You are on page 1of 15

Ennis, C.D. (1991). Discrete thinking skills in two teachers’ physical education classes.

Elementary School Journal, 91(5), 473-487.


Made available courtesy of the University of Chicago Press: http://www.press.uchicago.edu/
***Reprinted with permission. No further reproduction is authorized without written permission from the University of Chicago Press.
This version of the document is not the version of record. Figures and/or pictures may be missing from this format of the document.***

Discrete Thinking Abstract

Skills in Two Integratingthinking skills such as focusing, in-


formation gathering, and organization into all
Teachers' Physical subjects is becoming increasinglyimportantin
elementary schools. The nonacademicsubjects
Education Classes of physicaleducation,art,and music can provide
opportunitiesfor teachingthinkingskills. In this
study, 2 physical educationprograms(8 classes)
were examinedfor the presenceof opportunities
to use thinkingskills. The programswere taught
Catherine D. Ennis by 2 expert physical education teachers at dif-
ferent schools using the Logsdon physical ed-
Universityof Maryland uction curriculum.Data were analyzed using
constant comparison. Results indicated that
teachersused deductiveand inductivestrategies
associated with teacher- and student-structured
experiencesto encouragestudentmetacognition.
Propertiesof the data categoriesrepresenteddis-
crete thinking skills, such as attention focusing,
comparing, and analyzing, typically associated
with thinking-readinessexperiences.

Assisting students to improve their thinking


skills is increasingly recognized as a pri-
mary goal of education (Costa, 1985). Re-
searchers have defined a range of skills as-
sociated with thinking. Skills can be
conceptualized on a continuum based on
the level of complexity required or the dif-
ficulty of the problem to be addressed. The
broad term "cognitive processes" refers to
complex operations that usually require
substantial time and effort and the integra-
tion of general and specific knowledge
(Marzano et al., 1988). "Critical thinking"
is the term most frequently used in the lit-
erature to encompass a variety of thoughtful
actions from highly specific mathematical
problems to more general reasoning abili-
The Elementary School Journal ties. Sternberg (1985, p. 46), for example,
Volume 91, Number 5
o
1991 by The University of Chicago. All rights reserved.
defined critical thinking as "the mental pro-
0013-5984/91/9105-0009$01.00 cesses, strategies and representations peo-
474 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOL JOURNAL

ple use to solve problems, make decisions, Solomon, 1987), mathematics (e.g., Wirtz,
and learn new concepts." Ennis (1985, p. 1985), reading (e.g., Jones, 1985), and writ-
54) presented a much broader interpretation ing (e.g., Glatthorn, 1985). Although Pres-
of critical thinking, defining it "as reason- seisen (1985) suggested that creative think-
able, reflective thinking that is focused on ing might be appropriate in nonacademic
deciding what to believe or do." Elementary areas such as art and music, there have been
thinking consists of basic skills that are es- few efforts to examine the potential for
sential to cognitive functioning in a variety teaching thinking skills as part of physical
of dimensions (Marzano et al., 1988). Basic education.
thinking skills are useful within a particular The study reported here was conducted
context and facilitate learning of content to examine thinking skills included as part
knowledge. They may involve such skills of two elementary physical education pro-
as focusing, information gathering, remem- grams taught by expert physical education
bering, and organizing. specialists. The significance of this research
A variety of methods have been pro- lies in the examination of basic thinking
posed to teach thinking skills. These are skills in a subject that has been overlooked
typically organized within the two major as a potential resource for enhancing and
categories of content-specific and content- reinforcing cognitive skills. Most students
general methods. Advocates of content-spe- participate in physical education classes at
cific methods argue that each discipline em- some point during the first 6 years of formal
braces a set of unique thinking strategies or schooling. The intensity of the experience
heuristics that can be best learned within may range from daily classes taught by a
its related knowledge base (Schoenfeld & specialist to a weekly session taught by the
Herrmann, 1982). Research within this per- classroom teacher. Physical education pro-
spective is seeking to isolate problem-solv- vides an excellent opportunity to increase
ing heuristics that experts use within par- the academic learning time designated for
ticular disciplines (Perkins & Solomon, cognitive skills and the positive attitudes of
1989). Teaching strategies developed from young students toward thinking. More spe-
this position assist students in utilizing con- cifically, reinforcing cognitive skills in phys-
text- and content-specific heuristics that fa- ical education may contribute as much as 3
cilitate problem solution (Pitt, 1983; Polya, hours each week to the cognitive learning
1957). Conversely, proponents of the teach- time available to students. By emphasizing
ing of general thinking strategies identify the characteristics of basic thinking skills in
components of reasoning or matrices of experientially different environments,
thinking skills (e.g., Safrit, Ennis, & Nagle, teachers build relevant associations that
1988; Sternberg, 1985) that are believed to contribute to cognitive transfer (Perkins &
be effective in examining and solving a wide Salomon, 1989). Second, integrating think-
range of problems. Because these skills are ing skills into a subject that most students
useful within a variety of disciplines, they enjoy can contribute to positive attitudes.
can be integrated and reinforced across the Traditionally, physical education has
curriculum. proven interesting and enjoyable to some
The integrated approach to the teaching students who have limited success in other
of thinking involves a cross-disciplinary ef- subjects. As these students begin to asso-
fort to introduce, support, and maintain key ciate thinking skills with activities in which
thinking skills by using examples of content they are successful, they are more likely to
or problems from many disciplines. Recent perceive the skills' relevance to academic
articles supporting the integrated approach and other experiences. Most elementary
offer suggestions for incorporating thinking students eagerly anticipate physical edu-
skills in social studies (e.g., Beyer, 1985; cation class. Integrating thinking skills into
MAY 1991
THINKINGSKILLS 475

physical education can increases students' tured based on a modification of Laban's


exposure to these skills and foster the de- four movement aspects of body, space, ef-
velopment of positive attitudes toward fort, and relationships. In other words, stu-
thinking. dents in these physical education classes
were primarily interested in answering four
Method basic questions about movement: What can
This study was conducted within the sym- my body do? Where is my body moving?
bolic interactionist paradigm (Jacob, 1987) How is my body performing the move-
and was designed to include the teachers' ment? What relationships to other people,
interpretations of the educational settings. equipment, or boundaries are occurring as
The symbolic interactionist tradition within I move? This approach provides a carefully
qualitative research is responsive to both articulated body of knowledge as the focus
the conscious perceptions of the partici- of student thinking. The cognitive compo-
pants and the unconscious meanings that nent is emphasized within the Logsdon phi-
participants assign to events. The rationale losophy in the following statement: "Stu-
for the use of the methodology was based dents are capable of making decisions, and
on the premise that teachers are active par- education is responsible for helping stu-
ticipants in the instructional setting and dents develop the ability to make reasoned
thus both influence and respond to the choices so that they can adjust their role
learning environment. The teachers were appropriately as their social and physical
not informed of the exact purpose of the surroundings change. Physical education,
research prior to the study because such to share meaningfully in education, must
knowledge might have affected their plan- provide experiences that improve the ability
ning and changed their normal behavior. to move, that engage thought processes,
They were told that the purpose of the and that contribute positively to the devel-
study was to examine physical education opment of a value system and the esteem
classes taught with the Logsdon et al. (1984) in which students regard themselves and
curriculum approach and that they would others" (Logsdon et al., 1984, p. 13). It
receive a report of the findings at the com- seems logical to assume that the Logsdon
pletion of the study. Teachers were formally approach would be conducive to integrating
interviewed at the conclusion of the study the development of thinking skills with the
and asked to respond to questions regarding study of movement.
both their perceptions of the use of thinking
skills in their classes and the investigator's Subjects
interpretation of events. The Logsdon approach has been used as
the basis for the elementary program in a
Selection of Physical Education school district in a medium-sized midwest-
Programs ern city. Two expert physical education
Although it can be argued that many teachers agreed to allow the investigator to
physical education programs have the po- observe their classes taught with the Logs-
tential to encourage thinking skills, one ap- don approach. Both teachers had been in-
proach appears to have the conceptual and strumental in developing the district curric-
theoretical sophistication to substantiate ulum based on the Logsdon approach and
these claims at the elementary level. Logs- reported that they followed it fairly closely.
don et al.'s (1984) program combines La- Each teacher had completed her master's
ban's (1971) fundamental movement prin- degree with one of the developers of the
ciples with the mechanical principles of Logsdon approach. Both had been selected
force similar to those taught in an elemen- by the district to be involved in the curric-
tary science class. The curriculum is struc- ulum-writing process and later to lead in-
476 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOL JOURNAL

service workshops for their colleagues. In In naturalistic research, the investiga-


addition, both had received distinguished tor's participation can range from total in-
teaching awards from the district. Susan volvement to having no interactions with
Dillon, the physical education teacher at subjects (Patton, 1980). In this research, I
Meadow Run Primary School, was 43 years interacted with teachers during noninstruc-
of age and had 20 years of teaching expe- tional times. I collected observational data,
rience. Jean Harlan, the physical educator documenting classroom events while I sat
at Foxcroft Elementary, was 38 and had passively at the side of the gymnasium.
been teaching for 16 years (all names are Field notes were recorded on a small lap
pseudonyms). computer and then transferred to a micro-
Eight classes of elementary students (N computer for analysis. The data files were
= 153) composed the similar to an ethnographer's field note jour-
sample for the study.
Classes were selected to represent a typical nal in that they consisted of a description
of events and behaviors from a nonparti-
morning in the school day. Four consecutive
cipant perspective. A second, handwritten
physical education classes at Meadow Run
School-one kindergarten, one first, and journal that included my interpretive com-
ments was also updated daily.
two second grades-were observed; at Fox-
Both informal and formal interviews
croft the four classes observed included one
were used in this study. Informal interviews
third, one fifth, and two fourth grades. Kin- were part of the daily interactions with
dergarten through third-grade students had teachers. Prior to class, I asked teachers to
physical education for 30 minutes twice describe their plans for the lesson and ex-
each week. Fourth- and fifth-grade students
pectations for student learning. Following
participated for 45 minutes on a similar the class I asked them to evaluate their
meeting schedule. Schools were racially teaching and their students' progress. I al-
and culturally diverse under a voluntary
ways attempted to remain neutral to both
paired-school integration plan. Minority positive and negative comments. These rec-
students represented 33% of the enroll- ords became part of a third journal (separate
ment. Twenty-one percent of the students from the field note and interpretative jour-
were African-American, 8% were Asian, nals) in which I recorded the participants'
2% were Hispanic, and 2% represented perspectives.
other minorities. The second type of interview incorpo-
rated a 40-minute formal, open-ended for-
Data Collection mat (Spradley, 1979). I interviewed teachers
I collected data using observation and at the conclusion of the 6-month observa-
interview methods. Four classes in each of tion period. The interview began with gen-
the two programs were observed one morn- eral demographic questions followed by
nine standard questions that focused on
ing each week over a 6-month period. Sup- three themes: (a) the teacher's use of the
plementary visits to the morning classes
were made at 1-month intervals for an ad- Logsdon curriculum, (b) the importance of
ditional 3-month period. Field notes were thinking skills within a curriculum that em-
recorded with an emphasis on teacher and phasized movement, and (c) the rationale
for a teacher's emphasis on particular com-
student verbalizations and behaviors. Fol-
ponents of content and for the selection of
lowing the observation period, formal in-
terviews were conducted with teachers. teaching strategies. The interviews were au-
Written documents in the form of the dis- diotaped and transcribed for analysis.
trict curriculum, school policy statements, Data Analysis
and teacher-designed lesson plans were ex- Observational and interview data were
amined. analyzed using the procedures of constant
MAY 1991
THINKINGSKILLS 477

comparison (Glaser & Strauss, 1967) and constructs and premises analysis (Goetz &
typological analysis (Goetz & LeCompte, LeCompte, 1984). Delineation of the re-
1984). Constant comparison is an inductive searcher status position should include a
process that occurs in approximately four description of the investigator's affiliations
phases: (a) comparing incidents and gen- and potential biases. I am a physical edu-
erating categories, (b) integrating categories, cator who has taught in elementary schools.
(c) delimiting the theory, and (d) writing the However, because of my association with
theory. In this study, I analyzed the field the university and my strict adherence to
note and interview data to identify common the nonparticipatory role, I should be con-
elements or examples related to thinking sidered an outsider whose role was limited
skills. Every example that might be con- in scope. I am interested professionally in
strued as contributing to cognitive devel- promoting quality physical education and
opment was included in the initial scanning believe that cognition should be empha-
phase. Similar examples were then grouped sized with motor and affective goals. My
into categories and rescanned in an inte- experience with the Logsdon approach has
grative process to detect common proper- been limited to studying curriculum ques-
ties. Glaser and Strauss (1967, p. 36) de- tions regarding the model (Ennis, 1990; En-
fined properties as "concepts indicated by nis, Mueller, & Hooper, 1990).
the data" (and not the data themselves). Informants were selected because of
Properties may vary in conceptual abstrac- their expertise in elementary physical ed-
tion. They are derived from a comparison ucation using the Logsdon curriculum. The
of similarities and differences within data principal informants were the elementary
categories. Examples that did not readily teachers who had both been trained in the
fall into categories were held separately for Logsdon approach. A key factor in the ex-
later analysis. In the third phase, I com- ternal reliability of naturalistic research is
pared properties across categories to test for the clear delineation of constructs and
the integrity of category membership, thus premises. The ability to locate the presence
delimiting the emerging theory. The "writ- of constructs documented in this study in
ing of the theory" in this study consisted of other research is dependent on precise and
a presentation of hypotheses or empirical accurate description. In this article opera-
generalizations to address each of the three tional definitions that remained constant
research questions (Chilcott, 1987). I then throughout the study, such as basic think-
used typological analysis to compare the ing skills, were described in the introduc-
emerging categories with definitions pre- tory section; the cognitive-content relation-
sented in the thinking skills literature. It is ships examined in the field study will be
important to emphasize that the categories defined and developed within the remain-
and properties emerged from the data prior der of the article.
to my perusal of the literature and were not Problems of internal reliability in natur-
revised or modified to fit accepted termi- alistic research focus on the question, To
nology. what extent do multiple observers agree?
(LeCompte, & Goetz, 1982). This is espe-
Reliability and Validity cially critical if several research sites are
Threats to reliability and validity of the being examined. In this study, access to the
naturalistic study were addressed system- school was limited to a single investigator
atically (Howe & Eisenhart, 1990; Le- in order to minimize disruption to the class-
Compte & Goetz, 1982). External reliability room and to preserve the natural environ-
was enhanced by recognizing and focusing ment. Four strategies were used to address
on three potential problems: researcher sta- the threats to the internal reliability of the
tus position, informant choices, and analytic study. These were the use of low-inference
478 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOL JOURNAL

descriptors, participant researchers (the two from a direct and thorough knowledge of
teachers), peer examination, and mechani- the interviewer's teaching environment.
cal recording devices (LeCompte & Goetz, Changes that occurred in the environment
1982). during the observation were monitored and
Low-inference descriptors consisted of compared with baseline observations re-
concrete and precise explanations from ob- corded prior to the change.
servations and direct quotations from in- Threats to internal validity of the study
terviews. Descriptors were used to provide were met through triangulation procedures
reviewers, judges, and other researchers within each school. The research design
with the means to evaluate the conclusions called for the reporting of critical events
(Goetz & LeCompte, 1984). Standardized from my perspective and that of the teacher.
observation and interview protocols were I compared field notes, interview results,
used to increase the consistency of the data. and interpretations to identify both positive
Participant researchers assisted by exam- and negative instances that either sup-
ining the data and interpretations. ported or refuted theoretical constructs.
In addition, a colleague experienced in Triangulation procedures were also used
naturalistic research agreed to review all to address the threats to the external valid-
files, journals, and interview transcripts. ity of the research. The study of two schools
The reviewer's primary role was to question provided the opportunity to examine the
the interpretations based on the data and phenomena in two locations. Findings from
offer counterexamples as alternative expla- two sites studied concurrently supported
nations for occurrences. The fact that the the reliability of observations and enhanced
reviewer had never observed the classes nor the cross-site validity of the findings
met the teachers served as a control on my (Campbell, 1979). The sites were selectively
potential biases. The reviewer objectively sampled from the 27 elementary schools in
analyzed the data to determine the extent the district to reflect cultural diversity
to which the categories and properties were within the student population. Results pre-
consistent. The clarity of the narrative and sented in this article are limited to those
the use of low-inference descriptors facili- constructs and behaviors that were docu-
tated the review. Rigorous examinations of mented independently at both sites.
the descriptions, interpretations, and con-
clusions were used to locate discrepancies Results and Discussion
and facilitate the search for grounded the- In this study, physical educators using the
ory. Logsdon curriculum structured the environ-
The use of mechanical recording devices ment to emphasize several characteristics of
such as lap computers and tape recorders basic thinking skills. They employed teach-
increased the accuracy of the observation ing strategies that emphasized both induc-
and interview data. I typed data into the lap tive and deductive methods. Properties of
computer while observing students and thinking skills identified in these physical
teachers. During the interviews, I used a education programs were consistent with
tape recorder to record all information. This characteristics of discrete thinking skills
permitted me to focus on responses and to documented in the literature.
structure follow-up questions.
Internal validity was strengthened by Thinking Skills
the extended period that I spent as an ob- Analysis of the data from two programs
server in the schools. This permitted con- indicated that teachers encouraged cogni-
tinual data collection and comparison of tive involvement of their students with the
constructs (Goetz & LeCompte, 1984). Spe- movement content. Teachers verbally en-
cific interview questions were developed couraged students to focus their attention
MAY 1991
THINKINGSKILLS 479

on movement skills and structured the kinesthetic aspects of moving was critical in
physical education classroom to elicit think- enabling students to gather information
ing behaviors. they would need later. For example, Jean
Both teachers emphasized metacogni- Harlan encouraged third graders to concen-
tion by assisting students to manage and ad- trate on the strategies required to be good
just their work. Properties of the data to "interceptors" of objects in game situations:
support the hypothesis are reported in the
right column of Appendix A. Managing in- Jean Harlan: You are beginning to think, and
volved (a) directing students' attention to your body is beginning to warm up.
the movement pattern, (b) encouraging stu- Throwers, pass the ball to other throw-
dents to identify errors, and (c) integrating ers. If you are not a thrower, your job
the movement within a larger sequence. An is to try to catch or intercept the ball ....
example of managing occurred as students STOP! Now, who can tell me some strat-
incorporated different locomotor patterns egies for being a good interceptor?
into repeatable sequences. In Susan Dillon's Harold: Stand behind the catchers and jump
class, kindergarten students selected loco- in front of them.
motor patterns, such as a jump, hop, and Susan: You can jump in front of them at the
leap, to combine into movement sequences. last second and catch the ball.
Students were asked to perform the pattern Robert:You can follow the throwers around
and identify errors based on a criterion for so you can intercept their passes.
a good performance (i.e., jump-landing on Jean Harlan: That is called player-to-player
two feet; hop-take off and land on the defense. You recognize certain kinds of
same foot; and leap-take off on one foot throws and can intercept the ball be-
and land on the other). Students then com- cause you know what is going to hap-
bined the patterns into a sequence and per- pen. Now let's try our game again and
formed the sequence correctly three times. use these strategies to be good intercep-
Four properties of the adjusting category tors.
were observed in these physical education
programs. Both teachers emphasized ad- The interview with the teacher at the
justing by assisting students to (a) focus conclusion of the study confirmed the im-
their attention on thinking about move- portance of directing students' attention to
ment, (b) explore characteristics of move- the academic components of movement ed-
ment, and (c) draw conclusions regarding ucation: "I consider physical education to
the effectiveness of the movement or strat- be an academic subject. Students should
egy in given situations. Adjusting usually learn to think about and analyze movement
occurred over several class periods. I discuss patterns. I have very specific objectives each
examples of each of these properties as fo- day that I expect my students to learn and
cusing attention, exploring movement char- remember. I begin to create this atmosphere
acteristics, and drawing conclusions. when I get them as kindergartners. We have
Focusing attention usually involved fun, but we come to work and learn. Stu-
three components. First, teachers reminded dents who transfer in from other school dis-
students that physical education was an ed- tricts must adjust to the fact that we don't
ucational class, not recess. Second, students spend all of our time playing games."
were frequently reminded to "think" or to The property of "exploring movement
"use their minds" to understand and solve characteristics" (in the adjusting category)
movement problems. Third, students were received the strongest support in the ob-
encouraged to understand movements bet- servational data. Examples from field notes
ter by observing, feeling, and listening. were classified as comparing/contrasting,
Emphasis on the visual, auditory, and categorizing, analyzing, and recognizing re-
480 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOL JOURNAL

lationships. Students explored movement in the complex operations of problem solv-


patterns through a variety of teacher-struc- ing or decision making. However, it was
tured activities. First-grade students in Su- clear to me that they were carefully consid-
san Dillon's class, for example, spent sev- ering the alternatives and monitoring and
eral class periods categorizing locomotor regulating their performance based on the
patterns involving flight. Flight occurred stated criterion.
when a student's feet or body completely The cognitive focus evident in these
left the ground. Locomotor patterns (on the classes was similar to the concept of me-
feet) in this category include jumps, hops, tacognition. Flavell (1976, p. 276) defined
leaps, and skips. First graders compared metacognition as the learner's personal
takeoff and landing patterns for each move- knowledge concerning his or her "own cog-
ment. Working with the teacher, they ana- nitive processes or products." Presseisen
lyzed classmates' patterns, named each (1985) has elaborated this definition to in-
pattern, and provided a rationale for cate- clude the use of monitoring and regulating
gorization. Movements involving flight to compare performance with stated objec-
were later combined with other locomotor tives. Characteristics of metacognition are
patterns to make repeatable sequences. summarized in the left column of Appendix
Students' ability to draw conclusions, A. According to Presseisen, monitoring in-
the third property of the adjusting category, cludes keeping one's place in the sequence,
was observed primarily through motor per- detecting and correcting errors, and pacing
formance, not through verbalization as in one's work. Regulating emphasizes skills
other subjects. Drawing conclusions re- that focus attention on the required topic,
quired students to compare their perfor- relate the known to the unknown, and test
mances with a stated criterion or standard. the correctness of the strategy. These skills
Susan structured the difficulty of tasks to have typically been associated with the
reflect the range of abilities of students in a teacher's role rather than that of the stu-
particular class. At times, she spent as much dent. When responsibility for monitoring
as one-quarter of the class period explaining and regulating performance is transferred to
the criteria and encouraging student dem- the student, the teacher's role becomes one
onstrations of both good performances and of facilitation. According to Costa (1984),
those that required "additional practice." this ability to monitor and regulate one's
Kindergarten, first-, and second-grade stu- own performance is a key attribute of for-
dents were invited to select tasks that were mal thinking or cognitive process skills.
compatible with their ability. In order to do Sternberg (1983) confirmed that meta-
this, they were first asked to perform a skill, cognitive skills improve when students de-
such as vaulting over a bench. If they com- velop their own strategies and practice them
pleted the vault without permitting their while performing interesting and relevant
feet or legs to touch the bench (criterion for tasks. Student-generated thinking in re-
good performance), they could move to the sponse to the demands of a task or task
next most difficult level. As she moved criteria appears to be beneficial. Susan Dil-
around the gymnasium, Susan emphasized lon and Jean Harlan encouraged students to
three properties that were essential when monitor and regulate their own perfor-
drawing conclusions: she (a) reminded stu- mances. The thinking skills were useful in
dents of the criterion, (b) asked them to making decisions regarding sequencing and
think about their performance, and (c) en- self-evaluation based on the content of the
couraged them to decide whether they Logsdon curriculum. In Appendix A data
should move to an "easier or more difficult categories have been juxtaposed with Pres-
place to work." It was doubtful whether seisen's (1985) categories of monitoring and
these young students were really involved regulating. Similarities are evident between
MAY 1991
THINKINGSKILLS 481

the categories of managing and monitoring, people or objects. Within a confined space,
and adjusting and regulating. students moved slowly at first and then
more rapidly without touching anyone. As
Strategies for Teaching Thinking Skills the speed requirements increased, students
Although these physical education immediately began changing directions
teachers used a variety of methods to en- more quickly and shifting their weight in
courage thinking, such as questioning and anticipation of the next direction change. In
multiple-ability tasks, analysis of the data this instance the unique characteristics of
indicated that many of the strategies could the task elicited the correct response. How-
be grouped within comprehensive cate- ever, when students were asked initially to
gories of deductive and inductive teaching describe how they were avoiding others,
methods. most responded, "You just go around
Teaching methods that involved deduc- them."
tive strategies typically included an explicit Jean then asked students to think more
statement of skill components in the initial about their movement patterns and to iden-
introduction. Conversely, inductive strate- tify a strategy they used to avoid others. The
gies allowed students to articulate for them- second time the activity was stopped, stu-
selves the key attributes of the skill. The dents responded with a more varied and
deductive strategy was exemplified previ- sophisticated analysis of the movement pat-
ously in Susan Dillon's use of specific cri- terns employed. For example, one fourth
teria in the vaulting task. Students were grader demonstrated how he tucked his
asked to use the criteria to evaluate the shoulder as he changed direction. A second
quality of their performance and make ad- student noted that your "feet needed to be
justments. In this instance, Susan (a) intro- apart and your weight back if you are going
duced the vaulting skill and (b) explained to stop (before changing direction)." Jean
the steps and components. Then she asked continued to increase the difficulty of the
her students to (c) demonstrate the skill, (d) dodging activity by adding equipment and
practice it, (e) compare their performance additional tasks to perform while dodging
with the criteria, and (f) decide on the height (i.e., tossing and catching a ball). In this ex-
of the next vault. A summary of these prop- ample, students experimented with the skill
erties is presented in the right column of before the teacher formally introduced it. As
Appendix B. the teacher and students discussed events
I observed inductive strategies fre- that were occurring, the components of the
quently in these physical education classes dodging skill gradually emerged. Once stu-
taught with the Logsdon curriculum. Teach- dents stated a strategy explicitly, the teacher
ers typically structured the learning situa- then reminded them to incorporate it during
tion so that students were required to iden- subsequent attempts.
tify key elements necessary for successful The thinking skills elicited through de-
performance and then to incorporate them ductive strategies encouraged and influ-
in their own performance. Teachers assisted enced the extent to which metacognition oc-
students to (a) focus on other students' ex- curred. Placing students in interesting
amples of good performance, (b) analyze situations with problems that they were ea-
additional criteria, and (c) incorporate cri- ger to solve appeared to facilitate both the
teria in the next performance. use of thinking skills and the students' abil-
Jean Harlan used an inductive strategy ity to perform the movement task. During
to introduce the concept of dodging in her the final interview, Susan Dillon articulated
fourth-grade class. Students were asked to her rationale for the inclusion of thinking
determine which techniques or strategies skills: "Highly skilled athletes are good
were most effective in moving around other thinkers. They understand what they must
482 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOL JOURNAL

do to be successful and have developed the ars limited the categorization of cognitive
movement skills to allow their body to fol- skills to critical thinking skills in which
low their brain's commands. I feel my stu- identification of central issues, recognition
dents learn movement skills faster when of assumptions, and evaluation of evidence
they are analyzing and thinking about the play a primary role (e.g., Ennis, 1962). Cre-
movement. Teaching students to think ative thinking skills such as fluency, flexi-
about movement is just a natural part of my bility, originality, and elaboration (Torr-
class." ance, 1980) were also cited as additional
According to Beyer (1985), the deliber- components of the thinking process.
ate use of inductive and deductive strategies The thinking skills documented in the
can contribute to the development of think- two elementary physical education pro-
ing skills in social studies and other sub- grams reported here did not possess the so-
jects. Beyer described deductive and induc- phistication needed for complex reasoning
tive teaching as an interactive process and critical thinking required by Ennis
between teachers and students that can oc- (1962). Although this orientation may be
cur in a systematic series of steps. The steps possible in secondary and postsecondary
Beyer outlined were similar to the proper- physical education, it is unlikely to occur in
ties that emerged from data collected in this elementary physical education classes.
study (see App. B). Likewise, although it is possible to generate
There were a few differences in the de- creative thinking skills as part of physical
scriptions of deductive teaching styles be- education (Brockmeyer, 1987), creative
tween Beyer's categories and the field notes thinking was not a stated objective of the
based on my observations. A comparison of programs examined.
properties of inductive strategies, however, An analysis of the data from this study
revealed additional techniques that these suggested that skills documented were
teachers used to guide students' learning. quite basic and did not include the complex
For example, Beyer suggested that teachers operations defined in critical thinking.
should first introduce the skill, encourage However, the observed skills important in
student experimentation, and invite stu- the development of thinking might well be
dents to think reflectively about how they described as readiness skills for more com-
were performing. Susan and Jean appeared plex operations. Several schema related to
to refine this process by structuring the task teaching of thinking readiness or prepara-
so that students' attention was directed tion for thinking appeared to hold promise
toward key elements necessary for correct for classification of thinking skills observed
performance. Moreover, teachers assisted in these programs. Feuerstein's (1980) In-
students to improve their performance by strumental Enrichment Cognitive Functions
analyzing the performances of others and Taxonomy was consistent with the cate-
isolating critical components. gories generated in the constant comparison
analysis. Savell, Twohig, and Rachford
Classification of Thinking Skills (1986) conducted an extensive review of the
In this study the data were collected and research on "Feuerstein Instrumental En-
analyzed prior to the identification of the- richment." Costa (1985) simplified Feuer-
ory. The specific literature related to me- stein's categories to facilitate use by teach-
tacognition and inductive/deductive meth- ers in the classroom. This modification was
ods helped me to make sense of the selected because it was most compatible
extensive field notes. However, it was of with the teaching-learning environment.
limited use when classifying data properties The Costa (1985) modification presents
within the larger body of thinking skills lit- a hierarchy of thinking skills in four levels.
erature. Within that literature, some schol- Level 1 consists of discrete thinking skills
MAY 1991
THINKINGSKILLS 483

including input of data, elaboration of data, 1971; Logsdon et al., 1984), and many in-
and products of elaborations. Level 2 iden- stances of spatial, temporal, and sequential
tifies five thinking strategies or operations: relationships were documented. However,
problem solving, critical thinking, decision the inducing/deducing category was attrib-
making, strategic reasoning, and logic. uted more to teaching strategies than to stu-
Level 3 is composed of creative thinking dent-initiated use of thinking skills.
skills that include the generation of unique The "output" subcategory in level 1 was
products and innovative solutions to prob- most difficult to support with field notes
lems. Level 4, the cognitive spirit, empha- and interview data. This category, com-
sizes the importance of the impetus and posed of skills such as inferring, hypoth-
commitment to think. This level includes esizing, predicting, concluding, and evalu-
the qualities of open-mindedness, honesty, ating, was not represented as broadly as the
searching for alternatives, acceptance of input and elaboration subcategories. I could
ambiguity, and insistence on precision. not document inferring and hypothesizing
When the data from this study were through any direct examples. Predicting
reanalyzed using this classification system, may have occurred as students monitored
the categories and properties identified their abilities and selected the next level at
were most consistent with level 1, discrete which they should be successful. However,
thinking skills. A comparison of level 1 of it is more likely that these decisions should
the modified taxonomy and the properties be classified as monitoring or evaluating be-
generated in this research is presented in cause students often moved on to the next
Appendix C. Costa's level 1 categories ap- task after they had met the criteria, without
pear to be most consistent with the adjust- making an effort to predict success at the
ing category generated from the data. The next level. By providing experiences that as-
level 1 subcategory entitled "Input of Data" sist students to infer, hypothesize, and pre-
closely paralleled the attention-focusing dict, these physical education teachers
property discussed earlier. The input cate- could offer students additional opportuni-
gory included gathering data through the ties to use discrete thinking skills.
senses (listening, smelling, tasting, and feel- Presseisen (1985) suggested that skills
ing) and being alert to problems, discrep- associated with levels 2 and 3 of the mod-
ancies, and dilemmas (Costa, 1985). In the ified taxonomy are more appropriate for
study reported here, both teachers assisted middle and secondary students. She argued
students to focus on the importance of that elementary schools should focus on
thinking while moving and receiving infor- discrete level 1 thinking skills, just as the
mation visually, auditorially, and kinesthet- teachers in these physical education pro-
ically. grams were doing. Although it may be ar-
The property "exploring movement gued that Susan and Jean were structuring
characteristics" identified within the ad- situations in which students practiced the
justing category closely paralleled the level 2 strategies of problem solving and
"elaborating" subcategory in Costa's tax- decision making, the field notes and inter-
onomy. Elaborating included comparing/ views did not provide evidence that the
contrasting, analyzing/synthesizing, clas- teachers taught or intended to teach these
sifying/categorizing, inducing/deducing, processes to students (Presseisen, 1985).
and perceiving relationships (e.g., temporal,
spatial, and seriational). I identified numer- Implications for Elementary
ous examples of comparing/contrasting, Physical Education
analyzing, and categorizing. The impor- As the integrated approach to teaching dis-
tance of relationships was included as a cat- crete thinking skills continues to receive at-
egory of the curriculum approach (Laban, tention at the elementary level, the contri-
484 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOL JOURNAL

bution of the nonacademic subjects in Discrete skills that encourage students


meeting these goals should not be ignored. to perceive, elaborate, and communicate in-
Although there is much evidence support- formation for thinking can be included in
ing the contribution of physical education elementary physical education. Limited
to the physiological, psychological, and so- school budgets and time restraints require
ciological development of the elementary- that every subject contribute to the total ed-
age child (e.g., Albinson & Andrews, 1973; ucation of each child. Physical education at
Vogel, 1986), the cognitive development of the elementary level should nurture the
students has previously been left to more
cognitive development of students as well
academic subjects. The theoretical curricu- as contribute to the development of positive
lum structure of the Logsdon approach cou-
values, fitness, and motor goals. The use of
pled with the use of inductive and deduc-
tive strategies is effective in the presentation theoretically structured curriculum ap-
of discrete thinking skills in physical edu- proaches with appropriate inductive and
deductive teaching methodologies can in-
cation. In this study, expert teachers were
observed and interviewed with a focus on crease the cognitive involvement of stu-
dents in physical education. The introduc-
thinking skills. The emphasis on the teach-
tion, support, and maintenance of discrete
ing of discrete thinking skills was already
part of their teaching repertoire. In-service thinking skills are realistic goals for ele-
programs would be appropriate to assist mentary physical education. Curriculum co-
these teachers to provide additional expe- ordinators, principals, and classroom teach-
riences that encourage thinking during the ers should support the efforts of physical
"output" portion of the task or lesson and educators to incorporate these skills into
to articulate the components of problem physical education.
solving and decision making.

Appendix A
Comparison of Metacognitive Characteristics with Properties of the Data
Characteristics of Metacognition Categories and Properties
(from Presseisen, 1985, p. 46) Generated from the Data
Monitoring: Managing:
1. Keep place in the sequence 1. Focus on movement pattern
2. Detect and correcterrors 2. Identifyerrors
3. Pace work 3. Integrate movement with a larger se-
quence
Regulating: Adjusting:
1. Focus attention on requiredtopic 1. Focus attention on thinking about move-
ment
2. Relateknown to the unknown 2. Exploremovement characteristics
3. Test the correctnessof the strategy 3. Draw conclusionsregardingthe effective-
ness of the movement or strategy in a
given situation

MAY 1991
SKILLS
THINKING 485

Appendix B
Comparison of Inductive and Deductive Teaching Methods with Properties
of the Data
Characteristics of Deductive and Categories and Properties Generated
Inductive Teaching Methods (from from the Data
Beyer, 1985, pp. 298-300)
Deduction Deduction
Teacher: Teacher:
1. Introduced the skill 1. Introduced the skill
2. Explained components, procedures, and 2. Explained the steps or components (cri-
rules of the skill teria)
3. Demonstrated skill
Student: Student:
3. Demonstrated skill
4. Applied the skill following the rules 4. Practiced skill
5. Reflected about the thinking process as 5. Compared performance to criteria
skill was executed
6. Made decision regarding next performance 6. Selected the next task

Induction Induction
1. Teacher introduced the skill 1. Teacher structured situation that required:
a. identification of key elements prior to
successful performance
b. incorporation of new knowledge in next
performance
2. Students experimented with skill 2. Teacher assisted students to:
3. Students reflected on thinking process as a. focus attention on other students' ex-
skill was executed amples of successful performance
4. Applied this new knowledge as skill was b. analyze skill and isolate critical com-
performed ponents
5. Reflected on thinking process as skill was c. incorporate new knowledge in next per-
executed formance.

Appendix C
Comparisons of Thinking Skill Characteristics with Properties of the Data
Costa's Modification of Feuerstein's Categories and Properties Generated
Taxonomy (from Costa, 1985, pp. 67- from the Data
68)
Level 1: Discrete thinking skills Adjusting:
1. Input of data 1. Attention focusing
a. Gathering data through the senses a. Focus on thinking while moving
b. Being alert to problems, discrepancies b. Attending to sensory information (vi-
sual, auditory, kinesthetic)
c. Being fascinated by the environment
2. Elaborating (processing the data) 2. Exploring movement characteristics
a. Comparing/contrasting a. Comparing/contrasting
b. Analyzing/synthesizing b. Analyzing
c. Classifying/categorizing c. Categorizing
d. Inducing/deducing d. Teaching strategies
e. Perceiving relationships e. Recognizing relationships
3. Output of the products of elaboration 3. Concluding
a. Inferring a. Monitoring/evaluating effectiveness of
performance
486 THE ELEMENTARYSCHOOL JOURNAL

b. Hypothesizing b. Regulating performance based on cri-


teria
c. Predicting/forecasting/extrapolating
d. Concluding/generalizing/summariz-
ing
e. Evaluating

Notes Ennis, R. H. (1962). A concept of critical think-


ing. HarvardEducationalReview, 32(1), 81-
111.
This research was funded by a grant from Ennis, R. H. (1985). Goals for a critical thinking
the Wisconsin Alumni Research Foundation. curriculum. In A. L. Costa (Ed.), Developing
Special thanks are extended to the teachers and minds (pp. 54-57). Alexandria, VA: Associ-
students who participated in this study. The au- ation for Supervision and Curriculum De-
thor was a faculty member at the University of velopment.
Wisconsin-Madison at the time this research Feuerstein, R. (1980). Instrumental enrichment:
was conducted. An interventionprogramfor cognitionmodifi-
ability. Baltimore: University Park Press.
Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of
problem solving. In. L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The
References nature of intelligence (pp. 231-236). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The dis-
Albinson, J. G., & Andrews, G. M. (Eds.). (1973). coveryof groundedtheory:Strategiesfor qual-
The nationalconferenceon the child in sport itative research. New York: Aldine.
and physical activity. Toronto: Canadian Glatthorn, A. A. (1985). Thinking and writing:
Council on Children and Youth. The elusive but important connection. In
Beyer, B. K. (1987). Teaching critical thinking: A F. R. Link (Ed.), Essays on the intellect (pp.
direct approach. Social Education, 49, 297- 67-88). Alexandria, VA: Association for Su-
303. pervision and Curriculum Development.
Brockmeyer, G. (1987). Creativity in movement. Goetz, J. P., & LeCompte, M. D. (1984). Ethnog-
Journalof Teachingin PhysicalEducation,6, raphyandqualitativedesignin educationalre-
310-319. search. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Howe, K., & Eisenhart, M. (1990). Standards for
Campbell, D. T. (1979). "Degrees of freedom"
and the case study. In. T. D. Cook & C. S. qualitative (and quantitative) research: A
Reichardt(Eds.),Qualitativeand quantitative prolegomenon. EducationalResearcher,19(4),
methodsin evaluationresearch(pp. 49-67). 2-9.
Jacob, E. (1987). Qualitative research traditions:
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. A review. Reviewof EducationalResearch,57,
Chilcott, J. H. (1987). Where are you coming
from and where are you going? The reporting 1-50.
of ethnographic research. American Educa- Jones, B.F. (1985). Reading and thinking. In
tionalResearchJournal,24, 199-218. A. L. Costa (Ed.), Developing minds (pp. 108-
Costa, A. L. (1984). Mediating the metacogni- 113). Alexandria, VA: Association for Su-
tive. Educational Leadership,42(3), 57-62. pervision and Curriculum Development.
Laban, R. (1971). Mastery of movement (3d ed.).
Costa, A. L. (1985). The behaviors of intelli- Revised by L. Ullman. London: Unwin
gence. In A. L. Costa (Ed.), Developing minds Brothers.
(pp. 66-68). Alexandria, VA: Association for LeCompte, M. D., & Goetz, J. P. (1982). Prob-
Supervision and Curriculum Development. lems of reliability and validity in ethno-
Ennis, C. D. (1990). Analyzing curriculum as graphic research. Review of Educational Re-
participant perspectives. Journal of Teaching search, 52, 31-60.
in PhysicalEducation,9, 79-94. Logsdon, B. K., Barrett, K., Ammons, M., Broer,
Ennis, C. D., Mueller L. K., & Hooper, L. (1990). M. R., Halverson, L. E., McGee, R., & Rob-
The influence of teacher value orientation on erton, M. A. (1984). Physical education for
curriculum planning within the parameters children. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
of a theoretical framework. Research Quar- Marzano, R. J., Brandt, R. S., Hughes, C. S.,
terlyfor Exerciseand Sport,62, 170-187. Jones, B. F., Presseisen, B. Z., Rankin S. C.,
MAY 1991
THINKINGSKILLS 487

& Suhor, C. (1988). Dimensions of thinking: in expert and novice mathematical problem
A frameworkfor curriculumand instruction. solvers. Journalof ExperimentalPsychology:
Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision Learning,Memoryand Cognition,8, 484-494.
and Curriculum Development. Solomon, W. (1987). Improving students' think-
Patton, M. Q. (1980). Qualitative evaluation ing skills through elementary social studies
methods. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. instruction. ElementarySchool Journal, 87,
Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G. (1989). Are cog- 557-569.
nitive skills context-bound? Educational Re-
searcher, 18(1), 16-25. Spradley, J. (1979). The ethnographicinterview.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Pitt, R. B. (1983). Development of general prob-
lem-solving schema in adolescence and early Sternberg,R. J. (1983). How can we teach intel-
adulthood. Journalof ExperimentalPsychol- ligence? Philadelphia: Research for Better
ogy, 112, 547-584. Schools.
Polya, G. (1957). How to solve it: A new aspect Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Critical thinking: Its na-
of mathematicalmethod.Garden City, NY: ture, measurement and improvement. In
Doubleday. F. R. Link (Ed.), Essays on the intellect (pp.
Presseisen, B. Z. (1985). Thinking skills: Mean- 45-65). Alexandria, VA: Association for Su-
ings, models, materials. In A. L. Costa (Ed.), pervision and Curriculum Development.
Developing minds (pp. 43-48). Alexandria, Torrance, E. P. (1980). The search for satori and
VA: Association for Supervision and Curric-
creativity. Buffalo, NY: Creative Educational
ulum Development. Foundation.
Safrit, M. J., Ennis, C. D., & Nagle, F. (1988). The
use of problem solving skills in computer- Vogel, P. G. (1986). Effects of physical education
aided instruction: An evaluation. Journal of programs on children. In V. Seefeldt (Ed.),
ComputerAssistedLearning,4, 227-243. Physicalactivityand well being.Reston, VA:
Savell, J. M., Twohig, P. T., & Rachford, D. L. American Alliance for Health, Physical Ed-
(1986). Empirical status of Feuerstein's "in- ucation, Recreation and Dance.
strumental enrichment" (FIE) technique as a Wirtz, R. (1985). Some thoughts about mathe-
method of teaching thinking skills. Review of matics and problem solving. In A. L. Costa
EducationalResearch,56, 381-409. (Ed.), Developing minds (pp. 97-101). Alex-
Schoenfeld, A. H., & Herrmann, D. J. (1982). andria, VA: Association for Supervision and
Problem perceptions in knowledge structure Curriculum Development.

You might also like