You are on page 1of 11

Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Full length article

Examining the beneficial effects of individual's self-disclosure on the


social network site
Hsin-Yi Huang
Department of Computer Science and Information Management, Soochow University, No.56, Sec. 1, Kuiyang St., Taipei City, 10048, Taiwan

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Today, individual's self-disclosure on the social network sites (SNS) have become a prevalent phenom-
Received 28 February 2015 enon. This study proposes a research model for investigating the beneficial effects of individual's self-
Received in revised form disclosure on the social network sites (SNS). In this study, social support and online social well-being
28 November 2015
are seen as the beneficial effects of individual's self-disclosure on the SNS. Furthermore, this study ex-
Accepted 14 December 2015
Available online 23 December 2015
amines whether self-disclosure increases social support and online social well-being. For developing a
more parsimonious research model, this study chose self-disclosure and social support as second-order
constructs. Self-disclosure is devised as a formative second-order construct that includes five compo-
Keywords:
Social penetration theory
nents: amount, depth, honesty, intent, and valence. Social support is used as a reflective second-order
Self-disclosure construct comprising two factors: informational support and emotional support. In addition, this
Online social well-being research also proposes that social support increase online social well-being. Further, online social well-
Social support being is hypothesized to positively affect the continuance intention in relation to the SNS. Data collected
from 333 Facebook users provided support for all the hypotheses. Implications for theory and practice are
discussed, and suggestions are made for future research.
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction profound (Posey, Lowry, Roberts, & Ellis, 2010). Kozinets (2002)
indicated that consumer's disclosed information can be seen as
Social network sites (SNS) have become an international phe- an ideal and unique source of marketing research. Through the SNS,
nomenon. SNS are online services, platforms, or websites that business can gather the consumer's disclosed information and
enable the construction and reflection of social networks or social build the connection between the brand and consumers. Therefore,
relations among individuals. Recently, scholars have begun this study would like to examine individual's self-disclosure and its
exploring the social impacts of SNS (Boyd & Ellison, 2007; Ellison, beneficial effects in the SNS context.
Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007). As of November 2015, SNS were the According to social penetration theory (SPT) (Altman & Taylor,
second most popular websites worldwide (Facebook; Alexa Top 1973), self-disclosure is the key concept in relationship develop-
Sites, 2015). Globally, increasing numbers of people have begun ment. Self-disclosure can contribute to not only developing a close
to use SNS. For example, Facebook, the dominant player of the relationship but also maintaining a relationship (Derlega, Metts,
social networking industry, has on average 1.01 billion active, daily Sandra, & Margulis, 1993). The development of interpersonal pro-
users logging on to Facebook on any given day (Facebook cess is orderly and proceeds through stages over time (Altman &
Newsroom, 2015). Online social networking not only represent a Taylor, 1973). As interpersonal relationships develop to more inti-
prevalent phenomenon but also a trend that offer many potential mate levels, individuals generally disclose more private informa-
opportunities for business. tion on a more personal level (Derlega, 1984). Individuals may
For better enhancing the impact of branding or increasing de- receive support from their social networks after they disclose their
mand for products, business would like to use an ideal source of personal emotions, feelings, and the situations they find upsetting
marketing research (Kozinets, 2002). In other words, the potential (Derlega et al., 1993). Therefore, social support can be seen as the
business benefits of self-disclosure in online communities are social benefit derive from the reactions others provide (Derlega
et al., 1993).
On the other hand, from the perspective of positive psychology,
self-disclosure would improve the individual's physical and mental
E-mail address: hyhuang@csim.scu.edu.tw.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.030
0747-5632/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132 123

health (i.e., subjective well-being) (Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2.2. Social penetration theory
2002). When people share their story with others, i.e., communi-
cate personal information, thoughts, and feelings with other peo- Social penetration theory (SPT) was formulated in 1973 by
ple, whether the experience is positive or negative would gain a psychologists Altman & Taylor. It is a theory on communication and
psychological benefit to themselves (Esterling, L'Abate, Murray, & psychology, which provides a better understanding on relationship
Pennebaker, 1999; Gable, Reis, Impett, & Asher, 2004). Therefore, development. According to Altman and Taylor (1973), social
online social well-being can be seen as the beneficial effect of self- penetration deals primarily with “(1) overt interpersonal behaviors
disclosure. Consequently, in this study, social support and well- occurring in social interaction” and “(2) the internal cognitive
being can be seen as the beneficial effects which derived from processes that precede, accompany, and follow relationship for-
self-disclosure. mation.” Altman and Taylor (1973) argued that this theory hy-
Broadly, the purpose of this study is to examine the phenome- pothesizes that relationships develop gradually, moving from
non of individual's self-disclosure on the SNS context. In addition, superficial, non-intimate levels to more intimate, deeper levels.
the beneficial effects of self-disclosure, i.e., social support and the Therefore, SPT explains and predicts relational closeness, which is
satisfaction with individual's online social life (i.e., online social seen in the superficiality of self-disclosures in a relationship (Posey
well-being), would also be examined. Further, this study in- et al., 2010).
vestigates whether individual's online social well-being increases The powerful metaphor of SPT is its “onion” analogy. Altman &
their continuance intention toward the SNS. Specifically, the Taylor compared individuals to a multilayered onion. When in-
research questions of interest are as follows: dividuals get to know each other, the layers will shed away to
disclose the inner self. Therefore, disclosure plays an critical role in
(1) How does an individual's self-disclosure impact social sup- interpersonal relationship development. People who develop the
port and online social well-being in the SNS context? interpersonal relationships from more superficial exchange of in-
(2) To what extent do self-disclosure and social support matter formation to more personal levels of interactions (Altman & Taylor,
in driving individual's online social well-being? 1973). They further suggested that the development of interper-
(3) Does an individual's online social well-being increase his/her sonal relationships (the social penetration process) progresses
continuance intention toward the SNS? through four stages: orientation, exploratory affective exchange,
full affective exchange, and stable exchange (Altman & Taylor,
The remainder of the paper is organized as follows. The next 1973).
section discusses the conceptual foundation of the research. Sec- According to Altman and Taylor (1973), orientation is the earliest
tion 3 develops the research model and hypotheses. Section 4 ex- stage of interaction, which is supposed to take place at the pe-
plains the methods and analyzes the data. The results and their riphery of the persona in “public” fields. At this stage, the interac-
implications are discussed in Section 5. The paper concludes with tion is mostly limited to public fields, and communication is
the limitations of the study, along with suggestions for future cautious and tentative such as a conventional formula. The stage of
research directions. the exploratory affective exchange is similar to that of relationships
between casual acquaintances or friendly neighbors. In other
words, relationships at this stage are friendly, relaxed, and casual.
Although this represents an extension of the richness of commu-
2. Literature reviews nication in public outer fields, the exchange is still based on ste-
reotyped responses. Affective exchange characterizes a close
2.1. Social media usage friendship in which individuals know one another well and the
exchange is freewheeling and loose. In other words, the relation-
Today, social media usage, such as weblogs, social network sites, ship is enjoyable and comfortable. Although communication at this
and video platforms, has become a prevalent phenomenon and stage is characterized by an obvious increase in the very private and
brings significant impacts to our daily lives. According to Kaplan central fields of the person, in core fields, willingness is mixed with
and Haenlein (2010), social media is a group of Internet-based hesitancy. Stable exchange is the final stage of relationship devel-
applications that allow people to create and exchange the user opment, and few relationships achieve this level. Communication
generated content. See-To and Ho (2014) indicated that because of at this stage is one of oneness, richness, and spontaneity. Dyad
the rapid development of social media usage, prior studies exam- communicators know one another very well and can easily and
ined the impacts of social media to different facets in the society via immediately interpret and forecast the other's feelings and prob-
the theoretical lens of information systems (Pai & Arnott, 2013), able behaviors.
marketing (Naylor, Lamberton, & West, 2012), academic perfor-
mance (Alwagait, Shahzad, & Alim, 2015), and social networking 2.3. Self-disclosure
activity and connectivity (Shahzad & Alwagait, 2014; Shahzad,
Alwagait, & Alim, 2014). SPT states that the process of interpersonal-relationship devel-
Park, Jin, and Jin (2011) proposed that although scholars make opment primarily takes place through self-disclosure. Wheeless
an effort in understanding the rapidly growing use of SNS, few and Grotz (1976) conceptualized self-disclosure as “any message
studies have examined the SNS usage from the interpersonal re- about the self that a person communicates to another.” Derlega
lationships perspective. According to Pornsakulvanich, Haridakis, & (1984) reported that self-disclosure makes individuals show
Rubin (2008), self-disclosure is a key component of processes of others who they are and what their needs are. Therefore, self-
relationship formation and maintenance. Prior studies that disclosure can refer to what people voluntarily and intentionally
examine individual's self-disclosure in SNS context mostly focus on reveal about themselves to others, which includes thoughts, feel-
the relational outcome, such as intimacy with relational partners ings, and experiences (Derlega et al., 1993). Although self-
(Park et al., 2011) and quality of friendship (Wang, Jackson, & disclosure is not equivalent to have a close relationship, it can
Zhang, 2011). Therefore, this study investigates the beneficial ef- contribute to develop a close relationship and to relationship
fects of people who disclose their personal or private information to maintenance (Derlega et al., 1993).
others within an SNS. According to Altman and Taylor (1973), and Cozby (1973), there
124 H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132

are three basic dimensions of self-disclosure: breadth, depth, and support, instrumental support, and motivational support. House
duration. Breadth is the amount of disclosed information, depth is (1981) also delineated four types of social support that can
the intimacy of the disclosed information, and duration is the decrease work stress: emotional, instrumental, informational, and
amount of time spent disclosing information. Wheeless (1976) also appraisal support. According to Wills and Shinar (2000), esteem
indicated that self-disclosure is a multidimensional construct, and motivational support can be seen as two types of emotional
which has been conceptualized along five separate dimensions: support, while appraisal support can be treated as another form of
amount, depth, honesty, intent, and valence. The amount of self- informational support. SchXaefer Coyne, & Lazarus (1981) pro-
disclosure shows both the frequency and duration of the dis- posed that when an individual's health is under stress, they need
closed messages. The depth of the disclosure represents the degree both tangible and intangible support (i.e., emotional and informa-
of intimacy of the information topic revealed. Honesty reflects the tional support). In addition, online interactions are virtual in nature
accuracy of the information that is disclosed by the other about and often rely on the exchange of messages. Social-media users
oneself. Intent (willingness) refers to an individual's self-revealing may receive social support from others that is intangible in nature,
disclosure. Valence represents the positive and negative nature of including information support and emotional support (Coulson,
the information being disclosed in the communication. In this 2005; Huang et al., 2010; Madjar, 2008). Therefore, this study ex-
study, self-disclosure is conceptualized and measured by Wheel- amines both information and emotional support in the context of
ess's (1976) classification. the SNS.
Taylor and Altman (1987) indicated that interpersonal rewards According to Taylor et al. (2004) and Liang et al. (2011),
(benefits) can continue a relationship. Benefits shape the founda- emotional support refers to offering warmth to another person and
tion that maintains a relationship and moves it to deeper exchange reassuring the person that he/she is valuable and cared about.
levels. Previous studies have already focused on the perspective of Informational support can provide recommendations, advice, or
interpersonal benefits, such as studies by Ko and Chen (2009), and knowledge to the person who needs help in solving a problem.
Posey et al. (2010). In their online community self-disclosure Liang et al. (2011) argued that, in online communities, these two
model, Posey et al. (2010) treated reciprocity as the social benefit. types of social supports are the major support mechanisms for
Ko and Chen (2009) also argued that self-esteem, social life, and social interactions. Wills and Shinar (2000) reasoned that from the
positive effect are the benefits bloggers perceive from self- perspective of benefit, changes in emotional support impact one's
disclosure. However, these studies paid less attention on the role assessment of one's life events, improves self-esteem, decreases
of beneficial effects from the perspective of social support and anxiety and depression, and motivates coping; while informational
online well-being in the SNS context. Therefore, this study probes support, which can increase the amount of available useful infor-
into beneficial effects (i.e., social support and online well-being) of mation to the receivers, helps them acquire the services they need,
an individuals' self-disclosure in the interpersonal relationship and results in more effective coping. Therefore, emotional and
development. informational support could be seen as the beneficial effects of self-
disclosure because they are “the perceived value of the desired
2.4. Social support outcome to the individual disclosing” (Omarzu, 2000).

The term “social support” is used to refer to individuals' 2.5. Online social well-being
perceived available social resources or the social resources they
actually receive from nonprofessionals in the context of both formal According to Diener, Oishi, and Lucas (2009), subjective well-
support groups and informal helping relationships (Cohen, being refers to individuals' cognitive and affective evaluation of
Gottlieb, & Underwood, 2000). Cobb (1976) and House (1981) their life. Subjective well-being can be seen as the individual's
defined social support as an individual's experiences of being consciousness and feelings about their lives, which consists of
cared for, being responded to, and being helped by others in their perceptions of pleasure, positive emotions, and higher satisfaction.
social group. According to Schwarzer and Buchwald (2004), social Subjective well-being is defined as an individual's cognitive and
support is information that leads people to believe that they are affective evaluations of his/her whole life (Diener et al., 2009).
valued, respected, and loved. In addition, this kind of information Diener et al. (2009) argued that subjective well-being is a broad
helps people to deal with life stressors and the challenges of concept, which includes several experiences, such as pleasant
everyday life. Sarason, Levine, Basham, and Sarason (1983) argued emotions, small, negative moods, and high life satisfaction.
that social support plays positive roles, such as the social attach- Previous studies have examined the relationship between the
ments that contribute to psychological adjustment and health. use of SNS and a satisfaction with life (e.g., Ellison et al., 2007;
Therefore, social support promotes positive adjustment and per- Steinfield, Ellison, & Lampe, 2008; Valenzuela, Park, & Kee, 2009).
sonal development, and offers a buffer against the effects of stress When people experience a high quality of life, they may perceive
(Sarason et al., 1983). Liang, Ho, Li, and Turban (2011) also proposed subjective well-being (Diener, 1984). However, those studies
that receiving social support can warm the receivers' hearts. People focused on satisfaction with real life, such as college students'
can feel gratified by their good experiences with their supporters in satisfaction with life at university (Ellison et al., 2007; Johnston,
their social interactions, and these experiences help satisfy their Tanner, Lalla, & Kawalski, 2013; Steinfield et al., 2008; Valenzuela
social needs (Liang et al., 2011). et al., 2009), students' family relationships (West, Lewis, & Currie,
Social support is a multidimensional construct and the diversity 2009), adults' health (psychological distress) (Kontos, Emmons,
of components could differ from context to context (Huang, Puleo, & Viswanath, 2010), and individuals' happiness in their
Nambisan, & Uzuner, 2010; Madjar, 2008; Xie, 2008; Zimet, romantic relationships (Utz & Beukeboom, 2011). In brief, little
Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988). It is assumed that people can research has been done to examine the use of SNS from the
receive different types of supportive functions through their social perspective of satisfaction with an online social life. In addition, as
relationships, and that these functions may be differentially useful suggested in prior research, people who disclose themselves can be
for various types of problems or stressors (Cohen & McKay, 1984; seen as confession in some group therapy at treatment and increase
Cutrona & Russell, 1990; Sandler, Miller, Short, & Wolchik, 1989). individual's well-being (Esterling et al., 1999; Gable et al., 2004). In
Wills (1985) argued that the social relationship provides four types the other word, well-being can be seen as the beneficial effect of
of social supports, including esteem support, informational self-disclosure.
H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132 125

3. Research model and hypotheses intimate relationships and for increasing well-being (Schiffrin,
Edelman, Falkenstern, & Stewart, 2010). In addition, Ko and Kuo
Fig. 1 presents the proposed research model for exploring in- (2009) indicated that the more people self-disclose on the blogs,
dividual SNS usage behavior. Self-disclosure is operationalized as a the higher is the subjective well-being they experience. Finally, Lee,
formative second-order construct that consists of five dimensions: Lee, and Kwon (2010) also proposed that self-disclosure has a
amount, depth, honesty, intent, and valence. Social support is positive effect on subjective well-being in the SNS context.
devised as a reflective second-order construct that consists of two
H2. An individual's self-disclosure has a positive effect on his/her
dimensions: informational support and emotional support. This
online social well-being.
study hypotheses that self-disclosure would positively affect social
support and online social well-being. Social support is hypothe-
sized to positively affect an individual's online social well-being. 3.3. Social support to online social well-being
Furthermore, online social well-being is theorized to positively
affect the continuance intention toward the SNS. Social support provided by interpersonal relationships is one of
the most robust correlates of well-being (Schiffrin et al., 2010).
3.1. Self-disclosure to social support According to Wills (1985), the resources provided by interpersonal
relationships (e.g., social support) play a significant role in deter-
According to Derlega et al. (1993), when individuals disclose mining an individual's adaptive functioning and health outcomes
their distress or their upsetting situation to others, the listeners (e.g., subjective well-being). Myers (2000) also indicated that social
might be able to offer information, advice, and guidance on coping support is an important source of happiness. Therefore, social
with the problem. Even if the disclosed information is not easily support is related to increased psychological well-being (Cohen
resolvable, the listeners can offer encouragement or motivational et al., 2000; Kim & Lee, 2011; Okun, Stock, Haring, & Witter,
support (Derlega et al., 1993). In addition, the authors argued that 1984; Wills, 1985). Some studies even suggest that social support
the help seekers' willingness to divulge their private information is necessary for subjective well-being (Baumeister & Leary, 1995;
may encourage their listeners to understand their needs and pro- Diener & Oishi, 2005; Diener & Seligman, 2002). Cohen et al.
vide support. According to Ko and Kuo (2009), bloggers who dis- (2000) also proposed that social support is thought to enhance
closed more information about themselves, such as their thoughts well-being through influencing emotions, cognitions and behaviors
and feelings, or who openly and honestly shared them with others, in a way that accelerate positive affect. In the present study, it may
would receive more social support. This unfiltered self-disclosure be possible that the social support offered by an individual's
can be seen as the signal that alerts these individuals’ need for “Friends” on the SNS would positively contribute to the individual's
support from their “Friends” on Facebook (Kim & Lee, 2011). Hence online social well-being.
self-disclosure will increase the likelihood of receiving support on
H3. An individual's perceived social support has a positive effect on
the SNS.
his/her online social well-being.
H1. An individual's self-disclosure has a positive effect on his/her
perceived social support.
3.4. Online social well-being to continuance intention

3.2. Self-disclosure to online social well-being According to Deci and Ryan (1985), if people feel happy about
doing something, they are self-determined and intrinsically moti-
According to Niederhoffer and Pennebaker (2002), self- vated to continue doing whatever it was that made them happy.
disclosure can improve an individual's physical and mental health Sheldon and Houser-Marko (2001) proposed that the main goal in
from positive-psychology perspective. The content of the infor- life is the pursuit of happiness. Therefore, when people derive a
mation disclosed on the Internet may be helpful for developing positive effect from online a social interaction, their choice of action

Fig. 1. Research model.


126 H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132

will be affected by this positive effect. Dholakia, Bagozzi, and Klein 4.3. Data analysis
(2004) argued that when people are satisfied with their online
interactions, they will be inspired to participate in the online virtual A two-step approach, recommended by Anderson and Gerbing
community Accordingly, it is expected that individuals who (1988), was adopted for the data analysis. The first step analyzes
actively participate in a SNS are more likely to experience the measurement model; the second step tests the structural re-
connectedness and feel happier (Valkenburg, Peter, & Schouten, lationships among the latent constructs. The aim of the two-step
2006). Past research in several fields had examined the relation- approach is to establish the reliability and validity of the mea-
ship between well-being and loyalty (e.g., Chiu, Cheng, Huang, & sures before assessing the structural relationships of the model.
Chen, 2013; El Hedhli, Chebat, & Sirgy, 2013; Spector, 1997). Therefore, the first step involves the confirmation of the mea-
Applying these arguments to satisfaction with an online social life, surement model, and the second step validates the structural re-
this study supposes that an individual who experiences satisfaction lationships among the latent constructs. SmartPLS 2.0 M3 (Ringle,
and feels happier about their online social life is more likely actively Wende, & Will, 2005) was used because it allows the latent con-
to continue to participate in the SNS. structs to be modeled as formative or reflective indicators. Ac-
cording to Chin and Newsted (1999), PLS places minimal
H4. An individual's online social well-being has a positive effect on
restrictions on several parts, such as measurement scales, sample
his/her continuance intention toward the SNS.
size, and residual distribution.

4. Research methodology
4.3.1. Measurement model
4.1. Measurement development Self-disclosure and social support are modeled as second-order
constructs with the reflective-formative and reflectiveereflective
The measurement items in this study (see Appendix A) are approach respectively. For the self-disclosure dimension, the first-
adopted from the existing literature and adapted to the SNS order constructs are reflectively defined, and the second-order
context. First, items for measuring self-disclosure are slightly constructs are formatively defined. For the social support dimen-
modified from Wheeless (1976). Second, the emotional and infor- sion, the first-order constructs are reflectively defined, and the
mational support measures are adapted from Liang et al. (2011) and second-order constructs are reflectively defined. Since PLS does not
Lin (2011). Third, the measurement for online social well-being is directly support second order factors, this research-generated fac-
adapted from Diener, Emmons, Larsen, and Griffin (1985). Finally, tor scores for each of their first-order dimensions. These were then
items used to measure continuance intention are adapted from used as reflective measures (indicators) of the second-order con-
Bhattacherjee (2001). All items measure online users’ SNS experi- structs (see Chin, Marcolin, & Newsted, 2003; Polites & Karahanna,
ences. The scales are slightly modified for the SNS context. Items 2012). To do so, we first ran the full research model in SmartPLS
are measured on a seven-point Likert scale, with anchors from with the dimensions for each construct disaggregated. The result-
strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). The appendix lists the ing factor scores of the dimensions were then used as the measures
operational definitions, sources and measurement items. of the aggregate constructs (i.e., self-disclosure and social support).
Both a pre-test and a pilot test were conducted to validate the Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was applied to test the ade-
instrument. For the pre-test, a questionnaire was given to 3 experts, quacy of the measurement model. The adequacy of the measure-
with Facebook experience, to verify its logical consistency, ease of ment model was evaluated on the criteria of reliability, convergent
understanding, wording, and the appropriateness of the in- validity, and discriminant validity.
struments. A pilot test was conducted, with 105 users of the target The reliability of the measurement items was examined using
online SNS, to confirm the measurement properties of the final composite reliability (CR) values. To indicate adequate reliability, it
items. The results indicated that the measurement model fulfills is suggested that CR values should be above 0.70 (Fornell & Larcker,
the criteria of reliability, convergent validity, and discriminant 1981). Table 2 shows that all of the CR values were well above the
validity. 0.7 threshold. To establish convergent validity, all indicator load-
ings on their corresponding constructs must exceed 0.70 (Fornell &
4.2. Survey administration Larcker, 1981). For the research model, all indicators had loadings
above 0.7. (see Table 3). Another criterion for evaluating convergent
The research model was tested using data collected from Face- validity suggests that the average variance extracted (AVE) for each
book users in Taiwan. Facebook was chosen as the target SNS construct should be 0.5 or greater (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). Table 2
because it is the second most popular site in the world, with more shows that all of the AVE values of ranged from 0.641 to 0.951,
than a billion members (Alexa, 2015 traffic rankings; Facebook indicating adequate convergent validity.
Newsroom, 2015). Furthermore, Facebook is the dominant player To establish discriminant validity, the cross-loadings and the
in the SNS industry and is growing daily. In order to target Facebook square root of the AVEs were used. The correlation matrix, pre-
users, a hyperlink that led to the survey website was posted on sented in Table 4, indicates that the square roots of the AVE on the
several local bulletin-board systems, on online forums, and to vir- diagonal are greater than the corresponding off-diagonal inter-
tual communities. Online users who have Facebook experience will construct correlations. Table 3 also shows that the loading of each
be cordially invited to support the survey. measurement item onto its assigned latent variable is larger than
The first page of the questionnaire presented the purpose of this its loading onto any other construct (Chin, 1998). Thus, the
study and a guarantee of confidentiality. The respondents were discriminant validity of all factors is supported.
instructed to answer all the questions based on their experience Furthermore, variance inflation factors (VIF) were used to assess
using Facebook. After we finished the data-collection process, 16 the degree of multi-collinearity. This study conducted a regression
randomly selected respondents were contacted through e-mail to analysis by modeling the continuance intention as the dependent
obtain their contact information so we could mail them an incen- variable and the other nine variables as the independent variables.
tive of US$17 in the form of a gift certificate. The web survey yielded The VIFs ranged from 1.509 to 2.936, all below the suggested
a total of 333 complete, valid responses for the data analysis. Table 1 threshold of 3.3 (Diamantopoulos & Siguaw, 2006). Therefore, we
lists the demographic information of the respondents. did not find a significant multicollinearity problem in this study.
H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132 127

Table 1
Demographic information about the respondents (N ¼ 333).

Variable Items Freq. Percent Variable Items Freq. Percent

Gender Male 159 47.7 Education Less than high school 4 1.2
Female 174 52.3 High school students 3 0.9
High school degree 21 6.3
junior college students 3 0.9
junior college degree 8 2.4
University students 80 24.0
University degree 89 26.7
Graduate students 58 17.4
Graduate/postgraduate 67 20.1
Age Under 20 46 13.8 Tenure of membership Less than 1 Year 3 0.9
20e30 224 67.3 1 Yeare2 Years 7 2.1
31e40 55 16.5 2 Yearse3 Years 46 13.8
Over 40 8 2.4 More than 3 Years 277 83.2

Table 2
Descriptive statistics for the constructs.

Constructs Items Composite reliability Mean (STD) AVE

Amount (AM) 3 0.918 3.788 (1.306) 0.790


Depth (DE) 4 0.877 3.427 (1.143) 0.641
Honesty (HO) 4 0.931 4.236 (1.059) 0.771
Intent (IN) 3 0.945 5.217 (0.970) 0.851
Valence (VA) 3 0.899 4.771 (0.941) 0.747
Emotional support (ES) 5 0.929 5.029 (0.983) 0.725
Informational support (IS) 4 0.946 4.995 (0.942) 0.815
Online social well-being (SW) 4 0.936 4.767 (0.949) 0.784
Continuance intention (CI) 3 0.983 5.765 (0.985) 0.951

Table 3
PLS confirmatory factor analysis and cross-loadings.
The bold value represents the loading of each measurement item on its assigned latent variable.

AM DE HO IN VA ES IS SW CI

AM1 0.828 0.437 0.207 0.169 0.007 0.202 0.177 0.350 0.347
AM2 0.931 0.673 0.274 0.020 0.035 0.127 0.129 0.220 0.169
AM3 0.904 0.570 0.274 0.183 0.020 0.218 0.249 0.314 0.227
DE1 0.499 0.801 0.357 0.070 0.066 0.087 0.195 0.202 0.136
DE2 0.501 0.862 0.239 0.023 0.149 0.059 0.087 0.123 0.039
DE3 0.534 0.824 0.203 0.002 0.149 0.003 0.008 0.111 0.002
DE4 0.515 0.707 0.094 0.092 0.163 0.064 0.059 0.081 0.081
HO1 0.220 0.143 0.817 0.374 0.255 0.435 0.453 0.220 0.295
HO2 0.251 0.365 0.872 0.296 0.215 0.325 0.320 0.275 0.306
HO3 0.253 0.214 0.916 0.473 0.342 0.390 0.418 0.370 0.423
HO4 0.273 0.358 0.903 0.379 0.272 0.300 0.354 0.267 0.333
IN1 0.118 0.001 0.457 0.917 0.453 0.451 0.475 0.490 0.434
IN2 0.158 0.029 0.412 0.918 0.285 0.406 0.384 0.416 0.407
IN3 0.116 0.016 0.332 0.933 0.366 0.402 0.427 0.396 0.371
VA1 0.098 0.180 0.291 0.236 0.829 0.393 0.345 0.392 0.320
VA2 0.026 0.084 0.256 0.344 0.873 0.376 0.284 0.345 0.376
VA3 0.009 0.137 0.265 0.441 0.890 0.331 0.287 0.369 0.273
ES1 0.206 0.105 0.284 0.258 0.270 0.802 0.602 0.382 0.270
ES2 0.161 0.025 0.364 0.453 0.399 0.927 0.731 0.510 0.506
ES3 0.169 0.051 0.314 0.391 0.339 0.894 0.702 0.441 0.325
ES4 0.132 0.025 0.357 0.519 0.421 0.906 0.740 0.473 0.485
ES5 0.231 0.101 0.449 0.290 0.355 0.710 0.568 0.468 0.441
IS1 0.110 0.036 0.380 0.462 0.375 0.753 0.938 0.513 0.494
IS2 0.150 0.021 0.383 0.457 0.308 0.733 0.942 0.519 0.495
IS3 0.212 0.113 0.426 0.470 0.303 0.720 0.929 0.496 0.457
IS4 0.308 0.198 0.401 0.277 0.270 0.640 0.795 0.464 0.343
SW1 0.280 0.149 0.360 0.397 0.326 0.416 0.471 0.842 0.509
SW2 0.258 0.106 0.325 0.441 0.451 0.594 0.606 0.892 0.581
SW3 0.306 0.115 0.198 0.460 0.379 0.433 0.441 0.916 0.539
SW4 0.336 0.256 0.264 0.374 0.332 0.426 0.417 0.892 0.545
CI1 0.284 0.057 0.358 0.420 0.347 0.443 0.503 0.627 0.973
CI2 0.255 0.050 0.376 0.440 0.354 0.468 0.464 0.577 0.981
CI3 0.269 0.052 0.404 0.425 0.387 0.488 0.489 0.594 0.971

4.3.2. Structural model assessed by examining the structural paths and the R-square scores
In PLS analysis, the explanatory power of a structural model is of the endogenous variables. Fig. 2 shows the results of the
128 H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132

Table 4 (b ¼ 0.336, p < 0.001). After individuals reveal their intimate in-
Correlation among constructs and the square root of the AVE. formation to one another, they would experience being cared
AM DE HO IN VA ES IS SW CI about, being responded to, and being helped by friends on the SNS.
AM 0.889
Even when this kind of support does not provide direct help for
DE 0.632 0.801 solving his or her problem, they would still perceive the enthusi-
HO 0.284 0.308 0.878 astic feedback and feel better. In addition, people who live in a high-
IN 0.141 0.005 0.436 0.923 pressure environment and are remote in terms of social distance
VA 0.019 0.151 0.311 0.402 0.864
could write or express their personal information through the SNS
ES 0.206 0.043 0.411 0.456 0.420 0.852
IS 0.211 0.097 0.439 0.466 0.349 0.789 0.903 in order to relax, or to enhance their physical and mental health.
SW 0.331 0.174 0.325 0.473 0.424 0.534 0.552 0.886 People can get a psychological benefit from sharing their story with
CI 0.277 0.055 0.389 0.439 0.372 0.478 0.499 0.616 0.975 their social ties, no matter the experience is negative or positive
Note: Diagonal elements (in bold) are the square root of the average variance (Esterling et al., 1999; Gable et al., 2004). Therefore, individuals
extracted. who engage in self-disclosure on the SNS could promote their

Fig. 2. SEM analysis of the research model.

structural-path analysis. All of the hypotheses exhibited a p-value satisfaction with their online social life.
of less than 0.05. The path coefficients between self-disclosure and As H3 hypothesized, social support has a significant, positive
social support, self-disclosure and online social well-being are effect on online social well-being (b ¼ 0.380, p < 0.001). People who
0.576 (p < 0.001), and 0.336 (p < 0.001) respectively, thus sup- receive the informational or emotional support from their online
porting H1 and H2. As H3 hypothesizes, social support has a sig- friends would promote the satisfaction with their online social life.
nificant positive effect on online social well-being (path coefficient Since social support could bring warmth and understanding to
is 0.380, p < 0.001). Finally, online social well-being positively af- individuals (Liang et al., 2011), it can be seen as an important source
fects the continuance intention (path coefficient is 0.616, of happiness (Myers, 2000). Therefore, people who receive this
p < 0.001), H4 is supported. The significance of all of the paths was kind of social support may make a positive contribution to their
assessed via 500 bootstrap runs. Overall, the base model accounted online social life.
for 37.9% of the variance of the continuance intention. As predicted, online social well-being has a strong effect on the
individual's continuance intention toward the SNS (b ¼ 0.616,
p < 0.001). Individuals who are happy with their online social life
5. Discussion and implications
are more likely to continue use the SNS. Chiu et al. (2013) also
indicated that an individual's satisfaction with their online social
The purpose of this study is to understanding the individual's
life would have a positive effect on his/her loyalty toward an SNS.
SNS usage behavior. Further, this research focuses on examining the
Finally, for an individual's satisfaction with his/her online social
beneficial effects of an individual's self-disclosure on the SNS. In
life, social support (b ¼ 0.380, p < 0.001) is slightly important than
this study, social support and online social well-being are treated as
self-disclosure (b ¼ 0.336, p < 0.001) in relation to shaping an in-
the beneficial effects of an individual's self-disclosure in the SNS
dividual's online social well-being. The result implies that although
context. The results provide support for all of the hypothesized
received the information or emotional support from others is
relationships in the theoretical model.
important in promoting satisfaction with online social life, sharing
with others by disclose personal thoughts, feelings, and experi-
5.1. Summary of the results ences is also essential for mental health. Thus, both individual-
related (i.e., self-disclosure) and social-related (i.e., social support)
As predicted, self-disclosure has a positive, significant effect on factors have a positive contribution to an individual's online social
social support (b ¼ 0.576, p < 0.001), and online social well-being
H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132 129

well-being. people to shed the layers of their personal “onions” and engage in
self-disclosure. Finally, SNS managers should develop strategies to
5.2. Theoretical implications promote the exchange of information and emotional support
among SNS members.
On the basis of the findings of this research, this study would
like to share several points that are worthy of consideration in
theory development. First, based on SPT, people would develop
intimate interpersonal relationships through self-disclosure. This 5.4. Limitations and future research
study has contributed to our understanding of the beneficial effects
of an individual's self-disclosure in the SNS context. There are two Even though we made every effort to effectively design and
beneficial effects of an individual's self-disclosure on the SNS. First, implement this research, there are still some limitations. First, the
after people self-disclosure on the SNS, they can receive benefits respondents to the survey were limited to Taiwanese Facebook
from the other party, such as being cared about. Second, they can users. Therefore, this study may have limited applicability to other
increase their mental health by sharing their stories to other countries. Second, as the data are cross-sectional, the statistically
people. supported relationships can only be viewed as correlational. Third,
This study applies social support and online social well-being to our study may have been affected by self-selection bias. The sample
explain the beneficial effects associated with self-disclosure on the of this research consists of active users. Users who have already
SNS. The results imply that self-disclosure has a significant effect on stopped using Facebook may have different perceptions on the
social support and online social well-being toward the SNS. This influence of social support and online social well-being. Thus, the
means that if individuals who want to receive support from others, results of this study should be interpreted as explaining the
they can disclose their inner self to others on the SNS. On the other continuance intention of current online users only. Fourth, the data
hand, people may improve their psychological or mental health were collected from a single SNS site: Facebook. Although it is the
through revealing themselves on the SNS. In other words, people most popular SNS in the world, the generalizability of the model
would see the SNS as the channel through which to look for support and the findings to other SNSs requires additional research.
or share their inner self. Therefore, self-disclosure on the SNS can Even if self-disclosure can promote one's self-worth by offering
bring certain values that are the basis for the individual's overall social support during stressful periods, there is also the risk of being
assessment of the benefits that the self-disclosure gives him/her. rejected or exploited by others (Derlega et al., 1993). People who
Individual's self-disclosure on a SNS can be seen as a valuable disclose their personal information and feelings may be at risk.
behavior. According to Derlega et al. (1993), when other people know our
This study emphasizes the linkage between psychological state personal information or feelings, they may divulge this information
and behavioral intention. Individual's psychological or affective to others whom we do not know or like; people who disclose their
state is related to and results from a cognitive appraisal of using the personal information may discover that after they had talked about
SNS - satisfaction with online social life the individual's continu- certain matters, others may avoid them or decide they do not like
ance intention. In addition, online social well-being represents an them. The disclosures make the disclosers vulnerable to exploita-
important determinant of individual's continuance intention to- tion (Kelvin, 1977). In other words, revealing one's inner self to
ward the SNS that has been ignored in the past research. Although another makes the revealer more vulnerable to potential risks.
Lin and Lu (2011) found that enjoyment was the major antecedent Therefore, another interesting area for future research can examine
of SNS continuance intention, the measure of enjoyment focuses on the harmful effect of self-disclosure on the SNS.
the overall pleasurable experience of using the SNS. Therefore, this
study can provide a better understanding on the relationship be-
tween individuals' online social well-being and their continuance
intention toward the SNS. 5.5. Conclusion
Finally, from the perspective of individual-related and social-
related factors in shaping online social well-being, social support To better understand the continuance intention of SNS, this
is lightly important than self-disclosure to online social well-being. study examines the relationships among self-disclosure, social
This finding is consistent with Myers (2000), who indicated that support, online social well-being and repetitive behavior. Further,
social support is an important source of happiness. Therefore, in the this study examines the beneficial effects of an individual's self-
SNS context, the outcome of social interaction (social support) may disclosure in the SNS context. The findings indicate that an in-
be slightly important than solitary action (self-disclosure) to dividual's satisfaction with his/her online social life is a powerful
contribute to individual's online social well-being. driver of individuals' continuance intentions toward the SNS. In
addition, self-disclosure on a SNS is beneficial for individuals. Not
5.3. Implications for practice only people can receive the social support from other people on the
SNS, but also can facilitate the happiness and satisfaction with his/
A finding of potential interest to managers of SNS is that online her online social life. We believe that the model proposed in this
social well-being plays an important role in shaping members' paper is not conceptually limited to SNS, but should also be
continuance intention. Online social well-being, such as pleasure, is applicable to other online services. We hope the model proposed in
an emotion-oriented concept. Support tools (e.g., photo sharing and this study can lay a useful foundation for future work in this
video sharing) are among the complementary services that can be important area.
used to strengthen the impact of a satisfactory online social life. SNS
managers should continue to develop novel and fun support tools
or services that gratify members’ needs to have a happy online
social life. However, it is important to encourage members to self- Acknowledgments
disclose, on the SNS, so they can construct the relationships
necessary for a gratifying experience and for continuance with the This research and paper were supported by National Science
SNS. Therefore, less-intrusive designs may be useful for leading Council (Grant No. NSC-102-2410-H-031-053-).
130 H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132

Appendix A. Questionnaire items

Appendix. BThe operational definition, sources, and measurement


items.

Amount: The frequency and duration of the disclosive messages on the SNS.

AM1 I do not often talk about myself on Facebook.* Wheeless (1976)


AM2 I often talk about myself on Facebook.
AM3 I often discuss my feelings about myself on Facebook.
Depth: The intimate degree of the message topic revealed on the SNS.

DE1 I intimately disclose who I really am, openly and fully in my conversations on Facebook. Wheeless (1976)
DE2 On Facebook, once I get started, my self-disclosures last a long time.
DE3 On Facebook, I typically reveal information about myself without intending to.
DE4 Once I get started, I intimately and fully reveal myself in my self-disclosures on Facebook.
Honesty: The accuracy of the message revealed on the SNS.

HO1 I always feel completely sincere when I reveal my own feelings and experiences on Facebook. Wheeless (1976)
HO2 My self-disclosures on Facebook are completely accurate reflections of who I really am.
HO3 On Facebook, my statements about my feelings, emotions, and experiences are always accurate self-perceptions.
HO4 I am always honest in my self-disclosures on Facebook.
Intent: The individual's revealed messages on the SNS are self-revealing.

IN1 When I express my personal feelings on Facebook, I am always aware of what I am doing and saying. Wheeless (1976)
IN2 When I reveal my feelings about myself on Facebook, I consciously intend to do so.
IN3 When I am self-disclosing on Facebook, I am consciously aware of what I am revealing.
Valence: The positive and negative nature of the revealed messages on the SNS.

VA1 I usually disclose positive things about myself on Facebook. Wheeless (1976)
VA2 I normally express my “good” feelings about myself on Facebook.
VA3 On the whole, my disclosures about myself on Facebook are more positive than negative.
Informational support: The messages received from friends on the SNS provide the needed assistance.

IS1 Some “Friends” on the Facebook would offer suggestions when I needed help. Liang et al. (2011); Lin (2011)
IS2 When I encountered a problem, some “Friends” on the Facebook would give me information to help me overcome the problem.
IS3 When faced with difficulties, some “Friends” on the Facebook would help me discover the cause and provide me with suggestions.
IS4 Over the last one month, I obtained sufficient assistance from my “Friends” on Facebook.
Emotional support: The messages received from friends on the SNS include emotional concerns.

ES1 When faced with difficulties, some “Friends” on Facebook are on my side. Liang et al. (2011); Lin (2011)
ES2 When faced with difficulties, some “Friends” on Facebook comforted and encouraged me.
ES3 When faced with difficulties, some “Friends” on Facebook listened to me talk about my private feelings.
ES4 When faced with difficulties, some people on the Facebook expressed interest in and concern for my well-being.
ES5 Over the last one month, I received adequate emotional support from my “Friends” on Facebook.
Online social well-being: An individual's own assessment of his/her happiness and satisfaction with his/her social life on the SNS.

SW1 In most respects, my online social life in Facebook is close to my ideal. Diener et al. (1985)
SW2 The conditions of my online social life on Facebook are excellent.
SW3 I am satisfied with my online social life on Facebook.
SW4 So far, I have obtained the important things I want from my online social life on Facebook.
Continuance intention: The individual's willingness to revisit the SNS.

CI1 If I could, I would like to continue using Facebook in the future. Bhattacherjee (2001)
CI2 It is likely that I will continue using Facebook in the future.
CI3 I plan to continue using Facebook in the future.
*
Negatively worded items.

References
Monte Carlo simulation study and electronic-mail emotion/adoption study.
Alexa. (2015). Alexa top sites Accessed 23.11.15 http://www.alexa.com/topsites. Information Systems Research, 14(2), 189e217.
Altman, I., & Taylor, D. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal Chin, W. W., & Newsted, P. R. (1999). Structural equation modeling analysis with
relationships. NewYork: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. small samples using partial least squares. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Statistical stra-
Alwagait, E., Shahzad, B., & Alim, S. (2015). Impact of social media usage on students tegies for small sample research (pp. 307e341). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
academic performance in Saudi Arabia. Computers in Human Behavior, 51, Publications.
1092e1097. Chiu, C. M., Cheng, H. L., Huang, H. Y., & Chen, C. F. (2013). Exploring individuals'
Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a subjective well-being and loyalty towards social network sites from the
review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, perspective of network externalities: the Facebook case. International Journal of
411e423. Information Management, 33(3), 539e552.
Baumeister, R., & Leary, M. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine,
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 38(5), 300e314.
497e529. Cohen, S., Gottlieb, B., & Underwood, L. (2000). Theoretical and historical per-
Bhattacherjee, A. (2001). Understanding information systems continuance: an spectives. In S. Cohen, L. Underwood, & B. Gottlieb (Eds.), Social support mea-
expectation-confirmation model. MIS Quarterly, 25, 351e370. surement and intervention: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 3e28).
Boyd, D., & Ellison, N. (2007). Social network sites: definition, history, and schol- New York: Oxford University Press.
arship. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 210e230. Cohen, S., & McKay, G. (1984). Social support, stress, and the buffering hypothesis: a
Chin, W. W. (1998). Issues and opinions on structural equation modeling. MIS theoretical analysis. In A. Baum, J. E. Singer, & S. E. Taylor (Eds.), Handbook of
Quarterly, 22, 7e16. psychology and health (Vol. 4, pp. 253e267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chin, W. W., Marcolin, B. L., & Newsted, P. R. (2003). A partial least squares latent Coulson, N. S. (2005). Receiving social support online: an analysis of a computer-
variable modeling approach for measuring interaction effects: results from a mediated support group for individuals living with irritable bowel syndrome.
H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132 131

Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 8(6), 580e584. Psychologist, 55, 56e67.


Cozby, P. (1973). Self-disclosure: a literature review. Psychological Bulletin, 79, Naylor, R. W., Lamberton, C. P., & West, P. M. (2012). Beyond the ‘‘Like’’ button: the
73e91. impact of mere virtual presence on brand evaluations and purchase intentions
Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. (1990). Type of social support and specific stress: toward in social media settings. Journal of Marketing, 76(6), 105e120.
a theory of optimal matching. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Niederhoffer, K. G., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). Sharing one's story. In C. R. Snyder, &
Social support: an interactional view (pp. 319e366). New York: Wiley. S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 573e583). New York:
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human Oxford University Press, Inc.
behavior. New York: Plenum. Okun, M., Stock, W., Haring, M., & Witter, R. (1984). The social activity/subjective
Derlega, V. J. (1984). Self-disclosure and intimate relationships. In V. J. Derlega (Ed.), well-being relation: a quantitative synthesis. Research on Aging, 6, 45e65.
Communication, intimacy and close relationships (pp. 1e10). Orlando, FL: Aca- Omarzu, J. (2000). A disclosure decision model: determining how and when in-
demic Press. dividuals will self-disclose. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 174e185.
Derlega, V. J., Metts, S., Sandra, P., & Margulis, S. T. (1993). Self-disclosure. California: Pai, P., & Arnott, D. C. (2013). User adoption of social networking sites: eliciting uses
Sage Publications, Inc. and gratifications through a means-end approach. Computers in Human
Dholakia, U. M., Bagozzi, R. P., & Klein, L. R. (2004). A social influence model of Behavior, 29(3), 1039e1053.
consumer participation in network and small group-based virtual communities. Park, N., Jin, B., & Jin, S. A. (2011). Effects of self-disclosure on relational intimacy in
International Journal of Research in Marketing, 21(3), 241e263. Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 1974e1983.
Diamantopoulos, A., & Siguaw, J. A. (2006). Formative versus reflective indicators in Polites, G., & Karahanna, E. (2012). Shackled to the status quo: the inhibiting effects
organizational measure development: a comparison and empirical illustration. of incumbent system habit, switching costs, and inertia on new system
British Journal of Management, 17(4), 263e282. acceptance. MIS Quarterly, 36(1), 21e42.
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 542e575. Pornsakulvanich, V., Haridakis, P. M., & Rubin, A. M. (2008). The influence of dis-
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life positions and internet-use motivation on online communication satisfaction
scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71e75. and relationship closeness. Computer in Human Behavior, 24(5), 2292e2310.
Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2005). The nonobvious social psychology of happiness. Psy- Posey, C., Lowry, P. B., Roberts, T. L., & Ellis, T. S. (2010). Proposing the online
chological Inquiry, 16, 162e167. community self-disclosure model: the case of working professionals in France
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2009). Subjective well-being: the science of and the UK who use online communities. European Journal of Information Sys-
happiness and life satisfaction. In C. R. Snyder, & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Oxford hand- tems, 19, 181e195.
book of positive psychology (pp. 187e194). New York: Oxford University Press. Ringle, C. M., Wende, S., & Will, S. (2005). SmartPLS 2.0 (M3) Beta, Hamburg. http://
Diener, E., & Seligman, E. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13(1), www.smartpls.de.
81e84. Sandler, I. N., Miller, P., Short, J., & Wolchik, S. A. (1989). Social support as a pro-
El Hedhli, K., Chebat, J. C., & Sirgy, M. J. (2013). Shopping well-being at the mall: tective factor for children in stress. In D. Belle (Ed.), Children's social networks
construct, antecedents, and consequences. Journal of Business Research, 66(7), and social supports (pp. 277e307). New York: Wiley.
856e863. Sarason, I. G., Levine, H. M., Basham, R. B., & Sarason, B. R. (1983). Assessing social
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook ‘‘Friends:’’ support: the social support questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social
social capital and college students' use of online social network sites. Journal of Psychology, 44(1), 127e139.
Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 1143e1168. Schiffrin, H., Edelman, A., Falkenstern, M., & Stewart, C. (2010). The associations
Esterling, B. A., L'Abate, L., Murray, E. J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1999). Empirical among computer- mediated communication, relationships, and well-being.
foundations for writing in prevention and psychotherapy: mental and physical Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13(3), 299e306.
health outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 79e96. Schwarzer, C., & Buchwald, P. (2004). Social support. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.),
Facebook Newsroom.(2015). http://newsroom.fb.com/company-info/ accessed 23.11.15. Encyclopedia of applied psychology (pp. 435e441). Academic Press.
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with un- SchXaefer, C., Coyne, J. C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). The health-related functions of
observable and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 39e50. social support. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 381e406.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do when See-To, E. W. K., & Ho, K. K. W. (2014). Value co-creation and purchase intention in
things go right? The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing posi- social network sites: the role of electronic word-of-mouth and trust: a theo-
tive events. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 87, 228e245. retical analysis. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 182e189.
House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Shahzad, B., & Alwagait, E. (2014). Does a change in weekend days have an impact
Huang, K. Y., Nambisan, P., & Uzuner, O. € (2010). Informational support or emotional on social networking activity? Journal of Universal Computer Science, 20(15),
support: preliminary study of an automated approach to analyze online support 2068e2079.
community contents. In Paper presented at the International Conference on in- Shahzad, B., Alwagait, E., & Alim, S. (2014). Impact of change in weekend days on
formation systems (ICIS), St. Louis, MO. social networking culture in Saudi Arabia. Barcelona: Future Internet of Things
Johnston, K., Tanner, M., Lalla, N., & Kawalski, D. (2013). Social capital: the benefit of and Cloud (FiCloud).
Facebook “Friends”. Behaviour & Information Technology, 32(1), 24e36. Sheldon, K. M., & Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Self-concordance, goal-attainment, and
Kaplan, A., & Haenlein, M. (2010). Users of the world, unite! the challenges and the pursuit of happiness: can there be an upward spiral? Journal of Personality
opportunities of social media. Business Horizons, 53(1), 59e68. and Social Psychology, 80, 152e165.
Kelvin, P. (1977). Predictability, power and vulnerability in interpersonal attraction. Spector, P. E. (1997). Job satisfaction: Application, assessment, causes, and conse-
In S. Duck (Ed.), Theory and practice in interpersonal attraction (pp. 355e378). quences. California: Sage.
New York: Academic Press. Steinfield, C., Ellison, N., & Lampe, C. (2008). Social capital, self-esteem, and use of
Kim, J., & Lee, J. E. R. (2011). The Facebook paths to happiness: effects of the number online social network sites: a longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Develop-
of facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. Cyberp- mental Psychology, 29, 434e445.
sychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(6), 359e364. Taylor, D. A., & Altman, I. (1987). Communication in interpersonal relationships: social
Ko, H. C., & Chen, T. K. (2009). Understanding the continuous self-disclosure of penetration processes. In M. Roloff, & G. Miller (Eds.), Interpersonal processes: New
bloggers from the cost-benefit perspective. In Proceedings of the 2nd Conference directions in communication research (pp. 257e277). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
on human system interactions, Cantania, Italy. Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Kim, H. S., Jarcho, J., Takagi, K., & Dunagan, M. S. (2004).
Ko, H., & Kuo, F. (2009). Can blogging enhance subjective well-being through self- Culture and social support: who seeks it and why? Journal of Personality and
disclosure? CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12, 75e79. Social Psychology, 87(3), 354e362.
Kontos, E. Z., Emmons, K. M., Puleo, E., & Viswanath, K. (2010). Communication Utz, S., & Beukeboom, C. J. (2011). The role of social network sites in romantic re-
inequalities and public health implications of adult social networking site use in lationships: effects on jealousy and relationship happiness. Journal of Computer-
the United States. Journal of Health Communication, 15, 216e235. Mediated Communication, 16(4), 511e527.
Kozinets, R. V. (2002). The field behind the screen: using netnography for marketing Valenzuela, S., Park, N., & Kee, K. F. (2009). Is there social capital in a social network
research in online communities. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(1), 61e72. site? Facebook use, and college students' life satisfaction, trust, and participa-
Lee, G., Lee, J., & Kwon, S. (2010). Use of social-networking sites and subjective well- tion. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14(4), 875e901.
being: a study in South Korea. CyberPsychology, Behavior, & Social Networking, Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Schouten, A. P. (2006). Friend networking sites and
14, 151e155. their relationship to adolescents' well being and social self-esteem. CyberP-
Liang, T. P., Ho, Y. T., Li, Y. W., & Turban, E. (2011). What drives social commerce: the sychology and Behavior, 9, 584e590.
role of social support and relationship quality. International Journal of Electronic Wang, J. L., Jackson, L. A., & Zhang, D. J. (2011). The mediator role of self-disclosure
Commerce, 16(2), 69e90. and moderator roles of gender and social anxiety in the relationship between
Lin, C. P. (2011). Assessing the mediating role of online social capital between social Chinese adolescents' online communication and their real-world social re-
support and instant messaging usage. Electronic Commerce Research and Ap- lationships. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(6), 2161e2168.
plications, 10, 105e114. West, A., Lewis, J., & Currie, P. (2009). Students' Facebook “friends”: public and
Lin, K. Y., & Lu, H. P. (2011). Why people use social networking sites: an empirical private spheres. Journal of Youth Studies, 12(6), 615e627.
study integrating network externalities and motivation theory. Computers in Wheeless, L. R. (1976). Self-disclosure and interpersonal solidarity: measurement,
Human Behavior, 27, 1152e1161. validation, and relationships. Human Communication Research, 3, 47e61.
Madjar, N. (2008). Emotional and informational support from different sources and Wheeless, L. R., & Grotz, J. (1976). Conceptualization and measurement of reported
employee creativity. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, self-disclosure. Human Communication Research, 2(4), 338e346.
81(1), 83e100. Wills, T. A. (1985). Supportive functions of interpersonal relationships. In S. Cohen,
Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American & S. L. Syme (Eds.), Social support and health (pp. 61e82). New York: Academic
132 H.-Y. Huang / Computers in Human Behavior 57 (2016) 122e132

Press. Hsin-Yi Huang is an assistant professor in the Department of Computer Science and
Wills, T. A., & Shinar, O. (2000). Measuring perceived and received social support. In Information Management at the Soochow University, Taiwan. She holds a PhD in In-
S. Cohen, L. G. Underwood, & B. H. Gottlieb (Eds.), Social support measurement formation Management from the National Central University, Taiwan. Her research
and intervention: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 86e135). Oxford; interests include electronic commerce, online consumer behavior, and social network
New York: Oxford University Press. services. Her research has appeared in European Journal of Information Systems, In-
Xie, B. (2008). Multimodal computer-mediated communication and social support formation Systems Journal, International Journal of Information Management, and
among older Chinese internet users. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communi- Electronic Commerce Research and Applications.
cation, 13(3), 728e750.
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The multidimensional
scale of perceived social support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52(1), 30e41.

You might also like