You are on page 1of 8

Original Paper

Psychopathology 2002;35:272–279
DOI: 10.1159/000067065

Command Hallucinations:
Who Obeys and Who Resists When?
R. Erkwoh a K. Willmes b A. Eming-Erdmann a H.J. Kunert a
a Clinic
of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy and b Clinic of Neurology, Section Neuropsychology,
University of Aachen, Aachen, Germany

Key Words however, appear to be translated into behaviour in a more


Schizophrenia W Command hallucinations W Behaviour W indirect way. The gap between the symptom and action
Prediction should be bridged by a complex including speech, com-
prehension and readiness to act. With respect to clinical
conditions, some of the hallucinated commands are fol-
Abstract lowed by consecutive behaviour and others are not. There
The impact of auditory command hallucinations on the is considerable need for the clinician to predict whether or
behaviour of schizophrenic patients sometimes appears not hallucinations of the patients have consequences.
to be unpredictable. In order to tackle this problem, the In this study, we attempted to answer the following
psychopathological characteristics of command halluci- question: which psychopathological characteristics are of
nations in 31 schizophrenic patients were assessed using value in predicting whether a patient will obey or resist
a 24-item questionnaire. Using binary data and relative command hallucinations?
risk analysis methods, predictors were determined for Data of predictive value available from forensic stud-
obeying or resisting command hallucinations. Character- ies [1–3] and from phenomenological surveys [3–6] agree
istics of voices and the attitude toward the voices appear that several factors together may be important, but the
equally important for prediction. A set of three psycho- main emphasis is laid on characteristics of the ‘voices’.
pathological characteristics comprising a voice known to Our hypothesis, however, is that the patients’ attitude
the patient, emotional involvement during the hallucina- toward these experiences plays an additional if not deci-
tions, and seeing the voice as real provides significant sive role.
predictivity of behaviour following command hallucina-
tions. These results are interesting for clinical and foren-
sic psychiatrists. Patients and Methods
Copyright © 2002 S. Karger AG, Basel
All patients admitted consecutively to the Clinic of Psychiatry
and Psychotherapy in Aachen, Germany, in 1997 and 1998 were
included in the study if they met the criterion of hearing command
Introduction hallucinations. An a priori diagnosis of schizophrenia was not
required. Exclusion criteria were severe thought disorders, difficulty
in concentrating and uncooperative behaviour. A total of 31 patients
Some symptoms of schizophrenia are viewed as behav- was included in the study, either due to a beginning psychotic disor-
ioural abnormalities, such as catatonic symptoms or posi- der with command hallucinations or due to a relapse. All patients
tive formal thought disorders. Command hallucinations, gave informed written consent.

© 2002 S. Karger AG, Basel Prof. Ralf Erkwoh, MD, Senior Physician
ABC 0254–4962/02/0355–0272$18.50/0 Clinic of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy
131.211.208.19 - 11/19/2016 3:49:51 PM

Fax + 41 61 306 12 34 University of Technology, RWTH Aachen


E-Mail karger@karger.ch Accessible online at: Pauwelsstrasse 30, D–52074 Aachen (Germany)
www.karger.com www.karger.com/psp Tel. +49 241 8089653, Fax +49 241 8888401, E-Mail rerkwoh@ukaachen.de
University Library Utrecht
Downloaded by:
Ten patients were female, 21 male; female patients had a mean Table 1. Two-component solution for psychopathological symptoms
age of 44.4 years (median 48.5, range 23–57 years), males 36.1 years according to PANSS (varimax-rotated loading matrix)
(median 35.0, range 17–58 years). Sixteen patients were married, 14
lived alone and 1 was widowed. Twenty-five had finished middle Item Factor 1 Factor 2
school, 6 high school. Average duration of illness was 63 months (me-
dian 37, range 7–398 months). According to DSM-III-R [7] criteria, Delusions 0.617 0.581
27 patients had a schizophrenic disorder of the paranoid type Hallucinations 0.028 0.726
(295.3), 3 a schizoaffective disorder (295.7) and 1 an organically Hostility 0.672 0.037
caused hallucinosis (293.82). All patients received neuroleptic thera- Blunted affect –0.865 –0.030
py throughout the study. Emotional withdrawal –0.838 –0.062
Psychopathological symptoms were assessed using PANSS [8] Motor retardation –0.645 0.353
and selected PANSS symptoms were then submitted to principal Formal thought disorders 0.723 –0.448
component analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation and Kaiser nor- Mannerisms 0.472 –0.602
malisation (SPSS, PC version 9.0). An eigenvalue of 1 1 was chosen
as the criterion for the number of principal components to be Explained variance, % 44.1 18.4
extracted. Symptoms for the principal component analyses were cho-
sen according to DSM-III-R criteria: 3 positive symptoms (delusions, Figures in italics are correlating with the corresponding factor.
hallucinations, hostility) corresponding to the psychotic dimension,
3 symptoms corresponding to the negative dimension (blunted
affect, emotional withdrawal, motor retardation), as well as formal
thought disorders and mannerisms, corresponding to a disorganisa-
tion dimension (table 1). N the overall sum of the answers. The 5% limit for the bilateral z test
For a more exact qualification of the auditory hallucinations and is 1.950 [9].
the patients’ readiness to obey, a 24-item questionnaire was devel- (4) In a sample of n individuals, analysed for t binary characteris-
oped and used in a semi-structured interview (Appendix). tics which are independent from each other and each quoted at p =
Item selection was based on the literature [1, 4–6] and on our own 0.5, all of the 2t patterns are equally probable. By the use of the con-
clinical experience. The patients were required to answer either ‘yes’ figuration frequency analysis [9], the value of probability for each of
or ‘no’, but they could add further details or descriptions. Eight the 2t patterns (‘configurations’) is given by the expected value e =
patients answered question No. 24 – whether they obeyed the voices n/2t. ¯2 components are used for testing whether an observed pattern
– with ‘yes’ if they obeyed always (n = 2) or usually (n = 6), i.e. the frequency f is significantly higher than expected, given by (f – e)2/e.
so-called ‘compliers’; 23 patients with ‘no’ if they obeyed rarely (n = In our sample, distinct combinations of predictors were analysed for
12) or never (n = 11), i.e. the so-called ‘non-compliers’. defining demarcated patterns of binary characteristics in order to
Identification of variables for predicting compliance with the hal- compare them in compliers and non-compliers.
lucinated voices was done using a secondary coding procedure [9] as (5) In addition, the presence or absence of certain response pat-
follows. terns for each predictor was determined in order to establish their
(1) Answers to question No. 1–23 (predictors) were coded ‘1’ if accuracy in predicting a patient’s inclination to obey or to resist the
the answers pointed in the same direction as question No. 24; other- voices. To that end, the relative risk Ú+ for compliers and Ú – for non-
wise, they were coded ‘0’. This procedure yielded a binary 0/1 matrix compliers were determined and tested to see if the value was signifi-
with 24 variables and 31 cases. Question No. 7 – whether the voices cantly larger than 1. The relative risk values are estimated from Ú+ =
were located inside the head – was omitted for the evaluation as the 2/1 and Ú – = 1 – 1/1–2 derived from an implementation of the
response profile for this item was inverse to that of question No. 8 – data in the tables of Fisher’s exact test, which is called ‘two binomial
whether the voices were located outside of the body – thus, providing sample’ procedure [10].
no further information. Questions, the wording of which appear to
mirror each other, were nevertheless reserved when the answer pro-
files were not inversely related.
(2) We split all predictor variables into those which describe
Results
properties of the voices (voice variables, questions No. 1–13) and
those which describe individual dispositions of the hallucinating Table 1 shows a two-component solution including all
patients (disposition variables, questions No. 14–23). The coding psychopathological symptoms in the PCA. Principal com-
rules are shown in the Appendix.
ponent 1 is characterised by a high correlation with for-
(3) Thus, the data matrix analysed further includes 22 binary
variables for 31 patients. mal thought disorders, hostility and delusions, as well as
Fisher’s exact test for 2 ! 2 tables was used for comparisons of an absence of blunted affect, emotional withdrawal and
compliers and non-compliers and individual item responses with motor retardation, while component 2 shows a high corre-
positive/negative key answers. lation with hallucinations and a low correlation with
McNemar’s formula for the four-field ¯2 test is
motor retardation.
z = (ad – bc)/sqrt(ABCD/N) Compliers (n = 8) were expected to show relatively
sqrt being the square root and a, b, c, and d being the sum of the cells, more code 1 answers than non-compliers (n = 23), and
A and B the sums of each row, C and D the sums of each column, and non-compliers to show relatively more code 0 answers.

Command Hallucinations Psychopathology 2002;35:272–279 273


131.211.208.19 - 11/19/2016 3:49:51 PM
University Library Utrecht
Downloaded by:
Table 2. Comparison of compliers and non-compliers with positive (1) and negative (0) answers, divided into voice and disposition
variables

All variables Voice variables Disposition variables


1 0 sum 1 0 sum 1 0 sum

Compliers (n = 8) a = 121 b = 55 A = 176 a = 68 b = 28 A = 96 a = 53 b = 27 A = 80


Non-compliers (n = 23) c = 314 d = 192 B = 506 c = 176 d = 100 B = 276 c = 138 d = 92 B = 230
C = 435 D = 247 N = 682 C = 244 D = 128 N = 372 C = 191 D = 119 N = 310

All variables: z = 1.592 (n.s.); voice variables: z = 1.255 (n.s.); disposition variables: z = 0.990 (n.s.).

Table 3. Comparison of voice and


disposition variables with compliers and Compliers (n = 8) Non-compliers (n = 23)
non-compliers 1 0 sum 1 0 sum

Voice variables a = 68 b = 28 A = 96 a = 176 b = 100 A = 276


Disposition variables c = 53 d = 27 B = 80 c = 138 d = 92 B = 230
C = 121 D = 55 N = 176 C = 314 D = 192 N = 506

Compliers: z = 0.653 (n.s.); non-compliers: z = 0.869 (n.s.).

Surprisingly, the z value of the test was lower than the coded as being positively predictive of compliance. Non-
limit for the 5% niveau. Moreover, there was no signifi- indifference implies an affective reaction to the hallucina-
cant preference of voice nor of disposition items by com- tions; here, the additional answers described affects such
pliers or non-compliers (table 2). as fear, despair, anger or restlessness, sometimes also hap-
According to our hypothesis that the patient’s attitude piness. Twenty-one patients experienced these reactions.
towards these experiences plays an important role, apart Most of the patients (29 of 31) were alone when halluci-
from the properties of the hallucinated voices, voice and nating and a majority (24 of 31) was uncritical toward
disposition variables were tested versus compliance. their hallucinations.
There was no significant difference of the frequency of the Table 4 shows a comparison of compliers (n = 8) and
answers, neither between both types of variables in the non-compliers (n = 23) regarding the 0/1 coding for the
compliers nor in the non-complier group (table 3), affirm- four characteristics chosen in order to analyse the re-
ing our hypothesis. Therefore, for the ongoing analysis, sponse pattern for frequency and distribution.
both data sets could be pooled. The comparison of compliers and non-compliers re-
In the binary matrix, there was not one item coded 0 garding the frequency of patterns reveals that the posthal-
for all 23 non-compliers. There were, however, 3 items lucinatory affective reaction, being alone, the assumed
coded 1 (prone to agree with voices) for all 8 compliers, reality of the voice and its familiarity occur most often in
and 1 coded 1 for 7 compliers. The following 4 items were compliers. Seven of 8 compliers show a 1-1-1-1 pattern.
chosen for further examinations: (1) familiar voice (No. This pattern is sufficiently sensitive but not exclusively
5); (2) not indifferent following hallucination (No. 16); specific for compliers, as it is also found in 3 of the 23
(3) being alone (No. 18), and (4) voice is instantly seen as non-compliers. Moreover, the pattern of the complier,
real (No. 23). 0-1-1-1, is identical to that of 6 of the non-compliers. The
Nineteen patients recognized the voices as attributable possibility arises that two configurations exist with high
to a person known to them. Indifference following halluci- frequencies underlying the 2t answer patterns examined
nation was coded inversely, i.e., a negative answer was in table 5.

274 Psychopathology 2002;35:272–279 Erkwoh/Willmes/Eming-Erdmann/Kunert


131.211.208.19 - 11/19/2016 3:49:51 PM
University Library Utrecht
Downloaded by:
Table 4. A comparison of compliers vs. non- Table 5. Frequency analysis of configurations of 4 binary variables
compliers regarding the coding of 4 charac- with 31 patients
teristics of auditory hallucinations
Questions f e (f–e)2/e
Questions
5 16 28 22
5 16 18 22
1 1 1 1 10 1.9375 33.61
Compliers (n = 8) 1 1 1 0 2 1.9375 2.0
0 1 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 1.9375 1.9
1 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 5 1.9375 4.8
1 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 7 1.9375 13.21
1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 1.9375 1.9
1 1 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 1.9375 0.5
1 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1.9375 0.5
1 1 1 1 0 1 1 0 1 1.9375 0.5
1 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 1 1.9375 0.5
0 1 0 1 0 1.9375 1.9
Non-compliers (n = 23)
1 0 0 0 0 1.9375 1.9
0 1 1 0
0 1 0 0 1 1.9375 0.5
1 0 1 1
0 0 1 0 2 1.9375 2.0
1 1 1 1
0 0 0 1 0 1.9375 1.9
0 1 1 1
0 0 0 0 0 1.9375 1.9
1 0 1 1
1 1 1 1
f = Frequency of the configuration in the sample; e = expected
0 1 1 1
value of the configuration to occur.
1 0 1 1 1 1 ¯2 [unilateral; 0.01; (f 1 e); d.f. = 1] = 5.41.
0 0 1 0
1 0 1 1
1 0 1 1
1 0 0 1
0 1 1 1
0 1 1 1 fore, the item which occurred predominantly as code 1
0 0 1 1 among the (‘false’ group of) non-compliers, question No.
1 1 1 0
1 0 1 0
18 (being alone) is eliminated. For the remaining three
1 1 1 0 characteristics, a configural frequency analysis was car-
0 1 1 1 ried out in table 6.
1 1 1 1 As shown in table 6, the configuration of the 1-1-1 pat-
0 1 1 1 tern is unequivocal, with no counterpart that unambi-
0 1 0 0
0 0 1 0
guously samples non-compliers. These three characteris-
tics were also used to estimate the relative risk for com-
Questions: 5 = the patient knows who is pliance (table 7).
speaking; 16 = the patient is not indifferent The relative risk of patients showing all three signs
following hallucinations; 18 = the patient is (voice familiarity, posthallucinatory affective condition
alone when hearing voices; 22 = the patient
and reality check) being compliers compared with non-
immediately accepts the hallucinated voices
as real. compliers is estimated as Ú+ = 6.708. The exact 95% confi-
dence interval was computed using StatXact [10]. Since
the lower confidence interval limit (1.351–95.50) is well
above 1, there is a significantly higher risk of compliers to
have all three signs in common (p = 0.0113). Otherwise,
In fact, there are two significantly frequent configura- the relative risk of patients missing at least one of the
tions; one which comprises compliers (1-1-1-1) and anoth- three signs being non-compliers compared with compliers
er which characterises non-compliers (0-1-1-1). The sec- is Ú – = 6.957 with an exact 95% confidence interval lower
ond configuration, however, is very similar to the first limit of 1.401–85.07), also indicating that non-compliers
sharing three characteristics coded 1. No configuration present with a significantly higher probability of having
can be defined comprising a majority of 0 codes. There- less than all three signs (p = 0.0075).

Command Hallucinations Psychopathology 2002;35:272–279 275


131.211.208.19 - 11/19/2016 3:49:51 PM
University Library Utrecht
Downloaded by:
Table 6. Cluster analysis of 3 binary vari- Method
ables with 31 patients We used binary data analysis to answer the question
which symptoms accompanying command hallucinations
Questions f e (f–e)2/e
are of risk for the patient to obey or to resist. This method
5 16 22 is favourably applied in studies where responders should
answer in a predefined key direction (Appendix). Binary
1 1 1 10 3.875 9.71
1 1 0 2 3.875 0.9 data have, however, the disadvantage to truncate patient
1 0 1 6 3.875 1.2 answers to ‘yes’ and ‘no’, or to ‘1’ and ‘0’, respectively.
0 1 1 7 3.875 2.5 Configuration frequency analysis [9], developed on the
1 0 0 1 3.875 2.1 basis of binary data analysis, helped to determine the pat-
0 1 0 2 3.875 0.9
tern of symptoms (tables 5, 6) to be selected for the rela-
0 0 1 1 3.875 2.1
0 0 0 2 3.875 0.9 tive risk calculation [10]. Earlier studies of that topic used
correlation analyses [1, 26] which did not divide the
f = Frequency of the configuration in the patient samples into compliers and non-compliers and
sample; e = expected value of the configura- analysed only the interdependence of the variables.
tion to occur.
1 1 ¯2 [unilateral; 0.01; (f 1 e); d.f. = 1] =
PCA was performed to show the structure of the symp-
5.41. toms of our patient sample. We did not consider the psy-
chopathological symptoms found with PANSS since com-
ponent 2, a ‘hallucination factor’ (table 1), was correlated
with delusions and with no other symptom. Nevertheless,
on theoretical grounds, it is possible that formal thought
Table 7. A comparison of compliers vs. non-compliers regarding the disorders or uncooperative behaviour, used as exclusion
presence and the absence of 3 psychopathological characteristics criteria in our study, may provide conditions under which
(questions 5, 16 and 22) of auditory hallucinations in 31 schizo-
compliance with command hallucinations is arguable.
phrenics
Formal thought disorders combined with command hal-
Non- Compliers Total lucinations may prevent establishing an organized pattern
compliers of behaviour and may therefore prevent compliance. On
the other hand, formal thought disorders may be associat-
All 3 characteristics present 3 7 10
At least 1 characteristic missing 20 1 21 ed with increased impulsiveness as a trait of personality
disorders [35], and a lack of control of impulsiveness may
Total 23 8 31
favour behaviour induced by command hallucinations.
Similar considerations apply to uncooperative behav-
iour.

Risk Factors for Compliance with Command


Discussion Hallucinations
Voice variables and disposition variables which mirror
Command hallucinations can induce a behaviour the attitude of the patients to their hallucinations are of
which is hardly predictable and may sometimes be dan- equivocal importance (tables 2, 3). This result shows that
gerous. Will the patient do what the voices tell him or her there is no satisfactory answer given to the question of
to do? Although this question is of great practical value, prediction unless the psychopathological analysis in-
little is known about the connection between command cludes the nature of the ‘voices’. The triad of voice famil-
hallucinations and behavioural consequences. Since the iarity, affective condition following hallucinations and
inception of first-rank symptoms by Kurt Schneider [40], reality testing constitutes a combination which is helpful
auditory hallucinations are regarded as being of primary when making a prognosis. Based on relative risk calcula-
importance in the diagnosis of schizophrenia, which ac- tion, we found that there is a significantly higher risk of
cording to DSM-IV [41] can be made solely on the ground compliers to have all three signs in common. Otherwise,
of this one symptom. Competence to predict behaviour non-compliers show a significantly higher probability to
following command hallucinations is relevant for the lack at least one of these three signs (table 7). This symp-
treatment of schizophrenia. tom set appears suitable for clinical bed-side judgments.

276 Psychopathology 2002;35:272–279 Erkwoh/Willmes/Eming-Erdmann/Kunert


131.211.208.19 - 11/19/2016 3:49:51 PM
University Library Utrecht
Downloaded by:
To our knowledge, our cohort of patients exhibiting the tion, a new aspect, an emotional response, is emphasized
psychopathological symptom of command hallucinations as the third element bridging symptom and compliance.
is comparatively large. Nevertheless, cross-validation All compliers showed posthallucinatory affect (non-indif-
with a new patient group is needed. ferent), in contrast to their condition prior to hearing
Identification of the Voice. In analyses of auditory hal- voices. These patients gave additional descriptions when
lucination, it is well established to emphasize the proper- reporting non-indifference following hallucinations,
ties of the voices. Cognitive models of the pathogenesis of which were mainly negative emotions such as fear, de-
auditory hallucinations stress the hypothesis of dysfunc- spair, anger or restlessness, rarely happiness. These nega-
tional monitoring of inner speech [11–17]. Imaging tech- tive emotions could be a link between the semantic con-
niques have underlined the importance of linguistic neu- tent of the hallucinations and the patient’s behaviour.
ronal networks [18–22], but have also given ambiguous They evidently favour a connection between illusion and
findings [23]. Our results suggest that patients tend to action, or of compliance in Junginger’s [2] sense, i.e., a
obey command hallucinations if the voice is familiar. Lis- heightened readiness to obey.
tening to a voice that is known to the listener seems to
increase the credibility of the voice. Junginger [24] also Forensic Consequences
considered voice familiarity a risk factor for compliance. Predictors of behaviour following command hallucina-
Possibly, in these cases, a stored voice memory is acti- tions have been dealt with before, especially in the foren-
vated and via activation of the primary auditory cortex sic literature. However, the cases described included
(Heschl’s gyrus) reaches the hearing in form of a halluci- chronic schizophrenia [36], a history of violent crimes
nation [21]. As suggested by the findings of Frank et al. [37], delusional interpretations of hallucinations or, as
[25] and Oulis et al. [26], however, the linguistic charac- in our study, familiarity with the voice [24]. Patients
teristics of the hallucinations or their form of address with such abnormal experiences cannot be distinguished
hardly allow a conclusion regarding compliance. clinically or prognostically from paranoid-hallucinatory
Reality Decision. Accepting hallucinations as real is an schizophrenics [3].
important factor in obeying command hallucinations. All The forensic literature deals particularly with the dan-
compliers saw them as real, i.e., they were uncritical; but ger of acts resulting from command hallucinations. Exam-
not all uncritical patients obeyed their commands. There- ining a small cohort of convicted criminals showing these
fore, accepting hallucinations as real does not provide suf- symptoms, Rogers et al. [38] pointed out the singular
ficient reason for compliance. Reality testing theories [27, aggressivity of the voices heard and the fact that they
28] assume that schizophrenics see their hallucinations as often talked of self-punishment. On the other hand, Hel-
real when their source is externalised (given that real lerstein et al. [39], examining a psychiatric in-patient
acoustic signals usually originate from outside of the cohort, found no differences in the danger of harming
body) [29]. In our sample, there were 5 of 8 compliers who oneself or others between patients with command halluci-
externalised versus only 6 of 23 non-compliers. Drawing nations and those with other types, such as commenting
on Jasper’s [30] definition that ‘genuine’ hallucinations or dialoguing hallucinations. According to Junginger [2],
are real, objective and occur outside of the body, the command hallucinations show a distinct possibility of
voices inside the head described by our patients could influencing the patient’s behaviour precisely when they
thus be considered pseudohallucinations. do not give dangerous instructions, whereas the reverse
Being alone played a minor role as a predictor of com- would facilitate the patient’s compliance with other in-
pliance. It implies the absence of competing external stim- structions. This would lend particular weight to the corre-
uli to the cortex, which processes acoustic signals or, in the lations between command hallucinations and compliance
case of hallucinations, generates them. When asked to lis- or non-compliance found for this symptom in the present
ten to music or the news [31] or to read a short story out psychopathological analysis.
loud, schizophrenics undergoing controlled hallucination
report a suppression of their hallucinations [32, 33]. Fal-
loon and Talbot [34] reported that a number of patients Acknowledgement
they interviewed increased social contact in an attempt to
The authors thank A. Rodón for translation and editing.
cope with the voices.
Affective Alterations. From a scientific point of view, it
is noteworthy that with posthallucinatory affective reac-

Command Hallucinations Psychopathology 2002;35:272–279 277


131.211.208.19 - 11/19/2016 3:49:51 PM
University Library Utrecht
Downloaded by:
Appendix

Questionnaire, trait coding 0/1, hypothetical basis of rules for coding of compliance/non-compliance

1 How many voices do you hear? (If one voice, it is coded 1, as it is 15 Were you nervous before you started hearing the voices? (A sim-
easy to obey one voice; if more than one, it is coded 0, as it is ple ‘yes’ is coded 1, as nervousness is associated with a higher
difficult to obey several different voices) predisposition to obey; if not, it is coded 0)
2 Do the voices address you directly? (If the patient is addressed 16 Were you emotionally indifferent after hearing the voices? (For
personally, it is coded 1, as being addressed directly makes obey- not being indifferent, there is a variety of irritable or nervous
ing easier; if not addressed personally, it is coded 0) posthallucinatory conditions to note; they are all coded 1; for
3 Do the voices command using address in the third person (he or indifference, it is coded 0, as indifference is associated with sta-
she)? (If not, it is coded 1; if yes, it is coded 0, as being addressed bility and a lower predisposition to obey)
in the third person makes obeying more difficult) 17 Were you nervous after hearing the voices? (A simple ‘yes’ is
4 Is it a human voice? (If yes, it is coded 1, as it is familiar to obey a coded 1, as posthallucinatory nervousness is associated with a
human voice; if the sound is unsimilar to a human voice, it is higher predisposition to obey; if not, it is coded 0)
coded 0) 18 Were you alone when you heard the voices? (When alone, it is
5 Do you know who is talking to you or about you? (If the voice is coded 1, as there are fewer distractions when one is alone; when
attributed to known persons, it is coded 1, as it is easier to obey a not alone, it is coded 0)
familiar voice; in case of an alien voice, it is coded 0) 19 Were you doing something when the voices started? (When not
6 Do you hear the voices very often or constantly? (If often, it is engaged in activity, it is coded 1, as there would not be any dis-
coded 1, as it is more likely to obey an intrusive command; if the tractor; otherwise, it is coded 0, as a patient who is currently
voices are rarely heard, it is coded 0) engaged in an activity tends to be distracted from hallucina-
7 Are the voices located inside your head? (If not, it is coded 1; if tions)
yes, it is coded 0, as within-head localisation is unusual) 20 Were you talking to someone when the voices started? (When
8 Are the voices located outside of your body? (If yes, it is coded 1, silent, it is coded 1, as hallucinations can be suppressed by con-
as out-of-body localisation is normal in hearing; if not, it is versation; when in conversation, it is coded 0)
coded 0) 21 Can you suppress or stop the voices? (When there is no interrup-
9 Do the voices speak in regular conversation volume? (If yes, it is tion, it is coded 1, as hallucinations are experienced as indepen-
coded 1, as loudness of conversation corresponds to normal con- dent of the will; otherwise, it is coded 0, as suppression of halluci-
ditions; in deviating cases, it is coded 0) nations implies control)
10 Do the voices whisper? (If not, it is coded 1, as the voices should 22 When you hear the voices, do you think that the commands you
otherwise be understandable; if yes, it is coded 0, as whispering is hear were actually said? (If the hallucination is instantly taken as
unusual) real, it is coded 1, as commands believed to be real are followed
11 Do the voices speak in full sentences? (Speaking in full sentences more readily; if not, it is coded 0)
is coded 1, as commands tend to be obeyed more readily when 23 After hearing the voices, do you ever think about whether these
given in full sentences, any different version is coded 0) perceptions are actually possible? (If a belated consideration
12 Do the voices speak in single words? (If not, the voice is sup- comes to the judgment of the hallucination to be real, it is coded
posed to be well designed and is coded 1; single-word commands 1, as critical thinking constitutes an additional reality check; in
are harder to obey being coded 0) the negative case, it is coded 0)
13 Do the voices talk all at the same time? (For a single voice, the 24 Did you ever do what the voices told you to do? (A complier is
risk to obey is deemed high and coded 1; if yes, it is coded 0, as it coded 1; a non-complier is coded 0)
is hard to obey when the voices are all talking at the same time)
14 Were you calm before you started hearing the voices? (For not
being calm, there is a variety of irritable or nervous conditions to
note; they are all coded 1; calmness is coded 0, as a relaxed
patient is stabler and less likely to obey)

278 Psychopathology 2002;35:272–279 Erkwoh/Willmes/Eming-Erdmann/Kunert


131.211.208.19 - 11/19/2016 3:49:51 PM
University Library Utrecht
Downloaded by:
References

1 Junginger J, Frame CL: Self-report of the fre- 15 McGuire PK, Silbersweig DA, Wright I, Mur- 28 David A, Buchanan A, Reed A, Almeida O:
quency and phenomenology of verbal halluci- ray RM, Frackowiak RSJ, Frith CD: The neu- The assessment of insight in psychosis. Br J
nations. J Nerv Ment Dis 1985;173:149–155. ral correlates of inner speech and auditory ver- Psychiatry 1992;161:599–602.
2 Junginger J: Command hallucinations and the bal imagery in schizophrenia: Relationship to 29 Aggernæs A: The experience reality of halluci-
prediction of dangerousness. Psychiatr Serv auditory verbal hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry nations and other psychological phenomena:
1995;46:911–914. 1996;169:148–159. An empirical analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand
3 Zisook S, Byrd D, Kuck J, Jeste DV: Command 16 Johns LC, McGuire PK: Verbal self-monitor- 1972;48:220–238.
hallucinations in outpatients with schizophre- ing and auditory hallucinations in schizophre- 30 Jaspers K: Allgemeine Psychopathologie, ed 5.
nia. J Clin Psychiatry 1995;56:462–465. nia. Lancet 1999;353:469–470. Berlin, Springer, 1948, pp 59–60.
4 Sedman G: ‘Inner voices’. Phenomenological 17 Shergill SS, Brammer MJ, Williams SCR, Mur- 31 Feder R: Auditory hallucinations treated by
and clinical aspects. Br J Psychiatry 1966;112: ray RM, McGuire PK: Mapping auditory hal- radio headphones. Am J Psychiatry 1982;139:
485–490. lucinations in schizophrenia using functional 1188–1190.
5 Lowe GR: The phenomenology of hallucina- magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psy- 32 Margo A, Hemsley DR, Sladem PD: The ef-
tions as an aid to differential diagnosis. Br J chiatry 2000;57:1033–1038. fects of varying auditory input on schizophren-
Psychiatry 1973;123:621–633. 18 Cleghorn JM, Franco S, Szechtman B, Kaplan ic hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry 1981;139:
6 Nayani TH, David AS: The auditory hallucina- RD, Szechtman H, Brown GM, Nahmias C, 122–127.
tion: A phenomenological survey. Psychol Med Garnett S: Toward a brain map of auditory hal- 33 Gallagher AG, Dinan TG, Baker LJV: The
1996;26:177–189. lucinations. Am J Psychiatry 1992;149:1062– effects of varying auditory input on schizo-
7 American Psychiatric Association (ed): Diag- 1069. phrenic hallucinations: A replication. Br J Med
nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor- 19 McGuire PK, Shah GMS, Murray RM: In- Psychol 1994;67:67–75.
ders (DSM-III-R), ed 3, revised. Washington, reased blood flow in Broca’s area during audi- 34 Falloon IR, Talbot RE: Persistent auditory hal-
American Psychiatric Press, 1987. tory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Lancet lucinations: Coping mechanisms and implica-
8 Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA: The positive 1993;342:703–706. tions for management. Psychol Med 1981;11:
and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for 20 David AS, Woodruff PWR, Howard R, Ho- 329–339.
schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987;13:261– ward R, Mellers JDC, Brammer M, Bullmore 35 Herpertz SC, Sass H: Das Konzept der Störun-
276. E, Wright I, Andrew C, Williams SCR: Audito- gen der Impulskontrolle in den modernen Klas-
9 Krauth J: Einführung in die Konfigurationsfre- ry hallucinations inhibit exogenous activation sifikationssystemen; in Gross G, Huber G, Sass
quenzanalyse (KFA). Ein multivariates nicht- of auditory association cortex. Neuroreport H (eds): Moderne psychiatrische Klassifika-
parametrisches Verfahren zum Nachweis und 1996;7:932–936. tionssysteme. Implikationen für Diagnose und
zur Interpretation von Typen und Syndromen. 21 Dierks T, Linden DEJ, Jandl M, Formisano E, Therapie, Forschung und Praxis. Stuttgart,
Weinheim, Psychologie Verlag Union, 1993. Goebel R, Lanfermann H, Singer W: Activa- Schattauer, 1998.
10 Mehta C, Patel N: StatXact4 for Windows: Sta- tion of Heschl’s gyrus during auditory halluci- 36 Benjamin L: Is chronicity a function of the rela-
tistical Software for Exact Nonparametric In- nations. Neuron 1999;22:615–621. tionship between the person and the auditory
ference, User Manual. Cambridge, CYTEL 22 Lennox BR, Park SB, Medley I, Morris PG, hallucination? Schizophr Bull 1989;15:291–
Software Corporation, 1999, pp 435–441. Jones PB: The functional anatomy of auditory 309.
11 David AS: The neuropsychological origin of hallucinations in schizophrenia. Psychiatry 37 McNiel D: Hallucinations and violence; in
auditory hallucinations; in David AS, Cutting Res 2000;100:13–20. Monahan J, Steadman H (eds): Violence and
JC (eds): The Neuropsychology of Schizophre- 23 Shergill SS, Bullmore E, Simmons A, Murray Mental Disorder: Developments in risk assess-
nia. Hove, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, R, McGuire P: Functional anatomy of auditory ment. Chicago, University of Chicago Press,
1994, pp 269–313. verbal imagery in schizophrenic patients with 1994.
12 Frith CD: The positive and negative symptoms auditory hallucinations. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 38 Rogers R, Gillis J, Turner R, Frise-Smith T:
of schizophrenia reflect impairments in the 157:1691–1693. The clinical presentation of command halluci-
perception and initiation of action. Psychol 24 Junginger J: Predicting compliance with com- nations in a forensic population. Am J Psychia-
Med 1987;17:631–648. mand hallucinations. Am J Psychiatry 1990; try 1990;147:1304–1307.
13 Bentall R, Baker G, Havers S: Reality monitor- 147:245–247. 39 Hellerstein D, Frosch W, Koenigsberg HW:
ing and psychotic hallucinations. Br J Clin Psy- 25 Frank SM, Rendon MI, Siomopoulous G: Lan- The clinical significance of command halluci-
chol 1991;30:213–222. guage in hallucinations of adult schizophrenics; nations. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144:212–219.
14 McGuire PK, Silbersweig DA, Murray RM, in Rieber RW (ed): Applied Psycholinguistics 40 Schneider K: Klinische Psychopathologie, ed
David AS, Frackowiak RSJ, Frith CD: Func- and Mental Health. New York, Plenum, 1980; 12. Stuttgart, Thieme, 1970.
tional anatomy of inner speech and auditory pp 115–126. 41 American Psychiatric Association (ed): Diag-
verbal imagery. Psychol Med 1996;126:29–38. 26 Oulis PG, Mavreas VG, Mamounas JM, Ste- nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor-
fanis CM: Clinical characteristics of auditory ders (DSM-IV), ed 4. Washington, American
hallucinations. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1995;92: Psychiatric Press, 1994.
97–102.
27 David AS: Insight and psychosis. Br J Psychia-
try 1990;156:798–808.

Command Hallucinations Psychopathology 2002;35:272–279 279


131.211.208.19 - 11/19/2016 3:49:51 PM
University Library Utrecht
Downloaded by:

You might also like