You are on page 1of 11

This article was downloaded by: [Florida State University]

On: 28 December 2014, At: 21:09


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of the American College of Nutrition


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uacn20

Revisiting the Picky Eater Phenomenon: Neophobic


Behaviors of Young Children
a a
Betty Ruth Carruth PhD, RD & Jean D. Skinner PhD, RD
a
Nutrition Department, College of Human Ecology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville,
Tennessee
Published online: 14 Jun 2013.

To cite this article: Betty Ruth Carruth PhD, RD & Jean D. Skinner PhD, RD (2000) Revisiting the Picky Eater
Phenomenon: Neophobic Behaviors of Young Children, Journal of the American College of Nutrition, 19:6, 771-780, DOI:
10.1080/07315724.2000.10718077

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07315724.2000.10718077

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of
the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied
upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall
not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other
liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Original Research

Revisiting the Picky Eater Phenomenon: Neophobic


Behaviors of Young Children

Betty Ruth Carruth, PhD, RD, and Jean D. Skinner, PhD, RD


Nutrition Department, College of Human Ecology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee
Key words: neophobia, picky eater, problem eater, novel foods, unfamiliar foods

Objectives: To compare picky eater behaviors (food neophobia) of children as toddlers and at 42 to 84
months of age and to assess their mothers’ neophobic behaviors.
Methods: In a follow-up study of toddlers’ picky eater behaviors, trained interviewers conducted four
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

in-home interviews with mothers (n ⫽ 71) when their children were 42, 60, 72 and 84 months of age. Mothers
reported children’s diets (1 weekend day and 2 week days, 12 days total) and their behaviors; weight and height
were measured. An additional interview at 48 months involved only mothers’ behaviors. Nutritionist IV
software, correlations, t tests and repeated measures ANOVA were used to determine nutrient intake, behavioral
relationships and differences by picky eater status. Mothers’ descriptions of the children’s food neophobia and
bothersome behaviors were analyzed by qualitative methods.
Results: Mothers reported children’s neophobic behaviors at all ages. Mothers’ and children’s behaviors
were significantly and consistently correlated for number of attempts before deciding an unfamiliar food was
disliked (p ⬍ 0.01) and trying unfamiliar foods away from home (p ⬍ 0.01). Other behavioral associations were
significantly related for some ages but not for all the children’s ages. There were no significant differences by
picky eater status for nutrient intake or height and weight at any age. Mothers most frequently attempted
unfamiliar entrees away from home because of social setting/relationships. About 20% of mothers attempted
unfamiliar foods or new recipes just because they were different.
Conclusions: Findings suggest that some neophobic behaviors of children did not improve with maturity.
Mothers’ perceptions about their children’s picky eater status were inconsistent over time.

INTRODUCTION are defined as safe by their culture [11, 12]. Several investiga-
tors have studied infants, preschool and school-age children to
determine factors that influence a child’s acceptance of unfa-
The young child’s acceptance of unfamiliar foods is influ- miliar and different types of foods [13–19] and the overall
enced by parents and siblings in the home [1, 2], peers [3], the effect of picky eater behaviors on dietary variety [10].
social context of eating occasions [4] and the praise or negative Based on a longitudinal study of white, healthy children (2
response for consuming certain foods [4, 5]. For some types of to 84 months), the authors found that in the 24-to-36 month
foods, media and advertising strategies impact on how the child period, children who were perceived as picky eaters compared
views and selects certain foods [6 – 8]. Those children who to non-picky eaters had significantly less dietary variety [10].
accept only a limited number of foods and are unwilling to try Mothers reported that their toddlers exhibited repetitive behav-
unfamiliar foods may be labeled as neophobic, problem eaters iors, such as “eats only peanut butter and jelly” and “will not
or picky eaters [4, 9, 10]. eat anything green” (vegetables). Studies of preschool children
Children eating a variety of foods, rather than limiting the have shown that multiple exposures to unfamiliar foods en-
number consumed, is important because current guidelines for hanced their acceptance [14]. Some of the food-related behav-
dietary adequacy are more likely to be met when more types of iors found in toddlers may relate to developmental issues such
foods are consumed on a regular basis. Dietary variety also as independence [18]. However, as children mature they have
relates to willingness of children to taste unfamiliar foods that more opportunities for eating occasions apart from the family

Address reprint requests to: Betty Ruth Carruth, PhD, RD, Nutrition Department, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996-1900. E-mail: bcarruth@utk.edu

Journal of the American College of Nutrition, Vol. 19, No. 6, 771–780 (2000)
Published by the American College of Nutrition

771
Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

setting, and potentially they have more exposures to unfamiliar behaviors were observed, such as “How willing is your child to
foods which could lead to fewer dietary restrictions. In addi- try new and unfamiliar foods?” (1 ⫽ almost never, 7 ⫽ ex-
tion, the children’s dietary adequacy could be enhanced if they tremely willing, Table 1). Other behaviors of children involved
consumed a greater variety of foods. the variety and amounts of foods eaten, unfamiliar foods tried
This study revisited the picky eater phenomenon and other at home and when offered, the mothers’ use of persuasion/
food-related behaviors that were investigated in a group of reward and special foods, the degree that mothers were both-
toddlers, 24 to 36 months of age [10]. The research purposes ered by their children’s behaviors, and whether the children
were to contrast mothers’ perceptions about their children’s were considered problem eaters. These data were collected
picky eater behaviors (food neophobia) at 24 to 36 months with when the children were 42, 60, 72 and 84 months of age.
those perceptions held when their children were 42 to 84 In addition, mothers also reported the following informa-
months. We proposed that the limited food acceptance reported tion: the number of times children were exposed to unfamiliar
for the toddler age would continue in the 42-to-84 month foods at home and away from home (five time periods, weekly
period, although, as the children matured, they could have more to yearly), the number of times that a new food was attempted
exposures to novel and unfamiliar foods at home and away before mothers decided their child liked or disliked it, and if
from home. It was hypothesized that how mothers rated their foods served to the family reflected the children’s preferences
own food-related behaviors would be significantly related to or the mothers’ preferences.
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

how mothers rated their children’s. Also, it was postulated that At each of the interviews, mothers responded to open-ended
the children’s food neophobia, i.e., picky eater status, at each questions about whether they considered their children to be
age (42, 60, 72 and 84 months) would be significantly and picky eaters (yes/no) and described their children’s picky eater
negatively related to dietary adequacy based on current recom- behaviors. The yes answers at each interview were used to
mendations. classify the child as a picky eater. In a non-sequential section of
the same interviews, all mothers were asked to describe any-
thing about their children’s current behavior which bothered
METHODS them (apart from whether they considered their children to be
picky eaters). This process of asking mothers to verbally de-
Sample scribe the children’s behaviors as well as rate their behavior
provided a basis for the investigators to better interpret quan-
The study sample was composed of 71 mothers whose titative ratings (1–7 point scales) over time (42 to 84 months).
children were participating in an ongoing longitudinal study
from 2 to 96 months of age. The families were of middle and
Mothers’ Food Neophobia
upper socioeconomic status [19]. The major focus of the lon-
gitudinal study was to determine dietary intake and growth When the children were 48 months old, mothers rated their
patterns in white, healthy children [20]. The sample was pur- own food-related behaviors, using seven questions comparable
posively recruited to study children in a home environment to those questions asked about their children (aged 42, 60, 72
which was characterized by educated parents, adequate food and 84 months). Using an open-ended question format, mothers
availability and access to health care. also indicated if they had tried new recipes, the occasion and
time and reasons for doing so. Similarly, information was
collected about the time and occasion for mothers trying an
Interviews
unfamiliar food or unfamiliar foods, identity of the food or
As part of the longitudinal study, in-home interviews were foods and their reasons for trying a new food or new foods. The
conducted with mothers when the study children were 42, 48, open-ended questions were used to get further behavioral data
54, 60, 72 and 84 months of age. In this report on picky eaters about mothers and to relate these results to how mothers
revisited, data collected at 42, 60, 72 and 84 month interviews described/rated their children’s behaviors.
will be presented. Some of the behavioral questions specific to
the child were not asked at 48 and 54 month interviews. Children’s Diets
The children’s food intake, using 2-day food records and
Children’s Picky Eater Behaviors one 24-hour recall (two week days and one weekend day), were
In the initial study of children’s picky eater behaviors (24 to reported by the mothers at the 42, 60, 72 and 84 month
36 months), a questionnaire developed by Pelchat and Pliner interviews. Foods consumed away from home, such as at day
[9] and modified for the longitudinal study [10, 19] was used to care and in-home care, were obtained by mothers from the care
assess the mother’s perceptions about her child’s eating behav- givers, and amounts were reported as part of the three days of
iors. The same questionnaire was repeated in this study. It dietary intake at each of the four interviews. The same trained
contained 10 items that required mothers to rate their children interviewers (n ⫽ 2) who had collected data at 24 to 36 months
on a 1–7 point scale which indicated the degree that certain saw the same families from 42 to 60 months, and then one of

772 VOL. 19, NO. 6


Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

Table 1. Mothers’ Responses for Children Perceived as Picky Eaters and Non-Picky Eaters

34 months 42 months 60 months 72 months 84 months


Means ⫾ SD
Statement
P NP P NP P NP P NP P NP
(n ⫽ 28) (n ⫽ 42) (n ⫽ 34) (n ⫽ 37) (n ⫽ 35) (n ⫽ 36) (n ⫽ 25) (n ⫽ 46) (n ⫽ 21) (n ⫽ 50)
1. To what extent would you consider
your child to be a feeding problem?§ 2.9 ⫾ 1.6* 1.6 ⫾ 0.9 3.0 ⫾ 1.5* 1.9 ⫾ 1.1 —† —† 2.9 ⫾ 1.4* 1.9 ⫾ 1.1 3.2 ⫾ 1.6* 1.6 ⫾ 1.0
2. Overall, to what extent does your
child like a wide variety of foods
from those that you think he/she
should eat?§ 3.6 ⫾ 1.2* 5.5 ⫾ 1.0 3.4 ⫾ 1.2 5.6 ⫾ 1.0 4.0 ⫾ 1.5 5.2 ⫾ 1.3 3.4 ⫾ 1.2* 5.4 ⫾ 1.1 3.2 ⫾ 1.0* 5.4 ⫾ 1.0
3. Rank your child’s eating behavior as
a whole (extremely poor to
extremely good eater).£ 4.7 ⫾ 1.2* 5.8 ⫾ 0.9 4.6 ⫾ 1.0* 5.6 ⫾ 1.1 5.0 ⫾ 0.9 5.5 ⫾ 1.1 4.8 ⫾ 1.1* 5.8 ⫾ 0.6 4.9 ⫾ 1.0* 5.9 ⫾ 0.7
4. In general, at the end of the meal
how often has your child eaten the
amount you think he/she should
eat?¶ 4.0 ⫾ 1.4* 5.3 ⫾ 1.1 4.2 ⫾ 1.5 5.1 ⫾ 1.3 4.5 ⫾ 1.6* 5.5 ⫾ 0.9 4.5 ⫾ 1.4 5.3 ⫾ 1.2 4.9 ⫾ 1.2 5.6 ⫾ 1.1
5. How often do you attempt to
persuade your child to eat a food?¶ 4.5 ⫾ 1.8* 3.2 ⫾ 1.6 4.7 ⫾ 1.5* 3.4 ⫾ 1.5 4.9 ⫾ 1.5 4.0 ⫾ 1.7 4.7 ⫾ 1.3 3.8 ⫾ 1.8 4.4 ⫾ 1.5 3.4 ⫾ 1.4
6. How often do you provide a food
reward for eating a food you think

JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN COLLEGE OF NUTRITION


your child should eat?¶ 2.3 ⫾ 1.7 2.1 ⫾ 1.5 3.0 ⫾ 1.7 2.3 ⫾ 1.5 2.7 ⫾ 1.6 2.1 ⫾ 1.3 2.8 ⫾ 1.8 2.5 ⫾ 1.8 3.2 ⫾ 1.8* 2.0 ⫾ 1.3
7. How often do you prepare a special
food for your child because she/he
does not like what the rest of the
family is eating?¶ 3.1 ⫾ 1.9* 1.7 ⫾ 1.1 3.4 ⫾ 1.9* 1.9 ⫾ 1.0 —† —† 3.2 ⫾ 2.0* 1.5 ⫾ 0.8 3.5 ⫾ 2.1* 1.6 ⫾ 0.7
8. How often does your child try new
and unfamiliar foods at home?¶ 2.1 ⫾ 0.8 2.3 ⫾ 0.8 2.1 ⫾ 0.9 2.5 ⫾ 0.9 2.4 ⫾ 0.8 2.3 ⫾ 0.6 1.8 ⫾ 0.7* 2.7 ⫾ 0.9 2.5 ⫾ 0.7 2.6 ⫾ 0.9
9. How willing is your child to try new
and unfamiliar food when offered?** 3.1 ⫾ 1.6* 4.7 ⫾ 1.3 2.6 ⫾ 1.2* 4.7 ⫾ 1.3 3.1 ⫾ 1.7 4.5 ⫾ 1.6 2.8 ⫾ 1.5* 4.5 ⫾ 1.6 3.1 ⫾ 1.2* 4.8 ⫾ 1.4

Questions 1 and 7 were omitted at 60-month interviews because of incomplete data.
§
Items 1 to 2 rated on 7-point scale with 7 ⫽ to a great extent and 1 ⫽ not at all.
£
Item 3 was rated with 7 ⫽ extremely good and 1 ⫽ extremely poor eater.

Items 4 to 8 rated with 7 ⫽ always or almost always and 1 ⫽ never or almost never.
** Item 9 rated with 7 ⫽ extremely willing and 1 ⫽ almost never or never.
* Items that were significantly different (p ⱕ 0.005) by picky versus non-picky eater status at each time period.
Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

773
Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

the interviewers conducted the yearly interviews at 72 and 84 the 60-month interviews could not be used to generate factor
months. Beginning at two months, mothers were trained in scores because some of the questions were not asked at that
keeping accurate records, and the interviewers probed for pos- time period (Table 1).
sible omissions or errors in the food records/recall data reported For seven comparable questions, Pearson correlation coef-
by the mothers. ficients (p ⬍ 0.05) were used to determine significant relation-
ships between how mothers rated their own food-related be-
Anthropometry haviors at the 48 month interviews and the children’s behaviors
from 34 to 84 months. The content of these questions involved
The interviewer measured the child’s height (without shoes)
the child’s or the mother’s willingness to try an unfamiliar
with a steel tape (nearest 0.1 cm) using a wall or doorway in the
food, liking for a wide variety of foods, attempts (number of
home and a square (i.e., 2 boards attached at a right angle). The
tries) before deciding likes/dislikes for an unfamiliar food,
weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 pound, using a standard
overall eating behavior, frequency of eating unfamiliar foods
scale that was checked with standards of known weight (Model
away from home and likes/dislikes (of mother or child) were
707; SEA Columbia, MD).
reflected in foods served to the family.
Qualitative. For all interview periods (i.e., children’s ages),
Analyses of Children’s/Mothers’ Behaviors qualitative analyses [22] were used to develop major themes
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

Statistical. To contrast mothers’ ratings of their children’s based on mothers’ responses to open-ended questions about
behaviors (10 behavioral statements, 1–7 point rating scales), their children’s bothersome behaviors and mothers’ descrip-
group means [PROC MEANS, 21] were computed by picky tions about behaviors of picky eaters. The first author and a
eater status at 34, 42, 60, 72 and 84 months. In the toddler year, research assistant categorized mothers’ responses into themes,
the 10 statements were administered twice with no significant differences were discussed and a consensus reached. A rate of
difference in mean responses by time period. Thirty-four ⬎20 responses for each theme was arbitrarily established as a
months represented the average age of children at the second cut-off point for inclusion of data for further analyses and
interview (28, 32 or 36 months) and was used in the analyses. commentary. This rate represents ⬎28% of the 71 mothers
Significant mean differences for mothers’ ratings for 9 of 10 participating in the study. For the data related to mothers’
statements at each age and by picky eater versus non-picky trying new recipes and unfamiliar foods, frequency counts were
eater status were determined with t tests, and a significance made of the quantitative data, and qualitative analyses were
level of p ⫽ ⬍ 0.005 was used. Mothers’ responses for one performed to develop thematic reasons for mothers trying new
statement were incomplete at 60 months and were not used in recipes and unfamiliar foods.
rating the analyses. The higher probability level (p ⫽ ⬍ 0.005)
was set in order to address problems associated with doing Analyses of Dietary Data
multiple t tests and the potential for a Type 1 error.
Nutritionist IV software (Version 4.1, First Databank Divi-
To determine if trends in perceived picky eater behaviors
sion, The Hearst Corporation, San Bruno, CA) was used to
occurred over time for the group as a whole (n ⫽ 71 mothers),
calculate each child’s nutrient intake (2 days food record and
mean responses to the nine statements at 34 months were
one 24-hour recall) at 34, 42, 60, 72 and 84 months of age. For
contrasted with mean responses at other time periods (42, 60,
each age, mean nutrient intakes by group status (picky eater vs.
72 and 84 months) using repeated measures ANOVA. A prob-
non-picky eater) were calculated, and t tests were used to
ability level of p ⫽ 0.05 was used for significant mean differ-
determine significant group differences. Nutrient intakes by
ences by time effect.
picky eater status were compared to Recommended Dietary
Mothers’ responses at 34 months to the nine statements
Allowances [23] and Dietary Reference Intakes [24, 25, 26].
(Table 1) were factor analyzed using principal components
procedures. Three factors emerged with eigenvalues ⬎ 1.0 and Analyses of Anthropometry
factor loadings ⬎ 0.50. One statement loaded equally on two
factors and was dropped from analyses. With eight statements Means were calculated by picky eater status for children’s
loading on three factors (eigenvalues ⬎ 1.0 and factor load- heights and weights at 34, 42, 60, 72 and 84 months. Height
ings ⬎ 0.60), the factors were named food neophobia, persua- and weight differences were determined with 2-tailed t tests
sion and exposure to unfamiliar foods. For the group of chil- and p ⫽ 0.05 was established as a significance level.
dren perceived as picky eaters at 34 months (n ⫽ 29, 39%),
children’s individual factor scores were calculated for the three
factors and group means computed for those considered picky RESULTS
eaters versus non-picky eaters. For the same two groups, fac-
tors scores were also determined at 34, 42, 72 and 84 months of
Numerical Ratings of Children’s Behaviors
age, and 2-tailed t tests were used to determine significant Mothers’ mean ratings of children’s behaviors at 34, 42, 60,
group differences with a probability level of p ⫽ 0.05. Data for 72 and 84 months of age and by picky (P) and non-picky (NP)

774 VOL. 19, NO. 6


Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

Table 2. Mothers’ Mean Responses (n ⫽ 71) to Behavioral Statements that Showed a Significant Time Effect

34 months 42 months 60 months 72 months 84 months


Statement
Means ⫾ SD
1. Rank your child’s eating behavior as a
whole.£ 5.3 ⫾ 1.1 5.1 ⫾ 1.1 5.2 ⫾ 1.0 5.4 ⫾ .94 5.6 ⫾ .93*
2. In general, at the end of the meal how often
has your child eaten the amount you think
he/she should eat?¶ 4.7 ⫾ 1.3 4.7 ⫾ 1.5 5.0 ⫾ 1.4 5.0 ⫾ 1.3 5.4 ⫾ 1.2*
3. How often do you attempt to persuade your
child to eat a food?¶ 3.7 ⫾ 1.8 4.1 ⫾ 1.6 4.5 ⫾ 1.7* 4.1 ⫾ 1.7 3.7 ⫾ 1.5
4. How often do you provide a food reward
for eating a food you think your child
should eat?¶ 2.2 ⫾ 1.5 2.6 ⫾ 1.6* 2.4 ⫾ 1.5 2.6 ⫾ 1.8 2.4 ⫾ 1.6
5. How often does your child try new and
unfamiliar foods at home?¶ 2.3 ⫾ .76 2.3 ⫾ .92 2.3 ⫾ .69 2.4 ⫾ .92 2.6 ⫾ .85*
6. How willing is your child to try new and
unfamiliar food when offered?* 4.1 ⫾ 1.7 3.7 ⫾ 1.6* 3.8 ⫾ 1.8 3.9 ⫾ 1.8 4.3 ⫾ 1.5
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

£
Item 1 was rated with 7 ⫽ extremely good and 1 ⫽ extremely poor eater.

Items 2 to 5 rated with 7 ⫽ always or almost always and 1 ⫽ never or almost never.

Item 6 rated with 7 ⫽ extremely willing and 1 ⫽ almost never or never.
* Significant differences by time (p ⱕ 0.05) effect in mothers’ responses.

eater status are shown in Table 1. The number of mothers who children were consistently reported as picky eaters from 34 to
perceived their children as picky eaters fluctuated over time. 84 months of age, the sample size was not adequate to do
Twenty-three (32%) children were never perceived as picky analyses by individual data over time.
eaters, whereas 33 (46%) children were considered to be picky
eaters ⬎3 times during the study period. Seven children (10%) Factor Analyses of Mothers’ Responses by
were considered picky eaters throughout the study. Of 12 Children’s Picky Eater Status
(17%) mothers who considered themselves as picky eaters,
Three factors were generated from the mothers’ responses
only 3 (4%) of their children were considered picky eaters at
to eight statements (Table 1): food neophobia, persuasion and
each age.
exposure. A higher factor score was associated with picky eater
In the toddler year (34 months), mean ratings for eight of the
status. For the food neophobia and persuasion factors, scores
nine behavioral items shown in Table 1 were significantly
were significantly different and higher for picky eaters at 34
different by picky versus non-picky eater status (p ⬍ 0.005). In
months (p ⬍ 0.0001, p ⫽ 0.0298) and at 42 months (p ⬍
regard to a mother who considered her child to be a picky eater,
0.0001, p ⫽ 0.0178), respectively. At 72 months picky eaters
this status was significantly and consistently associated with
had significantly different food neophobia factors scores, and
whether she considered her child to have a feeding problem and
there was a trend for the persuasion factor (p ⫽ 0.0558). Only
whether she prepared special foods for the child. Over time
the food neophobia factor was different at 84 months (p ⬍
most of the mean ratings were significantly different by picky
0.0001). The data suggest that mothers’ showed consistency in
eater status in regard to the child’s overall eating behavior,
perceiving their children’s food neophobic behaviors over time.
willingness to try unfamiliar foods and eating a variety of
foods. Mean responses to other questions for each age were not
consistently and significantly associated with picky eater status
Correlations between Mothers’ and Children’s
(items 4, 5, 6, 8).
Behaviors
Our findings indicate that six of nine behavioral questions For comparable questions where mothers numerically rated
showed a significant time effect (p ⱕ .05) by contrasting mean their children’s (ages 34, 42, 60, 72 and 84 months) and their
ratings for the study group (n ⫽ 71) at 34 months with means own behaviors (48 months), five of seven items were signifi-
at the other ages and without the children’s picky eater status in cantly correlated. Mothers and children’s scores for the number
the analyses (Table 2). At 84 versus 34 months, the children of times an unfamiliar food was attempted before deciding it
were rated as better eaters, as trying more unfamiliar foods at was disliked were significantly correlated across all children’s
home and as eating the amounts of foods that their mothers ages, (p ⬍ 0.0001 at 34 months, p ⬍ 0.0001 at 42, p ⫽ 0.0002
thought they should consume. At 60 months versus 34 months, at 60, p ⫽ 0.0032 at 72 and p ⫽ 0.0031 at 84 months). An
mothers were using persuasion more often, and at 42 months average of fewer than three attempts was consistently reported
children were less willing to try unfamiliar foods, and mothers for both groups. For the number of times an unfamiliar food
were providing rewards more often. Because only 12 of the 71 was attempted before deciding to like it, the correlations were

JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN COLLEGE OF NUTRITION 775


Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

Table 3. Food-Related Behaviors of Children* Perceived as “Picky Eaters” by Their Mothers

Thematic Behaviors Example of Mothers’† Responses


} Unwilling to try new food (neophobia) and Not willing to try any new foods.
refusal to eat unfamiliar foods when offered Very limited variety of foods that (child) will try.
If he sets his mind out to not eat a new item, he will not.
Mommy, I don’t like it-she hasn’t even tried it!
} Limited number of acceptable foods/lack of Really selective on what he’ll eat without an argument.
dietary variety No variety, eats the same boring foods.
(Mother) cooks to cater because (he) only eats certain foods.
Eats maybe 10 things; all others she finds repulsive.
} Refusal to eat certain foods including Won’t eat anything green.
groups and types of foods within a group Eats no vegetables and very few fruits.
Won’t eat any meat except turkey.
Refuses to eat certain food groups-no veggies, no milk.
* Mothers interviewed when children (n ⫽ 71) were 42, 60, 72 and 84 months old.

Qualitative analyses of mothers’ responses used to develop thematic behaviors of “picky eaters.”
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

significant at 34 months (p ⫽ 0.0016), 60 months (p ⫽ 0.0004) n ⫽ 38 at 84 months, respectively). All study mothers had an
and 72 months (p ⫽ 0.0112). Significant correlations were opportunity to answer the open-ended picky eater and bother-
found for the extent that child or mother liked a wide variety of some behavior questions at the same interview (42, 60, 72 and
foods at 60 months (p ⫽ 0.0284) and that foods served to the 84 months), but in non-sequential parts of the interview. As
family reflected (mother or child) likes/dislikes at 72 months shown in Table 4, five bothersome behavior themes emerged
(p ⫽ 0.0203). Ratings of mother and child for the behavior of from qualitative analyses of mothers’ responses, and several
trying new and unfamiliar foods away from home were signif- bothersome behaviors were congruent with picky eater behav-
icantly correlated at 60 (p ⫽ 0.0016), 72 (p ⫽ 0.006) and 84 iors identified by mothers (42 to 84 months) shown in Table 3.
months (p ⬍ 0.0001). The most consistent and significant That is, whether or not mothers perceived their children as
correlations between mothers’ and children’s behaviors were picky eaters, a majority of mothers were bothered by the
the number of attempts before deciding an unfamiliar food was children’s lack of dietary variety, unwillingness to try new
disliked and trying new and unfamiliar foods away from home. foods and/or refusing to eat unfamiliar foods when offered, and
No correlations at any time period were significant for how limiting or rejecting certain groups of foods, e.g., vegetables
mothers rated their own eating behaviors as a whole and the and fruits particularly.
behavior of their children or whether a mother considered About one-third of the mothers were bothered about the
herself a picky eater vs. perceiving the child as a picky eater. quantity of foods consumed by their children. Both insufficient
and excessive food intakes were considered bothersome, and in
Qualitative Themes about Mothers’ and Children’s part these patterns involved children’s food preferences and
Behaviors inconsistencies in their eating patterns, e.g., “he likes cereal,
Children’s Food Neophobia. Three major themes emerged eats only cereal one day and not the next.” As the children
from analyses of mothers’ responses when describing behav- matured, they indicated their food preferences by using both
iors of a picky eater (Table 3). Their children were unwilling to verbal and physical behavioral tactics. For example, when the
try new foods and refused to eat unfamiliar foods when offered. children refused to eat and argued about foods that were pre-
These behaviors were described as food neophobia by Pelchat pared for the rest of the family, mothers thought these actions
and Pliner [11]. Picky eater behaviors included the children influenced meal preparations and the overall milieu of the
limiting the number of acceptable foods and the lack of dietary eating occasion.
variety. Some mothers catered to their children’s list of accept- Over time, mothers reported bothersome behaviors, such as
able foods (Table 3). Picky eaters rejected specific food groups messiness and manners, that occurred around the eating occa-
or types of foods within a food group, e.g., vegetables, some sion. Children played with rather than ate foods offered, or, in
fruits and some types of meat products. Although the number of some cases, the children carefully dissected the foods, ate some
mothers’ responses did not meet criteria (⬎20 responses/ portion and smeared uneaten portions on the table or fed the
theme) for some behaviors, there were trends for other themes family pet. From 42 to 84 months, mothers increasingly com-
such as children’s not finishing food portions, the appearance mented that children should be using forks and spoons instead
of food greatly influencing its acceptability and the develop- of their hands, should eat the food offered without being so
ment by some of intractable mind sets toward specific foods. messy and should exhibit behaviors that were less offensive/
Children’s Bothersome Behaviors. The number of moth- obnoxious (e.g., loud burping, placing feet on table).
ers who reported bothersome behaviors of their children de- Mothers’ Food Neophobia. The identity of new foods
creased over time (n ⫽ 51 at 42, n ⫽ 47 at 60, n ⫽ 43 at 72, attempted, the occasion and mothers’ reasons were categorized

776 VOL. 19, NO. 6


Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

Table 4. Bothersome Food-Related Behaviors of Children* Reported by Their Mothers

Bothersome Behaviors†‡ Examples of Mothers’ Responses


} Picky/lack of dietary variety Doesn’t drink enough milk.
Will not eat steak or roast, only chicken.
No veggies/no fruit.
} Limiting/refusing certain types of foods He’s become Mr. Picky.
Very hesitant to try new things, especially fruits and vegetables.
She doesn’t like variety.
} Non-food related behaviors associated I wish he would not want to play, get off his seat, and talk so much at mealtime.
with eating occasions Eats it in layers (sandwich).
Picks off chocolate part of cake and lays it all over the table.
} Quantity eaten Doesn’t eat enough or drink enough.
Has a tendency to overeat-will keep eating and eating.
Eats minuscule bites, not enough quantity, not a good eater.
} Manners/messiness Uses her hands for things she should use a spoon or fork.
Loud burping and gagging. Messy.
Puts her feet on the table when she eats.
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

* Mothers interviewed when children (n ⫽ 71) were 42, 60, 72 and 84 months of age.

Behaviors based on qualitative analyses of mothers’ responses when asked “Is there anything about your child’s current eating behaviors that bothers you?”

The number of mothers bothered by children’s behaviors: n ⫽ 51 at 42 months, n ⫽ 47 at 60, n ⫽ 43 at 72, and n ⫽ 38 at 84 months.

and are presented as summarized findings. Unfamiliar foods energy allowances/dietary reference intakes. As shown in Table
cited most frequently by mothers included entrees, ethnic foods 5, energy intakes for picky eaters approximated recommenda-
and vegetables/fruits or combinations of salad ingredients, tions. Protein, magnesium, calcium and vitamins A, C, B6 and
whereas grain products, desserts and beverages were cited less B12 exceeded 100%, whereas zinc, vitamin D and vitamin E
frequently. Reasons that mothers attempted unfamiliar foods were below 100% of the recommendations at 34, 42, 60, 72 and
involved social settings and/or a relationship (e.g., husband, 84 months. Folate intake of children considered picky eaters
office group, birthday), appearance/perceived goodness and fluctuated over time but exceeded recommendations for three
taste of foods or that the tried food was different. Some unfa- of the five time periods. The lower longitudinal dietary intakes
miliar sea foods and ethnic-type dishes were reported as both of zinc, vitamins D and E of the study group have been reported
entrees and appetizers, e.g., sushi, smoked salmon, chicken previously [19]. As percent of energy over time, fat ranged
nachos. The mean time since they had tried an unfamiliar food from 31 to 32 percent and protein 13 to 15 percent. The
was 3.25 ⫾ 5.62 months, with one mother reporting no at- consumption of fortified grain products increased dietary fo-
tempts in the previous three years. late, iron, vitamin B6 and B12. Except for zinc, vitamins D and
New recipes tried by mothers included entrees, fruits/veg- E, picky eater status was associated with meeting the age-
etables (as salad ingredients and individually) and grain prod- appropriate dietary recommendations. Mean nutrient intakes,
ucts. The major reason given by mothers involved convenience, using p ⫽ 0.001, by picky eater versus non-picky eater status
such as having ingredients on-hand, seeing recipes that looked were not significantly different at any time period.
easy to fix or having chicken in the freezer or zucchini in the
garden. Mothers relied on magazines and cookbooks that pic- Height/Weight and Picky Eater Status
torially displayed foods that looked good and mothers were
Results of t tests for comparing mean height and weight at
familiar with the ingredients of. As with trying unfamiliar foods
34, 42, 60, 72 and 84 months showed no significant difference
(primarily away from home), trying new recipes involved re-
by picky eater status. Mean heights and weights were within
lationships where a relative (e.g., aunt, mother-in-law), child or
normal values for age and genders [27]. Mothers’ perception
the husband was associated with the occasion. About 20% of
about their child’s picky eater status was not indicative of mean
mothers tried a new recipe because it was just different than
height and weight changes over time.
foods normally prepared. However, none of the unfamiliar
foods or recipes cited by mothers represented exotic or rare
foods. These results may be greatly influenced by the neopho-
bic behaviors of some children and the children’s age when DISCUSSION
data were collected.
Our hypothesis that the children’s limited food acceptance
as toddlers would continue in the 42-to-84 month age period
Picky Eaters’ Diets was supported by descriptions of children’s food neophobia
The dietary intake of children designated as picky eaters at and by some of the bothersome behaviors reported by mothers
34 to 84 months were compared to age-appropriate nutrient and who did not consider their children picky eaters. Comparisons

JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN COLLEGE OF NUTRITION 777


Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

Table 5. Children’s Nutrient Intake and Percent of Recommendations for Children Perceived as Picky Eaters

Mean Nutrient Intake ⫾ SD (% of Recommendation)


Nutrient RDA⑀/DRI† 34 months‡ 42 months§ 60 months§ 72 months§ 84 months§
(n ⫽ 28) (n ⫽ 34) (n ⫽ 35) (n ⫽ 25) (n ⫽ 21)
Energy (Kcal) 1300/1800 1468 ⫾ 318 (113) 1380 ⫾ 261 (106) 1716 ⫾ 426 (95) 1762 ⫾ 388 (98) 1812 ⫾ 338 (101)
Protein (g) 16/24 49 ⫾ 14 (306) 46 ⫾ 14 (192) 56 ⫾ 18 (233) 60 ⫾ 17 (250) 58 ⫾ 18 (242)
Fat (g) 0/0£ 53 ⫾ 15 (–) 45 ⫾ 13 (–) 61 ⫾ 21 (–) 62 ⫾ 15 (–) 64 ⫾ 16 (–)
Calcium (mg) 500/800 763 ⫾ 343 (153) 714 ⫾ 242 (143) 911 ⫾ 344 (114) 878 ⫾ 308 (110) 888 ⫾ 384 (111)
Iron (mg) 10 9 ⫾ 4 (90) 9 ⫾ 3 (90) 10 ⫾ 3 (100) 13 ⫾ 4 (130) 12 ⫾ 5 (120)
Magnes. (mg) 80/130 157 ⫾ 49 (196) 158 ⫾ 47 (197) 192 ⫾ 66 (148) 202 ⫾ 59 (155) 196 ⫾ 63 (151)
Zinc (mg) 10 6 ⫾ 3 (60) 7 ⫾ 3 (70) 8 ⫾ 3 (80) 9 ⫾ 2 (90) 8 ⫾ 3 (80)
Vit A (RE) 400/500 754 ⫾ 528 (188) 505 ⫾ 217* (101) 751 ⫾ 372 (150) 766 ⫾ 459 (153) 718 ⫾ 442 (144)
Vit D (mg) 5 4.0 ⫾ 2.6 (40) 3.9 ⫾ 2.2 (78) 4.3 ⫾ 2.3 (86) 4.5 ⫾ 2.3 (90) 4.8 ⫾ 2.6 (96)
Vit E (mg) 6/7 4.0 ⫾ 2.8 (66) 2.8 ⫾ 2.0 (40) 2.8 ⫾ 2.2 (40) 4.2 ⫾ 2.9 (60) 4.7 ⫾ 3.9 (67)
Vit C (mg) 15/25 88 ⫾ 74 (220) 67 ⫾ 49 (148) 68 ⫾ 38 (151) 75 ⫾ 39 (333) 88 ⫾ 54 (352)
Folate (␮g) 150/200 129 ⫾ 63 (86) 153 ⫾ 60 (102) 172 ⫾ 52 (86) 200 ⫾ 83 (100) 202 ⫾ 131 (102)
Vit B6 (mg) 0.5/0.6 1.1 ⫾ 0.4 (220) 1.1 ⫾ 0.4 (220) 1.3 ⫾ 0.4 (216) 1.5 ⫾ 0.6 (250) 1.5 ⫾ 0.6 (250)
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

Vit B12 (mg) 0.9/1.2 2.8 ⫾ 1.4 (400) 2.9 ⫾ 1.4 (414) 3.7 ⫾ 1.4 (370) 4.0 ⫾ 1.7 (333) 3.7 ⫾ 1.8 (308)
⑀,†
Recommended Dietary Allowances (1989, Abridged) ages 1 to 3 and 4 to 6 years; Dietary Reference Intake (1997–2000) for children 1 to 3 and 4 to 8 years. For ages
24 to 42 months, values for 1 to 3 years used; ⬎42 months, values for 4 to 8 years.

Mean intake (⫾ SD) based on average of two 24 hour recalls and four days of diet records for children 24 to 36 months old; 34 months represents average age of children
at second interview (28, 32 or 36 months).
§
Mean intake (⫾ SD) based on one 24 hour recall and two days of diet records for children at each age of 42, 60, 72 and 84 months.
£
No recommendation for fat.

of picky eaters’ factor scores at 34 months and over time also significantly related [12]. In 57 Swedish families, higher food
indicate a continuance of some children’s food neophobic neophobia score of mothers and their children were associated
behavior over time. with the mother serving fewer foods in the last six months from
As the children matured, bothersome behaviors increasingly a list of unfamiliar foods [13].
related to non-food events, such as manners. Sanders et al. In our study, the significant correlation between mothers
reported that children with feeding problems exhibited higher and their children for the number of exposures to an unfamiliar
levels of disruptive behaviors at mealtime, e.g., food refusal, food before deciding that the food was disliked may involve an
playing with food and oppositional behavior [4]. As reported expected sensory experience [28, 29], social expectations [15]
previously [10], picky eaters in our study rejected entire groups or the modeling of mothers or other adults in the child’s
of foods, particularly vegetables, and foods within groups, such environment [30]. Mothers and children in the current study
as no red muscle meats. However, there are some positive had an average of fewer than three exposures before deciding
outcomes for the total study group at 84 months; mothers’ they disliked an unfamiliar food. In the Birch et al. study of
mean ratings for their children’s eating behaviors overall, will- 3-to-5-year old children, 10 or 15 exposures were associated
ingness to try unfamiliar foods and amounts of food consumed with increased acceptance of a new food [14]. Although chil-
were more favorable than at 34 months. dren in our study may have avoided unfamiliar foods because
Non-food related behaviors in the toddler year may reflect they feared a negative sensory food experience, food neophobia
aspects of autonomy development [18]; however, data from the of younger children compared to older children has been re-
later ages (60 to 84 months) showed that the children were ported by other investigators [1, 11, 13]. Our results clearly
approaching eating occasions quite differently. Food prefer- show that the children were not given numerous and consistent
ences were strongly expressed (using verbal and physical tac- exposures to unfamiliar foods over time. As reported previ-
tics), and some mothers perceived these actions as picky eater ously, food preferences of the study children as toddlers and
behaviors of the children and other mothers reported them as concordance with preferences of family members were as-
bothersome behaviors. sessed [31]. The findings suggested that the most limiting
Several investigators have published methodologies which category related to foods consumed by the toddlers was that
provide operational definitions of neophobic behavior [12, 13]. foods were never offered to them.
In studies about neophobic traits of families and children, Twelve study mothers rated themselves as picky eaters and
behavioral measures, such as sampling novel (unfamiliar) foods had a limited number of acceptable foods. Even among mothers
and mothers’ assessment of their children’s willingness to taste who did not consider themselves picky eaters, there were also
unfamiliar foods, have been assessed [12, 13]. In 5-to-11-year- trends of attempting only certain types of new foods at home
old children, trait neophobic scores (pencil and paper assess- and away from home. Unfamiliar foods attempted primarily
ments) and behavioral scores of parents and their children were away from home were associated with social settings in which

778 VOL. 19, NO. 6


Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

conformity may have been expected or the mothers attempted children. In addition, the children’s food neophobia may reflect
new foods to avoid social rejection. Also unfamiliar foods the fact that over half of the children attended some type of day
recommended by a significant other (relatives, friends) within care where institutional regulations would dictate the variety of
a social setting context implied that the unfamiliar foods were foods offered at meals and snack times. It is likely that chil-
culturally defined as acceptable [15]. Sixty percent of mothers dren’s positive sensory experiences at home were repeated in
in our study tried unfamiliar foods of animal origin compared eating occasions away from home, rather than the children’s
to 40% who tried foods of vegetable/plant origin. This finding having an increased exposure and opportunity to taste an un-
is inconsistent with literature showing that unfamiliar foods of familiar food.
animal origin may be less acceptable than those of vegetable/ Change strategies for children perceived as picky eaters
plant origin [16]. should include at a minimum, pencil and paper assessment of
The mothers’ modeling of food neophobia may have neg- the parents’ neophobia traits. It is unlikely that children will be
atively influenced the study children’s behaviors. Harper and accepting of unfamiliar foods if the parents exhibit neophobia
Sander [32] studied mothers or other adults who offered a food traits. Educational strategies with young children must include
to young children without tasting or first tasted the food and multiple opportunities to taste new foods within social contexts
then offered it to the children. The children’s willingness to that reinforce that food’s acceptability [11, 13, 14, 30]. Mothers
taste the offered food was positively influenced by the adults in our study received child feeding information from multiple
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

tasting the food before offering it to the children. and concurrent sources throughout the 2-to-84-month study
Growth patterns of the study children irrespective of their [33]. Thus there are many educational venues (professionals,
picky eater status over time demonstrated they were receiving relatives, friends and media sources) for reaching parents
adequate diets to achieve normal growth for age and gender whose children exhibit food neophobia.
[27]. These findings also are compatible with the fluctuations
that mothers perceived their children to be picky eaters at one
age and not another age.
In addressing limitations of the study, longitudinal data have REFERENCES
an inherent strength that measurements of the same children are
1. Koivisto UK, Sjödén PO: Food and general neophobia in Swedish
made over time, and their environment and development can be
families: Parent-child comparisons and relationships with serving
monitored to better explain research findings. However, the
specific foods. Appetite 26:107–118, 1996.
participants may not be representative of the general popula- 2. Pliner P, Pelchat ML: Similarities in food preferences between
tion. In our study, the children’s ages (34 to 84 months) children and their siblings and parents. Appetite 7:333–342, 1986.
required that mothers’ report their perceptions about the chil- 3. Birch LL: Effects of peer models’ food choices and eating behav-
dren’s food neophobia and dietary intake. In studies with older iors on preschoolers’ food preferences. Child Develop 51:489–496,
children, they can respond to behavioral statements similar to 1980.
those used in our study, taste foods and/or rank foods [1, 13, 4. Sanders MR, Patel RK, Le Grice B, Shepherd RW: Children with
28]. With preschool children, tasting activities could be used to persistent feeding difficulties. An observational analysis of the
validate mothers’ perceptions about their children’s food neo- feeding interactions of problem and non-problem eaters. Health
phobia. This may be considered a limitation of our study, but Psychol 12:64–73, 1993.
5. Birch LL, Johnston SL, Fisher JA: Children’s eating: The devel-
the advantage of longitudinal data enabled the investigators to
opment of food-acceptance patterns. Young Children 50:71–78,
study adult modeling and family environment factors that in-
1995.
fluence young children’s food neophobia. 6. Kotz K, Story M: Food advertisements during children’s Saturday
morning television programming: Are they consistent with dietary
recommendations? J Am Diet Assoc 94:1296–1300, 1994.
CONCLUSION 7. Kuntz LA: Designing products for kids: More than child’s play.
Food Prod Jan:35–50, 1996.
Given that all study mothers described neophobic behaviors 8. Mizerski R: The relationship between cartoon trade character rec-
of their children at various ages, what can be done to modify ognition and attitude toward product category in young children. J
these behaviors and increase the number of acceptable foods? Marketing 59:58–70, 1995.
Would our findings be different over time if, beginning with the 9. Pelchat ML, Pliner P: Antecedents and correlates of feeding prob-
lems in young children. J Nutr Educ 18:23–29, 1986.
toddler age, mothers had increased the number of new food
10. Carruth BR, Skinner J, Houck K, Moran III J, Coletta F, Ott D: The
exposures (⬎ 10 exposures vs. ⱕ 6/year) at home and away
phenomenon of “picky eater”: A behavioral marker in eating
from home and had provided opportunities for tasting unfamil- patterns of toddlers. J Am Coll Nutr 17:180–186, 1998.
iar foods combined with positive adult role modeling? For 11. Pelchat ML, Pliner P: “Try it, You’ll like it.” Effects of informa-
example, only 20% of the mothers tried a new recipe just tion on willingness to try novel foods. Appetite 24:153–166, 1995.
because it was different. The mothers’ neophobic modeling 12. Pliner P: Development of measures of food neophobia in children.
may be influencing the minimal changes reported for the study Appetite 23:147–163, 1994.

JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN COLLEGE OF NUTRITION 779


Neophobic Behaviors of Preschool Children

13. Koivisto UK, Sjödén PO: Reasons for rejection of food items in 25. Food and Nutrition Board: “Dietary Reference Intakes for Thia-
Swedish families with children aged 2–17. Appetite 26:89–103, min, Riboflavin, Niacin, Vitamin B6, Folate, Vitamin B12, Panto-
1996. thenic Acid, Biotin, and Choline.” Washington, DC: National
14. Birch LL, McPhee L, Shoba BC, Pirok E, Steinberg L: What kind Academy Press, 1998.
of exposure reduces children’s food neophobia? Appetite 9:171– 26. Food and Nutrition Board: “Dietary Reference Intakes for Vitamin
178, 1987. E, Vitamin C, Selenium, and Carotenoids.” Washington, DC:
15. Birch LL, Gunder L, Grimm-Thomas K, Laing DG: Infants’ con- National Academy of Press, 2000.
sumption of a new food enhances acceptance of similar foods. 27. Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM,
Appetite 30:283–295, 1998. Guo SS, Wei R, Mei Z, Curtin LR, Roche AF, Johnson CL: “CDC
16. Pliner P, Pelchat ML: Neophobia in humans and the special status
Growth Charts: United States. Advance Data from Vital and
of foods of animal origin. Appetite 16:205–218, 1991.
Health Statistics,” No. 314. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for
17. Koivisto UK, Sjödén PO: Food and general neophobia and their
Health Statistics, 2000.
relationship with self-reported food choice: Familial resemblance
28. Raudenbush B, Frank RA: Assessing food neophobia. The role of
in Swedish families with children of ages 7–17 years. Appetite
stimulus familiarity. Appetite 32:261–271, 1999.
29:89–103, 1997.
29. Raudenbush B, Schroth F, Frank RA: Food neophobia, odor eval-
18. Kuczynski L, Kochanska G: Development of children’s noncom-
pliance strategies from toddlerhood to age 5. Dev Psychol 26:398– uation and exploratory sniffing behaviors. Appetite 31:171–183,
Downloaded by [Florida State University] at 21:09 28 December 2014

408, 1990. 1998.


19. Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Houck KS, Bounds W, Morris M, Cox 30. Hobden K, Pliner P: Effects of a model on food neophobia in
DR, Moran III J, Coletta F: Longitudinal study of nutrient and food humans. Appetite 25:101–114, 1995.
intakes of white preschool children aged 24 to 60 months. J Am 31. Skinner JS, Carruth BR, Moran III J, Houck K, Schmidhammer J,
Diet Assoc 99:1514–1521, 1999. Reed A, Coletta F, Cotter R, Ott D: Toddlers’ food preferences:
20. Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Houck KS, Coletta F, Cotter R, Ott D, Concordance with family members’ preferences. J Nutr Educ
McLeod M: Longitudinal study of nutrient and food intakes of 30:17–22, 1998.
infants aged 2 to 24 months. J Am Diet Assoc 97:496–504, 1997. 32. Harper LV, Sanders KM: The effect of adult’s eating on young
21. SAS Institute Inc: “SAS User’s Guide: Basics.” (Version 6.11) children’s acceptance of novel foods. J Exper Child Psychol 20:
Gary, NC: SAS Institute 1988. 206–214, 1975.
22. Taylor SJ, Brodgan R: “Introduction to Qualitative Research Meth- 33. Carruth BR, Skinner J: Mothers’ sources of information about
ods,” 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley and Sons, pp 123–145, 1984. feeding their children, aged 2–54 months. J Nutr Educ (In press).
23. Food and Nutrition Board: “Recommended Dietary Allowances,”
10th ed. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1989.
24. Food and Nutrition Board: “Dietary Reference Intakes for Cal-
cium, Phosphorus, Magnesium, Vitamin D and Fluoride.” Wash- Received June 14, 2000; revision accepted September 22, 2000.
ington, DC: National Academy Press, 1997.

780 VOL. 19, NO. 6

You might also like