You are on page 1of 4

Addictive Behaviors 37 (2012) 524–527

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Addictive Behaviors

Short Communication

Confirmatory factor analysis of the Brazilian version of the Drinking Motives


Questionnaire-Revised (DMQ-R)
Nelson Hauck-Filho a,⁎, Marco Antonio Pereira Teixeira a, M. Lynne Cooper b
a
Graduate Program in Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Porto Alegre, Brazil
b
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Keywords: The main objective of this study was to evaluate the factor structure of the Brazilian version of the Drinking
Drinking motives Motives Questionnaire-Revised (DMQ-R; Cooper, 1994) in a sample of 584 university students. A secondary
Alcohol use goal was to investigate the relationships between motives and measures of alcohol use and drinking problems.
Factor structure The DMQ-R assesses four motive dimensions: social, enhancement, coping, and conformity. Confirmatory factor
Confirmatory factor analysis analysis supported a revised four-factor model identical to the original, with the exception of one item that did
not load on its intended factor. Relative to coping and conformity motives, enhancement and social motives were
strongly related to both alcohol use and drinking problems. Overall results indicate that the factor structure of the
Brazilian DMQ-R parallels that observed in North America and Europe using the original English language
DMQ-R, despite a distinct pattern of relationships with alcohol use and drinking problems.
© 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Heppner, 2003; Mushquash, Stewart, Comeau, & McGrath, 2008), the
four-factor model has consistently shown the best fit to the data
Drinking motives can be understood as personal needs that people (Kuntsche, Knibbe, Gmel, & Engels, 2006; Kuntsche, Stewart, &
attempt to satisfy through alcohol consumption (Ham & Hope, 2003), Cooper, 2008; MacLean & Lecci, 2000; Martens, Rocha, Martin, &
and as efforts to attain desired social or internal psychological goals Serrao, 2008). Nevertheless, the DMQ-R has not been previously exam-
(Cooper, Kuntsche, Levitt, Barber, & Wolf, in press). Despite the exis- ined in Brazil, nor as far as we know in any Latin American country.
tence of many psychometric instruments developed to assess drinking Therefore, the main objective of this study was to determine if a four-
motives and related constructs (for reviews, see Cooper et al., in press; factor structure also fits the Brazilian version of the DMQ-R (Cooper,
and Kuntsche, Knibbe, Gmel, & Engels, 2005), the Drinking Motives 1994) in a sample of university students.
Questionnaire-Revised (DMQ-R; Cooper, 1994) is the most widely The relationships between motives and measures of alcohol use were
used measure (Kuntsche et al., 2005). DMQ-R has 20 items distributed also investigated to determine if patterns similar to those found in North
among four subscales: social, enhancement, coping, and conformity. America and Europe hold. Based on prior research (see Cooper, 1994;
According to Cooper (1994), social motives are positively reinforced Cooper et al., in press; Kuntsche et al., 2005, for reviews), we expect
and externally focused (e.g., drinking “to celebrate a special occasion that motives will predict alcohol consumption and drinking problems,
with a friend”), whereas enhancement motives are positively rein- and that each motive will show a pattern of associations with alcohol
forced, but internally driven or directed (e.g., drinking “because it's use and drinking problems. Specifically, we hypothesize that approach
fun”). On the other hand, conformity motives are negatively reinforce- motives (both enhancement and social) should be strongly related to pat-
ment and externally focused (e.g., drinking “so you won't feel left terns of alcohol use, but less strongly or even unrelated (once use has
out”), whereas coping motives are negatively reinforcement but inter- been controlled) to drinking problems. In contrast, avoidance motives
nally directed (e.g., drinking “to forget your worries”). (both coping and conformity) should be less strongly related to use per
In recent years, the four-factor structure of the DMQ-R has been se, but more strongly predictive of drinking problems, even after control-
investigated in diverse cultural contexts and compared with other plau- ling for levels of use (see Cooper et al., in press, for a detailed discussion).
sible models. With only a few exceptions (see Martens, Cox, Beck, &

2. Method

⁎ Corresponding author at: Institute of Psychology, Federal University of Rio Grande 2.1. Participants
do Sul (UFRGS), Ramiro Barcelos Street, 2600, room 117, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil Zip
Code: 90035-003. Tel.: + 55 51 33085454.
E-mail addresses: hauck_psychology@hotmail.com (N. Hauck-Filho), The initial sample was composed of 674 university students from
mapteixeira@yahoo.com.br (M.A.P. Teixeira), cooperm@missouri.edu (M.L. Cooper). two universities from the state of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil. After

0306-4603/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.11.023
N. Hauck-Filho et al. / Addictive Behaviors 37 (2012) 524–527 525

selecting those with lifetime drinking experience who had complete this item was dropped. After the exclusion, fit indices were acceptable
DMQ-R protocols, 584 participants remained. The mean age was for the 19-item four-factor model, χ² = 612.95, df = 146, TLI= .90,
23.56 years (SD = 6.55; range: 16–60). Females comprised 52.9% of CFI = .91, RMSEA= .07. Table 1 shows the factor loadings, internal con-
the sample. sistencies, means, standard deviations and factor correlations for the
19-item, four-factor model (excluding item 15).
2.2. Instruments DMQ-R subscale scores (based on the 19-item version) were used
to predict scores on alcohol use and drinking problems with multivar-
2.2.1. Drinking motives iate regression analysis (Enter method). Results, presented in Table 2,
The DMQ-R (Cooper, 1994) was translated to Brazilian Portuguese showed that enhancement and social motives were significant predic-
following procedures recommended by Geisinger (1994). Items were tors of all dependent variables. Coping motives significantly predicted
answered on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never or almost never; drinking problems, but not alcohol use per se, whereas conformity
5 = always or almost always). motives negatively predicted frequency of alcohol use, but did not
predict drinking problems. Because social and specially enhancement
2.2.2. Alcohol use intensity motives were strongly related to alcohol use per se, we wanted to de-
Three questions based on the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification termine whether their links to drinking problems were an indirect re-
Test (AUDIT; Babor, La Fuente, Saunders, & Grant, 1992) were used sult of consumption (i.e., individuals experienced more problems
to assess frequency of alcohol consumption, number of drinks con- simply because they drank more) or whether these motives also di-
sumed on a typical occasion, and frequency of heavy drinking epi- rectly predicted drinking problems. In North American and Western
sodes, defined as drinking six or more drinks on a typical occasion. European samples, typically only coping and conformity motives di-
rectly predict drinking problems once use is controlled (see Cooper
2.2.3. Drinking problems et al., in press). Thus a final series of regression models was estimated
Based on several existing instruments (Hurlbut & Sher, 1992; in which motive effects on drinking problems were re-estimated con-
White & Labouvie, 1989), a 24-item measure was developed to assess trolling for quantity and frequency of use. Results showed that social
alcohol-related problems common among university students. Stu- (β = .13, p b .05), enhancement, (β = .14, p b .05) and coping motives
dents were asked to indicate (Yes, No) all possible negative alcohol- (β = .13, p b .05) each uniquely predicted drinking problems. Only
related consequences they had experienced in the past 12 months. conformity motives (β = .06, ns) were unrelated. (Parallel effects
Sample items are “Problems with the law”, “Missing classes”, and were obtained controlling for binge drinking.)
“Having fights.” “Yes” responses were summed to compute a total
score. 4. Discussion

2.3. Procedures Results of the present study indicate that, with the exception of a
single item that failed to load on its intended factor (see Martens et
This research was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Psy- al., 2003, for similar findings), the hypothesized four-factor structure
chology Institute of Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul. The provided a good fit to the data and indeed fits the data better than a
data were collected in groups during class hours, after obtaining in- number of plausible alternative models. All scales showed adequate
formed consent. reliability and discriminant validity vis-à-vis model comparisons
and patterns of unique prediction of important outcomes in multivar-
2.4. Data analysis iate analyses. The correlation patterns among factors also generally
agreed with results from prior research (e.g., Cooper, 1994), with
Confirmatory factor analyses, using maximum likelihood estimation the social and enhancement factors being most strongly correlated,
(AMOS 18.0), were used to test the structure of the Brazilian DMQ-R. and conformity being the least strongly correlated. Furthermore, the
The following indicators were used to evaluate and compare competing relative rates of endorsement – with social motives being most strong-
models: χ² test, Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI; Tucker & Lewis, 1973), Com- ly endorsed followed by enhancement, coping, and conformity in that
parative Fit Index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), and Root Mean Square Error of order – were highly consistent with findings in the literature, includ-
Approximation (RMSEA; Steiger & Lind, 1980). Competing models ing studies conducted in the US, Canada, Switzerland, England, Ice-
were derived from the literature (Cooper, 1994), and included: 1) land, Hungary, and Spain (Cooper et al., in press). Overall, then the
one-factor; 2) two-factor, comparing positive and negative reinforce- results of the present study using the Brazilian DMQ-R largely repli-
ment (enhancement combined with social; coping combined with con- cate findings reported in previous studies.
formity); 3) two-factor, comparing internal vs. external source (social As hypothesized, both social and enhancement motives were signif-
combined with conformity; enhancement combined with coping); 4) icantly positively related to alcohol use (frequency, quantity, and binge
three-factor (coping; conformity; social combined with enhancement); drinking). Consistent with expectation, enhancement motives were
and 5) the hypothesized four-factor model. more strongly related than social motives to quantity and frequency
of drinking, though not to binge drinking. Also consistent with results
3. Results of at least some prior studies (see Cooper et al., in press), coping motives
were unrelated to alcohol use and conformity motives were significant-
Consistent with prior results, the four-factor model fit the data ly negatively related to frequency of use, once the effects of other
significantly better than any of the comparison models, although fit motives were controlled. Thus, results linking motives to alcohol use
indices were slightly lower than usually recommended, χ² = 891.20, were also largely consistent with expectations based on prior studies,
df = 164, TLI = .85, CFI = .87, RMSEA = .09. Inspection of modification with the exception that social motives were more strongly related to
indices suggested that item 15 (“Because you feel more self-confident use than is typically observed.
and sure of yourself,” originally intended to assess coping) was jointly However, contrary to findings typically observed in North America
explained by the coping, social, and conformity factors. Freeing this and Western Europe, enhancement, social and coping motives were
item to load on the aforementioned factors yielded the following equally strongly related to drinking problems once frequency and quan-
factor loadings: .74 (social), .65 (conformity), and .07 (coping). As tity of alcohol use were controlled. This contrasts with the typical
the large loadings were not theoretically expected, and the loading on pattern in which avoidance (coping, conformity), but not approach (en-
the intended (coping) factor was not statistically significant (p = .15), hancement, social) motives are significantly positively related to
526 N. Hauck-Filho et al. / Addictive Behaviors 37 (2012) 524–527

Table 1
Standardized regression weights, Cronbach's alphas, descriptive information for subscales, and factor correlations.

Items Soc Con Cop Enh

14. Porque melhora as festas e as comemorações .88


(Because it improves parties and celebrations)
11. Porque torna eventos sociais mais divertidos .84
(Because it makes social gatherings more fun)
3. Porque ajuda você a aproveitar uma festa .80
(Because it helps you enjoy a party)
5. Para ficar mais sociável (To be sociable) .62
16. Para comemorar uma ocasião especial com amigos .50
(To celebrate a special occasion with friends)
20. Para que você não se sinta deixado de lado .84
(So you won't feel left out)
19. Para que gostem de você (To be liked) .64
12. Para fazer parte de um grupo de que você gosta .59
(To fit in with a group you like)
2. Porque seus amigos fazem pressão para que você beba (Because your friends pressure you to drink) .54
8. Para que os outros não tirem sarro de você por não estar bebendo (So others won't kid you about not drinking) .53
17. Para esquecer seus problemas .91
(To forget your problems)
1. Para esquecer suas preocupações .83
(To forget your worries)
4. Porque ajuda quando você se sente deprimido ou nervoso .64
(Because it helps when you feel depressed or nervous)
6. Para se animar quando você está de mau humor .47
(To cheer up when you are in a bad mood)
7. Porque você gosta de como se sente .81
(Because you like the feeling)
13. Porque dá a você uma sensação agradável .79
(Because it gives you a pleasant feeling)
9. Porque deixa você animado (Because it's exciting) .79
18. Porque é divertido (Because it's fun) .74
10. Para ficar “alto” (To get high) .63
Cronbach's alpha .85 .75 .79 .87
Mean 3.04 1.23 1.81 2.71
SD 1.04 .46 .93 1.11
r with social – .26 .40 .85
r with conformity – .22 .19
r with coping – .43

Note. All factor loadings and correlations with p b .001. Soc = social; Con = conformity; Cop = coping; Enh = enhancement.

drinking problems after controlling for measures of use (see Cooper et validity, and to test the possibility that cultural differences in social
al., in press, for a review). Together these findings point to possible cul- norms for drinking explain these differences.
tural differences in the motivational processes that underlie alcohol use,
especially heavy or problematic drinking. 5. Conclusion
Results suggest that social motives more strongly influence and cop-
ing motives more weakly influence alcohol use among Brazilian college This study was the first to evaluate the factor structure of the DMQ-R
students than among their North American and European counterparts. in a Latin American country. The results provide strong support for the
Consistent with results of recent research showing that social motives hypothesized four-factor structure of the Brazilian version of the DMQ-
more strongly predict measures of use and abuse in social environments R. Finally, results of the present study raise the possibility of intriguing
that strongly support and encourage alcohol use (Lee, Geisner, Lewis, differences in the relative importance of motives underlying alcohol
Neighbors, & Larimer, 2007; Lewis & Neighbors, 2006), the pattern use, and lay the groundwork for future studies aimed at replicating
observed in the present study might be explained by the presence of and understanding the sources of these differences.
unusually strong social norms among Brazilian college students pro-
moting alcohol use. Future cross-cultural studies using the Brazilian Role of funding source
DMQ-R should seek to replicate this pattern of differential predictive This research was supported by an academic scholarship from Coordenação de
Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) to the first author.

Contributors
Table 2 The first two authors developed the study design and implementation. All authors
Motives predicting alcohol use and drinking problems. contributed to preparing the data analyses and writing of the manuscript. All authors
have approved the final version.
Bivariate correlations/betas

Frequency Quantity Binge Drinking problemsa Conflict of interest


All the authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Social .42⁎/.19⁎ .43⁎⁎/.20⁎ 48⁎⁎/.25⁎⁎ .53⁎⁎/.22⁎
Conformity −.01/−.08⁎ .05/−.05 .07/−.03 .13⁎/.04
Coping .26⁎⁎/.08 .24⁎⁎/.02 .29⁎⁎/.08 .39⁎⁎/.16⁎⁎ References
Enhancement .45⁎⁎/.29⁎⁎ .47⁎⁎/.32⁎⁎ .48⁎⁎/.26⁎⁎ .55⁎⁎/.31⁎⁎
R² adj. .22⁎⁎ .23⁎⁎ .26⁎⁎ .35⁎⁎ Babor, T. F., La Fuente, J. R., Saunders, J., & Grant, M. (1992). AUDIT: The alcohol use
disorders identification test: Guidelines for use in primary health care. WHO (World
⁎ p b .05. Health Organization)/PSA, 4, 1–29.
⁎⁎ p b .001. Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin,
a
Drinking problems were assessed only for part of the total sample (n = 427). 107, 238–246.
N. Hauck-Filho et al. / Addictive Behaviors 37 (2012) 524–527 527

Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development Lewis, M. A., & Neighbors, C. (2006). Social norms approaches using descriptive drink-
and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 6(2), 117–128. ing norms education: A review of the research on personalized normative feed-
Cooper, M. L., Kuntsche, E., Levitt, A., Barber, L. L., & Wolf, S. (in press). Motivational back. Journal of American College Health, 54, 213–218.
models of substance use: A review of theory and research on motives for using MacLean, M., & Lecci, L. (2000). A comparison of models of drinking motives in a uni-
alcohol, marijuana and tobacco. versity sample. Psychology of Addictive, 14(1), 83–87.
Geisinger, K. F. (1994). Cross-cultural normative assessment: Translation and adapta- Martens, M., Cox, R., Beck, N., & Heppner, P. (2003). Measuring motivations for intercolle-
tion issues influencing the normative interpretation of assessment instruments. giate athlete alcohol use: A confirmatory factor analysis of the drinking motives mea-
Psychological Assessment, 6(4), 304–312. sure. Psychological Assessment, 15(2), 235–239.
Ham, L., & Hope, D. (2003). College students and problematic drinking: A review of the Martens, M., Rocha, T., Martin, J., & Serrao, H. (2008). Drinking motives and college stu-
literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 23(5), 719–759. dents: Further examination of a four-factor model. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
Hurlbut, S. C., & Sher, K. J. (1992). Assessing alcohol problems in college students. Jour- 55(2), 289–295.
nal of American College Health, 41, 48–57. Mushquash, C., Stewart, S., Comeau, M., & McGrath, P. (2008). The structure of drinking
Kuntsche, E., Knibbe, R., Gmel, G., & Engels, R. (2005). Why do young people drink? A motives in First Nations adolescents in Nova Scotia. American Indian and Alaska
review of drinking motives. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(7), 841–861. Native Mental Health Research, 15(1), 33–52.
Kuntsche, E., Knibbe, R., Gmel, G., & Engels, R. (2006). Replication and validation of the Steiger, J. H., & Lind, J. C. (1980, May). Statistically based tests for the number of com-
Drinking Questionnaire Revised (DMQ-R, Cooper, 1994) among adolescents in mon factors. Paper presented at the annual meeting on the Psychometric Society, Iowa
Switzerland. European Addiction Research, 12, 161–168. City, IA.
Kuntsche, E., Stewart, S., & Cooper, M. L. (2008). How stable is the motive-alcohol use Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor
link? A cross-national validation of the drinking motives questionnaire revised analysis. Psychometrika, 38, 1–10.
among adolescents from Switzerland, Canada, and the United States. Journal of White, H. R., & Labouvie, E. W. (1989). Towards the assessment of adolescent problem
Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69(3), 388–396. drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 50, 30–37.
Lee, C. M., Geisner, I. M., Lewis, M. A., Neighbors, C., & Larimer, M. E. (2007). Social
motives and the injunction between descriptive and injunctive norms in college
student drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 68(5), 714–721.

You might also like