48.
side of school? Developmental Psychology, 45, Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
1545–1562. School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
Denault, A. S., & Poulin, F. (2009). Intensity and breadth the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
of participation in organized activities during the 59–109.
adolescent years: Multiple associations with youth Goodenow, C., & Grady, K. E. (1993). The relationship of
outcomes. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9, school belonging and friends’ values to academic
119–1213. motivation among urban adolescent students. The
Deschenes, S. N., Arbreton, A., Little, P. M., Herrera, C., Journal of Experimental Education, 62, 60–71.
Grossman, J. B., Weiss, H. B., et al. (2010). Engaging Goodenow, J. (1995). Differentiating among social
older youth: Program and city-level strategies to sup- contexts: By spatial features, forms of participation,
port sustained participation in out-of-school time. and social contracts. In P. Moen, G. H. Elder Jr., & K.
Harvard Family Research Project. Retrieved from Luschner (Eds.), Examining lives in context:
http://www.hfrp.org/out-of-school-time/publications- Perspective on the ecology of human development (pp.
resources/engaging-older-youth-program-and-city- 269–302). Washington, DC: American Psychological
level-strategies-to-support-sustained-participation-in- Association.
out-of-school-time. Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (1998).
Dumais, S. A. (2006). Elementary school students’ extra- Role of cognitively stimulating home environment in
curricular activities: The effects of participation on children’s academic intrinsic motivation: A longitudi-
achievement and teachers’ evaluations. Sociological nal study. Child Development, 69, 1448–1460.
Spectrum, 26, 117–147. Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001).
Eccles, J. S. (2004). Schools, academic motivation, and Continuity of academic intrinsic motivation from
stage-environment fit. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg childhood through late adolescence: A longitudinal
(Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 3–13.
pp. 125–153). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Gutman, L. M., & Eccles, J. S. (2007). Stage-environment
Eccles, J. S. (2005). The present and future of research on fit during adolescence: Trajectories of family relations
activity settings as developmental contexts. In and adolescent outcomes. Developmental Psychology,
J. Mahoney, R. W. Larson, & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Organized 43, 522–537.
activities as contexts of development: Extracurricular Hair, E., Halle, T., & Terry-Humen, E. (2006). Children’s
activities, after-school and community programs (pp. school readiness in the ECLS-K: Predictions to aca-
353–374). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. demic, health, and social outcomes in first grade. Early
Eccles, J. S., Midgefield, C., & Wigfield, A. (1993). Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 431–454.
Development during adolescence: The impact of Harari, O., & Covington, M. V. (1981). Reactions to
stage-environment fit on young adolescents’ experi- achievement behavior from a teacher and student per-
ences in schools and in families. American spective: A developmental analysis. American
Psychologist, 48, 90–101. Educational Research Journal, 18, 15–28.
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 61

Harter, S., & Jackson, B. K. (1992). Trait vs. nontrait con- Lerner, R. M., Brentano, C., Dowling, E. M., & Anderson,
ceptualizations of intrinsic/extrinsic motivational ori- P. M. (2002). Positive youth development: Thriving as
entation. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 209–230. the basis of personhood and civil society. New
Hughes, J. N., Luo, W., Kwok, O. M., & Loyd, L. K. Directions for Youth Development, 95, 11–33.
(2008). Teacher-student support, effortful engage- Lerner, R. M., & Castellino, D. R. (2002). Contemporary
ment, and achievement: A 3-year longitudinal study. developmental theory and adolescence: Developmental
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 1–14. systems and applied developmental science. Journal
Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. S. of Adolescent Health, 31, 122–135.
(2008). School engagement trajectories and their dif- Libbey, H. P. (2004). Measuring student relationships to
ferential predictive relations to dropout. Journal of school: Attachment, bonding, connectedness, and
Social Issues, 64, 21–40. engagement. Journal of School Health, 74, 274–283.
Kagan, S. L. (1990). Readiness 2000: Rethinking rhetoric Liew, J., McTigue, E., Barrois, L., & Hughes, J. (2008).
and responsibility. The Phi Delta Kappan, 72, Adaptive and effortful control and academic self-effi-
272–279. cacy beliefs on achievement: A longitudinal study of 1
Karcher, M. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Portwood, S. G., Sipe, through 3 graders. Early Child Research Quarterly,
C. L., & Taylor, A. S. (2006). Mentoring programs: 23, 515–526.
A framework to inform program development, Lippman, L., & Rivers, A. (2008). Assessing school
research, and evaluation. Journal of Community engagement: A guide for out-of-school time program
Psychology, 34, 709–725. practitioners. Child Trends Research Brief, 39, 1–5.
Keating, D. P. (2004). Cognitive and brain development. Lohman, B. J., Kaura, S. A., & Newman, B. M. (2007).
In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook Matched or mismatched environments? The relation-
of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 45–84). ship of family and school differentiation to adoles-
New York: Wiley. cents’ psychosocial adjustment. Youth & Society, 39,
Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships mat- 3–32.
ter: Linking teacher support to student engagement Luo, W., Hughes, J. A., Liew, J., & Kwok, O. (2009).
and achievement. Journal of School Health, 74, Classifying academically at-risk first graders into
262–282. engagement types: Association with long-term
Knollmann, M., & Wild, E. (2007). Quality of parental achievement trajectories. The Elementary School
support and students’ emotions during homework: Journal, 109, 380.
Moderating effects of students’ motivational orienta- Mahoney, J., & Bergman, L. (2002). Conceptual and
tions. European Journal of Psychology of Education, methodological considerations in a developmental
22, 63–76. approach to the study of positive adaptation. Applied
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The Developmental Psychology, 23, 195–217.
development of self-regulation in the first four years of Mahoney, J., & Magnusson, D. (2001). Parent participa-
life. Child Development, 72, 1091–1111. tion in community activities and the persistence of
Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as criminality. Development and Psychopathology, 13,
a personality characteristic of young children: 125–141.
Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Mahoney, J., Vandell, D. L., Simpkins, S., & Zarrett, N.
Personality, 71, 1087. (2009). Adolescent out-of-school activities. In R. M.
Kuperschmidt, J. B., Bryant, D., & Willoughby, M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent
(2000). Prevalence of aggressive behaviors among psychology (3rd ed., pp. 228–269). New York: John
preschoolers in Head Start and community child care Wiley.
programs. Behavioral Disorders, 26, 42–52. Mahoney, J., & Wheeden, C. A. (1999). The effect of
Ladd, G. W., Birch, S. H., & Buhs, E. S. (1999). Children’s teacher style of interactive engagement of preschool-
social and scholastic lives in kindergarten: Related aged children with special learning needs. Early
spheres of influence. Child Development, 70, Childhood Research Quarterly, 14, 51–68.
1373–1400. Malmskog, S., & McDonnell, A. P. (1999). Teacher-
Larson, R. W. (2000). Toward a psychology of positive mediated facilitation of engagement of children with
youth development. American Psychologist, 55, developmental delays in inclusive preschools. Topics
170–183. in Early Childhood Special Education, 19, 203–216.
Leone, C. M., & Richards, M. H. (1989). Classwork and Manke, B., McGuire, S., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M.,
homework in early adolescence: The ecology of & Plomin, R. (1995). Genetic contributions to adoles-
achievement. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18, cents’ extrafamilial social interactions: Teachers, best
531–548. friends, and peers. Social Development, 4, 238–256.
Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional
Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high
classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. school years. American Educational Research Journal,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 184–196. 37, 153–184.
62 D. Mahatmya et al.

McCormick, M. C., Brooks-Gunn, J., Buka, S. L., Raver, C. C. (2002). Emotions matter: Making the case for
Goldman, J., Yu, J., Salganik, M., et al. (2006). Early the role of young children’s emotional development
intervention in LBW premature infants: Results at 18 for early school readiness. Social Policy Report, 16,
years of age for infant health and development pro- 3–18.
gram. Pediatrics, 117, 771–780. Reschly, A. L. (2010). Reading and school completion:
McNeely, C. A., Nonnemaker, J. M., & Blum, R. W. Critical connections and Matthew effects. Reading &
(2002). Promoting school connectedness: Evidence Writing Quarterly, 26, 67–90.
from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Ripke, M. N., Huston, A. C., & Casey, D. M. (2006).
Health. Journal of School Health, 72, 138–146. Low-income children’s activity participation as a
McWilliam, R. A., & Bailey, D. B. (1992). Promoting predictor of psychosocial and academic outcomes in
engagement and mastery. In D. B. Bailty & M. Wolery middle childhood and adolescence. In A. C. Huston &
(Eds.), Teaching infants and preschoolers with dis- M. N. Ripke (Eds.), Developmental contexts of middle
abilities (2nd ed., pp. 230–255). New York: childhood: Bridges to adolescence and adulthood
MacMillian. (pp. 260–282). New York: Cambridge University Press.
McWilliam, R. A., & Casey, A. M. (2008). Engagement of Roeser, R. W., & Eccles, J. S. (1998). Adolescents’ per-
every child in the preschool classroom. Baltimore: ceptions of middle school: Relation to longitudinal
Paul H. Brooke s Publishing Co. changes in academic and psychological adjustment.
Midgely, C., & Urdan, T. (1995). Predictors of middle Journal of Research on Adolescence, 86, 123–158.
school students’ use of self-handicapping strategies. Rose-Krasnor, L. (2009). Future directions in youth involve-
Journal of Early Adolescence, 15, 389–411. ment research. Social Development, 18, 497–509.
Miller, P. H. (2002). Theories of developmental psychol- Ryan, A. (2001). The peer group as a context for the
ogy (4th ed.). New York: Worth. development of young adolescent motivation and
Morris, P., & Kalil, A. (2006). Out-of-school time use achievement. Child Development, 72, 1135–1150.
during middle childhood in a low-income sample: Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2001). The classroom social
Do combinations of activities affect achievement environment and changes in adolescents’ motivation
and behavior? In A. Huston & M. Ripke (Eds.), and engagement during middle school. American
Developmental contexts in middle childhood: Bridges Educational Research Journal, 38, 437–460.
to adolescence and adulthood (pp. 237–259). Shernoff, D. J. (2010). Engagement in after-school pro-
New York: Cambridge University Press. grams as a predictor of social competence and aca-
Mullis, R. L., Rathge, R., & Mullis, A. K. (2003). demic performance. American Journal of Community
Predictors of academic performance during early ado- Psychology, 45, 325–337.
lescence: A contextual view. International Journal of Sherrod, L. R., & Lauckhardt, J. (2009). The development
Behavioral Development, 27, 541–548. of citizenship. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.),
Newman, B. M., & Newman, P. R. (2009). Development Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.,
through life: A psychosocial approach (10th ed.). pp. 372–407). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cenage Learning. Shumow, L., & Miller, J. D. (2001). Parents’ at-home and
Parra, G. R., Dubois, D. L., Neville, H. A., Pugh-Lilly, A. at-school academic involvement with young adoles-
O., & Povinelli, N. (2002). Mentoring relationships cents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 21, 68–91.
for youth: Investigation of a process-oriented model. Simpkins, S. D., Fredricks, J. A., Davis-Kean, P. E., &
Journal of Community Psychology, 30, 367–388. Eccles, J. S. (2006). Healthy mind, healthy habits: The
Patrick, B. C., Tinsely, J., & Kempler, T. (2000). “What’s influence of activity involvement in middle childhood.
everyone so excited about?”: The effects of teacher In A. C. Huston & M. N. Ripke (Eds.), Developmental
enthusiasm on teacher on student intrinsic motivation contexts of middle childhood: Bridges to adolescence
and vitality. The Journal of Experimental Education, and adulthood (pp. 283–302). New York: Cambridge
68, 217–236. University Press.
Persson, A., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2007). Staying in or Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kinderman, T.
moving away from structured activities: Explanations (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom:
involving parents and peers. Developmental Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of
Psychology, 43, 197–207. Educational Psychology, 100, 765–781.
Pittman, K. J., Merita, I., Tolman, J., Yohalem, N., & Steinberg, L., Bradford, B., & Dornbusch, S. (1996). Beyond
Ferber, T. (2003). Preventing problems, promoting the classroom: Why school reform has failed and what
development, encouraging engagement: Competing parents need to do. New York: Simon & Schuster.
priorities or inseparable goals? Washington, DC: Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Dornbusch, S. M., &
Forum for Youth Investment. Darling, N. (1992). Impact of parenting practices on
Portes, A. (2000). The two meanings of social capital. adolescent achievement: Authoritative parenting,
Sociological Forum, 15, 1–12. school involvement, and encouragement to succeed.
Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (2004). Early learning and Child Development, 63, 1266–1281.
school readiness: Can early intervention make a differ- Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2001). Degree of
ence? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 471–491. embeddedness of ecological systems as a measure
3 Engagement Across Developmental Periods 63

of ease of adaptation to the environment. In E. L. Weiss, H. B., Little, P. M. D., & Bouffard, S. M. (2005).
Grigorenko & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Family environ- More than just being there: Balancing the participation
ment and intellectual functioning: A lifespan perspec- equation. New Directions for Youth Development, 105,
tive (pp. 243–262). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 15–31.
Vandell, D. L., Pierce, K. M., & Dadisman, K. (2005). Wigfield, A., & Karpathia, M. (1991). Who am I and what
Out-of-school settings as a developmental context for can I do? Children’s self-concepts and motivation in
children and youth. Advances in Child Development achievement situations. Educational Psychologist, 26,
and Behavior, 33, 43–77. 233–261.
Ethnicity and Student Engagement
4
Gary E. Bingham and Lynn Okagaki

Abstract
The underachievement of African American, Latino, and American
Indian students in the United States has been partially attributed to poor
engagement in school (e.g., Connell, Spencer & Aber, 1994; Steele,
1997). In this chapter, we consider the role of ethnicity in student engage-
ment. A number of factors have been posited to influence minority stu-
dents’ engagement in school. Okagaki (2001) conceptualized these
factors into three broad domains: the roles of the student, the family, and
the school. We begin with a discussion of factors within the student,
such as students’ ethnic identity beliefs, experiences with discrimina-
tion, and bicultural efficacy, and the relations of these factors to stu-
dents’ engagement in school. In the second section, we examine the role
that parents’ beliefs, expectations, and behaviors play in ethnic minority
students’ engagement in school, paying particular attention to beliefs
and values that can be attributed to parents’ cultural models of education
(Gallimore & Goldenberg, 2001; Lareau, 1996). Third, we consider how
factors associated with teachers, peers, and friends relate to ethnic
minority students’ engagement in school. In particular, we focus on stu-
dents’ access to same ethnic teachers and peers, the quality of relation-
ships with teachers and friends, and pedagogical practices that may
facilitate ethnic minority students’ engagement in school. Finally, we
identify the need for stronger empirical research around the identifica-
tion and amelioration of the discontinuities between home and school
cultures.

G.E. Bingham, Ph.D. (*)


Department of Early Childhood Education,
College of Education, Georgia State University,
Atlanta, GA, USA
e-mail: gbingham@gsu.edu
L. Okagaki, Ph.D.
College of Education and Human Development,
University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 65


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_4, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
66 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

In a reading class I observed, the teacher said, within the dimension of emotional and cognitive
“We are studying tall tales. This is something that engagement. As Skinner and colleagues articulate
cannot be true. Like Pecos Bill. They said he lived
with the coyotes. You see, it can’t be true.” Two (Skinner, Kindermann, & Furrer, 2009; Skinner,
Navajo students look at each other and in unison Marchand, Furrer, & Kindermann, 2008), the
said, “But us Navajo, we live on the reservation relation between student engagement and motiva-
with the coyotes.” The teacher replied, “Well, tion is cyclical and heavily influenced by contex-
I don’t know anything about that. Let’s talk about
parables now.” (Deyhle, 1995) tual variables outside the learner. Given the
relatively complex social worlds that many ethnic
What does engagement mean for a Navajo minority children and youth navigate, considering
student who lives his life traversing two very dif- both engagement and motivational factors may be
ferent worlds—home and school—each day? In important to ensuring their academic success.
this chapter, we consider the role of ethnicity in Second, researchers have defined ethnicity
student engagement. We begin with some cave- in different ways (e.g., Harwood, Handwerker,
ats. First, in the literature on student engagement, Schoelmerich, & Leyendecker, 2001; Phinney,
researchers have defined engagement in multiple 1996) and identified ethnic groups in various ways.
ways and have often used measures that combine For example, researchers sometimes report on
different aspects of engagement or include items Hispanic or Latino Americans as one ethnic group.
representing constructs other than engagement However, this broad category masks the variation
(Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; Libbey, in cultural heritage within the group, which
2004). Within the last decade, however, research- includes individuals who may identify themselves
ers have focused considerable efforts to refine as being, for example, Mexican, Puerto Rican,
the construct distinguishing among types of Cuban, Dominican, Guatemalan, or Ecuadorian.
engagement (e.g., emotional, behavioral, cogni- The histories, customs, economies, and political
tive) and move the field toward alignment of contexts of each of the countries of origin contrib-
measurement with type of engagement (e.g., ute to the heterogeneity of the Hispanic popula-
Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, 2008; tion (e.g., Carrasquillo, 1991; Torres, 2004). Not
Fredricks et al., 2004; Glanville & Wildhagen, only do these ethnic subgroups differ on basic
2007; Libbey, 2004). demographic characteristics, such as age (U.S.
For this chapter, we draw primarily from Department of Commerce, Census Bureau,
Fredericks and colleagues’ (2004) definition of 2001) and country of origin (U.S. Department of
engagement and consider the emotional, behav- Commerce, Census Bureau, 1993), they also differ
ioral, and cognitive aspects as separate, but over- on characteristics that may be relevant to chil-
lapping, constructs. We consider emotional dren’s engagement with school, such as fluency in
engagement to include students’ attitudes toward English (U.S. Department of Education, 2001a).
and feelings about school and schoolwork and Although there is great diversity within ethnic
their relationships with teachers and students. In groups, researchers are often limited by pragmatic
contrast, behavioral engagement has been broadly constraints (e.g., financial resources) and collapse
defined as including activities and behaviors that across subgroups in order to have a reasonable
suggest compliance with school norms, involve- sample size for their study. In secondary data anal-
ment in extracurricular activities at school (e.g., yses, researchers are limited by the definitions of
music, sports, student council), and participation ethnicity that were obtained for the original study.
in class (e.g., asking questions, being attentive, In this chapter, ethnic groups are identified by the
contributing to class discussions, staying on task). labels used by the authors of the cited studies, and
Finally, cognitive engagement encompasses readers should recognize that a sample that is iden-
intrinsic motivation for learning and metacogni- tified by a broad ethnic or racial group category
tive strategy use (e.g., planning, monitoring). (e.g., African American, Asian, Hispanic, Native
Utilizing this framework, we situate student moti- American) in one study may be very different from
vational processes as an overlapping construct a similarly identified sample in another study.
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 67

Third, although ethnic group variation is This chapter provides a framework for orga-
associated with differential learning outcomes nizing the research on and understanding the role
in many countries, the discussion in this chapter of ethnicity in student engagement. Examples of
is limited to ethnicity and education in the research on different ethnic groups are used to
United States. In addition, this chapter focuses illustrate points.
on ethnic groups of color, and both racial and
ethnic minority groups are included in this dis-
cussion. Using the term “ethnic group” in this Ethnicity and Student Engagement
way is not without its problems (e.g., Gutiérrez
& Rogoff, 2003; Helms & Talleyrand, 1997). At Why might one expect student engagement in
minimum, it overlooks the great variation and school to vary across ethnic groups? Behavioral
richness in the ethnic backgrounds of White scientists have generally taken one of two per-
Americans. In general, however, the majority of spectives to understanding the engagement and
research conducted on engagement has focused achievement of students of color. The cultural
on majority students and is covered in other discontinuity view focuses on specific differences
chapters in this handbook. Moreover, research (e.g., language, behavioral norms) between the
on ethnicity, engagement, and student outcomes ethnic culture and the mainstream culture (e.g.,
has grown out of recognition of the differential Machamer & Gruber, 1998; Tharp, 1989; Trueba,
achievement across ethnic groups and concern 1987; Tyler et al., 2008; Weisner, Gallimore, &
for the underachievement of many ethnic groups Jordan, 1988) and the extent to which the discon-
of color. In 2008, out of approximately 48 mil- tinuity between those beliefs and practices inter-
lion public school students in the United States, feres with students’ engagement and learning in
45% were children and adolescents from ethnic school. Researchers holding a contextual view of
groups of color (Aud et al., 2010). Despite cognition (e.g., Laboratory of Comparative
improvements in achievement over the last Human Cognition, 1982; Rogoff, 1990) have
40 years and a narrowing of the achievement argued that: (a) cultural context affects the
gap, Black and Hispanic students continue to lag development of social and cognitive processes,
behind their White counterparts (Rampey, Dion, (b) there are important differences between ethnic
& Donahue, 2009). A number of researchers minority cultures and the mainstream culture,
have theorized that student engagement may and (c) these cultural differences lead to the
explain variation in student achievement across development of different sets of cognitive and
ethnic groups (e.g., Connell, Spencer, & Aber, social behavioral repertoires. When children
1994; Finn & Rock, 1997; Steele, 1997). from ethnic minority cultures begin school, for
Fourth, some have argued that the variation example, they may find it difficult to decode the
observed across racial and ethnic groups may be cues that are presented in the classroom and
more attributable to socioeconomic differences actively engage in the teaching and learning pro-
within a culture than differences across cultural cess. They may experience failure. They may
groups (e.g., Hoff, Laursen, & Tardiff, 2002). perceive their own cultural practices to be deval-
Research that teases apart the contributions of ued in the school context. Ultimately, the enact-
racial/ethnic background and socioeconomic ment of this process is hypothesized to lead to a
backgrounds to cultural models of education is lack of engagement in school.
relatively limited. Where possible, we make An alternative perspective, the cultural eco-
explicit differences that reflect racial and ethnic logical or secondary discontinuity model, empha-
cultures versus socioeconomic differences. In the sizes processes of oppression and discrimination
descriptions of individual studies, we use the ter- toward racial and ethnic minority groups. Ogbu
minology for describing particular groups (e.g., (e.g., Gibson & Ogbu, 1991; Ogbu, 1986) argued
Latino, Hispanic) that was chosen by the authors that certain minority groups—those who became
of the studies. part of the United States through conquest or
68 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

slavery—are treated at the institutional and policy should be viewed from the perspective of the
levels within our society in ways that limit their Navajo culture (e.g., returning to the reservation
ability to succeed economically, professionally, and one’s Navajo community is a positive action).
socially, and politically. In such cases, poor In such cases, the behaviors may be motivated
school achievement may be a conscious choice, primarily by a desire to express the Navajo cul-
an active response to a system that has failed to ture rather than a reflection of the individual’s
work for a particular group of people. For example, disinterest in school.
Fordham and Ogbu (1986) described African Without a doubt, our education system has
American adolescents as developing identities historically been differentially successful in edu-
that were in opposition to the values of the main- cating students across racial and ethnic groups
stream culture, including intentionally not engag- (Farkas, 2003; KewalRamani, Gilbertson, Fox, &
ing in school. Ogbu (1992) posited that those Provasnik, 2007; Miller, 1995). Among the theo-
who face barriers to their success because of dis- ries that have been generated to explain differen-
criminatory practices and policies will find alter- tial achievement across racial and ethnic groups
native venues in which to achieve and will are hypotheses that hinge on variation in student
disengage themselves from trying to achieve in engagement in school. A number of factors have
those domains in which discrimination keeps been posited to influence minority students’
them from succeeding. Psychologists (e.g., engagement with school and achievement.
Osborne, 1997; Steele, 1997) have suggested that Okagaki (2001) conceptualized these factors into
members of groups that have been stigmatized as three broad domains: the roles of the individual,
having low ability in a particular domain will the family, and the school. These factors are dis-
psychologically distance themselves from or cussed in the following three sections, beginning
“disidentify” with that domain. Disidentification with a discussion of racial and ethnic minority
with a domain is hypothesized to protect the indi- students’ beliefs about their ethnicity. In the next
vidual’s self-esteem should the individual do section, we consider how families of color sup-
poorly in that domain. Along the same lines, port their child’s engagement in school. The last
some have proposed that perception of discrimi- section examines ways in which the nature and
nation may lead students to believe that achieving structure of the school may support or inhibit the
school will not make a difference in their lives, engagement of students of color.
and this belief may result in a lack of engagement
in school (e.g., Mickelson, 1990).
Finally, there are models that build on ele- The Role of the Student: Beliefs
ments of both the cultural discontinuity frame- About Ethnicity and Student
work and the cultural ecological model. For Engagement in School
example, Deyhle argued that the discontinuity
between the Navajo culture and the mainstream In what ways are students’ beliefs about their eth-
culture coupled with discrimination against the nicity related to their beliefs about, attitudes
Navajo community led Navajo youth to resist toward, and behavior in school? In this section,
assimilation into the behaviors of the mainstream we consider the ways in which ethnic identity,
culture. Behaviors that appeared to be lack of perception of discrimination, and bicultural iden-
engagement in school were not the development tity have been linked to students’ engagement in
of an oppositional identity as theorized by Ogbu school. Ethnic identity is generally viewed as
but rather the expression of their Navajo culture. being multidimensional, varying across members
Based on work with Navajo youth, Deyhle (1995) of an ethnic group, and changing over time within
has posited that behaviors that are typically inter- an individual, and the literature on ethnic identity
preted by teachers and school administrators as a reflects multiple ways in which ethnic identity
student’s lack of engagement in school (e.g., drop- has been conceptualized and measured by
ping out of college to return to the reservation) researchers (Phinney, 1990). Broadly defined,
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 69

beliefs, feelings, and behaviors contributing to the hypothesis has not been strong (e.g., Flores-
one’s ethnic identity include a cognitive compo- González, 1999; Spencer, Noll, Stoltzfus, &
nent (e.g., knowledge about one’s ethnic culture), Harpalani, 2001; Taylor, Casten, Flickinger,
an emotional component (e.g., attitudes toward Roberts, & Fulmore, 1994).
one’s ethnic culture, feelings of commitment and Based on studies in which ethnic identity and
belonging to one’s ethnic group, and attitudes engagement have been measured, it appears that
toward the majority culture), and a behavioral ethnic minority students who have strong ethnic
component (e.g., participation in traditional cul- identities are more likely to be engaged in school
tural activities, interaction with the majority cul- than those who do not. For example, in a study of
ture). Great diversity exists within any ethnic 606 African American adolescents, Chavous and
group in terms of members’ ethnic identity (e.g., colleagues (2003) examined three components of
Chavous et al., 2003; Keefe & Padilla, 1987; racial identity: (a) centrality, the degree to which
Okagaki & Moore, 2000; Witherspoon, Speight, a Black identity was important to their personal
& Thomas, 1997). Diversity comes in part identity and having relationships with other Black
because an individual’s ethnic identity develops people was important to them; (b) private regard,
over time (e.g., Phinney, 1996), because individ- the positive or negative valence of their feelings
uals within a group differ in their orientation to about being Black and Black people; and (c) pub-
their ethnic culture and to the mainstream culture lic regard, their perception of other people’s
(e.g., Buriel & DeMent, 1997), because within a views of Black people. Based on this multidi-
group, what is important to each group member’s mensional approach to racial identity, four clus-
ethnic identity varies (Phinney), and because eth- ters of students were identified, and differences
nic communities are living entities whose experi- across groups on measures of student engage-
ences, shared beliefs, and practices evolve over ment and academic attainment were observed,
time (Gutiérrez & Rogoff, 2003). with the most distinct differences being for those
Ogbu (1992; Ogbu & Matute-Bianchi, 1986) who exhibited alienation from their Black iden-
argued that to the extent that formal schooling is tity (i.e., low centrality, low private regard, low
associated with mainstream culture in the United public regard) and those who had strong positive
States and not with a minority culture, doing well Black identities (i.e., high centrality, high private
in school may be perceived by students from that regard, low public regard). Relative to other stu-
minority culture as not being compatible with dents, those students who exhibited alienation
their cultural identity. Further, in response to dis- from their Black identity had lower school attach-
crimination, minority students develop an iden- ment (degree to which students liked school) and
tity that is in opposition to the mainstream culture. school efficacy (belief in one’s ability to do well
In their classic ethnographic study of Black ado- in school) scores. Moreover, compared to stu-
lescents, Fordham and Ogbu (1986) introduced dents in the other groups, these adolescents were
the concept of “the burden of acting White.” They less likely than other students to be in school at
asserted that Black students who do well in school the time of the interviews and less likely to be
must do so at the cost of maintaining their own enrolled in college 2 years after their expected on-
racial identity. In their study, Black adolescents time high school graduation (i.e., 6 years after
said that Black students who were actively they entered ninth grade). In contrast, compared
engaged in school (e.g., studying, working hard to other groups, students with a strong, positive
on school work) were in effect trying to act White. racial identity were more likely to be in school at
High-achieving Black students in their study the time of the interview and more likely to be
employed strategies to conceal their engagement enrolled in college 2 years after their expected on-
in school work. Although the notion of “acting time high school graduation. Thus, in this study,
White” has become a popular explanation for the having a weak racial identity was associated with
underachievement of minority students (Starkey poor student engagement; a strong racial identity
& Eaton, 2008), subsequent empirical support for was linked to stronger student engagement.
70 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

In a study of 390 African American middle minority groups was positively associated with
and high school students, Smalls and colleagues academic persistence and negatively associated
(Smalls, White, Chavous, & Sellers, 2007) with efforts to conceal engagement in school
assessed racial centrality (i.e., the degree to which from peers. (Findings pertaining to discrimina-
being Black is important to how the student views tion are discussed below).
himself or herself), perception of discrimination, In general, the basic thesis that ethnic minority
academic persistence (i.e., the degree to which students must give up their ethnic identity to be
the student reports persevering when faced with engaged in school is not supported by current
an academic challenge), academic curiosity (i.e., research. In addition to the work by Chavous and
interest in new academic topics), negative school colleagues (2003; Smalls et al., 2007), other
behavior (e.g., cutting classes, cheating on tests), researchers have also found positive relations
and efforts to conceal engagement in school from between ethnic identity and student engagement
their peers. In addition, they measured the degree with school and achievement for African
to which students agreed with four types of racial American youth (e.g., Bennett, 2006; Dotterer,
ideology: assimilation (i.e., the belief that African McHale, & Crouter, 2009; Spencer et al., 2001;
Americans should be more like European Taylor et al., 1994). In her work with Navajo ado-
Americans to be successful), humanist (i.e., the lescents, Deyhle (1995) observed that the Navajo
belief that people should emphasize how groups students who were well grounded in their Navajo
are similar rather than different), minority (i.e., culture and identity were better able to be engaged
the view that African Americans share common in school than were those who did not have a
experiences with other oppressed minority strong ethnic identity.
groups), and nationalist (i.e., the belief that Similarly, the belief that minority students
African Americans should embrace their unique who do well in school must hide their efforts to
African American culture and identity). In a be engaged in school from their peers is not well
series of hierarchical regressions controlling for supported (Smalls et al., 2007). Researchers
gender, grade level, prior school performance, have found that publicly distancing oneself from
parent education, and racial centrality, they exam- the appearance of working hard in school is a
ined whether assimilation, humanism, minority, strategy employed by both Black and White
nationalism, and racial discrimination predicted adolescents and that positive attitudes toward
measures of student engagement in school and doing school work and the importance of doing
efforts to conceal engagement from peers. well in school for one’s future were associated
Assimilation was negatively correlated with aca- with feelings of alienation from peers for both
demic persistence and academic curiosity (mar- groups (Arroyo & Zigler, 1995). However, there
ginally significant), and positively correlated is some evidence that racial and ethnic minor-
with negative school behaviors and efforts to ity youth from traditionally underachieving
conceal engagement in school from peers. These groups may not consider academic engagement
findings do not support the notion that African and achievement to be part of their identity
American students who are engaged in school (Oyserman, Kemmelmeier, Fryberg, Brosh, &
must give up their racial identity. Instead, these Hart-Johnson, 2003).
results suggest that African American students Do students of color identify characteristics of
who are less engaged in school are more likely to academic engagement with members of their eth-
believe that they need to be more like their White nic group? In a study on stereotypes (Hudley &
counterparts in order to succeed in school. Graham, 2001), African American, Latino, and
However, they also report a negative association Anglo middle school students were shown pic-
between nationalism and academic persistence, tures of African American, Latino, and Anglo
but nationalism was not predictive of the other boys and girls and were given short descriptions
measures. The belief that African Americans of academically engaged students (e.g., someone
share a common experience with other oppressed who works hard on school work, studies, pays
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 71

attention, and participates in class discussions) these results by finding that the degree to which
and unengaged students (e.g., someone who fools African American and Latino elementary school
around in class, does not do homework, cuts students identified with nonacademic domains
classes). Based on their “first impressions,” (i.e., being cool, tough, popular, oppositional,
students were asked to pick the picture of the liked by girls, liked by boys, or fashionable) was
student who best fits the description. Across negatively associated with teachers’ ratings of
racial and ethnic groups, girls and boys were students’ behavioral engagement (e.g., follows
more likely to identify girls as being academi- directions, completes homework). Taken together,
cally engaged and boys as being unengaged in these studies suggest that Black and Hispanic
school. African American girls were more likely boys may not view engaging in school as an
to identify African American and Anglo girls as appropriate component of their identity.
being academically engaged. African American Behavioral scientists have posited that the
boys were more likely to select pictures of African degree to which students of color perceive or
American girls as being the best fit for an engaged experience discrimination may affect their
student. Latino and Anglo girls and boys were engagement and achievement in school. On the
more likely to identify pictures of Anglo girls as one hand, if students perceive discrimination
being academically engaged. All students were against themselves or members of their ethnic
more likely to select an African American or group, they may not believe that teachers treat
Latino boy as fitting the descriptor for a student them fairly and may not see any reason to try
who is uninterested in school. For our purposes, hard in school or even be in school (e.g., Huffman,
these data suggest that although African American 1991; Ogbu, 1992; Wong, Eccles, & Sameroff,
boys and girls hold an image of African American 2003). Alternatively, if students perceive dis-
girls that encompasses being engaged in school, crimination against their ethnic group, they may
African American boys and Latino boys and girls believe that a good education is the means for
did not identify someone like themselves as being overcoming discriminatory barriers that they may
highly engaged in school. Moreover, all boys and encounter in the future (e.g., Sue & Okazaki,
girls were more likely select pictures of African 1990). Current research indicates that the relation
American or Latino boys as fitting the descrip- between discrimination and students’ engage-
tion of someone who is unengaged in school. ment in school is not consistent across or within
Graham (1994; Graham, Taylor, & Hudley, racial and ethnic groups. Some studies have
1998) proposed that underachieving ethnic found a negative correlation between students’
minority students are not engaged in school perception of discrimination and measures of
because they have aspirations other than to do engagement, particularly students’ belief in the
well in school—that lack of motivation to achieve importance or value of education (e.g., Dotterer
in school stems from having different goals and et al., 2009; Okagaki, Frensch, & Dodson, 1996;
values. To examine students’ values, Graham Smalls et al., 2007; Wong et al., 2003). Others
and colleagues (1998) asked Black, Hispanic, and have obtained mixed results (e.g., Taylor et al.,
White middle school students to identify class- 1994) or positive relations between discrimina-
mates whom they admired, respected, and wanted tion and aspects of engagement (e.g., Okagaki,
to be like. In general, girls were more likely to Helling, & Bingham, 2009; Sanders, 1997).
identify girls of the same race and girls who were Consistent with the hypothesis that racism
high or average achieving over those who were leads to disengagement from school by ethnic
low achieving. The pattern of nominations for minority students, data from a study of fourth-
White boys paralleled those of the girls—a pref- and fifth-grade Mexican American students indi-
erence for average- and high-achieving boys of cated that students’ perception of general
the same race. In contrast, Black and Hispanic discrimination against members of their ethnic
boys admired low-achieving boys of their own group was negatively correlated with their
race. Strambler and Weinstein (2010) extended engagement in school (items included aspects of
72 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

behavioral and emotional engagement), nega- Although not directly assessing engagement,
tively correlated with intrinsic motivation for Oyserman, Harrison, and Bybee (2001) exam-
learning, and negatively correlated with their ined the relation between academic efficacy (e.g.,
belief that school is an appropriate domain in belief that one can do well on school tasks) mea-
which Mexican American children can and sured at the end of the school year and three
should excel (Okagaki et al., 1996). Among these aspects of racial identity measured in the fall
elementary school students, however, perception (start) of that school year: (a) awareness of rac-
of discrimination was not correlated with stu- ism, (b) feelings of connectedness with one’s
dents’ academic performance. Similarly, in their racial group, and (c) the degree to which students
study of African American middle and high believed that academic achievement was congru-
school students, Smalls and colleagues (2007) ent with their racial identity. Among African
found that students’ perceptions of general dis- American middle school boys, perceptions of
crimination within the last year were negatively racism in the fall did not predict school efficacy.
associated with academic persistence and aca- However, awareness of racism then was nega-
demic curiosity and positively correlated with tively associated with academic efficacy for
negative school behaviors. African American girls. In particular, academic
In a study of 164 public school and 180 efficacy was negatively associated with aware-
Catholic school African American adolescents ness of racism among girls who felt more closely
from working class families, Taylor and col- connected to the African American community
leagues (1994) assessed students’ perceptions of and whose racial identity did not have a strong
discrimination in employment practices, belief in academic achievement component. Believing
the importance of education, behavioral engage- that academic achievement is a characteristic of
ment in school (e.g., cutting class, spending time one’s racial group may help protect the academic
on homework, paying attention in class), ethnic efficacy of African American girls in the context
identity, self-concept of academic ability, and of discrimination.
school performance. Path analyses with the pub- There is considerable variability across indi-
lic school data revealed that perception of dis- viduals in response to discrimination. For exam-
crimination was negatively associated with belief ple, Deyhle (1995) conducted an extensive
in the importance of education, which in turn was ethnographic study of Navajo youth in a border
positively related to behavioral engagement in reservation community, including adolescents
school. For Catholic school African American who were in high school and those who did not
students, path analyses indicated that perception complete high school. Among the 168 Navajo
of discrimination was also negatively related to students who did not complete high school, more
importance of education, but the relation bet- than half of these youth cited problems of dis-
ween importance of education and behavioral crimination as contributing to their decision to
engagement was not significant. The difference leave school. However, she asserted that among
in findings between the public school and those students who were most engaged in learn-
Catholic school students highlights within- ing, were persisting in school, and were academi-
group differences in the relation between dis- cally successful were ones who were realistic in
crimination and importance of education in a recognizing and acknowledging discrimination
study in which the measures and procedures in their school and community. In a study of 67
were the same for both groups. The authors American Indian college students, Okagaki et al.
speculated that local contextual factors (e.g., dif- (2009) examined the relation between perception
ferences between Catholic and public schools, of discrimination and students’ belief in the prag-
parental involvement in education) may moder- matic benefits of education (e.g., a college
ate the links between perception of discrimina- education is necessary for obtaining a good job).
tion, importance of education, and behavioral A positive relation between perception of dis-
engagement for African American students. crimination and students’ belief in the value of
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 73

education was obtained. Scores on the perception would not be able to get a good job if they did not
of discrimination scale ranged from 1.3 to 5.5 on do well in school. In contrast, Hispanic and
a 6-point scale, indicating substantial variability African American youth were more likely to
among students in their perception of discrimina- maintain that they would be able to obtain a good
tion against American Indians. To explore the job even if they did not get a good education in
finding, the authors split the sample at the median high school. Steinberg and colleagues described
on the discrimination scale. Among those who the motivation of the Asian American students
perceived less discrimination, there was essen- as fear of the negative consequences of a poor
tially no correlation between discrimination and education.
students’ belief in the instrumental importance of In addition to belief in the instrumental impor-
education. For students who perceived greater tance of school, some researchers have posited
discrimination, the correlation was positive. that minority students must believe that they can
However, in a similar study with American Indian be true to their ethnic identity and still participate
adolescents, Bingham, Helling, and Okagaki effectively in the mainstream culture if they are
(2001) found no relation between students’ per- to function well in the mainstream culture and
ception of discrimination and their motivation in not be alienated from their ethnic community
school, expectations for achievement in school, (e.g., Buriel & DeMent, 1997; LaFromboise,
or time spent on homework. Coleman & Gerton, 1993; Miller, 1999; Ogbu,
In general, the research to date suggests that 1992; Okagaki, 2001). For student engagement
there is not a simple and consistent relation and achievement, this means that ethnic minority
between perception of discrimination and student students need to develop a positive academic
engagement. To explain Asian American stu- identity while holding onto positive ethnic iden-
dents’ achievement, Sue and Okazaki (1990) tity. In a study of American Indian college stu-
posited that when other avenues to upward dents, a measure of academic identity (e.g.,
mobility are blocked (e.g., business, politics, “Doing well in school and graduating from col-
sports) because of discrimination, then education lege are important to my view of myself ”) was
becomes more important as the means to upward positively correlated with a measure of bicultural
mobility. Several behavioral scientists have sug- efficacy (e.g., “I believe I can maintain my tribal
gested that the degree to which racial and ethnic identity and still participate in activities that are
minority students believe that doing well in traditionally part of the White culture”; Okagaki
school will have pragmatic benefits for their lives et al., 2009). That is, the more strongly students
contributes to students’ engagement and achieve- believed that they could be a good member of
ment in school (e.g., Matute-Bianchi, 1986; their tribal community and at the same time, do
Mickelson, 1990; Suarez-Orozco, 1993; Sue & well in school, the more positive their academic
Okazaki, 1990). For example, in a study of Black identity was. Among Hispanic high school stu-
and White high school seniors, Mickelson found dents, bicultural efficacy has been positively cor-
that the belief in the instrumental importance of related with students’ self-reported grades
education, rather than a general belief in the value (Okagaki, Izarraraz, & Bojczyk, 2003).
of education, was related to school achievement. The idea of bicultural efficacy or a bicultural
In a study of about 15,000 adolescents, Steinberg, identity is related to research on racial-ethnic
Dornbusch, and Brown (1992) reported that ado- self-schemas (Oyserman et al., 2003). Racial-
lescents across ethnic groups believed that get- ethnic self-schemas refer to an individual’s self-
ting a good education would be beneficial to them concept that forms through the integration of
(e.g., help them get a good job). Where the ethnic emotions, attitudes, and beliefs into a coherent
groups differed is in their response to the conse- cognitive structure about one’s membership of an
quences of not getting a good education. ethnic group (Oyserman et al.). It is hypothesized
Compared to other students, Asian American that individuals who develop racial-ethnic schemas
adolescents were more likely to believe that they that incorporate both in-group (i.e., identification
74 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

with one’s racial group) and larger society studying. Although bicultural efficacy did not
racial-ethnic schemas (i.e., positive identification predict student engagement, bicultural identity,
with the majority culture) will do better in school which was measured in the fall of the school year,
than individuals who do not. Although not directly was positively related to students’ grades at the
measuring student engagement with school, in a end of the academic year, even after controlling
series of studies, Oyserman and colleagues for prior grades. Perception of discrimination
obtained support for this hypothesis with respect was not related to engagement or achievement. In
to school achievement. For example, they found this study, the students had spent most of their
that, after controlling for fall grades, ethnically lives on a reservation, and most were attending
diverse middle school students with both in-group tribal schools or public schools near the reserva-
and larger society racial-ethnic schemas received tion at the time of the study. It is possible that
significantly higher last-quarter grades than students discrimination did not play a strong role in these
without racial-ethnic schemas or in-group-only adolescents’ daily lives.
racial-ethnic schemas students (Oyserman et al.).
In a separate study, students with both in-group Summary According to social identity theorists
and larger society racial-ethnic schemas evidenced and researchers, individuals have multiple ways
more persistence on a math task compared to of defining themselves that depend on the social
youth without racial-ethnic schemas (Oyserman, groups to which they belong by birth (e.g., eth-
Bybee, Terry, & Hart-Johnson, 2004). These data nicity, gender), achievement (e.g., prize-winning
suggest that students’ beliefs about themselves author or dancer), or choice (e.g., church mem-
and their ability to function within their own eth- ber, community activist) (e.g., Bernal, Saenz, &
nic culture and within the larger society may play Knight, 1991; Harwood et al., 2001; Tajfel,
a role in their engagement in school. 1981). To the extent that individuals have multi-
In a study of 98 American Indian adolescents, ple social identities, having a strong ethnic iden-
the degrees to which students’ ethnic identity, tity need not be incompatible with a strong
bicultural efficacy, belief in the instrumental academic identity. In general, the research seems
importance of school, and students’ general per- to indicate that racial and ethnic minority stu-
ceptions of discrimination predicted emotional dents can maintain a strong racial and ethnic
engagement (as measured by interest in school identity and be engaged in school. Generally, per-
and educational expectations), behavioral engage- ception of discrimination is negatively associated
ment (as measured by time spent studying), and with engagement. However, when students
school achievement were examined (Bingham believe that achieving in school is appropriate for
et al., 2001). Ethnic identity was measured by the members of their racial or ethnic group or they
Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (Phinney, develop a bicultural identity, it appears that this
1992; Roberts et al., 1999) and captured the indi- belief may act as a buffer to the negative effects
vidual’s sense of belonging to their ethnic group of discrimination on student engagement.
and their active engagement in learning about
their ethnic group. Multivariate analyses of cova-
riance indicated that ethnic identity and belief in The Role of Parents: Expectations,
the instrumental importance of school predicted Support, and Cultural Socialization
students’ orientation to school. In particular,
stronger identification with and participation in Much of the research on parenting and student
their American Indian community was associated engagement and achievement in racial and ethnic
with greater interest in school for American minority families has mirrored research among
Indian adolescents. Instrumental importance of mainstream families. Researchers have found,
school was consistently and positively correlated for instance, that parental involvement is posi-
with all aspects of engagement—interest in tively related to student engagement among
school, educational expectations, and time spent African American youth (e.g., Connell et al., 1994).
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 75

In this section, we discuss parental expectations, do well in school”; “I can work really hard in
support, and cultural socialization as they pertain school”) and their competence in school. Utilizing
to racial and ethnic minority students’ engage- hierarchical regression analyses, Murray exam-
ment in school. ined parent and teacher contributions to explain-
A meta-analysis of parenting and school ing the variance in students’ engagement after
achievement indicate that among the various controlling for students’ reading and math
components of parenting (e.g., communication, achievement. Parental variables accounted for
supervision, school contact and participation, about 24% of the variance in engagement; teacher
general involvement with child) parents’ aspira- variables explained an additional 25% of the
tions and expectations for their children have the variance. Of the parental variables, unclear paren-
strongest relation to children’s achievement (Fan tal expectations predicted student engagement
& Chen, 2001). In general, parents’ expectations such that higher scores on unclear parental expec-
for academic achievement differ across racial and tations were associated with lower student
ethnic groups (e.g., Alexander, Entwisle, & engagement. Parenting also accounted for about
Bedinger, 1994; Hao & Bonstead-Bruns, 1998; 18% of the variance in self-reported school com-
Okagaki & Frensch, 1998), and their expecta- petence. Higher scores on positive involvement
tions are correlated with children’s school perfor- with the child were associated with higher school
mance among economically disadvantaged Black competence scores; higher scores on unclear
families (e.g., Gill & Reynolds, 1999; Halle, parental expectations were associated with lower
Kurtz-Costes, & Mahoney, 1997; Luster & school competence scores.
McAdoo, 1996), Asian American families (Hao In a study of 95 low-income African American
& Bonstead-Bruns, 1998; Okagaki & Frensch, adolescents and their mothers, Taylor and Lopez
1998), and Hispanic families (e.g., Goldenberg, (2005) examined whether the degree to which
Gallimore, Reese, & Garnier, 2001; Hao & African American families maintained predict-
Bonstead-Bruns, 1998). Although most of this able routines and mother’s expectations for their
literature has focused on the relation between child’s achievement was related to student
expectations and achievement, rather than on engagement with school as measured by the
student engagement, researchers have found degree to which adolescents reported paying
that parental expectations are associated with attention in classes, attending classes, experienc-
student engagement among racial and ethnic ing a sense of challenge in their classes, and
minority students (e.g., Murray, 2009; Taylor & exhibiting negative behaviors in school (e.g., get-
Lopez, 2005). ting into a fight, damaging school property).
In a study of 104 low-income students in Family routine was positively correlated with
which 91% of the students were Latino, 4% paying attention, attending classes, and experi-
African American, and 5% White, Murray (2009) encing a sense of challenge in classes. Further
examined the relations between students’ engage- analyses indicated that student engagement in the
ment and support from parents and teachers. form of paying attention in class and attending
Because the analyses are not broken apart by eth- class mediated the relation between family rou-
nic and racial group, we discuss the results as tine and students’ achievement. Parental expecta-
reflecting generally Latino and African American tions were positively related to attendance, and
families. Along with aspects of the teacher- student attendance mediated the relation between
student relationship, four aspects of parenting were parental expectations and student achievement.
identified: (a) closeness of the parent–child rela- Finally, family routine was negatively associated
tionship, (b) parental involvement in the child’s with problem behavior in school, and this rela-
life, (c) unclear parental academic expectations, tion was mediated by student engagement in the
and (d) poor accessibility of parents. Students form of paying attention and attending class.
reported on their behavioral engagement in school In addition to parental expectations and family
(e.g., “Trying hard is the best way for me to routine, parental support and the quality of the
76 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

parent–child relationship have been associated drinking before or during school) were examined.
with student engagement in middle-class African Across groups, adolescents who perceived them-
American adolescents (Sirin & Rogers-Sirin, selves to be poorly connected to their families
2005) and at-risk, inner-city African American were less emotionally engaged in school and
adolescents (Annunziata, Hogue, Faw, & Liddle, more likely to be involved in problem behaviors
2006). For example, in a study of about 200 at school. Compared to their Black and White
inner-city, African American families, Annunziata counterparts, American Indian adolescents had
and colleagues (2006) examined whether family lower scores on family connectedness and stu-
cohesion and parental support were associated dent engagement and higher scores on problem
with middle school students’ emotional engage- behaviors. Machamer and Gruber argued that the
ment in school. The measure of family cohesion discontinuity between the cultural norms of the
assessed the degree to which the members of American Indian people and the school culture
their family were close to one another, communi- (e.g., prohibition against competition among
cated with and supported one another, and per- many American Indian communities, avoiding
ceived their family to be organized. Both parents direct eye contact with authority figures) as well
and students responded to the items, and the aver- as societal changes (e.g., American Indian fami-
age of their scores represented the family’s cohe- lies moving away from tribal communities) make
sion score. Similarly, both parents’ and students’ American Indian adolescents particularly vulner-
responses to items regarding parental support for able to disengagement in school.
the child (e.g., praising, doing special activities, Research on racial and ethnic minority parent-
talking with child) combined to form the parental ing brings to light the importance of examining
support score. Hierarchical regression analyses cultural models of education (Gallimore &
revealed that both family cohesion and parental Goldenberg, 2001; Lareau, 1996). Cultural models
support were positively related to emotional are widely shared “understandings of how the
engagement in school. However, there was a world works, or ought to work” (Gallimore &
significant interaction between family cohesion Goldenberg, 2001, p. 47), and there are differences
and parental support such that when parental and commonalities across ethnic groups in their
support was high, family cohesion was posi- conceptions of education, school, and learning.
tively correlated with student engagement. Cultural models of education may include the
Under conditions of low parental support, fam- ideas that parents have about the roles of par-
ily cohesion was not associated with student ents, teachers, and children in education (Lareau).
engagement. Subsequent analyses determined In many Asian cultures, the importance of educa-
that student gender moderated the relations tion is linked to a strong belief in human mallea-
among family cohesion, parental support, and bility and the efficacy of working hard and in the
student engagement. For boys, high parental importance of bringing honor to one’s family
support increased the relation between family (e.g., Ho, 1994). In a study of immigrant parents
cohesion and student engagement; for girls, the from Cambodia, Mexico, the Philippines, and
interaction was not significant. Vietnam and US-born parents of Mexican descent
Family support and cohesion has also been and European Americans, Okagaki and Sternberg
found to be important for American Indian ado- (1993) found that motivation was an important
lescents (Machamer & Gruber, 1998). In a large component of the Asian parents’ understanding
study of over 6,000 high school students, includ- of intelligence. A child who is intelligent is one
ing 8% American Indian, 7% Black, and 85% who tries hard to get good grades. Belief in the
non-Hispanic White adolescents, family connect- efficacy of hard work and effort is a consistent
edness (e.g., family cares about you, parents theme in studies of Asian American students.
understand you), student engagement (e.g., In an ethnographic study of immigrant Punjabi
school people care about you, like school), and Indian and non-Punjabi adolescents in California,
negative school behaviors (e.g., skipping classes, Gibson (Gibson, 1987) noted that both the Punjabi
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 77

students and their parents believed that effort was helping their children with homework or explicitly
the determining factor in educational achieve- talking about school with their children relative
ment. Based on interviews with Japanese to other parents (e.g., Wong, 1990). Several
American high school students who were primar- researchers have observed that Asian American
ily third-generation Japanese Americans, Matute- parents employ indirect strategies to encourage
Bianchi (1986) reported that “[b]elief in diligence, their children’s school achievement, such as
persistence, and hard work—as opposed to inher- structuring the home environment to facilitate the
ent ability—as the keys to academic success is child’s learning, rather than directly helping the
the single most commonly shared perception child with school work (e.g., Caplan et al., 1989;
among the Japanese-descent students” (p. 247). Chao, 2000; Schneider & Lee, 1990). For exam-
In their study of Southeast Asian immigrants, ple, parents may set aside a specific time for the
Caplan and his colleagues suggested that three child to do homework and restrict the amount of
common values—an emphasis on education and time the child spends watching television (e.g.,
achievement, belief in the importance of main- Schneider & Lee, 1990). Parents may not want or
taining a cohesive family, and belief in the effi- allow their adolescent to work after school
cacy and importance of hard work—served as the because they believe that the adolescent’s “job”
foundation for Southeast Asian children’s aca- is to study for school (e.g., Gibson, 1987). Based
demic achievement (Caplan, Whitmore, & Choy, on an ethnographic study of third-generation and
1989). Almost all of the Southeast Asian parents fourth-generation Japanese Americans, Hieshima
(97%) and children (93%) attributed academic and Schneider (1994) noted that even native-born
success to hard work; in contrast 86% of the par- parents’ encouragement of schoolwork was indi-
ents and 67% of the children identified intellec- rect. The third-generation Japanese American
tual ability as causally related to academic parents did not directly tell their child what to do;
achievement. In a comparison of several Asian instead, they made indirect comments such as
American subgroups (Chinese, Filipino, Japanese, “You sure finished with your homework fast” or
Korean, Vietnamese, and other Southeast Asian), “Not much homework tonight?” (p. 322).
Mizokawa and Ryckman (1990) found that over- Schneider and Lee (1990) found that Asian
all, Asian American students (including elemen- American parents were more likely than European
tary, junior high, and high school students) were American parents to encourage their child to take
more likely to attribute academic success and fail- private classes in music, language, and computer
ure to effort than to ability. Chen and Stevenson science and that their children spent more time
(1995) observed that relative to Caucasian practicing for their lessons than did European
American students, Asian American high school American children. Compared to their European
students were more likely to indicate that doing American counterparts, Chinese immigrant par-
well in math was the result of studying hard. In ents of primary grade students reported engaging
addition, Asian American students reported spend- in more “structural parental involvement in
ing more hours studying mathematics outside of school” (Chao, 2000, p. 240; e.g., setting rules
class (5.3 h per week) compared to their Caucasian for how the child spent time after school and buy-
American counterparts (3.2 h per week). Analyses ing extra workbooks or materials to give the child
of national data sets have revealed that Asian more practice on school tasks) than European
American students report spending more time American parents. Conversely, European American
working on school work and are more likely to parents had higher scores on what was called
engage in extracurricular classes than other stu- “managerial parental involvement in school”
dents (Peng & Wright, 1994; Tsang, 1988; (Chao, 2000, p. 240)—activities such as check-
Wong, 1990). What role do Asian American par- ing homework and attending school functions. It
ents play in their child’s approach to education? appears that in Asian American homes, an impor-
For the most part, it does not appear that Asian tant aspect of socialization for academic achieve-
American parents spend more time directly ment is the creation of an overall environment in
78 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

which discipline, studying, and practice are school achievement (e.g., being on the honors
integral elements of the child’s role in multiple roll, grade-point average). The measure of racial
contexts. Although for the most part, these stud- socialization included items about Black parents
ies did not directly assess students’ engagement teaching their children about racism, families
in school, these studies describe a cultural model teaching children to be proud to be Black, and
of education—one that emphasizes (a) that doing schools teaching children about Black history
well in school is the child’s job, (b) that practice and including signs of Black culture in the class-
and effort are the key to doing well in school, and room. Among other results, Bennett found that
(c) that the parent’s job is to create an environ- racial socialization was positively correlated with
ment at home which allows students to spend ethnic identity which in turn was positively cor-
time studying and to provide students with activi- related with student engagement with school.
ties that support education (e.g., giving students In an elegant study examining the relations
books to read or extra math assignments, encour- among parenting, discrimination, ethnic identity,
aging music and dance activities that create an and engagement, Dotterer et al. (2009) inter-
expectation for practice and hard work)—that viewed 148 working and middle-class African
lends itself to supporting engagement in school. American adolescents from two-parent families,
Finally, although much of the research on along with their mothers and fathers, to explore
racial and ethnic minority parenting and student the relations among experience with discrimina-
engagement is similar to the broader literature on tion, cultural socialization, preparation for dis-
parenting, researchers have also explored aspects crimination, ethnic identity, and emotional
that may be somewhat idiosyncratic to racial and engagement with school, and school self-esteem.
minority parents—cultural socialization and Perception of discrimination was negatively
preparation for discrimination. Although this is a related to emotional engagement and school self-
relatively new area of research, behavioral scien- esteem. A series of hierarchical regression analy-
tists are interested in learning if and how cultural ses were conducted to determine whether cultural
socialization may foster children’s development socialization, preparation for discrimination, or
(Hughes et al., 2006). As noted earlier, percep- ethnic identity moderated the relations between
tion of discrimination has generally been found discrimination and student engagement and
to be negatively associated with student engage- between discrimination and self-esteem. With
ment with school, and it appears that for the most respect to self-esteem, neither cultural socializa-
part, racial and ethnic identity is positively asso- tion, nor preparation for discrimination, nor eth-
ciated with student engagement. Is it possible nic identity moderated the negative relation
that through cultural socialization, parents may between discrimination and self-esteem. For stu-
protect their children from the negative conse- dent engagement with school, the results were
quences of discrimination either directly or by more complicated. Cultural socialization was
fostering children’s racial and ethnic identity? positively correlated with emotional engagement
Bennett (2006) surveyed African American for boys, but not girls. However, cultural social-
adolescents from grades 8 through 12. Of 131 ization did not moderate the negative relation
participants, about 40% lived with both parents, between discrimination and emotional engage-
48% lived with one parent, and the others reported ment. Preparation for discrimination was posi-
living with grandparents or in some other living tively associated with emotional engagement in
arrangement. Among the many measures that school, but it did not moderate the negative rela-
were collected, students reported on their ethnic tion between discrimination and emotional
identity, racial socialization, and engagement engagement. Finally, the analyses with ethnic
with school. For this study, the student engage- identity revealed a three-way interaction between
ment with school measure was a mix of behav- ethnic identity, discrimination, and gender,
ioral engagement items (e.g., participating in such that ethnic identity moderated the negative
student government, in an honors club) and relation between discrimination and emotional
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 79

engagement for girls, but not for boys. Girls who role to their experiences in school (see Chap. 18,
have strong ethnic identities maintain their emo- this volume), particularly in the areas of school
tional engagement with school in the context of adjustment (Berndt, 1999; Perdue, Manzeske, &
greater perceived discrimination. However, girls Estell, 2009; Ryan, 2000), ethnic identification
with low ethnic identities have lower emotional (Oyserman, Brickman, Bybee, & Celious, 2006;
engagement in school when discrimination is Shin, Daly, & Vera, 2007), and educational aspi-
higher. rations (Antonio, 2004). Because peers and
friends encourage some beliefs, values, behav-
Summary There is great diversity in parents’ iors, and activities, while rejecting others, they
ideas about education, how children learn, what can either play a positive role in an individual’s
children need to do to succeed in school, and engagement in school by helping a child develop
what parents should do to support their child’s a sense of belonging and affiliation with school
education (for reviews, see Bornstein, 2002; (Faircloth & Hamm, 2005; Finn & Voelkl, 1993)
Okagaki & Bingham, 2010). Many of these or a negative role by contributing to feelings of
beliefs and values ideas appear to be a function of isolation and disengagement (Oyserman et al.,
parents’ cultural models of education (Gallimore 2003; Rivas-Drake, Hughes, & Way, 2008; Way,
& Goderberg, 2001; Lareau, 1996). Although Santos, Niwa, & Kim-Gervey, 2008). For racial
more research is needed to connect parents’ cul- and ethnic minority youth, friends may serve as a
tural models of education to how they support resource for navigating differences between home
their child’s learning at home, research suggests and school cultures as well as a source of social
that ethnic minority parents who are emotionally support that reinforce one’s ethnic identification
supportive of their children, have strong and clear and connection with one’s racial or ethnic com-
academic expectations for their child and estab- munity (see Way, Becker, & Greene, 2006). In
lish regular routines, particularly learning or this section, we consider student engagement with
school-related routines in their homes, have chil- school from the perspective of minority students’
dren who are more engaged in school. (Annunziata access to peers of the same race or ethnic groups.
et al., 2006; Murray, 2009; Sirin & Rogers-Sirin, Racial and ethnic group membership is a
2005; Taylor & Lopez, 2005). salient feature in minority children’s lives and
has been shown to be one characteristic by which
children choose and define who their friends are
The Role of Schools: Friends,
and how they feel about themselves (Bellmore,
Teachers, and Instruction
Nishina, Witkow, Graham, & Juvonen, 2007;
Oyserman et al., 2003). For example, in a study
Schools play a major role in children’s lives and
of low-income African American, Latino, and
serve as an important context for their social,
Asian American adolescents (Way & Chen,
emotional, and cognitive development. In this
2000), 73% of ethnic minority students reported
section, we have focused on key aspects of the
having predominately same-race/ethnic group
school context that may be particularly important
friendship networks, with Asian American stu-
to the engagement of racial and ethnic minority
dents reporting the highest percentage (85%).
students. We begin by discussing the research on
Similarly, in a study of sixth-grade students
friendship and peer relationships.
(Bellmore et al., 2007), African American, Asian
Racial and Ethnic Minority Students’ American, Latino, and White middle school stu-
Peer Relationships and Their Engagement dents all showed race/ethnic group selection bias
in School in choosing their friends (i.e., they attributed
As children get older, they spend increasing more favorable ratings of acceptance to same-
amounts of time with their friends both in and out race/ethnic peers). It appeared that the availability
of school. As a result, children’s friendships and of same-race/ethnic group peers influenced stu-
peer relationships begin to play a more central dents’ ratings; students having access to a larger
80 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

numbers of same-race/ethnic group peers showed to racial discrimination (Gonzalez, 2009;


a greater selection bias compared to those with Johnson, Crosnoe, & Elder, 2001; Moody, 2001).
less access. Unlike Asian American, Latino, and The relations among school composition (i.e.,
White students, however, African American stu- presence of racial and ethnic minority students),
dents were significantly more likely to nominate same-race/ethnic group friendships, and emo-
a same-race peer as someone who they both did tional engagement, however, is not the same
and did not like to spend time with. Although across racial and ethnic groups. For example, in a
these studies suggest that students generally tend study using the National Longitudinal Study of
to prefer friends who look like them and come Adolescent Health, Ueno (2009) found positive
from similar racial-ethnic backgrounds, other associations between access to same-race/ethnic
research demonstrates that adolescents consider group peers in school and emotional engagement
other factors, such as a peer’s attitude toward for Asian and African American students, but not
school and participation in extracurricular activi- White and Latino students. Positive associations
ties, to be important (Epstein & Karweit, 1983; between the presence of same-race/ethnic group
Ryan, 2000; Shrum, Cheek & Hunter, 1988). For friendships and emotional engagement were
example, Hamm (2000) found that African found for Latino and White students, suggesting
Americans who attended multiethnic high schools for Latino and White students, it is the impor-
were more likely to have friendships with others tance of having friendships that contributes to
who shared their same ethnic group membership emotional engagement in school and not simply
as well as other characteristics, such as academic access to similar peers.
orientation or substance use. In contrast, Asian Although minority students’ access to same-
American and European American students were race/ethnic group peers and friends at school
more likely to have friendships based upon simi- appears to be related to student engagement, the
lar academic orientations (measured through a quality of relationship may determine the relative
composite variable containing self-reports of strength of this association. Vaquera (2009) found
effort in completing school work and educational that compared to students who did not report hav-
aspirations for future education past high school). ing a best friend at school, White and Latino high
The findings from this study are somewhat simi- school students who reported having a best friend
lar to findings obtained in a study of commonali- were more likely to be emotionally engaged and
ties between high school best friends. In their less likely to have behavioral engagement prob-
study, Tolson and Urberg (1993) found that lems (e.g., not being on task in school, not hand-
African American best friendships appear to be ing in homework). Although few differences in
less similar on many behaviors and attitudes (e.g., emotional and behavioral engagement persisted
smoking attitudes, in-school activities) than those across ethnic groups once individual (age, gen-
of European American and Asian American der, generational status) and school (public, low-
youth. Differences between these studies may be achieving school, and proportion of same race)
a result of the way that researchers assessed factors were added to the models, students from
friendships. Mexican and Central/South America reported
For this chapter, the more important question higher levels of emotional engagement than did
is whether racial and ethnic minority students’ White students.
friendships are associated with their engagement Not surprisingly, racial and ethnic minority
with school. The limited research that exists on students who have access to and relationships
the relation between minority students’ access to with peers and friends who value education and
same-race/ethnic group peers suggests a positive who participate in school activities are more
association between access to same-race/ethnic likely to be engaged in school than students with-
group peers and emotional engagement in school, out these relationships (Ryan, 2000; Shin et al.,
with many hypothesizing that having access to 2007). However, they may be less likely to have
similar peers may reduce students’ overall exposure access to such relationships. Using data from the
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 81

National Education Longitudinal Study of 1988 difficult for these adolescents to complete
(NELS:88), Ream and Rumberger (2008) found homework or other school preparation activities.
that in general, Mexican American youth were Three caveats should be considered relative to
less likely to participate in (a) unorganized aca- research on racial and ethnic minority students’
demic activities, such as homework and school friendships and their engagement with school.
preparation activities, and (b) organized extracur- First, the nature of the schools that students attend
ricular activities, such as athletics or the arts. In constrains or enables access to racial and ethnic
addition, Mexican American youth were more friendship experiences. In multiethnic schools in
likely than their non-Latino White peers to have which a large proportion of students are available
friends who dropped out of high school and were to form friendships, it may be that individuals are
less likely to have access to friends who valued more likely to choose friends based upon similar
education (measured by student report of friends values, interests, and abilities, rather than simply
attending class regularly, getting good grades, on the basis of the same ethnic characteristics.
studying, finishing high school, and continuing However, given the limited research on this sub-
education past high school). Utilizing a series of ject, it is unclear if this threshold hypothesis is
structural equation models, Ream and Rumberger true. Second, multiple types of peer social net-
found that Mexican American students’ home- works and possible friendship systems exist in
work and school preparation activities were posi- and out of schools that may exert an influence on
tively associated with school-oriented friendship student engagement. Best friends, regular friends,
networks, while these types of behavioral engage- and peer networks exert social pressure on the
ment were negatively related to identifying with student (see Brown, Hamm, Herman, & Heck,
students who dropped out of school. For both 2008), and the ways in which these relationships
Mexican American and non-Latino White stu- may be associated with student engagement with
dents, participation in athletic activities was posi- school may differ. This leads to a third point, not
tively related to having friends who valued all friendships are created equally (Flores-
education. For both groups of students, having Gonzalez, 2002). Some friendships and peer rela-
more access to friends who had dropped out of tionships may lead children to identify with and
school was associated with dropping out of become more engaged in school while others
school by the 12th grade. Having friends who may encourage disengagement. Given the com-
valued education was correlated with a decrease plexity of these issues and the limited research to
in dropout rates for non-Latino White students, date, more work is needed to understand the ways
but not for Mexican American students. On the in which racial and ethnic minority students’ peer
one hand, these findings suggest that having relationships are related to their engagement in
access to peers who have dropped out of school school.
may increase the likelihood that Mexican
American students will also drop out of school. Teachers’ Beliefs, Teacher-Child
On the other hand, they support the thesis that Relationships, and Instructional Practices
participation in extracurricular school activities In what ways are teachers’ beliefs about and
may be a protective factor against future school interactions with racial and ethnic minority stu-
disengagement (Eccles & Barber, 1999). Although dents related to minority students’ engagement
this study highlights the positive relation between with school? In general, researchers have
students’ activities and continued engagement observed positive associations between teachers’
with school, others have noted that limited finan- relationships with their students and children’s
cial resources may make it challenging for engagement and achievement in school (e.g.,
Mexican American adolescents to participate in Birch & Ladd, 1997; Decker, Dona, &
organized extracurricular activities (Spina, 2000; Christenson, 2007; Griffith, 2002; Hughes &
Updegraff, Kim, Killoren, & Thayer, 2009). Kwok, 2007; Mashburn & Pianta, 2006; Murray
Family and work obligations may also make it & Greenberg, 2000; Shaunessy & McHatton, 2009;
82 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

Urden & Schoenfelder, 2006; Wentzel, 1997). reported lower emotional engagement scores
The student-teacher relationship has been hypoth- compared to White students who were in school
esized to be one of the most important compo- with low percentages of minority teachers. Using
nents of the learning process (Garcia, the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent
Agbemakplido, Abdella, Lopez, & Registe, 2006; Health database, Crosnoe, Johnson and Elder
Hamre & Pianta, 2006). A positive teacher-child (2004) obtained similar findings with respect to
relationship may be particularly important for the relation between the proportion of White
racial and ethnic minority students who may need teachers at a school and students’ bonding with
support from teachers in order to successfully teachers (e.g., how students felt about teachers,
navigate school and classroom norms, which may whether teachers treated students fairly). In this
differ from those of the family culture (Au, 1998; study, the proportion of White teachers in the
D’Amato, 1993; Heath, 1983). In this section, we school was positively related to White students’
first consider research examining racial or ethnic ratings of emotional engagement, but was nega-
matching between teachers and their students. tively related to ratings of emotional engagement
Second, we examine how teachers’ perceptions for Latino girls and African American boys and
of racial and ethnic minority students relate to girls (Crosnoe et al., 2004). However, using the
teachers’ expectations for social and academic same data set, Johnson et al. (2001) found no
behavior. Finally, we discuss the research exam- relation between the proportion of White or
ining teachers’ instructional practices as they minority teachers at schools and students’ behav-
relate to students’ cultural backgrounds. ioral engagement.
As noted earlier, the public school population There is some evidence that matching racial
in the USA is remarkably diverse; children of and ethnic minority college students with univer-
color in kindergarten through high school repre- sity mentors who are of the same race or ethnicity
sent about 44% of the public school enrollment is associated with better behavioral engagement
(U.S. Department of Education, 2009). In stark (Campbell & Campbell, 2007). Although there
contrast to this diversity is the homogeneity of was no difference at the end of their first year in
the teacher population. Of the three million teach- grades or retention in college, 11 years after ini-
ers in public elementary and secondary educa- tial enrollment in college, Latino and African
tion, the vast majority (83%) are White (U.S. American college students, whose faculty men-
Department of Education, 2007). In this section, tors were of the same racial or ethnic group, had
we consider the importance of having a racial or attended college for more semesters, accumu-
ethnic match between teachers and students for lated more academic units, and were more likely
children’s achievement and engagement. to have entered a graduate program on campus
Although relatively little research has directly compared to students whose mentors were of a
examined this issue, there is some research exam- different race or ethnicity.
ining the relation between teacher-student match Given that the existing research on engage-
on race/ethnicity. ment and teacher-student racial/ethnic match is
In a study of African American eighth-grade so limited, we highlight one study that examines
students, Finn and Voelkl (1993) observed that the effect of teacher-student racial match on stu-
African American eighth graders who were in dent achievement. To examine the effect of hav-
schools with fewer minority teachers reported ing a teacher of the same race on student
lower emotional engagement scores (e.g., feel- achievement, Dee (2004) analyzed data from
ings of being welcomed, perception of a sup- Project STAR, the Tennessee large-scale random-
portive school environment, attachment to a ized field trial of the effects of small class size on
teacher) compared to peers who were in schools student achievement. In Project STAR, teachers
with higher percentages of minority teachers. and students in kindergarten through third grade
Conversely, in schools in which there were higher were randomly assigned to small classes, regular-
percentages of minority teachers, White students size classes or regular-size classes with teacher
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 83

aides within each of the 79 schools in the study. mechanisms. Some have posited that teachers’
Dee capitalized on the fact that this random beliefs about student achievement contribute to
assignment to classes also resulted in students the differences between White students and racial
being randomly assigned to either same-race or and ethnic minority students’ engagement and
other-race teachers. For both Black and non- achievement (e.g., Ferguson, 2003; Kuklinski &
Hispanic White students, assignment to a teacher Weinstein, 2001; Noguera, 2003; for reviews, see
of the same race resulted in a 3- to 5-percentile- Ferguson, 1998; Good & Brophy, 2000; Good &
point increase on math and reading achievement Nichols, 2001). For example, in a recent meta-
test scores (note, the limited numbers of teachers analysis of teacher expectations, Tenenbaum and
and students from other ethnic groups precluded Ruck (2007) found that teachers held more posi-
including them in Dee’s analyses). The advan- tive expectations for European American chil-
tage of having a same-race teacher occurred pri- dren compared to Latino or African American
marily in regular-size classes (22 students) as children and higher expectations for the school
opposed to small-size classes (15 students) and success of Asian American students relative to
with teachers who had been teaching less than European American students.
12 years. The effect also appeared to be stronger One way in which teachers’ beliefs about stu-
for students from economically disadvantaged dents may lead to differences in student engage-
homes. Finally, having a same-race teacher for ment with school between White students and
four consecutive years yielded a cumulative racial and ethnic minority students is through the
effect of 8-percentile points for reading and self-fulfilling prophecy hypothesis—if White
nearly 9-percentile points for math achievement teachers, as compared to racial and ethnic minor-
test scores in the primary grades. ity teachers, have more negative perceptions of
Dee’s findings taken together with the limited and expectations for minority students and stu-
findings from other studies (Crosnoe et al., 2004; dents live up (or down) to their teacher’s percep-
Finn & Voelkl, 1993) suggest that having a tions and expectations, then racial and ethnic
teacher-child racial/ethnic match or higher pro- minority students may do better in school if they
portions of minority teachers in schools that have have minority teachers. A second hypothesis,
substantial numbers of racial and ethnic minority which is discussed in more detail below, is that
students may be of benefit for racial and ethnic White teachers and minority teachers may treat
minority students. Why might having a teacher of students differently. Although considerable
the same race or ethnicity benefit a child? research has been conducted on teachers’ expec-
According to the cultural discontinuity perspec- tations since the late 1960s and has been reviewed
tive, differences between the ethnic culture at by others (e.g., Ferguson, 1998; Good & Brophy,
home and the mainstream culture at school may 2000; Good & Nichols, 2001; Tenenbaum &
interfere with student engagement and learning. Ruck, 2007), there is less research and mixed
A teacher who shares a common cultural back- findings on White and minority teachers’ percep-
ground may be able to help the child better under- tions and expectations of racial and ethnic minor-
stand the cultural norms of the school or may be ity students. For example, in a study of preservice
more tolerant of the child acting in accord with teachers from an urban university in the
his or her cultural norms. Alternatively, another Southeastern United States, White preservice
possible mechanism driving the positive associa- teachers, as compared to Black preservice teach-
tions between teacher-student matching and ers, rated Black elementary school students as
school experiences is that having a teacher of the being more dependent than their White class-
same race or ethnic group may provide minority mates (Kesner, 2000). In a study of elementary
students with a strong role model, which may be school teachers in an urban school district in the
particularly important for groups in which high- Northeastern United States, Pigott and Cowen
achieving academic role models are less salient. (2000) found that ratings of Black elementary
Research is needed to better understand underlying school students by both Black and White teachers
84 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

were more negative than were ratings of White between themselves and parents) being negatively
students. However, unlike White teachers, Black related to students’ ratings of behavioral engage-
teachers were more likely to judge all pupils as ment, but not their cognitive or emotional
being more competent and having fewer prob- engagement. Although far from conclusive, these
lems. Pigott and Cowen suggested that differ- studies suggest that teachers’ race and ethnic
ences in teachers’ ratings appear to support the group membership may be an important variable
notion that White and Black teachers may have to consider when examining associations
variable perceptions about child competence and between teachers’ expectations and students’
appropriate school behavior. Finally, in an exam- experiences at school.
ination of teachers’ perceptions of students from Considerable research indicates that teachers’
same or other racial and ethnic groups, Dee perceptions and beliefs about racial and ethnic
(2005) found that students were rated more nega- minority students are associated with their inter-
tively when they were rated by teachers who actions with students, which in turn, are related to
were not members of the same racial or ethnic students’ engagement and success in school
group as the student. However, when the data (Farkas, 2003; Gay, 2000; Hamre & Pianta, 2005;
from each region of the country (North, South, Kozol, 2006; Martin, Fergus, & Noguera, 2010).
East, and West based on Census classifications) Although a full review of literature connecting
were examined separately, Dee observed that the teachers’ interactions to student engagement is
effect was only statistically significant in the beyond the scope of this chapter, we briefly con-
South. These data suggest that conflicting find- sider the relations (a) between the quality of
ings in previous research may, at least in part, be teacher-child relationships and racial and ethnic
due to cultural differences or biases that exist minority students’ engagement and (b) between
across regions of the country. teachers’ pedagogical practices and racial and
There is some research addressing the relation ethnic minority students’ engagement.
between teachers’ expectations and minority stu- The amount of teacher support (often defined
dents’ engagement. Tyler and Boelter (2008) as the extent to which teachers’ listen to, encour-
found that low-income African American middle age, and respect students) and the quality of
school students’ perceptions of teachers’ expecta- teacher-child relationships may be especially
tions were positively related to self-reported rat- important to racial and ethnic minority students’
ings of cognitive, behavioral, and emotional engagement in school because they may help stu-
engagement. In a study of African American, dents navigate differences between home and
Latino, and European American low-income school environments (Gay, 2000; Heath, 1983),
middle school students, Tyler and colleagues cope with experiences of discrimination, failure,
(Tyler, Boelter, & Boykin, 2008) examined teach- and environmental risk (Faircloth & Hamm,
ers’ perceptions of the degree to which parents’ 2005; Garcia-Reid, 2007; Garcia-Reid, Reid, &
beliefs about education were similar to their own Peterson, 2005; Gay, 2000; Roeser, Strobel, &
beliefs and students’ self-reported cognitive, Quihuis, 2002), and facilitate positive beliefs
behavioral, and emotional engagement. White about learning and the importance of education
teachers were more likely than African American (Skinner et al., 2008; Strambler & Weinstein,
teachers to perceive parents’ endorsement of edu- 2010; Urden & Schoenfelder, 2006). Some
cational values (e.g., importance of parental role research suggests that strong teacher support may
in education, the teaching of literacy) to be differ- compensate for low support at home for Latino
ent from their own. Regardless of teacher race, a students. For example, in a study of low-income
significant negative association emerged between Latino middle and high school students, Brewster
teachers’ beliefs and European American, Latino, and Bowen (2004) found that students’ perceived
and African American students’ self-reported support from teachers was negatively associated
engagement behaviors, with teachers’ perceptions with students’ problem school behavior (i.e.,
of educational value discontinuity (i.e., differences showing up late for school, skipping class) and
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 85

positively associated with students’ emotional The studies reviewed here support the general
engagement (i.e., feelings about school). These hypothesis that having supportive adult relation-
associations were maintained even when account- ships at school is positively correlated with racial
ing for family and home support effects. In fact, and ethnic minority middle and high school stu-
with regard to student problem behavior, once dents’ behavioral and emotional engagement.
teacher support was entered into the equation, the However, most studies have examined the experi-
relationship between parent support and behavior ence of older students and focused on student-
was no longer significant. In a study of low- reported measures of engagement. In a recent
income, Latino middle school students, students’ study with African American and Latino elemen-
ratings of teacher closeness and trust were posi- tary students from low-income backgrounds,
tively related to students’ behavioral engagement, Strambler and Weinstein (2010) examined the
after controlling for student achievement and the associations among students’ perceptions of
quality of the parent–child relationship (Murray, teacher caring (e.g., my teacher likes me, my
2009). There was a significant interaction between teacher really cares about me), negative teacher
parent–child and teacher-child relationships such feedback (e.g., my teacher makes me feel bad
that, having a positive teacher-child relationship when I do not have the right answer), and stu-
seems to compensate for a poor parent–child dents’ feelings about school and their behavioral
relationship in terms of students’ reports of their engagement. On average, elementary students
school competence. rated their teachers as being emotionally support-
Although students’ positive feelings about ive, but student ratings of teacher emotional sup-
their teacher correlate with other aspects of port were not associated with teachers’ ratings of
engagement with school, it is important to student behavioral engagement (e.g., completing
remember that students’ relationships with their class work and homework, listening to and fol-
teachers are dynamic and change over time. In lowing directions), students’ valuing of education
the Longitudinal Immigration Student Adaptation (e.g., caring about how one does in school), or
Study, Green, Rhodes, Hirsch, Suárez-Orozco students’ devaluing of education (e.g., not caring
and Camic (2008) assessed students’ reports of about grades, believing that learning is not impor-
their behavioral engagement, related to complet- tant). However, students’ perceptions of teachers
ing school work, turning in homework, and pay- as negative or unsupportive were positively related
ing attention in class, over 3 years in a sample of to students’ academic devaluing. Academic deval-
Mexican and Central American immigrant stu- uing, in turn, was negatively related to students’
dents (Green et al., 2008). Students’ perceptions math and reading scores, but was not associated
of support from teachers and other adults at with teachers’ ratings of behavioral engagement.
school were also assessed in each year. The aver- Although the findings do not directly link teacher
age level of adult support across the 3 years was negativity to behavioral engagement, they do sug-
calculated for each student, and perceived sup- gest that, for younger students, who generally like
port for each year was represented as a deviation their teachers, instances of negativity and insensi-
from the 3 year average. Students’ reports of tivity may be very salient to children’s feelings
their behavioral engagement changed from year about school and their achievement.
to year, and those changes mirrored changes in Teachers’ instructional practices may support
their reports of adult support at school. That is, racial and ethnic minority students’ engagement.
when students perceived adult support to be Teachers who are sensitive to the challenges and
lower than their average level of support across needs of their minority students may use interac-
the 3 years, their self-reported level of behav- tion styles and pedagogical practices that capital-
ioral engagement was less. When students felt ize on these students’ strengths and life
like they were receiving more support from experiences and, as a result, benefit students’
adults at school, their behavioral engagement engagement (Gay, 2000). For example, in a case
was higher. study of a diverse urban high school in California,
86 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

Conchas (2001) observed that teacher interac- children to raise their hands and then ignoring
tions and the organizational features of the school their response or pulling the student’s card or put-
appeared to be associated with immigrant and ting him in time out appeared to result in feelings
native-born Latino students’ emotional engage- of rejection and behavioral disengagement (e.g.,
ment with peers at school. In this study, the per- children cried, stopped participating, or were
ceptions of students who participated in different removed from the group).
academic programs within the high school (e.g., As illustrated in the above study, the organiza-
a health-related career academy, general educa- tion of classroom activities may constrain the
tion program, Advanced Placement Program) formation of teacher-student and student-student
were contrasted. Conchas argued that the struc- relationships because of the frequency of teacher
ture and culture of these academic programs elic- reprimands and determination of who receives
ited very different attitudes from the students. attention and assistance from the teacher (Bossert,
When a program was racially and ethnically 1979). In studies of student engagement and the
diverse and teachers encouraged students to help organization of classroom activities, high school
each other so that all learned, it appeared that the students reported being less behaviorally engaged
Latino students were more likely to perceive their during whole-group instruction as compared to
peers as being supportive, have cross racial- and during small group or individual work time
ethnic-group friendships and feel empowered to (Shernoff, Csikszentmihalyi, Schneider, &
learn (Conchas). Shernoff, 2003). Research on ethnic minority
Unfortunately, many curricular approaches children’s early childhood experiences docu-
implemented with racial and ethnic minority stu- ments that Latino and African American children
dents, particularly those who are poor, have been spend significantly less time in free choice activi-
criticized as being behaviorally based, punitive, ties and significantly more time in teacher-led
and, over time, likely to lead to a decrease in stu- assigned time and African American children
dent engagement (Levine, Lowe, Peterson, & spend significantly more time engaged in routine
Tenoiro, 1995; Varenne & McDermott, 1998). activities, such as meals or lining up (Early et al.,
For example, in a case study of a second-grade 2010). The amount of teacher-directed instruc-
class with Black, Latino, and White students and tional time continues to increase into first grade
a young White teacher, Langhout and Mitchell (La Paro, Rimm-Kaufman, & Pianta, 2006).
(2008) documented the teacher’s implementation The instructional experiences of racial and
of a discipline strategy (i.e., the use of a behavior ethnic minority students in US schools often do
chart in which children’s cards are pulled by the not reflect the preferred values and correspond-
teacher each time the child misbehaves) and its ing behaviors that are reflected in the home cul-
relation to students’ behavioral and emotional tures of many racial and ethnic minority children
disengagement from the learning process. Their (Gay, 2000; Gutierrez & Rogoff, 2003; Heath,
analyses show that Black and Latino boys were 1983). For example, sharing, cooperation, and
significantly more likely to have their names primacy of group needs over individual needs
moved on the behavioral chart, despite the fact are commonly held values among American
that having one’s name moved did not increase or Indian People, and competitive behavior is often
decrease the likelihood of a student’s name being inhibited (Deyhle, 1995; Sanders, 1987; Yates,
pulled in the future (i.e., the discipline technique 1987). In mainstream schools, which generally
was not effective at reducing misbehavior). emphasize individualism and teacher-directed
Qualitative analyses suggested that use of the learning, American Indian children may be per-
chart was adversely related to Black and Latino ceived as being unmotivated or unengaged
boys’ behavioral and emotional engagement. because of their unwillingness to compete
Because many of the infractions occurred when (Castagno, McKinley, Brayboy, 2008).
the children were excited to participate and call Differences in rules of speaking, listening, and
out answers, the teacher’s response of telling turn taking in conversations may make it more
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 87

difficult for American Indian children to participate In general, the quality of students’ relationships
in classroom activities (Greenbaum, 1985; at school and the nature of teacher-student inter-
Sanders, 1987). actions appear to be related to racial and ethnic
Research on African American students also minority students’ engagement and achievement
emphasizes the importance of communalism (i.e., (e.g., Decker et al., 2007; Furrer & Skinner, 2003;
belief in a fundamental interdependence of peo- Griffith, 2002; Wentzel, 1997). Children who
ple) to African American students’ learning expe- report having supportive teachers are more likely
riences outside of school (Tyler, Boykin, Boelter, to be engaged in learning engagement behaviors.
& Dillihunt, 2005). Boykin, Tyler and Miller In addition, in some cases, having a racial or eth-
(2005) found that many African American stu- nic match between teachers and students may be
dents gravitate toward verve-related learning beneficial to student engagement and subsequent
behaviors, meaning that students demonstrated a school achievement. This may occur as a result of
preference for higher levels of movement or audi- positive teacher beliefs about children’s ability to
tory stimulation. The relationship between verve learn, having an ethnic minority role model in the
and student engagement and achievement was classroom, or through ethnic minority teachers’
demonstrated by Cole and Boykin (2008) in a ability to possibly create less discontinuity between
study examining the role that music and move- home and school environments. Unfortunately,
ment experiences might have on children’s learn- given the limited number of studies to date and
ing of a story that was read to them and their with few exceptions, their correlational nature,
mood during the experience. In the first experi- making any causal inferences about these relation-
ment, 48 African American children were ships is problematic.
randomly assigned to one of four treatment
conditions, while in the second experiment they
were assigned to one of the four conditions or a
control condition. Each was read a story and Conclusions
either exposed to (a) syncopated music with
movement, (b) syncopated music without move- The underachievement of African American,
ment, (c) nonsyncopated music with movement, Latino, and Native American students in the
(d) nonsyncopated music without movement, or United States has been partially attributed to poor
(e) no music and no movement (control; study 2) engagement in school (e.g., Connell et al., 1994;
during the reading. In experiment one, fourth- Steele, 1997). However, the relation between
grade children’s ability to recall the story was engagement and school achievement is not a sim-
highest in the syncopated music and movement ple one. Somewhat paradoxically, social scien-
condition and lowest in the nonsyncopated music tists have noted that on some dimensions of
without movement condition. In experiment two, engagement, underachieving minority students
fourth graders demonstrated better story recall have indicated stronger engagement relative to
under the syncopated music and movement con- their higher achieving peers (e.g., Dotterer et al.,
dition, but sixth graders performed higher on the 2009; Johnson et al., 2001; Mickelson, 1990;
story recall task in the nonsyncopated music, low Shernoff & Schmidt, 2008). In some instances,
movement condition. In experiment 2 undertaken for example, minority students more strongly
with 128 fourth- and sixth-grade students, both endorse the value or importance of education
fourth- and sixth-grade children performed the compared to White students even though the
lowest in the control (no music, no movement) minority students do less well in school (e.g.,
condition. Although more research is needed, Mickelson, 1990; Shernoff & Schmidt, 2008).
these data suggest that younger African American Similarly, self-report measures of emotional
students may benefit from an educational context engagement (e.g., belonging, feeling a part of
that incorporates the music and movement of school) may result in different engagement sce-
their home culture. narios compared to teacher reports of students’
88 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

behavioral engagement (Shernoff & Schmidt). example, although there is great diversity in
In this chapter, we have considered a number of parents’ ideas about education, how children
factors that are associated with racial and ethnic learn, what children need to do to succeed in
minority students’ engagement with school, and a school, and what parents should do to support
number of key themes have emerged. their child’s education, parents of racial and ethnic
First, ethnic or cultural membership appears minority youth who have (a) strong and clear
to be a critical feature related to racial and ethnic academic expectations for their child, (b) predict-
minority schoolchildren’s valuing of, beliefs able family routines, and (c) good relationships
about, and engagement in school. In contrast to with their child are more likely to have children
the cultural ecological perspective (e.g., Ogbu, who are engaged in school (Annunziata et al.,
1986), it appears that having a strong racial or 2006; Murray, 2009; Sirin & Rogers-Sirin, 2005;
ethnic identity can coexist with or encompass a Taylor & Lopez, 2005). Similar to parental sup-
strong academic identity (e.g., Bingham et al., port at home, peer support at school appears to
2001; Chavous et al., 2003; Smalls et al., 2007). matter to engagement. Students who have access
It may be that having a strong ethnic identity to same-ethnic peers, have friends who value
along with a bicultural identity—one that main- education, and have peer social support at school,
tains that achieving within the mainstream cul- particularly a best friend, appear to do better than
ture is appropriate for members of one’s racial or students who do not have these peer resources
ethnic minority group—is the key to minority (Brown et al., 2008; Gonzalez, 2009; Ryan, 2000;
students’ becoming engaged in school (e.g., Shin et al., 2007; Vaquera, 2009). Finally, teach-
Okagaki et al., 2009; Oyserman et al., 2001, ers appear to be a crucial component in ethnic
2003). As suggested by social identity theorists minority students’ engagement processes, with
and researchers, people belong to multiple groups the quality of teacher-child relationships, teach-
at any given time; they have multiple social iden- ers’ expectations for ethnic minority students’
tities. It is the interplay among those social learning, and teachers’ pedagogical practices all
identities that directs behavior. For ethnic minor- appearing to be related to students’ engagement
ity students, their ethnic, bicultural, and academic or disengagement from the learning process
identities may work together to support, or be at (Crosnoe et al., 2004; Faircloth & Hamm, 2005;
odds in relation to, the student’s motivation for, Ferguson, 2003; Skinner et al., 2008; Urden &
engagement in, and performance in school. Schoenfelder, 2006).
Second, perception of discrimination gener- There is so much that we still do not know
ally appears to be negatively associated with stu- about factors that support and inhibit the school
dents’ engagement (Dotterer et al., 2009; Okagaki engagement of racial and ethnic minority stu-
et al., 1996; Taylor et al., 1994), but this relation dents. Among the most promising lines of
may be mediated by students’ belief in the instru- research are studies that explicitly examine the
mental importance of school (Taylor et al.). Belief relations between engagement with school, eth-
in the pragmatic importance of education nicity, and their interactions with multiple fac-
(Bingham et al., 2001; Mickelson, 1990) is posi- tors, such as studies by Dotterer et al. (2009),
tively related to students’ engagement and Murray (2009), and Taylor and colleagues (1994).
achievement, as is having a bicultural identity, It is only by systematically looking at the interac-
one that includes participation and achievement tions among many factors that we are likely to
in the mainstream culture (Bingham et al., 2001; establish clear understandings of the relations
Oyserman et al., 2003). In addition, the negative between ethnicity and engagement.
relation between discrimination and engagement Finally, although many have theorized that
may be moderated by having a strong ethnic cultural discontinuity between children’s home
identity, at least for girls (Dotterer et al.). and school contexts inhibits racial and ethnic
Third, peers, teachers, and parents clearly minority students’ engagement in school, few
matter to children’s educational experiences. For have attempted to measure that discontinuity and
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 89

explicitly examine the link between discontinuity Antonio, A. L. (2004). The influence of friendship groups
and students’ cognitive, emotional, and behav- on intellectual self-confidence and educational aspira-
tions in college. Journal of Higher Education, 75,
ioral engagement. As posited earlier, one reason 446–471.
that teachers perceive minority students to be less Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J.
engaged in school relative to their peers is because (2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con-
teachers, particularly those who are White, may ceptual and methodological issues of the construct.
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386.
not understand when students are engaging in Arroyo, C. G., & Zigler, E. (1995). Racial identity, aca-
behaviors consistent with their home cultures demic achievement, and the psychological well-being
(e.g., Deyhle, 1995; Tyler et al., 2008). We need of economically disadvantaged adolescents. Journal
better identification of the discontinuities between of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5),
902–914.
home and school cultures. We need to know Au, K. H. (1998). Social constructivism and the school
which differences matter and how much of that literacy learning of students of diverse backgrounds.
difference actually matters. Journal of Literacy Research, 30, 297–319.
Public education in the United States is a com- Aud, S., Hussar, W., Planty, M., Snyder, T., Bianco, K.,
Fox, M., et al. (2010). The condition of education 2010
plex enterprise. Public schools are responsible (NCES 2010–028). Washington, DC: National Center
for providing an education that will enable stu- for Education Statistics, Institute of Education
dents from all backgrounds to learn and succeed. Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
Behavioral scientists may not be able to quickly Bellmore, A. D., Nishina, A., Witkow, M. R., Graham, S.,
& Juvonen, J. (2007). The influence of classroom eth-
identify all of the ways in which the cultural nic composition on same- and other-ethnicity peer
norms of ethnic and racial minority groups differ nominations in middle school. Social Development,
from those of the mainstream culture, but per- 16, 720–740.
haps, we can move with speed toward identifying Bennett, M. D., Jr. (2006). Cultural resources and school
engagement among African American youths: The
those differences that may make a substantial dif- role of racial socialization and ethnic identity. Children
ference in the engagement and achievement of and Schools, 28(4), 197–206.
racial and ethnic minority students. It is encour- Bernal, M. E., Saenz, D. S., & Knight, G. P. (1991). Ethnic
aging to find that supportive teacher-child rela- identity and adaptation of Mexican American youths
in school settings. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral
tionships are associated with better engagement Sciences, 13(2), 135–154.
among racial and ethnic minority children (e.g., Berndt, T. J. (1999). Friends’ influence on students’
Green et al., 2008; Murray, 2009) and that sup- adjustment to school. Educational Psychologist, 34,
portive teacher-child relationships may be able to 15–28.
Bingham, G., Helling, M. K., & Okagaki, L. (2001, April).
buffer low support from home (Brewster & Ethnic identity and school achievement in Native
Bowen, 2004; Murray). Can researchers expedi- American students. Presented at the Biennial Meeting
ently take the next steps and identify those behav- of the Society for Research in Child Development,
iors that lead to positive teacher-child relationships Minneapolis, MN.
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher-child
for minority students and then train teachers to relationship and children’s early school adjustment.
engage in such behaviors? For the sake of current Journal of School Psychology, 35, 61–79.
and future ethnic minority children and youth in Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of parenting:
US schools, we certainly hope so. Social conditions and applied parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 4).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bossert, S. (1979). Tasks and social relationships in class-
rooms. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
References Press.
Boykin, W. A., Tyler, K. M., & Miller, O. (2005). In search
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Bedinger, S. D. of cultural themes and their expressions in the dynam-
(1994). When expectations work: Race and socioeco- ics of classroom life. Urban Education, 40, 521–549.
nomic differences in school performance. Social Brewster, A. B., & Bowen, G. L. (2004). Teacher support
Psychology Quarterly, 57(4), 283–299. and the school engagement of Latino middle and high
Annunziata, D., Hogue, A., Faw, L., & Liddle, H. A. school students at risk of school failure. Child and
(2006). Family functioning and school success in at- Adolescent Social Work Journal, 21, 47–67.
risk, inner-city adolescents. Journal of Youth and Brown, B. B., Hamm, J. V., Herman, M., & Heck, D. J.
Adolescence, 35(1), 105–113. (2008). Ethnicity and image: Correlates of crowd
90 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

affiliation among ethnic minority youth. Child Dee, T. S. (2004). Teachers, race, and student achieve-
Development, 79, 529–546. ment in a randomized experiment. The Review of
Buriel, R., & De Ment, T. (1997). Immigration and socio- Economics and Statistics, 86(1), 195–210.
cultural change in Mexican, Chinese, and Vietnamese Dee, T. S. (2005). A teacher like me: Does race, ethnicity,
American families. In A. Booth, A. C. Crouter, & N. or gender matter? The American Economic Review,
Landale (Eds.), Immigration and the family (pp. 165–200). 95, 158–165.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. Deyhle, D. (1995). Navajo youth and Anglo racism:
Campbell, T. A., & Campbell, D. E. (2007). Outcomes of Cultural integrity and resistance. Harvard Educational
mentoring at-risk college students: Gender and ethnic Review, 65(3), 403–445.
matching effects. Mentoring and Tutoring, 15, Dotterer, A. M., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (2009).
135–148. Sociocultural factors and school engagement among
Caplan, N., Whitmore, J. K., & Choy, M. H. (1989). The African American youth: The roles of racial discrimi-
Boat People and achievement in American: A study of nation, racial socialization, and ethnic identity. Applied
family life, hard work, and cultural values. Ann Arbor, Developmental Science, 13(2), 61–73.
MI: The University of Michigan Press. Early, D. M., Iruka, I. U., Ritchie, S., Barbarin, O. A.,
Carrasquillo, A. L. (1991). Hispanic children and youth in Winn, D. C., et al. (2010). How do pre-kindergarteners
the United States: A resource guide. New York: spend their time? Gender, ethnicity, and income as pre-
Garland. dictors of experiences in pre-kindergarten classrooms.
Castagno, A. E., McKinley, B., & Brayboy, J. (2008). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 177–193.
Culturally responsive schooling for indigenous youth: Eccles, J., & Barber, B. (1999). Student council, volun-
A review of the literature. Review of Educational teering, basketball, or matching band: What kind of
Research, 78, 941–993. extracurricular involvement matters? Journal of
Chao, R. K. (2000). The parenting of immigrant Chinese Adolescent Research, 14, 10–43.
and European American mothers: Relations between Epstein, J. L., & Karweit, N. (Eds.). (1983). Friends in
parenting styles, socialization goals, and parental school: Patterns of selection and influence in second-
practices. Journal of Applied Developmental ary schools. New York: Academic.
Psychology, 21(2), 233–248. Faircloth, B. S., & Hamm, J. V. (2005). Sense of belonging
Chavous, T. M., Bernat, D. H., Schmeelk-Cone, K., among high school students representing four ethnic
Caldwell, C. H., Kohn-Wood, L., & Zimmerman, M. groups. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 293–309.
A. (2003). Racial identity and academic attainment Fan, X., & Chen, M. (2001). Parental involvement and
among African American adolescents. Child students’ academic achievement: A meta-analysis.
Development, 74(4), 1076–1090. Educational Psychology Review, 13(1), 1–22.
Chen, C., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Motivation and Farkas, G. (2003). Racial disparities and discrimination in
mathematics achievement: A comparative study of education: What do we know, how do we know it, and
Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East what do we need to know? Teachers College Record,
Asian high school students. Child Development, 66, 105(6), 1119–1146.
1215–1234. Ferguson, R. F. (1998). Teachers’ perceptions and expec-
Cole, J. M., & Boykin, A. W. (2008). Examining cultur- tations and the Black-White test score gap. In
ally structured learning environments with different C. Christopher & M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black-White
types of music-linked movement opportunity. Journal test score gap (pp. 273–317). Washington, DC:
of Black Psychology, 34, 331–355. Brookings Institution Press.
Conchas, G. Q. (2001). Structuring failure and success: Ferguson, R. F. (2003). Teachers’ perceptions and expec-
Understanding the variability in Latino school engage- tations and the Black-White test score gap. Urban
ment. Harvard Educational Review, 71(3), 475–504. Education, 38, 460–507.
Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994). Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success
Educational risk and resilience in African-American among students at risk for school failure. Journal of
youth: Context, self, action, and outcomes in school. Applied Psychology, 82, 221–234.
Child Development, 65, 493–506. Finn, J. D., & Voelkl, K. E. (1993). School characteristics
Crosnoe, R., Johnson, M. K., & Elder, G. H. (2004). related to student engagement. Journal of Negro
School size and the interpersonal side of education: Education, 62, 249–268.
An examination of race/ethnicity and organizational Flores-González, N. (1999). Puerto Rican high achiev-
context. Social Science Quarterly, 85, 1259–1274. ers: An example of ethnic and academic identity
D’Amato, J. (1993). Resistance and compliance in minority compatibility. Anthropology & Education Quarterly,
classrooms. In E. Jacob & C. Jordan (Eds.), Minority 30(3), 343–362.
education: Anthropological perspectives (pp. 181–207). Flores-Gonzalez, N. (2002). School kids/street kids:
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Identity development in Latino students. New York:
Decker, D. M., Dona, D. P., & Christenson, S. L. (2007). Teachers College Press.
Behaviorally at-risk African American students: The Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. (1986). Black students’ school
importance of student-teacher relationships for student success: Coping with the “burden of acting White”.
outcomes. Journal of School Psychology, 45, 83–109. Urban Review, 18(3), 176–206.
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 91

Fredericks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). Graham, S. (1994). Motivation in African Americans.
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of Review of Educational Research, 64, 55–118.
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, Graham, S., Taylor, A. Z., & Hudley, C. (1998). Exploring
59–109. achievement values among ethnic minority early ado-
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a lescents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(4),
factors in children’s academic engagement and perfor- 605–620.
mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, Green, G., Rhodes, J., Hirsch, A. H., Suarez-Orozco, C.,
148–162. & Camic, P. M. (2008). Supportive adult relationships
Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C. (2001). Analyzing cul- and the academic engagement of Latin American
tural models and settings to connect minority achieve- immigrant youth. Journal of School Psychology, 46,
ment and school improvement research. Educational 393–412.
Psychologist, 36(1), 45–56. Greenbaum, P. E. (1985). Nonverbal differences in com-
Garcia, V., Agbemakplido, W., Abdella, H., Lopez, O., & munication style between American Indian and Anglo
Registe, R. T. (2006). High school students’ perspec- elementary classrooms. American Educational
tives of the 2001 No Child Left Behind Act’s defini- Research Journal, 22, 101–115.
tion of a highly qualified teacher. Harvard Educational Griffith, J. (2002). A multilevel analysis of the relation of
Review, 76, 698–724. school learning and social environments to minority
Garcia-Reid, P. (2007). Examining social capital as a achievement in public elementary schools. The
mechanism for improving school engagement among Elementary School Journal, 102, 349–366.
low income Hispanic girls. Youth and Society, 39(2), Gutiérrez, K. D., & Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways of
164–181. learning: Individual traits or repertoires of practice.
Garcia-Reid, P., Reid, R. J., & Peterson, N. A. (2005). Educational Researcher, 32(5), 19–25.
School engagement among Latino youth in an urban Halle, T. G., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Mahoney, J. (1997).
middle school context: Valuing the role of social sup- Family influences on school achievement in low-
port. Education and Urban Society, 37, 257–275. income, African American children. Journal of
Gay, G. (2000). Culturally responsive teaching. Theory, Educational Psychology, 89(3), 527–537.
research, and practice. New York: Teachers College Hamm, J. V. (2000). Do birds of a feather flock together?
Press. The variable bases for African American, Asian
Gibson, M. A. (1987). The school performance of immi- American, and European American adolescents’ selec-
grant minorities: A comparative view. Anthropology & tion of similar friends. Developmental Psychology, 36,
Education Quarterly, 18, 262–275. 209–219.
Gibson, M. A., & Ogbu, J. U. (Eds.). (1991). Minority Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Can instructional
status and schooling: A comparative study of immi- and emotional support in the first-grade classroom
grant and involuntary minorities. New York: make a difference for children at risk of school failure?
Garland. Child Development, 76, 949–967.
Gill, S., & Reynolds, A. J. (1999). Educational expecta- Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2006). Student-teacher
tions and school achievement of urban African relationships. In G. G. Bear & K. M. Minke (Eds.),
American children. Journal of School Psychology, Children’s needs III: Development, prevention, and
37(4), 403–424. intervention (pp. 151–176). Washington, DC: National
Glanville, J. L., & Wildhagen, T. (2007). The measure- Association of School Psychologists.
ment of school engagement: Assessing dimensionality Hao, L., & Bonstead-Bruns, M. (1998). Parent–child dif-
and measurement invariance across race and ethnicity. ferences in educational expectations and the academic
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 67, achievement of immigrant and native students.
1019–1041. Sociology of Education, 71(3), 175–198.
Goldenberg, C., Gallimore, R., Reese, L., & Garnier, H. Harwood, R. L., Handwerker, W. P., Schoelmerich, A., &
(2001). Cause or effect? A longitudinal study of immi- Leyendecker, B. (2001). Ethnic category labels,
grant Latino parents’ aspirations and expectations, and parental beliefs, and the contextualized individual:
their children’s school performance. American An exploration of the individualism-sociocentrism
Educational Research Journal, 38(3), 547–582. debate. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1,
Gonzalez, R. (2009). Beyond affirmation: How the 217–236.
school context facilitates racial/ethnic identity among Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and
Mexican American adolescents. Hispanic Journal of work in communities and classrooms. New York:
Behavioral Sciences, 31, 5–31. Cambridge University Press.
Good, T., & Brophy, J. (2000). Looking in classrooms (8th Helms, J. E., & Talleyrand, R. M. (1997). Race is not eth-
ed.). New York: Longman. nicity. American Psychologist, 52(11), 1246–1247.
Good, T. L., & Nichols, S. L. (2001). Expectancy effects Hieshima, J. A., & Schneider, B. (1994). Intergenerational
in the classroom: A special focus on improving the effects on the cultural and cognitive socialization of
reading performance of minority students in first- third- and fourth-generation Japanese Americans.
grade classrooms. Educational Psychologist, 36, Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15,
113–126. 319–327.
92 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

Ho, D. Y. F. (1994). Cognitive socialization in Confucian La Paro, K., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Pianta, R. C.
heritage cultures. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (2006). Kindergarten to 1st grade: Classroom charac-
(Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child develop- teristics and the stability and change of children’s
ment (pp. 285–313). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. classroom experiences. Journal of Research in
Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Tardiff, T. (2002). Socioeconomic Childhood Education, 21, 189–203.
status and parenting. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Lareau, A. (1996). Assessing parent involvement in school-
Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority chil- ing: A critical analysis. In A. Booth & J. F. Dunn (Eds.),
dren development (pp. 285–313). Hillsdale, NJ: Family-school links: How do they affect educational out-
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. comes? (pp. 57–64). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.
Hudley, C., & Graham, S. (2001). Stereotypes of achieve- Levine, D., Lowe, R., Peterson, B., & Tenorio, R. (Eds.).
ment striving among early adolescents. Social (1995). Rethinking schools: An agenda for change.
Psychology of Education, 5, 201–224. New York: The New Press.
Huffman, T. E. (1991). The experiences, perceptions, and Libbey, H. P. (2004). Measuring student relationships to
consequences of campus racism among Northern school: Attachment, bonding, connectedness, and
Plains Indians. Journal of American Indian Education, engagement. Journal of School Health, 74, 274–283.
30(2), 25–34. Luster, T., & McAdoo, H. (1996). Family and child influ-
Hughes, J., & Kwok, O. (2007). Influence of student- ences on educational attainment: A secondary analysis
teacher and parent-teacher relationships on lower of the High/Scope Perry Preschool data. Developmental
achieving readers’ engagement and achievement in the Psychology, 32(1), 26–39.
primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, Machamer, A. M., & Gruber, E. (1998). Secondary school,
99, 39–51. family, and educational risk: Comparing American
Hughes, D., Rodriguez, J., Smith, E. P., Johnson, D. J., Indian adolescents and their peers. The Journal of
Stevenson, H. C., & Spicer, P. (2006). Parents’ ethnic- Educational Research, 91(6), 357–369.
racial socialization practices: A review of research and Martin, M., Fergus, E., & Noguera, P. (2010). Responding
directions for future study. Developmental Psychology, to the needs of the whole child: A case study of a high-
42, 747–770. performing elementary school for immigrant children.
Johnson, M. K., Crosnoe, R., & Elder, G. H. (2001). Reading and Writing Quarterly, 26, 195–222.
Students’ attachment and academic engagement: The Mashburn, A. J., & Pianta, R. C. (2006). Social relation-
role of race and ethnicity. Sociology of Education, 74, ships and school readiness. Early Education and
318–340. Development, 17, 151–176.
Keefe, S. E., & Padilla, A. M. (1987). Chicano ethnicity. Matute-Bianchi, M. E. (1986). Ethnic identities and pat-
Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press. terns of school success and failure among Mexican-
Kesner, J. E. (2000). Teacher characteristics and the qual- descent and Japanese-American students in a
ity of child-teacher relationships. Journal of School California high school: An ethnographic analysis.
Psychology, 28, 133–149. American Journal of Education, 95, 233–255.
KewalRamani, A., Gilbertson, L., Fox, M., & Provasnik, Mickelson, R. (1990). The attitude-achievement paradox
S. (2007). Status and trends in the education of racial among black adolescents. Sociology of Education, 63,
and ethnic minorities (NCES 2007–039). Washington, 44–61.
DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute Miller, D. B. (1999). Racial socialization and racial iden-
of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of tity: Can they promote resiliency for African American
Education. adolescents? Adolescence, 34(135), 493–501.
Kozol, J. (2006). The shame on the nation: The restora- Miller, L. S. (1995). An American imperative: Accelerating
tion of apartheid in schooling in America. New York: minority educational advancement. New Haven, CT:
Random House. Yale University Press.
Kuklinski, M. R., & Weinstein, R. S. (2001). Classroom Mizokawa, D. T., & Ryckman, D. B. (1990). Attributions
and developmental differences in a path model of of academic success and failure: A comparison of six
teacher expectancy effects. Child Development, 72, Asian-American ethnic groups. Journal of Cross-
1554–1578. Cultural Psychology, 21(4), 434–451.
Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. (1982). Moody, J. (2001). Race, school integration, and friendship
Culture and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), segregation in America. The American Journal of
Handbook of human intelligence (pp. 642–719). Sociology, 107, 679–716.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Murray, C. (2009). Parent and teacher relationships as
LaFromboise, T., Coleman, H. L. K., & Gerton, J. (1993). predictors of school engagement and functioning
Psychological impact of biculturalism: Evidence and among low-income urban youth. The Journal of Early
theory. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 395–412. Adolescence, 29, 379–404.
Langhout, R. D., & Mitchell, C. A. (2008). Engaging con- Murray, C., & Greenberg, M. T. (2000). Children’s rela-
texts: Drawing the link between student and teacher tionship with teachers and bonds with school. Journal
experiences of the hidden curriculum. Journal of of School Psychology, 38, 423–445.
Community and Applied Social Psychology, 18, Noguera, P. A. (2003). The trouble with Black boys: The
593–614. role and influence of environmental and cultural
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 93

factors on the academic performance of African Oyserman, D., Harrison, K., & Bybee, D. (2001). Can
American males. Urban Education, 38, 431–459. racial identity be promotive of academic efficacy?
Ogbu, J. U. (1986). The consequences of the American International Journal of Behavioral Development,
caste system. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The school 25(4), 379–385.
achievement of minority children: New perspectives Oyserman, D., Kemmelmeier, M., Fryberg, S., Brosh, H.,
(pp. 19–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum & Hart-Johnson, T. (2003). Racial-ethnic self-sche-
Associates. mas. Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 333–347.
Ogbu, J. U. (1992). Understanding cultural diversity and Peng, S. S., & Wright, D. (1994). Explanation of aca-
learning. Educational Researcher, 21(8), 5–14. demic achievement of Asian American students. The
Ogbu, J. U., & Matute-Bianchi, M. D. (1986). Journal of Educational Research, 87(6), 346–352.
Understanding sociocultural factors in education: Perdue, N. H., Manzeske, D. P., & Estell, D. B. (2009).
Knowledge, identity, and adjustment. In Beyond lan- Early predictors of school engagement: Exploring the
guage: Sociocultural factors in schooling, language, role of peer relationships. Psychology in the Schools,
and minority students (pp. 71–143). California State 46, 1084–1097.
Department of Education. Los Angeles: Education Phinney, J. S. (1990). Ethnic identity in adolescents and
Dissemination and Assessment Center, California adults: Review of research. Psychological Bulletin,
State University, Los Angeles. 108, 499–514.
Okagaki, L. (2001). Triarchic model of minority chil- Phinney, J. S. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity mea-
dren’s school achievement. Educational Psychologist, sure: A new scale for use with diverse groups. Journal
36(1), 9–20. of Adolescent Research, 7(2), 156–176.
Okagaki, L., & Bingham, G. E. (2010). Diversity in fami- Phinney, J. S. (1996). When we talk about American eth-
lies: Parental socialization and children’s development nic groups, what do we mean? American Psychologist,
and learning. In S. L. Christenson & A. L. Reschly 51, 918–927.
(Eds.), Handbook of school-family partnerships (pp. Pigott, R. L., & Cowen, E. L. (2000). Teacher race, child
80–100). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis race, racial congruence, and teacher ratings of chil-
Group. dren’s school adjustment. Journal of School
Okagaki, L., & Frensch, P. A. (1998). Parenting and chil- Psychology, 38(2), 177–196.
dren’s school achievement: A multi-ethnic perspec- Rampey, B. D., Dion, G. S., & Donahue, P. L. (2009).
tive. American Educational Research Journal, 35(1), NAEP 2008 trends in academic progress (NCES
123–144. 2009–479). Washington, DC: National Center for
Okagaki, L., Frensch, P. A., & Dodson, N. E. (1996). Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences,
Mexican-American children’s perceptions of self and U.S. Department of Education.
school achievement. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Ream, R. K., & Rumberger, R. W. (2008). Student engage-
Sciences, 18, 469–484. ment, peer social capital, and school dropout among
Okagaki, L., Helling, M. K., & Bingham, G. E. (2009). Mexican American and non-Latino White students.
American Indian college students’ ethnic identity and Sociology of Education, 81, 109–139.
beliefs about education. Journal of College Student Rivas-Drake, D., Hughes, D., & Way, N. (2008). A closer
Development, 50(2), 157–176. look at peer discrimination, ethnic identity, and psy-
Okagaki, L., Izarraraz, L., & Bojczyk, K. (2003, April). chological well-being among urban Chinese American
Latino adolescents’ ethnic beliefs, orientation to sixth graders. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37,
school and emotional well-being. Presented at the 12–21.
biennial meetings of the Society for Research in Child Roberts, R., Phinney, J., Masse, L., Chen, Y., Roberts, C.,
Development, Tampa, FL. & Romero, A. (1999). The structure of ethnic identity
Okagaki, L., & Moore, D. K. (2000). Ethnic identity in young adolescents from diverse ethnocultural
beliefs of young adults and their parents in families of groups. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 301–322.
Mexican descent. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Roeser, R., Strobel, K. R., & Quihuis, G. (2002). Studying
Sciences, 22(2), 139–162. early adolescents’ academic motivation, social-
Okagaki, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Parental beliefs emotional functioning, and engagement in learning:
and children’s early school performance. Child Variable- and person-centered approaches. Anxiety,
Development, 64(1), 36–56. Stress, and Coping, 15, 345–368.
Osborne, J. W. (1997). Race and academic disidentifica- Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive
tion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(4), development in social context. New York: Oxford
728–735. Press.
Oyserman, D., Brickman, D., Bybee, D., & Celious, A. Ryan, A. M. (2000). Peer groups as a context for the
(2006). Fitting in matters: Markers of in-group belong- socialization of adolescents’ motivation, engagement
ing and academic outcomes. Psychological Science, and achievement in school. Educational Psychologist,
17, 854–861. 35, 101–111.
Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., Terry, K., & Hart-Johnson, T. Sanders, M. G. (1997). Overcoming obstacles: Academic
(2004). Possible selves as roadmaps. Journal of achievement as a response to racism and discrimina-
Research in Personality, 38, 130–149. tion. Journal of Negro Education, 66, 83–93.
94 G.E. Bingham and L. Okagaki

Sanders, S. (1987). Cultural conflicts: An important factor Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes
in academic failures of American Indian students. shape intellectual identity and performance. American
Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
15(2), 81–90. Steinberg, L., Dornbusch, S. M., & Brown, B. B. (1992).
Schneider, B., & Lee, Y. (1990). A model for academic Ethnic differences in adolescent achievement: An eco-
success: The school and home environment of East logical perspective. American Psychologist, 47(6),
Asian students. Anthropology and Education 723–729.
Quarterly, 21, 358–377. Strambler, M. J., & Weinstein, R. S. (2010). Psychological
Shaunessy, E., & McHatton, P. A. (2009). Urban students’ disengagement in elementary school among ethnic
perceptions of teachers: Views of students in general, minority students. Journal of Applied Developmental
special, and honors education. Urban Review, 41, Psychology, 31, 155–165.
486–503. Suarez-Orozco, M. M. (1993). “Becoming somebody”:
Shernoff, D. J., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Schneider, B., & Central American immigrants in U.S. inner-city
Shernoff, E. S. (2003). Student engagement in high schools. In E. Jacob & C. Jordan (Eds.), Minority edu-
school classrooms from the perspective of flow theory. cation: Anthropological perspectives (pp. 129–143).
School Psychology Quarterly, 18, 158–176. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Shernoff, D. J., & Schmidt, J. A. (2008). Further evi- Sue, S., & Okazaki, S. (1990). Asian-American educa-
dence of an engagement-achievement paradox among tional achievement: A phenomenon in search of an
U.S. high school students. Journal of Youth and explanation. American Psychologist, 45(8),
Adolescence, 37, 564–580. 913–920.
Shin, R., Daly, B., & Vera, E. (2007). The relationships of Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories:
peer norms, ethnic identity, and peer support to school Studies in social psychology. Cambridge, UK:
engagement in urban youth. Professional School Cambridge University Press.
Counseling, 10, 379–388. Taylor, R. D., Casten, R., Flickinger, S. M., Roberts, D., &
Shrum, W., Cheek, N. H., & Hunter, S. M. (1988). Fulmore, C. D. (1994). Explaining the school perfor-
Friendship in school: Gender and racial homophily. mance of African-American adolescents. Journal of
Sociology of Education, 61, 227–239. Research on Adolescence, 4(1), 21–44.
Sinner, E., Kindermann, T. A., & Furrer, C. (2009). Taylor, R. D., & Lopez, E. I. (2005). Family management
A motivational perspective on engagement and disaf- practice, school achievement, and problem behavior in
fection: Conceptualization and assessment of chil- African American adolescents: Mediating processes.
dren’s behavioral and emotional participation in Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 39–49.
academic activities in the classroom. Educational Tenenbaum, H. R., & Ruck, M. D. (2007). Are teachers’
and Psychological Measurement, 69, 493–525. expectations different for racial minority than for
Sirin, S. R., & Rogers-Sirin, L. (2005). Components of European American students? A meta-analysis.
school engagement among African American adoles- Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 253–273.
cents. Applied Developmental Science, 9(1), 5–13. Tharp, R. G. (1989). Psychocultural variables and con-
Skinner, E., Marchand, G., Furrer, C., & Kindermann, T. stants: Effects on teaching and learning in schools.
(2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom: American Psychologist, 44(2), 349–359.
Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of Tolson, J. M., & Urberg, K. A. (1993). Similarity between
Educational Psychology, 100, 765–781. adolescent best friends. Journal of Adolescent
Smalls, C., White, R., Chavous, T., & Sellers, R. (2007). Research, 8, 274–288.
Racial ideological beliefs and racial discrimination Torres, V. (2004). The diversity among us: Puerto Ricans,
experiences as predictors of academic engagement Cuban Americans, Caribbean Americans, and Central
among African American adolescents. Journal of and South Americans. New Directions for Student
Black Psychology, 33(3), 299–330. Services, 105, 5–16.
Spencer, M. B., Noll, E., Stotzfus, J., & Harpalani, V. Trueba, H. T. (1987). Success or failure: Learning and the
(2001). Identity and school adjustment: Revisiting the language of minority students. Cambridge, MA:
“Acting White” assumption. Educational Psychologist, Newbury House.
36(1), 21–30. Tsang, S.-L. (1988). The mathematics achievement char-
Spina, S. U. (2000). Violence in schools: Expanding the acteristics of Asian-American students. In R. R.
dialogue. In S. U. Spina (Ed.), Smoke and mirrors: The Cocking & J. P. Mestre (Eds.), Linguistic and cultural
hidden context of violence in schools and society (pp. influences on learning mathematics (pp. 123–136).
1–39). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Starkey, B. S., & Eaton, S. (2008). The fear of “Acting Tyler, K. M., & Boelter, C. M. (2008). Linking Black
White” and the achievement gap: Is there really a middle school students’ perceptions of teachers’
relationship? Charles Hamilton Houston Institute for expectations to academic engagement and efficacy.
Race and Justice, Harvard Law School. Downloaded The Negro Educational Review, 59, 27–44.
on November 20, 2010, from http://www.charlesham- Tyler, K. M., Boelter, C. M., & Boykin, A. W. (2008).
iltonhouston.org/assets/documents/publications/ Linking teachers’ perceptions of educational value
BRIEF%20Acting%20White.pdf discontinuity to low-income middle school students’
4 Ethnicity and Student Engagement 95

academic engagement and self-efficacy. Middle by Elizabeth Greenberg, Reynaldo F. Macias, David
Grades Research Journal, 3(4), 1–20. Rhodes, and Tse Chan. Washington, DC.
Tyler, K. M., Boykin, A. W., Boelter, C. M., & Dillihunt, Vaquera, E. (2009). Friendship, educational engagement,
M. L. (2005). Examining mainstream and Afro- and school belonging: Comparing Hispanic and White
cultural value socialization in African American adolescents. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences,
households. Journal of Black Psychology, 31, 31, 492–514.
291–311. Varenne, H., & McDermott, R. (1998). Successful failure:
Tyler, K. M., Updah, A. L., Dillihunt, M. L., Beatty- The school America builds. Boulder, CO: Westview
Hazelbaker, R., Conner, T., Gadson, N., et al. (2008). Press.
Cultural discontinuity: Toward a quantitative investi- Way, N., Becker, B. E., & Greene, M. L. (2006).
gation of a major hypothesis in education. Educational Friendships among Black, Latino, and Asian American
Researcher, 7, 280–297. Adolescents in an Urban Context. In L. Balter & C. S.
Ueno, K. (2009). Same-race friendships and school attach- Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A hand-
ment: Demonstrating the interaction between personal book of contemporary issues (pp. 415–443). New
network and school composition. Sociological Forum, York: Psychology Press.
24(3), 515–537. Way, N., & Chen, L. (2000). Close and general friend-
Updegraff, K. A., Kim, J., Killoren, S. E., & Thayer, S. ships among African American, Latino, & Asian
M. (2009). Mexican American parents’ involve- American adolescents from low-income families.
ment in adolescents’ peer relationships: Exploring Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 274–301.
the role of culture and adolescents’ peer experi- Way, N., Santos, C., Niwa, E. Y., & Kim-Gervey, C.
ences. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, (2008). To be or not to be: An exploration of ethnic
65–83. identity development in context. In M. Azmitia, M.
Urdan, T., & Schoenfelder, E. (2006). Classroom effect on Syed, & K. Radmacher (Eds.), The intersection of per-
student motivation: Goal structures, social relation- sonal and social identities. New Directions for Child
ships, and competence beliefs. Journal of School and Adolescent Development, 120, 61–79.
Psychology, 44, 331–349. Weisner, T. S., Gallimore, R., & Jordan, C. (1988).
U.S. Department of Commerce, Census Bureau. (2001). Unpackaging cultural effects on classroom learning:
The Hispanic population in the United States: Native Hawaiian peer assistance and child-generated
Population characteristics by Melissa Therrien and activity. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 19,
Roberto R. Ramirez. U.S. Department of Commerce, 327–353.
Economics and Statistics Administration. Washington, Wentzel, K. (1997). Student motivation in middle school:
DC: U.S. Census Bureau. The role of perceived pedagogical caring. Journal of
U.S. Department of Commerce, U.S. Census Bureau. Educational Psychology, 89, 411–419.
(1993). We the American…Hispanics. Washington, Witherspoon, K. M. C., Speight, S. L., & Thomas, A. J.
DC: U.S. Department of Commerce, Economics (1997). Racial identity attitudes, school achievement,
and Statistics Administration, Bureau of the and academic self-efficacy among African American
Census. high school students. Journal of Black Psychology, 23,
U.S. Department of Education. (2007). The condition of 344–357.
education 2007 (NCES 2007–064). U.S. Department Wong, M. G. (1990). The education of white, Chinese,
of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Filipino, and Japanese students: A look at “High
Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S. School and Beyond”. Sociological Perspectives, 33(3),
Government Printing Office. 353–374.
U.S. Department of Education. (2009). The condition of Wong, C. A., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. (2003). The influ-
education 2009 (NCES 2009–081). U.S. Department ence of ethnic discrimination and ethnic identification
of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National on African American adolescents’ school and
Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S. socioemotinal adjustment. Journal of Personality, 71,
Government Printing Office. 1197–1232.
U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Yates, A. (1987). Current status and future directions of
Education Statistics. (2001a). English literacy and lan- research on the American Indian child. The American
guage minorities in the United States, NCES 2001–464, Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1135–1142.
Student Engagement: What Is It?
Why Does It Matter? 5
Jeremy D. Finn and Kayla S. Zimmer

Abstract
This chapter considers the relationships of student engagement with
academic achievement, graduating from high school, and entering post-
secondary schooling. Older and newer models of engagement are described
and critiqued, and four common components are identified. Research on
the relationship of each component with academic outcomes is reviewed.
The main themes are that engagement is essential for learning, that engage-
ment is multifaceted with behavioral and psychological components, that
engagement and disengagement are developmental and occur over a period
of years, and that student engagement can be modified through school
policies and practices to improve the prognoses of students at risk. The
chapter concludes with a 13-year longitudinal study that shows the rela-
tionships of academic achievement, behavioral and affective engagement,
and dropping out of high school.

Student Engagement: What is it? Why


does it matter?

This chapter considers the relationships of stu-


dent engagement with academic achievement,
graduating from high school, and entering post-
J.D. Finn (*)
secondary schooling. The concept of engagement
Graduate School of Education, has emerged as a way to understand—and
The State University of New York at Buffalo, improve—outcomes for students whose perfor-
Buffalo, NY 14260, USA mance is marginal or poor. The idea that engage-
e-mail: finn@buffalo.edu
ment behaviors can be manipulated to enhance
K.S. Zimmer educational performance promises significant
Department of Elementary Education,
St. Bonaventure University, St. Bonaventure,
payoff for students at risk of school failure.
NY 14778-9800, USA In this chapter, early and more recent models
e-mail: kzimmer@sbu.edu of engagement are described together with the

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 97


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_5, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
98 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

components of each model. Also, early and more are all highly related to academic outcomes.
recent research showing the relationship of these Educational risk factors (“events”) are educa-
components to academic achievement and attain- tional outcomes at one age/grade that interfere
ment is summarized. A first look at new longitu- with later academic achievement and educational
dinal data on student engagement in grades 4 and attainment. Low grades and test performance in
8, academic achievement, and high school gradu- the early grades, in-grade retention, and student
ation is described, showing the longitudinal misbehavior are associated with more severe
nature of students’ school engagement and disen- problems in later grades including school failure
gagement. The chapter concludes by summariz- and dropping out (see Rumberger & Lim, 2008).
ing the reasons to focus on engagement (and Mild forms of school misbehavior in early grades
disengagement) when addressing problems of can even escalate to acts of delinquency in later
low achievement and dropping out. Different years (Broidy et al., 2003; Loeber & Stouthamer-
terms are used for engagement in this chapter; Loeber, 1998). Dropping out is an educational
both student engagement and school engagement risk factor—an outcome of earlier school experi-
are used to connote students’ engagement in ences that becomes an obstacle to further
school. schooling.
Like medical risk factors, status and educa-
tional risk factors cluster, that is, multiple factors
tend to occur in the same individuals (Berenson,
Engagement and Risk 1986; Finn, 1989). The correlations among sta-
tus risk factors are well documented, and aca-
The recent emphasis on student engagement has demic risk factors tend to cluster because
evolved along with our understanding of what it academic problems in one grade make success in
means for students to be at risk. The ideas of risk the following grades more difficult (Alexander,
and risk factors derive largely from medicine. Entwisle, & Kabbani, 2001; Rumberger, 2001).
The Centers for Disease Control (CDC) defined For this reason, virtually every discussion of
health risk factors as “events, conditions, and dropping out or delinquency refers to the inter-
behaviors in the life of any individual modify the dependency with low academic performance,
probability of occurrence of death and disease for early behavior problems, and gender, race, and
that individual when compared to others …in the SES. The picture presented by status and aca-
[same] general population” (Breslow et al., 1985, demic risk factors gives educators little reason to
p. I-1). As an illustration, risk factors for car- expect that prognoses for at-risk students can be
diovascular disease (CVD) that cannot be improved.
altered—“conditions”—have been identified in Research focusing on behavioral risk factors
epidemiological studies; these include variables (the “behavior” component of the CDC defini-
such as gender, ethnicity, family history, and tion) addresses the question “what do some stu-
aging. Others risk factors are health outcomes at dents at risk due to status or educational risk
one point in time—“events”—but become precur- factors feel and do to be academically success-
sors of CVD at later points in time, for example, ful?” The attitudes and behaviors that answer this
obesity, hypertension, and hypercholesterolemia. question have been termed school engagement,
The parallel to educational risk is clear. that is, “the attention…investment, and effort
Research has identified a number of factors students expend in the work of school” (Marks,
associated with educational failure and dropping 2000, p. 155). Engagement behaviors include the
out. Status risk factors (“conditions”) are sociodemo- everyday tasks needed for learning, for example,
graphic characteristics that are difficult or impos- attending school and classes, following teachers’
sible to alter through school-based interventions. directions, completing in-class and out-of-class
Family socioeconomic status (SES), race/ethnicity, assignments, and holding positive attitudes about
whether or not English is spoken in the home, particular subject areas and about school in
family structure, and early pregnancy/parenthood general. Because of its direct relationship with
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 99

academic performance and inverse relationship a difficult class problem to graduating from
with negative outcomes, school engagement has high school to entering and completing post-
been viewed as a protective factor with respect to secondary studies.
educational risk (Finn & Rock, 1997; Resnick • Engagement behaviors are responsive to
et al., 1997; Steinberg & Avenevoli, 1998). teachers’ and schools’ practices, allowing
Disengaged students are those who do not par- for the possibility of improving achievement
ticipate actively in class and school activities, do and attainment for students experiencing
not become cognitively involved in learning, do not difficulties along the way. (See section
fully develop or maintain a sense of school belong- “Responsiveness to the school and class-
ing, and/or exhibit inappropriate or counterproduc- room context” in this chapter).
tive behavior. All of these risk behaviors reduce the
likelihood of school success. Disengaged students
may have entered school without adequate cogni-
tive or social skills, find it difficult to learn basic Early and Newer Models
engagement behaviors, and fail to develop positive of Engagement
attitudes that perpetuate their participation in class,
or they may have entered school with marginal or Student engagement (and disengagement) was
positive habits that become attenuated due to unad- conceptualized in the 1980s as a way to under-
dressed academic difficulties, dysfunctional inter- stand and reduce student boredom, alienation,
actions with teachers or administrators, or strong and dropping out. Educators argued that the
ties to other disengaged students. These students school setting mediates student involvement and
may begin what Rumberger (1987) has described engagement which are, in turn, necessary for
as a gradual process of disengagement often lead- learning (Newmann, 1981; Newmann, Wehlage,
ing to dropping out (see also Wehlage, Rutter, & Lamborn, 1992; Wehlage et al., 1989).
Smith, Lesko, & Fernandez, 1989). Engagement was defined as “the student’s psy-
chological investment in and effort directed
toward learning, understanding, or mastering the
Why Does Engagement Matter? knowledge, skills, or crafts that academic work is
intended to promote” (Newmann, 1992, p. 12).
The engagement/disengagement perspective One set of models emphasized the role of
is helpful to educators searching for strategies to school context. Newmann (1981) argued that
reduce the likelihood of school failure; for these only major school reform could reduce student
reasons: alienation and increase engagement. Six guiding
• Engagement behaviors are easily understood principles were identified as promising: reforms
by practitioners as being essential to learning. that encourage voluntary choice on the part of
Further, the relationship between engagement students and student participation in policy deci-
behavior and academic performance is con- sions, maintain clear and consistent educational
firmed repeatedly by empirical research. goals, keep school sizes small, encourage coop-
• Engagement behaviors can be seen in parallel erative student–staff relationships, and provide
forms in early and later years. As a result, an authentic curriculum. The need for school
dropping out of school can be understood as reform was echoed by Wehlage et al. (1989) who
an endpoint of a process of withdrawal that analyzed dropout prevention programs reputed to
may have had its beginnings in the elementary be effective, concluding that developing a strong
or middle grades. Students at risk of school sense of community with which students could
failure or dropping out can be identified ear- identify is of paramount importance. As a result
lier rather than later. of the analysis, a “theory of dropout prevention”
• Remaining engaged—persistence—is itself an was forwarded that asserted: (a) social–cultural
important outcome of schooling. Forms of conditions and student problems and impediments
persistence range from continuing to work on affect two aspects of student behavior, educational
100 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

engagement and school membership, and (b) these taught), and engaging in academic extracurricular
in turn affect students’ educational achievements. activities. Participation also included the social
Other models emphasize intrapersonal dynam- tasks of school, for example, attending classes
ics. A “self-system process model” was proposed and school, following classroom rules, interact-
based on the assumption that humans have basic ing positively and appropriate with teachers and
needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness peers, and not disrupting the class. The four types
(Connell, 1990; Connell & Wellborn, 1991). Self- of behavior were originally combined under one
system processes, that is, appraisals of the self in umbrella (participation) but have been viewed as
relation to ongoing activity, are generated as a distinct in more recent work.
means to evaluate whether these basic needs are The affective component (identification)
being met. If not, internal adjustments regarding referred to students’ “feelings of being a signifi-
the needs may be made. These processes are cant member of the school community, having a
assumed to develop within an individual through- sense of inclusion in school…” as well as the
out the lifespan and to be affected by cultural “recognition of school as both a social institution
context and interactions with others. and a tool for facilitating personal development”
The action that results from self-system pro- (Voelkl, 1997, p. 296). The first of these has been
cesses may take positive or negative forms, in referred to as “belonging,” “school membership,”
particular, engagement or disaffection; these, in “bonding,” “school connectedness,” and “attach-
turn, are followed by the development of skills, ment” by other researchers. The second was
social behavior, and adjustment (Connell & termed “valuing.”
Wellborn, 1991; Skinner, Kindermann, Connell, The participation-identification model (Fig. 5.1)
& Wellborn, 2009). The self-system model asserts described a cycle that begins with early forms of
that schools that support competence, autonomy, student behavior (participation), leading over
and relatedness have higher levels of student time to bonding with school (identification) and,
engagement and academic success (Connell, in turn, to continued participation. The cycle has
Spencer, & Aber, 1994). Empirical studies have been described as follows.
documented these relationships in diverse sam- Ideally, a child begins school as a willing
ples of elementary and secondary school children participant. He or she is
(Connell et al., 1994; Klem & Connell, 2004; …drawn to participate initially by encouragement
Patrick, Skinner, & Connell, 1993). from home and by classroom activities. Over time,
participation continues as long as the individual
has the minimal ability needed to perform required
tasks and as long as instruction is clear and appro-
Participation-Identification Model priate. There must be a reasonable probability that
the student will experience some degree of aca-
A third model had features of both the contextual demic success. As the student progresses through
the grades and autonomy increases, participation
and intrapersonal views. The participation-iden- and success may be experienced in a variety of
tification model (Finn, 1989) explained how ways, both within and outside the classroom. These
behavior and affect interact to impact the likeli- experiences encourage a student’s sense of identi-
hood of academic success. The behavioral com- fication with school and continuing participation.
(Finn, 1989, p. 129)
ponent (participation) referred to the behaviors
students engage in that involve them in the activi- According to the model, behavior in the early
ties of the classroom and school. These include grades is considered an important ingredient of
basic learning behaviors (e.g., paying attention to school success. The classroom and school con-
the teacher, responding to teacher’s questions, text need to be conducive to students’ develop-
completing assignments), initiative-taking behav- ing a sense of school identification; positive
iors (e.g., engaging in help-seeking activities, rewards for achievement are especially important.
doing more than the minimally required work, Less-than-successful experiences are inevitable
suggesting new ways to look at material being for all children, but the self-sustaining nature of
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 101

Quality of
Abilities
Instruction

Participation in School Identification with


Activities School
Successful
• Respond to requirements Performance • Belonging
• Class related Initiative Outcomes • Valuing
• Extracurricular activities

Fig. 5.1 Participation-identification model

the participation-identification cycle serves a The ideas of participation and identification


protective function that enables students to navi- were not as new to educators so much as the way
gate those situations. they were assembled into a developmental cycle.
On the negative side, The relationship between participation and aca-
…Students lacking the necessary encouragement demic achievement has been studied for decades.
at home may arrive at school predisposed to non- Attendance is a well-established factor in aca-
participation and nonidentification. While excep- demic performance (deJung & Duckworth, 1986;
tional teachers may engage the interest of some of
these students, others may resist participation,
Weitzman et al., 1985). Inattentive and disruptive
becoming distracted, bored, or restless, avoiding behavior were identified by psychologist George
the teachers’ attention or failing to respond appro- Spivack and his colleagues as having strong cor-
priately to questions. In later years, minimal com- relations with achievement test scores among
pliance or total noncompliance with course
requirements may persist. Students may refuse to
students in grades 1 through 6 (r’s from 0.15 to
participate in class discussions, turn in assignments 0.74; Swift & Spivack, 1968) and in grades 7
late, or arrive late or unprepared for class. As aca- through 12 (r’s from 0.26 to 0.32; Swift &
demic requirements become more demanding, this Spivack, 1969). The study of “time on task” or
behavior can result in marginal or failing course
grades. These students do not have the encourage-
“engaged time”—the period of time during which
ment to continue participating provided by positive a student is actively engaged in a learning activ-
outcomes. If the pattern is allowed to continue, ity—produced a number of studies of the connec-
identification with school becomes increasingly tions between classroom behavior and learning
unlikely. (Finn, 1989, p. 130)
(Anderson, 1975; Berliner, 1990; Fisher et al.,
This sequence of events can lead to disengage- 1980). As an example, Anderson (1975) rated
ment and dropping out, but other avenues can students in seventh through ninth grade as being
also lead to these outcomes. Some students make “on task” or “off task” at regular time intervals
reasoned decisions that time off (“stopping out”) and calculated the percentage of intervals that
work or family care is preferable at this point in each student was on task. This measure yielded
life. Others may begin school as full participants correlations between r = 0.59 and r = 0.66 with
but encounter obstacles (e.g., disciplinary mea- performance in particular math units. Follow-up
sures) that cause them to withdraw. Nevertheless, studies also assessed the context, events, and
“without a consistent pattern of participation and instructional mode at each time interval in order
the reinforcement provided by success experi- to understand the factors that promote participa-
ences, the emotional ingredient needed to maintain tion (Anderson & Scott, 1978).
a student’s involvement or to overcome the occa- Later research continued to find a strong rela-
sional adversity is lacking” (p. 130). tionship of participation with academic achievement.
102 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

This comes as little surprise given the obvious academic achievement (see “Affective engage-
importance of behavioral engagement for learning ment” in this chapter).
class material. One investigation correlated
teacher reports of “effort,” “initiative-taking,”
“negative behavior,” and “inattentive behavior” Newer Models
with achievement tests in over 1,000 fourth
graders (Finn, Pannozzo, & Voelkl, 1995). Corre- Other models of engagement have been for-
lations with end-of-year achievement scores warded in recent years with three, four, or more
were all significant at p < .001 and in the expected components (e.g., Appleton, Christenson, Kim,
directions; r’s ranged from 0.18 to 0.59. & Reschly, 2006; Darr, Ferral, & Stephanou,
Affective engagement has also been studied 2008; Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004;
for some time. For example, sociologists hypoth- Jimerson, Campos, & Greif, 2003; Libbey, 2004;
esized that identification with conventional insti- Luckner, Englund, Coffey, & Nuno, 2006;
tutions, including school and the workplace, Rumberger & Lim, 2008). Although different
serves to inhibit misbehavior (Hirschi, 1969; terminology makes comparison difficult, four
Kanungo, 1979; Liska & Reed, 1985). Affective dimensions appear repeatedly. Three correspond
engagement in this work was termed attachment, to the behavior component of the participation-
involvement, or bonding, and the obverse was identification model, and one corresponds to the
termed social isolation or alienation. Research in affective component.
school settings demonstrated that feelings of • Academic engagement refers to behaviors
alienation are related to delinquency and drop- related directly to the learning process, for
ping out and weakly related to academic perfor- example, attentiveness and completing assign-
mance (Elliott & Voss, 1974; Hindelang, 1973; ments in class and at home or augmenting
Hirschi, 1969). In the Hirschi and Hindelang learning through academic extracurricular
studies, large samples of middle- and high school activities. Certain minimal “threshold” levels
students were administered questionnaires that of academic engagement are essential for
included indicators of attachment/alienation and learning to occur.
a measure of delinquent behavior called • Social engagement refers to the extent to
“recency.” In both studies, the percentage of which a student follows written and unwritten
high-attachment students who were low on classroom rules of behavior, for example,
recency was substantially greater than the per- coming to school and class on time, interact-
centage of low-attachment students who were ing appropriately with teachers and peers, and
low on recency (e.g., 68% compared to 33% and not exhibiting antisocial behaviors such as
64% compared to 34% for two attachment indi- withdrawing from participation in learning
cators in the Hirschi study). This was interpreted activities or disrupting the work of other stu-
as showing that school attachment inhibits nega- dents. While a high degree of social engage-
tive behavior. ment may facilitate greater learning, a low
In the Elliott and Voss (1974) study, over 2,600 degree of social engagement usually interferes
high school students responded to questionnaires with learning, that is, it serves to moderate the
that yielded measures of normlessness and school connection between academic engagement
isolation. The correlations of normlessness with and achievement.
delinquency ranged from r = 0.59 to r = 0.63 and • Cognitive engagement is the expenditure of
with dropping out from r = 0.30 to r = 0.32; thoughtful energy needed to comprehend com-
the correlations of school isolation with delin- plex ideas in order to go beyond the minimal
quency ranged from r = 0.27 to r = 0.34 and with requirements.1 Behaviors indicative of cognitive
dropping out from r = 0.20 to r = 0.26 (all signifi- engagement include: asking questions for the
cant at p < .01). More recent research indicates
that affective engagement is related directly to
student behavior and persistence and indirectly to 1
Adapted from Fredericks et al. (2004, p. 60).
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 103

clarification of concepts, persisting with difficult the components directly (see Table 5.1). For
tasks, reading more than the material assigned, example, a self-report instrument for assessing
reviewing material learned previously, studying affective engagement might include questions
sources of information beyond those required, about feelings of belonging (e.g., “I feel con-
and using self-regulation and other cognitive nected to my school”) plus other questions about
strategies to guide learning. High levels of cog- relationships with teachers and classmates. An
nitive engagement facilitate students’ learning of assessment of cognitive engagement might
complex material. include students’ actual recall of the processes a
• Affective engagement is a level of emotional student used to solve a problem plus other behav-
response characterized by feelings of involve- iors that suggest cognitive engagement (e.g.,
ment in school as a place and a set of activities “Student uses a dictionary or the Internet on his/
worth pursuing. Affective engagement pro- her own to seek information.” Student does more
vides the incentive for students to participate than just the assigned work). These two types of
behaviorally and to persist in school endeav- engagement—cognitive and affective—often
ors. Affectively engaged students feel included require indirect measures because of the diffi-
in the school community and that school is a culty of assessing internal states directly
significant part of their own lives (belonging), (Appleton et al., 2006).
and recognize that school provides tools for Table 5.1 is intended to define and delimit the
out-of-school accomplishments (valuing). components of engagement, but is not intended
The components are summarized in Table 5.1. as an invitation to list every variable correlated
The first three indicate dynamism, or pull or, to with engagement. Some scales that purport to
use Marks’s (2000) term, “investment.” Affective measure engagement include antecedents or
engagement provides motivation for the invest- consequences of engagement that lie outside the
ment of energy the others require. The four com- limits of the concept. We agree with Fredricks
ponents may be exhibited by a student to different and colleagues (2004) that students’ perceptions
extents so it is difficult to label students as of their own abilities, parental support, peer
“engaged” or “disengaged.” But the components acceptance, teacher expectations, and other dif-
tend to be highly intercorrelated so that some ficult-to-change contextual factors should be
students are highly engaged, and others disen- considered as antecedents. Academic accom-
gaged, on multiple dimensions. This is likely to plishments and graduating or dropping out are
have a profound effect on their achievement and consequences. Even theory-based and well-
persistence. thought-out scales obfuscate this distinction.
There is a fine line between academic and One set of instruments includes items about
cognitive engagement. Academic engagement students’ perceptions of their peers, mobility,
refers to observable behaviors exhibited when a retention in grade, parental support, academic
student participates in class work; this was called performance, and drug and alcohol use, incorpo-
“participation” in the participation-identification rating both antecedents and outcomes in their
model (Finn, 1989). Cognitive engagement is an definition of engagement (Luckner et al., 2006).
internal investment of cognitive energy, roughly Others include questions about the fairness of
speaking, the thought processes needed to attain school rules, the appropriateness of the tests
more than a minimal understanding of the course given, parental support, feelings of safety in
content. school, the extent to which school facilitates
student autonomy, and the extent to which teachers
Measurement Issues like and support the student (Appleton et al.,
The measures used to assess student engagement 2006; Darr, 2009; Luckner et al., 2006). These
usually include indicators of engagement or dis- may all be antecedents of engagement, but none
engagement in addition to questions that address meets our criteria for engagement itself.
104

Table 5.1 Components of engagement and their indicators


Engagement component: definition Primary function Direct evidence Other indicators (examples)
Academic (behavioral): observable Threshold level essential for learning Observed or self-reported student Not required
behaviors related directly to the attentiveness, completing in-class and
learning process homework assignments, time on task,
academic extracurricular participation
Social (behavioral): the extent to which Moderates the connection between Observed or self-reported attendance, Not required
a student follows written and unwritten academic engagement and achievement social and antisocial behaviors,
classroom rules of behavior inattentive or disruptive behavior,
speaking out of turn, refusing to follow
directions
Cognitive (behavioral internal): the Facilitates learning of complex or “Think alouds,” where students Observed or self-reports of persistence,
expenditure of thoughtful energy challenging material verbalize their cognitive processes self-regulation. Questioning of content or
needed to comprehend complex ideas in during activity. Students reporting use “going beyond the minimum” (e.g., using
order to go beyond the minimal of cognitive strategies while solving dictionary or Internet to gain further
requirements problems or watching a recording of information). Voluntary after-school
their learning activity. Stimulated recall interaction with teacher
of cognitive processes
Affective: emotional response character- Provides the incentive for students to Self-reported valuing of school, Self-reports of positive reciprocal
ized by feelings of involvement in participate behaviorally and to persist feelings of acceptance and/or belonging relationships with teachers and classmates
school as a place and a set of activities in school endeavors
worth pursuing
J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 105

Clear definitions are also made difficult by terns and external motivators and gradually
attempts to sweep other terms under one umbrella. becoming internalized; the focus is on daily expe-
Liking for school, boredom, and anxiety are just riences and interactions with others.
that—liking for school, boredom, and anxiety Affective engagement is usually viewed
(cf. Fredricks et al., 2004); no constructive pur- more narrowly than is motivation or academic
pose is served by calling them engagement. Yet motivation. According to engagement models, it
some research and several reviews have included serves as a driving force for a specific set of
these and a plethora of other variables under the school-related behaviors and interacts with
engagement umbrella (Jimerson et al., 2003; those behaviors throughout the school years
Libbey, 2004). The issue of definition needs fur- (Appleton et al., 2006; Finn, 1989, 1993;
ther attention. Engagement models can be used to Fredricks et al., 2004; Furrer & Skinner, 2003;
bolster student performance only to the extent Newmann et al., 1992).
that the components—and engagement itself— The research summarized in this chapter
are well defined and easy for practitioners to shows that affective engagement is more highly
understand. related to behavioral forms of engagement than
to academic achievement (see review that fol-
Motivation and Engagement lows). Because of its connection with behaviors
The concepts of academic motivation and engage- conducive to learning, it may be more for help-
ment appear to have much in common, some- ful for understanding and enhancing educational
times leading to confusion. Indeed, the National outcomes than the broader concept of
Research Council book Engaging Schools (2004) motivation.
used the terms simultaneously throughout
(including a section title “Practices Enhancing
Motivation and Engagement”) (p. 172), without
discussing similarities or differences. Academic Responsiveness to the School
motivation, a form germane to educational per- and Classroom Context
formance, has been defined as “a general desire
or disposition to succeed in academic work and According to the developmental models of
in the more specific tasks of school” (Newmann engagement of Connell (1990) and Finn (1989),
et al., 1992, p. 13). Affective engagement—but many factors impact school engagement includ-
not academic, social, or cognitive engagement— ing the school context and the attitudes and
is also an internal state that provides the impetus behaviors of peers, parents, teachers, and other
to participate in certain academic behaviors. significant adults. It is outside the purview of this
According to both motivational and engagement chapter to review the antecedents of engagement.
models, the actual behaviors are shaped by the However, it is a basic tenet of the concept that it
context in which they occur. is responsive to the school and classroom prac-
Differences between the constructs are largely tices. Research listed here has identified aspects
a matter of focus. Theories of motivation (e.g., of classroom environment (the quality of student-
Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Maslow, 1970; teacher relationships, instructional approaches)
McClelland, 1985) attribute its source to inner and the school environment (school size, safety,
drives to meet underlying psychological needs, rules, and disciplinary practices) found to be
for example, the needs for competence, auton- important. Each is described in turn.
omy, and relatedness in the self-system model of • Substantial research has linked engagement to
Connell and Wellborn. Theories of engagement teacher warmth and supportiveness (Bergin &
(e.g., Finn, 1989; Hirschi, 1969; Newmann, 1981; Bergin, 2009; Fredricks et al., 2004; Furrer &
Voelkl, 1997) describe the development of affective Skinner, 2003; Hughes, Luo, Kwok, & Lloyd,
engagement as starting with early behavior pat- 2008; Marks, 2000; Skinner & Belmont, 1993;
106 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

Voelkl, 1995). In this research, teacher warmth • Perceptions of an unsafe environment and
is a collection of attributes including liking negative school sanctions can lead to student
and being interested in their students, believ- disengagement. Surveys have indicated that
ing in their capabilities, and listening to their teachers in up to one fourth of American
points of view. Supportive teachers show schools and students across the board per-
respect for each student as an individual, hold ceived that rules were unclear, too severe, or
clear and consistent expectations for student enforced unevenly (AFT in American
behavior, and provide academic assistance for Educator, 2008; Voelkl & Willert, 2006;
students who need it, including those who Wehlage & Rutter, 1986). Other studies have
have been absent for any reason. shown that student engagement was lower
• Instructional approaches that require student- when students felt unsafe or victimized
student interactions (e.g., cooperative learning), (Marks, 2000; Ripski & Gregory, 2009).
encourage discussion, or support the expres- Discipline policies perceived as too harsh are
sion of students’ viewpoints (e.g., use of dia- related to social forms of disengagement and
logue) have been found to facilitate student dropping out (Hyman & Perone, 1998;
engagement (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000; McNeely, Nonnemaker, & Blum, 2002), while
Johnson, Johnson, Buckman, & Richards, unfairness or apparent unfairness with which
1985; Osterman, 2000; Ryan & Patrick, 2001; rules are enforced is related to behavioral and
Wang & Holcombe, 2010). Strategies that affective disengagement (Ma, 2003; Marks,
promote in-depth inquiry and metacognition 2000; Ripski & Gregory, 2009). Fair treatment
have both been found to be related to increased by school staff has been described as funda-
student engagement. These include authentic mental to the development of identification
instruction in which students use inquiry to with school (Newmann et al., 1992).
construct meaning with value beyond the class- Several interventions to increase engagement
room (Newmann, 1992; Rotgans & Schmidt, have been tried and found to be effective. For
2011) and cognitive strategy use (Greene & example, First Things First (Connell & Klem,
Miller, 1996; Guthrie & Davis, 2003). 2006) is a school-wide program that attempts to
• Organizational features of the school includ- increase engagement at all grade levels by
ing school size are related to student engage- improving instruction and relationships between
ment. Early studies of high school size found teachers and students. The Child Development
that smaller schools were associated with Project (Battistich, Watson, Solomon, Schaps,
increased student participation, satisfaction & Solomon, 1991) attempts to create close-
and attendance, and social participation as a knit communities in classrooms and schools,
young adult (Lindsay, 1982, 1984). Since that thereby promoting several forms of student
time, a plethora of studies has confirmed the engagement. Both programs have been evalu-
small school—high engagement connection ated and shown to have positive results. (See
(Cotton, 1996; Lee & Smith, 1993, 1995; Voelkl, 2012).
National Research Council and Institute of
Medicine, 2004). Research on small learning
communities (SLCs) shows that small-school
dynamics can be produced even when school Engagement and Achievement/
buildings have large enrollments (US Attainment
Department of Education, 2001). This work
has found positive impacts of SLCs on vari- Recent years have produced many studies of the
ous forms of student engagement (e.g., relationships between engagement and educa-
Darling-Hammond, Ancess, & Ort, 2002; tional outcomes. In this section, we summarize
Kemple & Snipes, 2000). research conducted from the 1990s to the present
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 107

in three categories: (1) Research showing the room had strong, negative effects on reading
importance of engagement/disengagement to achievement and low reading achievement scores
learning when both are observed simultaneously. led to increased inattentiveness. Reciprocal
This research demonstrates that behavioral risk is effects were also found in a longitudinal study of
a major factor in producing academic risk. (2) low-achieving first- through third-grade students
Research that examined the relationship between (Hughes et al., 2008). These results offer partial
engagement/disengagement in earlier grades and support for the developmental cycle postulated
academic achievement and attainment in later by Finn (1989).
years. This research shows that, without interven- Some studies combined ratings of attentive-
tion, behavioral risk and academic risk grow in ness with other forms of classroom engagement.
tandem through the grades. (3) Research showing Across all age groups, and regardless of the
that school engagement can overcome the obsta- approach taken, substantial correlations are found
cles presented by status and academic risk factors, with students’ academic performance. For exam-
that is, engagement can protect students from ple, in a study of 1,013 fourth graders (Finn et al.,
harm that may accrue. 1995), teachers rated the students on the Student
Participation Questionnaire (SPQ) (see Appendix
for complete questionnaire). The 28-item instru-
ment questionnaire yields multi-item scale scores
The Importance of Engagement for “effort,” “initiative-taking,” “disruptive behav-
to Learning ior,” and “inattentive behavior” (Finn, Folger, &
Cox, 1991). The effort scale included items such
Academic Engagement as “student pays attention,” “student completes
assigned seatwork,” and “student is persistent
Students across grade levels who exhibit academic when confronted with difficult problems”; inat-
engagement behaviors, such as paying attention, tentive behavior included items such as “student
completing homework and coming to class pre- is withdrawn/uncommunicative,” “student does
pared, and participating in academic curricular not seem to know what is going on in class,” and
activities, achieve at higher levels than their less “student loses, forgets, or misplaces materials.”
academically engaged peers. These behaviors are Scale reliabilities ranged from 0.89 to 0.94.
especially important for students who face obsta- In this study, the correlations of effort and ini-
cles due to status risk factors such as coming tiative with achievement tests at the end of the
from a low-income home or having a first lan- school year, controlling for race, gender, and
guage other than English. classrooms, ranged from r = 0.40 to r = 0.59; cor-
Studies of inattentiveness continue to find relations of inattentive behavior with achieve-
strong correlations between students’ achieve- ment ranged from r = −0.52 to r = −0.34. All
ment and their ability to ignore distractions, per- correlations were significant at p < .001.2 Further,
severe on tasks, and act purposefully. A classic students classified as high on inattentiveness had
study of academic engagement (Rowe & Rowe, test scores that were substantially lower than
1992) examined the attentiveness and achieve- those of nonproblematic and disruptive students.
ment of over 5,000 children aged 5–14. Data Student- and teacher-reported engagement
were grouped by age (5–6, 7–8, 9–11, and was correlated with classroom grades in a study
12–14 years old), but regardless of age group or of third- through sixth-grade students (grades
other risk factors including SES and gender, sig- averaged across subject areas) (Furrer & Skinner,
nificant negative correlations were found between 2003). The engagement measure included ratings
lack of attention and reading achievement scores of effort, attention, and persistence. While both
(r’s from −0.87 to −0.48). The effects were fur-
ther shown to be reciprocal: path coefficients 2
Correlations for the other scales are discussed under
showed that inattentive behaviors in the class- Cognitive Engagement and Social Engagement.
108 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

correlations were significant, the correlation was significant correlations with teacher-assigned
higher for teacher reports of academic engagement grades among elementary students in second and
(r = 0.57) than for student self-reported academic fourth grades (n = 214, r = 0.23) and among mid-
engagement (r = 0.33). dle- and high school students (n = 424, r = 0.26).
Engagement-achievement connections have Other correlations were nonsignificant, including
been examined in the upper grades with some those between homework and standardized test
inconsistent findings. In a study of 586 ethnically scores in upper-grade students, and homework
and socioeconomically diverse tenth and 12th with attitudes toward homework (like/interest)
graders, students’ self-reports yielded a total and beliefs about homework (helps me learn)
score comprised of concentration (engagement) among elementary students. The effects of home-
and interest and enjoyment (not engagement); the work on academic achievement need further
reliability of the total scale was a = 0.64 (Shernoff study to understand the types of homework that
& Schmidt, 2008). The total was a significant but may be most useful and the impact of teachers’
modest predictor of students’ GPAs for the entire grading or not grading homework.
sample (b = 0.11). When the data were disaggre-
gated by race/ethnicity, the total was significantly Extracurricular Activities
but negatively related to GPAs among Black stu- In general, research on extracurricular activities
dents (b = −0.42). No further analysis or explana- has produced mixed results with respect to aca-
tion for the negative relationship was reported. demic achievement (Feldman & Matjasko, 2005).
Two studies used data from nationwide sam- However, when the nature of the activities is con-
ples of students, one based on eighth grade stu- sidered, a more consistent pattern emerges.
dents who participated in the National Educational Participation in academically oriented extracur-
Longitudinal Study of 1988 (NELS:88) (Finn, ricular activities, a form of academic engagement,
1993) and one based on tenth grade students who is significantly related to academic achievement.
participated in the Educational Longitudinal In contrast, the relationship between athletics and
Study of 2002 (ELS:2002) (Ripski & Gregory, achievement is generally nonsignificant, and cor-
2009). In the latter study, a measure of behavioral relations are significant but smaller when ath-
engagement was constructed from teacher ratings letic and academic activities are combined.
of students on five behaviors from the Student Studies that focus on academic extracurricular
Participation Questionnaire; the reliability of the activities are few and far between. A 7-year lon-
scale was a =. 76. Significant positive correlations gitudinal study of 1,259 Michigan school chil-
were found between engagement and reading and dren included measures of involvement in a
mathematics test scores (r = 0.36 and r = 0.39, limited set of academic activities, 4-year high
respectively). The data were not disaggregated by school GPAs, and enrollment in a full-time col-
race/ethnicity. These results were consistent with lege program (Eccles & Barber, 1999). Although
those from Finn, which reported strong positive the measures were limited, the regression coeffi-
relationships between engagement and achieve- cients for the two outcomes were small but statis-
ment tests in reading, mathematics, history, and tically significant at p < .01 (b = 0.11 for GPA,
science for all students combined. b = 0.13 for full-time college), with statistical
controls for gender, socioeconomic status, and
Homework student ability.
Academic engagement in the form of homework One of the most in-depth analyses used
completion was examined in relationship to aca- NELS:88 data for eighth- and tenth-grade girls
demic performance in two studies (Cooper, (Chambers & Schreiber, 2004). In this study, in-
Jackson, Nye, & Lindsay, 2001; Cooper, school academic extracurricular activities (ISAO)
Valentine, Nye, & Lindsay, 1999). The amount of were disaggregated from other forms. The all-girl
homework completed had small but statistically sample may not have been a severe limitation
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 109

because girls are significantly overrepresented in Sabo, 1992) have generally found nonsignificant
academic activities (Eccles & Barber, 1999). associations for most students studied. However,
Participation in ISAO was the total number of some significant relationships were found in spe-
academic activities, out of 16, in which a student cific subgroups. For example, Melnick and Sabo
participated. This was entered into multilevel used High School and Beyond (HS&B) to study
regressions controlled for socioeconomic status the relationships of athletic participation with
and other forms of school activity. ISAO had grades and graduation/dropping out among
significant positive impacts on academic achieve- African American and Hispanic male and female
ment (p < .001) in all four subject areas at both students from three urbanicities. When significant
grade levels when all students were considered interactions were discovered with urbanicity, 12
together. When the data were disaggregated by separate regressions were run for each dependent
race/ethnicity, the associations between ISAO variable. Weak but significant relationships
and academic achievement were nonsignificant between athletic participation and grades were
for African American and Latina eighth-grade found among suburban African American males
girls. With only one exception, all relationships (b = 0.20) and rural Hispanic females (b = 0.10).
for tenth-grade girls were positive and significant Athletics and graduation were weakly but signifi-
regardless of race/ethnicity or subject. This study cantly associated among rural Black males
provided evidence that academic extracurricular (b = 0.23), rural Hispanic females (b = 0.17), and
activities have a weaker relationship with achieve- suburban Hispanic males (b = 0.14). From the
ment in eighth grade than in tenth grade. In tenth small number and spottiness of the significant
grade, there is often a larger set of choices, and results, the authors concluded that “athletic par-
students tend to self-select either academic or ticipation has very little academic impact on
nonacademic extracurricular activities. minority youth” (p. 302).
When academic and nonacademic extracurric- In contrast, Chambers and Schreiber’s (2004)
ular activities were studied together, small-to- study of eighth- and tenth-grade girls revealed a
moderate but statistically significant correlations significant negative relationship between sports
with academic achievement were found. For participation and reading achievement; racial
example, in a separate study using the eighth- ethnic groups were not disaggregated in this
grade data from NELS:88, all extracurricular study. Despite the inconsistent findings, research-
activities considered together had weak but sig- ers have argued that sports may be one of the few
nificant correlations with achievement in mathe- remaining forms of engagement for students at
matics, reading, and science (Gerber, 1996). risk of total disengagement (Finn, 1989; Pittman,
Again, race/ethnicity was an important factor: 1991; Yin & Moore, 2004). This hypothesis is
White students had higher correlations of extra- best tested through a closer look at individual stu-
curricular activities to achievement (r’s from 0.16 dents, perhaps in a qualitative study.
to 0.23) than did their African American peers (r’s
from 0.07 to 0.13). Other research has produced
similar results for students in grades 6 through 12 Social Engagement
(Cooper et al., 1999) and for students in grades 10
and 12 (Marsh, 1992; Marsh & Kleitman, 2002). The written and unwritten rules of behavior, when
The latter also found significant small-to-moderate violated, often reduce academic performance.
effects of high school extracurricular participation Most research on classroom social behavior is
on university enrollment (r = 0.27) and months framed in the negative, that is, one or another form
spent in a university (r = 0.30). of misbehavior. In this section, we focus on atten-
Qualitative and quantitative studies of the dance and common forms of indiscipline, for
relationship of athletic activities with achieve- example, disrupting the class, failure to partici-
ment and high school graduation (Booker, pate in class discussions, refusing to follow direc-
2004; Chambers & Schreiber, 2004; Melnick & tions, disrespectful behavior, and fighting.
110 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

Attendance Classroom Social (and Antisocial) Behavior


It comes as little surprise that school attendance Researchers have given little attention to the
is highly related to academic achievement; time antecedents and consequences of “ordinary”
lost from exposure to teachers and teaching can classroom misbehaviors except for those attribut-
only reduce the opportunity for learning. In a able to child psychopathology. This is despite the
study of all Ohio public schools, Roby (2004) facts that most students misbehave one time or
found strong significant correlations between another and that certain classroom and school
attendance and achievement in grades 4, 6, 9, and conditions may actually promote misbehavior.
12 (r’s from 0.54 to 0.78). The 18 urban schools Ordinary misbehavior (e.g., speaking out of turn,
with the highest all-tests-passed rates on the Ohio leaving one’s seat during class, refusing to follow
test of Proficiency at fourth grade had higher directions, being late to class or school, talking
average attendance (95.6%) than the attendance back to the teacher, using an electronic device)
average at the 18 urban schools with the lowest interferes with teaching and learning and can
pass rates (89.6%), a highly statistically signifi- potentially interrupt all students’ engagement in
cant difference. The author estimated that a the classroom.
school of 400 students with a 93% attendance In a unique study of social engagement, sixth-
rate, the average for Ohio, lost 25,200 h of stu- and seventh-grade students were asked to nomi-
dent instructional time per year. nate classmates who exhibited two prosocial
The association is also strong at the student behaviors (e.g., shares, cooperates) and three
level. For example, African American freshmen’s antisocial behaviors (e.g., breaks rules) (Wentzel,
absenteeism was significantly and negatively 1993). Two composite scores were obtained for
correlated to GPAs (r = −0.64) in an urban high- each student by combining the ratings in such a
risk high school (Steward, Steward, Blair, Hanik, way as to make them comparable; these were
& Hill, 2008). While noting that absences from also validated by comparing them to teacher rat-
school in general are negatively correlated to ings of the same students. Correlation and regres-
achievement, Gottfried (2009) differentiated sion analysis showed significant relationships of
between excused and unexcused absences in an both scores with grades and standardized
investigation of second through fourth graders. achievement tests (correlations from r = 0.35 to
The large-scale study of students in Philadelphia r = 0.55) even when gender, ethnicity, absentee-
found that, as students trended toward more ism, student IQ, family structure, and teacher
unexcused than excused absences, their grades preference for the students were included in the
on SAT 9 reading and math standardized tests equations.
declined. Students with 100% of their absences In the Finn et al. (1995) study of fourth graders
excused performed higher on the reading test (above), the disruptive scale was comprised of
than students with 100% unexcused absences four items: the student “acts restless, is often
regardless of the total number of absences. unable to sit still,” “needs to be reprimanded,”
However, even excused absences began to neg- “annoys or interferes with peers’ work,” and “talks
atively affect achievement when students with classmates too much.” The scale had correla-
reached 20 absences per year. While the author’s tions from r = −0.29 to r = −0.18 with norm-
approach was informative, the children in the referenced and criterion-referenced achievement
study were approximately 7–9 years old and, tests when race, gender, and teachers were con-
presumably, did not make their own decisions trolled statistically; all were significant at p < .001.
about attending school. The author speculated The decrement in achievement scores for students
that high unexcused absences were indicative of who were high on the disruptive behavior scale
negative family environments. The issue is suf- was statistically significant but not as large as the
ficiently provocative that we believe the study decrement due to being high on the inattentive
should have delved into the actual reasons for scale. Antisocial behavior of eighth graders,
these absences. defined similarly, was also found to be correlated
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 111

significantly with mathematics and reading test and cognitive tool use (e.g., “Student goes to
scores, with and without statistical control for dictionary, encyclopedia, or other reference on
demographic characteristics (see “A study of his/her own to seek information”). Self-report
behavioral and affective engagement in school instruments include the Metacognitive Awareness
and dropping out” in this chapter). of Reading Strategies Inventory (Mokhtari &
Reichard, 2002) with items such as “I decide
what to read closely and what to ignore” and “I
Cognitive Engagement take notes while reading to help me understand
what I read.”
Studies of cognitive engagement and achieve- In a pivotal study of students’ cognitions,
ment have yielded mixed results, in part due to Peterson and colleagues (1984) used three
different methods of assessing internal pro- approaches in collecting information on cogni-
cesses. Direct assessments are accomplished by tive engagement of fifth-grade students: stimu-
asking students to report the processes they use lated recall, videotapes of student behavior, and
to learn course material, and indirect assess- student questionnaires. In terms of on-task behav-
ments use indicators that can be reported in ior, the researchers found that teacher observa-
paper-and-pencil form or observed by teachers. tions were less highly correlated with student
A direct approach was proposed by Benjamin achievement (r = 0.10) than were stimulated
Bloom: stimulated recall (Bloom & Broder, recall measures (r’s from 0.21 to 0.33) or the
1958) is a method through which events are attending subscale of the cognitive processing
recorded and then played back to students at a questionnaire (r = 0.48). The analysis of cogni-
time shortly after the events occurred. During tive functioning led the authors to conclude that
playback, the recordings are paused at critical “students with higher levels of attention were not
moments, such as when a problem is posed or merely listening passively; rather, they were
solved, and participants are asked to retell their more actively processing the material than stu-
thoughts or conscious actions. Stimulated recall dents with lower attention” (p. 504).
was used later to gather data on cognitive Studies of self-regulation and use of cognitive
engagement during reading and math lessons strategies in elementary and higher grades yield
(Juliebo, Malicky, & Norman, 1998; Peterson, significant results for some measures and not for
Swing, Stark, & Waas, 1984). To reduce bias others. In a study of 42 kindergarten and second-
due to the delay between the events and the time grade students, teacher-rated failure to self-
of recall, “think alouds” were developed in regulate was not associated with lower reading
which verbal reports are given concurrently with scores in kindergarten but became a significant
the cognitive event (Afflerbach & Johnston, influence (r’s from 0.37 to 0.51) on reading
1984). Think alouds, however, require cognitive achievement in second grade (Howse, Lange,
effort that may detract from learning the mate- Farran, & Boyles, 2003). Data collection in the
rial itself. study also included teacher ratings of cognitive
Indirect methods of assessment rely on engagement indicators and a direct measure
observable indicators that a high level of cogni- based on a computerized self-regulation task that
tive engagement has occurred, for example, stu- required that the child continue to work at a job
dents’ initiative-taking, undertaking more on one part of the screen while distracters were
difficult assignments, discussing class material presented (SRTC-AV; Kuhl & Kraska, 1993).
with the teacher after school. The Student The SRTC-AV by itself did not correlate signifi-
Participation Questionnaire (Finn et al., 1991) cantly with achievement scores for any group of
includes teacher ratings of student initiative- students in the study.
taking (e.g., “Student attempts to do his/her work Likewise, a combination of assessments was
thoroughly and well, rather than just trying to get by”) used to access cognitive engagement during reading
112 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

by 492 ethnically diverse fourth graders (Wigfield Affective Engagement


et al., 2008). Included in this study were three
measures that reflect cognitive engagement. First, Like cognitive engagement, affective engage-
teachers rated students on a short questionnaire ment is often assessed through external indica-
that included three academic engagement ques- tors rather than the internal states they reflect. In
tions and one about the use of cognitive strate- the case of affect, this leads to a wide range of
gies. Also, a question-writing task involved measures including some that seem remote from
students reviewing information in a science the definition of the construct. Unlike all other
packet and then writing as many “good ques- forms of engagement, however, the preponderance
tions” as possible on the topic. Questions were of research suggests that affective engagement is
graded with a rubric that considered both number related indirectly to academic achievement
of questions generated and complexity of the (See Voelkl, 2012). It appears instead to affect
questions written. Both variables had moderate- other forms of engagement (academic, social,
to-high significant correlations with scores on the cognitive) which, in turn, affect learning
Gates MacGinitie Test of Reading Comprehension: (Osterman, 2000).
the teacher report (r = 0.57) and the question- The relationships of feelings of belonging and
writing task (r = 0.74). valuing with academic achievement, motivation,
In high school, English students’ use of deep and academic and social engagement in grades 6
cognitive strategies (e. g., putting ideas in one’s through 8 were examined in studies by Goodenow
own words and self-regulation of what is and is (1993a, 1993b) and Voelkl (1997). In these stud-
not understood) was significantly correlated with ies, affective engagement was assessed through
classroom grades (r = 0.33) (Greene, Miller, student self-report measures. Generally, small or
Crowson, Duke, & Akey, 2004), as were seventh- inconsistent positive correlations were found
and eighth-graders’ general strategy use in with grades and standardized achievement tests.
English (r = 0.14) (Wolters & Pintrich, 1998). In the Voelkl study, identification with school
Cognitive strategies were also correlated to math was more strongly correlated with student par-
achievement (r = 0.11) and social studies (r = 0.22) ticipation than with achievement. A large-scale
When the middle school students reported the study of students in grades 7 through 12 used
use of regulatory strategies such as planning and data from the National Longitudinal Study of
monitoring, significant and moderate correlations Adolescent Health (ADD Health) (McNeely
between self-regulation and achievement were et al., 2002). The data included a measure of
found (r = 0.23 to r = 0.30). Self-regulation school connectedness together with a number of
appeared to have a somewhat greater effect on student and school characteristics. Although
achievement then does general strategy use. grade point average was significantly related to
Although we reviewed a limited number of student connectedness, the strongest predictor of
studies, the use of self-regulation and cognitive school connectedness of all individual character-
strategies was correlated with academic achieve- istics was skipping school (behavioral engage-
ment in all but the youngest (kindergarten) stu- ment). In a mixed-method study of 61 African
dents. Both direct and indirect measures of American high school students, Booker (2004,
cognitive engagement were notable in their rela- 2007) also found little to link a sense of belonging
tionships to achievement among students in to achievement. Participants’ self-reports of
fourth and higher grades. It is possible that mea- school belonging on questionnaires counted for
sures of cognitive engagement cannot capture the little or no variation in their achievement. This
nuances of cognitive functioning among very was corroborated by interviews. One student,
young students, or, as suggested by some psy- when asked about the importance of sense of
chologists, the skills involved in cognitive community in their school replied: “How is my
engagement have not yet crystallized in 5- or achievement [related]? …don’t think it really
6-year-olds. matters about that [belongingness]…the majority
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 113

of people here are cool” (Booker, 2004, p. 138). behavioral engagement and indirectly to learning,
Ninety-two percent of all student comments although the research base is rather sparse. The
echoed this sentiment. valuing component of affective engagement is
On the other hand, affective engagement is distinct from general valuing of education. In an
associated with a range of psychological and analysis of different meanings of valuing,
behavioral outcomes (Maddox & Prinz, 2003; Mickelson (1990) found that “concrete” school
Osterman, 2000). Students who report high levels attitudes such as the belief that schooling pays off
of belonging or identification with school also with good jobs were associated with positive
display higher levels of motivation and effort than school outcomes for Black students. More
do students who report lower levels of belonging abstract attitudes were not, for example, the belief
or identification (Goodenow, 1993a, 1993b; that “If everyone gets a good education, we can
Goodenow & Grady, 1993). The correlation of end poverty” (p. 51).
scores on Goodenow’s Psychological Sense of Concrete attitudes, or “utility,” are a promi-
School Membership (PSSM) scale with expecta- nent part of Eccles’s expectancy-value model of
tions for school success in a sample of 301 urban student decision-making (see Wigfield & Eccles,
junior high school students was r = 0.42 (p < .001) 2000). Research based on the model has demon-
(Goodenow, 1993b). Differences in average strated consistently that utility is related to stu-
PSSM scores among high-, medium-, and low- dents’ choices and behavior. The perceived
effort teacher ratings in a sample of 454 suburban utility of school and particular courses may be
middle-school students were statistically signifi- important in sustaining students’ participation
cant at the .001 level; effect sizes between in school—sometimes despite frustration and
adjacent groups were both approximately 0.5s failure.
(estimated from results in the published report). Student perceptions of the present and future
Low levels of belonging or identification are value of literacy (reading and English) has an
associated with negative behaviors including increasing, although still modest, effect on stu-
academic cheating (Voelkl & Frone, 2004), dent achievement in the upper grades. In a study
school misbehavior and discipline measures of over 5,000 students in 92 schools, perceived
(Stewart, 2003), drug and alcohol use on school usefulness of reading had nonsignificant rela-
grounds (Hawkins, Catalano, & Miller, 1992; tionships with achievement among children
Voelkl & Frone, 2000), delinquent and antisocial 5–11 years of age (r’s from 0.00 to 0.09) but
behaviors (Maddox & Prinz, 2003), and high-risk became a weak but significant factor among stu-
health behaviors including suicidality, violence dents from 12 to 14 years of age (r = 0.11) (Rowe
(Resnick et al., 1997), and dropping out of school & Rowe, 1992). Although not compared to prior
(Jessor, Turbin, & Costa, 1998; Rumberger & years, sophomore, junior, and senior high school
Lim, 2008). A study of sixth- and seventh-grade students’ perceptions of the value of English for
students found that after controlling for family future goals had higher correlations with course
relations, effortful control, earlier conduct prob- grades (r = 0.25 for all students combined)
lems, and gender, school connectedness was neg- (Greene et al., 2004).
atively related to subsequent conduct problems These findings are consistent with the partici-
(Loukas, Roalson, & Herrera, 2010). The interac- pation-identification model (Finn, 1989), which
tions in the study also showed that connectedness proposes that identification with school (or dis-
offset the adversity presented by poor family identification) develops over time as a function
relations or effortful control, that is, connected- of behavioral engagement accompanied by aca-
ness served as a protective factor. demic success (or failure) experiences. The model
proposes further that the development of positive
Valuing feelings of school belonging and valuing helps
The belief that school provides the individual perpetuate productive behavioral engagement
with useful outcomes may also be related to and academic performance.
114 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

Summary habits of engagement formed at this early stage


were shown to have enduring effects on student
Many studies of engagement bundle the compo- attainment.
nents in various ways and some fail to provide The importance of attendance was underscored
information about the composition of their mea- in other research that included attendance with
sures. Nevertheless, the picture is clear: the measures of antisocial behavior, for example,
effects of behavioral engagement on educational studies of a large sample of sixth-grade students
accomplishments are consistently statistically in Philadelphia (Balfanz, Herzog, & Iver, 2007)
significant and moderate to strong—no matter and eighth-grade students in Houston (Kaplan,
what student populations are studied, control Peck, & Kaplan, 1995). In the Philadelphia study,
variables taken into account or, for the most part, four warning flags of school problems in sixth
the composition of the measures. The effect of grade were identified (absenteeism, suspensions
affective engagement on achievement is less for poor behavior, low math or reading scores).
consistent, but its relationships with behavioral Of these, attendance rates of 80% or less were the
engagement and high school graduation are most predictive of failure to graduate on time or
consistently positive. in the following year.
A 9-year longitudinal study followed ethni-
cally and socioeconomically diverse children
from kindergarten through eighth grade (Ladd &
Engagement Predicts Later Dinella, 2009). Students were identified as having
Achievement and Attainment either stable (high or low) or changing (increas-
ing or decreasing) levels of engagement. Students
Studies of engagement show that early patterns who exhibited stable but poor combined engage-
of behavior affect students’ performance in later ment behaviors (e.g., school avoidance, not fol-
grades. Most of these studies used large-scale lowing rules, defiance) from first through third
longitudinal data collected on urban populations, grade made less academic progress through
and assessed combinations of the four types of eighth grade than did those who had stable but
engagement. higher combined engagement. First graders with
Longitudinal studies have identified students equivalent achievement had markedly different
who are at risk of dropping out for reasons other trajectories if they were increasingly behavior-
than status risk factors. The study with the lon- ally engaged, as opposed to those who decreased
gest duration was a 14-year longitudinal study of in behavioral engagement, ultimately resulting in
790 Baltimore City school children that began in lower grades on achievement tests for decreas-
first grade (Alexander, Entwisle, & Horsey, ingly engaged eighth graders. Thus, students with
1997). Attendance and engagement behaviors either high stable engagement or increasing
(academic, prosocial, and antisocial behaviors) engagement had higher levels of achievement in
were assessed in first grade by examining school. eighth grade than their less-engaged peers.
As expected, early scholastic achievement and
status risk factors were predictive of dropping
out. In addition, students high on the engagement Beyond High School
scale were significantly more likely to graduate
than their less-engaged peers (odds ratio = 2.4). Postsecondary outcomes have been found to
Attendance, more than tardiness or antisocial be affected by engagement in elementary and
behaviors, was particularly important; first graders high school. Using national longitudinal data
who missed 16 days of school were 30% less (NELS:88) on students when they were in grades
likely to graduate than students who missed 8 through 12 and of college age, Finn (2006)
10 days or fewer. Alexander et al. concluded that examined three sets of predictors: demographic
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 115

characteristics (status risk variables), high important to high school graduation and partici-
school achievement and attainment (academic pation in postsecondary education. Academic
risk variables), and measures of school engagement and social engagement stand out as especially
(behavioral risk variables). Four composites were salient, although we could not locate any predic-
formed for each participant in high school reflect- tive studies of cognitive engagement and found
ing academic participation (extracurricular par- only one recent study that included affective
ticipation), social engagement (attendance, engagement
classroom behavior) and affective engagement
(students’ perceptions of the usefulness of school
subjects).
In regressions that controlled for status and Engagement Mediates the Effects
academic risk factors, attendance and classroom of Status and Academic Risk Factors
behavior were significantly related to all three
postsecondary variables studied: entering a post- Resilient students are those who can overcome
secondary program, the number of credits earned, the barriers posed by status or academic risk fac-
and completing a postsecondary program (odds tors to achieve acceptable outcomes. The study
ratios of 1.2–1.5). Participation in extracurricular of resilience is important to help identify the fac-
activities was related to entering a postsecondary tors that distinguish these individuals from their
institution (odds ratio of 1.4), but not to credits less successful peers in order to apply those prin-
earned or completion of program. The affective ciples to other students at risk. Research has
measure, perceived usefulness of school subjects, shown that school engagement in the early, mid-
was not related to any postsecondary outcome. dle, and upper grades all contribute to student
When employment and income were examined at resilience.
age 26, the results were weak or nonsignificant. Students who were considered at risk in
Only 2 out of 12 possible relationships were sig- grades 1 through 6 due to home factors (57%
nificant, those of high school attendance with poverty, 42% single parent households, school in
current employment and classroom behavior with a high-crime neighborhood) participated in an
consistency of employment. For the most part, evaluation of the Seattle Social Development
engagement in high school did not impact Project (n = 643) (Hawkins, Guo, Hill, Battin-
employment as a young adult. Pearson, & Abbott, 2001). The 18 participating
Research done in Chicago schools corrobo- schools were assigned to one of three conditions:
rated these findings (Ou, Mersky, Reynolds, & full intervention in grades 1 through 6 designed
Kohler, 2007) and extended the conclusions to specifically to increase student engagement, late
adult criminal behavior by age 24. A troublemak- intervention in grades 5 and 6 only, and a control
ing composite score (social engagement) in (no intervention). Each year from age 13 to age
grades 3 through 6 was a significant predictor of 18, teachers rated students on academic, cogni-
incarceration and conviction (odds ratios of 1.4 tive, and affective dimensions of engagement. At
and 1.5, respectively). Neither academic engage- age 13 and every subsequent age, the groups
ment nor attendance was significantly related to showed substantial differences with the order
the income or measures of criminal behavior full intervention group having the highest
(conviction or incarceration). engagement and the control group the lowest.
The groups diverged, and differences became
larger still in the period from 16 to 18 years.
Summary Further, the engaged-at-18 students had higher
GPAs, a lower history of arrests, fewer instances
The principle that continuing engagement of dropping out, and less cigarette, alcohol, and
throughout from the earliest grades onward is drug use than did the other groups.
116 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

Several studies explored engagement and class at the culmination of 12th grade; nonresil-
resilience during transitions from elementary to ient completers who did not meet the achieve-
middle or junior high school. A study of 62 ment criteria but were still in school in 12th
African American students from low-income grade; and nonresilient dropouts who were
homes noted a significant drop in GPAs between reported as having left without graduating. Seven
fifth (M = 2.25) and sixth grade (M = 2.05), but academic and social engagement measures were
affective engagement was shown to protect recorded for each student (three teacher-reported,
against this drop (Gutman & Midgley, 2000). four student-reported), plus sports and academic
After controlling for psychological characteris- extracurricular activities.
tics, home background, and prior achievement, a Even when the analysis controlled for demo-
high sense of school belonging combined with graphic factors, self-esteem, and locus of control,
high parental involvement was related to elevated resilient completers were significantly higher
sixth grade GPAs; the mean GPA in sixth grade than both groups of nonresilient students on five
for students with high affective engagement was out of six measures of social and academic
approximately 3.2. engagement, that is, lower rates of absenteeism,
A second study examined the adverse effects higher levels of classroom effort and homework,
of parent and teacher “role strains,” that is, pres- and fewer behavior problems. Differences were
sure placed on adolescents by parents’ and teach- large, with effect sizes for the significant vari-
ers’ expectations (de Bruyn, 2005). In a Dutch ables ranging from 0.47s to 0.84s. Only student
study of 749 students just entering secondary self-reports of being prepared for class and par-
school, role strain negatively impacted academic ticipation in sports and academic curricular activ-
achievement (r = −0.19 to r = −0.36). A measure ities did not relate to student resilience.
of academic engagement was shown to mediate
these effects; students high on the scale had
higher achievement despite the role strain they A Study of Behavioral and Affective
felt. In all, academic engagement increased the Engagement and Dropping Out3
prediction of academic achievement from
R2 = 0.09 to R2 = 0.36. Academic engagement and Little if any research has explored the develop-
achievement in the study were highly correlated ment of engagement and its relationship to
(r = .50). Both studies demonstrated the roles of achievement over time, and even less has exam-
home and school factors in bolstering student ined the connection between affective engage-
resilience across school transitions. ment and dropping out of school. This study,
A nationwide American study was based on a based on the participation-identification model
high-risk sample of eighth graders who partici- (Finn, 1989), was designed to investigate drop-
pated in the NELS:88 longitudinal survey. The ping out as a developmental process related to
sample comprised 1,803 African American and students’ behavioral and affective engagement in
Hispanic students who attended public schools grades 4 and 8. We used a unique data set in
and lived in homes in the lower half of the SES which achievement scores were recorded from
distribution, based on a composite of parents’ kindergarten through eighth grade, engagement
education, parents’ occupations, and household measures were obtained at several intervals, and
income (Finn & Rock, 1997). Students were clas- high school graduation was later recorded. The
sified into three groups based on academic per- three primary research questions were (1) Is
formance in eighth and tenth grade and dropout
status in 12th grade: a small group of resilient
3
completers (8.4%) with math and reading test A partial version of this report was presented to the
American Educational Research Association (Pannozzo,
scores at or above the 40th percentile for all stu-
Finn, & Boyd-Zaharias, 2004). The authors are grateful to
dents, self-reported GPA’s of “half B’s and half Gina Pannozzo for her excellent work and contributions
C’s” or better, and who would graduate with their to the execution of the study.
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 117

behavioral engagement (academic and social) in tests administered in the respective subject in
grades 4 and 8 related to graduation/dropping out spring of each school year.
of high school above and beyond the effects of Academic and social engagements were mea-
academic achievement during the same time sured through teacher ratings of individual stu-
period? (2) Is affective engagement in grade 8 dents on the Student Participation Questionnaire
related to graduation/dropping out? (3) Does (SPQ; see Appendix) (Finn et al., 1991). Fourth-
affective engagement explain graduation/drop- grade teachers completed a questionnaire in
ping out above and beyond the effects of behav- November for up to ten randomly chosen stu-
ioral engagement? The results presented here dents in her class. Eighth-grade reading and
represent a first look at this database. mathematics teachers completed a shortened ver-
sion of the questionnaire (14 of the same items),
yielding two ratings of each student. For this
Procedures study, two subscales were created from the SPQ,
one that measured academic engagement as
Participants defined in Table 5.1 (e.g., paying attention, par-
Participants in this study were a subset of stu- ticipating in class discussion, completing assign-
dents who participated in Tennessee’s Project ments) and one that measured social/antisocial
STAR, a longitudinal class-size reduction exper- engagement (e.g., needing to be reprimanded,
iment. Students entered the study in kindergarten acting restless, interfering with classmates’
or first grade and were followed through high work). In fourth grade, these scales had 16 and 7
school. To be included in this study, they were items, respectively; scale reliabilities were
required to have graduation/dropout information a = 0.95 and a = 0.85. The eighth-grade scales
from high school transcripts or State Department had 6 and 5 items, respectively; scale reliabilities
of Education records and to have been rated on were a = 0.89 and a = 0.81.
the grade-4 and/or grade-8 engagement instru- Identification with school was assessed with
ments. The final sample of 2,728 students was the Identification with School Questionnaire
similar to the full STAR sample of 11,600 stu- (Voelkl, 1996) given to students in grade 8. The
dents in all ways except the sample for this study questionnaire is comprised of 16 items that assess
had a higher percentage of White/Asian students students’ sense of belonging in and valuing of
(74.9% compared to 63.1%) and a higher per- school. Belonging items include “I feel proud of
centage of students not eligible for free lunches being a part of this school” and “The only time I
(55.3% compared to 44.0%). Free lunch and get attention in school is when I cause trouble.”
race/ethnicity served as control variables in all Valuing of school includes items such as “School
analyses. is one of the most important things in my life”
In each phase of the analysis, the sample and “I can get a good job even if my grades are
included students who had key variables in grade bad.” Confirmatory factor analysis of the scale
4 and/or grade 8. The fourth grade sample con- indicated that it is best scored as a single dimen-
sisted of 1,421 students from 123 schools and the sion (Voelkl, 2012). For this study, the reliability
eighth-grade sample had 2,191 students from 119 of the total scale was a = 0.84.
schools. There were 753 students with both
grade-4 and grade-8 data. Analysis
The three research questions were answered
Measures through a series of two-level multilevel logistic
Achievement score composites in reading and regression analyses using the HLM program
math were derived for each student in grades K (Raudenbush, Bryk, & Congdon, 2000) with
through 3 and in grades 6 through 8, respectively. graduate/dropout as the dependent variable. In all
Each composite was the first principal compo- analyses, student variables were centered around
nent of norm-referenced and criterion-referenced the school mean, and school variables were
118 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

Table 5.2 Variables used in HLM analysis for each research question
Level of data Variables Question (1) Question (2) Question (3)
Level-1 Dependent variable
(students) Graduate/dropout from high school X X X
Independent variables
Grade 4 academic engagement Xa
Grade 8 academic engagement Xb X
Grade 4 social engagement Xa
Grade 8 social engagement Xb X
Grade 8 affective engagementc X X
Gender X X X
Race ethnicity
White/Asian students–minority students X X X
Free-lunch eligibility X X X
Reading achievement composite Grades K-3 Xa
Reading achievement composite Grades 6–8 Xb X X
Mathematics achievement composite Grades K-3 Xa
Mathematics achievement composite Grades 6–8 Xb X X
Level-2 School urbanicity
(schools) Suburban/urban schools–inner-city schools X X X
Rural schools–inner-city schools X X X
School enrollment X X X
a
Used in grade-4 analysis only
b
Used in grade-8 analysis only
c
Identification with school

grand-mean-centered. All effects were treated as Tests of significance reveal whether a relation-
fixed except for the student and school intercepts, ship is statistically reliable, but tell little about
which were treated as random. A type-1 error rate whether effects are weak or strong. A strength-of-
(a) of .01 was used throughout. effect measure in logistic regression is the odds
Each analysis was conducted with two runs of ratio. If the independent variable is dichotomous
HLM. The first run included all the main effects (e.g., female/male), the odds ratio is the odds that
listed in Table 5.2 for the particular question. In a member of the first group (female) would grad-
the second run, specific interactions were added uate from high school divided by the odds that a
to the model: the interactions of each engagement member of the second group (male) would gradu-
scale in the respective analysis with gender and ate. Odds ratios much below 1.0 or much above
free-lunch eligibility (student-level interactions) 1.0 indicate strong effects; 1.0 would be obtained
and with school enrollment (student-by-school- if the odds for both groups were the same. Odds
level interactions). These interactions indicate below 1.0 are sometimes “inverted” to make
whether the effects of engagement on graduating/ them easier to understand. For example, if the
dropping out varied as a function of gender, fam- odds for the first group are one third as large as
ily income groups, or school enrollment. For the odds for the second group, the ratio would be
effects that involved more than a single indepen- 0.33, which is a bit awkward to think about. It is
dent variable (i.e., academic achievement, aca- simpler to say that the odds for the second group
demic and social engagement, urbanicity), a are three times that of the first group; this is 1.0 ÷
blockwise test was conducted to see if the pair of 0.33 = 3.0. If the independent variable is continu-
variables were jointly related to graduation/drop- ous (e.g., academic, social, or affective engage-
ping out before tests of the individual variables ment), the odds ratio is the change in odds
were conducted. associated with a one-standard deviation change
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 119

Table 5.3 Graduation rates of sample by demographic Is Behavioral Engagement in Grades 4 and 8
characteristics Related to Graduation/Dropping Out of High
Fourth grade Eighth grade School?
Variable (n = 1,421) (n = 2,141) In this study, we asked whether behavioral engage-
Gender
ment in fourth grade was related to graduation/
Male 82.6 81.8
dropping out. The analysis had statistical controls
Female 91.4 89.3
for other precursors of dropping out (race/ethnicity,
Race/ethnicity
White/Asian 89.5 87.4
SES, and academic achievement in prior grades).
Minority 78.0 80.9 The fourth-grade and eighth-grade analyses
Free lunch produced similar results for background charac-
Yes 78.7 76.2 teristics (Table 5.5). In general, students in subur-
No 93.8 92.8 ban/urban and rural schools were two to three
All 87.1 85.8 times more likely to graduate than were students
in inner-city schools (odds ratios from 2.1 to 3.2).
Neither the enrollment of students’ elementary
in the particular engagement measure. Odds schools nor their eighth-grade schools was sig-
ratios are presented together with significance nificantly related to high school dropout rates.
levels for each independent variable in the Data from both grades indicated that females
regressions. were more likely to graduate from high school
than were males (Table 5.3), but the difference
was only marginally significant in eighth grade.
Results Students not eligible for free or reduced lunches
were approximately three times as likely to grad-
The percentage of students who graduated from uate from high school as were students who were
high school was 87.1% in the fourth-grade sam- eligible (1 ÷ 0.33 and 1 ÷ 0.34 for fourth and
ple and 85.8% in the eighth-grade sample eighth grade, respectively). In eighth grade,
(Table 5.3). For both samples, graduation rates White students were less likely to graduate than
were higher for females than for males, for Asian/ were minority students (opposite the direction in
White students than for minority students, and Table 5.3). This was an artifact of the distribution
for students who were not eligible for free or of minority students among schools; many
reduced-price lunches. schools had one to three minority students with a
The correlations among the main variables of graduation rate of 100%.
the study (Table 5.4) are consistent. With the
exception of eighth-grade reading with identifica- Behavioral engagement and graduation/dropout.
tion, all correlations were significant at p < .01. In The correlations of academic and social engage-
both grades, academic and social engagement ment with graduation were small to moderate but
were moderately positively correlated with read- statistically significant (Table 5.4). The regres-
ing and mathematics, with stronger correlations sions revealed that, as a set, academic and social
for academic engagement than for social engage- engagement in fourth and eighth grades were sig-
ment (r’s from 0.44 to 0.54 for academic engage- nificantly related to high school completion
ment, r’s from 0.33 to 0.36 for social). Academic (Table 5.5). When the two forms of behavioral
and social engagement were moderately and posi- engagement were viewed separately, only aca-
tively correlated with high school graduation (r’s demic participation was statistically significant in
from 0.23 to 0.32). Identification with school in fourth grade. The odds ratio indicated that a one-
eighth grade had lower correlations with achieve- standard deviation increase in academic engage-
ment (r = 0.04 and r = 0.09) and with dropping out ment scale in fourth grade would double a
(r = 0.09) but larger correlations with academic student’s odds of graduating (odds ratio = 2.1).
and social engagement (r = 0.26 and r = 0.22). Social behavior did not add to the prediction of
120 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

Table 5.4 Correlations among academic, social, and affective engagement, achievement, and graduation
Academic Social Reading Mathematics Identification
Variable engagement engagement achievement achievement with schoola Graduationb
Academic engagement – 0.72** 0.54** 0.52** N/A 0.29**
Social engagement 0.71** – 0.36** 0.34** N/A 0.22**
Reading achievement 0.44** 0.33** – 0.78** N/A 0.21**
Mathematics achievement 0.50** 0.33** 0.79** – N/A 0.23**
Identification with schoola 0.26** 0.22** 0.04* 0.09** – N/A
Graduationb 0.31** 0.32** 0.27** 0.26** 0.09** –
Note: Correlations for fourth grade are presented above the diagonal, and correlations for eighth grade are presented
below the diagonal
*p < .05; **p < .01
a
Not assessed in fourth grade
b
1 = graduation, 0 = dropout

Table 5.5 Summary of multilevel logistic regression analysis for graduation/dropout with academic and social engagement
in grades 4 and 8
Grade 4 Grade 8
Predictor variable Beta Odds ratioa Beta Odds ratioa
School level
Enrollment .001* 1.00 4.2 × 10−4
Suburban/urban–inner city .719* 2.05 .964** 2.62
Rural–inner city 1.173*** 3.23 .770* 2.16
Student level
Behavioral engagementb £.001 £.001
Academic .053*** 2.05 .123*** 1.69
Social .002 .111** 1.34
Female–male .614** 1.85 .316* 1.37
White/Asian–minority −.159 −1.221*** 0.30
Free lunch (yes–no) −1.078*** 0.34 −1.112*** 0.33
Achievementb .114 £.001
Reading composite −.251 .227
Mathematics composite .352 1.37 .252
Student level interactions
Gender × engagementb .093 >.500
Academic .022 .032
Social −.104* −.061
Free-lunch × engagementb >.500 .348
Academic .007 .072
Social −.014 .101
Student × school interactions
Engagement × enrollmentb >.500 .348
Academic 2.5 × 10−4 3.4 × 10−4
Social −2.2 × 10−4 −1.1 × 10−4
Note: School- and student-level main effects tested first (not controlling for interactions). Interactions tested in separate
analyses, controlling for main effects
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001
a
Odds ratios for significant effects computed from main-effect analysis
b
Bolded values are p values for blockwise test of the pair of variables
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 121

Table 5.6 Summary of multilevel logistic regression analysis for graduation/dropout including identification with
school
Without behavioral engagement With behavioral engagement
Predictor variable Beta Odds ratioa Beta Odds ratioa
School level
Enrollment 4.4 × 10−4 4.2 × 10−4
Suburban/urban–inner city .950** 2.59 .969** 2.64
Rural–inner city .844** 2.33 .773** 2.17
Student level
Behavioral engagementb £.001
Academic .120*** 1.67
Social .109** 1.33
Identification with school .037** 1.26 .011
Female–male .555*** 1.74 .295
White/Asian–minority −1.151*** 0.32 −1.211*** 0.30
Free lunch (yes–no) −1.131*** 0.32 −1.121*** 0.33
Achievementb £.001 £.001
Reading composite .223* 1.26 .234
Mathematics composite .550*** 1.79 .247
Student level interactions
Gender × identification with school −.013 N/A
Free-lunch × identification with school −.021 N/A
Student × school interactions
Enrollment × identification −5.1 × 10−4 N/A
Note: School- and student-level main effects tested first (not controlling for interactions). Interactions tested in separate
analyses, controlling for main effects
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001
a
Odds ratios for significant effects computed from main-effect analysis
b
Bolded values are p values for blockwise test of the pair of variables

high school graduation at this point in students’ were all nonsignificant. That is, the impact of
schooling. academic and social participation on graduating/
Students’ academic and social behaviors in dropping out is approximately the same for males
eighth grade, considered independently and and females, higher and lower SES students, and
jointly, were significantly related to graduation. in smaller and larger schools.
The odds ratios for the two separate measures
were 1.69 and 1.34, respectively. That is, a one- Is Affective Engagement in Grade 8 Related to
standard deviation increase in academic engage- Graduation/Dropping Out?
ment increased the odds of graduating from high By the time the students reached eighth grade,
school by 69%; a one-standard deviation increase they had undergone many experiences that could
in social engagement increased the odds of grad- affect their chances of completing high school,
uating by 34%. These results were obtained even for example, transition from elementary grades to
after academic achievement, and individual stu- middle or junior high school, a series of academic
dent and school characteristics were controlled successes and/or failures, changes in school, and
statistically. Of the two, academic engagement changes in attitudes to school. All of these can
appeared consistently more important than social promote or discourage the development of identi-
engagement. fication with school.
The interactions of behavioral engagement The correlation between identification with
with school enrollment, gender, and free lunch school and graduation/dropping out in Table 5.4
122 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

was small but statistically significant (r = 0.09, standard deviation increase in social engagement
p < .01). The regression analysis (Table 5.6) by 33% (odds ratio = 1.33).
showed a statistically significant positive effect Can a similar conclusion be drawn for affec-
as well. Students who identified more positively tive engagement? When behavioral engagement
with school were more likely to graduate than was included in the model, the effect of identifica-
were students with lower levels of identification. tion with school became nonsignificant. Although
A one-standard deviation increase in identification affective engagement alone was correlated with
in eighth grade increased the odds of graduating whether or not students graduated or dropped out
by 26% (odds ratio = 1.26), above and beyond the of high school, it contributed less, if anything,
effects of academic achievement in grades 6 above and beyond observable academic and
through 8 and student and school characteristics. social behaviors. Consistently with research cited
Affective engagement appeared to be an impor- in this chapter, it appears that identification with
tant factor in sustaining a student’s persistence school affected academic achievement and attain-
through high school, although the effect was ment indirectly through its impact on students’
not as strong as that of behavioral engagement classroom behavior.
(Table 5.5).
None of the interactions of identification with
gender, free-lunch eligibility, and school enroll- Summary and Discussion
ment were statistically significant. The impact of
identification with school on the likelihood of The results of the study are summarized in
graduating was similar for male and female Table 5.7. Academic and social engagement in
students, students from higher- and lower-SES fourth and eighth grade contributed to students’
homes, and smaller and larger schools alike. decisions to remain in school and graduate or to
leave school early. Academic engagement pre-
Does Affective Engagement Explain dominated; its connection with high school grad-
Graduation/Dropping Out Above and Beyond uation is stronger than that of social participation.
Behavioral Engagement? These connections were robust, that is, they were
The measures of affective and behavioral engage- found to be significant when achievement levels
ment in eighth grade were significantly correlated and affective engagement in eighth grade were
with each other (r = 0.26 and r = 0.22). Of these, controlled statistically, and the absence of signifi-
behavioral engagement (academic and social) cant interactions with gender, SES, or school
had higher correlations with achievement and location indicates that it applies similarly to sub-
dropping out than did affective engagement. In a groups of students.
regression analysis of eighth-grade data, the Students who are academically and socially
blockwise test of both behavioral measures, and engaged in school are likely to have higher
of each individual measure, was virtually achievement and to receive positive responses
unchanged by the addition of identification with from teachers for their work and behavior. These
school to the model (right-hand portion of forms of reinforcement help students maintain
Table 5.6). That is, above and beyond identifica- habits of high engagement throughout the grades,
tion with school, and above and beyond actual leading to school completion. Students who are
school performance, the academic and social not engaged academically or who exhibit nega-
behaviors of eighth graders continued to contrib- tive social behaviors create academic risk: they
ute to high school graduation. A one-standard have lower achievement levels and are more
deviation increase in academic engagement likely to experience frustration and to receive
increased the odds of attaining a high school negative responses from teachers. Continued
diploma by 67% (odds ratio = 1.67) and a one- nonengagement, accompanied by low or failing
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 123

Table 5.7 Summary of regression analysis for predicting graduation/dropping out


Odds ratios and p values
Question/variable(s) Grade 4 Grade 8
Question 1: Is behavioral engagement in grades 4 and 8 related to graduation/
dropping out of high school?
Answer 1: Yes, in both grades
Behavioral engagement (academic and social) p < .001a p < .001a
Unique effect of academic engagement 2.1** 1.7**
Unique effect of social engagement NS 1.3*
Question 2: Is affective engagement in grade 8 related to graduation/dropping out?
Answer 2: Yes, weak association
Affective engagement (identification with school) – 1.3*
Question 3: Does affective engagement explain graduation/ dropping out above
and beyond behavioral engagement?
Answer 3: No
Behavioral engagement controlling for affective engagement – p < .001a
Unique effect of academic engagement – 1.7**
Unique effect of social engagement – 1.3*
Affective engagement controlling for behavioral engagement – NS
Note: Odds ratios only given for significant effects
NS not statistically significant
*p < .01; **p < .001
a
Tests of pairs of predictors (no odds ratios)

grades and negative responses from teachers, Implications: Student Engagement


increases the likelihood of dropping out. and Disengagement
Exactly how affective engagement and other
school-related attitudes influence achievement It is well supported by empirical research that
and persistence is not clear. The data of this study engagement is a precursor to academic achieve-
indicated that identification with school may pro- ment and attainment. Further, forms of engagement
mote academic and social engagement. However, are intuitive, observable, and easily understood
it had a weak correlation with dropping out when by teachers as being important to learning. The
considered by itself and did not contribute to impact of engagement is both direct (e.g., paying
dropping out above the impact of observable attention or completing assignments) and indi-
behavior. More research on the role of affective rect (e.g., antisocial behavior that disrupts
engagement is needed. instruction thus interfering with learning oppor-
Much remains to be done with the data. The tunities). The research reviewed in this chapter
same variables would benefit for being assembled shows that (a) engagement has a concurrent
into an inclusive structural equation model in impact on academic achievement. The connec-
which direct and indirect effects of the indepen- tion is likely to be reciprocal, that is, high
dent variables on dropping out and the effects of achievement is likely to promote continuing
the independent variables on one another could engagement and low achievement is likely to
be examined simultaneously. Other variables discourage further engagement; (b) engagement
could also be considered including characteris- in early and middle grades is predictive of
tics of the teachers and the schools. The analysis achievement and attainment in later grades,
is continuing. even up through the postsecondary years; and
124 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

(c) engagement behaviors and attitudes can help At this point in time, extensive research into
students overcome the obstacles presented by the antecedents or consequences of academic and
status and academic risk factors, including fac- social engagement is unlikely to produce much in
tors associated with behavior problems outside the way of new understandings. The research dis-
of school. cussed in this chapter and the other chapters in
Unfortunately, many students fail to become this book show that a very large knowledge base
fully engaged, and others begin to disengage at is already in place.
some point during their schooling. This can lead In contrast, three areas need further research
to academic problems, mild and severe forms of and development. First, research on cognitive
misbehavior, and attenuated school careers. engagement is disjointed and needs to be assem-
Status and academic risk factors are sometimes bled into a consistent explanatory framework.
used as explanations for these problems, for Most studies have been conducted in specific
example, students’ attitudes are poor (“blame the academic subjects, leaving questions about com-
student”), single parents, parents who do not monalities unanswered. For example, is cognitive
monitor their children’s behavior or who are not engagement subject specific or do students have
involved with school activities are at the root of general propensities to become cognitively
the problem (“blame the family”), and/or friends engaged (or not engaged) in all subjects? If so,
or street life are not conducive to staying in what is the nature of these propensities and how
school (“blame the community”). can they be assessed? How do students develop
The engagement/disengagement perspective the capacity to be cognitively engaged and how
acknowledges that behavioral risk is at least par- do they remain cognitively engaged outside a
tially situated in the school and classroom and specific setting? And finally, how is the learning
thus partially under our control. It assumes that that results from cognitive engagement different
engagement develops over a period of years—an from learning without the in-depth thinking it
assumption supported by empirical data pre- requires? A theoretical perspective that brings
sented in this chapter. This view has strong impli- diverse findings together into one broad frame-
cations for educators: efforts to prevent work would be informative.
disengagement should be targeted toward the Second, we have limited understanding of
elementary and middle grades as well as high how affective engagement develops. On one
school. Unlike the status- and academic-risk hand, research has explored the relationship of
explanations, attention is focused on behaviors academic achievement with specific forms of
that are wholly or partially manipulable and affect, for example, liking for school and
responsive to school and classroom practices. school subjects, liking the teacher(s), aca-
This perspective also emphasizes that engage- demic motivation, frustration, and boredom.
ment is multifaceted, although scholars have On the other hand, the theory and research
somewhat different views about what the compo- summarized in this chapter indicate that early
nents are. The four components presented in this transitory forms of affect evolve into more
chapter—academic, social, cognitive, and affec- stable forms in later grades. To our knowledge,
tive—are ingredients common to multiple defini- no study has examined this assertion in depth
tions; they avoid ingredients outside the concept or in its entirety.
of engagement, and they are conceptually dis- There is pressing need for research that
tinct. Each plays a different role in supporting (a) assesses various forms of affect experi-
academic outcomes, and each, if weak or lacking, enced in early grades to examine the relation-
contributes to academic or behavior problems or ships among them, (b) examines stability and
early school leaving. change in affect as students mature, and the
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 125

experiences that affect stability and change, observations of teacher-student interactions


and (c) examines the relationship between (with specific foci), and reactions from students
affect that is more transitory and affect that is themselves. It would help guide interventions to
more trait-like and generalizes across settings make classrooms and schools more conducive
within schools or between one school and the to student engagement.
next school a student attends. This research
would need to be longitudinal and incorporate
both quantitative and qualitative approaches. Appendix
The third area in need of further work involves
application more than theory: creating more Fourth Grade
complete ways to identify students at risk of non-
engagement or disengagement. The approach Student Participation Questionnaire
advocated most widely is to consider the charac-
teristics of students and their school experiences. The codes in parentheses indicate the subscale to which
the item belongs:
This is exemplified in the What Works
Subscale reliability
Clearinghouse’s dropout prevention guide E = Effort .94
(Dynarski et al., 2008). The first recommenda- I = Initiative .89
tion, of seven, is “Utilize data systems that… N = Nonparticipatory behavior .89
help identify individual students at high risk of V = Value .68
dropping out” (p. 10); the recommendation is The sign (+, −) indicates the direction of scoring. Items
accompanied by a list of student risk factors such marked “−” should be reverse-scored before summing
as academic problems, truancy, behavior prob- the items in the subscale.
(Items 29–31 are not part of these subscales).
lems, retentions, and academic and social
performances. Notes:
This approach does not give adequate atten- The items in this questionnaire have been com-
tion to the school context. In this chapter, we have bined in different ways for use in different research
identified four conditions of the school setting studies.
that promote engagement—teacher warmth and This questionnaire is in the public domain and
supportiveness, instructional approaches that may be used without permission. Notification to
encourage student participation, small school the author is requested.
size, and a safe environment with fair and effective The eighth-grade version of the questionnaire
disciplinary practices—and there are more. When is available from the author upon request.
these conditions are less than optimal, or lacking
altogether, the likelihood of student disengagement Fourth Grade
goes up. To date, there have been few if any attempts
to assess the classroom and school context in Student Participation Questionnaire
addition to student characteristics to identify the
threats to student engagement. A package of Student’s Name:_________________________
assessments for this purpose would involve _______________________________________
observations of students in the school setting,
126 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

Below are items that describe children’s behavior Thank you for your time. Please enclose the
in school. Please consider the behavior of the stu- teacher/class information sheet and all the ques-
dent named above over the last 2–3 months. Circle tionnaires—those completed and not complete—
the number that indicates how often the child in the envelope provided and return it to your
exhibits the behavior. Please answer every item. principal.

This student: Never Sometimes Always


(E+) 1. pays attention in class 1 2 3 4 5
(E+) 2. completes homework on time 1 2 3 4 5
(E+) 3. works well with other children 1 2 3 4 5
(E−) 4. loses, forgets, or misplaces materials 1 2 3 4 5
(E−) 5. comes late to class 1 2 3 4 5
(I+) 6. attempts to do his/her work thoroughly and well, 1 2 3 4 5
rather than just trying to get by
(N+) 7. acts restless, is often unable to sit still 1 2 3 4 5
(I+) 8. participates actively in discussions 1 2 3 4 5
(E+) 9. completes assigned seat work 1 2 3 4 5
(V+) 10. thinks that school is important 1 2 3 4 5
(N+) 11. needs to be reprimanded 1 2 3 4 5
(N+) 12. annoys or interferes with peers’ work 1 2 3 4 5
(E+) 13. is persistent when confronted with difficult problems 1 2 3 4 5
(E−) 14. does not seem to know what is going on in class 1 2 3 4 5
(I+) 15. does more than just the assigned work 1 2 3 4 5
(I−) 16. is withdrawn, uncommunicative 1 2 3 4 5
(E+) 17. approaches new assignments with sincere effort 1 2 3 4 5
(V−) 18. is critical of peers who do well in school 1 2 3 4 5
(I+) 19. asks questions to get more information 1 2 3 4 5
(N+) 20. talks with classmates too much 1 2 3 4 5
(E−) 21. does not take independent initiative, must be helped 1 2 3 4 5
to get started, and kept going on work
(E−) 22. prefers to do easy problems rather than hard ones 1 2 3 4 5
(V−) 23. criticizes the importance of the subject matter 1 2 3 4 5
(E+) 24. tries to finish assignments even when they are difficult 1 2 3 4 5
(I+) 25. raises his/her hand to answer a question or volunteer 1 2 3 4 5
information.
(I+) 26. goes to dictionary, encyclopedia, or other reference 1 2 3 4 5
on his/her own to seek information
(E−) 27. gets discouraged and stops trying when encounters 1 2 3 4 5
an obstacle in schoolwork, is easily frustrated
(I+) 28. engages teacher in conversation about subject matter 1 2 3 4 5
before or after school, or outside of class
29. attends other school activities such as athletic 1 2 3 4 5
contests, carnivals, and fund-raising events

30. The student’s overall academic performance is Above average Average Below average
1 2 3
31. Does this student attend special education classes No Yes
outside of your classroom?
1 2
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 127

Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human


References Services, Centers for Disease Control, Center for
Health Promotion and Education.
Afflerbach, P. P., & Johnston, P. (1984). Research method- Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E.,
ology: On the use of verbal reports in reading research. Brame, B., Dodge, K. A., et al. (2003). Developmental
Journal of Reading Behavior, 14, 307–322. trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and ado-
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Horsey, C. S. (1997). lescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study.
From first grade forward: Early foundations of high Developmental Psychology, 39, 222–245.
school dropout. Sociology of Education, 70, 87–107. Chambers, E. A., & Schreiber, J. B. (2004). Girls’ academic
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Kabbani, N. S. achievement: Varying associations of extracurricular
(2001). The dropout process in life course perspective: activities. Gender and Education, 16, 327–346.
Early risk factors at home and school. Teachers College Connell, J. P. (1990). Context, self and action: A motiva-
Record, 103, 760–822. tional analysis of self-system processes across the life
American Federation of Teachers. (2008). We asked, you span. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in
answered. American Educator, 32(2), 6–7. transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 61–97). Chicago:
Anderson, L. W. (1975). Student involvement in learning University of Chicago Press.
and school achievement. California Journal of Connell, J. P., & Klem, A. M. (2006). First things first:
Educational Research, 26(2), 53–62. What it takes to make a difference. In R. W. Smith
Anderson, L. W., & Scott, C. C. (1978). The relationship (Ed.), Time for change: New visions for high school
among teaching methods, student characteristics, and (pp. 15–40). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
student involvement in learning. Journal of Teacher Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994).
Education, 29(3), 52–57. Educational risk and resilience in African-American
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. youth: Context, self, action, and outcomes in school.
L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological Child Development, 65, 493–506.
engagement: Validation of the student engagement Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence,
instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445. autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of
Balfanz, R., Herzog, L., & Iver, D. J. M. (2007). Preventing self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe
student disengagement and keeping students on the (Eds.), Self process and development: The Minnesota
graduation path in urban middle-grades schools: Early symposia on child development (Vol. 23, pp. 43–77).
identification and effective interventions. Educational Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychologist, 42, 223–235. Cooper, H., Jackson, K., Nye, B., & Lindsay, J. J. (2001).
Battistich, V., Watson, M., Solomon, D., Schaps, E., & A model of homework’s influence on the performance
Solomon, J. (1991). The Child Development Project: evaluations of elementary school students. The Journal
A comprehensive program for the development of of Experimental Education, 69, 181–199.
prosocial character. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz Cooper, H., Valentine, J. C., Nye, B., & Lindsay, J. J.
(Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development (1999). Relationships between five after-school activi-
(Vol. 3, pp. 1–34). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ties and academic achievement. Journal of Educational
Berenson, G. S. (1986). Evolution of cardiovascular risk Psychology, 91, 369–378.
factors in early life: Perspectives on causation. In G. S. Cotton, K. (1996). School size, school climate, and stu-
Berenson (Ed.), Causation of cardiovascular risk fac- dent performance (Close-up No. 20, Series X).
tors in children: Perspectives on cardiovascular risk in Portland, OR: School Improvement Research Service,
early life (pp. 1–26). New York: Raven. Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory.
Bergin, C., & Bergin, D. (2009). Attachment in the class- Darling-Hammond, L., Ancess, J., & Ort, S. (2002).
room. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 141–170. Reinventing high school: Outcomes of the Coalition
Berliner, D. C. (1990). The nature of time in schools: Campus Schools Project. American Educational
Theoretical concepts, practitioner perceptions. New Research Journal, 39, 639–673.
York: Teachers College Press. Dar, C, (2009). The Me and My School survey. In J.
Bloom, B. S., & Broder, L. (1958). Problem-solving pro- Morton (Ed.), Engaging young people in learning:
cesses of college students. Chicago: The University of why does it matter and what can we do? (pp.85–100).
Chicago Press. Wellingtun NZ: NZCER press.
Booker, K. C. (2004). Exploring school belonging and aca- Darr, C., Ferral, H., & Stephanou, A. (2008, January). The
demic achievement in African American adolescents. development of a scale to measure student engage-
Curriculum and Teaching Dialogue, 6(2), 131–143. ment. Paper presented at the Third International Rasch
Booker, K. C. (2007). Likeness, comfort, and tolerance: Measurement Conference, Perth, Western Australia.
Examining African American adolescents’ sense of de Bruyn, E. H. (2005). Role strain, engagement and aca-
school belonging. The Urban Review, 39, 301–317. demic achievement in early adolescence. Educational
Breslow, L., Fielding, J., Afifi, A. A., Coulson, A., Kheifets, Studies, 31(1), 15–27.
L., Valdiviezo, N., et al. (1985). Risk factor update deJung, J. K., & Duckworth, K. (1986, April). Measuring
project: Final report (Contract No. 200–80–0527). student absences in the high schools. Paper presented
128 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

at the annual meeting of the American Educational achievement. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 13(1),
Research Association, San Francisco, CA. 21–43.
Dynarski, M., Clarke, L., Cobb, B., Finn, J., Rumberger, Goodenow, C. (1993b). The psychological sense of school
R., & Smink, J. (2008). Dropout prevention: A prac- membership among adolescents: Scale development
tice guide (NCEE 2008–4025). Washington, DC: and educational correlates. Psychology in the Schools,
National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional 30, 79–90.
Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Goodenow, C., & Grady, K. E. (1993). The relationship of
Department of Education. school belonging and friends’ values to academic
Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1999). Student council, vol- motivation among urban adolescent students. The
unteering, basketball, or marching band: What kind of Journal of Experimental Education, 62, 60–71.
extracurricular involvement matters? Journal of Gottfried, M. A. (2009). Excused versus unexcused: How
Adolescent Research, 14, 10–43. student absences in elementary school affect academic
Elliott, D. S., & Voss, H. L. (1974). Delinquency and achievement. Education Evaluation and Policy
dropout. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath. Analysis, 31, 392–415.
Feldman, A. F., & Matjasko, J. L. (2005). The role of Greene, B. A., & Miller, R. B. (1996). Influences on
school-based extracurricular activities in adolescent achievement: Goals, perceived ability and cognitive
development: A comprehensive review and future engagement. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
directions. Review of Educational Research, 75, 21, 181–192.
159–210. Greene, B. A., Miller, R. B., Crowson, H. M., Duke, B. L.,
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of & Akey, K. L. (2004). Predicting high school students’
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. cognitive engagement and achievement: Contributions
Finn, J. D. (1993). School engagement and students at risk of classroom perceptions and motivation.
(NCES report 93–470). Washington, DC: U.S. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 462–482.
Department of Education, National Center for Guthrie, J. T., & Davis, M. H. (2003). Motivating strug-
Education Statistics. gling readers in middle school through an engagement
Finn, J. D. (2006). The adult lives of at-risk students: The model of classroom practice. Reading & Writing
roles of attainment and engagement in high school Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 19(1),
(NCES 2006–328). Washington, DC: U. S. Department 59–85.
of Education, National Center for Educational Guthrie, J. T., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and
Statistics. motivation and reading. In P. Kamil, P. D. Mosenthal,
Finn, J. D., Folger, J., & Cox, D. (1991). Measuring partici- P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading
pation among elementary grade students. Educational research (Vol. III, pp. 403–424). Mahwah, NJ:
and Psychological Measurement, 51, 393–402. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Finn, J. D., Pannozzo, G. M., & Voelkl, K. E. (1995). Gutman, L. M., & Midgley, C. (2000). The role of protec-
Disruptive and inattentive-withdrawn behavior and tive factors in supporting the academic achievement of
achievement among fourth graders. The Elementary poor African American students during the middle
School Journal, 95, 421–434. school transition. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success 29, 223–248.
among students at risk for school failure. Journal of Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., & Miller, J. Y. (1992).
Applied Psychology, 82, 221–234. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug
Fisher, C. W., Berliner, D. C., Nikola, N., Filby, R. M., problems in adolescence and early adulthood:
Cahen, L. C., & Dishaw, M. M. (1980). Teaching Implications for substance abuse prevention.
behaviors, academic learning time, and student achieve- Psychological Bulletin, 112, 64–105.
ment: An overview. In C. Denham & A. Lieberman Hawkins, J. D., Guo, J., Hill, K. G., Battin-Pearson, S., &
(Eds.), Time to learn (pp. 7–32). Washington, DC: U.S. Abbott, R. D. (2001). Long-term effects of the Seattle
Department of Education, National Institute of Social Development intervention on school bonding
Education. trajectories. Applied Developmental Science, 5,
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). 225–236.
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of Hindelang, M. J. (1973). Causes of delinquency: A partial
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, replication and extension. Social Problems, 20,
59–109. 471–487.
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley, CA:
factor in children’s academic engagement and perfor- University of California Press.
mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, Howse, R. B., Lange, G., Farran, D. C., & Boyles, C. D.
148–162. (2003). Motivation and self-regulation as predictors of
Gerber, S. B. (1996). Extracurricular activities and aca- achievement in economically disadvantaged young
demic achievement. Journal of Research & children. The Journal of Experimental Education, 71,
Development in Education, 30(1), 42–50. 151–174.
Goodenow, C. (1993a). Classroom belonging among early Hughes, J. N., Luo, W., Kwok, O.-M., & Loyd, L. K.
adolescent students: Relationships to motivation and (2008). Teacher-student support, effortful engagement,
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 129

and achievement: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal enduring effects of schooling. Educational Evaluation
of Educational Psychology, 100, 1–14. and Policy Analysis, 6, 73–83.
Hyman, I. A., & Perone, D. C. (1998). The other side of Liska, A. E., & Reed, M. D. (1985). Ties to conven-
school violence: Educator policies and practices that tional institutions and delinquency: Estimating
may contribute to student misbehavior. Journal of reciprocal effects. American Sociological Review,
School Psychology, 36, 7–27. 50, 547–560.
Jessor, R., Turbin, M. S., & Costa, F. M. (1998). Risk and Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1998). Juvenile
protection in successful outcomes among disadvantaged aggression at home and at school. In D. S. Elliot, B.
adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 2, Hamburg, & K. R. Williams (Eds.), Violence in
194–208. American schools: A new perspective (pp. 84–126).
Jimerson, S. R., Campos, E., & Greif, J. L. (2003). Toward New York: Cambridge University Press.
an understanding of definitions and measures of school Loukas, A., Roalson, L. A., & Herrera, D. E. (2010).
engagement and related terms. California School School connectedness buffers the effects of negative
Psychologist, 8, 7–27. family relations and poor effortful control on early
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., Buckman, L. A., & adolescent conduct problems. Journal of Research on
Richards, P. S. (1985). The effect of prolonged imple- Adolescence, 20, 13–22.
mentation of cooperative learning on social support Luckner, A. E., Englund, M. M., Coffey, T., & Nuno, A.
within the classroom. The Journal of Psychology, 119, A. (2006). Validation of a global measure of school
405–411. engagement in early and middle adolescence. In M.
Juliebo, M., Malicky, G. V., & Norman, C. (1998). M. Englund (Chair.), Adolescent engagement in
Metacognition of young readers in an early intervention school: Issues of definition and measurement sympo-
programme. Journal of Research in Reading, 21, 24–35. sium conducted at the biennial meeting of the Society
Kanungo, R. N. (1979). The concepts of alienation and for Research on Adolescence, San Francisco, CA.
involvement revisited. Psychological Bulletin, 86, Ma, X. (2003). Sense of belonging to school: Can schools
119–138. make a difference? The Journal of Educational
Kaplan, D. S., Peck, B. M., & Kaplan, H. B. (1995). Research, 96, 340–349.
A structural model of dropout behavior: A longitudinal Maddox, S. J., & Prinz, R. J. (2003). School bonding in
analysis. Applied Behavioral Science Review, 3, children and adolescents: Conceptualization, assess-
177–193. ment, and associated variables. Clinical Child and
Kemple, J. J., & Snipes, J. C. (2000). Career academics: Family Psychology Review, 6, 31–49.
Impacts on students’ engagement and performance in Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional
high school. New York: Manpower Demonstration activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle and high
Research Corporation. school years. American Educational Research Journal,
Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships matter: 37, 153–184.
Linking teacher support to student engagement and Marsh, H. W. (1992). Extracurricular activities: Beneficial
achievement. Journal of School Health, 74, 262–273. extension of the traditional curriculum or subversion
Kuhl, J., & Kraska, K. (1993). Self-regulation: of. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 553–562.
Psychometric properties of a computer-aided instru- Marsh, H. W., & Kleitman, S. (2002). Extracurricular
ment. German Journal of Psychology, 17, 11–24. school activities: The good, the bad, and the nonlinear.
Ladd, G. W., & Dinella, L. M. (2009). Continuity and Harvard Educational Review, 72, 464–514.
change in early school engagement: Predictive of chil- Maslow, A. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.).
dren’s achievement trajectories from first to eighth New York: Harper & Row.
grade? Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, McClelland, D. C. (1985). Human motivation. Dallas,
190–206. TX: Scott Foresman.
Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. B. (1993). Effects of school restruc- McNeely, C. A., Nonnemaker, J. M., & Blum, R. W.
turing on the achievement and engagement of middle- (2002). Promoting school connectedness: Evidence
grade students. Sociology of Education, 66, 164–187. from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent
Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. B. (1995). Effects of high school Health. Journal of School Health, 72, 138–146.
restructuring and size on early gains in achievement Melnick, M. J., & Sabo, D. F. (1992). Educational effects
and engagement. Sociology of Education, 68, of interscholastic athletic participation on African-
241–270. American and Hispanic youth. Adolescence, 27,
Libbey, H. P. (2004). Measuring student relationships to 295–308.
school: Attachment, bonding, connectedness, and Mickelson, R. A. (1990). The attitude-achievement para-
engagement. Journal of School Health, 74, 274–283. dox among black adolescents. Sociology of Education,
Lindsay, P. (1982). The effect of high school size on stu- 63, 44–61.
dent participation, satisfaction, and attendance. Mokhtari, K., & Reichard, C. A. (2002). Assessing stu-
Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 4, dents’ metacognitive awareness of reading strategies.
57–65. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 249–259.
Lindsay, P. (1984). High school size, participation in National Research Council and The Institute of Medicine
activities, and young adult social participation: Some [NRC & IOM]. (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering
130 J.D. Finn and K.S. Zimmer

high school students’ motivation to learn. Washington, of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
DC: The National Academies Press. Psychiatry, 31, 349–356.
Newmann, F. M. (1981). Reducing student alienation in Rumberger, R. W. (1987). High school dropouts: A review
high schools: Implications of theory. Harvard of issues and evidence. Review of Educational
Educational Review, 51, 546–564. Research, 57, 101–121.
Newmann, F. M. (1992). Student engagement and achieve- Rumberger, R. W. (2001, January). Why students drop out
ment in American secondary schools. New York: of school and what can be done. Paper presented at the
Teachers College Press. forum Dropouts in America: How severe is the prob-
Newmann, F. M., Wehlage, G. G., & Lamborn, S. D. lem? What do we know about intervention and preven-
(1992). The significance and sources of student tion? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University, Civil
engagement. In F. M. Newmann (Ed.), Student engage- Rights Project.
ment and achievement in American secondary schools Rumberger, R. W., & Lim, S. A. (2008). Why students
(pp. 11–30). New York: Teachers College Press. drop out of school: A review of 25 years of research
Osterman, K. F. (2000). Students’ need for belonging in (Project Report No. 15). Santa Barbara, CA: University
the school community. Review of Educational of California, California Dropout Research Project.
Research, 70, 323–367. Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2001). The classroom social
Ou, S. R., Mersky, J. P., Reynolds, A. J., & Kohler, K. M. environment and changes in adolescents’ motivation
(2007). Alterable predictors of educational attainment, and engagement during middle school. American
income, and crime: Findings from an inner-city cohort. Educational Research Journal, 38(2), 437–460.
The Social Service Review, 81, 85–128. Shernoff, D., & Schmidt, J. (2008). Further evidence of an
Pannozzo, G. M., Finn, J. D., & Boyd-Zaharias, J. (2004, engagement-achievement paradox among U.S. high
April). Behavioral and affective engagement in school school students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
and dropping out. Paper presented at the annual meet- 37, 564–580.
ing of the American Educational Research Association, Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the
San Diego, CA. classroom: Reciprocal effect of teacher behavior and
Patrick, B., Skinner, E., & Connell, J. (1993). What moti- student engagement across the school year. Journal of
vates children’s behavior and emotion? Joint effects of Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581.
perceived control and autonomy in the academic Skinner, E., Kindermann, T., Connell, J., & Wellborn, J.
domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (2009). Engagement and disaffection as organiza-
65, 781–791. tional constructs in the dynamics of motivational
Peterson, P. L., Swing, S. R., Stark, K. D., & Waas, G. A. development. In Handbook of motivation at school
(1984). Students’ cognitions and time on task during (pp. 223–245). New York: Routledge/Taylor &
mathematics instruction. American Educational Francis Group.
Research Journal, 21, 487–515. Steinberg, L., & Avenevoli, S. (1998). Disengagement
Pittman, R. B. (1991). Social factors, enrollment in voca- from school and problem behavior in adolescence:
tional/technical courses, and high school dropout rates. A developmental-contextual analysis of the influences
The Journal of Educational Research, 84, 288–295. of family and part-time work. In R. Jessor (Ed.), New
Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., & Congdon, R. T. (2000). perspectives in adolescent risk behavior (pp. 392–424).
HLM 5 for Windows. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific New York: Cambridge University Press.
Software International, Inc. Steward, R. J., Steward, A. D., Blair, J., Hanik, J., & Hill,
Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K. E., M. F. (2008). School attendance revisited: A study of
Harris, K. M., Jones, J., et al. (1997). Protecting ado- urban African American students’ grade point aver-
lescents from harm: Findings from the national longi- ages and coping strategies. Urban Education, 43,
tudinal study on adolescent health. Journal of the 518–536.
American Medical Association, 278, 823–832. Stewart, E. A. (2003). School social bonds, school cli-
Ripski, M. B., & Gregory, A. (2009). Unfair, unsafe, and mate, and school misbehavior: A multilevel analysis.
unwelcome: Do high school students’ perceptions of Justice Quarterly, 20, 575–604.
unfairness, hostility, and victimization in school pre- Swift, M. S., & Spivack, G. (1968). The assessment of
dict engagement and achievement? Journal of School achievement-related classroom behavior: Normative,
Violence, 8, 355–375. reliability and validity data. Journal of Special
Roby, D. E. (2004). Research on school attendance and Education, 2, 137–153.
student achievement: A study of Ohio schools. Swift, M. S., & Spivack, G. (1969). Achievement-related
Educational Research Quarterly, 28(1), 3–14. classroom behavior of secondary school normal and
Rotgans, J. I., & Schmidt, H. G. (2011). Situational inter- disturbed students. Exceptional Children, 35, 677–684.
est and academic achievement in the active-learning US Department of Education. (2001). An overview of
classroom. Learning and Instruction, 21(1), 58–67. smaller learning communities in high schools.
Rowe, K. J., & Rowe, K. S. (1992). The relationship Washington, DC: Office of Elementary and Secondary
between inattentiveness in the classroom and reading Education and Office of Vocational and Adult
achievement: Part A: Methodological issues. Journal Education.
5 Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter? 131

Voelkl, K. E. (1995). School warmth, student participa- Wehlage, G. G., Rutter, R. A., Smith, G. A., Lesko, N.,
tion, and achievement. The Journal of Experimental & Fernandez, R. R. (1989). Reducing the risk:
Education, 63, 127–138. Schools as communities of support. Philadelphia:
Voelkl, K. E. (1996). Measuring students’ identification Falmer Press.
with school. Educational and Psychological Weitzman, M., Klerman, L. V., Lamb, G. A., Kane, K.,
Measurement, 56, 760–770. Geromini, K. R., Kayne, R., et al. (1985).
Voelkl, K. E. (1997). Identification with school. American Demographic and educational characteristics of
Journal of Education, 105, 294–318. inner city middle school problem absence students.
Voelkl, K. E. (2012). School identification. In S. L. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55,
Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), 378–383.
Handbook of research on student engagement Wentzel, K. R. (1993). Does being good make the grade?
(pp. 193–218). New York: Springer. Social behavior and academic competence in middle
Voelkl, K. E., & Frone, M. R. (2000). Predictors of sub- school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85,
stance use at school among high school students. 357–364.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 583–592. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value
Voelkl, K. E., & Frone, M. R. (2004). Academic perfor- theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary
mance and cheating: Moderating role of school identi- Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81.
fication and self-efficacy. The Journal of Educational Wigfield, A., Guthrie, J. T., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada,
Research, 97, 115–122. A., Klauda, S. L., McRae, A., et al. (2008). Role of
Voelkl, K. E., & Willert, H. J. (2006). Alcohol and drugs reading engagement in mediating effects of reading
in schools: Teachers’ reactions to the problem. Phi comprehension instruction on reading outcomes.
Delta Kappan, 88(1), 37–40. Psychology in the Schools, 45, 432–445.
Wang, M., & Holcombe, R. (2010). Adolescents’ percep- Wolters, C. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (1998). Contextual differ-
tions of school environment, engagement, and aca- ences in student motivation and self-regulated learning
demic achievement in middle school. American in mathematics, English and social studies classrooms.
Educational Research Journal, 47, 633–662. Instructional Science, 26, 27–47.
Wehlage, G. G., & Rutter, R. A. (1986). Dropping out: Yin, Z., & Moore, J. B. (2004). Re-examining the role of
How much do schools contribute to the problem? interscholastic sport participation in education.
Teachers College Record, 87, 374–392. Psychological Reports, 94(3, Pt 2), 1447–1454.
Part I Commentary:
So What Is Student Engagement 6
Anyway?

Jacquelynne Eccles and Ming-Te Wang

Abstract
This chapter, by the esteemed motivation scholar Jacque Eccles and
Ming-Te Wang, provides a commentary on the five chapters in Part I. The
authors offered additional critique of the issues raised by authors in this
part, as well as their perspectives on future directions for the engagement
construct. In addition, Eccles and Wang described links between the theo-
ries of engagement and the Expectancy-Value Theoretical Model of
Achievement-Related Behavior espoused by Eccles.

These chapters provide excellent overviews of from reading them much better informed about
the current thinking in the broad field of engage- engagement theory (ET) and its incredible
ment. Doing the commentary has provided us importance for our thinking about school success
with a great opportunity to think through our and school reform. By and large, the authors of
own take on student engagement as a concept. each of the chapters did an admirable job of
We have been working closely for the last year summarizing the extant theory and research
on a series of papers most directly related to the relevant to their chapter as well as critiquing the
Frederick et al.’s (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & field and proposing important and promising
Paris, 2004) perspective on school engagement. future directions. Our goal was not to repeat
Both of us have been less involved with the work their conclusions but to offer additional critiques
on engagement growing out of Finn’s partici- and future directions as well as to expand the
pation-identification theory and the work on range of theoretical perspectives considered to
dropout prevention and whole school reform. be relevant to the general idea of school and
Reading all five of these chapters with an eye classroom engagement.
toward writing this commentary has helped me We have divided our comments into several
(Eccles) put this collaborative work into a much parts. In the first, we comment directly on each
larger framework. We both thoroughly enjoyed of the five chapters and end with a few summary
reading each of these chapters and came away comments on the full set of chapters. In the
second part, we discuss our thinking on the
general topic of these five chapters – what is
J. Eccles (*) • M.-T. Wang
student engagement? In the third part, I (Eccles)
Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan,
Ann Arbor, MI, USA discuss my view on the link between general
e-mail: jeccles@umich.edu; wangmi@umich.edu ET and my own work on school success. I focus

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 133


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_6, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
134 J. Eccles and M.-T. Wang

more specifically on the links I see between Finn and Zimmer (2012) provide an even more
general ET and the Eccles et al. (1983) expec- extensive review of the research most directly
tancy-value theoretical model of achievement- linked to Finn’s perspective on ET. Although not
related behavior (EEVT). entirely the case, much of the research they
review has emerged either from Finn’s early
participation-identification theory or from work
Comments on Specific Chapters on dropout prevention and school misbehavior.
Reading this chapter made it clear to us that two
These five chapters provide a very broad review fairly parallel lines of research on school success
of the ways in which student engagement (SE) have been going over the last 20–30 years: (1) the
has been conceptualized, defined, and measured work grounded in ET and linked closely to drop-
as well as a comprehensive review of the extant out prevention and at-risk populations – a tradi-
literature on the association of SE with school tion that includes scholars like Finn, Newmann,
achievement. The chapters represented a breadth Wehlage, Reschly, and Rumburger (see the
of perspectives and approaches to the study of chapters by Reschly and Christenson and by
SE that will open the eyes of all but the most Finn and Zimmer for extensive reviews of this
well-versed reader. This is even truer of the tradition) – and (2) the work grounded in psy-
entire volume, which includes perspectives chological motivation theory that is more closely
from several fields outside of educational and linked to academic motivation within the class-
developmental psychology. The diversity of room learning context – a tradition that includes
fields represented makes it likely that most scholars associated with self-determination
readers will be exposed to previously unknown theory, achievement goal theory, achievement
work. In addition, the looser format of an edited motivation theory, attribution theory, self-efficacy
volume allowed the authors to expand more theory, and expectancy-value theory of achieve-
freely in their writing than is normally allowed ment (e.g., scholars such as Anderman, Bandura,
in the tight confines of a journal article. Because Blumenfeld, Connell, Deci, Dweck, Eccles,
the intended audience of this volume includes Elliott, Meece, Midgley, Pintrich, Roeser,
both researchers and practitioners, many Schunk, Skinner, and Wigfield; see Wigfield,
authors also discussed applied aspects of their Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-Kean, 2006
work that rarely appear in published research for a full review of this tradition). Scholars in each
articles. Thus, reading these chapters will pro- of these traditions have tried to understand the
vide readers with broader insights into the psychological characteristics that underlie both
research than is possible in journal accounts of academic success and failure. In each tradition,
their scientific findings. a wide variety of constructs has emerged and
This part begins with an overview chapter by extensive research has documented the associa-
Reschly and Christenson (2012). In this chapter, tion of these various constructs with indicators
they discuss the conceptual haziness that has of school success, ranging from basic learning
emerged in this field as the definition of SE has to school completion. What is clear in reading
broadened. They point out the need for greater Finn and Zimmer is how separate these two tra-
clarity. They also point out the overlap between ditions have been from each other despite a quite
four basic theories of engagement and discuss the common goal. As members of the second of
important relevance of ET for designing and these two traditions, we greatly appreciated the
evaluating school intervention efforts aimed at breadth of Finn and Zimmer’s review of the
reducing school dropout. The chapter provides an work from the first tradition. We also appreci-
excellent introduction to this part by laying the ated the clear implication of this work for school
groundwork for the other four chapters. We reiter- interventions.
ate several of Reschly and Christenson’s critiques Skinner and Pitzer (2012) provide an excellent
about the definition of SE in our discussion. overview of an alternative theory of engagement
6 Part I Commentary: So What Is Student Engagement Anyway? 135

– one based in the second tradition alluded to in By so doing, we are broadening Skinner and
the previous paragraph – self-determination the- Pitzer’s perspective to include something akin to
ory (SDT). This model stresses the importance of Finn’s notion of identification and to the kinds of
intrinsic motivation and the ways in which social identity-based self-system beliefs discussed by
contextual features facilitate the emergence of Bingham and Okagaki (2012) and to a more lim-
intrinsic motivation to engage in a task. In this ited extent by Mahatmya, Lohman, Matjasko,
sense, SDT focuses attention on the precursors of and Farb (2012).
behavioral engagement and assumes that positive Mahatmya et al. (2012) provide a develop-
engagement is most likely when the context pro- mental perspective on student engagement. They
vides opportunities for individuals to fulfill their link the ideas associated with notions of develop-
needs for competence, belonging, and autonomy. mental tasks and developmental stages to age-
Skinner and Pitzer further hypothesize that related changes in the three types of engagement
teacher warmth, adequate structure, and support proposed by Fredricks et al. (2004): behavioral,
for autonomy are the three contextual features emotional, and cognitive. This chapter is much
mostly likely to meet these needs and thus facili- more speculative than the other four chapters in
tate engagement. Although Skinner and Pitzer this part because there has been less research and
begin by stressing their view that “engagement less theorizing about developmental changes in
is the outward manifestation of motivation,” engagement. As noted by these authors, the most
they then enlarge their definition of engagement developmental work directly related to the idea
to bring it more in line with the broader view of of student engagement is the work done by
SE salient in all five of these chapters. They do Eccles, Midgley, and their colleagues under the
this by including both a behavioral and an emo- notion of stage-environment fit (see Eccles et al.,
tional component to their definition of engage- 1993). Hopefully, this chapter will stimulate such
ment. By so doing, they increase the overlap work in the future.
between SDT and ET substantially. They add to We do have one concern about the chapter
the ET theorists’ view of engagement a theoreti- that should be voiced as a cautionary note, given
cal perspective on the immediate contextual pre- the early state of this line of scholarship. To
cursors most likely to influence behavioral and their credit, the authors try to bring in thinking
cognitive engagement in the classroom. They from the new neuroscience of brain growth and
also add a very thoughtful discussion of disen- development. It is important that we take the
gagement as a key construct and discuss the cognitive neuroscience perspective seriously as
types of coping behaviors that will either facili- we consider developmental changes in things
tate or undermine learning and achievement. By like student engagement. But we are uncomfort-
bringing in these ideas, they focus attention on able with the use of terms like “more cognitive
classroom-level engagement in learning activi- engagement” rather than a different type of cog-
ties, and they acknowledge that disengagement nitive engagement to describe the nature of such
might be an appropriate coping response in some changes. For example, they say, “during adoles-
contexts. cence, individuals experience rapid physical
Not surprisingly, we like this chapter very maturation as well as rapid development of cog-
much because we are quite familiar and comfort- nitive skills. …Thus, the ability to become cog-
able with Skinner and Pitzer’s way of framing nitively engaged with school is greater during
their approach to the issue of motivation, engage- adolescence compared to both early and middle
ment, and learning. We particularly like their childhood.” Although the first statement may be
emphasis on engagement as the behavioral mani- correct (however, early brain development is
festation of motivation. We would add to this the pretty rapid and major neurological changes are
idea that engagement is also the behavioral mani- also going on in the age 5–7 shift), the second
festation of social and personal identities (see is very controversial and may not be true. It is
Eccles, 2009, and later part in this commentary). not even clear to us what evidence one would
136 J. Eccles and M.-T. Wang

look at to demonstrate that the “ability to become


cognitively engaged is greater during adoles- General Comments on This Part
cence.” This depends on what is defined as cog-
nitive engagement. To the extent that cognitive As noted earlier, each of these five chapters pro-
engagement includes the mental behaviors asso- vides the reader with solid overviews of the lit-
ciated with learning, then it is not clear to us that erature related to student engagement at school
learning to read and write evidences less cogni- within the frame of the chapter. In each of these
tive engagement than learning algebra. These five chapters, the authors also point out a variety
two learning tasks may require different types of future directions that could help us better
of cognitive engagement, and the brain matura- understand student engagement. We agree with
tion occurring during adolescence may allow the authors that developing (1) a more integrative
for different and more conscious forms of cog- developmental-contextual approach to investi-
nitive engagement than were available during gate the individual and contextual factors that
early and middle childhood. At this point, we predict student engagement, and (2) appropriate
need to be cautious when using terms like instruments to measure student engagement at
“more” rather than “different,” particularly if different levels is critical. To develop appropriate
we are talking about the extent to which indi- theories, we agree with the authors that it is
viduals use their “full capacity” to be cogni- important to revisit current relevant theories,
tively engaged at each point in development as such as SDT, person-environment fit theory, flow
our marker of individual differences in the theory, and expectancy-value theory, for more
extent of cognitive engagement. integrated insights into the nature of and influ-
Finally, Bingham and Okagaki (2012) pro- ences on student engagement.
vide a very comprehensive review of the litera- A related approach could be to synthesize or
tures aimed at understanding ethnicity and modify existing theories. The chapter by Skinner
student engagement. Essentially, this chapter and Pitzer (2012) on the self-system model of
focuses on the more distal precursors of student motivational development is an excellent exam-
engagement. They draw on several theoretical ple of such an approach. Yet another approach for
frameworks and research traditions, including SDT, studying SE would be to build new models or
ET, critical race theory, various sociocultural the- theories that could be applied to classroom set-
ories, ecological theories, person-environment tings as well as to situations outside the class-
fit theories, collective identity theories, social room. Such models or theories could focus more
psychological perspective of race, discrimina- on person-centered approaches. For example,
tion, and identity and social developmental the- they could focus on the ways in which behavior,
ories of contextual influences. This makes for a emotion, and cognition develop as coordinated
very rich chapter, particularly because they have engagement-related processes in some individu-
skillfully interweaved these various theoretical als but as more disconnected aspects of engage-
frameworks to help us understand very subtle ment in others. These person-centered patterns
nuances in ethnic differences in student engage- cannot easily be studied using the kinds of linear
ment. Thus, this chapter, like Skinner and Pitzer or hierarchical conceptualizations presented in
(2012), focuses on the precursors of engage- these five chapters. New approaches might also
ment, focusing in particular on the three catego- use patterns of engagement over time to explain
ries of engagement set out by Fredricks et al. and predict effective learning experiences within
(2004). We wish they had been more specific multiple contexts across time.
when they reported the findings of various stud- There is also a great need for better measures
ies as to exactly which subtypes of engagement and methods of study, particularly after more
was actually measured and when engagement comprehensive theories of SE and learning have
was measured as opposed to learning and emerged. We cannot know whether we are
achievement. improving student engagement unless we can
6 Part I Commentary: So What Is Student Engagement Anyway? 137

measure it accurately and appropriately. Neither how these moment-to-moment experiences of


can we design good programs to improve SE engagement congeal into more integrated
until we better understand which aspects of stu- “engagement” in a specific subject area or a more
dent engagement influence which aspects of global attachment to an institutional setting like
learning and performance for which students. In a school building. Such methods might also
addressing complex questions such as how dif- allow us to study the ways in which and for
ferent types of SE are involved in learning, we whom “engagement” in a school institution leads
need to think carefully about such measurement to greater moment-to-moment engagement in
issues as (1) how we define the constructs that we particular courses.
are measuring or recording, (2) how we achieve
internal and external validity, and (3) how we
interpret our findings. Such measurement and
interpretation issues will necessarily include mul- What Is Engagement?
tiple perspectives, multiple methods, and multiple
levels. Student engagement processes are rela- There is no doubt that “engagement” is currently
tional and dynamic; they involve ongoing inter- a very hot topic in the broad field of school
action between individuals and contexts. The achievement. But what is engagement? This is
different components of engagement cannot be the topic of the five chapters in this part of this
wholly captured by collecting data from individ- handbook. Each of the chapters deals with this
uals’ self-reports. The use of multiple methods issue in one way or another. However, after read-
(e.g., survey and interviews) and multiple infor- ing all five, we were still left unsure and unsatis-
mants (e.g., teacher, student, and parent) to assess fied. First, it is critical that we understand that the
SE would offer a more comprehensive and answer to this question is definitional. We can
diverse perspective. However, we also need to be define student engagement in any way we would
cautious about relying on teacher or parent reports like, and how we define it will determine its use-
of internal processes linked to emotion and cog- fulness for various communities. Defining it
nitive engagement, particularly if we then use broadly will make it more useful for the policy
these measures to predict outcomes that are also making and educated lay thinker communities
generated by the teacher (e.g., course grades). but less useful for the research and scholarly
Moreover, it is important to include and dis- community. Defining it narrowly will have the
tinguish the different levels (e.g., classroom ver- opposite effect. Defining it broadly will increase
sus school building, as discussed by Skinner and the overlap of engagement with other theories
Pitzer, 2012) and different time frames of student and research literatures, making its unique contri-
engagement (e.g., in-the-moment task engage- bution less clear. Defining it narrowly will force
ment versus longer term engagement/commit- “engagement” scholars to make its unique contri-
ment to a particular subject area, as discussed by bution and value-added clear. The chapters by
Finn and Zimmer (2012) and by Reschly and Reschly and Christenson (2012), Finn and
Christenson (2012)). Many of the existing Zimmer (2012), and Skinner and Pitzer (2012) all
engagement measures are quite general, rarely deal directly with this issue. However, at times, it
focusing on specific tasks, situations, or subjects. seemed as though the terms student engagement,
Incorporating domain-specific measures will help and school engagement, were being used rather
to determine to what extent engagement repre- loosely to mean everything that is good about an
sents a general tendency and to what extent it is individual’s relationship with his or her school.
content specific. Innovative methods such as the To the extent that this is true, then the hypothesis
experience sampling method or daily diary meth- that engagement improves achievement seems
ods, for example, could help us capture the rather circular because doing well in school both
moment-to-moment experiences of different increases engagement and is used as evidence
subtypes of engagement and then over time that engagement is high.
138 J. Eccles and M.-T. Wang

Policy makers and lay educational pundits overlap between the terms engagement and
love broad terms like engagement that appear to motivation: to quote, “Academic motivation …
“explain” everything: “Students are not doing has been defined as ‘a general desire or disposi-
well because they are not engaged. So let’s tion to succeed in academic work and in the more
increase engagement and they will do better.” specific tasks of school’.” As experts in achieve-
But what exactly is engagement? What should be ment motivation as it applies to motivation in
the specific focus of an intervention to increase school contexts, we find such a broad definition
engagement? If “engagement” encompasses unsatisfying. On the one hand, such a broad defi-
everything from feeling like one belongs in the nition is very useful in discussions with policy
school to doing one’s homework, or to participat- makers and practitioners because it orients them
ing in the school band, then almost anything we to the broad domain of motivation as contrasted
do to improve schools can be seen as an interven- with other broad domains such as achievement or
tion to increase engagement. As scientists, we problem behaviors. On the other hand, it is not
find this level of generality unsatisfying because particularly useful for discussions among
it encompasses everything and anything that is researchers and for increasing our fundamental
related to students’ and teachers’ functioning in understanding of human behavior in school
the school context. Probably the most concrete contexts.
example of what we are talking about here is in There is a subfield of educational psychology
the debate alluded to in several of the chapters that focuses specifically on academic motivation.
about whether to include the concept of motiva- The scholars in this field have spent the last
tion as part of engagement. Those who prefer a 50 years building a taxonomy of various beliefs,
broad, inclusive definition of engagement seem attitudes, needs, and emotions that comprise aca-
comfortable including motivational concepts demic motivation. They have demonstrated the
such as affect, liking, feelings of belonging, and following: (1) The various components of aca-
valuing within the definition of affective engage- demic motivation influence different aspects of
ment. In contrast, Skinner and Pitzer (2012) pre- achievement in schools; (2) these components
fer a more restricted definition of engagement, are influenced by different contextual features
such as “the behavioral manifestation of motiva- and experiential histories; (3) as a consequence
tion,” that makes the concept of engagement dis- of 1 and 2, different intervention strategies are
tinct from the many other related concepts. We needed for the various components, and these
agree that a more precise definition will make different interventions will influence different
“engagement” easier to measure and study as aspect of school-related behaviors; (4) the devel-
well as to be related to other theories of achieve- opmental patterns associated with these various
ment and learning. At several points in their components differ; and (5) the salience of these
chapter, Finn and Zimmer (2012) also seem to different components in motivating behavior dif-
prefer a more limited notion of engagement that fer across developmental time and social context
focuses on behavior (both overt and covert men- (see Wigfield et al., 2006).
tal behaviors), but then they also appear to be Given these specific facts regarding academic
quite comfortable with a much broader defini- motivation and a very broad definition of engage-
tion. Reschly and Christenson (2012) also discuss ment, it is impossible to address the question of
this tension in their chapter. whether motivation should be seen as part of
It is important to note that the same problem engagement or vice versa. The answer is both yes
exists in the field of achievement motivation. The and no depending on which part of each elephant
tension between broad, inclusive versus more one is touching – in other words, “It depends.”
specific perspectives pervades this area of psy- For example, at the most general level, we can
chology as well. For example, Finn and Zimmer say motivation influences behavior which, in
(2012) cite Newmann and colleagues’ definition turn, influences subsequent outcomes or A →
of motivation in their discussion of the possible B→ C, with A = motivation, B = behavior, and
6 Part I Commentary: So What Is Student Engagement Anyway? 139

C = let us say learning. C of course could also be illustrate a more general point that is not so
high school graduation, GPA, etc. Such a model extreme: Confirmation of specific predictions
seems quite satisfying because it is general and depends on the specifics of the predictions. And it
because it makes clear the mediating role of is in the specifics that clear definitions really mat-
behavior. In this general case, one could say that ter. So, for example, policy makers have asked
B is engagement. If so, then motivation leads to researchers to demonstrate that extracurricular
engagement; motivation is thus a precursor of activities affect academic achievement in large
engagement rather than a part of engagement, part because the research community has argued
and engagement mediates the relationship that student engagement is important for school
between motivation and school success. There success, and then we have included participation
are elements of this general model in each of the in extracurricular activities as part of our defini-
chapters, and ample evidence to support its valid- tion of school engagement. As is pointed out in
ity is provided in each chapter. several of these chapters, there is evidence that
But when affective engagement is considered participation in extracurricular activities is
to be a form of engagement, then the distinction weakly but significantly associated with GPA in
between A, B, and C begins to blur. This problem some studies, for some programs, and for some
is further exacerbated when one takes a develop- populations. Thus, at a quite general level, these
mental perspective, as is explicitly acknowledged studies provide support for the general prediction
by both Finn in his participation-identification that engagement in extracurricular activities
model (1989) and Skinner and Pitzer (2012) in influences achievement. But why does this asso-
their conceptual model of engagement and disen- ciation exist? Here the specifics matter, and they
gagement in the classroom. In Finn’s model, for matter when one tries to develop an intervention
example, participation in various aspects of designed, let us say, to increase the learning of
school leads to success experiences which in turn the academic content being taught at school or
lead to identification with school which then students’ performance on high-stakes tests. Why
influences subsequent participation. Similar iter- would we expect, for example, that increasing
ative processes are assumed in most models of opportunities for participation in extracurricular
achievement motivation and behavior and are at activities would lead to better understanding of
the core of such fundamental and classic devel- algebra or a foreign language, or science? We can
opmentally focused psychological theories as certainly generate a logic model for why this
conditioning, internalization, and social learning. might be so. For example, getting students to
The problem stems from the fact that these itera- come to school and attend their classes might
tive processes make it very difficult to keep our increase the likelihood of their learning algebra.
definitions of A, B, and C distinct. Is identifica- But this will depend on the extent to which
tion part of engagement or the result of engage- increased feelings of belonging at school actually
ment or the precursor of future engagement? influence the students’ specific cognitive and
This general 3-component model is even more behavioral engagement with algebra while they
problematic as we expand our definition of what are in their algebra class. It will also depend on
fits into A, B, and C. As these general categories the extent to which the teachers are providing the
of constructs become more inclusive, the likeli- type of high-quality algebra instruction that is
hood of making incorrect or at least not well- necessary to help many formerly disengaged stu-
informed specific predictions increases. For dents master algebra. Thus, we need to be very
example, if we select the desire to become a great careful when we make broad claims about the
singer as our indicator of A, the participation in likely impact of various engagement-focused
extracurricular activities for B, and the learning interventions on different indicators of school
of the content in specific courses for C, then it is success. We need detailed logic models that lay
quite unlikely that the study will confirm the out the likely connections between our interven-
prediction. We pick this extreme example to tions and the outcomes we hope to influence.
140 J. Eccles and M.-T. Wang

Otherwise, we are at risk of proving that some These examples illustrate another concern we
very well-designed interventions aimed at increas- have with defining what engagement is – a con-
ing school attendance have little impact on con- cern very well-articulated by Skinner and Pitzer
tent mastery in specific courses. Additionally and (2012). This concern focuses on the need to be
perhaps even more importantly, is increasing more specific about what level of engagement we
options, for example, to participate in high-quality are talking about. In Fig. 2.1, Skinner and Pitzer
extracurricular activities, the most efficient or (2012) provide a very informative picture of the
least expensive way to increase this type of aca- various levels at which the term engagement has
demic performance? We are not saying that such been used as well as the types of outcomes likely
programs are not useful for increasing other to be associated with changes in engagement at
aspects of school success; the evidence reviewed each level. Keeping such distinctions salient in
indicates that some are and some are not. What our discussions is critical to prevent misunder-
we are saying is that we need to be careful and standings regarding the effectiveness of interven-
rigorous in our operationalization of concepts tions and overgeneralizations of our findings.
like engagement, as well as in our design of These dilemmas are exacerbated even more
appropriate interventions for specific outcomes. when we move beyond motivation as the “A”
In other words, uncovering the right engagement- construct in the generalized 3-component model
related interventions requires us to look very outlined above. When we let A stand for all pre-
closely at the specifics of terms A, B, and C in our cursors of engagement (B), then we have a very
general formula. general model of human development that over-
We are also not saying that the authors of laps with many theories and is probably too gen-
these five chapters have not made similar points. eral to be of great scientific significance. Although
They absolutely have in both these chapters and at the general level, it is bound to be true; at the
their other writings. What we are saying is that specific level, it is not particularly useful for either
we need to be very mindful of the need for pre- theory testing or intervention design. It becomes a
cision in our definitions and conceptualizations variation of the truism that good things lead to
if we really want to change the various subcom- good things, which lead, in turn, to other good
ponents of students’ “engagement” in different things, except when they do not. As noted by all
aspects of their schooling experiences. If our of the authors, it is essential that we keep the dis-
goal is to reduce dropout rates, then increasing tinction between precursors of engagement and
the salience and number of reasons students indicators of engagement very clear. But, as also
have to stay in school until graduation may be noted by the authors and by us in this commen-
the most efficient strategy. If our goal is to tary, achieving this task is often hard to do when
increase science or math or English literacy and one is operating at the psychological or personal
understanding, then we should probably focus level. It is easier when one is looking at social
on more specific targeted strategies within contextual precursors of either engagement or
these classrooms. We worry that using a single school success, as is evident in the chapters by
phrase like “increasing engagement” for both of Bingham and Okagaki (2012) and by Mahatmya
these types of intervention strategies decreases et al. (2012). Bingham and Okagaki (2012), in
the likelihood that the most efficient strategies particular, provide an excellent overview of the
will be picked for these two very different types research linking the many contextual features of
of “outcomes.” It also increases the likelihood families, schools, neighborhoods, and peer groups
that people will want evaluators of extracurricu- that both co-occur with ethnic group membership
lar activities to assess standardized test scores as and likely influence engagement, and thus may
a reasonable indicator of the effectiveness of the explain ethnic group differences in both engage-
extracurricular activities, even though there may ment and school success. Mahatmya et al. (2012)
be nothing going on in these activities that discuss the ways in which various contextual
should influence test scores. features in combination with individual-level
6 Part I Commentary: So What Is Student Engagement Anyway? 141

developmental changes might influence age-related down into its operative subcomponents. Such
changes in various indicators of engagement. As is microtheorizing is critical in both fields. We also
clear in each of these chapters, various family and find it very interesting, but not surprising, that the
school contextual features have been shown to be overlap between these more global theoretical
precursors of the psychological processes linked frameworks becomes clearer and more salient as
to both motivation and engagement (see other this microtheorizing proceeds in both frame-
chapters in this volume as well). Similarly, most works. The overlap is particularly salient when
psychological theories of human development one adopts a developmental orientation for one’s
would agree that these psychological processes theorizing: From a general developmental per-
mediate the associations between distal contextual spective, these two general frameworks for
characteristics and school success. At the general understanding school success are very similar in
level, this has to be true, except when it is not. both their explanatory mechanisms and their
The dilemma is finding out when it is true, when it recommendations for general and specific school
is not, and why. The answers to these questions reforms. But even more importantly, this
will come by being very specific about which microtheorizing is making the unique contribu-
A, B, and C constructs are being considered. tions of these two frameworks clearer, as well as
In closing this part, let us stress how much we the ways in which the unique constructs within
sympathize with engagement theorists about each each of these frameworks articulate with each
of these dilemmas because these same issues per- other over developmental time.
vade the field of motivation as well. These are
inherent problems in various models of motivated
behavior as well. We bring it up here just to point
Links Between Engagement
out how difficult these definitional issues can be
and why we need to be as clear as possible about Theory and the Eccles et al.
what we mean by various terms at each point in Expectancy-Value Theory
our discussions. Both engagement and motiva- of Achievement-Related Behaviors
tional theorists within both the educational psy-
chology and the educational sociology traditions Now let us turn to a more specific discussion of
would like to have quite general theories of behav- the link of engagement theory to one quite simi-
ior and achievement in school contexts – theories lar theory of school achievement: the Eccles
that explain a broad range of behaviors and educa- et al. expectancy-value theoretical model
tional outcomes. Certainly, the motivational theo- (EEVT). We focus on this theory because it is
rists associated with three motivational theoretical not discussed to any great extent in any of the
systems dominant today (self-determination the- other chapters in this book. The other major the-
ory, achievement goal theory, and the Eccles et al. ories of motivated behavior in academic settings
expectancy-value theory) have this goal, as do the are discussed extensively in Skinner and Pitzer
theorists associated with engagement theory (2012) and Finn and Zimmer (2012) and the set
described in these five chapters. To have such a of chapters in Part II of this handbook.
general theory, one needs to have very broad defi- There are two ways in which Eccles’ theoretical
nitions of one’s core constructs. Similarly, to frameworks connect to ET. First, the general EEVT
inspire policy makers and educational pundits, model was designed to explain individual and
one needs sexy and overgeneralized core con- group differences in individuals’ decisions to
structs. But to design effective interventions and engage in, and the extent of their actual engage-
move our theoretical understanding forward, we ment in, various achievement-related activities.
need to be much more specific. Here the overlap is quite explicit. The second over-
We see the efforts to articulate the various lap relates to Eccles and Midgley’s extension of
subtypes of “engagement” as akin to the efforts the EEVT into their stage-environment fit theory
in motivation to break the global construct of motivation. We discuss each of these in turn.
142 J. Eccles and M.-T. Wang

Eccles began her career interested in the sive theoretical approach to task choice and
question of why males and females engage in dif- behavioral engagement. The focus on personal
ferent types of achievement-related behaviors in agency led me to expectancy-value theoretical
various spheres of life including school, extracur- perspectives on task choice and task engagement.
ricular activities, educational and occupational The focus on socialization and internalization led
choices, and achievement-related avocational me to a focus on the ways in which external forces
behaviors and contexts. Her initial studies focused and context become a part of the individuals’
on why females were less likely than males to expectations for success and subjective task
aspire to careers in STEM, and as a result, to be values across a wide array of tasks. Finally, the
less likely than males to enroll in advanced math person-environment fit perspective led me to the-
and physical science courses in secondary school orize about those aspects of the context that
and college. To address this question, Eccles and would increase or decrease the expectations
her colleagues (see Eccles [Parsons] et al., 1983; and values of the individuals’ deciding to engage
Meece et al., 1982) developed a socio-cultural in various tasks or contexts. Thus, the person-
embedded expectancy value theoretical model environment fit perspective focused my and Carol
of task choice/engagement – the Eccles et al. Midgley’s attention on the link between the
expectancy-value theoretical model (EEVT) – nature of contexts and the needs of the persons;
and applied it to students’ course-taking deci- we assumed that motivation would be highest
sions. Eccles and her colleagues have since used when the demands of the task fit well with both
this model to guide their research into a much the person’s sense of agency (in this case, their
broader question: Why does anyone do anything? expectations of success) and the values, needs,
Although they typically used terms like behav- and goals of the individual. In this way, the theo-
ioral choices, persistence, and achievement when retical approach adopted by my colleagues and
describing their major dependent measures or me is quite consistent with the approaches guid-
outcomes, Eccles has always included the idea of ing both ET and SDT.
engagement in her set of major dependent mea- The EEVT general model that emerged is illus-
sures. When doing so, she was referring to both trated in Fig. 6.1. Our ultimate goal was to predict
the behavioral and cognitive aspects of engage- task choice and intensity of task engagement. The
ment as defined by Fredericks et al. and to behav- task could be as focused as doing a homework
ioral engagement as defined by Skinner et al. assignment or as broad as being engaged at school
rather than to sense of belonging to the institution with sufficient intensity to lead to graduation. We
in which the activities are placed. I (Eccles) would argued that such engagements would be directly
put emotional engagement as either an antecedent predicted by two major psychological constructs:
of behavioral/cognitive engagement (i.e., antici- expectations of success and perceived/subjective
pated positive or negative emotional arousal task value. Thus, the most proximal precursors of
resulting from engagement) or an emotional engagement where beliefs that are commonly
reaction to being engaged in doing the task. thought to be part of modern achievement motiva-
My (Eccles) general perspective was heavily tional theories. We then specified the more distal
influenced by three broad theoretical perspec- psychological and social processes likely to influ-
tives: (1) The life course view that both personal ence these proximal beliefs. Thus, like Skinner
agency and social structure are prime forces in and Pitzer (2012), Finn and Zimmer (2012) and
life span development; (2) the social processes Bingham and Okagaki (2012), most of our theo-
underlying socialization and internalization; and rizing has focused on the precursors of engage-
(3) the person-environment fit perspective that ment rather than on the definition of engagement
people fare best and are likely to be most engaged itself. We put both engagement and achievement
when they are in contexts that meet their psycho- in our outcome box. However, like both engage-
logical needs. I felt that bringing together these ment and SDT theorists (e.g., Finn & Zimmer,
three perspectives would provide a comprehen- 2012; Reschly & Christenson, 2012; Skinner &
6 Part I Commentary: So What Is Student Engagement Anyway? 143

Cultural Milieu Individual's Perception of… Individual's Goals, Explicit Expectation of Success /
1. Gender role Motivations, and Personal Efficacies
1. Socializer's beliefs, General Self-Schemata
stereotypes expectations, attitudes,
2. Cultural and behaviors 1. Self-schemata -
stereotypes 2. Gender roles personal and social
of subject matter identities
3. Activity stereotypes
and occupational 2. Short-term goals
characteristics and task demands
3. Long-term goals Achievement-Related
3. Family 4. Ideal self
Demographics Choices and Performance
5. Self-concept of one's
abilities

Socializer's
Beliefs and
Behaviors

Stable Individual
Characteristics Such Individual's Affective
as Reactions and Subjective Task Value
1. Aptitudes, Memories
Temperament
1. Interest -enjoyment value
2. Gender 2. Attainment value
Individual's 3. Utility value
Interpretations 4. Relative cost
Previous
Achievement-
Related
Experiences

Across Time

Fig. 6.1 Eccles et al. Expectancy-value model of achievement behaviors

Pitzer, 2012; Finn & Zimmer, 2012), we consider increase identification with or attachment to the
our larger perspective to be a general theory of institutional settings in which such activities take
motivated behavior that helps understand individ- place. We include such experiences in the box
ual and group differences in school achievement. labeled affective memories and the link of these
We differ more in the labels for specific constructs memories to the box labeled self-related beliefs.
than in the overarching goal of the theoretical We further assume that these beliefs increase the
system. subjective task value of engaging in tasks in such
Perhaps the aspect of EEVT that overlaps settings.
most with ET is the role of affect and of determi- We also believe that subjective task value is
nants of subjective task value. Because we are composed of beliefs about how enjoyable the
developmentalists, we see affect as both a precur- task will be, how useful the task is for fulfilling
sor and a consequence of engagement rather than one’s various short- and long-term goals, how
as a part of engagement. Like Finn in his partici- well the task helps one manifest one’s personal
pation-identification model (1989), we proposed needs and both personal and social identities, and
that success (or failure) experiences in various finally how much engaging in the task costs in
settings create emotional reactions, which over terms of time, effort, energy, external assets, and
time accumulate within the person to form stable the ability to engage in other tasks either more or
positive (or negative) feelings about similar set- less directly related to one’s personal and social
tings and activities, which, in turn both raise (or needs and goals. These same ideas are evident in
lower) the value one attaches to these settings and, all of the five chapters in this part. For example,
to the extent that these association become part of they overlap with (1) what Skinner and Pitzer
the person’s core self-beliefs or self-schema, (2012) include under emotion and cognitive
144 J. Eccles and M.-T. Wang

orientation in Fig. 2.2; (2) what both Finn and Our own longitudinal data from the 1980s showed
Zimmer (2012) and Reschly and Christenson major declines in the students’ expectations for
(2012) include under the rubrics of affective and interest in various school subjects as they
engagement, cognitive engagement, and school moved from elementary school into secondary
identification; and (3) what Martin 2012 (chapter school. Carol Midgley and I became intrigued by
in this volume) refers to as adaptive cognitions. what might be going on to explain these differ-
However, EEVT places greater emphasis on the ences. We decided that the decline reflected
role of both personal and social identities and changes in the kinds of experiences the students
short- and long-term goals as key mediators of were having in their classrooms. We predicted
engagement through their influence on the indi- that students on average experienced what we
vidual’s hierarchies of subjective task values. The called a developmentally inappropriate shift in
central role of personal and social identities the types of classroom and teacher characteristics
has become a major focus on my work over the as they moved into secondary school, leading to
last 10–15 years (see Eccles 2009), as I have increasingly poor fit between the needs of many
extended the model to look at racial identities as adolescents and the opportunities provided by
well as more personal identities that underlie their school. We called this perspective stage-
what we labeled attainment value. By focusing environment fit or misfit (Eccles & Midgley,
on these dimensions of the self, we have 1989; Eccles et al., 1993).
expanded the range of self-related characteris- Much like SDT, we argued that individuals
tics that might influence engagement through thrive best in classrooms that meet their personal
their impact on perceived person-environment and social needs. Like SDT, we argued (1) that
fit and thus subjective task value. students have a strong need to feel competent if
We also place greater emphasis than such moti- they are to maintain high expectations for suc-
vational theoretical frameworks as SDT or AGT cess and (2) that students need to have positive
on the social or more distal precursors of engage- relationships with their teachers for a variety of
ment. In this way, we are more like Bingham reasons. We felt that maintaining this sense of
and Okagaki (2012) in our orientation than we are competence would become more difficult for
like Skinner and Pitzer (2012), and Finn and many adolescents, particularly in large schools
Zimmer (2012). However, like Skinner and Pitzer where their teachers are working with so many
(2012), Finn and Zimmer (2012), Mahatmya et al. other students, because their academic deficits
(2012), and Reschly and Christenson (2012), we would make mastery of the material being taught
are very interested in interventions designed to more difficult. We felt that this problem would be
increase engagement. This has lead us to focus, in further exacerbated by an increased focus on
the various extensions of the EEVT, on those social comparative grading. Similar to SDT, we
characteristics of the school, family, and peer also argued that adolescents, in particular, have a
groups that influence school-related task engage- growing need for greater autonomy and self-
ment through increasing the perceived subjective direction, while at the same a growing need to
task value of these tasks or activities. Like have close relationships with nonfamilial adults.
Mahatmya et al. (2012), we have been particularly We also argued that they need the chance to try
interested in those aspects of the changing school new, challenging tasks, activities, and contexts in
context that may underlie the declines in many an environment that also provides a strong safety
students’ interest in engaging in school academic- net. Finally, we argued that the emergence of
related task as they move into and through pri- more well-articulated personal and social identi-
mary and secondary school. ties would increase the need for opportunities to
This concern with social precursors lead explore activities directly related to these identi-
Eccles and Midgley to take a closer look at devel- ties – thus increasing the importance of the rele-
opmental changes in both expectations for success vance of the material they were learning for the
in and the subjective task value associated with subjective task value they would attach to various
the learning aspects of classroom experiences. courses and activities in the secondary school
6 Part I Commentary: So What Is Student Engagement Anyway? 145

setting. Directly related, the emergence of more


well-articulated social identities would increase References
the salience of ethnic relevance and peer group
Bingham, G. E., & Okagaki, L. (2012). Ethnicity and
norms. Thus, like Bingham and Okagaki (2012), student engagement. In S. L. Christenson, A. L.
we argued that ethnic compatibility and sensitivi- Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on
ties would become increasingly important during student engagement (pp. 65–95). New York: Springer.
Eccles, J. (2009). Who am I and what am I going to do
the secondary school years. Many of these same
with my life? Personal and collective identities as
points were discussed by Lohman et al. (2012). motivators of action. Educational Psychologist, 44(2),
Thus, we see many overlaps between the work 78–89.
associated with Eccles and her colleagues and the Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. M. (1989). Stage/environment
fit: Developmentally appropriate classrooms for early
work being done by theorists in the engagement
adolescents. In R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research
theory tradition. on motivation in education: Goals and cognitions
(Vol. 3, pp. 139–186). New York: Academic Press.
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Buchanan, C. M., Wigfield, A.,
Reuman, D., & Mac Iver, D. (1993). Development
during adolescence: The impact of stage/environment
General Conclusions fit. American Psychologist, 48, 90–101.
Eccles (Parsons), J., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B.,
In summary, there is substantial overlap across the Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., & Midgely, C. (1983).
Expectancies, values and academic behaviors. In
various general theories of motivated behavior in J. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motiva-
the school context discussed in these five chap- tion (pp. 75–146). San Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Co.
ters, in the other chapters in this handbook, and in Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
our commentary. At the most general level, this Educational Research, 59, 117–142.
Finn, J. D., & Zimmer, K. (2012). Student engagement:
overlap suggests that we are converging on a What is it? Why does it matter? In S. L. Christenson,
shared theoretical framework across both the psy- A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of
chology and the sociology of education. Thus, it is research on student engagement (pp. 97–131). New
not surprising that the measures used to assess York: Springer.
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
engagement itself as well as its precursors and School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
consequences are quite similar to the measures the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
used to assess various key constructs in these 59–109.
other theories – a point well made by Reschly and Mahatmya, M., Lohman, B. J., Matjasko, J. L., & Farb, A.
F. (2012). Engagement across developmental periods.
Christenson (2012) and by Finn and Zimmer In S. L.Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
(2012). Finally, and also not surprisingly, much of Handbook of research on student engagement
the research presented in these chapters provides (pp. 45–63). New York: Springer.
support for the basic tenets of each of the theories Meece, J. L., Eccles-Parsons, J., Kaczala, C. M., Goff, S.
E., & Futterman, R. (1982). Sex differences in math
of motivated behavior discussed in this commen- achievement: Toward a model of academic choice.
tary and in the other chapters in this book. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 324–348.
At the more specific level, it is important that Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2012). Jingle, jan-
we compare and contrast the details of each of gle, and conceptual haziness: Evolution and future
directions of the engagement construct. In S. L.
these theories in order to determine the most pow- Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
erful and the most easily controlled influences on Handbook of research on student engagement
school engagement. It is also important that we (pp. 3–19). New York: Springer.
find the places where these theories and approaches Skinner, E. A., & Pitzer, J. R. (2012). Developmental
dynamics of student engagement, coping, and every-
make different predictions and then test these day resilience. In S. L.Christenson, A. L. Reschly, &
varying hypotheses. Such research is critical to C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student
both the research and policy/practice communi- engagement (pp. 21–44). New York: Springer.
ties. Thus, it is critical that we focus on both the Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., &
Davis-Kean, P. (2006). Development of achievement
similarities and the differences among these vari- motivation. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child
ous theoretical approaches as we move forward psychology (6th edition, Vol. 3, pp. 933–1002). New
on both the theoretical and applied fronts. York: Wiley.
Part II
Engagement as Linked to Motivational
Variables
A Self-determination Theory
Perspective on Student Engagement* 7
Johnmarshall Reeve

Abstract
This chapter pursues three goals. First, it overviews self-determination
theory (SDT). SDT is a macrotheory of motivation comprised of five inter-
related minitheories—basic needs theory, organismic integration theory,
goal contents theory, cognitive evaluation theory, and causality orienta-
tions theory. Each minitheory was created to explain specific motivational
phenomena and to address specific research questions. Second, the chapter
uses the student-teacher dialectical framework within SDT to explain how
classroom conditions sometimes support but other times neglect and frus-
trate students’ motivation, engagement, and positive classroom function-
ing. Third, the chapter highlights student engagement. In doing so, it
overviews recent classroom-based, longitudinally designed research to
reveal three new and important functions of student engagement—namely,
that student engagement fully mediates and explains the motivation-to-
achievement relation, that changes in engagement produce changes in the
learning environment, and that changes in engagement produce changes in
motivation, as students’ behavioral, emotional, cognitive, and agentic
engagements represent actions taken not only to learn but also to meet
psychological needs. The chapter concludes with implications for teachers
and with suggestions for future research.

*This research was supported by the WCU (World Class


Watch as a student engages herself in a learning
University) Program funded by the Korean Ministry of activity. What catches your eye? What really
Education, Science and Technology, consigned to the matters in terms of whether she will learn some-
Korea Science and Engineering Foundation (Grant no. thing new or develop her skills? What unseen
R32-2008-000-20023-0).
motivational processes contribute to and explain
J. Reeve (*) her attention, effort, emotionality, strategic think-
Department of Education, Korea University,
Anam-Dong, Seongbuk-Gu Seoul, 136-701 Korea ing, and sense of initiative and agency? As you
e-mail: reeve@korea.ac.kr watch the ebb and flow of her engagement, can

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 149


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_7, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
150 J. Reeve

you predict what will happen next? Can you pre- 2. What overarching framework or theory do
dict what she will and will not do in the coming you use to study and explain engagement or
minute? If you were to provide some assistance motivation?
(the way a teacher might), what would you say? 3. What is the role of context in explaining
What would you do? engagement or motivation?
Using the eyes and experience of the teacher
and using the theory and tools of the researcher,
the present chapter reflects on these questions to What Is Engagement?
pursue three goals. First, the chapter overviews What Is Motivation?
self-determination theory (SDT). SDT is a theory How Do You Differentiate the Two?
of motivation that helps researchers and practi-
tioners alike to understand and enhance not only Engagement refers to the extent of a student’s
student motivation but also the engagement that active involvement in a learning activity, a defini-
arises out of that motivation. It is a macrotheory tion borrowed from Wellborn’s (1991) pioneer-
of motivation comprised of five interrelated mini- ing work on the subject. The definitional emphasis
theories, including basic needs theory, organis- on “learning activity” is important because the
mic integration theory, goal contents theory, present chapter focuses rather narrowly on
cognitive evaluation theory, and causality orien- engagement as a task- or domain-specific event,
tations theory (Ryan & Deci, 2002; Vansteenkiste, as the student is engaged in a particular learning
Niemiec, & Soenens, 2010). Second, using an activity (for a matter of minutes) or in a particular
SDT perspective, the chapter explains how class- course (for a matter of months).
room conditions sometimes support but other Engagement is a multidimensional construct.
times interfere with students’ motivation, engage- As depicted in Fig. 7.1, engagement features four
ment, and positive school functioning. The focus distinct, but highly intercorrelated, aspects. Each of
in this discussion will be on the student-teacher these four aspects will be discussed in depth in the
dialectical framework that is embedded within an chapter, but for now recall the observational episode
SDT analysis. Third, the chapter highlights stu- from the opening paragraph in which you might
dent engagement. Recent classroom-based, lon- find yourself observing a student reading, practic-
gitudinally designed research has produced ing, or playing. From the perspective of the present
several new and important insights into defining, chapter, making a judgment of how actively
understanding, and promoting students’ engage- involved the student was in the learning activity
ment. In the discussion of these new findings, would involve assessments of her concentration,
particular emphasis will be paid to the functions attention, and effort (behavioral engagement),
of student engagement. the presence of task-facilitating emotions such
as interest and the absence of task-withdrawing
emotions such as distress (emotional engagement),
Three Questions her usage of sophisticated rather than superficial
learning strategies (cognitive engagement), and
To help readers compare, contrast, and integrate the extent to which she tries to enrich the learning
the various chapters of the Handbook of Research experience rather than just passively receive it as a
on Student Engagement, the editors asked each given (agentic engagement).
contributing author team to address the following
three questions: Motivation refers to any force that energizes
1. What are your definitions of engagement and and directs behavior (Reeve, 2009a). Energy
motivation, and how do you differentiate gives behavior its strength, intensity, and persis-
between the two? tence. Direction gives behavior its purpose and
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 151

Engagement
during a Learning Activity

Behavioral Emotional Cognitive Agentic


Engagement Engagement Engagement Engagement

• On-task attention • Presence of task-facilitating • Use of sophisticated, deep, • Proactive, intentional, and
and concentration. emotions (e.g., interest, and personalized learning constructive contribution
curiosity, and enthusiasm). strategies (e.g., elaboration). into the flow of the learning
• High effort. activity (e.g., offering
• Absence of task-withdrawing • Seeking conceptual input, making suggestions).
• High task persistence. emotions (e.g., distress, anger, understanding rather than
frustration, anxiety, and fear). surface knowledge. • Enriching the learning
activity, rather than
• Use of self-regulatory passively receiving
strategies (e.g., planning). it as a given.

Fig. 7.1 Four interrelated aspects of students’ engagement during a learning activity

goal-directedness. While motivation arises from motivation and engagement are inherently linked
many different sources (e.g., needs, cognitions, (each influences the other), those who study
emotions, environmental events), it is viewed motivation are interested in engagement mostly
in the present chapter from a needs-based per- as an outcome of motivational processes, whereas
spective within the self-determination theory those who study engagement are interested
framework; hence, motivation is equated with mostly in motivation as a source of engagement.
students’ psychological need satisfaction. That So, motivation is the relatively more private, sub-
is, students who perceive themselves to be jectively experienced cause, while engagement is
acting with a sense of autonomy, competence, the relatively more public, objectively observed
and relatedness during the learning activity effect.
experience high-quality motivation, while those
who have these three needs neglected or frus-
trated during instruction experience low-quality What Overarching Framework Informs
motivation. Your Understanding of Motivation
The distinction between the two constructs is and Engagement?
that motivation is a private, unobservable psy-
chological, neural, and biological process that Self-determination theory provides the overarch-
serves as an antecedent cause to the publically ing theoretical framework to guide my research
observable behavior that is engagement. While questions and empirical study of both motivation
152 J. Reeve

and engagement. A particular emphasis is placed social context → motivation → engagement →


on the student-teacher dialectical framework outcomes) because one also needs to think about
embedded within SDT. Both of these frameworks these relations in a reciprocal way.
will be presented in the present chapter.

Self-determination Theory
What Is the Role of Context in
Explaining Engagement or Motivation? Self-determination theory (SDT) is a theory of
motivation that uses traditional empirical meth-
Students have needs, goals, interests, and values ods to build its theory and to inform its classroom
of their own, and these motivations sometimes applications. The theory, which has been 40 years
manifest themselves in a context-free way, as in the making, assumes that all students, no mat-
when a student adopts a mastery goal orientation ter their age, gender, socioeconomic status,
across all achievement contexts. These motiva- nationality, or cultural background, possess inher-
tions also express themselves when student are ent growth tendencies (e.g., intrinsic motivation,
alone, as when an adolescent clicks on a web page, curiosity, psychological needs) that provide a
finds it interesting, and reads about the topic for motivational foundation for their high-quality
hours, all in the privacy of his or her personal time classroom engagement and positive school func-
on a computer. When students are in the class- tioning (Deci & Ryan, 1985a, 2000; Reeve, Deci,
room, however, context matters. In the classroom, & Ryan, 2004; Ryan & Deci, 2000, 2002;
students live and interact in a social world that Vansteenkiste et al., 2010). While other motiva-
offers supports for and threats against their needs, tion theories explain how students’ expectations,
goals, interests, and values. In the classroom, the beliefs, and goals contribute to their classroom
teacher and the learning environment are so instru- engagement, self-determination theory is unique
mental in supporting versus frustrating student in that it emphasizes the instructional task of
motivation and engagement that it almost does not vitalizing students’ inner motivational resources
make sense to refer to “student” engagement as the key step in facilitating high-quality engage-
because it cannot be separated or disentangled ment (Reeve & Halusic, 2009). That is, SDT
from the social context in which it occurs. That is, identifies the inner motivational resources that all
every student’s classroom engagement is invari- students possess, and it offers recommendations
ably a joint product of his or her motivation and as to how teachers can involve, nurture, and vital-
classroom supports versus thwarts. ize these resources during the flow of instruction
This view on the role of context in motivation to facilitate high-quality student engagement
and engagement foreshadows three implications. (Niemiec & Ryan, 2009).
First, to flourish, student motivation and student The theory acknowledges that students some-
engagement need supportive conditions, espe- times lack self-motivation, display disaffection,
cially supportive student-teacher relationships. and act irresponsibly. To resolve this seeming par-
Second, the role of the teacher (or the classroom adox of possessing inner motivational resources
context more generally) is not to create or manu- on the one hand yet displaying disaffection on the
facture student motivation or engagement. Rather, other, SDT research identifies the classroom con-
the teacher’s role is to support the student moti- ditions that support and vitalize students’ inner
vation and engagement that is already there and motivational resources versus those that neglect,
to do so in a way that allows for high- (rather than undermine, and thwart them (Deci & Ryan, 1985a;
low-) quality motivation and engagement. Third, Reeve et al., 2004; Ryan & Deci, 2000). In doing
it is only partially valid to think of the relations so, SDT addresses how students’ inner resources
among social context, motivation, engagement, interact with classroom conditions to result in
and student outcomes in a linear fashion (i.e., varying levels of students’ engagement.
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 153

Self-Determination Theory

Basic Needs Organismic Goal Contents Cognitive Evaluation Causality


Theory Integration Theory Theory Theory Orientations Theory

Elaborates the concept Introduces types of Distinguishes intrinsic Predicts the effect that Identifies individual
of psychological needs extrinsic motivation. goals from extrinsic any external event will differences in how
and specifies their Specifies the goals to explain how have on intrinsic students motivate and
relation to intrinsic antecedents, the former supports motivation. Explains engage themselves. To
motivation, high- characteristics, and psychological needs why some external engage themselves,
quality engagement, consequences of each and well-being events support some students rely on
effective functioning, type. Explains whereas the latter autonomy, competence, autonomous guides to
and psychological students’ successful neglects these needs and intrinsic motivation, action while others rely
well-being. versus unsuccessful and fosters ill being. while other events on controlling and
academic socialization. interfere with and thwart environmental guides.
these motivations.

Fig. 7.2 Five minitheories of self-determination theory and the motivational phenomena each was developed to
explain

As SDT research has advanced, different processes, as external events sometimes support
motivational phenomena (e.g., intrinsic motiva- but other times interfere with and thwart students’
tion) and different research questions (e.g., how psychological needs and perceptions of autonomy
do extrinsic rewards affect intrinsic motivation?) and competence. Causality orientations theory
have emerged and required empirical study. As highlights individual differences in how students
summarized in Fig. 7.2, five minitheories emerged motivate themselves. To initiate and sustain their
to explain these motivational phenomena and to classroom engagement, some students tend to
answer their associated research questions. Basic rely on autonomous and self-determined guides
needs theory focuses on psychological needs as to action, while others tend to rely on controlled
inherent inner motivational resources and speci- and environmentally determined guides.
fies their foundational and nutriment-like relation
to students’ motivation, high-quality engagement,
effective functioning, and psychological well- Basic Needs Theory
being. Organismic integration theory focuses on
internalization and why students initiate socially Basic needs theory identifies the three psycho-
important, but not intrinsically motivating, behav- logical needs of autonomy, competence, and
iors. It represents SDT’s theory of extrinsic moti- relatedness as the source of students’ inherent
vation, specifies types of extrinsic motivation, and proactive intrinsically motivated tendency to
and explains students successful versus unsuc- seek out novelty, pursue optimal challenge, exer-
cessful academic socialization. Goal contents cise and extend their capabilities, explore, and
theory focuses on the “what” of motivation— learn. Autonomy is the psychological need to
what goals students strive for—to distinguish experience behavior as emanating from and as
intrinsic from extrinsic goals. This minitheory endorsed by the self; it is the inner endorsement
explains how intrinsic goals support psychologi- of one’s behavior (Deci & Ryan, 1985a). Students
cal need satisfaction and foster well-being and experience autonomy need satisfaction to the
also how extrinsic goals neglect psychological extent to which their classroom activity affords
needs and foster ill-being. Cognitive evaluation them opportunities to engage in learning activi-
theory explains how external events (e.g., ties with an internal locus of causality, sense of
rewards, feedback) affect intrinsic motivational psychological freedom, and perceived choice
154 J. Reeve

over their actions (Reeve, Nix, & Hamm, 2003). Organismic Integration Theory
Competence is the need to be effective in one’s
pursuits and interactions with the environment. It Organismic integration theory recognizes that
reflects the inherent desire to exercise one’s students engage in many behaviors that are inher-
capacities and, in doing so, to seek out and mas- ently interesting or enjoyable. It is probably the
ter environmental challenges (Deci, 1975). case that most behaviors students engage in at
Relatedness is the need to establish close emo- school are extrinsically motivated and enacted as
tional bonds and secure attachments with others. a means to an outcome that is separate from the
It reflects the desire to be emotionally connected engaged task itself, such as practicing a musical
to and interpersonally involved in warm, caring, instrument to develop skill or to please a teacher,
and responsive relationships (Deci & Ryan, rather than simply enjoying the playing itself.
1991). Students experience relatedness need sat- Organismic integration theory explains under
isfaction to the extent to which they relate to oth- which conditions students do, do not, and only
ers in an authentic, caring, and reciprocal way sort of acquire, internalize, and integrate extrin-
(Ryan, 1993). sic motivational processes into the self-motiva-
Basic needs theory contributes to the over- tion system. It proposes that students are naturally
arching SDT framework in three important ways. and volitionally inclined to internalize aspects of
First, basic needs theory identifies the origin of their social surroundings and to integrate some of
students’ active nature in the three psychological these values and ways of behaving as acquired
needs (Deci & Ryan, 2000). In this way, basic motivations. That is, students want to internalize
needs theory presents psychological need satis- societal norms, rules, and behaviors; indeed, they
faction as its unifying principle (Vansteenkiste proactively seek out such opportunities. The
et al., 2010), as psychological needs energize motivation to internalize societal prescriptions
engagement and are conceptualized as psycho- (“do this”) and proscriptions (“don’t do that”)
logical nutriments that the daily life events need exists because students naturally want to discover
to fulfill if one is to be psychologically, physi- new ways to increase their competence in the
cally, and socially well. Second, basic needs the- social world and to relate the self more closely to
ory explains why students sometimes show active others (e.g., shared values, shared goals, greater
engagement in learning activities but other times sense of community). To the extent that students
show a passive or even antagonistic involvement, internalize and integrate healthy external regula-
as need satisfaction promotes active engagement, tions (i.e., achieve “organismic integration”),
whereas the neglect and thwarting of these needs they experience greater autonomy and show rela-
anticipates various manifestations of disaffection tively more positive functioning in the relevant
(Deci & Ryan, 2000; Reis, Sheldon, Gable, domain, including the school context (Ryan,
Roscoe, & Ryan, 2000; Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1993; Ryan & Connell, 1989).
1996). Third, the three needs provide the basis According to organismic integration theory,
for predicting a priori which aspects of the class- extrinsic motivation (unlike intrinsic motivation)
room environment will be supportive versus is a differentiated construct. Different types of
undermining of students’ engagement—namely, extrinsic motivation are associated with different
those conditions that affect students’ perceptions degrees of autonomous motivation. To be auton-
of autonomy, competence, and relatedness (Deci, omous is not so much to be free from external
Koestner, & Ryan, 1999). This study of how forces; rather, students experience autonomy in
teachers support students’ autonomy has been accordance with how much they personally
the foundation of my own program of research endorse the value and significance of the way of
on teachers’ autonomy-supportive versus con- thinking or behaving. Because students feel vary-
trolling motivating styles (e.g., Reeve, 2009b) ing degrees of ownership of their beliefs and
as well as others’ research on competence sup- behaviors, the four types of extrinsic motivation
port and relatedness support (e.g., Skinner & can be conceptualized along a unipolar contin-
Belmont, 1993). uum of autonomous motivation.
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 155

External regulation is the least autonomous complements basic needs theory in the overall
type of extrinsic motivation. It exists as a SDT framework, as basic needs theory identifies
contingency-based “in order to” type of motiva- students’ inherent motivational resources,
tion in which the student engages in an activity whereas organismic integration theory identifies
in order to obtain a reward or in order to avoid a students’ acquired motivational resources.
punishment. With external regulation, the per-
sonal value of the behavior itself is very low.
Introjected regulation is slightly autonomous Goal Contents Theory
extrinsic motivation. With introjected regula-
tion, the student complies with external requests Organismic integration theory was created to
to affirm or maintain self-worth in the eyes of answer questions of why people engage in unin-
others or to silence a self-esteem threat (avoid teresting activities—why does he study? Why
feeling guilty or ashamed). Both external regu- does he participate in class? Why does he do his
lation and introjected regulation are associated homework? In contrast, goal contents theory was
with an external perceived locus of causality, created to answer questions of what people strive
sense of pressure, and perceived obligation (i.e., to attain—what is his goal while studying? What
controlled motivation). Moving up the ladder of is her goal as she participates in class? Goal con-
autonomous types of extrinsic motivation, iden- tents theory arose out of the distinction between
tified regulation represents an autonomous type intrinsic and extrinsic goals (or intrinsic and
of extrinsic motivation. With identified regula- extrinsic aspirations) and out of the finding that
tion, the student sees value in the external regu- the differing goal content affects motivation and
lation (“that is important, useful”) and willingly well-being in different ways (Ryan, Sheldon,
transforms it into a self-endorsed (internalized) Kasser, & Deci, 1996; Vansteenkiste, Lens, &
regulation that has a sense of choice and per- Deci, 2006). Specifically, engagement in pursuit
sonal commitment behind it. Integrated regula- of intrinsic goals such as personal growth and
tion is the most autonomous type of extrinsic deeper interpersonal relationships affords basic
motivation. It occurs as the student evaluates need satisfactions and thus enhances effort and
and brings an identification into coherence with psychological well-being, whereas engagement
other aspects of the self-system, as when “study- in pursuit of extrinsic goals such as enhanced sta-
ing hard on this assignment” is brought into the tus, increased popularity, or material success
“I’m a scholar” sense of self. Integrated regula- neglects basic need satisfactions and therefore
tion approximates intrinsic motivation in its foreshadows ill-being (e.g., anxiety, depression,
degree of self-determination, though the two and physical symptoms).
motivational constructs clearly differ, as inte- Importantly, engagement in pursuit of extrin-
grated regulation is based on the importance of sic goals undermines learning and well-being
the activity and requires considerable reflection even for those who actually attain their extrinsic
and self-awareness, whereas intrinsic motiva- goals (Niemiec, Ryan, & Deci, 2009; Vansteen-
tion is based on interest in the activity and kiste et al., 2006; Vansteenkiste, Timmermans,
emerges spontaneously. Both identified regula- Lens, Soenens, & Van den Broeck, 2008). So,
tion and integrated regulation are associated psychological need satisfaction and psychologi-
with an internal perceived locus of causality, cal well-being do not depend so much on whether
sense of psychological freedom, and perceived people attain the goals they seek as much as they
choice (i.e., autonomous motivation). depend on what people seek to attain in the first
In adding organismic integration theory to the place—intrinsic or extrinsic goal content. This
SDT framework, SDT ceased contrasting intrin- conclusion stands in contrast to all other theories
sic motivation against extrinsic motivation and of student motivation that argue that the pursuit
now distinguishes between autonomous motiva- and attainment of valued goals is central to
tion and controlled motivation (Vansteenkiste students’ psychological well-being (e.g., expec-
et al., 2010). Organismic integration theory nicely tancy x valence theory, social cognitive theory).
156 J. Reeve

According to goal contents theory, the pursuit events preserve autonomy and maintain intrinsic
and attainment of intrinsic goals fosters deeper motivation. The informational aspect of an exter-
learning, better performance, enduring persis- nal event communicates competence feedback.
tence, and greater psychological well-being than That is, students experience a reward as an infor-
does the pursuit of extrinsic goals (Vansteenkiste, mational event if the reward is offered to com-
Simons, Lens, Sheldon, & Deci, 2004a; municate competent or improved functioning
Vansteenkiste et al., 2004b; Vansteenkiste et al., (e.g., “Because your punctuality has improved so
2006) because intrinsic goals tap into and vitalize much, you have earned a special privilege.”).
students’ inner motivational resources in ways Informational, competence-enhancing external
that extrinsic goals do not. The pursuit and attain- events increase intrinsic motivation, whereas
ment of extrinsic goals does not foster these competence-undermining external events (e.g.,
motivational and well-being benefits and is, in criticism) decrease it.
fact, typically counterproductive. Cognitive evaluation theory is a crucial mini-
theory in the overall SDT framework (and the
first to emerge; Deci & Ryan, 1980) because it
Cognitive Evaluation Theory specifies how classroom conditions can enhance
and support students’ intrinsic motivational
Cognitive evaluation theory explains how and processes or undermine and thwart them. For
why external events such as rewards or praise instance, some common classroom autonomy
affect intrinsic motivation. Intrinsically moti- thwarts are surveillance (Lepper & Greene,
vated behaviors are those that are initiated and 1975), deadlines (Amabile, DeJong, & Lepper,
maintained by the spontaneous satisfactions stu- 1976), imposed rules and limits (Koestner, Ryan,
dents experience while engaged in an activity. Bernieri, & Holt, 1984), imposed goals
The two inherent satisfactions within intrinsic (Mossholder, 1980), directives/commands (Reeve
motivation are feeling autonomous and feeling & Jang, 2006), competition (Deci, Betley, Kahle,
competent, though relatedness satisfaction may Abrams, & Porac, 1981), and evaluation (Ryan,
also play a role in intrinsic motivation (Ryan & 1982). Some common classroom autonomy and
Deci, 2000). According to cognitive evaluation competence supports are choice (Katz & Assor,
theory, any external event that affects students’ 2007), opportunities for self-direction (Reeve
perceived autonomy or perceived competence et al., 2003), explanatory rationales (Reeve, Jang,
will necessarily affect their intrinsic motivation. Hardre, & Omura, 2002), acknowledgement of
According to the theory, all external events feelings (Koestner et al., 1984), encouragement
(e.g., tests, rewards, grades, scholarships, dead- (Reeve & Jang, 2006), and positive feedback
lines, written feedback on a paper) have two (Ryan). As will become a crucial point later in the
functional aspects: a controlling aspect and an chapter, the interpersonal climate in which the
informational aspect (Deci & Ryan, 1980, 1985a), external event is administered—autonomy sup-
and it is the relative salience of the controlling portive or controlling—predicts additional impor-
versus informational aspect of the event that tant variance in intrinsic motivation, even to the
determines its effect on intrinsic motivation. The point that the same external event will have dif-
controlling aspect of an external event pressures ferent effects on intrinsic motivation when applied
students toward a specific outcome or toward a in an autonomy-supportive versus controlling
specific way of behaving. That is, students expe- way—for instance, autonomy-supportive versus
rience a reward as a controlling event if the controlling praise (Ryan), autonomy-supportive
reward is offered in exchange for compliant versus controlling rewards (Ryan, Mims, &
behavior (e.g., “If you come to class on time, Koestner, 1983), autonomy-supportive versus
then I will give you a special privilege.”). controlling limits on behavior (Koestner et al.,
Controlling external events diminish intrinsic 1984), and autonomy-supportive versus control-
motivation, whereas noncontrolling external ling competitions (Reeve & Deci, 1996).
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 157

Causality Orientations Theory endorse a highly controlled causality orientation


as they rely heavily on external regulation and
Causality orientations theory describes personality- introjected regulation as sources of motivation
level individual differences in students’ orienta- but only lightly or occasionally on intrinsic moti-
tions toward the motivational forces that cause vation, integrated regulation, and identified
their behavior (Deci & Ryan, 1985b). In the regulation.
classroom, some students tend to adopt an orien- Individual differences in causality orientations
tation in which they rely on autonomous or self- are important because they foreshadow students’
determined guides—interests, personal goals, adjustment outcomes, as students with autonomy
and self-endorsed values—for the initiation and causality orientations tend to display greater self-
regulation of their classroom activity, while other esteem, greater self-awareness, more mature ego
students tend to adopt an orientation in which development, and less self-derogation, while
they rely on controlling guides—environmental students with control causality orientations tend
incentives, social prescriptions, and pressuring to display greater daily stress, defensiveness,
internal language—for the initiation and regula- and public self-consciousness (Deci & Ryan,
tion of their classroom activity. To the extent that 1985b). By adding the personality perspective to
students rely on self-determined sources of moti- complement the other four minitheories, causal-
vation to guide their plans and actions, they ity orientations theory completes the overall
embrace an autonomy causality orientation; to SDT framework.
the extent that students rely on controlled sources
of motivation to guide their plans and actions,
they embrace a control causality orientation. Student-Teacher Dialectical
Whereas cognitive evaluation theory reflects Framework Within SDT
the social psychology of self-determination
theory, causality orientations theory reflects a The starting point to understand student motiva-
personality approach. In examining students’ tion and engagement within a SDT perspective is
motivation and engagement from a personality to appreciate that students possess inner motiva-
perspective, it is important to distinguish students’ tional resources that allow them to be fully capa-
causality orientation dispositions from other types ble of engaging themselves constructively in the
of personality dispositions such as the widely learning environment. The learning environment,
studied Big Five personality traits. Whereas the in turn, features conditions that tend either to sup-
Big Five traits are stable and biologically rooted port or to thwart the inner motivational resources
core dimensions of personality, causality orienta- that students bring with them as they walk into the
tions are surface individual differences that are school and into the classroom. Hence, student
relatively malleable and influenced by socializa- motivation and the learning environment affect
tion experiences (Vansteenkiste et al., 2010). one another, as students tap into their inherent
Also, causality orientations theory suggests that motivational resources to change the learning
each student possesses both causality orientations environment even as they simultaneously receive
within the personality. What makes individual and internalize new sources of motivation from
differences in causality orientations is the relative the learning environment. This reciprocal relation
degree to which the two causality orientations are between student and teacher lies at the center of the
endorsed, as some students dispositionally student-teacher dialectical framework within SDT.
endorse a highly autonomous causality orienta- To the extent that students are able to express
tion as they rely heavily on intrinsic motivation, themselves, pursue their interests and values, and
integrated regulation, and identified regulation as acquire constructive new sources of motivation,
sources of motivation but only lightly or occa- the dialectical outcome of student-teacher interac-
sionally on external regulation and introjected tions will be synthesis, resulting in greater student
regulation, while other students dispositionally autonomy, engagement, and well-being. But if
158 J. Reeve

High-Quality Student Engagement


during Learning Activities

Quality of Student Motivation Learning Environment

Inherent Sources Acquired Sources Relationships External Events


of Motivation of Motivation
Unequal Statuses External Events
Relationship Rewards
Intrinsic Motivation Self-Endorsed Values with Teacher Goals
Equal Statuses Feedback
Psychological Needs Intrinsic Goals Evaluation
Relationships
Autonomy
Personal Aspirations with Peers Classroom Affordances
Competence
Relatedness Cultural Influences Interesting Activities
Autonomy-Oriented Expectations Optimal
Causality Orientation Prescriptions Challenges
Values Enriched Learning

Quality of the Teacher’s Motivating


Style toward the Student

Fig. 7.3 Student-teacher dialectical framework within self-determination theory

controlling classroom events interfere with and As shown in the large upper arrow in the figure,
thwart students’ autonomous engagement, syn- high-quality student engagement arises out of the
thesis will be impaired, interpersonal conflict will quality of the student’s inherent and acquired
arise, new sources of motivation will be rejected, sources of motivation and out of the twin desires
and less optimal student outcomes will result. to interact effectively with the environment and
The dialectical framework appears in Fig. 7.3. to grow as a person and as a learner. The twofold
The box on the left hand side of the figure repre- purpose of the large upper arrow is to communi-
sents the quality of the student’s motivation dur- cate that student engagement, first, arises out of
ing instruction, as informed by SDT’s basic and expresses the underlying quality of students’
needs theory, organismic integration theory, and inner motivational resources and, second, feeds
causality orientations theory. According to basic forward to affect changes in the learning
needs theory, students’ inherent sources of moti- environment.
vation include those that are universal across The box on the right hand side of the figure
cultures, genders, and backgrounds, including represents various aspects of the learning environ-
intrinsic motivation and the three psychological ment. As explained by cognitive evaluation theory,
needs of autonomy, competence, and related- some classroom influences are specific external
ness. According to organismic integration the- events, such as rewards, classroom goal structures,
ory, students’ acquired sources of motivation feedback, evaluations, and the Friday-afternoon
include those that are internalized through cul- vocabulary quiz. Because of its ubiquity, goals
tural experience and self-reflections that vary represent particularly important external events in
from student to student, including self-endorsed the classroom, and goal contents theory explains
values, intrinsic goals, and personal aspirations. when goals support versus interfere with students’
According to causality orientations theory, stu- motivation, engagement, and well-being. Other
dents’ acquired sources of motivation further classroom influences are interpersonal relation-
include the disposition-like individual differ- ships, including those with teachers, peers, par-
ences of general causality orientations. ents, and school administrators as well as more
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 159

general affiliations with school-related groups, More recently, classroom-based, longitudinally


communities, organizations, or the nation in gen- designed research investigations have been
eral. All of these relationships have implications undertaken to test the student-teacher dialectical
for students’ motivation, but empirical research on framework as a whole.
the student-teacher dialectical framework within Figure 7.4 shows the research design and the-
SDT focuses special attention on those relation- oretical predictions from one classroom-based,
ships in which people of high status or expertise longitudinally designed, data-based study that
attempt to motivate or socialize people of lower was specifically undertaken to test the student-
status or lesser expertise, as with parents relating teacher dialectical framework as a whole (Jang,
to children (Grolnick, 2003), coaches relating to Kim, & Reeve, 2011). Students’ perceptions of
athletes (Mageau & Vallerand, 2003), and teach- their teacher’s motivating style and self-reports
ers relating to students (Reeve, 2009b). Other of their own motivation, and engagement, and
classroom influences are social and cultural forces their objective class-specific achievement were
such as the learning climate (e.g., home schooling; assessed in three waves—at the beginning, middle,
Cai, Reeve, & Robison, 2002) or high-stakes test- and end of the semester. The three downward-
ing policies (Ryan & LaGuardia, 1999). slopping boldface lines in Fig. 7.4 represent sepa-
Finally, as shown in the large lower arrow in rate predictions from the student-teacher
the figure, external events and interpersonal rela- dialectical framework as (1) the teacher’s moti-
tionships that collectively comprise the learning vating style affects midsemester changes in
environment provide students with opportunities, students’ motivation, as defined by the extent of
hindrances, and an overall climate in which their students’ psychological need satisfaction, (2)
self-motivation develops (Ryan & LaGuardia, changes in students’ motivation during the course
1999). The most constant aspect of the learning affect corresponding changes in how engaged
environment is the quality of the teacher’s moti- versus disengaged students are, and (3) changes
vating style. And the most important aspect of the in student engagement over the course of the
teacher’s motivating style toward students is semester predict gains versus losses in students’
whether that style is autonomy supportive or con- achievement, controlling for students’ anticipated
trolling, as students develop autonomous motiva- achievement at the beginning of the course.
tions when teachers are autonomy supportive, Each of the five boldface lines from Fig. 7.4
while they develop controlled motivations when parallels an important feature within the student-
teachers are controlling (Reeve, 2009b). teacher dialectical framework depicted in Fig. 7.3.
The student-teacher dialectical framework Specifically, the path from teacher-provided
within SDT was first proposed in 2004, and it was autonomy support at Time 1 to changes in stu-
built on experimental studies and cross-sectional dents’ motivation (need satisfaction) at Time 2 in
survey investigations that largely examined how Fig. 7.4 represents the lower U-shaped arrow in
one variable within the framework affected Fig. 7.3. The path from student motivation at Time
another. For example, an extensive body of 2 to student engagement at Time 3 in Fig. 7.4 rep-
research has accumulated to understand how resents the upper rainbow-shaped arrow in Fig. 7.3.
extrinsic rewards (Deci et al., 1999), interper- In addition, Fig. 7.4 depicts three important exten-
sonal feedback (Ryan et al., 1983), and the teach- sions of the student-teacher dialectical framework.
ers’ motivating style (Deci, Schwartz, Sheinman, Each new path will be highlighted in the third
& Ryan, 1981) affect student motivation, just section of the present chapter, but each is intro-
as an extensive body of empirical research duced here. The path from student engagement at
has accumulated to understand how psychologi- Time 3 to students’ end-of-course achievement in
cal needs (Skinner & Belmont, 1993), intrinsic Fig. 7.4 explains the first new function of student
goals (Vansteenkiste et al., 2004a), and causal- engagement—namely, that it predicts students’
ity orientations (Deci & Ryan, 1985b) affect positive outcomes, including achievement. The
students’ engagement and learning outcomes. upward-slopping path from student engagement
160

Autonomy Autonomy Autonomy


Support Support Support
Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Need Need Need


Satisfaction Satisfaction Satisfaction
Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Autonomy Competence Relatedness Autonomy Competence Relatedness Autonomy Competence Relatedness

Quality of Quality of Quality of


Engagement Engagement Engagement
Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Behavioral Emotional Cognitive Agentic Behavioral Emotional Cognitive Agentic Behavioral Emotional Cognitive Agentic

Anticipated
End-of-Course
Achievement
Achievement
Time 1
Time 3

Fig. 7.4 Longitudinal research design to test predictions from the student-teacher dialectical framework
J. Reeve
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 161

at Time 2 to teacher-provided autonomy support at on students’ behavioral, emotional, and cognitive


Time 3 explains the second new function of stu- engagement during a learning activity unwittingly
dent engagement—namely, that it predicts con- embraces a unidirectional flow of instructional
structive changes in the learning environment. The activity from the teacher to the student, as the
upward-slopping path from student engagement at teacher says “Here is an assignment for you” and
Time 2 to changes in students’ motivation (need students respond with some display of behavioral,
satisfaction) explains the third new function of stu- emotional, and cognitive engagement. What is
dent engagement—namely, that it predicts con- missing from such a conceptualization of student
structive changes in students’ own motivation. engagement can be seen in the large upper arrow
in Fig. 7.3. That rainbow-shaped arrow represents
student engagement in general, but it specifically
Student Engagement Within the SDT represents students’ constructive contribution into
Dialectical Framework the flow of the instruction they receive, as stu-
dents try to enrich and personalize that instruc-
Among those who study the relation between tion. To understand this process of how students
student motivation and student engagement, a enrich learning activities, we proposed the con-
general consensus has emerged to characterize cept of agentic engagement, and we proposed it
engagement as a three-component construct fea- as a fourth aspect that was distinct from—yet also
turing behavioral (on-task attention, effort, per- highly intercorrelated with—the original three
sistence, lack of conduct problems), emotional aspects (as through exploratory and confirmatory
(presence of interest and enthusiasm, absence of factor analyses; Reeve & Tseng, 2011).
anger and boredom), and cognitive (strategic Agentic engagement refers to students’ inten-
learning strategies, active self-regulation) tional, proactive, and constructive contribution
aspects (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; into the flow of the instruction they receive. It is
Jimerson, Campos, & Grief, 2003; National assessed with both behavioral observation (Reeve
Research Council [NRC], 2004). Echoing this et al., 2004) and self-report (Reeve & Tseng,
three-component conceptualization, SDT-based 2011), with questionnaire items such as “During
research investigations routinely assess these class, I express my preferences and opinions”
same engagement constructs (e.g., Reeve, Jang, and “I let the teacher know what I’m interested
Carrell, Jeon, & Barch, 2004; Skinner & in.” Conceptually, agentic engagement is the pro-
Belmont, 1993). For instance, Skinner and her cess in which students proactively try to create,
colleagues (2009) showed how autonomous enhance, and personalize the conditions and cir-
motivation leads to behavioral engagement and cumstances under which they learn. For instance,
to emotional engagement, while Vansteenkiste upon hearing the learning objective for the day
and colleagues (2005) showed how autonomous (e.g., “Today, class, we are going to learn about
motivation leads to deep rather than to superfi- Mendel’s experiments on heredity.”), an agenti-
cial learning (i.e., cognitive engagement). cally engaged student might offer input, make a
While each of these three aspects of engage- suggestion, express a preference, contribute
ment is certainly important to understanding stu- something helpful, seek clarification, request an
dent engagement, this three-component model of example, ask for a say in how problems will be
student engagement represents only an incom- solved, or a 100 other constructive and personal-
plete understanding—not an incorrect one, just an izing acts that functionally enhance the condi-
incomplete one. The reason why any conceptual- tions under which the student learns. Such agentic
ization of student engagement as a three-compo- engagement arises out of students’ high-quality
nent construct is an incomplete one can be motivation, and it potentially affects changes in
understood through the earlier-presented student- the learning environment (i.e., the upper arrow in
teacher dialectical framework. That is, any focus Fig. 7.3).
162 J. Reeve

Why Agentic Engagement Needs


Three Newly Discovered Functions
to Be Added as a Fourth Aspect
of Student Engagement
of Student Engagement
Student engagement is important, and this is so
The present chapter is likely to be the only chap-
for many reasons. Student engagement is impor-
ter in the handbook to mention the concept of
tant because it makes learning possible, as it is
agentic engagement. For this reason, its value to
difficult to imagine learning a foreign language
both researchers and practitioners needs to be
or mastering a musical instrument without con-
explained, as does it status as a potential fourth
siderable engagement. Student engagement is
aspect of engagement.
important because it predicts how well students
As teachers provide them with learning activi-
do in school, including the academic progress
ties, students clearly react by displaying varying
they make or fail to make (Ladd & Dinella, 2009).
levels of involvement (engagement) in the learn-
Student engagement is also important because it
ing activities they receive. That is, when the
is a relatively malleable student characteristic
teacher asks students to analyze a poem, students
than is unusually open to constructive influences,
will show varying levels of attention, put forth
such as a teacher’s support (Birch & Ladd, 1997).
much or only little effort, enjoy or feel anxious
Student engagement is further important because
about the activity, and utilize deep and concep-
it affords teachers the moment-to-moment feed-
tual learning strategies or rely on only superficial
back they need during instruction to assess how
ones. The existing concepts of behavioral engage-
well their efforts to motivate students are work-
ment, emotional engagement, and cognitive
ing, as there is no better telltale signal about stu-
engagement nicely capture the extent to which
dent’s private motivation than their public
students differentially react to teacher-provided
engagement. But all of these reasons, important
learning activities. Such a linear model (teacher
as they are, are fairly well known.
presents a learning activity → students more or
If you accept agency as a fourth aspect of
less engage themselves → student learn in pro-
engagement, however, three new and important
portion to their engagement) overlooks students’
functions of engagement emerge, as illustrated
agentic involvement in the learning process. In
graphically in Fig. 7.5. Figure 7.5 mirrors the
actuality, students not only react to learning
earlier-presented student-teacher dialectical
activities but they proact on them—enriching
framework from Fig. 7.3, though it expands on
them (e.g., transforming them into something
the concept and functions of student engagement
more interesting or optimally challenging), mod-
within that framework. In comparing Figs. 7.3
ifying them (e.g., seeking to learn with a partner
and 7.5, notice what has changed is that the single
rather than alone), personalizing them (e.g., gen-
upper rainbow-shaped arrow in Fig. 7.3 has been
erating options, communicating preferences),
differentiated into three distinct upper arrows in
and even creating or requesting the learning
Fig. 7.5.
opportunity in the first place, rather than merely
reacting to them as a given (Bandura, 2006). The first new function, depicted in the vertical
Stated differently, engaged involvement includes arrow arising out of the quality of student moti-
not only reacting to the learning task one has vation and extending to the newly added “Positive
been given by showing more or less persistence, Student Outcomes” box, is that student engage-
enjoyment, and strategic thinking, but it also ment directly causes positive student outcomes.
means initiating a process in which the student Of course, many researchers argue this point, as
generates options that expand his or her freedom can be seen throughout the pages in this hand-
of action and increase the chance for that student book. But what is new and important about this
to experience both strong motivation and mean- first new function of student engagement is the rather
ingful learning. strong assertion that engagement fully mediates
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 163

Positive Student Outcomes


· Achievement
· Learning
· Skill Development

Function #1:
High-Quality
Student
Engagement Function #2: High-Quality Student
Causes Engagement Contributes Constructively into
Gains in the Flow of Instruction and to the
Student Responsiveness of the Learning Environment
Outcomes Function #3:
High-Quality
Student
Engagement
Causes
Quality of Student Motivation
Gains in
Psychological External
Acquired Sources Inherent Sources Relationships
Need Satisfaction Events
of Autonomous of Autonomous
Motivation Motivation
Learning Environment

Quality of the Teacher’s Motivating


Style toward the Student

Fig. 7.5 Three new functions of student engagement within the student-teacher dialectical framework

and explains the motivation-to-outcomes relation, an important educational construct is because it


as will be discussed in the next section. The sec- anticipates and predicts the sort of positive stu-
ond new function, depicted in the rightmost, rain- dent outcomes identified in the upper left box in
bow-shaped arrow extending to the learning Fig. 7.5, such as academic achievement, course
environment, is that student engagement affects grades, learning, and skill development. That is,
the future quality of the learning environment, engagement bridges students’ motivation to
especially its flow of instruction, the external highly valued outcomes. In statistical terms, this is
events it provides, and the teacher’s motivating to say that engagement mediates the motivation-
style. The third new function, depicted in the cen- to-achievement relation. This is not, however, a
termost arrow that circles back into student moti- controversial assertion (see Skinner & Belmont,
vation, proposes that the extent of a student’s 1993), as researchers who focus on a range of
active involvement in a learning activity affects student motivations—including autonomous
his or her subsequent motivation toward that motivation (Black & Deci, 2000), self-efficacy
same activity—that increases in student engage- (Multon, Brown, & Lent, 1991), and achievement
ment feedback to enrich future student motiva- goal orientations (Greene & Miller, 1996)—rou-
tion, just as declines in student engagement tinely show that motivation exerts a direct effect
feedback to impoverish it. on achievement. It becomes controversial (and
hence worthy of future research), however, with
the assertion that engagement fully mediates and
New Function #1: Engagement explains the motivation-to-achievement relation
Fully Mediates the Motivation-to- (Reeve & Tseng, 2011). That is, when student
Achievement Relation engagement is considered as a predictor of stu-
dents’ positive school outcomes, the direct effect
Part—and probably most—of the reason that that student motivation has on student achieve-
educators embrace student engagement as such ment drops to zero.
164 J. Reeve

Two features differentiate our research showing competence, and relatedness) during instruction.
that engagement fully mediates the motivation- We further operationally defined student engage-
to-achievement relation from others’ research ment in a way that was consistent with Fig. 7.1
showing a direct effect of motivation on achieve- (the four-aspect conceptualization of engage-
ment. First, we include agentic engagement ment) and student achievement as end-of-course
within our conceptualization of student engage- grade. Both studies used a longitudinal research
ment, whereas others do not. When engagement design and structural equation modeling to show
is operationally defined as students’ behavioral, that the significant direct effect that beginning-
emotional, and cognitive engagement, we often of-course motivation had on end-of-course
find that some residual variance in end-of-course achievement was fully explained by the mediat-
achievement remains unexplained. Each of these ing variable of engagement. In one of the studies
three aspects of engagement does function as an (Reeve & Cheon, 2011), we expanded our con-
important (though partial) mediator to explain ceptualization of students’ positive outcomes to
end-of-course achievement, but the significant include not only end-of-course achievement but
direct effect of motivation often remains—the also skill development, a research strategy that
direct effect of motivation on achievement is allowed us to test for the full mediating effect of
reduced, but it is not eliminated. This means engagement on student outcomes in general
that motivation is contributing to achievement rather than on student achievement in particular.
in a way that is not fully explained by students’ This study investigated middle-school students in
behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engage- Korea taking classes in physical education, and
ment. It is only when we add agentic engage- student engagement fully mediated the effect that
ment as a fourth component that engagement motivation had on both outcomes.
fully mediates and explains the motivation-to- A second feature that makes our research dif-
achievement relation (Reeve & Tseng, 2011). ferent from previous research (besides including
The reason why agentic engagement explains agentic engagement) is that we operationally
unique variance in student achievement (or defined student motivation as the extent of psy-
other outcomes, such as skill development or chological need satisfaction during instruction.
learning) is because it is through agentic acts When student motivation is conceptualized in
such as making suggestions, asking questions, this way, several studies have now shown that
and personalizing lessons that students find student engagement fully mediates the effect that
ways to enrich and to adapt the lessons they student motivation has on their positive end-of-
receive into improved opportunities for learn- course outcomes (Reeve & Cheon, 2011; Reeve
ing, skill development, and achievement to & Tseng, 2010; Reeve et al., 2011). Recognizing
occur. Hence, agentic engagement contributes this, we expanded our conceptualization of
achievement-enabling behaviors that the behav- student motivation in two studies beyond the
ioral, emotional, and cognitive aspects of self-determination theory framework (i.e., psy-
engagement fail to capture. chological need satisfaction) to include both aca-
This is both a new hypothesis and a new find- demic efficacy and mastery goal orientation. We
ing, so the finding that engagement fully medi- did this because we wanted to assess the general-
ates the motivation-to-achievement relation will izability of our assertion that engagement fully
need to be replicated before it might advance mediates and explains the direct effect of motiva-
from a tentative hypothesis to a reliable principle. tion (in general) on student outcomes. Both stud-
However, we have now tested for this full media- ies that included academic efficacy (as assessed
tion effect twice and found the effect both times by the PALS questionnaire) showed that engage-
(Reeve & Cheon, 2011; Reeve, Lee, Kim, & Ahn, ment fully mediated the effect that academic effi-
2011). In both studies, we operationally defined cacy had on student achievement (Reeve &
student motivation as the extent of students’ Cheon, 2010; Reeve et al., 2011) and also on stu-
psychological need satisfaction (i.e., autonomy, dent skill development (Reeve & Cheon, 2011).
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 165

One study assessed students’ mastery goal orien- The empirical evidence to support the second
tation as a third operational definition of student (rainbow-shaped) arrow in Fig. 7.5 has been
motivation (Reeve et al., 2011). In this study with mixed. One the one hand, laboratory research
Korean middle- and high-school students enrolled shows that experimentally manipulated levels of
in a wide range of courses, engagement did not how engaged versus disengaged a student is in a
fully mediate the mastery goal-to-achievement learning activity causally affects the teacher’s
main effect. We found this result surprising, as it subsequent motivating style toward that student
challenges our assertion that engagement fully (Pelletier & Vallerand, 1996). That is, teachers
mediates the effect that motivation in general has generally react to student displays of high-quality
on achievement. We will return to this issue in the engagement with a more autonomy-supportive
Future Research section at the end of the motivating style, while they react to student dis-
chapter. plays of disengagement with a more controlling
style (Pelletier, Seguin-Levesque, & Legault,
2002; Skinner & Belmont, 1993). Our own
New Function #2: Engagement research, however, has not always found this
Changes the Learning Environment engagement-to-motivating style causal effect in
naturally occurring classrooms. We sometimes
Adding agency as a new component of student find that changes in student engagement during
engagement paints a fuller picture of how stu- the course of the semester do not cause subse-
dents really engage themselves in learning activi- quent changes in the teachers’ end-of-course
ties. Recognizing that students (sometimes) motivating style (Jang et al., 2011). The biggest
proactively, intentionally, and constructively difference between the hypothesis-confirming
contribute into the instruction they receive clari- experimental research and our hypothe-
fies how students learn and profit from classroom sis-questioning classroom research is likely the
learning opportunities—or even how they create student-teacher ratio difference, as the ratio is 1:1
new learning opportunities for themselves. What in the laboratory experiments but something like
agentically engaged students are doing (that 30:1 in classroom studies. For this reason, the
agentically disengaged students are not doing) is “relationships” variable within the Learning
offering input, personalizing and enriching the Environment box in Fig. 7.5 is drawn with an
lesson, and modifying and adapting it into an open-ended funnel shape. The funnel shape com-
improved opportunity for learning. municates the teacher’s need to be attuned to
Such agency is the ideal complement to a classroom expressions of high-quality engage-
teacher’s autonomy-supportive motivating style, ment, especially to expressions of agentic engage-
just as the lack of student agency is the ideal ment. Teachers might become more attuned to
complement to a teacher’s controlling style. That students’ engagement, for instance, by listening,
is, agency involves students asking questions, by asking students as to what they would like to
expressing opinions, and communicating inter- do, and by communicating perspective-taking
ests, while autonomy support involves teachers comments (Reeve & Jang, 2006).
creating the classroom conditions in which stu-
dents feel free to ask questions, express opinions,
and pursue interests. What adding the concept of New Function #3: Engagement
agentic engagement can do for any view of Changes Motivation
student motivation (e.g., need satisfaction, self-
efficacy, personal goals, possible selves, individ- Another new, interesting, and important finding to
ual interests, and mastery goal orientation) is to emerge out of our recent longitudinal classroom-
draw greater attention to students’ intentional, based investigations of the student-teacher dialec-
proactive, and origin-like motivational involve- tic is the consistent finding that changes in the
ment in learning activities. quality of students’ engagement during the
166 J. Reeve

course of the semester predict gains versus the extent of psychological need satisfaction,
declines in students’ end-of-semester motivation while student engagement was operationally
(i.e., psychological need satisfaction). This effect defined in a way that was consistent with the
that changes in engagement make on subsequent four-component depiction in Fig. 7.1. Both stud-
changes in motivation appears in Fig. 7.5 as the ies used a full longitudinal design and structural
circular arrow in which changes in the quality of equation modeling to test the hypothesis [i.e., the
students’ engagement loop back to predict subse- boldface upward-slopping arrow in Fig. 7.4 that
quent changes in the quality of students’ motiva- extends from engagement at Time 2 to motiva-
tion. That is, students’ motivation (the “Quality of tion (need satisfaction) at Time 3]. That path in
Student Motivation” box in the figure) is both a Fig. 7.4 does not look the same as the slopping
cause and a consequence of student engagement. arrow from Fig. 7.5, but the effect and the inter-
The role of this third new function of engagement pretation are the same—changes in the quality of
is to highlight the causal role that changes in engagement predict (and temporally cause)
engagement contribute to changes in motivation. changes in the quality of motivation.
The hypothesis that changes in engagement Consider why this new function of engage-
cause changes in motivation is premised on the ment might be important to future engagement
idea that students can take action to meet their research. In an SDT framework, changes in stu-
own psychological needs. According to SDT, the dents’ psychological need satisfaction occur in
needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness response to the teacher’s motivating style. That
provide the psychological nutriments necessary is, when teachers relate to students in autonomy-
for positive psychological well-being. That is, supportive ways, students experience greater
students need autonomy, competence, and relat- autonomy, competence, and relatedness, and
edness experiences to be well. This assertion has when teachers relate to students in controlling
received considerable empirical support. For ways, they experience lesser autonomy, compe-
instance, research participants reliably report tence, and relatedness. Again, this is a reliable
having a “good day” (feeling joyful, enthusiastic) finding (Black & Deci, 2000; Deci et al., 1981;
when they experience high levels of daily auton- Jang, Reeve, Ryan, & Kim, 2009; Ryan &
omy, competence, and relatedness, while they Grolnick, 1986). But the findings from the longi-
just as reliably report having a “bad day” (feeling tudinal classroom-based investigations summa-
distress, anger) when they experience low (or rized above confirm a second reliable source of
frustrated) levels of daily autonomy, competence, changes in students’ course-related motivation—
and relatedness (Kasser & Ryan, 1993, 1996; namely, changes in students’ course-related
Sheldon et al., 1996). But to experience auton- engagement. In fact, these studies find that
omy, competence, and relatedness, one first has changes in student engagement is a stronger pre-
to take action and actually engage in environ- dictor of end-of-course motivation than is the
mental transactions that are capable of producing quality of the teacher’s motivating style (in terms
such experiences and feelings (e.g., read a book, of the magnitude of the two respective beta coef-
exercise with friends, try something new). Thus, ficients predicting end-of-course motivation;
high-quality engagement in what one is doing Jang et al., 2011). This means that students can
would seem necessary to produce need-satisfying take action to meet their own psychological needs
and positive subjective experiences. and to enhance the quality of their own motiva-
We have tested for this “changes in engage- tion. This also means that students can be (and
ment causes changes in motivation” effect twice, are) architects of their own motivation, at least to
and we have found the effect both times (Jang the extent that students can be architects of their
et al., 2011; Reeve et al., 2011). In both studies, own course-related behavioral, emotional, cogni-
student motivation was operationally defined as tive, and agentic engagement.
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 167

purposes of the present chapter because it shows


Implications for Teachers that (1) teachers can learn how to become more
autonomy supportive and (2) the more autonomy
If you are a teacher and have invested your behav- supportive they become, the more high-quality
ioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement in engagement their students show (e.g., Reeve
reading and reflecting on the chapter to this point, et al., 2004). Stated differently, the first recom-
it is now time to give you an opportunity to agen- mendation seeks to offer teachers a reliable path
tically voice your interests and hopes for the to enhancing student engagement—namely,
chapter. Accordingly, the chapter now turns to adopt a more autonomy-supportive motivating
two key implications for teachers. style toward students.
The first implication is to recommend that The second implication is to recommend that
teachers work to increase their capacity to prac- teachers intentionally monitor and enhance stu-
tice a more autonomy-supportive motivating dents’ classroom engagement. Monitoring and
style toward their students. Generally speaking, enhancing students’ motivation and engagement
autonomy support is whatever a teacher says and is an important skill, but these are also difficult
does during instruction to facilitate students’ per- responsibilities for teachers to fulfill on a reliable
ceptions of autonomy and experiences of psycho- basis. Monitoring students’ motivation and
logical need satisfaction. More specifically, an engagement is difficult not only because class-
autonomy-supportive motivating style is the rooms are large, fluid, and diverse environments
interpersonal sentiment and behavior teachers but also because motivation is a private, subjec-
provide to identify, vitalize, and develop their tive, and unobservable student experience. That
students’ inner motivational resources during is, teachers cannot objectively see their students’
instruction (Assor, Kaplan, & Roth, 2002; Reeve, underlying psychological need satisfaction, self-
2009b; Reeve et al., 2004). It is important because efficacy, interest, goal orientation, etc. The
it predicts students’ constructive motivation, instructional task of monitoring what is unob-
engagement, and functioning in a reliable way, as servable and only privately experienced (i.e., stu-
discussed earlier. It is also important because, dent motivation) would seem overly difficult. In
like student engagement, a teacher’s motivating contrast to motivation, however, student engage-
style is malleable. While it is true that teachers’ ment is a relatively public, objective, and observ-
naturally occurring motivating styles (control- able classroom event. That is, teachers can see
ling, neutral, or autonomy supportive) tend to whether or not a student is paying attention, put-
remain fairly consistent throughout the school ting forth effort, enjoying class, solving problems
year, it is also true that teachers can learn how to in a sophisticated way, and contributing construc-
be more autonomy supportive toward students. tively into the flow of instruction. The instruc-
Intervention training programs have shown that tional task of monitoring what is observable and
teachers can learn how to be more autonomy sup- publically expressed (i.e., student engagement)
portive and also that such a change in one’s moti- would seem possible.
vating style endures well beyond the initial To test this logic, we asked a group of middle-
training experience (Su & Reeve, 2011). In and high-school Korean teachers who taught a
these intervention studies, teachers randomly wide range of different subject matters to rate each
assigned into an experimental group participate student in their class on how motivated and how
in a training program that, first, tells them what engaged the teacher thought the student was (Lee
autonomy support is and how beneficial it typi- & Reeve, 2011). In particular, we asked these
cally is for students and, second, provides them teachers to rate their students on three aspects of
with the modeling, scaffolding, and “how-to” motivation—psychological need satisfaction,
problem-solving discussions they need to be able self-efficacy, and mastery goal orientation—and
to support students’ autonomy during classroom on four aspects of engagement—behavioral
practice. This research is important for the engagement, emotional engagement, cognitive
168 J. Reeve

0.2

0.15
Partial Correlation Coefficient

0.1

0.05

0
Behav Eng Emotion Eng Cognit Eng Agentic Eng Needs Mot SelfEff Mot MGoal Mot

-0.05

-0.1

Fig. 7.6 Accuracy scores for teachers’ ratings of three aspects of students’ motivation and four aspects of students’
engagement

engagement, and agentic engagement. At the same side of the figure). Overall, results were clear:
time, we asked the students of these teachers to Teacher ratings of their students’ motivation were
self-report the same three aspects of their course- inaccurate, while teachers’ ratings of their stu-
related motivation and the same four aspects of dents’ engagement were accurate. Further, teach-
their course-related engagement, using previously ers’ inaccuracy scores did not depend on the type
validated questionnaires. How accurate teachers of student motivation they rated, just as teacher
were in rating their students’ motivation and accuracy scores did not depend on the type of
engagement appears in Fig. 7.6. student engagement they rated.
Accuracy scores were determined with partial The data summarized in Fig. 7.6 are important
correlation statistics that showed the association because they suggest that the instructional effort
between each teacher rating and each student to monitor students’ motivation is probably too
self-report after partialling out variance attribut- difficult (because it is a private, unobservable
able to student achievement. It was necessary to student experience), while the instructional effort
partial out student achievement scores because to monitor students’ engagement is probably
teachers generally rate high-achieving students manageable (because it is a public, observable
as more motivated and as more engaged than student behavior). We are not suggesting that stu-
they rate low-achieving students. Looking at the dent motivation is not important, and we are not
composite scores only, teacher rated their stu- suggesting that teachers not think about how to
dents’ motivation unreliably (pr = −.03, ns) but facilitate it. After all, student motivation is the
their students’ engagement reliably (pr = .13, key variable underlying and causing students’
p < .05), as reported in Lee and Reeve (2011). classroom engagement. Instead, we recommend
The bars in Fig. 7.6 show the partial correlations that teachers allocate a significant proportion of
observed for each of the four aspects of engage- their attention during instruction to the effort of
ment (on the left side of the figure) and for each monitoring and enhancing students’ engagement.
of the three aspects of motivation (on the right Doing so allows teachers to invest their attention
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 169

on the variable that changes students’ academic as to the nature and function of students’ agentic
lives for the better—namely, high-quality student engagement during learning activities, future
engagement. research will be better positioned to improve the
assessment of the construct. Part of that empirical
effort will be to better assess the construct, and
Future Research part of the empirical effort will be to better distin-
guish agentic engagement both from similar con-
The present chapter relied on a pair of well- structs (e.g., instructional help seeking) and from
established theoretical perspectives—namely, the other three aspects of engagement.
SDT and the student-teacher dialectical frame- The second new finding that requires exten-
work within SDT—to present three new ideas sive future research is the functional claim that
about the nature and function of student engage- student engagement serves purposes beyond
ment. Each one of these ideas is both new and those that are already well established and under-
somewhat controversial, so each requires exten- stood. Specifically, the SDT perspective sug-
sive future research to assess its reliability, valid- gests three new and important functions of
ity, and potential contribution to the larger student engagement—namely, that student
literature on student engagement. engagement fully mediates and explains the
The first new finding that requires extensive motivation-to-achievement relation, that changes
future research is the definitional claim that in student engagement produce changes in the
student engagement is better conceptualized as learning environment, and that changes in stu-
a four-component construct than as a three-com- dent engagement produce changes in student
ponent construct. The difference between the two motivation, as students’ behavioral, emotional,
conceptualizations is that the four-component cognitive, and agentic engagement represents
conceptualization includes agentic engagement, action taken to meet their psychological needs.
whereas the three-component conceptualization The validity of the claim that student engage-
excludes it. This future research will need to ment fully mediates and explains the motivation-
examine both the conceptual status of agentic to-achievement relation may boil down to how
engagement as well as its assessment procedures. the engagement construct itself is conceptual-
In terms of conceptualizing agentic engagement, ized and assessed. As emphasized above, when
Reeve and Tseng (2011) proposed five defining engagement is conceptualized and assessed as a
features of the construct: (1) it is proactive in that four-component construct, student engagement
it occurs before or during the learning activity; does seem to consistently and fully mediate the
(2) it is intentional in that agentic engagement is direct effect that motivation has on students’
both deliberate and purposive; (3) it attempts to positive outcomes. If this rather strong assertion
enrich the learning activity by making the learn- does not eventually hold up to future analysis,
ing experience more personal, more interesting, there will still be important insights to gain from
more challenging, or more valued; (4) it contrib- asking the question as to what effect motivation
utes constructive input into the planning or ongo- has on student outcomes that lies outside of its
ing flow of the teacher’s instruction; and (5) it effect on engagement. It is actually rather diffi-
does not connote teacher ineffectiveness or cult to think of a path from motivation to
incompetence. In terms of assessing agentic achievement that does not go through student
engagement, researchers currently assess the engagement, though both social engagement and
construct either by having trained raters score an improved teacher-student relationship might
students’ agency from the Hit-Steer Observation be two candidates. The idea that student motiva-
System (for illustrations see, Jang, Reeve, & tion might improve the teacher-student relation-
Deci, 2010; Koenig, Fielder, & deCharms, 1977; ship is nicely captured by the student-teacher
Reeve et al., 2004) or from a five-item self-report dialectical framework within SDT, though our
scale (Reeve & Tseng, 2011). As clarity emerges recent evidence suggests rather strongly that
170 J. Reeve

what affects the quality of the student-teacher Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-
relationship during learning activities is more analytic review of experiments examining the effects
of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation.
the publically observable engagement that stu- Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.
dents show and less the privately experienced Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical explora-
motivation they harbor. tion of intrinsic motivational processes. In L. Berkowitz
Finally, the finding that changes in student (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(Vol. 13, pp. 39–40). New York: Academic Press.
engagement produce changes in student motiva- Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985a). Intrinsic motivation
tion requires extensive future study. It is an excit- and self-determination in human behavior. New York:
ing finding that students can take self-initiated Plenum.
action—in terms of their behavioral, emotional, Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985b). The General Causality
Orientations Scale: Self-determination in personality.
cognitive, and agentic engagements—to meet Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109–134.
their psychological needs. The reason this finding Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational
has such potential to improve our understanding approach to self: Integration in personality. In R.
of the functions of student engagement is partly Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation:
Perspectives on motivation (Vol. 38, pp. 237–288).
because the effect of engagement on students’ Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
motivation seems to be as strong as the effect of Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why”
the teacher’s motivating style on student motiva- of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determina-
tion and partly because it illustrates empirically tion of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
Deci, E. L., Schwartz, A., Sheinman, L., & Ryan, R. M.
that students can be architects of their own (1981). An instrument to assess adult’s orientations
motivation—for better or for worse. toward control versus autonomy in children: Reflections
on intrinsic motivation and perceived competence.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 642–650.
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
References School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
Amabile, T. M., DeJong, W., & Lepper, M. R. (1976). 59–109.
Effects of externally-imposed deadlines on subsequent Greene, B. A., & Miller, R. B. (1996). Influences on
intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social achievement: Goals, perceived ability, and cognitive
Psychology, 34, 92–98. engagement. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good, 21, 181–192.
but relevance is excellent: Autonomy-enhancing and Grolnick, W. S. (2003). The psychology of parental con-
suppressing teaching behaviors predicting students’ trol: How well-meant parenting backfires. Mahwah,
engagement in schoolwork. British Journal of NJ: Erlbaum.
Educational Psychology, 27, 261–278. Jang, H., Kim, E., & Reeve, J. (2011). Longitudinal test of
Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human self-determination theory in a school context.
agency. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, Manuscript submitted for publication
164–180. Jang, H., Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (2010). Engaging stu-
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The student-teacher dents in learning activities: It is not autonomy support
relationship and children’s early school adjustment. or structure but autonomy support and structure.
Journal of School Psychology, 35, 61–79. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 588–600.
Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of instruc- Jang, H., Reeve, J., Ryan, R. M., & Kim, A. (2009). Can
tors’ autonomy support and students’ autonomous self-determination theory explain what underlies the
motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self- productive, satisfying learning experiences of collec-
determination theory perspective. Science Education, tivistically-oriented Korean adolescents? Journal of
84, 740–756. Educational Psychology, 101, 644–661.
Cai, Y., Reeve, J., & Robinson, D. T. (2002). Home Jimerson, S. J., Campos, E., & Grief, J. L. (2003). Toward
schooling and teaching style: Comparing the motivat- an understanding of definitions and measures of school
ing styles of home school and public school teachers. engagement and related terms. The California School
Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 372–380. Psychologist, 8, 7–27.
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the
Plenum. American dream: Correlates of financial success as a
Deci, E. L., Betley, G., Kahle, J., Abrams, L., & Porac, J. central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and
(1981). When trying to win: Competition and intrinsic Social Psychology, 65, 410–422.
motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the
7, 79–83. American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic
7 A Self-determination Theory Perspective on Student Engagement 171

and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Reeve, J., & Cheon, S. H. (2011). Interrelations among
Bulletin, 22, 280–287. teachers’ motivating styles and students’ motivation,
Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2007). When choice motivates and engagement, and skill development in a physical edu-
when it does not. Educational Psychology Review, 19, cation context. Manuscript submitted for publication.
429–442. Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Elements of the competi-
Koenig, S. S., Fielder, M. L., & deCharms, R. (1977). tive situation that affect intrinsic motivation. Personality
Teacher beliefs, classroom interaction and personal and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 24–33.
causation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 7, Reeve, J., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Self-
95–114. determination theory: A dialectical framework for
Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984). understanding the sociocultural influences on student
Setting limits on children’s behavior: The differential motivation. In D. McInerney & S. Van Etten (Eds.),
effects of controlling versus informational styles on Research on sociocultural influences on motivation
intrinsic motivation and creativity. Journal of and learning: Big theories revisited (Vol. 4, pp. 31–59).
Personality, 52, 233–248. Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Ladd, G. W., & Dinella, L. M. (2009). Continuity and change Reeve, J., & Halusic, M. (2009). How K-12 teachers can
in early school engagement: Predictive of children’s put self-determination theory principles into practice.
achievement trajectories from first to eighth grade? Theory and Research in Education, 7, 145–154.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 190–206. Reeve, J., & Jang, H. (2006). What teachers say and do to
Lee, W., & Reeve, J. (2011). Teacher accuracy in judging support students’ autonomy during learning activities.
students’ motivation and engagement. Manuscript for Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 209–218.
publication. Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J.
Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (1975). Turning play into (2004). Enhancing high school students’ engagement
work: Effects of adult surveillance and extrinsic by increasing their teachers’ autonomy support.
rewards on children’s intrinsic motivation. Journal of Motivation and Emotion, 28, 147–169.
Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 479–486. Reeve, J., Jang, H., Hardre, P., & Omura, M. (2002).
Mageau, G. A., & Vallerand, R. J. (2003). The coach- Providing a rationale in an autonomy-supportive way
athlete relationship: A motivational model. Journal of as a strategy to motivate others during an uninteresting
Sports Science, 21, 883–904. activity. Motivation and Emotion, 26, 183–207.
Mossholder, K. W. (1980). Effects of externally mediated Reeve, J., Lee, W., Kim, H., & Ahn, H. S. (2011).
goal setting on intrinsic motivation: A laboratory exper- Longitudinal test of the hypothesis that student engage-
iment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 202–210. ment fully mediates the motivation-to-achievement
Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). relation. Manuscript submitted for publication
Relation of self-efficacy beliefs to academic outcomes: Reeve, J., Nix, G., & Hamm, D. (2003). Testing models of
A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling the experience of self-determination in intrinsic moti-
Psychology, 38, 30–38. vation and the conundrum of choice. Journal of
National Research Council. (2004). Engaging schools: Educational Psychology, 95, 375–392.
Fostering high school students’ motivation to learn. Reeve, J., & Tseng, C. – M (2011). Agency as a fourth aspect
Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. of students’ engagement during learning activities.
Niemiec, C. P., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). Autonomy, compe- Contemporary Educational psychology, 36, 257–267.
tence, and relatedness in the classroom: Applying self- Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., &
determination theory to educational practice. Theory Ryan, R. M. (2000). Daily well-being: The role of
and Research in Education, 7, 133–144. autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality
Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 419–435.
path taken: Consequences of attaining intrinsic and Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrap-
extrinsic aspirations in post-college life. Journal of ersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation
Research in Personality, 43, 291–306. theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Pelletier, L. G., Seguin-Levesque, C., & Legault, L. (2002). 43, 450–461.
Pressure from above and pressure from below as deter- Ryan, R. M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic
minants of teachers’ motivation and teaching behav- motivation, autonomy and the self in psychological
iors. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 186–196. development. In J. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska symposium
Pelletier, L. G., & Vallerand, R. J. (1996). Supervisors’ on motivation: Developmental perspectives on moti-
beliefs and subordinates’ intrinsic motivation: vation (Vol. 40, pp. 1–56). Lincoln, NE: University of
A behavioral confirmation analysis. Journal of Nebraska Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 331–340. Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of
Reeve, J. (2009a). Understanding motivation and emotion causality and internalization: Examining reasons for
(5th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and
Reeve, J. (2009b). Why teachers adopt a controlling moti- Social Psychology, 57, 749–761.
vating style toward students and how they can become Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory
more autonomy supportive. Educational Psychologist, and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop-
44, 159–175. ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
172 J. Reeve

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). An overview of self- Su, Y.-L., & Reeve, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of the
determination theory: An organismic-dialectical per- effectiveness of intervention programs designed to
spective. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook support autonomy. Educational Psychology Review,
of self-determination research (pp. 3–33). Rochester, 23, 159–188.
NY: University of Rochester Press. Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic
Ryan, R. M., & Grolnick, W. S. (1986). Origins and pawns versus extrinsic goal contents in self-determination
in the classroom: Self-report and projective assess- theory: Another look at the quality of academic moti-
ments of individual differences in children’s percep- vation. Educational Psychologist, 41, 19–31.
tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vansteenkiste, M., Niemiec, C. P., & Soenens, B. (2010).
50, 550–558. The development of the five mini-theories of self-
Ryan, R. M., & LaGuardia, J. G. (1999). Achievement determination theory: An historical overview, emerg-
motivation within a pressured society: Intrinsic and ing trends, and future directions. Advances in
extrinsic motivation to learn and the politics of school motivation and achievement: The decade ahead:
reform. In T. Urdan (Ed.), Advances in motivation and Theoretical perspectives on motivation and achieve-
achievement (Vol. 11, pp. 45–85). Greenwich, CT: JAI ment, 16A, 105–167.
Press. Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M.,
Ryan, R. M., Mims, V., & Koestner, R. (1983). Relation of & Deci, E. L. (2004a). Motivating learning, perfor-
reward contingency and interpersonal context to intrin- mance, and persistence: The synergistic role of intrin-
sic motivation: A review and test using cognitive eval- sic goals and autonomy support. Journal of Personality
uation theory. Journal of Personality and Social and Social Psychology, 87, 246–260.
Psychology, 45, 736–750. Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., &
Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. Matos, L. (2005). Examining the impact of extrinsic
(1996). All goals are not created equal: An organismic versus intrinsic goal framing and internally controlling
perspective on the nature of goals and their regulation. versus autonomy-supportive communication style
In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychol- upon early adolescents’ academic achievement. Child
ogy of action: Linking cognition and motivation to Development, 76, 483–501.
behavior (pp. 7–26). New York: Guilford Press. Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B.,
Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., & Reis, H. T. (1996). What Matos, L., & Lacante, M. (2004b). “Less is sometimes
makes for a good day? Competence and autonomy in more”: Goal-content matters. Journal of Educational
the day and in the person. Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 755–764.
Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1270–1279. Vansteenkiste, M., Timmermans, T., Lens, W., Soenens,
Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the B., & Van den Broeck, A. (2008). Does extrinsic
classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and goal framing enhance extrinsic goal oriented indi-
student engagement across the school year. Journal of viduals’ learning and performance? An experimental
Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581. test of the match-perspective vs. self-determination
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., & Furrer, C. J. (2009). theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100,
A motivational perspective on engagement and disaf- 387–397.
fection: Conceptualization and assessment of chil- Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Engaged and disaffected action:
dren’s behavioral and emotional participation in The conceptualization and measurement of motivation
academic activities in the classroom. Educational and in the academic domain. Unpublished doctoral disser-
Psychological Measurement, 69, 493–525. tation, University of Rochester, Rochester.
Achievement Goal Theory,
Conceptualization of Ability/ 8
Intelligence, and Classroom Climate

Eric M. Anderman and Helen Patrick

Abstract
In this chapter, we examine relations between achievement goal theory and
student engagement. Achievement goal theorists generally examine two
types of goals (mastery and performance goals), each of which has been
conceptualized as having both approach and avoid components. After
reviewing the history and development of achievement goal theory and
describing the current four-factor model, we examine correlates of achieve-
ment goal orientations; these include students’ beliefs about intelligence,
academic achievement, and engagement (cognitive, emotional, and behav-
ioral). We then review research on classroom goal structures; we specifi-
cally examine how classroom contexts, as conceptualized through goal
orientation theory, are related to student engagement. We also review
instructional practices that are related to both mastery and performance goal
structures and how those practices are related to academic achievement.

Achievement goal theory is a framework that is Maehr & Zusho, 2009). Currently, achievement
used to explain and study academic motivation. goal theory informs both educational research
The theory became particularly prominent during and classroom practice, given its strong empirical
the 1980s and 1990s and has emerged as one of support. Relevant to the present chapter, achieve-
the most accepted and supported theories in the ment goal theory has been, and continues to be, a
field of educational psychology (Elliot, 1999; predominant perspective used to understand stu-
dents’ engagement in academics.
In the present chapter, we review many aspects
E.M. Anderman of achievement goal theory. In addition to describ-
School of Educational Policy and Leadership, ing the theory and its relation to valued educa-
The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
e-mail: eanderman@ehe.osu.edu
tional outcomes, we also argue that achievement
goal theory is related in important ways to stu-
H. Patrick (*)
Department of Educational Studies,
dent engagement. Although the constructs uti-
Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA lized by achievement goal theorists differ from
e-mail: hpatrick@purdue.edu the constructs used by researchers who study

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 173


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_8, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
174 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

engagement, there is much overlap. We believe identified two types—“mastery” (i.e., a focus on
that a more thorough examination and possible understanding and personal improvement) and
integration of research conducted by achieve- “performance” (i.e., a focus on outperforming
ment goal theorists and by engagement research- others), although different researchers used dif-
ers will lead to a broader and more conceptually ferent names. There was also some consideration,
useful understanding of academic motivation. however, of students’ perceptions of what is
emphasized in their classrooms or schools in
terms of reasons for engaging in schoolwork and
The Basic Tenets of Achievement the meaning of success (i.e., classroom goal
Goal Theory structures; Ames, 1984). Although personal goal
orientations continue to receive most attention,
Achievement goal theory has a rich history within consideration of classroom goal structures has
the field of motivation. This history includes both become more prevalent, consistent with the
the original development of the theory, as well as greater attention to the role of social contexts in
more recent subtle changes in the ways in which motivational research (Anderman & Anderman,
goal theory constructs are operationalized. These 2000; Meece, Anderman, & Anderman, 2006;
changes are reflected in research examining cor- Midgley, 2002).
relates of achievement goal orientations; indeed, As we will review later, goal theorists also draw
as measurement of goal orientations has changed strongly from the approach/avoid distinctions often
over time, results of research examining the rela- made in psychology (Elliot, 1999; Elliot &
tions of goal orientations to other outcomes also Covington, 2001). Approach and avoidance moti-
have evolved. vations are distinguished by whether or not
behavior is directed by desirable (approach) or
undesirable (avoid) potential outcomes. As research
Historical Development of Achievement on achievement goal theory progressed over the
Goal Theory past two decades, in particular, psychometric stud-
ies focusing on the measurement of goal orienta-
The study of achievement goal orientations for- tions have drawn in significant ways from approach/
mally began in the late 1970s, although many avoid distinctions (Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996).
aspects of the theory can be traced back to much In addition, numerous methodological devel-
earlier conceptions of achievement motivation. opments over the past few decades have enhanced
Researchers at the University of Illinois were par- our understanding of achievement goal orienta-
ticularly prominent in early developments of the tions. Whereas many of the original studies used
theory. In particular, Martin Maehr, Carole Ames, survey methodology to examine students’ per-
John Nicholls, and Carol Dweck all were influen- sonal goal orientations, later studies have included
tial in early work on goal orientation theory. classroom observations, discourse analyses, mul-
As we will review in this chapter, the theory tilevel models, experimental designs, and mixed-
has developed and changed in quite remarkable method approaches. These methodological
ways during the past three decades. The theory, advances have allowed motivation researchers to
which was originally conceptualized in terms of understand the nature of achievement goals more
two types of goal orientations, has blossomed fully, as well as their many correlates.
into a robust theoretical framework that now
includes the original conceptions of goal orienta-
tions, as well as numerous additional distinctions Variations in Operationalizations
between subtypes. Originally, the theory focused of Goal Orientations
predominantly on students’ personal goal orien-
tations (i.e., the reasons that students give for Personal goal orientations have been defined and
engaging personally in specific tasks). Researchers operationalized differently by various researchers.
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 175

Although we use the terms “mastery” and “perfor- Elliott & Dweck, 1988). Learning goals are
mance” to broadly characterize goal orientations, described as goals “in which individuals are con-
it is important to note that a variety of develop- cerned with increasing their competence,”
ments have occurred over the years. Maehr (1984) whereas performance goals are described as goals
called his version of mastery goals “task goals,” “in which individuals are concerned with gaining
which he defined as focusing on (a) an individual’s favorable judgments of their competence”
involvement with a specific task and (b) an indi- (Dweck & Leggett, 1988, p. 256).
vidual’s perceptions of his or her competence at In summary, although a variety of terms have
the task. Maehr noted in particular that when indi- been used to describe these two broad classes of
viduals hold task goals, “social comparisons of goals, we have chosen to refer to these goals as
performance are remote or are virtually nonexis- “mastery” and “performance” goals for the
tent” (Maehr, 1984, p. 129). In contrast, Maehr remainder of this chapter. All of the various defi-
defined “ego goals”—his version of performance nitions suggest that when students pursue mas-
goals—in terms of being able to exceed a standard tery goals, they are interested in truly mastering
of performance, particularly as related to the per- the task, they are concerned with gaining compe-
formance of other individuals. Interestingly, tence, and they are willing and eager to exert
Maehr distinguished ego goals from “extrinsic effort in order to achieve mastery. In contrast,
goals,” which he described as a separate class of when students pursue performance goals, they
goals that are related to earning rewards (e.g., are interested in demonstrating their ability rela-
money or a prize) that are not directly aligned with tive to others, in outperforming others, and in
the reasons why an individual would engage with being judged by others as being competent at
a given task in the first place. academic tasks.
Nicholls (1989) described students’ goals as
motivational orientations; he labeled the two
dimensions as task orientation and ego orientation. Current Four-Factor Model
The specific types of survey items that he and his of Achievement Goal Theory
colleagues developed to measure these orienta-
tions focused on whether students feel “pleased” The mastery/performance distinction has been
when they accomplish certain tasks. For example, studied by many researchers, for many years (see
a student with a high task orientation is a student Anderman & Wolters, 2006; Urdan, 1997, for
who feels pleased when he or she works hard, reviews). However, in the mid-1990s, several
tries hard, and understands the material. In con- researchers argued that the distinction between
trast, a student with a high ego orientation feels approach and avoid orientations also should be
pleased when he or she feels superior to others considered within a goal orientation framework.
and beats others (Nicholls, Cobb, Wood, Yackel, Elliot and Harackiewicz (1996) noted that some
& Patashnick, 1990). In contrast, Ames’ (1987) of the early work by achievement goal research-
descriptions of goal orientations were influenced ers such as Dweck and Nicholls did distinguish
by earlier work by Maehr and Nichols. She between approach and avoid forms of perfor-
described task-oriented students as those who mance goals, but these distinctions were lost in
“are interested in developing their ability and later definitions.
gaining mastery,” and ego-oriented students as A trichotomous framework for achievement
those who “want to demonstrate that they have goals suggests that in addition to mastery goals, a
ability” (Ames, 1987, p. 127). distinction should be made between performance-
Dweck and her colleagues distinguished approach and performance-avoid goals (Elliot,
between learning (analogous to mastery) and per- 1999). Elliot and Harackiewicz (1996) initially
formance goals and argued that learners frame conducted experiments in which participants were
their responses to and interpretations of events asked to solve puzzles using mastery goals, per-
based on these goals (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; formance-approach goals, and performance-avoid
176 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

goals. Participants in the performance-approach mastery-avoid construct. A student who endorses


condition were informed that students who solve mastery-avoid goals wants to avoid misunder-
the puzzles more successfully than other students standing or losing a sense of competence. The
at the same university “have good puzzle solving 2 × 2 model has been supported in both North
ability” (p. 468); in contrast, students in the per- American (Conroy, Elliot, & Hofer, 2003) and
formance-avoid condition were told that if they international samples (Bong, 2009).
solved fewer puzzles than others, they would Currently, goal orientation theorists generally
demonstrate that they “have poor puzzle solving support the 2 × 2 model. Nevertheless, the valid-
ability” (p. 468). Results indicated that partici- ity of mastery-avoid goals has been questioned
pants in the performance-avoid condition dis- (e.g., Sideridis & Mouratidis, 2008). Specifically,
played lower intrinsic motivation toward the some researchers question whether individuals
puzzles than those in the performance-approach actually think about mastery-avoid goals in real-
condition. life situations. Ciani and Sheldon (2010) con-
Midgley and her colleagues developed a ducted a qualitative study in which they
widely used measure of achievement goals, the interviewed Division I college baseball players
Patterns of Adaptive Learning Survey (PALS) about their endorsement of mastery-avoid goals
(Midgley et al., 2000). Initially, separate mea- while playing baseball. Although players
sures of performance-approach and performance- endorsed both high and low levels of mastery-
avoid goal orientations were developed avoid goals, when players who endorsed mastery-
(Middleton & Midgley, 1997). Using a large avoid goals were probed about these beliefs,
sample of middle school students, Middleton and results suggested that the players actually were
Midgley demonstrated that performance goals referring to mastery-approach goals in many
could be separated into performance-approach cases. Ciani and Sheldon suggested that one of
and performance-avoid goal orientations. They the reasons for this may be that it is difficult to
operationalized performance-approach goals in truly get study participants to understand the
terms of students (a) wanting to do better than nuances of what a “mastery-avoid” goal is, using
other students in their class and (b) wanting to a survey instrument.
demonstrate that they are more competent than In addition, some research suggests some stu-
others; in contrast, performance-avoid goals were dents may have difficulty distinguishing between
operationalized in terms of wanting to avoid performance-approach and performance-avoid
appearing incompetent or “dumb.” Skaalvik goals. For example, Urdan and Mestas (2006)
(1997) also examined different types of perfor- conducted an interview study with 53 high
mance goals. Specifically, using a sample of school seniors who all reported high levels of
Norwegian sixth and eighth grade students, performance-avoid goals (as determined by
Skaalvik developed a measure of self-enhancing responses to a survey). Students were probed
ego orientation (similar to a performance- about their responses to various survey items.
approach goal orientation) and a measure of a Results indicated that students often did not eas-
self-defeating ego orientation (similar to a per- ily distinguish between performance-approach
formance-avoid goal orientation). and performance-avoid goals. In addition, stu-
The approach/avoid distinction was also dents indicated that they pursue performance
applied to mastery goal orientation, resulting in a goals for a variety of different reasons (e.g., to
2 × 2 framework for achievement goals (Elliot & look smart, to please parents, to look smart to
McGregor, 2001). In this model, mastery goals one’s peers, or simply because students enjoyed
are broken down into mastery-approach and competition). Additional work on the measure-
mastery-avoid goals, matching the separation of ment, interpretation, and predictive validity of
performance-approach and performance-avoid mastery-avoid goal orientation will be an impor-
goals. The new addition to the model was the tant area for future research.
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 177

Goal Orientations and Academic


Correlates of Goal Orientations
Achievement
Much of the research conducted over the past two
Academic achievement often is regarded as one
decades by achievement goal theorists has
of the most important educational outcomes.
focused on relations between students’ goal ori-
Researchers and practitioners have been particu-
entations and a variety of academic outcomes,
larly interested in the relations of goal orienta-
including implicit beliefs about intelligence, aca-
tions to achievement since academic achievement
demic achievement, and numerous aspects of
is greatly valued as an indicator of educational
engagement. In the following sections, we review
performance (Anderman, Anderman, Yough, &
the major findings of these studies.
Gimbert, 2010; Hattie & Anderman, in press).
Relations between goal orientations and aca-
demic achievement are somewhat inconsistent.
Goal Orientations and Beliefs About Although reasons are not clear, much depends
Intelligence on how student achievement is measured.
Achievement can be measured in a variety of
Carol Dweck and her colleagues have examined ways (e.g., scores on standardized tests, teacher-
students’ beliefs about intelligence and how those made tests, or teacher-assigned grades that may
beliefs are related to a variety of academic out- or may not include homework or conduct) and do
comes (Dweck, 2000). When students endorse an not necessarily reflect students’ real understand-
entity theory of intelligence, they believe that their ing. A mastery goal orientation, with its accom-
intellectual abilities are fixed (i.e., generally panying thoughtfulness and strategic effort, is only
unchangeable); in contrast, when students endorse likely to be important if achievement tests require
an incremental view of intelligence, they believe students to demonstrate deep understanding; if
that their intellectual abilities are malleable (Dweck simple memorization is sufficient to score well,
& Leggett, 1988). Research generally indicates then a mastery goal orientation is not likely to be
that incremental beliefs about intelligence are related differentially to test scores or grades.
associated with a host of adaptive outcomes, Furthermore, a very strong desire to outscore oth-
including self-regulated learning (Dweck & ers may lead students to having inflated achieve-
Master, 2008), academic achievement (Blackwell, ment scores through means such as cheating.
Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007), and the utilization
of remedial (as opposed to defensive) strategies Mastery Goal Orientation
when self-esteem is threatened (Nussbaum & In a comprehensive study examining over 90
Dweck, 2008). peer-reviewed articles that addressed the rela-
Beliefs about intelligence also have been tions of achievement goals to academic achieve-
examined in relation to goal orientations. ment, Linnenbrink-Garcia, Tyson, and Patall
Research generally indicates that when students (2008) reported that mastery goals appear some-
believe that intelligence is incremental, they are times to be beneficial for academic achieve-
likely to endorse mastery goals; in contrast, when ment, as expected theoretically. For example,
students believe that intelligence is fixed and Bong (2009) found positive correlations between
unchangeable, they are likely to adopt perfor- upper elementary and middle school students’
mance goals (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). Although mastery-approach goals and math achievement.
a variety of studies have revealed similar rela- However, across studies, results are somewhat
tions between implicit theories of intelligence mixed; numerous studies have not shown the
and goal orientations, some studies have failed expected positive direct relations between mas-
to replicate these findings (e.g., Dupeyrat & tery goals and achievement (Ames & Archer,
Marine, 2005). 1988; Anderman & Johnston, 1998; Barron &
178 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

Harackiewicz, 2001; Daniels et al., 2009; Elliot in goal measures are assessed differently, research
& Church, 1997; Elliot & McGregor, 2001; results may vary (Hulleman, Schrager, Bodmann,
Harackiewicz, Barron, Carter, Lehto, & Elliot, & Harackiewicz, 2010).
1997; Pintrich, 2000a; Skaalvik, 1997). Considering relations between performance-
In addition, results of several studies indicate approach orientation and achievement for stu-
that mastery goals are sometimes indirectly dents as a whole may also mask possible
related to achievement. Specifically, mastery differential relations depending on student char-
goals often are predictive of mediators, such as acteristics. For example, there has been concern
affect or certain types of behaviors, that are in about the long-term outcomes for performance-
turn related to achievement. Thus, students who approach oriented students who seem to do well
endorse mastery goals are more likely to either in the short term (Midgley, Kaplan, & Middleton,
engage in achievement-promoting behaviors or 2001): What happens when these students move
experience affect that is related to achievement. to a new, more competitive, or challenging envi-
For example, in one study, adolescents who ronment (e.g., a larger school, a class with more
reported being mastery-oriented toward current advanced content or more high-achieving stu-
events were more likely to engage in news- dents)? One possibility is that students accus-
seeking behaviors outside of school; in turn, tomed to outperforming others and being viewed
these behaviors were directly and positively that way by other people, but who are not confi-
predicted knowledge of current events dent about maintaining their rank, will move to
(Anderman & Johnston, 1998). In another study, an avoidance focus. This was what Middleton,
mastery orientation, although not directly pre- Kaplan, and Midgley (2004) found. Specifically,
dictive of achievement, was related inversely to sixth graders’ performance-approach orientation
indicators of negative affect (e.g., boredom, predicted a performance-avoid orientation in sev-
anxiety), which in turn were related to lower enth grade, but only for students with high self-
academic achievement (Daniels et al., 2009). efficacy in sixth grade. That is, students who were
concerned with outscoring others and who felt
Performance Goal Orientation confident of their abilities were more likely, as
The relations between performance-approach they progressed through middle school, to become
goals and academic achievement are fairly con- more focused on protecting their image and not
sistent for college students. In many studies, the looking incompetent compared to other students.
adoption of performance-approach goals is related This is concerning, given the poor outcomes
to high achievement (Church, Elliot, & Gable, associated with a performance-avoid orientation.
2001; Daniels et al., 2009; Elliot & Church, 1997; Performance-avoid goal orientations are con-
Elliot & McGregor, 2001; Elliot, McGregor, & sistently and negatively related to achievement.
Gable, 1999; Harackiewicz et al., 1997). These results have been documented for both
For younger students, a similar pattern is found college students (e.g., Elliot et al., 1999) and
in some studies, although relations at times are younger adolescents (e.g., Middleton & Midgley,
not as strong. For example, Bong (2009) reported 1997; Wolters, 2004).
a low (.19) correlation between performance-
approach goals and achievement for students in
middle school and lower elementary grades, but Goal Orientations and Engagement
no relation for middle and upper elementary stu-
dents. Wolters (2004) found a weak positive rela- Since we use goal orientation theory as our
tion between performance-approach goals and framework for explaining motivation and engage-
math grades in middle school students. Some of ment, it is important to distinguish how we are
these differential relations may at least in part be defining goal orientations and how we are defin-
explained by how researchers operationalize goal ing engagement. As noted by Appleton,
orientations on survey instruments. When items Christenson, and Furlong (2008), the phrase
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 179

“academic engagement” needs empirical and goal orientations supports this since the adoption
conceptual clarification; similar arguments have of mastery goals is related to more effective aca-
been made regarding the need for the clarifica- demic strategy usage (Graham & Golan, 1991;
tion of terms in the motivation field in general Nolen, 1988). In the following sections, we
(Murphy & Alexander, 2000) and within goal describe the relations of each type of engagement
theory specifically (Pintrich, 2000b). For our to goal orientations.
review of the relations between goal orientations
and engagement, we have adopted the model Cognitive Engagement
described by Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and Paris The types of goal orientations that students adopt
(2004). In that model, the construct of engage- are related to the kinds of cognitive and self-regu-
ment is described as being multidimensional; latory strategies they use when engaged with aca-
specifically, engagement consists of behavioral, demic tasks. Results of numerous studies indicate
emotional, and cognitive forms of engagement. that when students focus on mastery, they tend to
Cognitive engagement refers to learners’ willing- be willing to think deeply and broadly about their
ness to exert the necessary effort to understand academic work; they use effective learning and
and master complex phenomena; emotional self-regulatory strategies, including monitoring
engagement refers to learners’ positive and nega- their comprehension and thinking about how cur-
tive affective reactions to aspects of schooling; rent academic tasks are related to previously
and behavioral engagement refers to actual par- learned information (e.g., Anderman & Young,
ticipation in specific activities that are related to 1994; Graham & Golan, 1991; Nolen & Haladyna,
achievement (Fredricks et al., 2004, p. 60). 1990; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Wolters, 2004).
Specifically, the definitions of engagement pro- For example, Nolen (1988), in an early study,
vided by Fredricks and her colleagues imply that found that both general and task-specific mastery
students are engaged while they are working on a (task) goal orientations were related positively to
specific task; thus, a student is cognitively middle students’ use of both deep-processing
engaged when he or she is exerting appropriate strategies (e.g., figuring out how new information
effort while completing a task; a student’s emo- fits with prior knowledge, monitoring one’s com-
tional engagement with a task is operationalized prehension) and surface-level strategies (e.g.,
in terms of her affective reactions to the task memorizing words, rehearsing information). More
(while engaging with the task); and a student’s recent research with a large sample of South
behavioral engagement with the task is opera- Korean adolescents, measuring both mastery-
tionalized in terms of her actual behaviors during approach and mastery-avoid orientations, indi-
the task. Goal orientations, in contrast, are opera- cated that both were related positively to use of
tionalized in terms of the goals that students have cognitive strategies (rehearsal, elaboration, and
toward tasks both prior to and during task par- organizational strategies) and more adaptive
ticipation. Thus, in our conceptualization of the self-regulation, although the associations with
relations between goal orientations and engage- mastery-avoid goals were weaker (Bong, 2009).
ment, the specific goal orientation that a student The evidence for students holding a perfor-
holds for a particular task will determine the mance goal orientation is more mixed, although
quality of the student’s engagement with the task no study has identified positive links between
(Ames, 1992a, 1992b). For example, when a stu- performance-avoid goals and cognitive engage-
dent is highly mastery goal–oriented toward a ment. In Nolen’s (1988) study, students’ adoption
particular task, the quality of cognitive, emo- of performance (ego) goals was either unrelated or
tional, and behavioral engagement will likely be negatively related to their use of deep-processing
adaptive for learning (because the student’s goal strategies and either unrelated or positively related
is task mastery, which requires high levels of to using surface-level strategies; approach and
engagement). Indeed, evidence from studies avoid orientations were not yet differentiated.
examining students’ effective strategy usage and Bong (2009) found that performance-approach
180 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

goals were related to greater use of cognitive Duchesne and colleagues examined a longitudinal
strategies and more adaptive self-regulation, sample of 498 early adolescents (Duchesne &
whereas performance-avoid goals were not. When Ratelle, 2010). Students reported their percep-
students are focused on their relative performance tions of general parental involvement and control
and are busy thinking about ability differences, and completed measures of anxiety and depres-
they simply may not have the cognitive resources sion at the end of the sixth grade in elementary
to devote to the use of effective cognitive and school; students then reported their achievement
self-regulatory strategies. goals during the following year, at the end of the
first year of middle school (seventh grade). Results
Emotional Engagement indicated that mastery goals were predicted by
Several researchers have examined the relations perceptions of parental involvement; however,
between goal orientations and various indicators anxiety mediated the relation between perceptions
of emotional engagement, such as affect and of parents as controlling and performance goals
motivation. Results generally indicate that mas- (combined approach and avoid). Specifically, stu-
tery goals are related to positive affect about dents who perceived their parents as controlling
school (e.g., Roeser, Midgley, & Urdan, 1996) experienced greater anxiety; anxiety in turn posi-
and different aspects of motivation, such as tively predicted performance goals.
intrinsic motivation, positive self-concept, and
self-efficacy (e.g., Murayama & Elliot, 2009). Behavioral Engagement
However, the relations of performance goals to The goal orientations that students adopt are also
affect and motivation are somewhat mixed. associated with a range of behaviors evident in
Daniels et al. (2009) examined the relations the classroom. For example, a mastery orientation
between emotions, goal orientations, and achieve- is associated with positive academic behaviors,
ment in a large sample of college students. Results such as expending effort (Miller, Greene,
indicated that feelings of hopefulness were Montalvo, Ravindran, & Nichols, 1996), discuss-
related positively to both mastery- and perfor- ing schoolwork with other students (Patrick,
mance-approach goals, whereas feelings of help- Ryan, & Kaplan, 2007), engaging in relevant
lessness were inversely related to mastery goals, activities outside of school (Anderman &
but unrelated to performance goals. Skaalvik Johnston, 1998), and seeking help when needed
(1997) examined the relations between mastery (Ryan & Pintrich, 1997). Conversely, a perfor-
(task) goals, self-defeating ego goals (i.e., perfor- mance orientation is related to avoiding seeking
mance-avoid goals), and self-enhancing ego goals needed help (Ryan & Pintrich) and being disrup-
(i.e., performance-approach goals), and several tive during lessons (Ryan & Patrick, 2001).
measures of affect. Results indicated that the
adoption of mastery goals was related positively
to self-esteem, and negatively to math anxiety. Summary
The adoption of performance-approach goals was
related weakly and positively to self-esteem, and Achievement goal theory has developed into a
weakly and negatively to math anxiety. The adop- robust, empirically supported framework for
tion of performance-avoid goals was related neg- examining student motivation. The types of goal
atively to self-esteem and positively to both math orientations that students adopt are related in
and verbal anxiety. important ways to their achievement, affect,
Recent research suggests that the achievement beliefs about the nature of intelligence, and
goals of early adolescents may be predicted by cognitive/self-regulatory strategy use. Goal
parental involvement and control (i.e., related to orientations also are related to cognitive, emo-
numerous aspects of students’ lives, not just aca- tional, and behavioral engagement. Although
demics), as well as anxiety and depression during goal orientations represent cognitions that are
the elementary school years. In a recent study, related to behavior, goals are influenced by the
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 181

social contexts in which students participate. In a perception that achievement and success entail
the next section, we examine the relations outperforming others or surpassing normative
between social contexts and achievement goals. standards (Ames, 1992b). Classroom goal struc-
tures are usually measured by student self-report
surveys, predominantly with scales from the
Classroom Goal Structures Pattern of Adaptive Learning Survey (PALS;
Midgley et al., 1996, 2000). Just as mastery and
A particularly important aspect of achievement performance goal orientations are orthogonal, so
goal theory is its attention to the educational con- too are classroom goal structures. That is, class-
texts within which students are, or are supposed rooms may be high in both mastery and perfor-
to be, engaged. This is because, according to goal mance goal structure, high in just one, low in
theory, students’ motivation is influenced not both, or any other configuration.
only by their individual personal characteristics, Classroom goal structures are not “objective”
beliefs, and achievement histories, but also by characteristics but instead depend on how indi-
the contexts in which they learn. Within these vidual students perceive and give meaning to their
environments, students’ construals of what is val- classroom experiences (Ames, 1992b). Because
ued in terms of schooling and what constitutes students’ individual past and current experiences
achievement and success influence their goal ori- and interpretations contribute to their current per-
entations and therefore play a significant role in ceptions, students in the same class will not nec-
affecting the nature and quality of engagement in essarily perceive the classroom goal structures in
learning tasks (Ames, 1992b; Maehr, 1984; the same way (Ames, 1992b). Adding to variabil-
Nicholls, 1989). We focus here on research within ity in perceptions, students in the same classroom
goal theory that addresses an especially critical are often treated differently and therefore do not
and salient educational context—classrooms. even experience the same educational context
During the considerable amount of time stu- (Brophy, 1985; Turner & Patrick, 2004).
dents spend in classrooms, they construct mean- Teachers play a potent role in contributing to
ing systems or schema about the purpose and the classroom goal structures through explicit
meaning of schooling and academics from their and implicit messages about the purpose of
experiences and perceptions of what is empha- school activities, what counts as learning, and the
sized in the classroom. These perceptions of what role of student talk and through the norms and
is emphasized are termed classroom goal struc- rules they establish for student behavior. These
tures (Ames, 1984, 1992b). Specifically, class- norms begin from the first days of the school
room goal structures encompass students’ year—indeed, they are particularly explicit at this
subjective perceptions of the meaning of aca- time when teachers introduce and socialize stu-
demic tasks, competence, success, and purposes dents to their philosophies and beliefs. Early
for students’ engaging in schoolwork. From a teacher practices foreshadow significant differ-
goal theory perspective, classroom goal struc- ences in mastery and performance classroom
tures represent a powerful empirical tool that can goal structures, both after a few months and near
be used to examine the roles of classroom con- the end of the year (Patrick, Anderman, Ryan,
texts in student motivation (Meece et al., 2006). Edelin, & Midgley, 2001; Patrick, Turner, Meyer,
Personal mastery and performance goal orien- & Midgley, 2003).
tations, reviewed in the previous section, have
parallels in classroom mastery and performance
goal structures. Accordingly, a classroom mas- Classroom Mastery Goal Structure
tery goal structure involves a perception that
learning and understanding are valued and that A classroom mastery goal structure involves
success is indicated by personal improvement. a perception that students’ real learning and
A classroom performance goal structure involves understanding, rather than just memorization, are
182 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

valued and that success is accompanied by effort Anderman, 1999; Kaplan & Midgley, 1999),
and indicated by personal improvement. Thus, a feelings of belonging at school (Anderman, 2003;
classroom mastery goal structure emphasizes an Anderman & Anderman, 1999; Stevens,
incremental theory of ability (Ames, 1992b). Hamman, & Olivarez, 2007), and desire to fol-
Theoretically, perceptions of a classroom mas- low the school’s expectations (i.e., social respon-
tery goal structure influence students’ invoking a sibility goal; Anderman & Anderman). Students
mastery goal orientation for themselves in that in these environments express adaptive motiva-
context; that is, students are likely to focus on tional beliefs, such as self-efficacy and intrinsic
their own improvement and understanding when motivation (Fast et al., 2010; Murayama & Elliot,
these aspects are emphasized. Mastery goal ori- 2009; Wolters, 2004). Moreover, students express
entation, in turn, is believed to influence students’ more positive views about their schoolwork, such
optimal effort, affect, use of adaptive learning as preference for challenge (Ames & Archer,
strategies, and, ultimately, achievement (Ames, 1988), the usefulness of learning strategies
1992b). There is a considerable body of empiri- (Nolen & Haladyna, 1990), satisfaction with
cal studies that provide support for these tenets, their learning (Nolen, 2003), and adaptive coping
as we discuss next. responses after failure (Kaplan & Midgley, 1999),
compared to those in settings with low classroom
Associations with Student Engagement mastery goal structure.
A classroom mastery goal structure constitutes a
holistic system of meanings. Accordingly, it is Cognitive Engagement
associated with all aspects of engagement— Not surprisingly, given students’ positive affect
emotional (e.g., enjoyment, interest, efficacy, and motivation, they tend to be more cognitively
commitment), cognitive (e.g., thoughtfulness, engaged in classrooms with a high (compared to
use of learning strategies, self-regulation), and low) classroom mastery goal structure. Specifically,
behavioral (e.g., effort, persistence, asking for a classroom mastery goal structure is associated
help). From both theoretical and practical stand- positively with the use of effective cognitive strat-
points, all aspects of engagement should be high egies (e.g., elaboration) and metacognitive strate-
in classrooms that are perceived as emphasizing gies (e.g., planning, monitoring, regulating)
mastery. Specifically, when the overarching focus (Ames & Archer, 1988; Wolters, 2004), just as
in the classroom is perceived as increasing each personal mastery goal orientation is.
student’s understanding and skill, with success
gauged by personal improvement (i.e., classroom Behavioral Engagement
mastery structure), it is adaptive and beneficial Underscoring the close connections of emotional
for students to be fully and thoroughly engaged and cognitive engagement with behavior, class-
with those tasks. room mastery goal structure is related positively
to many forms of adaptive behavioral engage-
Emotional Engagement ment. This is because working to learn the mate-
Students’ perceptions that their teacher and class- rial is likely to pay off for students if all can
room emphasize mastery are related significantly experience success, rather than just a few.
to their personal mastery goal orientation (Nolen Classrooms that are perceived, on average, as
& Haladyna, 1990; Wolters, 2004). Also, given having a high (compared to low) mastery goal
that all students can be successful when success structure tend to have students who expend effort,
is viewed as personal improvement, students tend persist with tasks (Wolters, 2004), and use adap-
to experience positive affect and motivational tive help-seeking strategies such as asking for
beliefs in mastery goal structured classrooms. explanations but not answers (Karabenick, 2004).
Specifically, a perceived classroom mastery goal They also have the lowest average rates of mal-
structure is related positively to students’ posi- adaptive student behaviors, including not asking
tive school-related affect (Ames & Archer, 1988; for help when it is needed (Karabenick, 2004;
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 183

Ryan, Gheen, & Midgley, 1998), self-handicapping is for individuals’ personal orientations. During
(i.e., purposefully withdrawing effort; Midgley & naturalistic classroom observations, we see teachers
Urdan, 2001; Urdan & Midgley, 2003), being suggesting, implicitly or explicitly, that students
disruptive (Kaplan, Gheen, & Midgley, 2002), who score the highest are “smarter” or more able
procrastinating (Wolters, 2004), and cheating than are those with lower scores; however, we
(Murdock, Hale, & Weber, 2001). have not observed teachers or classrooms pro-
moting either a distinguishable approach or
avoidance orientation. We think that students in
Classroom Performance Goal Structure performance-focused classrooms evaluate, per-
haps subconsciously, their likelihood of being
A classroom performance goal structure conveys ranked highly. If they view outperforming others
to students that learning is predominantly a as realistic, they will likely take an approach ori-
means of achieving recognition and prestige, and entation, and if they are pessimistic about their
it is characterized by relative ability comparisons chances of outscoring others, they will instead
among students. Success is indicated by outper- likely adopt an avoidance orientation. Therefore,
forming others or surpassing normative standards a general classroom performance goal structure
(Ames, 1992b). An integral characteristic of may invoke some students taking a performance-
classroom performance goal structure is that approach orientation and others in the same class-
students are compared to each other, with an room being performance-avoid oriented.
inherent assumption that this hierarchy is rela-
tively stable and reflects some aspect of students’ Associations with Student Engagement
ability. That is, it reflects an entity view of intel- Perceiving a classroom performance goal struc-
ligence (Dweck, 2000). ture is associated with affective, cognitive, and
A classroom performance goal structure is dif- behavioral engagement. In contrast to the mixed
ferent from an extrinsic goal structure; the latter findings associated with a personal performance-
conveys that the purpose of engaging in academic approach goal orientation, perceiving a class-
tasks is to gain external incentives; however, the room performance goal structure is generally
success of any one student does not affect the associated with students’ beliefs and behaviors
success of others (see Urdan, 1997). That is, if that are less conducive, and often detrimental, to
students are graded on a curve, with grades indi- learning and achievement. We review this
cating relative position, a classroom performance research briefly next.
goal structure is invoked; however, if grades (or
other incentives) are very salient but do not sig- Emotional Engagement
nify students’ relative placement, a classroom Students’ perceptions that their teacher and class-
extrinsic goal structure is involved. room emphasize relative ability comparisons
After the recognition that personal perfor- (i.e., have a high classroom performance goal
mance goal orientations could be separated, theo- structure) are related to the adoption of personal
retically and empirically, into approach and performance-approach and/or performance-avoid
avoidance dimensions, some researchers have goals (Wolters, 2004). A pervasive focus on how
made the same distinction with classroom perfor- students “stack up” against each other can pro-
mance goal structure (e.g., Karabenick, 2004; voke students to focus on the outcomes of their
Murayama & Elliot, 2009). That is, they suggest efforts, rather than the process of learning. This
that some performance-focused classrooms state of affairs is not comfortable for many stu-
emphasize approach characteristics, such as scoring dents, not just those near the bottom of the
better than others, whereas others emphasize achievement continuum, and therefore students
avoidance characteristics, such as not doing tend to experience negative affect and motiva-
worse than others. However, we do not find this tional beliefs in these types of classrooms.
distinction to be meaningful in classrooms, like it Students in classrooms with a strong performance
184 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

goal structure tend to express more negative affect often detrimental, to learning. With an emphasis
about school (Ames & Archer, 1988; Anderman, on outcomes but not process, students may feel
1999; Kaplan & Midgley, 1999), and less sense encouraged to disregard how they come to out-
of belonging at school (Anderman & Anderman, score others and be concerned only that they do.
1999), compared to those in classrooms with low Consistent with this, cheating is most prevalent in
perceived performance goal structure. Similarly, classrooms with a high performance goal struc-
students view teachers of performance-focused ture (Anderman, Griesinger, & Westerfield, 1998;
classrooms as less fair (Murdock, Miller, & Murdock et al., 2004). In performance-structured
Kohlhardt, 2004) and more to blame for student classrooms, students who are not successful at a
dishonesty (Murdock, Miller, & Goetzinger, task immediately may be unlikely to continue
2007), compared to teachers of mastery-focused trying, given both that a hierarchy of ability tends
classrooms. Students’ intrinsic motivation and to invoke an entity view of ability, and high effort
academic self-concepts are related inversely to without success is suggestive of low ability. As
classroom performance goal structure (Ames & posited, classroom performance goal structure is
Archer, 1988; Murayama & Elliot, 2009). There related inversely to students’ task persistence
is also greater use of maladaptive coping strate- (Wolters, 2004).
gies after failure, such as denial or projecting Furthermore, in classrooms with a perfor-
blame onto other people or events (Kaplan & mance goal structure, students who are pessimis-
Midgley, 1999) or attributing failures to one’s tic about their chances of placing near the top of
own lack of ability (Ames & Archer, 1988). the hierarchy may find ways to avoid engaging in
schoolwork and therefore protect their self-worth
Cognitive Engagement by not providing evidence that their ability is
There is some evidence indicating that perceiv- lower than their classmates’. Again, research
ing a classroom as being focused on ability diffe- supports this premise. Classrooms perceived, on
rences is related to lower academic achievement. average, as being highly performance-focused
Anderman and Midgley (1997) examined the tend to have the highest rates of students not
relations between perceptions of classroom per- seeking help when they need it (Ryan et al.,
formance goal structures and end-of-year grades 1998), procrastinating (Wolters, 2004), self-
both before and after the transition from elemen- handicapping (Midgley & Urdan, 2001; Urdan,
tary school into middle school. Results in both 2004; Urdan, Midgley, & Anderman, 1998), and
English and math classes indicated that when stu- being disruptive (Kaplan et al., 2002; Ryan &
dents perceived a classroom performance goal Patrick, 2001).
structure, their end-of-year grades after the tran- Findings of associations between classroom
sition were lower in both subjects than they had performance goal structures and student achieve-
been a year previously. This is related in part to ment have been mixed across studies. For exam-
the fact that grading practices often become more ple, classroom performance goal structure has
focused on relative ability of students after the been related inversely to test scores (Nolen,
middle school transition (Eccles & Midgley, 2003), but not related to grades (Wolters, 2004).
1989; Midgley, Anderman, & Hicks, 1995). Researchers have long noted, however, that the
Similar patterns of relations between perceptions different ways that achievement is measured,
of classroom performance goal structures and including differences among teachers in how
achievement have been reported in other studies grades are assigned and differences between
as well (e.g., Anderman & Anderman, 1999). standardized assessments and teacher-assigned
grades, make for difficulties with conducting
Behavioral Engagement research on these relations. In addition, grade-
When classrooms are perceived as emphasizing a level differences in assessment procedures (e.g.,
hierarchy of ability and students’ relative position developmental differences in grading practices
within that hierarchy, students are likely to report across elementary and high school settings) com-
engaging in behaviors that are not conducive, and pound these difficulties.
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 185

Teacher Practices Associated open-ended questions (Meece, 1991; Patrick &


with Classroom Goal Structures Ryan, 2008; Patrick et al., 2001). However, two
other facets of classrooms associated with mas-
Classroom Mastery Goal Structure tery goal structure have also been identified:
Because a classroom mastery goal structure repre- social relationships and pedagogical practices.
sents a particularly adaptive learning environment, A sizable body of research has documented the
goal theorists recommend that teachers create importance of social relationship features for per-
mastery goal–focused classrooms (e.g., Midgley, ceptions of classroom mastery goal structure.
1993). To this end, researchers have identified Teachers in high mastery-focused classrooms
teacher practices associated with a classroom appear to promote a more interpersonally positive
mastery goal structure. Importantly, classroom climate and engage in more motivationally sup-
mastery goal structure is established by a coherent portive interactions (e.g., encouraging and sup-
set of practices that together communicate a con- porting students’ persistence, using humor,
sistent perspective toward learning and task engage- showing enthusiasm) compared to teachers in low
ment; isolated practices are generally not sufficient mastery-focused classrooms (Patrick et al., 2001,
to influence students’ overall meaning systems. 2003; Turner et al., 2002). Teacher support (for
The holistic approach to creating a mastery students’ learning and for students as people),
goal–structured classroom was first represented mutual respect, positive affect, and teacher enthu-
by Ames’ (1990, 1992a) conceptual framework, siasm are salient in high, but not low, mastery-
where she organized teaching principles and focused classrooms (Miller & Murdock, 2007;
strategies associated with a classroom mastery Patrick et al., 2001, 2003; Turner et al., 2002).
goal structure into six categories. This framework, Those findings have led to the revised acronym
represented by the acronym TARGET (see TARGETS. That is, for classroom mastery goal
Epstein, 1983), is comprised of the academic task, structure, in addition to features of the other six cat-
authority, recognition, grouping, evaluation, and egories, social relationships should be respectful,
time. Specifically, tasks should be meaningful, supportive of students both socioemotionally and
challenging, and interesting, and there should be a academically, and convey positive affect about
range of task options available so that ability dif- both students and the content to be learned.
ferences are not accentuated. The teacher should There is also evidence that teachers’ pedagog-
share authority and responsibility for rules and ical approaches comprise another category of
decisions with the students. Recognition should practices associated with a classroom mastery
be available to all students, should involve prog- goal structure (e.g., Murdock et al., 2004; Patrick
ress or effort, and there should be few opportuni- & Ryan, 2008). For example, students report that
ties for social comparison among students. the extent to which teachers make efforts to
Grouping should be flexible and heterogeneous, explain the material to them, help them under-
and students should not be grouped by ability. stand, and use a variety of approaches as neces-
Evaluation should be criterion-referenced, not sary influences their views of the classroom’s
made public, and grades and test scores should be mastery goal structure (Patrick & Ryan, 2008).
interpreted in terms of improvement and effort. Observational studies support this finding. High,
And, finally, there should be flexible use of time in but not low, mastery-focused teachers use active
the classroom and opportunities for student self- instructional approaches and adapt instruction to
pacing. As mentioned, Ames was clear that prac- their students’ developmental levels (Meece,
tices within all six categories must be integrated 1991) and engage in academic press (Anderman,
in order for a classroom mastery goal structure to Andrzejewski, & Allen, 2011). They also provide
be evident (see also Maehr & Anderman, 1993). supportive instructional discourse, or scaffold-
Support for the relevance and utility of ing, comprised of negotiating with students what
TARGET has come from multimethod studies, academic tasks involve and transferring responsi-
whereby survey measures were triangulated with bility for tasks to students in accordance with
classroom observations or students’ responses to their capabilities (Turner et al., 2002).
186 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

Classroom Performance Goal Structure are generally related to beneficial outcomes,


There has been less interest in identifying teacher whereas perceptions of a classroom performance
practices associated with classroom performance goal structure are related to a mixed array of out-
goal structure than with classroom mastery goal comes. In terms of the relations of classroom goal
structure. One reason is the possibility that the structures to engagement, the goal structure that
crucial element of learning environments is a is perceived in the classroom is related to the
high classroom mastery goal structure, regardless quality of engagement evidenced by the student.
of the extent of classroom performance goal As we have reviewed, perceptions of classroom
structure (Midgley, 2002; Midgley et al., 2001). mastery and performance goal structures are
Nevertheless, it may be valuable to identify prac- related to cognitive, emotional, and behavioral
tices associated with classroom performance goal engagement in different ways. The fact that class-
structure, given its associations with negative room goal structures are related to the types of
indicators of students’ engagement; to decrease instructional practices used by teachers in class-
the prevalence of practices that contribute to per- rooms suggests that changes in instructional prac-
ceptions of a performance focus, it is necessary tices may yield benefits for student engagement.
to know the specific practices involved. In addi-
tion, classroom mastery and performance goal
structures can be perceived simultaneously within Conclusion
the same classroom (Midgley, 2002). For example,
a science teacher can emphasize the importance In this chapter, we have reviewed the relations
of effort and persistence in order to understand between personal achievement goals, classroom
and master a principle (mastery), but the same goal structures, and academic engagement.
teacher can also simultaneously emphasize grades Whereas motivation researchers who study
and relative ability (performance). achievement goals and researchers who study
From classroom observation studies, it appears academic engagement operationalize and discuss
that teachers perceived as having a high perfor- constructs in different ways, there is substantial
mance focus emphasize formal assessments, grades, and important overlap. Future research that draws
and students’ relative performance to a substantially upon both achievement goal theory and research
greater extent than do low performance-focused on student engagement will be fruitful, particularly
teachers (Patrick et al., 2001). That information, in terms of developing interventions designed to
however, is considerably less than what is known more fully engage students with academic tasks.
about the aforementioned practices related to class- We briefly reviewed the history of the devel-
room mastery goal structure. This relative paucity opment of achievement goal theory, and we noted
of information is consistent with the argument that the measurement of achievement goal con-
that a focus on external incentives (i.e., classroom structs has changed in subtle yet important ways
extrinsic goal structure) is more prevalent and over the past three decades (for more comprehen-
salient to students than are messages about relative sive reviews, see Elliot, 2005, and Maehr &
performance or ability (Brophy, 2005). Perhaps, Zusho, 2009). We then examined the relations of
ubiquitous societal messages about outscoring personal goal orientations to a variety of educa-
others do more to promote students’ performance tional outcomes, including achievement, strategy
goal orientation than do more proximal teacher usage, and affect. We noted in particular that the
classroom practices. relations between personal goal orientations and
achievement are complex. Finally, we reviewed
research on classroom goal structures. We noted
Summary in particular that facets of classroom contexts that
are controlled by teachers (i.e., instructional prac-
Students’ perceptions of classroom goal struc- tices) affect students’ perceptions of classroom
tures are related to valued motivational outcomes. goal structures, which in turn affect the types of
Perceptions of a classroom mastery goal structure personal goal orientations that students adopt.
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 187

Throughout this chapter, we have noted that the goals and engagement while students are partici-
two main classes of achievement goals (mastery pating in actual academic tasks may be particu-
and performance) are related to engagement in larly fruitful. Studies that utilize the experience
different ways. Engagement researchers typically sampling method (e.g., Shernoff, 2010), where
discuss three distinct forms of engagement (behav- students report on their motivation and engage-
ioral, emotional, and cognitive) (Fredricks et al., ment during actual task participation, may be
2004). Although these three forms of academic especially useful. In addition, it will be particu-
engagement differ, all forms of engagement focus larly important to address developmental shifts in
on students’ involvement with academic tasks motivation and engagement. Given that much
(either behaviorally, emotionally, or cognitively). research indicates that goal orientations and class-
Goal orientation theorists also are concerned room goal structures change as students move
with students’ involvement with academic tasks. from elementary schools into middle schools (e.g.,
When students pursue mastery goals, the stu- Anderman & Midgley, 1997), it will be important
dents’ goal is to truly learn or “master” the task. to examine changes in the relations between goals
Goal orientations can be adopted by students for and engagement across developmental shifts.
many types of learning, including specific activi- In summary, both achievement goal orienta-
ties (e.g., a particular science lab experiment), tion researchers and engagement researchers can
more general academic tasks (e.g., book reports), benefit greatly from collaborative efforts.
or subject domains (e.g., mathematics) (Anderman Although achievement goal researchers and
& Anderman, 2010; Anderman & Wolters, 2006). engagement researchers use different terminolo-
From an engagement perspective, students who gies and constructs, we all are concerned with
hold mastery goals are likely to be more cogni- students’ involvement with academic tasks. As
tively, emotionally, and behaviorally engaged these two lines of research continue to develop, a
with tasks because the overarching “goal” is task convergence and sharing of ideas should lead to
mastery. In contrast, when students pursue vari- richer interventions for students and more effec-
ous types of performance goals, the goal is to tive training for teachers.
demonstrate one’s ability at the task, or, in the
case of avoidance coals, to avoid appearing
incompetent at the task. When students hold such References
goals, their engagement may not be as deep as
with mastery goals; rather, students may engage Ames, C. (1984). Competitive, cooperative, and individu-
with the task at more of a surface level in order to alistic goal structures: A cognitive-motivational analy-
sis. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on
merely demonstrate ability. motivation in education: Vol. 1. Student motivation
For example, a student who holds an avoid- (pp. 177–207). New York: Academic.
ance goal may avoid extensive cognitive engage- Ames, C. (1987). The enhancement of student motivation.
ment with a task (i.e., spend little time on the In M. Maehr & D. Kleiber (Eds.), Advances in motiva-
tion and achievement: Vol. 5. Enhancing motivation
actual task), in order to preserve the appearance (pp. 123–148). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
of competence. Specifically, the student might Ames, C. (1990, April). Achievement goals and classroom
perceive that spending a great deal of time structure: Developing a learning orientation in stu-
engaged with a task would make the student “look dents. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association, Boston,
dumb” to his or her classmates; therefore, although MA.
extensive cognitive engagement might be benefi- Ames, C. (1992a). Achievement goals and the classroom
cial to the student, such engagement may be motivational climate. In D. H. Schunk & J. L. Meece
avoided in order to preserve appearances. (Eds.), Student perception in the classroom
(pp. 327–348). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Future research examining more specifically Associates.
the relations between the various forms of engage- Ames, C. (1992b). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and stu-
ment and goal orientations will be important. dent motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology,
In particular, research that examines students’ 84, 261–271.
188 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the achievement across an adolescent transition: A longi-
classroom: Students’ learning strategies and motiva- tudinal study and an intervention. Child Development,
tion processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 246–263.
80, 260–267. Bong, M. (2009). Age-related differences in achievement
Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2010). Classroom goal orientation. Journal of Educational Psychology,
motivation. Columbus, OH: Merrill/Prentice Hall. 101, 879–896.
Anderman, E. M., Anderman, L. H., Yough, M. S., & Brophy, J. (2005). Goal theorists should move on from
Gimbert, B. J. (2010). Value added models of assess- performance goals. Educational Psychologist, 40,
ment: Implications for motivation and accountability. 167–176.
Educational Psychologist, 45, 123–137. Brophy, J. E. (1985). Teacher-student interaction. In J. B.
Anderman, E. M., Griesinger, T., & Westerfield, G. (1998). Dusek (Ed.), Teacher expectancies (pp. 303–328).
Motivation and cheating during early adolescence. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 84–93. Church, M. A., Elliot, A. J., & Gable, S. L. (2001).
Anderman, E. M., & Johnston, J. (1998). Television news Perceptions of classroom environment, achievement
in the classroom: What are adolescents learning? goals, and achievement outcomes. Journal of
Journal of Adolescent Research, 13, 73–100. Educational Psychology, 93, 43–54.
Anderman, E. M., & Midgley, C. (1997). Changes in Ciani, K. D., & Sheldon, K. M. (2010). Evaluating the
achievement goal orientations, perceived academic mastery-avoidance goal construct: A study of elite col-
competence, and grades across the transition to middle lege baseball players. Psychology of Sport and
level schools. Contemporary Educational Psychology, Exercise, 11, 127–132.
22, 269–298. Conroy, D. E., Elliot, A. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2003). A 2 x
Anderman, E. M., & Wolters, C. (2006). Goals, values, 2 achievement goals questionnaire for sport: Evidence
and affect: Influences on student motivation. In P. for factorial invariance, temporal stability, and exter-
Alexander & P. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educa- nal validity. Journal of Sport and Exercise Physiology,
tional psychology (2nd ed., pp. 369–389). Mahwah, 25, 456–476.
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., Pekrun, R., Hanyes, T.
Anderman, E. M., & Young, A. J. (1994). Motivation and L., Perry, R. P., & Newall, N. E. (2009). A longitudinal
strategy use in science: Individual differences and analysis of achievement goals: From affective ante-
classroom effects. Journal of Research in Science cedents to emotional effects and achievement out-
Teaching, 31, 811–831. comes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101,
Anderman, L. H. (1999). Classroom goal orientation, 948–963.
school belonging, and social goals as predictors of stu- Duchesne, S., & Ratelle, C. (2010). Parental behaviors
dents’ positive and negative affect following the tran- and adolescents’ achievement goals at the beginning
sition to middle school. Journal of Research and of middle school: Emotional problems as potential
Development in Education, 32, 89–103. mediators. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102,
Anderman, L. H. (2003). Academic and social percep- 497–507.
tions as predictors of change in middle school stu- Dupeyrat, C., & Marine, C. (2005). Implicit theories of
dents’ sense of school belonging. The Journal of intelligence, goal orientation, cognitive engagement,
Experimental Education, 72, 5–22. and achievement: A test of Dweck’s model with return-
Anderman, L. H., & Anderman, E. M. (1999). Social predic- ing to school adults. Contemporary Educational
tors of changes in students’ achievement goal orientations. Psychology, 30, 43–59.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 21–37. Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motiva-
Anderman, L. H., & Anderman, E. M. (2000). Considering tion, personality, and development. Philadelphia:
contexts in educational psychology: Introduction to the Psychology Press.
special issue. Educational Psychologist, 35, 67–68. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive
Anderman, L. H., Andrzejewski, C. E., & Allen, J. L. approach to motivation and personality. Psychological
(2011). How do teachers support students’ motivation Review, 95, 256–273.
and learning in their classrooms? Teachers College Dweck, C. S., & Master, A. (2008). Self-theories motivate
Record 113(5) 969–1003. http://www.tcrecord.org. ID self-regulated learning. In D. H. Schunk & B. J.
Number: 16085. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulated
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. learning: Theory, research, and applications (pp.
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- 31–51). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage-environment fit:
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. Developmentally appropriate classrooms for young
Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement adolescents. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research
goals and optimal motivation: Testing multiple goal on motivation in education: Goals and cognitions
models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (Vol. 3, pp. 139–186). New York: Academic.
80, 706–722. Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34,
(2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict 169–189.
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 189

Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achieve- Kaplan, A., & Midgley, C. (1999). The relationship
ment goal construct. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck between perceptions of the classroom goal structure
(Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. and early adolescents’ affect in school: The mediating
52–72). New York: Guilford. role of coping strategies. Learning and Individual
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical Differences, 11, 187–212.
model of approach and avoidance achievement moti- Karabenick, S. A. (2004). Perceived achievement goal
vation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, structure and college student help seeking. Journal of
72, 218–232. Educational Psychology, 96, 569–581.
Elliot, A. J., & Covington, M. V. (2001). Approach and Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Tyson, D. F., & Patall, E. A.
avoidance motivation. Educational Psychology (2008). When are achievement goal orientations ben-
Review, 13, 73–92. eficial for academic achievement? A closer look at
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and moderating factors. International Review of Social
avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: Psychology, 21, 19–70.
A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Maehr, M. L. (1984). Meaning and motivation: Toward a
Social Psychology, 70, 461–475. theory of personal investment. In R. Ames & C. Ames
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 x 2 achieve- (Eds.), Research on motivation in education: Student
ment goal framework. Journal of Personality and motivation (Vol. 1, pp. 115–143). New York:
Social Psychology, 80, 501–519. Academic.
Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Gable, S. (1999). Maehr, M. L., & Anderman, E. M. (1993). Reinventing
Achievement goals, study strategies, and exam perfor- schools for early adolescents: Emphasizing task goals.
mance: A mediational analysis. Journal of Educational The Elementary School Journal, 93, 593–610.
Psychology, 91, 549–563. Maehr, M. L., & Zusho, A. (2009). Achievement goal
Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach theory: The past, present, and future. In K. R. Wentzel
to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at
and Social Psychology, 54, 5–12. school (pp. 77–104). New York: Routledge.
Epstein, J. L. (1983). Longitudinal effects of family-
Meece, J., Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2006).
school-person interactions on student outcomes. In
Classroom goal structure, student motivation, and aca-
A. C. Kerckhoff (Ed.), Research in sociology of educa-
demic achievement. In S. T. Fiske, A. E. Kazdin, & D.
tion and socialization: Vol. 4. Personal change over the
L. Schacter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology: Vol.
life course (pp. 101–127). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
57. Stanford, CA: Annual Reviews.
Fast, L. A., Lewis, J. L., Bryant, M., Bocian, K. A.,
Meece, J. L. (1991). The classroom context and students’
Cardullo, R. A., Rettig, M., et al. (2010). Does math
motivational goals. In M. L. Maehr & P. Pintrich
self-efficacy mediate the effect of the perceived class-
(Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Vol.
room environment on standardized math test perfor-
7: Goals and self-regulatory processes (pp. 261–286).
mance? Journal of Educational Psychology, 102,
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
729–740.
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). Middleton, M. J., Kaplan, A., & Midgley, C. (2004). The
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of change in middle school students’ achievement goals
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, in mathematics over time. Social Psychology of
59–109. Education, 7, 289–311.
Graham, S., & Golan, S. (1991). Motivational influences Middleton, M. J., & Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the
on cognition: Task involvement, ego involvement, and demonstration of lack of ability: An underexplored
depth of information processing. Journal of aspect of goal theory. Journal of Educational
Educational Psychology, 83, 187–194. Psychology, 89, 710–718.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Carter, S. M., Lehto, Midgley, C. (1993). Motivation and middle level schools.
A. T., & Elliot, A. J. (1997). Predictors and conse- In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in
quences of achievement goals in the college class- motivation and achievement: Vol. 8. Motivation and
room: Maintaining interest and making the grade. adolescent development (pp. 217–274). Greenwich,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, CT: JAI Press.
1284–1295. Midgley, C. (Ed.). (2002). Goals, goal structures, and pat-
Hattie, J., & Anderman, E. M. (2011). International hand- terns of adaptive learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
book of student achievement. New York: Routledge. Erlbaum Associates.
Hulleman, C. S., Schrager, S. M., Bodmann, S. M., & Midgley, C., Anderman, E. M., & Hicks, L. H. (1995).
Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). A meta-analytic review of Differences between elementary and middle school
achievement goal measures: Different labels for the teachers and students: A goal theory approach. Journal
same constructs or different constructs with similar of Early Adolescence, 15, 90–113.
labels? Psychological Bulletin, 136, 422–449. Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. J. (2001).
Kaplan, A., Gheen, M., & Midgley, C. (2002). The classroom Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for
goal structure and student disruptive behavior. British whom, under what circumstances, and at what cost?
Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 191–211. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77–86.
190 E.M. Anderman and H. Patrick

Midgley, C., Maehr, M. L., Hicks, L., Roeser, R., Urdan, Nussbaum, A. D., & Dweck, C. S. (2008). Defensiveness
T., Anderman, E. M., et al. (1996). The Patterns of versus remediation: Self-theories and modes of self-
Adaptive Learning Survey (PALS). Ann Arbor, MI: esteem maintenance. Personality and Social
University of Michigan. Psychology Bulletin, 34, 599–612.
Midgley, C., Maehr, M. L., Hruda, L. A., Anderman, E., Patrick, H., Anderman, L. H., Ryan, A. M., Edelin, K., &
Anderman, L., Gheen, M., et al. (2000). Manual for Midgley, C. (2001). Teachers’ communication of goal
the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scale. Ann Arbor, orientations in four fifth-grade classrooms. The
MI: University of Michigan. Elementary School Journal, 102, 35–58.
Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (2001). Academic self-handi- Patrick, H., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). What do students think
capping and achievement goals: A further examina- about when evaluating their classroom’s mastery goal
tion. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, structure? An examination of young adolescents’
61–75. explanations. The Journal of Experimental Education,
Miller, A. D., & Murdock, T. B. (2007). Modeling latent 77, 99–123.
true scores to determine the utility of aggregate stu- Patrick, H., Ryan, A. M., & Kaplan, A. (2007). Early ado-
dent perceptions as classroom indicators in HLM: The lescents’ perceptions of the classroom social environ-
case of classroom goal structures. Contemporary ment, motivational beliefs, and engagement. Journal
Educational Psychology, 32, 83–104. of Educational Psychology, 99, 83–98.
Miller, R. B., Greene, B. A., Montalvo, G. P., Ravindran, Patrick, H., Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., & Midgley, C.
B., & Nichols, J. D. (1996). Engagement in academic (2003). How teachers establish psychological environ-
work: The role of learning goals, future consequences, ments during the first days of school: Associations
pleasing others, and perceived ability. Contemporary with avoidance in mathematics. Teachers College
Educational Psychology, 21, 388–422. Record, 105, 1521–1558.
Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2009). The joint influence Pintrich, P. R. (2000a). Multiple goals, multiple pathways:
of personal achievement goals and classroom goal The role of goal orientation in learning and achievement.
structures on achievement-relevant outcomes. Journal Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 544–555.
of Educational Psychology, 101, 432–447. Pintrich, P. R. (2000b). An achievement goal theory per-
Murdock, T. B., Hale, N. M., & Weber, M. J. (2001). spective on issues in motivation terminology, theory,
Predictors of cheating among early adolescents: and research. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
Academic and social motivations. Contemporary 25, 92–104.
Educational Psychology, 26, 96–115. Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and
Murdock, T. B., Miller, A., & Goetzinger, J. (2007). self-regulated learning components of classroom aca-
Effects of classroom context variables on university demic performance. Journal of Educational
students’ judgments about cheating: Mediating and Psychology, 82, 33–40.
moderating processes. Social Psychology of Education, Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996).
106, 141–169. Perceptions of the school psychological environment
Murdock, T. B., Miller, A., & Kohlhardt, J. (2004). Effects and early adolescents’ psychological and behavioral
of classroom context variables on high school stu- functioning in school: The mediating role of goals and
dents’ judgments of the acceptability and likelihood of belonging. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88,
cheating. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 408–422.
765–777. Ryan, A. M., Gheen, M., & Midgley, C. (1998). Why do
Murphy, P. K., & Alexander, P. A. (2000). A motivated some students avoid asking for help? An examination of
exploration of motivation terminology. Contemporary the interplay among students’ academic efficacy, teach-
Educational Psychology, 25, 3–53. er’s social-emotional role and classroom goal structure.
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and demo- Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 528–535.
cratic education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2001). The classroom social
Press. environment and changes in adolescents’ motivation
Nicholls, J. G., Cobb, P., Wood, T., Yackel, E., & and engagement during middle school. American
Patashnick, M. (1990). Assessing students’ theories of Educational Research Journal, 38, 437–460.
success in mathematics: Individual and classroom dif- Ryan, A. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). “Should I ask for
ferences. Journal for Research in Mathematics help?” The role of motivation and attitudes in adoles-
Education, 21, 109–122. cents’ help seeking in math class. Journal of
Nolen, S. B. (1988). Reasons for studying: Motivational Educational Psychology, 89, 329–341.
orientations and study strategies. Cognition and Shernoff, D. J. (2010). Engagement in after-school pro-
Instruction, 5, 269–287. grams as a predictor of social competence and aca-
Nolen, S. B. (2003). Learning environment, motivation, demic performance. American Journal of Community
and achievement in high school science. Journal of Psychology, 45, 325–337.
Research in Science Teaching, 40, 347–368. Sideridis, G. D., & Mouratidis, A. (2008). Forced choice
Nolen, S. B., & Haladyna, T. M. (1990). Motivation and versus open-ended assessments of goal orientations:
studying in high school science. Journal of Research A descriptive study. International Review of Social
in Science Teaching, 27, 115–126. Psychology, 21, 219–248.
8 Achievement Goal Theory, Conceptualization of Ability/Intelligence, and Classroom Climate 191

Skaalvik, E. M. (1997). Self-enhancing and self-defeating Urdan, T. (2004). Predictors of academic self-handicapping
ego orientation: Relations with task and avoidance ori- and achievement: Examining achievement goals, class-
entation, achievement, self-perceptions, and anxiety. room goal structures, and culture. Journal of
Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 71–81. Educational Psychology, 96, 251–264.
Stevens, T., Hamman, D., & Olivarez, A. (2007). Hispanic Urdan, T., & Mestas, M. (2006). The goals behind perfor-
students’ perception of White teachers’ mastery goal mance goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98,
orientation influences sense of school belonging. 354–365.
Journal of Latinos and Education, 6, 55–70. Urdan, T., & Midgley, C. (2003). Changes in the perceived
Turner, J. C., Midgley, C., Meyer, D. K., Gheen, M., classroom goal structure and pattern of adaptive learn-
Anderman, E., Kang, Y., et al. (2002). The classroom ing during early adolescence. Contemporary
environment and students’ reports of avoidance behav- Educational Psychology, 28, 524–551.
iors in mathematics: A multi-method study. Journal of Urdan, T. C., Midgley, C., & Anderman, E. M. (1998).
Educational Psychology, 94, 88–106. The role of classroom goal structure in students’ use of
Turner, J. C., & Patrick, H. (2004). Motivational influ- self-handicapping strategies. American Educational
ences on student participation in classroom learning Research Journal, 35, 101–122.
activities. Teachers College Record, 106, 1759–1785. Wolters, C. A. (2004). Advancing achievement goal
Urdan, T. (1997). Achievement goal theory: Past results, theory: Using goal structures and goal orienta-
future directions. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich tions to predict students’ motivation, cognition, and
(Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96,
10, pp. 99–141). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 236–250.
School Identification
9
Kristin E. Voelkl

Abstract
This chapter provides a framework for understanding the integral role of
school identification in shaping students’ social and learning behavior. In
the first part of this chapter, the components of identification (belonging
and valuing) are described from a theoretical perspective. Next, the devel-
opment of identification in students is described, and contextual factors
that affect the development of identification are highlighted. These con-
textual factors are: association with similar others, feelings of safety, being
treated fairly, and teacher supportiveness. A model is forwarded that relates
identification to student behavior and learning. Finally, behavioral corre-
lates of school identification that explain the direct and indirect relation-
ships of identification with students’ academic success are presented.
Three assumptions underlie the position taken in this chapter. First, iden-
tification with school is “affective”; that is, it involves emotion more than
cognition, and it is comprised of a particular set of attitudes toward school
and school work. Second, these attitudes shape student behavior and vice
versa. Third, identification with school develops over time so that its pre-
cursors may be seen in the early grades.

It comes as no surprise that positive behavior is may be expressed in many forms such as liking,
associated with positive attitudes. This relation- acceptance, attachment, valuing, and perceived
ship is particularly important in the context of supportiveness. Taken together, these attitudes
school or employment where productive behavior may result in the development of a bond or sense
is a consequence of maintaining positive attitudes of identification with the institution and positive
toward the institution. In school, positive attitudes outcomes are likely to follow. On the other hand,
students who fail to develop a positive emotional
bond with school are likely to disengage, exhibit
dysfunctional behavior, and withdraw from
K.E. Voelkl, Ph.D. (*)
Department of Adolescence Education,
school (Finn, 1989; Hawkins, Catalano, & Miller,
Canisius College, Buffalo, NY 14208, USA 1992; Maddox & Prinz, 2003; Rumberger & Lim,
e-mail: finnk@canisius.edu 2008; Voelkl & Frone, 2000, 2004).

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 193


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_9, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
194 K.E. Voelkl

This chapter provides a framework for under- which students value success in school-relevant
standing the integral role of school identification goals. According to the model, dropping out of
in shaping students’ social and learning behavior. school is a developmental process that ensues
In the first part of this chapter, the components of when students fail to participate in school or class-
identification are described from a theoretical room activities and fail to identify with school.
perspective. Next, the development of identifica- More contemporary views of engagement
tion in students is described, and contextual fac- have broadened the model to include additional
tors that affect the development of identification dimensions and terms, for example, academic
are highlighted. A model is forwarded that relates engagement (Appleton, Christenson, Kim, &
identification to student behavior and learning. Reschly, 2006; Johnson, Crosnoe, & Elder, 2001),
Finally, empirical data that support the model are social or conduct engagement (Hughes, Luo,
summarized: behavioral correlates of school Kwok, & Loyd, 2008; Pannozzo, Finn, & Boyd-
identification that explain the direct and indirect Zaharias, 2004), cognitive engagement (Appleton
relationships of identification with students’ aca- et al., 2006; Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris,
demic success are presented. 2004; Greene, Miller, Crowson, Duke, & Akey,
Three assumptions underlie the position taken 2004), affective engagement (Jimerson, Campos,
in this chapter. First, identification with school is & Greif, 2003), psychological engagement
“affective”; that is, it involves emotion more than (Appleton et al., 2006; Christenson et al., 2008;
cognition, and is comprised of a particular set of Rumberger & Lim, 2008), and emotional engage-
attitudes toward school and school work. Second, ment (Connell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Fredricks
these attitudes, like attitudes generally, help shape et al., 2004; Ladd & Dinella, 2009). The first
student behavior and vice versa. Third, identifica- three terms correspond to the behavioral compo-
tion with school develops over time so that only nent in the participation-identification model,
its precursors may be seen in the early grades. that is, behaviors related directly to the learning
Identification is not internalized in early grades, process and to classroom behavior, and cognitive
but becomes established over time under appro- efforts beyond a minimal investment in learning.
priate conditions. Empirical evidence for the sec- The remaining terms describe affect, that is,
ond and third assumptions is summarized in the attitudes and emotions associated with school and
sections that follow. school work. Educators agree that affective
engagement in school is important, but research
has not clarified its exact role in the learning pro-
Identification as a Form cess. This chapter focuses on affective engage-
of Engagement ment, showing how affect develops over time as a
result of many interactions and experiences includ-
Nearly two decades ago, Finn (1989) proposed ing academic performance. Further, affect predicts
one of the earliest models of student engagement. academic achievement because of its impact on
The participation-identification model was an school and classroom behavior (i.e., behavioral
attempt to explain how the interplay of school engagement) which, in turn, affects learning.
attitudes and behaviors affects the likelihood of In this chapter, identification is viewed as an
academic success. In this two-component model, intrinsic form of achievement motivation that
participation referred to behaviors that engage encourages students to engage in appropriate
students in learning activities and keep students learning behaviors. Achievement motivation is a
on-task. Identification referred to students’ atti- “general desire or disposition to succeed in aca-
tudes about school, in particular, feelings of demic work and in the more specific tasks of
belongingness and valuing. Belongingness was school” (Newmann, Wehlage, & Lamborn, 1992,
students’ sense of being a part of the school envi- p. 13). Motivated students exert effort and persist
ronment and that school is an important part of on academic tasks. Affectively, they enjoy and are
their own experience. Valuing was the extent to eager to approach learning tasks, are optimistic
9 School Identification 195

about the chances of success, and take pleasure in the terms as suggested by the actual measures
their academic work. Beyond the extrinsic rein- used in research studies. This component rests on
forcements provided by teachers and parents, stu- classic psychological theory asserting that indi-
dents are motivated by internal factors, in viduals have a fundamental need to belong to
particular, individual needs, values, and goals groups and institutions (Baumeister & Leary,
(for a comprehensive review, see Stipek, 2004). 1995; Maslow, 1968). Outside the home, school
Internalized achievement values arise from pre- and the work place are the most salient institu-
cepts conveyed by parents and teachers that tions for most youth.
achievement is valued. Over time, most students Humans also have a need to feel that their
internalize these values and make them their own. actions are worthwhile, that is, of value. This
Identification with school is regarded as intrinsic assumption too is based in classical psychologi-
motivation, that is, an internal desire to achieve, cal theories asserting that individuals have a need
develop competencies, and take pleasure in aca- for feelings of competence (Bandura, 1977;
demic success. When internal motivation is weak, Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Deci & Ryan, 2000)
students are less likely to engage in learning and and self-esteem based on competence (Maslow,
have successful school experiences. 1968). Both of these needs rest on the assumption
that the arena in which a person is competent is
important—of value—to the individual or to
The Components of Identification other people. Valuing can be experienced as a
personal sense of fulfillment (“It gives me plea-
The framework for studying identification as an sure” or “I get praise for doing this”) or in practi-
affective form of student engagement is rooted in cal terms as a means to an end, that is, goal
psychological theories of human needs (Maslow, attainment. The reason behind the value, how-
1968) and in theory that explains individuals’ ever, is less important for identification than the
need to experience a sense of community value attribution itself. A person may pursue an
(McMillan, 1996; McMillan & Chavis, 1986). activity because of its perceived importance or
Sense of community is a “feeling that members rationalize that an activity at which she/he is
have of belonging, a feeling that members matter competent is of value, but in either case, it is
to one another and to the group, and a shared accompanied by a sense of fulfillment or being
faith that members’ needs will be met through worthwhile.
their commitment to be together” (McMillan &
Chavis, 1986, p. 9). Individuals also share a need
to feel their actions are worthwhile and to have a The Need to Belong
sense of competence and positive self-regard.
Both needs are reflected in the components of Belongingness has been defined as “feelings that
identification in Finn’s (1989) model, that is, one is a significant member of the school com-
belonging and valuing. Both components derive munity, is accepted and respected in school, has a
from basic human needs, and both can motivate sense of inclusion in school, and includes school
productive learning behavior. The lion’s share of as part of one’s self-definition” (Voelkl, 1996,
research to date has focused on sense of belong- p. 762). The bidirectional nature of belonging-
ing and closely allied concepts including psycho- ness is described by Whitlock (2006) as “[belong-
logical investment (Newmann et al., 1992), ingness] is conceptualized as something not
relatedness (Connell & Wellborn, 1991), school merely received (e.g., ‘To what extent do you feel
membership (Goodenow, 1993), school connect- cared for?’) but reciprocated as well (e.g., ‘To
edness (Libbey, 2004; Whitlock, 2006), and what extent do you care about your school?’)”
school attachment (Mouton & Hawkins, 1996), (p. 15). Several attempts have been made to
among other terms. Jimerson et al. (2003) compare the terms that have been used in place
discussed similarities and differences among of or in addition to belongingness (Jimerson
196 K.E. Voelkl

et al., 2003; Libbey, 2004; O’Farrell & Morrison, associated with a broad range of psychological,
2003). By and large, these analyses conclude that behavioral, and health problems (Deci, Vallerand,
behind the multiple definitions, there are multiple Pelletier, & Ryan, 1991; Newmann, 1981; Ryan,
similar constructs, each arising from a particular 1995).
measurement instrument. There is little point in These ideas have been used to explain motiva-
reiterating these analyses here; they are complex tion and behavior in the work place and in school.
and tend to change as new terms enter the field. For several decades, management researchers
Instead, this chapter discusses only studies that have studied job involvement of employees, that
match the definitions of belonging and/or valuing is, “the degree to which a person is identified psy-
as used here. It was discovered, however, that most chologically with his work” (Rabinowitz & Hall,
measures of belongingness yield similar correla- 1977, p. 266). Identification was indicated by the
tions with other educational variables (Goodenow, extent to which success or failure on the job
1993; McNeely, Nonnemaker, & Blum, 2002; affects an individual’s self-esteem. Indeed, suc-
Rumberger & Lim, 2008; Voelkl, 1997). cesses and failures can affect a fundamental trait
The importance of a sense of belonging can be like self-esteem only in individuals who feel that
traced back at least to the classic work of Maslow the work place is an important part of their own
(1968) who proposed a hierarchy of innate human self-definition (i.e., belongingness). The phrases
needs: physiological (e.g., food, shelter), safety “work engagement” and “job embeddedness”
(e.g., security, peace), love (e.g., relationships, have also been used in place of job involvement,
bonds with others), esteem (e.g., efficacy, mas- although some researchers have explained that
tery), knowledge (e.g., understanding), esthetic there are subtle differences among the terms (e.g.,
(e.g., order, beauty), and self-actualization (e.g., Halbesleben & Wheeler, 2008; Kanungo, 1982;
avocation). The first four levels were classified as Saleh & Hosek, 1976; Simpson, 2009).
“deficiency needs,” deemed essential for physi- Despite the use of different terms, empirical
cal and psychological well-being. research in the workplace has supported two
Maslow’s assertion about the importance of common principles. First, sense of belonging is
nutrition, safety, and emotional bonds has impli- impacted by structural and interpersonal features
cations for student success. Recognizing that stu- of the work place such as management style,
dents who are hungry tend to perform poorly, the workplace safety, and autonomy (Harter, Schmidt,
US Department of Agriculture provides lunch & Keyes, 2003; Kahn, 1990; Lawler & Hall,
subsidies for student from low-income homes 1970). Second, sense of belonging is associated
(Institute of Medicine, 2010). Similarly, federal with employee job performance, satisfaction, and
initiatives such as the Safe and Drug-Free Schools intention to stay or leave (Kanungo, 1979;
Act of 1990, the Gun-Free School Zones Act of Simpson, 2009).
1990, and the widespread implementation of These conclusions apply to students and
zero-tolerance policies demonstrate the recent schools as well, where sense of belonging has
emphasis placed on the health and safety of stu- been viewed in terms of “school community.”
dents in public schools (Cornell & Mayer, 2010). A community is both a territorial or geographic
As with food and safety, the need to feel that unit (a “place”) and a set of human relationships
one is part of a group or institution also shapes (McMillan & Chavis, 1986; Osterman, 2000).
behavior. In their extensive review of belonging- According to McMillan and Chavis, community
ness, Baumeister and Leary (1995) summarized membership serves four major purposes for the
evidence that humans are naturally driven toward individual, “shared emotional connection,”
establishing and sustaining bonds with others. To “influence,” “integration and fulfillment of
satisfy this drive, there is a need for frequent, pos- needs,” and “membership,” the feeling of belong-
itive personal interactions in the context of long- ing. “[I]n a community, the members feel that the
term, caring relationships. They also provide group is important to them and that they are
evidence that the deprivation of belongingness is important to the group” (Osterman, 2000, p. 324).
9 School Identification 197

Likewise, this two-part description is the basis of The connections between students’ sense of
Voelkl’s (1996) definition of belongingness. community and behavioral engagement have
Outside the home, youth spend large amounts been confirmed in a number of empirical studies
of time at school and in classes—from an early (Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Royal & Rossi, 1996)
age onward. They establish relationships with and are reviewed in the final section of this chap-
fellow students and teachers and, for those who ter. Education researchers have also proposed
succeed, experience the achievements and that a sense of membership in home and school
rewards that ensue. These experiences promote a settings serves a protective function that offsets
sense of connectedness or belonging with the the negative effects of social handicaps (e.g.,
institution itself, that is, “the place” (McMillan & poverty or a language other than English being
Chavis, 1986; Sarason, Pierce, & Sarason, 1990). spoken at home) (Connell et al., 1994; Finn &
School is where students come to be with their Rock, 1997; Maddox & Prinz, 2003; Marcus &
friends, to participate in organized academic and Sanders-Reio, 2001; Resnick, Harris, & Shew,
social group activities, and receive encourage- 1997). Using home interview data from the
ments or discouragements for their successes and National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent
failures. Building on McMillan and Chavis’s Health, Resnick et al. (1997) found that parent-
concept of four functions of communities, family connectedness and school connectedness
researchers have proposed that sense of belonging reduced the likelihood of a host of health risk fac-
is enhanced in schools where students are active tors among 7th through 12th graders including
and frequent participants in the learning process, emotional distress and suicidality, drug and alco-
where students develop feelings of academic and hol use, sexual activity, and violence. School
social competence, and where students’ needs for connectedness was associated (negatively) with
autonomy, for engaging in challenging activities, adolescent emotional distress and suicidality.
and for a social comfort zone are met (Bateman, Connell and colleagues forwarded a model of
2002). contextual and personal factors, including attach-
The need for belongingness, then, can be ful- ment to peers in school and emotional engage-
filled by the school community. In turn, through its ment in school on outcomes including attendance,
impact on motivation and behavior, students’ feel- grades, and disciplinary measures. Three studies
ings of belonging can facilitate academic persis- of 10–16-year-old African-American adolescents
tence and performance. According to classic were conducted to test these models. Although
sociological theory, the school serves a normative specific relationships differed among the studies,
function, encouraging and reinforcing behavior they all showed that combinations of personal
like that of others in the same setting (Elliott & connectedness and emotional engagement were
Voss, 1974; Hirschi, 2005; Polk & Halferty, 1972; associated with positive education outcomes
Seeman, 1975). Social control theory proposes that despite that many of the participants were from
bonds to institutions are accompanied by sensitivity low-income homes.
to the opinions and behaviors of others and a When the need for belonging is not satisfied,
tendency to emulate those opinions and behaviors. diminished motivation, impaired development,
When the behaviors of others are positive and goal- and alienation may follow (Connell & Wellborn,
oriented, belongingness provides incentive for stu- 1991; Furrer & Skinner, 2003; see Juvonen,
dents to work hard for the same goals, that is, 2006). Sense of belonging may fail to develop as
grades and continuing progress. When the bond a student matures or be attenuated by experiences
fails to develop or is broken, individuals may reject encountered in school, for example, unfair or
the legitimacy of the institution and perceive it as disproportionate discipline or close association
unfair and alienating. In an often-cited study of with peers who decide to leave school. The edu-
these principles, Hirschi documented a causal chain cational harm that students can suffer in these
of events from poor school performance to weak- situations include emotional and behavioral with-
ened bonds with school to juvenile delinquency. drawal and dropping out.
198 K.E. Voelkl

The Need for Personal reliably distinguish between the three types of
and Practical Value values, but are able to do so by the fifth grade
(Wigfield & Eccles, 1992). All three, however,
Valuing is feeling that school and school outcomes can influence students’ task choices, persistence,
have personal importance and/or practical impor- and performance. Tests of the model showed that
tance, that is, that they are worthwhile (Anderman students’ perception of the usefulness of a subject
& Wolters, 2006; Eccles et al., 1983; Schiefele, was related to intentions to enroll in future course
1999; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992). Personal impor- work, and that task values predicted career
tance can evolve from an internal sense of fulfill- choices and course plans to enroll in math, phys-
ment (e.g., interest, enjoyment, satisfaction from ics, and English (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Eccles
completing school tasks) or external sources et al., 1983). Among middle school students, peer
(e.g., satisfactory grades, encouragement from group influence has been related to intrinsic but
teachers or parents). Practical importance is the not utility value. The between-group HLM model
recognition that school experiences have utility accounted for 46% of the variance between peer
in attaining future goals (e.g., a high school groups in average intrinsic value. Students with
diploma, a particular job, or access to postsec- peers who disliked school showed decreased
ondary schooling). enjoyment of school (intrinsic value) over the
Theory and empirical research support that school year compared with students who spent
students are most likely to be engaged, to expend time with friends who liked school. However,
more effort in the classroom, and to persist in peer group did not influence student beliefs about
learning tasks when they place high value on the usefulness or importance of school (utility
schoolwork (Eccles, 2008; Pintrich & De Groot, value) in their lives (Ryan, 2001).
1990). To the extent that values have been inter- In an attempt to explain the attitude-achieve-
nalized by a student, they are an intrinsic motiva- ment disparity for African-American students,
tor of behavior and engagement (Deci & Ryan, Mickelson (1990) distinguished between con-
1985; Deci et al., 1991). Indeed, one of the earli- crete and abstract attitudes. Concrete attitudes
est theories of achievement motivation proposed (practical values) represent the perception of
that one’s tendency to approach success (or avoid one’s probable returns on education from the
failure) is partially a function of the internalized opportunity structure in society. Abstract atti-
incentive value of success or failure (Atkinson, tudes represent the dominant ideology of society
1964). More contemporary models show that that education will bring opportunity. She found
achievement-related behaviors are related to the that, for African-American students, abstract atti-
value of a task, which is a function of the per- tudes were unrelated to GPA, but the more
ceived qualities of the task and the person’s needs, students valued schooling as a realistic means
goals, and self-perception (Eccles et al., 1983). toward future success (concrete values), the
Research on values as motivators recognizes higher their performance in school. In addition,
the distinction of personal and practical values of research by Schiefele (1991, 1999) showed that
school. According to the expectancy-value model individual interest or enjoyment of a topic (per-
of achievement forwarded by Eccles et al. (1983), sonal value) was associated with more meaning-
“subjective task value” is based on perceptions of ful processing of text, use of deep-level learning
the task to be performed, namely, its attainment strategies, and perception of skills. His review of
value, intrinsic or interest value, and utility value. evidence found that although interest was only
Attainment value is the personal importance of moderately related to deep-level learning, the
doing well on a task. Interest value is the inher- relations were stronger than the correlation
ent, immediate enjoyment or pleasure derived between interest and surface-level learning
from engaging in the activity, and utility value is (below the .30 level).
the importance of the task for current and future In sum, research and theory support the idea
goals. Students in early elementary grades do not that other forms of engagement, and academic
9 School Identification 199

success, are related to beliefs about school activi- altogether. This student has become disengaged
ties being worthwhile. Valuing has both a per- from school, and his attitudes are likely to become
sonal dimension and a practical dimension; both congruent with his behavior. Experimental
provide intrinsic motivation for student engage- research on the impact of behavior on attitudes
ment. The personal dimension reflects a student’s has shown extensive support for these theories
feelings that schoolwork is rewarding because (Olson & Stone, 2005).
s/he receives pleasure from doing it. For example, Behavior also shapes attitudes through a
a first-grade student values learning to read sequence of events linking the two. A restless stu-
because she finds the activity fun and feels pride dent or one with short attention span may attract
when she demonstrates competence. The practi- the teacher’s attention due to his/her behavior. If
cal dimension reflects the student’s belief that the teacher reacts to the behavior rather than to
schoolwork is associated with the attainment of learning, this may lead to punishment followed
future goals. For example, a high school student by resentment and dislike for school on the stu-
values learning new math concepts because she dent’s part. A classic example of this was described
believes that math skills are important for by Bernstein and Rulo (1976) who explained the
entrance to college. Following their review of possible consequences of undiagnosed learning
research on intrinsic motivation in education, problems. If the student is not following the mate-
Deci et al. (1991) summarized the combined rial being presented, she/he may exhibit inappro-
impact of values on behavior as follows: “For priate behavior. The more attention teachers pay
students to be actively engaged in the educational to the behavior, the further behind the student
endeavor, they must value learning, achievement, becomes academically, bringing with it frustra-
and accomplishment even with respect to topics tion and negative attitudes toward school.
and activities they do not find interesting… When It is also commonly acknowledged that behav-
the value of an activity is internalized, people do ior is guided by attitudes. The “model of reasoned
not necessarily become more interested in the action” (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2005) asserts that
activity…but they do become willing to do it behavior is rational and follows from intentions
because of its personal value” (p. 338). which, in turn, are shaped by attitudes and beliefs.
Salient beliefs and attitudes include the perceived
likely consequences of the behavior, and the per-
Connections Between Attitudes ceived approval or disapproval of the behavior
and Behavior by respected others. Empirical studies support
the connections among the components of this
Social psychologists have long studied the link model (Ajzen & Fishbein).
between attitudes and behaviors and have con- Whether or not the assumption of rationality is
cluded that the relationship is likely to be recipro- correct, the principle of attitudes shaping behav-
cal. The influence of behaviors on attitudes is ior is seen in many arenas. The needs that under-
explained in terms of two prominent theories: lie students’ identification with school in
cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger, 1957) particular—needs for belonging and valuing—
and the closely related self-perception theory are strong motivators of school and classroom
(Bem, 1972). In simple terms, dissonance theory behavior and misbehavior (see, e.g., Eccles &
postulates that people who become aware they Wigfield, 2002; Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Pannozzo
have behaved in a manner that conflicts with their et al., 2004; Royal & Rossi, 1996; Voelkl, 1997).
beliefs tend to form or change their attitudes to be Students who have positive attitudes about school
consistent with behavior. For example, an are more engaged in school, and those who do
engaged student who enters a high school with a not like school are more likely to be disengaged
high dropout rate may be influenced by peers or withdraw (Connell & Wellborn, 1991;
to skip school and eventually stop attending Fredricks et al., 2004).
200 K.E. Voelkl

responding to questions and even completing


The Development of School assignments (academic engagement) have some
Identification level of discretion to them. Also, students learn to
cope with having to wait their turn, working well
This section discusses the role of identification as
with others, and the teacher-student power struc-
a mediator of student behavior. Figure 9.1 is a
ture (social engagement). All of these behaviors
pictorial representation of theory and research on
are reinforced by extrinsic motivators including
the development of identification and the ways in
teacher praise and encouragement, gold stars,
which it becomes associated with academic
awards, candy, and stickers. It should be noted,
achievement. According to this view, students do
however, that the use of rewards for motivating
not begin schooling with established feelings of
learning is controversial (e.g., Cameron & Pierce,
school identification. Instead, identification is
1994). Early behaviors are accompanied by basic
portrayed as having its roots in relatively simple
emotional reactions such as liking the teacher,
attitudes developed in the early grades. Over
having fun with peers, feelings of safety, and
time, early attitudes become crystallized, and the
having pride in a picture drawn or work sheet
need for external motivators is replaced increas-
completed.
ingly by the student’s own intrinsic motivation.
As students progress through the grades, they
According to Ryan (1995), through a process of
exhibit new forms of academic and social engage-
internalization, behaviors that were motivated by
ment. They take increased initiative and persist in
external requirements become matters of per-
completing their school work and establish rela-
sonal choice instead.
tionships with teachers and friendships with
In the early years of school, some behaviors
peers. Peer relationships contribute increasingly
are required and others are encouraged. Parents
to the sense of belonging. As behaviors become
take students to school, and teachers require them
habits and habits continue to be reinforced from
to sit in their seats and follow directions, but

Contextual Facilitating
Conditions
• Similar Others
• Safety
• Fairness
• Supportive Classroom
Environment

Extrinsic
Motivators

Early Forms Intrinsic Motivators


of Behavioral Identification with Behavioral Achievement
Engagement school: Engagement and Attainment
• Belonging
• Valuing

Early
Forms of
Affect

Fig. 9.1 School identification model: development and consequences


9 School Identification 201

teachers and parents or by a personal sense of Third, identification with school is ultimately
accomplishment, students increase their sense of a set of affective responses or attitudes likely to
belongingness and the value they attribute to have greater impact on other attitudes or on in-
school and academic performance. For students school and out-of-school behaviors than directly
who establish patterns of consistent classroom on academic achievement. To the extent that atti-
engagement, external motivators are gradually tudes impact learning behavior, the development
replaced by well-learned behaviors and internal of school identification can facilitate academic
motivation. success. On the other hand, the failure to identify
Over time and under appropriate conditions, with school can create insurmountable obstacles
identification with school crystallizes and pro- to high performance.
vides internal motivation for continued academic, Other developmental models that include
social, and cognitive engagement. Because of the identification with school or its correlates have
academic outcomes that follow, the behavior is been proposed. These include a social develop-
reinforced by grades, praise from parents and ment model used to predict adverse outcomes
teachers, recognition from classmates, and also (e.g., antisocial behavior, substance use, delin-
by personal pride and sense of ownership of the quency) from individuals’ social bonds with other
skills acquired. Students form deeper emotional individuals (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996), and a
bonds with school if they feel accepted by peers, general model of interpersonal, intrapsychic, and
respected and supported by teachers, and perceive behavioral influences on educational outcomes
that their accomplishments are recognized. That (Connell et al., 1994). The reciprocal nature of
is, continued positive behavior helps solidify stu- identification and school outcomes was given
dents’ identification with school (the reverse more attention in a longitudinal study of students
arrow in Fig. 9.1). as they progressed from seventh to ninth grade
The model portrayed in Fig. 9.1 carries with it (Kaplan, Peck & Kaplan, 1995). Beginning with
three assumptions about student development. a large sample of seventh graders attending junior
First, identification (or disidentification) with high schools in a Houston school district, the
school develops over a period of time as the result authors found that negative academic outcomes
of numerous interactions, achievements, and (grades over the previous 7 years) tended to lead
other related experiences. The precursors of iden- to perceived rejection by teachers followed by
tification (or disidentification) can be seen in ear- association with negative peers who, in turn, con-
lier grades. In later grades, when motivation tributed to further negative academic experiences
derives more from internal sources, identification (grades in junior high school). The study did not
with school has a continuing impact on student identify observable processes that could be
behavior. That is, students do not begin schooling altered by school practices to improve students’
with a well-developed sense of identification, but academic prognoses.
early behaviors lead to early affect which, in turn,
leads to continued or modified behavior rein-
forced by more well-developed identification Contextual Factors That Facilitate
with school. Identification
Second, the development of sense of identifi-
cation is mediated by contextual factors (“appro- Children spend large amounts of time in school
priate conditions”), namely, similarity to others where contextual factors play an important role in
in a common setting, perceptions of being safe in shaping student motivation. Interpersonal relation-
school, fair distribution of discipline and recogni- ships in the classroom, among peers and between
tion for accomplishments, and caring teachers students and teachers, are important elements that
who provide academic and personal support. All help individuals meet their basic needs of belong-
of these can be altered, if necessary, to improve ing and valuing. Research has identified four con-
school outcomes. textual conditions that affect the likelihood a
202 K.E. Voelkl

student will identify with school: association with The same conclusion would be drawn from a
similar others, feelings of safety, being treated school-as-community perspective. Many schools
fairly, and being supported by teachers. To the and classes serve as cohesive groups, that is,
extent that each condition is absent, the likelihood groups in which members are tied together by
that identification will develop is reduced, along many shared characteristics—including shared
with the probability that students will remain values—and have an affinity for one another as
behaviorally engaged. These conditions can be well as for the group as a whole (Homans, 1974;
altered by changing classroom and school prac- McMillan & Chavis, 1986). Cohesive groups
tices. But much of the responsibility lies in the tend to exert pressure for individuals to conform
hands of teachers who are in a unique position to to group expectations, creating even more simi-
impact feelings of safety, fairness, and student larity among participants. This has been demon-
support. strated over several decades with activities
ranging from forming opinions to interpreting
ambiguous stimuli to completing questionnaires
Similar Others (Hogg, 1992; Shaw, 1976). From either view,
similarity among students in a class or school
It has long been understood that individuals tend tends to “draw students in” and foster their iden-
to form relationships with those similar to them- tification with the institution and its activities
selves, whether similarity is based on physical (Bateman, 2002; Royal & Rossi, 1996).
characteristics (e.g., age, weight, racial-ethnic The classroom would appear to be an intense
background), social characteristics (e.g., religious environment for fostering student identification
origins, attitudes and interests, sexual orienta- with school. Yet despite the structural similarity
tion), or common characteristics of the setting, of the classroom and social pressure toward simi-
for example, a common power structure or shared larity, many students do not become strongly
goals and activities (Byrne, 1997; Pearson, identified with school. Some of this may be attrib-
Muller, & Wilkinson, 2007; Schug, Yuki, utable to school practices that create conspicuous
Horikawa, & Takemura, 2009; Ueno, 2009). dissimilarities among students. Retaining a stu-
When youngsters are free to choose among dent in grade who is then older than most of his/
peers, research has shown that two mechanisms her classmates may cause emotional distress and
are at work: selection of those similar to oneself behavioral or emotional withdrawal (Resnick
and the homogenizing influence of those who are et al., 1997). Discipline practices that remove
already in one’s proximity. However, the class- individual students from the class group are also
room lacks the element of personal choice. De likely to interfere with students’ sense of identifi-
facto, it is populated by students who share many cation. On the other hand, looping, or keeping,
characteristics and who are also subject to com- the same class together for several years can serve
mon underlying dynamics, which can be charac- increase students’ identification with school.
terized as “crowds, praise, and power” (Jackson,
1990). Students learn together the implications of
being one of many, needing to share space and Feeling Safe
time, needing to wait for other students to finish
their work, take their turns, and give their answers. When students do not feel physically safe, feel-
Most class activities are based on a system of eval- ings of belongingness are less apt to develop,
uations and rewards for the products students pro- while feelings of being safe facilitate the likeli-
duce; in general, some will be praised highly and hood of identifying with school. This connection
others less so, but for responses to the same learning has been documented empirically. A mixed-
tasks. Finally, the classroom is controlled by one methods study of 350 eighth-, tenth-, and 12th-
person, and all students are required to behave in grade students in the northeastern United States
accordance with that person’s authority. explained school connectedness in terms of a
9 School Identification 203

number of structural and process variables includ- Given the salience of unsafe environments
ing perceived safety (Whitlock, 2006). The cor- to students, it is no surprise that the findings of
relation of the two scales for the full sample was studies of safety and identification, as well as
.29 (significant at the .01 level), and in the regres- other forms of engagement, are consistently posi-
sion, the contribution of safety was significant at tive (Bateman, 2002; Ripski & Gregory, 2009).
the .05 level independently of a host of other Any safety-related issue that causes a student
variables included in the analysis. to be wary and hesitant when going to school
Some fairly common circumstances raise con- is likely to reduce the strength of connection
cerns about safety, namely, teachers’ lack of con- between students and the institution if not
trol over students’ behavior, the presence of gangs between students and their teachers and peers.
or gang measures in the school, and witnessing or Eccles et al. (1993) proposed that adolescents, in
being the victim of bullying. Research has con- particular, need to feel safe and have a “zone of
nected bullying to identification with school. For comfort” as they transition to from elementary to
example, a group of 517 students in sixth through middle or junior high school.
eighth grade were administered a questionnaire In the classroom, safety may be construed in
that included a school attachment scale, and another way, namely, safety from ridicule and
scales that assessed the student’s attitudes toward public criticism. Studies of the perceived sup-
bullying, whether friends engaged in acts of bul- portiveness of teachers sometimes allude to “feel-
lying, and the students own history of bullying ing welcome and safe in the classroom,” but few,
others (Cunningham, 2007). The correlations if any, studies have examined the relationship of
between attachment and the three bullying scales this form of safety with identification with school
ranged from .25 to .41 and were statistically sig- directly.
nificant at the .01 level. Based on the bullying
scales and additional information, the students
were classified as “bully,” “victim,” or “neither” Fair Treatment
(a comparison group). The highest mean on
school attachment was obtained by the compari- Fair treatment is essential to a student’s develop-
son group, and the lowest mean attachment was ing strong identification with school (Newmann
obtained by the victims; victims had significantly et al., 1992), but inequities can occur in several
lower attachment to school than did bullies or the forms. Schools’ discipline practices may be
comparison students. unclear, disproportionate to the infraction, or
A British study of 364 students in years four administered unevenly across student groups. Or
through six of primary school provided self- students may perceive that teachers are biased
reports of being bullied, of their perceived rela- against them based on personal characteristics
tionships with the teacher, and of their perceived such as race, ethnicity, gender, appearance, or
safety in the classroom and on the playground ability. Both of these create barriers to the devel-
(Boulton et al., 2009). All participants completed opment of a sense of belongingness.
the questionnaires in small groups with a Research reports have documented students’
researcher present. The teacher relationship scale perceptions of negative treatment by teachers and
included several school bonding questions, for other school staff based on race (e.g., Irvine,
example, “I can talk to my teacher about any- 1986; Kailin, 1999; Thompson, 2002). For exam-
thing” and “My teacher makes sure I am OK.” ple, Leitman, Binns, and Unni (1995) found that
The main analysis focused on predicting per- 64% of nonminority students reported encour-
ceived safety, but the correlations reported agement by teachers or counselors to take high
showed that being bullied was significantly nega- school mathematics and science, compared to
tively correlated with perceived safety in the 49% of African-American students. Few, if any,
classroom and playground and also with the qual- studies have documented whether teachers’ actual
ity of the relationship with the teacher. behavior is consistent with the perceptions or the
204 K.E. Voelkl

impact of the perceptions on student attitudes and punishments (e.g., out-of-school suspensions;
behavior. Nevertheless, the perceptions them- zero-tolerance policies) create rifts between stu-
selves may stand in the way of students’ feelings dents and school and cause students to be absent
of belonging in class or in school generally. physically and emotionally. Students who per-
According to Steele (1997), African-American ceive that their everyday behavior can result in
students experience disidentification from school punishment are less likely than their peers to
because they must contend with negative stereo- identify with school. Indeed, one large-scale
types about their academic abilities. “Stereotype study of students in grades 7 through 12, using a
threat” arises for African-American students self-report measure of school belonging, docu-
when they are placed in a predicament (e.g., test mented that “connectedness is lower in schools
taking) where they may be treated stereotypically that expel a student temporarily or permanently
or face the prospect of conforming to the nega- for infractions more serious than cheating or
tive stereotype (i.e., intellectual inferiority). This smoking” (McNeely et al., 2002, p. 140). This is
threat pressures students to disidentify from not to say that harsh punishments are not needed,
school so as to remove this domain from their but the circumstances under which they are used
self-identity and to avoid the risk of confirming should be reasonable, stated clearly, and adminis-
the negative stereotype. tered equitably across student groups. Care-based
Discipline policies may be unclear to both disciplinary practices may be more effective in
teachers and students. In a survey of K-12 teach- maintaining school connectedness than are the
ers commissioned by the American Federation of traditional punishment-based practices (Cassidy,
Teachers, 11% of teachers reported that their 2005; McCloud, 2005).
schools did not have a clearly stated discipline If rules and consequences are not stated clearly
policy, and an additional 50% reported that the or not disseminated, then teachers “or adminis-
policy in effect was not enforced consistently trators” disciplinary actions can be or appear
(American Federation of Teachers, 2008). inequitable. “[S]tudents may experience school
Likewise, a survey of junior high and high school staff as lacking in consistency or impartiality”
teachers found that 27% did not think their (Ripski & Gregory, 2009, p. 369). With these
school’s drug policy was clear to staff, and 25% negative perceptions, students are less likely to
did not think it was enforced fairly (Voelkl & form bonds with teachers that could dampen their
Willert, 2006). In terms of students’ views, 31% sense of identification with school in general
of a national sample of eighth graders reported (Pianta, 1999). Using data on sixth- and eighth-
that their school’s discipline was unfair grade students in a province-wide survey in New
(Rumberger, 1995). A recent survey of school Brunswick, Ma (2003) used multilevel modeling
crime and safety reported that 17% of students to predict eighth-grade students’ sense of belong-
aged 12–18 felt that school punishment for rule ing from student and school characteristics
breaking was inconsistent (U.S. Department of including students’ perceptions of the disciplin-
Justice, 2007). And several studies have docu- ary climate of the school (e.g., rules are clear,
mented that many students—both minorities and consistent, and fair). The analysis showed a par-
whites—perceive that harsher discipline mea- ticularly large impact of school climate on sense
sures are administered to minority than to white of belonging among schools, with an effect size
students (Skiba, Peterson, & Williams, 1997; of 5.70 with all other student and school variables
Wayman, 2002; Wehlage & Rutter, 1986). In one included in the analysis. Although perceptions of
national study of students in grades six through the disciplinary climate were collected from each
12, 9% of white students regarded school rules as student, the analysis included the mean climate
unfair compared to 18% of black students (U.S. rating for each school. Thus, this effect describes
Department of Justice). differences among schools. The results for vari-
The discipline practices of school hold a lot of ability among students’ perceptions would have
potential for alienating students. Unduly harsh differed from this.
9 School Identification 205

A Supportive Class Environment and impersonality presented by middle or junior


high school (Eccles et al., 1993). In later grades,
Students need to be in caring, supportive class they can encourage students to persist when
environments to develop and maintain a sense of faced with difficult tasks, serving a protective
identification with school. This principle has been function against failure (Furrer & Skinner, 2003;
echoed many times over by practitioners and Hudley & Daoud, 2007; Newmann et al., 1992;
researchers alike (Pianta, 1999). Positive rela- Ryan & Patrick, 2001). Supportive teachers also
tionships with teachers and peers are necessary to encourage students to engage in prosocial behav-
create a positive environment, but teachers are ior with other students, which is likely to benefit
primary in establishing relationships with stu- all students socially and academically (Wentzel,
dents, setting the tone in the classroom, providing 1997).
personal and academic support, and encouraging Throughout, teachers’ expressions of support
positive student-student relationships. From a are likely to be interpreted as a sign of caring.
student’s perspective, teachers serve as authority A caring, supportive teacher can impact students’
figures to be respected, provide a feeling of being identification with school. In a study of 300
a worthwhile and welcome member of the school eighth-grade students, Roeser, Midgley, and
community, and give reinforcement for personal Urdan (1996) assessed aspects of the school con-
and academic accomplishments. text including close teacher-student relationships,
The importance of teachers and peers for iden- feelings of belonging, affective outcomes of
tification has been confirmed by countless empir- schooling, and academic achievement. The sta-
ical studies. And many of the same teacher tistical association of student-teacher relation-
qualities that impact identification also affect ships with belonging was robust: the simple
other school-related attitudes, student behavior, correlation between the two was .35, which
and academic achievement. These are discussed remained significant when prior achievement and
in two broad groupings: teacher qualities that demographic variables were controlled statisti-
shape their direct relationships with students and cally. Belonging, in turn, was related significantly
behaviors that impact the classroom community to all other affective outcomes and achievement
which, in turn, affect individual students. with correlations ranging from .17 to .52.
Several programs have been designed to
Teachers’ Relationships with Students improve student-teacher relationships including
Teachers provide encouragement to students in First Things First, a school reform program
three important ways: by showing concern for intended to improve relationships and improve
students’ welfare and supporting their school instruction by reallocating school resources to
efforts, by articulating clear norms and expecta- achieve these ends (Institute for Research and
tions for students, and by encouraging student Improvement in Education, 2002). An evaluation
autonomy. In early grades, caring teachers come of First Things First in elementary and middle
to know each student personally and distribute schools was conducted using a comprehensive
praise and rewards to all students. These teachers measure of teacher caring and support and a com-
often provide a reason for a child to want to go to posite measure of behavioral engagement and
school and to try hard to do assigned work. identification with school (Klem & Connell,
Teachers’ encouragement contributes to students’ 2004). Students who received optimal levels of
school identification above and beyond support teacher support were more likely to be engaged
and encouragement from home (Battistich, than were students receiving low levels of teacher
Solomon, Kim, Watson, & Schaps, 1995; Brewster support. In elementary grades, students with opti-
& Bowen, 2004; Hughes & Kwok, 2007). mal support were 89% more likely to be engaged
In middle grades, supportive teachers may than were students with low levels of support;
help young adolescents over the hurdles of striving middle school students with optimal support were
for independence despite the increased structure almost three times more likely to be engaged, and
206 K.E. Voelkl

those with low levels of support were 68% more Perry, Turner, & Meyer, 2006; Ryan & Patrick,
likely to be disengaged. The authors concluded 2001; Skinner & Belmont, 1993; Wang &
that high levels of teacher support are a resource Holcombe, 2010). In the Skinner-Belmont (1993)
that students may or may not take advantage of, study, support for the autonomy of third- through
while low levels of support are a liability. fifth-grade students was significantly related to
Teachers also support students by setting clear engagement across the school year. In the study of
standards for academic and social behavior and high schoolers, Davidson and Phelan (1999)
holding students to those standards (Yowell, found that students were more engaged in class-
1999). The importance of clear expectations was rooms where they felt they were respected for
highlighted in a study of 144 third- through fifth- their unique capabilities and interests, and where
grade students (Skinner & Belmont, 1993). The teachers were supportive of individual autonomy.
researchers assessed teacher involvement with
students, structure (including clear expectations), Teachers’ Impact on the Classroom
support for autonomy, and student engagement Community
including both behavioral and emotional reac- Teachers can play a role in promoting positive
tions. Although the study did not include indica- interactions between students and their peers and
tors of school identification, it showed that in creating a caring classroom community. The
teacher-provided structure in the classroom was powerful effects of peers on students’ behavior,
related to students’ engagement across the school work habits, and values—especially when young-
year. Further, teachers who provided less support sters enter adolescence—are well established. For
and structure were viewed as less consistent and students entering their teen years, the influences
more coercive (Deci et al., 1991; Reeve, Bolt, & of peers may even override those of parents. In
Cai, 1999). school, the presence of positive support from
Consistent expectations for all students are peers can increase identification, and the absence
also important. If teachers hold differential of peer support can hinder its development
expectations and display differential treatment (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Ladd, Kochenderfer,
for some students based on gender, race/ethnic- & Coleman, 1996; Radziwon, 2003).
ity, or achievement levels, this can reduce stu- Negative peer influences can lead a student to
dents’ trust or receptivity to the teacher as a disidentify and engage in dysfunctional behavior.
source of support, motivation, and feelings of Interestingly, in a study of 331 seventh-grade stu-
belonging. This has been found empirically dents in one urban school, Ryan (2001) found
among African-American students (Chavous, that peers affected students’ intrinsic value for
Rivas-Drake, Smalls, Griffin, & Cogburn, 2008; school (defined to include several elements of
Felice, 1981) and students of Hispanic origin belongingness) more than its utility value:
(Rubie-Davies, 2006). In interviews with 56 high “Students who ‘hung out’ with a group of friends
school students (Davidson & Phelan, 1999), stu- who disliked school showed a greater decrease in
dents were critical of teachers who expressed dif- their own enjoyment of school over the course of
ferential expectations for academic or economic the school year” (p. 1146). Students who engage
futures across ethnic or racial lines. If teachers in destructive behavior may lead others down
hold lower expectations for some students than that path. To the extent that teachers can encour-
for others, this can translate into less optimal age positive student behavior, these harmful
interactions with those students, poorer academic effects can be avoided.
performance, and disidentification from school. Some research suggests that working in groups
Appreciating each student as an individual and increases affinity among students. Interviews
promoting their individual predispositions sup- with elementary students elicited a number of
ports school identification. This is shown by positive comments about working together
teachers who respect students’ uniqueness and including that they learn better themselves and
encourage their autonomy (McNeely et al., 2002; help other students learn (Allen, 1995).
9 School Identification 207

Instructional strategies that create close working perceptions of sense of community, an affective
groups include cooperative learning and dialogue. measure that includes components of identifica-
Cooperative learning increases student-student tion with school, with effect sizes from one-third
interactions and affects learning conditions (coop- to one-half standard deviation (Solomon, Watson,
erative instead of competitive). Research shows Battistich, Schaps, & Delucchi, 1992). Continuing
that the improvements due to cooperative learn- research led to the conclusion that students’
ing include increases in interpersonal attraction, engagement was affected, not only by individual
more prosocial interactions, and enhanced classrooms, but by school community in general.
feelings of belongingness (Johnson, Johnson, Paramount among the empirical results was the
Buckman, & Richards, 1985; Osterman, 2000). finding that the classroom practices included in
Dialogue, also a component of cooperative the CDP program were related to students’ sense
learning, is discussion among students that allows of community and, in turn, “a positive orientation
each participant to express their own feelings and toward school and learning, including attraction
opinions while working on learning tasks. to school…task orientation toward learning, edu-
Although research is sparse, arguments presented cational aspirations, and trust in a respect for
by Osterman (2000) indicate that dialogue in the teachers” (p. 143). Further, caring school com-
classroom gives students the opportunity to munities appeared to be most beneficial for the
express themselves to their classmates and to dis- neediest students.
cover that they are accepted by others; it is a
mechanism for enhancing belongingness. A study
of eighth-graders’ perceptions of the classroom The Correlates of School Identification
environment (Ryan & Patrick, 2001) revealed
that teachers’ attempts to promote interactions in School identification has been examined in
the classroom were themselves related to numerous educational and psychological studies,
increased student motivation and engagement. usually in the form of separate components
The culture of the classroom community gen- (belonging or valuing). Recent research on the
erally is also important (Bateman, 2002; connection of identification with school outcomes
Battistich, Solomon, Watson, & Schaps, 1997; is summarized in this section. Some studies pur-
McMillan & Chavis, 1986; Osterman, 2000). port to measure belonging, bonding, attachment,
Research on school communities and students’ or connectedness but on close examination do not
psychological sense of community are based on assess these constructs as defined in this chapter.
the assumption that the basic needs for belonging They are not included in this summary. The stud-
and valuing are best met in cohesive, caring group ies show that identification with school, being an
settings with a shared purpose. This follows from affective construct, is more directly related to
Battistich et al.’s (1997) description of a “caring other attitudes and behaviors than it is to academic
school community.” The Child Development achievement or attainment. However, the consis-
Project (CDP) is an attempt to create classroom tency and strength of the association of identifica-
and school communities that enhance behavioral tion with student behavior is impressive.
and affective engagement. CDP encourages stu- The mechanisms through which identification
dents to collaborate with other students, help is connected to different outcomes may vary.
other students, discuss the experiences of others, Identification has been shown to have a direct
reflect on their own behavior, develop appropri- link with behavioral engagement, with positive
ate prosocial behavior, and take responsibility for identification (internal motivation) prompting
personal decision-making (Battistich et al.). The positive academic and social behavior (Voelkl,
intervention is implemented largely by teachers 1997). Misbehavior out of school may result from
with support from others. weakened bonds to school which would serve
The original evaluation showed that CDP otherwise to control students’ behavior. And iden-
increased fourth- through sixth-grade students’ tification may be related to academic achievement
208 K.E. Voelkl

indirectly through its impact on engagement in beyond all other measures. These findings are
the classroom. mirrored in other studies of identification and
Although many studies consider identifica- classroom participation (Leithwood & Jantzi,
tion in its positive forms (i.e., more identification 1999; Pannozzo et al., 2004).
associated with better behavior), some examine Other studies focused on belongingness. In
low levels of identification or disidentification one, over 1,000 students in three high schools
and their consequences. This is seen in the con- were administered a self-report sense of commu-
nection between identification with in-school nity scale and a questionnaire regarding their
misbehavior and out-of-school misbehavior (e.g., behavioral engagement (e.g., class cutting,
substance use or delinquency). These conse- thoughts of dropping out, perceptions of class
quences are of greater concern to educators. disruptiveness, and preparedness) (Royal &
Rossi, 1996). The zero-order correlations of sense
of community with all engagement behaviors
Identification and Behavioral were positive and statistically significant in each
Engagement high school. Depending on the engagement
behavior, correlations ranged from .17 to .56. In a
Research has shown that students who identify separate study, school membership was found to
with school are more likely than others to engage be related to time spent on homework among
in classroom activities, follow written and unwrit- middle school students (Hagborg, 1998). And
ten rules of behavior, and invest more energy in several studies have found identification to be
understanding academic subject matter. It is little related to extracurricular participation, but the
surprise that the connections are found consis- results were less consistent than those for class-
tently in school-based research: classroom behav- room participation (Eccles & Barber, 1999;
ior is the most proximal outcome of identification Leithwood & Jantzi, 1999)
of those discussed in this chapter. Conversely, research has shown that low lev-
Several studies have used Voelkl’s (1996) els of identification affect negative in-school
Identification with School scale. The 16-item behaviors. For example, in a study of over 800
self-report instrument assesses both belonging students in grades 3 through 12, Hill and Werner
and valuing. The instrument was pilot tested on (2006) used self-report questionnaires to assess
over 3,500 eighth-grade students. Confirmatory levels of affiliative orientation (need for affilia-
factor analysis showed that it could be scored as tion), school attachment, and aggression.
two separate belonging and valuing subscales or Aggression was the frequency of a number of
as one combined identification scale; the choice aggressive acts, out of seven, the student dis-
would depend on the particular context in which played in the past semester. In this study, school
it was being used. Scale reliabilities were .76 and attachment was directly (negatively) related to
.73 for the two subtests and .84 for total identifi- aggression and also mediated the connection
cation scores. between affiliative orientation and aggression.
In one longitudinal study (Voelkl, 1997), aca- Both aspects of identification (belonging and
demic achievement was assessed in 1,335 fourth- valuing) were assessed in studies of school mis-
and seventh-grade students, and participation in behavior, which the authors called “school delin-
learning activities and identification were quency” (Jenkins, 1995; Payne, 2008; Stewart,
assessed in eighth grade. Of all the demographic 2003). In one study (Jenkins) middle school stu-
and educational variables in the study, identifica- dents responded to a self-report measure of school
tion was correlated most strongly with classroom commitment. School delinquency was comprised
participation (.30). In an analysis predicting iden- of three indicators: school crime, school miscon-
tification from the other variables, participation duct, and nonattendance. School commitment
had the largest standardized regression weight, (i.e., valuing educational goals) was a strong pre-
which was statistically significant above and dictor of all measures of delinquency even when
9 School Identification 209

a number of background characteristics were factors were controlled statistically. The size of
taken into account. In a separate study of high the effects was −.27 and −.32 standard deviations
school students, Payne measured identification for aggression and vandalism, respectively.
through two scales: attachment and commitment. The connection of identification with school
The study also included a third “belief” scale, but with student behavior and misbehavior is found
this particular scale does not fall within the defi- consistently. In no study reviewed except those
nition of identification in this chapter. Delinquency concerning extracurricular activities was the rela-
was the number of in-school crimes, out of 13, tionship nonsignificant or could it be considered
committed in the past 12 months. Both compo- weak. For the most part, this consistency is found
nents of identification were significant: “students with regard to out-of-school behavior as well.
who are more attached to their school and teach-
ers…are less likely to engage in delinquency”
(Payne, 2008, p. 447). Identification and Out-of-School
Identification with school has also been found Misbehavior
to be connected with particular types of misbe-
havior, specifically bullying, cheating, and alco- Students who develop positive bonds with school
hol use during the school day. A study of “bullies, are more likely to succeed in school and refrain
victims, and bully victims” (Cunningham, 2007) from delinquent behavior. Conversely, students
examined bullying and school bonding in a sam- who reject school norms are more likely to engage
ple of sixth- through eighth-grade students in in antisocial behavior (Maddox & Prinz, 2003;
Catholic schools. Students who were neither bul- Simons-Morton, Crump, Haynie, & Saylor,
lies nor bullied had the highest average bonding 1999). Several theories have been forwarded to
scores. Both groups of victims had the lowest explain the impact of school attitudes on misbe-
scores, indicating to the authors that being bul- havior. Jessor and Jessor’s (1977) problem behav-
lied puts students at risk for disidentification from ior theory asserted that due to underlying motives,
school. perceptions, and attitudes, behaviors are linked
A study of high school students’ academic across contexts. Thus, problem behaviors in one
cheating (Voelkl & Frone, 2004) yielded a strong context tend to be related to problem behavior in
correlation (−.43) of the Identification with other contexts. According to this logic, the asso-
School scale with self-reported cheating (defined ciation of identification with in-school behavior
as cheating on tests, not doing one’s own home- would extend to out-of-school settings. Also,
work, and plagiarism). The results also revealed according to social control theory, individuals are
an interaction between identification and aca- more likely to commit delinquent acts when ties
demic performance: students who were less iden- to conventional social institutions such as school
tified with school and who had low achievement are weakened (Hirschi, 1969). Thus, students
scores had the highest rates of cheating of all who devalue teachers’ expectations, do not value
groups studied. In a separate study of aggression educational goals, and regard school rules as
and vandalism at school, Voelkl and Frone (2003) unfair are more likely to commit delinquent acts
administered lengthy questionnaires to 208 high (Jenkins, 1995; Krohn & Massey, 1980).
school students that included measures of aggres- Evidence for the association between school
sion and vandalism, in-school and out-of-school bonds and out-of-school problem behavior has
alcohol use, the Identification with School scale, focused largely on substance use and delinquency.
and other academic and personality measures. Maddox and Prinz (2003) conducted an extensive
In-school alcohol use, the main focus of the study, review of conceptualizations, measurements, and
was related to aggression and vandalism, but out- theories of school bonding. The authors concluded
of-school use was not. Identification with school that despite the multitude of definitions and mea-
was (negatively) significantly related to both out- sures of bonding, higher levels of school bonding
comes even when demographic and personality have been found consistently to be related to less
210 K.E. Voelkl

substance use and delinquency. School bonding Shears, Edwards, and Stanley (2006) exam-
was identified as an important target for interven- ined the relationship between school bonding as
tion in order to protect against negative outcomes a “protective factor” and substance use in a
and promoting positive outcomes. national study of students in grades 7 through 12.
Classic sociological work identified identifi- The measure of school bonding included the
cation with school as an important antecedent of degree to which students liked school and their
juvenile delinquency. Hirschi (1969) used ques- teachers, felt their teachers liked them, and
tionnaires to assess attachment to school, parents, regarded school as fun. Two measures of use
and peers, and self-reported delinquency in a were assessed for each substance (alcohol, mari-
sample of 1,200 adolescent boys. In this study, juana, inhalant, amphetamine): having ever tried
attachment to delinquent friends was found to be the substance and level of involvement with each
associated with delinquency, and delinquency substance. The study revealed that for all
was inversely related to attachment to school. substances, greater school bonding was associ-
Elliott and Voss (1974) studied over 2,600 stu- ated with lower odds of having tried the substance
dents from ninth grade onward, assessing dis- and lower levels of involvement. Bonding was
identification in the form of “normlessness” and found to be more protective for female, white,
“school isolation.” Both factors were related sig- and Mexican-American students and for students
nificantly to serious delinquent acts and dropping living in isolated rural communities.
out of school. Correlations for normlessness The assumption that schools vary in their
ranged from .30 to .52, and from .20 to .30 for impacts on substance use lead Henry and Slater
school isolation. (2007) to study the effect at the school level.
In a more recent national study, Resnick et al. Using a national sample of students in middle
(1997) analyzed data from the Add Health survey and junior high schools, they examined the effects
that included measures of connectedness and a of both student-level and school-level indicators
range of negative behaviors. Connectedness was of school attachment on five measures of alcohol
defined as perceiving fair treatment from teach- use. A composite measure of school attachment
ers, closeness to others, and belonging. The included feelings of liking school and teachers,
authors of this study identified school connected- sense of belongingness, and academic success.
ness as a protective factor for both adolescent Using multilevel modeling, the findings showed
substance use and violence. Among both middle that students’ own level of school attachment was
school and high school students, perceived school significantly associated with recent alcohol use,
connectedness was associated with less frequent intention to use alcohol, beliefs about peer use,
cigarette use, alcohol use, and marijuana use. and favorable attitudes toward alcohol use. In
Also, higher levels of school connectedness were addition, a strong contextual effect of school
associated with lower levels of violence such as attachment was found. Attending schools where
physical fighting and weapon use. students were more attached was associated with
In a review of research on adolescent sub- lower odds of recent or anticipated future alcohol
stance use, Hawkins et al. (1992) identified four use, a decreased perception that students in their
contextual and 13 individual risk factors associ- school use alcohol, and a stronger belief that
ated with the use of alcohol and other illicit sub- alcohol use is detrimental to life aspirations.
stances. School factors included academic failure While much of the research on adolescent
and low commitment to school; commitment was substance use has measured general use (not tied
considered as liking for school, perceived rele- to any particular setting), Voelkl and colleagues
vance of course work (valuing), educational have focused use during the school day (Voelkl,
expectations, truancy, and time spent on home- 2004; Voelkl & Frone, 2000; Voelkl, Willert, &
work. All of the reviewed evidence demonstrated Marable, 2003). Data from national and local
that lower commitment to school was associated investigations indicate that anywhere between
with higher levels of drug use. 6% and 25% of adolescents in the USA reported
9 School Identification 211

using alcohol or marijuana during school hours tested it in three samples of fifth- through eighth-
(Voelkl et al., 2003). Teachers and principals are grade students. The correlation between PSSM
also aware that their schools are not drug free scores and measures of achievement in the three
(Heaviside, Rowand, Williams, & Farris, 1998; studies were 36, .55, and .33, respectively. All
Mansfield, Alexander, & Farris, 1991; Voelkl & were statistically significant at the .001 level.
Willert, 2006). Hagborg developed a shortened form of the
According to Voelkl et al. (2003), substance PSSM and tested it with 120 middle school stu-
use in school is largely a function of the degree to dents. This study also revealed a significant cor-
which students feel identified with or disidenti- relation (.35) between PSSM scores and grade
fied from their school and also the degree to point averages.
which schools provide the opportunity to use Other studies discovered that the relationship
drugs. This hypothesis was tested empirically in between belonging and achievement was more
an investigation of personal and situational pre- complex. Ladd and Dinella (2009) followed 383
dictors of substance use in school (Voelkl & children from kindergarten through eighth grade,
Frone, 2000). The results confirmed the hypoth- obtaining measures of school liking-avoidance in
eses; identification with school was significantly first through third grade and academic achieve-
related to both alcohol and marijuana use at ment in first through eighth grade. The authors
school, but the effect was moderated by ease of called school liking “emotional engagement.”
use. That is, school identification was negatively Although it does not fit the definition of identifi-
related to alcohol and marijuana use among stu- cation with school used in this chapter, the items
dents who perceived they had ample opportunity appear to reflect aspects of identification, and the
to use these substances at school without being study added an important consideration—the
caught. When students felt they were likely to continuity of affect over several years. In a set of
be caught, school identification was unrelated to sophisticated analyses, the authors concluded
either type of substance. that “average levels of school liking-avoidance
during the primary grades predicted growth in
achievement” (p. 200) over the 8-year period. If
Identification and Academic identification with school is related directly to
Achievement/Attainment academic achievement, it may be long-term
growth of identification rather than identification
Research on the components of identification with at one point in time that is important.
school has typically found weak or indirect rela- Wang and Holcombe (2010) used data on over
tionships with academic achievement. This is 1,000 adolescents from the Maryland Adolescent
consistent with the framework depicted in Fig. 9.1; Development in Context Study to test structural
behavioral engagement is shown as intervening equation models of the relationships among stu-
between identification and achievement. Rela- dents’ perceptions of the school environment
tively, little research has explored the relationship (seventh grade), school engagement (eighth
between dropping out of school and identification grade), and grade point average (GPA) at the end
although the theory and the limited data indicate of eighth grade. The engagement measures were
that students who become disidentified from indicators of school participation, the use of self-
school have increased odds of dropping out. regulation strategies, and identification with
Despite inconsistent findings in general, some school, the latter including belonging and valu-
studies found significant positive linkages ing items. Two conclusions emerged from the
between a component of identification and aca- study regarding identification and achievement.
demic achievement (Goodenow, 1993; Hagborg, First, the direct paths from the three engagement
1998; LeCroy & Krysik, 2008). Goodenow measures to GPA were statistically significant
developed the 18-item psychological sense of with school identification having the strongest
school membership (PSSM) questionnaire and impact of the three. Second, the connections
212 K.E. Voelkl

between perceptions of the school environment Pannozzo et al. found the effect of identification
and GPA were mediated by school identification. was reduced to nonsignificance. Even though
It can be concluded from this study that there are dropping out is often described as a gradual pro-
both direct and indirect effects of identification cess of disengagement from school, both of these
on academic achievement. studies indicate that behavioral engagement is
These studies stand in contrast to others that the most important.
show only weak, inconsistent, or indirect rela- Based on a review of a large number of studies
tionships. For example, Goodenow (1993) of affective engagement and school achievement
reported that the correlations between school published through 1999, Osterman (2000) con-
membership and grades were lower than those cluded “There is little evidence demonstrating
between membership and motivation (as mea- that sense of belonging is directly related to
sured by expectations for success and valuing achievement, but there is substantial evidence
schoolwork). Voelkl (1997) found that identifica- showing or suggesting that sense of belonging
tion was more strongly correlated with student influences achievement through its effects on
participation than with grades; the latter ranged [behavioral] engagement” (p. 341). More recent
from .02 to .13. Ma (2003) found a negative cor- achievement studies and studies using newer
relation in grade 6 and a positive correlation in methodology tended to find positive relation-
grade 8. Strambler and Weinstein (2010) assessed ships, but the inconsistencies remain to be
valuing and devaluing of academic subjects in a resolved. The research on identification and drop-
sample of elementary-grade African-American ping out suggests that identification with school
and Latino students. They reported that devalu- has an indirect effect on graduation, if any. These
ing was significantly related to lower standard- linkages require further study to understand the
ized achievement test scores in language arts and processes by which identification may be related
math, but valuing was not significantly related to to these particular outcomes.
either.
Research on identification and graduation/
dropping out also introduces some complexities. Conclusion
For example, a longitudinal Canadian survey of
over 13,000 seventh- through 11th-grade students This chapter provides a theoretical perspective
attending low-SES high schools administered on students’ identification with school and accom-
measures of behavioral and affective engagement panying empirical evidence. Several themes
(identification with school) (Archambault, emerge from this chapter. School identification is
Janosz, Morizot, & Pagani, 2009). The analysis as an affective form of engagement comprised of
revealed that, of the three, behavioral disengage- students’ sense of belonging in school and feeling
ment was the most highly associated with drop- that school is valuable. Both components are
ping out. Above and beyond that, dropouts tended based on psychological theory that asserts that
to have low scores on multiple types of engage- humans have basic needs to belong and to feel
ment including school identification. In a sepa- their actions are worthwhile. A host of affective
rate study of over 2,000 eighth-grade students, responses to school have been identified as forms
Pannozzo et al. (2004) compiled teacher ratings of student engagement (Jimerson et al., 2003;
of students’ academic and social engagement, Libbey, 2004; Maddox & Prinz, 2003). Terms
reading and mathematics achievement, identifi- such as interest, liking, boredom, and motivation
cation with school, and graduation/dropout status have also been used to conceptualize students’
4 years later. The correlation between identifica- relationship with school (Fredricks et al., 2004).
tion with school and graduation/dropping out was Although considerable progress has been made
small but statistically significant (.09). However, in on distinguishing among these concepts, more
several regressions predicting dropping out from research is needed on how these affective
behavioral engagement and school identification, responses are related and how they are measured.
9 School Identification 213

It is important to distinguish between simple be measured as strong, moderate, or weak in a


emotional responses to school (e.g., liking school) particular school. With proper school conditions
and more complex psychological responses. and appropriate support, most students can
Indeed, children who have internalized the value develop the internal motivation that drives school
of doing well in school may work on tasks that behavior and school success.
are less interesting and for which no external
rewards are expected (Deci et al., 1991; Ryan,
Connell, & Grolnick, 1992). References
Identification with school has not evolved
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes
when children enter school. Rather, identification
on behavior. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M. P.
develops over a period of time in response to aca- Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 173–221).
demic accomplishments and failures, and to Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
interactions with parents, peers, and teachers. Allen, J. (1995). Friends, fairness, fun, and the freedom to
choose: Hearing student voices. Journal of Curriculum
Identification is preceded by elementary forms of
and Supervision, 10, 286–301.
affect (e.g., regarding school as fun, enjoying American Federation of Teachers. (2008). We asked, you
school) and extrinsic rewards (e.g., stickers, answered. American Educator, 32(2), 6–7.
praise) which can lead to early forms of behav- Anderman, E. M., & Wolters, C. A. (2006). Goals, values,
and affect: Influences on student motivation. In P. A.
ioral engagement (e.g., attending school, com-
Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educa-
pleting homework). In addition, appropriate tional psychology (pp. 369–389). Mahwah, NJ:
school conditions—feelings of safety, being Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
treated fairly, and being supported by teachers— Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A.
L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological
are important factors that help shape students’
engagement: Validation of the student engagement
identification and engagement behaviors. Over instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44,
time, identification becomes an internal source of 427–445.
motivation for continued engagement in school. Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L.
(2009). Adolescent behavioral, affective, and cogni-
More research is needed to understand the
tive engagement in school: Relationship to dropout.
process by which identification becomes inter- Journal of School Health, 79, 408–415.
nalized. Longitudinal studies or overlapping Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation.
cohort studies would be particularly useful for Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying the-
understanding the maturation of attitudes and
ory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84,
behaviors. Because individual needs change as 191–215.
students progress from elementary to middle Bateman, H. V. (2002). Sense of community in the school:
school, research should identify critical age peri- Listening to students’ voices. In A. T. Fisher, C. C.
Sonn, & B. J. Bishop (Eds.), Psychological sense of
ods when attitudes toward school are most vul-
community: Research, applications, and implications
nerable (Eccles et al., 1993). To what extent are (pp. 161–179). New York: Academic/Plenum
external rewards important to students in elemen- publishers.
tary, middle, and high school? Is it possible to Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Kim, D., Watson, M., &
Schaps, E. (1995). Schools as communities: Poverty
develop a sense of identification with school in
levels of student populations, and students’ attitudes,
middle school after years of negative attitude/ motives, and performance: A multilevel analysis.
behavior patterns have been established? Finally, American Educational Research Journal, 32, 627–658.
work is needed to understand the roles of contex- Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Watson, M., & Schaps, E.
(1997). Caring school communities. Educational
tual factors such as a welcoming school environ-
Psychologist, 32, 137–151.
ment, student and faculty composition, availability Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to
of help for students who need it, and safety in the belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fun-
development of school identification. This knowl- damental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin,
117, 497–529.
edge would be particularly important to educa-
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self perception theory. In L. Berkowitz
tors and administrators. Future research should (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
assess the degree to which each factor can (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York: Academic.
214 K.E. Voelkl

Bernstein, S., & Rulo, J. H. (1976). Learning disabili- Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation
ties and learning problems: Their implications for and self-determination in human behavior. New York:
the juvenile justice system. Juvenile Justice, 27, Plenum.
43–47. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why”
Boulton, M. J., Duke, E., Holman, G., Laxton, E., of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determina-
Nicholas, B., Spells, R., et al. (2009). Associations tion of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
between being bullied, perceptions of safety in class- Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R.
room and playground, and relationship with teacher M. (1991). Motivation and education: The self-deter-
among primary school pupils. Educational Studies, mination perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26,
35, 255–267. 325–346.
Brewster, A. B., & Bowen, G. L. (2004). Teacher support Eccles, J. S. (2008, June). Can middle school reform
and the school engagement of Latino middle and high increase high school graduation rates? (California
school students at risk of school failure. Child and Dropout Research Policy Brief #12). Santa Barbara,
Adolescent Social Work Journal, 21, 47–67. CA: University of California.
Byrne, D. (1997). An overview (and underview) of Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1999). Student council, vol-
research and theory within the attraction paradigm. unteering, basketball, or marching band: What kind of
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, extracurricular involvement matters? Journal of
417–431. Adolescent Research, 14, 10–43.
Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1994). Reinforcement, Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M.,
reward, and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis. Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development
Review of Educational Research, 64, 363–423. during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment
Cassidy, W. (2005). From zero tolerance to a culture of fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools and in
care. Education Canada, 45(3), 40–42. families. American Psychologist, 48, 90–101.
Catalano, R., & Hawkins, J. (1996). Delinquency and Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs,
crime: Current theories. New York: Cambridge values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,
University Press. 109–132.
Chavous, T. M., Rivas-Drake, D., Smalls, C., Griffin, T., Eccles (Parsons), J., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S.
& Cogburn, C. (2008). Gender matters, too: The influ- B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., et al. (1983).
ences of school racial discrimination and racial iden- Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In J. T.
tity on academic engagement outcomes among African Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motives:
American adolescents. Developmental Psychology, Psychological and sociological approaches (pp.
44, 637–654. 75–146). San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman.
Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., Appleton, J. J., Berman, Elliott, D. S., & Voss, H. L. (1974). Delinquency and
S., Spanjers, D., & Varro, P. (2008). Best practices in dropout. Lexington, MA: D.C. Health.
fostering student engagement. In A. Thomas & Felice, L. G. (1981). Black student dropout behavior:
J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology Disengagement from school rejection and racial dis-
V (pp. 1099–1120). Washington, DC: National crimination. The Journal of Negro Education, 50,
Association of School Psychologists. 415–424.
Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994). Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance.
Educational risk and resilience in African American Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
youth: Context, self, action, and outcomes in school. Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
Child Development, 65(2), 493–506. Educational Research, 59, 117–142.
Connell, J., & Wellborn, J. (1991). Competence, auton- Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success
omy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of self- among students at risk for school failure. Journal of
system processes. In M. Gunnar & L. Sroufe (Eds.), Applied Psychology, 82, 221–234.
Self processes in development. Minnesota symposia on Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
child psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 43–77). Chicago: School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
University of Chicago Press. the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
Cornell, D. G., & Mayer, M. J. (2010). Why do school 59–109.
order and safety matter? Educational Researcher, 39, Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a
7–15. factor in children’s academic engagement and perfor-
Cunningham, N. J. (2007). Level of bonding to school and mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,
perceptions of the school environment by bullies, vic- 148–162.
tims, and bully victims. Journal of Early Adolescence, Goodenow, C. (1993). The psychological sense of school
27, 457–478. membership among adolescents: Scale development
Davidson, A. L., & Phelan, P. (1999). Students’ multiple and educational correlates. Psychology in the Schools,
worlds: An anthropological approach to understanding 30, 79–90.
students’ engagement with school. In T. Urdan (Ed.), Greene, B. A., Miller, R. B., Crowson, H. M., Duke, B. L.,
Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 11, & Akey, K. L. (2004). Predicting high school students’
pp. 233–273). Stamford, CT: JAI. cognitive engagement and achievement: Contributions
9 School Identification 215

of classroom perceptions and motivation. Irvine, J. J. (1986). Teacher-student interactions: Effects


Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 462–482. of student race, sex, and grade level. Journal of
Hagborg, W. J. (1998). An investigation of a brief measure of Educational Psychology, 78, 14–21.
school membership. Adolescence, 33, 461–468. Jackson, P. W. (1990). Life in classrooms. New York:
Halbesleben, J. R. B., & Wheeler, A. R. (2008). The rela- Teachers College Press.
tive roles of engagement and embeddedness in pre- Jenkins, P. H. (1995). School delinquency and school
dicting job performance and intention to leave. Work commitment. Sociology of Education, 68, 221–239.
& Stress, 22, 242–256. Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1977). Problem behavior and
Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., & Keyes, C. L. M. (2003). psychosocial development: A longitudinal study of
Well-being in the workplace and its relationship to youth. New York: Academic.
business outcomes: A review of the Gallup studies. In Jimerson, S. R., Campos, E., & Greif, J. L. (2003). Toward
C. L. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: The posi- an understanding of definitions and measures of school
tive person and the good life (pp. 205–224). Washington, engagement and related terms. California School
DC: American Psychological Association. Psychologist, 8(7), 27.
Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., & Miller, J. Y. (1992). Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., Buckman, L. A., &
Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug Richards, P. S. (1985). The effect of prolonged imple-
problems in adolescents and early adulthood: mentation of cooperative learning on social support
Implications for substance abuse prevention. within the classroom. The Journal of Psychology, 119,
Psychological Bulletin, 112, 64–105. 405–411.
Heaviside, S., Rowand, C., Williams, C., & Farris, E. Johnson, M. K., Crosnoe, R., & Elder, G. H. (2001).
(1998). Violence and discipline problems in U.S. pub- Students’ attachment and academic engagement: The
lic schools: 1996–97 (NCES 98–030). Washington, role of race and ethnicity. Sociology of Education, 74,
DC: U.S. Department of Education, National Center 318–340.
for Education Statistics. Juvonen, J. (2006). Sense of belonging, social bonds, and
Henry, K. L., & Slater, M. D. (2007). The contextual effect school functioning. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne
of school attachment on young adolescents’ alcohol (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology
use. Journal of School Health, 77, 67–74. (pp. 655–674). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hill, L. G., & Werner, N. E. (2006). Affiliative motivation, Kahn, W. A. (1990). Psychological conditions of personal
school attachment, and aggression in school. engagement and disengagement at work. Academy of
Psychology in the Schools, 43, 231–246. Management Journal, 33, 692–724.
Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley, CA: Kailin, J. (1999). How white teachers perceive the prob-
University of California Press. lem of racism in their schools: A case study in “liberal”
Hirschi, T. (2005). Causes of delinquency. New Brunswick, Lakeview. Teachers College Record, 100, 724–750.
NJ: Transaction Publishers. Kanungo, R. N. (1979). The concept of alienation and
Hogg, M. A. (1992). The social psychology of group cohe- involvement revisited. Psychological Bulletin, 86,
siveness: From attraction to social identity. New York: 119–138.
New York University Press. Kanungo, R. N. (1982). Measurement of job and work
Homans, G. C. (1974). Social behavior: Its elementary involvement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67,
forms. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. 341–349.
Hudley, C., & Daoud, A. (2007). High school students’ Kaplan, D. S., Peck, B. M., & Kaplan, H. B. (1995).
engagement in school: Understanding the relationship A structural model of dropout behavior: A longitudi-
to school context and student expectations. In F. Salili nal analysis. Applied Behavioral Science Review, 3,
& R. Hoosain (Eds.), Culture motivation and learn- 177–193.
ing: A multicultural perspective (pp. 365–389). New Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships mat-
York: Information Age. ter: Linking teacher support to student engagement and
Hughes, J., & Kwok, O.-M. (2007). Influence of student- achievement. Journal of School Health, 74, 262–273.
teacher and parent-teacher relationships on lower Krohn, M., & Massey, J. (1980). Social control and delin-
achieving readers’ engagement and achievement in the quent behavior: An examination of the elements of the
primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, social bond. The Sociological Quarterly, 21, 529–543.
99, 39–51. Ladd, G. W., & Dinella, L. M. (2009). Continuity and change
Hughes, J. N., Luo, W., Kwok, O.-M., & Loyd, L. K. in early school engagement: Predictive of children’s
(2008). Teacher-student support, effortful engage- achievement trajectories from first to eighth grade?
ment, and achievement: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 190–206.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 1–14. Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C.
Institute for Research and Improvement in Education. (1996). Friendship quality as a predictor of young
(2002). First Things First’s approach to small learn- children’s early school adjustment. Child Development,
ing communities: An overview. Philadelphia: Institute 67, 1103–1118.
for Research and Reform in Education. Lawler, E. E., & Hall, D. T. (1970). Relationship of job
Institute of Medicine. (2010). School meals: Building characteristics to job involvement, satisfaction, and
blocks for healthy children. Washington, DC: The intrinsic motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology,
National Academies Press. 54, 305–312.
216 K.E. Voelkl

LeCroy, C. W., & Krysik, J. (2008). Predictors of aca- Olson, J. M., & Stone, J. (2005). The influence of behav-
demic achievement and school attachment among ior on attitudes. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M.
Hispanic adolescents. Children and Schools, 30, P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 223–272).
197–209. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Leithwood, K., & Jantzi, D. (1999). The relative effects of Osterman, K. F. (2000). Students’ need for belonging
principal and teacher sources of leadership on student in the school community. Review of Educational
engagement in school. Educational Administration Research, 70, 323–367.
Quarterly, 35, 679–706. Pannozzo, G. M., Finn, J. D., & Boyd-Zaharias, J. (2004,
Leitman, R., Binns, K., & Unni, A. (1995). Uninformed April). Behavioral and affective engagement in school
decisions: A survey of children and parents about math and dropping out. Paper presented at the American
and science. NACME Research Letter, 5, 1–9. Educational Research Association Conference, San
Libbey, H. P. (2004). Measuring student relationships to Diego, CA.
school: Attachment, bonding, connectedness, and Payne, A. A. (2008). A multilevel analysis of the relation-
engagement. Journal of School Health, 74, 274–283. ships among communal school organization, student
Ma, X. (2003). Sense of belonging in school: Can schools bonding, and delinquency. Journal of Research in
make a difference? The Journal of Educational Crime and Delinquency, 45, 429–455.
Research, 96, 340–349. Pearson, J., Muller, C., & Wilkinson, L. (2007). Adolescent
Maddox, S. J., & Prinz, R. J. (2003). School bonding in same-sex attraction and academic outcomes: The role
children and adolescents: Conceptualization, assess- of school attachment and engagement. Social
ment, and associated variables. Clinical Child and Problems, 54, 523–542.
Family Psychology Review, 6, 31–49. Perry, N. E., Turner, J. C., & Meyer, D. K. (2006).
Mansfield, W., Alexander, D., & Farris, E. (1991). Teacher Classrooms as contexts for motivating learning. In
survey on safe, disciplined, and drug-free schools P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of
(NCES 91–091). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 327–348).
Education, National Center for Education Statistics. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Marcus, R. F., & Sanders-Reio, J. (2001). The influence of Pianta, R. C. (1999). Enhancing relationships between
attachment on school completion. School Psychology children and teachers. Washington, DC: American
Quarterly, 16, 427–444. Psychological Association.
Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and
New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. self-regulated learning components of classroom aca-
McCloud, S. (2005). From chaos to consistency. demic performance. Journal of Educational
Educational Leadership, 62(5), 46–49. Psychology, 82, 33–40.
McMillan, D. W. (1996). Sense of community. Journal of Polk, K., & Halferty, D. (1972). School cultures, adoles-
Community Psychology, 24, 315–325. cent commitments, and delinquency: A preliminary
McMillan, D. W., & Chavis, D. M. (1986). Sense of com- study. In K. Polk & W. E. Schafer (Eds.), Schools and
munity: A definition and theory. Journal of Community delinquency (pp. 70–90). Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Psychology, 14, 6–23. Prentice Hall.
McNeely, C. A., Nonnemaker, J. M., & Blum, R. W. Rabinowitz, S., & Hall, D. T. (1977). Organizational
(2002). Promoting school connectedness: Evidence research on job involvement. Psychological Bulletin,
from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent 84, 265–288.
Health. Journal of School Health, 72, 138–146. Radziwon, C. D. (2003). The effects of peers’ beliefs on
Mickelson, R. A. (1990). The attitude-achievement para- 8th grade students’ identification with school. Journal
dox among black adolescents. Sociology of Education, of Research in Childhood Education, 17, 236–249.
63, 44–61. Reeve, J., Bolt, E., & Cai, Y. (1999). Autonomy-supportive
Mouton, S. G., & Hawkins, J. (1996). School attachment: teachers: How they teach and motivate students.
Perspectives of low-attached high school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 537–548.
Educational Psychology, 16, 297–304. Resnick, M. D., Harris, K. M., & Shew, M. (1997).
Newmann, F. M. (1981). Reducing student alienation in Protecting adolescents from harm: Findings from the
high schools: Implications of theory. Harvard National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health.
Educational Review, 51, 546–564. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278,
Newmann, F. M., Wehlage, G. G., & Lamborn, S. D. 823–832.
(1992). The significance and sources of student Ripski, M. B., & Gregory, A. (2009). Unfair, unsafe, and
engagement. In F. M. Newmann (Ed.), Student engage- unwelcome: Do high school students’ perceptions of
ment and achievement in American secondary schools. unfairness, hostility, and victimization in school pre-
New York: Teachers College Press. dict engagement and achievement? Journal of School
O’Farrell, S. L., & Morrison, G. M. (2003). A factor anal- Violence, 8, 355–375.
ysis exploring school bonding and related constructs Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996).
among upper elementary students. The California Perceptions of the social psychological environment
School Psychologist, 8, 53–72. and early adolescents’ psychological and behavioral
9 School Identification 217

functioning in school: The mediating role of goals and adolescent problem behavior. Health Education
belonging. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, Research, 14, 99–107.
408–422. Simpson, M. R. (2009). Engagement at work: A review of
Royal, M. A., & Rossi, R. J. (1996). Individual-level cor- the literature. International Journal of Nursing Studies,
relates of sense of community: Findings from work- 46, 1012–1024.
place and school. Journal of Community Psychology, Skiba, R. J., Peterson, R. L., & Williams, T. (1997). Office
24, 395–416. referrals and suspension: Disciplinary intervention in
Rubie-Davies, C. M. (2006). Teacher expectations and middle schools. Education and Treatment of Children,
student self-perceptions: Exploring relationships. 20, 295–315.
Psychology in the Schools, 43(5), 37–552. Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the
Rumberger, R. W. (1995). Dropping out of middle school: classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and
A multilevel analysis of students and schools. American student engagement across the school year. Journal of
Educational Research Journal, 32, 583–625. Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581.
Rumberger, R. W., & Lim, S. A. (2008). Why students Solomon, D., Watson, M., Battistich, V., Schaps, E., &
drop out of school: A review of 25 years of research Delucchi, K. (1992). Creating a caring community:
(Project Report No. 15). Santa Barbara, CA: University Educational practices that promote children’s proso-
of California, California Dropout Research Project. cial development. In F. K. Oser, A. Dick, & J.-L. Patry
Ryan, A. M. (2001). The peer group as a context for the (Eds.), Effective and responsible teaching: The new
development of young adolescent motivation and synthesis (pp. 383–396). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
achievement. Child Development, 72, 1135–1150. Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes
Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2001). The classroom social shape intellectual identity and performance. American
environment and changes in adolescents’ motivation Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
and engagement during middle school. American Stewart, E. A. (2003). School social bonds, school cli-
Educational Research Journal, 38, 437–460. mate, and school misbehavior: A multilevel analysis.
Ryan, R. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation Justice Quarterly, 20, 575–604.
of integrative processes. Journal of Personality, 63, Stipek, D. (2004). Motivation and instruction. In D. C.
397–427. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educa-
Ryan, R., Connell, J., & Grolnick, W. (1992). When tional psychology (pp. 85–113). Mahwah, NJ:
achievement is not intrinsically motivated: A theory of Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
internalization and self-regulation in school. In K. Strambler, M. J., & Weinstein, R. S. (2010). Psychological
Boggiano & T. Pittman (Eds.), Achievement and moti- disengagement in elementary school among ethnic
vation: A social developmental perspective (pp. 167–188). minority students. Journal of Applied Developmental
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Psychology, 31(155), 165.
Saleh, S. D., & Hosek, J. (1976). Job involvement: Thompson, G. L. (2002). African-American teens discuss
Concepts and measurements. Academy of Management their schooling experiences. Westport, CT: Bergin &
Journal, 19, 213–224. Garvey.
Sarason, B. R., Pierce, G. R., & Sarason, I. G. (1990). Ueno, K. (2009). Same-race friendships and school attach-
Social support: The sense of acceptance and the role of ment: Demonstrating the interaction between personal
relationships. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, & G. R. network and school composition. Sociological Forum,
Pierce (Eds.), Social support: An interactional view 24, 515–537.
(pp. 95–128). New York: John Wiley. U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics.
Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning, and motivation. (2007). School crime supplement to the National crime
Educational Psychologist, 26, 299–323. victimization survey. Retrieved Dec 15, 2010, from
Schiefele, U. (1999). Interest and learning from text. http://nces.ed.gov/surveys/ssocs/tables/scs_2007_
Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 257–279. tab_04.asp.
Schug, J., Yuki, M., Horikawa, H., & Takemura, K. (2009). Voelkl, K. (2004). School characteristics related to in-school
Similarity attraction and actually selecting similar others: substance use. Research in the Schools, 11, 22–33.
How cross-societal differences in relational mobility Voelkl, K. E. (1996). Measuring students’ identification
affect interpersonal similarity in Japan and the USA. with school. Educational and Psychological
Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 12, 95–103. Measurement, 56, 760–770.
Seeman, M. (1975). Alienation studies. In A. Inkeles Voelkl, K. E. (1997). Identification with school. American
(Ed.), Annual review of sociology (Vol. 1, pp. 91–123). Journal of Education, 105, 294–318.
Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews, Inc. Voelkl, K. E., & Frone, M. R. (2000). Predictors of sub-
Shaw, M. E. (1976). Group dynamics: The psychology of small stance use at school among high school students.
group behavior (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 583–592.
Shears, J., Edwards, R. W., & Stanley, L. R. (2006). Voelkl, K., & Frone, M. R. (2003). Predictors of aggres-
School bonding and substance use in rural communi- sion at school: The effect of school-related alcohol
ties. Social Work Research, 30, 6–18. use. NASSP Bulletin, 87, 38–54.
Simons-Morton, B. G., Crump, A. D., Haynie, D. L., & Voelkl, K., & Frone, M. R. (2004). Academic performance
Saylor, K. E. (1999). Student-school bonding and and cheating: Moderating role of school identification
218 K.E. Voelkl

and self-efficacy. The Journal of Educational Research, Wehlage, K., & Rutter, R. A. (1986). Dropping out: How
97, 115–122. much do schools contribute to the problem? Teachers
Voelkl, K., & Willert, J. H. (2006). Alcohol and drugs in College Record, 3, 374–392.
schools: Teachers’ reactions to the problem. Phi Delta Wentzel, K. (1997). Student motivation in middle school:
Kappan, 88, 37–40. The role of perceived pedagogical caring. Journal of
Voelkl, K., Willert, J. H., & Marable, M. A. (2003). Substance Educational Psychology, 89, 411–419.
use in schools. Educational Leadership, 60, 80–84. Whitlock, J. L. (2006). Youth perceptions of life at
Wang, M., & Holcombe, R. (2010). Adolescents’ percep- school: Contextual correlates of school connectedness in
tions of school environment, engagement, and aca- adolescence. Applied Developmental Science, 10, 13–29.
demic achievement in middle school. American Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1992). The development of
Educational Research Journal, 47, 633–662. achievement values: A theoretical analysis.
Wayman, J. C. (2002). Student perceptions of teacher eth- Developmental Review, 12, 265–310.
nic bias: A comparison of Mexican-American and Yowell, C. (1999). The role of the future in meeting the
non-Latino white drop outs and students. The High challenge of Latino school dropouts. Educational
School Journal, 85(1), 27–37. Foundations, 13, 5–28.
Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner
10
Dale H. Schunk and Carol A. Mullen

Abstract
Student underachievement brought about by low academic motivation is a
major factor contributing to school dropout. Motivation affects students’
engagement, or how their cognitions, behaviors, and affects are energized,
directed, and sustained during academic activities. According to Bandura’s
social cognitive theory, self-efficacy (perceived capabilities for learning or
performing actions at designated levels) is a key cognitive variable influ-
encing motivation and engagement. The conceptual framework of social
cognitive theory is described to include the roles played by vicarious,
symbolic, and self-regulatory processes. We discuss how self-efficacy
affects motivation through goals and self-evaluations of progress and how
various contextual factors may influence self-efficacy. Research is
described that relates self-efficacy to underachievement and dropout. This
chapter concludes with programs designed to raise school success and rec-
ommendations for future research.

School dropout is a major issue in the USA. It is (Bloom, 2010; Bloom & Haskins, 2010).
estimated that in the 50 largest US cities, the Although dropout affects youth from all back-
dropout rate is almost 50%, with 3.5–6 million grounds, culturally ethnic and immigrant students
students dropping out of high school each year are disproportionately represented: “The dropout
rate is 6% for whites, 12% for blacks, and 12%
for Hispanics” (Bloom, 2010, p. 91). Dropout
D.H. Schunk, Ph.D. (*)
incurs a major economic loss, likely totaling
Department of Teacher Education and Higher Education, more than $3 trillion over the next decade (PR
The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Newswire, 2009). Dropout also perpetuates such
Greensboro, NC 27412, USA social problems as unemployment, underemploy-
e-mail: dhschunk@uncg.edu
ment, welfare, teen pregnancy, and incarceration
C.A. Mullen, Ph.D. (PR Newswire, 2009).
Department of Educational Leadership and Cultural
Foundations, The University of North Carolina
Underlying these widespread problems is the
at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC 27412, USA disengagement of urban youth in their learning and
e-mail: camullen@uncg.edu success (U.S. Department of Education, 2008).

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 219


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_10, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
220 D.H. Schunk and C.A. Mullen

Approximately 72% of high school students who for learning or performing actions at designated
perform poorly are from lower-income families, levels (Bandura, 1977a, 1997). Research has
and 53% of English-language learners are under- shown that a higher sense of self-efficacy can
performing (Cuban, 2010). These trends of positively affect learning, achievement, self-
dropout and underachievement continue at the regulation, and motivational outcomes such as
postsecondary level, with disproportionate attri- individuals’ choices of activities, effort, persis-
tion among undergraduates from nontraditional tence, and interests (Bandura, 1997; Pajares,
groups, including culturally ethnic students, immi- 1996; Schunk & Pajares, 2009; Usher & Pajares,
grants, and nontraditional students (e.g., older, 2008). Self-efficacious students are motivated
part-time; Smedley, Myers, & Harrell, 1993; and engaged in learning, which promotes their
Zajacova, Lynch, & Espenshade, 2005). competence as learners. Conversely, a lower
Many factors contribute to school dropout, but sense of self-efficacy for learning and performing
a major one is underachievement brought about well in school can negatively affect students’
by low academic motivation. As used in this motivation and engagement (Pajares, 1996),
chapter, motivation refers to the process whereby increasing the risk of underachievement and
goal-directed activities are energized, directed, dropout. Teachers who help students experience
and sustained (Schunk, Pintrich, & Meece, 2008). success by fostering their development of skills,
Motivation is a complex process that can be learning strategies, and a positive outlook on life
affected by personal factors (e.g., individuals’ and their future can positively impact self-effi-
thoughts, beliefs, and emotions) and contextual cacy in their classrooms (McInerney, 2004; Miller
factors, such as classrooms, peer groups, and & Brickman, 2004).
community and home influences. Despite the solid foundation of self-efficacy
Herein we present the case that low academic research pertaining to school-aged children and
motivation perpetuates poor engagement in learn- school-to-work interventions, fewer scholars
ing and that certain strategies and interventions have assessed its relevance for urban youth strug-
can make a difference in the education of gling at school. Given that school dropout affects
America’s youth. By engagement, we mean the youth from all backgrounds but particularly those
manifestation of students’ motivation, or how who are culturally and economically disadvan-
their cognitions, behaviors, and affects are ener- taged, the self-efficacy of urban youth has unde-
gized, directed, and sustained during learning and niable importance (Mullen & Schunk, 2011).
other academic activities (Skinner, Kindermann, Self-efficacy has been identified as a predictor of
Connell, & Wellborn, 2009). Although different adolescent success in life (Perry, DeWine, Duffy,
theoretical approaches explain student motiva- & Vance, 2007).
tion and engagement, we utilize Bandura’s Examining the predictors of academic self-
(1977b, 1986, 1997, 2001) social cognitive the- efficacy in ethnic adolescents to include resil-
ory of psychological functioning, which empha- iency and persistence despite hardships and
sizes that much human learning and behavior obstacles, perceived control over one’s own suc-
occur in social environments. By interacting with cesses, and school and community engagement,
others, people learn knowledge, skills, strategies, will contribute to the emerging literature on this
beliefs, norms, and attitudes. Students act in topic (Vick & Packard, 2008). Our particular
accordance with their beliefs about their capabili- focus is the roles of personal and contextual fac-
ties and the expected outcomes of their actions. tors on disadvantaged adolescents’ academic
Social cognitive researchers have explored the motivation.
operation and outcomes of cognitive and affec- We next describe the conceptual framework of
tive processes hypothesized to underlie motiva- social cognitive theory and the key roles played
tion (Pintrich, 2003; Schunk & Pajares, 2009). by vicarious, symbolic, and self-regulatory pro-
Our interest is in a key social cognitive vari- cesses. We then discuss self-efficacy and the pro-
able: self-efficacy, or one’s perceived capabilities cess whereby self-efficacy affects motivation
10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner 221

through goals and self-evaluations of progress, as associated with them (e.g., low skills) rather than
well as how self-efficacy can affect student based on their actual capabilities (person → social/
engagement and how contextual factors may environment). In turn, environmental feedback
influence self-efficacy. The research evidence can affect self-efficacy, such as when teachers
presented relates self-efficacy to underachieve- encourage students by communicating, “I know
ment and dropout. We also briefly highlight some you can do this” (social/environment → person).
programs designed to enhance school success The link between behaviors and environmen-
through such means as school engagement, com- tal factors is evident in many instructional
munity activism, and career decision-making. sequences. Environmental factors can direct
Recommendations for future research conclude behaviors, such as when teachers point to a dis-
this chapter. play and say, “Look here,” which students do
with little conscious effort (social/environ-
ment → behavior). Behaviors can alter learners’
Conceptual Framework instructional environments. When students give
incorrect answers, teachers are apt to reteach the
material, temporarily discontinuing the lesson
Reciprocal Interactions
(behavior → social/environment).
A central tenet of Bandura’s (1977b, 1986, 1997, Social cognitive theory presents a view of
2001) social cognitive theory is that human behav- human agency in which individuals proactively
ior operates within a framework of triadic recipro- engage in creating their own career and life tra-
cality involving interactions among personal jectories (Schunk & Pajares, 2005). They hold
factors (e.g., cognitions, beliefs, skills, affects), beliefs that allow them to exert control over their
behaviors, and social/environmental factors thoughts, feelings, and actions. In reciprocal
(Fig. 10.1). These interacting influences can be fashion, people influence and are influenced by
demonstrated using self-efficacy as the personal their actions and environments. But the scope of
factor. Regarding the interaction of self-efficacy this reciprocal influence is broader than individu-
and behavior, studies have shown that self-efficacy als because they live in social environments.
influences achievement behaviors such as task Collective agency refers to people’s shared beliefs
choice, effort, persistence, and use of effective about what they are capable of accomplishing as
learning strategies (person → behavior; Schunk & a group. Groups, too, affect and are affected by
Pajares, 2009). These behaviors also affect self- their actions and environments.
efficacy. As students perform tasks and observe
their learning progress, self-efficacy for continued
Vicarious, Symbolic,
learning is enhanced (behavior → person).
Many students with learning disabilities hold
and Self-Regulatory Processes
low self-efficacy for performing well (Licht &
Social cognitive theory stresses that people pos-
Kistner, 1986). The link between personal and
sess capabilities that distinguish them from oth-
contextual factors is seen when individuals react
ers and motivate them to strive for a sense of
to these students based on attributes typically
agency (Bandura, 1986). Among the most promi-
nent of these are vicarious, symbolic, and self-
regulatory processes.

Vicarious processes. Much human learning occurs


vicariously through observing modeled perfor-
mances (e.g., live, filmed symbolic; Bandura,
Fig. 10.1 Reciprocal interactions in social cognitive 1977b). The capability for learning vicariously
theory allows individuals to acquire beliefs, cognitions,
222 D.H. Schunk and C.A. Mullen

affects, skills, strategies, and behaviors from


observations of others in their social environ-
Self-Efficacy
ments and vicariously via media outlets, which
Self-efficacy is a key personal factor in social
saves time because learning is not demonstrated
cognitive theory, which postulates that achieve-
when it occurs. This capability also allows people
ment depends on interactions among behaviors,
to shape their lives because they proactively select
personal factors, and social/environmental condi-
environmental features (e.g., individuals, materi-
tions (Perry et al., 2007). Academic self-efficacy,
als) to which they want to attend. Thus, students
or the perceived confidence in one’s ability to
who want to become teachers enroll in education
execute actions for attaining academic goals,
programs and put themselves in situations where
plays a crucial role in adolescent motivation and
they can learn vicariously, such as by observing
learning. Self-efficacy is hypothesized to influ-
and working with classroom teachers.
ence behaviors and environments and be affected
by them (Bandura, 1986, 1997). Self-efficacy
Symbolic processes. Symbolic processes involve
affects choice of tasks, effort, persistence, and
language, mathematical and scientific notation,
achievement. Research in academic settings
iconography, and cognition. These processes help
shows that students who feel efficacious about
people adapt to and alter their environments
learning tend to be competent and engaged and
(Bandura, 1986). They use symbolic processes
are likely to set learning goals, use effective
when they formulate thoughts and take action
learning strategies, monitor comprehension,
and, perhaps unconsciously, to guide their actions.
evaluate goal progress, and create supportive
Cognitively in tune, students do not simply react
environments (Schunk & Pajares, 2005). In turn,
to events but rather resolve issues and generate
self-efficacy is influenced by the outcomes of
new courses of action. Symbolic processes also
behaviors (e.g., goal progress, achievement) and
foster verbal and written communications, which
by inputs from the environment (e.g., feedback
further promotes learning.
from teachers, comparisons with peers).
Individuals’ self-efficacy impacts motivation and
Self-regulatory processes. Social cognitive theory
learning, as well as decisions and events that
assigns a prominent role to self-regulation, or the
affect their lives (Schunk & Pajares, 2009).
processes individuals use to activate and sustain
their behaviors, cognitions, and affects that are
focused on attaining goals (Zimmerman, 2000).
People regulate their behaviors to conform to Sources of Information
their internal standards and goals. Before embark- About Self-Efficacy
ing on a task, individuals determine their goals
and what strategies to use, and they feel self-effi- Information for assessing one’s self-efficacy is
cacious about performing well. As they engage in acquired from actual performances, observations
tasks, they monitor their performances, assess of others (vicarious experiences), social persua-
their progress toward goals, and decide whether sion, and physiological indexes (Table 10.1;
their strategy needs adjusting. As tasks are com- Bandura, 1997). Because these are tangible indi-
pleted, they reflect on their experiences, make cators of individuals’ capabilities, one’s perfor-
modifications, and determine next steps. Believing mances constitute the most reliable information
they have learned and made progress strengthens (Schunk & Pajares, 2009). Interpretations of
their self-efficacy and motivates further learning. one’s performances as successful raise self-effi-
People who are continually engaged while learn- cacy whereas perceived failures may lower it,
ing are apt to be self-regulated (Schunk & Pajares, although an occasional failure or success should
2009; Zimmerman & Cleary, 2009). not have much impact.
10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner 223

Table 10.1 Self-efficacy sources and consequences affective reactions can lower self-efficacy
Sources of self-efficacy information (Zajacova et al., 2005). Conversely, when people
• Mastery experiences (actual performances) feel less stressful (e.g., anxiety subsides while
• Vicarious (modeled) experiences taking a test), they may experience higher self-
• Forms of social persuasion
• Physiological indexes efficacy for performing well.
Consequences of self-efficacy Sources of self-efficacy information do not
• Motivational outcomes (task choice, effort, persistence) automatically affect self-efficacy (Bandura,
• Learning 1997). Individuals interpret the results of events,
• Achievement and these interpretations generate information on
• Self-regulation
which judgments are based (Schunk & Pajares,
2009). Some ways that research has shown to
Interpretations of one’s performances are effectively build students’ self-efficacy are to
important, along with the performances them- have students set difficult but attainable goals
selves. Individuals engage in metacognitive and assess their own goal progress (mastery
mediation by thinking of areas of their learning experiences), allow students to observe models
such as planning and problem-solving. In a study similar to themselves learning skills (vicarious
of college undergraduates (mixed gender, no race experiences), and provide students with feedback
specified), Coutinho (2008) found that students’ that links their learning progress to their dili-
metacognition and self-efficacy influenced their gently applying a learning strategy (social per-
performances. suasion; Schunk, 1995).
Individuals acquire information about their Important as it is, self-efficacy is not the only
capabilities through social comparisons with oth- influence on behavior; no amount of it will pro-
ers (Bandura, 1997). Similarity to others is a cue duce a competent performance when requisite
for gauging one’s self-efficacy (Schunk, 1995). skills are lacking (Schunk & Pajares, 2009). Also
Observing others succeed can raise observers’ important are outcome expectations (beliefs
self-efficacy and motivate them to try the task at about the likely consequences of actions;
hand because they are apt to believe that if others Bandura, 1997) and values (perceptions of the
can achieve, they can as well. But a vicarious importance and utility of learning and acting in
increase in self-efficacy can be negated by subse- given ways; Wigfield, Tonks, & Eccles, 2004).
quent difficulties. Persons who observe peers fail Even students who feel efficacious about per-
may believe they lack competence, which can forming well in school may disengage from
dissuade them from attempting the task. learning if they do not value it or believe that
People also may assess self-efficacy when they negative outcomes may result, such as rejection
receive persuasive information from others (e.g., by peers. Assuming the activation of requisite
“I know you can do this”; Bandura, 1997); how- skills, positive values, and outcome expectations,
ever, such persuasion must be credible for people self-efficacy is a key determinant of individuals’
to believe that success is attainable. Although pos- motivation, learning, self-regulation, and achieve-
itive feedback can raise individuals’ self-efficacy, ment (Schunk & Pajares, 2009).
the effects will not endure if they subsequently
perform poorly (Schunk & Pajares, 2009).
Physiological and emotional reactions such as Consequences of Self-Efficacy
anxiety and stress also provide input about self-
efficacy (Bandura, 1997). Strong emotional reac- Self-efficacy has diverse effects on various moti-
tions can signal anticipated success or failure. vational outcomes associated with student
When people experience negative thoughts and engagement, including task choice, effort, and
fears about their capabilities (e.g., feeling ner- persistence (Bandura, 1997; Pajares, 1996;
vous when thinking about taking a test), those Schunk & Pajares, 2005, 2009; Table 10.1).
224 D.H. Schunk and C.A. Mullen

Individuals typically select tasks and activities at effort needed to succeed and evaluate progress.
which they feel competent. Self-efficacy can Goals also are distinguished by how far they
affect how much cognitive and physical effort project into the future. Because it is easier to
people expend on an activity, how long they per- determine progress toward goals that are closer at
sist when they encounter difficulties, and their hand, proximal (short-term) goals enhance self-
levels of learning and achievement. Students with efficacy and motivation better than do distant
high self-efficacy tend to set challenging goals, (long-term) goals (Bandura & Schunk, 1981).
work diligently, persist in the face of failure, and Goal difficulty, which refers to the level of task
recover their sense of self-efficacy after setbacks. proficiency required as assessed against a stan-
As a consequence, they develop higher levels dard, influences the effort people expend. In gen-
of competence. In contrast, those with low eral, learners work harder to attain more challenging
self-efficacy may set easier goals, expend mini- goals; however, perceived difficulty and motiva-
mal effort, disassociate as difficulties arise, and tion do not bear an unlimited positive relation to
feel dejected by failure, all of which negatively one another. Goals that students believe are overly
affect engagement and learning. trying can obstruct motivation because they hold
low self-efficacy for attaining them. Learners are
apt to feel self-efficacious for attaining goals that
Goals and Self-Evaluations of Progress they perceive as difficult but attainable.
A distinction can be drawn between learning
Social cognitive theory highlights the importance and performance goals. A learning goal refers to
of various symbolic processes for motivation. what knowledge, behavior, skill, or strategy stu-
Among the most critical are self-efficacy, goals, dents are to acquire, and a performance goal
and self-evaluations of goal progress, which work refers to what task is to be completed. These
together to enhance motivation and engagement goals can have differential effects on achieve-
in learning. ment behaviors (Anderman & Wolters, 2006).
Goals, or what people are consciously trying Learning goals motivate by focusing and sustain-
to attain, are symbolic processes that instigate ing attention on both processes and strategies that
and sustain actions. Because goals do not affect help students acquire competence and new skills.
behavior without commitment, learners must Self-efficacy is substantiated as they work on the
commit to attempting goals (Locke & Latham, task and assess their progress (Schunk, 1996).
2002). As learners work on a task, they compare In contrast, performance goals focus attention
their current performance with their specific on completing tasks. They may not highlight the
goals. Positive self-evaluations of progress value of the processes and strategies underlying
strengthen self-efficacy and sustain motivation. task completion or raise self-efficacy for learning.
A perceived discrepancy between present perfor- As they engage in tasks, students may not com-
mance and the goal may create dissatisfaction, pare their present and past performances to deter-
which can propel effort. Goals motivate learners mine progress. Performance goals can lead to
to expend the effort necessary and persist at the social comparisons with the work of others to
task (Locke & Latham, 2002), resulting in better determine progress. These comparisons can
performance and enhanced engagement. lower self-efficacy when students experience
Although goals are motivational catalysts, learning difficulties, which adversely affects
their effects depend on their properties: specificity, motivation and engagement in learning.
proximity, and difficulty. Goals that include spe- Research supports these hypothesized effects
cific performance standards are more likely to of learning and performance goals. Schunk and
activate self-evaluations of progress and enhance Ertmer (1999) conducted two studies with col-
self-efficacy and motivation than are general lege undergraduates as they worked on computer
goals (e.g., “Do your best”; Bandura, 1986). projects. Students received the goal of learning
Specific goals are a better indicator of the kind of computer applications or the goal of performing
10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner 225

them. In the first study, half of the students in learning and know how to keep themselves from
each goal condition evaluated their learning prog- becoming discouraged. For example, if they can-
ress midway through the instructional program. not answer the easier questions on a test, they
The learning goal led to higher self-efficacy, self- change their strategy by moving onto questions
judged progress, and self-regulatory competence they can answer and reassuring themselves that
and strategy use. The opportunity to self-evaluate they are making progress while internally check-
progress promoted self-efficacy. In the second ing their understanding.
study, self-evaluation students assessed their Self-efficacy comes into play at all points in
progress after each instructional session. Frequent engaged learning. Prior to starting on a task, stu-
self-evaluation produced comparable results dents hold a sense of self-efficacy for learning
when linked with a learning or performance goal. (Schunk, 1995). Their self-efficacy is substanti-
These results suggest that multiple self-evalua- ated as they work on tasks and observe the prog-
tions of learning progress can raise motivation ress being made toward their goal. Self-efficacy
and achievement outcomes. helps to keep students motivated and engaged in
learning activities. Students who feel efficacious
about learning but perceive that their progress is
Self-Efficacy and Student Engagement inadequate make adjustments to improve their
learning (e.g., changing strategy, seeking help,
Engaged Learning improving one’s environment). Such modifica-
tions help foster engagement in learning.
Student engagement in learning reflects cogni-
tive, behavioral, and affective variables that encom-
pass aspects of motivation and self-regulation Contextual Influences
(Schunk, 1995; Zimmerman, 2000). Among cog-
nitive variables, students engaged in learning As noted, self-efficacy is affected by contextual
have a sense of self-efficacy for learning. They factors such as familial, sociocultural, and educa-
hold positive outcome expectations and value the tional influences that are critical for engaged
learning. They set goals and evaluate their prog- learning.
ress, and they decide what they believe are effec-
tive strategies for learning the material and Familial influences. Families influence self-effi-
succeeding. They focus their attention on the task cacy in different ways, such as through their capi-
and strive to avoid distraction. tal. Capital includes resources and assets (Bradley
Students who are engaged also display pro- & Corwyn, 2002), primarily material resources
ductive achievement behaviors. They create work (e.g., income), human resources (e.g., education),
environments conducive to learning. Disadvan- and social resources (e.g., networks). Cultural
taged students must especially endeavor to over- capital refers to the wealthy norm reflected in an
come barriers where they lack necessary materials accumulation of specific types of knowledge,
and equipment. While engaged with tasks, stu- skills, and abilities that are acquired by families
dents expend effort and persist when they and valued in school settings (e.g., technological
encounter difficulties. If they become stuck, they resources such as computers in the home; Yosso,
seek help (e.g., teachers, parents, peers, manuals). 2005). Children are motivated to learn when the
Engaged learners self-monitor to ensure good use home has activities and materials that arouse their
of time. They may keep records of their accom- curiosity and offer challenges that can be met
plished tasks and what remains to be done. (Schunk & Pajares, 2009). Parents who are better
Affective variables include creating and main- educated and have social connections are apt to
taining a positive attitude toward learning. Engaged stress education and enroll their children in
learners value learning; by succeeding, they expe- school and extramural programs that foster their
rience a sense of pride. They are strategic about self-efficacy and learning.
226 D.H. Schunk and C.A. Mullen

Families that foster a responsive and support- positive visions of themselves over time and as
ive environment, encourage exploration and related to school, career, and life. Metacognitive
stimulate curiosity, and facilitate learning experi- processing of information and development are
ences accelerate their children’s intellectual fostered as longer-term goals are formed (e.g.,
development. Because mastery experiences con- “future time perspective”), and self-schemas
stitute a powerful source of self-efficacy infor- (e.g., “possible selves”) are imagined (Borkowski
mation, parents who arrange for their children to & Thorpe, 1994; Shell & Husman, 2001). Future
experience mastery in concert with their personal time perspective is not a self-schema per se, but
interests are apt to develop efficacious youngsters these two concepts share similarities. Notably,
(Schunk & Pajares, 2009). Activities conducive future time perspective is implicit in an individu-
to learning may include playing a musical instru- al’s capability for projecting possible selves into
ment or a sport in which children have the free- the near and distant future (Miller & Brickman,
dom to explore. In contrast, parents can negatively 2004; Shell & Husman, 2001; Simons, Vansteen-
affect their children’s academic competence and kiste, Lens, & Lacante, 2004).
achievement through such behaviors as providing For example, students who relate to their school
rewards extrinsic to academic tasks, making subjects in the context of what they want to become
unrealistic demands, avoiding conflict arising (e.g., lawyer, teacher) improve their mental com-
from learning expectations, and not valuing self- petence and engagement in learning goals and
directed learning (Borkowski & Thorpe, 1994). tasks (Shell & Husman, 2001). Based on their
Another means of influence is vicariously study involving almost 200 primarily White under-
through role models. Family members who model graduate students, Shell and Husman found that
ways to cope with difficulties, persistence, and students’ future time beliefs (i.e., relative impor-
effort strengthen their children’s self-efficacy. tance of attaining immediate versus long-term
Family members also provide persuasive informa- future outcomes) were associated with higher self-
tion. Parents who encourage their children to try efficacy, achievement, and study time and effort.
different activities as appropriate to their ages Youth and children from different sociocul-
facilitate their capability for welcoming challenges tural backgrounds must be guided to express
and meeting them (Schunk & Pajares, 2009). future-oriented conceptions of themselves (pos-
The plight of delayed adulthood affects self- sible selves) and of society (Borkowski & Thorpe,
efficacy as well. Western societies now have a 1994). The idea is that the present self imagines
longer transition to adulthood and thus a pro- the future, envisioning a future self to orient cur-
longed time for youth to finish school, become rent choices and behaviors. Notably, short- and
employed, and start families (Settersten & Ray, long-range goals are critical building blocks for
2010). Youth from impoverished backgrounds do the development of possible selves, which repre-
not meet these adult milestones at the same rate sent goals and opportunities for making execu-
as their more privileged peers. Modern families tive decisions about the future (Borkowski &
can experience undue stress where their youth Thorpe, 1994; Oyserman & James, 2009).
remain semidependent for different types of Teachers who have a future time perspective can
assistance. Youth from low-income families influence engagement and motivate students by
receive approximately 70% less material assis- explaining the “future importance of their present
tance than those in the top quarter of the income behavior” in fostering ideas of development,
distribution (Settersten & Ray, 2010). identity, and community (Simons et al., 2004,
p. 122). While student goal setting needs to be
Sociocultural influences. A major factor associ- clear and specific, future goals—and especially their
ated with self-efficacy and achievement is socio- anticipated benefits—also play a role in motiva-
economic status. Borkowski and Thorpe (1994) tion (Bandura, 1986). Optimal outcomes can be
reviewed empirical studies and found that increased where students understand that their
students from lower-income families tend to lack “current task engagement is instrumental to attain
10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner 227

a future goal” (Simons et al., p. 122). Intrinsic receptive to imagining their futures and project-
benefits (e.g., personal development) and extrin- ing themselves into colleges and jobs.
sic benefits (e.g., career satisfaction) can increase Peers constitute another sociocultural influ-
overall motivation by way of instructional inter- ence. With development, peers become important
ventions that change individuals’ limited attitudes influences on self-efficacy (Schunk & Meece,
toward their future and time. 2006). Parents who steer their children toward
Teachers engage their students by taking into efficacious peers provide opportunities for vicari-
account each individual’s capacity to think about ous increases in self-efficacy. When children
the future and by being attuned to their discovery observe their peers succeed, they are likely to
process. One direction for personal development experience higher self-efficacy and motivation.
involves integrating the meaning or instrumental Social influence also operates through peer
value of activities into one’s concept of self networks, or groups of friends and others with
(Husman & Lens, 1999). Importantly, as Miller whom students associate. Students who belong to
and Brickman (2004) attest, teachers exert socio- networks tend to be similar (Cairns, Cairns, &
cultural influence as role models when they help Neckerman, 1989), which enhances the likeli-
students understand what possibilities can be hood of influence by modeling. Networks help
acted upon in their environment and when they define students’ opportunities for interactions and
assist with problem-solving in such areas as lim- observations of others’ interactions, as well as
ited knowledge of one’s context and goal setting their access to activities. Over time, network
for achieving future goals. On the other hand, members tend to become even more similar, as in
teachers must be aware of students’ impressions the case of racially and psychologically identified
of or beliefs about negative teacher bias and/or members. Some researchers, such as Arroyo and
obstacles to learning. Teachers can exert a positive Zigler (1995) who studied African American and
influence by changing the classroom environ- White peer groups in urban high schools, have
ment, modifying their instructional or interper- found that the “racial identification” can “impact
sonal strategies, or addressing students’ individual academic achievement and affective states”
goals (Miller & Brickman, 2004). where members believe that others hold a nega-
Possible selves is a concept that places value tive perception of their group (p. 912). The
on unrealized but better selves and habits or ori- African American participants reported having
entations that learners wish to possess. Habits lessened their identification and engagement with
such as persistence, flexibility, and civic centering their racial group, concerned about jeopardizing
are high-level ideas that should be integrated in the approval of nonmembers.
the early stages of students’ education (Settersten Peer groups promote motivational socializa-
& Ray, 2010). Because a gap exists between the tion when perceived in reassuring ways. Changes
present self that dwells on what is and the possi- in children’s motivation across the school year are
ble self on what can be, individuals mentally predicted by their peer group membership
strain to see the future. In a 5-year study of the (Kindermann, McCollam, & Gibson, 1996).
motivational levels of Native Americans and Children affiliated with highly motivated groups
White Americans, McInerney, Hinkley, Dowson, change positively, whereas those in less motivated
and Van Etten (1998) found that middle school- groups change negatively. Steinberg, Brown, and
ers generally experienced difficulty in imagining Dornbusch (1996) tracked students throughout
the future (e.g., employability and other long- their high school years, finding that those with
term goals). Students may need to be encouraged similar grades but affiliated with academically
to connect their present and future goals by deter- oriented crowds achieved more than those affili-
mining an instrumental route to the future ated with less academically inclined peers. Peer
(McInerney, 2004). McInerney et al. (1998) group academic socialization can influence the
found that some of the middle schoolers, by the academic self-efficacy of individual members and
time they reached high school, became more their groups (Schunk & Pajares, 2009).
228 D.H. Schunk and C.A. Mullen

Another influence on academic self-efficacy is (1995) found that mathematics self-efficacy had a
perceived stress and anxiety. Stress has the poten- direct effect on performance and that it mediated
tial to depress students’ self-efficacy, especially the influence of mental ability on performance.
among disadvantaged college populations (e.g., Experimental research has shown that instruc-
nontraditional, immigrant, and minority; Zajacova tional and social practices that convey to students
et al., 2005) and urban high school students that they are making progress and becoming
(Gillock & Reyes, 1999). Although stress affects competent learners raise self-efficacy, motiva-
performance, self-efficacy has been shown to be tion, and achievement (Schunk & Pajares, 2009).
the stronger influence, as demonstrated by Pajares Some beneficial instructional and social practices
and Kranzler (1995) who found that mathe- are having students pursue proximal and specific
matics anxiety exerted a weaker influence than goals, using social models in instruction, provid-
self-efficacy on high school students’ mathemati- ing feedback indicating competence, having stu-
cal performances. Zajacova et al. assessed self- dents self-monitor and evaluate their learning
efficacy and the stress of freshmen immigrant progress, and teaching students to use metacogni-
and minority college students. While they found tive strategies while learning (Coutinho, 2008;
that social stress did not seem to have a negative Schunk & Ertmer, 2000). Other benefits on stu-
effect on the students’ GPA and credits, stress did dents’ self-efficacy occur from role models who
seem to have an effect, albeit marginal, on persis- provide encouragement of and high expectations
tence and enrollment. for achievement, a feeling of control over and
Researchers have emphasized the important empowerment within one’s environment, and
role of self-efficacy in alleviating the effect of rewards for doing well in school (Jonson-Reid,
stressors on perceived stress and academic suc- Davis, Saunders, Williams, & Williams, 2005;
cess (Pajares & Kranzler, 1995; Zajacova et al., Miller & Brickman, 2004).
2005). Minority and immigrant students experi- Research also shows that competence beliefs
ence “acculturative stress,” making them more such as self-efficacy, as well as academic motiva-
susceptible to social stress than native-born and tion, often decline as students advance through
White students (Zajacova et al., 2005). For such school (Eccles, Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998;
reasons, King (2005) argued that despite the Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield,
increasing diversity within their classrooms, 2002). However, a few studies caution that the
many African American and Hispanic students attitudinal and developmental patterns of young
feel disengaged and culturally segregated. adolescents defy tidy summarization (McInerney,
2004). This widely reported decline has been
Educational influences. Self-efficacy has been attributed to such factors as increased competi-
explored in various educational domains and tion, more norm-referenced grading, less teacher
among individuals differing in age, developmen- attention on individual student progress, and
tal level, and cultural background. Researchers stresses associated with school transitions
have established that self-efficacy influences (Schunk & Meece, 2006). These and other school
individuals’ motivation, achievement, and self- problems, including teacher bias and obstacles to
regulation (Bandura, 1997; Pajares, 1997; Schunk learning (Miller & Brickman, 2004), can nega-
& Pajares, 2009; Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998). tively affect the development of academic self-
Multon, Brown, and Lent (1991) found that self- efficacy, especially among those who are poorly
efficacy accounted for 14% of the variance in prepared to cope with academic challenges and
academic performance. Stajkovic and Luthans first-generation college students (i.e., those whose
(1998) determined that self-efficacy resulted in a parents are not college graduates; Majer, 2009).
28% gain in performance. Schunk (1981) obtained Rigid sequences of instruction frustrate some stu-
evidence that self-efficacy exerted a direct effect dents, and lower-ability groupings can weaken
on children’s achievement and persistence in the self-efficacy of members. Classrooms in
mathematics. Additionally, Pajares and Kranzler which students are allowed to socially compare
10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner 229

their work can have the unintended effect of low- Self-Efficacy and Underachievement
ering self-efficacy for those who judge them-
selves deficient. The role of self-efficacy in student underachieve-
Periods of transition in schooling also can ment and dropout is receiving much attention
affect self-efficacy (Schunk & Meece, 2006). (Alexander, Entwisle, & Kabbani, 2001; Hardre
Because elementary students remain with the & Reeve, 2003; Lee & Burkam, 2003; Rumberger
same teacher and peers for most of the day, teach- & Thomas, 2000). Factors contributing to under-
ers can better provide focused attention and feed- achievement and dropout are varied. These
back on their individual progress. In middle include poorly developed academic and social
school, though, children move among rooms for skills, little interest in school subjects, classrooms
subjects and are exposed to new peers. Learning that stress competition and ability social compari-
often is rote, evaluation becomes normative, and sons, low perceived value of school learning, little
teacher attention to individual progress lessens sense of belonging or relatedness to the school
(McInerney, 2004). The expanded social refer- environment, and no sense of purpose or vision of
ence group and the shift in evaluation standards the future (Alexander et al., 2001; McInerney,
require students to reassess their academic capa- 2004; Meece, Anderman, & Anderman, 2006;
bilities and regulate their learning, which can Wentzel, 2005). Students’ involvement and par-
lower self-efficacy for some. ticipation in school depend in part on how much
Educational influences on self-efficacy also the environment promotes their perceptions of
vary depending on the sociodemographics of the autonomy and relatedness, which in turn can
institution. Allen (1992) reviewed studies that influence self-efficacy and achievement (Hymel,
investigated educational advantage and disad- Comfort, Schonert-Reichl, & McDougall, 1996).
vantage as linked to type of institution and race. Parents, teachers, and peers affect students’ feel-
While historically Black colleges and universi- ings of autonomy and relatedness, and peer groups
ties (HBCUs) have fewer educational resources exert increasing influence during adolescence
than many predominately White institutions, the (Kindermann, 2007; Steinberg et al., 1996).
self-efficacy and competence of African American We have discussed how low self-efficacy can
students at HBCUs often is higher. For example, weaken motivation and lessen engagement in
they earn higher grades and are more academi- learning. But high self-efficacy does not automat-
cally socialized, better psychologically adjusted, ically translate into strong motivation and deep
and more culturally aware than their counterparts engagement. Students who feel efficacious about
at White institutions. On White college cam- learning but disconnected from the school envi-
puses, African American males may display ronment or mainstream society may be unmoti-
lower academic motivation, in contrast with vated and disengaged. Families supporting youth
African American females, whereas at HBCUs, who have low motivation to succeed and who are
African American males exhibit less anxiety disengaged from school, other educational insti-
about their peer networks and role. The African tutions, and military and service programs are
American females’ experience on White cam- particularly burdened. Families with low incomes
puses is thought to be mixed, though, with accel- and educational levels would benefit from new
eration in their assertiveness and competence due kinds of institutions that can help fulfill this nec-
to a decrease in the need to cultivate relationships essary role of provider and motivator, as well as
with same-race males, on the one hand, and feeling civic pathway to lifelong success (Gibbons &
socially isolated and even ostracized, on the other. Shoffner, 2004; Settersten & Ray, 2010).
Hence, educational institutions can play a signifi- Socially, structurally, and historically, students
cant role in the acculturative stress and adapta- who have been socialized through caste systems
tion of culturally ethnic and disadvantaged (i.e., segregated schools and neighborhoods)
students. have had to overcome multiple challenges to
230 D.H. Schunk and C.A. Mullen

nourish developing belief systems that support states and 15 countries prepares students, includ-
achievement and self-efficacy (Cuban, 2010). ing first-generation populations, for 4-year col-
Disadvantaged students’ academic self-efficacy leges (Chapel Hill–Carrboro City Schools, 2009).
and engagement are “deeply entangled in histo- Strategies that the AVID program uses include
ries of segregation, desegregation, and resegrega- developing analytic thinking, improving organi-
tion” (Cuban, 2010, p. 204), and the negative zational skills, providing tutoring support, and
consequences of school desegregation on Black exposing students to higher education institutions,
families have been documented (Horsford, 2010). all of which have the potential of raising
Cuban’s analysis of school district reforms and self-efficacy and motivation. We suspect that these
leadership is associated with failed initiatives programs might benefit from thorough evaluation
across the USA. Given the discriminatory forces of their effect on participants’ self-efficacy.
at work in socially stratified hierarchical systems, Studies of community college students indicate
lower socioeconomic status and personal cogni- that success interventions are necessary for facili-
tive deficits, then, are only part of a multifaceted tating the academic self-efficacy of diverse first-
problem that drives underachievement and generation students (Majer, 2009).
engagement. Other researchers also have viewed Despite the importance of such societal inter-
the poor academic performance of students of ventions, their degree of effectiveness has yet to
color, particularly African Americans and be established. Some postdropout programs
Hispanics, as perpetuated by systems of inequity select the most motivated and competent indi-
and other social ills that make academic efforts viduals, making high-risk dropouts especially
seem futile and penalizing (Horsford, 2010). To difficult to engage in any organized way (Bloom,
succeed academically and vocationally in main- 2010). While the long-term effects of such pro-
stream communities, disadvantaged students have grams are unknown, consolidated efforts across
had to minimize their associations with same- communities and the USA are needed. Such pro-
race peers, unlike privileged White students grams would gain from becoming more inclu-
(Arroyo & Zigler, 1995; Cuban, 2010). sive, cohesive, and intensive enough to engage
Intervention as seen in the forms of social youth over a long period.
policies and second-chance programs have been
in effect for years; however, many of these are
restrictive in scope and problem-based, not devel- Future Research Directions
opmental (Bloom, 2010). They often have not
assessed students’ self-efficacy. These programs There is much evidence that self-efficacy relates
should also focus on ethnic identity issues and to achievement outcomes including motivation
prevention orientation at the high school level or and engagement. Students who hold a sense of
earlier to not only be more effective but also have self-efficacy for learning and performing well are
a lasting effect (Bloom, 2010). Engagement strat- apt to be engaged, competent learners.
egies for assisting high-risk dropout populations But our discussion also raises many issues.
(e.g., immigrants, disabled, young mothers, fos- We recommend more research, especially on
ter care youth, and youth offenders) include iden- contextual factors and influences, students from
tity development, paid work, internships, job different cultures, and high-performing schools.
training, community service, and life skills.
Some of these components appear to be
evidenced in YouthBuild and Service and Contextual Influences on Self-Efficacy
Conservation Corps, and other programs. The
Challenge and City Year programs engage partici- Self-efficacy—a personal factor—can affect and
pants in residential building projects and team- be influenced by contextual factors. Enhancing
based civic work. For high school and middle students’ self-efficacy, motivation, and engage-
school students, the Advancement Via Individual ment requires that we understand how contextual
Determination (AVID) program found in 45 US variables operate.
10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner 231

We have noted that school transitions (e.g., Cross-Cultural Research


middle school to high school) bring about many
changes in learning contexts. Research is needed More needs to be known about students from dif-
that explores which contextual factors affect self- ferent cultures and countries. Most self-efficacy
efficacy and how students combine the influences studies have focused on students from the USA
of these new contexts with their prior experiences without sufficient attention on issues of diversity,
to arrive at self-efficacy judgments. New practi- especially as related to learning and engagement.
cal knowledge can inform the design of effective Cross-cultural studies will expand understanding
learning environments at school, home, and of the operation and generality of self-efficacy.
elsewhere. Klassen’s (2004b) review of 20 cross-cultural
Social factors are crucial. Students who lack a studies found that although self-efficacy was
sense of belonging within their school environ- lower for non-Western students (e.g., Asian and
ment are at risk for underachievement and dropout. Asian-immigrant students) than for Western stu-
Research on factors that affect students’ sense of dents (e.g., Western Europe, Canada, USA), the
belonging will suggest ways to improve their self- more modest self-efficacy expressed by non-
efficacy and engagement in learning. For example, Western students predicted academic outcomes
one self-efficacy-enhancing strategy involves acti- better than the higher self-efficacy of Western
vating possible selves by envisioning one’s future students. Klassen posited that immigration status
and understanding the links between present and and political factors can modify the mean self-
later goals (Borkowski & Thorpe, 1994; Jonson- efficacy of a cultural group.
Reid et al., 2005). Thus, high school students who Research that focuses on culturally ethnic
want to become medical doctors might picture students’ experiences at different types of institu-
themselves using science and mathematics in their tions is also needed, especially when unemploy-
work as doctors, which underscores the impor- ment and underemployment are on the increase
tance of their studying in their current courses. We (Allen, 1992). Hand in hand with this focus is
encourage probing of academic self-efficacy that of social policies and programs that can
among African American students and other non- address in a more specific way not only the lower
White student populations. Research can investi- achievement and higher attrition for African
gate their self-conceptions and possible selves, American college students but also what types of
perceived influences on their self-images and interventions and resources foster ethnic stu-
learning, and experiences of academic identifica- dents’ self-efficacy and success (Allen, 1992).
tion and disassociation (Kerpelman, Eryigit, & As Jonson-Reid et al. (2005) attest, given that
Stephens, 2008). research on self-efficacy has mostly focused on
Political factors are yet another important White students at predominately White institu-
contextual variable. For example, school districts tions, we need a better understanding of African
have been urged to systematically analyze the American youths’ sense of self-efficacy, in addi-
effects of policies aimed at increasing student tion to strategies that foster a belief in the value
achievement (Cuban, 2010). Studies of changes of education.
in test scores by both racial and socioeconomic Cultural dimensions such as individualism
status need to follow from district-level policy and collectivism may influence the relation of
implementation, with a focus on students’ self- self-efficacy to academic outcomes (Oettingen &
efficacy resulting from standardized testing. As Zosuls, 2006). Kim and Park (2006) argued that
another example, districts will need to anticipate theories that emphasize individualistic values—
the effect on student self-efficacy of new assign- such as self-efficacy—cannot explain the high
ment plans that enforce attendance zones closer achievement of East Asian students. Instead, the
to students’ homes. Critics argue that such initia- Confucian-based socialization practices that pro-
tives undermine achievement by resegregating mote close parent–child relationships seem
schools and confining ethnic students to their responsible for high levels of self-regulatory,
own neighborhoods (Cuban, 2010). relational, and social efficacy. In these cultures,
232 D.H. Schunk and C.A. Mullen

relational efficacy (i.e., perceived competence in in such environments could benefit from research
family and social relations), as well as social sup- that is attuned to this practical focus.
port from parents, may influence students’ aca-
demic performances. Self-efficacy may be more
other-oriented in some non-Western (particularly Conclusion
Asian) cultures than in Western cultures (Klassen,
2004a). In short, cross-cultural research has Social cognitive theory stresses learning from the
implications for educational practices, especially social environment. The conceptual focus of
given the influx of immigrants in US schools. Bandura’s theory postulates reciprocal interac-
tions among personal, behavioral, and social/
environmental factors. Self-efficacy is a critical
Self-Efficacy in High-Performing personal factor that can affect motivation, engage-
Schools ment, learning, and achievement. Self-efficacy is
shaped by personal, cultural, and social factors,
High-performing schools create a positive envi- making learning and achievement complex socio-
ronment for learning and support teachers and cultural phenomena.
students so that learning can occur. The literature Attention to ways of building students’ skills
on high-performing schools focuses on their and self-efficacy will help more learners become
effects on student achievement and teacher satis- academically motivated and engaged in learning.
faction (Muncey & McQuillan, 1993; Sizer, These outcomes should help to diminish the
1992). We recommend that self-efficacy research- pervasive problem of student underachievement
ers devote attention to the features of high-per- and dropout. Important questions remain to be
forming schools that contribute to students’ and addressed by researchers and school leaders,
teachers’ self-efficacy. which will refine theory, expand practical knowl-
Some characteristics of high-performing edge, and help prepare better-educated citizens.
schools that should have positive effects on self- Finally, we urge legislators to advocate more
efficacy are parental involvement, supportive strongly for interventions that promote student
learning environments, and smooth transitions success, with the goals of alleviating the nation’s
between grades and levels (Maehr & Midgley, dropout problem and increasing educational
1996; Muncey & McQuillan, 1993; Sizer, 1992). opportunities for all youth.
Research directions include examining the influ-
ence of these and other factors to determine how
they create and build self-efficacy for learners. References
Another area deserving attention is the self-
efficacy of low-income students who have transi- Alexander, K., Entwisle, D., & Kabbani, N. (2001). The
tioned to better schools and are being socialized dropout process in life course perspective: Early risk
in new surroundings within school districts that factors at home and school. Teachers College Record,
103, 760–822.
favor economic integration. Kahlenberg (2004)
Allen, W. R. (1992). The color of success: African-
contended that lower-income students who attend American college student outcomes at predominately
middle-class and high-performing schools can White and historically Black public colleges and uni-
feel out of place because their peers have clearer versities. Harvard Educational Review, 62(1), 26–44.
Anderman, E. M., & Wolters, C. A. (2006). Goals, values,
goals for their learning and are better prepared
and affects: Influences on student motivation. In P. A.
and more academically engaged. The culture of Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educa-
the school is unfamiliar to the lower-income pop- tional psychology (2nd ed., pp. 369–389). Mahwah,
ulation in other respects as well, in that parents NJ: Erlbaum.
Arroyo, C. G., & Zigler, E. (1995). Racial identity, aca-
are likely more active in the school’s programs,
demic achievement, and the psychological well-being
and teachers generally are better qualified. Ways of economically disadvantaged adolescents. Journal of
to raise the self-efficacy of low-income students Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 903–914.
10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner 233

Bandura, A. (1977a). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying Hardre, P., & Reeve, J. (2003). A motivational model of
theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, rural students’ intentions to persist in, versus drop out
84, 191–215. of, high school. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Bandura, A. (1977b). Social learning theory. Englewood 95, 347–356.
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Horsford, S. D. (2010). Black superintendents on educat-
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and ing Black students in separate and unequal contexts.
action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, Urban Review, 42(1), 58–79.
NJ: Prentice Hall. Husman, J., & Lens, W. (1999). The role of the future in
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. student motivation. Educational Psychologist, 34(2),
New York: Freeman. 113–125.
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic Hymel, S., Comfort, C., Schonert-Reichl, K., &
perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–26. McDougall, P. (1996). Academic failure and school
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating compe- dropout: The influence of peers. In J. Juvonen & K. R.
tence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through Wentzel (Eds.), Social motivation: Understanding
proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality and children’s school adjustment (pp. 313–345).
Social Psychology, 41, 586–598. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Bloom, D. (2010). Programs and policies to assist high Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., &
school dropouts in the transition to adulthood. The Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-com-
Future of Children, 20(1), 89–108. petence and values: Gender and domain differences
Bloom, D., & Haskins, R. (2010). Helping high school across grades one to twelve. Child Development, 73,
dropouts improve their prospects [policy brief]. The 509–527.
Future of Children, 20(1), 1–8. Retrieved April 19, Jonson-Reid, M., Davis, L., Saunders, J., Williams, T., &
2010, from www.futureofchildren.org Williams, J. H. (2005). Academic self-efficacy among
Borkowski, J. G., & Thorpe, P. K. (1994). Self-regulation African American youths: Implications for school social
and motivation: A life-span perspective on under- work practice. Children and Schools, 27(1), 5–14.
achievement. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman Kahlenberg, R. D. (2004). America’s untapped resource:
(Eds.), Self regulation of learning and performance: Low-income students in higher education. Washington,
Issues and educational applications (pp. 45–73). DC: Century Foundation Press.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kerpelman, J. L., Eryigit, S., & Stephens, C. J. (2008).
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic African American adolescents’ future education orien-
status and child development. Annual Review of tation: Associations with self-efficacy, ethnic identity,
Psychology, 53, 371–399. and perceived parental support. Journal of Youth and
Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., & Neckerman, J. J. (1989). Adolescence, 37, 997–1008.
Early school dropout: Configurations and determi- Kim, U., & Park, Y. S. (2006). Factors influencing aca-
nants. Child Development, 60, 1437–1452. demic achievement in collectivist societies: The role
Chapel Hill–Carrboro City Schools. (2009, July 10). of self-, relational, and social efficacy. In F. Pajares &
AVID: Decade of college dreams. Retrieved April 20, T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents
2010, from http://www2.chccs.k12.nc.us/education/ (pp. 267–286). Greenwich, CT: Information Age
components/scrapbook/default.php?sectiondetailid= Publishing.
73208& Kindermann, T. A. (2007). Effects of naturally existing
Coutinho, S. (2008). Self-efficacy, metacognition, and peer groups on changes in academic engagement in a
performance. North American Journal of Psychology, cohort of sixth graders. Child Development, 78,
10(1), 165–172. 1186–1203.
Cuban, L. (2010). As good as it gets: What school reform Kindermann, T. A., McCollam, T. L., & Gibson, E., Jr.
brought to Austin. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University (1996). Peer networks and students’ classroom engage-
Press. ment during childhood and adolescence. In J. Juvonen &
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). K. R. Wentzel (Eds.), Social motivation: Understanding
Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & children’s school adjustment (pp. 279–312). Cambridge,
N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychol- England: Cambridge University Press.
ogy: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel- King, J. E. (2005). Black education: A transformative
opment (5th ed., pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley. research and action agenda for the new century.
Gibbons, M. M., & Shoffner, M. F. (2004). Prospective New York: Routledge.
first-generation college students: Meeting their needs Klassen, R. M. (2004a). A cross-cultural investigation of
through social cognitive career theory. Professional the efficacy beliefs of south Asian immigrant and
School Counseling, 8(1), 91–97. Anglo Canadian nonimmigrant early adolescents.
Gillock, K. L., & Reyes, O. (1999). Stress, support, and Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 731–742.
academic performance of urban, low-income, Klassen, R. M. (2004b). Optimism and realism: A review
Mexican–American adolescents. Journal of Youth and of self-efficacy from a cross-cultural perspective.
Adolescence, 28(2), 259–282. International Journal of Psychology, 39, 205–230.
234 D.H. Schunk and C.A. Mullen

Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (2003). Dropping out of high Pajares, F., & Kranzler, J. (1995). Self-efficacy beliefs and
school: The role of school organization and structure. general mental ability in mathematical problem-solv-
American Educational Research Journal, 40, 353–393. ing. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20(4),
Licht, B. G., & Kistner, J. A. (1986). Motivational prob- 426–443.
lems of learning-disabled children: Individual differ- Perry, J. C., DeWine, D. B., Duffy, R. D., & Vance, K. S.
ences and their implications for treatment. In J. K. (2007). The academic self-efficacy of urban youth:
Torgesen & B. W. L. Wong (Eds.), Psychological and A mixed-methods study of a school-to-work program.
educational perspectives on learning disabilities Journal of Career Development, 34, 103–126.
(pp. 225–255). Orlando, FL: Academic. Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practi- on the role of student motivation in learning and teach-
cally useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: ing contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,
A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 667–686.
705–717. PR Newswire. (2009). Panel discussion on crisis of high
Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school school dropouts and cost to our economy. Retrieved
cultures. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. April 18, 2010, from http://www.forbes.com/feeds/
Majer, J. M. (2009). Self-efficacy and academic success prnewswire/2010/02/24/prnewswire201002241350PR_
among ethnically diverse first-generation community NEWS_USPR_____DC60266.html
college students. Journal of Diversity in Higher Rumberger, R. W., & Thomas, S. L. (2000). The distribu-
Education, 2(4), 243–250. tion of dropout and turnover rates among urban and
McInerney, D. M. (2004). A discussion of future time per- suburban high schools. Sociology of Education, 73(1),
spective. Educational Psychology Review, 16(2), 39–67.
141–151. Schunk, D. H. (1981). Modeling and attributional effects
McInerney, D. M., Hinkley, J., Dowson, M., & Van Etten, on children’s achievement: A self-efficacy analysis.
S. (1998). Aboriginal, Anglo, and immigrant Australian Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 93–105.
students’ motivational beliefs about personal academic Schunk, D. H. (1995). Self-efficacy and education and
success: Are there cultural differences? Journal of instruction. In J. E. Maddux (Ed.), Self-efficacy, adap-
Educational Psychology, 90, 621–629. tation, and adjustment: Theory, research, and applica-
Meece, J. L., Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. tion (pp. 281–303). New York: Plenum Press.
(2006). Classroom goal structure, student motivation, Schunk, D. H. (1996). Goal and self-evaluative influences
and academic achievement. Annual Review of during children’s cognitive skill learning. American
Psychology, 57, 487–503. Educational Research Journal, 33, 359–382.
Miller, R. B., & Brickman, S. J. (2004). A model of future- Schunk, D. H., & Ertmer, P. A. (1999). Self-regulatory
oriented motivation and self-regulation. Educational processes during computer skill acquisition: Goal and
Psychology Review, 16(1), 9–33. self-evaluative influences. Journal of Educational
Mullen, C. A., & Schunk, D. H. (2011). The role of pro- Psychology, 91, 251–260.
fessional learning community in dropout prevention. Schunk, D. H., & Ertmer, P. A. (2000). Self-regulation
AASA Journal of Scholarship and Practice, 8, 26–29. and academic learning: Self-efficacy enhancing inter-
Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). ventions. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner
Relation of self-efficacy beliefs to academic outcomes: (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 631–649).
A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling San Diego, CA: Academic.
Psychology, 38, 30–38. Schunk, D. H., & Meece, J. L. (2006). Self-efficacy devel-
Muncey, D., & McQuillan, P. (1993). Preliminary findings opment in adolescence. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan
from a five-year study of the Coalition of Essential (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 71–96).
Schools. Phi Delta Kappan, 74, 486–489. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Oettingen, G., & Zosuls, C. (2006). Self-efficacy of ado- Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2005). Competence percep-
lescents across culture. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), tions and academic functioning. In A. J. Elliot & C. S.
Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 245–266). Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motiva-
Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. tion (pp. 85–104). New York: Guilford Press.
Oyserman, D., & James, L. (2009). Possible selves: From Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2009). Self-efficacy theory.
content to process. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of
& J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and motivation at school (pp. 35–53). New York:
mental simulation (pp. 373–394). New York: Routledge.
Psychology Press. Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2008).
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in achievement set- Motivation in education: Theory, research, and appli-
tings. Review of Educational Research, 66, 543–578. cations (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson
Pajares, F. (1997). Current directions in self-efficacy Education.
research. In M. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Settersten, R. A., Jr., & Ray, B. (2010). What’s going on
Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, with young people today? The long and twisting path
pp. 1–49). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. to adulthood. The Future of Children, 20(1), 19–41.
10 Self-Efficacy as an Engaged Learner 235

Shell, D. F., & Husman, J. (2001). The multivariate dimen- Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy
sionality of personal control and future time perspec- in school: Critical review of the literature and future
tive beliefs in achievement and self-regulation. directions. Review of Educational Research, 78,
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 481–506. 751–796.
Simons, J., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Lacante, M. Vick, R. M., & Packard, B. W.-L. (2008). Academic suc-
(2004). Placing motivation and future time perspec- cess strategy use among community-active urban
tive theory in a temporal perspective. Educational Hispanic adolescents. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral
Psychology Review, 16(2), 121–139. Sciences, 30(4), 463–480.
Sizer, T. (1992). Horace’s compromise: Redesigning the Wentzel, K. R. (2005). Peer relationships, motivation, and
American high school. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. academic performance at school. In A. J. Elliot & C. S.
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motiva-
Wellborn, J. G. (2009). Engagement and disaffection as tion (pp. 279–296). New York: Guilford Press.
organizational constructs in the dynamics of motiva- Wigfield, A., Tonks, S., & Eccles, J. S. (2004). Expectancy
tional development. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield value theory in cross-cultural perspective. In D. M.
(Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 223–245). McInerney & S. Van Etten (Eds.), Big theories revis-
New York: Routledge. ited (pp. 165–198). Greenwich, CT: Information Age
Smedley, B. D., Myers, H. F., & Harrell, S. P. (1993). Publishing.
Minority-status stresses and the college adjustment of Yosso, T. J. (2005). Whose culture has capital? A critical
ethnic minority freshmen. Journal of Higher race theory discussion of community cultural wealth.
Education, 64(4), 434–452. Race, Ethnicity, and Education, 8(1), 69–91.
Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and Zajacova, A., Lynch, S. M., & Espenshade, T. J. (2005).
work-related performances: A meta-analysis. Self-efficacy, stress, and academic success in college.
Psychological Bulletin, 124, 240–261. Research in Higher Education, 46(6), 677–706.
Steinberg, L., Brown, B. B., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1996). Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation:
Beyond the classroom: Why school reform has failed A social cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R.
and what parents need to do. New York: Simon & Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regula-
Schuster. tion (pp. 13–39). San Diego, CA: Academic.
U.S. Department of Education. (2008). Dropout preven- Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. J. (2009). Motives to self-
tion (pp. 1–66). [Institute of Education Sciences]. regulate learning: A social cognitive account. In K. R.
Retrieved March 14, 2010, from http://ies.ed.gov/ Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motiva-
ncee/wwc/pdf/practiceguides/dp_pg_090308.pdf tion at school (pp. 247–264). New York: Routledge.
A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account
of Student Engagement: Theoretical 11
Foundations and Applications

Timothy J. Cleary and Barry J. Zimmerman

Abstract
Educators have long been interested in understanding the variables or
factors underlying student motivation and desire to engage in and regulate
their academic behaviors. In this chapter, we delineate a social-cognitive
theoretical framework of self-regulatory engagement that integrates a set
of highly related yet distinctive constructs such as motivation, engage-
ment, and metacognition. Central to our self-regulation framework is a
cyclical feedback loop, a process that operates in a temporal sequence
(before, during, and after a learning activity) and is largely cognitive in
nature. We also draw a distinction between the “will” of students to engage
in learning and the “skill” with which they regulate or self-manage their
level of engagement. The historical evolution and the conceptual and
empirical advantages of cyclical feedback loops will be emphasized along
with a description of various academic intervention programs designed to
teach “cyclical” thinking and strategic behaviors to academically at-risk
students. Finally, an innovative alternative assessment approach, called
self-regulated learning microanalysis, is presented to illustrate how
researchers and practitioners can reliably and accurately capture students’
regulatory engagement in particular contexts and settings.

T.J. Cleary (*) Introduction


Department of Educational Psychology,
University of Wisconsin – Milwaukee,
Milwaukee, WI 53211, USA Educators have long been interested in under-
e-mail: tcleary@uwm.edu standing the factors underlying student motiva-
B.J. Zimmerman tion or desire to engage and regulate their
Learning, Development, and Instruction Area academic behaviors and functioning. In fact,
at the Graduate School, University Center recent survey research shows that school psychol-
of the City University of New York,
ogists and regular and special education teachers
New York, NY 10016-4309, USA
e-mail: bzimmerman@gc.cuny.edu report student motivation, self-regulatory skills,

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 237


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_11, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
238 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

and self-determination to be critical professional perceptions of connectedness and belongingness


development areas of interest (Agran, Snow, & to school (i.e., affective or emotional engage-
Swaner, 1999; Cleary, 2009; Cleary & ment). Christenson’s engagement model also
Zimmerman, 2006; Coalition for Psychology in posits that these observable and covert forms of
Schools and Education, 2006). This line of engagement are best understood within the social
research further indicates that these areas are context or milieu in which they occur. Thus, the
important to school-based professionals because extent to which students engage in learning activ-
of the relatively high frequency with which they ities may vary depending on the classroom envi-
encounter youth exhibiting these deficits as well ronments or school contexts in which they learn
as their lack of formal training and clinical – a key similarity with many self-regulation mod-
skills to work effectively with disengaged or els (Boekaerts, Pintrich, & Zeidner, 2000).
poorly regulated youth (Cleary, 2009; Cleary, Although engagement is a broad multidimen-
Gubi, & Prescott, 2010a; Wehmeyer, Agran, & sional construct, we will devote particular atten-
Hughes, 2000). Although different lines of tion to cognitive engagement, that is, the process
research separately target constructs such as through which students become cognitively and
self-regulation, engagement, and motivation, it strategically invested in learning. As with most
is important to note that these types of con- definitions of engagement reported in the litera-
structs are highly related and complementary. ture, cognitive engagement is a loosely defined
As a result, presenting a theoretical framework construct encapsulating students’ use of cogni-
integrating all of these terms can be quite valu- tive strategies and regulatory processes and or
able and informative to both researchers and the extent to which their perceptions of interest
practitioners. Accordingly, a primary goal of and value stimulate their attention and immersion
this manuscript is to delineate a theoretical into the learning process (Bandura, 1986; Corno
framework that integrates motivation, engage- & Mandinach, 1983; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002;
ment, and self-regulation to describe the pro- Pintrich & de Groot, 1990). Although these gen-
cess through which students initiate and sustain eral depictions of cognitive engagement provide
a high level of investment in school-based learn- clear examples of what a cognitively engaged
ing activities. student might look like, they do not comprehen-
Historically, engaged students have been sively describe the dynamic process through
defined as those who “concentrate on their work, which students initially become engaged in learn-
are enthusiastic about it, and are deeply interested ing and sustain this level of investment over time.
in academic content” (Pressley & McCormick, From our perspective, self-regulation theories
1995, p. 328). More recently, researchers have can address this conceptual gap because such
conceptualized engagement as a multidimen- models delineate a clearly defined “process”
sional concept that extends across various behav- account of student learning, emphasizing the role
ioral, academic, cognitive, and emotional or of cognitive and metacognitive subprocesses as
psychological domains (Christenson et al., 2008; well as the related motivation beliefs impacting
Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004). these processes (Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006;
Christenson and colleagues have argued that Zimmerman & Labuhn, 2011).
these broadly defined engagement categories can
be further understood as either an observable
indicator, such as academic (e.g., task comple- Self-Regulation, Cognitive Engagement,
tion, work productivity) and behavioral engage- and Motivation
ment (e.g., class participation), or as an internal
process, such as students’ self-reflection and Self-regulation researchers have long been inter-
evaluation of the effectiveness of a learning ested in studying the mechanisms through which
strategy (i.e., cognitive engagement) or their individuals engage in strategic thinking or the
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 239

manner in which they cognitively manage their engagement of a student attempting to solve
learning (Bandura, 1986; Boekaerts et al., 2000; algebraic expression problems, researchers
Carver & Scheier, 2000; Winne & Perry, 2000). would seek to examine the extent to which this
The self-regulation literature is replete with theo- student plans or approaches these types of prob-
retical frameworks explaining cognitive engage- lems (before), directs attention to use and moni-
ment, although most models differ in terms of tor specific strategies while solving math problems
essential processes, sources of motivation, and (during), and uses internal or external feedback to
impact of social environment (Mace, Belfiore, & reflect on the effectiveness of their strategies
Hutchinson, 2001; Puustinen & Pulkkinen, 2001; (after). Although the phases of this sequential
Weinstein, Husman, & Dierking, 2000; Winne & feedback loop have been labeled in different
Hadwin, 1998; Zimmerman & Schunk, 2001). ways, such as preaction, action, and postaction
For example, operant theories typically empha- (Schmitz & Wiese, 2006) or forethought, perfor-
size reinforcement as a key motivational influence mance control, and self-reflection (Zimmerman,
on regulatory behaviors, whereas phenomeno- 2000), they all delineate a strategic process of
logical perspectives highlight self-actualization thinking that guides planning, performance, and
as the major driving force (Mace et al., 2001; evaluation to optimize academic success.
McCombs, 2001). Furthermore, information In this manuscript, we seek to accomplish
processing theorists pay particular attention to several objectives. First, we highlight the primary
how individuals process, store, and transfer infor- features of a social-cognitive account of SRL
mation through the use of cognitive strategies engagement, emphasizing a cyclical feedback
and tactics (Winne & Hadwin), whereas social- loop as the key foundational component.
cognitive theoretical models tend to highlight Although self-regulation clearly involves the
individuals’ self-motivational beliefs, such as integration of cognition, affect, behavior, and
self-efficacy, task interest and value, and goal ori- environmental factors, our model of engagement
entation, as well as the types of self-evaluations, will be discussed primarily relative to the cogni-
attributions, and self-reactions students make fol- tive and metacognitive dimensions. Accordingly,
lowing learning (Bandura, 1997; Pintrich, 2000; we use the terms SRL engagement and cognitive
Schunk, 2001). engagement interchangeably throughout the
Despite these differences, several researchers manuscript.
have noted the conceptual overlap among these Our second objective involves highlighting
models. In fact, Puustinen and Pulkkinen (2001) the connection between student motivation and
noted, “In sum, the differences in the definitions self-regulation. In our model, we include motiva-
become blurred and when one examines the mod- tion and self-regulation as a set of interrelated
els in more detail, suggesting that it is the relative processes operating within a single framework.
weight given to the component parts, more than Social cognitive researchers have described
the components themselves, that varies from one motivation as a process in which goal-directed
model to another” (p. 280). Of particular impor- behavior is initiated and sustained (Schunk,
tance to our attempt to link the constructs of cog- Pintrich, & Meece, 2008). Motivation has often
nitive engagement and self-regulated learning been operationally defined in behavioral terms,
(SRL) is that most theorists describe self-regula- such as effort, persistence, and choice of activi-
tion in terms of a cyclical feedback loop that is ties (Multon, Brown, & Lent, 1991; Schunk,
largely cognitive in nature (Schmitz & Wiese, 1981; Zimmerman, 1995). In our account, these
2006; Schunk, 2001; Winne, 2001; Zimmerman, motivated behaviors are distinguished from the
2000). These loops tend to function in a temporal motivation beliefs, such as self-efficacy, task
sequence (i.e., before, during, and after dimen- interest, and goal orientation, which influence
sions) as students engage in specific academic such behaviors. Thus, students’ beliefs of per-
tasks. For example, to understand the SRL sonal capabilities and their interest in learning do
240 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

not only influence one’s behavioral engagement Consistent with recent models of engagement,
or investment in learning but also the extent to social-cognitive theorists emphasize situation
which one engages cognitively in the cyclical dependence or context specificity (Bandura,
feedback loop of self-regulation (Zimmerman & 1997; Christenson et al., 2008; Urdan & Midgley,
Cleary, 2006). In essence, our model of self- 2003). Thus, the extent to which students become
regulated learning engagement distinguishes cognitively or strategically immersed in learning
between the “will” of student to engage in learn- activities will vary across academic settings and
ing (as highlighted by self-motivation beliefs) situational demands. This premise has been estab-
and the “skill” with which one regulates or self- lished empirically (Cleary & Chen, 2009; Hadwin,
manages his or her level of engagement (as Winne, Stockley, Nesbit, & Woszczyna, 2001;
described by the cyclical loop). Urdan & Midgley, 2003; Winne & Jaimeson-
Third, we provide some illustrative examples Noel, 2002). For example, Hadwin et al. (2001)
of school-based academic interventions that found that the types of strategies that students
directly teach students this strategic cycle of employ during learning varied across three types
thought and action as they perform different aca- of academic tasks – reading for learning, com-
demic activities, such as writing, performing pleting a brief essay, and studying for an exam.
math, or studying. Fourth, we detail an alterna- Furthermore, Cleary and Chen found that the
tive assessment methodology called SRL micro- relationship between student motivation and self-
analysis, specifically designed to capture the regulation with math achievement was stronger in
dynamic, context-specific dimension of cognitive more demanding and academically challenging
engagement. Finally, we highlight key areas of learning contexts than in less demanding settings
future research. (e.g., honors versus regular math courses).
Of particular relevance to our model of
engagement is that social-cognitive models
Overview of a Social-Cognitive emphasize the importance of cyclical feedback
Perspective of Engagement loops. As will be discussed in the following sec-
tion, cyclical feedback loops are particularly
Our model of cognitive engagement is grounded useful because they underscore the essential
in a social-cognitive perspective of self-regulated processes that prompt individuals to initiate and
functioning. In general, social-cognitive theory sustain cognitive engagement during learning
places primary importance on reciprocal deter- (Cleary, Zimmerman, & Keating, 2006; Schunk,
minism; that is, human functioning involves 2001). We now turn to presenting a formal defini-
reciprocal interactions among environment, per- tion of SRL engagement as well as the historical
son (beliefs, affect), and behavior. Bandura evolution of the cyclical model serving as the
argued, however, that cognition, and in particular centerpiece of this definition.
an individual’s beliefs of personal agency or effi-
cacy, is the primary factor underlying the proac-
tive and self-directed nature of human behavior SRL Engagement as a Cyclical
(Bandura, 1986, 2001). Bandura also empha- Feedback Loop
sized that humans will use various types of cog-
nitive processes to regulate their behaviors and Social-cognitive researchers have defined self-
performance, such as self-observation, self-judg- regulation as self-generated thoughts, feelings,
ments, and self-reactions. Thus, from this per- and behaviors that are strategically oriented or
spective, in order to understand the nature or directed toward the attainment of personal goals
causal impetus underlying students’ investment (Schunk, 2001). Although some researchers
in learning, one needs to consider these cognitive equate cognitive engagement to characteristics
variables. of self-regulation, it is important to note that
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 241

self-regulation is more than a cognitive construct. in an entirely different domain or for a different
It also involves the intentional enactment of overt task (Carver & Scheier, 2000).
behaviors, such as using strategies during learn- The distinction between closed and open feed-
ing or self-recording the number of incorrectly back loops is not a trivial one. Closed loops are
solved homework problems, and the management based on the negative feedback principle illus-
of environmental or internal constraints during trated previously. Thus, it is assumed that indi-
learning. As previously discussed, the source of viduals simply attempt to reduce performance
these behaviors involves students’ self-efficacy discrepancies against a fixed standard and will
beliefs, their level of interest in a task or the inher- disengage after the goal is attained. Although
ent value of that task, or their beliefs regarding some current models of self-regulation, such as
why they are engaging in learning. For the pur- information processing, emphasize negative or
poses of this manuscript, however, our definition “closed” feedback loops, social-cognitive
of SRL engagement focuses heavily on the notion accounts embrace open feedback models, recog-
of being cognitively and strategically engaged in nizing that individuals will often seek to expand
learning. In short, SRL engagement is defined as their knowledge or skills and to redefine one’s
the extent to which individuals think strategically standards of success.
before, during, and after performance on some In the 1980s, Bandura postulated an open
learning activity. From our perspective, SRL cyclical feedback loop based on three recursive
engagement is not an all or nothing phenomenon social-cognitive processes: self-observation, self-
but rather exists along a continuum across and judgment, and self-reactions. Self-observation is
within each of the three phases. Given the impor- a regulatory process that typically occurs during
tance of the feedback loop to our definition of performance or learning and involves attending
cognitive engagement, we will provide a brief to and tracking specific aspects of performance,
overview of the historical evolution of this loop such as quantity, accuracy, or quality, as well as
as well as a rationale detailing why adherence to the personal and environmental conditions sur-
such a framework is informative and valuable. rounding that performance (Bandura, 1986;
Zimmerman & Paulsen, 1995). Bandura cau-
Historical Aspects of the Cyclical tioned that information generated via self-
Feedback Loop observation will do little to impact behavioral or
Historically, models of self-regulation have strategic change unless one first effectively
included some type of feedback loop to explain judges or evaluates one’s performance. Self-
the process by which individuals manage their judgment involves the conclusions one draws
learning (Carver & Scheier, 2000; Miller, Galanter, about performance outcomes, regarding success
& Pribham, 1960; Powers, 1973). Theorists or failure as well as the reasons for these perfor-
espoused the importance of negative feedback mance attainments. The quality of one’s self-
loops, whereby an individual seeks to reduce a reactions is critically important in our model of
discrepancy between a goal state and current level SRL engagement because they impact the extent
of performance. According to this formulation, a to which students will seek to initiate new cycles
person’s initial performance level is first tested of regulated learning.
against a standard. If the feedback is “negative,”
indicating that a discrepancy still exists, then an Current Conceptualization
individual will be motivated to decrease this aver- of the Cyclical Feedback Loop
sive state by recursively self-correcting or adapt- Zimmerman (2000) expanded Bandura’s model
ing one’s efforts to learn until that discrepancy is to a more comprehensive and descriptive three-
sufficiently diminished. Once a student reaches phase cyclical loop (see Fig. 11.1). From this per-
his or her standard, his or her engagement in the spective, engagement in regulatory or strategic
activity will cease until a new discrepancy learning consists of three sequential phases: fore-
between a target and goal is identified, typically thought (i.e., processes that precede efforts to
242 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

Performance Control
Phase
Self-Control
Self-instruction
Attention focusing
Task strategies

Self-Observation
Metacognitive monitoring
Self-recording

Self-Reflection Phase
Forethought Phase
Self-Judgment
Task Analysis

Self-Reaction
Self-Motivation Beliefs

Fig. 11.1 Phases and processes of self-regulation (From Zimmerman & Campillo (2003), p. 239. Reprinted with
permission)

learn or perform), performance control (i.e., pro- outcomes they hope to accomplish and methods
cesses occurring during learning efforts), and used to attain these goals (Locke & Latham,
self-reflection (i.e., processes occurring after 1990; Zimmerman, 2000). It is also important
learning or performance) (Zimmerman, 2000). to note that self-motivation beliefs are also
These phases are hypothesized to be interdepen- considered forethought phase processes and
dent so that changes in forethought processes are important because they impact the extent
impact performance control, which, in turn, influ- to which students engage in self-control and
ence self-reflection phase processes. Relative to self-observation processes during learning
our definition of SRL engagement, we are pri- (Zimmerman & Cleary, 2009).
marily concerned with the extent to which indi- During learning or performance of an aca-
viduals become cognitively engaged in learning, demic task, such as studying for a test or solving
with particular emphasis placed on students’ math problems, a SRL-engaged learner will uti-
selection, use, and reflection on the effectiveness lize various self-control phase processes, such as
of learning strategies. self-instruction, attention-focusing, and task
In terms of forethought, highly SRL-engaged strategies, to optimize their focus on the task and
students seek to identify the essential require- their effort and persistence (Wolters, 2003;
ments of a learning task (task analysis), set out- Zimmerman, 2000). Thus, a student who proac-
come, and or process goals (goal setting), and tively develops a strategic plan (e.g., use of cog-
develop learning plans (strategic planning) to nitive maps, environmental structuring strategies,
achieve one’s goals. Students who proactively and time management tactics), prior to studying
engage in strategic goal setting and planning for a social studies exam, is more likely to enlist
prior to learning are more likely to be aware of strategic cognition during studying, such as mak-
the subtleties of a learning activity as well as the ing self-statements to prompt implementation of
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 243

one’s study plan or using self-consequences to


optimize one’s motivation to use these strategies
Importance and Applications
(Wolters, 2003).
of a Dynamic Feedback Cycle
During learning, a SRL-engaged student
of Engagement
will also utilize various forms of self-observation
In this section, we underscore the value and
procedures, such as self-monitoring or self-
importance of conceptualizing cognitive engage-
recording. This regulatory process involves track-
ment as a cyclical feedback loop. At the outset, it
ing one’s task performance and the conditions
is important to emphasize that Zimmerman’s
surrounding it. From an engagement perspective,
(2000) three-phase model represented a signifi-
this process is important because it facilitates
cant theoretical advance over prior models
error analysis and the capacity to make fine-
because of its emphasis on forethought processes,
grained adjustments to one’s strategies when not
such as goal setting and strategic planning.
learning effectively. For example, a student who
Including forethought in this regulatory loop was
is struggling to learn how to write essays in
important because it reinforced the premise that
English class may benefit from cognitively track-
students have the potential to proactively engage
ing the specific steps of a writing strategy that are
with academic tasks and are not simply reactive
challenging (Graham & Harris, 2005). In a cycli-
organisms to environmentally imposed circum-
cal framework, self-monitored feedback helps
stances (Bandura, 2001). In addition, students
students strategically reflect on their learning and
who learn to use adaptive forms of forethought
to make the necessary strategic adjustments to
will also enhance the quality of their strategic
improve task performance.
engagement during learning as well as their skill
According to Zimmerman (2000), self-
in adaptively reflecting on their performance out-
reflection is a multicomponent cognitive pro-
comes. For example, if a student does not strate-
cess involving various subprocesses, such as
gically plan or develop goals prior to reading a
self-evaluation, causal attributions, and adaptive
chapter from a science textbook, the student is
inferences. Following performance on some aca-
less likely to implement purposeful, goal-directed
demic activity, students who are highly SRL-
strategies when reading the text. A lack of aware-
engaged will first compare self-monitored or
ness or poor mindfulness of the strategies one
externally provided feedback to some standard in
uses during learning makes it extremely difficult
order to judge their level of success. This process
to evaluate the effectiveness of one’s learning
is important because it ultimately determines
methods following learning or performance.
whether students perceive their learning efforts
Unfortunately, these reactive types of regula-
in favorable or unfavorable terms. Following
tors try to “fix” poorly defined problems in a post
these performance judgments, students who are
hoc fashion, an approach characterized by trial
strategically engaged in learning are more likely
and error learning, ambiguity, frustration, and a
to attribute their success and failure to the strate-
reliance on normative comparisons (Zimmerman,
gies that they utilized during learning and will
2000; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1997). Thus,
seek to make adjustments to their learning tactics
suppose a 12-year-old middle school student,
in order to improve their future performances
Betsy, attained an average math quiz grade of 63
(i.e., adaptive inferences) (Cleary et al., 2006;
on her prior five math quizzes. She is largely
Schunk, 2001). Although students can attribute
unaware of how she should prepare for math
their successes and failures to other personal and
quizzes and displays an inconsistent pattern of
contextual factors that also can be adaptive or
studying behaviors. Betsy also rarely thinks about
helpful for modifying one’s behaviors, the key
the types of grades that she wants to attain, aside
point here is that a hallmark feature of a SRL-
from a general, ambiguous belief “to do better.”
engaged learning is exhibiting consistent “think-
Without a particular standard from which to judge
ing in the language of strategies.”
244 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

her performance, Betsy is prone to making social these motivation beliefs have been found to affect
comparison types of self-evaluative judgments, the strategic choices that students make, their
such as comparing her quiz performance to that effort and persistence during learning, as well as
of her classmates. In this scenario, if the math the quality of their reflective processes following
quiz class average was 89 and Betsy attained a performance (Schunk & Zimmerman, 2008).
76, she would likely view this performance as a Although space limitations prevent us from
failure and thus not recognize that her score was providing a comprehensive review of all types of
13 points higher than the average of her prior the motivational beliefs (For a comprehensive
math quizzes. Furthermore, because Betsy has review see Schunk & Zimmerman, 2008), we will
very little awareness of her approach or the par- focus specifically on the relationship between stu-
ticular strategies that she uses to learn, it is highly dents’ efficacy beliefs and their cognitive engage-
unlikely that she will attribute her “poor perfor- ment in cyclical phase processes. Self-efficacy is
mance” to these strategies, further crippling her often defined as beliefs about one’s capabilities to
skill in adjusting or modifying her studying learn or perform at designated levels and has been
behaviors for future quizzes (Cleary et al., 2006; shown to be a particularly potent source of motiva-
Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1997). tion (Pajares, 2006). More specifically, these types
In addition to the conceptual advantage of link- of beliefs have been shown to not only enhance
ing forethought, performance, and self-reflection, students’ adaptive behaviors, such as effort, persis-
we would like to highlight three additional points tence, and choice of activities (Bandura, 1986,
underscoring the utility of this three-phase con- 1997; Schunk & Pajares, 2004; Zimmerman,
ceptual framework: (a) integration of motivation 1995), but also to induce and sustain their cogni-
and regulatory processes, (b) empirical support tive engagement in cyclical strategic thinking.
for multiphase self-regulation training, and (c) the For example, a voluminous literature shows
level of correspondence between the cyclical that high perceptions of personal efficacy predict
model and self-regulation intervention programs. the types and quality of learning goals and choice
of strategies that individuals select prior to learn-
ing (Cleary & Zimmerman, 2001; Zimmerman &
SRL Engagement and Motivation Bandura, 1994; Zimmerman, Bandura, &
Martinez-Pons, 1992). Students’ beliefs of per-
An important aspect of our cyclical account of sonal efficacy have also been shown to predict
engagement is that it integrates metacognition, the type and quality of strategies that students use
motivation, and strategic processes and behav- during learning as well as their use of self-control
iors. Although students can learn powerful learn- and self-monitoring tactics, all key performance
ing strategies, such as summarization when control phase processes (Bandura, Barbaranelli,
reading text or using graphic organizers to learn Caprara, & Pastorelli, 1996; Bouffard-Bouchard,
science material, if students are not motivated to Parent, & Larivee, 1991; Schunk & Swartz,
plan out their learning efforts, to use these strate- 1993). For example, Schunk and Swartz found
gies, or to reflect on the effectiveness of their that elementary school children who exhibited
learning tactics – quite an effortful process – it is high levels of self-efficacy following an experi-
highly probable that their overall performance mental manipulation were more likely to use
will be greatly diminished. From our perspective, writing strategies during follow-up assessments
the primary sources underlying students’ motiva- than youth possessing low self-efficacy.
tion to initiate and sustain a high level of SRL Furthermore, Bouffard-Bouchard et al. (1991)
engagement during learning include a host of found that inducing higher levels of self-efficacy
self-motivation beliefs, such as self-efficacy, out- in middle school and high school youth not only
come expectations, task interest or valuing, and impacted their persistence and effort, but also the
goal orientation (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; extent to which they monitored their working
Pintrich, 2000; Zimmerman, 2000). Collectively, time during a verbal concept formation task.
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 245

Students who are highly efficacious also tend informational function in that students obtain
to set higher self-evaluative standards and will data about the frequency, duration, or intensity of
often perceive failure to be the result of control- their behaviors or cognition. For example, self-
lable or changeable factors, such as effort or observation can motivate youth by conveying
strategy use (Bandura, 1997; Cleary & enhanced performance over time or by highlight-
Zimmerman, 2001; Silver, Mitchell, & Gist, ing the conditions under which a particular strat-
1995; Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994). Clearly, as egy or behavior is most effective. Using the case
students’ beliefs of personal efficacy increase, example in the preceding paragraph, suppose the
there is a greater likelihood that they will engage student was asked to self-record the number of
in the cyclical feedback loop and will focus their points lost on her prior three math tests across
cognition on the selection, use, and adaptation of five different types of problems. Performing this
learning strategies. type of self-recording can serve a motivational
In addition to motivational beliefs, however, function if it helps the student to not only isolate
social-cognitive researchers recognize that regu- patterns of errors that she made but to stimulate
latory subprocesses within the feedback loop also reflective thinking to develop solutions to rem-
impact student motivation. We will consider a edy these problems.
couple of performance- and self-reflection-phase Two self-reflection processes that also pro-
processes to illustrate this point. Relative to the mote motivation in youth include self-evaluation
“during” or performance phase of the feedback and causal attributions. Self-evaluation repre-
loop, researchers have shown that various self- sents the initial cognitive dimension of self-
control processes, such as self-instruction, self- reflection, whereby individuals compare their
consequences, and environmental control, can current performance on a task to some standard
increase students’ effort and persistence or benchmark (Kitsantas & Zimmerman, 2006;
(Meichenbaum, 1977; Wolters, 1999, 2003; Zimmerman, 2000). Many empirically supported
Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1986), particu- self-regulation interventions emphasize the use
larly when such tactics focus students’ attention of mastery or self-related self-evaluation stan-
on process or strategic aspects of learning. dards, such as improvement from prior perfor-
Suppose a tenth grade student needs to attain mance or a benchmark score of 80% on a test
a grade of A on her next math test in order to (Butler, 1998; Cleary, Platten, & Nelson, 2008;
obtain a personal semester goal of B+ in the Fuchs et al., 2003; Graham & Harris, 2005).
course. During math study sessions, the student These types of standards are considered optimal
repeatedly tells herself, “I can do this if I just because they shift students’ attention and cogni-
keep studying what is on the study guide” (self- tion toward indicators of personal progress or the
instruction). She also decided to reward herself effectiveness of one’s learning strategies. In con-
with 30 min of TV time following at least 1 h of trast, if students who struggle in school are
studying (self-consequences) and to study with a prompted to make normative comparisons, such
few of her friends who are highly motivated to as when a teacher publicly displays class grades,
get into college (environment control). The latter their attention and thinking will often shift to
tactic is particularly motivating to this student variables that are not essential or unrelated to
because it prompts her to visualize the possibility their personal success, such as lack of personal
of her going to college, thereby elevating study- ability (Schunk et al., 2008).
ing behaviors as a valued and important activity The types of causal attributions students make
(Wolters, 2003). following performance, and in particular failure
Self-monitoring is another regulatory process outcomes, can also have a major impact on stu-
that induces cycles of SRL engagement (Bandura, dents’ motivation to cognitively adapt ineffective
1997; Lan, 1998; Schunk, 2001; Shapiro, Durnan, learning methods (Borkowski, Weyhing, & Carr,
Post, & Skibitsky-Levinson, 2002). Self- 1988; Clifford, 1986; Weiner, 1986). From a
monitoring serves an awareness building or motivational perspective, encouraging students
246 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

to make internal, controllable, and unstable attri- outcomes during practice sessions exhibited
butions following failure, such as to effort and higher dart-throwing skill, self-efficacy, and
strategy use, is adaptive because they sustain stu- satisfaction than those who only engaged in
dents’ efficacy beliefs and direct their cognition forethought (i.e., goal setting).
on the effectiveness of one’s methods to learn In a more recent study, Cleary et al. (2006)
(Cleary et al., 2006; Kuhl, 1985; Schunk, 1982; examined the additive effects of self-regulatory
Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1997). In one study training on novice basketball players’ free-throw
examining the self-regulatory characteristics of shooting skill and self-reflection phase processes
expert, nonexpert, and novice basketball players during a short practice session. Fifty college stu-
in high school, Cleary and Zimmerman (2001) dents were randomly assigned to either one of
reported that the types of attributions that stu- three experimental groups, to a practice control
dents make following missed free throws was not group, or to a no-practice control group. Each of
only highly predictive of expertise status, but the three experimental groups received either
also of the types of cognitive reactions and adap- three-, two-, or one-phase training in the cyclical
tations players make. For example, players who feedback loop. Participants assigned to the three-
made strategic attributions following poor free phase condition were instructed to set process
throws were more likely to report making strate- goals (forethought phase), to self-record effective
gic adaptations or modifications prior to taking shooting processes (performance phase), and to
subsequent shots. Regardless of the domain of make strategic attributions and adjustments fol-
interest, individuals who believe that the strate- lowing missed free throws (self-reflection phase).
gies they use to learn or to perform a task are The two-phase group received the same fore-
ineffective (i.e., strategic attributions) will initi- thought and performance phase training but no
ate attempts to change these strategies to opti- self-reflection instruction, whereas the one-phase
mize their learning (i.e., strategic adaptive group only received training in goal setting. The
inferences) (Cleary & Zimmerman, 2001; Cleary participants in the two control conditions did not
et al., 2006; Schunk et al., 2008). receive any self-regulatory instruction.
All five groups received identical shooting
strategy instructions to insure that achievement
Empirical Advantages of the Cyclical differences were not due to variations in knowl-
Feedback Loop edge of shooting technique. Furthermore, all
groups (except for the no-practice control group)
A marked advantage of a cyclical model of were given 12 min to practice using the shooting
engagement is its strong empirical support in the strategy. An important finding was that the three-
literature (Cleary et al., 2006; Schunk & Swartz, phase and two-phase groups performed signifi-
1993; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1996, 1997). cantly better in free-throw shooting than all other
That is, students who are trained in multiple SRL groups even though they shot significantly fewer
phase processes, such as forethought and perfor- free throws during the practice sessions. Fewer
mance control, are more likely to display greater shots were taken in the multiphase conditions
achievement and strategic cyclical thinking than because participants were required to self-record
those who are only trained in one of these phases. and or reflect on missed free-throw attempts dur-
For example, Zimmerman and Kitsantas con- ing the practice session. These results suggest
ducted a series of studies examining the effects of that it was not simply behavioral engagement in
forethought and performance control training routine practice sessions that facilitated success
on the self-regulatory skills and dart-throwing but rather one’s cognitive and strategic engage-
proficiency of high school girls. Across studies, a ment in the cyclical loop during these practice
consistent finding was that students who were opportunities. Furthermore, although the three-
trained in both goal setting prior to dart-throwing phase and two-phase groups were highly similar
practice and self-recording their performance in terms of performance outcomes and adaptive
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 247

self-regulation processes, the three-phase inter- Self-Regulated Strategy Development


vention group exhibited the most adaptive self- (SRSD)
reflection profile, exhibited by their strategic Graham and Harris developed the SRSD inter-
attributions and reactions following failure as vention program over 20 years ago to improve
well as the use of process-oriented self-evaluative the writing performance of elementary school
criteria to judge their level of success and satis- students (Graham & Harris, 2005; Graham,
faction (i.e., personal improvement, use of cor- Harris, & Troia, 1998). In general, SRSD involves
rect shooting strategy) (Cleary et al., 2006). a six-step process that actively engages students
in the three-phase cyclical process during various
types of writing activities. To promote fore-
Academic Applications thought, tutors typically engage students in dis-
of the Cyclical Loop cussions about their prior knowledge of writing
as well as the essential components of specific
A final advantage of the SRL cyclical engagement writing tasks. Students also discuss their current
model is that it can serve as the foundation or strategies to write and learn how to set process
framework through which researchers and or prac- goals, such as learning how to write better
titioners develop and implement engagement- essays.
related academic interventions. As Zimmerman During writing practice sessions, SRSD teach-
revealed in a recent interview in the Journal of ers or tutors explain, model, and prompt students
Advanced Academics, one of the reasons why the to use a specific writing strategy (i.e., strategies
cyclical model was developed was because it links often vary based on writing assignment). Students
together the processes that precede, guide, and are typically provided several structured guided
come after learning. Such a model allows one to practice sessions in which tutors provide hints
examine the causal links among these processes and feedback to students as they refine their use
and to also guide intervention development and application of the writing strategies. During
(Bembenutty, 2008). A variety of academic- these practice sessions, students are also taught to
focused SRL interventions reported in the litera- self-record the number of writing elements
ture devote primary attention to teaching students included in their essay and to make adaptive self-
how to become engaged in a cyclical process of instruction statements pertaining to problem defi-
thought and action across diverse academic tasks. nition, planning, and self-evaluation (Sexton,
There are several comprehensive self-regulation Harris, & Graham, 1998). This instructional fea-
intervention programs available in the literature ture is particularly important as it continually
that seek to induce cyclical phase changes in stu- directs students’ attention to the essential ele-
dents’ cognitive engagement during specific aca- ments of the writing process and the key tactics
demic tasks (Butler, 1998; Cassel & Reid, 1996; to optimize performance across these elements.
Cleary et al., 2008; Fuchs et al., 2003; Graham & To complete the cyclical loop, SRSD instruc-
Harris, 2005). Although most authors of these pro- tors prompt students to reflect on their writing
grams do not specifically refer to the dynamic performance, such as using criterion-based
feedback loop as the guiding framework for devel- self-evaluative standards to judge their success
oping the intervention, it is quite apparent that (e.g., number of story elements included) and
instruction in cyclical regulatory thought and action attributing success or failure to effort and or
is a core focus of these interventions. Our primary strategy use (Graham et al., 1998).
goal in this section is not to provide an exhaustive
review of all SRL interventions that address Strategic Content Learning (SCL)
elements of the cyclical loop but rather to provide Butler developed this innovative self-regulation
a few illustrative examples of interventions that instructional program to enhance the academic
comprehensively target all three phases of the achievement, motivation, and regulatory behav-
cognitive loop. iors of college-aged and secondary school students
248 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

(Butler, 1998; Butler, Beckingham, & Lauscher, applying these strategies to solve particular math
2005). The program has been administered to problems.
college students across multiple content areas, Self-reflection is naturally infused within each
including reading, writing, and math (Butler, SCL session, whereby students are often encour-
1995, 1998) and to middle school students in aged to continually self-evaluate their perfor-
math (Butler et al., 2005). Regardless of aca- mance during math problem solving. Students
demic context or developmental level of students, are not only encouraged to evaluate correctness
SCL adheres to a largely constructivist frame- of their answers to problems but to also consider
work. Thus, students are encouraged to become the role and impact of their problem-solving
active participants in learning, via establishing strategies. Accordingly, SCL tutors use attribu-
personal goals, selecting and modifying their tion and adaptive inference questions in order to
learning strategies, and reflecting on the effec- prompt students to reflect on the strategic reasons
tiveness of their learning strategies. In addition, for their success or failure when solving a prob-
consistent with the engagement framework that lem and to consider alternative methods or tactics
we present in this manuscript, engaging students to improve math performance.
in a recursive cycle of cognitive regulatory activ-
ities is a core SCL instructional principle. Self-Regulation Empowerment
In terms of forethought, students who receive Program (SREP)
SCL are typically instructed in task analysis A more recent intervention, Self-Regulation
skills, which involve interpreting the basic Empowerment Program (SREP), was developed
demands and requirements of specific academic to empower at-risk middle school or high school
assignments or tasks. Task analysis is crucial in youth to become more strategic, motivated, and
this model because it enables one to purposefully regulated during more complex and comprehen-
develop strategic plans to accomplish specific sive academic activities, such as studying for
goals relative to the task (Butler & Cartier, 2004). content-area exams (Cleary et al., 2008; Cleary
SCL tutors will often use strategic questioning to & Zimmerman, 2004). This approach emphasizes
assess students’ knowledge of math problems and the use of evidence-based learning tactics or
their use of learning strategies. These activities strategies that are linked particularly to academic
not only help students become more aware of tasks, such as concept maps and mnemonic
their learning methods when solving math prob- devices for learning science concepts. This pro-
lems but also help tutors to better understand the gram was borne out of social-cognitive theory
types of questions and prompts that are needed to and research and thus emphasizes the importance
impact and guide students’ strategic learning of social change agents, such as teachers and
(Butler et al., 2005). tutors, in cultivating adaptive cognition during
Regarding the performance phase, an impor- instruction (Bandura, 1997; Cleary & Zimmerman,
tant element of math-based SCL is for tutors to 2004). This instructional program is closely
continually engage students in interpreting math aligned with each of the three phases of the
problems successfully and to prompt them to use dynamic feedback loop and thus involves train-
and refine their self-generated learning strategies ing in task analysis, goal setting and strategic
and metacognitive strategies. In one case study, planning, self-recording, self-evaluation, strate-
Butler et al. (2005) reported that over the course gic attributions, and adaptive inferences.
of SCL instruction, a student generated seven The initial components of SREP instruction
new strategy steps when solving fraction prob- involve enhancing students’ awareness of their
lems, with these tactics increasing in complexity maladaptive beliefs, such as poor causal attribu-
and sophistication over time. In addition, the stu- tions (e.g., failure on tests is due to poor ability),
dent became more skillful in describing the and providing explicit instruction in core fore-
nature of her math problem-solving strategies thought processes such as task analysis, goal set-
and showed a high level of flexibility when ting, and strategic planning. Similar to most
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 249

Biology Test Goal


Biology Test Grade
90
Assess goal
85 progress

Assess goal
80 Assess goal progress
progress
75 Teach students to attribute
success to their use of
Test score (%)

70 Teach students to strategies and modifications


Teach students to attribute failure to
attribute failure to their strategy plan
65 their strategy plan

60

55

50

45 Teach students to make strategic Teach students to make strategic Teach students to make strategic
adjustments following failure adjustments following failure adjustments following success

40
Test #1 Test #2 Test #3
Test Number

Fig. 11.2 Example of a self-regulation graph used to teach students to evaluate goal progress and to make strategic
attributions and adaptive inferences (From Cleary et al. (2008), p. 87. Copyright 2008 by Prufrock Press. Reprinted with
permission)

effective self-regulation programs, SREP tutors SREP tutor, such as a trained graduate student
teach regulatory processes and learning strategies or school support staff, helps students adopt
within the context of authentic curriculum mate- outcome- and process-related goals as well as a
rials and content. Thus, students not only learn specific plan for addressing those goals. On the
about the value and importance of goal setting, graph, students are taught to plot their outcome
use of learning strategies, and self-recording, goal and to record the strategic plan that will
but also the use of these processes to learn spe- be used to accomplish their goal. As part of the
cific course material. In addition, primary emphasis performance phase dimension, students are
is placed on shifting students’ attention to power- encouraged to add, modify, or adjust their strate-
ful learning strategies before, during, and after gic plans listed on the graph when appropriate
studying and test performance. In Cleary et al. and to self-record their test or exam grades. This
(2008), given that science was the target content self-monitored outcome and process information
area of interest, the authors elected to utilize con- is used to stimulate self-reflective activities dur-
cept maps and mnemonic devices as the primary ing SREP sessions. Thus, after each test perfor-
learning strategies to be taught within the cyclical mance, a SREP tutor leads students through a
framework (Nesbit & Adesope, 2006). series of self-reflection questions and activities
The primary instructional mechanism through targeting self-evaluation, causal attributions, and
which students are taught to cognitively engage adaptive inferences. In terms of self-evaluation,
in strategic, cyclical thinking is the use of a self- two types of criteria are emphasized: prior per-
regulation graph (Cleary et al., 2008; see formance and forethought goals. Thus, students
Fig. 11.2). To stimulate forethought thinking, a judge success or failure based on comparing their
250 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

current performance to both their prior test grades (MSLQ) (Pintrich, Smith, & Garcia, 1993), and
as well as their test grade goal. Both of these more recently the Strategy Motivation and
types of self-criteria facilitate SRL engagement Learning Strategy Inventory (SMALSI) (Stroud
because they shift students’ attention and think- & Reynolds, 2006). Although these measures
ing on their own performance and the strategies can capture general aspects of students’ self-
linked to this performance. regulation of learning in a psychometrically reli-
Finally, SREP tutors also ask students to able way, they may not be well suited to capture
answer reflection questions targeting attributions the dynamic, context-specific aspects of SRL
(“What is the main reason why you got that grade engagement. Because these measures are typi-
on the test?”) and adaptive inferences (“What cally decontextualized in nature, they do not lend
do you need to do to improve your next test themselves to evaluating the actual “process” of
score?”). Ultimately, the SREP tutors guide stu- engagement in real time and are interpreted based
dents’ thinking to focus on the relationship on aggregated scores and normative criteria
between their performance outcomes and the (Perry & Winne, 2006; Zimmerman, 2008).
effort that they displayed in using their strategic To more accurately and comprehensively
plan. In this way, students learn how to make stra- evaluate students’ engagement in a cycle of
tegic attributions following failure and continu- thought and action, researchers have developed a
ously focus on how to sustain or adapt their variety of alternative measures, such as think-
methods of learning to attain their personal goals. aloud protocols (Greene & Azevedo, 2007),
Although our review of SRL intervention pro- direct observations (Perry, Vandekamp, Mercer,
grams was not exhaustive, it illustrated that a & Nordby, 2002), behavioral traces (Perry &
cyclical account of cognitive and strategic engage- Winne, 2006), and SRL microanalysis (Cleary &
ment can successfully be taught to students across Zimmerman, 2001; Kitsantas & Zimmerman,
academic tasks and development levels. 2002). These assessment approaches are labeled
as “event” measures because they seek to target
students’ thoughts and behaviors as they engage
SRL Microanalysis: Measuring Cyclical in particular tasks or activities. Consistent with
Regulatory Engagement our three-phase model of engagement, an event is
considered to be a temporal entity with a before,
Consistent with most models of self-regulation, during, and after component (Cleary, 2011;
our definition of SRL engagement is best repre- Winne & Perry, 2000).
sented by a dynamic, cyclical process that varies A cyclical event assessment method that has
across learning contexts and tasks (Bandura, been receiving increased attention in recent years
1997; Hadwin et al., 2001; Schunk, 2001; Winne is SRL microanalysis (Cleary, 2011; Cleary &
& Jaimeson-Noel, 2002). This “event” conceptu- Zimmerman, 2001). In short, this approach is
alization of SRL engagement contrasts from grounded in social-cognitive theory and was influ-
“ability” perspectives, the latter of which depicts enced by various lines of research and clinical
self-regulation to be a fixed or stable trait-like practice, including think-aloud protocols, emer-
construct. This conceptual distinction parallels gence of cognitive-behavioral therapy, and the
the difference made in the assessment literature importance of situation-dependence or specificity
regarding event and aptitude measures of self- (Bandura, 1977; Beck, 1963; Cleary, 2011;
regulation (Cleary, 2011; Winne & Perry, 2000; Ericsson & Simon, 1980). In short, this methodol-
Zimmerman, 2008). Aptitude measures are ogy is designed to examine students’ regulatory
described as those targeting an enduring attribute beliefs and reactions as they participate in con-
of an individual and typically include self-report text-specific tasks and activities. This approach
scales, such as the Learning and Study Strategies differs from typical self-report scales because it
Inventory (LASSI) (Weinstein & Palmer, 1990), entails observing human functioning as it occurs
Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire in real time and directly targets the self-motivation
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 251

beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy) and regulatory pro- shot was, “Why do you think you missed those
cesses (e.g., goal setting, attributions) outlined in last two shots?”, whereas the adaptive inference
the three-phase cyclical model (Cleary, 2011; question involved, “What do you need to do to
Kitsantas & Zimmerman, 2002). An essential make the next shot?” These latter two questions
component of this methodology is the use of a were considered microanalytic in nature because
structured interview protocol whereby task-spe- they were context- and task-specific (i.e., linked
cific regulatory questions delineated in the three- to basketball free throwing), targeted regulatory
phase cyclical loop are administered in a processes delineated in the three-phase model,
predetermined sequence during a specific activ- and were administered immediately following a
ity. In order for questions to fully qualify as specific performance outcome.
SRL microanalytic, they must be directly linked Kitsantas and Zimmerman (2002) expanded
to the phase dimensions of the academic event the scope of SRL microanalysis by including
and thus administered in a temporally appropri- additional microanalytic questions across all
ate sequence. That is, forethought phase ques- three phases: perceived instrumentality and task
tions are administered “before” an event, interest (forethought), self-monitoring (perfor-
performance processes “during” the activity, and mance), and self-evaluation (self-reflection). This
self-reflection “after” performance on the event study examined differences across achievement
(Cleary, 2011). groups relative to volleyball skills. Thus, in addi-
To illustrate this process, we will discuss the tion to goal setting, strategic planning, and self-
methodology of two studies that were the first to efficacy beliefs, Kitsantas and Zimmerman also
use SRL microanalysis in a comprehensive fash- targeted students’ level of interest in volleyball
ion. Cleary and Zimmerman (2001) examined serving (“How interesting is serving a volleyball
forethought (self-efficacy, goal setting, strategic overhand to you on a scale from 0 to 100?”) and
planning) and self-reflection (i.e., attributions, the importance of volleyball serving (“How
adaptive inferences, satisfaction) differences important is volleyball serving in attaining your
among expert, nonexpert, and novice high school future goals on a scale from 0 to 100?”) prior to
basketball players as they practiced shooting free the volleyball practice session. With respect
throws. The participants were asked to shoot free to self-evaluation, all of the players were asked to
throws individually at a basket during a 10-min report their self-evaluative methods, if any, fol-
practice session. This activity ultimately served lowing a volleyball serving practice session.
as the event around which microanalytic mea- Consistent with the temporal sequencing of
sures were administered. To examine partici- microanalytic methodology, this self-reflection
pants’ strategic approach to shooting free throws, question along with attribution and adaptive
the researchers asked participants a series of fore- inference questions was administered after the
thought questions prior to the practice session: practice session to effectively link them to the
(a) self-efficacy (e.g., “How sure are you that you “after” dimension of the event.
can make two shots in a row”), (b) goal setting Microanalytic methodology consists of both
(e.g., “Do you have a goal when practicing free metric and categorical questions, with most
throws?” “If so what is it?”), and (c) strategy involving single-item scales. The metric ques-
choice (e.g., “What do you need to do to accom- tions consist of motivation beliefs, such as self-
plish that goal?”). efficacy, task interest, instrumentality, and
Although this study did not examine stu- satisfaction. Participants rate their responses to
dents’ performance phase processes, such as self- these closed-ended questions on a Likert scale,
monitoring or self-control tactics, the participants typically ranging from 0 to 100. The categorical
were asked attribution and adaptive inference microanalytic questions target self-regulation
questions at two points: following two consecu- phase processes including goal setting, strategic
tive missed shots and two consecutive made planning, self-monitoring, self-evaluation, attri-
shots. The attribution question following a missed butions, and adaptive inferences. These questions
252 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

are open-ended and thus allow participants to a different aspect of regulation than that which is
provide elaborate or detailed responses to measured by self-report scales (Cleary et al.,
describe their regulatory cognitions. To quantify 2011).
these variables, two individuals are trained to
independently code the responses using a struc-
tured scoring rubric (Cleary & Zimmerman, Future Research Directions
2001). The interrater reliability of these scales
has been shown to be strong as evidenced by There are many fruitful lines of research that can
Kappa coefficients ranging from .81 to .98 be generated based on our cyclical account of
(Cleary, 2011). cognitive engagement. First, although several
Collectively, both the metric and categorical SRL academic interventions target various sub-
scales have been shown to differentiate experts, processes within each of the three phases of the
nonexperts, and novices across different motoric feedback loop, very few intervention studies have
activities. The predictive validity of microana- directly measured the dynamic shifts or changes
lytic questions has also been investigated. in students’ regulatory processes as they occur
Kitsantas and Zimmerman (2002) conducted an during academic activities. To capture these
ex post facto study to examine differences in vol- cyclical phase changes, researchers are encour-
leyball serving skill and self-regulatory processes aged to use event measures, such as SRL micro-
among expert, nonexpert, and novice volleyball analysis. Using these types of measures will also
players. The authors combined 12 self-regulatory allow one to draw inferences about the causal
measures into a single scale to predict women’s links among the specific regulatory processes
volleyball serving skill score. The authors that promote these sustained cycles of learning.
reported that the scale was highly reliable (a = 90) In addition, although the use of SRL microanalysis
and correlated extremely well with volleyball has recently been utilized in academic contexts
serving score (r = .95); the scale accounted for (Cleary et al., 2008, 2011), most microanalytic
90% of the variance in volleyball serving skill. studies have employed motoric tasks. To effec-
More recently, researchers have begun to use tively use microanalysis in academic contexts,
microanalysis within academic contexts (Cleary researchers need to first identify specific tasks
et al., 2008; Cleary, Callan, Peterson & Adams, with a clear beginning and end, such as writing
2011; Zimmerman, Moylan, Hudesman, White, an essay, solving math problems, reading a text-
& Flugman, 2008). Cleary et al. (2011) used hier- book, etc. Then, one can employ SRL microana-
archical regression analysis to examine the extent lytic questions before, during, and after this task
to which self-reflection microanalytic questions to evaluate students’ level of cognitive and strate-
(attributions, adaptive inferences, and self-evalu- gic engagement.
ation) accounted for a unique amount of variance Second, aside from the self-motivation litera-
in final course grades of college students over and ture, there is a paucity of studies examining the
above that accounted for by an abbreviated ver- external mechanisms or environmental factors
sion of the MSLQ. In general, the results indi- that facilitate or inhibit students’ engagement in
cated that the MSLQ did not account for a the cyclical model. Thus, another interesting line
negligible amount of a significant amount of of research involves examining the extent to
variance in final course grade, whereas three which particular components of instruction, such
self-reflection phase processes, causal attribu- as the amount of autonomy support or the type,
tion, adaptive inferences, and self-evaluative stan- quantity, or quality of feedback provided to stu-
dards accounted for a statistically and clinically dents, impact students’ cyclical cognitive engage-
significant change in R2. It is also of interest to ment during learning. Of particular interest is for
note the microanalytic questions exhibited low to future research to examine the most effective
non significant corrections with the MSLQ, sug- ways to engage students to continuously self-
gesting that the microanalytic questions measure reflect on their academic progress and performance
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 253

across key content areas, such as math, science, received in graduate school may not have ade-
and English. That is, researchers are encouraged quately addressed such issues (Cleary, 2009;
to examine the personal (i.e., cognitive and affec- Cleary et al., 2010). Most school psychology pro-
tive) and environmental factors which either grams in the United States emphasize coursework
inhibit or facilitate students’ skill in adapting and addressing several key content areas including
modifying their behaviors following difficulty or core psychology topics such as learning and
failure in school and whether such reflective development, academic, behavioral, and mental
activities directly change student performance health interventions, consultation activities, psy-
and behavior. choeducational assessment, and professional eth-
Third, self-efficacy perceptions have been ics and behavior (Jimerson & Oakland, 2007).
shown to be an important motivational source of Although information pertaining to student self-
students’ SRL engagement. Although these management, regulation, and motivation may be
beliefs are typically assessed during the fore- embedded within such courses, research suggests
thought phase, research has clearly revealed that that students may not receive enough extensive
they also affect regulatory processes during other training in these processes during graduate
phases in the cyclical loop, such as the accuracy school. For example, Cleary (2009) found that
of metacognitive monitoring during the perfor- the average school psychologist reported that
mance control phase and self-evaluation judg- their graduate training programs did not ade-
ments during the self-reflection phase (Ramdass quately prepare them to work with youth exhibit-
& Zimmerman, 2008). The primary implica- ing motivation and self-regulation difficulties.
tion here is that researchers and practitioners may Along the same lines, Wehmeyer et al. (2000)
find it valuable to examine the influence of self- showed that 41% of teachers indicated that a key
efficacy perceptions on other self-regulatory pro- barrier to providing instruction in self-determina-
cesses before, during, and after performance in tion, a closely related concept to self-regulation,
order to evaluate shifts in these beliefs and to was that they did not receive sufficient training or
more closely examine microanalytic proximal information on how to do so in graduate school
links between efficacy beliefs and regulatory (Wehmeyer et al. 2000). In short, although self-
processes. regulation intervention programs are highly
As a concluding thought, it is important to effective in improving the performance of youth
emphasize the emergent research demonstrating across multiple domains and that such programs
that educators and school psychologists perceive help to cultivate adaptive beliefs and cognitive
issues of student motivation and self-regulation processes, there is much work to be done to
to be highly valuable and relevant to student per- ensure that these programs are integrated into the
formance and to their own professional activities, curriculum and services provided by schools.
yet they do not frequently engage in assessment,
instructional, or intervention activities relative to
these areas (Cleary, 2009; Cleary et al., 2010, References
2011; Cleary & Zimmerman, 2006; Grigal,
Neubart, Moon, & Graham, 2003; Wehmeyer Agran, M., Snow, K., & Swaner, J. (1999). Teacher percep-
et al., 2000). It is highly probable that this prac- tions of self-determination: Benefits, characteristics, and
tice gap is due to a variety of factors, such as edu- strategies. Education and Training in Mental Retardation
and Developmental Disabilities, 34, 293–301.
cators’ limited knowledge of, and potentially Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying the-
limited access to, key assessment tools and inter- ory of behavior change. Psychological Review, 84,
vention programs. 191–215.
Another reason why both teachers and school Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and
action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs,
psychologists express a strong desire for profes- NJ: Prentice-Hall.
sional development training in motivation and Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control.
self-regulation is because the training that they New York: W. H. Freeman.
254 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Handbook of self-
perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–29. regulation of learning and performance. New York:
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, Routledge.
C. (1996). Multifaceted impact of self-efficacy beliefs Cleary, T. J., & Chen, P. P. (2009). Self-regulation, moti-
on academic functioning. Child Development, 67, vation, and math achievement in middle school:
1206–1222. Variations across grade level and math context.
Beck, A. T. (1963). Thinking and depression. Archives of Journal of School Psychology, 47, 291–314.
General Psychiatry, 9, 324–333. Cleary, T. J., Gubi, A., & Prescott, M. (2010). Motivation
Bembenutty, H. (2008). The last word: An interview with and self-regulation assessments: Professional prac-
Barry J. Zimmerman. Journal of Advanced Academics, tices and needs of school psychologists. Psychology in
20, 174–192. the Schools, 47(10), 985–1002.
Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R., & Zeidner, M. (Eds.). Cleary, T. J., Callan, G., Peterson, J., & Adams, T. (2011).
(2000). Handbook of self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Validity of Self-Regulated Learning (SRL) in an aca-
Academic. demic context. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Borkowski, J. G., Weyhing, R. S., & Carr, M. (1988). Cleary, T. J., Platten, P., & Nelson, A. C. (2008).
Effects of attributional retraining on strategy-based Effectiveness of self-regulation empowerment pro-
reading comprehension in learning disabled students. gram with urban high school students. Journal of
Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 46–53. Advanced Academics, 20, 70–107.
Bouffard-Bouchard, T., Parent, S., & Larivee, S. (1991). Cleary, T. J., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2001). Self-regulation
Influence of self-efficacy on self-regulation and per- differences during athletic practice by experts, non-
formance among junior and senior high-school age experts, and novices. Journal of Applied Sport
students. International Journal of Behavioral Psychology, 13, 185–206.
Development, 14, 153–164. Cleary, T. J., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2004). Self-regulation
Butler, D. (1995). Promoting strategic learning by post- empowerment program: A school-based program to
secondary students with learning disabilities. Journal enhance self-regulated and self-motivated cycles of
of Learning Disabilities, 28, 170–190. student learning. Psychology in the Schools, 41,
Butler, D. (1998). The strategic content learning approach 537–550.
to promoting self-regulated learning: A report of three Cleary, T. J., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2006). Teachers’ per-
studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, ceived usefulness of strategy microanalytic assessment
682–697. information. Psychology in the Schools, 43, 149–155.
Butler, D. L., Beckingham, B., & Lauscher, H. J. N. Cleary, T. J., Zimmerman, B. J., & Keating, T. (2006).
(2005). Promoting strategic learning by eighth-grade Training physical education students to self-regulate
students struggling in mathematics: A report of three during basketball free-throw practice. Research
case studies. Learning Disabilities Research and Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 77, 251–262.
Practice, 20, 156–174. Clifford, M. (1986). Comparative effects of strategy and
Butler, D. L., & Cartier, S. C. (2004). Promoting effective effort attributions. British Journal of Educational
task interpretation as an important work habit: A key Psychology, 56, 75–83.
to successful teaching and learning. Teachers College Coalition for Psychology in Schools and Education.
Record, 106, 1729–1758. (2006). Report on the teacher needs survey.
Carver, C. H., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). On the structure of Washington, DC: American Psychological Association,
behavioral self-regulation. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, Center for Psychology in Schools and Education.
& M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation Corno, L., & Mandinach, E. B. (1983). The role of cogni-
(pp. 41–84). Orlando, FL: Academic. tive engagement in classroom learning and motiva-
Cassel, J., & Reid, R. (1996). Use of a self-regulated strat- tion. Educational Psychologist, 18, 88–108.
egy intervention to improve word problem-solving Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs,
skills of students with mild disabilities. Journal of values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,
Behavioral Education, 6, 153–172. 109–132.
Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., Appleton, J. J., Berman- Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as
Young, S., Spanjers, D. M., & Varro, P. (2008). Best data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.
practices in fostering student engagement. In A. Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
psychology (5th ed., pp. 1099–1119). Bethesda, MD: the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
National Association of School Psychologists. 59–109.
Cleary, T. J. (2009). School-based motivation and self- Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Prentice, K., Burch, M., Hamlett,
regulation assessments: An examination of school C. L., Owen, R., et al. (2003). Enhancing third-grade
psychologist beliefs and practices. Journal of Applied students’ mathematical problem solving with self-
School Psychology, 25, 71–94. regulated learning strategies. Journal of Educational
Cleary, T. J. (2011). Emergence of self-regulated learning Psychology, 95, 306–315.
microanalysis: Historical overview, essential features, Graham, S., & Harris, H. R. (2005). Improving the writing
and implications for research and practice. In B. J. performance of young struggling writers: Theoretical
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 255

and programmatic research from the center on acceler- Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991).
ating student learning. The Journal of Special Relation of self-efficacy beliefs to academic outcomes:
Education, 39, 19–33. A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling
Graham, S., Harris, K., & Troia, G. A. (1998). Writing and Psychology, 18, 30–38.
self-regulation: Cases from the self-regulated strategy Nesbit, J. C., & Adesope, O. O. (2006). Learning with
development model. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman concept and knowledge maps: A meta-analysis. Review
(Eds.), Self-regulated learning: From teaching to self- of Educational Research, 76, 413–448.
reflective practice. New York: Guilford Press. Pajares, F. (2006). Self-efficacy during childhood and
Greene, J. A., & Azevedo, R. (2007). Adolescents’ use of adolescence: Implications for teachers and parents. In
self-regulatory processes and their relation to qualitative F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of
mental model shifts while using hypermedia. Journal of adolescents (pp. 339–367). Greenwich, CT:
Educational Computing Research, 36, 125–148. Information Age Publishing.
Grigal, M., Neubart, D. A., Moon, S. M., & Graham, S. Perry, N. E., VandeKamp, K. O., Mercer, L. K., & Nordby,
(2003). Self-determination for students with disabili- C. J. (2002). Investigating teacher-student interactions
ties: Views of parents and teachers. Exceptional that foster self-regulated learning. Educational
Children, 70, 97–112. Psychologist, 37, 5–15.
Hadwin, A. F., Winne, P. H., Stockley, D. B., Nesbit, J. C., Perry, N. E., & Winne, P. H. (2006). Learning from learn-
& Woszczyna, C. (2001). Context moderates students’ ing kits: Study traces of students’ self-regulated
self-reports about how they study. Journal of engagements with Computerized content. Educational
Educational Psychology, 93, 477–487. Psychology Review, 18, 211–228.
Jimerson, S. R., & Oakland, T. D. (2007). School psychol- Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in
ogy in the United States. In S. R. Jimerson, T. D. self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich,
Oakland, & P. T. Farrell (Eds.), The handbook of inter- & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
national school psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: (pp. 452–502). Orlando, FL: Academic.
Sage Publications. Pintrich, P. R., & de Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and
Kitsantas, A., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Comparing self-regulated components of classroom academic
self-regulatory processes among novice, non-expert, performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82,
and expert volleyball players: A microanalytic study. 33–40.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 91–105. Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A., & Garcia, T. (1993). Reliability
Kitsantas, A., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2006). Enhancing and predictive validity of the Motivated Strategies for
self-regulation of practice: The influence of graphing Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and
and self-evaluative standards. Metacognition and Psychological Measurement, 53, 801–813.
Learning, 1, 201–212. Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of percep-
Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of cognitive-behavior tion. Chicago: Aldine.
consistency: Self-regulatory processes and action Pressley, M., & McCormick, C. B. (1995). Advanced edu-
versus state orientation. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman cational psychology for educators, researchers, and
(Eds.), Action control: From cognition to behavior policymakers. New York: Harper Collins.
(pp. 101–128). West Berlin, Germany: Springer. Puustinen, M., & Pulkkinen, L. (2001). Models of self-
Lan, W. Y. (1998). Teaching self-monitoring skills in sta- regulated learning: A review. Scandinavian Journal of
tistics. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Educational Research, 45, 269–286.
Self-regulated learning: From teaching to self-reflective Ramdass, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Effects of
practice (pp. 86–105). New York: Guilford Press. self-correction strategy training on middle school stu-
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal dents’ self-efficacy, self-evaluation, and mathematics
setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: division learning. Journal of Advanced Academics, 20,
Prentice-Hall. 18–41.
Mace, F. C., Belfioree, P. J., & Hutchinson, J. M. (2001). Schmitz, B., & Wiese, B. S. (2006). New perspectives for
Operant theory and research on self-regulation. In B. J. the evaluation of training sessions in self-regulated
Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning: Time series analyses of diary data.
learning and academic achievement (2nd ed., pp. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 64–96.
39–66). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Schunk, D. H. (1981). Modeling and attributional effects
McCombs, B. L. (2001). Self-regulated learning and aca- on children achievement: A self-efficacy analysis.
demic achievement: A phenomenological view. In B. J. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 93–105.
Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learn- Schunk, D. H. (1982). Progress self-monitoring: Effects
ing and academic achievement (2nd ed., pp. 67–123). on children’s self-efficacy and achievement. The
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Journal of Experimental Education, 51, 89–93.
Meichenbaum, D. (1977). Cognitive-behavior modifica- Schunk, D. H. (2001). Social cognitive theory and self-
tion: An integrative approach. New York: Plenum. regulated learning. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribham, K. (1960). Plans Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic
and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, achievement (2nd ed., pp. 125–151). Mahwah, NJ:
Rinehart and Winston. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
256 T.J. Cleary and B.J. Zimmerman

Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2004). Self-efficacy in edu- ed., pp. 153–189). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
cation revisited: Empirical and applied evidence. In Associates.
D. M. McInerney & S. Van Etten (Eds.), Big theories Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (1998). Studying as self-
revisited (pp. 115–138). Greenwich, CT: Information regulated learning. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A.
Age Publishing. C. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational the-
Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2008). ory and practice (pp. 279–306). Hillsdale, NJ:
Motivation in education: Theory, research, and appli- Erlbaum.
cations (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Winne, P. H., & Jamieson-Noel, D. L. (2002). Exploring
Education. students’ calibration of self-reports about study tactics
Schunk, D. H., & Swartz, C. W. (1993). Writing strategy and achievement. Contemporary Educational
instruction with gifted students: Effects of goals and Psychology, 28, 259–276.
feedback on self-efficacy and skills. Roeper Review, Winne, P. H., & Perry, N. E. (2000). Measuring self-
15, 225–230. regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, &
Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (Eds.). (2008). M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation.
Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory, Orlando, FL: Academic.
research, & applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Wolters, C. A. (1999). The relation between high school
Erlbaum Associates. students’ motivational regulation and their use of
Sexton, M., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (1998). Self- learning strategies, effort, and classroom performance.
regulated strategy development and the writing pro- Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 281–299.
cess: Effects on essay writing and attributions. Wolters, C. A. (2003). Regulation of motivation:
Exceptional Children, 64, 295–311. Evaluating an underemphasized aspect of self-
Shapiro, E. S., Durnan, S. L., Post, E. E., & Skibitsky- regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 38,
Levinson, T. (2002). Self-monitoring procedures for 189–205.
children and adolescents. In M. R. Shinn, H. M. Zimmerman, B. J. (1995). Self-regulation involves more
Walker, & G. Stoner (Eds.), Interventions for aca- than metacognition: A social cognitive perspective.
demic and behavior problems II: Preventive and reme- Educational Psychologist, 30, 217–221.
dial approaches (pp. 433–454). Washington, DC: Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation:
National Association of School Psychologists. A social-cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts,
Silver, W. S., Mitchell, T. R., & Gist, M. E. (1995). P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-
Responses to successful and unsuccessful perfor- regulation (pp. 13–39). Orlando, FL: Academic.
mance: The moderating effect of self-efficacy on the Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Investigating self-regulation
relationship between performance and attributions. and motivation: Historical background, methodologi-
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision cal developments, and future prospects. American
Processes, 62, 286–299. Educational Research Journal, 45, 166–183.
Stroud, K. C., & Reynolds, C. R. (2006). School motiva- Zimmerman, B. J., & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of self-
tion and learning strategy inventory. Los Angeles, regulatory influences on writing course attainment.
CA: Western Psychological Services. American Educational Research Journal, 31,
Urdan, T., & Midgley, C. (2003). Changes in the perceived 845–862.
classroom goal structure and pattern of adaptive learn- Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M.
ing during early adolescence. Contemporary (1992). Self-motivation for academic attainment.
Educational Psychology, 28, 524–551. American Educational Research Journal, 31,
Wehmeyer, M. L., Agran, M., & Hughes, C. A. (2000). 845–862.
National survey of teachers’ promotion of self- Zimmerman, B. J., & Campillo, M. (2003). Motivating
determination and student-directed learning. The self-regulated problem solvers. In J. E. Davidson &
Journal of Special Education, 34, 58–68. R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The nature of problem solving.
Weiner, B. (1986). An attribution theory of motivation and New York: Cambridge University Press.
emotion. New York: Springer. Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. J. (2006). Adolescents’
Weinstein, C. E., Husman, J., & Dierking, D. R. (2000). development of personal agency: The role of self-
Self-regulation interventions with a focus on learning efficacy beliefs and self-regulatory skill. In F. Pajres &
strategies. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescence
(Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation. Orlando, FL: (pp. 45–69). Greenwich, CT: Information Age
Academic. Publishing.
Weinstein, C. E., & Palmer, D. R. (1990). Learning and Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. J. (2009). Motives to self-
study strategies inventory: High school version user regulate learning: A social cognitive account. In K. R.
manual. Clearwater, FL: H&H Publishing Company. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
Winne, P. H. (2001). Self-regulated learning viewed from at school (pp. 247–264). New York: Routledge/Taylor
models of information processing. In B. J. Zimmerman & Francis Group.
& D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1996). Self-regulated
academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd learning of a motoric skill: The role of goal-setting and
11 A Cyclical Self-Regulatory Account of Student Engagement… 257

self-monitoring. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, Zimmerman, B. J., Moylan, A., Hudesman, J., White, N.,
8, 60–75. & Flugman, B. (2008, June). Assessing the impact of
Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1997). Developmental self-reflection training with at-risk college students:
phases in self-regulation: Shifting from process to outcome Error analysis, calibration, and math attainment.
goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 29–36. Poster presented at the 2008 Research Conference of
Zimmerman, B. J., & Labuhn, A. S. (2011). Self-regulation the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department
of learning: Process approaches to personal develop- of Education, Washington, DC.
ment. In K. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA Zimmerman, B. J., & Paulsen, A. S. (1995). Self-
educational psychology handbook. Volume. 1: Theories, monitoring during collegiate studying: An invaluable
constructs, and critical issues (pp. 397–423). Washington, tool for academic self-regulation. In P. Pintrich (Ed.),
DC: APA Press. New directions in college teaching and learning. San
Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1986). Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass, Inc.
Development of a structured interview for assessing stu- Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, D. H. (2001). Self-regulated
dent use of self-regulated learning strategies. American learning and academic achievement (2nd ed.).
Educational Research Journal, 23, 614–628. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Academic Emotions and Student
Engagement 12
Reinhard Pekrun and Lisa Linnenbrink-Garcia

Abstract
Emotions are ubiquitous in academic settings, and they profoundly affect
students’ academic engagement and performance. In this chapter, we sum-
marize the extant research on academic emotions and their linkages with
students’ engagement. First, we outline relevant concepts of academic
emotion, including mood as well as achievement, epistemic, topic, and
social emotions. Second, we discuss the impact of these emotions on stu-
dents’ cognitive, motivational, behavioral, cognitive-behavioral, and
social-behavioral engagement and on their academic performance. Next,
we examine the origins of students’ academic emotions in terms of indi-
vidual and contextual variables. Finally, we highlight the complexity of
students’ emotions, focusing on reciprocal causation as well as regulation
and treatment of these emotions. In conclusion, we discuss directions for
future research, with a special emphasis on the need for educational inter-
vention research targeting emotions.

Emotions are ubiquitous in academic settings. of lack of accomplishment. Furthermore, these


Remember the last time you studied some learn- emotions affected your effort, motivation to per-
ing material? Depending on your goals and the sist, and strategies for learning—even if you were
contents of the material, you may have enjoyed unaware of these effects. Similarly, think of the
learning or been bored, experienced flow forget- last time you took an important exam. You may
ting time or been frustrated about never-ending have hoped for success, been afraid of failure, or
obstacles, felt proud of your progress or ashamed felt desperate because you were unprepared, but
you likely did not feel indifferent about it. Again,
these emotions likely had profound effects on
R. Pekrun (*) your motivational engagement, concentration,
Department of Psychology, University of Munich, and strategies used when taking the exam.
Munich, Germany
e-mail: pekrun@lmu.de
Empirical findings corroborate that students
experience a wide variety of emotions when
L. Linnenbrink-Garcia
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience,
attending class, doing homework assignments,
Duke University, Durham, NC, USA and taking tests and exams. For example, in
e-mail: llinnen@duke.edu exploratory research on emotions experienced by

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 259


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_12, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
260 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

university students, emotions reported frequently behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement
included enjoyment, interest, hope, pride, anger, described by Fredricks et al. (2004); however, we
anxiety, frustration, and boredom in academic have expanded this framework to clarify the
settings (Pekrun, Goetz, Titz, & Perry, 2002a). unique ways in which emotions relate to engage-
Until recently, these emotions did not receive ment. Specifically, within Fredricks et al.’s broad
much attention by researchers, two exceptions category of cognitive engagement, we differenti-
being studies on test anxiety (Zeidner, 1998, ate among motivational, cognitive, and cognitive-
2007) and research on causal attributions of suc- behavioral engagement. Our conceptualization of
cess and failure as antecedents of emotions behavioral engagement is similar to that proposed
(Weiner, 1985). During the past 10 years, how- by Fredricks et al.; however, we take a narrower
ever, there has been growing recognition that view focusing specifically on effort and persis-
emotions are central to human achievement striv- tence. We also extend Fredricks et al.’s frame-
ings. Emotions are no longer regarded as epiphe- work to include social-behavioral engagement to
nomena that may occur in academic settings but better capture forms of engagement related to
lack any instrumental relevance. In this nascent peer-to-peer learning.
research, affect and emotions are recognized as Before discussing the relation of emotions to
being of critical importance for students’ aca- engagement, we begin by outlining different con-
demic learning, achievement, personality devel- cepts describing students’ emotions, including
opment, and health (Efklides & Volet, 2005; affect, mood, achievement emotions, epistemic
Linnenbrink, 2006; Linnenbrink-Garcia & emotions, topic emotions, and social emotions.
Pekrun, 2011; Schutz & Lanehart, 2002; Schutz Next, the effects of emotions on the five types of
& Pekrun, 2007). student engagement and resulting academic
In this chapter, we consider academic emotions achievement are addressed. In the third section,
and their functions for students’ engagement. As we discuss the individual and social origins of stu-
noted by Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and Paris (2004; dents’ emotions, including a brief discussion of the
see also Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, 2008), relative universality of mechanisms of emotions
student engagement is best viewed as a metacon- and engagement across contexts. We conclude by
struct consisting of several components. In line considering principles of reciprocal causation of
with this view, we define student engagement as a emotion and engagement and their implications
multicomponent construct, the common denomi- for emotion regulation, treatment of emotions, and
nator being that all the components (i.e., types of the design of learning environments.
engagement) comprise active, energetic, and
approach-oriented involvement with academic
tasks. We distinguish five types of engagement: Concepts of Academic Emotions
cognitive (attention and memory processes), moti-
vational (intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, Emotion, Mood, and Affect
achievement goals), behavioral (effort and persis-
tence), cognitive-behavioral (strategy use and In contemporary emotion research, emotions are
self-regulation), and social-behavioral (social on- defined as multifaceted phenomena involving
task behavior), as detailed in our later discussion sets of coordinated psychological processes,
of emotions and engagement. Given our focus on including affective, cognitive, physiological,
emotions as precursors to these five forms of motivational, and expressive components
engagement, emotional engagement (e.g., in terms (Kleinginna & Kleinginna, 1981; Scherer, 2000).
of enjoyment of learning) is considered as an For example, a students’ anxiety before an exam
antecedent of other components of engagement in can be comprised of nervous, uneasy feelings
this chapter. (affective); worries about failing the exam (cog-
These five categories of engagement overlap nitive); increased heart rate or sweating (physio-
substantially with the three broad categories of logical); impulses to escape the situation
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 261

HIGH ACTIVATION

Activated Negative Activated Positive


Tense Excited

Angry Elated

Happy
NEGATIVE POSITIVE
Sad

Relaxed
Tired
Calm
Exhausted
Deactivated Negative Deactivated Positive

LOW ACTIVATION

Fig. 12.1 Affective circumplex (Model adapted with permission from Feldman Barrett and Russell [1998], published
by the American Psychological Association)

(motivation); and an anxious facial expression negative affect on psychological functioning,


(expressive). As compared to intense emotions, without further distinguishing between different
moods are of lower intensity and lack a specific emotions or moods.
referent. Some authors define emotion and mood
as categorically distinct (see Rosenberg, 1998).
Alternatively, since moods show a similar profile Valence and Activation
of components and similar qualitative differences
as emotions (as in cheerful, angry, or anxious Two important dimensions describing emotions,
mood), they can also be regarded as low-intensity moods, and affect are valence and activation.
emotions (Pekrun, 2006). In terms of valence, positive (i.e., pleasant) states,
Different emotions and moods are often com- such as enjoyment and happiness, can be differ-
piled in more general constructs of affect. Two entiated from negative (i.e., unpleasant) states,
variants of this term are used in the research litera- such as anger, anxiety, or boredom. In terms of
ture. In the educational literature, affect is often activation, physiologically activating states can
used to denote a broad variety of noncognitive be distinguished from deactivating states, such as
constructs including emotion, but also including activating excitement versus deactivating relax-
self-concept, beliefs, motivation, etc. (e.g., ation. These two dimensions are orthogonal,
McLeod & Adams, 1989). In contrast, in emotion making it possible to organize affective states in a
research, affect refers to emotions and moods two-dimensional space. In circumplex models of
more specifically. In this research, the term is affect, affective states are grouped according to
often used to refer to omnibus variables of posi- the relative degree of positive versus negative
tive versus negative emotions or moods, with valence and activation versus deactivation (e.g.,
positive affect being compiled of various positive Feldman Barrett & Russell, 1998; see Fig. 12.1).
states (e.g., enjoyment, pride, satisfaction) and By classifying affective states as positive or
negative affect consisting of various negative negative, and as activating or deactivating, the
states (e.g., anger, anxiety, frustration). For exam- circumplex can be transformed into a 2 × 2
ple, in experimental mood research, most studies taxonomy including four broad categories of
have compared the effects of positive versus emotions and moods (positive activating: e.g.,
262 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

Table 12.1 A three- Positivea Negativeb


dimensional taxonomy
of achievement emotions Object focus Activating Deactivating Activating Deactivating
Activity Enjoyment Relaxation Anger Boredom
Frustration
Outcome/ Hope Reliefc Anxiety Hopelessness
Prospective Joyc
Outcome/ Joy Contentment Shame Sadness
Retrospective Pride Relief Anger Disappointment
Gratitude
a
Positive = pleasant emotion
b
Negative = unpleasant emotion
c
Anticipatory joy/relief

enjoyment, hope, pride; positive deactivating: Achievement Emotions


relief, relaxation; negative activating: anger, anx- We define achievement emotions as emotions
iety, shame; negative deactivating: hopelessness, that relate to activities or outcomes that are
boredom; Pekrun, 2006). judged according to competence-related stan-
dards of quality. In the academic domain,
achievement emotions can relate to academic
Academic Emotions activities like studying or taking exams and to the
success and failure outcomes of these activities.
In addition to valence and activation, emotions Accordingly, two groups of achievement emo-
can be grouped according to their object focus tions are activity-related emotions, such as
(Pekrun, 2006). For explaining the psychological enjoyment or boredom during learning, and out-
functions of emotions, this dimension is no less come-related emotions, such as hope and pride
important than valence and activation. Specif- related to success, or anxiety, hopelessness, and
ically, regarding the functions of emotions for shame related to failure. Within the latter cate-
students’ academic engagement, object focus is gory, an important distinction is between pro-
critical because it determines if emotions pertain spective emotions related to future success and
to the academic task at hand or not. In terms of failure, such as hope and anxiety, and retrospec-
object focus, the following broad groups of emo- tive emotions related to success and failure that
tions and moods may be most important in the already occurred, such as pride, shame, relief,
academic domain. and disappointment. Combining the valence,
activation, and object focus (activity versus out-
General and Specific Mood come) dimensions renders a 3 × 2 taxonomy of
Students may experience moods that lack a refer- achievement emotions (Pekrun, 2006; see
ent, but may nevertheless strongly influence their Table 12.1). To date, research on achievement
performance. Moods can be generalized, being emotions has focused on outcome emotions such
experienced as just positive (pleasant) or nega- as anxiety, pride, and shame (Weiner, 1985;
tive (unpleasant), without clear differentiation of Zeidner, 2007), but failed to pay sufficient atten-
specific affective qualities. Alternatively, moods tion to activity emotions such as enjoyment and
can be qualitatively distinct, as in joyful, angry, boredom.
or fearful mood. While moods, by their very
nature, may not be directly tied to a specific aca- Epistemic Emotions
demic activity, they nonetheless have the poten- Emotions can be caused by cognitive qualities of
tial to shape the way in which students’ engage task information and of the processing of such
academically. For instance, a student in a nega- information. A prototypical case is cognitive
tive mood may have difficulty focusing on the incongruity triggering surprise and curiosity. As
task at hand, thus limiting engagement. suggested by Pekrun and Stephens (in press),
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 263

these emotions can be called epistemic emotions of social emotions related to other persons. These
since they pertain to the epistemic aspects of emotions include social achievement emotions,
learning and cognitive activities. During learn- such as admiration, envy, contempt, or empathy
ing, many emotions can be experienced either as related to the success and failure of others, as
achievement emotions or as epistemic emotions, well as nonachievement emotions, such as love
depending on the focus of attention. For example, or hate in the relationships with classmates and
the frustration experienced by a student not find- teachers (Weiner, 2007). Social emotions can
ing the solution to a mathematical problem can directly influence students’ engagement with aca-
be regarded an epistemic emotion if it is focused demic tasks, especially so when learning is situ-
on the cognitive incongruity implied by a non- ated in teacher-student or student-student
solved problem, and as an achievement emotion interactions. They can also indirectly influence
if the focus is on personal failure and inability to learning by motivating students to engage or dis-
solve the problem. A typical sequence of engage in task-related interactions with teachers
epistemic emotions induced by a cognitive prob- and classmates (Linnenbrink-Garcia, Rogat, &
lem may involve (1) surprise, (2) curiosity and Koskey, 2011).
situational interest if the surprise is not dissolved,
(3) anxiety in case of severe incongruity and
information that deeply disturbs existing cogni- Functions for Students’ Engagement
tive schemas, (4) enjoyment and delight experi- and Achievement
enced when recombining information such that
the problem gets solved, or (5) frustration when Cognitive and neuroscientific research has shown
this seems not to be possible (also see Craig, that emotions, and affect more broadly, are funda-
D’Mello, Witherspoon, & Graesser, 2008). mentally important for human learning and devel-
opment. Specifically, experimental mood studies
Topic Emotions have found that affect influences a broad variety
During studying or attending class, emotions can of cognitive processes that contribute to learning,
be triggered by the contents covered by learning such as perception, attention, social judgment,
material. Examples are the empathetic emotions cognitive problem-solving, decision-making, and
pertaining to a protagonist’s fate when reading a memory processes (Clore & Huntsinger, 2007,
novel, the emotions triggered by political events 2009; Loewenstein & Lerner, 2003; Parrott &
dealt with in political lessons, or the emotions Spackman, 2000). However, one fundamental
related to topics in science class, such as the frus- problem with much of this research is that it used
tration experienced by American children when global constructs of positive versus negative
they were informed by their teachers that Pluto affect or mood but did not attend to the specific
was reclassified as a dwarf planet (Broughton, qualities of different kinds of affects. As will be
Sinatra, & Nussbaum, 2010). In contrast to detailed below, this implies that it may be diffi-
achievement and epistemic emotions, topic emo- cult and potentially misleading to use the findings
tions do not directly pertain to learning and prob- for explaining students’ emotions and learning in
lem-solving. However, they can strongly real-world academic contexts. Specifically, as
influence students’ engagement by affecting their argued both in Pekrun’s (1992a, 2006; Pekrun
interest and motivation in an academic domain et al., 2002a) cognitive/motivational model of
(Ainley, 2007). emotion effects and in Linnenbrink-Garcia’s
research on affect and engagement (Linnenbrink,
Social Emotions 2007; Linnenbrink-Garcia et al., 2011; Linnen
Academic learning is situated in social contexts. brink & Pintrich, 2004), it is not sufficient to
Even when learning alone, students do not act in differentiate positive from negative affective
a social vacuum; rather, the goals, contents, and states but imperative to also attend to the degree
outcomes of learning are socially constructed. By of activation implied. As such, the minimum nec-
implication, academic settings induce a multitude essary is to distinguish between the four groups
264 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

of emotions outlined earlier (positive activating, was first addressed in interference models of test
positive deactivating, negative activating, nega- anxiety, which posited that anxiety reduces per-
tive deactivating). For example, both anxiety and formance on complex and difficult tasks; this
hopelessness are negative (unpleasant) emotions; occurs because anxiety involves worries and pro-
however, their effects on students’ engagement duces task-irrelevant thoughts that interfere with
can differ dramatically, as anxiety can motivate a task completion (e.g., Eysenck, 1997; Wine,
student to invest effort in order to avoid failure, 1971; see Zeidner, 1998). For example, while
whereas hopelessness likely undermines any kind preparing for an exam, a student may fear failure
of engagement. Even within each of the four and worry about the consequences of failure,
categories, however, it may be necessary to fur- which in turn may distract her attention away
ther distinguish between distinct emotions. For from the task. Interference models of anxiety
example, both anxiety and anger are activating were expanded by H. Ellis’ resource allocation
negative emotions; however, paradoxically, model, which postulated that any negative emo-
whereas anxiety is associated with avoidance, tions can consume cognitive resources (Ellis &
anger is related to approach motivation (Carver & Ashbrook, 1988). Further expanding the perspec-
Harmon-Jones, 2009). tive, recent studies found that not only negative
Emotions can influence students’ engagement, emotions, but positive emotions as well can
which in turn impacts their academic learning reduce working memory resources and attention
and achievement. By implication, as suggested in (Meinhardt & Pekrun, 2003).
our earlier work (Linnenbrink, 2007; Linnenbrink However, the resource consumption effect likely
& Pintrich, 2004; Pekrun, 1992a, 2006), we is bound to emotions that have task-extraneous
regard engagement as a mediator between stu- objects and produce task-irrelevant thinking, such
dents’ emotions and their achievement. In the fol- as affective pictures in experimental mood research,
lowing sections, we first summarize research on or worries about impending failure on an exam in
the relation of emotions to the five types of test anxiety. In contrast, in task-related emotions
engagement outlined at the outset (i.e., cognitive, such as curiosity and enjoyment of learning, the
motivational, behavioral, cognitive-behavioral, task is the object of emotion. In positive task-related
and social-behavioral engagement). We then out- emotions, attention is focused on the task, and
line implications for the effects of different emo- working memory resources can be used for task
tions on students’ academic achievement. completion. However, it is possible that some posi-
tive task-related emotions, such as overexcitement,
may also distract attention away from the task.
Cognitive Engagement Corroborating these expectations, empirical stud-
ies with K-12 and university students found that
In our discussion of cognitive engagement, we negative emotions such as anger, anxiety, shame,
focus on cognitive processes related to attention, boredom, and hopelessness were associated with
mood-congruent memory recall, and memory task-irrelevant thinking and reduced flow, whereas
storage and retrieval implying active involve- enjoyment related negatively to irrelevant thinking
ment with academic tasks. Specifically, cognitive and positively to flow (Pekrun, Goetz, Daniels,
engagement refers to the way in which emotions Stupnisky, & Perry, 2010; Pekrun, Goetz, Perry,
shape cognitive resources and memory processes Kramer, & Hochstadt, 2004; Pekrun, Goetz, Titz,
that are activated automatically (for intentional & Perry, 2002b; Pekrun et al., 2002a; Zeidner,
and more complex cognitive processes, see the 1998). A similar pattern was observed with more
section on “Cognitive-behavioral engagement”). global measures of positive and negative affect for
college students (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2002a;
Attention and Flow Linnenbrink, Ryan, & Pintrich, 1999). These find-
Emotions consume cognitive resources (i.e., ings suggest that students’ emotions have pro-
resources of the working memory) by focusing found effects on their attentional engagement with
attention on the object of emotion. This effect academic tasks.
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 265

Mood-Congruent Memory Recall (Bäuml & Kuhbandner, 2007). Conversely, it can


Memory research has shown that emotions influ- be expected that positive emotions should facili-
ence storage and retrieval of information. Two tate retrieval-induced facilitation since they pro-
effects that are especially important for the aca- mote the relational processing of information
demic context are mood-congruent memory underlying such facilitation. However, the gener-
recall and retrieval-induced forgetting and facili- alizability of these laboratory findings to academic
tation. Mood-congruent retrieval (Parrott & learning is open to question. If these mechanisms
Spackman, 2000) implies that mood facilitates operate under natural conditions as well, they
the retrieval of like-valenced material, with posi- would imply that negative emotions can be help-
tive mood facilitating the retrieval of positive ful for learning lists of unrelated material (such as
self- and task-related information, and negative lists of foreign language vocabulary), whereas
mood facilitating the retrieval of negative infor- positive emotions should promote learning of
mation. Mood-congruent recall can impact stu- coherent material.
dents’ motivation. For example, positive mood
can foster positive self-appraisals and thus bene-
fit motivation to learn and performance; in con- Motivational Engagement
trast, negative mood can promote negative-self
appraisals and thus hamper motivation and per- Motivation refers to processes shaping goal direc-
formance (e.g., Olafson & Ferraro, 2001). tion, intensity, and persistence of behavior
(Heckhausen, 1991; Schunk, Pintrich, & Meece,
Retrieval-Induced Forgetting 2008). Given the active, energetic, and approach-
and Facilitation oriented role of these processes in both initiating
Retrieval-induced forgetting and facilitation are and sustaining goal-directed academic effort, it is
basic functional mechanisms of human learning important to consider motivation directed toward
that currently get widespread attention in cogni- task involvement as a form of engagement (for an
tive research. Retrieval-induced forgetting alternative perspective, see Appleton et al., 2008).
implies that practicing some learning material Of course, motivational engagement can in turn
impedes later retrieval of related material that shape other forms of engagement (e.g., behavioral,
was not practiced, presumably so because of cognitive, or cognitive-behavioral engagement),
inhibitory processes in memory networks. In and motivational processes such as interests and
contrast, retrieval-induced facilitation implies values may not always translate into actually
that practicing enhances memory for related but initiating and sustaining behavior. Nonetheless, it
unpracticed material (Chan, McDermott, & is useful to consider how emotions shape motiva-
Roediger, 2006). With learning material consist- tional engagement.
ing of disconnected elements, such as single As compared to cognitive effects, the effects
words, retrieval-induced forgetting has been of emotions on motivational engagement have
found to occur. For example, after learning a list been less well studied. However, emotion
of words, practicing half of the list can impede research traditionally assumed that specific emo-
memory for the other half. In contrast, facilita- tions function to trigger and facilitate impulses
tion has been shown to occur for connected mate- for specific action and thus play a role in initiat-
rials consisting of elements that show strong ing behaviors. Specifically, each of the major
interrelations. For example, after learning coher- negative emotions is associated with distinct
ent text material, practicing half of the material action impulses and serves to prepare the organ-
leads to better memory for the nonpracticed half. ism for action (or nonaction), such as fight, flight,
Emotions have been shown to influence and behavioral passivity in anger, anxiety, and
retrieval-induced forgetting. Specifically, negative hopelessness, respectively. For positive emo-
mood can undo forgetting, likely because it can tions, motivational consequences are less spe-
inhibit spreading activation in memory networks cific. Likely, one of the functions of positive
which underlies retrieval-induced forgetting emotions such as joy and interest is to motivate
266 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

exploratory behavior and an enlargement of one’s effects on different kinds of motivation, the
action repertoire, as addressed in Fredrickson’s effects of these emotions on students’ overall
(2001) broaden-and-build metaphor of positive motivation to learn can be variable as well.
emotions.
In the academic domain, emotions can pro-
foundly influence students’ motivational engage- Behavioral Engagement
ment. The little empirical evidence available to
date suggests that affect influences students’ Behavioral engagement refers to effort and per-
adoption of achievement goals, as addressed in sistence, with an emphasis on the amount or
Linnenbrink and Pintrich’s (2002b) bidirectional quantity of engagement rather than its quality
model of affect and achievement goals. (Fredricks et al., 2004; Pintrich, 2000). Several
Specifically, it has been shown that pleasant emo- psychological models suggest that positive affect
tions can have positive effects, and unpleasant leads to behavioral disengagement, either because
emotions negative effects, on undergraduate stu- one is progressing at a sufficient rate toward one’s
dents’ adoption of mastery-approach goals goals (Carver, Lawrence, & Scheier, 1996) or
(Daniels et al., 2009; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, because it signals that all is well and there is no
2002b). In line with this evidence, positive need to engage (Schwartz & Clore, 1996). Other
achievement emotions such as enjoyment of learn- models question this perspective and instead sug-
ing, hope, and pride have been shown to relate gest that positive affect frees resources away
positively to K-12 and university students’ inter- from a threat, allowing more expansive task-
est and intrinsic motivation, whereas negative related action (Fredrickson, 2001). Negative
emotions such as anger, anxiety, shame, hopeless- emotions such as sadness (for approach goals)
ness, and boredom related negatively to these and anxiety (for avoidance goals) may signal that
motivational variables (Helmke, 1993; Pekrun one is not making sufficient progress toward
et al., 2002a, 2002b, 2004, 2010; Zeidner, 1998). one’s goals or that there is a threat in the environ-
However, as addressed in Pekrun’s (1992a, ment, suggesting that they may also contribute to
2006) cognitive/motivational model of emotion intensified effort (Carver et al., 1996).
effects, motivational effects may be different for However, these perspectives do not consider
activating versus deactivating emotions. This the interplay between valence and activation and
model posits that activating positive emotions thus may not fully capture the way in which emo-
(e.g., joy, hope, pride) promote motivational tions shape behavioral engagement in academic
engagement, whereas deactivating emotions settings. As noted, activating versus deactivating
(e.g., hopelessness, boredom) undermine motiva- emotions can exert different effects on students’
tional engagement (Pekrun et al., 2010). In con- motivation. By implication, the effects on result-
trast, effects are posited to be more complex for ing effort and persistence can differ as well. There
deactivating positive emotions (e.g., relief, relax- is general support that positive activating emo-
ation) and activating negative emotions (e.g., tions such as enjoyment of learning are posi-
anger, anxiety, and shame). For example, relaxed tively associated with effort (Ainley, Corrigan, &
contentment following success can be expected Richardson, 2005; Efklides & Petkaki, 2005;
to reduce immediate motivation to reengage with Pekrun et al., 2002a, 2002b; Pekrun, Frenzel,
learning contents, but strengthen long-term moti- Goetz, & Perry, 2007), and that negative deacti-
vation to do so. Regarding activating negative vating emotions such as hopelessness and bore-
emotions, anger, anxiety, and shame have been dom are negatively associated with effort
found to reduce intrinsic motivation but strengthen (Linnenbrink, 2007; Pekrun et al., 2002a, 2010).
extrinsic motivation to invest effort in order to In contrast, effects have been shown to be more
avoid failure, especially so when expectations to variable for negative activating emotions such as
prevent failure and attain success are favorable anger, anxiety, and shame. These emotions often
(Turner & Schallert, 2001). Due to these variable show negative overall correlations with effort,
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 267

but in some cases, they may support behavioral cautious ways. Furthermore, positive emotions
engagement as they can serve to energize stu- may facilitate flexible problem-solving via increas-
dents (Linnenbrink, 2007; Pekrun et al., 2002a; ing brain dopamine levels (Ashby, Isen, & Turken,
Turner & Schallert, 2001). 1999), and negative moods may promote effort
investment and performance on analytical tasks by
inducing a need for “mood repair” (e.g., Schaller
Cognitive-Behavioral Engagement & Cialdini, 1990).

Cognitive-behavioral engagement refers to com- Learning Strategies


plex cognitive processes that are intentionally Judging from the experimental evidence on
instigated by the learner, including cognitive prob- problem-solving, positive activating emotions
lem-solving, use of cognitive and metacognitive such as enjoyment of learning should facilitate
learning strategies, and self-regulation of learning. use of flexible, holistic learning strategies like
These processes are similar to what Fredricks elaboration and organization of learning material
et al. (2004) referred to as cognitive engagement. or critical thinking. Negative emotions, on the
We use the term cognitive-behavioral engagement other hand, should sustain more rigid, detail-
to differentiate these processes both from auto- oriented learning, like simple rehearsal of learn-
matic cognitive processes described earlier and ing material. Correlational evidence from studies
from pure quantity of effort as reflected by behav- with university students generally supports this
ioral engagement. view (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2002a; Pekrun
et al., 2002a, 2004). However, for deactivating
Problem-Solving positive and negative emotions, these effects may
Experimental mood research has shown that be less pronounced. Deactivating emotions, like
positive and negative moods impact problem- relaxation or boredom, may produce shallow
solving. Specifically, experimental evidence sug- information processing rather than any more
gests that positive mood promotes flexible, intensive use of learning strategies.
creative, and holistic ways of solving problems
and a reliance on generalized, heuristic knowl- Metastrategies and Self-Regulation
edge structures (Fredrickson, 2001; Isen, Self-regulation of learning includes the use of
Daubman & Nowicki, 1987). Conversely, nega- metacognitive, metamotivational, and metaemo-
tive mood has been found to promote focused, tional strategies (Wolters, 2003) making it possi-
detail-oriented, and analytical ways of thinking ble to adopt goals, monitor and regulate learning
(Clore & Huntsinger, 2007, 2009). A number activities, and evaluate their results in flexible
of theoretical explanations have been proffered ways, such that learning activities can be adapted
for these findings. For example, in mood-as- to the demands of academic tasks. An application
information approaches, it is assumed that posi- of these strategies presupposes cognitive flexibil-
tive affective states signal that all is well (e.g., ity. Therefore, it can be assumed that positive
sufficient goal progress), whereas negative states emotions foster self-regulation and the implied
signal that something is wrong (e.g., insufficient use of metastrategies, whereas negative emotions
goal progress; e.g., Bless et al., 1996). “All is can motivate the individual to rely on external
well” conditions imply safety and the discretion guidance. Correlational evidence from studies
to creatively explore the environment, broaden with university students is generally in line with
one’s cognitive horizon, and build new actions, these propositions (Linnenbrink & Pintrich,
as addressed by Fredrickson’s broaden-and-build 2002a; Pekrun et al., 2002a, 2004, 2010). However,
theory of positive emotions. In contrast, “all is the reverse causal direction may also play a role in
not well” conditions may imply a threat to well- producing such correlations—self-regulated
being and agency, thus making it necessary to learning may instigate enjoyment, and external
focus on these problems in analytical, cognitively directions for learning may trigger anxiety.
268 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

Social-Behavioral Engagement research involving small groups and class discus-


sion. This research generally suggests that posi-
With the growing emphasis on constructivist tive emotions such as feeling happy or calm
forms of learning, student-student interactions promote social-behavioral engagement including
have become increasingly important in shaping active listening, supporting one’s peers, and
students’ learning and achievement. Within these increasing group cohesion, while negative deacti-
settings, students must engage socially with their vating states, such as feeling tired, undermine
peers. This type of social engagement includes engagement (Bramesfeld & Gasper, 2008; Do &
behavioral engagement, such as engaging in dis- Schallert, 2004; Linnenbrink-Garcia et al., 2011).
cussion or listening to one’s peers (Fredricks et al., Linnenbrink-Garcia et al. (2011) also found that
2004), but it can also include higher-order quality both activated (tense) and deactivated (tired) neg-
forms of social participation such as working ative affective states were associated with
cohesively, respectfully, and supporting other stu- decreased social-behavioral engagement in the
dents’ learning. Thus, we use the term social- form of social loafing or allowing the other stu-
behavioral engagement to refer to a range of social dents during small group work to do all the work.
forms of engagement around academic tasks Moreover, within small group settings, negative
including participation with peers as well as emotions seemed to sustain negative cycles of
higher-quality social interactions (Linnenbrink- group interactions such as disrespecting other
Garcia et al., 2011). Social-behavioral engagement group members and discouraging their participa-
is distinct from other forms of engagement such tion. However, this research also suggests that the
as emotional engagement, which is focused more interplay between emotions and social-behavioral
on students’ emotions in relation to learning tasks, engagement is complex, such that negative emo-
and on feelings of belonging which refer to a sense tions can at times support rather than undermine
of general connectedness with peers, teachers, social-behavioral engagement (Do & Schallert,
or the school (see Appleton et al., 2008). In this way, 2004; Linnenbrink-Garcia et al., 2011).
social-behavioral engagement includes support
for high-quality social interactions that directly Online Interaction
facilitate students’ engagement and learning Studies analyzing online discussions and group
within peer-to-peer learning contexts through work also suggest that emotions and social
collaboration. engagement are related (Nummenmaa &
Instructional settings that require interactions Nummenmaa, 2008; Vuorela & Nummenmaa,
with peers may present unique emotional chal- 2004; Wosnitza & Volet, 2005). For example,
lenges and evoke strong emotional responses in a study of undergraduates working in an
(Crook, 2000; Do & Schallert, 2004; Jarvenoja & asynchronous web environment (e.g., students
Jarvela, 2009; Linnenbrink-Garcia et al., 2011; post comments and discuss ideas but are not
Wosnitza & Volet, 2005). This is not surprising, required to interact in real time), social interac-
especially given the key role that social agents tions were more likely to evoke emotional
play in shaping emotions across time (Denzin, responses, as compared with other aspects of
1984; Frenzel, Goetz, Lüdtke, Pekrun, & Sutton, the learning environment such as the online
2009; Schutz, Hong, Cross, & Osbon, 2006). As web program or the technology (Vuorela &
such, we consider the interplay between emo- Nummenmaa). There was no relation between
tions and social-behavioral engagement both in mean levels of emotion with social-behavioral
terms of direct peer-to-peer interactions as well engagement; however, students who had more
as online peer interactions. variability (ranging from pleasant to unpleasant
emotions) were found to engage more in the
Direct Interaction online exchange.
There is growing evidence that emotions relate to In sum, there is growing evidence that emotions
social-behavioral engagement in direct peer are related to social-behavioral engagement when
interaction, in both laboratory and field-based students work with their peers on academic tasks.
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 269

Broadly speaking, positive emotions seem to The evidence cited earlier suggests that enjoyment
support social-behavioral engagement, while neg- preserves cognitive resources and focuses atten-
ative emotions can undermine it. However, with tion on the task; promotes relational processing
social-behavioral engagement as well, it is impor- of information; induces intrinsic motivation; and
tant to note that the nature of these relations is facilitates use of flexible learning strategies and
complex, suggesting the need to consider recip- self-regulation, thus likely exerting positive
rocal and cyclical relations between emotions effects on overall performance under many task
and social-behavioral engagement. conditions. In contrast, deactivating positive emo-
tions, such as relief and relaxation, can reduce
task attention; can have variable motivational
Academic Achievement effects by undermining current motivation while
at the same time reinforcing motivation to reen-
Since many different mechanisms of engagement gage with the task; and can lead to superficial
can contribute to the functional effects of emo- information processing, thus likely making effects
tions, the overall effects on students’ academic on overall achievement more variable.
achievement are inevitably complex and may Related empirical evidence is scarce, but sup-
depend on the interplay between different mech- ports the view that activating positive emotions
anisms, as well as between these mechanisms can enhance achievement. Specifically, enjoy-
and task demands. Nevertheless, it seems possi- ment of learning was found to correlate moder-
ble to derive inferences from the existing evi- ately positively with K-12 and college students’
dence and the above considerations. academic performance (Helmke, 1993; Pekrun
et al., 2002a, 2002b). Furthermore, students’
Positive Emotions enjoyment, hope, and pride correlated positively
Traditionally it was assumed that positive emo- with college students’ interest, effort invested in
tions, notwithstanding their potential to foster cre- studying, elaboration of learning material, and
ativity, are often maladaptive for performance as a self-regulation of learning, in line with the view
result of inducing unrealistically positive apprais- that these activating positive emotions can be
als triggered by mood-congruent retrieval, foster- beneficial for students’ academic agency (Pekrun
ing nonanalytical information processing, and et al., 2002a, 2002b). Consistent with evidence
making effort expenditure seem unnecessary by on discrete emotions, general positive affect has
signaling that everything is going well (Aspinwall, also been found to correlate positively with stu-
1998; Pekrun et al., 2002b). From this perspec- dents’ cognitive engagement (Linnenbrink,
tive, “our primary goal is to feel good, and feel- 2007). However, some studies have found null
ing good makes us lazy thinkers who are oblivious relations between activating positive emotions
to potentially useful negative information and (or affect) and individual engagement and
unresponsive to meaningful variations in infor- achievement (Linnenbrink, 2007; Pekrun, Elliot,
mation and situation” (Aspinwall, 1998, p. 7). & Maier, 2009). Also, caution should be exer-
However, as noted, positive mood has typi- cised in interpreting the reported correlations.
cally been regarded as a unitary construct in Linkages between emotions and achievement are
experimental mood research. As argued earlier, likely due not only to performance effects of
such a view is inadequate because it fails to dis- emotions, but also to effects of performance
tinguish between activating versus deactivating attainment on emotions, implying reciprocal
moods and emotions. As detailed in Pekrun’s rather than unidirectional causation.
(2006) cognitive/motivational model, deactivat-
ing positive emotions, like relief or relaxation, Negative Activating Emotions
may well have the negative performance effects As noted, emotions such as anger, anxiety, and
described for positive mood, whereas activating shame produce task-irrelevant thinking, thus
positive emotions, such as task-related enjoy- reducing cognitive resources available for task
ment or pride, should have positive effects. purposes, and undermine students’ intrinsic
270 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

motivation. On the other hand, these emotions 2001). Similarly, while achievement-related anger
can induce motivation to avoid failure and facili- correlated negatively with academic performance
tate the use of more rigid learning strategies. By in a few studies (Boekaerts, 1993; Pekrun et al.,
implication, the effects on resulting academic 2004), the underlying mechanisms may be com-
performance depend on task conditions and may plex and imply more than just negative effects. In
well be variable, similar to the proposed effects a study by Lane, Whyte, Terry, and Nevill (2005),
of positive deactivating emotions. The available depressed mood interacted with anger experi-
evidence supports this position. enced before an academic exam, such that anger
Specifically, it has been shown that anxiety was related to improved performance in students
impairs performance on complex or difficult tasks who reported no depressive mood symptoms—
that demand cognitive resources, such as difficult presumably because they were able to maintain
intelligence test items, whereas performance on motivation and invest necessary effort. In sum,
easy, less complex, and repetitive tasks may not the findings for anxiety, shame, and anger support
suffer or is even enhanced (Hembree, 1988; the notion that performance effects of negative
Zeidner, 1998, 2007). In line with experimental activating emotions are complex, although rela-
findings, field studies have shown that test anxi- tionships with overall performance are negative
ety correlates moderately negatively with stu- for many task conditions and students.
dents’ academic performance. Typically, 5–10%
of the variance in students’ achievement scores is Negative Deactivating Emotions
explained by self-reported anxiety (Hembree, In contrast to negative activating emotions, nega-
1988; Zeidner, 1998). Again, in explaining the tive deactivating emotions, such as boredom and
correlational evidence, reciprocal causation of hopelessness, are posited to uniformly impair
emotion and performance has to be considered. performance by reducing cognitive resources,
Linkages between test anxiety and achievement undermining both intrinsic and extrinsic motiva-
may be caused by effects of success and failure tion, and promoting superficial information pro-
on the development of test anxiety, in addition to cessing (Pekrun, 2006). However, in spite of the
effects of anxiety on achievement. The scarce frequency of boredom experienced by many indi-
longitudinal evidence available suggests that test viduals in school today, this emotion has received
anxiety and students’ achievement are in fact scant attention, as has the less frequent, but dev-
linked by reciprocal causation across school years astating emotion of achievement-related hope-
(Meece, Wigfield, & Eccles, 1990; Pekrun, lessness. An exception is experimental research
1992b). Furthermore, correlations with perfor- on boredom induced by very simple, repetitive
mance variables have not been uniformly nega- tasks, such as assembly-line, vigilance, or data
tive across studies. Zero and positive correlations entry tasks. Boredom was found to reduce perfor-
have sometimes been found, in line with our view mance on these tasks (Fisher, 1993). In educa-
that anxiety can exert ambiguous effects. Anxiety tion, boredom has been discussed as being
likely has deleterious effects in many students, experienced by gifted students (Sisk, 1988). The
but it may facilitate overall performance in those little evidence available corroborates that bore-
who are more resilient and can productively use dom and hopelessness relate uniformly nega-
the motivational energy provided by anxiety. tively to students’ achievement, in line with
Few studies have addressed the effects of neg- theoretical expectations (Goetz, Frenzel, Pekrun,
ative activating emotions other than anxiety. Hall, & Lüdtke, 2007; Maroldo, 1986; Pekrun
Similar to anxiety, shame related to failure showed et al., 2002a, 2004, 2010).
negative overall correlations with college stu- In sum, theoretical expectations, the evidence
dents’ academic achievement and negatively pre- produced by experimental studies, and findings
dicted their exam performance (Pekrun et al., from field studies imply that students’ emotions
2004, 2009). However, as with anxiety, shame have profound effects on their engagement and
likely exerts variable effects (Turner & Schallert, academic achievement. As such, administrators
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 271

and educators should pay attention to the Test Anxiety Research


emotions experienced by students. Most likely, Test anxiety studies were the first to address the
the effects of students’ enjoyment of learning are appraisal antecedents of students’ emotions. In
beneficial, whereas hopelessness and boredom these studies, appraisals concerning threat of fail-
are detrimental for engagement. The effects of ure have been addressed as causing anxiety. In
emotions like anger, anxiety, or shame are more terms of R. S. Lazarus’ transactional stress model
complex, but for the average student, these emo- (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), threat in a given
tions also have negative overall effects. achievement setting is evaluated in terms of the
likelihood and subjective importance of failure
(“primary appraisal”) and in terms of possibili-
Origins of Academic Emotions ties to cope with this threat (“secondary
appraisal”). A student may experience anxiety
Given the relevance of students’ emotions for when her primary appraisal indicates that failure
their engagement, it pays to analyze their origins on an important exam is likely, and when her sec-
as well. While a more detailed review of the lit- ondary appraisal indicates that this threat is not
erature is beyond the scope of this chapter, we sufficiently controllable. Empirical research con-
provide a short overview of current research in firms that test anxiety is closely related to per-
this section (for more comprehensive treatments, ceived lack of control over performance.
see Schutz & Pekrun, 2007; Zeidner, 1998). We Specifically, numerous studies have shown that
first address appraisals and achievement goals as K-12 and postsecondary students’ self-concept of
individual antecedents of students’ emotions, and ability, self-efficacy expectations, and academic
subsequently the role of learning tasks and social control beliefs correlate negatively with their test
environments. anxiety (Hembree, 1988; Pekrun et al., 2004;
Zeidner, 1998).

Appraisals as Proximal Antecedents Attributional Theory


In attributional theories explaining emotions fol-
Generally, emotions can be caused and modu- lowing success and failure, perceived control
lated by numerous individual factors, including plays a central role as well. In B. Weiner’s
situational perceptions, cognitive appraisals (1985, 2007) approach, attributions of success
and emotion schemata, neurohormonal processes, and failure to various causes are held to be pri-
and sensory feedback from facial, gestural, and mary determinants of these emotions, except
postural expression (Davidson, Scherer, & Gold- “attribution-independent” emotions which are
smith, 2003; Scherer, Schorr, & Johnstone, 2001). directly instigated by perceptions of success or
However, the emotions experienced in an aca- failure (happiness and sadness/frustration for
demic context pertain to culturally defined success and failure, respectively). Pride is
demands in settings that are a recent product of assumed to be aroused by attributions of success
civilization. In these settings, the individual has to internal causes (i.e., causes located within the
to learn how to adapt to situational demands while person, such as ability and effort). Shame is seen
preserving individual autonomy—inevitably a to be instigated by failure attributed to internal
process guided by appraisals. As such, cognitive causes that are uncontrollable (like lack of abil-
appraisals of task demands, personal compe- ity), and gratitude and anger by attributions of
tences, the probability of success and failure, success and failure, respectively, to external
and the value of these outcomes likely play a causes that are under control by others. The sta-
major role in the arousal of academic emotions, bility of perceived causes is posited to be impor-
and research on the determinants of academic tant for hopefulness and hopelessness regarding
emotions from early on has focused on such future performance. Findings from scenario stud-
appraisals. ies asking students how they, or others, might
272 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

react to success and failure were largely in line also seen to be induced by appraisals of control
with Weiner’s propositions, as were findings and value.
from correlational field studies (Heckhausen, Regarding activity emotions, enjoyment of
1991; Weiner, 1985). achievement activities is proposed to depend on a
combination of positive competence appraisals
Control-Value Theory and positive appraisals of the intrinsic value of
While test anxiety theories and attributional the action (e.g., studying) and its reference objects
theories have addressed outcome emotions per- (e.g., learning material). For example, a student
taining to success and failure, they have neglected is expected to enjoy learning if she feels compe-
activity-related emotions. In Pekrun’s (2006; tent to meet the demands of the learning task and
Pekrun et al., 2007) control-value theory of values the learning material. If she feels incom-
achievement emotions, core propositions of the petent, or is disinterested in the material, study-
transactional stress model and attributional theo- ing is not enjoyable. Anger and frustration are
ries are revised and expanded to explain a broader aroused when the intrinsic value of the activity is
variety of emotions. The theory posits that negative (e.g., when working on a difficult proj-
achievement emotions are induced when the indi- ect is perceived as taking too much effort which
vidual feels in control of, or out of control of, is experienced as aversive). Finally, boredom is
achievement activities and outcomes that are sub- experienced when the activity lacks any intrinsic
jectively important—implying that appraisals of incentive value (Pekrun et al., 2010).
control and value are the proximal determinants
of these emotions (e.g., Goetz, Frenzel, Stoeger, Nonreflective Induction of Emotions
& Hall, 2010). Control appraisals pertain to the Importantly, emotions need not always be medi-
perceived controllability of actions and outcomes, ated by conscious appraisals. Rather, recurring
as implied by related causal expectations (self- appraisal-based induction of emotions can become
efficacy expectations and outcome expectations), automatic and nonreflective over time. When aca-
causal attributions, and competence appraisals. demic activities are repeated over and over again,
Value appraisals relate to the subjective impor- appraisals and the induction of emotions can
tance of these activities and outcomes. become routinized to the extent that there is no
Different combinations of control and value longer any conscious mediation of emotions—or
appraisals are proposed to instigate different no longer any cognitive mediation at all
achievement emotions. Prospective, anticipatory (Reisenzein, 2001). In the procedural emotion
joy and hopelessness are expected to be triggered schemata established by routinization, situation
when there is high perceived control (joy) or a perception and emotion are directly linked such
complete lack of perceived control (hopeless- that perceptions can automatically induce the emo-
ness). For example, a student who believes he has tion (e.g., the mere smell of a chemistry lab induc-
the necessary resources to get an A+ on an impor- ing joy). However, when the situation changes or
tant exam may feel joyous about the prospect of attempts are made to change the emotion (as in
receiving such a grade. Conversely, if he believes psychotherapy), appraisals come into play again.
he is incapable of preventing to fail the exam, he
may experience hopelessness. Prospective hope
and anxiety are instigated when there is uncer- The Role of Achievement-Related Goals
tainty about control, the attentional focus being and Orientations
on anticipated success in the case of hope, and
on anticipated failure in the case of anxiety. For To the extent that cognitive appraisals are proxi-
example, a student who is unsure about being mal determinants of achievement emotions, more
able to master an important exam may hope for distal individual antecedents, such as gender or
success, fear failure, or both. Similarly, retro- achievement-related beliefs, should affect these
spective pride, shame, gratitude, and anger are emotions by first influencing appraisals (Fig. 12.2;
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 273

Engagement +
Environment Appraisal Emotion Achievement

Cognitive Quality Engagement


Task Demands Achievement - Cognitive
(e.g., cognitive Emotions
Motivational Quality - Motivational
incongruity)
Epistemic - Behavioral
Autonomy Support
Control Emotions - Cognitive-
Goal Structures + behavioral
Topic - Social-
Expectations Value
Emotions behavioral
Achievement Goal Social
- Feedback Attainment Emotions
- Consequences
Achievement

Achievement Goals Genes Intelligence


Beliefs Temperament Competences

Competence-
Appraisal-oriented Emotion-oriented oriented
Design of Tasks Regulation Regulation Regulation
and Learning
Environments
Cognitive Treatment Emotion-oriented Competence
Treatment Training

Fig. 12.2 Reciprocal causation of academic emotions, engagement, and their antecedents and outcomes

Pekrun, 2006). This can also be assumed for the (Elliot, 2005), with different goals being related
influence of achievement-related goals and goal to different definitions of achievement. In mas-
orientations which are thought to direct atten- tery goals, achievement is judged by intraindi-
tional focus in the course of achievement activi- vidual standards or absolute criteria; in
ties. Specifically, these goals and orientations performance goals, achievement is judged by
provide a lens through which individuals inter- normative standards comparing performance
pret and respond to achievement-related settings across individuals. Achievement goal orienta-
(Dweck & Leggett, 1988). Achievement goals tions are broader cognitive schemas that comprise
can be defined as the competence-relevant aims achievement goals as well as associated reasons
that individuals strive for in achievement settings to pursue these goals (Maehr & Zusho, 2009;
274 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

Pintrich, 2000). Mastery goal orientations focus & Hill, 2009) bidirectional model of goals and
on developing competence and learning, whereas affect proposes that mastery goals promote per-
performance goal orientations focus on demon- ceptions of progress toward success since prog-
strating competence, often in relation to the oth- ress is judged relative to one’s own improvement,
ers (Dweck & Leggett). Researchers have also thus facilitating emotions such as elation and hap-
proposed that these primary goals and orienta- piness. Performance-approach goals are thought
tions can be further differentiated into approach to promote emotions such as sadness for the many
and avoidance dimensions (Elliot, 1999; Elliot & individuals who perceive insufficient progress
McGregor, 2001; Pintrich, 2000). In this way, toward success due to competition with others,
individuals can strive toward success or away and happiness for those who do perceive suffi-
from failure, resulting in four possible goals and cient progress; performance-avoidance goals pro-
goal orientations (mastery-approach, mastery- mote perceptions of moving away from or toward
avoidance, performance-approach, performance- failure, thus facilitating relief or anxiety, respec-
avoidance; for a recent revision of this framework, tively. Both performance-approach and perfor-
see Elliot, Murayama, & Pekrun, 2011). mance-avoidance goals are proposed to be
As achievement-related goals and goal orien- associated with anxiety, due to the heightened
tations are central to achievement motivation focus on the self. As such, performance-approach
(Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Elliot & McGregor, goal orientations in particular should be associ-
2001; Nicholls, 1984), understanding their rela- ated with a range of emotions including elation,
tions with emotions is of specific importance for happiness, sadness, and anxiety, depending both
explaining students’ engagement. The relation on perceived progress and the salience of the self.
can be explained by assuming that different goals Overall, the predictions derived from the two
and orientations focus attention on different models are complementary and largely consistent,
aspects of current academic activities, thus pro- with few exceptions such as differences in the
moting different kinds of appraisals. Specifically, proposed links for hopelessness and sadness (see
goals can promote appraisals of the controllabil- Pekrun & Stephens, 2009; Tyson et al., 2009).
ity and value of achievement, and of the rate of The available evidence corroborates that stu-
progress toward goal attainment. Furthermore, dents’ goals affect their emotions. Relations
they can differentially focus the individual on the between achievement goals and omnibus vari-
task versus the self. ables of general positive and negative affect
In terms of controllability and value, Pekrun’s tend to lack consistency (Linnenbrink &
(2006; Pekrun, Elliot, & Maier, 2006; Pekrun et al., Pintrich, 2002b; Pekrun et al., 2006, 2009);
2009) control-value theory implies that mastery however, there are fairly clear linkages with dis-
goals should focus attention on the controllability crete achievement emotions, especially for mas-
and positive values of task activities, thus promot- tery and performance-avoidance goals. The
ing positive activity emotions such as enjoyment of relation between performance-avoidance goals
learning and reducing negative activity emotions and test anxiety is best documented, but recent
such as boredom. Performance-approach goals research also shows consistent relations for
should focus attention on the controllability and mastery goals and activity emotions (positive
positive values of success, thus facilitating positive for enjoyment, negative for boredom) and for
outcome emotions such as hope and pride, and performance goals and outcome emotions other
performance-avoidance goals should focus atten- than anxiety, such as pride, shame, and hope-
tion on the uncontrollability and negative value of lessness (Daniels et al., 2009; Linnenbrink,
failure, thus inducing negative outcome emotions 2007; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2002b;
such as anxiety, shame, and hopelessness. Mouratidis, Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Auweele,
In terms of the rate of progress toward goal 2009; Pekrun et al., 2006, 2009). The close
attainment, Linnenbrink and Pintrich’s (2002b; relation between achievement-related goals and
Linnenbrink, 2007; Tyson, Linnenbrink-Garcia, subsequent emotions also implies that emotions
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 275

can function as mediators of the effects of tive feedback after performance, and negative
achievement goals on engagement and achieve- consequences of poor performance (e.g., public
ment. For example, in research by Linnenbrink humiliation) show moderate to strong positive
et al. (1999), general negative affect was a correlations with students’ test anxiety (Pekrun,
mediator of mastery goal effects on task perfor- 1992b; Zeidner, 1998). Also, individual competi-
mance. Similarly, in studies by Elliot and tion in classrooms is positively related to stu-
McGregor (1999) and Pekrun et al. (2009), per- dents’ anxiety, presumably because competition
formance-avoidance goals predicted anxiety reduces expectancies for success and increases
which in turn was a negative predictor of the importance of avoiding failure (Wigfield &
achievement, implying that anxiety mediated Eccles, 1990). In contrast, in K-12 research,
the effects of performance-avoidance goals on social support from parents and teachers and a
achievement. cooperative classroom climate have been found
to be uncorrelated with students’ test anxiety
scores (Hembree, 1988). Negative feedback
The Influence of Tasks loops of support and anxiety may account for this
and Environments surprising noncorrelation. Social support can
alleviate anxiety (negative effect of support on
The impact of task design and learning environ- anxiety), but anxiety can provoke support in the
ments on students’ emotions is largely unex- first place (positive effect of anxiety on support),
plored, with the exception of research on the thus yielding an overall zero correlation.
antecedents of test anxiety (see Wigfield & The quality of tasks, expectations from sig-
Eccles, 1990; Zeidner, 1998, 2007) and task nificant others, and functional importance of
interest/enjoyment (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 1987). achievement likely influence academic emotions
Lack of structure and clarity in classroom instruc- other than anxiety as well. Related evidence is
tion and exams, as well as excessively high task largely lacking to date. The following factors
demands, relate positively to students’ test anxi- may be relevant for a broad variety of academic
ety. These effects are likely mediated by students’ emotions (see Fig. 12.2).
perceptions of low control and resulting expec-
tancies of failure (Pekrun, 1992b). Furthermore, Cognitive Quality
the format of tasks has been found to be relevant. The cognitive quality of classroom instruction
Open-ended formats (e.g., essay questions) seem and tasks as defined by their structure, clarity, and
to induce more anxiety than multiple-choice for- potential for cognitive stimulation likely has a
mats, likely due to higher working memory positive influence on perceived competence and
demands which are difficult to meet when mem- the perceived value of tasks (e.g., Cordova &
ory capacity is used for worrying about failure Lepper, 1996), thus positively influencing stu-
(Shaha, 1984; Zeidner, 1987). In contrast, giving dents’ emotions and engagement. Specifically,
individuals the choice between tasks, relaxing the cognitive quality of tasks in terms of inducing
time constraints, and giving second chances in appropriate levels of cognitive incongruity may
terms of retaking tests have been found to reduce be of primary importance for the arousal of
test anxiety, presumably so because perceived epistemic emotions such as surprise and curios-
control is enhanced under these conditions ity. In addition, the relative difficulty of tasks can
(Zeidner, 1998). These findings are in line with influence perceived control, and the match
research demonstrating that task structures that between task demands and competences can
function to promote autonomy and a sense of influence subjective task value, thus also influ-
control are positively related to intrinsic motiva- encing emotions. If demands are too high or too
tion, cognitive flexibility, positive affect, and low, the incentive value of tasks may be reduced
well-being (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 1987). to the extent that boredom is experienced (Acee
Regarding social environments, high achieve- et al., 2010; Csikszentmihalyi, 1975; Pekrun
ment expectancies from important others, nega- et al., 2010).
276 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

Motivational Quality goal structures imply, by definition, that some


Teachers and peers deliver both direct and indi- individuals have to experience failure, thus induc-
rect messages conveying academic values. Two ing negative outcome emotions such as anxiety
ways of inducing emotionally relevant values in and hopelessness in these individuals. Similarly,
indirect ways may be most important. First, if the demands implied by an important other’s unre-
tasks and environments are shaped such that they alistic expectancies for achievement can lead to
meet students’ needs, positive activity-related negative emotions resulting from reduced subjec-
emotions should be fostered. For example, learn- tive control.
ing environments that support cooperation should
help students fulfill their needs for social related- Feedback and Consequences
ness, thus making working on academic tasks of Achievement
more enjoyable and promoting their social Cumulative success can strengthen perceived
engagement as discussed earlier. Second, teach- control, and cumulative failure can undermine
ers’ own enthusiasm in dealing with tasks can control. In environments involving frequent
facilitate the adoption of achievement values and assessments, performance feedback is likely of
related emotions (Frenzel et al., 2009; Turner, primary importance for the arousal of academic
Meyer, Midgley, & Patrick, 2003). Observational emotions. In addition, the perceived consequences
learning and emotional contagion may be prime of success and failure are important, since these
mechanisms mediating these effects (Hatfield, consequences affect the instrumental value of
Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994). achievement outcomes. Positive outcome emo-
tions (e.g., hope for success) can be increased if
Autonomy Support success produces beneficial long-term outcomes
Tasks and environments supporting autonomy can (e.g., future career opportunities) and provided
increase perceived control and, by meeting needs sufficient contingency between one’s own efforts,
for autonomy, the value of related achievement success, and these outcomes. Negative conse-
activities (Tsai, Kunter, Lüdtke, Trautwein, & quences of failure (e.g., unemployment), on the
Ryan, 2008). However, these beneficial effects other hand, may increase achievement-related
likely depend on the match between individual anxiety and hopelessness (Pekrun, 1992b).
competences and needs for academic autonomy, In sum, individual antecedents as well as
on the one hand, and the affordances of these envi- social environments and academic tasks shape
ronments, on the other. In case of a mismatch, loss students’ academic emotions and, consequently,
of control and negative emotions could result. any emotion-dependent engagement with learn-
ing. Environments, goals, and appraisals can
Goal Structures and Social Expectations induce, prevent, and modulate students’ emo-
Different standards for defining achievement can tions, and they can shape their objects and con-
imply individualistic (mastery), competitive (nor- tents. Depending on individual goals and the
mative performance), or cooperative goal struc- learning environment provided, students’ aca-
tures (Johnson & Johnson, 1974). The goal demic life can be infused with positive affect and
structures provided in academic settings conceiv- joyful task engagement, or with anxiety, frustra-
ably influence emotions in two ways. First, to the tion, and boredom. However, the strong impact
extent that these structures are adopted, they influ- of tasks and the social environment does not
ence individual achievement goals (Murayama & imply that basic mechanisms linking students’
Elliot, 2009) and any emotions influenced by these emotions with their engagement vary as a func-
goals (Kaplan & Maehr, 1999; Roeser, Midgley, & tion of task and social context. Rather, these
Urdan, 1996). Second, goal structures determine mechanisms seem to be pretty stable across con-
relative opportunities for experiencing success texts (Pekrun, 2009). For example, concerning
and perceiving control, thus influencing control- the context provided by different task domains,
dependent emotions. Specifically, competitive students’ emotions experienced in mathematics,
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 277

science, and languages differed in mean levels Creative, self-directed involvement with tasks
across domains, but showed equivalent internal may in turn promote students’ enjoyment,
structures and linkages with academic achieve- suggesting that students’ enjoyment and their
ment across domains in recent research with high strategy use are reciprocally linked. Similarly,
school students (Goetz et al., 2007). Similarly, in emotions influence students’ motivational engage-
a cross-cultural comparison of Chinese and ment in terms of adopting various achievement
German high school students’ emotions in math- goals, but these goals reciprocally influence stu-
ematics, Frenzel, Thrash, Pekrun, and Goetz dents’ emotions (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2002b).
(2007) found that mean levels of emotions dif- Third, by impacting engagement, students’ emo-
fered between cultures, with Chinese students tions have an influence on their achievement.
reporting more achievement-related enjoyment, Academic achievement outcomes and feedback
pride, anxiety, and shame, and less anger in math- on these outcomes, however, are primary forces
ematics. However, the functional linkages of shaping students’ emotions, again suggesting
these emotions with perceived control, important reciprocal causation.
others’ expectations, and academic achievement In line with perspectives of dynamical systems
in mathematics were equivalent across cultures. theory (Turner & Waugh, 2007), it is assumed
Most likely, the general functions of emotions for that such reciprocal causation can take different
students’ engagement and achievement described forms and can extend over fractions of seconds
earlier are universal across different task domains, (e.g., in linkages between appraisals and emo-
social environments, and cultural contexts. tions), days, weeks, months, or years. Positive
feedback loops likely are commonplace (e.g., in
reciprocal linkages between teachers’ and stu-
Reciprocal Causation, Emotion dents’ enjoyment as cited earlier), but negative
Regulation, and Therapy feedback loops can also be important (e.g., when
failure on an exam induces anxiety in a student,
Academic emotions influence students’ engage- and anxiety motivates the student to successfully
ment and achievement, but achievement out- avoid failure on the next exam).
comes are expected to reciprocally influence Reciprocal causation has implications for the
appraisals, emotions, and the environment regulation of academic emotions, for the treat-
(Pekrun, 2006; see Fig. 12.2). As such, academic ment of excessively negative emotions, and for
emotions, their antecedents, and their effects are the design of “emotionally sound” (Astleitner,
thought to be linked by reciprocal causation over 2000) learning environments. Since emotions,
time. Reciprocal causation may involve a number their antecedents, and their effects can be recipro-
of feedback loops, including the following three cally linked over time, emotions can be regulated
that may be especially important. First, learning and changed by addressing any of the elements
environments shape students’ appraisals and emo- involved in these cyclic feedback processes.
tions, as argued earlier, but these emotions recip- Regulation and treatment can target (a) the emo-
rocally affect students’ learning environments tion itself (emotion-oriented regulation and treat-
and the behavior of teachers and classmates. For ment, such as using drugs and relaxation
example, teachers’ and students’ enjoyment of techniques to cope with anxiety or employing
classroom instruction are likely linked in recipro- interest-enhancing strategies to reduce boredom;
cal ways, emotional contagion being one of the Sansone, Weir, Harpster, & Morgan, 1992); (b)
mechanisms producing these links (see Frenzel the control and value appraisals underlying
et al., 2009). Second, emotions impact students’ emotions (appraisal-oriented regulation and treat-
engagement, and engagement affects students’ ment; e.g., attributional retraining, Ruthig, Perry,
emotions. For example, enjoyment of learning Hall, & Hladkyj, 2004); (c) the competences
can facilitate students’ self-regulation and use of determining individual agency (competence-
creative learning strategies, as outlined earlier. oriented regulation and treatment; e.g., training of
278 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

learning skills); and (d) tasks and learning envi- classroom. Given the close proximity of epistemic
ronments (design of tasks and environments). and social emotions to the learning activity itself,
Emotion regulation and ways to treat exces- studying emotions at this level may be especially
sive negative academic emotions have mainly fruitful for understanding how emotions shape
been studied for test anxiety and related test emo- engagement in school. Second, diverse theoretical
tions (e.g., Davis, DiStefano, & Schutz, 2008). definitions have plagued emotion research in other
Specifically, test anxiety treatment is among the fields. Thus, we urge researchers conducting
most successful psychological therapies avail- research on emotions in educational settings to be
able, effect sizes often being above d = 1 clear about how they define emotions within the
(Hembree, 1988; Zeidner, 1998). Empirical evi- context of education and to carefully match the
dence on ways to regulate and modify academic theoretical conceptualization of emotions with
emotions more generally is still largely lacking to their assessment instruments. Third, within the
date, with few exceptions (c.f., Nett, Goetz, & field of psychological neuroscience, great strides
Hall, 2010). have been made in understanding the neurological
bases for emotions and their link to other aspects
of neurological functioning (c.f., Davidson,
Conclusion Pizzagalli, Nitschke, & Kalin, 2003; Immordino-
Yang, McColl, Damasio, & Damasio, 2009).
As argued in this chapter, emotions are critically Researchers studying emotions in the classroom
important for students’ engagement with aca- should be aware of the implications of this
demic tasks. This is likely true for all major research, especially with respect to the implicit
types of cognitive, motivational, and behavioral aspects of emotions and the way in which emo-
engagement contributing to students’ academic tions shape underlying cognitive processing.
success. However, much of the research sup- Fourth, as noted earlier, the reciprocal aspects of
porting this conclusion has been conducted by emotions are often neglected. Yet the models we
cognitive psychologists, social psychologists, discussed highlight the dynamic quality of emo-
and neuroscientists in laboratory studies and is tions and engagement. Future research needs to
far removed from the reality of academic con- develop better methods for unpacking these
texts. Except for studies examining test anxiety, dynamic relations across time.
which has been a popular construct in educa- Finally, if we are to truly understand the role
tional research since the 1950s (Zeidner, 1998), of emotions in classroom settings, we need to
research on students’ emotions in real-world design learning environments that are emotion-
academic settings is clearly in a nascent stage. ally adaptive for students and test the effective-
Educational research is just beginning to ness of these environments. As yet, the few
acknowledge the importance of affect and attempts to design academic environments that
emotions. foster students’ positive academic emotions have
To better understand the role of emotions for met with partial success at best (e.g., Glaeser-
engagement in school, we suggest several areas Zikuda, Fuss, Laukenmann, Metz, & Randler,
for future research. First, researchers should inves- 2005). The limited success may be due, at least in
tigate a variety of forms of emotions (mood, part, to the need for additional research about
achievement, epistemic, topic, social) that may be which emotions are especially beneficial in edu-
relevant in educational contexts. There is a grow- cational settings. Nevertheless, the success story
ing body of research on achievement emotions, of test anxiety research suggests that future
but relatively little research on epistemic emotions research can be successful in developing ways to
or social emotions. We still know very little about shape academic settings so that adaptive student
how emotions emerge in response to specific task emotions fostering students’ engagement are pro-
elements or in relation to social interactions in the moted and maladaptive emotions prevented.
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 279

material can benefit from prior testing. Journal of


References Experimental Psychology. General, 135, 533–571.
Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions
Acee, T. W., Kim, H., Kim, H. J., Kim, J.-I., Chu, H. R., & inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in
Kim, M. (2010). Academic boredom in under- and Cognitive Sciences, 11, 393–399.
over-challenging situations. Contemporary Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2009). How the object
Educational Psychology, 35, 17–27. of affect guides its impact. Emotion Review, 1, 39–54.
Ainley, M. (2007). Being and feeling interested: Transient Cordova, D. I., & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motiva-
state, mood, and disposition. In P. A. Schutz & R. tion and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of
Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in education (pp. 147–163). contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal
San Diego, CA: Academic. of Educational Psychology, 88, 715–730.
Ainley, M., Corrigan, M., & Richardson, N. (2005). Craig, S. D., D’Mello, S., Witherspoon, A., & Graesser,
Students, tasks and emotions: Identifying the contri- A. (2008). Emote aloud during learning with
bution of emotions to students’ reading of popular cul- AutoTutor: Applying the Facial Action Coding System
ture and popular science texts. Learning and to cognitive-affective states during learning. Cognition
Instruction, 15, 433–447. and Emotion, 22, 777–788.
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. Crook, C. (2000). Motivation and the ecology of collab-
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- orative learning. In R. Joiner, K. Littleton, D. Faulkner,
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. & D. Miell (Eds.), Rethinking collaborative learning
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. (pp. 161–178). London: Free Association Books.
Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuro- Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxi-
psychological theory of positive affect and its influence ety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550. Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., Pekrun, R., Haynes, T.
Aspinwall, L. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive L., Perry, R. P., & Newall, N. E. (2009). A longitudinal
affect in self-regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 22, analysis of achievement goals: From affective anteced-
1–32. ents to emotional effects and achievement outcomes.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 948–963.
Astleitner, H. (2000). Designing emotionally sound
Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., & Kalin, N.
instruction: The FEASP-approach. Instructional
H. (2003). Parsing the subcomponents of emotion and
Science, 28, 169–198.
disorders of emotion: Perspectives from affective neu-
Bäuml, K.-H., & Kuhbandner, C. (2007). Remembering
roscience. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H.
can cause forgetting – but not in negative moods.
Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences
Psychological Science, 18, 111–115.
(pp. 8–24). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Bless, H., Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., Golisano, V., Rabe, Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.).
C., & Wölk, M. (1996). Mood and the use of scripts: (2003). Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford, UK:
Does a happy mood really lead to mindlessness? Oxford University Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, Davis, H. A., DiStefano, C., & Schutz, P. A. (2008).
665–679. Identifying patterns of appraising tests in first-year
Boekaerts, M. (1993). Anger in relation to school learn- college students: Implications for anxiety and emotion
ing. Learning and Instruction, 3, 269–280. regulation during test taking. Journal of Educational
Bramesfeld, K. D., & Gasper, K. (2008). Happily putting Psychology, 100, 942–960.
the pieces together: A test of two explanations for Deci, E. C., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of auton-
the effects of mood on group-level information pro- omy and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality
cessing. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, and Social Psychology, 53, 1024–1037.
285–309. Denzin, N. K. (1984). A new conception of emotion and
Broughton, S. H., Sinatra, G. M., & Nussbaum, M. (2011). social interaction. In N. K. Denzin (Ed.), On under-
“Pluto has been a planet my whole life!” Emotions, standing emotion (pp. 49–61). San Francisco: Jossey-
attitudes, and conceptual change in elementary stu- Bass.
dents’ learning about Pluto’s reclassification. Do, S. L., & Schallert, D. L. (2004). Emotions and class-
Manuscript submitted for publication. room talk: Toward a model of the role of affect in stu-
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an dents’ experiences of classroom discussions. Journal
approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. of Educational Psychology, 96, 619–634.
Psychological Bulletin, 135, 183–204. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive
Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. F. (1996). A approach to motivation and personality. Psychological
control-process perspective on the origins of affect. In Review, 95, 256–273.
L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Efklides, A., & Petkaki, C. (2005). Effects of mood on
Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation students’ metacognitive experiences. Learning and
(pp. 11–52). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Instruction, 15, 415–431.
Associates. Efklides, A., & Volet, S. (Eds.). (2005). Feelings and emo-
Chan, C. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2006). tions in the learning process [Special issue]. Learning
Retrieval-induced facilitation: Initially nontested and Instruction, 15, 377–515.
280 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. New
and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, York: Springer.
169–189. Helmke, A. (1993). Die Entwicklung der Lernfreude vom
Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achieve- Kindergarten bis zur 5. Klassenstufe [Development of
ment goal construct. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck enjoyment of learning from kindergarten to grade 5].
(Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 7, 77–86.
(pp. 52–72). New York: Guilford Press. Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects, and treat-
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. (1999). Test anxiety and the ment of test anxiety. Review of Educational Research,
hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achieve- 58, 47–77.
ment motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Immordino-Yang, M., McColl, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio,
Psychology, 76, 628–644. A. (2009). Neural correlates of admiration and compas-
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 × 2 achieve- sion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
ment goal framework. Journal of Personality and of the United States of America, 106(19), 8021–8026.
Social Psychology, 80, 501–519. Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987).
Elliot, A. J., Murayama, K., & Pekrun, R. (2011). A 3 x 2 Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving.
achievement goal model. Journal of Educational Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,
Psychology, 103, 632-648. 1122–1131.
Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource alloca- Jarvenoja, H., & Jarvela, S. (2009). Emotion control in
tion model of the effect of depressed mood states on collaborative learning situations: Do students regulate
memory. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cog- emotions evoked by social challenges? British Journal
nition, and social behavior. Toronto, Canada: Hogrefe of Educational Psychology, 79, 463–481.
International. Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1974). Instructional
Eysenck, M. W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition. Hove, goal structure: Cooperative, competitive or individual-
UK: Psychology Press. istic. Review of Educational Research, 4, 213–240.
Feldman Barrett, L., & Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. L. (1999). Achievement goals
and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. Journal and student well-being. Contemporary Educational
of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 967–984. Psychology, 24, 330–358.
Fisher, C. D. (1993). Boredom at work: A neglected con- Kleinginna, P. R., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized
cept. Human Relations, 46, 395–417. list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consen-
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). sual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 345–379.
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the Lane, A. M., Whyte, G. P., Terry, P. C., & Nevill, A. M.
evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59–109. (2005). Mood, self-set goals and examination perfor-
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in mance: The moderating effect of depressed mood.
positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 143–153.
positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal,
Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Lüdtke, O., Pekrun, R., & and coping. New York: Springer.
Sutton, R. E. (2009). Emotional transmission in the Linnenbrink, E. A. (Ed.). (2006). Emotion research in
classroom: Exploring the relationship between teacher education: Theoretical and methodological perspec-
and student enjoyment. Journal of Educational tives on the integration of affect, motivation, and
Psychology, 101, 705–716. cognition [Special issue]. Educational Psychology
Frenzel, A. C., Thrash, T. M., Pekrun, R., & Goetz, T. Review, 18(4), 315–341.
(2007). Achievement emotions in Germany and China: Linnenbrink, E. A. (2007). The role of affect in student
A cross-cultural validation of the Academic Emotions learning: A multi-dimensional approach to consider-
Questionnaire-Mathematics (AEQ-M). Journal of ing the interaction of affect, motivation, and engage-
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 302–309. ment. In P. A. Schutz & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in
Glaeser-Zikuda, M., Fuss, S., Laukenmann, M., Metz, K., education (pp. 107–124). San Diego, CA: Academic.
& Randler, C. (2005). Promoting students’ emotions Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2002a). The role of
and achievement – Instructional design and evaluation motivational beliefs in conceptual change. In M.
of the ECOLE-approach. Learning and Instruction, Limon & L. Mason (Eds.), Reconsidering conceptual
15, 481–495. change: Issues in theory and practice (pp. 115–135).
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., & Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic
Lüdtke, O. (2007). Between- and within-domain rela- Publishers.
tions of students’ academic emotions. Journal of Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2002b). Achievement
Educational Psychology, 99, 715–733. goal theory and affect: An asymmetrical bidirectional
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Stoeger, H., & Hall, N. C. model. Educational Psychologist, 37, 69–78.
(2010). Antecedents of everyday positive emotions: Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2004). Role of affect
An experience sampling analysis. Motivation and in cognitive processing in academic contexts. In D.
Emotion, 34, 49–62. Dai & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Motivation, emotion, and
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual
Emotional contagion. New York: Cambridge functioning and development (pp. 57–87). Mahwah,
University Press. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
12 Academic Emotions and Student Engagement 281

Linnenbrink, E. A., Ryan, A. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (1999). Pekrun, R. (1992a). The impact of emotions on learning
The role of goals and affect in working memory func- and achievement: Towards a theory of cognitive/moti-
tioning. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, vational mediators. Applied Psychology, 41, 359–376.
213–230. Pekrun, R. (1992b). Expectancy-value theory of anxiety:
Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., & Pekrun, R. (2011). Students’ Overview and implications. In D. G. Forgays, T.
emotions and academic engagement: Introduction to Sosnowski, & K. Wrzesniewski (Eds.), Anxiety:
special issue. Contemporary Educational Psychology, Recent developments in self-appraisal, psychophysio-
36(1), 1–3. logical and health research (pp. 23–41). Washington,
Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Rogat, T. M., & Koskey, K. L. DC: Hemisphere.
(2011). Affect and engagement during small group Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achieve-
instruction. Contemporary Educational Psychology, ment emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and impli-
36, 13–24. cations for educational research and practice.
Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in Educational Psychology Review, 18, 315–341.
decision making. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & Pekrun, R. (2009). Global and local perspectives on human
H. Hill Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences affect: Implications of the control-value theory of
(pp. 619–642). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. achievement emotions. In M. Wosnitza, S. A. Karabenick,
Maehr, M. L., & Zusho, A. (2009). Achievement goal A. Efklides, & P. Nenniger (Eds.), Contemporary moti-
theory: The past, present, and future. In K. R. Wentzel vation research: From global to local perspectives
& A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at (pp. 97–115). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe.
school (pp. 77–104). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Pekrun, R., & Stephens, E. J. (in press). Academic emo-
Francis Group. tions. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.),
Maroldo, G. K. (1986). Shyness, boredom, and grade APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 2).
point average among college students. Psychological Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Reports, 59, 395–398. Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2006).
McLeod, D. B., & Adams, V. M. (Eds.). (1989). Affect Achievement goals and discrete achievement emo-
and mathematical problem solving: A new perspec- tions: A theoretical model and prospective test. Journal
tive. New York: Springer. of Educational Psychology, 98, 583–597.
Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990). Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement
Predictors of math anxiety and its influence on young goals and achievement emotions: Testing a model of their
adolescents’ course enrollment intentions and perfor- joint relations with academic performance. Journal of
mance in mathematics. Journal of Educational Educational Psychology, 101, 115–135.
Psychology, 82, 60–70. Pekrun, R., Frenzel, A., Goetz, T., & Perry, R. P. (2007).
Meinhardt, J., & Pekrun, R. (2003). Attentional resource The control-value theory of achievement emotions:
allocation to emotional events: An ERP study. An integrative approach to emotions in education. In
Cognition and Emotion, 17, 477–500. P. A. Schutz & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in education
Mouratidis, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Auweele, (pp. 13–36). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Y. V. (2009). Beyond positive and negative affect: Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., &
Achievement goals and discrete emotions in the Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings:
elementary physical education classroom. Psychology Control-value antecedents and performance outcomes
of Sport and Exercise, 10, 336–343. of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational
Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2009). The joint influence Psychology, 102, 531–549.
of personal achievement goals and classroom goal Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Perry, R. P., Kramer, K., &
structures on achievement-relevant outcomes. Journal Hochstadt, M. (2004). Beyond test anxiety:
of Educational Psychology, 101, 432–447. Development and validation of the Test Emotions
Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Hall, N. C. (2010). Coping with Questionnaire (TEQ). Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 17,
boredom in school: An experience sampling perspective. 287–316.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 49–59. Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002a).
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learn-
of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and perfor- ing and achievement: A program of qualitative and
mance. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346. quantitative research. Educational Psychologist, 37,
Nummenmaa, M., & Nummenmaa, L. (2008). University 91–106.
students’ emotions, interest and activities in a web- Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002b).
based learning environment. British Journal of Positive emotions in education. In E. Frydenberg
Educational Psychology, 78, 163–178. (Ed.), Beyond coping: Meeting goals, visions, and
Olafson, K. M., & Ferraro, F. R. (2001). Effects of emo- challenges (pp. 149–174). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
tional state on lexical decision performance. Brain and Pekrun, R., & Stephens, E. J. (2009). Goals, emotions,
Cognition, 45, 15–20. and emotion regulation: Perspectives of the control-
Parrott, W. G., & Spackman, M. P. (2000). Emotion and value theory of achievement emotions. Human
memory. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Development, 52, 357–365.
Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 476–490). New Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-
York: Guilford Press. regulated learning. In M. Boekarts, P. R. Pintrich, &
282 R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia

M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Sisk, D. A. (1988). The bored and disinterested gifted
Theory, research and applications (pp. 451–502). San child: Going through school lockstep. Journal for the
Diego, CA: Academic. Education of the Gifted, 11, 5–18.
Reisenzein, R. (2001). Appraisal processes conceptual- Tsai, Y.-M., Kunter, M., Lüdtke, O., Trautwein, U., &
ized from a schema-theoretic perspective. In K. R. Ryan, R. M. (2008). What makes lessons interesting?
Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal The role of situational and individual factors in three
processes in emotion (pp. 187–201). Oxford, UK: school subjects. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Oxford University Press. 100, 460–472.
Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., Midgley, C., & Patrick, H.
Perceptions of the school psychological environment (2003). Teacher discourse and sixth graders’ reported
and early adolescents’ psychological and behavioral affect and achievement behaviors in two high-mastery/
functioning in school: The mediating role of goals and high-performance mathematics classrooms. The
belonging. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, Elementary School Journal, 103, 357.
408–422. Turner, J. E., & Schallert, D. L. (2001). Expectancy-value
Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the orga- relationships of shame reactions and shame resiliency.
nization of affect. Review of General Psychology, 2, Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 320–329.
247–270. Turner, J. E., & Waugh, R. M. (2007). A dynamical systems
Ruthig, J. C., Perry, R. P., Hall, N. C., & Hladkyj, S. (2004). perspective regarding students’ learning processes:
Optimism and attributional retraining: Longitudinal Shame reactions and emergent self-organizations. In
effects on academic achievement, test anxiety, and vol- P. A. Schutz & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotions in education
untary course withdrawal in college students. Journal (pp. 125–145). San Diego, CA: Academic.
of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 709–730. Tyson, D. F., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., & Hill, N. E. (2009).
Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Regulating debilitating emotions in the context of per-
Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a formance: Achievement goal orientations, achieve-
self-regulatory mechanism. Journal of Personality ment-elicited emotions, and socialization contexts.
and Social Psychology, 63, 379–390. Human Development, 52, 329–356.
Schaller, M., & Cialdini, R. B. (1990). Happiness, sad- Vuorela, M., & Nummenmaa, L. (2004). Experienced
ness, and helping: A motivational integration. In R. emotions, emotion regulation and student activity in a
Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of moti- web-based learning environment. European Journal
vation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior of Psychology of Education, 19, 423–436.
(Vol. 2, pp. 265–296). New York: Guilford Press. Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement
Scherer, K. R. (2000). Emotions as episodes of subsys- motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92,
tems synchronization driven by nonlinear appraisal 548–573.
processes. In I. Granic & M. D. Lewis (Eds.), Emotion, Weiner, B. (2007). Examining emotional diversity in the
development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems classroom: An attribution theorist considers the moral
approaches to emotional development (pp. 70–99). emotions. In P. A. Schutz & R. Pekrun (Eds.),
New York: Cambridge University Press. Emotion in education (pp. 73–88). San Diego, CA:
Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.). (2001). Academic.
Appraisal processes in emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990). Test anxiety in the
University Press. school setting. In M. Lewis & S. M. Miller (Eds.),
Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2008). Handbook of developmental psychopathology:
Motivation in education: Theory, research, and appli- Perspectives in developmental psychology (pp. 237–250).
cations (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill New York: Plenum Press.
Prentice Hall. Wine, J. D. (1971). Test anxiety and the direction of atten-
Schutz, P. A., Hong, J. Y., Cross, D. I., & Osbon, J. N. tion. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92–104.
(2006). Reflections on investigating emotion in educa- Wolters, C. A. (2003). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating
tional activity settings. Educational Psychology an underemphasized aspect of self-regulated learning.
Review, 18, 343–360. Educational Psychologist, 38, 189–205.
Schutz, P. A., & Lanehart, S. L. (Eds.). (2002). Emotions Wosnitza, M., & Volet, S. (2005). Origin, direction and
in education [Special issue]. Educational Psychologist impact of emotions in social online learning. Learning
37(2), 67-135. and Instruction, 15, 449–464.
Schutz, P. A., & Pekrun, R. (Eds.). (2007). Emotion in Zeidner, M. (1987). Essay versus multiple choice type
education. San Diego, CA: Academic. classroom exams: The students’ perspective. The
Schwartz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feelings and phe- Journal of Educational Research, 80, 352–358.
nomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. New
Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of York: Plenum.
basic principles (pp. 433–465). New York: The Zeidner, M. (2007). Test anxiety in educational contexts:
Guilford Press. What I have learned so far. In P. A. Schutz & R. Pekrun
Shaha, S. H. (1984). Matching-tests: Reduced anxiety and (Eds.), Emotion in education (pp. 165–184). San
increased test effectiveness. Educational and Diego, CA: Academic.
Psychological Measurement, 44, 869–881.
Students’ Interest and Engagement
in Classroom Activities 13
Mary Ainley

Abstract
This chapter focuses on interest as a key motivational construct for investi-
gating the relation between motivation and engagement. The emphasis is
on identifying the links between the underlying interest processes and stu-
dents’ participation in achievement activities. It is suggested that a dynamic
systems perspective with its emphasis on the individual as a self-organizing
system provides a productive framework for future developments in under-
standing the sets of processes that support engagement. Central in this
analysis will be an examination of the microprocess level to identify the
process variables that combine in different patterns of student engagement.
Addressing the relation between motivation and engagement at the micro-
process level puts a major emphasis on the immediate context of the task
and the broader classroom. However, these are nested within expanding
contexts of school and community cultures, and the relation between inter-
est processes and engagement in achievement activities will be considered
within the framework of investigations into patterns of interest and engage-
ment using data from the PISA 2006 international survey of science
achievement.

At the beginning of this century, Hidi and to engage, but not all children have academic
Harackiewicz (2000) reviewed research on interests and motivation to learn to the best of
achievement goals and interest to address the their abilities in school” (p. 168). This statement
issue of how educators might motivate the aca- foreshadows a number of the questions that will
demically unmotivated. At one point in their be addressed in this chapter. It points to a range
review, Hidi and Harackiewicz stated that “All of motivations that can be seen in students and
children have interests, motivation to explore, acknowledges that there may be a mismatch
between students’ motivation and what is required
to achieve within school contexts. In this chapter,
I explore the relation between students’ interest
M. Ainley, Ph.D. (*)
and engagement in classroom activities.
Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne,
Melbourne, VIC, 3010 Australia It is clear from the recent literature that a lot
e-mail: maryda@unimelb.edu.au of attention has been given to researching the

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 283


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_13, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
284 M. Ainley

motivation of achievement. As Fredricks, early 1990s and the bulk of references in education
Blumenfeld, and Paris (2004) pointed out, the lit- over the last decade have taken the broad distinc-
erature on motivation in education is more exten- tion between situational and individual interest as
sive and has more precise distinctions drawn a starting point. On the other side, some of the
within component categories than has been typi- major reviews synthesizing the debate on engage-
cal in the engagement literature. One strand in ment have been published more recently and are
the motivation literature takes the form of presenting some degree of consensus on the need
research into the functioning of specific, tightly to distinguish between types, components, or indi-
defined variables, for example, achievement cators of engagement. For example, Jimerson,
goals (Hulleman & Senko, 2010; Sideridis, Campos, and Greif (2003) summarized the find-
2009). Another strand explores interconnections ings of their review of engagement and associated
and overlaps between pairs of motivational vari- terms such as “school bonding” and “school
ables, for example, interest and self-efficacy attachment” by distinguishing three dimensions:
(Hidi & Ainley, 2007), and sets of motivation affective, behavioral, and cognitive. Fredricks
variables, (Murphy & Alexander, 2000). Yet et al. (2004) referred to the multifaceted nature of
another strand focuses attention on how these engagement specifying behavioral, emotional, and
variables and interconnections between variables cognitive facets: a “multidimensional construct
play out in classrooms and other schooling con- that unites the three components in a meaningful
texts (Boekaerts, de Koning, & Vedder, 2006; way” (p. 60). Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, and
Turner, 2010; Wosnitza & Volet, 2009). In this Kindermann (2008) separated factors that are
chapter, I focus attention on the motivation con- “outside of the construct” as facilitators of engage-
struct of interest and use contemporary directions ment from features that are “inside the construct,”
in the understanding of interest to explore rela- that is, are indicators of engagement, or part of the
tions between students’ interest and engagement essential character of engagement. The two
in classroom activities. My exploration will be included in their model are behavioral and emo-
organized in terms of the three key questions tional indicators but with acknowledgement that
posed to the handbook authors: cognitive facets of engagement could be added to
1. What is your definition of engagement and the model. While there seems to be general agree-
motivation, and how do you differentiate ment concerning the tripartite dimensionality of
the two? engagement, there is also recognition of potential
2. What overarching framework or theory do overlaps between behavioral, emotional, and cog-
you use to study and explain engagement and nitive aspects of engagement.
motivation? When it comes to the place of interest in these
3. What is the role of context in explaining models, for Fredricks et al. (2004), interest is one
engagement or motivation? of the variables grouped as emotional engage-
ment along with values and emotions. Cognitive
engagement includes “motivation, effort, and
On Definition strategy use” (p. 64). Using somewhat different
groupings, the taxonomy proposed by Christenson
Across most areas of educational research, there and Reschly (Appleton, Christenson, Kim, &
are differences in definition and differences in Reschly, 2006; Reschly & Christenson, 2006)
emphasis within definitions of motivation and includes motivational concepts concerned with
engagement, and these contribute to the challenge values and goals as cognitive engagement, while
of the debate. Over the last two decades, the litera- psychological engagement includes feelings of
tures on both interest and engagement have wit- belonging, identification, and interpersonal rela-
nessed a large growth in volume. The interest tionships. Following this taxonomy, interest falls
literature struggled with conceptual issues in the within the cognitive engagement domain.
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 285

At the same time as these distinctions are with terms like engagement. Jimerson et al.
articulated, reviews of the engagement literature (2003) make a similar point from their review of
qualify their analysis with the caveat that there usage of terms associated with school engage-
are overlaps between the groupings, and the clas- ment: terms such as “school bonding” and “school
sification of concepts such as interest and moti- attachment.” Jimerson et al. found that often
vation highlight some of the difficulty in these terms were not defined and the meaning of
developing a single taxonomy of engagement the term had to be deduced from the content of
that encompasses all of the relevant perspectives. the measures used.
While Fredricks et al. (2004) separate interest as But, back to the dictionary definition. An addi-
a subset of emotional engagement and motiva- tional meaning in general dictionary definitions
tion as a subset of cognitive engagement, this comes from the field of mechanics, more specifi-
chapter identifies interest as a subset of motiva- cally, the operation of gears. Gears engage or
tion, and this is consistent with the general under- interlock for the purpose of moving the parts of a
standing in the motivation literature. However, machine. The metaphor can be pursued further.
the overlaps within the engagement literature Gears used for driving a car can be mapped onto
highlight the need for close scrutiny of these con- levels of engagement in classroom activities. For
structs and the relations between them. example, top gear represents the level of being
fully engaged. Low gear and neutral gear are
indicative of lower levels of engagement, mini-
Turning the Lens on Engagement mal activity and marking time, respectively. The
metaphor can be followed through into disen-
For my analysis of the engagement side of gaged behavior and to dropping out of school,
the relation, the starting point is a dictionary which map onto reverse gear. Hence, engagement
definition (The Australian Oxford Paperback as a construct for understanding involvement in
Dictionary, 1999). Engagement and the associ- education, irrespective of educational level,
ated verb “to engage” have a range of meanings implies connection between students and the
that go far beyond the educational domain. All of activities of schooling; it implies participation
the meanings, including those to do with betrothal, whether that is measured as attendance (e.g.,
drawing battle lines, and employment, involve Willms, 2003), participation in extracurricular
being occupied in an activity, for example, being activities (e.g., Fullarton, 2002), completion of
engaged in conversation. However, whether used homework, attitudes to school, student-teacher
as the verb “to engage” or the noun “engage- relationships, and sense of belonging (e.g.,
ment,” to convey meaning unambiguously, the Linnakylä & Malin, 2008; Willms, 2003).
term requires further specification of the activity
that is occupying the subject’s time and attention.
Distinguishing Motivation
Sometimes, in the educational research literature,
and Engagement
the referent for engagement is explicit, for exam-
ple, student engagement with school (Appleton
One way of distinguishing motivation and
et al., 2006, p. 427), engagement in instructional
engagement is to identify motivation as underly-
activity (Marks, 2000, p. 172), styles of engage-
ing psychological process and engagement as a
ment with learning (Ainley, 1993, p. 395), or
descriptor for the level of involvement or con-
engagement in school life (Linnakylä & Malin,
nection between person and activity:
2008, p. 585). Sometimes, the activity is not
specified although with knowledge of the context Motivation is about energy and direction, the rea-
sons for behavior, why we do what we do.
often the activity is clear by implication. However,
Engagement describes energy in action, the con-
the referent needs to be explicit to avoid the nection between person and activity. (Russell,
vagueness and overinclusiveness that can come Ainley, & Frydenberg, 2005)
286 M. Ainley

The same point distinguishing underlying relevant learning domains as well as connections
psychological motivational processes from with many other forms of participation in school-
engagement as motivated action has been used ing. The interest relation expressed in engage-
and elaborated further by Appleton et al. (2006). ment with classroom activities implicates both
They suggested that “one can be motivated but situational and personal factors to different
not actively engage in a task. Motivation is thus degrees and in different combinations.
necessary, but not sufficient for engagement” A range of metaphors can be used to help elab-
(p. 428). It is important to notice here that the orate qualities of connection inherent in the rela-
implication for the meaning of the term engage- tion between interest and engagement. Two will
ment is that the individual has connected with the be explored. The first metaphor is a hook. When
content of the task (top gear) rather than simply an activity triggers interest, students readily
performing the activity mechanically or pretend- engage with that activity. It is as if the activity or
ing to perform the activity (low gear). Hence, specific features of the activity snare the student,
describing students’ engagement with learning drawing them in to engage. The second metaphor
mathematics implicates motivation for the activity, is a switch that connects students’ existing per-
just as describing students’ disengagement or sonal interests with opportunities to express those
disaffection with learning mathematics impli- interests. Rather than being snared into the activ-
cates lack of motivation for the activity. ity, this metaphor carries the implication that the
activity is within the student’s range of valued
activities and that simply encountering the oppor-
Turning the Lens on Interest tunity to engage switches open connections, and
the person immediately engages with the activity.
To this point, the discussion has used the more As with all metaphors, both of these highlight
general term motivation, and this sets the context some aspects of interest processes; neither cap-
for specific attention to the relation between tures the full spectrum. Each metaphor resonates
interest, as a specific type of motivation, and with a different type of interest. Situational inter-
engagement. Interest researchers and writers (see est (Hidi, 1990; Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Krapp,
e.g., Hidi, 1990, 2006; Krapp, 2005; Rathunde & 2005) refers to interest that is triggered or acti-
Csikszentmihalyi, 1993) often take Dewey’s vated by features of a specific situation (the hook
writings as a reference point for their definitions. metaphor). As has been shown in research on sit-
Referring to interest, Dewey asserted that “the uational interest, especially in relation to reading,
root idea of the term seems to be that of being exposure to a specific text or topic that is novel, or
engaged, engrossed, or entirely taken up with ambiguous (Schraw & Lehman, 2001; Wade,
some activity because of its recognized worth” 2001), or that has to do with life and death issues
(1913, see Boydston, 1979, p. 160). This is “top or themes of universal significance (Hidi & Baird,
gear” engagement, and the underlying psycho- 1988) will trigger arousal, attention, and positive
logical process is interest. affect which together prompt the person to engage
Consideration of the relation between interest with the text. With physical education activities,
and engagement directs attention to the underly- it has been shown that situational interest is trig-
ing processes that function to connect learners gered when students perceive the presence of
and learning tasks. At its simplest level, interest some basic task features. Features such as nov-
is a core psychological process energizing and elty, opportunities for exploration and challenge,
directing students’ interaction with specific class- and instant enjoyment triggered situational inter-
room activities: a very specific situational engage- est in physical education tasks (Chen & Ennis,
ment. In addition, there are more complex levels 2004; Sun, Chen, Ennis, Martin, & Shen, 2008).
of interest identified as individual or personal In mathematics classes, Mitchell (1993) reported
interest, and these depend on both the immediate how novel features of computer presentation of
situation and students’ past experience with mathematical problems triggered situational
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 287

interest (catch factors), but these features often an opportunity for congruent activities can be
did not maintain or hold interest. Perception of sufficient for activity to occur. Opportunity is the
certain task features triggers the energy that is switch that opens the connection between student
then invested in engagement with the activity. and activity.
On the other hand, individual interest refers to This exposition would be incomplete without
a positive orientation toward an activity or consideration of another set of circumstances
domain that has value for the person. For exam- where interest has been shown to energize
ple, a young student is known to spend hours engagement. A form of interest with different ori-
reading about “life in the middle ages,” to watch gins is illustrated in findings reported by Sansone
any available films depicting stories of heroes who has explored students’ behavior when con-
from the middle ages, and is known to find the strained to perform a boring task which involved
best websites for discovering more about medi- copying a letter matrix (Sansone & Thoman,
eval characters. What has been observed is the 2005; Sansone, Weir, Harpster, & Morgan, 1992).
student’s intense engagement with “life in the Confronted with this type of condition (an unin-
middle ages.” The intensity of involvement and teresting task but contextual reasons for staying
the fact that the student seeks every opportunity with the task), students used self-generated strat-
to engage and reengage with activities from this egies to enhance their interest in the task and so
domain is an indicator of an individual or per- maintained engagement. This form of interest is
sonal interest in “life in the middle ages.” With akin to situational interest, but there is no hook
this form of interest, it is not that the activity has from the task. The student reorganizes or recon-
triggered engagement. Engagement occurs when structs their perception of the task, so there is
a person who has accrued knowledge and value some aspect of it that is ambiguous, uncertain, or
for an activity, and who knows they enjoy the has new meaning. Alternatively, the student may
activity, perceives an opportunity to reengage. generate interest in the task through reshaping
The individual interest in the learning domain their perception of it to fit with personal mean-
(“life in the middle ages”) adheres in the legacy ings and interests.
of past experience that engenders anticipatory In sum, whether it is situational interest, or
affect and expectations of finding out something individual interest, or a form of self-generated
important. The switch metaphor gives emphasis interest serving a self-regulatory function, inter-
to the quickening of intention and activity that est is an important source of the energy and direc-
ensues when the student perceives an opportunity tion that sustains engagement with classroom
to engage in their valued activity. The complex activities.
set of psychological processes subsumed under
the individual interest concept emphasize that
students’ existing interests represent their ways Theoretical Orientation
of valuing and interpreting contextual features in
relation to potential activities. When there is a In this section, we explore the implications of a
match between students’ individual interest and dynamic systems perspective for understanding
specific contextual affordances, students readily the relation between interest and students’
embrace the activity expressing enjoyment, con- engagement in classroom activities. By referring
centration, and a desire to find out more. to a dynamic systems perspective, we are draw-
In short, when there is little or no existing ing on a set of theories loosely grouped as
interest relation between person and task, some- dynamic systems theories (see Lewis & Granic,
thing needs to happen to start the forging of a 2000). One of the basic proposals in these theo-
link; specific situational features are the hook ries is that an individual’s behavior is best repre-
that initiates activity. On the other hand, when the sented as a self-organizing system whereby
student has an existing positively valued organiza- pattern and internal organization emerge from the
tion or schema, an individual interest, perceiving interactive experience of the individual in a
288 M. Ainley

changing environment (Thelen & Smith, 2006). of what actions and effort might be required.
From their research into perceptual, motor, and cog- However, because learning tasks are never com-
nitive systems in infants and young children, Thelen pletely novel, these perceptions will also draw on
and Smith suggested that self-organization can existing schemas with their inherent organization
be observed in the “patterns assembled for of feelings, thoughts, and action tendencies, and
task-specific purposes whose form and stability this adds to individual variability. In short, aware-
depended on both the immediate and more dis- ness and perception of particular properties of a
tant history of the system” (p. 284). Within the new situation activates schemas that are orga-
educational domain, Jörg, Davis, and Nickmans nized units combining perceptions activated by
(2007) have suggested that this general perspec- the new situation and associated elements from
tive on behavior is equally appropriate for the existing motivational schemas. Such schemas
learning sciences. They suggest that complexity guide students’ behavior in relation to the new
is central in this paradigm. It involves dealing learning situation.
with concepts such as interactivity, reciprocal
interactions, and connectivity, and more attention
to these aspects of learning will advance under- Process Schemas and Task Engagement
standing to support student learning. Dynamic
systems concepts are gaining wider currency in Some findings from a recent study illustrate this
the educational literature and are being applied to interdependence of past, present, and individual
explore particular educational phenomena, for variability in students’ engagement with a class-
example, the consequences for learning processes room activity. A range of variables, including
when students experience academic shame mood, interest, achievement goal orientation, and
(Turner & Waugh, 2007). students’ assessment of their confidence and sat-
If we apply a dynamic systems perspective to isfaction with their performance on the task, were
the motivation of achievement behavior, each monitored as students worked online to complete
motivational construct can be viewed as an orga- an open-ended problem task (see Ainley, 2007;
nization of psychological processes such as feel- Ainley, Flowers, & Patrick, 2005). Before com-
ings, thoughts, and action tendencies. A student’s mencing the task, students completed a mood
current state of motivation is a unique organiza- scale adapted from a short form of the positive
tion of immediate experiences interacting with and negative affect schedule (PANAS, Watson &
their history of experiences in relation to a spe- Clark, 1994). This was repeated at the end of the
cific learning situation. Students come to a new session. Using cluster analysis techniques, three
learning situation with active organizations or mood profiles were identified: a happy group
motivation schema. These are likely to include with a profile of higher than average positive
components such as achievement goals, interest, affect and lower than average negative affect, an
and competency beliefs. In addition, experiences unhappy group whose profile indicated low nega-
that have occurred immediately prior to the new tive affect coupled with lower than average posi-
learning situation become part of the schema. At tive affect, and an anxious group distinguished
any point in time, the motivation schemas cur- by a profile of very high negative affect and low
rently active play a part in the system of factors positive affect.
that influence how an individual student reacts to The same three mood profiles were identified
a new situation. at the end of the session. However, there were a
At the simplest level in any new situation, the number of students who changed their mood sta-
student has a perception of the task which may or tus. One third of the students reporting the
may not align with the intentions of the task unhappy profile at the start had changed (20 of 61
designer. Task perceptions are likely to involve a students), and at end of the task were reporting a
distinct combination of expectations of what the mood profile that classified them with the happy
task is about, anticipatory feelings, and estimates group. Contrasting this group with those who
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 289

maintained their unhappy mood profile provided acquisition and creativity. More recently, Izard
an opportunity to investigate student characteris- (2007) has drawn attention to the pervasive role
tics and task responses that were associated with that interest plays in a wide range of complex
a change in mood while working on the problem human behavior.
scenario. Its (i.e., interest) ubiquity is further enhanced by
The students whose mood became more posi- its effectiveness in engaging and sustaining the
tive across the task had more positive personal individual in person-environment interactions that
achievement goal orientations as measured by an facilitate exploration, learning and constructive
endeavours. (Izard, 2007, p. 272)
initial self-report. After the task was explained,
self-reports of task interest and confidence about In his earlier writings, Izard (1977) used the
their performance were recorded. There was no construct of affective-cognitive structures to
significant difference between the group whose highlight the system of processes that through
unhappy mood changed and the group with no experience becomes organized around basic emo-
change from their unhappy mood. However, what tions. Beginning in infancy, the emotion of inter-
differed between these groups were their on-task est as an immediate positive state is triggered by
responses to working on the problem scenario. small changes in the perceptual field. Interest
A probe part way into the task asked students to engages an infant with the world of objects and
report on their goal at that specific moment. leads to exploration; the brightly colored object
Students whose mood became more positive that moves or emits a new sound is approached,
reported higher on-task salience of both mastery inspected, and manipulated. With each new expe-
and performance-approach goals. They were rience, these basic organizations of affect and
more likely to record higher ratings for the goals cognition are expanded and reorganized. In the
of understanding what they were doing (mastery: more recent formulations, these are referred to as
“right now my aim is to understand and learn as emotion schemas, and within this model, interest
much as I can”) and showing they could outper- schemas involve integrated systems of ideas and
form other students (performance-approach: feelings (Izard, 2007). These propositions con-
“right now my aim is to show I can do better than cerning the way that interest is implicated in a
other students”) than were the students whose wide range of human behavior underscore the
mood did not change. Interest also increased significance of understanding the development of
across the task. In addition, post-task reflections interest schema for students’ engagement with
from students whose mood changed reported a classroom activities.
stronger sense of achieving their goals and feel- Therefore, when we examine the concept of
ing satisfied with the quality of their submitted interest, we expect to find variation in the compo-
answer. Hence, engagement with the new learn- nent processes and in relations between component
ing situation involved a complex interaction of processes at different levels of organization of
initial and on-task mood changes, feelings of interest schema. These variable patterns reflect
competence, and on-task goals, and one of the the experiences that have built students’ interest
important components was interest. schemas, and so it is reasonable to expect com-
mon experiences within groups of students as
well as experiences that are unique to individual
The Role of Interest students. In a recent paper, Frenzel, Dicke, Goetz,
and Pekrun (2009) have demonstrated how the
A number of current theories of development content of the meaning of interest varies when
focus on the adaptive significance of positive 5th grade and 9th grade students rate their interest
emotions. For example, Fredrikson’s (2001) in mathematics. Affective terms were prominent
broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions for both groups of students, but older students
proposes that positive emotions, interest, joy, also included references to value and competency
and contentment are key factors in knowledge aspects. As has been argued previously, “part of
290 M. Ainley

the challenge for interest research is to identify combination of emotions. It is conceivable that
those combinations of affect and cognitions func- negative emotion may sometimes be part of these
tioning as interest schema at different levels of schemas. More knowledge about the situation
interest development” (Ainley, 2010, p. 237). has been incorporated into the interest schema,
In what follows, some of our current under- and this extension in time makes it more likely
standings of interest schema specifically as they that the interest schema will be triggered again in
relate to the relation between interest and engage- further similar situations. Continued opportuni-
ment will be elaborated. ties to engage with the domain entail new and
varied experiences. Each outcome adds to the
Interest: Developmental Phases developing interest schema, and over time, the
and Stages schema becomes an important area of activity for
Interest is not a unitary construct. Some of the the person. The interest schema is starting to take
most recent perspectives from educational on the character of an individual interest or per-
research that are informative for identifying psy- sonal orientation, and this type of self-sustaining
chological processes that constitute interest are organization or schema is referred to in the four-
the models describing phases (Hidi & Renninger, phase model as an emerging individual interest.
2006), or stages (Krapp, 2003), in interest devel- This phase of interest development is character-
opment. Situational interest, defined as interest ized by positive feelings, an accrued body of
triggered by a specific situation, is the simplest knowledge, and a sense that this domain is per-
form of interest, and both of these models concep- sonally important and valued. The final develop-
tualize situational interest as the first step in the mental level is a well-developed individual
development of individual or personal interest. interest and is identified by the depth of value,
Hidi and Renninger’s (2006) four-phase model knowledge, and feeling that the person has for
of interest development describes a sequence the domain and by independent seeking of oppor-
extending from the initial triggering of a situa- tunities for reengagement.
tional interest through to a well-developed indi- Krapp (2003) has proposed a similar model
vidual interest. At the most basic level, a newly but distinguishes only three stages: “awakened or
triggered situational interest involves arousal of triggered” and “stabilized” stages of situational
affect and focused attention toward the object interest and individual interest. The underlying
triggering interest. If the situation is completely developmental trajectory is similar, but more
novel, this is the beginnings of a new schema. emphasis is placed on value and feeling compo-
However, most educational contexts are not com- nents as the core of individual interest.
pletely novel, and so elements of past experience Although these phases or stages define a
with related situations are also aroused and have developmental progression, it is possible for
the potential to become part of the developing development to be terminated as interest in the
interest organization or schema. Further, interac- situation lapses or dies. For example, when we
tion with and exploration of the new situation next encounter the student who was passionate
with associated feelings and thoughts allow about “life in the middle ages,” we observe that
dimensions of those feelings and new knowledge they have ignored the latest release movie set in
to become part of the developing interest schema. the middle ages, that they have closed down their
If this continues over time or is repeated a num- links to previously frequented middle ages web-
ber of times across a relatively short period, the sites, and that they have stopped reading books
triggered situational interest develops into a on “life in the middle ages.” Their former pas-
maintained situational interest. The implication sionate individual interest has lapsed. In addition,
here is that there is a more stable organization of under certain circumstances, interest develop-
feeling, knowledge, and experience that makes ment may regress and revert to a simpler form.
up the interest schema. The initial positive affect For example, our student who had the passionate
may now be more differentiated and involve a interest in “life in the middle ages” may have
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 291

their former interest activated when they see an their basket of interests, participants use rating
advertisement for a new movie set in the middle scales and open-ended comments to provide an
ages, but they no longer seek out sources of infor- in-depth profile of affective, cognitive, and expe-
mation as they did previously. Their former indi- riential aspects of their selected interests. At the
vidual interest appears to have reverted to a theoretical level, the interest profiles are being
situational interest. Interest can still be triggered used to give further definition to some of the
by certain events, such as the movie advertise- affective and cognitive dimensions associated
ment, but they no longer independently seek with interest at the different phases of develop-
opportunities for reengagement. ment. At the applied level, they will be used to
Both the Hidi and Renninger (2006) and the inform curriculum design and development with
Krapp (2003) models of interest development the objective of supporting engagement in pro-
sketch distinguishing features for interest at each ductive classroom activities for students with
of the phases or stages of development. However, challenging behavior.
applying this framework to support student In short, interest is not a unitary construct.
engagement with classroom activities requires Knowledge of the phases of interest development
ways to identify some of the richness and vari- and the psychological processes that go to make
ability of the affective and cognitive dimensions up specific interest schema at all phases of inter-
that make up students’ interest schemas. This est development will provide greater insight into
issue has recently been taken up in relation to a ways that all students can be supported to engage
particular applied educational issue: the problem with classroom activities.
of engaging students with challenging behavior
in productive classroom learning activities. Ely
and colleagues (Ely, Ainley, & Pearce, 2010) are Interest and Processes That Contribute
using innovative software to develop a system to Engagement
for profiling the component processes distin-
guishing students’ interest schemas. In particular, In this section, we describe findings from studies
the tool has been designed to identify phases of using a general research design that involves stu-
interest development to assist teachers. Providing dents working on tasks constructed to highlight
this level of detail about students’ interests should how interest and associated processes relate to
assist teachers in their endeavors to establish and some of the more readily observed indicators of
maintain students’ engagement with classroom students’ engagement with learning tasks.
activities especially with students who might oth- Individual projects have recruited students as
erwise be uncommunicative. young as 5th grade and from secondary classes
Prototype software (MINE: Ely et al., 2010) up to and including 10th grade. The number of
has been developed and is being tested with participants has varied between 100 and 300 stu-
young adolescent students. Using an interactive dents, and while most of the studies referred to
exploratory environment populated with approx- have been conducted with Australian students,
imately 60 “interest cells,” students are able to some projects have included groups of Canadian
explore the interest space in a way that allows students (see e.g., Ainley, Hidi, & Berndorff,
them to discover ideas, activities, or “stuff” that 2002). Two general types of task have been used,
they may not have previously encountered and one a reading task, the other an open-ended prob-
might be interested in, as well as other familiar lem scenario requiring students to generate a
ideas, activities, and “stuff” that they may be solution which they present with supporting rea-
interested in already. Students are asked to select sons. Both types of tasks are administered using
a basket of at least three interests. Importantly, interactive computer software, and the underly-
these interests may represent anything from ing structure of the software allows monitoring
newly triggered situational interests to well- of students’ decisions about the extent and direc-
developed individual interests. After choosing tion of their actions. Hence, decisions and level
292 M. Ainley

of task activity are the indicators of engagement, content was positively related to the level of
while specific probes and questions concerning interest triggered by the text title and with initial
processes such as interest, other emotions, task interest responses to the text (Ainley, Corrigan
efficacy, and task difficulty allow parallel self- et al., 2005; Ainley, Hidi et al., 2002; Ainley,
report monitoring of key psychological process Hillman et al., 2002). As might be expected, the
variables. strength of these associations differed for differ-
For the reading tasks, texts were divided into ent content domains and different text topics. For
three, sometimes four, sections and were pre- example, with groups of 7th and 8th graders,
sented through a sequence of computer screens. individual interest in the domain of social issues
Students were in control of the text screens, and was a significant predictor of how much interest
the simple click of a button labeled “NEXT” was triggered by the Body Image text topic and
allowed them to move to the next screen. When a individual interest in the domain of sports pre-
student chose to move on, a screen asked whether dicted interest in the Formula 1 Racing text topic
they wanted to read more about the topic or to (see Ainley, Corrigan et al., 2005).
quit. From the sequence of possible choices, an In addition, recording students’ self-report rat-
index of persistence (number of text sections ings in the real-time sequence of task perfor-
accessed) was generated. Initial interest in the mance has allowed mapping of trajectories of
topic as measured by a 5-point Likert rating was interest. As described above, when students were
predictive of this index of persistence with the able to decide after each section of text whether
reading task (see Ainley, Hidi et al., 2002). The to continue or to quit, levels of on-task interest
same pattern of findings has been replicated in a were predictive of these decisions. In other tasks,
number of similar studies using other texts and students were not given this choice, allowing the
separate groups of secondary school students from trajectory of successive states of interest to be
7th to 8th grades (Ainley, Corrigan, & Richardson, plotted. Consistent with the basic character of
2005) and from 10th grade (Ainley, Hillman, & interest as a quality of relation between student
Hidi, 2002). Interest is one of the variables that and the text, there was considerable variation in
powers decisions about engaging and maintaining these trajectories. In one study (Buckley, Hasen,
engagement with classroom activities. & Ainley, 2004), female secondary students were
presented with a set of texts dealing with social
Individual Interest and Trajectories and political issues. The interest trajectory in
of On-Task Interest response to a text dealing with euthanasia indi-
As has been argued throughout this chapter, inter- cated that interest in the text was high and was
est in a task is dynamic, and so the level of task maintained across all three text sections. On the
interest and its relation with engagement, even other hand, in the same study, a text on ecotour-
for a task that is limited to the relatively short ism initially drew moderate interest ratings, but
time duration of a single class lesson, may involve over successive text sections, the level of interest
changing patterns of relations between process declined significantly. In addition to the variabil-
variables. The structure of these research tasks ity in trajectory related to the text content, there
has allowed these changing reactions to be moni- were differences in interest trajectories related to
tored and recorded. students’ general patterns of individual interest.
Most of the tasks we have used have asked Students with strong individual interest in social
students to make ratings of their individual inter- and political issues generally recorded higher
est in domains related to the task prior to finding interest trajectories across successive text sec-
out details of the task. Across all of the studies, tions than did students whose individual interests
we have found a significant predictive relation were in areas such as sports and popular music.
between measures of individual interest and on- Recent findings reported by Rotgans and
task interest. For the reading tasks, individual Schmidt (2011) support our findings. In their
interest in content domains related to specific text research with polytechnic students, task interest
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 293

measures (referred to as situational interest) were subgroup trajectories have been identified using
administered five times during a problem-based cluster analysis. For the fantasy detective prob-
learning program. Each problem involved teams lem, three trajectories were identified represent-
of five students working on that problem over a ing high, medium, and low interest. When
whole day. Tutors observed and recorded students’ problem tasks based on curriculum issues have
achievement-related classroom behaviors including been used with successive cohorts of 9th grade
“participation, teamwork, presentation skills, and students, most of the problem scenario data sets
self-directed learning” (p. 5). Situational interest was have generated two trajectories: a medium-interest
significantly related to patterns of achievement- trajectory showing the interest level maintained
related classroom behavior, but at the same time, and sometimes increasing across the task and a
there was considerable variability in interest levels second low-interest trajectory starting with low
across the five measurement points. interest in the task and most often decreasing as
Similar patterns in the variability of the trajec- the task progressed.
tories of on-task interest to those observed with In summary, the strongest impression from
the reading tasks have been found in our studies inspection of these trajectories across a number
using problem scenarios. The problem scenario of student cohorts has been that students’ initial
software was designed to present students with reaction to the task sets a direction for their level
an open problem where their task is to explore of engagement. Students who initially responded
sets of information and to use the products of with low interest generally stayed at that level.
their research to construct a solution to the prob- These were often students who came to the task
lem and give reasons to support their solution. At with little individual interest in the domain. If
critical points in the task, students respond to there was a change, it was often in the direction
probes monitoring aspects of what they were of their interest decreasing further. On the other
thinking and feeling. Most commonly, the probes hand, students who responded with moderate or
have been programmed to appear immediately high interest were likely to maintain that interest,
after students had been introduced to the problem and if there was change interest increased.
details and instructions, after students have been This pattern is consistent with the findings of
working on the problem for about 10 minutes, Skinner and colleagues who reported trajectories
and then the final probe appears immediately of achievement motivation across a number of
after a student has submitted their answer. years of schooling suggesting that “In general,
As expected, the levels of interest recorded these dynamics seem to be amplifying, in that
reflect task content. For example, our fantasy children who start out motivationally rich main-
detective problem presented to 9th grade students tain their engagement as the year(s) progress,
indicated a moderately high level of interest whereas children who start out motivationally
which was maintained across the whole task (see poor tend to become more disengaged over time”
Ainley, Buckley, & Chan, 2009). However, when (Skinner et al., 2008, p. 765). These diverging
further problems were presented to the same stu- trajectories suggest that more attention needs to
dents but with content related to the social issues be directed to understanding the dynamics of
being researched as part of a course on research reactivity to classroom activities. For students
skills, the overall level of interest barely reached who do not come with strong individual interest
the midpoint of the 5-point interest rating scale. in the content domains of schooling and do not
Across the course of these tasks, the overall level have their interest triggered by the classroom
of interest appeared to decrease. activities set for them, there is little evidence of
As has already been asserted, individual vari- engagement.
ability is part of what is expected of dynamic sys- While this was the overall impression from
tems, and these data lend themselves to these studies, there were also signs of individual
identification of trajectories of interest develop- variability associated with different topics.
ment across the task. Within the student cohorts, Overall, students who were identified with the
294 M. Ainley

medium-interest trajectory maintained this status a task and how these experiences are linked with
across similar tasks. The same was also generally interest in the task, the results from a cohort of
the case for the low-interest trajectory groups. 9th grade students who completed the fantasy
However, just as with the mood study reported detective problem are shown in Fig. 13.1. The
earlier where a small number of students were figure shows the successive emotion states for the
responsive to the task and developed more posi- three interest trajectories identified in that cohort
tive mood as they engaged with the task, in the of students: low, moderate, and high interest. The
cohort of 9th grade students working on social panel of icons included an icon for neutral, and
issues problems, there were a number of students this was selected by a large proportion of students
who showed a different interest status on differ- but with decreasing frequency as the task pro-
ent tasks. Students who were in the low-interest gressed. Students in the moderate-interest cluster
trajectory on the first task recorded similar low were more likely to select neutral across the
interest at the first measurement point when com- whole task, and the major deviation from this pat-
pleting another problem scenario approximately tern was the choice of relieved at the end of the
3 months later. However, as the second task pro- task. The patterns for the high-interest and low-
gressed, interest ratings for these students interest groups show some important contrasts.
increased to the extent that their overall profile The high-interest group was more likely to choose
was more like the medium-interest group. an emotion icon rather than selecting neutral, and
A major implication that emerges from these their choices were more likely to be for positive
findings is that interest and its relation to stu- emotions such as happy and hopeful, while the
dents’ engagement with classroom activities is a low-interest group, when not choosing neutral,
dynamic system that draws on more enduring was more likely to choose negative emotions such
individual interests while at the same time being as sad, angry, and hopeless.
responsive to the particular contents of the imme- As can be seen in these pattern of emotions
diate task. associated with the interest trajectories, other
achievement emotions vary systematically with
interest and are part of the system of psychologi-
Interest in Combination with Other cal processes that energize and direct task
Emotions engagement.

Another component of the dynamic system of Interest in Combination with Other


interest-related processes associated with task Motivation Variables: Achievement Goals
engagement is the emotion states aroused simul- Further evidence that the relation between inter-
taneously with interest. Along with variations in est and engagement involves a dynamic system
levels of task interest, it is likely that there are connecting interest with a range of psychological
changes in emotions that students experience processes can be seen in the findings we have
while working on a single task. One of the probes reported relating trajectories of interest with
included in the problem scenario tasks used in achievement goals. Achievement goal orienta-
our research asked how students were feeling at tions have been linked with interest by a number
the three critical points: before commencing, of researchers. In particular, in longitudinal stud-
mid-task, and after the task was finished. Sets of ies of achievement in college students
face icons representing achievement emotions as (Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, & Elliot, 2002),
described by Pekrun and colleagues (Pekrun, interest has been demonstrated to mediate the
Goetz, Titz, & Perry, 2002) were presented, and influence of mastery goals on academic achieve-
students selected an icon to indicate “How are ment. At the task level, through tracking on-task
you feeling right now?” This was followed by an achievement goals and interest, we have demon-
intensity rating for the selected icon. To illustrate strated (Ainley & Patrick, 2006) that general
the differences in emotions students report across achievement goals predict congruent on-task
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 295

A High Interest Group

B Moderate Interest Group

C Low Interest Group

Fig. 13.1 Profiles of emotions for high (panel A)-, moderate (panel B)-, and low (panel C)-interest trajectory groups
showing emotions recorded pre-task, mid-task, and post-task
296 M. Ainley

achievement goals and that both mastery and per- reported on the dynamic of interest development
formance goals measured as on-task goals are across the task for individual students.
associated with levels of interest as the task pro- Consideration of the relation between interest
gresses. Across a number of research programs, and engagement requires parallel consideration
the relations between these variables are increas- of a broad network of variables both within the
ingly being represented as interactive and cyclic time frame of a classroom activity but also going
systems with each variable being both predictor beyond to include broader personal orientations
and outcome, rather than as simple linear rela- and characteristics as well as a myriad of contex-
tions (see e.g., Harackiewicz, Durik, Barron, tual factors.
Linnenbrink-Garcia, & Tauer, 2008; Tapola,
Dayez, & Veermans, 2009).
The dynamic model of classroom engagement Context and Relations Between Interest
and disaffection developed by Skinner and col- and Engagement
leagues (Skinner et al., 2008) also represents lev-
els of interaction among sets of processes that In this final section, I consider relations between
underpin what is observed as classroom engage- interest and engagement and focus on the nested
ment and disaffection. In particular, their repre- contexts within which learning occurs. The title
sentation of the internal dynamic or system for this chapter refers to classroom activities, and
allows for simultaneous feedback and interaction in most of the research program I have been
between what they define as engaged behavior, describing, the emphasis has been on the micro-
disaffected behavior, engaged emotion, and dis- process level exploring the dynamics of interest
affected emotion. Of particular significance for and its role in classroom learning. The research tasks
this approach is their modeling of the contin- we have used include activities that are similar to
gency between these components across two the tasks students encounter in their classes, and
measurement points within the school year for our research tools have monitored and recorded
cohorts of 5th, 6th, and 7th graders. This dynamic sequences of action and reaction in real time. This
system operates within a broader system linking has provided rich data from which to view ways
aspects of the school context and the individual that a variety of psychological processes interact
student’s self-system represented in their analysis and combine with interest to influence students’
by components of autonomy, competence, and engagement with classroom activities. However,
relatedness. It is my contention that a similar sys- this is only one level of the dynamic system that is
tems approach is needed to allow for ways that the student in their environment.
the complexity of the components of the external Following the ecological model of
system, for example, interactive loops between Bronfenbrenner, first articulated as an approach
the relatedness, competence, and autonomy, also to the study of human development (Bronfen-
interact with the internal system dynamics. brenner, 1992; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998),
A range of evidence has been described sup- it is informative to view the individual learner
porting the contention that the relation between and their world as a microsystem embedded
interest and engagement is most productively within layers of ever more general contexts that
viewed as a dynamic system. The classroom tasks have the potential to influence a student’s life
we have focused on have generally been individ- space including their opportunities for learning
ual activities. The inclusion of classroom peers and academic development. In this section, we
working together on a problem adds to the com- describe some findings from analyses (Ainley &
plexity of the system as separate individual sys- Ainley, 2011a, 2011b) of international science
tems adjust their dynamic to the functional achievement data (OECD, 2007), to consider
requirements of the joint working arrangements. whether relations between interest and engage-
Rotgans and Schmidt (2011) collected measures ment differ according to the different contexts
of situational interest and achievement-related represented by countries with varied historical
behaviors from students working in teams and and cultural traditions.
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 297

The Macrocontext and Relations of these two dimensions. The four countries
Between Interest and Engagement were Sweden (secular-rational/self-expression),
Estonia (secular-rational/survival), USA (tradi-
Over the last two decades, there have been a tional/self-expression), and Colombia (traditional/
number of important research findings that high- survival). The other criterion for selection was
light how broad social and cultural contexts influ- that the country had participated in the Programme
ence students’ achievement and development. for International Student Assessment (PISA)
Strong family traditions and cultural values have 2006 (OECD, 2007). These data on the science
been associated with differences in achievement achievement of 15-year-old students, included a
favoring Asian-American students (Asakawa & student questionnaire, with a number of scales
Csikszentimihalyi, 1998). Other studies have measuring variables that are pertinent for under-
shown that cultural values impact on students’ standing relations between interest and engage-
perception of choice (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999) ment. This provided an opportunity to test some
and the meaning of achievement goals (Dekker of the relations between interest and engagement
& Fischer, 2008). In addition, it is clear from as represented in the four-phase model of interest
Larson and Verma’s (1999) review of patterns of development and to test whether the same rela-
work and leisure for children and adolescents tions between the variables operated for students
around the world that time in school and opportu- from countries representing different cultural
nities for educational achievement vary across contexts.
countries.
The studies we describe now had the broad
goals of testing relations between interest and Interest and Engagement in PISA 2006
engagement as well as determining whether the
structure of these relations was consistent for stu- All of the PISA 2006 measures have been
dents from countries with different historical and designed and trialed by international teams to
cultural traditions. Decisions for selection of the ensure that the measurement model is robust
comparison countries were made using Inglehart’s across countries. Interest in science was the spe-
cultural world map (Inglehart & Baker, 2000; cial focus of PISA 2006, and a set of attitudinal
Inglehart & Welzel, 2005). According to Inglehart items covering a range of aspects of how stu-
and colleagues, two orthogonal value factors dents feel and think about science were adminis-
underlie the patterns of responses that have been tered (OECD, 2007, 2009). Hidi and Renninger’s
gathered in the World Values Surveys and (2006) four-phase model of interest develop-
European Surveys (1981–1982; 1990–1991; ment identifies the components of individual
1995–1998). The first dimension consists of an interest as positive affect, knowledge, and value.
orientation to authority which contrasts more We chose measures from the PISA 2006 student
traditional values emphasizing religion and obe- questionnaires to represent these components
dience to traditional authorities, with secular- and to test their relations with individual
rational values where family and social values interest.
are viewed as relative rather than absolute and The main achievement measure in PISA 2006
where deference to religious authority is not a is a measure of students’ science knowledge. In
high priority. The second dimension contrasts addition, a student questionnaire included a mea-
survival values where the physical and economic sure of general interest in learning science which
security of the community is central to values, asked how much interest students had in learning
with self-expression where values such as subjective about a range of science domains, a measure
well-being, individual autonomy, and personal of enjoyment of science, and a measure of per-
quality of life are prominent concerns. From this sonal value of science focusing specifically on
cultural map, four countries were chosen to repre- ways that science might be of value to them
sent the four quadrants formed by the intersection personally.
298 M. Ainley

As outlined in an earlier section, a key feature and the two engagement variables. Hence, the
of the behavior associated with an individual predictive effect of personal value of science on
interest is that the student seeks opportunities to the engagement variables was partially mediated
engage and reengage with content from their by its relation with enjoyment of science.
interest domain. Hence, an important motivation Simultaneously, general interest in learning sci-
for the modeling studies (Ainley & Ainley, 2011a, ence was also linked with the two engagement
2011b) was to test the relations: firstly, among variables, but again the coefficients were smaller
knowledge, value, and enjoyment of science and than those linking enjoyment of science with the
their relations with the general interest in learn- engagement variables. A very small coefficient
ing science measure, and secondly to model the linked science knowledge with the enjoyment of
relations between these variables and engage- science variable. These patterns held for all four
ment variables. of the selected countries, and the full set of coef-
Several measures from PISA 2006 represented ficients is available in Ainley and Ainley (2011a).
aspects of engagement. The first was a scale ask- The coefficients were strongest for Sweden and
ing about current participation in out-of-school generally smaller for Colombia, with the other two
science activities such as projects, watching tele- countries in between. For example, the model
vision programs about science, and reading about accounted for 50% of the variance in score on the
science. The second was a scale called future- intended future engagement variable for Sweden
oriented motivation to learn science and was and 31% for Colombia. Differences between the
designed to measure “how many students actu- patterns of relations for the four countries were
ally intended to continue their interest in science,” seen in the magnitude of the coefficients and of
that is, intentions for future engagement. Items special interest were the science knowledge effects.
referred to future science careers, studies in sci- For Colombia, the predictive relation between sci-
ence, and participation in science projects. These ence knowledge and the central mediating variable
two scales provided a measure of current engage- of enjoyment of science was close to zero (−0.01)
ment and intentions for future engagement. compared with a coefficient of 0.19 for Sweden.
Using AMOS 7.0 (see Arbuckle, 2005), we Coefficients for the other countries were in between
tested a number of models to determine the best these two values. These findings support the con-
fit depicting relations between the variables rep- clusion that the relations between these variables
resenting general interest in learning science, sci- should be interpreted as a network of interacting
ence knowledge, value of science, and enjoyment processes. In this model, the central mediating
of science, and their combined predictive rela- variable was enjoyment of science which was
tions with current engagement and intended strongly correlated with interest in learning sci-
future engagements (Ainley & Ainley, 2011a). ence (Colombia 0.43 and Sweden 0.69).
The final model gave an acceptable fit (Byrne, Except for the knowledge scale, all of the mea-
2001) for all four countries suggesting that the sures described so far are based on self-report
structure of relations between these variables questionnaires. A feature of the PISA 2006 data
could be represented in the same way across collection was the inclusion of what are referred to
the four countries (RMSEA: Sweden = 0.052, as embedded interest items. These items were
USA = 0.054, Colombia = 0.056, and designed to assess the degree to which students
Estonia = 0.076). The strongest coefficients in were interested in finding out more about the spe-
this model predicted from personal value of sci- cific science topics that they were working on in the
ence to enjoyment of science, and enjoyment of knowledge assessment problems. For example,
science predicted each of general interest in the content of one of the problem topics concerned
learning science, current engagement, and tobacco smoking. At the end of the problem ques-
intended future engagements. Additional paths tions, students were asked, “How much interest do
with lower coefficients linked personal value of you have in the following information?” Students
science with general interest in learning science were given three statements: “knowing how tar in
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 299

tobacco reduces lung efficiency,” “understanding this evidence supports our major contention that
why nicotine is addictive,” and “learning how the relations between interest in science and engage-
body recovers after stopping smoking” (OECD, ment with science consist in networks of psycho-
2006, p. 164). This measure is informative as it logical processes related to learning, including
represents students’ willingness to reengage with value, enjoyment, and, to a lesser extent, existing
a specific science topic to find out and understand knowledge of science.
more about it.
Similar modeling techniques were used to test
the pattern of relations between the set of predic- Conclusion
tor variables and the form of engagement repre-
sented in the embedded interest scores (Ainley & In this chapter, it has been argued that a useful
Ainley, 2011b). Again, we found that our final way to conceptualize the relations between inter-
model mapping the network of relations had est and engagement in relation to classroom activ-
acceptable fit for all four countries (RMSEA: ities consists in a dynamic self-organizing system.
Sweden = 0.043; USA = 0.061; Colombia = 0.051; Interest is not static. At all educational levels,
and Estonia = 0.039). Again, the coefficients link- activities that are attractive, in terms of color,
ing the variables were strongest for Sweden and sound, and movement, novel, complex, or uncer-
weakest for Colombia, and the proportion of vari- tain can be used to trigger students’ interest or to
ance in the engagement indicator (embedded capture students’ attention. However, this is only
interest) predicted by the model varied from 46% the first step to the forms and quality of engage-
for Sweden to 22% for Colombia. The network as ment with classroom activities that are likely to
represented in the final model showed a sequence lead to acquisition of knowledge and understand-
linking knowledge of science and personal value ing. Using a dynamic systems perspective, it has
of science with the engagement variable through been argued that an interest reaction akin to trig-
the mediators of firstly, enjoyment of science and gered situational interest is the first step in the
secondly, general interest in learning science. development of a more extended individual inter-
There were also some additional direct effects on est. This in turn consists of an organized schema
engagement, but these were relatively small. The combining knowledge, value, and affect and is
main path linked knowledge and value through manifest in students seeking opportunities to
enjoyment and interest to predict engagement in engage and reengage with content from that inter-
the form of wanting to reengage with the topic to est domain (Hidi & Renninger, 2006).
find out and understand more. From a systems’ perspective, specific local-
The strength of the relations within the net- ized interest processes are significant as they
work of science variables differed between sam- contribute to the overall configuration of a stu-
ples of students from countries with different dent’s behavior. These extend from the micropro-
historical and cultural traditions. They were stron- cess level, nested within increasingly broader
gest in the country chosen to represent countries units of process and action, to the macrolevel of
with secular-rational and self-expression values the historical, cultural traditions within which
and weaker in the country chosen to represent that student’s educational experiences exist.
more traditional and survival values. However, Approaching interest and engagement in class-
these analyses do provide evidence that our mod- room activities from this perspective, at the theo-
els linking the set of variables referred to by the retical level, provides direction for further
PISA researchers as interest in science has been investigation of the substance and dynamic of
shown to apply beyond the countries where much interest and its related processes. At the practical
of the work on motivation and engagement has level, the challenge is to apply this knowledge to
been conducted. Although limited in scope to an support students’ learning. How many of the stu-
assessment of relations between the specific pro- dents in classes characterized as difficult or chal-
cess variables included in the PISA framework, lenging might become engaged if the content of
300 M. Ainley

learning activities connected with their existing ahead: Theoretical perspectives on motivation and
interests? Knowledge of situational factors or the achievement (Vol. 16A, pp. 235–264). Bingley, UK:
Emerald Group.
hooks that can attract students to an activity is Ainley, M., & Ainley, J. (2011a). Interest in science: Part
part of the story, but for the gears to engage and of the complex structure of student motivation in sci-
generate forward movement, other processes ence. International Journal of Science Education,
must come into play. The analyses of PISA data 33(1), 51–71.
Ainley, M., & Ainley, J. (2011b). Student engagement
demonstrated how personal value and enjoyment with science in early adolescence: The contribution of
are two key processes that are part of the dynamic enjoyment to students’ continuing interest in learning
system when students express a desire to reen- about science. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
gage with content they have been examining. 36(1), 4–12.
Ainley, M., Buckley, S., & Chan, J. (2009). Interest and
When the content of learning activities pertains efficacy beliefs in self-regulated learning: Does the
to something that is valued and/or is perceived to task make a difference? In M. Wosnitza, S. A.
be enjoyable, students choose to engage and Karabenick, A. Efklides, & P. Nenniger (Eds.),
often seek to reengage if given the opportunity. Contemporary motivation research: From global to
local perspectives (pp. 207–227). Göttingen, Germany:
The hook is no longer essential, but opportunities Hogrefe & Huber.
for reengagement with the achievement domain Ainley, M., Corrigan, M., & Richardson, N. (2005).
are critical. To a large extent, what we know Students, tasks and emotions: Identifying the contri-
about the workings of the system of processes bution of emotions to students’ reading of popular cul-
ture and popular science texts. Learning and
that connect students with learning domains Instruction, 15(5), 433–447.
relate to individual students or groups of students. Ainley, M., Flowers, D., & Patrick, L. (2005). The impact
Simultaneously, individual students are nested of mood on learning – The impact of task on mood.
within contexts extending from local friendship Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the
European Association for Research in Learning and
groups within their classes to the broader cultural Instruction (EARLI), Nicosia, Cyprus.
context that surrounds them. At each level, these Ainley, M., Hidi, S., & Berndorff, D. (2002). Interest,
contexts can influence the development of stu- learning and the psychological processes that mediate
dents’ personal interests, values, and access to their relationship. Journal of Educational Psychology,
94(3), 545–561.
knowledge and understanding. Ainley, M., Hillman, K., & Hidi, S. (2002). Gender and
Our analysis has specifically focused on rela- interest processes in response to literary texts:
tions between interest and engagement and has Situational and individual interest. Learning and
Instruction, 12(4), 411–428.
shown that what is known about the relations
Ainley, M., & Patrick, L. (2006). Measuring self-regulated
between these two constructs necessarily impli- learning processes through tracking patterns of student
cates a wider range of psychological processes interaction with achievement activities. Educational
that together function as dynamic systems when- Psychology Review, 18(3), 267–286.
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A.
ever students interact with classroom activities.
L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological
engagement: Validation of the student engagement
instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44(5),
427–445.
References Arbuckle, J. (2005). AMOS 6.0 user’s guide. Chicago, IL:
SPSS Inc.
Ainley, M. (1993). Styles of engagement with learning: Asakawa, K., & Csikszentimihalyi, M. (1998). The qual-
Multidimensional assessment of their relationship ity of experience of Asian American adolescents in
with strategy use and school achievement. Journal of academic activities: An exploration of educational
Educational Psychology, 85(3), 395–405. achievement. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
Ainley, M. (2007). Being and feeling interested: Transient 8(2), 241–262.
state, mood, and disposition. In P. Schutz & R. Pekrun Boekaerts, M., de Koning, E., & Vedder, P. (2006). Goal-
(Eds.), Emotions and education (pp. 147–163). directed behavior and contextual factors in the class-
Burlington, MA: Academic. room: An innovative approach to the study of multiple
Ainley, M. (2010). Interest in the dynamics of task behav- goals. Educational Psychologist, 41(1), 33–51.
ior: Processes that link person and task in effective Boydston, J. A. (Ed.). (1979). John Dewey: The middle
learning. In T. Urdan & S. A. Karabenick (Eds.), works 1899–1924 (Vol. 7: 1912–1914). London:
Advances in motivation and achievement. The decade Southern Illinois University Press.
13 Students’ Interest and Engagement in Classroom Activities 301

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. Hidi, S., & Ainley, M. (2007). Interest and self-regulation:
In R. Vasra (Ed.), Six theories of child development Relationships between two variables that influence
(pp. 187–249). London: Jessica Kingsley Publications. learning. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.),
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory,
of developmental processes. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), research, and applications (pp. 77–109). Mahwah,
Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., Vol. 1: NJ: Erlbaum.
Theoretical models of human development, pp. Hidi, S., & Baird, W. (1988). Strategies for increasing
535–584). New York: Wiley. text-based interest and students’ recall of expository
Buckley, S., Hasen, G., & Ainley, M. (2004, November). texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 23(4), 465–483.
Affective engagement: A person-centred approach to Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the
understanding the structure of subjective learning academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st
experiences. Paper presented at the Australian century. Review of Educational Research, 70(2),
Association for Research in Education (AARE), 151–179.
Melbourne, Australia. Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with model of interest development. Educational
AMOS. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Psychologist, 41(2), 111–127.
Chen, A., & Ennis, C. D. (2004). Goal, interest, and learn- Hulleman, C. S., & Senko, C. (2010). Up and around the
ing in physical education. The Journal of Educational bend: Forecasts for achievement goal theory and
Research, 97(6), 329–338. research in 2020. In T. Urdan & S. A. Karabenick
Dekker, S., & Fischer, R. (2008). Cultural differences in (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement. The
academic motivation goals: A meta-analysis across 13 decade ahead: Theoretical perspectives on motivation
societies. The Journal of Educational Research, and achievement (Vol. 16A, pp. 71–104). Bingley,
102(2), 99–110. UK: Emerald Group.
Ely, R., Ainley, M., & Pearce, J. (2010). A new method for Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, cul-
identifying dimensions of interest: MINE. Paper pre- tural change, and the persistence of traditional values.
sented at the International Conference on Motivation American Sociological Review, 65(1), 19–51.
(ICM), Porto, Portugal. Inglehart, R., & Welzel, C. (2005). Modernization, cul-
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). tural change and democracy: The human development
Student engagement: Potential of the concept, state of sequence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1), Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the
59–109. value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions motivation. Journal of Personality and Social
in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory Psychology, 76(3), 349–366.
of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum
218–226. Press.
Frenzel, A., Dicke, A.-L., Goetz, T., & Pekrun, R. (2009). Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emo-
Quantitative and qualitative insights into the develop- tion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on
ment of interest in adolescence. Paper presented at the Psychological Science, 2(3), 260–280.
European Association for Research in Learning and Jimerson, S. R., Campos, E., & Greif, J. L. (2003). Toward
Instruction (EARLI), Amsterdam, The Netherlands. an understanding of definitions and measures of school
Fullarton, S. (2002). Student engagement with school: engagement and related terms. The California School
Individual and school level influences (LSAY Research Psychologist, 8(1), 7–27.
Report No. 27). Camberwell, Australia: ACER. Jörg, T., Davis, B., & Nickmans, G. (2007). Towards a
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, new, complexity science of learning and education.
A. J. (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitu- Educational Research Review, 2(2), 145–156.
dinal study of achievement goals and ability measures Krapp, A. (2003). Interest and human development: An
as predictors of interest and performance from fresh- educational-psychological perspective. Development
man year through graduation. Journal of Educational and Motivation, BJEP Monograph Series, II(2),
Psychology, 94(3), 562–575. 57–84.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Durik, A. M., Barron, K. E., Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of
Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., & Tauer, J. M. (2008). The interest and intrinsic motivational orientations.
role of achievement goals in the development of interest: Learning and Instruction, 15(5), 381–395.
Reciprocal relations between achievement goals, Larson, R. W., & Verma, S. (1999). How children and
interest, and performance. Journal of Educational adolescents spend time across the world: Work, play,
Psychology, 100(1), 105–122. and developmental outcomes. Psychological Bulletin,
Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental 125(6), 701–736.
resource for learning. Review of Educational Research, Lewis, M. D., & Granic, I. (Eds.). (2000). Emotion, devel-
60(3), 549–571. opment, and self-organization: Dynamic systems
Hidi, S. (2006). Interest: A unique motivational variable. approaches to emotional development. Cambridge,
Educational Research Review, 1(2), 69–82. UK: Cambridge University Press.
302 M. Ainley

Linnakylä, P., & Malin, A. (2008). Finnish students’ regulation in achievement situations. In M. Wosnitza,
school engagement profiles in the light of PISA 2003. S. A. Karabenick, A. Efklides, & P. Nenniger (Eds.),
Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 52(6), Contemporary motivation research: From global to
583–602. local perspectives (pp. 321–338). Göttingen, Germany:
Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional Hogrefe & Huber.
activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, T.
school years. American Educational Research Journal, (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom:
37(1), 153–184. Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of
Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: Its multifaceted Educational Psychology, 100(4), 765–781.
structure in the secondary school mathematics class- Sun, H., Chen, A., Ennis, C. D., Martin, R., & Shen, B.
room. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), (2008). An examination of the multidimensionality of
424–436. situational interest in elementary school physical edu-
Murphy, P. K., & Alexander, P. A. (2000). A motivated cation. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,
exploration of motivation terminology. Contemporary 79(1), 62–70.
Educational Psychology, 25(1), 3–53. Tapola, A., Dayez, J.-B., & Veermans, M. (2009).
OECD. (2006). Assessing scientific, reading and mathe- Students’ experience in a simulation-based science
matical literacy: A framework for PISA 2006. Paris: learning task: The role of individual and situational
OECD. factors. Paper presented at the European Association
OECD. (2007). PISA 2006: Science competencies for for Research in Learning and Instruction (EARLI),
tomorrow’s world (Vol. 1: Analysis). Paris: OECD. Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
OECD. (2009). PISA 2006 technical report. Paris: The Australian Oxford Paperback Dictionary. (1999).
OECD. (2nd ed.). Melbourne: Oxford University Press.
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002). Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (2006). Dynamic systems theo-
Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learn- ries. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook
ing and achievement: A program of qualitative and of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 258–312).
quantitative research. Educational Psychologist, Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
37(2), 91–105. Turner, J. C. (2010). Unfinished business: Putting motiva-
Rathunde, K., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). Undivided tion theory to the “classroom test”. In T. Urdan & S. A.
interest and the growth of talent: A longitudinal study Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in motivation and
of adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, achievement: The decade ahead: Applications and
22(4), 385–405. contexts of motivation and achievement (Vol. 16B,
Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2006). Prediction of pp. 109–138). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group.
dropouts among students with mild disabilities: A case Turner, J. E., & Waugh, R. M. (2007). A dynamical sys-
for the inclusion of student engagement variables. tems perspective regarding students’ learning pro-
Remedial and Special Education, 27(5), 276–292. cesses: Shame reactions and emergent
Rotgans, J. I., & Schmidt, H. G. (2011). Situational inter- self-organizations. In P. Schutz & R. Pekrun (Eds.),
est and academic achievement in the active-learning Emotions and education (pp. 125–145). Burlington,
classroom. Learning and Instruction, 21(1), 58–67. MA: Academic.
doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.1011.1001. Wade, S. E. (2001). Research on importance and interest:
Russell, J., Ainley, M., & Frydenberg, E. (2005). Schooling Implications for curriculum development and future
issues digest: Student motivation and engagement. research. Educational Psychology Review, 13(3),
Canberra, Australia: Australian Government, 243–261.
Department of Education Science and Training. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Emotions, moods,
Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. B. (2005). Interest as the traits and temperaments: Conceptual distinctions and
missing motivator in self-regulation. European empirical findings. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson
Psychologist, 10(3), 175–186. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions
Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). (pp. 89–93). New York: Oxford University Press.
Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a Willms, J. D. (2003). Student engagement at school: A
self-regulatory mechanism. Journal of Personality sense of belonging and participation. Results from
and Social Psychology, 63(3), 379–390. PISA 2000. Paris: OECD.
Schraw, G., & Lehman, S. (2001). Situational Interest: Wosnitza, M., & Volet, S. (2009). A framework for per-
A review of the literature and directions for future sonal goals in collaborative learning contexts. In M.
research. Educational Psychology Review, 13(1), Wosnitza, S. A. Karabenick, A. Efklides, & P. Nenniger
23–52. (Eds.), Contemporary motivation research: From
Sideridis, G. D. (2009). Normative vs. non-normative per- global to local perspectives (pp. 49–67). Gottingen,
formance goals: Effects on behavioral and emotional Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.
Part II Commentary: Motivation
and Engagement: Conceptual, 14
Operational, and Empirical Clarity

Andrew J. Martin

Abstract
A noted scholar in the field of engagement, Andrew Martin, provided com-
mentary on the chapters in Part II. Martin summarized the theories and
definitions offered by authors in this part and shared his perspective on
motivation and engagement. He argued for the inclusion of disengagement
in addition to engagement in future research and discourse in this area.
Martin concluded by proposing a framework and analytic model to inte-
grate authors’ ideas and to test tenets of various conceptualizations of
engagement and motivation.

As discussed by all authors in this part, motivation ment) are presented. The commentary concludes
and engagement are fundamental components of with a proposed framework for integrating central
the learning process. Motivation and engagement ideas from these chapters and a suggested ana-
provide the energy, direction, and skill set required lytic model that can test many contentions made
to effectively tackle academic subject matter. (here and elsewhere) about motivation and
Further, being relatively malleable, they are a engagement.
point of important educational intervention for
students, practitioners, and parents/caregivers.
This commentary on motivation and engagement Contributions by Part Authors
summarizes the essence of theories, definitions,
factors, and processes presented by authors in this Perhaps the most difficult task for motivation and
part. Following this, some additional perspectives engagement researchers relates to conceptual and
on motivation and engagement (and disengage- operational clarity. In each chapter, the authors
demarcated motivation and engagement in terms
of the theories underpinning them, the factors or
constructs relevant to them, and the processes
A.J. Martin, Ph.D. (*)
Faculty of Education and Social Work,
and contexts in which they occur in students’ aca-
University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia demic lives. Major ideas relevant to each of these
e-mail: andrew.martin@sydney.edu.au are briefly summarized.

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 303


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_14, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
304 A.J. Martin

Theories Explaining and Describing comprises “publicly observable behavior.” This


Motivation and Engagement inner vs. outer concept is also described by Cleary
and Zimmerman, demarcating engagement into
Conceptual approaches to differentiating motiva- observable factors (e.g., behavioral) and internal
tion and engagement spanned self-determination, factors (e.g., cognition and affect) (see also
goal, attribution, social-cognitive, dynamic sys- Christenson et al., 2008). Cleary and Zimmerman
tems, humanistic, achievement-motivation, and focus on cognitive engagement that encompasses
ecological theories. According to Reeve (2012), strategies and regulatory processes and position
self-determination theory (SDT; Ryan & Deci, self-efficacy as a form of motivation that leads to
2000) focuses on vitalizing students’ inner states cognitive self-regulatory activity (cognitive
and thus is centrally concerned with motivation. engagement).
Anderman and Patrick (2012) emphasize achieve- Anderman and Patrick describe the engage-
ment goal theory (Elliot, 1999) describing how ment framework of Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and
this is a useful way to disentangle motivation and Paris (2004), disaggregating engagement in
engagement – with goals (motivation) being a affective, cognitive, and behavioral terms.
significant basis for subsequent engagement. In Similarly, Schunk and Mullen describe engage-
shaping arguments around school identification, ment in terms of affective, cognitive, and behav-
Voelkl (2012) draws on theories of human needs ioral components and motivation in terms of an
(e.g., Maslow, 1968) and perspectives on identifi- energizing function that impacts these. Pekrun
cation by Finn (1989). Schunk and Mullen (2012) and Linnenbrink-Garcia extend the cognitive,
draw on social-cognitive theory (Bandura, 1997, affective, and behavioral engagement framework
2001) to articulate a model comprising self-effi- to five components: motivational (e.g., goals),
cacy, motivation, and engagement. In their chapter, cognitive (e.g., attention, memory), behavioral
two aspects of social-cognitive theory are empha- (e.g., effort, persistence), cognitive-behavioral
sized: self-efficacy (as a social-cognitive variable) (e.g., self-regulation), and social-behavioral (e.g.,
and context (as the social environment in which social on-task behavior). Pekrun and Linnenbrink-
motivation and engagement occur). Cleary and Garcia then focus on emotional engagement and
Zimmerman (2012) also draw on social-cognitive further unpack emotional engagement into affect,
perspectives to describe how environment, person, mood, achievement emotions, epistemic emo-
and behavior interact such that, for example, cogni- tions, topic emotions, and social emotions.
tion underlies effective academic behavior. Through the school identification perspective,
Focusing on emotion, Pekrun and Linnenbrink- Voelkl makes a clear distinction between affec-
Garcia (2012) traverse cognitive, social-cognitive, tive and behavioral factors and suggests identifi-
and achievement theories to differentiate multiple cation with school as a form of motivation that
aspects of emotion and their role in the motivation- then leads to engagement in appropriate learning
engagement process. Harnessing dynamic systems behaviors. Ainley also describes motivation in
(e.g., Lewis & Granic, 2000) and ecological terms of inner psychological factors and engage-
perspectives (Bronfenbrenner, 1992), Ainley ment in terms of the level and nature of involve-
(2012) situates interest as a motivational factor ment in an activity. Ainley’s focus is on interest
impacting subsequent engagement. and, similar to Voelkl, describes interest in terms
of motivation – in contrast to other perspectives
that position interest as part of engagement (e.g.,
Factors Residing Within Motivation see Reschly & Christenson, 2006).
and Engagement Frameworks Importantly, in one way or another, all authors
recognize an “agentic” perspective on motivation
According to Reeve, motivation comprises and engagement. For example, Reeve explicitly
“private, unobservable, psychological, neural, includes agentic engagement in his framing of
and biological” factors, whereas engagement engagement and its factors. The social-cognitive
14 Part II Commentary: Motivation and Engagement… 305

model described by Schunk and Mullen also Other authors unpacked processes relevant to
explicitly positions human agency as part of the affect, emotion, and interest. For example, Voelkl
motivation-engagement process such that students separates affective school identification from
exert control over their cognition, affect, and behavioral school identification and argues that
behavior. Cleary and Zimmerman emphasize stu- affective identification precedes behavioral iden-
dents’ proactive engagement with tasks – moving tification. Voelkl further suggests engagement as
beyond “reactive” models of engagement. a mediator between motivation and achievement.
Pekrun and Linnenbrink-Garcia emphasize the
role of emotions and detail evidence showing
The Process of Motivation how emotions impact motivational, cognitive,
and Engagement behavioral, cognitive-behavioral, and social-
behavioral engagement. They also suggest that
Striving for clarity in the motivation-engagement engagement mediates the relationship between
domain also requires researchers to articulate the emotion and achievement. Another argument for
operational process underlying motivation and the mediating role of engagement is proposed by
engagement. At this point, theories, definitions, Ainley who describes how interest (part of moti-
and factors can be disentangled and tested empir- vation) leads to achievement via engagement.
ically. Across the chapters, there appears to be Consistent with Hidi and Renninger (2006; see
broad agreement that motivation underpins also Krapp, 2005), Ainley further delineates the
engagement and that engagement leads to out- sequence of interest from an initial triggering of
comes such as achievement. Thus, while there situational interest through to well-established
may be some disagreement as to what factors are and sustained individual interest.
deemed to be motivation and what are seen as All authors recognize a loop or reciprocity in
engagement, it seems to be the case that motiva- which motivation and engagement (and achieve-
tion is a basis for subsequent engagement. ment) occur. Reeve notes the reciprocal relation-
Based on SDT, Reeve describes how students’ ships linking context, motivation, engagement,
inner motivational resources allow them to fully and outcomes. Cleary and Zimmerman describe a
engage in the classroom. Anderman and Patrick cyclical feedback loop for cognitive engagement
point out that motivation occurs both before and (self-regulation) that functions in a temporal
during a task whereas engagement is predomi- sequence comprising cognitive engagement
nantly operational during a task. They describe before, during, and after a learning task. Invoking
how goals (motivation) precede students’ cogni- dynamic systems theory (Turner & Waugh, 2007;
tive engagement (e.g., self-regulation), emotional see also Lewis & Granic, 2000), Pekrun and
engagement (e.g., positive affect about school), Linnenbrink-Garcia describe loops in the process
and behavioral engagement (e.g., effort). as achievement impacts appraisals and emotions
Similarly, Cleary and Zimmerman distinguish relevant to subsequent motivation and engage-
between the “will” of the student and the “skill” ment. Ainley also invokes the dynamic systems
of the student (see also Covington, 1998), with the perspective to assist understanding of the rela-
former indicating motivation and the latter indi- tions between interest and engagement.
cating engagement. Thus, for example, motiva-
tional beliefs will predict cognitive self-regulation.
Schunk and Miller also detail the energizing Context Considerations Relevant
role of motivation on engagement, with engage- to Motivation and Engagement
ment seen as “the manifestation of students’
motivation.” They also detail the role of self- All authors emphasize the central role of context
efficacy in impacting both motivation and in formulations of motivation, engagement, and
engagement and the impact of these processes achievement. Reeve points out that for motiva-
on achievement. tion and engagement to flourish, students require
306 A.J. Martin

a supportive environment, particularly a strong exploring developmental shifts and trajectories in


teacher-student relationship. In similar vein, students’ motivation and engagement. Ainley also
Voelkl identifies contextual conditions central to indicates the need for longitudinal research into
school identification including being supported motivation and engagement trajectories. Along
by teachers, being treated fairly, and feelings of similar methodological lines, Voelkl suggests lon-
safety. Under a social-cognitive framework, gitudinal research that examines how school iden-
Cleary and Zimmerman identify situation depen- tification is internalized and how context influences
dence and context specificity in shaping individ- school identification. Schunk and Mullen also
ual student engagement. highlight the need for contextual research and the
Anderman and Patrick extend the individual- need for cross-cultural studies into learning and
level goal orientation constructs to describe engagement.
classroom goal structures that are individuals’ Cleary and Zimmerman recommend interven-
subjective perceptions of the meaning and pur- tion work that tracks shifts (including at the
pose of tasks within the classroom. Notably, these microlevel) in engagement as a function of tar-
goal structures impact students’ individual moti- geted practice. They also suggest that this research
vation (goals) that then impacts cognitive, behav- should focus on tasks with a clear beginning and
ioral, and emotional engagement and achievement. end so as to understand the full process of regula-
Pekrun and Linnenbrink-Garcia also draw on tory engagement within them (e.g., writing
goal theory to describe how goal structures an essay). Pekrun and Linnenbrink-Garcia sug-
impact emotions and emotional engagement. gest investigating the impact of task design
Similar to goal structure concepts, Schunk and and learning environments on emotions. Also,
Mullen describe how collective agency refers to having identified numerous emotions relevant
students’ shared beliefs about what they are capa- to the learning process, they urge research
ble of achieving as a class. In the development of that investigates the distinct influence of each
self-efficacy and engagement, they also identify on learning and achievement and intervention
family, sociocultural (e.g., cultural capital), peer, research seeking to foster positive emotion in the
and educational (e.g., instruction) influences. classroom. They also signal the importance of
Ainley draws on Bronfenbrenner’s (1992) eco- emerging neuroscientific research in shedding
logical model to describe the layers of contextual further light on emotional engagement as relevant
influence that impact individual development, to learning.
including their academic development by way of
motivation and engagement.
Additional Contributions
to Motivation and Engagement
Future Directions Offered by Authors
In addition to what has been offered in these
Collecting together such a wealth of expertise chapters, it is perhaps useful to recognize other
enables some powerful observations about future approaches that explicitly integrate motivation
directions for motivation and engagement research. and engagement into operationalization and mea-
Reeve suggests a number of directions for future surement. This author has suggested one such
research, including testing a hypothesized four- approach – the Motivation and Engagement
factor model of engagement comprising cognition, Wheel – that encompasses diverse motivation
affect, behavior, and agency and demonstrating and engagement theorizing and research (Martin,
that changes in these lead to changes in hypothe- 2007, 2009, 2010). The Motivation and
sized outcomes. Anderman and Patrick suggest Engagement Wheel reflects thinking offered by
research into engagement while students are actu- Pintrich (2003) who identified seven substantive
ally carrying out tasks using methods such as areas for the development of an integrative moti-
experience sampling. They also suggest further vational science. He underscored the importance
14 Part II Commentary: Motivation and Engagement… 307

ADAPTIVE COGNITION ADAPTIVE BEHAVIOR


(Adaptive Motivation) (Adaptive Engagement)
Persistence
Valuing

Mastery Planning
orientation

Self- Task
efficacy management

Anxiety
Disengagement

Failure
avoidance
Self-
handicapping Uncertain
control

MALADAPTIVE/IMPEDING
MALADAPTIVE BEHAVIOR COGNITION
(Maladaptive Engagement) (Maladaptive Motivation)

Fig. 14.1 Motivation and Engagement Wheel (Reproduced with permission from Martin 2010)

of considering a model of motivation from salient and (vi) persistence; (3) impeding/maladaptive
and seminal theorizing related to: self-efficacy, cognition (or maladaptive motivation), reflecting
attributions, valuing, control, self-determination, students’ attitudes and orientations inhibiting aca-
goal orientation, need achievement, self-regulation, demic learning, including (vii) anxiety, (viii) failure
and self-worth. Martin (2007, 2009) identified avoidance, and (ix) uncertain control; and (4) mal-
congruencies across a number of these themes adaptive behavior (or maladaptive engagement),
and integrated them into a multidimensional reflecting students’ problematic learning behav-
framework representing adaptive and maladap- iors, including (x) self-handicapping and (xi) dis-
tive cognition and behavior. engagement. The wheel is presented in Fig. 14.1.
This framework – the Motivation and While seeking to integrate conceptual terrain,
Engagement Wheel – and its accompanying mea- there is also a much applied purpose to the wheel.
surement tool, the Motivation and Engagement It seeks to articulate a motivation and engagement
Scale (MES, see Martin, 2011), comprised four framework that is readily accessible to practitio-
higher-order factors (or clusters) and 11 first-order ners (e.g., teachers, counselors, psychologists),
factors: (1) adaptive cognition (or adaptive moti- parents/caregivers, and students. In so doing, it
vation), reflecting students’ positive attitudes attempts to bridge a gap between diverse dimen-
and orientations to academic learning, including sions of motivation and engagement theorizing on
(i) self-efficacy, (ii) valuing, and (iii) mastery ori- the one hand and, on the other hand, the need for
entation; (2) adaptive behavior (or adaptive practitioners to draw on the strengths of these
engagement), reflecting students’ positive behav- dimensions within a parsimonious framework
iors and engagement in academic learning, includ- that they can clearly communicate to students and
ing (iv) planned behavior, (v) task management, their parents/caregivers (Martin, 2007).
308 A.J. Martin

relates to the space in which the learning-related


Locating Motivation and Engagement factors operate. These dimensions are presented
in Time and Space in Fig. 14.2, referred to here as a Sequence ×
Space Learning Map. The horizontal axis repre-
The set of chapters and other recent perspectives
sents sequence, and the vertical axis represents
(e.g., the Motivation and Engagement Wheel)
space.
provide substantial detail on seminal and emerg-
The lower end of the sequence axis reflects
ing theories, concepts, methodologies, and ana-
learning antecedents (e.g., motivation), and the
lytical approaches to motivation and engagement.
upper end reflects learning consequences (e.g.,
The task now is to distill and synthesize funda-
achievement). Between the lower and upper ends
mental ideas and contributions in a way that
are mediators such as engagement (as suggested
assists conceptual and operational clarity. One
in most chapters in this part). The lower end of
approach is to consider motivation and engage-
the space axis reflects the individual (e.g., stu-
ment in time and space.
dent). The space axis is then progressively repre-
sented by classroom and school levels. Taken
The Sequence × Space Learning Map together, this Sequence × Space Learning Map
seeks to synthesize some of the major conceptual
Considering each contribution in this part, it is and operational arguments represented in this
evident that two primary dimensions consistently chapter. It specifies the ordering of motivation
emerge. One dimension relates to the sequence of and engagement in determining achievement and
learning-related factors. The other dimension the level/s at which these processes play out.

SPACE
SPACE

SCHOOL

School goal School-average School-average School-average


structure anxiety attendance achievement

Class goal Class-average Class-average Class-average


structure anxiety attendance achievement

SEQUENCE

MOTIVATION ENGAGEMENT ENGAGEMENT OUTCOME

Student goal Student Student Student


orientation anxiety attendance achievement

STUDENT

Fig. 14.2 Sequence × Space Learning Map – conceptualizing motivation and engagement in sequence and space
14 Part II Commentary: Motivation and Engagement… 309

TIME 1 TIME 2 TIME n

Motivation Outcome Motivation Outcome


(Goal structure) (Achievement) (Goal structure) (Achievement)

Cog/ Emot Behavioral Cog/ Emot Behavioral


Engagement Engagement Engagement Engagement
LEVEL 3: SCHOOL (Anxiety) (Attendance) (Anxiety) (Attendance)

Motivation Outcome Motivation Outcome


(Goal structure) (Achievement) (Goal structure) (Achievement)

Cog/ Emot Behavioral Cog/ Emot Behavioral


Engagement Engagement Engagement Engagement
(Anxiety) (Attendance) (Anxiety) (Attendance)
LEVEL 2: CLASS

Motivation Outcome Motivation Outcome


(Goal orient) (Achievement) (Goal orient) (Achievement)

Cog/ Emot Behavioral Cog/ Emot Behavioral


Engagement Engagement Engagement Engagement
LEVEL 1: STUDENT (Anxiety) (Attendance) (Attendance)
(Anxiety)

Fig. 14.3 Sequence × Space Learning Analysis – empirically investigating motivation and engagement sequence and
space

To illustrate, four factors from the part are The Sequence × Space Learning
plotted in the Sequence × Space Learning Map. Analysis
At far left is motivation represented by student
goals and classroom and school goal structures. The Sequence × Space Learning Map offers con-
Further along is emotional/cognitive (depending ceptual and operational integration of core moti-
on an author’s perspective) engagement repre- vation and engagement constructs. This map can
sented by student anxiety and anxiety at class- also be used to specify a Sequence × Space
room and school levels. Further again is Learning Analysis that tests major arguments and
behavioral engagement represented by student processes relevant to motivation, engagement,
attendance and also by class- and school-average and achievement.
attendance numbers/patterns. Following from As Fig. 14.3 shows, the Sequence × Space
attendance is student achievement as well as Learning Analysis is a multilevel design with stu-
class- and school-average achievement. dent at level 1, classroom at level 2, and school at
Consistent with most accounts in this part, level 3. It comprises motivation, engagement, and
there is a feedback loop (indicated by an arrow) achievement measures at each level on the space
on the sequence axis, indicating that achievement axis. It is longitudinal in terms of the sequence of
then impacts motivation. There are feedback motivation, engagement, and achievement and
loops at both ends of the space axis, indicating also in terms of feedback loops across time.
that context impacts the individual and the indi- Parameters operate across sequence and space
vidual impacts context. There are also diagonal with student-level factors, for example, predict-
axes indicating that motivation, engagement, and ing class- and school-level factors.
achievement impact each other across sequence The Sequence × Space Learning Analysis can
and space. For example, class-level goal struc- be adapted to differentiate or expand the sequence
ture (motivation) impacts subsequent student- and space axes. For example, authors in this part
level anxiety (cognitive/emotional engagement). frequently referred to microanalysis of processes
310 A.J. Martin

inherent in their models. In this case, the sequence and operationalize persistence at school in terms
axis would be further differentiated into subele- of the joint forces of “switching on” (engage-
ments, and these elements can be plotted in ment) and “switching off” (disengagement). To
Fig. 14.3 and tested accordingly. Conversely, the the extent that this is the case, there will be impli-
sequence axis can be expanded to test processes cations of this multidimensional engagement/dis-
across years – particularly important for under- engagement perspective for theorizing and
standing the developmental nature of engage- analyses involving motivation, learning, and
ment and motivation. Other authors commented achievement.
on national and international contexts. In this
case, the space axis would be expanded to the
country level, such as in PISA datasets. Or the Engagement and Disengagement:
space axis can be further differentiated to encom- A Shared Responsibility
pass intraindividual factors such as genetics and
neural profiles – a direction suggested by Pekrun A major message from these chapters is that
and Linnenbrink-Garcia. engagement logically follows from a constella-
tion of contextual (e.g., teacher) and student fac-
tors. Put another way, it seems that engagement is
What of Disengagement? a rational response to contextual and individual
influences. It is good to know that this is the case
It is also worth observing that across the set of because it enables the logical development of tar-
contributions, disengagement received relatively geted educational intervention to promote
little direct attention (though it was implied engagement. It also underscores the need for
throughout all chapters). Across approximately action on the part of practitioners (e.g., teachers,
70,000 words of text in seven chapters, the word psychologists, counselors), parents/caregivers,
“disengagement” (“disengag*”) was used about and students – a shared responsibility frequently
20 times. In contrast, the word “engagement” emphasized in the chapters in this part. A neces-
(“engag*”) was used approximately 900 times. In sary corollary to this is that we must also con-
important ways, this differential is quite under- clude that disengagement is a rational response to
standable in a volume focusing on “engagement.” contextual and individual influences. In some
On the other hand, one could be forgiven to expect ways, this is a more challenging notion. It means
somewhat greater direct attention to its counter- that disengagement is a shared responsibility –
part, “disengagement,” and its relations/juxtapo- and for various reasons, practitioners (e.g.,
sition with motivation. Is this because we tend to teachers, psychologists, counselors), parents/
see engagement and disengagement on a unidi- caregivers, and students may have difficulty rec-
mensional continuum? Is low (or no) engagement ognizing and/or accepting this.
simply disengagement? Is shared variance The authors in this part of this volume have
between disengagement and motivation the same clearly demonstrated that sharing the responsibil-
as that between engagement and motivation? ity for engagement and disengagement will pro-
In very recent work, Martin, Anderson, Bobis, mote positive academic pathways. They have
Way, and Vellar (2011) argue that learning, moti- identified specific factors, processes, and ideas
vation, and achievement require attention to both practitioners, parents/caregivers, and students
engagement and disengagement. Although the can harness to make this happen. Thanks to these
two are significantly correlated, it appears that authors – and also the researchers and work on
they explain unique variance in the academic which they draw – there is a solid conceptual and
process and thus should be addressed in comple- empirical basis for optimism as we support stu-
mentary but distinct ways. They conceptualize dents’ learning through school and beyond.
14 Part II Commentary: Motivation and Engagement… 311

Martin, A. J. (2007). Examining a multidimensional


References model of student motivation and engagement using a
construct validation approach. British Journal of
Ainley, M. (2012). Students’ Interest and engagement in Educational Psychology, 77, 413–440.
classroom activities. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Martin, A. J. (2009). Motivation and engagement
Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on across the academic lifespan: A developmental
student engagement (pp. 283–302). New York: construct validity study of elementary school, high
Springer. school, and university/college students. Educational
Anderman, E. M., & Patrick, H. (2012). Achievement and Psychological Measurement, 69, 794–824.
goal theory, conceptualization of ability/intelligence, Martin, A. J. (2010). Building classroom success: Eliminating
and classroom climate. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. academic fear and failure. London: Continuum.
Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on Martin, A. J. (2011). The motivation and engagement
student engagement (pp. 173–191). New York: scale (11th ed.). Sydney: Lifelong Achievement Group
Springer. (www.lifelongachievement.com).
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Martin, A. J., Anderson, J., Bobis, J., Way, J., & Vellar, R.
New York: Freeman. (2011). Switching on and switching off in mathemat-
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic ics: An ecological study of future intent and disen-
perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–26. gagement amongst middle school students. Journal of
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. Educational Psychology. 10.1037/a0025988.
In R. Vasra (Ed.), Six theories of child development Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being.
(pp. 187–249). London: Jessica Kingsley Publications. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., Appleton, J. J., Berman- Pekrun, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2012). Academic
Young, S., Spanjers, D. M., & Varro, P. (2008). Best emotions and student engagement. In S. L. Christenson,
practices in fostering student engagement. In A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of
A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in research on student engagement (pp. 259–282). New
school psychology (5th ed., pp. 1099–1119). Bethesda, York: Springer.
MD: National Association of School Psychologists. Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective
Cleary, T. J., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2012). A cyclical self- on the role of student motivation in learning and teach-
regulatory account of student engagement: theoretical ing contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,
foundations and applications. In S. L. Christenson, A. 667–686.
L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research Reeve, J. (2012). A self-determination theory perspective on
on student engagement (pp. 237–257). New York: student engagement. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly,
Springer. & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student
Covington, M. V. (1998). The will to learn: A guide for engagement (pp. 149–172). New York: Springer.
motivating young people. New York: Cambridge Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2006). Prediction of
University Press. dropouts among students with mild disabilities: A case
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation for the inclusion of student engagement variables.
and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, Remedial and Special Education, 27, 276–292.
169–189. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. development, and well-being. American Psychologist,
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). 55, 68–78.
Student engagement: Potential of the concept, state of Schunk, D. H., & Mullen, C. A. (2012). Self-efficacy as an
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, engaged learner. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, &
59–109. C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student
Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase engagement (pp. 219–235). New York: Springer.
model of interest development. Educational Turner, J. E., & Waugh, R. M. (2007). A dynamical sys-
Psychologist, 41, 111–127. tems perspective regarding students’ learning pro-
Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of cesses: Shame reactions and emergent self-organizations.
interest and intrinsic motivational orientations. In P. A. Schutz & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotions in educa-
Learning and Instruction, 15, 381–395. tion (pp. 125–145). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Lewis, M. D., & Granic, I. (Eds.). (2000). Emotion, devel- Voelkl, K. E. (2012). School identification. In S. L.
opment, and self-organization: Dynamic systems Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
approaches to emotional development. Cambridge, Handbook of research on student engagement
UK: Cambridge University Press. (pp. 193–218). New York: Springer.
Part III
Engagement and Contextual Influences
Parental Influences on Achievement
Motivation and Student 15
Engagement

Janine Bempechat and David J. Shernoff

Abstract
Underachievement and school disengagement have serious consequences,
both at individual and societal levels. In this chapter, we adopt a strength-
based perspective to examine the multiple ways in which parents foster
achievement motivation and student engagement. Our theoretical orienta-
tion is grounded in Bronfenbrenner’s (1977) ecological systems theory in
which the child is situated at the center of increasingly distal and intercon-
nected spheres of influence, from family and school to community and
societal institutions. Given the increasingly diverse composition of our
nation’s schools, we place a premium on understanding how varied ethnic
and cultural models of learning and socialization, particularly among low-
income families, differentially influence parents’ educational socialization
strategies and how these come to affect children’s developing achieve-
ment-related beliefs and behaviors. We examine several theoretical mod-
els of engagement, motivation, and parental involvement and highlight
some notable research efforts that seek to explain parents’ roles in foster-
ing motivation and engagement. We then share several models of innova-
tive programs that have experienced success in creating authentic
partnerships between parents, children, schools, and communities toward
the goal of stemming the tide of underachievement and disengagement.

J. Bempechat, Ed.D. (*) The Role of Parents in Student


Department of Psychology and Human Development, Motivation and Engagement
Wheelock College, Boston, MA, USA
e-mail: jbempechat@wheelock.edu
Underachievement and school disengagement
D.J. Shernoff, Ph.D. have serious consequences, both at individual
Department of Leadership, Educational Psychology,
and societal levels. Disengagement often mani-
and Foundations, Northern Illinois University,
DeKalb, IL, USA fests in a gradual cycle of withdrawal from
e-mail: dshernoff@niu.edu schooling, culminating in school dropout for

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 315


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_15, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
316 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

large numbers of youth (Finn, 1989). On some and higher teacher turnover than in higher
estimates, over one million (or 30%) of all ninth income neighborhoods. (Byrd-Blake et al., 2010;
graders in the USA fail to graduate from high Ingersoll, 2004; Johnson, 2006). Children in
school, with the dropout (nongraduation) rate those schools are also more likely to be held
approaching 50–55% in some urban communi- back one or more grades and experience higher
ties (Alliance for Excellent Education, 2009; rates of suspension, school dropout, and teen
Toppo, 2010). This dropout rate includes students pregnancy (Cauthen & Fass, 2008; Duncan &
from states in which graduation is dependent on Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Hauser, Pager, & Simmons,
both the successful completion of the 4-year high 2004).
school curriculum and the state-administered Parents are their children’s first and primary
high school leaving or exit examination. Cur- guides through their schooling experiences, and
rently, 17, or just over one-third of states, man- therefore can serve to greatly buffer or com-
date such an exit exam.1 pound risk factors for disengagement and low
Problems associated with underachievement achievement. The achievement-related beliefs
and disengagement are perhaps the most intense and behaviors of parents can have a profound
for students who live in poverty. Among US influence on how children come to perceive
children under 18 years of age, 39% live in their intellectual abilities and the value of learn-
poverty, disproportionate numbers of which ing and education (Eccles, Roeser, Vida,
are from ethnic minority groups (i.e., 60% of Fredricks, & Wigfield, 2006). Parents’ educa-
African American, 61% of Latino, 30% of Asian tional socialization strategies operate at two
American, and 26% of Caucasian children; levels that are inherently interconnected. Long
Cauthen & Fass, 2008). Immigrant students and before the start of formal schooling and through-
children of immigrants face additional chal- out their children’s schooling experiences, par-
lenges of acculturation (García Coll & Marks, ents engage in cognitive socialization strategies
2009; Suárez-Orozco, Suárez-Orozco, & Todo- to foster the development of intellectual skills
rova, 2008). Low-income children are more children need to succeed in school. For exam-
likely than their middle-class peers to live in ple, when parents ask questions about an
poor neighborhoods with higher rates of crime, assigned reading or possible solutions to a math
violence, and unemployment (Brooks-Gunn, problem, they are helping their children develop
Linver, & Fauth, 2005; Leventhal, Fauth, & critical thinking skills. Equally important, how-
Brooks-Gunn, 2005). In such neighborhoods, ever, are the subtle ways in which parents
children’s schools are likely to be of lower engage motivational socialization strategies to
quality, characterized by less qualified teachers foster the kinds of beliefs about learning that
encourage persistence, diligence, and the ability
to delay gratification. Thus, it is critically
important to shed light on how parents, in
collaboration with their children, their chil-
1
It is important to note that there is controversy around dren’s teachers, schools, and communities, can
how the high school dropout rate is calculated. The com- work to stem the tide of underachievement and
monly reported metric, based on a dataset (Common Core
disengagement.
of Data or CCD) managed by the US Department of
Education, is the averaged freshman graduation rate, In this chapter, we adopt a strength-based
which is said to reflect the percent of 9th graders who perspective to examine the multiple ways in
graduate on time, 4 years later. However, the CCD reports which parents foster achievement motivation
enrolled, but not entering, 9th graders. This means that the
and student engagement. Our theoretical ori-
dropout rate can include, at any time, the number of stu-
dents who were not promoted out of or are voluntarily entation is grounded in Bronfenbrenner’s
repeating the 9th grade (Roy & Mishel, 2008). (1977) ecological systems theory in which the
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 317

child is situated at the center of increasingly


distal and interconnected spheres of influence,
Theoretical Perspective
from family and school to community and
on Motivation and Engagement
societal institutions. Given the increasingly
Usually referring to students’ involvement with
diverse composition of our nation’s schools,
schooling, academics, or learning (e.g., Finn,
we place a premium on understanding how
1989, 1993; Marks, 2000; Skinner & Belmont,
varied ethnic and cultural models of learning
1993), there is fairly broad agreement that stu-
and socialization, particularly among low-
dent (or school) engagement involves both
income families, differentially influence par-
behaviors (e.g., completing assignments) and
ents’ educational socialization strategies and
emotions (e.g., belongingness) and encompasses
how these come to affect children’s developing
effort and persistence in schoolwork (Connell &
achievement-related beliefs and behaviors.
Wellborn, 1991; Newmann, 1992; Skinner &
Clearly, not all economically disadvantaged
Belmont, 1993; Smerdon, 1999). Engagement
and minority students are disengaged from
and motivation to learn (Stipek, 1993) are highly
school. There are many academically resilient
related and overlapping concepts, having many
students who beat the odds and demonstrate
commonalities as measurable constructs (see
high levels of achievement and engagement in
Fig. 15.1). However, motivation has been tradi-
school.
tionally viewed as a psychological construct,
In our own research, we are interested in
whereas engagement, even in its common defini-
understanding the more positive educational
tion, refers to an emotional involvement or
outcomes within these populations (Bempechat,
1998; Shernoff & Schmidt, 2008). We are inter-
ested in discovering: What are the learning
beliefs and dispositions that characterize the
motivational orientation of low-income and
racial/ethnic minority students? What emotions
and perceptions characterize such students’ Substantive/Deep
Engagement
engagement with schoolwork when they are Motivation
completing it? What are the influences on their
motivation and engagement? Specifically, to Flow
what extent may parental involvement and other
family-related variables influence such students’
motivation and engagement with school learning Short Term Outcomes
and achievement? Finally, what is the role of
culture in influencing educational socialization, Continuing Motivation
and what differences in parental influences and Mastery-Approach Goals
socialization strategies exist within different Academic Performance
cultures? We will here examine several theoreti-
cal models of engagement, motivation, and Long Term Outcomes
parental involvement and highlight some notable
Sustained Commitment
research efforts that seek to explain parents’
Domain Mastery
roles in fostering motivation and engagement.
Accomplishment in a Field
We will then share several models of innovative
programs that have experienced success in creat- Fig. 15.1 Conceptual model of the interaction between
ing authentic partnerships between parents, chil- deep engagement and motivation, with associated short-
dren, schools, and communities. term and long-term outcomes
318 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

commitment to some object and describes the engagement have much in common with those
experiential intensity of a relationship or interac- used previously in the motivational literature.
tion. It can refer to a sustained relationship, as Emotional engagement refers to students’ affect
with engagement in the process of schooling or a and emotions in schools, including interest, bore-
domain of interest, and also to one’s temporal dom, happiness, sadness, and anxiety (Finn, 1989;
involvement or interactions with activities and Shernoff, Csikszentmihalyi, Schneider, & Shernoff,
social partners in the immediate environments. 2003; Voelkl, 1997).
An increasing amount of attention is directed to Both motivation and engagement have been
student engagement because it is presumed to be conceptualized as a personal trait and context-
malleable and highly influenced by the learning varying psychological state (Fredricks et al.,
environment, and thus considered a means to 2004; Schunk, Pintrich, & Meece, 2008). We find
ameliorate downward student trajectories. this to be a useful distinction, and to simplify, we
Engagement is increasingly recognized to be a generally think of engagement as the quality of
complex, latent construct involving both observ- temporal interactions with the learning activity,
able (e.g., attending class) and unobservable (i.e., task, social companions, and other components of
“investment” in learning) psychological events the proximal environment, not dissimilar from the
and positive emotions (e.g., enjoyment and inter- concept of situational interest (Hidi & Anderson,
est) (Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, 2008). 1992; Mitchell, 1993), whereas we characterize
It is presumed to encompass actions and behaviors, motivation as a more global set of personal orien-
effort, as well as ambient emotional states. tations that influence how students approach
Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and Paris (2004) observed schoolwork, learning, and achievement.
that a multitude of conceptualizations and mea-
surements of engagement run throughout the liter-
ature and concluded that that student engagement Conceptualizing Engagement
should be conceptualized as a multidimen-
sional metaconstruct made up of three distinct Many studies rely on observer ratings of engage-
but related dimensions: cognitive engagement (i.e., ment, but as both a latent and multidimensional
investment in learning, self-regulation), behavioral construct, engagement may not always be an
engagement (i.e., positive conduct, demonstration observable characteristic. In addition, behaviors
of effort), and emotional engagement (e.g., interest rated high on engagement by observers may repre-
and boredom). Behavioral engagement is based sent only compliance to authority figures or “going
on observational and self-reported measures of through the motions” characteristic of procedural
on-task behavior, effort, participation, attendance, engagement. This conceptualization of engage-
or other desirable behaviors typical of good stu- ment contrasts with substantive engagement char-
dents (Finn & Voelkl, 1993; Green, Rhodes, Hirsch, acterized by deep processing and intrinsic
Suarez-Orozco, & Camic, 2008; Marks, 2000). motivation (Brophy, 1983; Nystrand & Gamoran,
Cognitive engagement is usually measured as stu- 1991). For purposes of identifying students who
dents’ investment or mastery in learning and depth are academically resilient, we are interested in
of processing (Blumenfeld, 1992; Newmann, identifying engagement that is more substantive
1992; Newmann & Wehlage, 1993), students’ and less procedural because substantive engage-
intrinsic motivation to learn within a given learn- ment is likely to be more strongly related to authen-
ing environment (Brophy, 1987; Covington, 2000; tic motivational orientations and educational
Ryan & Deci, 2000; Sansone & Harackiewicz, attitudes that are transferrable into higher levels of
2000), and/or the use of self-regulated meta- academic performance (see Fig. 15.1). We believe
cognitive strategies such as planning, monitor- that when students are substantively and deeply
ing, and evaluating one’s understanding of a engaged with learning, the psychological state is
text (Zimmerman, 1990). Many of these con- similar to that characterized as flow experiences
ceptualizations and measurements of cognitive (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). Flow is a state of
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 319

deep absorption in an activity that is intrinsically experience (Henri, 1923/2007). These individual
interesting and enjoyable, such as when athletes episodes may gradually accrue meaning, culmi-
are focused on their play, dancers are immersed in nating in a strengthened, sustained, and persistent
their performance, or scientists are engrossed in involvement in an area of interest (Nakamura,
solving a new problem (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, 2001). Thus, engagement may naturally progress
1997; Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, and develop into a more stable, continuing moti-
1988). The state of flow is all-encompassing, with vational orientation that is directly related to aca-
no psychic energy left for distractions, including demic performance (Maehr, 1976). For example,
consciousness of time and self. During this state, we found that student engagement reported at
individuals function at their fullest capacity, random moments in high school science classes
and the experience becomes its own reward was predictive of the choice of a college major in
(DeCharms, 1968; Deci, 1975). Notably, students science 2 years later and that momentary interest
with multiple risk factors have stated that concen- and enjoyment in math and science classes were
trated experiences like dance can be therapeutic, also predictive of academic performance in col-
providing a sort of sanctuary in which they can for- lege (Shernoff, 2010; Shernoff & Hoogstra,
get about their problems and allow their creative 2001). These associations were statistically sig-
energies to “flow” (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). nificant after controlling for grades in high school
Based on flow theory, we have found it useful (which were not a significant predictor of a
to define and operationalize engagement in edu- related college major or performance in college)
cational contexts as the simultaneous experience as well as demographic characteristics. These
of concentration, interest, and enjoyment in the findings are consistent with Hidi and Renninger’s
task at hand (Shernoff & Schmidt, 2008; Shernoff (2006) four-phase model of interest development,
& Vandell, 2007; Shernoff et al., 2003). Because in which early interest is often situational, much
all three components are strongly related to learn- like episodes of flow, but as value in the activity
ing (Shernoff & Csikszentmihalyi, 2009), we or topic deepens, interest becomes an enduring
argue that engagement defined in this way is very and sustained trait of the individual.
close to learning itself, or at least the experience
of learning. It is of course unrealistic to concep-
tualize adolescent-aged students as routinely in Conceptualizing Motivation
flow during school; and indeed our research has
demonstrated that this is not the case (Shernoff & We, therefore, see substantive engagement with
Csikszentmihalyi). We concur with the observa- learning activities as having a cumulative pro-
tion of the National Research Council (2004) in gression toward more general motivational orien-
writing, “We are not proposing that all high tations and proclivities. We are particularly
school students be in a constant state of flow, but interested in motivational orientations that sup-
we have seen youth deeply and enthusiastically port learning and school achievement, or achieve-
engaged in schoolwork and we believe that this ment motivation. Broadly speaking, achievement
high standard should be our goal” (p. 32). In other motivation consists of a constellation of beliefs
words, deep problem-solving, authentic interest, influencing patterns of school achievement,
and enjoyment in creating “works” encapsulated including expectations and standards for perfor-
by the concept of flow are a way to conceptualize mance, value placed on learning, and self-
ideal engagement in learning and, indeed, the act perceptions of ability (Deci & Ryan, 1985;
of learning itself. Dweck, 2006; Eccles et al., 2006; Nicholls, 1989;
The type of experience that occurs as one Weiner, 2005). Research in achievement motiva-
becomes engrossed or entirely taken up with tion today is located within a social-cognitive
objects or activities of interest often results in a framework (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). That is,
creative or scientific attitude toward the activity achievement-related beliefs are seen as influ-
somewhat set apart from the ordinary range of enced by the ways in which students interpret or
320 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

make meaning from their educational experi- challenging over easy tasks, and otherwise are
ences. Their school-related experiences may better oriented toward learning (Ames, 1992a;
include feedback from parents and teachers, Brophy, 1983; Meece, Blumenfeld, & Hoyle,
motivational and affective responses to success 1988; Nicholls, 1983, 1984, 1989; see Kaplan &
and failure, and their placement within school Maehr, 2007). Recent research also confirms
structures such as ability grouping. Importantly, that mastery goals are more likely to lead to
children’s achievement beliefs influence their subsequent, continuing interest including col-
achievement-related behavior. For example, chil- lege course enrollment en route to early career
dren who believe that ability is fixed are more development (Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, &
likely to avoid challenging tasks than those who Elliot, 2002; Hulleman, Durik, Schweigert, &
view ability as malleable (Dweck, 2006). Harackiewicz, 2008).
Goal theory is one of the more prominent con-
temporary approaches to studying achievement
motivation. Early research focused on two Motivational Orientations Among
achievement goal orientations that students adopt Low-Income Ethnic Minority
about the nature and purpose of learning, beliefs Adolescents
about ability, and conceptions of school success,
referred to as mastery and performance goals In our recent research (e.g., Bempechat, Li,
(Ames, 1992b; Elliott & Dweck, 1988; Maehr & Wenk, & Holloway, submitted; Bempechat,
Nicholls, 1980; Nicholls, 1978, 1989). Mastery Shernoff, Li, Holloway, & Arendtz, 2010), we
goals are conceptualized as the desire to attain conducted a series of in-depth interviews with an
knowledge and understanding (a mastery- ethnically diverse sample of 92 ninth graders
approach orientation), implying a positive form (approximately one third African American, one
of motivation. As illustrated in Fig. 15.1, this third European American, and one third Mexican
motivational pattern is maintained over time both American), all low-SES students (i.e., eligible for
in the short term and in the long term (Weiner, free/reduced-price lunch). We focused our analy-
1979), underscoring the quality of involvement ses on the difference in achievement goals and
and a continued commitment to learning (Paris & educational attitudes between the higher and
Winograd, 1990; Pelletier et al., 1995; Pintrich & lower achievers, as determined by a median split.
De Groot, 1990). Thus, we would expect cumula- We asked the students about their perceptions of
tive episodes of substantive or flow-like engage- school and factors related to processes of learn-
ment to progress into mastery-approach goals ing and schooling in the context of their daily
and dispositions. lives and routines.
Mastery goals can also include avoidance. To be sure, the high achievers expressed many
Mastery-avoidance goals reflect a concern for more beliefs and attitudes expressive of a mas-
maintaining one’s skills that derives from the fear tery orientation. However, our coding categories
of losing them (Elliot, 1999). In contrast, revealed that the predominant themes character-
performance goals represent a desire to appear izing these students’ educational attitudes and
competent (a performance-approach orientation), learning goals were more nuanced and varied
or at least to avoid appearing incompetent (a than is typically reflected in the achievement
performance-avoidance orientation) (Ames, 1984; goals literature. For example, high achievers
Dweck & Bempechat, 1983; Nicholls, 1984; were more likely to express three different types
Pintrich, 2000). An impressive body of experi- of mastery-related behaviors and habits: mastery-
mental, survey, and, to a much lesser extent, learning goals (e.g., the desire to learn new things;
qualitative research has demonstrated that indi- see Ames, 1992b), mastery behaviors (e.g., per-
viduals with mastery goals perform better, have sistence, investment of effort, self-discipline),
more positive affect and self-efficacy beliefs, are and mastery-emergent standards (e.g., high per-
more persistent in the face of difficulty, prefer sonal standards for performance or learning).
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 321

High achievers were also more likely to express a to be high. Higher achievers appeared to possess
healthy conscientiousness for their future educa- greater confidence in their academic competen-
tional or career goals. The lower achievers also cies, but became more worried when higher lev-
expressed mastery-related dispositions, but with els of challenge threatened it. Consistent with
the tendency to invest effort inconsistently, such their interview testimonies, lower achievers
as putting in more effort for some classes than reported higher levels of choice and guilt when
others, seemingly perceiving their effort as more they were doing their schoolwork.
optional than high achievers. They were also Overall, both the interview and ESM data
more likely to avoid work altogether. Although converged on the central difference that higher
on the surface, these motivational orientations achievers were more invested in schooling and
appear similar to performance-approach and seemed to take both their school learning and
performance-avoidance goals, comparative per- performance goals more seriously. On the whole,
formance did not seem to be a central con- their educational values and future educational
cern; rather, lower achievers preferred activities goals were decisively stronger. Although the
perceived as more enjoyable until teachers or goals literature may classify their goals to reach
parents forced their attention to schoolwork— clearly defined future goals as performance-ori-
i.e., they had the need for external discipline in ented (i.e., performance-approach goals), such
the absence of self-discipline. goals, which guided the investment to learn and
We also sought to measure and compare indi- master material in a mature and purposeful way,
vidual levels of engagement in learning environ- may be better characterized as educational values
ments (mostly when in school) and while doing for which the higher achievers demonstrated
schoolwork in particular. To reach this goal, we identification and integration in terms of Ryan
utilized the Experience Sampling Method (or and Deci’s (2000) taxonomy of motivation.
ESM; see Hektner, Schmidt, & Csikszentmihalyi, Even though their educational goals and stan-
2007). Students wore a watch programmed to dards appeared to be internalized and integrated,
beep randomly seven times per day, from this does not mean that this process of internal-
8:00 a.m. until 10:00 p.m. When signaled, stu- ization was not highly influenced. On the con-
dents recorded their location, activity, preferred trary, we suspect that their values may be strongly
activity, companions, subjective experiences, and influenced by a number of interacting contextual
emotions (e.g., challenge, importance, etc.) into variables based on current theory and research.
Experience Sampling Forms (ESFs). We then We turn to that theory and research next. An
compared subjective experiences between the important question becomes: What are the salient
higher and lower achievers while completing contexts of influence on children’s educational
schoolwork specifically. Results showed that values? Are family factors, and parents in partic-
higher achievers had significantly higher engage- ular, especially salient influences?
ment (i.e., average of concentration, interest, and
enjoyment) and consistently reported greater
feelings of understanding and competence when Theoretical Background
completing their schoolwork than the lower on Contextual Influences
achievers. However, they also reported signifi-
cantly higher levels of negative affect such as In proposing his ecological model underscoring
feeling scared and confused. The magnitude of the primacy of context in child development,
this difference was generally small (on average Bronfenbrenner (1977) observed that develop-
about 0.20 of a standard deviation, though this mental psychology had become “…the science of
varied by the specific emotion examined), but the the strange behavior of children in strange situa-
difference was greater (e.g., 0.60 of standard tions with strange adults for the briefest periods
deviation in feeling confused) when the chal- of time” (p. 513). His theory of nested and recip-
lenge of the school-related activity was perceived rocal spheres of influence in child development
322 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

served to highlight a widely accepted organizing co-construct their developing understanding of


principle of development—individuals do not the nature and value of learning and education
evolve in a vacuum, but rather are active partici- through their ongoing interactions with their
pants in multiple social and historical contexts caregivers, teachers, and mentors. Flow experi-
that shape their emerging beliefs about how the ences are also considered to be the product of the
world around them functions. A great deal of quality of interaction between a person and the
research in parent involvement focuses on the environment; thus, conceptualizing student
two proximal systems. The microsystem consists engagement based on flow theory is consistent
of the immediate settings that contain the child— with bioecological views. Bronfenbrenner and
for example, what parents and families say and Ceci’s (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994) bioeco-
do in support of academic achievement. The logical model of development provides a way of
mesosystem represents interactions between the understanding the influences of an individual’s
environments that contain the child and thus the- background, personal attributes, peers, teachers,
oretically situates the home-school connection in parents, the school, community, and macrolevel
studies of how bonds between these settings can factors like society and culture through proximal
foster achievement and engagement. There may processes described as “complex reciprocal
be many influences on student engagement within interaction(s) between an active, evolving bio-
the microsystem and mesosystem alone. However, physical human organism and the persons,
much of the research on flow in adolescents dem- objects, and symbols in the immediate environ-
onstrates that certain contexts in the microsystem ment” (p. 572). Proximal processes vary as a
(e.g., structured extracurriculars or organized function of the developing person and both distal
sports) may exert a motivational pull so strong and immediate environment. As we will discuss
(i.e., to the point of complete absorption in below, ecocultural theory and bioecological mod-
the activity or context) that it can overcome els of development are critical in understanding
the potentially distracting influence of other con- ethnic and cultural diversity in parental influ-
texts within the microsystem or mesosystem ences on student engagement.
(e.g., Csikszentmihalyi, 1975; Csikszentmihalyi
& Larson, 1984; Schmidt, Shernoff, & Csikszent-
mihalyi, 2007). Theoretical Approaches
No less important for the influence of parent- to Understanding Parent Involvement
ing on developmental outcomes, however, are the
two more distal circles of influence. Psychological Several decades of research have demonstrated
anthropologists and cultural psychologists alike that parental involvement in children’s schooling
have argued that societal contexts (the exosys- is associated with a variety of positive academic
tem) as well as cultural and historical contexts and motivational outcomes. These include such
(the macrosystem) are critical in shaping think- indicators as higher GPA and achievement test
ing and the development of belief systems, scores, improved attendance in school, greater
including those that guide parental educational rates of high school graduation, and positive atti-
socialization practices (Cole, 1996; Harkness & tudes about schooling (Comer, 2005; Eccles &
Super, 1992; Rogoff, 2003; Vygotsky, 1978; Gootman, 2002; Epstein, 1995; Mapp, Johnson,
Weisner, 2002). In her work, Rogoff (1990) has Strickland, & Meza, 2008). There is no universal
noted that cultural and historical contexts are key pattern of parent involvement that results in
factors that drive the content and nature of mean- higher achievement, nor do all forms of involve-
ing making. ment enhance learning outcomes (Hill & Taylor,
Culture and context thus play central roles in 2004; Jeynes, 2010; Pomerantz, Moorman, &
helping us understand how parents foster their Litwak, 2007). The overall benefits of parental
children’s engagement with school. The child’s involvement when taken as a whole, however,
role, however, is equally critical. Children actively have been substantial enough to influence public
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 323

policy. For example, under the mandate of the No across the domains of the self and not limited to
Child Left Behind Act (NCLB), schools must only academic growth. This mesosystemic
enact policies and procedures that involve par- approach is at the core of the model of 4 A’s—the
ents in their children’s school lives (NCLB, 2002 school conditions necessary to establish strong
Title I, Sec. 1001 [12]). partnerships (Christenson & Sheridan, 2001).
There exists a shared understanding that These include an approach to families that com-
healthy academic and psychosocial development municates genuine respect for parents and the
cannot occur if key constituents of the child’s different ways they become involved; an attitude
upbringing—parents, teachers, and community that respects parents’ perspectives and views
members—operate in a vacuum or, worse, are in parental involvement as essential for student suc-
conflict. While prominent models of parental cess; an atmosphere that supports interactions
involvement stress the co-construction of partici- between home and school; and, with these three
pation, they also place primary responsibility for components in place, actions that can be adopted
engaging parents on schools and their agents— to support strong family-school relationships.
teachers, administrators, and support staff As we describe toward the end of this chapter,
(Epstein, 1995; Reschly & Christenson, 2009). successful practitioners, such as James Comer and
Epstein’s influential theory of overlapping spheres Geoffrey Canada, have embodied the 4 A’s
of influence derives from Bronfenbrenner’s eco- approach in their efforts to build meaningful con-
logical theory and, like it, places the child at the nections between home, school, and communities.
center of three contexts—home, school, and
community—that need to work in concert to fos-
ter learning and development (Epstein, 1995). The Influence of Parental Involvement
Epstein’s typology of six types of family-school on Well-Being and Engagement
connections identifies family and school respon-
sibilities that, when enacted, can create a seam- Research evidence suggests at least two funda-
less partnership between home and school. At the mental reasons that parental involvement influ-
same time, schools are seen as bearing the respon- ences engagement and motivation. The first is the
sibility for assisting parents to embrace beliefs strong association between parental relations
(e.g., seeing their children as students) and engage with their children and overall psychological
in actions that support their children’s learning. well-being, which positions parental involvement
This is particularly so in high-poverty urban cen- as a primary protective factor against disengage-
ters, where parents may not have the resources or ment. The second is the more direct influence of
social capital to maximize their own abilities to caring and supportive relationships with parents.
foster achievement.
Over time, conceptions of parent involvement
have evolved from a focus on activities that Parental Relations and Psychological
schools can design to engage parents to the more Well-Being
recent realization that relationships are the foun-
dation upon which successful partnerships are Our conceptualization of engagement as defined
built (Reschly & Christenson, 2009). These rela- by students’ self-perceptions of their level of
tionships need to be perceived as warm, caring, involvement in an activity places an emphasis on
and respectful in order to gain the trust and endur- the relational and emotional well-being of the stu-
ing participation of parents and family members. dent. Such an outlook is based on the premise that
With Bronfenbrenner’s mesosystem as its start- engagement with learning environments is situ-
ing point, Reschly and Christenson have argued ated within the larger context of psychological and
that family involvement represents a collabora- relational well-being emanating from effective
tion between homes and schools, one that is adaptation to the environment (Griffiths, Sharkey,
devoted to enhancing children’s development & Furlong, 2009). Within this larger perspective,
324 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

meaningful engagement that leads to sustained Chang, Stewart, & Au, 2003), but attesting to the
motivation may be seen as a key driver of positive long-ranging and formative influence of parental
youth development (Larson, 2006), and fostering relations, a longitudinal study of 17,000 youth in
it is a primary goal of educational approaches that Great Britain found that feeling close to one’s
emphasize strengths and well-being of students mother at age 16 explained 5–11% of the variance
rather than deficit-driven and reactive approaches in life satisfaction at age 42 (Flouri, 2004).
(Gilman, Huebner, & Furlong, 2009). Conversely, parental conflict has been associated
Indeed, there appears to be a strong relation- with diminished life satisfaction both concurrently
ship between engagement and well-being. and 1 year later in a sample of 429 12–16-year-
Students who are interested and involved in skill olds in Hong Kong (Shek, 1998).
building and productive pursuits score higher on
measures of psychological adjustment, including
measures of self-esteem, responsibility, compe- The Influence of Parental Relationship
tence, and social relations (Steinberg, 1996), Support
whereas students who report feeling alienated
from school are more likely to have behavioral Caring and supportive relationships with peers
problems ranging from withdrawal to depression and adults are an integral feature of settings pro-
to aggression (Jessor & Jessor, 1977). Research moting the motivation and development of youth
has shown that the resources of families, schools, (Eccles & Gootman, 2002). Developmentally
and communities may foster the positive devel- speaking, adolescents are fully embedded in a
opment of youth through provisions of physical world of interpersonal relationships (Kegan,
safety and security, developmentally appropriate 1982). As students enter middle school, their
structure, and expectations for behavior; emo- social networks have an increasingly important
tional and moral support; and opportunities to social-emotional influence on their attitudes
make a contribution to one’s community (Eccles toward school and motivation to succeed (Furlong
& Gootman, 2002). In support of this ecological- et al., 2003). Due to the pervasive influence of
adaptation view of engagement and positive relationships on multiple facets of student motiva-
development, Reschly, Huebner, Appleton, and tion, Martin and Dowson (2009) recently demon-
Antaramian (2008) recently found that a signifi- strated that many of the most dominant
cant relationship between positive emotions and motivational theories may be conceptualized in
student engagement was mediated by broadened relational terms. A growing number of studies
cognitive capacities (i.e., problem-solving), support this view. Typically, engagement research
behavioral coping strategies (i.e., social support has examined the effect of connectedness with
seeking), and other proclivities toward healthy specific social partners: most commonly, teachers,
adaptation. peers, parents, and mentors. Research evidence
Because family life and parental relations are has rapidly accumulated, demonstrating that relat-
such powerful forces in overall adaptation and edness in all of these categories has important and
relational well-being, family cohesiveness and unique contributions to student engagement (e.g.,
parental relations may be seen as a primary protec- Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Rhodes, 2002; Steinberg,
tive factor against behavioral and psychological 1996; See recent meta-analysis by Roorda et al.
problems including disengagement from school, 2011). We here focus on the influence of parental
while reciprocally serving as a salient influence on relationship support.
resiliency and positive psychological outcomes The importance of parental relations for social
(Suldo, 2009). For example, spending time with competence and other positive developmental out-
one’s family in grade five has been associated with comes is rooted in the continuity view of relation-
positive affect (Larson & Richards, 1991). Not ships, which suggests that one’s relationship style
only have children’s ratings of parents’ warmth is relatively stable and strongly influenced by one’s
been related to life satisfaction (Chang, McBride- attachment to a primary caregiver as early as
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 325

infancy, like relational “templates” carried forward parents’ participation in their children’s learning
into to adolescence and adulthood (Furrer & enhances achievement and the development of
Skinner, 2003). During this time, the quality of adaptive motivational tendencies, such as mastery
parental relations may also operate in a great vari- orientation toward learning. Research identifies at
ety of ways to influence students’ motivation. For least three ways that parents and families can influ-
example, many students are motivated by grades, ence student engagement: through (a) parent
but their interpretation of what the grades signify involvement with homework, (b) parenting style,
is often mediated though their relationship with and (c) the transmission of educational values.
their parents (Steinberg, 1996). The quality of
parental relations has thus been linked not only to Parent Involvement in Homework
higher engagement (Chen, 2008), but also to aca- The literature on parent involvement in home-
demic performance (Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Sirin work has been particularly illustrative of the pro-
& Rogers-Sirin, 2004) and achievement (Hughes found influence that parents have to enhance as
& Kwok, 2007). The quality of parent–child rela- well as hinder the development of adaptive moti-
tions has also been associated with school satisfac- vational beliefs and behaviors. This influence is
tion (Huebner & Diener, 2008). Findings like these not trivial, in light of the fact that students’ sub-
suggest that supportive parental relations are jective experiences while doing homework tend
important for students’ engagement and attitudes to be characterized by negative affect, including
about schooling beyond providing the child with high apathy and low engagement (Leone &
templates for relating to others in the early years of Richards, 1989; Shernoff & Vandell, 2007).
life (Furrer & Skinner, 2003). Interestingly, this research suggests that the worst
part of homework for children and adolescents
may be that it is a solitary activity; affect improves
How Parents Influence Engagement considerably as a shared activity with parents or
and Foster Adaptive Achievement peers. Parents who provide assistance with home-
Beliefs work play a critical role not only in fostering
learning, but in scaffolding strategies for time
A considerable body of research has focused on management and problem-solving. Further, their
what parents can do to foster their children’s interest in and assistance with homework predicts
engagement and achievement in school and how their children’s self-perceptions of competence
schools can support parents in their efforts. While (Grolnick & Slowiaczek, 1994; Hoover-Dempsey
much attention is rightfully paid to particular et al., 2001; Pomerantz, Ng, & Wang, 2006). For
activities (e.g., reading to children, assisting with example, Pomerantz and her colleagues examined
homework) that positively contribute to school mothers’ mastery-oriented involvement with their
grades and achievement test scores, our under- children’s homework as a function of children’s
standing of the benefits of parent involvement perceptions of competence. Mothers who used
has expanded to include the motivational factors mastery-oriented techniques to help their 8- to
involved. Research at the intersection of ecocul- 12-year-old children with homework (e.g., helped
tural theory and social cognitive theory has them to understand their work, encouraged them
revealed that parents’ own attitudes about learn- to solve problems on their own) were particularly
ing, the value placed on education, achievement influential in enhancing mastery orientation in
expectations, and approaches with the school and their children 6 months later. Importantly, these
its agents have a profound influence on the devel- mothers’ mastery-oriented practices predicted
opment of their children’s achievement-related heightened self-perceptions of competence among
beliefs and behaviors (Bempechat, 2004; Grolnick children with initially low self-perceptions of
& Slowiaczek, 1994; Jeynes, 2010). ability (Pomerantz et al., 2006).
As we shall see, there is considerable variety in Parents also create environments for study that
the ways in which, and circumstances under which, help students learn to deal with and manage their
326 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

homework behavior (Epstein & Van Vooris, 2001; homework enhances achievement, especially at
Hong & Lee, 1999; Xu & Corno, 1998, 2003). In the elementary school level, have contributed to a
their observation study of parents and their third popular view that its role should be very limited
grade children, Xu and Corno (1998) found that (Kohn, 2006; Kralovec & Buell, 2001). The
parents effectively arranged their children’s above body of research makes clear that home-
homework environment by minimizing distrac- work can be a powerful vehicle for fostering the
tions, focusing their children on their assign- development of adaptive motivational tendencies.
ments, and helping to make homework more When parent involvement with homework is
interesting. Children of these parents actively warm and supportive, it serves to enhance both
engaged in strategies to help them complete their academic achievement and the development of
work, including preparing a place to work, keep- adaptive beliefs about learning.
ing track of time, and self-monitoring their affect
by praising themselves. These children, then, The Influence of Parenting Styles
were able to model adaptive attitudes and behav- Despite the variation that may exist in supportive
iors that their parents had scaffolded for them. parenting relations, Baumrind’s (1971) typology
This and other studies demonstrate the extent of parenting styles has figured prominently in
to which homework can be a social experience, in research on children’s psychosocial outcomes.
which children’s subjective experience is cocon- Research has found that authoritative parents—
structed through interactions with parents. Indeed, those who effectively set limits and enforce
parents’ attitudes about homework have been appropriate boundaries (i.e., a demandingness
found to have a direct and positive influence on dimension, in which parents make age-appropri-
their children’s subsequent attitudes and aca- ate demands for mature behavior) in the context
demic outcomes (Cooper, Lindsay, Nye, & of a caring and communicative relationship
Greathouse, 1998; Else-Quest, Hyde, & Hejmadi, that supports children’s independence (i.e., a
2008; Leone & Richards, 1989). In a recent study, responsiveness dimension, in which parents are
Else-Quest and her colleagues videotaped and sensitive to, but not indulgent of, children’s
coded mothers’ and their 11-year-old children’s requests)—promote social competence, espe-
affect during a teaching session. The authors cially in comparison with parents who provide
invoke the notions of emotional contagion only one or neither of the two dimensions
(unconscious copying of another’s emotions) and (Baumrind, 1989). Various problem behaviors
emotional convergence (wherein members of a such as delinquency, drug and alcohol abuse, and
dyad express the same emotion) to suggest that conformity to antisocial peer pressure are lower
mothers may in fact be able to shape their chil- among youth reared by authoritative parents
dren’s emotions during the homework experience (Lamborn, Brown, Mounts, & Steinberg, 1992;
by carefully monitoring the emotions they model Steinberg, 1996). There is evidence that parent-
(Else-Quest et al., 2008). These researchers doc- ing that supports school success has similar
umented a variety of positive (e.g., pride, posi- authoritative characteristics—monitoring chil-
tive interest, affection) and negative (e.g., dren’s school activities, openly showing affection
frustration, distress, tension) emotions and found and becoming involved, and encouraging chil-
that these were associated with performance. For dren to communicate their point of view. Actively
example, positive interest and pride were associ- participating in school activities and support for
ated with higher achievement, while tension was learning as an end in itself may instill intrinsic
linked to poorer performance. interest in learning and a tendency to persist in
The ongoing debate about the influence of academic challenges (Lamborn et al., 1992).
homework on academic achievement has at times Thus, studies have found that children of authori-
pitted parents against educators and educators tative parents are more engaged in school, spend
against homework researchers (Bempechat, more time in homework, have higher educational
2004). Mixed findings on the extent to which expectations and GPAs, and are less involved in
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 327

school deviance than children of nonauthoritative delinquency) transcended ethnicity, socioeco-


parents (Lamborn et al., 1992). nomic status, and family structure.
More relevant to our own research, other stud- Mandara’s research has shed some light on the
ies have demonstrated that support and challenge inconsistent findings associated with ethnicity and
experienced from the family is related to intrinsi- parenting style (Mandara & Murray, 2002). In a
cally motivated interest and flow experiences recent study of African American parents and their
during work-related activities specifically, as 15-year-old children, he identified three styles of
measured by the Experience Sampling Method parenting that were similar to Baumrind’s typol-
(ESM). The family challenge and support dimen- ogy. Important differences set African American
sions in Rathunde’s (1996) study of 165 talented authoritative parents apart from their European
high school students (aged 14–15 years) some- American counterparts. For example, African
what expand on Baumrind’s responsiveness and American authoritative parents were found to be
demandingness dimensions in the early years of more exacting of their children and gave in less to
life. Because developmental tasks during adoles- their demands. From a European American per-
cence revolve around developing self-identity, spective, this might be considered a sign of
managing unsupervised time, and directing one’s authoritarian parenting, yet this qualitatively dif-
pursuits, family challenge emanates from high ferent kind of authoritative parenting was associ-
expectations for doing one’s best and behavioral ated with positive academic and social outcomes
control, and family support is provided by an (Gorman-Smith, Tolan, Henry, & Florsheim,
authentic interest in the child, unconditional posi- 2000; Taylor, Hinton, & Wilson, 1995).
tive regard, acceptance/involvement, and auton- Parenting styles are further influenced by
omy granting. The study found that students’ and social contexts. Recent research has demonstrated
parents’ perceptions along both dimensions were a positive association between residence in high-
corroborative. Moreover, students who reported poverty neighborhoods and diminished parental
their families to be high on both dimensions warmth, increased control, and harsh discipline
reported more flow experiences during produc- (Pinderhughes, Nix, Foster, & Jones, 2001;
tive activities. Tendulkar, Buka, Dunn, Subramanain, & Koenen,
Research suggests that authoritative parenting 2010; Zhang & Anderson, 2010). The stresses
styles are associated with children’s mastery goal associated with living in communities character-
motivational orientations as a potential mediator ized by violence, limited social resources, in
of positive school outcomes, whereas nonauthori- addition to concerns for children’s safety may
tative styles are associated with performance goal undermine parents’ ability to engage in more
orientations (Suldo, 2009). Some studies have positive parenting (Pinderhughes et al., 2001).
suggested that the effect of parenting styles may Together, these findings underscore the extent to
not operate the same way for all ethnicities. For which researchers need to understand parenting
example, the use of physical discipline in child- and educational socialization in cultural, ethnic,
hood, generally associated with an authoritarian and social contexts.
style, predicted more externalizing problems in
adolescents for European Americans, but fewer The Transmission of Educational Values
among African Americans (Lansford, Deater- As children progress through the school years,
Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & Petit, 2004). However, their interactions with schools and other contexts
this study did not examine parenting styles per se. within the microsystem and mesosystem provide
Larger-scale research on parenting styles in ado- the basis for more stable, enduring educational
lescence, such as Steinberg, Mounts, Lamborn, attitudes and dispositions. While manifesting in a
and Dornbusch’s (1991) study of 10,000 high variety of ways, parents’ own educational atti-
school students, has found that positive correlates tudes and beliefs may be the major influence
of authoritative parenting (i.e., higher grades and on the educational attitudes that their children
self-reliance, and less psychological stress and gradually adopt. Messages of educational values
328 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

are often communicated more implicitly than toward high-quality work within a profession
explicitly. For example, if parents do not take were found to become absorbed by multiple sub-
advantage of opportunities to become involved sequent generations of mentees in the context of
with the child’s schooling, even if they say that supportive relationships (Nakamura & Shernoff,
school is important, children may get the mes- 2009). Values that get transmitted from one gen-
sage that their parents do not have the time for eration to the next can be conceptualized as
school and conclude that they do not, either. memes, the cultural units of intergenerational
Similarly, when parents support their children’s inheritance, as an analogue to genes. In the case
opinions and encourage self-expression in an of mentoring and apprenticeship within profes-
affirming and noncontingent manner, their chil- sions, the tacit transmission of values and prac-
dren are more likely to express their beliefs and tices appears to be one way in which professions
act in an autonomous and self-regulating manner are maintained and evolve. Similarly, cultural
important for success in school. Providing strong values transmitted from parents to children may
research evidence for this viewpoint, Jeynes be an important mechanism for the evolution and
(2010) conducted a series of meta-analyses of par- maintenance of culture itself, speaking to the
ent involvement research which showed that par- potential interaction of parental influences with
ent expectations, communicated through parental the macrosystem.
sacrifice, low stress communication, and a shared Social class in particular has been found to
valuing of education, were more powerful in pre- interact with parental involvement. In research
dicting academic outcomes than open communi- with elementary school–aged children and their
cation, in which parents and children freely parents, Grolnick and Slowiaczek (1994) catego-
express themselves without fear of retribution. rized types of parental involvement: overt behav-
Grolnick and her colleagues (Grolnick & ior (going to parent-teacher and other school
Slowiaczek, 1994) also found evidence for the events), personal involvement (demonstrating
indirect influence of parent involvement on stu- that the parent enjoys the child’s school and inter-
dent achievement through motivational factors. actions with school personnel), and cognitive/
Grolnick and Ryan (1989) found that youth intellectual involvement (providing intellectual
whose parents were both autonomy supportive resources and help with schoolwork). These
and involved in their schoolwork (i.e., talked dimensions of involvement were differentially
with them about school and helped them with associated with social class. Specifically, cogni-
challenges) internalized the value of doing well tive/intellectual involvement tended to be more
in school, as demonstrated by regularly complet- characteristic of parents with higher levels of
ing homework, enjoying their schoolwork, and education. However, parent education was unre-
doing their best to succeed. With greater internal- lated to behavioral involvement. This supports
ization of their parents’ educational values also the notion that low-income, less educated parents
came higher achievement and better psychologi- are indeed involved in their children’s education,
cal adjustment. Thus, parents who are present at albeit in different ways. This involvement is no
school meetings or events may be communicat- less important in conveying to children that their
ing its importance to children and also modeling parents care about their schooling and value their
ways to deal with questions or concerns. As a education.
result, children also come to view schooling as
within their realm of control. Similarly, parents
who are involved intellectually, by reading to Parental Involvement
their children or helping with homework, may and the Building of Social Capital
foster beliefs that these are manageable and con-
trollable tasks. The importance of parent involvement is well
Studies of mentoring have likewise shown that illustrated through the construct of social capital,
a mentor’s tacit values and practices leading the notion that individuals have at their disposal
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 329

cultural resources they can access through their families, resulting in less exposure to key infor-
social networks (Bourdieu, 1985). Parent networks mation about school and schooling
operate as a form of social capital in which indi- In a later ethnography extended to include
viduals share tangible (books, educational vid- low-income, working-class, and middle-income
eos) and intangible (knowledge about the college African American families, social class was again
application process) resources to enhance their the primary determinant of differences in educa-
children’s learning (Lareau, 2000). A body of tional socialization, whereas there was no effect
ethnographic research has emerged to show that of race/ethnicity (Lareau, 2002). Middle-class
parents’ means of creating and accessing social parents adopted efforts characterized by “con-
capital varies as a function of both social class certed cultivation” to enhance their children’s
and ethnicity (e.g., Delgado-Gaitan, 1994; Horvat, intellectual and social development (e.g., enroll-
Weininger, & Lareau, 2003). Lareau’s influential ment in extracurricular activities, use of reason-
work underscores the power of social class in how ing as a means of socialization). By virtue of their
parents build and use social networks to enhance parents’ efforts, the middle-class children came
their children’s educational experiences (Lareau, to view themselves as both talented and entitled.
2002). Her early work on working- and middle- Greater involvement in organized activities fur-
class European American parents of first graders ther extended middle-class parents’ social net-
showed how parental involvement practices are works by exposing them to similarly well-educated
determined by social class, in spite of similarly and connected adults while at the same time lim-
active and supportive attempts on the part of teach- iting their exposure to extended family.
ers to enlist parental involvement, and the deep In contrast, low-income parents of both
value that both working- and middle-class parents European American and African American
placed on education (Lareau, 1987). groups socialized their children toward the
Relative to their middle-class peers, working- “accomplishment of natural growth” by structur-
class parents were less involved in fostering ing their children’s lives around more spontane-
learning at home, less knowledgeable about their ous events, such as family gatherings. This pattern
children’s classroom and school lives, initiated of kinship ties clearly had its own advantages, but
less contact with teachers, and largely limited the resulted in a social network composed of few
content of their interactions to nonacademic professionals and more limited in its understand-
issues such as the length of the lunch period. ing of how to negotiate the school system. This
These parents believed that their own limited limitation was observed to hinder poor and work-
education rendered them less able to help their ing-class parents’ attempts at school to intervene
children with schoolwork, and therefore turned on behalf of their children. Middle-class parents
the responsibility of teaching to their children’s were found to mobilize their social network in
teachers. In contrast, middle-class parents, often order to customize their children’s curriculum
as well or better educated than the teachers, per- (e.g., secure placement in special education or a
ceived their children’s school progress as a part- gifted program), while low-income and working-
nership between themselves and their children’s class parents largely operated individually to try
teachers. They initiated contact with teachers, to bring about curricular change or contest a
participated in school events, were keenly aware school policy (Horvat et al., 2003).
of classroom dynamics and curricular issues, and Ogbu’s contribution to the discourse on social
felt empowered to voice concerns. Importantly, capital and race continues to be controversial but
their social networks revolved around their chil- nonetheless important. Ogbu’s (2003) study of
dren’s friends’ families, with the result that they the achievement gap at Shaker Heights High
benefited from a greater number of sources of School revealed that the advantages associated
information about their children’s school and with middle-class status did not serve to inocu-
education. In contrast, working-class parents’ late African American students from lower than
social networks revolved around their extended average performance, as measured by GPA and
330 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

SAT scores. Ogbu’s ethnographic study showed they provided a regular place for homework com-
that many of the middle-class African American pletion, rewarded good grades, and adhered to
parents, professionals in most cases, embraced a strict rules, such as those around homework com-
model of learning more closely tied with the pletion and bedtime hour. For these families, the
working-class parents. That is, while they all meaning of education—educación—went beyond
noted that effort is essential to school success, individual achievement to include social behav-
they believed it was largely the school’s responsi- ior inherent to the concept of “buen educado,”
bility to ensure student success. These middle- including respect and compliance (see also Reese,
class parents were minimally involved at school, Balzano, Gallimore, & Goldenberg, 1995).
attending few meetings or events, even when Similarly, researchers within the funds of
these were targeted toward improving their chil- knowledge framework (see Moll, Amanti, Neff,
dren’s school performance. At home, parents did & Gonzalez, 1992) argue that culturally-based
not effectively monitor homework, guide the and historically accumulated resources and
development of organizational skills, or foster knowledge about education are powerful motiva-
self-efficacy beliefs. From a social capital per- tors of school achievement. An illustration of this
spective, this finding is puzzling, since these par- comes from Espinoza-Herold’s (2007) recent
ents’ social networks included professionals like case study of a young Mexican American immi-
themselves. Yet as Mandara and colleagues (e.g., grant woman pursuing her doctorate. Her persis-
Mandara, Varner, Greene, & Richman, 2009) tence in face of sometimes considerable odds,
suggest, it is important to consider that many of including a serious car accident, was made pos-
the African American parents were first-genera- sible in large part by the support and encourage-
tion professionals. They did not come from mid- ment of her mother, who communicated her value
dle-class, high-achieving families whose parents on education, belief in education, and the impor-
would have understood what it takes to do well in tance of future orientation through cultural folk
school. Thus, the Shaker Heights parents may not tales and sayings.
have had the benefit of observing, experiencing, Li and her colleagues recently demonstrated
and learning from parenting practices that foster the primacy of culture in the creation and use of
high achievement. social networks among low-income Chinese
Research on parents’ social networks has American families (Li, Holloway, Bempechat, &
begun to consider how culture and ethnicity, in Loh, 2008). Individual interviews with ninth
conjunction with social class, may explain stu- graders revealed that their relatively high level of
dents’ academic achievement. This work presents achievement (mean GPA of 3.27) was attained
a challenge, both to “deficit model” approaches with little practical assistance from parents.
to ethnic underachievement and to the premium Instead, students described parents as engaging
placed on higher SES in social capital explana- in three strategies that supported their learning.
tions of achievement. A variety of survey, ethno- First, they had identified and designated at least
graphic, and qualitative studies of Latino students one person in the home or extended family (older
and their families have converged to show that, sibling, relative)—an “anchor helper”—to be
contrary to popular stereotypes, Latino parents charged with guiding the student’s school prog-
care deeply about their children’s learning ress and providing tutoring. Second, according to
(Delgado-Gaitan, 1992; Gandara, 1995; Golden- students, their parents tried to motivate them by
berg & Gallimore, 1995; Valenzuela, 1999; invoking good learning models—an exemplary
Valenzuela & Dornbusch, 1994) and operate to individual(s) in the home or community whom
socialize their children for learning within diverse they urged their children to emulate. Finally, stu-
cultural models. For example, parents within six dents reported that their parents enlisted the long
Mexican American low-income immigrant fami- reach of kin—family members who were invited
lies were found to support their children’s school- or obliged to be involved in their schooling, but
ing and conveyed the importance of education in who also willingly became involved by staying
a variety of ways (Delgado-Gaitan). At home, current about their progress in school.
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 331

The Student and Staff Support Team is com-


Programs Modeling Parental posed of the school’s principal and staff members
Involvement and Home-School trained in mental health, charged with developing
Partnerships prevention programs and coordinating student
services so that students’ needs can be addressed
Perceptions of warmth, care, and support have
in the most optimal ways. Finally, the Parent
emerged as critical factors in students’ engage-
Team operates to support the school’s social and
ment in school. It should not be surprising, then,
academic goals and programs. Multimethod
to find that these qualities are essential for the
analyses have revealed a variety of positive short-
development of home-school partnerships that
term and long-term outcomes of SDP students as
also draw on community resources. Prominent
compared to non-SDP students attributable to the
partnership models around the nation have dem-
welcoming school climate, including improved
onstrated ways in which expressions of support
self-concept, higher achievement, improved
for parents (and children) lead to more engaged
attendance, and higher high school graduation
parents, who are then able to successfully engage
rates (Comer, 2005; Haynes, Emmons, Gebreye-
their children in learning. The importance of an
sus, & Ben-Avie, 1996).
atmosphere of warmth, care, and respect is evi-
More recently, the Harlem Children’s Zone
dent in James Comer’s School Development
(HCZ) project, initiated in 1997 by educator
Program, Geoffrey Canada’s Harlem Children’s
Geoffrey Canada, has received increasing atten-
Zone project, and effective Catholic schools,
tion from educators and public policy makers. The
three highly respected and successful initiatives
HCZ is a community-based approach dedicated to
modeling the importance of parental involvement
reversing the negative effects of poverty on aca-
and home-school partnerships. School-based
demic and psychosocial development. Its two
family centers and quality after-school programs
guiding principles are that the participation of a
have also effectively modeled the building of
critical mass of adults, trained in and oriented
social capital through networking with family,
around healthy child development, can reverse the
neighborhood, and community resources.
deleterious effects of poverty and that early and
ongoing intervention is needed to ensure success.
The initial success of this model based on the orig-
Comer’s School Development Program inally targeted 24-block area prompted a 10-year
and the Harlem Children’s Zone Project initiative to expand to 100 blocks starting in 2004.
At its core, the HCZ is a comprehensive program
Comer’s School Development Program (SDP, of social services embedded within the commu-
Comer, 2005) has been operating in the New nity and oriented around serving the needs of fam-
Haven public schools since 1968 and has since ilies with children from in utero through 18 years
been replicated around the nation. Through a of age and beyond. These community investment
mesosystemic approach, the SDP is a compre- services include year-round after-school programs
hensive home-school-community partnership that include parent education components for
that operates according to three guiding princi- expectant parents of infants, called Baby College;
ples—“no fault” problem-solving, decision mak- Promise Academy charter schools (with longer
ing through consensus, and collaboration across school days than the New York City public
the school’s constituencies. Schools are orga- schools); college preparatory and employment
nized around three primary teams or structures. centers; and a College Success Office to support
The School Planning and Management Team is college students’ academic and psychosocial
composed of administrators, teachers, support needs. Beyond the academic and intellectual
staff, and parents charged with developing a activities, community members have access to a
vision for the school’s academic and social goals variety of family, community, and health pro-
for its students and is responsible for the overall grams, such as foster care prevention, obesity pre-
operations of the school. vention, housing advocacy, and legal services.
332 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

The core of the Promise Academy schools, school, staffed by community insiders, and
into which students gain access by lottery, is the designed to engage family members in activities
Academic Case Management system (ACM), a that foster student achievement. These included
holistic orientation toward youth development in programs and workshops to build and sustain
which each student from middle school through home-school bonds, programs for parents’ own
college is assigned a student advocate. This indi- educational enhancement, and workshops to
vidual is charged with working collaboratively enhance parents’ social capital by explaining, for
with students and the adults in their lives (primar- example, how to negotiate the curriculum and
ily parents and teachers) to develop goals and understand the meaning, uses, and outcomes of
action plans that will enhance academic, social, standardized tests. Successful family centers wit-
and personal growth across the life span. These nessed improved attendance, academic perfor-
goals and plans are regularly reviewed, assessed, mance, graduation rates, and college acceptance.
and modified as necessary in order to keep stu- Mapp et al.’s observations and interviews with
dents on track toward success and provide support key stakeholders (students, parents and family
services, particularly at critical transition points, members, teachers, administrators, staff mem-
such as the move from high school to college. bers, and community members) revealed that
A recent evaluation of the impact of the HCZ these successful partnerships were maintained by
project revealed that students had made signifi- a “joining process” in which schools were con-
cant gains in math and English language arts, nected with families in different ways that wel-
either eliminating or significantly reducing black- comed, honored, and validated family members
white achievement gap (Dobbie & Fryer, 2009). for their contributions to their children’s learning.
For example, after 3 years in the Promise Academy Successful family centers were characterized by
middle schools, students closed the black-white three qualities: (1) a strong infrastructure, in
achievement gap in math, actually outperforming which stakeholders shared a common vision and
the average European American New York stu- were supported by the principal; (2) skilled staff
dent. Relative to peers who did not win admit- who could serve as “cultural brokers” connecting
tance to Promise Academy schools, enrolled family and school; and (3) responsive program-
students recorded lower absenteeism and higher ming, designed to meet parents’ needs and
rates of high school graduation. These gains were enhance their parenting efficacy. These charac-
attributed to a combination of both high-quality teristics lead to a “zone of community” wherein
schools and investments that had been made in parents perceived an atmosphere of mutual
the community. However, the latter alone could respect, allowing four positive outcomes to flour-
not account for the remarkable academic gains ish: (1) stronger and more trusting relationships
achieved by the students. The success of the HCZ between adults, born of mutual respect; (2) par-
project has garnered much public policy support, ents’ increased perceptions of themselves as
with President Obama having recently set aside active and effective participants in their chil-
$10 million to replicate the HCZ model in 20 dren’s learning; (3) the sense among students that
high-poverty urban areas (Shulman, 2009). teachers and administrators cared for them as
individuals, resulting in a stronger trusting rela-
tionship; and (4) the development of greater con-
School-Based Family Centers fidence and self-efficacy on the part of students.

In response to the challenge posed by decreased


family involvement at the secondary level, Mapp Embracing the Home-School
et al. (2008) conducted an ethnographic investi- Partnership: Catholic Schools
gation spotlighting eight family centers in several
low-income urban communities. Each family Academic achievement among low-income urban
center enjoyed a designated space in the high Catholic school students has been an enduring
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 333

topic of interest and research since 1980s parents to support their children’s learning by
(Coleman, Hoffer, & Kilgore, 1982). Research seeing them, as Epstein (1995) would argue, as
has consistently demonstrated that, relative to students. Because of this, Catholic schools are
their peers, poor and ethnic minority students in perceived to foster greater parent participation
Catholic high schools attain greater levels of pro- and more involvement at home (Bauch &
ficiency, as measured by an array of objective Goldring, 1995). In the pursuit of their shared
indicators, including participation in upper track vision, parents, teachers, and administrators serve
courses, higher GPAs and SAT scores, greater as a social network, which fosters greater cohe-
rates of high school graduation and college atten- siveness between home and school. Indeed,
dance, and admittance to more selective colleges norms for parent participation appear to be clearly
(Altonji, Elder, & Taber, 2005; Bryk, Lee, & communicated.
Holland, 1993; Carbonaro, 2003; Eide, Goldhaber, For example, research has found that parents
& Showalter, 2004; Ellison & Hallinan, 2004; in Catholic as compared to public schools are
Morgan, 2001; Sander & Krautman, 1995). One more likely to initiate contact with schools and
exception to this pattern emerged in a recent adhere to rules at home. They are also more likely
analysis of National Assessment of Educational to perceive that they were on the receiving end of
Progress (NAEP) data. This study confirmed that clearly communicated messages from their chil-
overall, Catholic school fourth and eighth graders dren’s school, to feel more welcomed when they
outperformed their public school peers in mathe- contact the school, and to perceive that the school
matics (Lubienski, Lubienski, & Crane, 2008). is more responsive to their needs (Bauch &
However, unlike other research, this school-type Goldring, 1995). Students similarly report an
advantage disappeared when the authors con- atmosphere of warmth and care in which teachers
trolled for variables such as social class, teacher take a genuine interest not only in their students,
certification, and reform-driven instructional but in their students’ families as well. Indeed, stu-
practices. Through complex statistical modeling, dents describe their schools as “feeling like a
these researchers showed that public school stu- family,” which ironically means that parents also
dents outperformed their Catholic school peers in trust teachers and administrators to see their chil-
math achievement. It is important to note that this dren not only as students but also as children
study focused on one narrow outcome measure— (Bempechat, Boulay, Piergross, & Wenk, 2008).
math achievement—and relied on statistical Critics might argue that self-selection might be
assumptions (i.e., linearity) that may not have more implicated than other factors in the achieve-
been met. In any case, these findings suggest that ment of urban Catholic schools’ students. Issues
further research on a variety of achievement out- of self-selection aside (see Bempechat et al.,
comes in different types of schools is warranted. 2008), research suggests that Catholic schools
The generally higher achievement of Catholic are emblematic of Christenson and Sheridan’s
high school students has been attributed in part to (2001) mesosystemic 4 A’s model of parent
the perception of the school as a communal orga- engagement.
nization in which all stakeholders share a com-
mon vision—to prepare students for postsecondary
education through high expectations and rigorous Out-of-School Time Programs
curricula. In their mixed methods study of urban
Catholic high schools, Bryk and his colleagues Developmental psychologists have taken a keen
(1993) described the relationship between par- interest in out-of-school time because structured
ents and schools as fiducial, founded in an abid- and supervised after-school and extracurricular
ing sense of mutual trust. Many low-income activities can help children and adolescents nego-
parents, with low levels of education, trust the tiate salient developmental tasks (Mahoney,
schools to operate in their children’s best educa- Larson, & Eccles, 2005). Organized after-school
tional interests. In return, these schools expect programs, in particular, can be a unique context
334 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

for supporting positive youth development (i.e., access to school resources) and voice within
(Lerner, Lerner, Phelps, & colleagues, 2008). the community. The programs not only provide a
Other than helping youth to develop talents in safe haven from violent crime and poverty in
skill-building activities like sports, art, music, which valuable skills and resiliency are modeled,
community projects, and special-interest academic but also provide a broad network of partnerships
pursuits, one reason programs are so developmen- with resources throughout the city such as muse-
tally supportive is that they can foster enhanced ums and park districts. Because building collab-
relations between peers and adults (Durlak & orative, cooperative, and trusting relationships is
Weissberg, 2007; Eccles & Gootman, 2002) and the norm throughout the organization by virtue of
improved social competence among participants its central philosophy, staff members understand
(Mahoney, Parente, & Lord, 2007). For example, implicitly the importance and value of involving
youths have reported learning cooperation and families. The program’s ability to leverage social
teamwork (Hansen, Larson, & Dworkin, 2003; and community capital and increase parental
Jarrett, 1998; Rogoff, Baker-Sennett, Lacasa, & involvement was found to be a primary factor in
Goldsmith, 1995) and experiencing increased its overall effectiveness.
empathy and understanding essential to perspec-
tive taking (Dworkin, Larson, & Hansen, 2003).
Some researchers have also cited social net- Discussion
working within the community and the building
of social capital as a primary reason for the effec- As we have seen, Bronfenbrenner’s ecological
tiveness of evidence-based programs. For exam- systems theory is an especially useful lens, one
ple, in her studies of Los Angeles’ BEST After that has enhanced our understanding of parents’
School Enrichment Program (http://www.lasbest. roles in fostering student motivation and engage-
org/), Huang and colleagues (Huang, Coordt, ment. Several effective programs that we have
et al., 2009; Huang, Miyoshi, et al., 2009) not presented demonstrate the extent to which inter-
only found evidence for increased engagement actions between the proximal (micro- and meso-
with schooling and other educational outcomes systems) and distal (exo- and macrosystems)
among participants of the BEST programs, but spheres of influences are dynamic and ever evolv-
described the overall effectiveness of the program ing in order to meet the varied and changing
in terms of building social, intellectual, and orga- needs of children and families. This greater
nizational capital among a network of partici- understanding has allowed researchers and prac-
pants and stakeholders. Parental involvement is a titioners to design family-centered and culturally
large part of the networking and building of social sensitive programs that operate from a strength-
capital. Parents of the participants in the BEST based perspective.
program saw program staff not only as providing The central message underlying the consider-
a caring and safe environment, but also as acces- able body of research that we have reviewed is
sible liaisons between themselves and the school. that affective and instrumental relational support
Site coordinators and other staff became a bridge across contexts is essential to student motivation
between parents and the school fostering trust and engagement. Perceptions of general accep-
and open communication. Los Angeles’s BEST tance, respect for and interest in students as indi-
program has a high proportion of Latino and viduals, and expressions of warmth and care are
Spanish-speaking participants, many of whom critical to well-being and essential in students’
live in low-income neighborhoods. The liaison or motivation to learn and expressions of engage-
“cultural broker” role between families and the ment with schooling. This includes pragmatic
school is particularly important for such families assistance (e.g., shepherding students through the
because the staff help to translate Spanish and college application process) that adults such as
English, reducing the language barrier and thus teachers and mentors can provide to enable stu-
providing families with increased social capital dents to meet their goals. Importantly, research
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 335

has demonstrated the extent to which relational However, educators and scholars often hold lim-
support is vital as well to those individuals and ited visions of school-community partnerships.
entities that serve students. The programs initi- This is especially the case for school-based
ated by Comer, Canada, and Mapp illustrate that models putting most of the responsibility for
respect for the various ways in which parents including the community on the school, because
socialize their children for schooling creates a the efforts of even the best-intentioned schools to
foundation of trust upon which strong parent- facilitate community involvement can be impeded
school connections can be forged. Systemically, by bureaucratic immobility and lack of resources.
administrative support for programs and individ- A review of some of the most influential efforts to
uals who work with students and their families foster engagement between inner city schools
(e.g., dedicated physical space) further signals and low-income neighborhoods reveals that some
the valuing of everyone’s efforts—teachers, of the most promising models, such as Comer’s
counselors, and mentors—in supporting student School Development Program and the Harlem
achievement. These elements of relational sup- Children’s Zone project, have emerged from
port serve as sources of guidance and represent the community (Schutz, 2006). Models like these
protective factors that can help initiate, maintain, can also suggest to educators how parents become
and reengage students’ adaptive beliefs about involved in partnerships when trust is not already
learning and engagement in school. in place as it is in the case of many Catholic
Regrettably, public schooling in the USA is schools—for example, by offering adult educa-
fraught with difficulties that challenge even the tion classes and other resources that entice par-
best teachers and administrators. The pressures ents to become involved. Realistically, most
inherent in high-stakes testing, pressures from the schools and school districts are unlikely to be on
charter school movement, and ongoing school the receiving end of the kinds of financial
reform efforts may make it a great challenge for resources enjoyed by programs such as Comer’s
many educators to adopt and sustain partnerships and Canada’s. Yet as the research described above
with their students’ families and communities. suggests, strong leadership and buy-in from
For one, it is difficult to create the ideal condi- teachers and other members of the school com-
tions and time for supportive relationships, munity can serve to launch and sustain strong
engagement, and learning for students when they home-school partnerships.
do not exist for teachers. Furthermore, teachers
cannot be effective in providing support when
they hold deficit-driven models of students, fami- Implications and Future Directions
lies, and communities (Ramirez, 2003). More for Research
generally, teachers, families, and schools as rep-
resented in Epstein’s three circles cannot work Recently, and perhaps because our understanding
together in a mutually supportive fashion if they of achievement goals has advanced so far,
do not understand each other. The research evi- researchers have noted that an overreliance on
dence and models of effective programming we experimental and survey methods may limit our
have reviewed suggests that larger collaborative understanding of the complex nature of achieve-
networks of schools, families, community organi- ment goals (Dowson & McInerney, 2001; Kaplan
zations, and public institutions can provide for the & Maehr, 2007). For example, experimental set-
nurturing and supportive socialization of youth, tings can bear little resemblance to the complex
promoting engagement beyond what may be nature of classroom learning (Urdan & Turner,
achieved by a single individual teacher or parent. 2005). As Dowson and McInerney have also
A variety of observers have concluded that pointed out, the deductive approach to studying
school reform or transformation cannot be sus- students’ achievement goal orientations involves
tained in the long term without robust participa- making a priori assumptions about the presence
tion from the community (Schutz, 2006). of certain achievement goals and then using
336 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

quantitative, decontextualized measures to test styles, it is important to recognize that within


these assumptions. Furthermore, there may be a cultures and ethnicities, there exists variation in
variety of ways children construct meaning and how individuals interpret cultural beliefs.
form goals from their everyday educational expe-
riences (Bempechat & Boulay, 2001). In particu-
lar, students who differ in social class, culture Engaging Students, Families,
and ethnicity, and educational experiences may and Communities
interpret survey or questionnaire items about
their achievement goals differentially, further In moving away from a deficit perspective to
limiting our understanding. a strength-building approach, research and
Researchers in achievement motivation have theory in achievement motivation and student
therefore recognized the need to integrate quali- engagement have expanded and deepened our
tative methods in their investigations of students’ understanding of how some low-income or
and parents’ learning beliefs. Indeed, we have minority children may succeed against the odds.
seen the extent to which ethnographic research Engagement and motivation appear to be strong
has enhanced our understanding of meaning mak- mediators of resiliency to thrive in school as well
ing among ethnic and racial minority children as life in general. Superior engagement in skill-
and parents. The works of Gandara (1995), building tasks and an adaptive motivational ori-
Goldenberg and colleagues (Goldenberg & entation to succeed in school are often based in
Gallimore, 1995), Valenzuela (1999), and strong values for education and learning. Those
Delgado-Gaitan (1994) illustrate the value inher- values are neither created nor maintained in a
ent in understanding the underlying cultural vacuum, however. Parents, guardians, and teach-
meanings of words and expressions that are meant ers are perhaps the best poised to foster the moti-
to encourage and motivate students. For example, vation and engagement of children, with the
as mentioned earlier, Delgado-Gaitan’s (1992) potential to make a long-lasting influence, since
ethnographic study of six Mexican American they may have the most intimate understanding
low-income immigrant families found that par- of their needs and potentialities. The most suc-
ents supported their children’s schooling and con- cessful models converge to reveal that healthy
veyed the importance of education in a variety of patterns of engagement and motivation are fos-
ways. In addition, their reliance on “consejos,” tered in supportive networks including students,
culturally based nurturing advice, to help their teachers, parents, and community members who
children through difficult school-related issues, share a mutual interest and commitment in the
was itself a form of social capital, the sense that future welfare of youth.
parents provided caring advice that allowed their
children to successfully navigate the teacher’s
classroom rules (Delgado-Gaitan, 1994). For
example, one mother responded to the teacher’s
References
concern about her child’s inattentiveness by Alliance for Excellent Education. (2009). Fact sheet: High
encouraging her child to view the teacher as a school dropouts in America. Retrieved September
second mother, an individual to whom he had to 28, 2009, from http://www.all4ed.org/files/Graduation
pay attention. Rates_FactSheet.pdf.
Altonji, J. G., Elder, T. E., & Taber, C. R. (2005). Selection
Despite the knowledge gleaned from research on observed and unobserved variables: Assessing the
that has examined differences between ethnic effectiveness of Catholic schools. The Journal of
groups, researchers and educators must be wary Political Economy, 113, 151–184.
about adopting stereotypic views of “Latino” Ames, C. (1984). Goal structures and motivation. The
Elementary School Journal, 85, 39–52.
or “African American” or “Chinese American” Ames, C. (1992a). Achievement goals and the classroom
parents’ educational socialization practices. motivational climate. In D. H. Schunk (Ed.), Student
While cultural beliefs may indeed guide parenting perceptions in the classroom. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 337

Ames, C. (1992b). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and stu- Brophy, J. E. (1987). Synthesis of research on strategies
dent motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, for motivating students to learn. Educational
84, 261–271. Leadership, 44, 40–48.
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. Bryk, A., Lee, V., & Holland, P. (1993). Catholic schools
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- and the common good. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. University Press.
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. Byrd-Blake, M., Afolayan, M., Hunt, J. W., Fabunmi, M.,
Bauch, P. A., & Goldring, E. B. (1995). Parent involve- Pryor, B. W., & Lender, R. (2010). Morale of teachers
ment and school responsiveness: Facilitating the home- in high poverty schools: A post-NCLB mixed methods
school connection in schools of choice. Educational analysis. Education and Urban Society, 42, 450–472.
Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 17, 1–21. Carbonaro, W. J. (2003). Sector differences in student learn-
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. ing: Differences in achievement gains across school
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. years and during the summer. Catholic Education:
Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. A Journal of Inquiry and Practice, 7, 219–245.
In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and Cauthen, N. K., & Fass, S. (2008). Measuring income and
tomorrow. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. poverty in the United States. New York: National
Bempechat, J. (1998). Against the odds: How ‘at risk’ stu- Center for Children in Poverty, Columbia University,
dents exceed expectations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Mailman School of Public Health.
Bempechat, J. (2004). The motivational benefits of home- Chang, L., McBride-Chang, C., Stewart, S. M., & Au, E.
work: A social-cognitive perspective. Theory Into (2003). Life satisfaction, self-concept, and family rela-
Practice, 43, 189–196. tions in Chinese adolescents and children. International
Bempechat, J., & Boulay, B. (2001). Beyond dichotomous Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 82–189.
characterizations: New directions in achievement Chen, J. J.-L. (2008). Grade-level differences: Relations
motivation research. In D. McInerney & S. V. Etten of parental, teacher and peer support to academic
(Eds.), Research on sociocultural influences on moti- engagement and achievement among Hong Kong stu-
vation and learning (Vol. 1, pp. 15–36). Greenwich, dents. School Psychology International, 29, 183–198.
CT: Information Age Publishing. Christenson, S. L., & Sheridan, S. M. (2001). School and
Bempechat, J., Boulay, B. A., Piergross, S., & Wenk, K. families: Creating essential connections for learning.
(2008). Beyond the rhetoric: Understanding achieve- New York: Guilford Press.
ment and motivation in Catholic school students. Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future
Education and Urban Society, 40, 167–178. discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bempechat, J., Li, J., Wenk, K., & Holloway, S. D. Coleman, J., Hoffer, T., & Kilgore, S. (1982). Cognitive
Achievement goals of low income students: A qualita- outcomes in public and private schools. Sociology of
tive study of adolescent meaning making. Manuscript Education, 55, 65–76.
submitted for publication. Comer, J. P. (2005). The rewards of parent participation.
Bempechat, J., Shernoff, D. J., Li, J., Holloway, S. D., & Educational Leadership, 62, 38–42.
Arendtsz, A. L. (2010). Achievement beliefs and Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence,
school engagement in low income adolescents: autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of
A mixed-methods study. Paper presented at the self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe
American Educational Research Association, Denver. (Eds.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology
Blumenfeld, P. C. (1992). Classroom learning and motiva- (Vol. 23). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
tion: Clarifying and expanding goal theory. Journal of Cooper, H., Lindsay, J. J., Nye, B., & Greathouse, S.
Educational Psychology, 84, 272–281. (1998). Relationships among attitudes about home-
Bourdieu, P. (1985). The forms of capital. In J. G. work, amount of homework assigned and completed,
Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of theory and research and student achievement. Journal of Educational
for the sociology of education (pp. 241–258). Psychology, 90, 70–83.
New York: Greenwood. Covington, M. V. (2000). Intrinsic versus extrinsic moti-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecol- vation in schools: A reconciliation. Current Directions
ogy of human development. American Psychologist, in Psychological Science, 9, 22–25.
32, 513–531. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Play and intrinsic rewards.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 15(3), 41–63.
reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bio- Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of
ecological model. Psychological Review, 101, 568–586. optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial.
Brooks-Gunn, J., Linver, M. R., & Fauth, R. C. (2005). Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: The psychol-
Children’s competence and socioeconomic status in ogy of engagement with everyday life. New York:
the family and neighborhood. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Basic Books.
Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motiva- Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.).
tion (pp. 414–435). New York: Guilford. (1988). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of
Brophy, J. E. (1983). Conceptualizing student motivation. flow in consciousness. New York: Cambridge University
Educational Psychologist, 18, 200–215. Press.
338 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adoles- Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation
cent: Conflict and growth in the teenage years. and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34,
New York: Basic Books. 149–169.
DeCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation; the internal Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach
effective determinants of behavior. New York: to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality
Academic. and Social Psychology, 54, 5–12.
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Ellison, B. J., & Hallinan, M. T. (2004). Ability grouping
Plenum Press. in catholic and public schools. Catholic Education:
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation A Journal of Inquiry and Practice, 8, 107–129.
and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Hejmadi, A. (2008).
Plenum. Mother and child emotions during mathematics home-
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1992). School matters in the work. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10,
Mexican-American home: Socializing children to 5–35.
education. American Educational Research Journal, Epstein, J. L. (1995). School/family/community partner-
29, 495–513. ships: Caring for the children we share. Phi Delta
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1994). Consejos: The power of cul- Kappan, 76, 701–712.
tural narratives. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, Epstein, J. L., & Van Vooris, F. L. (2001). More than min-
25, 298–316. utes: Teachers’ roles in designing homework.
Dobbie, W., & Fryer, R. G. (2009). Are high-quality Educational Psychologist, 36, 181–193.
schools enough to close the achievement gap? Espinoza-Herold, M. (2007). Stepping beyond Si Se
Evidence from a bold social experiment in Harlem Puede: Dichos as a cultural resource in mother-daugh-
(No. 15473). Cambridge, MA: NBER. ter interaction in a Latino family. Anthropology &
Dowson, M., & McInerney, D. M. (2001). Psychological Education Quarterly, 38(3), 260–277.
parameters of students’ social and work avoidance Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
goals: A qualitative investigation. Journal of Educational Research, 59, 117–142.
Educational Psychology, 93, 35–42. Finn, J. D. (1993). School engagement and students at risk
Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). Family poverty, (No. NCES-93–470). Washington, DC: National
welfare reform, and child development. Child Center for Education Statistics.
Development, 71, 188–196. Finn, J. D., & Voelkl, K. E. (1993). School characteristics
Durlak, J. A., & Weissberg, R. P. (2007). The impact of related to student engagement. The Journal of Negro
after-school programs that promote personal and Education, 62, 249–268.
social skills. Chicago: Collaborative for Academic, Flouri, E. (2004). Subjective well-being in midlife: The
Social, and Emotional Learning (CASEL). role of involvement of and closeness to parents in
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of childhood. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5, 335–358.
success. New York: Random House. Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
Dweck, C. S., & Bempechat, J. (1983). Children’s theories School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
of intelligence: Consequences for learning. In S. G. the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
Paris, G. M. Olson, & H. W. Stevenson (Eds.), Learning 59–109.
and motivation in the classroom (pp. 239–256). Furlong, M. J., Whipple, A. D., St. Jean, G., Simental, J.,
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Soliz, A., & Punthuna, S. (2003). Multiple contexts of
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social cognitive school engagement: Moving toward a unifying frame-
approach to motivation and personality. Psychological work for educational research and practice. The
Review, 95, 256–273. California School Psychologist, 9, 99–114.
Dworkin, J. B., Larson, R., & Hansen, D. (2003). Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a
Adolescents’ accounts of growth experiences in youth factor in children’s academic engagement and perfor-
activities. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,
17–26. 148–162.
Eccles, J. S., & Gootman, J. A. (2002). Community pro- Gandara, P. C. (1995). Over the ivy walls: The educational
grams to promote youth development. Washington, mobility of low income Chicanos. Albany, NY: SUNY
DC: National Academy Press. Press.
Eccles, J. S., Roeser, R., Vida, M., Fredricks, J. A., & García Coll, C., & Marks, A. K. (2009). Immigrant sto-
Wigfield, A. (2006). Motivational and achievement ries: Ethnicity and academics in middle childhood.
pathways through middle childhood. In L. Balter & Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A hand- Gilman, R., Huebner, E. S., & Furlong, M. J. (Eds.).
book of contemporary issues (2nd ed., pp. 325–355). (2009). Handbook of positive psychology in schools.
New York: Psychology Press. New York: Routledge.
Eide, E. R., Goldhaber, D. D., & Showalter, M. H. (2004). Goldenberg, C., & Gallimore, R. (1995). Immigrant
Does Catholic high school attendance lead to atten- Latino parents’ values and beliefs about their chil-
dance at a more selective college? Social Science dren’s education: Continuities and discontinuities
Quarterly, 85, 1335–1352. across cultures and generations. In M. Maehr, P. R.
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 339

Pintrich, & D. E. Bartz (Eds.), Advances in motivation interest in learning and development (pp. 215–238).
and achievement: Culture, motivation, and achieve- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ment (Vol. 9, pp. 183–228). San Francisco: JAI. Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase
Gorman-Smith, D., Tolan, P. H., Henry, D. B., & model of interest development. Educational
Florsheim, P. (2000). Patterns of family functioning Psychologist, 41, 111–127.
and adolescent outcomes among urban African Hill, N. E., & Taylor, L. C. (2004). Parental school
American and Mexican American families. Journal of involvement and children’s academic achievement.
Family Psychology, 14, 436–457. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13,
Green, G., Rhodes, J., Hirsch, A. H., Suarez-Orozco, C., 161–164.
& Camic, P. M. (2008). Supportive adult relationships Hong, E., & Lee, K. (1999, April). Chinese parents’
and the academic engagement of Latin American awareness of their children’s homework style and
immigrant youth. Journal of School Psychology, 46, homework behavior and its effects on achievement.
393–412. Paper presented at the American Educational Research
Griffiths, A.-J., Sharkey, J. D., & Furlong, M. J. (2009). Association, Montreal, Canada.
Student engagement and positive school adaption. In R. Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Battiato, A. C., Walker, J. M.,
Gilman, E. S. Huebner, & M. J. Furlong (Eds.), Reed, R. P., DeLong, J. M., & Jones, K. P. (2001).
Handbook of positive psychology in schools. New York: Parental involvement in homework. Educational
Routledge. Psychologist, 36, 195–209.
Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles asso- Horvat, E. M., Weininger, E. B., & Lareau, A. (2003).
ciated with children’s self-regulation and competence From social ties to social capital. American Educational
in school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, Research Journal, 40, 319–351.
143–154. Huang, D., Coordt, A., La Torre, D., Leon, S., Miyoshi, J.,
Grolnick, W. S., & Slowiaczek, M. L. (1994). Parents’ Perez, P., et al. (2009). The after-school hours:
involvement in children’s schooling: A multidimen- Examining the relationship between afterschool staff-
sional conceptualization and motivational model. based social capital and student engagement in LA’s
Child Development, 65, 237–252. BEST. Los Angeles, CA: National Center for Research
Hansen, D. M., Larson, R. W., & Dworkin, J. B. (2003). on Evaluation, Standards, and Student Testing.
What adolescents learn in organized youth activities: Huang, D., Miyoshi, J., La Torre, D., Marshall, A., Perez,
A survey of self-reported developmental experiences. P., & Peterson, C. (2009). Exploring the intellectual,
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 25–56. social, and organizational capitals at LA’s BEST. Los
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, Angeles: National Center for Research on Evaluation,
A. J. (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitu- Standards, and Student Testing.
dinal study of achievement goals and ability measures Huebner, E. S., & Diener, C. (2008). Research on life sat-
as predictors of interest and performance from fresh- isfaction of children and youth. In M. Eid & R. J.
man year through graduation. Journal of Educational Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being.
Psychology, 94, 562–575. New York: The Guilford Press.
Harkness, S., & Super, C. (1992). Parental ethnotheories in Hughes, J. N., & Kwok, O.-M. (2007). Influence of stu-
action. In A. Sigel, J. McGillicuddy-DeLisi, & J. J. dent-teacher and parent-teacher relationships on lower
Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief systems: The psycho- achieving readers’ engagement and achievement in the
logical consequences for children (2nd ed., pp. 373–391). primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 99, 39–51.
Hauser, R. M., Pager, D. I., & Simmons, S. J. (2004). Hulleman, C. S., Durik, A. M., Schweigert, S. B., &
Race-ethnicity, social background, and grade retention. Harackiewicz, J. M. (2008). Task values, achievement
In H. J. Walberg, A. J. Reynolds, & M. C. Wang (Eds.), goals, and interest: An integrative analysis. Journal of
Can unlike students learn together? (pp. 97–114). Educational Psychology, 100, 398–416.
Greenwich, CT: IAP. Ingersoll, R. M. (2004). Why do high-poverty schools
Haynes, N. M., Emmons, C. L., Gebreyesus, S., & Ben- have difficulty staffing their classrooms with qualified
Avie, M. (1996). The School Development Program teachers? Washington, DC: Center for American
evaluation process. In N. M. Haynes (Ed.), Rallying Progress.
the whole village: The Comer process for reforming Jarrett, R. L. (1998). African American children, families,
education (pp. 123–146). New York: Teachers College and neighborhoods: Qualitative contributions to
Press. understanding developmental pathways. Applied
Hektner, J. M., Schmidt, J. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. Developmental Science, 2, 2–16.
(2007). Experience sampling method: Measuring the Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. (1977). Problem behavior and psy-
quality of everyday life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. chosocial development: A longitudinal study of youth.
Henri, R. (1923/2007). The art spirit. New York: Basic New York: Academic.
Books. Jeynes, W. H. (2010). The salience of the subtle aspects of
Hidi, S., & Anderson, V. (1992). Situational interest and parental involvement and encouraging that involve-
its impact on reading and expository writing. In K. A. ment: Implications for school based programs.
Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of Teachers College Record, 112, 747–774.
340 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

Johnson, S. M. (2006). The workplace matters: Teacher for child and adolescent development (Vol. 121,
quality, retention, and effectiveness. Washington, DC: pp. 9–25). San Francisco: Wiley.
National Education Association. Lubienski, S. L., Lubienski, C., & Crane, C. C. (2008).
Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. (2007). The contributions and Achievement differences and school type: The role of
prospects of goal orientation theory. Educational school climate, teacher certification, and instruction.
Psychology Review, 19, 141–184. American Journal of Education, 115(1), 97–138.
Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self: Problem and process Maehr, M. L. (1976). Continuing motivation: An analysis
in human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard of a seldom considered educational outcome. Review
University Press. of Educational Research, 46, 443–462.
Kohn, A. (2006). The homework myth: Why our kids get Maehr, M. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1980). Culture and
too much of a bad thing. Cambridge, MA: De Capo achievement motivation: A second look. In N. Warren
Lifelong Books. (Ed.), Studies in cross cultural psychology. San Diego,
Kralovec, E., & Buell, J. (2001). End homework now. CA: Academic.
Educational Leadership, 58(7), 39–42. Mahoney, J. L., Larson, R. W., & Eccles, J. S. (Eds.).
Lamborn, S. D., Brown, B. B., Mounts, N. S., & Steinberg, (2005). Organized activities as contexts of develop-
L. (1992). Putting school in perspective: The influence ment: Extracurricular activities, after-school and com-
of family, peers, extracurricular participation, and munity programs. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
part-time work on academic engagement. In F. M. Mahoney, J. L., Parente, M. E., & Lord, H. (2007). After-
Newmann (Ed.), Student engagement and achieve- school program engagement: Links to child compe-
ment in American secondary schools (pp. 153–181). tence and program quality and content. The Elementary
New York: Teachers College Press. School Journal, 107, 385–404.
Lansford, J. E., Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, Mandara, J., & Murray, C. B. (2002). Development of an
J. E., & Petit, G. S. (2004). Ethnic differences in the empirical typology of African American family func-
link between physical discipline and later adolescent tioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 318–337.
externalizing behaviors. Journal of Child Psychology Mandara, J., Varner, F., Greene, N., & Richman, S. (2009).
and Psychiatry, 45, 801–812. Intergenerational family predictors of the black–white
Lareau, A. (1987). Social class differences in family- achievement gap. Journal of Educational Psychology,
school relationships: The importance of cultural capi- 101, 867–878.
tal. Sociology of Education, 60, 73–85. Mapp, K. L., Johnson, V. R., Strickland, C. S., & Meza, C.
Lareau, A. (2000). Home advantage: Social class and (2008). High school family centers: Transformative
parental intervention in elementary education. spaces linking schools and families in support of student
Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc. learning. Marriage and Family Review, 43, 338–368.
Lareau, A. (2002). Invisible inequality: Social class and Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional
childrearing in black families and white families. activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle and high
American Sociological Review, 67, 747–776. school years. American Educational Research Journal,
Larson, R. W. (2006). Positive youth development, willful 37, 153–184.
adolescents, and mentoring. Journal of Community Martin, A. J., & Dowson, M. (2009). Interpersonal rela-
Psychology, 34, 677–689. tionships, motivation, engagement, and achievement:
Larson, R. W., & Richards, M. H. (1991). Boredom in the Yields for theory, current issues, and educational prac-
middle school years: Blaming schools versus blaming tice. Review of Educational Research, 79, 327–365.
students. American Journal of Education, 99, 418–443. Meece, J. L., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Hoyle, R. H. (1988).
Leone, C. M., & Richards, M. H. (1989). Classwork and Students’ goal orientations and cognitive engagement
homework in early adolescence: The ecology of in classroom activities. Journal of Educational
achievement. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18, Psychology, 80, 514–523.
531–548. Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: Its multifaceted
Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., Phelps, E., & Colleagues. structure in the secondary school mathematics class-
(2008). The positive development of youth: Report of room. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 424–436.
the findings from the first four years of the 4-H study of Moll, L. C., Amanti, C., Neff, D., & Gonzalez, N. (1992).
positive youth development. Retreived Dec. 4, 2011 Funds of knowledge for teaching: Using a qualitative
from http://www.ase.tufts.edu/iaryd/documents/4HStudy approach to connect homes and classrooms. Theory
Findings2008.pdf. Into Practice, 31(2), 132–141.
Leventhal, T., Fauth, R. C., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2005). Morgan, S. L. (2001). Counterfactuals, causal effect het-
Neighborhood poverty and public policy: A 5-year erogeneity, and the Catholic school effect on learning.
follow-up of children’s educational outcomes in the Sociology of Education, 74, 341–374.
New York City moving to opportunity demonstration. Nakamura, J. (2001). The nature of vital engagement
Developmental Psychology, 41, 933–952. in adulthood. In M. Michaelson & J. Nakamura
Li, J., Holloway, S. D., Bempechat, J., & Loh, E. (2008). (Eds.), Supportive frameworks for youth engagement
Building and using a social network: Nurture for low (pp. 5–18). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
income Chinese American adolescents’ learning. In Y. Nakamura, J., & Shernoff, D. J. (2009). Good mentoring:
Hirokazu & N. Way (Eds.), The social contexts of Fostering excellent practice in higher education. San
immigrant children and adolescents. New directions Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
15 Parental Influences on Achievement Motivation and Student Engagement 341

National Research Council, Institute of Medicine of the Pomerantz, E. M., Ng, F. F., & Wang, Q. (2006). Mothers’
National Academies. (2004). Engaging schools: mastery-oriented involvement in children’s home-
Fostering high school students’ motivation to learn. work: Implications for the well-being of children
Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. with negative perceptions of competence. Journal of
Newmann, F. M. (Ed.). (1992). Student engagement Educational Psychology, 98, 99–111.
and achievement in American secondary schools. Ramirez, A. Y. (2003). Dismay and disappointment:
New York: Teachers College Press. Parental involvement of Latino immigrant parents.
Newmann, F. M., & Wehlage, G. G. (1993). Five stan- The Urban Review, 35(2), 93–110.
dards of authentic instruction. Educational Leadership, Rathunde, K. (1996). Family context and talented adoles-
50, 8–12. cents’ optimal experience in school-related activities.
Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The development of the concepts Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 605–628.
of effort and ability, perception of academic attain- Reese, L., Balzano, S., Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C.
ment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require (1995). The concept of “educaciòn”: Latino family
more ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814. values and American schooling. International Journal
Nicholls, J. G. (1983). Conceptions of ability and achieve- of Educational Research, 23, 57–81.
ment motivation: A theory and its implications for Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2009). Parents as
education. In S. G. Paris, G. M. Olson, & H. W. essential partners for fostering students’ learning out-
Stevenson (Eds.), Learning and motivation in the comes. In R. Gilman, E. S. Huebner, & M. J. Furlong
classroom (pp. 211–237). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology in schools
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: (pp. 257–272). New York: Routledge.
Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task Reschly, A. L., Huebner, E. S., Appleton, J. J., &
choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, Antaramian, S. (2008). Engagement as flourishing:
328–346. The contribution of positive emotions and coping to
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and demo- adolescents’ engagement at school and with learning.
cratic education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Psychology in the Schools, 45, 419–431.
Press. Rhodes, J. (2002). Stand by me: The risks and rewards of
No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act of 2001, Pub. L. No. mentoring today’s youth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
107–110, § 115, Stat. 1425 (2002). University Press.
Nystrand, M., & Gamoran, A. (1991). Instructional dis- Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive
course, student engagement, and literature achieve- development in a social context. New York: Oxford
ment. Research in the Teaching of English, 25, University Press.
261–290. Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human develop-
Ogbu, J. U. (2003). Black American students in an affluent ment. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
suburb: A study of academic disengagement. Mahwah, Rogoff, B., Baker-Sennett, J., Lacasa, P., & Goldsmith, D.
NJ: Erlbaum. (1995). Development through participation in socio-
Paris, S. G., & Winograd, P. (1990). Promoting metacog- cultural activity. In J. J. Goodnow, P. J. Miller, &
nition and motivation of exceptional children. Rase: F. Kessel (Eds.), Cultural practices as contexts for
Remedial & Special Education, 11, 7–15. development (Vol. 67, pp. 45–65). San Francisco:
Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., Tuson, K. Jossey-Bass.
M., Briere, N. M., & Blais, M. R. (1995). Toward a Roorda, D. L., Koomen, H. M. Y., Spilt, J. L., & Oort, F.
new measure of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motiva- J. (2011). The influence of affective teacher-student
tion, and amotivation in sports: The sport motivation relationships on students’ school engagement and
scale (SMS). Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, achievement: A meta-analytic approach. Review of
17, 35–53. Educational Research, 81, 493–529.
Pinderhughes, E. E., Nix, R., Foster, E. M., & Jones, D. Roy, J., & Mishel, L. (2008). Using administrative data to
(2001). Parenting in context: Impact of neighborhood estimate graduation rates: Challenges, proposed solu-
poverty, residential stability, public services, social tions and their pitfalls. Education Policy Analysis
network. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, Archives, 16(11), 2–27.
941–953. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic
Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: motivations: Classic definitions and new directions.
The role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 544–555. Sander, W., & Krautman, A. C. (1995). Catholic schools,
Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and dropout rates and educational attainment. Economic
self-regulated learning components of classroom aca- Inquiry, 33, 217–233.
demic performance. Journal of Educational Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Intrinsic and
Psychology, 82, 33–40. extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motiva-
Pomerantz, E. M., Moorman, E. A., & Litwak, S. D. tion and performance. San Diego, CA: Academic.
(2007). The how, whom, and why of parents’ involve- Schmidt, J. A., Shernoff, D. J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M.
ment in children’s academic lives: More is not (2007). Individual and situational factors related to the
always better. Review of Educational Research, 77(3), experience of flow in adolescence: A multilevel
373–410. approach. In A. D. Ong & Mv Dulmen (Eds.),
342 J. Bempechat and D.J. Shernoff

The handbook of methods in positive psychology Stipek, D. J. (1993). Motivation to learn: From theory to
(pp. 542–558). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. practice (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (Eds.). Suárez-Orozco, C., Suárez-Orozco, M., & Todorova, I.
(2008). Motivation in education: Theory, research, (2008). Learning a new land: Immigrant students in
and applications (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: American society. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/
Merrill Prentice Hall. Harvard University Press.
Schutz, A. (2006). Home is a prison in the global city: Suldo, S. M. (2009). Parent-child relationships. In R.
The tragic failure of school-based community engage- Gilman, E. S. Huebner, & M. J. Furlong (Eds.),
ment strategies. Review of Educational Research, 76, Handbook of positive psychology in schools. New
691–743. York: Routledge.
Shek, D. T. L. (1998). A longitudinal study of the rela- Taylor, L. C., Hinton, I. D., & Wilson, G. J. (1995).
tions between parent-adolescent conflict and adoles- Parental influences on academic performance in
cent psychological well-being. Journal of Genetic African American students. Journal of Child and
Psychology, 159, 53–67. Family Studies, 4, 293–302.
Shernoff, D. J. (2010). The experience of student engage- Tendulkar, S., Buka, S., Dunn, E. C., Subramanian, S. V.,
ment in high school classrooms: Influences and effects & Koenen, K. C. (2010). A multilevel investigation of
on long-term outcomes. Saarbrucken, Germany: neighborhood effects on parental warmth. Journal of
Lambert Academic Publishing. Community Psychology, 38(5), 557–573.
Shernoff, D. J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Flow in Toppo, G. (2010). In Philadelphia, a bold move against
schools: Cultivating engaged learners and optimal “dropout factories.” USA Today, 1A–2A.
learning environments. In R. C. Gilman, E. S. Urdan, T., & Turner, J. C. (2005). Competence motivation
Heubner, & M. J. Furlong (Eds.), Handbook of posi- in the classroom. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.),
tive psychology in schools (pp. 131–145). New York: Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 297–
Routledge. 317). New York: Guilford.
Shernoff, D. J., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Schneider, B., & Valenzuela, A. (1999). Subtractive schooling: US-Mexican
Shernoff, E. S. (2003). Student engagement in high youth and the politics of caring. New York: SUNY
school classrooms from the perspective of flow theory. Press.
School Psychology Quarterly, 18, 158–176. Valenzuela, A., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1994). Familism and
Shernoff, D. J., & Hoogstra, L. (2001). Continuing moti- social capital in the academic achievement of Mexican
vation beyond the high school classroom. New origin and Anglo Americans. Social Science Quarterly,
Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 93, 75, 18–36.
73–87. Voelkl, K. E. (1997). Identification with school. American
Shernoff, D. J., & Schmidt, J. A. (2008). Further evi- Journal of Education, 105, 294–318.
dence of an engagement-achievement paradox among Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development
U.S. high school students. Journal of Youth and of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA:
Adolescence, 36, 891–903. Harvard University Press.
Shernoff, D. J., & Vandell, D. L. (2007). Engagement in Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some class-
after-school program activities: Quality of experience room experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology,
from the perspective of participants. Journal of Youth 71, 3–25.
and Adolescence, 36, 891–903. Weiner, B. (2005). Motivation from an attributional per-
Shulman, R. (2009, August 2). Harlem program singled spective and the social psychology of perceived com-
out as model. Washington Post. petence. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook
Sirin, S. R., & Rogers-Sirin, L. (2004). Exploring school of competence and motivation (pp. 73–84). New York:
engagement of middle-class African American adoles- Guilford Publications.
cents. Youth & Society, 35, 323–340. Weisner, T. (2002). Ecocultural understanding of chil-
Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the dren’s developmental pathways. Human Development,
classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and 45, 275–281.
student engagement across the school year. Journal of Xu, J., & Corno, L. (1998). Case studies of families doing
Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581. third grade homework. Teachers College Record, 100,
Smerdon, B. A. (1999). Engagement and achievement: 402–436.
Differences between African-American and white Xu, J., & Corno, L. (2003). Family help and homework
high school students. Research in Sociology of management reported by middle school students. The
Education and Socialization, 12, 103–134. Elementary School Journal, 103, 503–519.
Steinberg, L. (1996). Beyond the classroom: Why school Zhang, S., & Anderson, S. G. (2010). Low-income single
reform has failed and what parents need to do. New mothers’ community violence exposure and aggres-
York: Simon & Schuster. sive parenting practices. Children and Youth Services
Steinberg, L., Mounts, N. S., Lamborn, S. D., & Review, 32, 889–895.
Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Authoritative parenting and Zimmerman, B. J. (1990). Self-regulated learning and
adolescent adjustment across varied ecological niches. academic achievement: An overview. Educational
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 1, 19–36. Psychologist, 25, 3–17.
Families as Facilitators
of Student Engagement: Toward 16
a Home-School Partnership Model

Jacquelyn N. Raftery, Wendy S. Grolnick,


and Elizabeth S. Flamm

Abstract
Homes make key contributions to children’s achievement. Parents are
salient facilitators of engagement, and schools, through their active col-
laboration with families, can help caregivers create home environments
that promote academic success. Engagement, from a Self-determination
Theory (SDT) framework, is the outward manifestation of motivation and
occurs most readily in contexts that satisfy children’s needs for related-
ness, autonomy, and competence. We review the substantial literature
pointing to three corresponding parent variables, involvement, autonomy
support, and structure that contribute to motivation and thereby engage-
ment. We then consider the role of schools in promoting facilitative par-
enting. Research identifies barriers to involving families as well as
sociodemographic and school structural variables that predict parent par-
ticipation in children’s learning. Emerging work also highlights the effi-
cacy of system-wide interventions to develop school-family partnerships
wherein educators and parents work together toward enhancing student
success. Suggestions for future directions include developing interven-
tions that target parent autonomy support and structure and conducting
research that embraces the bidirectional and transactional nature of home
and school influences on student engagement.

It is Multicultural Night at Center School, and


J.N. Raftery (*)
students are presenting to teachers, parents, and class-
Frances L. Hiatt School of Psychology,
mates the creative projects they have completed
Clark University, Worcester, MA, USA
that celebrate their own cultural backgrounds. In
e-mail: jraftery@clarku.edu
Ms. Jones’ 6th grade class, it is Maria’s turn to
W.S. Grolnick, Ph.D. present her project. Maria, whose family is from
Frances L. Hiatt School of Psychology, Clark University, Ecuador, has spent weeks working on her presen-
Worcester, MA, USA tation. She begins by talking about the upcoming
e-mail: wgrolnick@clarku.edu holiday, El Día de los Muertos. “On this day,” she
says “we gather and pray for and remember friends
E.S. Flamm
and family members who have passed away.”
Frances L. Hiatt School of Psychology,
Maria glances over at her mother who is sitting in
Clark University, Worcester, MA, USA
the back of the classroom, smiling, as Maria talks
e-mail: eflamm@clarku.edu

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 343


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_16, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
344 J.N. Raftery et al.

about the deep-rooted traditions and meaning effort by the school and its families to work
behind the holiday. Maria gives her mother a nod together, based on a mutual interest and shared
and continues, “And on El Día de los Muertos, we
eat colada morado, a spiced fruit porridge that is responsibility for children’s learning. Involving
purple! My mother and I made some last night and families in this event recognizes the importance
she will pass out samples for you all to try.” Maria’s of parents in children developing the attitudes
classmates giggle with anticipation to try the bright and motives necessary for success in school.
purple porridge. After passing out all the samples,
Maria’s mother joins Maria at the front of the Further, it illustrates how schools can play a key
classroom. Together, they look out onto the audi- role in facilitating parent involvement.
ence of teachers, parents and students, and can In this chapter, we explore the roles of homes
only laugh as a chorus of “yum!” fills the room. in facilitating student success and, in addition,
“How did you get this to be such a deep purple?”
Maria’s teacher eagerly asks. Maria looks at her how schools and families can interact in ways
mother and whispers, “Should we tell them our that will positively affect students. While educa-
secret family recipe?” Maria’s mother smiles and tional success can encompass a number of out-
gives an approving nod, and together, they take comes, we will focus on student engagement.
turns sharing the recipe that has been in their fam-
ily for more than six generations. And when they Most psychologists and educators agree that
are done, the audience roars in applause and Maria facilitating students’ active engagement in learn-
feels a sense of satisfaction and pleasure that she ing—like the engagement Center School’s stu-
has shared a piece of her culture with others. dents displayed on Multicultural Night—is a key
goal for education. Researchers are beginning to
There are a number of things going on in Ms.
recognize that enhancing student engagement is a
Jones’ classroom on Multicultural Night. First,
complex process involving a number of factors
the students are engaged in the presentations—
and institutions including those within students,
they are enthusiastically asking and answering
schools, families, and communities (Southwest
questions of one another, intensely and atten-
Educational Developmental Laboratory, 2002).
tively listening to their peers, and expressing a
In this chapter, we will explore the role of fami-
genuine desire to understand differences in the
lies in student engagement, taking into account
world. They seem to be enjoying one another’s
interactions with student and school characteris-
projects and are having fun learning about other
tics. Most notably, we look at these interactions
cultures.
in bidirectional and transactional frameworks as
And if we take a step back, we can see that
dynamic processes ultimately affecting student
there is even more going on in this classroom.
engagement.
There are parents who, too, are involved in the
presentations. They are not only members of the
audience, but active participants, presenting
right alongside their children. Further, it is the Engagement
parents who envisioned this night from the start.
They recognized that there was immense diver- We begin by defining engagement and then
sity in their school community that should be move to a motivational perspective that consid-
celebrated. And so, these parents created and ers engagement as the quality of children’s
organized this night devoted to honoring and interactions with academic activities (Skinner,
appreciating difference. And then there are the Kindermann, Connell, & Wellborn, 2009). There
teachers. Sitting among the students and parents, have been a variety of definitions of engagement
these educators are teaching, yet are also learn- as well as some controversy over the nature of
ing from the families about the diversity of engagement and its boundaries (Appleton,
experience. Christenson, & Furlong, 2008). Some perspec-
This night, as successful as it has been, did not tives on academic engagement have focused on
happen by accident. Instead, it was an active behavior, especially participation in the school
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 345

process (e.g., Natriello, 1984). Many perspec- with their environments. In this chapter, we take
tives also include a second affective or emotional a motivational perspective on this issue.
component concerning students’ positive attitudes, Motivational perspectives focus on the energy
interest, and sense of belonging (e.g., Finn, and direction of behavior, for example, what
1989). Recent views have added a cognitive makes children engaged in particular activities,
component involving the types of goals or values rather than behavior alone. Thus, we focus on the
students have with regard to learning (e.g., processes that underlie behavior and how the
Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004) and thus active individual constructs self-related beliefs and
include three components. In this chapter, we affects relevant to him or herself and his or her
adopt Skinner et al.’s (2009) definition of engage- learning that fuel engagement. Our chapter utilizes
ment and three-component conceptualization. the motivational perspective of Self-determination
According to Skinner et al., engagement is the Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985).
“outward manifestation of motivation-namely
energized, directed, and sustained action” (p. 225).
Their model includes the three components of A Motivational Perspective
behavior, cognition, and affect. The behavioral on Engagement
component concerns a child’s determination,
effort, intensity, persistence, and perseverance in Self-determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985)
response to challenges. The emotional dimen- posits that individuals have psychological needs
sion includes an enthusiasm about learning, for autonomy, competence, and relatedness which
enjoyment and pleasure in activities, and a sense they attempt to satisfy in their interactions with
of satisfaction when completing challenges. their environments. The need for autonomy
Finally, cognitive engagement encompasses a concerns a need to feel volitional regarding one’s
student’s attention, participation, focus, and pro- actions and to feel that behaviors are self-initiated,
pensity to set goals beyond what is minimally rather than externally regulated (Ryan & Connell,
expected. 1989; Ryan, Connell, & Grolnick, 1992). The
Researchers suggest that engagement in need for competence concerns individuals’ need
school may contribute to important academic to feel effective in interactions with the environ-
outcomes because it is through active participa- ment, that is, to feel like they can produce positive
tion that children learn best (Klem & Connell, or prevent negative outcomes and that they have
2004). Research has found that engagement in the capacity to master challenges (Harter, 1982;
academic activities predicts critical school out- Skinner, Wellborn, & Connell, 1990). Lastly, the
comes, such as academic resilience (Finn & need for relatedness is a need to feel related or
Rock, 1997), attendance and retention in school connected to others and loved and valued by
(Connell, Halpern-Felsher, Clifford, Crichlow, & them.
Usinger, 1995; Connell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Based on their experiences with contexts that
Goodenow, 1993; Sinclair, Christenson, Lehr, & support or neglect their needs, children will con-
Anderson, 2003), as well as grades and achieve- struct and revise self-system processes, which are
ment test scores (Connell et al., 1995; Skinner, their attitudes, beliefs, and motivational propen-
Zimmer-Gembeck, & Connell, 1998). Given its sities with regard to themselves and the world.
critical role in students’ learning and school suc- These self-system processes can be organized
cess, it is crucial to understand the factors that around the psychological needs of autonomy,
influence engagement. competence, and relatedness and filter children’s
In addressing ways in which families, in inter- experiences of the environment and future social
action with schools, influence student engage- interactions (Connell & Wellborn, 1991). The
ment, it is important to use a framework experience of autonomy can be seen in the degree
specifying what children need to fully engage to which individuals initiate and sustain their
346 J.N. Raftery et al.

behavior volitionally versus doing so out of a et al., 2009). When children feel connected to
sense of pressure or coercion. This manifests in others and feel that they are lovable and impor-
children engaging in inherently interesting activi- tant, they will show effortful participation and
ties out of a sense of pleasure and enjoyment— persistence in the classroom (Murdock, 1999). In
that is, displaying intrinsic motivation to learn these ways, motivational resources result in pat-
and master materials. In addition, autonomy can terns of action involving affect, attention, and
be seen in children increasingly taking on or behavior that encompass engagement—enthusi-
internalizing the regulation of activities that may astic, focused, and purposeful participation in
not be inherently interesting and doing them out learning.
of a sense of importance or value. Ryan and Linked to the needs are three aspects of the
Connell (1989) thus described children’s self- environment: autonomy support, structure, and
regulation of learning activities as varying along involvement, which should facilitate satisfaction
a continuum of autonomy from external regula- of the needs. Autonomy-supportive environments
tion, in which children engage in activities support children’s autonomous problem-solving,
because of externally imposed contingencies action, and decision-making and take children’s
(e.g., I do my homework because I’d get in trouble perspectives and points of view (Grolnick &
if I didn’t), to identified regulation where stu- Ryan, 1989). In addition to autonomy-supportive
dents engage in activities because of a sense of contexts which support autonomy, structured
their value or importance (e.g., I do my home- environments meet the need for competence by
work because it is important for my learning). providing clear and consistent expectations, pre-
The motivational resources centering around dictable consequences for not meeting expecta-
competence concern whether children believe tions, and feedback about how to better meet
that they have control over outcomes and whether expectations in the future. Lastly, involved con-
they see themselves as capable or as incompetent texts that meet children’s need for relatedness are
in their world (Bandura, 1977; Dweck, 1991; those that provide tangible resources, such as
Skinner, 1996; Weisz, 1986). Thus, perceived attention and time, and psychological resources
control and perceived competence are motiva- such as emotional support and warmth (Grolnick
tional resources connected to the satisfaction of & Ryan, 1989). With regard to academics, these
the need for competence (Skinner et al., 1990). contexts are communicating to children that
Finally, experiences of relatedness involve indi- learning is connected, important, and relevant to
viduals feeling secure in their relationships and their personal goals, that they have the capacity
feeling worthy of love and positive regard to succeed in school, and that they belong and are
(Bretherton, 1985; Crittenden, 1990). Thus, the valued by others.
experience of relatedness can be seen in chil- Taking this perspective on engagement (see
dren’s sense of worth and security with them- Fig. 16.1 for a summary of the model), we explore
selves and others. contexts that are likely to satisfy students’ psy-
Children’s motivational resources will then chological needs and thus increase their engage-
relate to their engagement in learning. For exam- ment. We first turn to work that focuses on the
ple, if children’s school behavior is autonomously role of parents. Notably, families provide envi-
regulated, they will prefer a challenge and will ronments higher or lower on dimensions of
set goals for themselves beyond what is required involvement, autonomy support, and structure,
(Deci & Ryan, 1985). When students have a sense and these resources can affect student motivation
of who or what controls outcomes and are con- and engagement. Yet, whether families provide
vinced of their own efficacy, they will persevere these resources is not solely a function of qualities
in the face of setbacks. They will be enthusiastic, of the families themselves—the larger social con-
even when confronted with obstacles, concen- text and schools can play an active role in whether
trate on learning activities, and expend effort families are involved and how they see them-
beyond what is minimally expected (Skinner selves in terms of their roles in student learning.
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 347

Motivational
Context Engagement
Processes
Involvement Experience of Relatedness Behavior

Autonomy Support Self-Regulation Affect

Structure Perceived Control Cognition


Perceived Competence

Fig. 16.1 A motivational model of the effects of context and motivational processes on children’s engagement (From
Connell & Wellborn, 1991, p. 51, Copyright 1991 by Erlbaum. Adapted with permission)

Further, schools’ attitudes about families are in school achievement across school level and vari-
part a function of the backgrounds of families. ous ethnic and racial groups. For example, a recent
Thus, we will then turn to a discussion of families meta-analysis of 41 studies addressing the rela-
and schools as overlapping spheres of influence tions between parent involvement and the aca-
(in the tradition of Epstein). We will focus on how demic achievement of elementary students
schools and families can move beyond one-way (Jeynes, 2005) revealed an overall effect size of
and traditional interactions to more effective and .74. Such a strong effect size was seen in studies
dynamic interactions that can have important including white and African American families
implications for student engagement. and boys and girls. Nye, Turner, and Schwartz
(2006) reported results of a review of 19 studies of
parent involvement programs involving elemen-
Parent Involvement tary age children. These studies resulted in an
average effect size of .43, suggesting that children
Parent involvement in children’s schooling has of parents receiving an intervention achieved at a
been a frequent target of research and interven- level a half standard deviation higher than those
tion in the past two decades. Importantly, parent of controls. Hill and Tyson (2009) reviewed 50
involvement has been identified as a way to close studies involving middle school students. The
gaps in achievement between more and less dis- average correlation between parent involvement
advantaged children and minority and majority and achievement in middle school was .18, a
youth (e.g., Dearing, Kreider, Simpkins, & Weiss, highly significant, though low to moderate, effect.
2006; Hara, 1998). There have been a number of Finally, a second meta-analysis by Jeynes exam-
definitions of parent involvement. Grolnick and ining 52 studies of secondary school students
Slowiaczek (1994) defined involvement as the demonstrated positive effects of parent involve-
dedication of resources by the parent to the child ment on academic achievement, with an overall
in a given domain. Gonzalez-DeHass, Willems, effect size of .53 and an effect size for parent
and Holbein (2005) defined parent involvement involvement programs of .36 again across white
as “parenting behaviors directed toward chil- and minority children (Jeynes, 2007). Thus,
dren’s education” (p. 101). Hill and Taylor (2004) although somewhat stronger for younger children,
suggested that involvement is “parents’ interac- across a wide range of ages and in studies of vari-
tion with schools and with their children to pro- ous types, parent involvement appears to have a
mote academic success” (p. 1491). All these robust effect on children’s achievement.
definitions are purposefully broad and ultimately One key question researchers have addressed
include multiple components to account for the is how parent involvement impacts children’s
myriad ways in which parents are involved and in achievement. Grolnick and Slowiaczek (1994)
which involvement can affect children. suggested two models for understanding the
A growing and consistent literature supports effects of parent involvement—a direct effects
the importance of parent involvement in children’s model and an indirect or motivational model.
348 J.N. Raftery et al.

The direct effects model would suggest that activities such as volunteering at school, commu-
parent involvement in children’s schooling helps nication between parents and teachers, and
children by teaching them the academic skills involvement in school governance. Home-based
they require to do well in school. Thus, parents, strategies included helping with homework and
for example, would be increasing children’s math providing children experience with cultural activ-
and reading skills through their interactions with ities. Finally, academic socialization involved
them, particularly at home. Another model sug- activities such as communicating expectations
gests that parent involvement impacts children for the value of education, linking schoolwork to
by facilitating their motivation to engage in and current events, and fostering children’s educa-
do well in school. According to this model, when tional and occupational aspirations. The research-
parents place importance on school by discussing ers’ analyses for middle school students showed
school with students, going to the school, and no significant effects of home-based involvement
linking school topics with outside activities, chil- and a significant relation for school-based
dren themselves come to value school and involvement. However, the strongest effects on
develop the sense of competence that would achievement were for academic socialization.
enable them to put forth effort in learning activi- Jeynes (2005) showed a similar finding with par-
ties. In short, the motivational model suggests ent expectations, the degree to which parents held
that parent involvement facilitates the motiva- high expectations of students’ potential to achieve
tional resources that would enhance children’s at high levels, yielding larger effect sizes than
engagement in school. What is the evidence for parental reading, checking homework, parental
this proposition? styles, and specific types of parent involvement
While studies have generally supported the such as participation at school. It should be noted,
overall relations between parent involvement and however, that parents may have higher expecta-
children’s achievement, most have also found tions for their higher-performing students, and
different effects of various types of involvement, thus the finding for parental expectations may
and the results may well speak to the question of represent a bidirectional effect. Overall though
how involvement is related to school success. the findings from these studies suggest that par-
McWayne, Hampton, Fantuzzo, Cohen, and ent involvement may likely have its largest effects
Sekino (2004) divided parent involvement into by facilitating the attitudes and values children
supportive home learning environments, includ- need to put forth effort in school, especially for
ing behaviors such as talking about school and older students.
structuring the home environment to support Interestingly, there is some evidence that the
learning; direct involvement, including involve- effects of various types of involvement may differ
ment in school activities and direct communica- somewhat for families from different backgrounds.
tion between parents and school personnel; and Hong and Ho (2005), using data from the National
inhibited involvement, which included barriers to Education Longitudinal Survey, examined four
involvement such as time constraints and com- types of involvement in parents of eighth graders:
peting responsibilities. In 307 urban ethnic communication, which included students
minority kindergarteners, the supportive environ- discussing their school activities and plans with
ment type of involvement was positively and the their parents; parent educational aspirations for
inhibited type negatively associated with chil- their children; participation in school activities;
dren’s reading and math achievement. There were and supervision. Results showed that for white and
no significant relations for direct involvement. Asian American families, communication and
The strongest findings were for the supportive educational aspirations were most predictive of
home learning environment type of involvement. initial achievement and growth in achievement.
Hill and Tyson (2009) divided parent involve- For Asian American families, participation was
ment in the studies they reviewed into three types. also predictive. For African American families,
School-based involvement strategies included supervision predicted achievement outcomes,
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 349

while for Hispanic families, communication was involvement on engagement may differ for
key. The results underscore the importance of con- families differing in economic advantage. This
sidering family background in understanding study examined the effects of parent involve-
which involvement strategies may work best ment on children’s academic orientations in 489
(Bempechat, Graham, & Jimenez, 1999). 11–15-year-old children and mothers. Academic
Fewer studies have specifically examined orientations were defined as the degree to which
motivational outcomes of parent involvement for students are committed to school and education
students. Sanders (1998) examined 12–17-year- and to doing well. To assess this construct, stu-
old students’ perceptions of parents’ encourage- dents responded to questionnaire items such as,
ment of academic endeavors and achievement. “In general, you like school a lot,” and “You usu-
She found positive relations, over and above ally finish your homework”—items that indicate
background variables such as single-parent status greater student engagement. Parent involvement
and poverty, with academic self-concept, school (a mix of involvement at school and home and
behavior, and achievement ideology, which was socialization of positive attitudes toward school)
the students’ perceptions of the importance of was associated with children’s greater endorse-
academic achievement for future success. ment of an academic orientation in disadvantaged
Fan and Williams (2010) examined whether families. However, for nondisadvantaged fami-
various dimensions of parent involvement pre- lies, the results were in the opposite direction:
dicted tenth graders’ academic self-efficacy, greater involvement was associated with lower
school engagement, and intrinsic motivation. academic orientation. The authors suggested that
Results showed that both parents’ educational academic orientation and school performance are
aspirations for their children and school-based more tightly coupled in more advantaged chil-
involvement had strong effects on these out- dren; in other words, academic orientation may
comes. Similarly, Hong and Ho (2005) showed become more actualized. Thus, parents of advan-
the importance of parents’ educational aspira- taged children may become less involved when
tions for children’s educational aspirations, a their children are high in academic orientation
finding that did not differ across ethnic groups. because the children are doing well and do not
Marchant, Paulson, and Rothlisberg (2001) need these resources. On the other hand, parents
examined fifth and sixth grade students’ percep- of disadvantaged children may keep their involve-
tions of their parents’ value for school and aca- ment levels consistent regardless of orientation
demics as well as involvement in school activities as these resources are still necessary.
and events in relation to the importance students Grolnick and Slowiaczek (1994) directly
place on effort, ability, and grades—a composite addressed the motivational model suggesting that
that is highly related to engagement. Interestingly, parent involvement has its impact on student
while parent participation in school was not school success by facilitating motivation. These
related to motivational outcomes, children who authors examined three types of involvement—
reported that their parents valued academics had behavioral (involvement in school activities and
higher perceived competence and placed a higher events), cognitive-intellectual (exposing children
priority on academic ability, effort, and grades. to educationally stimulating activities and experi-
Consistent with the results of these studies, ences), and personal (interest in the school, ask-
Gonzalez-DeHass et al. (2005) synthesized mul- ing about the school day)—on children’s
tiple studies of relations between parent involve- motivational resources of self-regulation, per-
ment and children’s motivation and concluded ceived control, and perceived competence.
that parent involvement boosts students’ per- Results showed modest relations between moth-
ceived control and competence and helps them to ers’ behavioral and cognitive-intellectual involve-
internalize educational values. ment and students’ perceived competence and
Cooper and Crosnoe (2007) conducted an control understanding (correlations .18–.28). For
interesting study showing that the effects of fathers, there were relations between behavior and
350 J.N. Raftery et al.

cognitive intellectual involvement and students’ children toward specific ends, deny them a chance
perceived competence (correlations .15–.23) to solve problems for themselves, and ignore
Further, regression analyses supported a media- their points of view. These behaviors characterize
tional model in which parent involvement affects parental control; they hinder children’s autonomy
children’s grades by facilitating children’s per- and instead cause them to feel coerced and exter-
ceived competence and their understanding of the nally regulated (Grolnick & Ryan, 1989).
sources of control of their success and failure From an SDT perspective, a sense of auton-
outcomes, crucial motivational resources that omy and a lack of feeling controlled are crucial to
have been found to be associated with student children’s engagement in learning. This section
engagement. Marchant et al. (2001) also demon- outlines evidence that parents enhance motiva-
strated support for a mediational model in which tion, and thereby engagement, by meeting their
parent values affect student achievement by children’s need for autonomy.
facilitating students’ motivation and perceptions Research demonstrates a clear link between
of competence. parental autonomy support and a variety of out-
Overall, the research on parent involvement comes relevant to children’s success in school.
supports its importance for students’ engagement Studies support the positive relation between
in school. The consistently positive effects of parental autonomy support and children’s behav-
parent expectations, value for education, and ior regulation (Grolnick et al., 1997b), emotion
knowledge of what goes on for the child in school regulation (Ryan, Deci, Grolnick, & La Guardia,
suggest that such attitudes and behaviors help 2006), and social competence (Soenens &
children to internalize the attitudes and motiva- Vansteenkiste, 2005). Parental control, on the
tion necessary to truly engage in the school enter- other hand, relates to greater internalizing and
prise. While the level of involvement parents externalizing symptomatology (Barber, Olsen, &
provide is clearly important, we now turn to ano- Shagle, 1994; Barber, Stolz & Olsen, 2005;
ther parental resource addressing the way parents Soenens et al., 2005) and stronger propensities to
engage with their children around school—auton- engage in risky social (Goldstein, Davis-Kean, &
omy support versus control. Eccles, 2005) and health-related behaviors
(Turner, Irwin, Tschann, & Millstein, 1993).
Largely, these patterns exist across demographi-
Autonomy Support Versus Control cally (Barber, 1996; Goldstein et al., 2005) and
ethnically (Barber et al., 2005; Hill & Bush, 2001;
Parents impact student engagement not only in Hill, Bush, & Roosa, 2003) diverse samples.
terms of the extent of their involvement, but Of particular importance here is the growing
through the style with which they engage with their body of work highlighting the significance of
children around school (Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, parental autonomy support for academic motiva-
1997b; Pomerantz, Moorman, & Litwack, 2007). tion, which then fuels engagement and achieve-
A second parenting dimension, autonomy support ment in children. We present some particularly
versus control, addresses qualities of parents’ com- illustrative studies below. Grolnick and Ryan
munications and interactions that can have mean- (1989) assessed parents’ support for autonomy in
ingful consequences for children’s motivation, elementary school-age children with a structured
engagement, and academic performance. interview. Parenting that favored autonomous
When children experience themselves as problem-solving, choice, and joint decision-
agents and their actions as volitionally initiated, making over pressure, punishment, and controlling
they have a sense of autonomy. Parents foster this rewards was moderately associated with children’s
experience by encouraging their children’s initia- reports of more autonomous self-regulation—
tions and autonomous problem-solving and tak- that is, more autonomously initiated and managed
ing their perspectives. At the opposite end of this learning and achievement behaviors. Autonomy-
spectrum lie parenting practices that pressure supportive parenting was further strongly related
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 351

to teachers’ ratings of children’s competence and adjustment, IQ, and SES. Specifically, children
moderately related to classroom behavior, as well from more autonomy-supportive homes listened
as two measures of school performance: grades more attentively, set higher standards for their
and achievement test scores. work, and used downtime more productively.
Ginsburg and Bronstein (1993) also showed This focus and preference for challenge exem-
that fifth graders from more autonomy-supportive plify engaged learning. In older children, paren-
family contexts were more likely to have an tal autonomy support has been shown to serve as
intrinsic orientation toward learning—they a protective factor, buffering against the increased
engaged in mastery processes with curiosity and risk for academic and behavior problems associ-
interest, had internal standards for success, and ated with the transition to middle school
initiated schoolwork without direction. In con- (Grolnick, Kurowski, Dunlap, & Hevey, 2000).
trast, controlling parenting behaviors, such as Grolnick, Ryan, and Deci (1991) hypothesized
intrusively monitoring homework and respond- a motivational link between elementary school
ing to grades with punishment, criticism, or students’ perceptions of their parents’ autonomy
external rewards, were associated with children’s support and their achievement. Specifically, the
lower intrinsic orientation and lower academic authors examined the mediating role of children’s
performance. The strength of associations in this inner motivational resources, namely, their sense
study was moderate to low. of autonomy, perceptions of control (Connell,
Research suggests that the relations between 1985), and perceptions of competence (Harter,
parent autonomy support and student engage- 1982) in school activities. Perceived autonomy
ment hold in diverse populations (d’Ailly, 2003; support evidenced a low but significant correla-
Soenens et al., 2007; Vansteenkiste, Zhou, Lens, tion with the three motivational resources, and
& Soenens, 2005; Wang, Pomerantz, & Chen, these resources accounted for 13% of the vari-
2007). Most of the studies on this topic have ance in children’s grades, 17% of the variance in
taken place in East Asia, within cultures charac- achievement test scores, and 16% of the variance
terized by communalism and interdependence. In in teacher-rated competence. In addition, struc-
their review of this work, Pomerantz and Wang tural equation results supported the process
(2009) conclude that, despite the notion that model: perceived autonomy support predicted
aspects of East Asian culture may make children enhanced motivational resources, which in turn
more accepting of parental control, the effects of predicted achievement.
a controlling parenting style on children’s aca- Mothers’ self-reported emphasis on the plea-
demic and psychological functioning are nega- sure inherent in academic tasks and the value of
tive across both United States and East Asian learning, rather than their implementation of
cultures, though the effects sometimes appear task-extrinsic contingencies, initiates an analo-
stronger in the United States. gous process. Such “task endogeny” was associ-
These and other studies establish a consistent ated with enhanced intrinsic motivation in
picture of three related variables: autonomy sup- 9-year-olds (Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried,
port, motivation, and achievement; but what is 1994). Further, it was through this enhanced
the nature of these relations? Motivation scholars intrinsic motivation that mothers’ motivational
have employed longitudinal designs and struc- practices ultimately predicted their children’s
tural equation modeling to address this question. math and reading achievement 1 year later. This
Joussemet, Koestner, Lekes, and Landry longitudinal path model explained 8% of the
(2005) coded interviews for the style with which variance in 10-year-olds’ reading achievement
mothers communicated standards for their 5-year- and 32% of the variance in math achievement.
old children’s behavior. A more autonomy- There is also other research addressing the
supportive style was associated with children’s pathways through which parental autonomy sup-
academic adjustment and reading performance port predicts children’s motivation and achieve-
3 years later, even after controlling for initial ment. Bronstein, Ginsburg, and Herrera (2005)
352 J.N. Raftery et al.

demonstrated that mothers’ autonomy-supportive adolescents’ subsequent academic outcomes


behaviors were associated with higher concurrent above and beyond the autonomy support they
achievement in fifth graders. In turn, higher perceived from teachers and the school adminis-
achievement led to greater perceived compe- tration. In this study, parental control led ulti-
tence and greater intrinsic motivation 2 years mately to an increased likelihood of deciding to
later. The model yielded a squared multiple drop out of high school.
correlation of .60. In sum, clearly parent autonomy support con-
The compelling evidence for the positive tributes to children’s motivation. It is a critical
impact of autonomy support likely also repre- element in the bidirectional process whereby
sents a bidirectional transaction, whereby a par- autonomy support affects motivation, motivation
ent’s autonomy-supportive or controlling style is results in engagement, and engagement feeds
partly a response to her child’s level of self-regu- back to motivational processes and parenting.
lation and competence (Grolnick & Ryan, 1989). Given this ongoing transaction, facilitating par-
In Bronstein et al.’s (2005) longitudinal study, ents’ autonomy support is a worthy goal for edu-
autonomy support led to higher achievement, cators as doing so can likely have long-lasting
which in turn brought about greater autonomy and broad effects.
support. Similarly, Pomerantz and Eaton (2001)
found that fourth through sixth graders’ poor aca-
demic performance elicited heightened intrusive Parental Structure
homework checking and helping from their moth-
ers 6 months later. Two mechanisms mediated Self-determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985)
this relation: mothers’ worry over children’s low posits that in addition to providing autonomy
achievement and children’s uncertainty about support and involvement, environments need to
how to meet academic standards, which appar- provide resources that facilitate children’s com-
ently manifested in behaviors that cued parental petence. According to this theory, social contexts
assistance. facilitate competence by providing structure.
Importantly, children with more negative From an SDT framework, environments that are
competence experiences may be particularly sen- structured include clear guidelines, expectations,
sitive to autonomy-supportive versus controlling and rules as well as predictable consequences
parenting practices (Pomerantz, Wang, & Ng, and clear feedback (Farkas & Grolnick, 2010).
2005). In Ng, Kenney-Benson, and Pomerantz’s Such structure allows individuals to anticipate
(2004) laboratory study, mothers’ autonomy- outcomes and plan their behavior accordingly.
supportive behaviors during a challenging, home- Thus, homes that provide structure should give
work-like task predicted enhanced achievement, children a clear sense of how their actions are
whereas control predicted diminished engage- connected to important outcomes. When this is
ment, more for low-achieving than for high- the case, children should feel more in control of
achieving elementary school students. their successes and failures, in other words, have
Certainly, autonomy support is crucial to child a sense of perceived control (Skinner et al., 1990).
engagement. But just how important is it for chil- By contrast, when expectations, consequences
dren to experience autonomy-supportive interac- for action, and feedback are unclear or left
tions in the context of the family, as compared to unspecified, children may experience themselves
the other environments in which they grow and as unable to make outcomes happen. Feelings of
learn? Soenens and Vansteenkiste (2005) found incompetence and lack of control may undermine
that, above and beyond teacher autonomy sup- effective engagement.
port, parent autonomy support predicted adoles- Relative to involvement and autonomy sup-
cents’ self-determination and adjustment in port, there is much less work addressing the
school. Vallerand, Fortier, and Guay (1997) also structure dimension and school outcomes.
showed that parental autonomy support predicted Grolnick and Ryan (1989) interviewed parents
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 353

and coded them for level of structure, which apparent above and beyond the effects of parental
included two components: the degree to which autonomy support and involvement.
there were clear rules, expectations, and guide- In a second study of over 160 sixth grade chil-
lines in the home and the degree to which rules dren from nine schools within an urban school
and expectations were consistently enforced. district, our lab group identified three key compo-
Higher levels of parental structure were associ- nents of parental structure: clear and consistent
ated with children’s greater understanding of how guidelines and expectations, predictability of con-
to attain successes and avoid failures. Grolnick sequences, and authority. Similar to the above
and Wellborn (1988) developed a questionnaire study, children were interviewed about rules and
to assess parental structure including the predict- expectations in the home with regard to home-
ability of consequences for children’s actions and work and studying, and interviews were rated for
clarity of expectations. Children who described these aspects of structure. However, in addition to
their parents as higher in structure also reported examining the effects of the components of struc-
lower levels of maladaptive control beliefs (i.e., ture on children’s motivation and engagement, we
believing they succeeded in school because of also looked at the manner in which parents imple-
luck or powerful others) and higher perceived mented structure on the autonomy support to con-
competence. Skinner, Johnson, and Snyder trol continuum. In contrast to views that
(2005) developed parent and child reports of conceptualize structure as at odds with autonomy
parental structure and its obverse, chaos. High support, our framework specifies that these
parental structure and low parental chaos were dimensions are separable: parents can provide
associated with higher levels of perceived con- structure in a manner that supports children’s
trol, engagement in school, and self-worth. autonomy (e.g., by allowing children input about
In our recent work, we have been attempting the rules and expectations, being open to discuss-
to specify the particular components of structure ing the rules, and being empathic about children
and how they relate to motivational outcomes. not wanting to follow the rules) or in a way that
Farkas and Grolnick (2010), in particular, speci- controls children (e.g., by dictating rules and
fied six components of structure: (1) clear and expectations, prohibiting dissension and discus-
consistent guidelines, rules, and expectations; (2) sion, and providing parent-oriented rationales for
predictability of consequences for action; (3) rules) (Grolnick & Pomerantz, 2009). Thus, raters
information feedback; (4) opportunities to meet also coded the degree to which rules and expecta-
expectations; (5) provision of rationales for rules tions were implemented in an autonomy-supportive
and expectations; and (6) parental authority (i.e., or controlling manner. Autonomy-supportive
parents taking a leadership role in the home). In a versus controlling implementation of rules and
first study of these components, 75 seventh and expectations was coded for five components: (1)
eighth grade students from two large middle whether rules and expectations were jointly estab-
schools were interviewed about their homes with lished with children (or parent dictated), (2)
regard to homework and grades. From the inter- whether there was open exchange around rules
views, parents were rated on the six components and expectations, (3) parent empathy, (4) provi-
of structure. Children completed questionnaires sion of choice, and (5) provision of rationales that
about their perceptions of control and compe- would be meaningful for children’s goals.
tence and academic engagement. Schools pro- Consistent with the previous study, structure,
vided children’s grades. Structure components including more clear and consistent rules and
correlated with these outcomes—clear and con- expectations and higher parental authority, was
sistent guidelines and expectations were associ- associated with children’s lower endorsement of
ated with children feeling more in control of maladaptive control beliefs. Lower levels of
outcomes and with perceived competence, maladaptive control beliefs then led to less use
engagement in school, and grades. Further, rela- of maladaptive coping strategies for dealing
tions between structure and outcomes were with academic failure including less avoidance,
354 J.N. Raftery et al.

rumination, and blaming the teacher (Raftery & The idea of lack of separation between families
Grolnick, 2010). The degree to which parents and schools is consistent with the new work on
implemented structure in an autonomy-supportive family-school partnerships. This model moves
manner was also associated with children’s beyond families and schools as separate influ-
competence. In particular, greater use of empa- ences to see them as partners with shared respon-
thy, provision of choice, and provision of mean- sibility for ensuring the success of students.
ingful rationales were associated with lower Christenson and Sheridan (2001) defined school-
endorsement of maladaptive control. Further, family partnerships as “[developing an] inten-
when parents implemented structure in a more tional and ongoing relationship between school
autonomy-supportive manner, children reported and family designed to directly or indirectly
feeling more competent in school, were more enhance children’s learning and development,
engaged in the classroom, and attained better and/or to address the obstacles that impede it”
grades. Though these correlations were low to (p. 38). They further suggested that family-school
moderate, generally, in the .2 range, given the partnerships are characterized by: (1) a student
myriad of factors that can effect children’s moti- focus, (2) a belief that families and schools share
vation, engagement, and achievement, the results responsibility for student success and are both
are worthy of attention, particularly since they essential to it, (3) an emphasis on families and
are so focused on concrete behaviors parents can schools working together, and (4) a preventive
engage in to facilitate their children’s motivation solution-focused approach where families and
and engagement. schools strive to create conditions that will facili-
The results of studies of structure indicate the tate learning. This theory of partnership entails a
importance of parents providing rules and expec- deeper understanding of families and the poten-
tations for children. Such rules and expectations tial barriers that some families, including those of
help children to develop the competence and low income and minority status, may face in ini-
confidence they need to channel their efforts in tiating or maintaining this school-home connec-
school. However, it is also important for parents tion. In addition, true partnerships not only
to institute and enforce rules and expectations in recognize these potential barriers but address
a way that supports children’s agency and respects them. In particular, there are many ways that
their roles as active participants in their school schools can work to help families remain involved
activities. in light of barriers by using an ecological
approach, taking into consideration the needs and
resources of the community, and by seeking to
Supporting Families to Facilitate understand parents’ unique needs and views of
Student Engagement the education system (Rafaele & Knoff, 1999).
This way of thinking about connections among
Substantial research points to parental involve- families and schools is best exemplified in
ment, autonomy support, and structure as key Epstein’s (1990) work in which she identified six
elements in facilitating student engagement. Yet, types of parent involvement that should be
most research on parenting has treated parents as included in any comprehensive family-school
an influence separate from that of schools. partnership program. The first type of involve-
However, researchers are recognizing that ment refers to basic responsibilities of families.
whether families provide facilitative resources is This includes obligations such as providing ade-
not solely a function of qualities of the families quate housing; ensuring the child’s health, safety,
themselves; schools play an active role in whether and general well-being; and providing a home
and how families are involved in student learning. environment that supports children’s learning
Thus, a key direction in research is understanding across grade levels. A second type of involve-
what schools can do to help families influence ment refers to home-school communication.
student engagement. Epstein suggested that it is important for parents
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 355

and schools to communicate about children’s common language in which to communicate and
progress and school programs through the use discuss issues, these families may feel isolated
of conferences, notices, memos, or report cards. from the school system or may not be aware of
A third type, parent involvement at the school, their important role in their children’s learning.
includes parent volunteering in the classroom or To help parents be involved, it is important for
for other in-school activities. Parent involvement schools to understand what it is that makes lower
at the school also includes parent attendance at class and minority families less involved. In her
school-wide events such as student sports games work on social class and parent-school relation-
or performances. Epstein labeled the fourth type ships, for example, Lareau (1987) suggested that
involvement in learning activities at home. This there are differences between working-class and
refers to parents’ role in monitoring or helping middle-class families in cultural, economic, and
children complete homework assignments or social resources that influence parent involve-
other take-home learning activities. Fifth, parents ment. For example, during qualitative interviews,
can also take on roles in advocacy, governance, many working-class parents reported doubting
and decision-making. This type of involvement their ability to help their children in academic
includes parents’ participation on committees at activities because of their limited formal educa-
the school, district, or even state level such as the tion. Further, many working-class parents viewed
PTA/PTO, Advisory Councils, or Advocacy educators as possessing superior education skills
Groups. Lastly, the sixth type of involvement and prestige and thus felt they should defer to
refers to family collaboration with community these professionals the responsibility of educat-
stakeholders, such as agencies, businesses, or ing their children. Lareau also addressed how dif-
other groups in the community. ferences in financial resources have implications
Epstein (1990) suggested that these six parent for school involvement. Many working-class
involvement practices are not responsibilities of families have limited time and disposable income,
parents alone but that there are many things that making parent involvement a challenge. For
schools can do to help parents take a more active example, many working-class parents reported
role in their children’s education. Before provid- difficulty in even attending school events because
ing more detail about what explicitly schools can they lacked transportation, childcare arrange-
do to help families provide these facilitative ments, flexible work hours, and other resources
resources to their children, it is important to iden- more readily available to middle-class families.
tify some of the challenges of involving families. In more recent work, Horvat et al. (2003) have
In the true spirit of partnerships, schools will be focused on parental networks as one type of social
most effective in involving families if they capital that influences the relationship between
address realistic barriers that many families face families and schools. The researchers found that
in becoming and remaining involved. whereas social networks of working-class and
poor families are bounded by kinship, middle-
class families form social networks that include
Challenges to Involving Families parents of their children’s peers and are much
more likely to include a variety of professionals.
Low-income and less educated parents have been These network differences were found to parallel
found to be less involved in their children’s class differences in how parents interact with
schooling than parents with a higher education schools. In particular, middle-class families were
level and income (Grolnick, Benjet, Kurowski, & more likely to rely on their social networks when
Apostoleris, 1997a; Horvat, Weininger, & Lareau, problems arose with their child in school or to cus-
2003; Lareau, 1987). Other research suggests tomize their children’s learning and educational
that language may be a barrier to involvement as experience. For instance, these families used their
minority families are less involved in school networks to obtain additional resources for a child
activities (Epstein, 1990; Pena, 2000). Without a with a learning disability or to challenge a child’s
356 J.N. Raftery et al.

placement decision. In contrast, working-class becoming involved or sustaining involvement


and poor parents were less likely to use their social and consider innovative ways to overcome such
networks and often responded to school problems barriers. We now turn to research addressing the
in an individualized fashion. These families were ways in which schools can affect parents’ provi-
also less likely to dispute school decisions and the sion of facilitative resources, with a particular
authority they perceived the school to have. focus on how to address some of the logistical,
In a comprehensive study identifying chal- structural, and interpersonal barriers preventing
lenges to parent involvement, Grolnick et al. caregiver involvement.
(1997a) examined individual, contextual, and
institutional factors that might affect parent
involvement. At the individual level, the authors What Can Schools Do?
examined parent characteristics (i.e., thoughts
and beliefs about their role in their children’s There has been an abundance of research that
learning and perceived efficacy) and child char- suggests that schools are salient contexts that
acteristics (i.e., temperament). At the contextual influence the kinds of support parents may pro-
level, they assessed stressful life events and sup- vide for their children’s academic engagement.
port for the family. The institutional context, that Much of the research in this area has focused on
is, teachers’ attitudes and practices toward par- how exogenous characteristics of schools and
ents, will be discussed in the next section. In a teachers (e.g., sociodemographic variables,
sample of 209 mothers and their third to fifth school compositional and structural characteris-
grade children, the authors found that factors tics, and school resources; Stone, 2006) influence
from both individual and contextual levels were parents’ involvement in their children’s school-
important. In particular, parents who believed ing. Researchers have found that teachers from
more strongly that their role was to be a teacher schools with high proportions of low-income and
and those who felt efficacious were more minority students reported poorer relationships
involved. Further, mothers who rated their chil- with parents than those from schools with few
dren as more difficult were less involved in per- minority students (Metropolitan Life Survey of
sonal and cognitive activities. In addition, mothers the American Teacher, 2001). Likewise, Gardner,
who experienced a more difficult context includ- Riblatt, and Beatty (2000) found that small high
ing frequent stressful life events and stretched schools had higher parent involvement than
economic resources and those who reported little larger schools. Others have recognized grade
social support tended to be less involved person- level as an important factor, finding decreases in
ally and in cognitive activities. Interestingly, the parent involvement with increases in grade level,
effects of a difficult context were stronger for perhaps due to the greater bureaucracy of sec-
mothers of boys than mothers of girls. Grolnick ondary schools (Grolnick et al., 2000; Stevenson
and her colleagues (1997a) suggested that moth- & Baker, 1987). However, research that moves
ers may perceive their sons as more independent beyond these exogenous factors and focuses on
than daughters. Thus, in difficult contexts, moth- what teachers and schools can actually do to
ers may be more likely to withdraw resources facilitate more involvement may be more crucial
from their sons, whereas they may be more likely in understanding how to facilitate student
to remain involved for daughters who they per- engagement.
ceive as more needy of support.
When thinking about what schools can do to
help parents influence student engagement, it Teacher Beliefs and Practices
will be important to consider these challenges
in involving families. In particular, schools’ There is some evidence that teachers’ attitudes
efforts to involve families must take into account and practices impact parent involvement. Epstein
the barriers that families may experience in and Becker (1982) found that teachers widely
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 357

vary in their beliefs about the utility of involving activities from the teacher, believed they should
parents in their student’s education. Some teach- help their children more at home, felt that they
ers were positive about parent involvement and had an increased understanding about what their
believed that efforts to involve parents are essen- child was learning in school, and judged their
tial. In particular, these teachers reported that children’s teacher higher in overall teaching abil-
parents are interested and willing to be involved ity and interpersonal skills. These results suggest
in their children’s education and can be helpful that teachers’ attitudes are important in influenc-
resources to educators if they are shown how to ing parents’ self-efficacy around school involve-
help their children. Epstein (1986) also found ment and ultimately parental behaviors that have
that many teachers see the value of school-family been found most crucial to facilitating academic
cooperation and believe the two contexts should motivation and engagement.
share responsibilities, particularly given their Patrikakou and Weissberg (2000) explored the
shared goals and investment in students. relationship between teacher outreach practices
Other teachers, discouraged by unsuccessful and parent involvement at home and school to
attempts to involve parents, reported concerns understand ways in which schools can help par-
about the likely success of parent involvement ents support their children’s academic efforts.
practices. In particular, these teachers expressed The researchers found that parents’ perceptions
concern that involving parents may cause undue of teacher outreach were the strongest predictor
stress on parents and their children. These teach- of parent involvement, even after controlling for
ers also reported that some parents are less will- background characteristics (e.g., parental educa-
ing or able to be involved in their children’s tion level, parent employment status, child age,
education, identifying parents of older students, gender, or race). For example, the more parents
parents with little formal education, working par- reported that teachers encouraged them to visit
ents, and single parents as having unique barriers the school (Epstein’s third type of parent involve-
that challenge the utility and effectiveness of par- ment; Epstein, 1990), the more likely they were
ent involvement practices. Namely, these teach- to participate in a variety of school activities.
ers saw efforts to involve parents as being Similarly, when parents perceived that teachers
overwhelmingly time-consuming and “not worth were keeping them informed about their child’s
the trouble” (p. 104). Further, Epstein (1986) progress and school programs through the use of
suggests that some teachers believe that there is phone calls, notes, home-school journals, etc.
competition, conflict, and incompatibility (Epstein’s second type of involvement; Epstein,
between families and schools and that parents 1990), they were more involved in their chil-
and teachers should fulfill their roles indepen- dren’s schooling. In another study on teachers’
dently. Some teachers may in fact believe that efforts to initiate and maintain contact with par-
their professional status may be undermined if ents, Ames (1993) found that teacher communi-
parents become involved in academic activities cation to parents about student progress, in-school
generally considered to be responsibilities of learning activities, and feedback to parents on
educators. how to best help their children with learning
Researchers have found that teachers’ atti- activities at home influenced parents’ feelings of
tudes, beliefs, and practices toward parent involve- comfort with the school. Further, such feelings of
ment impact parental behavior and thus the extent comfort were found to be associated with paren-
to which they provide facilitative resources for tal involvement.
their children. For instance, Epstein (1986) iden- In the previously described study, Grolnick
tified teachers who strongly supported parent and colleagues (1997a) also examined teacher
involvement and were considered “leaders” in attitudes and practices to understand how schools
using parent involvement practices. Parents whose may facilitate or undermine parent involvement
children’s teachers were “leaders” reported that efforts. In particular, teachers reported on the
they received the most ideas for home learning extent to which they believed parent involvement
358 J.N. Raftery et al.

was important and the frequency with which they and assess progress toward meeting these goals.
used various practices to involve parents. Encouraging parents to join these “action teams”
Teachers’ practices to solicit involvement were is one way in which schools may help increase
found to predict parent involvement, but only parent involvement in governance and advocacy
when other contexts (e.g., parental attitudes and as in Epstein’s (1990) fifth type of parent involve-
social context) were optimal. For example, par- ment. In a longitudinal study of schools in the
ents who experienced more optimal contexts, felt NNPS, Sheldon and Van Voorhis (2004) examined
efficacious, and viewed their role as that of a whether the quality of schools’ partnership pro-
teacher became more involved when teachers grams predicted family involvement. They found
used more parent involvement practices. However, that in schools with higher quality partnership pro-
parents who reported experiencing more difficult grams, higher percentages of parents volunteered
contexts, did not feel efficacious, and did not in school, were involved on school councils and
believe they had a role in the teaching-learning committees, and were involved in school-assigned
process were less affected by teacher beliefs or homework. Epstein (2005) found further support
practices. Grolnick et al. (1997a) speculated that for the importance of school-family partnerships
parents in the most difficult contexts or those in increasing parental involvement. In one partner-
whose beliefs clash with the attitudes and values ship school, school climate changed from one in
of the teachers may not receive the teachers’ mes- which teachers worked alone to an environment in
sages or be able to benefit from them, even with which educators and parents collaborated to reach
active attempts to involve families. Given these agreed-upon goals. During this transition, the
findings, it seems important for schools to think school implemented activities that encouraged
creatively, beyond traditional classroom-based communication and exchange of information
individual teacher efforts to involve parents, to be between parents and teachers, which increased
most successful in targeting these harder to reach family involvement. These results suggest that
families. when schools devote attention and resources to
family-school relationships, parents are likely to
be better able to support their children’s academic
School Interventions to Increase endeavors.
Family Involvement Epstein and Van Voorhis (2001) also found
that system-wide efforts to improve family-
Certainly there is a need for schools to think cre- school relationships may impact parenting behav-
atively about how to involve traditionally hard to ior. In particular, these researchers reviewed the
reach families, and we present some examples of Teachers Involve Parents in Schoolwork (TIPS)
systematic approaches below. Less work has spe- program, an innovative approach to homework in
cifically examined ways in which schools system- which teachers assign homework that requires
atically organize and implement programs to students to interact with family members. Epstein
increase connection and collaboration with fami- and Van Voorhis found that in classrooms using
lies. However, one approach to developing, main- the TIPS program, more families were involved
taining, and improving school-family relationships in their children’s education, even during middle
that has received some attention is the National school when parents are traditionally less
Network of Partnership Schools (NNPS; Sanders involved in school activities (Grolnick et al.,
& Epstein, 2000). Schools that are members of the 2000; Stevenson & Baker, 1987). The research-
NNPS receive materials, tools, and training on ers also found that family socioeconomic status
how to foster family-school relationships and cre- did not predict whether the TIPS design was
ate “action teams,” (p.128) made up of school, effective in involving parents. Thus, the TIPS
family, and community stakeholders. These design may be one creative and effective way to
“action teams” generate specific goals for imple- support Epstein’s (1990) fourth type of involve-
menting more effective family-school partnerships ment, parent involvement in learning activities
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 359

at home. In addition, this program may be high-pressure condition as more controlling in


particularly useful for reaching families who are their nonverbal behavior and in their practices
often less involved in learning activities given than controlling mothers in the low-pressure con-
limited social, cultural, and economic resources. dition or autonomy-supportive mothers in either
Other more systemic school practices may of the two manipulation groups. Autonomy-
include organizing workshops for families to help supportive mothers, however, did not appear
them fulfill their basic home obligations, as in influenced by the pressure manipulation. The
Epstein’s first type of involvement, and future findings underscore the importance of individual
research should address the effectiveness of this differences and situational factors, such as pres-
and other school efforts to increase parent sure, in predicting autonomy-supportive versus
involvement. controlling parental practices.
While Grolnick et al. (2002) looked at in-the-
moment pressure that parents experienced in the
Schools and Parent Autonomy laboratory, another type of pressure that may be
Support particularly salient for parents is their perception
of threat in society. Gurland and Grolnick (2003)
Research suggests that parental autonomy support found that mothers who perceived the world
is crucial for engagement and is associated with as threatening (a world in which there is little
children’s self-regulation, motivation, and achieve- predictability, a lack of security, and limited
ment in the classroom. Thus, it is important to resources) endorsed more controlling attitudes
consider whether there are things schools can do and values and used more controlling behaviors
to help parents provide more autonomy support. with their children compared to mothers who
Though there is no specific research on this ques- perceived less threat in their child’s current and
tion, work on why parents provide more control future environment. The researchers interpreted
can identify areas of possible intervention. these findings as suggesting that mothers who
There has been some work suggesting that the perceive threat in the world may try to ensure
pressure parents experience may predict their outcomes by over-directing their children’s
autonomy-supportive and controlling practices. behavior and problem-solving for them. Although
For example, Grolnick, Gurland, DeCourcey, and well intentioned, this may actually undermine
Jacob (2002) examined the role of evaluative children’s own autonomous self-regulation and
pressure in influencing mothers’ behavior while motivation.
they worked on tasks in the laboratory with their Findings that pressure influences parents’ pro-
children. Mother-child dyads were asked to com- vision of autonomy support have important impli-
plete two homework-like tasks, a map task and a cations for ways in which school-home
poem task. Dyads were assigned to either a high- partnerships are designed. For example, several
pressure condition, in which the experimenter programs have been developed that encourage
told them their child would be evaluated and that parents to be involved in their children’s home-
there were performance standards, or a low- work. It seems particularly necessary for schools
pressure condition in which there was no men- and teachers to clarify this role for parents, stress-
tion of performance standards. Children also ing that these homework assignments are not
completed a questionnaire which assessed the evaluative, but instead are used to provide infor-
extent to which they perceived their mother as mation to the teacher about children’s progress. In
being autonomy-supportive or controlling. On this way, parents will not feel pressured for their
the poem task, mothers in the high-pressure con- child to meet performance standards or feel com-
dition used more controlling practices with their pelled to “take over” when their child is struggling
children, whether or not their style was more to ensure achievement. This seems particularly
autonomy-supportive or controlling. On the map important in light of Pomerantz and Eaton’s (2001)
task, observers rated controlling mothers in the finding that mothers of low-achieving children
360 J.N. Raftery et al.

were more likely to use intrusive practices such as to make contact and interact with families.
checking or helping their child with homework Hoover-Dempsey, Bassler, and Brissie (1992)
without the child’s request. suggested that the correlation they uncovered
between parent involvement and teacher self-
efficacy could indicate that teachers with
Summary and Future Directions higher self-efficacy are more likely to involve
parents or that when parents are involved in
Student engagement is a key outcome for stu- the classroom, teachers actually develop
dents as it is through active participation that stu- higher levels of teaching self-efficacy.
dents best learn. The description of Mr. Jones’ Certainly more research is needed that consid-
sixth grade class at the beginning of the chapter ers family-school relationships in more bidi-
illustrates what this engagement can look like, rectional and transactional frameworks.
particularly in the context of family involvement. 2. Just as the investigation into environmental
Our examination of families, students, and influences on parent involvement has proven
schools suggests the important role that families immensely productive, research on the school
play in facilitating student engagement. In par- and community factors that promote parent
ticular, our chapter focuses on the important con- autonomy support and structure would likely
tributions of parental autonomy support, make valuable contributions to our under-
involvement, and structure to student engage- standing of student engagement in context.
ment. It also illustrates the challenges families What demographic characteristics, teacher
face in providing resources to children and the attitudes, and school programming affect
crucial role that schools can play in fostering par- autonomy support and structure? And how do
ent engagement. Through their attitudes and these factors translate into student engage-
practices, schools can help families provide facil- ment and achievement outcomes? Grolnick
itative contexts for children. At the same time, and colleagues provide some preliminary
parent involvement, autonomy support, and answers with their work on evaluative pres-
structure at home can boost schools’ missions to sure and threat; however, there is much more
develop engaged, committed learners. Clearly, it to learn. Further research can inform our
is only through an active partnership that the development of home-school partnerships
optimal context for student engagement is such that they promote more optimally facili-
possible. tative family contexts for children.
We end with some future directions for 3. Another key direction for future research is to
research: focus on the needs and circumstances of tradi-
1. While there has been work, much of which we tionally hard to reach families. As was seen in
have reviewed in this chapter, that has explored the Grolnick et al. (1997a) study, teacher
how schools can help caregivers provide facil- efforts to involve families may not reach
itative resources to their children, future stressed families or those with limited
research needs to address the bidirectionality resources, as barriers such as time, transporta-
of these home-school relations. There has tion, and language may interfere. Further,
been some evidence suggesting that while teachers’ and parents’ assumptions and atti-
schools affect parent behavior, parents can tudes about parents’ roles in student learning
also have a powerful influence on teacher may not coincide (Christenson, 2004). Finally,
behaviors and school climate. For instance, research on social capital (Horvat et al., 2003)
Epstein and Dauber (1991) found that teach- suggests that families with limited economic
ers who believed parents shared their beliefs resources may not have the same interpersonal
about the importance of a strong home-school resources, such as network ties connecting
connection had more positive attitudes about parents of school peers, which are key in helping
involving parents and made more active efforts parents effectively intervene in school issues.
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 361

Such findings point to the need for schools to Barber, B. K., Olsen, J. E., & Shagle, S. C. (1994).
identify and overcome these logistical, struc- Associations between parental psychological and
behavioral control and youth internalized and external-
tural, and interpersonal barriers through ized behaviors. Child Development, 65, 1120–1136.
active communication between families and Barber, B. K., Stolz, H. E., & Olsen, J. E. (2005). Parental
schools and innovative approaches such as support, psychological control, and behavioral con-
flexible meeting times (e.g., alternating eve- trol: Assessing the relevance across time, method, and
culture. Monographs of the Society for Research in
ning and morning activities) and provision of Child Development, 70(70), 1–137.
transportation, babysitting, and translation Bempechat, J., Graham, S. E., & Jimenez, N. V. (1999).
services. The socialization of achievement in poor and minority
students: A comparative study. Journal of Cross-
4. Finally, our review suggests the need for more
Cultural Psychology, 30, 139–158.
research on interventions targeting families. Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and
More information is needed on the kinds of prospect. Monographs of the Society for Research in
programs that are most effective in increasing Child Development, 50, 3–35.
Bronstein, P., Ginsburg, G. S., & Herrera, I. S. (2005).
positive interactions between families and
Parental predictors of motivational orientation in early
schools. Further, as suggested by Christenson adolescence: A longitudinal study. Journal of Youth
and Carlson (2005), when successful programs and Adolescence, 34, 559–575.
are identified, research addressing the key fea- Christenson, S. L. (2004). The family-school partnership:
An opportunity to promote the learning competence of
tures responsible for such success and the pro-
all students. School Psychology Review, 33, 83–104.
cesses through which they ultimately affect Christenson, S. L., & Sheridan, S. M. (2001). Schools and
student engagement is crucial. Isolating the families: Creating essential connections for learning.
active ingredients of effective approaches can New York: Guilford Press.
Connell, J. P. (1985). A new multidimensional measure of
provide crucial information to schools attempt-
children’s perceptions of control. Child Development,
ing to create active partnerships. 56, 1018–1041.
The engagement literature to date covers tre- Connell, J. P., Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Clifford, E.,
mendous ground in illuminating the complex Crichlow, W., & Usinger, P. (1995). Hanging in there:
Behavioral, psychological, and contextual factors
relations among families, schools, and children, affecting whether African-American adolescents stay
but there is still much more to learn. Fascinating in high school. Journal of Adolescent Research, 10,
research questions abound regarding how these 41–63.
transactions ultimately impact students and how Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994).
Educational risk and resilience in African-American
to apply this knowledge toward a worthy goal of youth: Context, self-action, and outcomes in school.
parents, educators, and community members Child Development, 65, 493–506.
alike, fostering motivation, engagement, and love Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence,
of learning in future generations. autonomy and relatedness: A motivational analysis of
self-system processes. In M. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe
(Eds.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology:
Vol. 23. Self processes in development (pp. 43–77).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
References Cooper, C. E., & Crosnoe, R. (2007). The engagement in
schooling of economically disadvantaged parents and
Ames, C. (1993). How school-to-home communications children. Youth and Society, 38, 372–391.
influence parent beliefs and perceptions. Equity and Christenson, S.L., & Carlson, C. (2005). Evidence-based
Choice, 9(3), 44–49. parent and family interventions in school psychology:
Appleton, J., Christenson, S., & Furlong, M. (2008). State of scientifically based practice. School
Student engagement with school: Critical conceptual Psychology Quarterly, 20, 525–528.
and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology Crittenden, P. M. (1990). Internal representational models
in the Schools, 45(5), 369–386. of attachment relationships. Infant Mental Health
Bandura, A. (1977). Self efficacy: Toward a unified theory Journal, 11, 259–277.
of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, d’Ailly, H. (2003). Children’s autonomy and perceived
191–215. control in learning: A model of motivation and achieve-
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: ment in Taiwan. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, 95, 84–96.
67, 3296–3319.
362 J.N. Raftery et al.

Dearing, E., Kreider, H., Simpkins, S., & Weiss, H. B. problem behavior. Developmental Psychology, 41,
(2006). Family involvement in school and low-income 401–413.
children’s literacy: Longitudinal association between Gonzalez-DeHass, A., Willems, P., & Holbein, M. (2005).
and within families. Journal of Educational Examining the relationship between parental involve-
Psychology, 98, 653–664. ment and student motivation. Educational Psychology
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation Review, 17(2), 99–123.
and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Goodenow, C. (1993). The psychological sense of school
Plenum Press. membership among adolescents: Scale development
Dweck, C. S. (1991). Self theories and goals: Their role in and educational correlates. Psychology in the Schools,
motivation, personality, and development. In R. 30, 79–90.
Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994).
Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Role of parental motivational practices in children’s
Epstein, J. L. (1986). Parents’ reactions to teacher prac- academic intrinsic motivation and achievement.
tices of parent involvement. The Elementary School Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 104–113.
Journal, 86, 277–294. Grolnick, W. S., Benjet, C., Kurowski, C. O., &
Epstein, J. L. (1990). School and family connections: Apostoleris, N. (1997a). Predictors of parent involve-
Theory, research, and implications for integrating ment in children’s schooling. Journal of Educational
sociologies of education and family. Marriage and Psychology, 89, 538–548.
Family Review, 15, 99–126. Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997b).
Epstein, J. L. (2005). A case study of the partnership Internalization within the family: The self-determina-
schools comprehensive school reform (CSR) model. tion theory perspective. In J. Grusec & L. Kuczynski
The Elementary School Journal, 106, 151–170. (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internalization of val-
Epstein, J. L., & Becker, H. J. (1982). Teachers’ reported ues: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 135–
practices of parent involvement: Problems and possi- 161). New York: Wiley.
bilities. The Elementary School Journal, 83, 103–113. Grolnick, W. S., Gurland, S., DeCourcey, W., & Jacob, K.
Epstein, J. L., & Dauber, S. L. (1991). School programs (2002). Antecedents and consequences of mothers’
and teacher practices of parent involvement in inner autonomy support. An empirical investigation.
city elementary and middle schools. The Elementary Developmental Psychology, 38, 143–155.
School Journal, 91, 289–303. Grolnick, W. S., Kurowski, C. O., Dunlap, K. G., & Hevey,
Epstein, J. L., & Van Voorhis, F. L. (2001). More than C. (2000). Parental resources and the transition to
minutes: Teachers’ roles in designing homework. junior high. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10,
Educational Psychologist, 36, 181–193. 465–488.
Fan, W., & Williams, C. M. (2010). The effects of parental Grolnick, W., & Pomerantz, E. (2009). Issues and chal-
involvement on students’ academic self-efficacy, lenges in studying parental control: Toward a new
engagement and intrinsic motivation. Educational conceptualization. Child Development Perspectives,
Psychology, 30, 53–74. 3, 165–170.
Farkas, M. S., & Grolnick, W. S. (2010). Examining the Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles asso-
components and concomitants of parental structure in ciated with children’s self-regulation and competence
the academic domain. Motivation and Emotion, 34, in school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81,
266–279. 143–154.
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. resources for school achievement: Motivational medi-
Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success ators of children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal
among students at risk for school failure. Journal of of Educational Psychology, 83, 508–517.
Applied Psychology, 82, 221–234. Grolnick, W. S., & Slowiaczek, M. L. (1994). Parents’
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). involvement in children’s schooling: A multidimen-
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of sional conceptualization and motivational model.
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, Child Development, 65, 237–252.
59–109. Grolnick, W. S, & Wellborn, J. (1988). Parent influences
Gardner, P., Riblatt, S., & Beatty, N. (2000). Academic on children’s school-related self-system process. Paper
achievement and parental involvement as a function of presented at the annual meeting of the American
school size. High School Journal, 83, 21–27. Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
Ginsburg, G. S., & Bronstein, P. (1993). Family factors Gurland, S. T., & Grolnick, W. S. (2003). Perceived threat,
related to children’s intrinsic/extrinsic motivational controlling parenting, and children’s achievement ori-
orientation and academic performance. Child entations. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 103–121.
Development, 64, 1461–1474. Hara, S. R. (1998). Parent involvement: The key to
Goldstein, S. E., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2005). improved student achievement. School Community
Parents, peers, and problem behavior: A longitudinal Journal, 8(2), 9–19.
investigation of the impact of relationship perceptions Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for
and characteristics on the development of adolescent children. Child Development, 53, 87–97.
16 Families as Facilitators of Student Engagement: Toward a Home-School Partnership Model 363

Hill, N. E., & Bush, K. R. (2001). Relationships between Metropolitan Life Survey of the American Teacher.
parenting environment and children’s mental health (2001). Key elements of quality schools. http://www.
among African American and European American metlife.com/Companyinfo/Community/Found/
mothers and children. Journal of Marriage and Family, Docs/2001ats.pdf
63, 954–966. Murdock, T. B. (1999). The social context of risk: Status
Hill, N. E., Bush, K. R., & Roosa, M. W. (2003). Parenting and motivational predictors of alienation in middle
and family socialization strategies and children’s school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91,
mental health: Low-income Mexican-American and 62–76.
Euro-American mothers and children. Child Develop- Natriello, G. (1984). Problems in the evaluation of stu-
ment, 74, 189–204. dents and student from secondary schools. Journal of
Hill, N. E., & Taylor, L. C. (2004). Parental school Research and Development in Education, 17, 14–24.
involvement and children’s academic achievement: Ng, F. F., Kenney-Benson, G. A., & Pomerantz, E. M.
Pragmatics and issues. Current Directions in (2004). Children’s achievement moderates the effects
Psychological Science, 13, 161–164. of mothers’ use of control and autonomy support.
Hill, N. E., & Tyson, D. F. (2009). Parental involvement in Child Development, 75, 764–780.
middle school: A meta-analytic assessment of the Nye, C., Turner, H., & Schwartz, J. (2006). Approaches to
strategies that promote achievement. Developmental parent involvement for improving the academic per-
Psychology, 45(3), 740–763. formance of elementary school age children. Oslo,
Hong, S., & Ho, H. (2005). Direct and indirect longitudi- Norway: The Campbell Collaboration, Campbell
nal effects of parental involvement on student achieve- Systematic Reviews.
ment: Second order latent growth modeling across Patrikakou, E. N., & Weissberg, R. P. (2000). Parents’ per-
ethnic groups. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, ceptions of teacher outreach and parent involvement
32–42. in children’s education. Journal of Prevention &
Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Bassler, O. C., & Brissie, J. S. Intervention in the Community, 20, 103–119.
(1992). Explorations in parent-school relations. The Pena, D. C. (2000). Parent involvement: Influencing fac-
Journal of Educational Research, 85, 287–294. tors and implications. The Journal of Educational
Horvat, E. M., Weininger, E. B., & Lareau, A. (2003). From Research, 94, 42–54.
social ties to social capital: Class differences in the rela- Pomerantz, E. M., & Eaton, M. M. (2001). Maternal intru-
tions between schools and parent networks. American sive support in the academic context: Transactional
Educational Research Journal, 40, 319–351. socialization processes. Developmental Psychology,
Jeynes, W. H. (2005). A meta-analysis of the relation of 37, 174–186.
parental involvement to urban elementary school stu- Pomerantz, E. M., Moorman, E. A., & Litwack, S. D.
dent academic achievement. Urban Education, 40, (2007). The how, whom and why of parents’ involve-
237–269. ment in children’s academic lives: More is not always
Jeynes, W. H. (2007). The relationship between parental better. Review of Educational Research, 77, 373–410.
involvement and urban secondary school student aca- Pomerantz, E. M., & Wang, Q. (2009). The role of paren-
demic achievement: A meta-analysis. Urban tal control in children’s development in Western and
Education, 42, 82–110. East Asian countries. Current Directions in
Joussemet, M., Koestner, R., Lekes, N., & Landry, R. Psychological Science, 18, 285–289.
(2005). A longitudinal study of the relationship of Pomerantz, E. M., Wang, Q., & Ng, F. F. (2005). The role
maternal autonomy support to children’s adjustment of children’s competence experiences in the socializa-
and achievement in school. Journal of Personality, 73, tion process: A dynamic process framework for the
1216–1235. academic arena. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child
Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships mat- development and behavior (Vol. 33, pp. 193–227). San
ter: Linking teacher support to student engagement Diego, CA: Academic.
and achievement. Journal of School Health, 74, Rafaele, L. M., & Knoff, H. M. (1999). Improving home-
262–273. school collaboration with disadvantaged families:
Lareau, A. (1987). Social class differences in family – Organizational principles, perspectives, and
School relationships: The importance of cultural capi- approaches. School Psychology Review, 28, 448–466.
tal. Sociology of Education, 60, 73–85. Raftery, J. R., & Grolnick, W. S. (2010, May). Coping
Marchant, G. J., Paulson, S. E., & Rothlisberg, B. A. with academic failure: Effects of structure and per-
(2001). Relations of middle school students’ percep- ceived control. Fourth International Conference on
tions of family and school contexts with academic Self-Determination Theory, Gent, Belgium.
achievement. Psychology in the Schools, 38, Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of
505–519. causality and internalization: Examining reasons for
McWayne, C., Hampton, V., Fantuzzo, J., Cohen, H. L., & acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and
Sekino, Y. (2004). A multivariate examination of parent Social Psychology, 57, 749–761.
involvement and the social and academic competen- Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Grolnick, W. S. (1992).
cies of urban kindergarten. Psychology in the Schools, When achievement is not intrinsically motivated: A
41, 363–377. theory and assessment of self-regulation in school. In
364 J.N. Raftery et al.

A. K. Boggiano & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Achievement Soenens, B., Elliot, A. J., Goossens, L., Vansteenkiste,
and motivation: A social-developmental perspective M., Luyten, P., & Duriez, B. (2005). The intergenera-
(pp. 167–188). New York: Cambridge University tional transmission of perfectionism: Parents’ psycho-
Press. logical control as an intervening variable. Journal of
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., Grolnick, W. S., & La Guardia, Family Psychology, 19, 358–366.
J. G. (2006). The significance of autonomy and auton- Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Antecedents
omy support in psychological development and psy- and outcomes of self-determination in three life
chopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), domains: The role of parents’ and teachers’ autonomy
Developmental psychopathology: Theory and method support. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34,
(2nd ed., pp. 795–849). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 589–604.
Sanders, M. G. (1998). The effects of school, family, and Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., Luyckx, K.,
community support on the academic achievement of Goossens, L., Beyers, W., et al. (2007). Conceptualizing
African American adolescents. Urban Education, 33, parental autonomy support: Adolescent perceptions of
385–409. promotion of independence versus promotion of voli-
Sanders, M. G., & Epstein, J. L. (2000). The National tional functioning. Developmental Psychology, 43,
Network of Partnership Schools: How research influ- 633–646.
ences educational practice. Journal of Education for Southwest Educational Developmental Laboratory. (2002).
Students Placed at Risk, 5, 61–76. A new wave of evidence: The impact of school, family,
Sheldon, S. B., & Van Voorhis, F. L. (2004). Partnership and community connections on student achievement
programs in U.S. schools: Their development and (Annual Synthesis 2002). www.sedl.org/connections/
relationship to family involvement outcomes. School Stevenson, D., & Baker, D. (1987). The family-school
Effectiveness and School Improvement, 15, 125–148. relation and the child’s school performance. Child
Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Lehr, C. A., & Development, 58, 1348–1357.
Anderson, A. R. (2003). Facilitating student learning Stone, S. (2006). Correlates of change in student reported
and engagement: Lessons learned from Check and parent involvement in schooling: A new look at the
Connect longitudinal studies. The California School National Longitudinal Study of 1988. Journal of
Psychologist, 8, 29–41. Orthopsychiatry, 76, 518–530.
Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Turner, R. A., Irwin, C. E., Jr., Tschann, J. M., & Millstein,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, S. G. (1993). Autonomy, relatedness, and the initiation
549–570. of health risk behaviors in early adolescence. Health
Skinner, E. A., Johnson, S., & Snyder, T. (2005). Six Psychology, 12, 200–208.
dimensions of parenting: A motivational model. Vallerand, R. J., Fortier, M. S., & Guay, F. (1997). Self-
Parenting: Science and Practice, 5(2), 175–235. determination and persistence in a real-life setting:
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & Toward a motivational model of high school dropout.
Wellborn, J. G. (2009). Engagement as an organiza- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
tional construct in the dynamics of motivational devel- 1161–1176.
opment. In K. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook Vansteenkiste, M., Zhou, M., Lens, W., & Soenens, B.
of motivation in school (pp. 223–245). Mahwah, NJ: (2005). Experiences of autonomy and control among
Erlbaum. Chinese learners: Vitalizing or immobilizing? Journal
Skinner, E. A., Wellborn, J. G., & Connell, J. P. (1990). of Educational Psychology, 97, 468–483.
What it takes to do well in school and whether I’ve got Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E. M., & Chen, H. (2007). The role
it: The role of perceived control in children’s engage- of parents’ control in early adolescents’ psychologi-
ment and school achievement. Journal of Educational cal functioning: A longitudinal investigation in the
Psychology, 82, 22–32. United States and China. Child Development, 78,
Skinner, E. A., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Connell, J. P. 1592–1610.
(1998). Individual differences and the development of Weisz, J. R. (1986). Contingency and control beliefs as
perceived control. Monographs of the Society for predictors of psychotherapy outcomes among children
Research in Child Development, 63 (nos 2 and 3) and adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
whole no. 254 pp. v-220. Psychology, 54, 789–795.
Teacher-Student Relationships
and Engagement: Conceptualizing, 17
Measuring, and Improving the
Capacity of Classroom Interactions*

Robert C. Pianta, Bridget K. Hamre,


and Joseph P. Allen

Abstract
Classrooms are complex social systems, and student-teacher relationships
and interactions are also complex, multicomponent systems. We posit that
the nature and quality of relationship interactions between teachers and
students are fundamental to understanding student engagement, can be
assessed through standardized observation methods, and can be changed
by providing teachers knowledge about developmental processes relevant
for classroom interactions and personalized feedback/support about their
interactive behaviors and cues. When these supports are provided to teach-
ers’ interactions, student engagement increases. In this chapter, we focus
on the theoretical and empirical links between interactions and engagement
and present an approach to intervention designed to increase the quality of
such interactions and, in turn, increase student engagement and, ultimately,
learning and development. Recognizing general principles of development
in complex systems, a theory of the classroom as a setting for development,
and a theory of change specific to this social setting are the ultimate goals
of this work. Engagement, in this context, is both an outcome in its own

*Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by the


Wm. T. Grant Foundation, the Foundation for Child
Development, and the Institute of Education Sciences.
R.C. Pianta, Ph.D. (*)
Curry School of Education, University of Virginia,
PO Box 400260, Charlottesville, VA 22904-4260, USA
e-mail: rcp4p@virginia.edu
B.K. Hamre, Ph.D.
Center for Advanced Study of Teaching and Learning,
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, USA
e-mail: bkh3d@virginia.edu
J.P. Allen, Ph.D.
Department of Psychology, University of Virginia,
Charlottesville, VA, USA
e-mail: allen@virginia.edu

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 365


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_17, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
366 R.C. Pianta et al.

right and a mediator of impacts that teachers have on student outcomes


through their interactions with children and youth. In light of this discus-
sion, we offer suggestions or directions for further research in this area.

program of study to conceptualize, measure, and


Introduction ultimately improve the quality of teacher-child
relationships through a focus on their interac-
Students spend at least one-quarter of their wak-
tions, starting in the preschool and early elemen-
ing hours in schools, most of it in classrooms,
tary period. This work resulted in an observational
one of the most proximal and potentially power-
tool for assessing interactions in early childhood
ful settings for influencing children and youth.
and elementary classrooms, the Classroom
Students’ relationships and interactions with
Assessment Scoring System (CLASS; Pianta, La
teachers either produce or inhibit developmental
Paro, & Hamre, 2004); an accompanying concep-
change to the extent that they engage, meaning-
tualization of classrooms, the CLASS framework
fully challenge, and provide social and relational
(Hamre, Pianta, Mashburn, & Downer, 2010);
supports. In this sense, relationships between
and an approach to enhancing the quality of
teachers and students reflect a classroom’s capac-
teacher-child interactions that we call
ity to promote development, and it is precisely in
MyTeachingPartner. Recently, we extended this
this way that relationships and interactions are
approach to measuring and improving relation-
the key to understanding engagement. As just one
ships to middle and high school classrooms
example of this connection between engagement
(Pianta, Hamre, & Mintz, 2010). As we have
and relationships, the National Research Council
deepened this work in the early grades and
(NRC, 2004) published a groundbreaking recast-
extended these ideas toward classrooms serving
ing of settings in terms of features that engage
older children, evidence has been revealed not
developmental mechanisms in adolescence in
only for the NRC report but also for the recasting
positive, promotive ways. Notably, the NRC
of classrooms as contexts in which perhaps the
report shifted discussions from how various con-
key mechanism through which classroom expe-
texts (e.g., classrooms, clubs) and programs
riences add value for development is through the
should focus on reducing the rate of problems in
pivotal role of student-teacher relationships in
child and adolescent development to one that rec-
the very process of engagement.
ognizes that perhaps the best way for these con-
In our view, and reflected throughout this
texts to benefit youth is to emphasize the positive
chapter, engagement is a relational process. It
ways that relational experiences in these settings
reflects students’ cognitive, emotional, behav-
provide children and youth experiences that draw
ioral, and motivational states and capacities but is
them in—that engage with their desires and needs
conditioned in part on interpersonal relationships
for feeling competent and connected to others.
as activators and organizers of these states and
From the perspective of the NRC report, relation-
capacities in the service of some larger develop-
ships are a mechanism or medium through which
mental task or aim (Allen & Allen, 2009; Crosnoe,
settings engage developmental processes.
2000; Dornbusch, Glasgow, & Lin, 1996; Eccles,
Building on extensive observational work that
Lord, & Midgley, 1991). From this perspective,
had been underway in early childhood settings
engagement is best understood by understanding
for the past two decades, as well as a very com-
relationships and their behavioral expression in
pelling literature demonstrating the value of
interpersonal interactions in the classroom—
adult-child relationships for promoting compe-
through observation of exchanges and interpre-
tence in the birth to 8 years period (see Pianta,
tation of their value and meaning with regard
Hamre, & Stuhlman, 2003), we embarked on a
to fostering opportunity to learn and develop.
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 367

Engagement reflects relationally mediated par- of the instructional and social supports actually
ticipation in opportunity. offered to early adolescents in classrooms is gen-
In this chapter, we describe this and related erally low and even lower for the groups noted
work in an effort to frame conceptually the dis- above. Moreover, findings from studies of large
cussion of student engagement not as a property and diverse samples of middle schools demon-
of a child but rather as embedded in interactions strate quite clearly that competitive, standards-
and relationships. We organize our discussion in driven instruction in decontextualized skills and
three main sections: the first provides a depiction knowledge contributes directly to this sense of
of classrooms as a relational setting for develop- alienation and disengagement (Eccles et al.,
ment, the second describes efforts to conceptu- 1997; Shouse, 1996). Engagement in school
alize and measure teacher-student classroom begins to decline early in adolescence, and by
interactions, and the third reports early results entry into high school this decline is pronounced
from efforts to enhance engagement in class- to the point where more than half of high school
rooms as a function of improving the quality of students from all types of schools report that they
teacher-student interactions. do not take their school or their studies seriously
(Marks, 2000; Steinberg, Brown, & Dornbusch,
1996). Further, adolescents bring their peers
Underperformance of the Classroom along with them: doing well in school switches
Setting as a Context for Youth from being a positively valued behavior among
Development peers in childhood to a somewhat negatively val-
ued behavior by mid-adolescence. Yet, engage-
There is little question that academic achieve- ment and intrinsic motivation become pivotal in
ment, personal well-being, and civic-related adolescence, as students at this age have the
outcomes for children and adolescents are in means to not only withdraw energy from educa-
dire need of improvement and enhancement tional pursuits but also the ability to drop out
(Carbonaro & Gamoran, 2002; National Center altogether (NRC, 2004).
for Education Statistics [NCES], 2003). For all With regard to achievement outcomes, there is
of the resources devoted to schooling, the capa- recent evidence that middle and high school
city of classrooms as settings that promote and youth are underperforming in relation to expecta-
enhance development is sorely lacking. For tions set by state standards tests and in interna-
example, adolescents report that social and task- tional competitions. Moreover, performance gaps
related disengagement and alienation are directly related to culture, race, and income are not clos-
tied to classroom experiences that are discon- ing despite years of rhetoric and attention (NCES,
nected from youths’ developmental needs and 2003). For example, after years of standards-
motivations (Crosnoe, 2000; Dornbusch et al., based educational reform under No Child Left
1996; Eccles et al., 1991). Youth describe school Behind (NCLB), roughly 40% of poor or African-
experiences as irrelevant and lacking appropriate American eighth graders in Virginia perform
and meaningful challenges. These patterns are below standards for reading achievement, and the
exacerbated dramatically for youth attending corresponding rates of failure for youth in the
schools in low-income communities, rural com- District of Columbia are close to 80% (Aratani,
munities, large schools, and for those with histo- 2006). These rates of failure in reading, which
ries of poor achievement or problem behavior was one of the spurs for NCLB, reflect a funda-
(e.g., Crosnoe, 2001; Eccles, Lord, Roeser, mental misunderstanding of the mechanisms by
Barber, & Jozefowicz, 1997). which students are engaged through relationships
Even more disconcerting is recent evidence and the need to reconceptualize and redesign
from observational studies of large samples of how we support teachers to build upon and foster
fifth grade classrooms that the nature and quality relationships with students.
368 R.C. Pianta et al.

Consider a second target of school reform, student interactions or youth reports, experiences
the dropout rate. Fewer than 60% of ninth grad- in classrooms too often fail to capitalize on stu-
ers in certain demographic groups (NCES, 2003) dent interests, goals, and motivation and rather
actually graduate 4 years later. Yet for 10 years, promote disengagement and alienation. One
decreasing the dropout rate has been a singular cannot read these accounts and escape the sense
focus of most secondary schools, and the average that school and classroom settings and the
annual dropout rate remains near 10% and ranges adults responsible for their quality are simply not
up for some groups. These figures make strik- involved relationally (Crosnoe, 2000; Dornbusch
ingly clear that the high school classroom as a et al., 1996; Eccles et al., 1991). Yet, despite this
setting for youth development is fundamentally generally dismal picture of classrooms, it is also
flawed. Put another way, it does not appear to us true that nearly every student can describe, with
that the central problem in school reform is cur- enthusiasm and passion, a relationship with a
riculum, school/class size, or outcomes assess- teacher that they felt was meaningful and impor-
ment but rather the extent to which teachers are tant to them, often with considerable evidence to
supported to interact with students and form rela- back up those claims (Resnick et al., 1997).
tionships with them that engage them in opportu- The impressions gleaned from youth reports
nities to learn and develop. are confirmed in observations, some of which are
Youth report that they are highly concerned ethnographic in nature while others rely on large-
with the actual experiences they have in class- scale assessments of hundreds of classrooms. For
room settings, which they find lacking in terms example, evidence gleaned from observing large
of supportive relationships that draw them into numbers of typical American classrooms in first,
meaningful challenges and competence-building third, and fifth grades shows clearly that the
experiences (Crosnoe, 2001; Csikszentmihalyi nature and quality of adult-student interactions in
& Schneider, 2000; Marks, 2000; NRC, 2004; classrooms are lacking in the kind of assets out-
Roeser, Eccles, & Sameroff, 2000). Perhaps they lined in the NRC report. For example, in the
are right, and the capacity of schools to support NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth
youth development, particularly for “high risk” Development observations in more than 2,500
youth, depends on whether the relationships and elementary classrooms, of the opportunities for
interactions among students and teachers within academic activities and learning to which a typi-
a classroom offer a developmentally meaningful cal student is exposed, more than 85% of those
and challenging experience (NRC). Because opportunities take place in the context of teacher-
teacher-student interactions embody the rela- directed whole group instruction or individual
tional capacity of the classroom to promote posi- seatwork, in contrast to small-group work that
tive development, our focus is on improving and might capitalize on teacher-student relationships
changing these relationships and interactions as key mediators of engagement. The typical stu-
and involves working with teachers. Thus, our dent interacted with their teacher (individually or
theory and method of change is centered on in a small group) fewer than four times in an hour,
teachers’ relationships and interactions with and in most cases, these exchanges were perfunc-
students. tory and compliance-directed. Furthermore, most
instructional exchanges had a pronounced and
almost singular focus on performing basic skills,
A Theory of Engagement tasks that require a discrete answer that is correct
Within Classroom Settings or not rather than eliciting analysis, reasoning, or
problem-solving around a more ambiguous chal-
We start with a brief description of a typical class- lenge. From a relational standpoint, these
room experience in a school in the United States, exchanges were devoid of personal, emotional,
public or private, regardless of grade or content motivational properties that would engage the
area. Whether based on observations of teacher- student in the task at hand. Recalling the NRC
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 369

report’s emphasis on meaningful challenges for A fundamental principle in addressing the


cognitive development (as well as recent calls for chronically resource-starved classroom is that
raising standards for “twenty-first century skills”), modifying the classroom as a relational setting
this focus on basic skills neglects the ways in to engage children and youth more fully may
which reasoning, problem-solving, and more be the single best way to unleash and expand
advanced cognition can be a force for engaging the level of human resources (e.g., relationships
students in activities that are highly salient devel- and interpersonal interactions) available to the
opmentally but which also require relational sup- educational process (Sarason, 1982). Below, we
ports to sustain students’ participation. Despite discuss three features of classrooms likely to
rhetoric that paints a picture of middle and high influence levels of behavioral/psychological
school as challenging and interesting, the actual engagement—relational supports, competence
experiences youth have in classroom settings supports, and relevance. These features form the
(observed or reported) are often lacking in terms core theoretical foundation of our subsequent
of meaningful challenges, supportive relationships, efforts to assess and improve the relational prop-
and competence-building opportunities (Crosnoe, erties of classrooms and, thereby, engagement.
2001; Csikszentmihalyi & Schneider, 2000; Understanding the primary role of interactions
Marks, 2000; NRC, 2002; Roeser et al., 2000). and relationships in creating the capacity for chil-
Schools all fundamentally rise or fall on the dren and youth to engage the classroom as a set-
success of what occurs within the classroom (e.g., ting for development is a fundamental precursor to
Crosnoe, 2001; Nye, Konstantopoulos, & Hedges, understanding our approach to measuring interac-
2004; Resnick et al., 1997) Ironically, close obser- tions and to changing classroom settings’ capacity
vation of most any secondary school in America for engagement. Readers will recognize applica-
reveals that adolescents—both at risk and high tions and extensions of Vygotsky’s (1978, 1991)
functioning—often display remarkably high ideas about the contextualized nature of learning
degrees of motivation and engagement within the and development and close, interdependent con-
school setting. Rarely, however, does this occur nection among relational supports, task-related
within the classroom. High school hallways, play- challenges, and learning. Pianta (1999) also has
ing fields, and lunchrooms literally brim over with discussed the connection between classroom con-
youthful energy, excitement, and enthusiasm. texts and learning in terms of the relational, struc-
Intense interactions occur in sports and extracur- tural, and motivational affordances available in
ricular activities, and interactions with peers dom- classrooms. Central to each of these perspectives,
inate students’ perception of the social ecology of and elaborated below, is an appreciation of engage-
school. It is only when these students enter their ment as a contextualized process mediated by rela-
classroom that energy levels decline precipitously, tionships and interpersonal interactions.
and it is rare that a given student will “connect”
with a teacher or material in classroom or subject Relational Supports
area in such a way that they perform at high lev- As a behavior setting, the classroom runs on
els of capacity or “flow” (Csikszentmihalyi & interactions between and among participants: the
Schneider, 2000). The classroom setting looks relationship between the student and the teacher
equally bleak from the perspective of teachers, and the relationships of students with one another.
who are also dropping out and becoming more These relationships and their value emotionally,
disengaged. Fifteen percent of the entire teaching instrumentally, and psychologically are funda-
workforce turns over every year. Rates of teachers mental supports to the value of their experience
leaving the profession are increasing. And those in the classroom setting for furthering develop-
who stay report a sense of malaise and frustra- ment. It is not an overstatement to suggest that
tion—they feel their job is getting harder and they most children and adolescents live for their social
have fewer tools with which to work and feel relationships (Collins & Repinski, 1994), and for
effective (Hart, Stroot, Yinger, & Smith, 2005). many young people, relationships with teachers
370 R.C. Pianta et al.

are core organizers of experience; they are funda- with students are a fundamental facet of class-
mental to core developmental functions. Yet, the rooms’ capacity to support development.
qualities of teacher-student relationships are fre-
quently afterthoughts in battles over curricula, Autonomy/Competence Supports
testing, school structure, and funding. Positive Children and youth are engaged by challenges
relationships with adults are perhaps the single that are within reach and that provide a sense of
most important ingredient in promoting positive self-efficacy and control: experiences that offer
student development. For example, when teachers challenges viewed as “older” or adultlike but for
learn to make modest efforts to form a personal which appropriate scaffolding and support are
connection with their adolescent students—such provided (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, &
that the students feel known—they can dramati- Pastorelli, 1996; Eccles et al., 1993). Any setting
cally enhance student motivation in school that intends to advance development and learning
and emotional functioning outside of school outcomes for children or youth must carefully
(Roeser, Eccles, & Sameroff, 1998; Skinner, craft the nature of experience it provides in order
Zimmer-Gembeck, & Connell, 1998). In the to give participants a developmentally calibrated
early grades, when teachers spend nondirective sense of control, autonomy, choice, and mastery.
individual time with children who they find Absent these considerations or in settings that
challenging, the disruptive behavior of these rely on approaches characterized as overly top-
students drops, and teachers report more har- down or passive, in which teachers are over- or
monious and learning-oriented interactions underinvolved, classrooms are doomed to be
(Mashburn et al., 2008). places lacking in engaged participants. For exam-
Adolescents report both that they would learn ple, one of the most tragically avoidable errors
more if their teachers cared about them person- that some secondary school teachers make is to
ally and that such personal connections are rare assume that youth strivings for autonomy and
(Public Agenda, 1997). A close, supportive rela- self-expression represent negative forces to be
tionship with a teacher is a key feature distin- countered rather than positive energy to be har-
guishing at-risk children and adolescents who nessed. This basic misunderstanding of adoles-
succeed in school from those who do not (Pianta, cent development (one often promoted in teacher
Steinberg, & Rollins, 1995; Resnick et al., 1997), education courses and reinforced by school poli-
and youths’ sense of social connection within set- cies) then takes form in highly controlling and
tings predicts outcomes ranging from higher punitive classroom and school settings and in
achievement scores to greater student engage- instruction that is highly teacher-driven and dis-
ment and more positive academic attitudes (Bryk couraging of exploration and curiosity. At the
& Driscoll, 1988; Bryk, Lee, & Holland, 1993; other end of the age spectrum, all too often, teach-
Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Crosnoe, Johnson, ers espouse a “child-centered” or “play-based”
& Elder, 2004; Ryan & Deci, 2000; see also, philosophy around learning and development
NRC, 2004, for extended review of other similar that all too often expresses itself in children wan-
findings). Notably, even for relatively highly dering around activity centers while teachers are
motivated late adolescents in college, recent not involved in actively scaffolding learning
experimental work has shown that a sense of iso- (Pianta, Mashburn, Downer, Hamre, & Justice,
lation can significantly reduce energy for intel- 2008). In both instances, overcontrolled responses
lectual pursuits and that this reduction is powerful to adolescents and underinvolved responses to
enough to temporarily depress results on IQ tests young children, adult-child relationships, and
(Baumeister, Twenge, & Nuss, 2002), while interactions are not calibrated to developmental
increasing irrational and risk-taking behavior tendencies of students. This mismatch of class-
(Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister, 2002). Thus, room and development results in schools narrow-
regardless of age or grade, interpersonal relational ing, rather than expanding, the “space” in which
supports provided through teachers’ interactions zones of proximal development can be created.
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 371

Relevance solving a somewhat ambiguous and perhaps


For children and youth, the connection of aca- uncertain real-life problem, teachers end up rely-
demic skills and knowledge to their real-life ing on relationships and interactions to cajole or
experience is a near-universal property of class- to address behavioral disruptions and inattention
rooms that foster engagement. Adolescents, like (i.e., disengagement) that are the inevitable
adults, deploy a considerable amount of effort in by-product of miscalibration.
attempts to make meaning in their lives. For Consciously addressing the relevance of what
many, adolescence is a period in which this occurs within the classroom to the larger world is
becomes a focus for the first time. This process critical to engaging otherwise restless young
ultimately leads to a bias in adolescents’ evalua- minds. On a smaller scale, teachers may increase
tion of experience (particularly those experiences the relevance of the classroom by making
offered by adults) toward choices they view as repeated, explicit ties between curricular material
relevant, or connected to their emerging views on and real-world applications and engaging rela-
what is meaningful and what is not. Too often, tional processes that scaffold participation in
the high school curriculum and the rationales learning that is somewhat less constricted. The
behind it are taken as a “given” without recogni- key factor here is that the real-world connections
tion that these rationales need to be made clear to must be made in ways that are meaningful as per-
each new cohort of students. Drawing even very ceived by the student. For some, it may be through
distal connections between what occurs within a very close and comforting emotional connec-
high school and the larger “real world” can alter tion to a teacher, while for others it will be through
student behavior. For example, involving stu- a teacher providing challenging problems.
dents in significant, real-world, voluntary com- These ideas about the central role of teacher-
munity service and then discussing it within the student interactions and relationships as the pri-
classroom in an ongoing way has been found to mary mechanism by which student engagement
reduce disruptive behavior by 50% in randomly is fostered form the basis for our developmen-
controlled trials, with similar effects upon other tally informed analysis of classroom effects on
outcomes in youths’ lives as well (Allen, Philliber, student outcomes. In our view, the capacity of
Herrling, & Kuperminc, 1997). Centuries ago, classroom settings to engage children and youth
late adolescents were commanding armies and is the core “criterion” by which they should be
running countries (Barzun, 2000). Today, a gen- judged, and the features of relational supports,
eration of children and adolescents who grew up autonomy/competence supports, and relevance
with the internet, social networks, and sophisti- are how classrooms, through relationships and
cated video games is confined to a classroom for interactions, accomplish that goal.
hours a day with little vision of how what occurs These supports, enacted in teacher-student
within that classroom relates to the larger world. interactions, produce cycles of student engage-
In the early grades, as we recounted previ- ment, teacher efficacy, and student performance.
ously, virtually no instruction occurs that does We suggest that in the best classrooms, these sup-
not have a “correct/incorrect” focus. Thinking, ports operate in concert to initiate self-reinforcing
problem-solving, and reasoning with real-world linkages among engaged students, effective
information is conspicuously absent in the vast teachers, and growth in student performance.
majority of classrooms (see Pianta, Belsky, Relationships and interactions in the classroom
Houts, Morrison, & NICHD ECCRN, 2007). are the media through which relational, compe-
When academic learning is almost completely tence, and relevance supports are made available
organized and focused in this way, there is virtu- to students. In the next section, we present our
ally no way in which teachers can make the con- conceptualization and technical approach to
tent or activity relevant. Rather than drawing on interactions and relationships between teachers
relationships and interpersonal interactions with and students as the focus of measurement and
students as a front-end asset to draw them into change.
372 R.C. Pianta et al.

Conceptualizing and Measuring however, as is evident in the discussion above


Teacher-Student Classroom Interactions and in reports such as that of the NRC (2004),
these concepts of the TTI framework and their
To help organize the diverse literatures that relevance for understanding engagement are
inform conceptualization and assessment of applicable to adolescents as well.
classroom processes, Hamre and Pianta (2007)
presented the Teaching Through Interactions
(TTI) framework, a theoretically driven and Emotional Interaction Domain
empirically supported system for conceptualiz-
ing, organizing, and measuring classroom inter- Teacher efforts to support students’ social and
actions between teachers and students into three emotional functioning in the classroom, through
major domains—emotional supports, classroom positive facilitation of teacher-student and
organization, and instructional supports. This student-student interactions, are key elements of
framework recognizes that the starting point for effective classroom practice. Two broad areas of
understanding contextual influences on develop- developmental theory guide much of the work on
ment is to recognize that development occurs emotional support in classrooms—attachment
through interactions between the capacities and (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978;
skills of the person and the resources available to Bowlby, 1969; Pianta, 1999) and self-determi-
them in various settings, and that this process is nation theory (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Ryan
very dynamic (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998; & Deci, 2000; Skinner & Belmont, 1993).
Magnusson & Stattin, 1998). Attachment theorists posit that when parents pro-
A feature of the TTI framework is that the vide emotional support, and a predictable, con-
latent structure of teacher-child interactions sistent, and safe environment, children become
applies consistently across grades from preschool more self-reliant and are able to take risks as they
through to secondary grades; thus, the three- explore the world because they know that an
domain TTI latent structure is hypothesized as adult will be there to help them if they need it
grade invariant. Critically, although latent struc- (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby, 1969). This
ture is hypothesized as invariant, the TTI frame- theory has been broadly applied to and validated
work reflects the developmentally relevant in school environments (Birch & Ladd, 1998;
construct of heterotypic continuity and allows for Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Howes, Hamilton, &
variation across grades in the specific behavioral Matheson, 1994; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1992;
indicators that reflect positive and negative fea- Pianta, 1999). Self-determination (or self-sys-
tures of interactions. tems) theory (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Ryan &
In the section that follows, we briefly review Deci, 2000; Skinner & Belmont, 1993) suggests
the three major domains of teacher-student that children and youth are most motivated to
interactions described in the TTI framework learn when adults support their need to feel com-
(emotional, organizational, and instructional), petent, positively related to others, and autono-
including a summary of the developmental theo- mous. Throughout schooling, students who are
ries and empirical studies on which they are more emotionally connected to teachers and
based. Within each of these three broad domains peers demonstrate positive trajectories of devel-
of interaction, we then describe in subsections a opment in both social and academic domains
number of specific dimensions that form the basis (Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Harter, 1996; Ladd,
of behavioral interactions and observations of Birch, & Buhs, 1999; Pianta et al., 1995; Roeser
interactions. Thus, we present two levels of the et al., 2000; Ryan, Stiller, & Lynch, 1994; Silver,
TTI framework—three broad domains and the Measelle, Essex, & Armstrong, 2005; Wentzel,
dimensions of behavioral interactions between 1998). Within this domain, we focus on behav-
teachers and students that more specifically define ioral interactions related to emotional climate,
these domains. Much of what we present below teacher sensitivity, and regard for student
is based on work in elementary classrooms; perspectives.
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 373

Emotional Climate see adults as a resource and create environments


Classrooms are, by their very nature, social in which students feel safe and free to explore
places. Teachers and children laugh and play and learn (Pianta et al., 2004). Highly sensitive
together, share stories about their lives outside of teaching requires teachers to attend to, process,
the classroom, and work together to create an and respond to a lot of information simultane-
environment in which all learning occurs. The ously. For example, during whole group instruc-
classroom climate can be described along posi- tion, a sensitive teacher may, within quick
tive and negative dimensions. Positive climate succession, notice some children not paying
encompasses the degree to which students expe- attention, see that one child is frustrated because
rience warm caring relationships with adults and he does not understand her questions, and observe
peers and enjoy the time they spend in the class- a sad look on a child she knows is generally very
room. Negative climates are those in which stu- happy and engaged. The sensitive teacher not
dents experience frequent yelling, humiliation, or only notices these subtle cues from students, but
irritation in interactions with teachers and peers. knows her students well enough to respond in
The aspect of climate that has been studied ways that help alleviate their problems. She may,
most extensively in the past 10 years is the nature for example, change the tone of her voice to reen-
and quality of teachers’ relationships with stu- gage those students not participating, take a quick
dents. There is strong evidence for the salience of moment to restate her question in simpler lan-
student-teacher relationships as an important guage, and make a mental note to check in with
context for children’s development (see Pianta the sad student at recess. In contrast, an insensi-
et al., 2003); student-teacher relationships are tive teacher may completely miss these subtle
associated with children’s peer competencies cues or respond in ways that aggravate, rather
(e.g., Birch & Ladd, 1998; Howes, 2000; Howes than alleviate, students’ problems.
et al., 1994) and trajectories toward academic Students in classrooms with sensitive teachers
success or failure (Birch & Ladd, 1996, 1998; are more engaged and self-reliant in the class-
Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Ladd et al., 1999; Pianta room and have lower levels of mother-reported
et al., 1995; Silver et al., 2005; van Ijzendoorn, internalizing problems than do those with less
Sagi, & Lambermon, 1992). There is evidence sensitive teachers (NICHD ECCRN, 2003;
that certain teachers have tendencies to develop Rimm-Kaufman, Early, & Cox, 2002). Sensitive
more positive relationships, across multiple stu- teaching is important to not only social outcomes,
dents in their classroom, than do others (Hamre, but also to academic outcomes. For example,
Pianta, Downer, & Mashburn, 2005; Mashburn, among a group of preschoolers, those who expe-
Hamre, Downer, & Pianta, 2007). Children and rienced more responsive teacher interactions in
youth in classrooms with higher levels of teacher preschool displayed stronger vocabulary and
support have higher levels of peer acceptance decoding skills at the end of first grade (Connor,
and classroom engagement than do their peers in Son, & Hindman, 2005). Sensitivity—timing and
less supportive classrooms, even after controlling responsiveness to student cues—is perhaps one
for individual levels of teacher-support (Hughes, of the single most important features of interac-
Zhang, & Hill, 2006). tion in relation to engagement as these behaviors
on the part of the teacher literally denote the
Teacher Sensitivity extent of calibration in drawing the student
Teachers provide more than a warm and caring toward an opportunity.
social environment. They must be attuned and
responsive to the individual cues and needs of Regard for Students’ Perspectives
students in their classrooms, a dimension of The final dimension of emotional support is
teaching referred to here as teacher sensitivity. the degree to which classrooms and interactions
Highly sensitive teachers, through their consistent, are structured around the interests and motiva-
timely, and responsive interactions, help students tions of the teacher, versus those of the students.
374 R.C. Pianta et al.

In some classrooms, teachers frequently ask for and attention (Emmer & Stough, 2001). Teachers
students’ ideas and thoughts, follow students’ using more effective behavior management
lead, and provide opportunities for students to strategies (Arnold, McWilliams, & Arnold, 1998;
have a formative role in the classroom. In these Emmer & Strough, 2001; Evertson, Emmer,
classrooms, students are not just allowed to talk Sanford, & Clements, 1983; Evertson & Harris,
but are actively encouraged to talk to one another 1999), having more organized and routine man-
(Pianta et al., 2004). At the other end of the con- agement structures (Bohn, Roehrig, & Pressley,
tinuum are classrooms in which teachers follow 2004; Cameron, Connor, & Morrison, 2005), and
very scripted plans for how the day should run, implementing strategies that make students active
show little flexibility or response to students’ participants in classroom activities (Bowman &
interests and motivations, and provide few oppor- Stott, 1994; Bruner, 1996; Rogoff, 1990;
tunities for students to express their thoughts or Vygotsky, 1978) have less oppositional behavior,
to assume responsibility for activities in the class- higher levels of engagement in learning, and ulti-
room. Teachers in these classrooms may also be mately, students who learn more. Thus, the
very controlling of student movement, requiring, dimensions of teacher-student interaction that are
for example, young children to sit quietly on the reflected in the classroom organization domain
rug with their legs crossed and hands in their laps include effective behavior management, produc-
for long periods of time, or for older children, tivity, and learning formats.
requiring long stretches of drill.
Children and adolescents report more positive Effective Behavior Management
feelings about school, display more motivation, Behavior management is a term that is often
and are more engaged when they experience applied to a broad spectrum of classroom man-
more student-focused and autonomy-supportive agement strategies, including teachers’ abilities
instruction (deKruif, McWilliam, Ridley, & to engage students and make constructive use of
Wakely, 2000; Gutman & Sulzby, 2000; Pianta, time. Within the TTI framework, behavior man-
La Paro, Payne, Cox, & Bradley, 2002; Valeski & agement is defined more narrowly as teacher-
Stipek, 2001). Students in more teacher-directed student interactions intended to promote positive
classrooms have higher levels of internalizing behavior and prevent or terminate misbehavior
problems (NICHD ECCRN, 2003). There are in the classroom. There is general consensus
some findings, however, suggesting that the opti- around a set of practices associated with more
mal level of teacher control may vary depending positive student behavior including: (a) provid-
on factors such as learning objectives (Brophy & ing clear and consistent behavioral expectations;
Good, 1986; Soar & Soar, 1979) and grade (b) monitoring the classroom for potential prob-
(Valeski & Stipek, 2001). Interestingly, there is lems and proactively preventing problems rather
ample support that adolescents also thrive when than being reactive; (c) efficiently redirecting
given some degree of control and choice over minor misbehavior before it escalates; (d) using
their learning (NRC, 2004). positive, proactive strategies such as praising
positive behavior rather than calling attention to
misbehavior; and (e) spending a minimal amount
Classroom Organization Domain of time on behavior management issues (Emmer
& Stough, 2001; Pianta et al., 2004). At the low
Educational research and practice place tremen- end of this dimension, classrooms are chaotic
dous emphasis on the role of organization and with very few consistently enforced rules and a
management in creating a well-functioning class- great deal of student misbehavior.
room. In the TTI framework, classroom organi- Most of the research on behavior management
zation is the domain of teacher-student interactions was conducted by process-product researchers in
through which teachers organize behavior, time, the 1970s and 1980s with studies consistently
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 375

showing that classrooms with positive behavior Instructional Learning Formats


management tend to have students who make The instructional learning formats dimension of
greater academic progress (Good & Grouws, interaction focuses directly on the extent to which
1977; Soar & Soar, 1979). Intervention studies teachers provide interesting activities, instruc-
suggest that teachers who adopt these types of tion, centers/projects, and materials and facilitate
practices after training are more likely than teach- those activities so that students are actively
ers in control groups to have students who are engaged through various modalities. Consistent
engaged and learning (Emmer & Strough, 2001; with constructivist theories as well as informa-
Evertson & Harris, 1999; Evertson et al., 1983). tion-processing views of learning and cognition
Surprisingly, researchers have yet to examine the (Bowman & Stott, 1994; Bruner, 1996; Rogoff,
extent to which these specific behavioral strate- 1990; Vygotsky, 1978), formats for instruction
gies are associated with the more recent concept should foster active participation in a specific
of self-regulated learning behaviors, though prior learning opportunity such that the students are
work would suggest clear linkages. not only participating behaviorally but they are
engaged cognitively as well. In classrooms low
Productivity on this dimension, teachers may rely on one for-
In productive classrooms, teachers are not only mat, typically lecture, and fail to format instruc-
effective managers of behavior, but are well orga- tion or provide opportunity for interaction that
nized, spend a minimal amount of time on basic foster students’ engagement. Again, formatting
management activities such as taking attendance instruction developmentally is not solely contin-
or passing out and collecting homework, and are gent on the type of instruction or number of mate-
prepared for instructional activities so that little rials a teacher uses but rather how effectively the
time is lost in transition. Highly productive class- teacher interacts to use instruction and materials
rooms may resemble a “well-oiled machine” in to engage students (Rimm-Kaufman, La Paro,
which everyone in the classroom seems to know Downer, & Pianta, 2005).
what is expected of them and how to go about
doing it (Pianta et al., 2004). In contrast, when
teachers do not manage time efficiently, students Instructional Interaction Domain
may spend extraordinary amounts of time look-
ing for materials, waiting for the next activity, or Instructional methods have been put in the spot-
simply sitting around. light in recent years, as more emphasis has been
Early work by process-product researchers placed on the translation of cognitive science,
focused attention on the importance of time man- learning, and developmental research to educa-
agement, providing consistent evidence that stu- tional environments (Carver & Klahr, 2001). The
dents are most engaged in productive classrooms, theoretical foundation for the conceptualization
and that this engagement is, in turn, directly asso- of instructional supports in the TTI framework
ciated with student learning (Brophy & Evertson, comes primarily from research on cognitive and
1976; Coker, Medley, & Soar, 1980; Good & language development (e.g., Carver & Klahr,
Grouws, 1979; Stallings, 1975; Stallings, Cory, 2001; Catts, Fey, Zhang, & Tomblin, 2001; Fujiki,
Fairweather, & Needels, 1978). Several more Brinton, & Clarke, 2002; Romberg, Carpenter, &
recent studies suggest that teachers observed to Dremock, 2005; Taylor, Pearson, Peterson, &
foster productive classrooms spend more time Rodriguez, 2003; Vygotsky, 1991; Wharton-
creating efficient routines at the beginning of the McDonald, Pressley, & Hampston, 1998). This
school year and that this early investment pays literature highlights the distinction between sim-
off for students and teachers by enabling them to ply learning facts and gaining “usable knowl-
spend less time in transition and more time in edge,” which is built upon learning how facts are
child-managed activities later in the school year interconnected, organized, and conditioned upon
(Bohn et al., 2004; Cameron et al., 2005). one another (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking,
376 R.C. Pianta et al.

1999; Mayer, 2002). A student’s cognitive and higher-order thinking, but they take advantage of
language development is contingent on the the moment-to-moment opportunities within their
opportunities adults provide to express existing daily interactions to push students toward deeper
skills and scaffold more complex ones (Davis & thinking. In contrast, in classrooms low on con-
Miyake, 2004; Skibbe, Behnke, & Justice, 2004; cept development, interactions between teachers
Vygotsky, 1991). The development of “metacog- and students focus on remembering facts, or simple
nitive” skills, or the awareness and understanding tasks in which they must recognize or recall
of one’s thinking processes, is also critical information.
(Veenman, Kok, & Blöte, 2005; Williams, Blythe, Interactions that stimulate concept develop-
& White, 2002). The exemplary work of the ment predict greater achievement gains for stu-
National Research Council’s series, How Students dents (Romberg et al., 2005; Taylor et al., 2003;
Learn (Donovan & Bransford, 2005), summa- Wharton-McDonald et al., 1998). As noted by
rizes research across disciplines to emphasize Brophy (1986), this does not require that all of a
how specific teaching strategies can enhance stu- teacher’s questions are “higher level” questions,
dents’ development and application of these core but that there is a balance in which teachers use
thinking skills (Bransford et al., 1999). Within higher level questions to help focus student atten-
this broad, cognitively focused definition of tion on the process of learning rather than solely
instruction, we describe below three aspects of on the product. In one recent study, Taylor and
teachers’ interactions with students that not only colleagues (2003) examined the role of these
promote engagement but student learning out- teacher practices in reading development among
comes as well. children in 88 high-poverty classrooms (first to
fifth grade) across the United States. They
Concept Development observed in classrooms three times over the
Through instructional behaviors, conversations, course of the year and examined growth in a ran-
and activities, teachers foster students’ develop- domly selected nine students per classroom.
ment of concepts and higher-order thinking skills Their observations consisted of mixed methods
(Pianta et al., 2004). In an extension of Bloom’s in which they collected quantitative information
Taxonomy (Bloom, Engelhart, Furst, Hill, & on the types and frequency of questions used by
Krathwohl, 1956), Mayer (2002) offers a helpful teachers, as well as detailed qualitative informa-
description of the teaching and learning practices tion on teacher practices. Results suggested that
associated with the development of these cogni- children in classrooms in which teachers empha-
tive skills. According to Mayer, learning requires sized higher-order thinking skills, through ques-
not only the acquisition of knowledge (retention), tioning and activities, displayed more reading
but the ability to access and apply this knowledge growth over the course of the year.
in new situations (transfer). Teachers can facili-
tate this transfer process by providing students Feedback
with opportunities to: understand—build con- In order to get the most benefit from the instruc-
nections between new and previous knowledge; tional opportunities described above, students
apply—use procedures and knowledge to help need feedback about their learning. Feedback
solve new problems; analyze—divide informa- refers to a broad range of teachers’ interactions
tion into meaningful parts; evaluate—make con- with students in which the teacher provides some
clusions based on criteria or standards; and information back to the student about their per-
create—put pieces of knowledge together to pro- formance or effort. Research on feedback has
duce new ideas. These features of students’ cog- typically focused on praise (Brophy & Evertson,
nitive engagement are directly promoted through 1976; Stallings, 1975), behavioral feedback, or
teacher-student interactions. At the high end of attributional feedback, in which teachers make
this dimension, teachers are opportunists who not statements to students attributing their perfor-
only plan activities in ways that will stimulate mance to either ability (e.g., “you did this well
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 377

because you are a good reader”) or effort (e.g., Wilkinson, & Moore, 2002), and narrative skills
“you did this well because you worked hard”) (Catts et al., 1999; Zevenbergen, Whitehurst, &
(Burnett, 2003; Dohrn & Bryan, 1994; Mueller & Zevenbergen, 2003). In classrooms offering high
Dweck, 1998). Although the TTI definition levels of language modeling, teachers often con-
includes these forms of feedback, the focus is on verse with students, ask many open-ended ques-
feedback that provides students with specific tions, repeat or extend children’s responses, and
information about the content or process of learn- use a variety of words, including more advanced
ing. High-quality feedback is described as com- language which is explicitly linked to words the
munication from teachers that provides students students already know. Although there is a mix of
with specific information about not only whether teacher and student talk in these classrooms, there
or not they are correct (Brophy, 1986), but about is a clear and intentional effort by teachers to pro-
how they might get to the correct answer, how mote students’ language use, including explicit
they might perform at a higher level, or how their attempts to facilitate peer conversations (Justice,
performance meshes with larger goals. Teachers Mashburn, Hamre, & Pianta, 2008; Pianta et al.,
providing high-quality feedback provide frequent 2004). At the low end, classrooms are dominated
feedback loops or back-and-forth exchanges in by teacher talk, and student utterances are rarely
which a teacher responds to an initial student attended or responded to in any meaningful way.
comment by engaging with the student, or group Young children exposed to high-quality lan-
of students, in a sustained effort to reach deeper guage modeling, at home and at school, display
understanding (Pianta et al., 2004). more positive language development (Catts
Most research on feedback has focused on et al., 1999; Justice, 2002; Ninio & Snow, 1999;
quantity rather than the quality. For example, Penno et al., 2002; Reese & Cox, 1999; Schuele,
within a group of elementary, middle, and sec- Rice & Wilcox, 1995; Whitehurst et al., 1988;
ondary Kentucky schools, those identified as suc- Zevenbergen et al., 2003) which, in turn, is asso-
cessful in reducing the achievement gap between ciated with more positive social adjustment
White and African-American students had teach- (Hemphill & Siperstein, 1990; Pianta & Nimetz,
ers who were more likely to provide frequent cor- 1991) and greater reading abilities (Catts et al.,
rective and immediate feedback to students 1999). In one example, Justice, Meier, and
(Meehan, Hughes, & Cavell, 2003); in this regard, Walpole (2005) tested the degree to which
timing was clearly important. In studies in which teacher-child interactions influenced kindergar-
quality of feedback was observed, these interac- ten children’s increases in vocabulary. Results
tions were associated with gains in literacy and suggest that when children are explicitly intro-
language across the preschool and kindergarten duced to new words through providing a defini-
years (Howes et al., 2008) and a closing of the tion (e.g., a marsh is a very wet place where there
achievement gap among first grade students com- are wetlands covered with grasses) and using the
ing from disadvantaged backgrounds (Hamre & new word in a supportive context (e.g., like, we
Pianta, 2005). took a boat through the marsh and we saw lots of
birds and alligators), they show greater vocabu-
Language and Instructional Discourse lary development relative to a comparison group
Children’s ability to navigate the instructional (Justice et al., 2005). In contrast, simple exposure
and social opportunities in classrooms is depen- to new words through book reading was not asso-
dent in large part on their language skills (Catts, ciated with significant vocabulary gains.
Fey, Zhang, & Tomblin, 1999; Fujiki et al., 2002) In the upper grades, language-related interac-
and in turn requires that teachers engage students tions between teachers and students can be char-
in conversations that promote the development of acterized in terms of instructional discourse in
specific language skills such as social language the classroom. Teachers promoting rich instruc-
and pragmatics (Ninio & Snow, 1999; Whitehurst tional discourse do so through verbal interactions
et al., 1988), vocabulary (Justice, 2002; Penno, that foster exchanges of ideas, concepts, and
378 R.C. Pianta et al.

perspectives as well as student control over In an attempt to test the validity of the three-
discourse. Because of the fundamental impor- domain organization of the TTI across multiple
tance of language as both a social medium and a grade levels, Hamre and colleagues (2010) drew
medium for conveying information, teachers’ from a sample of over just under 4,000 preschool
language and their interactions around language to fifth grade classrooms that were a part of sev-
with and among students are fundamental to the eral large national and regional studies. Results
ways in which teacher-student interactions are a of a confirmatory factor analysis suggested ade-
medium for student engagement. quate fit of the three-factor model, and that the fit
of this model was superior to a one- or two-factor
model. This means that all three of the domains
Measuring Teacher-Student Interactions of teacher-child interactions described in the TTI
framework and assessed by CLASS are impor-
When approaching the task of translating the tant for describing teacher-child interactions and
Teaching Through Interactions framework into a understanding the impacts that classrooms have
measurement tool for observing teacher-student on students; no single domain on its own may be
relationships and interactions, we proposed a enough. That is, interactions and relationships
model (Hamre & Pianta, 2007) that organizes between teachers and students reflect a number
teacher-student interactions at four levels, from of facets and features, common across grades and
broad to micro in nature. As described earlier, ages, but nonetheless multidimensional.
the broad domain of emotional supports is We also were interested in the extent to which
defined in terms of three dimensions: classroom classroom processes at different levels (behavior,
climate, teacher sensitivity, and regard for stu- indicator, dimension, domain) predict differen-
dent perspectives. Each dimension is operation- tially to outcomes (gains). Put another way, do
alized at more granular levels of analysis in terms teacher-child interactions encoded at the level of
of a set of specific behavioral indicators that are dimension based on global 1–7 ratings of teacher-
then defined in terms of observable behavioral interaction across a 15–20-min period predict to
interactions. Classroom climate includes observ- student achievement gains better or worse than
able behavioral indicators such as the frequency teacher-student behavior encoded as counts or
and quality of teachers’ affective communica- checklists of discrete teacher interactions toward a
tions with students (further specified in terms of student? This question concerning level of analy-
smiles, positive verbal feedback) as well as the sis reflects major conceptual issues regarding the
degree to which students appear to enjoy spend- actual level at which developmentally meaningful
ing time with one another. This multilevel con- or salient connections between the child and class-
ceptualization of the interactions between a room context occur. Drawing on Sroufe’s (1996)
student and teacher can be observed in actual work and a developmentally informed theory of
classroom environments, moving from broad teacher-child interactions as embedded in a rela-
theoretically based domains (as described above tionship (Pianta, 1999), the TTI framework posits
in the TTI framework) to very specific behav- that the level at which interactions with adults
iors. The resulting articulation of the TTI frame- predicts development is best captured at the level
work into the four levels of description and of dimensions of interaction that take place over
accompanying scaling into examples of interac- time. In the case of the CLASS as a measure, this is
tion from “low” to “high” quality along a seven- operationalized by ratings on a seven-point dimen-
point rating scale is described in the Classroom sion made after 20 min of observation. In prelimi-
Assessment Scoring System, or CLASS (Pianta, nary analyses, we find fairly consistent support
Mashburn, et al., 2008). The CLASS is the mea- for prediction of achievement and social gains at
surement tool for observing and evaluating the level of dimensions of teacher-student interac-
teacher-student interactions derived from the tion (i.e., seven-point ratings) rather than for counts
TTI theoretical framework. or time samplings of discrete teacher behaviors.
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 379

Moreover, we find that discrete teacher behaviors Changing Interactions Between


are highly unstable from moment to moment Teachers and Students in Classrooms
across time, and the frequency of their display is
highly contingent on the nature of the activity. In In this section, we briefly outline results from
terms of a theory of relationships and engage- descriptive research using CLASS that forms the
ment, these results suggest that to capture the basis and rationale for the steps we have taken to
qualities of interactions and relationships that improve teacher-student interactions. It then
foster and reflect engagement, it may be impor- summarizes our approach to professional devel-
tant to conceptualize and assess those interactions opment, which we call MyTeachingPartner,
over episodes and patterns of behavior rather than which is designed explicitly around the CLASS
discrete instances. In other words, the whole may as a focus for changing interactions.
be greater than the sum of the parts.
Conceptually, the reason for expecting that
discrete teacher behaviors would be less strongly Improving Teacher-Student Interactions
related to student growth is that measures of iso-
lated behaviors, by definition, do not capture We posit four levers producing developmental
aspects of the teacher’s behavior that reflects change for teacher-student relationships and
either a response to the child or a calibrated interactions: (1) teachers’ knowledge and cogni-
intent to stimulate development that are both sta- tions related to their interactions with students,
ble across moment-to-moment fluctuations and (2) availability of ongoing relational supports for
reflect reliable differences between individual teachers themselves, (3) teachers’ regular expo-
teachers in their approach (Magnusson & Stattin, sure to individualized feedback about their actual
1998). This ongoing process of calibration is interactions with students, and (4) a standard and
where we believe the focus on interactions maps valid “target” around which to focus efforts to
well onto the discussion of student engagement change interactions. The hypothesis we are test-
and its importance. Not surprisingly, we find that ing in our ongoing work is that intervention pack-
indeed, dimensions of teacher-student interac- ages that activate these levers in a coordinated
tions are rather stable across time and reflect way are most likely to induce and maintain
variance that is reliably located between teachers change, given the systemic nature of teacher-
(Mashburn et al., 2007). student relationships and interactions in class-
The dimensions of interaction assessed by the rooms. Here we describe the theoretical and
CLASS elementary version predict growth in lit- technical features of MyTeachingPartner (MTP),
eracy and math as well as reduced teacher-child an innovative professional development approach
conflict and problem behavior from pre-K through that by design incorporates these four levers for
fifth grade (Hamre & Pianta, 2005; Howes et al., changing teacher-student interactions and rela-
2008; NICHD ECCRN, 2004). The CLASS is tionships. MTP utilizes a collaborative consulta-
one of the most current and widely used stan- tion process and web-based resources to provide
dardized assessments of social and instructional ongoing, classroom-focused in-service training
interactions in classrooms (Hart et al., 2005; across a distance.
NICHD ECCRN, 2002, 2005; McCaslin, Burross, MTP is an ongoing, systematic professional
& Good, 2005). The CLASS-Secondary version, development program for teachers, one feature
or CLASS-S, is explicitly designed to capture of which centers on a supportive consultation
precisely those aspects of classroom interactions relationship, which is sustained via web-based
that we hypothesize above to be resources for interactions in which teachers have the opportu-
adolescent engagement. As such, it builds on and nity to view video of their own and others’ inter-
incorporates all of the strengths of the CLASS actions with students, annotated using the
system at elementary levels, while adding spe- CLASS framework in language that is both at
cific dimensions conceptualized and operational- the level of specific behaviors and indicators
ized to maximize adolescent engagement. but also connects to the level of dimensions.
380 R.C. Pianta et al.

These opportunities are provided in the context teachers’ effective interactions with students shows
of a college course (Hamre et al., 2010), a library positive effects for improvements in teacher-child
of annotated video clips that are exemplars of interactions (assessed by the CLASS) for teachers
highly rated interaction, and a web-mediated with low levels of experience, and consultation
process of ongoing individualized consultation involving ongoing observation, analysis, and
(Pianta, Mashburn, et al., 2008). feedback regarding one’s own behavior shows
The web-based consultation revolves around clear positive impacts on teacher-child interac-
observation-based reflection, and feedback is tions, with particular benefits for teachers in
enacted through a regular cycle of interactions high-need classrooms.
between a teacher and consultant. Every 2 weeks, Interestingly, as we further interpret these
teachers videotape their practices in the class- results, particularly in light of focus group inter-
room and share this footage with consultants. views with teachers, we have started to hypoth-
Together, they then use the CLASS (Pianta et al., esize that the process of changing teacher-child
2007) as a common lens with which to observe relationships and interactions involves entering
and reflect upon aspects of teaching and teacher- the systems (behavioral, psychological, emo-
child interactions that have known links to chil- tional) that teachers use to self-regulate around
dren’s skill development and start by choosing a their interactions with students. In terms of the
dimension of the CLASS that will serve as the psychological processes involved, we find that
basis for consultation and feedback. teachers routinely report the value of the CLASS
MTP consultants provide direct, individual- as a “roadmap” for how to improve their teach-
ized, regular, and systematic feedback to teachers ing, or that CLASS validates and provides a
based on validated, observational assessment of structure for their own explanations, interpreta-
the classroom environment. The MTP consultancy tions, and analysis of their practice. Teachers
process functions by increasing teachers’ knowl- regularly note that having a common language
edge and skills to observe the qualities of their and lens for their interactions with students that
interactions with students and the contingencies is directly, overtly, and explicitly articulated in a
involved, and their awareness of the meanings of set of professional development resources is of
these interactions in terms of their contributions to great benefit to them as it grounds those resources
motivational, relational, and competence-enhanc- in the realities of their practice and experience.
ing processes. The process also encourages reflec- Although teachers describe their interactions
tion on the teachers’ own personal motivations with reference to the more molar dimensions of
and tendencies in these interactions and their the CLASS framework, what is of most use to
impact on interactive behaviors in an effort to them is the very detailed and explicit descrip-
internalize change and sustain it. tions of interaction at the levels of behavioral
Recent controlled evaluations of these profes- indicators and behavioral interactions. Our
sional development assets demonstrate several hypothesis is that this more granular level of
benefits for improving the quality of teachers’ analysis meshes well with the psychological and
interactions with children (Pianta, Belsky, et al., behavioral systems that teachers and students
2008), children’s attentiveness and literacy out- use to calibrate their engagement with one
comes in pre-K (Mashburn et al., 2008), as well another and with the focus of classroom activi-
as student reports and observation of engagement ties. We plan further tests of this idea in subse-
in secondary classrooms, and student test scores quent studies.
(Allen et al., 2010). Preliminary evaluations of a
course that focuses on teachers’ learning of
CLASS dimensions and indicators show positive Conclusions and Future Directions
effects on teachers’ knowledge and beliefs about
teaching and significant effects on their interac- Although classrooms are complex social systems
tive behaviors in the classroom. Opportunities and student-teacher relationships and interactions
for observing annotated video exemplars of other are also complex, multicomponent systems, we
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 381

posit that the nature and quality of interactions Relatedly, a molar, pattern-oriented view of
between teachers and children are fundamental relationships and interactions appears most help-
to understanding student engagement, can be ful when using assessments to capture classroom
assessed through standardized observation meth- inputs related to engagement. Approaches that are
ods, and can be changed by providing teachers highly focused on occurrences of granular, dis-
knowledge about developmental processes rele- crete behaviors captured in isolation or extracted
vant for classroom interactions and personalized from the ongoing behavioral stream are less likely
feedback/support about their interactive behav- to yield interpretable or meaningful findings. This
iors and cues. does not mean that a focus on specific teacher
A theory of classroom settings must be pre- behaviors is not of use; in fact, in our professional
mised on an understanding of the developmental development work, we are highly focused on
significance of those settings’ influence on chil- analysis of teachers’ specific behaviors but always
dren and youth and the mechanisms of these in reference to broader dimensions and patterns of
effects. Once that knowledge base is established, interaction. It appears important to us that pro-
then theory can move to how those mechanisms grams of research conceptualize and assess rela-
(in this case, student-teacher interactions) them- tionships and interactions in coherent systems that
selves can be changed. In this chapter, we focused reflect multiple levels of analysis.
on the theoretical and empirical links between Finally, we believe it is critical to subject
interactions and engagement and presented an hypotheses to experimental tests in research on
approach to intervention designed to increase the classroom processes. Classrooms are indeed
quality of such interactions and in turn increase complex, and there is no shortage of description
student engagement and, ultimately, learning and and theoretical narrative available. In too many
development. Recognizing general principles of cases, descriptive studies simply confirm the nar-
development in complex systems, a theory of the rative and theory and do not provide tests that
classroom as a setting for development and a could actually disconfirm hypotheses and helps
theory of change specific to this social setting are simplify complexity into actionable models. In a
the ultimate goals of this work. Engagement, in literature focused so heavily on processes—
this context, is both an outcome in its own right engagement, relationships, and interaction—it
and a mediator of impacts that teachers have on might be even more important for research
student outcomes through their interactions with designs to have the capacity to disconfirm hypoth-
children and youth. In light of this discussion, we eses or speculation. Thus, we posit that advances
offer the following suggestions or directions for in both theory and intervention concerning
further research in this area. engagement and relational processes can benefit
First, it is apparent that researchers must dis- from a dialectical balance in research design—
tinguish, in their conceptual models and empiri- experiments and rich description of processes.
cal work, the positioning of engagement in the
causal chain—as an input to learning, a mediator
situated between experience and outcomes, or as References
an outcome in its own right. Failure to specify
this role can easily lead to confusion and misin- Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, D.
terpretation. In the context of a focus on interac- (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study
of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tions and relationships, we have focused on
Allen, J. P., & Allen, C. W. (2009). Escaping the endless
engagement as a mediator and potential outcome. adolescence: How we can help our teenagers grow up
By specifying the role of engagement in a puta- before they grow old. New York: Random House.
tive causal chain, investigators can then more Allen, J. P., Gregory, A., Mikami, A., Lun, J., Hamre, B.
K., & Pianta, R. C. (2010). Observations of effective
strategically and systematically confirm or dis-
secondary school teaching: Predicting student
confirm hypotheses rather than report assort- achievement with the CLASS-S. Manuscript in prepa-
ments of correlations. ration, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA.
382 R.C. Pianta et al.

Allen, J. P., Philliber, S., Herrling, S., & Kuperminc, G. P. Brophy, J., & Evertson, C. (1976). Learning from teach-
(1997). Preventing teen pregnancy and academic fail- ing: A developmental perspective. Boston: Allyn &
ure: Experimental evaluation of a developmentally Bacon.
based approach. Child Development, 68(4), 729–742. Brophy, J. E., & Good, T. L. (1986). Teacher behavior and
Aratani, L. (2006, July 13). Upper grades, lower reading student achievement. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Hand-
skills. The Washington Post, B1. book of research on teaching (3rd ed., pp. 328–375).
Arnold, D. H., McWilliams, L., & Arnold, E. H. (1998). New York: Macmillan.
Teacher discipline and child misbehavior in day care: Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge,
Untangling causality with correlational data. MA: Harvard University Press.
Developmental Psychology, 34, 276–287. Bryk, A. S., & Driscoll, M. (1988). The high school as
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, a community: Contextual influences and conse-
C. (1996). Multifaceted impact of self-efficacy beliefs quences for teachers. Madison, WI: University of
on academic functioning. Child Development, 67(3), Wisconsin, National Center on Effective Secondary
1206–1222. Schools.
Barzun, J. (2000). From dawn to decadence: 500 years of Bryk, A. S., Lee, V. E., & Holland, P. B. (1993). Catholic
western cultural life 1500 to the present. London: schools and the common good. Cambridge, MA:
Harper Collins. Harvard University Press.
Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. (2002). Burnett, P. C. (2003). The impact of teacher feedback on
Effects of social exclusion on cognitive processes: student self-talk and self-concept in reaching and
Anticipated aloneness reduces intelligent thought. mathematics. Journal of Classroom Interaction, 38(1),
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 11–16.
817–827. Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2005).
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1996). Interpersonal rela- Effects of variation in teacher organization on class-
tionships in the school environment and children’s room functioning. Journal of School Psychology,
early school adjustment: The role of teachers and 43(1), 61–85.
peers. In K. Wentzel & J. H. Juvonen (Eds.), Social Carbonaro, W. J., & Gamoran, A. (2002). The production of
motivation: Understanding children’s school adjust- achievement inequality in high school English. American
ment. New York: Cambridge University Press. Educational Research Journal, 39, 801–827.
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Children’s interper- Carver, S. M., & Klahr, D. (Eds.). (2001). Cognition and
sonal behaviors and the teacher-child relationship. instruction: 25 years of progress. Mahwah, NJ:
Developmental Psychology, 34, 934–946. Erlbaum.
Bloom, B. S., Engelhart, M. D., Furst, E. J., Hill, W. H., & Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B.
Krathwohl, D. R. (1956). Taxonomy of educational (1999). Language basis of reading and reading dis-
objectives: The cognitive domain. New York: abilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation.
Longman. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(4), 331–361.
Bohn, C. M., Roehrig, A. D., & Pressley, M. (2004). The Coker, H., Medley, D. M., & Soar, R. S. (1980). How valid
first days of school in the classrooms of two more are expert opinions about effective teaching? The Phi
effective and four less effective primary-grades Delta Kappan, 62, 131–134.
teachers. The Elementary School Journal, 104(4), Collins, W. A., & Repinski, D. J. (1994). Relationships
269–287. during adolescence: Continuity and change in inter-
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Attachment, Vol. 1). personal perspective. In R. Montemayor, G. Adams, &
New York: Basic Books. T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Personal relationships during
Bowman, B., & Stott, F. (1994). Understanding develop- adolescence (pp. 7–36). San Francisco: Sage
ment in a cultural context: The challenge for teachers. Publications.
In B. Mallory & R. New (Eds.), Diversity and devel- Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence,
opmentally appropriate practices: Challenges for autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of
early childhood education (pp. 19–34). New York: self-system processes. In M. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe
Teachers College Press. (Eds.), Self processes in development: Minnesota sym-
Bransford, J., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (Eds.). posium on child psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 43–77).
(1999). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
and school. Washington, DC: National Academy Connor, C. M., Son, S., & Hindman, A. H. (2005).
Press. Teacher qualifications, classroom practices, family
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology characteristics, and preschool experience: Complex
of developmental processes. In W. Damon & R. M. effects on first graders’ vocabulary and early reading
Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology outcomes. Journal of School Psychology, 43,
(Theoretical models of human development 5th ed., 343–375.
Vol. 1, pp. 993–1029). New York: Wiley. Crosnoe, R. (2000). Friendships in childhood and adoles-
Brophy, J. (1986). Teacher influences on student achieve- cence: The life course and new directions. Social
ment. American Psychologist, 41(10), 1069–1077. Psychology Quarterly, 63, 377–391.
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 383

Crosnoe, R. (2001). Academic orientation and parental Good, T. L., & Grouws, D. A. (1977). A process-product
involvement in education during high school. study in fourth-grade mathematics classrooms. Journal
Sociology of Education, 74, 210–230. of Teacher Education, 28(3), 49–54.
Crosnoe, R., Johnson, M. K., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2004). Good, T. L., & Grouws, D. A. (1979). The Missouri math-
Intergenerational bonding in school: The behavioral ematics effectiveness project in fourth-grade class-
and contextual correlates of student-teacher relation- rooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71,
ships. Sociology of Education, 77(1), 60–81. 355–362.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Schneider, B. (2000). Becoming Gutman, L. M., & Sulzby, E. (2000). The role of autono-
adult: How teenagers prepare for the world of work. my-support versus control in the emergent writing
New York: Basic Books. behaviors of African American kindergarten children.
Davis, E. A., & Miyake, N. (2004). Explorations of scaf- Reading Research and Instruction, 39, 170–184.
folding in complex classroom systems. The Journal of Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher-child
the Learning Sciences, 13(3), 265–272. relationships and the trajectory of children’s school
de Kruif, R. E. L., McWilliam, R. A., Ridley, S. M., & outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development,
Wakely, M. B. (2000). Classification of teachers’ inter- 72(2), 625–638.
action behaviors in early childhood classrooms. Early Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Can instructional
Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 247–268. and emotional support in the first grade classroom
Dohrn, E., & Bryan, T. (1994). Attributional instruction. make a difference for children at risk of school fail-
Teaching Exceptional Children, 26, 61–63. ure? Child Development, 76(5), 949–967.
Donovan, M. S., & Bransford, J. D. (2005). How students Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2007). Learning opportu-
learn: History, mathematics, and science in the class- nities in preschool and early elementary classrooms.
room. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. In R. C. Pianta, M. J. Cox, & K. L. Snow (Eds.),
Dornbusch, S. M., Glasgow, K. L., & Lin, I.-C. (1996). School readiness and the transition to kindergarten in
The social structure of schooling. Annual Review of the era of accountability (pp. 49–84). Baltimore:
Psychology, 47, 401–429. Brookes.
Eccles, J., Lord, S., & Midgley, C. (1991). What are we Hamre, B. K., Pianta, R. C., Burchinal, M., & Downer,
doing to early adolescents? The impacts of educational J. T. (2010, March). A course on supporting early lan-
contexts on early adolescents. American Educational guage and literacy development through effective
Journal, 99, 521–542. teacher-child interactions: Effects on teacher beliefs,
Eccles, J. S., Lord, S. E., Roeser, R. W., Barber, B. L., & knowledge and practice. Paper presented at the annual
Jozefowicz, D. M. H. (1997). The association of school meeting of the Society for Research on Educational
transitions in early adolescence with developmental Effectiveness, Washington, DC.
trajectories during high school. In J. Schulenberg, J. L. Hamre, B. K., Pianta, R. C., Downer, J. T., & Mashburn,
Maggs, & K. Hurrelmann (Eds.), Health risks and A. J. (2005). Teachers’ perceptions of conflict with
developmental transitions during adolescence young students: Looking beyond problem behaviors.
(pp. 283–321). New York: Cambridge University Press. Social Development, 17(1), 115–136.
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Hamre, B. K., Pianta, R. C., Mashburn, A. J., & Downer,
Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development J. T. (2010). Building a science of classrooms:
during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment Application of the CLASS framework in over 4,000
fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools and in U.S. early childhood and elementary classrooms.
families. American Psychologist, 48(2), 90–101. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Emmer, E. T., & Strough, L. (2001). Classroom manage- Hart, P., Stroot, S., Yinger, R., & Smith, S. (2005). Meeting
ment: A critical part of educational psychology, with the teacher education accountability challenge:
implications for teacher education. Educational A focus on novice and experienced teacher studies.
Psychologist, 36(2), 103–112. Mount Vernon, OH: Teacher Quality Partnership.
Evertson, C., Emmer, E., Sanford, J., & Clements, B. Harter, S. (1996). Teacher and classmate influences on scho-
(1983). Improving classroom management: An experi- lastic motivation, self-esteem, and level of voice in ado-
ment in elementary classrooms. The Elementary lescents. In J. Juvonen & K. Wentzel (Eds.), Social
School Journal, 84, 173–188. motivation: Understanding children’s school adjustment
Evertson, C., & Harris, A. (1999). Support for managing (pp. 11–42). New York: Cambridge University Press.
learning-centered classrooms: The Classroom Hemphill, L., & Siperstein, G. M. (1990). Conversational
Organization and Management Program. In H. J. competence and peer response to mildly retarded chil-
Freiberg (Ed.), Beyond behaviorism: Changing the dren. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 1–7.
classroom management paradigm (pp. 59–74). Boston: Howes, C. (2000). Socio-emotional classroom climate in
Allyn & Bacon. child care, child–teacher relationships and children’s
Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., & Clarke, D. (2002). Emotion second grade peer relations. Social Development, 9,
regulation in children with specific language impairment. 191–204.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Pianta, R., Bryant, D.,
33, 102–111. Early, D., Clifford, R., & Barbarin, O. (2008). Ready
384 R.C. Pianta et al.

to learn? Children’s pre-academic achievement in resources for aggressive children. Child Development,
pre-kindergarten programs. Early Childhood Research 74, 1145–1157.
Quarterly, 23, 27–50. Mueller, C., & Dweck, C. (1998). Praise for intelligence
Howes, C., Hamilton, C. E., & Matheson, C. C. (1994). can undermine children’s motivation and performance.
Children’s relationships with peers: Differential asso- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1),
ciations with aspects of the teacher-child relationship. 33–52.
Child Development, 65, 253–263. National Center for Education Statistics. (2003). The con-
Hughes, J. W., Zhang, D., & Hill, C. R. (2006). Peer dition of education 2003. Washington, DC: U.S.
assessment of normative and individual teacher-stu- Department of Education, Institute of Education
dent support predict social acceptance and engage- Sciences.
ment among low-achieving children. Journal of School National Research Council. (2002). Achieving high edu-
Psychology, 43, 447–463. cational standards for all. Washington, DC: National
Justice, J. M. (2002). Word exposure conditions and pre- Academy Press.
schoolers’ novel word learning during shared story- National Research Council. (2004). Engaging schools:
book reading. Reading Psychology, 23(2), 87–106. Fostering high school students’ motivation to learn.
Justice, L. M., Mashburn, A. J., Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
R. C. (2008). Quality of language and literacy instruc- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network [ECCRN].
tion in preschool classrooms serving at-risk pupils. (2002). The relation of global first-grade classroom
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 51–68. environment to structural classroom features and
Justice, L., Meier, J., & Walpole, S. (2005). Learning new teacher and student behaviors. The Elementary School
words from storybooks: An efficacy study with at-risk Journal, 102(5), 367–387.
kindergartners. Language, Speech, and Hearing NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2003).
Services in Schools, 36, 17–32. Social functioning in first grade: Prediction from
Ladd, G. W., Birch, S. H., & Buhs, E. S. (1999). Children’s home, child care and concurrent school experience.
social and scholastic lives in kindergarten: Related spheres Child Development, 74, 1639–1662.
of influence? Child Development, 70, 1373–1400. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004).
Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1992). Maltreated children’s Social functioning in first grade: Associations with
reports of relatedness to their teachers. In R. C. Pianta earlier home and child care predictors and with current
(Ed.), Relationships between children and non-paren- classroom experiences. Child Development, 75,
tal adults: New directions in child development 1639–1662.
(pp. 81–108). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005).
Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (1998). Person-context A day in third grade: A large-scale study of classroom
interaction theory. In W. Damon & R. M. Learner quality and teacher and student behavior. The
(Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Theoretical Elementary School Journal, 105, 305–323.
models of human development 5th ed., Vol. 1, Ninio, A., & Snow, C. E. (1999). The development of
pp. 685–760). New York: Wiley. pragmatics: Learning to use language appropriately. In
Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of
activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high child language acquisition (pp. 347–383). San Diego,
school years. American Educational Research Journal, CA: Academic.
37(1), 153–184. Nye, B., Konstantopoulos, S., & Hedges, L. (2004). How
Mashburn, A. J., Hamre, B. K., Downer, J. T., & Pianta, R. large are teacher effects? Educational Evaluation and
C. (2007). Teacher and classroom characteristics asso- Policy Analysis, 26, 237–257.
ciated with teachers’ ratings of pre-kindergartners’ Penno, J. F., Wilkinson, A. G., & Moore, D. W. (2002).
relationships and behavior. Journal of Pyscho- Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and
educational Assessment, 24, 367–380. repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the
Mashburn, A. J., Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B. K., Downer, J. Matthew effect? Journal of Educational Psychology,
T., Barbarin, O., Bryant, D., Burchinal, M., Early, D., 94, 23–33.
& Howes, C. (2008). Pre-k program standards and Pianta, R. C. (1999). Enhancing relationships between
children’s development of academic, language, and children and teachers. Washington, DC: American
social skills. Child Development, 79, 732–749. Psychological Association.
Mayer, R. E. (2002). Rote versus meaningful learning. Pianta, R. C., Belsky, J., Houts, R., Morrison, F., & The
Theory into Practice, 41, 226–233. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2007).
McCaslin, M., Burross, H. L., & Good, T. L. (2005, Opportunities to learn in America’s elementary class-
January 2). Change and continuity in student achieve- rooms. Science, 315, 1795–1796.
ment from grades 3 to 5: A policy dilemma. Education Pianta, R. C., Belsky, J., Vandergrift, N., Houts, R.,
Policy Analysis Archives, 13(1). Retrieved February 2, Morrison, F., & The NICHD Early Child Care Research
2006, from http://epaa.asu.edu/epaa/v13n1/. Network. (2008). Classroom effects on children’s
Meehan, B. T., Hughes, J. N., & Cavell, T. A. (2003). achievement trajectories in elementary school. American
Teacher-student relationships as compensatory Educational Research Journal, 45(2), 365–397.
17 Teacher-Student Relationships and Engagement… 385

Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B. K., & Mintz, S. L. (2010). The social-emotional development: A summary of research
CLASS-secondary manual. Unpublished measure, findings. The Elementary School Journal, 100,
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA. 443–471.
Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B. K., & Stuhlman, M. (2003). Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive
Relationships between teachers and children. In W. development in social context. New York: Oxford
Reynolds & G. Miller (Eds.), Comprehensive hand- University Press.
book of psychology (Educational psychology, Vol. 7, Romberg, T. A., Carpenter, T. P., & Dremock, F. (2005).
pp. 199–234). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Understanding mathematics and science matters.
Pianta, R. C., La Paro, K. M., & Hamre, B. K. (2004). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Classroom assessment scoring system [CLASS]. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination
Unpublished measure, University of Virginia, theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
Charlottesville, VA. social development, and well-being. American
Pianta, R. C., La Paro, K. M., Payne, C., Cox, M., & Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78.
Bradley, R. (2002). The relation of kindergarten class- Ryan, R. M., Stiller, J. D., & Lynch, J. H. (1994).
room environment to teacher, family, and school char- Representations of relationships to teachers, parents,
acteristics and child outcomes. The Elementary School and friends as predictors of academic motivation and
Journal, 102(3), 225–238. self-esteem. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14(2),
Pianta, R. C., Mashburn, A. J., Downer, J. T., Hamre, 226–249.
B. K., & Justice, L. (2008). Effects of web-mediated Sarason, S. B. (1982). The culture of the school and the
professional development resources on teacher-child problem of change (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
interactions in pre-kindergarten classrooms. Early Schuele, C. M., Rice, M. L., & Wilcox, K. A. (1995).
Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(4), 431–451. Redirects: A strategy to increase peer interactions.
Pianta, R. C., & Nimetz, S. (1991). Relationships between Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28,
children and teachers: Associations with classroom 1319–1333.
and home behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Shouse, R. C. (1996). Academic press and sense of com-
Psychology, 12, 379–393. munity: Conflict, congruence, and implications for
Pianta, R. C., Steinberg, M. S., & Rollins, K. B. (1995). student achievement. Social Psychology of Education,
The first two years of school: Teacher-child rela- 1(1), 47–68.
tionships and deflections in children’s classroom Silver, R. B., Measelle, J., Essex, M., & Armstrong, J. M.
adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 7, (2005). Trajectories of externalizing behavior prob-
295–312. lems in the classroom: Contributions of child charac-
Public Agenda. (1997). Getting by: What American teen- teristics, family characteristics, and the teacher-child
agers really think about their schools. New York: relationship during the school transition. Journal of
Public Agenda. School Psychology, 43, 39–60.
Reese, E., & Cox, A. (1999). Quality of adult book- Skibbe, L., Behnke, M., & Justice, L. M. (2004). Parental
reading style affects children’s emergent literacy. scaffolding of children’s phonological awareness
Developmental Psychology, 35, 20–28. skills: Interactions between mothers and their pre-
Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K., schoolers with language difficulties. Communication
Harris, K. M., Jones, J., Tabor, J., et al. (1997). Disorders Quarterly, 25(4), 189–203.
Protecting adolescents from harm: Findings from the Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the
National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and
Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, student engagement across the school year. Journal of
823–832. Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581.
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Early, D. M., & Cox, M. J. (2002). Skinner, E. A., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Connell, J. P.
Early behavioral attributes and teachers’ sensitivity as (1998). Individual differences and the development of
predictors of competent behavior in the kindergarten perceived control. Monographs of the Society for
classroom. Journal of Applied Developmental Research in Child Development, 63(2–3).
Psychology, 23(4), 451–470. Soar, R., & Soar, R. (1979). Emotional climate and man-
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., La Paro, K. M., Downer, J. T., & agement. In P. Peterson & H. Walberg (Eds.), Research
Pianta, R. C. (2005). The contribution of classroom on teaching: Concepts, findings, and implications
setting and quality of instruction to children’s behavior (pp. 97–119). Berkeley, CA: McCutchan.
in the kindergarten classroom. The Elementary School Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The orga-
Journal, 105(4), 377–394. nization of emotional life in the early years. Cambridge,
Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (1998). UK: Cambridge University Press.
Academic and emotional functioning in early adoles- Stallings, J. (1975). Implementation and child effects of
cence: Longitudinal relations, patterns, and prediction teaching practices in follow through classrooms.
by experience in middle school. Development and Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Psychopathology, 10(2), 321–352. Development, 40(7–8), Serial No. 163.
Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (2000). Steinberg, L., Brown, B. B., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1996).
School as a context of early adolescents’ academic and Beyond the classroom: Why school reform has failed
386 R.C. Pianta et al.

and what parents need to do. New York: Simon and Vygotsky, L. S. (1991). Genesis of the higher mental
Schuster. functions. In P. Light, S. Sheldon, & M. Woodhead
Taylor, B. M., Pearson, P. D., Peterson, D. S., & Rodriguez, (Eds.), Learning to think (pp. 32–41). Florence, KY:
M. C. (2003). Reading growth in high-poverty class- Taylor & Frances/Routledge.
rooms: The influence of teacher practices that encour- Wentzel, K. (1998). Social relationships and motivation in
age cognitive engagement in literacy learning. The middle school: The role of parents, teachers, and peers.
Elementary School Journal, 104, 3–28. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2), 202–209.
Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Wharton-McDonald, R., Pressley, M., & Hampston, J. M.
Social exclusion causes self-defeating behavior. (1998). Literacy instruction in nine first-grade class-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), rooms: Teacher characteristics and student achieve-
606–615. ment. The Elementary School Journal, 99(2),
Valeski, T., & Stipek, D. (2001). Young children’s feelings 101–128.
about school. Child Development, 72, 1198–1213. Whitehurst, G. J., Falco, F. L., Lonigan, C. J., Fischel,
van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Sagi, A., & Lambermon, M. W. E. J. E., DeBaryshe, B. D., Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., &
(1992). The multiple caretaker paradox: Data from Caulfield, M. (1988). Accelerating language develop-
Holland and Israel. In R. C. Pianta (Ed.), Beyond the ment through picture book reading. Developmental
parent: The role of other adults in children’s live (New Psychology, 24, 552–559.
directions for child development, Vol. 57, pp. 5–24). Williams, W. M., Blythe, T., & White, N. (2002). Practical
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. intelligence for school: Developing metacognitive
Veenman, M. V. J., Kok, R., & Blöte, A. W. (2005). The sources of achievement in adolescence. Developmental
relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills Review, 22(2), 162–210.
in early adolescence. Instructional Science, 33(3), Zevenbergen, A. A., Whitehurst, G. J., & Zevenbergen,
193–211. J. A. (2003). Effects of a shared-reading intervention
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and society: The develop- on the inclusion of evaluative devices in narratives of
ment of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: children from low-income families. Journal of Applied
Harvard University Press. Developmental Psychology, 24, 1–15.
The Role of Peer Relationships
in Student Academic and 18
Extracurricular Engagement

Jaana Juvonen, Guadalupe Espinoza,


and Casey Knifsend

Abstract
Friends and other peer relationships can motivate students to engage in
school work as well as in extracurricular activities. To understand when
and how peers matter, research on the positive and negative engagement
“effects” of friends, peer support, and socially marginalizing experiences,
such as peer rejection and bullying, is reviewed. The chapter starts with a
brief summary of research demonstrating the links between school belong-
ing and academic engagement and extracurricular involvement. The ways
in which selection of friends and the influence of friends, quality of friend-
ships, and type of friendship support (academic or emotional) are related
to academic engagement and extracurricular involvement in school are
then discussed. Studies examining whether the number of friends or the
size of peer network is related to school engagement are also included.
The chapter ends with a discussion about future research needs in relation
to the role of peer relationships and student engagement, and implications
for school policies (e.g., academic tracking, grade retention, and extracur-
ricular practices).

Peers are a major part of schooling. Given the have friends and feel socially connected and
amount of time students spend with their class- supported at school, one would expect these
mates and friends in school, they are likely to be factors to predispose them to feel positively
influenced by them. Moreover, when students toward academic work and other school activi-
ties. The assumptions guiding this review are first,
that friends and other peer relationships can moti-
J. Juvonen, Ph.D. (*) vate students to engage in school work as well as
Department of Psychology, University of California, in extracurricular activities. However, we recog-
Los Angeles, CA, USA nize that some peers and social experiences in
e-mail: j_juvonen@yahoo.com
school can also discourage engagement. To be
G. Espinoza, M.A. • C. Knifsend, M.A. able to understand when and how peers matter,
Department of Psychology, University of California,
Los Angeles, CA, USA
we review research on the positive and negative
e-mail: g.espinoza@ucla.edu; cknifsend@ucla.edu “effects” of friendships and peer support, and

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 387


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_18, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
388 J. Juvonen et al.

belonging and academic engagement and extra-


Peer relationships Sense of belonging Student engagement
curricular involvement. We then proceed to
Fig. 18.1 Conceptual framework guiding this review review the ways in which selection of friends
and the influence of friends is related to stu-
dents’ school engagement. Quality of friend-
socially marginalizing experiences, such as peer ships and type of friendship support (academic
rejection and bullying, on student engagement. or emotional) are discussed. Studies examining
Our second guiding assumption is that positive the relationships between number of friends and
relationships with schoolmates facilitate a sense the size of peer networks and student engage-
of belonging to school. We presume that both peer ment are also reviewed. Research on students
relationships and belonging to school are related who are rejected or bullied by their peers shows,
to student engagement, with peer relationships in turn, the ways in which negative social expe-
contributing to both the sense of belonging and riences may alienate students from school and
student engagement, as indicated in Fig. 18.1. possibly increase the chances of their dropping
In our review, terms referring to school belong- out. The chapter ends with a discussion about
ing (i.e., sense of connection) and peer relation- future research needs in relation to the role of
ships are used broadly. For example, we use peer relationships and student engagement,
“belongingness” and “connectedness” inter- and implications from the work already done on
changeably. The term “peer relationships” is used this topic for school policies (e.g., academic
as a superordinate construct to refer to close tracking, grade retention, and extracurricular
friendships (i.e., relationships characterized by practices).
mutual liking) as well as to peer group affiliations
(i.e., less tight relationships united by common
interests and activities). School Belonging
We also use a broad definition of student
engagement, focusing primarily on observable Research on school belonging is based on the
indicators, such as attendance and classroom assumption that environments characterized by
participation. Although we primarily focus on caring and supportive relationships facilitate stu-
engagement behaviors as a means to achieve dent engagement (e.g., Brand, Felner, Shim,
good grades, we also refer to findings regarding Seitsinger, & Dumas, 2003; Felner & Felner,
academic performance as an indication of stu- 1989; Goodenow & Grady, 1993; Voelkl, 1997).
dent engagement. School-based extracurricular Consequently, motivation and achievement are
involvement in sports, arts, and other activities is presumed to be undermined when students feel
included in this review for two reasons. First, by unsupported and disconnected from others (e.g.,
assessing engagement in both academic and non- Becker & Luthar, 2002; Finn, 1989, 1993). A par-
academic activities, we are able to determine ticularly strong association between peer accep-
whether peer relationships operate in similar ways tance and school belonging (Adelabu, 2007)
across these two domains. Second, although extra- suggests that school-based relationships are criti-
curricular participation mostly involves nonaca- cal. Although both relationships with teachers
demic activities, such involvement is related to and peers are likely to matter (Furrer & Skinner,
student engagement in academic activities, includ- 2003), the need to “fit in” with one’s peers is
ing school attendance (e.g., Mahoney, 2000). especially pronounced during adolescence
Thus, we review how peer relationships affect and (LaFontana & Cillessen, 2010). Hence, it is not
are affected by extracurricular involvement in surprising that much of the existing research on
ways that can facilitate academic engagement. school belonging has focused on middle and high
We start the chapter with a brief summary of school students. Yet, school belonging matters as
research demonstrating the links between school early as elementary school.
18 The Role of Peer Relationships in Student Academic and Extracurricular Engagement 389

Does School Belonging Promote homework completion, exam preparation, and


Academic Engagement? better school attendance. Consistent with these
findings, analyses of the National Longitudinal
Capitalizing on a large sample of over 4,000 stu- Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) of
dents across 24 elementary schools, Battistich, 20,000 ethnically diverse students from 132 sec-
Solomon, Kim, Watson, and Schaps (1995) inves- ondary schools showed that higher levels of school
tigated the association between students’ sense of belonging were associated with fewer school
school community (e.g., perceptions of caring absences (Anderman, 2002). A large-scale longi-
and supportive school-based relationships) and a tudinal study of Australian secondary school stu-
range of measures tapping attitudes, motivation, dents, in turn, demonstrated that low school
and achievement. Using hierarchical linear mod- connectedness decreased the likelihood of stu-
eling techniques that allow examination of stu- dents finishing school (Bond et al., 2007).
dents nested within schools, the findings revealed In sum, these findings suggest that students’
that a greater sense of school community was school belonging, which we presume to be inte-
associated with higher levels of class enjoyment, grally linked with school-based peer relation-
lower levels of work avoidance, and higher math- ships (although student school belonging also
ematics scores. Generally, stronger associations encompasses relationships with adults in the
were documented in schools serving the most school) (e.g., Hamm & Faircloth, 2005), is an
economically disadvantaged families, suggesting important factor associated with engagement in
that school belonging might be particularly academic work especially in secondary school.
important for students from educationally and However, we are not in the position to conclude
financially disadvantaged homes. that school belonging causes students to engage.
In one of the earliest studies on school belong- The association between school belonging and
ing in middle school, Goodenow and Grady engagement may operate in both directions, pos-
(1993) demonstrated that a strong sense of school sibly in a mutually reinforcing manner. That is,
belonging was associated with increased aca- the more engaged students are, the stronger their
demic engagement among an ethnically diverse sense of belonging; and the more strongly they
sample of students. Based on self-report mea- feel they belong, the more actively they engage
sures, a positive association between school academically. In the next section, we turn to
belonging, the importance of schoolwork, and extracurricular engagement to review research on
persistence with schoolwork was observed. sense of belonging and participation in voluntary
Sampling middle schools serving predominantly activities in school.
White youth from working class families, Roeser,
Midgley, and Urdan (1996), in turn, showed that Is Extracurricular Participation a Way
school belonging was associated with higher levels to Strengthen School Belonging?
of academic performance. The association was A handful of survey and qualitative studies have
robust inasmuch as other relevant motivational examined the association between school belong-
constructs (e.g., goal structures fostered by the ing and students’ engagement in extracurricular
school, personal achievement goal orientations) activities. Students with a stronger sense of
were taken into account in the analyses. school belonging are more likely to engage in
The link between school belonging and student activities, such as after-school sports or extracur-
engagement has been studied most extensively ricular academic programs. Sampling an ethni-
among high school students. Focusing on a pre- cally diverse group of seventh through twelfth
dominantly Latino sample of urban high school grade students, Brown and Evans (2002) showed
seniors, Sánchez, Colón, and Esparza (2005) doc- that extracurricular activity participation was sig-
umented that school belonging was associated nificantly associated with greater school connec-
with more frequent classroom participation, tion, which was measured with school belonging
390 J. Juvonen et al.

as one of its main dimensions. Although the


authors only tested a direct path from extracur-
Peer Selection and Socialization
ricular activities to school connection, they pos-
Children tend to have relationships and affiliate
ited that participation in extracurricular activities
with similar others (Hallinan, 1983). That is, stu-
facilitates positive school-related experiences,
dents engaged in classwork form friendships with
which in turn, facilitate school belonging and
engaged classmates, whereas students who are
commitment to school. In a study using daily
not so engaged are friends with similarly disen-
phone interviews of African-American students
gaged peers. Given the similarities between
in sixth to ninth grade, Dotterer, McHale, and
friends, it is not surprising that friendships amplify
Crouter (2007) found that the more time students
students’ school-related behaviors (Dishion,
spent on extracurricular activities, the more
Spracklen, Andrews, & Patterson, 1996; Mounts
strongly they bonded with school. Such a positive
& Steinberg, 1995). In other words, engaged stu-
association may, however, merely indicate that
dents get more involved in academic work,
youth who are school-oriented and who feel that
whereas disengaged students become alienated
they belong and fit in at school spend time in
from school-related activities. Whether these peer
school with peers sharing similar interests.
“effects” are due to selection of friends, or their
Research utilizing mixed methods provides
influence – or both – is less clear (Kandel, 1996).
some insights into whether extracurricular involve-
ment in fact affects school belonging or whether
those who feel they belong are more likely to par-
ticipate in activities provided by school. Barnett Characteristics of Friends
(2006) surveyed female high school students and the Relation with Academic
before, and interviewed them after, they received Engagement
notification of whether they had been selected to
the cheer or dance team following competitive try- Perhaps the best evidence for peer influence on
outs. In the initial surveys, all applicants reported academic engagement comes from studies on
liking school and wanting to be at school, which is peer networks (Cairns, Cairns, & Neckerman,
partly tapping into the sense of school belonging. 1989; Kindermann, 1993; Kindermann,
The girls who made the team maintained their McCollam, & Gibson, 1996). Kindermann et al.
high levels of school liking, whereas school liking (1996) found that when students were members
significantly decreased among the unsuccessful of groups with high average academic engage-
aspirants not only the day after the decision was ment, their own individual academic engagement
made, but also 2 months after the decision. When improved over time. The opposite effect was
interviewed, one of the nonselected girls explained obtained for members of groups with low aca-
that one of the main reasons why she wanted to be demic engagement profiles. In spite of relatively
on the dance team was “to find a way to be con- high turnover of specific members across the
nected with my school.” Thus, individuals may school year, the groups’ engagement “profiles,”
have different reasons to pursue extracurricular or overall orientation toward school work,
activities. remained stable. This finding highlights that stu-
Research shows positive links between school dents select peer groups, and groups accept mem-
belonging and academic engagement, such as bers based on similarities.
classroom participation and school attendance, Besides academic orientation, a wide range of
and involvement in extracurricular activities. characteristics of friends is related to academic
Although the rest of our review is based on the engagement. A survey study of almost 1,000
premise that positive peer relationships are impor- adolescents examined how a set of academic,
tant in facilitating a sense of school belonging, it social, and mental health attributes of friends
becomes evident that not all peer relationships are was related to students’ academic engagement
related with increased levels of engagement. and performance from seventh to eighth grade
18 The Role of Peer Relationships in Student Academic and Extracurricular Engagement 391

(Cook, Deng, & Morgano, 2007). Cook and behaviors (e.g., being disrespectful of teachers)
colleagues discovered that students with all- reported making less effort in class and lower
around adjusted friends spent more time doing motivation to do well in school over the course of
homework and in extracurricular activities, and sixth to ninth grade. These findings are particu-
were absent less frequently, than were students larly troublesome because decreases in academic
with friends who obtained lower grades and engagement levels appear to be a precursor of
engaged in drug use or other misbehaviors. dropping out of school (Janosz, Archambault,
Students with all-around adjusted friends also Morizot, & Pagani, 2008), indicating that friends
improved their grade point average from seventh may indirectly influence school dropout (a topic
to eighth grade. Thus, friends’ academic behav- that we will return to later in the chapter).
iors and socioemotional well-being were each
related to student engagement in academic work
and extracurricular activities. Extracurricular Engagement:
The studies described above relied on indepen- Are Friends a Reason to Get
dent assessments of friends’ behaviors and other and Stay Involved?
attributes (i.e., friends were identified and they
provided self-reports). Methodologically less Consistent with findings regarding academic
strong research relies on subjective perceptions of engagement, students with friends who are
friends’ behaviors or values, perceptions which highly involved in extracurricular activities are
may be biased by the student’s own values and more likely to participate in activities them-
behavior. However, the same patterns are evident. selves. An interview study with highly involved
In cross-sectional studies, student perceptions of high school students explored the factors that
their friends’ behaviors and values are consistently motivate students to become involved and main-
related to students’ own engagement and conduct. tain their involvement in extracurricular activi-
For example, in a survey study of seventh and ties (Fredricks et al., 2002). Students discussed
ninth grade students, Nelson and DeBacker (2008) their friends’ involvement in the activities as a
showed that perceptions of one’s best friend hav- reason to continue their own participation. The
ing high academic values (e.g., “My best friend role of friends seems to be especially important
believes that school is more important than most in encouraging continued involvement, poten-
people think”) were related to self-reports of tially even when individual interest in the activity
a greater desire for mastery of school work (e.g., itself has waned.
“I do the work in this class because I like to under- Beyond the influence of existing friendships,
stand what I am learning”). Fredricks et al. (2002) found that high school stu-
Perceptions of friends’ behavior also predict dents were motivated to join extracurricular
changes in engagement over time. Berndt and activities in order to acquire new friendships.
Keefe (1995) found that seventh and eighth grad- Moreover, through extracurricular participation,
ers became more involved in classroom activities students are likely to be exposed to peers they may
over the course of the school year, as indicated by not normally associate with over the course of the
self-reports, when they perceived that their three school day. Dworkin, Larson, and Hansen (2003)
closest friends were highly involved in classroom used focus group methodology to examine the
activities at the beginning of the school year. ways in which extracurricular involvement is
Conversely, students who perceived their friends related to friendships among high school stu-
to disrupt class in the beginning of the year dents. Students specifically commented on the
become more disruptive themselves across the opportunities that extracurricular activities pro-
year. With a sample of about 2,500 students, vided to socialize with peers outside of their typi-
Simons-Morton and Chen (2009) showed that cal friendship groups, including students of
students who perceived a higher proportion of different racial backgrounds. This research sug-
their five closest friends engaging in negative gests that extracurricular activities can play an
392 J. Juvonen et al.

important role in helping students form new rela- described above, Berndt and Keefe (1995)
tionships with peers with whom they might oth- discovered that students who retained stable
erwise not interact. friendships over the course of the academic year
In sum, friends’ behaviors and engagement reported less disruptive behavior, were rated by
are related to student academic and extracurricu- their teachers as involved in class, and also
lar engagement. Although the mechanisms of received higher grades than peers with unstable
peer influence and selection are not necessarily friendships. Because stable friendships with spe-
investigated in most studies, research suggests cific qualities might encourage student engage-
that students with more academically engaged ment, it is also possible that students with good
friends perform better academically than those grades select friends with whom they can study
whose friends are disengaged. Similarly, those together. In a longitudinal survey study of sev-
with friends involved in extracurricular activities enth through ninth grade students examined at
are more likely to start and stay engaged in the two time points, an earlier high grade point aver-
activity. Thus, friends seem to amplify students’ age indeed correlated with subsequent social sup-
initial level of involvement. What is not clear port obtained from friends (DuBois, Felner,
from these studies is whether the quality of Brand, Adan, & Evans, 1992). Thus, the associa-
friendships and the type of peer support might tion between supportive friends and academic
matter also in terms of student engagement. engagement is likely to work both ways.
The type of peer support received might also
matter. That is, while academic support might be
Quality of Friendships and Type particularly critical in allowing students to work
of Peer Support together on homework or projects, emotional or
social support might be especially critical at times
High-quality friendships typically involve posi- of heightened distress. In concurrent and short-
tive features such as support, companionship, term longitudinal analyses (i.e., start and end of
and commitment, as well as low levels of conflict kindergarten), Ladd, Kochenderfer, and Coleman
(Berndt, 2002). A number of studies have shown (1996) found that when young elementary school
direct effects of high-quality friendships on stu- students considered their friends as sources of aid
dent engagement behaviors. For example, Berndt and validation, they were particularly likely to
and Keefe (1995) examined the importance of develop positive attitudes toward school as the
friendship quality in addition to friends’ school- year progressed.
related behaviors (class involvement and disrup- Wentzel (1994) examined whether social sup-
tiveness) in a study of seventh and eighth graders. port, defined as peers’ concerns about an individ-
The perceived quality of the friendship predicted ual’s emotions (e.g., “My classmates care about
changes in self-reported behaviors across the my feelings”), and academic support, defined as
school year. Students with a supportive, intimate, peers’ concern for an individual’s learning (e.g.,
and validating closest friend became more “My classmates care about how much I learn”),
involved in class across the school year. In con- were related to students’ pursuit of socially valued
trast, students whose closest friendship involved outcomes in middle school. The results revealed
frequent conflict and rivalry or competition that sixth and seventh grade students’ perceptions
increased in disruptive behavior during the school of both social and academic support were associ-
year. These results highlight that it is not only the ated with willingness to follow classroom rules.
behaviors of friends, but also the relationship Peers’ academic support was additionally related
qualities of friendships, that matter. to what Wentzel described as students’ academic
The quality of friendships also matters because social responsibility goals, such as the desire to
stable, supportive relationships with classmates comply with teacher requests.
encourage student engagement through consis- Most importantly, perceived academic support
tent reinforcement. In the same investigation from peers is related to active class participation.
18 The Role of Peer Relationships in Student Academic and Extracurricular Engagement 393

Focusing on seventh grade students, Murdock larger friendship networks are more engaged in
(1999) demonstrated that students who reported school? Focusing on initial school entry and the
high levels of academic support from peers were year of kindergarten, Ladd (1990) found that
rated by their teachers as attending classes, par- children with multiple existing friendships during
ticipating in class, and completing assignments school entrance developed more favorable school
more frequently than those who did not feel aca- attitudes during the first 2 months of kindergar-
demically supported by their peers. Perceived ten. Those maintaining these friendships also
academic support from peers was also related to liked school more over time. These findings are
lower rates of discipline problems (e.g., deten- particularly robust because students’ preschool
tion, in-school suspension). experience, mental age, and gender were taken
While relatively little is known about the rela- into account in the analyses. Ladd (1990) also
tion between extracurricular involvement and found that children who formed new friendships
peer support, it is possible that at least some types during kindergarten performed better academi-
of extracurricular activities foster skills that allow cally (as measured by teacher reports and student
students to be more supportive of one another. In performance on school readiness and achieve-
a focus group study of high school students who ment tests) than children who did not establish
took part in extracurricular and community-based friendships. New friendships accounted for a sig-
activities, students reported that their involve- nificant proportion of the variance in academic
ment in the activities helped them develop a performance even when controlling for existing
stronger sense of empathy and ability to handle friendships.
stress and anxiety (Dworkin et al., 2003). This In a study of students transitioning from fifth
may mean that the effects of extracurricular to sixth grade, Kingery and Erdley (2007) relied
activities on academic engagement are indirect. on both student self-reports and peer nominations
Personal skills and competencies to understand to examine the role of schoolmates as students
and support peers in distress gained in the context acclimate to their new middle school. Correlation
of extracurricular activities may help students to analysis showed that greater peer acceptance and
provide academic support. number of friends prior to the transition to middle
In sum, the research available suggests that the school was related to greater involvement (e.g.,
quality of student friendships and peer support participating in class and other school activities)
are each related to academic engagement. at the start of the sixth grade. Hence, having more
Students with stable, nonconflictive friendships friends even before the transition seems to help
are likely to engage in academic tasks. While students when transitioning to a new school.
close friends can encourage student engagement, However, larger friendship networks may simply
students are also likely to seek friends who can reflect the social skills of students. That is, the
help them with academic work. Although friends most socially skillful students (who are likely to
are in the position to provide various types of have lots of friends) may have the easiest time
support, not surprisingly, academic support is navigating in a new environment, and therefore
consistently related to academic engagement. they remain highly engaged.
Extracurricular involvement, in turn, may aid the Although a greater number of friends might
ability to support others. help, having one friend may be sufficient to help
adjust to a new school environment. The power
of one friend is highlighted in research on school
Does the Number of Friends transitions, when students frequently experience
and Ability to Make Friends Matter? a disruption in peer networks and loss of friends
(Kenny, 1987). Linking early middle school
As shown above, school-based friendships often friendships with school outcomes in a longitudi-
serve as sources of instrumental and social sup- nal survey study over the course of middle school,
port. Does this mean then that students with Wentzel, Barry, and Caldwell (2004) found that
394 J. Juvonen et al.

students with no friends in the first year of middle both academic and extracurricular activities. One
school were initially more distressed and received study also suggests that academic progress is
lower grades in their school record than students possible without friends. But what happens when
with at least one friend. Although a lack of friends a student is rejected or bullied in school? We now
may have caused distress which interfered with turn to research on negative social experiences
achievement, it is also possible that stress caused with peers.
by low grades from elementary school made it
hard for students to make friends. Nevertheless,
this study demonstrates that an absence of even Negative Social Experiences: Rejected
just one friend is related with compromised aca- and Bullied Students
demic performance.
Research on extracurricular activities also Given the literature covered thus far, it appears
suggests that one friend may be sufficient to get that having high-quality, supportive friendships
students engaged in nonacademic activities. can promote school engagement behaviors pos-
Huebner and Mancini (2003) showed that high sibly because such relationships facilitate school
school students with just one friend whom they belonging. Conversely, students who are friend-
could “count on” were more likely to report that less are less engaged, perhaps because they feel
they participated in after-school extracurricular they do not belong in school. In this section, we
activities (e.g., sports, clubs), regardless of go beyond the lack of friends to examine how
whether that friend participated in that activity or negative peer experiences (rejection and bully-
not. Thus, it is possible that a close friendship ing) are related to academic disengagement, and
provides enough support and confidence for stu- potentially to alienation from school.
dents to explore and become involved in school, Peer rejection is commonly defined as peers’
much like secure attachment to a caregiver is social avoidance of, dislike of, or reluctance to
related to exploration early in life. affiliate with a student. Therefore, rejection by
While one good friendship may be enough to classmates may threaten school belonging even
get students more engaged in school, friends are more than lack of friends, inasmuch as rejection
not the only way to improve academic outcomes. affects group membership at the classroom level
Wentzel et al. (2004) also found that the students (Furman & Robbins, 1985). Indeed, peer rejection
with no friends in the first year of middle school is associated with avoidance of school, less posi-
did improve their academic performance over the tive perceptions about school, and lower academic
course of middle school, despite initially having performance in kindergarten (Ladd, 1990), as well
lower grades in sixth grade than those with friend- as lower grades in the first and second grade
ships. It is possible that friendless students (O’Neil, Welsh, Parke, Wang, & Strand, 1997). In
obtain support for academic engagement from secondary school, peer rejection is associated
other sources (e.g., adults at school, parents). with increased absenteeism and truancy (DeRosier,
In sum, the existing research shows that lack Kupersmidt, & Patterson, 1994; Kupersmidt &
of close friendships is associated with lower stu- Coie, 1990) as well as subsequent grade retention
dent engagement (especially at times of school (Coie, Lochman, Terry, & Hyman, 1992).
transitions), while the ability to develop and Even temporary rejection is associated with
maintain friendships is related with academic negative academic outcomes. Examining peer
engagement. Although a larger number of friends rejection across time among elementary school
might increase the probability of receiving posi- students, Greenman, Schneider and Tomada
tive support for academic performance, the size (2009) showed that students rejected at even just
of the peer network may simply reflect social one time point performed worse academically
skills that are particularly helpful to students dur- than children who had never been rejected.
ing school transitions. Yet, having just one friend Moreover, Buhs, Ladd, and Herald (2006) dem-
is enough to help students become involved in onstrated that students who were excluded and
18 The Role of Peer Relationships in Student Academic and Extracurricular Engagement 395

victimized in elementary school became increas- tary school are less likely to feel that they belong
ingly less engaged over time. Thus, negative in school and are more likely to disengage.
experiences with schoolmates can also be associ- Kochenderfer and Ladd (1996) showed that bul-
ated with lasting disengagement. lied kindergartners displayed increased loneliness
Given that aggressive students are at high risk and school avoidance by the end of the school
for being rejected by classmates at least in year. Examining the association between bullying
elementary school (Asher & Coie, 1990), it is experiences and teacher-rated academic engage-
important to understand whether peer rejection ment as well as grade point average in middle
independently contributes to subsequent prob- school, Juvonen, Wang, and Espinoza (2011) dis-
lems or whether it functions merely as a marker covered that bullied students were less engaged
of problem behaviors (Parker & Asher, 1987). and obtained lower academic grades across 3 years
Following a large sample of African-American of middle school. Although the study did not test
children from elementary school to middle the directionality of the associations (i.e., whether
school, Coie et al. (1992) demonstrated that child- bullying experiences preceded disengagement or
hood peer rejection contributed to behavior prob- vice versa), the robust association between bully-
lems 3 years later, over and above earlier levels ing experiences (regardless of being based on
of aggression. Subsequent analyses of data from self-reports or peer nominations) and the aca-
the same sample revealed that the combination of demic indicators among an ethnically diverse
childhood aggression and peer rejection signifi- sample of about 1,500 students suggest that bully-
cantly increased the risk of committing assaults ing cannot be ignored when trying to improve
by the second year in high school (Coie, Terry, academic engagement and performance.
Lenox, & Lochman, 1995). Because aggression Nishina, Juvonen, and Witkow (2005) reported
is associated with school disengagement, inde- evidence for both direct and mediated effects of
pendent of rejection (e.g., Lessard et al., 2008; bullying on middle school functioning. Among
Schwartz, Gorman, Nakamoto, & Toblin, 2005), close to 2,000 students of diverse ethnic back-
it is therefore likely that rejection amplifies the grounds, bullying experiences at the start of the
risk for subsequent school disengagement. sixth grade were linked with subsequent psycho-
In the studies described above, peer rejection logical maladjustment as well as health com-
is assessed via peer nominations by asking stu- plaints, which were related to end-of-the-year
dents to name classmates they do not like to sit absences and grades. At the same time, symp-
next to or spend time with. But self-reports also toms of psychological distress at the start of the
show associations between feeling rejected by sixth grade also increased the chances of students
peers and student, disengagement. Buhs (2005) being bullied by the end of the year, which was
found that fifth grade students who reported that associated with higher absences and lower grades.
they were excluded by their peers, were less Hence, negative peer experiences and distress are
likely to participate in class. In a cross-sectional interrelated in a cyclical manner (see also Egan &
study of sixth and seventh graders, Lopez and Perry, 1998) and therefore especially likely to
DuBois (2005) showed that students who felt dis- compromise academic engagement (see also
approved of by their peers had lower grade point Juvonen, Nishina, & Graham, 2000).
averages and were absent from school on more Bullying research suggests that emotional dis-
days than students who felt accepted. The authors tress associated with hostile peer interactions con-
suggested that both perceived rejection and the tributes also to negative school attitudes and a
low self-esteem associated with such perceptions desire to withdraw from or avoid school. The mere
make it difficult for students to concentrate on prospect of potential rejection may discourage
schoolwork and engage in productive, collabora- academic success, at least among older students.
tive work with peers. Ishiyama and Chabassol (1985) surveyed seventh
Consistent with the findings of research on to twelfth graders about their concerns of the
rejected students, victims of bullying in elemen- social implications of high academic achievement
396 J. Juvonen et al.

(e.g., peer rejection and/or criticism, pressure to from dropping out of school. Focusing on
continue success). Seventh to ninth grade students Mexican-American and White non-Hispanic high
(particularly girls) expressed more concern about school–aged students who were either in good
the social repercussions of performing well than academic standing or had dropped out of school,
older participants. Hence, students’ concern about Davalos, Chavez, and Guardiola (1999) found
rejection may temper their classroom participa- that students who had been involved in any
tion. Given that earlier academic performance extracurricular activity were more than twice as
sets the stage for subsequent performance, it is likely to be enrolled in school. In a prospective
particularly troublesome if young teens down- longitudinal study, Mahoney and Cairns (1997)
play their academic success and engagement. demonstrated that students who participated in
In sum, both peer rejection and bullying expe- extracurricular activities in middle or high school
riences are associated with lower levels of aca- were less likely to drop out of school. This effect
demic engagement and academic performance. It was particularly strong for those considered at
is likely that negative social experiences cause high risk of dropping out who participated in
students to disengage. However, it is also possi- extracurricular activities early in high school
ble that low-performing students are bullied and (Mahoney & Cairns, 1997). In a subsequent
rejected by their classmates. In the latter case, the study, Mahoney (2000) showed that participation
odds against these students accumulate. Their in extracurricular activities before 11th grade
distress and concerns about being ridiculed or decreased the chance of leaving school early or
excluded can propel students into avoiding school engaging in criminal behavior as an adult among
altogether. Thus, the associations are likely to be students considered at high risk. Moreover, the
cyclical. Moreover, even mere concerns about likelihood of dropping out was reduced further
rejection are related to decreased academic when the students’ friends also participated in
engagement in middle and high school. Although school extracurricular activities. These findings
additional longitudinal research on this topic is suggest that opportunities to engage in school-
warranted, there is important evidence illustrat- related activities together with peers are critical,
ing that a sense of social alienation precedes an especially for youth who might otherwise be at
ultimate form of disengagement, namely drop- risk of leaving school prematurely (Hymel,
ping out of school, as summarized below. Comfort, Schonert-Reichl, & McDougall, 1996).
Consistent with the importance of the sense of
school belonging, Kaplan, Peck, and Kaplan
Social Alienation and Dropping Out (1997) showed that in addition to low grades and
lack of motivation, social alienation from school-
When interviewed about reasons for dropping based peer networks and relationships with devi-
out, one out of four youth reported that they did ant schoolmates during eighth and ninth grade
not belong at school (U.S. Department of independently contributed to the risk of dropping
Education, Center for Education Statistics, 1993). out. Also, students who were held back during
Finn (1989, 1993) proposed that the relationship middle school were seven times more likely to
between students not participating in school and drop out of school than their peers with similar
dropping out is explained by a lack of sense of academic performance who were not held back
belonging and identification with school. (Alexander, Entwisle, & Kabbani, 2001). The
Consistent with this idea, an early study (Dillon authors concluded that this independent effect of
& Grout, 1976) reported that students become grade retention partly reflects a lack of social
alienated from school when they feel they are integration. Hence, feeling that one does not
denied meaningful participation in both class- socially fit in or belong is an important risk factor
room and other school activities. for dropping out.
Extracurricular involvement may serve as a In sum, socially alienated youth who feel that
meaningful activity, and thereby protect youth they do not fit in and are not engaged in school
18 The Role of Peer Relationships in Student Academic and Extracurricular Engagement 397

are at risk of dropping out of school. Although sense of belonging in school that in turn promotes
both grade retention and behavior problems may engagement, as suggested by the pathway
in part alienate youth from their peers as well as depicted in the beginning of the chapter.
their teachers, negative peer experiences may
also increase sense of alienation. In addition to
not retaining students, encouraging socially vul- Summary of Negative Peer “Effects”
nerable youth to participate in extracurricular
activities might help keep these students engaged Not all friendships are beneficial, however. Not
in the schooling process. only do critical qualities (e.g., supportiveness,
validation) of friendships vary, but also the
level of support and collaboration on school
Conclusion assignments varies depending on the abilities
and aspirations of friends (e.g., Berndt, 1989,
One of the main reasons given by high school 2002). Students who have disengaged friends
students for attending school is that they get to are unlikely to excel academically. Additionally,
see their friends (Brown & Theobald, 1998). negative social experiences with classmates
Students select to affiliate with certain types of may make rejected youth seek the company of
peers, and the way they feel about fitting in with other students who misbehave and encourage
their schoolmates is associated with their level of bullied students to avoid school. Feelings of
engagement in school. We now briefly summa- social alienation from the institution and
rize some of the positive and negative effects of repeated absences, in turn, increase the risk of
peers, as well as point out questions that need to dropping out of school. Thus, particular types
be further examined. of friendships, lack of any friendships, as well
as bullying and rejection experiences are all
related to school disengagement.
Summary of Positive Peer “Effects”

Relationships with friends who are academically Are Peers Necessary to Maintain School
engaged in school are associated with higher aca- Engagement?
demic motivation and achievement. Friends’
overall social adjustment (e.g., lack of behavior Although many students are motivated to attend
problems) is also associated with academic school to spend time with their friends, it should
engagement and involvement in extracurricular be clear from the research reviewed that peers are
activities. Although having a greater number of not always essential for student engagement and
friends may help students get engaged in school, achievement. There is evidence suggesting that
having just one friend helps alleviate the stress parent support and teacher support may be more
related with transitioning to a new school. Friends important than peer support for student engage-
are typically good sources of emotional and ment (Chen, 2005; Garcia-Reid, 2007; Wentzel,
social support; however, it is academic support 1998). When and if these other sources of sup-
that is most clearly associated with increased port can compensate for the support that friends
achievement motivation and classroom participa- provide in relation to engagement in academic
tion. Extracurricular activities, in turn, provide work is a critical question to further investigate.
students with opportunities to form new friend- This issue may be best studied with students who
ships, just as those with friends are more likely to lack friendships. It would be equally important to
explore new extracurricular options and stay know whether other sources of support, besides
involved. Based on the research reviewed, we support from peers, can alleviate the distress
conclude that friendships and peer affiliations associated with negative social experiences (such
with engaged classmates generally facilitate a as bullying).
398 J. Juvonen et al.

The studies reviewed in this chapter also con- among the top performers within their extracur-
vey that not all peer relationships promote aca- ricular activities.
demic engagement. Clearly, there are peer groups The benefits of having at least one friend
of disengaged students whose effects are more through the transition to a new school are consis-
harmful than productive. Also, while a lack of tent across studies of kindergartners to middle
friends might be a sign of social isolation or school students. Similarly, research on bullying
alienation, there are students with no friends in suggests that one friend is enough to both decrease
school who do well. For some, it may be to their the risk of getting bullied as well as to buffer the
benefit not to form close ties with classmates who emotional distress associated with peer harass-
are not engaged. Moreover, youth can form valu- ment (Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowski,
able peer relationships outside of school. That is, 1999; Hodges, Malone, & Perry, 1997). Whether
neighborhood friends or friends from out-of- one friend or any friend is enough in other stress-
school activities may compensate for the lack of ful situations as well is less clear. It is therefore
close ties in school. These are questions that important to examine the potential power of one
remain to be investigated. friend when youth experience academic difficul-
ties or when they get cut from a team. Equally
important is research examining the ways in
Implications for Future Research which some extracurricular involvement (e.g.,
and School Policies team sports) might help students provide support
to one another. Unless group work and other
A few key longitudinal studies suggest that both cooperative methods are used in classrooms, cer-
selection of friends and their influence play a tain extracurricular activities may be one of the
part in whether students engage in class or get only ways to learn support giving.
involved in extracurricular activities. It is there- Because the bodies of research on academic
fore important to consider the opportunities that and extracurricular activities are largely separate,
schools provide for students to seek and find it is valuable to compare the two domains of
friends who are in the position to provide sup- engagement. It is interesting not only to note
port. This is particularly critical when consider- differences in assumptions and research tradi-
ing how certain educational policies and practices tions for each but also to learn about the general-
may restrict students’ opportunities to establish izability of the findings across the two domains.
and maintain positive peer relationships. Based For example, it appears that rejection by peers
on the current review, it seems that academic and exclusion from a sports team may have simi-
tracking is particularly problematic. In low-track larly alienating effects that are related to disen-
classrooms that often have an overrepresentation gagement. Whether course selections, much like
of disengaged students, youth lack opportunities extracurricular choices, might be influenced in part
to form positive peer relationships supporting by whether friends or high-status (i.e., popular)
academic involvement. Similar problems can peers are involved in the class is also needed.
arise in classrooms that segregate students with Particularly intriguing is the idea that extracur-
disabilities. That is, the range of potential friends ricular activities or peer relations fostered by
is limited. those activities might help academic engagement.
For extracurricular activities, in turn, selection
procedures are problematic. Exclusion based on
tryouts can disengage and alienate students from
school. When nonselected students are the ones References
who need most support, an opportunity to make
Adelabu, D. H. (2007). Time perspective and school
them feel part of the school is lost. Therefore, membership as correlates to academic achievement
schools should consider offering meaningful among African American adolescents. Adolescence,
alternative activities for students who are not 42, 525–538.
18 The Role of Peer Relationships in Student Academic and Extracurricular Engagement 399

Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Kabbani, N. (2001). Buhs, E. S., Ladd, G. W., & Herald, S. L. (2006). Peer
The dropout process in life course perspective: Early exclusion and victimization: Processes that mediate
risk factors at home and school. Teachers College the relation between peer group rejection and chil-
Record, 103, 760–822. dren’s classroom engagement and achievement?
Anderman, E. M. (2002). School effects on psychological Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 1–13.
outcomes during adolescence. Journal of Educational Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., & Neckerman, H. J. (1989).
Psychology, 94, 795–809. Early school dropout: Configurations and determi-
Asher, S. R., & Coie, J. D. (1990). Peer rejection in child- nants. Child Development, 60, 1437–1452.
hood (Cambridge studies in social and emotional Chen, J. J. (2005). Relation of academic support from par-
development). New York: Cambridge University ents, teaches, and peers to Hong Kong adolescents’
Press. academic achievement: The mediating role of aca-
Barnett, L. A. (2006). Flying high or crashing down: demic engagement. Genetic, Social, and General
Girls’ accounts of trying out for cheerleading and Psychology Monographs, 131(2), 77–127.
dance. Journal of Adolescent Research, 21, 514–541. Coie, J. D., Lochman, J., Terry, R., & Hyman, C. (1992).
Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Kim, D., Watson, M., & Schaps, Predicting early adolescent disorder from childhood
E. (1995). Schools as communities, poverty levels of aggression and peer rejection. Journal of Consulting
student populations, and students’ attitudes, motives and Clinical Psychology, 60, 783–792.
and performance: A multilevel analysis. American Coie, J. D., Terry, R., Lenox, K., & Lochman, J. (1995).
Educational Research Journal, 32, 627–658. Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors
Becker, B. E., & Luthar, S. S. (2002). Social-emotional of stable patterns of adolescent disorder. Development
factors affecting achievement outcomes among disad- and Psychopathology, 7, 697–713.
vantaged students: Closing the achievement gap. Cook, T. D., Deng, Y., & Morgano, E. (2007). Friendship
Educational Psychologist, 37(4), 197–214. influences during early adolescence: The special role
Berndt, T. J. (1989). Obtaining support from friends dur- of friends’ grade point average. Journal of Research
ing childhood and adolescence. In D. Belle (Ed.), on Adolescence, 17, 325–356.
Children’s social networks and social supports (pp. Davalos, D. B., Chavez, E. L., & Guardiola, R. J. (1999).
308–331). Oxford, UK: Wiley. The effects of extracurricular activity, ethnic identifi-
Berndt, T. J. (2002). Friendship quality and social devel- cation, and perception of school on student dropout
opment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, rates. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 21,
11, 7–10. 61–77.
Berndt, T. J., & Keefe, K. (1995). Friends’ influence on DeRosier, M. E., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Patterson, C. P.
adolescents’ adjustment to school. Child Development, (1994). Children’s academic and behavioral adjust-
66, 1312–1329. ment as a function of the chronicity and proximity of
Bond, L., Butler, H., Thomas, L., Carlin, J., Glover, S., peer rejection. Child Development, 65, 1799–1813.
Bowes, G., & Patton, G. (2007). Social and school Dillon, S. V., & Grout, J. A. (1976). Schools and alien-
connectedness in early secondary school as predictors ation. The Elementary School Journal, 76, 481–489.
of late teenage substance use, mental health and aca- Dishion, T. J., Spracklen, K. M., Andrews, D. W., &
demic outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Health, 40, Patterson, G. R. (1996). Deviancy training in male ado-
357.e9–357.e18. lescent friendships. Behavior Therapy, 27, 373–390.
Brand, S., Felner, R., Shim, M., Seitsinger, A., & Dumas, Dotterer, A. M., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (2007).
T. (2003). Middle school improvement and reform: Implications of out-of-school activities for school
Development and validation of a school-level assess- engagement in African American adolescents. Journal
ment of climate, cultural pluralism, and school safety. of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 391–401.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 570–588. DuBois, D. L., Felner, R. D., Brand, S., Adan, A. M., &
Brown, R., & Evans, W. P. (2002). Extracurricular activity Evans, E. G. (1992). A prospective study of life stress,
and ethnicity: Creating greater school connection social support, and adaptation in early adolescence.
among diverse student populations. Urban Education, Child Development, 63, 542–557.
37, 41–58. Dworkin, J. B., Larson, R., & Hansen, D. (2003).
Brown, B. B., & Theobald, W. (1998). Learning contexts Adolescents’ accounts of growth experiences in youth
beyond the classroom: Extracurricular activities, activities. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32,
community organizations, and peer groups. In 17–26.
K. Borman & B. Schneider (Eds.), The adolescent Egan, S. K., & Perry, D. G. (1998). Does low self-regard
years: Social influences and educational challenges invite peer victimization? Developmental Psychology,
(The 97th yearbook of the National Society for the 34, 299–309.
Study of Education. Part I, pp. 109–141). Chicago: Felner, R. D., & Felner, T. Y. (1989). Primary prevention
National Society for the Study of Education. programs in the educational context: A transactional-
Buhs, E. S. (2005). Peer rejection, negative peer treat- ecological framework and analysis. In L. A. Bond &
ment, and school adjustment: Self-concept and class- B. E. Compas (Eds.), Primary prevention and promo-
room engagement as mediating processes. Journal of tion in the schools (pp. 13–49). Thousand Oaks, CA:
School Psychology, 43, 407–424. Sage.
400 J. Juvonen et al.

Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Ishiyama, F. I., & Chabassol, D. J. (1985). Adolescents’
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. fear of social consequences of academic success as a
Finn, J. D. (1993). School engagement and students at risk function of age and sex. Journal of Youth and
(National Center for Education Statistics Research and Adolescence, 14, 37–46.
Development Reports). Washington, DC: U.S. Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. S.
Department of Education, National Center for (2008). School engagement trajectories and their dif-
Education Statistics. ferential predictive relations to dropout. Journal of
Fredricks, J. A., Alfeld-Liro, C. J., Hruda, L. Z., Eccles, J. Social Issues, 64, 21–40.
S., Patrick, H., & Ryan, A. M. (2002). A qualitative Juvonen, J., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2000). Peer
exploration of adolescents’ commitment to athletics and harassment, psychological adjustment, and school
the arts. Journal of Adolescent Research, 17, 68–97. functioning in early adolescence. Journal of
Furman, W., & Robbins, P. (1985). What’s the point?: Educational Psychology, 92, 349–359.
Selection of treatment objectives. In B. Schneider, K. Juvonen, J., Wang, Y., & Espinoza, G. (2011). Bullying
H. Rubin, & J. E. Ledingham (Eds.), Children’s peer experiences and compromised academic performance
relations: Issues in assessment and intervention (pp. across middle school grade. Journal of Early
41–54). New York: Springer. Adolescence, 31, 152–173.
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a Kandel, D. B. (1996). The parental and peer contexts of
factor in children’s academic engagement and perfor- adolescent deviance: An algebra of interpersonal influ-
mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, ences. Journal of Drug Issues, 26, 289–315.
148–163. Kaplan, D. S., Peck, B. M., & Kaplan, H. B. (1997).
Garcia-Reid, P. (2007). Examining social capital as a Decomposing the academic failure-dropout relation-
mechanism for improving school engagement among ship: A longitudinal analysis. The Journal of
low income Hispanic girls. Youth and Society, 39(2), Educational Research, 90, 331–343.
164–181. Kenny, M. (1987). Family ties and leaving home for col-
Goodenow, C., & Grady, K. E. (1993). The relationship of lege: Recent findings and implications. Journal of
school belonging and friends’ values to academic College Student Personnel, 28, 438–442.
motivation among urban adolescent students. The Kindermann, T. A. (1993). Natural peer groups as con-
Journal of Experimental Education, 62, 60–71. texts for individual development: The case of chil-
Greenman, P. S., Schneider, B. H., & Tomada, G. (2009). dren’s motivation in school. Developmental
Stability and change in patterns of peer rejection: Psychology, 29, 970–977.
Implications for children’s academic performance over Kindermann, T. A., McCollam, T., & Gibson, E. (1996).
time. School Psychology International, 30, 163–183. Peer networks and student’s classroom engagement
Hallinan, M. R. (1983). Commentary: New directions during childhood and adolescence. In J. Juvonen &
for research on peer influence. In J. L. Epstein & K. Wentzel (Eds.), Social motivation: Understanding
N. Karweit (Eds.), Friends in school: Patterns of selec- children’s school adjustment (pp. 279–312).
tion and influence in secondary schools (pp. 219–231). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
New York: Academic. Kingery, J. N., & Erdley, C. A. (2007). Peer experiences
Hamm, J. V., & Faircloth, B. S. (2005). The role of friend- as predictors of adjustment across the middle school
ship in adolescents’ sense of school belonging. New transition. Education and Treatment of Children, 30,
Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 73–88.
107, 61–78. Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. W. (1996). Peer victim-
Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. ization: Cause or consequence of school maladjust-
(1999). The power of friendship: Protection against an ment? Child Development, 67, 1305–1317.
escalating cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Kupersmidt, J. B., & Coie, J. D. (1990). Preadolescent
Psychology, 25, 94–101. peer status, aggression, and school adjustment as pre-
Hodges, E. V. E., Malone, M. J., & Perry, D. G. (1997). dictors of externalizing problems in adolescence.
Individual risk and social risk as interacting determi- Child Development, 61, 1350–1362.
nants of victimization in the peer group. Developmental Ladd, G. W. (1990). Having friends, keeping friends, and
Psychology, 33, 1032–1039. being liked by peers in the classroom: Predictors of
Huebner, A. J., & Mancini, J. A. (2003). Shaping struc- children’s early school adjustment? Child Development,
tured out-of-school time use among youth: The effects 61, 1081–1100.
of self, family, and friend systems. Journal of Youth Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C.
and Adolescence, 32, 453–463. (1996). Friendship quality as a predictor of young
Hymel, S., Comfort, C., Schonert-Reichl, K., & children’s early school adjustment. Child Development,
McDougall, P. (1996). Academic failure and school 67(1103), 1118.
dropout: The influence of peers. In K. Wentzel & J. LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2010).
Juvonen (Eds.), Social motivation: Understanding Developmental changes in the priority of perceived
children’s school adjustment (pp. 313–345). New status in childhood and adolescence. Social
York: Cambridge University Press. Development, 19(1), 130–147.
18 The Role of Peer Relationships in Student Academic and Extracurricular Engagement 401

Lessard, A., Butler-Kisber, L., Fortin, L., Marcotte, D., Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and
Potvin, P., & Royer, E. (2008). Shades of disengage- later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children
ment: High school dropouts speak out. Social at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357–389.
Psychology of Education, 11(1), 25–42. Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996).
Lopez, C., & DuBois, D. L. (2005). Peer victimization Perceptions of the school psychological environment
and rejection: Investigation of an integrative model of and early adolescents’ psychological and behavioral
effects on emotional, behavioral, and academic adjust- functioning in school: The mediating role of goals and
ment in early adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child belonging. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88,
and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 25–36. 408–422.
Mahoney, J. L. (2000). School extracurricular activity par- Sánchez, B., Colón, Y., & Esparza, P. (2005). The role of
ticipation as a moderator in the development of antiso- sense of school belonging and gender in the academic
cial patterns. Child Development, 71, 502–516. adjustment of Latino adolescents. Journal of Youth
Mahoney, J. L., & Cairns, R. B. (1997). Do extracurricular and Adolescence, 34, 619–628.
activities protect against early school dropout? Schwartz, D., Gorman, A. H., Nakamoto, J., & Toblin, R. L.
Developmental Psychology, 33, 241–253. (2005). Victimization in the peer group and children’s
Mounts, N. S., & Steinberg, L. (1995). An ecological academic functioning. Journal of Educational
analysis of peer influence on adolescent grade point Psychology, 97, 425–435.
average and drug use. Developmental Psychology, 31, Simons-Morton, B., & Chen, R. (2009). Peer and parent
915–922. influences on school engagement among early adoles-
Murdock, T. B. (1999). The social context of risk: Status cents. Youth and Society, 41, 3–25.
and motivational predictors of alienation in middle U.S. Department of Education. (1993). Dropout Rates in
school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, the United States: 1992 (NCES 93–901). Washington,
62–75. DC: National Center for Education Statistics.
Nelson, R. M., & DeBacker, T. K. (2008). Achievement Voelkl, K. E. (1997). Identification with school. American
motivation in adolescents: The role of peer climate Journal of Education, 105, 294–318.
and best friends. The Journal of Experimental Wentzel, K. R. (1994). Relations of social goal pursuit to
Education, 76, 170–189. social acceptance, classroom behavior and perceived
Nishina, A., Juvonen, J., & Witkow, M. R. (2005). Sticks social support. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86,
and stones may break my bones, but names will make 173–182.
me feel sick: The psychosocial, somatic and scholastic Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social relationships and motivation
consequences of peer harassment. Journal of Clinical in middle school: The role of parents teachers and peers.
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 37–48. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 202–209.
O’Neil, R., Welsh, M., Parke, R. D., Wang, S., & Strand, Wentzel, K. R., Barry, C. M., & Caldwell, K. A. (2004).
C. (1997). A longitudinal assessment of the academic Friendships in middle school: Influences on motiva-
correlates of early peer acceptance and rejection. tion and school adjustment. Journal of Educational
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 290–303. Psychology, 96, 195–203.
Understanding Student
Engagement with a Contextual 19
Model*

Shui-fong Lam, Bernard P.H. Wong,


Hongfei Yang, and Yi Liu

Abstract
In the present study, student engagement was conceptualized as a meta-
construct with affective, behavioral, and cognitive dimensions. As the
indicators in each of the three dimensions were unpacked from facilitators
and outcomes, we were able to investigate how student engagement was
associated with its antecedents and outcomes in a sample of Chinese junior
secondary school students (N = 822). The results supported a contextual
model for understanding student engagement. They revealed that students
were engaged in school when they felt that their teachers adopted motivat-
ing instructional practices and they had social-emotional support from
their teachers, parents, and peers. Their engagement was high when they
had high self-efficacy, endorsed learning goals, and effort attribution.
Most importantly, when students were engaged in schools, they experi-
enced positive emotions frequently and their teachers rated them high on
academic performance and conduct. The findings have implications for
interventions for the enhancement of student engagement in school.

In recent years, the concept of student engagement


has attracted much attention from educators
and researchers (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris,
*The study reported in this chapter is part of an interna-
tional research project initiated by the International School 2004). Many studies have indicated that student
Psychology Association (Lam et al., 2009). engagement has both short-term and long-term
S.-f. Lam, Ph.D. (*) • B.P.H. Wong, Ph.D. impacts on students. In the short term, it is predic-
Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong, tive of students’ learning, grades, and conduct in
Pokfulam, Hong Kong school (Connell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Hill &
e-mail: lamsf@hku.hk
Werner, 2006; Marks, 2000; Skinner & Belmont,
H. Yang, Ph.D. 1993; Voelkl, 1997). Over the long term, it is
Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences,
linked to a variety of life outcomes, such as aca-
University of Zhejiang, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
demic achievement, self-esteem, and socially
Y. Liu, M.A.
appropriate behaviors (Finn & Rock, 1997;
Urban Community and Mental Health Education Office,
Yunnan Health Education Institute, Kunming, Hawkins, Gou, Hill, Battin-Pearson, & Abbott,
Yunnan, China 2001; Maddox & Prinz, 2003). It is considered

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 403


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_19, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
404 S.-f. Lam et al.

as a protective factor against school dropout, elusive metaconstruct may cause more confusion
substance abuse, delinquency, and antisocial than understanding. Three concerns regarding
behaviors (Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, the conceptualization and measurement of this
2008; Chung, Hill, Hawkins, Gilchrist, & Nagin, metaconstruct need to be addressed. The first
2002; O’Farrell & Morrison, 2003). Given the relates to the distinction between indicators ver-
abundant evidence that student engagement is sus facilitators of student engagement. Indicators
related not only to an adaptive orientation toward refer to the features that define student engage-
school but also to a wide range of developmental ment, while facilitators are contextual factors
and adjustment outcomes, no wonder it has that influence student engagement (Sinclair,
emerged in recent decades as an important con- Christenson, Lehr, & Anderson, 2003; Skinner,
cept in the field of education. Furrer, Marchand, & Kindermann, 2008).
Indicators are the characteristics that belong
inside the construct of student engagement
Conceptualization and Measurement proper, e.g., students’ effort and enthusiasm in
of Student Engagement school work. By contrast, facilitators are the
causal factors outside the construct, e.g., teacher
While there is a consensus about the importance support that contributes to student engagement.
of student engagement and the necessity to inves- We agree with Skinner et al. (2008) that a clear
tigate how to enhance it, there is little consensus demarcation between these two is needed. If
about its conceptualization and measurement. facilitators are defined as part of student engage-
Most researchers agree that it is a metaconstruct ment itself, researchers cannot explore how con-
encompassing multiple dimensions of involve- textual factors, such as teacher support, influence
ment in school or commitment in learning student engagement. Therefore, facilitators
(Appleton et al., 2008; Fredricks et al., 2004; should not be included in the conceptualization
Jimerson, Campos, & Greif, 2003). However, the and measurement of student engagement.
number and nature of dimensions within this The second concern relates to the distinction
metaconstruct remain confusing and require clar- between indicators versus outcomes of student
ification. Some researchers use a three-part typol- engagement. This concern is parallel with the
ogy and conceptualize it as comprising affective, first one. Similarly, outcomes such as grades, dis-
behavioral, and cognitive dimensions (Fredricks cipline, and number of credits the student has
et al., 2004; Jimerson et al., 2003; Lam et al., accrued should not be defined as part of student
2009), whereas some researchers use a four-part engagement itself. Otherwise, researchers cannot
typology, adding an academic dimension to this explore the consequences of student engagement.
metaconstruct (Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Therefore, there is also a need for a clear demar-
Reschly, 2006). Some researchers include ante- cation between indicators and outcomes of stu-
cedents of student engagement, such as teacher dent engagement. Outcomes should not be
support and peer relationships, in the measure- included in the conceptualization and measure-
ment of student engagement (e.g., Appleton et al., ment of student engagement.
2006), whereas others include outcomes, such as The third concern relates to the uniqueness
grades and discipline, in the measurement (e.g., and redundancy of the dimensions in student
Archambault, Janosz, Fallu, & Pagani, 2009). engagement. Although the dimensions in this
The fusion of several dimensions under the metaconstruct are not isolated processes and
idea of student engagement is valuable because it should be interrelated dynamically within indi-
may provide a richer characterization of students vidual students, their features should not be over-
than is possible in research on a single dimension lapping with one another across the dimensions.
(Fredricks et al., 2004). However, to capitalize on Otherwise, the justification for the proposed
the merits of this metaconstruct, clarification dimensions is in question. For example, in a four-
must be made regarding the number and nature of part typology (Appleton et al., 2006), the amount
its dimensions. Otherwise, a comprehensive but of time spent on schoolwork and the amount
19 Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model 405

of homework completed are considered as aca- the literature. They have been addressed by robust
demic engagement. However, involvement in bodies of work separately. For example, enjoy-
academic activities and on-task behavior can also ment in learning, a component of affective
be considered as behavioral engagement (Skinner engagement, is intrinsic motivation, the eager-
& Belmont, 1993). The overlapping between the ness that comes from the pleasure in learning
academic and behavior engagement may result in itself. This component is a well-researched con-
redundancy and confusion. Parsimony is impor- struct in the field of motivation (Ryan & Deci,
tant in the development of theoretical models 2000). To build a metaconstruct on well-defined
(Gauch, 2003). There is a need to streamline the and well-researched constructs enables research-
dimensions in the metaconstruct of student ers to tap into their existing body of knowledge
engagement and to avoid redundancy. and examine their additive and interactive effects
To address the above concerns, we have simultaneously and dynamically. Compared to
adopted a three-part typology and conceptualized the research that focuses on only one construct,
student engagement as a metaconstruct that com- the study of student engagement as metaconstruct
prises affective, behavioral, and cognitive dimen- provides a new and comprehensive perspective.
sions. They are the most critical dimensions of In this three-part typology of student engage-
student involvement in school (Fredricks et al., ment, indicators are conceptually unpacked from
2004; Jimerson et al., 2003). Affective engage- facilitators and outcomes. The clear demarca-
ment refers to students’ feelings about learning tion among the three enables researchers to
(Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Skinner & Belmont, examine the consequences of student engage-
1993) and the school they attend (Finn, 1989; ment in both the short and the long run. Most
Voelkl, 1997). The feelings about learning activi- importantly, it also enables researchers to exam-
ties are reflections of intrinsic motivation, while ine how and what contextual factors contribute
the feelings about the school are a manifestation to the development of student engagement. As
of school bonding. Students with high affective Furlong and Christenson (2008) pointed out,
engagement enjoy learning and love going to student engagement is “a state of being that is
school. Behavioral engagement refers to student highly influenced by contextual factors – home,
participation in learning (Birch & Ladd, 1997; school, and peers – in relation to the capacity of
Skinner & Belmont, 1993) and extracurricular each to provide consistent support for student
activities in school (Finn, Pannozzo, & Voelkl, learning” (p. 366). It is not a nonmalleable trait
1995). Students with high behavioral engage- of the student. The conceptualization of student
ment are diligent in learning activities and active engagement as a state instead of a trait is very
in extracurricular activities. Cognitive engage- important because it makes intervention possi-
ment refers to the amount and types of cognitive ble and legitimate. If student engagement is a
strategies that students employ (Walker, Greene, nonmalleable trait, there is no point to do any
& Mansell, 2006). Students may employ deep or intervention. By contrast, if student engagement
shallow processing strategies. Deep processing is is influenced highly by contextual factors, inter-
associated with cognitive elaboration of the to- vention with these factors will bring changes to
be-learned material, whereas shallow processing student engagement.
involves rote memorization, basic rehearsal, and
other types of superficial engagement with the
new material. Students who engage in deep cog- Contextual Factors
nitive processing have better understanding and
retention of meaningful learning materials. Given the important impact of student engage-
In this three-part typology, the three dimen- ment on the wide range of developmental and
sions of student engagement have clear and dis- adjustment outcomes, researchers and educators
tinctive features that do not overlap with one need to know how and what contextual factors
another. The components in each of the dimen- can enhance it. The contextual factors of student
sions are actually well-established constructs in engagement are best conceptualized from the
406 S.-f. Lam et al.

Student
Contextual Personal Student
engagement
Factors Factors Outcomes
in schools

Instructional Contexts Affective Engagement


- Challenge - Liking for Learning
- Real-Life Significance - Liking for School
- Curiosity
- Autonomy
- Recognition
- Evaluation Positive Outcomes
Motivational Beliefs Behavioral Engagement
- Goal Orientations - Emotional Functioning
- Effort in Learning - Academic Performance
- Self-efficacy - Involvement in School
- Attribution - Conduct
Activities

Social Relatedness
Contexts
- Teacher Support Cognitive Engagement
- Parent Support - Deep or Meaningful
- Peer Support Processing
- Aggression to Peers
- Aggression from Peers

Fig. 19.1 A contextual model for student engagement

ecological system theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1986). six important components of motivating instruc-
According to this theory, human development tional contexts: (1) challenge, (2) real-life signifi-
occurs in a nested arrangement of systems, each cance, (3) curiosity, (4) autonomy, (5) recognition,
contained within the next. The most immediate and (6) evaluation. The more the students reported
systems in which a human organism develops are that their teachers assigned challenging work,
the microsystems (e.g., school, family, work- integrated real-life significance to learning tasks,
place). The dynamics and relationships in these aroused their curiosity, supported their autonomy,
microsystems have a significant impact on human recognized their effort or improvement, and used
development. To learn about how student engage- formative evaluation, the stronger was the intrin-
ment develops in an intricate web of mutually sic motivation they reported in learning.
influencing contexts, it is important to explore its Social-relatedness factors can also affect stu-
antecedents in the school and family. dent engagement. Children who report a higher
Figure 19.1 presents a contextual model of the sense of relatedness with teachers and peers show
antecedents and outcomes of student engage- greater affective and behavioral engagement
ment. In the school, at least two sets of contextual (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Eccles et al., 1993;
factors are likely to influence students’ personal Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Gest, Welsh, &
motivational beliefs and their engagement in Domitrovich, 2005; Murray & Greenberg, 2000;
school. The first set pertains to instructional con- Wentzel, 1998). Research on school bullying and
texts, and the second pertains to social-related- victimization has also revealed that children with
ness contexts. How teachers teach in classrooms larger circles of friends, higher levels of peer
has tremendous impact on student motivation acceptance, and lower levels of peer victimization
(Perry, Turner, & Meyer, 2006). On the basis of tend to like school more (Ladd, Kochenderfer, &
social-cognitive theories and empirical research Coleman, 1997). Students’ enthusiasm, interest,
findings in motivation and instructional strate- happiness, and comfort in school, then, seem to
gies, Lam, Pak, and Ma (2007) have identified be shaped by their sense of relatedness to others.
19 Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model 407

By contrast, feelings of boredom, frustration, performance goals are positive predictors of


sadness, and anxiety in the school are exacer- surface processing.
bated when children feel alienated from others. Attribution can also be an important anteced-
Other than social relatedness in school, social ent of student engagement. Weiner (1985) postu-
relatedness at home is also influential to student lated that differences in effort expenditure by
engagement in school. Family is one of the most students can be explained by differences in how
immediate microsystems for human develop- they explain their successes and failures. When
ment. Parent support is expected to play an students attribute success and failure to effort,
important role in student engagement in school. they are more likely to invest effort in future
It is well documented that parenting styles (e.g., tasks. Another potential determinant of students’
Donrbush, Ritter, Leiderman, Roberts, & effort expenditure is self-efficacy (Bandura,
Fraleigh, 1987) and parental involvement (e.g., 1977). Students with high self-efficacy believe
Hoover-Dempsey & Sandler, 1995) contribute to that they are capable of successfully performing
children’s academic performance in school. the course of action that will lead to success.
Students will be engaged in school when they They attempt challenging tasks and do not give
perceive that their parents have high expectation up easily. It is reasonable to expect that students
on them and provide them with encouragement with high self-efficacy tend to be engaged in
and assistance. school.

Personal Factors Overview of the Study

Some personal factors may have direct impact on The study reported in this chapter is a part of a
student engagement. They may mediate the effect multicountry project initiated by the International
of contextual factors on student engagement. It is School Psychology Association (Lam et al.,
well documented that some motivational beliefs 2009). Twelve countries (Austria, Canada,
are essential to students’ intrinsic interest and China, Cyprus, Estonia, Greece, Korea, Malta,
may be important proximal determinants of stu- Portugal, Romania, United Kingdom, and
dent engagement (see Schunk & Zimmerman, United States) participated in this project with
2006 for a review). These beliefs include goal the purpose of investigating both the personal
orientations (Dweck, 2006), attribution (Weiner, and contextual antecedents of student engage-
1985), and self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977). Students ment in schools across different countries. This
with learning goals are more persistent after fail- was a large-scale international project that
ure than students with performance goals (Lam, involved many themes of investigation. In this
Yim, Law, & Cheung, 2004). They focus on gain- chapter, the focus is on the validation of the
ing new skills and knowledge even if failures contextual model presented in Fig. 19.1. With
occur during the process. On the contrary, stu- the data from China, we examined how student
dents with performance goals focus on gaining engagement was associated with contextual fac-
positive evaluation of their ability. They tend to tors, personal factors, and student outcomes. It
avoid challenges when they are not sure that they is noteworthy that the relations among these
can gain positive feedback from others. Goal ori- constructs may be bidirectional. Better student
entations affect not only students’ persistence outcomes may reinforce student engagement,
and effort in learning but also their cognitive which in turn may have positive impact on per-
engagement (Elliot, McGregor, & Gable, 1999; sonal and contextual factors. Reciprocal rela-
Graham & Golan, 1991; Meece, Blumenfeld, & tionships between contextual factors and student
Hoyle, 1988; Nolen, 1988). Learning goals are engagement were found in previous research
positive predictors of deep processing, whereas (Skinner & Belmont, 1993).
408 S.-f. Lam et al.

Measures
Method
Student Engagement
Participants
Student engagement in school was measured by a
scale that consisted of three subscales, namely,
The participants were 822 junior secondary
affective engagement, behavioral engagement,
school students from three cities in China,
and cognitive engagement Subscales. The affec-
namely, Hangzhou, Hong Kong, and Kunming.
tive engagement subscale consisted of nine items
The three cities are located in different regions of
that measured the student’s liking for learning
the country and are considered as big cities in
and school (e.g., “I like what I am learning in
their regions. The population of these three cities
school.”). The behavioral engagement subscale
ranged from 6.25 million to 7 million. The sample
consisted of 12 items that measure students’ per-
consisted of 280 seventh graders, 236 eighth
sistence and effort in learning (e.g., “I try hard to
graders, and 306 ninth graders from the three
do well in school.”). The cognitive engagement
cities. About 34% of the students came from
subscale consisted of 12 items that measured stu-
Hangzhou, 29% from Hong Kong, and 37% from
dents’ use of meaningful information processing
Kunming. Parental consent was obtained in Hong
strategies in learning (e.g., “When I study, I try to
Kong and approval was sought from local educa-
connect what I am learning with my own experi-
tion authorities in Hangzhou and Kunming. All
ences.”). The students were asked to indicate
the students gave assent to the participation. Their
their agreement to the items in the affective and
mean age was 14.14, with a range of 12–19 and a
behavioral subscales on a 5-point Likert scale
standard deviation of 1.21. The percentages of
with 1 for strongly disagree and 5 for strongly
boys and girls were 54.8% and 45.2%, respec-
agree. As for the cognitive subscale, responses
tively. To make sure that the sample was repre-
were made on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 for
sentative of the average urban Chinese students,
never and 5 for always. We used the average of
the students were recruited from an ordinary
the three subscale scores to indicate student
school with an average academic performance in
engagement. A high score indicated high engage-
each city. Elite schools or special schools were
ment and a low score indicated otherwise. The
not included in the present study.
Cronbach’s a of the three subscale-scores was
.78 for this sample.
Procedures
Motivating Instructional Contexts
The participants were asked to complete a ques- Students’ perceptions of their teachers’ instruc-
tionnaire in their schools. The questionnaire tional practices were measured by the Motivating
included questions about their engagement in Instructional Contexts Inventory (MICI) (Lam
school and antecedent factors of their engage- et al., 2007). The MICI consisted of 24 items
ment. The questionnaire was either administered with four items in each of the six subscales: chal-
by project research assistants or the teachers in lenge (e.g., “Teachers give assignments at the
their respective schools. The survey was admin- right level, neither too difficult nor too easy.”),
istered at the end of a semester, and the students real-life significance (e.g., “Teachers point out
were asked to answer the questions with refer- the relation between the subject and our everyday
ence to their experience in that semester. At about life.”), curiosity (e.g., “During the course of
the same time, their teachers completed a rating teaching, teachers will pinpoint the intriguing
form to report each student’s academic perfor- part and demand us to think it over and sort it
mance and conduct in that semester. out.”), autonomy (e.g., “Teachers let us choose
19 Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model 409

exercises that match our individual interests.”), Functional Assessment of Academic Behavior
recognition (e.g., “Teachers give recognition to (Ysseldyke & Christenson, 2003). These items
our self-improvement and care not so much if we described parent involvement in their children’s
can win over others.”), and evaluation (e.g., learning, such as asking their children about
“When giving comments on our work, teachers school, monitoring their children’s academic
specifically point out those areas for improve- progress, and discussing schoolwork with their
ment instead of just grading it good or bad.”). children at home. The students indicated the fre-
These six subscales, respectively, measured stu- quency of their parent support as stated in these
dents’ perceptions of the proportion of their items on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 for never
teachers who had provided them with challeng- and 5 for always. A high score indicated percep-
ing tasks, ensured real-life significance in their tions of high parent support and a low score indi-
learning activities, stimulated their curiosity, cated otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of the eight
granted them autonomy, recognized their efforts, item-scores was .85 for this sample.
and provided useful feedback for their improve-
ment. The students were asked to indicate the Peer Support
proportions on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 for It was measured by the Caring Peer Relationships
none of them and 5 for all of them. We used the in School Scale of the California Healthy Kids
average of the six subscale-scores as an indicator Survey (WestEd, 2000). The scale consisted of
of the students’ perceptions of their teachers’ three items: (1) “At my school, I have a friend
instructional practices. A high score indicated who really cares about me”; (2) “At my school, I
that the students perceived that most of their have a friend who talks with me about my prob-
teachers adopted instructional practices that were lems”; and (3) “At my school, I have a friend
motivating. A low score indicated otherwise. The who helps me when I’m having a hard time.” The
Cronbach’s a of these six subscale-scores was students were asked to indicate how much they
.92 for this sample. agreed to these three statements on a 5-point
Likert scale with 1 for strongly disagree and 5 for
Teacher Support strongly agree. A high score indicated percep-
Student perception of teacher support was mea- tions of high peer support and a low score indi-
sured by the Caring Adult Relationships in School cated otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of the three
Scale of the California Healthy Kids Survey item-scores was .79 for this sample.
(WestEd, 2000). The scale consisted of three
items: (1) “At my school, there is a teacher who Aggression to Peers
cares about me”; (2) “At my school, there is a This was measured by a 7-item scale of peer
teacher who is kind to me”; and (3) “At my aggression (Hill & Werner, 2006). The students
school, there is a teacher who listens to me when indicated how often over the semester they had
I have something to say.” Students were asked to engaged in aggressive behaviors toward their
indicate how much they agreed to these three peers (e.g., “hit someone because you didn’t like
statements on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 for what that person said or did.”). Responses were
strongly disagree and 5 for strongly agree. A high made on a 5-point scale with 1 for never and 5 for
score indicated perception of high teacher sup- at least once every day. A high score indicated
port and a low score indicated otherwise. The high aggression to peers in the school and a low
Cronbach’s a of the three item-scores was .79 for score indicated otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of
this sample. the seven item-scores was .76 for this sample.

Parent Support Aggression from Peers


Student perception of parent support was mea- This was measured by a scale modified from the
sured by eight items adapted from the compo- scale that measured aggression to peers (Hill &
nents of home support for learning in the Werner, 2006). The students indicated how often
410 S.-f. Lam et al.

over the semester someone was aggressive to items were (1) “It’s important to me that the other
them (e.g., “Someone who didn’t like you hit students in my classes think that I am good at my
you.”). Responses were made on a 5-point scale work,” (2) “I’d like to show my teachers that I’m
with 1 for never and 5 for at least once every day. smarter than the other students in my classes,”
A high score indicated high aggression from and (3) “Doing better than other students in
peers in the school and a low score indicated oth- school is important to me.” The students were
erwise. The Cronbach’s a of the seven item- asked to indicate their agreement to these items
scores was .86 for this sample. on a 5-point scale with 1 for strongly disagree
and 5 for strongly agree. The average of the three
Self-Efficacy item-scores reflected the extent to which the stu-
This was measured by a 7-item scale adapted dents endorsed the goals to seek positive evalua-
from the self-efficacy scale used by Pintrich and tion of their performances or abilities. High
de Groot (1990). The students indicated the extent scores indicated high endorsement of perfor-
to which they agreed with the statement about mance approach goals and low scores indicated
their self-efficacy in learning (e.g., “I can do very otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of the three item-
well in this class if I work hard.”). Responses scores was .60 for this sample.
were made on a 5-point scale with 1 for strongly
disagree and 5 for strongly agree. We used the Performance Avoidance Goals
average of the five item-scores to indicate stu- These were also measured by a 3-item scale
dents’ self-efficacy. A high score indicated that adapted from the Scales of Achievement Goal
students believed strongly that they were capable Orientations (Midgley et al., 1998). These three
of successfully performing the course of action items were (1) “It’s very important to me that I
that would lead to success and a low score indi- don’t look stupid in my classes,” (2) “An impor-
cated otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of the five tant reason I do my school work is so that I don’t
item-scores was .74 for this sample. embarrass myself,” and (3) “The reason I do my
work is so others won’t think I’m dumb.” The
Learning Goals students were asked to indicate their agreement
A 3-item scale, adapted from the Scales of to these items on a 5-point scale with 1 for
Achievement Goal Orientations (Midgley et al., strongly disagree and 5 for strongly agree. The
1998), was used to measure learning goals. These average of the three item-scores reflected the
three items were (1) “I like school work best extent to which the students endorsed the goals to
when it really makes me think,” (2) “An impor- avoid negative evaluation of their performance or
tant reason I do my school work is because I want ability. A high score indicated high endorsement
to get better at it,” and (3) “I do my school work of performance avoidance goals and a low score
because I am interested in it.” The students were indicated otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of the
asked to indicate their agreement to these items three item-scores was .59 for this sample.
on a 5-point scale with 1 for strongly disagree
and 5 for strongly agree. The average of the three Attribution
item-scores reflected the extent to which the stu- To measure students’ beliefs in attribution, we
dents endorsed the goals to develop their ability asked them to indicate how much their academic
or master the task. A high score indicated high performances in that semester were influenced by
endorsement of learning goals and a low score their abilities, efforts, luck (e.g., boring learning
indicated otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of the materials), and situations (e.g., being sick). They
three item-scores was .68 for this sample. were asked to write down the percentage of each
factor’s contribution and the total was required to
Performance Approach Goals add up to 100%. The higher the percentage that a
These were also measured by a three-item scale student assigned to a factor indicated the more
adapted from the Scales of Achievement Goal that the student attributed his/her academic per-
Orientations (Midgley et al., 1998). These three formance to that factor.
19 Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model 411

Emotional Functioning
This was measured by a scale adapted from the
Results
Emotional Functioning Scale (Diener, Smith, &
Intraclass Correlations
Fujita, 1995). The item with the highest factor
loading in each of the six clusters of emotion in
Before completing the main analyses to examine
this scale was selected. The students were asked to
how student engagement was related to the ante-
indicate how often they had felt happiness, anxi-
cedent factors and outcomes, it was essential to
ety, anger, shame, sadness, or caring in that semes-
determine the proportion of total variance that
ter. Their responses were made on a 5-point scale
occurred systematically between the three cities,
with 1 for never and 5 for always. The scores for
i.e., the intraclass correlation (ICC). In the cur-
happiness and caring were averaged to indicate
rent study, the students were nested within cities.
positive emotion. The scores for anxiety, anger,
If the ICC was high, one could not treat the stu-
shame, and sadness were averaged to indicate
dents as independent subjects and do the analyses
negative emotion. The Cronbach’s a of the posi-
as if they were not nested within cities. Ignoring
tive emotion scores and negative emotion scores
their cities would have resulted in an overestima-
were .50 and .70, respectively, for this sample.
tion of the correlation among the variables. Lee
(2000) argued that researchers should consider a
Academic Performance multilevel analytic method when the ICC is more
The students’ academic performances were than trivial (i.e., greater than 10% of the total
reported by their teachers. The teachers reported variance in the outcome). To determine the ICC,
how much each of the students in their class was we conducted analyses of unconditional models
“good at school work,” had “good performance for the three subscales and the full scale of stu-
on tests,” and did “well on assignments.” They dent engagement using Hierarchical Linear
were asked to indicate their agreement to the Modeling (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). The
above three statements on a 5-point Likert scale between-city ICCs of affective engagement,
with 1 for strongly disagree and 5 for strongly behavioral engagement, cognitive engagement,
agree. The average of these three item-scores and the full scale were .07, .05, .04, and.04,
was used as an indicator of the students’ academic respectively. All the ICC indicated that less than
performances in school. A high score indicated 10% of the total variance in these variables
good academic performances and a low score occurred systematically between cities. Thus, it
indicated otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of the six was justifiable to pool the data from the three cit-
item-scores was .89 for this sample. ies and to run the analyses with the students as
independent subjects.
Conduct
The students’ conduct was also reported by their
teachers. The teachers reported how much each Student Engagement and Instructional
of the students in their class was “well behaved in Contexts
class,” “followed all of the rules,” and “never got
in trouble in class.” They were asked to indicate The means for the subscale scores of affective
their agreement to the above three statements on engagement, behavioral engagement, cognitive
a 5-point Likert scale with 1 for strongly disagree engagement, and the full-scale score of student
and 5 for strongly agree. The average of these engagement were 3.32, 3.56, 3.18, and 3.36,
three item-scores was used as an indicator of the respectively. The correlation coefficients of these
students’ conduct in school. A high score indi- scores with the subscale and full-scale scores of
cated good conduct and a low score indicated the Motivating Instructional Contexts Inventory
otherwise. The Cronbach’s a of the six item- are presented in Table 19.1. Given the many cor-
scores was .92 for this sample. relation tests and large sample size, attention
412 S.-f. Lam et al.

Table 19.1 Means of the subscale and full-scale scores of the Motivating Instructional Contexts Inventory (MICI) and
their correlations with student engagement
Affective Behavioral Cognitive Student
Mean (SD) engagement engagement engagement engagement
Challenge 2.80 (.86) .30** .24** .31** .34**
Curiosity 3.50 (.86) .39** .35** .31** .42**
Real-life significance 3.29 (.92) .47** .38** .35** .48**
Autonomy 2.77 (1.02) .35** .25** .30** .36**
Recognition 3.41 (.96) .32** .31** .26** .35**
Evaluation 3.11 (.86) .40** .34** .31** .42**
Full-scale score 3.15 (.78) .43** .36** .36** .46**
Note: **p < .01

Table 19.2 Means of the factors in the social-relatedness contexts and their correlations with student engagement
Affective Behavioral Cognitive Student
Mean (SD) engagement engagement engagement engagement
Teacher support 3.80 (.84) .46** .42** .32** .48**
Parent support 3.62 (.84) .32** .34** .30** .38**
Peer support 4.07 (.78) .25** .29** .25** .31**
Aggression toward peers 1.46 (.57) −.26** −.27** −.16** −.27**
Aggression from peers 1.50 (.72) −.10** −.09* −.01 −.08*
Note: **p < .01, * p < .05

should be focused on the effect size of the corre- the highest correlation with all the six motivating
lation instead of the p value. As suggested by instructional practices. It seemed that liking for
Cohen (1992), r = .1 − .23 is considered as small; learning and for school was most sensitive to
r = .24 − .36 is considered medium; and r > 0.37 is motivating instructional contexts.
considered as large. The correlations in Table 19.1
were mostly medium and large. Student engage-
ment was associated significantly with instruc- Student Engagement
tional contexts. The more the students perceived and Social-Relatedness Contexts
that their teachers assigned challenging work,
integrated real-life significance to learning tasks, The correlation coefficients between the factors in
aroused their curiosity, supported their autonomy, social-related contexts and the subscales and full
recognized their effort or improvement, and used scale of student engagement are presented in
formative evaluation, the more they reported that Table 19.2. All the correlation coefficients were
they were engaged affectively, behaviorally, and significant except the one between aggression
cognitively in school. It is noteworthy that among from peers and cognitive engagement. The results
the six instructional practices, the practice to inte- indicated that student engagement was related
grate real-life significance with learning tasks had closely to teacher support, parent support, peer
the highest correlation with student engagement. support, aggression to peers, and aggression from
We regressed student engagement on the six peers. It is interesting to note that teacher support
instructional practices and obtained similar results. had a stronger association with student engage-
Real-life significance had the highest predictive ment than parent support and peer support. We
power of student engagement (b = .33, p < .001). It regressed student engagement on all the five con-
is also noteworthy that, among the three subscales textual variables and found that teacher support
of student engagement, affective engagement had had the highest predictive power of student
19 Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model 413

Table 19.3 Means of motivational beliefs and their correlations with student engagement
Affective Behavioral Cognitive Student
Mean (SD) engagement engagement engagement engagement
Self-efficacy 3.83 (.61) .41** .55** .46** .56**
Learning goals 3.36 (.83) .58** .52** .45** .62**
Performance approach goals 3.20 (.78) .22** .17** .25** .26**
Performance avoidance goals 2.80 (.84) −.06 −.11** .04 −.05
Effort attribution 37.50 (19.20) .20** .17** .14** 20**
Ability attribution 29.46 (17.21) −.03 .03 .01 .00
Luck attribution 12.25 (11.75) −.06 −.06 −.07* −.08*
Situation attribution 21.59 (14.98) −.20** −.29** −.14** −.21**
Note: **p < .01, *p < .05

engagement (b = .35, p < .001). In addition, p < .01). Among the four types of attribution,
Table 19.2 shows that aggression to peers had a effort attribution and situation attribution had the
stronger association with student engagement than strongest association with student engagement.
aggression from peers. The results of the multiple The more the students attributed their academic
regression analysis also corroborated with this performances to their efforts, the more they would
finding. Aggression to peers had higher predictive be engaged in school. By contrast, the more the
power of student engagement (b = −.26, p < .001) students attributed their academic performances
than aggression from peers (b = −.14, p < .001). In to situations, such as teachers’ teaching strategies
other words, the chances for the bullies to be dis- or boring learning materials, the less they would
engaged from school were higher than those of the be engaged. In addition, the associations of ability
victims who got bullied. attribution and luck attribution with student
engagement were not obvious.
Given the strong association between self-
Student Engagement and Motivational efficacy and student engagement, self-efficacy
Beliefs was very likely a mediator in the relationship
between instructional contexts and student
The correlation coefficients between the motiva- engagement. To verify this mediation model, we
tional beliefs and the subscales and full scale of examined the mediation effect of self-efficacy in
student engagement are presented in Table 19.3. the relationship between instructional contexts
Self-efficacy had a strong association with stu- and student engagement. The Sobel Test indi-
dent engagement. The more the students believed cated that the effect of instructional contexts on
that they were capable of successfully performing student engagement was mediated partially by
the course of action that would lead to success, self-efficacy, z = 6.63, p < .01. The indirect and
the more they were engaged affectively, behavior- direct effects of instructional contexts on student
ally, and cognitively in school. Among the three engagement were .12, p < .01, and .35, p < .01,
goal orientations, learning goals had the strongest respectively.
association with student engagement. It is note-
worthy that performance approach goals were
also associated positively with student engage- Student Engagement and Student
ment although the effect size was not as big as Outcomes
that of learning goals. Performance avoidance
goals did not have much association with student The correlation coefficients between the four out-
engagement although it had a small negative asso- come variables and the subscales and full scale of
ciation with behavioral engagement (r = −.11, student engagement are presented in Table 19.4.
414 S.-f. Lam et al.

Table 19.4 Means of the student outcomes and their correlations with student engagement
Affective Behavioral Cognitive Student
Mean (SD) engagement engagement engagement engagement
Positive emotion 3.63 (.92) .38** .32** .26** .38**
Negative emotions 2.55 (.76) −.06 −.03 .02 −.03
Academic performance 3.72 (1.05) .18** .24** .15** .22**
Conduct 3.99 (.98) .16** .18** .07* .16**
Note: **p < .01, *p < .05

As predicted, student engagement was correlated they experienced positive emotions frequently
significantly with positive emotions. The more and their teachers rated them high on academic
the students reported that they were engaged performance and conduct.
affectively, behaviorally, and cognitively in The data of the present study were collected
school, the more they would report that they often from junior secondary students in China, a devel-
had positive emotions. However, there was little oping country where a collectivistic culture pre-
association between negative emotions and stu- vails. One may query the generalizability of the
dent engagement. Both academic performances results to other countries with different cultures.
and conduct had significant correlations with stu- As this is a subproject of an international research
dent engagement. The more the students reported project that involved 12 countries (Lam et al.,
that they were engaged in school, the more their 2009), cross-country comparisons could be made.
teachers would report that they had good aca- Lam et al. found that the proposed contextual
demic performance and conduct. model was consistent across the 12 countries.
They found that how student engagement was
related to the contextual factors and student out-
Discussion comes did not vary between countries according
to gross domestic product (GDP) per capita, an
With the conceptualization and measurement of important indicator of economic development.
student engagement with indicators in three Neither did the relationships vary between coun-
dimensions that were unpacked from facilitators tries according to Hofstede’s Individualism Index
and outcomes, we were able to investigate how (2009), an indicator of cultural value that distin-
student engagement was associated with its ante- guished individualistic cultures from collectivis-
cedents and outcomes. The results indicated that tic cultures. The contextual model in Fig. 19.1 is
student engagement was associated significantly valid for the 12 countries although they are very
with the contextual factors, motivational beliefs, different in economic development and cultures.
and student outcomes. They provided empirical There are more cultural similarities than differ-
support to the contextual model proposed in ences when it comes to matters about how stu-
Fig. 19.1. Students were engaged affectively, dent engagement is related to its contexts,
behaviorally, and cognitively in school when they antecedents, and outcomes.
felt that their teachers adopted motivating instruc-
tional practices and they had social-emotional
support from their teachers, parents, and peers. Instructional and Social-Relatedness
Their engagement in school was also high when Contexts
they had high self-efficacy, endorsed learning
goals, and attributed their academic performances The results of the present study indicated that
to how much effort they had made. Most impor- instructional contexts were related closely to
tantly, when students were engaged in schools, student engagement. A close examination of the
19 Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model 415

correlations between student engagement and the because they may provide leverage to uplift
subscales of the Motivating Instructional Contexts student engagement as a whole.
Inventory revealed an interesting pattern. Real- In the present study, most of the factors in
life significance stood out to be the subscale that social-relatedness contexts were associated with
had the highest correlation with student engage- student engagement. The most outstanding one
ment. The more the students perceived that many was teacher support. Its correlation with the full-
of their teachers integrated real-life significance scale score of student engagement (r = .48, p < .01)
into their learning tasks, the more they reported was much higher than those of parent support
that they were engaged affectively, behaviorally, (r = .38, p < .01) and student support (r = .32,
and cognitively in school. According to expec- p < .01). The findings that peer support ranked the
tancy x value theory, the amount of effort invested last does not seem to be consistent with the com-
in a task is a product of expectation of success and mon belief that peers are influential to adoles-
the values of the task (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). cents. However, these findings are understandable
To increase the value of a task, one strategy is to when the support from teachers, parents, and
incorporate real-life significance into the task. teachers is compared across various outcomes of
Students are more likely to be interested in a task children. In a study with sixth grade students,
and to think highly of its successful completion if Wentzel (1998) found that peer support was a
it is relevant to their lives. The results of the pres- positive predictor of prosocial goal pursuit,
ent study support the claims of the expectancy x teacher support was a positive predictor of class-
value theory and have important implications for related and school-related interest, and parent
instructional practices. To enhance student support was a positive predictor of school-related
engagement, teachers need to provide learning interest and goal orientations. Different outcomes
materials and activities relevant to their students’ were associated with support from different
real-life experiences. Instructional strategies spe- socializing agents. Peer support was still impor-
cific to the promotion of real-life significance tant; however, it was not as important as teacher
include explaining the text with reference to daily and parent support when the matter of concern
life examples and pointing out the practical use of was school-related interest. The findings of the
the learning activities. present study supported the importance of teacher
Among the three dimensions of student support in student engagement. Students will be
engagement, affective engagement had the high- engaged in school when they feel that their teach-
est positive association with instructional con- ers provide them with social-emotional support.
texts. This seemed to be the most responsive and Teacher support can be a pivotal factor in the
sensitive dimension to motivating instructional enhancement of student engagement.
strategies. Affective engagement refers to the In the present study, aggression to peers was
intrinsic motivation (liking for learning) and found to have negative association with student
school bonding (liking for school). It is the direct engagement (r = −.27, p < .01). This association
feeling toward learning and school. Compared to was much higher than that of aggression to peers
behavioral and cognitive engagement, the (r = −.08, p < .05) with student engagement. The
response of affective engagement to instructional findings suggest that the chances for the bullies to
contexts may be more direct and intuitive. In a be disengaged from school are higher than those
longitudinal study of the internal dynamics of of the victims who get bullied. The vulnerable
student engagement, Skinner et al. (2008) found victims are usually the center of attention for the
that emotional components of engagement con- research in peer aggression (Juvonen & Graham,
tributed significantly to changes in their behav- 2001; Ladd et al., 1997). However, the findings
ioral counterparts. The affective dimension may of the present study remind us that the bullies
be the engine that drives the other dimensions of may be more susceptible to disengagement from
student engagement. Interventions that target school. They are also a group of students who
affective dimension are particularly important need attention from researchers and educators.
416 S.-f. Lam et al.

Motivational Beliefs study, its results were consistent with those of the
previous correlational studies and showed that
As presented in Table 19.3, self-efficacy was performance approach goals had a positive asso-
associated positively with student engagement. ciation with student engagement. It was only the
The more the students believed that they were performance avoidance goals that had any nega-
capable of successfully performing the course of tive association with behavioral engagement. The
action that would lead to success, the more they discrepancy between the findings of experimental
were engaged in schools. It was found in the pres- and correlational studies may be due to the differ-
ent study that self-efficacy was a mediator in the ences in methodology. Studies with experimental
relationship between instructional contexts and design usually manipulate performance goals and
student engagement. The instructional contexts look into how students with these goals respond
had impact on students’ self-efficacy, which in to setbacks. As Dweck described clearly in a
turn had impact student engagement. This media- seminal paper (1986), performance goals with
tion model can be explained by expectancy high self-confidence are as motivating as learn-
x value theory (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). ing goals. It is only in the condition of low
According to this theory, the amount of effort self-confidence that performance goals will elicit
invested in a task is a product of expectation of avoidance and self-handicapping behaviors.
success and the values of the task, the increase in Performance goals with high self-confidence are
the expectation of success would be motivational. similar to performance approach goals, whereas
When teachers adopt instructional practices that performance goals with low self-confidence are
enable students to master challenging tasks suc- similar to performance avoidance goals. It is
cessfully, they will increase their students’ self- understandable that correlational studies, without
efficacy. As indicated in the challenge subscale of controlling the level of self-confidence, will find
the Motivating Instructional Contexts Inventory, that performance approach goals are associated
these instructional practices include providing with positive outcomes. The positive role of per-
scaffolding and assigning a task at the appropriate formance approach goals in learning and achieve-
difficulty level. The results of the mediation analy- ment is unstable because it may turn negative
sis showed that the more the teachers adopted these once self-confidence is low. Educators must be
practices, the more the students would feel effica- cautious in promoting performance approach
cious. When the students felt more efficacious, goals because it may backfire when learning
they would be more engaged in school. These becomes difficult and challenging.
results illustrate the mechanism by which instruc- The results of the present study indicated that,
tional contexts affect student engagement. They among the four types of attribution, effort attribu-
help teachers understand how they can enhance tion had the highest correlation with student
student engagement by promoting self-efficacy. engagement. The more the students attributed
Among the three goal orientations, learning their academic performances to their efforts, the
goals had the strongest correlation with student more they reported that they were engaged affec-
engagement. Nevertheless, performance approach tively, behaviorally, and cognitively in school.
goals also had a positive association with student Effort is an internal, controllable, and changeable
engagement. The role of performance approach factor (Weiner, 1985). Students will believe that
goals in learning and achievement has been con- they can control and change their academic per-
troversial (Midgley, Kaplan, & Middleton, 2001). formance if they endorse effort attribution. By
Experimental studies with manipulation (e.g., contrast, attribution to external and uncontrolla-
Lam et al., 2004) have usually shown that perfor- ble factors, such as luck and situation, does not
mance goals have detrimental effects on learning help them think that they can control and change
and achievement, but correlational studies with their academic performance. It is interesting to
observed data (e.g., Pintrich, 2000) showed other- note that ability attribution did not have any asso-
wise. As the present study was also a correlational ciation with student engagement. Ability is an
19 Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model 417

internal factor, but whether it is controllable and for learning and for school, the results of the
changeable depends on one’s implicit theory of present study would be much stronger if mea-
intelligence (Dweck, 1986). If students believe sures other than self-reports were included. For
that ability is inherited and nonmalleable, ability example, instructional contexts can be measured
attribution does not help them much. This is par- with a third party’s observation. This objective
ticularly so in the face of setback. Out of good measure might provide stronger evidence for the
intention, many teachers may praise their stu- current contextual model.
dents’ abilities for good academic performance. The findings of the present study have only
This practice may also backfire if their students presented a general picture or overview about
believe that ability is inherited and nonmalleable how student engagement is related to its anteced-
(Mueller & Dweck, 1998). ents and outcomes in a contextual model. It pro-
vides understanding in a broad stroke. Details
about the mechanisms among the variables in
Limitations and Future Directions this contextual model still need further investiga-
tion. For example, we only did one mediation
The present study has provided support to a analysis to see the relationship among instruc-
contextual model for understanding student tional contexts, self-efficacy, and student engage-
engagement. With the conceptualization and ment. Actually, there may be more mediation
measurement of student engagement with indica- relations among other variables in this contextual
tors in three dimensions that were unpacked from model. In addition, the internal dynamics among
facilitators and outcomes, the present study the three dimensions of student engagement
showed how student engagement was associated should also be studied. Further investigation into
with its antecedents and outcomes. The findings the details of this contextual model will definitely
have significant implications for strategies for the enhance our understanding of student engage-
enhancement of student engagement. ment and its facilitators.
Despite its contributions, the present study
also has some obvious limitations. This is a cor-
relational study with observed data, so causal References
relations between variables cannot be ascertained.
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J.
To address this limitation, future studies may
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con-
consider field experiments on the effects of inter- ceptual and methodological issues of the construct.
vention (e.g., motivating instructional practices) Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386.
on student engagement. Another possibility is to Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A.
L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological
employ longitudinal designs that allow time
engagement: Validation of the student engagement
series analyses in field studies. With longitudinal instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44,
data, one can justify the temporal ordering of 427–445.
variables and possible causal effects according to Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Fallu, J.-S., & Pagani, L. S.
(2009). Student engagement and its relationship with
the time of measurement.
early high school dropout. Journal of Adolescence, 32,
Another limitation of the present study is its 651–670.
dependence on self-report measures from stu- Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying the-
dents. Almost all of the measures were reported ory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84,
191–215.
by students. The exceptions were the measures of
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher-child
academic performance and conduct, which were relationship and children’s early school adjustment.
reported by teachers. There is a possibility of Journal of School Psychology, 35, 61–79.
inflation of correlations when variables are mea- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a
context for human development: Research perspec-
sured at the same time from the same participants.
tives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 723–742.
Although self-reports are valid measures of sub- Chung, I. J., Hill, K. G., Hawkins, J. D., Gilchrist, L. D.,
jective psychological constructs, such as liking & Nagin, D. S. (2002). Childhood predictors of offense
418 S.-f. Lam et al.

trajectories. Journal of Research in Crime and and liking school in elementary school. Journal of
Delinquency, 39, 60–90. School Psychology, 43, 281–301.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, Graham, S., & Golan, S. (1991). Motivational influences
112, 155–159. on cognition: Task involvement, ego involvement,
Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994). and depth of information processing. Journal of
Educational risk and resilience in African-American Educational Psychology, 83, 187–194.
youth: Context, self, action, and outcomes in school. Hawkins, J. D., Gou, J. G., Hill, K. G., Battin-Pearson, S.,
Child Development 65, 493–506. & Abbott, R. D. (2001). Long term effects of the
Connell, M. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, Seattle social development intervention on school
autonomy and relatedness: A motivational analysis of bonding trajectories. Applied Developmental Science,
self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe 5, 225–236.
(Eds.), Self processes and development: The Minnesota Hill, L. G., & Werner, N. E. (2006). Affiliative motivation,
symposia on child psychology (pp. 43–78). Hillsdale, school attachment, and aggression in school.
NJ: Erlbaum. Psychology in the Schools, 43, 231–246.
Diener, E., Smith, H., & Fujita, F. (1995). The personality Hofstede, G.. (2009). Geert HosfstedeTM cultural dimen-
structure of affect. Journal of Personality and Social sions. Retrieved from http://www.geert-hofstede.com/
Psychology, 69, 130–141. hofstede_dimensions.php.
Donrbush, S. M., Ritter, P. L., Leiderman, P. H., Roberts, Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., & Sandler, H. M. (1995). Parental
D. F., & Fraleigh, M. J. (1987). The relation of parent- involvement in children’s education: Why does it make
ing style to adolescent school performance. Child a difference? Teachers College Record, 97, 310–331.
Development, 58, 1244–1257. Jimerson, S., Campos, E., & Greif, J. (2003). Towards an
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting understanding of definitions and measures of school
learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. engagement and related terms. The California School
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of Psychologist, 8, 7–28.
success. New York: Random House. Juvonen, J., & Graham, S. (Eds.). (2001). Peer harass-
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., ment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and vic-
Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development timized. New York: Guilford Press.
during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C.
fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools and in (1997). Classroom peer acceptance, friendship, and
families. American Psychologist, 48, 90–101. victimization: Distinct relational systems that contrib-
Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Gable, S. (1999). ute uniquely to children’s school adjustment? Child
Achievement goals, study strategies, and exam perfor- Development, 68, 1181–1197.
mance: A mediational analysis. Journal of Educational Lam, S.-f., Jimerson, S., Basnett, J., Cefai, C., Duck, R.,
Psychology, 91(3), 549–563. Farrell, P., et al. (2009, July). Exploring student
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of engagement in schools internationally: A collabora-
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. tive international study yields further insights. A sym-
Finn, J. D., Pannozzo, G. M., & Voelkl, K. E. (1995). posium at the 31st annual International School
Disruptive and inattentive withdrawn behavior and Psychology Association Colloquium, Malta.
achievement among fourth graders. The Elementary Lam, S.-f., Pak, T. S., & Ma, W. Y. K. (2007). Motivating
School Journal, 95, 421–454. Instructional Contexts Inventory. In P. R. Zelick (Ed.),
Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success Issues in the psychology of motivation (pp. 119–136).
among students at risk for school failure. Journal of Huppauge, NJ: Nova Science.
Applied Psychology, 82, 221–234. Lam, S.-f., Yim, P.-s., Law, J. S. F., & Cheung, R. W. Y.
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). (2004). The Effects of competition on achievement
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of motivation in Chinese classrooms. British Journal of
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, Educational Psychology, 74, 281–296.
59–109. Lee, V. (2000). Using hierarchical linear modeling to
Furlong, M. J., & Christenson, S. L. (2008). Engaging stu- study social contexts: The case of school effects.
dents at school and with learning: A relevant construct Educational Psychologist, 35, 125–141.
for ALL students. Psychology in the Schools, 45, Maddox, S. J., & Prinz, R. J. (2003). School bonding in
365–368. children and adolescents: Conceptualization, assess-
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a ment, and associated variables. Clinical Child and
factor in children’s academic engagement and perfor- Family Psychology Review, 6, 31–49.
mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional
148–162. activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high
Gauch, H. G. (2003). Scientific method in practice. school years. American Educational Research Journal,
Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. 37, 153–184.
Gest, S. D., Welsh, J. A., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2005). Meece, J. L., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Hoyle, R. H. (1988).
Behavioral predictors of changes in social relatedness Student’s goal orientations and cognitive engagement
19 Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model 419

in classroom activities. Journal of Educational Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic
Psychology, 80, 514–523. motivations: Classic definitions and new directions.
Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. (2001). Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67.
Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2006). Competence
whom, under what circumstances, and at what cost? and control beliefs: Distinguish the means and ends. In
Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77–86. P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winnie (Eds.), Handbook of
Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., Middleton, M., Maehr, M. L., educational psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 349–367).
Urdan, T., Anderman, L. H., et al. (1998). The devel- Manhwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
opment and validation of scales assessing students’ Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Lehr, C. A., & Anderson,
achievement goal orientations. Contemporary A. R. (2003). Facilitating school engagement: Lessons
Educational Psychology, 23(2), 113–131. learned from Check & Connect longitudinal studies.
Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelli- The California School Psychologists, 8, 29–41.
gence can undermine children’s motivation and per- Skinner, E., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the
formance. Journal of Personality and Social classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and
Psychology, 75, 33–52. student engagement across the school year. Journal of
Murray, C., & Greenberg, M. T. (2000). Children’s Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581.
relationships with teachers and bonds with school: Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, T.
An investigation of patterns and correlates in mid- (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom:
dle childhood. Journal of School Psychology, 38, Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of
423–445. Educational Psychology, 100, 765–781.
Nolen, S. B. (1988). Reasons for studying: Motivational Voelkl, K. E. (1997). Identification with school. American
orientations and study strategies. Cognition and Journal of Education, 105, 204–319.
Instruction, 5, 269–287. Walker, C. O., Greene, B. A., & Mansell, R. A. (2006).
O’Farrell, S. L., & Morrison, G. M. (2003). A factor anal- Identification with academics, intrinsic/extrinsic moti-
ysis exploring school bonding and related constructs vation, and self-efficacy as predictors of cognitive
among upper elementary students. The California engagement. Learning and Individual Differences, 16,
School Psychologist, 8, 53–72. 1–12.
Perry, N. E., Turner, J. C., & Meyer, D. K. (2006). Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement
Classrooms as contexts for motivating learning. In P. motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92,
A. Alexander & P. H. Winnie (Eds.), Handbook of 548–573.
educational psychology (Vol. 2). Mahwah, NJ: Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social relationships and motiva-
Lawrence Erlbaum. tion in middle school: The role of parents, teachers and
Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: peers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90,
The role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. 202–209.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 544–555. WestEd. (2000). California healthy kids survey. Los
Pintrich, P. R., & de Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and Alamitos, CA: WestEd.
self-regulated learning components of classroom aca- Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value
demic performance. Journal of Educational theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary
Psychology, 82(1), 33–40. Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical Ysseldyke, J., & Christenson, S. (2003). Functional
linear models: Applications and data analysis methods assessment of academic behavior: Creating successful
(2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. learning environments. Longmont, CO: Sopris West.
Allowing Choice and Nurturing
an Inner Compass: Educational 20
Practices Supporting Students’
Need for Autonomy*

Avi Assor

Abstract
This chapter focuses on seven practices of autonomy support which are
likely to promote two major components of the need for autonomy:
(a) lack of coercion and optional choice and (b) formation and realization
of an inner compass: authentic, direction-giving values, goals, and inter-
ests. A special emphasis is put on research pertaining to three autonomy
supportive practices which are assumed to support formation and realiza-
tion of authentic, direction-giving values, goals, and interests, whose
impact on perceived autonomy was not sufficiently examined so far:
(a) IVD – intrinsic value demonstration, (b) SVE – support for value/goal/
interest examination, and (c) FIV – fostering inner-directed valuing pro-
cesses. The autonomy supportive practices that foster the development of
stable authentic values and goals might be especially important in western
countries, in which postmodern moral relativism and the abundance of
information and options make it particularly difficult for youth to form
stable and authentic values and goals.

The hope to see students motivated and engaged theory (SDT, Ryan & Deci, 2000), one major fac-
in activities that contribute to their intellectual, tor which may explain why students are often
socioemotional, and moral growth is widely poorly motivated and poorly engaged is that they
shared. However, students often appear less do not feel that school-related activities support
and less engaged in learning as they grow older their need for autonomy.
(e.g., Gottfried, Marcoulides, Gottfried, Oliver, & The need for autonomy is conceptualized in
Guerin, 2007). According to self-determination this chapter as involving two major components:
(a) the striving to avoid coercion and have
*The research reported in this chapter was supported by optional choice and (b) the striving to form and
grants from the Israel Science Foundation, the US-Israel realize authentic and direction-giving values,
Binational Science Foundation, and the chief scientist of
the Israel education ministry.
goals, and interests (i.e., the striving for an inner
compass). In this chapter, I describe various prac-
A. Assor (*)
Educational and School Psychology Program,
tices of teachers, parents, and schools that can
Ben Gurion University, Be’er-Sheva, Israel support students’ need for autonomy and there-
e-mail: assor@bgu.ac.il fore promote engagement. In particular, I describe

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 421


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_20, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
422 A. Assor

three practices that promote the examination and nonoptimal, and (2) Motives or intentions that
(and consequent formation) of authentic goals and are experienced as autonomous, emanating from
values and are relatively underemphasized in one’s true self, highly volitional, and therefore
extant work on autonomy support: leading to full engagement and well-being (e.g.,
(a) Intrinsic value demonstration (IVD) Ryan & Deci, 2000). Specifically, controlled
(b) Support for value/goal/interest examination motives can be driven by (1) the desire to avoid
(SVE) external punishments and threats and/or the hope to
(c) Fostering inner-directed valuing processes attain rewards (e.g., doing your homework in order
(FIV) to avoid being grounded or in order to win a desired
These practices are especially important in the gadget) and/or (2) the desire to avoid internal feel-
postmodern era in which many face considerable ings of guilt and shame and/or the hope to feel
value confusion. grand and unique (e.g., Assor, Vansteenkiste, &
Kaplan, 2009). Autonomous motives can be guided
by (1) the perception of the task as valuable, per-
Motivation haps even central to the realization of one’s central
values (but not necessarily pleasant), and/or (2) the
I view the concept of motivation as referring to perception of the task as interesting and enjoyable.
people’s intentions to perform actions. These The concept of engagement refers to the
inclinations or intentions (i.e., motives) have two amount and quality of actual efforts and actions
important attributes: intensity (strength) and phe- aimed at reaching a certain goal. While motives
nomenological quality. The intensity dimension refer to intentions or inclinations to do something
refers to the amount of effort which people intend in order to reach a certain goal, engagement refers
to put in an attempt to reach a certain goal, often to actual actions that are performed as one
in the face of difficulties. The quality dimension attempts to reach a certain goal. Simply put, the
refers to people’s perception and experience of difference between motives and engagement is
the reasons or sources of their intentions or the difference between goal-oriented intention
motives. Specifically, when people perceive their and action. If the goal is learning new concepts or
intentions as emanating from their authentic self, skills, then people can differ in the amount of
the phenomenological quality of the motivation effort they invest (i.e., persistence, determina-
is high because the intentions are experienced as tion), as well as the effectiveness, flexibility, or
autonomous, whereas intentions that are per- creativity that characterize their efforts. Research
ceived as unauthentic are experienced as control- guided by SDT indicates that while controlling
ling and unpleasant (e.g., Roth, Assor, Niemiec, motives can lead to a great deal of effort, autono-
Ryan, & Deci, 2009; Ryan & Deci, 2000). For mous motives are much more likely to promote
example, two students may intend to invest a flexible and creative engagement in learning
great deal of effort in a school assignment, so the (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 1985; Roth et al., 2009).
intensity of their motivation is similar (both are Given the pleasant emotional experience asso-
high on intensity). However, the quality of the ciated with autonomous motives and the role of
motivation may be high for the student who per- such motives in facilitating flexible and creative
ceives the assignment as something that she/he engagement in learning, it is important to discover
would authentically want to do. In contrast, the factors which may contribute to autonomous
quality of the motivation to do the assignment motivation. SDT posits that there are at least three
would be low for the person who feels that the basic human needs whose satisfaction promotes
assignment is a task that is completely uncon- autonomous motivation and therefore high-quality
nected to her/his authentic values and interests. engagement: the need for relatedness, compe-
Based on SDT, then, I differentiate between: (1) tence, and autonomy (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2000).
Motives or intentions that are experienced as con- While the needs for competence and relatedness
trolling, nonautonomous and therefore stressful have received considerable attention from other
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 423

theorists (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Elliot notion of “Freedom For,” as well as Berlin’s
& Dweck, 2005; White, 1959), SDT is unique in (1969) notion of positively defined autonomy
its emphasis on the need for autonomy. (see also Aviram & Assor, 2010). The formation
of this inner compass is very important because it
provides inner criteria for making important
Freedom from Coercion and Optional decisions. When people do not have clear and
Choice authentic values, goals, and interests, the avail-
ability of choices might be a threat or a burden, as
The first striving within the need for autonomy, to indicated in Fromm’s writings on the phenomena
be free from coercion and have the possibility to of escape from freedom. It is only when one has
choose one’s actions, is similar to Fromm’s clear and authentic inner compass that one wel-
(1941) notion of “Freedom From,” as well as comes choice (if this choice is meaningful; see
Berlin’s (1969) notion of negatively defined Katz & Assor, 2007). Thus, it is possible that the
autonomy (see also Aviram & Assor, 2010, on finding that people feel burdened by too much
this issue). Research anchored mainly in SDT has consumerist choice (e.g., Iyengar & Lepper,
shown that when people are pressured and coerced 2000) may at least, in part, be a product of lack of
(from outside or from within) to behave in spe- clear and authentic values which enable people to
cific ways, they experience frustration. This frus- quickly discard many of the choices offered to
tration has been shown to undermine engagement, them as irrelevant and harmful.
well-being, and vitality (e.g., Assor, Kaplan, Authentic, direction-giving goals, values, and
Kanat-Maymon, & Roth, 2005; Assor, Kaplan, & interests also provide people with internal criteria
Roth, 2002; Reeve, 2006; Reeve & Jang, 2006). for evaluating others and themselves and a foun-
As for choice, there is ample research showing dation a for feeling that their actions are coherent
that people, in general. prefer to have the option and meaningful, and they also make people less
to choose (see Patall, Cooper & Robinson, 2008), dependent on others’ evaluations (Assor, 2010,
although they do not always need to be the ones 2011; Reeve & Assor, 2011). Highly developed
who make the choice, especially when someone and authentic value systems which guide deci-
else chooses for them what they anyway want sions and actions function as elaborate, multilevel,
(e.g., Katz & Assor, 2007). For example, Katz categories which are anchored in a more general
and Assor showed that when parents choose for self and world view, embedded in a historical
children learning and leisure activities that chil- perspective (e.g., Assor, 1999, 2011; Assor,
dren have a sustained interest in, children will- Cohen-Melayev, Kaplan, & Friedman, 2005).
ingly engage in these activities. In contrast, when
parents choose activities that are inconsistent
with the child’s interests, children do feel con- Teacher, Parent, and School Practices
trolled and nonautonomous. Thus, it appears that That Support the Need for Autonomy
while people do not always have to choose things
by themselves, they do want to have the option to In this section, I will describe seven practices of
choose so that if they lose trust in a person who teachers, parents, and schools that are likely to
does the choice for them, they could determine support the two components of the need for auton-
their choices and actions themselves. omy. Research demonstrating the contributions of
these practices to students’ perceived autonomy,
engagement, achievement, high-quality learning,
Inner Compass and/or well-being will be briefly described. I will
start with practices that support the striving for no
The striving to develop and realize direction- coercion and optional choice and then move to
giving and authentic values, goals, and interests practices supporting the formation of authentic
(an inner compass) is similar to Fromm’s (1941) values, goals, and interests (Fig. 20.1).
424 A. Assor

Autonomy supportive Components of the


practices need for autonomy

Minimizing controls

Perspective taking/empathy, openness No coercion,


to criticism and respect – especially optional choice
when children do not behave in line
with expectations or disagree

Providing choice
Formation &
Providing authentic rationale realization of
Intrinsic value demonstration (IVD) – direction-giving,
especially in moral & emotion regulation authentic, goals,
domains
values &
Supporting value/goal/interest interests (inner
examination (SVE)
compass)
Fostering inner directed valuing (FIV)

Fig. 20.1 Practices supporting the need for autonomy

Minimizing Controls bribes or material rewards that are not informa-


tive in terms of level or quality of one’s perfor-
This practice refers to behaviors of other people mance. Research surveyed by Deci, Koestner,
or features of the educational context which cause and Ryan (1999) indicated that when people are
students to feel controlled. Minimizing controls already intrinsically motivated to perform certain
may be a necessary but not sufficient condition tasks (including learning), the offering of mate-
for supporting the need for autonomy. That is, rial rewards, in general, tends to undermine
while the presence of controls can undermine the intrinsic motivation, perceived autonomy, and
need for autonomy, its absence may not be enough performance quality. Other external controls
to make people feel that they can choose and involve direct commands and surveillance,
organize their actions or formulate and realize imposing of deadlines, intruding and interfering
direction-giving values, goals, and interests. with the child’s natural rhythm of work, and sup-
Soenens and Vansteenkiste (2010) describe pressing the expression of disagreement or criti-
two forms of control: (a) behaviors that pressure cal opinions.
one to behave in a specific way in order to avoid One study that examined the impact of the
unpleasant bodily experiences, loss of material teachers’ use of external controls on children was
benefits and privileges, as well as in order to gain conducted by Assor, Kaplan, Kanat-Maymon and
various material benefits and privileges (external Roth (2005). Israeli fourth and fifth graders com-
control), and (b) behaviors that pressure one to pleted questionnaires assessing teachers’ ten-
behave in a specific way in order to feel worthy dency to intrude and interfere as children worked
of love and esteem (internal control; see also on their assignments, as well as to discourage any
Assor, Roth & Deci, 2004). answer that diverts from teachers’ opinion.
External control includes behaviors such as Children’s academic engagement was assessed
physical punishment, withdrawing privileges, by their primary teachers. Path analyses sup-
physical threats, and power assertion, as well as ported the hypothesis that children’s perceptions
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 425

of their teachers as using external controls arouse et al., thus indicating that the negative emotional
children’s anger and anxiety, and these emotions effects of parental conditional regard are not sim-
then undermine academic engagement. Similarly, ply an artifact of adolescents’ self-reports.
Assor et al. (2002), in a study conducted with Indirect evidence for the autonomy suppres-
Israeli students in grades 3–8, also showed that sive nature of conditional regard comes also from
interfering with children’s or early adolescents’ research on the construct of psychological con-
preferred pace of learning and not allowing criti- trol. The concept and measure of psychological
cal and independent opinions predicted negative control (Barber, 1996; Barber, Stolz, & Olsen,
emotions during learning and poor academic 2005) is essentially similar to that of conditional
engagement. Importantly, these findings were negative regard, as both concepts include clear
obtained also when the effects of autonomy sup- elements of love withdrawal when children do
portive behaviors such as providing choice were not comply with parents’ expectations. Research
held constant via regression analyses. on this widely used concept has shown that par-
Research by Assor, Roth, and their colleagues ents’ use of psychological control in the domain
(Assor & Roth, 2005; Assor, Roth, & Deci, 2004; of achievement predicts a variety of maladaptive
Roth et al., 2009) has identified one type of con- child outcomes such as depressive feelings, poor
trol that undermines students’ need of autonomy self esteem, and maladaptive perfectionism (e.g.,
by linking their sense of love-worthiness and Barber et al., 2005; Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
self-worth to the enactment of expected behav- 2010; Soenens et al., 2005). The offspring par-
iors. This type of controlling behavior was termed ticipating in these studies ranged in age from 11
“using conditional regard as a socializing prac- to 24 years and came from ethnic groups charac-
tice.” In this practice, educators provide more terized by widely different religions and cultural
affection or esteem when children enact behav- values in Europe, North and South America,
iors or attain outcomes that are valued by educa- Africa, and Asia. The major negative effects of
tors; similarly, children lose affection or esteem psychological control were observed in all the
when they do not comply with expectations (see ethnic groups examined.
Assor et al., 2004). Research with ninth-grade
Israeli adolescents has shown that when parents
were perceived by their children as using condi- Perspective Taking, Empathy, Openness
tional regard to promote academic achievement to Criticism, and Respect When Children
and investment, children felt a sense of internal Disagree or Display Negative Feelings
compulsion, as well as anger and resentment in
relation to parents. Importantly, research by Roth This practice refers to the ability and inclination
et al. (2009) has shown that even the seemingly to try to understand and respect the other’s perspec-
benign practice of using conditional positive tive, including perspectives that are inconsistent
regard (i.e., providing more affection and esteem with one’s own views or seem unreasonable or
when the child invests and achieves in school) is wrong. Perspective taking and respect in the case
also associated with feelings of internal compul- of disagreements can take different forms
sion and a rather rigid, grade-focused mode of depending on the content of the disagreement.
engagement in school. For example, when children do not want to
Assor, Roth, Israeli, and Freed (2007) con- engage in studying a certain topic, the teacher
ducted a follow-up study to examine whether the can first ask them why they do not invest much
Roth et al. (2009) findings concerning conditional in this topic. If the students say that they are
regard predicting adolescents’ sense of internal bored or that they feel that no matter what they
compulsion would emerge also when conditional would do, they would never succeed, the
regard was assessed via parents’ reports. Results empathic teacher acknowledges those feelings
obtained with ninth-grade Israeli adolescents and respects them, and then relies on additional
clearly replicated the pattern obtained by Roth autonomy supportive practices such as offering a
426 A. Assor

rationale and some choice to try to promote showed that their perceptions of their teachers as
autonomous internalization of the value of learn- suppressing the expression of different and some-
ing the task at hand. time critical opinions were negatively associated
The task of being empathic, taking others’ with their engagement in studying and positive
perspective, and respecting their feelings and feelings during studying. Importantly, this effect
opinions is relatively easy when others feel was also detected when the effects of other
incompetent, confused, or distressed and there- aspects of teacher behavior (as perceived by stu-
fore need someone to help them. In such cases, dents) were statistically controlled for. The
our interest and respect may only mean that we aspects of teacher behavior that were held con-
care and are not threatened by the distress or the stant were teachers’ provision of choices, clarifi-
other person. Moreover, such cases usually do cation of the relevance of the subject matter to
not pose a threat to our beliefs, values, power, or students’ goals, competence supporting feedback,
self-esteem (as people who know what is right or and a warm behavior toward the student. While
wrong). in these studies acceptance of differing opinions
In contrast, being empathic and respectful is or negative feelings was not found to have posi-
much more challenging when others do not nec- tive correlates when entered with other variables
essarily feel incompetent and/or in need of help in the regression analyses, it should be noted that
but simply hold opposite views and beliefs. Let openness to differing opinions was found to have
us consider, for example, a high school student positive and significant Pearson correlations with
who is interested in arts and literature and there- engagement.
fore wants to take a minimal load of studies in the The importance of perspective taking and
natural sciences. For parents who admire the nat- empathy when teachers or parents disagree with
ural sciences and want their child to study these children was also demonstrated in several studies
subjects because they know she/he is very intel- conducted by Assor, Roth, and their colleagues,
ligent (and/or think these subjects can secure a as in these studies, one major component of the
high income), it might be extremely difficult to autonomy support measure reflects the capacity
show respect for the child’s different opinions, to take children’s perspective when they disagree
plans, and feelings on this issue. The difficulties with adults regarding school issues (e.g., Assor
in respecting the child’s view may arise not only et al., 2007; Roth et al., 2009). The Roth et al.
from our concern about the child’s well-being (2009) study is described in some detail in the sec-
and future opportunities but also from the feeling tion focusing on intrinsic value demonstration.
that respecting opposite opinions may indicate
that our view is not so valid or even that we have
no authority or are unsure of our views. Providing Rationale
Because children and adolescents have at least
some understanding of the challenge involved in This attribute refers to teachers’ inclination and
respecting their opinions and feelings because ability to provide a coherent, age-appropriate
they have already have at least some capacity for rationale for their expectations. When students
perspective taking (e.g., Epley, Morewedge, & are provided with clear and convincing rationale
Keysar, 2004), they really value the capacity of for actions they do not find particularly interest-
significant others to respect their differing opin- ing or valuable, they tend to feel less coerced
ions and negative feelings. Consequently, empa- (Assor, 2011; Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997).
thy and respect for oppositional opinions and Moreover, when students understand and identify
feelings may provide particularly strong support with the rationale for their school-related activi-
for the child’s need for autonomy. Consistent ties, they feel that the act of studying supports
with this view, reports by Assor et al. (2002) their need for autonomy because studying allows
and Assor and Kaplan (2001) on research with them to express and promote their values and
elementary and high school students in Israel goals. For example, when a high school teacher
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 427

shows students how good writing ability has whether they were given choice on not given
allowed many past graduates to attain jobs they choice. Thus, it appears that if one is able to do
find interesting and socially valuable, this ratio- what one values as important or interesting, the
nale enables many students to believe that by provision of choice becomes insignificant. Thus,
improving their writing abilities, they would be choice appears to be important mainly when it
able to realize their authentic goals and values allows student to better realize their interests and
and therefore feel autonomous. Consistent with values, or at least avoid activities that undermine
this view, Assor et al. (2002) and Assor and their interests or values.
Kaplan (2001) have shown that the provision of a
sound rationale to students in elementary school,
middle school, or high school was a particularly Intrinsic Value Demonstration
good predictor of engagement and positive feel-
ings regarding studying. Other research, con- This attribute refers to the demonstration of val-
ducted with adolescents and young adults in ued attributes and behaviors by parents and teach-
Israel and the USA, also supports the benefits of ers. It is important that the people demonstrating
providing a rationale for actions expected from the valued behavior would not only enact it often
children (e.g., Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, & Leone, but would also fully identify with it and perhaps
1994; Koestner, Ryan, Bernieri, & Holt, 1984; would also appear to enjoy it. These feelings
Roth et al., 2009). In all these studies, it was indicate that the behavior is worth engaging in,
found that children showed more autonomous and as a result, children may feel that they really
motivation to act in accordance with adults’ want to adopt these behaviors and they do not
requests when adults provided a rationale for have to be forced to do them. IVD differs from
their requests. modeling in that in IVD, it is important not only
that the behavior is demonstrated often but also
that the modeled behavior appears intrinsically
Supporting Choice and Initiation worthy (valuable and/or enjoyable). IVD might
be even more convincing than the provision of
This attribute directly supports the striving for rationale because rather than talking about the
optional choice. Studies examining the impact of importance of the valued behavior, you demon-
choice provision on the motivation to perform a strate its value in your own life and ongoing
relatively uninteresting task or on effort invest- actions.
ment in class have shown that choice provision The identification with the demonstrated val-
often enhances autonomous motivation and posi- ued behaviors helps children to internalize values
tive feelings while working on the task at hand and goals around which their identity is formed
(e.g., Assor et al., 2002; Cordova & Lepper, 1996; and then renegotiated as they grow older (Erikson,
Deci et al., 1994; Katz & Assor, 2007; Reynolds 1950, 1968). As such, IVD by significant others
& Symons, 2001; Zuckerman, Porac, Lathin, may be necessary to support one key striving
Smith, & Deci, 1978). constituting the need for autonomy, namely, the
However, Katz and Assor (2007) noted that striving for the formation of authentic direction-
choice may be unimportant or even frustrating giving values and goals. One implication of the
when students do not have clear goals or values or important role of IVD in the formation of values
when the options available do not allow realiza- and goals is that when parents and educators do
tion of students’ goals and values. For example, not provide IVD, youth may find it very difficult
Katz and Assor described research showing that to feel that they have authentic values and goals
when students can work on a task that they are that they really identify with and experience as a
highly interested in, the choice factor makes no source of perceived autonomy and vitality. Thus,
difference. That is, students show similar (high) lack of IVD by significant others, especially in
level of autonomous motivation irrespective of the moral, religious, and character domain, may
428 A. Assor

lead to a state of identity diffusion that is charac- regard. Conditional Positive Regard (CPR) in the
terized by the feeling and the belief that nothing academic domain involves the provision of more
is really valuable and worthy to put effort into affection and esteem when the child studies and
(e.g., Assor, 2011; Marcia, Waterman, Matteson, achieves more. It was found that CPR, unlike the
Archer, & Orlofsky, 1993). And indeed, research autonomy supportive practices, predicted feel-
carried by Cohen-Malayev (2009) (see also ings of internal compulsion with regard to study-
Assor, Cohen-Malayev et al., 2005; Cohen- ing, which in turn predicted teacher ratings of
Malayev, Assor, & Kaplan, 2009) indicates that adolescents’ engagement as grade-focused rather
young adults raised in modern Orthodox Jewish than interest-focused. (see Roth et al., 2009). It
families had significant difficulties developing a should be noted that the correlation between CPR
firm and satisfying religious identity and experi- and IVD was very low and not significant.
enced many of the features characterizing iden- The importance of modeling, a construct
tity diffusion when their parents were low on somewhat similar to IVD, for engagement in
IVD of religious behavior. prosocial acts has long been demonstrated by
The first study demonstrating the value of IVD empirical research (see Eisenberg, Fabes, &
from an SDT perspective was conducted by Roth Spinrad, 2006). However, so far there is almost
and Assor (2000). They found that Israeli college no research comparing IVD and modeling in the
students’ perceptions of their parents as demon- domain of prosocial behavior.
strating the intrinsic value of prosocial actions Intrinsic value demonstration obviously does
predicted autonomous prosocial motivation, which not apply to domains which students may find
in turn predicted students’ engagement in proso- interesting and educators may value yet have no
cial behavior. All variables in this research were expertise or interest in. For example, adolescents
assessed via students’ self-reports, and therefore may develop a serious individual interest in a
the findings may, in part, reflect self-report bias. musical instrument or a certain kind of art or
A second, and more comprehensive, study of sports domain which the parents know little
the correlates of IVD was conducted by Roth about. In such cases, of course, parents cannot
et al. (2009) with ninth-grade Israeli adolescents. engage in IVD. However, if they value the devel-
IVD was assessed via items such as “My mom opment of intrinsic interests, engagement, and
enjoys studying and expanding her knowledge.” competence in their children and/or value the
In addition, the study also assessed parents’ per- domain, they can rely on other autonomy sup-
spective taking and rationale provision when the portive practices to foster the continual engage-
parent and child disagreed about how much ment and growth of their child within the relevant
studying the child should do. Results showed that domain. For example, they can minimize controls
adolescents’ perceptions of their parents as using and provide choice and, in addition, also support
the practices of IVD, perspective taking, and their child’s need for competence by helping
rationale provision in the academic domain pre- them find a teacher who presents optimal chal-
dicted adolescents’ sense of choice with regard to lenges and feedback (for applied implications of
studying, which in turn predicted teacher ratings this point, see Madjar & Assor, in press).
of adolescents as showing interest-focused Moreover, although parents may not be able to
engagement in learning. The participants in the engage in IVD in the specific domain the child is
study came from diverse socioeconomic back- interested in, they can demonstrate the value of
grounds, but there was no information available general attitudes and skills that can help their
on the socioeconomic status of each participant; children to persist and overcome difficulties in the
consequently potential effects of SES on the rela- domain they have chosen. For example, parents
tions detected were not examined. can show persistence and task-oriented coping
Another interesting finding in that research skills in their personal hobby or some volunteer
pertained to adolescents’ perception of their par- work, and their children can then adopt this orien-
ents as using the practice of conditional positive tation and skills in their specific interest domain.
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 429

While IVD may not be crucial to the develop- It appears then that the accumulating experience
ment of individual interests, it is likely to be espe- of being exposed to adults who have again and
cially important in the moral, prosocial, and again demonstrated the value of certain values
emotion regulation domains (Assor, 2011), and to and virtues, and at the same time were careful not
some extent also in the academic domain. In these to force these values on their children, is likely to
domains, the great majority of parents and teach- create a deep appreciation for the importance of
ers believe that it is important that their children these values and virtues even in a cynical and
adopt certain values and attitudes, for example, morally relativistic world. Consistent with the
being honest, responsible, and considerate; regu- above view, it is probable that students would be
lating one’s anger and aggressive reactions in much more engaged in various school-related
order to avoid inflicting extreme pain on others or activities if the intrinsic value of persistence, try-
avoid undermining one’s own future chances; and ing to understand things in depth, high-quality
being able to persist in the face of difficulties in performance, and learning were often demon-
the pursuit of important self-determined goals. In strated by their parents and teachers in their own
all these domains, parents are likely to face behavior. However, among adolescents in post-
significant difficulties in transmitting their values modern western cultures, IVD may not be sufficient
to their children if they do not demonstrate these to promote persistent engagement in learning
valued behaviors in their every day conduct. (or other parentally valued activities) and might
Importantly, if parents demonstrate behaviors have to be complemented by educators’ willing-
and use additional autonomy supportive methods ness to support youth inclination to examine
(such as minimizing controls, perspective taking, parentally valued behaviors in terms of their own
rationale, and choice), the demonstrated behav- personal judgements, goals and values. The next
iors can serve as a potential foundation for off- section focuses on the practice of support for
spring’s sense of authentic self and identity. In value examination and its potential contribution
particular, deep internalization of parents’ values to youth engagement in various tasks.
is more likely to occur if parents support adoles-
cents’ inclination to seriously examine the values
endorsed and demonstrated by them. Research Supporting Value/Goal/Interest
supporting this idea is presented in the next sec- Examination
tion which focuses on the practice of supporting
value and goal examination. This practice refers to acts that encourage youth
The role of IVD in supporting the construc- to engage in activities, experiences, and discus-
tion of direction-giving values and goals might sions that allow them to examine and reflect seri-
be especially important in postmodern western ously and critically on their goals, values, and
societies and perhaps also to other societies in a interests. The notion of support for value exami-
globalized world (e.g., Aviram & Assor, 2010). nation (SVE) is illustrated in Table 20.1 via items
In these societies, clear guidelines regarding the of a scale assessing school-based support for
worthy and unworthy do not exist anymore due value examination (Assor, 2010, 2011; Kanat-
to increasing moral relativism, and the collapse Maymon & Assor, 2011). As the concept of SVE
of traditional moral and ideological authorities is fairly new, research demonstrating the utility
and norms. As in postmodern societies it is less of this scale is only now emerging and is described
possible to rely on recognized societal authori- in the following sections.
ties and traditions, IVD by parents and teachers While SVE shares some similarities with per-
can fill an important social gap and become a spective taking, it also differs from the latter in
particularly important source of authentic direc- that in a typical perspective-taking act, the child
tion-giving values and goals, which then provide already has relatively clear feelings or desires,
a relatively solid basis for youth emerging iden- which the educator tries to understand. In con-
tity and future goals. trast, in the case of SVE, the youngster does not
430 A. Assor

Table 20.1 A scale assessing school support for value values can be prioritized, they have to be seriously
examination (SVE) examined. The exploration process allows such
1. The activities/studies in school sometimes cause me an examination.
to think about important things I would like to do in As shown in Fig. 20.2, an open and reflective
my life
examination process is assumed to advance a deep
2. The activities/studies in school help me to find out
what are the things I value in people integration process, one in which values and goals
3. School studies/activities enable me to examine my are experienced as authentic, fit each other, and
attitude to important issues in life are embedded in a rich world view. As cognitive-
4. School studies/activities do not provide me with emotional structures, these values constitute elab-
opportunities to examine important questions that orate, multilevel categories. Consequently, when
I am concerned with (Reverse coded)
youth hold a certain value, they not only endorse
5. School studies/activities cause me to think of traits
(attributes) I would like to have abstract categories such as social justice or self-
6. School studies/activities help me to think about just direction (as is typically the case in value ques-
and desirable ways of acting in complex situations tionnaires), but they also have representations of
7. School studies/activities help me to think about what specific attitudes and actions reflecting these val-
is more important and what is less important in life ues, as well cultural and historical narratives,
Students respond to the above items on a 5-point Likert symbols, and memories associated with these val-
scale
ues. The formation of such integrated schemas is
assumed to support youth sense of autonomy and
know what she/he feels or wants, and it is the role also contributes directly to the enactment of
of the educator to support an active, reflective, demanding actions (e.g., Assor, 1999). Sense of
open-ended search of what one truly values, autonomy is then assumed to enhance vitality and
wants and feels. SVE also shares some similari- positive affect, as well as engagement in demand-
ties with choice provision. However, here too, the ing actions. Empirical evidence for the processes
options may not be clear or they do not even exist depicted above is presented below.
at the present. But, as part of the exploration pro- As was already noted, the formation of inte-
cess they can be discovered or created. grated, direction-giving values and goals might
As depicted in Fig. 20.2, the examination and be especially important in postmodern western
reflection process is assumed to be part of a larger societies in which clear guidelines regarding the
integration process, in which youth construct worthy and unworthy do not exist anymore (see
direction-giving, authentic goals, values, and Aviram & Assor, 2010). Under these conditions,
interests (i.e., an inner compass). In Fig. 20.2, the reflection-based integrated values and goals may
process is depicted as a linear one. However, in be a crucial source of sense of autonomy and
reality, the process may often be spiral; for exam- therefore vitality. These integrated values and
ple, sense of autonomy may support deeper goals can then provide a solid foundation for an
reflection and examination of one’s goals and identity that can resist the pressures and pulls of
values. However, there is a general progress in passing fads and contradictory cultural beliefs.
the direction depicted in Fig. 20.2. The above model was supported by two
Following Ryan and Deci (2000), I view the research projects. The first set of studies (Assor,
integration process as a gradual attempt to resolve 2010, 2011; Kanat-Maymon & Assor, 2011)
intra- and interpersonal inconsistencies between focused on adolescents’ perceptions of the extent
important goals and values (including conflicts to which their schools or their youth movement
between parents’ and teachers’ values and new contexts support value and goal examination. The
ideas the adolescent develops). This can be second project focused on parents’ support for
accomplished by prioritizing goals and modify- value examination in the religious domain.
ing practices so they fit together and, most impor- Inspection of the model in Fig. 20.3 indicates
tantly, so they reflect one’s authentic inclinations, that the first research project focused on two
values, and goals. However, before goals and components of the comprehensive model presented
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 431

Autonomy Integration Process Experiential and action outcomes


Supportive
practices

Supporting Reflective Integrated Sense of Vitality,


value/goal value/goal revised autonomy positive affect
examination examination values & and
(SVE) goals that volition
fit each
other & are Enactment of
embedded difficult &
in a rich costly, but
world view satisfying,
(multi-level value-
categories) reflecting acts

Fig. 20.2 The process by which support for value examination (SVE) is assumed to promote the formation of inte-
grated and authentic values/goals and consequent positive outcomes

Contextual school Experiential &


supports performance outcomes

Support for Vitality,


value examination Sense of Positive
(SVE) autonomy affect
&
volition
- Academic
regarding
engagement
Choice support being in
(teacher
school and
reports),
studying
Relational support - High grades

Fig. 20.3 The model examined in research project 1: SVE as a predictor of perceived autonomy and consequent affec-
tive and behavioral outcomes in educational contexts

in Fig. 20.2. Specifically, this project focused on their schools (Assor, 2010). Adolescents came
SVE as a predictor of perceived autonomy and from four schools serving students coming mostly
consequent positive outcomes. As can be seen from middle class neighborhoods. Factor analy-
from Fig. 20.3, this research did not examine the sis indicated that students clearly distinguished
hypothesis that SVE promotes an integrative between school supports for value examination,
value examination process which then leads to choice, and relatedness. The construct validity of
the formation of authentic and elaborate value the SVE scale (presented in Table 20.1) was
and goal schemas. This aspect of the comprehen- supported by the finding that this scale was unre-
sive model is examined in the second project to lated to social desirability or neuroticism mea-
be described later. sures. Moreover, as expected, perceptions of the
The first test of the model described in school context as high on SVE were positively
Fig. 20.3 was conducted with adolescents (11th and significantly associated with reporting that
grade) who completed a self-report questionnaire studying and participating in school activities
assessing the variable of interest with regard to increased the importance of the intrinsic values
432 A. Assor

of community contribution, self-understanding, The second test of the model was conducted
and health, and at the same time reduced the with many of the adolescents who completed
importance of the extrinsic values of wealth and questionnaires on their school context. However,
power and prestige (see Kasser & Ryan, 1996 on in this part of the study, the context and the invest-
intrinsic versus extrinsic values). ment referred to youth movement activities
The degree to which participating in school (Assor, 2010; Kanat-Maymon & Assor, 2011).
activities increased or decreased the importance The term “youth movement” refers to informal
of intrinsic or extrinsic values was assessed via a education set ups like the scouts or other frame-
questionnaire which included a list of 15 items works. Specifically, the youth movement studied
reflecting the three intrinsic values noted above was the Israeli Scouts, which is an organization
and 10 items reflecting the two extrinsic values. that tries to promote youth activities that contrib-
In relation to each item, participants were asked ute to society, as well as conduct social activities
to indicate: “To what extent did the activities, and nature trips. The instructors in this setup are
studies and discussions in school strengthen or typically only several years older than the mem-
weaken your belief in the following values, goals bers of the group, and the relation between
and aspirations?” The response scale ranged from instructors and members is rather informal.
1(weakened the importance of this value for me) Results again highlighted the importance of
to 5 (strengthened the importance of this value SVE as a predictor of perceived autonomy and
for me). An example of an extrinsic value is consequent positive outcomes. Choice support
“being wealthy,” and an example of intrinsic again had no unique effect. The fact that choice
value is “work for the improvement of society.” support did not have a unique effect suggests that
Each value was assessed via five items. perhaps the choices offered to students were not
According to SDT, authentic value examina- sufficiently meaningful to students, and conse-
tion should connect people with their basic needs quently, the provision of choice did not have
and the intrinsic values associated with them. positive effects.
Therefore, the positive correlations between One limitation of the first two studies was their
SVE and enhanced intrinsic values and the nega- complete reliance on students’ self-report measures.
tive correlation between SVE and enhanced This problem was addressed by the second study
extrinsic values suggested that the process (Assor, 2010; Kanat-Maymon & Assor, 2011), in
assessed by our measure of SVE indeed pro- which students’ engagement in studying was
moted a relatively authentic, intrinsically ori- assessed by teacher ratings. In addition, students’
ented value examination. grades and student-rated positive affect were also
Results of structural equation modeling and assessed. Results were consistent with the results of
mediation analyses confirmed that perceptions of the first two studies. The third study (Assor, 2010;
the school as supporting value examination pre- Kanat-Maymon & Assor, 2011) used the same
dicted perceived autonomy regarding studying in measures employed in the third study but, in addi-
the school, which in turn predicted engagement tion, also included two additional scales: social
in studying and feelings of vitality while in desirability and students’ perception of teacher as
school. Interestingly, choice support did not have providing warmth and caring (relational support).
a unique effect on the outcomes examined (i.e., Results again replicated the findings of previous
choice provision was not significantly associated studies, showing that SVE had unique positive
with engagement and vitality when the effects of effects on teacher-rated student engagement, stu-
SVE were also considered). Importantly, the dent grades, and student-rated positive affect.
effects of SVE were detected also when the In sum, the studies described in this section
effects of choice support were statistically con- suggest that when adolescents and children feel
trolled for. Together, these findings indicate that that being in their school (or in the youth move-
SVE may be a more important determinant of ment) helps them to form personal goals and
student engagement and vitality in high school. values, this promotes a sense of autonomy and
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 433

volition regarding being in the school (or the Jewish students face a rather demanding task of
youth movement), which in turn leads to increased forming values and goals that integrate contradic-
engagement, vitality, and positive affect. tory religious and secular viewpoints in ways that
Importantly, in all these studies, perceived sup- feel coherent, authentic, and autonomous.
port for value examination (SVE) had unique To enable these youth to cope with the value inte-
positive effects on perceived autonomy, invest- gration task successfully, parents and teachers
ment, and well-being also when perceived choice may need to be particularly understanding and
and perceived teacher warmth and caring were supportive of their children’s need to examine val-
statistically controlled for. In fact, SVE consis- ues and goals gradually and thoroughly. Thus, the
tently had considerably stronger effects on per- practice of SVE might be particularly important in
ceived autonomy, investment, and well-being the case of modern Orthodox Jewish youth.
than did choice support. This is not surprising in Cohen-Malayev and her colleagues (e.g.,
view of other studies showing that choice provi- Assor, Cohen-Melayev, et al., 2005; Cohen-
sion is useful only when the choices are meaning- Malayev, 2009) conducted two quantitative stud-
ful or when choice recipients have clear interests ies and one qualitative study. The quantitative
and goals (e.g., Assor et al., 2002; Katz & Assor, study was based mainly on free-response ques-
2007). The detection of unique effects of SVE is tionnaires asking participants coming from a
of special interest because it indicates that SVE is modern Orthodox Jewish background to describe
a unique component of autonomy support and their experiences, feelings, and thoughts on reli-
consequent engagement and well-being. Thus, gion, challenges they face in this domain, and
provision of choice and relational support may ways of coping with these challenges. In addition,
not be enough to promote desired student pro- all the variables appearing in Fig. 20.3 were also
cesses and outcomes. Moreover, the fact the SVE assessed by questionnaires developed for this pur-
had unique effects beyond two other positive pose (using Likert type scales). The question-
teacher supports indicates that the positive effects naires were validated via small space analyses
of SVE cannot be ascribed to a general positive (see Guttman, 1968) and correlations supporting
perception of the teacher or the youth movement their divergent and convergent validity relative to
instructor. However, one limitation of the above other measures (see Assor, Cohen-Melayev, et al.,
studies is their reliance on correlations, which pre- 2005; Cohen-Malayev, 2009). Results of qualita-
cludes conclusions regarding causal inferences. tive analyses, as well as regression analyses and
The second research project examined the cluster analyses, generally supported the model
complete model depicted in Fig. 20.1, referring presented in Fig. 20.3.
to the process by which SVE is assumed to pro- Qualitative analysis of the free response ques-
mote the construction of integrated, direction- tionless indicated that contribution of SVE to the
giving goals and values and consequent positive development of integrated values, perceived
outcomes. This research project was conducted autonomy, and consequent positive outcomes
by Cohen-Malayev and her colleagues (Assor, was particularly apparent in relation to the issue
Cohen-Melayev et al., 2005; Cohen-Malayev, of women’s roles. This contribution and process
2009; Cohen-Malayev et al., 2009). Participants is summarized in Fig. 20.4.
were modern Orthodox Jewish students in Israel, The issue of women’s role in Jewish religion
coming from religious homes who also embraced is particularly problematic for modern Orthodox
modern technology and ways of life, and were Jewish women studying in nonreligious colleges
therefore exposed to views and materials dissem- because in Orthodox Judaism, women are not
inated by the secular mass media. Moreover, allowed to take leading religious roles. However,
these students also studied in a nonreligious and these religious norms are clearly inconsistent
fairly secular institution. Because the norms and with the values and norms of the surrounding
values of the secular contexts and of Orthodox secular college context regarding gender roles
Judaism often differ widely, modern Orthodox and egalitarianism. Consistent with our general
434 A. Assor

Autonomy Integration Process Experiential and Action Outcomes


Supportive
Practices

Parents Examination Integrated Sense of Well-being,


support the of religious autonomy: self-acceptance
examination values & values and Youth feel
of religious practices: views: authentic and Enacting
values (SVE): Youth revise autonomous difficult
Parents Youth values & regarding religious
encourage examine views religious practices in
examination of different concerning practices ways that are
different religious and women's role because the consistent with
approaches to non religious in religion, so problem of the revised
women's role approaches to they fit her women's role values
in Jewish women's role other values in religion is
religious in religion and general beginning to
practices and in general world view be resolved

Fig. 20.4 Effects of SVE on the integration of religious values concerning women’s roles and consequent outcomes in
modern orthodox Jewish youth

model, we found that when parents encourage Similar findings were obtained in relation to
young women to examine different approaches another difficult religious issue: women’s sexual
to women’s role in religion, these young women behavior and appearance. In Orthodox Judaism,
are indeed more inclined to examine this issue women’s sexual behavior is expected to be rather
through talks with different people and by study- conservative. These religious norms are clearly
ing different sources (e.g., Assor, Cohen- inconsistent with the less conservative values
Melayev, et al., 2005). This examination and and norms of the surrounding secular college
reflection process then helps women to revise context. As was the case for women’s gender
their values and views concerning women’s reli- roles, here too parents’ support for value exami-
gious role so that these values and views then fit nation promoted a value examination process
their more liberal and egalitarian world view. that yielded values and beliefs that integrated
For example, they now endorse a practice where modern secular views regarding sexual behavior
women are allowed to lead prayer or sit next to with traditional Jewish Orthodox command-
men in the synagogue. As a result, these women ments. For example, women who conducted
feel more autonomous and experience better value examination in this domain allowed them-
well-being and are also are more willing to enact selves to wear clothing items that although not
difficult religious practices that do not pertain to seductive, are considered illegitimate in most
women’s role. Importantly, when compared to orthodox religious circles.
women whose parents did not support religious While the results of our studies generally sup-
value examination, women whose parents did ported the proposed model, there was one unex-
support value examination (SVE) showed a sim- pected, yet interesting, finding in the second
ilar level of religious behavioral enactment of quantitative study, which employed more refined
religious practices, but for these women (those measures (see Cohen-Malayev, 2009). Thus,
receiving high SVE), this enactment was accom- results of separate cluster analyses yielded two
panied by a higher level of sense of autonomy types of examination: revisionist and orthodox.
and well-being. In revisionist value examination, there is a more
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 435

comprehensive exploration of religious princi- for the domain of religious education in countries
ples in relation to nonreligious ideas, as a result other than Israel, and in particular, religious edu-
of introspection and a personal spiritual quest. cation of youth that is exposed to the general
For example, in revisionist examination, a woman mass media and secular ideas and lifestyles.
can ask herself why is it that women are not Thus, it appears that religious schools that would
allowed to be Rabbis. In orthodox examination, support value examination in the religious domain
there is an examination of more practical issues, are likely to help students to develop values and
with an attempt to seek answers by consulting views that would enable them to feel more auton-
various religious sources. For example, a woman omous and more vital in relation to their religious
feels uncomfortable with various sex-related behavior. The school’s willingness to address
prohibitions or commandments and therefore these religious questions openly and seriously
tries to see if there is a way of doing things dif- may also result in an increased willingness to
ferently that is more in line with what she feels attend the school and to participate in various
and yet is also acceptable to some religious nonreligious school activities (as was the case for
authorities. the students studied by Assor and Kanat-Maymon
Interestingly, it was found that when SVE was [Assor, 2010, 2011; Kanat-Maymon & Assor,
associated with parents’ demonstration of the 2011]).
intrinsic value of religious practices (IVD), it led As we end the discussion of SVE, I would like
to a more limited value examination, whereas to present one note of caution regarding this gen-
when SVE was not accompanied by IVD, it led to erally desirable autonomy supportive practice.
the more comprehensive revisionist examination. Although the first studies on SVE suggest that it
Moreover, perhaps not surprisingly, orthodox has fairly positive correlates, it is possible that
examination was more predictive of: (a) self educators support for value examination can
acceptance, (b) positive relations with other peo- sometimes fail to enhance perceived autonomy
ple, and (c) enactment of traditional religious because the exploration is too difficult or confus-
practices (Cohen-Malayev, 2009). ing. To increase the likelihood that support for
It appears, then, that when parents provide a value examination would result in value and goal
convincing and authentic demonstration of the integration, commitment, and vitality, it appears
practices they would like to transmit (IVD), and important to support a special type of examina-
in addition also support value examination (SVE), tion, which can be termed Optimal Support for
youth may not feel a need to seriously challenge Value Examination (OSVE). Optimal support for
their parents’ values and thus engage in a more value examination can include:
limited type of value examination. This limited (a) Gradual exposure of adolescents to views
examination tries to modify parents’ values so and experiences that might lead them to
they fit better with offspring’s everyday needs examine their goals and values
and constraints. Importantly, this limited explora- (b) Help in reflecting on the new ideas and their
tion also seems to promote somewhat greater potentially unsettling personal implications
well-being in the sense of more peaceful relations Future research may attempt to identify the
with self and others. Thus, in contrast to what a attributes of optimal versus harmful ways of
romantic view of identity exploration might sug- encouraging value and goal examination in
gest, it is possible that for most youth, the more youth. However, research conducted by Assor
natural and preferred type of exploration is grad- and his colleagues (Assor, 2010, 2011; Madjar,
ual and nonradical, that is, exploration that grad- Assor, & Dotan, 2010) already identified one
ually builds on values that are already well educational attribute that appears to increase the
internalized and only need minor revisions. capacity of adolescents to engage in value exam-
While the studies focusing on religious behav- ination also when this examination is difficult
ior did not focus on student motivation and engage- and confusing. This attribute is discussed in the
ment, they appear to have important implications next section.
436 A. Assor

Table 20.2 Items illustrating the three components of FIV


1. Enhancing children’s ability to withstand confusion and take their time before they make serious decisions
“When other kids pressure me to accept their opinion, my mom let’s me feel that it is better to take the time and
calm down before I decide what to do”
2. Encouraging examination of one’s authentic values and goals when faced with a difficult decision or social
pressures
“When I have to make a tough decision, my mom encourages me to first examine what I think is the right and
desirable thing to do”
3. Encouraging consideration of alternatives and relevant information before making a decision
“As a child, when I had to choose what to do, my mom and me thought together on the consequences of each
possible choice”

Fostering Inner-Directed Valuing Figure 20.5 shows how FIV is posited to


Processes operate together with SVE in supporting reflec-
tive value and goal examination, and consequent
FIV is another autonomy supportive practice perceived autonomy and its positive outcomes.
which promotes the formation and realization of Inspection of Fig. 20.5 shows that FIV and
authentic direction-giving values, goals, and SVE are assumed to have additive influence on
interests. This construct refers to a cluster of edu- reflective value and goal examination. However,
cator’s behaviors which help students pay atten- future research may examine the possibility that
tion to their authentic values and needs more than FIV may also moderate the effects of SVE on
to social pressures. FIV is important because it is value examination. This moderating effect may
posited to enhance youth capacity to persist in the occur because although SVE may foster the ini-
often frustrating task of exploring one’s authentic tial urge to engage in value examination, FIV
goals, values, and interests, as well as strengthen may allow this examination to proceed when the
their capacity to make decisions based on their exploration process gets difficult and confusing.
authentic values and needs. As such, FIV can be Research on the correlates and potential
viewed as training in authentic decision making. benefits of FIV has just begun. The first study
More specifically, FIV is assumed to include focusing on this construct (see Assor, 2009, 2011;
three components: (a) enhancing students’ ability Madjar et al., 2010) showed that adolescents’
to withstand confusion and take their time before perceptions of their parents as high on FIV were
they make serious decisions, (b) encouraging the found to predict identity exploration and the for-
examination of one’s values and goals when mation of commitments that are experienced as
faced with a difficult decision and/or social pres- autonomous. In this study, FIV was also found to
sures, and (c) encouraging the consideration of predict adolescents’ capacity to experience anger
alternatives and relevant information before mak- and anxiety without losing control or immedi-
ing a decision. FIV differs from general support ately suppressing these feelings, as well as their
for value examination in that it is a socializing tendency to try to understand the sources of these
practice that is used only when the child faces feelings and the implications of these feelings for
difficult decisions and social pressures, and one’s life and relationships.
unlike SVE, it provides a certain “training” in So far, research did not examine the implica-
authentic and rational decision making under tions of FIV for engagement in studying and
stress. school activities. However, it is reasonable to
Items assessing parents, fostering of assume that the increased capacity for inner valu-
inner-directed valuing processes (FIV) are ing and for resisting social pressure would enable
presented in Table 20.2. students to engage in studies that are less popular
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 437

Autonomy Integration Process Experiential and action outcomes


Supportive
Practices

Supporting Reflective Integrated Sense of Vitality,


value/goal value/goal revised autonomy positive affect
examination examination values & and
(SVE) goals that volition
fit each
other & are Enactment of
embedded difficult &
Fostering in a rich costly, but
inner world view satisfying
valuing (multi-level value-
(FIV) categories) reflecting acts

Fig. 20.5 The joint contribution of FIV and SVE to integrated values/goals and consequent positive outcomes

but are interesting for them. In addition, increased In future research, it may be interesting to
capacity to tolerate ambiguity during the explora- examine the unique contributions to students’
tion of one’s authentic interests and values may school engagement and well-being of the three
enable students to engage in a more thorough autonomy supportive practices that were rela-
examination of various subjects relevant to their tively underemphasized in SDT-based research:
emerging interests, goals, and values. intrinsic value demonstration (IVD), support for
value examination (SVE), and fostering inner
valuing (FIV). In this research, it would be impor-
Conclusion tant to assess autonomy support practices and
outcomes based on measures that are not based
Summing up, in this chapter I focused on seven solely on self-reports, using longitudinal designs
practices of autonomy support which are likely to that can point to possible causal effects.
promote two major components of the need for
autonomy: (a) lack of coercion and optional
choice and (b) formation and realization of an References
inner compass: authentic, direction-giving val-
ues, goals, and interests. A special emphasis was Assor, A. (1999). Value accessibility and teachers’ ability
to encourage independent and critical thought in stu-
put on three autonomy supportive practices which
dents. Social Psychology of Education, 2, 1–24.
are assumed to support formation and realization Assor, A. (2010). Two under-emphasized components of
of authentic, direction-giving values, goals, and autonomy support: Supporting value examination
interests, whose impact on perceived autonomy and inner valuing. Paper presented in the 4th interna-
tional conference on self determination theory, Gent,
was not sufficiently examined so far: (a) IVD –
Belgium.
intrinsic value demonstration, (b) SVE – support Assor, A. (2011). Autonomous moral motivation:
for value/goal/interest examination, and (c) FIV – Consequences, socializing antecedents and the unique
fostering inner-directed valuing processes. The role of integrated moral principles. In M. Mikulincer
& P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Social psychology of morality:
autonomy supportive practices that foster the
Exploring the causes of good and evil. Washington,
development of stable authentic values and goals DC: American Psychological Association.
might be especially important in western coun- Assor, A., Cohen-Melayev, M., Kaplan, A., & Friedman,
tries, in which postmodern moral relativism and D. (2005). Choosing to stay religious in a modern
world: Socialization and exploration processes leading
the abundance of information and options make
to an integrated internalization of religion among
it particularly difficult for youth to form stable Israeli Jewish youth. Advances in Motivation and
and authentic values and goals. Achievement, 14, 105–150.
438 A. Assor

Assor, A., & Kaplan, H. (2001). Mapping the domain of contextualization, personalization and choice. Journal
autonomy support: Five important ways to enhance of Educational Psychology, 88, 715–730.
or undermine students’ experience of autonomy in Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R.
learning. In A. Efklides, R. Sorrentino, & J. Kuhl (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-determi-
(Eds.), Trends and prospects in motivation research nation theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62,
(pp. 99–118). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. 119–142.
Assor, A., Kaplan, H., Kanat-Maymon, Y., & Roth, G. Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-
(2005). Directly controlling teacher behaviors as pre- analytic review of experiments examining the effects
dictors of poor motivation and engagement in girls and of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation.
boys: The role of anger and anxiety. Learning and Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.
Instruction, 15, 397–413. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation
Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good, and self-determination in human behavior. New York:
but relevance is excellent: Autonomy-enhancing and Plenum Press.
suppressing teaching behaviors predicting students’ Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Pro-
engagement in schoolwork. British Journal of social development. In R. M. Lerner, N. Eisenberg, &
Educational Psychology, 27, 261–278. W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol.
Assor, A., & Roth, G. (2005). Conditional love as a social- 3. Social, emotional and personality development
izing approach: Costs and alternatives. Scientific (pp. 646–718). New York: Wiley.
Annals of the Psychological Society of Northern Elliot, A. J., & Dweck, C. S. (2005). Handbook of compe-
Greece, 7, 17–34. tence and motivation. New York: Guilford Press.
Assor, A., Roth, G., & Deci, E. L. (2004). The emotional Epley, A., Morewedge, C. K., & Keysar, B. (2004).
costs of parents’ conditional regard: A self-determina- Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent
tion theory analysis. Journal of Personality, 72, 47–88. egocentrism but differential correction. Journal of
Assor, A., Roth, G., Israeli, M., & Freed, A. (2007). The Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 760–768.
harmful costs of parental conditional regard. Paper Erikson, E. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: W.
presented in the Society for Research in Child W. Norton.
Development, Boston. Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York:
Assor, A., Vansteenkiste, M., & Kaplan, A. (2009). W. W. Norton.
Identified versus introjected-approach and introjected- Fromm, E. (1941). Escape from freedom. New York:
avoidance motivations in school and in sports: The Rinehart.
limited benefits of self-worth strivings. Journal of Gottfried, A. E., Marcoulides, G. A., Gottfried, A. W.,
Educational Psychology, 2, 482–497. Oliver, P. H., & Guerin, D. W. (2007). Multivariate
Aviram, R., & Assor, A. (2010). In defense of personal latent change modeling of developmental decline in
autonomy as a fundamental educational aim in liberal academic intrinsic math motivation and achievement:
democracies. Oxford Review of Education, 36, 111–126. Childhood through adolescence. International Journal
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: of Behavioral Development, 31, 317–327.
Revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997).
67, 3296–3319. Internalization within the family: The self-determina-
Barber, B. K., Stolz, H., & Olsen, J. (2005). Parental sup- tion theory perspective. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski
port, psychological control, and behavioral control: (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internalization of val-
Assessing relevance across time, culture, and method. ues (pp. 135–161). New York: Wiley.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Guttman, L. (1968). A general nonmetric technique for
Development, 70, 1–151. finding the smallest coordinate space for a configura-
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to tion of points. Psychometrika, 33, 469–506.
belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fun- Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is
damental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, demotivating: Can one desire too much of a good
117, 497–529. thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Berlin, I. (1969). Four essays on liberty. Oxford, UK: 79, 995–1006.
Oxford University Press. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the
Cohen-Malayev, M. (2009). Religious exploration and iden- American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic
tity – The tension between religion and religiosity: and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology
Examination of the internalization process of Jewish reli- Bulletin, 22, 80–87.
gious values in modern-orthodox context. Doctoral dis- Kanat-Maymon, M., & Assor, A. (2010). Perceived
sertation, Ben Gurion University, Be’er-Sheva, Israel. Maternal Control and Responsiveness to Distress as
Cohen-Malayev, M., Assor, A., & Kaplan, A. (2009). Predictors of Young Adults’ Empathic Responses.
Religious exploration in a modern world: The case of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 33–46.
Modern-Orthodox Jews in Israel. Identity: An International Kanat-Maymon, Y. & Assor, A. (2011). Supporting Value
Journal of Theory and Research, 9, 233–251. Exploration: Another Important Aspect of Autonomy
Cordova, D. I., & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motiva- Support. Ben GUrion University, Israel: Unpublished
tion and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of manuscript.
20 Allowing Choice and Nurturing an Inner Compass… 439

Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2007). When choice motivates and Reynolds, P. L., & Symons, S. (2001). Motivational vari-
when it does not. Educational Psychology Review, 19, ables and children’s text search. Journal of Educational
429–442. Psychology, 93, 14–22.
Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984). Roth, G., & Assor, A. (2000). The effect of conditional
Setting limits on children’s behavior: The differential parental regard and intrinsic value demonstration
effects of controlling versus informational styles on on academic and pro-social motivation. Paper pre-
children’s intrinsic motivation and creativity. Journal sented at the conference of the European Association
of Personality, 54, 233–248. for Learning and Instruction (EARLI), Malmoe,
Madjar, N., & Assor, A. (in press). Two types of perceived Sweden.
control over learning: Perceived efficacy and per- Roth, G., Assor, A., Niemiec, P. C., Ryan, R. M., & Deci,
ceived autonomy. In J. A. C. Hattie & E. M. Anderman E. L. (2009). The negative consequences of parental
(Eds.), International handbook of student achieve- conditional regard: A comparison of positive condi-
ment. New York: Routledge. tional regard, negative conditional regard, and auton-
Madjar, N., Assor, A., & Dotan, L. (2010). Fostering inner omy support as parenting strategies. Developmental
valuing processes in adolescents. Paper presented in Psychology, 4, 1119–1142.
the convention of the American Educational Research Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination
Association, Denver, CO. theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
Marcia, J., Waterman, A., Matteson, D., Archer, S., & social development, and well-being. American
Orlofsky, J. (1993). Ego identity. New York: Springer. Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The Soenens, B., Elliot, A. J., Goossens, L., Vansteenkiste,
effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related M., Luyten, P., & Duriez, B. (2005). The intergenera-
outcomes: A meta-analysis of research findings. tional transmission of perfectionism: Parents’ psycho-
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 270–300. logical control as an intervening variable. Journal of
Reeve, J. (2006). Teachers as facilitators: What autonomy- Family Psychology, 19, 358–366.
supportive teachers do and why their students benefit. Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical
The Elementary School Journal, 106, 225–236. upgrade of the concept of parental psychological
Reeve, J., & Assor, A. (2011). Do social institutions neces- control: Proposing new insights on the basis of self-
sarily suppress individuals’ need for autonomy? The determination theory. Developmental Review, 30,
possibility of schools as autonomy promoting contexts 74–99.
across the globe (pp. 111–132). In V. Chirkov, A. M. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The con-
Ryan, & K. Sheldon (Eds.), Personal autonomy in cul- cept of competence. Psychological Review, 66,
tural context: Global perspectives on the psychology of 297–333.
freedom and people’s well-being. New York: Springer. Zuckerman, M., Porac, J., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci,
Reeve, J., & Jang, H. (2006). What teachers say and do to E. L. (1978). On the importance of self-determination
support students’ autonomy during a learning activity. for intrinsically motivated behavior. Personality and
Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 209–218. Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 443–446.
The Engaging Nature of Teaching
for Competency Development 21
Rosemary Hipkins

Abstract
Teachers’ curricular intentions and the manner they construct learning
opportunities in the classroom have an impact on engagement. This chap-
ter is set in the context of a curriculum intention to develop senior high
school students’ competencies/capabilities, which has implications for the
manner in which teachers ‘talk up’ reasons for engaging with learning.
Differences in perceptions of the learning affordances their teachers offer
are described for the students’ most and least enjoyed subjects, with enjoy-
ment standing as a proxy for emotional engagement. The responses of the
teachers of each student’s two classes add to the rich contextual picture of
more and less engaging classroom learning contexts and point to the
importance of creating spaces for metacognitive conversations about
learning, and of supporting students to more actively link current learning
to their personal lives. This is practically useful knowledge because many
of the dimensions of engagement discussed can arguably be influenced by
teachers’ actions and beliefs.

(Russell, Ainley, & Frydenberg, 2005). Following


Introduction one much-cited literature review (Fredricks,
Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004), the scope of engage-
This chapter explores students’ engagement with
ment is taken to encompass behavioural, cogni-
learning in their final years of schooling.
tive and emotional dimensions of participation in
Engagement is framed as active participation in
learning. Motivation, by contrast, is taken to be
learning with competency development in mind.
about ‘energy and direction, the reasons for
The link to competencies is intended to capture
behaviour, why we do what we do’ (Russell et al.,
the sense of engagement as ‘energy in action’
2005, p.3, emphasis in the original).
The nature of engaging classrooms is explored
through the complementary lenses of ‘opportuni-
ties to learn’ (as orchestrated by the teacher) and
R. Hipkins, Ph.D. (*)
New Zealand Council for Educational Research,
‘affordances’ that support and enable learning (as
Wellington, New Zealand perceived by the student). Whether tacitly
e-mail: Rose.hipkins@nzcer.org.nz assumed or explicitly identified, teachers create

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 441


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_21, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
442 R. Hipkins

opportunities for students to engage in class and perhaps pushing back against the strictures
through the purposes they envision for learning. of school. However, contexts for schooling are
These purposes in turn influence their selection changing in ways that unsettle tidy relationships
of curriculum content, their choice of learning where examination prescriptions become de facto
resources, the instructional processes they deploy, curriculum, teaching is directed towards content
and how they ‘talk up’ and generally prepare stu- acquisition, and traditional exit examinations
dents for any subsequent assessment events. assess the extent to which the prescribed content
Assuming all these choices are coherent and has been acquired and understood (Bolstad &
broadly appropriate to the learning needs of the Gilbert, 2008).
students, the opportunities to learn that the teacher Outside of education, rapidly changing social
shapes are necessary for learning, but a sociocul- and economic conditions are creating new uncer-
tural framing posits that they are not sufficient to tainties. It is no longer possible to assume a
ensure the engagement of all or even necessarily ‘known future, a known set of options to choose
most of the students in the class (Haertel, Moss, between, each requiring a known set of skills
Pullin, & Gee, 2008). and aptitudes, and therefore a known – and well-
Opportunities to learn, as envisaged and trodden – pathway’ (Bolstad & Gilbert, 2008,
enacted by the teacher, may or may not be recog- p.35). With global changes and uncertainties in
nised by the students as offering affordances for mind, this chapter argues that new ways of think-
their personal learning. Gee (2008) described ing about keeping students engaged in the final
affordances as ‘action possibilities posed by years of schooling are now needed. It draws on
objects or features in the environment’ (p.81). To data from the longitudinal study Competent
name just a few, affordances could include stu- Children/Competent Learners (Wylie & Hodgen,
dents’ understanding of what the learning is really 2012) to describe student and teacher views of
about and for; their estimations of their likely classroom learning conditions at age 16 and to
success in completing the tasks in relation to their discuss implications for changes in pedagogy.
motivation to do so; their personal interest in and The survey items discussed in the chapter were
connections to the contexts of learning, including designed with New Zealand’s recent curriculum
prior knowledge and experiences on which they and assessment reforms in mind, specifically a
might draw; and any possibilities for social and focus on learning as competency development.
intellectual interaction as students learn together. New Zealand’s national curriculum and school
Thus opportunities to learn are realised only exit qualification system are briefly outlined next,
when individual students see ways to transform to provide the context for the data and discussion
the intended learning into action and are willing that follows.
to invest the necessary effort to do so.
Within the scope just outlined, discussions of
engagement include considerations of the broader Curriculum and Assessment Reform
purposes that frame learning at any specific stage in New Zealand
of schooling. This chapter is set in the context of
the final years of high school, when students are In common with many other nations, New
preparing for and being assessed to gain exit Zealand is wrestling with questions of what it
qualifications, all the while making choices that means to educate students for the rapidly chang-
take into account their likely options for work or ing economic, environmental and social condi-
further study in the immediate post-school years. tions that characterise life in the twenty-first
Traditionally, teachers have used the necessity to century (Bolstad & Gilbert, 2008; Gilbert, 2005).
prepare for high-stakes examinations as a means The most recent New Zealand Curriculum (NZC)
of keeping the majority of students engaged at a is a future-focused framework curriculum whose
stage of their learning when adolescents can purpose is to provide a sense of national direction
become restless, ready as they see it for adult life for local decision-making. Each school has to
21 The Engaging Nature of Teaching for Competency Development 443

Table 21.1 The origins of five NZC key competencies range of competencies, but those labelled as ‘key’
New Zealand Curriculum are seen to be universal rather than situation
version (note that these specific (Rychen & Salganik, 2003). The impli-
Name given to competency are ‘best matches’ not 1–1 cation is that these competencies are transferrable
by OECD equivalents)
across contexts and continue to develop across
Acting autonomously Managing self
the life span.
Functioning in socially Relating to others
heterogeneous groups Participating and
Key competencies integrate knowledge and
contributing skills with attitudes and values, and are demon-
Using tools interactively Using language, symbols strated as complex responses to any challenges
and texts learners confront as they adapt what they already
Thinking (as a ‘cross- Thinking (not identified as know and can do to new contexts, or to more
cutting’ competency that cross-cutting)
interacts with all the others)
demanding aspects of familiar contexts (Rychen
& Salganik, 2003). In this way, a focus on com-
petency development draws attention to disposi-
work out how best to build up a detailed local tional aspects of learning and to ideas such as
curriculum based on the national framework, action competence: knowing how best to respond,
with the identified learning needs of its own stu- having the necessary knowledge and skills to do
dent community demonstrably addressed. A so and being disposed to use these. These dispo-
vision statement and a set of principles guide the sitional aspects of learning have been character-
reading and interpretation of the whole. The ised as being ‘ready, willing and able’ to undertake
vision is for students to become ‘confident, con- the learning task and confront its challenges
nected, actively involved lifelong learners’ (Carr, 2006, 2008). Engagement here is not
(Ministry of Education [MoE], 2007, p.8), and optional but rather a necessary condition of learn-
the principles highlight the following as key ing. It is ‘energy in action, the connection between
design considerations: coherence, inclusion, cul- person and activity’ (Russell et al., 2005, p.4,
tural diversity, high expectations, a future-focus, emphasis in the original).
learning-to-learn and community engagement If students are to strengthen their personal
with local curriculum design and enactment, competencies as demonstrable outcomes of learn-
together with a focus on the Treaty of Waitangi as ing, schools must weave competencies together
the foundation for bicultural relationships with with traditional content. The latter is specified in
the indigenous people of New Zealand. NZC as sets of achievement objectives for eight
The vision and principles are given life when learning areas, each differentiated into eight cur-
schools design learning programmes that weave riculum levels that broadly indicate progress
more traditional content with specified values across all the years of school from age 5 to around
and key competencies. Eight broad sets of val- age 17 or 18. (Students can leave after they turn
ues, identified and shaped via a national consulta- 16, but this is discouraged because by then they
tion exercise, are expected to be encouraged, would be unlikely to have any qualifications that
modelled and explored. As outlined in Table 21.1, would keep them on a learning pathway). Each
five NZC key competencies were adapted from a learning area is framed by a one-page ‘essence
set of four developed by the OECD’s DeSeCo statement’ that sets out the unique contribution
project. This project defined ‘key’ competencies that this learning area makes to the enacted cur-
as those learners need to develop during their riculum. Schools are expected to discuss these
schooling in order to maximise their chances of ‘high level’ ideals as they plan how to give
living meaningfully in, and contributing to, well- expression to curriculum as a complex whole
functioning societies, both during and well (Hipkins, 2011).
beyond their school years (OECD, 2005). Some This local curriculum planning will ideally
people use the word ‘capabilities’ with similar result in the provision of learning experiences that
intent (Reid, 2006). Learners draw on a wide support all students to develop and strengthen
444 R. Hipkins

their current competencies and to explore and and challenges for high schools as they create
model the curriculum values, all in the context of coherent pathways through and beyond the senior
also learning the concepts and skills specified in high years. Ideally, all assessment should be
the achievement objectives. Planning appropriate competency-focused, but in practice, revising the
curriculum is thus a highly complex design task. existing suites of achievement standards to reflect
Even with the vision and principles to provide discipline-specific opportunities for competency
guidance, there could be very many different development is proving to be demanding, with
ways to assemble these pieces. There are also considerable implications for teacher profes-
strong implications for pedagogy: the ‘how’ of sional learning and pedagogical change.
teaching is as important as the ‘what’ and both
come together in the ‘why’, i.e. the purposes for
learning that are envisaged, or perhaps simply Changing Pedagogy for Changing
assumed, by both students and their teachers Times?
(Hipkins, Bull, & Reid, 2010). Framing the
engagement issue thus directs the inquiry focus An Effective Pedagogy section included in the
beyond the individual student as engaged in learn- NZC framework provides advice about creating a
ing or not (although that remains important) to supportive learning environment, encouraging
take account of teacher-student interactions, reflective thought and action, enhancing the rel-
teachers’ curriculum decision-making and the evance of new learning, facilitating shared learn-
classroom learning conditions they co-construct ing, making connections to prior learning and
with their students – in other words, the manner in experience, providing students with sufficient
which affordances for learning play out in action. opportunities to learn and inquiring into one’s
New Zealand does not have a programme of own teaching practice to ensure student learning
national testing, so effectively carrying out the needs are being met (MoE, 2007, p. 34). All of
processes specified in NZC for local design and these aspects of pedagogy could be seen as fun-
review is an important professional responsibility damental to improving traditional teaching prac-
for every school. Even the school exit qualifica- tice. None necessarily implies pedagogical
tion, awarded at three levels broadly correspond- change or curriculum transformation for new
ing to the final 3 years of high school, the National times. However, the demands of competency
Certificate in Educational Achievement (NCEA), development do potentially bring new pedagogi-
has a flexible, modular structure that continues cal imperatives. NZC defines the key competen-
opportunities for local curriculum design right cies drawing on ‘knowledge, attitudes and values
through to the end of schooling (Bolstad & in ways that lead to action’ (MoE, 2007, p.12,
Gilbert, 2008; Hipkins & Vaughan, with Beals, emphasis added) and the dispositional challenges
Ferral, & Gardiner, 2005). Standards-based assess- of competency development have already been
ment is underpinned by suites of ‘achievement noted. One engagement challenge here is that
standards’ that can be mixed and matched, at least action contexts that are new and challenging for
in theory. Some standards are internally assessed one learner might not offer any learning ‘stretch’
by each school, and these typically specify types to another. This implies that some degree of per-
of learning that cannot be assessed in traditional sonalisation is needed if key competencies are to
examinations. Externally assessed standards do be fostered via participatory learning.
often entail examinations, but even here, some The NZC notes the development of key com-
innovation is possible; for example, portfolio petencies is ‘both an end in itself (a goal) and a
assessments are often used in the arts and tech- means by which other ends are achieved’ (MoE,
nology learning areas. NCEA is part of a National 2007, p.12). Key competencies ‘enable learn-
Qualifications Framework (NQF) that extends ing’ (ibid, p.38) with the clear implication that
to post-school learning pathways. Thus, there there is a strong link between the development
are additional curriculum design opportunities of key competencies and learning-to-learn.
21 The Engaging Nature of Teaching for Competency Development 445

Russell et al.’s meta-analysis of engagement and every subject area, and there is an element of
motivation begins with summary of learning out- unpredictability in their outcomes. Teachers need
comes for the twenty-first century. Interestingly to be sufficiently confident to be responsive to
they make essentially the same learning-to-learn students’ ideas and reactions, and to follow new
connection: ‘Engagement in learning is both an learning possibilities as these unfold.
end in itself and a means to an end’ (Russell et al., This chapter is not intended to argue for com-
2005, p.3). They also link engagement to more petency development per se. Rather, it uses the
dynamic learning processes and better quality idea of key competencies as a lens for re-examin-
educational outcomes as foundations for con- ing curriculum assumptions and pedagogical
tinuing to learn in the years beyond school. practices, and ensuring that any initiatives
Developing learning-to-learn dimensions chal- intended to strengthen student engagement take
lenges teachers to offer opportunities that draw the whole learning context into account, includ-
students into metacognitive conversations that ing adopting a more nuanced view of opportuni-
support them and reflect on acts of meaning- ties to learn and how these are impacted by the
making, including how and why they are learn- classroom environment and teacher’s actions.
ing, not just what they have acquired (Hipkins, Many teachers are unfamiliar with sociocultural
2006). For such conversations to be rich and theories of learning and so are likely to miss the
meaningful, the learning that is planned must be subtle language cues in NZC. If they think about
intellectually engaging for both students and the learning as being mainly the individual acquisi-
teacher, and the teacher must be clear about the tion of knowledge and skills, they are likely to
nature of the ‘big picture’ to which the learning is miss the part played by the affordances of learn-
making a contribution. ing environment they are responsible for orches-
NZC further notes that social contexts are trating for their students. If they are unaware of
important enablers of progress in developing key constructivist theories of learning, the very pos-
competencies; the manner in which competencies sibility that different students will perceive dif-
develop over time is shaped by students’ ‘interac- ferent purposes for the new learning offered, and
tions with people, places, ideas and things’ (MoE, hence create different links to what they already
2007, p.12). The sociocultural idea of affordances know and can do, might pass the teacher by. The
is cued by these words, as is the related idea that research presented in this chapter did not engage
learning is mediated by whether and how students explicitly with teachers’ reasoning, but rather
understand and take up these affordances with the manner in which their (likely tacit) cur-
(Wertsch, 1998). Thus a sociocultural framing for riculum and pedagogical beliefs were translated
learning that fosters competency development into the opportunities to learn that they offered in
positions learning as social, contextually bound their classes.
and emergent (Davis & Sumara, 2010).
Competencies come into view during learning
interactions that vary according to the demands Determining Engagement as a Situated
of the specific subject, the affordances that the and Mediated Construct
planned learning offers individual students, and
the various new contextual links that become This section of the chapter introduces the engage-
apparent. This description stands in contrast to a ment data drawn from the longitudinal Competent
more universalist view of learning where compe- Children, Competent Learners project. This proj-
tency might be seen as a relatively stable charac- ect has tracked around 500 New Zealand students
teristic, separately owned by discrete individuals from pre-school education through their school
(Delandshere & Petrosky, 1998). A sociocultural years and on into the world of work or further
interpretation implies that key competencies can- education. Well before the OECD key competen-
not be taught generically: they have to be explored cies were developed, the prescient decision was
from a disciplinary perspective by teachers in taken to focus on competency development as
446 R. Hipkins

children moved through school (see Wylie & classes nominated by a student were contacted
Hodgen, 2012 for a more detailed project descrip- and invited to complete a survey that included
tion). At age 16, when the students were in a wide questions about both the class and the student. We
range of high schools, they were invited to respond see two different classes through the eyes of the
to a set of items that described aspects of the same student, but also as perceived by the teacher
learning they experienced during classes in the of each of those classes. One part of the survey
subjects they most and least enjoyed, as well as in addressed opportunities to learn through the
English which they would all have been studying. teacher’s eyes. This part comprised 32 items that
This chapter focuses on data about most and least described the general learning conditions in the
enjoyed subjects. Thus, self-reported enjoyment class. A 5-point Likert scale (strongly agree to
of learning in a class is the situated measure used strongly disagree with neutral in the middle) was
in this chapter to determine comparative engage- provided for the teacher to indicate how well the
ment of an individual student in two different set- item description accorded with the class in ques-
tings, each with a different teacher. tion. Other parts of the survey asked the teacher
The construct of ‘most enjoyed’ subjects to respond to questions about the named student
directs attention to emotional components of as a learner. One bank of 36 Likert-scaled items
engagement (Fredricks et al., 2004). It could be asked the teacher to estimate how often the stu-
argued that focusing on enjoyment is not a good dent did what the item described (never, occa-
proxy for engagement because students may well sionally, sometimes, often, or always) while
enjoy subjects that make few demands on them learning in the nominated class. Another bank of
cognitively or even behaviourally – they can have 13 items described aspects that imply motiva-
a good time and not do much work. However, the tional underpinnings for engagement (e.g. ‘always
student responses outlined shortly do not bear out strives for excellence’) and asked the teacher to
this sceptical view. Also, there is evidence in the judge how well that item applied to the student on
Competent Learners project that enjoyment was a 5-point Likert scale (strongly agree to strongly
linked to experiencing academic success (Wylie, disagree with neutral in the middle). A full discus-
Hipkins, & Hodgen, 2009; Wylie & Hodgen, sion of all these rich data can be accessed in the
2012), which implies that both cognitive and project report (Wylie et al., 2009).
behavioural dimensions of engagement are also The selection of teacher items for inclusion
present when students indicate positive affective in this chapter was informed by a consideration
responses to their learning. A second possible of their potential to illuminate aspects of com-
objection to the use of enjoyment as a proxy for petency development, and by being able to match
engagement runs the opposite way. Students may them to student items that broadly encompassed
be cognitively and behaviourally engaged in sub- the same idea. Students completed questions
jects they do not enjoy, especially if they are moti- about learning conditions in their most and least
vated by strongly held instrumental reasons for enjoyed classes. For each class, they responded
choosing these. Indeed, in other research, we have to a bank of 58 items (X is a class where…) using
found instances of students taking physics ‘under a 5-point Likert scale (strongly agree to strongly
sufferance’ because they need it for pre-entry disagree with neutral in the middle). Some of the
courses leading to limited-entry study pathways items concerned the affordances they perceived
into highly valued professions such as medicine in that class setting. Table 21.2 matches these to
(Hipkins, Roberts, Bolstad, & Ferral, 2006). teacher items related to opportunities to learn.
Many studies that include a classroom compo- Other student items concerned their personal
nent compare different context/cohort combina- behaviour in the class. Table 21.3 matches these
tions and hence conflate two sets of variables responses to corresponding teacher perceptions
(different settings/different students). The Compe- about the student as a learner in that class.
tent Learner study provided an illuminating lens Note that the distinction teachers were asked
on classroom contexts when teachers of the two to draw between the student as an individual and
21 The Engaging Nature of Teaching for Competency Development 447

Table 21.2 Comparing teachers’ perceptions of opportunities to learn with students’ perceptions of the affordances
offered in these classes
Item Most enjoyed class = 418 students and teacher of each student % agree or strongly agree
set no Least enjoyed class = 417 students and teacher of each student Most enjoyed Least enjoyed Difference
1 Student view: The teacher uses examples that are relevant to my 77 27 50
experience
Student view: My teacher knows what interests us 72 20 52
Teacher view: I relate the context to students’ experiences 77 66 11
2 Student view: We have a lot of hands-on practical activities 73 24 49
Teacher view: Students do a lot of practical activities 72 38 34
3 Student view: The teacher gives me useful feedback on my work 86 40 46
that helps me see what I need to do next and how to do it
Teacher view: Feedback I give students shows them their next steps 84 75 9
4 Student view: I can try out new ideas/ways of doing things 81 35 46
Student view: We discuss different ways of looking at things/ 65 27 38
interpretations
Student view: I get to think about ideas and problems in new ways 67 30 37
Teacher view: Students are given time to reflect on their learning 65 57 8
5 Student view: I get time to think and talk about how I’m learning 62 17 45
Teacher view: I encourage students to think and talk about how 57 52 5
they are learning (the methods they are using)
6 Student view: Students help and support each other 78 44 34
Teacher view: Students can work out problems together 74 78 −4
7 Student view: I can make mistakes and learn from them without 84 50 34
getting into trouble
Teacher view: Students can make mistakes and learn from them 92 92 –
without getting into trouble
8 Student view: We do projects about real things/issues 54 25 29
Teacher view: Students have the opportunity to act on issues that 50 33 17
concern them
9 Student view: We assess each other’s work and give feedback 47 20 27
Teacher view: Students are encouraged to assess each others’ 39 30 9
work and give feedback
10 Student view: We learn things outside the classroom, e.g. on 41 14 27
fieldtrips
Teacher view: Students interact with people outside school 43 23 20
as part of their school work
11 Student view: I work with other students on group tasks 71 52 19
Teacher view: Students do a lot of group activities and discussions 54 37 14
12 Student view: We can choose the topics we want to do 28 10 18
Teacher view: Students are encouraged to lead group projects/ 37 25 12
class activities
Teacher view: Students are given input into the context 64 52 12
and direction of learning activities

the class as a whole does not apply to student least enjoyed subjects will have been completed
responses – they were who they were in that set- by two different teachers. A further caution con-
ting, and hence all their items comprised one large cerns the likelihood that the items were not inter-
bank. Note also that each student completed the preted in comparable ways by the student and
same item set for both their most and least enjoyed the teacher. Notwithstanding these cautions, the
subjects. Unless they had the same teacher for following data patterns paint a compelling picture
both these subjects (which in view of the differ- of opportunities to strengthen competencies that
ences about to be reported seems fairly unlikely), can make a positive contribution to student
the corresponding teacher items for most and engagement.
448 R. Hipkins

Table 21.3 Comparing teachers and student perceptions of the student as a learner in the class
Students:% agree or strongly agree
Item Most enjoyed class = 418 students and teacher of each student, Teachers:% happens often or always
set no Least enjoyed class = 417 students and teacher of each student Most enjoyed Least enjoyed Difference
1 Student item: I learn things that are challenging 86 22 64
Teacher item: Where there is a choice, chooses work that allows 44 27 17
him/her to gain further knowledge and skills
2 Student item: My teacher is interested in my ideas 85 27 58
Teacher item: Clearly explains things so you get a very good 60 37 23
idea of what is happening and what s/he is thinking
3 Student item: I get totally absorbed in my work 64 13 51
Teacher item: Has a good concentration span when working 59 40 19
4 Student item: I organise my time so I get things done 64 24 40
Teacher item: Finishes all class work 70 45 35
Teacher item: Is organised and well prepared for assessments 61 43 18
Teacher item: Finishes all homework 58 38 20
5 Student item: When I finish my work, I check and make changes 68 29 39
if needed before handing it in
Teacher item: Assess his/her work and makes improvements 47 31 16
before completing or handing it in
6 Student item: I expect to get lots of NCEA credits 71 32 39
Teacher item: S/he is realistic about likely achievement in 68 60 8
assessment tasks
7 Student item: We discuss different ways of looking at things/ 65 27 38
interpretations
Teacher item: Aware that there are different ways of interpreting 42 30 12
knowledge
8 Student item: I meet any goals I set myself 64 27 37
Teacher item: Meets any goals s/he sets her/himself 57 39 18
9 Student item: I can make mistakes and learn from them without 84 50 34
getting into trouble
Teacher item: Learns from mistakes/experience 65 47 18
10 Student item: When I’m doing something I think about whether 74 47 27
I understand what I’m doing
Teacher item: Asks questions so s/he understands 63 41 22
11 Student item: Students can safely express different views from 79 53 26
each other
Teacher item: Respects other points of view or different ways 71 60 11
of doing things
12 Student item: I work with other students on group tasks 71 52 19
Teacher item: Takes full part in a group that is working to 58 36 22
complete a learning task together
13 Student item: I can choose which assessments I want to do for 17 14 3
NCEA
Teacher item: S/he makes strategic decisions not to do assessments 5 8 3
Teacher item: S/he makes impulsive decisions not to do 8 12 4
assessments

Engaging Students in Whole-Class their most and least enjoyed classes. Each set of
Settings items shows overall frequencies for the affor-
dances of the class as the students perceived these,
Table 21.2 documents 12 matched sets of items, matched to overall frequencies for opportunities
ranked by the size of the difference between stu- to learn as perceived by the teacher of each stu-
dents’ perceptions of the learning conditions in dent in those same classes. To illustrate, item set
21 The Engaging Nature of Teaching for Competency Development 449

1 shows that three-quarters of the students (77%) to be active in all of the following ways in their
perceived that the teacher of their most enjoyed most enjoyed classes:
subject used examples relevant to their experi- • Taking part in reflective conversations about
ences. Congruent with this, 72% believed that the meaning of new learning (item set 4),
this teacher knew what interested them. By con- looking ahead to next learning steps (item set
trast only 27% of these same students thought the 3) and discussing acts of learning per se (item
teacher of their least enjoyed classes used relevant set 5), with all three item sets showing close to
experiences and just 20% thought this teacher 50% differences in frequency of occurrence in
knew what interested students. Three-quarters of most and least enjoyed classes
the teachers of the nominated most enjoyed • Building connections between school and life
classes (77%) thought they related contexts of beyond school (item set 1), learning in con-
learning to students’ experiences, as did 66% of texts beyond the classroom (item set 10) and
those who taught in students’ least enjoyed engaging with real issues (item set 8)
classes. Thus the frequency difference between • Interacting with peers, both during learning
the perceptions of teachers of most and least (item sets 6 and 11) and when assessing learn-
enjoyed classes that they orchestrated opportuni- ing (item set 9)
ties to draw links between current learning and • Making and correcting one’s own mistakes
students’ wider experiences was just 11%, com- (item set 7) and exercising some autonomy
pared to a 50% difference in students’ recognition over learning directions and/or showing lead-
of such linking as an affordance of the learning in ership in class (item set 12)
most and least enjoyed classes. A similar pattern In the most enjoyed classes, frequencies for
holds for all the item sets in Table 21.2. student recognition of the various affordances
Note that some item sets in Table 21.2 are were largely matched by teacher perceptions of
closely matched, with only a slight change of opportunities to learn in those classes. The pat-
wording for teacher and student versions. tern is very different when student responses are
However, some groupings bring together items compared with those for teachers of least enjoyed
with similar intent but different descriptions. For classes. In these least enjoyed classes, the oppor-
example, item set 4 contrasts one teacher item tunities teachers perceived they offered were not
that asked about reflection as a general activity recognised as affordances by many of the stu-
with three student items that each described a dif- dents. It may be that some of the teachers of the
ferent possibility for reflecting on learning. least enjoyed classes did not make certain oppor-
Similarly, item set 12 explores students’ percep- tunities to learn as visible to students as they
tions of choice as residing in actual selection of thought they did. It is also possible that some
topics and opportunities to show leadership in teachers of these classes were out of touch with
class. By contrast, the matched teacher item cues students’ interests and learning needs, or perhaps
student ‘input’ which need not imply the same simply not focused on students as individual
level of freedom, or ultimate determination of learners, which could be the case for a teacher
curriculum topics and directions. This difference with a very strong content orientation, for exam-
doubtless explains the atypically large difference ple. Alternatively, students might be less active in
in item set 12 between teachers of most enjoyed seeking connections, perceiving relevance and
classes and students’ views of learning in those participating actively when they are not enjoying
classes. a class. Either way, it seems less likely that oppor-
The pattern of responses in Table 21.2 sug- tunities to participate actively in learning will be
gests that enjoyment of learning, as a proxy for recognised or embraced in students in their less
engagement in learning, is associated with a enjoyed classes.
range of opportunities to be actively participat- One caveat for the comparisons in Table 21.2
ing as a learner. In addition to a traditional focus is that teachers were thinking about that class as
on ‘hands-on’ learning, students were more likely a whole, whereas each student was focused on
450 R. Hipkins

their personal learning. We have no way of know- mind here that these are comparisons of the same
ing if all the students in any one class would have students, as they variously engage with learning
answered the survey in a similar way. What we in two different settings. Classes that were seen
can say is that not enjoying a class is often linked as least enjoyable by these students were associ-
to having a teacher who appears less attuned to a ated with:
specific student’s personal learning needs, com- • Lack of intellectual challenge (item set 1), or
pared to the teacher of their most enjoyed class. learning ‘stretch’ as indicated by getting totally
This is borne out by a comparison of items that absorbed in a task (item set 3), or getting
did apply specifically to an individual student, as involved in conversations about ideas (item
discussed next. set 2), where for all three items again we see
student frequency differences of 50% or more
between most and least enjoyed classes
Associations Between Expectations • Lack of opportunities for learning from mis-
and Engagement takes (item set 9), safely exploring alternative
views and ways of interpreting knowledge
Table 21.3 follows a similar format to Table 21.2, (item sets 7 and 11), and asking questions to
but here the teacher is responding to items about develop a better understanding (item set 10)
the student as a named individual in their class. • Not valuing the work sufficiently to take care
Some of the student items have already been over its completion (item set 4), or checking it
introduced, but here, they are matched to teacher for potential improvements (item set 5); not
items specifically related to them personally. working purposefully in class (item set 8),
Where the wording matches closely, the item set including with other students (item set 12);
draws a contrast between how the student sees and the slightly greater likelihood (at least
themself as a learner and how their two teachers from the teacher’s perspective) of skipping an
see them. Some item sets are not as closely NCEA assessment (item set 13)
matched but have been paired because they • Not expecting to gain intrinsic rewards in the
inform the same opportunity or learning chal- form of personal goals met (item set 8) or the
lenge. For example, item set 2 probes student extrinsic reward of assessment credits gained
perceptions that their teachers are interested in towards an NCEA qualification (item set 6)
what they think, whereas the matched teacher Interestingly, students’ intellectual involve-
item asks about how well the student can express ment tended to be underestimated by teachers in
what they think (the teacher Likert scale changes most enjoyed classes, compared to students’ own
accordingly). This pairing assumes that teacher perceptions. For example, whereas 86% of stu-
awareness of the relevant behaviours is actually dents thought learning was challenging in their
linked to opportunities to demonstrate these. Item most enjoyed classes, just 44% of the teachers of
set 6 is different again. This item set contrasts those classes thought students would choose work
students’ expectations of gaining credits from that allowed them to gain further knowledge and
their NCEA (qualifications) assessments with the skills (and hence, by implication, would be more
teacher’s view of whether or not those expecta- challenging). It may be simply that some teachers
tions are likely to be realistic. felt they lacked the evidence to comment, but
Again we see, through the students’ eyes, then that could be indicative of lacking overt
much lower frequencies of occurrence in their opportunities to make the relevant observations
least enjoyed classes of the various potential during class. Alternatively, it may be that students
affordances described. For most item sets, the overestimate the extent of their active meaning-
teacher-reported frequencies of occurrence were making or simply do not see the challenges that
also considerably lower in least enjoyed classes the teacher sees to be inherent in learning implied
than in those the students most enjoyed. Keep in by some items. What we can say is that, from the
21 The Engaging Nature of Teaching for Competency Development 451

students’ perspectives, there are indications that seldom happens. In a recent national survey, just
opportunities for active and challenging meaning- 10% of high school teachers said they always or
making are associated with greater enjoyment of quite often involved their students in building
learning. Both Tables 21.2 and 21.3 show such NCEA assessment plans (Hipkins, 2010a).
items at the top of the student rankings for fre- If students perceived that NCEA did in fact
quency differences between most and least offer them the affordance of making strategic
enjoyed classes. This in turn suggests that for assessment choices, would this enhance their
many respondents, ‘enjoyment’ did not signal a enjoyment in the same way that perceptions of
preference for taking an easy route in class. greater autonomy in other aspects of their learn-
Comments made by some of the teachers of ing appear to do? What would need to change for
least enjoyed classes suggested they saw it as teachers to perceive that they can in fact support
unreasonable to be expected to know personal students to take up this opportunity, which already
attributes of individual students. Non-response or exists in principle? Would both they and their stu-
choice of ‘neutral’ in this part of the survey was dents experience rewards in the form of greater
correspondingly higher than for returns from enjoyment of learning in the parts of the curricu-
teachers of favourite classes. Notice too that these lum in which they choose to aspire for assess-
teachers were consistently more pessimistic in ment success? These are questions that bear
their expectations of students’ likely learning further investigation. Some pointers to the chal-
effort and success. Elsewhere in the survey, teach- lenges that teachers face as practice imperatives
ers were asked to predict students’ likely highest change are implied by a small but growing
level of qualification in their post-school years. body of research on teaching for competency
The teacher of a student’s most enjoyed class development.
typically indicated a higher qualification than the
teacher of the same student’s least enjoyed class
(Wylie et al., 2009). Students also held lower The Engaging Nature of Competency
expectations of success in their least enjoyed Development
classes, and in this instance overall frequencies
for their views were much closer to those of their A recent analysis of the challenges of integrating
teachers. One student item simply stated ‘I do key competencies with learning in one very sim-
well [in this class’]. Most students (89%) agreed ple science topic [the water cycle] (Hipkins,
this was so in their most enjoyed class, compared 2010b) identified the following four key points of
to 34% in their least enjoyed class. difference from traditional teaching of this topic.
Notice that active participation of students in First, the teacher must hold a clear ‘big picture’
making decisions about assessment for NCEA, as purpose in mind, so that the learning matters for
opposed to learning in general, was not seen by something more than just acquisition of new con-
most students as something they could or would tent knowledge. Second, the learning should be
do, nor did their teachers see this as an option set in context and linked to students’ life experi-
open to the students. Unlike almost every other ences, and where possible, these links should be
item set reported in this chapter, there was no sufficiently open that students can personalise the
substantive difference for most and least enjoyed connections to what matters to them. Third, acts
classes (item set 13). NCEA is built from stan- of meaning-making within the discipline of sci-
dards-based modules, and so students have a ence should be an explicit focus of learning, not
degree of choice in shaping the composition of just something that happens serendipitously (or
their certificates, at least in theory (Hipkins et al., not). Finally, students’ ideas should be used in
2005). Our 16 year olds could be supported to ways that establish and sustain their connection
develop considerable autonomy in charting their to the intended learning while also setting up new
course through NCEA, but it appears that this challenges that strengthen their learning-to-learn
452 R. Hipkins

capabilities (Hipkins). It will be evident that all within different disciplinary conventions (i.e.
four of these areas of potential difference align addressing the ‘nature’ of the subject, not just the
with the aspects of pedagogy highlighted in content). One competency identified as a specific
Tables 21.2 and 21.3 as more likely to be happen- challenge for twenty-first century learning that
ing in students’ most enjoyed classes. could be developed here is the willingness and
Notwithstanding these strong potential links intellectual means to explore ideas as ideas, not
between teaching for competency development just as received wisdom (Bereiter & Scardamalia,
and student engagement with learning, a growing 2006). Tables 21.2 and 21.3 include items that
body of key competencies research has revealed could be read as entailing active this type of
that they are likely to be interpreted, at least ini- meaning-making, although it is again likely they
tially, as requiring only a surface level changes to were not read very deeply by many respondents.
pedagogy, and perhaps a strengthening of current Even so, the tables reveal considerable differ-
‘good practice’ (Hipkins, 2011). For example, ences between the affordances that student per-
the title ‘managing self’ underplays the intent of ceive their least and most enjoyed classes offer
the OECD equivalent ‘acting autonomously’ (see for: exploring ideas, discussing multiple interpre-
Table 21.1). As cued by its NZC title, managing tations of knowledge, and thinking and talking
self has been widely interpreted to entail involv- about acts of learning.
ing students in goal setting and managing rou- The DeSeCo definition of competency devel-
tines of learning such as arriving at class on time opment draws attention to the need to mobilise
and with the necessary materials, in contrast to knowledge and skills for use in challenging new
the OECD definition that includes aspects such contexts (Rychen & Salganik, 2003). At the very
as ‘acting within the big picture’ (Rychen, 2004). least, the key competency ‘participating and con-
Some items reported in Table 21.3 are set at this tributing’ implies that students need to be able to
surface level of competency development, yet make personally meaningful links between the-
even this is sufficient to impact enjoyment, and ory and action and between classroom learning
hence by implication engagement with learning. and life beyond school (Bolstad, Roberts, Boyd,
Self-managing behaviours certainly create & Hipkins, 2009). ‘Contribution’ also implies
conditions where school learning can be initiated, giving something in exchange for learning, which
but they will not necessarily strengthen students’ is suggestive of an action component where
ability to apply some self-direction to their learn- appropriate. The items included in Table 21.2
ing, or to develop self-awareness of a learning-to- tend to position teachers as the orchestrators of
learn nature. Arguably, the combination of the opportunities for learners to be active, rather than
key competencies ‘thinking’ and ‘using language supporting students to be proactive for them-
symbols and texts’ could refocus learning in ways selves. Nevertheless, there are clear indications
that make acts of learning per se a focus of class- in both tables that enjoyment of learning is linked
room conversations. On a surface level, ‘thinking’ to opportunities for some level of active partici-
might be envisaged as teaching a set of skills pation in practical activities, addressing real-life
(Harpaz, 2007), while ‘using language, symbols issues and in conversation and interaction with
and texts’ has been characterised by some as the other learners.
‘literacy and numeracy’ competency (Hipkins, The final key competency in the NZC set of
2007). While basic academic skills are founda- five is titled ‘relating to others’. At a surface
tional to other learning, a skill-based generic level, this competency can be seen as being about
interpretation seriously underestimates the intel- appropriate interpersonal behaviour in class and
lectual challenge that these competencies can add at school. With the OECD equivalent ‘function-
to learning. In combination, these two key com- ing in socially heterogeneous groups’ in mind,
petencies could invoke semiotic dimensions that pairing this competency with ‘managing self’
require meaning-making to be explicitly addressed points towards building greater self-awareness in
21 The Engaging Nature of Teaching for Competency Development 453

relation to diverse others and the need to modify need to gain greater clarity around multiple
personal cultural expectations and behaviours in potential purposes for learning, while also refram-
different contexts. Taking a different tack, pair- ing their subjects as disciplinary tools that do
ing ‘relating to others’ with ‘participating and specific sorts of work in the world, within certain
contributing’ draws attention to other people as a agreed conventions. That is, they will need to
learning resource, and to the need to strengthen become more ‘literate’ about the nature of their
skills for interacting and developing ideas in the specialist subjects, so they can help their students
spaces between learners, which is often cited as do the same (Hipkins, 2010b). Given the data
important for ‘knowledge work’ in the twenty- presented in this chapter, we could hypothesise
first century (Gilbert, 2005; Bereiter & Scarda- that any shifts to affording students greater auton-
malia, 2006). The items presented in Tables 21.2 omy in their learning will also help strengthen
and 21.3 are more clearly aligned with the latter student engagement. Whether the complex inter-
pairing, again with the caveat that they may not related changes sketched in this section happen
have been read particularly deeply by respon- more widely in practice remains to be seen.
dents. Regardless of the level of interpretation
and application in the classroom, the potential of
teaching for competency development to impact Advancing Teacher Conversations
engagement is again evident in clear differences About Student Engagement
between the opportunities that teachers offer and
students perceive as affordances in their most and This chapter has explored student engagement in
least enjoyed classes. relation to the opportunities for learning that
Items that describe practices that hint at fos- teachers say they offer and the affordances for
tering greater learner autonomy ranked lower in learning that senior high school students perceive
teachers’ estimation of the opportunities they to be available to them in most and least enjoyed
offer and students’ estimation of the affordances classes. Framing learning in terms of developing
available to them, even in most enjoyed classes. or strengthening key competencies adds a critical
Just 39% of teachers in most enjoyed classes said curriculum dimension to the discussion and
that students were encouraged to assess each aligns curriculum change imperatives with peda-
others’ work and give feedback, and 37% said gogical change. The analysis has presented
students could sometimes lead classroom learn- teacher and student data separately in order to
ing. Just 28% of students said they could choose contrast differences in perceptions, but, in reality,
study topics in their most enjoyed classes, and engagement is co-constructed in the classroom
only 17% perceived they could make choices moment as interactions play out between teacher
about the NCEA assessment they would under- and students, and between the students them-
take. These options were hardly available at all in selves. This section of the chapter proposes a
least enjoyed classes. If teachers are serious about complex, dynamic framing of the relationships
fostering greater student autonomy, they need to between teacher and student actions, motivations
scaffold opportunities for greater self-determina- and engagement and identifies some implications
tion of learning pathways, greater self-awareness for teacher professional learning.
of purposes, habits and progress in strengthening As well as having separate teacher and learner
competencies as a learner and as a citizen in a components, there is an element of simultaneity
diverse and rapidly changing world. If the imper- to engagement as it emerges in the classroom
ative for greater self-direction in combination moment (Davis & Sumara, 2010). Davis and
with greater participation is not to be misrepre- Sumara noted that it is unhelpful to debate the
sented as a relativistic ‘anything goes and noth- merits of either student-centred or teacher-cen-
ing matters’ view of curriculum (Hipkins et al., tred learning as if they are an inevitable duality.
2010), many teachers of high school students will Learning is simultaneously both individual and
454 R. Hipkins

situated. The classroom environment is antici- Believing that learning that is worth the invest-
pated and orchestrated by the teacher in the first ment of effort and time doubtless acts as a con-
instance but ultimately co-created by all those tinuing personal motivation, while also increasing
present. Engagement also has temporal dimen- the likelihood that opportunities offered by the
sions. It emerges in the flow of time, building on teacher will be recognised as affordances for
past experiences and looking to possible futures. learning by the student and hence taken up.
Within a complex framing such as this, the choice However, the clear student and teacher differ-
of feelings about individual subjects is a useful ences between most and least enjoyed subjects
proxy for engagement because it is likely to point to the strong influence teachers can exert on
include aspects of all three temporal dimensions students’ personal preferences in the moment. As
(past, present, future), whether students and they focus and shape the learning possibilities
teachers are aware of the impact of these or not. offered, teachers influence cognitive engage-
For the student, the identification of a subject ment. Interestingly, the cognitive quality of inter-
as ‘most enjoyed’ is likely to relate at least in part actions is the pedagogical dimension where the
to their personal interests and preferences, under- data show the strongest differences between most
pinned by the goals and aspirations that motivate and least enjoyed subjects. Students do appear to
them, which are grounded in past learning experi- be engaged by challenging learning that stretches
ences and in all the other factors that impact on them (see also Wylie & Hodgen, 2012), espe-
their general engagement trajectory across the cially when metacognitive dimensions such that
years of school (Wylie & Hodgen, 2012). Although learning-to-learn are also in the frame. Teachers
the chapter has focused on overall frequency dif- can help students envisage new personal and col-
ferences between most and least enjoyed subjects, lective learning possibilities here.
there is evidence that some students’ perceptions Teachers also help enlarge personal percep-
of specific affordances did not differ for the two tions of relevance when they support students to
classes they nominated. Selecting two of the more look beyond the personal to interpersonal differ-
metacognitive statements, ‘I get time to think and ences in perspectives and outwards again to the
talk about how I’m learning’, and ‘I like to reflect world beyond school. Again the data show strong
on how I’ve learned something’, we cross-tabu- associations with engagement. Most enjoyed
lated students’ responses for each class. We found classes are participatory spaces where students
that the manner in which individuals responded in interact safely and enjoyably with each other, and
these two settings was significantly more likely to where learning is meaningfully linked to their
be similar than different. Students who agreed that life experiences and to issues that concern them.
they got time for reflecting on their learning in With competency development in view, the pur-
their most enjoyed class were also more likely to poses for learning that teachers ‘talk up’ need not
agree that this time was also available in their be limited to near-horizon possibilities such as
least enjoyed class. Those who selected the neu- passing examinations but can extend to the sorts
tral response for one class were also more likely to of young people students wish to become and
select it for the other, suggesting perhaps that they the sorts of futures they could potentially help
were not sure what these items were about. build for themselves and others (Bolstad et al.,
Interestingly, the pattern did not hold at the very 2009). This framing illustrates why some define
strong level of response: students who totally engagement as ‘energy in action, the connec-
agreed they got this time in their most enjoyed tion between person and activity’ (Russell et al.,
class were as likely to totally disagree about their 2005, p.3).
least enjoyed class as to totally agree. The rela- The Competent Learners research shows that
tionship between individual and contextual dimen- teachers who are more successful at engaging stu-
sions of engagement in class is clearly complex dents appear able to make more realistic assess-
and could well be the subject of a further level of ments of the opportunities they offer and that
analysis of the data set reported here. students take up. They know their students better
21 The Engaging Nature of Teaching for Competency Development 455

and in general hold higher expectations of their Bolstad, R., Boyd, S., & Hipkins, R. (2005). Students as
achievement. One powerful implication from the lifelong learners: Reflections on student data from the
Curriculum Innovations Projects. Paper presented at
findings is that teachers need not simply accept the New Zealand Association for Research in
students’ feelings about their class. They can take Education annual conference, Dunedin, New Zealand,
the lead in co-creating a learning environment December. Retrieved November 25, 2010, from http://
that is more engaging and simultaneously more www.nzcer.org.nz/default.php?products_id=2714
Bolstad, R., & Gilbert, J. (2008). Disciplining and draft-
likely to build students’ competencies in powerful ing, or 21st century learning? Rethinking the New
and useful ways. However, in order to do so, they Zealand senior secondary curriculum for the future.
may need to let go of some control of the learning Wellington, New Zealand: New Zealand Council for
action, affording more space for students to create Educational Research.
Bolstad, R., Roberts, J., Boyd, S., & Hipkins, R. (2009).
links of personal relevance to them and in which Kick starts; Key competencies: Exploring the poten-
they can exercise responsible choices about learn- tial of participating and contributing. Wellington,
ing options and pathways. In one recent case New Zealand: NZCER Press.
study project, we found that making these types Carr, M. (2006). Dimensions of strength for key competen-
cies. Retrieved February 10, 2008, from http://nzcur-
of pedagogical changes appeared to be easier for riculum.tki.org.nz/curriculum_project_archives/
some teachers than for others (Bolstad et al., references
2005). Why is that? This question bears further Carr, M. (2008). Can assessment unlock and open the
investigation. There are implications for profes- doors to resourcefulness and agency? In S. Swaffield
(Ed.), Unlocking assessment. Understanding for
sional learning in relation to extending teachers’ reflection and application (pp. 36–54). London/New
pedagogical repertoire, but also in relation to York: Routledge.
challenging them to rethink their views of curric- Davis, B., & Sumara, D. (2010). “If things were sim-
ulum and of purposes for learning. ple…”: Complexity in education. Journal of Evaluation
in Clinical Practice, 16, 856–860.
This chapter has positioned key competencies Delandshere, G., & Petrosky, A. (1998). Assessment of
as potential drivers of profound curriculum complex performances: Limitations of key measurement
change, albeit with modest success so far in New assumptions. Educational Researcher, 27(2), 14–24.
Zealand. Doubtless other similar initiatives could Fredricks, J., Blumenfeld, P., & Paris, A. (2004). School
engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evi-
achieve the same impetus by addressing the same dence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1), 59–109.
pedagogical (and perhaps curriculum) differ- Gee, J. (2008). A sociocultural perspective on opportunity
ences between classes that students enjoy and to learn. In P. Moss, D. Pullin, J. Gee, E. Haertel, & L.
those that they do not. This chapter is not an argu- Young (Eds.), Assessment, equity and opportunity to
learn (pp. 76–108). Cambridge, UK/New York/
ment for foregrounding competency develop- Melbourne, Australia/Madrid, Spain/Cape Town,
ment per se but for re-examining curriculum South Africa/Singapore/Sao Paulo, Brazil/Delhi,
assumptions and pedagogical practices and ensur- India: Cambridge University Press.
ing that any initiatives intended to strengthen stu- Gilbert, J. (2005). Catching the knowledge wave? The
knowledge society and the future of education.
dent engagement take the whole learning context Wellington, New Zealand: NZCER Press.
into account. This must include adopting a more Haertel, E., Moss, P., Pullin, D., & Gee, J. (2008).
nuanced view of opportunities to learn and how Introduction. In P. Moss, D. Pullin, J. Gee, E. Haertel,
these are impacted by the classroom environment & L. Young (Eds.), Assessment, equity and opportu-
nity to learn (pp. 1–16). Cambridge, UK/New York/
and teacher’s actions. Melbourne, Australia/Madrid, Spain/Cape Town,
South Africa/Singapore/Sao Paulo, Brazil/Delhi,
India: Cambridge University Press.
Harpaz, Y. (2007). Approaches to teaching thinking:
References Towards a conceptual mapping of the field. Teachers
College Record, 109(8), 1845–1874.
Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (2006). Education for the Hipkins, R. (2006). The nature of the key competencies. A
knowledge age: Design centered models of teaching background paper. Wellington, New Zealand: New
and instruction. In P. Alexander & P. Winne (Eds.), Zealand Council for Educational Research. Retrieved
Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., November 25, 2010, from http://keycompetencies.tki.
pp. 695–713). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum org.nz/Resourcebank/Introducing-key-competencies2/
Associates. Key-resources
456 R. Hipkins

Hipkins, R. (2007). Assessing key competencies: Why November 25, 2010, from http://www.nzcer.org.nz/
would we? How could we? Retrieved February 10, default.php?products_id=1583
2009, from http://nzcurriculum.tki.org.nz/implemen- Ministry of Education. (2007). The New Zealand curricu-
tation_packs_for_schools/assessing_key_competen- lum. Wellington, New Zealand: Learning Media.
cies_why_would_we_how_could_we OECD. (2005). The definition and selection of key competen-
Hipkins, R. (2010a). The evolving NCEA. Wellington, cies: Executive summary. Retrieved November 25, 2010,
New Zealand: New Zealand Council for Educational from http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/47/61/35070367.pdf
Research. Retrieved November 25, 2010, from http:// Reid, A. (2006). Key competencies: a new way forward or
www.nzcer.org.nz/default.php?products_id=2529 more of the same? Curriculum Matters, 2, 43–62.
Hipkins, R. (2010b). More complex than skills: Rethinking Russell, J., Ainley, M., & Frydenberg, E. (2005). Issues
the relationship between key competencies and cur- digest: Motivation and engagement. Canberra,
riculum content. Paper presented at the international Australia: Australian Government: Department of
conference on Education and Development of Civic Education, Science and Training.
Competencies, Seoul, South Korea, October. Retrieved Rychen, D. (2004). An overarching conceptual framework
November 25, 2010, from http://www.nzcer.org.nz/ for assessing key competences in an international con-
default.php?products_id=2713 text: Lessons from an interdisciplinary and policy-ori-
Hipkins, R., & Boyd, S. (2011). The recursive elaboration ented approach. Retrieved November 25, 2010, from
of key competencies as agents of curriculum change. http://www.cedefop.europa.eu/etv/Upload/Projects_
Curriculum Matters, 7, 70–86. Networks/ResearchLab/ResearchReport/BgR1_
Hipkins, R., Bull, A., & Reid, A. (2010). Some reflections Rychen.pdf
on the philosophical and pedagogical challenges of Rychen, D., & Salganik, L. (Eds.). (2003). Key competen-
transforming education. Curriculum Journal, 21(1), cies for a successful life and a well-functioning soci-
109–118. ety. Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe and Huber.
Hipkins, R., Roberts, J., Bolstad, R., & Ferral, H. (2006). Wertsch, J. (1998). Mind as action. New York: Oxford
Staying in science 2: Transition to tertiary study from University Press.
the perspectives of New Zealand Year 13 science stu- Wylie, C., Hipkins, R., & Hodgen, E. (2009). On the edge
dents. Wellington, New Zealand: Report prepared for of adulthood: Young people’s school and out-of-school
New Zealand Ministry of Research, Science and experiences at 16. Wellington, New Zealand: Ministry
Technology (MoRST). Retrieved November 25, 2010, of Education. Retrieved November 25, 2010, from
from http://www.nzcer.org.nz/pdfs/14605.pdf http://www.nzcer.org.nz/content/competent-learners-
Hipkins, R., Vaughan, K., with Beals, F., Ferral, H., & edge-adulthood.pdf
Gardiner, B. (2005). Shaping our futures: Meeting Wylie, C., & Hodgen, E. (2012). Trajectories and patterns
secondary students’ learning needs in a time of of student engagement: Evidence from a longitudinal
evolving qualifications (Final report of the Learning study. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie
Curves project). Wellington, New Zealand: New (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement
Zealand Council for Educational Research. Retrieved (pp. 585–599). New York: Springer.
Assessment as a Context for Student
Engagement 22
Sharon L. Nichols and Heather S. Dawson

Abstract
The purpose of this chapter is to examine the ways in which assessment-
related instructional practices empirically and theoretically link to student
motivation and engagement. We discuss these links in three sections. First,
we briefly look at the history of standardized testing in America’s schools,
drawing connections between the use of testing in practice and student moti-
vation. Next, we look at research on classroom-based assessment practices to
discuss how they connect to student motivation. We organize our discussion
according to summative and formative distinctions, concluding that summa-
tive testing systems tend to connect with traditional motivation processes
such as goals and efficacy-related beliefs, whereas formative systems tend to
connect with engagement-related processes such as self-regulated learning
and self-determination. In the last section, we extrapolate from lessons
learned in previous sections to hypothesize on the ways in which high-stakes
testing practices may undermine student motivation and engagement.

their students’ level of understanding so that they


Introduction and Chapter Overview can adjust instruction accordingly. These assess-
ment-related tools and activities come in a variety
The cornerstone of a teacher’s job is to assess and
of forms and serve many different purposes, but all
evaluate their students’ academic status and prog-
feed the same goal of informing the teacher of stu-
ress. Using a variety of tools and activities, teach-
dents’ academic progress so that they can construct
ers are constantly collecting information about
meaningful daily lessons that target learners’ aca-
demic strengths and weakness (McMillan, 2001).
The term assessment is used to describe any
S.L. Nichols, Ph.D. (*) activity, tool, or interaction, planned or unplanned,
Department of Educational Psychology, that provides teachers with academic-related
University of Texas, San Antonio, TX, USA
e-mail: Sharon.nichols@utsa.edu
information about students. Assessments include
formal, structured activities such as tests,
H.S. Dawson, Ph.D.
Department of Educational Policy and Leadership,
strategically designed small group activities or
The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA whole-class question-and-answer sessions, as
e-mail: dawson.282@osu.edu well as informal, unstructured activities such as

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 457


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_22, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
458 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

impromptu discussions that emerge naturally and into their cognitive representations that later
spontaneously throughout a school day (Stiggins became Expectancy X Value Theory (Feather,
& Conklin, 1992). Structured and unstructured 1982; McClelland, 1985; Wigfield & Eccles,
assessment activities are equally critical compo- 1992). Cognitive approaches dominate current
nents of effective teaching practice (Good & theorizing about motivation.
Brophy, 2008; Peterson & Walberg, 1979). The concept of student “engagement” emerged
Assessment-related activities pervade teach- from the cognitive revolution in motivational the-
ing and learning, and yet there is a paucity of orizing. In our view, student “engagement” is a
research examining the connection between these specific type of motivation that involves cognitive,
activities and students’ motivation or engage- affective, and self-regulatory processes (Corno &
ment. One explanation rests with the fact that Mandinach, 2004; Elliot & Dweck, 2005). In con-
assessment contexts are difficult to operational- trast to the term “motivation” that encompasses
ize and measure due to their inherent complexi- many different theories for explaining human
ties. Brookhart (2004) put it this way, behavior, engagement refers to a particular view
of motivated action that presumes students’ active
The reason the field [classroom assessments] is a
bit scattered at present is that classroom assess- involvement in and appropriation of effort related
ment sits at intersections in both theory and prac- to learning and achievement outcomes.
tice and that the resulting array of relevant practical
and theoretical material creates tensions for those
who try to chart this territory (p. 429).
Chapter Goals and Structure
Another challenge is that teachers vary widely
in the ways in which they construct, implement, In spite of the challenges of isolating specific
and utilize assessment-related devices in their assessment-related practices (Brookhart, 2004)
classrooms, making it difficult to extract general- and the evolving conceptions of motivation, there
izable data about how these activities connect have been some meaningful efforts aimed at
with student motivation and engagement. Further, understanding their connections (Black &
the nested nature of such data (students in class- Wiliam, 1998, 2006; Brookhart, 1997; Crooks,
rooms, in schools) also makes it difficult to iso- 1988; Natriello, 1987). Our goal is to try to bridge
late cause-effect relationships among assessment ideas gleaned from assessment and motivation/
and motivation variables (Miller & Murdock, engagement fields and to discuss tests (especially
2007; Murdock & Miller, 2009). high-stakes tests) as contexts of learning and to
Another challenge in studying the connection trace the ways in which these contexts empiri-
between assessment and student motivation rests cally and theoretically connect to student motiva-
with evolving definitions of what student motiva- tion and engagement. We begin with a discussion
tion is or entails. Throughout the past century, of low-stakes standardized tests in American
presumptions about the core mechanisms of education. Their rise in popularity and use in
motivated action have evolved. McCaslin and classroom contexts prompted a wave of research
DiMarino-Linnen (2000), for example, argued investigating their effects on teacher practice.
that earlier views were seemingly more complex Although direct links to student motivation are
and inclusive in that they tended to involve bio- lacking, we extrapolate indirect links between
logically based variables (needs, motives) as they standardized test scores and student motivation
simultaneously interacted with achievement- from the teacher expectation literature.
related dispositions (expectancy, values, goals) Next we turn to classroom-based (teacher-
(Hull, 1943, 1952; McClelland, 1980, 1985). made) assessment practices. In this chapter,
Later, Atkinson’s achievement motivation theory “classroom-based tests” and “teacher-made tests”
(Atkinson, 1974, 1981a, b) prompted a signifi- are used interchangeably to refer to those tests
cant shift in theorizing when he translated Hull’s developed and administered by individual teach-
biologically based variables of habit and drive ers. We also refer to “classroom-based” or
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 459

“teacher-made” assessment practices as a way to and evaluate instructional effectiveness) and are
discuss all of the activities subsumed within the used frequently by teachers to influence instruc-
development, administration, and evaluation of tional decision-making (Crooks, 1988; Stiggins &
those tests. We organize this section of the chapter Bridgeford, 1985; Stiggins & Conklin, 1992). For
according to summative and formative distinc- most students, teacher-made tests are a low-stakes
tions made in the measurement literature, drawing situation since passing/failing a single classroom
connections to student motivation and engage- test does not lead to life-altering consequences
ment, respectively. Finally, we discuss high-stakes (i.e., students have multiple opportunities through-
standardized testing. Drawing from lessons out any given year to demonstrate proficiencies).
learned from studies with low-stakes standardized Standardized tests by contrast, can be either high
tests as well as with classroom-based assessments, or low stakes, are developed outside of the class-
we hypothesize on the effects of high-stakes test- room, are administered infrequently, and have
ing and student motivation and engagement. We historically had a minimal influence on teacher
conclude with recommendations for future practice (Stiggins & Bridgeford, 1992).
research, practice, and policy implementation.

Standardized Tests and Testing


Assessment Contexts
Standardized tests emerged in the late eighteenth
An essential component of a teacher’s job is century at the same time psychologists were
to assess (evaluate) the academic progress of becoming increasingly interested in developing
their students through “structured” and “sponta- scientific methods for measuring human qualities
neous” activities designed to inform instructional such as intelligence (Gamson, 2007; Sacks,
decision-making. Structured assessments include 1999). The widespread use of such tests, how-
activities that are planned and purposeful such as ever, was not popularized until the construction
pencil-and-paper teacher-made tests, standard- of the “National Intelligence Test” spearheaded
ized tests, and performance-based activities, by Edward Thorndike, Lewis Terman, and Robert
homework, and teacher-student question-and- Yerkes in the early 1900s (Giordano, 2005).
answer sessions (Crooks, 1988; Rodriguez, 2004; Following several prototypes, this National Test
Stiggins & Conklin, 1992;). By contrast, “spon- included ten tasks (such as printed directions,
taneous” assessments emerge “from the naturally comparison, picture completion, vocabulary) that
occurring classroom environment and lead the were organized into a booklet “that could be
teacher to a judgment about an individual stu- applied to any child in the elementary school who
dent’s level of development” (Stiggins & Conklin, could read well enough to participate in a group
1992, p. 33). Thus, classrooms are naturally examination” (Whipple, 1921, p. 17, as cited in
evolving assessment contexts in which teachers Giordano, 2005, p. 21).
use planned and unplanned activities as data to Growing familiarity with these standardized
inform their teaching. tests ignited debates about how best to assess stu-
Tests are a familiar form of structured assess- dents’ academic success in school settings.
ment activity that assume a variety of roles and Proponents, skeptical of subjective teacher grad-
purposes in the classroom (Black & Wiliam, 1998; ing systems (Starch & Elliott, 1912, as referenced
Brookhart, 1997; Crooks, 1988). Tests are either in Giordano, 2005), believed that standardized
teacher-made or standardized and can be either tests were the perfect way to elicit meaningful
high or low stakes. Teacher-made tests have varied and reliable data about students. Opponents
formats (multiple choice, essay, short answer, per- worried about test bias and their limited capacity
formance assessments), roles (graded, ungraded, to adequately account for student differences
quiz, unit test), and purposes (to diagnose student (e.g., race, income). These familiar worries about
needs, clarify achievement expectations, motivate, the appropriate use of standardized tests have
460 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

persisted since their inception (Sacks, 1999).In systems pervade modern American classrooms
spite of ongoing debates regarding the fundamen- (Herman & Haertel, 2005; Orfield & Kornhaber,
tal purposes of schools (and therefore, use of 2001), creating a particular type of assessment
tests, e.g., Cuban, 1988; Tyack & Cuban, 1997), context that we will visit in the second half of this
proponents of standardized tests were convinced chapter. First, we explore some data indicating
of their necessity and role. Eminent psychologist how low-stakes standardized tests may connect
and respected psychometrician E. L. Thorndike to student motivation.
put it this way,
Educational science and educational practice alike
need more objective, more accurate and more con- Low-Stakes Standardized Tests:
venient measures…Any progress toward measur- Student-Teacher Connections
ing how well a child can read with something of
the objectivity, precision, commensurability, and
convenience which characterize our measurement Research in the teacher expectation tradition pro-
of how tall he is, how much he can lift with his vides some initial clues about how teachers’
back or squeeze with his hand, or how acute his knowledge of their students’ standardized test
vision is, would be of great help in grading, pro-
moting, testing the value of methods of teaching scores may impact students’ motivation (e.g.,
and in every other case where we need to know Good, 1981; Good & Brophy, 1972; Kellaghan,
ourselves and to inform others how well an indi- Madaus, & Airasian, 1982). In their seminal
vidual or a class or a school population can read work, Pygmalion in the Classroom, Rosenthal
(Thorndike, 1923, p. 1–2).
and Jacobson (1968) imposed ability-level expec-
Since Thorndike’s time, the form and function tations on teachers by informing them that a spe-
of standardized tests have expanded swiftly and cial test taken by their students at the beginning
have been used for many purposes over the years of the year revealed which students were about to
(Sacks, 1999). Norm-referenced tests (e.g., IQ) “bloom” and which were not. At the end of the
gave us a way to rank students according to apti- year, Rosenthal and Jacobson found that those
tude. Criterion-referenced tests provided measures randomly identified as “bloomers” had higher
of students’ progress against externally defined grades than those not so identified by the end of
standards. Other standardized tests gave us a way to the school. From this data, they concluded that
make predictions about students’ academic poten- teachers’ initial impressions of student abilities
tial (e.g., SAT, GRE) (Giordano, 2005; Herman & somehow had created a self-fulfilling prophecy
Haertel, 2005; Moss, 2007; Sacks, 1999). effect—transforming perceived “bloomers” into
Until the 1990s, performance on any one of actual ones by the end of the year.
these types of tests had relatively low stakes for Methodological inadequacies in this original
students and essentially no stakes for teachers. study were addressed in subsequent observational
Although some states and districts have a history studies in which teacher interactions with their
of experimenting with high-stakes testing as a students were measured after imposed (or natu-
way to reform and improve schools, this practice ral) expectations were induced (or measured)
was inconsistent and relatively inconsequential (Good & Brophy, 1972, 1974; Good & Thompson,
for large groups of students (Allington & McGill- 1998). A growing literature on expectation effects
Franzen, 1992; Tyack, 1974). This has changed revealed that teachers treated students differen-
radically over the past few decades as the rhetoric tially and according to their perceptions of stu-
of public schools in “crisis” expanded (Berliner dents’ ability. For example, teachers often gave
& Biddle, 1995; Glass, 2008; National their perceived high-ability students more time to
Commission for Excellence in Education think when they were asked a question, whereas
[NCEE], 1983), culminating with the No Child perceived low-ability learners were given less
Left Behind Act (NCLB, 2002) that mandated time and were treated in ways in which the teacher
the use of high-stakes testing in all schools for attempted to “protect” their esteem on tasks (Good,
all students. Consequential standardized testing 1981).
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 461

Importantly, we learned that students are are given more frequently throughout a unit or
aware of their teachers’ beliefs of them. One topic, the results of which inform ongoing teach-
fourth grader put it this way, ing decisions and learner assessments. Both forms
Like half the class is pretty smart and…the other of classroom-based assessments are important
half isn’t. And these people…that’s not smart, sources of information to teachers, each with dis-
she’ll just let ‘em go, she won’t really care what tinct purposes and with potential to influence stu-
they do. ‘Cause she knows they don’t care. And the dent motivation and engagement (Black & Wiliam,
rest of the people, she’ll just push and push, and
says, We gotta have some survive, you know (as 1998, 2006; Brookhart, 2004; McInerney, Brown,
quoted in Weinstein, 2002, p. 89). & Liem, 2009).

Not surprisingly, then, data seem to suggest that


teachers’ expectations may relate to students’ Summative Tests and Student
self-esteem and motivation (e.g., Brophy, 1998;
Motivation
Meichenbaum & Smart, 1971; Weinstein, 2002).
However, the link of standardized test scores and Summative tests are those given at the end of
student attitudes is fuzzy at best. Among all the units or topic areas and are typically used to
sources of information teachers use to form assign grades. They consist of test items designed
impressions about their students, the role of tests to gauge how well students may (or may not)
in influencing teachers’ treatment of students may have mastered a given content area. We review
be minimal. Kellaghan et al. (1982, p. 16) note, data demonstrating that the use of these tests can
[standardized] Test information is of itself insuffi- influence student motivation in two primary
cient to overcome the effects of other factors such as ways: (a) test content influences students’ effort
the information teachers glean from assignments
completed by students and from a knowledge of and value formation, and (b) grading/feedback
students’ home backgrounds…test information is systems influence students’ goal adoption.
merely a part, and probably a relatively small part, of
the pressure exerted on students by the educational Tests Inform Students What to Value
environment. Even if expectancy processes operate
in the classroom, test information is only one factor According to Expectancy X Value Theory, moti-
in the network that creates such expectancies, and vation is the result of students’ perceptions of task
any possible role it may have to play in affecting outcome and value. Expectancies are typically
students has to be considered in this context. defined as “individuals’ beliefs about how well
Thus, in spite of all we know about expecta- they will do on upcoming tasks, either in the
tion effects and students (Brophy, 1998; Wein- immediate or longer-term future” (Eccles &
stein, 2002), we know little about the specific role Wigfield, 2002, p. 119), whereas value involves
standardized testing data play in teachers’ inter- four types: attainment, intrinsic, utility, and cost
actions with students and subsequently students’ (Eccles, 1983; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992, 2000,
motivation. 2002). Attainment value refers to the importance
an individual places on the task, resulting in the
individual adjusting self-beliefs accordingly.
Classroom-Based Tests and Testing Intrinsic value refers to the amount of enjoyment
the student acquires from the task. Utility value
Classroom-based (CB) or teacher-made (TM) refers to the perception of usefulness the task has
tests are used to evaluate students’ academic to the student. Cost addresses the sacrifices the
understanding as it pertains to specific topics or student must accept in order to engage in the task.
units of instruction and can be either summative Studies have shown that the form and content
or formative in nature. Summative tests (measures of tests exerts a powerful influence on what is
of learning) are given at the end of units, the results important to learn (i.e., informing students what
of which inform teachers of students’ learning. to value). According to Rogers (1969, as stated in
Formative assessments (measures for learning) Crooks, 1988, p. 445),
462 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

Examinations tell them [students] our real aims, at and evaluation practices set forth through national
least so they believe. If we stress clear understand- policy prescriptions impact student study habits
ing and aim at a growing knowledge of physics, we
may completely sabotage our teaching by a final in two different countries: Britain and Hungary.
examination that asks for numbers to be put into At the time, Britain’s educational culture was
memorized formulas. However loud our sermons, steeped in a summative standardized testing sys-
however intriguing the experiments, students will tem that emphasized “correctness,” whereas
judge by that examination—and so will next year’s
students who hear about it. Hungarian culture had prioritized creativity and
higher-order processing in their schools. Using
Research also suggests that test content may surveys to measure students’ study strategies with
influence students’ study habits and effort approximately 1,200 thirteen to seventeen-year
(Natriello & Dornbusch, 1984; Snyder, 1971). olds in Hungary (n = 579) and Britain (n = 614),
For example, Snyder noticed that although class- Entwistle and Kozeski found main effect differ-
room curricula may emphasize meaning, depth, ences in how students approached studying.
and problem solving, if the test emphasized rote Britain’s students were more apt to employ sur-
memorization, then students, wanting to perform face-level strategies in learning (memorization)
well on tests, would often disregard classroom while Hungarian students were more likely to
activities focusing on problem-oriented learning emphasize deeper learning strategies. Entwistle
and focus on rote memorization to optimize time, and Kozeski cautiously conclude that educational
effort, and academic success on tests (see also assessment environments may influence students’
Fredericksen, 1984). approach to learning.
Tests are important mechanisms that convey to From this data, it seems as if tests’ inherent
students not only what they should know but also utility value as a doorway to academic advance-
how they should know it (Bloom, 1956; ment and personal satisfaction (e.g., doing well
Krathwohl, 2002). Research suggests, for exam- leads to better grades, pleases the teacher) con-
ple, that students vary in their capacity to recog- nects to students’ effort and study strategies.
nize the level of processing demands made by a Students seem to adapt their study strategies
test as well as their ability to adapt to those according to the nature and content of the test
demands. Miller and Parlett (1974) found that they face and want to pass. Of course, students do
some students were highly adept “cue seekers”— not respond in a monolithic way. Some students
those who actively noticed the features of test engage in adaptive, deep-processing study strate-
questions and adapted their study habits accord- gies even if the test promotes memorization and
ingly to maximize their test performance. By con- rote learning (Crooks & Mahalski, 1986). Still,
trast, others were “cue conscious,” less active in testing content conveys important message to
seeking out test features but still relatively con- students about what the culture values, and many
scious about test-related cues that were handed to adjust their efforts accordingly.
them (i.e., by the teacher). Others have shown
that students who generally use “surface level” Tests Influence Student Goals
processing approaches (rote memorization) to and Classroom Goal Structures
tests have difficulty adjusting when deeper-level Achievement goal research has dominated much
processing is necessary (Martin & Ramsden, of the motivation literature in recent decades.
1987). But perhaps more worrisome are data that Relying heavily on self-report measures, this lit-
suggest students capable of deeper-level process- erature has yielded some important constructs for
ing switch to surface-level approaches when understanding how students’ beliefs about them-
faced with surface-level learning environments selves and the task at hand influence outcomes
(i.e., in classrooms that emphasize rote memori- such as persistence and achievement (grades).
zation) (Crooks & Mahalski, 1986). Prior to discussing how goals intersect with tests,
Elsewhere, Entwistle and Kozeski (1985) it seems useful to briefly review some of the main
examined the question of how curriculum content findings from this literature base.
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 463

Goals 1999; Urdan, 1997), and other research to show


Students’ achievement goals are an integrated pat- that performance goals are associated with less
tern of beliefs, attributions, and affect that pro- adaptive outcomes such as use of surface-level
duces the intentions of behavior (Weiner, 1986, cognitive strategies, avoiding help seeking, and
see also Ames, 1992; Ames & Archer, 1988; Elliott low knowledge retention (Kaplan & Maehr,
& Dweck, 1988, 2005). Achievement goals vary 2007; Midgley, Kaplan, & Middleton, 2001). The
according to the context, or are context specific, mixed findings regarding the effects of perfor-
and vary between individuals (Ames & Archer, mance goals on outcomes contributed to the
1988; Kaplan, 2004). A mastery (learning) goal development of the approach-avoid distinction
orientation for learning purports that the individu- for performance goals (Elliot).
al’s purpose is to develop competence (Ames, Performance-approach goals are often associ-
1992; Kaplan & Maehr, 2007). Students with a ated with positive and negative outcomes in the
mastery orientation are more invested in learning literature (Elliot, 1999; Midgley et al. 2001),
for learning’s sake, gaining a genuine understand- whereas performance-avoid goals are more
ing of the material, and acquiring the skills neces- clearly maladaptive in nature (Kaplan & Maehr,
sary to succeed at a given task (Kaplan & Maehr). 2007). Performance-approach goals have been
Mastery-oriented students typically also have associated with positive (persistence, positive
higher self-efficacy, greater persistence for a task, affect, and grades, Elliot, 1999; Kaplan & Maehr,
prefer challenges, are more self-regulated, and 2007) and negative (anxiety, disruptive behavior,
have a greater sense of well-being (Ames, 1992; and less knowledge retention, Kaplan & Maehr,
Elliot, 1999; Kaplan & Maehr, 2007; Kaplan, 2007; Midgley et al. 2001) outcomes. By con-
Middleton, Urdan, & Midgley, 2002). Mastery trast, performance-avoidant goals are consis-
goals also elicit greater effort, elaborative process- tently associated with negative outcomes
ing strategies, and intrinsic motivation (Elliot). including low efficacy, anxiety, avoidance of help
While a mastery orientation for achievement seeking, self-handicapping, and low grades and
describes the individual’s focus on developing share similarities with the work-avoidant goals,
competence, the performance orientation although these two goal orientations stem from
describes the individual’s focus on demonstrat- different cognitive-affective frameworks (Kaplan
ing competence. Performance orientations toward & Maehr). Performance-avoidant goals were the
academic achievement lend themselves to a more only type of goal orientation to inhibit motivation
prominent focus on ability, the desire to achieve (Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996). There is evidence
with little effort, and desire to avoid demonstrat- to support the notion that the performance-
ing lack of ability (Ames, 1992; Ames & Archer, avoidant goal orientation stems from a fear of
1988). The individual with a performance orien- failure rather than from the desire to manage
tation is more concerned with appearing ade- impressions that others hold about the individual
quate. This implies that a student with this (Elliot & Church, 1997).
orientation will succeed or fail based upon their
desire to appear competent or avoid appearing Summative Feedback and Goals
incompetent in front of their peers or mentors. Summative tests yield data that highlight perfor-
Performance-oriented students attribute their mance comparisons among students. After all,
success and failure to their ability rather than to the whole point of a summative test is to high-
the amount of effort they invested and believe light what students know and/or do not know.
ability is fixed, often referred to as an “entity” The way in which teachers formulate their feed-
view of intelligence (Kaplan et al., 2002). back on the basis of summative testing results
There has been research that shows that per- can directly influence students’ goal adoption
formance goals are associated with positive out- (Black & Wiliam, 1998). In a study by Butler
comes such as increased self-efficacy (Elliot, (1988) of 48 eleven-year-old Israeli students,
464 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

participants were given different types of feed- precedence over learning. Further, inherent in the
back following a series of structured paired practice of competition is the necessity for some-
tasks. One-third of the group was given indi- one to lose. If the same students lose over and
vidual comments and feedback, one-third given over despite their best efforts, they may come to
grades only as feedback, and one-third were see the world as unfair and are likely to give up
given comments and grades combined. Results when faced with challenging academic tasks, as
showed the “comments only” group did signifi- they have learned that failure will be the outcome
cantly better than the “grades only” or “com- no matter how hard they try to succeed.
ments plus” groups of students. Butler concludes Conversely, students who routinely win at com-
that normative feedback can undermine motiva- petitive tasks may lose interest in the instructional
tion and that informational feedback enhances material and over time may put forth the minimal
motivation and achievement. amount of effort required to outperform other
Schunk’s (1996) work has revealed similar students rather than maximizing effort in order to
patterns. In one study, he randomly assigned 44 master the task or material.
nine to ten-year olds to one of four conditions. Competition-oriented classrooms may pro-
Half of the students were assigned to tasks where mote the development of performance goals
teachers emphasized performance goals and the rather than mastery goals (Ames, 1984). Similarly,
other half into tasks where teachers emphasized competition and performance goals may decrease
learning goals. Then, within each of these condi- intrinsic motivation toward academic tasks
tions, students were randomly assigned to one of because students rely on rewards from others to
two subsequent conditions: half were asked to be motivate them to complete tasks rather than com-
self-reflective at the conclusion of the task, pleting tasks for the reward of building compe-
whereas the rest were only asked to fill out a tence and skills (Henderlong & Lepper, 2002;
questionnaire. Participants assigned to the per- Lepper, Corpus, & Iyengar, 2005). Competition
formance goal group with no self-evaluation in the classroom also might distract students from
scored lower in skills and efficacy, while partici- learning: they become so focused on performing
pants assigned to the mastery goal group with better than peers that they get distracted from
frequent evaluation scored higher on motivation learning or anxious about losing. In short, the
and achievement outcomes. Feedback which quality and content of summative test feedback
“draws attention away from the task and towards may promote students’ performance (or learn-
self-esteem can have a negative effect on atti- ing), goal orientation, and/or competitive (or col-
tudes and performance” (Black & Wiliam, 1998, laborative) contexts which in turn may impede
p. 13; see also Bangert-Drowns, Kulik, & Morgan, (or enhance) students’ motivation.
1991).
Classroom Goal Structures
Competition and Cooperation In addition to individual teacher-student interac-
Tests and other tasks or classroom activities can tion, teachers also communicate to the whole
also be viewed as competitive or cooperative class, establishing rules and feedback systems
events. In general, classroom activities, tasks, that impact all students. Reciprocally, these
and/or feedback systems that promote competi- events influence students’ interactions with one
tion tend to be associated with lower student another, which in turn also influence teacher
achievement than classes that promote coopera- practice.
tion (Johnson & Johnson, 1985, 1999). Good and Classroom goal structures emerge throughout
Brophy (2008) argued that if students become the many layers of the teaching process including
preoccupied with “winning” or “losing” the com- the type of tasks assigned (difficulty, variety), the
petitive activity, they may lose sight of important nature of student evaluations (norm-based or crite-
instructional objectives and content. From the rion-based), and the way teacher authority is com-
student’s perspective, performance then takes municated (e.g., Ames, 1992; Blumenfeld, 1992).
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 465

Classroom goals are often measured by aggregat- Self-Regulation


ing individual students’ perceptions of the types Growing attention to the concept of self-regulation
of goals emphasized in their classes1 and fall into emerged in the 1970s with the advent of process-
the familiar categories of mastery- or perfor- product research constructed to understand
mance-based classroom goals. Tasks promote how teachers’ instructional practice and use of
performance orientation when they prompt stu- time correlated with student outcomes (Good,
dents to compare their abilities with other stu- 1987; Peterson & Walberg, 1979). During this
dents. Similarly, evaluation systems (i.e., grades, period, observational research was flourishing,
achievement feedback) that stress social compari- and researchers were beginning to understand the
son in achievement are associated with perfor- important connections between specific instruc-
mance goal classroom structures (Ames, 1984; tional activities and successful student engage-
Johnson & Johnson, 1985). And authority systems ment and achievement (Berliner, 1979). At the
or classroom management techniques that restrict same time, researchers were focusing on teacher
student autonomy and control are related to per- practice (Good & Brophy, 1972, 1974) and others
formance-based goal structures. Studies also sug- were becoming more concerned with student
gest that classrooms with a performance goal motivation variables (e.g., Corno & Mandinach,
structure are associated with higher levels of stu- 1983). If there were “optimal” ways teachers
dent cheating when compared to classroom with a might distribute their time during instruction, then
mastery structure (Anderman, 2007). researchers wanted to know what were the “opti-
mal” ways students might also be engaged in their
Formative Tests and Student own learning.
Engagement Corno and Mandinach (1983) described “self-
Student engagement is a type of motivation that regulated learning” (SRL) as one of the “most
involves cognitive and affective processes that sophisticated forms of engagement that students
unfold over time. This dynamic view of motiva- could display in school-related (academic) activi-
tion is more consistent with formative approaches ties and events” (Corno & Mandinach, 2004,
to assessment because they both conceptualize p. 300). According to Corno and Mandinach
learning as a process instead of a product. To (1983, 2004), SRL involved not just effective
explore these connections, we focus on two cognitive processing skills (e.g., appropriate use
approaches to the concept of student engagement: of knowledge and critical thinking) but also con-
self-regulated learning and self-determination. ative (willful or purposeful striving) and affective
components. Since 1983, conceptions of student
engagement have broadened over time, moving
from a focus on the individual to a focus on social
processes that support or impede engagement and
1
Studies of classroom goal structures have been heavily self-regulated learning. Research has also grown
critiqued on methodological and theoretical grounds.
more sophisticated to allow for the interacting
For example, Miller and Murdock (2007) cautioned that
aggregating students’ perceptions undermined the validity influences of in- and out-of-school contexts while
of any classroom-based measure. Similarly, they argued examining both short- and long-time impacts on
that personal goals and classroom goal structures should academic engagement and outcomes (Boekaerts,
not be viewed as orthogonal contributors to individual stu-
Pintrich, & Zeidner, 2000; Corno & Mandinach,
dent effort. Using linear modeling techniques, they argued
that when entered at level 2 instead of level 1, the effects 1983, 2004; Csikszentmihalyi, Abuhamdeh, &
of the classroom mastery goal structure are much smaller, Nakamura, 2005).
suggesting that these are mediated by their effects on Most models of self-regulation assume that
students’ personal goals. Loosely translated, this suggests
optimization involves students’ active participa-
that a mastery goal orientation may be a product of both
personality and classroom structures (Murdock & Miller, tion in and commitment to the learning activity.
2009). Boekaerts and Corno (2005) note,
466 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

All theorists assume that students who self-regulate need for secure and satisfying connections with
their learning are engaged actively and construc- others; and autonomy is the ability to regulate
tively in a process of meaning generation and that
they adapt their thoughts, feelings, and actions as one’s own actions and behaviors (Deci et al.).
needed to affect their learning and motivation. Needs are central to human behavior, and, there-
Similarly, models assume that biological, develop- fore, opportunities to satisfy these needs contrib-
mental, contextual, and individual difference con- ute to the likelihood to be motivated.
straints may all interfere with or support efforts at
regulation (p. 201).
Formative Assessment Links
In spite of these common assumptions, differ- to Engagement Processes
ent theoretical models emphasize different com- Formative assessment practices are ideally suited
ponents. For example, Corno (2001) considers for enhancing (or impeding) engagement-related
the role of volition (or will), Winne (1995) empha- processes. Whereas summative tests are one-time
sizes the cognitive aspects of self-regulation, and experiences that provide specific data about stu-
McCaslin (2009) looks at sociocultural processes. dent performance, formative assessments are
Others are beginning to advocate a view of given over time and are used to inform the learn-
engagement as an inherently developmental pro- ing and teaching process. Importantly, summa-
cess related to identity that unfolds and emerges tive tests are designed and controlled by the
over time, through academic-related interactions teacher, whereas formative assessments require
in the classroom (Kaplan & Flum, 2009). Common active participation of students. According to
to all of these approaches to self-regulation is the Black and Wiliam (1998, p. 11),
notion of student will, volition, or “buy-in.”
The core of the activity of formative assessment
lies in the sequence of two actions. The first is the
Self-Determination perception by the learner of a gap between a
Another form of student engagement comes from desired goal and his or her present state (of knowl-
self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1995). A edge, and/or understanding, and/or skills). The
second is the action taken by the learner to close
distinguishing characteristic of self-determination that gap in order to attain the desired goal.
theory (SDT) is the extent to which an individual
is acting of their own volition or whether their Teachers and students play equally important
intent is being controlled to some extent (Deci, roles in actualizing this sequence of events.
Vallerand, Pelletier, & Ryan, 1991). The individ- According to Black and Wiliam (1998), for the
ual acting “wholly volitionally” is self-deter- first event to occur,
mined, meaning the person perceives the locus of The prime responsibility for generating the infor-
causality to be internal. The individual acting mation may lie with the student in self-assessment,
without volition perceives the locus of causality or with another person, notably the teacher, who
discerns and interprets the gap and communicates a
to be external to them. These differences are sig- message about it to the student. Whatever the pro-
nificant and possess different components of reg- cedures by which the assessment message is gener-
ulation; that is, the self-determined individual ated, in relation to action taken by the learner it
possesses a regulatory process of choice, while would be a mistake to regard the student as the pas-
sive recipient of a call to action. There are complex
the controlled individual possesses a regulatory links between the way the message is received, the
process that is compliant (Deci et al., 1991). way in which that perception motivates a selection
SDT incorporates needs-based aspects of amongst different courses of action, and the learn-
human life in its framework. There are three ing activity which may or may not follow.
innate psychological needs each human pos- Formative assessment practices, therefore,
sesses, according to the theory (Deci et al., 1991). become a venue through which engagement-
They are the need for competence, the need for related beliefs and actions emerge and are
relatedness, and the need for autonomy or self- sustained.
determination. Competence is an individual’s Studies suggest that formative assessment
need for attainment of various outcomes and effi- approaches, which inherently emphasize engage-
cacy for doing so; relatedness is an individual’s ment-related processes such as self-reflection
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 467

and self-determination, are more effective than tests took up little time and had only minimal
summative assessment practices in promoting impact on teachers and students (Sacks, 1999).
persistence and academic achievement. For exam- The advent of high-stakes testing accountability,
ple, Frederiksen and White (1997) compared however, has radically changed this educational
achievement outcomes among middle school sci- landscape such that teachers rely more heavily on
ence students, half of whom were exposed to self- standardized test results than ever before and in
reflective assessment activities and half who were ways that are largely detrimental to instructional
not. Their results demonstrate that students who practice (Herman & Haertel, 2005; Nichols &
participate in self-reflective assessment practices Berliner, 2007).
understand curriculum better than those not The theory of action undergirding the practice
prompted to self-reflect. Similarly, formative of high-stakes testing is that when faced with
assessment practices provide students with greater large incentives and threatening punishments,
opportunities to demonstrate autonomy and choice teachers will be more effective, students will be
through feedback processes that are more infor- more motivated, and parents will become more
mational than controlling, thus enhancing engage- involved (e.g., Amrein & Berliner, 2002;
ment-related action and beliefs (Deci & Ryan, McDonnell, 2005; Raymond & Hanushek, 2003).
1995; Grolnick & Ryan, 1987). In general, efforts In short, it is believed that the pressure of doing
made to draw students’ attention to their own well on a test will spur everyone into action, thus
learning progress through activities such as self- improving American public schools significantly
reflection and self-evaluation enhance learning (Peterson & West, 2003; Phelps, 2005). Yet,
and achievement outcomes (Black & Wiliam, 7 years after NCLB was enacted, there is no con-
1998; Thomas, 1993). vincing evidence that student learning has
increased in any significant way (e.g., Nichols,
2007). By contrast, there is a wealth of documen-
The Popularization of High-Stakes tation that this carrot-and-stick approach to school
Standardized Tests reform has resulted in educationally deleterious
side effects (e.g., Jones & Egley, 2004; Jones,
High-stakes standardized testing has increasingly Jones, & Hargrove, 2003; Orfield & Kornhaber,
assumed a more dominant role in American class- 2001; Pedulla et al., 2003; Valenzuela, 2005).
rooms (Giordano, 2005; Herman & Haertel, 2005; These side effects and the pursuant collateral
Stiggins, 2001). Part of its rise in popularity has damage to student motivation and engagement
been due to rapid advances in technology (allow- are the focus of the next section in the chapter.
ing for easier distribution and scoring) and psy-
chometric techniques. But another influence has
been political—attaching stakes to standardized High-Stakes Tests and Student
test performance (i.e., high-stakes testing) has Motivation: Some Correlational Data
become a convenient policy position for the advo-
cacy of evaluating, monitoring, and judging the Students and teachers are immersed in high-
perceived inadequacies of a public school system stakes tests. Data reveal that in addition to taking
(Berliner & Biddle, 1995; Cuban, 1988; Good, standardized tests, teachers spend large amounts
1996; Herman & Haertel, 2005; McDonnell, of time preparing students for tests (Nichols &
2005; NCEE, 1983; Tyack & Cuban, 1997). The Berliner, 2007). For example, survey research
passage of NCLB in 2002 mandated use of high- found that 80% of elementary teachers in North
stakes testing as a way to hold teachers, students, Carolina reported that they spent more than 20%
and schools accountable for learning. of their total teaching time practicing for high-
Since the passage of the No Child Left Behind stakes tests (Jones et al., 1999). This is about the
Act (NCLB), students are immersed in testing equivalent of 36 days of test preparation. Further,
and test-related activities at unprecedented levels. the survey found that 28% of those teachers
Prior to NCLB, data suggested that standardized reported spending more than 60% of their time
468 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

practicing for the state’s tests—amounting to 2005). Further, there is a wealth of data to show
over 100 of the typical 180 days of instruction that students’ sense of self-competency also
spent in various forms of test preparation. declines over time (Anderman & Maehr, 1994;
In spite of the overwhelming amount of time Eccles et al., 1989; Wigfield, Eccles, MacIver,
teachers and students spend engaged with high- Reuman, & Midgley, 1991; Wigfield & Wagner,
stakes standardized tests, there is shockingly lit- 2005). Thus, growing “disillusionment” with tests
tle data on the impact it has (or may have) on could simply be a by-product of a naturally occur-
student motivation. One exception is a set of cor- ring developmental trend whereby students gener-
relation studies from the early 1990s. Paris, ally grow disenchanted with school. On the other
Turner, and Lawton (1990, as cited in Paris, hand, it could be that there is an accumulation
Lawton, Turner, & Roth, 1991) surveyed nearly effect brought upon by excessive test-taking expe-
1,000 students in states then practicing high- riences. Data suggest that when it comes to test-
stakes testing (Florida, Michigan, California, and related pressures, there emerges a “cumulative,
Arizona) at the time. This cross-sectional corre- negative impact on students that can be summa-
lational analysis of students’ views of tests rized in three general trends: growing disillusion-
revealed a pattern in which younger students ment about tests, decreasing motivation to give
viewed these standardized tests as useful and genuine effort, and increasing use of inappropriate
valid representations of what they know. By con- [test-taking] strategies” (Paris et al., 1991, p. 14).
trast, older students demonstrated “disillusion-
ment” in their perceptions in that they felt tests
were not valuable or valid and that the growing High-Stakes Tests: Exaggerated
preoccupation with tests seemed to undermine Emphasis of Tests
their views of their teachers.
A decade later, Paris and colleagues followed Survey and anecdotal data suggest that as the
up with another set of longitudinal analyses of pressure to perform well on the test rises, teachers
students’ perceptions of the value of high-stakes and administrators increasingly emphasize its
testing (e.g., Paris, Roth, & Turner, 2000; Wong importance to students (Abrams, Pedulla, &
& Paris, 2000) as well as the effect of tests on Madaus, 2003). One way this has emerged is in
teachers and schools (Paris & Urdan, 2000). Both the surge in test-day pep rallies. For example, in
cross-sectional and longitudinal survey data one NY school, the principal holds pep rallies
revealed that students generally view high-stakes every spring where students come together and
tests negatively and that there are developmental sing “inspiring” songs such as “I’ve been working
differences between sixth and eighth graders in on my writing,” and “I’m a believer” (Toy, 2006).
their views. Specifically, older students tend to Elsewhere in Texas, school walls are adorned
hold more negative views of tests and testing, with “inspirational” guidance to students to “Beat
and report increasing cynicism about achieve- the TAKS” (Texas Assessment of Knowledge and
ment tests and increasing worries about teachers Skills) and students meet every spring for a rally
devaluing them as a result of their test perfor- complete with a full dinner and cheerleaders lead-
mance (Paris et al., 2000). By contrast, fifth grad- ing everyone in chants such as “let’s beat the
ers held more positive views of testing (saw tests TAKS” and “Use our skills!” (Foster, 2006 as
as more valuable and useful and felt more posi- reported in Nichols & Berliner, 2007). The advent
tively about them) than their eighth-grade peers. of high-stakes testing has influenced schools to
At issue, then, is what explains these develop- rally students in ways that exaggerate the impor-
mental trends, and there are two probable explana- tance of the test in students’ lives.
tions. On the one hand, a rich literature already High-stakes tests become overexaggerated
informs us that students’ motivation generally when they change what is taught in schools. Data
declines as they move from elementary into mid- reveal that high-stakes testing systems are
dle and high school (e.g., Eccles & Midgley, 1989; increasingly narrowing the curriculum; content
Wigfield & Guthrie, 1997; Wigfield & Wagner, that will appear on the test is emphasized while
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 469

content that is not tested is eliminated from the school is no longer a forum where students can
curriculum. We know, for example, that the arts discuss the effects of alcohol, or the best method to
achieve a life filled with value and pleasure, or the
(music and art) as well as physical education are simple antics of their daily life. Instead, learning
being cut at high rates (Nichols & Berliner, 2007; institutions have become places where multiple-
Vasquez Heilig, Cole, & Aguilar, 2010). One choice tests, quadratic equations, and the size of a
study revealed that 71% of districts surveyed had cell are deemed as significant pieces of knowledge
which students are required to know in order to
reduced instructional time in at least one other survive.
subject to make more time for reading and math-
ematics, the subjects tested under NCLB (Center Or, as another student stated,
on Education Policy [CEP], 2006). In some dis- I know what it’s like to be tested over and over and
tricts, struggling students received double peri- over and over again from the time you’re in third
ods of reading, math, or both. Data suggested that grade until you’re a junior in high school. I know
what it’s like having to go over multiple-choice
the narrowed curriculum disproportionately questions for two weeks straight right before
impacted poor and minority students. According TAKS. I know how incredibly boring it is to sit in
to the CEP report, 97% of high-poverty districts a classroom with 29 other people and wait for the
(where more than 75% of students are eligible for rest of the school to finish the test.
free or reduced price lunch) compared with Elsewhere, nefarious retention practices were
55–59% of lower-poverty districts had policies revealed when administrators described practices
that restricted curriculum offerings (CEP, 2006). that seemingly pushed low-achieving students
Teachers have confirmed this trend (Taylor, out of school (Vasquez Heilig & Darling-
Shepard, Kinner, & Rosenthal, 2003). One Hammond, 2008):
teacher from Colorado noted, “I eliminated a lot
I think that the kids are being forced out of school.
of my social studies and science. I eliminated I had a kid who came here from [school name] and
Colorado History. What else? Electricity. Most of said, “Miss, I if I come here, could I ever take the
that because it’s more stressed that the kids know [exit exam]?” and I said, “What do you mean? If
the reading and the math, so, it was pretty much you come here, you must take the [exam].” And he
said, “Well, every time I think I’m going to take
said, you know, you do what you gotta do.” the test, they either say, ‘You don’t have to come to
Another teacher reiterated the point, “[I] elimi- school tomorrow or you don’t have to [take the
nated curriculum such as novels I would teach, test]’…we’re told different things.” (p. 99)
we didn’t have time to go to the library, we didn’t
Students understand not only how important
have time to use the computer labs because they
the test is but also how valuable they are (or are
had to cut something. [I] Cut things I thought we
not) to the school simply on the basis of how they
could live without. [I] Cut presentations, anything
score on the test. These implicit and explicit mes-
that takes very much time, I cut film. We have
sages about tests and student value have potential
been cutting like crazy.”
to seriously erode students’ motivation over time
It is unclear how the overexaggeration of tests
(Nichols & Berliner, 2007; Perlstein, 2007).
conveyed by test pep rallies, curriculum narrow-
ing, and other forms of instructional manipula-
tion in the current high-stakes testing climate
impacts student motivation. But anecdotal data The Dropout Crisis
from students reveals something ominous. In
Texas, for example, where the stakes are rela- There is perhaps no greater evidence of lowered
tively high compared to most other states motivation for school than when a student drops
(Nichols, Glass, & Berliner, 2006), students are out of school. Tragically, data suggest that the
jaded. One high school junior notes the following pressures associated with high-stakes testing are
(San Antonio Express-News, 2007): related to students’ decisions to leave school.
Perhaps the most revealing trend on the impact of
In Texas, many public school districts have found
raising their standardized testing averages to be the high-stakes testing and poor, minority youth
No. 1 goal of classroom curriculum. Consequently, emerges in dropout/graduation rate data. Data are
470 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

relatively clear that when students have to pass a These effects also impact high-achieving stu-
test to receive a diploma, poor and minority stu- dents. McNeil, Coppola, Radigan, and Vasquez
dents are at greater risk of dropping out of school Heilig (2008) found that African-American honor
all together (e.g., Orfield, 2004; Orfield, Losen, students noticed significant shifts in the curriculum
Wald, & Swanson, 2004; Warren, Jenkins, & and in the ways in which their teachers interacted
Kulick, 2006). Graduation rate data demonstrate a with them relating to the test. One student shared,
similar trend where poor students and those who
Instead of teaching us the real life things that we
are African-American, Latino/a, ELL, and/or who are going to need for college and stuff, they started
have disabilities graduate at a much lower rate than zeroing in just on that test. So it makes everybody
their white, more advantaged peers (Gayler, nervous, and it threw everybody off. So, like, our
Chudowsky, Hamilton, Kober & Yeager, 2004). curriculum is thrown off, ‘cause what they origi-
nally were teaching us in the subjects, all of the
This connection demonstrates that for certain sudden they switched, and then they were just
groups of students, testing pressures not only zeroing into this test (McNeil et al., 2008, p. 28).
diminish their motivation but undermine it com-
pletely. Thus, for many students, but especially for Another African-American honors senior
poor minority students, the repeated failures on a added this, “Some teachers are so scared and
test that might be their last obstacle to obtaining a don’t know what to expect on the test, that they
high school diploma may directly undermine their zero in on that test and it bugs us just hearing
motivation to stay in school and to keep trying. about this test” (p. 29). Students of all achieve-
There is also growing data on troubling prac- ment levels notice how their teachers respond to
tices emerging in high-stakes testing contexts the test. It seems that for these students, who oth-
which indirectly impact poor, minority students’ erwise are extremely motivated, teachers’ preoc-
motivation. For example, Vasquez Heilig and cupation with tests present missed opportunities
Darling-Hammond (2008) found that a dispro- for authentic learning experiences.
portionate number of ninth-grade minority stu- High-stakes testing pressures are also con-
dents were held back in order to avoid their test nected with lowered graduation rates (another
participation during an accountability year. One indicator of dropout trends). Jacob (2001) for
student shared her perspective, “I have a friend example, looked at dropout data as well as
that was in ninth grade for 2 years, and she was twelfth-grade achievement data in reading and
19 or 20 years old. She did not pass algebra, and math as reported on the National Educational
the school told her that if she didn’t improve her Longitudinal Survey (NELS) in states with and
grades, they were going to drop her since she was without high school graduation exams. After
older. So she…dropped out of school” (p. 98). accounting for prior achievement and other back-
Haney’s research in Florida revealed that ground characteristics (e.g., SES, ethnicity),
increases in students’ test performance were not Jacob found no significant differences in stu-
the result of learning gains, but the result of dents’ achievement between states with manda-
manipulation. He found that, “Florida started tory testing and those without. However, students,
flunking more students—including dispropor- especially lower-ability students, were more
tionately high numbers of minority students— likely to drop out when faced with mandatory
and requiring them to repeat grade three” (Haney, graduation exams than students in states without
2008, p. 94). Over and over, evidence suggests such an exam. Marchant and Paulson (2005)
that in response to high-stakes testing pressures, looked at the effect of high school graduation
efforts are made to game the system (retain/flunk exams on state-level graduation rates, aggregated
low-scoring students, manipulate data, lower per- SAT scores, and individual student SAT scores.
formance standards) with these effects dispropor- By comparing graduation rates and SAT scores in
tionately impacting poor, minority students (see states with a graduation exam against states with-
also Darling-Hammond, 2007; Madaus, Russell out a graduation exam, they also found that states
& Higgins, 2009; Nichols & Berliner, 2007; with graduation exams had lower graduation rates
Ryan, 2004; Valenzuela, 2005). and lower aggregate SAT scores. These findings
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 471

have been replicated in other studies (e.g., Nichols any substantive references to standardized test
et al., 2006; Orfield et al., 2004), with the caution practices and student motivation.
that the data are correlational and the measures of Next, we reviewed some of what we know
graduation vary (Mishel & Roy, 2006). about how classroom-based assessment practices
High-stakes standardized testing practices now (teacher-made tests, summative and formative
pervade American classrooms. The resultant pres- uses) might connect with motivation and engage-
sure on teachers and students to pass the test “or ment. Test content and feedback systems associ-
else” has yielded noticeable shifts in teacher prac- ated with summative testing practices have strong
tice that theoretically may erode student motiva- associations with effort and student and class-
tion and engagement. For example, the exaggerated room-level goal orientations/structure. Classroom
importance of this test in students’ lives draws goal structures and feedback systems that empha-
attention not only to how students perform but size performance comparisons are largely detri-
also to how they perform in relation to their peers. mental to student motivation, whereas emphasis
Feedback systems that encourage performance on learning goals enhances motivation and
comparisons are associated with maladaptive achievement.
motivational orientations. Further, the summa- We also reviewed formative assessment prac-
tive nature of high-stakes testing precludes forma- tices and discussed their links with student
tive assessment relationships between teachers engagement. We view engagement in terms of
and students. The pressure to perform on a single self-regulation and self-determination theories,
test undermines teachers’ capacities to encourage both of which presume students’ active involve-
and promote engagement-related processes such ment in and reflection on their own learning. In
as self-reflection and self-direction. contrast to summative assessment, formative
assessment practices are well suited for enhanc-
ing students’ engagement-related behaviors.
Conclusion Using tests as feedback systems from which stu-
dents can learn to identify their own strengths
In this chapter, we have attempted to draw links and weaknesses, as well as their own learning-
between assessment-related practices and student related successes and failures, is ideal for pro-
motivation and engagement. The complexity of moting student ownership, responsibility, and,
the terrain in both the assessment and motivational ultimately, student achievement. Lastly, we
literature makes it exceedingly difficult to emerge reviewed some of the classroom conditions asso-
with meaningful conclusions. Still, there have ciated with high-stakes standardized testing prac-
been some data to provide us with a glimpse of the tices. We discussed data showing how high-stakes
ways in which teachers’ use of tests may coincide testing is changing educational practice and the
with and/or influence students’ motivation. ways in which these changes impact student
We organized our review by test type, focus- motivation—largely for the worse.
ing first on what we have learned about the role
and use of standardized tests in American class-
rooms. Although the literature on low-stakes Implications
standardized testing systems in America is vast
(Sacks, 1999), the most pertinent data for our High-stakes testing systems have created a very
topic comes from how standardized test scores specific type of learning context. One worry
influence teachers expectation effects (Good & about their effects on student motivation has to
Brophy, 1974). A rich literature on teacher expec- do with the onslaught of messages regarding abil-
tations informs us that norm-referenced standard- ity. Students who repeatedly fail or struggle are
ized aptitude tests may influence how teachers cast as problem students and treated differently.
treat students, subsequently influencing student Ironically, educators worried 100 years ago about
motivation. Importantly, these links are loosely the cumulative effect of repeated test failures. In
understood at best and, therefore, cannot make 1913, Dougherty (1913) cautioned teachers
472 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

“when a child fails continually on test after test if ers communicate to students? How do students
he is conscious of his own failure, as he must interpret these messages? Is there a relationship
sometimes be in spite of the examiner’s efforts to between the magnitude of the pressure and the
encourage him, it is not wise to proceed until he type of feedback teachers provide to students?
merely fails from habit” (p. 339). Thus, educators In what ways do teacher-student relationships
have long worried about the cumulative effects of change as a function of test-related pressure?
chronic failure on summative tests. We also need to better understand the effect of
We believe the pervasive use of high-stakes the testing culture on student achievement,
standardized testing in American schools including literacy, deeper-level cognition and
(Giordano, 2005) is deleterious to student motiva- reasoning, strategy use, or complex problem
tion and engagement. When students are told over solving. Research is beginning to look at these
and over that learning is only as good as their test questions, but there has been little empirical work
scores (Boohrer-Jennings, 2005; Jones et al., to connect these important skills to policy devel-
1999, 2003; Nichols & Berliner, 2007; Perlstein, opment and implementation. As the United States
2007), it seems likely that their beliefs about the and other countries grapple with ways to reform
value of learning become compromised. When education, and as tests increasingly grow in pop-
students learn for extrinsic reasons, their motiva- ularity, it seems more imperative than ever for
tion is likely to diminish (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, researchers to examine the potential motivational
1999; Deci, Ryan, & Koestner, 2001). And the and learning costs associated with the continu-
costs manifest in a multitude of ways. Struggling ance of high-stakes testing systems.
students might become less equipped to handle
failure and frustration. Talented students might
become bored and therefore fail to notice oppor- References
tunities that might otherwise provide new ways of
Abrams, L., Pedulla, J. J., & Madaus, G. (2003).
learning (Carver & Scheier, 2005). Similarly, Views from the classroom: Teachers’ opinions of
high-achieving students, noticing teachers’ preoc- statewide testing programs. Theory into Practice,
cupations with tests, may lose motivation to per- 42(1), 18–28.
sist. After all, if it is only about tests and testing Allington, R., & McGill-Franzen, A. (1992). Unintended
effects of educational reform in New York. Educational
(and if tests are easy for me), then why try? Policy, 6(4), 397–414.
Ames, C. (1984). Competitive, cooperative, and individu-
alistic goal structures: A cognitive-motivational analy-
Needed Research for Practice sis. In R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on
motivation in education: Vol. I. Student motivation.
and Policy
New York: Academic.
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and stu-
We need more research to understand how assess- dent motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology,
ment-related instructional practice relates to stu- 84(3), 261–271.
dent motivation (Stiggins, 2001). But we especially Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the
classroom: Students’ learning strategies and motiva-
need data on how high-stakes testing systems tion processes. Journal of Educational Psychology,
influence teachers and in turn, how that influence 80(3), 260–267.
impacts students. Our review of extant data casts a Amrein, A., & Berliner, D. (2002). High-stakes testing,
looming shadow over the use of high-stakes test- uncertainty, and student learning. Educational Policy
and Analysis Archives, 10(8). Retrieved from http://
ing and its largely deleterious influence on student
www.epaa.asu.edu/epaa/v10n18
motivation. However, more research is needed to Anderman, E. M., & Maehr, M. L. (1994). Motivation and
more clearly identify the particular assessment- schooling in the middle grades. Review of Educational
related practices that enhance or impede students’ Research, 64(2), 287–309.
developing motivation and engagement. For Anderman, E. (2007). The effects of personal, classroom,
and school goal structures on academic cheating
example, what role does test-related pressure play (pp. 87–106). In E. M. Anderman & T. B. Murdock
in the types of motivation-related messages teach- (Eds.), Psychology of academic cheating. NY: Elsevier.
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 473

Atkinson, J. W. (1974). Motivational determinants of Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2005). Engagement,
intellective performance and cumulative achievement. disengagement, coping, and catastrophe. In A. J. Elliot
In J. W. Atkinson & J. O. Raynor (Eds.), Motivation & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and
and achievement. Washington, DC: V. H. Winston. motivation (pp. 527–547). New York: Guilford Press.
Atkinson, J. W. (1981a). Studying personality in the con- Center on Education Policy. (2006). From the capital to
text of an advanced motivational psychology. the classroom: Year four of the No Child Left Behind
American Psychologist, 36(2), 117–128. Act. Washington DC: Author. http://www.cep-dc.org/
Atkinson, J. W. (1981b). Thematic apperceptive measure- nclb/Year4/CEP-NCLB-Report-4.pdf
ment of motivation in 1950 and 1980. In G. D’Ydewalle Corno, L. (2001). Volitional aspects of self-regulated
& W. Lens (Eds.), Cognition in human motivation and learning. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.),
learning (pp. 159–198). Hillsdale, NH: Erlbaum. Self-regulated learning and academic achievement:
Bangert-Drowns, R. L., Kulik, J. A., & Morgan, M. T. Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 191–226).
(1991). The instructional effect of feedback in test-like Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
events. Review of Educational Research, 61, 213–238. Corno, L., & Mandinach, E. B. (1983). The role of cogni-
Berliner, D. C. (1979). Tempus educare. In P. L. Peterson tive engagement in classroom learning and motiva-
& H. J. Walberg (Eds.), Research on teaching: tion. Educational Psychologist, 18(2), 88–108.
Concepts, findings, and implications (pp. 120–135). Corno, L., & Mandinach, E. B. (2004). What we have
Berkeley, CA: McCutchan Publishing. learned about student engagement in the past twenty
Berliner, D. C., & Biddle, B. J. (1995). The manufactured years. In D. M. McInerney & S. V. Etten (Eds.), Big
crisis: Myths, fraud, and the attack on America’s pub- theories revisited (pp. 299–328). Greenwich, CT:
lic schools. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Information Age Publishing.
Black, P., & Wiliam, D. (1998). Assessment and class- Crooks, T. J. (1988). The impact of classroom evaluation
room learning. Assessment in Education: Principles, practices on students. Review of Educational Research,
Policy and Practice, 5(1), 7–74. 58, 438–481.
Black, P., & Wiliam, D. (2006). Developing a theory of Crooks, T. J., & Mahalski, P. A. (1986). Relationships
formative assessment. In J. Gardner (Ed.), Assessment among assessment practices, study methods, and
and learning (pp. 81–100). London: Sage. grades obtained. In J. Jones & M. Horsburgh (Eds.),
Bloom, B. S. (Ed.). (1956). A taxonomy of educational Research and development in higher education (Vol.
objectives: Handbook I, the cognitive domain. New 8). Sydney, Australia: Higher Education Research and
York: Longman. Development Society of Australasia.
Blumenfeld, P. C. (1992). Classroom learning and motiva- Csikszentmihalyi, M., Abuhamdeh, S., & Nakamura, J.
tion: Clarifying and expanding goal theory. Journal of (2005). Flow. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.),
Educational Psychology, 84(3), 272–281. Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 598–608).
Boekaerts, M., & Corno, L. (2005). Self-regulation in the New York: Guilford Press.
classroom: A perspective on assessment and interven- Cuban, L. (1988). Constancy and change in schools
tion. Applied Psychology: An International Review, (1880s to the present). In P. W. Jackson (Ed.),
54(2), 199–231. Contributing to educational change: Perspectives on
Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R., & Zeidner, M. (Eds.). research and practice. Berkeley, CA: McCutchan
(2000). Handbook of self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Publishing.
Academic. Darling-Hammond, L. (2007). Race, inequality, and edu-
Boohrer-Jennings, J. (2005). Below the bubble: cational accountability: The irony of ‘No Child Left
‘Educational triage’ and the Texas accountability sys- Behind’. Race Ethnicity and Education, 10(3),
tem. American Educational Research Journal, 42(2), 245–260.
231–268. Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-
Brookhart, S. M. (1997). A theoretical framework for the analytic review of experiments examining the effects
role of classroom assessment in motivating student of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation.
effort and achievement. Applied Measurement in Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.
Education, 10, 161–180. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The
Brookhart, S. M. (2004). Classroom assessment: Tensions basis for true self-esteem. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy,
and intersections in theory and practice. Teachers agency, and self-esteem (pp. 31–49). New York:
College Record, 106(3), 429–458. Plenum Publishing Co.
Brophy, J. (Ed.). (1998). Advances in research on teach- Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., & Koestner, R. (2001). The per-
ing. Expectations in the classroom (Vol. 7, pp. 273– vasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic motiva-
308). Greenwich, CT: JAI. tion: Response to Cameron (2001). Review of
Butler, R. (1988). Enhancing and undermining intrin- Educational Research, 71(1), 43–51.
sic motivation: The effects of task-involving and Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan,
ego-involving evaluation on interest and perfor- R. M. (1991). Motivation and education: The self-
mance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, determination perspective. Educational Psychologist,
1–14. 26(3&4), 325–346.
474 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

Dougherty, M. L. (1913). Report on the Binet-Simon tests making: 106th yearbook of the National Society for
given to four hundred and eighty-three children in the the Study of Education, Part I (pp. 15–45). Malden,
public schools of Kansas City, Kansas. Journal of MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Educational Psychology, 4, 338–352. Gayler, K., Chudowsky, N., Hamilton, M., Kober, N., &
Eccles, J. S. (1983). Expectancies, values, and academic Yeager, M. (2004). State high school exit exams:
behavior. In J. T. Spencer (Ed.), Achievement and A maturing reform. Washington, DC: Center on
achievement motivation (pp. 75–146). San Francisco: Education Policy. Retrieved June 5, 2007, from http://
W. H. Freeman. www.cep-dc.org/highschoolexit/ExitExamAug2004/
Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage/environment fit: ExitExam2004.pdf
Developmentally appropriate classrooms for early Giordano, G. (2005). How testing came to dominate
adolescents. In R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research American schools: The history of educational assess-
on motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 139–186). New ment. New York: Peter Lang.
York: Academic. Glass, G. V. (2008). Fertilizers, pills, and magnetic strips:
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational belief, The fate of public education in America. Charlotte,
values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, NC: Information Age Publishing.
109–132. Good, T. L. (1981). Teacher expectations and student per-
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Flanagan, G., Miller, G., ceptions: A decade of research. Educational
Reuman, D., & Yee, D. (1989). Self-concepts, domain Leadership, 38, 415–423.
values, and self-esteem: Relations and changes at early Good, T. L. (1987). Two decades of research on teacher
adolescence. Journal of Personality, 57, 283–310. expectations: Findings and future directions. Journal
Elliot, A. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and of Teacher Education, 38(4), 32–47.
achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34(3), Good, T. L. (1996). Educational researchers comment on
169–189. the educational summit and other policy proclama-
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical tions from 1983–1997. Educational Researcher, 25(8),
model of approach and avoidance achievement moti- 4–6.
vation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (1972). Behavioral expres-
72, 218–232. sion of teacher attitudes. Journal of Educational
Elliot, A. J., & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of Psychology, 63, 617–624.
competence and motivation. New York: Guilford Press. Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (1974). Changing teacher and
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and student behavior: An empirical investigation. Journal
avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: of Educational Psychology, 66, 390–405.
A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (2008). Looking in class-
Social Psychology, 70(3), 461–475. rooms (10th ed.). Boston: Pearson.
Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach Good, T. L., & Thompson, E. K. (1998). Research on the
to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality communication of performance expectations: A review
and Social Psychology, 54, 5–12. of recent perspectives. In J. Brophy (Ed.), Advances in
Entwistle, N. J., & Kozeski, B. (1985). Relationship research on teaching: Expectations in the classroom
between school motivation, approaches to studying, (Vol. 7, pp. 273–308). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
and attainment, among British and Hungarian adoles- Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in chil-
cents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, dren’s learning: An experimental and individual dif-
124–137. ferent investigation. Journal of Personality and Social
Feather, N. T. (Ed.). (1982). Expectations and actions: Psychology, 52, 890–898.
Expectancy-value models in psychology. Hillsdale, Haney, W. (2008). Evidence on education under NCLB
NJ: Erlbaum. (and How Florida boosted NAEP scores and reduced
Foster, S. (2006). How Latino students negotiate the the race gap). In G. L. Sunderman (Ed.), Holding
demands of high-stakes testing: A case study of one NCLB accountable: Achieving accountability, equity,
school in Texas. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, and school reform (pp. 91–102). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ. Corwin Press.
Fredericksen, N. (1984). The real test bias: Influences of Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of
testing on teaching and learning. American praise on children’s intrinsic motivation: A review and
Psychologist, 39, 193–202. synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 774–795.
Frederiksen, J. R., & White, B. J. (1997). Reflective Herman, J. L., & Haertel, E. H. (Eds.). (2005). Uses and
assessment of students’ research within an inquiry- misuses of data for educational accountability and
based middle school science curriculum. Paper pre- improvement: The 104th yearbook of the National
sented at the annual meeting of the American Society for the Study of Education, part II. Malden,
Educational Research Association, Chicago. MA: Blackwell.
Gamson, D. (2007). Historical perspectives on democratic Hull, C. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton.
decision making in education: Paradigms, paradoxes, Hull, C. (1952). A behavior system. New Haven: Yale
and promises. In P. Moss (Ed.), Evidence and decision University Press.
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 475

Jacob, B. (2001). Getting tough? The impact of high Martin, E., & Ramsden, P. (1987). Learning skills and
school graduation exams. Educational Evaluation and skill in learning. In J. T. E. Richardson, M. W. Eysenck,
Policy Analysis, 23(2), 99–121. & D. W. Piper (Eds.), Student learning: Research in
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1985). Motivational education and cognitive psychology. Milton Keynes,
processes in cooperative, competitive, and individual- England: Open University Press & Society for
istic learning situation. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research into Higher Education.
Research on motivation in education: Vol. 2. The McCaslin, M. (2009). Co-regulation of student motivation
classroom milieu. New York: Academic. and emergent identity. Educational Psychologist,
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1999). Learning together 44(2), 137–146.
and alone: Cooperative, competitive, and individualis- McCaslin, M., & DiMarino-Linnen, E. (2000). Motivation
tic learning (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. and learning in school: Societal contexts, psychologi-
Jones, B. D., & Egley, R. J. (2004, August 9). Voices from cal constructs, and educational practices. In T. Good
the frontlines: Teachers’ perceptions of high-stakes (Ed.), Schooling in America: Yesterday, today, and
testing. Education Policy Analysis Archives, 12(39). tomorrow. 100th yearbook of the National Society for
Retrieved December 2, 2004, from http://epaa.asu. the Study of Education (pp. 84–151). Chicago:
edu/epaa/v12n39/ University of Chicago Press.
Jones, M. G., Jones, B. D., Hardin, B., Chapman, L., McClelland, D. C. (1980). Motive disposition: The merits
Yarbrough, T., & Davis, M. (1999). The impact of of operant and respondent measures. In L. Wheeler
high-stakes testing on teachers and students in North (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology
Carolina. The Phi Delta Kappan, 81(3), 199–203. (Vol. 1). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Jones, M. G., Jones, B. D., & Hargrove, T. (2003). The McClelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, and values
unintended consequences of high-stakes testing. determine what people do. American Psychologist,
Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. 40(7), 812–825.
Kaplan, A. (2004). Achievement goals and intergroup McDonnell, L. (2005). Assessment and accountability
relations. In P. R. Pintrich & M. L. Maehr (Eds.), from the policymaker’s perspective. In J. L. Herman
Advances in research on motivation and achievement: & E. H. Haertel (Eds.), Uses and misuses of data for
Vol. 13: Motivating students, improving schools: The educational accountability and improvement: The
legacy of Carol Midgley (pp. 97–136). Oxford, UK: 104th yearbook of the National Society for the Study of
Elsevier. Education, Part II. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Kaplan, A., & Flum, H. (Guest Eds.). (2009). Motivation McInerney, D. M., Brown, G. T. L., & Liem, G. A. D.
and identity [Special issue]. Educational Psychologist, (Eds.). (2009). Student perspectives on assessment:
44(2). What students can tell us about assessment for learn-
Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. L. (2007). The contributions and ing. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
prospects of goal orientation theory. Educational McMillan, J. H. (2001). Classroom assessment: Principles
Psychology Review, 19, 141–184. and practice for effective instruction (2nd ed.). Boston:
Kaplan, A., Middleton, M. J., Urdan, T., & Midgley, C. Allyn & Bacon.
(2002). Achievement goals and goal structures. In McNeil, L. M., Coppola, E., Radigan, J., & Vasquez
C. Midgley (Ed.), Goals, goal structures, and patterns Heilig, J. (2008). Avoidable losses: High-stakes
of adaptive learning (pp. 21–53). Mahwah, NJ: accountability and the dropout crisis. Education Policy
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Analysis Archives, 16(3). Retrieved March 16, 2009,
Kellaghan, T., Madaus, G. F., & Airasian, P. W. (1982). from http://epaa.asu.edu/epaa/v16n3/
The effects of standardized testing. Boston: Kluwer- Meichenbaum, D. H., & Smart, T. (1971). Use of direct
Nijhoff. expectancy to modify academic performance and atti-
Krathwohl, D. R. (2002). A revision of Bloom’s tudes of college students. Journal of Counseling
Taxonomy: An overview. Theory into Practice, 41(4), Psychology, 18, 531–535.
212–218. Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. J. (2001).
Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for
Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in whom, under what circumstances, and at what cost?
the classroom: Age differences and academic corre- Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77–86.
lates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(2), Miller, A. D., & Murdock, T. B. (2007). Modeling latent
184–196. true scores to determine the utility of aggregate stu-
Madaus, G., Russell, M., & Higgins, J. (2009). The para- dent perceptions as classroom indicators in HLM: The
doxes of high-stakes testing: How they affect students, case of classroom goal structures. Contemporary
their parents, teachers, principals, schools, and soci- Educational Psychology, 32, 83–104.
ety. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing. Miller, C. M. L., & Parlett, M. (1974). Up to the mark:
Marchant, G. J., & Paulson, S. E. (2005, January 21). The A study of the examination game. London: Society for
relationship of high school graduation exams to grad- Research into Higher Education.
uation rates and SAT scores. Education Policy Analysis Mishel, L., & Roy, J. (2006). Rethinking high school
Archives, 13(6). Retrieved June 30, 2006, from http:// graduation rates and trends. Washington, DC:
epaa.asu.edu/epaa/v13n6/ Economic Policy Institute.
476 S.L. Nichols and H.S. Dawson

Moss, P. (Ed.). (2007). Evidence and decision making. teachers. Boston: Boston College, National Board on
The 106th yearbook of the National Society for the Educational Testing and Public Policy. Retrieved
Study of Education, Part 1. Malden, MA: Blackwell January 7, 2004, from http://www.bc.edu/research/
Publishing. nbetpp/statements/nbr2.pdf
Murdock, T. B., & Miller, A. D. (2009). Specification Perlstein, L. (2007). Tested: One American school strug-
issues in the use of multilevel modeling to examine the gles to make the grade. New York: Henry Holt & Co.
effects of classroom context. In M. Wosnitza, S. A. Peterson, P. L., & Walberg, H. J. (Eds.). (1979). Research
Karabenick, A. Efklides, & P. Nenniger (Eds.), on teaching: Concepts, findings, and implications.
Contemporary motivation research: From global to Berkeley, CA: McCutchan Publishing.
local perspectives (pp. 249–264). Cambridge, MA: Peterson, P. E., & West, M. R. (Eds.). (2003). No Child
Hogrefe. Left Behind? The politics and practice of school
National Commission for Excellence in Education. accountability. Washington, DC: Brookings Institute.
(1983). A nation at risk: The imperatives for educa- Phelps, R. P. (Ed.). (2005). Defending standardized test-
tional reform. Washington, DC: Author. ing. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Natriello, G. (1987). The impact of evaluation processes on Raymond, M. E., & Hanushek, E. A. (2003, Summer).
students. Educational Psychologist, 22(2), 155–175. High-stakes research. Education Next, 3(3), 48–55.
Natriello, G., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1984). Teacher evalu- Retrieved from http://www.educationnext.org/
ative standards and student effort. New York: Rodriguez, M. C. (2004). The role of classroom assess-
Longman. ment in student performance on TIMSS. Applied
Nichols, S. L. (2007). High-stakes testing: Does it increase Measurement in Education, 17(1), 1–24.
achievement? Journal of Applied School Psychology, Rogers, E. M. (1969). Examinations: Powerful agents for
23(2), 47–64. good or ill in teaching. American Journal of Physics,
Nichols, S., & Berliner, D. C. (2007). Collateral damage: 37, 954–962.
How high-stakes testing corrupts America’s schools. Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the
Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press. classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Nichols, S. L., Glass, G. V., & Berliner, D. C. (2006). Ryan, J. (2004). The perverse incentives of the No Child
High-stakes testing and student achievement: Does Left Behind Act. New York University Law Review, 79,
accountability pressure increase student learning? 932–989.
Education Policy Analysis Archives, 14(1). Retrieved Sacks, P. (1999). Standardized minds: The high price of
July 20, 2009, from http://epaa.asu.edu/epaa/v14n1/ America’s testing culture and what we can do to
No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act of 2001, 20 U.S.C.A. change it. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Publishing.
§ 6301 et seq. (2002). San Antonio Express-News (2007, March 9). Teen talk:
Orfield, G. (Ed.). (2004). Dropouts in America: Tackling TAKS. San Antonio Express-News, p. 1F.
Confronting the graduation rate crisis. Cambridge, Schunk, D. H. (1996). Goal and self-evaluative influences
MA: Harvard Education Press. during children’s cognitive skill learning. American
Orfield, G., & Kornhaber, M. L. (Eds.). (2001). Raising Educational Research Journal, 33, 359–382.
standards or raising barriers? Inequality and high Snyder, B. R. (1971). The hidden curriculum. Cambridge,
stakes testing in public education. New York: The MA: M.I.T. Press.
Century Foundation Press. Starch, D., & Elliott, E. C. (1912). Reliability of the grad-
Orfield, G., Losen, D., Wald, D., & Swanson, C. (2004). ing of high-school work in English. School Review,
Losing our future: How minority youth are being left 20, 442–457.
behind by the graduation rate crisis. Cambridge, MA: Stiggins, R. J. (2001). The unfulfilled promise of class-
The Civil Rights Project at Harvard University. room assessment. Educational Measurement: Issues
Paris, S. G., Lawton, T. A., Turner, J. C., & Roth, J. L. (1991). and Practice, 20(3), 5–15.
A developmental perspective on standardized achieve- Stiggins, R. J., & Bridgeford, N. J. (1985). The ecology of
ment testing. Educational Researcher, 20, 12–20. classroom assessment. Journal of Educational
Paris, S. G., Roth, J. L., & Turner, J. C. (2000). Developing Measurement, 22(4), 271–286.
disillusionment: Students’ perceptions of academic Stiggins, R. J., & Conklin, N. F. (1992). In teachers’ hands:
achievement tests. Issues in Education, 6(1/2), 17–46. Investigating the practices of classroom assessment.
Paris, S. G., Turner, J. C., & Lawton, T. A. (1990, April). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Students’ views of standardized achievement tests. Taylor, G., Shepard, L., Kinner, F., & Rosenthal, J. (2003).
Paper presented at Educational Research Association, A survey of teachers’ perspectives on high-stakes test-
Boston. ing in Colorado: What gets taught, what gets lost
Paris, S. G., & Urdan, T. (2000). Policies and practices of (CSE Technical Report 588). Los Angeles: University
high-stakes testing that influence teachers and schools. of California.
Issues in Education, 6(1/2), 83–108. Thomas, J. W. (1993). Promoting independent learning in
Pedulla, J. J., Abrams, L. M., Madaus, G. F., Russell, M. the middle grades: The role of instructional support prac-
K., Ramos, M. A., & Miao, J. (2003, March). Perceived tices. The Elementary School Journal, 93, 575–591.
effects of state-mandated testing programs on teach- Thorndike, E. L. (1923). Education: A first book. New
ing and learning: Findings from a national survey of York: Macmillan.
22 Assessment as a Context for Student Engagement 477

Toy, V. (2006, January 1). Elmont’s school success is a Whipple, G. M. (1921). The national intelligence tests.
lesson to others. New York Times, p. 14L. The Journal of Educational Research, 4, 16–31.
Tyack, D. (1974). The one best system: A history of Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (1992). The development of
American urban education. Cambridge, UK: Harvard achievement task values: A theoretical analysis.
University Press. Developmental Review, 12, 265–310.
Tyack, D., & Cuban, L. (1997). Tinkering toward utopia: Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (2000). Expectancy x value
A century of public school reform. Cambridge, MA: theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary
Harvard University Press. Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81.
Urdan, T. (1997). Achievement goal theory: Past results, Wigfield, A., Eccles, J., MacIver, D., Reuman, D., &
future directions. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich Midgley, C. (1991). Transitions at early adolescence:
(Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. Changes in children’s domain-specific self-percep-
10, pp. 99–141). Greenwich, CT: JAI. tions and general self-esteem across the transition to
Valenzuela, A. (Ed.). (2005). Leaving children behind: junior high school. Developmental Psychology, 27,
How “Texas-style” accountability fails Latino youth. 552–565.
Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of
Vasquez Heilig, J., Cole, H., & Aguilar, A. (2010). From competence beliefs, expectancies for success, and
Dewey to No Child Left Behind: The evolution and achievement values from childhood through adoles-
devolution of public arts education. Arts Education cence. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development
Policy Review, 111, 136–145. of achievement motivation (pp. 91–120). San Diego,
Vasquez Heilig, J., & Darling-Hammond, L. (2008). CA: Academic.
Accountability Texas-style: The progress and learning Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations of chil-
of urban minority students in a high-stakes testing dren’s motivation for reading to the amount and
context. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, breadth of their reading. Journal of Educational
30(2), 75–110. Psychology, 89, 420–432.
Warren, J. R., Jenkins, K. N., & Kulick, R. B. (2006). Wigfield, A., & Wagner, A. L. (2005). Competence and
High school exit examinations and state-level comple- motivation during adolescence. In A. J. Elliott & C. S.
tion and GED Rates, 1975 through 2002. Educational Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motiva-
Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 28(20), 131–152. tion (pp. 222–239). New York: Guilford Press.
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation Winne, P. H. (1995). Inherent details in self-regulated
and emotion. New York: Springer. learning. Educational Psychologist, 30, 173–187.
Weinstein, R. S. (2002). Reaching higher: The power of Wong, C. A., & Paris, S. G. (2000). Students’ beliefs
expectations in schooling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard about classroom tests and standardized tests. Issues in
University Press. Education, 6(1/2), 47–6677.
Part III Commentary: Socio-Cultural
Contexts, Social Competence, 23
and Engagement at School

Kathryn Wentzel

Abstract
A highly regarded motivation researcher, Kathryn Wentzel, shared her
perspectives in a commentary on the chapters in Part III. Wentzel explored
questions relating to student competence including its definition, relation
to engagement, and the role of support from important contexts (home,
school, peers, and community) in fostering competence and engagement.
The chapter concludes with directions for future research.

The social contexts and interactions that define and why advances in learning and cognitive
children’s lives can have a profound effect on development might reflect aspects of social
their ability and willingness to engage in the aca- competence.
demic life of schools. As illustrated clearly and In support of this approach are traditional
convincingly in the chapters in this section of the developmental perspectives that recognize the
Handbook on Engagement, interpersonal rela- interdependent relations of cognitive and social
tionships, family and peer group dynamics, functioning in descriptions of intellectual devel-
instruction-based social interactions, and indica- opment (e.g., Piaget, 1983; Vygotsky, 1978).
tors of competence all determine to some extent More recently, scholars also have argued con-
how and why students strive to achieve academic vincingly that the ability to excel at tasks
success. Beyond this recognition that learning is designed to assess cognitive abilities is highly
embedded in social contexts, however, how might dependent on broad-level social influences that
a social-ecological approach help scholars and reflect cultural belief systems and practices (e.g.,
educators better understand children’s engage- Greenfield, 1997), as well as intra-individual dif-
ment at school? One strategy is to bring to the ferences in social and emotional skills and self-
forefront the notion that educational and intellec- regulation (e.g., Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki,
tual endeavors are inherently social in nature Taylor, & Schellinger, 2011). If scholars adopt
and in doing so, consider more explicitly how this ecological perspective, however, questions
arise as to what it means to be a socially compe-
tent student, how social competence supports
K. Wentzel (*) various forms of intellectual engagement, and
Department of Human Development and Quantitative
Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park,
how competence development can be supported
MD, USA across multiple contexts of home, school, peer
e-mail: wentzel@umd.edu group, and community.

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 479


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_23, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
480 K. Wentzel

In this commentary, I focus on three central of several theorists (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, 1989;
issues that reflect these questions. The first con- Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Ford, 1992). Bronfen-
cerns the notion that social competencies contrib- brenner (1989) argued that competence is a prod-
ute to successful academic and learning-related uct of personal attributes such as goals, values,
outcomes. Implicit in this notion are fundamen- self-regulatory skills, and cognitive abilities, and
tal questions concerning how to define social of ways in which these attributes contribute to
competence and understand it within the context meeting situational requirements and demands.
of schooling. Second, issues surrounding the Bronfenbrenner further suggested that compe-
socially-valued goals and objectives that are rele- tence is facilitated by contextual supports that
vant for understanding school adjustment are con- provide opportunities for the growth and devel-
sidered. Indeed, if social competence is an integral opment of these personal attributes, including
part of school success, how do we identify and communications concerning what is expected by
examine the socially-valued goals that we would the social group. Ford expanded on this notion by
like students to achieve? Finally, processes of specifying dimensions of social competence that
influence and theoretical issues related to social are framed within a model in which personal and
contexts and schooling are discussed. If socio- context-specific goals are coordinated to address
cultural contexts are important for students’ the needs of the individual as well as those of the
school-based competencies, how and why might social group.
this be so? I close with some general conclusions The application of these ecologically-based
and provocations for future research in this area. models of social competence to the realm of
schooling results in a multi-faceted description of
children who are engaged in the social and aca-
Defining Social Competence at School demic life of their school. First, competent students
are engaged in achieving goals that are personally
In the social developmental literature, social valued as well as those that are valued by others.
competence has been described from a variety of Second, the goals they pursue result in social
perspectives ranging from the development of integration as well as in positive developmental
individual skills to more general adaptation outcomes for the student. Socially-integrative out-
within a particular setting. In these discussions, comes are those which promote the smooth func-
social competence frequently is associated with tioning of social groups at school (e.g., cooperative
person-level outcomes such as effective behav- behavior) and are reflected most proximally in
ioral repertoires, social problem-solving skills, social acceptance and socially interdependent
positive beliefs about the self, achievement of actions; student-related outcomes reflect healthy
social goals, and positive interpersonal relation- development of the self (e.g., perceived social
ships (see Rose-Krasnor, 1997). In addition, competence, feelings of self-determination) and
however, central to many definitions of social feelings of emotional well-being (Bronfenbrenner,
competence is the notion that social contexts play 1989; Ford, 1992).
an integral role in providing opportunities for the The systemic nature of this approach high-
development of these outcomes as well as in lights the fact that a student’s school-based com-
defining the appropriate parameters of children’s petencies are a product of social reciprocity
social accomplishments (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, between themselves, their teachers and their
1989). In this view, social competence reflects a classmates. Just as students must behave in ways
more systemic phenomenon in which a balance is that meet the expectations of others, so must
achieved between the accomplishment of posi- teachers and peers provide support for the
tive outcomes for the individual and context-spe- achievement of a student’s multiple goals. In this
cific effectiveness. regard, the authors in this section reflect on the
Support for defining social competence as potential threats to children’s social, emotional,
person-environment fit can be found in the work and intellectual engagement when a balance
23 Part III Commentary: Socio-Cultural Contexts, Social Competence, and Engagement at School 481

between the goals and needs of the student and achieve and would like their classmates to achieve
those valued by others is not achieved. This can are not well-documented (cf., Dowson &
occur at the level of a students’ broader cultural McInerney, 2003).
background and community (Bempechat & Given this lack of empirical work on educa-
Shernoff, 2012), within the peer group (Juvonen, tional goals, several issues are especially relevant
Espinoza, & Knifsend, 2012), or when teachers for future research on engagement. With respect
and students do not achieve common purpose to adults, perhaps the most important task for
(Assor, 2012; Hipkins, 2012). understanding the socio-cultural contexts of
From this work, it is clear that a central tenet learning is to come to terms with the fundamental
of an ecological approach to understanding stu- questions central to the education of children: As
dent engagement is that achieving a balanced parents and educators, what are our educational
“fit” between the needs of the individual and goals for our children? As Nichols and Dawson
those of the broader educational environment (2012) suggest, do we want to teach simply to the
requires a focused consideration of the goals that test or nurture our children in ways that will help
children expect and are expected to achieve when them become productive and healthy adults and
they are at school. Assuming that socially compe- citizens? By the same token, what are the goals
tent students and responsive social systems con- that children bring with them to school and how
tribute in meaningful ways to academic learning can we accommodate these goals in educational
and achievement, an essential task is to identify settings? Do they strive to excel academically, to
the goals for education that we hold for students satisfy their curiosities, to establish relationships
and consider their contribution to effective and with others, or simply to feel safe? Finally, how
sustainable engagement. This issue is discussed can we support children’s willingness to engage
in the following section. in academic pursuits and create learning environ-
ments in which all of these outcomes can be
achieved?
Social Goals and Objectives In addressing the latter question, Raftery,
for Education Grolnick, and Flamm (2012) suggest that paren-
tal practices reflecting involvement, autonomy
Surprisingly, research on educational goals is support, and structure can be instrumental in
sparse. On the one hand, public schools were ini- building a strong foundation for meeting stu-
tially developed with an explicit function of edu- dents’ goals that support engagement. Hipkins
cating children to become healthy, moral, and (2012) also highlights the importance of identify-
economically productive citizens; social out- ing curricular goals that afford students the
comes in the form of moral character, conformity opportunity to relate learning to their personal
to social rules and norms, cooperation, and posi- lives and interests. Assor (2012) discusses simi-
tive styles of social interaction have been pro- lar classroom practices that support the internal-
moted consistently by policy makers as goals for ization of personal goals while also providing
students to achieve (see Wentzel, 1991, for a opportunities for students to achieve more funda-
review). On the other hand, researchers rarely mental needs for relatedness, competence, and
have asked parents and teachers about their spe- autonomy. Practices that promote effective goal
cific goals for students, although teachers often pursuit, that are common across socialization
describe their “ideal” students with regard to out- contexts as well as unique to particular settings,
comes in social (e.g., sharing, helping, and fol- clearly deserve systematic exploration and fur-
lowing rules), motivational (e.g., persistence, ther clarification.
being intrinsically interested), and performance In addition, the role of peers in helping stu-
(e.g., earning high grades) domains (Wentzel, dents define social competence for themselves
2003). Similarly, the social and academic class- and each other should not be ignored when trying
room goals that students themselves wish to to address these questions. As Juvonen et al. (2012)
482 K. Wentzel

illustrate, a consideration of self-enhancing as to include more behavioral and process-oriented


well as socially-integrative outcomes as dual com- activities can only enhance our understanding of
ponents of social competence is especially impor- school-based competence.
tant because the achievement of personal goals
and social acceptance are not always compatible.
Indeed, the process of achieving optimal levels of Processes of Influence
engagement will always include negotiations,
compromise, and coordination of the multiple and In addition to issues concerning the nature of
often conflicting goals of teachers, peers, students social competence and what it is that we would
themselves, and their parents. It is imperative that like students to engage in at school, the authors in
we identify ways to help students coordinate these this section remind us that it also is necessary to
often antagonistic goals to achieve a healthy bal- understand how and why social and contextual
ance of multiple objectives. supports can facilitate active engagement. In this
Finally, just as we need to specify further the regard, many authors reflect on the ongoing social
socially valued goals we would like students to interactions that children have with parents,
achieve, it is important that definitions of engage- teachers, and peers, with a specific focus on the
ment also reflect these socially-derived outcomes opportunities and resources that these relation-
more explicitly. For example, behavioral engage- ships provide to support or hinder academic
ment is routinely defined as behavior specific to engagement (see chapters by Raftery et al.;
learning tasks such as effort and persistence Juvonen et al.; & Pianta et al.). These discus-
(Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; Skinner, sions, however, also highlight the need for more
Kindermann, Connell, & Wellborn, 2009). In precise understanding of the nature of interper-
contrast, Pianta, Hamre, and Allen (2012) offer a sonal relationships and the mechanisms whereby
more systemic approach, defining behavioral the supports they provide have influence on stu-
engagement as a process, embedded in relation- dents’ engagement at school. More specifically,
ships and social interactions (see also Hipkins, they call into question what it is that we mean
2012). This more inclusive approach to behav- when we refer to a relationship, and what it is
ioral engagement is especially valuable given about a relationship that promotes positive
that being successful at school requires children engagement in children.
to perform a range of social as well as academic
competencies.
In fact, displays of prosocial (e.g., helping and Defining Interpersonal Relationships
sharing) and socially responsible (e.g., following
rules) behavior are essential for developing posi- Relationships are typically defined as enduring
tive relationships with teachers and peers, and connections between two individuals, uniquely
have been associated positively and consistently characterized by degrees of continuity, shared
to a range of academically-related outcomes, history, and interdependent interactions across
including motivation and academic performance settings and activities (Collins & Repinski, 1994;
(see Wentzel, 1999, 2005, 2009 for reviews). Hinde, 1997). Definitions also are frequently
Similarly, establishing and maintaining healthy extended to include the qualities of a relation-
relationships with teachers and peers has been ship, as evidenced by levels of trust, intimacy,
related positively to a range of academic out- and sharing; the presence of positive affect, close-
comes, including motivation and engagement ness, and affective tone; and the content and qual-
(Wentzel, 1999, 2005, 2009). Therefore, if efforts ity of communication (Collins & Repinski, 1994;
to develop and maintain interpersonal relation- Laible & Thompson, 2007). Along each of these
ships and to display positive forms of social dimensions, relationships can evoke positive as
behavior are important for understanding school well as negative experiences (see also, Juvonen
success, defining desirable forms of engagement et al., 2012).
23 Part III Commentary: Socio-Cultural Contexts, Social Competence, and Engagement at School 483

In addition, relationships are often thought of primarily by promoting a positive sense of self
in terms of their influence and what they provide and emotional well-being, and motivation to
the individual. From a developmental perspec- engage with the environment (see chapters by
tive, relationships are believed to be experienced Bempechat & Shernoff; Juvonen et al.; Lam,
through the lens of mental representations devel- Wong, Yang, & Liu, 2012).
oped over time and with respect to specific expe- An additional strategy has been to consider
riences (Bowlby, 1969; Laible & Thompson, relationships as serving a broader range of func-
2007). Early representations of relationships with tions that contribute to students’ competence at
caregivers are believed to provide the foundation school (see chapters by Pianta et al.; Raftery
for developing relationships outside the family et al.; see also, Wentzel, 2004, 2005; Wentzel,
context, with the quality of parent-child relation- Russell, Garza, & Merchant, 2011). Although the
ships (i.e., levels of warmth and security) often affective tone of interpersonal interactions is a
predicting the quality of peer and teacher rela- central focus of these models, additional dimen-
tionships in early and middle childhood (see sions of relationships that reflect levels of pre-
Wentzel & Looney, 2007). Mental representa- dictability and structure, instrumental resources,
tions that associate relationships with a personal and concern with a student’s emotional and phys-
sense of power and agency, predictability and ical well-being also are considered. In line with
safety, useful resources, and reciprocity are ecological perspectives on competence develop-
believed to be optimal for the internalization of ment, Wentzel’s model (2004) described how
social influence (see Kuczynski & Parkin, 2007; teacher-student and peer interactions along these
Raftery et al., 2012). Researchers also have dimensions can promote student motivation and
focused on the additional benefits of relation- academic performance. The utility of this model
ships, such as emotional well-being, a sense of for guiding work on engagement lies in a differ-
cohesion and connectedness, instrumental help, entiated definition of social support and a more
knowledge of what is expected, and a sense of complete picture of how perceived supports
identity for promoting positive developmental might influence academic engagement and learn-
outcomes (Bukowski & Hoza, 1989). ing in the classroom.
Of relevance for the current discussion is that As depicted in Fig. 23.1, Wentzel’s model pre-
research on students’ relationships with others dicts that multiple social supports promote posi-
rarely captures the conceptually rich nature of tive engagement in the social and academic life
these definitions or the developmental implica- of the classroom in part, by influencing the psy-
tions of their influence. Expanding models and chological and emotional functioning of students.
assessment strategies to include these multiple Specifically, Wentzel suggested that students will
aspects of interpersonal relationships would come to value and subsequently pursue academic
undoubtedly enhance our understanding of how and social goals valued by teachers and peers
they support engagement at school. A description when they perceive their interactions and rela-
and discussion of illustrative models follows in tionships with them as providing clear direction
the next section. concerning goals that should be achieved; as
facilitating the achievement of their goals by pro-
viding help, advice, and instruction; as being safe
Elaborating on Models of Influence and responsive to their goal strivings; and as
being emotionally supportive and nurturing.
Similar to socialization perspectives, the models These dimensions reflect essential components of
used to guide research on the influence of interper- social support discussed in this volume, in that:
sonal relationships on engagement described in (1) information is provided concerning what is
this section propose causal pathways by which the expected and valued in the classroom (Bempechat
affective quality of relationships (e.g., those that & Shernoff; Nichols & Dawson; Raftery et al.);
are emotionally close and secure), have influence (2) attempts to achieve these valued outcomes are
484 K. Wentzel

Relationship Provisions
• Emotional support
• Help
• Safety
• Expectations & values
Engagement Competent
Outcomes
Self-Processes
• Efficacy
• Attributions/Control beliefs
• Affect
Fig. 23.1 A Model of Social Supports and Classroom Competence

met with help and instruction (Assor; Bempechat arousal or anxiety or a positive sense of well-
& Shernoff; Pianta et al.; Raftery et al.); (3) being) (Meece, Wigfield, & Eccles, 1990; Pekrun,
attempts to achieve outcomes can be made in a 2009). Each of these self-perceptions are central
safe, non-threatening environment (Juvonen to theories of motivation and engagement and are
et al.); and (4) individuals are made to feel like a consistent predictors of student goals, values,
valued member of the group (Bempechat & interests, and positive forms of classroom behav-
Shernoff; Juvonen et al.; Pianta et al.). ior (see, Wentzel & Wigfield, 2009).
As a set of interacting processes, these dimen- Finally, this model predicts that social sup-
sions create a climate within which specific ports and self-perceptions are related to academic
instructional practices and academic content are outcomes by way of classroom engagement in
delivered. Moreover, the degree to which these social and academic outcomes that are central to
practices and content result in tangible learning the learning process. These outcomes can take
outcomes depends on the quality of the relation- many forms, including the active pursuit of
ship climate (see Darling & Steinberg, 1993). In socially valued goals such as to behave appropri-
other words, the affective quality of these educa- ately and to learn, effort and persistence at aca-
tional climates will determine the effectiveness demic tasks, displays of appropriate classroom
of other contextual supports such as communica- behavior, and focused attention on learning and
tion of expectations and instrumental help in pro- understanding subject matter (e.g., Wentzel,
moting engagement. With regard to classrooms, 1994, 1997, 1998, 2002). Students’ pursuit of
therefore, engagement in socially-valued activi- academic and social goals that are personally as
ties, including academic pursuits, will be more well as socially valued should then serve as a
likely to occur if students believe that others care mediator between opportunities afforded by posi-
about them and want them to engage (e.g., tive interactions with teachers and peers, and
Wentzel, Baker, & Russell, 2012; Wentzel, their academic and social accomplishments.
Russell, Garza, & Merchant, 2012). Wentzel’s model provides an example of a
The model shown in Fig. 23.1 suggests that more complex set of processes that link process
these various aspects of social support can pro- model of interpersonal relationships to engage-
mote classroom engagement indirectly by having ment that can move the field forward. In addi-
an impact on students’ beliefs about themselves. tion, however, greater focus on broader-level
Several belief systems are likely to be critical in context supports also is needed. For example,
this regard, including self-perceptions of academic within the context of schools, structural features
efficacy (Bandura, 1986), perceived control and such as school and class size, teacher:student
autonomy (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2000), and affect ratios, and funding can influence the amount and
associated with academic pursuits (e.g., negative quality of social and instructional resources and
23 Part III Commentary: Socio-Cultural Contexts, Social Competence, and Engagement at School 485

opportunities available to students. Similarly, engagement and competence also need to account
additional research on classroom reward struc- for a diversity of student backgrounds and experi-
tures (Nichols & Dawson, 2012; Slavin, 2012), ences (e.g., chapters by Bempechat & Shernoff;
organizational culture and climate (Roeser, Urdan, Raftery et al.). Indeed, much of what we know
& Stephens, 2009), and person-environment fit about these processes comes from studies of
(Bempechat & Shernoff, 2012; Eccles & Midgley, White, middle-class children. In addition to
1989) also might inform our understanding of the research described herein, other researchers
how the social institutions and contexts within have found that supportive relationships with
which learning takes place can motivate children teachers might benefit minority students and girls
to engage in learning activities and positive forms in achieving positive behavioral and academic
of social interaction. outcomes to a greater extent than Caucasian stu-
Finally, work that clearly delineates the pro- dents and boys (e.g., Crosnoe & Needham, 2004).
cesses and mechanisms whereby contexts and Studies of adolescent peer groups have docu-
relationships can be improved warrants careful mented that some African-American youth might
attention. To illustrate, work in the area of peer face disproportionate levels of conflict between
relationships has provided evidence that teach- parental and peer values, with the potential to have
ers’ beliefs and behaviors, classroom organiza- a negative impact on academic achievement
tion, and school-wide structure, composition, and (Steinberg, Brown, & Dornbusch, 1996). Hispanic
climate affects students’ choice of friends, their adolescents are more likely than their non-Hispanic
general propensity to make friends, and levels of peers to be highly connected to parents and family
peer acceptance and friendship networks in class- members, with levels of family interdependence
rooms (Juvonen et al., 2012; Pianta et al., 2012; and closeness being related positively to healthy
see also, Wentzel, Baker, & Russell, 2009). academic and social functioning (e.g., Phinney,
Similar work on teacher-student relationships has Kim-Jo, Osorio, & Vilhjalmsdottir, 2005).
been less frequent although professional devel- Along these lines, the moderating effects of
opment efforts to improve teachers’ classroom broader contextual factors requires further study.
management strategies (Evertson & Weinstein, For instance, in response to findings reported by
2006), disciplinary strategies (Developmental the NICHD Child Care Study, researchers have
Studies Center), and interpersonal interactions argued that when childcare variables are assessed
and relationships with students (Pianta, 2006; in more diverse samples that include a broader
Pianta et al., 2012) have shown promise. Finally, range of SES and ethnicity, different results are
in this volume, chapters by Bempechat and obtained (e.g., Sagi, Koren-Karie, Gini, Ziv, &
Shernoff (2012), Raftery et al. 2012, highlight Joels, 2002). Researchers of older children also
ways in which families and schools can build have found that race moderates relations between
stronger and more interdependent connections. dropping out of school and features of their
These are excellent examples of work that is chal- schools and families, such that the SES of fami-
lenging but necessary to move the field forward. lies and schools predicts dropping out for White
and Hispanic adolescents but not for African-
American students (Rumberger, 1995). Some
Final Provocations for the Field studies also have demonstrated differential
teacher treatment of students as a function of stu-
The nested quality of socio-cultural contexts dent gender, race (Irvine, 1986), and behavioral
described by the authors in this section provides styles (Chang, 2003), with these differences
interesting and provocative challenges for future sometimes attributed in part, to teachers’ own race
research. Many of these challenges have already and gender (Saft & Pianta, 2001). Expanding
been noted, However, several remaining issues research to incorporate the experiences of all
deserve comment. First, as noted by many of the students would provide valuable information
authors in this volume, models of school-based about the generalizability of extant theories and
486 K. Wentzel

empirical findings, and provide practitioners with In closing, the authors of chapters in this sec-
needed guidance for working with diverse popu- tion are to be applauded for their extremely rich
lations of students. and insightful work on socio-cultural contexts
In addition to studying ways in which fami- and student engagement. The goal of this com-
lies, communities, and cultures can support stu- mentary has been to provide some additional
dent engagement, additional research on ways in thoughts and insights into the nature of school-
which schools can have effects on children by related competence and how it might be sup-
way of their positive impact on the economic and ported by students’ experiences within broader
political life of communities also is warranted socio-cultural contexts, including relationships
(e.g., Sederberg, 1987; Reynolds, 1995). School- with their parents, teachers and peers, social
to-work and service learning programs provide aspects of learning structures, and the value and
excellent examples of school-based resources belief systems that define school cultures and the
that have the potential to provide positive benefits communities they live in. In conjunction with the
to communities and families by engaging adults other chapters in this volume, my hope is to pro-
and children in activities outside of the class- vide a strong foundation to explore further the
room. The notion that community and family role of social experiences and contexts in sup-
effects might mediate the impact of schools on porting the social and intellectual accomplish-
children is intriguing, but rarely studied in sys- ments of all children.
tematic fashion. Therefore, a necessary next step
is the development of conceptual models that
consider ways in which children and the various References
social systems in which they develop, including
home, peer groups, communities, and schools, Assor, A. (2012). Allowing choice and nurturing an inner
interact to support the development of school- compass: Educational practices supporting students’
related competence. How the coordination of need for autonomy. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly,
& C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student
these systems changes as children develop and
engagement (pp. 421–439). New York: Springer.
ways in which they jointly contribute to chil- Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and
dren’s developing school-related goals should be action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs,
a primary target of researchers’ efforts. NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bempechat, J., & Shernoff, D. J. (2012). Parental influ-
Finally, identifying ways in which social con-
ences on achievement motivation and student engage-
texts promote the development of social and aca- ment. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie
demic competencies at school requires systematic (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement
experimental research over time. However, exper- (pp. 315–342). New York: Springer.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Attachment (Vol.
imental studies designed to examine processes
1). New York: Basic Books.
that support social competence development in Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). Ecological systems theory. In
schools are rare. Moreover, most school reform R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development (Vol. 6,
efforts focus on improving achievement test pp. 187–250). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Bukowski, W. M., & Hoza, B. (1989). Popularity and
scores and other academic outcomes (e.g., No
friendship: Issues in theory, measurement, and out-
Child Left Behind Act of 2001), without consider- come. In T. J. Berndt & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Peer rela-
ation of the social and psychological consequences tionships in child development (pp. 15–45). New York:
of these efforts. Given the strong inter-relations Wiley.
Chang, L. (2003). Variable effects of children’s aggres-
among school success, qualities of relationships
sion, social withdrawal, and prosocial leadership as
with teachers and peers, classroom climate, and functions of teacher beliefs and behaviors. Child
school cultures, it seems essential that reform ini- Development, 74, 535–548.
tiatives involving experimentation in schools and Collins, W. A., & Repinski, D. J. (1994). Relationships
during adolescence: Continuity and change in inter-
evaluation of student progress incorporate assess-
personal perspective. In R. Montemayor, G. Adams, &
ments of processes and outcomes informed by a T. Gullotta (Eds.), Personal relationships during ado-
broader socio-cultural perspective. lescence (pp. 7–36). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
23 Part III Commentary: Socio-Cultural Contexts, Social Competence, and Engagement at School 487

Crosnoe, R., & Needham, B. (2004). Holism, contextual model. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie
variability, and the study of friendships in adolescent (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement
development. Child Development, 75, 264–279. (pp. 403–419). New York: Springer.
Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990).
context – An integrative model. Psychological Bulletin, Predictors of math anxiety and its influence on young
113, 487–496. adolescents’ course enrollment intentions and perfor-
Developmental Studies Center (n.d.). Retrieved August 2, mance in mathematics. Journal of Educational
2007, from www.devstu.org/ Psychology, 82, 60–70.
Dowson, M., & McInerney, D. M. (2003). What do students Nichols, S., & Dawson, H. (2012). Assessment as a con-
say about their motivational goals?: Towards a more text for student engagement. In S. L. Christenson,
complex and dynamic perspective on student motivation. A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 91–113. research on student engagement (pp. 457–477).
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, New York: Springer.
R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2012). The impact of Pekrun, R. (2009). Emotions at school. In K. R. Wentzel
enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at
meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. school (pp. 575–604). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Child Development, 82, 405–432. Phinney, J. S., Kim-Jo, T., Osorio, S., & Vilhjalmsdottir,
Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage-environment fit: P. (2005). Autonomy and relatedness in adolescent-
Developmentally appropriate classrooms for young parent disagreements: Ethnic and developmental fac-
adolescents. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research tors. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 8–39.
on motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 139–186). New Piaget, J. (1983). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),
York: Academic Press. Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 103–128).
Evertson, C., & Weinstein, C. (2006). Handbook of New York: Wiley.
Classroom Management – Research, Practice, and Pianta, R. (2006). Classroom management and relation-
Contemporary Issues. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. ships between children and teachers: Implications for
Ford, M. E. (1992). Motivating humans: Goals, emotions, research and practice. In C. Evertson & C. Weinstein
and personal agency beliefs. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. (Eds.), Handbook of classroom management – Research,
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). practice, and contemporary issues (pp. 685–710),
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B., & Allen, J. P. (2012). Teacher-
59–109. student relationships and engagement: Conceptual-
Greenfield, P. M. (1997). You can’t take it with you – Why izing, measuring, and improving the capacity of
ability assessments don’t cross cultures. American classroom interactions. In S. L. Christenson, A. L.
Psychologist, 52, 1115–1124. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on
Hinde, R. A. (1997). Towards understanding relation- student engagement (pp. 365–386). New York: Springer.
ships. London: Academic Press. Raftery, J. N., Grolnick, W. S., & Flamm, E. S. (2012).
Hipkins, R. (2012). The engaging nature of teaching for Families as facilitators of student engagement:
competency development. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Toward a Home-School Partnership Model. In S. L.
Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Hand-
student engagement (pp. 441–456). New York: Springer. book of research on student engagement (pp. 343–364).
Irvine, J. J. (1986). Teacher-student interactions: Effects New York: Springer.
of student race, sex, and grade level. Journal of Reynolds, D. R. (1995). Rural education: Decentering the
Educational Psychology, 78, 14–21. consolidation debate. In E. N. Castle (Ed.), The chang-
Juvonen, J., Espoinoza, G., & Knifsend, C. (2012). The ing American countryside: Rural people and places
role of peer relationships in student academic and (pp. 451–480). Lawrence, KS: University Press of Kansas.
extracurricular engagement. In S. L. Christenson, Roeser, R., Urdan, T., & Stephens, J. (2009). School as a
A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research context of student motivation and achievement. In
on student engagement (pp. 387–401). New York: K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of
Springer. motivation at school (pp. 381–410). New York: Taylor
Kuczynski, L., & Parkin, M. (2007). Agency and bidirec- & Francis.
tionality in socialization: Interactions, transactions Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social compe-
and relational dialectics. In J. Grusec & P. Hastings tence: A theoretical review. Social Development, 6,
(Eds.), Handbook of social development (pp. 259– 111–135.
283). New York: Guilford. Rumberger, R. W. (1995). Dropping out of middle school:
Laible, D., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Early socializa- A multilevel analysis of students and schools. American
tion: A relationship perspective. In J. Grusec & Educational Research Journal, 32, 583–625.
P. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of social development Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination
(pp. 181–207). New York: Guilford. theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social
Lam, S., Wong, B. P. H., Yang, H., & Liu, Y. (2012). development, and well-being. American Psychologist,
Understanding student engagement with a contextual 55, 68–78.
488 K. Wentzel

Saft, E. W., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Teachers’ perceptions Wentzel, K. R. (2002). Are effective teachers like good
of their relationships with students: Effects of child parents? Interpersonal predictors of school adjustment
age, gender, and ethnicity of teachers and children. in early adolescence. Child Development, 73, 287–301.
School Psychology Quarterly, 16, 125–141. Wentzel, K. R. (2003). School adjustment. In W. Reynolds
Sagi, A., Koren-Karie, N., Gini, M., Ziv, Y., & Joels, T. & G. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 7:
(2002). Shedding further light on the effects of various Educational Psychology (pp. 235–258). New York:
types and quality of early child care on infant-mother Wiley.
attachment relationship: The Haifa study of early child Wentzel, K. R. (2004). Understanding classroom compe-
care. Child Development, 73, 1166–1186. tence: The role of social-motivational and self-processes.
Sederberg, C. H. (1987). Economic role of school districts In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and
in rural communities. Research in Rural Education, 4, behavior (Vol. 32, pp. 213–241). New York: Elsevier.
125–130. Wentzel, K. R. (2005). Peer relationships, motivation,
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & and academic performance at school. In A. Elliot &
Wellborn, J. P. (2009). Engagement and disaffection as C. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and moti-
organizational constructs in the dynamics of motiva- vation (pp. 279–296). New York: Guilford.
tional development. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield Wentzel, K. R. (2009). Students’ relationships with teach-
(Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 223–246). ers as motivational contexts. In K. Wentzel & A.
New York: Taylor & Francis. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school
Slavin, R. (2011). Instruction based on cooperative learn- (pp. 301–322). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
ing. In R. Mayer & P. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of Wentzel, K. R., & Wigfield, A. (2009). Handbook of moti-
research on learning and instruction (pp. 344–360). vation at school. New York, NY: Taylor Francis.
New York: Routledge. Wentzel, K. R., Baker, S. A., & Russell, S. (2009). Peer
Steinberg, L., Brown, B. B., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1996). relationships and positive adjustment at school. In
Beyond the classroom: why school reform has failed R. Gillman, S. Huebner, & M. Furlong (Eds.),
and what parents need to do. New York: Simon & Promoting wellness in children and youth: A handbook
Schuster. of positive psychology in the schools (pp. 229–244).
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Wentzel, K. R., & Looney, L. (2007). Socialization in
Harvard University Press. school settings. In J. Grusec & P. Hastings (Eds.),
Wentzel, K. R. (1991). Social competence at school: Handbook of social development (pp. 382–403). New
Relations between social responsibility and academic York: Guilford.
achievement. Review of Educational Research, 61, Wentzel, K., Russell, S., & Baker, S. (2011). Multiple
1–24. goals of teachers, parents, and peers as predictors of
Wentzel, K. R. (1994). Relations of social goal pursuit to young adolescents’ goals and affective functioning.
social acceptance, classroom behavior, and perceived Unpublished manuscript, University of Maryland,
social support. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, College Park.
173–182. Wentzel, K. R., Russell, S., Garza, E., & Merchant, B.
Wentzel, K. R. (1997). Student motivation in middle (2011). Understanding the role of social supports in
school: The role of perceived pedagogical caring. Latina/o adolescents’ school engagement and achieve-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 411–419. ment. In N. Cabrera, F. Villarruel, & H. Fitzgerald
Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social support and adjustment in (Eds.), Volume of Latina/o adolescent psychology
middle school: The role of parents, teachers, and peers. and mental health: Vol. 2: Adolescent development.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 202–209. (pp.195–216). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.
Wentzel, K. R. (1999). Social-motivational processes and Wentzel, K. R., Baker, S. A., & Russell, S. L. (2012).
interpersonal relationships: Implications for under- Young adolescent’s perceptions of teachers’ and peers’
standing students’ academic success. Journal of goals as predictors of social and academic goal pursuit.
Educational Psychology, 91, 76–97. Applied Psychology.
Part IV
Student Engagement:
Determinants and Student Outcomes
The Relationship Between
Engagement and High School
24
Dropout*

Russell W. Rumberger and Susan Rotermund

Abstract
This chapter first reviews some prominent models of dropping out and the
role that individual factors, including engagement, and contextual factors
play in the process. It then reviews empirical research related to those fac-
tors, with a focus on engagement-related factors. Scholars have proposed
a number of models to explain the process of dropping out of school. While
there is a fair amount of overlap in the models, they differ with respect to
the specific factors that are thought to exert the most influence on dropping
out and the specific process that leads to that outcome. The review of con-
ceptual models of the empirical research literature leads to several conclu-
sions about why students drop out. First, no single factor can completely
account for a student’s decision to continue in school until graduation. Just
as students themselves report a variety of reasons for quitting school, the
research literature also identifies a number of salient factors that appear to
influence the decision. Second, the decision to drop out is not simply a
result of what happens in school. Clearly, students’ behavior and perfor-
mance in school influence their decision to stay or leave. But students’
activities and behaviors outside of school—particularly engaging in devi-
ant and criminal behavior—also influence their likelihood of remaining in
school. Third, dropping out is more of a process than an event.

The problem of high school dropouts in the United


*Much of the material for this chapter comes from a book
by the first author, Dropping Out: Why Students Quit School States has been characterized as a national crisis
and What Can Be Done About It (Cambridge, MA: and a “silent epidemic” (Bridgeland, DiIulio, &
Harvard University Press, 2011). We would like to thank Morison, 2006). According to Education Week,
the editors, Sandra Christenson and Cathy Wylie, for their
the nation’s leading education periodical, 1.3 mil-
helpful feedback on an earlier version of this paper.
lion students from the high school class of 2010
R.W. Rumberger (*) will fail to earn a high school diploma (Education
Graduate School of Education, University of California,
Week, 2010). This means that the nation’s schools
Santa Barbara, CA, USA
e-mail: russ@education.ucsb.edu are losing almost 7,200 students each school day.
Moreover, the problem is getting worse. Nobel
S. Rotermund
MPR Associates, Berkeley, CA, USA laureate economist James Heckman examined the
e-mail: srotermund@mprinc.com various sources of data used to calculate dropout

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 491


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_24, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
492 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

and gradation rates and, after correcting for errors would generate more than $45 billion in savings
in previous calculations, concluded that the high for the economy (Belfield & Levin, 2007).
school graduation rate in the USA is currently Understanding why students drop out of
77%, lower than it was 40 years ago (Heckman & school is the key to designing effective interven-
LaFontaine, 2010). tions to help solve this critical and costly prob-
Dropping out of school has consequences for lem. Yet identifying the causes of dropping out is
both dropouts and the larger society. Dropouts extremely difficult. Like other forms of educa-
face bleak economic futures. They are the least tional achievement, such as test scores and grades,
educated workers in the labor market and thus dropping out of school is likely influenced by an
have the poorest job prospects compared to more array of factors, some immediately preceding
educated workers. This means they are less departure from high school and others occurring
likely to find jobs and when they do find jobs, years earlier in middle and even elementary
the jobs generally pay the lowest wages. For school. These factors may be related to the char-
example, in 2008, the median annual earnings of acteristics and experiences of the students them-
high school dropouts were 28% less than the selves as well as the characteristics and features
earnings of high school graduates (Snyder & of their environment—their families, their
Dillow, 2010, Table 385). Over their working schools, and the communities where they live.
lives, the US Census Bureau estimates that drop- To understand why students drop out, it is
outs will earn about $200,000 less than high most useful to consider dropping out as a process
school graduates (Day & Newburger, 2002, that culminates in students quitting or finishing
Figure 3). Dropouts are more likely to engage in high school (Rumberger, 2011). Researchers
crime and, consequently, are more likely to be have developed a number of models to explain
arrested and incarcerated (Pettit & Western, the process of dropping out and the underlying
2004). They also have poorer health and, as a factors that contribute to it. In fact, some scholars
result, have shorter life spans than persons with have also characterized dropping out as “symp-
more education (Currie, 2009). tom” in recognition of the role and importance of
The negative impacts from dropping out not these underlying factors:
only affect dropouts themselves. They also impact Dropping out of high school is overrated as a prob-
the larger society. The fact that dropouts have lem in its own right—it is far more appropriately
lower employment and earnings means they viewed as the end result or symptom of other prob-
make smaller contributions to their local and state lems which have their origin much earlier in life
(Bachman, Green, & Wirtanen, 1971, P. 169).
economies as well as the national economy. It
also means they pay fewer taxes. At the same Researchers have also developed models to
time, they are more likely to require public assis- explain some of the factors that figure promi-
tance in the form of unemployment benefits, wel- nently into the process of dropping out. One of the
fare, and public health care. Dropouts generate most prominent factors found in many of these
further social costs because of their increased models is student engagement. Finally, research-
criminal activity. These costs are not simply ers have examined the role of context or settings
related to the additional costs from law enforce- in shaping various aspects of adolescent develop-
ment and incarceration, but also the costs borne ment, including engagement and dropping out.
by the victims of crime in the form of property This chapter first reviews some prominent
damage, hospitalization, and loss of life. One models of dropping out and the role that individ-
recent study estimated that each new high school ual factors, including engagement, and contex-
graduate would generate more than $200,000 in tual factors play in the process. It then reviews
government savings, and that cutting in half the empirical research related to those factors, with a
dropout rate from a single cohort of dropouts focus on engagement-related factors.
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 493

participation in school activities (classroom


Conceptual Models of Dropout participation, homework, and participation in the
and Engagement social, extracurricular, athletic, and governance
aspects of the school), which, in turn, leads to
Models of Dropping Out
poor school performance and then to less identi-
fication (a sense of “belonging” and “valuing”)
Scholars have proposed a number of models to
with school. Over time, the lack of identification
explain the process of dropping out of school.
with school leads to less participation, poorer
Some models focus specifically on dropping out
school performance, less identification with
while others attempt to explain student outcomes
school, and eventually dropping out of school.
in general, with dropping out representing simply
Both of Finn’s models include three types
one. Most of the models focus on an individual
of factors: school performance, behaviors, and
perspective and identify a number of general
psychological conditions. The models differ in
types of factors: prior school experiences, par-
the specific behavioral and psychological factors
ticularly academic performance (grades, test
they highlight: the “frustration-self-esteem” model
scores, etc.); behaviors, including academic (e.g.,
focuses on problem behaviors (skipping class, tru-
doing homework), cognitive (exerting effort
ancy, disruptive behavior, and juvenile delinquency,
toward academic ends), social (getting along with
as well as dropping out) and self-esteem; while the
teachers and classmates); and psychological con-
“participation-identification” model focuses on
ditions, such as self-esteem and identification
participation and identification with school.
with school. While there is a fair amount of over-
lap in the models, they differ with respect to the
specific factors (italicized in the discussion Life Course Models
below) that are thought to exert the most influ- A number of long-term longitudinal studies have
ence on dropping out and the specific process that been conducted in the USA that have tracked the
leads to that outcome. In some cases, the models educational experiences and outcomes of small,
are developed from a review of the literature, but local samples of children. These studies have
not tested empirically; in other cases, the models developed and tested empirical models of the
are derived from a specific empirical study. dropout process to determine the direct and indi-
rect effects of various early and late factors on
Finn’s Models whether students dropped out or completed high
In a widely cited review of the literature, Finn school. Because of their long-term perspective,
proposed two alternative, developmental models some scholars refer to these as life course
to explain dropping out (Finn, 1989). The first, models.
which he labeled the “frustration-self-esteem” In their study of 1,242 children who were
model, posits that the initial antecedent to school enrolled as first graders in pubic and parochial
withdrawal is early school failure, which, in turn, schools in the poor, black community of
leads to low self-esteem and then problem behav- Woodlawn in Chicago in the 1966–1967 school
iors (skipping class, truancy, disruptive behavior, year, Ensminger and Slusarcick found that vari-
and juvenile delinquency). Over time, problem ables in five domains—family background, fam-
behaviors further erode school performance, ily educational expectations and values,
which leads to further declines in self-esteem parent-child interaction concerning school, the
and increases in problem behaviors. Eventually, social integration of the family in terms of school,
students either voluntarily quit school or are and the child’s cognitive and behavioral perfor-
removed from school because of their problem- mance in school—predicted whether children
atic behavior. dropped out or graduated from high school
The second model Finn labels the “participa- (Ensminger & Slusacick, 1992).
tion-identification” model. In this model, the In their study of 205 Euro-American families
initial antecedent to withdrawal is the lack of with varied living arrangements (two-parent
494 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

families, single mothers, cohabitating couples, Tinto Model


and commune and group living arrangements) Another theoretical perspective that is useful in
from California that started in 1974–1975, explaining dropout behavior is a widely acknowl-
Garnier, Stein, and Jacobs found that nonconven- edged theory of institutional departure at the
tional family lifestyles and values, cumulative postsecondary level developed by sociologist
family stresses, and family socioeconomic status Vincent Tinto (Tinto, 1987, 1994). Tinto focused
(SES) affected school performance and motiva- on the role of the institutional environment in
tion, adolescent stress and substance use, and, influencing students’ adjustment and ultimately
ultimately, school dropout (Garnier, Stein, & their departure decision. The process of departure
Jacobs, 1997). is first influenced by a series of personal attri-
In their study based on data from the Beginning butes, which predispose students to respond to
Baltimore Study (BBS), a panel study of 661 chil- different situations or conditions in particular
dren who entered first grade in 20 Baltimore city ways. These personal attributes include family
schools in the fall of 1982—Alexander and background, skills and abilities, and prior school
Entwisle present and test a model of dropping out experiences, including goals (intentions) and
from a life course perspective that views dropping motivation (commitments) to continue their
out as a long-term “process of progressive aca- schooling. Once students enroll in a particular
demic disengagement” (Alexander, Entwisle, & school, two separate dimensions of that institu-
Kabbini, 2001). Their model identified three types tion influence whether a student remains there: a
of factors—students’ school experiences (school social dimension that deals with the social inte-
performance, grade retention, and track-like gration of students with the institution and the
placements), students’ personal resources (what value of schooling; and an academic dimension
they labeled “engagement behaviors” and that deals with the academic integration or
“engagement attitudes”), and parental attitudes, engagement of students in meaningful learning.
behaviors, and support—in several developmen- Both dimensions are influenced by the informal
tal periods of students’ school careers—first grade, as well as the formal structure of the institution.
elementary years (grades 5–8), middle school For example, academic integration may occur in
(grades 6–8), and early high school (grade 9)—that the formal system of classes and in the informal
predicted whether students dropped out or stayed system of interactions with faculty in other
in school. settings.
In two studies based on data from the Chicago These two dimensions can have separate and
Longitudinal Study—an ongoing investigation of independent influences on whether students leave
1,569 low-income, minority children born in 1979 an institution, depending on the needs and attri-
or 1980 who grew up in high-poverty neighbor- butes of the student, as well as external factors. To
hoods of Chicago—Reynolds, Ou, and Topitzes remain in an institution, students must become
developed a conceptual model of the various integrated to some degree in either the social sys-
pathways that preschool participation affects such tem or the academic system. For example, some
long-term outcomes as educational attainment students may be highly integrated into the aca-
and juvenile delinquency (Reynolds, Ou, & demic system of the institution, but not the social
Topitzes, 2004). They found that preschool par- system. Yet as long as their social needs are met
ticipation directly affected school performance elsewhere and their goals and commitment remain
(cognitive ability, retention), social adjustment, the same, such students will remain in the same
and family support which, in turn, affected school institution. Likewise, some students may be highly
quality, persistence, and motivation in middle integrated into the social system of the institution,
grades which, in turn, affected educational attain- but not the academic system. But again, as long as
ment and delinquency in later adolescence. they maintain minimum academic performance
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 495

and their goals and commitment remain the same, and has belief in the legitimacy and efficacy of
such students will remain in the same institution. the institution” (p. 117). Drawing on Tinto’s
Tinto’s model offers several insights into the work, they then identified four common impedi-
process of institutional departure, which can ments to school membership: adjustment to a new
involve either transferring to another school or and often larger and more impersonal school set-
quitting school altogether. First, it distinguishes ting; difficulty in doing more rigorous academic
between the commitment to the goal of finishing work; incongruence between students’ values,
college and the commitment to a particular insti- experiences, and projected future and the goals
tution, and how these commitments can be influ- and rewards of the school; and isolation from
enced by students’ experiences in school over teachers and peers in both academic and social
time. Some students who are not sufficiently inte- experiences, something very similar to Tinto’s
grated into their current college may simply concept of academic and social integration.
transfer to another educational setting rather than Educational engagement for Wehlage et al
drop out, if they can maintain their goals and referred to the “psychological investment
commitment to schooling more generally. Other required to comprehend and master knowledge
students, however, may simply drop out rather and skills explicitly taught in school” (p. 177).
than transfer to another school if their current They identified three impediments to educational
school experiences severely diminish their goals engagement: (1) schoolwork is not extrinsically
and commitment to schooling. Second, it sug- motivating for many students because achieve-
gests that schools can have multiple communities ment is not tied to any explicit and valued goal;
or subcultures to accommodate and support the (2) the dominant learning process pursued in
different needs of students. Third, it acknowl- schools is too abstract, verbal, sedentary, indi-
edges the importance of external factors that can vidualistic, competitive, and controlled by others
influence student departure. For example, exter- (and therefore not intrinsically motivating) as
nal communities, including families and friends, opposed to concrete, problem-oriented, active,
can help students better meet the academic and kinesthetic, cooperative, and autonomous; and
social demands of school by providing necessary (3) classroom learning is often stultifying because
support. External events can also change a stu- educators are obsessed with the “coverage” of
dent’s evaluation of the relative costs and benefits the subject matter, which makes school knowl-
of staying in a particular school if other alterna- edge superficial and also intrinsically unsatisfy-
tives change (e.g., job prospects). ing, preventing students from gaining a sense of
competence (p. 179).
Wehlage and Colleagues’ Model
Drawing on their research on at-risk students and Models of Deviance
programs as well as Tinto’s model, Gary Wehlage While most of the models reviewed thus far have
and his colleagues developed a model to explain focused on student attitudes and behaviors within
dropping out and other high school outcomes school, social scientists in such fields as psychol-
that focuses on the contribution of school fac- ogy, sociology, economics, and criminology have
tors (Wehlage, Rutter, Smith, Lesko, & Fernandez, focused on a range of deviant behaviors of ado-
1989). In this model, student outcomes are lescents outside of school—including juvenile
jointly influenced by two broad factors: school delinquency, drug and alcohol abuse, teenage par-
membership (or social bonding) and educational enting and childbearing—and their relationship
engagement. to school dropout (Lerner & Galambos, 1998).
Social bonding, which is critical to connecting Sara Battin-Pearson and her colleagues identi-
students to the school, has four aspects: “A stu- fied five alternative theories of dropout that con-
dent is socially bonded to the extent that he or she ceptualize the process and the salient influences
is attached to adults and peers, committed to the of dropping out differently (Battin-Pearson et al.,
norms of the school, involved in school activities 2000). The first model, academic mediation
496 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

theory, posits that all predictors of dropping out, reasons, that culminates in the final act of leav-
including deviant behavior, low social bonding, ing” (Rumberger, 1987, p. 111). Similarly, in her
and family background, are mediated by poor study of students in two California continuation
academic achievement. In the remaining four high schools, Kelly preferred to use the term dis-
models, poor academic achievement only medi- engagement to either the term dropout, which she
ates some of the effects of the other predictors, so argued put inordinate blame on the student’s
that at least some predictors also exert a direct agency, or pushout, which put inordinate blame
influence on dropping out. In the second model, on schools (Kelly, 1993). Student engagement is
general deviance theory, several types of deviant also an important precursor to other aspects of
behavior—juvenile delinquency, drug and alco- school performance, particularly academic per-
hol use, smoking, and teenage pregnancy—exert formance in the classroom. Because of its impor-
a direct influence on dropping out. In the third tance, scholars have proposed a number of models
model, deviant affiliation theory, bonding with to explain student engagement.
antisocial or delinquent friends exerts a direct In a follow-up to his work on at-risk students
influence on dropping out. In the fourth model, and programs, Gary Welhage and his colleagues,
family socialization theory, poor family social- Fred Newman and Susie Lamborn, developed a
ization, as related to parental expectations, fam- model of student engagement in academic work,
ily stress, and parental control, exerts a direct which they define as “the student’s psychological
influence on dropping out. And in the fifth model, investment in and effort directed toward learning,
structural strains theory, demographic factors understanding, or mastering the knowledge,
such as race, ethnicity, and family socioeconomic skills, or crafts that academic work is intended to
status, exert a direct influence on dropping out. In promote” (Newmann, 1992, p. 12). As they point
addition to general models of deviance, crimi- out, because engagement is an inner quality of
nologists have developed a number of alternative concentration and effort, it is not readily observed,
theories to explain why involvement with the so it must be inferred from indirect indicators
juvenile justice system may be detrimental or such as the amount of participation in academic
beneficial to subsequent delinquent behavior and work (attendance, amount of time spent on aca-
school dropout (Sweeten, 2006). demic work), and interest and enthusiasm exhib-
The models reviewed in this section identify a ited by students. They further suggest that
number of factors and processes to explain why engagement is related to but differs from motiva-
students drop out. They suggest the process of tion, a subject of longstanding concern to educa-
dropping out is complex, influenced by a number tional psychologists:
of factors in and out of school and over time. Some Academic motivation usually refers to a general
factors, such as school performance, are common desire or disposition to succeed in academic work
to virtually all the models, while other factors, such and in the more specific tasks of school.
Conceivably a student can be motivated to perform
as misbehavior or attitudes toward school, are
well in a general sense without being engaged in
found in only some models. The models are not the specific tasks of school. Engagement in spe-
necessarily competitive, but rather may be useful in cific tasks may either precede or presume general
identifying different patterns or types of dropouts. motivation to succeed. By focusing on the extent to
which students demonstrate active interest, effort,
and concentrations in the specific work that teach-
ers design, engagement calls special attention to
The Role of Engagement the contexts that help activate underlying motiva-
tion, and also to the conditions that may generate
new motivation (p. 13).
Student engagement figures prominently in the
process of dropping out. In fact, in an early review They posit that engagement in academic work
of the research literature published in 1987, is largely influenced by three major factors: “stu-
Rumberger suggested, “dropping out itself might dents’ underlying need for competence, the extent
be better viewed as a process of disengagement to which students experience membership in the
from school, perhaps for either academic or social school, and the authenticity of the work they are
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 497

asked to complete” (p. 17). They identify a num- In their extensive review of research literature,
ber of factors that influence school membership Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and Paris (2004) identi-
and authentic work similar to those identified by fied three dimensions of this broader concept of
Wehlage and his colleagues in their model of stu- engagement: (1) behavioral engagement, which
dent dropout. represents behaviors that demonstrate students’
In 2004, the National Research Council issued attachment and involvement in both the academic
a report, Engaging Schools: Fostering High and social aspects of school, such as doing home-
School Students’ Motivation to Learn, which simi- work and participating in extracurricular activi-
larly made the distinction between motivation and ties like athletics or student government;
engagement in schoolwork (National Research emotional engagement, which refers to students’
Council, Committee on Increasing High School affective reactions to their experiences in school
Students’ Engagement and Motivation to Learn and in their classes, such as whether they are
[NRC, CIHSSEML], 2004). The committee that happy or bored; and cognitive engagement, which
issued the report stated that engagement involved represents mental behaviors that contribute to
both observable behaviors (active participation in learning, such as trying hard and expending effort
class, completing work, taking challenging classes) on academic tasks. Their review went on to
and unobservable behaviors (effort, attention, examine both the outcomes and the antecedents
problem solving, and the use of metacognitive to engagement. The antecedents include school-
strategies) as well as emotions (such as interest, level factors, such as school size, communal
enthusiasm, and pride in success), similar to how structures, and disciplinary practices; and class-
Newmann, Wehlage, and Lamborn characterized room-level factors, such as teacher support, peers,
it. But unlike Newmann and his colleagues, the classroom structure, and task characteristics. In a
committee developed a model that suggested the more recent review, Appleton, Christenson, and
impact of the educational context (such as instruc- Furlong (2008) identified a myriad of defini-
tion, school climate, school organization, school tions and measures of engagement that help illu-
composition, and school size) is mediated by three minate the various dimensions of the concept,
psychological variables: students’ beliefs about but also make it difficult to collect and use
their competence and control (I can), their values consistent information on engagement to design
and goals (I want to), and their sense of social interventions.
connectiveness or belonging (I belong). This
model incorporates more explicit aspects of stu-
dent motivation from the research literature. The Role of Context
Connell, for example, postulated that the more
students perceive the school setting as meeting The models presented thus far focus largely on
their psychological needs for autonomy, compe- dropping out as a process influenced by a broad
tence, and relatedness, the more engaged they will array of individual factors, including attitudes,
be in school activities (Connell, 1990). behaviors, and school performance. Yet these
Both the Wehlage and NRC models focus on factors and students’ experiences more generally
engagement in academic work. But as Tinto and are shaped by three settings or contexts where
Finn suggested in their models, participation and youths spend their time: families, schools, and
integration in school can take place in other are- communities. Increasingly, social scientists have
nas besides the classroom. So while academic come to realize the importance of these settings
engagement may be sufficient to improve aca- in shaping child and adolescent development. In
demic achievement, engagement in other areas of psychology, for example, Bronfenbrenner’s influ-
the school, such as extracurricular activities, may ential book, The Ecology of Human Development
be equally valuable in getting students to stay in (1979), helped to focus attention of psychologists
school (although not necessarily sufficient to on how the various contexts of the family,
have them improve their academic performance schools, peer groups, and communities shape all
or to graduate). aspects of adolescent development—physical,
498 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

psychological, cognitive and social—as well as well as the salient factors underlying that process.
how the relationship between context and devel- The framework, illustrated in Fig. 24.1, considers
opment change over time (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; dropping out and graduation as specific aspects
Lerner & Galambos, 1998; Steinberg & Morris, of student performance in high school and identi-
2001). The importance of context was further fies two types of factors that influence that per-
emphasized by the National Research Council formance: individual factors associated with
Panel on High-Risk Youth in their 1993 report, students, and institutional factors associated with
Losing Generations: Adolescents in High-risk the three major contexts that influence students—
Settings, which argued that too much emphasis families, schools, and communities.
had been placed on high-risk youth and their Individual factors can be grouped into four
families, and not enough on the high-risk settings areas or domains: educational performance,
in which they live and go to school: behaviors, attitudes, and background. Although
The work of this panel began as an attempt to bet- the framework suggests a causal ordering of these
ter understand why some adolescents are drawn to factors, from background to attitudes to behav-
risky life-styles while others, similarly situated, iors to performance, the various models of drop-
engage in only normal adolescent experimenta- out and engagement discussed earlier indicate a
tion. As our work progressed, however, we became
convinced that a focus on individual characteris- less linear relationship. In particular, the relation-
tics of adolescents would contribute to the overem- ship between attitudes and behaviors is generally
phasis of the last two decades on the personal considered to be more reciprocal; for example,
attributes of adolescents and their families at the initial attitudes may influence behaviors, which,
expense of attention to the effects or settings or
context. We concluded that it was important to in turn, may influence subsequent attitudes (as
right the balance by focusing on the profound suggested by Tinto’s model). But the purpose of
influence that settings have on the behavior and this framework is not to suggest a particular
development of adolescents (p. 1). model of the dropout process, but simply a frame-
Social scientists have long recognized the crit- work for organizing a review of the literature.
ical role that context plays in understanding such The factors listed within each group represent
phenomena as poverty, racial inequality, gang conceptual categories that may be measured by
behavior, and unwed motherhood (Edin & one or more specific indicators or variables
Kefalas, 2005; Evans, 2004; Sullivan, 1989; The first domain is educational performance.
Wilson, 1987). The framework posits three inter-related dimen-
Although a wide variety of contextual factors sions of educational performance: (1) academic
have been shown to influence adolescent devel- achievement, as reflected in grades and test scores,
opment and school performance, they can be cat- (2) educational persistence, which reflects whether
egorized into four major areas: (1) composition, students remain in the same school or transfer
such as the characteristics of the persons within (school mobility) or remain enrolled in school at
the setting or context; (2) structure, such as size all (dropout), and (3) educational attainment,
and location; (3) resources, such as physical, fis- which is reflected by progressing in school (e.g.,
cal and human resources; and (4) practices, such earning credits and being promoted from one
as parenting practices within families and instruc- grade to another) and completing formal educa-
tional practices within schools. tion by earning of degrees or diplomas. The
framework suggests that high school graduation is
dependent on both persistence and achievement.
A Conceptual Model of Student That is, students who either interrupt their school-
Performance in High School ing by dropping out or changing schools, or who
have poor academic achievement in school, are
These models can be used to construct a compre- less likely to progress in school and to graduate.
hensive conceptual framework for understanding The second domain consists of a range of
the process of dropping out and graduation, as behaviors that are associated with educational
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 499

Conceptual Model of High School Performance

BACKGROUND ATTITUDES BEHAVIORS PERFORMANCE


Demographics Engagement
Individual Goals Achievement
Health Coursework
Factors
Prior performance Values Deviance Persistence
Past experiences Peers
Self-perceptions Attainment
Employment

FAMILIES SCHOOLS COMMUNITIES

Structure Composition Composition


Institutional
Resources Structure Resources
Factors
Practices Resources
Practices

Fig. 24.1 Conceptual model of high school performance

performance. The first factor is student engage- three institutional contexts—families, schools,
ment, which we list in the behavioral group even and communities—and several key features
though some conceptions of engagement, as dis- within them: composition, structure, resources,
cussed earlier, can have attitudinal (emotional) as and practices. This conceptual framework can be
well as behavioral components. Other behaviors used to review the empirical research on high
that have been identified in the research literature school dropouts.
include coursetaking, deviance (misbehavior,
drug and alcohol use, and childbearing), peer
associations, and employment. Empirical Research
The third domain consists of attitudes, which
we use as a general label to represent a wide Scholars have conducted literally hundreds of
range of psychological factors including expecta- studies to understand how and why students drop
tions, goals, values, and self-perceptions (e.g., out or graduate from high school. They have also
perceived competence, perceived autonomy, and employed a wide range of research methodolo-
perceived sense of belonging). gies, from in-depth case studies of students and
The last domain consists of student back- schools designed to understand the process of
ground characteristics, which include demo- dropping out and the salient factors that contrib-
graphic characteristics, health, prior performance ute to that process (Fine, 1991; Flores-Gonzalez,
in school, and past experiences, such as partici- 2002; Kelly, 1993; Romo & Falbo, 1996;
pation in preschool, after-school activities, and Valenzuela, 1999) to large-scale statistical stud-
summer school. ies designed to identify the unique contribution
The framework further posits that these indi- of specific factors on whether students drop out
vidual-level characteristics are influenced by or graduate from high school (Bryk & Thum, 1989;
500 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

Lee & Burkam, 2003; Rumberger, 1995; Rumberger Behaviors


& Thomas, 2000). In the earlier discussion about the process of drop-
It should be pointed out that scholars, for the ping out, engagement emerged as an important
most part, are unable to establish definitively that part of the educational process and a powerful pre-
any specific factor “causes” students to drop out. cursor to dropping out. Students who are engaged
Even complex, statistical models with large num- in school, whether in the academic arena or the
bers of variables are often unable to control for social arena, are more likely to attend, to learn, and
other, unobservable factors that may contribute eventually to finish high school; students who are
to the dropout process and that may mediate the disengaged are not. Research studies have mea-
effects of other variables. However, in recent sured engagement in several ways, but no matter
years, new research designs and statistical mod- how it is measured, it predicts dropping out.
els do allow stronger, more causal inferences to One of the most direct and visible indicators
be made (Schneider, Carnoy, Kilpatrick, Schmidt, of engagement is attendance. To graduate, stu-
& Shavelson, 2007). Yet because most existing dents must not only enroll in school, they must
studies do not employ these techniques, it is more attend school. Yet some students have poor atten-
accurate to refer to these various factors as “pre- dance and such students are more likely to drop
dictors” or “influences” rather than “causes” of out of school. In our review of the literature, 13
dropping out. of the 19 analyses of attendance found that high
Because the empirical research is extensive, in absenteeism predicted dropping out (Rumberger
this section we only provide a short review of the & Lim, 2008, p. 41).
literature on high school dropouts. The review Another indicator of engagement is participa-
focuses on engagement-related predictors of drop- tion in extracurricular activities. Most, but not all,
ping out at the individual level and then a short middle and high school students participate in
summary of the predictors at the family and school extracurricular activities. In 2002, more than 50%
levels. The discussion below draws heavily on a of second-year high school students reported par-
2008 review of the research literature by Rumberger ticipating in athletics, 11% in cheerleading and
and Lim that examined 389 separate analyses drill teams, 22% in music, and 10% in hobby
found in 203 statistical dropout studies published clubs (Cahalan, Ingles, Burns, Planty, & Daniel,
in academic journals between 1983 and 2007 2006, Tables 23 and 24). In 2007, 59% of eighth
(Rumberger & Lim, 2008). The review identified graders reported participating in sports, 40% in
specific factors that the studies found had a direct drama or music, and 32% in clubs (Walston,
effect on dropping out or graduation, controlling Rathbun, & Huasken, 2008, Table 4). In our
for other factors. As a result, the review did not review of the literature, 14 of the 26 analyses
examine indirect effects or total effects. The review found that participation in extracurricular activi-
also only examined whether the factors were sta- ties reduced the odds of dropping out (with most
tistically significant, not the size of the effects. of the other studies showing non-significant
effects). At the middle school level, only two out
of seven analyses found that involvement in extra-
Individual Factors curricular activities reduced the odds of dropping
out of high school. Participation in sports, espe-
A range of individual factors encompasses vari- cially among males, shows more consistent effects
ous aspects of engagement. We first review than participation in other extracurricular activi-
behavioral factors, then attitudes, and finally ties or participation in extracurricular activities
composite measures of risk, which often include more generally (McNeal, 1995; Pittman, 1991;
engagement measures. Yin & Moore, 2004).
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 501

Other research studies created multiple indi- analyses focusing on the high school level.
cators of student engagement often based on Among the 31 analyses at the high school level,
information from student and teacher question- 14 found that misbehavior was significantly asso-
naires. For example, one recent study of high ciated with higher dropout and lower graduation
school sophomores (second-year high school rates (Rumberger & Lim, 2008, Table 2). Of the
students) created an index of academic engage- 17 analyses at the middle school level, 14 found
ment based on four questions from the student that misbehavior in middle school was signifi-
questionnaire: cantly associated with higher dropout and lower
• How many times during the first semester or graduation rates in high school. The one analysis
term were you late to school? that focused on the elementary school level found
• How many times during the first semester or that misbehavior in elementary school increased
term did you cut or skip class? the odds of dropping out of high school (Ou,
• How many times during the first semester or Mersky, Reynolds, & Kohler, 2007). It is inter-
term were you absent from school? esting to note that misbehavior in middle school
• How much do you agree or disagree that the was a more consistent predictor of dropping out
subjects you’re taking are interesting and chal- than high school misbehaviour, while academic
lenging? (Dalton, Glennie, & Ingels, 2009, performance in high school is a more consistent
p. A-22) predictor than academic performance in middle
The least engaged students (those who ranked school.
in the bottom third of this index) were five times Misbehavior may contribute to dropping out
more likely to drop out (12.1% v. 2.5%) as the in at least three ways. Students who misbehave
most engaged students (those who ranked in the may be suspended or even expelled, as Sullivan
top third) (Dalton et al., 2009, Table 7). documents in his study of youth crime and work
Of the 35 analyses that examined various in three neighborhoods of Brooklyn (Sullivan,
composite measures of student engagement in 1989, p. 56). They also may be transferred to
high school in Rumberger and Lim’s review of alternative school settings. Two-thirds of the boys
the literature, 24 found that higher levels of in Kelly’s study of two continuation high schools
engagement reduced the likelihood of dropping were sent to the schools because of discipline
out or increased the likelihood of graduating from problems (Kelly, 1993, Table 3). Finally, misbe-
high school. Of the 31 analyses that examined havior in elementary or middle school could lead
student engagement in middle school, 10 analy- to misbehavior in high school.
ses found engagement reduced dropout and A final behavior indicator of engagement is
increased graduation from high school. At the student mobility. The research literature shows
elementary level, only one of three analyses that student mobility, at least during middle and
found that engagement reduced the odds of drop- high school, affects school dropout and gradua-
ping out of high school (Alexander et al., 2001). tion. At the high school level, ten of 14 analyses in
The fact that high school measures of engage- Rumberger and Lim’s review of the literature
ment are more reliable predictors of dropping out found that student mobility increased the odds of
than middle and elementary predictors is consis- dropping out or decreased the odds of graduating.
tent with the growing literature on early warning At the middle school level, nine of 13 analyses
systems that shows proximal (high school) indi- found a positive impact of student mobility. At the
cators are more powerful predictors than middle elementary level, eight of 14 analyses found a sig-
and elementary school predictors (Allensworth & nificant relationship. One possible reason for the
Easton, 2005; Meyer, Carl, & Cheng, 2010). impact at the middle and high school levels is that
Another indicator of engagement is school secondary students are more sensitive to the dis-
misbehavior. In Rumberger and Lim’s review, ruptions to their friendship networks (Ream, 2005;
49 analyses examined the relationship between Ream & Rumberger, 2008). Of course, the signifi-
misbehavior and dropping out, with most of the cant association between mobility and dropout
502 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

may not be causal; instead, it could be due to pre- That study collected a wide range of attitudinal
existing, common factors, such as academic and behavioral information on students in grades
achievement or misbehavior, which influence both 1–9 from student self-reports, teachers’ reports,
mobility and dropout. Nonetheless, even studies and school report cards. The attitudinal informa-
that control for a host of preexisting factors, such tion included self-expectations for upcoming
as student achievement, conclude that some causal grades, educational attainment, self-ability and
association between mobility and educational per- competence, and measures of psychological
formance is likely (Pribesh & Downey, 1999). The engagement (“likes school”) and school commit-
impact also depends on the reasons students ment (pp. 810–812). The attitudinal items (as
change schools. Students who change schools in a well as the behavioral items) were all combined
purposeful way—to find a more suitable school into a single construct for grade 1, grades 2–5,
environment—may have more positive impacts grades 6–8, and grade 9. This allowed the
than students who change schools in a reactive researchers to examine not only the relative
way, such as getting into trouble and being asked effects of student attitudes and behaviors overall
or even forced to find a new school (Ream, 2005; relative to other predictors, but also their relative
Rumberger, Larson, Ream, & Palardy, 1999). effects over different grade levels or stages of
schooling. The authors found that while the
Attitudes effects of behavioral engagement on school drop-
Students’ beliefs, values, and attitudes are related out appear in grade 1, even after controlling for
to both their behaviors and to their performance the effects of school performance and family
in school. These psychological factors include background in grade 1, student attitudes do not
motivation, values, goals, and a range of students’ demonstrate a separate effect on school dropout
self-perceptions about themselves and their abili- until grade 9, with behavioral engagement still
ties. These factors change over time through stu- showing the stronger effect (Alexander et al.
dents’ developmental periods and biological 2001, Table 9). Interestingly, the authors also find
transformations, with the period of early adoles- that the correlation between attitudes and behav-
cence and the emergence of sexuality being one iors increases from grade 1 to grade 9 (p. 796).
of the most important and often the most difficult
period for many students: Goals
To succeed in school, students must value school.
For some children, the early-adolescent years mark
the beginning of a downward spiral leading to aca- That is, they have to believe that it will be instru-
demic failure and school dropout. Some early ado- mental in meeting their short-term or long-term
lescents see their school grades decline markedly goals (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). Most students,
when they enter junior high school, along with
as well as their parents, believe that education is
their interest in school, intrinsic motivation, and
confidence in their intellectual abilities. Negative the key to a better job and a better life. Yet some
responses to school increase as well, as youngsters youths, such as those profiled in Sullivan’s study
become more prone to test anxiety, learned help- of youth crime and work in Brooklyn, were more
lessness, and self-consciousness that impedes con-
ambivalent about whether finishing high school
centration on learning tasks (Eccles, 1999, p. 37).
would lead them into better jobs in their neigh-
Although there is a substantial body of borhoods where such jobs were scarce and their
research that has explored a wide range of stu- fathers and older brothers were employed in jobs
dent beliefs, values, and attitudes, far less research that did not require educational credentials
has linked them to student dropout (Eccles & (Sullivan, 1989, p. 54).
Wigfield, 2002). Most students and their parents also expect to
One exception is a detailed longitudinal study not only complete high school, but to finish col-
of a cohort of first-grade students from the lege. In 2002, more than 80% of high school
Baltimore Beginning School Study (BSS) that sophomores (and their parents) expected that
began in the fall of 1982 (Alexander et al., 2001). they would earn a bachelor’s degree or more
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 503

advanced college degree (Dalton et al., 2009, viewed as important (Ibid.). Another construct is
Table 5). locus of control, which measures whether stu-
Students who expect to graduate from college dents feel they have control over their destiny
are much less likely to drop out of high school (internal control) or not (external control). Poor
than students who only expect to finish high self-perceptions can undermine motivation,
school. Among 2002 high school sophomores thereby increasing the risk of dropping out.
who expected to earn a bachelor’s degree, only Although such perceptions are a central com-
4% dropped out of high school, compared to 21% ponent of motivation for remaining in school,
for students who only expected to complete high relatively few studies have found a direct rela-
school (Ibid.). These findings are confirmed in tionship between any of these self-perceptions
other research studies. In our review of the and dropping out, suggesting that the effects are
research literature, we identified 82 analyses that most likely mediated by academic performance.
examined the relationship between educational The most studied has been locus of control. Of
expectations and school dropout. At the high the 22 analyses of locus of control in Rumberger
school level, 33 of the 41 analyses found that and Lim’s review, only three analyses in three
higher levels of educational expectations were studies found a direct, significant relationship
associated with lower dropout rates, even after with dropout, with students who had an external
controlling for academic performance. At the locus of control—the feeling of little control over
middle school level, 23 of the 38 analyses found one’s destiny—even as early as first grade, show-
the same relationship. Three analyses examined ing a higher propensity to drop out (Alexander
educational expectations in elementary school et al., 2001; Ekstrom, Goertz, Pollack, & Rock,
and none found a significant effect on high school 1986; Rumberger, 1983).
dropout or graduation.

Self-perceptions Combining Factors


To be successful in school, students not only must
value school, they must believe they are capable While it is useful to identify individual predictors
of achieving success. Students’ perceptions of of dropping out, it is also useful to understand
themselves and their abilities are a key com- how various factors jointly contribute to the pro-
ponent of achievement motivation and an impor- cess of dropping out. Researchers have used three
tant precursor of student engagement (NRC, approaches for combining factors: creating a
CIHSSEML, 2004). composite index of risk; creating taxonomies to
Research studies have examined a number of identify different types of dropouts; and testing
self-perceptions and their relationship to high structural models that link factors together.
school dropout and graduation. All of these per-
ceptions are constructed as composite measures Risk Factors
based on student responses to a number of ques- Instead of examining the effects of individual
tions about themselves. One such construct is student predictors on dropping out, a number of
self-concept. Self-concept is basically a person’s studies combined a series of factors into a com-
conception of himself or herself (Bong & posite index of risk. Some studies only included
Skaalvik, 2003). Although self-concept can be student factors (Connell, Halpern-Felsher, Cliffor,
viewed and measured as a general construct, Crichlow, & Usinger, 1995; Lee & Burkam,
scholars have come to realize that it is multidi- 1992), while other studies included both student
mensional and that it should be measured with and family factors (Benz, Lindstrom, & Yovanoff,
respect to a particular domain, such as academic 2000; Cabrera & La Nasa, 2001; Croninger &
self-concept or self-concept with respect to read- Lee, 2001). For example, one study based on a
ing. A related construct is self-esteem, which national longitudinal study of eighth grade stu-
measures self-assessments of qualities that are dents, created a “social risk” index based on five
504 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

demographic factors (poverty, language minority who demonstrated low commitment, average per-
status, ethnic minority, single-parent household, formance, and average to low level of school mis-
and having a dropout parent) and an “academic behavior; and (4) low-achiever dropouts (10%)
risk” index based on five school performance who demonstrated low commitment, very poor
factors (grades below C, retained between grades performance, average to low level of school mis-
2 and 8, educational expectations no greater than behavior, and very poor school performance
high school, sent to the office at least once in the (Janosz, LeBlanc, Boulerice, & Tremblay, 2000).
first semester, and parents notified at least once of
a problem with their child during the first semes- Structural Models
ter) (Croninger & Lee, 2001). The study found Another approach for examining how various
that about one third of the students had at least factors jointly influence the dropout process is to
one academic risk factor and those students were construct and test a structural model using a sta-
twice as likely to drop out as students with no tistical technique know as structural equation
academic risk factors, whereas having at least modeling (Kline, 2010). This technique allows
one social risk factor increased the odds of drop- researchers to examine the strength of the direct
ping out by 50% both for academically at-risk and indirect relationships among predictors and
student and students who were not academically to estimate how well the resulting model “fits”
at risk. Another study based on institutional data the data. One recent study estimated a model
from the Philadelphia Public Schools created an based on the NRC report, Engaging Schools,
index based on four factors measured in the sixth which suggested dropping out is influenced by
grade that indicated whether the student: (1) student engagement and a set of psychological
attended school 80% or less of the time; (2) failed antecedents (Rotermund, 2010). The study, based
math; (3) failed English; and (4) received an out- on a national longitudinal study of tenth grade
of-school suspension (Balfanz, Herzog, & Mac students who were tracked for 2 years, found that
Iver, 2007). This study found that odds of gradu- that only two tenth grade factors directly influ-
ating declined precipitously with each additional enced dropping out in high school: student
risk factor—one risk factor reduced the percent- achievement and behavioral engagement (not
age graduating by one-third, two risk factors absent, late, skipping classes, or getting into trou-
reduced the percentage by one-half, and three ble). Both behavioral engagement and cognitive
risk factors reduced the percentage by three- engagement (works hard, puts forth effort) also
fourths. All five studies in our review of the sta- influenced dropping out through their effects on
tistical research found that academic (and in some grades, while affective engagement (likes school,
cases academic and family) risk was a significant finds classes interesting and challenging) affected
predictor or whether students graduated or both cognitive and behavioral engagement.
dropped out of high school (Rumberger & Lim, Finally, three psychological antecedents—
2008, Table 2). perceived competence, valuing school and a
sense of belonging—influenced the three dimen-
Typologies sions of engagement. A number of other studies
Another approach is to use various factors to cre- have estimated models of dropping out using this
ate a typology that distinguishes different types or technique, including studies that incorporate pre-
profiles of dropouts. One study of high school dictors from early childhood (Archambault,
students in Montreal, Canada identified four types Janosz, Fallu, & Pagani, 2009; Battin-Pearson
of dropouts: (1) quiet dropouts (40% of dropouts) et al., 2000; Ensminger & Slusacick, 1992;
who demonstrated moderate to high levels of Garnier et al., 1997; Reynolds et al., 2004).
commitment and no evidence of school misbe- Together, these studies support the notion that
havior; (2) maladjusted dropouts (40%) who dropping out is indeed a long-term process influ-
demonstrated low commitment and poor school enced by a wide variety of factors, including
performance; (3) disengaged dropouts (10%) engagement factors.
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 505

Institutional Factors income (Perreira, Harris, & Lee, 2006; Rumberger,


1995; Rumberger & Lim, 2008, Table 3). There is
While a large array of individual attitudes, behav- less consistent evidence that family size matters.
iors, and aspects of educational performance
influence dropping out and graduating, these indi- Resources
vidual factors are shaped by the institutional set- Family resources are critical for supporting the
tings or contexts where children live—families, emotional, social, and cognitive development of
schools, and communities. Research has identi- children. The most widely used indicator of fam-
fied a number of factors within students’ families, ily resources is socioeconomic status (SES),
schools, and communities that influence whether which is typically constructed as a composite
students dropout or graduate from high school. index based on several measures of financial and
As in the case of individual factors, it is difficult human resources, such as both parents’ years of
to verify a causal connection between institu- education, both parents’ occupational status, and
tional factors and dropping out, but research has family income. A national study of high school
demonstrated that a number of factors affect the sophomores found that students from the lowest
odds that students will drop out or graduate from quartile of SES were five times more likely to
high school, as well as the antecedents to dropout drop out of school (12.4% v. 1.8%) as students
and graduation, including engagement factors. from the highest quartile of SES (Dalton et al.,
Due to space limitations, the review will only 2009, Table 1). The majority of studies in
highlight some of the research findings. Rumberger and Lim’s review found that students
from high SES families were less likely to drop
out than students from low SES families
Families (Rumberger & Lim, 2008, Table 3).

Family background has long been recognized as Practices


the single most important contributor to success in Fiscal and human resources simply represent the
school (Coleman et al., 1966; Jencks et al., 1972). means or the capacity to improve the engagement
What is less clear is what aspects of family back- and educational outcomes of children. This capac-
ground matter and how they influence school ity is realized through the actual practices and
achievement (Hoover-Dempsey & Sandler, 1997; behaviors of parents. These practices, manifested in
Pomerantz, Moorman, & Litwack, 2007). Although the relationships parents have with their children,
the research literature has identified a wide array their schools, and the communities, are what soci-
of family factors that contribute to dropping out, ologist James Coleman labeled social capital
three aspects appear to be most important: (1) (Coleman, 1988). Other researchers have labeled
structure, (2) resources, and (3) practices. such practices parental involvement or parenting
style (Fan & Chen, 2001; Jeynes, 2007; Pomerantz
Structure et al., 2007; Spera, 2005). Parenting practices related
Family structure generally refers to the number to student engagement include educational expecta-
and types of individuals in a child’s household. In tions (how much schooling they want or expect
2005, 67% of families in the United States with their children to get), within-home practices (gen-
children under 18 were married-couple families, eral supervision, helping with or monitoring home-
25% were female-headed families, and 8% were work), and home-school practices (participation in
male-headed families (KewalRamani, Gilbertson, school activities, communication with the school).
& Fox, 2007, Table 3). The research finds that Rumberger and Lim’s review did not reveal a
students living with both parents had lower drop- consistent, direct relationship between specific
out rates and higher graduation rates compared to parenting practices and school dropout. Yet
students living in other family living arrange- several studies found that multiple indicators of
ments, even controlling for the effects of family parenting practices at the secondary level reduced
506 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

the risk of dropping out. One early study of 1980 composition may affect student achievement in
high school sophomores found that four parent- two ways: first, by serving as a proxy for other
ing practices (as reported by the students) during characteristics of schools—for example, high-
high school had significant effects on whether poverty and high-minority schools generally
students dropped out or graduated: (1) whether have more inexperienced teachers and suffer
their mother wanted them to graduate from col- from high teacher turnover (Clotfelter, Ladd,
lege, (2) whether their mother monitored their Vigdor, & Wheeler, 2007; Hanushek, Kain, &
school progress, (3) whether their father moni- Rivkin, 2004; Reed, 2005); second, by influenc-
tored their school progress, and (4) whether their ing motivation, engagement, and achievement
parents supervised their school work (Astone & directly through interactions with peers (Jencks
McLanahan, 1991). Another study of eighth grad- & Mayer, 1990; Kahlenberg, 2001; Ryan, 2000).
ers from 1988 found four parenting practices A number of studies have found that several
that predicted whether students dropped out by dimensions of student composition—the average
grade 12: (1) parental educational aspirations for socioeconomic status of the students attending
their child in grade 8, (2) parental participation the school, the proportion of at-risk students (stu-
in school activities in grade 8, (3) parental dents who get poor grades, cut classes, have dis-
communication with the school in grade 12, cipline problems, or were retained) (Rumberger,
and (4) a measure of intergenerational closure— 1995; Rumberger & Thomas, 2000), the propor-
how many parents of their children’s friends tion of racial or linguistic minorities (Rumberger,
they know—which is a key component of social 1995; Sander, 2001), the proportion of students
capital that provides a source of information, who had changed schools or residences (Sander,
norms, expectations, and standards of behavior 2001) , the proportion of students from non-tradi-
(Carbonaro, 1998). tional (not both parents) families—affect dropout
rates above and beyond other school characteris-
tics (Bryk &Thum, 1989; McNeal, 1997;
Schools Rumberger, 1995; Rumberger & Palardy, 2005;
Rumberger &Thomas, 2000; Sander, 2001).
It is widely acknowledged that schools exert pow-
erful influences on student achievement, includ- Structure
ing dropout rates. Using statistical techniques to Several structural characteristics of schools con-
disentangle institutional factors from individual tribute to student performance—school location
factors, some studies have found that about (whether the school is located in an urban, subur-
20–25% of the variability in student outcomes ban, or rural location), school size, and type of
can be attributed to the characteristics of the school (public comprehensive, public charter,
schools that students attend (Li, 2007; Rumberger and private).
& Palardy, 2004). Nonetheless, as in the case of Research evidence is inconsistent on the rela-
individual factors, it is hard to verify a causal tionship between high school size and dropout or
relationship between school factors and dropout graduation rates. Some studies found that stu-
rates without using sophisticated statistical tech- dents were more inclined to drop out of large
niques (Pohl, Steiner, Eisermann, Soellner, & (greater than 1,200 and less than 1,500 students)
Cook, 2009; Schneider et al., 2007). Four school high schools (Lee & Burkam, 2003; Marsh, 1991;
factors have been shown to influence dropout and Rumberger &Palardy, 2005) other analyses found
graduation rates: (1) student composition (2) that students were less likely to drop out of large
structure, (3) resources, and (4) practices. schools (Pirog & Magee, 1997; Rumberger &
Thomas, 2000); and still others found that
Student Composition school size had no significant effects (Bryk
Student characteristics not only influence student &Thum, 1989; Grogger, 1997; McNeal, 1997;
achievement at an individual level, but also at an Pittman & Haughwout, 1987; Rumberger, 1995;
aggregate or social level (Gamoran, 1992). Social Sander, 2001; Van Dorn, Bowen, & Blau, 2006).
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 507

One reason for the mixed effects is that the rela- recent large-scale studies have found that some
tionship between school size and student out- charter schools outperform public schools and
comes may be non-linear, with middle-size some do not (Betts & Tang, 2008; Center for
schools (500–1,200 students) more effective than Research on Education Outcomes (CREDO),
either small or large schools (Lee & Burkam, 2009; Gleason, Clark, Tuttle, & Dwoyer, 2010;
2003). Another reason is that there may be offset- Zimmer et al., 2009), although one of the studies
ting features of schools associated with size, with found that in Florida and Chicago “attending a
large schools offering more curriculum and pro- charter high school is associated with statistically
gram options, but also having a poorer social cli- significant and substantial increases in the prob-
mate (Pittman & Haughwout, 1987). School size ability of graduating and attending college”
may have different and conflicting effects on dif- (Zimmer et al., 2009, pp. xii–xv).
ferent school outcomes; one recent study found
larger schools had greater improvement in stu- Resources
dent learning, perhaps because of curricular ben- Another area of considerable debate concerns the
efits, but they also had higher dropout rates, extent to which school resources contribute to
perhaps because of poorer climate (Rumberger & school effectiveness (Greenwald, Hedges, &
Palardy, 2005). Laine, 1996; Hanushek, 1989, 1994, 1997;
There is also mixed evidence on the effects of Hanushek & Jorgenson, 1996; Hedges, Laine, &
school type. Research has not found that private Greenwald, 1994). A number of studies have
schools as a whole have consistently higher grad- examined the relationship between various types
uation rates than public schools (Rumberger & of school resources—average expenditures per
Lim, 2008), but the evidence is stronger that drop- pupil, teacher salaries, the number of students
out rates are lower and graduation rates higher in per teacher, and measures of teacher quality, such
Catholic schools, even after controlling for stu- as the percentage of teachers with advanced
dent background characteristics and other school degrees—and dropout or graduation rates. Over-
inputs (Evans & Schwab, 1995; Rumberger & all, relatively few studies in Rumberger and Lim’s
Larson, 1998; Rumberger & Palardy, 2005; review found significant effects (Rumberger &
Rumberger & Thomas, 2000; Sander, 1997; Lim, 2008, Table 3). Several additional studies
Sander & Krautmann, 1995; Teachman, Paasch, that used district- and state-level data, along with
& Carver, 1997). Some of those studies also find more sophisticated statistical techniques to better
that the Catholic school effect is mediated by control for unobserved factors, found that higher
school practices, which further supports the claim per-pupil expenditures or higher teacher salaries
that Catholic schools provide a more rigorous aca- were associated with lower dropout rates (Li,
demic and supportive school environment com- 2007; Loeb & Page, 2000; Warren, Jenkins, &
pared to public and other private schools (Coleman Kulick, 2006). For example, one study that used a
& Hoffer, 1987; Lee & Burkam, 2003; Rumberger more sophisticated model of teacher salaries,
& Palardy, 2005; Rumberger & Thomas, 2000). which took into account the non-monetary job
Yet empirical studies have also found that students characteristics and alternative employment oppor-
from private and Catholic schools typically trans- tunities in the local job market (what economists
fer to public schools instead of or before dropping refer to as “opportunity costs”), found that raising
out, meaning that student turnover rates in private teacher wages by 10% reduced high school drop-
schools are not statistically different than turnover out rates by 3–4% (Loeb & Page, 2000).
rates in public schools (Lee & Burkam, 2003;
Rumberger & Thomas, 2000). Practices
The research is also mixed on the effective- School policies and practices affect student persis-
ness of charter schools—publicly funded schools tence in two ways. One way is through policies
run by for-profit and non-profit organizations and practices that lead to students’ disengaging
largely free from public school regulations. from school and eventual voluntary withdrawal—
A 2008 review of 14 studies as well as several either dropping out or transferring—from school.
508 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

The other way is through policies and conscious


decisions that cause students to involuntarily with-
Summary and Conclusion
draw from school through suspensions, expulsions,
Understanding why students drop out of high
or forced transfers (Bowditch, 1993; Fine, 1986,
school is critical for informing efforts to address
1991). Some scholars argue that the social rela-
this critical educational problem. Our review of
tionships or ties among students, parents, teachers,
conceptual models of dropping out and of the
and administrators—which have been character-
empirical research literature leads to several con-
ized as social resources or social capital—are a
clusions about why students drop out.
key component of effective and improving schools
First, no single factor can completely account
(Ancess, 2003; Bryk & Schneider, 2002; Elmore,
for a student’s decision to continue in school until
2004; Hoy, Tarter, & Hoy, 2006).
graduation. Just as students themselves report a
A number of indicators of school practices
variety of reasons for quitting school, the research
have been shown to influence dropout and gradu-
literature also identifies a number of salient fac-
ation rates. One study, which created a single
tors that appear to influence the decision.
composite indicator of school climate from stu-
Second, the decision to drop out is not simply a
dent responses to questions about various aspects
result of what happens in school. Clearly, students’
of the school, such as school loyalty and student
behavior and performance in school influence
behavior (i.e., fighting, cutting class), found that
their decision to stay or leave. But students’ activi-
a positive school climate reduced the likelihood
ties and behaviors outside of school—particularly
of dropping out, net of other factors (Worrell &
engaging in deviant and criminal behavior—also
Hale, 2001). Another study found that schools
influence their likelihood of remaining in school.
with higher attendance rates, a school-level mea-
Third, dropping out is more of a process than
sure of student engagement—had lower dropout
an event. For many students, the process begins in
rates (Rumberger & Thomas, 2000). Several
early elementary school. A number of long-term
studies found that students were less likely to
studies that tracked groups of students from pre-
drop out if they attended schools with a stronger
school or early elementary school through the end
academic climate, as measured by more students
of high school were able to identify early indica-
in the academic track (versus general or voca-
tors that could significantly predict whether stu-
tional) or taking academic courses, and students
dents were likely to drop out or finish high school.
reporting more hours of homework—another
The two most consistent indicators were early
school-level measure of engagement (Bryk &
academic performance and students’ academic
Thum, 1989; Lee & Burkam, 2003; Rumberger
and social engagement (behaviors) in school.
& Palardy, 2005). Some studies have found that
Fourth, contexts matter. The research litera-
students were more likely to drop out in schools
ture has identified a number of factors within
with a poor disciplinary climate, as measured by
families, schools, and communities that affect
student reports of student disruptions in class or
whether students are likely to drop out or gradu-
discipline problems in the school (Pittman, 1991;
ate from high school. These include access to not
Rumberger, 1995; Rumberger & Palardy, 2005),
only fiscal and material resources, but also social
or in schools where students reported feeling
resources in the form of supportive relationships
unsafe (Bryk & Thum, 1989; Pittman, 1991;
in families, schools, and communities.
Rumberger, 1995; Rumberger & Palardy, 2005).
Finally, there is no single model that fully
Several studies have found that positive relation-
explains the dropout process for all dropouts.
ships between students and teachers—an aspect
Rather, because students drop out of school for
of school social capital—reduced the risk of
different reasons, some factors are more salient
dropping out, especially among high-risk stu-
for some students than for others. For example,
dents (Croninger & Lee, 2001; Rumberger &
boys are more likely to drop out because of
Palardy, 2005).
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 509

behavior problems, while girls are more likely to Balfanz, R., Herzog, L., & Mac Iver, D. J. (2007).
“silently” disengage from school by skipping Preventing student disengagement and keeping stu-
dents on the graduation path in urban middle-grades
class or being absent. schools: Early identification and effective interven-
One implication of this review is that there tions. Educational Psychologist, 42, 223–235.
are numerous leverage points for addressing the Barnett, W. S., & Belfield, C. R. (2006). Early childhood
problem of high dropout rates. Clearly, early development and social mobility. The Future of
Children, 16, 73–98.
intervention in preschool and early elementary Battin-Pearson, S., Newcomb, M. D., Abbott, R. D., Hill,
school is warranted. Rigorous experimental K. G., Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (2000).
evaluations have proven that high quality pre- Predictors of early high school dropout: A test of five
school programs and small classes in early ele- theories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92,
568–582.
mentary school improve high school graduation Belfield, C., & Levin, H. M. (Eds.). (2007). The price we
rates (Barnett & Belfield, 2006; Finn, Gerber, & pay: Economic and social consequences of inadequate
Boyd-Zaharias, 2005). Such programs are also education. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution
cost-effective—they generate two to four dollars Press.
Benz, M. R., Lindstrom, L., & Yovanoff, P. (2000).
in economic benefits for every dollar invested Improving graduation and employment outcomes of
(Belfield & Levin, 2007). But there are other students with disabilities: Predictive factors and stu-
leverage points as well. Even high school is not dent perspectives. Exceptional Children, 66, 509–529.
too late—both small programs serving a limited Betts, J. R., & Tang, Y. E. (2008). Value-added and exper-
imental studies of the effect of charter schools on stu-
number of high-risk students and comprehensive dent achievement. Seattle, WA: University of
school reform models have been proven to Washington Bothell, Center on Reinventing Public
improve graduation rates (Ibid.). Education, National Charter School Research Project.
Retrieved August 19, 2010, from http://www.crpe.org/
cs/crpe/view/csr_pubs/253
Bong, M., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2003). Academic self-con-
References cept and self-efficacy: How different are they really?
Educational Psychology Review, 15, 1–40.
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Kabbini, N. S. Bowditch, C. (1993). Getting rid of troublemakers: High
(2001). The dropout process in life course perspective: school disciplinary procedures and the production of
Early risk factors at home and school. Teachers dropouts. Social Problems, 40, 493–509.
College Record, 103, 760–882. Bridgeland, J. M., DiIulio, J. J., Jr., & Morison, K. B.
Allensworth, E., & Easton, J. Q. (2005). The on-track indi- (2006). The silent epidemic: Perspectives on high
cator as a predictor of high school graduation. Chicago: school dropouts. Washington, DC: Civil Enterprises.
Consortium on Chicago School Research, University of Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human devel-
Chicago. Retrieved December 20, 2010, from http:// opment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
ccsr.uchicago.edu/content/publications.php?pub_id=10 Bryk, A. S., & Schneider, B. (2002). Trust in schools:
Ancess, J. (2003). Beating the odds: High schools as com- A core resource for improvement. New York: Russell
munities of commitment. New York: Teachers College Sage.
Press. Bryk, A. S., & Thum, Y. M. (1989). The effects of high
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. school organization on dropping out: An exploratory
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- investigation. American Educational Research
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Journal, 26, 353–383.
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. Cabrera, A. F., & La Nasa, S. M. (2001). On the path to
Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Fallu, J.-S., & Pagani, L. S. college: Three critical tasks facing America’s disad-
(2009). Student engagement and its relationship with vantaged. Research in Higher Education, 42,
early high school dropout. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 119–149.
651–670. Cahalan, M. W., Ingles, S. J., Burns, L. J., Planty, M., &
Astone, N. M., & McLanahan, S. S. (1991). Family struc- Daniel, B. (2006). United States high school sopho-
ture, parental practices and high school completion. mores: A twenty-two year comparison, 1980-2002.
American Sociological Review, 56, 309–320. (NCES 2006-327). Washington, DC: National Center
Bachman, J. G., Green, S., & Wirtanen, I. D. (1971). Youth for Education Statistics.
in transition, Vol. III: Dropping out: Problem or symp- Carbonaro, W. J. (1998). A little help from my friend’s
tom? Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, parents: Intergenerational closure and educational out-
The University of Michigan. comes. Sociology of Education, 71, 295–313.
510 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

Center for Research on Education Outcomes (CREDO). Ekstrom, R. B., Goertz, M. E., Pollack, J. M., & Rock, D. A.
(2009). Multiple choice: Charter school performance (1986). Who drops out of high school and why?
in 16 states. Stanford, CA: CREDO, Stanford Findings from a national study. Teachers College
University. Retrieved December 20, 2010, from http:// Record, 87, 356–373.
credo.stanford.edu/ Elmore, R. F. (2004). School reform from the inside out.
Clotfelter, C. T., Ladd, H. F., Vigdor, J. L., & Wheeler, J. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press.
(2007). High poverty schools and the distribution of Ensminger, M. E., & Slusacick, A. L. (1992). Paths to
teachers and principals. North Carolina Law Review, high school graduation or dropout: A longitudinal
85, 1345–1379. study of a first-grade cohort. Sociology of Education,
Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of 65, 95–113.
human capital. American Journal of Sociology, 94, Evans, G. W. (2004). The environment of childhood pov-
S95–S120. erty. American Psychologist, 59, 77–92.
Coleman, J. S., Campbell, E. Q., Hobson, C. J., McPartland, Evans, W. N., & Schwab, R. M. (1995). Finishing high
J., Mood, A. M., Weinfeld, F. D., & York, R. L. (1966). school and starting college: Do Catholic schools make
Equality of educational opportunity. Washington, DC: a difference? The Quarterly Journal of Economics,
U.S. Government Printing Office. 110, 941–974.
Coleman, J. S., & Hoffer, T. (1987). Public and private Fan, X., & Chen, M. (2001). Parental involvement and
high schools: The impact of communities. New York: students’ academic achievement: A meta-analysis.
Basic Books. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 1–22.
Connell, J. P. (1990). Context, self and action: A motiva- Fine, M. (1986). Why urban adolescents drop into and out
tional analysis of self-system processes across the life of public high school. Teachers College Record, 87,
span. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in 393–409.
transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 61–97). Chicago: Fine, M. (1991). Framing dropouts: Notes on the politics
University of Chicago Press. of an urban public high school. Albany, NY: State
Connell, J. P., Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Cliffor, E., Crichlow, University of New York Press.
W., & Usinger, P. (1995). Hanging in there: Behavioral, Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
psychological, and contextual factors affecting Educational Research, 59, 117–142.
whether African-American adolescents stay in high Finn, J. D., Gerber, S. B., & Boyd-Zaharias, J. (2005).
school. Journal of Adolescent Research, 10, 41–63. Small classes in the early grades, academic achieve-
Croninger, R. G., & Lee, V. E. (2001). Social capital and ment, and graduating from high school. Journal of
dropping out of high school: Benefits to at-risk stu- Educational Psychology, 97, 214–223.
dents of teachers’ support and guidance. Teachers Flores-Gonzalez, N. (2002). School kids/street kids:
College Record, 103, 548–581. Identity development in Latino students. New York:
Currie, J. (2009). Healthy, wealthy, and wise: Teachers College Press.
Socioeconomic status, poor health in childhood, and Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
human capital development. Journal of Economic School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
Literature, 47, 87–122. the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
Dalton, B., Glennie, E., & Ingels, S. J. (2009). Late high 59–109.
school dropouts: Characteristics, experiences, and Gamoran, A. (1992). Social factors in education. In M. C.
changes across cohorts. (NCES 2009-307). Alkin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Educational Research
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. (pp. 1222–1229). New York: Macmillan.
Retrieved December 20, 2010, from http://nces.ed. Garnier, H. E., Stein, J. A., & Jacobs, J. K. (1997). The
gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2009307 process of dropping out of high school: A 19-year per-
Day, J. C., & Newburger, E. C. (2002). The big payoff: spective. American Educational Research Journal, 34,
Educational attainment and synthetic estimates of 395–419.
work-life earnings. Washington, DC: U.S. Census Gleason, P., Clark, M., Tuttle, C. C., & Dwoyer, E. (2010).
Bureau. The evaluation of charter school impacts: Final report.
Eccles, J. S. (1999). The development of children ages 6 (NCEE 2010-4029). Washington, DC: National Center
to 14. The Future of Children, 9, 30–44. for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance,
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of
values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, Education. Retrieved August 20, 2010, from http://ies.
109–132. ed.gov/ncee/pubs/20104029/index.asp
Edin, K., & Kefalas, M. (2005). Promises I can keep: Why Greenwald, R., Hedges, L. V., & Laine, R. D. (1996). The
poor women put motherhood before marriage. effect of school resources on student achievement.
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Review of Educational Research, 66, 361–396.
Education Week. (2010, June 10). Diplomas Count 2010: Grogger, J. (1997). Local violence and educational attain-
Graduation by the numbers: Putting Data to work for ment. The Journal of Human Resources, 32, 659–682.
student success. Washington, DC: Education Week. Hanushek, E. A. (1989). The impact of differential expen-
Retrieved August 28, 2010, from http://www.edweek. ditures on school performance. Educational Researcher,
org/ew/toc/2010/06/10/index.html 18, 45–62.
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 511

Hanushek, E. A. (1994). Money might matter somewhere: Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (1992). Transferring high
A response to Hedges, Laine, and Greenwald. schools: An alternative to dropping out? American
Educational Researcher, 23(4), 5–8. Journal of Education, 100, 420–453.
Hanushek, E. A. (1997). Assessing the effects of school Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (2003). Dropping out of
resources on student performance: An update. high school: The role of school organization and
Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 19, structure. American Educational Research Journal,
141–164. 40, 353–393.
Hanushek, E. A., & Jorgenson, D. W. (Eds.). (1996). Lerner, R. M., & Galambos, N. L. (1998). Adolescent
Improving America’s schools: The role of incentives. development: Challenges and opportunities for
Washington, DC: National Academy Press. research, programs, and policies. Annual Review of
Hanushek, E. A., Kain, J. F., & Rivkin, S. G. (2004). Why Psychology, 49, 413–46.
public schools lose teachers. Journal of Human Li, M. (2007). Bayesian proportional hazard analysis of
Resources, 39, 326–354. the timing of high school dropout decisions.
Heckman, J. J., & LaFontaine, P. A. (2010). The American Econometric Reviews, 26, 529–556.
high school graduation rate: Trends and levels. The Loeb, S., & Page, M. E. (2000). Examining the link
Review of Economics and Statistics, 92, 244–262. between teacher wages and student outcomes: The
Hedges, L. V., Laine, R. D., & Greenwald, R. (1994). importance of alternative labor market opportunities
Does money matter? A meta-analysis of studies of the and non-pecuniary variation. The Review of Economics
effects of differential school inputs on student out- and Statistics, 82, 393–408.
comes. Educational Researcher, 23(3), 5–14. Marsh, H. W. (1991). Employment during high school:
Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., & Sandler, H. M. (1997). Why Character building or a subversion of academic goals.
do parents become involved in their children’s educa- Sociology of Education, 64, 172–189.
tion? Review of Educational Research, 67, 3–42. McNeal, R. B. (1995). Extracurricular activities and high
Hoy, W. K., Tarter, C. J., & Hoy, A. W. (2006). Academic school dropouts. Sociology of Education, 68, 62–80.
optimism of schools: A force for student achievement. McNeal, R. B. (1997). High school dropouts: A closer
American Educational Research Journal, 43, examination of school effects. Social Science
425–446. Quarterly, 78, 209–222.
Janosz, M., LeBlanc, M., Boulerice, B., & Tremblay, R. E. Meyer, R., Carl, B., & Cheng, H. E. (2010). Accountability
(2000). Predicting different types of school dropouts: and performance in secondary education in Milwaukee
A typological approach with two longitudinal cohorts. public schools. Washington, DC: Council of Great
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 171–190. City Schools.
Jencks, C., & Mayer, S. E. (1990). The social conse- National Research Council, Panel on High-Risk Youth.
quences of growing up in a poor neighborhood. In L. (1993). Losing generations: Adolescents in high-risk
Lynn Jr. & M. G. H. McGeary (Eds.), Inner-City Pov- settings. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
erty in the United States (pp. 111–186). Washington, National Research Council, Committee on Increasing
DC: National Academy Press. High School Students’ Engagement and Motivation to
Jencks, C., Smith, M., Acland, H., Bane, M. J., Cohen, D., Learn. (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering high
Gintis, H., Heyns, B., & Michelson, S. M. (1972). school students’ motivation to learn. Washington, DC:
Inequality: A reassessment of the effects of family and The National Academies Press.
schooling in America. New York: Basic Books. Newmann, F. M. (Ed.). (1992). Student engagement and
Jeynes, W. H. (2007). The relationship between paren- achievement in American secondary schools. New
tal involvement and urban secondary school student York: Teachers College Press.
academic achievement—A meta-analysis. Urban Ou, S.-R., Mersky, J. P., Reynolds, A. J., & Kohler, K. M.
Education, 42, 82–110. (2007). Alterable predictors of educational attainment,
Kahlenberg, R. D. (2001). All together now: Creating income, and crime: Findings from an inner-city cohort.
middle-class schools through public school choice. Social Service Review, 81, 85–128.
Washington, DC: Brookings Institution. Perreira, K. M., Harris, K. M., & Lee, D. (2006). Making
Kelly, D. M. (1993). Last chance high: How girls and it in America: High school completion by immigrant
boys drop in and out of alternative schools. New and native youth. Demography, 43, 511–536.
Haven: Yale University Press. Pettit, B., & Western, B. (2004). Mass imprisonment and
KewalRamani, A., Gilbertson, L., & Fox, M. A. (2007). the life course: Race and class inequality in US incar-
Status and trends in the education of racial and ethnic ceration. American Sociological Review, 69, 151–169.
minorities. (NCES 2007-039). Washington, DC: Pirog, M. A., & Magee, C. (1997). High school comple-
National Center for Education Statistics. Retrieved tion: The influence of schools, families, and adoles-
December 20, 2010, from: http://nces.ed.gov/pub- cent parenting. Social Science Quarterly, 78,
search/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2007039 710–724.
Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural Pittman, R. B. (1991). Social factors, enrollment in voca-
equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford tional/technical courses, and high school dropout
Publications. rates. Journal of Educational Research, 84, 288–295.
512 R.W. Rumberger and S. Rotermund

Pittman, R. B., & Haughwout, P. (1987). Influence of high Santa Barbara, CA: California Dropout Research
school size on dropout rate. Educational Evaluation Project. Retrieved December 20, 2010, from http://
and Policy Analysis, 9, 337–343. cdrp.ucsb.edu/dropouts/pubs_reports.htm#15
Pohl, S., Steiner, P. M., Eisermann, J., Soellner, R., & Rumberger, R. W., & Palardy, G. J. (2004). Multilevel
Cook, T. D. (2009). Unbiased causal inference from an models for school effectiveness research. In D. Kaplan
observational study: Results of a within-study com- (Ed.), Handbook of quantitative methodology for the
parison. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, social sciences (pp. 235–258). Thousand Oaks, CA:
31, 463–479. Sage Publications.
Pomerantz, E. M., Moorman, E. A., & Litwack, S. D. Rumberger, R. W., & Palardy, G. J. (2005). Test scores,
(2007). The how, whom, and why of parents’ involve- dropout rates, and transfer rates as alternative indica-
ment in children’s academic lives: More is not always tors of high school performance. American Educational
better. Review of Educational Research, 77, 373–410. Research Journal, 41, 3–42.
Pribesh, S., & Downey, D. B. (1999). Why are residential Rumberger, R. W., & Thomas, S. L. (2000). The distribu-
and school moves associated with poor school perfor- tion of dropout and turnover rates among urban and
mance? Demography, 36, 521–534. suburban high schools. Sociology of Education, 73,
Ream, R. K. (2005). Uprooting children: Mobility, social 39–67.
capital, and Mexican American underachievement. Ryan, A. M. (2000). Peer groups as a context for the
New York: LFB Scholarly Publishing. socialization of adolescents’ motivation, engagement,
Ream, R. K., & Rumberger, R. W. (2008). Student engage- and achievement in school. Educational Psychologist,
ment, peer social capital, and school dropout among 35, 101–111.
Mexican American and non-Latino white students. Sander, W. (1997). Catholic high schools and rural aca-
Sociology of Education, 81, 109–139. demic achievement. American Journal of Agricultural
Reed, D. (2005). Educational resources and outcomes in Economics, 79, 1–12.
California, by race and ethnicity. San Francisco: Sander, W. (2001). Chicago public schools and student
Public Policy Institute of California. achievement. Urban Education, 36(1), 27–38.
Reynolds, A. J., Ou, S.-R., & Topitzes, J. W. (2004). Paths Sander, W., & Krautmann, A. C. (1995). Catholic schools,
of effects of early childhood intervention on educa- dropout rates and educational attainment. Economic
tional attainment and delinquency: A confirmatory Inquiry, 33, 217–233.
analysis of the Chicago Child-Parent Centers. Child Schneider, B., Carnoy, M., Kilpatrick, J., Schmidt, W. H.,
Development, 75, 1299–1328. & Shavelson, R. J. (2007). Estimating causal effects
Romo, H. D., & Falbo, T. (1996). Latino high school using experimental and observations designs. Report
graduation: Defying the odds. Austin, TX: University from the Governing Board of the American Educational
of Texas Press. Research Association Grants Program. Washington,
Rotermund, S. L. (2010). The role of psychological ante- DC: American Educational Research Association.
cedents and student engagement in a process model of Snyder, T. D., & Dillow, S. A. (2010). Digest of Education
high school dropout. Doctoral dissertation. Santa Statistics 2009. (NCES 2010-013). U.S. Department
Barbara, CA: Gevirtz Graduate School of Education, of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.
University of California, Santa Barbara. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Rumberger, R. W. (1983). Dropping out of high school: Retrieved December 20, 2010 from http://nces.ed.gov/
The influence of race, sex, and family background. pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2010013
American Educational Research Journal, 20, Spera, C. (2005). A review of the relationship among par-
199–220. enting practices, parenting styles, and adolescent
Rumberger, R. W. (1987). High school dropouts: A review school achievement. Educational Psychological
of issues and evidence. Review of Educational Review, 17, 120–146.
Research, 57, 101–121. Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Adolescent devel-
Rumberger, R. W. (1995). Dropping out of middle school: opment. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 83–110.
A multilevel analysis of students and schools. American Sullivan, M. L. (1989). “Getting paid”: Youth crime and work
Educational Research Journal, 32, 583–625. in the inner city. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Rumberger, R. W. (2011). Dropping out: Why students Sweeten, G. (2006). Who will graduate? Disruption of
quit school and what can be done about it. Cambridge, high school education by arrest and court involve-
MA: Harvard University Press. ment. Justice Quarterly, 23, 462–480.
Rumberger, R. W., & Larson, K. A. (1998). Student Teachman, J. D., Paasch, K., & Carver, K. (1997). Social
mobility and the increased risk of high school drop capital and the generation of human capital. Social
out. American Journal of Education, 107, 1–35. Forces, 75, 1343–1359.
Rumberger, R. W., Larson, K. A., Ream, R. K., & Palardy, Tinto, V. (1987). Leaving college: Rethinking the causes
G. A. (1999). The educational consequences of mobil- and cures for student attrition. Chicago: University of
ity for California Students and Schools. Berkeley, CA: Chicago Press.
Policy Analysis for California Education. Tinto, V. (1994). Leaving college: Rethinking the causes
Rumberger, R. W., & Lim, S. A. (2008). Why students and cures for student attrition (2nd ed.). Chicago:
drop out of school: A review of 25 years of research. University of Chicago Press.
24 The Relationship Between Engagement and High School Dropout 513

Valenzuela, A. (1999). Subtractive schooling: U.S.-Mexican Wehlage, G. G., Rutter, R. A., Smith, G. A., Lesko, N., &
youth and the politics of caring. Albany, NY: State Fernandez, R. R. (1989). Reducing the risk: Schools as
University of New York Press. communities of support. New York: Falmer Press.
Van Dorn, R. A., Bowen, G. L., & Blau, J. R. (2006). The Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged: The inner
impact of community diversity and consolidated city, the underclass, and public policy. Chicago: The
inequality on dropping out of high school. Family University of Chicago Press.
Relations, 55, 105–118. Worrell, F. C., & Hale, R. L. (2001). The relationship of
Walston, J., Rathbun, A., & Huasken, E. G. (2008). Eighth hope in the future and perceived school climate to
grade: First findings from the final round of the Early school completion. School Psychology Quarterly, 16,
Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 370–388.
1998-99 (ECLS-K). Washington, DC: U.S. Department Yin, Z. N., & Moore, J. B. (2004). Re-examining the role
of Education. Retrieved December 20, 2010, from http:// of interscholastic sport participation in education.
nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2008088 Psychological Reports, 94, 1447–1454.
Warren, J. R., Jenkins, K. N., & Kulick, R. B. (2006). Zimmer, R., Gill, B., Booker, K., Lavertu, S., Sass, T. R.,
High school exit examinations and state-level comple- & Witte, J. (2009). Charter schools in eight states:
tion and GED rates, 1975-2002. Educational Effects on achievement, attainment, integration, and
Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 28, 131–152. competition. Santa Monica, CA: Rand.
High School Reform and Student
Engagement 25
Marcia H. Davis and James M. McPartland

Abstract
This chapter describes how internal high school reforms can be aimed at
six different dimensions of student motivation and engagement. Students
will respond to more accessible immediate rewards such as good grades
and teacher praise when high schools improve with focused extra help for
needy students and other interventions to narrow skill gaps or recognize
individual progress. Students will benefit from embedded intrinsic interest
in their school program when innovations are introduced to challenge their
minds and creativity. Students will find more functional relevance in their
studies when high schools integrate academic and career education.
Students will enjoy a more positive interpersonal climate for learning
when high schools use smaller learning communities with teacher teams
and advisors. Students will find opportunities to exercise their own per-
sonal nonacademic talents when schools provide more diverse electives
and extracurricular activities. Students will feel more connected to shared
communal norms when high schools practice fair disciplinary procedures
and provide for some shared decision-making. Different combinations and
sequences of high school reforms are discussed in terms of implementa-
tion strategies and the interactions of the six dimensions of student moti-
vation and engagement. High school reform can be aimed at either the
external constraints and incentives for school improvement or the internal
conditions for student engagement and learning. This chapter puts reforms
of the internal conditions in the context of alternative strategies for improv-
ing American high schools and examines six different aspects of student
engagement in high school and how specific internal reform efforts can
activate and maximize each component.

M.H. Davis (*) • J.M. McPartland


Center for Social Organization of Schools,
Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
e-mail: marcy@jhu.edu; jmcpartland@jhu.edu

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 515


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_25, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
516 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

positively motivated if he or she looks forward to


External and Internal Conditions the experience and expects to do well and benefit
of High School Reform from the participation. On the other hand, a stu-
dent may have no prior attitudes going into an
Most high school reform proposals focus on the
experience or may approach it in negative ways
external conditions that set the boundaries and
such as fear of failure or unpleasant outcomes.
incentives for improved student achievement
There are numerous ways that student motivation
(Clark, 2009), when changes within the school
may develop the expectations about a new oppor-
and classroom walls can be more closely linked
tunity or challenge, including from past experi-
to student engagement with their school program
ences in similar situations, through formal or
and their motivation to learn (American Youth
informal preparatory activities, or by way of social
Policy Forum, 2000; National Association of
influences from friends or those in authority.
Secondary School Principals, 1996, 2002;
Student engagement occurs during the actual
National Research Council and the Institute of
experience of an activity or event. Engagement is
Medicine, 2004).
positive when the individual finds the experience
External conditions for reforms include paren-
to be enjoyable, is successful in meeting the
tal choice of public or charter schools, high stan-
demands and fulfilling the roles, or appreciates
dards of curriculum coverage and associated
the value to be derived for personal goals and ful-
achievement testing, direct accountability of
fillment. Student engagement can sometimes be a
school principals and teachers for student suc-
mixed bag of both positive and negative dimen-
cess, and the reallocation of financial resources to
sions with a particular experience, where the net
schools with different student populations and
balance can determine whether the individual
needs. While these issues of school governance,
persists in the activity with strong energy or grad-
parental choice, district teacher and union pre-
ually withdraws emotionally or physically.
rogatives, assessments and their consequences,
Student motivation and engagement can inter-
and curriculum coverage can set the terms and
act in different ways. An individual who
incentives for how different adult interests and
approaches a task or activity with strong positive
stakeholders may address high school reform,
motivation will be more alert to seek out and
evidence is still weak connecting them to actual
appreciate those positive aspects of actual partici-
improvements in student outcomes. In contrast to
pation that can result in powerful engagement for
such external policy levers, students themselves
high energy and sustained commitment. On the
are more likely to be responsive to how the rela-
other hand, a well-motivated student’s expecta-
tionships and daily learning activities within their
tions may be either disappointed or frustrated
schools and classrooms are presented. Indeed,
when the actual experience occurs, so engage-
high school reforms will touch student motiva-
ment is weak and future motivation is threatened.
tion and engagement only when their proximate
At the same time, strong positive student
learning environment is improved.
engagement need not assume a parallel positive
motivation going into the activity. A student with-
out any vivid preconceptions can take the experi-
Motivation and Engagement ence on its own terms and decide to become
positively engaged or not depending upon how
We make a distinction between student motiva- well the actual activities offer satisfactions, can
tion and engagement according to the timing rel- be handled successfully, or excite other worth-
ative to a particular event or activity. Student while payoffs. Even a student who approaches an
motivation occurs before participation as a activity with negative motivation by expecting
precursor to the actual experience. It involves difficulties or punishments may be pleasantly
students’ anticipations in terms of potential enjoy- surprised that the participation itself proves to
ment, challenge, or usefulness. A student will be be the opposite and becomes engaged with
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 517

the enjoyment, fulfillment or follow-up benefits Table 25.1 Dimensions of student engagement and asso-
actually experienced. ciated high school reforms
Dimension of
student motivation
or engagement Associated high school reforms
Framework of Reforms
1. Accessible – Different course sequences
and Engagement Immediate without tracking
Awards – Levels of focused extra help
We will use a framework of six dimensions of – Multiple criteria for grades and
student motivation and engagement that can be recognition
2. Embedded – Thinking skills for problem
activated by certain high school reforms that
Intrinsic solving and influence
either prepare students for or offer them actual Interest – Content literacy and disciplinary
opportunities to experience different sources of thinking
satisfaction, stimulation, or benefits. – Project-based learning
We will consider how high school reforms can 3. Direct – Career Academies or majors
Functional – Integration of academic and
address six dimensions of student motivation and Relevance career education
engagement: 4. Positive – Small learning communities
1. Immediate rewards for school work are made Interpersonal – Interdisciplinary teacher teams
more accessible for all students, such as good Climate – Adult mentors and advisors
grades, respect and recognition from teachers, 5. Alternative – Career and nonacademic skill
Talent explorations
and timely grade promotions and graduation. Development – Elective courses
2. Intrinsic interest is embedded in more learn- – Extracurricular activities
ing tasks, so students get a sense of accom- 6. Shared – Firm and fair rules
plishment and self-improvement from their Communal – Student participation in
classroom activities. Engagement decision-making
– Trust and self-regulating norms
3. Functional relevance of the curriculum and
program of studies is strengthened, so students
appreciate how working hard at their studies
can pay off for personal interests and career
We will use examples from the Talent
goals.
Development High School model, a comprehen-
4. A positive climate for learning with good
sive reform approach developed at Johns Hopkins
adult-student relationships is established, so
University, to show how different organizational,
students look forward to coming to school
instructional, and governance improvements
each day in a safe, serious, and sensitive
relate to dimensions of student motivation and
environment.
engagement.
5. Personal nonacademic strengths are explored
and reinforced at school, so that the full range
of human talents can also flourish in high
school along with the strictly academic sub- Immediate Rewards for School
jects and pursuits. Success Become Accessible
6. A positive climate of trust in school gover- to All Students
nance and fairness of disciplinary rules exists,
so students feel part of a shared community The biggest barrier to students’ engagement with
with appropriate responsibilities and decision- their high school program is likely to be their
making opportunities. failure to obtain good or even passing grades in
Table 25.1 outlines the kinds of high school their courses and the other immediate rewards
reforms we will discuss that are associated attached to success at school work. Missing out
with each dimension of student motivation or on the positive incentives of good course grades
engagement. can become accompanied by absence of teacher’s
518 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

esteem for the student, unwelcome pressure from increase, students will place more value on these
home, and lower status from some peers. Repeated immediate incentives and their pursuit.
failures in their courses can leave students in a Three high school reforms are approaches to
state of learned helplessness, where they believe make immediate school success and the associ-
that they will not be able to improve no matter ated good grades and positive esteem as a learner
how hard they try (Mark, 1983). When course more accessible to every student. Students’
failures build up, the negative consequences of chances of doing well at school work to earn pos-
being left back to repeat a grade in high school itive immediate rewards can be improved both by
often leads to dropping out, the ultimate case more transitional courses as preparation for high-
of student disengagement. Research on Philadel- standards requirements and also by focused extra
phia data indicated that students who failed help when needed to pass high-standards courses.
English in the sixth grade were 42% less likely to In addition, grading practices themselves may be
graduate than students who did not fail English. modified to reward elements of student success
Students who failed math in sixth grade were that now go unrecognized.
54% less likely to graduate (Balfanz, Herzog, &
Mac Iver, 2007).
But even when students manage to get by with Transitional Preparation Courses
mostly low grades, the daily struggle to keep up Without Tracking
with the school learning standards can be a sig-
nificant source of discouragement and alienation. Many students enter high school poorly prepared
Each time these struggling students put some for a high-standards curriculum. They are not
effort to manage the material in their class they ready for Algebra in mathematics and classic
are often presented with poor grades for their novels and plays in English, as well as demand-
efforts. Within a behavioral learning theory view, ing content in history and science. In the past,
following the Operant Conditioning framework high schools used program tracking to deal with
of B.F. Skinner, the poor grades serve as a pun- student diversity of prior preparations. Under
ishment for not turning in work at the level of the tracking, only selected students would enter an
other students in the class (Skinner, 1953). Academic or College Preparatory program with
Punishments work to decrease the behavior pre- all high-standards course offerings. Those stu-
ceding the punishments. Therefore, poor grades dents who did not qualify for the top track would
will decrease the behavior of the students, which, enter a Vocational or Business program where
in this case is trying to honestly complete an many occupational courses took up the schedule,
assignment. After their efforts are paired with the or a General program where less demanding aca-
consequence of bad grades often enough, they demic courses were taken, such as Consumer
will eventually change their behavior to avoid Math instead of Algebra and Remedial English
feeling bad about receiving poor grades. This can emphasizing basic grammar skills rather than lit-
show up as weak daily attendance, as struggling erature appreciation. But such comprehensive
students choose to avoid the negative experi- program tracking is no longer part of educational
ences of being a weak student. Instead of always policy, since limited exposure to a challenging
receiving poor grades as a consequence of try- curriculum has been shown to be a major fac-
ing, teachers should be setting up types of posi- tor in poor student achievement test scores and
tive reinforcement for even small attempts at is seen as an unfair lack of equal educational
understanding their course material. By giving opportunities (Lucas, 1999; Oakes, Gamoran, &
these students time to understand the material Page, 1992). Further, tracking has been shown
and by giving opportunities to show their under- to exacerbate racial and ethnic achievement
standing of the material in multiple ways, their gaps (Burris, Wiley, Welner, & Murphy, 2008;
attempts at understanding will be reinforced Chambers, Hugins, & Scheurich, 2009; Gamoran,
rather than punished. As positive reinforcements Nystrand, Berends, & LePore, 1995).
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 519

Yet, the problems of major student differences both the conceptual understandings and procedural
in academic preparation for high school remain, fluencies that are the prerequisites for success
with the prospects for extensive student course in the high-standards requirements. In English,
failures when a high-standards curriculum is the transition course takes into account students’
required for all (Balfanz, McPartland, & Shaw, current reading levels by using high-interest low-
2002). A reform is to revise the curriculum sched- frustration novels, short stories, and nonfiction to
uling so needy students can get more class time practice comprehension strategies before, during,
to succeed at the required high-standards courses, and after reading. A study of ten matched pairs of
often with transition courses to address prerequi- high schools in Philadelphia found statistically
site skill gaps taken before the required courses significant positive benefits from transition courses
are attempted (Balfanz et al., 2002). on ninth grade students’ mathematics achievement
This approach is often called the “double gains with a 10% decrease in students scoring
dose” of math or English courses, where twice as below basic on their eleventh grade scores on the
much time is scheduled in these core academic Pennsylvania System of School Assessment com-
subjects. The extra time can include a “transi- pared to a 4% decrease in control schools (Kemple,
tion” course that precedes and prepares students Herlihy, & Smith, 2005). An evaluation of the
for success in the required courses, or added time ninth grade transition courses in English with 64
can “stretch” the required course time itself so a high schools across the nation showed a statisti-
teacher can interrupt with background learning cally significant moderate positive relationship
when necessary or adjust to the actual learning between implementation quality of the program
pace of the class. Research has found small posi- and reading comprehension gains over a school
tive benefits on student learning gains and course year (Davis & McPartland, 2009).
pass rates from adding transition courses or from Since not all students or all schools may need
increasing the time to cover a core course a transition course in high school math or English,
(Gamoran, Porter, Smithson, & White, 1997). there are concerns that this approach is just
Another study to compare transition courses with another form of unfair tracking when students are
stretch courses in algebra is underway with early separated into groups with different need levels.
indications that both approaches have similar But no student is being withheld from the oppor-
beneficial effects on success in algebra grades tunity to learn the same high-standards math or
and achievement tests, while students receiving English curriculum, which is the usual definition
transition-course experiences had the highest of program tracking. The grouping of students
gain in general math skills and knowledge (Neild, for transition courses is aimed to give all a better
Byrnes, & Sweet, 2010). chance to succeed at high standards after prepara-
One example of using transition courses is the tion gaps are first addressed. Moreover, when an
Talent Development High School reform model. entire school is scheduled with double-dose offer-
This model was initially developed in 1992 in ings in English and math, because all students
Baltimore and Philadelphia high schools and is can profit from the transition courses, there
currently used in more than 120 sites across should be no stigma as in other tracking. Still, the
America to provide double doses of time in math issues of heterogeneous or homogeneous student
and English for most students in ninth, tenth, and groupings can still remain, when some class-
eleventh grades. Using an extended period block rooms enroll more far-behind students to better
schedule of 90 daily class minutes in each subject focus the transition preparations. Some studies
throughout the school year, a transition course is indicate lower achievement expectations of both
offered in the first 18-week term to prepare for the teachers and students are inevitable in classrooms
high standards required course in the second term. where poorly prepared students predominate
In mathematics, pre-algebra and pre-geometry (Hallinan, 1990; Oakes et al., 1992; Slavin,
courses have been developed as first-term transi- 1990), but other research shows nevertheless that
tional offerings that establish a firm foundation of skill gaps can be significantly narrowed or closed
520 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

when needy students are gathered together for under the Talent Development model, with extra
additional focused “catch up” classes (Balfanz, services from two existing national school sup-
Legters, & Jordan, 2004; Gamoran, 1992, 1993). port entities, City Year and Communities in
Schools. With financial support from a large fed-
eral Investing in Innovation (i3) grant, Diplomas
Levels of Focused Extra Help Now will be implemented in 65 high schools with
middle school feeders across America in 2010
The double-dose transition-course approach is one through 2015. Diplomas Now draws upon the
instance of a more general reform strategy using Talent Development model to provide a double-
different levels of student intervention based upon dose curriculum and interdisciplinary teams of
earlier indicators of specific student needs. Often teachers sharing the same student group for
discussed as an example of “response to interven- improving attendance and discipline.
tion” (RTI) that was developed in special educa- The second level of intervention is for smaller
tion programs (Duffy, 2007), high schools are subsets of students who receive small group
being reformed to offer different levels and inten- interventions to address academic or other per-
sities of help to students with various academic sonal difficulties. For example, at this level, a
and behavioral needs. It is a strategy to help all “triple dose” of academic interventions is used,
students achieve the immediate rewards of good such as a pull-out course for remedial math or
school work by dealing directly with the individ- English having a small class enrollment and using
ual needs that may stand in the way of success at different instructional interventions like comput-
school (U.S. General Accounting Office, 1993). ers or project-based learning. Likewise, a small
This approach can be aimed at preventing high group intervention can be used to improve stu-
school dropouts, the ultimate stage of student dis- dents’ coping skills where they have had serious
engagement, where learning difficulties in aca- disciplinary encounters, or to shape behavior
demic courses are only one potential problem where substance abuse is a serious problem.
source that needs to be addressed. Dropout risks The third level of intervention is at the indi-
also include students with poor attendance habits, vidual level, where help needs to be very focused
disciplinary problems, and personal issues of and intense because of the seriousness of aca-
immaturity, weak sense of personal responsibil- demic or personal needs. For example, tutoring
ity, low self-confidence as a learner, or interfer- in word attack skills may address an unusual high
ences with school work from outside issues of school student need that stands in the way of
substance abuse or serious family problems good reading. Or personal counseling to find
(Roderick & Camburn, 1999). direction and enhance personal responsibility and
A high school reform model that uses early positive choices may be unavoidable for certain
warning signs from elementary and middle grades troubled youth.
to prescribe the levels of extra help needed by dif-
ferent students is the Diplomas Now approach
currently being evaluated in multiple sets of Enhancing Criteria for Grades
schools across the country. Diplomas now defines and Recognition
three different levels of support for needy students
and draws extra resources from volunteer and High school students’ positive behaviors may go
community agencies to address particular student unmeasured and unrewarded, especially when
issues and to provide more intensive support. the conventional basis for course grades and pub-
The first level of support is school-wide inter- lic recognition leaves many without sources of
ventions for large segments of the student enroll- pride for future engagement.
ment that come poorly prepared for high school The conventional way of assigning course
success. Diplomas Now combines the school- grades in US schools is to record how well a stu-
wide high school reform program developed dent ranks in class or performs relative to an
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 521

absolute standard in the subject, which leaves verbally praised for their work, when tangible
many students who are consistently below aver- rewards are presented in an informational manner,
age with little chance for high marks and the asso- when rewards signify competence at an activity,
ciated positive recognitions. Many students’ and when the rewards are offered and given
positive motivation and engagement must surely for achieving performance standards or goal”
suffer under a grading system that leaves little (Cameron et al., 2005, p. 642). The reason rewards
room for top rewards except to those who can beat presented in this manner could potentially
all the rest in class rank or national norms. But increase motivation is because they help children
these same low-ranking students are frequently form or maintain their self-efficacy for a task. If
learning well in terms of progressing in knowl- recognition and praise is given for meeting small
edge and skills across the term, without any indi- tasks, self-efficacy may increase and the student
cation at report card time of these accomplishments. may try a more difficult task in the future.
Even their teachers may not appreciate these stu- Teachers can use different behavioral and sub-
dents’ efforts and results when at grading time all jective indicators for progress or effort grades,
attention is to standing in class or meeting very including parallel tests at the start and end of a
high standards. So while a conventional grade tells marking period, records on good attendance and
a student where he or she stands according to an completed assignments, as well as judgments
acknowledged standard (which is useful informa- from students’ performance in class or in confer-
tion), it will not serve as a source of motivation for ences with the teacher. But the progress or effort
those who have little chance even with heroic scores should be separately added grades so they
efforts to significantly move up in class. For sure, can highlight and reward individual student
just avoiding a course failure is an incentive for accomplishments, rather than combined together
some students, but school reforms to give every in a single grade including class achievement
student a realistic chance of a top grade should standing which muddles the message and dilutes
offer a stronger incentive to all students’ efforts. the potential rewards. Research remains limited
Some high schools are attempting more on the practical approaches for more responsive
responsive grading, by adding a progress score or grading and the effects on student engagement.
an effort score to the usual skills grade. The prog- But since immediate rewards for classwork can
ress score is to reward growth in course skills be so motivating for students, developing more
from the individual student’s own starting point responsive grading practices where all students
so gains in knowledge and skills from the begin- can earn positive recognition seems to be a prom-
ning of the term will be recognized. Even if a stu- ising new direction (Trumbull & Farr, 2000).
dent does not move up in class rank or has yet to
meet a high achievement standard, a good prog-
ress score should motivate a student with the rec- Increase Intrinsic Interest
ognition that he or she has improved themselves of School Work
over the time of the course. Likewise, a good
effort score for a student who attends every day, Students will be more engaged in learning tasks
completes the assignments, and tries hard to learn that are inherently satisfying rather than the more
can be earned by anyone regardless of the mundane drill and practice exercise to master
achievement standing in class, and may have a individual skills. For example, a student who
positive effect on motivation. enjoys reading mystery novels in her spare time
Meta-analytic research on extrinsic motivation is motivated primarily from the enjoyment of
has indicated that responsive rewards, such as reading these books and not from an outside
progress scores, can increase intrinsic motivation influence, such as for a school grade or to impress
for uninteresting tasks (Cameron, Banko, & her teacher. This enjoyment will still be present
Pierce, 2001; Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999) and even when reading a mystery novel is a school
for interesting activities when “participants are task. This enjoyment and motivation that comes
522 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

from within a person is called intrinsic motiva- in each subject. Sometimes called the “reading
tion (Cordova & Lepper, 1996; Eccles & Wigfield, wars” or the “math wars,” strong opinions have
2002; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Activities will be been held on each side of the issue. In early read-
more intrinsically motivating when a student is ing, proponents of phonics-based instruction
using skills that bring pleasure in themselves, where mastery of word attack skills are empha-
such as using the mind to solve a challenging sized have conflicted with whole language advo-
problem or to create a novel product, or when a cates who recommend immersing young readers
student is responsible for a project from begin- in real books to learn as they enjoy the experi-
ning to end that has an outcome to be proud of. In ences. In mathematics, one side urges an instruc-
these cases, a student will be working not so tional concentration on basic arithmetic and other
much to earn a good grade as to learn or improve operational skills including memorizing multipli-
oneself for his or her own sake. High intrinsic cation tables and fraction-percent equivalencies,
motivation for academic tasks leads to increases while others recommend an emphasis on under-
in learning and academic achievement (Fredricks, standing core concepts through discovery activi-
Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004). ties, so that students understand the ratio basis for
Students who are intrinsically motivated in a thinking when working with fractions rather than
task are more likely to set mastery goals as the counting framework for whole number arith-
opposed to performance goals. Mastery goals are metic, for example. The outcome of these dis-
made by students who want to learn and improve agreements is important for student engagement
their abilities and who are not as concerned about as well as for learning, since the mental chal-
what others think of their performance as they are lenges of conceptual understanding should be
learning (Pintrich & Schunk, 2002). They see the more intrinsically motivating.
end result of an academic task as increased A balance of both instructional elements is
knowledge or skills. Performance goals, how- now emerging among high school reformers in
ever, are made by those who want to look good both English and mathematics. A middle ground
for others and get good grades. Although a student to the reading wars, balanced literacy instruction,
may have a mix of both of these goals, mastery is becoming more accepted where basic word
goals are more related to help seeking, deep strat- attack skills as well as comprehension strategies
egy use, and confidence (Midgley, 2001). To sup- are being taught (National Reading Panel, 2000;
port mastery goals, teachers need to de-emphasize Pressley, 1998, 2000). A similar phenomenon has
the importance of grades and have a classroom occurred in high school math to encourage the
climate that supports, rather than punishes, student math educator not to overly focus on basic math
mistakes and errors as they attempt to learn diffi- skills without an emphasis on thinking and prob-
cult material in their classrooms. lem solving (National Council of Teachers of
Three examples illustrate instructional reforms Mathematics, 2009).
that should bring more intrinsic interest to school In high school reading instruction, reading flu-
work: teaching for conceptual understanding; ency to process text with ease and automaticity is
distinguishing disciplinary thinking in each sub- being developed by giving students regular oppor-
ject, and project-based learning. tunities for extensive reading, often of books of
their own choice. This should appeal to students’
intrinsic motivation by offering enjoyable learn-
Teaching for Conceptual ing experiences where each individual can take
Understanding some initiative. Reading comprehension instruc-
tion teaches students to actively think along with
In recent years, curriculum specialists in both the author and to regularly check for understand-
English and mathematics have been arguing about ing with corrective strategies. This has potential
the proper instructional emphasis between proce- for intrinsic satisfaction by challenging students to
dural knowledge and conceptual understanding use their minds while reading, including making
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 523

proper inferences to fully appreciate an author’s learn how the symbolic language and reasoning
intentions and to be an active participant in under- tools can be applied in problem formulation and
standing the material. solutions (Kilpatrick, 2001; Yore, Pimm, & Tuan,
In high school mathematics instruction, stu- 2007). Intrinsic motivation should be strength-
dents are still expected to remember basic facts ened when students assume the role of specialists
and formulas but primarily as tools for mathe- in each discipline and appreciate the unique dis-
matical reasoning and problem solving. Even course and thinking patterns of each subject.
when they do not immediately recognize the The centrality of disciplinary thinking has also
algorithm to apply with a math problem, students recently emerged in “content literacy,” with
learn how to get started toward a solution with instruction on how to read for understanding in
various thinking strategies. Students will learn each of the major subjects (Heller & Greenleaf,
how to use handheld calculators and other tech- 2007; Lee & Spratley, 2010; Shanahan &
nologies, but also to have the mental estimation Shanahan, 2008). Besides teaching students use-
skills to determine whether they have arrived at a ful strategies to use in all subjects before, during,
reasonable answer. Students will build their con- and after reading, it is important for them to learn
fidence that they can reason as a mathematician how the text structures and presentation of infor-
to frame a formulation that uses the symbolic lan- mation reflect the unique discourse patterns and
guage and tools to model a complex process or to disciplinary aspects of each separate subject.
solve an interesting problem. Intrinsic motivation Upgrading instruction in each subject to include
should be strong for these learning experiences disciplinary thinking should enhance the intrinsic
that challenge the mind and encourage analytic motivation for learning. When students are
thinking (National Research Council, 2000). expected to use their minds as an expert would in
each subject and to participate in relevant thinking
activities, the learning challenges should be more
Disciplinary Thinking exciting and satisfying as they are mastered.

High school reformers are also recommending


that instruction in each of the core academic sub- Project-Based Learning
jects plays close attention to the unique disciplin-
ary thinking of each case, so the intrinsic interest Project-based learning is also a growing feature
becomes clear (Newman, 1992; Nystrand & under high school reforms, where students learn
Gamoran, 1992; Resnick, 2010). Students in his- by conducting an extended interdisciplinary proj-
tory should not only learn the events and ect. These projects are meant to be authentic,
sequences of important periods, but also how his- complex problem-solving tasks (Bell, 2010;
torians use primary sources to make judgments Blumenfeld et al., 1991). The need for authentic
about causations and long-term consequences in tasks dates back to Dewey’s philosophy on learn-
writing historic narratives (Brown, 2009; ing through practical experiences.
Lattimer, 2008). Likewise, science students need Project-based learning can take many forms.
to learn major processes and laws of nature, but Sometimes students work in teams to complete a
at the same time understand how scientists com- project, but individual project assignments are
bine empirical evidence with alternative theories also used (Johnson & Johnson, 1985). The length
to advance knowledge at different points of time from beginning to end of a project can also vary
(Zimmerman, 2007). Literature and writing from 1 or 2 days to a couple of weeks or more.
classes should help students grow in their appre- The product of a project may be a written out-
ciation of the writer’s craft within the various come, a performance or presentation, a business
genres of fiction and forms of nonfiction, so that enterprise or simulation, or a construction using
their own writing can improve for different goals artistic or practical materials. The content of a
and audiences. Mathematics students should project may be an application or example from a
524 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

single subject such as science, history, literature, a task has “utility value” when a person under-
or mathematics or a combination of disciplinary stands how a task will help him or her reach their
contributions to the final product. Students can be goals (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Eccles, Wigfield,
part of the selection of the driving question or & Schiefele, 1998). If the “utility value” of the
problem as well as determining the nature of the task is strong, the person will value the outcome
final product. Studies on project-based learning of the task greatly and will put in more effort.
show a benefit of this approach for standardized Compare a student who is always asking his
and state assessments (Geier et al., 2008). or her teachers “Why are you making us learn
The instructional benefits of applying disci- this stuff?” to one who has chosen a program of
plinary skills and tools for a practical outcome studies because it makes sense for personal career
also contribute to the intrinsic motivation appeals. goals and interests. In the first case, engagement
Student should draw satisfaction from a task that with learning will be weak because no useful
demonstrates the actual usefulness of one or more connection is readily seen between school work
of their academic courses. and the student’s personal needs or goals. In the
The project task itself and outcome also has latter case, we expect this student to come to
intrinsic motivation components (Blumenfeld school each day anticipating a useful learning
et al., 1991). An individual who works through a experience that is directly relevant for personal
complex sequence of connected activities should career expectations, and to be regularly engaged
derive satisfaction from the mature planning and with a program of choice.
coordination required. Also, producing an end The integration of academic and career educa-
product can deliver a sense of accomplishment tion is an element of high school reform to
and pride that is often a defining characteristic of enhance the functional relevance of learning for
intrinsic motivation. Many projects involve col- student engagement. Two ways this can be
laboration which is motivating for some students. accomplished are through a Career Academy
Finally, since project-based instruction should be structure with appropriate elective courses and
student-based, students have the opportunity to experiences or by merging career applications
make choices and have more control over their into core academic courses.
learning which is more motivating than teacher-
directed activities (Deci & Ryan, 1987). Students’
intrinsic motivation for a project, however, will Career Academies
depend to an extent on their teacher’s intrinsic
motivation during project-based learning (Lam, Career Academies may be an organizational com-
Cheng, & Ma, 2009). Teachers need to constantly ponent of comprehensive reform models such as
monitor and support motivation throughout a Talent Development and Diplomas Now, or a
lengthy project. stand-alone innovation to integrate academic and
occupational education in high schools (Maxwell
& Rubin, 2000; Stern, Dayton, & Raby, 2010;
Functional Relevance of Learning Stern, Raby, & Dayton, 1992). Career Academies
are usually self-contained units within a high
School work will also be more engaging for high school with an instructional program centered
school students when it is clearly connected to an around a broad occupational cluster—such as sci-
individual’s personal long-range goals or when it ence or health, arts and expression, business and
feeds into current personal concerns and coping data management, construction or engineering—
skills (Schneider & Stevenson, 1999). The rele- which a student has selected to attend based on a
vance of school work for a student’s long-run or careful exploration of individual career interests,
short-run needs should be explicit or easily under- strengths, and goals. Each different Career
stood to have most value for increased student Academy may cover three or more high school
engagement. Motivational psychologists say that grades, such as 10–12, and enroll enough students
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 525

to require its own teaching faculty for all academic Academy program is another symbol of its func-
and career courses (usually 350 or more students tional value.
for three grades). The instructional program Several studies have demonstrated the value
includes the core college preparatory academic of curriculum programs like Career Academies
subject courses as well as a sequence of elective that integrate academic and career education in a
career education courses that can lead to a special relevant learning experience. In a field experi-
credential at graduation. The program also offers ment across 30 high schools in five districts
some experiential learning opportunities, where where students were randomly assigned to pro-
students pursue activities off-site at an actual busi- grams using career academies or not, the evalua-
ness or agency or where on-site career simulations tion firm of MDRC has published several reports
or enterprises are established for students to prac- on short-term and long-term effects (Kemple &
tice career roles. Snipes, 2000; Kemple & Willner, 2008). The
When separate Career Academies are formed most impressive outcomes found were the bene-
within a high school, precautions should be taken fits after high school graduation for career acad-
to offset informal forces to make undesirable dis- emy students, who were more likely to succeed
tinctions about selectivity and academic standards in college and in holding well-paying jobs in their
(Lee & Ready, 2007). Career Academies should be selected occupational area. In separate research
available for every student, so a residual program using national high school survey and records
does not exist as the college preparatory track. data, Plank (2001) found positive short-term ben-
Likewise, each Career Academy should be open to efits during the high school years for students
all students and have a complete set of college pre- who enrolled in a combined academic and career
paratory academic courses including a share of program of studies. The results on the relation-
advanced placement electives, so informal distinc- ship between percentage of career courses taken
tions of different standards and outcomes can be and the risk of dropping out suggested a U-shaped
avoided. Sometimes, career education is intro- pattern in that students with a mix of both career
duced for all students in a high school without and academic courses had a lower risk for drop-
separate Academies in their own space, when stu- ping out than students who took primarily either
dents choose a career major and add elective career or academic courses.
courses to their program from career cluster offer-
ings (Grubb, 1995). But career majors without a
complete Academy structure will not provide the Blending Career Applications
full integration of academic and career education
in all courses that would otherwise be possible. Even without a complete program of studies to
Student engagement is strengthened by career focus on the career relevance of school work,
academies in multiple ways. The program of each individual academic course can demonstrate
studies has functional relevance for the student its useful relevance by including interesting
by including elective courses and additional career and practical applications.
learning experiences focused on the career themes Perhaps mathematics instruction provides the
of strong personal interest to the individual. The most opportunities to include various practical
opportunity to choose among alternative Career applications for most of the course units and top-
Academies personifies the practical usefulness of ics. For example, a unit on exponential functions
the curriculum and represents a personal commit- can cover applications to several fields, including
ment to the program of studies. Attending classes compound interest in business, epidemic growth
with like-minded students who selected the same or drug decay in health, or population dynamics in
Career Academy also can bring some shared history and social studies. Although most high
pride in the program and reinforces its functional school math textbooks include some brief, practi-
relevance. The chance to earn an additional cre- cal examples and exercises, some packages of
dential at graduation for completion of the Career more extensive career applications that encourage
526 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

discovery and extended student discussions are the school for several reasons. Students will be
available for high school reformers. more eager to earn respect and praise for good
Language arts courses also have room for var- work and accept the goals set for them from
ious practical applications, such as writing teachers and administrators who know them well
assignments for various career goals and audi- and demonstrate real care for their success
ences, or coordinated reading experiences that (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994). Teachers and staff
overlap characters or situations from selected who can communicate on a personal basis with
industries or occupations. Likewise, science all the students they interact with in the halls and
knowledge and procedure can be used to address classrooms will be better able to establish and
current affairs issues and debates through appli- enforce a proper disciplinary climate that is safe
cations in selected science courses. The mode of and serious.
presentations using tables, graphs, and trend lines High school reforms that can foster good
found in mathematics and social studies courses teacher-student relations and a positive climate
can also be employed for parallel practical appli- conducive to learning include schools organized
cations in understanding daily newspaper and into smaller learning communities, teachers
popular media accounts. working in interdisciplinary teams that share the
same group of students, and adults who serve
over multiple years as the mentor and advisor to
Positive Teacher-Student Relations a limited number of individual students.

Students are likely to be more engaged with their


high school when they have a personal and Small Learning Communities
respectful relationship with the teachers and
administrators they encounter during the school Very large high schools pose special problems for
day (Baker, Terry, Bridges, & Winsor, 1997; establishing positive teacher-student relations
Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Hallinan, 2005; Wentzel, (Lee, 2000; Louis & Marks, 1998). The sheer
1997, 2009). If instead a student is largely anony- number of students and staff make it likely that
mous to the adults of the school, because the traffic in the halls, stairways, and cafeteria will
school size is large and teachers have too many involve many anonymous encounters. Likewise,
different students to get to know each one well, teachers who have many different students each
few human connections outside of one’s own stu- day in their courses will find it difficult to get to
dent peer group will exist to socially connect to know each one well along with his or her indi-
the school. As students move up in grades and vidual motivational and learning needs. Large
have more teachers during the day, their sense of student enrollments can also make the enforce-
school belonging decreases (Anderman, 1999). ment overwhelming of school regulations about
Research on teacher involvement and caring absenteeism, tardiness, class cutting, or deport-
notes that the effective teachers show affection ment, when the number of detentions and other
and appreciation for their students, know a lot punishments each day are hard to monitor and
about their students, dedicate time and energy to follow up if frequently ignored by students. As a
their students, and are dependable (Skinner & consequence, the climate in large schools can
Belmont, 1993). Some have argued that good often be chaotic in the halls and public spaces,
teaching practice is similar to good parenting absent of the safe feeling of adults-in-charge, and
styles, with consistent enforcement of rules, high haphazard in the chances of close, respectful
and reasonable expectations, democratic commu- individual adult-student relationships.
nication, nurturance, and an emphasis on self- A strong reform movement for smaller high
reliance (Baumrind, 1971; Wentzel, 2002). school learning communities has been pursued
Positive social connections with the adult edu- for several years around the turn of the twentieth
cators are valuable for student engagement with century, with major backing from the Bill and
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 527

Melinda Gates Foundation and other funders. major negative consequences. Ninth grade
Sometimes this involved starting new small high students during their transition year into high
schools of 400 students or less, or breaking up a school are especially prone to having early mis-
large facility into several separate self-contained steps escalate to severe problems before anyone
high schools (called “multiplex” high schools at notices and effectively intervenes.
times). Another common arrangement is a Interdisciplinary teams of teachers that share
“schools-within-the-school” organization where the same group of students and have regular com-
a large high school would create several smaller mon planning time is a high school reform to per-
learning communities or “houses” defined by sonalize the learning experience, take notice of
grade level or by curriculum focus. The Talent individual students in difficulty and address their
Development High School model created a sepa- problems. In the Ninth Grade Success Academy
rate Ninth Grade Success Academy and several of the Talent Development High School model,
Career Academies, each covering grades 10–12 two or more teams of four or five teachers, includ-
with about 300 students or less. In this case, each ing an English, math, science, and history/social
Academy has its own contiguous building space, studies teacher, serve the same group of 120–150
preferably with its own entrance door, as well as students for all of their major instruction. Each
its own Academy Principal and administrative team has a common planning period every day
staff and its own teaching faculty for all required when all their students are in an elective course,
and elective courses. Research studies of this to monitor individual student problems and to
model have shown the Academy’s structure can pursue corrective action plans. Thus, not only do
turn around a weak school climate or enhance a the numbers involved make detailed personal
strong one, as well as foster widespread positive knowledge possible in all teacher-student rela-
student-teacher relations (Legters, Balfanz, tionships, time is available for adults to be proac-
Jordan, & McPartland, 2002). An evaluation of tive in short circuiting student difficulties. When
123 small schools of choice (SSC) in New York working well, the teacher team takes responsibil-
City where students were randomly enrolled to ity for individual student success without shifting
these sites in comparison to other students who authority to school-wide principals and disciplin-
were not admitted in the random lottery process ary officials except in the most extreme cases.
showed positive benefits between 2002 and 2010 Evaluation research has demonstrated how the
on timely progression across the grades and grad- use of such teacher teams has significant positive
uation rates. A difference of 10 percentage points impacts on improved student attendance, course
in favor of SSC students was found in being on passing, and grade promotions, and reduced disci-
track toward graduation over 3 years as well as plinary incidents and removals, as well as on mea-
an overall 6.8% advantage in actual graduation sures of positive school climate and caring and
rates, which is equivalent to one-third of the city- respectful adult-student relationships (McPartland,
wide gap between white students and students of Balfanz, Jordan, & Legters, 1998).
color (Bloom, Thompson, & Unterman, 2010).

Adult Mentors and Advisors


Interdisciplinary Teacher Teams
Student engagement with their high school pro-
Another threat to student engagement is when gram can also be better ensured when every indi-
initial problems with attendance, course grades, vidual has their own adult mentor or advisor in
or discipline go unattended and grow into major the school who can advocate in various ways for
barriers to grade promotion and graduation. In their success. To work well, the individual stu-
many high schools, there are no early warning dent “case load” for each participating adult
systems with corrective actions, so troubled stu- should not be too large, and the time to get to
dents can “fall between the cracks” with eventual know one another well should be sufficient.
528 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

Some high schools establish an expanded pool two (Rottinghaus, Hees, & Conrath, 2009). Under
of adults to serve the mentor-advisor role, includ- the categorization developed by Holland (1997),
ing nonteaching department heads, counselors, some students may be particularly adept at build-
coaches, and specialist staff as well as the total ing or fixing mechanical things, while others are
teaching faculty. Sometimes, schools strive to especially good in working with others in team or
maintain each adult as mentor for several years of helping roles. In addition, individual student non-
the same students, including all 4 years of high academic strengths may be in areas of creative
school. Schools will schedule time for advising imagination and expression. Figuring out how
and mentoring in different ways, such as a short things work and thinking like a scientist can char-
advisory period each week or at the start of each acterize other students. Fascination with business
day, as well as selected periods for private discus- or financial affairs and organizing data for deci-
sions at report card time or other junctures for sion-making can be other sets of strengths.
“taking stock” on how things are going with The idea of multiple intelligences (Gardner,
school life and outcomes. 1983, 1999) is another conceptualization of the dif-
A study of 94 ninth grade high school students, ferent skills and talents that individuals can bring to
half of whom were randomly assigned to receive learning tasks. Gardner posited eight different intel-
adult mentoring services, showed positive differ- ligences including logical-mathematical, linguistic,
ences on not dropping out of school (9% compared musical, spatial, bodily-kinesthetic, interpersonal,
with 30%) and on earning significantly more intrapersonal, and naturalist. Later he introduced
course credits toward high school graduation the idea that there may even be spiritual and exis-
(Sinclair, Christenson, Evelo, & Hurley, 1998). In tential intelligences. In the USA, most school sys-
general, research shows that positive relationships tems place the highest value on both linguistic
with adults in the school, including teachers, (verbal) and logical-mathematical intelligences.
relates to more interest in school (b = .33, p <. 001) However, others such as bodily-kinesthetic and
(Wentzel, 1998), classroom engagement (b = .46, musical intelligences are not as valued, although
p <. 01) (Furrer & Skinner, 2003), and academic they could be used in the classroom to strengthen
effort (b = .31, p <. 001) (Wentzel, 1997). learning. For example, students with high musical
intelligence may be able to compose a song to sum-
marize information in science or a student high in
Opportunities for Personal Expression bodily-kinesthetic ability could be asked to act out
and Nonacademic Interests a scene as a soldier in World War 1.
Some high school reforms, such as the Talent
Another source for strong student engagement Development High School model, are based in
with their high school derives from the range of part on the concept that there are many talents, in
nonacademic talents that individual students addition to the academic competencies which are
come with and the degree to which schools can the focus of most achievement tests and grades,
respond to these diverse strengths. Nonacademic that should also be identified and developed dur-
talents include not only the athletic skills and ing the secondary grades because of their impor-
music or art abilities that can flourish in extracur- tance in adult life. Indeed, while academic grades
ricular teams and clubs but also the other dimen- and test scores may be important for advancing to
sions of human talents that vocational and higher levels of education, research has found
learning psychologists have identified that could them to be a weak predictor of adult occupational
find expression in some high school courses and success. After controlling for the level of educa-
classroom activities. tional attainment, the relationship of grades
Vocational psychologists have defined six cat- and test scores obtained earlier in life do not
egories of human talents or personalities that explain much of the variation in adult job status
match different occupational requirements, where or income (Jencks, 1975). Moreover, studies of
individual satisfaction and success are maxi- renowned high accomplishment in various fields
mized when there is a good match between the of business, law, creative design or performance,
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 529

governmental and community leadership, and personality profiles. The SDS is a booklet where
humanitarian service frequently identify out- students report and score their own experiences,
standing examples who had ordinary academic preferences, strengths, and interests along each
records but possessed other personal traits that broad dimension. This information is then orga-
were the foundation of their exemplary careers nized into a resulting two- or three-letter code
(Coyle, 2009; Scott, 2007). Economists have also which summarizes each student’s nonacademic
successfully used the diversity of human cogni- personal priorities. Students understand that the
tive and noncognitive skills to estimate models of SDS is not a test but a method for organizing their
adult accomplishment (Cunha & Heckman, own personal information for further awareness of
2008). Apparently, there are a wide range of other personal nonacademic strengths and interests.
nonacademic talents that society needs and The personal exploration continues with vari-
rewards, but go largely unnoticed and unrewarded ous possible implications and planning options
in American high schools’ preoccupation with that derive from the individual’s nonacademic or
traditional academic achievement goals. More- career codes. Students learn that there are many
over, it is likely that every individual in high educational levels for different occupations
school has personal nonacademic strengths and within the same code. They see that different
interests which could be further developed as a orderings of their own codes reveal a variety of
source of engagement in high school and as an alternative careers that would respond to their
investment for successful adult occupational own strengths and interests. They find supple-
careers or satisfying personal pursuits. mentary materials about specific careers of poten-
High school reforms that may increase student tial interest to them and the kinds of people who
engagement by appealing to nonacademic skills have been leaders or have found success in these
and talents include personal exploration oppor- careers. The SDS with follow-up activities is
tunities, elective courses, and extracurricular used to motivate Career Academy choices in
activities. Talent Development high schools. There are also
other useful tools available for student explora-
tions (Gottfredson, 1986).
Personal Exploration

An initial high school reform to engage students Course Planning and Selection
through nonacademic talents is to acknowledge
the range of human talents and to help each indi- Individual explorations of nonacademic strengths
vidual student to explore their own personal and interests can also be included in a student’s
strengths and interests. This would help students high school planning process. In a Career
understand that each of them has strong personal Academy high school, as described above, infor-
strengths in which to take pride, even if they are mation about the alternative choices can be con-
not a top student in academic course grades and nected to a student’s own codes for nonacademic
tests. When exploring the range of human traits, and career interests. An informed choice will
more students would see school as a means to have the appeal of new opportunities for using
exercise and enjoy their own special talents and personal strengths, as well as reinforcing student
as a route to preparing for later success and engagement for personal long-term goals.
satisfaction. Even in high schools without a Career
Several instruments and experiences are avail- Academy structure, students can leverage their
able for high schools to help their students collect awareness of personal nonacademic strengths
and organize data about their personal nonaca- with elective course opportunities. When a stu-
demic strengths and interests. Among the voca- dent finds electives that overlap with their per-
tional interest inventories that high school students sonal strengths, the school experience should be
can take, the Self-Directed Search (SDS) is based more engaging as a way to further develop one’s
upon Holland’s six types of careers and associated individuality. At its best, well-informed students
530 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

may find course offerings and sequences where governed (Lee, Bryk, & Smith, 1993; Shouse,
their own passions for learning are welcomed 1997). Communal engagement is connected by
and stimulated. the concepts of relational trust and social capital
that sociologists have developed to describe the
quality of social exchanges in key role relation-
Extracurricular Activities ships (Bryk & Schneider, 2002) and the power of
relational ties within a social system (Coleman,
High school reforms should also not overlook the 1988). A school with high levels of relational
out-of-class opportunities to allow a wide variety trust and social capital has individuals who
of students’ nonacademic strengths to flourish strongly value their membership in this commu-
and further develop. Some students may draw nity, are willing to work together with others for
their strongest points of school engagement from the common good, and to look out for one anoth-
the teams or clubs where they can exercise their er’s legitimate interests. The school members
personal strengths and contribute to group or share common norms about how individuals
individual accomplishments. Numerous research should behave (“do what is right”) in their orga-
studies have identified extracurricular activities nizational community, expect mutual caring, and
as a positive setting for personal growth and for respect and personally identify with and take
strengthening students’ engagement at school pride in “my school” (Bryk & Schneider, 2002).
(Eccles & Barber, 1999; Fredricks & Eccles, Students who have had the experiences to develop
2005; Gilman, Meyers, & Perez, 2004). relational trust and to rely upon the social capital
The Talent Development High School model is of their school we define to have communal
using the benefits for personal growth by extend- engagement with the school.
ing extracurricular activities beyond the traditional Communal engagement is valuable in its own
teams and clubs as after-school entities. Several right as students enjoy the experiences of a close-
Talent Development sites have added a daily period knit supportive community, and it can also benefit
at the end of each day for personal exploration and students’ academic learning processes and be a
activities that use artists, musicians, dancers, cir- source of their general citizenship socialization.
cus performers, political debaters, and other cre- We discussed above how positive teacher-student
ative individuals from the community to lead the relationships can enhance the incentives for stu-
classroom activities. A goal is to expand the school dents to exert effort at school work to earn recog-
curriculum so that every student gets a wide vari- nition from a valued adult, and these roles can
ety of nonacademic experiences that may expand lead to further learning benefits when embedded
interests for some and reinforce personal strengths in a school with strong social ties where all adults
in others. In addition, Career Academies in many are sensitive to each student’s needs. In this case,
of the Talent Development high schools are pro- teachers will share information with one another
viding learning experiences for students in com- to monitor and improve individual student behav-
munity enterprises and agencies to appreciate iors, just as neighbors in a small community with
how skills from academic courses combine with strong social capital will watch out for each
more specialized abilities for success in adult other’s children and help them grow up well
occupations (Epstein, 2001; Pearson, 2002). (Coleman, 1988). Moreover, students who par-
ticipate in a well-governed school where author-
ity is clearly exercised for the common good and
School Governance, Disciplinary each individual has community responsibilities,
Practices, and Communal can be expected to develop general appreciation
Engagement for good citizenship and shared values in broader
society.
Communal engagement is another way students Students will be more likely to feel positively
can become attached to their high schools, which attached to a school that is largely governed
will be affected by how the school as a whole is by shared norms of civility and trust, where the
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 531

necessary rules are legitimized by participatory versus other misbehaviors, such as use of profane
decision-making, and infractions are adjudicated or abusive language, tobacco possession, and tru-
and discussed as learning experiences (Osterman, ancy and some physical fighting. But most
2000; Webb, Covington, & Guthrie, 1993). In schools fall down on how well the rules are
contrast, students can feel estranged and alien- applied and on the range of responses made to
ated from their schools if rules seem excessive both desired and inappropriate conduct.
and arbitrary, and discipline appears inconsistent Although potential consequences of breaking
and without recourse even for harsh penalties school rules might extend from detention, repri-
such as suspensions or expulsions. mands, notifying parents, brief exclusion from
To be sure, some high schools are more diffi- class, loss of privileges, and school or community
cult to manage because of large size, awkward service, to removal from school for short or long
traffic patterns in stairways and corridors, student suspensions or even permanent expulsion, the sur-
bodies that are more prone to mischief or group vey found that about three quarters of the schools
tensions, or reputations for chaotic behavior. used the same one or two responses to all offenses.
Indeed, getting a high school under control that Indeed, the more extreme punishment of disci-
has unsafe or disorderly climates is usually the plinary removals was often found to be the promi-
first priority of a reform program (McPartland nent and initial responses in many high schools to
et al., 1998). Two aspects of effective school gov- both dangerous and less consequential student
ernance closely tied to student engagement are offenses. Likewise, a wide range of positive
the school rules and disciplinary practices them- responses, if any at all, for positive student behav-
selves as well as opportunities for student partici- iors was found missing in most secondary schools.
pation in school governance and disciplinary The praise or recognition, material or privilege
enforcement. rewards, or other potential reinforcements of good
student behavior were used in combination by
only 20% of the surveyed schools.
Rules and Disciplinary Practices Students are more likely to appreciate “firm
but fair” enforcement of school rules, but the sur-
A national survey to analyze school-wide disci- vey showed that the quality of disciplinary prac-
plinary practices from a stratified random sample tice is often poor. Less than half of secondary
of 848 public and private schools, sponsored by schools were found to practice consistent disci-
the US Department of Justice, was completed in pline, where punishments do not depend upon
2000 (Gottfredson et al., 2000, 2004). When the what teacher makes the referral or what kind of
quality of disciplinary practices was scored, most student is involved. Less than one-third were pre-
schools had good communications and documen- dictable in their disciplinary decision-making,
tation practices, but much fewer were adequate in where both teachers and students are clear about
the consistency and predictability of their disci- the specific disciplinary consequences that fol-
plinary enforcements or in the range of responses low each kind of infraction.
to misconduct and to desirable conduct. Thus, student engagement with their school is
Almost all schools have established a set of frequently threatened when they perceive school
school rules and policies with the consequences rules as too harsh or capricious and school use of
for different misbehaviors that are made known punishments to far outweigh rewards for good
to the student population, especially for danger- conduct as the usual official responses to their
ous misconduct such as possession of a weapon behavior. Students who feel that the discipline
at school. Most schools are also good at keeping system of their school is ineffective or unfair are
records of the disciplinary actions taken during a less likely to participate in class and attend school
school year. These rules usually distinguish (Finn & Voelkl, 1993; Osher, Bear, Sprague, &
between crimes and other very serious infrac- Doyle, 2010) and more likely to drop out of
tions, such as having weapons in school, posses- school (Fine, Valenzuela, & Bowditch, 1993;
sion of drugs or alcohol, and criminal attacks, Wehlange & Rutter, 1986).
532 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

Student Involvement of adherence. The concept of “mutual trust” has


in Decision-Making been cited as a feature of a good problem-solving
and self-regulating school that leads to strong
Student engagement is also likely to be a function student engagement (Bryk & Schneider, 2002).
of their own opportunities to influence how rules
are established and enforced and other decisions
about school life are made. Interaction of Dimensions of High
Student opportunities to help shape disciplin- School Student Motivation
ary rules can occur at both the classroom level and Engagement
and school wide. At the beginning of the term, a
teacher can invite the students to help shape the The six dimensions of student motivation and
rules that will guide classroom behavior, includ- engagement discussed in this chapter can interact
ing how discussions and interactions with peers with one another and the associated high school
and adults are to proceed, and how school disci- reforms. Different dimensions call for certain
pline policies will be applied. Similarly, student preparatory actions to establish a motivational
representatives from the elected grade or school foundation for later student engagement with
councils or from appointed committees can help each reform. Some dimensions may be more
set disciplinary policies and priorities to govern dependent on others, with implications for plan-
student behavior throughout the building and ning the order and sequence of different reforms.
grounds (Rosenberg & Jackman, 2003). Different combinations of school improvements
Some high schools have also used student par- can enhance one another for a total impact on stu-
ticipation in the enforcement of school rules, dent engagement, which highlights the needs for
through student courts where students are given a comprehensive high school reform.
hearing before punishment for an offense, or by
way of peer mediation services where conflicts
between individuals are worked out through face- Preparing for Student Motivation
to-face discussions facilitated by other student and Engagement
helpers. Student monitors in halls and cafeterias
can also be a way to involve others in the mainte- Using the distinction between student motivation
nance of an orderly and respectful environment. that is established before participating in activi-
But how students are treated for disciplinary ties and student engagement that is activated
purposes in their relationship to school adults and during experiences, sets of the six dimensions
authorities can also determine how often strong and associated high school reforms seem to
informal norms of good behavior develop and require particular anticipatory preparations for
make frequent official reactions less necessary. motivation to lead to engagement.
As discussed earlier, a self-regulating school can Helping students gain insight into their own
develop when individual teachers seek to address personal strengths and career interests can be
most student behavioral issues without sending a valuable preliminary activities to set up strong
student to the office for an official disciplinary student motivation under the two dimensions of
officer to take action. Teachers working in teams the functional relevance and the opportunities for
can organize a sequence of responses that bring talent development of their school program.
in other adults who share responsibility for a stu- When early on in their high school years students
dent to discuss a disciplinary issue with the indi- have the opportunities to explore what they bring
vidual and seek solutions. Even when a student in terms of personal strengths to be further devel-
referral is made to a school authority for disci- oped and what they hope to accomplish in terms
plinary reasons, the manner in which discussions of building new skills and a record of accom-
proceed for learning experiences can shape how plishment for goals after graduation, a motiva-
the student views rules as legitimate and worthy tional foundation can be established for actually
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 533

experiencing their school program with high for learning can set the overriding goal of
interest and energy. For example, a student who self-improvement and the growth of the mind,
is helped to appreciate that she or he is espe- students will also be ready for engagement in the
cially interested in working with others as a thinking challenges of their course and the intrin-
career goal is ready to choose a Career Academy sic satisfactions of project-based learning.
program and to seek out extracurricular activities
that can foster this emphasis. Likewise, a student
who understands that she or he has major strengths Setting Reform Priorities
in building or fixing things and especially enjoys and Sequences
figuring out how things work can get strongly
involved with curriculum choices and out-of- Three dimensions of student engagement we
class learning opportunities that build on these have described in this chapter are aspects of stu-
personal talents. In these and other parallel cases, dents’ daily classroom learning activities: acces-
preparatory activities and choices can establish sible immediate awards, embedded intrinsic
the motivational fulcrums to leverage student interest, and direct functional relevance. The
engagement with reforms that bring functional other three dimensions are more concerned with
relevance and opportunities for talent develop- the school-level factors that cover the general cli-
ment to the school program and activities. mate for learning and personal development at
Preparatory activities can also strengthen stu- the school: positive interpersonal climate, oppor-
dent motivation for responding to the reforms’ tunities for alternative talent development, and
dimensions of shared communal engagement and governance for communal engagement.
positive interpersonal climate. When students are Often, high schools begin reforms with the
initially invited to help establish the school rules school-level factors because good student atten-
for school discipline with opportunities to partici- dance and a positive learning climate that derive
pate in their enforcement, important first steps can from these factors must be established first before
be made toward a school governance climate of any other reforms can take hold.
trust and self-regulation. Likewise, when students Good student attendance is a necessary condi-
are assigned to teacher teams that spend up front tion for improved learning. If a student does not
time building group spirit and ways for students come to school every day, there is no way he and
to seek adult guidance and assistance, a motiva- she can take full advantage of improved class-
tional platform can occur that supports student room instruction and learn better. Moreover, a
engagement with reforms for an ongoing learning student usually needs good attendance to earn
environment of positive interpersonal relations. passing grades regardless of individual achieve-
The motivational stage can also be set to fos- ment levels because most teachers will require
ter student engagement with the two reform adequate attendance as a major criterion for
dimensions regarding either immediate rewards report card credits. As discussed earlier, reforms
or embedded intrinsic interest of their learning aimed at good attendance can make a significant
activities. Students can receive valuable orienta- difference when teacher teams reach out to absen-
tions toward high school learning that balance tees (positive interpersonal climate), when stu-
the performance goals of earning good grades dents are drawn to a school program that addresses
and the mastery goals of improving one’s own their own goals and interests (alternative talent
skills and understandings. When students begin development), and when the school is perceived
with the belief that their teachers want them to to be safe and considerate (shared communal
pass and earn good grades and will provide extra engagement).
help when needed for these accomplishments, Likewise, the learning environment of the
they will be more likely to value these immediate school cannot be a distraction from serious edu-
rewards and work hard for them. At the same cational purposes nor a situation where students
time, when the initial motivational orientation feel estranged or uncared for, before any other
534 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

improvements can occur. If discipline in the of assessing a high school’s starting point before
school building is weak, with chaotic behavior in reforms to target student engagement. If a school
the halls and stairways, and lack of mutual respect is fortunate enough to begin with high levels of
between teachers and students in the classrooms, student attendance and can assume a climate that
adult energies will be drawn to enforcing school is safe, serious, and sensitive to individual stu-
rules and managing classroom deportment and dents, major school-level reforms may not be
away from inspiring educational ambitions and needed to organize smaller units with teacher
motivating good learning. If teachers and stu- teams, to expand nonacademic options, or to
dents do not get to know each other well and revise school rules and decision-making. Such a
develop respectful and caring relationships, both school would not have to delay instructional
will be deprived of the foundations for learning improvements until the school-level factors are
activities that can be both challenging and joyful. gotten under control and could begin with a cam-
Thus, troubled high schools will often begin with paign to engage students more in learning activi-
reforms for positive learning environments that ties, while perhaps also making the school
encourage positive interpersonal relationships, dimensions even more attractive. However, a dif-
provide student choices of activities in and out- ferent very troubled school with serious disci-
side of class that coincide with personal interests, pline and attendance problems would establish
and govern the school for common norms of its reform credibility by the early victories of an
good behavior and mutual trust. So, if a high improved school climate and better student atten-
school is afflicted with a dangerous or unruly dis- dance that can be achieved from school organiza-
ciplinary environment or a pattern of high student tion changes. Once these improvements are
absenteeism, reform priorities will be the school achieved, the prospects for successful reforms of
organization dimensions that are aimed at the stu- the instructional program to engage students will
dent engagement factors related to a safe and have a firm foundation.
serious school climate, programmatic opportuni-
ties for personal fulfillment, and a communal
environment of shared beliefs and goals. Comprehensive Reforms
Once an environment conducive to good
attendance and serious learning has been estab- While each dimension of student engagement
lished at the school level, the reforms for motiva- plays a separate role in how an individual
tion and engagement in classroom instructional approaches the daily learning activities and life
activities can proceed. Instructional arrangements in school, improvements in one dimension can
can be made to provide extra help focused on strengthen others. For example, if a student
particular student needs, so every learner has a believes major long-run payoffs will follow com-
good chance to earn good grades and positive pletion of their school studies because of the
feedback for skill improvements. At the same functional relevance of their courses, she or he
time, teachers can work to improve the motiva- may be able to endure possible less motivating
tional challenges to students of their daily learn- aspects of their instruction when they cannot earn
ing tasks, by emphasizing intrinsically interesting high grades or find many of their classes tedious
aspects of using thinking skills in each major or boring. On the other hand, a student may be
subject and applying new knowledge to useful turned on by courses that are intellectually chal-
projects. Moreover, time can be devoted to inte- lenging and intrinsically interesting, even if the
grating career and academic learning in course links to practical applications are weak or the
offerings and classroom applications to reinforce chances for high grades are difficult. But, when a
students’ engagement through the functional rel- learning opportunity combines all sources of sat-
evance of their program of studies. isfaction, with activities of intrinsic interest, very
Giving priorities to student attendance and accessible recognition for good work and clear
school learning environment highlights the value connections to personal goals, the total impact on
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 535

student engagement is likely to be more than just comprehensive package of school and classroom
the sum of the parts. This argues for comprehen- reforms can address a wide set of dimensions for
sive high school instructional reforms that address student engagement.
multiple dimensions, and teacher professional Some important needs of further research and
development that prepares them for all aspects. development on high school reform for student
Not only should extra help be timely and focused engagement are indicated by next steps being
to help all students earn passing grades, the learn- taken with the Talent Development model. Because
ing tasks also should have high appeal themselves the dropout problem is so severe in many high
as worthy of a strong and inquisitive mind and schools, reforms must be intensified to reach all
with clear relevance for practical use and long- the students, even those with very debilitating
term goals. individual circumstances. We will be evaluating
Individual school-level reforms aimed at prac- how adding strong community resources and
tical dimensions of student engagement can also intensive tutoring services to the school-wide
have synergistic value to one another in a com- Talent Development organizational and instruc-
prehensive program of change. School gover- tional reforms may be successful in eliminating
nance of student participation for firm and fair the dropout problem. Studies of student engage-
discipline is important in its own right, but can ment across the full-grade spectrum are needed,
also improve the chances that positive student- so early warning signs at elementary or middle
teacher relations can emerge from smaller learn- grades can also be used for more timely responses
ing communities with stronger aspects of mutual to individual student needs before ultimate risks of
respect and caring. Likewise, school-wide oppor- school dropouts can develop. Besides this focus
tunities for individual students to explore and on the most needy students, we are also studying
engage their own nonacademic strengths and how stronger instructional reforms can more
interests can flourish best in a setting with a sta- successfully challenge all students’ critical think-
ble and supportive learning environment. Thus, ing skills in each high school subject with the
comprehensive school-level reforms can create additional benefits of strengthening their intrinsic
positive feedbacks where different dimensions of motivations for learning. And while priority is
student engagement are collectively enhanced. given to internal high school reforms for increas-
Comprehensive high school reforms can also ing student motivation and engagement, some
address both the more basic dimensions of stu- future attention should return to external factors
dent engagement, where individuals strive in a such as the kinds of curriculum requirements,
safe environment to meet the formal success assessment tests, and teacher accountability mech-
requirement of passing grades and a good disci- anisms that either confine or enable the best inter-
plinary record, with the deeper dimensions of nal improvements.
student engagement, where individuals pursue
their own learning passions, can work in a caring
and supportive environment, and experience ful- References
fillment of personal growth and strong prepara-
tion for a desired future. Thus, a program of high American Youth Policy Forum. (2000). High schools of
school reform can begin with changes to make the millennium. Washington, DC: American Youth
the school safe and extra instructional help avail- Policy Forum.
Anderman, L. H. (1999). Classroom goal orientation,
able to those in need, so no one needs to drop out
school belonging and social goals as predictors of stu-
before earning a high school diploma. By con- dents’ positive and negative affect following the tran-
tinuing with a reform program to develop and sition to middle school. Journal of Research and
engage individual students’ personal strengths Development in Education, 32, 131–147.
Baker, J., Terry, T., Bridger, R., & Winsor, A. (1997).
and interests and to establish a supportive human
Schools as caring communities: A relational approach
environment for individual exploration and to school reform. School Psychology Review, 26,
investments in diverse learning opportunities, a 586–602.
536 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

Balfanz, R., Herzog, L., & Mac Iver, D. J. (2007). textualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of
Preventing student disengagement and keeping stu- Educational Psychology, 88, 715–730.
dents on the graduation path in urban middle-grades Coyle, D. (2009). The talent code: Greatness isn’t born.
schools: Early identification and effective interven- It’s grown. Here’s how. New York: Bantam.
tions. Educational Psychologist, 42(4), 223–235. Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. J. (2008). Formulating, identi-
Balfanz, R., Legters, N., & Jordon, W. (2004). Catching up: fying and estimating the technology of cognitive and
Effects of the Talent Development ninth-grade instruc- non-cognitive skill formation. Journal of Human
tional interventions in reading and mathematics in Resources, 43, 738–782.
high-poverty high schools. NASSP Bulletin, 88, 3–30. Davis, M. H., & McPartland, J. (2009). Supporting high
Balfanz, R., McPartland, J., & Shaw, A. (2002). school teachers to close adolescent literacy gaps.
Re-conceptualizing extra help for high school students Paper presented at the American Educational Research
in a high standards era. Journal of Vocational Special Association, San Diego, CA.
Needs Education, 25, 24–41. Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental author- analytic review of experiments examining the effects
ity. Developmental Psychology Monograph, 4(1, Pt. of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation.
2), 1–103. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.
Bell, S. (2010). Project-based learning for the 21st cen- Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of auton-
tury: Skills for the future. Clearing House: A Journal omy and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality
of Educational Strategies, Issues and Ideas, 83(2), and Social Psychology, 53, 1024–1037.
39–43. Duffy, H. (2007). Meeting the needs of significantly strug-
Bloom, H. S., Thompson, S. L., & Unterman, R. (2010). gling learners in high school: A look at approaches to
Transforming the high school experience: How New tiered intervention. Washington, DC: National High
York City’s new small schools are boosting student School Center.
achievement and graduation rates. New York: MDRC. Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1999). Student council, vol-
Blumenfeld, P. C., Soloway, E., Marx, R. W., Krajcik, J. unteering, basketball, or marching band: What kind of
S., Guzdial, M., & Palincsar, A. (1991). Motivating extracurricular involvement matters? Journal of
project-based learning: Sustaining the doing, support- Adolescent Research, 14(1), 10–43.
ing the learning. Educational Psychologist, 26(3–4), Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs,
369–398. values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,
Brown, S. D. (2009). History circles: The doing of teach- 109–132.
ing history. The History Teacher, 42(2), 191–203. Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation
Bryk, A. S., & Schneider, B. (2002). Trust in schools. to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg
New York: Russell Sage. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3.
Burris, C. C., Wiley, E., Welner, K. G., & Murphy, J. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th
(2008). Accountability, rigor, and detracking: ed., pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley.
Achievement effects of embracing a challenging cur- Epstein, J. L. (2001). School, family, and community part-
riculum as a universal good for all students. Teachers nerships: Preparing educators and improving schools.
College Record, 110(3), 571–607. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Cameron, J., Banko, K. M., & Pierce, W. D. (2001). Fine, M., Valenzuela, A., & Bowditch, C. (1993). Getting
Pervasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic rid of troublemakers: High school disciplinary proce-
motivation: The myth continues. Behavior Analyst, dures and the production of dropouts. Social Problems,
24(1), 1–44. 40, 493–509.
Cameron, J., Pierce, W. D., Banko, K. M., & Gear, A. Finn, J. D., & Voelkl, K. E. (1993). School characteristics
(2005). Achievements-based rewards and intrinsic related to student engagement. The Journal of Negro
motivation: A test of cognitive mediators. Journal of Education, 62(3), 249–268.
Educational Psychology, 97(4), 641–655. Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
Chambers, T. T. V., Hugins, K. S., & Scheurich, J. J. School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
(2009). To track or not to track: Curricular differentia- the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1),
tion and African American students at Highview High 59–109.
School. Journal of Cases in Educational Leadership, Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2005). Developmental
12(1), 38–50. benefits of extracurricular involvement. Do peer char-
Clark, D. (Ed.). (2009). Improving No Child Left Behind: acteristics mediate the link between activities and
Linking world-class education standards to America’s youth outcomes? Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
economic recovery. The Aspen Institute Congressional 34, 507–520.
Program: Washington, DC. Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as
Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital and the creation of a factor in children’s academic engagement and per-
human capital. The American Journal of Sociology, formance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,
94, 95–120. 148–162.
Cordova, D., & Lepper, M. (1996). Intrinsic motivation Gamoran, A. (1992). The variable effects of high school
and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of con- tracking. American Sociological Review, 57, 812–828.
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 537

Gamoran, A. (1993). Alternative uses of ability grouping Heller, R., & Greenleaf, C. L. (2007). Literacy instruction
in secondary schools: Can we bring high-quality in the content areas. Washington, DC: Alliance for
instruction to low-ability classes? American Journal Excellent Education.
of Education, 102, 1–22. Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices: A the-
Gamoran, A., Nystrand, M., Berends, M., & LePore, P. C. ory of vocational personalities and work environments
(1995). An organizational analysis of the effects of (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
ability grouping. American Educational Research Resources.
Journal, 32, 687–715. Jencks, C. S. (1975). Effects of high schools on their
Gamoran, A., Porter, A. C., Smithson, J., & White, P. A. students. Harvard Educational Review, 45(3),
(1997). Upgrading high school mathematics instruc- 273–324.
tions: Improving learning opportunities for low- Johnson, D., & Johnson, R. (1985). Motivational process
achieving, low-income youth. Educational Evaluation in cooperative, competitive, and individualistic learn-
and Policy Analysis, 19, 325–338. ing situations. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multi- motivation in education: Vol. II. The classroom milieu
ple intelligences. New York: Basic Books. (pp. 249–286). Orlando, FL: Academic.
Gardner, H. (1999). Are there additional intelligences? In Kemple, J., & Snipes, J. (2000). Career academies:
J. Kane (Ed.), Education, information, and transfor- Impacts on student engagement and performance in
mation: Essays on learning and thinking (pp. 111– high school. New York: MDRC.
131). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Kemple, J. J., Herlihy, C. M., & Smith, T. J. (2005).
Geier, R., Blumenfeld, P. C., Marx, R. W., Krajcik, J. S., Making progress toward graduation: Evidence from
Fishman, B., Soloway, E., et al. (2008). Standardized the Talent Development High School model. New
test outcomes for students engaged in inquiry-based York: MDRC.
science curricula in the context of urban reform. Journal Kemple, J. J., & Willner, C. J. (2008). Career academies:
of Research in Science Teaching, 45(8), 922–939. Long-term impacts on labor market outcomes, educa-
Gilman, R., Meyers, J., & Perez, L. (2004). Structured tional attainment, and transitions to adulthood. New
extracurricular activities among adolescents: Findings York: MDRC.
and implications for school psychologists. Psychology Kilpatrick, J. (2001). Where’s the evidence? Journal for
in the Schools, 41, 31–41. Research in Mathematics Education, 32(4), 421–427.
Gottfredson, G. D., Gottfredson, D. C., Czeh, E. R., Lam, S., Cheng, R. W.-Y., & Ma, W. Y. K. (2009). Teacher
Cantor, D., Crosse, S. B., & Hantman, I. (2000). and student intrinsic motivation in project-based learn-
National study of delinquency prevention in schools. ing. Instructional Science, 37(6), 565–578.
Ellicott City, MD: Gottfredson Associates. Lattimer, H. (2008). Challenging history: Essential ques-
Gottfredson, G. D., Gottfredson, D. C., Czeh, E. R., tions in the social studies classroom. Social Education,
Cantor, D., Crosse, S. B., & Hantman, I. (2004). 72(6), 326–329.
Toward safe and orderly schools – The national study Lee, C. D., & Spratley, A. (2010). Reading in the disci-
of delinquency prevention in schools. Washington, plines: The challenges of adolescent literacy. New
DC: National Institute of Justice, U.S. Department of York: Carnegie Corporation of New York.
Justice. Lee, V., Bryk, A., & Smith, J. (1993). The organization of
Gottfredson, L. S. (1986). Occupational Aptitude Patterns effective secondary schools. Review of Research in
Map: Development and implications for a theory of Education, 19, 171–268.
job aptitude requirements. Journal of Vocational Lee, V. E. (2000). School size and the organization of sec-
Behavior, 29, 254–291. ondary schools. In M. T. Hallan (Ed.), Handbook of
Grubb, W. N. (1995). Coherence for all students: High sociology of education (pp. 327–344). New York:
schools with career clusters and majors. In W. N. Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Grubb (Ed.), Education through occupations in Lee, V. E., & Ready, D. D. (2007). Schools within schools.
American high schools: Approaches to integrating New York: Teachers College Press.
academic and vocational education: Vol. I (pp. Legters, N. E., Balfanz, R., Jordan, W. J., & McPartland,
97–113). New York: Teachers College Press. J. M. (2002). Comprehensive reform for urban high
Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental school: A Talent Development approach. New York:
discipline methods on the child’s internalization of Teachers College Press.
values: A reconceptualization of current points of Louis, K., & Marks, H. (1998). Does professional com-
view. Developmental Psychology, 30, 4–19. munity affect the classroom? Teachers’ work and stu-
Hallinan, M. (2005). The normative culture of a school dent experiences in restructuring schools. American
and student socialization. In L. V. Hedges & B. Journal of Education, 106, 532–575.
Schneider (Eds.), The social organization of schooling Lucas, S. R. (1999). Tracking inequality: Stratification
(pp. 129–146). New York: Russell Sage. and mobility in American high schools. New York:
Hallinan, M. T. (1990). The effects of ability grouping in Teachers College Press.
secondary schools: A response to Slavin’s best-evi- Maxwell, N., & Rubin, V. (2000). High school career acad-
dence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 60, emies: A pathway to educational reform in urban school
501–504. districts. Kalamazoo, MI: W.E. Upjohn Institute.
538 M.H. Davis and J.M. McPartland

Mark, S. F. (1983). To succeed or not to succeed: A criti- Osterman, K. F. (2000). Students’ need for belonging in the
cal review of issues in learned helplessness. school community. Review of Educational Research, 3,
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8(1), 1–19. 323–367.
McPartland, J., Balfanz, R., Jordan, W., & Legters, N. Pearson, S. S. (2002). Finding common ground: Service
(1998). Improving school climate and achievement in learning and education reform. Washington, DC:
a troubled urban high school through the Talent American Youth Policy Forum.
Development Model. Journal of Education for Pintrich, P. R., & Schunk, D. H. (2002). Motivation in
Students Placed at Risk, 3(4), 337–361. education: Theory, research, and applications (2nd
Midgley, C. (2001). A goal theory perspective on the cur- ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice-Hall.
rent status of middle level schools. In T. Urdan & F. Plank, S. (2001). Career and technical education in the
Pajares (Eds.), Adolescence and education: Vol. I (pp. balance: An analysis of high school persistence, aca-
33–59). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. demic achievement, and post secondary destinations.
National Association of Secondary School Principals. Minneapolis, MN: National Research Center for
(1996). Breaking ranks: Changing an American insti- Career and Technical Education.
tution. Reston, VA: NASSP. Pressley, M. (1998). Reading instruction that works: The
National Association of Secondary School Principals. case for balanced teaching. New York: Guilford
(2002). What the research shows: Breaking ranks in Press.
education. Reston, VA: NASSP. Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruc-
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2009). tion be the instruction of? In M. L. Kamil, P. B.
Focus in high school mathematics: Reasoning and Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook
sense making. Reston, VA: NCTM. of reading research: Vol. III (pp. 545–561). Mahwah,
National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the National NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Reading Panel: Teaching people to read. Washington, Resnick, L. B. (2010). Nested learning systems for the think-
DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human ing curriculum. Educational Researcher, 39, 183–197.
Development. Roderick, M., & Camburn, E. (1999). Risk and recovery
National Research Council. (2000). How people learn: from course failure in the early years of high school.
Brain, mind, experience, and school, Expanded edi- American Educational Research Journal, 36(2),
tion. In J. D. Bransford, A. L. Brown, & R. R. Cocking 303–343.
(Eds.), Committee on Developments in the Science of Rosenberg, M. S., & Jackman, L. A. (2003). Development,
Learning, Committee on Learning Research and implementation, and sustainability of comprehensive
Educational Practice, Commission on Behavioral and school-wide behavior management systems.
Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: Intervention in School and Clinic, 39(1), 10–21.
National Academy Press. Rottinghaus, P. J., Hees, C. K., & Conrath, J. A. (2009).
National Research Council and the Institute of Medicine. Enhancing job satisfaction perspectives: Combining
(2004). Engaging Schools: Fostering high school stu- Holland themes and basic interests. Journal of
dents’ motivation to learn. Committee on Increasing Vocational Behavior, 75, 139–151.
High School Students’ Engagement and Motivation to Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic
Learn. Board on Children, Youth and Families, Division motivations: Classic definitions and new directions.
of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67.
Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. Schneider, B., & Stevenson, D. (1999). The ambitious
Neild, R., Byrnes, V., & Sweet, T. (2010, March). Early generation: America’s teenagers, motivated but direc-
results from a randomized trial of two sequences for tionless. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
helping underprepared students to master algebra. Scott, S. (2007). Do grades really matter? Maclean’s,
Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the 120(35/36), 70–74.
Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness, Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, C. (2008). Teaching disciplin-
Washington, DC. ary literacy to adolescents: Rethinking content-area
Newman, F. (1992). Student engagement and achievement literacy. Harvard Educational Review, 78(1), 40–59.
in American secondary schools. New York: Teachers Shouse, R. D. (1997). Academic press, sense of commu-
College Press. nity, and student achievement. In J. S. Coleman, B.
Nystrand, M., & Gamoran, A. (1992). Instructional dis- Schneider, S. Plank, K. S. Schiller, R. Shouse, & H.
course and student engagement. In D. H. Schunk & J. Wang (Eds.), Redesigning American education (pp.
Meece (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom 60–86). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
(pp. 149–179). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Evelo, D. L., & Hurley,
Oakes, J., Gamoran, A., & Page, R. (1992). Curriculum C. M. (1998). Dropout prevention for youth with dis-
differentiation: Opportunities, outcomes, and mean- abilities: Efficacy of a sustained school engagement
ings. In P. Jackson (Ed.), Handbook of research on procedure. Exceptional Children, 65(1), 7–21.
curriculum (pp. 570–608). New York: Macmillan. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New
Osher, D., Bear, G. G., Sprague, J. R., & Doyle, W. (2010). York: Macmillan.
How can we improve school discipline? Educational Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the
Researcher, 39, 48–58. classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and
25 High School Reform and Student Engagement 539

student engagement across the school year. Journal of Wehlange, G. G., & Rutter, R. A. (1986). Dropping out:
Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581. How much do schools contribute to the problem?
Slavin, R. E. (1990). Achievement effects of ability group- Teachers College Record, 87(3), 374–392.
ing in secondary schools: A best-evidence synthesis. Wentzel, K. R. (1997). Student motivation in middle
Review of Educational Research, 60, 471–499. school: The role of perceived pedagogical caring.
Stern, D., Dayton, C., & Raby, M. (2010). Career acade- Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 411–419.
mies: A proven strategy to prepare high school Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social relationships and motiva-
students for college and careers. Berkeley, CA: Career tion in middle school: The role of parents, teachers,
Academy Support Network. and peers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2),
Stern, D., Raby, M., & Dayton, C. (1992). Career acade- 202–209.
mies: Partnerships for reconstructing American high Wentzel, K. R. (2002). Are effective teachers like good
schools. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. parents? Teaching styles and student adjustment in
Trumbull, E., & Farr, B. (Eds.). (2000). Grading and early adolescence. Child Development, 73,
reporting student progress in an age of standards. 287–301.
Portland, OR: Christopher-Gordon. Wentzel, K. R. (2009). Students’ relationships with teach-
U.S. General Accounting Office. (1993). School linked ers as motivational contexts. In K. R. Wentzel & A.
human services: A comprehensive strategy for aiding Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook on motivation at school
students at risk of school failure (Report to the (pp. 301–322). New York: Routledge.
Chairman, Committee on Labor and Human Yore, L. D., Pimm, D., & Tuan, H. (2007). The literacy
Resources). Washington, DC: Department of Health component of mathematical and scientific literacy.
and Human Services. International Journal of Science and Mathematics
Webb, F. R., Covington, M. V., & Guthrie, J. W. (1993). Education, 5(4), 559–589.
Carrots and sticks: Can school policy influence student Zimmerman, C. (2007). The development of scientific
motivation? In T. M. Tomlinson (Ed.), Motivating stu- thinking skills in elementary and middle school.
dents to learn (pp. 99–124). Berkeley, CA: McCutchan. Developmental Review, 27(2), 172–223.
The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing
Engagement, Motivation, 26
and Resilience in Students

Robert Brooks, Suzanne Brooks, and Sam Goldstein

Abstract
In this chapter, three interrelated concepts—student engagement, motiva-
tion, and resilience—are examined through the lens of “mindsets.”
Mindsets are assumptions that we possess about ourselves and others that
guide our behavior. The mindset that educators hold about the factors that
contribute to student engagement, motivation, and resilience determines
their expectations, teaching practices, and relationships with students. We
identify the key components of these three concepts, highlighting those
that overlap. We distinguish between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation
and the ways in which the latter is more closely attuned with student
engagement and resilience than the former. We encourage the ongoing
discussion of mindsets at staff meetings so that teachers become increas-
ingly aware of the mindset of engaged, motivated learners and consider
how to nurture this mindset in the classroom. We offer many strategies to
facilitate the enrichment of this mindset in all students.

In this chapter, we will describe the close link “mindsets.” Mindsets may be understood as
among three interrelated concepts: motivation, assumptions that we possess about ourselves and
student engagement, and resilience. We will others that guide our behavior. The mindsets that
examine these concepts through the lens of educators hold about the basic components of
motivation and engagement will determine their
expectations, teaching practices, and relation-
R. Brooks (*) ships with students (Brooks, 2001, 2004; Brooks
Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, & Goldstein, 2001, 2004, 2007, 2008; Goldstein
Boston, MA, USA & Brooks, 2007).
e-mail: contact@drrobertbrooks.com
The concept of mindsets has become an
S. Brooks increasingly prominent area of study, especially
Weston Public Schools, Weston, MA, USA
e-mail: Suzbrooks@comcast.net; Brooks@mail.weston.org
with the emergence of the field of “positive psy-
chology.” As examples, Carol Dweck authored a
S. Goldstein
University of Utah School of Medicine,
book titled Mindset (2006) in which she distin-
Salt Lake City, UT, USA guished between a “fixed” and “growth” outlook;
e-mail: info@samgoldstein.com; sam@samgoldstein.com the research of Martin Seligman and his

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 541


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_26, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
542 R. Brooks et al.

colleagues about “learned helplessness” and with learning and he feels very vulnerable every
“learned optimism” as well as resilience (Reivich day when he enters the school. I think that as a staff
we should figure out a different way of teaching
& Shatte, 2002; Seligman, 1990) have underpin- him because what we are doing now is a prescrip-
nings in attribution theory, which is basically tion for failure.”
about mindsets, examining how we understand In listening to these two descriptions of the
the reasons for our successes and setbacks same student, one could not help but conclude that
the teachers were offering opinions of two distinctly
(Weiner, 1974). different youngsters. It would not be surprising to
Educators bring assumptions about student discover that these vividly contrasting opinions or
behavior into all of their interactions with those in mindsets and the teacher behaviors they triggered
their classrooms and schools. The more aware they would likely contribute to John having markedly
different mindsets and responses to each of these
are of these assumptions, the more they can change two teachers. In fact, this was the case.
those beliefs that may work against the creation of After the meeting, Bob interviewed John and
a positive classroom environment. Even those asked him to describe his teachers, not revealing
assumptions about which we may not be cognizant what they had said about him. In describing the
teacher who had portrayed him very negatively,
have a way of being expressed to students. For John said with noticeable force, “She hates me, but
example, a teacher may be annoyed or frustrated that’s okay because I hate her. And I won’t do any
with a child without realizing that the anger is work in her class.”
rooted in the teacher’s assumption that the child’s John continued, “And don’t tell me that I’m
only hurting myself by not doing work (he must
constant asking of questions is an intentional ploy have heard that advice on numerous occasions).
to distract the class. In addition, the teacher may What you don’t understand, Dr. Brooks, is that in
not be aware that his annoyance is not as disguised her eyes I am a failure. Whatever I do in her class is
as he believes and is being communicated through never going to be good enough. She doesnt’ expect
me to pass, so why even try?” He said that from the
facial expressions and tone of voice. first day of class he felt “angry vibes” from her.
In contrast, another teacher with the same stu- She just didn’t like me and soon I didn’t like
dent may assume that the child’s ongoing ques- her. I could tell she didn’t want me in her class just
tions represent an attempt to understand the by the way she spoke to me. Right away she
seemed so angry at me. I really don’t know why
material being presented. This teacher is more she felt that way. So after a while I knew there was
likely to express positive verbal and nonverbal no way I could succeed in her class so I just decided
messages and to offer assistance, perceiving the that I wouldn’t even try. It would just be a waste of
child as being vulnerable and motivated rather time. She told me I was lazy, but if she was honest
she would have to admit that she doesn’t think
than being oppositional. I could ever get a good grade in her class.
The impact that the mindset of educators has John’s face lit up as he described the teacher who
in determining their approach to students and the thought that the primary issues that should be
extent to which they nurture motivation, student addressed were his struggles with learning and his
sense of vulnerability. He said, “I love her. She went
engagement, and resilience is apparent in the fol- out of her way the first week of school to tell me
lowing example: something. She said that she knew I was having trou-
ble with learning, but she thought I was smart and she
Parents of a high school student, John, contacted the
had to figure out the best way to teach me. She said
first author several years ago. They asked Bob to
that one of the reasons she became a teacher was to
serve as a consultant to John’s school program. An
help all students learn. She’s always there to help.”
earlier evaluation revealed that John was struggling
with learning disabilities and academic demands. In hearing John’s perception or mindset of these
When Bob met with John’s teachers and requested
that they share their perceptions of him, one imme- two teachers, it is not difficult to appreciate why he
diately responded with obvious anger, “John is one was a discipline problem with the first teacher but
of the most defiant, oppositional, lazy, unmotivated, not the second. His behavior with each of them
irresponsible students we have at this school!” reflected what he believed were their mindsets and
Another teacher seemed surprised by the harsh-
ness of this assessment. In a manner that maintained expectations for him. We recognize that it typi-
respect of her colleague’s opinion, she said, “I have cally takes “two to tango,” and most likely at some
a different view. I think John is really struggling point, John bore some responsibility for adding
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 543

fuel to the “angry vibes,” thereby confirming the promoting motivation. As has often been
first teacher’s negative perceptions of him. expressed, “Students don’t care what you
However, it is essential for educators to identify know until they first know you care.”
and modify those features of their mindset that Motivated students feel that teachers genu-
work against student motivation and student inely care about them as individuals and want
engagement and serve as barriers to students them to learn and to succeed (Klem & Connell,
becoming more optimistic and resilient. 2004; McCombs & Pope, 1994; Middleton &
Pettit, 2010; Wagner, Kegan, Lahey, &
Lemons, 2005). When struggling with an aca-
Guiding Questions for Consideration demic task or with nonacademic issues, the
successful student feels comfortable in taking
Given the power of mindsets in determining the the initiative and asking the teacher for assis-
social-emotional and learning climate created in tance. They do not perceive requesting help as
classrooms, several key questions can be raised: a weakness, but rather as an integral feature of
• What are the characteristics of the mindset of the classroom environment.
students who are motivated and engaged? 2. To believe that whether they learn as students
• What are the characteristics of the mindset of is based in great part on their own motivation,
resilient students? How do resilient students perseverance, and effort (Adelman & Taylor,
see themselves differently from their peers 1983; Brooks, 1991; Deci, Hodges, Pierson,
who are not resilient? In what ways does a & Tomassone, 1992; DiCintio & Gee, 1999;
“resilient mindset” overlap with the mindset Seligman, 1995; Weiner, 1974). This does not
of motivated, engaged students? minimize the role that teachers play, but if stu-
• What are the characteristics of the mindset of dents do not view themselves as active partici-
educators who are most effective in nurturing pants in the learning process, but rather as
motivation, engagement, and resilience in passive recipients of what is being taught,
students? their interest, enthusiasm, and involvement
• What specific strategies or interventions can for learning will be greatly diminished.
teachers with positive mindsets develop and 3. To recognize that making mistakes and not
implement to nurture motivation, engagement, immediately comprehending certain concepts
and resilience in their classrooms? or material are expected features of the learning
To answer these questions, one must also process. Students who persist when confronted
examine the following related question: with challenging learning tasks are those who
What are the main components housed in the believe that mistakes serve as the basis for
concepts of motivation, student engagement, and future learning and that mistakes invite new
resilience? learning strategies (Andrews & Debus, 1978;
Canino, 1981; Dweck, 1986, 2006). This out-
look is in sharp contrast to students who inter-
Characteristics of Students pret their mistakes as an indication that they are
not very intelligent and thus, they are incapable
The Mindset of Engaged, Motivated of correcting the situation. If they believe that
Students any efforts they make to learn will not eventu-
ate in success, they will not persevere in that
Goldstein and Brooks (2007) have identified five activity, demonstrating what Seligman (1990)
major characteristics of the mindset of motivated labeled as “learned helplessness.”
students. They include: 4. To have a clear understanding of their learn-
1. To perceive the teacher as a supportive adult. ing strengths and learning vulnerabilities. It is
We place this first to capture the essential rela- essential that learning strengths and vulnerabil-
tionship that teachers form with students in ities be identified for students (Levine, 2003).
544 R. Brooks et al.

As students gain insight into their learning 7. To feel comfortable relating with others and to
profile, the more they can develop and apply rely on effective interpersonal skills with peers
effective strategies to learn successfully and adults alike. This enables them to seek out
(Schunk & Rice, 1993). When students do assistance and nurturance in a comfortable,
not understand why they are struggling with appropriate manner from adults who can pro-
learning or when they believe they are dumb vide the support they need.
or stupid or lazy, they are more vulnerable 8. To believe that there is a purpose to their exis-
to engage in self-defeating ways of coping tence and that they are making a positive dif-
represented by noncompliant behaviors. ference in the lives of others.
5. To treat classmates with respect and avoid 9. To define the aspects of their lives over which
teasing or bullying, recognizing that such they have control and to focus their energy
behaviors work against a positive school cli- and attention on those rather than on factors
mate and adversely affect the learning of all over which they have little, or any, influence.
students (Davis, 2003; Olweus, 1994). Students Numerous researchers and clinicians have
must realize that maintaining a caring, respect- studied and articulated different features of this
ful classroom and school is the responsibility mindset (Masten, 2001; Masten & Coatsworth,
of each member of that classroom and school. 1998; Rutter, 1987; Seligman, 1995; Sheridan,
Eagle, & Dowd, 2005; Shure, 1996, 2003; Werner
& Smith, 1992, 2001; Wright & Masten, 2005).
The Mindset of Resilient Children As will be apparent, many of these features over-
and Adolescents lap with those associated within the mindset of
motivated learners.
Brooks and Goldstein (2001) have defined resil- It is our belief that educators can nurture
ience as the capacity to cope effectively and posi- mindsets associated with increased motivation,
tively with past or present adversity. They have engagement, and resilience as a natural part of
identified the outlook and skills associated with a their classroom teaching practices. It is important
“resilient mindset.” They include: to note that reinforcing social-emotional skills
1. To be able to set realistic goals and expecta- should not be perceived as an “extra curriculum”
tions for themselves. that ciphers already limited time from teaching
2. To believe that they have the ability to solve academic subject matter. In fact, our position is
problems and make thoughtful decisions and that the more secure and engaged students are,
thus are more likely to view mistakes, set- the more motivated they will be to meet academic
backs, and obstacles as challenges to confront requirements.
rather than as stressors to avoid. Let us turn now to examining the concepts and
3. To rely on effective coping strategies that pro- components of motivation and student engage-
mote growth and are not self-defeating. ment before identifying the mindset and practices
4. To be aware of and not deny their weaknesses of teachers who are skilled in nurturing these
and vulnerabilities. They do not view these qualities in students.
vulnerabilities as flaws but rather as areas for
improvement. They also realistically accept
when certain tasks may be beyond their abili- Motivation, Student Engagement,
ties at the present time but open to change in and Resilience
the future.
5. To recognize, enjoy, and engage in their strong Motivation: Intrinsic or Extrinsic—
points and talents. Autonomous or Controlled
6. To possess a self-concept that is filled with
images of strength and competence or what There is no simple answer to the question, “What
we have referred to as “islands of competence” is the relationship between student engagement
(Brooks, 2004; Brooks & Goldstein, 2001). and motivation?” As we shall see, not only is the
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 545

concept of student engagement multidimensional students becoming more engaged with learning
(Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, 2008; tasks or becoming more resilient unless features
Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, 2006; of intrinsic motivation are incorporated within
Christenson & Anderson, 2002) but so too is the practices of extrinsic motivation.
motivation, which without wishing to simplify Lepper, Greene, and Nisbett (1973) conducted
things has primarily been cast as residing in two a study in the early 1970s that generated much
broad camps, namely, motivation that is intrinsi- dialogue about those factors involved in motivat-
cally or extrinsically driven. ing children to engage in particular activities.
To capture the key dimensions of intrinsic and Their research is often cited in the literature about
extrinsic motivation, psychologists Edward Deci motivation, not simply as a result of the topic it
and Richard Ryan at the University of Rochester examined, but because their findings were coun-
in New York have advanced “self-determination terintuitive to what many anticipated.
theory” (SDT) (Deci & Flaste, 1995; Deci, Lepper, Greene, and Nisbett observed a pre-
Koestner, & Ryan, 2001; Deci & Ryan, 2000). school class and identified those children who
Instead of the words intrinsic and extrinsic, they chose to draw during their “free time” play. Then,
prefer to use the concepts autonomous and they designed an experiment to discover what
controlled. happens when you reward an activity that the
They distinguish autonomous from controlled children already enjoyed doing. The researchers
in the following way (Deci & Flaste, 1995): divided the children into three groups. The first
was called the “expected-award” group. They
To be autonomous means to act in accord with
showed each of the children in this group a “Good
one’s self—it means feeling free and volitional in
one’s actions. When autonomous, people are fully Player” certificate featuring a blue ribbon and the
willing to do what they are doing, and they embrace child’s name; they told these children that they
the activity with a sense of interest and commit- would receive an award for drawing. The second
ment. Their actions emanate from their true sense
group was designated the “unexpected award”
of self so they are being authentic. In contrast, to
be controlled means to act because one is being group. These children were asked if they wanted
pressured. When controlled, people act without a to draw, and if they did, they were given one of
sense of personal endorsement. Their behavior is the “Good Player” certificates when the session
not an expression of the self, for the self has been
concluded. They did not know in advance that
subjugated to the controls. In this condition, people
can reasonably be described as alienated (p. 2). they would receive an award. The third group
was the “no award” group. These preschoolers
As we attempt to understand the relationship were asked if they wanted to draw, but they were
between motivation and student engagement and neither promised a certificate prior to drawing
consider the two main types of motivation spot- nor given one at the end.
lighted by Deci and Ryan, we might be better Two weeks later, the teachers of the preschool-
served to ask the questions, “Does intrinsic ers put out paper and markers during the “free
(autonomous) or extrinsic (controlled) motiva- play” period while the researchers secretly
tion contribute more to the enrichment of student observed the students. A central question being
engagement? Or, is there any difference at all? Or studied was whether being involved in one of the
can aspects of intrinsic motivation be applied three groups 2 weeks earlier would have any
even when extrinsic motivation is used?” impact on the child’s behavior now. If so, what
We would argue that the variables associated would it be? One prediction was that an award
with intrinsic motivation are much more closely given 2 weeks earlier would not impact apprecia-
aligned with both student engagement and resil- bly or at all on the child’s behavior today. Another
ience than those embedded within extrinsic moti- possibility, strongly rooted in what Pink (2009)
vation. To take this argument a step further, it is called “The Motivation 2.0 Operating System,”
our belief that practices predicated upon extrinsic would be that the children who received awards
motivation may, at times, actually work against for engaging in drawing would display even
546 R. Brooks et al.

greater interest in and motivation to draw since and Ryan (2000) who contended that there are
they were rewarded for that behavior. Motivation three basic, innate, psychological needs that we
2.0 is based on the premise that the way you all possess: the need to belong or feel connected,
motivate people to do what you want is to reward the need to feel competent, and the need for
them for the behavior you seek and punish them autonomy or self-determination. Deci and Ryan
for behavior you do not want to appear. It is pred- asserted that when these needs are satisfied, moti-
icated on extrinsic motivation. vation and productivity are increased, but when
The tenets of Motivation 2.0 would lead one they are not met, motivation and satisfaction are
to assume that those children told in advance they diminished.
would receive a reward for drawing would be Ryan observed, “This is a really big thing in
most motivated 2 weeks later to engage in this management. When people aren’t producing,
activity since it had been rewarded previously. companies typically resort to rewards or punish-
This seemed to be a logical conclusion, based on ment. What they haven’t done is the hard work of
the notion that providing external rewards for diagnosing what the problem is. You’re trying to
accomplishing particular tasks would increase run over the problem with a carrot or a stick”
involvement in these tasks. It was basically the (Pink, 2009, p. 72). Deci added that self-determi-
model articulated by famed psychologist B. F. nation theory does not unequivocally oppose the
Skinner in which the occurrence of certain behav- use of rewards. “Of course, they’re necessary in
iors was either increased or decreased by the use workplaces and other settings, but the less salient
of rewards and punishment. they are made, the better” (Pink, 2009, p. 72).
However, what those subscribing to an extrin- Pink (2009) summarized the limited conditions
sic motivation model may have hypothesized was under which extrinsic motivation may be benefi-
not in keeping with what Lepper, Greene, and cial. “For routine tasks, which aren’t very inter-
Nisbett discovered. Children in the “unexpected- esting and don’t demand much creative thinking,
award” and “no award” groups drew just as much rewards can provide a small motivational booster
and with the same enthusiasm as they had before shot without harmful side effects. In some ways,
the experiment. But children in the first group— that’s just common sense” (p. 62). Pink recom-
the ones who had expected and then been given mended that even routine tasks can be made more
an award—displayed much less interest and spent enticing by lessening control and introducing
much less time drawing. Even 2 weeks later, the autonomy. “Allow people to complete the task
prizes—so common in many classrooms—had their own way. Think autonomy not control. State
seemingly transformed play into work. It is the outcome you need. But instead of specifying
important to point out that it was not necessarily precisely the way to reach it, give them freedom
the rewards themselves that reduced the chil- over how they do the job” (2009, p. 64).
dren’s interest since when children did not expect Appleton and colleagues (2008) captured the
a reward, receiving one had little impact on their complexity of both SDT and the concepts of
intrinsic motivation. Only contingent rewards— extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. They high-
if you do this, then you will get that—had the lighted at least two features of SDT that are espe-
negative effect. cially relevant for educators. First, similar to
The results of this study invite the question of Pink’s contention that even seemingly external
why did not the so-called “extrinsic motivators” demands can be offered in a way that provide a
heighten interest in drawing? Also, do the results modicum of internal control, SDT posited that in
represent an anomaly not to be replicated in other those situations in which the catalyst for behavior
studies? Pink (2009), in reviewing the literature, is external to oneself, aspects of internal control
cited many other examples of the negative impact can be established (Ryan & Deci, 2000). In sup-
of rewarding particular behaviors. port of this position, Appleton et al. wrote, “The
An explanation for these unexpected findings theory (SDT) specifies qualitative differences in
may be found in the position advanced by Deci the level of self-determination associated with
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 547

external motivation; situates these levels along a challenge of the task. A task that is either too easy
continuum; and contends that external expecta- or too difficult given the skills of the individual
tions can be internalized, integrated, and result in will not permit the experience of flow to emerge.
highly autonomous functioning” (p. 378). The concept of flow as proposed by
The second aspect of SDT Appleton et al. Csikszentmihalyi is linked to both motivation
(2008) identified that is highly relevant for teach- and engagement and houses major implications
ing practices is related to the first feature. It high- for the teaching style and curriculum presented
lighted the importance of contextual factors and by teachers. If students are to experience flow,
suggested that teachers have greater power than they must be challenged to move beyond their
they may recognize to accentuate and reinforce current levels of competence in activities that are
autonomous behaviors in the school environment interesting and relevant to them and that encour-
even when external demands appear to dominate age their input and feedback.
the school arena. In the face of educational In addition to autonomy and mastery, the third
requirements and curricula that seem fixed or nutriment of motivation emphasized by Pink is
perhaps rigid, teachers are empowered to ask, purpose, which relates to commitment, meaning,
“How can I implement teaching strategies that and the belief that one’s activities are of benefit to
integrate intrinsic motivation principles within a others. This sense of purpose and commitment
more controlled environment?” This question has been identified as a notable feature of resil-
encourages educators to reflect upon and appreci- ience (Rutter, 1980; Werner, 1993) and of a resil-
ate the impact they have on enhancing student ient mindset (see point #8 above and Brooks &
motivation and engagement even within a more Goldstein, 2001). As we shall see later in this
restrictive educational milieu. chapter, purpose and commitment also serve as a
Appleton et al. (2008) provided some guidance foundation for becoming stress hardy (Kobasa,
for moving toward greater autonomy regardless Maddi, & Kahn, 1982). Pink (2009) wrote,
of the environmental restraints. “Educators can “Autonomous people working toward mastery
facilitate student self-determination with extrinsi- perform at very high levels. But those who do so
cally motivated tasks by using relationships, set- in the service of some greater objective can
ting up students for success in course tasks (via achieve even more. The most deeply motivated
scaffolding of lessons and attention to develop- people—not to mention those who are most pro-
mental level), and orchestrating student opportu- ductive and satisfied—hitch their desires to a
nities for decision making and other authentically cause larger than themselves” (p. 133).
autonomous experiences” (pp. 378–379). Support Earlier, we expressed our position that in com-
for this position is found in a number of research parison with extrinsic or controlled motivation,
studies (Grolnick, Ryan, & Deci, 1991; Maehr & the components of intrinsic or autonomous moti-
Meyer, 1997; Miserandino, 1996; Vansteenkiste, vation were most in accord with nurturing resil-
Simons, Lens, Sheldon, & Deci, 2004). ient, engaged students. Let us turn our attention
In considering SDT, Pink (2009) reframed to to the concept of “student engagement” to under-
some extent Deci and Ryan’s three basic needs of stand the basis for this position.
autonomy, belonging, and competence, casting
autonomy as the essential component, but
describing “mastery” and “purpose” as two other Student Engagement:
dimensions of intrinsic motivation. Mastery is A Multidimensional Concept
viewed as the pleasure that accrues from being
engaged in a task that is exciting and challenging. Christenson and her colleagues articulated the
Csikszentmihalyi (1975, 1998) introduced the various dimensions of engagement in schools
concept of flow, a state in which people are and developed the Student Engagement
absorbed and challenged by what they are doing. Instrument (SEI) (Anderson, Christenson,
A key quality producing flow is the level of the Sinclair, & Lehr, 2004; Appleton et al., 2006,
548 R. Brooks et al.

2008; Christenson et al., 2008). They noted that developing and adhering to learning goals), and
the distinction between motivation and engage- emotional or affective (e.g., showing an interest
ment remains an ongoing issue. As a point of in and positive attitude toward learning, having a
illustration, they identified one conceptual frame- sense of belonging and connectedness) (Fredricks,
work in which motivation is cast in terms of the Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; Jimerson, Campos,
direction and intensity of one’s energy (Maehr & & Greif, 2003).
Meyer, 1997). In this framework, motivation is Christenson and her colleagues (Betts,
linked to underlying psychological processes Appleton, Reschly, Christenson, & Huebner,
such as autonomy, belonging or connectedness, 2010; Christenson & Anderson, 2002; Reschly &
and competence and is perceived to answer the Christenson, 2006) proposed a taxonomy for
question of “why” for a given behavior. defining student levels of engagement as well as
In contrast, engagement has been described as for identifying the goodness of fit between the
“energy in action,” the connection between per- student, the learning environment, and factors
son and activity (Russell, Ainley, & Frydenberg, that impact upon the fit. They viewed engagement
2005, p. 1), and reflects a person’s active involve- as comprised of four subtypes: academic, behav-
ment in a task or activity. Appleton et al. (2006) ioral, cognitive, and psychological. Appleton
wrote, “Although motivation is central to under- et al. (2006) elaborated on this taxonomy:
standing engagement, the latter is a construct There are multiple indicators for each subtype. For
worthy of study in its own right” (p. 428). example, academic engagement consists of vari-
Engagement, achievement, and school behav- ables such as time on task, credits earned toward
ior were found to be associated with each other. graduation, and homework completion, while
attendance, suspensions, voluntary classroom par-
Low student engagement heightened the likeli- ticipation, and extra-curricular participation are
hood of students dropping out of school. Check indicators of behavioral engagement. Cognitive
& Connect is one illustration of a targeted inter- and psychological engagement includes less
vention program designed to promote student observable, more internal indicators, such as self-
regulation, relevance of schoolwork to future
engagement (Appleton et al., 2006; Christenson endeavors, value of learning, and personal goals
& Thurlow, 2004; Sinclair, Christenson, & and autonomy (for cognitive engagement), and
Thurlow, 2005). Key components of Check & feelings of identification or belonging, and rela-
Connect are closely related to the features of a tionships with teachers and peers (for psychologi-
cal engagement) (p. 419).
resilient mindset, perhaps the most important of
which is a mentor who works with students and Appleton et al. (2006) also emphasized the
their families for a minimum of 2 years. Mentors importance of the context in which these subtypes
promote problem-solving skills, persistence, and occur such as relationships with adults at school,
learning within a supportive relationship. Mentors encouragement from family members, and sup-
also focus on nurturing their mentee’s sense of port from peers. In addition, they wrote that while
autonomy, belonging, and competence, which the majority of research has been directed toward
parallel the main ingredients of SDT proposed by the academic and behavioral components of stu-
Deci and Ryan (2000). dent engagement since they tend to lend them-
Finn (1989) advanced the view that engage- selves to more precise observation, “measuring
ment can be conceptualized as being comprised cognitive and psychological engagement is rele-
of two main components: behavioral (e.g., par- vant because there is an overemphasis in school
ticipation in school activities) and affective (e.g., practice on indicators of academic and behavioral
identifying oneself with the school, having a engagement. Such overemphasis ignores the bud-
sense of belonging and connectedness). More ding literature that suggests that cognitive and psy-
recent reviews of the literature have posited that chological engagement indicators are associated
engagement is made up of three variables: behav- with positive learning outcomes, related to moti-
ioral (e.g., appropriate demeanor, effort, active vation, and increase in response to specific teach-
participation), cognitive (e.g., self-regulation, ing strategies” (p. 431). The SEI was developed to
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 549

Table 26.1 Common components or beliefs (mindset) associated with student engagement, motivation, and resilience
• I believe that adults are encouraging and supportive rather than judgmental and accusatory
• I am connected to and welcome in the school environment
• My opinion is respected, that I have, within reason, some say or input into my own education
• I am accountable for my actions
• My interests and strengths (“islands of competence”) are identified and reinforced
• Academic demands are challenging, but in keeping with my abilities; my teachers and I are aware of my learning
strengths and vulnerabilities
• Mistakes are perceived as expected and accepted. I never feel criticized because of these mistakes, but rather I use
mistakes as the basis for future learning
• I am provided with opportunities to contribute to the well-being of both the school community and beyond
• All members of the school community are respectful toward each other

measure both cognitive and psychological engage- 4. Students take leadership roles in representing
ment, which has subsequently been labeled affec- and “owning” the school, exhibiting energy
tive engagement (Appleton et al., 2008). and enthusiasm about their institution.
The International Center for Leadership in 5. The physical space is clean and safe.
Education (ICLE), which researched and devel- 6. Regular forums, structures, and interactions
oped a model of teaching based on the concepts of acknowledge and celebrate school and indi-
rigor and relevance, advanced the view that stu- vidual success.
dent engagement is an essential underpinning of 7. The school actively involves and engages
these dimensions of the learning process (Jones, family and community members in the life of
Marrazo, & Love, 2007). Jones (2009) asserted the school.
that while student engagement is not an exact sci- 8. The school promotes and supports student
ence, it can be planned, measured, and enriched. activism by helping students engage in com-
He described student engagement as the: munity change (pp. 37–38).
Positive behaviors that indicate full participation
by the student in the learning process. When stu-
dents are engaged, we can hear, see, or feel their Commonalities Among Motivation,
motivation in completing a task, taking pride in
their work, or going beyond the minimum work Engagement, and Resilience
required. Engaged students demonstrate a feeling
of belonging by the way they act, the positive If educators are to nurture motivation, engage-
things they say about school, and through their ment, and resilience in students, they should
passionate involvement in class activities (p. 1).
attend to and reinforce the common components
Based on a review of the literature and research associated with the mindset of each of these con-
conducted by ICLE, Jones identified those factors cepts. There are many commonalities, especially
that contribute to a school milieu in which student if the underpinnings of intrinsic motivation as
engagement is nurtured. Many of these factors opposed to extrinsic motivation are used in the
are similar to those described above for the mind- comparison. A summary of the common beliefs
set of motivated, resilient learners. They include: (mindset) are included in Table 26.1.
1. Interactions between and among students, It is important to emphasize that each of the
teachers, administrators, parents, etc., are beliefs listed in Table 26.1 are part of the founda-
respectful, collegial, and warm. tions of student engagement, motivation, and resil-
2. There is an atmosphere of mutual accountabil- ience. They are also part of a student’s mindset and
ity; people feel a sense of responsibility to one therefore open to reinforcement. Teachers who are
another and to the larger school community. most effective in reinforcing these beliefs in stu-
3. Signs of positive community identity and a dents and thereby creating a school climate in
sense of belonging permeate the school. which motivation, engagement, and resilience are
550 R. Brooks et al.

nurtured are guided by their own specific beliefs 4. To believe that attending to the social-
and mindsets, a topic to which we now turn. emotional needs of students is not an “extra
curriculum” that draws time away from
teaching academic subjects, but rather a sig-
Educators’ Beliefs and Practices nificant feature of effective teaching that
enriches learning.
The Mindset of Effective Educators 5. To recognize that if educators are to relate
effectively to students, they must be empathic,
A consideration of the mindset of students who always attempting to perceive the world
are motivated, engaged, and resilient invites sev- through the eyes of the student and consider-
eral other questions, including two listed earlier: ing the ways in which students view them.
What are the characteristics of the mindset of 6. To appreciate that the foundation for suc-
educators who are most effective in nurturing cessful learning and a safe and secure class-
motivation, engagement, and resilience in stu- room climate is the relationship that teachers
dents? What specific strategies or interventions forge with students.
can teachers with positive mindsets develop and 7. To recognize that students will be more moti-
implement to nurture motivation, engagement, vated to learn and more engaged in the class-
and resilience in their classrooms? room when they feel a sense of ownership or
It is essential for educators to appreciate that autonomy for their own education.
the assumptions they hold for themselves and 8. To understand that one of the main functions
their students, often unstated, have profound influ- of an educator is to be a disciplinarian in the
ence in determining effective teaching practices, true sense of the word, namely, to perceive
the quality of relationships with students, and the discipline as a teaching process rather than as
positive or negative climate that is created in the a process of intimidation and humiliation.
classroom and school building. It is also essential Disciplinary practices should reinforce self-
that teachers discuss and examine the mindsets of discipline, which is a critical behavior asso-
effective, motivated learners and consider how to ciated with resilience.
nurture this mindset in the classroom. 9. To realize that one of the greatest obstacles to
The following are assumptions and beliefs learning is the fear of making mistakes and
held by educators about students that appear most feeling embarrassed or humiliated and to
likely to eventuate in practices that nurture stu- take active steps to minimize this fear.
dent motivation, engagement, and resilience 10. To subscribe to a strength-based model,
(Goldstein & Brooks, 2007): which includes identifying and reinforcing
1. To appreciate that they have a lifelong impact each student’s “islands of competence.”
on students, including on their sense of hope 11. To develop and maintain positive, respectful
and resilience. relationships with colleagues and parents.
2. To believe that the level of motivation and
learning that occurs in the classroom and the
behavior exhibited by students has as much, Themes and Exercises to Nurture
if not more, to do with the influence of teach- a Positive Mindset in Educators
ers than what students might bring into the
situation. Information can be imparted to teachers, and
3. To believe that all students yearn to be suc- exercises can be introduced to articulate and rein-
cessful, and if a student is not learning, edu- force these beliefs associated with nurturing stu-
cators must ask how they can adapt their dent motivation, engagement, and resilience. The
teaching style and instructional material to goal is for all faculty and staff in a school to share
meet student needs. within reason a common perspective or mindset.
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 551

The following are suggested themes for discus- those from whom they learned most effectively.
sion and exercises to facilitate this task: It has been our experience that the teachers they
select are those who not only taught academic
The focus on a student’s social/emotional devel- content but, in addition, supported the emotional
opment and well-being is not an extra curriculum well-being of students and were interested in the
that takes time away from teaching academic “whole child.” Very importantly, as educators
skills and content. As we noted earlier in this reflect upon their teachers as well as their own
chapter, it is unfortunate that a dichotomy has teaching practices, they can be asked to consider
arisen in many educational quarters prompting the following question: “Do you believe that
some educators to perceive that attending to a developing a positive relationship with your stu-
student’s emotional and social health is mutually dents enhances or detracts from teaching aca-
exclusive from the goal of teaching academic demic material? Please offer examples.”
material. This dichotomy has been fueled, in part, Examples should be encouraged whether the
by the emergence of high stakes testing and an answer is yes, no, or maybe. It is important for
emphasis on accountability. The following refrain educators to give serious consideration to this
is heard in many schools: “We barely have time question. In our experience, most educators are
to get through the assigned curriculum. We really able to offer examples of “small gestures” on
don’t have the time to focus on anything else.” their part (or on the part of their teachers) that
We are not opposed to assessment or account- took little, if any, time, but communicated to stu-
ability. We welcome research conducted to define dents a message of respect and caring (Brooks,
effective teaching practices. However, what we 1991). If teachers contend they would like to
question is relegating a student’s emotional life develop more meaningful relationships with stu-
to the background and not appreciating its impor- dents, but are unable to allot the time to do so,
tant role in the process of learning. This attitude other educators who have been able to accom-
was captured at one of our workshops. A high plish this task can offer specific suggestions.
school science teacher challenged our viewpoint
by contending: “I am a science teacher. I know Educators have a lifelong impact on students and
my science and I know how to convey science their resilience. Closely associated with this pre-
facts to my students. Why should I have to spend vious point is the belief of teachers that what they
time thinking about a student’s emotional or say and do each day in their classroom can have
social life? I don’t have time to do so and it will a lifelong influence on their students (Brooks,
distract me from teaching science.” 1991; Brooks & Goldstein, 2001). While most
While many teachers and school administra- teachers appreciate that they are and will con-
tors would take issue with the views expressed by tinue to be influential in the lives of their students
this science teacher, others might not. We believe for years to come, many are not aware of the
that strengthening a student’s feeling of well- extent of their impact.
being, self-esteem, and dignity is not an extra It is important that teachers are acquainted
curriculum. If anything, a student’s sense of with research findings from the resilience litera-
belonging, security, and self-confidence in the ture to highlight this impact. Such knowledge
classroom provides the scaffolding that supports will add meaning and purpose to their role as
the foundation for enhanced learning, engage- teachers and lessen disillusionment and burnout.
ment, motivation, self-discipline, responsibility, In the past 25 years, there has been an increased
and the ability to deal more effectively with effort to define those factors that help children
obstacles and mistakes (Brooks, 1991, 2004; and adolescents to deal more effectively with
Cohen, 2006; Cohen & Sandy, 2003; Elias, Zins, stress, to overcome adversity, and to become
Graczyk, & Weissberg, 2003). resilient (Brooks, 1994; Brooks & Goldstein,
To highlight this point, educators can be asked 2001; Goldstein & Brooks, 2005; Katz, 1997;
to reflect on their own teachers and think about Werner & Smith, 1992). We highlight that schools
552 R. Brooks et al.

have been spotlighted as environments in which motivating forces described by Pink (2009) in his
self-esteem, hope, and resilience can be fortified, elaboration of SDT, namely, the existence of
frequently quoting the late psychologist Julius “purpose” in our lives.
Segal (1988) who wrote:
All students wish to learn and to succeed, and if
From studies conducted around the world, research-
ers have distilled a number of factors that enable they seem unmotivated or disengaged, they may
such children of misfortune to beat the heavy odds believe they lack the ability to achieve in school.
against them. One factor turns out to be the pres- We often hear teachers refer to students as lazy or
ence in their lives of a charismatic adult—a person unmotivated. As we have noted, once these accu-
with whom they can identify and from whom they
gather strength. And in a surprising number of satory labels are used and a negative mindset dom-
cases, that person turns out to be a teacher (p. 3). inates, educators are more likely to respond to
these students with annoyance. The mindset of an
It is important for teachers to recognize that effective educator constantly echoes, “I believe
they are in a unique position to be a “charismatic that all students come to school desiring to learn. If
adult” in a student’s life and that even seemingly they are disinterested and feel defeated, we must
small gestures can have a lifelong impact. A figure out how best to reach and teach them.”
smile, a warm greeting, a note of encouragement, Subscribing to this view has a profound impact
a few minutes taken to meet alone with a student, on the ways in which we respond to students,
and an appreciation of and respect for different especially those who are struggling. When stu-
learning styles are but several of the activities that dents lose faith in their ability to learn and when
define a “charismatic teacher” (Brooks, 1991). feelings of hopelessness pervade their psyche,
Teachers are often unaware that they are or they are vulnerable to engaging in counterpro-
have been “charismatic adults” in the life of a ductive or self-defeating ways of coping. They
student. To emphasize this issue, faculty can be may quit at tasks, clown around, pick on other
asked if they have ever received unexpectedly, a students, or expend little time and effort in aca-
note from a former student thanking them for the demic requirements. When a student feels that
positive impact they had on the student’s life. failure is a foregone conclusion, it is difficult to
While many have been fortunate to be the recipi- muster the energy to consider alternative ways of
ent of such a note, others have not although they mastering learning demands.
are equally deserving of such feedback. Teachers who observe such counterproductive
We frequently ask participants at our work- behaviors may easily reach the conclusion that
shops if there are teachers who had a significant the student is unmotivated or lazy, or not caring
influence on their lives whom they have failed to about school. As negative assumptions and mind-
acknowledge via a note or letter. It is not unusual sets dominate, teachers are less likely to consider
for many teachers to voice regret they have not more productive strategies for reaching the stu-
thanked several such “charismatic adults.” Some dent. Instead, thoughts turn to punitive actions;
have written notes to the latter following the for example, what punishments would finally get
workshop. through to the student. However, if educators
We use these exercises to suggest that while subscribe to the belief that each student wishes to
we may not receive formal confirmation that we succeed, negative assumptions are less likely to
have worn the garb of “charismatic adults,” if we prevail.
approach each day with the belief that today may A shift in perspective was obvious in a consul-
be the day we say or do something that directs a tation Bob did about Sarah, a problematic high
student’s life in a more positive path, we will be school student. One of her teachers began by ask-
more optimistic about our role, and our students ing, “Don’t you think it’s okay for a 16 1/2-year-
will be the beneficiaries of more realistic, hopeful old to drop out of school?” The agenda was clear.
expectations. The belief that we can serve as These teachers, who typically displayed a caring
“charismatic adults” serves as one of the major and encouraging attitude, were very frustrated
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 553

and angry with Sarah to the extent of wishing her appear on the road to success—obstacles that
to drop out of school. The teachers elaborated teachers working in concert with students could
that Sarah was a student who “sabotaged” all of remove.
their efforts. “Even if Sarah agrees to do some-
thing, she doesn’t follow through. It’s obvious If our strategies are not effective, we must ask,
that she dislikes school and she’s disruptive and “What is it that I can do differently?” rather than
disrespectful. She couldn’t care less about how continuing to wait for the student to change first.
she does in school.” A basic underpinning of motivation and resil-
As we shall see, Sarah cared a great deal about ience is the belief of “personal control,” namely,
wanting to achieve in school, but entertained little that we are the “authors of our own lives,” and it
hope for doing so. It was only when her teachers makes little sense to continue to do the same
truly accepted that each student desperately wants thing repeatedly if our actions are not leading to
to succeed that a positive mindset emerged, which positive results (Brooks & Goldstein, 2004).
permitted them to consider new solutions. A turn- While many educators and others say they sub-
ing point occurred when Bob empathized with scribe to this assumption, their actions frequently
the teachers about their frustration but then asked, belie their assertion. For example, it is not unusual
“Can anyone tell me how you think Sarah feels to hear the following statements offered by edu-
each day when she enters the school building?” cators at consultations we have conducted: “This
After several moments of silence, one teacher student is unmotivated to change. She just won’t
responded, “How Sarah feels. I never really take responsibility for her behavior.”
thought about that before.” Another teacher fol- Or, “We’ve been using this strategy with this
lowed, “I never really thought about that before student for five months. He’s still not responding.
either, but as I’m doing so now, only one word He’s resistant and oppositional.” We believe in
comes to mind, defeated. I think everyday when perseverance, but if a staff has been employing
Sarah comes in to the school building she feels the same approach for 5 months without any pos-
defeated.” itive outcome, one can ask, “Who are the resis-
As this teacher shared her observation, the shift tant ones here?”
in mindset that permeated the room was palpable, As one perceptive teacher emphasized,
highlighted by one teacher asking Bob, “You’ve “Asking what is it that we can do differently
written a lot about helping kids be more confident should not be seen as blaming ourselves but
and resilient in the school setting. So what can we rather as a source of empowerment.” She contin-
do to help a student who feels defeated begin to ued, “Isn’t it better to focus on what we can do
feel less defeated?” A lively, creative discussion differently rather than continue to wait for some-
ensued, filled with ideas that had not been consid- one else to change first? We may have to wait
ered previously, including having Sarah, who rel- forever and continue to be frustrated and
ished being helpful, assist in the office. The unhappy.” This same teacher summarized her
teachers also shifted their focus from what puni- belief with the statement, “If the horse is dead,
tive action to take to a desire to “get to know” get off.” We have found that there are many dead
Sarah, not via a tense, confrontational meeting but horses strewn on the grounds of a school.
rather by having lunch with her. The assumption of personal control should be
This new approach prompted Sarah to be more addressed directly at staff meetings. Teachers
responsible, and a positive cycle was set in should recognize that a change in strategy on
motion. The catalyst for this new cycle was when their part is not the equivalent of “giving in” (this
her teachers shifted their mindset, no longer view- is a belief that often crops up), but rather as a sign
ing Sarah’s behaviors as oppositional, but rather that we are seeking a more productive interven-
as a reflection of the despair and defeatism she tion. If change on a teacher’s part is interpreted as
experienced. They adopted the assumption that acquiescing to the student, any new strategy will
students wish to succeed, but at times, obstacles be tainted by feelings of resentment.
554 R. Brooks et al.

A helpful exercise to illustrate the power of way that my students will be most likely to hear
personal control and the need to change “nega- and respond constructively to my message?”
tive scripts” that exist in our lives is to ask educa- As an example, a teacher may attempt to moti-
tors to think about one or two instances when vate a student who is not performing adequately
they changed their usual script and to consider by exhorting the student to “try harder.” While the
what resulted as a consequence of their new teacher may be well-intentioned, the comment is
script. Many educators, such as those involved based on the assumption that the student is not
with Sarah, are able to describe very positive willing to expend the time and energy necessary to
results. Unfortunately, others report less satisfac- succeed. Thus, such a remark is frequently experi-
tory results, often believing that they had gone enced as accusatory and judgmental. When stu-
out of their way for students, but the students did dents feel accused, they are less prone to be
not reciprocate. When the outcome of a change in cooperative. Consequently, the teacher’s comment
script is not positive, a problem-solving attitude is not likely to lead to the desired results, which, in
should be introduced by asking, “With hindsight, turn, may reinforce the teacher’s belief that the
is there anything you would do differently today student is unmotivated and not interested in “try-
to lessen the probability of an unfavorable ing.” In contrast, an empathic teacher might won-
result?” der, “If I were struggling in my role as a teacher,
The possibility that a modification of a script would I want another teacher or my principal to
may not eventuate in a positive outcome should say to me, ‘If you just tried a little harder you
be addressed. When a new script is implemented, wouldn’t have this problem’?” When we have
educators should have one or two backup scripts offered this question at workshops, many teachers
in mind should the first prove ineffective. Having laugh and say they would be very annoyed if they
a backup script conveys the positive message that were accused of not trying. The question prompts
if a strategy that sounds promising does not yield them to reflect upon how their statements are inter-
the results we wish, rather than feel exasperated preted by their students.
or defeated, we should learn from the experience There are several exercises that can be intro-
and be prepared with alternative actions. We must duced at staff meetings to reinforce empathy. A
keep in mind that a new script may create the favorite is to have teachers think of a teacher they
conditions that encourage students to change liked and one that they did not like when they
their behaviors. were students and then to describe each in several
words. Next, they can be reminded, “Just as you
Empathy is an essential skill for effective teach- have words to describe your teachers, your stu-
ing and relationships with students as well as dents have words to describe you.” They can then
parents and colleagues. Empathic educators are consider these questions: What words would you
able to place themselves inside the shoes of their hope your students used to describe you? What
students and others and perceive the world have you done in the past month so they are likely
through their eyes, just as Sarah’s teachers to use these words? What words would they actu-
attempted to do, eventually understanding that ally use to describe you? How close would the
she felt defeated. Goleman (1994) highlighted words you hope they use parallel the words they
empathy as a major component of emotional would actually use? (One teacher jokingly said,
intelligence. “I would love my students to use the word ‘calm,’
Being empathic invites educators to ask, but I don’t think they would since I feel I have
“Would I want anyone to say or do to me what I been raising my voice a great deal the past month
have just said or done to this student (or parent or or two and not showing much patience”).
colleague)?” or “Whenever I say or do things Another exercise that educators have found
with students (parents or colleagues), what is my useful in reinforcing empathy revolves around our
goal and am I saying or doing these things in a own memories of school. Teachers can be requested
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 555

at workshops to share with their colleagues their There are various ways for teachers to obtain
response to the following questions: student feedback and input. For instance, teach-
• Of all of the memories you have as a student, ers can request anonymous feedback from stu-
what is one of your favorite ones, something dents. One high school teacher asked students to
that a teacher or school administrator said or draw him, describe him, list what they liked
did that boosted your motivation and self- about his teaching style and the class, and what
dignity? they would recommend he change. While one of
• Of all of the memories you have as a student, his colleagues scoffed at this practice, contend-
what is one of your worst ones, something that ing that such feedback was not important and
a teacher or school administrator said or did took valuable time from teaching, the outcome
that lessened your motivation and self-dignity? of the exercise proved the colleague wrong. The
• As you reflect upon both your positive and exercise actually increased achievement scores
negative memories of school, what did you and cooperation; this was not surprising since
learn from both, and do you use these memo- the students felt respected. Another teacher
ries to guide what you are doing with your stu- requested that students complete a one-page
dents today? report card about him whenever he filled out
Recounting one’s own positive and negative report cards on them. The students actually
memories of school with one’s colleagues often developed the report card, which evaluated the
proves very emotional and leads teachers to ask: teacher on such dimensions as discipline style,
What memories are my students taking from their response to student questions, teaching style, and
interactions with me? Are they the memories I fairness toward all students. Recommendations
would like them to take? If not, what must I for change were elicited.
change so that the memories they will take will Ownership in students can also be reinforced
be in accord with the memories I hope they take? by engaging students in a discussion about the
These exercises to nurture empathy often prompt benefits or drawbacks of educational practices
teachers to consider how best to obtain feedback that are typically seen as “givens,” including such
from students to gain a realistic picture of how activities as tests, reports, and homework. In
they are perceived. We will address this question addition, educators can strengthen a feeling of
in the next point. student ownership by incorporating a variety of
choices in the classroom, none of which dimin-
Ongoing feedback and input from students ishes a teacher’s authority but rather empowers
enhances empathy and promotes a sense of students to feel a sense of control over their own
engagement, responsibility, and ownership in education.
students. Effective teachers not only welcome the Choice and ownership can also be applied to
input of students, but they appreciate that such disciplinary practices by asking students to con-
input must be incorporated on a regular basis. sider such questions as:
When students feel their voice is being heard, • What rules do you think we need in this class-
they are more likely to be engaged in academic room for all students to feel comfortable and
requirements, work more cooperatively with learn best? (It is not unusual for teachers to
teachers, and demonstrate greater motivation to report that the rules recommended by students
meet academic challenges. Eliciting student opin- often parallel those of the teacher).
ion reinforces a feeling of personal control and • Even as your teacher, I may forget a rule. If I
responsibility—essential ingredients of a posi- do, this is how I would like you to remind me.
tive school climate; encouraging student input is (Teachers can then list one or two ways they
also a basic feature of motivation, engagement, would like to be reminded). Now that I have
and resilience (Adelman & Taylor, 1983; Cohen, mentioned how I would like to be reminded,
2006; Deci et al., 1992; DiCintio & Gee, 1999; how would you like me to remind you? (When
Henderson & Milstein, 1996; Jacobson, 1999; students inform teachers how they would like
Thomsen, 2002). to be reminded should they forget a rule, they
556 R. Brooks et al.

are less likely to experience the reminder as a Each student has different “islands of compe-
form of nagging and more likely to hear what tence” and learning styles that must be identified,
the teacher has to say. It is easier for students respected, and reinforced. This belief is at the
to consider ways of being reminded if teachers core of a strength-based approach to education
first serve as models by offering how they and overlaps with many of the other points
would like to be reminded). reviewed in this chapter. Effective teachers appre-
• What should the consequences be if we forget ciate that one must move beyond a philosophy
a rule? (We have heard teachers report, espe- that fixates on a student’s problems and vulnera-
cially when asking these questions to angry bilities and affords equal, if not greater space, to
students, that the consequences suggested by strengths and competencies.
the students are more severe than any teacher Researchers and clinicians have emphasized
would use). the significance of recruiting selected areas of
These questions pertaining to disciplinary strength or “islands of competence” in building
practices encourage a sense of ownership for self-confidence, motivation, and resilience (Deci
rules and consequences, thereby promoting & Flaste, 1995; Katz, 1994; Rutter, 1985). Rutter,
responsibility and self-discipline in students. in describing resilient individuals, observed,
The second author regularly reinforces a sense “Experiences of success in one arena of life led
of control in her therapy sessions with children to enhanced self-esteem and a feeling of self-
who have problems in school. For instance, Anna, efficacy, enabling them to cope more success-
an 8-year-old, was burdened by social anxiety. fully with the subsequent life challenges and
Although she was willing to talk with Suzanne adaptations” (p. 604). Katz noted, “Being able to
about her interests, she became frozen whenever showcase our talents, and to have them valued
the discussion turned to friends and school. Her by important people in our lives, helps us to
teacher told Suzanne that Anna frequently strug- define our identities around that which we do
gles to enter groups of two or more children, par- best” (p. 10).
ticularly on the school playground. Suzanne
applied a very effective, well-known therapeutic Understanding How You Learn Best
technique involving the use of “displacement.” One of the most obvious guideposts for assisting
She told Anna that she knew a little boy who was students to feel competent is to teach them in
having a problem talking with friends and did not ways in which they can learn best. Educators
know how to help him. Anna immediately replied, must appreciate that each student has different
“Does he have a hard time on the playground?” learning strengths and vulnerabilities (Gardner,
Suzanne responded, “Yes, the playground is 1983; Levine, 2002). This requires that teachers
where he has the most trouble.” familiarize themselves with such topics as multi-
Anna continued, “Is he scared to talk with ple intelligences and learning styles.
other children?” Eventually, the discussion led At the beginning of the school year, teachers
her to assert, “I think he might be worried they can meet with each student for a few minutes and
will make fun of him.” ask, “What are you interested in? What do you
Once this worry was verbalized, Suzanne like to do? What do you think you do well?”
engaged Anna in considering strategies for help- While some students will respond eagerly, others
ing the boy deal with his problems, which, of may simply say, “I don’t know.” In that case,
course, were the same strategies that Anna could teachers can respond, “That’s okay, it often takes
implement to deal with her own problems. In time to figure out what you’re good at. I’ll try to
essence, Anna was placed in a position of control, be of help.”
which encouraged her to discuss her own strug- When the second author evaluates students
gles more directly, leading to a lessening of her referred for learning difficulties, she always asks
anxiety. them how they prefer to learn. Some students are
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 557

not able to answer immediately, and many are and weaknesses and how they learned best. In the
surprised by the question, perhaps expecting that 7 years in which he has sent out the questionnaire,
testing will only highlight their weaknesses. To not one student has failed to return it. This teacher
encourage students to reflect upon their learning found the information he obtained to be an invalu-
style, Suzanne often raises more specific ques- able resource in connecting with students.
tions. For example, she asked Noah, a 15-year-
old high school freshman who was described by Providing Opportunities to Help Others
his parents as “highly intelligent and curious but Another strategy to enhance a sense of compe-
completely unmotivated in school and often dis- tence is to provide students with an opportunity
tracted in class,” if he had ever gone on a trip that to help others. Students experience a more posi-
he really enjoyed and still thinks about. tive attachment to school and are more motivated
Noah’s expression, which had previously been to learn if they are encouraged to contribute to
rather flat and tired looking, lit up as he began to the school milieu (Brooks, 1991; Rutter, 1980;
describe his trip to China with his family last sum- Werner, 1993). Examples include older students
mer. He talked about the landscape, the culture, with learning problems reading to younger chil-
and the people with much excitement. Suzanne dren; a hyperactive child being asked to assume
used his response to discuss the different ways we the position of “attendance monitor,” which
learn and to note that he appeared to be an “expe- involved walking around the halls to take atten-
riential learner.” Noah, with obvious excitement dance of teachers while the latter were taking
in his voice, said, “That’s it. Is that why I’m so attendance of students; and the use of coopera-
bored in class all the time?” Suzanne explained tive learning in which students of varying abili-
that although most of our learning occurs in the ties work together as a team bringing their own
classroom, we could consider ways to supplement unique strengths to different projects.
his learning with hands-on experiences once he
reaches high school to make school feel less bor- Lessening the Fear of Failure
ing. Noah loved this idea, and as it turned out, the One of the most powerful approaches for rein-
high school he will attend has a practicum option forcing a feeling of competence in students is to
for students, which connects what they are learn- lessen their fear of failure. Many students equate
ing in the classroom with real-life experiences. making mistakes with feeling humiliated and,
By asking Noah how he learned best, Suzanne consequently, will avoid learning tasks that
was not only able to understand his struggles more appear very challenging. There are students who
clearly but in addition was able to develop a plan would rather be bullies or quit at tasks or assert
that would in essence adapt more traditional the work is dumb rather than engage in a learning
teaching methods to fit with his learning style. By activity that they feel may result in failure and
encouraging his input, she also reinforced his embarrassment. In a desperate attempt to avoid
sense of ownership in the school environment. failure, they journey down a path that takes them
A high school teacher noted that given all of farther away from possible success.
the students attending his classes, he did not have The fear of making mistakes and failing per-
the time to meet with each individually at the meates every classroom, and if it is not actively
beginning of the year. Instead, he devised a ques- addressed, it remains an active force, compromis-
tionnaire that he sent out to each student a week ing the joy and enthusiasm that should be part of
before school began. He told them that it was not the learning process. Effective educators can
mandatory that they complete the questionnaire, begin to overcome the fear of failure by identify-
but if they did, it would help him to be a more ing and openly addressing it with students. One
effective teacher. The questionnaire focused on a technique for doing so is for teachers to ask their
number of areas, several of which asked students class at the beginning of the school year, “Who
to list what they perceived to be their strengths feels they are going to make a mistake and not
558 R. Brooks et al.

understand something in class this year?” Before Some are hesitant to leave their “comfort zone”
any of the students can respond, teachers can raise even when this zone is filled with stress and pres-
their hand as a way of initiating a discussion of sure. They would rather continue with a known
how the fear of making mistakes affects learning. situation that is less than satisfying than engage
It is often helpful for teachers to share some of in the task of entering a new, unexplored territory
their own anxieties and experiences about mak- that holds promise but also uncertainty.
ing mistakes when they were students. They can If educators are to be effective in applying
recall when they were called upon in class, when many of the ideas described in this chapter for
they made mistakes, or when they failed a test. nurturing motivation, engagement, and resilience
This openness often invites students to share in students, they must venture from their “com-
some of their thoughts and feelings about making fort zone” by utilizing techniques for dealing
mistakes. Teachers can involve the class in prob- with the stress and pressure that are inherent in
lem solving by encouraging them to suggest what their work. Each teacher can discover his or her
they can do as teachers and what the students can own ways for managing stress. For instance,
do as a class to minimize the fear of failure and some can rely on exercise, others on relaxation or
appearing foolish. Issues of being called on and meditation techniques, all of which can be very
not knowing the answer can be discussed. beneficial. In addition to these approaches, there
One middle school English teacher frequently has been research conducted by Kobasa and her
uses a method he refers to as “playing dumb” colleagues (Kobasa et al., 1982; Kobasa &
when he is seeking their responses to a book that Puccetti, 1983) under the label of “stress hardi-
was read. He starts by saying, “I completely for- ness” that examines the characteristics or mind-
got what happens in the end, does anyone remem- set of individuals who experience less stress than
ber?” He has found that this question is typically their colleagues while working in the same envi-
followed by an enthusiastic show of hands with ronment. Kobasa’s work has been applied to the
students explaining the ending to their teacher. teaching profession (Holt, Fine, & Tollefson,
Although his questioning may seem contrived, 1987; Martinez, 1989).
this technique empowers students to take risks This mindset involves three interrelated com-
through the acknowledgement that even teachers ponents: commitment, challenge, and control
can forget information and make mistakes. (“3Cs”). When we describe them at our work-
Effective teachers recognize that when the fear of shops, we encourage educators to reflect upon
failure and humiliation are actively addressed in how they might apply this information to lessen
the classroom, students will be more motivated to stress and burnout.
take realistic risks and to learn. The first C represents “commitment.” Stress-
hardy individuals do not lose sight of why they are
To realize that one must strive to become stress doing what they are doing. They maintain a genu-
hardy rather than stressed out. At the conclusion ine passion or purpose for their work, which as we
of one of our workshops, a teacher said, “I love have seen is a critical dimension of intrinsic moti-
your ideas, but I’m too stressed out to use them.” vation. While we may all have “down” days, it is
While the remark had a humorous tone, it also sad to observe educators who basically say to
captured an important consideration. themselves each morning in a resigned way, “I’ve
At first glance, the remark seems paradoxical got to go to school. I’ve got to see those kids.”
since numerous educators have informed us that Once a feeling of “I’ve got to” or “being forced to”
the strategies we advocate do not take time away pervades one’s mindset, a sense of commitment
from teaching, but rather help to create a class- and purpose is sacrificed, replaced by feelings of
room environment that is more conducive to stress and burnout. As an antidote to burnout, a
learning and less stressful. Yet, we can appreciate staff meeting might be dedicated to sharing why
their frustration that change requires additional one became a teacher, a school administrator, a
time, a commodity that is not readily available. counselor, a nurse, or a psychologist. Such an
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 559

exercise helps staff to recall and invigorate their change things that are beyond their sphere of
dreams and goals. control. Although many individuals believe they
The second C is for “challenge.” Educators engage in activities over which they have influ-
who deal more effectively with stress have devel- ence or control, in fact, many do not. We worked
oped a mindset that views difficult situations as with a group of teachers who were feeling burned
opportunities for learning and growth rather than out. We reviewed the basic tenets of stress hardi-
as stress to avoid. For example, a principal of a ness theory and asked if they focused their ener-
school faced a challenging situation. Her school gies on factors within their domain of control.
was located in a neighborhood that had changed They replied in the affirmative.
in a few short years from a middle class popula- We then asked them to list what would help
tion with much parent involvement to a neigh- their jobs to be less stressful. Their answers
borhood with a lower socioeconomic makeup included, “If the students came from less dys-
and less parent involvement. There were several functional families, if they came to school better
key factors that contributed to the decrease in prepared to learn, if they had more discipline at
parent involvement, including less flexibility for home.” After a few moments, one of the teachers
many parents to leave work in order to attend a smiled and said, “We first said that we focus on
school meeting or conference as well as many what we have control over, but everything that
parents feeling unwelcome and anxious in school we are mentioning to help us feel less stressed are
based upon their own histories as children in the things over which we have little control.” After
school environment. the teacher said this, the group engaged in a lively
Instead of bemoaning this state of affairs and discussion focusing on what educators might do
becoming increasingly upset and stressed, this to create classroom climates that nurtured learn-
particular principal and her staff realized that the ing and engagement even if the students came
education of their students would be greatly from home environments that were less than sup-
enhanced if parents became active participants in portive of education. One teacher astutely noted,
the educational process; consequently, they “We are expecting our students to come to school
viewed the lack of involvement as a challenge to excited about learning and when they do not we
meet rather than as a stress to avoid. Among get frustrated and annoyed. Instead, what I’m
other strategies, they scheduled several staff hearing is that we must ask, ‘What can we do dif-
meetings in the late afternoon and moved the site ferently to help motivate students who are not
of the meetings from the school building to a motivated and what can we do to help students
popular community house a few blocks away. who feel hopeless about learning to feel more
These changes encouraged a number of the par- hopeful? As the discussion continued, the teach-
ents to attend the meetings since the new time ers recognized that by focusing on what they
was more accommodating to their schedules and could do differently to improve the learning envi-
the new location helped them to feel more com- ronment was empowering and lessened stressful
fortable since it was held on their “turf.” The feelings. The mood of pessimism and burnout
relationship between parents and teachers was that had pervaded the room began to change.
greatly enhanced, and the children were the
beneficiaries.
The third C is “control” or what we earlier Concluding Thoughts
referred to as “personal control” since some indi-
viduals may mistakenly view the word control as The concept of mindsets can help us to under-
a form of controlling others. Control, as used in stand the underpinnings of three interrelated con-
stress hardiness theory, implies that individuals cepts: motivation, student engagement, and
who successfully manage stress and pressure resilience. Future research can evaluate the out-
focus their time and energy on factors over which come of implementing within the school culture
they have influence rather than attempting to different components of the mindset that cut
560 R. Brooks et al.

across these three concepts. For instance, educa- Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. L.
tors can examine the impact of introducing a (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological engage-
ment: Validation of the Student Engagement Instrument.
mentoring system and the specific activities of the Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445.
mentors (such as offered by the Check & Connect Betts, J. E., Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., Christenson,
program; Sinclair et al., 2005), increasing student S. L., & Huebner, E. S. (2010). A study of the factorial
input and ownership by having students regularly invariance of the Student Engagement Instrument
(SEI): Results from middle and high school students.
attend parent-teacher conferences, or engaging School Psychology Quarterly, 25, 84–93.
students to contribute to the welfare of others. Brooks, R. (1991). The self-esteem teacher. Loveland,
When these interventions are introduced, OH: Treehaus Communications.
researchers can study changes in a number of vari- Brooks, R. (1994). Children at risk: Fostering resilience
and hope. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
ables including: learning and achievement, student 64, 545–553.
attendance, dropout rates, acts of bullying, occur- Brooks, R. (2001). To touch a student’s heart and mind:
rence of behavioral problems, and teacher reten- The mindset of the effective educator. In Proceedings
tion. Well-researched and field-tested assessment of the 1999 Plain Talk conference sponsored by the
Center for Development and Learning, New Orleans
instruments such as the Comprehensive School (pp. 167–177). Cambridge, MA: Educators Publishing
Climate Inventory (CSCI) developed by the Service.
National School Climate Center (formerly the Brooks, R. (2004). To touch the hearts and minds of stu-
Center for Social and Emotional Education dents with learning disabilities: The power of mindsets
and expectations. Learning Disabilities: A Contempo-
[CSEE]) can be used to obtain input from students, rary Journal, 2, 9–18.
parents, and school personnel to measure changes Brooks, R., & Goldstein, S. (2001). Raising resilient chil-
in school climate when mindsets for motivation, dren. New York: McGraw-Hill.
student engagement, and resilience are reinforced Brooks, R., & Goldstein, S. (2004). The power of resil-
ience. New York: McGraw-Hill.
in a systematic way (Cohen, 2006). Brooks, R., & Goldstein, S. (2007). Raising a self-disci-
The more aware educators are of the mindset plined child. New York: McGraw-Hill.
of motivated, engaged, resilient students and the Brooks, R., & Goldstein, S. (2008). The mindset of teach-
more aware they are of their own mindset, the ers capable of fostering resilience in students. Canadian
Journal of School Psychology, 23, 114–126.
more capable they will be in implementing strat- Canino, F. (1981). Learned helplessness theory:
egies to develop this mindset in all students. The Implication for research in learning disabilities.
result will be classroom environments filled with Journal of Special Education, 15, 471–484.
excitement, safety, eagerness to learn, engage- Christenson, S. L., & Anderson, A. R. (2002).
Commentary: The centrality of the learning context
ment, self-discipline, respect, and resilience. for students’ academic enabler skills. School
Both faculty and students will thrive in such an Psychology Review, 31, 378–393.
environment. Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., Appleton, J. J., Berman,
S., Spanjers, D., & Varro, P. (2008). Best practices
in fostering student engagement. In A. Thomas &
J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology
References (5th ed., pp. 1099–1119). Bethesda, MD: National
Association of School Psychologists.
Adelman, H. S., & Taylor, L. (1983). Enhancing motiva- Christenson, S. L., & Thurlow, M. L. (2004). School drop-
tion for overcoming learning and behavior problems. outs: Prevention considerations, interventions, and
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 16, 384–392. challenges. Current Directions in Psychological
Anderson, A. R., Christenson, S. L., Sinclair, M. F., & Science, 13(1), 36–39.
Lehr, C. A. (2004). Check & Connect: The importance Cohen, J. (2006). Social, emotional, ethical, and academic
of relationships for promoting engagement with education: Creating a climate for learning, participa-
school. Journal of School Psychology, 42, 95–113. tion in democracy, and well-being. Harvard
Andrews, G. R., & Debus, R. L. (1978). Persistence and Educational Review, 76, 201–237.
the causal perceptions of failure: Modifying cognitive Cohen, J., & Sandy, S. (2003). Perspectives in social-emo-
attributions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, tional education: Theoretical foundations and new
154–166. evidence-based developments in current practice.
Appleton, A. R., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. Perspectives in Education, 21, 41–54.
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxi-
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. ety: Experiencing flow in work and play. San Francisco:
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. Jossey-Bass.
26 The Power of Mindsets: Nurturing Engagement, Motivation, and Resilience in Students 561

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1998). Finding flow: The psychology Jones, R. D. (2009). Student engagement: Teacher hand-
of engagement with everyday life. New York: Basic book. Rexford, NY: International Center for Leadership
Books. in Education.
Davis, S. (2003). Schools where everyone belongs. Wayne, Jones, R. D., Marrazo, M. J., & Love, C. J. (2007). Student
ME: Stop Bullying Now. engagement: Creating a culture of academic achieve-
Deci, E. L., & Flaste, R. (1995). Why we do what we do: ment. Rexford, NY: International Center for Leadership
Understanding self-motivation. New York: Penguin. in Education.
Deci, E. L., Hodges, R., Pierson, L., & Tomassone, J. (1992). Katz, M. (1994, May). From challenged childhood to
Autonomy and competence as motivational factors in achieving adulthood: Studies in resilience. Chadder,
students with learning disabilities and emotional handi- 8–11.
caps. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 457–471. Katz, M. (1997). On playing a poor hand well. New York:
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Extrinsic Norton.
rewards and extrinsic motivation in education: Klem, A., & Connell, R. (2004). Relationships matter:
Reconsidered once again. Review of Educational Linking teacher support to student engagement and
Research, 71, 1–27. achievement. Journal of School Health, 74, 262–273.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” Kobasa, S., Maddi, S., & Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness and
of goal pursuits. Human needs and the self-determina- health: A perspective inquiry. Journal of Personality
tion of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. and Social Psychology, 42, 168–177.
DiCintio, M. J., & Gee, S. (1999). Control is the key: Kobasa, S., & Puccetti, M. (1983). Personality and social
Unlocking the motivation of at-risk students. resources in stress resistance. Journal of Personality
Psychology in the Schools, 36, 231–237. and Social Psychology, 42, 839–850.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973).
learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrin-
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of sic rewards: A test of the “overjustification” hypothe-
success. New York: Random House. sis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28,
Elias, M. J., Zins, J. E., Graczyk, P. A., & Weissberg, R. P. 129–137.
(2003). Implementation, sustainability, and scaling up Levine, M. D. (2002). A mind at a time. New York: Simon
of social-emotional and academic innovations in pub- & Schuster.
lic schools. School Psychology Review, 32, 303–319. Levine, M. D. (2003). The myth of laziness. New York:
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Simon & Schuster.
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. Maehr, M. L., & Meyer, H. A. (1997). Understanding
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). motivation and schooling: Where we’ve been, where
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of we are, and where we need to go. Educational
the evidence. Review of Educational Psychology, 74, Psychology Review, 9, 371–408.
59–109. Martinez, J. (1989). Cooling off before burning out.
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. New York: Basic Academic Therapy, 24, 271–284.
Books. Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience pro-
Goldstein, S., & Brooks, R. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of cesses in development. American Psychologist, 56,
resilience in children. New York: Springer. 227–238.
Goldstein, S., & Brooks, R. (2007). Understanding and Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The develop-
managing classroom behavior: Creating resilient, sus- ment of competence in favorable and unfavorable
tainable classrooms. New York: Wiley. environments: Lessons from successful children.
Goleman, D. (1994). Emotional intelligence. New York: American Psychologist, 53, 205–220.
Bantam. McCombs, B. L., & Pope, J. E. (1994). Motivating hard to
Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner reach students. Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
resources for school achievement: Motivational medi- ical Association.
ators of children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal Middleton, K., & Pettit, E. A. (2010). Simply the best: 29
of Educational Psychology, 83, 508–517. things students say the best teachers do around rela-
Henderson, N., & Milstein, M. (1996). Resiliency in tionships. Bloomington, IN: Author House.
schools: Making it happen for students and educators. Miserandino, M. (1996). Children who do well in school:
Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin. Individual differences in perceived competence and
Holt, P., Fine, M., & Tollefson, N. (1987). Mediating autonomy in above-average children. Journal of
stress: Survival of the hardy. Psychology in the Educational Psychology, 88, 203–214.
Schools, 24, 51–58. Olweus, D. (1994). Bullying at school: What we know
Jacobson, L. (1999). Three’s company: Kids prove they and what we can do. Malden, MA: Blackwell
have a place at the parent-teacher conference. Teacher Publishers.
Magazine, 11, 23. Pink, D. H. (2009). Drive: The surprising truth about
Jimerson, S. R., Campos, E., & Greif, J. L. (2003). Toward what motivates us. New York: Riverhead Books.
an understanding of definitions and measures of school Reivich, K., & Shatte, A. W. (2002). The resilience factor:
engagement and related items. California School 7 keys to finding your inner strength and overcoming
Psychologist, 8, 7–27. life’s hurdles. New York: Random House.
562 R. Brooks et al.

Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2006). Prediction of Shure, M. B. (1996). Raising a thinking child: Help your
dropout among students with mild disabilities: A Case young child resolve everyday conflicts and get along
for the inclusion of student engagement variables. with others. New York: Pocket Books.
Remedial and Special Education, 27, 276–292. Shure, M. B. (2003). A problem-solving approach to pre-
Russell, V. J., Ainley, M., & Frydenberg, E. (2005). School venting early high-risk behaviors in children and pre-
issues digest: Student motivation and engagement. teens. In D. Romer (Ed.), Preventing adolescent risk
From: http://dest.gov.au/setors/school_education/pub- (pp. 85–92). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
lication_resources/schooling_issues_digest_motiva- Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., & Thurlow, M. L.
tion_engagement.htm (2005). Promoting school completion of urban sec-
Rutter, M. (1980). School influences on children’s behav- ondary youth with emotional or behavioral disabili-
ior and development. Pediatrics, 65, 522–533. ties. Exceptional Children, 71(4), 465–482.
Rutter, M. (1985). Resilience in the face of adversity: Thomsen, K. (2002). Building resilient students:
Protective factors and resistance to psychiatric disor- Integrating resiliency into what you already know and
der. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 598–611. do. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin.
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M.,
factors. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learning, perfor-
316–331. mance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive con-
theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, texts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
social development, and well-being. American 87, 246–260.
Psychologist, 55, 68–78. Wagner, T., Kegan, R., Lahey, L. L., & Lemons, R. L.
Schunk, D. H., & Rice, J. M. (1993). Strategy fading and (2005). Change leadership: A practical guide to trans-
progress feedback: Effects on self-efficacy and com- forming our schools. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
prehension among students receiving remedial read- Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribu-
ing services. The Journal of Special Education, 27, tion theory. Morristown, NJ: General Learning
257–276. Press.
Segal, J. (1988). Teachers have enormous power in affect- Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, and recovery:
ing a child’s self-esteem. Brown University Child Perspectives from the Kauai Longitudinal Study.
Behavior and Development Newsletter, 10, 1–3. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 503–515.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1990). Learned optimism: How to Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. (1992). Overcoming the odds:
change your minds and life. New York: Pocket High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca,
Books. NY: Cornell University Press.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). The optimistic child. New Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. (2001). Journeys of childhood
York: Houghton Mifflin. to midlife: Risk, resilience, and recovery. Ithaca, NY:
Sheridan, S. M., Eagle, J. W., & Dowd, S. E. (2005). Cornell University Press.
Families for contexts for children’s adaptation. In S. Wright, M. O., & Masten, A. S. (2005). Resilience pro-
Goldstein & R. Brooks (Eds.), Handbook of resilience cesses in development. In S. Goldstein & R. Brooks
in children (pp. 165–180). New York: Kluwer (Eds.), Handbook of resilience in children (pp. 17–37).
Academic/Plenum Publishers. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
The Relations of Adolescent Student
Engagement with Troubling 27
and High-Risk Behaviors

Amy-Jane Griffiths, Elena Lilles, Michael J. Furlong,


and Jennifer Sidhwa

Abstract
Nearly one third of secondary school students report decreased engage-
ment in school during their teen years. When considering the emotional or
psychological aspects of engagement, which are routinely associated with
high-risk behaviors, a student must somehow conclude that, at a mini-
mum, at least one specific person at their school truly cares about him or
her not only as a student, but as a person. This caring individual, be it a
teacher, coach, administrator, or counselor, does not simply express
respect, concern, and trust in the student as part of their job, but also the
student comes to believe that this person sees intrinsic value in him or her
as a human being. In this chapter we underscore the association between
student engagement and high-risk behaviors in adolescence. Although all
aspects of student engagement are important to the full development of
youth, the salience of student engagement when considering troubling and
high-risk behaviors in schools warrants educators’ attention. We summa-
rize research in this area and provide an overview of system-level

A.-J. Griffiths, Ph.D. (*)


The Help Group, Sherman Oaks, CA, USA
e-mail: agriffiths@thehelpgroup.org
E. Lilles, M.Ed.
University of Oregon, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
e-mail: elilles@education.ucsb.edu
M.J. Furlong, Ph.D.
Department of Counseling, Clinical,
and School Psychology, University of California
Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
e-mail: mfurlong@education.ucsb.edu
J. Sidhwa, B.A.
Educational Leadership and Organizations Program,
Gevirtz Graduate School of Education, University
of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
e-mail: jsidhwa@education.ucsb.edu

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 563


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_27, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
564 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

interventions and strategies to build bonding and connectedness, particu-


larly for those students who engage in high-risk behaviors. We conclude
that clear definitions and unified research in the area of student engage-
ment can allow for continued advancements in understanding how to best
engage students, specifically high-risk students, and yield positive aca-
demic and life outcomes for youth.

explaining student engagement. We then summa-


Introduction rize research about the identified relations
between student engagement and troubling and
We approach the topic of adolescent student
high-risk behaviors. Finally, we provide an over-
engagement, particularly considering high-risk
view of system level interventions and strategies
behaviors, from the perspective that engagement
to build bonding and connectedness, particularly
research is incomplete if it only considers stu-
for those students who engage in high-risk behav-
dents’ individual academic behaviors or personal
iors. We hope reader will recognize the impor-
scholastic incentives. In our view, the student and
tance of this research, noting that the topic of this
his or her personal beliefs and perceptions about
chapter applies to many students because nearly
school and the schooling process are central to
one third of secondary school students report
engagement considerations. When considering
decreased engagement during their teen years
the emotional or psychological aspects of engage-
(Archambault, Janosz, Morizot, & Pagani, 2009).
ment, which are routinely associated with high-
In addition, even for the majority of students who
risk behaviors, a student must somehow conclude
are generally involved in their schooling experi-
that, at a minimum, at least one specific person at
ence, affective engagement is lower than behav-
their school truly cares about him or her not only
ioral and cognitive engagement (Archambault
as a student but as a person (Murray & Malmgren,
et al., 2009).
2005). This caring individual, be it a teacher,
coach, administrator, or counselor, does not sim-
ply express respect, concern, and trust in the stu-
Definitions of Engagement
dent as part of their job (Johnson, 2009), but also
the student comes to believe that this person sees
Various aspects of student engagement have been
intrinsic value in him or her as a human being.
studied under a variety of terms including school
As was stated by one of the teachers in Gregory
connectedness, teacher support, school bonding,
and Ripski’s (2008) study of student trust, “The
school climate, school engagement, and more
one thing that seems to mean the most to her (the
recently student engagement (Blum & Libbey,
student) is my affection and my caring about her
2004; O’Farrell & Morrison, 2003). Researchers
as a person” (p. 343).
have suggested that the term represents a multi-
Literature reviewed in this chapter under-
faceted construct that involves student thoughts,
scores the association between student engage-
beliefs, emotions, and behaviors as it relates to
ment and high-risk behaviors in adolescence. To
school. Researchers have recently organized the
examine this topic, we first define the engage-
conceptualization of engagement into three sub-
ment terms presented in the chapter and provide
types: behavioral, cognitive, and emotional or
brief comments to address the three common
affective (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris,
topics in each chapter of this volume by (a) pro-
2004; Jimerson et al., 2003). However, Appleton,
viding our definition of engagement and motiva-
Christenson, and Furlong (2008) made a convinc-
tion, (b) describing the framework and theory we
ing argument for four components of student
use to study and explain engagement and motiva-
engagement: academic, behavioral, cognitive, and
tion, and (c) defining the role of context in
psychological. These four components are based
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 565

on a comprehensive review of literature related to Martinez, Pinto, Salanova, & Bakker, 2002). This
student engagement and particularly the work of describes what has been labeled the “behavioral”
Finn (1989), Connell (Connell, Halpern-Felsher, and “academic” aspects of engagement (Appleton
Clifford, Crichlow, & Usinger, 1995; Connell & et al., 2008; Fredricks et al., 2004). Among the
Wellborn, 1991), and McPartland (1994). markers that have been used to assess behavioral
Academic engagement includes variables such engagement—the student is at school nearly every
as points earned, homework completion, and day, completes tasks in a timely manner, is atten-
time on task. Behavioral engagement may include tive to and responsive to teacher questions, and
variables such as attendance, the absence of dis- asks teachers how they did on assignments—all
ruptive behaviors, adhering to school rules, extra- suggest personal commitment to the schooling
curricular activities participation, and student process. From our view, confusion arises between
participation in learning and academic assign- the terms motivation and engagement because (a)
ments (Fredricks et al., 2004). Emotional engage- motivation is an internal experience that can only
ment is the student’s emotional reactions at school be inferred indirectly by others and (b) the evolu-
that includes interest, boredom, happiness, sadness, tion of the term engagement has expanded to
and anxiety (Fredricks et al., 2004). Otherwise include both behavioral components and internal
labeled as psychological engagement, this may psychological ones (affective and cognitive). The
include relationships with teachers and peers, as term engagement implies some level of involve-
well as feelings of belonging. Cognitive engage- ment and activity: Are you doing the work of a
ment may include indicators such as personal student? Do you value the work being done? Are
goal development, self-regulation relevance of you in it for the long haul? When we speak of
schoolwork to future goals, and the value of motivation, we see it as being the combination of
learning. Fredricks et al., 2004 suggested cogni- these three key questions. As has been concep-
tive engagement can be described as the students’ tualized by others (Appleton et al., 2008), the
investment in learning, self-regulation, and the behavioral and cognitive components also clearly
use of strategies to gain knowledge and skills. address these key questions. What then distin-
guished the historical conceptualization of engage-
ment from motivation is the affective component
Engagement and Motivation that can be understood as the link between the stu-
dent as an individually motivated learner and the
How does one know if a student is motivated? student as a member of a social network that encom-
When others “see” motivation, they describe what a passes both one-on-one relationships (e.g., student-
student does, the products he or she produces (qual- teacher and friend-friend) and being the part of
ity and quantity), and perhaps mention comments larger social networks (e.g., classroom, social
that the student makes, from which attitudes, groups, school-wide climate). In this chapter, we
goals, and dreams are inferred. That is, the stu- will focus on the affective elements of engagement,
dent’s behaviors signal that he or she values utilizing various terms seen in the literature, such as
schoolwork and is striving to do it well, perhaps school engagement, student engagement, school
to fulfill higher life aspirations. These are infer- connectedness, and school bonding. The term
ences made by others rather than motivation. By school bonding is the oldest term and connotes the
its nature, motivation is an internal, personal personal and relational links associated with reduced
experience. In our view, motivation is the psycho- participation in risky behaviors.
logical driving force that increases the probability
that a student engages in behaviors that lead
toward desired scholastic goals. In this regard, The Engagement Process
engagement is the more visible manifestation of
such motivational tendencies. Students who are Social development researchers (e.g., Hawkins,
motivated to learn and do well in school can be Guo, Hill, Battin-Pearson, & Abbott, 2001) have
observed doing the “work” of a student (Schaufeli, suggested that student engagement develops in
566 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

the individual as they are provided the opportu- The Engaged and Disengaged Student
nity for behavioral involvement, social skills
training, and rewards for using these social skills In this section, we offer the distinction between
in interpersonal situations. Extending this model active and positive engagement in school and
to include the various terms that have been used active and negative disengagement; that is, disen-
in the student engagement literature, Furlong gagement is not merely the absence of engage-
et al. (2003) offered the PACM model. Partici- ment. Guthrie (2001), for example, described an
pation (behavioral involvement) contributes to the engaged reader in this way,
formation of interpersonal Attachments (social Devoted students are intent on reading to under-
bonding), which in turn results in a student dev- stand. They focus on meaning and avoid distrac-
eloping a sense of personal Commitment (valuing tions. Strategies such as self-monitoring and
of education), and ultimately to incorporating inferencing are used with little effort. These readers
exchange ideas and interpretations with fellow
school Membership (identification as a school students. We refer to these students who are intrin-
community citizen) as part of his or her self- sically motivated to read for knowledge and enjoy-
identity (P → A → C → M). Such a model is ment as ‘engaged’ readers (p. 2).
relevant to all students, particularly those consid- In this regard, cognitive engagement focuses
ered to be “high risk.” This model, if used as the on how deeply the student participates in the
basis for educational practice, has the potential to tasks of being a student and on using academic
structure overall school improvement efforts. tasks for broader personal skill development and
O’Farrell, Morrison, and Furlong (2006) enhancing self-efficacy. However, researchers
reviewed five levels of engagement supported (Abbott et al., 1998; Hirschi, 1969) have long
within the school environment. First, schools can recognized that some students do not participate
conduct school-wide activities (e.g., clubs, sports) in such personally facilitative ways in the aca-
that reaffirm relationships with the majority of demic context. Drawing from resilience research
students who are not at risk. Second, schools can (Catalano, Hawkins, & Smith, 2001; Herrenkohl,
reach out to and reconnect with students who are Hawkins, Chung, Hill, & Battin-Pearson, 2000),
marginally involved with school and may not models show that youth with the accumulation of
respond to universal strategies. Third, schools multiple challenges (e.g., poverty, inconsistent
may need to reconstruct relationships with stu- parenting experiences) are at an increased risk of
dents who show serious emotional and behavioral negative developmental outcomes. These life
difficulties through intensive interventions such experiences may make it more difficult for a
as family therapy or behavioral assessments and youth to be able to focus on and be behavior-
interventions. Fourth, for a small group of students, ally engaged at school. These youth are seen as
schools will need to repair the relationships of being, in fact, more likely to be “disengaged,”
students who may have been marginalized, and/ “disconnected,” or at best inconsistently commit-
or victims of serious or chronic violence at school ted to the educational values and mission of the
and require interventions to renew a sense of school.
school safety and membership. For marginalized Such students are likely to be less motivated
students, opportunities to repair bonds across by task mastery or performance goals (Eccles &
various social contexts may be of particular Wigfield, 2002; Finn, 1989, 1993). They are more
importance. If a student is significantly disen- likely to be suspended from school for behaviors
gaged from school and possibly other environ- such as defiance, disobedience, or disrespect
ments (home and community), it may be necessary directed toward the teacher (Morrison & Skiba,
to use multiple agencies to intervene and create 2001), which can strain the formation of a caring
opportunities for attachment and the develop- supportive relationship and undermines the legit-
ment of self-efficacy. We will focus on recon- imacy of a teacher’s authority for the student
struction and repair of relationships for youth (Gregory & Ripski, 2008). In fact, disengaged
engaging in high-risk behavior. students may not just ignore or disregard teachers
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 567

and other school authority figures, but if they aggressive behavior (Roehlkepartain, Benson, &
conclude that school is not a place that is accept- Sesma, 2003).
ing and inclusive, they can actively resist teacher In addition, California Healthy Kids Survey
directives (Solorzarno & Delgado Bernal, 2001). (CHKS) data provide information about the rela-
It is not just that disengaged students may believe tions between student engagement and risky
that their teachers and others at school do not behaviors. The CHKS includes sections about
have positive regard for them, but they conclude violence, perceptions of safety, harassment, bul-
that the school context actively rejects them and lying, and the use of alcohol and other drugs. The
does not promote or have a supportive caring cli- CHKS also has a Resilience Youth Development
mate (Noddings, 1995). Module to measure external resources (protective
A number of organizations and measures factors). RYDM external assets items measure
focus on student and community assets. The students’ perceptions of caring relationships, high
Search Institute is one of these organizations, expectations, and opportunities for meaningful
which uses their framework of 40 developmental participation in school. Hanson and Kim (2007)
assets and the relationship of these assets to conducted several factor analyses and found that
negative outcomes, to inform asset building in the six items from the Caring Relationship and
communities. The Search Institute has partnered High Expectation subscales combined to form
with cities and schools to utilize these data to one factor that they called “school support” with
assist in the development of programs that target the three meaningful participation items holding
student engagement. The Search Institute has led together in a separate factor.
a multiyear study of developmental assessment We examined the CHKS sample of 92,600 stu-
among school-aged youth and linked asset pro- dents collected during the 2006–2007 and 2007–
files to the students’ individual school records. 2008 school years who were in grades 9 (52%) and
The results of this research show that low assets 11 (48%). These students were from 50 of the 58
are associated with increased participation in California counties. Students were placed into one
high-risk behaviors such as substance use and of three groups, as shown in Table 27.1. The first

Table 27.1 Percentage of California students in grades 9 and 11 reporting troubling and high-risk behaviors by per-
ceptions of school support (caring adult relations and high expectations) and opportunities for meaningful participation
in school activities (N = 92,600)
Higha level of Lowb level of
meaningful participation All other meaningful participation
Troubling and high-risk behaviors and school supports (%) students (%) and school supports (%)
Any past 30-day cigarette use 6 9 16
Any past 30-day marijuana use 8 12 20
In past 30-days had at least 1 alcoholic drink 12 19 37
Any past 30-day binge drinking 12 16 23
Any past 12-month fighting at school 13 16 25
Any past 30-day carried gun at school 2 3 8
Any past 12-month skipped school or cut class 31 38 49
Self-report gang member 6 7 12
Note: School supports is the total of the following six items: At my school, there is a teacher or some other adult…
(1 = Not at All True, 2 = A Little True, 3 = Pretty Much True, 4 = Very Much True) who really cares about me, who tells
me when I do a good job, who notices when I am not there, who always wants me to do my best, who listens to me when
I have something to say, and who believes that I will be a success. Meaningful participation is the total of the follow-
ing three items: At school… (1 = Not at All True, 2 = A Little True, 3 = Pretty Much True, 4 = Very Much True), I do
interesting activities; I help decide things like class activities or rules; I do things that make a difference (see Furlong
et al., 2009; Hanson & Kim, 2008 for more information on the CHKS survey and these scales). Missing responses for
each item ranged from 0.5% to 1.0%
a
z-Scores > 1.0
b
z-Scores < 1.0
568 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

group included youth whose z-scores on the are most salient when considering students who
School Supports and Meaningful Participation might otherwise be unmotivated or disengaged
scales were both more than one standard deviation from school? Most of the research on adolescents
above the means for the entire sample. These stu- and their involvement in troubling and high-risk
dents perceived their relationships with teachers to behaviors has identified the affective component
be very positive and caring, and they believed they of engagement as being particularly important.
had ample opportunities to participate in meaning- This research has multidisciplinary origins (pub-
ful activities at school. In brief, these students lic health, education, development, psychopa-
reported being highly connected and engaged with thology), but has coalesced to encompass the
school (note that this was only about 6% of 9th core notion that adolescents’ perceptions of the
graders and 8% of 11th graders). At the other end commitment and caring of adults at school are
of the connectedness continuum, a second group associated with reduced involvement in troubling
included youth whose z-scores on the School and high-risk behaviors.
Supports and Meaningful Participation scales Research has established positive relations
were both more than one standard deviation below between student engagement and student devel-
the means for the entire sample. These students opmental outcomes including academic achieve-
were generally disengaged (note that this was ment (Fredricks et al., 2004; Lee & Smith, 1995),
about 21% of 9th graders and 18% of 11th graders substance use, physical and mental health prob-
in this sample). The remaining students were lems (Carter, McGee, Taylor, & Williams, 2007),
somewhere in between these two extreme exem- school dropout (National Research Council and
plar groups. As shown in Table 27.1, about one in Institute of Medicine, 2004; Perry, 2008), as well
five students who reported low levels of connect- as conduct problems and violence (Henrich,
edness and engagement also consistently reported Brookmeyer, & Shahar, 2005; Loukas, Suzuki, &
higher rates of involvement in substance use and Horton, 2006). This section reviews both short-
aggression-related behaviors. The students who term and long-term benefits of student engage-
reported being disengaged from school typically ment as well as the negative correlates of student
reported engaging in risky behaviors about twice disengagement.
as often as highly engaged students. It is inaccu-
rate to conclude that these behaviors are typical Academic Achievement
for most students, but they do illustrate that when According to the National Research Council
students are able to form positive relationships and Institute of Medicine (2004), one of the
with adults at school, they are less likely to report most consistently documented correlates of stu-
engaging in troubling and risky behaviors. dent engagement is student academic achieve-
ment. Research has found a strong positive
relation between the level of student engage-
The Impact of Student Engagement ment and student academic achievement as
measured by scores on standardized assess-
Student Engagement and Troubling ments (e.g., Finn & Voelkl, 1993; Lee & Smith,
and High-Risk Behaviors 1995; Roeser, Midgley, & Urdan, 1996). This
relation has been found to be consistent across
When youth consider engaging in risky and trou- demographic variables including gender, race/
bling behaviors (i.e., if they are not acting on ethnicity, and socioeconomic status, and was
impulse), various factors can influence their found to be a positive correlate for academic
choices—the behavior’s danger, excitement, achievement among high-risk students (Finn,
legality, morality, and, of relevance to this chap- 1993; Finn & Rock, 1997).
ter’s topic, the opinions of peers and adults Student engagement has been identified to
(Abbott-Chapman, Denholm, & Wyld, 2008). Of function as a key mediator of academic achieve-
these considerations, which aspects of engagement ment through academic performance, grade
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 569

promotion, and grade retention (Perry, 2008; Early problems with school engagement, or
Perry, Liu, & Pabian, 2010). Among a sample of school disengagement, have long-term effects
1,803 high-risk students, Finn and Rock (1997) and put students at risk for academic achieve-
identified engagement to be a significant compo- ment difficulties. Research findings suggest that
nent of “academic resilience” even after control- student engagement continues to parallel achieve-
ling for background and psychological ment patterns through high school (Roscigno &
characteristics. Results indicated a significant Ainsworth-Darnell, 1999).
effect size of .78 (p = .001) when examining the Finn (1989) described the long-term effects
teacher report of student engagement of students of student engagement on academic achieve-
who dropped out of school compared to students ment through a participation-identification model.
who completed school. Students who are better This model suggests that early disengagement
engaged with various aspects of their schooling from school (e.g., lack of behavioral participation)
show higher academic achievement compared to leads to unsuccessful academic outcomes. These
disengaged students (Fredricks et al., 2004). poor school outcomes lead to student withdrawal
Anderman and Anderman (1999) found that stu- and lack of identification with the school. This
dent sense of school belonging was associated lack of identification results in nonparticipation
with motivation and focus toward academic in school-related activities, which, in turn, results
tasks, which subsequently yielded academic in negative academic outcomes. The participa-
achievement. Similarly, disengaged students tion-identification model is a cyclical process,
attend school irregularly, do not complete meaning that school participation and school
coursework, and subsequently learn less than identification reciprocally influence each other
their academically engaged peers. This over time. Overall, literature suggests that
disengaged pattern of behavior results in lower engagement with the school community and
levels of overall academic achievement (National academic schoolwork is a proximate determinant
Research Council and Institute of Medicine, of both current and future student academic
2004) and likely leads to reduced opportunities achievement.
for positively engaging adults at school and sub-
sequently developing positive relationships. Substance Use and Physical and Mental
The relation between a student’s level of Health Correlates
school engagement and academic achievement is Student engagement has also been identified as
evident even in primary grades (Alexander, having an impact on substance use and physical
Entwisle, & Dauber, 1993); however, the conse- and mental health outcomes among adolescents.
quences of disengagement may not be observed Student disengagement during the teenage years
until later years (i.e., middle school and high may lead to the failure to acquire basic proficien-
school; Roscigno & Ainsworth-Darnell, 1999). cies needed to survive in society. A lack of such
Early achievement researchers have found that skill sets put individuals at risk for poor overall
engagement in early primary grades predicts health outcomes (National Research Council and
long-term scholastic growth (Ladd & Dinella, Institute of Medicine, 2004). Unhealthy behav-
2009). Alexander and colleagues (1993) and iors that begin during adolescence are more often
Alexander, Entwisle, and Dauber (1996) found found among students with low levels of engage-
that teachers’ ratings of student school engage- ment compared to students with high levels of
ment based on student interest and participation engagement. These behaviors can have lifelong
in the classroom in the first grade of school were negative consequences. A key study by Resnick
related to later achievement, as observed through and colleagues (1997) generated unique, substan-
academic test scores (direct effect coefficients of tial interest in the potential protective role of
.33 in reading and .28 in math, p = .01), and grades what they called “school connectedness” with
over the first 4 years of school (direct effect coef- adults at school. Resnick et al. (1997) conducted
ficients of .37 in year 1 to .20 in year 4, p = .01). a study to identify various risk and protective fac-
570 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

tors both inside and outside of the school setting. School Dropout
The researchers performed cross-sectional analy- School dropout is one of the most visible out-
sis on interview data of 12,118 (of 90,118) high comes of pervasive student disengagement
school and middle school students who partici- (Alliance for Excellent Education, 2009; National
pated in the National Longitudinal Study of Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2004).
Adolescent Health (ADD Health). When analyz- In a review of research on outcomes associated
ing the data separately for the adolescents in with student engagement, Fredricks and colleagues
grades 7–8 and 9–12, respectively, they found (2004) found that student disengagement from
that high levels of school connectedness and use school including low academic participation, poor
of cigarettes were negatively correlated at a mag- attendance, minimal work involvement, and dis-
nitude of −.19 (p < .001) for students in grades plays of negative conduct is a precursor of school
7–8 and −.25 (p < .001) for students in grades dropout (Barrington & Hendricks, 1989; Fredricks
9–12. The use of alcohol was negatively corre- et al., 2004). It is clear that students who are not
lated at a magnitude of −.23 (p < .001) for stu- engaged in school are at a greater risk for low aca-
dents in grades 7–8 and −.21 (p < .001) for demic achievement and school failure and subse-
students in grades 9–12. In other words, students quently exhibit higher dropout rates compared to
with high levels of school engagement were sig- high achieving students (Bridgeland, DiJulio, &
nificantly less likely to use cigarettes and alcohol. Morison, 2006; Janosz, Archambault, Morizot, &
Similarly, high levels of school connectedness Pagani, 2008; National Research Council and
for the two groups were associated with less fre- Institute of Medicine, 2004; Perry, 2008). Janosz
quent marijuana use. School engagement and and colleagues (2008) found that out of a sample
marijuana use were correlated at a magnitude of of 13,300 students between the ages of 12 and 16,
−.22 (p < .001) for students in grades 7–8 and those students who were identified as demonstrat-
−.24 (p < .001) for students in grades 9–12. As ing negative or inconsistent school engagement
might be expected, they found that parent/family patterns were between 10 and 80 times more at
connectedness was associated with the frequency risk for dropping out of school than peers who
of substance use, but that positive school social exhibited typical school engagement patterns.
connections explained unique variance in risk- Consequences of being disengaged from
related behavior patterns. school are serious for high-risk youth who may
Additionally, student engagement is associ- not have other resources available to help coun-
ated with student mental health and well-being. terbalance the effects of school failure. Disengaged
Students with high levels of engagement were students from challenging backgrounds (e.g.,
found to have reduced risk of depression and poverty) in urban school settings are more likely
suicidal ideation compared to students with low to drop out than disengaged students who are not
engagement (Carter et al., 2007). Highly engaged from disadvantaged backgrounds (Perry, 2008).
students were found to report better overall men- However, just as student disengagement can lead
tal health and well-being outcomes compared to to student dropout, student engagement can act as
disengaged youth (Holdsworth & Blanchard, a protective factor against academic failure
2006). A high level of student engagement was (Fredricks et al., 2004). Students with high levels
found to be related to healthy behaviors, for of engagement are more likely to exhibit high
example, high engagement related to higher academic achievement and are less likely to drop
levels of physical activity, better eating habits out of school (Crosnoe, Mistry, & Elder, 2002). A
and nutrition, safer sex, and cycle helmet use student’s perception of his or her connection to
(Carter et al., 2007). Strong student engagement the school, teachers, and peers can act as a pro-
was also found to be associated with a decreased tective factor that keeps high-risk children in
likelihood of pregnancy among teenage girls school (Fine, 1991; Finn & Rock, 1997; Fredricks
(Manlove, 1998). et al., 2004; Mehan et al., 1996).
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 571

Conduct Problems/Violence findings of key studies published within the past


Many forms of community and school violence decade that evaluate student engagement and the
have been perpetrated by students who had a his- associated outcomes. This table serves as a refer-
tory of social alienation and detachment at school ence guide to review both positive and negative
(Sandhu, Arora, & Sandhu, 2001). Students with outcomes associated with student engagement and
high levels of engagement were identified as disengagement from school.
exhibiting lower levels of problem behaviors (Finn
& Rock, 1997; Gutman & Midgley, 2000).
Students with low levels of engagement were more Prevention and Intervention:
likely than engaged peers to display negative What Can Be Done?
behaviors or conduct problems such as fighting
which leads to additional negative consequences Although all aspects of student engagement are
including school suspension and further disen- important to the full development of youth, the
gagement from school (Carter et al., 2007; salience of student connectedness when consid-
Fredricks et al., 2004). Along this same vein, high ering troubling and high-risk behaviors in
levels of student engagement are correlated to a schools is recognized in a vast body of research.
lower likelihood of being involved in violent Research evidence supporting the recommenda-
behaviors for ethnically and socioeconomically tion to promote positive and caring relationships
diverse male and female adolescents in grades among students parents and staff is substantial
7–12 (Henrich et al., 2005; Loukas et al., 2006). and is strongly backed by randomized control
Furthermore, student engagement has been identi- trials (Langberg et al., 2006, 2008; Molina
fied as a protective factor against weapon carrying et al., 2008; Murray & Malgren, 2005; Sinclair,
for ethnically diverse males and African-American Christenson, Evelo, & Hurley, 1998; Sinclair,
females (Kodjo, Auinger, & Ryan, 2003). Last, Christenson, & Thurlow, 2005), quasi-experi-
students who feel more engaged in school, mental studies (Gottfredson, Gerstenblith, Soule,
empowered by their teachers, and supported Womer, & Lu, 2004) and single case studies
by their teachers and peers are less likely to bully (Hawken & Horner, 2003; Moore, Cateledge, &
others or be victimized by peers (Brookmeyer, Heckaman, 1995), and single case studies
Fanti, & Henrich, 2006). These findings hold true (Hawken & Horner, 2003; Moore, Cateledge, &
for both urban and suburban, ethnically diverse Heckaman, 1995). Most of these studies were
adolescents. specifically designed to improve the school rela-
tionships of students who were at risk or had
already exhibited behavior problems.
Summary of Recent Studies: Hybrid Risk Although contexts outside the school setting
and Protective Factors contribute to student engagement, schools still
need to consider ways to engage students, avoid
Student disengagement from school is associated disengaging students, as well as reconstruct and
with considerable negative academic, behavioral, repair relationships with students who have dis-
and physical outcomes. Similarly, strong student engaged. Fortunately, research indicates that
engagement has been identified as a protective and alterable school-based assets influence student
promotive factor contributing to both academic engagement for youth at all levels of family risk,
and overall success. Recent research on student even when individual traits are considered
engagement and compatible terms (e.g., school (Sharkey, You, & Schnoebelen, 2008). Furlong
connectedness, school bonding) continue to high- and colleagues (2003) suggested that student
light the positive outcomes associated with a posi- engagement can be conceptualized as a set of
tive relationship between students and school behaviors along a continuum from high to low
community, and the negative effects associated with levels of school involvement. Therefore, interven-
student disengagement. Table 27.2 summarizes the tion strategies to encourage student engagement
Table 27.2 Key studies of relations between school connectedness/student engagement/school bonding and its effect on alcohol use, cigarette use, marijuana use, dropping out, and
conduct disorder/violence among adolescents
Authors\year Term and measure used Alcohol Cigarettes Marijuana Dropout Conduct problems Findings
Substance use
Resnick et al. (1997)School connectedness X X X High levels of school connectedness related to less
ADD Health Scalea frequent alcohol, cigarette, and marijuana use
Simons-Morton (2004) School engagement X Students engaged in school were less likely to start
Three items about school in drinking alcohol
generalb
Rasmussen, School connectedness X Students connected to school were less likely to smoke
Damsgaard, Holstein, (Danish) Health Behavior in cigarettes
and Poulsen (2005) School-Aged Children
National Studyc
Henry (2008) School attachment X X X Low levels of school attachment were positively
Five items about teacher, correlated to involvement with friends who use drugs,
safety, and enjoymentd which was related to use or intention to use alcohol and
drugs
Students who are more engaged in school are less likely to start using alcohol than less engaged students. In addition, those who are more engaged use alcohol, marijuana, and
cigarettes at a lower rate than their less engaged peers. These effects are found for both males and females from grades 6 through 12 in both rural and urban communities. Findings
suggest that student engagement may be a protective factor against substance use in other first world countries as well. Moreover, low student engagement is related to involvement
with friends who use drugs, which is subsequently related to using drugs (alcohol, cigarettes, marijuana).
Dropout
Croninger and Lee Teacher-based social capital X Students who receive less “teacher-based” social capital
(2001) National Educational had a higher likelihood of dropping out of high school
Longitudinal Studye
Janosz et al. (2008) Student engagement X Students with normative levels of student engagement
Three-dimensional factor were less likely to drop out than those with varying levels
structuref of engagement. Also, students with an overall reduction in
levels of student engagement were more likely to drop out
Archambault et al. Student engagement X Adolescents with high global or behavioral student
(2009) Behavior, affective, and engagement were less likely to drop out of school compared
cognitive engagementg to adolescents with lower levels of student engagement
Students who feel more supported by their teachers are less likely to drop out of school compared to those who do not. Behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement (compli-
ance to rules, enjoyment of school, and willingness to learn) together are related to lower rates of school dropout. However, when examining the parts separately, only behavioral
engagement by itself is a protective factor against dropping out of school. Lastly, students with consistent levels of student engagement are less likely to drop out than those who
had varying levels, including decreasing, increasing, or both. Also, those with lower baseline levels and those who experience an overall reduction are at a higher risk for dropping
out of school. These findings are nationally representative of the USA and some French provinces in Canada.
Authors\year Term and measure used Alcohol Cigarettes Marijuana Dropout Conduct problems Findings
Conduct problems/violence
Kodjo et al. (2003) School connectedness X High levels of school connectedness significantly reduced
ADD Health Scalea weapon carrying for all adolescent males and for
African-American females
Henrich et al. (2005) School connectedness X Adolescents who were more connected to school were
ADD Health Scalea less likely to commit a violent crime than those less
connected to school
Brookmeyer et al. School engagement X Adolescents who reported high levels of student
(2006) ADD Health Scalea engagement participated in less violent behavior over time
than those who reported lower levels of school
connectedness
Loukas et al. (2006) School connectedness X Adolescents more connected to school were involved in
ADD Health Scalea less conduct problems than those who were not as
connected to school
Frey et al. (2008) School attachment X Boys who showed consistently elevated levels of school
Attachment to School Scaleh attachment during the transition from grade 8 to 9
exhibited lower levels of violent behavior
Nation, Vieno, Empowerment with teachers X Disempowered relationships with teachers predicted both
Perkins, and and classmates bullying and victimization among youth
Santinello (2008) Teacher and Classmate
Support Scalei
Flaspohler, Elfstrom, Teacher and social support X Students who were not involved in bullying (perpetrator
Vanderzee, Sink, and Child and Adolescent Social or victim) felt more supported by their teachers and peers
Birchmeier (2009) Support Scalej than those who bullied others or were victimized
Student engagement is a protective factor against weapon carrying for ethnically diverse males and African-American females. In addition, high levels of student engagement are
correlated to a lower likelihood of being involved in violent behaviors for diverse (ethnic and socioeconomic) male and female adolescents in grades 7–12. One study found a
similar relationship that also included lying and cheating as conduct behaviors. Lastly, students who feel more engaged in school, empowered by their teachers, and supported by
their teachers and peers are less likely to bully others or be victimized. These findings hold true for both urban and suburban, ethnically diverse adolescents.
(continued)
Table 27.2 (continued)
Authors\year Term and measure used Alcohol Cigarettes Marijuana Dropout Conduct problems Findings
Hybrid risks
Dornbusch, Erikson, School connectedness X X X X High levels of school connectedness predicted less
Laird, and Wong ADD Health Scalea delinquent behaviors on a whole. Specifically, school
(2001) connectedness predicted a decline in cigarette use and less
violent behavior
McNeely and Falci School connectedness X X X X Strong school connectedness was correlated to less
(2004) ADD Health Scalea initiation and low escalation of drinking alcohol, smoking
marijuana, and smoking cigarettes. Likewise, strong
school connectedness was related to less participation in
delinquent and violent behaviors
Bond et al. (2007) School connectedness X X X X High levels of school connectedness were related to low
The Communities That levels of substance use and a better chance of completing
Care Youth Survey Scalek school
Simons-Morton, School-bonding and X X X X High levels of school bonding and perceived school
Crump, Haynie, and perception of school climate climate were negatively associated with several problem
Saylor (1999) School bonding indexl behaviors
and instrument to measure
school climatem
High levels of student engagement are correlated with less substance use, dropping out, and problem behaviors such as fighting, bullying, stealing, weapon carrying, and vandalizing.
This was found for both male and female students of diverse ethnicities and socioeconomic statuses. The association between school engagement and hybrid risks became more
refined when separating school engagement into two parts: teacher support and social belonging. Teacher support and social belonging are protective against cigarette smoking
initiation and escalation from occasional to regular smoking. Teacher support decreases drinking initiation and escalation from being an occasional drinker to a regular drinker, while
social belonging does not. Teacher support also acts as a buffer for marijuana initiation, but not escalation to regular use. In addition, adolescents who feel supported by their teachers
are less likely to engage in violent behaviors and more likely to cease violent participation if they have been violent in the past.
Note: Measurement scales for student engagement used in studies
a
Resnick et al. (1997), bSimons-Morton (2004), cCurrie et al. (2000), dHenry (2008), eIngels et al. (1994), fFredricks et al. (2004), gArchambault et al. (2009), hWeissberg et al. (1991),
i
Torsheim, Wold, and Samdal (2000), jMalecki et al. (2000), kArthur et al. (2002), lSimons-Morton and Chen (2009), mPyper et al. (1987)
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 575

can occur in multiple systems. Sharkey et al. trajectory. Some of these systems may include
(2008) argued that school administrators may fostering positive attachment relationships (i.e.,
need to be encouraged to focus on relationship with teachers), increasing youth’s self-regulation
building, school safety, and school climate in skills (i.e., teaching appropriate behaviors and
order to promote positive outcomes. Providing self-monitoring), or providing opportunities for
youth with opportunities for meaningful school the child to experience success in order to increase
involvement and reinforcing this involvement can self-efficacy and motivation to succeed in life
lead to the development of facilitative school (i.e., acknowledging students and pointing out
bonds (Catalano, Kosterman, Hawkins, Newcomb, successes through reinforcement). Other strate-
& Abbott, 1996). gies may be employed to increase the resources
required for children to build competence.
Providing additional tutoring, free extracurricu-
Intervention and Engaging Protective lar activities, and providing job programs for par-
Mechanisms ents may lead to an increase in the resources
available to at-risk youth (Benson, Galbraith, &
Based on research indicating that high-risk Espeland, 1995; Benson, Scales, Leffert, &
behavior is related to numerous contextual influ- Roehlkepartain, 1999).
ences ranging from individual factors to social/
community factors, it will be important to take all
of these into account when understanding how to Utilizing System Level Interventions
intervene, engage protective mechanisms in their
school environment, and provide these children Given the importance of activating protective
with an ability to attain positive outcomes. Rutter mechanisms across multiple systems, schools
(1987) described four protective mechanisms. may begin to intervene with high-risk and
The first mechanism is the reduction of risk troubling behaviors from a systems perspective.
impact, meaning the negative impact may be Those students who have been involved in the
reduced by preparing the child for the situation, juvenile justice system or who have exhibited
exposing the child when he or she can handle the antisocial behavior represent a unique population
situation, decreasing exposure to the risk factor, who are at a greater risk of school and lifelong
providing the child with practice in coping, and problems. The use of system-wide proactive
reducing the demands of the risk factor. The sec- approaches to prevent further problems will be
ond mechanism is the reduction of negative chain especially important to decrease problem behav-
reactions (the snowball effect), by implementing ior and increase student achievement, although
interventions that prevent a chain of reactions information to support the effectiveness of these
that perpetuate the risk effects in the future. The strategies with high-risk populations is limited.
third mechanism is described as the development Schools should be able to use specific behav-
of self-esteem and self-efficacy so that the child ioral strategies, practices, and processes beyond
feels that he or she has the ability to deal with the individual student and apply them to the
life’s challenges, is satisfied with his or her social whole school with integrity. Specific to imple-
relationships, and feels success in the completion menting a behavioral program, Mayer (1995)
of some tasks. Finally, the fourth mechanism provided three factors in schools that are related
described is the opening of opportunities. These to antisocial behavior. These factors include (a)
four mechanisms are child-focused but may be unclear rules and policies, (b) inconsistent staff
implemented throughout the various systems. (i.e., lack of staff agreement on policies, inconsis-
An effective intervention strategy may be tent with rules, staff do not support one another),
to organize and activate positive institutions or and (c) lack of allowances for individual differ-
systems that will promote healthy develop- ences (i.e., academic and social skills of students
ment and potentially alter a child’s negative vary, the selection of reinforcers, and punishers is
576 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

not individualized). Overall, academic programs and out-of-school suspensions by preventing


that successfully manage behavior adjust their disruptive behaviors from occurring, although
programs to the student’s level of functioning and little is known about the impact of these types of
build skills in areas of struggle. This adjustment programs on youth with extreme behavior disor-
allows the student to be successful and work in a ders or those already placed in more restrictive
positive environment. Implementing these strate- educational programs. According to the PBIS
gies has been associated with a decrease in drop- Brief Guide (n.d.), the school district staff noted
out rates and suspensions (Griffiths, Parson, that PBIS implementation has had an overall pos-
Burns, & VanDerHeyden, 2007). itive effect on school climate by improving the
positive interactions among staff, which provides
a positive model for students to follow, although
School-Wide and Targeted there is little empirical evidence to support this
Interventions statement.
Recently, Griffiths (2010) investigated the appli-
School-wide intervention programs may be an cation of school-wide positive behavior support
effective strategy to increase student engagement (PBIS) in an alternative school setting. The main
(Maddox & Prinz, 2003), particularly for those purpose of this 1-year evaluation case study was
youth engaging in high-risk behaviors. These pro- to evaluate the impact of a high school PBIS
grams may help students bond with their schools model on school-wide discipline outcomes (inci-
and experience fewer negative outcomes. Despite dent reports, teacher reports of student behavior).
the research available on school-wide interven- A secondary aim was to gain an increased under-
tions, there is limited research supporting the use standing of the psychological well-being, engage-
of these interventions for the high-risk population ment, and adjustment of students in the alternative
discussed in this chapter. Scott et al. (2002) education setting, specifically as it relates to indi-
provide a description of how Positive Behavior vidual student’s participation in or response to
Interventions and Supports (PBIS) may be used in this particular intervention (PBIS). The impact of
alternative education settings as a means of pre- intervention implementation was measured using
vention of problem behavior for youth who are at quantitative and qualitative methods. The overall
a greater risk. Increasingly, alternative education level of implementation of PBIS during the first
programs have been identified as schools for dis- year of implementation reached 69%, as mea-
ruptive youth (Foley & Pang, 2006). Already sured by the School-Wide Evaluation Tool.
socially marginalized, these students have failed The results indicated that the overall number
to meet the expectations of traditional schools and of incident reports did not significantly differ
most likely experienced difficulty developing between the baseline year and the implementa-
positive connections with peers and adults at these tion year. However, there were some significant
schools. These schools are often located outside reductions in defiance-related behaviors (z = 2.46,
of mainstream school campuses so as to not “dis- p < .05). Based on student participation in the
tract others” from learning; however, they con- program, students were divided into two groups:
tinue to perpetuate the social marginalization of “responders” and “nonresponders.” Between
these students (Munoz, 2004). these groups, students’ responses to a number of
One of the fundamental aims of PBIS is to pre- measures (obtained prior to intervention) assess-
vent violence and substance abuse among young ing student perception of individual, school,
people. Within most school systems, acts of vio- social/community, and home systems were
lence are most often punished by suspensions or compared. Results indicated that the individual
expulsions, which remove the student from the system model and the school system model were
learning environment and at times place them able to distinguish between responders and
into an alternative setting. PBIS programs are nonresponders. Specifically, a one-way between-
intended to reduce the number of both in-school groups multivariate analysis of variance
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 577

(MANOVA) was performed to explore the differ- An example of a small group intensive inter-
ence between groups (responders and nonre- vention program is Check & Connect (C&C), a
sponders) on hostility, destructive expression of targeted intervention used to facilitate student
anger, hope, life satisfaction, depression, and engagement and school completion for a small
sense of inadequacy. There was a statistically group of students already identified to be at risk.
significant difference between responders and The C&C model includes the core elements of
nonresponders on the combined variables (F relationship building, routine monitoring of alter-
(1, 38) = 3.28, p = .012; Wilks’ lambda = .63; par- able risk factors, individualized intervention,
tial eta squared = .374). When the results for the continuous monitoring of targeted students, teach-
variables were considered separately, the univari- ing problem-solving skills, building affiliation
ate differences to reach statistical significance with school, and a persistent reinforcement of
were hostility, destructive expression of anger, academic behaviors. Within this program, data
and depression. An inspection of the mean scores are systematically used to guide intervention
indicated increased scores on all of these vari- plans and improve the program at each school site
ables for nonresponders. (Sinclair, Christenson, Lehr, & Anderson, 2003).
Within the school system model, there was In an evaluation of C&C, 80 elementary and
a statistically significant difference between middle school students involved in the program
responders and nonresponders on the combined served as participants. Results indicated that
variables (F (1, 38) = 3.20, p = .035; Wilks’ after accounting for student risks and prior atten-
lambda = .794; partial eta squared = .206). When dance, intervention staff and student perceptions
the results for the univariate analyses were consid- of the quality and closeness of their relationship
ered, the differences to reach statistical significance were positively correlated with the behavioral
were academic self-concept, attitude to teachers, engagement indicator of school attendance. The
and attitude to school. An inspection of the mean implementers’ perception of their relationship
scores indicated increased scores (indicating a with students was related to teacher-rated aca-
problem) on the attitude to teachers and attitude to demic engagement, which includes being pre-
school subtests for nonresponders. Responders had pared for class, work completion, and persistence
higher mean scores on academic self-concept. (Anderson, Christenson, Sinclair, & Lehr, 2004).
A logistic regression revealed that hostility,
destructive expression of anger, depression, aca-
demic self-concept, attitude to school, and atti- Classroom-Based Interventions
tude to teachers, as a group, were able to and Student-Teacher Relationships
distinguish responders from nonresponders (c2
(6, N = 40) = 12.58, p = .05). These findings seem In addition to considering school-wide variables
to indicate that PBIS had some impact on improv- and targeted interventions, a variety of classroom
ing outcomes for specific behavior types (defi- variables may be manipulated to increase a stu-
ance) for some students in alternative school dent’s sense of belonging to a positive learning
settings. However, it must be considered that community and may lead to an increase in stu-
given the path these students have been on to get dent engagement (Furlong et al., 2003). These
to an alternative school, they have likely devel- factors include the use of cooperative learning
oped an ingrained distrust and negative attitude instructional strategies, positive student-teacher
toward school and teachers that may require more relationships, and promotion of mutual respect
intensive intervention. In addition, these students, within the classroom.
particularly those classified as “nonresponders,” Some classroom interventions may include
tend to experience numerous mental health con- reducing or eliminating visible academic competi-
cerns and contextual risk factors, and will require tion among peers, as it may improve engagement
more intensive supports in conjunction with uni- of students of varying academic achievement lev-
versal interventions. els but for various reasons. For example, Wehlage
578 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

and Rutter (1986) found that students who were school students’ perceptions of teacher emotional
average achieving and found their secondary support were related to their global subjective
schools to be interpersonally unsupportive and well-being. On the other hand, behaviors such as
academically frustrating were more likely to noncompliance may damage student-teacher
drop out of school. By minimizing competition relationships and result in missed opportunities
and privileges for honor roll students and for learning (Walker & Walker, 1991). Students
high achievers, perceptions of frustration and who exhibit defiant behavior may frequently
defeat may be altered (Wehlage & Rutter, 1986). engage in a negative pattern of interactions with
Recently, Morgan (2006) reviewed studies that teachers, with noncompliance being a frustrating
examined preference and choice making as class- experience for teachers. Based on observations,
room interventions for increasing behavioral task children considered to be noncompliant and
engagement. These 15 reviewed studies supported aggressive or disruptive spend less time on task
the hypothesis that preference assessment and than comparison students and experience a dis-
choice making improve the behavior and academic ruption in academic skills development (Shinn,
performance of students. Morgan concluded that Ramsey, Walker, O’Neill, & Steiber, 1987).
teachers who use preference assessment, in addi- Although the link between adolescent stu-
tion to choice making, are more likely to improve dents’ perceptions of the quality of their relation-
the students’ engagement than those using choice- ships with teachers and classroom behavior is
making procedures alone. proximal (e.g., classroom behavior), other
Teaching techniques are crucial to increasing researchers report that it is associated with more
engagement. Reeve, Jang, Carrell, Jeon, and distal high-risk behaviors that are of concern to
Barch (2004) examined the use of autonomy sup- educators and parents such as substance use
port in a teacher motivation style as a way to (Rostosky, Owens, Zimmerman, & Riggle, 2003),
promote engagement during instruction. Two aggressive/conduct disorder behavior (Frey,
aspects of engagement were measured: task Ruchkin, Martin, & Schawb-Stone, 2008), and
involvement (attention, effort, verbal participa- school dropout (Christenson & Thurlow, 2004).
tion, persistence, and positive emotion) and influ- These relations are particularly important for
ence attempts (teacher and student verbal and those students who already engage in high-risk
nonverbal attempts to influence the behavior or behaviors. Many students who engage in high-
decision of the other party in a constructive man- risk behaviors have had multiple experiences of
ner). Teachers trained in these techniques dis- failure in the school setting, as well as a series of
played more autonomy-supportive behaviors than negative interactions with adults at school, at
teachers who were not trained. The more teachers home, and in the community.
used autonomy support, the more the students When further examining relationships between
were engaged. students and their teachers, Hughes and Kwok
With regard to student-teacher relationships, (2007) investigated the influence of student-
Gregory and Ripski (2008) found that student teacher and parent-teacher relationships on
trust mediated the relation between teacher rela- engagement and achievement. Their model sug-
tional (personal) discipline approaches and both gests that the quality of teacher’s relationships
student and teacher reported defiant behavior. In with students and their parents explained the rela-
this study, the teachers purposefully sought to tion between students’ background and student
make a personal emotional connection with stu- engagement. Engagement, in turn, mediated the
dents, and their students reciprocated. In other relations between student-teacher and parent-
words, even at the classroom level, the develop- teacher relatedness and student achievement the
ment of a positive trusting student-teacher rela- following year. Results indicated that African-
tionship is associated with decreases of troubling American children and their parents had less sup-
behaviors (Gregory & Ripski, 2008). In a related portive relationships with teachers when
study, Suldo et al., (2009) found that middle compared with Latino and Caucasian children
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 579

and their parents. Schools should not only work at their school care about them are at substan-
on parental involvement in school but also tially lower risk of involvement in troubling
develop the relationship between parents and externalizing behaviors. Positive connectedness
teachers, particularly with the families of low- to adults at school may serve as a barrier to high-
income and minority students. Teachers may risk or troubling behaviors. It is almost as if when
need training in how to build successful relation- faced with choices related to high-risk behaviors,
ships with parents and how to create a supportive a student would consider the question of “Who at
classroom environment. this school would I disappoint if I engaged in this
behavior?” This highlights the importance of
schools focusing on system-wide programs and
Interventions Beyond the School intensive interventions that provide the opportu-
Context nity to reconstruct and repair bonds with margin-
alized students across various contexts.
Given the multiple risk factors present in the lives In addition, one must consider that most youth
of “high-risk youth,” it is likely that intervention do not engage in serious troubling or risky behav-
should extend beyond the immediate school set- iors whether or not they are bonded or connected
ting. However, interventions that are useful and to school. There are other protective forces in
impact more than one environment for the child youth lives such as extended family members,
may be identified by school professionals. community organizations, mentors, music teach-
Multisystemic therapy has shown some promise ers, and many others. As Masten (2009) suggested,
in making changes for these youth (Timmons- youth seem to need, and benefit from having, life
Mitchell, Bender, Kishna, & Mitchell, 2006). conditions that include the caring attention of
This speaks to the importance of understanding adults. For many youth, a natural, meaningful
each child within the context of the systems context for this to occur is in the school with
within which he or she is embedded. teachers and other adults who are engaged with
Understanding these students’ needs on both a them on a daily basis, often over several years.
broad and in-depth level will allow school pro- A final point that merits some attention is that
fessionals to measure the student’s current status, of which aspects of affective engagement are the
set goals for the student, coordinate services, and focus of research. Since the Resnick et al., (1997)
evaluate whether or not the interventions were article appeared in the Journal of the American
effective. Rather than pouring multiple resources Medical Association and refocused attention on
into an individual without a precontemplated out- what was called “school connectedness,” it has
come or plan, coordination of services may prove been cited more than a 1,000 times by other pub-
to be effective, economical, and efficient. lications indexed in the PsyInfo database. This
study led to a special journal issue in the Journal
of School Health, September 2004, with articles
Summary of Strategies to Build Bonding focusing on school connectedness (Blum &
and Connectedness Libbey, 2004). In addition, the Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention (CDC, 2009)
When it comes to high-risk behaviors, it is well released a document that specifically summarizes
established that positive family relatedness and research-supported strategies to build school con-
other factors can be both promotive and protec- nectedness. As defined in the CDC document,
tive factors against involvement in high-risk connectedness is a “…belief by students that
behaviors such as substance use, aggression, and adults in the school care about their learning as
other externalizing behavior problems (Wither- well as about them as individuals” (CDC, 2009,
spoon, Schotland, Way, & Hughes, 2009). In p. 3). However, as used in practice, the same scale
addition, it is known that students who report that used in the Resnick et al. (1997) study and called
they have formed a generalized belief that adults school connectedness has been used in other
580 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

studies (e.g., Anderman, 2002) and called “school Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Dauber, S. L. (1993).
bonding.” This imprecision in the labeling of the First grade classroom behavior: Its short- and long-
term consequences for school performance. Child
latent traits of interest is not trivial. If researchers Development, 64, 801–814. doi:10.2307/1131219.
interested in the forces that have a protective Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Dauber, S. L. (1996).
influence on youth aim to examine student- Children in motion: School transfers and elementary
teacher relationships, then the term that makes school performance. The Journal of Educational
Research, 90, 3–12.
the most sense is “caring adult relationships.” Alliance for Excellent Education. (2009). The high cost of
This contrasts with researchers who are interested high school dropouts: What the nation pays for inade-
in whether a student broadly perceives his or her quate high schools. Available at http://www.all4ed.org/.
school environment to be one that is accepting Anderman, E. M. (2002). School effects on psychologi-
cal outcomes during adolescence. Journal of
and to which they feel included in as a member of Educational Psychology, 94, 795–809. doi:10.1037//
the school community (Shochet, Smith, Furlong, 0022-0663.94.4.795.
& Homel, 2011). This latter notion of student Anderman, L. H., & Anderman, E. M. (1999). Social pre-
engagement has been called school bonding or, dictors of changes in students’ achievement goal ori-
entations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24,
when the student personally identified as being a 21–37. doi:10.1006/ceps.1998.0978.
“citizen” of the school community, “school mem- Anderson, A. R., Christenson, S. L., Sinclair, M. F., &
bership” (Finn, 1989; Goodenow, 1993; Wehlage, Lehr, C. A. (2004). Check & Connect: The importance
1989). This is what Wehlage defined as “more of relationships for promoting engagement with
school. Journal of School Psychology, 42, 95–113.
than simple technical enrollment in the school. It doi:10.1016/j.jsp. 2004.01.002.
means that students have established a social Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J.
bond between themselves, the adults in the (2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con-
school, and the norms governing the institution” ceptual and methodological issues of the construct.
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. doi:10.1002/
(Wehlage, 1989, p. 10). pits.20303.
Future research examining the salvative effects Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L.
of the affective component of student engage- (2009). Adolescent behavioral, affective, and cogni-
ment, in our view, will be enhanced by more pre- tive engagement in school: Relationship to dropout.
The Journal of School Health, 79, 408–415.
cision in the latent traits being examined and doi:10.1111/j.1746-1561.2009.00428.x.
assuring that the measures being used actually Arthur, M. W., J.D. Hawkins, J. D., Pollard, J. A.,
measure those traits (see You, Ritchey, Furlong, Catalano, R. F., & Baglioni, A. J. (2002). Measuring
Shochet, & Boman, 2011; and Furlong, O’Brennan, risk and protective factors for substance use, delin-
quency, and other adolescent problem behaviors: The
& You, 2011 for more discussion of this topic). Communities That Care Youth Survey. Evaluation
Unified research in the area of student engage- Review, 26, 575–603. doi:10.1177/019384102237850.
ment can allow for continued advancements in Barrington, B. L., & Hendricks, B. (1989). Differentiating
understanding how to best engage students, spe- characteristics of high school graduates, dropouts, and
nongraduates. The Journal of Educational Research,
cifically high-risk students, and yield positive aca- 82, 309–319.
demic and life outcomes for youth. Benson, P. L., Galbraith, J., & Espeland, P. (1995). What
kids need to succeed. Minneapolis, MN: Free Spirit.
Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Leffert, N., & Roehlkepartain,
References E. C. (1999). A fragile foundation: The state of devel-
opmental assets among American youth. Minneapolis,
MN: Search Institute.
Abbott, R. D., O’Donnell, J., Hawkins, J. D., Hill, K. G., Blum, R. W., & Libbey, H. P. (2004). School connected-
Kosterman, R., & Catalano, R. F. (1998). Changing ness: Strengthening health and educational outcomes
teaching practices to promote achievement and bond- for teenagers. Executive summary. The Journal of
ing to school. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, School Health, 74, 231–232.
68, 542–552. doi:10.1037/h0080363. Bond, L., Butler, H., Deip, E., Dip, G., Lydnal, T., Carlin,
Abbott-Chapman, J., Denholm, C., & Wyld, C. (2008). J., et al. (2007). Social and school connectedness in
Social support as a factor inhibiting teenage risk-tak- early secondary school as predictors of late teenage
ing: Views of students, parents and professionals. substance use, mental health, and academic outcomes.
Journal of Youth Studies, 11, 611–627. doi:10.1080/ Journal of Adolescent Health, 40, e9–e18.
13676260802191938. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.10.013.
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 581

Bridgeland, J. M., DiIulio, J., & Morison, K. B. (2006, Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
March). The silent epidemic: Perspectives of high school Educational Research, 29, 141–162. doi:10.3102/
dropouts. Washington, DC: Civic Enterprises, LLC. 00346543059002117.
Brookmeyer, K., Fanti, K., & Henrich, C. (2006). Schools, Finn, J. D. (1993). School engagement and students at
parents, and youth violence: A multilevel ecological anal- risk. Washington, DC: National Center for Education
ysis. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Statistics.
35, 504–514. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_2. Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success
Carter, M., McGee, R., Taylor, B., & Williams, S. (2007). among students at risk. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Health outcomes in adolescence: Associations with 82, 221–234. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.82.2.221.
family, friends and school engagement. Journal of Finn, J. D., & Voelkl, K. E. (1993). School characteristics
Adolescence, 30, 51–62. related to student engagement. The Journal of Negro
Catalano, R. F., Hawkins, J. D., & Smith, B. H. (2001). Education, 62, 249–268. doi:10.2307/2295464.
Delinquent behavior. Pediatrics in Review, 23, 387–392. Flaspohler, P., Elfstrom, J., Vanderzee, K., Sink, H., &
Catalano, R. F., Kosterman, R., Hawkins, J. D., Newcomb, Birchmeier, Z. (2009). Stand by me: The effects of
M. D., & Abbott, R. D. (1996). Modeling the etiology peer and teacher support in mitigating the impact of
of adolescent substance use: A test of the social devel- bullying on quality of life. Psychology in the Schools,
opment model. Journal of Drug Issues, 26, 429–455. 46, 636–649. doi:10.1002/pits.20404.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2009). Foley, R. M., & Pang, L. S. (2006). Alternative education
School connectedness: Strategies for increasing pro- programs: Program and student characteristics. The
tective factors among youth. Atlanta, GA: U.S. High School Journal, 89, 10–21.
Department of Health and Human Services. Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
Christenson, S. L., & Thurlow, M. L. (2004). School drop- School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
outs: Prevention considerations, interventions, and chal- the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
lenges. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 59–109. doi:10.3102/00346543074001059.
36–39. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301010.x. Frey, A., Ruchkin, V., Martin, A., & Schawb-Stone, M.
Connell, J. P., Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Clifford, E., Crichlow, (2008). Adolescents in transition: School and family
W., & Usinger, P. (1995). Hanging in there: Behavioral, characteristics in the development of violent behaviors
psychological, and contextual factors affecting whether entering high school. Child Psychiatry and Human
African American adolescents stay in high school. Development, 40, 1–13. doi:10.1007/s10578-008-0105.
Journal of Adolescent Research, 10, 41–63. Furlong, M. J., O’Brennan, L., & You, S. (2011).
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, Psychometric properties of the Add Health School
autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of Connectedness Scale for 18 sociocultural groups.
self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe Psychology in the Schools, 48, 986–997.
(Eds.), Self processes and development (Vol. 23). Furlong, M. J., Ritchey, K., & O’Brennan, L. (2009).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Developing norms for the California Resilience Youth
Croninger, R. G., & Lee, V. E. (2001). Social capital and Development Module: Internal assets and school
dropping out of high school: Benefits to at-risk students resources subscales. The California School
of teacher’s support and guidance. Teachers College Psychologist, 14, 35–46.
Record, 103, 548–581. doi:10.1111/0161-4681.00127. Furlong, M. J., Whipple, A. D., St. Jean, G., Simental, J.,
Crosnoe, R., Mistry, R. S., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2002). Soliz, A., & Punthuna, S. (2003). Multiple contexts of
Economic disadvantage, family dynamics, and adoles- school engagement: Moving toward a unifying frame-
cent enrollment in higher education. Journal of work for educational research and practice. The
Marriage and the Family, 64, 690–702. doi:10.1111/ California School Psychologist, 8, 99–114.
j.1741-3737.2002.00690.x. Goodenow, C. (1993). The psychological sense of school
Currie, C., et al. (Eds.). Health and health behaviour among membership among adolescents: Scale development and
young people. health behaviour in school-aged children: educational correlates. Psychology in the Schools, 30,
a WHO Cross-National Study (HBSC), International 79–90. doi:10.1002/1520-6807(199301)30:1<79::AID-
Report. Copenhagen: WHO, 2000. Available, from PITS2310300113>3.0.CO;2-X.
http://www.hbsc.org/downloads/Int_Report_00.pdf. Gottfredson, D. C., Gerstenblith, S. A., Soule, D. A., Womer,
Dornbusch, S., Erikson, K., Laird, J., & Wong, C. (2001). S. C., & Lu, S. (2004). Do after school programs reduce
The relation of family and school attachment to ado- delinquency? Prevention Science: The Official Journal
lescent deviance in diverse groups and communities. of the Society for Prevention Research, 5, 253–266.
Journal of Adolescent Research, 16, 396–422. doi:10.1023/B:PREV.0000045359.41696.02.
doi:10.1177/0743558401164006. Gregory, A., & Ripski, M. B. (2008). Adolescent trust in
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, val- teachers: Implications for behavior in the high school
ues, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109– classroom. School Psychology Review, 37, 337–353.
132. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153. Griffiths, A. J. (2010). Positive behavior support in the
Fine, M. (1991). Framing dropouts: Notes on the politics alternative education setting: A case study.
of an urban public high school. Albany, NY: State Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
University of New York Press. California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA.
582 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

Griffiths, A. J., Parson, L. B., Burns, M. K., & Ingels, S. J., Dowd, K. L., Stipe, J. L., Baldridge, J. D.,
VanDerHeyden, A. M. (2007). Response to interven- Bartot, V. H., & Frankel, M. R. (1994, October).
tion: Research for practice. Alexandria, VA: National Second follow-up: Dropout component data file user’s
Association of State Directors of Special Education. manual. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Guthrie, J. T. (2001, March). Contexts for engagement Education, Office of Educational Research and
and motivation in reading. Reading Online, 4(8). Improvement. Available, from http://nces.ed.gov/pub-
Available, from http://www.readingonline.org/articles/ search/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=94375.
art_index.asp?HREF=/articles/handbook/guthrie/ Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L.
index.html. (2008). School engagement trajectories and their
Gutman, L. M., & Midgley, C. (2000). The role of protec- differential predictive relations to dropout. Journal
tive factors in supporting the academic achievement of of Social Issues, 64, 21–40. doi:10.1111/j.1540-
poor African American adolescents during the middle 4560.2008.00546.x.
school transition. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Jimerson, S. R., Campos, E., & Greif, J. L. (2003). Toward
29, 223–248. doi:10.1023/A:1005108700243. an understanding of definitions and measures of school
Hanson, T. L., & Kim, J. O. (2007). Measuring resilience engagement and related terms. California School
and youth development: The psychometric properties Psychologist, 8, 7–27. Retrieved from www.csa.com.
of the Healthy Kids Survey (Issues & Answers Report, Johnson, L. S. (2009). School contexts and student belong-
REL 2007–No. 034). Washington, DC: U.S. Department ing: A mixed methods study of an innovative high
of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National school. The School Community Journal, 19, 99–118.
Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Kodjo, C., Auinger, P., & Ryan, S. (2003). Demographic,
Assistance, Regional Educational Laboratory West. intrinsic, and extrinsic factors associated with weapon
Hawken, L. S., & Horner, R. H. (2003). Implementing a carrying at school. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent
targeted intervention within a school-wide system of Medicine, 157, 96–103.
behavior support. Journal of Behavioral Education, Ladd, G. W., & Dinella, L. M. (2009). Continuity and
12, 225–240. doi:10.1023/A:1025512411930. change in early school engagement: Predictive of
Hawkins, J. D., Guo, J., Hill, K. G., Battin-Pearson, S., & children’s achievement trajectories from first to
Abbott, R. D. (2001). Long-term effects of the Seattle eighth grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101,
Social Development intervention on school bonding 190–206. doi:10.1037/a0013153.
trajectories. In J. Maggs & J. Schulenberg (Eds.), Langberg, J. M., Epstein, J. N., Urbanowicz, C. M.,
Applied developmental science: Special issue: Simon, J. O., & Graham, A. J. (2008). Efficacy of an
Prevention as altering the course of development, 5, organization skills intervention to improve the aca-
225–236. doi:10.1207/S1532480XADS0504_04. demic functioning of students with attention-deficit/
Henrich, C., Brookmeyer, K., & Shahar, G. (2005). hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly,
Weapon violence in adolescence: Parent and school 23, 407–417. doi:10.1037/1045-3830.23.3.407.
connectedness as protective factors. Journal of Langberg, J. M., Smith, B. H., Bogle, K. E., Schmidt, J.
Adolescent Health, 37, 306–312. doi:10.1016/j. D., Cole, W. R., & Pender, C. A. S. (2006). A pilot
jadohealth.2005.03.022. evaluation of small group challenging horizons pro-
Henry, K. L. (2008). Low prosocial attachment, involve- gram (CHP): A randomized trial. Journal of Applied
ment with drug-using peers, and adolescent drug use: School Psychology, 23, 31–58. doi:10.1300/
A longitudinal examination of meditational mecha- J370v23n01_02.
nisms. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 22, 302– Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. B. (1995). Effects of high school
308. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.22.2.302. restructuring and size on early gains in achieve-
Herrenkohl, T. I., Hawkins, J. D., Chung, I.-J., Hill, K. G., ment and engagement. Sociology of Education, 68,
& Battin-Pearson, S. (2000). School and community 241–270. doi:10.2307/2112741.
risk factors and interventions. In R. Loeber & D. P. Loukas, A., Suzuki, R., & Horton, K. (2006).
Farrington (Eds.), Child delinquents: Development, Examining school connectedness as a mediator of
intervention, and service needs (pp. 211–246). school climate effects. Journal of Research on
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Adolescence, 16, 491–502. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.
Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley, CA/ 2006.00504.x.
Los Angeles: University of California Press. Maddox, S. J., & Prinz, R. J. (2003). School bonding in
Holdsworth, R., & Blanchard, M. (2006). Unheard voices: children and adolescents: Conceptualization, assess-
Themes emerging from studies of the views about ment, and associated variables. Clinical Child and
school engagement of young people with high support Family Psychology Review, 6, 31–49. doi:10.1023/A:
needs in the area of mental health. Australian Journal 1022214022478.
of Guidance and Counselling, 16, 14–28. doi:10.1375/ Malecki, C. K., Demaray, M. K., & Elliott, S. E. (2000).
ajgc.16.1.14. The Child and Adolescent Social Support Scale.
Hughes, J., & Kwok, O. (2007). Influence of student- DeKaub: Northern Illinois University.
teacher and parent-teacher relationships on lower Manlove, J. (1998). The influence of high school dropout
achieving readers’ engagement and achievement in the and school disengagement on the risk of school-age
primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, pregnancy. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 8,
99, 39–51. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.39. 187–220. doi:10.1207/s15327795jra0802_2.
27 The Relations of Adolescent Student Engagement with Troubling and High-Risk Behaviors 583

Masten, A. S. (2009). Ordinary magic: Lessons from learn. Washington, DC: The National Academies
research on resilience in human development. Press.
Education Canada, 49, 28–32. Noddings, N. (1995). Teaching themes of care. Phi Delta
Mayer, G. R. (1995). Preventing antisocial behavior in the Kappa, 76, 675–679.
schools. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, O’Farrell, S., Morrison, G. M., & Furlong, M. J. (2006).
467–478. School engagement. In G. Bear & K. Minke (Eds.),
McNeely, C., & Falci, C. (2004). School connectedness Children’s needs III (pp. 45–58). Bethesda, MD:
and the transition into and out of health-risk behavior National Association of School Psychologists.
among adolescents: A comparison of social belonging O’Farrell, S. L., & Morrison, G. M. (2003). A factor anal-
and teacher support. The Journal of School Health, 74, ysis exploring school bonding and related constructs
284–292. doi:10.1111/j.1746-1561.2004.tb08285.x. among upper elementary students. The California
McPartland, J. M. (1994). Dropout prevention in theory School Psychologist, 8, 53–72.
and practice. In R. J. Rossi (Ed.), Schools and students PBIS Brief Guide. (n.d.). Retrieved May 2, 2008, from
at risk: Context and framework for positive change www.pbis.org.
(pp. 255–276). New York: Teachers College. Perry, J. C. (2008). School engagement among urban
Mehan, H., Villanueva, I., Hubbard, L., Linz, A., youth of color. Journal of Career Development, 34,
Okamato, D., & Adams, J. (1996). Constructing 397–422. doi:0.1177/0894845308316293.
school success: The consequences of untracking low Perry, J. C., Liu, X., & Pabian, Y. (2010). School engage-
achieving students. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge ment as a mediator of academic performance
University Press. among urban youth: The role of career preparation,
Molina, B. S. G., Flory, K., Bukstein, O. G., Greiner, A. R., parental career support, and teacher support. The
Baker, J. L., Krug, V., et al. (2008). Feasibility and pre- Counseling Psychologist, 38, 269–295. doi:10.1177/
liminary efficacy of an after school program for middle 0011000009349272.
schoolers with ADHD: A randomized trial in a large Pyper, R., Freiberg, H. J., Ginsburg, M., & Spuck, D. W.
public middle school. Journal of Attention Disorders, (1987). Instruments to measure teacher, parent, and
12, 207–217. doi:10.1177/1087054707311666. student perceptions of school climate. In L. W. Barber
Moore, R. J., Cartledge, G., & Heckaman, K. (1995). The (Ed.), School climate (pp. 87–96). Bloomington, IN:
effects of social skill instruction and self-monitoring Center on Evaluation, Phi Delta Kappa.
on game-related behaviors of adolescents with emo- Rasmussen, M., Damsgaard, M., Holstein, B., &
tional or behavioral disorders. Behavioral Disorders, Poulsen, L. P. (2005). School connectedness and
20, 253–266. daily smoking among boys and girls: The influence
Morgan, P. L. (2006). Increasing task engagement using of parental smoking norms. European Journal of
preference or choice making: Some behavioral and Public Health, 2005(15), 607–612. doi:10.1093/eur-
methodological factors affecting their efficacy as pub/cki039.
classroom interventions. Remedial and Special Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J.
Education, 27, 176–187. doi:10.1177/0741932506027 (2004). Enhancing students’ engagement by incre-
0030601. asing teachers’ autonomy support. Motivation
Morrison, G., & Skiba, R. (2001). Predicting violence and Emotion, 28, 147–169. doi:10.1023/B:MOEM.
from school misbehavior: Promises and perils. 0000032312.95499.6f.
Psychology in the Schools, 38, 173–184. doi:10.1002/ Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman,
pits.1008. K. E., Harris, K. M., Jones, J., et al. (1997). Protecting
Munoz, J. S. (2004). The social construction of alternative adolescents from harm: Findings from the National
education: Re-examining the margins of public educa- Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health. Journal of
tion for at-risk Chicano/a students. The High School the American Medical Association, 278, 823–832.
Journal, 88, 3–22. doi:10.1001/jama.278.10.823.
Murray, C., & Malmgren, K. (2005). Implementing a teach- Roehlkepartain, E. C., Benson, P. L., & Sesma, A. (2003).
er-student relationship program in a high poverty urban Signs of progress in putting children first:
school: Effects on social, emotional, and academic adjust- Developmental assets among youth in St. Louis Park,
ment and lessons learned. Journal of School Psychology, 1997–2001. Prepared by Search Institute for St. Louis
43, 137–152. doi:10.1016/j.jsp. 2005.01.003. Park’s Children First Initiative, Minneapolis, MN.
Nation, M., Vieno, A., Perkins, D., & Santinello, M. Roeser, R., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). Per-
(2008). Bullying in school and adolescent sense of ception of the school environment and early adoles-
empowerment: An analysis of relationships with par- cents’ psychological and behavioral functioning in
ents, friends, and teachers. Journal of Community & school: The mediating role of goals and belonging.
Applied Social Psychology, 18, 211–232. doi:10.1002/ Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 408–422.
casp. 921. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.88.3.408.
National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Roscigno, V. J., & Ainsworth-Darnell, J. W. (1999). Race
(2004). Engaging schools: Fostering high school stu- and cultural/educational resources: Inequality, micro-
dents’ motivation to learn. Committee on increasing political processes, and achievement returns. Sociology
high school students’ engagement and motivation to of Education, 72, 158–178. doi:10.2307/2673227.
584 A.-J. Griffiths et al.

Rostosky, S. S., Owens, G. P., Zimmerman, R. S., & Riggle, Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Lehr, C. A., &
E. D. B. (2003). Associations among sexual attraction Anderson, A. R. (2003). Facilitating student engage-
status, school belonging, and alcohol and marijuana use ment: Lessons learned from Check & Connect longi-
in rural high school students. Journal of Adolescence, tudinal studies. The California School Psychologist, 8,
26, 741–751. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence. 2003.09.002. 29–42.
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., & Thurlow, M. L.
mechanisms. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, (2005). Promoting school completion of urban sec-
57, 316–331. ondary youth with emotional or behavioral disabili-
Sandhu, D. S., Arora, M., & Sandhu, V. S. (2001). School ties. Exceptional Children, 71, 465–482.
violence: Risk factors, psychological correlates, pre- Solorzano, D. G., & Delgado Bernal, D. (2001). Examining
vention and intervention strategies. In D. Sandhu transformational resistance through a critical race and
(Ed.), Faces of violence: Psychological correlates, LatCrit theory framework: Chicana and Chicano students
concepts and intervention strategies (pp. 45–71). in an urban context. Urban Education, 36, 308–342.
Huntington, NY: Nova Science. doi:10.1177/0042085901363002.
Schaufeli, W. B., Martinez, I., Pinto, A. M., Salanova, M., Suldo, S. M., Friedrich, A. A., White, T., Farmer, J.,
& Bakker, A. (2002). Burnout and engagement in uni- Minch, D., & Michalowski, J. (2009). Teacher support
versity students: A cross-national study. Journal of and adolescents’ subjective well-being: A mixed-
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 464–481. doi:10.1177 methods investigation. School Psychology Review, 38,
/0022022102033005003. 67–85.
Scott, T. M., Nelson, C. M., Liaupsin, C. J., Jolivette, K., Timmons-Mitchell, J., Bender, M. B., Kishna, M. A., &
Christle, C. A., & Riney, M. (2002). Addressing the Mitchell, C. C. (2006). An independent effectiveness
needs of at-risk and adjudicated youth through posi- trial of multisystemic therapy with juvenile justice youth.
tive behavior support: Effective prevention practices. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
Education and Treatment of Children, 25, 532–551. 35, 227–236. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_6.
Sharkey, J. D., You, S., & Schnoebelen, K. J. (2008). The Torsheim, T., Wold, B., & Samdal, O. (2000). The teacher and
relationship of school assets, individual resilience, and classmate support scale: Factor structure, test–retest reli-
student engagement for youth grouped by level of ability and validity in samples of 13- and 15-year-old ado-
family functioning. Psychology in the Schools, 45, lescents. School Psychology International, 21, 195–212.
402–418. doi:10.1002/pits.20305. doi:10.1177/0143034300212006.
Shinn, M. R., Ramsey, E., Walker, H. M., O’Neill, R., & Walker, H. M., & Walker, J. T. (1991). Coping with non-
Steiber, S. (1987). Antisocial behavior in school set- compliance in the classroom: A positive approach for
tings: Initial differences in an at-risk and normal popu- teachers. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
lation. Journal of Special Education, 2, 69–84. Wehlage, G. (1989). Dropping out: Can schools be
Shochet, I. M., Smith, C. L., Furlong, M. J., & Homel, R. expected to prevent it? In L. Weise, E. Farrar, & H.
(2011). A prospective study investigating the impact Petrie (Eds.), Dropouts from school. Albany, NY: State
of school belonging factors on negative affect in ado- University of New York Press.
lescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Wehlage, G. G., & Rutter, R. A. (1986). Dropping out:
Psychology, 40, 586–595. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1 How much do schools contribute to the problem?
5374416.2011.581616. Teachers College Record, 87, 374–392.
Simons-Morton, B. (2004). Prospective association of doi:10.2307/1164483.
peer influence, school engagement, drinking expectan- Weissberg, R. P., Caplan, M., & Harwood, R. L. (1991).
cies, and parent expectations with drinking initiation Promoting competent young people in competence-
among sixth graders. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 299– enhancing environments: A systems-based perspective
309. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.005. on primary prevention. Journal of Consulting and
Simons-Morton, B., & Chen, R. (2009). Peer and parent Clinical Psychology, 59, 830–841. doi:10.1037/0022-
influences on school engagement among early adoles- 006X.59.6.830.
cents. Youth and Society, 41, 3–25. doi:10.1177/ Witherspoon, D., Schotland, M., Way, N., & Hughes, S.
0044118X09334861. (2009). Connecting the dots: How connectedness to mul-
Simons-Morton, B., Crump, A., Haynie, D., & Saylor, K. tiple contexts influences the psychological and academic
(1999). Student-school bonding and adolescent prob- adjustment of urban youth. Applied Developmental
lem behaviors. Health Education Research, 14, Science, 13, 199–216. doi:10.1080/10888690903288755.
99–107. doi:10.1093/her/14.1.99. You, S., Ritchey, K., Furlong, M. J., Shochet, I., & Boman,
Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Evelo, D. L., & Hurley, P. (2011). Examination of the latent structure of the
C. M. (1998). Dropout prevention for youth with dis- Psychological Sense of School Membership Scale.
abilities: Efficacy of a sustained school engagement Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29(3),
procedure. Exceptional Children, 65, 7–21. 225–237. doi:10.1177/0734282910379968.
Trajectories and Patterns
of Student Engagement: Evidence 28
from a Longitudinal Study

Cathy Wylie and Edith Hodgen

Abstract
Longitudinal study of student engagement patterns is relatively rare
but sheds useful light on the factors that contribute to different levels of
student engagement in school and its role in student achievement. This
chapter uses data from a New Zealand study to focus on changes in student
engagement patterns between the ages of 10 and 16, to show (a) the range
of individual trajectories of student engagement that lie behind overall
declines, and (b) how these different trajectories are related to differences
in competency levels and to activities and relationships outside school in
ways that compound the patterns of engagement in learning in the school
environment and vice versa. Looking at student engagement longitudi-
nally raises the question of whether decline in student engagement levels
overall is related to transitions between schools or occurs more as part of
general human development that may be better supported by different
learning opportunities than schools currently provide. The chapter ends
with the case for more longitudinal research into the nature and role of
student engagement across different schooling contexts.

opportunities for employment without school qual-


Introduction ifications grow tighter. The link between disen-
gagement in school and eventually dropping out of
Student engagement in school is important to stu-
school, or leaving without a qualification that will
dent outcomes because of the co-productive
allow meaningful employment and further educa-
nature of learning. School success is receiving
tion opportunities, is now well established (Finn,
ever-increasing emphasis as an essential compo-
1989; Rumberger & Rotermund, 2012). The bar
nent of moving into productive adulthood, as
has also been raised on expectations of schools that
they succeed with a majority, or all, of their stu-
dents, as evident in policies such as No Child Left
C. Wylie, Ph.D. (*) • E. Hodgen Behind in the United States. Systematic focus on
New Zealand Council for Educational Research,
patterns of student engagement in school provides
Wellington 6040, New Zealand
e-mail: cathy.wylie@nzcer.org.nz; a lens for schools to gauge how well they are acti-
Edith.hodgen@nzcer.org.nz vating student energy, interest and self-regulation.

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 585


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_28, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
586 C. Wylie and E. Hodgen

Much of the research focus on student engage- terns of engagement were likely to reflect levels of
ment in school as a whole (rather than engage- stability in individuals, their families and school
ment in particular learning tasks, at a more environments, and in the match between these.
micro-level) has focused on those at the disen- This chapter aims to build on this important
gaged end of the spectrum. Janosz, Archambault, contribution to the deepening understanding of
Morizot, and Pagani (2008) questioned whether the development and role of student engagement
there is in fact a uniform trajectory of disengage- by exploring another longitudinal data-set from a
ment culminating in dropping out—and therefore, different country. This data-set, from the Competent
whether there was also a uniform trajectory of Learners study, allows us to start a little earlier,
engagement—using two analyzes of longitudinal before adolescence (age 10), to track engagement
data which show a range of patterns. Categorization levels in relation to competency levels over time
of the close to a third of a sample of 1,582 and to situate student engagement in school in
Montreal high school students who had not com- relation to student engagement in activities outside
pleted high school by age 22 showed that 40% school, including risk behaviour, friendships and
had had high levels of school motivation—the relations with family, as well as learning opportu-
‘quiet dropouts’ (Janosz, LeBlanc, Boulerice, & nities in the final years of high school.
Tremblay, 2000). Analysis of student engagement We start with a brief description of the
trajectories between ages 12 and 16 of 13,280 stu- Competent Learners study to provide a context
dents from 69 middle and high schools serving for the way student engagement and motivation
low-socioeconomic areas in Quebec showed are operationalized in this chapter. Next, the over-
seven different pathways. Three of these path- all pattern of student engagement from ages 10 to
ways were relatively stable, accounting for 91% 16 is charted, before the trajectories found for this
of the students. Fourteen percent had high engage- New Zealand sample are described. Then we out-
ment levels throughout the period. Just over half line the links between different trajectories of stu-
(53%) had somewhat high levels of engagement, dent engagement and competency levels, high
with a slight decline over the period. Twenty-four school qualifications and post-school learning
percent had a ‘stable moderate’ trajectory of experiences up to age 20. A closer look at two dif-
engagement, at lower levels than the first two ferent dimensions of student engagement in school
groups. The other four groups comprised those at ages 14 and 16 follows, to explore their relative
whose student engagement decreased over the weight in relation to student competency levels
period (2%); those whose engagement increased and high school qualifications and out-of-school
(1%); those whose engagement was low at age experiences and relationships. The conclusion
12, increased at 14, then became low again at age discusses implications of the findings, with some
16 (3%); and those whose engagement was mod- suggestions for further longitudinal research.
erate at age 12, decreased to low levels at age 14,
then returned to moderate levels at age 16 (3%).
This last group was most likely to drop out—42% The Competent Learners Study
did so. By contrast, only 1% of the first three
groups dropped out of school. However, because The Competent Learners study originated in the
most students were in the first three groups, 21% early 1990s to analyze the impact of early child-
of the dropouts in fact came from the group with hood education and, if Ministry of Education
the stable student engagement trajectories. funding continued for more than 3 years, to pro-
Janosz et al. (2008) found that those whose vide a second cohort for a linked study on
engagement trajectories were unstable over this the impact of policy changes which increased
adolescent period were more likely to have lower family choice of school and school competition.
self-reported grades or be in special classes; they The linked study did not continue, but the
had also began the period with lower levels of Competent Learners study did, as an exploratory
engagement. The authors suggested that the differ- study of the roles of education and home in the
ences found between the stable and unstable pat- development of competencies. Data collection
28 Trajectories and Patterns of Student Engagement: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study 587

Table 28.1 Competent Learners study: data collection


Number of early
Age of childhood education
participants Number in study (ECE) services or schools Data collected
Near 5 307 (Full study) 87 ECE services Assessment tasks, observations of children’s
activities in ECE centre, parent interview, teacher
rating of child’s attitudinal competencies, ECE
director interview, ECE service quality rating
Near 5 767 (Light interview) 56 ECE services Education participation, family resources, home
activities (from parent and teacher survey)
6 298 (Full study) 121 schools Assessment tasks, child interview, parent
interview, teacher rating of child’s attitudinal
competencies, and contextual information
8 523 (Full study + 242 168 schools Assessment tasks, child interview, parent
from light interview) interview, teacher rating of child’s attitudinal
competencies, and contextual information
10 507 185 schools Assessment tasks, child interview, parent
interview, teacher rating of child’s attitudinal
competencies, and contextual information
12 496 129 schools Assessment tasks, child interview, parent
interview, teacher rating of child’s attitudinal
competencies, and contextual information
14 476 76 schools Assessment tasks, student interview, student
self-report, parent interview, teacher rating of
student’s attitudinal competencies (3), dean survey
16 447, 420 at school 74 schools Assessment tasks, student interview, student
(school is compulsory self-report, parent interview, teacher rating of
until age 16) student’s attitudinal competencies (3), dean survey
20 401 None Young person interview, on-line self-report

began when the participants were aged near 5 and experiences in the scaffolding of learning
in their final months of early childhood education (Vygotsky, 1978); it was also informed by Sartre’s
(children start school in New Zealand on their concepts of praxis and project in the constitution
individual fifth birthdays). Table 28.1 sets out the of the self (Craib, 1976; Sartre, 1965).
data collection phases of the study. This study did not set out, then, with an explicit
When the study began, there were few longitu- focus on student engagement as such. When we
dinal studies of ‘everyday’ early childhood educa- sought to gain information on student experi-
tion (as compared with early childhood education ences and reactions to school, we did not have a
as an intervention, such as the Perry Preschool). distinct measure for engagement. In our items
Most studies of the ‘impact’ of early childhood asked of students about their school experiences,
education had little information about its wider we did touch on what have since been described
context, such as learning experiences in homes. (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004) as dis-
Existing studies also tended to either focus on aca- tinct, if interwoven, aspects of engagement:
demic or on socio-emotional outcomes. This study behavioral (e.g. ‘I skip classes’), emotional (e.g.
aimed to cover both kinds of outcomes, calling ‘I like my teachers’), and cognitive (e.g. ‘I get
them ‘competencies’, and to include the wider tired of trying’), as well as aspects of support, fair
context of children’s lives as well as their formal treatment and a sense of belonging at school.
educational context. The theoretical framework We also touched on some aspects of cognitive
that informed the initial conceptualization of the engagement in a set of items asking what infor-
study came from Bronfenbrenner’s ecological mation students used to think about the progress
framing of human development (Bronfenbrenner, they were making (e.g. ‘I learn something inter-
1979) and Vygotsky’s work on the role of previous esting’). At high school level, where students
588 C. Wylie and E. Hodgen

have different teachers for different subjects, we ‘intermediate’, commonly between the ages of 11
asked them items about three different classes; and 13) and high school, we have used items
the items included aspects of their own engage- from a wider set of items that were asked at ages
ment (behavioral, cognitive, emotional), as well 10, 12, 14 and 16. These items had the stem
as the opportunities for learning. Findings on the ‘School is a place where…’, and students were
relationship between student engagement and asked to rate how often the experience (e.g. ‘I
opportunities for learning provided in classes are enjoy learning’) occurred for them, on a 4-point
described in Hipkins (2012). In this chapter, we scale at age 10 (always, often, sometimes and
focus on student engagement as effort and enjoy- never), a 3-point scale at age 12 (usually, occa-
ment of learning, including both behavioral and sionally, rarely/never) and a 4-point scale at ages
cognitive aspects, and experience of supportive 14 and 16 (almost always/always, usually, occa-
relationships with teachers. sionally, rarely/never). To enable comparison
Motivation in this longitudinal study of over- across the ages, the age-12 engagement measure
all development is treated in terms of whether was calculated as if the responses were on a scale
education—school—is seen to serve a purpose with values 1, 2.5 and 4 rather than 1, 2, 3.
that lies beyond the day-to-day effort and atten- Initially, we tried a set of only six items, defin-
dance, and the future beyond school. This is akin ing student engagement only in terms of effort
to the Maehr and Meyer (1997) conceptualiza- and reaction, indicating behavioral and cognitive
tion, summarized by Appleton, Christenson and aspects of student engagement. While this set of
Furlong (2008, p. 379), as ‘answering the ques- six items showed a reasonable level of coherence
tion of “why am I doing this?”’. (reliability) at ages 14 and 16 (Cronbach’s alpha
To operationalise this concept of motivation, of 0.71 and 0.72, respectively), it did not at ages
we used a cluster analysis undertaken in the age- 10 and 12 (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.54 and 0.56,
14 phase of the Competent Learners study (Wylie respectively). This difference in coherence may
& Hipkins, 2006). This analysis included stu- relate to slight changes in wording over the
dent answers across a range of different questions period, but it may also indicate that the items and
at the age-14 phase, including how long they the rating levels mean different things to students
thought they would stay at school (whether past in high school and those at the end of elementary
the age of 16, when school attendance is no lon- school or in middle school. It would be interest-
ger compulsory), whether they thought they were ing to research this further, with even younger
gaining useful knowledge for their future in each students. Preliminary analysis of the draft Me
of the three compulsory subjects (English, math- and My School student engagement survey for
ematics, science), what they wanted to do when New Zealand students in Years 4–6 in relation to
they left school, and the kind of work they thought the Me and My School survey for Years 7–10 sug-
they might do (related to the kinds of qualifica- gested that there may be some differences in
tions they might need). Thus, our operational meaning (Darr, 2012).
definition of motivation is reasonably instrumen- To get a measure of student engagement that
tal and long term. had more coherence, we needed to add another
dimension. Our data allowed us to add one aspect
of the dimension of emotional engagement, with
Overall Patterns of Student material on student views of their relationships
Engagement in School from Ages with teachers. The final measure of student
10 to 16 engagement used in this chapter has ten items, as
listed below:
To compare patterns of student engagement from I enjoy myself/I enjoy learning.
the end of elementary school through middle I could do better work if I tried.
school (48% of New Zealand Year 7 and 8 I get bored.
students attend a 2-year middle school, called I get tired of trying.
28 Trajectories and Patterns of Student Engagement: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study 589

Table 28.2 Student engagement: mean scores on the Years 9 and 10. The change from one level of
student engagement items from ages 10 to 16 (n = 401) schooling to another, from the largely single-
Mean item score teacher format of elementary and intermediate
for the 10-item Median item schools to the multiteacher format of high
measure (1 = highest score for the
Age level, 4 = lowest) SD 10-item measure schools, seems a self-evident reason for the drop.
10 1.465 0.408 1.400 But without comparable data from further back
12 1.787 0.439 1.750 in schooling careers, as we report here, locating
14 2.120 0.438 2.100 change in engagement in change in schooling
16 2.187 0.448 2.167 level may be misleading. Further analysis of the
Competent Learners patterns showed no signifi-
I feel restless. cant differences in the mean scores on the student
I keep out of trouble. engagement scale we use here between students
I like my teacher(s). who attended intermediate school and those who
Teacher(s) listens to what I have to say/is inter- did not, at ages 12, 14 and 16, suggesting that
ested in my ideas. transitions into another school or another school-
Teacher(s) treats me fairly in class. ing level do not per se alter engagement levels.
I get all the help I need. Longitudinal analysis of changes in engage-
This multidimensional measure of student ment levels raises the question of whether there
engagement had reasonable coherence (reliabil- may be different reasons for changes in overall
ity), and again, more coherence at the high school levels of student engagement at different school-
level than at the elementary or middle school lev- ing levels. For example, preliminary analysis of
els (Cronbach’s alphas of 0.68, 0.70, 0.81 and the Me and My School trial survey for younger
0.81 for ages 10, 12, 14 and 16, respectively). New Zealand students, in Years 4–6, with two
Overall student engagement levels for the sample separate samples, indicated a drop in average
as a whole, as indicated by the mean score for the scores between Years 4 and 5, when most stu-
ten items comprising the measure, did decrease dents remain in the same school (C. Darr, per-
over time (Table 28.2). sonal communication, 2010; R. Dingle, personal
However, while a comparison of age 10 with communication, 2010). One reason for this may
age 16 engagement scores would show decline be the ‘fourth grade slump’, as some students
happens between ages 10 and 14—there is little struggle with moving from ‘learning to read’ to
difference in the mean engagement level of stu- ‘reading to learn’ (Chall, 1996).
dents aged 14 and 16, between the early and mid- The overall patterns we found in the Competent
dle years of high school. Yazzie-Mintz (2009) Learners study raise questions about changes in
also found little difference in student engagement the experience of education, as well as education’s
levels within high school, across grades 9–12 in place in what children experience in different con-
the USA (ages 14–18). However, Archambault, texts. Does the rate of change in engagement levels
Janosz, Morizot, and Pagani (2009) showed some evident in this sample reflect changing understand-
decline for most of the engagement trajectory ing or reference points as children grow into ado-
groups between ages 14 and 16 in a sample of lescence? Do mid-adolescents understand the
students from Quebec. items relating to engagement as effort differently
Darr (2012), with a larger New Zealand sam- from those who are younger? Does the context of
ple covering the ages 12–14 (school Years 7, 8, 9 high school give a different meaning to these
and 10), and a more comprehensive scale of stu- items? Do students expect different things of edu-
dent engagement, Me and My School, found a cation as they grow older, or do their views reflect
drop in engagement levels between Years 8 (when changes in the nature of their schooling experi-
many students are aged 12) and 9, the first year of ences? Is there a growing mismatch between their
high school (when many students are aged 13), developmental needs and how school is framed
but little difference between Years 7 and 8, and (Eccles, 1999)?
590 C. Wylie and E. Hodgen

Looking at changes in individual items over we used may simply reflect immediate experi-
time suggests a mixture of these possible reasons, ences colouring overall views. It would be very
as well as differences for individuals. Enjoyment interesting to ask students to rate their engage-
of learning declined somewhat (from 84% of the ment in school a number of times over the course
age-10 students to 70% of the age-14 and age-16 of a school year, or to ask students to describe
students), but experiences of boredom increased why they chose the rating they did after they had
between ages 12 and 14, from 12% to 34%. completed it, to see just what role immediate
Feeling they could do better work if they tried was experiences play or whether they carry more
reported as usually or more often the experience weight for some students than others.
for much the same proportion of 10- and 12-year We found nine different trajectories for indi-
olds, around 30%, increasing to 46% of the 14-year vidual students by dividing scores on the engage-
olds, and again to 57% of the 16-year olds, as the ment measure for each year into quartile groups
students encountered more assessment at high and categorising the resulting patterns. We
school, including the national qualifications. regrouped these to provide meaningful larger
Usually or almost always feeling restless doubled groups that we could use in cross tabulation with
from 4% of the 10-year olds, to 8% of the 12-year other data, so that we could explore differences in
olds, to 16% of the 14-year olds, and then increased experiences, relationships and competency levels
less markedly to 25% of the 16-year olds. that might be associated with differences in stu-
The New Zealand data on student engage- dent engagement trajectories.
ment, whether using the small set of indicators Seventeen percent of the sample showed a
we have used in this analysis, or the more com- stable high level of student engagement (compa-
prehensive Me and My School measure (Darr, rable with the 13% reported in the Quebec analy-
2012), show more changes between early adoles- sis, using a different measure of student
cence (age 12) and mid-adolescence (age 14) engagement, Janosz et al., 2008). Thirteen per-
than between ages 14 and 16. This is consistent cent showed a stable low level of student engage-
with the pattern of less continuity between the ment. In between came those who showed a stable
ages of 12 and 14 than between the ages of 14 moderately high level and those who showed
and 16 in individuals’ activities outside school, some variability around a moderate level (21%);
values and friendships that we have found in the those whose engagement was at a moderately low
Competent Learners study, suggesting that the years level throughout or decreased (24%) and those
of early adolescence may be the more volatile, and whose engagement was variable or increased
the ones where support to encourage engagement in (26%). Figure 28.1 shows these five trajectories.
learning and productive time use outside school
may be particularly critical (Wylie, Hipkins, &
Hodgen, 2008; Wylie & Hodgen, 2011). Social Characteristics of Different
Student Engagement Trajectories

Trajectories of Student Engagement Student engagement trajectories do reflect some


differences in the family resources available,
Yet this overall pattern of student engagement both at an early age and when they attended high
over time masks variation within—and between— school, gender and ethnicity, with the differences
individuals. Correlations between individuals’ most evident between those at either end of the
scores at different ages on this measure of student student engagement spectrum. So, students
engagement were not strong, though they did whose trajectory was low between ages 10 and
increase over time: r = 0.36 between ages 10 and 16 were more than twice as likely as those whose
12, r = 0.49 between ages 12 and 14, and r = 0.54 trajectory was high to have come from homes
between ages 14 and 16. To some extent, this lack that were low income as they approached school
of marked individual consistency on the measure starting age (38% and 14%, respectively) and six
28 Trajectories and Patterns of Student Engagement: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study 591

Fig. 28.1 Student engagement trajectories

times as likely to come from homes in difficult group). Mixed feelings about school at age 8
financial situations when they were age 14 (30% were most likely among those who later had low
compared to 6% of the high trajectory group). engagement levels, or who were in the variable/
Only 6% of the low trajectory group had mothers increasing group, or the moderately low/decreas-
with a university qualification compared with ing group. These patterns were also evident in
36% of those in the high trajectory group. Males parent responses when the sample was aged 10.
were more likely to be in the low or variable/ What is interesting also about parent perspec-
increasing trajectory groups (56% of these groups tives—at least as indicated at this broad level—is
compared to 33% of the high trajectory group). that many of those with children whose age-10
Mäori (New Zealand’s indigenous people) or self-report suggests a low student engagement
Pasifika students were least likely to be in the level described them as enjoying school.
high trajectory group (5%, where they comprised
13% of the sample).
Links with Competency Levels, School
Qualifications and Post-school Learning
Links with Early Enjoyment of School
Competency Levels
When the sample was aged 6, 8 and 10, we asked The different trajectories of student engagement
their parents an open-ended question about how show some marked differences in student compe-
their child liked school. At age 6, almost all the tency levels, both prior to age 10 and during the
parents thought their children were enthusiastic. age 10–16 period. The competencies measured in
At age 8, most parents thought their child showed the Competent Learners study are twofold. The
enthusiasm about going to school (73%), but first set we describe as ‘cognitive’ comprises
some differences related to these later student reading, writing, mathematics and the Raven’s
engagement trajectories were evident (84% of the Standard Progressive Matrices (New Zealand
later high trajectory group showed enthusiasm at Council for Educational Research, 1984; Raven,
age 8, compared to 67% of those in the variable/ 2000), all assessed through age-appropriate stan-
increasing group, and 66% of the low trajectory dardised assessments. The second set we describe
592 C. Wylie and E. Hodgen

Fig. 28.2 Student engagement trajectories and levels on cognitive competencies over time

as ‘attitudinal’ was originally derived from think- on standardised scores of the composite measures
ing about the purpose of early childhood educa- of each of these two sets of competencies from
tion and thinking of its role as the entrance to ages 8 to 16 and in relation to their achieve-
formal learning, a platform for developing skills ment level on the first tier of the New Zealand
and understandings that would enable ongoing three-level secondary qualification, the National
learning in other contexts. The ‘attitudinal’ com- Certificate of Educational Achievement (NCEA).
petencies comprise communication (expressive New Zealand has a criterion-referenced high
and receptive oral language), curiosity, persever- school qualification, the NCEA, with three levels.
ance, social skills (with adults and with peers) Level 1 is usually the goal of Year 11 students
and self-management. These areas were among (aged 14–15); Level 2 usually the goal of Year 12
those later identified by the Organisation for students (aged 15–16); and Level 3, the goal of
Economic Development and Co-operation Year 13 students (aged 17–18).
(OECD)’s DeSeCo project identifying key com- The five engagement trajectory groups show
petencies that allow the use of learning in ways three different trends. The high engagement tra-
that positively contribute to personal well-being jectory group had above average mean scores on
and support social participation (Hipkins, 2011; the cognitive competency composite measure.
Rychen, 2004). However, this group was not clearly distinct from
Figures 28.2 and 28.3 show the average scores the stable moderate trajectory group or the vari-
for the five student engagement trajectory groups able/increasing trajectory group. The latter two
28 Trajectories and Patterns of Student Engagement: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study 593

Fig. 28.3 Student engagement trajectories and levels on attitudinal competencies over time

groups sat in the middle of the average cognitive competency levels at one age made a separate
competency score spectrum, and at age 10, started contribution to attitudinal competency levels at
to be distinct from the two groups at the other end the next age but not vice versa, as shown in
of the spectrum, those in the always low trajectory Fig. 28.4. Gaining understanding from the read-
or moderately low/decreasing trajectory groups. ing given in school when one is aged 10, for
These tripartite trends are not quite so distinct for example, feeds into helpful levels of persever-
the attitudinal composite measure until age 14. ance, curiosity and communication at age 12,
This tendency for differences to show first on which are then of use in tackling more complex
the cognitive composite and then on the attitudi- reading and analysis.
nal composite is evident in other analyzes we
have done (Wylie, with Ferral, 2005), suggest- School Leaving Age and Highest School
ing that early school success, or conversely a Qualification
sense of failure, has a bearing on attitudes and The trends are slightly different when looking at
skills; this is consistent with the research on the school leaving age and school qualification, with
development of competence beliefs (Eccles, sometimes more success among those whose
Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998). Structural equa- engagement level had been at the low end of
tion modelling of the relationship between the moderate and then decreased than among those
attitudinal and cognitive competencies between whose engagement level had remained low
the ages of 8 and 16 also showed that cognitive between ages 10 and 16. Schooling is compulsory
594 C. Wylie and E. Hodgen

Fig. 28.4 Structural equation modelling of relations between attitudinal and cognitive competencies from ages 8 to 16
(CC cognitive composite, AC attitudinal composite)

in New Zealand until a student turns 16, though it high on average group, and 1% of those in the
has been possible to be granted an ‘early exemp- high trajectory group.
tion’ if a student had evidence of full-time employ- These links between student engagement
ment or tertiary study. As one might expect, those trajectories—as the effort put in and the percep-
in the consistently low engagement trajectory tion of supportive teacher-student relationships
group were most likely to be among those who over a 6-year period—and academic school
left as soon as they legally could (29% of this outcomes are quite marked. This is consistent
group). Early school leaving rates were more than with our finding that it was higher scores on the
twice as high for the moderately low and decreas- attitudinal composite measure that distinguished
ing engagement trajectory group and the variable students who gained a Level 2 NCEA from those
or increasing group than for the moderately highly who gained only Level 1 NCEA, or no qualifica-
engaged group or the high engagement trajectory tion, not higher scores on the cognitive compos-
group (19%, 15%, 7% and 6%, respectively). ite measure (Wylie & Hodgen, 2011).
Level 2 NCEA is now spoken of as the mini-
mal qualification that New Zealand students Motivation Levels
need for a secure adult future. Almost half of Motivation levels—here defined as seeing a long-
those (45%) in the low engagement trajectory term purpose in schooling at the age of 14—were
group left school without this qualification, as clearly linked with different engagement trajecto-
did 28% of those in the moderate-decreasing tra- ries. Fifty-one percent of the high trajectory group
jectory group, 19% of those in the variable- also had high motivation levels compared to 11%
increasing group, 14% of those in the moderately of the low trajectory group and 20% of the moder-
28 Trajectories and Patterns of Student Engagement: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study 595

ately low-decreasing trajectory group, with the between school and out-of-school engagement
other two groups in between the latter and the high patterns. Enjoyment of reading between the ages
trajectory group. A similar pattern was evident in of 8 and 14 shows the most consistency for those
relation to a factor focused on whether students in the high student engagement trajectory group:
gauged their progress in learning through interest 75% enjoyed reading all through this period.
in mastery and through self-regulation. Reading enjoyment consistency matches pretty
well with the other trajectory groups, too, from
Post-school Study 53% of those in the middling/variable trajectory
Different levels of student engagement at school group to 15% of those in the low trajectory group.
do not always lead on predictably or uniformly to We found a similar pattern, with smaller gaps
patterns of post-school learning. For example, at between the trajectory groups, for time spent
the age of 20, 22% of the high student engage- watching television over the 8–14 age period. At
ment trajectory group had not gone on to under- age 14, the low trajectory group were most likely
take further study, and 26% of the low student to include those who either had no particular lei-
engagement trajectory group were enrolled for a sure interests or who spent time on electronic
bachelor’s degree. However, just under half games, and the high and middling/variable trajec-
(47%) of the low and moderate-decreasing groups tory groups were most likely to include those
did no further formal study once school was who had creative leisure interests. The low trajec-
behind them. A quarter of those in the low trajec- tory group was least likely to include those whose
tory group had left a post-school course without values (when thinking of what was important to
completing it, as had 17% of those in the moder- them in the future) we summarized as ‘anchored
ately low-decreasing trajectory group. Student and achieving’ (an emphasis on having an inter-
engagement levels in school also seemed to have esting job, a good education, being with family,
a bearing on post-school learning approaches, being helpful or kind, and enjoying the things
with lower scores for the low and the moderately they did) and most likely to include those who
low-decreasing trajectory groups on our measure wanted to ‘stand out’ (have lots of money to
of ‘disciplined approaches to learning’, and the spend, lots of friends, wear the right clothes or
least need for support to learn among the high look cool, and have an important job). Thus, low
trajectory group. Thus, relying on ‘second chance’ levels of student engagement in school may also
education to make up for missed learning and be further weighed down by lack of engagement
qualifications at the school level may not be real- in activities outside school which could also pro-
istic for young adults who have not brought with vide goals for effort and experiences of absorp-
them habits of engaging in learning. tion and achievement related to effort.
Relations with others are also related to
engagement trajectories. Over the period between
Student Engagement in School age 10 and 14, the low trajectory group was much
and Experiences of Engagement more likely than the high trajectory group to
Out of School report some experience of bullying (as victim,
bully or both), 83% compared to 55%, with the
How do patterns of student engagement in school other three trajectory groups in between these
fit with experiences of engagement out of school? two. Similar patterns are evident in relation to
Can activities at home and in the wider commu- having friends with risky behaviour and under-
nity counter a lack of student engagement in taking risky behaviour (using alcohol, having
school—or does it become harder to engage in sex) oneself at the age of 14, and, not unrelated,
other activities if school does not provide produc- having less experience of good family communi-
tive avenues to sharpen knowledge, attitudes and cation and inclusion, and reporting more pressure
skills that support confidence and a sense of effi- from family—and, by parent account, more fric-
cacy? Our data show considerable overlaps tion in parent–child relationships.
596 C. Wylie and E. Hodgen

This relative consistency of experience in and items related to engagement in schoolwork.


out of school in approaches to activity—the depth The correlation between these two factors, school
and manner of engagement—and then the kinds belonging and engagement in schoolwork, was a
of relationships which are likely to accompany or moderate 0.58. Thus, while it was likely that a
be triggered by different approaches to activity, 16-year-old student who was comfortable in the
points to dialectical rather than unidirectional or school environment would also put energy into
passive relationships between individuals and the the work of learning, it did not always follow,
contexts in which they form themselves. Shiner and vice versa. School belonging and engage-
and Caspi (2003, p. 14) wrote of the ways in ment in schoolwork were both associated with
which individuals build continuity of personality student reports of being absorbed in their learn-
‘not merely through the constancy of behaviour ing, with positive classroom learning activities
across time and diverse circumstances, but also and with positive relations with teachers. But
through the consistency over time in the ways each factor also had some different associations
that persons characteristically modify their with family, peers and events. Having a support-
changing contexts as a function of their behav- ive family, friendships which contained some
iour’. We take this to mean in thinking about challenge as well as support, and experiences of
engagement that young people’s behaviour can being praised for achievement were more likely
elicit responses from family, peers and teachers to be associated with belonging in school than
that may compound their habits: the ways they with engagement in schoolwork. Risky behav-
spend time, the ways they engage in learning, and iour, friends with risky behaviour, family pres-
the avenues open to them as a result. sure and experience of adverse events in the past
year were more likely to be negatively associated
with engagement in schoolwork than with feel-
Engagement in Learning ings of belonging in school; school can be a place
and Belonging in School for social reinforcement as well as learning.
Belonging in school had a much lower correla-
Relationships are often mentioned in relation to tion with performance levels on the first level of
student engagement in learning. Teacher-student NCEA than did student engagement in school-
interactions, particularly responsiveness to indi- work (0.36 compared to 0.57). But the proportion
vidual students, active guidance and encourage- of variance in each of these factors accounted for
ment in activities, joining in children’s play and by age-14 motivation levels was slightly higher
asking open-ended questions, were enduring for belonging in school than for student engage-
aspects of early childhood education experience ment (11.2% compared to 7%). Thus, both school
that multivariate analysis showed still making a belonging and engagement in schoolwork were
contribution to cognitive and social skills compe- playing a role in student attachment to school.
tency levels at age 16 in the Competent Learners However, if one is interested in improving stu-
study, after taking into account family income dent achievement through improving student
and maternal qualification levels (Wylie & engagement levels, then the interaction between
Hodgen, 2007). Of interest in thinking about how learning opportunities and what individuals bring
student engagement in learning can be developed with them warrants attention. A second strand of
and improved are two other strands in our analy- this study shows that student perceptions that
sis of high school experiences. they were offered the kinds of learning opportu-
The first strand is the dimension of belonging nities that develop self-regulation and thinking
at school, feeling safe and treated as an individual. (see Hipkins, 2012) were closely correlated with
In both the age-14 and age-16 phases exploratory positive views of teachers. Both aspects of stu-
factor analysis of our large set of items, items dent engagement analyzed here (engagement
relating to this dimension were identified within a with schoolwork and feelings of belonging) had
separate factor from a factor that brought together higher correlation levels (r = 0.43 and 0.51,
28 Trajectories and Patterns of Student Engagement: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study 597

respectively) with these reports of learning oppor- shaping their learning through the effort they put in
tunities to develop self-regulation than did expe- and the responses of others as a result, but their
rience of adverse events or relations with family effort is also susceptible to changing learning
and friends (with the exception of similar correla- opportunities (some classes offer more in the way
tion levels [negative] with student engagement as of experiences that build engagement) and events
effort and friends with risky behaviour). (such as accidents, loss of family or friends).
Decline of student engagement over ages 10–16
raises some questions which are important in think-
Conclusion ing about how to improve student engagement. Is
the pattern we found with a New Zealand sample
Longitudinal patterns of student engagement, universal? Are there universal developmental dif-
when they can be related to academic perfor- ferences, as Eccles (1999) suggests in her discus-
mance and to both school and out-of-school con- sion of the importance of the fit between individual
texts, do confirm that student self-reports of their psychological needs and the opportunities students
engagement, here operationalized as effort and had in school and with their families, particularly
support, are worth attending to. Student self- through middle-childhood years? Eccles identified
reports of engagement do provide reasonable the ‘key psychological challenges’ for middle-
indicators of competency levels and, probably childhood as self-awareness, social comparison
through that link, to likely later academic perfor- and self-esteem. The key psychological challenges
mance and post-school learning. There was an she identified for early adolescence were more to
almost linear relationship between the five do with ‘a drive for autonomy paired with a con-
engagement trajectory patterns (at low, middle tinuing need for close, trusting relationships with
and high levels) and school achievement. The adults’ (Eccles, 1999, p. 41).
patterns described here also point to the impor- Longitudinal studies of student engagement in
tance of student engagement in school—in the context are still rare, but as the field grows, we
work of school—for the use of attention and need more studies that follow students over time
effort in the activities, interests and relationships to find out more about the role of different con-
outside school that also form identity. If nothing texts and different learning opportunities in dif-
else, then, the importance of formal education for ferent levels and trajectories of engagement and
the whole of life is evident. The relationships to find out more about how different aspects of
between formal education experiences and expe- engagement are related. Ideally, a longitudinal
riences outside school are not unidirectional— study of student engagement and its links with
but reinforcing and shaping the attention and student achievement would use a single scale
effort likely to be made in either sphere. with strong psychometric properties that could be
Yet while longitudinal patterns of student used across different ages and one that contained
engagement show consistencies, they also show sub-scales of behavioral, emotional and cogni-
some fluidity—only three of the original nine tra- tive dimensions of engagement, so that the way
jectories we found remained stable over the 6-year these are related and whether these relationships
period from when students were aged 10 to when change over time could also be investigated in
they were aged 16. The most consistent patterns relation to theories of human development.
found in this study were for those who by the age Archambault et al. (2009) provided a very inter-
of 10 had experienced school success at a reason- esting analysis of changes in student engagement
ably high level and, conversely, for those who at over ages 12–16, separately for behavioral, cog-
that age were struggling with school work. Those nitive and affective engagement and for differ-
with high engagement levels by age 10 appear most ent trajectories of engagement over that period.
likely to maintain those levels; but others with They suggested that it would be valuable to
moderate or low levels are more open to change include in such analysis more information on
(both down and upward). Students play a role in cognitive engagement, for example, the use of
598 C. Wylie and E. Hodgen

self-regulation strategies. We would agree and go Darr, C. (2011). Measuring student engagement: The
further. A comprehensive investigation of changes development of a scale for formative use. In S. L.
Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
in student engagement over time would need to Handbook of research on student engagement.
include measures of learning opportunities in stu- New York: Springer.
dent classrooms, based on the research about the Eccles, J. S. (1999). The development of children ages 6
kinds that are linked with student engagement, to 14. The Future of Children. When School is Out, 9,
30–44.
learning enjoyment and the development of self- Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998).
regulation and metacognitive skills. Such an Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N.
investigation would need a more complex mea- Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology
sure of motivation than the somewhat utilitarian (5th ed., pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley.
Finn, J. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
one we used in this study. It would also need to Educational Research, 59, 117–142.
include measures of student achievement and Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. (2004).
competencies, so that we could gain a full picture School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
of the dynamic relations between engagement the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
59–109.
dimensions, achievement (including and extend- Hipkins, R. (2012). The engaging nature of teaching for
ing what we covered here in the attitudinal com- competency development. In S. L. Christenson, A. L.
petencies) and learning opportunities. Material Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on
would be collected on activities and values student engagement (pp. 441–456). New York: Springer.
Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. S.
beyond school, and relations with family and (2008). School engagement trajectories and their dif-
friends, to deepen our understanding of the cir- ferential predictive relations to dropout. Journal of
cumstances in which learning identities become Social Issues, 64(1), 21–40.
consistent in positive, or negative ways, within Janosz, M., LeBlanc, M., Boulerice, B., & Tremblay, R.
E. (2000). Predicting types of school dropouts: A typo-
and beyond school, and of the ways in which logical approach with two longitudinal samples.
these constituents of individual identity and Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, 733–759.
source of action can be combined differently to Maehr, M., & Meyer, H. (1997). Understanding motiva-
markedly and durably alter declining or low stu- tion and schooling: Where we’ve been, where we are,
and where we need to go. Educational Psychology
dent engagement in learning. Longitudinal analy- Review 9, 371–409.
sis shows that while earlier patterns of student New Zealand Council for Educational Research. (1984).
engagement contribute to later patterns, student Standard progressive matrices: New Zealand norms
engagement levels are not immutable and do supplement. Wellington, New Zealand: NZCER.
Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s progressive matrices:
respond to different opportunities. Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive
Psychology, 41, 1–48.
Rumberger, R. A., & Rotermund, S. (2011). The relation-
ship between engagement and high school dropout. In
References S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
Handbook of research on student engagement. New
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. York: Springer.
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- Rychen, D. S. Tiana, A., & Ferrer, A. T. (Eds.), (2004).
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Key competencies for all: An overarching conceptual
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. frame of reference. In Developing key competencies in
Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. education: Some lessons for international and national
(2009). Adolescent behavioral, affective, and cogni- experience (pp. 5–34). Paris: UNESCO International
tive engagement in school: Relationship to dropout. Bureau of Education.
Journal of School Health, 79, 408–415. Sartre, J. P. (1965). The philosophy of Jean-Paul Sartre.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human devel- Edited and introduced by R. Denoon Cumming. New
opment: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, York: Random House.
MA: Harvard University Press. Shiner, R., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in
Chall, J. S. (1996). Stages of reading development (2nd childhood and adolescence: Measurement, develop-
ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. ment, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology
Craib, I. (1976). Existentialism and sociology: A study of and Psychiatry, 44, 2–32.
Jean-Paul Sartre. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA:
University Press. Harvard University Press.
28 Trajectories and Patterns of Student Engagement: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study 599

Wylie, C., with Ferral, H. (2005). Patterns of cognitive and Wylie, C., & Hodgen, E. (2007). The continuing contribu-
personality development: Evidence from the longitudi- tion of early childhood education to young people’s
nal Competent Children/Learners study. In J. Low & competency levels. Wellington, New Zealand: Ministry
P. Jose (Eds.), Lifespan development – New Zealand per- of Education.
spectives (pp. 90–100). Auckland: Pearson Education. Wylie, C., & Hodgen, E. (2011). Forming adulthood. Past,
Wylie, C., & Hipkins, R. (2006). Growing independence: present and future in the experiences and views of the
Competent learners @ 14. Wellington, New Zealand: competent learners @ 20. Wellington, New Zealand:
Ministry of Education. Ministry of Education.
Wylie, C., Hipkins, R., & Hodgen, E. (2008). On the edge Yazzie-Mintz, E. (2009). Engaging the voice of students:
of adulthood: Young people’s school and out-of-school A report on the 2007 & 2008 high school survey of
experiences at 16. Wellington, New Zealand: Ministry student engagement. http://www.indiana.edu/~ceep/
of Education. hssse/images/HSSSE_2009_Report.pdf.
Instructional Contexts
for Engagement and Achievement 29
in Reading*

John T. Guthrie, Allan Wigfield, and Wei You

Abstract
In this chapter, we review research on students’ engagement in reading
activities and how classroom instructional practices influence engagement
in reading and other academic activities. We define engaged readers as
motivated to read, strategic in their approaches to reading, knowledgeable
in their construction of meaning from text, and socially interactive while
reading. We present a conceptual model of reading engagement linking
classroom practices directly and indirectly to students’ motivation to read,
behavioral engagement in reading, and reading achievement. A major
premise of this model is that behavioral engagement in reading mediates
the effects of classroom practices on reading outcomes. We present evi-
dence from a variety of experimental and correlational studies documenting
the direct and indirect links among classroom practices, motivation, behav-
ioral engagement, and achievement outcomes. One reading comprehension
instructional program on which we focus is Concept-Oriented Reading
Instruction. This program integrates strategy instruction and instructional
practices to foster students’ reading motivation, and teaches reading, in
particular, in content domains such as science and social studies.

*The project described was supported by Grant Number Defining Engagement


R01HD052590 from the National Institute of Child and
Human Development. The content is solely the responsi-
bility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the The construct of engagement is increasingly
official views of the National Institute of Child Health and prominent in the educational and developmental
Human Development or the National Institutes of Health. psychology literatures and is defined generally as
J.T. Guthrie (*) • A. Wigfield involvement, participation, and commitment to
Department of Human Development some set of activities. Skinner, Kindermann,
and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland,
Connell, and Wellborn (2009a) described engage-
College Park, MD, USA
e-mail: jguthrie@umd.edu; awigfiel@umd.edu ment as a reflection or manifestation of motivated
action and noted that action incorporates emo-
W. You
Department of Curriculum & Instruction, tions, attention, goals, and other psychological
University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA processes along with persistent and effortful
e-mail: weiyou@umd.edu behavior. Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and Paris

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 601


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_29, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
602 J.T. Guthrie et al.

(2004) defined behavioral, emotional, and cogni- umenting this point throughout the chapter. By
tive aspects of school engagement. Behavioral focusing on reading, we address an urgent prob-
engagement is direct involvement in a set of lem in education which is that high proportions
activities and includes positive conduct, effort of students are disaffected with reading. They
and persistence, and participation in extracurric- overwhelmingly shun books in science, history,
ular activities. Emotional engagement covers and math that carry the substance of their edu-
both positive and negative affective reactions cation. In other words, in elementary and sec-
(e.g., interest, boredom, anxiety, frustration) to ondary education, disengagement from reading
activities, as well as to the individuals with whom is a national dilemma (Grigg, Ryan, Jin, &
one does the activities (teachers, peers). It also Campbell, 2003; Perie, Grigg, & Donahue,
comprises identification with school. Cognitive 2005).
engagement means willingness to exert the men- Engagement and motivation are related terms
tal effort needed to comprehend challenging con- that sometimes are used interchangeably in the
cepts and accomplish difficult tasks in different literature (e.g., National Research Council, 2004),
domains, as well as the use of self-regulatory and but we believe the constructs should be distin-
other strategies to guide one’s cognitive efforts. guished from one another (see also Fredricks
We have focused on students’ engagement et al., 2004; Skinner et al., 2009a, 2009b for dis-
in reading activities and defined reading tinctions between these constructs). As just noted,
engagement as interacting with text in ways engagement is a multidimensional construct that
that are both strategic and motivated (Guthrie includes behavioral, cognitive, and affective attri-
& Wigfield, 2000). More broadly, we and our butes associated with being deeply involved in an
colleagues have described engaged readers as activity such as reading; indeed, Fredricks et al.
motivated to read, strategic in their approaches (2004) called engagement a meta-construct. By
to comprehending what they read, knowledge- contrast, motivation is a more specific construct
able in their construction of meaning from that relates to engagement but can be distin-
text, and socially interactive while reading guished from it. Motivation is what energizes
(Guthrie, McGough, Bennett, & Rice, 1996; and directs behavior and often is defined with
Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000; Guthrie, Wigfield, respect to the beliefs, values, and goals individu-
& Perencevich, 2004; see also Baker, Dreher, als have for different activities (Eccles &
& Guthrie, 2000). In this review, we introduce Wigfield, 2002; Wigfield, Eccles, Schiefele,
the construct of behavioral engagement to this Roeser, & Davis-Kean, 2006).
set of engagement processes. Specific indica- Motivation often is domain specific; in the
tors of behavioral engagement of reading reading domain, we defined reading motivation
include students’ report of effort and persistence as follows: “Reading motivation is the individu-
(Skinner, Kindermann, & Furrer, 2009b), stu- al’s personal goals, values, and beliefs with regard
dents’ report of time spent reading (Guthrie, to the topics, processes, and outcomes of read-
Wigfield, Metsala, & Cox, 1999), and teachers’ ing” (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000, p. 405).
observations of students’ reading behaviors Motivation also is important for the maintenance
(Wigfield et al., 2008). of behavior, particularly when activities are cog-
Students’ engagement in reading is enhanced nitively demanding (Wolters, 2003). Reading is
when the contexts in which reading occurs fos- one such activity, as many different cognitive
ter it. There are a variety of instructional prac- skills are involved. These range from processing
tices that foster students’ reading engagement, individual words to generating meaning from
and we discuss them below. We believe that complex texts. Furthermore, although reading is
engagement in reading is crucial to the devel- required for many school tasks and activities, it is
opment of reading comprehension skills and also something students can choose to do or not;
reading achievement; we present evidence doc- “Am I going to read or do something else?” Given
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 603

these characteristics, motivation is especially areas (Guthrie, Wigfield, & Perencevich, 2004;
crucial to reading engagement. Like Skinner et al. Guthrie et al., 1996). As the instructional prac-
(2009a, 2009b), then, we believe that engage- tices have been described fully elsewhere (e.g.,
ment reflects motivated action. When students Guthrie, McRae, & Klauda, 2007), we briefly
are positively motivated to read, they will be mention them here and present an example lesson
more engaged in reading. We discuss how spe- later in this chapter.
cific aspects of motivation relate to engagement
later in this chapter. Learning and knowledge goals refer to core
learning goals for particular topic areas that pro-
vide students with compelling cognitive reasons
Engagement Perspective on Reading for learning the material.

We (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000) developed an Real-world interactions are connections between
engagement perspective on reading that connects the academic curriculum and the personal experi-
classroom instructional practices to students’ ences of the learners and, more specifically, are
motivations, strategy use, conceptual knowledge, stimulating activities that connect students to the
and social interactions, and ultimately to their content they are learning. These real-world inter-
reading outcomes. Students’ motivation includes actions also provide motivation for students to
multifaceted aspects such as goals, intrinsic and read more about what they are learning.
extrinsic motivation, values, self-efficacy, and
social motivation. These motivational aspects of Instructional Practices
the reader propel students to choose to read and • Autonomy support is based on premises from
to use cognitive strategies to comprehend. The self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci,
strategies in the model refer to students’ multiple 2009) that giving students some control over
cognitive processes of comprehending, self- their own learning is motivating.
monitoring, and constructing their understanding • Interesting texts refers to the practice of pro-
and beliefs during reading. Conceptual knowl- viding an abundance of high interest texts in
edge refers to the notion that reading is knowl- the classroom.
edge-driven. Social interactions include • Strategy instruction concerns the kinds of
collaborative practices in a community and the reading strategies teachers teach; in CORI, a
social goals of helping other students or cooper- set of strategies shown to have strong empiri-
ating with a teacher. These in turn influence stu- cal support (National Reading Panel, 2000)
dents’ reading achievement, knowledge gained are the strategies used in the program.
from reading, and the kinds of practices in which • Collaboration is the social discourse among
they engage. students in a learning community that enables
We chose the instructional practices in the them to see perspectives and to socially con-
model for two primary reasons. First, each prac- struct knowledge from text (Johnson &
tice has been shown to relate to students’ motiva- Johnson, 2009).
tion and achievement in a variety of correlational • Praise and rewards involve the ways in which
and classroom-based studies (see Guthrie & teachers provide feedback to students (Brophy,
Humenick, 2004, for a meta-analytic review 1981). Rewards are often used in reading
of the work on a number of these practices). instruction and other instructional programs
Second, several of the practices are included in as a way to build students’ motivation
Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI), a (Gambrell & Marniak, 1997).
reading comprehension instruction program that • Students are evaluated in classrooms in a myr-
combines reading strategy instruction, support iad of ways. Some methods of evaluation can
for student motivation, and connections to content provide meaningful information about student
604 J.T. Guthrie et al.

learning and actually can support student this framework is to describe how instruction,
motivation (Afflerbach, 1998). motivation, behavioral engagement, and achieve-
• Finally, teacher involvement represents the ment are related. The classroom practices and
teacher’s knowledge of individual learners, conditions in the box at the left of the figure
caring about their progress and pedagogical include many of those incorporated in Guthrie
understanding of how to foster their active and Wigfield’s (2000) engagement model of read-
participation (Skinner & Belmont, 1993). ing development, and others that are particularly
Guthrie and Wigfield (2000) reviewed evi- relevant to middle school reading, the focus of
dence for the connections of each of these prac- our current research project on enhancing adoles-
tices to reading outcomes. cents’ engagement in reading (Guthrie, Klauda,
A crucial assumption in this model is that the & Morrison, 2012; Guthrie, Mason-Singh, &
effects of instructional practices on the student Coddington, 2012). Our framework is consistent
outcomes of achievement, knowledge, and read- with the perspective of Appleton, Christenson,
ing practices are mediated by the engagement and Furlong (2008) in that we seek the character-
processes (see also Skinner & Belmont, 1993). istics of classrooms that are sufficiently powerful
That is, classroom contexts only affect student to impact variables for which educators are held
outcomes to the extent that they produce high accountable, such as achievement on major tests
levels of student engagement. Behavioral engage- as well as experimental measures. Furthermore,
ment is one of these processes and is increased by we attempt to identify and document the engage-
CORI. ment processes that serve as links between the
practices of teachers and students’ outcomes.
Depicted in this graphic are the engagement
Conceptual Framework processes in reading consisting of motivations to
for Engagement Processes in Reading read, behavioral engagement in reading, and
reading competence, along with classroom con-
Purposes of the Framework texts. Many of the studies we review in this chap-
ter show that behavioral engagement in reading
Figure 29.1 presents our current framework on impacts reading competence, and motivations to
engagement that depicts both the direct and indirect read impact behavioral engagement in reading.
(or mediated) effects of classroom practices and Our rationale for considering these all to be
conditions on student reading outcomes, particularly engagement processes is that they represent moti-
their reading competence. Our aim in building vational, behavioral, and cognitive dimensions of

Direct Effects of Practices on Behaviors Direct Effects of Motivations on Competence

B C
Behavioral
Classroom
Motivations Engagement Reading
Practice and D E F
to Read in Reading Competence
Conditions
(Dedication)

Engagement Processes in Reading A

Direct Effects of Practices on Competence

Fig. 29.1 Model of reading engagement processes within classroom contexts


29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 605

interacting with text. On the far left of the graphic, 2007). As facilitators of reading competence,
classroom practices and conditions are shown to these processes may be termed “cognitive engage-
represent their role in impacting motivations, ment.” These processes demand effort and atten-
behavioral engagement, and reading competence. tion sustained over substantial amounts of time
We will review empirical evidence documenting during which this cognitive system is acquired to
that classroom practices have both direct effects a level of expertise (Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-
on competence and indirect effects mediated by Romer, 1993).
motivations and behavioral engagement. In this While students are expending effort in the
graphic, the capital letters represent pathways behavior of reading, motivational processes are
from classroom practices through engagement occurring simultaneously. If the book is interest-
processes to reading competence. Although ing, the reading act may be intrinsically motivat-
empirical research is likely to reveal reciprocal ing. If the book is perceived as important, the
pathways throughout this model, we do not reading behavior may contribute to a student’s
include them here because they have not been sense of identification with reading in school. It
widely studied in reading and the current evi- is during this passage of time that motivations
dence for them is limited. We do not address impact students’ cognitive proficiencies. When
emotional engagement or affective processes motivations are positive (intrinsic motivation),
because they have not been studied frequently in cognitions increase; when motivations are nega-
reading, and space constraints prevent us from tive (avoidance or disaffection), behaviors
examining them here. We do address cognitive become aversive, leading to a gradual decline in
engagement, such as the use of strategies for cognitive proficiency. It is evident that cognitive
reading, because it is important to control for expertise cannot be attained without sustained
them statistically when investigating the associa- behaviors, and the absence of reading behaviors
tions of behavioral engagement and reading is a precursor to cognitive decline.
achievement. This review of context effects is
organized by reviewing evidence for each path-
way, beginning with the effects of behavioral Evidence for the Effects of Behavioral
engagement on reading competence, and then Engagement on Competence in Reading
discussing each pathway in the model. In several
instances, a particular study provides evidence In this section, we document that time, effort, and
for more than one pathway in the model. We dis- persistence in reading behaviors impacts a variety
cuss these studies in each pathway where it sup- of indicators of reading competence. The studies
plies documentation. for this section are presented in Table 29.1.
However, this documentation cannot be simple.
A student who spends a high amount of time
Behavioral Engagement Impacts reading and also has high competence, according
Reading Competence to standardized test scores, is likely to have a vari-
ety of correlated characteristics. Most basically,
Our rationale for linking behavioral engagement this student is likely to have high amounts of
in reading and reading competence (Path F) is background knowledge about the topic or genre
grounded in cognitive science (van den Broek, of the reading behavior. The relationship of
Rapp, & Kendeou, 2005; Walczyk et al., 2007). behavioral engagement and achievement is con-
Experimental studies show that acquiring declar- founded by many variables. For example, a
ative knowledge from text demands the complex behaviorally engaged student with high reading
system of rapid, automatic processes at the word competence is likely to have high levels of motiva-
and sentence level integrated with effortful, tion, such as self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation
deliberate processes of inferencing and reasoning for reading, and so these variables must be taken
(Kendeou, van den Broek, White, & Lynch, into account in analyses. Guthrie et al. (1999)
Table 29.1 Effects of behavioral engagement on competence in reading
606

Citationa Number of participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Duckworth 1,545 Adults aged NA 1. Grit (a) Age h2 = 0.05*** for educational attainment
et al. (2007a) 25 and older (b) Educational attainment
Duckworth 690 Adults aged NA 1. Grit (a) Age h2 = 0.05*** for educational attainment
et al. (2007b) 25 and older (b) Educational attainment
Duckworth 139 Undergraduate NA 1. GPA (a) Grit r1a = 0.34*** after SAT scores were held
et al. (2007c) students (b) SAT constant
Duckworth 1,218 Freshman in the NA 1. Summer retention (a) Grit b1a = 0.44**d after self-control and Whole
et al. (2007d) US Military Academy, 2. First-year academic GPA (b) Self-control Candidate Score were held constant, partial
West Point 3. Military performance score (c) Whole Candidate Score r2a = −0.01, ns; partial r3a = 0.09**
(MPS)
Duckworth 1,308 Freshman in the NA 1. Summer retention (a) Grit b1a = 0.39*e
et al. (2007e) US Military Academy, (b) Conscientiousness
West Point (c) Whole Candidate Score
Duckworth 175 National Spelling NA 1. Final round (a) Grit b1a = 0.62**f
et al. (2007f) Bee finalists from 7 to 2. Prior competition (b) Self-control b3a = 0.28***g
15 years old 3. Study time (c) Verbal IQ Score b1d = 0.30***h
(d) Study time
(e) Prior competition Study time was a mediator between grit and
performance
b2a = 0.48*i
b1e = 1.21***j
Prior final competition was a mediator
between grit and performance
Duckworth and 140 8 1. Final GPA (a) Self-discipline r1a = 0.55***
Seligman (2005a) (b) First-making-period GPA R2 = 0.85***k
b1a = 0.10*
Duckworth and 164 8 1. Final GPA (a) Self-discipline r1a = 0.67***
Seligman (2005b) (b) First-making-period GPA R2 = 0.90***l
(c) IQ
b1a = 0.08*
Fredricks Review
et al. (2004)
Guthrie, Klauda, 1,200 7 1. Reading achievement (a) Amount of school reading r1a = 0.20** with FARMS status partialed out
et al. (2012) 2. Avoidance of reading (b) Amount of out-of-school reading r1b = 0.19** with FARMS status partialed out
information books for (c) FARMS status r1d = 0.04 (ns) with FARMS status partialed
school (d) Amount of information book reading out
(e) Devaluing b2e = 0.75***
(f) Perceived difficulty
J.T. Guthrie et al.

b2f = 0.14***
Citationa Number of participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc 29
Guthrie 271 3, 5 1. Passage comprehension (a) Past achievement ΔR2 = 0.015m for reading amount
et al. (1999a) 2. Conceptual learning from (b) Prior knowledge b1e = 0.126*
multiple texts (c) Motivation ΔR2 = 0.022m for reading amount
(d) Self-efficacy b2e = 0.150*
(e) Reading amount
Guthrie 17,424 10 1. Text comprehension (a) Reading amount b1a = 0.12***
et al. (1999b) (b) Past reading comprehension r1a = 0.29***
achievement ΔR2 = 0.014m for reading amount
(c) Reading efficacy
(d) Reading motivation
(e) SES
Jang (2008) 136 College students NA 1. Conceptual learning (a) Rationale Model 1: b13 = 0.45*n R2 = 0.20
2. Identified Regulation Model 2: b13 = 0.44* R2 = 0.20
3. Engagement Model 3: b13 = 0.44* R2 = 0.20
4. Interest regulation
Ladd and 383 Children Ages 1. Initial levels of achievement (a) Changes in school liking- b2b = 0.31**o
Dinella (2009) 5.5–13.5 (achievement intercepts) avoidance from first through b2d = 0.37**
2. Growth in scholastic third grade
achievement from first through (b) Changes in cooperative-resistant
eighth grade (achievement classroom participation from first
slopes) through third grade
(c) Average school liking-avoidance
from first through third grade
(d) Average cooperative-resistant
classroom participation from first
through third grade
Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading

Orvis 274 College 1. Learning (a) Off-task attention b1a = −0.15*p


et al. (2009) students (b) Training satisfaction
(c) Training motivation
(d) Goal orientation
(e) Learner control
Reeve 141 College 1. Identification experience (a) Reason to try b1a = 0.26**
et al. (2002a) students (perceived importance and b21 = 0.59**
perceived self-determination
as the indicator variables)
2. Effort
Reeve 70 College 1. Identification experience (a) Reason to try b1a = 0.34**
et al. (2002b) students 2. Pre-lesson identified b21 = 0.54**
regulation b13 = 0.45**
3. Effort
607

(continued)
Table 29.1 (continued)
608

Citationa Number of participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Salamonson 126 Second-year NA 1. Academic performance in (a) Overall homework completion R2 = 0.381q
et al. (2009) nursing students pathophysiology (b) Overall lecture attendance b1a = 0.44***
(c) Hours spent in part-time b1b = 0.21*
employment b1c = −0.26***
Schwinger 231 11, 12 1. GPA (a) Motivational regulation strategies b1b = 0.29**r
et al. (2009) (b) Effort management R2 = 0.11
(c) Intelligence
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Notes
a
We only included the studies documenting path F in Fig. 29.1
b
We did not include those variables which the authors reported as covariates or as control variables in their analyses
c
We only reported the effect sizes of behavioral engagement on outcomes of our interest
d
The effect size reported in this study is the standardized beta coefficient in the final model in which three predictors (grit, self-control, and Whole Candidate Score) were simul-
taneously entered to predict summer retention
e
The effect size reported in this study is the standardized beta coefficient in the final model in which three predictors (grit, conscientiousness, and Whole Candidate Score) were
simultaneously entered to predict summer retention
f
The effect size reported here is the beta coefficient in the model in which three predictors (grit, self-control, and age) were simultaneously entered to predict final round
g
The effect size reported here is the beta coefficient in the model in which two predictors (grit and age) were simultaneously entered to predict study time
h
The effect size reported here is the beta coefficient in the model in which three predictors (grit, study time, and age) were simultaneously entered to predict final round
i
The effect size reported here is the beta coefficient in the model in which two predictors (grit and age) were simultaneously entered to predict prior competition
j
The effect size reported here is the beta coefficient in the model in which three predictors (grit, number of prior competition, and age) were simultaneously entered to predict 2005
final round
k
The effect size reported here is R2 for the overall regression with two predictors (self-discipline and first-making-period GPA)
l
The effect size reported here is R2 for the overall regression with four predictors (self-discipline, IQ, April achievement test scores, and first-making-period GPA)
m
The effect size reported here ΔR2 was the unique contribution of reading amount to the outcome above and beyond the other predictors in the model
n
The effect sizes reported in this study are standardized coefficients in structural equation modeling
o
The effect sizes reported in this study are path coefficients in path analysis
p
The effect sizes reported in this study are standardized path coefficients in structural equation modeling
q
The effect sizes reported in this study is R2 for the overall regression with seven predictors (overall homework completion, overall lecture attendance, hours spend in part-time
employment, study time on pathophysiology, age, sex, and non-English speaking at home)
r
The effect sizes reported in this study is R2 of GPA predicted by effort management
J.T. Guthrie et al.
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 609

found that third- and fifth-grade students’ self- tion in classwork. Other students showed behav-
report of amount of time spent reading in school ioral disengagement consisting of resistance by
and out of school was associated with competency not performing tasks, not completing homework,
tests of students’ reading comprehension, even and acting defiantly toward academic activities.
when controlling for background knowledge, Statistically controlling for reading achievement
previous grades, intrinsic motivation, and self- in grade 1, the gain in reading from grades 1 to 8
efficacy. This finding appeared not only for single was higher for students whose behavioral engage-
passages but for acquisition of knowledge from ment increased in grades 1–3 than for students
text in a 2-day learning activity. They also found whose resistance and behavioral disengagement
in a nationally representative sample of tenth- increased in grades 1–3. In other words, increas-
grade students that behavioral engagement in ing behavioral engagement produced the positive
reading (assessed by time spent) was correlated slope for achievement, whereas decreasing
with reading comprehension test scores, when the behavioral engagement produced a less positive
potentially confounding variables of past achieve- slope in measured reading competence.
ment, SES, and self-efficacy were controlled sta- A variety of studies document the generalizabil-
tistically. Thus, behavioral engagement impacted ity of this effect of behavioral engagement in read-
reading competence for samples of elementary ing on reading competence, using different
and secondary students, when potentially con- indicators of engagement. Schwinger, Steinmayr,
founding cognitive and motivational variables and Spinath (2009) measured 11th- and 12th-grade
were statistically controlled. German high school students’ effort management
As indicated previously, students’ aversion to as an indicator of behavioral engagement.
reading information texts in secondary school is a Investigators used items such as “I study hard
widespread crisis. In middle school, the highest whether I am interested or not.” Such behavioral
achievers overwhelmingly rate information text engagement predicted students’ GPAs, although
to be uninteresting (Guthrie, Klauda, et al., 2012). intelligence also predicted GPA and the behavioral
In this light, it is valuable to understand the vari- engagement effect was not statistically controlled.
ables that influence students’ competence in read- Salamonson, Andrew, and Everett (2009) used
ing uninteresting text (Reeve, Jang, Hardre, & homework completion in a nursing program, which
Omura, 2002). In one experimental study, Jang consisted of textbook reading, as an indicator of
(2008) gave one group of college students the behavioral engagement; this variable likely reflected
task of reading some text about statistical correla- time spent reading. Controlling for age and ethnic-
tions, which was not interesting to them, with the ity, this indicator was a positive predictor of aca-
rationale that the text was important to their pro- demic performance in a course on pathophysiology.
fessions. The behavioral engagement of this In an electronic learning environment, off-task
group increased compared to a group not told that attention was an indicator of behavioral disengage-
this material was beneficial to them. After reading ment from learning, which predicted subsequent
the texts, the group given the “importance ratio- posttest scores (Orvis, Fisher, & Wasserman, 2009).
nale” was superior in conceptual understanding Although statistical controls were often absent,
of the text. Thus, experimentally increasing these studies suggest that behavioral engagement is
behavioral engagement enhanced students’ con- a robust variable impacting competence for a vari-
ceptual learning. Note that this study also pro- ety of reading tasks at a variety of ages.
vides evidence for other pathways in our model; In related research, students’ reports of effort
we discuss this evidence later. In a longitudinal and perseverance have been referred to as self-
study with children ages 5–13, Ladd and Dinella discipline (Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews, &
(2009) examined the effect of behavioral engage- Kelly, 2007). In characterizing self-discipline,
ment on a wide variety of reading achievement they used items such as “I am a hard worker,” “I
tests. Some students showed high behavioral finish whatever I begin,” and “I have achieved a
engagement of interest, attention, and participa- goal that took years of work.” Using this measure
610 J.T. Guthrie et al.

in a series of six studies, the investigators found potentially confounding variables. For example,
that this measure of self-discipline correlated sig- both motivation and behavioral engagement are
nificantly with GPAs, even when SAT scores were likely to be correlated with achievement, as indi-
held constant for college students. Duckworth cated by test scores or grades, and declarative
and Seligman (2005) used a similar measure of knowledge of the world, which facilitates com-
self-discipline in a study of eighth-grade students prehension and is associated with motivation for
and found that it predicted GPA more strongly reading. As discussed earlier, Guthrie et al. (1999)
than did IQ. Furthermore, this indicator of behav- reported that intrinsic motivation predicted
ioral engagement predicted GPA when control- behavioral engagement measured by students’
ling for previous IQ scores. The authors concluded self-reported frequency and breadth of reading
that the duration and direction of effort predict the activities, even when students’ prior knowledge,
development of expertise more fully than indica- past achievement, and self-efficacy in reading
tors of talent or aptitude. One limitation of this were controlled. Thus, while controlling for self-
research was that the motivational sources of efficacy and the cognitive variables of back-
behavioral engagement were not explicitly inves- ground knowledge and school achievement,
tigated. However, such sources have been exam- intrinsic motivation was associated with behav-
ined in the engagement literature. ioral engagement in reading for both elementary
and secondary level students. These results
extended previous findings by Wigfield and
Motivations Impact Behavioral Guthrie (1997) that intrinsic motivation con-
Engagement in Reading structs such as challenge, curiosity, and involve-
ment correlated with students’ amount and
We turn next to a consideration of Path E, links of breadth of reading behaviors.
motivation with behavioral engagement. The Students’ behavioral engagement in reading,
studies for this section are presented in Table 29.2. according to their self-reported frequency and
Our argument in this section is that motivations breadth of reading activities, has further been
such as self-efficacy, intrinsic motivation, and associated with multiple motivations. Lau (2009)
valuing that are related to reading increase an found that 11–18-year olds’ intrinsic motivation
individual’s reading behaviors, that is, the effort, and social motivation each made unique contri-
attention, time spent, concentration, and long- butions to their amount of reading, although self-
term persistence in reading activities. As dis- efficacy and extrinsic motivation for reading,
cussed earlier, we distinguish motivations from which were also included in the model, did not
behavioral engagement because they are refer- make significant contributions. While this find-
ring to goals, values, beliefs, and dispositions ing appeared for younger secondary students,
rather than physical behaviors (Wigfield & only intrinsic motivation uniquely contributed to
Guthrie, 2010). In the literature that relates moti- amount of reading when the other motivational
vation to reading, motivational constructs have constructs were controlled in the model for older
been drawn from four theoretical perspectives secondary students. Thus, intrinsic motivation,
including expectancy value theory (Wigfield & which was measured as enjoying reading,
Eccles, 2002), social cognitive theory (Bandura appeared to predominate as a predictor of behav-
& Schunk, 1981), goal theory (Maehr & Zusho, ioral engagement in reading for both age groups
2009), and self-determination theory (Ryan & when several other motivational constructs were
Deci, 2000). Relations of key constructs from statistically controlled.
these theories to reading are portrayed in Guthrie As discussed earlier, Jang (2008) found that
and Coddington (2009). when college students were required to perform
In attempting to characterize a relationship the aversive task of reading uninteresting material,
between motivations and behavioral engagement the extent to which they valued the content of the
in reading, it is beneficial to consider controlling text determined the extent of their behavioral
Table 29.2 Effects of motivations on behavioral engagement and on reading competence
29

Citationa Number of participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Anmarkrud and 104 9 1. Reading comprehension (a) Topic knowledge r1e = 0.46***
Bräten (2009) (b) Deeper strategies b1e = 0.24**
(c) Surface strategies ΔR2 = 0.06** for reading efficacy and
(d) Reading efficacy reading task value on reading
(e) Reading task value comprehensiond
(f) Previous semester social
studies grades
Baker and 371 5, 6 1. Students’ reports of their reading (a) Challenge r1a = 0.51*** r1b = 0.43***
Wigfield (1999) activity (b) Self-efficacy r1c = 0.43*** r1d = 0.51***
2. Gates-MacGinitie Reading (c) Curiosity r1e = 0.46***
Comprehension Test Scores (d) Involvement r2j = −0.26***
3. CTBS reading (e) Social r3j = −0.24***
4. Performance assessment (f) Importance r4j = −0.13*
(g) Recognition r4k = 0.21***
(h) Grades r4h = 0.14*
(i) Competition r4g = 0.14*
(j) Work avoidance
(k) Compliance
Chan (1994) 338 5, 7, 9 1. Reading comprehension (a) Perceived cognitive competence For grade 5, b1c = −0.32 b2a = 0.44e
2. Use of reading strategies (b) Belief in personal control For grade 7, b1b = 0.24 b2a = 0.20
3. Knowledge of reading strategies (c) Learned helplessness For grade 9, b2a = 0.22 b2b = 0.29
Durik et al. (2006) 606 4, 10 1. Time per week spent reading (a) Tenth-grade self-concept of b1a = 0.22f
for pleasure in 10th grade ability b2a = 0.18
2. Number of language arts courses (b) Tenth-grade importance
Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading

b2d = 0.13
per year of high school (c) Tenth-grade intrinsic value b2b = 0.19
3. Reading relatedness of (d) Fourth-grade self-concept of b2e = 0.23
12th-grade career aspirations ability b1c = 0.16
(e) Fourth-grade importance b2c = 0.18
(f) Fourth-grade intrinsic value b1f = 0.18
(g) Third-grade reading grade
(h) Eighth-grade English grade
Greene et al. (2004) 220 High school 1. Percentage of course points in (a) Self-efficacy r1a = 0.47**
students the English class, which is a (b) Perceived instrumentality r1b = 0.245**
combination of exams, projects, (c) Autonomy support r1c = 0.24**
and homework assignments (d) Motivating tasks r1d = 0.20**
Guthrie, Klauda, 1,200 7 1. Information text comprehension (a) Perceived difficulty r1a = −0.22**
et al. (2012) (b) Self-efficacy r1b = 0.18**
(continued)
611
Table 29.2 (continued)
612

Citationa Number of participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Guthrie 271 3, 5 1. Passage comprehension (a) Past achievement b3c = 0.383*** ΔR2 = 0.138g
et al. (1999a) 2. Conceptual learning from (b) Prior knowledge b3f = 0.333*** ΔR2 = 0.107
multiple texts (c) Reading motivation b3g = 0.364*** ΔR2 = 0.124
3. Reading amount (d) Self-efficacy
(e) Reading amount
(f) Intrinsic motivation
(g) Extrinsic motivation
Guthrie 17,424 10 1. Text comprehension (a) Reading amount r2d = 0.29****
et al. (1999b) 2. Reading amount (b) Past reading comprehension r1d = 0.59****
achievement b1d = 0.419*** ΔR2 = 0.229g
(c) Reading efficacy b2d = 0.266*** ΔR2 = 0.083
(d) Reading motivation
(e) SES
Guthrie, Wigfield, 361 3 1. Multiple text comprehension (a) Motivation composite, r1a = 0.76*
Barbosa, 2. Passage comprehension including self-efficacy, r2a = 0.82**
et al. (2004a) challenge, involvement,
and curiosity
Guthrie, Wigfield, 524 3 1. Gates-MacGinitie Reading (a) Intrinsic motivation r1a = 0.88** r1b = 0.81**
Barbosa, Comprehension Test (b) Self-efficacy r2a = 0.75** r2b = 0.75**
et al. (2004b) 2. Passage comprehension (c) Extrinsic motivation
Guthrie, Hoa, 31 4 1. Gates-MacGinitie Reading (a) Interest ΔR2 = 0.12 for interesth
et al. (2007) Comprehension Test (b) Choice ΔR2 = 0.22 for choice
2. Multiple text comprehension (c) Involvement ΔR2 = 0.12 for involvement
(d) Efficacy ΔR2 = 0.03 for efficacy
(e) Social motivation ΔR2 = 0.06 for social motivation
Jang (2008) 136 College students NA 1. Conceptual learning (a) Rationale Model 1: b32 = 0.33*iR2 = 0.19
2. Identified regulation Model 2: b34 = 0.25* b3a = 0.22*
3. Engagement R2 = 0.16
4. Interest regulation Model 3: b32 = 0.32* R2 = 0.19
Lau (2009) 1,146 11–18 years 1. Reading amount (a) Perception of reading b3a = 0.49 b5a = 0.55
of age 2. Self-efficacy instruction b2a = 0.44 b4a = 0.46
3. Intrinsic motivation b13 = 0.28 b15 = 0.26
4. Extrinsic motivation
5. Social motivation
Legault et al. 351 12–18 years 1. Academic amotivation (a) Value of taskj rab = 0.36***
(2006a) of age (b) Ability beliefsj rac = 0.66***
(c) Task characteristicsj rad = 0.51***
(d) Effort beliefsj rbc = 0.30***
rbd = 0.55***
rcd = 0.64***
J.T. Guthrie et al.
Citationa Number of participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
349 12–18 years 1. Academic performance (a) Value of taskj
29
Legault r1a = −0.12*
et al. (2006b) of age 2. Time spent studying (b) Ability beliefsj r2a = −0.33***
3. Intention to drop out (c) Task characteristicsj r3a = 0.46***
(d) Effort beliefsj r1b = −0.42***
r2b = −0.18**
r3b = 0.36***
r1d = −0.15**
r2d = −0.23***
Legault 741 12–19 years 1. Academic performance (a) Value of taskj b1b = −0.39*k
et al. (2006c) of age 2. Problem behaviors (b) Ability beliefsj b1d = −0.34*
3. Academic self-esteem (c) Task characteristicsj b2a = 0.21*
4. Intention to drop out (d) Effort beliefsj b2d = 0.38*
b3b = −0.65*
b4a = 0.49***
b4b = 0.28*
Morgan and Review
Fuchs (2007)
Nolen (1988) 50 8 1. Use of deep processing strategies (a) General task orientation b1a = 0.31**l
(b) Task-specific task involvement b1b = 0.39***
(c) Perceived value of deep process- b1c = 0.18
ing strategy bac = 0.40***
(d) Perceived ability bab = 0.28*
(e) Science grade
Pintrich and 173 7 1. Cognitive strategy use (a) Self-efficacy r1b = 0.63***
de Groot (1990) 2. Self-regulation (b) Intrinsic value r2b = 0.73***
3. Student performance on seatwork (c) Test anxiety r1a = 0.33***
Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading

4. Student performance on exams/ r2a = 0.44***


quizzes Partial r23 = 0.18*
5. Student performance on essays/reports Partial r24 = 0.26***
6. Student performance on average grade Partial r25 = 0.22***
for the course Partial r6a = 0.18*
Partial r62 = 0.22**
Rapp and van Review
den Broek (2005)
Schiefele (1999) Review
Schunk and Review
Zimmerman (2007)
Strambler and 111 1–5 1. 2004–2005 Language arts score. (a) Academic valuing b1b = −0.41*m
Weinstein (2010) 2. 2004–2005 Math score (b) Academic devaluing b2b = −0.45*
(c) Alternative ID
613

(continued)
Table 29.2 (continued)
614

Citationa Number of participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Wang and 187 US students 4 1. Text comprehension (a) Intrinsic motivation b1a = 0.64n b1b = −0.57
Guthrie (2004) and 197 Chinese 2. Enjoyment reading amount (curiosity, involvement, and b2a = 0.85 b2b = −0.44
students 3. School reading amount preference for challenge) b3a = 0.26 b3b = 0.04
(b) Extrinsic motivation
(recognition, grades, social,
competition, and compliance)
Wigfield and 105 4, 5 1. Reading amount (a) Reading efficacy r1a = 0.36** r2a = 0.30**
Guthrie (1997) 2. Reading breadth (b) Curiosity r1b = 0.24* r2b = 0.22*
(c) Involvement r1c = 0.24* r2c = 0.35**
(d) Recognition r1d = 0.24* r2d = 0.25*
(e) Grades ΔR2 = 0.07 for extrinsic composite on
(f) Social children’s reading amounto
(g) Challenge ΔR2 = 0.15 for intrinsic composite on
(h) Intrinsic composite children’s breadth of readingp
(i) Extrinsic composite
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001
Notes
a
We only included the studies documenting paths E and C in Fig. 29.1
b
We did not include those variables which the authors reported as covariates or as control variables in their analyses
c
We only reported the effect sizes of motivations on behavioral engagement (path E) and motivations on reading competence (path C)
d
This ΔR2 was the unique contribution of reading efficacy and reading task value to reading comprehension above and beyond gender, prior achievement, topic knowledge, deeper
strategies, and surface strategies
e
All the effect sizes reported in this study are path coefficients in path analysis
f
All the beta coefficients reported in this study are standardized path coefficients in structural equation modeling
g
All the ΔR2 reported in this study are the unique contributions of the variable of interest to the outcome, after controlling for the other variables in the model
h
All the ΔR2 reported in this study are the unique contributions of the variables of interest to Gates-MacGinitie Reading Comprehension Test in December, after controlling for
prior reading achievement in September
i
All the beta coefficients reported in this study are standardized path coefficients in structural equation modeling
j
These variables are negatively coded in these studies, with high scores representing high levels in the four dimensions of academic amotivation and low scores representing low
levels in the four dimensions of academic amotivation
k
All the beta coefficients reported in this study are standardized path coefficients in structural equation modeling
l
All the effect sizes reported in this study are causal correlations obtained in path analysis
m
All the effect sizes reported in this study are standardized path coefficients in structural equation modeling
n
All the effect sizes reported in this study are the direct effects of the variables of interest on the outcomes, which were obtained from the decomposition of effects for structural
paths. Due to the space limit, we only reported the effects sizes for the US students
o
This ΔR2 was the unique contribution of spring extrinsic composite to 1993 reading amount above and beyond 1992 reading amount, fall intrinsic composite, fall extrinsic
composite, and spring intrinsic composite
p
This ΔR2 was the unique contribution of spring intrinsic composite to children’s spring reading breadth above and beyond their fall reading breadth, fall intrinsic composite, fall
extrinsic composite, and spring extrinsic composite
J.T. Guthrie et al.
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 615

engagement in the reading activity. Behavioral amount of behavioral engagement but also influ-
engagement was optimized for students who ence the quality of behavioral engagement by
showed identified regulation, which referred to activating cognitive strategies that are productive
believing that the text content was beneficial to for full comprehension of complex text.
their professional work. Students high on identi- Behavioral engagement in reading can be
fied regulation believed that the task was important expanded to choices that students make during
and worthwhile to them. In this context, identified school and leisure time. In a longitudinal study of
regulation (perceived value) contributed to behav- students from grades 3 to 12, Durik, Vida, and
ioral engagement, but interest in the text did not Eccles (2006) tracked the extent to which moti-
significantly contribute to behavioral engagement vational constructs influenced behavioral engage-
in reading the texts. Consequently, although intrin- ment in the form of selection of courses and the
sic motivation is consistently associated with pursuit of leisure reading. Behavioral engage-
behavioral engagement in academic reading tasks, ment was characterized by the number of lan-
when those reading tasks are inherently uninterest- guage arts classes students took per year,
ing, valuing the content for personal reasons other including composition, American literature,
than intrinsic motivation is likely to be associated speech, and humanities. In a longitudinal path
with behavioral engagement in reading. model, students’ valuing (ratings of importance
To this point, we have documented that intrin- for reading and language arts) in grade 4 pre-
sic motivation and valuing contribute to the dicted the behavioral engagement in terms of the
behavioral engagement in reading in terms of its number of courses selected in grade 10. Self-
quantity, such as amount of time spent, frequency efficacy in grade 4 predicted number of courses
of behavioral activities, and breadth of reading. In taken in grade 10, but intrinsic value for reading
addition, motivations are associated with the qual- in grade 4 did not predict number of courses
ity of behavioral engagement. To oversimplify a directly but was mediated by intrinsic valuing for
range of cognitive science phenomena (Rapp & reading in grade 10. In comparison, the behav-
van den Broek, 2005), reading can be deep or ioral engagement of leisure reading out of school
superficial. Deep processing strategies consist of was predicted by intrinsic motivation in grade 4,
making inferences, forming summaries, integrat- although leisure reading was not predicted by
ing diverse elements, and monitoring one’s com- valuing or self-efficacy in grade 4.
prehension during reading. Superficial strategies In summary, it is evident that motivations
are typified by underlining, memorizing, and (such as valuing) activated in a brief laboratory
seeking to complete tasks rather than compre- activity increased behavioral engagement in an
hending fully. Nolen (1988) investigated motiva- assigned reading task (Jang, 2008), and more sus-
tions that were associated with deep processing tained, wide-ranging intrinsic motivation for
strategies of eighth graders who were asked to reading in elementary grades predicted amount of
read expository passages. Intrinsic motivation for participation in reading intensive courses in high
learning (or the goal of understanding and learn- school (Durik et al., 2006). The linkage between
ing for its own sake) was positively associated motivation and behavioral engagement in reading
with the use of deep processing strategies for text appears to be viable within highly situated class-
comprehension. In contrast, ego orientation (the room contexts and across a broad sweep of time
goal of demonstrating high ability in comparison and place of reading in the schooling process.
to others) was positively related to use of surface-
level strategies only. This finding confirmed
reports of Pintrich and de Groot (1990) that intrin- Motivations Impact Reading
sic motivation for classwork in reading/language Competence
arts was associated with deep processing strate-
gies for text comprehension. Thus, it is evident Figure 29.1 includes a direct pathway from moti-
that motivational constructs not only increase the vations to read to reading competence. In the
616 J.T. Guthrie et al.

schematic, it is labeled Path C: Direct Effects of However, ratings of interestingness for a particular
Motivations on Competence. This refers to studies text are highly associated with the outcome of
that document the association between a variety rich conceptual understanding from reading.
of motivations and reading competence; the Although such deep understanding is highly cor-
studies relevant to this path are summarized in related with amount of background knowledge,
Table 29.2. Similar to other constructs in the Schiefele’s studies showed that interest has a
model, motivations and reading competence are unique contribution to reading competence after
both likely to be correlated with other variables background has been controlled either statisti-
(Chan, 1994). These variables need to be con- cally or experimentally. Although Schiefele’s
trolled to examine clearly the relations of motiva- studies were based on measures of self-reported
tion to reading competence, as Guthrie et al. interest in text, other investigators have deter-
(1999) did in their study showing that intrinsic mined interest through questionnaires and inter-
reading motivation predicted text comprehension views. In one interview study, students’ interests
more highly than SES, past achievement, reading were based on their positive affect toward texts,
amount, or self-efficacy, although the controlling topics in texts, authors, or series of books.
variables were all statistically significant. Reliable rubrics were used to gauge levels of
The potential mediation of the effect of moti- interest based on two 30-min interviews. With
vation on reading competence by behavioral this measure, fourth graders’ interest in reading
engagement was investigated by Jang (2008). In in September of the academic year predicted their
his classroom study with college students, he growth in reading comprehension from September
reported that valuing the content of the text to December. Interest in reading explained 12%
increased test scores reflecting reading compre- of the variance in reading comprehension in
hension. This effect was mediated by the amount December after September levels of reading
and quality of students’ behavioral engagement in comprehension were controlled. In addition, a
the reading activity; thus, valuing impacted read- person-centered profile analysis showed that stu-
ing competence through the activation of compe- dents who increased in motivation from
tence-relevant reading behaviors. Anmarkrud and September to December showed higher reading
Bräten (2009) examined how reading task value comprehension growth than students who did not
predicted ninth-grade students’ social studies increase in motivation during that time period. In
reading comprehension. Reading competence other words, not only does high interest in read-
was measured by a test of reading comprehension ing forecast comprehension growth, but an
containing inferential and literal items. Reading increase in the motivations of self-efficacy and
task value, which consisted of perceived impor- involvement in reading forecast reading growth
tance and utility of reading, predicted text com- as well (Guthrie, Hoa, et al., 2007).
prehension while controlling for the variables of Self-efficacy is argued to contribute to reading
gender, grades, topic knowledge, deep strategy competence through its effect on students’ self-
use, surface strategy use, and self-efficacy in regulation (Schunk & Zimmerman, 2007). These
reading. This shows that valuing was associated authors said that “Self-efficacy refers to learners’
with reading competence, even when multiple perceived capabilities for learning or performing
cognitive and motivational variables that may actions at designated levels, while self-regulation
have been present in the Jang study were statisti- refers to self-generated thoughts, feelings, and
cally controlled. actions that are systematically designed to affect
Interest in reading is a motivational construct one’s learning of knowledge and skills” (p. 7). In
that has frequently been associated with reading other words, self-efficacy is expected to influence
competence. In a review of the empirical litera- the quality of students’ behavioral engagement
ture, Schiefele (1999) observed that interest is with reading tasks, which will consequently have
akin to intrinsic motivation, but interest is more a positive influence on reading competence.
tightly tied to a particular text. Students rarely Consistent with this formulation, self-efficacy is
have an interest in all texts and all genres. correlated with reading comprehension in many
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 617

studies (Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Greene, Miller, dents in the later elementary grades. However,
Crowson, Duke, & Akey, 2004; Guthrie et al., this issue has not been fully examined for later
1999; Guthrie, Wigfield, Barbosa, et al. 2004). In elementary and secondary students or for special
addition, the highly related measure of perceived groups of lower or higher achievers.
difficulty in reading has been observed to corre-
late with reading competency measures in middle
school students. For example, for the total sample, Classroom Practices Impact Students’
perceived difficulty correlated −.22 with informa- Motivations
tion text comprehension, whereas self-efficacy
correlated .18 with the same variable when other We turn next to the direct path from classroom
motivations and gender were statistically con- practices to students’ motivation (Path D). The
trolled (Guthrie, Klauda, et al., 2012). studies for this section are presented in Table 29.3.
Another motivational variable associated with In this chapter, we characterize the classroom
reading competence is devaluing. Legault, Green- context in terms of teachers’ explicit teaching
Demers, and Pelletier (2006) defined devaluing activities and practices. A widely promoted and
as the rejection of importance or utility of aca- documented classroom practice that impacts stu-
demic work and disidentification with schooling. dents’ motivation is autonomy support (Green
Strambler and Weinstein (2010) studied devalu- et al., 2004; Reeve, Jang, Carrell, Jeon, & Barch,
ing of language arts among African American 2004; Zhou, Ma, & Deci, 2009). This construct,
and Hispanic students. Devaluing was character- based in self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci,
ized by questionnaire items such as the follow- 2009), refers to the instructor taking the students’
ing: “I don’t care about learning.” “I don’t care perspectives, acknowledging students’ feelings,
about getting a bad grade.” In a path analysis, and providing them with opportunities for choice
devaluing negatively predicted language arts test or self-direction. Such teaching minimizes the use
scores significantly at −.45 when positive valuing of controlling pressures and demands. Across a
and alternative identification (seeking to be pop- range of subjects including English, students who
ular, fashionable, cool) were statistically con- were afforded autonomy support by the teacher
trolled (Strambler & Weinstein, 2010). were more likely than other students to report
The relationship between motivations and placing a high value on reading (identified regula-
competence is almost certainly reciprocal. As tion) or intrinsically motivated reading (integrated
Morgan and Fuchs (2007) documented, when regulation). The identified student believes that
end of year achievement in reading is controlled school activities and materials such as books are
for beginning of year levels, motivations are important and useful, whereas the integrated stu-
associated with end of year performance. dent is intrinsically motivated to read, which
Simultaneously, when end of year motivations involves “doing an activity out of interest because
are controlled for beginning of year levels of it is rewarding in its own right” (Zhou et al., 2009,
motivation, reading comprehension is associated p. 492). Thus, autonomy support fosters valuing
with end of year motivation levels. In other and intrinsic motivation. In elementary school,
words, achievement predicts motivation growth autonomy support may assume the form of pro-
and motivation predicts achievement growth viding challenging and interesting texts for read-
simultaneously. These findings appear for moti- ing (Miller & Meece, 1999).
vational constructs of task orientation (interest in In our current study with middle school read-
reading), self-efficacy, and perceived difficulty. ers, we increasingly are focused on the teaching
By contrast, Guthrie, Hoa, et al. (2007) reported practice of relevance along with autonomy sup-
that while reading motivation levels (interest in port (Guthrie, Mason-Singh, et al., in press).
reading books for enjoyment) predicted reading Relevance means instructional activities that are
comprehension growth, reading comprehension related to students’ lives. Perceived relevance is
levels did not predict motivation growth for stu- associated with self-efficacy and social motivation
Table 29.3 Effects of classroom practices on motivations, behavioral engagement, and competence
618

Number of
Citationa participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Assor 862 3–8 1. Behavioral and cognitive (a) Intruding b1c = 0.25***d for grades 3–5
et al. (2002) engagement (b) Suppressing criticism R2 = 0.15
(c) Fostering relevance b1c = 0.24* for grades 6–8
(d) Allowing criticism R2 = 0.19
(e) Providing choice rbe = −0.44** rbf = −0.59**
(f) Fostering understanding and interest reg = −0.25** rfg = −0.18**
(g) Forcing meaningless activities rae = −0.31** raf = −0.49**
Decker 44 K-6 1. Student-report social skills (a) Teachers’ perspective of the b1a = 0.38* ΔR2 = 0.14e
et al. (2007) 2. Student-report engagement student-teacher relationship b2a = 0.42** ΔR2 = 0.18
3. Teacher-report social skills (b) Student perspective—psychological b3a = 0.47*** ΔR2 = 0.22
4. Teacher-report engagement proximity seeking b4a = 0.38** ΔR2 = 0.14
5. Academic engaged time (c) Student perspective—emotional quality b5a = −0.37* b5c = 0.49**
6. Behavioral referrals b6b = 0.36* b6c = −0.41**
7. Suspensions b7a = −0.38** b7c = −0.31*
Dill and 72 5 1. Text-recall performance (a) Learning context: communal h2 = 0.112 Between learning
Boykin (2000) 2. Evaluative item: liked the (b) Learning context: peer context and recall
learning period (c) Learning context: individual M1a > M1b*M1a > M1c*f
3. Evaluative item: would do (d) Gender r3a = 0.478*
the project again r4a = 0.596**
4. Evaluative item: cared r2b = 0.406*
about peer r4b = 0.467*
Filaka and 220 College NA 1. Course approval (a) Teacher autonomy support b5a = 0.35**g b4a = 0.76**
Sheldon (2008) students 2. Instructor approval b45 = 0.26** b14 = 0.74**
3. Grade prediction b24 = 0.72** b34 = 0.28**
4. Need satisfaction
5. Student self-determined
motivation
Furrer and 641 3–6 1. Teacher-report student (a) Relatedness to teacher b1a = 0.14**h ΔR2 = 0.01
Skinner (2003) behavioral engagement (b) Relatedness to peer b1b = 0.11* ΔR2 = 0.01
2. Child-report student (c) Overall relatedness in the fall b2a = 0.26** ΔR2 = 0.05
behavioral engagement b2b = 0.16** ΔR2 = 0.02
3. Changes in child-report b3c = 0.12** R2 = 0.59
engagement from the
beginning to the end of the
school year
J.T. Guthrie et al.
Number of
Citationa participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Greene 220 High school 1. Percentage of course points (a) Autonomy support
29
b2a = 0.22*
et al. (2004) students in the English class, which (b) Self-efficacy b1b = 0.38*
is a combination of exams, (c) Strategy use b1c = 0.15*
projects, and homework b3b = 0.24*
assignments b4b = 0.22*
2. Self-efficacy
3. Mastery goals
4. Performance-approach goals
Guthrie, Klauda, 1,200 7 1. Dedication in reading (a) Relevance r1a = 0.36**
et al. (2012) 2. Self-efficacy (b) Choices r1b = 0.19**
(c) Collaboration r1d = 0.23**
(d) Success (in text) r2d = 0.61**
(e) Thematic unit
Guthrie, 1,200 7 1. Social motivation (a) Collaboration b1a = 0.30*i For African
Mason-Singh, 2. Self-efficacy (b) Thematic unit American students
et al. (in press) 3. Intrinsic motivation (c) Reading awareness b2b = 0.46** For African
4. Value American students
b2b = 0.24** For European
American students
b4c = 0.29* For African American
students
b4c = 0.38* For European
American students
b3c = 0.32* For European
American students
Guthrie, McRae, Review
Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading

et al. (2007)
Guthrie, Wigfield, 361 3 1. Multiple text comprehension (a) Motivation composite, including self- r1a = 0.76*
Barbosa, et al. (2004a) 2. Passage comprehension efficacy, challenge, involvement, r2a = 0.82**
and curiosity
Guthrie, Wigfield, 524 3 1. Gates-MacGinitie Reading (a) Intrinsic motivation r1a = 0.88** r1b = 0.81**
Barbosa, et al. (2004b) Comprehension Test (b) Self-efficacy r2a = 0.75** r2b = 0.75**
2. Passage comprehension (c) Extrinsic motivation
Hamre and 908 5–6 Years 1. Achievement (Woodcock- (a) Instructional support h2 = 0.02** For maternal
Pianta (2005) of age Johnson) (b) Emotional support education × instructional support
(c) Maternal education h2 = 0.01* For high functional
(d) High functional risk risk × emotional support
(e) Maternal education × instructional support
(f) High functional risk × emotional support
(continued)
619
620

Table 29.3 (continued)


Number of
Citationa participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Jang (2008) 136 College NA 1. Conceptual learning (a) Rationale Model 1:j b2a = 0.42* b32 = 0.33*
students 2. Identified regulation (b) Identified regulation b13 = 0.45*
3. Engagement (c) Engagement Model 2: b4a = 0.41* b34 = 0.25*
4. Interest regulation (d) Interest regulation b13 = 0.44*
Model 3: b2a = 0.41* b32 = 0.32*
b13 = 0.44* b2b = 0.41*
Lau (2009) 1,146 11–18 years 1. Reading amount (a) Perception of reading instruction b3a = 0.49k b5a = 0.55
of age 2. Self-efficacy b2a = 0.44 b4a = 0.46
3. Intrinsic motivation b13 = 0.28 b15 = 0.26
4. Extrinsic motivation
5. Social motivation
Miller and 24 3 1. Students’ value ratings (a) Academic task (high/low challenge) Main effect F (1, 36) = 5.04*,
Meece (1999) of academic task (b) Type of value question: liking and indicating students expressing a
interest in an academic task greater liking and interest in the
(c) Exposure (maximum/minimum) high-challenge academic tasks
(d) Achievement (high/average/low)
Perry et al. (2007) 257 1 1. Math achievement (a) Child-centered practices g1a = 3.17**l
2. Interpersonal behavior g2a = −12.39*
3. Intrapersonal behavior g3a = −15.94*
4. Self-perceived competence g4a = 0.09**
Ponitz et al. (2009) 171 K 1. Spring reading (a) Classroom quality b2a = 0.16*m
2. Behavioral engagement (b) Fall reading b12 = 0.18*
(c) Sociodemographic risk
Reeve et al. (2004) 20 High school NA 1. Teacher’s autonomy- (a) Teacher exposure to information 2
Unique R 2b = 0.37n
teachers supportive behaviors on how to support students’ autonomy
b2b = 0.61**
2. Students’ engagement: (b) Teacher’s autonomy support 2
Unique R 3b = 0.29
task involvement (third (third observation)
b3b = 0.54**
observation)
3. Students’ engagement:
influence attempts (third
observation)
Ryan and Deci (2009) Review
Schunk and Review
Zimmerman (2007)
J.T. Guthrie et al.
Number of
Citationa participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Shih (2008) 343 8 1. Behavioral engagement: (a) Perceived autonomy support
29
b1a = 0.24***o b1b = 0.30***
involvement (b) Mastery-approach goal b1f = −0.20*** b1g = 0.35***
2. Behavioral engagement: (c) Mastery-avoidance goal b2b = 0.39*** b2g = 0.31***
persistence (d) Performance-approach goal b3a = 0.17** b3b = 0.23**
3. Behavioral engagement: (e) Performance-avoidance goal b3g = 0.26*** b4b = −0.34***
participation (f) Controlled motivation b4e = 0.24*** b4f = 0.19**
4. Behavioral engagement: (g) Autonomous motivation b4g = −0.23** b5a = −0.17**
avoiding b5b = −0.29** b5f = 0.17**
5. Behavioral engagement:
ignoring
Skinner and 144 3, 4, 5 1. Student perception of (a) Teacher involvement b1a = 0.28**p b2a = 0.25***
Belmont (1993) structure (b) Teacher autonomy support b3a = 0.25** b41 = 0.33***
2. Student perception of (c) Teacher structure b53 = 0.20* bcd = 0.28**
autonomy support (d) Teacher perception of student bbd = 0.72*** bad = 0.61***
3. Student perception of behavioral engagement
involvement (e) Teacher perception of student
4. Student behavioral emotional engagement
engagement
5. Student emotional
engagement
Skinner et al. (2008) 805 4–7 1. Changes in engaged (a) Student report of teacher support b1a = 0.23*** R2 = 0.37
behavior from fall to spring (b) Teacher report of teacher support b2a = −0.12*** R2 = 0.46
2. Changes in disaffected b1b = 0.07*
behavior from fall to spring
Skinner et al. (2009b) 1,018 3–6 1. Student reports of engage- (a) Teacher warmth r1a = 0.55** r1b = 0.55**
ment in spring of year 3 (b) Teacher structure r1c = 0.47** r2a = 0.26**
Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading

2. Teacher reports of engage- (c) Teacher autonomy support r2b = 0.28** r2c = 0.20**
ment in spring of year 3
Souvignier and 593 5 1. Reading comprehension (a) Instructional group: MSR + strat + CSR Ta−b = 2.25*q For reading
Mokhlesgerami (2006) 2. Understanding the use of (b) Instructional group: strat + CSR comprehension in retention
reading strategies (c) Instructional group: strat da−d = 0.82 For understanding the
3. Application of reading (d) Instructional group: control use of reading strategies in
strategies retention
4. Self-efficacy Ta−b = 2.20* For application of
5. Motivational orientation reading strategies in retention
da−d = 1.12 For application of
reading strategies in retention
Ta−b = 2.20* For motivational
orientation in retention
(continued)
621
622

Table 29.3 (continued)


Number of
Citationa participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc
Strambler and 111 1–5 1. 2004–2005 Language arts (a) Academic valuing b1b = −0.41*r
Weinstein (2010) score (b) Academic devaluing b2b = −0.45*
2. 2004–2005 Math scores (c) Alternative ID
Vansteenkiste 200 College NA 1. Autonomous motivation (a) Intrinsic goal framing vs. 2
η1a2 = 0.59***s η3a = 0.42***
et al. (2004a) students 2. Superficial processing extrinsic goal
3. Deep processing (b) Autonomy-supportive context vs. η24a = 0.21*** η25a = 0.12***
4. Test performance controlling contexts
5. Persistence
Vansteenkiste 377 College NA 1. Autonomous motivation (a) Intrinsic goal framing vs. extrinsic goal η1a2 = 0.59***s 2
et al. (2004b) students 2. Superficial processing (b) Autonomy-supportive context vs. η3a = 0.35***
2
3. Deep processing controlling contexts η24a = 0.17*** η6a = 0.12***
4. Underlining
5. Test performance
6. Persistence
Vansteenkiste 224 10, 11 1. Autonomous motivation (a) Intrinsic goal framing vs. extrinsic goal η1a2 = 0.50***s η22a = 0.16***
et al. (2004c) 2. Test performance (b) Autonomy-supportive context vs. 2
3. Persistence controlling contexts η3a = 0.09***
Vansteenkiste 130 5, 6 1. Perceived autonomy (a) Goal orientation (intrinsic, extrinsic) Main effect:
et al. (2005a) 2. Conceptual learning T1 (b) Communication style (autonomy h2 = 0.13**t for goal orientation
3. Rote learning T1 support, internal control, external h2 = 0.30*** for communication
4. Conceptual learning T2 control) style
5. Rote learning T2 η24a = 0.05*
Vansteenkiste 113 11- to 12- 1. Perceived autonomy (a) Goal orientation (intrinsic, extrinsic) Main effect:
et al. (2005b) year-olds 2. Conceptual learning T1 (b) Communication style (autonomy h2 = 0.25*** for goal orientation
3. Conceptual learning T2 support, internal control) h2 = 0.52*** for communication
2
η3a = 0.30***u
Vansteenkiste 80 11-to 12- 1. Conceptual learning (a) Goal orientation (intrinsic, extrinsic) Main effect:
et al. (2005c) year-olds 2. Task involvement (b) Communication style (autonomy h2 = 0.31***v for goal orientation
3. Relative autonomy support, internal control) h22 = 0.87*** for communication
η1a = 0.07* η2 = 0.15***
2a
Wentzel (2009) Review
J.T. Guthrie et al.
Number of
Citationa participants Grade levels Dependent variables Independent variablesb Strength of associationc 29
Wigfield et al. (2008) 315 4 1. Gates-MacGinitie (a) Instructional groups (CORI, SI, TI) Main effect of instructional
Reading Comprehension (b) Engagement group:
2. Multiple text F (8, 18) = 4.10 **
comprehension b(CORI) > b(SI)**
3. Strategy composite b(CORI) > b(TI)*
With engagement as covariate,
main effect of instructional
group:
F (3, 6) < 1 ns
Zhou et al. (2009a) 195 4–6 1. Interest (a) Autonomous motivation b1a = 0.60** R2 = 0.36***w
2. Perceived competence (b) Controlled motivation b2a = 0.58*** R2 = 0.39***
3. Perceived choice (c) Interaction term b3a = 0.38*** R2 = 0.20***
Zhou et al. (2009b) 48 4, 5 1. Autonomous motivation (a) Autonomous motivation at time 1 b1f = 0.31*x
at time 2 (b) Controlled motivation at time 1 b3f = 0.50***
2. Controlled motivation (c) Perceived competence at time 1
at time 2 (d) Perceived choice at time 1
3. Perceived competence (e) Interest at time 1
at time 2 (f) Change in perceived teacher autonomy
4. Perceived choice at time 2 support from time 1 to time 2
5. Interest at time 2
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001
Notes
a
We only included the studies documenting paths A, B, and D in Fig. 29.1
b
We did not include those variables which the authors reported as covariates or as control variables in their analyses
c
We only reported the effect sizes of classroom practices on motivations (path D), behavioral engagement (path B), and reading competence (path A)
Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading

d
The effect sizes reported in this study are the standardized beta coefficients obtained in simultaneous multiple regressions. The R2 refers to the contributions of the five predictors
(intruding, suppressing criticism, fostering relevance, allowing criticism, providing choice) to the outcome
e
The effect sizes reported in this study are the standardized beta weights in multiple regressions. ΔR2 are the unique contributions of each predictor to the outcomes
f
The post hoc analysis indicated that students assigned to communal learning (M = 6.67) context outperformed students assigned to both the peer (M = 4.29) and individual (M = 4.21)
g
All the effect sizes reported in this study are path coefficients in path analysis
h
All the effect sizes reported in this study are beta coefficients in multiple regressions. The ΔR2 were the unique contributions of the variable of interest to the outcomes
i
The effect sizes reported here are standardized beta coefficients in stepwise multiple regression
j
All the effect sizes reported in this study are standardized coefficients in structural equation modeling
k
Due to the space limit, we only reported the effects sizes for junior secondary students. All the effects sizes reported in this study are significant path coefficients in path
analysis
(continued)
623
Table 29.3 (continued)
624

l
The effect sizes reported in this study are the gamma coefficients in hierarchical linear modeling
m
The effect sizes reported in this study are standardized coefficients in structural equation modeling
n
Due to space limit, we only reported the effect sizes for the third observation. The ΔR2 is the unique contribution of teacher autonomy support (during the third observation) to
student engagement outcomes, after controlling for students’ engagement and teachers’ autonomy support at the second observation
o
The effect sizes reported in this study are the standardized beta coefficients in multiple regressions
p
The effect sizes reported in this study are standardized regression coefficients in path analysis
q
The original study reported the effect sizes in both posttest and retention. Due to the space limit, we only reported the effect sizes in retention
r
All the effect sizes reported in this study are standardized coefficients in structural equation modeling
s
Due to the space limit, we only reported the effect sizes for goal content
t
Due to the space limit, we only reported the effect sizes for goal content.
was the effect of intrinsic goal on long-term conceptual learning after controlling for short-term conceptual learning
u 2
Due to the space limit, we only reported the effect sizes for goal content. η3a was the effect of intrinsic goal on long-term conceptual learning after controlling for short-term
conceptual learning
v
Due to the space limit, we only reported the effect sizes for goal content
w
The effect sizes reported in this study are beta coefficients in multiple regressions. The R2 are the contributions of the three predictors (autonomous motivation, controlled
motivation, interaction term) to each outcome
x
These effect sizes reported in this study are the beta coefficients in hierarchical regressions. Time 2 motivation and self-perception variables were regressed onto the correspond-
ing variables in time 1 and then onto the residual scores of perceived teacher autonomy support at time 2 in the math and English classes, while controlling for time 1 autonomy
support from their previous math or English classroom teachers
J.T. Guthrie et al.
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 625

(Assor, Kaplan, & Roth, 2002; Lau, 2009). That is, teachers give specific direction to students
Providing students with an awareness of the ben- about the effectiveness of their strategy for per-
efits of reading increases their valuing of reading forming work and help students set realistic
work in the classroom. For example, Jang (2008) goals in their learning domain. Experimental
told prospective teachers that reading about com- studies summarized by these researchers confirm
plications of statistical analyses would benefit that these practices increase students’ belief in
their professions, which increased their perceived their capacity, perceived competence, and even-
value for reading texts about statistics. Likewise, tually, their achievement in reading tasks. Also
providing middle school students with an aware- beneficial to students’ self-efficacy in reading is
ness that reading about science is important to their perception of coherence in the texts and
their ability to explain their world and succeed in tasks of instruction. When students can identify
school increased students’ valuing of information the links across specific domains of knowledge
books such as science texts (Guthrie, Mason- in their reading and perceive themes in the sub-
Singh, et al., 2012). stance of their reading materials, they gain a
Another important classroom characteristic is belief that they can succeed in reading and writ-
the quality of teacher-student relationships. When ing about text (Guthrie, Mason-Singh, et al., in
teachers emphasize collaboration and positive press).
interpersonal relationships (between themselves Effects of teachers’ practices on students’
and students and among students in the class- motivations are sufficiently powerful that they
room), students’ motivation increases for school can have deleterious effects. Some teachers
in general and for reading. When students believe behave in ways that are devaluing for students.
their teachers think they are important, they are For example, negative feedback from teachers
likely to participate more socially in the class- may be devaluing for students. When teachers
room (Furrer & Skinner, 2003). As both teacher consistently scold or make students feel bad for
and student reports of the quality of teacher-stu- having the wrong answers, they respond by
dent relationships increase, there are also devaluing academic work, as indicated by their
enhancements in positive social interactions and expressions that they do not care about learning
engagement outcomes (Decker, Dona, & or grades (Strambler & Weinstein, 2010). In addi-
Christenson, 2007). For African American stu- tion, middle school students who experience no
dents in particular, collaborative learning envi- choices or limited choices in reading in Language
ronments enhance students’ recall of stories and Arts or Science classes show losses of intrinsic
desire to participate in similar activities in the motivation for reading, according to self-report
future (Dill & Boykin, 2000). Across a range of questionnaires. Likewise, when books are
contexts, explicit arrangements for student col- extremely difficult to read, students report
laborations in reading and writing increased stu- declines in self-efficacy for reading. When books
dents’ satisfaction with the classroom (Guthrie, are irrelevant, as indicated by students’ failure to
Mason-Singh, et al., in press). report being able to connect the content to their
Support for students’ self-efficacy in reading prior knowledge or their life experiences, they
and other subjects is crucial because self-efficacy report low levels of interest or dedication to read-
is exceptionally low for struggling students. As ing (Guthrie, Klauda, et al., 2012). What this
portrayed by Schunk and Zimmerman (2007), shows is that classroom practices are a sword that
several explicit teaching practices increase stu- cuts in two directions. Affirming practices may
dents’ self-efficacy. The self-efficacy-fostering foster positive affect and motivational growth,
framework consists of providing students with while at the same time undermining practices,
process goals, which consists of steps for per- such as negative feedback, controlling instruc-
forming academic tasks successfully. Teachers tion, and irrelevance, may generate decreases in
provide feedback for success in the process goals motivation. These findings are consistent with
rather than the students’ products or outcomes. the correlational findings reported by Assor et al.
626 J.T. Guthrie et al.

(2002) and reciprocal relationships between effect was mediated by autonomous motivation
classroom instruction and student motivations which combined interest and valuing for the con-
found by Skinner and Belmont (1993). tent of the reading materials (Vansteenkiste,
Simons, Lens, Soenens, & Matos, 2005).
Lau (2009) found middle and high school stu-
Direct Effects of Classroom Practices dents’ perception of instruction as relevant
on Behavioral Engagement because it was related to their lives, useful for
their goals, and interesting, showing higher vol-
This assertion is represented in the schematic as umes of reading activity (more reading engage-
Path B, which forms a connection between ment) than students who perceived the instruction
explicit practices and observed behavioral as less relevant to them. The effect of relevance
engagement. The studies for this section are pre- as a teaching practice was on behavioral engage-
sented in Table 29.3. In three studies with high ment, as measured by amount of reading, and
school and college students, Vansteenkiste, was fully mediated by intrinsic motivation and
Simons, Lens, Sheldon, and Deci (2004) exam- social motivation for younger secondary students.
ined the effects of intrinsic goal framing as an The effect of relevance of instruction was medi-
instructional practice. The definition of intrinsic ated for older secondary students by intrinsic
goal framing is that the purpose for reading motivation only. The behavioral engagement
relates to the students’ personal interests and impacted by this instruction was educationally
goals. For prospective teachers, intrinsic goal significant because highly engaged students were
framing consisted of stating that reading the text reading eight times more than disengaged stu-
will “help you teach toddlers well” or “help you dents on a scale that measured frequency, time
make the world a better place.” For adolescents spent, and breadth of materials. These findings
with obesity issues, intrinsic goal framing con- are similar to Vansteenkiste and colleagues’
sisted of showing that reading would enable stu- (2005) findings on intrinsic framing and were
dents to improve their health and lose weight. In obtained in actual classroom contexts. In both
contrast, extrinsic goal framing consisted of stat- cases, Path B in the model was affirmed, showing
ing that students should read to learn how to save that the quality of classroom practices impacted
money or improve one’s physical image. In sev- behavioral engagement in reading mediated by
eral experiments, students were given texts to intrinsic motivation and, in the latter case, also
read with one of the two goal frames. They were social motivation.
then given measures of reading comprehension Another characteristic of the classroom con-
that reflected either deep processing or surface text that is related to behavioral engagement is
memorizing. Finally, students were given a mea- teacher support. This global indicator emphasizes
sure of behavioral engagement, which was an students’ perceptions of teacher involvement
opportunity to persist in reading more about this (warmth, knowledge, and dependability) and
topic following the experimental reading task classroom structure (clarity of goals and expecta-
and the assessment. Results showed that intrinsic tions) (Skinner et al., 2009b). In this line of
goal framing increased deep processing of text research, teacher support represents student-cen-
(conceptual learning) and persistence, as indi- teredness of instruction and contrasts with a
cated by time spent reading related materials. domineering or controlling approach by the
The effect of intrinsic goal framing on the behav- teacher. Furrer and Skinner (2003) found that
ioral indicator of engagement, which was persis- teacher support is associated with increases in
tence, was mediated by students’ autonomous student engagement from fall to spring for stu-
motivation, which was a composite of their valuing dents in grades 3–6. Students’ behavioral engage-
and interest in the texts. In sum, this set of studies ment referred to their self-reported effort,
confirms experimentally that intrinsic goal fram- attention, and persistence while participating in
ing increased behavioral engagement, and its classroom learning activities. Consistent with
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 627

this finding, Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, and Path A in the model. Although we believe that the
Kindermann (2008) reported that in grades 4–7, effects of classroom practices on achievement
students’ behavioral disaffection decreased from are fully mediated by motivations and engage-
fall to spring as a consequence of teacher sup- ment, in the initial portion of this section, we
port. This decrease consisted of a reduction in briefly review research that has addressed the
students’ lack of effort or withdrawal from learn- direct effect of motivational practices in the class-
ing activities. Although teacher support is not a room on reading competence. In the second por-
specific practice, but rather a broad attribute that tion of the section, we identify a more limited set
may be associated with a number of specific of contextual variables that have been shown to
practices such as assuring success, providing rel- affect competence mediated either by students’
evance, offering choices, arranging collabora- motivations or their behavioral engagements in
tions, and providing themes for learning, it was reading or both.
strongly associated with students’ increases in A number of studies based in self-determina-
behavioral engagement (standardized regression tion theory (Ryan & Deci, 2009) document the
coefficient of .23 (p < .001)) and decreases in effects of two forms of autonomy support on stu-
behavioral disaffection (standardized regression dents’ conceptual learning. It is reasonable to
coefficient of −.12 (p < .001)). The researchers include those studies in this chapter on reading
did not examine the possible mediation by moti- because the conceptual learning outcome has
vations of the relationship between teacher sup- referred to knowledge gained from students’
port and engagement. interaction with text. We discussed above
Akin to these findings, Shih (2008) reported Vansteenkiste and colleagues’ (2005) experimen-
that Taiwanese eighth graders who reported per- tal work on intrinsic framing, which refers to rea-
ceptions of autonomy support from their teachers sons for reading and studying texts that are
were likely to show relatively high levels of personally significant to students, and also Jang’s
behavioral engagement in the form of listening (2008) study on how giving students a rationale
carefully in class, persisting with hard problems, for reading uninteresting texts about statistics that
and participating in class discussions, while not will benefit students’ careers and professional
ignoring classroom activities or avoiding hard effectiveness increased students’ conceptual
challenges. In this case, perceived autonomy learning from text. In some cases, the control
referred to the instructors’ openness and accep- condition of extrinsic framing increased factual
tance of students. memory and surface processing of text.
Consequently, the effects of these practices on
reading competence are firmly established exper-
Classroom Practices Impact Student imentally. One limitation of these investigations
Competence is that they are short term, with brief interventions
and limited measures of conceptual learning that
In the graphic representation of the model of may not be generalizable to academically signifi-
reading engagement processes with classroom cant success. A second limitation is that they have
contexts, we present classroom practices and been performed mainly with college students.
conditions on the far left. The purpose of this Studies of classroom practices that increase
location is to indicate that these contextual vari- students’ motivation have also been performed
ables may influence students’ motivations, behav- with elementary and middle school students in
ioral engagement, and reading competence. At Reading and Language Arts classrooms over peri-
the most general level, a number of studies have ods from 6 to 36 weeks. We and our colleagues
shown that contextual variables of the classroom have examined how Concept-Oriented Reading
such as instructional practices, teacher support, Instruction (CORI) influences third-, fourth-,
and other conditions may directly impact stu- fifth-, and seventh-grade students’ reading com-
dents’ reading competence; we denote this with prehension and engagement in reading (Guthrie,
628 J.T. Guthrie et al.

Wigfield, Barbosa, et al. 2004; see Guthrie, ive). Pairs of students select an option for self-
McRae, et al., 2007, for review of the findings). expression and complete the task, entering it in
CORI includes the classroom practices of provid- their portfolio.
ing relevance, choices, collaboration, leveled Teacher closes the lesson by asking, “What
texts, and thematic units. This cluster of practices choices did you have today and how did they help
is designed to increase intrinsic motivation, self- you?” (In this 5-minute reflection, the students’
efficacy, social motivation, and valuing for read- awareness of autonomy support enhances their
ing (Guthrie, Wigfield, & Perencevich, 2004). perception that the instruction affirms their moti-
To exemplify CORI, a synopsis of a lesson is vational development as well as their acquisition
presented next. The CORI goal is to teach read- of cognitive expertise in reading.)
ing comprehension with motivation support. This Guthrie, Hoa, et al. (2007) performed a meta-
lesson is from a 6-week CORI unit that teaches analysis of CORI’s effects across 11 experiments
the reading strategies of inferencing, summariz- with 75 effect sizes. CORI was found to surpass
ing, and concept mapping to foster seventh grad- comparison treatments in increasing students’
ers’ comprehension of information text. Using competence according to standardized tests of
the conceptual theme “Diversity of Life,” this is reading comprehension (ES = .90), 2-day reading
lesson 9 which occurs in week 2. and writing tasks (ES = .93), passage comprehen-
The teacher posts the question for the day: sion (ES = .73), and reading fluency (ES = .59), as
“What are some special features of wetland plants well as word recognition (ES = .75). CORI also
that enable them to survive in their environment?” fostered students’ self-reported reading motiva-
To begin, students view a 5-minute video about tion (ES = 1.2) and teacher-reported students’
aquatic plants, showing their locations, stems, engagement in reading (ES = 1.0), as well as
root systems, and leaf varieties. (This video sup- amount of reading (ES = .49). This confirms that
ports intrinsic motivation by providing relevance an integrated cluster of motivational practices
for the texts that follow). Individuals record their over extended time can increase students’ perfor-
observations of the video in a journal and share mance on educationally significant measures of
them with a partner. Partners then select one of reading comprehension. The bulk of the evidence
two texts on aquatic plants. Together they locate shows that CORI impacted reading comprehen-
a 2–4-page section that addresses the day’s ques- sion outcomes, although the one study that exam-
tion (partnerships support social motivation; ined the issue also showed that this instructional
choice of texts supports students’ autonomy). effect was mediated by behavioral engagement
Teachers guide students to select 2–5 key (Wigfield et al., 2008; see further discussion
words that represent the main idea of the text, below). Furthermore, these effects were con-
which they enter into their journals. Then teach- firmed by investigators who showed that an inter-
ers guide students to identify 3–4 supporting facts vention that added motivational supports to
for each key word (scaffolding of the summariz- instruction in self-regulation increased students’
ing process enables students to learn a widely self-regulated reading more effectively than
applicable strategy for summarizing information instruction that did not include motivational prac-
text—the essence of comprehension instruction; tices (Souvignier & Mokhlesgerami, 2006).
this scaffolding also assures success in grappling A burgeoning literature exists documenting
with complex information text, giving students the effect of perceived emotional support from
support for increasing self-efficacy in reading teachers on students’ academic performance
these texts). (Wentzel, 2009). The outcomes of these studies
Next, the teacher gives students the choice of are often grades rather than test scores, which
showing their understanding about “special fea- may reflect students’ motivational and social attri-
tures of wetland plants” by either writing a sum- butes in addition to their reading competence. In
mary or drawing and labeling a diagram (choice these studies, teacher support refers to students’
of knowledge expression is autonomy support- relationships with teachers that enable them to
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 629

perceive the goals of teaching clearly, belief that competence in reading. The Jang (2008) study
their teachers will help them attain the goals effi- that we have discussed in this chapter documents
ciently, and that the students are in a safe and this double mediation. All three pathways (D, E,
trusting environment. The findings range from and F) were tested in the study, illustrating that
grade 1 (Hamre & Pianta, 2005; Perry, Donohue, classroom practices impacted motivations, which
& Weinstein, 2007) to college classrooms (Filaka increased behavioral engagements, which influ-
& Sheldon, 2008). For example, teacher support enced reading competence. Jang found that col-
was found to increase competence in reading lege students who were given a rationale
words and passages in the middle of first grade emphasizing the value of reading an uninterest-
for students placed at risk due to low maternal ing statistics text passage perceived the text as
education. The instructional effect of motivation more important than did students not given the
practices was stronger than the effect of excellent rationale; Jang stated that the students who
pedagogy in word recognition for at-risk students received the value rationale increased in their
(consisting of direct instruction in phonological identified regulation. Students whose perceived
knowledge and letter sound correspondences) importance/identified regulation increased also
(Hamre & Pianta, 2005). showed enhanced behavioral engagement.
One dominant construct in the teacher support According to the reports of external raters who
literature is teacher caring, which correlates posi- observed students during their reading and learn-
tively with academic achievement in reading and ing, behaviorally engaged students were atten-
English courses (Wentzel, 2009). However, the tive, on task, effortful, and persistent in the face
specific ways in which teachers express caring of challenges. Behaviorally disengaged students
for students have been little studied. It also is tended to be off task, passive, and give up quickly
unclear how teacher caring relates to some of the on the reading activity. In addition, highly behav-
other practices discussed earlier, such as helping iorally engaged students gained deeper concep-
students to see the relevance of instruction, make tual understanding than less behaviorally engaged
meaningful choices during learning, interact with students, although behavioral engagement did
classmates for academic purposes, enjoy the not influence students’ learning of minor facts.
acquisition of expertise, and learn in meaningful, This confirms the proposition that the classroom
coherent themes; this remains an important topic practice of affording students a value rationale
for future research. for learning increased students’ perceived impor-
tance/identified regulation, which in turn
increased their behavioral engagement during the
Indirect Effects of Classroom Practices reading activity, which enhanced their perfor-
on Students’ Reading Competence mance on the conceptual learning aspect of a
reading test on this text. It should be noted that
A few of the studies documenting the effects of intrinsic motivation was not a mediator in this
classroom motivation practices on reading com- study. Although the effect of the value rationale
petence have attempted to quantify the mediation on reading comprehension was mediated by stu-
of these effects by students’ motivations or behav- dents’ values (identified regulation), it was not
ioral engagements. As previously stated, we are significantly mediated by students’ intrinsic moti-
proposing that under a majority of conditions, vation (interest regulation). Thus, for the ecologi-
classroom practices and conditions that support cally valid task of reading an uninteresting text,
student motivation in the classroom context are the mediating motivation was perceived impor-
most likely to impact students’ reading compe- tance, but not intrinsic motivation (reading inter-
tence by virtue of their effects on students’ moti- esting material), which is frequently shown to be
vations, which are then expected to increase a contributor to reading achievement.
behavioral engagement in reading, which is Other investigators have shown that the impact
the proximal variable that influences cognitive of motivational practices on students’ reading
630 J.T. Guthrie et al.

competencies is mediated by students’ behavioral students’ interests. Classrooms with high global
engagement in actual classroom contexts. quality induced high levels of behavioral engage-
Wigfield et al. (2008) reported that the effects of ment, which consisted of attending to tasks, com-
CORI on fourth-grade students’ reading compre- pleting reading activities, following rules,
hension were mediated by students’ behavioral persisting in the face of difficulty, and exercising
engagement in reading. In this investigation, stu- control. Students with high behavioral engage-
dents receiving CORI showed higher reading ment showed more gain in reading competencies
comprehension outcomes than students in con- than students with lower behavioral engagement
trol classrooms, but the effect of instructional and lower global quality of instruction (Ponitz,
conditions was fully mediated by the extensive- Rimm-Kaufman, Grimm, & Curby, 2009). At the
ness of their behavioral engagement in reading other end of the educational continuum, in a col-
activities. lege journalism course, students reported varying
The effects of intrinsic goal framing on con- levels of perceived autonomy support from their
ceptual learning described previously have occa- laboratory instructor. They also reported intrinsic
sionally been examined for their mediational motivation for learning in the course. In this situ-
processes. In Vansteenkiste et al.’s (2005) study, ation, the effect of autonomy-supportive instruc-
the effect of the autonomy-supportive communi- tion on students’ grades was mediated by their
cation style given by the experimenters during intrinsic motivation for learning in the course
the study was mediated by students’ autonomous (Filaka & Sheldon, 2008). These examples illus-
motivation, which referred to their value and trate that the mediating processes of behavioral
interest in the task. In this study, young adoles- engagement and motivation are sufficiently
cents who were obese were given a text on food prominent to be measured in research and to be
nutrition and health. In one case, the material was functional in influencing students’ grades and
presented sympathetically from the students’ per- tested achievement in classrooms.
spective and explained how students who under-
stood it could improve their health and comfort.
The control experimental condition presented the Limitations and Next Steps
material didactically as a task they should attempt
to master. Students who received the autonomy- The work reviewed in this chapter clearly docu-
supportive communications valued the reading ments how classroom practices and conditions
activity more highly and gained conceptual impact student motivation, engagement, and
knowledge (although not rote information) from competence. Equally, if not more important, there
it more fully. The motivational practice impacted now is clear evidence that students’ motivation
students’ understanding of major concepts, but and engagement mediate the effects of classroom
not minor material in the texts, which shows that practices on student achievement outcomes. That
not all learning, but primarily high-level concep- is, the impact of classroom practices on student
tual learning, was facilitated by motivational outcomes depends upon the level of student
practices. engagement in classroom activities.
Examples of single mediation are also pro- Although we have learned much about the
vided in practical classroom learning environ- linkages between classroom practices, motiva-
ments. For example, in Hamre and Pianta’s tion, engagement, and outcome as presented in
(2005) study of reading instruction in kindergar- Fig. 29.1, there are several limitations in this lit-
ten, global classroom quality was assessed in erature which should be noted. First, the large
terms of teachers’ provision of effective instruc- majority of studies of mediation entail structural
tion while building warm emotional connections equation modeling. In the absence of experimen-
with students, which includes support for stu- tal designs, the inferences to causality are
dents’ self-regulation, a balance of activities for extremely limited. Although mediated effects are
children’s diverse skill levels, and sensitivity to often assumed to have a causal direction, the
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 631

direction of causality cannot be inferred confi- with learning from highly interesting electronic
dently any more easily than it can be with a zero media due to its relatively high interest level.
order correlation. Specifically, mediation is a Because electronic text is nearly universal in
procedure to characterize overlapping variances, schools, homes, and students’ backpacks, it seems
but it does not yield strong inferences about warranted examining whether motivation, behav-
causal relationships. Experiments of the kind ioral engagement, and competence in the domain
conducted by Vansteenkiste et al. (2004) should of electronic text interaction is subject to the
be extended. Instructional supports such as rele- same principles as traditional interaction with
vance, choice, student-centeredness, and teach- printed text. It is conceivable that electronic text
ers’ emotional support have very rarely been is highly motivating due to the autonomy, effi-
investigated with experimental designs, and thus, cacy, and apparent value it affords the student. If
their causal characteristics remain unknown. so, academic learning may be accelerated through
Second, minority students are rarely disaggre- instructional use of this medium, and properties
gated within these studies or serve as the target of the medium may have motivational impacts on
populations for investigations. Consequently, our motivation, learning, and achievement.
knowledge base about African American and
Hispanic students is not established, and it cannot
be assumed to be identical to the knowledge base References
for European Americans. The exception is that
Asian students, including Taiwanese, Hong Afflerbach, P. (1998). Reading assessment and learning to
Kong, Chinese, and Korean populations have read. In J. Osborn & F. Lehr (Eds.), Literacy for all:
Issues in teaching and learning (pp. 239–263). New
been investigated from the viewpoint of effects York: Guilford Press.
of classroom practices on motivational outcomes Anmarkrud, O., & Bräten, I. (2009). Motivation for read-
(Jang, Reeve, Ryan, & Kim, 2009; Lau, 2009; ing comprehension. Learning and Individual
Shih, 2008; Zhou et al., 2009). Differences, 19, 252–256.
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J.
Third, students who are low achieving in read- (2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con-
ing have not been the focus of a sufficient num- ceptual and methodological issues of the construct.
ber of investigations. For example, it is unknown Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386.
whether the effects of behavioral engagement on Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good,
but relevance is excellent: Autonomy-enhancing and
reading competence are higher, lower, or the suppressing teacher behaviours predicting students’
same for low-achieving readers in comparison to engagement in schoolwork. British Journal of
average- or high-achieving readers. As noted by Educational Psychology, 72, 261–278.
Quirk and Schwanenflugel (2004), most reading Baker, L., Dreher, M. J., & Guthrie, J. T. (Eds.). (2000).
Engaging young readers. New York: Guilford Press.
programs for low achievers are strongly cogni- Baker, L., & Wigfield, A. (1999). Dimensions of chil-
tive and tend to neglect motivational practices, dren’s motivation for reading and their relations to
although researchers would agree that explicitly reading activity and reading achievement. Reading
supporting self-efficacy would be valuable for Research Quarterly, 34, 452–477.
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating compe-
this population. tence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through
Fourth, motivation for reading electronic text proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality and
should be studied. Although students are intrinsi- Social Psychology, 41, 586–598.
cally motivated by interacting with electronic Brophy, J. (1981). Teacher praise: A functional analysis.
Review of Educational Research, 51, 5–32.
media, relatively few studies have been con- Chan, L. K. S. (1994). Relationship of motivation, strate-
ducted that examine how students’ motivation gic learning, and reading achievement in grades 5, 7,
and competence are impacted by reading digital and 9. The Journal of Experimental Education, 62,
text (see Mills, 2010, for review of this work). As 319–339.
Decker, D. M., Dona, D. P., & Christenson, S. L. (2007).
Jang (2008) found, interest was not associated Behaviorally at-risk African American students: The
with learning from uninteresting text, and it is importance of student-teacher relationships for student
possible that interest regulation is not associated outcomes. Journal of School Psychology, 45, 83–109.
632 J.T. Guthrie et al.

Dill, E. M., & Boykin, A. W. (2000). The comparative Guthrie, J. T., & Humenick, N. M. (2004). Motivating stu-
influence of individual, peer tutoring, and communal dents to read: Evidence for classroom practices that
learning contexts on the text recall of African American increase reading motivation and achievement. In P.
children. Journal of Black Psychology, Special issue: McCardle & V. Chhabra (Eds.), The voice of evidence
African American culture and identity: Research in reading research (pp. 329–354). Baltimore: Brookes
directions for the new millennium, 26, 65–78. Publishing.
Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M., & Kelly, D. Guthrie, J. T., Klauda, S. L., & Morrison, D. A. (2012).
(2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term Motivation, achievement and classroom contexts for
goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, information book reading. In J. T. Guthrie, A. Wigfield, &
92, 1087–1101. S. L. Klauda (Eds.), http//www.corilearning.com/research-
Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Self- publications/ Adolescents’ engagement in academic liter-
discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic perfor- acy. Sharjah, UAE: Bentham Science Publishers.
mance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, Guthrie, J. T., Mason-Singh, A., & Coddington, C. S. (in
939–944. press). Instructional effects of Concept-Oriented
Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Task val- Reading Instruction on motivation for reading infor-
ues and ability beliefs as predictors of high school lit- mation text in middle school. In J. T. Guthrie,
eracy choices: A developmental analysis. Journal of A. Wigfield, & S. L. Klauda (Eds.), Adolescents’
Educational Psychology, 98, 382–393. engagement in academic literacy. Sharjah, UAE:
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, Bentham Science Publishers.
values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, Guthrie, J. T., McGough, K., Bennett, L., & Rice, M. E.
109–132. (1996). Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction: An
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. integrated curriculum to develop motivations and
(1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisi- strategies for reading. In L. Baker, P. Afflerbach, & D.
tion of expert performance. Psychological Review, Reinking (Eds.), Developing engaged readers in
100, 363–406. school and home communities (pp. 165–190).
Filaka, V. F., & Sheldon, K. M. (2008). Teacher support, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
student motivation, student need satisfaction, and col- Guthrie, J. T., McRae, A. C., & Klauda, S. L. (2007).
lege teacher course evaluations: Testing a sequential Contributions of Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction
path model. Educational Psychology, 28, 711–724. to knowledge about interventions for motivations in
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). reading. Educational Psychologist, 42, 237–250.
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of Guthrie, J. T., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, motivation in reading. In M. L. Kamil & P. B.
59–109. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol.
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a III, pp. 403–422). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
factor in children’s academic engagement and perfor- Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Barbosa, P., Perencevich,
mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, K. C., Taboada, A., Davis, M. H., et al. (2004).
148–162. Increasing reading comprehension and engagement
Gambrell, L., & Marniak, B. (1997). Incentives and intrin- through Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction.
sic motivation to read. In J. T. Guthrie & A. Wigfield Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 403–423.
(Eds.), Reading engagement: Motivating readers Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Metsala, J. L., & Cox, K. E.
through integrated instruction (pp. 205–217). Newark, (1999). Motivational and cognitive predictors of text
DE: International Reading Association. comprehension and reading amount. Scientific Studies
Greene, B. A., Miller, R. B., Crowson, H. M., Duke, B. L., of Reading, 3, 231–256.
& Akey, K. L. (2004). Predicting high school students’ Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., & Perencevich, K. C. (Eds.).
cognitive engagement and achievement: Contributions (2004). Motivating reading comprehension: Concept-
of classroom perceptions and motivation. Oriented Reading Instruction. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 462–482. Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Can instructional
Grigg, W. S., Ryan, R. M., Jin, Y., & Campbell, J. R. and emotional support in the first-grade classroom
(2003). The nation’s report card: Reading 2002 make a difference for children at risk of school fail-
(Publication No. NCES 2003–521). Washington, DC: ure? Child Development, 76, 949–967.
U. S. Government Printing Office. Jang, H. (2008). Supporting students’ motivation, engage-
Guthrie, J. T., & Coddington, C. S. (2009). Reading moti- ment, and learning during an uninteresting activity.
vation. In K. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 798–811.
of motivation at school (pp. 503–525). New York: Jang, H., Reeve, J., Ryan, R. M., & Kim, A. (2009). Can
Routledge. self-determination theory explain what underlies the
Guthrie, J. T., Hoa, L. W., Wigfield, A., Tonks, S. M., productive, satisfying learning experiences of collec-
Humenick, N. M., & Littles, E. (2007). Reading moti- tivistically oriented Korean students? Journal of
vation and reading comprehension growth in the later Educational Psychology, 101, 644–661.
elementary years. Contemporary Educational Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009). An educational
Psychology, 32, 282–313. psychology success story: Social interdependence
29 Instructional Contexts for Engagement and Achievement in Reading 633

theory and cooperative learning. Educational and adjustment in first grade. Journal of School
Researcher, 38, 365–379. Psychology, 45, 269–292.
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. Pintrich, P. R., & de Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and
(2007). Comprehension in preschool and early ele- self-regulated learning components of classroom aca-
mentary children: Skill development and strategy demic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology,
interventions. In D. S. McNamara (Ed.), Reading com- 82, 33–40.
prehension strategies (pp. 27–43). New York: Ponitz, C. C., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Grimm, K. J., &
Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis Group. Curby, T. W. (2009). Kindergarten classroom quality,
Ladd, G. W., & Dinella, L. M. (2009). Continuity and behavioral engagement, and reading achievement.
change in early school engagement: Predictive of chil- School Psychology Review, 38, 102–120.
dren’s achievement trajectories from first to eighth Quirk, M. P., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2004). Do supple-
grade? Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, mental remedial reading programs address the motiva-
190–206. tional issues of struggling readers? An analysis of five
Lau, K. (2009). Reading motivation, perceptions of read- popular programs. Reading Research and Instruction,
ing instruction and reading amount: A comparison of 43, 1–19.
junior and secondary students in Hong Kong. Journal Rapp, D., & van den Broek, P. (2005). Dynamic text com-
of Research in Reading, 32, 366–382. prehension: An integrative view of reading. Current
Legault, L., Green-Demers, I., & Pelletier, L. (2006). Why Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 276–279.
do high school students lack motivation in the class- Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J.
room? Toward an understanding of academic amotiva- (2004). Enhancing students’ engagement by increas-
tion and the role of social support. Journal of ing teachers’ autonomy support. Motivation and
Educational Psychology, 98, 567–582. Emotion, 28, 147–169.
Maehr, M. L., & Zusho, A. (2009). Achievement goal Reeve, J., Jang, H., Hardre, P., & Omura, M. (2002).
theory: The past, present, and future. In K. R. Wentzel Providing a rationale in an autonomy-supportive way
& A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at as a strategy to motivate others during an uninteresting
school (pp. 77–104). New York: Routledge/Taylor & activity. Motivation and Emotion, 26, 183–207.
Francis Group. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination
Miller, S. D., & Meece, J. L. (1999). Third graders’ moti- theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
vational preferences for reading and writing tasks. The social development, and well-being. American
Elementary School Journal, 100, 19–35. Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Mills, K. A. (2010). A review of the “digital turn” in the Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). Promoting self-deter-
New Literacy studies. Review of Educational Research, mined school engagement: Motivation, learning, and
80, 246–271. well-being. In K. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.),
Morgan, P. L., & Fuchs, D. (2007). Is there a bidirectional Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 171–195). New
relationship between children’s reading skills and read- York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
ing motivation? Exceptional Children, 73, 165–183. Salamonson, Y., Andrew, S., & Everett, B. (2009).
National Reading Panel. (2000). Fluency. In Teaching Academic engagement and disengagement as predic-
children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the tors of performance in pathophysiology among nurs-
scientific research literature on reading and its impli- ing students. Contemporary Nurse, 32, 123–132.
cations for instruction. Bethesda, MD: National Schiefele, U. (1999). Interest and learning from text.
Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Health Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 257–279.
and Human Development. Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2007). Influencing
National Research Council. (2004). Engaging schools: children’s self-efficacy and self-regulation of reading
Fostering high school students’ motivation to learn. and writing through modeling. Reading & Writing
Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Quarterly, 23, 7–25.
Nolen, S. B. (1988). Reasons for studying: Motivational Schwinger, M., Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2009). How
orientations and study strategies. Cognition and do motivational regulation strategies affect achieve-
Instruction, 5, 269–287. ment: Mediated by effort management and moderated
Orvis, K. A., Fisher, S. L., & Wasserman, M. E. (2009). by intelligence. Learning and Individual Differences,
Power to the people: Using learner control to improve 19, 621–627.
trainee reactions and learning in web-based instruc- Shih, S. (2008). The relation of self-determination and
tional environments. Journal of Applied Psychology, achievement goals to Taiwanese eighth graders’
94, 960–971. behavioral and emotional engagement in schoolwork.
Perie, M., Grigg, W., & Donahue, P. (2005). The nation’s The Elementary School Journal, 108, 313–334.
report card: Reading 2005 (U.S. Department of Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the
Education, National Center for Educational Statistics, classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and
NCES 2006–451). Washington, DC: U.S. Government student engagement across the school year. Journal of
Printing Office. Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581.
Perry, K. E., Donohue, K. M., & Weinstein, R. S. (2007). Skinner, E. A., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann,
Teaching practices and the promotion of achievement T. A. (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the
634 J.T. Guthrie et al.

classroom: Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Wang, J. H.-Y., & Guthrie, J. T. (2004). Modeling the
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 765–781. effects of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation,
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & amount of reading, and past reading achievement on
Wellborn, J. G. (2009a). Engagement and disaffec- text comprehension between U.S. and Chinese stu-
tion as organizational constructs in the dynamics of dents. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 162–186.
motivational development. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wentzel, K. R. (2009). Students’ relationships with teach-
Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school ers as motivational contexts. In K. R. Wentzel &
(pp. 223–245). New York: Routledge/Taylor & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school
Francis Group. (pp. 301–322). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., & Furrer, C. J. (2009b). Group.
A motivational perspective on engagement and disaf- Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of
fection: Conceptualization and assessment of chil- competence beliefs, expectancies for success, and
dren’s behavioral and emotional participation in achievement values from childhood through adoles-
academic activities in the classroom. Educational and cence. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development
Psychological Measurement, 69, 493–525. of achievement motivation (pp. 91–120). San Diego,
Souvignier, E., & Mokhlesgerami, J. (2006). Using self- CA: Academic.
regulation as a framework for implementing strategy Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., &
instruction to foster reading comprehension. Learning Davis-Kean, P. (2006). Development of achievement
and Instruction, 16, 57–71. motivation. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M.
Strambler, M. J., & Weinstein, R. S. (2010). Psychological Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3,
disengagement in elementary school among ethnic Social, emotional, and personality development (6th
minority students. Journal of Applied Developmental ed., pp. 933–1002). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Psychology, 31, 155–165. Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations of chil-
van den Broek, P., Rapp, D. N., & Kendeou, P. (2005). dren’s motivation for reading to the amount and
Integrating memory-based and constructionist pro- breadth or their reading. Journal of Educational
cesses in accounts of reading comprehension. Psychology, 89, 420–432.
Discourse Processes, 39, 299–316. Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (2010). The impact of
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., Concept-Oriented Reading instruction on students’
& Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learning, perfor- reading motivation, reading engagement, and reading
mance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of comprehension. In J. Meece & J. S. Eccles (Eds.),
intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive con- Handbook on schools, schooling, and human develop-
texts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ment (pp. 463–477). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
87, 246–260. Wigfield, A., Guthrie, J. T., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A.,
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., & Klauda, S. L., McRae, A., et al. (2008). The role of
Matos, L. (2005). Examining the motivational impact reading engagement in mediating effects of reading
of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal framing and auton- comprehension instruction on reading outcomes.
omy-supportive versus internally controlling commu- Psychology in the Schools, 45, 432–445.
nication style on early adolescents’ academic Wolters, C. A. (2003). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating
achievement. Child Development, 76, 483–501. an underemphasized aspect of self-regulated learning.
Walczyk, J. J., Wei, M., Griffith-Ross, D. A., Cooper, A. L., Educational Psychologist, 38, 189–205.
Zha, P., & Goubert, S. E. (2007). Development of the Zhou, M., Ma, W. J., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The impor-
interplay between automatic processes and cognitive tance of autonomy for rural Chinese children’s moti-
resources in reading. Journal of Educational vation for learning. Learning and Individual
Psychology, 99, 867–887. Differences, 19, 492–498.
A Self-regulated Learning
Perspective on Student Engagement 30
Christopher A. Wolters and Daniel J. Taylor

Abstract
Models of both self-regulated learning and student engagement have been
used to help understand why some students are successful in school while
others are not. The goal of this chapter is to provide greater insight into the
relations between these two theoretical frameworks. The first section pres-
ents a basic model of self-regulated learning, outlining the primary phases
and areas involved in that process. The next section discusses key similari-
ties and differences between aspects of self-regulated learning and fea-
tures of student engagement, drawing on both theoretical suggestions and
empirical research. The final section offers ideas and avenues for addi-
tional research that would serve to better link self-regulated learning and
student engagement.

Self-regulated learning and student engagement (Baumeister & Vohs, 2004; Boekaerts et al.,
both represent research and theoretical frame- 2000). The subset of these models developed to
works used to understand students’ functioning understand and explain individuals’ active man-
and performance with regard to academic con- agement of their own motivational, behavioral,
texts. Models of self-regulation, broadly speak- and cognitive functioning within academic set-
ing, have been developed with regard to a variety tings use the term self-regulated learning. Even
of domains to understand how individuals take an within this more narrowly defined domain, mod-
active, purposeful, and reflective role in their own els of self-regulated learning have emerged from
functioning (Baumeister & Vohs, 2004; Boekaerts, a diverse set of theoretical roots that incorporate
Pintrich, & Zeidner, 2000). For instance, models research investigating cognitive and social devel-
exist to understand individuals’ self-management opment, metacognition, volition, and motivation
of chronic illness, smoking, exercise, eating, (Zimmerman & Schunk, 2001). Across most
shopping, and other noneducational processes models, self-regulated learning can be viewed as
an active, constructive process through which
learners set goals for their learning and then work
to monitor, regulate, and control their cognition,
C.A. Wolters, Ph.D. (*) • D.J. Taylor, M.A.
Department of Educational Psychology, motivation, and behavior in order to accomplish
University of Houston, Houston, TX, USA those goals (Pintrich, 2004; Wolters, Pintrich, &
e-mail: cwolters@uh.edu; djtaylor@uh.edu Karabenick, 2005).

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 635


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_30, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
636 C.A. Wolters and D.J. Taylor

The history of research on student engagement are both conceptualized as multidimensional


is populated mostly by school psychologists ini- because they bring together distinct facets or sub-
tially focused on understanding behavioral indica- processes of students’ academic functioning into
tors of students’ participation in academic settings a more global model. For example, prominent
(Jimerson, Campos, & Greif, 2003). Broadly models of self-regulated learning and student
defined, student engagement was viewed as a engagement both attempt to account for behav-
person’s active participation in school-related ioral, cognitive, and emotional processes. Third,
endeavors and as “energy in action” (Russell, both self-regulated learning and student engage-
Ainley, & Frydenberg, 2005, p. 1). Over the past ment have been viewed as mediating processes
few decades, the concept of engagement has been that provide a bridge between contextual and per-
expanded to incorporate the involvement produced sonal factors on one side and students’ academic
by emotional and cognitive processes as well performance or achievement on the other
(Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004). Hence, (Appleton et al., 2006; Christenson et al., 2008;
student engagement is now viewed by most Fredricks et al., 2004; Pintrich, 2004). Hence,
researchers as a multidimensional construct that student engagement and self-regulated learning
reflects both observable, external factors as well as both have been used to differentiate between
less observable, internal factors (Appleton, more and less effective learners or to explain why
Christenson, & Furlong, 2008; Fredricks et al., some students are more successful in school.
2004; Reschly, Huebner, Appleton, & Antaramian, Perhaps even more telling than these general
2008; Skinner, Kinderman, & Furrer, 2009). The similarities, definitions of self-regulated learning
particular way in which these factors are divided and student engagement often invoke terminol-
varies across particular models. For instance, some ogy or concepts that are central to the other. For
models break down these facets further into aca- instance, self-regulated learners are commonly
demic and/or behavioral (observable, external) described as students who are actively engaged
subtypes and cognitive and/or affective/psycho- in their own learning (Wolters, 2003a;
logical (less observable, internal) subtypes Zimmerman, 2002). At the same time, recent
(Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, 2006; theoretical descriptions identify self-regulation
Fredricks et al., 2004; Jimerson et al., 2003). In the as one component or facet of student engagement
present discussion, we follow the structure pre- (Fredricks et al., 2004). That is, self-regulation or
sented by Fredricks et al. and differentiate between the active use of self-regulation strategies is por-
behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement. trayed as a key feature of what it means for stu-
An initial consideration reveals a number of dents to exhibit engagement in school contexts.
similarities between these two broad conceptual In sum, these many similarities suggest that self-
frameworks (i.e., self-regulated learning, student regulated learning and student engagement
engagement). One, the research on both self-reg- should be theoretically and practically linked.
ulated learning and student engagement includes The purpose of this chapter is to advance a
a variety of models that share key assumptions deeper understanding of the relations between
but also show variability. For instance, there are these two theoretical frameworks or areas of
prominent models of self-regulated learning that research by evaluating their conceptual similari-
have been spearheaded by Pintrich (2004; ties and differences. To accomplish this goal, the
Pintrich & Zusho, 2002; Wolters et al., 2005), remainder of the chapter is divided into three
Winne (Winne & Hadwin, 1998, 2008), and major sections. In the initial section, we briefly
Zimmerman (2000). Similarly, related but dis- present a basic model of self-regulated learning.
tinct models of student engagement have been Next, we discuss similarities and differences
proposed by Finn (1989), Skinner (Skinner & between this view of self-regulated learning and
Belmont, 1993), Christenson (Christenson et al., key features of how student engagement is con-
2008), and others (Fredricks et al., 2004). Two, ceptualized. Finally, in the last section, we pro-
self-regulated learning and student engagement vide some concluding remarks and map out a few
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective on Student Engagement 637

directions for future research that would serve to task. Cognitively, this phase would reflect stu-
better link self-regulated learning and student dents’ efforts to consider what they already know
engagement. about the topic or subject area, what they know
about how to learn the type of material, and what
particular learning strategies should be used to
Self-regulated Learning complete the task.
A second phase, called monitoring by Pintrich
We review the main components or facets of self- (2004; Pintrich et al., 2000), describes students’
regulated learning using a model emerging pri- efforts to keep track or be aware of their ongoing
marily from the work of Pintrich and his progress and performance at a task or learning
colleagues (Pintrich, 2004; Pintrich & De Groot, activity. Zimmerman (2000) included self-obser-
1990; Pintrich, Wolters, & Baxter, 2000; Pintrich vation or students’ tracking of their performance,
& Zusho, 2002; Wolters, 2003a; Wolters et al., the conditions of the task, and the products of
2005). According to this model, self-regulated their efforts. Winne and Hadwin (1998) described
learning is characterized as involving four inter- metacognitive monitoring that occurs during
dependent phases (see Table 30.1). In other enactment of the task. As stressed by Butler and
models, similar dimensions have been labeled as Winne (1995) an important by-product of this
stages, operations, subprocesses, classes, or com- process is different forms of feedback including
ponents of self-regulated learning (Greene & rate of progress toward the goal, effectiveness of
Azevedo, 2007; Winne & Hadwin, 2008; particular strategies, and personal abilities or
Zimmerman, 2000). skills. The generation of these various forms of
feedback provides the information or products
needed by other processes within self-regulated
Phases of Self-regulated Learning learning. Monitoring also allows students to gen-
erate evaluations such as the task is too difficult
One phase, often labeled forethought or planning or that they may not have the ability to accom-
(Pintrich, 2004; Zimmerman, 2000), reflects stu- plish their goals.
dents’ planning, prior knowledge activation, and In addition to monitoring, a third phase or sub-
other processes that frequently occur before for- process that often occurs while students partici-
mally initiating participation in a task. Setting pate in tasks is labeled control, management, or
goals, a core feature of all models of self-regulated just regulation (Greene & Azevedo, 2007; Winne
learning, is one central process within this phase. & Hadwin, 2008; Zimmerman, 2000). This pro-
As well, this phase is key to motivation because it cess reflects what Zimmerman and others have
includes the activation of students’ initial atti- identified as simply performance, enactment, or
tudes and beliefs regarding the perceived impor- completion of the task. Most centrally, this phase
tance or usefulness of the material to be learned involves students’ actual use and management of
and the interestingness of the activity. Similarly, the various learning strategies and tactics intended
Zimmerman (1989) stressed that this initial phase to reach the goals that have been established
includes students’ activation of their perceived (Pintrich et al., 2000; Winne & Hadwin, 1998;
self-efficacy or confidence in their ability to suc- Zimmerman, 2000). It reflects learners’ efforts to
cessfully reach a goal or complete a learning task actively manage, modify, or change what they are
at some given level of proficiency. Consistent doing in order to maintain their effectiveness or
with Winne and Hadwin (1998), Pintrich (2004) progress toward their goals. Zimmerman
proposed that students initially define the task by explained how students demonstrate self-control
producing perceptions about what the task entails by utilizing the specific methods or strategies
and what limitations and resources are currently they chose during the initial phase. Similarly,
available. Once students generate these ideas, Corno (2001) emphasized the role that volition
they set goals and create a plan to carry out the plays in completing a task. Once students have
638

Table 30.1 Phases and areas for self-regulated learning


Areas for self-regulation
Phases of
self-regulation Cognition Motivation Behavior Context
Forethought/ Setting goals for learning and Achievement goal adoption Setting behavioral and time Considering and selecting contexts
planning understanding management goals for learning
Activation of cognitive Activation of motivational beliefs and Recall of prior behavior and Perceptions of context, situation, task
and metacognitive knowledge attitudes experiences
Planning/selecting cognitive Planning/selecting motivational strategies Planning/selecting cognitive Planning/selecting context-relevant
strategies strategies strategies
Monitoring Monitoring of cognitive processes Monitoring of motivation and affect Monitoring of effort, time use, Monitoring of task and context conditions
behavior Monitoring need for help
Control/ Enacting and regulating cognitive, Enacting and regulating motivational and Enacting and regulating Enacting and regulating context,
management metacognitive, critical thinking affective strategies behavioral strategies help-seeking strategies
strategies
Reaction/ Learning and cognitive judgments, Affective reactions, generation of Choice behavior, generation of Generation of new context-related
reflection generation of cognition-related motivational and affect-related beliefs behavior-related knowledge knowledge
knowledge
Note: Adapted from Pintrich (2004)
C.A. Wolters and D.J. Taylor
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective on Student Engagement 639

decided to undertake a learning activity, voli- Areas of Self-regulated Learning


tional processes are used to protect those inten-
tions from distractions or possible temptations As illustrated in Table 30.1, these subprocesses
that would thwart efforts to complete the task. represented in these phases are used by students
The fourth phase proposed in this model, to manage their own academic functioning with
termed reaction or reflection, includes students’ regard to at least four areas of learning (Pintrich,
efforts to review and respond to the information 2004; Wolters et al., 2005). Put differently, these
produced through monitoring and feedback and four areas represent different dimensions of aca-
to their experiences with a task more generally. demic learning that can be the target of regulation
One core aspect of this phase is the generation of by the learner. One dimension, cognition, con-
new metalevel knowledge about the tasks, strate- cerns the various mental processes individuals
gies, or self. For instance, Winne and Hadwin use to encode, process, or learn while partici-
(1998) argued that it is through this process that pating in academic tasks (Pintrich, 2004). Most
students obtain object-level information used to typically, this area has included students’ use of
adapt their approach to task engagement in order cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies.
to reduce discrepancies between actual perfor- For example, students monitor and control their
mance and ideal standards found during monitor- use of rehearsal, organization, elaboration, or
ing. In line with this idea, Zimmerman (2000) other information-processing strategies needed to
stated that students self-evaluate their current per- learn new material or skills. Researchers from an
formance with some preset standard or goal and information-processing perspective have stressed
produce self-judgments about that performance the significance of this cognitive dimension
during this phase of self-regulated learning. (Winne & Hadwin, 1998). For instance, informa-
As a general rule, theorists do not view these tion in long-term memory may be retrieved for
different phases as a strict time-ordered sequence use in working memory, different knowledge,
or as causally connected in a linear fashion ideas, or beliefs may be restructured in a manner
(Pintrich, 2004; Winne & Hadwin, 2008; that is beneficial for the task, or information rep-
Zimmerman, 2000). Rather, the phases simply resented in one form (e.g., an image) may be con-
provide a structure and emphasize that self-regu- verted into another form (e.g., words). Although
lated learning is dependent on students’ active less often studied, this area would also include
engagement before, during, and after the comple- students’ engagement in critical thinking, problem
tion of academic tasks. Self-regulated learners solving, or other forms of higher-order reasoning.
are expected to engage or re-engage in the pro- Students can, that is, set goals and monitor, regu-
cesses endemic to particular phases at any time in late, and reflect on their use of problem-solving
a cyclic, flexible, and adaptive fashion so that strategies and efforts at critical thinking.
they can efficiently and successfully complete Motivation refers both to the process through
their academic work. For example, a student which goal-directed behavior is instigated and
might first set goals for a paper that is due in a few sustained (Schunk, Pintrich, & Meece, 2008) as
weeks. As the student begins to research and write, well as an individual’s willingness to engage in
he monitors his progress and begins to realize that and persist at academic tasks (Wolters, 2003a).
his initial plans for completing the paper need to As such, motivation represents a second dimen-
be altered, so he sets new goals. Also, he wonders sion of learning that individuals can self-regulate
if outlining the paper as he continues would be (Boekaerts, 1996; Pintrich, 2004; Wolters,
beneficial, so he uses that strategy. He again mon- 2003a). In other words, their own level of moti-
itors his progress and acknowledges that this vation or motivational processing represents an
method is useful for accomplishing his goals. important target for students who are working to
After completing the paper, this student recog- manage their own learning. Motivational pro-
nizes the advantages of tactics he used and cesses or beliefs tend to promote intentions to
resolves to utilize them again in the future. complete a task or to learn new information
640 C.A. Wolters and D.J. Taylor

(Corno, 2001). Students can establish goals or who could be involved with the task. As an exam-
form expectations about the level of motivation ple, help-seeking strategies in which students
they anticipate in a task, they can maintain an manage their learning by effectively utilizing
awareness of how motivated (or unmotivated) teachers, parents, peers, or others within the
they are feeling, and they can work to control social environment fit within this dimension
their level of motivation for completing a task. (Newman, 1998). After a study session, students
Prior work has identified several kinds of strate- can reflect on whether a particular environment is
gies that students use to sustain or improve their conducive to learning or form expectations about
own motivation. These strategies include self- the advantages and disadvantages of trying to do
provided rewards, self-talk about the importance work within a particular context or in collabora-
or usefulness of material, and making learning tion with specific people.
activities into a game so they are more enjoyable Although it is possible to distinguish among
(Sansone & Thoman, 2006; Wolters, 2003a). them conceptually, these four dimensions over-
Although some models of self-regulation have lap and intertwine with one another in practice
stressed emotional processing more exclusively (Pintrich, 2004). Self-regulating the functioning
(e.g., Carver & Scheier, 2000), affective pro- associated with one area (e.g., motivation) may
cesses often have been grouped together with also involve changes in the functioning within
motivation within this one area (e.g., Pintrich). In the other areas (e.g., cognition, behavior). As an
either case, models of self-regulated learning example, students may set a particular goal (e.g.,
commonly assume that students can manage their go to the library to study for 2 h in the afternoon
emotional processes as another important dimen- with classmates) based on the implications it has
sion of their academic functioning (Corno, 1993; for their cognition (e.g., additional resources at
Schutz & Davis, 2000). hand if needed), motivation (e.g., studying with
According to this model, a third area that stu- friends is more enjoyable), behavior (e.g., study-
dents can self-regulate is their overt behaviors, ing in the afternoon frees up time in the evening),
actual participation, conduct, or other physical and context (e.g., can get help from friends).
actions necessary for the completion of learning Hence, students’ overall efforts to plan and con-
tasks. Time management strategies that students trol where, when, and how they complete aca-
use to organize and control where and when they demic tasks likely involve consideration of all
study fit into this area. Pintrich (2004) also indi- four of these different areas.
cated that part of time management includes stu-
dents’ decisions about how they will apportion
their efforts to complete the task. The observation Comparing Self-regulated Learning
of one’s own behavior is also consistent with and Student Engagement
Zimmerman’s (2000) model of self-regulated
learning. In this section, we evaluate relations between self-
A fourth dimension of learning that is a poten- regulated learning and student engagement. This
tial target of students’ regulation is the context or discussion is based most directly on the model of
environment (Pintrich, 2004). This area includes self-regulation described above and a view of stu-
facets of the immediate task, classroom, or even dent engagement consistent with Fredricks et al.
cultural environment that students can monitor (2004). However, additional perspectives from
and manage. Students, for instance, might set both frameworks are also included when relevant.
goals and monitor and control the lighting, tem- Initially, several conceptual similarities between
perature, and noise level in their study environ- the two areas of research with regard to cognitive,
ment. In line with this view, Corno (2001) emotional, and behavioral functioning are dis-
explained that environmental control includes cussed. These similarities indicate that, as a whole,
both changes made to task circumstances and models of self-regulated learning and student
changes made toward the actions of other people engagement appear consistent with one another
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective on Student Engagement 641

with regard to the characteristics and forms of Also in line with the model of self-regulated
academic functioning attributed to highly effec- learning, researchers have further characterized
tive students. Next, points on which the two cognitive engagement as involving increased
frameworks differ or appear more incongruous metacognitive awareness and use of metacogni-
with one another are considered. tive control strategies (Appleton et al., 2006;
Fredricks et al., 2004; Furlong & Christenson,
2008). Strategies that reflect planning, goal set-
Similarities ting, and monitoring, for instance, are staples of
how theorists describe the metacognitive activi-
The extent of conceptual overlap between these ties displayed by both self-regulated learners and
two frameworks is perhaps most apparent when students who are cognitively engaged (Blumenfeld
considering the area of cognition or cognitive et al., 2006; Fredricks et al., 2004; Pintrich, et al.,
engagement. One might easily argue that there is 2000; Yazzie-Mintz, 2007). Even when they are
little practical difference between what research- not clearly labeled as metacognitive, empirical
ers studying student engagement describe as measures of students’ cognitive engagement often
high levels of cognitive engagement and what include planning, monitoring, and other types of
others identify as the cognitive aspects of self- control strategies that do reflect this type of pro-
regulated learning. In line with the model of self- cessing (Appleton et al., 2006; Greene et al., 2004;
regulated learning, one core element of student’s National Center for School Engagement, 2006).
cognitive engagement is the increased use of The conceptual consistency between these two
cognitive strategies that promote the encoding frameworks is even more obvious when research-
and retention of the material to be learned ers examining cognitive engagement explicitly
(Appleton et al., 2006; Fredricks et al., 2004; define it as including students’ self-regulation or
Reschly et al., 2008). These strategies typically use of self-regulation strategies (Appleton et al.,
include efforts at rehearsal, elaboration, summa- 2008; Furlong & Christenson, 2008; Kortering &
rization, organization, as well as more domain- Christenson, 2009). As an example, Furlong and
specific learning strategies (e.g., math algorithms, Christenson incorporated the idea that students
writing strategies). For instance, Blumenfeld, self-regulate their performance as part of dis-
Kempler, and Krajcik (2006) defined cognitive playing cognitive engagement. Other researchers
engagement as including students’ willingness to have even more plainly stated that being a self-
expend effort to learn through utilizing cogni- regulated learner typifies cognitive engagement
tive, metacognitive, and volitional strategies that (Fredricks et al., 2004; Sinclair, Christenson,
enhance understanding. They further discussed Lehr, & Anderson, 2003). Overall, both theoreti-
that learning strategies can be classified as either cal frameworks converge on the belief that more
superficial or deep. Strategies involving mne- efficient, more effective, and higher quality
monics and rehearsal reflect more superficial engagement follows when students utilize an
cognitive engagement, whereas those for elabo- array of cognitive strategies and have the meta-
ration and organization reflect deeper-level cognitive knowledge and skills necessary to
thinking and engagement. In some cases, there is deploy and manage these strategies successfully.
little empirical difference between how these A comparable level of conceptual agreement
processes are operationalized. For instance, as emerges when considering emotional and affec-
part of their measurement of cognitive engage- tive processes within academic contexts. Students
ment in high school students, Greene, Miller, viewed as self-regulated learners, generally, are
Crowson, Duke, and Akey (2004) included thought to have more positive and fewer negative
a self-report subscale of study strategy use that emotional experiences within academic settings
is substantially parallel to items used to mea- (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2000; Schutz & Davis,
sure cognitive aspects of self-regulated learning 2000). In particular, they tend to report a constel-
(Wolters, 2004). lation of emotional experiences with regard
642 C.A. Wolters and D.J. Taylor

academics that includes increased enjoyment, influence on students’ academic performance


pride, feelings of autonomy, and other positive (Kondo, 1997; Schutz & Davis, 2000; Schutz,
emotions, and decreased instances of anxiety, Davis, & Schwanenflugel, 2002). In a similar
shame, frustration, anger, or other negative emo- way, research on student engagement has also
tions (Pekrun, Goetz, Titz, & Perry, 2002). explicitly examined the role of negative emotions
Consistent with this work, but to an even including boredom, sadness, and anxiety. For
greater extent, researchers studying student instance, Yazzie-Mintz (2007) indicated that
engagement also portray students’ emotional and about two out of every three students in the high
affective processing as an important dimension schools he studied felt bored every day in school
of their academic functioning. In particular, many and that the vast majority also reported that they
prominent models identify emotional or affective did not like the school or the teachers. Within the
processing as one major form of student engage- Skinner et al. (2009) model of student engage-
ment (Finn, 1989; Fredricks et al., 2004; Skinner ment, disengagement is important to conceptu-
et al., 2009; Yazzie-Mintz, 2007). Within these alize as well. This model of engagement includes
models and most similar to the research on self- the concept of disaffection which encompasses
regulated learning, students’ emotional engage- lethargic emotions (e.g., boredom, tired), alien-
ment has been evidenced by their affective ated emotions (e.g., frustration, anger), and
experiences in response to their schoolwork coerced participation. Within both frameworks,
(Fredricks et al., 2004). For instance, positive these negative forms of emotions are viewed as
emotions such as interest, happiness, and excite- precursors to a host of maladaptive academic
ment have been studied as indicators of students’ outcomes such as disaffection, withdrawal of
emotional engagement (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; effort, and lack of investment in school tasks.
Skinner & Belmont, 1993). In addition, emotional Models of self-regulated learning and student
engagement has also been understood as a func- engagement also share the view that overt
tion of students’ relationship with their teacher, behaviors represent an important facet of stu-
their sense of belonging, and their general identi- dents’ academic functioning that need to be
fication with school (Finn, 1989; Fredricks et al., understood and explained. Evidence that students
2004; Goodenow, 1993; Stipek, 2002). For are self-regulating their behavior is typically indi-
instance, Finn described the emotional dimension cated by examining their level of effort, persis-
of engagement as relating to identification with tence, or time on task (Corno, 1993; Pintrich,
the school and includes the concepts of belonging 2004; Schunk & Zimmerman, 2003; Vrugt &
and valuing. The degree to which students are Oort, 2008). Individuals’ use of time management
attached and committed to the school indicates strategies to plan when and where they complete
their emotional involvement in the school. The academic tasks also reflects this facet of self-reg-
more the students identify with the school, the ulation (Housand & Reis, 2008; Kitsantas,
stronger emotional engagement they exhibit. Winsler, & Huie, 2008; Stoeger & Ziegler, 2008).
Yazzie-Mintz echoed this conceptualization of In line with this perspective but elaborated to
emotional engagement in what he called “engage- a greater degree, the behavioral aspect of student
ment of the heart” (p. 8). He described emotional engagement includes three main ideas (Finn,
engagement as students’ feelings of where they 1989, 1993; Finn & Voelkl, 1993; Fredricks et al.,
fit in their school, how well the school functions, 2004). In some models, forms of behavioral
and their interactions with people in their school. involvement are termed academic rather than
In addition to work examining more positive behavioral engagement (Appleton et al., 2006;
emotions, researchers examining self-regulated Furlong & Christenson, 2008). Whereas behav-
learning have also studied more negative emo- ioral engagement reflects students’ attendance,
tional experiences within academic contexts. classroom participation, overt behavioral effort,
Most notably, there is extensive work examining and involvement in extracurricular activities,
the importance of test anxiety as one maladaptive academic engagement refers more to time spent
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective on Student Engagement 643

on learning tasks, amount of assignments com- and other forms of self-handicapping as indicators
pleted, and credits earned in school. One dimen- of lapses in students’ self-regulation (Stoeger &
sion of behavioral engagement is evidenced when Ziegler, 2008; Wolters, 2003b). In line with this
students simply attend class each day, follow work, some research on student engagement has
school rules, abide by classroom norms, and do investigated students’ withdrawal of efforts in
not participate in troublesome behaviors. This learning activities, such as merely pretending to
facet reflects aspects of behavior that have been work during class (Skinner et al., 2009). In gen-
described as universal in that they are expected of eral, however, research concentrating on mal-
all students (Furlong, Whipple, St. Jean, Simental, adaptive behaviors leading to disengagement has
& Punthuna, 2003). investigated more global behaviors such as lack of
Most similar to the research in self-regulated school attendance and conduct problems,
learning, another aspect of behavioral engage- (Appleton et al., 2008; Finn & Rock, 1997;
ment consists of students being more involved in Skinner et al., 2009).
learning and academic tasks and displaying overt
behaviors that reflect effort, persistence, and
adaptive help-seeking. This aspect of behavioral Differences
engagement involves behaviors that reflect stu-
dents showing initiative in their learning, dis- Despite these many consistencies, there also are
playing more enthusiasm, actively participating points on which these two areas of research
in classroom activities, and spending more time diverge and that counter the notion that models of
on their work (Finn & Rock, 1997). Finally, self-regulated learning and student engagement
behavioral engagement also includes students’ can fit together seamlessly. In the following dis-
participation in behaviors that reflect optional cussion, we review three particular incongruities
forms of involvement such as band, athletics, involving conceptualization and organization
school governance, and other extracurricular across the frameworks, the centrality of personal
interests (Furlong et al., 2003). This final dimen- agency, and the reach of metalevel knowledge.
sion suggests behavioral engagement that goes Within this discussion, moreover, we highlight
beyond what is expected of the typical student. issues involving motivation.
Unlike this work on student engagement, few Most basically, it is apparent when examining
studies examining self-regulated learning have the components of each framework that there are
focused on explaining the broader educational incongruities with regard to the categorizations
behaviors such as enrollments, graduation, atten- used to differentiate among certain concepts that
dance, or extracurricular activities. Research are very similar. As well, there are some clear dif-
examining whether self-regulated learners evi- ferences in the particular constructs that have
denced increased participation in extracurricular emerged and been examined most closely within
activities or whether this participation can be each framework. As noted previously, for
seen as an outgrowth of their self-regulatory instance, there is differential treatment of con-
functioning is particularly scarce. The important structs such as sense of belonging, identity, and
role that involvement in these activities can play help-seeking across these two areas of research.
with regard to adolescents’ resilience to an array As well, there are clear differences in the atten-
of risky behaviors suggests that this is a serious tion given to behavioral forms of engagement
oversight (Feldman & Matjasko, 2005; Linver, such as course taking, graduation, and involve-
Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2009). ment in extracurricular activities. Space limita-
Indicators of students’ academic involvement tions prohibit any exhaustive review of all these
within both frameworks also include more mal- differences. Instead, we focus our discussion on
adaptive forms of behavior. Research on self- the differences that appear with regard to the
regulated learning, for instance, has sought to understanding and categorization of motivation
understand procrastination, defensive pessimism, across the two frameworks.
644 C.A. Wolters and D.J. Taylor

Unlike the history of research on student cause of self-regulation and not actually part of
engagement, motivation has been included as an the process itself (Pintrich & De Groot, 1990).
important part of what it means to be a self-regu- Motivation was seen as providing the drive,
lated learner from its inception. In an oft-quoted energy, or force that instigated and sustained the
description, for instance, Zimmerman (1986) processes necessary for self-regulation but was
characterized self-regulated learners as metacog- not necessarily inherent to the system itself. As
nitively, motivationally, and behaviorally active well, some descriptions of self-regulated learning
participants in their own learning (emphasis have focused more exclusively on highlighting
added). Other early descriptions also incorpo- the cognitive, metacognitive, or information-
rated the importance of students’ motivation to processing aspects of the model (Winne, 2001).
the process of self-regulated learning (McCombs More recently, however, motivation has consis-
& Marzano, 1990; Paris & Oka, 1986; Pintrich & tently been viewed as so deeply integrated and
De Groot, 1990; Schunk, 1990). Motivation has vital that it is considered part of the process of
only more recently been included in explanations self-regulated learning and not separate from it
of the main components of student engagement (Pintrich, 2004; Winne & Hadwin, 2008; Wolters,
(Appleton et al., 2006; Fredricks et al, 2004; 2003a). From this latter point of view, being
Jimerson et al., 2003). motivated and being a self-regulated learner are
Through its history, furthermore, the research irreducibly linked in a larger system that explains
on self-regulated learning has incorporated a wide students’ academic functioning. That is, motiva-
variety of different motivational constructs. tional processes play a vital and ongoing role
Perhaps most prominently, students’ beliefs con- throughout the self-regulation of learning.
cerning their ability to successfully complete aca- Motivation is not simply a catalyst that ignites a
demic tasks have been highlighted as a central process that then continues unabated and unten-
motivational construct within models of self- ded until a task is completed.
regulated learning (Pintrich, 1999; Schunk, 1990; This distinction and relation between cognition
Zimmerman, 1989). The importance of self- and motivation is less well established within the
efficacy was presented by Bandura (1986, 1997) research on student engagement. In this work,
as vital to cognitive and behavioral functioning motivation is not often considered a separate form
and to self-regulation. Focusing more specifically of involvement but is incorporated into what it
on self-regulated learning, self-efficacy has con- means to be cognitively and emotionally engaged.
tinued to be stressed by some of the most influen- For instance, according to Fredricks et al. (2004),
tial researchers in this area (Schunk & Ertmer, cognitive engagement is reflected in those who
2000; Zimmerman, 2000). Achievement goal the- value learning and exert efforts to understand or
ory has also been incorporated repeatedly into master certain knowledge or skills. Cognitively
models of self-regulated learning (Fryer & Elliot, engaged students include those who espouse
2008; Pintrich, 1999; Wolters, Yu, & Pintrich, learning goals as opposed to performance goals,
1996). As well, others have detailed the role of strive to understand material, master a task, and
motivational constructs such as value, interest, persist in challenging activities. Others have also
and autonomy in the process of self-regulated described cognitive engagement as including
learning (Hidi & Ainley, 2008; Reeve, Ryan, Deci, concepts such as value of school in terms of
& Jang, 2008; Wigfield, Hoa, & Klauda, 2008). future plans, goal setting, and sense of autonomy,
Within most of this research, motivation is (Appleton et al., 2006; Reschly et al., 2008). At
conceptualized and considered distinct from the same time, emotional engagement is also
other areas of functioning, especially cognition explained in a way that incorporates motivational
and metacognition but also behavior, context, concepts. Skinner et al. (2009) described engaged
and in some cases even emotion. At times, this emotions as those that indicate energized emo-
differentiation of motivation from cognitive and tional states, like enthusiasm, enjoyment, and
metacognitive processing has extended to a point interest. Although this model includes interest as
that motivation has been described more as a an engaged emotion, only the state of being “caught
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective on Student Engagement 645

and held” (p. 495) is used in the conceptualiza- prime example of this distinction. Wolters (1998,
tion of emotional engagement, as opposed to 2003a, 2011), for instance, described the strate-
including factors that catch and hold students’ gies that students use to actively and purposefully
interest. In sum, motivation is more intricately manage their motivational processing. In one
weaved into conceptualizations of what is viewed study, college students were presented with 12
as cognitive and emotional engagement. different situations by crossing four types of aca-
Consistent with most contemporary models of demic situations and three motivational prob-
achievement motivation (Schunk et al., 2008), lems. Students were asked to consider each
views of self-regulated learning would more typi- situation and identify what they might do to over-
cally view these as forms of motivation and there- come the motivational problem, provide effort,
fore conceptually distinct from cognition. and complete the task. Analysis of their written
Less apparent, but perhaps more acute, another responses revealed more than ten distinct types
distinction between the research on self-regulated of strategies that students identified as ways of
learning and student engagement is the centrality sustaining or improving their level of motivation
that is attributed to the agency of students. within the situations. These strategies included
Characterizing it as a core aspect of his social efforts to give themselves rewards, use self-talk,
cognitive theory, Bandura (2006) described reduce distractions, and make the task into a
agency as the ability of individuals to influence game (Wolters, 1998). Using a forced-choice
their own cognitive and behavioral functioning. survey based on the strategies found in this early
This view of individuals as potentially influential study, additional work showing that some early
in their own functioning stands in contrast to adolescents have and use a variety of these moti-
more deterministic views of behavioral and aca- vational strategies provides further support for
demic outcomes. At their core, models of self- the importance of this type of engagement
regulated learning adopt this assumption about (Wolters, 1999; Wolters & Rosenthal, 2000).
human agency and focus on understanding stu- Others have studied similar issues with regard
dents’ academic engagement as a function of their to students’ emotional processes during academic
own purposeful, planful, or goal-directed efforts tasks, especially during tests (Corno, 1993; Kondo,
(Greene & Azevedo, 2007; Winne & Hadwin, 1997; Schutz et al., 2002). In this work, it is pre-
2008; Zimmerman, 2000). From this standpoint, sumed that students can set goals about the emo-
students ultimately qualify as self-regulated tions they want or expect to experience and can
learners only to the extent that their active engage- monitor whether they experience those emotions.
ment in academic contexts is a function of agentic As well, they can take steps to manipulate their
processes. Students coerced to finish worksheets affect and reflect on their affective experience
using specific tactics rigidly dictated by a teacher when a task is complete. For instance, students in
may appear cognitively and behaviorally engaged a testing situation might monitor their emotional
but likely would not be considered self-regulated. state and take steps to reduce feelings of anxiety,
Models of student engagement generally do rec- frustration, or anger (Schutz & Davis, 2000).
ognize and incorporate this planfulness within the Finally, theoretical models of self-regulated
metacognitive aspects of cognitive engagement. learning propose that students also plan, monitor,
However, models of self-regulated learning control, and reflect on different aspects of their
expand the ability to be purposeful beyond the behavior. As an example, students may employ
cognitive domain into other areas, including organizational and other time management strate-
motivation, context, and behavior. That is, mod- gies as a way to control their overt behaviors
els of self-regulated learning assume that students related to studying (Housand & Reis, 2008;
can be consciously aware and purposefully inter- Kitsantas et al., 2008; Stoeger & Ziegler, 2008).
vene to improve and reflect on their actions with In sum, models of self-regulated learning have
regard to a wider set of processes. highlighted the ability of students to assume a
The ability to set goals, monitor, reflect upon, purposeful and agentic role across many different
and manage one’s own motivation serves as a facets of their academic functioning. In contrast,
646 C.A. Wolters and D.J. Taylor

beyond the importance of metacognition, models domain-specific beliefs provide a foundation


of student engagement do not stress this type of necessary for the regulation of motivation. In
agency when considering students’ academic particular, she emphasized the term metamotiva-
functioning. Rather, these models focus on defin- tional to describe individuals’ knowledge or
ing engagement as a state or event based on what understanding of their own motivation and
students are doing, feeling, or thinking and less motivational processing more generally.
on the underlying explanations for why they are Metamotivational knowledge provides the aware-
doing it (Furlong & Christenson, 2008). Put dif- ness necessary for students’ to plan, monitor, and
ferently, models of student engagement are not manage their level of motivation within academic
restricted by the assumption of agency or pur- tasks. In line with this work, Wolters (2003a,
posefulness in students’ academic functioning. 2011) also argued for the importance of this type
Instead, these models allow that students’ engage- of knowledge with regard to students’ regulation
ment can result from a more diverse set of influ- of their motivation. In his view, students need
ences that include contextual, social, family, and declarative, procedural, and conditional knowl-
instructional factors. Models of self-regulated edge about motivational strategies in order to use
learning acknowledge these influences but tend them effectively to sustain or improve motiva-
to focus on how they play a role in fostering or tion. Empirical evidence for the importance of
hampering students’ efforts to manage their own this knowledge about motivation strategies
learning. comes from Cooper and Corpus (2009). Using
A third closely related, but theoretically dis- interviews based on short fictional scenarios,
crete, disparity in these two frameworks is the these researchers found that adults demonstrated
significance and reach attributed to various types more knowledge of the effectiveness of motiva-
of metalevel knowledge. Researchers from both tional strategies than older elementary students
traditions agree that students’ metalevel knowl- who, in turn, evidenced greater understanding
edge regarding their cognitive functioning is than younger students. In contrast to this research
critical. Metacognitive knowledge about the self, based on models of self-regulated learning,
tasks, and strategies is one basis for students’ researchers examining student engagement have
cognitive engagement and serves as a basis for not explicitly identified these additional forms of
effective self-regulation of cognitive functioning metalevel knowledge as important or incorpo-
(Blumenfeld et al., 2006; Winne & Hadwin, rated them into their models. Rather, students’
1998). Within both models, students who have conscious awareness, understanding, or meta-
greater awareness, understanding, and knowledge level knowledge regarding their functioning typ-
relevant to academic task are more likely to be ically is restricted to considering metacognitive
engaged and efficient learners. Descriptions of knowledge.
self-regulated learning, however, move beyond
this assumption by incorporating metalevel
knowledge associated with other areas of aca- Concluding Remarks and Directions
demic functioning (Boekaerts, 1996; Pintrich, for Research
2004; Winne & Hadwin, 2008; Wolters, 2003a).
That is, self-regulated learners also are presumed Overall, there is substantial overlap among
to have metalevel knowledge that provides the researchers studying self-regulated learning and
foundation necessary for planning, monitoring, student engagement with regard to many of the
and regulating their emotional, behavioral, and characteristics that are viewed as central to stu-
motivational processing. dents being effective, efficient, and high-per-
This more far-reaching emphasis on metalevel forming learners. These similarities suggest that
knowledge is well illustrated when considering students who are characterized as self-regulated
motivation. For instance, Boekaerts (1996, 1997) learners will exhibit the types of cognitive activi-
has described how students’ knowledge or ties, emotional experiences, and overt behaviors
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective on Student Engagement 647

that reflect increased student engagement. One involvement in extracurricular activities (Finn &
obvious conclusion, therefore, is that the research Rock, 1997; National Center for School
on self-regulated learning and student engage- Engagement, 2006). Yet, there is little research
ment can, and should, be integrated to a greater examining how students might actively manage
extent. A more active integration of these two their engagement in these broader types of aca-
areas of research should benefit each of them demic behaviors. For instance, to what extent do
separately, as well as the broader goal of under- students purposefully plan their involvement in
standing and improving students’ functioning extracurricular activities in service of learning
within academic contexts. goals? This type of research is needed to comple-
At the same time, there are some points on ment the ongoing efforts to examine students’
which these two theoretical frameworks show self-regulated learning using relatively short-
greater divergence. In many cases, however, term, computerized tasks or self-reported study
these issues do not appear intractable but rather behaviors. These latter efforts provide valuable
signify a need for greater conceptual integration insights into the cognitive and metacognitive
and opportunities for research. Disparities in how processes important to self-regulated learning
particular constructs are labeled or the emphasis with regard to small grain size behaviors. Self-
they are given within each framework reflect this regulation and engagement that plays out over
type of difference. It is worth noting, further- more extended periods and involves all aspects
more, that conformity in all areas is absent even of students’ functioning (e.g., cognitive, motiva-
with regard to models within each framework. tional, emotional, behavioral) is also needed.
For instance, some models of student engage- A second and related recommendation for
ment make distinctions among three types of additional research emerging from this evaluation
engagement, whereas others expand this to four is the need to expand the work on self-regulated
types. As well, even the most prominent models learning to better account for the additional types
show variation in the processes, skills, or abilities of engagement shown to be important. One spe-
that are viewed as most central to self-regulated cific need is for work that integrates emotional
learning. In the end, it is likely that most concep- forms of engagement such as sense of belonging
tual discrepancies would not provide an insur- and identity more thoroughly. Interestingly, the
mountable obstacle to the development of a more research on development of the self or identity
integrated model of self-regulated learning and has received relatively little attention by research-
student engagement. ers examining self-regulated learners. To be sure,
One point that emerges when considering both some researchers have stressed the importance of
the commonalities and differences across these better understanding these aspects of being a self-
two frameworks is the need for additional research regulated learner (McCombs, 1989; Paris, Byrnes,
that advances their integration. Here, we identify & Paris, 2001; Roeser & Peck, 2009). As well, it
several recommendations that would both serve is clear that self-related beliefs (e.g., self-concept,
to integrate these two frameworks and advance self-efficacy) are central to all models of self-
the broader understanding of what makes students regulated learning. Nonetheless, few empirical
more effective, more efficient learners within aca- studies have examined specifically how individu-
demic contexts. Based on our own background, als’ development of identity or self is connected to
we frame these ideas for research as ways to their ability to engage in self-regulated learning.
improve the work on self-regulated learning. Also, students’ sense of belonging or identifica-
One recommendation is for additional research tion with school is not well understood as either
that documents students’ efforts to plan, monitor, influences or outcomes of the self-regulated
and manage their participation in broader, larger learning process. Yet, researchers have found that
grain, or longer term academic behaviors. Prior junior high school students who perceived greater
research demonstrates the importance of aca- interpersonal support and had a stronger sense of
demic behaviors such as attending class and belonging reported higher motivation and were
648 C.A. Wolters and D.J. Taylor

more likely to show increased effort in school and A final recommendation is for a more com-
to obtain higher grades (Goodenow, 1993; plete treatment of context as an area that can be
Goodenow & Grady, 1993). Self-regulated learning self-regulated. Pintrich (2000, 2004) has argued
processes such as planning, monitoring, and con- convincingly that students can and do work to
trol could be instrumental as explanations for manage different facets of their context in order
these relations. to reach learning goals. Individuals’ efforts to
Expanding the research examining how stu- structure or control their environment are a dis-
dents’ metalevel knowledge influences their tinct part of volitional accounts of self-regulation
engagement and self-regulated learning would (Corno, 1993, 2001), and processes can also be
also be fruitful. Past work has tended to focus on found in other prominent models (Zimmerman,
metacognitive knowledge about learning strate- 2000). Whether or how this type of self-regulation
gies. Less well understood is students’ under- of the environment fits with models of student
standing, beliefs, or knowledge regarding other engagement is uncertain. Ultimately, work tar-
aspects of their learning. Questions remain about geting each of these issues will advance both
students’ metalevel knowledge about motivation, theoretical frameworks and allow for a more
emotion, behavior, and contexts (Wolters, 2003a, robust and integrated understanding of students’
2011). Assuming it is connected to students’ self-regulated learning and engagement.
engagement and self-regulated learning, it would
also be good to better understand how it is fos-
tered or supported by instructional practices. References
Another recommendation arises from the dif-
ferent emphasis within these two frameworks on Appleton, J., Christenson, S., & Furlong, M. (2008).
the purposefulness or planfulness of students’ Student engagement with school: Critical conceptual
engagement. Models of self-regulated learning, and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology
in the Schools, 45, 369–386.
more so than those concerning student engage-
Appleton, J., Christenson, S., Kim, D., & Reschly, A.
ment, have emphasized the importance of stu- (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological engage-
dents being goal-directed or purposeful in their ment: Validation of the Student Engagement Instrument.
actions. At the same time, there is reason to ques- Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445.
Bandura, A. (1986). Self-regulatory mechanisms. In A.
tion the extent to which students alone are really
Bandura (Ed.), Social foundations of thought and
consciously responsible for controlling their own action: A social–cognitive theory (pp. 335–389).
learning processes (Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2004; Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
McCaslin & Hickey, 2001). These questions sug- Bandura, A. (1997). Self–efficacy: The exercise of control.
New York: W. H. Freeman.
gest that more research is needed to document
Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human
better how often and under what conditions stu- agency. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1,
dents’ engagement in different learning processes 164–180.
can actually be considered as self-regulated. Baumeister, R., & Vohs, K. (Eds.). (2004). Handbook of
self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications.
As is always the case, the success of future
New York: Guilford Press.
research will depend on the availability of instru- Blumenfeld, P. C., Kempler, T. M., & Krajcik, J. S. (2006).
ments, designs, and procedures that allow for the Motivation and cognitive engagement in learning
reliable and valid assessment of all the relevant environments. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge
handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 475–488). New
constructs. The research on self-regulated learning,
York: Cambridge University Press.
however, has often struggled with how best to Boekaerts, M. (1996). Self-regulated learning at the
assess this complex, multifaceted process (Winne junction of cognition and motivation. European
& Perry, 2000). Moving forward, there is a con- Psychologist, 1, 100–112.
Boekaerts, M. (1997). Self-regulated learning: A new
tinuing need for measures that better differentiate
concept embraced by researchers, policy makers, edu-
between the more event-like and more trait-like cators, teachers, and students. Learning and
aspects of self-regulated learning. Instruction, 7, 161–186.
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective on Student Engagement 649

Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R., & Zeidner, M. (Eds.). Furlong, M., & Christenson, S. (2008). Engaging students
(2000). Handbook of self-regulation: Theory, research, at school and with learning: A relevant construct for all
and applications. San Diego, CA: Academic. students. Psychology in the Schools, 45, 365–368.
Butler, D., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Feedback and self- Furlong, M., Whipple, A., St. Jean, G., Simental, J., &
regulated learning: A theoretical synthesis. Review of Punthuna, S. (2003). Multiple contexts of school
Educational Research, 65, 245–281. engagement: Moving toward a unifying framework
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). On the structure of for educational research and practice. The California
behavioral self-regulation. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, School Psychologist, 8, 99–113.
& M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation Goodenow, C. (1993). Classroom belonging among early
(pp. 41–84). San Diego, CA: Academic. adolescent students: Relationships to motivation and
Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., Appleton, J. J., Berman, S., achievement. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 13,
Spanjers, D., & Varro, P. (2008). Best practices in 21–43.
fostering student engagement. In A. Thomas & J. Goodenow, C., & Grady, K. (1993). The relationship of
Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology school belonging and friends’ values to academic
(5th ed., pp. 1099–1120). Bethesda, MD: National motivation among urban adolescent students. The
Association of School Psychologists. Journal of Experimental Education, 62, 60–71.
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, Greene, B., Miller, R., Crowson, H., Duke, B., & Akey, L.
autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of (2004). Predicting high school students’ cognitive
self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe engagement and achievement: Contributions of class-
(Eds.), Self processes and development (Vol. 23, pp. room perceptions and motivation. Contemporary
43–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Educational Psychology, 29, 462–482.
Cooper, C., & Corpus, J. (2009). Learners’ developing Greene, J., & Azevedo, R. (2007). A theoretical review of
knowledge of strategies for regulating motivation. Winne and Hadwin’s model of self-regulated learning:
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, New perspectives and directions. Review of
525–536. Educational Research, 77, 334–372.
Corno, L. (1993). The best–laid plans: Modern concep- Hidi, S., & Ainley, M. (2008). Interest and self-regulation:
tions of volition and educational research. Educational Relationships between two variables that influence
Researcher, 22(2), 14–22. learning. In D. Schunk & B. Zimmerman (Eds.),
Corno, L. (2001). Volitional aspects of self–regulated Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory,
learning. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self- research, and applications (pp. 77–109). Mahwah,
regulated learning and academic achievement: NJ: Erlbaum Associates.
Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 191–225). Housand, A., & Reis, S. (2008). Self-regulated learning in
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. reading: Gifted pedagogy and instructional settings.
Feldman, A., & Matjasko, J. (2005). The role of school- Journal of Advanced Academics, 20, 108–136.
based extracurricular activities in adolescent develop- Jimerson, S., Campos, E., & Greif, J. (2003). Toward an
ment: A comprehensive review and future directions. understanding of definitions and measures of school
Review of Educational Research, 75, 159–210. engagement and related terms. The California School
Finn, J. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Psychologist, 8, 7–27.
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. Kitsantas, A., Winsler, A., & Huie, F. (2008). Self-
Finn, J. (1993). School engagement and students at risk. regulation and ability predictors of academic success
Washington, DC: National Center for Educational during college: A predictive validity study. Journal of
Statistics. Advanced Academics, 20, 42–68.
Finn, J., & Rock, D. (1997). Academic success among Kondo, D. S. (1997). Strategies for coping with test anxi-
students at risk for school failure. Journal of Applied ety. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 10, 203–215.
Psychology, 82, 221–235. Kortering, L., & Christenson, S. (2009). Engaging stu-
Finn, J., & Voelkl, K. (1993). School characteristics dents in school and learning: The real deal for school
related to school engagement. The Journal of Negro completion. Exceptionality, 17, 5–15.
Education, 62, 249–268. Linnenbrink, E., & Pintrich, P. (2000). Multiple pathways
Fitzsimons, G., & Bargh, J. (2004). Automatic self-regu- to learning and achievement: The role of goal orienta-
lation. In R. Baumeister & K. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook tion in fostering adaptive motivation, affect, and cog-
of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications nition. In C. Sansone & J. Harackiewicz (Eds.),
(pp. 151–170). New York: Guilford Press. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for opti-
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). mal motivation and performance (pp. 195–254). San
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the Diego, CA: Academic.
evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59–109. Linver, M., Roth, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2009). Patterns
Fryer, J., & Elliot, A. (2008). Self-regulation and achieve- of adolescents’ participation in organized activities:
ment goal pursuit. In D. Schunk & B. Zimmerman Are sports best when combined with other activities?
(Eds.), Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory, Developmental Psychology, 45, 354–367.
research, and applications (pp. 53–75). Mahwah, NJ: McCaslin, M., & Hickey, D. (2001). Self-regulated learn-
Erlbaum Associates. ing and academic achievement: A Vygotskian view.
650 C.A. Wolters and D.J. Taylor

In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated Reeve, J., Ryan, R., Deci, E., & Jang, H. (2008).
learning and academic achievement: Theoretical per- Understanding and promoting autonomous self-regu-
spectives (2nd ed., pp. 227–252). Mahwah, NJ: lation: A self-determined theory perspective. In D.
Erlbaum Associates. Schunk & B. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and self-
McCombs, B. (1989). Self-regulated learning and aca- regulated learning: Theory, research, and applications
demic achievement: A phenomenological view. In B. (pp. 223–244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.
Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learn- Reschly, A., Huebner, E., Appleton, J., & Antaramian, S.
ing and academic achievement: Theory, research, and (2008). Engagement as flourishing: The contribution
practice (pp. 51–82). New York: Springer. of positive emotions and coping to adolescents’
McCombs, B., & Marzano, R. (1990). Putting the self in engagement at school and with learning. Psychology
self-regulated learning: The self as agent in integrating in the Schools, 45, 419–431.
will and skill. Educational Psychologist, 25, 51–69. Roeser, R., & Peck, S. (2009). An education in awareness:
National Center for School Engagement. (2006). Self, motivation, and self-regulated learning in con-
Quantifying school engagement: Research report. templative perspective. Educational Psychologist, 44,
Available at http://www.schoolengagement.org/. 119–136.
Accessed March 11, 2010. Russell, V. J., Ainley, M., & Frydenberg, E. (2005).
Newman, R. (1998). Adaptive help-seeking: A role of Schooling issues digest: Student motivation and engage-
social interaction in self–regulated learning. In S. ment. Canberra, Australia: Department of Education,
Karabenick (Ed.), Strategic help-seeking: Implications Science and Training, Australian Government.
for learning and teaching (pp. 13–37). Hillsdale, NJ: Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. (2006). Maintaining activity
Erlbaum Associates. engagement: Individual differences in the process of
Paris, S., Byrnes, J., & Paris, A. (2001). Constructing self-regulating motivation. Journal of Personality, 74,
theories, identities, and actions of self-regulated learners. 1697–1720.
In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated Schunk, D. (1990). Goal setting and self-efficacy during
learning and academic achievement: Theoretical per- self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 25,
spectives (2nd ed., pp. 253–287). Mahwah, NJ: 71–86.
Erlbaum Associates. Schunk, D., & Ertmer, P. (2000). Self-regulation and aca-
Paris, S., & Oka, E. (1986). Children’s reading strategies, demic learning: Self–efficacy enhancing interventions.
metacognition, and motivation. Developmental In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.),
Review, 6, 25–56. Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 631–649). San
Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. (2002). Diego, CA: Academic.
Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learn- Schunk, D., & Zimmerman, B. (2003). Self-regulation
ing and achievement: A program of qualitative and and learning. In W. Reynolds & G. Miller (Eds.),
quantitative research. Educational Psychologist, 37, Handbook of psychology: Vol. 7. Educational psychol-
91–105. ogy (pp. 59–78). New York: Wiley.
Pintrich, P. (1999). The role of motivation in promoting Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2008).
and sustaining self-regulated learning. International Motivation in education: Theory, research, applica-
Journal of Educational Research, 31, 459–470. tions (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson
Pintrich, P. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self– Education.
regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Schutz, P., & Davis, H. (2000). Emotions and self-regula-
Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 451– tion during test taking. Educational Psychologist, 35,
502). San Diego, CA: Academic. 243–256.
Pintrich, P. (2004). A conceptual framework for assessing Schutz, P., Davis, H., & Schwanenflugel, P. (2002).
motivation and self–regulated learning in college stu- Organization of concepts relevant to emotions and
dents. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 385–407. their regulation during test taking. The Journal of
Pintrich, P., & De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self– Experimental Education, 70, 316–342.
regulated learning components of classroom academic Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Lehr, C. A., & Anderson,
performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, A. R. (2003). Facilitating student engagement: Lessons
33–40. learned from Check & Connect longitudinal studies. The
Pintrich, P., Wolters, C., & Baxter, G. (2000). Assessing California School Psychologist, 8, 29–42.
metacognition and self–regulated learning. In G. Skinner, E., Kinderman, T., & Furrer, C. (2009). A moti-
Schraw (Ed.), Metacognitive assessment. Lincoln, vational perspective on engagement and disaffection:
NE: University of Nebraska Press. Conceptualization and assessment of children’s behav-
Pintrich, P., & Zusho, A. (2002). The development of aca- ioral and emotional participation in academic activi-
demic self-regulation: The role of cognitive and moti- ties in the classroom. Educational and Psychological
vational factors. In A. Wigfield & J. Eccles (Eds.), Measurement, 69, 493–525.
Development of achievement motivation (pp. 249–284). Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the
San Diego, CA: Academic. classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and
30 A Self-regulated Learning Perspective on Student Engagement 651

student engagement across the school year. Journal of Wolters, C. (2003a). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating
Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581. an underemphasized aspect of self–regulated learning.
Stipek, D. (2002). Good instruction is motivating. In A. Educational Psychologist, 38, 189–205.
Wigfield & J. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achieve- Wolters, C. (2003b). Understanding procrastination from
ment motivation (pp. 309–332). San Diego, CA: a self-regulated learning perspective. Journal of
Academic. Educational Psychology, 95, 179–187.
Stoeger, H., & Ziegler, A. (2008). Evaluation of a classroom Wolters, C. (2004). Advancing achievement goal theory:
based training to improve self-regulation in time man- Using goal structures and goal orientations to predict
agement tasks during homework activities with fourth students’ motivation, cognition, and achievement.
graders. Metacognition and Learning, 3, 207–230. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 236–250.
Vrugt, A., & Oort, F. (2008). Metacognition, achievement Wolters, C. (2011). Regulation of motivation: Contextual and
goals, study strategies, and academic achievement: social aspects. Teacher’s College Record, 113, 265–283.
Pathways to achievement. Metacognition and Wolters, C., Pintrich, P., & Karabenick, S. (2005). Assessing
Learning, 3, 123–146. academic self-regulated learning. In K. Moore & L.
Wigfield, A., Hoa, L., & Klauda, S. (2008). The role of Lippman (Eds.), What do children need to flourish?:
achievement values in the regulation of achievement Conceptualizing and measuring indicators of positive
behaviors. In D. Schunk & B. Zimmerman (Eds.), development (pp. 251–270). New York: Springer.
Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory, Wolters, C., & Rosenthal, H. (2000). The relation between
research, and applications (pp. 169–195). Mahwah, students’ motivational beliefs and attitudes and their
NJ: Erlbaum Associates. use of motivational regulation strategies. International
Winne, P. (2001). Self-regulated learning viewed from Journal of Educational Research, 33, 801–820.
models of information processing. In B. Zimmerman Wolters, C., Yu, S., & Pintrich, P. (1996). The relation
& D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and aca- between goal orientation and students’ motivational
demic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed., beliefs and self-regulated learning. Learning and
pp. 153–189). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. Individual Differences, 8, 211–238.
Winne, P., & Hadwin, A. (1998). Studying as self-regu- Yazzie-Mintz, E. (2007). Voices of students on engage-
lated learning. In D. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. ment: A report on the 2006 High School Survey of
Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory Student Engagement. Bloomington, IN: Center for
and practice (pp. 277–304). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Evaluation & Education Policy, Indiana University.
Winne, P., & Hadwin, A. (2008). The weave of motivation Zimmerman, B. (1986). Becoming a self–regulated
and self-regulated learning. In D. Schunk & B. learner: Which are the key processes? Contemporary
Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulated Educational Psychology, 11, 307–313.
learning: Theory, research, and applications (pp. Zimmerman, B. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-
297–314). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. regulated learning and academic learning. Journal of
Winne, P., & Perry, N. (2000). Measuring self-regulated Educational Psychology, 81, 329–339.
learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner Zimmerman, B. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A
(Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 531–566). social cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R.
San Diego, CA: Academic. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regu-
Wolters, C. (1998). Self-regulated learning and college lation: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 13–29).
students’ regulation of motivation. Journal of San Diego, CA: Academic.
Educational Psychology, 90, 224–235. Zimmerman, B. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner:
Wolters, C. (1999). The relation between high school stu- An overview. Theory Into Practice, 41, 64–70.
dents’ motivational regulation and their use of learn- Zimmerman, B., & Schunk, D. (Eds.). (2001). Self-regulated
ing strategies, effort, and classroom performance. learning and academic achievement: Theoretical per-
Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 281–299. spectives (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Classroom Strategies to Enhance
Academic Engaged Time 31
Maribeth Gettinger and Martha J. Walter

Abstract
A strong predictor of student achievement is the amount of time students
are actively engaged in learning, or academic engaged time (AET).
Sustained engagement, in turn, is influenced by the extent to which stu-
dents are motivated to invest time in learning. Despite the importance of
AET, studies reveal that engagement (determined by motivation) may be
as low as 45–50% in some classrooms. Beginning with a model developed
by Carroll in 1963, several theoretical conceptualizations of school learn-
ing have emphasized the critical role of engaged time in determining stu-
dent achievement. Subsequently, empirical studies focusing on the
relationship between time and learning have documented the role of the
instructional context in explaining both student motivation (willingness to
invest time in learning) and student engagement (actual involvement or
participation in learning). In addition to discussing theory and research
that implicate time in the teaching-learning process, this chapter describes
three groupings of evidence-based practices that contribute to student
engagement and motivation, including classroom management, instruc-
tional design, and student-mediated strategies.

view engagement as a psychological process


Introduction and Overview underlying students’ social and cognitive devel-
opment to more narrow conceptualizations that
Student engagement in learning contributes to
view engagement as a behavioral index of atten-
overall achievement and, in itself, is an important
tion to learning tasks (Marks, 2000). Notwith-
outcome of schooling. Various conceptualiza-
standing the significant contributions to academic
tions of student engagement have appeared in the
success made by students’ cognitive, affective,
literature, ranging from broad perspectives that
and social engagement in the schooling process,
the specific focus in this chapter is on time-based
indices of student engagement in learning. For
M. Gettinger (*) • M.J. Walter
Department of Educational Psychology,
purposes of this chapter, student engagement is
University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA conceptualized as direct, measurable involve-
e-mail: mgetting@wisc.edu; mjwalter@wisc.edu ment or participation in learning activities.

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 653


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_31, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
654 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

A strong predictor of academic achievement is was established in 1991 to conduct a comprehen-


the amount of time students are actively engaged sive examination of time use in American schools.
in learning. The link between academic engaged In 1994, the NECTL released its report, entitled
time (AET) and learning is one of the most endur- Prisoners of Time, which concluded, “it would be
ing and consistent findings in educational research unreasonable to believe that…the quality…of
(Gettinger & Ball, 2007). Simply put, learning American schools could be improved without
requires engagement or investment of time on the substantial changes in the amount and use of time
part of the learner. The greater the amount of time allowed for teachers and students to do their
students are engaged in learning, the higher their work” (p. 15). According to this report, despite
achievement. Student motivation is distinct from, longer school days, students in American schools
yet highly related to, student engagement. spend less time engaged in academic instruction
Specifically, engagement is determined, in large and learning than do students in other nations. In
part, by the extent to which students are moti- 2005, the NECTL updated and reissued its report
vated to participate in learning. In this chapter, to reiterate the critical need for better use of
student motivation is viewed as leading to stu- instructional time in schools and to underscore
dent engagement, with engagement being the the importance of academic engaged time for
point of entry for instruction. In other words, sus- enhancing achievement among all learners.
tained and continuous engagement in learning The purpose of this chapter is to address class-
over time (determined by motivation) is the room strategies designed to enhance academic
mechanism through which classroom instruction engaged time (AET). The chapter is predicated
directly influences student outcomes. on the contention that AET is a useful variable for
Reliable approaches to assessing levels of measuring instructional processes and that it
engaged learning often rely on observable crite- plays a key role in contributing to student achieve-
ria. Thus, the amount of observed time students ment. To examine the concept of AET, the chap-
are actively involved in learning serves as the ter begins with a historical review of theoretical
index of student engagement in both descriptive models and research paradigms that place time as
and empirical research on time and learning a central variable in school learning, in terms of
(Adelman, Haslam, & Pringle, 1996). To the both student engagement and motivation. Next,
extent that engagement is determined by motiva- academic engaged time is defined. The compo-
tion, the amount of time students choose to engage nents of AET are explained, and factors that
themselves in learning (i.e., under conditions of determine each AET component are delineated.
student choice or self-determined study time) has Finally, research-supported practices for maxi-
also been used as a time-based metric for stu- mizing students’ AET are reviewed, with a focus
dents’ motivation (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000). classroom management practices, instructional
Despite the importance of AET, descriptive approaches, and student-mediated strategies.
studies reveal that (a) as little as half of each
school day is typically devoted to academic
instruction, (b) students are engaged in learning Historical Perspectives
activities only 28–56% of the total time they
spend in school during a given year, and (c) the Both theory and research on the association
level of students’ on-task behavior may be as between time and learning affirm that time spent
low as 45% in some classrooms (Black, 2002; in learning, a construct closely related to AET, is
Fisher, 2009; Hollywood, Salisbury, Rainforth, a crucial factor in influencing achievement. In one
& Palombaro, 1995; Rangel, 2007; Smith, 2000). of the earliest reviews on the relationship between
In response to growing national concerns about time and learning, Fredrick and Walberg (1980)
declining levels of achievement and engage- found that the correlation between time spent in
ment among students, the National Education learning and achievement ranged from .13 to .71,
Commission on Time and Learning (NECTL) depending on how time was operationalized and
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 655

measured. Subsequent research on learning time require for learning and the amount of time they
during the 1980s and 1990s focused, first, on the actually engage in learning, the higher their level
extent to which differences in achievement among of mastery. As such, the primary metric for both
learners can be explained by time spent in learning student motivation and student engagement in
(e.g., Gettinger, 1985, 1989) and, second, on iden- Carroll’s model was time.
tifying instructional factors that maximize time Carroll identified five factors that influence
spent in learning and, in turn, achievement (Wang, either “time spent” or “time needed” in his model.
Haertel, & Walberg, 1993; Wyne & Stuck, 1982). Three factors determine time needed for learning:
A review of prominent theories and research on (a) student aptitude, (b) ability to understand
school learning that stress the role of time in learn- instruction, and (c) quality of instruction.
ing is provided below. Specifically, the higher a learner’s aptitude for
learning content and ability to comprehend
instruction, the less time needed for learning.
Theoretical Foundations for Academic Conversely, poor quality of instruction increases
Engaged Time the amount of time needed for learning beyond
what would be necessary under optimal condi-
Interest in engaged learning time can be traced to tions. Two factors affect time spent in learning:
several theories that implicated time in the learn- (a) time allocated for learning, or opportunity to
ing process, beginning with John Carroll’s origi- learn, and (b) perseverance, or the amount of time
nal model of school learning (1963). Collectively, the learner is willing to spend in learning, a vari-
multiple theoretical perspectives, described briefly able most closely related to student motivation.
in the following paragraphs, provided the founda- According to Carroll, the relationship between
tion for later empirical and applied research focus- these latter two factors and student learning tends
ing on the construct of AET and its importance for to be linear. Specifically, degree of learning is
school success. higher or lower to the extent that adequate learn-
ing time is provided. Moreover, learning will be
Carroll’s Model of School Learning incomplete if students are not motivated to spend
One of the earliest and most influential models the necessary amount of time for learning. Thus,
for school learning was proposed in 1963 by John in Carroll’s model, motivation leads to engage-
Carroll (1963, 1984, 1989). Carroll theorized ment. The measure Carroll proposed for opportu-
about the functional relationship between time nity to learn was the amount of time a teacher
variables and measures of learning. The major makes available for learning; the proposed mea-
premise of Carroll’s model was that school learn- sure for perseverance was engagement rate or per-
ing is a function of two time variables, the amount centage of allocated time during which students
of time students spend in learning relative to the are actually on task. In the equation, allocated
amount of time they actually need for learning. time is multiplied by engagement rate to produce
According to Carroll, students master instruc- the “time spent” numerator, or number of minutes
tional objectives to the extent that they are that students are engaged in learning. In Carroll’s
allowed and are willing to invest the time required original model, there was significant overlap
to learn the content. Carroll’s model can be between the constructs of student engagement
expressed as a simple mathematical equation: and student motivation. Subsequent theoretical
degree of learning = f [time spent/time needed] conceptualizations of time and learning (described
(Carroll, 1963). Based on this equation, the below) drew sharper distinctions between these
degree to which a learner succeeds in learning a two time-based determinants of learning.
task is dependent on the amount of time she/he By placing time as a pivotal variable in school
spends in relation to the amount of time she/ learning, Carroll’s model initiated a major shift in
he needs. The closer individuals come to achiev- educational thought and research on the teaching-
ing equilibrium between the amount of time they learning process (Anderson, 1985; Ben-Peretz &
656 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

Bromme, 1990; Gettinger, 1984). Influenced by conditions of school learning often pervaded
Carroll’s work, researchers directed greater atten- educational thought. Moreover, the influence of
tion to the variable of time in student learning, these assumptions on educational practices was
acknowledging that individual differences in vari- frequently counterproductive. In Bloom’s mastery
ous time metrics (time needed, time engaged, learning model, achievement level was held con-
time allocated) accounted for significant variabil- stant, and the amount of time needed to attain a
ity in educational performance (Anderson, 1984; targeted level of achievement was variable. As
Haertel, Walberg, & Weinstein, 1983). Carroll’s such, the goal was to fix the degree of learning at
work was the major impetus for subsequent mod- an acceptable criterion level and vary time and
els of school learning and contributed to a number instructional methods, according to learner needs,
of research projects designed to explicate further so that nearly all students attained it. The mastery
the relationship between time and learning. learning model rested on the belief that with ade-
The primary appeal of Carroll’s model was his quate time to learn, sufficient motivation to engage
specification of measurable, time-related vari- in learning, and high-quality instruction, all
ables that affect individual learning. Carroll’s students could learn what only a percentage of
model was viewed as an individual-differences students were able to learn under traditional fixed-
framework in that most of the time-related time instruction. In fact, mastery learning theorists
variables in his model (perseverance, time needed proposed that 80% of students could achieve a
to learn, etc.) were conceptualized as individual criterion level usually attained by only about 20%,
learner characteristics (Berliner, 1990a). Class- if they were given sufficient time and appropriate
room learning, however, is affected by variables help to maximize their engagement in learning
outside the learner, such as opportunity to learn (Block, 1971; Block & Anderson, 1975). The suc-
and quality of instruction. Moreover, schooling is cess of mastery learning models rested not only on
often organized to provide group instruction, allocating time for learning but also on motivating
with fixed time periods allocated to curricular students to be engaged in learning. According to
content. Thus, subsequent models were devel- mastery learning theorists, understanding and
oped to refine Carroll’s theory and adapt it for accommodating individual learners’ need for
classroom settings. These models (described choice, autonomy, encouragement, modeling, and
below) placed greater emphasis on time factors feedback enable teachers to motivate students and
external to the learner (including strategies to keep them engaged with academic tasks.
motivate learners) and incorporated features of There is a contrast between Carroll’s concep-
the instructional process at the classroom level. tual notion that time needed for learning is a rela-
tively stable student characteristic (as a function
Bloom’s Theory of School Learning of aptitude and ability to understand instruction)
Most notable of the models that were based on and Bloom’s instructional notion that time needed
Carroll’s constructs and attempted to incorporate for learning can be reduced with appropriate
time factors within an instructional theory of teaching-learning experiences. Researchers dem-
classroom learning was that of Benjamin Bloom onstrated that variation in students’ time needed
(1974). The development of the mastery learning for learning does, in fact, decrease across learn-
model by Bloom and his students generated ing tasks within a mastery learning paradigm
extensive research on time and school learning (Block, 1983; Guskey, 2001). Guskey theorized
during the 1980s. Although developed as an that individualized learning time and high-quality
instructional approach, mastery learning pro- instruction within a mastery learning approach
vided an appropriate experimental paradigm develop students’ skills, resulting in increased
within which variation in both time needed and confidence and competence. Greater confidence,
time spent in learning was systematically evalu- in turn, motivates students to engage in learning
ated (Gettinger, 1984). and successfully complete academic tasks.
According to Bloom, the notions of fixed According to Berliner (1990a), Bloom’s recon-
time and variable achievement as unavoidable ceptualization of learner variables, such as time
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 657

needed for learning, as malleable characteristics viewed by Wiley and Harnishfeger as the most
represented an important contribution to thinking important time variable in school learning
about students and schools. (Karweit, 1989).
Within the Wiley-Harnischfeger’s compre-
Wiley and Harnischfeger’s Model hensive model, achievement is determined by
of Instructional Exposure four time variables, including maximum expo-
Wiley and Harnischfeger offered another model sure time, percent usable exposure time, percent
of the teaching-learning process based on active learning time, and total time needed for
Carroll’s theoretical work. Similar to Carroll’s learning. Their conceptualization of achievement
model, the Wiley-Harnischfeger model rested on was expressed as the following equation: achieve-
the conviction that quantity of education (total ment = f [(allocated time) (percent usable time)
amount of actual learning time) determines the (percent active time)/time needed]. In this model,
degree of student learning (Wiley & Harnischfeger, the percentage of time students are actively
1974). Wiley and Harnischfeger, however, placed engaged in learning plays a prominent role in
significant emphasis on the total amount of time explaining achievement (Harnischfeger & Wiley,
allocated to students and for specific learning 1985). Students, in turn, are motivated to be
topics, which typically varied across districts, actively engaged in learning when they have an
schools, and classrooms. In elaborating their understanding of the purpose or goal for doing
model, Wiley and Harnischfeger discussed this so. Thus, according to Wiley and Harnishfeger,
concept of quantity of education, which they one mechanism for schools to accomplish instruc-
labeled “instructional exposure” (Harnishfeger & tional goals is to focus on the tasks in which stu-
Wiley, 1976). For Wiley and Harnishfeger, dents are engaged, ensure that students understand
Carroll’s factor of opportunity to learn or time the purpose of those tasks, and, in turn, maximize
allowed, in combination with the amount and the duration of their engagement.
manner in which allocated time is used for
instruction, comprised a major determinant of Cooley and Leinhardt’s
achievement. According to their model, the maxi- Classroom-Process Model
mum time available for instruction is established Like Wiley and Harnishfeger, Cooley and
by the length of the school year and school day, Leinhardt proposed a refinement of Carroll’s
as well as individual teacher allocations and model that also underscored the importance of the
scheduling. Maximum time, however, is typically use of instructional time over scheduled time per
reduced by several intervening factors, such as se in influencing achievement. The model of
attendance, instructional design, and time spent classroom processes proposed by Cooley and
by teachers for disciplining or making transitions. Leinhardt (1976) focused on the relationship
According to Wiley and Harnishfeger, only a between school practices (including instructional
percentage of the allocated time becomes actual time use) and school performance. The Cooley-
exposure time, and, in turn, only a portion of expo- Leinhardt model was a revision of Carroll’s model
sure time translates into usable time (i.e., time in that it provided a more precise specification of
without interruptions). Thus, Carroll’s factor of instructional processes that account for learning.
opportunity or time allowed was refined to the Their model imposed several qualifications on the
point of representing only a fraction of the total quantity of instruction, to which they referred as
time allocated. Within the Wiley-Harnischfeger the “opportunity construct.” Consistent with
model, achievement is a function of the ratio of Carroll’s model, opportunity was defined as the
maximum allocated exposure time (reduced, first, amount of time students can potentially work on
to active learning time within the maximum specific content. Motivators, another factor in
limits, and further to usable learning time) rela- their model, are the behaviors or attitudes (e.g.,
tive to the total time needed. Because maximum self-efficacy beliefs, understanding the purpose
allocated exposure time is amenable to manipula- for learning) that promote learning; these motiva-
tion and educational policy modifications, it was tors include both internal factors (student interest)
658 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

and external factors (e.g., teacher praise) that and (c) success, or the extent to which students
serve to maximize the amount of time students accurately complete the assignments they have
engage themselves in learning. Cooley and been given (Squires, Huitt, & Segars, 1983).
Leinhardt (1980) found that amount of time
scheduled for a specific subject may bear little Summary of Theoretical Foundations
relation to achievement. Within their model, the In sum, prominent theoretical conceptualiza-
efficient and effective use of allocated time is tions of school learning evolved since Carroll’s
more important than allocation per se; however, (1963) model that incorporated a focus on
as Wiley and Harnischfeger emphasized, alloca- instructional time and emphasized the critical
tion is always the upper bound of time use. In role of engaged time in determining student
addition to opportunity, Cooley and Leinhardt achievement. Whereas Carroll’s original model
identified three other constructs that explain vari- focused primarily on the quantity of time (e.g.,
ation in student performance under constant amount of time allocated, amount of time
instructional time conditions, specifically student engaged), more recent models stress the impor-
motivation, instructional events, and structure. tance of quality of time as well. Beginning with
All three constructs, when taken together, repre- Bloom’s mastery learning model, research on
sent Carroll’s quality of instruction variable, time and learning has devoted greater attention
thereby reflecting the relative emphasis placed on to the role of the instructional context, such as
this factor in determining overall degree of learn- quality of instruction, performance feedback, or
ing. Thus, within the Cooley-Leinhardt model, student interest in content, in explaining both
opportunity to learn, combined with motivators to student motivation (willingness to invest time in
encourage learning and appropriate instruction, is learning) and student engagement (actual
facilitative in promoting achievement (Leinhardt, involvement or participation in learning).
1978). The Cooley-Leinhardt model was the first
to articulate the explicit role of student motivation
in determining student engagement in learning. Process-Product Paradigm

Huitt Model of the Teaching-Learning The conceptualization of AET is integrally linked


Process to the emergence of research during the 1970s
Huitt (1995) focused on student behavior within which defined teaching effectiveness in terms of
the classroom as well as the influence of teacher specific classroom behaviors. The primary goal
behaviors on students’ engagement in learning. of teaching-effectiveness research was to deter-
Specifically, in the Huitt model, academic learn- mine the relationship between classroom pro-
ing time replaced the “time spent” variable in cesses and student performance, with the intent of
Carroll’s model. According to Huitt, student identifying teaching practices and behaviors asso-
behavior encompasses all actions of students in ciated with school learning and, in particular, stu-
classrooms and, importantly, includes academic dent engagement (Gettinger & Stoiber, 2009).
learning time (ALT). Within this model, ALT was The search for relations between classroom pro-
specifically defined as the amount of time stu- cesses (e.g., teaching behaviors) and outcomes
dents are successfully covering content that will (e.g., student achievement) came to be known as
be tested. Thus, ALT was a combination of process-outcome (or process-product) research.
three variables: (a) content overlap, which is the Process-outcome research has been the most
percentage of the content included on a test that common paradigm for establishing the impor-
is actually covered by students in the classrooms tance of AET for student learning and for identi-
and sometimes referred to as “time on target”; fying classroom variables that contribute to AET.
(b) involvement, which is the amount of time stu- Within a process-outcome approach, research-
dents are actively involved in the learning process ers are concerned with identifying relation-
and is most often equated with “time on task”; ships between teaching behaviors and student
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 659

outcomes, both of which can be defined, observed, AET provides a way of understanding and inter-
and measured (Berliner, 1990b). Process-outcome preting these results. For example, process-
studies use AET as an indicator or outcome of outcome research provides correlational evidence
teaching effectiveness as well as a predictor of that when teachers provide structure in their les-
student learning. Process-outcome research can sons (e.g., communicate expectations, monitor
be characterized on the basis of several method- student work, provide feedback, etc.), their stu-
ological features. In a typical study, teachers and dents have high achievement. The concept of
students are observed in classrooms. Teaching AET enables researchers to offer multiple expla-
processes are described in a series of low-inference nations for this relation. First, lesson structure
behavioral categories, often mutually exclusive, helps students understand their responsibility for
such that any classroom event (e.g., giving direc- learning, thereby increasing their perseverance or
tions for completing a task) is coded in only one motivation and, in turn, academic engaged time.
way. Relationships between patterns of teaching Alternately, providing explicit structure and
behaviors and student outcomes (specifically, expectations may guard against students being
student engagement or achievement) are engaged on the wrong task and is likely to increase
described as statistical correlations (Gettinger & their engagement and success on appropriate
Kohler, 2006). tasks. In other words, effective teaching (process)
One of the most significant contributions to is linked to achievement (outcome) through its
the advent of process-outcome research has been direct effect on maximizing students’ AET.
the emphasis on measurement of classroom pro- The earliest and most extensive process-
cesses and student engagement through system- outcome research program to document the rela-
atic, direct observation. With advances in tionship between time and learning and to provide
observation technology, researchers are able to empirical support for the importance of AET was
focus directly on the process of instruction and its the Beginning Teacher Evaluation Study (BTES)
effects on learning and behavior. Moreover, direct conducted during the 1980s (Denham &
observations can be used to measure and quantify Lieberman, 1980). Although the original purpose
students’ level of engagement in learning tasks. of the BTES was to evaluate beginning teacher
Although definitions vary, most observation pro- competencies, the focus of the study shifted
tocols incorporate fairly broad indices to assess toward identifying teaching activities and class-
student engagement. The Code for Instructional room conditions that promote student learning.
Structure and Student Academic Response Thus, the BTES became a major study on teach-
(CISSAR; Stanley & Greenwood, 1981), for ing effectiveness that generated educational
example, defines engagement in terms of observ- implications dealing with time, instructional pro-
able behaviors such as attending (e.g., looking at cesses, and classroom environment. Based on
the teacher), working (e.g., reading silently), or observations in classrooms over a 6-year period,
managing learning resources (e.g., looking for a BTES researchers developed an operational defi-
library book). Regardless of the definition of nition and measurable index of what they termed
engagement, most observational procedures use academic learning time. Specifically, they defined
some form of a time-sampling system. In these academic learning time (ALT) as the amount of
methods, observers note whether engaged behav- time a student spends engaged in academic tasks
iors are occurring or not occurring during brief of appropriate difficulty, on which they achieve
observation intervals to derive an estimate of AET. 80% success or accuracy (Denham & Lieberman).
In general, process-outcome research has con- The use of success rate in the BTES for determin-
cluded that teachers’ use of effective management ing ALT was of particular significance because it
and teaching strategies (classroom processes) is represented an attempt to provide a time metric
associated with increased student engagement for two variables in Carroll’s original model,
and higher achievement (learner outcomes). quality of instruction and ability to understand
Within process-outcome research, the concept of instruction. That is, if a students’ success rate was
660 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

high, then quality of instruction and/or ability to reveals a fairly consistent pattern. Compared to
understand instruction must be high. Conversely, scheduled or allocated time, engaged time dem-
if success rate was low, then either or both of onstrates the stronger relationship with achieve-
these variables must be low (Fisher & Berliner, ment. As Karweit and Slavin noted, increasing
1985). the amount of scheduled time per se is an ineffi-
Within the process-outcome tradition, the cient means to increase engaged time. It is more
BTES used ALT as an index of student learning. effective to strengthen instructional strategies to
In attempting to identify the key components of focus students’ attention and cognitive engage-
effective teaching, BTES researchers also dis- ment on learning tasks.
covered that a high level of ALT can be taken as Learning time research by Gettinger (1985,
evidence of effective teaching. One of the most 1989) also emphasized the importance of student
significant findings from the BTES project is that motivation – what Carroll called perseverance –
ALT results from specific measurable, teaching or the amount of time students are motivated to
behaviors and has a strong influence on students’ spend engaged in learning. As predicted by
academic achievement. Beyond engagement in Carroll’s model, Gettinger found that spending
academic tasks, BTES researchers investigated less time than needed in learning had a direct
how students’ success rates during engagement negative impact on both initial degree of learning
affect later achievement. They found that the pro- and later retention. When elementary school chil-
portion of time during which academic tasks are dren were permitted to self-determine their
performed with high success was positively asso- amount of study time, they spent, on average,
ciated with level of learning. Likewise, when stu- only 68% of the time they actually needed (deter-
dents experienced low success rates in learning mined on the basis of a baseline condition in
activities, they had lower achievement. In evalu- which they were required to learn material to a
ating the interactions between teachers and stu- criterion level of accuracy). As such, Gettinger’s
dents during instruction, the BTES data suggested research demonstrated the link between motiva-
that more frequent, substantive interactions (such tion and engagement and between engagement
as teachers presenting explicit content to be and learning, suggesting that student engagement
learned, closely monitoring students’ work, and can be differentiated from motivation to the
providing performance feedback) between the extent that it mediates the relationship between
student and the teacher were associated with high motivation and learning. Although motivating
levels of ALT. Higher levels of ALT, in turn, con- students to engage in learning is important, it is
tributed to achievement. In sum, the BTES find- ultimately student engagement (an outcome of
ings provided evidence that teaching behaviors student motivation) that contributes to achieve-
and classroom processes which increase motiva- ment. Similar research conducted with adult
tion and enable students to accrue high levels of learners evaluated the extent to which quantity of
ALT have a strong influence on academic learn- instruction influences time spent devoted to self-
ing and student achievement. study (a proxy for AET) and achievement
Research on the relationship between time and (Gijselaers & Schmidt, 1995; van den Hurk,
learning since the BTES spans at least four Wolfhagen, Dolmans, & van der Vleuten, 1998).
decades. The majority of studies have examined The results of these studies suggested that the
one of three time variables – allocated time, time association between allocated time and engaged
on task, or academic engaged time. The inconsis- self-study time may be described as a trade-off
tent use of time indices makes it difficult to draw mechanism. Allocated instructional time proved
comparisons across studies. It also explains why instrumental in influencing time spent on self-
there appear to be mixed findings about the study and achievement; however, increasing
degree to which time influences student learning instructional time was only effective to the
(Karweit & Slavin, 1981). Despite this variability extent that students were willing to spend an
in measurement choices, however, the literature increased amount of time engaged in self-study.
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 661

In other words, a higher level of student engage- measured, and directly controlled time index, i.e.,
ment (afforded by allocating more learning time) the number of hours in a school day and number
resulted only when students were motivated to of days in a year. At the bottom is the time vari-
actually engage in learning for longer periods of able which is most narrowly focused, challenging
available time. to measure, and difficult to modify, i.e., the num-
In sum, beginning with the BTES, research on ber of minutes when learning is actually taking
time and classroom learning has consistently place, or AET.
shown that the more time students are engaged in Theoretical conceptualizations of AET iden-
learning activities, the more they learn. There is a tify multiple constituent components along the
strong positive relationship between AET and continuum illustrated in Fig. 31.1. The first com-
student achievement. Whereas schools and teach- ponent of AET is available time, which represents
ers may schedule and allocate the appropriate the total number of hours or days that potentially
amount of time for learning, descriptive studies can be devoted to instruction. Available time is
reveal that teachers may not always ensure that typically established by school district policies
students are actively engaged in learning during and state requirements. The second component,
the allocated time (Mulholland & Cepello, 2006). scheduled or allocated time, is the amount of time
In some classrooms, students spend less than determined by classroom teachers for instruction
50% of the allocated time actually engaged in within each content domain. Scheduled time rep-
learning (Black, 2002). Three major factors that resents the upper limit of in-class opportunities
contribute to such low engagement are (a) instruc- for students to be engaged in learning. The pro-
tional design, (b) classroom management, and cess by which scheduled time is converted into
(c) student self-study. A later section of this chap- productive learning time depends on classroom
ter provides a review of strategies for increasing instruction and management practices, as well as
academic engaged and active responding time. student characteristics. Despite variation across
classrooms in the amount of time scheduled for
instruction, Marks (2000) found that most vari-
Academic Engaged Time: Definition ability in AET is attributable to differences in
and Differentiation Among Key individual student characteristics, not classrooms.
Concepts This finding may be due to several reasons. First,
as implicated in Carroll’s model and demon-
A theoretical focus on the relationship between strated through Bloom’s research, some students
time and learning, combined with a research par- simply require more time for learning than do
adigm that implicates time as an index of effec- others. In fact, individual differences in the
tive teaching, has contributed to the current amount of time needed for learning, within a con-
conceptualization and definition of AET. An stant level of scheduled time, contribute to vari-
explanation and distinction among several con- able achievement more so than do differences
cepts is important for applying the construct of in scheduled time (Gettinger, 1984). A second
AET to classroom practices. reason is that students in the same classroom
Research on the relationship between time and often self-allocate variable time for independent
learning has been complicated by the variety of or self-study. Thus, the amount of self-determined
ways in which researchers have conceptualized or self-scheduled learning time (a time-based
and measured time. Learning time, from which metric of student motivation) will vary across
the concept of AET is derived, is best understood individual learners and, in turn, determine indi-
as a superordinate concept which encompasses vidual levels of student engagement, even in the
subordinate and more refined concepts of time. same classroom with constant allocated time.
As such, time concepts can be ordered on a verti- As shown in Fig. 31.1, scheduled time can be
cal continuum (see Fig. 31.1). At the top of the further broken down into instructional and nonin-
continuum is the most broadly described, easily structional time. Instructional time is the amount
662 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

Fig. 31.1 Continuum of components and determinants of academic engaged time

of scheduled time directly devoted to learning noninstructional time that occurs before (e.g.,
and instruction. Noninstructional time, by con- teacher gives back homework at the start of an
trast, is the portion of scheduled time that is spent instructional activity) and after (e.g., children put
in nonlearning activities, for example, lunch, away materials after a science lesson) instruction.
recess, or transitions. Research shows that a vast Whereas transition time is usually a constant time
portion of instruction is often eroded by factors variable for all students, wait time is the amount
such as time spent in maintaining discipline, such of time an individual student must wait to receive
that only 38% of a school day is typically spent instructional help, for example, waiting for the
engaged in learning activities (Aronson, teacher’s attention after raising one’s hand.
Zimmerman, & Carlos, 1999). Multiple events Although a 60-min period may be scheduled for
occur in classrooms that may reduce the amount instruction, some portion of that time is inevitably
of scheduled time that is converted to actual consumed by noninstructional activities having
instructional time (Caldwell, Huitt, & Graeber, little to do with learning. The NECTL analysis of
1982; Hollywood et al., 1995; Kubitschek, how time is used in American schools makes it
Hallinan, Arnett, & Galipeau, 2005; NECTL, clear that “reclaiming” the academic day (i.e.,
1994). According to Kubitschek et al. (2005), the protecting and preserving scheduled time for aca-
two primary sources of lost instructional time are demic content) could nearly double the amount
transition time and wait time. Transition time is of instructional time students receive.
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 663

Engaged time is the proportion of instructional that leads directly to demonstrated learning. An
time during which students are cognitively and index of AET is derived by subtracting from the
behaviorally on task or engaged in learning, as total instructional time not only the amount of
evidenced by paying attention, completing work, time spent on classroom management tasks but
listening, or participating in relevant discussion. also time spent on instructional activities that do
Engaged time includes both passive attending and not successfully translate into learning. The qual-
active responding. Time on task is engaged time ities of both relevance and success are critical for
on particular learning tasks. Because time on task discerning AET. Neither succeeding at irrelevant
reflects engaged time on specific learning tasks, it tasks nor failing at relevant and worthwhile tasks
carries a more restricted meaning than engaged contributes to effective learning. Students gain
time. In terms of learning outcomes, engaged time the most from learning time when they experi-
among students is a necessary, but not sufficient ence a balance of high and medium success on
condition for achievement. Time on task reflects meaningful learning activities. Thus, accurately
engaged time spent on targeted tasks that have measuring and ensuring success (e.g., through
clear instructional or learning goals. For example, ongoing progress monitoring) is critical for
engagement may be coded as occurring when a increasing students’ AET.
student is involved in completing math problems In sum, a high level of academic engaged time
or when reading a book during an instructional exists when: (a) students are covering content
time period allocated to science. On-task behavior, that holds their interest and is viewed as being
however, would not be coded in either situation important and relevant, (b) they are paying atten-
because the task in which students are engaged is tion or on task for most of the class period, and
not science. As Carroll (1989) noted, learning (c) they are experiencing a high level of success
time must be filled with activities that are appro- or accuracy with most of the assignments they
priate and relevant. In observational research, complete. The nexus between student engage-
time on task is measured in the same way as ment and student motivation is evident in this
engaged time; however, the curriculum, instruc- conceptualization of AET. Students are motivated
tional activities, or tasks in which the student is when they are interested in and understand the
engaged, all of which may account for student purpose of what they are learning, when there are
motivation, are also recorded and enter into the environmental factors to keep them on task (e.g.,
determination of total time on task. teacher praise), and when they experience suc-
Integral to overall engaged time is what Carroll cess in learning (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000). Each
termed perseverance, or the amount of time a stu- variable that contributes to motivation results in
dent is willing to spend on learning a task or unit high engagement. Whereas learning time compo-
of instruction. Based on Carroll’s original con- nents demonstrate varying levels of relationship
ceptualization, the construct of motivation is with student outcomes, AET, determined by both
transformed into a time-based and measurable motivation and engagement, has been shown to
concept. That is, perseverance is viewed as a time- have the strongest link with school learning and
determined motivational construct. Thus, when achievement (Gettinger & Ball, 2007).
measured as the amount of time on task that a stu-
dent willingly devotes to learning, motivation is a
variable that can also be measured in time. Practices for Maximizing Academic
Finally, a certain percentage of engaged time, Engaged Time
or time on task, represents the amount of time
during which learning actually occurs; this repre- Current knowledge about evidence-based
sents academic engaged time. AET is the most approaches to maximize AET derives from effec-
carefully delineated conception of time in the lit- tive teaching research which has identified strate-
erature; it is the portion of time students are gies to actively involve students in learning
actively engaged in relevant academic instruction (Gettinger & Stoiber, 2009). As research sug-
664 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

Table 31.1 Practices for maximizing academic engaged time


Managerial strategies Instructional strategies Student-mediated strategies
• Monitor student behavior Interactive teaching • Teach students to employ
• Minimize classroom • Focus on explicit learning objectives metacognitive and study strategies
disruptions and off-task • Facilitate active student responding • Incorporate self-monitoring
behavior • Provide frequent feedback procedures into the classroom
• Reduce transition time Instructional design • Support students’ self-manage-
• Establish consistent and • Match instruction with students’ abilities ment skills
efficient classroom routines • Use multiple teaching methods • Establish consistent classroom
• Decrease class size and • Deliver instruction at a quick, smooth, routines and structure
learning group sizes and efficient pace • Have students set their own goals
• Ensure that students understand for learning
directions • Use homework effectively to
enhance student learning

gests, rather than simply increasing available poor classroom management can erode instruc-
instructional time, schools should make better use tional time and decrease opportunities for stu-
of existing time and find ways to increase the pro- dents to learn (Smith, 2000). Therefore, it is
portion of time students are engaged in instruc- imperative that teachers implement management
tional activities (NECTL, 1994). This means strategies to reduce the amount of instructional
ensuring, first, that adequate allocated time is time that may be lost to noninstructional activi-
devoted to instruction and, second, that activities ties. Several managerial strategies have been
which reduce engaged time are minimized. shown to promote AET, including closely moni-
Creating more engaged time, despite its impor- toring student behavior, minimizing classroom
tance, does nothing to advance achievement disruptions and off-task behavior, reducing tran-
unless the instructional activities in which stu- sition time, establishing consistent and efficient
dents are engaged are appropriate (Prater, 1992). classroom routines, and decreasing class size or
Thus, quality of teaching is the key to enhancing learning group size.
AET. Research demonstrates that when coupled When teachers consistently monitor the behav-
with effective teaching, increased time has a sig- ior of their students during learning activities,
nificant impact on student achievement. In effect, AET is maximized. There are myriad ways to
providing more instructional time alone cannot be facilitate close monitoring, such as planning seat-
expected to have a significant effect on student ing arrangements that enable teachers to view
learning unless the additional time is devoted to student behavior from anywhere in the class-
instruction, with students being engaged in well- room, decreasing the amount of time spent at the
designed and appropriate learning activities. The teacher’s desk by frequently circulating the room,
purpose of the following sections is to address going to students when they have questions
strategies supported by research that increase rather than requiring students to come to the
AET. The literature points to three factors that, in teacher, utilizing student volunteers for handling
conjunction with time, contribute to student learn- classroom materials, and acknowledging and
ing. Two factors (classroom management and responding to appropriate engagement behaviors
appropriateness of instruction) rest primarily with (Gettinger & Ball, 2007). Teachers can system-
teachers; the third (self-management) rests more atically provide reinforcement for student engage-
with students (see Table 31.1). ment by implementing a token economy in their
classrooms. There is strong evidence supporting
the effectiveness of token economy interventions
Managerial Strategies for a range of classroom behaviors, including
student engagement (DuPaul & Stoner, 2003).
Effective classroom management strategies com- Token economies involve the use of secondary
prise one factor that maximizes AET and contrib- reinforcers, or tokens, which are delivered to
utes to student learning. Research has shown that students to produce positive behavioral change.
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 665

Tokens (e.g., poker chips, coins, check marks, periods or math instruction. To decrease time
points, stickers) can be tailored to the develop- spent in transitions, the authors recommended
mental level and individual preferences of chil- that teachers develop specific entrance and exit
dren in a classroom. In the case of student routines for students who must leave the class-
engagement, for example, tokens may be dis- room for related services. Moreover, the authors
pensed when students are on task and involved in recommended that these routines be explicitly
relevant work for a clearly specified amount of taught and practiced to maximize the amount of
time. Token economy interventions typically uti- time students are actively engaged in instruction-
lize group contingencies that may be indepen- related activities (Stichter et al., 2008).
dent, interdependent, or dependent. Independent Another strategy to minimize time spent in
contingencies involve providing reinforcement to transitions involves using verbal or nonverbal
individual students contingent on their behavior; transition cues when the entire class is making a
an interdependent contingency requires all stu- noninstructional transition, such as going from
dents in the classroom to be engaged in order for the classroom to the music room (Kauchak &
any single student to receive the reinforcer; and, Eggen, 2003). Procedures for executing transi-
finally, dependent contingencies involve target- tions should be explicitly communicated and
ing specific students or a group of students who actively taught not only at the beginning of the
must meet the engagement criterion level for all year but throughout the year as necessary.
students to receive the reinforcer (Litow & Teachers can also provide class-wide incentives
Pumroy, 1975). Although research supports the for following transition routines quickly and
overall effectiveness of group-oriented contin- accurately. Although the type of effective transi-
gencies, studies comparing the relative benefits tion cues varies across classrooms (e.g., flickering
of different group contingency procedures have lights, using key signal words or phrases), what is
yielded mixed results, indicating that group pro- most critical is that all students are able to recog-
cedures should be developed and implemented nize and interpret the cue. If a transition is requir-
based on the unique characteristics and structure ing too much time, teachers can establish a time
of each classroom (Kelshaw-Levering, Sterling- limit for the transition and provide reinforcement
Turner, Henry, & Skinner, 2000). for students who complete the transition within
Other managerial strategies that minimize dis- the limit. After students are able to complete
ruptions, off-task behavior, and long transitions transitions quickly, a goal can be set to gradually
also contribute to AET. Instructional time that is reduce the time until the targeted transition
lost to nonlearning activities, such as disruptions, time is achieved (Ostrosky, Jung, Hemmeter, &
disciplinary issues, or transitions, can be quite Thomas, 2003).
substantial in some schools and classrooms Other managerial strategies to increase AET
(Aronson et al., 1999). Research has shown that include having clearly defined and taught rules,
the proportion of noninstructional time may be expectations for being engaged during learning
even greater in schools or classrooms with high activities, and consistent routines. Rules and
percentages of at-risk students. A study by expectations are most effective when they are
Stichter, Stormont, and Lewis (2008), for exam- clearly posted in the classroom to support stu-
ple, investigated the amount of scheduled instruc- dents’ awareness and knowledge of them. In
tional time lost to noninstructional activities in addition to rules being understood by students,
Title I versus non-Title I elementary schools. appropriate behavior in accordance with rules
Results showed that teachers in Title I schools and expectations should be positively reinforced,
spent more time in noninstructional activities whereas failure to comply should receive consis-
(e.g., transitions, disciplining students) than did tent, immediate, and effective consequences. To
teachers in non-Title I schools. In addition, in the extent possible, teachers can also preserve
Title I schools, there was a higher occurrence of engaged learning time through implementing
students entering and leaving classrooms during consistent routines for noninstructional activities
periods of academic instruction, such as literacy (Odden & Archibald, 2009). Doing so involves
666 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

handling noninstructional obligations (e.g., taking teaching have been shown to have a significant
attendance) after students have begun working on impact on AET (Rosenshine, 1995).
their tasks or by completing all noninstructional
activities during a predetermined time of day set Interactive Teaching
aside for such activities (Harmin & Toth, 2006). Teaching behaviors that actively engage in stu-
Soliciting assistance from the administration dents in learning are collectively referred to as
to minimize external interruptions (e.g., inter- interactive teaching. Interactive teaching strate-
com announcements, unscheduled visitations) is gies involve (a) focusing on explicit learning
another effective way to protect instructional time objectives, (b) facilitating active student respond-
and increase students’ engaged learning time ing, and (c) providing frequent feedback (Good
(Odden & Archibald). & Brophy, 2003).
Finally, there is clear evidence that the size of To maximize AET, it is critical for teaching to
a class or learning group can significantly impact incorporate a strong focus on academic content
AET (Fowler, 1995). Research has demonstrated and learning objectives. Academic focus is deter-
that small class sizes, particularly in the primary mined by the amount of time devoted to academic
grades, allow teachers to use time more effi- activities as well as by the extent to which instruc-
ciently to increase AET and promote student tion is linked with learning goals and student
achievement (Odden & Archibald, 2009). accountability for reaching those goals.
Blatchford, Russell, Bassett, Brown, and Martin Preplanning is necessary to ensure that instruc-
(2007) documented more positive learning and tion incorporates a strong academic focus.
social outcomes in schools with class sizes less Effective planning involves, first, developing
than 15 students compared to schools with larger clear learning goals and objectives for the lesson.
class sizes. Using a multimethod approach, these Next, learning activities are designed to help stu-
researchers found that small class sizes allowed dents achieve the goals and objectives. According
students to receive a higher degree of teacher to Kauchek and Eggen (2003), designing effec-
attention and take a more active, engaged role in tive learning activities with an academic focus
their learning (Blatchford et al., 2007). Because requires determination of the instructional format
manipulating class size may not be an option in (e.g., whole group, small group), specification of
all schools, teachers can also minimize the size of lesson materials, and establishment of assess-
learning groups in their classrooms. Smaller ment procedure to measure student learning and
learning groups, especially when teacher- attainment of goals.
directed, have been shown to promote a higher In addition to an explicit academic focus,
level of student engagement in learning (Gettinger interactive teaching involves promoting high lev-
& Ball, 2007). els of active student responding or engagement.
One mechanism to ensure that all students remain
engaged in class discussion is the use of effective
Instructional Strategies questioning techniques (Good & Brophy, 2003).
“Good questions” that promote engagement are
Effective instructional practices comprise the sec- clear, purposeful, brief, sequenced, and focused
ond factor related to students’ academic engaged on extending students’ thinking (Gall, 1984). An
time (see Table 31.1). Effective teaching research, effective questioning strategy that has been
conceptualized within a process-outcome per- shown to increase engagement among all students
spective, has identified several instructional vari- follows three sequential steps (Chuska, 1995).
ables that are positively correlated with AET Teachers, first, pose a question to the entire class.
(Gettinger & Kohler, 2006; Gettinger & Stoiber, Then they give students wait time to think about
2009). Generally, strategies related to how teach- the question. After a sufficient amount of wait
ers deliver instruction (i.e., interactive teaching time, they call on an individual student to respond.
behaviors) and how they design or structure their Using this method requires all students to be
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 667

responsible for carefully considering an answer challenged and able to experience a high rate of
to the question because the teacher may call on success. Students differ in the amount of time,
any student. In contrast, when teachers pose a exposure, practice, and instruction needed to
question to a specific student, only that student learn. Therefore, being able to differentiate
has the responsibility for answering, and others instruction based on individual student needs is
may be less likely to contribute their thoughts. To an instructional practice that will promote AET
heighten engagement among all learners, it is (Tomlinson, 2003). Using collaborative planning
important for teachers to distribute questions with students; assigning students to work with
evenly to a range of students in their classrooms peer tutors, volunteers, or aides; and monitoring
and not allow a select group of students to answer student understanding are examples of ways to
the majority of the questions (Good & Brophy). accommodate the diverse needs of children in a
Other strategies for promoting active responding classroom and ensure that all students are aca-
include using choral responding, peer tutoring, demically engaged (Freiberg & Driscoll).
and cooperative learning groups. Many existing Along with employing strategies to increase
assignments and activities can be easily adapted academic engagement, it is important for teach-
to involve a higher degree of active student ers to foster students’ motivation for learning, a
responding, such as having students work in pairs key determinant of student engagement.
rather than individually on projects or assigned According to self-determination theory (SDT;
work (Gettinger & Ball, 2007). Ryan & Deci, 2000), students will be engaged
Providing frequent feedback is another key versus disengaged in learning, in large part, as a
element of interactive teaching that fosters stu- function of motivation. Specifically, students ini-
dent engagement (Bangert-Drowns, Kulik, Kulik, tiate and maintain involvement in learning activi-
& Morgan, 1991). Effective feedback is charac- ties to the extent that they believe sustained
terized as being detailed, accurate, and immedi- engagement will lead to desired outcomes or
ate, as well as encouraging and supportive. goals. Thus, within SDT, engagement is viewed
Effective feedback acknowledges the accom- as a goal-directed or motivated behavior. Research
plishments students have made and contributes to on students’ goal-directed behavior has distin-
students’ self-efficacy, confidence they can attain guished among different types of goals which, in
goals, and motivation to work toward their goals turn, lead to different long-term behavioral con-
(Butler & Winne, 1995; Kluger & DeNisi, 1996). sequences, such as level of engagement (Deci &
Self-evaluation is a skill that can be taught to stu- Ryan, 2000). According to SDT, students experi-
dents as a means of providing immediate feed- ence two types of motivation, autonomous moti-
back about performance (Bandura, Martinez-Pons, vation and controlled motivation (Deci & Ryan).
& Zimmerman, 1992). Although strategies may Autonomous motivation involves students’
vary, providing feedback to students that is fre- intrinsic motivation as well as extrinsic motiva-
quent, specific, and relevant has strong research tion in which students have integrated external
support as effective means of enhancing AET values (e.g., the importance of academic engage-
(Gettinger & Ball, 2007; Kluger & DeNisi, 1996; ment) into their own personal value system.
Mory, 1992). Conversely, controlled motivation involves exter-
nal regulation (e.g., by a teacher) that controls
Instructional Design students to behave in a certain way. For example,
In addition to interactive teaching, several students could be motivated to engage in learning
instructional design features have been shown to new skills because they are interested in the con-
increase students’ academic engaged time tent or understand its value. Conversely, students’
(Frieberg & Driscoll, 2000). The first design fea- motivation for engagement in learning might be to
ture is the degree to which instruction is appro- get a good grade or to avoid punishment. In these
priately matched with students’ ability. To be examples, the amount of motivation may not
academically engaged, students must be both vary, but the nature and focus of motivation does.
668 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

Research has demonstrated that autonomous importance of having instructions and expecta-
motivation, as illustrated in the first scenario, tions that are clearly explained and understood
contributes to higher levels of self-determined by all students. Research has identified several
engagement in learning and, more importantly, methods for maximizing the likelihood that stu-
better long-term learning outcomes (Deci & dents understand directions, including (a) having
Ryan, 2008; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Therefore, students paraphrase directions, (b) having them
teachers should seek to promote students’ auton- write the directions down, (c) visually displaying
omous motivation for learning (e.g., through pro- directions in the classroom, (d) keeping direc-
viding student choice, incorporating adequate tions simple, (e) obtaining all students’ attention
challenge into learning tasks, establishing mean- before giving directions, (f) modeling steps of the
ingful learning goals) to achieve high levels of directions, and (g) allowing students to begin
academic engagement over time. only after all directions have been delivered
Using multiple and diverse teachings methods (Frieberg & Driscoll, 2000; Gettinger & Ball,
is another aspect of instructional design to 2007; Marzano, Pickering, & Pollock, 2001).
increase AET. Students often become disengaged Research has consistently demonstrated that clear
if the same format (e.g., teacher lecture) is always directions and expectations increase students’
used. Because students learn differently, not all understanding of learning tasks as well as the
students may be able to benefit from a strict lec- amount of time they spend in learning (Marzano
ture format. Therefore, blending lecture formats et al., 2001).
with other teaching strategies (e.g., questioning,
discussions) will promote active learning and
engagement for a greater proportion of students Student-Mediated Strategies
(Freiberg & Driscoll, 2000). It is also important
that the instructional sequence is effective and Consistent with Carroll’s model, the amount of
appropriative given students’ developmental time students spend engaged in learning is, to
level. Using a variety of teaching methods is par- some extent, self-determined and indicative of
ticularly critical when reteaching material to low- their level of motivation for learning. Although
achieving students; reteaching content will be there are many instructional and managerial
effective only if the instruction is delivered in a strategies teachers can employ to promote AET
different format and with different examples than and increase students’ motivation, students ulti-
the first time when instruction was given mately play a major role in determining their
(Gettinger & Ball, 2007). own learning and levels of engagement in learn-
Considering the pace of the lesson is another ing. Factors such as low self-efficacy or limited
feature of instructional design that promotes self-monitoring skills are important to consider
AET. A quick, smooth, and efficient instructional as they typically function to decrease the amount
pace increases the amount of content covered as of time students spend engaged in learning
well as the amount of active learning time avail- (Bandura et al., 1992). Student-mediated strate-
able during a class period or school day (Gettinger gies focus on supporting cognitive engagement,
& Ball, 2007). Breaking lessons into small steps, autonomous motivation, and self-regulation
changing the topic or procedure when the teacher among students. Specifically, these strategies
notices a decline in student engagement, and include teaching students to employ metacogni-
maintaining high expectations for student tive and study strategies, self-monitoring proce-
involvement are all examples of effective strate- dures, and self-management skills (Borkowski &
gies for delivering fast-paced instruction (Harmin Muthukrishna, 1992; Shapiro & Cole, 1994;
& Toth, 2006). Zimmerman, Greenberg, & Weinstein, 1994).
Finally, instructional time and AET is often According to a metacognitive perspective,
lost when students fail to understand directions the amount of time students are academically
for an assignment or task. This underscores the engaged is influenced by their learning and
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 669

studying skills, such as their ability to plan and Jenkins, & Edelen-Smith, 1999). Regardless of
organize study time or their approach to taking the recording method, the process of self-
tests (Borkowski & Muthukrishna, 1992; Butler monitoring involves having students self-observe
& Winne, 1995; Zimmerman et al., 1994). and record predetermined behaviors or outcomes
Providing instruction to develop these cognitive at signaled intervals, such as the duration of
strategies can enhance student’s academic engaged time, frequency of engagement behav-
engaged time as it enables them to be organized iors, task completion, or overall accuracy.
and efficient in their learning. Learning strategies Evidence-based resources are also available to
also support active engagement during indepen- individualize the self-monitoring procedure to
dent work because students are better equipped match children’s needs and preferences. For
to approach the material and plan their studying. younger children or children with disabilities,
Furthermore, these strategies may lead to for example, a self-monitoring tool called
increased student motivation for learning, which “Countoons” has been developed and evaluated
in turn may foster a greater level of engagement. (Daly & Ranalli, 2003). Countoons are cartoon
Effective learning strategies include note-taking illustrations of students’ appropriate and inappro-
skills, time management skills, test-taking skills, priate behaviors, paired with a contingency for
and accessing resources that support students’ meeting an established criterion of appropriate
understanding and interactions with text, such as behaviors. The drawings also contain space for
story maps or outlines (Gettinger & Ball, 2007; children to record the frequency of their own
Marzano et al., 2001). behaviors. This tool is particularly appealing
Another student-mediated method for increas- because it enables very young students to moni-
ing academic engaged time is self-monitoring tor their own behavior even when they cannot
(Cole & Bambara, 2000; Cole, Marder, & read. Moreover, by allowing students to draw the
McCann, 2000). Students must be able to self- cartoons themselves, they may be more motivated
monitor when they are learning so they can plan to take an active role in their learning and engage-
and adjust time use according to their learning ment. For example, when the goal is for students
needs. Even highly motivated students may fail to increase their engaged learning time, they can
to appropriately monitor their own learning or draw a picture of themselves reading at their desk
use of time (Gettinger, 1985; Zimmerman et al., and circle the number of times they perform the
1994). Self-monitoring alone can be a highly behavior at designated intervals (Daly & Ranalli).
effective intervention to increase AET among Self-monitoring methods are part of the
students (Levendoski & Cartledge, 2000). In broader set of student-mediated or self-manage-
addition to contributing to AET, self-monitoring ment skills (Cole & Bambara, 2000; Shapiro &
strategies have several documented benefits, Cole, 1994). Self-management skills enable chil-
including promoting positive classroom behav- dren to self-direct their learning behaviors, expe-
ior; increasing student motivation for learning, rience a greater degree of autonomy in the
especially when students are able to exercise classroom, and maximize their academic engaged
choice in selecting the self-recording method or time (Shapiro & Cole, 1994; Wehmeyer et al.,
target behavior; and providing immediate feed- 2007; Zimmerman et al., 1994). Research has
back to students about their behavior and learn- shown that self-management skills are associated
ing (Carr & Punzo, 1993; Moxley, 1998; Trammel, with positive educational outcomes, including
Schloss, & Alper, 1994). higher student engagement, academic perfor-
Several approaches for self-monitoring in mance, and work productivity (Wehmeyer et al.).
classrooms have been documented as being effec- There are many strategies teachers can use to
tive in promoting students’ engagement in learn- promote students’ self-management skills, such
ing, such as keeping a tally count on a paper taped as using guided questioning to teach students a
to the child’s desk or the inside of a notebook systematic way of approaching tasks. Questions
(Reid, 1996; Rock, 2005; Shimabukuro, Prater, may be used to cue or prompt students to gather
670 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

necessary materials, review the instructions, engaged time because it extends learning time
approximate the amount of time needed for the beyond the typical school day (Cooper et al.,
assignment, and consider whether they are 2006). To ensure that homework is being used
equipped with the skills and knowledge required most effectively, teachers should assign work that
for the task. This strategy has been used effec- is relevant to current learning objectives, matched
tively with individual students or posted in the to students’ abilities, motivating, assigned in con-
classroom for all students to use (Rock, 2005). sistent amounts, and reviewed the following day.
Structure and routines in the classroom also Other effective homework strategies may include
promote self-management skills and, in turn, (a) communicating with parents and families
function to increase AET. When routines are con- about homework expectations, (b) providing fre-
sistent and explicit, students are able to follow quent progress reports when needed, (c) clearly
them independently, requiring less assistance explaining homework assignments to students,
from the teacher. For example, if a teacher always (d) establishing classroom homework routines
has her students do a warm-up activity at the and a system for helping students with homework,
beginning of class, if students understand instruc- (e) teaching effective study skills, and, when pos-
tions for the warm-up activity, and if they know sible, (f) incorporating student or creating a menu
where to access the appropriate materials, they of homework assignments (Dawson).
will be able to begin working on the task imme-
diately upon arrival to the classroom and mini-
mize the loss of instructional time. Conversely, Concluding Remarks
without consistent expectations, students are not
able to direct their own learning and will likely The link between time and learning remains well
spend less time academically engaged. documented in educational research. The rela-
Another student-mediated approach to tionship between time and learning, however,
increase AET involves having students set their depends on the degree to which available time is
own goals for engagement and learning (Bandura devoted to high-quality instruction and the extent
et al., 1992). Goal-setting strategies provide to which students are motivated to invest time in
opportunities for students to establish and moni- learning. Any addition to allocated time will
tor a personal learning goal for each day. By improve achievement to the extent that it is actu-
allowing students to set their own goals for learn- ally used for instruction and, in turn, converted to
ing, they can become empowered and may expe- academic engaged time for learners. Simply
rience greater motivation to work toward their assigning more study time to a topic will not auto-
goals. Harmin and Toth (2006) described a goal- matically increase the student’s learning. When
setting procedure in which at the beginning of the instructional quality, appropriateness of instruc-
day, the teacher invites students to identify one tion relative to student abilities, and incentives for
goal for themselves and record it in an individu- learning, however, are all high, then more time
alized log book. At the end of the day, students will pay off in greater learning. The key is to max-
reflect on how successful they were at reaching imize students’ academic engaged time.
their goals and share this with the class. In one form or another, learning time plays an
Establishing daily goals has been shown to main- important role in understanding, predicting, and
tain students’ engagement in learning activities controlling instructional processes across a range
throughout the day (Rock, 2005). of activities. Indeed, AET has the potential to cap-
Self-management skills, particularly self-mon- ture critical aspects of the teaching-learning pro-
itoring and goal setting, are also effective for cess, including student motivation. For researchers,
increasing the degree to which students complete AET is not only an instructional time variable, it
homework assignments (Cooper, Robinson, & is actually a measure or quantifiable index of
Patall, 2006; Dawson, 2007). Effective use of quality of instruction. The assumption is that as
homework in itself is a way to maximize academic AET is accrued, quality instruction is taking place.
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 671

To the extent that research underscores the need to aged 7–11 years. School Effectiveness and School
maximize AET, investigators must continue to Improvement, 18(2), 147–172.
Block, J. H. (Ed.). (1971). Mastery learning: Theory and
address what can be done to enhance or increase practice. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
AET for all learners, particularly learners who Block, J. H. (1983). Learning rates and mastery learning.
may be at risk for school failure (Goodman, 1990; Outcomes, 2(3), 18–23.
Woelfel, 2005). Making good use of existing time, Block, J. H., & Anderson, L. W. (1975). Mastery learning
in classroom instruction. New York: Macmillan.
whereby students experience high success on Bloom, B. S. (1974). Time and learning. American
meaningful tasks, is more likely to substantially Psychologist, 29, 682–688.
increase both AET and student achievement than Borkowski, J. G., & Muthukrishna, N. (1992). Moving
simply allocating more instructional time (e.g., metacognition into the classroom: “Working models”
and effective strategy teaching. In M. Pressley, K. R.
lengthening the school day or year). It is only Harris, & J. T. Guthrie (Eds.), Promoting academic
when instructional time is used efficiently and competence and literacy in school (pp. 477–501). San
effectively, when student motivation for engage- Diego, CA: Academic.
ment is high, and when AET is maximized that Butler, D. L., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Feedback and self-
regulated learning: A theoretical synthesis. Review of
more time will result in improved academic out- Educational Research, 65, 245–281.
comes for all learners. Caldwell, J. H., Huitt, W. G., & Graeber, A. O. (1982).
Time spent in learning: Implications from research.
The Elementary School Journal, 82, 471–480.
Carr, S. C., & Punzo, R. P. (1993). The effects of self-
References monitoring of academic accuracy and productivity on
the performance of students with behavioral disorders.
Adelman, N. E., Haslam, M. B., & Pringle, B. A. (1996). Behavior Disorders, 18, 241–250.
The uses of time for teaching and learning (Vol. 1). Carroll, J. B. (1963). A model of school learning. Teachers
Washington, DC: Policy Studies Associates, Inc. College Record, 64, 723–733.
Anderson, L. W. (Ed.). (1984). Time and school learning: Carroll, J. B. (1984). The model of school learning:
Theory, research and practice. New York: St. Martin’s Progress of an idea. In L. W. Anderson (Ed.), Time and
Press. school learning: Theory, research and practice
Anderson, L. W. (Ed.). (1985). Perspectives on school (pp. 15–46). New York: St. Martin’s Press.
learning: Selected writings of John B. Carroll. Carroll, J. B. (1989). The Carroll model: A 25-year retro-
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. spective and prospective view. Educational Researcher,
Aronson, J., Zimmerman, J., & Carlos, L. (1999). 18, 26–31.
Improving student achievement by extending school: Chuska, K. (1995). Improving classroom questions:
Is it just a matter of time? San Francisco: WestEd. A teacher’s guide to increasing student motivation,
Bandura, A., Martinez-Pons, M., & Zimmerman, B. J. participation, and higher level thinking. Bloomington,
(1992). Self-motivation for academic attainment: The IN: Phi Delta Kappa Educational Foundation.
role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting. Cole, C. L., & Bambara, L. M. (2000). Self-monitoring:
American Educational Research Journal, 29, 663–674. Theory and practice. In E. S. Shapiro & T. R.
Bangert-Drowns, R. L., Kulik, C. C., Kulik, J. A., & Kratochwill (Eds.), Behavioral assessment in schools:
Morgan, M. (1991). The instructional effects of feed- Theory, research, and clinical foundations (2nd ed.,
back in test-like events. Review of Educational pp. 202–232). New York: Guilford Press.
Research, 61, 213–238. Cole, C. L., Marder, T., & McCann, L. (2000). Self-
Ben-Peretz, M., & Bromme, R. (Eds.). (1990). The nature monitoring. In E. S. Shapiro & T. R. Kratochwill
of time in schools: Theoretical concepts, practitioner (Eds.), Conducting school-based assessments of child
perceptions. New York: Teachers College Press. and adolescent behavior (pp. 121–149). New York:
Berliner, D. (1990a). What’s all the fuss about instruc- Guilford Press.
tional time? In M. Ben-Peretz & R. Bromme (Eds.), Cooley, W. W., & Leinhardt, G. (1976). The application of
The nature of time in schools: Theoretical concepts, a model for investigating classroom processes.
practitioner perceptions (pp. 3–35). New York: Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh, Learning
Teachers College Press. Research & Development Center.
Berliner, D. (1990b). The place of process-product research Cooley, W. W., & Leinhardt, G. (1980). The instructional
in developing the agenda for research on teaching dimensions study. Educational Evaluation and Policy
thinking. Educational Psychologist, 24, 325–344. Analysis, 2, 7–26.
Black, S. (2002). Time for learning. American School Cooper, H., Robinson, J. C., & Patall, A. (2006). Does
Board Journal, 189, 58–62. homework improve academic achievement? A synthe-
Blatchford, P., Russell, A., Bassett, P., Brown, P., & Martin, C. sis of research, 1987–2003. Review of Educational
(2007). The effect of class size on the teaching of pupils Research, 76, 1–62.
672 M. Gettinger and M.J. Walter

Daly, P. M., & Ranalli, P. (2003). Using Countoons to Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (2003). Looking in class-
teach self-monitoring skills. Teaching Exceptional rooms (9th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Children, 35(5), 30–35. Goodman, L. (1990). Time and learning in the special
Dawson, P. (2007). Best practices in managing homework. education classroom. Albany, NY: State University of
In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in New York Press.
school psychology V (pp. 1073–1084). Bethesda, MD: Guskey, T. R. (2001). Mastery learning. In N. J. Smelser
National Association of School Psychologists. & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic moti- social and behavioral sciences (pp. 9372–9377).
vations: Classic definitions and new directions. Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67. Guthrie, J. T., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and moti-
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (2008). Self-determination theory: vation in reading. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D.
A macrotheory of human motivation, development Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research
and health. Canadian Psychology, 49(3), 182–185. (Vol. 3, pp. 403–424). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Denham, C., & Lieberman, A. (Eds.). (1980). Time to learn. Haertel, G. D., Walberg, H. F., & Weinstein, T. (1983).
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. Psychological models of educational performance:
DuPaul, G. J., & Stoner, G. (2003). ADHD in the schools: A theoretical synthesis of constructs. Review of
Assessment and intervention strategies. New York: Educational Research, 53, 75–91.
Guilford Press. Harmin, M., & Toth, M. (2006). Inspiring active learning:
Fisher, C. W., & Berliner, D. C. (1985). Perspectives on A complete handbook for today’s teachers. Alexandria,
instructional time. New York: Longman. VA: Association of Supervision and Curriculum
Fisher, D. (2009). The use of instructional time in the typi- Development.
cal high school classroom. The Educational Forum, Harnishfeger, A., & Wiley, D. (1976). The teaching-learn-
73, 168–173. ing process in elementary schools: A synoptic view.
Fowler, W. (1995). School size and student outcomes. Curriculum Inquiry, 6, 5–43.
Advances in Educational Productivity, 5, 3–26. Harnishfeger, A., & Wiley, D. (1985). Origins of active
Fredrick, W. C., & Walberg, H. J. (1980). Learning as a learning time. In C. W. Fisher & D. C. Berliner (Eds.),
function of time. The Journal of Educational Research, Perspectives on instructional time (pp. 133–156).
73, 183–194. New York: Longman.
Frieberg, H. J., & Driscoll, A. (2000). Universal teaching Hollywood, T. M., Salisbury, C. I., Rainforth, B., &
strategies (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Palombaro, M. (1995). Use of instructional time in
Gall, M. (1984). Synthesis of research on teachers’ ques- classrooms serving students with and without severe
tioning. Educational Leadership, 42, 40–47. disabilities. Exceptional Children, 61, 242–254.
Gettinger, M. (1984). Individual differences in time Huitt, W. (1995). A systems model of the teaching/learn-
needed for learning: A review of the literature. ing process. Educational Psychology Interactive.
Educational Psychologist, 19, 15–29. Valdosta, GA: College of Education, Valdosta State
Gettinger, M. (1985). Time allocated and time spent rela- University. Retrieved March, 2010, from http://teach.
tive to time needed for learning as determinants of valdosta.edu/whuitt/materials/tchlrnmd.html
achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, Karweit, N. (1989). Time and learning: A review. In R. E.
3–11. Slavin (Ed.), School and classroom organization
Gettinger, M. (1989). Effects of maximizing time spent (pp. 69–95). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
and minimizing time needed on pupil achievement. Karweit, N., & Slavin, R. E. (1981). Measurement and
American Educational Research Journal, 26, 73–91. modeling choices in studies of time and learning.
Gettinger, M., & Ball, C. (2007). Best practices in increas- American Educational Research Journal, 18, 151–171.
ing academic engaged time. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes Kauchak, D. P., & Eggen, P. D. (2003). Learning and
(Eds.), Best practices in school psychology V teaching: Research-based methods (4th ed.). Boston:
(pp. 1043–1075). Bethesda, MD: National Association Allyn & Bacon.
of School Psychologists. Kelshaw-Levering, K., Sterling-Turner, H. E., Henry, J.
Gettinger, M., & Kohler, K. (2006). Process-outcome R., & Skinner, C. H. (2000). Randomized interdepen-
approaches to classroom management and effective dent group contingencies. Psychology in the Schools,
teaching. In C. M. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), 37, 523–534.
Handbook of classroom management: Research, prac- Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feed-
tice, and contemporary issues (pp. 73–96). Mahwah, back interventions on performance: A historical review,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback interven-
Gettinger, M., & Stoiber, K. (2009). Effective teaching tion theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 254–284.
and effective schools. In T. B. Gutkin & C. R. Reynolds Kubitschek, W. N., Hallinan, M. T., Arnett, S. M., &
(Eds.), The handbook of school psychology (4th ed., Galipeau, K. S. (2005). High school schedule changes
pp. 769–790). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. and the effect of lost instructional time on achieve-
Gijselaers, W. H., & Schmidt, H. G. (1995). Effects of ment. High School Journal, 89, 63–71.
quantity of instruction on time spent on learning and Leinhardt, G. (1978). Applying a classroom process
achievement. Educational Research and Evaluation, model to instructional evaluation. Curriculum Inquiry,
1, 183–201. 8, 155–176.
31 Classroom Strategies to Enhance Academic Engaged Time 673

Levendoski, L. S., & Cartledge, G. (2000). Self-monitoring Shapiro, E. S., & Cole, C. L. (1994). Behavior change
for elementary school children with emotional in the classroom: Self-management interventions.
disturbances: Classroom application for increased New York: Guilford Press.
academic responding. Behavioral Disorders, 25, Shimabukuro, S. M., Prater, M. A., Jenkins, A., & Edelen-
211–224. Smith, P. (1999). The effects of self-monitoring of aca-
Litow, L., & Pumroy, D. (1975). A brief review of class- demic performance on students with learning
room group-oriented contingencies. Journal of Applied disabilities and ADD/ADHD. Education and Treatment
Behavior Analysis, 8, 341–347. of Children, 22, 397–414.
Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional Smith, B. A. (2000). Quantity matters: Annual instruction
activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high time in an urban school system. Educational Admini-
school years. American Educational Research Journal, stration Quarterly, 36, 652–682.
37, 153–184. Squires, D., Huitt, W., & Segars, J. (1983). Effective class-
Marzano, R. J., Pickering, D. J., & Pollock, J. E. (2001). rooms and schools: A research-based perspective.
Classroom instruction that works: Research-based Washington, DC: Association for Supervision and
strategies for increasing student achievement. Curriculum Development.
Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Stanley, S. O., & Greenwood, C. R. (1981). CISSAR:
Curriculum Development. Code for instructional structure and student academic
Mory, E. H. (1992). The use of informational feedback in response. Observer’s manual. Kansas City, KS:
instruction: Implications for future research. University of Kansas, Bureau of Child Research,
Educational Technology Research and Development, Juniper Gardens Children’s Project.
40, 5–20. Stichter, J. P., Stormont, M., & Lewis, T. J. (2008).
Moxley, R. A. (1998). Treatment-only designs and student Instructional practices and behavior during reading:
self-recording as strategies for public school teachers. A descriptive summary and comparison of practices in
Education and Treatment of Children, 21, 37–61. Title I and non-Title I elementary schools. Psychology
Mulholland, R., & Cepello, M. (2006). What teacher can- in the Schools, 46, 172–183.
didates need to know about academic learning time. Tomlinson, C. A. (2003). Differentiating instruction for
International Journal of Special Education, 21, academic diversity. In M. Cooper (Ed.), Classroom
63–73. teaching skills (7th ed., pp. 134–165). New York:
National Education Commission on Time and Learning Houghton Mifflin.
(NECTL). (1994). Prisoners of time: Report of the Trammel, D. L., Schloss, P. T., & Alper, S. (1994). Using
National Commission on Time and Learning. self-recording, evaluation, and graphing to increase
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. completion of homework assignments. Journal of
(reprinted in 2005). Learning Disabilities, 27, 75–81.
Odden, A. R., & Archibald, S. J. (2009). Doubling student van den Hurk, M., Wolfhagen, H., Dolmans, D., & van der
performance…and finding the resources to do it. Vleuten, C. (1998). The relation between time spent
Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin. on individual study and academic achievement in a
Ostrosky, M. M., Jung, E. Y., Hemmeter, M. L., & Thomas, problem-based curriculum. Advances in Health
D. (2003). Helping children make transitions between Sciences Education, 3, 43–49.
activities. Washington, DC: Center on the Social and Wang, M. C., Haertel, G. D., & Walberg, H. J. (1993).
Emotional Foundations for Early Learning. Toward a knowledge base for school learning. Review
Prater, M. A. (1992). Increasing time-on-task in the class- of Educational Research, 63, 249–294.
room: Suggestions for improving the amount of time Wehmeyer, M. L., Agran, M., Hughes, C., Martin, J. E.,
learners spend in on-task behaviors. Intervention in Mithaug, D. E., & Palmer, S. B. (2007). Promoting
School and Clinic, 28, 22–27. self-determination in students with developmental dis-
Rangel, E. S. (2007). Time to learn. Research Points, 5(2), abilities. New York: Guilford Press.
104. Wiley, D. E., & Harnishfeger, A. (1974). Explosion of a
Reid, R. (1996). Research in self-monitoring with stu- myth: Quantity of schooling and exposure to instruc-
dents with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning tion: Major educational vehicles. Educational
Disabilities, 29, 317–331. Researcher, 3, 7–11.
Rock, M. L. (2005). Use of strategic self-monitoring to Woelfel, K. (2005). Learning takes time for at-risk stu-
enhance academic engagement, productivity, and dents. Education Digest, 71(4), 28–30.
accuracy of students with and without disabilities. Wyne, M., & Stuck, G. (1982). Time and learning:
Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 7, 3–17. Implications for the classroom teacher. The Elementary
Rosenshine, B. (1995). Advances in research on instruc- School Journal, 83, 67–75.
tion. The Journal of Educational Research, 88, Zimmerman, B., Greenberg, D., & Weinstein, C. (1994).
262–268. Self-regulating academic study time: A strategy
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination approach. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.),
theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, Self-regulation of learning and performance: Issues
social development, and well-being. American and educational applications (pp. 181–199). Hillsdale,
Psychologist, 55, 68–78. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Deep Engagement as a Complex
System: Identity, Learning Power 32
and Authentic Enquiry

Ruth Deakin Crick

Abstract
This chapter develops a definition of engagement which is underpinned by
a participatory enquiry paradigm and invites an exploration of patterns
and relationships between variables rather than a focus on a single vari-
able. It suggests that engagement is best understood as a complex system
including a range of interrelated factors internal and external to the learner,
in place and in time, which shape his or her engagement with learning
opportunities. The implications of this approach are explored first in terms of
student identity, learning power and competences and second in terms
of student participation in the construction of knowledge through authen-
tic enquiry. Examples are used to illustrate the arguments which have been
generated from research into the theory and practice of Learning Power
and from the Learning Futures programme in the UK and Australia. The
chapter argues that what is necessary for deep engagement in the twenty-
first century is a pedagogy and an assessment system which empower
individuals to become aware of their identity as learners through making
choices about what, where and how they learn and to make meaningful
connections with their life stories and aspirations in authentic pedagogy.
In this context, the teacher is a facilitator or coach for learning rather than
a purveyor of expert knowledge.

and leaders on teaching and the acquisition of


Introduction knowledge, skills and understanding predeter-
mined by national curricula and assessed against
The focus in education policy in the last two
‘standards’. Essentially the process is ‘top down’ –
decades on measuring and raising academic stan-
students are recipients of predetermined knowl-
dards has increased the attention of policy makers
edge sets and the task of teachers is to make the
experience as engaging as possible for young peo-
ple. Whilst this ‘delivery’ model works for some,
R.D. Crick (*)
Graduate School of Education,
particularly students whose social and cultural
University of Bristol, Bristol, UK capital enables them to ‘buy in’ to this agenda, for
e-mail: Ruth.Deakin-Crick@bristol.ac.uk too many there is increasing disengagement which

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 675


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_32, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
676 R.D. Crick

manifests as either passive compliance or more human – and thus contemporary approaches to
active rejection of the status quo (Wehlage & student engagement in learning reflect these
Rutter, 1986). The compliant disengaged may not worldviews. Bottery’s (1992) analysis of four
be noticed unless they are at a critical borderline major Western educational ideologies demon-
in terms of the school’s target outcomes, but active strates how each has a differing view of the child,
rejecters of the status quo vote with their feet, the teacher, the nature of knowledge, assessment
causing considerable political concern. The focus and purpose of schooling. In the intervening two
on outcomes concentrates pedagogical attention decades since Bottery’s analysis, a dominant ide-
on the public and measurable aspects of learning. ology influencing approaches to the reform of
Whilst this is important, if it is at the expense of education combines managerialism (or the ‘new
the personal and less easily measurable aspects of public management’) and public choice theory
learning, such as learning identity and the disposi- (Aucoin, 1990; Self, 2000). For Goldspink
tions, values and attitudes necessary for students (2007b, p. 77), managerialism is an application
to be able to take advantage of particular learning of managerial method to public institutions and
opportunities, then there is an impact on the qual- public choice theory is an extension of the logic
ity of student engagement. Engagement in the of economic markets to administrative and politi-
form of compliance with a particular school and cal exchange (Stretton & Orchard, 1994; Udehn,
family culture may yield learning that is fragile 1996). This ideology, combined with curricula
and dependent, with a passive acceptance and shaped by traditional subjects, with underlying
memorisation of rules, concepts and information assumptions of scientific reductionism, leads to a
and ways of doing things transmitted in traditional tendency towards what Perkins (2010) describes
ways. Such ‘passive’ engagement does not equip as ‘elementitis’. This is a way of approaching
the learner to cope when things go wrong, or are complexity by focusing on the elements rather
no longer straightforward, or when knowledge than the whole, or what Darling-Hammond
needs to be applied in complex situations or inte- (1997) described as a ‘piecemeal curriculum’, or
grated into a personal narrative. In contrast to this, Langer (1989) as ‘mindless’ education.
deep engagement in learning requires personal It is perhaps not surprising in this context that
investment and commitment – learning has to be studies of engagement in learning have often
meaningful and purposeful in the life of the learner focused on elements rather than the whole.
and this is not procured simply by external Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and Paris (2004) sum-
demands (Haste, 2001). marise their review of engagement by suggesting
that the individual types of engagement (behav-
ioural, cognitive, emotional) have ‘not been stud-
Worldview Challenges ied in combination, either as results of antecedents
nor as influences on outcomes’ and that research
Underpinning these issues of student engagement has tended to use variable-centred rather than pat-
are two key ‘taken for granted’ worldview issues. tern-centred techniques, cross-sectional rather
The first is an epistemological one, to do with the than longitudinal. In other words, studies have
nature of knowledge and how human beings focused on particular elements of engagement,
come to know – that is, to encounter and appro- and few, if any, have attempted to look at engage-
priate existing funds of knowledge and to gener- ment from the perspective of all the relevant ele-
ate and re-formulate knowledge in new contexts. ments and the patterns and relationships between
The second is anthropological, to do with the them. The result is that we have little information
nature of the person who is learning – and how he about the interactions between different aspects of
or she develops a sense of self, learning, identity engagement and little information about the devel-
and purpose in different sociocultural contexts. opment and malleability of engagement over time
Educational practices are shaped by paradigmatic (Fredricks et al., 2004, p. 87). If engagement is a
views of both knowledge and what it means to be multidimensional construct, influenced by place,
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 677

Fig. 32.1 Understanding engagement in learning as a system of systems

time, cultural and social context, as well as factors (such as assessment practices and motivation for
internal to the person, then it follows that it is learning) rather than a focus on a single variable
important to understand the complex and dialecti- (such as only cognitive engagement). Next I explore
cal relationships between the relevant aspects and the implications of this in two ways: first for learn-
to understand engagement as a complex system of ing – in terms of student identity, learning power
systems, including systems internal to the student and competences; and second for curriculum – in
(such as motivation, agency, meaning making and terms of student participation in the construction of
identity) and in the environment (such as peda- knowledge. I will illustrate my argument with
gogy, management of learning and culture). empirical resources generated from two sources.
Figure 32.1 sets this out in diagrammatic form. First is the 10-year ‘ELLI’ research programme
In this chapter, I first explore a definition of (www.vitalpartnerships.com), which has explored
engagement which is underpinned by a participa- and examined the development of engaged learners
tory enquiry paradigm which invites an exploration who understand and are able to deploy their
of patterns and relationships between variables own learning power and the implications for
678 R.D. Crick

pedagogy, using the Effective Lifelong Learning many countries and is the approach advocated
Inventory (ELLI), a self-report inventory designed here because it enables a fuller theorisation about
to assess a person’s learning power. The more recent the person who is learning, his or her develop-
Learning Futures programme in the UK with its ment as a person in the community and the ways
innovative school level practices aimed at increas- in which proximal and distal social environments
ing student engagement in learning is the second influence that learning, which is important for
source. The Learning Futures programme was understanding deep engagement.
funded by the Paul Hamlyn Foundation, in partner- Deep engagement in learning is particularly
ship with the Innovation Unit and worked with a important in the fluid, networked and global
cluster of 15 schools to develop a model of deep twenty-first century world for two reasons, as
engagement in learning (Innovation Unit 2008). Bauman eloquently argues. First, the contempo-
These two programmes of professional devel- rary search for identity is ‘the side-effect and
opment and research, generated from different by-product of the combination of globalising and
sources, have involved several hundred teachers individualising pressures and the tensions they
and tutors and several thousand learners. The spawn’ (Bauman, 2001, p. 52) and, second, ‘edu-
focus of the ELLI programme has been on the cational philosophy and theory face the unfamiliar
dynamic assessment of learning power and ways and challenging task of theorising a formative
in which teachers and schools can progressively process which is not guided from the start by the
hand over responsibility for learning to students target form designed in advance’ (Bauman, 2001,
(e.g., Deakin Crick, 2009a; Deakin Crick & p. 139). We need a theory and practice of engage-
Grushka, 2010; Deakin Crick & Yu, 2008; ment in learning which facilitates the formation
Goodson & Deakin Crick, 2009; Jaros & Deakin of identity and combines this with processes for
Crick, 2007). The focus of the Learning Futures scaffolding and supporting the processes of
programme has been on the organizational condi- knowledge creation in a world where relevant
tions in schools which support engagement, outcomes can no longer be predetermined.
including enquiry-based learning, coaching and When a learner is deeply engaged in learning,
mentoring, school as base camp and school as he or she is an intentional participant in a social
learning commons (Deakin Crick, Jelfs, Ren, & process which is taking place over time. Seely
Symonds, 2010; Paul Hamlyn Foundation & Brown and Thomas (2009, p. 1) argue that we
Innovation Unit, 2010). need to embrace a theory of ‘learning to become’
in contrast to theories of learning which see
learning as a process of becoming something.
Deep Engagement They say that the twentieth century worldview shift
from learning as transmission to learning as inter-
Within the literature, it is common to distinguish pretation is now being replaced by learning as
between engagement measured by conformance participation – fuelled by structural changes in
or compliance (e.g., attendance), academic the way communication happens through new
engagement (e.g., commitment to a limited range technologies and media. Participation is embod-
of academic performance criteria or passing the ied and experienced – and embraces tacit as well
tests) and intellectual engagement. The former is as explicit knowledge (Polanyi, 1967).
concerned with whether students conform to the
The potential revolution for learning that the net-
rules of an institution – it has little to say about
worked world provides is the ability to create scal-
processes or outcomes of learning. The second able environments for learning that engages the
concentrates on a very limited subset of outcomes tacit as well as the explicit dimensions of knowl-
of schooling, whilst the last implies a more com- edge. The term we have been using for this, bor-
rowed from Polanyi is indwelling. Understanding
plete concern with learning process and outcomes
this notion requires us to think about the connection
at the whole person level. This last approach is between experience, embodiment and learning.
reflected in current policy goals for education in (Thomas & Seeley Brown, 2009, p. 10).
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 679

This way of knowing is fundamentally experi- & Beista, 2010; Goodson & Deakin Crick, 2009).
ential (Heron & Reason, 1997; Reason, 2005) It is critical because it involves ‘humanisation’
and positions the person, as learner, as part of a (Freire, 1972) and emancipatory rationality
whole in relation to fellow humans and the natu- (Habermas, 1973) and is embodied and located
ral world. Experiential knowing – through direct within the personal and communal narratives
encounter – is the distinguishing feature of a par- through which human beings seek and make
ticipatory enquiry paradigm and is the foundation meaning; thus, it is also ethical. Deep engage-
for the development of critical subjectivity (Heron ment leads to what Bateson (1972) describes as
& Reason, 1997). third-level learning, which involves personal
The experience of deep engagement then is transformation – rather than only repetition (pri-
multidimensional and implies participation and mary learning) or learning to learn (secondary
experience which leads to personal commitment learning).
and investment in learning over time (Fredricks
et al., 2004, p. 82). This form of engagement can
be understood as ‘deep’ in that it is prolonged, Engagement and Motivation
purposeful and enacted in a sociohistorical tra- for Learning
jectory. It inevitably includes an ethical dimen-
sion because it is about how a person embodies It is a sine qua non that in order to be engaged in
and enacts their learning in the world. The first learning, a person needs to be motivated to learn –
part of a working definition of deep engagement to have a ‘desire to engage’ of sufficient quality
in learning, or our way of recognising it when it that it drives the individual to take advantage of
happens, is when a learner becomes personally particular learning opportunities. Motivation thus
absorbed in and committed to participation in the precedes engagement. In a systematic review of
processes of learning and the mastery of a (cho- the impact of testing and assessment on students’
sen) topic, or task, to the highest level of which motivation for learning, Harlen and Deakin Crick
they are capable. This means that he or she will (2003a, 2003b) identified 19 studies from a total
be aware of, and attend to, the processes of learn- collection of 183 which explored, through differ-
ing, rather than just the outcome, and will utilise ent research designs, the impact of assessment on
his or her own power to learn to serve his or her students’ motivation for learning. Overall, the
chosen purpose – developing his or her learning review suggested that summative testing and
identity and mindfully using the scaffolding pro- assessment can unwittingly depress motivation
vided to pursue the journey towards his or her for learning and that motivation itself is a complex
chosen outcome. He or she will increasingly take construct which should be an outcome of educa-
responsibility for his or her own learning trajec- tion as well as a precedent. The study argued that
tory, and his or her learning will be meaningful to motivation for learning is influenced by a range of
him or her, both in his or her life beyond the psychosocial factors both internal to the learner
classroom and in the trajectory of his or her par- and present in the learner’s social and natural
ticular life story. environment. The American Psychological
This definition of engagement goes beyond Association’s Learner Centred Principles (1997)
the more recent consensus which has emerged focus on factors that are internal to and under the
around the integration of the cognitive, affective control of the learner, as well as taking account of
and behavioural elements of engagement the environmental and contextual factors which
(Fredricks et al., 2004; Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000) interact with those internal factors (McCombs &
because it assumes a critical sociocultural con- Lauer, 1997). Of these 14 principles, three deal
text in which students identify value and purpose directly with motivation for learning. The first of
in their learning and take responsibility as agents these has to do with the motivational and emo-
of their learning, embodied in a particular context tional influences on learning, which are affected
in place and over time (Goodson, 2009; Goodson by the learner’s emotional state, beliefs, interests,
680 R.D. Crick

goals and habits of thinking. The second refers to (Bloomer, 2001; Bloomer & Hodkinson, 2000).
the learner’s creativity, higher-order thinking and Attending to the self raises challenges for con-
natural curiosity that contribute to intrinsic moti- temporary pedagogy, particularly within a high
vation to learn. Intrinsic motivation for learning is accountability, outcomes-focused framework;
stimulated by tasks of optimal novelty and diffi- the theoretical and practical implications are sig-
culty, relevant to personal interests and providing nificant. Finding ways of enabling learners to
for personal choice and control. The third principle make meaningful connections between their own
has to do with the effect of motivation on extended life story, the world in which they live, their
learner effort and guided practice – without moti- particular community and tradition and the pro-
vation to learn, the willingness to exert this effort cesses and content of their learning in school
is unlikely without coercion. These three broad requires a personalised and local approach and
principles indicate the range of factors that have to learning and assessment strategies which can
be taken into account when considering motiva- move easily between personal and public
tion for learning. They have to do with the learner’s domains. In the following sections, I begin to
sense of self, expressed through values and atti- explore some of the aspects of learning which are
tudes; with the learner’s engagement with learn- relevant to engagement, using the metaphor of
ing, including their sense of control and efficacy; learners as ‘authors’ heuristically. To be an
and with the learner’s willingness to exert effort to ‘author’ of one’s own learning suggests that (a)
achieve a learning goal. there is an agentic self who is producing the
None of the studies in this review dealt with ‘texts’ of learning, (b) there is a coherent story to
all the variables included in the concept of moti- be told and (c) there is a context in time and place
vation for learning, but the reviewers grouped within which the learning is taking place.
them according to the particular outcomes that
were investigated in terms of motivation for
learning. Expressed from a learner’s perspective, Elements of Deep Engagement
these three groups were as follows: in Learning
1. What I feel and think about myself as a learner
2. The energy I have for the task Perezhivanie: Resources of the Self
3. How I perceive my capacity to undertake the
task. An author does not arrive at the creation of a
This tripartite construction of the term moti- story empty handed. Rather he or she has already
vation for learning was developed in response to an idea to pursue, drawn from his or her experi-
the range of empirical studies on aspects of moti- ence and interest. In the same way, the learner
vation for learning drawn from around the world. arrives at a learning opportunity already possess-
It goes beyond a behavioural definition and draws ing a way of knowing and being in the world
attention to the ‘personhood’ and the identity of which is the sum of their experience to date.
the learner. This attention to the self of the learner Vygotsky (1962/1934, 1978) described this as
is important because the capacity of the individ- ‘Perezhivanie’, the term used for accumulated
ual to become the ‘author’ of their own learning lived emotional experience, including values,
is another defining feature of both motivation for attitudes, beliefs, schemas and affect. For
and deep engagement in learning. The ‘author’ Vygotsky, Perezhivanie is the process through
metaphor implies intentional self-direction which interactions in the ‘zone of proximal devel-
(Black, McCormick, James, & Pedder, 2006) and opment’ are perceived by the learner. The ‘zone
the creation of a unique story. However, beyond of proximal development’ is entered when a
this, and relevant to engagement, are the lateral learner and a more experienced other participate
and temporal connectivities which shape a per- in a relationship of ‘cognitive scaffolding’
son’s sense of self, particularly personal and through which the learner becomes more capable
communal stories and networks of relationships of achieving particular learning outcomes through
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 681

world
views &
values Social context

family
relationship

The Learner
friendships

cognition affect
community
culture values
Agency,
Identity,
schema
Story
Learning relationships
scripts Coaching for learning
Trust affirmation and
behaviour challenge
assessment
Learning tools
practices
Learning
pedagogical Facilitator
practices

Assessment
practices

Fig. 32.2 Perezhivanie and the zone of proximal development

modelling, imitation and repetition. What a Identity: The Missing Link


learner brings to learning in this context is deeply
personal and unique, although necessarily expe- Sfard and Prusak (2005) suggest that the notion
rienced and accumulated over time in the context of identity is the missing link between learning
of relationship, community and tradition. Mahn and its sociocultural context.
and John-Steiner (2002) argued that by expand- We believe that the notion of identity is a perfect
ing the scope of the examination of the zone of candidate for the role of “the missing link” in the
proximal development, we can understand it as a researchers’ story of the complex dialectic between
complex whole, a system of systems which learning and its socio-cultural context. We thus
concur with the increasingly popular idea of replac-
includes the interrelated and interdependent ele- ing the traditional discourse on schooling with the
ments of participants, environments, artefacts talk about “construction of identities” (Lave &
(such as computers or tools) and context. In order Wenger, 1991, p. 53) or about the “longer-term
to develop a theoretical purchase on this concept agenda of identity building” (Lemke, 2000; Nasir
& Saxe, 2003). (Sfard & Prusak, 2005, p. 15).
of ‘Perezhivanie’ and explore further its implica-
tions for engagement in learning, we will break it For Sfard and Prusak, identities are stories
down into ‘identity’, ‘story’ and ‘values, attitudes about persons. They define identities as ‘collec-
and dispositions’ (see Fig. 32.2). tions of stories about persons that are ‘reifying,
682 R.D. Crick

endorsable by others and significant’ and argue identity – then the facilitation task begins to look
that a person’s stories about themselves are pro- more like counselling because the task will be to
foundly influenced by the stories that important explore those factors in a person’s story which
others tell about that person. Identities are discur- block movement forwards and to help them to
sive counterparts of one’s lived experiences – they re-imagine a designated identity: who they want
are stories which are told and re-told and which to become. This relates to knowledge construc-
are open and susceptible to change. The impor- tion in that the starting point for engagement is
tance of this for understanding engagement in interest in something such as an object, or arte-
learning is that positive identity talk – that is, fact, or event or place. To be interested in some-
reifying statements such as ‘I am resilient’, or thing, that something has to have meaning to the
‘You are creative’ – makes people more able to ‘learner’ to connect to their life story in a particu-
engage with new challenges or opportunities in lar way. Building on that interest (which is per-
terms of their past experiences. Identity as a dis- sonal and idiosyncratic) are certain thinking and
cursive activity becomes an important bridge learning capabilities such as observing, generat-
between the lived experience a person brings to ing questions or more sophisticated knowledge
the learning encounter and the movement forwards mapping. These are all activities undertaken by
towards the construction of a new identity. Since the learner in the process of knowledge construc-
all learning includes a knowledge content – learn- tion about something and engagement in the task
ing is always about some new knowledge of some of knowledge construction is fuelled by its mean-
sort – it follows that the process of knowledge ingfulness to the learner.
construction can also scaffold identify formation.
Sfard and Prusak go on to operationalise their
definition of identity for learning by describing Personal and Community Stories
the gap between a person’s actual identity and
their designated identity: Within this participatory framing of learning, the
individual learner is not a ‘monad’ or an ‘island’
The reifying, significant narratives about a person
can be split into two subsets: actual identity, con- but is defined and realised in relation to other
sisting of stories about the actual state of affairs, people. He or she constructs meaning through
and designated identity, consisting of narratives time in the form of stories which are developed in
presenting a state of affairs which, for one reason the context of relationships, through telling, wit-
or another, is expected to be the case, if not now
then in the future (2005, p. 18). nessing and retelling. A person’s ‘reifying,
endorsable and significant stories’, which consti-
For the learner as ‘author’, the space between tute his or her identity, are developed discursively
the ‘actual’ and the ‘designated’ is a powerful site in relationships and community. This discursive
for engagement and another way of conceptualis- process inevitably draws on the wider commu-
ing the zone of proximal development. Pedagogy nity stories and worldviews that shape the ‘oughts
for engagement must first acknowledge this space and permissions’, the symbols and values and
and second facilitate the learner in actively and power structures in a particular community. Such
critically narrating the terrain it represents. Such stories are particular to time and place, embodied,
pedagogical skills of facilitation are more akin to told and re-told locally. They shape the habits,
coaching than to traditional teaching or mentor- traditions and rituals of learning – the disposi-
ing because the purpose is to facilitate the learner tions, values and attitudes which a learner brings
to become the author of his or her own learning to each encounter with new learning opportuni-
journey rather than to transmit information or ties. For example, in a community where unem-
know-how from an expert to a novice. Where a ployment is historical and widespread, young
person is severely disengaged from learning – for people growing up are likely to imbibe the wider
example, a young offender in prison for vio- community story of resignation and low aspira-
lent crime who may be ‘stuck’ with their actual tion and internalise it as part of their own actual
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 683

and designated identity. Their identity as learners – confidence in his or her capacity to move towards
that is, their sense of confidence to learn and his or her designated identity (changing and
change and their awareness of their own power to learning) and to utilise his or her social resources
learn – is therefore a key vehicle for self-directed to optimise his or her learning (learning relation-
change, aspiration and movement towards ships). Such personal qualities constitute learn-
designated identity. ing power – empirically derived clusters of
values, attitudes and dispositions which are nec-
essary for an individual’s engagement with learn-
Personal Qualities: Values, Attitudes ing opportunities.
and Dispositions for Learning These seven dimensions emerged from suc-
cessive factor analytic studies (Deakin Crick,
The process of moving from a particular identity 2004; Deakin Crick, Broadfoot, & Claxton,
towards a designated one is a discursive activity. 2004; Deakin Crick & Yu, 2008). They have been
In Vygotskian terms, it is also scaffolded – the constituted into the ELLI, a self-report question-
quality of relationships and the language used naire designed both to measure a person’s learn-
within this discursive activity are of crucial ing power at any moment in time and to stimulate
importance. As well as language about the con- personal change through providing a framework
tent of learning, it is the language of the values, for a coaching conversation between a learner
attitudes and dispositions for learning which the and teacher/facilitator. The online questionnaire
learner needs in order to engage with the task of produces a spider diagram, with no numbers,
change. The term ‘disposition’ is not sufficient to which represents what the individual says about
describe this because although it is in part a rich themselves in terms of the seven dimensions of
progeny of Aristotelian ‘hexus’ and connects with learning power. Ten years of studies with
Bourdiuesian ‘habitus’, it is all too often reduced school-age children, students in further and
simply to a ‘tendency to behave in a certain way’. higher education and adults in the workplace
What is being described here is a set of personal have demonstrated the value of awareness of
qualities or orientations towards learning these dimensions of learning power in stimulat-
which are understood and manifested in thought, ing engagement in learning.
feeling and action and derived from values and
attitudes – sets of beliefs with affective loading.
The term ‘learning power’ is more appropriate Learning Power and Engagement
because it incorporates values, attitudes and dis- at Work: An Illustration
positions and in addition invokes the important
concept of agency and use. The importance of the relationship between per-
For example, say a learner has chosen to sonal learning power and engagement is particu-
engage with learning about volcanoes. His or her larly stark in remote indigenous communities in
designated identity is to become ‘someone who northern Australia where there is a powerful leg-
knows a lot about volcanoes’. To become that acy of marginalisation and the systematic disen-
designated person, he or she will need to utilise franchisement over 200 years of a particular way
his or her curiosity in uncovering information, he of life with its unique ways of knowing, being
or she will need to be creative in order to devise and relating, traditions and rituals. As an extreme
ways of understanding how volcanoes work and example (alas not the only one), it has explana-
resilient in the face of challenge. He or she will tory power for mainstream pedagogy. What fol-
need to map new knowledge to what he or she lows is an explanation of how learning power can
already knows (meaning making) and have a form a bridge between individual and community
sense of the extent and purpose of the task and identity and engagement in formal learning
what resources he or she needs to deploy (strate- opportunities and then a particular example of
gic awareness). He or she will need a level of their application.
684 R.D. Crick

In order for young people in these communi- Learning Power and Engagement
ties to become authors of their own learning, and in Indigenous Australian Communities
to articulate positive designated identities, there
is a pedagogical imperative for the facilitation of The following example, drawn from research and
authentic connections between these particular ways development projects in Northern Territory,
of knowing, being and doing in particular com- Australia, provides a graphic illustration of the
munities and the discursive tasks of identity for- power of cyberspace to enable a remote indige-
mation and the generation of learning power. nous community to connect their traditional cul-
Metaphor, symbol, image and narrative are pow- ture with the ideas and practices of learning
erful ways of forming a bridge between two power drawn from research. Damien is a teacher
worlds, between a particular culture and learning in Gapuwiyak School who has led the commu-
power (Deakin Crick & Grushka, 2010; Grushka, nity in identifying six birds from the Yolongu
2009). They are epistemologically rich because sacred songlines, and a seventh bird, which is not
they form a link between two worlds – the experi- sacred, which function as metaphors and symbols
ence and ‘Perezhivanie’ of the learner and his or for the seven dimensions of learning power. For
her community and ideas and practices for learn- example, the Sea Eagle, or Djert, was chosen to
ing and (re) engagement in public ‘curricula’. represent the quality of critical curiosity, and the
Metaphor, image and story can create conditions Emu, or Wurrpan, was chosen to represent the
for the development of deep learning which car- quality of strategic awareness. After long discus-
ries the qualities of the development of critical sions with the whole community, these seven
subjectivity (Heron & Reason, 1997) through birds were ratified by the elders, painted in origi-
generating and linking experiential, presenta- nal indigenous art forms and used to communi-
tional, narrative and propositional knowledge. cate about learning power with the community. In
In the twenty-first century, even the most this picture, Damian has copied his own learning
remote communities are both local and global – power profile from the computer onto a white-
cyberspace is ubiquitous. Remote and relatively board and attached the original paintings of the
underdeveloped communities are connected to seven birds at the relevant points of his own spi-
cyberculture through mobile technologies. The der diagram. He is facilitating his community in
shaping power of cyberspace creates both chal- understanding learning power as part of the dis-
lenges and opportunities for identity construction cursive act of identity formation – and giving an
and engagement in learning. The sheer complexity invitation to the community to participate in
and volume of communication and information learning, to re-create constructive learning iden-
overwhelm traditional ways of organising and tities, which will facilitate the construction of
communicating knowledge whilst opening up new designated identities (Deakin Crick, Grushka,
new opportunities and necessities. Cyberspace Heitmeyer, & Nicholson, 2010) (Fig. 32.3).
challenges traditional ways of living and learning
whilst at the same time enabling their reconstruc-
tion and reformulation because it makes knowl- The Relationship Between Learning
edge and information widely available and Power and Engagement
provides new tools and artefacts for participation
which can transgress geographical and economic The model of deep engagement described in this
boundaries. For example, the ELLI tool is stored chapter is one which connects the learner’s sense
online on cloud servers, in a secure repository of identity and agency with their personal learning
called ‘The Learning Warehouse’ and local power, and these are utilised by the learner in a
organisations use a ‘portal’ to access the tools meaningful process of knowledge construction
and ideas, whilst a ‘trade entrance’ enables which leads to active engagement in the world.
researchers with appropriate permissions to These have been referred to elsewhere as four sta-
access anonymised data for analysis. tions in the learning journey (Deakin Crick, 2009b).
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 685

Fig. 32.3 Identity formation as a discursive community activity

Self Competent agent

Identity Dispositions Skills Competent learner,


Desire Values Knowledge Citizen, mathematician, scientist
etc
Motivation Attitudes Understanding

Personal Public

Fig. 32.4 Four pedagogical moments for deep engagement

They represent four pedagogical moments which in the world (Heron & Reason, 1997). The follow-
require attention for deep engagement in learning ing sections address key pedagogical themes
(Fig. 32.4). which are important in this process: language and
Research into learning power suggests that its place, coaching relationships and conversations
pedagogical value is important for engagement and scaffolding the process of knowledge
because self-assessment of values, attitudes and construction.
dispositions for learning (learning power) pro-
vides a framework for a coaching conversation
which both reflects ‘backward’ to the individual’s Language and Place
learning identity whilst also providing a frame-
work for scaffolding the journey forwards The research into learning power provides a lan-
towards the construction of new knowledge and guage which can be appropriated differently in
its meaningful application. This is a pathway diverse communities, in diverse places, and can be
for the development of critical subjectivity since used to conduct ‘identity talk’ about learning and
it engages experiential, presentational, proposi- about how a person might choose to engage with
tional and practical ways of knowing as a pathway learning opportunities. If identity formation is a
towards intelligent and principled participation discursive activity, then it follows that language is
686 R.D. Crick

Fig. 32.5 An individual learning power profile with pre- and post-measures

required as a medium for that discourse. The richer the zone between the identity of the learner and a
the language and the more it reflects, harmonises particular negotiated learning outcome. The fact
and critiques the community’s stories, which that the assessment is based on a measurement
shape the individual’s, the more useful it is in cre- derived from a self-report questionnaire is impor-
ating the conditions for engagement. What Damien tant because it reflects back to the individual what
was doing in the picture, extending the verbal lan- they have said about themselves. The feedback
guage of learning power through art, was creating (Fig. 32.5) is in the form of a spider diagram
a visual language which connects the deep experi- without numbers, and the purpose is for the
ential knowledge of his people with the concepts learner and a coach or facilitator to reflect on the
and ideas of learning power. Engagement in learning shape, how it connects to lived experience and
is always placed and particular (Deakin Crick & how it might be changed, or on the changes to the
Grushka, 2010; Deakin Crick, Grushka et al., 2010; shape after a second assessment event. The
Goodson & Deakin Crick, 2009). In this example, shaded inner spider diagram represents the pre-
it was significant simply to be having these conver- test measure from the self-report questionnaire,
sations with people who have been traditionally and the post-test measure is the single, outer line.
disenfranchised from formal western schooling In this case, the individual’s second self-assess-
traditions. The first stage is to open up the possibil- ment shows an increase in critical curiosity, cre-
ity of learning and change – the next step is to util- ativity, learning relationships and strategic
ise that hopefulness, through coaching conversations awareness. This was a young person in higher
in the context of trustful relationships so that the education in Bahrain, who had experienced
individual can begin to identify and appropriate coaching conversations with a tutor about her
more formal learning opportunities. spider diagram, which included the formulations
of targets – what the individual wanted to change
and why and how she could achieve that change
Framework for a Coaching in the highly academic context in which she was
Conversation learning.
Feedback alone is not sufficient for deep
A learning power profile provides a framework engagement. For deep engagement, the assess-
for such coaching conversations which move in ment event needs to be located within a pedagogy
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 687

which attends to ‘identity’ and ‘authorship’ in Chicago by Newman and colleagues (Newmann,
learning, in a community which operationalises 1996; Newmann & Wehlage, 1995; Newmann
both a shared language with which to describe et al., 1996) and the related research in Australia
learning power and pedagogical skill in coaching into quality teaching (Goldspink, 2007a, 2007b,
as well as teaching. What is necessary is a peda- 2008; Ladwig & Gore, 2004; Ladwig & King,
gogy and an assessment system which empowers 1991, 2003).
individuals to become aware of their identity as The dimensions of learning power contribute
learners through making choices about both what, to approaches to enquiry which are authentic and
where and how they learn, and to make meaning- active because they bring a structure to learning
ful connections with their life stories and aspira- which is assessable at key stages in the process
tions in authentic pedagogy. In this context, the and facilitates the process of identity construc-
teacher is a facilitator or coach for learning, rather tion. Research and development studies focusing
than a purveyor of expert knowledge. The quality on the learning power dimensions and enquiry
of trust is a core resource for such coaching (Bryk identified eight distinct stages in a sequential but
& Schneider, 2002). iterative and cumulative enquiry pathway which
map onto four key aspects of pedagogy for
engagement: the self who is learning, the per-
Scaffolding the Process of Knowledge sonal learning power necessary for engaging with
Construction learning opportunities; the construction of knowl-
edge and its application in the real world (Deakin
The third theme is that the dimensions of learning Crick et al., 2007; Jaros & Deakin Crick, 2007).
power provide a framework for the journey from These begin with the personal, local and experi-
a chosen starting point, where the self is engaged, ential choice of the learner and move from there,
towards a negotiated learning outcome. The most invoking an increasingly complex sequence of
powerful engagement for learning occurs where thinking and learning capabilities, to an encoun-
learning is authentic, active and enquiry led ter with pre-existing funds of knowledge which
(Newmann, 1996; Newmann, Marks & Gamoran, constitute the formal curriculum. The learner is
1996). The first condition for this is when the coached in that journey by a facilitator/coach
learner is personally involved in selecting the who supports him and provides prompts, guid-
focus for their enquiry which has meaning and ance and resources at key points. The sequence
relevance to them in their lives beyond the class- begins with the person of the learner and her
room and where the learner is the ‘author’ of their choice. It is described in Fig. 32.6 in table form,
own learning journey. The second is where learn- then in narrative.
ing is designed as enquiry: the co-construction of
knowledge through disciplined enquiry which First: Choosing: The student is encouraged to
involves building on a prior knowledge base, choose an object or place that fascinates them.
striving for in-depth understanding and express- Careful, ‘hands-off’ prompting and guidance
ing findings through elaborated communication. may be needed from the facilitator/coach to
The third is when the learner is actively engaged ensure that personal interest is strong and authen-
in the production of discourse, products or per- tic. The rest of the process will be highly influ-
formances that have relevance to learners beyond enced by the integrity of this choosing process.
school and require more active engagement than Sometimes the ‘object’ turns out to be a person or
simply repetition, retrieving information and event – it is its susceptibility to observation and
memorisation of facts or rules (Deakin Crick, the strength of the student’s interest and engage-
Jelfs et al., 2010). These findings from the ment that are important.
Learning Futures project, drawing on both quan-
titative and qualitative data, are consistent with Second: Observing/describing: The learner
the research in authentic pedagogy developed in observes and describes the chosen object/place,
688 R.D. Crick

Fig. 32.6 Sequence Eight steps-for framing pedagogy


of stages in personalised
enquiry Thinking and Learning Capabilities

Choosing and deciding PERSONAL CHOICE

Observing and describing OBSERVE AND DESCRIBE

Questioning GENERATE QUESTIONS

Storying UNCOVER NARRATIVES

Mapping CREATE KNOWLEDGE MAP

Connecting CONNECT TO EXISTING KNOWLEDGE

Reconciling RECONCILE WITH ASSESSMENT PURPOSE

Validating CONDUCT ASSESSMENT EVENT

Applying APPLY NEW KNOWLEDGE

both as a separate, objective entity and in relation Fourth: Storying: The questioning leads to a sense
to their own interest and reasons for choosing it. of narrative, both around the chosen object and in
In this, the learner is developing their sense of the unfolding of new learning. Historical and
personal responsibility. This initiates the cycles present realities lead to a sense of ‘what might be’
of a personal development process which is both for the object/place and for the learner and
recorded in a workbook and in which the student, their learning. She or he is becoming the author of
tutor and later others participate. It requires the his or her own ‘learning story’ or journey.
student to develop the critical curiosity and stra-
tegic awareness necessary for independent learn- Fifth: Mapping: The learner begins to discern that
ing, in the context of learning relationships. The this ‘ad hoc’ narrative leads in turn to new con-
student is also developing a sense of himself or cepts, propositions and knowledge. Self-
herself as a learner who can change and grow referenced learning starts to be related to a wider
over time. awareness of the ‘other’. The learning becomes a
‘knowledge map’ which can be used to make
Third: Questioning: The learner starts asking sense of the journey and of new learning as it
questions: obvious, but open ones, such as How comes into view. The student is ‘making meaning’
did it get there? What was there before? Why is it by connecting new learning to the ‘story so far’.
how it is? Who uses it? How and why did they get
involved? He or she is initiating and conducting a Sixth: Connecting: With informed guidance and
process of inquiry and investigation, driven by support from the teacher, the student’s widening
personal interest and shaped in turn by the ‘map’ of knowledge can be related to existing
answers to his or her own questions. The learner maps or models of the world: scientific, histori-
is exercising and developing critical curiosity. All cal, social, psychological, theological, philo-
the time, the student is encouraged to reflect on sophical… This is where awareness of the
their motivation, reasoning and identity as a diversity of possible ‘avenues of learning’
motivator of their own learning. becomes useful. It requires the tutor or teacher to
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 689

act as supporter, encourager and ‘tour guide’ in These sequential but iterative stages of authen-
the student’s encounter with established and spe- tic enquiry frame a pedagogy which integrates
cialist sources and forms of knowledge. the identity and personhood of the learner with
the process of knowledge construction. Although
Seventh: Reconciling: The student arrives at the in practice, they are not linear or strictly sequen-
interface between their personal inquiry and the tial – each stage may be revisited in a spiral for-
specialist requirements of curriculum, course, mation throughout the enquiry project – these are
examination or accreditation. The student’s nevertheless key aspects of knowledge construc-
development as learner enables them to encoun- tion which frame enquiry from the ‘bottom up’,
ter specialist knowledge and make sense of it, in that is, from the lived experience of the learner in
relation to what they already know and in the way the real world to an outcome which can demon-
they already learn, interrogating it and interacting strate higher-order creative and critical thinking,
with it, instead of simply ‘receiving’ it, using the in contrast to traditional pedagogy which is ‘top
model of learning and ‘knowledge mapping’ down’ and begins with the (prescribed) knowl-
skills they have developed through the inquiry. edge itself, where the teacher’s job is to make the
This is where the resilience will be tested, that experience of acquiring that knowledge as mean-
will have started to grow through the responsibil- ingful as possible to the learner. In authentic
ity and challenge of a self-motivated inquiry. enquiry, the problems are formulated by the
learners themselves – they are seeking to answer
Eighth: Validating: The student can forge links questions which they own, rather than find solu-
between what he or she now knows and institu- tions to other people’s problems or questions.
tional and social structures receptive to it: quali- For deep engagement, it is this connection
fications, job opportunities, learning opportunities, between ‘experiential knowing’ and a ‘knowledge
needs, initiatives, outlets, relationships, accredi- product’ which is crucial. Even where a com-
tation, publication…. This may take the form of a pletely free choice of starting points in enquiry
portfolio, presentation or written essay, based on projects is not possible, for example, in some for-
the workbook, making explicit both process and mal educational contexts where ‘coverage of con-
outcomes of the inquiry. The learning has met its tent’ is a political necessity, these stepped processes
communicative purpose. The learner has created of enquiry ground and engage the learner in an
a pathway from subjective response and observa- authentic and active process of learning. Even in
tion towards the interface with established knowl- these circumstances, teachers can facilitate the
edge. In doing so, he or she has also achieved sort of authentic choice which connects with stu-
life-enhancing personal development by asking dents’ experiential knowledge, even though that
and answering such questions as: Who am I? choice may be boundaried by curricular demands.
What is my pathway? How did I get there? Where Stepped processes of enquiry can provide a form
does it lead me? What were the alternatives? of structured freedom which enables learners to
Who helped me and how? connect their learning with their own identity,
story and purpose and thus experience deep
Ninth: Applying: The student has completed an engagement. Without authentic choice on the part
authentic enquiry about an issue of significance of the student, there is less likelihood of making
and meaning in his or her life. This might be the these deep connections – and students may not get
solution to a problem, which can now be proto- the opportunity to frame their own questions and
typed and tested, or it might have identified an formulate authentic problems. Indeed, some forms
unfolding employment trajectory or niche or of project-based learning do not allow for this sort
raised citizenship issues which can be addressed of enquiry at all if they begin with predetermined
in the community. At this stage, the question is: problems or questions which already have prede-
How do I build on and consolidate this knowledge termined answers. The danger then is that the
that I have acquired? The enquiry is authentic and learner is more concerned with finding the right
useful in terms of both content and process. answer than formulating a solution.
690 R.D. Crick

The key role of the seven dimensions of learn- who were successfully and deeply engaging in
ing power is that where the learner is aware of their own learning. The school serves an econom-
their own learning power profile and chooses to ically deprived community, and ‘Craig’ himself
take responsibility for developing himself or her- faced many challenges arising from these condi-
self as a learner, then the dimensions of learning tions. He was 12 and he and his year group were
power provide scaffolding for negotiating these working in a specially framed curriculum slot (of
steps. For example, good choosing requires cre- about 7 hours per week) called ‘My World’, free
ativity and questioning requires critical curiosity. of prescribed content and framed by authentic
Knowledge mapping requires meaning making enquiry projects. Within this space, the class
and strategic awareness and reconciling requires selected their own focus for their enquiry, using
resilience and so on. Reciprocally, the enquiry learning power language as scaffolding. In this
process provides salient opportunities for build- case, the teacher had used the metaphor of an
ing strengths in selected learning power dimen- island where the class were marooned and had to
sions. There is thus an intimate relationship survive on their own, without their teacher. The
between learning power (dispositions, values and focus in this project in terms of content was on
attitudes) and authentic enquiry-based learning. ‘taking responsibility for my own life and learn-
Research in the Learning Futures project sug- ing’. The teacher framed the project, deliberately
gests that this approach represents a substantively gave the students responsibility for the selection
different paradigm for learning and schooling of content and process and was available to coach
from conventional models. In order to realise the and mentor them individually. The project con-
potential of engagement in learning that this cluded with an authentic assessment event in
vision represents, schools have needed to engage which groups of students presented their work to
in processes of profound change. Authentic learn- each other and community members. In the fol-
ing has been modelled at all levels in the system: lowing excerpt, Craig was being asked what he
student, teacher, school and networks. Such a had learned in his My World project:
school is personified by teachers, leaders and a Craig: Well .. when we was in our groups,
community who take collective responsibility for something I learnt really well is
student learning and work together in profes- my learning relationships and my
sional enquiry which is aligned to schools’ changing and learning……..Just
authentic goals. The school is characterised by sometimes …I used just to go off
people’s openness to learn, willingness to change, task. Then something happened,
professional courage, engagement in disciplined like a spark in my brain or some-
professional enquiry and a shared commitment to thing and all of a sudden I thought
a locally owned and defined language for organi- I may as well get a good education
zational, professional and personal learning and do like stuff, don’t talk about
(Deakin Crick, Jelfs, et al., 2010, p. 185). something I’m not meant to.
Craig: It’s just like … it’s given me an
experience of like the future. Like
A Narrative Example from Practice if I keep acting like a free child in
the future, I’m never going to get
In order to ground these ideas in student language anywhere….I think it’s kind of a
and practice, I shall draw on a piece of narrative gift like that I can actually develop
data from the Learning Futures project in the UK new skills without acting up or
in which we compiled over 180 hours of student nothing. I reckon yeah, it’s a gift.
talk about learning in schools which were seek- Interviewer: So if I looked at your map would
ing to be radical in their approach to engagement. I see this journey on your map?
This example is particularly useful for illustra- Craig: In the forest there’s a waterfall
tion and representative of many other students and I can’t get past it… God gives
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 691

Mr M the helicopter, helps me This excerpt demonstrates Craig’s sense of


over the waterfall – drops me identity and a movement towards a designated
down to a place I need to be. identity. He is moving from someone who was not
Interviewer: If you had to think of the one best engaged or focused on learning in school to some-
thing about My World what would one with an emerging vision of himself with a dif-
it be? The single most important ferent future. In a previous interview, he talked
thing that you value the most. about seeing people on the streets asking for money
Craig: The ELLI dimensions and not wanting to end up like that. In this same
Interviewer: Why is that then? interview, he talks about himself going to sixth
Craig: It’s helped me get a long life. form and getting a good job. It also shows how the
Interviewer: Helped you? language of learning power has enabled him to
Craig: Get a long life. understand himself and to project forwards towards
Interviewer: Say a bit more? a particular outcome. His perceptions of the differ-
Craig: Like I never used to know like all ence between the enquiry approach and the more
this stuff, like (inaudible) I never traditional ‘top down’ pedagogy are insightful and
knew it existed, like changing and demonstrate the beginnings of critical subjectivity.
learning and resilience. And as He is using experiential and presentational know-
soon as I got it all into my head, ing when he describes being stuck on the island by
I’ve never ever gave up on stuff a waterfall that he could not get past, until God
I need to reach my goal. gave his teacher a helicopter to help him get to the
Interviewer: So that’s the best thing, because place he needed to be. This was describing the pro-
it’s given you … found change he has experienced in his engage-
Craig: Strength to develop skills and ment in learning in school. He instinctively knows
get along life easier. that this level of engagement in own learning
Interviewer: And what … how does it differ enables him to critique what he experiences around
from your other classes then? him and what he is told. He is able to evaluate
Craig: Like French and all that? these capabilities in terms of a newly acquired
Interviewer: Like French and History and … strategic awareness of their value to his lifelong
Craig It’s like [French and History] just journey. He has seen, for himself it seems, the lim-
given me reams and reams of itations of the ‘free child’ which ‘used just to go off
facts, but like this has given me task’ and was ‘never going to get anywhere’ and
like tips, hints and helping me to has accepted as ‘a gift’ his newfound idea of him-
get a long life easier. self (designated identity) as someone who is
Interviewer: But do you think the facts are changing and learning and never giving up. He has
important? become engaged in his learning and its story and
Craig: Yes, sometimes I need them. the effect on his life is transformative.
Interviewer: Does My World help you with the
French and stuff?
Craig: Yeah. Conclusions
Interviewer: Yeah how does it help you with
the other stuff then? The ideas discussed in this chapter are in many
Craig: Like French and all that? …… It ways in their infancy, based on only 10 years of
helps me with my French to never research and development, within a growing, but
give up. Helps me to like develop nonetheless still limited, professional community.
skills with other people – critical At the heart of this work is the imperative to find
curiosity, like to tell … if they say and develop forms of pedagogy which apportion
something and they want you to equal significance to the formation of identity and
think it’s true, you can actually the development of personal learning power, as to
say it’s not … the traditional acquisition of knowledge, skills and
692 R.D. Crick

understanding beloved of conventional curricula. influence the level of a student’s engagement in


For learners to be deeply engaged in learning over learning in school. These range from the deeply
a life time, the learning needs to be personally sig- personal (such as identity) to the public (such as
nificant and meaningful to the learner, who also encounters with existing funds of knowledge and
needs to develop the necessary values, attitudes, assessment events). In a world of almost infinite
and dispositions – learning power – to engage complexity, endless change and multiple possibili-
with new learning opportunities and to forge their ties, our approach to engagement in learning needs
own purpose for learning and acting in the world. to be as complex and rich as the challenges we
There are many limitations of this research face. Understanding deep engagement as partici-
programme – much of it is small scale and mixed patory enquiry, with a set of pedagogical design
methodologically which brings its own chal- principles, which integrate the personal with the
lenges. Much of it has been practitioner led, and public, the process with the outcome, the local
Western contemporary structures of schooling are with the global, means that we can move beyond
not hospitable to it. There is scope for large impact the confines of the ‘classroom’ and ‘one size fits
studies, to explore the impact of this approach to all’ solutions towards a more flexible, imaginative
learning on both engagement over time and stan- and professionally rewarding way of designing
dard achievement outcomes; the subject matter and managing learning that is deep and engaging.
calls for new methods of educational enquiry that
can do justice to narrative, qualitative and quanti-
tative evidence as attention moves between the
References
personal (ipsative) and the public (standardised)
assessment outcomes of education, only some of American Psychological Association. (1997). Learner
which can be measured quantitatively. When all centred principles: A framework for school reform and
is said and done, the light in a learner’s eye that redesign. Washington, DC: American Psychological
denotes engagement may be recognised in prac- Association.
Aucoin, P. (1990). Administrative reform in public man-
tice but is much more challenging to investigate agement: Paradigms, principles, paradoxes and pen-
through traditional research methods. dulums. Governance, 3(2), 115–137.
What also becomes clear is the importance of Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. San
particular concepts of place and time, which have Francisco: Chandler.
Bauman, Z. (2001). Community seeking safety in an inse-
not been traditionally theorised in pedagogy: a cure world. Cambridge, MA: Polity Press.
learner is always embedded and embodied in a Black, P., McCormick, R., James, M., & Pedder, D.
particular place at a particular time and his or her (2006). Assessment for learning and learning how to
learning is a journey of which he or she must pro- learn: A theoretical enquiry. Research Papers in
Education, 21(2), 119–132.
gressively become the author. The language and Bloomer, M. (2001). Young lives, learning & transforma-
assessment practices of learning power provide a tion: Some theoretical considerations. Oxford Review
way of connecting the deeply personal with the of Education, 27(3), 429–447.
public and scaffolding the journey of learning as Bloomer, M., & Hodkinson, P. (2000). Learning careers:
Continuity and change in young people’s dispositions
enquiry rather than only as received transmission. to learning. British Educational Research Journal,
Exploring the concept of engagement from the 26(5), 583–597.
perspective of a participatory paradigm allows us Bottery, M. (1992). The ethics of educational manage-
to see it as a complex system of systems which ment. London: Cassell Educational Ltd.
Bryk, A., & Schneider, B. (2002). Trust in schools: A core
better reflects the reality of learners, classrooms,
resource for improvement. New York: Russell Sage.
schools and communities. A complex systems lens Darling-Hammond, L. (1997). The right to learn: A blue-
may be particularly valuable for understanding the print for creating schools that work. San Francisco:
ways in which the development of learning identi- Jossey Bass.
Deakin Crick, R. (2004). The ecology of learning. Paper
ties and deep engagement is history and commu-
presented at the ESRC Knowledge and Skills for
nity dependent. By accounting for complexity, it Learning to Learn Seminar Series, University of
becomes clear that there are many factors which Newcastle, Newcastle Upon Tyne, UK.
32 Deep Engagement as a Complex System… 693

Deakin Crick, R. (2009a). Inquiry-based learning: Grushka, K. (2009). Meaning and identities: A visual per-
Reconciling the personal with the public in a demo- formative pedagogy for socio-cultural learning.
cratic and archaeological pedagogy. Curriculum Curriculum Journal, 20(3), 237–251.
Journal, 20(1), 73–92. Guthrie, J., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and moti-
Deakin Crick, R. (2009b). Assessment in schools – dispo- vation in reading. In M. Kamil & P. Mosenthal (Eds.),
sitions. In B. McGaw, P. Peterson, & E. Baker (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 403–422).
The international encyclopedia of education (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Amsterdam: Elsevier. Habermas, J. (1973). Knowledge and human interests.
Deakin Crick, R. (2009c). Pedagogical challenges for per- Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
sonalisation: Integrating the personal with the public Harlen, W., & Deakin Crick, R. (2003a). A systematic
through context-driven enquiry. Curriculum Journal, review of the impact of summative assessment and
20(3), 185–189. testing on pupils’ motivation for learning. London:
Deakin Crick, R., Broadfoot, P., & Claxton, G. (2004). Evidence for Policy and Practice Co-ordinating Centre
Developing an effective lifelong learning inventory: Department for Education and Skills.
The ELLI Project. Assessment in Education, 11(3), Harlen, W., & Deakin Crick, R. (2003b). Testing and
248–272. motivation for learning. Assessment in Education,
Deakin Crick, R., & Grushka, K. (2010). Signs, symbols 10(2), 169–207.
and metaphor: Linking self with text in inquiry based Haste, H. (2001). Ambiguity, autonomy and agency. In
learning. Curriculum Journal, 21(1). D. Rychen & L. Salganik (Eds.), Definition and selec-
Deakin Crick, R., Grushka, K., Heitmeyer, D., & tion of competencies: Theoretical and conceptual
Nicholson, M. (2010). Learning, place and identity: foundations. Seattle, WA: OECD/Hogreffe/Huber.
An exploration of the affordances of a pedagogy of Heron, J., & Reason, P. (1997). A participatory inquiry
place amongst Indigenous Australian students. Bristol, paradigm. Qualitative Inquiry, 3(3), 274–294.
UK: University of Bristol. Innovation Unit. (2008). Learning futures: Next practice
Deakin Crick, R., Jelfs, H., Ren, K., & Symonds, J. (2010). in learning and teaching. London: Innovation Unit.
Learning futures. London: Paul Hamlyn Foundation. Jaros, M., & Deakin Crick, R. (2007). Personalised learn-
Deakin Crick, R., Small, T., Jaros, M., Pollard, K., Leo, ing in the post mechanical age. Journal of Curriculum
E., Hearne, P., et al. (2007). Inquiring minds: Studies, 39(4), 423–440.
Transforming potential through personalised learn- Ladwig, J., & Gore, J. (2004). Quality teaching in NSW
ing. London: Royal Society of Arts. public schools: An assessment practice guide. Sydney,
Deakin Crick, R., & Yu, G. (2008). The effective lifelong Australia: Professional Support and Curriculum
learning inventory (ELLI): Is it valid and reliable as a Directorate, NSW Department of Education and
measurement tool? Education Research, 50(4), Training.
387–402. Ladwig, J., & King, M. (1991). Restructuring secondary
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). social studies: The association of organizational fea-
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of tures and classroom thoughtfulness. Madison, WI:
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1), National Center on Effective Secondary Schools
59–109. [BBB24601].
Freire, P. (1972). Pedagogy of the oppressed. Ladwig, J., & King, M. (2003). Quality teaching in NSW
Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin. public schools: An annotated bibliography. Sydney,
Goldspink, C. (2007a). Rethinking educational reform – Australia: NSW Department of Education and Training.
A loosely coupled and complex systems perspective. Langer, E. (1989). Mindfulness. Reading, MA: Addison-
International Journal of Educational Management, Wesley.
Administration and Leadership, 35(1), 27–50. Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning:
Goldspink, C. (2007b). Transforming education: Evidential Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge, UK:
support for a complex systems approach. Emergence: Cambridge University Press.
Complexity and Organisation, 9(1–2), 77–92. Lemke, J. (2000). Across the scales of time: Artifacts,
Goldspink, C. (2008). Student engagement and quality activities, and meanings in ecosocial systems. Mind,
pedagogy. Paper presented at the European Education Culture, and Activity, 7, 273–290.
Research Conference, Gottenburg, Sweden. Mahn, H., & John-Steiner, V. (2002). Developing the
Goodson, I. (2009). Listening to professional life stories: affective zone of proximal development. In G. Wells
Some cross-professional perspectives. In M. Bayer, U. & C. Claxton (Eds.), Learning for life in the 21st cen-
Brinkkjær, H. Plauborg, & S. Rolls (Eds.), Teachers’ tury: Socio-cultural perspectives on the future of edu-
career trajectories and work lives (Vol. 3, pp. 203–210). cation (pp. 46–58). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. McCombs, B., & Lauer, P. (1997). Development and vali-
Goodson, I., & Beista, G. (2010). Narrative learning. dation of the learner-centred battery: Self assessment
London/New York: Routledge. tools for teacher reflection and professional develop-
Goodson, I., & Deakin Crick, R. (2009). Curriculum as ment. Professional Educator, 20(1), 1–20.
narration: Tales from the children of the colonised. Nasir, N., & Saxe, G. (2003). Ethnic and academic identi-
Curriculum Journal, 20(3), 225–236. ties: A cultural practice perspective on emerging
694 R.D. Crick

tensions and their management in the lives of minority Gillon Colloquium, University of Southern California,
students. Educational Researcher, 32(5), 14–18. Los Angeles, CA.
Newmann, F. (1996). Authentic achievement: Restructuring Self, P. (2000). Rolling back the market. New York: St
schools for intellectual quality (1st ed.). San Francisco: Martin’s Press.
Jossey-Bass. Sfard, A., & Prusak, A. (2005). Telling identities: In
Newmann, F., Marks, H., & Gamoran, A. (1996). search of an analytic tool for investigating learning as
Authentic pedagogy and student performance. a culturally shaped activity. Educational Researcher,
American Journal of Education, 104(4), 280–312. 34(4), 14–22.
Newmann, F., & Wehlage, G. (1995). Successful school Stretton, H., & Orchard, L. (1994). Public goods, public
restructuring. Madison, WI: Center on Organisation enterprise, public choice: Theoretical foundations of
and Restructuring of Schools. the contemporary attack on government. Basingstoke,
Paul Hamlyn Foundation, & Innovation Unit. (2010). UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Learning futures engaging schools: Principles and Udehn, L. (1996). The limits of public choice: A socio-
practice. London: Paul Hamlyn Foundation. logical critique of the economic theory of politics.
Perkins, D. (2010). Making learning whole: How seven London: Routledge.
principles of teaching can transform learning. San Vygotsky, L. (1962/1934). Thought and language (E.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Polanyi, M. (1967). The tacit dimension. New York: Press.
Anchor/Doubleday. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of
Reason, P. (2005). Living as part of the whole: The impli- higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA:
cations of participation. Journal of Curriculum and Harvard University Press.
Pedagogy, 2(2), 35–41. Wehlage, G., & Rutter, R. (1986). Dropping out: How
Seely Brown, J., & Thomas, D. (2009). Learning for a much do schools contribute to the problem? Teachers
world of constant change. Paper presented at the 7th College Record, 87, 374–392.
Part IV Commentary: Outcomes
of Engagement and Engagement 33
as an Outcome: Some Consensus,
Divergences, and Unanswered
Questions

Michel Janosz

Abstract
The well-respected engagement scholar, Michel Janosz, shared his
thoughts on the chapters in Part IV of this volume. His commentary articu-
lated the areas of agreement and disagreement across scholars regarding
the conceptualization of engagement and views on engagement as a pro-
cess or outcome. He argued for the consideration of (1) the contexts of
engagement and understanding the relations between engagement in the
classroom and engagement in school; (2) systematic study of the roles of
emotional, cognitive, and behavioral engagement; (3) investigation of the
relation of engagement with other aspects of psychosocial and neurobio-
logical development; and (4) exploration of engagement within a categori-
cal and person-centered approach in addition to the predominant
dimensional and variable-oriented perspective.

and acquire new knowledge, individuals must


Introduction consciously mobilize and devote some of their
physical and psychological (cognitive, emo-
One extraordinary evolutionary skill of mankind
tional) energy; they must engage themselves in
is its capacity to learn. If, at one time, learning
the learning situation.
was a matter of basic survival, we can still be
The amount and the quality of the effort put
struck by the fact that knowledge and skills
into school learning activities vary between stu-
acquisition continues to be, nowadays, one of the
dents. Some students are less engaged than oth-
most powerful determinants of health and well-
ers. How important is that? Is there a price to pay
being (Heckman, 2006; Muennig, 2007). The
for disengaging from school? This is one of the
benefits of learning are indisputable, but they
two fundamental questions eminent scholars have
come with a price: effort. To develop new skills
been invited to address in this chapter of the book.
The second is about influencing student engage-
ment. What organizational and educational
M. Janosz (*) actions affect student engagement? In deciding to
School Environment Research Group
and School of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal,
tackle this question, the authors shared their
Montreal, Canada understanding of what are the determinants of
e-mail: Michel.janosz@umontreal.ca engagement and how we can influence them.

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 695


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_33, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
696 M. Janosz

Hence, while the first issue refers to the outcomes has been shown to predict different aspects of
of engagement, the second concentrates on school success. Nevertheless, we think that stu-
engagement as an outcome. dent engagement in school is not merely an
In this chapter, I highlight some major conver- aggregate version of classroom engagement. Not
gences and divergences in authors’ responses to only is the operationalization of engagement
these questions and share some of the thoughts changing according to the context, but I believe
they inspire. As this book illustrates, the recent that engagement in school encompasses a differ-
mobilization of the scientific community over the ent reality than engagement in the classroom or,
construct of engagement has led to the emergence even more circumscribed, in learning activities.
of fundamental theoretical and methodological For that reason, I think we can expect differ-
debates (e.g., definition of engagement, differ- ences in the outcomes and determinants of
ence between engagement and motivation, mea- engagement, which in turn, can have implications
sures of engagement). Although most authors of for intervention.
this chapter have shared their views on these
important topics, I will only briefly comment on
those since others have been specifically invited Outcomes and Contexts of Engagement
to do so. I propose instead to underline some con-
ceptual and methodological issues that emerged Some authors address student engagement in the
from the reading of these enlightening texts. context of the classroom and learning activities
Because the systematic study of student engage- (Gettinger & Walter, 2012; Guthrie, Wigfield, &
ment is still young, it is easier to identify the You, 2012; Wolters & Taylor, 2012). For exam-
unexplored territories, the unanswered questions. ple, Gettinger and Walter referred to engagement
This task is also made much easier and stimulat- as the time a student is involved or participates in
ing when authors do a superb job of reviewing the classroom. With a similar definition, Guthrie
the state of the knowledge in their area. Thus, in et al. (2012) restricted their analysis of engage-
revisiting the chapters in this book, I will argue ment to reading activities. Interestingly, these
that our comprehension of the relations between authors tend to concentrate on the role of behav-
the determinants and outcomes of engagement ioral and cognitive dimensions of engagement
would benefit from (1) taking into account the while recognizing the emotional component of it
contexts of engagement and understanding the (see also Wolters & Taylor). In any case, they all
relations between engagement in the classroom demonstrate that behavioral and cognitive
and engagement in school; (2) studying more engagement in learning activities strongly pre-
systematically the specific roles of emotional, dicts achievement and learning competencies.
cognitive, and behavioral engagement; (3) inves- Other authors tackle engagement at the school
tigating the relation of engagement with other level. Behavioral (attendance, participation in
aspects of psychosocial and neurobiological extracurricular activities, misbehavior, mobility)
development; and (4) exploring engagement and emotional engagement (identification to
within a categorical and person-centered approach school, belongingness, student-teachers and peer
in addition to the predominant dimensional and relations, emotions accompanying learning tasks)
variable-oriented perspective. seem to be the most frequent aspects studied in
regard to school engagement (Brooks, Brooks, &
Goldstein, 2012; Griffiths, Lilles, Furlong, &
Engagement in Learning Activities Sidwha, 2012; Rumberger & Rotermund, 2012).
and Engagement in School A recent overview of longitudinal studies
(Rumberger & Lim, 2008) indicated that student
Some researchers study student engagement in engagement, and especially behavioral engagement,
relation to learning activities in the classroom. is one of the strongest predictors of persistence
Others address engagement within the broader and school dropout. Longitudinal studies show
context of school. Engagement in both contexts that academic achievement and engagement are
33 Part IV Commentary: Outcomes of Engagement and Engagement as an Outcome… 697

among the strongest predictors of school dropout 2004). Some researchers have proposed a fourth
(Alexander, Entwisle & Kabbini, 2001; Battin- dimension labeled academic engagement, refer-
Pearson et al., 2000; Janosz, LeBlanc, Boulerice, & ring to things like time on task, credits earned, and
Tremblay 2000; Rumberger & Rotermund, 2012). homework completion (Appleton, Christenson,
In sum, being engaged during learning activi- Kim, & Reschly, 2006; Reschly & Christenson,
ties makes a significant difference in how much 2006). Although fundamental, we do not think
is learned and how well intellectual skills are that academic engagement should be considered
developed. Being engaged or not in school makes along the same taxonomy since emotions, cogni-
a difference in how long a student will persist in tions, and behaviors all refer to developmental
their schooling career. aspects, while academic engagement refers the
Nevertheless, the specific contribution of the context to which engagement is linked (e.g.,
different dimensions of engagement is much less behavioral engagement in the classroom).
known and demonstrated empirically. It is my Defining and measuring engagement with
contention that in order to advance our compre- rigor and tracing the boundaries of this construct
hension of student engagement, we need to better with related concepts like motivation, grit
understand to what extent emotional, cognitive, (Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews, & Kelly, 2007),
and behavioral engagement have separate and and self-regulation (see the challenging and
cumulative impacts on student outcomes and the critical thoughts of Wolters & Taylor, 2012) are
potential meditational or transactional processes undisputable critical issues (Appleton, Chris-
involved. In fact, theoretical elaboration and tenson, & Furlong, 2008). As the frontiers of stu-
empirical demonstration of the relations between dent engagement are still a matter of scientific
emotional, cognitive, and behavioral engagement discussion, we would plead for the integration of
are still lacking. For example, in a recent longitu- an additional vector in the actual debate: the con-
dinal study on engagement in school, Archambault, text of engagement. We can think of engagement
Janosz, Morizot, and Pagani (2009) demonstrated (a) in the context of a specific academic learning
that cognitive and emotional engagement tended activity (i.e., reading lesson); (b) in the context of
to evolve in synchronicity while behavioral the classroom, a setting strongly oriented toward
engagement seemed to evolve differently, espe- academic learning but also providing socioemo-
cially for students with low and unstable overall tional learning opportunities (in a more or less
engagement (which are the students more at risk systematic way according to teachers’ practices);
of dropping out; see Janosz, Archambault, and (c) in the context of school, which can be
Morizot, & Pagani, 2008). Furthermore, some conceptualized as a more global educational
recent studies propose that academic achieve- environment providing many social learning
ment and engagement mediate the influence of opportunities in addition to stimulating intellec-
emotional and cognitive engagement on dropout tual development (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). We
(Archambault, Janosz, Fallu, & Pagani, 2009; could even extend this nested conceptualization
Rotermund, 2010). Thus, while we can affirm to engagement in learning activities in the com-
that student engagement is a major determinant munity (see Wylie & Hodgen, 2012). As we move
of school success, we have still a lot to learn on from specific learning activities to a more global
how the different dimensions are related to it. educational environment, the educational setting
(structure, practices) becomes less specific and
constraining (in space and time) with regard to
The Nature of Engagement According the learning it provides.
to the Context I think that the multidimensionality of
engagement is invariant, cross-sectional to the
There appears to be a shared consensus about the contexts, that there are always some emotions,
fact that engagement is multidimensional and cognitions, and behaviors involved when one is
comprises a behavioral, cognitive, and emotional making efforts to learn. This does not imply,
component (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, however, that the expression of engagement is
698 M. Janosz

invariant. To the contrary, what can be considered school is viewed as a social learning environment
as the expression of emotional, cognitive, and as well as a setting for intellectual growth, using
behavioral engagement may be quite context spe- participation in extracurricular activities as an
cific and, by extension, lead to the identification of indicator of engagement seems more than
different determinants and outcomes. Consider the appropriate.
example of belongingness. For many, belonging- In addition to distinguishing contexts of
ness (or bonding for social development research- engagement, we also call for a better understand-
ers and criminologists) is an indicator of emotional/ ing of the relations between engagement in dif-
psychological engagement (Appleton et al., 2006; ferent settings. In Finn’s model of engagement
Finn, 1989; Fredricks et al., 2004; Griffiths et al., (1989), participation in extracurricular activities
2012). For others, belongingness is more a basic represents a deeper manifestation of engagement.
psychological need that must be fulfilled in order to This interesting stage-developmental approach
be motivated (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Ryan & has yet to be empirically tested however.
Deci, 2009). Thus, the sense of belongingness is Furthermore, there may be no necessary equiva-
conceptualized by some as a determinant and by lence between engagement in school and engage-
others as a manifestation (or even an outcome) of ment in specific learning activities (Davis &
engagement. McPartland, 2012). For example, we can easily
Especially for those studying engagement at a think of a student highly engaged in extracurricu-
more molecular level (e.g., learning activity) lar activities without being deeply engaged in the
(Gettinger & Walter, 2012; Guthrie et al., 2012), classroom or in reading activities. Nonetheless, it
motivation is perceived as a determinant of would also be improbable for a student to be
engagement as it expresses, at least in part, the highly engaged in the classroom and not be
extent to which the learning situation (context, engaged in school. Is student engagement in the
content) responds to the need for belongingness. classroom (partially) determined by their engage-
Thus, sense of belongingness appears to be a ment in learning activities? Is engagement in
determinant of engagement rather than an out- school dependent on the level of engagement in
come. Emotional engagement in learning situa- the classroom and in learning activities?
tions may reflect the affects involved during the Conversely, can increased engagement at a
activity (interest, boredom, happiness, sadness, broader level (school) have a positive impact on
anxiety). However, in the context of school, the classroom engagement? Wylie and Hodgen
sense of being related to others may be more than (2012) expand even more the complexity of this
just a determinant. School is more than just an issue by drawing our attention on the stability
academic learning setting; it is a social learning (depth and manner) of engagement in and out of
environment, a life environment, and a socializa- school. Their results raise interesting questions
tion agent (Wentzel & Looney, 2007). School is about the transactional nature of engagement and
also about social learning experiences occurring the relative contribution of the individual and its
outside the classroom and between academic educational environments (school, but also the
activities. In that sense, how one feels about oth- family, peer group, community). This stability
ers can be more easily conceived as part of the across contexts introduces also the possibility
emotions elicited by being and going to school. that student engagement is, in part at least, the
This representation is echoed in theories of school manifestation of more stable personality traits.
dropout, where social integration or bonding is
perceived as important determinants of school
dropout rather than academic engagement (Tinto, Determinants, Intervention,
1994; Wehlage, Rutter, Smith, Lesko, & Fernandez, and Contexts of Engagement
1989). Participation in extracurricular activities
is another example. Such conduct, by defini- Although researchers address student engage-
tion, can hardly be used as an indicator of behav- ment in several contexts, most of them share the
ioral engagement in the classroom. However, if idea that motivation, understood as a set of
33 Part IV Commentary: Outcomes of Engagement and Engagement as an Outcome… 699

affects, attitudes, and intentions toward learning their schooling experience. This perspective also
(values, aspirations, perceived competence/con- recognizes the active role the student has and
trol, goals, etc.) (Wentzel & Wigfield, 2009), is must have to become and be engaged, a point of
the proximal determinant of engagement. There view insufficiently shared in the engagement
appears to be a consensus that motivation pre- research field according to Wolter and Taylors
cedes engagement in the sense that the intensity (2012). Nonetheless, in my opinion, the most
and the quality of student self-mobilization powerful and challenging implication of this lat-
(action) depends directly upon their values, goals, ter perspective is to be more critical about the
perceived competency/control, and expectancies dominant vision of teachers as transmitters of
regarding the learning activity or environment knowledge and students as receptacles of the
(school). This is not to say however that motiva- teachers’ words. The benefits of asking teachers
tion ceases to exist when the action begins or that to become supportive guides of responsible and
engagement does not impact on later motivation. active learners should certainly be tested empiri-
This is presumably why some authors like Guthrie cally and more systematically.
et al. (2012) prefer to talk of the relation between In sum, whatever specific theoretical back-
motivation and engagement as engagement ground researchers adhere to, most of them rec-
processes. ognize that to increase motivation and
This shared perspective introduces another engagement, we must privilege age-appropriate
important point of convergence with regard to interventions, educational environments, and
intervention: (1) to increase student school suc- learning situations that respond to fundamental
cess (e.g., competencies, achievement, gradua- individual needs: to feel secure and respected, be
tion), we must increase engagement; (2) to active and autonomous, experience success, feel
increase engagement, we must increase motiva- competent and have control over the outcome
tion; and (3) to increase motivation, we must pro- (success) of a learning task or situation, be related
vide the organizational conditions and educational to others, understand the meaning and value of
practices known to sustain or increase student’s the effort demanded, etc. (Deci & Ryan, 2002;
motivation. The paper from Guthrie et al. (2012) Eccles & Roeser, 2010).
is particularly exemplary of that perspective and To complete this commentary, I would like to
offers a substantial demonstration of the validity highlight two aspects I think have received insuf-
of this pathway of change at the classroom. To ficient attention and that may provide new insights
these principles, Gettinger and Walter (2012) on the determinants/outcomes of engagement and
bring to our attention that the quantity of engaged for intervention: understanding the relation of
time is determinant for success; that engagement engagement with other aspects of the biopsycho-
is not only determined by individual motivation social development and tackling engagement
and learning skills but also by organizational and within a more person-oriented approach.
educational practices (e.g., classroom manage-
ment and instructional strategies) that maximize
the quantity of quality time for learning (time Student Engagement
effectively used for learning). For her part, Deakin and Biopsychosocial Development
Crick (2012) focuses on the other side of the
coin: the quality of engagement. She argues con- As there is a necessity to clarify the definition and
vincingly that deep engagement generates better conceptual boundaries of student engagement, I
and sustained student outcomes, and that it is also think we should move toward understanding
closely linked to the foundation of identity. how it is linked to other aspects of children and
Brooks et al. (2012) further develop the social- adolescent development. Manifestations of lack
constructivist view, partially introduced by of engagement could express difficulties in other
Deakin Crick, by showing how motivation and spheres of development and not be the direct or
engagement are profoundly influenced by the sole consequence of lack of motivation.
feedback the student receives and interprets from Rumberger and Rotermund (2012) remind us that
700 M. Janosz

40 years ago, Bachman and his colleagues asked of the other. This polarized or dimensional view is
themselves whether school dropout was a symp- certainly widely shared in the present literature on
tom of social maladjustment or a problem of its student engagement. Recently however, Skinner
own (Bachman, Green, & Wirtanen, 1971). They et al. (2009) challenged this vision and demon-
showed that many young people who dropped out strated that disengagement, or what they prefer to
of school had problems in several other spheres call disaffection, is best measured and conceived
of their development, as part of a general devi- as a related but separate construct. This perspec-
ance syndrome (Jessor & Jessor, 1977). Student tive suggests, for example, that (very) low engage-
lack of engagement may also be the manifestation ment is not exactly the same thing as being
of other causes than lack of motivation. For disengaged (see also Griffiths et al., 2012). This
example, taken individually, behaviors used to proposition reminds how positive and warm
measure school behavioral (dis)engagement student-teachers relations are not the opposite of
(skipping school, not responding to teachers, not negative and conflicting relations (Jerome, Hamre,
doing homework, etc.) could likewise be used as & Pianta, 2009). One implication of this finding is
indicator of externalizing problem behaviors or that we should verify to what extent (emotional,
even delinquency. Griffiths et al. (2012) do a very cognitive, behavioral) engagement is related to
good job of reminding us of the correlates of the same determinants and outcomes as disen-
engagement with other types of difficulties in gagement/disaffection.
adolescence (e.g., substance use, mental health). Unraveling the existence and strength between
We should also think of students with attention engagement and psychosocial adjustment is
deficit disorder or with executive functions important for intervention and targeted preven-
vulnerabilities (Blair, 2002). These students tion efforts. If motivational and engagement dif-
most probably manifest symptoms (lack of atten- ficulties are not so much affected by weaknesses
tion, poor self-regulation) that could be easily in the educational environment than by other
interpreted as cognitive disengagement. What determinants (family, peers, community, neuro-
about students with depression or drug abuse biological development), then interventions
problems? should focus on these other targets as well. For
Thus, many scenarios of relationships are example, we could easily assume that the inter-
plausible and not mutually exclusive: disengage- vention plan should not be the same for a student
ment could be a cause or a risk factor for psycho- with a lack of cognitive engagement that has, or
social maladjustment; disengagement and not, an attention deficit disorder or for a student
concurrent psychosocial difficulties could be the with a lack of behavioral engagement that has, or
consequence of the same underlying cause (e.g., not, a drug abuse problem.
ADHD); school motivation may (partially) medi- We are certainly in need of more comprehen-
ate the influence of socioemotional or neurobio- sive longitudinal studies, beginning in early
logical problems on engagement; and moreover, childhood, to help us disentangle the relations
we could explore the potential moderating influ- between engagement, motivation, and other biop-
ence (protective role) of engagement over the sychosocial aspects of the child and adolescent
relationship between biopsychosocial early diffi- development. This can be done by examining the
culties and later adjustment problems like drop- potential direct and indirect (mediating and mod-
out, criminality, etc. erating) relations between variables, including all
Underlining the importance of these issues the appropriate controls. Well-designed interven-
without referring to the recent work of Skinner, tion studies can also be very instructive of the
Kinderman, and Furrer (2009) would be an incom- relative importance of different factors and medi-
plete comment. Throughout this chapter, we have tational processes involved (Lacourse et al.,
used the expressions engagement and disengage- 2002). Another approach would be to adopt a
ment interchangeably, as if one was the contrary person-centered perspective.
33 Part IV Commentary: Outcomes of Engagement and Engagement as an Outcome… 701

approaches (Archambault, Janosz, et al., 2009;


A Person-Centered Approach Janosz et al., 2008). Indeed, recent statistical
in the Study of Engagement developments, stimulated by the power increase
of personal computers, now permit researchers to
The vast majority of studies of student engage-
examine how different groups of students, char-
ment examine how isolated aspects of human
acterized by different levels or types of engage-
experience are related (e.g., the relation between
ment and other concomitant individual or
self-efficacy and engagement or student-teachers
contextual characteristics, evolve over time
relations and engagement). These studies pro-
(Muthén, 2004; Muthén & Muthén, 2008). This
vide us with extremely valuable information,
method increases our capacity to study quantita-
especially when having a longitudinal design.
tive and qualitative differences in the develop-
Nonetheless, a complementary approach to the
ment of student engagement, integrating a
study of human development is trying not to iso-
person-oriented perspective.
late specific aspects but rather to better capture its
multidimensionality and diversity (see Bergman
& Trost, 2006; Cairns, Bergman, & Kagan, 1998).
Conclusion
For example, in a longitudinal and replication
study using cluster analyses, Janosz et al. (2000)
With the growing recognition that engagement is
showed that the dropout population was quite het-
a multidimensional construct comes the scientific
erogeneous. In both longitudinal samples, approx-
duty of verifying more systematically if the
imately 40% of high school dropouts previously
dimensions of engagement share the same deter-
reported high levels of school motivation and
minants or lead to the same outcomes. This task
showed similar, and sometimes even better,
is only beginning. Reviewing the authoritative
behavioral and psychological profiles than the
chapters of this book, I have first come to suggest
average graduate (labeled quiet dropouts). Another
that, since the nature of engagement is not inde-
40% (maladjusted dropouts) had experienced
pendent of the context to which it refers, we
severe levels of school and psychosocial difficul-
should try to answer the causes and consequences
ties. Two other interesting profiles emerged: the
questions by distinguishing the distinct however
disengaged dropout (10%), strongly unmotivated
nested contexts of engagement: the learning
while in school yet showing no socioemotional
activity, the classroom, and the school environ-
difficulties and getting average grades; and the
ments. Second, because of the importance of
low-achiever dropout (10%), disengaged in addi-
schooling in social development and the multiple
tion to experiencing academic failure yet not
nonschool factors that may interfere or facilitate
showing any externalizing problem behaviors.
student engagement, I proposed that we expand
This study illustrates that by looking at profiles of
our understanding of determinants and outcomes
individuals, we may more easily identify how
of engagement to biopsychosocial aspects of
engagement and other aspects of development
development. Third, as the study of engagement
tend to associate and the prevalence of such inter-
is largely dominated by a dimensional and vari-
actions. A typological and person-oriented
able-centered perspective, which tends to mask
approach may also be very useful to plan differen-
the heterogeneity of trajectories toward engage-
tial interventions and programs, more closely
ment/disengagement, I suggested we approach
suited to student needs, to the extent that actions
more often the study of engagement within a per-
are taken to prevent the potential iatrogenic effects
son-centered perspective.
of labeling or, paraphrasing Brooks et al. (2012),
The quality and quantity of effort a student put
mindsetting negatively the educators.
in school greatly influence the benefits of school-
Some recent studies of student engagement,
ing. Learning will be better, and the probabilities
like the one of Wylie and Hodgen (2012), are
of pursuing higher education or integrating the
now combining person-oriented and longitudinal
workforce with success will be higher. As a
702 M. Janosz

whole, the quality of life will be superior. Cairns, R. B., Bergman, L. R., & Kagan, J. (Eds.). (1998).
Increasing our understanding of the modifiable Methods and models for studying the individual.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
precursors of engagement is thus a key issue Davis, M. H., & McPartland, J. M. (2012). High school
toward increasing the education level of the pop- reform and student engagement. In S. L. Christenson,
ulation, especially for the most vulnerable chil- A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of
dren of the society. research on student engagement (pp. 515–539). New
York: Springer.
Deakin Crick, R. (2012). Deep engagement as a complex
system: Identity, learning power and authentic enquiry.
In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
References Handbook of research on student engagement
(pp. 675–694). New York: Springer.
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Kabbini, N. S. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (Eds.). (2002). Handbook of
(2001). The dropout process in life course perspective: self determination theory research. Rochester, NY:
Early risk factors at home and school. Teachers University of Rochester Press.
College Record, 103, 760–882. Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M., & Kelly,
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. D. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- term goals. Journal of Personality and Social
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology, 92, 1087–1101.
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2010). An ecological view
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. of schools and development. In J. L. Meece & J. S.
L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological Eccles (Eds.), Handbook of research on schools,
engagement: Validation of the student engagement schooling, and human development (pp. 6–22). New
instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445. York: Routledge.
Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Fallu, J.-S., & Pagani, L. S. Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2011). Schools as develop-
(2009). Student engagement and its relationship with mental contexts during adolescence. Journal of
early high school dropout. Journal of Adolescence, 32, Research on Adolescence, 21, 225–241.
651–670. Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. Educational Research, 59, 117–142.
(2009). Adolescent behavioral, affective, and cogni- Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
tive engagement in school: Relationship to dropout. School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
Journal of School Health, 79, 408–415. the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
Bachman, J. G., Green, S., & Wirtanen, I. D. (1971). Youth 59–109.
in transition, vol. III: Dropping out: Problem or symp- Gettinger, M., & Walter, M. J. (2012). Classroom strate-
tom? Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, gies to enhance academic engaged time. In S. L.
The University of Michigan. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
Battin-Pearson, S., Newcomb, M. D., Abbott, R. D., Hill, Handbook of research on student engagement
K. G., Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (2000). (pp. 653–673). New York: Springer.
Predictors of early high school dropout: A test of five Griffiths, A.-J., Lilles, E., Furlong, M., & Sidwha, J.
theories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, (2012). The relations of adolescent student engage-
568–582. ment with troubling and high-risk behaviors. In S. L.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fun- Handbook of research on student engagement
damental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, (pp. 563–584). New York: Springer.
117, 497–529. Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., & You, W. (2012). Instructional
Bergman, L. R., & Trost, K. (2006). The person-oriented contexts for engagement and achievement in reading.
versus variable-oriented approach: Are they comple- In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
mentary, opposites, or exploring different worlds? Handbook of research on student engagement
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 601–632. (pp. 601–634). New York: Springer.
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition Heckman, J. J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics
and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312,
children’s functioning at school entry. American 1900–1902.
Psychologist, 57, 111–127. Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L.
Brooks, R., Brooks, S., & Goldstein, S. (2012). The power (2008). School engagement trajectories and their dif-
of mindsets: Nurturing engagement, motivation, and ferential predictive relations to dropout. Journal of
resilience in students. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, Social Issues, 64, 21–40.
& C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student Janosz, M., LeBlanc, M., Boulerice, B., & Tremblay, R. E.
engagement (pp. 541–562). New York: Springer. (2000). Predicting types of school dropouts: A typological
33 Part IV Commentary: Outcomes of Engagement and Engagement as an Outcome… 703

approach with two longitudinal samples. Journal of Rumberger, R. W., & Rotermund, S. (2012). The relation
Educational Psychology, 92, 171–190. between engagement and high school dropout. In S. L.
Jerome, E. M., Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
Teacher—child relationships from kindergarten to Handbook of research on student engagement
sixth grade: Early childhood predictors of teacher- (pp. 491–513). New York: Springer.
perceived conflict and closeness. Social Development, Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). Promoting self-deter-
18, 915–945. mined school engagement: Motivation, learning, and
Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1977). Problem behavior and well-being. In K. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.),
psychosocial development: A longitudinal study of Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 171–195). New
youth. New York: Academic. York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Lacourse, E., Côté, S., Nagin, D. S., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, Skinner, E., Kinderman, T., & Furrer, C. (2009). A moti-
M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2002). A longitudinal-experi- vational perspective on engagement and disaffection:
mental approach to testing theories of antisocial Conceptualization and assessment of children’s behav-
behavior development. Development and ioral and emotional participation in academic activi-
Psychopathology, 14, 909–924. ties in the classroom. Educational and Psychological
Muennig, P. (2007). How education produces health: Measurement, 69, 493–525.
A hypothetical framework. Teachers College Record, Tinto, V. (1994). Leaving college: Rethinking the causes
109, 1–17. and cures for student attrition (2nd ed.). Chicago:
Muthén, B. (2004). Latent variable analysis: Growth mix- University of Chicago Press.
ture modeling and related techniques for longitudinal Wehlage, G. G., Rutter, R. A., Smith, G. A., Lesko, N.,
data. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), Handbook of quantitative & Fernandez, R. R. (1989). Reducing the risk:
methodology for the social sciences (pp. 345–368). Schools as communities of support. New York:
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Falmer Press.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2008). Mplus user’s Wentzel, K. R., & Looney, L. (2007). Socialization in
guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. school settings. In J. E Grusec, & P. D. Hastings (Eds.),
Reschly, A., & Christenson, S. L. (2006). Research lead- Handbook of socialization: Theory and research
ing to a predictive model of dropout and completion (pp. 382–403). New York: Guilford Press.
among students with mild disabilities and the role of Wentzel, K. R., & Wigfield, A. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook
student engagement. Remedial and Special Education, of motivation at school. New York: Routledge.
27, 276–292. Wolters, C. A., & Taylor, D. J. (2012). A self-regulated
Rotermund, S. L. (2010). The role of psychological ante- learning perspective on student engagement. In S. L.
cedents and student engagement in a process model of Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.),
high school dropout. Doctoral dissertation. Gevirtz Handbook of research on student engagement
Graduate School of Education, University of (pp. 635–651). New York: Springer.
California, Santa Barbara, CA. Wylie, C., & Hodgen, E. (2012). Trajectories and patterns
Rumberger, R. W., & Lim, S. A. (2008). Why students of student engagement: Evidences form a longitu-
drop out of school: A review of 25 years of research. dinal study. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly,
Santa Barbara, CA: California Dropout Research & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on
Project. Retrieved December 20, 2010, from, http:// student engagement (pp. 585–599). New York:
cdrp.ucsb.edu/dropouts/pubs_reports.htm#15 Springer.
Part V
Measurement Issues, Instruments,
and Approaches
Measuring Student Engagement:
The Development of a Scale 34
for Formative Use

Charles W. Darr

Abstract
An important part of the short history of student engagement has been the
development of self-report instruments designed to measure engagement.
This chapter describes the development of a self-report tool designed for
Year 7–10 students (11- to 15-year olds) in New Zealand schools. A fea-
ture of the development was the use of Rasch Measurement, which allows
raw survey scores to be transformed to locations on a described equal-
interval scale. Once located on the scale, students’ scores can be compared
with the scores of nationally representative reference groups and inter-
preted using the scale descriptors. The chapter begins by describing the
development of the survey instrument, including how researchers used a
multi-faceted definition of engagement to select and develop items. It then
goes on to describe findings from the national trial of the instrument. The
last part of the chapter looks at possible future directions for the survey.

to measure engagement (Appleton, Christenson,


Introduction Kim, & Reschly, 2006; Finn, 1989; Martin, 2009;
Yazzie-Mintz, 2007). Although varied in their
Interest in the construct of student engagement
approaches, these tools represent attempts to pro-
has been growing for the last 25 years or so
vide an operational definition of engagement that
(Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, 2008). As a
can be used to explore important issues in the
construct, it appeals to a range of educational
field in a consistent and psychometrically sound
stake holders, particularly because of its associa-
manner.
tion with positive outcomes and its relevance to
In recent years, New Zealand schools have
all students. An important part of the short his-
invested an increasing amount of time and
tory of student engagement has included the
resources collecting achievement data for forma-
development of self-report instruments designed
tive use in core learning areas. To assist them to
do this, several new sophisticated assessment
tools have been developed that are capable of
C.W. Darr, B.Sc., M.Ed. (Dist), Dip. Tchng (*)
tracking achievement over time and supported by
New Zealand Council for Educational Research (NZCER),
Te Aro, Wellington, New Zealand online administration and analysis options. When
e-mail: Charles.darr@nzcer.org.nz it comes to measuring student engagement,

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 707


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_34, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
708 C.W. Darr

schools have not been so well supported. For the school with the requirement that the student
most part, when motivated to collect data around enrols elsewhere), expulsions (formal exclusion
engagement, schools have used more ad hoc indi- for over 16-year olds with no requirement for
cators. Most often these are not comparable enrolment at another school) and early-leaving
across schools and are not supported by national exemptions (formal permission to leave school
reference data. before the age of 16).
This chapter describes the development of a Elsewhere, a richer concept of engagement
new survey tool for New Zealand schools has been adopted, for instance, in significant
designed to measure self-perceived levels of research projects (see Wylie & Hodgen this edition)
engagement. Called Me and My School and and in professional development programmes.
developed by researchers at the New Zealand The Te Kötahitanga initiative (Bishop, Berryman,
Council for Educational Research (NZCER), the Cavanagh, & Teddy, 2007) is an example of the
survey reports levels of engagement on a described later. Te Kötahitanga emphasised the develop-
interval scale. Once located on the scale, engage- ment of connectedness between teachers and stu-
ment scores can be compared with descriptors dents to raise the engagement levels of the
that illustrate the types of survey responses typi- indigenous Mäori students to improve their
cal at different levels of the scale and the engage- school achievement.
ment scores of nationally representative groups of Prior to the development of Me and My School
students at each of Years 7–10. and despite the general interest in student engage-
At the heart of Me and My School is a belief ment, there were no standardized tools that New
that student engagement matters, that students’ Zealand schools could use to measure student
voices provide an essential perspective on engagement. This lack of a standardized instru-
engagement, and that schools should have access ment was one motivator for the development of
to tools that they can use to investigate engage- Me and My School. Other motivators included
ment. This chapter describes the development of evidence in the research literature that:
the survey from the pilot stage through to a • High levels of engagement are associated with
national standardisation exercise involving over positive educational and health outcomes
8,000 students. It also includes an exploration of (National Research Council and the Institute
some of the patterns found in the national data. of Measurement, 2004).
• Unlike some educational variables, for
instance, socioeconomic status or previous
Measuring Student Engagement academic success, student engagement can be
in New Zealand influenced by the ways we teach and the ways
we organise our schools (Appleton et al.,
The term ‘student engagement’ is widely used in 2008).
New Zealand, albeit with a range of meanings. • The middle school years (Years 7–10) are piv-
For instance, a series of official reports on stu- otal years for students, marked by school tran-
dent engagement published since 2001 has used sitions, emotional and physical changes and
it in a relatively narrow sense to describe patterns increased rates of suspensions and stand-
of attendance at school (see Lane [2008] for an downs (Dinahm & Rowe, 2007; Ng, 2006).
example). Somewhat ironically, the reports pro- The overall intention in developing the survey
vide statistics on indicators of disengagement then was to create a set of survey items and asso-
such as stand-downs (formal removal from school ciated reporting functions with sound psycho-
for up to 5 days), suspensions (formal removal metric credentials that New Zealand schools
from the school until a formal hearing takes could use to inform their understanding of how
place), exclusions (formal exclusion from a students perceived their own engagement.
34 Measuring Student Engagement… 709

of engagement individually separates students’


Defining Student Engagement behaviour and cognition. In reality these factors
are dynamically interrelated within the individual;
The development of Me and My School began they are not isolated processes (Fredricks et al.,
2004, p. 61).
with discussion regarding how the survey would
define student engagement. Student engagement The terms engagement and motivation are
is an abstract idea—it does not exist as a physical sometimes used interchangeably. However, they
entity that can be poked, prodded or contained in can be distinguished, with motivation understood
a vial. However, most educators would feel that as the ‘why’ or reasoning behind a given behav-
they can sense when it is present and believe that iour, and engagement as the actual patterns of
they themselves have an important role in creat- action and involvement a person displays in tasks
ing the conditions where high levels of engage- and activities (Appleton et al., 2008). These pat-
ment can occur. terns can involve physical, cognitive and emo-
In the research literature, student engagement tional responses and commitments.
is defined in various ways and, at times, used The researchers involved in the development
interchangeably with terms such as school bond- of the Me and My School survey decided to
ing, school attachment, school engagement and develop an item set that spanned the three defini-
school connectedness (Libbey, 2004). In a major tions of engagements described above. This broad
review of the literature, Fredricks, Blumenfeld, approach reflected the national curriculum in
and Paris (2004) recognised three definitions of New Zealand, which outlined a comprehensive
engagement. The first of these is behavioral agenda for education involving a range of com-
engagement and refers to students’ actual partici- petencies, values and attitudes. In a vision state-
pation in school and learning. This includes ment, it described young people ‘… who in their
observable behaviours such as positive conduct, school years, will continue to develop the values
persistence in learning and involvement in school knowledge, and competencies that will enable
life. Behavioral engagement is seen as crucial to them to live full and satisfying lives’ (Ministry of
academic achievement and the prevention of Education, 2007, p. 8). It was thought important
dropping out of school. The second definition is not to narrow the idea of engagement to a set of
emotional engagement and refers to students’ behaviours and attitudes sometimes associated
emotional responses to teachers, peers, learning with academic success, such as following rou-
and school. Emotional engagement is seen as cre- tines and spending time on task. It was also
ating connections with school and influencing decided to take a global perspective on engage-
willingness to do the work. Finally, cognitive ment, focusing on students’ overall perceptions
engagement stresses investment in learning, of their connection with school and involvement
seeking challenge and going beyond the require- in learning, rather than their engagement in a spe-
ments. It also includes employing self-regulation cific classroom context or learning area. Particular
strategies to control and monitor learning. value was placed on items that indicated:
The distinctions between these definitions are • Positive, trusting, active relationships with
blurred. In research studies, similar survey items teachers and peers
are often used to assess the different types of • Feelings of personal safety and belonging
engagement (Jimerson, Campos, & Greif, 2003). • Positive beliefs and commitments involving
The multifaceted nature of engagement led the purpose, relevancy and efficacy of school
Fredricks et al. (2004, p. 60) to describe engage- • Active involvement in learning situations and
ment as a ‘meta’ construct. They noted that: a preparedness to persist
The fusion of behaviour, emotion and cognition In terms of response format, it was decided to
under the idea of engagement is valuable because use Likert-type items with four response catego-
it may provide a richer characterisation of chil-
dren than is possible in research on single compo- ries: strongly disagree, disagree, agree and
nents. Defining and examining the components strongly agree. Some of the item stems were
710 C.W. Darr

sourced from the research literature, for instance: When transforming a raw survey score to a
‘Most mornings I look forward to going to location on the scale, the model takes into account
school’, which points towards the emotional defi- that some survey items are harder to agree with
nition of engagement (Battin Pearson et al., as (involve higher levels of the trait) than others. It
cited in Jimerson et al., 2003, p. 16). Others had also takes into account that the differences, or
been used in previous survey work done by changes in the level of agreement indicated by
NZCER or were written specifically for the the response categories for an item (strongly dis-
instrument, for example: ‘There is just the right agree to strongly agree), are not necessarily con-
amount of challenge at school for me’, which stant within the item or between items. For
links to the cognitive definition. As part of the instance, on one item selecting strongly agree
development process, researchers from both rather than agree might represent only a small
inside and outside NZCER reviewed the item difference in the underlying trait, while for
choices. another this could be an indication of a much
An effort was made to use item stems that greater trait level.
were expressed in terms of beliefs, attitudes and Constructing a survey using Rasch Measure-
behaviours that the students responding held or ment begins with a theory regarding the trait to
exhibited themselves, rather than as observations be measured. The theory anticipates the kinds of
or judgments they made of the school, their teach- questions and responses that will be associated
ers or their peers in general. For instance, ‘I feel with higher and lower points on the scale. The
safe’, rather than ‘This school is a safe place’, theory is then operationalised in the form of
and ‘I pay attention in class’, rather than ‘Students survey items (indicators of the latent trait under
in our school pay attention’. Only one item in the consideration). Sets of items are trialled and the
final survey was of an observational nature responses analysed to see how well they fit the
(‘People care about others at this school’). measurement model.
A basic assumption of the Rasch model is that
the survey is measuring one dominant trait. Good
Rasch Measurement and Me and My model fit indicates that this assumption has been
School met well enough for most practical purposes—
the survey items are discriminating in a uniform
A fundamental aspect of the development of way to measure the same trait. When responses
Me and My School was the application of for a survey item show poor fit to the model, evi-
Rasch Measurement (Bond & Fox, 2007). dence exists that the item could be measuring
Rasch Measurement is a probabilistic approach something different from the other items, or that
to measurement in the social sciences built it is causing confusion for respondents. When
around a mathematical model that transforms this happens, the item becomes a candidate for
raw survey scores into locations on an equal- exclusion from the final instrument.
interval scale. High scores on the survey indi- Fit to the model is investigated using a number
cate higher levels of the trait being investigated of different statistical and graphical indicators.
and are transformed to higher locations on the Figure 34.1 shows an example of a graph used to
scale. Low survey scores on the other hand check the fit of the Me and My School item ‘Most
indicate lower levels of the trait and are trans- mornings I look forward to going to school’. The
formed to lower locations on the scale. Unlike horizontal axis is used to show the measurement
scales that are based on raw scores or percen- scale, while the vertical axis shows the range of
tiles, each unit on an equal-interval scale indi- raw scores possible for this item (0–3). The black
cates the same amount of the construct or trait curve is used to plot the expected response score
being measured. The unit of measurement used (agreement level) calculated using the Rasch
by the Rasch model is called the logit. model for respondents at different locations along
34 Measuring Student Engagement… 711

Fig. 34.1 An example of an expected score curve used to investigate model fit

the scale. As the scale score shown on the hori-


zontal axis increases, the expected score on the
item goes up. The black dots show the average
item scores for groups of actual respondents
located at different points along the scale. As can
be seen, the average scores are very close to the
expected scores; in other words, responses to this
item show good fit to the model.
The outcome of the item calibration process is
a set of threshold locations for each item on the
scale. A threshold locates the point on the scale
where the probabilities of choosing two adjacent
response categories for a particular item (for
instance, agree and strongly agree) are equally
likely. Figure 34.2 illustrates this using the
response categories for the statement ‘Most
mornings I look forward to going to school’. Fig. 34.2 Locating a survey item on the scale
Each response category is associated with the
region or part of the scale (shown by the vertical are used to show the thresholds, where choosing
line) where it is the most probable response to the either of the adjacent response categories is
item stem. The dotted lines between these regions equally likely.
712 C.W. Darr

status of the school community). The other high


Piloting school was coeducational, had a diverse ethnic
mix and a much lower socioeconomic rating.
The large pool of items was trialled in two pilot
Data from the second pilot were first analysed
studies. The first pilot used a survey form consist-
school by school and then for all of the schools
ing of 64 items. Responses were collected from
together, again using the Partial Credit Model.
approximately 1,200 Year 7 and 8 students at a
The analyses showed that overall the data fitted
large multi-ethnic North Island school. An analy-
the measurement model well, and the items func-
sis of the data was carried out using a polytomous
tioned consistently across the different schools.
form of the Rasch model called the Partial Credit
Once more, the analysis was used to inform the
Model (Wright & Masters, 1982). The Quest
construction of a subsequent survey form, which
software (Adams & Khoo, 1999) was used to per-
was used in a much larger national trial.
form the analysis.
Statistical fit indicators from Quest and graphi-
cal fit indicators generated using the open source The National Trial
statistical package R suggested that overall the
response data fitted the measurement model well. In August 2007, data were collected from approx-
Although the items had been purposefully chosen imately 8,500 students in Years 7–10 (11- to
to represent the three different definitions of student 15-year olds). Two nationally representative sam-
engagement described by Fredricks et al. (2004), ples of schools were used, one based on schools
the strength of the overall fit suggested a single with Year 7 and 8 students and the other on schools
underlying dimension made up of three aspects. with students in Years 9 and 10. Both samples
We also investigated the dimensionality of were stratified according to decile and school size.
response data using factor analysis. A three-factor Schools were invited to involve 1, 2 or 3 classes at
model generated three highly correlated factors, each year level depending on the size of the
and a single-factor model showed that one pre- school. Each school was asked to select classes of
dominant factor explained most of the variance. students within each year level that they believed
It was decided therefore to produce one measure- were representative of the range of engagement
ment scale. levels in the school. Some schools asked if they
The analysis of data from the first pilot was could involve more than three classes. This was
used to help inform the development of a more allowed, and a later sub-sampling procedure was
concise instrument. We carried out a careful used to remove any potential bias from school
‘weeding’ process, examining each item’s ability types that were over-represented.
to represent one or more of the engagement As had happened in the pilot trials, students
aspects, demonstrate a level of uniqueness when completed the survey anonymously. We felt that
compared to other items, and show satisfactory anonymity would lead to more valid engagement
fit to the measurement model. As a result of this with the survey, particularly as some of the sur-
work, the original instrument was reduced from vey items related directly to teachers. Teachers
64 to 36 items. A subsequent analysis using data were asked to read a short introduction regarding
from the remaining items suggested negligible the survey to the students, which included some
changes in overall reliability. general instructions. The students then read and
The shorter instrument was used in the second completed the survey independently. Teachers
pilot trial involving approximately 1,800 Year were instructed that they could read a question to
7–10 students from four schools chosen for their any students struggling to read the survey but that
contrasting student populations. For instance, this should be done on an individual basis rather
one of the schools was a girls’ high school with a than to the whole group. Once the students had
student population of predominantly European completed the survey, a class member was
descent and a high decile rating (the New Zealand selected to collect the forms and return them in a
government’s index indicating the socioeconomic sealed envelope to the school office. When all
34 Measuring Student Engagement… 713

classes had completed the survey, they were was decided to discard these items from further
returned to NZCER for analysis. analysis. In addition, a number of items exhibited
little or no separation between at least two adja-
cent response categories. In these cases, we
Analysis of National Trial Data merged (collapsed) the adjoining response cate-
gories. For instance, for some items, strongly
Data from the national trial were analysed using disagree and disagree were merged into one
the Partial Credit Model. Graphical and statistical disagreement category. Subsequent analyses
fit indicators were used to show how well the data indicated improved fit to the model.
fitted the measurement model. The overall fit was Table 34.1 shows the final threshold calibra-
very good. Three items, however, persisted in tions for the 33 remaining items. Often referred
showing poor fit, and after some deliberation, it to as deltas, these are provided in the columns

Table 34.1 Item calibrations for the student engagement scale


Q Aspecta Item stem d01 d12 d23 Infit
1 E Most mornings I look forward to going to school −1.25 0.34 2.83 0.97
2 E I am proud to be at this school −2.13 −0.71 1.55 0.97
3 E Most of the time being at school puts me in a good mood −1.74 0.00 2.64 0.97
4 B I think it is important for me to behave well at school −1.54 0.42 0.88
5 E (R) School often feels like a waste of time to me −1.47 −0.51 0.99 1.15
6 B I respect other students’ space and property at this school −1.92 0.34 1.16
7 E I feel safe at school −0.78 1.10 1.18
8 E People care about each other in this school 0.03 2.55 1.22
10 E/B I have a lot of respect for my teachers −2.19 −1.17 1.11 0.81
11 E My family’s culture is treated with respect by the teachers −1.65 −0.05 1.13
12 E I am comfortable talking to the teachers at this school about −0.76 0.34 2.07 1.20
problems
13 E I care a lot about what my teachers think of me −1.03 −0.23 1.33 1.09
14 E Most of my teachers like me −0.67 1.71 0.92
15 E/B It is easy for me to talk about my schoolwork with most of my −1.79 −0.82 1.23 1.05
teachers
16 E I feel my teachers help me learn −1.24 0.74 0.81
17 C I feel like I am making progress at school −1.17 0.89 0.84
18 C There is just the right amount of challenge for me at school −0.63 1.41 1.13
19 C I pay attention in class −2.00 −0.52 2.22 0.87
20 B/C I take school seriously −1.88 −0.73 1.47 0.77
22 B/C (R) I do as little work as possible; I just want to get by −0.49 1.09
23 C I am interested in what I am learning at school −1.81 −0.62 1.85 0.81
24 C I look for ways to improve my school work −2.05 −0.46 1.68 0.93
25 B/C (R) When schoolwork is difficult I stop trying −0.55 1.07
26 C I like learning new things in school −1.32 0.72 0.90
27 B/C I take care that my homework is done properly −1.14 −0.08 1.58 0.97
28 C I find it easy to concentrate on what I am doing in class −1.49 −0.04 2.61 1.00
29 C I take notice of the comments my teachers make about my work −0.57 1.07 0.90
30 C At school I really care that I do my best work −2.06 −0.68 1.30 0.77
31 C My schoolwork helps in things I do outside of school −1.19 −0.18 1.68 1.09
33 B (R) I think most of my classes are a waste of time −0.37 0.55 0.94
34 B (R) I often feel bored in class −0.30 0.78 2.34 1.13
35 E My friends think school is important −1.05 0.03 2.25 1.24
36 C I talk to other people about what I am learning at school. 0.49 1.93 1.07
a
E emotional, B behavioral, C cognitive, (R) reversed
714 C.W. Darr

labelled d01, d12 and d23. For instance, d01 is the ple, with a stem such as ‘Most mornings I look
scale location of the threshold between category forward to going to school’, the different catego-
0 (usually strongly disagree) and category 1 (usu- ries where described as:
ally disagree). • Strongly disagrees that they look forward to
Response categories were merged for the 15 going to school each day
items shown with fewer than three deltas. In most • Disagrees that they look forward to going to
cases, this merging has involved the two lowest school each day
categories (strongly disagree and disagree). For • Agrees that they look forward to going to
two items however, the two lowest and two high- school each day
est categories have been collapsed, resulting in a • Strongly agrees that they look forward to
dichotomous item with disagree and agree cate- going to school each day
gories. The final column in the table is used to Descriptors were not generated for response
display the Infit statistic for each item. The Infit categories that had been merged with adjacent
statistic provides an overall indication of how response categories during the analysis.
well the item fits the measurement model. Values There are several ways to demarcate a scale
that are less than or greater than indicate devia- according to response categories. For the purposes
tion from the model. Although there is no set cut of describing the student engagement scale, a
off, items with Infit statistics greater than 1.2 or descriptor for each response category was associ-
less than 0.8 are often examined further to see ated with the location on the scale where the cate-
whether they should be excluded. gory has the greatest modelled probability of being
Figure 34.3 locates both the item thresholds selected. The descriptor can then be said to signal
and the national distribution of engagement a behaviour or belief most likely to be associated
scores on the scale. In the figure, each ‘X’ is used with respondents scoring at this scale location.
to represent 31 student scores. The thresholds are For middle response categories, for example,
represented by number labels. For instance, ‘19.1’ the ‘agree’ and ‘disagree’ categories, finding the
is used to plot the threshold between categories 0 point of maximum probability is relatively
and 1 (strongly disagree and disagree) for item 19 straightforward. These points for inner categories
in the survey. As can be seen, the items target occur around about the halfway point between
(match) the range of engagement scores well. adjacent category thresholds. Using the halfway
point as the most probable location for a response
category is precise enough for the purposes of
Describing the Student Engagement describing the scale.
Scale Outer response categories, on the other hand,
do not have these points of maximum probabil-
Rasch measurement scales can be described to pro- ity; the probability of choosing the ‘strongly dis-
vide qualitative information regarding different agree’ or ‘strongly agree’ categories reaches a
parts of the scale. To do this, item descriptors must maximum at minus and plus infinity, respectively.
be developed and a decision made regarding the For the purposes of describing the scale, points
best way to associate the descriptors with locations must be selected where it is reasonable and con-
on the scale. Once this has been decided, descrip- sistent to attach the description for these outside
tors that cluster around a given point on the scale categories. Locating the descriptors for these cat-
can be used to describe the typical types of responses egories half a logit below the lowest threshold
for respondents located at that part of the scale. and half a logit above the highest threshold
In order to describe the Me and My School achieves this. In each case, this is clearly the most
scale, descriptors for each response category probable category at the chosen scale location.
within an item were generated by combining the Figure 34.4 illustrates this by showing the cate-
item stem with the different levels of agreement gory probability curves for item 3: ‘Most of the
(strongly disagree to strongly agree). For exam- time being at school puts me in a good mood’. Each
34 Measuring Student Engagement… 715

ENGAGEMENT SURVEY - NATIONAL TRIAL SEPTEMBER 2007

Item Estimates (Thresholds)


all on all (N = 8641 L = 33 Probability Level=0.50)

6.0

5.0

4.0 X

X
XX
XX
3.0 XX Q1. 3
XXX Q3 .3
XXXXXX Q8 .2 Q28 .3
XXXX Q34 .3 Q35 .3
XXXXXXXX Q12 .3 Q19 .3 Q36 .2
2.0 XXXXXXXXXX Q23 .3
XXXXXXXXXXX Q14 .2 Q24 .3 Q27 .3 Q31 .3
XXXXXXXXXXXX Q2 .3 Q13 .3 Q18 .2 Q20 .3
XXXXXXXXXXXX Q15 .3 Q30 .3
XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Q5 .3 Q7 .2 Q10 .3 Q29 .2
1.0 XXXXXXXXXXXXX Q17 .2
XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Q16 .2 Q26 .2 Q33 .2 Q34 .2
XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Q4 .2
XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Q1 .2 Q6 .2 Q12 .2 Q36 .1
XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Q11 .2 Q35 .2
0.0 XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Q3 .2 Q8 .1 Q27 .2 Q28 .2 Q31 .2
XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Q13 .2
XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX Q5 .2 Q19 .2 Q22 Q23 .2 Q24 .2
XXXXXXXXXXX Q2 .2 Q15 .2 Q20 .2 Q25 .1 Q30 .2 Q33 .1 Q34 .1
XXXXXXXX Q14 .1 Q18 .1 Q29 .1
-1.0 XXXXXXXX Q7 .1 Q10 .2 Q12 .1
XXXXXX Q17 .1 Q35 .1
XXX Q1 .1 Q13 .1 Q16 .1 Q26 .1 Q27 .1 Q31 .1
XXX Q4 .1 Q28 .1
X Q3 .1 Q5 .1 Q11 .1
-2.0 X Q6 .1 Q15 .1 Q20 .1 Q23 .1
X Q19 .1 Q24 .1 Q30 .1
X Q2 .1 Q10 .1
X

-3.0

-4.0

-5.0

Each X represents 31 students

Fig. 34.3 Item calibration on the student engagement scale

curve, from left to right plots the modelled proba- are indicated by the vertical lines, along with the
bility of selecting a particular response category locations on the scale chosen to represent the outer
(strongly disagree to strongly agree). The points of categories (one-half logit below and above the low-
maximum probability for the two middle categories est and highest category thresholds, respectively).
716 C.W. Darr

Fig. 34.4 Selecting locations to position scale descriptors using category probability functions

is interesting to note which statements are associ-


The Described Scale ated with high locations on the scale. For these
statements, strong agreement only becomes most
Figure 34.5 shows the described engagement
probable when students’ overall survey scores
scale numbered from 20 to 105 ‘es’ (engagement
are very high. Statements at this high level
scale) units. The numbers themselves are simply
include: ‘I find it easy to concentrate on what I
markers and indicate increasing scale locations.
am doing in class’; ‘People care about each other
The ‘es’ unit is a linear transformation of
in this school’; and ‘Most of the time being at
the original logit unit used in the scale construc-
school puts me in a good mood’. Strong disagree-
tion: Student Engagement Scale Score (es) =
ment with the statement ‘I often feel bored in
(logit + 5) × 10. It follows that the mean category
class’ is also located high on the scale.
threshold is set to 50 es units, and 10 es units is
Conversely, some statements are relatively
the equivalent of 1 logit. Scale descriptors, each
much easier to agree with than others. These
covering a specific section of the scale, are pre-
occur at lower levels on the scale and include
sented to the right of the scale. As can be seen,
items such as: ‘I respect other students’ space and
high locations on the scale are associated with
property at this school’; ‘I think it is important
strong positive responses to the survey items and
for me to behave well at school’; and ‘My fami-
low scale locations with strongly negative
ly’s culture is treated with respect by the teacher’.
responses.
These latter statements could be seen to represent
There are some interesting observations that
an interesting pivot or tipping point. Their loca-
can be made using the scale description. First, it
tion on the scale indicates that they are generally
34 Measuring Student Engagement… 717

Fig. 34.5 The described student engagement scale

the first positive indicators to ‘turn on’ as per- down the scale or some of the last negative
ceived engagement increases and the last to ‘turn responses to disappear as we go up the scale.
off’ as it decreases. In other words, a person who The negative responses at this level include
cannot agree with these statements is generally disagreeing with the statements: ‘Most mornings
unlikely to agree with other positive statements I look forward to going to school’; ‘I am comfort-
about engagement. It is also interesting to note able talking to the teachers about problems’;
the negative responses associated with this part ‘I take care that my homework is done properly’;
of the scale. These could be seen as either some and agreeing with the statement ‘I often feel
of the first negative statements to appear as we go bored in class’.
718 C.W. Darr

from each of the 200 subsamples provided a sam-


National Patterns of Perceived ple of means and standard deviations from which
Engagement to estimate national norms accurately and pre-
cisely. The national data exposed some interest-
Once the scale was finalised, it became possible
ing patterns. These include differences by year
to estimate students’ survey scores on the scale
level, gender, ethnicity, school and class.
and obtain the distribution of student engagement
by year level. As previously noted, to ameliorate
against the effect of some schools being over rep- Perceived Engagement by Year Level
resented, an iterative sub-sampling procedure was and Gender
used to see if any adjustments should be made to
the initial estimate of national norms. The proce- Figure 34.6 shows that overall the level of per-
dure involved repeatedly sub-sampling the stu- ceived engagement recorded by the national
dent data according to national proportions in New Zealand sample decreases from Years 7–10.
relation to decile group and gender. The estimates At the median level, scores drop from 68 units

Fig. 34.6 Perceived engagement by year level


34 Measuring Student Engagement… 719

Table 34.2 Comparison of the scale descriptors for students scoring at the 25th and 75th percentile for Year 10
Scale description for 25th percentile Scale description for 75th percentile
Students agree that it is important to behave well and that they Students have a strong belief that it is
respect other people’s space and property. They agree that teachers important to behave well at school. They
respect their family’s culture. Students disagree that their friends strongly agree that they respect other people’s
think school is important and that their schoolwork helps in things space and property. Students agree that they
they do out of school. They disagree that they do their homework pay attention in class and that their homework
properly and that they find it easy to concentrate on what they are is done properly. They care what the teachers
doing in class. They do not associate school with good moods and think of them and agree that most of their
disagree that they care what teachers think of them. Students teachers like them. Students are interested in
disagree that their teachers help them learn. They strongly agree what they learn at school and agree that they
that they often feel bored in class look for ways to improve their work

on the scale in Year 7–53 units in Year 10. This from New Zealand European, Mäori, Pacific,
pattern of decline in engagement scores mirrors Asian and Other. If they choose to, they can select
findings from studies of engagement in the United more than one ethnic group. Figure 34.7 shows
States (National Research Council and the the median scale score by year level for New
Institute of Measurement, 2004). A very notice- Zealand European, Mäori, Pacific and Asian stu-
able feature of this decline is the steeper drop that dents. At Years 7 and 8, the median scale score for
occurs between Years 8 and 9. This is a transition Mäori students is slightly lower than the medians
point for most students in New Zealand when for other ethnic groups but only slightly. At Year
they will often change to a much larger school, 9, however, there is a much larger drop for Mäori
usually with very different organizational struc- than for any other ethnic group. The result is a
tures and patterns. fairly large difference at the median for Mäori in
Figure 34.6 uses triangles and squares to indi- their overall perception of engagement at Years 9
cate the median scale scores for boys and girls, and 10. As has already been noted, most students
respectively. As can be seen, at the median level in Year 9 are in their first year of high school.
Year 7, female students tend to report higher Interestingly, students who choose Pacific as
levels of perceived engagement than their male their ethnicity are generally more positive than
counterparts. What is interesting, however, is that students who choose other ethnicities. Figure 34.7
while overall engagement for both boys and girls shows them as recording higher median levels at
declines from Years 7 to 10, the downward trajec- Years 7, 8 and 9 on the scale than students of all
tory for females is much greater than that for other ethnicities. However, the drop that occurs
males. By Year 10, the median engagement scores between Years 8 and 9 is as steep as that recorded
for both genders are almost exactly the same. by Mäori students.
As an example of how the scale description
can provide an interpretation of the scale scores,
it is interesting to observe how the most probable Perceived Engagement by School
responses for students scoring at the 25th percen- and by Class
tile for Year 10 differ from those scoring at the
75th percentile. These are juxtaposed in Table 34.2 The national survey results also show that there
and suggest a readiness on the part of the lower are differences between schools. Figure 34.8
scoring Year 10 to nominate some negative shows a range of schools with Year 8 students. As
responses. can be seen, students at some schools have
responded much more positively overall than stu-
dents at other schools.
Perceived Engagement by Ethnicity Within a school, there can be a large amount
of variance at the class level. Figure 34.9 plots
When Me and My School is administered, students the scale scores by class for a large intermediate
are asked to indicate their ethnicity by choosing school. As shown, some students in particular
720 C.W. Darr

Fig. 34.7 Perceived engagement by ethnic group

classes tend to respond much more positively


than students in other classes. This might be
Future Directions and Final Thoughts
explained by differences in the class context, for
The Me and My School survey is available for
instance, differences in teaching style, or because
use by New Zealand schools on a subscription
the class contains a particular group of students
basis. As well as the paper and pencil version, an
or both. It is interesting to note that even when a
online version of the survey has been developed.
class or school has higher overall perceived
NZCER supports the survey with data entry and
engagement, there is still a spread of results indi-
reporting services. Over the last 3 years, more
cating different experiences for different individ-
than 200 schools have subscribed to use the
ual students.
34 Measuring Student Engagement… 721

Fig. 34.8 Perceived engagement for a group of schools

survey, some of them multiple times. Reactions version. Many of the items were reworded to
have been favourable with schools describing it make them more age appropriate. To administer
as a catalyst for deeper involvement and interest the survey, the questions were read to the stu-
in student engagement issues. The survey has also dents, rather than being read independently. The
been used outside of New Zealand. In these cases, work carried out so far has suggested that there
NZCER has worked with interested organisations are developmental differences that need to be
and researchers to provide support with analysis. taken into account when measuring engagement
A number of avenues for future directions at different year levels. It is plausible that the
have begun to be explored. One of these involves nature of engagement changes over time and that
the extension of the survey to other year levels. indicators of engagement can be more or less rel-
Recently, national trials occurred for a version of evant at different ages. A Rasch analysis of the
the survey aimed at students in Years 4–6 (8- to two surveys, linked through common items, sug-
11-year olds). This version of the survey used a gests that there are differences in item function-
subset of the items contained in the Year 7–10 ing between the two levels.
722 C.W. Darr

Fig. 34.9 Perceived engagement for a group of classes within a school

Another avenue for future research involves it brings an important voice to the table—student
exploring how the survey can better recognise voice. As a mechanism for providing student voice,
aspects of engagement that are pertinent to Mäori it is systematic and has been developed with some
students. Recently the Ministry of Education’s rigour. However, it is not necessarily a strong
Mäori Education Strategy, Ka Hikitia—Managing example of student voice—it is, at best, a starting
for Success: The Mäori Education Strategy 2008– point, or as schools describe it, a catalyst for some-
2012 (Ministry of Education, 2009) has placed an thing deeper and more real.
emphasis on developing educational contexts that A voice requires a listener.
engage Mäori students by recognising the impor- Brent Davis (1996) explained listening as a
tance of language, identity and culture, and which coming together, a kind of touching from a dis-
develop a reciprocal learning relationship between tance. He argued that the most powerful type of
teachers and students. It has been suggested that listening involves a conversation where, rather
an engagement survey for Mäori students should than trying to impose or provide a viewpoint to
include items which are sensitive to this type of another—as in a discussion—we work together to
engagement. Future work in this direction could reach a joint understanding. This type of listening
involve the development of additional items for means that:
the Me and My School survey or the development There is no winner, no gaining of the upper hand,
of a stand-alone instrument. no final word, no compulsion to stick with the
The Me and My School survey provides a stan- topic. Rather, the conversation allows us to move
dardized mechanism that students can use anony- freely and interactively towards those questions
that animate us while enabling us to explore not
mously to make judgements on a series of indicators just the topics that emerge, but why such topics
related to their own level of engagement. As such, capture our interest in the first place (p. 40).
34 Measuring Student Engagement… 723

For Davis, ‘A goal of the conversation is to Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
deepen understanding and, in that deepening, to School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1),
create knowledge’ (p. 40). 59–109.
Perhaps the real secret of engagement is to Jimerson, S., Campos, E., & Greif, J. (2003). Towards an
develop our ability to listen to students’ voices and understanding of definitions and measures of school
to involve them in conversations. Conversations engagement and related terms. The California School
Psychologist, 8, 7–27.
will sometimes be about learning areas like math- Lane, R. (2008). A report to schools on New Zealand stu-
ematics, science, English and history and/or about dent engagement 2007. Wellington, New Zealand:
the students themselves, their heritage, culture, Ministry of Education. Retrieved from http://www.
intuitions and experiences, and their emerging educationcounts.govt.nz/publications/series/2303/
28883/28884
place in the world. If Me and My School helps us Libbey, H. (2004). Measuring student relationships to
listen, then it will have served its purpose. school: Attachment, bonding, connectedness, and
engagement. Journal of School Health, 74, 7.
Martin, A. J. (2009). The motivation and engagement
scale. Sydney, Australia: Lifelong Achievement
References Group. Retrieved from http://www.lifelongachieve-
ment.com
Adams, R. J., & Khoo, S. T. (1999). ACER QUEST: The Ministry of Education. (2007). The New Zealand curricu-
interactive test analysis system. Melbourne, Australia: lum for English-medium teaching and learning in
Australian Council for Educational Research. years 1–13. Wellington, New Zealand: Learning
Appleton, J., Christenson, S., & Furlong, M. (2008). Media.
Student engagement with school: Critical conceptual Ministry of Education. (2009). Ka Hikitia—Managing for
and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology success: The Mäori education strategy 2008–2012.
in the Schools, 45(5), 369–386. Wellington, New Zealand: Ministry of Education.
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, Retrieved from http://www.minedu.govt.nz/theMinis-
A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological try/PolicyAndStrategy/KaHikitia/PublicationsAnd
engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement Resources-EnglishLanguageVersions.aspx
Instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44, National Research Council and the Institute of
427–445. Measurement. (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering
Bishop, R., Berryman, M., Cavanagh, T., & Teddy, L. high school students’ motivation to learn. Committee
(2007). Te Kötahitanga Phase 3 Whanaungatanga: on increasing high school students’ engagement and
Establishing a culturally responsive pedagogy of rela- motivation to learn. Board on Children, Youth, and
tions in mainstream secondary school classrooms. Families, Division of Behavioural and Social Science
Wellington, New Zealand: Ministry of Education. and Education. Washington, DC: The National
Bond, T. G., & Fox, C. M. (2007). Applying the Rasch Academy Press.
Model: Fundamental measurement in the human sci- Ng, L. (2006). Attendance, absence and truancy in New
ences (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Zealand schools in 2006. Wellington, New Zealand:
Davis, B. (1996). Teaching mathematics: Toward a sound Ministry of Education.
alternative. New York: Garland Publishing. Wright, B., & Masters, G. (1982). Rating scale analysis.
Dinahm, S., & Rowe, K. (2007). Teaching and learning in Rasch measurement. Chicago: MESA Press.
middle schooling: A review of the literature. A report Yazzie-Mintz, E. (2007). Voices of students on engage-
prepared for the Ministry of Education. Wellington, ment. A report on the 2006 High School Survey of
New Zealand: Australian Council for Educational Student Engagement. Bloomington, IN: Center for
Research. Evaluation & Education Policy, Indiana University.
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Retrieved from http://www.indiana.edu/~ceep/hssse/
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. reports.html
Systems Consultation: Developing
the Assessment-to-Intervention Link 35
with the Student Engagement
Instrument

James J. Appleton

Abstract
Schools and districts encounter many challenges when attempting to sup-
port systems-level use of engagement data. These challenges range from
integrating disparate sources of data to collecting (and ensuring the mean-
ingfulness of ) data on the less observable cognitive and affective subtypes
of engagement, to increasing the frequency with which data can be updated
and disseminated, and to implementing appropriate interventions based
upon assessed engagement. Acknowledging the need for further study on
school systems’ empirically guided efforts to effectively use engagement
data, this chapter details one, large, urban-fringe district’s effort to use these
types of data. The delineation is intended as a tangible example with suffi-
cient detail to support commentary, suggestions for improvements, and calls
for relevant further research. The example should also provide guidance
sufficient for other school systems to consider and select types of informa-
tion and methods of dissemination useful within their efforts to promote
student engagement and outcomes related to it. Suggestions for further
research and visions of future use of engagement data are also provided.

Student engagement has emerged as a desirable importance of engaged learners themselves.


target for intervention efforts due to perceptions At the intersection of theory and practical appli-
of its value as an outcome, critical contributions cation stand many challenges, decisions, and les-
to authentic and continued learning, and mallea- sons learned. This chapter focuses on the
bility (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004). experiences of a single, large district in address-
School and district efforts to focus upon engage- ing the challenges, explicating the decisions, and
ment have the potential to not only impact valued sharing the resulting successes and shortcomings
student outcomes but also to underscore the of an effort to systematically analyze, dissemi-
nate, and utilize student engagement data. The
intent is to offer a forthright example that can
serve as a beginning point for further conversa-
J.J. Appleton, Ph.D. (*)
Office of Research and Evaluation, Gwinnett County
tions and research on how engagement theory
Public Schools, Suwanee, GA, USA can practically influence district-wide data use
e-mail: Jim_Appleton@gwinnett.k12.ga.us and intervention efforts.

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 725


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_35, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
726 J.J. Appleton

honors the traditional theoretical adherence to


Definition of Student Engagement three subtypes while acknowledging the practical
concern that aggregation of data across academic
This chapter adopts a perspective of engagement
and behavioral subtypes may provide similar
that is closely aligned with Newmann and col-
profiles for socially but not academically active
leagues’ (Newmann, Wehlage, & Lamborn, 1992)
students and those involved academically but not
definition as the “…student’s psychological [men-
socially.
tal, cognitive, emotional] investment in and effort
[behaviors] directed toward learning, understand-
ing, or mastering the knowledge, skills, or crafts Student Engagement Versus
that academic work is intended to promote” (p. 12). Motivation
Key in this perspective is the multidimensional
nature implied by psychological as well as behav- Discussions of student engagement often raise
ioral components and the importance of a persis- questions concerning the construct’s relationship
tent focus on educational outcomes. The adherence to motivation. From the perspective of this chapter,
to a multidimensional view of the engagement motivation is perceived to be necessary, but not
construct is consistent with seminal theoretical sufficient for engagement (Appleton et al., 2008).
and empirical work in the field (e.g., Finn, 1989; Engagement operates within a motivational
Fredricks et al., 2004; National Research Council framework (Connell & Wellborn, 1991) and is
and Institute of Medicine, 2004). Specifically, dependent upon psychological processes such as
the definition adopted here considers four sub- autonomy (Skinner, Wellborn, & Connell, 1990),
types of engagement: academic, behavioral, cog- relatedness/belonging (Goodenow & Grady,
nitive, and affective/emotional. Academic 1993), and competence (Baumeister & Leary,
engagement is defined by behaviors that have, as 1995; Schunk, 1991). Yet, engagement represents
their purpose, the high-quality accomplishment action taken upon that motivation or “energy in
of the academic tasks of schooling and can be action, the connection between person and activ-
indexed by student data such as asking questions ity” (Russell, Ainley, & Frydenberg, 2005, p. 1)
on content in class, completing assigned class that is dependent upon the fit between student
work, and accruing credits toward graduation. and context (Reschly & Christenson, 2006a,
Behavioral engagement is defined as additional 2006b). A comparison between the breadth of
school supportive behaviors that demonstrate an motivational constructs (Maehr & Meyer, 1997)
allegiance or adherence to the school and/or its and efforts to sample aspects of these constructs
staff and can be observed in actions such as to predict engagement quickly demonstrates the
prompt and persistent school attendance, involve- minimalist nature of the engagement construct.
ment in extracurricular activities, and the avoid- Engagement represents a pragmatic effort to
ance of deviant behaviors. Cognitive engagement reduce queries of motivation to fewer items
is defined as investment in the work of learning as across constructs in order to predict directed
well as the refinement and deployment of strate- energy/action.
gic thinking (Fredricks et al.). Affective/emo-
tional engagement is defined as affiliation/
identification with school (e.g., Finn), including Context
the staff and students that populate it and the emo-
tions experienced during the tasks of schooling. Gwinnett County Public Schools (GCPS) is a
Cognitive and affective/emotional subtypes of large, urban-fringe district northeast of Atlanta
engagement are frequently assessed via student with nearly 161,000 students, 130 facilities, and a
perceptions and considered less observable than fiscal year 2011 annual budget of 1.76 billion
academic or behavioral subtypes (Appleton, dollars. The student demographic characteristics,
Christenson, & Furlong, 2008). This definition as of spring 2010, included 0.4% American
35 Systems Consultation… 727

Indian, 28.6% African American, 10.3% Asian concerns also surfaced in comparing engagement
American, 25.3% Hispanic, 3.8% multiracial, factors consisting of different numbers of items
and 31.6% White. Additionally, 12% of students and attempting to link engagement to meaningful
were served in special education, 16% were des- district-, school-, and student-level outcomes.
ignated as English language learners (ELLs), and Data privacy issues and database limitations
50% were eligible for free or reduced-price occurred during efforts to quickly and widely dis-
school lunch (GCPS, 2010). seminate reports only to those staff with a need
In many respects, GCPS could be considered for the information. The sections that follow
a near best-case scenario for a district attempt to detail the efforts and processes undertaken to
systematically analyze and utilize engagement implement a systems-level use of student engage-
data. First, the importance of promoting engage- ment data as well as the vision these efforts
ment, as a critical component of student learning, engendered for an improved set of future pro-
is recognized and supported by the CEO/superin- cesses. The current implementation and vision of
tendent. This support is demonstrated within dis- future implementations are offered in a manner
cussions of teaching and learning, advocacy for a as transparent and thorough as possible. The
district-wide Student Advisement Program belief is that transparency and thoroughness will
(SAP), fall and spring semester surveying of stu- support detailed conversations and research agen-
dent perspectives of engagement, and the imple- das for furthering the effectiveness of using
mentation of several smaller, complementary engagement data within districts.
engagement-supportive programs. Schools are
required to provide an advisement program with
every student assigned to an advisor. Research- Implementation
based optimal and minimal parameters are
provided for the frequency and duration of advise- Data Integration
ment group meetings, size of advisement groups,
and duration of advisor-advisee relationships. As documented by many, data indicating the aca-
Secondly, the diversity and size of the student demic and behavioral engagement of students
population of GCPS increased the likelihood of a typically exist within district current electronic
rich dataset for examining baseline characteris- collections (Appleton et al., 2008; Fredricks
tics of engagement as well as trends not only over et al., 2004). The decision within GCPS to begin
time but also across and within students of differ- to gather data on the cognitive and affective sub-
ing demographic backgrounds. Thirdly, the dis- types of engagement was mainly the result of the
trict focus on data-based decision-making and an need to evaluate the SAP. The conceived purpose
engagement-based logic model ensured the need for these data was to represent a mediating or
for several years of engagement data to properly moderating variable linking or attenuating the
evaluate the effectiveness of the SAP. Finally, relationship between implementation of the SAP
many of the challenges of scale were mitigated, and academic outcomes of interest. Beginning in
and often outweighed, by the data-processing the fall of the 2007–2008 school year, the Student
infrastructure (e.g., survey scanning, data inte- Engagement Instrument (SEI) (Appleton,
gration, data-querying capacity) of GCPS’ Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, 2006; Betts,
Information Management Division (IMD). Appleton, Reschly, Christenson, & Huebner,
Despite the existing facilitative components, 2010) was administered, each fall and spring
several challenges also confronted this effort to semester, as the means for collecting cognitive
make wider use of engagement data. These chal- and affective engagement data on nearly all 6th–
lenges included logistical issues with integrating 12th grade GCPS students. To date, SEI data have
existing academic and behavioral data as well as been collected each fall and spring semester.
reporting in a format considered relevant and Moreover, as school and district staff learned of
meaningful to report to consumers. Statistical the plan to gather data on student perceptions of
728 J.J. Appleton

Fig. 35.1 SEI introductory text

engagement, they requested reports and data, and desire to receive student-level engagement data-
the initial purpose for the results expanded into sets in addition to reports.
other potential uses. SEIs were administered during SAP group
Given this expansion, appropriate language meetings using paper surveys with results scanned
for conveying the importance of forthright into a data file by the IMD. IMD and Office of
answers, informing students that their data would Research and Evaluation (ORE) staff spent time
be merged with other student-level data, and clarifying several critical aspects of the data. Since
guaranteeing the confidentiality of their responses students were responsible for entering their unique
was an important initial challenge. The selected GCPS student ID on the SEI, a convention was
introductory text (see Fig. 35.1) highlighted the needed to ensure engagement responses were used
lack of access of any school staff member to indi- only when one could have great confidence that
vidual student responses while informing stu- the student was correctly identified. Also, a deci-
dents of staff access to aggregations of their sion on whether to include the multiple SEI com-
responses at the factor/theme1 level. An addi- pletions of concurrently enrolled students (e.g., a
tional goal of the introductory text on the GCPS survey response at both their home high school
SEI was to convey the intended use of the results and at a district technological charter school) was
for modifying the school climate and improving needed. IMD and ORE staff decided that an exact
the experiences of those less engaged students. match from the provided student ID (PSID) and
The intent was to increase student perceptions of location code from the school at which the SEI
the value of providing forthright responses. Also, was completed (CLC) to the GCPS data systems
a GCPS version of the standardization procedures student ID (DSID) and enrolled location code
used in the piloting of the SEI was provided to (ELC) would indicate a correct identification.
staff administering the engagement instrument Moreover, to avoid losing mobile students from
(see Fig. 35.2). Subsequent presentations to advi- the datasets, the PSID and CLC would be consid-
sors on the role of student engagement, plan to ered a correct identification if they matched the
survey cognitive and affective engagement, and DSID and the former location code of a student
intent to report results provided rich qualitative (FLC). ORE staff decided that the SEI’s focus
data on advisor concerns and opportunities for upon the school at which the student responded
clarification of processes. Advisors provided supported the relevance of including responses
comments to clarify the introductory text on the from both schools of a concurrently enrolled stu-
GCPS SEI. Moreover, they preferred “affective” dent. Finally, the inability to differentiate the true
to “psychological” engagement to avoid confu- response (or even if both responses originated
sion with psychopathology and expressed the from the same student given that a one-digit change
in a PSID can be an exact match with another stu-
1
“Theme” is the term used in descriptions of results to dent’s DSID), multiple responses with the same
consumers of these reports in our district. PSID and CLC were deleted from the datasets.
35 Systems Consultation… 729

Fig. 35.2 SEI administration standardization procedures

After distilling all responses to those matched, 65–75% of all GCPS middle and high school
with great confidence, to GCPS students, the students across the years of administrations.
IMD merged several other data identifiers of The provision of engagement results could be
engagement including days enrolled, days tardy, grouped into two main categories: student-focused
days absent, disciplinary incidents, in-school sus- and aggregated. Student-focused reports and data-
pension days, out-of-school suspension days, sets were those whose primary purpose was juxta-
credits accrued, as well as demographic data and posing data on individual students with the goal of
each student’s advisor’s name. This data file was identifying students demonstrating disengagement
used to generate all reports and datasets provided from school. Aggregated results were summary
to advisors and schools. The proportion of cogni- reports whose purpose was providing schools and
tive and affective engagement data that matched the district with information on the general engage-
district student data records ranged from around ment of students across meaningful subgroups and
730 J.J. Appleton

for assisting in detecting engagement changes that The distinction between level of engagement
may be associated with group intervention efforts. and change in engagement is critical for under-
standing the meaning and potential uses of the
advisor-advisee reports produced in the fall and
Student-Focused Results spring of the academic year. After each adminis-
tration (fall and spring of each academic year),
The information provided and the dissemination advisor-advisee engagement level reports were
formats of results were the outgrowth of iterative prepared to detail the average of each student, at
processes of reporting, eliciting responses, and that administration, within each SEI factor. These
issuing modified reports for further response. reports were intended to enable advisors to com-
Additionally, the information reported was influ- pare the overall cognitive and affective engage-
enced by large dataset analyses attempting to link ment of each advisee to the group as a whole as
student engagement to district-valued outcomes. well as discern student areas of relatively higher
Through this process, several reports and datasets and lower engagement among the SEI factors.
were settled upon for routine distribution. When compared with aggregated reports of
Student-focused information included the advisor- school and district results, these reports could be
advisee report and the engagement pattern data- used to situate students within typical school and
set. The advisor-advisee reports contained a district levels of engagement. In addition, means
stacked horizontal bar graph with each of the six that were relatively low could be considered
SEI factors contributing a portion (i.e., one sixth) along with available academic and behavioral
to the total bar length. Each of the SEI factors engagement data sources to note potential stu-
was represented by an average of the respective dents in need of intervention as well as the rela-
items which were scaled from one to four tive strengths and weaknesses that might be
(responses are reversed to 1 (strongly disagree) consulted in implementing an intervention.
to 4 (strongly agree)). Currently, averages are In the spring of each academic year, advisor-
calculated from the unweighted response values advisee engagement change reports were pro-
of one to four. Another approach would be to duced. These reports again used stacked bar
present the value of an item according to its con- graphs but displayed the differences between the
tribution to the factors estimated in factor analyses spring and fall means for each SEI factor.
of the SEI (see Appleton et al., 2006; Betts et al., Differences were reported for all students com-
2010). The six factors, teacher-student relation- pleting an SEI at both the fall and spring admin-
ships (TSR), control and relevance of schoolwork istrations. Since the fall mean was subtracted
(CRSW), peer support at school (PSS), future from the spring mean, values were positive only
aspirations and goals (FG), family support for when student reports of cognitive and affective
learning (FSL), and intrinsic motivation (IM), engagement increased from fall to spring on a
are composed of two to nine items each. To particular SEI factor. Also, rather than the values
ensure that factor means contained a meaningful from 6 to 24 (the typical range of scores on the
portion of the items composing them, means level reports), the focus in these reports was zero
were only computed if respondents answered on the change scale. Zero represented a student
50% or more of the items (except the IM factor who maintained the same level of engagement
for which the completion of both items was from fall to spring. While bars to the positive
required). In this report, a stacked horizontal bar side (right) of zero suggested improvements
was presented for each advisee in an advisor’s in engagement, bars to the negative side (left)
group along with a single lower stacked horizon- of zero suggested declines in engagement (see
tal bar indicating the averages for the group of Fig. 35.4). The values in these reports were
advisees (see Fig. 35.3). Since each factor is weighted as described below and enabled advi-
scaled from one to four, the stacked bar for a sors to compare engagement changes among
student with responses sufficient to calculate a their individual advisees and in reference to dis-
mean for each factor ranged from 6 to 24. trict change values. Also, the interpretation of
35 Systems Consultation… 731

Fig. 35.3 Spring student-focused SEI-based advisor-advisee level report

change within the context of the level data of fall contained four. For a student completing all of the
or spring was important. The use of these two items for each of these factors, the TSR factor
types of reports together (see Figs. 35.3 and would result in a mean with eight potential non-
35.4) enabled an advisor to not only evaluate the zero decimal suffixes while the FSL factor pro-
meaningfulness of the change of a student (rela- duced a resulting mean with the potential for only
tive to the district) but also to determine whether three. That is, the mean for the TSR factor was cal-
the amount of change resulted in a satisfactory culated with division by nine enabling a decimal
concluding position or, alternatively, represented suffix the same as that of any integer multiple of
the influence of a floor or ceiling effect. That is, 1/9 (or approximately .11) while the mean of the
how much change occurred given the “change FSL factor resulted from division by four allowing
space” (i.e., room for change) and did that change for a suffix the same as integer multiples of 1/4 (or
result in a final level that (relative to district data) .25). Further, fall-to-spring differences on these
seems to position the student well among his or factors would be constrained to differences con-
her peers? taining these same suffixes. The critical issue arose
In striving to ensure student-focused change upon examination, across SEI factors, of the size
reports were meaningful, an important issue sur- of student changes from fall to spring. Inevitably,
faced. This issue involved the divisibility of fac- the change of a single response to a single item by
tors and arose as a result of the varying number of a one-unit move upward or downward on the
items across SEI factors. Consider that the TSR (1 = strongly disagree (SD) to 4 = strongly agree
factor contained nine items while the FSL factor (SA)) scale would produce a change of 1/9 on the
732 J.J. Appleton

Fig. 35.4 Spring student-focused SEI-based advisor-advisee change report

TSR factor but1/4 on the FSL factor. Essentially, a This standardization also limits the total amount
change of approximately .11 could be miscon- of change on a factor to be a function of the num-
strued as smaller than a change of .25 when, in ber of items. For instance, the maximal change
fact, both represented a change of the smallest unit on a nine-item factor is between 9 (all SDs) and
possible on that factor. 36 (all SAs) or 27 units of change, while for a
To offset this issue and produce reports that four-item factor, it is between 4 and 16 or 12 units
better-supported comparisons of change across of change. Nearly all fall-to-spring changes on
SEI factors, the difference in means was multi- the SEI were not extreme enough to use all (or
plied by the number of items composing that fac- even most) units of change, but the difference in
tor. For instance, the one-item, one-unit change change units available due to factor size does
on the TSR of 1/9 was multiplied by 9, and that deserve mention.
same change of 1/4 on the FSL was multiplied Beyond the reports, student-focused datasets
by 4. This standardization would produce a value have also been generated for use by schools.
of 1 for each of these changes. The interpretation of These datasets evolved as student-reported
the value, for these weighted change factors, then engagement was statistically related to academic
became the number of smallest changes possible and behavioral outcomes of interest. At both the
that were made from fall to spring. Implicitly, middle and high school levels, SEI-reported
this standardization elevated the importance of cognitive and affective engagement level was
focusing upon the smallest unit of change and related to significant and substantive differences
considered these changes equal across factors. in state test performance as well as the frequency
35 Systems Consultation… 733

Fig. 35.5 Fall SEI portion of student-focused dataset

and severity of disciplinary infractions and atten- The intent of the formatting was to enable staff
dance (see Appleton, Reschly, & Martin, 2011). to differentiate the intensity of intervention
These differences in outcomes were pronounced needed across groups of students. Staff were
between reported cognitive and affective engage- advised to first examine the percentile category
ment values in the percentiles of top 10%, middle value since it related most closely to district-level
80%, and lowest 10% for the district. Moreover, analyses relating engagement to desired out-
these relationships persisted, at substantive comes. Suggested subsequent examinations were
levels, across 3 years of data (2007–2008, 2008– to consider the standardized SEI factor values as
2009, and 2009–2010). Therefore, separately for well as other academic and behavioral engage-
middle and high school levels, SEI all items (SEI ment data included in the dataset. Guidance was
total) response averages at or above the 90th per- also provided that staff should consult daily
centile, between the 90th and 10th percentiles, updated web-accessible sources of attendance
and at the 10th percentile or lower were differen- and disciplinary data and, potentially, stand-alone
tiated in the school-provided datasets. In the databases containing current course performance
dataset, each student provided a unique case with data. The simple goal of the guidance was to
a single value for the SEI total mean and percen- encourage consideration of the total and factor-
tile category. In addition to these values based level cognitive and affective engagement data
upon the SEI total, student means on each of the within the context of current information offered
SEI factors were expressed in standard deviation by more dynamic academic and behavioral
units from the GCPS mean (i.e., standardized). engagement data. As an example, one school may
To assist users, Excel cells were modified (con- have several students within the “lowest 10%”
ditionally formatted) to automatically set back- category of the district while another may have
ground color and font such that percentile few. The first school may exhaust their resources
category values of “lowest 10%” would be high- attending solely to their large number of students
lighted among the values of “middle 80%” and
“highest 10%” (see Fig. 35.5). In the reports pro-
vided to district staff, an automatic formatting 2
The category-driven methods used to convey student-
color scheme was also adopted for the standard-
level information to school staff within these datasets are
ized SEI factor means with those greater than or an improvement upon the single values provided in the
equal to 1.0 changed to green, between −1.5 advisor-advisee reports, but the concern with measure-
and −2.5 to yellow, and those less than or equal ment error is one which requires consideration in future
research and report production efforts.
to −2.5 to red.2
734 J.J. Appleton

in the lowest percentile category while the sec- letter. In this case, the components of the SPSS
ond may also be able to add students with moder- syntax that changed from one group of advisees
ate percentile category (e.g., “middle 80%”) to the next were merged from the Excel file into
values but some (or several) low standardized the SPSS syntax contained in Word. Following
SEI factor values. In either case, upon identifying the generation of the merged syntax within Word,
the students at greatest risk based upon SEI the syntax was copied and pasted into Notepad to
results, both schools would then consult current remove most formatting and then back into the
academic and behavioral data to triangulate infor- SPSS syntax editor for generating the reports.
mation and confirm the students most in need of The created SPSS syntax iteratively filtered out
intervention at that specific point in time. all but the advisees for a particular advisor, gen-
Logistically, these student-level reports requi- erated the graphs for that advisor, and then
red numerous iterations with the dataset as well applied that school-advisor title before moving
as an efficient method for outputting the results onto the next advisor.
into a format accessible to the users. To provide In many cases, looping commands are able to
an understanding of the scale of the project in be used directly within the syntax to avoid the
GCPS, there are approximately 3,500 advisors merging process. The looping command would
with spring reports including four graphs per direct the processor to iterate graphs between a
advisor resulting in the need to generate, and effi- specified beginning and end point. In this case,
ciently output, around 14,000 graphs. Fortunately, the graphing function called was not able to be
several relatively common software applications combined with a looping command. In the pro-
enabled the systematic and efficient generation of cess used, graphs were generated with menu-
these graphs.3 The main application was SPSS driven options until generating as expected and
15.0 for formatting and cleaning the data as well until the page setup specifications ensured graphs
as generating the graphs. SPSS provides the abil- required minimal additional formatting before
ity to use menu-driven options to filter and create exporting. Then syntax was pasted, and the merge
the desired graphs and then to be able to paste the process was conducted and tested until also gen-
syntax into the editor. Using an export of the fre- erating graphs as expected. Then as many graphs
quency table of school names merged with advi- as possible were generated with a single section
sor names (e.g., “Gwinnett High – John Advisor”) of syntax4 with the graphs of each SPSS output
to MS Excel, one can paste the SPSS syntax into file exported to individual PDFs named according
MS Word and then merge all school-advisor to the first and last school and advisor name con-
combinations into the syntax using a common tained within the graphs. These individual files
Word feature. The frequency table was also used were merged into a single PDF, and the large
to find advisor-advisee groups too large for all PDF was updated to contain any information
advisee stacked bars to fit within single-page considered important to convey to users. A confi-
graphs. These groups were reduced to alphabet- dentiality warning, interpretive guide, and com-
ized subgroups (about 32–34 students per graph) mentary on the scale values and date of assessment
using an algorithm and the “compute” option were all added to the single PDF (see Figs. 35.3
within SPSS. The Word feature used to combine and 35.6). Finally, the comprehensive PDF was
the Excel data and SPSS syntax was “mail merge” split by school and pasted to the Lotus Notes
which is often used to integrate names and database used to provide information to the lead-
addresses within the context of a Word-created ership of individual schools.

3
District-owned software specific to GCPS’ method of
generating graphs are highlighted, but freeware such as R
4
(see http://www.r-project.org/) and Open Office (see Memory limitations of the computer used will dictate the
http://www.openoffice.org/) may be suitable alternatives. maximum able to be generated at once.
35 Systems Consultation… 735

Fig. 35.6 SEI data interpretive guide including item and factor descriptions

Aggregated Results divisibility across factors with differing numbers


of items, discussed above, was able to be
In addition to student-level results, school and addressed using a more common method. It was
district-level results were also generated. These believed that larger increments in terms of divis-
types of aggregated results, whether stand-alone ibility would result in larger standard deviations.
or embedded within advisor-advisee reports, are Therefore, the change values for both school- and
critical for supporting appropriate and useful district-grade levels were subjected to paired-
comparisons. For instance, given that previous samples t-tests as well as reported as effect sizes
research has suggested that student levels of in terms of Cohen’s d. These steps were taken to
engagement tended to decrease across time in increase the likelihood that differences repre-
secondary schools (Eccles et al., 1993), how sented as meaningful changes would be so. The
should one interpret decreases in engagement use of Cohen’s d also supported the reference to
among their group of advisees or at their particu- common effect size differences for interpreting
lar school? Deferring to previous research sug- the meaning of small, moderate, and large
gesting differences in engagement for students at changes (i.e., .20, .50, and .80, respectively).
higher secondary grade levels, school and district Though some have recommended the develop-
SEI data were differentiated by grade level (see ment of empirical benchmark rather than rules of
Fig. 35.7). Given the usefulness of evaluating thumb (e.g., see Hill, Bloom, Black, & Lipsey,
both level and change results, these data were 2008), and alternatives are being considered for
combined in a single report. Also, the issue of future reports.
736 J.J. Appleton

Fig. 35.7 Spring aggregated SEI-based school and district report

Ad Hoc Results
Future Directions
Beyond the data produced routinely following
The sections above describe efforts to attend to
each SEI administration, requests have also been
aspects of engagement beyond the academic and
received for specialized analyses. Examples
behavioral subtypes that are generally either easily
included the use of SEI data in the evaluation of
accessible from district web-based data systems or
both the effect of the graduation coach (GC) and of
extractable, with more effort, from separate data
the community-based mentoring (CBM) pro-
systems. The vision for the future focuses on the
grams. The GC program assigned a school adult to
results of further analyses with newly acquired
monitor, coordinate resources, and intervene with
outcome data as well as advancing the delivery
middle and high students at-risk for dropping out
methods of timely engagement data.
based upon state-provided risk ratios and ongoing
school-retrieved student data. The CBM program
matched community mentors with at-risk, male, Vision
middle school students to provide support for aca-
demic and social development. The analyses con- Effectively monitoring student engagement
ceptualized student cognitive and affective requires dynamic data converted to useful infor-
engagement as variables moderating the relation- mation and delivered routinely across those
ships between the influence of these programs and adults interacting with the student. To date, SEI
desired academic and behavioral outcomes. data provide values on subtypes of engagement
35 Systems Consultation… 737

Fig. 35.8 Model for building, evaluating, and refining a dynamic, engagement-based assessment-to-intervention report

previously not assessed or monitored. Yet, the uses of student engagement data. Through a part-
current sources of information are difficult to act nership with the National Student Clearinghouse
upon at times (especially the farther from the (NSC), data were able to be acquired on the post-
administration date the reports and datasets are secondary education outcomes of several years
utilized). Also, a comprehensive view of a stu- of the district’s students. These data include
dent’s engagement currently requires the juxta- enrollment and persistence within, as well as
position of a number of different resources. graduation from, over 92% of the nation’s institu-
Moreover, the resources, once assembled, are not tions of higher education (including 2–4-year,
easily shared between the adults interacting with trade, and vocational schools). As depicted in
the students examined. Ideally, the most impor- Fig. 35.8, the NSC data provide objective valued
tant engagement data would be provided across outcomes to which earlier benchmarks can be
the contexts interacting with the student (e.g., statistically linked. These efforts at linking will
school staff, family, community mentors). have to heed the advice of researchers such as
Moreover, the engagement data would have been Gleason and Dynarski (2002) and Hintze and
statistically related to valued outcomes and there- Silberglitt (2005) to attend to the properties of
fore provide meaningful cut scores on engage- predictors. Predictive power (PP), both in terms
ment variables to facilitate efficient intervention of the percentage of those predicted to fall short
efforts. Additionally, all values with data accru- of some valued outcome that actually do and
ing across the academic year would be updated as those predicted to achieve an outcome that actu-
frequently as relevant and meaningful. ally do, will be important. The important role of
Figures 35.8 and 35.9 provide a graphical rep- the first aspect of PP is ensuring that those desig-
resentation of the vision for future systems-level nated for an intervention truly are in need of it.
738 J.J. Appleton

Fig. 35.9 Vision for types and format for reporting engagement indicators to staff

Additional properties include sensitivity (yield) Additionally, the intent will be to continue to add
and specificity which examine the percentage of even more formative predictors, each reducing the
all those falling short of an outcome that are time between cut scores to assess a given student’s
flagged by the cut score on the predictor as well trajectory. In Fig. 35.8, these ideas are illustrated in
as those achieving the outcome that are also above the “Analysis Level” with the results arriving for
the cut score, respectively. Perhaps most critical consumption in the “User Level” and a more
are the first aspect of PP (also called efficiency) detailed report illustrated in Fig. 35.9. The report
which represents a loss of efficient use of resources displayed will be the level below a single icon rep-
if students are incorrectly designated at-risk (see resenting cumulative risk and the level above more
Gleason & Dynarski) and sensitivity which repre- detailed information specifying the exact source
sents those students truly at-risk but missing out responsible for the color and shape change of an
on interventions due to their classification. icon (e.g., the specific subject and assignments
Cut scores will be generated on predictors missed that resulted in the “Assignment Completion
across the subtypes of engagement as well as pri- Rate” indicator changing from a green square to
mary and secondary grades and then refined as yellow triangle or red exclamation point).
their properties are examined. Initial efforts will Also, the student engagement indicators are
involve a chain of predictors extending from the separated by subtype to facilitate intervention,
final valued outcome with most connected via and the values of indicators are separated based
their prediction of a subsequent predictor. As the upon whether they provide context (and are
longitudinal dataset extends over time, more fixed) or represent the accruing reality of the
intermediary predictors will be examined for util- present. Some indicators are relevant both when
ity in predicting the final valued outcome. fixed and as they accrue (e.g., actual GPA and an
35 Systems Consultation… 739

approximation of GPA). Others are relevant as predictors and then commissioned and received a
approximations (e.g., Assignment Success Rate literature review documenting the extent of these
and Assignment Completion Rate) for other types of predictors. Moreover, our district has
semester and year values (i.e., class grade count transitioned from several stand-alone databases
of Ds or Fs) as well as informative for interven- to an operational data store (ODS) that will be
tion (i.e., “can’t do” or “won’t do”). The idea is updated nightly and integrate numerous previ-
akin to that of a limit in calculus. Information can ously disconnected academic and behavioral
be gleaned from a function predictably approach- engagement data elements. The ODS supports a
ing a value, so much so, that as it gets close enough reporting tool that utilizes established cut scores
to the value, it is considered to be that value. to act upon virtually live data. Further, we have
Accordingly, it is not enough to wait for a student been refining our analyses of disciplinary data (an
to fail a core class and then decide that risk is cer- aspect of behavioral engagement) to the point
tain enough to intervene; it is better to use approx- where our metrics are producing useful approxi-
imations of that failure, when reliable enough, to mations of frequency, average severity, and maxi-
establish risk and intervention efforts earlier. mum severity of disciplinary infractions. We are
Additional aspects of Fig. 35.8 focus on the in contact with districts also embarking on these
link between assessment of risk and intervention types of predictive efforts and continue to gain
as well as the difference between the efficacy of useful information through these collaborations.
intervention using these types of electronic risk Finally, the long-term negative outcomes of stu-
reports and its effectiveness. Given the tremen- dents that do not receive interventions at critical
dous technological advances in linking data points in their tenure in our schools are often not
sources via data warehouses, the characteristics witnessed by those tasked with providing those
of a student displaying risks on the engagement interventions, and the standardization of the mon-
report should be able to be linked to database- itoring and intervention process is often not as
housed intervention characteristics such as those rigorous as could be beneficial. Perhaps, if cut
contained within the What Works Clearinghouse scores and trajectories are sufficiently predictive,
(WWC). If student characteristics are contained likely negative outcomes could be rendered in a
within our district systems, then these, as well as more tangible manner to the user. Also, the check-
specific aspects of risk, should be utilized in list approach whereby communication is improved
matching the details of the most appropriate and the consistency of key aspects of monitoring
intervention for a student displaying risks. Finally, and intervention could be increased across con-
Fig. 35.8 also displays the iterative process of texts may be a useful one (Gawande, 2009).
examining the results of report-guided interven-
tions implemented under best-case scenarios (i.e.,
efficacy) and when implemented in the typical Limitations
setting in which they will customarily be used
(i.e., effectiveness). Admittedly, guidance for A systems-level use of engagement data requires
staff, family, and community users of data will well-trained personnel acting upon accessible,
necessarily be ongoing and iterative based upon timely, and relevant information in order to mon-
information gained from use. itor all students and intervene more intensively as
the information indicates. Yet, limitations of adult
(e.g., teachers, advisors, or parents/guardians)
Progress time and training on using engagement data
within an assessment-to-intervention paradigm
To date, we have cleaned and merged our NSC would seem to be commonalities across many
data into a format able to be used for longitudinal districts. Moreover, the benefits of engagement-
analyses. Also, we have initially examined the focused efforts as well as a thorough understand-
research literature for robust engagement-based ing of the engagement reports and data are not
740 J.J. Appleton

as prevalent as necessary for systems-level is needed to include that consideration. However,


decision-making. Given the many worthwhile issues of meaningful differences and changes
endeavors vying for staff time, greater efforts and have been more thoroughly addressed in the stu-
increasingly convincing evidence of relationships dent-focused engagement pattern datasets as well
to valued outcomes need to be provided to facili- as in the school and district aggregated reports.
tate school-wide support in engaging students. Some of the formatting on the reports is not as
Additionally, many concepts common to research refined as desired due to limitations of the mass
personnel, included in reports and datasets of production process (e.g., reduction of numbers to
engagement information (e.g., significance tests, “…” when value space is limited on the bar and
effect sizes), are often not as familiar across font colors set to match bar color not retained on
school staff. Also, research personnel may lack change graphs). In addition, delivery is currently
familiarity with the training required of skilled limited to a Lotus Notes database that is depen-
classroom teachers and other school staff and dent upon leadership for dissemination to most
would benefit from collaborations with these staff advisors, counselors, graduation coaches, etc.
to determine the timeliest opportunities for inter- Also, PDFs of advisor-advisee reports are not yet
vention. Thus, careful thought and planning as able to be efficiently split into single page docu-
well as additional time will need to be part of any ments named for the title atop the graph in them.
presentations that aim for staff to walk away pre- Logistic functions and item response theory
pared to effectively use engagement information models have not yet been created for SEI
to guide intervention. responses. Increased information on item charac-
Beyond the above general limitation, several teristics and item functioning across important
specific limitations of the approach specified in demographic characteristics and ethnic and pov-
this chapter should be mentioned. Advisor- erty subgroups would increase understanding of
advisee level reports juxtapose the means of SEI the strengths and limitations of the SEI.
factors meaningfully among school and district
values and even among other relevant student
data. Yet, report users are left to decide when val- Conclusion
ues are meaningfully above or below scores on
other SEI factors or school and district values. While the efforts and ideas described in this
The advisor-advisee change reports address this chapter are fraught with shortcomings and rid-
somewhat by standardizing differences by the dled with opportunities for improvement, they
smallest unit of change possible. Nevertheless, a are offered as an exciting, forthright documenta-
more empirical method for comparing engage- tion of one large and diverse district’s effort to
ment levels would be beneficial. Such a method make meaning of a construct considered impor-
is not viewed as a replacement for sound judg- tant, even critical, by the authors in this hand-
ment; rather it is meant to provide increased book. The intent is to provide a tangible example
information and improved guidance to comple- of systematic utilization of engagement data
ment the best practice efforts of clinicians, school across subtypes and to offer a forum for the nec-
staff, and parents/guardians alike. essary commentary and research agendas to fur-
Questions have been raised about the useful- ther understanding and improve school and
ness of the intrinsic motivation factor of the SEI district use of engagement data.
(see Betts et al., 2010). Given that the item is Future research could consider robust engage-
composed of two items, both negatively worded, ment-subtype predictors of valued outcomes as
suggestions have been made to remove that fac- well as reliable and meaningful cut scores on
tor from the SEI and subsequent reports. these predictors. Examinations of the efficiency
The current student-focused reports of aca- and yield of these scores remain critical. Also
demic year changes in engagement did not account important will be continued evaluation of stake-
for measurement error, and further development holder uses of dynamic engagement reports in
35 Systems Consultation… 741

order to isolate the training and presentation of Gawande, A. (2009). The checklist manifesto: How to get
information most closely related to positive out- things right. New York: Henry Holt and Company, LLC.
GCPS. (2010). 2010–11 Fast facts. Retrieved December
comes. Within the context of increased knowl- 12, 2010, from http://www.gwinnett.k12.ga.us/gcps-
edge on the meaning of specific values on mainweb01.nsf/7b206fefc3472ddf85257523004bcb4
predictors paired with a committed district effort 6/7b6e15e7f04b0bae852577670052026d?OpenDocu
to promote engagement, the implementation of a ment&1~QuickLinks
Gleason, P., & Dynarski, M. (2002). Do we know whom
coherent systems-level assessment-to-interven- to serve? Issues in using risk factors to identify drop-
tion paradigm is well within reach. outs. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk,
7, 25–41.
Acknowledgments The author would like to express Goodenow, C., & Grady, K. E. (1993). The relationship of
sincere gratitude to his colleagues within the offices of school belonging and friends’ values to academic
Research and Evaluation, Advisement and Counseling, the motivation among urban adolescent students. The
Information Management Division, as well as within the Journal of Experimental Education, 62, 60–71.
advisement programs at the schools for the many contribu- Hill, C. J., Bloom, H. S., Black, A. R., & Lipsey, M. W.
tions that furthered the efforts described within this chapter. (2008). Empirical benchmarks for interpreting effect
sizes in research. Child Development Perspectives,
2(3), 172–177.
Hintze, J. M., & Silberglitt, B. (2005). A longitudinal
References examination of the diagnostic accuracy and predictive
validity of R-CBM and high-stakes testing. School
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. Psychology Review, 34(3), 372–386.
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- Maehr, M. L., & Meyer, H. A. (1997). Understanding
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. motivation and schooling: Where we’ve been, where
Psychology in the Schools, 45(5), 369–386. we are, and where we need to go. Educational
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. L. Psychology Review, 9, 371–408.
(2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological engage- National Research Council and Institute of Medicine.
ment: Validation of the Student Engagement Instrument. (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering high school stu-
Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445. dents’ motivation to learn. Washington, DC: The
Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., & Martin, C. (2011). National Academies Press.
Research to practice: Measurement and reporting of Newmann, F. M., Wehlage, G. G., & Lamborn, S. D.
student engagement data in applied settings. (1992). The significance and sources of student
Manuscript submitted for publication. engagement. In F. M. Newmann (Ed.), Student engage-
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to ment and achievement in American secondary schools
belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fun- (pp. 11–39). New York: Teachers College Press.
damental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, Reschly, A., & Christenson, S. L. (2006a). Promoting suc-
117, 497–529. cessful school completion. In G. Bear & K. Minke
Betts, J., Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., Christenson, (Eds.), Children’s needs III: Development, prevention,
S. L., & Huebner, S. (2010). A study of the factorial and intervention (pp. 103–113). Bethesda, MD:
invariance of the Student Engagement Instrument National Association of School Psychologists.
(SEI): Results from middle and high school students. Reschly, A., & Christenson, S. L. (2006b). Research lead-
School Psychology Quarterly, 25(2), 84–93. ing to a predictive model of dropout and completion
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, among students with mild disabilities and the role of
autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of student engagement. Remedial and Special Education,
self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe 27, 276–292.
(Eds.), Self processes and development (Vol. 23, pp. Russell, V. J., Ainley, M., & Frydenberg, E. (2005).
43–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Schooling issues digest: Student motivation and
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., engagement. Retrieved November 9, 2005, from http://
Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development www.dest.gov.au/sectors/schooleducation/publications
during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment resources/schooling issues digest/schooling issues digest
fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools and motivation engagement.htm
families. American Psychologist, 48, 90–101. Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motiva-
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of tion. Educational Psychologist, 26, 207–231.
Educational Research, 59, 117–142. Skinner, E. A., Wellborn, J. G., & Connell, J. P. (1990).
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). What it takes to do well in school and whether I’ve got
School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of it: A process model of perceived control and children’s
the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, engagement and achievement in school. Journal of
59–109. Educational Psychology, 82, 22–32.
Finding the Humanity in the Data:
Understanding, Measuring, 36
and Strengthening Student
Engagement

Ethan Yazzie-Mintz and Kim McCormick

Abstract
Within the emerging field of research on student engagement, there exists
a wide variety of work in terms of definitions, constructs, and methodolo-
gies. In this chapter, we argue for the importance of “finding the humanity
in the data,” understanding and investigating student engagement and dis-
engagement as a function of the perceptions of students about their experi-
ences in the learning environment. After setting out a conceptualization of
engagement, we examine data from the High School Survey of Student
Engagement (HSSSE), a self-report survey containing both multiple-option
and open-response questions. Our analysis is focused on the words and
school experiences of students as a way of understanding levels and dimen-
sions of student engagement in school. Using examples from the field, we
describe ways in which schools use student perception data to understand
and strengthen student engagement. We conclude by setting out challenges
for research, policy, and practice in the field of student engagement.

My school is a good school, but I feel the only thing they care about are scores on
standardized tests, not us students as individuals.
– Student respondent on the High School Survey of Student Engagement (HSSSE),
Spring 2010

Introduction

E. Yazzie-Mintz, Ed.D. (*) In this age of accountability as defined by stan-


First Light Education Project, Denver, CO, USA dardized measures of achievement and rigorous
e-mail: e@post.harvard.edu
research on schooling as defined by the utiliza-
K. McCormick, M.A. tion of highly controlled input-output models,
Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology,
Indiana University School of Education,
data – as in “data-driven instruction” and “data-
Bloomington, IN, USA driven decision-making” – are invariably com-
e-mail: mccormkm@indiana.edu prised of those measures that can be counted,

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 743


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_36, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
744 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

standardized, replicated, and predictive of an learning. However, on the survey, the students’
academic outcome. The resulting focus on scores data looked very similar to data from students
and outcomes, incentivized and dictated by state across the country who had participated in
and federal policy, leaves many students feeling HSSSE; students were no more engaged at “Tech
like the student quoted above, in which their High School” than students at other high schools.
value to the school community, particularly to Rather than investigate more deeply the source
the adults, is not based on who they are as devel- of the students’ perceptions or evaluate more
oping adolescents, their potential for learning closely why the technological improvements to
and growing, or their emerging passions and the building had not generated greater engage-
curiosity, but rather on their performance on ment in learning among students, the principal,
standardized assessments. on seeing the results of the survey, proclaimed,
These student perceptions cut across student “They’re making that up…They just want to
demographics: race/ethnicity, gender, socioeco- make us look bad.” The first reaction of the prin-
nomic status, grade level, academic track, and cipal was to distrust the students’ perceptions,
level of academic achievement. The High School create an us (school) versus them (students)
Survey of Student Engagement (HSSSE) is one demarcation, and imply that the students com-
instrument that serves to measure student engage- pleted the surveys with a group intent to tarnish
ment through self-reported perceptions of the the image of the school.
students themselves. Schools use the data from We pointed the school team toward the open-
HSSSE in a variety of ways; effective school uses response question at the end of the survey,
of these data include understanding the experi- which asks students: Would you like to say more
ence of students in the school, investigating par- about any of your answers to these survey ques-
ticular interventions, and improving a particular tions? Students at “Tech High School” gave these
aspect of students’ connection to school (aca- answers:
demic, social, emotional). • I feel like administrators put too much empha-
Several years ago, a school that participated in sis on making the school look good, not the
HSSSE, after receiving their data report, was par- students’ best interest. High school years are
ticularly interested in and baffled by their data, supposed to be a prime time of growing in all
asking: Why are our kids just as bored as the rest ways but ridiculous busy work prevents per-
of the country? This high school, which we will sonal growth outside of school.
call “Tech High School,” was a well-resourced • I am not engaged in any classes and think
school located in a wealthy suburb of a large urban there is too much emphasis on TESTS.
center in the USA. There was little poverty or con- • I often feel that we are pushed to learn answers
cern about resources in this community – “all of instead of material, or to do well on a test
the students’ basic needs are met,” according to a instead of understand the concept.
school staff member – and academic achievement • I just wished the school cared more about what
was high in the school based on standard measures the student wants, not just how they can make
such as standardized tests and graduation rates. the school look good.
Amid such economic advantage and high • I personally feel that too much time is spent in
achievement, the school administrators expected preparation for standardized tests, not enough
that the students would be more engaged in work time is spent learning relevant information.
and learning at school. Their surprise at the results • [This school] cares a lot about its image.
of the student survey was compounded by the fact Maybe too much!
that the school had recently spent a large amount Many of these students described their percep-
of money getting the building “wired up”: install- tion of a school that put a heavy focus on the image
ing high-tech equipment in every classroom, that the school presented to the outside world.
including advanced projectors, interactive white- Despite the advanced technology, the students
boards, and laptops – all technologies purported described a school that was focused on test scores
to engage students more intensely in classroom and image, but not as much on deep understanding
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 745

of concepts, relevance of learning, and students’ learning environment from the perspectives of
growth and development. These open responses, students and then implementing changes that
in combination with students’ responses to the address the academic, social, and emotional
multiple-option questions on the survey, present needs of students can have an enormous impact
a picture of “Tech High School” as a school that, on both school processes and outcomes.
though possessing the resources to provide more
materials and structures, was both facing the
same challenges of engagement as other schools What Is Student Engagement?
across the country and failing to use their
resources to deepen learning or engagement. The question, “What is student engagement?,” is
Further, the principal’s assertion that the students deceptively complex. An informal poll of teach-
were “trying to make the school look bad” ers, principals, researchers, and policymakers
through their survey responses – that is, they yields a range of responses to this question: one-
weren’t really giving honest answers, but were word definitions including “involvement,” “par-
using the survey to create a negative image of the ticipation,” and “investment”; more expansive
school – was picked up by the students and high- answers, such as “deep connection to learning”
lighted by their written perceptions on the survey and “interest in the subject matter”; articulation
that the school is more focused on its image than of ways to measure engagement, such as “amount
on teaching, learning, and development. In a sim- of time students spend on tasks” and “students
ilar way as the student quoted at the top of this asking and answering questions in class”; and
chapter, these students of “Tech High School” metaphors – “I see students huddled together lit-
see themselves as data points in service of a par- erally digging in.” The wide spectrum of under-
ticular school agenda revolving around account- standings about engagement present a picture of a
ability to the outside world, and not as individuals concept that has lent itself better to identification
experiencing a place and time to learn, grow, and by observation – “I know it when I see it” – than
develop curiosity about the world. to a commonly accepted definition in research
This discrepancy between adult and student and practice, leaving the question “How do we
perceptions of the student experience in school measure student engagement?” up for debate.
raises several questions regarding student engage- This complexity is reflected in the multiple
ment that will be examined in this chapter: and evolving definitions, indicators, and concep-
• What is student engagement, and how is it tions of engagement guiding research and prac-
conceptualized and measured? tice. Despite the complexity, the thrust of applied
• What do students say about their learning research focuses heavily on discrete predictors
experiences and environments? and indicators of engagement, primarily student
• Why does student engagement matter for behaviors and school structures. For example,
research and practice? schools such as “Tech High School” are driven
In this chapter, we argue for the importance of by a body of current research that links the use of
“finding the humanity in the data,” understanding technology with higher student engagement. The
and investigating student engagement and disen- prevailing philosophy is that students in K-12
gagement as a function of the perceptions of stu- institutions today have grown up with a burgeon-
dents about their experiences in the learning ing array of visual media options and advanced
environment. The culture of schools is too often technology (e.g., laptops, social networking web-
focused on the needs of adults in the school envi- sites, cell phones); hence, the thinking goes, these
ronment, preventing deep reforms and improve- students will learn best if these technologies are
ments; students are expected to function brought into the classroom. However, just as with
effectively and productively within learning any school structure or pedagogical strategy, the
environments from which they feel alienated and “how” is even more important than the “what.”
within which they feel unimportant. Strengthening Technology in and of itself is not engaging,
student engagement by first understanding the though it can provide opportunities to engage
746 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

students in learning. A recent study of math teach- and learn tend to be curious, persistent, and eager
ing and learning in Australia provides insight into for challenging tasks; these are students who
the relationship between engagement and learn- want to build a body of knowledge instead of
ing as mediated by structures such as technology learning something solely for a reward or other
(Attard, 2009, 2010). Students, transitioning from outcome (Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 1994;
primary school to secondary school, experienced Gottfried & Gottfried, 1996). While these stu-
as well a transition in pedagogy – from a hands- dents need little more than an appropriate task to
on, active style of math teaching to a computer- stimulate them to work, research shows that
based pedagogy that involved less interaction intrinsic motivation lessens as students move
with the teacher. The effect of this change in ped- from elementary to middle to high school (Lepper,
agogy was a weakening of teacher-student rela- Corpus, & Lyengar, 2005; Lepper, Sethi, Dialdin,
tionships and a lowering of student engagement & Drake, 1997). As time in school increases, stu-
in math. The further danger is the possibility that dents’ interest and curiosity in learning lessens,
such changes in student engagement can precipi- and many seek tasks that are not challenging
tate decreases in student achievement. (Harter, 1992; Harter & Jackson, 1992).
In its most fundamental sense, engagement Focusing on students’ motivation to work and
is about relationships (Yazzie-Mintz, 2007). learn, then, may lead to a classroom trap: create
Whether two people choose to become “engaged” rewards that activate extrinsically motivated stu-
by embarking on a permanent and intimate rela- dents, leading to a focus on the reward rather than
tionship, or two forces “engage” in battle by enter- the project or related learning, or create appropri-
ing a violent and confrontational relationship, the ate tasks that activate the intrinsically motivated
necessary component of “engagement” is a rela- students, though the number of these types of stu-
tionship; engagement cannot be achieved or dents decreases as students move through the K-12
accomplished by oneself. system (perhaps as a result of the system itself).
Engagement is often misunderstood as syn- Often, adults expect students who are motivated to
onymous with motivation. For the purposes of be engaged in school. In fact, those students who
this chapter, the authors view motivation as one are motivated intrinsically may be more likely to
component of what makes a student engaged in seek opportunities for learning and engagement
their learning and school. Motivation describes outside of school, if school does not provide such
the processes and factors that drive or move a opportunities (Jordan & Nettles, 1999). Students
student to take action. These processes and fac- who are motivated by external rewards may par-
tors are not generally defined in relationship, but ticipate in schooling to the extent they receive the
as a function of the individual student in response rewards they seek, but not engage deeply in learn-
to a set of internal or external stimuli. What hin- ing or work. Further, adults in school tend to give
ders or supports motivation can be contingent on up on students who are not motivated in tradition-
the context or environment. ally understood ways (either intrinsically or
through an external rewards system). Focusing on
Extrinsic motivation describes an external reward engagement provides both an opportunity and a
as the stimulus for action; for example, a student responsibility for school communities – adults and
will engage in a behavior to receive a positive students – to work collectively and systemically
reward or consequence, such as a grade, that is toward greater learning for all.
separate from the behavior (deCharms, 1968; We approach this research from an engage-
Lepper & Greene, 1978). Intrinsic motivation ment perspective in which motivation is one
describes an internal stimulus – for example, aspect of the larger engagement construct. In the
enjoyment of the task or project – that drives case of students and schools, engagement is
action; the reward is the behavior itself, which about the student’s relationship to the school
brings enjoyment to the individual student. and learning community: the people (including
Students who are intrinsically motivated to work adults and peers), the structures (including rules,
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 747

schedules, and the organization of the school), of their schooling (achievement, persistence, and
the instruction, the curriculum and content, and graduation).
the opportunities for participation in this commu- In the course of the short history of the study
nity (including curricular, cocurricular, and extra- of student engagement and the growing under-
curricular). Researching student engagement is standing of the importance of the concept, many
essentially about investigating the dimensions different definitions and models have been cre-
and depth of the relationship between the student ated. The most common definition of student
and the school community, and the ways in which engagement consists of two components: a
this relationship is enacted. The HSSSE focuses behavioral component associated with positive
on investigating and understanding, from the behavior, effort, and participation, and an emo-
students’ perspectives, the relationship between tional/affective component that includes interest,
the student and the learning community and uti- identification with school, a sense of belonging,
lizing research and professional development to and a positive attitude about learning (Finn, 1989;
strengthen this relationship. Marks, 2000; Newmann, Wehlage, & Lamborn,
1992; Willms, 2003). Some researchers have
added a third cognitive component to the defini-
Research on Student Engagement tion of student engagement, which includes self-
regulation, learning goals, and a measure of
As a result of efforts to study and formalize think- students’ investment in their learning (Fredricks
ing and research on student engagement, multiple et al., 2004; Jimerson, Campos, & Greif, 2003;
models of engagement have been developed. Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2003).
Within the research literature, a complex, multi- Behavioral engagement typically describes
dimensional definition of “student engagement” student actions that can easily be observed inside
is drawn (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004), the classroom – for example, students actively
though understandings of student engagement participating in classroom activities, assignments,
continue to evolve. The concept of student and projects. This type of engagement focuses on
engagement as an area of study has emerged rela- the degree to which students take an active role in
tively recently in education research, becoming school-related activities, both inside the class-
increasingly prevalent over the last 20 years room and across all school areas (Fredricks et al.,
(Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, 2008), 2004; Munns & Woodward, 2006). Cognitive
though the notion of students being engaged and engagement describes students’ investment in the
involved in learning is a foundational concept in actual learning process – not just demonstration
the history of education and schooling. Given of involvement through external behaviors but
that student engagement is often associated with internal investment, where the mind is engaged
student choice (i.e., students choosing classes, in classroom work. For example, cognitive
schools, how to spend their time, etc.), compul- engagement can be demonstrated by a student’s
sory schooling at the K-12 level in the USA has mastery of the full meaning of curricular mate-
made student engagement less widely studied rial, taking a position more of an expert than that
(particularly at the systemic level) than at the of a novice (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2003;
postsecondary level, where student engagement Munns & Woodward, 2006). Motivational/emo-
often falls under the purview of offices of institu- tional engagement describes the degree to which
tional research as well as student services. From students see the value of what they are doing in
a policy perspective, schooling at the K-12 level school. Students engaged in this way are not just
is predominantly viewed as something that stu- going through the motions of the academic expe-
dents have to do, as a matter of law and as an rience but are self-reflective about their learning
entry into the world of work or further schooling; and feel connections between their lives and their
whether students are involved or engaged in their work (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2003; Munns &
learning is less consequential than the outcomes Woodward, 2006).
748 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

This three-component model of student


engagement continues to evolve (Appleton et al.,
The Culture of Schools and Student
2008). The behavioral component of engagement
Engagement
has been split into two more specific subtypes to
How can we conceptualize student engagement
include academic engagement, encompassing
in a systemic way? Given the focus on motiva-
measures, such as time on task, credit hours,
tion and the current research imperative to give
and homework completion, and behavioral
primacy to “countable” measures, student
engagement, which includes attendance, volun-
engagement is largely understood as an individ-
tary classroom participation, and extracurricular
ual student behavioral issue. However, this nar-
participation. The expansion of the model also
row focus generates only a partial picture.
includes the varying contexts that influence stu-
Conceptualizing student engagement as a cultural
dent engagement (e.g., family, peers, and school),
issue in schools rather than a policy, behavioral,
students’ interactions within these contexts, and
or structural issue provides a window through
the degree to which students’ interactions within
which the student experience can be understood.
these contexts help students meet fundamental
In “Thick Description: Toward an Interpretive
needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness
Theory of Culture,” Clifford Geertz (1973)
(Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, 2006;
described the individual as “an animal suspended
Appleton et al., 2008).
in webs of significance he himself has spun,” and
A number of studies have asserted a connec-
“culture” as these “webs of significance.” Culture
tion between engagement and school processes
is a set of relationships that are interrelated, over-
or outcomes. For example, engagement has been
lapping, nonlinear, and rarely compartmental-
associated with student achievement (Appleton
ized; when looked at all together and from a
et al., 2008; Finn, 1993, Furrer & Skinner, 2003;
distance, the picture is that of a web or a set of
Klem & Connell, 2004), positive classroom and
webs. Understanding the student experience in
school climate (Appleton et al., 2006; Cohen,
schools as a set of “webs of significance” pro-
McCabe, Michelli, & Pickeral, 2009; Newmann
vides the opportunity to explore the complexity
et al., 1992), and effective instructional practices
of student engagement and highlights the traps
(Mant, Wilson, & Coates, 2007; Shernoff,
inherent in parsing out the various pieces of
Csikszentmihalyi, Schneider, & Shernoff, 2003).
school so common to the input-output models.
In the policy climate heavily dependent on aca-
This conception illuminates why implementing a
demic outcomes, it is important to note that a
structure does not necessarily have a direct and
number of studies have asserted a relationship
uniform impact on student outcomes. For exam-
between student engagement and student aca-
ple, though block scheduling (longer class times
demic outcomes (Yazzie-Mintz, 2010). However,
for key subject areas than traditional class peri-
much of the research literature on engagement and
ods) is often asserted to increase student learning
achievement focuses on two primary areas: stu-
and achievement, implementing block schedul-
dent behaviors (e.g., self-efficacy, self-regulation,
ing, absent other changes, does not necessarily
and motivation; see, for example, Furrer &
accomplish this purpose due to the need to change
Skinner, 2003; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2003;
styles of teaching to adapt to the longer class peri-
Skinner, Wellborn, & Connell, 1990) and school
ods; the importance of making effective use of
structures (e.g., class scheduling, school and class
the increased time for interaction between teach-
size, attendance, use of technology). Few studies
ers and students, students and peers, and students
focus on all the parts of the school system simulta-
and subject matter; and the resistance members of
neously, including the interaction among the vari-
the school community will have to a change in
ous relationships, structures, and stakeholders in
established routine. As a structural issue, longer
schools in connection with student engagement
classes are theorized to have a positive impact on
and its association with student achievement
learning and achievement; however, failure of the
(Johnson, Crosnoe, & Elder, 2001).
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 749

structure is a near certainty unless the ways in doing research with (or on, as is frequently the
which a structural change has an impact on the case) minors.
culture of the school – the various and multiple Though it is easier to do research with school
“webs of significance” – are investigated, under- adults, on externally observable behavior, and
stood, and addressed. using countable data and records, students’ per-
In attempting to investigate and measure stu- spectives on their own experiences are critical in
dent engagement, the importance of understand- understanding student engagement. The school is
ing student engagement as a culture is further not a set of isolated pieces that function in their
highlighted. Geertz (1973) described the process own realms, but rather a “social organism,” in
of studying culture (through ethnography) as which “the life of the whole is in all its parts, yet
much more than just a set of “techniques” and the whole could not exist without any of its parts”
“procedures”; the focus must be the “intellectual (Waller, 1932). To understand how, why, and to
effort” and creation of “thick description” that what degree students engage in or disengage
lights the way toward understanding the culture from the life and work of the school, it is neces-
rather than just observing and counting it. sary to understand the ways in which all of these
Applying these insights to research on student different parts function and interact as part of the
engagement, we are compelled to look beyond whole. As in any social system, an understanding
just countable and observable measures – time on of the complexities of the system does not neces-
task, attendance/truancy, and lateness – to gain an sarily reside in those at the top of the system, who
understanding of the relationships, connections, only have a narrow understanding and perspec-
and multiple pathways that lead to student tive on the ways in which the system operates;
engagement with work and learning. those at the bottom of the social hierarchy within
Frequently left out of the conversations and a system often have the greatest insights into the
research on student engagement are the experi- whole system. Within schools, those at the bot-
ences of the students from the perspectives of the tom of the social hierarchy are the students;
students themselves. Students are in a unique understanding a complex process such as student
position where they not only see and feel what is engagement necessitates understanding the stu-
going on in their schools, but they have the voices dent experience from the perspective of the stu-
to let people know what they think. Giving power dents themselves. As Senge (2000) wrote,
to student perspectives can directly improve edu- “Students are the only players who see all sides
cation practice because when teachers and school of the nested systems of education, yet they are
officials listen to and learn from students, they typically the people who have the least influence
can begin to see the world from those students’ on its design.”
perspectives (Cook-Sather, 2001). Why would
these important and relevant voices be so often
excluded? First, as we have heard from numerous The High School Survey of Student
school leaders and other school adults, including Engagement
the principal of “Tech High School” cited earlier,
adults do not trust what students say. Second, The HSSSE is designed to both help schools
school adults often believe they are charged with ascertain students’ beliefs about their school
creating, organizing, and directing the teaching, experience and provide assistance to schools in
learning, and school structures; what students say translating data into action. HSSSE investigates
or believe does not matter to them or they assume deeply the attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs of
they know what the student perspective is. Finally, students about their work, the school learning
researchers often avoid studying students directly environment, and their interaction with the school
due to the increased requirements – including community. Survey questions investigate the lev-
consent forms, parent/guardian permission, and els and dimensions of student engagement in the
scrutiny from Institutional Review Boards – for life and work of high schools, providing schools
750 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

with rich and valuable data on students’ beliefs, activities, can be described as engagement of the
attitudes, and behaviors. The data from the sur- mind. Survey questions that are grouped within
vey help schools explore the causes and condi- this dimension of engagement include questions
tions that lead to student success or failure, about homework, preparation for class, classroom
engagement or “disengagement,” and persistence discussions and assignments, and the level of aca-
or dropping out. demic challenge that students report. Cronbach’s
Built on the foundation of the National Survey alpha for this dimension is 0.943 (Johnson &
of Student Engagement (NSSE) for postsecond- Dean, 2011).
ary students, the HSSSE survey originally mea-
sured similar constructs of engagement (Kuh, Social/behavioral/participatory engagement emp-
2003). More recently, HSSSE has been a research hasizes students’ actions and participation within
and professional development project directed by the school outside of instructional time, includ-
the Center for Evaluation and Education Policy ing nonacademic school-based activities, social
(CEEP) at Indiana University in Bloomington. and extracurricular activities, and interactions
From 2006 through 2010, more than 350,000 stu- with other students – the ways in which students
dents in over 40 states took the survey. interact within the school community beyond the
There are 35 major items on the survey; classroom. This dimension, with its focus on stu-
including sub-questions, there are over 100 items dent actions, interactions, and participation
to which students respond. Survey questions within the school community, can be described as
cover a wide range of aspects of engagement, engagement in the life of the school. Survey ques-
including how students spend their time, the tions that are grouped within this dimension of
importance they place on particular activities, the engagement include questions about extracurric-
rigor and challenge of classes, reasons for going ular activities, students’ interactions with other
to school, tendency toward boredom in school, students, and students’ connections to the com-
potential for dropping out, and types of teaching munity within and around the school. Cronbach’s
they find engaging. The last question on the sur- alpha for this dimension is 0.760 (Johnson &
vey is an open-response question, to which stu- Dean, 2011).
dents can provide longer-form thoughts on their
school experience. Emotional engagement encompasses students’
HSSSE is built on a three-component frame- feelings of connection to (or disconnection from)
work of engagement, taking into account both their school – how students feel about where they
the demands of research and the utility of are in school, the ways and workings of the
the construct to schools and other learning orga- school, and the people within the school. This
nizations. As such, the High School Survey of dimension, focusing largely on students’ internal
Student Engagement utilizes three dimensions of lives not frequently expressed explicitly in
engagement for analysis of data (Yazzie-Mintz, observable behavior and actions, can be described
2007, 2009, 2010): cognitive/intellectual/aca- as engagement of the heart. Survey questions that
demic engagement, social/behavioral/participa- are grouped within this dimension include ques-
tory engagement, and emotional engagement. tions about general feelings regarding the school,
A recent psychometric study of these scales found level of support students perceive from members
the three-dimension construct to be valid and of the school community, and students’ place in
reliable (Johnson & Dean, 2011). the school community. Cronbach’s alpha for this
dimension is 0.937 (Johnson & Dean, 2011).
Cognitive/intellectual/academic engagement cap-
tures students’ effort, investment in work, and
strategies for learning: the work students do and Data Sample
the ways students go about their work. This dimen-
sion, focusing primarily on engagement during In this chapter, we examine data from HSSSE 2009
instructional time and with instruction-related to provide examples of the kinds of school-wide
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 751

perspectives that can be gathered through HSSSE. respondents on the High School Survey of
In 2009, 42,754 students participated in HSSSE, Student Engagement (n = 352,140) expected not
representing a response rate of 74%. These stu- to finish high school. Even looking at the rates
dents came from 103 high schools across 27 differ- within schools, and across grade levels, the aspi-
ent states within the USA. Schools came from all rations outpace the actual graduation numbers.
five regions of the country (Northeast, Southeast, What happens between aspiration and gradua-
Midwest, Southwest, and West), with 63% of the tion? Student perspectives provide insight.
participating schools located in the West and Several questions in particular are highlighted
Midwest. The smallest participating school here as describing students’ perspectives on their
enrolled 20 students, and the largest participating school experiences:
school enrolled 3,143 students; average student • Why do you go to school?
enrollment was 787. By grade level, 30% of • Have you ever been bored in class in high
respondents were in grade 9, 27% in grade 10, school?
23% in grade 11, and 20% in grade 12. Respondents • If you have been bored in class, why?
were 52% female and 48% male. Of the sample, • Have you ever considered dropping out of
25% reported being eligible for free and reduced- high school?
price lunch, 54% reported not being eligible, and • If you have thought about dropping out of
21% did not know or preferred not to respond to high school, why?
the question of eligibility. By race/ethnicity, 49.9% • To what degree does each of the following
of the respondents categorized themselves as types of work in class excite and/or engage
White, 12.5% as Black or African American, 6.5% you?
as Latino or Hispanic, 5.9% as Asian, 1.8% as These questions were selected for analysis in
Native American (including Native Alaskan or this chapter for several reasons: (1) these items
Hawaiian), and 1.0% as Middle Eastern; 9.3% represent important cornerstones in understand-
identified themselves as more than one race (mul- ing students’ thinking in terms of their school
tiracial), and 13.1% preferred not to respond to the attendance, daily school and classroom experi-
question (Yazzie-Mintz, 2010). ence, and possibility of exiting school prior to
graduation, and (2) these items provide rich data
for schools in several critical areas and are espe-
Data Analysis cially useful for schools beginning the process of
using student perspectives as a starting point for
Why is it important to understand the school analysis and school improvement.
experience from the perspective of students?
There is the notion that for students to be full par- Reasons for Going to School
ticipating members of a school community, their As a first step in the process of understanding stu-
voices and beliefs must be a part of conversations dent perspectives, it is critical to know why stu-
and decisions on school policies, structures, and dents attend school. While K-12 schooling in the
teaching and learning. In addition, there are aca- USA is compulsory, compliance with the law is
demic and policy considerations. There is a real only the fifth most reported reason for why stu-
and serious gap between student aspirations and dents attend high school. In fact, there are three
rates of student graduation. Data from the US primary purposes students have for attending
Department of Education indicate that for the school: an academic purpose, a social purpose, and
class of 2008 in public high schools in the USA, a family related purpose (Yazzie-Mintz, 2010).
25% of students did not graduate within 4 years On the survey, students were asked, “Why do
of entering high school, considered “on time” you go to school?” Students could provide as
graduation (Stillwell, 2010). many responses to this question as they wanted.
In 2009, only 1.5% of HSSSE respon- As can be seen in Table 36.1, the most-reported
dents expected to leave high school without a reason for attending school was “Because
diploma; from 2006 to 2010, only 3% of student I want to get a degree and go to college” (73.1%).
752 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

Table 36.1 HSSSE Spring 2009 aggregate Table 36.2 HSSSE Spring 2009 aggregate
Why do you go to school? Have you ever been bored in class in high school?
I enjoy being in school 35.8% Never 2.5%
What I learn in classes 40.5% Once or twice 4.2%
My teacher(s) 23.1% Once in a while 27.6%
My parents/guardians 64.0% Every day 48.7%
My peers/friends 66.1% Every class 17.0%
It’s the law 55.7%
I want to get a degree and go to college 73.1%
I want to acquire skills for the workplace 44.8% Table 36.3 HSSSE Spring 2009 aggregate
There’s nothing else to do 17.2% If you have been bored in class, why?
I want to get a good job 66.8% Work wasn’t challenging enough 32.8%
To stay out of trouble 21.5% Work was too difficult 26.3%
Other 10.3% Material wasn’t interesting 81.3%
Material wasn’t relevant to me 41.6%
No interaction with teacher 34.5%
Close behind were “Because I want to get a good Other 16.4%
job” (66.8%), “Because of my peers/friends”
(66.1%), and “Because of my parents/guardians”
(64.0%). These reasons can be understood as report being bored “once in a while.” Only 4.2%
focused on the future, on the people with whom report being bored “once or twice,” and only
the students attend school, and on family. 2.5% report “never” being bored in class in high
By contrast, students’ experiences inside the school.
school and inside the classroom were reasons for While there is widespread boredom among
going to school for fewer than half of respon- students in high school, even more important than
dents: “Because of what I learn in classes” the frequency of boredom is why students are
(40.5%), “Because I enjoy being in school” bored. Students could provide as many answers as
(35.8%), and “Because of my teachers” (23.1%). applicable to the second question, with results
Schools can use these student responses to dig presented in Table 36.3. The most frequent reason
deeper into why the essential functions of schools cited is “Material wasn’t interesting” (81.3%),
(teaching and learning) are not the reasons most followed by “Material wasn’t relevant to me”
of their students are attending their schools on a (41.6%). These responses provide a foundation
daily basis. for understanding the challenges schools face in
creating engaging curriculum and instruction;
Boredom while schools are often focused on outcome mea-
Student boredom is an important variable to sures and assessments, most students are report-
investigate in understanding engagement and dis- ing that they do not find the content engaging
engagement. On the survey, students were asked enough, and many report that they do not under-
two specific questions related to boredom: “Have stand the importance of what they are learning,
you ever been bored in class in high school?” and and the connection between what they are learn-
“If you have been bored in class, why?” ing and other content or their lives outside of
Table 36.2 presents results from student school. Further, 34.5% of the students report that
responses to the first question. Predictable as it they are bored because they have “No interaction”
may be, two-thirds of student respondents report with their teachers; given the importance of inter-
being bored in class at least every day, if not in action to the instructional process, it is important
every class: 17.0% of students report being bored to look at why more than one-third of this sample
in every class, and 48.7% of students report being of students report that a key source of their bore-
bored every day. In addition, 27.6% of students dom lies in a lack of teacher-student interaction.
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 753

Table 36.4 HSSSE Spring 2009 aggregate dropping out; students could give as many reasons
Have you ever considered dropping out of high school? as were applicable. Of the students who have con-
Never 79.0% sidered dropping out, the most frequently indicated
Once or twice 14.3% reasons are: “I didn’t like the school” (49.8%),
Many times 6.7% “I didn’t see the value in the work I was being
asked to do” (42.3%), and “I didn’t like the teach-
ers” (38.5%). It is interesting and important to note
Table 36.5 HSSSE Spring 2009 aggregate that students’ experiences in school and in class
If you have thought about dropping out of high school, why? were reported as reasons for going to school by
The work was too hard 35.0% fewer than half of the respondents, as reported
The work was too easy 12.9% above; these experiences in school and in class
I didn’t like the school 49.8% were also reported most frequently by students as
I didn’t like the teachers 38.5% the reasons why they consider dropping out of
I didn’t see the value in the work I was being 42.3% school. While there are other factors that students
asked to do
I was picked on or bullied 16.3%
report as moving them to consider dropping out,
I needed to work for money 20.0% including family issues (29.2%), schools can have
No adults in the school cared about me 16.0% great influence over the in-school and in-class
Family issues 29.2% factors and can look to these data for guidance in
I felt I was too far behind in credits to graduate 21.5% addressing the dropout issue.
I failed required standardized tests for graduation 8.5%
Adults in school encouraged me to drop out 9.4% Pedagogy
Other 22.2% Looking inside the classroom, it is important to
investigate the kinds of instruction that engage
students and activate their interest in learning.
Potential for Dropping Out Table 36.6 presents students’ responses to the
Bridgeland, DiIulio, and Morison (2006) have question “To what degree does each of the fol-
described the process of dropping out of school lowing types of work in class excite and/or
as a “slow process of disengagement.” Schools engage you?”
across the USA struggle with keeping students in “Teacher lecture” is the least-preferred type of
school through completion of the diploma, reduc- teaching; 44.2% of students report liking teacher
ing dropout rates and raising graduation rates. As lecture “not at all” and 29.8% of students report
a policy issue, the debate between encourage- liking teacher lecture “a little,” while only 6.0%
ment and punishment as a means of dealing with of students like it “very much.” By contrast,
students who drop out remains active, and the 60.9% of students reported that “Discussion and
costs to individuals and society of dropping out debate” excited and/or engaged them “some” or
are significant (Sum, Khatiwada, McLaughlin, “very much,” while only 15.8% reported liking
with Palma, 2009). this type of work “not at all”; similarly, 60.1% of
Table 36.4 presents data from student students reported that “Group projects” excited
responses to the survey question “Have you ever and/or engaged them “some” or “very much,”
considered dropping out of high school?” Though while only 16.4% reported liking this type of
79% of the students have never considered drop- work “not at all.” Both “Discussion and debate”
ping out of high school, 21% have considered and “Group projects” provide learning forums in
dropping out either “once or twice” (14.3%) or which students are interacting with their peers
“many times” (6.7%). and the teacher and working collaboratively to
As with the question on boredom, the reasons learn and generate knowledge.
students give for considering dropping out are Data from this question can be instructive for
instructive in addressing the issue. Table 36.5 pres- schools, as they work to examine and improve
ents the reasons students provide for considering the learning experiences of students in class.
754 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

Table 36.6 HSSSE Spring 2009 aggregate


To what degree does each of the following types of work in class excite and/or engage you?
Not at all (%) A little (%) Some (%) Very much (%)
Teacher lecture 44.2 29.8 20.0 6.0
Discussion and debate 15.8 23.3 32.3 28.6
Individual reading 33.6 29.7 26.3 10.4
Writing projects 37.0 29.6 24.4 9.0
Research projects 33.0 29.7 27.9 9.4
Group projects 16.4 23.5 37.9 22.2
Presentations 27.5 26.9 31.2 14.4
Role plays 31.3 25.9 25.6 17.1
Art and drama activities 27.8 22.7 25.2 24.3
Projects and lessons involving technology 19.2 25.8 34.1 21.0

Students are less likely to be engaged by work in the student who has the least amount of say about
which they are passively receiving knowledge, what goes into the policies and practices of the
such as teacher lecture, but will be more engaged school; it is the adults who determine what hap-
by instructional methods in which they are work- pens in schools and what changes are made. It is
ing and learning with the teacher and peers and/or more than ironic that the very people who every-
where they are active participants in their learn- one wants to better serve typically are not con-
ing (such as presentations, which 45.6% of stu- sulted (Cook-Sather, 2002).
dents report liking “some” or “very much”; role The student responses to this open-response
plays, which 42.7% of students report liking question expand on and enlighten the responses
“some” or “very much”; and art and drama activi- students give to the multiple-option questions.
ties, which 49.5% of students report liking “some” For researchers, these responses provide another
or “very much”). In addition, schools can explore check on the interpretation of the multiple-option
effective uses of technology within the curricu- survey responses. For schools, these responses
lum, as 55.1% of students report being engaged provide deeper insight into student thinking about
by “Projects and lessons involving technology.” their school experience, in a way that schools
might otherwise not be able to access. In 2009,
Student Engagement in Students’ Words 8,150 students provided a response to the open-
The last question on the survey is an open- response question on the survey. From 2006
response question asking respondents “Would through 2010, more than 50,000 students pro-
you like to say more about your answers to these vided responses to this question.
survey questions?” This question provides a Several themes that emerge in students’ open
space for students to give longer-form, narrative responses highlight important issues in the stu-
thoughts on their school experiences and school dent experience. One common theme, frequently
engagement. Students are in a unique position expressed by students, is that what they have to
within the social hierarchies of schools, seeing say does not matter within the school community
the school from a perspective that adults cannot and that no change will come about as a result of
access easily. For this reason, the open-response their participation in this survey. One student
statements that students share on the High School respondent expressed this thought in this way:
Survey of Student Engagement offer myriad These surveys are pointless because no mat-
insights into what students think about their expe- ter what we say, none of the supervisors will lis-
riences and what improvements can be made. ten to us. Over several years, this theme has
Schools, for their part, must be prepared to hear come up among students from across the spec-
and act on the perspectives of students. Commonly, trum of schools and demographics, expressed
of all stakeholders in the school community, it is as some variation of this student’s response.
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 755

All of the students who respond in this way have knowledge. Students look for challenge and
actually completed this survey and then noted meaning in their work, not just memorization of
that they do not believe their words and ideas will facts and figures and tasks out of context. Students
be taken seriously in any meaningful, action- distinguish between work that is necessary to
resulting way. It will be important for further complete for academic reasons and work that
research to examine those students who also feel challenges them to think and learn. Engagement
this way but did not complete a survey, and for can be pursued and sustained through intellectu-
schools to find a way to solicit those students’ ally challenging work.
ideas. This body of students is sending the mes- Instructional methods play a role in engage-
sage that schools need to be places that actually ment and disengagement within the classroom, as
listen and respond to what students have to say, evidenced by data cited from the multiple-option
particularly when they ask for students’ viewpoints survey question on pedagogy. Students follow
through surveys such as HSSSE. Schools need to through on this theme on the open-response ques-
demonstrate to students that they use the data in tion in which they frequently assert the need for
meaningful ways; students feeling disconnected more hands-on, interactive learning. One student
from the workings of the school may be prone to captured this idea by writing out this question:
be disconnected from the learning as well. Why won’t they bring what we are learning to
Another theme that emerges is the power of life? Interaction is high on the list of students’
teachers in the lives of students, expressed by a desires for engaging methods of teaching and
student in this way: I always wished at least one learning. Both on the multiple-option question
teacher would see a skill in me that seemed and on the open-response question, students cite
extraordinary, or help to encourage its growth. those methods in which they are interacting with
While the current policy debates in state legisla- the teacher, peers, and the material – group proj-
tures across the country often focus on the inef- ects, discussion, and debate – as the most engag-
fectiveness of teachers, students in their open ing. Students do not want to be told what to know,
responses on the survey identify teachers as the but instead want to discover knowledge, see how
possible link to engagement with school and information is related to them, and how it can be
learning. On the multiple-option questions on beneficial for their future. Teachers can accom-
the survey cited above, students point to their plish this by using a variety of hands-on, active
in-class experiences as being low on the list of instructional practices that elicit interaction and,
reasons they go to school and high on the list of ultimately, engagement.
reasons why they have considered dropping out; Schools are most frequently assessed on out-
however, on the open-response questions, stu- comes, which raises the question of the purpose
dents often identify individual teachers and other of teaching and learning in schools: Is it just to
adults who have created engaging learning envi- get students to the finish line and earn the
ronments for them. Teachers are most often and diploma? Or is it to produce students who are
publicly judged on their progress with students lifelong learners actively in pursuit of knowl-
based on standardized outcome measures; stu- edge? Students ask this question as well, as they
dents most often look to teachers for supportive identify the limits of their schooling experience:
and meaningful relationships. Through both posi- School does not determine how smart a student
tive and negative student responses regarding is. A “smart” student is one who absorbs every-
teachers, schools can identify ways to strengthen thing they are told. I hate school because it limits
the student learning experience in the classroom. students to one kind of smart. Students who are
One student’s response – I wish school could highly engaged in their school experience have
be intellectually challenging as well as academi- higher achievement, better images of themselves
cally challenging – raises another common issue, as students and learners, and better overall atti-
the distinction between tasks to complete and tudes about school and learning. The data from
assignments that provide opportunities to pursue the open-response question indicate that, while
756 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

adults (teachers, administrators, policymakers, multiple-option and open-response questions


researchers, etc.) are focused on achievement and provide opportunities for schools to begin an
outcomes, students are overwhelmingly looking examination of what engaged learning means to
for engaging learning experiences: I hate learn- students and what students are looking for in their
ing for tests. I want to learn for the joy of school experience. The data open up the possibil-
learning. ity for transformation and genuine improvement
Often because adult perspectives are at the of teaching, learning, and school culture.
forefront of policy and structural decisions in high
schools, there is a mismatch between what is being
implemented and how the needs of the actual stu- Strengthening Student Engagement:
dents are being met. Expressing both conscious- The Importance of Context
ness that the popular image of students is that they
do not want to learn in schools and a critique that Student engagement in school work and learning
the school is not focused on learning, one student is often viewed as a psychological or behavioral
wrote: I can’t stress enough that we want to learn, issue (Fredricks et al., 2004), with the responsibil-
but the focus at our school is not on knowledge ity for engagement resting solely on the individual
nearly as much as it is on letter grades. Students student. However, research focused on student
feel and resist the intense focus on measurement perspectives indicates that the school and the
and outcomes at the expense of learning and school community – including the people, pro-
knowledge: I hate school because I love learning. grams, and practices – play important roles in
All they ever want is work. They don’t care if you engaging or disengaging students. For example,
learn…Just get good grades and make the school while school leaders often point to factors external
look good. These perspectives on the purpose of to the school when identifying reasons why stu-
going to school and the kinds of work that stu- dents disengage (Bridgeland, DiIulio, & Balfanz,
dents experience are expressed often within stu- 2009), dropout research finds that, from the stu-
dents’ responses to this question on the survey. dent perspective, many of the factors that lead stu-
When students feel that they are being taken dents to disengage from school, either temporarily
seriously and are included in conversations where or permanently, are within the purview of the
they are viewed as knowledgeable stakeholders school leader (Bridgeland et al., 2006).
in their school, they feel empowered (Hudson- The placement of responsibility for engage-
Ross, Cleary, & Casey, 1993). This empowerment ment on individual students has limited the
can be achieved by listening and responding to opportunities for understanding and strengthen-
what students have to share about their high ing student engagement, in large part by focusing
school experiences. For example, a school could on student behaviors separate and apart from stu-
begin by looking at an actual open-response state- dent experience or school factors. While narrow-
ment such as: I find that some of my teachers do ing the focus creates a manageable body of data,
the same exact thing every day. This makes class many school-based variables associated with
very boring. School needs to be more interesting. engagement – such as the structural and regula-
This could begin school conversations about what tory environment of the school (Finn & Voelkl,
makes school “boring” and what would make 1993), the role of teachers (Reeve, Jang, Carrell,
school more “interesting” to students. The school Jeon, & Barch, 2004), and the interaction among
could then look at the overall results from the school people, structures, and relationships
question about how often in the last school year (Johnson et al., 2001) – tend to be overlooked.
students have “made a class presentation” or Schools address student engagement, and use
“discussed questions in class that have no clear data from the HSSSE, in a variety of ways based
answers.” Schools could also examine the peda- on their purpose, needs, and context (Yazzie-
gogy questions regarding what kinds of work Mintz, 2010). For example, the Chesterfield
excite and/or engage students. Data from both the County Public Schools is a high-achieving district
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 757

in Virginia (a “banner district” in the state) that the district, and research on the connection
has recently incorporated student engagement and between time and achievement. However, what
HSSSE into their 6-year strategic plan for improve- was not accounted for was how the students
ment. The district asserts a connection between experienced the outcomes of these decisions. In
engaging students and improving achievement, fact, what worked structurally for the adults was
identifying students who are struggling academi- actually disengaging for the students.
cally to provide support and working to make stu- The principal, as a result of analysis of HSSSE
dents feel part of more individualized learning data, now asserts that “engagement will drive
environments. One of the high schools in the dis- structures.” Yorkville is moving to a process of
trict, James River High School, has been particu- decision-making that includes the perspectives of
larly aggressive about using their HSSSE data. students, transitioning from a philosophy of “If
The staff, through analysis of their data, identified we teach it, they will learn it” to a process of ask-
two specific issues: (1) providing individual sup- ing questions such as: “Are the students learning
port to students who were struggling academically it?” “How are we teaching it?” “How are students
and (2) reaching out to make stronger connections experiencing what we are teaching?” Yorkville,
with students of color, who were a minority in the in exploring academic issues, has decided that
school and were thought to be largely disengaged. engagement is the area where they will focus
The school, rather than put the responsibility on their most intense efforts; they have discovered
the students to address these issues of engage- that strengthening student engagement is their
ment, put into place a set of mentoring and reme- path to making a school with high graduation
diation programs built on the idea that relationships rates into a school where students are engaged in
and academics work together. The results are that learning all the way through and in which the stu-
students previously struggling at James River are dents are participating members of the school
now getting back to the level of their original community in both words and actuality.
classes, and, on the HSSSE survey, students are A rough sketch of a continuum can be drawn,
reporting greater levels of engagement and con- in which schools are situated based on how they
nection to the school community. use student viewpoints, from not listening to stu-
At Yorkville High School in Illinois, high dents, to listening but not taking action, to listen-
graduation and college-going rates masked ing and taking action to change and improve
some internal signs of academic concern for the schools. “Tech High School” has solicited stu-
school – in particular, course failure rates among dents’ perspectives but is not yet at the point to
ninth graders and stagnant ACT scores. The prin- believe students enough to analyze the data and
cipal, familiar with HSSSE from another high take action. Both James River High School and
school, decided to investigate student engage- Yorkville High School have moved across the
ment within the school, though it was entirely continuum to a place where they are listening to
unclear to him what they would discover from students and taking action, and seeing the result-
the data relative to the academic issues they were ing positive changes.
facing. Getting students’ perspectives about their
experiences from the HSSSE survey – including
their beliefs about support, challenge, priorities, Finding the Humanity in the Data
and structures – led to a shift in thinking at
Yorkville. As is the case within many high Listening to students about their schooling experi-
schools, adult needs drove policy and structure ences is not a new concept, just an infrequently
decisions at Yorkville: scheduling, space, profes- invoked one. The importance of student data in
sional development, and discipline. For example, investigating and evaluating schooling is, in fact, a
scheduling decisions – including whether or not cornerstone of research, policy, and practice today.
to move to some form of block scheduling – were However, the kinds of data that researchers, poli-
driven by issues of space availability, growth in cymakers, and school administrators are interested
758 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

in for analysis and evaluation represent a very nar- habits and processes – to the next stage of school-
row band of the student experience in school. ing or work.
The current input–output model of schooling – One of the critiques of student perception data,
largely analogous to assembly line production in raised often in the form of a question, is: Can we
which materials and parts are assembled to pro- really trust the students? In truth, researchers,
duce identical products over and over again – policymakers, and administrators trust students
puts the focus on only those factors that are all the time – as long as the data come from a
directly associated with a countable output mea- standardized test, a transcript, or another “verifi-
sure of achievement (standardized test scores, able” quantitative measure. When the data exist
graduation rates, etc.). What about the processes, in the form of student perceptions or student per-
interactions, and relationships that are important spectives, even if the instrument or method used
parts of students’ schooling experiences as stu- to gather the data is “valid” and “reliable,” adults
dents move from being “inputs” to “outputs”? do not often trust what students say. Why do
What about the consideration of other mea- adults trust student performance on assessments
sures of achievement, success, and output? What but not their perspectives on their own school
about the differential ways in which students experiences? In fact, understanding student per-
experience schooling? Unlike the assembly line, spectives on their school experiences can lead to
identical schooling processes can create very dif- changes in school processes (including teaching
ferent products, given the human factor: students and learning) that may ultimately have a positive
experience the same processes differently, adults effect on the holy grail of success in today’s
do not mechanically teach and interact with every schooling context: quantifiably measured student
student in exactly the same way, and engagement achievement.
is a complex process that does not happen the The data that are given primacy in the current
same way every time and with every person. policy and evaluation context have three basic
Contrary to much popular criticism of schooling characteristics: the data are quantitative, the data
today, this is a good thing. are standardized across populations, and the data
The input-output model attempts to create describe only a very narrow slice of students’
identical products quantitatively described: that schooling experiences. And students know this.
is, students who have achieved a certain score on The student quoted at the beginning of this chap-
a standardized test or set of tests, students who ter echoes the views of many other students:
have achieved an agreed-upon number of course I feel the only thing they care about are scores on
credits, and/or students who have acquired a par- standardized tests, not us students as individuals.
ticular grade point average. The data required to In a system guided by a policy titled No Child
measure these types of achievement do not pro- Left Behind, a phrase focused on individual stu-
vide information on how much students have dents, these students are pointing out a serious
learned or the kinds of learning they have done, contradiction at the root of the system.
the depth with which students have engaged in This chapter argues for finding the humanity in
their learning, the causes of or processes by which the data, focusing on who the students are who
students have come to reach or not reach particu- constitute the quantitative data that dominate the
lar levels of achievement, or what adults might policy and popular conversation around schooling
do differently to help students learn more, achieve in the USA today. While quantifiable data (includ-
greater, or experience school in a more engaged ing data on student engagement) describe a
way. Though these data are used to declare particular slice of the student experience, we
whether or not a student is academically ready to argue for understanding and investigating student
leave one level of schooling and move to the engagement and disengagement as a function of
next, these data do not provide any indication of the perceptions of students about their experiences
what the student is actually taking – cognitively, in the learning environment. Student perspectives
intellectually, substantively, or in terms of work provide a different view of schools from what
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 759

other data show, and a variety of types of data on accomplish specified goals. Student engagement,
student perspectives – including traditional mul- though, as we argued earlier, is a cultural aspect
tiple-option survey questions and open-response of schools, rather than an issue that can be ade-
questions – deepen our understanding of the stu- quately addressed solely through policy or struc-
dent experience. As Yorkville High School came tures. For example, raising high school graduation
to realize, the key question was not just focused rates and lowering dropout rates have been domi-
on whether teachers were teaching the content, nant issues in education policy in recent years.
but whether the students were learning the con- Policymakers and implementers have most fre-
tent and how the students were experiencing quently attempted to deal with this problem
the instruction; the way to collect data on these through structures and directives: punishment for
questions is to target the students as research dropouts (such as preventing dropouts from
participants. obtaining driver licenses), programs (such as
Finding the humanity in the data presents credit recovery, alternative paths to attaining a
challenges for research, policy, and practice. high school diploma, or reducing the number of
The challenges for research include expanding credits needed to graduate), and new organiza-
the definitions and understandings of student tions of schooling (early college and dual high
engagement, moving beyond traditional indica- school/college credit programs). While imple-
tors and measures of engagement (including time mentation of these policies may have the effect of
on task, internal motivation, and attendance/ getting some more students to graduate, thus
truancy), and investigating student engagement accomplishing the paired goals of raising gradua-
from the perspective of students themselves. The tion rates and lowering dropout rates, by and
current model of research that narrowly draws a large, they do not take into account or address the
direct line from input to output needs to be altered reasons that students opt to disengage from
and expanded. For example, lower attendance in school in the first place; they attempt to change
school is associated with low student engagement an outcome rather than address what is often a
and low student achievement (Morse, Anderson, very individual – and human – decision on the
Christenson, & Lehr, 2004; Willms, 2003). By part of students. Finding the humanity in the data
logic and analysis, data indicate that an important means that policy will need to understand why
intervention will be one that gets students to students disengage and focus on the culture of
attend school more frequently; however, if the engagement instead of creating policies and
only goal is to get students into school, then the structures designed merely to drag students across
effect of the intervention may be marginal at best. the high school graduation line. Addressing stu-
Finding the humanity in the data involves dig- dents’ reasons for disengagement, including rel-
ging deeper into the data: finding out from stu- evance, interest, challenge, and instructional
dents what causes them to attend school method – and the processes by which students
infrequently if at all, addressing those factors disengage – is likely to have a greater impact on
through school-based programs and relation- the graduation/dropout problem than any isolated
ships, examining and improving in-school factors policy or structure.
that may lead to low student attendance, and fol- The challenges for practice are illuminated by
lowing up to see if the inputs (attendance) have the varying degrees of effort and success of the
been accompanied by strengthening of the school three high schools highlighted in this chapter –
factors and processes. Focusing solely on the “Tech High School,” Yorkville High School, and
direct line from input to output may create higher James River High School – in hearing, under-
student attendance but no greater engagement or standing, and acting on students’ perspectives as
achievement. part of school improvement initiatives. Whether
The challenges for policy are related to those by design or evolution, schools have been created
for research. Policy creation and implementation largely as a culture of adult needs: adult expertise,
is designed to create structures and directives that adult schedules, adult structures and programs.
760 E. Yazzie-Mintz and K. McCormick

The greatest shift for schools to make in finding and principals on the high school dropout problem.
the humanity in the data is to view schools through Washington, DC: Civic Enterprises.
Bridgeland, J. M., DiIulio, J. J., Jr., & Morison, K. B.
the culture and perspectives of students. Yorkville (2006). The silent epidemic: Perspectives of high
High School, for example, as a result of focusing school dropouts. Washington, DC: Civic Enterprises.
on engagement and data in this way, has shifted Cohen, J., McCabe, M. E., Michelli, N. M., & Pickeral, T.
the foundation and purpose of their work. A pro- (2009). School climate: Research, policy, practice, and
teacher education. Teachers College Record, 111(1),
ductive way of understanding the challenges for 180–213.
practice of finding the humanity in the data is to Cook-Sather, A., & Shultz, J. (2001). Starting where the
view schools as sitting along a continuum of the learner is: Listening to students. In J. Shultz & A.
ways in which they look for and incorporate stu- Cook-Sather (Eds.), In our own words: Students’ per-
spectives on school (pp. 1–17). Lanham, MD: Rowman
dent perspectives into the work of the school, & Littlefield.
from fully adult-focused on one end, to listening Cook-Sather, A. (2002). Authorizing students’ perspec-
to students but not taking action in the middle, to tives: Toward trust, dialogue, and changes in educa-
listening to students and acting on student per- tion. Educational Researcher, 31(4), 3–14.
DeCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal
spectives on the other end. affective determinants of behavior. New York:
What is the truth about the student experience? Academic Press.
Ultimately, data from the HSSSE– both the Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
multiple-option questions and the open responses – Educational Research, 59(2), 117–142.
Finn, J. D. (1993). School engagement and students at risk.
indicate that students want to be interacted with, Washington, DC: National Center for Educational
challenged, cared for, and valued. The challenge for Statistics, U.S. Department of Education (NCES 93 470).
us adults – researchers, policymakers and policy Finn, J. D., & Voelkl, K. E. (1993). Social characteristics
implementers, teachers, administrators, youth- related to school engagement. The Journal of Negro
Education, 62, 249–268.
serving workers – is to create learning environments Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
in which students experience exactly those four School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of
things. That will provide the foundation for the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1),
engagement and point the way toward all types of 59–109.
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a
stronger outcome measures. The first step is to factor in children’s academic engagement and perfor-
look for and find the humanity in the data. mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1),
148–162.
Geertz, C. (1973). Thick description: Toward an interpre-
tive theory of culture. New York: Basic Books.
References Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994).
Role of parental motivational practices in children’s
Appleton, J. A., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. academic intrinsic motivation and achievement.
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con- Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 104–113.
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Gottfried, A. E., & Gottfried, A. W. (1996). A longitudi-
Psychology in the Schools, 45(5), 369–386. nal study of academic intrinsic motivation in intellec-
Appleton, J. A., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. tually gifted children: Childhood through early
L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological adolescence. Gifted Child Quarterly, 40, 179–183.
engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement Harter, S. (1992). The relationship between perceived
Instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445. competence, affect, and motivational orientation
Attard, C. (2009, November). Student perspectives of within the classroom: Process and patterns of change.
mathematics teaching and learning in the upper pri- In A. Boggiano & T. Pittman (Eds.), Achievement and
mary classroom. Paper presented at the annual meet- motivation: A social-developmental perspective (pp.
ing of the International Conference on Science and 77–114). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Mathematics Education, Penang, Malaysia. Harter, S., & Jackson, B. K. (1992). Trait vs. nontrait con-
Attard, C. (2010, July). Students’ experiences of mathe- ceptualizations of intrinsic/extrinsic motivational ori-
matics during the transition from primary to second- entation. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 209–230.
ary school. Paper presented at the annual meeting of Hudson-Ross, S., Cleary, L. M., & Casey, M. (Eds.).
the Mathematics Education Research Group of (1993). Children’s voices: Children talk about literacy.
Australasia, Fremantle, Western Australia. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Bridgeland, J. M., DiIulio, J. J., Jr., & Balfanz, R. (2009). Jimerson, S. R., Campos, E., & Greif, J. L. (2003). Toward
On the frontlines of schools: Perspectives of teachers an understanding of definitions and measures of school
36 Finding the Humanity in the Data… 761

engagement and related terms. California School Munns, G., & Woodward, H. (2006). Student engagement
Psychologist, 8, 7–27. and student self-assessment: The REAL framework.
Johnson, M. K., Crosnoe, R., & Elder, G. H. (2001). Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy and
Students’ attachment and engagement: The role of race Practice, 13(2), 193–213.
and ethnicity. Sociology of Education, 74, 318–340. Newmann, F. M., Wehlage, G. G., & Lamborn, S. D.
Johnson, M. S., & Dean, M. (2011). Student engagement (1992). The significance and sources of student
and International Baccalaureate: Measuring the engagement. In F. M. Newmann (Ed.), Student engage-
social, emotional, and academic engagement of IB ment and achievement in American secondary schools
students. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the (pp. 11–39). New York: Teachers College Press.
American Educational Research Association. New Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J.
Orleans, LA. (2004). Enhancing student’s engagement by increas-
Jordan, W., & Nettles, S. (1999). How students invest their ing teacher’s autonomy support. Motivation and
time out of school: Effects on school engagement, per- Emotion, 28(2), 147–169.
ceptions of life chances, and achievement (Report No. Senge, P. M. (2000). Systems change in education.
29). Baltimore, MD: Center for Research on the Reflections, 1(3), 52–60.
Education of Students Placed At Risk (ERIC Document Shernoff, D. J., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Schneider, B., &
Reproduction Service No. ED 428 174). Shernoff, E. S. (2003). Student engagement in high
Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships mat- school classrooms from the perspective of flow theory.
ter: Linking teacher support to student engagement School Psychology Quarterly, 18(2), 158–176.
and achievement. Journal of School Health, 74(7), Skinner, E. A., Wellborn, J. G., & Connell, J. P. (1990).
262–273. What it takes to do well in school and whether I’ve got
Kuh, G. (2003). The National Survey of Student it: A process model of perceived control and children’s
Engagement: Conceptual framework and overview of engagement and achievement in school. Journal of
psychometric properties. Bloomington, IN: Center for Educational Psychology, 82(1), 22–32.
Postsecondary Research. Stillwell, R. (2010). Public school graduates and dropouts
Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Lyengar, S. S. (2005). from the common core of data: School year 2007–08
Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the (NCES 2010–341). Washington, DC: National Center
classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. for Education Statistics, Institute of Education
Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(2), 184–196. Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved
Lepper, M. R., & Green, D. (1978). The hidden costs of June 4, 2010, from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/
reward. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2010341
Lepper, M. R., Sethi, S., Dialdin, D., & Drake, M. (1997). Sum, A., Khatiwada, I., McLaughlin, J., with Palma, S.
Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: A developmental (2009). The consequences of dropping out of high
perspective. In S. S. Luthar, J. A. Burack, D. Cicchetti, school: Joblessness and jailing for high school dropouts
& J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Developmental psychopathol- and the high cost for taxpayers. Boston, MA: Center for
ogy: Perspectives on adjustment, risk, and disorder Labor Market Studies, Northeastern University.
(pp. 23–50). New York: Cambridge University Press. Waller, W. (1932). The sociology of teaching. New York:
Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2003). The role of Russell & Russell.
self-efficacy beliefs in student engagement and learn- Willms, J. D. (2003). Student engagement at school: A sense
ing in the classroom. Reading and Writing Quarterly: of belonging and participation. Paris, France: Organisation
Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 19, 119–137. for Economic Co-Operation and Development.
Mant, J., Wilson, H., & Coates, D. (2007, July). The effect Yazzie-Mintz, E. (2007). Voices of students on engage-
of increasing conceptual challenge in primary science ment: A report on the 2006 High School Survey of
lessons on pupils’ achievement and engagement. Student Engagement. Bloomington, IN: Center for
International Journal of Science Education, 29(14), Evaluation & Education Policy.
1707–1719. Yazzie-Mintz, E. (2009). Engaging the voices of students:
Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional A report on the 2007 & 2008 High School Survey of
activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high Student Engagement. Bloomington, IN: Center for
school years. American Educational Research Journal, Evaluation & Education Policy.
37(1), 153–184. Yazzie-Mintz, E. (2010). Charting the path from engage-
Morse, A. B., Anderson, A. R., Christenson, S. L., & ment to achievement: A report on the 2009 High
Lehr, C. A. (2004, February). Promoting school com- School Survey of Student Engagement. Bloomington,
pletion. Principal Leadership Magazine, 4(5), 9–13. IN: Center for Evaluation & Education Policy.
The Measurement of Student
Engagement: A Comparative 37
Analysis of Various Methods
and Student Self-report Instruments

Jennifer A. Fredricks and Wendy McColskey

Abstract
One of the challenges with research on student engagement is the large
variation in the measurement of this construct, which has made it chal-
lenging to compare findings across studies. This chapter contributes to our
understanding of the measurement of student in engagement in three ways.
First, we describe strengths and limitations of different methods for assess-
ing student engagement (i.e., self-report measures, experience sampling
techniques, teacher ratings, interviews, and observations). Second, we
compare and contrast 11 self-report survey measures of student engage-
ment that have been used in prior research. Across these 11 measures, we
describe what is measured (scale name and items), use of measure, sam-
ples, and the extent of reliability and validity information available on
each measure. Finally, we outline limitations with current approaches to
measurement and promising future directions.

Researchers, educators, and policymakers are (i.e., uninvolved, apathetic, not trying very hard,
increasingly focused on student engagement as and not paying attention) (Steinberg, Brown, &
the key to addressing problems of low achieve- Dornbush, 1996; Yazzie-Mintz, 2007). The con-
ment, high levels of student boredom, alienation, sequences of disengagement for middle and high
and high dropout rates (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & school youth from disadvantaged backgrounds
Paris, 2004). Students become more disengaged are especially severe; these youth are less likely
as they progress from elementary to middle to graduate from high school and face limited
school, with some estimates that 25–40% of employment prospects, increasing their risk for
youth are showing signs of disengagement poverty, poorer health, and involvement in the
criminal justice system (National Research
J.A. Fredricks, Ph.D. (*) Council and the Institute of Medicine, 2004).
Human Development, Connecticut College, Although there is growing interest in student
New London, CT, USA
e-mail: jfred@conncoll.edu
engagement, there has been considerable varia-
tion in how this construct has been conceptual-
W. McColskey, Ph.D.
SERVE Center, University of North Carolina,
ized over time (Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong,
Greensboro, NC, USA 2008; Fredricks et al., 2004; Jimerson, Campos,
e-mail: wmccolsk@serve.org & Grief, 2003). Scholars have used a broad range

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 763


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_37, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
764 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

of terms including student engagement, school in prior research are compared and contrasted on
engagement, student engagement in school, aca- several dimensions (i.e., what is measured, pur-
demic engagement, engagement in class, and poses and uses, samples, and psychometric prop-
engagement in schoolwork. In addition, there has erties). Finally, we discuss limitations with
been variation in the number of subcomponents current approaches to measurement.
of engagement including different conceptuali-
zations. Some scholars have proposed a
two-dimensional model of engagement which What is Student Engagement
includes behavior (e.g., participation, effort, and
positive conduct) and emotion (e.g., interest, We define student engagement as a meta-construct
belonging, value, and positive emotions) (Finn, that includes behavioral, emotional, and cognitive
1989; Marks, 2000; Skinner, Kindermann, & engagement (Fredricks et al., 2004). Although
Furrer, 2009b). More recently, others have out- there are large individual bodies of literature on
lined a three-component model of engagement behavioral (i.e., time on task), emotional (i.e.,
that includes behavior, emotion, and a cognitive interest and value), and cognitive engagement
dimension (i.e., self-regulation, investment in (i.e., self-regulation and learning strategies), what
learning, and strategy use) (e.g., Archaumbault, makes engagement unique is its potential as a mul-
2009; Fredricks et al., 2004; Jimerson et al., 2003; tidimensional or “meta”-construct that includes
Wigfield et al., 2008). Finally, Christenson and these three dimensions. Behavioral engagement
her colleagues (Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & draws on the idea of participation and includes
Reschly, 2006; Reschly & Christenson, 2006) involvement in academic, social, or extracurricu-
conceptualized engagement as having four lar activities and is considered crucial for achiev-
dimensions: academic, behavioral, cognitive, and ing positive academic outcomes and preventing
psychological (subsequently referred to as affec- dropping out (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Finn,
tive) engagement. In this model, aspects of behav- 1989). Other scholars define behavioral engage-
ior are separated into two different components: ment in terms of positive conduct, such as follow-
academics, which includes time on task, credits ing the rules, adhering to classroom norms, and the
earned, and homework completion, and behavior, absence of disruptive behavior such as skipping
which includes attendance, class participation, school or getting into trouble (Finn, Pannozzo, &
and extracurricular participation. One common- Voelkl, 1995; Finn & Rock, 1997). Emotional
ality across the myriad of conceptualizations is engagement focuses on the extent of positive (and
that engagement is multidimensional. However, negative) reactions to teachers, classmates, aca-
further theoretical and empirical work is needed demics, or school. Others conceptualize emotional
to determine the extent to which these different engagement as identification with the school,
dimensions are unique constructs and whether a which includes belonging, or a feeling of being
three or four component model more accurately important to the school, and valuing, or an appre-
describes the construct of student engagement. ciation of success in school-related outcomes
Even when scholars have similar conceptual- (Finn, 1989; Voelkl, 1997). Positive emotional
izations of engagement, there has been consider- engagement is presumed to create student ties to
able variability in the content of items used in the institution and influence their willingness to do
instruments. This has made it challenging to the work (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Finn, 1989).
compare findings from different studies. This Finally, cognitive engagement is defined as stu-
chapter expands on our understanding of the dent’s level of investment in learning. It includes
measurement of student engagement in three being thoughtful, strategic, and willing to exert the
ways. First, the strengths and limitations of dif- necessary effort for comprehension of complex
ferent methods for assessing student engagement ideas or mastery of difficult skills (Corno &
are described. Second, 11 self-report survey mea- Mandinach, 1983; Fredricks et al., 2004; Meece,
sures of student engagement that have been used Blumenfeld, & Hoyle, 1988).
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 765

An important question is how engagement dif- they can achieve desired ends (Fredricks et al.,
fers from motivation. Although the terms are used 2004). In contrast, if students experience schools
interchangeably by some, they are different and as uncaring, coercive, and unfair, they will become
the distinctions between them are important. disengaged or disaffected (Skinner et al., 2009a,
Motivation refers to the underlying reasons for a 2009b). This model assumes that motivation is a
given behavior and can be conceptualized in terms necessary but not sufficient precursor to engage-
of the direction, intensity, quality, and persistence ment (Appleton et al., 2008; Connell & Wellborn,
of one’s energies (Maehr & Meyer, 1997). A pro- 1991).
liferation of motivational constructs (e.g., intrin-
sic motivation, goal theory, and expectancy-value
models) have been developed to answer two broad Methods for Studying Engagement
questions “Can I do this task” and “Do I want to
do this task and why?” (Eccles, Wigfield, & Student Self-report
Schiefele, 1998). One commonality across these
different motivational constructs is an emphasis Self-report survey measures are the most com-
on individual differences and underlying psycho- mon method for assessing student engagement. In
logical processes. In contrast, engagement tends this methodology, students are provided items
to be thought of in terms of action, or the behav- reflecting various aspects of engagement and
ioral, emotional, and cognitive manifestations of select the response that best describes them. The
motivation (Skinner, Kindermann, Connell, & majority of these self-report engagement mea-
Wellborn, 2009a). An additional difference is that sures are general and not subject specific, though
engagement reflects an individual’s interaction there are some examples of measures that assess
with context (Fredricks et al., 2004; Russell, engagement in a specific domain like math (Kong,
Ainsley, & Frydenberg, 2005). In other words, an Wong, & Lam, 2003) or reading (Wigfield et al.,
individual is engaged in something (i.e., task, 2008). One of the arguments for using self-report
activity, and relationship), and their engagement methods is that it is critical to collect data on stu-
cannot be separated from their environment. This dents’ subjective perceptions, as opposed to just
means that engagement is malleable and is respon- collecting objective data on behavioral indicators
sive to variations in the context that schools such as attendance or homework completion rates,
can target in interventions (Fredricks et al., 2004; which are already commonly collected by schools
Newmann, Wehlage, & Lamborn, 1992). (Appleton et al., 2006; Garcia & Pintrich, 1996).
The self-system model of motivational devel- Self-report methods are particularly useful for
opment (Connell, 1990; Connell & Wellborn, assessing emotional and cognitive engagement
1991; Deci & Ryan, 1985) provides one theoreti- which are not directly observable and need to be
cal model for studying motivation and engage- inferred from behaviors. In fact, Appleton et al.
ment. This model is based on the assumption that (2006) argue that self-report methods should only
individuals have three fundamental motivational be used to assess emotional and cognitive engage-
needs: autonomy, competence, and relatedness. If ment because collecting data on these subtypes
schools provide children with opportunities to through other methods, such as observations and
meet these three needs, students will be more teacher rating scales, is highly inferential.
engaged. Students’ need for relatedness is more Self-report methods are widely used because
likely to occur in classrooms where teachers and they are often the most practical and easy to
peers create a caring and supportive environment; administer in classroom settings. They can be
their need for autonomy is met when they feel like given to large and diverse samples of children at
they have a choice and when they are motivated a relatively low cost, making it possible to gather
by internal rather than external factors; and their data over several waves and compare results
need for competence is met when they experience across schools. However, one concern with self-
the classroom as optimal in structure and feel like report measures is that students may not answer
766 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

honestly under some conditions (e.g., if adminis- Teacher Ratings of Students


tered by their teacher with no anonymity pro-
vided), and thus, self-reports may not reflect their Another method for assessing student engage-
actual behaviors or strategy use (Appleton et al., ment is teacher checklists or rating scales. Teacher
2006; Garcia & Pintrich, 1996). Furthermore, ratings of individual students’ engagement, when
these measures generally contain items that are averaged across students in their classrooms,
worded broadly (e.g., I work hard in school) offer an alternative perspective on student
rather than worded to reflect engagement in par- engagement from that reported by the students
ticular tasks and situations. For researchers inter- themselves. Some teacher rating scales include
ested in studying how much engagement varies items assessing both behavioral and emotional
as a function of contextual factors, the general engagement (Skinner & Belmont, 1993), and
items may not be appropriate. others reflect a multidimensional model of
engagement (i.e., behavioral, emotional, and cog-
nitive) (Wigfield et al., 2008). Researchers have
Experience Sampling also developed teacher ratings of student partici-
pation as indicative of behavioral engagement
Experience sampling (ESM) is another technique (Finn, Folger, & Cox, 1991; Finn et al., 1995),
that has been used to assess student engagement and teacher ratings of adjustment to school, as
in the classroom (Shernoff, Csikszentmihalyi, indicative of engagement (Birch & Ladd, 1997;
Schneider, & Shernoff, 2003; Shernoff & Schmidt, Buhs & Ladd, 2001). This methodology can be
2008; Uekawa, Borman, & Lee, 2007; Yair, 2000). particularly useful for studies with younger chil-
ESM methods grew out of research on “flow,” a dren who have more difficulty completing self-
high level of engagement where individuals are so report instruments due to the reading demands
deeply absorbed in a task that they lose awareness and limited literacy skills. Some studies have
of time and space (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). In included both teacher ratings and students’ self-
this methodology, individuals carry electronic reports of engagement in order to examine the
pagers or alarm watches for a set time period. In correspondence between the two measurement
response to ESM signals, students fill out a self- techniques (Skinner, Marchand, Furrer, &
report questionnaire with a series of questions Kindermann, 2008; Skinner et al., 2009b).
about their location, activities, and cognitive and These studies show a stronger correlation
affective responses (see Hektner, Schmidt, & between teacher and student reports of behavioral
Csikszentmihalyi, 2007, for more description of engagement than teacher and student reports of
ESM methods). This methodology allows emotional engagement. This finding is not sur-
researchers to collect detailed data on engage- prising as behavioral indicators are directly
ment in the moment rather than retrospectively observable. In contrast, emotional indicators need
(as with student self-report), which reduces prob- to be inferred from behavior, and it is possible
lems with recall failure and the desire to answer that some students have learned to mask their
in socially desirable ways (Hektner et al., 2007). emotions (Skinner et al., 2008).
This technique can be used to collect information
on variations in engagement across time and situ-
ations. However, this methodology also has some Interviews
limitations. ESM methods require a large time
investment for respondents, and the success of the A few studies have used interview techniques to
method depends largely on participants’ ability assess engagement in school (Blumenfeld et al.,
and willingness to comply. In addition, engage- 2005; Conchas, 2001; Locke Davidson, 1996).
ment is a multifaceted construct and may not be Interviews fall on a continuum from structured
adequately captured by the small number of items and semistructured interviews with predesignated
included in ESM studies. questions to interviews where participants are
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 767

asked to tell their stories in more open-ended and work, seatwork) to get an accurate picture of stu-
unstructured ways (Turner & Meyer, 2000). One dent behavior. There are also concerns about the
benefit of interview methods is they can provide reliability of observational methods without
insight into the reasons for variability in levels of proper training. Finally, another potential prob-
engagement to help understand why some stu- lem with individual observational measures is
dents do engage while others begin to withdraw they provide limited information on the quality
from school. Interviews can provide a detailed of effort, participation, or thinking (Fredricks
descriptive account of how students construct et al., 2004; Peterson, Swing, Su, & Wass, 1984).
meaning about their school experiences, which For example, Peterson and colleagues found that
contextual factors are most salient, and how these some students judged to be on-task by observers
experiences relate to engagement (Blumenfeld reported in subsequent interviews that they were
et al.). However, interviews are not without prob- not thinking about the material while being
lems. The knowledge, skills, and biases of the observed. In contrast, many of the students who
interviewer can all impact on the quality, depth, appeared to be off-task reported actually being
and type of responses. There are also questions very highly cognitively engaged.
about the reliability (stability and consistency) Rather than assessing engagement with pre-
and validity of interview findings (McCaslin & specified coding categories, other studies have
Good, 1996). Finally, concerns about social desir- used narrative and descriptive techniques to mea-
ability are an issue with interview techniques. sure this construct. For example, Nystrand and
colleagues (Gamoran & Nystrand, 1992; Nystrand
& Gamoran, 1991; Nystrand, Wu, Gamaron,
Observations Zeiser, & Long, 2001) assessed the quality of
instructional discourse in the classroom as an
Observational methods at both the individual and indicator of substantive engagement, defined as a
classroom level have also been used to measure sustained commitment to the content of school-
engagement. At the individual level, observa- ing. In these studies, the frequency of high-level
tional measures have been developed to assess evaluation questions, authentic questions, and
individual students’ on and off task behavior as uptake (i.e., evidence that teachers incorporate
an indicator of academic engagement (Volpe, students’ answers into subsequent questions) was
DiPerna, Hintze, & Shapiro, 2005). Academic observed as indicative of substantive engagement.
engagement refers to a composite of academic That is, these teacher behaviors were assumed to
behaviors such as reading aloud, writing, answer- involve active student engagement. Furthermore,
ing questions, participating in classroom tasks, Helme and Clarke (2001) observed math classes
and talking about academics (Greenwood, for indicators of cognitive engagement such as
Horton, & Utley, 2002). These measures use a self-monitoring, exchanging ideas, giving direc-
form of momentary time sampling, in which an tions, and justifying answers. Finally, Lee and her
observer records whether a predetermined cate- colleagues used observational techniques to exam-
gory of behavior is present or absent for an indi- ine the quality of students’ task engagement when
vidual student during a defined time interval involved in science activities (Lee & Anderson,
(Salvia & Ysseldyke, 2004). In addition to use in 1993; Lee & Brophy, 1996). In these studies, they
research studies, these techniques have been used noted behaviors such as relating the task to prior
by school psychologists to screen individual chil- knowledge, requesting clarification, and using
dren in both typical and special needs popula- analogies as measures of cognitive engagement.
tions, especially those at risk for disengagement The prime advantage of using observation
and academic failure (Shapiro, 2004). techniques to study engagement is that they can
One concern with these types of observations provide detailed and descriptive accounts of the
is that they can be time consuming to administer, contextual factors occurring with higher or lower
and observers may need to collect data across engagement levels. These descriptions enhance
various types of academic settings (i.e., group our understanding of unfolding processes within
768 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

contexts. Observational methods also can be used American’s Families (NSAF)], (5) did not have
to verify information about engagement collected enough published information on the measure,
from survey and interview techniques. The major (6) adapted from other instruments already
disadvantages of observations are that they are included in the list, or (7) developed for use in
labor intensive, and they usually involve only a nonacademic subject areas (e.g., physical educa-
small number of students and contexts. This raises tion). This resulted in a total of 21 measures (14
concerns about the generalizability to other set- self-report, 3 teacher report, and 4 observation
tings. Finally, the quality of descriptive observa- methods) which had been used with upper ele-
tions depends heavily on the skills of the observer mentary to high school years.
and on his or her ability to capture and make sense By way of describing the substantial variation
of what was observed (Turner & Meyer, 2000). that exists across engagement measures, in this
chapter, we describe 11 of these self-report mea-
sures. The 11 self-report measures in this chapter
Comparison of Self-report Measures are for illustrative purposes and should not be
considered an exhaustive list but rather are
In the next section, we describe survey measures included to show the types of self-report instru-
that have been developed and used in prior ments available. We compared these 11 self-
research on student engagement and compare report surveys on several dimensions including:
these surveys on several dimensions. This chap- definition of engagement, usage, samples, and
ter builds on a literature review conducted to psychometric information. The self-report mea-
identify measures of student engagement avail- sures ranged in length from a 4-item scale [School
able for use in the upper elementary through high Engagement Scale Questionnaire (SEQ)] to the
school years (Fredricks & McColskey, 2010). We High School Survey of Student Engagement
focus on student self-report measures because (HSSSE), a broad 121-item questionnaire. In
this is the most common method for assessing some cases, the engagement items are a subset of
engagement and most likely to be of interest to a longer self-report instrument that assesses con-
researchers. As a first step toward identifying stu- structs other than student engagement.
dent engagement instruments, a literature search Table 37.1 lists the names of the 11 self-report
was conducted by members of the research team measures and the availability of the measure (i.e.,
using terms that were broad enough to capture journal article, website, and contact person). Eight
both subject-specific and general measures of measures are either available in a published
student engagement. The search was restricted to source, can be accessed online, or are available by
studies published between 1979 (which was contacting the developer. Three of the instruments
selected to predate the earliest emergence of have commercially available services for pur-
engagement studies in the early 1980s) and May chase (School Success Profile [SSP], High School
2009 and resulted in 1,314 citations. Survey of Student Engagement [HSSSE], and the
The research team systematically reviewed Motivation and Engagement in Schools Scale
the 1,314 citations to identify named instruments [MES]). This cost covers questionnaire materials,
used to measure student engagement. A total of administration of surveys, data preparation, indi-
156 instruments were identified from the cita- vidual and school reports, and other technical
tions. From this initial list of 156 instruments, we assistance related to the use of the information.
excluded measures for a variety of reasons includ-
ing (1) developed and used only with college age
samples, (2) used only with special education What Is Measured
populations, (3) measured a construct other than
engagement (e.g., school bonding, attitudes Table 37.2 lists the student self-report measures,
toward school), (4) based on items from a larger the subscales/domains measured, and sample
national dataset [e.g., National Education items for each of the subscales. Some of these
Longitudinal Study (NELS), National Survey of survey instruments were explicitly designed to
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 769

Table 37.1 Overview of 11 instruments [Attitudes Toward Math (ATM), Engagement vs.
Instrument name Availability Disaffection with Learning (EvsD), Motivated
Attitudes Toward Miller, Greene, Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ),
Mathematics Montalvo, Ravindran, and School Engagement Questionnaire (SEQ)].
Survey (ATM) and Nichols (1996) The self-report measures also differ in whether
Engagement vs. Disaffection Skinner, Kindermann, and
and how they conceptualize disengagement. Some
with Learning – Student Furrer (2009b) or www.
Report (EvsD) pdx.edu/psy/ellen-skinner-1 of the measures include subscales that assess the
High School Survey of www.indiana.edu/ opposite of engagement, which has been referred
Student Engagement (HSSSE) ~ceep/hssse/ to as disengagement, disaffection, and alienation
Identification with School Voelkl (1996) (Skinner et al., 2009a, 2009b). For example, three
Questionnaire (ISQ) instruments have subscales measuring the extent
Motivated Strategies Pintrich and
for Learning DeGroot (1990)
of negative engagement (disengagement in the
Questionnaire (MSLQ) MES, trouble avoidance in the SSP, and behav-
Motivation and www.lifelongachievement. ioral disaffection and emotional disaffection in
Engagement Scale (MES) com Engagement vs. Disaffection with Learning).
Research Assessment irre.org/sites/default/files/ Other measures imply that negative engagement
Package for Schools publication_pdfs/RAPS_
is simply a low engagement score indicating a
(RAPS) manual_entire_1998.pdf
School Engagement Measure Fredricks, Blumenfeld,
lack of engagement (Appleton et al., 2006).
(SEM) – MacArthur Friedel, and Paris (2005) Finally, some of the measures blur the lines
School Engagement Scale/ Available by contacting between engagement and contextual precursors
Questionnaire (SEQ) Dr. Steinberg at Temple (e.g., quality of students’ social relationships). For
University example, the three Student Engagement Instrument
School Success Profile (SSP) www.schoolsuccessprofile.
(SEI) “psychological engagement” subscales
org
Student Engagement Appleton et al. (2006) include items about students’ relationships with
Instrument (SEI) teachers and peers and support for learning from
families that are not direct measures of engage-
ment but indirect measures. Other self-report
assess engagement, while other measures were measures include separate scales for the aspects
designed to assess constructs such as identifica- of classroom or school context that are assumed
tion with school, motivation, and self-regulation to influence or be related to engagement (e.g.,
and strategy use, but have been used in subse- Research Assessment Package for Schools).
quent studies as measures of engagement. For Another way to compare the self-report survey
example, the Motivated Strategies for Learning measures is in terms of the extent to which they
Questionnaire (MSLQ) was initially designed to represent the multidimensional nature of engage-
measure self-regulation and strategy use but has ment. Table 37.3 shows the various self-report
been used in some studies as an indicator of cog- measures in terms of whether they reflect behav-
nitive engagement (Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990). ioral, emotional, or cognitive aspects of engage-
Similarly, the Identification with School question- ment. In addition to differences in scale names
naire has been used in some studies as a measure used by developers (see Tables 37.2 and 37.4),
of student identification with school and in other there were differences in how the developers
studies as a measure of emotional engagement. aligned similar items within the behavioral, emo-
There are a variety of ways to compare these tional, and cognitive engagement constructs. For
measures. First, the surveys differ in terms of example, class participation was used as an indica-
whether they focus on general engagement or tor of both behavioral and cognitive engagement,
subject- or class-specific engagement. Seven of and students’ valuing of school was used as an
the measures have items worded to reflect general indicator of both emotional and cognitive engage-
engagement in school, while 4 of the self-report ment. Below we describe the subscales and items
instruments are worded for use at the class level, found across the 11 instruments by behavioral,
in particular classes, or in particular skill areas emotional, and cognitive engagement.
770 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

Table 37.2 Self-report subscales with sample items


Name of measure Subscales Sample items
Attitudes Toward Self-regulation (12 items) “Before a quiz or exam, I plan out how to study the
Mathematics material”
Survey (ATM) Deep cognitive strategy use “I work several examples of the same type of problem
(9 items) when studying mathematics so I can understand the
problems better”
Shallow cognitive strategy use “I find reviewing previously solved problems to be a
(5 items) good way to study for a test”
Persistence (9 items) “If I have trouble understanding a problem, I go over
it again until I understand it”
Engagement vs. Behavioral engagement (5 items) “When I am in class, I listen very carefully”
Disaffection with Behavioral disaffection (5 items) “When I am in class, I just act like I am working”
Learning (EvsD) Emotional engagement (5 items) “I enjoy learning new things in class”
Emotional disaffection (7 items) “When we work on something in class, I feel
discouraged”
High School Survey Cognitive/intellectual/academic Thinking about this school year, how often have you
of Student Engagement engagement (65 items) done each of the following?
(HSSSE) (A) Asked questions in class; (B) contributed to class
discussions; (C) made a class presentation; (D)
prepared a draft of a paper or assignment before
turning it in; (E) received prompt feedback from
teachers on assignments or other class work
Social/behavioral/participatory Thinking about this school year, how often have you
engagement (17 items) done each of the following? (a) had conversations or
worked on a project with at least one student of a race
or ethnicity different from your own; (b) picked on or
bullied another student
Emotional engagement How do you feel about the following statements
(39 items) related to your high school?
Overall, (a) I feel good about being in this school; (b)
I care about this school; (c) I feel safe in this school;
(d) I have a voice in classroom and/or school decisions
Identification with School Belongingness (9 items) “School is one of my favorite places to be”
Questionnaire (ISQ) Valuing of school (7 items) “Most of the things we learn in class are useless”
Motivated Strategy Self-regulation (9 items) “I outline the chapters in my book to help me study”
and Learning Use Cognitive strategy use (13 items) “I ask myself questions to make sure I know the
Questionnaire (MSLQ) material that I have been studying”
Motivation and Self-belief (4 items) “If I try hard I believe I can do my schoolwork well”
Engagement Learning focus (4 items) “I feel very happy with myself when I really
Scale (MES) understand what I am taught at school”
Valuing school (4 items) “Learning at school is important”
Persistence (4 items) “If I cannot understand my schoolwork, I keep trying
until I do”
Planning (4 items) “Before I start a project, I plan out how I am going
to do it”
Study management (4 items) “When I do homework, I usually do it where I can
concentrate best”
Disengagement (4 items) “I have given up being interested in school”
Self-sabotage (4 items) “Sometimes I do not try at school so I can have
reason if I do not do well”
Failure avoidance (4 items) “The main reason I try at school is because I do not
want to disappoint my parents”
Anxiety (4 items) “When I have a project to do, I worry a lot about it”
Uncertain control (4 items) “When I do not do well at school, I do not know how
to stop that happening next time”
(continued)
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 771

Table 37.2 (continued)


Name of measure Subscales Sample items
Research Assessment Ongoing engagement (5 items) “I work hard on my schoolwork”
Package for Schools Reaction to challenge (6 items) “When something bad happens to me in school, I say
(RAPS) the teacher did not cover the things on the test”
School Behavioral engagement (5 items) “I pay attention in class”
Engagement Measure Emotional engagement (6 items) “I am interested in the work at school”
(SEM)-MacArthur Cognitive engagement (8 items) “When I read a book, I ask myself questions to make
sure I understand what it is about”
School Engagement School engagement scale “How much time do you put into homework each
Scale/Questionnaire (4 items in 3 subject areas) week, including reading assignments?”
(SEQ)
School Success Profile School engagement (3 items) “I find school fun and exciting”
(SSP) Trouble avoidance (11 items) “I turned in a homework assignment late or not at all”
Student Engagement Affective engagement: teacher- “Adults at my school listen to the students”
Instrument (SEI) student relationships (9 items)
Affective engagement: peer “I have some friends at school”
support for learning (6 items)
Affective engagement: family “My family/guardian(s) are there for me when
support for learning (4 items) I need them”
Cognitive engagement: control “The tests in my classes do a good job of measuring
and relevance of schoolwork what I am able to do”
(9 items)
Cognitive engagement: future “I am hopeful about my future”
aspirations and goals (5 items)

Table 37.3 Dimensions of engagement assessed by ble avoidance) assess the extent of negative
instruments behavioral engagement (pretending to work, not
Instrument Behavioral Emotional Cognitive turning in homework, and cutting class). A third
Multidimensional self-report instruments subscale (disengagement subscale of the MES)
HSSSE   
includes items assessing both behavioral disen-
MES   
SEM    gagement (e.g., each day I try less and less) and
Bidimensional student self-report instruments emotional disengagement (I have given up being
ATM    interested in school). Across the various behav-
EvsD   ioral engagement scales/subscales, individual
RAPS  
SSP 
items ask students to report on their attention,
SEI  
attendance, time on homework, preparation for
Unidimensional student self-report instruments class, class participation, concentration, partici-
ISQ    pation in school-based activities, effort, adher-
MSLQ ence to classroom rules, and risk behaviors.
SEQ

Emotional Engagement

Behavioral Engagement Eight subscales, either by subscale name or items,


appear to reflect aspects of emotional engagement.
Eight measures have scales that seem to reflect Some subscales assess emotional reaction to class
(either by the subscale name or the sample items) or school, while others assess the quality of stu-
aspects of behavioral engagement (see dents’ relationships with peers and teachers as an
Tables 37.2, 37.3 and 37.4). Two of the behav- indicator of emotional engagement. Two subscales
ioral subscales (behavioral disaffection and trou- (emotional disaffection and disengagement)
772 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

Table 37.4 Scales and subscales by each engagement dimensiona


Behavioral Emotional Cognitive
Instrument subscales/ Behavioral disaffection Anxiety Cognitive engagementc
subscale name Behavioral engagementc Belonging Cognitive/intellectual/
Disengagement Emotional engagementc academic
Persistenceb, c Emotional disaffection Cognitive strategy use
Social/behavioral/ Failure avoidance Deep cognitive strategy use
participatory engagement Affective engagement – Learning focus
School Engagement family support for learning Control and relevance of
Questionnaire Affective engagement – peer schoolwork
Trouble avoidance support for learning Future aspirations and goals
Affective engagement – Planning
teacher-student relationships Self-regulationc
Reaction to challenge Shallow cognitive strategy use
School engagement Study management
Self-belief
Valuingc
Uncertain control
a
Disengagement could also be listed under the Emotional engagement column, as they contain items reflecting both
b
Persistence is also considered an aspect of Cognitive engagement
c
These subscale/scale names were used by more than one instrument

include items assessing the extent of negative engagement incorporate aspects of motivation,
emotions (discouragement when working on self-regulated learning, and strategy use.
something, given up being interested in school).
Overall, emotional engagement scales include
questions about a myriad of topics related to emo- Purposes and Uses
tional reactions to school such as being happy or
anxious; expressing interest and enjoyment; report- The measures included in this chapter were
ing fun and excitement; reacting to failure and developed from a range of disciplinary perspec-
challenge; feeling safe; having supportive or posi- tives and for a variety of purposes. A number of
tive relationships with teachers and peers; having the measures were developed by psychologists
family support for learning; expressing feelings of studying motivation, cognition, and engagement.
belonging; and perceiving school as valuable. For example, one widely used measure, the
Engagement versus Disaffection with Learning
scale, was part of a larger instrument that was ini-
Cognitive Engagement Subscales tially developed to test the self-system model of
student engagement. According to this model, the
Six surveys include subscales measuring cognitive relation between classroom context (i.e., struc-
engagement, though there is large variation in how ture, autonomy support, and involvement) and
this is defined and measured. Cognitive engage- patterns of action (cognitive, behavioral, and
ment is used as a broad umbrella term for (1) emotional engagement) is mediated through self-
beliefs about the importance or value of schooling, system processes (competence, autonomy, and
learning goals, and future aspirations; (2) cognitive relatedness) (Connell, 1990; Connell & Wellborn,
strategy use (how deeply students study material); 1991). The Engagement versus Disaffection scale
(3) self-regulatory or meta-cognitive strategies has been most recently used in research by
(how students manage the learning processes such Skinner and her colleagues (see Furrer & Skinner,
as planning and seeking information); and (4) 2003; Skinner et al., 2008, Skinner et al., 2009b,
doing extra work and going beyond the require- for examples). In 1998, Connell and others at
ments of school. These measures of cognitive the Institute for Research and Reform in
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 773

Education (www.irre.org) revised the original Lehr, 2004). The Student Engagement Instrument
instruments to provide a shorter set of instru- (SEI) is currently being used to evaluate the effec-
ments (RAPS) for use in evaluating school reform tiveness of district initiatives to improve student
efforts based on the same theoretical framework. engagement in the Gwinnett County Public
Two of the survey measures identified in the Schools (this volume). The Identification with
review (Attitudes Toward Mathematics Survey School questionnaire was developed to assess the
[ATM] and the Motivated Strategies for Learning extent to which students identify with or disen-
Questionnaire [MSLQ]) were developed as part gage from school, and was based on the theory
of research exploring the relationships between that school identification is a crucial factor in the
students’ self-regulation, cognitive strategy use, prevention of school dropouts (Finn, 1989).
and achievement outcomes. Research in this area Other measures have been developed to help
examines the use of cognitive, meta-cognitive, schools and districts monitor engagement and to
and self-regulatory strategies that foster active assist schools in identifying areas in need of
cognitive engagement in learning (Corno & improvement. For example, the High School
Mandinach, 1983; Meece et al., 1988). Survey of Student Engagement (HSSSE) was
Other measures were developed by researchers developed to provide descriptive and compara-
studying the relationship between context and tive data on high school students’ views about
engagement. For example, the Student Engage- their schoolwork, the school learning environ-
ment Measure (SEM) – MacArthur was developed ment, and interactions with the school commu-
for a longitudinal study of the relationship between nity, relative to the responses of other schools
classroom context and engagement in urban minor- (Yazzie-Mintz, 2007). Each school that partici-
ity youth in the upper elementary grades (Fredricks pates receives a customized report that compares
et al., 2005). In addition, the School Engagement the students’ responses to that of other schools.
Scale/Questionnaire (SEQ) was developed as part Similarly, the School Success Profile (SSP) was
of a large study in nine high schools that reported developed to provide insight into how students
on ways that parents, peers, and communities perceive themselves and their environments and
influence students’ commitment to, or engagement to compare school scores relative to a national
with, school (Steinberg et al., 1996). This scale has sample (Bowen, Rose, & Bowen, 2005).
subsequently been used by researchers trying to Finally, one survey measure [the Motivation
understand factors that explain differences in voca- and Engagement Survey (MES)] was developed
tional attitudes and career development behaviors to diagnose and identify students who are strug-
among subgroups of high school students (Perry, gling or at risk for disengagement and academic
2008; Wettersten et al., 2005). failure. The MES creates profiles for individual
Increasing student engagement is the primary students based on responses to 11 different
goal of many interventions to reduce dropout rates subscales reflecting a multidimensional model of
(Appleton et al., 2008; Finn, 1989). Two measures motivation and engagement. This measure has
were developed in the context of this work on been used to diagnose students with low motiva-
dropout prevention [Identification with School tion and engagement, in studies evaluating the
Questionnaire (ISQ) and Student Engagement effectiveness of interventions and in studies exam-
Instrument (SEI)]. For example, the Student ining demographic differences in engagement and
Engagement Instrument (SEI) was developed to motivation (Fredricks & McColskey, 2010).
measure affective (formerly psychological) and
cognitive engagement and to expand on the behav-
ioral and academic indicators that were collected Samples
as part of Check & Connect, an intervention model
designed to improve student engagement at The surveys included in this chapter have been
school, reduce dropouts, and increase school used with students from the upper elementary
completion (Anderson, Christenson, Sinclair, & school years (third to fifth grades) through the
774 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

Table 37.5 Samples


Instrument name Samples
Attitudes Toward Original sample 297 suburban, southeastern high school students in their math courses
Mathematics Survey Versions of the cognitive engagement items also have been used with high school English
(ATM) students in a Midwestern high school and college-level samples (educational psychology
students, preservice teachers, and students in statistics classes)
Engagement vs. Sample of 1,018 elementary school students in grades 3–6 in suburban and rural schools
Disaffection with The items have also been used with samples of elementary, middle, and high school White
Learning – Student and low-income minority youth in urban and suburban districts
Report (EvsD)
High School Survey of Original sample 7,200 students from four high schools
Student Engagement Survey has been administered to 200,000 students from across the nation. Students are
(HSSSE) ethnically and economically diverse and attend rural, suburban, and urban schools
Identification with Original sample 539 eighth grade students from 163 schools in rural, urban, suburban, and
School Questionnaire inner-city settings (25% Black, 75% White)
(ISQ) Survey has been used with racially diverse samples including Black, Hispanic, Asian, and
American Indian students, and with low-income students in the middle and high school grades
Motivated Strategies for Original sample 173 primarily White middle and working class seventh graders across 15
Learning Questionnaire classrooms
(MSLQ) Survey has been used in both English- and non-English-speaking countries across the world
Motivation and The Junior High version normed with 1,249 students in Australia, aged 9–13, across 63
Engagement Scale classes in 15 schools. The High School version normed with 21,579 students, aged 12–18,
(MES) across 58 schools
Samples were from urban, rural, and suburban areas of Australia, and predominately middle
class students
Research Assessment Large populations of Black, White, Hispanic, and low-income youth in urban districts
Package for Schools engaged in comprehensive school reform
(RAPS)
School Engagement Original sample 641 urban, low-income, primarily Black and Hispanic students in grades 3
Measure to 5 attending neighborhood schools
(SEM) – MacArthur Survey also used with other low-income ethnically diverse upper elementary school students
School Engagement Original sample 12,000 ethnically and economically diverse students in nine high schools in
Scale/Questionnaire Wisconsin and Northern California
(SEQ) Items also used with racially diverse high school students in rural and urban areas in the
Northeast and Midwest
School Success Profile Original sample 805 middle school students in 26 schools in North Carolina totaling
(SSP) approximately 805 students
Survey also used with racially diverse and low-income students in middle and high schools
Student Engagement Original sample 1,931 ninth grade students from an ethnically diverse, majority low income,
Instrument (SEI) urban school district
Survey also used with students in grades 6 through 12

high school years. Two of the measures were ini- was modeled after the National Survey of Student
tially developed for use with upper elementary Engagement (NSSE), a widely used measure of
school populations [Engagement vs. Disaffection student engagement at the college level.
with Learning and MacArthur (SEM)]. On the Table 37.5 shows that the majority of measures
other end of the spectrum, the Motivated have been used with ethnically and economically
Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) diverse samples. In addition, four of the measures
was originally developed for use with college have been translated into other languages [MSLQ,
samples, but a version was adapted for use with MacArthur measure, SSP, and SEI]. For example,
middle school students. In addition, the High the MSLQ has been translated into multiple lan-
School Survey of Student Engagement (HSSSE) guages and has been used in English-speaking
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 775

and non-English-speaking countries all over the Table 37.6 Reliability information
world (Garcia-Duncan & McKeachie, 2005). The Internal Test-retest
SSP and the MacArthur measure have been trans- Instrument name consistency interrater
lated into Spanish. Sections of the SSP have also Attitudes Toward .63–.81 –
been translated into Hebrew, Lithuanian, Mathematics Survey (ATM) –
Engagement vs. Disaffection .61–.85 .53
Romanian, and Portuguese (Fredricks &
with Learning (EvsD) –.68
McColskey, 2010). Finally, the SEI has been High School Survey – –
translated into Portuguese and Mandarin (Moreira, of Student Engagement –
Vaz, Dias, & Petracchi, 2009). (HSSSE)
Identification with .54–.84 –
School Questionnaire (ISQ) –
Motivated Strategies .63–.88 –
Psychometric Information for Learning Questionnaire –
(MSLQ)
Technical information on reliability and validity Motivation and Engagement .70–.87 .61–.81
was found on all but one self-report measure, Scale (MES) –
though there were variations in the amount and Research Assessment package .68–.77 –
for Schools (RAPS)
types of technical information available (see
School Engagement Measure .55–.86 –
Fredricks & McColskey, 2010, for more detailed (SEM)-MacArthur –
information on psychometric properties). The one School Engagement Scale/ .74–.86 –
exception was the High School Survey of Student Questionnaire (SEQ) –
Engagement (HSSSE) which currently has no School Success Profile (SSP) .66–.82
published information on the psychometric prop- Student Engagement .72–.92 .60 –.62
erty of this measure. However, developers of this Instrument (SEI)
measure have indicated that a reliability and valid- Note: Ranges within cells indicate either differing results
for individual subscales, differing results based on age
ity study is currently underway. They also make
groups, or differing results from various researchers
reference to the reliability and validity reported
on the National Survey of School Engagement, a
widely used measure of engagement at the college measure ranged from .54 to .93, with most scales
level, from which the HSSSE was adapted. in the range of the .70 to .80 (see Table 37.6).
Because of the variation in alphas across mea-
sures and subscales, it is important to examine
Reliability the information on reliability more closely in
light of the particular sample and intended use.
Internal consistency is the extent to which indi- In addition, three of the measures [Motivation
viduals who respond in one way to items tend to and Engagement Survey (MES), Engagement vs.
respond the same way to other items intended to Disaffection with Learning (EvsD), and the
measure the same construct. A Cronbach’s alpha Student Engagement Instrument (SEI)] reported
of .70 or higher for a set of items is considered information on test-retest reliability, or the extent
acceptable (Leary, 2004). Cronbach’s alpha of the to which two different administrations of the
engagement scales/subscales was reported for all measure give the same results.
but one measure. The HSSSE was originally
reported on an item-by-item basis, but recently the
developers began grouping the 121 items in the Validity
questionnaire by the three aspects of engagement.
However, no information on the internal consis- In this chapter, we summarize the information
tency of these subscales is currently available. available on these measures under the broad
The reliabilities of these scales reported umbrella of construct validity (see Fredricks &
by both the developers and other users of the McColskey, 2010, for more information on
776 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

validity). One way to investigate construct techniques to examine how survey items load onto
validity is to examine whether the correlations the engagement constructs. Seven of the instru-
between the engagement scales and the other ments reported results from either exploratory or
related constructs are in the hypothesized direc- confirmatory factor analyses. However, because
tion based on theory and prior empirical work. of large differences in both the number and types
The following three examples from the surveys of items, it is challenging to compare the resulting
illustrate these relations. First, the engagement scales from these analyses. The following three
scales in the School Success Profile (SSP) were examples illustrate this variability. Voelkl (1997)
positively correlated with teacher, parent, and used confirmatory factor analysis with 16 items
peer support variables (Bowen, Rose, Powers, & on the Identification with School Questionnaire
Glennie, 2008). Additionally, the three engage- (ISQ) on a sample of 3,539 urban eighth graders.
ment subscales (i.e., behavioral, emotional, and These analyses confirmed two subscales: belong-
cognitive) in the MacArthur measure (SEM) were ing and value. Martin (2008, 2009a, 2009b) used
moderately correlated with students’ perceptions confirmatory factor analyses on 44 items of the
of aspects of the academic and social context, Motivation and Engagement Survey with large
school value, and school attachment (Fredricks samples of Australian middle and high school stu-
et al., 2005). Finally, the cognitive strategy use dents. These analyses resulted in 11 subscales
and self-regulation scales of the MSLQ were (self-belief, learning focus, valuing of school, per-
positively correlated with students’ self-reports sistence, planning, study management, disengage-
of interest, efficacy, and task value (Pintrich, ment, self-sabotage, anxiety, failure avoidance,
1999; Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990). and uncertain control). Finally, Appleton et al.
We also found evidence of criterion-related (2006) used confirmatory factor analysis with 56
validity, or the extent to which a measure is asso- items from the Student Engagement Instrument
ciated with a key behavior or outcome (Leary, on a sample of 1,931 ninth graders. These analy-
2004) on the majority of measures. Eight out of ses resulted in six subscales (teacher-student rela-
the 11 measures reported positive correlations tionships, peer support, family support, control
between engagement and indicators of academic relevance of schoolwork, future aspirations, and
performance. For example, several studies using extrinsic motivation). A more recent confirmatory
the MSLQ have documented that cognitive strat- factor analysis of the Student Engagement
egy use and self-regulation scales are positively Instrument (SEI) showed evidence of the validity
related to course assignments, exams, and grades of five subscales, dropping extrinsic motivation as
(Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990; Wolters & Pintrich, a subscale (Betts, Appleton, Reschly, Christenson,
1998; Wolters, Yu, & Pintrich, 1996). Similarly, & Huebner, 2010).
the two engagement scales of the RAPS were Finally, construct validity can be assessed by
positively correlated with indicators of perfor- examining the correlations between engagement
mance (Institute for Research and Reform in measured by different methodological approaches.
Education, 1998). In addition, Appleton, Reschly, Three of the measures (EvsD, RAPS, and SEM)
and Martin (under review) documented signifi- reported correlations of scores from student self-
cant differences between affective and cognitive report measures with other techniques to assess
engagement data and academic performance; stu- engagement (teacher ratings, external observers,
dents with the lowest reports of engagement had and interviews). For example, the student and
the lowest scores on state tests and the lowest teacher versions of the Engagement versus
graduation rates. Finally, three of the measures Disaffection with Learning scale were moderately
(SSE, ISQ, and SEM) reported correlations correlated with each other. In addition, teacher
between engagement and indicators of participa- reports of behavioral and emotional engagement
tion (i.e., attendance, teacher ratings of participa- correlated with external observations of on and off
tion) (Fredricks & McColskey, 2010). task behavior, but student self-reports did not cor-
Another way to assess construct validity is to relate with external observations of engagement
use exploratory or confirmatory factor analyses (Skinner et al., 2008). Similarly, the developers of
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 777

the SEM-MacArthur measure correlated students’ of engagement used across studies. Although
self-reports with teachers’ reports of student scholars have used a broad range of terms for
behavior. In addition, students’ responses on the engagement, the two most common are student
survey were compared to interviews about engage- engagement and school engagement. Differences
ment with the same sample of students. They between the terms were raised in a prior review of
reported a positive correlation between the three the literature (see Appleton et al., 2006, for more
subscales of engagement (behavioral, emotional, discussion). We echo Appleton et al. (2006) point
and cognitive engagement) and numerical ratings that greater attention needs to be paid to the use of
given to interview responses (Blumenfeld et al., the terms student engagement and school engage-
2005; Fredricks et al., 2005). ment in future work and potential differences in
Overall, the psychometric information on the meaning of these constructs. Another concern
these measures suggests that student engagement is that many of the definitions of engagement
can be reliably measured through self-report overlap with other educational constructs (i.e.,
methods. In addition, the measures of engage- school bonding, belonging, and school climate). It
ment relate to both contextual variables and out- is important that researchers acknowledge this
come variables as expected. Moreover, the fact overlap with earlier literatures, many of which
that engagement has been shown to positively have stronger bodies of literature supporting the
correlate with achievement indicates that it could construct, and be clearer in terms of both research
serve as a worthwhile intermediate outcome to and practice about the “value added” from study-
monitor. Finally, the results of exploratory and ing engagement (Fredricks et al., 2004; Jimerson
confirmatory factor analyses demonstrate the et al., 2003).
variability in the different conceptualizations of Although there is some agreement that engage-
engagement and challenges in comparing across ment is a multidimensional construct, there is vari-
different survey measures. ation in both the number (i.e., 2–4) and types
(academic, behavioral, emotional, and cognitive)
of engagement dimensions. As can be seen in this
Conclusions and Future Directions chapter, different conceptualizations of engage-
ment have resulted in a variation in the content of
As evident from this chapter, there are a variety items used in instruments. Moreover, even within
of methods for assessing engagement, each with the same dataset, researchers sometimes use differ-
strengths and limitations and useful for particular ent variables to operationalize engagement, often
purposes. However, even when researchers use without a strong theoretical or conceptual frame-
the same methodology (i.e., self-report surveys), work guiding the choice of indicators. For exam-
there is variation in how engagement is defined ple, researchers have selected different items from
and measured. For example, some of the surveys large nationally representative datasets like the
in this chapter focus primarily on behaviors such National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88)
as effort, homework, and attendance. In contrast, to create different scales of engagement (Glanville
other surveys include items related to emotional & Wildhasen, 2007). This makes it difficult to
dimensions such as relationships with teachers compare findings concerning both the predictors
and cognitive dimensions such as strategy use. and outcomes of engagement. An additional prob-
Below we outline some of the key concerns lem is that similar items have sometimes been used
related to measurement. to assess different dimensions of engagement. For
example, student effort is used by some to describe
the degree of psychological investment in learning
Operationalization of Engagement (i.e., cognitive engagement) and by others to reflect
basic compliance with schoolwork (i.e., behavioral
As outlined in prior reviews (Appleton et al., engagement). In addition, students’ valuing of
2008; Fredricks et al., 2004; Jimerson et al., 2003), school has been used as part of both emotional and
there is considerable variation in the definitions cognitive engagement scales.
778 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

Given the variations in the definitions and can range from liking school to a deeper attach-
measures of student engagement, one of the first ment and identification with the institution
steps to improving measurement is for research- (Fredricks et al.). Cognitive engagement can
ers to more clearly describe their particular defi- range from the use of shallow rote strategies to
nition of engagement. It will also be important as the use of deep processing strategies that promote
a field to come to a stronger consensus on the deep understanding (Greene, Miller, Crowson,
operationalization of engagement (Appleton Duke, & Akey, 2004). Future research should
et al., 2008; Fredricks et al., 2004). Currently, we explore qualitative differences in engagement
believe that the strongest empirical and theoreti- across different contexts (i.e., teacher directed as
cal support exists for a tripartite conceptualiza- compared to small group work).
tion of student engagement which includes a
behavioral, emotional, and cognitive subcompo-
nent. However, further empirical research is Developmental Differences
needed to determine what are the best indicators
of each subtype and the extent to which behav- Another important research area concerns devel-
ioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement are opmental differences in the measurement of
separate constructs. engagement. There may be different indicators of
engagement depending on the age of the child,
and these different types of engagement may
Assessing Malleability of Engagement change and evolve over time. For example, stu-
dents might not be cognitively engaged in learn-
Several scholars have argued that one of the ing until they are able to self-regulate and become
strengths of engagement is that it represents a intentional learners (Fredricks et al., 2004). There
shift from the focus on individual characteristics is a critical need for research that uses confirma-
toward an investigation of potentially malleable tory factor analytic techniques to validate surveys
contextual factors that can be targeted in inter- at different ages. One example of this is research
ventions (Appleton et al., 2008; Fredricks et al., using the Motivated Learning and Strategy Use
2004; Sinclair, Christenson, Lehr, & Anderson, Questionnaire (MSLQ). Exploratory and confir-
2003). Appleton (Chap. 35) presents an example matory factor analyses with this measure demon-
of how the Student Engagement Instrument (SEI) strated different factor structures in college and
is being used to guide intervention efforts aimed middle school classrooms. In college samples,
at improving student engagement and identifying analyses resulted in four cognitive strategy fac-
students who are at risk. Unfortunately, many of tors (rehearsal, elaboration, organization, and
the current measures make it difficult to test ques- meta-cognitive strategy use) (Pintrich, Smith,
tions of malleability. The majority of engagement Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993). In contrast, factor
measures tend to be general (i.e., I like school), analyses with younger students resulted in one
though there are a few examples of domain-specific general cognitive strategy use scale and one
measures (i.e., Kong et al., 2003; Wigfield et al., meta-cognitive strategy use scale, suggesting that
2008). Furthermore, measures are rarely worded younger students do not make as fine of distinc-
to reflect specific situations or tasks, making it tions between types of strategy use as older stu-
difficult to examine the extent to which engage- dents (Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990; Wolters,
ment varies across contexts. In addition, most Pintrich, & Karbenick, 2005).
current survey measures do not adequately
address qualitative differences in each of the
dimensions of engagement. For example, behav- Variation Across Groups
ioral engagement can range from basic compli-
ance with school rules to doing more than is Another important question is whether engage-
required (Finn, 1989). Emotional engagement ment can be measured similarly for all groups of
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 779

students. If measures of engagement behave dif- to assessing the dynamic nature of engagement is
ferently by race, SES, gender, and grade, and experience sampling methods which can track
these differences are not taken into account, com- fluctuations in engagement over time.
parisons in the level of engagement or effects In sum, although the construct of student
across groups are invalid (Glanville & Wildhagen, engagement has considerable promise, measure-
2007). For example, Glanville and Wildhagen ment issues should continue to be explored in
used confirmatory factor analyses to create a order to fully realize this promise (Glanville &
measurement model for school engagement using Wildhagen, 2007). We believe that a more system-
NELS:88 across White, African American, atic and thoughtful attention to the measurement
Latino, and Asian youth. They found that this of student engagement is one of the most pressing
measurement model was invariant across ethnic and imperative directions for future research.
groups, and it was therefore appropriate to com- First, it is important that researchers are clearer
pare the effects of disengagement across these about their definitions of student engagement and
groups. In addition, Betts et al. (2010) used con- how their conceptualizations of this construct
firmatory factor analysis to test model invariance relate to both other scholars’ operationalization of
across gender and grade. They found that the student engagement and to other related educa-
Student Engagement Instrument (SEI) had a sim- tional constructs. The field will benefit if research-
ilar factor structure across gender and grade-level. ers spent less time generating slight variations on
Further research should use confirmatory factor this construct and spent more time on theory
analytic techniques to validate existing instru- development and integration of the different con-
ments and factor structures across different groups ceptualizations of engagement. Greater consis-
of students (i.e., age, gender, race, and SES). tency in the use of measures across studies will
also make it easier to compare findings about the
outcomes and precursors of student engagement.
Use of Multiple Methods However, as our review has also highlighted, there
is large variation in the extent of psychometric
Finally, we recommend researchers use multiple evidence available on current measures. Future
methods to assess engagement. Qualitative meth- research testing the psychometric properties of
ods can help to supplement our understanding of these measures is critical. Finally, we strongly
the contextual factors that are associated with support the use of a wide range of methods to
engagement. In depth descriptions of context and assess engagement including observations, inter-
engagement are critical for knowing how and views, and experience sampling techniques.
where to intervene. Moreover, the measurement
of engagement is often related to the affordances
in the environment, and it may be difficult to References
assess cognitive engagement in classrooms where
tasks involve only superficial strategy use. Anderson, A. R., Christenson, S. L., Sinclair, M. F., &
Lehr, C. A. (2004). Check & Connect: The importance
Observational methods can be used to better
of relationships for promoting engagement with
understand variations in engagement across dif- school. Journal of School Psychology, 42(2), 95–113.
ferent contexts and how this variation may relate doi:10.1016/j.jsp. 2004.01.002.
to affordances within the context. Qualitative Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J.
(2008). Student engagement with school: Critical con-
methods also are a useful method for describing
ceptual and methodological issues of the construct.
how the different types of engagement evolve Psychology in the Schools, 45, 369–386. doi:10.1002/
and develop to help understand why some youth pits.20303.
begin to disengage from school (Fredricks et al., Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly,
A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological
2004). Finally, most current methods do not ade-
engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement
quately capture the dynamic and interactive Instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445.
nature of engagement. One promising approach doi:10.1016/j.jsp. 2006.04.002.
780 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., & Martin, C. (under review). Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review
Research to practice: Linking assessment of student of Educational Research, 59, 117–142. doi:10.3102/
cognitive and affective engagement to intervention. 00346543059002117.
Archambault, I. (2009). Adolescent behavioral, affective, Finn, J. D., Folger, J., & Cox, D. (1991). Measuring partici-
and cognitive engagement in school: Relation to drop- pation among elementary grade students. Educational
out. Journal of School Health, 79, 408–415. and Psychological Measurement, 51, 393–402.
Betts, J. E., Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., Christenson, S. L., Finn, J. D., Pannozzo, G. M., & Voelkl, K. E. (1995).
& Huebner, E. S. (2010). A study of the factor invari- Disruptive and inattentive-withdrawn behavior and
ance of the Student Engagement Instrument (SEI): achievement among fourth graders. The Elementary
Results from middle and high school students. School School Journal, 95, 421–454.
Psychology Quarterly, 25, 84–93. doi:10.1037/ Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success
a0020259. among students at risk for school failure. Journal of
Birch, S., & Ladd, G. (1997). The teacher-child relation- Applied Psychology, 82, 221–234.
ship and children’s early school adjustment. Journal of Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., Friedel, J., & Paris,
School Psychology, 35, 61–79. A. (2005). School engagement. In K. A. Moore &
Blumenfeld, P., Modell, J., Bartko, W. T., Secada, W., L. Lippman (Eds.), Conceptualizing and measuring
Fredricks, J., Friedel, J., et al. (2005). School engage- indicators of positive development: What do children
ment of inner city students during middle childhood. need to flourish (pp. 305–321). New York: Kluwer
In C. R. Cooper, C. Garcia Coll, W. T. Bartko, H. M. Academic/Plenum Press.
Davis, & C. Chatman (Eds.), Developmental pathways Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. (2004).
through middle childhood: Rethinking diversity and School engagement: Potential of the concept: State of
contexts as resources (pp. 145–170). Mahwah, NJ: the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74,
Lawrence Erlbaum. 59–119. doi:10.3102/00346543074001059.
Bowen, G. L., Rose, R. A., & Bowen, N. K. (2005). The Fredricks, J., & McColskey, W., with Meli, J., Mordica, J.,
reliability and validity of the school success profile. Montrosse, B., and Mooney, K. (2010). Measuring stu-
Blomington, IN: Xlibris Corporation. dent engagement in upper elementary through high
Bowen, G. L., Rose, R. A., Powers, J. D., & Glennie, E. J. school: A description of 21 instruments (Issues &
(2008). The joint effects of neighborhoods, schools, Answers Report, REL 2010–No. 098). Washington, DC:
peers, and families on changes in the school success of U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education
middle school students. Family Relations, 57, 504–516. Sciences, National Center for Education Evaluation and
Buhs, E. S., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as an ante- Regional Assistance, Regional Educational Laboratory
cedent of young children’s school adjustment: An exam- Southeast. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/edlabs.
ination of mediating process. Developmental Psychology, Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a
37, 550–560. doi:10.1037/0012-1649_37.4.50. factor in children’s academic engagement and perfor-
Conchas, G. Q. (2001). Structuring failure and success: mance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 148–162.
Understanding the variability in Latino school engage- doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.148.
ment. Harvard Educational Review, 71, 475–504. Gamoran, A., & Nystrand, M. (1992). Taking students
Connell, J. P. (1990). Context, self, and action: A motiva- seriously. In M. N. Fred (Ed.), Student engagement
tional analysis of self-system processes across the life- and achievement in American secondary schools (pp.
span. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), The self in transition: 40–61). New York: Teachers College Press.
Infancy to childhood (pp. 61–97). Chicago: University Garcia-Duncan, T. G., & McKeachie, W. J. (2005). The
of Chicago Press. making of the motivated strategies for learning ques-
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, tionnaire. Educational Psychologist, 40(2), 117–128.
autonomy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P. (1996). Assessing students’
self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe motivation and learning strategies in the classroom
(Eds.), Self-processes and development: Minnesota context: The motivation and strategies in learning
symposium on child psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 43–77). questionnaire. In M. Birenbaum & F. J. Dochy (Eds.),
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Alternatives in assessment of achievements, learning
Corno, L., & Mandinach, E. (1983). The role of cognitive processes, and prior knowledge (pp. 319–339). New
engagement in classroom learning and motivation. York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press.
Educational Psychologist, 18, 88–108. Glanville, L., & Wildhagen, T. (2007). The measurement
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of of school engagement: Assessing dimensionality and
optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial. measurement in variance across race and ethnicity.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation Educational and Psychological Measurement, 6,
and self-determination in human behavior. New York: 1019–1041. doi:10.1177/0013164406299126.
Plenum Press. Greene, B. A., Miller, R. B., Crowson, H. M., Duke, B. L.,
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). & Akey, K. L. (2004). Predicting high school students’
Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), & cognitive engagement and achievement: Contributions
N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychol- of classroom perceptions and motivation. Contemporary
ogy: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality devel- Educational Psychology, 29(4), 462–482. doi:10.1016/j.
opment (5th ed., pp. 1017–1094). New York: Wiley. cedpsych.2004.01.006.
37 The Measurement of Student Engagement… 781

Greenwood, C. R., Horton, B. T., & Utley, C. A. (2002). Meece, J., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Hoyle, R. H. (1988).
Academic engagement: Current perspectives on Students’ goal orientation and cognitive engagement
research and practice. School Psychology Review, 31, in classroom activities. Journal of Educational
328–349. Psychology, 80, 514–523.
Hektner, J. M., Schmidt, J. A., & Csikzentmihalyi, M. Miller, R. B., Greene, B. A., Montalvo, G. P., Ravindran, B.,
(2007). Experience sampling method: Measuring the & Nichols, J. D. (1996). Engagement in academic
quality of everyday life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. work: The role of learning goals, future consequences,
Helme, S., & Clarke, D. (2001). Identifying cognitive pleasing others, and perceived ability. Contemporary
engagement in the mathematics classrooms. Educational Psychology, 21(4), 388–422.
Mathematics Educational Journal, 13, 133–153. Moreira, P. A. S., Vaz, F. M., Dias, P. C., & Petracchi,
Institute for Research and Reform in Education. (1998). P. (2009). Psychometric properties of the Portuguese
Research Assessment Package for Schools (RAPS) version of the Student Engagement Instrument.
manual for elementary and middle school assessments. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 24, 303–307.
Retrieved August 1, 2009, from http://www.irre.org/ doi:10.1177/0829573509346680.
publications/pdfs/RAPS_manual_entire_1998.pdf. National Research Council and the Institute of Medicine.
Jimerson, S. R., Campos, E., & Grief, J. L. (2003). Toward (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering high school stu-
an understanding of definitions and measures of school dents’ motivation to learn. Committee on Increasing
engagement and related terms. California School High School Students’ Engagement and Motivation to
Psychologist, 8, 7–27. Learn. Board on Children, Youth, and Families,
Kong, Q., Wong, N., & Lam, C. (2003). Student engage- Division of Behavioral and Social Science and
ment in mathematics: Development of instrument and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academy
validation of a construct. Mathematics Education Press.
Research Journal, 54, 4–21. Newmann, F., Wehlage, G.G., & Lamborn, S. D. (1992).
Leary, M. R. (2004). Introduction to behavioral research The significance and sources of student engagement. In
methods (4th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education, Inc. F. Newmann (Ed.), Student engagement and achieve-
Lee, O., & Anderson, C. W. (1993). Task engagement and ment in American secondary schools (pp. 11–39).
conceptual change in middle school science class- New York: Teachers College Press.
rooms. American Educational Research Journal, 30, Nystrand, M., & Gamoran, A. (1991). Instructional dis-
585–610. course, student engagement, and literature achieve-
Lee, O., & Brophy, J. (1996). Motivational patterns ment. Research in the Teaching of English, 25,
observed in sixth-grade science classrooms. Journal 261–290.
of Research in Science Teaching, 33, 303–318. Nystrand, M., Wu, L. L., Gamaron, A., Zeiser, S., & Long, D.
Locke, D. A. (1996). Making and molding identity in (2001). Questions in time: Investigating the structure
school: Student narratives on race, gender and aca- and dynamics of unfolding classroom discourse.
demic engagement. Albany, NY: State University Albany, NY: National Research Center on English
Press. Learning & Achievement.
Maehr, M. L., & Meyer, H. A. (1997). Understanding Perry, J. (2008). School engagement among urban youth of
motivation and schooling: Where we’ve been, where color: Criterion pattern effects of vocational explora-
we are, and where we need to go. Educational tion and racial identity. Journal of Career Development,
Psychology Review, 9, 371–408. 34(4), 397–422. doi:10.1177/0894845308316293.
Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional Peterson, P., Swing, S., Stark, K., & Wass, G. (1984).
activity: Patterns in the elementary, middle, and high Students’ cognitions and time on task during mathe-
school years. American Educational Research Journal, matics instruction. American Educational Research
37, 153–184. doi:10.3102/00028312037001153. Journal, 21, 487–515.
Martin, A. J. (2008). Motivation and engagement in Pintrich, P. R. (1999). The role of motivation in promoting
diverse performance domains: Testing their generality and sustaining self-regulated learning. International
across school, university/college, work, sport, music, Journal of Educational Research, 31(6), 459–470.
and daily life. Journal of Research in Personality, Pintrich, P. R., & DeGroot, E. (1990). Motivational and
42(6), 1607–1612. doi:10.1016/j.jrp. 2008.05.003. self-regulated learning components of classroom aca-
Martin, A. J. (2009a). Motivation and engagement across demic performance. Journal of Educational
the academic life span: A developmental construct Psychology, 82, 33–40.
validity study of elementary school, high school, and Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W.
university/college students. Educational and (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the moti-
Psychological Measurement, 69(5), 794–824. vated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ).
doi:10.1177/0013164409332214. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53(3),
Martin, A. J. (2009b). The motivation and engagement 801–813.
scale. Sydney, Australia: Lifelong Achievement Group. Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2006). Prediction of
Retrieved from http://www.lifelongachievement.com. dropout among students with mild disabilities: A case
McCaslin, M. M., & Good, T. L. (1996). Listening in for inclusion of student engagement variables.
classrooms. New York: HarperCollins. Remedial and Special Education, 27, 276–292.
782 J.A. Fredricks and W. McColskey

Russell, V. J., Ainsley, M., & Frydenberg, E. (2005). Voelkl, K. E. (1996). Measuring students’ identification
Schooling issues digest: Student motivation and with school. Educational and Psychological
engagement. Retrieved March 1, 2010, from http:/ Measurement, 56(5), 760–770. doi:10.1177/00131644
www.dest.gov/au/sectors/school_education/publica- 96056005003.
tion_resources/schooling_issues_digest/schooling_ Voelkl, K. E. (1997). Identification with school. American
issues_digest_motivation_engagement.htm. Journal of Education, 105, 204–319. doi:10.1007/
Saliva, J., & Ysseldyke, J. E. (2004). Assessment (9th ed.). s11256-006-0039-1.
Princeton, NJ: Houghton Mifflin. Volpe, R. J., DiPerna, J. C., Hintze, J. M., & Shapiro, E. S.
Shapiro, E. S. (2004). Academic skills problems: Direct (2005). Observing students in classroom settings:
assessment and intervention (3rd ed.). New York: A review of seven coding schemes. School Psychology
Guilford Press. Review, 34(4), 454–474.
Shernoff, D. J., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Schneider, B., & Wettersten, K. B., Gulmino, A., Herrick, C. G., Hunter, P.
Shernoff, E. S. (2003). Student engagement in high J., Kim, G. Y., Jagow, D., et al. (2005). Predicting edu-
school classrooms from the perspective of flow theory. cational and vocational attitudes among rural high
School Psychology Quarterly, 18, 158–176. school students. Journal of Counseling of Psychology,
Shernoff, J. D., & Schmidt, J. A. (2008). Further evidence 52(4), 658–663. doi: 10.1037/0022–0167.52.4.658.
of the engagement-achievement paradox among U.S. Wigfield, A., Guthrie, J. T., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A.,
high school students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Klauda, S. L., McRae, A., et al. (2008). Role of read-
5, 564–580. doi:10.1007/s10964-007-9241-z. ing engagement in mediating the effects of reading
Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Lehr, C. A., & Anderson, comprehension instruction on reading outcomes.
A. R. (2003). Facilitating student learning and engage- Psychology in the Schools, 45, 432–445. doi: 10.10002/
ment: Lessons from Check & Connect longitudinal pits.20307.
studies. The California School Psychologist, 8, 29–41. Wolters, C., & Pintrich, P. R. (1998). Contextual differ-
Skinner, E., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the ences in student motivation and self-regulated learn-
classroom: Reciprocal effect of teacher behavior and ing in mathematics, English, and social studies
student engagement across the school year. Journal of classrooms. Instructional Science, 26, 27–47.
Educational Psychology, 85, 571–581. Wolters, C., Yu, S., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The relation
Skinner, E. A., Marchand, G., Furrer, C., & Kindermann, between goal orientation and students’ motivational
T. (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the beliefs and self-regulated learning and academic learn-
classroom: Part of a larger motivational dynamic. ing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 329–339.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 765–781. Wolters, C. A., Pintrich, P. R., & Karabenick, S. A. (2005).
doi:10.1037/a0012840. Assessing academic self-regulated learning. In K. A.
Steinberg, L. D., Brown, B. B., & Dornbush, S. M. (1996). Moore & L. H. Lippman (Eds.), What do children
Beyond the classroom: Why school reform has failed need to flourish: Conceptualizing and measuring indi-
and what parents need to do. New York: Simon and cators of positive development. New York: Springer.
Schuster. Yair, G. (2000). Educational battlefields in America: The
Turner, J. C., & Meyer, D. K. (2000). Studying and under- tug of war over students’ engagement with instruction.
standing the instructional context of classroom: Using Sociology of Education, 73, 247–269.
our past to forge our future. Educational Psychologist, Yazzie- Mintz, E. (2007). Voices of students on engage-
35, 69–85. ment: A report on the 2006 High School Survey of
Uekawa, K., Borman, K., & Lee, R. (2007). Student Student Engagement. Bloomington, IN: Center for
engagement in the U.S. urban high school mathematics Evaluation & Educational Policy, Indiana University.
and science classrooms: Findings on social organiza- Retrieved February 1, 2010, from http://ceep.indiana.
tion, race, and ethnicity. Urban Review, 39, 1–43. edu/pdf/HSSSE_2006_Report.pdf.
Issues and Methods
in the Measurement of Student 38
Engagement: Advancing
the Construct Through Statistical
Modeling

Joseph Betts

Abstract
This chapter will provide an overview of statistical modeling to further the
measurement of student engagement. After a discussion of the complexity
of defining and therefore measuring engagement, a general introduction
and guide to the construction of productive measures of engagement will
be provided. Confirmatory factor analysis and item response theory will
be elaborated and used to highlight modern methods of evaluating and
scoring instruments to measure engagement. Additionally, the bifactor
model will be displayed and elaborated upon as a potentially useful model
for disentangling some of the intricacies of engagement along with pars-
ing the relationship with motivation. The Student Engagement Instrument
(SEI) will be utilized throughout the chapter to highlight the specific meth-
ods and provide some guidance on potentially useful applications related
to theoretical issues.

Student engagement is an important aspect of an However, there are a number of outstanding


educational milieu. Students must invest consid- conceptual and methodological issues to be
erable time and effort on a daily basis to acquire addressed by the nascent research into student
the knowledge and skills necessary to facilitate engagement (Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong,
ongoing learning and be successful in their edu- 2008; Fredricks et al., 2004).
cational careers. Additionally, engagement with While the construct of engagement has unique
peers, teachers, and administrators can help to theoretical importance, there is some question
foster prosocial relationships. As engagement is about what key elements constitute it. While
vital to education, it has become an important many issues are yet to be evaluated with respect
construct for researchers with quite a broad to how student engagement is intertwined with
appeal (Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, student outcomes and other psychological/educa-
2006; Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004). tional variables, a foundational issue that needs
to be contended with before any results can be
meaningfully interpreted is the appropriate mea-
J. Betts, MMIS, Ph.D., NCSP (*)
Center for Cultural Diversity & Minority Education,
surement of the construct. Without appropriate
Madison, WI, USA measurement, there can be little intriguing or
e-mail: Jbetts5118@aol.com useful research.

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 783


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_38, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
784 J. Betts

This chapter will discuss the construct of have attempted to further this perspective while
student engagement with an emphasis on mea- also situating engagement within a broader
surement and statistical modeling. The chapter contextualization where engagement is seen as a
will highlight the use of modern techniques of mediating variable between specific contexts and
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA; Bartholomew important outcomes.
& Knott, 1999) and item response theory Appleton et al. (2006) utilize their general
(IRT; Crocker & Algina, 1986; Yen & Fitzpatrick, theoretical framework to develop an instrument
2006) for evaluating measures of engagement. to measure student engagement. Their work can
The Student Engagement Instrument (SEI; be seen as an instantiation of the previous work
Appleton et al., 2006) will be used to exem- by Fredricks et al. (2004) with an understanding
plify the approaches, but the methodology will of the potential multidimensionality of the con-
be generally extensible for any measurement struct of engagement but also a broadening as
instrument. they take the conceptualization and measurement
of engagement beyond the general types, that is,
cognitive, behavioral, and emotional, to encom-
Contemplating and Defining pass academic engagement and specific aspects
Engagement or subcomponents of the general type of engage-
ment related to unique contexts. For instance,
One of the first issues when attempting to mea- there is no attempt to simply measure a singu-
sure anything is deciding upon a relevant defini- lar, broad construct called affective/emotional
tion of the object of interest and what attributes engagement, but care is taken to specify specific
or properties of that object are most salient aspects where emotional engagement might show
(Crocker & Algina, 1986; Downing & Haladyna, itself within different contexts, that is, relation-
2006; Lord & Novick, 1968; Thorndike, 1982; ships with teachers, support from peers, and
Wilson, 2005) for one’s purposes. In the area of family support. Similarly with cognitive engage-
educational and psychological measurement, it is ment, they define two subcomponents of cogni-
sometimes difficult to conceptualize the object of tive engagement that are related to different
measurement because it is usually an abstract aspects of cognitive engagement with one focus-
entity whose attributes of interests for measure- ing on a student’s sense of control and relevance
ment can be arguable and not easily observable of schoolwork and one related to the student’s
(Crocker & Algina). Given that constructs are future aspirations and goals.
hypothetical concepts, there can be a great deal Fredricks et al. (2004) conceptualization of
of difference in conceptualization, for instance, three broad types will serve as a starting point for
see the debate concerning IQ, in general, and the an initial definition of student engagement in this
existence of g-factor, in particular (Jensen, 1998). paper. Behavioral engagement relates to a stu-
Thus, authoritative definitions or widespread dent’s willingness to become involved in school-
acceptance of a specific set of attributes can be related activities in a positive manner. This
difficult to establish in general for psychological can take the form of following rules of conduct,
variables and student engagement is no different participating in school activities, or showing per-
(Appleton et al., 2008). sistence and positive effort in learning tasks.
Presently there are different models of student Emotional engagement subsumes students’ feel-
engagement that accentuate different aspects but ings about the educational milieu within which
also have substantial overlap (Appleton et al., they find themselves. This can relate to reactions
2006; Finn, 1989; Fredricks et al., 2004). Fredricks to teachers, peers, or even the school culture, in
et al. propose a multidimensional approach to general. Cognitive engagement generally reflects
investigating engagement through the use of three the student’s willingness to invest cognitive facil-
broad types of engagement: behavioral, emo- ities into learning and mastering new and poten-
tional, and cognitive. Appleton and colleagues tially difficult skills. This type of engagement can
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 785

also relate to the setting of learning goals and


consciously striving to meet those goals.
Disentangling Motivation
These three types of engagement can readily
and Engagement
be seen to have a significant level of crossover
An outstanding issue in the study of student
and overlap with other important areas of study
engagement is the relationship between motiva-
in psychology. For instance, research on cogni-
tion and engagement (Appleton et al., 2006;
tive engagement can easily be related to research
Wentzel & Wigfield, 2007). Much of the problem
in other areas of psychology and education, such
is related to defining the difference between the
as setting goals, planning, monitoring, and evalu-
two constructs and highlighting the potential inter-
ating progress that are common metacognitive
actions. This area of study is potentially fecund
(Blumenfeld, Kempler, & Krajcik, 2006) or
due both to the dearth of specific research and the
self-regulated learning (Cleary & Zimmerman,
vast array of possible theoretical forays. What fol-
Chap. 11) themes. Additionally, research in the
lows is a brief sortie into some incipient issues
area of social development models related to
with attempted adumbration of research ideas and
such diverse issues as delinquency (Hawkins &
statistical models to be presented in later sections.
Weis, 1985) and drug use (Catalano, Kosterman,
Motivation can be conceived broadly as the
Hawkins, Newcomb, & Abbott, 1996; Guo,
cause for a person’s action in the world (Franken,
Hawkins, Hill, & Abbott, 2001) bears directly on
2001; Kleinginna & Kleinginna, 1981). It is that
the emotional engagement students have with
thing which compels us to action. However,
their teachers and social environment. Similarly,
motivation also carries within it the connotation
research in areas as diverse as internal motivation
of its root, motive, which implies both a cause to
and reward contingencies (Cameron & Pierce,
act and the implicit goal of the action. Therefore,
1994) has relevance to behavioral engagement.
motivation seems to suggest both the movement
As can be inferred, the area of student engage-
to action and the goal associated with that action.
ment has a substantial body of work from which
This goal-directed action can be elicited either by
it is able to draw.
internal or external/social stimulus. For instance,
Further complicating the elucidation of
one can be motivated to play a sport because
engagement is the growing relevance of situating
there is something appealing to the individual or
engagement within a more complex ecology
because one’s friends are playing. Likewise, one
where specific antecedents and outcomes are con-
can be motivated to avoid actions due to internal/
ceptualized (Appleton et al., 2006, 2008; Fredricks
external stimuli.
et al. 2004). Within this conceptualization are
Engagement appears to engender a sense of
important antecedent variables related to such
active commitment or participation (Fredricks
things as school and classroom contexts (subsum-
et al., 2004). However, care should be taken when
ing diverse variables such as relationships with
attempting to equate active commitment and par-
teachers or peers to the more overt demands and
ticipation, as one might participate in an activity
contingencies of the organizational structure),
but with little active commitment. This might
home contexts (subsuming such domains as rela-
have some interesting theoretical repercussions
tions with family members and their ideations
for engagement research as there might be differ-
concerning education), and individual level con-
ent relationships between engagement and out-
text related to the student’s comportment and atti-
come variables of interest, such as drop out or
tudes. Additionally, there are a number of key
achievement, when one measures active commit-
outcomes of interest, two of which appear most
ment to engage in activity as opposed to passive
important are achievement and dropping out of
participation. Simply coming to school can be
school, as both of these outcomes have lifelong
either an active commitment to learning and inter-
repercussions (Alexander, Entwisle, & Horsey,
acting with others or a passive participation in a
1997; Balfanz, Herzog, & Mac Iver, 2007;
socially manifest activity that one feels compelled
Christenson et al., 2008; Finn & Rock, 1997).
786 J. Betts

to do to avoid adverse repercussions. Additionally, The relationship becomes more of a recursive


different underlying types of motivation could be type of interplay, where motivation facilitates
at play in those situations, with active engagement engagement, which facilitates new learning,
potentially motivated by an internal stimulus experiences, and growing competence in an area,
where as passive participation motivated by exter- which then facilitates an increase in motivation
nal/social expectations or punishing consequences to continue the learning and discovery process.
for lack of participation. As research on talent development has also
One possible way to conceptualize the rela- shown (Bloom, 1985), there can be unique
tionship between motivation and engagement is dynamics to this interplay at different develop-
that motivation can be seen as the underlying mental periods as the child moves from initial
psychological state that sets the stage for engage- exploration to professional development. There
ment (Blumenfeld et al., 2006). Appleton et al. is active research in exploring the more dynamic
(2006) suggest that motivation is a necessary but model where engagement acts as a mediator
not sufficient condition for engagement. From between motivation and achievement (Appleton
this perspective, if one is engaged, then one could et al., 2006; Blumenfeld et al., 2006).
be seen to have been motivated toward that The relationship between motivation and
action. However, one might be motivated to act engagement is not one that is easily disentangled.
but not actually initiate an active involvement or However, there are numerous lines of research
participation. That lack of moving from a state of potential. While the severability of engagement
motivation to a state of engagement could derive and motivation might not be easily accomplished,
from personal/individual reasons or from barriers the measurements of engagement and motivation
in the environment that act as a bulwark inhibit- are necessary precursors to any research program
ing engagement. that will attempt to elucidate the possible rela-
There can be structural barriers within an edu- tionships and interdependencies. The following
cational environment that hinder a student from sections will outline some important concepts
bridging the gap between being motivated to act and ideas for extending the present research into
and the realization of that act. For instance, a stu- engagement by explicitly focusing on issues
dent might be motivated to learn a martial art or related to measurement.
piano but not have that option available in school.
A student might be motivated to learn about his-
tory but have significant peer pressure to not Measurement Issues in Student
develop erudition in that area or even actively Engagement
encouraged by peers to “act dumb” or devalue
educational attainment in general. Here is where Given the complexity of the engagement variable
interesting questions within the motivation/ and its potential multidimensionality, developing
engagement research milieu can extend from the an instrument to measure engagement could be
study of individuals and individual differences to seen as quite difficult. It is likely that numerous
the study of environments and local ecologies measures would need to be developed for the
that foster or inhibit active engagement related to multitude of potentially salient research ques-
education and school. tions in the broad field of student engagement.
An interesting scenario arises when consider- Fredricks et al. (2004) provide an insightful cri-
ing the interplay of motivation and engagement: tique of the difficulties in measuring different
To what extent does engagement in an activity types of engagement. They also highlight the
increase an individual’s motivation to continue conceptual issues and inherent problems related
with active participation? Additionally, what to the great deal of overlap between the different
aspects of being engaged in an activity would types of engagement that could make measure-
continually discourage an individual from par- ment more complex. Appleton et al. (2006) also
ticipating and potentially lead to disengagement? provide a perceptive method of organizing the
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 787

construct of engagement within a structure that open issues that can drive the development of
accounts for both antecedent and consequent appropriate scales for measuring different aspects
variables. of engagement and also help to begin to parse out
The three types of engagement, behavioral, the answers about the interrelated aspects of
cognitive, and emotional, might facilitate the engagement.
construction of three different measures of The next section will provide a general over-
engagement with each focused on a single type. view of the process for constructing measures.
However, it would also seem appropriate to iden- This is meant to provide a basis and foundation
tify specific aspects of each general type that for future developers of instruments to measure
could constitute a subcomponent, or subdimen- those aspects of engagement that suite their
sion, of the general type. For instance, the con- research endeavors. There are a number of good
ceptualization of a measure of behavioral references on test construction (see, for instance,
engagement would need to account for work- DeVellis, 2003; Downing & Haladyna, 2006;
related behaviors such as completing school Spector, 1992; or Wilson, 2005) to help guide the
assignments and the level of achievement on that specifics of test development. The following sec-
work but might also have to account for the stu- tion is not meant to be a complete or definitive
dents’ behavior with respect to following rules or methodology for test construction, but rather to
interfering with others. It is easy to see that a stu- provide a general outline for moving the mea-
dent might have serious problems completing surement of engagement forward.
work but have a high regard for following rules,
and vice versa. Therefore, the extent to which
each of the three engagement types are homoge- General Approach to Constructing
neous constructs is highly questionable, and Appropriate Measures
therefore, measurement should focus on examin-
ing specific aspects of each general type. An After a psychological construct, for instance,
example of this can be seen with the SEI, which cognitive engagement or some relevant subcom-
attempts to measure specific aspects of cognitive ponent, of interest has been defined, one would
engagement relating to both future aspirations attempt to delineate those relevant aspects or
and control/relevance of schoolwork. While both attributes that constitute the construct. One might
of these subcomponents are related to cognitive consult the research and find only a single dimen-
engagement, it was thought they represented sion could constitute the construct or that multi-
unique and important aspects of cognitive engage- ple aspects of the construct are important and a
ment and therefore required distinct measures, multidimensional approach should be consid-
for example, independent scales. ered. If the construct was thought to be multidi-
Further complicating the measurement of mensional, then not only would there be a
engagement is the extent to which differing con- necessity to define each dimension of importance
texts of engagement are important. For instance, but also a need to identify the possible relation-
is it important to measure emotional engagement ship between those dimensions.
separately with respect to teachers than to peers, For instance, the SEI identified two important
or can they be comprised as an overall measure types of engagement, cognitive and affective,
of engagement within the school context? from the research literature (Appleton et al.,
Moreover, should school level structure vari- 2006). Furthermore, there was an identification
ables, such as student beliefs and thoughts about of aspects of those general engagement types that
the rules and enforcement of those rules in school could be thought of as different manifestations,
or the student sense of the type of work and or subcomponents or subtypes, of the general
assessments they are given, be measured within type. For instance, in the emotional/affective type
the context of a general engagement construct of engagement, the SEI measures distinct types
related to the school environment? These are of relationships between the student and teacher,
788 J. Betts

the student and their peers, and the student and content and the construct of interest is vitally
their family. In the area of cognitive engagement, important, as inferences from the scores on the
subtypes of control and relevance of schoolwork instrument are only generalizable to the extent
and future aspirations and goals are measured. It that the items appear to have been sampled from
was believed that these were unique subcompo- an appropriate domain. For instance, if we were
nents or context within which each general type developing a test for eighth graders’ mathematics
of engagement would be expressed, but differ- ability and only sampled items from single-digit
ently enough to constitute measuring them addition, there would be legitimate questions
independently. about how well we represented the appropriate
The next step is to bring the theoretical and set of appropriate mathematical problems. Fur-
abstract construct into a tool to measure the pur- thermore, there would be legitimate questions
ported construct. The basic idea is to find some about how well the scores from the test general-
manner of eliciting observable responses from ized to making inferences about the students
individuals that relate to the latent construct and overall mathematical ability.
then apply a numerical quantity to those After an appropriate number of items have
responses. The area of psychometrics in educa- been compiled for which one believes are broad
tion and psychology is the field that applies the enough to cover the relevant domain of the
scientific study of the measurement of latent traits construct, there are three basic steps that follow:
and uses a range of test theories and statistical (1) evaluating the extent to which the items
models to help construct a useful instrument for measure the intended attribute of the construct,
measurement (Crocker & Algina, 1986; Lord & (2) computing a method to score the aggregated
Novick, 1968; Wilson, 2005). There can be responses, and (3) providing evidence that the
numerous methods for obtaining information rating scale is measuring what it was intended
such as observations, structured interviews, or to measure and other issues related to score
basic psychological assessments. This chapter validity, such as measurement invariance and
will focus on a specific method of obtaining differential item functioning.
information through the use of a summated rating The following sections will highlight issues
scale, which will be elaborated in the following (1) and (2) from above, and later sections will
sections. focus on using confirmatory factor analysis
(CFA) and item response theory (IRT) to guide
item analysis and scale development. Thus, the
Moving from the Theoretical chapter will provide a more general theoretical
to the Applied overview of the issues (1) and (2), but then pro-
vide applicable statistical methods for analyzing
This step in the process attempts to bring the the- data for scale construction. These sections pro-
oretical aspect of the construct of interest into vide a foundation for the development of the
focus by developing items to measure the con- underlying measurement model of the instru-
struct. We expect to observe the salient aspects of ment. Note that a glossary of terms appears in
the construct by eliciting student responses to Appendix A.
items/questions that are thought to reveal the Issue (3) is related to the compilation of evi-
continuum of the construct. We would like to find dence for validity and will not be directly dis-
out how individuals differ in the amount of a con- cussed in this chapter. The reason for this is
struct they have, so we ask them questions and validity-related research is usually developed
seek their responses. within the specific context of the intended infer-
Item development is critical to the process of ences of the measurement instrument. Therefore,
measurement, as the items are the only window research design and analytical methodologies are
we have into the construct for the measurement usually more defined by that context than a gen-
instrument. The relationship between the item eral approach (see, for instance, Crocker &
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 789

Algina, 1986; Kane, 2006; Thorndike, 1982; or are measuring a common hypothetical construct.
Wainer & Braun, 1988 for more comprehensive This is a basic psychometric consideration related
approaches to the study of test score validity). to the homogeneity of items that form the scale
Also, only after the items can be confirmed to (McDonald, 1999; Thorndike, 1982). If the items
measure a common construct (1) and the aggre- do not appear to measure a common construct,
gation of those items into a single scale justified then it would be difficult to make inferences from
(2) can research related to validity issues (3) be scores as to a particular attribute of the construct
undertaken. Thus, the following sections seek to because measurement would be confounded.
provide a basis for the foundational aspects of For instance, if an item had little relationship
developing an instrument which would precede with the construct being measured, all measure-
issues related to the validity of that score(s) from ments would be useless in measuring the con-
the instrument, which would be addressed with struct. Imagine using a temperature gauge to
an ongoing research program (Crocker & Algina, measure your basement’s dimensions or using a
1986; Embretson & Reise, 2000; Kane, 2006; student’s math scores to make judgments about
McDonald, 1999; Thorndike, 1982). a student’s reading skills. In these situations,
if the items are not measuring the intended
construct, then they do not give us any useful
Evaluating the Items information about the construct and would be
for Appropriateness misleading. Additionally, if an item appears to
be measuring two or more attributes, for exam-
It is vital to ensure that all the items are indeed ple, measured both cognitive engagement and
measuring a similar construct and therefore pro- emotional engagement, then there would be a
vide the basis for a unified interpretation of an question as to what we should attribute higher
aggregate of those items. Thus, field-testing the or lower response option by the student. Here,
items in an appropriate sample of individuals on the issue of confounded measurement would
an intended measurement instrument is crucial to be apparent. It would be difficult to understand if
evaluate the functioning of the items themselves. the higher response was due to higher levels of
Not only is the sample of items important, so is cognitive or emotional engagement.
the sample of students. Just as one wishes to gen- Generally, when constructing a measure, we
eralize from the item responses to the construct of would like each item to measure only a single
interest, one would like to generalize from the attribute. This ensures that higher or lower scores
sample performance to the population of interest. on the items can be referenced to higher or lower
It is important to evaluate the extent to which levels of the single variable/construct being mea-
each item contributes to the understanding of the sured. For instance, we would like to attribute
construct of interest. Each item needs to contrib- higher scores on cognitive engagement items to
ute to the measurement and should be positively something related to cognitive engagement. Item
related to the overall construct. As item responses analysis can provide both justification for item
across multiple items will be compiled into a score use and retention and also help in the nascent
for the student, each item needs to add signifi- theory development.
cantly to the overall understanding. So not only
must the items themselves be positively related,
they must also conform to the intended measure- Issues Related to Scoring
ment model. In essence, the items must fit to the the Instrument
intended structure of the measurement model.
A few issues should be noted. First of all, as One of the common methods of obtaining
there are a number of items to be aggregated in response information from students is by asking
order to inform the measurement of the construct, questions in a survey format and eliciting
we must provide evidence that indeed the items responses on a forced-choice, ordered category
790 J. Betts

scale (McDonald, 1999). We will refer to this


method of obtaining information as a rating scale
Analyzing the Rating Scale
for this chapter; it is generally a method that
and the Intended Measurement
assigns a series of fixed, ordered categories to the
Model
item response options. In general, the respondent
The following sections will provide an overview
is asked a series of questions thought to elicit the
of two complementary models for item analysis
attributes of the latent construct of interest, and
and the extent to which the intended measure-
then the respondent is asked to respond to each
ment model appears to hold. These analyses will
question by selecting one of a number of catego-
help provide evidence for (1) and (2) above. The
ries which are ordered from one end of the spec-
first model will be an extension of classical test
trum to the other. A common set of ordered
theory (CTT) called the congeneric measurement
categories could be as follows: strongly agree
model. The second will be one of a number of
(SA), agree (A), disagree (D), and strongly dis-
modern item response theory models specifically
agree (SD). For any question, the respondent
from the Rasch family of measurement models.
simply chooses the level of the ordered category
for which they feel best applies.
For scoring the rating scale, a bijective, or
one-to-one, relationship is usually established Congeneric Measurement Model
between the qualitative representations, for and Confirmatory Factor Analysis
example, SA, A, D, and SD, and a numerical rep-
resentation, for example, 3, 2, 1, and 0. For One common method of evaluating the intended
instance, if one were interested in having higher measurement model of a psychometric instru-
scores correspond to higher levels of positive ment is through the use of congeneric measure-
feelings/beliefs related to the item content, then ment theory (Jöreskog, 1971) and confirmatory
one could use the following bijective relation: factor analytic models (CFA; Bartholomew &
SA = 3, A = 2, D = 1, and SD = 0. Thus, higher Knott, 1999; Bollen, 1989; Brown, 2006; Harman,
scores would result in higher levels of agreement. 1967; Jöreskog, 1969; McDonald, 1985, 1999).
Student scores are then computed by summing The congeneric measurement theory can be seen
the numeric representations of all items in order as an extension of the basic classical test theory
to obtain a total score and can be referred to as a (Gulliksen, 1950; Lord & Novick, 1968) where
summated rating scale. Furthermore, to make items measuring a common construct can have
scores interpretable, a norming process can be different levels of relationship to that construct;
undertaken (Thorndike, 1982). for instance, some items do a better or worse job
After evaluating the items, settling on a set of of measuring that construct. The congeneric mea-
items that appear to provide useful measurement surement model can be expressed as follows:
of the construct, and deciding on a transforma-
tion from the qualitative response format to a X i = a i + g iT + e i . (38.1)
quantitative measure, some method of scaling the
test must ensue in order to provide score scale Here the score, X, on an item i, Xi, is com-
with interpretable scores. Scaling basically entails posed of a linear function of the underlying true
combining the scores across items into a single score T. In the common classical test theory, all
metric expressing the continuum of the construct. items have a similar level of difficult, ai = aj = 0,
So, after a field test has provided information on and measure the true score with equivalent preci-
actual response patterns from the intended popu- sion, gi = gj = 1. Additionally, a strong true score
lation and some analysis has provided insights model would presume the error term, e, was simi-
into which set of items provide productive mea- lar across items. The congeneric model can be
surement for the construct, then one would focus seen as a more robust model when attempting to
on the most appropriate method of combining measure a common true score factor. The model
item responses into a total score. presented above is the unidimensional model
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 791

statistical background)). Figure 38.1 represents a


5-item scale measuring a common factor, or a
true score representative of a unidimensional
latent trait, with each item measuring the com-
mon factor with different levels of precision.
CFA provides a unified statistical model proce-
dure to evaluate different aspects of the underly-
ing measurement models. For instance, evaluation
of the factor loadings provides evidence of the
extent to which an item appears to be related to
the underlying latent factor. Thus, evidence can
be obtained to evaluate whether or not an item
appears to be functioning in the manner expected,
given it was expected to contribute significantly
to the measurement of the latent factor. Items with
small or insignificant factor loadings could be
considered for deletion from the instrument or in
need of reworking and further field-testing.
Another attribute of CFA that makes it an
attractive method for testing measurement mod-
els is that it has a number of well-known and
Fig. 38.1 Congeneric measurement model represented highly utilized measures of model fit, for exam-
within the context of confirmatory factor analysis. Circle ple, Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) and Root Mean
with “T” represents the latent variable, X1 to X5 represent
distinct items measuring “T,” l1 to l5 represent the factor
Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), to
loadings, and e1 to e5 represent the error terms gauge the extent to which the intended model
comports with the observed data (Bollen, 1989;
Brown, 2006; Cheung & Rensvold, 2002; Hu &
where a single factor is thought to underlie Bentler, 1999). Additionally, CFA is easily used
responses to the items; however, the model can within the context of structural equation models
be extended to multidimensional models by add- to help test the interrelations between numer-
ing additional factors into the model. ous variables of interest (Bollen, 1989) and to
The congeneric model can be evaluated within test not only the measurement models of the
the context of CFA (Bollen, 1989; Brown, 2006; measuring instruments but hypothesized relation-
Jöreskog, 1969, 1971; McDonald, 1985, 1999). ships between variables of interest.
Figure 38.1 shows a pictorial representation of A potentially useful application of CFA is for
the congeneric measurement model within the scoring student responses across items to obtain
context of CFA. The square boxes represent an overall score on the factor of interest. When
observed responses to items, and the circular items measuring a common factor, or common
variable represents the underlying latent trait of latent trait, provide different levels of measure-
interest that is hypothesized to be producing those ment precision then a simple summation of those
responses. The gi variables represent the factor item scores can result in less than optimal scoring
loadings on the true score, T, and the ei represents for the common factor (Lord, 1980). Additionally,
the error or residual term for each item i (please this would contradict some of the basic assump-
note the mean structure aspect of the model to tions of the classical theory of tests (Lord &
evaluate the intercept term, ai, has been left off Novick, 1968; McDonald, 1999). Obtaining opti-
Fig. 38.1 for simplicity but would be a part of the mal scoring is critically important for making
congeneric model (see Brown, 2006, for a practi- inferences about student scores and can help to
cal example and Jöreskog, 1969, 1971, for the reduce the random error associated with statistical
792 J. Betts

tests (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002), which categories. The RSM takes the ordered categories
is important for interpretation of research results. and transforms them into a linear scale (Bond &
The congeneric model evaluated within the con- Fox, 2001; Wright & Masters, 1982). One
text of CFA can utilize the measurement model to assumption of the model is that the category
differentially weight items and improve upon scores and thus the thresholds are similar across
simple summation. all items in the scale (Andersen, 1997; Wright,
1999; Wright & Masters, 1982).
The RSM is based on a probability model for
Item Response Theory for Modeling an individual choosing category k, for example,
Rating Scales Strongly Agree (SA), on an item as opposed to
choosing the adjacent category, k – 1, for exam-
For decades, CTT was the underlying methodol- ple, Agree (A). The RSM is intended to provide a
ogy for constructing psychological and educa- set of threshold parameters within each item that
tional tests (Crocker & Algina, 1986; Gulliksen, specifies the transition from one category to the
1950; Lord & Novick, 1968). Recent movement adjacent category (see Fig. 38.3 for an example
in the field of psychometrics has highlighted using a single item). The relative values of
some of the inherent problems with CTT and the the thresholds suggest the higher ratings indi-
emerging utility of item response theory (IRT) to cate higher levels of agreement with the items.
overcome those problems (Embretson, 1995; The RSM can be written as follows:
Embretson & Reise, 2000; Fischer & Molenaar,
1995; Hambleton, Swaminathan, & Rogers, p n,i,k e[ bn - (d i +t k )]
fnik = = (38.2)
1991; Thorndike, 1982; van der Linden & p n,i,k -1 + p n,i,k 1 + e[ bn - (d i +t k )]
Hambleton, 1997). IRT addresses many of the
same issues as those outlined above with the CFA which represents the probability of person n
implementation of the congeneric measurement responding to item i with category k is the ratio of
model. that person scoring in category k on item i to the
Analysis of item level data can be facilitated sum of the probability of scoring in category k
through the use of a particular set of item response and k – 1. Thus, the resulting form provides a per-
models derived from the work of Georg Rasch son estimate for their position on the variable
(Andersen, 1997; Bond & Fox, 2001; Rasch, being measured, bn, a scale value for each item,
1980). Numerous advantages of the Rasch model di, and the threshold values for the number of cat-
have been outlined (Keeves & Masters, 1999; egories of responses minus one, tk.
Wright & Masters, 1982). A specific model in Each item on the scale would provide infor-
the family of Rasch models (Wright, 1999) has mation about the ease for which a person high on
been developed to address issues with calibrat- engagement would respond positively to the item.
ing items structured in an ordered category For instance, given two items on the Peer Support
response manner called the Rating Scale Model at School scale (SEI; Appleton et al., 2006), the
(RSM; Andrich, 1978a, 1978b, 1999; Wright & item with the higher scale value would indicate
Masters, 1982). that students needed a higher level of feelings of
The RSM provides a method for scaling the engagement to respond positively. Put another
internal transition from one category response way, if a student were responding to two different
within an item to the subsequent category. Rather items, then they would potentially choose a lower
than simply providing an integer value to each category on the item with the higher scale value
subsequently higher category, each item has a set than they would on the item with a lower scale
of internal thresholds that provides information value. Thus, the higher the scale value for an
about the progression from each sequential cate- item, the more difficult it is to endorse a higher
gory, for example, from the Disagree to the Agree positive rating.
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 793

type of engagement measures three unique


Examples of the Measurement subtypes called Teacher-Student Relationships
Approaches: Evaluating the SEI (Teacher), Peer Support at School (Peer), and
Family Support for Learning (Family). The three
This section will provide an evaluation of the
affective engagement subtypes support the mea-
Student Engagement Instrument (SEI; Appleton
surement of the sense of belongingness through
et al., 2006) to highlight the measurement issues.
mutually positive relationships with important
First of all, CFA will be used to evaluate the
groups in the student’s life, for example, family,
intended 5-factor, correlated structure of the SEI
teachers, and peers. Methods of evaluating the
using a congeneric measurement model. This
sense of belongingness can be done through elic-
will provide cross-validation evidence for sup-
iting a student’s sense of the extent to which the
port of the intended measurement model underly-
important groups have respect and concern/car-
ing the SEI. Next, we will highlight the use of the
ing for them and their learning performance
RSM as a complementary method to the CFA for
(Battistich, Solomon, Watson, & Schaps, 1997;
calibrating the items for the 5-factor model and
Davis, 2003; Wentzel, 1997).
show some methods of evaluating the item statis-
The SEI is comprised of 33 items covering the
tics. Finally, we will highlight a potentially valu-
five subtypes, which we will also refer to as fac-
able measurement model called the bifactor
tors in the following analysis. There is no item
model that could potentially be useful in disen-
overlap between the scales. The five subtypes
tangling the complex issues related to measuring
were expected to correlate positively. A visual
student engagement.
representation of the SEI is provided in Fig. 38.2
All results were based on the same sample of
where the circles represent the latent factors, the
students (N = 1,796). The sample was obtained
boxes represent the sets of items for which stu-
from middle (N = 793) and high (N = 1,003)
dents respond on the rating scale, and the double-
school students attending schools in Upper
headed arrows represent the correlations between
Midwest. The CFA and bifactor models were
factors. The SEI uses the following ordered cat-
analyzed using MPlus 4.1 software (Muthén &
egories: Strongly Agree (SA), Agree (A),
Muthén, 1998–2005) and the RSM analysis used
Disagree (D), and Strongly Disagree (SD). Higher
WinSteps 3.69 (Linacre, 2007). Common metrics
scores represent higher levels of student engage-
for evaluating CFA models (Bollen, 1989; Brown,
ment for each subtype.
2006; Hu & Bentler, 1999) and RSM models
Research has provided evidence of good score
(Bond & Fox, 2001; Fischer & Molenaar, 1995;
reliability and validity (Appleton et al., 2006;
Linacre, 2007; Wright & Masters, 1982) were
Betts, Appleton, Reschly, Christenson, &
used to interpret the empirical fit of the data to the
Huebner, 2010; Reschly, Betts, & Appleton,
underlying models.
2011). Additionally, recent research has sug-
gested that the SEI provides invariant measure-
ments across students from 6th to 12th grade and
The Student Engagement Instrument also across genders (Betts et al., 2010). This sug-
gested that the factors being measured by the SEI
The Student Engagement Instrument (SEI: probably are similar across genders and grades
Appleton et al., 2006) was constructed to mea- and could be used to follow students across their
sure five important subtypes of student engage- middle to high school years. Recent research
ment falling within two broad types of student (Reschly et al., 2011) has provided initial evi-
engagement. The SEI measures two unique sub- dence of the strong relationship between motiva-
types of cognitive engagement called Control and tion and cognitive engagement along with
Relevance of School Work (Control) and Future evidence for score validity to important outcomes
Aspirations and Goals (Aspire). The affective thought to be related to engagement.
794 J. Betts

Fig. 38.2 Student


engagement instrument
measurement model with
correlated factors. The
latent engagement factors
are represented by the
circles, the set of items
measuring the latent
factors are represented by
the boxes, and the
double-headed arrows
represent the correlations
between factors

Cross-Validation of the SEI Using only the one intended factor with no cross-
the Congeneric Measurement Model loadings. The first item on each scale had its
unstandardized factor loading set to unity to set
Figure 38.2 is a visualization of the expected fac- the scale for that factor, but all other factor load-
tor structure of the SEI. To evaluate the extent to ings were freely estimated. Additionally, the cor-
which this model holds for the sample of middle relations between factors were freely estimated.
and high school students, a CFA model was spec- Results suggested good to excellent fit of the
ified to test the correspondence between the data to the model, c2(485) = 6839.98, CFI = .92,
intended measurement model based on the theo- TLI = .92, RMSEA = .047, and SRMR = .044. The
retical structure of the SEI and the observed probability that the RMSEA was less than or
response data. In Fig. 38.2, the boxes represent equal to .05 was >.99 suggesting that the RMSEA
the set of items measuring the factors (shown in was probably below the level suggesting excel-
circles); however, rather than simply summing lent model fit. All the fit indexes suggested that
the items in the set to get a total score for each the intended model fit the observed data very
subtypes, the relationship was structured to be well and could probably not be rejected. Thus,
like that in Fig. 38.1 showing the congeneric this cross-validation sample provided evidence
measurement model. Thus, each factor in of the stability of the SEI in a new sample from
Fig. 38.2 has the measurement model structure of the intended population of use. Table 38.1 pro-
Fig. 38.1, and each factor was specified to be cor- vides the reliability estimates for the factors (the
related (represented by the double-headed arrows) first entry on the diagonal) and the correlations
with all other factors. For this model, simple between the factors (below the diagonal on the
structure was enforced with all items measuring lower triangle). These reliability estimates and
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 795

Table 38.1 Reliabilities and correlations between factors say a value of −2.00, then the most likely category
in the 5-factor SEI model response for the student would be within the
Teacher Control Peer Aspire Family Strongly Disagree. However, a student with a
Teacher .74/.84 .64 .47 .43 .47 higher level of engagement with a score of 1.75
Control .70 .70/.79 .38 .56 .51 would be more inclined to agree with the
Peer .49 .39 .76/.76 .39 .41 statement.
Aspire .48 .71 .41 .80/.70 .52 To evaluate the fit of the data to the intended
Family .54 .62 .47 .61 .79/.67 measurement model, mean-square-based statis-
Note: IRT model results are above the diagonal, and tics called Infit and Outfit were utilized. Common
CFA results are below the diagonal. Reliability esti-
interpretations of these fit statistics suggest that
mates are given in bold on the diagonal with CFA results
before the IRT results values between 0.5 and 1.5 are deemed produc-
tive for measurement, values less than 0.5 or
correlation levels are similar to those found in between 1.5 and 2.0 are less productive but not
previous research on the SEI (Appleton et al. degrading to measurement, but values over 2.0
2006; Betts et al., 2010), providing evidence of are deemed to degrade the measurement system
score stability and interrelations between factors (Linacre, 2007). Using these heuristics as lower
to be similar in this sample when compared to and upper bound magnitudes, all items appear to
other samples. provide useful measurement of the intended fac-
tors. Additionally, all the items show a good level
of relationship between item scores and the total
Utilizing the Rating Scale Model score for the factor with all item-total correla-
for the SEI tions (given in the last column) except two below
.60. The two items with item-total correlations of
We calibrated the items for each factor indepen- .58 also show higher levels on both Infit and
dently for the RSM in order to derive five inde- Outfit statistics, but not high enough to be con-
pendent scales. Table 38.2 provides the item level sidered to degrade measurement.
statistics for all 33 items. The Measure variable
in Table 38.2 provides the calibrated value for
each item, with the lower values suggesting Utilizing the Bifactor Model for the SEI
somewhat easier ability for respondents to make
positive responses. For instance on the Teacher- The bifactor model was originally developed as a
Student Relationships scale, the item “At my model for exploring individual differences in the
school, teachers care about students” has a value domain of intelligence assessment (Holzinger &
of −0.44 which suggested it was somewhat easier Swineford, 1937; Thomson, 1948) and has
to agree with than the item asking, “The school recently been incorporated into a general psycho-
rules are fair,” which had a value of 0.74. metric model for educational and psychological
Figure 38.3 provides an example of the measurements (Gibbons & Hedeker, 1992).
response model for the “I feel safe at school” item Figure 38.4 provides a visualization of the model
from the Teacher-Student Relationships scale. using the SEI and shows that each set of items
The vertical axis is a probability metric and the written to measure a specific and unique subtype
horizontal axis is the person ability score, or in in the boxes is caused by two factors, a general
this case, a linear measure of affective engage- and group level factor. The group level factors in
ment related to teacher-student context. The fig- this example are the five SEI subtype/factors that
ure shows the relationship between levels of represent specific aspects of engagement. The
engagement and the probability of answering with general factor is seen as a factor underlying all
one of the four categorical answer choices. For the items and could be interpreted as a general
instance, if one had a low level of engagement, engagement variable.
796 J. Betts

Table 38.2 RSM item calibration values, fit statistics, and item-total correlations by scale for the five-factor SEI model
Item-
Items Measure Infit Outfit total
Teacher-student relationships (teacher)
Overall, adults at my school treat students fairly 0.08 0.89 0.87 .74
Adults at my school listen to the students 0.13 0.91 0.89 .73
At my school, teachers care about students −0.44 0.75 0.71 .77
My teachers are there for me when I need them −0.22 0.85 0.83 .73
The school rules are fair 0.74 1.22 1.25 .67
Overall, my teachers are open and honest with me −0.41 0.89 0.85 .73
I enjoy talking to the teachers here 0.08 1.00 0.99 .72
I feel safe at school −0.46 1.36 1.32 .63
Most teachers at my school are interested in me as a person, not just as a student 0.51 1.07 1.08 .70
Control and relevance of school work (control)
The tests in my classes do a good job of measuring what I am able to do 0.37 1.00 0.99 .64
Most of what is important to know you learn in school 0.14 0.88 0.90 .66
The grades in my classes do a good job of measuring what I am able to do 0.19 1.18 1.16 .66
What I am learning in my classes will be important in my future −0.87 0.94 0.90 .68
After finishing my schoolwork, I check it over to see if it is correct 1.09 0.96 1.01 .63
When I do schoolwork, I check to see whether I understand what I am doing −0.19 0.84 0.84 .65
Learning is fun because I get better at something 0.12 0.93 0.92 .67
When I do well in school, it is because I work hard −1.31 0.98 0.96 .62
I feel like I have a say about what happens to me at school 0.46 1.29 1.31 .58
Peer support at school (peer)
Other students at school care about me 0.64 0.79 0.75 .81
Students at my school are there for me when I need them 1.05 0.91 0.92 .73
Other students here like me the way I am 0.07 0.97 0.92 .75
I enjoy talking to the students here −0.87 1.07 1.07 .72
Students here respect what I have to say 1.62 0.99 1.05 .73
I have some friends at school −2.52 1.31 1.40 .58
Future aspirations and goals (aspire)
I plan to continue my education following high school −0.31 0.87 0.87 .79
Going to school after high school is important 0.00 0.99 0.97 .76
School is important for achieving my future goals 0.32 0.87 0.87 .79
My education will create many future opportunities for me 0.05 1.05 1.06 .74
I am hopeful about my future −0.06 1.18 1.15 .70
Family support for learning (family)
My family/guardian(s) are there for me when I need them −0.57 1.06 1.01 .76
When I have problems at school, my family/guardian(s) are willing to help me 0.26 0.83 0.80 .83
When something good happens at school, my family/guardian(s) want 0.88 1.01 1.03 .79
to know about it
My family/guardian(s) want me to keep trying when things are tough at school −0.57 1.10 1.08 .72

It is also noticeable from the model that the motivation is a necessary condition for engage-
group factors and the general factor are uncorre- ment, then some sense of motivation should
lated. This model assumes the general factor underlie all aspects of engagement, as the student
extracts what is common to all the items on the would be motivated and goal directed to act in a
scale and then extracts the group factors from the manner that suggests engagement. Therefore, it
residual correlations between the items measur- might be hypothesized that a common factor
ing a specific engagement factor. The utility of underlying all specific and potentially unique
this type of a model might help to parse out some aspects of engagement would be some type or
of the concern about the relationship between level of motivation. The use of the bifactor model
engagement and motivation. For instance, if could potentially be used to evaluate a model
1
Strongly Disagree
0.9 Disagree
Agree
0.8 Strongly Agree

0.7
Category Probability

0.6

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
−6 −5 −4 −3 −2 −1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
Rasch Metric

Fig. 38.3 Category probabilities and the smoothed response curves for item “I feel safe at school”

Fig. 38.4 Visualization of the bifactor model for the SEI Note: (1) The error terms for the latent variables have
where the general factor is a latent factor underlying been left off the figure for clarity, and (2) there is no
responses to all the items and the group factors, control to correlation between any of the factors, as the bifactor
peer, are group factors that explain the variance over and model assumes the factors are independent and therefore
above the general factor for independent sets of items. uncorrelated
798 J. Betts

Fig. 38.5 Hypothetical use of the bifactor model to measure relationships between important types of engagement and
social relations

such as this by entertaining the hypothesis that type of engagement that were related to a similar
the general factor underlying all the engagement context or subtype/component of the broad
variables is a proxy for motivation and therefore engagement type. For instance, there might be a
would be more highly related to motivation than specific factor related to parents that affect cogni-
the group factors, which would be more specific tive engagement and a specific factor that affects
aspects of engagement. Additionally, the model emotional engagement independently, but, as
could be used to support a measurement model both are related to the same parental context,
that had, for instance, a general factor related to there might be some level of relationship between
one of the broad types of engagement such as the two factors over and above that relating sim-
cognitive engagement and group factors related ply to engagement. We will explore a model sim-
to specific aspects of cognitive engagement. ilar to this with the SEI data as represented in
Furthermore, this model could be extended Fig. 38.6 but without the restriction of having
beyond just measurement models to testing rela- similar group level variables across cognitive and
tionships between variables of interest. Figure affective engagement.
38.5 provides a possible approach to generalizing
the bifactor model to measure two broad types of Initial Bifactor Results
engagement, cognitive and affective, along with Using the bifactor structure in Fig. 38.4, the data
three specific contexts related to important social were analyzed to evaluate the extent to which the
relationships, with parents, teachers, and peers. SEI fits this intended model. To specify the model,
Here, the bifactor model could represent the rela- all initial items on a factor were set to unity to set
tionships with the cognitive and affective engage- the scale, and all correlations were fixed at zero to
ment factors independently, and then allow for model independent factors. Results suggested
there to be a correlations between these general there was evidence of good fit of the data to
engagement factors. Furthermore, it could allow the model, c2(462) = 6054.19, CFI = .93, TLI = .92,
for correlations between the group factors of each RMSEA = .045, and SRMR = .047. The probability
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 799

Fig. 38.6 Extended bifactor model for analyzing the SEI. for cognitive are control and aspire. In this model,
The affective and cognitive factors act as general factors there are correlations between the general and group fac-
from the bifactor model. The group factors for affective tors of affective with the general and group factors of
are family, teacher, and peer, and the group factors cognitive

that the RMSEA was less than or equal to .05 was multidimensional construct or, as the bifactor
>.99 suggesting that the RMSEA was probably model suggests, some space between a single
below the level suggesting excellent model fit. dimension and multidimensional?
Thus, a model that posits a general engage-
ment factor underlying item responses and unique Extended Bifactor Model
factors related to the five group factors appeared Evaluating the model for the SEI represented
to represent the structure in the data rather well. in Fig. 38.6 suggested good fit of the data to this
These results suggest that it could be possible to model also, c2(451) = 5658.19, CFI = .93, TLI = .92,
conceive of an engagement model with a broad RMSEA = .044, and SRMR = .074. The probabil-
general factor accounting for variation in student ity that the RMSEA was less than or equal to .05
responses across various important areas of was >.99 suggesting that the RMSEA was prob-
engagement types, but also allow for the mea- ably below the level suggesting excellent model
surement of those specific factors of interest. This fit. While the model fit very well, there were
could also provide initial evidence for a few some interesting results from the correlations
hypotheses: (1) Is the general factor really a fac- between the factors (see Table 38.3). Four of the
tor related to motivation that underlies all impe- 12 correlations were not significantly different
tuses to enact in goal-oriented behavior?; or (2) Is from zero. It is important to note that the correla-
there really a single engagement factor that tions here will be different, in most cases, from
broadly defines engagement and that the types the correlations presented earlier in Table 38.1.
of engagement, as suggested by the modifiers The reason for this is that the correlations between
cognitive, behavioral, or emotional, are spe- the group factors in this model are computed after
cific instances of that broad factor expressed in the variation related to the general factor has been
different contexts?; or (3) Is engagement really a extracted.
800 J. Betts

Table 38.3 Correlations between factors in the bifactor The results from this extension of the bifactor
model model provides for various theoretical positions
Affective Teacher Peer Family to be evaluated. Only a few have been outlined
Cognitive .41 .12 -- .41 here briefly. Not only would the significant cor-
Control .53 .10 -- -- relations be interesting to follow up but also so
Aspire .08 -- .17 .12 would the nonsignificant relationships as these
Note: Correlations significant at the p < .05 level are might suggest truly orthogonal variables. More-
given, and “--” indicates nonsignificant correlations. No over, nonsignificant relationships can be used to
adjustments were made for multiple comparisons
help elucidate theory. For instance, the lack of
relationship between Teacher-Student Relation-
The general factors for affective and cognitive ships and Control and Relevance of School Work
engagement were found to have a significant cor- has potentially interesting implications. However,
relation (r = .41). Thus, whatever the general any relationships found in the present research
source of variation underlying the cognitive would need further evaluation and more rigorous
factor and the variation underlying the affective replication in order to move beyond conjecture
factor only have moderate levels of correlation. and would need to be contextualized within a
This might suggest that the affective and cogni- useful theoretical framework.
tive types have some moderate level of relation-
ship but also have some distinct differences.
Looking at the group variables, the largest Conclusion
correlation was found between the general
Affective factor and the Control and Relevance This chapter highlighted some of the basic issues
of School Work group factor of cognitive domain related to measuring student engagement. While
(r = .53). This might suggest that there is some the study of student engagement is just begin-
correspondence between emotional sense of well- ning, this line of research can profit from decades
being and positive relationship to school when of inquiry in areas of psychology that have direct
the student feels in control of their work and sees bearing on three main types of engagement: emo-
the relevance for the work in which they are tional, cognitive, and behavioral. The chapter has
engaged. There was also a moderately high cor- also hinted at some potentially interesting ave-
relation (r = .41) between the general Cognitive nues of future research. The study of engagement
factor and the Family Support group factor of the could bear great fruit as it is not only an impor-
affective domain. This might suggest that some tant variable in educational and psychological
sense of support from one’s family helps to elicit research but it also has potentially interesting
a sense of focus and willingness to exert cogni- intersections with other areas of well-established
tive effort to tackle difficult or new skills. research lines.
The Peer Support group factor was only At present, the measurement of engagement is
slightly related to Future Aspirations (r = .17). still inchoate and thus provides unparalleled
This suggests that after partitioning out the gen- opportunity to explore the structure of the multi-
eral Affective factor from the Peer variable and farious nature of engagement. Appropriate mea-
the Cognitive factor from Future Aspirations, surement is vital to support the ongoing
there was some level of residual correlation investigation of engagement, as poor measure-
between these two group variables. This might ment leads to poor and potentially difficult to
suggest that as students feel more support from interpret results. Some possible research-related
their peers in school, the more they aspire to scenarios within the context of developing useful
maintain a commitment to furthering their educa- and meaningful measures were presented. It is
tion. Thus, a sense of well-being with peers might quite likely that the development of appropriate
engender an aspirational outlook for future learn- measures of the different types and subtypes of
ing and educational achievement. engagement can also foster a more coherent theory
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 801

of engagement as the abstract must contend with Overall, the study of student engagement is an
reality, but such is the nature of science. important venture in educational and psychologi-
Two general models for constructing and eval- cal research. Many open questions still exist, and
uating measures of engagement were highlighted. basic definitions and relationships between
The congeneric measurement model was dis- important variables remain open for investiga-
played within the context of confirmatory factor tion. However, measurement of the variables of
analysis (CFA). This model provided an exten- interest is a necessity before any quantitative
sion of the classical theory of test and is robust to research program can move forward, and appro-
violations of the stringent assumptions of the priate measurement is needed to ensure that the
classical test theory. Using CFA has a direct ben- research is based on a firm foundation. Hopefully
efit of numerous and historically documented this chapter will provide insights or inspirations
approaches to evaluating model fit which can be to engender the development of new and impor-
beneficial for evaluating expected results. Modern tant measures of student engagement to move the
item response theory (IRT) was used to exem- field forward and beyond.
plify a methodology for calibrating items on a
rating scale to provide a linear measure. This
chapter used only one of a number of potentially Appendix
useful models for rating scale data called the rat-
ing scale model. Glossary of Terms
These statistical methods for evaluating
engagement instruments can be used produc- Bifactor model a model devised in early re-
tively together. While the above analysis was search into individual differences in intelligence.
done to show the basics of each approach, com- The model posits that there is a general factor that
bining the CFA and IRT approaches can be com- underlies responses and that there are also uncor-
plementary. For instance, the CFA model can related group factors that account for substantial
help to provide information about the overall residual variation between the responses.
measurement model expected from the instru- Bijective is a functional relationship that places
ment, and the IRT model can help to identify all elements of one set into a one-to-one corre-
items that appear to degrade measurement. Thus, spondence with all of the elements of another set.
the ongoing development of a measuring instru- Confirmatory factor analysis a robust and
ment can use both approaches in a reciprocal and well-researched method of analyzing the rela-
recursive manner to evaluate the items and the tionship between observed variables and latent
scales being constructed. variables where restrictions are placed on the
model to test or provide confirmatory evidence
This chapter also described a potentially use-
of latent structure.
ful CFA model called the bifactor model. Not
Congeneric measurement an extension of the
only can this model provide an interesting
classical theory of tests that allowed for relaxation
approach to instrument development, it can also
of restrictions inherent in classical test theory and
provide a unique method for evaluating complex elucidated the relationship between confirmatory
relationships. Two different types of models were factor analysis and test analysis.
highlighted here: the basic bifactor model for all Cross-loadings when items have significant
five subtypes of engagement on the SEI and an positive factor loadings across two or more latent
extension of the model thought to describe the factors.
relationships between two general types and their Factor loadings the relationship between an
related subtypes of engagement. This type of observed variable and the hypothetical latent
modeling can also be useful in disentangling the variable.
relationship between cognitive engagement and Fit indexes statistical tests that evaluate the
motivation and would constitute an interesting extent to which observed data conform to an in-
line of research. tended model structure. Infit and Outfit are two
802 J. Betts

examples of fit indices used to examine the Rasch Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Watson, M., & Schaps, E.
models. (1997). Caring school communities. Educational
Psychologist, 32, 137–151.
Item response theory a modern method of test
Betts, J. E., Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., Christenson, S.
scoring that utilizes an underlying probability L., & Huebner, E. S. (2010). A study of the factorial
model to place examinee ability and item diffi- invariance of the Student Engagement Instrument
culty on the same scale. (SEI): Results from middle and high school students.
Latent variable a variable that is not directly School Psychology Quarterly, 25, 84–93.
Bloom, B. (Ed.). (1985). Developing talent in young peo-
observable. A variable hypothesized to underlie
ple. New York: Ballantine Books.
responses to sets of items or variables, often des- Blumenfeld, P., Kempler, T., & Krajcik, J. (2006).
ignated as a hypothetical construct. Motivation and cognitive engagement in learning envi-
Rating scale a method of obtaining and scoring ronments. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge hand-
responses to items that have response categories book of learning sciences (pp. 475–488). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
that represent increases in the magnitude of the
Bollen, K. (1989). Structural equations with latent vari-
variable those items define. ables. New York: Wiley.
Score reliability stability or consistency of Bond, T., & Fox, C. (2001). Applying the Rasch model:
scores on a test. Fundamental measurement in the human sciences.
Score validity the ongoing process of gathering Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brown, T. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for
evidence to support the inferences from scores
applied research. New York: The Guildford Press.
and all intended interpretations. Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. (1994). Reinforcement, reward,
and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis. Review of
Educational Research, 64, 363–423.
Catalano, R., Kosterman, R., Hawkins, J., Newcomb, M.,
References & Abbott, R. (1996). Modeling the etiology of adoles-
cent substance use: A test of the social development
Alexander, K., Entwisle, D., & Horsey, C. (1997). From model. Journal of Drug Issues, 26, 429–455.
first grade forwards: Early foundations of high school Cheung, G., & Rensvold, R. (2002). Evaluating goodness-
dropouts. Sociology of Education, 70, 87–107. of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance.
Andersen, E. (1997). The rating scale model. In W. van Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233–255.
der Linden & R. Hambleton (Eds.), Handbook of mod- Christenson, S., Reschly, A., Appleton, J., Berman, S.,
ern item response theory (pp. 67–84). New York: Spanjers, D., & Varro, P. (2008). Best practices in fos-
Springer. tering student engagement. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes
Andrich, D. (1978a). A rating formulation for ordered (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology (5th ed.,
response categories. Psychometrika, 43, 561–573. pp. 1099–1119). Bethesda, MD: National Association
Andrich, D. (1978b). Applications of a psychometric rating of School Psychologists.
model to ordered categories which are scored with suc- Crocker, L., & Algina, J. (1986). Introduction to classical
cessive integers. Applied Psychological Measurement, and modern test theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart and
2, 581–594. Winston.
Andrich, D. (1999). Rating scale analysis. In G. Masters Davis, H. (2003). Conceptualizing the role and influence
& J. Keeves (Eds.), Advances in measurement in edu- of student-teacher relationships in children’s social
cational research and assessment (pp. 110–121). New and cognitive development. Educational Psychologist,
York: Pergamon. 38, 207–234.
Appleton, J., Christenson, S., & Furlong, M. (2008). DeVellis, R. (2003). Scale development: Theory and
Student engagement with schools: Critical conceptual applications (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology Downing, S., & Haladyna, T. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of
in the Schools, 54, 369–386. test development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Appleton, J., Christenson, S., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. Embretson, S. (1995). The new rules of measurement.
(2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological engage- Psychological Assessment, 8, 341–349.
ment: Validation of the Student Engagement Instrument. Embretson, S., & Reise, S. (2000). Item response theory
Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445. for psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Balfanz, R., Herzog, L., & Mac Iver, D. (2007). Preventing Finn, J. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of
student disengagement and keeping students on the Educational Research, 59(2), 117–142.
graduation path in urban middle-grades schools: Early Finn, J., & Rock, D. (1997). Academic success among
identification and effective intervention. Educational students at risk for school failure. Journal of Applied
Psychologist, 42, 223–235. Psychology, 82, 221–234.
Bartholomew, D., & Knott, M. (1999). Latent variable Fischer, G., & Molenaar, I. (Eds.). (1995). Rasch models:
models and factor analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Foundations, recent developments, and applications.
Oxford Press. New York: Springer.
38 Issues and Methods in the Measurement of Student Engagement… 803

Franken, R. (2001). Human motivation (5th ed.). Pacific Lord, F., & Novick, M. with A. Birnbaum. (1968).
Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Statistical theories of mental test scores. Reading,
Fredricks, J., Blumenfeld, P., & Paris, A. (2004). School MA: Addison-Wesley.
engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evi- McDonald, R. (1985). Factor analysis and related meth-
dence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59–109. ods. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gibbons, R., & Hedeker, D. (1992). Full-information item McDonald, R. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment.
bi-factor analysis. Psychometrika, 57, 423–436. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gulliksen, H. (1950). Theory of mental tests. New York: Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (1998–2005). Mplus statistical
Wiley. analysis with latent variables: User’s guide. Los
Guo, J., Hawkins, J., Hill, K., & Abbott, R. (2001). Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Childhood and adolescent predictors of alcohol abuse Rasch, G. (1980). Probabilistic models for some intelli-
and dependence in young adulthood. Journal of gence and attainment tests. Chicago: The University
Studies on Alcohol, 62, 754–762. of Chicago Press.
Hambleton, R., Swaminathan, H., & Rogers, H. (1991). Reschly, A., Betts, J., & Appleton, J. (2011). An examina-
Fundamentals of item response theory. Newbury Park, tion of the validity of two measures of student engage-
CA: Sage. ment. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Harman, H. (1967). Modern factor analysis (2nd ed.). Shadish, W., Cook, T., & Campbell, D. (2002).
Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for gen-
Hawkins, J., & Weis, J. (1985). The social development eralized causal inference. New York: Houghton
model: An integrated approach to delinquency preven- Mifflin.
tion. Journal of Primary Prevention, 6, 73–97. Spector, P. (1992). Summated rating scale construction:
Holzinger, K., & Swineford, F. (1937). The bi-factor An introduction. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
method. Psychometrika, 2, 41–54. Thomson, G. (1948). The factorial analysis of human
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes ability. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria Thorndike, R. (1982). Applied psychometrics.
versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, Lawrenceville, NJ: Houghton Mifflin.
6, 1–55. van der Linden, W., & Hambleton, R. (Eds.). (1997).
Jensen, A. (1998). The g-factor: The science of mental Handbook of modern item response theory. New York:
ability. Westport, CT: Praeger. Springer.
Jöreskog, K. (1969). A general approach to confirmatory Wainer, H., & Braun, H. (Eds.). (1988). Test validity.
maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
34, 183–202. Wentzel, K. (1997). Student motivation in middle school:
Jöreskog, K. (1971). Statistical analysis of sets of conge- The role of perceived pedagogical caring. Journal of
neric tests. Psychometrika, 36, 109–133. Educational Psychology, 89, 411–419.
Kane, M. (2006). Validation. In R. Brennan (Ed.), Wentzel, K., & Wigfield, A. (2007). Motivational inter-
Educational measurement (4th ed., pp. 17–64). ventions that work: Themes and remaining issues.
Westport, CT: Praeger. Educational Psychologist, 42, 261–271.
Keeves, J., & Masters, G. (1999). Introduction. In Wilson, M. (2005). Constructing measures: An item
G. Masters & J. Keeves (Eds.), Advances in mea- response modeling approach. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
surement in educational research and assessment Erlbaum.
(pp. 1–19). New York: Pergamon. Wright, B. (1999). Rasch measurement models. In
Kleinginna, P., & Kleinginna, A. (1981). A categorized G. Masters & J. Keeves (Eds.), Advances in mea-
list of motivation definitions, with suggestions for a surement in educational research and assessment
consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, (pp. 85–97). New York: Pergamon.
263–291. Wright, B., & Masters, G. (1982). Rating scale analysis:
Linacre, J. (2007). A user’s guide to WINSTEPS. Author. Rasch measurement. Chicago, IL: MESA.
Lord, F. (1980). Applications of item response theory to Yen, W., & Fitzpatrick, A. (2006). Item response theory.
practical testing problems. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence In R. Brennan (Ed.), Educational measurement (4th
Erlbaum. ed., pp. 111–153). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Part V Commentary: Possible
New Directions in the Measurement 39
of Student Engagement

Karen M. Samuelsen

Abstract
Karen Samuelsen, a respected researcher and methodologist, provided
commentary on the chapters in Part V of this volume. She described the dif-
ficulty of measuring latent constructs like student engagement. She argued
for greater complexity in study designs and hypotheses to account for the
complex nature of proposed relationships between engagement, contexts,
and outcomes. She provided examples of how various statistical methods
(Structural Equation Modeling, Differential Item Functioning) may be used
to address some of the current measurement issues in engagement and out-
lined several areas of future research to advance the study of engagement.

Physical scientists would undoubtedly be frus- explicating the said construct, pilot test the instru-
trated if faced with inexact measurement instru- ment, run statistical tests to investigate the infer-
ments or inaccessible subjects. As an example, ences that can be drawn from it, and so on. Of
let us consider NASA scientists attempting to course, those items will not cover every aspect of
measure a crater on a distant planet. They would that psychological variable, no matter how care-
rail about the inaccuracy of the measurement of fully they are written. So what we are left with is
the depth of that crater, but they would have little a measurement of something related to our origi-
doubt that it was depth they were attempting to nal construct but not exactly identical. The cor-
measure. The only thing in question would be the relation between what we set out to measure and
level of accuracy they would be able to achieve. what we end up measuring is limited by the qual-
The measurement of a psychological construct ity of our definition of that construct, with ill-
is a much trickier business, one that is so laden defined constructs being measured more poorly
with error it is a wonder we ever attempt it at all. than those that are well defined.
The first step in this process should be simple; It is not really simple at all to define a con-
it is to fully define what we are attempting to struct in full since that definition often includes
measure. Once that is done, we can write items multiple dimensions that may be related to each
other or to other constructs. Unfortunately, this is
a step that is often glossed over by researchers
K. Samuelsen (*)
Department of Educational Psychology and Instructional
who think that the definition of their construct
Technology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA must be evident to all. Given that construct defi-
e-mail: ksam@uga.edu nition is so important, I was particularly gratified

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 805


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_39, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
806 K.M. Samuelsen

to see the extent to which all of the authors in this need to get high grades or their need for cognitive
section explained their conception of engagement stimulation. Similarly, students who are not cog-
and clarified how this construct differs from nitively engaged but have high levels of affective
motivation. Still, further discussion of construct engagement might also evidence the same high
definition seems relevant, so part of this chapter “scores” on those behavioral outcomes because
will be devoted to possible explorations that they want to please their parents or teachers.
might help provide more nuance to our defini- This same rationale could hold true for the dif-
tions of engagement. ferent sources of affective engagement: teacher-
Once we have adequately defined our con- student relationships, peer support at school, and
struct, we give the items developed to a sample family support for learning. Students with high
from the population of interest. At that point, levels of peer and family support might be able to
more error creeps into our measurement. The make up for poor teacher-student relationships.
people in our sample may respond inaccurately Likewise, the three measures of cognitive engage-
due to construct-irrelevant factors such as ment encompassing control and relevance of
difficulty reading or a different understanding of school work, future aspirations and goals, and
certain ideas. This commentary will address the intrinsic motivation could interact in a compen-
issue of construct-irrelevant item-specific factors, satory fashion.
particularly as those pertain to differential item Whatever the nature of the compensatory
function. More systematic issues such as social structure, if such relationships exist, then reports
desirability, acquiescence, and central tendency such as the one provided by Appleton (2012)
may also impact our measurement precision. must be considerably more complex. It might not
Social desirability (the propensity to respond in a be appropriate to say that a student is at high risk
manner that would be deemed appropriate by because they evidence low levels of peer support
most people) and acquiescence (the propensity to at school. If the other forms of affective engage-
answer positively to all questions) will not be ment were high enough to compensate for low
discussed in this commentary. However, central levels of peer support, then that student might not
tendency will be addressed as it pertains to the be at high risk after all. On the other hand, there
investigation of differential item function. may be a threshold for peer support below which
the other forms of affective engagement cannot
compensate, in which case very low peer support
Further Definition of the Construct is an important indicator. The point here is not to
suggest precisely what the compensatory rela-
It occurs to me that the definition of engagement tionship is but rather to note that the possibility of
embodied in the Student Engagement Instrument such relationships means that the model for build-
(SEI: Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, ing and refining a dynamic, engagement-based
2006) and other similar instruments may be more assessment-to-intervention report might need to
complex than is currently being modeled. be more nuanced to isolate students who are truly
Specifically, it seems that there could be compen- at high risk. As Appleton (2012) noted, it is cru-
satory relationships that are useful to consider. cial to ensure that students designated for an
I will address two of these. First, consider the intervention actually are in need of that interven-
potential for a compensatory relationship between tion so that resources are not incorrectly and inef-
affective and cognitive engagement, particularly ficiently allocated.
as these relate to the behavioral manifestations of These sorts of structures can be modeled using
engagement (e.g., days present at school, lack of compensatory IRT (item response theory) models,
negative incidents reported). Students with high wherein the contributions of the different dimen-
levels of cognitive engagement and a low sense sions are additive. These models are sometimes
of affective engagement would still evidence high applied to achievement tests where there are mul-
levels on those behavioral outcomes due to their tiple ways to solve problems. For example, in
39 Part V Commentary: Possible New Directions in the Measurement of Student Engagement 807

mathematics there could be questions that can The results from any psychological instrument
either be solved using addition or multiplication. In are really just blurry snapshots of what is occur-
that case, students who do not know how to multiply ring at any given time. When constructs are sta-
can still respond correctly by adding. Students who ble, we expect to see similar snapshots at different
respond incorrectly would be those who are low in time points. The problem in this case is that it
terms of both addition and multiplication profi- seems unrealistic to expect the constructs to be
ciency. It is important to note that the inferences stable. For example, it would be natural for
made about students may be incorrect if a unidi- teacher-student relationships to vary greatly, even
mensional model is applied in a situation that is within the same school year. Students may
actually multidimensional (Ackerman, 1992). become disenchanted with a teacher if their
Compensatory structures, should they exist, grades are not good, or they may become more
could also be modeled within a structural equation attached over time. In either case, the relation-
modeling (SEM) framework; however, certain ship is changing, meaning student responses on
assumptions would need to be made. One such the SEI would change. To create change reports
assumption could be that students who were high that are meaningful, it is necessary to quantify
on affective engagement but low on cognitive the difference between normal and problematic
engagement evidenced the same overall level of change and not just to classify problems based on
engagement as those who were high on the cogni- a norm-referenced interpretation of change.
tive but low on affective. If this were the case, an Appleton noted that, “The current student-focused
interaction model could be employed with the levels reports of academic year changes in engagement
of affective and cognitive engagement having both did not account for measurement error and fur-
direct and interactive effects on engagement out- ther development is needed to include that con-
comes (Bandalos, 2011, personal communication). sideration.” I would concur and add that
In this situation, the interactive effects would, in measurement error may be more complex and
essence, be compensatory. If this assumption can- come from more sources than one might think.
not be made, then it would be necessary to
categorize both the levels of affective and cogni-
tive engagement. From those categories, groups Differential Item Function
representing the different combinations (e.g., low
cognitive/medium affective or high cognitive/low Two of the chapters in this section touch on cul-
affective) can be formed, and hypotheses can tural diversity and the issue of whether the infer-
be tested regarding each combination. Though ences one can draw about all respondents will
making these assumptions seems less than opti- be correct. Fredericks and McColskey (2012,
mal, the ability to use SEM and model causality is pg. 779) stated, “If measures of engagement behave
a compelling reason to want to try this method. differently by race, SES, gender, and grade, and
One final consideration in terms of fully expli- these differences are not taken into account, com-
cating the construct of engagement is in consid- parisons in the level of engagement or effects
ering the stability of the different dimensions across groups are invalid” (Glanville &
over time. Questions such as the following should Wildhagen, 2007). Betts (2012) also discusses
be investigated. Precisely how much variation in the idea of validity and introduces the concepts of
the different dimensions is “normal”? Is there measurement invariance and differential item
more variability in some than in the others? Is function as one source of validity evidence.
this variation consistent across groups (gender, Though both chapters introduce this topic, nei-
ethnic, socioeconomic status, etc.)? An under- ther gives it the attention I believe it merits.
standing of this variation is vital if one is to create A study of the equivalence of the Rosenberg
and use change reports similar to the one pre- Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) across cultures, which
sented by Appleton in this volume. compared responses of almost 17,000 respondents
808 K.M. Samuelsen

across 53 countries (Schmitt & Allik, 2005), factor structure (3 affective and 2 cognitive), latent
provides a case in point. Though the authors found factor relationships, and score reliability across
the factor structure of the RSES to be replicated grades 6–12 and genders. However, these authors
across most countries, they cautioned that differ- found issues when examining the item-level
ences between cultures can make using that scale results in terms of a cross loading and more com-
problematic under certain conditions. Specifically, plicated error structure. Betts et. al (2010) stated
they noted that there is a tendency for individuals (pg. 91), “These results portend the need for fur-
from some cultures to report lower levels of self- ther replication research to examine the extent to
esteem on the negatively worded items on the which these deviations from expectations exist in
scale, meaning their overall levels of self-esteem different samples or whether it was just a byproduct
will be underreported. The authors also uncov- of the present sample.” I believe they are correct
ered a neutral response bias in which people from but would add that their findings could be caused
collectivist cultures tended to avoid the extremes by cultural differences in their samples.
of the rating scale as compared to those from Backing this empirical research on noninvari-
more individualistic cultures. Perhaps the most ance is the literature on culture and engagement
important finding was that the two dimensions of (for an overview, see Bingham and Okagaki,
self-esteem, typically self-competence and self- 2012). Cultural discontinuity theory posits that
esteem, seemed to vary across cultures, once children raised in a distinct, minority culture
again in a manner that was related to the indi- often find themselves in schools where the values
vidual/collectivist nature of the culture. As an of the majority culture are endorsed at the
example, Portes (2011) reported that when the expense of their own. According to the cultural
RSES is administered to Japanese females, a two- discontinuity theory, this dissonance may make
factor solution emerges wherein the one factor is the child feel the need to choose one culture over
again self-respect, but the other factor is negative the other, thereby making it impossible to be
in nature. That second factor is characterized by successful in both cultures. Cultural ecological
feelings like uselessness, incompetence, praise theory asserts that the type of cultural minority is
rejection, and diffidence. In this structure, those key, with involuntary minorities being the ones at
who are dissatisfied with their own accomplish- risk when immersed within a dominant culture.
ments and achievements may be the ones with the Though these theories provide rationales for why
highest aspirations. the differences between the minority (sometimes
An open question is whether these sorts of find- ethnic) and mainstream cultures may influence
ings regarding the RSES would be replicated in student engagement, it seems less clear precisely
cross-cultural studies of either the SEI or, in a similar how these cultural mismatches impact engage-
fashion, the Me and My School survey. Two recent ment. Does choosing to act like those in the
studies may help to answer that. Moreira, Vaz, dominant culture impact one’s attitude regarding
Dias, and Petracchi (2009) examined the psycho- the relevance of school work and one’s future
metric properties of the SEI for a population dif- aspirations and goals? Or are future aspirations
ferent from that of the original validation study the driver when it comes to student engagement
(Appleton et al., 2006); specifically, their study regardless of cultural background? Do those
looked at 760 Portuguese high school students. from the cultural minority view teacher-student
Though the 6-factor structure of the original instru- and peer relationships through the same lens as
ment was confirmed, the authors noted that some those from the cultural majority? Or do they see
of the items loaded on different subscales. This a relationship with a teacher or peer as a choice
raises questions about the comparability of the of another culture over one’s own? The research
definitions of those dimensions across the original on the impact of culture on engagement is
population of interest and this new group. inconclusive in terms of answering the sorts of
Another study (Betts, Appleton, Reschly, questions I just posed, but there seems to be
Christenson, & Huebner, 2010) investigated the evidence that culture is related to engagement in
factorial invariance of the SEI and confirmed the some way.
39 Part V Commentary: Possible New Directions in the Measurement of Student Engagement 809

Given that, it seems clear that studies must be counterparts, even after they are matched in terms
undertaken to investigate this issue. One such of their levels of cognitive engagement.
type of study involves investigating differential In terms of the threshold values describing the
item functioning (DIF). In educational settings, items, it is useful to refer to Fig. 38.3 provided by
we say that DIF occurs “when examinees from Betts (2012, pg. 797). The thresholds are the loca-
different groups have different probabilities or tions where the probabilities of choosing a spe-
likelihoods of success on an item, after they have cific category are equal. Graphically, we see that
been matched on the ability of interest” (Clauser there are curves for each of the response options:
& Mazor, 1998, pg. 31). Put more simply within strongly disagree, disagree, agree, and strongly
the context of an example, if DIF is not present disagree. The first threshold is where the strongly
and we look at only the most able students, we disagree curve crosses the disagree curve; this
find that males and females are equally likely occurs at approximately −1.7 on the Rasch metric.
to get an item right. This would hold true across Another threshold occurs where the disagree
the ability continuum. If DIF is present, at curve crosses the agree curve (at approximately
some abilities we might find that females have a −0.6), and the final threshold is where the agree
higher probability of correctly answering an curve crosses the strongly agree curve (at approxi-
item than males; meaning the item is more diffi- mately 2.3 on that same metric). This figure repre-
cult for males. sents a single item, but the RSM assumes the same
This concept is readily transferrable to instru- pattern of the first two crossing locations being
ments measuring psychological constructs, but relatively close together, and the final one quite
our notion of what constitutes DIF must be distant, would be found for all items, just shifted
expanded. In his chapter, Betts provided an expla- up or down the x-axis for the different items.
nation of the Rating Scale Model (RSM: Andrich, This pattern indicates that people with
1978). That will serve as a useful springboard for extremely low levels of affective engagement
my discussion of DIF, starting with the RSM and will tend to strongly disagree with the items.
extending to the more general Partial Credit A relatively small percentage of people are likely
Model (PCM: Masters, 1982). to disagree; those are the respondents whose level
As Betts (2012) noted, the RSM includes two of affective engagement would measure between
item parameters: one describing the scale or loca- −1.7 and −0.6 on a standardized metric. We can
tion value for each item and the other capturing contrast this with the relatively large percentage
the threshold values. Examples from the SEI may of respondents, between −0.6 and 2.3 on this
help to explain the differences between these metric, who are most likely to agree with the
item parameters for those not accustomed to items. The respondents above that level would
these types of models. Based on the output pro- most likely strongly agree with the affective
vided by Betts (2012), the following items are engagement items. An alternative way to express
starkly different in terms of their scale values: this would be to say that relatively small changes
• What I’m learning in my classes will be impor- in one’s level of affective engagement would
tant in my future. result in a change from strongly disagreeing to
• After finishing my schoolwork, I check it over agreeing, while a larger change is necessary for
to see if it’s correct. one to go from disagreeing to strongly agreeing.
Not surprisingly, students are much less likely Schmitt and Allik (2005), in their study of the
to agree with the latter item than the former. If we RSES, found that people from collectivist cul-
placed these items on a horizontal scale, the latter tures may exhibit a neutral response bias, mean-
would be to the right since it is “more difficult” to ing they have a tendency to not use extremes on
agree with that item, or conversely, it is easier to that psychological scale. If the same were to hold
disagree with it. If DIF were to exist with regard true with regard to student engagement, we
to this scaling parameter, we might see that might see DIF with regard to these thresholds,
minority students are less likely to see what they meaning the locations of those thresholds would
are learning to be important than their majority be different for those from collectivist versus
810 K.M. Samuelsen

individualist cultures. Specifically, we would factor structure differs between the groups under
expect to see the first and last thresholds pushed consideration.
further to the extremes, meaning one from a col- In the mid-1990s, researchers began applying
lectivist culture would need to have an extremely DIF procedures that had traditionally been used
low level of affective engagement (as an exam- with dichotomous items to the examination of
ple) before he/she would be willing to strongly polytomous, generally ordinal, items (see Zwick
disagree or an extremely high level of engage- & Thayer, 1996 as an example of research from
ment to be willing to strongly agree. Under this that time). The problem with these traditional
scenario, it would be very difficult to make methods was that they yielded a single measure
appropriate inferences about respondents, espe- of DIF for each item, thereby providing no infor-
cially those with average levels of affective mation regarding what about the item or items
engagement. One would have to wonder whether was problematic. The body of research on methods
those average levels were due to the truth about for examining DIF for polytomous items has
the respondents or were aberrant findings due to grown significantly over the last several years.
the neutral response bias. One of the two predominant frameworks that
The PCM frees the constraint on the RSM that have emerged would be differential step function
the pattern of thresholds is the same across items. (see Penfield, Gattamorta, & Childs, 2009 for an
Instead, these patterns are estimated freely for overview) which provides the more nuanced
each item. Using the PCM, we can again have information discussed in the prior paragraphs.
DIF at the item level, but even more specifically Polytomous DIF can also be studied within an
at the level of the threshold. As an example, let us SEM framework using the Multiple Indicators/
consider the item from the SEI that states “I feel Multiple Causes (MIMIC) models (see Finch,
safe at school.” It seems possible for minority 2005 for a comparison with non-SEM methods).
and nonminority students who are matched in This too provides a mechanism for examining
terms of their affective engagement to respond to DIF from a variety of perspectives.
this item differently. Only nonminority students
who were extremely disengaged might strongly
disagree with this statement, meaning the thresh- Final Thoughts
old between disagree and strongly disagree
would be extremely low. Minority students, on This chapter has focused on potential new direc-
the other hand, might think about safety quite tions for the study of student engagement and in
differently, and highly engaged students might doing so discussed some rather complicated
still feel unsafe, meaning that the threshold under models. As the substantive and measurement
consideration would be higher. models for engagement get more complex, it is
The presence of DIF, whether at the item or useful to remember the assumptions that underlie
threshold level, means that scores from different these models and to test those assumptions or at
groups are not comparable. Therefore, the infer- least question them. Evidence regarding linearity,
ences made regarding respondents are compro- normality, and factor analyzability can be gath-
mised. In educational assessment, differential item ered quite easily with scatter plots, descriptive
functioning indicates that multidimensionality statistics, and simple statistical tests. This evi-
exists due to the presence of some nuisance dence should be provided so the reader knows the
dimensions (Ackerman, 1992). For psychological degree to which solutions might be degraded. On
measurement, where multidimensionality is often that same topic of providing the reader with
the norm, the presence of DIF could again signal information, standard errors should also be calcu-
the presence of a nuisance dimension or dimen- lated and reported whenever possible.
sions. However, it could also mean that there are Assumptions go beyond these well-known
differences in the manner in which respondents statistical ones, however. For example, consider
utilize the rating scale itself, or that the underlying something as simple as the fact that a 4-point
39 Part V Commentary: Possible New Directions in the Measurement of Student Engagement 811

Likert-like scale is used in the SEI with strongly Handbook of research on student engagement
disagree, disagree, agree, and strongly agree (pp. 725–741). New York: Springer.
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A.
being the response options. This choice is also an L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological
assumption of a sort and as such should be tested. engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement
The question should be: Is 4 scale points the Instrument. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427–445.
appropriate number? The even number pushes Betts, J. E. (2012). Issues and methods in the measurement
of student engagement: Advancing the construct
respondents to choose to either agree or disagree through statistical modeling. In S. L. Christenson,
without giving them the option of neither agree- A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research
ing nor disagreeing. When a middle or neutral on student engagement (pp. 783–803). New York:
option is provided, it does become a catchall for Springer.
Betts, J. E., Appleton, J. J., Reschly, A. L., Christenson, S. L.,
respondents who are truly neutral, those who are & Huebner, E. S. (2010). A study of the factorial
unsure, and those who are confused; so I under- invariance of the Student Engagement Instrument
stand trying to avoid the neutral option. But what (SEI): Results from middle and high school students.
about 6 scale points? Since data from SEI have School Psychology Quarterly, 25(2), 84–93.
Bingham, G. E., & Okagaki, L. (2012). Ethnicity and stu-
been factor analyzed and 5 or more ordinal levels
dent engagement. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly,
are generally recommended to minimize bias in & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student
the parameter estimates, 4 seems an odd choice. engagement (pp. 65–95). New York: Springer.
However, it is possible that if 6 scale points were Clauser, B. E., & Mazor, K. M. (1998). Using statistical
procedures to identify differentially functioning test
used, respondents would not use two of the
items. Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice,
options, resulting in a 4-point scale anyway. 17(1), 31–44.
I come back to my initial point that the Finch, H. (2005). The MIMIC model as a method for
measurement of latent variables is difficult. It detecting DIF: comparison with Mantel-Haenszel,
SIBTEST, and the IRT likelihood ratio. Applied
requires patience and a commitment to continu-
Psychological Measurement, 29, 278–295.
ally gather evidence. I encourage researchers to Glanville, L., & Wildhagen, T. (2007). The measurement
continue to develop a richer model of the rela- of school engagement: Assessing dimensionality and
tionships between and within the affective and measurement in variance across race and ethnicity.
cognitive dimensions of student engagement and Educational and Psychological Measurement, 6,
1019–1041. doi:10.1177/0013164406299126.
to more fully investigate the invariance of this Masters, G. N. (1982). A Rasch model for partial credit
model. At the same time, those researchers must scoring. Psychometrika, 47, 149–174.
be diligent about investigating all of the assump- Moreira, P. A. S., Vaz, F. M., Dias, P. C., & Petracchi, P.
tions underlying that model and communicating (2009). Psychometric properties of the Portuguese ver-
sion of the Student Engagement Instrument. Canadian
information related to the reliability of their mea- Journal of School Psychology, 24(4), 303–317.
surements to the reader. Without this foundation Penfield, R. D., Gattamorta, K., & Childs, R. A. (2009).
to rest upon, that rich model has little value. An NCME instructional module on using differential
step functioning to refine the analysis of DIF in poly-
tomous items. Educational Measurement: Issues and
Practice, 28(1), 38–49.
References Portes, P. R. (2011). Examining the problem of cultural
validity in psychological measures: The case of the
Ackerman, T. A. (1992). A didactic explanation of item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale and implications for
bias, item impact, and item validity from a multidi- practice. Paper presented at the American Educational
mensional perspective. Journal of Educational Research Association 2011 Annual Conference, New
Measurement, 29(1), 67–91. Orleans, LA.
Andrich, D. (1978). Application of a psychometric model Schmitt, D. P., & Allik, J. (2005). Simultaneous adminis-
to ordered categories which are scored with successive tration of the Rosenberg self-esteem scale in 53
integers. Applied Psychological Measurement, 2, nations: Exploring the universal and culture-specific
581–594. features of global self-esteem. Journal of Personality
Appleton, J. J. (2012). Systems consultation: and Social Psychology, 89(4), 623–642.
Developing the assessment-to-intervention link Zwick, R., & Thayer, D. T. (1996). Evaluating the magnitude
with the Student Engagement Instrument. In S. L. of differential item functioning in polytomous items.
Christenson, A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Journal of Educational Statistics, 21(3), 187–201.
Epilogue

The purpose of this handbook was to convene It was clear that student engagement is no longer
and engage international scholars on the topic of just of interest in relation to high school dropout.
student engagement with the goal of enhancing Rather, student engagement is associated with
future research and the potential for interventions academic achievement, lower-risk health and
to positively affect student outcomes. Authors of sexual behaviors, social emotional well-being,
34 chapters and the 5 commentaries for each of and long-term outcomes, such as work success.
the sections of this handbook have contributed to Thus, engagement is relevant for all students who
the dialogue about research on student engage- cross our school doors. Scholars study engage-
ment, namely, how it is defined, the nature or ment from a variety of levels and structures –
degree of relationship with motivation to learn, instructional methods, curricula, individuals and
role of contextual influences, and future research groups, family and peers, the person-environment
directions for the construct. As we contacted fit, and so on. The authors in this volume clearly
authors and read submissions, it became clear endorsed the importance of context for either
that there are a variety of perspectives and several enhancing or hindering student engagement at
points of agreement, as well as a number of areas school and with learning; however, it is increas-
that warrant clarification through future scholarly ingly apparent that there is also an important role
efforts. In addition, the esteemed scholars in this for individual student responsibility and invest-
volume have provided excellent suggestions for ment, which may have been underemphasized or
future research in student engagement. We have even absent from earlier work.
attempted to distil what we see as primary themes
in this epilogue.
In Search of Definitional Clarity

Big Ideas in Engagement In recent years, it is clear that there are several
conceptualizations of student engagement, from
Apparent in the scholarship represented by these those that are very narrow to those that are quite
authors is that more and more attention is being broad, subsuming other historically prominent
paid to the construct of student engagement by constructs, such as motivation. A lack of defini-
both developmental and educational psychology tional clarity has hindered efforts to synthesize
researchers and practitioners. Although common results of studies, understand effects of interven-
ground – some similarities – exists with respect to tions, and more accurately detail what is needed
dimensions of student engagement, differences in future research. In this volume, we asked
surfaced in how it is conceptualized and measured – authors to define student engagement. The vast
often unique to the programmatic line of research. majority endorsed a three-part typology offered

S.L. Christenson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 813


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
814 Epilogue

by Fredricks, Blumfeld, and Paris (2004). Thus, ment. What is, in our view, imperative is that,
most engagement scholars view student engage- hereafter, all scholars provide detailed information
ment as multidimensional, comprised of observable about the models they espouse, their definitions of
behavior, internal cognition, and emotion. Many engagement, and their measurement tools.
scholars also endorsed the notion that these
types of engagement are interrelated. However,
conceptualizations become more differentiated Future Directions
when these subtypes of engagement are opera-
tionalized into items and scales for measurement Several common themes are apparent in the
purposes. Or in other words, one author’s con- authors’ thoughts of where and how student
ceptualization of the components of behavioral engagement research could most fruitfully
engagement is another’s operationalization of develop. Longitudinal studies were recommended
cognitive engagement. In the future, it will be by quite a few authors. Studies of engagement
necessary to clearly detail how engagement is over time were recommended by those wanting
operationalized in measurement so as to facilitate to go beyond descriptive, correlational studies to
understanding across authors. establish causality (both for the role of student
A major point of contention has arisen in the engagement in learning and in the factors that
conceptual and theoretical overlap between the predict or determine student engagement). Others
constructs of motivation and engagement. We were interested in seeing how different aspects of
asked authors to offer their perspectives on this student engagement develop over time, how they
engagement-motivation issue. As with defini- influence each other, and how they may react to
tions, a few essentially endorsed engagement as a changing school contexts, particularly through
meta-construct, with cognitive engagement sub- developmental transitions, for example, from
suming previous work on intrinsic motivation. elementary to middle school, middle school to
However, most authors – motivation and engage- secondary school. Authors suggested that a wor-
ment researchers alike – postulated that motiva- thy focus of investigation would address the
tion is an antecedent or precursor of engagement, question, “how and which students change (i.e.,
echoing a view offered several years ago by become more or less engaged)?”
Russell, Ainley, and Frydenberg (2005) that moti- Another question around student engagement
vation is intent and engagement is action. This lies in gaining further evidence over time about
view allows for the merging and coexistence of the interaction of different influential factors
the rich, distinguished history of motivational (e.g., out-of-school and school experiences, fam-
research with the more nascent and intervention- ily and teacher expectations and support). Student
focused field of engagement, thereby linking engagement data were recommended as an
motivation to engagement and, in turn, to impor- important accompaniment to the collection of
tant outcomes of interest to scholars, parents, and student achievement data, particularly to under-
educational personnel. stand differential levels of student performance;
And yet, as with the operationalization of defi- the impact of policy, including high-stakes assess-
nitions of engagement, the overlap between ment; and the impact of school-level interven-
engagement and motivation remains murky in tions designed to improve student performance.
measurement and, by extension, in intervention Another set of themes for the next stage of
research. Consensus across scholars regarding all development of research on student engagement as
theoretical models, measurement, and definitions noted by most authors addresses the desirability of
may not be possible, or even desirable, as we firmly more clarity and consistency in its conceptualiza-
believe the perspectives of scholars from different tion as well as the relation among subtypes of
disciplines add to the richness and quality of schol- engagement. Authors desired a better sense of how
arly inquiry around the topic of student engage- student engagement works and when it works to
Epilogue 815

have an impact on student learning and develop- particular. There should be an eye toward exam-
mental outcomes. There was recognition that defi- ining the effect of student engagement on the
nitional clarity is needed to advance research on student-teacher interaction (or parent-adolescent
student engagement and to build a research-oriented or family-school) as well as the impact of these
community. There was, however, no recommenda- interactions on student performance data; bidirec-
tion that a specific definition of student engagement tional effects are an area for future research.
be shared among the research community.
Consensus about the universality of the
Our Position: A Call for Action
engagement construct for all students offers hope
to see how engagement affects student groups
Working with the authors of the chapters and the
(e.g., ethnicity, culture, general or special educa-
commentaries has been a wonderful professional
tion) similarly or differently. At the same time,
and learning experience. Each of the coeditors
authors recognize that the need to know more
believes that her understanding and knowledge
about student engagement for specific student
of student engagement has been embellished
groups and cultures is a void in the research.
as a result of coediting this handbook. For these
Hence, there is an awareness that the field needs
reasons, we extend our appreciation to the
to check the universality of measures of student
authors.
engagement, and that it is important to undertake
We have reflected on the contents of the chap-
studies with a range of students from different
ters, in particular, the responses of the authors to
backgrounds, at different achievement levels, as
the questions that framed the purpose of the
well as cross-culturally. There is also an interest
handbook. It is our hope that this will be a call for
in mixed methods studies and an awareness of the
action to advance the quantity and quality of
value of both qualitative and quantitative research
research on student engagement to improve
and of the importance of being able to link a
learning outcomes. We offer the following rec-
close-grained focus (e.g., of self-regulation and
ommendations to advance the quality and util-
goals during the completion of a particular task)
ity of research on the construct of student
with the layers of context. For example, contex-
engagement:
tual influences that have a bearing on student
capability for self-regulation and goal-setting, 1. Researchers must provide their definition and
include class learning opportunities; assess- conceptualization of student engagement in
ments; relations with teacher, peers, and parents; each investigation conducted. Given the level
and out-of-school interests and experiences. of consensus across researchers about the mul-
Finally, there is a strong interest and support for tidimensional nature of student engagement,
having the results of current research put to practi- the engagement subtype should be specified;
cal, evaluated use. Many authors noted that more basically, the adjective (e.g., cognitive, behav-
student engagement interventions must be devel- ioral, affective, academic) and operationaliza-
oped, evaluated, and refined, striving to establish tion of this adjective are important to clarify in
the efficacy of these interventions. Some authors research. Hence, reporting of scientific find-
referred to the void of interventions as “the prac- ings should avoid the vague use of engagement
tice gap.” Both the implementation of interven- (e.g., ‘increases in engagement’), substituting
tions already in practice with success (as described rather the subtype of engagement.
in some of the chapters) as well as research-derived 2. Researchers must not only define student
implications for teaching and parental practices engagement but use measures that align with
would advance the impact of student engagement their specific definition of engagement.
research. Additionally, authors called for new 3. Student engagement is best understood when
approaches that tackle the intersections between contextualized; therefore, researchers must
individual students, school and class programs and specify information about the learning,
approaches, and school-family interactions in school, or family context under study.
816 Epilogue

4. Of the various subtypes noted by authors, it between present performance and later
is clear that more information on the effect of goals. The role of goals – whether student or
cognitive engagement on academic learning teacher goals – is evident, plays a seminal
and how affective engagement develops are part in much student engagement research,
needed areas of study. Cognitive and affec- and warrants increased attention in theoretical
tive engagement require understanding the models and interventions.
student perspective and voice. 10. As we move forward, focus must be on the
5. Adopting an engagement orientation, due to complex systems – learners, classrooms, schools,
the multidimensional nature, integrates stu- and communities that characterize develop-
dents’ thoughts (cognitions), feelings, and ment and human interaction. We identify disen-
behaviors toward achieving positive learning gaged learners, but we also need to identify
outcomes and/or improving one’s academic disengaged learning environments. What makes
competencies. It is not sufficient to focus learning engaging for students? What is an
only on completion of learning activities engaging context? How do engaging contexts
(e.g., behavior) to foster a student’s identity differ across age and student differences?
as a learner. Student feelings, interests, and 11. Because context matters, it is important to
attitudes as well as self-perceived compe- examine for whom schools exist. Martin
tence on the task or use of strategy for doing Maehr and Carol Midgley (1996) suggested
one’s best are part of this identity. that too often they are designed for creating
6. Many aspects of student engagement, irre- positive conditions for teachers and the
spective of subtype, need to be better under- staff versus positive conditions for the stu-
stood. For example, is student engagement dent. School policies and practices will
separate from disengagement? Is it linear? Is have to change if schools are to increase
more really better? Is there a tipping point their holding power – and research must
with disengagement where it is very hard to document the impact of such contextual
recover? Is there a fairly wide band of useful influences.
engagement levels? 12. The effect of discontinuity across environ-
7. Interventions to enhance engagement sub- ments for students (e.g., classroom, home)
types must be developed and evaluated. should be documented with an eye toward
Student engagement is not immutable – it understanding how students resolve incon-
can change – but for whom and under what gruent messages about the role of schooling
conditions is still unclear. Establishing a and learning.
person-environment fit whether for indi- 13. Although no recommendation that a specific
vidual students or student groups (e.g., Maori, definition of student engagement be shared
Native American) is inherent to fostering among the research community surfaced
varied engagement outcomes. The interaction from authors, we contend that to define stu-
of both student and environment are critical dent engagement solely as multidimensional
targets for change to achieve increased student with specified indicators for each subtype
engagement. dances around, unfortunately, a definition.
8. Whether student engagement is conceptual- Additionally, enhancing clarity to what stu-
ized as an input, mediator, or desired out- dent engagement refers was embedded in the
come needs to be specified in the research purpose of the handbook and reinforced by
design. Engagement may be any of these. the commentary from Eccles and Wang
There is no need to delineate engagement (2012). Therefore, we offer this definition:
only as a process or outcome.
9. Engagement seems to be embedded in Student engagement refers to the student’s active
thinking about what one wants to achieve, participation in academic and co-curricular or
school-related activities, and commitment to
whether in the classroom or as the link
educational goals and learning. Engaged students
Epilogue 817

find learning meaningful, and are invested in


their learning and future. It is a multidimensional References
construct that consists of behavioral (including
academic), cognitive, and affective subtypes. Eccles, J., & Wang, M. -T. (2012). Part I commentary: so
Student engagement drives learning; requires what is student engagement anyway? In S. L. Christenson,
energy and effort; is affected by multiple con- A. L. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research
textual influences; and can be achieved for all on student engagement (pp. 133–145). New York:
learners. Springer.
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004).
As coeditors, from different countries and School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the
professional backgrounds, we share a common evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59–109.
interest in engagement as being the most prom- Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school
cultures. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
ising means for understanding students’ school Russell, V. J., Ainley, M., & Frydenberg, E. (2005).
experiences and outcomes and, most importantly, Schooling issues digest: Student motivation and
that by understanding engagement, we can engagement. Retrieved November 9, 2005, from http://
improve educational and life outcomes for www.dest.gov.au/sectors/school education/publica-
tions resources/schooling issues digest/schooling
youth. It is our hope that this handbook advances issues digest motivation engagement.htm.
the field so that we may someday accomplish
this goal.

Respectfully submitted,
Sandra L. Christenson
Amy L. Reschly
Cathy Wylie
Index

A Academic engaged time (AET), 3


Academic achievement, 177, 332–333 components and determinants continuum, 661–662
emotional engagement, 211 description, 654
GPA, 211–212 enhancement
graduation/dropping out, 212 description, 663–664
long-term effects, 568–569 instructional design, 667–668
researchers, 568 instructional strategies, 666
Academic Case Management (ACM) system, 332 interactive teaching, 666–667
Academic coping, 32 managerial strategies, 664–666
Academic emotions student-mediated strategies, 668–670
achievement, 262 NECTL analysis, 662
appraisals as proximal antecedents, 271–272 task, 663
description, 259 theoretical foundations, 655
emotion, mood and affect, 260–261 Academic engagement, 102, 116, 117, 120, 269, 389–391,
epistemic, 262–263 424–425, 548, 564, 568–569, 697, 726, 750, 767
functions, SE and achievement engagement-achievement connections, 108
behavioral, 266–267 extracurricular activities, 108–109
cognitive-behavioral engagement, 267 homework, 108
engagement, cognitive, 264–265 studies, inattentiveness, 107
motivational, 265–266 and teacher-reported, 107
negative activating emotions, 269–270 Academic mediation theory, 495–496
negative deactivating emotions, 270–271 Academic motivation, 105, 138, 173, 220, 496
neuroscientific and cognitive research, 263 Academic work, 28–29, 35, 496
positive emotions, 269 Achievement/attainment, SE, 69, 99, 123, 211–212, 461,
social-behavioral engagement, 268–269 528, 606, 786
orientations and achievement-related goals attendance, 114
controllability and value, 274 high school, 114–115
motivation, 274 longitudinal studies, 114
performance approach and avoidance, 274–275 research categories, 106–107
reciprocal causation, 272–273 Achievement goal theory, 134, 141, 644
psychological functions, 262 academic motivation, 173
reciprocal causation, regulation and therapy, 277–278 academic tasks, 187
social, 263 classroom climate (see Classroom climate)
and specific mood, 262 collaborative efforts, 187
task and environments description, 173–174
autonomy support, 276 educational psychology, 1980s and 1990s, 173
cognitive quality, 275 Four-Factor Model, 175–176
feedback and consequences, achievement, 276–277 goal orientations, 174–175, 177–180
motivational quality, 276 historical developments, 174
structures and social expectations, 276 learning types, 187
topic emotions, 263 original development, 174
valence and activation, 261–262 relationships, goal orientation and engagement, 187

819
820 Index

Achievement motivation theory, 134, 458 popularization, high-stakes standardized tests


Adolescent student engagement dropout crisis, 469–471
academic achievement, 568–569 exaggerated emphasis, 468–469
behavioral engagement NCLB, 467
definition, 55 and student motivation, 467–468
extracurricular and school activities, 55–56 reform and curriculum
individual interactions and characteristics, 56 local curriculum planning, 443–444
platonic and romantic peer relationships, 55 origins, NZC key competencies, 443
CDC document, 579 vision and principles, 443
and childhood standardized testing
early childhood, 47 low-stakes, 460–461
middle childhood, 47–48 norm and criterion-referenced, 460
positive child development, 48 psychologists, 459
societal expectations, 47 use, 459–460
cognitive engagement, 56 strategies, 483
conduct problems/violence, 570 “structured” and “spontaneous”, 459
definitions, 564 struggling and talented students, 472
emotional engagement use, 111–112
decrease, GPA, 56 Assessment-to-intervention See Student engagement
description, 56 instrument (SEI)
family factors, 57 Attributional theory, 134, 271–272
participating, after-school programs, 57 Authentic enquiry
research, 57 application, 689
schools and disciplinary policies, 56 learning, 690
emotional/psychological aspects, 563 Authority systems/classroom management techniques, 465
engaged and disengaged, 565–567 Autonomy, 27, 153–154, 359–360, 370
high-risk behaviors, 563, 578–579 adolescents, 144
and motivation, 564–565 charting, NCEA, 451
prevention and intervention vs. control, 350–352
mechanisms, 574–575 educational practices and supporting students
school context, 578 competence, 422–423
school-wide and targeted, 575–577 components, 421–422, 437
student-teacher relationships, 577–578 contributions, 437
utilizing system level, 575 emotional experience, 422
process, 565 formation, inner compass, 423
risk and factors, 570–573 freedom, coercion and optional choice, 423
school dropout, 55, 569–570 motivation, 422
substance use and physical and mental health, 569 SDT and motivation, 421
troubling and high-risk behaviors, 567–568 environments, 346
AET See Academic engaged time feelings, 642
Affective engagement, 102, 103, 116–122, 415 formative assessment practices, 467
identification, school, 113 individual, 206, 271
multilevel logistic regression analysis, 121–122 learner, 453
self-report measures, student, 112 psychological needs, 345
valuing, 113 self-system model, 100
Appraisals as proximal antecedents student, 103, 205, 453, 465
attributional theory, 271–272 support, 276, 603
control-value theory, 272 teachers, 206
emotions causes and modulation, 271
nonreflective induction, 272
test anxiety research, 271 B
Assessment, 366, 677, 681 Bandura’s theory, 232, 241
CB tests and testing, 461–467 Basic needs theory, 158
classroom-based practices, 471 affecting students’ perceptions, 154
cognitive engagement, 103 autonomy, 153–154
description, 457–458 competence, 154
environment, 276 description, 153
instruments, 278 relatedness, 154
needed research, practice and policy, 472 satisfaction, 154
package, 125 student active engagement, learning activities, 154
Index 821

Behavioral engagement, 47, 51, 53–56, 119–122, 180, NCEA, 451, 453
182–184, 208–209, 266–268, 405, 564, neutral, 451
618–624, 626–627, 726 See also Engagement parental, 516
and achievement, reading personal, 200, 202
Behavioral learning theory, 518 and preparatory activities, 533
Behavior management reforms, voluntary, 99
description, 374 regulatory process, 466
positive student, 374–375 students’ perceptions, 449
self-regulations, 375 and task engagement, 142
Bifactor model, SEI, 793 Circumplex models, 261
correlations, 799–800 Classic psychological theory, 195, 197
factors, 796 Classroom activities, 86, 157, 391, 464
hypothetical use, 798 analysis, engagement, 285
initial items, 798–799 distinctions, motivation and engagement, 285–286
RMSEA, 799 dynamic systems perspective, 299–300
SEI data, 798, 799 educational levels and activities, 299
visualization, 795, 797 and high school courses, 528
Biopsychosocial development indicators, engagement, 284
definition and conceptual boundaries, 699 interconnections and exploration, 284
disengagement, 700 motivation, educators, 283–284
motivation, 700 SE, 630
school dropout, 700 talent development sites, 530
Bloom’s theory, 656–658 tests and tasks, 464
Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory, 334 theoretical orientation
achievement behavior, 288
achievement goals, 294–296
C complexity, 288
Cardiovascular disease (CVD), 98 context and relations, 296
Carroll’s model, 660, 661, 668 contribution, interest, 291–292
Cooley-Leinhardt model, 657 developmental phases and stages, 290–291
school learning dynamic systems perspective, 287, 288
appeal, 656 educational domain, 288
description, 655 emotions, 294
factors, 655 individual interest and trajectories, 292–294
time, 655–656 learning situation and motivation
Wiley-Harnischfeger model, 657 schemas, 288
Catholic schools, 72, 332–333, 507 PISA 2006, 297–299
academic achievement, low-income process schemas and task engagement, 288–289
students, 333 proposals, 287–288
higher achievement and parent participation, 333 role, interest, 289–290
NAEP data, 333 task perceptions, 288
in 1980s, 332–333 Classroom-based (CB) tests, 458–459
warmth and care, teachers, 333 formative tests and SE, 465–467
Causality orientations theory, 153, 157, 158 student and classroom goals structures, 462–465
CDP See Child Development Project value, 461–462
Centers for disease control (CDC), 98, 579 Classroom climate, 35, 373, 378
Check & Connect (C&C), 7–8, 548, 576–577 cooperative, 275
Child Development Project (CDP), 207 engagement theory
Choice description, experiences, 368
and autonomy, 467 enthusiasm and passion, 368
and behavior, 113 impressions, 368
behavioral engagement, 615 instructional exchanges, 368–369
career and course plans, 198 NRC report, 368
curriculum, 533 perspective and motivation, 369
educational and occupational, 142 relational setting and capacity, 369
family, 586 goal structures, 181–184
feelings, individual subjects, 454 teacher practices
friends, 485 mastery goal structure, 185
learning options and pathways, 455 performance goal structure, 186
learning resources, 442 Classroom community, teachers’ impact on, 206–207
822 Index

Classroom practices, 167, 207, 372, 481, 604 measures, 112


achievement, 627 mood-congruent memory recall, 265
autonomy-supportive communication, 630 observable indicators, 111
behavioral engagement, 626–627 retrieval-induced forgetting and facilitation, 265
CORI, 627–628 studies, self-regulation and use, 111
effects, 617–624 “think alouds”, 111
global quality, 630 Cognitive evaluation theory, 153, 158
motivation, 629, 631 autonomy-supportive vs. controlling way, 156
perceived emotional support, 628–629 classroom conditions and student motivational
perceived relevance, 617, 625 processes, 156
SDT, 627 external events and functions, 156
students student and classroom conditions, 156
achievement, 631 Cognitive/motivational model, 263, 266, 269
behaviorally engaged/disengaged, 629 Comer’s SDP and HCZ project
devaluing, 625 Academic Case Management (ACM)
minority, 631 system, 332
self-efficacy, 625 community investment services, 331
teacher impacts, HCZ project, 332
caring, 629 mesosystemic approach, 331
teacher-student relationships, 625 negative effects, poverty, 331
teaching, 617 Competency development
Classroom setting and youth development affordances, 442
achievement outcomes, 367 curriculum and assessment reform, New zealand,
engagement and intrinsic motivation, 367 442–444
fifth grade, nature and quality, 367 description, 441
high school, 367 engagement determination
improvement and enhancement, 367 classroom component, 446
school reform, 368 teachers vs. students perception, 446–448
supportive relationships, 368 expectations and engagement
Classroom strategies classes, students association, 450
AET, 654, 661–663 least enjoyed classes, 451
Bloom’s theory, 656–657 NCEA, 450, 451
Carroll’s model, 655–656 student and teacher items, 450
classroom setting and youth development, 367–368 pedagogy changes
Cooley and Leinhardt’s classroom-process learning-to-learn connections, 444–445
model, 657–658 NZC framework, 444
description, 653 sociocultural framing, 445
enhancement AET, 663–670 self-managing behaviours, 452
Huitt model, 658 students engagement, 448–450, 453–455
language and instructional discourse teaching, traditional, 451–452
children’s ability, 377 Competency levels
classrooms offering, 377 attitudinal and cognitive, 593, 594
positive language development, 377 average scores, trajectory groups, 592, 593
social medium, 378 competent learners study, 591–592
verbal interactions, 377–378 track engagement levels, 586
NECTL, 654 Competent learners, 445, 446, 596
process-product paradigm, 658–661 childhood education, 587
theoretical foundations, 658 data collection, 586–587
time and learning, 654–655 motivation, 588
Wiley and Harnischfeger’s model, 657 SE patterns, school
Coaching conversation, 683, 685 behavioral and cognitive, 588
identity and authorship, 686–687 changes, education experience, 589
profile, 686 changes, engagement levels, 589
Cognitive dissonance theory, 199 enjoyment, learning, 590
Cognitive engagement, 10–11, 15, 47, 52, 54, 56, mean score, items, 589
102–103, 179–180, 182, 184, 238–240, 408, measure, items, 588–589
564, 602, 747, 764, 772, 793 New Zealand data, 590
and achievement, 111 students, school experiences, 587
attention and flow, 264 Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI)
Index 823

classroom practices, 627–628 D


defined, 603 Deep engagement, 317, 686–687, 689, 692
goal, 628 consensus, 679
reading comprehension outcomes, 630 description, 675–676
Conceptualizing engagement, 308 engagement and motivation, learning, 679–680
controlling grades, 319 identity, 681–682
experience types, 319 learning power and engagement (see Learning power
flow theory and experiences, 318–319 and engagement)
observer ratings, 318 participation, 678–679
procedural and substantive, 318 perezhivanie, 680–681
Conceptualizing motivation, 308 personal
beliefs, 319–320 and community stories, 682–683
goal theory, 320 qualities, 683
learning and achievement, 319 worldview challenges
mastery-avoidance goals, 320 ELLI, 677–678
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), 793, 801 epistemology and anthropology, 676
application, 791–792 ideology, 676
congeneric measurement model, 791 understanding engagement, 677
description, 790–791 Developmental context
Constructivist theories, 375, 445 early childhood
Continuum/continua, 12–13, 567, 661, 662 education and intervention programs, 50–51
Control-value theory, 272, 274 participation, education programs, 51
Cooley and Leinhardt’s classroom-process model, school context, 51
657–658 middle childhood
Coping behavioral engagement, 53–54
behaviors types, 135 cognitive engagement, 54
description, 31 description, 53
educational implications emotional engagement, 54
developmental aspects, 34 individual student characteristics, 53
engagement and disaffection, 33 research, 54–55
intrinsic motivation and engagement, 33 role, parents and teachers, 53
larger school, 37 schooling and learning environments, 53
motivation and engagement, teachers, 36–37 Developmental dynamics
nature, academic work, 35 academic development, 24
SDT, 33 children and youth involvement, 22
social learning environment, 35–36 classroom, 22–23
student disaffection and failure, 34–35 construction, academic engagement, 21–22
teachers enthusiasm and excitement, 36 coping (see Coping)engagement and disaffection,
teacher-student interactions, 36 motivation
tracking, 33 cycles, 30–31
motivational model, 31 parents and peers, 30
response, 135 teachers, 29–30
skills, 520, 524 trajectories, 31
students’ ability, school life, 31 everyday school experiences, students, 24
CORI See Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction meaning, engagement, 22
Cultural discontinuity, 67, 68, 83, 808 motivational dynamics model (see Motivational
Curriculum and assessment reform, 442–444 dynamics model)motivation and psychological
CVD See Cardiovascular disease processes, 22
Cyclical feedback loop, 239–243 proximal processes, 23–24
academic applications, 247 youth development and children protection, 22
empirical advantages Developmental periods
achievement differences, 246–247 adolescence (see Adolescent student
effects, self-regulatory training, 246 engagement)application and policy
goal setting, 246 implications
SRL phase processes, 246 growth and development, SE, 59
SCL, 247–248 manifestation, 58–59
self-regulated strategy development recognition, educators and policymakers, 58
(SRSD), 247 behavioral engagement, 51
SREP, 248–250 “chain of events”, 46
824 Index

Developmental periods (cont.) Finn’s seminal theory, 7


childhood and adolescence, 47–48 high schools, 5
cognitive engagement, 52 participation-identification model, 7
contribution and human development, 57–58 predictive variables, 6–7
definition, student engagement, 47 publicity and interest, 5
description, 45–46 Dropout prevention theory, 99–100
vs. developmental tasks Drugs and relaxation techniques, 277
codevelopment, 48–49
emotional engagement and deficits, 48
friendship, middle childhood, 48 E
group cooperation, 48 Eccles et al.expectancy-value theoretical (EEVT) model
motivation, 49 See Engagement theory (ET)
relationship, school-related activities, 48 Educational practices, 34, 555, 565, 656, 676, 699
restrictions, 48 components and contributions, 423
schoolwork and extracurricular activities, cross-cultural research, 231–232
student, 49 FIV processes, 436–437
social skills, 48 high-stakes testing, 471
early childhood, 50–51 IVD, 427–429
emotional engagement, 52 minimizing controls
individual’s own capacities, 58 colleagues and expected behaviors, 425
middle childhood, 53–54 external control, behaviors, 424
participation-identification model, 57 impact, teachers external controls, 424–425
person-environment fit perspective, 58 internal compulsion, 425
prominent developmental tasks, 46 psychological control, 425
social and structural resources, 58 negative feelings, 425–426
social skills, children, 46 rationale, 426–427
theoretical considerations self-efficacy
activity participation, 50 benefits, instructional and social practices, 228
bioecological theory, 49 competence beliefs, 228
characteristics, child/adolescent, 50 description, 228
congruence/synchrony, 50 effect, children’s achievement and
encourage engagement, 50 persistence, 228
manifestation, 50 lower-ability students, 228–229
person-environment fit, 49–50 role models, 228
proximal processes, 49 sociodemographics, 229
understanding growth, 46–47 teacher bias and obstacles, 228
Developmental task, childhood and adolescence, 48–49, transition periods, schools, 229
51, 327 supporting choice and initiation, 427
description, 47 SVE, 430–435
early childhood, 47 Educators’ beliefs and practices, 81–87, 181, 185–186,
middle childhood, 47–48 356–358, 376, 416, 425, 443, 458, 461,
positive child development, 48 464–466, 484, 486, 541, 544, 547, 549, 625
societal expectations, 47 fear, 557–559
Deviance model, 495–496 learning and teaching, 556–557
Deviant affiliation theory, 496 mindset, 550
DIF See Differential item function opportunities, help, 557
Differential item function (DIF), 806 themes and exercises, 550–555
collectivist vs. individualist cultures, 809–810 Effective lifelong learning inventory (ELLI) programme,
cultural discontinuity theory, 808 677, 678, 683, 684, 691
PCM, 810 Emotional engagement, 10–12, 14, 46–49, 52, 54–58,
polytomous, 810 66, 72, 74, 76, 78–80, 82–87, 142, 150, 161,
RSES, 807–808 162, 167, 179–184, 211, 238, 260, 268, 285,
RSM, 809 304–306, 309, 497, 588, 602, 605, 621, 642,
threshold values, 809 644, 645, 696–698, 709, 726, 750, 764, 766,
Disengagement, 71, 86, 88, 99, 106, 107, 219, 310, 769–772, 776, 778, 784, 785, 787, 789, 798
315–316, 395 Engaged time, 3, 86, 101, 187, 618, 653–671, 699
areas, research and development, 124–125 Engagement and achievement, reading, 108, 120, 262,
empirical research, 123 423, 548
Dropout prevention behavioral, emotional and cognitive, 601–602
difference, theory and direct intervention, 5 behavioral engagement
Index 825

effects, 605–608 student attitudes and emotional engagement, 66


GPA, 609–610 students role, 68–74
motivations impact, 610–615 variation, ethnic group, 67
reading comprehension, 609 Every-day resilience
reading, motivational processes, 605 academic coping as mechanism, 32
students’ aversion, 609 academic resources
variety, studies, 609 educators and parents responsibility, 32
classroom practices, 626–631 mindsets and ages, 32
conceptual knowledge and social interactions, 603 positive motivational dynamics, 33
conditions and classroom practices, 604, 605 qualitative changes, early adolescence, 32–33
instructional practices, 603–604 Expectancy-value theory, 134, 136, 141–145, 198, 610
model, classroom contexts, 604 Expectancy X value theory, 415, 416, 458, 461
and motivation, 602–603 Experience sampling forms (ESFs), 321
motivations impact, competence, 615–617 Experience sampling method (ESM), 321, 327, 766
real-world interactions, 603 Extant theory, 133
Engagement models, 12, 105, 238, 247, 493–496, 604, 648 Extracurricular activities, 81, 108–109, 333, 387–398,
Engagement outcomes, 625, 814 405, 530, 698, 726, 750
biopsychosocial development, 699–700 academic, 100, 102, 116
description, 695–696 effectiveness, 140
learning activities and school engagement, 139
academic, 697 high school reforms, 530
definition and measurement, 697 involvement, 647
description, 696 ISAO, 108–109
determinants, intervention and contexts, 698–699 participation, 115, 500, 643
emotional, cognitive and behavioral, 698 policy makers, 139
Finn’s model, 698 talent development high school model, 530
outcomes and contexts, 696–697
person-centered approach, 701
Engagement theory (ET), 10, 53, 133–136, 725 F
and EEVT Family, 5–8, 10, 15, 32, 48–50, 57–59, 75, 76, 82, 88,
achievement behaviors, 142, 143 124, 226, 232, 297, 317, 321–324, 327–334,
description, 141 343–361, 407, 479, 483, 494, 497, 502, 557,
goal, 142 565, 569, 574, 578, 586, 595–597, 646, 676,
personal and social identification role, 144 698, 730, 751–753, 771, 772, 784, 785, 790,
person-environment fit, 142 793, 796, 800, 806, 811, 813
research and policy/practice communities, 145 care, 101
SDT, 144 and communities, 486
and SDT, 135, 142 EEVT, 144
Epstein theory, 323, 335, 355, 357, 358, 360 environments, 110
ESM See Experience sampling method (ESM) factors, 505
ET See Engagement theory (ET) and friends, 596
Ethnicity, 5, 6, 98, 108–110, 117–119, 203, 327, 329, history, 98
330, 336, 470, 485, 496, 568, 590, 718, 719, income and material qualification levels, 596
744, 751, 770, 813 See also Ethnicity and interdependence and closeness, 485
student engagement members, 485
Ethnicity and student engagement, 136, 327, 329, 330, practices, 505–506
336, 485, 590, 719, 744, 813 problems, 520
children’s educational experiences, 88 relations, 113
cultural discontinuity, 67, 88–89 resources, 505, 590
discontinuity framework and ecological model, 68 school
discrimination, 88 connectedness, 197
ethnic groups, 66 contextual features, 141
motivation processes, 66 environments, 586
parents role, 74–79 levels, 500
poor school achievement, 68 practices, 507–508
public education, 89 resources, 507
researchers and ethnic groups, 66 structure, 506–507
schools role, 79–87 student composition, 506
secondary discontinuity model, 67–68 socialization theory, 496
socioeconomic differences, 67 structure, 98, 110, 505
826 Index

Family influences, 317 Gwinnett County Public Schools (GCPS), 726, 729,
children motivation, 225 733, 734
cultural capital, 225 IMD, 727
mastery experiences, 226 SEI, 728
resources and assets, capital, 225 student population, 727
responsibility and support, 226
vicarious influence, 226
Family socialization theory, 496 H
Fear, 56, 73, 223, 262, 264, 272, 320, 463, 516, 550 High-performing schools and self-efficacy, 232
challenge, 559 High school, 4, 5, 7, 9, 11, 12, 17, 22, 30, 31, 54,
commitment, 558–559 69, 70 See also High school dropout; High
control, 559 school reform
description, 557 analysis, 596
middle school English teacher, 558 assessment, 590
Feedback, 8, 9, 24, 29–31, 34, 36, 85, 153, 156, 158, Baltimore and Philadelphia, 519
159, 163, 221–223, 228–248, 252, 271, 275– Chinese and German, 277
277, 296, 305, 309, 320, 346, 352, 353, 357, criterion-referenced, 592
407, 409, 426, 428, 447, 453, 461, 463–465, curriculum design opportunities, 444
467, 471, 472, 534, 535, 547, 552, 555, 603, diploma, 470, 491
625, 637, 639, 656, 658–660, 664, 666, 667, dropout rate, 199
686, 699, 770 final years, 442, 444, 586
behavior, 376–377 graduation exams, 139, 470
forms, 377 graduation rate, 491–492
high-quality and quantity, 377 Houston school district, 201
learning, 376 low-SES, 212
Finn’s seminal theory, 3, 4, 7, 139, 493, 698 mathematics and science, 203
Flow theory, 136, 319, 322 mathematics instruction, 523
Fostering inner-directed valuing (FIV) processes, 422 and middle students perception, 626
additive influence, reflective value and goal multiteacher format, 589
examination, 436 postsecondary program, 115
authentic decision making, 436 predictors sets, 114–115
components, 436 program tracking, 518
correlates and potential benefits, 436 qualifications and post-school learning
educator’s behaviors and student attention, 436 experiences, 586
implications, 436–437 reading instruction, 522
joint contribution, 437 school belonging, 389
Four-factor model, 306 student motivation and engagement
approach/avoid orientations, 176 comprehensive reforms, 534–535
distinctions, mastery and performance goals, 175–176 preparation, 532–533
mastery-avoid goals, 176 reform priorities and sequences, 533–534
measures, 176 student performance, 498–499
mid-1990s, 175 teachers, 204, 451
PALS, 176 High school completion, 119
demographic risk, 5
participation processes, 4
G postsecondary outcomes, 5
GCPS See Gwinnett County Public Schools (GCPS) skills and behaviour, 4–5
General deviance theory, 496 High school dropout, 5, 6, 17, 119, 316, 502, 811
Goal contents theory, 150, 153, 158 attitudes, 502
explanation, 155 behaviors
extrinsic goals, 155–156 attendance, 500
personal growth and interpersonal relationships, 155 participation, extracurricular activities, 500
Goal theory, 134, 141, 150, (173–187), 304, 306, 320, school misbehavior, 501
610, 765 sophomores, 501
GPA See Grade point average (GPA) student mobility, 501
Grade point average (GPA), 26, 56, 108, 110, 115, 116, deviance model, 495–496
139, 198, 228, 322, 326, 329, 330, 333, 606, engagement-related predictors, 500
608–610, 738, 739 factors, 492
engagement measures, 211 families, 505–508
and school environment, 212 Finn’s models, 493
Index 827

goals, 502–503 “double math”, 519


institutional factors, 505 high-standards curriculum, 518, 519
life course models, 493–494 students grouping, 519
median annual earnings, 492 talent development high school reform model, 519
models, factors types identification, 493 types and definitions, 11
national crisis and “silent epidemic”, 491 High School Survey of Student Engagement (HSSSE),
preschool and early elementary school, 509 744, 747, 760, 768–770, 773–775
research literature, factors, 508 boredom, 752
risk factors, 503–504 cognitive/intellectual/academic, 750
role data analysis, 751
adolescent development and school data sample, 750–751
performance, 498 description, 749–750
behavioral, emotional and cognitive, 497 emotional, 750
educational context impact, 497 NSSE, 750
factors and students’ experiences, 497 pedagogy, 753–754
SE, 496 potential, dropping out, 753
Wehlage and NRC models, 497 reasons, going to school, 751–752
scholars, 499 social/behavioral/participatory, 750
self-perceptions, 503 students’ words
structural models, 504 instructional methods, 755
student performance, 498–499 multiple-option questions, 754
student’s behavior and performance, 508 open-response question, 755–756
student’s decision, 508 schools, 755
Tinto model, 494–495 teachers power, 755
typologies, 504 High-stakes standardized tests
Wehlage and colleagues’ model, 495 dropout crisis, 469–471
High school reform, 4, 10, 17, 515–535 exaggerated emphasis, 468–469
academic/personal needs, 520 NCLB, 467
behavioral and cognitive engagement, 10–11 role, 467
behavioral learning theory, 518 and student motivation, 467–468
communal engagement, 530 HLM model, 117, 118, 198
comprehensive reforms, 534–535 Home-school partnership, 335, 360
continuum/continua, 12–13 Catholic schools, 332–333
criteria, grades and recognition, 520–521 Comer’s SDP and HCZ project, 331–332
diplomas, 520 out-of-school time programs, 333–334
engagement and motivation, 14–15 perceptions and development, 331
external and internal conditions school-based family centers, 332
dimensions, student engagement, 517 Home-school partnership model
motivation and engagement, 516–517 autonomy support vs. control, 350–352
policy levers, 516 description, 343–344
functional relevance, learning engagement
career academies, 524–525 academic resilience, 345
career applications, blending, 525–526 definitions, 344–345
school work, 524 motivational perspective, 345
group interventions, small, 520 innovative approaches, 360–361
indicators and facilitators, 13–14 interventions
personal expression and nonacademic interests, NNPS, 358
528–530 TIPS design, 358–359
process/outcome, 11–12 involvement, challenges
prominent engagement models, 12 individual, contextual and institutional
reform priorities and sequences, 533–534 factors, 356
rules and disciplinary practices, 531 low-income and less educated parents, 355
school-wide interventions, 520 social networks, 355–356
school work, 521–524 motivational perspective, engagement
student engagement, 11 children’s motivational resources, 346
student involvement, decision-making, 532 context effects, 346–347
student motivation and engagement, 532–533 SDT, 345
teacher-student relations, 526–528 self-system processes, 345–346
theory and intervention, 10 parental structure, 352–354
transitional preparation courses parent autonomy support, 359–360
828 Index

Home-school partnership model (cont.) intrinsic motivation, 521–522


parent involvement, 347–350 mastery performance goals, 522
schools, 356 and motivation, 263
supports, student engagement personal expression and nonacademic
characterization, 354 course planning and selection, 529–530
parent involvement, 354–355 exploration, 529
role, 354 extracurricular activities, 530
teacher beliefs and practices high school reforms, 528, 529
attitudes and practices, 357–358 multiple intelligences, 528
“leaders”, 356–357 vocational psychologists, 528
teaching-learning process, 358 and personal lives, 481
HSSSESee High School Survey of Student project-based learning, 523–524
Engagement (HSSSE) reading, 616, 617
Huitt model, 658 and relationships, 597
Human development theories, 597 research, emotions, 259–260
Humanity, data and self-regulation, 585
challenges students, 101, 106, 144, 442, 449, 454, 596, 626
policy, 759 teaching
practice, 759–760 curriculum specialists, 522
research, 759 high school mathematics instruction, 523
characteristics, 758 reading instruction, high school, 522
description, 743–744 reading/math wars, 522
input–output model, 758 texts, 603, 615
listening, students, 757–758 valuing, 198, 626
schools, 760 Interest and students’ engagement, 144, 266, 269
student engagement (see Student engagement) achievement behavior, 288
student perception data, 758 achievement goals
“Tech High School”, 744–745 classroom engagement and disaffection, 296
classroom peers, 296
description, 294
I mastery and performance goals, 295
ICLE See International Center for Leadership in personal orientations and characteristics, 296
Education (ICLE) complexity, 288
IMDSee Information management division (IMD) definitions, 286
Indicators and facilitators, 13–14, 25–26 developmental phases and stages
Information management division (IMD), 727–729 definition, 290
Inschool academic extracurricular activities emerging and well-developed individual
(ISAO), 108, 109 interest, 290
Instructional interaction domain identification and features, 291
cognitive and language development, 375 personal interest, 290
distinctions, 375–376 progression, 290–291
student learning outcomes, 376 selection and profiles, 291
Intelligences, 76, 173–187, 417, 463, 554, 556, 608, 609, triggered and maintained situational interest, 290
795, 801 dynamics, 296
bodily-kinesthetic and musical, 528 dynamic systems perspective, 287
“National Intelligence Test”, 459 educational domain, 288
spiritual and existential, 528 emotions, 294
test items, 270 individual interest, 287
Interactive teaching, enhancement AET, 664 learning situation and motivation schemas, 288
academic focus, 666 macrocontext and relations, 297
feedback, 667 metaphor and ranges, 286
question, 666–667 microsystem, 296
Interest on-task interest, 292–294
behavioral engagement, 609 PISA 2006, 297–298
creative leisure, 595 and processes, 291–292
disciplinary thinking, 523 process schemas and task engagement
and enjoyment, 108 achievement goal orientations, 289
and enthusiasm, 496 assessment, 289
extracurricular, 643 mood, 288–289
individual, 198 post-task reflections, 289
Index 829

proposals, 287–288 practice, 690–691


relationship, person and task, 286 research, 685
role work
classroom learning, 296 cyberspace, 684
component processes, 289 description, 683
experiences, 289 ELLI tool, 684
interest schema, 289–290 Life course models
significance, positive emotions, 289 Euro-American families, 493–494
schema and form, 287 long-term longitudinal studies, USA, 493
self-regulatory function, 287 preschool participation, 494
situational interest, 286–287 Low-stakes standardized tests, 460–461
task perceptions, 288
Interference models, 264
International Center for Leadership in Education M
(ICLE), 549 MacArthur measures, 775
Intrinsic value demonstration (IVD), 422, 435, 437 Managerial strategies, enhancement AET, 664–665
adolescents’ perceptions, 428 Mastery goal structure, 185, 186
Conditional Positive Regard (CPR), 428 behavioral engagement, 182–183
development, 428 emotional and cognitive engagement, 182
general attitudes and skills, students, 428 perceptions and student influences, 182
identification, 427 students, learning and understanding, 181–182
individual interests, 429 Me and My School survey, 709, 714, 722
lack of IVD, 427–428 Measurement, 15, 404–405
parents behaviors, 429 approaches, SEI (see Student engagement
people behavior, 427 instrument (SEI))
prosocial acts and empirical research, 428 cognitive engagement, 641
role and accumulating experience, 429 components, engagement and indicators, 103, 104
SDT perspective, 428 instruments, 196
IRT See Item response theory (IRT) and interpretation issues, 137
Item response theory (IRT), 784, 788, 792, 795, 801, 806 uses, engagement models, 105
Measuring student engagement, 9–10, 793
assessment tools, 707–708
J definition, 709–710
Jingle, Jangle, 3–17 Me and My School scale, 714
middle response categories, 714
national trial data, analysis
K final threshold calibrations, 713
Knowledge construction process, 682, 684 fit indicators, 713
application, 689 infit statistic, 714
choose, 687 item calibration, scale, 714, 715
connection, 688–689 New Zealand, 708
mapping, 688 NZCER, 708
observing/describing, 687–688 perceived engagement, national patterns, 718–722
pedagogy, 687 piloting, 712
questioning, 688 Rasch measurement and Me and My School, 710–711
reconciling, 689 scale
schooling, 690 description, 716–717
stages sequence, 687, 688 descriptors, 715–716
storying, 688 Mentors, 8
validating, 689 MES See Motivation and Engagement Survey (MES)
Mindsets, 32, 34
commonalities, 549–550
L description, 541
Learning power, 247–248, 687, 690 educators, 542
Learning power and engagement, 677–678 educators’ beliefs and practices (see Educators’
coaching conversation, 686–687 beliefs and practices)engaged, motivated
Indigenous Australian communities, 684, 685 students, 543–544
knowledge construction process, 687–690 motivation, 544–547
language and place, 685–686 questions, consideration, 543
pedagogical moments, 685 resilience, 542–543
830 Index

Mindsets (cont.) performance-approach/avoidant goals, 463


resilient children and adolescents, 544 performance orientation, 463
student engagement preparatory activities, 533
Check & Connect, 548 preschool class, 545–546
components, 548 purpose and commitment, 547
description, 547–548 reading competence
ICLE, 549 behavioral engagement, 616
subtypes, 548 comprehension, 617
teacher, 542 devaluing, 617
Models, SE, 134, 136 self-efficacy, 616–617
dropout prevention theory, 99–100 students’ interests, 616
participation-identification, 100–105 reform priorities and sequences
school reform, 99 classroom learning activities, 533
self-system process, 100 learning environment, 533–534
Motivation, 150–151, 220 student attendance, 533, 534
academic, 134, 138, 496 school program, 531–532
achievement, 138, 139, 142, 194, 198, 462, 503, 645 and school success, 139
autonomy SDT, 546–547
people’s perception and experience, 422 shaping student, 201
quality and differentiation, 422 strategies, 646
SDT, 421 student and high-stakes tests, 467–468
and behavioral engagement, characterization, 610, 615 student engagement, 544–545
beliefs students’ achievement goals, 463
attribution types, 416–417 summative feedback and goals, 463–464
goal orientations, 416 task and ego orientation, 175
self-efficacy, 416 tenets, 546
classroom goal structures, 464–465 tests, students
classroom practices cue seekers and conscious, 462
devaluing, student, 625 curriculum content and evaluation practices, 462
effects, 617–624 expectancy X value theory, 461
perceived relevance, 617, 625 theorists, 141
students’ self-efficacy, 625 Motivational dynamics model, 33, 37
teacher-student relationships, 625 action and behavioral dimension, 24–25
teaching, 617 emotional reactions, 25
cluster analysis, 588 engagement and disaffection
cognitive and metacognitive processing, 644 cycles, 30–31
competition and cooperation, 464 parents and peers, 30
comprehensive reforms reciprocal feedback effects, 29
individual school-level reforms, 535 school contexts, 29
research and development, 535 self-system processes, 29
SE, 534 teachers, 29–30
defined, 639 trajectories, 31
development, 136 indicators vs. facilitators, 25–26
effects, behavioral engagement and reading nature, academic work, 28–29
competence, 610–614 parents shape engagement, 28
and effort, 113 peers shape engagement, 28
energy and direction, 441 SDT, 26–27
and engagement, 265–266, 445, 785–786 social contexts, 27
extrinsic and intrinsic, 746 teachers shape engagement, 27–28
flow concept, 547 Motivation and engagement, 27, 139, 152, 265–266
formative tests and SE cognition and behavior, 307
assessment links and processes, 466–467 components, 303
SDT, 466 contributions, 303
self-regulation, 465–466 description, 303
high school students, 497 disengagement and engagement, 310
instructional contexts, 408–409 frameworks, 304–305
intrinsic, 135, 195, 199, 200, 610, 615 measurement tools, 307
levels, 594–595, 796 role, 305–306
and membership, 212 theories, 304
performance and mastery goals, 533 time and space
Index 831

analysis, sequence × space learning, 309–310 self-report ratings, 292


sequence × space learning map, 308–309 trajectories, 293
Motivation and Engagement Survey (MES), 773, 775 Optimal support for Value Examination (OSVE), 435
Motivation and Engagement Wheel, 306, 307 ORE See Office of Research and Evaluation (ORE)
Motivation engagement, 14–16 Organismic integration theory, 158
Multilevel logistic regression analysis, 120, 121 description, 154
external and integrated regulation, 155
extrinsic motivation and types, 154
N identified and integrated regulations, 155
National Certificate in Educational Achievement societal norms, rules and behaviors, students, 154
(NCEA), 592, 594 Out-of-school misbehavior, 208
assessment, 450, 451, 453 adolescent substance use, 210–211
NQF, 444 connectedness, 210
qualification, 450 “normlessness” and “school isolation”, 210
National Education Commission on Time and Learning school bonding, 209–210
(NECTL), 654 Out-of-school time programs
National qualifications framework (NQF), 444 BEST program, 334
National Student Clearinghouse (NSC), 737, 739 develop youth talents, 334
NCLB See No Child Left Behind Act (NCLB) effectiveness, 334
NECTL See National Education Commission on Time extracurricular activities, 333
and Learning (NECTL) social networking, 334
Negative activating emotions, 269–270
Negative deactivating emotions, 270–271
Negative feelings P
children and adolescents, 426 Parental influences
disagree, teachers/parents, 426 administrative support, 335
feelings and opinions, 426 Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory, 334
opposite views and beliefs, 426 conceptualizing engagement, 318–319
Negative social experiences, 388 contextual influences
direct and mediated effects, 395 child development, 321–322
emotion, 395–396 development, belief systems, 322
engagement and academic performance, 396 micro and mesosystem, 322
kindergartners, 395 proximal processes, 322
literature, 394 difficulties, public school, 335
peer rejection and nominations, 394–395 educational practices, 430
teachers academic engagement, 395 engaging students, families, and communities, 336
Network of partnership schools (NNPS), 358 interactions, deep engagement and motivation, 317
New Zealand Council for Educational Research low-income ethnic minority groups, 320–321
(NZCER), 708 measurements, 318
New Zealand Curriculum (NZC), 452 motivation, 319–320
achievement objectives set, 443 parental involvement, 328–330
defined, 442, 444 perspectives, 317–318
local design and review, 444 pragmatic assistance, teachers, 334–335
NNPS See Network of partnership schools (NNPS) programs modeling and home-school partnerships,
No Child Left Behind Act (NCLB), 460, 467, 469 331–334
NSC See National Student Clearinghouse (NSC) relational support, 335
NZCER See New Zealand Council for Educational research and future directions
Research (NZCER) achievement motivation, 336
complex nature, classroom learning, 335
deductive approach, 335–336
O educational socialization practices, 336
Office of Research and Evaluation (ORE), 728 limitations, 335
On-task interest school reform, 335
achievement and classroom behaviors, 293 theoretical approaches
changing reactions, 292 benefits, 322–323
inspection, trajectories, 293 Epstein theory, 323
learning program, 292–293 positive academic and motivational outcomes, 322
measures, 292 psychosocial development, 323
medium and low interest groups, 293–294 relationships, homes and schools, 323
relationship, student and text, 292 school conditions, 323
832 Index

Parental influences (cont.) uses, engagement models, 105


underachievement and school disengagement, motivation and engagement, 105
315–317 normlessness measures and school isolation, 102
well-being and engagement, 324–328 social and academic engagement, 102
Parental involvement, 74, 116, 407 types, 100
Asian American families, 348–349 Participation-identification theory, 133, 134
children’s schooling, 347 Patterns of Adaptive Learning Survey (PALS), 176
communication, 348–349 PCM See Partial credit model
culture and student learning, 330 Peers selection and socialization
“deficit model” approaches, 330 effects, 390
description, 328–329 extracurricular engagement
direct effects and motivational model, 347–348 helping students, 391–392
effects, 349 mechanisms, 392
knowledge framework, 330 motivation, 391
low-income and middle-class parents’, 329 friends and relations characteristics, academic
meta-analysis, 347 engagement
middle-class peers and working class parents, 329 academic orientation, 390–391
motivational model, 349–350 adjusted friends improve grade point, 391
Ogbu’s contribution, 329–330 behaviors and attribution, 391
perspectives, 330 classroom activities, 391
social networks use, 329 opposite effect, 390
tangible and intangible resources, 329 maintain school engagement, 397–398
Parental structure positive and negative effects, 397
components, 353 support and quality friendships
maladaptive control beliefs, 353–354 develop positive attitudes, 392
rules and expectations, 354 effects, 392
SDT, 352 encourage student engagement, 392
Parent autonomy support, 351, 352 and extracurricular involvement, 393
mother-child dyads, 359 social and academic support, 392–393
pressure, 359–360 Peers shape engagement, 28
Parents role, 317, 334, 356 Perceived engagement, national patterns
adolescent’s “job”, 77 class, 719, 720, 722
and child relationship, 75–76 description, 718
cultural model education, 78 ethnic group, 719–720
cultural socialization, 77–78 listening, 722
diversity, 79 Me and My School survey, 722
encourage child’s learning, Asian American, 77 school, 719, 721
ethnic identities, girls, 78–79 year level and gender
expectation and school achievement, 75 median scale scores, 718, 719
family support and cohesion, 76 scale descriptors comparison, 719
hierarchical regression analyses, 76 Perezhivanie, 680–681, 684
identify relationship, teacher and student, 75 Performance goal structure, 186
immigrant, 76–77 beliefs and behaviors, 183
involvement, 74–75 characteristics, 183
low-income students, 75 cognitive and behavioral engagement, 184
measure, racial socialization, 78 distinctions, 183
national data analysis, 77 emotional engagement, 183–184
students’ engagement and support, 75 grades, 183
Parents shape engagement, 28 student learning, 183
Partial credit model (PCM), 712, 713, 809 Person-centered approach, 136, 616, 696, 701
Participation-identification model, 7, 194 Positive behavior interventions support (PBIS), 575–576
academic engagement, 102 Process-product paradigm
affective engagement, 102–103 AET, 658, 659
behavioral component, 100 BTES, 659–660
cognitive engagement, 102–103 learning time research, 660–661
components, engagement and indicators, 103, 104 low engagement, 661
developmental cycle, 101 outcome research, 658–659
measurement issues teachers’ use, 659
affective and cognitive engagement, 103 Programme for International Student Assessment
antecedents consequences, engagement, 103 (PISA) 2006
Index 833

accepted variable structures, 298 student, 116


achievement measure, 297 Resilience theory, 7–8
administered science, 297 Resource allocation model, 264
behavior and individual interest, 298 Responsiveness, school and classroom, 105–106
coefficients and personal value, 298 Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA),
future oriented motivation, 298 298, 299, 791, 794, 799
historical and cultural traditions, 299 Rosenberg self-esteem scale (RSES)
modeling techniques, 299 factors, 808
out-of-school science activities, 298 structure, 807–808
Provided student ID (PSID), 728 RSES See Rosenberg self-esteem scale
PSSM See Psychological Sense of School Membership RSM See Rating scale model (RSM)
Psychological motivation theory, 134 Ryan and Deci’s motivation, 321
Psychological Sense of School Membership
(PSSM), 211
Psychological well-being, 323–324 S
Psychometric information, 768 School belonging, 568, 596–597
HSSSE, 775 high school students, 389
reliability, 775 increase academic engagement, 389
validity, 775–777 relationships, 388
Psychometric techniques, 467 research, 388
strengthen, 389–390
stronger associations, 389
Q techniques, hierarchical linear
Quality of friendships modeling, 389
extracurricular activities, 394 School identification, 100
lack of close friendships, 394 and academic achievement/attainment, 211–212
school-based friendships, 393 attitudes and behavior, 199
school transitions, 393–394 and behavioral engagement
social skills, 393 belongingness, 208
classroom participation, 208
“school delinquency”, 208
R types, misbehavior, 209
Race theory, 136 children, 201
Rasch measurement and Me and My School classroom community, teachers’ impact, 206–207
assumption, 710 components
fit model, 710–711 belonging and valuing, 195
survey item, scale, 711 education researchers, 197
Rating scale model (RSM), 809 motivation and behavior, 196
infit and outfit, 795 personal and practical value, 198–199
item calibration values, 795, 796 psychological theories, human needs, 195
response model, 795, 797 school community, 196, 197
Reading, 603–631 sense of belonging, 195–196
achievement, 107, 109, 111 student success, 196
and analysis, 593 development
comprehension instruction, 522 behaviors, 200–201
eighth-grade, 117, 119 and consequences, 200
enjoyment, 595 student, 201
levels, 519 educators and administrators, 213
and mathematics, 469 engagement form, 194–195
and mathematics achievement, 212 feeling safe, 202–203
and math lessons, 111 mechanisms, 207
mystery novels, 521 and out-of-school misbehavior, 209–211
strategies inventory, 111 positive attitudes, 193
Reciprocal feedback effects, 29 teachers’ relationships, students, 205–206
Regression analysis, SE, 122, 123 treatment
Resilience See also Every-day resilience African-American students, 203, 204
adolescents, 643 defined, 203
defined, 115 discipline policies, 204
and engagement, 116 multilevel modeling, 204
834 Index

Schools culture, 76, 79, 89, 559 relatedness and competence and autonomy, 27
adults, 749 reliability, validity, and potential contribution, 169
conceptualization, 748 SE, 466
experience, 748 and self-reflection, 466–467
leaders, 749 and self-regulation, 471
learning and achievement, 748–749 self-system processes, 27
Schools role student engagement
academic orientations, 80 autonomous motivation, 161
children’s friendships, 79 components, 161
extracurricular activities, 80 description, 150
identification, children, 81 learning activities, 161
individual engagement, 79 during learning activity, 150, 151
Latino and White students, 80 student-teacher dialectical framework, 157–161
Mexican American youth, 80–81 student-teacher relationships, 152
nature, schools, 81 teacher role, 152
racial and ethnic minority youth, 79 Self-efficacy, 244, 410
same-race/ethnic group peers, 79–80 Bandura’s theory, 232
school friendship, 80 consequences of, 223–224
social networks and possible friendship systems, 81 contextual influences
School success profile (SSP), 773 description, 230
engagement scales, 776 effects, learning environments, 231
MacArthur measure, 775 political factors, 231
School-wide and targeted interventions school transitions and learning, 231
Check & Connect, 576–577 cross-cultural research
PBIS, 575–576 difference, cultures and countries, 231
SDT See Self-determination theory (SDT) ethnic students’ experiences, 231
SE See Student engagement (SE) individualism and collectivism, 231–232
SEI See Student engagement instrument (SEI) social policies and programs, 231
Self-determination theory (SDT), 345 description, 222
autonomy support, 603 disadvantaged adolescents, 220
basic needs theory, 153–154 dropout rate, 219–220
causality orientations theory, 157 engagement, 220
cognitive evaluation theory, 156 high-performing schools, 232
differences, conceptualization, 169 identification, 220
distinction, motivation and engagement, 151 information sources
empirical effort, 169 anxiety and stress, 223
and ET, 135, 142 description, 222
features, 546 interpretations, 222–223
functional claim, 169 observations, 223
functions, student engagement outcome expectations and values, 223
concept and functions, 162 learning and performing, 220
engagement changes and motivation, 165–166 motivation, 220
learning and prediction, 162 perceived confidence, 222
learning environment and engagement predictors, 220
changes, 165 processes
motivation and achievement relation, 163–165 self-regulatory, 222
positive student outcomes, 162–163 symbolic, 222
student-teacher dialectical framework, 162, 163 vicarious, 221–222
teachers efforts, 162 reciprocal interactions, 221
goal contents theory, 155–156 research, 220
human and social activities, 26–27 school dropouts, 219
implications, teachers, 167–169 school success, 221
intrinsic motivation, 26 and self-evaluations, 224–225
motivation, 150–153 social cognitive theory, 220–221, 232
organismic integration theory, 154–155 and student engagement
overarching theoretical framework, 151–152 educational influences, 228–229
positive motivational development, 26 familial influences, 225–226
precursors, behavioral and positive engagement, 135 learning, 225
produce changes, student motivation, 170 sociocultural factor, 226–228
Index 835

students’ skills, 232 and achievement, 112


student success, 232 cognitive functioning, 646
underachievement, 229–230 and cognitive strategies, 103, 111, 112
Self-efficacy theory, 134 computerized task, 111
Self-evaluations, 245, 249–250, 667 defined, 616
behavior, 224 and engagement, 647
learning and performance, 224–225 environment, 648
positive, 224 learning, 267, 269, 644
students, 225 metastrategies, 267
Self-perception theory, 199 SDT theories, 471
Self-regulated learning (SRL), 465 and social and emotional skills, 479
academic behaviors, 647 strategies, 211, 636
areas Self-regulation and student engagement
cognitive and metacognitive strategies, 639 academic behaviors and functioning, 237
motivation, 639–640 cognitive engagement and motivation
time management strategies, 640 academic tasks, 239
behaviors, 244 assessment methodology, 240
categorization, students, 646–647 cyclical feedback loop, 239
control and monitoring, 245 future research, 240
description, 635 impact, social environment, 239
evaluation, 647 learning engagement, 240
integration and opportunities, research, 647 motivation and behaviors, 239–240
microanalysis school-based academic interventions, 240
aptitude measures, 250 SRL, 239
attribution and adaptive inference, 251 cyclical feedback loops, 240–243
components, 251 definition, student, 238
cyclical event assessment method, 250–251 description, 237–238
definition, 250 importance and applications, dynamic feedback
differences, metric and categorical, 252 cycle, 243–244
“event” conceptualization, 250 learning activities
forethought, self-monitoring and evaluation, 251 and academic tasks, 240
hierarchical regression analysis, 252 classroom environments, 238
single-item scales, 251–252 research directions
performance and impacts, 245–246 changes, students’ regulatory
phases processes, 252
learners, 639 educators’ knowledge, 253
monitoring, 637 environmental factors, 252–253
planning, 637 professional development training, 253
reaction/reflection, 639 psychology programs and report, 253
use and management, 637 self-efficacy perceptions, 253
reflection processes, 245 school-based professionals, 238
and SE SRL engagement and motivation, 244–246
academic functioning, 645–646 SRL microanalysis, 250–252
behavioral engagement, 642–643 theoretical framework, 238
cognitive engagement, 641, 644 Self-Regulation Empowerment Program (SREP)
emotional and affective processes, 641–642 components, 248–249
motivation, 644 development, 248
positive and negative emotions, 642 evidence-based learning, 248
researchers, 641 primary instructional mechanism, 249
students agency, 645 self-evaluation, 249–250
students indicators, academic involvement, 643 self regulation graph, 249
treatment, constructs, 643 tutors guide, 250
self-efficacy, 244, 644 Self-system model, 136
self-evaluative standards, 245 Self-system process model, 100
strategic attributions, 246 Sequence × space learning map, 308–310
students, metalevel knowledge, 646, 647 Social-behavioral engagement, 268–269
success, research, 648 Social cognitive theory, 610, 645
Self-regulated strategy development (SRSD), 247 Social-competence, 197, 324–325
Self-regulation, 51, 222, 238–240 Social control theory, 197, 209
836 Index

Social emotions, 3, 260, 263, 278, 324, 414, 415, 480, self-reflection, 248
542, 550, 551 skills, 248
Social engagement, 102, 119, 268 Structural equation models, 211
attendance, 110 Structural strains theory, 496
behavior rules, 109 Student’ academic and extracurricular participation,
classroom (and antisocial) behavior, 110–111 108–109
Socio-cultural contexts, 679, 681 conceptual framework, 388
cognitive and social functioning, 479 definition, student engagement, 388
goals and objectives, education high-quality friendships and peer support, 392–393
behavioral engagement, 482 implications, research and school policies, 398
peers role, 481 maintaining school engagement, 397–398
research, engagement, 481 negative social experiences, 394–396
and interactions, 479 peer selection and socialization, 390–392
interpersonal relationships, 482–483 peers relationship, 387–388
models, influence, 483–485 positive and negative peer effects, 397
NICHD child care study, 485 quality of friendships, 393–394
schools, 486 school belonging, 388–390
self-efficacy social alienation and dropping out, 396–397
habits, 227 Student engagement (SE), 47, 50–57, 68–74,
intrinsic and extrinsic benefits, 226–227 161–163, 183–184, 263–271, 453–455,
longer-term goals and self-schemas, 226 699–700 See also Academic emotions
peers groups, 227 academic motivation, 107–108, 138
personal development, 227 achievement and attainment, 106–107, 114–115
stress and anxiety, 228 achievement and competencies, 598
students, lower-income families, 226 affective, 112–113, 121–122
teachers perspective, 227 analysis
youth and children, future-oriented odds ratios, 118
conceptions, 226 variables used, HLM, 117, 118
social competence, school, 480–481 behavioral and graduation/dropout, 119, 121
Sociocultural theories, 445 categorisation and analysis, 586
SRL See Self-regulated learning (SRL) cognitive, 111–112
SSP See School success profile (SSP) competency levels, 591–593
Stage-environment fit theory, 141 competent learners study, 586–590
Statistical model, 333 complexity, 745
contemplating and defining engagement context importance, 756–757
behavioral, 784–785 correlations, variables, 119, 120
cognitive, 785 definition, 3
theoretical framework, 784 description, 133–134, 745
types, 784 discrepancy, 745
description, 783–784 and disengagement, 123–125
measurement issues, 786–787 dropout prevention, 5–7
measures construction, 787–789 effects, status and academic risk factors, 115–116
motivation and engagement, 785–786 enhancement, educational performance, 97
rating scale and intended measurement model enjoyment, school, 591
congeneric measurement model, 790–792 ET and EEVT, 141–145
description, 790 and ethnicity, 136
item response theory, 792 extracurricular activities, 139
SEI, 793 future directions, 15–16
student engagement, 783 graduation rates, sample, 119
Statistical modeling, 500 high school completion (see High school completion)
Status and academic risk factors, 123–124 HSSSE (see High school survey of student
conditions, participating schools, 115 engagement (HSSSE))
nationwide American study, 116 human development model, 140
resilient, 115 intervention, components, 7
“role strains”, 116 learning and belonging, school, 596–597
Strategic content learning (SCL) learning opportunities, 597
description, 247–248 levels and time frames, 137
instruction principle, 248 measurement, 9–10, 117
performance phase, 248 motivational theorists, 141
Index 837

motivation levels, 594–595 description, 764–765, 805–806


multilevel logistic regression analysis, 120, 121 developmental difference, 778
neuroscience, brain growth and development, 135 DIF, 807–810
participants, 117 emotional, 771–772
participation-identification model, 139 ESM, 766
person-centered approaches, 136 interview, 766–767
policy makers and lay educational pundits, 138 malleability assessment, 778
post-school study, 595 motivation, 765
primary research questions, 116–117 observations, 767–768
regression analysis, 122, 123 operationalization, 777–778
research psychological construct, 805
definitions and models, 747 psychometric information, 775–777
schooling, K-12 level, 747 scholars, 763–764
student achievement, 748 SEI, 810–811
three-component model, 748 self-report measures comparison
research, school success, 134 description, 768
resilience theory, 7–9 instrument names, 768, 769
responsiveness, school and classroom, 105–106 self-report methods
school achievement, 134 survey measurement, 765
school and experience, 595–596 usage, 765–766
school leaving age and highest qualification, 593–594 subscales, sample items, 768, 770, 771
school reform (see High school reform) teacher ratings, 766
schools culture, 748–749 variation, groups, 778–779
SDT theory, 135 varieties, 769
SE and learning, 136–137 Student-mediated strategies, 664
self-reports, 597 cognitive engagement, 668–669
social characteristics, 590–591 “countoons”, 669
student learning, 3 description, 668
student participation, 125–126 goal-setting, 670
teacher conversations self-management skills, 670
competent learners research, 454–455 self-monitoring methods, 669–670
learning opportunities, 453 structure and routines, 670
most and least enjoyed subjects, 454 Student participation questionnaire, 125–126
subject identification, 454 Student performance, 105, 812
trajectories, five, 590, 591 attitudes, 499
whole-class settings, 448–449 background characteristics, 499
Student engagement instrument (SEI), 773 behaviors range, 498–499
administration standardization procedures, 728–729 educational, 498
bifactor model, 796–800 Students’ perspectives, 751, 757
cross-validation classrooms and interactions, 373–374
congeneric measurement model, 791, 794 formative role, 374
factor structure, 794 positive feelings, 374
reliabilities and correlations, 794–795 responsibility, 374
evaluation, 793 teacher control, 374
factors, 730–731, 793, 794 Students role, 181, 586
introductory text, 727–728 academic efficacy, 72
likert-like scale, 811 adolescents’ daily lives, 74
ORE, 728 African Americans students, 70
properties, 808 Asian American students’, 73
reliability and validity, 793 assimilation, 70
RSM, 795–797 awareness, racism, 72
Student engagement measurement, 404–405 beliefs, 73
behavioral, 771 bicultural efficacy, 73–74
cognitive, 772 black students, 69
compensatory relationships, 806 color perceive/experience discrimination, 71
construct definition components, racial identity, 69
affective, sources, 806 discrimination scale, 73
cognitive engagement, 807 disengagement, schools, 71–72
IRT models, 806–807 diversity, ethnic group, 69
psychological instrument, 807 education and behavioral engagement, 72
838 Index

Students role (cont.) data privacy issues and limitations, 727


education value, 72–73 GCPS, 726–727
elementary school students, 72 data integration
engagement and achievement, 73 administration standardization procedures,
gender, 71 728, 729
identity and discrimination, 68–69 aggregated results, 729–730
individuals identities, 74 cognitive and affective engagement, 729
instruments, school and student, 74 IMD and ORE staff, 728
measurement, 69 introductory text, 728
middle school students, 74 PSID and CLC, 728
minority students, 70 description, 725
pragmatic benefits, 73 limitations, 739–740
values, 71 progress, 739
Student-teacher dialectical framework, 169–170 student engagement
autonomous engagement and synthesis, 157–158 definition, 726
autonomy support, student motivation, 159 vs. motivation, 726
high-quality student engagement, 158 student-focused results
learning environment and inner motivational advisor-advisee change report, 730, 732
resources, 157 advisor-advisee level report, 730, 731
learning environment, opportunities and hindrances, data interpretive guide, 734–735
158–159 datasets, 732–733
longitudinal research design, 160 SPSS, 734
organismic integration, 158 standardization, 732
quality, student’s motivation, 158 TSR and SEI factors, 731–732
survey, 159 vision
teacher’s motivating style and reports, 159 GPA, 739
Student-teacher relationships, 205–206, 526 graphical representation, 737–738
classroom environment, 578 iterative process, 739
classroom interventions, 577 NSC data, 737
engagement, 578 student engagement, 736–737
opportunities, 578
teaching techniques, 577
Support for value examination (SVE), 436, 437 T
adolescents’ perceptions, 430 Teacher and peer support, 409
authentic value, 432 Teacher-made (TM) tests See Classroom-based
direction, values and goals, 430 (CB) tests
effects, SVE and modern orthodox Jewish youth, 434 Teacher practices, classroom goal structures, 181–183
examination and reflection process, 430 mastery
inspection model, 430–432 instructional approaches, 185
integration, religious values, 434 learning environment, 185
intrinsic/extrinsic values importance, 432 ‘set of practices’, 185
limitation, 432 social relationship, 185
OSVE, 435 support student learning, 185
parents’ support, 430 TARGET, 185
promote autonomy and positive effects, 432–433 teaching principles and strategies, 185
qualitative analysis, 433 performance
quantitative study and valid questionnaires, 433 description, 186
religious education, 435 high and low performance, 186
revisionist and orthodox, 434–435 instructional practices, 186
structural equation modeling and mediation paucity information, 186
analyses, 432 societal messages, 186
women’s role, 433–434 Teachers’ beliefs, 356–358
youth movement activities, 432 adult relationships, school, 85
Systems consultation advantages, same-race teachers, 83
Ad Hoc results, 736 behavioral engagement, minority college students, 82
aggregated results child and cultural norms, 83
description, 735 cultural discontinuity perspective, 83
school and district report, 735–736 description, 81–82
context diversity, 82
Index 839

effect, same-race teachers and student achievement, NRC report, 366


82–83 productivity, 375
ethnic match, teachers and students, 82 programs, 205
expectations and minority students’ engagement, 84 quality, 105, 625
family support, 85 real-world connections, 371
hypothesis, 83 relational supports, 369–370
interactions, 84 research, teacher involvement and caring, 526
limitation, teacher-student racial match, 82 standardized observation method, 380–381
minority teachers, 82 students’ perspectives, 373–374
students’ perceptions, 85 teacher sensitivity, 373
students’ positive feelings, 85 theory, classroom, 381
student-teacher relationship, 82 understanding engagement and behavioral
supporting, 84–85 expression, 366–367
teacher perceptions and expectations, 83–84 Themes and exercises
white teachers and emotional engagement, 82 description, 550
Teachers implication, 444, 455 empathy, 554
accuracy scores and teachers’ ratings, 168 feedback and input, 555
autonomy-supportive motivating style, 167 observation, 553
capacity increases, 167 ownership, 555–556
classroom engagement, 168 personal control, 553–554
high-quality student engagement, 169 student’s social/emotional development, 551
instructional effort, 168 teachers and school administrators, 551
monitoring and enhancing students’ motivation, 167 teachers position, 552
motivation aspects, 167–168 workshops, 554–555
Teachers involve parents in schoolwork (TIPS), 358–359 Tinto model, 494–495, 498
Teachers shape engagement, 27–28 TIPS See Teachers involve parents in schoolwork
Teachers-students relationship, 806, 807 (TIPS)
adolescents efforts, 371 TLI See Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI)
adult mentors and advisors, 527–528 Topic emotions, 263
appreciation, 206 Tracking engagement, 34
autonomy/competence supports, 370 Transactional stress model, 271, 272
behavior management, 374–375 Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), 791
bias, adolescents’ experience evaluation, 371 Two-component model, 194
caring, 205
changing interactions, 379
classroom assessment scoring system, 366 U
classroom organization, 374 Underachievement, 220
classroom setting and youth development (see beliefs and behaviors, 316
Classroom strategies) Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems
concept development, 376 theory, 316–317
conceptualization and measuring, 372 description, 315–316
consistent expectations, 206 discriminatory forces, 230
disengagement, 371 education and socialization, 316
effects, student outcomes, 371 effectiveness, 230
emotion exit examination, 316
climate, 373 family support, 229
interactions, 372 high-risk dropout populations, 230
encouragement modes, 205 interventions, 230
engagement theory (see Classroom Climate) poor academic performance, students, 230
failures, 381 positive educational outcomes, 317
feedback, 376–377 poverty, 316
high school reforms, 526 role and contributing factors, 229
improving interactions, 379–380 socialization and caste systems, 229–230
instructional interaction domain, 375–376 Understanding student engagement, 380–381
interactions and development, 366 abundant evidence, 404
interdisciplinary teams, 527 antecedent factors and outcomes
language and instructional discourse, 377–378 factors, social environment, 412–413
learning communities, 526–527 intraclass correlations, 411
measuring interactions, 378–379 motivational beliefs, 413
840 Index

Understanding student engagement (cont.) self-efficacy, 410


subscale and full-scale scores, engagements, teacher and peer support, 409
411–412 motivational beliefs
variables, outcomes student, 413–414 attribution types, 416–417
conceptualization and measurement goal orientations, 416
behavioral and cognitive engagement, 405 self-efficacy, 416
feelings and learning activities, 405 personal factors, 407
indicators vs. facilitators, 404 student outcomes, 407
indicators vs. outcomes grades, 404
metaconstruct and multiple dimensions, 404
typology and consequences, 405 V
uniqueness and redundancy, 404–405 Validity, 775
contextual factors confirmatory factor analyses, 776
dynamics and relationships, 406 engagement measurement, 776
ecological system theory, 405–406 engagement scales and constructs, 775–776
family and parent support, 407 psychometric information, 777
social-relatedness factors, 406–407 SEM-MacArthur, 777
validation, contextual model, 406
description, 403–404
instructional and social context W
affective, behavioral and cognitive Wehlage and colleagues’ model, 495
engagement, 415 Well-being and engagement, 437
peers and negative association, 415 caring and supportive relationships, 324–325
real-life significances, 415 foster adaptive achievement beliefs, 325
subscales and engagement, 414–415 homework, 325–326
teacher support, 415 parenting styles, 326–327
limitations and directions, 417 psychological well-being, 323–324
methods and measures relations and psychological, 323–324
academic performances and conduct, 411 transmission, educational values, 327–328
affective engagement subscale, 408 Wentzel’s model, 483, 484
aggression from peers, 409–410 Whole-class settings, 464
aggressive behaviors, 409 frequencies, student recognition, 449
attribution, beliefs, 410 ‘hands-on’ learning, 449
emotional functioning, 411 learning conditions, 448
learning and performance goals, 410 Wiley and Harnischfeger’s model, 657
motivating instructional contexts, 408–409
participants, 408
performance avoidance goals, 410 Z
procedures, 408 Zimmerman’s model, 640

You might also like