You are on page 1of 13

Work 57 (2017) 43–54 43

DOI:10.3233/WOR-172533
IOS Press

Analysis of air temperature changes


on blood pressure and heart rate
and performance of undergraduate students
Joseana C.F. Siqueiraa , Luiz Bueno da Silvaa,∗ , Antônio S. Coutinhoa and Rafaela M. Rodriguesb
a Department of Production Engineering, Federal University of Paraiba, João Pessoa, Paraiba, Brazil
b Department of Psychology, Federal University of Paraiba, João Pessoa, Paraiba, Brazil

Received 15 March 2016


Accepted 17 October 2016

Abstract.
BACKGROUND: The increase in air temperature has been associated with human deaths, some of which are related to
cardiovascular dysfunctions, and with the reduction of physical and cognitive performance in humans.
OBJECTIVE: To analyze the relationship between blood pressure (BP) and heart rate (HR) and the cognitive performance
of students who were submitted to temperature changes in classrooms.
METHODS: The university students answered a survey that was adapted from the Battery of Reasoning Tests over 3
consecutive days at different air temperatures while their thermal state and HR were measured. During those 3 days, BP and
HR were evaluated before and after the cognitive test.
RESULTS: The average and final HR increased at high temperatures; the tests execution time was reduced at high tempera-
tures; and the cognitive tests was related to Mean BP at the beginning of the test, the maximum HR during the test and the
air temperature.
CONCLUSIONS: The cognitive performance of undergraduate students in the field of engineering and technology will
increase while performing activities in a learning environment with an air temperature of approximately 23.3◦ C (according
to their thermal perception), if students have an initial MBP of 93.33 mmHg and a 60 bpm HRmax.

Keywords: Thermal comfort, thermoregulation, cognitive activity, productivity, cardiovascular system

1. Introduction the physical environment reduces the overload of


regulatory systems of the body and improves the
Concern about the effects of the environment ability to participate in activities [1, 2]. This con-
on humans has aroused the interest of several cern has been influenced by climate change and by
researchers, resulting in a comprehensive literature content change within environments. According to
on the subject. Inadequacies in the environment Spector [3], an intelligent environment refers to a
may lead to health disorders such as sick build- place that uses adaptive technologies or models that
ing syndrome (SBS) and productivity loss. This include innovative resources to improve the under-
fact underscores the notion that the adequacy of standing and performance of individuals. However,
according to the same author, few plans of sus-
∗ Address for correspondence: Luiz Bueno da Silva, Depart-
tainable strategies have been designed to improve
ment of Production Engineering, Federal University of Paraiba, the quality of those environments; consequently, no
Cidade Universitária, s/n - Castelo Branco, João Pessoa -
PB, 58051-900, Brazil. Tel.: +55 83 991066287; E-mail:
attention has been given to empirical research in this
bueno@ct.ufpb.br. field.

1051-9815/17/$35.00 © 2017 – IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved
44 J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR

When the adequacy or lack thereof of the physical students. Although cardiovascular parameters and
environment refers to a school environment, the envi- tasks that involve mental solving skills have displayed
ronment must be recognized as a key factor in an area an association, the relationship between these factors
of human activity in which excellence is a goal [4]. and performance has been little explored, according
The characteristics of the environment directly affect to Ickovics et al. [18].
student performance, particularly regarding crystal- Factors such as air temperature and quality, light-
lized intelligence (Gc), the development of which ing and acoustics contribute to the quality of the
depends mainly on culture and formal education [5]. indoor environment. Furthermore, recent studies have
This intelligence refers to the type of knowledge and identified that human performance is largely depen-
skills there are acquired through the education pro- dent on environmental quality [19]. Higgins et al. [20]
cess and life experience [6]. found that extreme temperatures, inadequate light-
Thus, issues regarding comfort derived from the ing and poor air quality have negative effects on the
environment must be considered by educational insti- cognitive development of students.
tutions as promoters of scholastic skills. The concept Parsons [21], Wargocki and Wyon [22] and Cui
of comfort is related to the mental satisfaction over et al. [19] advocate that air temperature affects moti-
the environment [7], including the functionality and vation, which, in turn, largely explains students’
thermal, acoustic and lighting conditions of it [8]. performance at class. Accordingly, Primi et al. [5]
Some authors, including Heath and Mendell [9], have emphasize that the motivation, interest and dedication
already highlighted that indoor physical environmen- of students are major predictors of the development
tal conditions directly affect student performance. of intelligence.
Also, according to Silva et al. [10], the character- According to Yang et al. [16], given the rapidly
istics of the environment, such as thermal conditions, increasing use of technologies during class (e.g. com-
could affect individuals’ heath, with great impact in puters, projectors) researchers have been motivated
psychological and physiological factors. to examine the effect of those equipment have both
Learning is considered one of the most important on learning and on the perception of students in the
part of the human’s life, however it could be very educational environment.
sensitive to the environment characteristics such as However, a consensus regarding their benefit or
indoor air temperature, affecting not only the perfor- harm to students has not been reached because some
mance but also the health of students and teachers authors advocate that the technology used in the class-
[11]. Besides, Pawar and Khedkar [12] had previ- room is a positive factor whereas others claim that it
ously evidenced the need of more implementation of has a negative effect on learning [16]. Nevertheless,
ergonomics at schools. according to Yang et al. [16], temperature is one of
Although many studies about air temperature, air the most significant factors affecting the perception
quality and other variables of the learning environ- of students regarding comfort in the classroom.
ment [13–15], recognize the importance of thermal Zhang et al. [23], using undergraduates in a cli-
comfort on students’ performance, these same studies mate chamber in China, noted that temperature
do not use any measure of cognitive performance, nor affects both psychological and physiological factors.
even relating it to the comfort and thermal sensation. However, in regards to temperature change, psycho-
Yang et al. [16] conducted a study to investigate logical conditions are stabilized first, followed by
how the environmental variables, such as temper- physiological conditions. These results suggest that
ature, air quality and room layout, influence the physiological conditions have more negative impact
performance of students, according to a subjective of the temperature change, in comparison with psy-
score answered by them. Although the temperature chological conditions, evidencing the importance of
has shown itself one of the most important attributes studies about both temperature and physiological
in the classroom, it was not related with the students’ variables.
performance, according to their own perception. Cao et al. [24] emphasize that the discomfort
In a critical review on how environmental con- sensation felt by people is not caused by a single
ditions affect performance and health, Mendell and factor. Instead, it is affected by several factors con-
Heath [17] claim there is little consistent evidence comitantly. Thus, temperature does not operate alone
available, although they indicate that common condi- regarding the promotion of discomfort because it is
tions found in education environments have adverse usually related to lighting and air quality, as shown
effects on the health and academic performance of by Barrett et al. [25].
J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR 45

Lan et al. [26] argue that the effect of the environ- changes in HR variability and to a sustained increase
ment on the cognitive performance of students may in BP, and these effects are reversed with rest.
be systematically measured by psychometric tests of Vuksanovic and Gal [36] observed that changes
neurobehavioral functions. Thus, the Battery of Rea- in autonomic modulation are expressed in the high-
soning Tests 5 (BPR-5) was used in this study to frequency (HF) spectral power and are responsible
measure the cognitive performance of undergraduate for increasing HR under mental stress. Furthermore,
students under different temperature conditions. Schnell et al. [37] showed that thermal load levels,
The BPR-5 is an instrument that helps prepare psy- which are measured using the physiological equiva-
cho diagnoses, selection and vocational guidance and lent temperature, are associated with low frequency
school evaluation; it also assesses general cognitive (LF) and HF levels.
functioning, according to Almeida and Primi [27]. Changes in an electrocardiogram (ECG) are asso-
This instrument consists of 5 subtests: the Abstract ciated with environmental variations. These changes
Reasoning (AR) Test, the Verbal Reasoning (VR) do not have a linear trend when related to tempera-
Test, the Numerical Reasoning (NR) Test, the Spatial ture and humidity and may have higher values at low
Reasoning (SR) Test and the Mechanical Reasoning or high temperatures [23, 30, 38, 39]. Liu et al. [30]
(MR) Test, which are all used to measure specific identified that the LF/HF ratio was higher at a dis-
skills [28]. comfort level than at a comfort level when describing
Similar to performance, heart rate (HR) and blood the association of cardiac activity with the thermal
pressure (BP) are also affected by environmental con- sensation of occupants. The increased LF/HF ratio
ditions depending on the type of activity performed indicates that the activity of the sympathetic nervous
[29, 30]. The variation in HR is more sensitive to system (SNS) was high. The SNS stimulates auto-
demands of perception than of attention [31]. Whar- matic actions that allow our body to react in stressful
ton et al. [32] found a positive relationship between situations by accelerating the heartbeat and increas-
BP and the performance of visuospatial tasks. Lyn- ing BP, adrenaline, blood sugar concentration and
gdoh et al. [33] found an association between some activation of the general metabolism of the body.
cognitive functions and BP; however, the associations Although Akimoto et al. [40] failed to identify dif-
were gender specific or exclusively related to systolic ferences in the perceived mental workload in office
BP (SBP) or diastolic BP (DBP), thus precluding their workers, Lan et al. [2] demonstrated that the increase
generalization. in temperature produces an increase in perceived
The dependence of the activity performed may mental workload.
be related to the cortical area activated while per- The results from a study conducted by Taelman
forming the task and temperature control areas of the et al. [41] confirmed that HR was higher and vagal
human body. The negative effects observed in health modulation was lower when assessing the relation-
and performance when people are in thermally hot ship of mental tasks with normal office physical
environments may be caused by physiological mech- activity; that is, the effect of a mental workload
anisms, according to Lan et al. [2]. outweighs the cardiovascular effects of a physical
It should be noted that cardiovascular param- workload, suggesting an additional effect in the com-
eters are affected by cognitive activity. However, bination of multiple tasks.
cognitive performance, in turn, may be affected by However, regarding environmental effects on
these parameters. This fact was exemplified in the health, it is noteworthy that no mono-causal relation-
study conducted by Lan et al. [2], who observed ship between temperature and health exists according
that reduced oxygenation (SpO2 ) may reduce to Hübler et al. [42]. Different risk factors, including
cognitive functioning and consequently student age (children and the elderly), physical conditions
performance. (pre-existing conditions) and low fitness exacer-
Some studies have only found a relationship bate the thermal load effects. Indeed, Luque-Casado
between performance and changes in HR and BP. et al. [31] observed that fitness is a key factor to
Bernardi et al. [34] found a reciprocal relationship improve cardiac autonomic control and cognitive per-
between mental activity and stress that was associ- formance.
ated with sympathetic activation of the cardiovascular Health-promoting strategies may also improve
system; this activation may be affected by the indi- learning in addition to fulfilling their original pur-
vidual’s breathing. Hjortskov et al. [35] noted that pose, according to Ickovics et al. [18]. The authors
mental stress suffered during computer work leads to suggest that the management of chronic health condi-
46 J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR

Table 1
Study variables and indicators
Variables Indicators
Comfort Parameters Personal Activity performed, M, (W/m2 );
Thermal insulation of garments, Icl , (clo); Height (m); weight (kg); age
(years); gender; HR (bpm); BP (mmHg)
Environmental Air temperature (◦ C);
Average radiant temperature, trm , (◦ C);
Air velocity, va , (m/s);
Partial pressure of water vapor in ambient air, pa , (kPa)
Subjective Parameters Thermal sensation Seven-point scale of perceptions and preferences of the International
Thermal assessment Organization for Standardization (ISO) standards 10551/1995
Performance Reasoning Hit rate and time spent in the Verbal Reasoning (RV);
Hit rate and time spent in the Abstract Reasoning (AR);
Hit rate and time spent in the Mechanical Reasoning (MR);
Hit rate and time spent in the Spatial Reasoning (SR);
Hit rate and time spent in the Numeric Reasoning (NR);
Hit rate and time spent in the Overall Score (BPR)

tions increases physical activity and proper nutrition tus. The others interested were excluded based on
promotion among the strategies, which results in the exclusion criteria: 1) self-reported cardiovascu-
healthy school environments that improve emotional lar and metabolic diseases; 2) calculated body mass
and behavioral health. index (BMI) greater than 30; and 3) failure to attend
Based on the above information, the present study the 3 study days.
sought to answer the following research question: The study environments were computer laborato-
“Could blood pressure and heart rate influence the ries with 35 to 40 computers. Each computer had
cognitive performance of students under different a case placed below the workstation and a cath-
air temperatures in learning environments?” The ode ray tube (CRT) monitor, keyboard and mouse
aim of this study was to analyze the relationship placed above the workstation. Each bench had 3 to
between heart rate, blood pressure and cognitive 4 terminals placed side by side. The rooms were
performance of students who were subjected to air-conditioned with split air conditioning, artificial
air temperature variations in learning environments lighting distributed on the ceiling, and audiovisual
with devices called news ICT (Information, Com- resources and wireless technologies.
munication and Technology) that include systems as The variables of interest that are shown in Table 1,
computers, projectors, air conditioners, wireless sig- and their respective indicators were analyzed in the
nals, audio-visual systems, etc. present study. The method used for data collection is
explained below.
2. Experimental methods
2.1. Comfort parameters
This study was approved by the Ethics Committee
of the Federal University of Paraı́ba (Universidade 2.1.1. Personal characteristics
Federal da Paraı́ba; CAAE: 31037614.0.0000.5188). Data on height, weight, HR and BP were col-
The study was conducted in learning environments lected from all of the students who volunteered for
with news ICT, specifically in computer labs of pub- this study. Weight was measured using a digital scale
lic and private higher education institutions in the with a 150-kg capacity and a 100-gram variation. The
field of exact sciences. All students were invited height of the subjects was measured using astadiome-
to participate after explaining the objectives and ter. This measurement was manually recorded on the
methods of the study. The students who showed data spreadsheet by the same researcher to avoid any
interest in participating signed an informed consent disparities. Based on these data, the Excel’s spread-
form and moved on to the initial data collection sheet was used to calculate the body mass index
step. (BMI) of each participant, and this parameter was
Twenty-eight volunteers were selected based on classified as low weight (<18,5), normal weight (18,5
the following inclusion criteria: 1) aged 17 to 30 < BMI < 24,9), overweight (25 < BMI < 29,9) and
years and 2) good self-reported general health sta- obese (>30).
J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR 47

The BP and HR were assessed in the experimen- scale of perceptions and preferences of the ISO Stan-
tal days before and after the cognitive performance dard 10551/1995 [45]. These questions were included
tests using the automatic arm blood pressure monitor at the end of the reasoning test.
model HEM-7220by OMRON, which was validated
according to the international protocol of the Euro- 2.3. Performance evaluation
pean Society of Hypertension. The procedures for
the measurements were in accordance with the rec- Based on the study conducted by Vasconcelos [46],
ommendations of the VI Brazilian Hypertension the BPR-5 was broken down and reorganized into 3
Guideline [43]. brief tests with questions of the same level of dif-
The mean and maximum HRs were recorded dur- ficulty, with each test being applied on a different
ing the test using the POLAR FT07 heart rate monitor. experimental day, to allow for a comparison of the
The thermal resistance of the students’ garments was performance and comfort parameters of the students
also calculated according to the self-report of the at all temperature ranges at the end of the experi-
students in the questionnaire regarding the clothing ment. A survey was prepared for this purpose on the
items used. The metabolism of the students was set website Qualtrics.com that applied the questions and
at 70 W/m2 because this was a sitting activity with- measured the answering time for each question. Thus,
out a significant physical effort. The maximum HR the number of hits and the time spent to answer the
for age (HRmax = 220 − age) and the mean BP for questions were also measured. Each test consisted of
each individual were calculated after tabulating these 4 questions on each type of reasoning.
data in Excel’s spreadsheet. To divide the questions and prepare each of the
3 tests, the items were categorized according to
2.1.2. Environment Campos [47] into the following: grammatical class
The measurements of environmental variables, and type of relationship between words for the Ver-
including air temperature (ta), wet-bulb temperature bal Reasoning (VR) Test; size of the numerical
(wbt), globe temperature (tg) and air velocity, were sequence, mathematical operations used, number of
performed using the TGD300 heat stress meter and logical sequences used and complexity of the logi-
the microclimatic station BABUC A, which records cal sequences for the Numeric Reasoning (NR) Test;
data by creating a local file and transferring the data types of modification rules and number of types of
to a computer, where in the data may be processed modification rules for the Abstract Reasoning (AR)
through specialized applications or by using spread- Test; number of rotational axes, rotation directions,
sheets. The devices met the demands of the ISO-DIS visible faces of cubes and presence of visual stimuli
7726/1998 [44] and were calibrated by accredited on the edges of the faces of cubes for the Spatial Rea-
institutions of reference. soning (SR) Test; and types of physics knowledge
The meter was installed in the center of the for the Mechanical Reasoning (MR) Test. Similar
classroom, at the height of the torso of the seated items were distributed randomly between the tests
participants, according to the recommendations of based on this classification and categorization. The
the ISO Standard 7726 [44]. For each day and tests were randomly distributed to ensure that each
classroom, these measurements were calculated indi- student would answer one test each day and would
vidually using the mean of the data recorded during answer all 3 by the end of the experimental period.
the testing period. The devices were programmed to The BPR-5 is divided into 2 parts, A and B; the
record measurements every minute, beginning from first part is intended for elementary school students,
the entry of the participants into the classroom. The and the second part is intended for students attending
devices were placed in the classroom 30 minutes secondary school or higher [28].
before data collection for stabilization.
2.4. Statistical analysis
2.2. Subjective parameters
After data collection, the data were tabulated in
Subjective parameters, including thermal sensa- Excel’s spreadsheet and prepared for use in SPSS
tion and thermal preference of the respondents, software. The Excel’s spreadsheet was used to tab-
were determined by administering a questionnaire on ulate, transform and calculate the data. The SPSS
perception, evaluation and thermal preference of stu- was adopted to correlation analyses between the
dents; this questionnaire was based on the 7-point variables. Descriptive statistics were initially per-
48 J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR

Table 2 Table 3
Student characteristics Indoor thermal data of the study environment
Variable Values Day Humidity Air Globe Wet Bulb Air
Age (mean) 21 ± 2.89 years temperature temperature temperature velocity
Weight (mean) 72.88 ± 14.75kg 1 49.33 22.60 23.11 17.41 0.17
Height (mean) 1.76 ± 0.09 m 2 48.65 23.24 23.76 18.14 0.17
BMI (mean) 23.32 ± 3.55 3 50.44 27.95 27.50 25.71 0.05
Tobacco smoker (rate) 4%
Alcohol drinker (rate) 71.4%
Sedentary lifestyle (rate) 14.3% Table 4
Mean PMV and PPD for each experimental day
DIA PMV PPD
formed to assess the characteristics of the sample Mean SD Mean SD
data, including the central tendencies, dispersion and 1 0.3074 0.5994 14.1489 12.1986
distribution of collected variables. Histograms were 2 0.3424 0.4121 10.8768 7.3270
designed in this step to assess the distribution of the 3 1.0806 0.2019 30.2877 9.1285
blood pressure, heart rate and performance parameter.
When the data were not continuous, the Kruskal-
air conditioner settings. The air temperature and black
Wallis and Wilcoxon nonparametric tests were
globe temperature were similar to one another. This
subsequently used to test the hypothesis that the dis-
similarity shows that thermal radiation was not sig-
tributions of the parameters analyzed were similar in
nificant in these environments. Air velocity was also
all experimental days.
lower on day 3, when the room was under natural
ventilation, than on days 1 and 2.
Additionally, Table 4 shows the data regarding the
3. Results predicted mean vote (PMV) of the environment and
the percentage of people dissatisfied with the envi-
3.1. Student characteristics ronment (PPD).
The indoor environments on days 1 and 2 were
A total of 23 men and 5 women who were similar according to the PMV, with the mean values
undergraduate students in the field of exact sciences within the interval considered thermally acceptable;
participated in this study, including 13 students who however, this value was higher than 0.5 on day 3,
studied in a private university and 15 students who indicating that it was outside the acceptability inter-
studied in a public university. Table 2 outlines the val. In addition, the PPD was also much higher on
characteristics of these participants. the last experimental day when the average tempera-
In addition to biometric data, it is important to ana- ture was 27.95◦ C. The indoor environment that was
lyze the environment to which these individuals are studied is a category B environment according to ISO
subjected to understand the changes that occur in their 7730/2005; that is, it is considered thermally accept-
bodies. able with a PMV ranging from –0.5 to 0.5, and a
PPD lower than 10% of the participants is expected.
3.2. Environmental characteristics The analysis of Table 4 shows that the PMV val-
ues on the 3rd day were greater than the acceptable
On the first day, the temperature was adjusted to limits. Although days 1 and 2 showed PMV values
20◦ C, and the measured temperature ranged from within acceptable limits, they had PPD values above
20.3◦ C to 23.35◦ C. On the second day, air con- the admissible level of 10%; however, it should be
ditioner was adjusted to 24◦ C, but it fluctuated noted that the standard deviation was also high.
between 21.88◦ C and 23.45◦ C. On the last day, the In addition to the predicted values of people dissat-
air-conditioner was turned off and the range of fluc- isfied with the environment as assessed by the PPD,
tuation was between 21.96◦ C and 29.15◦ C. Table 3 the actual votes of these students regarding thermal
shows the average indoor environmental conditions comfort were also assessed. The analysis of Table 5
of the laboratories during the data collection process. shows that the percentage of people comfortable with
The analysis of Table 3 shows that the humid- the environment was much lower on the 3rd day and
ity was consistent during the 3 experimental days, was similar on days 1 and 2 of the experiment, which
whereas the air temperature varied according to the corroborated the expected values (in Table 4).
J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR 49

Table 5 reduced satisfaction with the environment from 60%


Percentage of comfort and discomfort votes of the students on to 4%.
each experimental day (ISO 10551/95)
In addition to thermal comfort, temperature gen-
Day Comfortable Slightly Uncomfortable Very
erates a thermal load that requires adaptations of
uncomfortable uncomfortable
the thermoregulatory system. Therefore, the relation-
1 50% 39% 7% 4%
2 53% 39% 4% 4% ship between thermal comfort and blood pressure and
3 11% 50% 32% 7% heart rate must be assessed.

Table 6 3.3. Cardiovascular characteristics


Thermal sensation votes of the participants on each experimental
day (ISO 10551/95) The cardiovascular system of the participants was
Day Hot Warm Slightly Indifference Slightly Cool Cold evaluated on each experimental day to analyze the
warm cool relationship between thermal conditions and body
1 0% 0% 7% 57% 29% 7% 0% responses. The mean values of SBP, DBP and HR
2 0% 4% 7% 60% 25% 4% 0%
3 11% 67% 18% 4% 0% 0% 0%
measured on the 3 experimental days are shown in
Table 7. In addition, the value of MBP calculated
according Equation 2 was also presented.
Furthermore the predicted data (Table 4), Table 6
shows that most participants voted that the environ-
[(2 × DBP) + SBP]
ment was indifferent, that is, neither hot nor cold (i.e. MBP = (2)
comfortable), on days 1 and 2, and the complaint of 3
discomfort was more associated with the cold. Con- The analysis of the results in Table 7 shows that the
versely, 86% voted for some level of heat on the 3rd initial values of days 1 and 2 were similar. However,
day. That is, the discomfort on day 3 was associated they differed from those on the 3rd day, during which
with heat, when temperature was over 29 degrees. HR was increasing and BP values were decreasing.
The average air temperature of 27.95◦ C on the The final values varied between the 3 experimental
3rd day (Table 3) was less thermally accepted days, albeit maintaining the initial trend of BP val-
(Table 5); it is interesting to note how far this temper- ues decreasing and HR values increasing over the
ature was from the optimal temperature. Therefore, course of the experiment. In regards to the variation,
a linear regression model was built based on the the highest absolute values were observed for SBP,
thermal sensation vote of the students (dependent that is, in BP during contraction of the heart, par-
variable) and the air temperatures to which they ticularly on the 2nd day, when the students reported
were submitted (independent variable), enabling a greater acceptability of the thermal environment.
the calculation of the neutral temperature for this These variation values are negative, indicating that
sample. the initial values were higher than the values follow-
This procedure was performed in rising the vari- ing the cognitive activity.
able thermal sensation (TS) as a function of air In addition, the MBP was normal, according to
temperature (Ta). The model (1) showed reliability: the Brazilian Society of Cardiology (below 130 × 80
F: 121,3; p = 2.2–16 < 0.05, R∼ =0.8, R2 ∼
=0.6, adjusted mmHg). Some students showed values of SBP and

R =0.6. The model is expressed in Equation 1.
2
DBP that were indicative of stage-1 hypertension.
TS = −7.937 + 0.341ta. (1) However, these students were not excluded because
a one-time measurement cannot be used to diagnose
The term TS was set to 0 (value equivalent to high BP.
the thermal sensation of comfort) to identify the air The thermal conditions of the environment (TCE)
temperature that was associated with neutral ther- are factors that may affect cardiovascular parame-
mal sensation, and the temperature considered neutral ters. The Kruskal-Wallis test was performed with
by the students was thus obtained, with a value of ␣ = 0.05 to assess the effect of the thermal condi-
approximately 23.3◦ C. tions of the environment on each experimental day
This result indicates that the average temperature and whether the distributions of the values of cardio-
of 27.95◦ C on the 3rd day was 4.65◦ C greater than vascular parameters were similar over the 3 days. The
the temperature considered neutral. This increase results are shown in Table 8.
50 J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR

Table 7
Mean values of the cardiovascular parameters measured
Day 1 Day 2 Day 3
Start End Variation Start End Variation Start End Variation
SBP 127.46 124.36 –3.11 127.45 120.96 –6.48 121.16 120.18 –0.98
DBP 75.04 74.05 –0.98 73.89 72.34 –1.55 72.27 71.39 –0.87
MBP 92.51 90.82 –1.69 91.74 88.55 –3.20 88.57 87.65 –0.91
HR 76.14 73.27 –2.87 76.30 75.91 –0.39 81.04 81.59 0.55

Table 8 Table 10
P-values of the Kruskal-Wallis test Mean measured values of maximum and mean HR
p-value Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 p-value
Start End Variation HR mean 80.86 81.79 87.36 0.04
SBP 0.118 0.458 0.140 HRmax 105.86 104.25 103.39 0.880
DBP 0.230 0.483 0.892
MBP 0.368 0.565 0.565
HR 0.162 0.007 0.052
Table 11
Values obtained in the distribution-free confidence intervals test
for the mean HR
Table 9
Distribution-free confidence intervals for the comparison of Days p-value Maximum Minimum Estimates
final HR differences differences differences
1 and 2 0.5934
Days p-value Maximum Minimum Expected 1 and 3 0.01835 –1.999948 –11.499993 –7.327381
differences differences difference 2 and 3 7.006e–05 –4.000049 –8.500020 –6.499983
1 and 2 0.0634
1 and 3 0.00132 –4.250031 –13.249964 –8.250048
2 and 3 9.318e–5 –3.250044 –7.999986 –5.500029
were performing the activities. The non-parametric
Kruskal-Wallis H Test for independent samples was
The analysis of these results showed that the dis- applied again (␣ = 0.05) to determine whether the dis-
tribution of the (systolic, diastolic and mean) BP tributions of HR mean and HRmax were the same
levels of the participants was similar between the over the 3 days. Data regarding the mean and maxi-
3 experimental days (p-value >0.05). No significant mum HR are shown in Table 10.
difference in HR occurred initially, although the vari- These results show that the maximum HR was sim-
ation of HR before and after the activity was close to ilar between the 3 experimental days, whereas the
the level of significance, which should be considered mean HR showed a significant difference between the
in future studies. Conversely, the HR of the students 3 days. Thus, distribution-free confidence intervals
at the end of the cognitive activity was the only were estimated based on the Wilcoxon signed-rank
value with a significant difference between the days test to confirm the identified difference (Table 11).
[p-value = 0.007 (<0.05)]. Thus, as observed for the final HR, the results out-
Thus, it was important to determine which day dif- lined in Table 11 indicate that the mean HR was
fered in terms of the HR of the students. Therefore, significantly higher on the 3rd day than on days 1
distribution-free confidence intervals were estimated and 2. This finding suggests that both the final and
based on the Wilcoxon signed-rank test to identify mean HR of individuals increase throughout a period
any differences between days. The results are shown of exposure to high temperatures.
in Table 9.
These results show that no significant difference 3.4. Performance
occurred between the values for days 1 and 2. How-
ever, the values for day 3 were significantly higher A total of 84 questionnaires were analyzed (28 for
(p-value >0.05) than those of days 1 and 2. That is, each of the 3 experimental days); the analysis related
HR increased significantly at the end of the cognitive the thermal perception of the environment (based on
activity at an average air temperature of 27.95◦ C. the ISO standard 10551/1995) and the answers to
HR was also monitored while performing the activ- questions from the BPR-5. This test, as described
ity, allowing us to determine the maximum and above, consisted of 5 subtests, and the overall score
mean HR of the students for each day when they ranged from 0 to 20 hits.
J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR 51

Table 12
Mean number of hits and time spent (in seconds)to perform
the tests
Day 1 2 3
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
BPR 14 3.22 13.54 2.502 14.54 3.26
BPRT 838.2441 181.4175 725.3536 197.8429 659.4104 147.8613

In addition to the number of hits (BPR), the time Table 13


spent in seconds (BPRT) to perform the test was also Distribution-free confidence intervals comparing the total time
spent on each day
analyzed. The means and standard deviations of these
Days p-value Maximum Minimum Expected
data are shown in Table 12.
Differences Difference Difference
Most of the students had scores higher than 10 hits
1 and 2 0.01911 221.345 16.085 110.5775
(50% of the total number of hits) on all days. The 1 and 3 0.0004729 277.010 77.985 179.6625
range of scores was lower on the 2nd day, with an 2 and 3 0.01911 115.86 8.88 60.72
interval ranging from 9 to 18 hits, whereas the scores
on the 1st and 3rd days ranged from 5 to 19 hits.
Furthermore, the average score also differed on each under different air temperatures in learning environ-
day, with 15 hits on the 1st day, 14 hits on the 2nd ments? Studies associating the cognitive performance
day and 16 hits on the 3rd day. However, despite these of undergraduate students separately, either with
visual differences, the result from the Kruskal-Wallis environmental conditions or with the cardiovascu-
test for independent samples (␣ = 0.05) showed that lar system, were identified, but there were not found
the distribution of hits was similar over the 3 days, articles that evaluate these three variables together.
with a p = 0.237 > 0.05. The most studies that relate performance and thermal
To corroborate the descriptive analyses, the comfort are carry out in chamber or offices. The main
Kruskal-Wallis test (␣ = 0.05) was performed to contribution of this current paper is that the experi-
determine whether the distribution of time spent to ment was carried out in the usual class rooms, which
answer the questions was similar in the 3 experimen- the students were already adapted.
tal days. The results from this test showed that the
distribution of time spent was significantly different, 4.1. Comfort and thermal sensation
with a p-value = 0.001461 < 0.05.
The identification of this difference made it nec- The rate of students who reported dissatisfaction
essary to estimate the distribution-free confidence with the environment on the 3rd experimental day,
intervals to determine which days differed. The during which the mean air temperature was approx-
results for the total time spent for each day are out- imately 27.95◦ C, was greater than the 10% that is
lined in Table 13. It shows a significant decrease in the considered acceptable for this type of environment,
time spent throughout the experimental period; the according to the ISO 7730/2005 [48]. Furthermore,
students took longer to answer the questions on the the students showed a reduced acceptability of that
1st day, whereas the cognitive performance test was temperature.
completed more quickly on the 3rd day. Students Based on the above results, it is important to deter-
reported a warm or hot thermal sensation on the days mine how much theme a temperature of 27.95◦ C
when the time spent was the shortest. However, the differs from the optimal temperature that is consid-
test took longer when the students reported a thermal ered acceptable. In addition, the possible implications
sensation between slightly cold or cold. should be considered of the thermal discomfort of
students that causes an overload to the body when
maintaining the internal temperature.
4. Discussion The air temperature has implications for phys-
iological responses because the thermoregulatory
This study aimed to fill the gaps in the litera- system must adapt to the environment [49, 50]. Thus,
ture, identified in the literature review section, and there was also a need to examine the relationship
answer the question: could blood pressure and heart between thermal comfort and cardiovascular param-
rate influence the cognitive performance of students eters, including BP and HR.
52 J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR

According to the thermal sensation votes, the tem- Compared to the predicted value for age, theme a
perature considered neutral by the sample of students value for HRmax was 53% of the predicted HRmax.
was 23.3◦ C; this study showed that adjusting the tem- This value is very far from posing a risk to the
perature to the optimum value reduced the error rate individual. However, HRmax reached 92% in one
and improved performance. observation, surpassing the safety zone of 85 to 90%
The main practical objective of this study was to (anaerobic threshold zone), according to Lumingu
provide an understanding of the implications of dif- and Dessureault [54].
ferent temperatures on cognitive and cardiovascular This study showed that temperatures common in
responses of a body of undergraduate students in several Brazilian regions are considered uncomfort-
intelligent learning environments. Currently, there is able by students and can affect HR. This finding
no standardization specific to this type of environ- reveals a little explored subject: the risk of increased
ment and population in Brazil; therefore, there is no HR.
optimization of the teaching-learning process. Awareness campaigns usually focus on the risks
and consequences of increased BP. However, there is
4.2. Blood pressure and heart rate little research on the risks of increased HR. Thus,
these effects must be further studied because this
The final and mean HR were significantly higher study demonstrated that keeping students in environ-
on the 3rd experimental day because this day was the ments without proper air conditioning might increase
hottest day. The thermal satisfaction of students on the the risk of cardiovascular diseases secondary to
same day consisted of approximately 89% of thermal tachycardia.
discomfort votes, 96% of which were associated with
some degree of heat. These findings corroborate the 4.3. Cognitive performance
hypothesis that high temperatures, which cause ther-
mal discomfort, also cause increased HR throughout Temperature affects both the thermoregulatory
the exposure period of the subject [51]. system and the academic performance of students,
BP values lower than 120 × 80 mmHg are con- according to Schellen et al. [55]. Thus, the BPR-5
sidered optimal according to the Brazilian Society of was used to measure the cognitive performance of
Cardiology [43]. Thus, the optimal MBP is lower than students because of its validity and accuracy [56].
93 mmHg. Conversely, values lower than 90 × 60 There was a reduction in time spent to perform
mmHg are considered hypotensive. Therefore, the the battery of tests in the BPR-5 throughout the
MBP should be higher than 70 mmHg. experimental period. Therefore, the time spent was
The predicted HRmax of this sample ranged from the longest in the hot test temperature period. Con-
190 to 203bpm, depending on age. The rate of 50 to versely, the longest time spent occurred on days when
75% of predicted HRmax is reached in moderately the air temperature and thermal sensation were the
intense physical activities, as established in guide- lowest.
lines and handbooks of the American College of This phenomenon may have occurred because the
Sports Medicine [52]. The HR should not exceed reduction in total time spent may have been associ-
50% of the predicted rate during cognitive activity. ated with the learning effect of questions in general.
That is, the HRmax of this sample should be lower However, given the high level of discomfort (89%) of
than 85 to 101.5bpm, depending on age. However, the the participants on the 3rd experimental day, it is pos-
HRmax should be higher than 60bpm because values sible that behavioral acclimation contributed to the
below this limit may hinder proper body circulation reduction in time spent; that is, there was a conscious
(bradycardia) and oxygenation of the CNS and nega- adjustment to situations of thermal discomfort [21,
tively affect performance [53]. Thus, the HR should 57]. Thus, the students reduced the execution time of
be above this limit. activities with the goal of leaving the uncomfortable
The relationship between cognitive performance environment faster.
and HRmax that was obtained during the tests When thermal variation causes discomfort to stu-
showed that performance improves with a decrease dents, thermal variation may render students unable
in HRmax. However, the lowest HRmax reached in to adequately develop cognitive skills, thus hindering
the sample was 77bpm (which exceeded the limit concentration and learning. Although, it is important
of 60bpm), resulting in a considerable range that to remember that to maintain proper cognitive per-
may be used to assess the effects on performance. formance even in uncomfortable environments, the
J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR 53

execution speed is more commitment than the accu- subjects who participated in the experiments and the
racy of the activity performed [58, 59]. This can be the members of CESET research group.
reason of the differences between the running times
but not in the number of hits each subtest.
Studies must reach a better understanding of the Conflict of interest
role of cardiac variables in cognitive performance,
The authors have no conflict of interest to report.
particularly through monitoring the same parameters
that were adopted in this study but with more thor-
ough cardiac tests (including ECG and ambulatory
BP monitoring). References
Furthermore, studies that use samples without age
[1] Fang L, Wyon D, Clausen G, et al. Impact of indoor air
limits should be conducted to determine whether age,
temperature and humidity in an office on perceived air qual-
together with cardiovascular and thermal variables, ity, SBS symptoms and performance. Indoor Air 2004;14:
may affect cognitive performance. 74-81.
[2] Lan L, Wargocki P, Wyon DP, et al. Effects of thermal dis-
comfort in an office on perceived air quality, SBS symptoms,
physiological responses, and human performance. Indoor
5. Conclusion Air 2011;21:376-90.
[3] Spector JM. Conceptualizing the emerging field of smart
The increase in air temperature occurring in Brazil learning environments. Smart Learn Environ 2014;1:2.
as a result of climate change should be a focus [4] Almeida RMSF, Freitas VP de. Indoor environmental qual-
of research. In addition to that increase, the com- ity of classrooms in Southern European climate. Energy
Build 2014;81:127-40.
puter laboratories in which this study was conducted
[5] Primi R, Couto G, Almeida LS, et al. Intelligence, age and
contain audiovisual resources, wireless technologies, schooling: Data from the Battery of Reasoning Tests (BRT-
computers and air conditioning system, resulting in 5). Psicol Reflexão e Crı́tica 2012;25:79-88.
an environment with various heat sources as well [6] Thorsen C, Gustafsson J-E, Cliffordson C. The influence
of fluid and crystallized intelligence on the development of
as heat derived from the students themselves. The knowledge and skills. Br J Educ Psychol 2014;84:556-70.
sum of the above factors, the data collection per- [7] Ritter VM. Avaliação das condições de conforto térmico,
formed in these laboratories, and the development of lumı́nico e acústico no ambiente escolar, no perı́odo de
a generalized linear mathematical model of cognitive inverno. Universidade Federal de Pelotas, 2014.
[8] Bernardi N. Environmental comfort in school buildings: A
performance as a function of thermal and cardio- case study of awareness and participation of users. Environ
vascular variables enabled the following conclusion: Behav 2006;38:155-72.
student performance will likely increase while per- [9] Heath GA, Mendell MJ. Do indoor environments in schools
forming activities in a learning environment with an influence student performance? a review of the literature.
Ernest Orlando Lawrence Berkeley Natl Lab 2002;7.
air temperature of approximately 23.3◦ C (according [10] Da Silva LB, Coutinho AS, Da Costa Eulálio EJ, et al.
to their thermal perception), if students have an IMBP School furniture and work surface lighting impacts on the
of 93.33 mmHg (120 × 80 mmHgBP) and a 60bpm body posture of Paraı́ba’s public school students. Work
2012;42:579-87.
HRmax, which are values considered normal by the
[11] Gilavand A. The impact of educational furniture of schools
Brazilian Society of Cardiology (SBC, 2010). on learning and academic achievement of students at ele-
mentary level. Int J Med Res Heal Sci 2016;5:343-8.
[12] Pawar PY, Khedkar EB. Review of Ergonomics practices in
Acknowledgments difference Sectors. Int Res J Multidiscip Stud 2016;2:1-4.
[13] Hwang R-L, Lin T-P, Kuo N-J. Field experiments on ther-
The National Council of Scientific and Techno- mal comfort in campus classrooms in Taiwan. Energy Build
2006;38:53-62.
logical Development of Brazil (Conselho Nacional [14] Corgnati SP, Filippi M, Viazzo S. Perception of the ther-
de Desenvolvimento Cientı́fico e Tecnológico - mal environment in high school and university classrooms:
CNPQ) financially supported this project (No Subjective preferences and thermal comfort. Build Environ
2007;42:951-9.
404034/2013-6). The authors would like to thank the
[15] Lee MCC, Mui KWW, Wong LTT, et al. Student learn-
following partner institutions: theTechnology Center ing performance and indoor environmental quality (IEQ)
of Maurı́cio de Nassau University and the Institute in air-conditioned university teaching rooms. Build Environ
of Mathematical Sciences and Computing (Instituto 2012;49:238-44.
[16] Yang Z, Becerik-Gerber B, Mino L. A study on student
de Ciências Matemáticas e de Computação - ICMC) perceptions of higher education classrooms: Impact of class-
of Sao Paulo University (Universidade de São Paulo room attributes on student satisfaction and performance.
- USP). The authors would also like to thank the Build Environ 2013;70:171-88.
54 J.C.F. Siqueira et al. / Analysis of air temperature changes on BP and HR

[17] Mendell MJ, Heath GA. Do indoor pollutants and ther- [38] Yao Y, Lian Z, Liu W, et al. Experimental study on phys-
mal conditions in schools influence student performance? A iological responses and thermal comfort under various
critical review of the literature. Indoor Air 2005;15:27-52. ambient temperatures. Physiol Behav 2008;93:310-21.
[18] Ickovics JR, Carroll-Scott A, Peters SM, et al. Health and [39] Yao Y, Lian Z, Liu W, et al. Heart rate variation and elec-
academic achievement: Standardized test scores among troencephalograph - the potential physiological factors for
Urban youth in the United States. J Sch Health 2014; thermal comfort study. Indoor Air 2009;19:93-101.
84:40-8. [40] Akimoto T, Tanabe S, Yanai T, et al. Thermal comfort and
[19] Cui W, Cao G, Park JH, et al. Influence of indoor air productivity - Evaluation of workplace environment in a
temperature on human thermal comfort, motivation and per- task conditioned office. Build Environ 2010;45:45-50.
formance. Build Environ 2013;68:114-22. [41] Taelman J, Vandeput S, Vlemincx E, et al. Instantaneous
[20] Higgins S, Wall K, Mccaughey C. The Impact of School changes in heart rate regulation due to mental load in simu-
Environments: Produced for the Design Council. Des lated office work. Eur J Appl Physiol 2011;111:1497-505.
Counc. [42] Hübler M, Klepper G, Peterson S. Costs of climate change:
[21] Parsons KC. Maintaining health, comfort and productivity The effects of rising temperatures on health and productivity
in heat waves. Glob Health Action 2009;2:1-7. in Germany. Ecol Econ 2008;68:381-93.
[22] Wargocki P, Wyon DP. Providing better thermal and air [43] SBC SB de C, SBH SB de H, SBN SB de N. VI
quality conditions in school classrooms would be cost- diretizes brasileiras de hipertensão arterial. Arq Bras Cardiol
effective. Build Environ 2013;59:581-9. 2010;95:1-51.
[23] Zhang Y, Zhang J, Chen H, et al. Effects of step changes of [44] ISO 7726. Ergonomics of the thermal environment - Instru-
temperature and humidity on human responses of people in ments for measuring physical quantities. 1998.
hot-humid area of China. Build Environ 2014;80:174-83. [45] ISO 10551. Ergonomics of the thermal environment -
[24] Cao B, Ouyang Q, Zhu Y, et al. Development of a multi- Assessment of the influence of the thermal environment
variate regression model for overall satisfaction in public using subjective judgement scales. 1995.
buildings based on field studies in Beijing and Shanghai. [46] Vasconcelos PE de M. Estudo da relação entre variáveis
Build Environ 2012;47:394-9. térmicas e o desempenho de estudantes da academia
[25] Barrett P, Zhang Y, Moffat J, et al. A holistic, multi-level de polı́cia militar do estado da paraı́ba. Dissertação de
analysis identifying the impact of classroom design on Mestrado em Engenharia de Produção da Universidade Fed-
pupils’ learning. Build Environ 2013;59:678-89. eral da Paraı́ba, 2013.
[26] Lan L, Wargocki P, Lian Z. Quantitative measurement of [47] Campos HR. Análise de Conteúdo e sua Relação com a
productivity loss due to thermal discomfort. Energy Build Dificuldade dos Itens da Bateria de Provas de Raciocı́nio
2011;43:1057-62. (BPR-5). Dissertação de Mestrado em Psicologia da Uni-
[27] Almeida LS, Primi R. Perfis de capacidades cognitivas na versidade São Francisco, 2005.
bateria de provas de raciocı́nio (BPR-5): Capacidades cog- [48] ISO7730. Ergonomics of the thermal environment – Ana-
nitivas e BPR-5. Psicol Esc e Educ 2004;8:135-44. lytical determination and interpretation of thermal comfort
[28] Primi R, Almeida LS. Estudo de validação da bateria de using calculation of the PMV and PPD indices and local
provas de raciocı́nio (BPR-5). Psicol Teor e Pesqui; 16. thermal comfort criteria. 2005.
Epub ahead of print August 2000. doi: 10.1590/S0102- [49] Tham KW, Willem HC. Room air temperature affects occu-
37722000000200009 pants’ physiology, perceptions and mental alertness. Build
[29] Burov O, Tsarik O. Educational workload and its psy- Environ 2010;45:40-4.
chophysiological impact on student organism. Work [50] Mishra AK, Ramgopal M. Field studies on human thermal
2012;41(Suppl 1):896-9. comfort: An overview. Build Environ 2013;64:94-106.
[30] Liu W, Lian Z, Liu Y. Heart rate variability at different [51] Siqueira JCF, Neves AIA, Silva LB da, et al. Influence of air
thermal comfort levels. Eur J Appl Physiol 2008;103:361-6. temperature in cardiovascular parameters of undergraduate
[31] Luque-Casado A, Zabala M, Morales E, et al. Cognitive per- students. CESET J 2015;21:1-12.
formance and heart rate variability: The influence of fitness [52] ACSM. American College of Sports Medicine.
level. PLoS One 2013;8:e56935. [53] SOBRAC SB de AC. BRADIARRITMIA.
[32] Wharton W, Hirshman E, Merritt P, et al. Lower blood [54] Lumingu HMM, Dessureault P. Physiological responses to
pressure correlates with poorer performance on visuospa- heat strain: A study on personal monitoring for young work-
tial attention tasks in younger individuals. Biol Psychol ers. J Therm Biol 2009;34:299-305.
2006;73:227-34. [55] Schellen L, Loomans MGLC, Kingma BRM, et al. The use
[33] Lyngdoh T, Viswanathan B, Kobrosly R, et al. Blood pres- of a thermophysiological model in the built environment to
sure and cognitive function: A prospective analysis among predict thermal sensation. Build Environ 2013;59:10-22.
adolescents in Seychelles. J Hypertens 2013;31:1175-82. [56] Rodrigues RM, Celiza JSF, Souza EL. Using bpr- 5 test to
[34] Bernardi L, Wdowczyk-Szulc J, Valenti C, et al. Effects of analyze university students’ cognitive performance. CESET
controlled breathing, mental activity and mental stress with J 2015;21:95-103.
or without verbalization on heart rate variability. J Am Coll [57] Bradshaw V. Human comfort and health requirements. In:
Cardiol 2000;35:1462-9. The Building Environment: Active and Passive Control Sys-
[35] Hjortskov N, Rissén D, Blangsted AK, et al. The effect of tems. New Jersey: John Wiley and sons Inc., 2006, pp. 3-38.
mental stress on heart rate variability and blood pressure [58] Lan L, Wargocki P, Lian Z. Thermal effects on human per-
during computer work. Eur J Appl Physiol 2004;92:84-9. formance in office environment measured by integrating
[36] Vuksanović V, Gal V. Heart rate variability in mental stress task speed and accuracy. Appl Ergon 2014;45:490-5.
aloud. Med Eng Phys 2007;29:344-9. [59] Vimalanathan K, Babu TR. The effect of indoor office envi-
[37] Schnell I, Potchter O, Epstein Y, et al. The effects of expo- ronment on the work performance, health and well-being of
sure to environmental factors on Heart Rate Variability: An office workers. J Environ Heal Sci Eng 2014;12:113.
ecological perspective. Environ Pollut 2013;183:7-13.
Copyright of Work is the property of IOS Press and its content may not be copied or emailed
to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like