You are on page 1of 24

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/51245608

Are Cultures Becoming Individualistic? A Cross-Temporal Comparison of


Individualism-Collectivism in the United States and Japan

Article in Personality and Social Psychology Review · June 2011


DOI: 10.1177/1088868311411587 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

364 7,732

1 author:

Takeshi Hamamura
Curtin University
85 PUBLICATIONS 2,571 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

cultural change View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Takeshi Hamamura on 08 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Personality and Social Psychology
Review http://psr.sagepub.com/

Are Cultures Becoming Individualistic? A Cross-Temporal Comparison of Individualism−Collectivism in


the United States and Japan
Takeshi Hamamura
Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2012 16: 3 originally published online 23 June 2011
DOI: 10.1177/1088868311411587

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://psr.sagepub.com/content/16/1/3

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Society for Personality and Social Psychology

Additional services and information for Personality and Social Psychology Review can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://psr.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://psr.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - Dec 21, 2011

OnlineFirst Version of Record - Jun 23, 2011

What is This?

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com by guest on October 11, 2013


411587
uraPersonality and Social Psychology Review
PSR16110.1177/1088868311411587Hamam

Personality and Social Psychology Review

Are Cultures Becoming Individualistic? 16(1) 3­–24


© 2012 by the Society for Personality
and Social Psychology, Inc.
A Cross-Temporal Comparison of Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Individualism–Collectivism in the
DOI: 10.1177/1088868311411587
http://pspr.sagepub.com

United States and Japan

Takeshi Hamamura1

Abstract
Individualism–collectivism is one of the best researched dimensions of culture in psychology. One frequently asked but
underexamined question regards its cross-temporal changes: Are cultures becoming individualistic? One influential theory
of cultural change, modernization theory, predicts the rise of individualism as a consequence of economic growth. Findings
from past research are generally consistent with this theory, but there is also a body of evidence suggesting its limitations. To
examine these issues, cross-temporal analyses of individualism–collectivism in the United States and Japan were conducted.
Diverging patterns of cultural changes were found across indices: In both countries, some of the obtained indices showed rising
individualism over the past several decades, supporting the modernization theory. However, other indices showed patterns
that are best understood within the frameworks of a shifting focus of social relationships and a persisting cultural heritage. A
comprehensive theory of cultural change requires considerations of these factors in addition to the modernization effect.

Keywords
culture, individualism–collectivism, modernization, heritage, history, cross-temporal analysis

A pattern of culture can persist for centuries. Nisbett and 1893/1964). In psychological research, this distinction is best
colleagues have suggested that cultural differences in per- known as the dimension of individualism–collectivism.
ception, attention, and reasoning reflect differences in intel- Triandis (1995) defined the dimension as follows:
lectual heritages: Daoism and Confucianism in East Asia and
Greek philosophy in North America (Nisbett, Peng, Choi, Collectivism may be initially defined as a social pattern
& Norenzayan, 2001). However, sometimes a pattern of cul- consisting of closely linked individuals who see them-
ture can transform itself in a matter of a few decades, as seen selves as parts of one or more collectives (family, co-
in post–cold war Eastern European societies. Putnam (2000) workers, tribe, nation); are primarily motivated by the
amassed substantial evidence documenting the decline of norms of, and duties imposed by, those collectives; are
social engagement and civic participation in American society willing to give priority to the goals of these collectives
within the past several decades. This consideration illustrates over their own personal goals; and emphasize their con-
two intuitive but seemingly contradictory aspects of culture: nectedness to members of these collectives. A prelimi-
Culture can persist for centuries, but it can change in a matter nary definition of individualism is a social pattern that
of a few decades. To the extent that culture and mind are mutu- consists of loosely linked individuals who view them-
ally constitutive (Markus & Kitayama, 1991), understanding selves as independent of collectives; are primarily moti-
the ways in which culture changes and continues is important vated by their own preferences, needs, rights, and contracts
for psychological research. The current research takes on this they establish with others; give priority to their personal
challenging question by focusing on one specific aspect of goals over the goals of others; and emphasize rational
culture, the dimension of individualism–collectivism. analysis of the advantages and disadvantages to associ-
ating with others. (p. 2)
Individualism–Collectivism in Psychological Research. Individu-
als differ in their relative independence from and interde-
1
pendence to a social environment. This distinction is one of Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
the most important in social science research and has been
Corresponding Author:
introduced repeatedly in the past, for example, as the dis- Takeshi Hamamura, Chinese University of Hong Kong,
tinctions between gemeinschaft and gesellschaft (Tonnies, Department of Psychology, Shatin, NT, Hong Kong
1887/1957) or mechanical and organic solidarity (Durkheim, Email: hamamura@psy.cuhk.edu.hk
4 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

Individualism and collectivism are “cultural syndromes,” or several studies demonstrated cultural differences in approach
central themes of culture around which various social and and avoidance motivation based on the rationale that the approach
psychological processes are organized (Triandis, 1995). For focus is more beneficial for seeking socially independent goals,
this reason, this dimension affords an examination of a wide whereas the avoidance focus is more beneficial for seeking
range of social psychological processes of significant impor- socially interdependent goals (Elliot, Chirkov, Kim, & Sheldon,
tance (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Also for this reason, 2001; Hamamura, Meijer, Heine, Kamaya, & Hori, 2009; Lee,
individualism–collectivism has received more attention than Aaker, & Gardner, 2000).
other dimensions of culture that past research has introduced. This approach has been productive in uncovering cross-
For example, Geert Hofstede, in his landmark study of IBM cultural variations in many psychological processes, even ones
employees worldwide, introduced three dimensions of cul- that were previously regarded as universal, such as basic pro-
ture aside from individualism–collectivism: uncertainty avoid- cesses of cognition (Nisbett et al., 2001) or the need for posi-
ance, power distance, and masculinity–femininity (Hofstede, tive self-regard (Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama, 1999).
1984). Nevertheless, findings on individualism–collectivism However, one consequence of this approach is that the field
are probably the most frequently cited aspect of this research. is left with a rather impoverished understanding of ways in
In contemporary cross-cultural research, the dimension of which various psychological processes of individualism–
individualism–collectivism has been used most intensively collectivism cohere. As one example of this challenge,
in comparisons of East Asians and North Americans. This Kitayama, Park, Sevincer, Karasawa, and Uskul (2009) mea-
framework has given rise to hundreds of studies on a diverse sured individualism–collectivism with five tasks that are theo-
range of topics, such as self-concept (Markus & Kitayama, retically related (dispositional bias, focused vs. holistic attention,
1991), cognition (Nisbett et al., 2001), emotion (Kitayama, experience of disengaging and engaging emotions, personal
Mesquita, & Karasawa, 2006), subjective well-being (Diener, vs. social happiness, and relative self-size) in four countries
Oishi, & Lucas, 2003), and choice making (Iyengar & (United States, United Kingdom, Germany, and Japan). These
Lepper, 1999), among others (see Kitayama & Cohen, 2007). five tasks converged in finding the expected cross-cultural
These studies have greatly contributed to psychologists’ under- differences: In all tasks, Americans were most individualistic,
standing of the interplay between culture and mind. Japanese were most collectivistic, and British and Germans
With accumulating findings, however, several challeng- were individualistic but to a lesser extent compared to
ing issues have been uncovered. One issue is the appropriate- Americans. At the individual level, however, these five tasks
ness of applying the dimension at country level (e.g., the were uncorrelated with each other.
international ranking obtained by Hofstede) as a schema for Faced with this issue, greater attention has been paid to
cross-cultural variations observed at individual level. In fact, the mechanism by which various psychological processes of
a meta-analysis conducted on this topic identified discrepan- individualism–collectivism are organized. For instance,
cies between the so-called “common view” (i.e., collectivism Kitayama and colleagues (2009) suggested the role of inde-
in East Asia and individualism in North America) and the pendence and interdependence as the unifying theme of a cul-
empirical findings (Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). tural syndrome. Within each culture, according to this theory,
For example, there was no significant difference between various psychological processes are used in an idiosyncratic
Americans and Koreans on the level of collectivism, and fashion to strive toward independence or interdependence. For
Americans were more collectivistic than Japanese. This meta- example, to reach toward the goal of social interdependence,
analysis incited much subsequent research. Several studies some individuals may adopt conformity, some may adopt
have investigated the possibility of methodological artifacts holistic attention, and yet others may adopt self-criticism.
causing the identified divergence from the “common view.” Consequently, one’s inclination toward conformity does not
These studies have found that the measures that control for necessarily predict one’s holistic attention or self-criticism.
these problems, for example, by specifying a reference group Nevertheless, at the collective level, these psychological
in self-report items with the Likert-type scale, do yield a pat- processes are distributed in a systematic manner. That is, at
tern that is consistent with the “common view” (Heine, Lehman, the society level, conformity, holistic attention, and self-
Peng, & Greenholtz, 2002). criticism are more prevalent in collectivistic societies rela-
Another issue facing individualism–collectivism research tive to individualistic societies. In sum, recent research has
is the demand for a more refined conceptualization of the introduced new ways of thinking about ways in which vari-
dimension (Brewer & Chen, 2007). Individualism–collectivism ous psychological processes of individualism and collectivism
research has flourished by treating individualism and collec- cohere.
tivism as cultural syndromes encompassing a collection of
social and psychological processes (Triandis, 1995). From Theories of Cross-Cultural Variation in Individualism–Collectivism.
this perspective, much research has taken the form of theoreti- Why is it that cultures differ on individualism–collectivism?
cally deriving a particular psychological process from the indi- Recent research has started to examine factors (ecological,
vidualism–collectivism framework and then demonstrating economic, or historical) that have produced cross-cultural
cross-cultural variation in that process. To cite just one example, variation in individualism–collectivism. These factors include
Hamamura 5

economic development (Hofstede, 1984), prevalence of GDP, which is a frequently used index of societal moderniza-
pathogens (Fincher, Thornhill, Murray, & Schaller, 2008), tion because of its wide availability and convergence with other
residential mobility (Oishi, 2010), language use (Y. indices of social development (e.g., infant mortality rate,
Kashima & Kashima, 2003), and voluntary migration (Kita- level of education, urbanization), is high (r = .82; Hofstede,
yama, Ishii, Imada, Takemura, & Ramaswamy, 2006). How- 1984). Greenfield (2009) observed a shift from traditional com-
ever, of these factors, economic development has received the munal orientation to a more urban individualistic orientation
most attention. In fact, the idea that individualism is caused by in many societies around the world, including small-scale
economic development has been enormously influential, to societies. In a large cross-cultural study, Inglehart and Baker
the extent that it has penetrated the lay theory of cultural (2000) also found that per capita GDP is correlated with less
change (Y. Kashima et al., 2009). The current research exam- emphasis placed on collectivistic ways of living (e.g., less
ines predictions from this theory, hereinafter called moderniza- emphasis on family and religion) and greater emphasis placed
tion theory, in cross-temporal comparisons of the United States on individualistic ways of living (e.g., the pursuit of happiness
and Japan on the dimension of individualism–collectivism. or an emphasis on self-expression).
The relationship between social development and individu-
Modernization Theory: Individualism as a Consequence of alism is found within a society as well. Modernization trig-
Modernization. Modernization research emerged out of gers a series of changes in social structure, such as changes in
multidisciplinary interests in understanding cross-cultural family structure, advances in the level of education, and urban-
differences in development (Inkeles, 1975). In psychological ization (Inkeles, 1975). Within a society, individuals from a
research, modernization theorists have focused on identify- high socioeconomic status (SES) background tend to have
ing the psychological consequences of societal moderniza- greater exposure to these modern institutions. Hence, the
tion (Inkeles, 1983). Studies conducted in this tradition have modernization theory predicts a positive correlation between
yielded a long list of characteristics. Although the list differs individualism and SES. Studies conducted around the world
among theorists, it is generally composed of such things as have indeed found such a correlation. For example, Inkeles
personal efficacy, gender equality, achievement motivation, (1975) found that individuals’ exposure to modern institutions
independence, greater civic participation, greater social rela- predicts individualism in all countries investigated (Argentina,
tionships beyond kin, distancing from tradition and religion as Bangladesh, Chile, India, Israel, and Nigeria). Among Kenyans,
a source of authority, and acceptance of social mobility (for a Ma and Schoeneman (1997) found that whereas university
review, see Yang, 1988). A brief survey of these lists suggests students tended to describe themselves with personality attri-
that individualism is one construct that encompasses many butes on the Twenty Statements Test, context-dependent
of the identified characteristics. In fact, it has been suggested that descriptions of the self were more common among workers
contemporary individualism–collectivism research emerged out in Nairobi, and even more so among members of traditional
of the tradition of modernity research (Kagitcibasi, 2005; communities, such as Masaai and Samburu. Among Americans,
Yang, 1988). Snibbe and Markus (2005) found that self-expression and the
How does societal modernization foster individualism? pursuit of uniqueness are more important for Americans with
Sachs (2005) illustrated this process with an example of gar- a high SES background relative to a low SES background.
ment factories in urban centers of developing countries, such This research indicates that individualism is more descrip-
as Dhaka, Bangladesh. These factories often provide employ- tive of Americans with high (relative to low) SES backgrounds
ment opportunities that are attractive to many workers, par- (also see Kraus, Piff, & Keltner, 2009; Stephens, Markus, &
ticularly young women from rural areas where there are no Townsend, 2007). A positive correlation between SES and
viable alternatives. This “foothold in the modern economy” individualism has also been reported among Filipinos (Guthrie,
in turn affords these workers an opportunity to manage their 1977), Chinese (Hamamura, Xu, & Du, 2011), and Japanese
own income, own living space, and own life, such as making (Kameda, Takezawa, & Hastie, 2005). In sum, evidence link-
decisions regarding whom to date and marry and when to have ing societal modernization and individualism is robust and is
children, decisions that are traditionally made in accordance found across cultures.
with the interests of kin. This observation suggests that a soci-
ety’s participation in the modern economy triggers changes in Problems With the Modernization Theory. Despite the converg-
social institutions, such as greater urbanization, smaller house- ing evidence, there are a few problems with modernization
hold size, and a lower fertility rate (also see Newson, Postmes, theory. First, the theory does not explain collectivism
Lea, & Webley, 2005). At the psychological level, these social observed in highly developed societies in East Asia. Although
changes not only free individuals from traditional sources of there is a debate regarding the extent to which East Asian soci-
social influence (e.g., kin relations) but also afford means of eties are collectivistic relative to Western societies (Oyserman
autonomous decision making. et al., 2002), findings from much research in cultural psychol-
Empirically, the link between societal modernization and ogy generally converge in suggesting the relatively collectivis-
individualism is well established. A correlation between tic nature of East Asian societies (for a review, see Kitayama
Hofstede’s international index of individualism and per capita & Cohen, 2007). To the extent that there is little difference in
6 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

economic development between these two regions, collectiv- for variance in values, even after controlling for differences
ism in highly developed East Asian societies poses a challenge in GDP. For example, predominantly Protestant countries in
to the modernization theory. Europe were more individualistic relative to other countries
Another problem with the modernization theory is the after controlling for differences in GDP. In contrast, countries
issue of the direction of causality. Modernization theory belonging to the former communist bloc were more collectiv-
argues that economic development causes individualism. istic relative to other countries. In sum, this research suggests
However, the case for the opposite—that particular patterns that a society’s cultural heritage influences its current orienta-
of culture cause economic development—has been made, most tion on the individualism–collectivism dimension, indepen-
prominently by Max Weber (1904/1992). In sum, despite dently of modernization.
available evidence pointing to the robust relationship between
societal modernization and individualism, a few issues con- Review of Prior Cross-Temporal Comparison Studies. Two broad
cerning the modernization theory raise doubts. theories of cultural change were considered. On one hand,
the modernization theory suggests that the rise of individual-
Modernization Without Westernization. An alternative to the ism is one of the consequences of economic growth, and the
modernization theory is to consider the role of a society’s cul- theory predicts such a trend across societies. On the other
tural heritage in shaping the course of its change over time. hand, the cultural heritage theory considers the role of a
Samuel Huntington (1996), in The Clash of Civilizations, society’s cultural heritage in shaping the course of its cross-
rejected modernization as a framework for cross-cultural differ- temporal changes. This theory predicts continuity in a pattern
ences and proposed that a society’s cultural heritage sets a of culture over time. Findings from past cross-temporal research
path for its evolution. Huntington and other critics (e.g., support both of these theories.
Gusfield, 1967; Kagitcibasi, 1994) have argued against the Some studies are consistent with the modernization theory.
conflation of modernization with “Westernization.” To some Inglehart and Baker (2000) found increasing importance of
extent, such a conflation is inevitable, as modernized societ- individualistic values between the 1980s and the 1990s in
ies have predominately been Western societies. Nonetheless, those societies that posted economic growth. This effect was
Huntington (1996) stated, “[T]he West was the West long before independently replicated across societies in Asia and the
it was modern” (p. 69), and argued that modernization of the Pacific with a different set of values (Allen et al., 2007).
West is a product of Western cultural heritage. Among Chinese, Yang (1996) concluded that the personality
Under this framework, hereinafter called cultural heritage profile of Chinese people has shifted toward individual orien-
theory, the process of modernization is not uniform across cul- tation from social orientation over the years. Many commen-
tures. Moreover, individualism, which is seen as a uniquely tators in Japan have noted the rise of individualism over the
Western cultural phenomenon, is not a necessary consequence past several decades (e.g., Yamagishi, 1999). One telling
of modernization. Much has been written on the idea of mul- example of this trend is found in the changing norms of mar-
tiple pathways for societal modernization. Huntington (1996) riage. In the 1950s, the norm of Japanese marriage was to marry
found evidence in Japan. Modernization in Japan, starting in someone arranged by the concerned parties (e.g., parents,
the mid-19th century, thoroughly transformed key political, senior colleagues at work); arranged marriages accounted for
economic, and technological institutions. Traditional cultural more than half of all marriages, whereas love-based mar-
ideas and practices (e.g., Confucianism, filial piety) were seen riages accounted for only about one third. However, in a mat-
as obstacles, and not only did Japanese adopted Western ter of several decades, the norm reversed. In recent years, the
products, institutions, and technologies but also many rul- norm of marriage has become love based, which accounts for
ing elites experimented with Western ideas, values, and ways more than 80% of all marriages, and arranged marriages have
of living (Hirakawa, 1989). Nevertheless, in a matter of a few become rare, accounting for only about 5% of all marriages
decades, the trend swung back and newly introduced Western (National Institute of Population and Social Security Research,
ideas and technologies began to coexist with traditional cul- 2009).
tural practices (Rozman, 1991). Findings from the United States suggest a similar trend.
Empirical support for the multiple pathways of modern- Alwin (1989) noted the rising importance of independence
ization was found in a large cross-cultural survey (Inglehart & and the decline of obedience and conformity among Americans.
Baker, 2000). With data obtained from 65 countries, Inglehart A meta-analysis of conformity found that the conformity effects
and Baker (2000) investigated the relationship among economic among Americans decreased over time (Bond & Smith, 1996).
development, cultural heritage, and values. One of the value Over the years, Americans have become more withdrawn
dimensions investigated was that of survival versus self- from social groups and institutions (Glenn, 1987; Putnam,
expression orientation, which is highly similar to individual- 2000). Roberts and Helson (1997) found a rise in the individu-
ism–collectivism (Inglehart & Oyserman, 2004). This research alistic personality profile over time. Research by Twenge and
found the following. First, consistent with modernization colleagues also found a pattern of rising individualism in
theory, the endorsement of individualistic values was posi- many psychological processes over the past several decades,
tively correlated with GDP. Second, cultural heritage accounted including self-esteem and self-enhancement (Twenge &
Hamamura 7

35,000

30,000
Per capita GDP (inflation adjusted)

25,000

20,000

Japan
15,000 U.S.

10,000

5,000

0
1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
Year

Figure 1. Inflation adjusted per capita G.D.P (from Maddison, 2008)

Campbell, 2001, 2008), narcissism (Twenge, Konrath, Foster, cross-temporal patterns found in the United States do not fit
Campbell, & Bushman, 2008), and a decreasing need for social the pattern of rising individualism, and findings from East
approval (Twenge & Im, 2007). In addition, Twenge, Abebe, Asian societies suggest the persistence of traditional collec-
and Campbell (2010) reported that American parents are tivistic patterns of culture along with rising individualism.
increasingly choosing unique names for their children, suggest-
ing an increasing emphasis placed on uniqueness. In sum, Current Research. The current research investigates the rela-
findings from these studies are consistent with the modern- tionship between economic development and individualism–
ization theory in showing the rise of individualism in those collectivism by cross-temporarily examining two highly
societies that experienced economic growth. developed societies with distinct cultural heritages: the United
However, other findings do not fit this pattern. Among States and Japan. The period under investigation is from 1950
Americans, despite the rise of individualistic psychological to 2008, which is characterized by steady economic develop-
processes, the importance of family life has remained relatively ment for both countries (see Figure 1). In the United States, per
stable (Thornton & Young-DeMarco, 2001), and external locus capita GDP adjusted for inflation increased from $9,561 in
of control has increased (Twenge, Zhang, & Im, 2004). 1950 to $31,049 in 2006. Similarly, the Japanese economy
In addition, some indices of individualism have shown no grew significantly, from $1,921 in 1950 to $22,464 in 2006
cross-temporal change (Trzesniewski & Donnellan, 2010). (Maddison, 2008). The two theories considered above make
Furthermore, several studies conducted in East Asian societies diverging predictions. On one hand, the modernization theory
have reported the coexistence of rising individualism and predicts a rise of individualism as both countries have experi-
traditional patterns of culture. Findings from Japanese social enced substantial economic development. In contrast, the cul-
surveys indicated continuing emphasis on group orientation tural heritage theory predicts continuity in patterns of American
(Caudill, 1973) and traditional values, such as filial piety, obe- and Japanese cultures, even during periods marked by signifi-
dience to authority, and group solidarity (Flanagan, 1979; cant economic growth.
Trommsdorff, 1983). Similar trends were also reported among
Chinese (Yang, 1996).
In sum, the large body of evidence generally supports the Method
modernization theory: Societies that experienced economic Measure of Individualism–Collectivism. Individualistic and col-
growth have generally become more individualistic. However, lectivistic cultural syndromes manifest in a variety of social
there are also findings that do not fit this trend: Some and psychological processes. As such, its measurement should
8 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

also tap into a broad array of social and psychological phe- a society allows pronouns to be dropped (E. Kashima &
nomena. Moreover, to carry out cross-temporal analyses, Kashima, 1998), and the index of pathogen prevalence, or
this research requires indices with data available for decades. the extent to which disease-causing pathogens were prevalent
These indices were obtained from two broad sources: demo- in a society’s historical environment (Fincher et al., 2008;
graphics and archives of surveys. Murray & Schaller, 2010). The linguistic practice of pronoun
Demographics compiled by various government agencies drop indicates collectivism as the practice reduces the sym-
were examined. With an interest in cross-temporal analysis, bolic prominence of an actor and contextualizes the action
the search was limited to those indices that were available (E. Kashima & Kashima, 1998). The pathogen prevalence
for at least a decade. indicates collectivism because collectivistic tendencies, such
Similarly, indices were searched from surveys that were as conformity, reluctance toward interactions with out-groups,
conducted repeatedly for an extended period. Survey data and adherence to social norms, are an effective defense against
were searched from relevant literature (e.g., Putnam, 2000) as disease-causing pathogens (Fincher et al., 2008).
well as online resources that archive these surveys (e.g., the These three indices are highly correlated (rs > .63).
Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research). Nevertheless, they differ in the number and coverage of coun-
Identified surveys included international surveys (the World tries. For this reason, the three resulting correlations were
Values Survey [WVS] and the World Youth Survey [WYS]) averaged, and indices were retained if the average correlation
and national surveys (the General Social Survey [GSS], the exceeded |.30| in the theorized direction (see Table 1). For
American National Election Survey [ANES], the DDB example, the worldwide data on the percentage of population
Lifestyle Survey [DDB], and the National Characteristic living in urban areas (United Nations Statistics Division,
Survey [NCS]; see the appendix also found online at http:// 2009) had an average correlation of .32 with individualism (rs
pspr.sagepub.com/supplemental for details). Items in these with Hofstede = .30, pathogen prevalence = .43, and pronoun
surveys cover a variety of issues, such as religion, government drop = .23), and thus this item was retained. Of the interna-
spending, gender relations, race relations, immigration, and tional surveys, the WYS is administered in only five coun-
confidence in various public institutions. Although none of the tries. To increase the reliability of the validation, for the items
surveys was designed to measure individualism–collectivism, obtained from this source, data from the three most recent
some of the items investigated issues pertaining to this dimen- administrations were obtained.
sion. Prior research guided the identification of these items. For items identified in national surveys, the empirical
Again, because of the interest in cultural changes, this search relationship to individualism–collectivism was examined at
was limited to items with data available for at least a decade. the regional level within each country. For the items identi-
fied in American surveys, data from the three most recent
Validation. Next, the items’ conceptual linkage with administrations were obtained for all states (the ANES and
individualism–collectivism was empirically verified. This step the DDB) or regions (the GSS) and were correlated with the
is important for two reasons. First, many of the indices have not scores of individualism–collectivism for American states that
been used in prior research, and the empirical relationships were compiled from a set of demographic indices (Vandello &
with individualism–collectivism are unknown. Second, Cohen, 1999). Regional scores of individualism–collectivism
research in cultural psychology cautions against interpreting were obtained by averaging the scores for the encompassing
self-report responses on explicit self-beliefs and values, as states. Indices with a correlation greater than |.30| in the theo-
these responses not only are confounded with response style rized direction were obtained (see Table 1). For example, an
differences (e.g., Heine et al., 2002) but also are poorly cor- item from the ANES asked, “Generally speaking, would you
related with other types of measures (Kitayama et al., 2009; say that most people can be trusted, or that you can’t be too
Na et al., 2010). To ensure that the findings from the survey careful in dealing with people?” The percentage of agreement
items extend beyond self-report responses, the items’ linkage with the former option across states correlated at .62 with
with individualism–collectivism was verified against behav- individualism, and thus the item was retained.
ioral indices of individualism–collectivism. For items from the Japanese survey, data from the three most
Items were validated either internationally or domestically. recent administrations were obtained for all regions and were
Those indices where data were available for both countries correlated with the scores of individualism–collectivism
(i.e., demographics and items from the international surveys) (Hitokoto, Murabe, Narita, & Tanaka-Matsumi, 2010). Hitokoto
were validated by international indices of individualism– et al. (2010) developed this index using a method similar to
collectivism using the following procedure. First, for each Vandello and Cohen (1999) to reveal regional variations in
index, the most recent worldwide data were obtained, and individualism–collectivism within Japan. Regional scores of
these data were correlated with three independent indices of individualism–collectivism were obtained by averaging the
individualism–collectivism. The three indices were the index scores for the encompassing prefectures. Items with a correla-
developed by Hofstede (1984), which is based on self-report tion greater than |.30| in the theorized direction were obtained
data, as well as two indices that rely on non-self-report (see Table 1). For example, an item from the NCS asked about
data: the index of pronoun drop, or whether the language of important moral principles, with “respect individual rights”
Hamamura 9

Table 1. Obtained Indices and Their Theoretical and Empirical Relations to Individualism–Collectivism

Empirical relation
Index Source Theoretical relation (r)
International indices
1. Household size Government Family relationship −.73
2. Divorce Government Family relationship .35
3. Percentage of population living in urban areas Government Urbanization .32
4. “Regardless of what the qualities and faults of one’s parents are, one must WVS Family relationship −.65
always love and respect them” (% agreement)
5. “Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted or WVS General trust .49
that you need to be very careful in dealing with people?” (% choosing
the former)
6. % choosing “independence” as an important quality that children can be WVS Socialization .32
encouraged to learn at home
7. “How important is friend?” (% responding important) WVS Importance of .48
friendship
8. “When do you feel that you are living a fulfilling life?” (% choosing “when WYS Social values −.37
I’m involved with something helpful for society”)
9. “Which two factors on this list do you think are most important for WYS Importance of effort −.53
becoming successful in life?” (% choosing “personal effort”)
American indices
10. “Most people are honest.” (% agreement) DDB General trust .61
11. “Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted, or that ANES General trust .62
you can’t be too careful in dealing with people?” (% choosing the former)
12. “Would you say that most of the time people try to be helpful, or that ANES General trust .43
they are mostly just looking out for themselves?” (% choosing the
former)
13. “Do you think most people would try to take advantage of you if they got ANES General trust .63
a chance, or would they try to be fair?” (% choosing the latter)
14. % choosing “to obey” as most important for a child to learn to prepare GSS Socialization −.66
him or her for life
15. % choosing “to think for himself or herself” as most important for a child GSS Socialization .71
to learn to prepare him or her for life
16. “Spend a social evening with relatives?” (% more than once a month) GSS Family relationship −.49
17. “Would you say that most of the time people try to be helpful, or that they GSS General trust .69
are mostly just looking out for themselves?” (% choosing the former)
18. “Do you think most people would try to take advantage of you if they got GSS General trust .76
a chance, or would they try to be fair?” (% choosing the latter)
19. “Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted, or that GSS General trust .75
you can’t be too careful in dealing with people?” (% choosing the former)
Japanese indices
20.“What are important moral principles?” (% choosing “respect individual right”) NCS Social values .36
21. “Suppose A’s parent is dying, but A has an extremely important business NCS Family relationship −.63
meeting. What should A do?” (% choosing “A should go see parent”)
22. “What are important moral principles?” (% choosing “honoring NCS Social values −.74
obligations”)
23. “Do you think that you should do what you feel is right even if that goes NCS Social values −.79
against tradition or that there is no mistake in following tradition?” (%
choosing following tradition)
24. “Between someone who emphasizes “being logical” and someone who NCS Social values −.33
emphasizes “being harmonious,” which type do you prefer?” (% choosing
“harmonious”)
25. “Do you think people would try to take advantage of you if they got a NCS General trust .38
chance, or not?” (% choosing the latter)
26. “What is most important to you?” (% mentioning “society/nation”) NCS Social values −.35

WVS = World Values Survey; WYS = World Youth Survey; DDB = DDB Lifestyle Survey; ANES = American National Election Survey; GSS = General Social
Survey; NCS = National Characteristic Survey. Correlations are between responses and the index of individualism–collectivism used for validation. A
positive correlation indicates individualism, whereas a negative correlation indicates collectivism.
10 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

being one of the options. The percentage of participants beyond in-group members, whereas in individualistic societ-
choosing this option across regions in Japan correlated at .36 ies social relationships tend to be more expansive and trust
with individualism and thus the item was retained. is not confined to in-groups: in fact, general trust of others
Two issues need to be noted concerning the item validation. serves as a lubricant for expansive social interactions (Yamagishi
First, the validation resulted in many indices that were theo- & Yamagishi, 1994).
retically relevant to individualism–collectivism not being Finally, one survey item assessed the importance of per-
retained for lack of empirical relationships (e.g., the theoretical sonal effort for success. Research suggests that effort is val-
relationship was not verified) or for lack of sufficient data for ued more in collectivistic societies than in individualistic
validation (e.g., regional data were not available). Second, societies, as it is seen as a key for goals valued in these societ-
because all of the benchmarks of individualism–collectivism ies, such as self-improvement and social harmony (Heine et al.,
used for validation operationalize the single-dimension struc- 1999). For instance, a study asking participants about the
ture, the obtained indices were also analyzed on the single relative contribution of effort and the innate ability for intelli-
dimension with individualism and collectivism being two gence found that whereas Japanese participants, on average,
opposing ends, which is appropriate for the analysis of a culture- attributed 55% of intelligence to effort, that proportion was
level trend (Triandis, Bontempo, Villareal, Asai, & Lucca, only 36% among European American participants (Heine
1988; Vandello & Cohen, 1999). et al., 2001). This linkage between the importance of effort
and collectivism was confirmed in the validation that collec-
Indices of Individualism–Collectivism. A total of 26 indices met tivistic societies placed relatively greater importance on effort
the above inclusion criteria (see Table 1). These indices fell than individualistic societies did (r = –.53).
into one of the following themes. Some items assessed the
strength of family relationships. Research indicates that
family relationships tend to be stronger and closer in collec- Results
tivistic societies (Triandis, 1995). Sample items in this cate- Data Analysis. All of the indices except average household size
gory are demographic, such as household size and divorce and the divorce rate were obtained via percentage (see Table
rates, and survey items on family values (e.g., unconditional 1). Historical changes in the obtained indices were analyzed in
love toward parents) and frequency of social interactions with three ways: ecological correlation, annual change, and scatter
family members. plot. First, for each index, a correlation with year was com-
One survey item assessed the importance of friendship. puted (see Table 2). This correlation captured a linear trend of
Research suggests that individualistic societies place greater each index over time. The direction of these correlations was
value on friendship than collectivistic societies do: For example, standardized based on the validated relationship with the
individualists have more positive attitudes toward friends dimension: A positive correlation was set to indicate an
than collectivists, whose attitudes toward friends tend to be more increase of individualism, whereas a negative correlation was
ambivalent (Adams & Plaut, 2003). set to indicate an increase of collectivism. These were ecologi-
A demographic on urbanization tracks the proportion of cal correlations (correlations between group-level values;
population living in urban areas. Research suggests that urban- Robinson, 1950), which are appropriate for studying culture-
ization is characterized by social relations that are detached level phenomenon, but they could not be used to infer individ-
from traditional collectivistic social relations, and it is regarded ual-level phenomena. The sample size of these correlations
as one of the primary societal forces behind individualism corresponded to the number of years in which a particular item
(Y. Kashima et al., 2004). was administered; thus, they were small, especially for the
A number of survey items assessed social values. A few survey indices, despite the fact that the items were adminis-
of them assessed values in child socialization: independence tered to thousands of participants over the years. For this rea-
and obedience. Research suggests that child socialization son, analyses of the correlations focused on their direction and
focuses on nurturing independence in individualistic societ- magnitude and not on significance tests.
ies and obedience in collectivistic societies (Triandis, 1995). There is no established convention for interpreting the size
Other items assessed values such as social obligation, social of ecological correlations, partly because ecological correla-
harmony, tradition following, contribution to a collectivistic tions are known to produce a larger magnitude compared to
goal, and the importance of society or nation: All of these are individual-level correlations (Robinson, 1950). Furthermore,
characteristic of collectivism (Triandis, 1995). In addition, correlations that are large in magnitude could be obtained from
one item assessed the importance of individual right, which changes that are relatively small in absolute magnitude (i.e., a
is associated with individualism (Triandis, 1995). linear trend for a small change). For these reasons, analyses
A number of survey items assessed general trust of others. of absolute magnitude accompanied the correlations. For each
Research finds that individualists are more trusting of general index, the difference between the two earliest and the two
others (vs. intimate others) than collectivists are. Specifically, latest values was obtained, and the difference was divided by
in collectivistic societies, where important social relations the number of years the index was available (see Table 2).
tend to concentrate within in-groups, trust does not generalize For example, an item from the GSS assessed the importance
Hamamura 11

Table 2. Patterns of Cultural Change: Ecological Correlations and Annual Changes


Ecological
Item Source correlation (r) Annual change (%)
International indices
1. Household size Government USA: .96
Japan: .94
2. Divorce Government USA: .52
Japan: .90
3. Urban population Government USA: .96 USA: .26
Japan: .97 Japan: .52
4. Love and respect for parents WVS USA: –.78 USA: .21
Japan: –.22 Japan: –.05
5. Most people can be trusted WVS USA: –.35 USA: –.33
Japan: –.30 Japan: –.02
6. Independence important for child socialization WVS USA: .77 USA: .64
Japan: .96 Japan: 1.07
7. Friend important WVS USA: .43 USA: .03
Japan: .47 Japan: .04
8. Social contribution important for fulfilling life WYS USA: .89 USA: –.80
Japan: –.71 Japan: .45
9. Effort important for successful life WYS USA: –.65 USA: .16
Japan: –.76 Japan: .20
American indices
10. Most people are honest. DDB −.88 −.60
11. Most people can be trusted ANES −.42 −.01
12. Most people try to be helpful ANES .81 .35
13. Most people would try to be fair ANES .16 .14
14. Obedience important for child socialization GSS .73 −.24
15. Thinking for oneself important for child socialization GSS −.86 −.31
16. Socialize with relatives at least once a month GSS −.13 .01
17. Most people try to be helpful GSS −.35 −.06
18. Most people would try to be fair GSS −.82 −.23
19. Most people can be trusted GSS −.80 −.39
Japanese indices
20. Respecting individual right is an important moral principle NCS −.81 −.26
21. Family life more important than business engagement NCS .36 −.11
22. Honoring obligation is an important moral principle NCS −.55 .16
23. Following tradition is important NCS .19 −.12
24. Social harmony is important NCS −.39 .03
25. Most people would not take advantage of you even if they got a chance NCS .71 .20
26. Most important: society/nation NCS −.77 −.06

WVS = World Values Survey; WYS = World Youth Survey; DDB = DDB Lifestyle Survey; ANES = American National Election Survey; GSS = General Social
Survey; NCS = National Characteristic Survey. Numbers next to the items correspond to numbers in Table 1. For change, a positive correlation indicates
the rise of individualism, whereas a negative correlation indicates the rise of collectivism.

of obedience in child socialization. In 1986, 23.4% of the Indices with correlations below |.10| or annual rates less
participants thought obedience was important, and in 1987, than 0.1% (or changes that are less than 1% per decade) were
19.9% of the respondents had the same response. In contrast, operationalized as not showing changes. This conservative
in 2006 and 2008 only 17.0% and 15.7% of the participants criterion was selected to conduct analyses that are sensitive
thought obedience was important, respectively. This index to cultural changes. In addition, all of the indices were plotted
had an average annual change rate of 0.24% (or a 5.3% differ- against year and visually inspected for nonlinear trends.
ence over a 22-year period). Given the average population of
more than 110 million in Japan and 230 million in the United Changes in the United States. In the United States, average
States, for the past several decades, a 1% change roughly cor- household size decreased over time (r = .96), whereas the
responds to annual changes in more than 1.1 million Japanese proportion of people living in urban areas (r = .96) and the
and 2.3 million Americans. divorce rate (r = .52) increased over time (see Table 2,
12 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

Household: US (1)
Household: JP (1)
3 Divorce: US (2)
Divorce: JP (2)
Urban: US (3)
2 Urban: JP (3)

0
1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
Year

Figure 2. Changes in divorce rate, household size, and urban population

Figure 2). These are dramatic changes. In 1950, the average and the frequency of interactions with relatives have not changed
American household consisted of 3.4 people, but in 2006 the over time (r = –.13, annual change = .01%).
average was 2.6 people. Urban population increased at an Two items assessed the importance of independence in
average annual rate of 0.3%, which equates to about 700,000 child socialization. The item from the WVS (importance of
Americans migrating to urban areas every year since 1950. “independence”) indicated a rising importance of independence
Similarly, in 1950, only 2.6 out of every 1,000 Americans (r = .77). In contrast, a similar item from the GSS (impor-
experienced divorce, whereas the rate was 3.8 in 2003. These tance of “think for oneself”) showed the opposite pattern (r =
findings are consistent with the modernization theory: During –.86). The divergence between these two items is surprising,
a period of steady economic growth, individualism increased. as the two items not only are similar in content and time cov-
In the scatter plot, one nonlinear trend was apparent: The erage but also are correlated with individualism in the valida-
divorce rate in the United States peaked around 1980 and has tion (r with individualism was .32 for the WVS item and .71
gradually decreased since then (r = –.98 since 1980 vs. .52 over- for the GSS item).
all). In past research, this trend has been attributed to factors Several indices assessed Americans’ trust of other people.
such as an aging population and the rise of cohabitation in Three indices asked participants whether most people in soci-
the United States (Goldstein, 1999). ety could be trusted (see Figure 5). Findings across these three
Next, findings from the survey indices were examined items converged in showing the decline of trust in American
(see Table 2, Figures 3 through 6). The importance of obedience society (rs < –.35). In terms of annual changes, the item from
in child socialization decreased (r = .73). The percentage of the ANES showed no change (annual change = –.01%),
young adults feeling that social contribution is important for whereas two other indices showed a decline (the GSS: -.39%;
a fulfilling life also decreased over time (r = .89). These find- the WVS: -.33%). Hence, a discrepancy among surveys was
ings also indicate a rise in individualism. found. The decline of trust was also found in a decreasing
However, this pattern did not emerge in the other indices. percentage of Americans feeling that most people are honest
Over the years, agreement with unconditional love and respect (r = –.88). These findings generally replicate prior research
for parents increased (r = –.78), and, among young adults, documenting the decline of trust in American society
the perceived importance of effort for success increased (r = (Putnam, 2000).
–.65). To the extent that strong family relations and importance There were four additional items on trust. Two items asked
of personal effort for success are associated with collectivism, whether most people would try to be helpful or just look out
increases in these indices suggest a rise in collectivism. Furthermore, for themselves (from the ANES and the GSS), and two items
the importance of friendship (r = .43, annual change = .03%) asked whether most people would try to be fair or try to take
Hamamura 13

100%

80%

60% Respect parents (4)


Trust (5)
Independence (6)
Friend (7)
40%
Social contribution (8)
Effort (9)

20%

0%
1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
Year

Figure 3. Indices from international surveys


The U.S. Numbers inside the parentheses correspond to numbers in Table 1.

advantage of another if they got the chance (also from the Changes in Japan. Similar to in the United States, in Japan the
ANES and the GSS; see Figure 6). However, for both of these average household size decreased (r = .94), whereas the
items, the findings from the GSS and the ANES diverged. proportion of people living in urban areas (r = .97) and the
Although items from the GSS showed that Americans have divorce rate (r = .90) increased (see Table 2, Figure 2). These
developed a less benevolent view of others over time (rs < changes are even more pronounced than the American pattern:
–.35), items from the ANES showed the opposite pattern In 1950, the average household consisted of 5 people, but
(rs > .16). With an inspection of scatter plots, one trend that in 2006 the average was 2.7 people. The urban population
was common to all of the ANES items (but not to items from increased at a rate of 0.6% per year, equivalent to about
other sources) was that they showed a rise in trust since 2000. 600,000 Japanese migrating to urban areas every year since
Nevertheless, the source of the discrepancy is not clear: These 1950. In 1950, only 1 out of every 1,000 Japanese experienced
items not only are very similar in item content and time cov- divorce, whereas the rate was 2.3 out of every 1,000 in 2004.
erage but also have a similar empirical relationship to individu- The corresponding trend was found in some but not all sur-
alism in the validation process (rs with individualism > .43). vey indices (see Table 2, Figures 7 and 8). The importance of
independence in child socialization increased (r = .96), and
Findings Summary. Diverging patterns were observed across following tradition is seen as less important today (r = .19).
indices. The rise of individualism was observed in both the When family life is pitted against business engagement, a
residential and the family environment: Over the years, urban greater proportion of Japanese today would choose business
population increased, household size became smaller, and over family (r = .36). These trends suggest a rise of individual-
divorce became more prevalent. These changes were accom- ism in Japan over the past several decades, a period character-
panied by changes in some attitudes and values: A smaller ized by steady economic growth (see Figure 1).
percentage of Americans today value social contribution and However, this pattern did not apply to other indices. The
obedience in children. However, other indices did not fit this importance of social obligation increased (r = –.55), whereas
pattern. Relationships with family and friends have been largely the importance of individual rights decreased (r = –.81).
stable or have even strengthened over time, as indicated by Among young adults, the perceived importance of effort for
the continuing importance of friendship and interactions with success increased (r = –.76). Furthermore, the percentage of
relatives and an increase in unconditional love and respect young adults feeling that making a positive contribution to
for parents. Moreover, young adults in the United States today society is important for a fulfilling life has increased over time
perceive a greater importance of effort for success. In addition, (r = –.71). These trends suggest a rise of collectivism in Japan.
Americans have become less trusting of others over the years. Some indices indicated no changes over time. For example,
14 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

100%

80%

60%

Obedience (14)
Think for self (15)
40% Relative (16)

20%

0%
1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
Year

Figure 4. Indices from U.S. national surveys (excluding items related to trust)
Numbers inside the parentheses correspond to numbers in Table 1.

Japanese’s feelings of unconditional love and respect for parents addition, an increasing number of young adults perceived a
were unchanged over time (r = –.22, annual change = –.05%), as greater importance of effort for success and felt that social
were the importance of friendship (r = .47, annual change = contribution is important for a fulfilling life. Concerning trust
.04%), social harmony (r = –.39, annual change = .03%), and of others, the available indices do not indicate a decline of
society or nation (r = –.77, annual change = –.06%). trust in Japan.
Two items assessed trust among the Japanese. The item
asking whether most people can be trusted showed no change
(r = –.30, annual change -.02%). In contrast, when asked Discussion
whether people would try to take advantage of another if The modernization theory has been enormously influential
they got the chance, the percentage of Japanese disagreeing as a theory of cultural change and has also shaped our every-
with this item actually increased over time (r = .71), indicating day thinking (Y. Kashima et al., 2009). The current research
an increase in the benevolent view of others in society. examined an empirical foundation to this theory in the context
of cross-temporal changes in individualism–collectivism in
Findings Summary. In Japan, the rise of individualism was the United States and Japan. Diverging patterns of changes
observed in both the residential and family environments: emerged across indices.
Over the years, Japanese migrated to urban areas, household
size became smaller, and divorce became more prevalent. Modernization. Some findings from this research clearly
These changes were accompanied by changes in attitudes support the modernization theory. In both countries, the aver-
and values: Following tradition has become less important age household has become smaller, urban population has
over time, and independence is seen as more important in increased, and divorce has become more prevalent. In addi-
child socialization today. Furthermore, when given a choice tion to these changes in living and family environments,
between seeing a dying parent and attending an important busi- corresponding changes were observed in attitudes and val-
ness meeting, more Japanese today would go to the business ues. In the United States, obedience is less important in child
meeting. However, this pattern did not apply to other indices. socialization today and a smaller proportion of young adults
The importance of unconditional love toward parents, friend- value social contribution. Similarly, in Japan, following
ship, and social harmony has not changed over time. Moreover, tradition has become less important over time and indepen-
the importance of social obligations has increased, whereas dence in child socialization is valued more. To the extent that
respect for individual rights has become less important. In these changes occurred in a period characterized by steady
Hamamura 15

100%

80%
Most people can be trusted

60%

WVS (5)
ANES (11)
40% GSS (19)

20%

0%
1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
Year

Figure 5. “Most people can be trusted or you need to be very careful in dealing with people?”
Numbers inside the parentheses correspond to numbers in Table 1.

economic growth, these findings are consistent with the the modernization theory. First, findings on the importance
modernization theory. of family and friends diverged across indices. On one hand,
One pattern that is also consistent with the modernization demographic indices of family relations (i.e., household size
theory is the rising importance of effort for success observed and divorce rates) indicate a weakened family relationship in
in both countries. One of the implications of modernization is both countries. On the other hand, findings from survey
labor specialization, which would require employees to have items suggest continuing importance of family and friends.
specialized training. Moreover, with the constant advances in Specifically among Americans, the strength of relationships
technology, the competency of a modern workforce demands with relatives was unchanged, and the proportion of people
constant updating of skills and knowledgebase. In other words, feeling unconditional respect and love toward their parents
the success of a modern workforce necessitates hard work and actually increased over time. Similarly, among Japanese uncon-
an achievement-oriented mind-set in employees (McClelland, ditional love toward parents has not changed over time. In
1961). In fact, rapid modernization of East Asian societies both countries, the importance of friends was unchanged.
has been attributed to cultural systems that sanction hard The continuing importance of family among Americans
work and effort (Dore, 1973). This rationale suggests that the has been reported before (Thornton & Young-DeMarco, 2001).
increasing importance of effort for a successful life felt among It has been argued that these patterns highlight changes in the
young adults may reflect the continuing growth of the American meaning of family life: Although Americans continue to value
and Japanese economies. family life, these relationships are much less obligatory (e.g.,
In sum, the modernization theory successfully affirms some more aged people needing support live independently of their
of the cultural changes observed in the United States and Japan children today compared to previous decades) and more voluntary
for the past several decades. At the same time, however, in nature today than in the past (Thornton & Young-DeMarco,
other changes observed in this research are inconsistent with 2001). Current findings suggest that a similar process might
the modernization theory. In fact, these findings highlight its be occurring in Japan. Hence, one interpretation of these find-
limitations as a theory of cultural change. In the following ings is that although modernization processes transformed
sections, findings that did not fit the modernization theory the physical environment surrounding relationships with fam-
are considered. ily and friends, Americans and Japanese continue to value
family and friends, probably in a less obligatory and more vol-
Changes in Family, Friendship, and Trust of Others. Findings on untary fashion today compared to previous decades. However,
family life, friendship, and general social trust did not fit with these patterns await further research. The current data set had
16 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

100%

80%

60%
Honest (10)
Helpful: ANES (12)
Fair: ANES (13)
Helpful: GSS (17)
40%
Fair: GSS (18)

20%

0%
1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
Year

Figure 6. Indices from U.S. national surveys: other items related to trust
Numbers inside the parentheses correspond to numbers in Table 1.

just two demographic indices, a few attitudinal items of family in a society. Research suggests that racial and cultural diver-
relations, and a single attitudinal item on friendship. Moreover, sity is one of the predictors of trust at the community level,
one Japanese survey item that pitted the importance of family presumably because it is more difficult to trust others who are
against business engagement showed that more Japanese racially and culturally dissimilar (Alesina & La Ferrara, 2002).
today would choose business over family, showing a conflict- This suggests that trust of others might have decreased in
ing pattern, although this item does not necessarily suggest the American society because of increased racial and cultural
declining importance of family irrespective of the importance heterogeneity in American society, whereas trust has remained
of business. Clearly, more research, for example through a cre- unchanged in Japan because of its continuing racial and cul-
ative analysis of archival data as discussed below, is warranted. tural homogeneity. This rationale is consistent with the statis-
Unlike relationships with family and friends, trust of others tics on immigration: Although in the United States the proportion
showed a diverging pattern between the two countries: Although of foreign-born citizens increased in the past several decades,
the level of trust declined among Americans (replicating in Japan such a proportion has remained small and largely
Putnam, 2000) the same pattern did not emerge in Japan. The unchanged over time (World Bank, 2010). Hence, the rise of
finding that the decline of trust observed in American society racial and cultural diversity in the United States and continuing
may not generalize to Japan was suggested in the past (Inoguchi, racial and cultural homogeneity in Japan may account for the
2000) but to my knowledge this is the first documentation of diverging pattern of trust.
such a pattern.
Research on declining trust in American society has attrib- Collectivism in Modern Japan. Perhaps the most intriguing
uted the trend to such factors as increased pressure for time aspect of this research is the persistence of collectivism in
and money, more time spent in private, and a rise of electronic Japan: In several indices, there were signs of persisting,
entertainment (Putnam, 2000). However, in Japanese society, even rising, collectivism. One commonality in these indices
where these changes seem to have occurred as well, trust has is that they assessed the importance of collectivistic living
largely been stable. This consideration casts some doubt on (increased importance in social obligation, social harmony, and
the generality of Putnam’s theory of declining trust in cross- social contribution and decreased importance in individual
cultural examinations (Inoguchi, 2000). An alternative possi- rights). In fact, these findings confirm prior cross-temporal anal-
bility for the diverging pattern is racial and cultural diversity yses of Japanese society that reported continuing emphasis on
Hamamura 17

100%

80%

60% Respect parents (4)


Trust (5)
Independence (6)
Friend (7)
40%
Social contribution (8)
Effort (9)

20%

0%
1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
Year

Figure 7. Indices from international surveys: Japan


Numbers inside the parentheses correspond to numbers in Table 1.

group orientation, social harmony, and obligation (Caudill, hypotheses, and esteeming self-directed learning, the East
1973; Flanagan, 1979; Trommsdorff, 1983). These findings Asian approach to learning is influenced by the Confucian tra-
are consistent with the cultural heritage theory and show that dition characterized by habits of hard work, self-improve-
the importance of collectivistic living has continued in Japa- ment, and pragmatic orientation to learning. Hence, even
nese society even during a period of significant economic though greater accessibility of education is one of the conse-
development. quences of modernization, societies’ educational philosophy
Other research has also suggested the role of cultural heri- is profoundly influenced by their cultural heritage, and
tage in continuing cultural tradition (Inglehart & Baker, 2000). schooling actually serves to reinforce cultural heritage.
However, the processes through which heritage sustains cul- Another example of a pattern of culture shaping the effect of
tural tradition has not received much systematic attention. In modernization can be found in mass advertisement. Although
the following sections, the mechanism of cultural continuity the rise of mass advertisement is another consequence of
is considered. modernization (Inkeles, 1975), the ways in which advertise-
ments are designed differ across cultures, as successful adver-
Cultural Heritage and the Persistence of Cultural Tradition. One tisements accommodate the tastes of local consumers. For
way in which cultural heritage sustains a pattern of culture is example, Kim and Markus (1999) found that advertisements
by shaping the effects of modernization. There are a number of in American and Korean magazines differ in that American
examples of this process in prior research. One of the conse- ads tended to focus more on the theme of the pursuit of unique-
quences of modernization is mass education, as modernized ness, whereas Korean ads focused more on the theme of
societies require a workforce that is trained for a socially and social harmony. To the extent that advertisements are sources
technologically complex environment. Across societies, of social influence, this example also suggests that mass
there is commonality in the contents of education (i.e., skills advertisements may reinforce cultural heritage, in that exposure
taught in school), but research suggests that societies vary to advertisements portraying conformity may reinforce con-
greatly in how these contents are taught. For example, Tweed formity among Koreans.
and Lehman (2002) argued that although the Western One difficult question regarding the idea of cultural heri-
approach to learning is influenced by the Socratic method tage is its origin. That is, how is it that American society came
characterized by habits of questioning, expressing personal to value uniqueness, whereas Korean society came to value
18 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

100%

80%

Individual right (20)


60%
Family over business (21)
Obligation (22)
Tradition (23)
40% Harmony (24)
Not take advantage (25)
Society most important (26)

20%

0%
1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
Year

Figure 8. Indices from Japanese national surveys


Numbers inside the parentheses correspond to numbers in Table 1.

conformity? More broadly, how is it that Western societies in worldwide studies of ethnographies (Barry et al., 1959)
came to become individualistic and East Asian societies as well as in cross-cultural experiments (Berry, 1967).
collectivistic? Another possible socioecological cause of individualism–
Research has examined this question from a number of per- collectivism is the historical prevalence of disease-causing
spectives. Some theorists have sought the origin to intellec- pathogens in the environment (Fincher et al., 2008). According
tual and religious traditions, such as the tradition of Greek to this theory, collectivism works as a defense mechanism
civilization (Nisbett, 2003) or the Protestant Reformation against pathogens. For example, a reluctance to interact with
in the West (Weber, 1904/1992) and the tradition of members of an out-group can protect individuals from poten-
Buddhism and Confucianism in East Asia (Collcutt, 1991; tial exposure to novel pathogens. Similarly, the norm of con-
Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Nisbett, 2003). formity, to the extent that it helps preserve rituals and customs
Others have focused on ecological factors. Barry, Child, that buffer against the risk of infectious disease, is an effec-
and Bacon (1959) focused on the schedule of food accumula- tive defense against pathogens. Supporting this idea, Fincher
tion. In this framework, an agriculture-based community repre- et al. found that the historical prevalence of pathogens was
sents one end of the continuum, a high-food-accumulation positively correlated with collectivism (also see Hamamura
community. In such a community, food is harvested in large & Park, 2010; Schaller & Murray, 2008).
quantities infrequently, and the processes of food production Another socioecological theory of individualism–collectivism
are managed carefully to ensure the availability of enough has focused on the role of mobility. For example, Kitayama,
food for the community. Adherence to social norms and con- Ishii, et al. (2006) focused on the role of voluntary settlement.
formity are encouraged, and risk taking is discouraged, as They theorized that voluntary migrants are a self-selected
the potential benefit from innovation is outweighed by the group of highly autonomous, independent, and goal-oriented
danger of a food shortage in the case of failure. On the other individuals. Moreover, to the extent that settlements are
hand, hunting represents a low level of food accumulation. occupied by these individuals, a highly individualistic ethos
Obtaining food is largely dependent on one’s skill. Moreover, develops in these areas, which further reinforces individualistic
as food is obtained more frequently, the benefit of innovation mentality in a mutually reinforcing fashion. Supporting this
outweighs its cost: Successful innovation could bring an abun- hypothesis, in a series of experiments, Japanese living in
dant accumulation of food, whereas a failure is not necessar- the northern island of Hokkaido, which was the destination
ily life threatening. These predictions have empirical support: for many voluntary migrants in the late 19th century, exhibited
Frequency of food accumulation was indeed found to predict psychological processes of individualism (e.g., internal
societies’ orientation toward individualism–collectivism attribution, dissonance reduction for personal choices,
Hamamura 19

happiness predicted by socially disengaging emotions) of culture, which in turn shapes their cultural environment.
more so than Japanese living outside Hokkaido. Recent This process of mutual reinforcement continues in a function-
research has also identified other forms of mobility (e.g., resi- ally autonomous fashion, independently of the external fac-
dential, employment, social relationships) in predicting indi- tor that gave rise to cultural differences in the first place.
vidualistic psychological processes (Chen, Chiu, & Chan, To summarize, research has identified several distal fac-
2009; Oishi, Lun, & Sherman, 2007; Schug, Yuki, & Maddux, tors of cross-cultural variations in individualism–collectivism.
2010). In sum, this line of research suggests historical and Influence of these factors can be carried across generations by
ecological factors that presumably gave rise to cross-cul- subjective representation in a functionally autonomous fash-
tural variations in individualism–collectivism. ion. When viewed under this framework, the persistence of
collectivism in modern Japan is not surprising. Collectivism
Functional Autonomy: Subjective Representation in Continuing in Japan today is likely reflecting legacies of historical and
Culture. Another important question is the process by which ecological conditions that evoked collectivism in the first
the influence of distal factors continues into contemporary place, and such a legacy has been carried forward via
environments, where at times these original factors are no Japanese’s representations of their culture. Thus, findings
longer present. On this question, research introduced the from the current research underscore the importance of
idea of “functional autonomy,” a mechanism in which cul- research examining the role of societies’ cultural heritage and
ture, once evoked, can have a life of its own (Cohen, 2001). its persistence within the population.
The persistence of the culture of honor in contemporary the
southern United States, which began with a herding economy Implications and Limitations. The current findings showed
that is no longer prevalent, provides one illuminating case of diverging patterns across different aspects of individualism–
functional autonomy (Nisbett & Cohen, 1996). collectivism. Previous sections have illustrated the factors and
When applied to the individualism–collectivism research, processes behind the diversion.
the theory of functional autonomy suggests that the individ- However, one alternative explanation of this pattern is the
ualism–collectivism differences of today may be legacies process of cultural lag, or the idea that aspects of individualism–
of historically evoked patterns of culture that have been car- collectivism differ in the speed in which they are modernized.
ried forward by a subjective representation of that culture That is, this theory would suggest that even those aspects of
(Yamagishi, Hashimoto, & Schug, 2008). That is, cross-cultural Japanese culture that remain collectivistic today will eventu-
variations in individualism–collectivism today may reflect ally modernize and will shift toward greater individualism.
differences in individuals’ representation of culture that are Such an account is plausible, although there are at least two
maintained independently of the external environment that problems. First, it is unclear how much time is needed for
gave rise to such differences in the first place. modernization effects to take place: Without the specifica-
Research has located processes in which individuals’ rep- tion of time, the theory of cultural lag is unfalsifiable. Second,
resentation of culture are maintained culturally in socializa- even if cultural lag is taking place and the rise of individualism
tion practices (Ng, Pomerantz, & Lam, 2007), communication will be observed eventually in all aspects of individualism–
(E. Kashima & Kashima, 1998), media (Kim & Markus, 1999), collectivism, why some of these changes lag others needs to
and legal institutions (Nisbett & Cohen, 1996). Similarly, be explained. In turn, it seems inevitable that such an expla-
Nisbett and colleagues have maintained that the heritage of nation incorporates the role of cultural heritage. Hence, even
Greek civilization in the West and Buddhism and Confucianism if the cultural lag theory were made falsifiable, and the changes
in East Asia are maintained today by social norms, socializa- it predicted were observed eventually, it seems that such a
tion, communication practices, aesthetic preferences (Masuda, theory would still require the role of cultural heritage in
Gonzalez, Kwan, & Nisbett, 2008), architecture and landscape accounting for patterns of cultural changes.
(Miyamoto, Nisbett, & Masuda, 2006), and social and legal Methodologically, this research’s reliance on survey indices
institutions (Nisbett et al., 2001). has implications. A large majority of the indices in this research
Recent research has started to examine directly the role of were self-report items of beliefs and values. Research suggests
subjective representation in shaping psychological processes. not only that self-report items are compromised by method-
In a series of studies, Zou et al. (2009) found that individual- ological issues (e.g., Heine et al., 2002) but also that the
istic and collectivistic psychological processes are predicted abstract beliefs and values assessed by these items are poorly
by people’s representations of culture. In these studies, Hong correlated with behavioral measures (Kitayama et al., 2009;
Kong Chinese who believed Hong Kong was a collectivistic Na et al., 2010). In the current research, the process of vali-
society were more collectivistic in their behaviors (e.g., showed dation, in which these indices were validated against behav-
more compliance and situational attribution) compared to oth- ioral indices of individualism–collectivism, ensures that the
ers (also see Yamagishi et al., 2008, for similar findings among findings extend beyond self-reported responses. Nevertheless,
Japanese). In sum, people’s representation of their culture and some ambiguous patterns in the findings may be attributable
their cultural environment reinforce each other in a mutually to the self-report method. For example, there were cases in
constitutive way (Markus & Kitayama, 1991): Exposure to which similarly worded items (e.g., items on the importance
social and cultural institutions shapes people’s representation of independence in child socialization in the United States
20 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

and different indices of trust showing diverging patterns among (2004) maintained that prior to Japan’s encounter with modern
Americans) administered for comparable time period showed Western cultures in the late 19th century, the idea of marriage as
diverging patterns despite having the same validated relation- a sacred and eternal union was largely foreign in Japan. In sub-
ship with individualism–collectivism. In both cases, it is sequent years, however, with increased exposure to Western
unclear what factor accounts for the discrepancy. Research cultures, and with the government’s policy to stabilize marriage
in cultural psychology has overcome the limitations of as one of the key institutions for modernization, the longevity of
self-report data by employing a diversified methodological marriage has increased and the divorce rate has declined gradu-
approach. Researchers gain confidence in their findings when ally, reaching its lowest level in the 1960s. This example sug-
a converging pattern emerges, for example, from a self-report gests that if one were to compare individualism–collectivism of
measure, an experiment, and an analysis of cultural product. 1900 and 2000 in Japan, using the divorce rate would be prob-
Similarly, alternative methodological approaches to cross- lematic, as marriage and its relationship to individualism–col-
temporal analysis of culture seeking data outside self-report lectivism were quite different between these two periods. More
data should help overcome some of the ambiguities in the generally, this example highlights the difficulty of establishing
current findings. One promising approach is to analyze cul- indices’ validity across different periods.
tural product. Recent research in cultural psychology suggests To remedy this issue, in this research, cultural changes were
that patterns of culture can be measured reliably and accu- analyzed in a data-driven, bottom-up manner. This approach
rately in analyzing proverbs, news articles, magazine adver- was fruitful in identifying discrepant patterns across indices:
tisements, children’s storybooks, song lyrics, and landscapes, Identification of these patterns would have been difficult if
among others (for a review, see Morling & Lamoreaux, 2008). the data analysis had imposed a single dimension. Nevertheless,
These techniques could be applied to cross-temporal research. the limitation posed by the validity issue is best addressed by
Finally, one challenge to studies of cross-temporal changes future research that examines the patterns identified here.
is that the investigations are necessarily confined to preexisting For example, analyses of cultural products such as individu-
data: Unlike with other topic areas, it would be impossible to alistic and collectivistic themes in magazine advertisements
administer a new measure to study psychological processes in or song lyrics would be important not only in extending this
the past. There are a few issues implicated by this limitation, research but also in providing a more comprehensive and
one of which is the issue of validation. Although the indices in richer picture of cultural change.
this research were validated against independent indices of indi-
vidualism–collectivism, the validated relationships apply only
to contemporary data. In other words, whether the validated Conclusion
relationships remain across time requires further examination. One common belief inside and outside the field of cultural
However, carrying out such an examination is a challenge, as psychology has been that cultures are becoming more indi-
there are no clear benchmarks of individualism–collectivism vidualistic over time, especially in those parts of the world
across different periods. An example of the divorce rate in Japan where the economy is growing. Findings from this research
illustrates the nature of this challenge. In the beginning of the showed that the modernization theory is applicable to some,
20th century, divorce rates in Japan were among the highest in but not all, changes in individualism–collectivism over time. A
the world, much higher than today’s level (Fuess, 2004). Fuess comprehensive theory of cultural change requires consideration

Appendix
Characteristics of the surveys examined
Survey Country Time coverage Sample characteristic Reference
American National Election Studies United States Conducted every 2 About 1,000– American National Election
years since 1948 2,000, nationally Studies (2009)
representative sample
DDB Lifestyle Survey United States Conducted every About 3,500–4,000 Putnam (2000)
year between 1975 participants recruited by
and 1999 commercial polling firms
General Social Survey United States Conducted every 1 to About 1,500, nationally Davis, Smith, and Marsden
2 years since 1972 representative sample (2009)
National Characteristic Survey Japan Conducted every 5 About 2,000– Institutes of Statistical
years since 1953 3,000, nationally Mathematics (2009)
representative sample
World Values Survey International Conducted 5 times About 1,000– World Values Survey
since 1981 (as of 2,000, nationally Association (2009)
2009) representative sample
World Youth Survey International Conducted every 5 About 1,000, nationally Government of Japan Cabinet
years since 1972 representative sample, Office (2009)
ages 18–24
Hamamura 21

of modernization effects as well as the role of cultural heri- Davis, J., Smith, T., & Marsden, P. (2009). General Social Surveys,
tage, considering questions such as how a pattern of culture 1972–2008 [Machine-readable data file]. Chicago, IL: University
emerges and how such a pattern continues over time, even in of Chicago, National Opinion Research Center.
the face of a changing environment. Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. (2003). Personality, culture, and
subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of
Acknowledgments life. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403-425.
I thank Izumi Hori and Li Man Wi for data collection help and Emma Dore, R. (1973). British factory, Japanese factory: The origins of
Buchtel, Julian Dierkes, Steve Heine, Darrin Lehman, and Ara national diversity in industrial relations. Berkeley: University
Norenzayan for helpful comments. of California Press.
Durkheim, E. (1964). The division of labor in society. New York,
Declaration of Conflicting Interests NY: Free Press. (Original work published 1893)
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect Elliot, A. J., Chirkov, V. I., Kim, Y., & Sheldon, K. M. (2001). A
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. cross-cultural analysis of avoidance (relative to approach) per-
sonal goals. Psychological Science, 12, 505-510.
Funding Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2008).
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings of the Royal Society
research was supported by the Direct Allocation Grant from the B: Biological Sciences, 275, 1279-1285.
Chinese University of Hong Kong. Flanagan, S. (1979). Value change and partisan change in Japan: The
silent revolution revisited. Comparative Politics, 11, 253-278.
References Fuess, H. (2004). Divorce in Japan: family, gender, and the state,
Adams, G., & Plaut, V. C. (2003). The cultural grounding of personal 1600–2000. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
relationship: Friendship in North American and West African Glenn, N. (1987). Social trends in the United States: Evidence from
worlds. Personal Relationships, 10, 333-347. sample surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 109-126.
Alesina, A., & La Ferrara, E. (2002). Who trusts others? Journal of Goldstein, J. (1999). The leveling of divorce in the United States.
Public Economics, 85, 207-234. Demography, 36, 409-414.
Allen, M. W., Ng, S. H., Ikeda, K., Jawan, J. A., Sufi, A. H., Wilson, M., & Government of Japan Cabinet Office. (2009). The world youth
Yang, K.-S. (2007). Two decades of change in cultural values and survey. Retrieved from http://www8.cao.go.jp/youth/english/
economic development in eight East Asian and Pacific Island worldyouth7-e/html/mokuji-e.html
nations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 247-269. Greenfield, P. (2009). Linking social change and developmental
Alwin, D. (1989). Changes in qualities valued in children in the United change: Shifting pathways of human development. Developmental
States, 1964 to 1984. Social Science Research, 18, 195-236. Psychology, 45, 401-418.
American National Election Studies. (2009). American National Gusfield, J. (1967). Tradition and modernity: Misplaced polarities
Election Studies. Retrieved from http://www.electionstudies.org/ in the study of social change. American Journal of Sociology,
Barry, H., III, Child, I., & Bacon, M. (1959). Relation of child train- 72, 351-362.
ing to subsistence economy. American Anthropologist, 61, 51-63. Guthrie, G. (1977). A social-psychological analysis of modernization in the
Berry, J. W. (1967). Independence and conformity in subsistence- Philippines. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 8, 177-206.
level societies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Hamamura, T., Meijer, Z., Heine, S., Kamaya, K., & Hori, I. (2009).
7, 415-418. Approach-avoidance motivation and information processing:
Bond, R., & Smith, P. (1996). Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis A cross-cultural analysis. Personality and Social Psychology
of studies using Asch’s (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psycho- Bulletin, 35, 454-462.
logical Bulletin, 119, 111-137. Hamamura, T., & Park, J. H. (2010). Regional differences in pathogen
Brewer, M., & Chen, Y. (2007). Where (who) are collectives in prevalence and defensive reactions to the “swine flu” outbreak
collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individualism among East Asians and Westerners. Evolutionary Psychology, 8,
and collectivism. Psychological Review, 114, 133-151. 506-515.
Caudill, W. (1973). The influence of social structure and culture on Hamamura, T., Xu, Q., & Du, Y. (2011). Social class and indepen-
human behavior in modern Japan. Ethos, 1, 343-382. dence and interdependence among Chinese adolescents. Manu-
Chen, J., Chiu, C.-Y., & Chan, F. S.-F. (2009). The cultural effect of script submitted for publication.
job mobility and the belief in a fixed world. Journal of Personality Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C.,
and Social Psychology, 97, 851-865. & Matsumoto, H. (2001). Divergent consequences of success
Cohen, D. (2001). Cultural variation: Considerations and implica- and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-
tions. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 451-471. improving motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Person-
Collcutt, M. (1991). The legacy of Confucianism in Japan. In ality and Social Psychology, 81, 599-615.
G. Rozman (Ed.), The East Asian region: Confucian heritage and its Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999).
modern adaptation (pp. 111-154). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univer- Is there a universal need for positive self-regard. Psychological
sity Press. Review, 106, 766-794.
22 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K., & Greenholtz, J. (2002). Kashima, Y., & Kashima, E. (2003). Individualism, GNP, climate,
What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective and pronoun drop: Is individualism determined by affluence and
Likert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality climate, or does language use play a role? Journal of Cross-
and Social Psychology, 82, 903-918. Cultural Psychology, 34, 125-134.
Hirakawa, S. (1989). Japan’s turn to the West. In M. B. Jansen Kashima, Y., Kokubo, T., Kashima, E., Boxall, D., Yamaguchi, S., &
(Ed.), The Cambridge history of Japan: Volume 5, The nine- Macrae, K. (2004). Culture and self: Are there within-culture dif-
teenth century (pp. 432-498). New York, NY: Cambridge ferences in self between metropolitan areas and regional cities?
University Press. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 816-823.
Hitokoto, H., Murabe, T., Narita, K., & Tanaka-Matsumi, J. (2010, Kim, H., & Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony
January). Through time and space: Regional variation of individ- or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and
ualism–collectivism in Japan. Poster presented at the 11th annual Social Psychology, 77, 785-800.
meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Las Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (2007). Handbook of cultural psychology.
Vegas, NV. New York, NY: Guilford.
Hofstede, G. H. (1984). Culture’s consequences: International dif- Kitayama, S., Ishii, K., Imada, T., Takemura, K., & Ramaswamy, J.
ferences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. (2006). Voluntary settlement and the spirit of independence:
Huntington, S. P. (1996). The clash of civilizations and the remaking Evidence from Japan’s “northern frontier.” Journal of Person-
of world order. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. ality and Social Psychology, 91, 369-384.
Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, globalization Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2006). Cultural affor-
and the persistence of tradition: Empirical evidence from 65 soci- dances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and dis-
eties. American Sociological Review, 65, 19-55. engaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of
Inglehart, R., & Oyserman, D. (2004). Individualism, autonomy, self- Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 890-903.
expression. The human development syndrome. In H. Vinken, Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, T. A., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K.
J. Soeters, & P. Ester (Eds.), Comparing cultures, dimensions of (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Com-
culture in a comparative perspective (pp. 74-96). Leiden, Neth- paring North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal
erlands: Brill. of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 236-255.
Inkeles, A. (1975). Becoming modern: Individual change in six Kraus, M., Piff, P., & Keltner, D. (2009). Social class, the sense
developing countries. Ethos, 3, 323-342. of control, and social explanation. Journal of Personality and
Inkeles, A. (1983). Exploring individual modernity. New York, NY: Social Psychology, 97, 992-1004.
Columbia University Press. Lee, A. Y., Aaker, J. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2000). The pleasures and
Inoguchi, T. (2000). Social capital in Japan. Japanese Journal of pains of distinct self-construals: The role of interdependence in
Political Science, 1, 73-112. regulatory focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Institutes of Statistical Mathematics. (2009). national character study 78, 1122-1134.
of Japanese and International comparisons. Retrieved from http:// Ma, V., & Schoeneman, T. (1997). Individualism versus collectivism:
www.ism.ac.jp/kokuminsei/index.html A comparison of Kenyan and American self-concepts. Basic and
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 261-273.
choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal Maddison, A. (2008). Statistics on world population, GDP and per
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 349-366. capita GDP, 1-2006 AD. Retrieved from http://www.ggdc.net/
Kagitcibasi, C. (1994). A critical appraisal of individualism and col- maddison/
lectivism: Toward a new formulation. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Impli-
C. Kagitcibasi, S. C. Chol, & S. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and cations for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological
collectivism (pp. 52-65). London, UK: Sage. Review, 98, 224-253.
Kagitcibasi, C. (2005). Modernization does not mean Western- Masuda, T., Gonzalez, R., Kwan, L., & Nisbett, R. (2008). Culture
ization: Emergence of a different pattern. In W. Friedlmeier, and aesthetic preference: Comparing the attention to context of
. Chakkarath, & B. Shwarz (Eds.), Culture and human develop- East Asians and Americans. Personality and Social Psychology
ment: The importance of cross-cultural research for the social sci- Bulletin, 34, 1260-1275.
ences (pp. 255-272). New York, NY: Psychology Press. McClelland, D. (1961). The achieving society. New York, NY: Free
Kameda, T., Takezawa, M., & Hastie, R. (2005). Where do social Press.
norms come from? The example of communal sharing. Current Miyamoto, Y., Nisbett, R., & Masuda, T. (2006). Culture and the
Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 331-334. physical environment. Psychological Science, 17, 113-119.
Kashima, E., & Kashima, Y. (1998). Culture and language: The case Morling, B., & Lamoreaux, M. (2008). Measuring culture outside
of cultural dimensions and personal pronoun use. Journal of the head: A meta-analysis of individualism–collectivism in cul-
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 461-486. tural products. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12,
Kashima, Y., Bain, P., Haslam, N., Peters, K., Laham, S., Whelan, J., . . 199-221.
. Fernando, J. (2009). Folk theory of social change. Asian Journal Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2010). Historical prevalence of
of Social Psychology, 12, 227-246. infectious diseases within 230 geopolitical regions: A tool for
Hamamura 23

investigating origins of culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy- Stephens, N., Markus, H., & Townsend, S. (2007). Choice as an act
chology, 41, 99-108. of meaning: The case of social class. Journal of Personality and
Na, J., Grossmann, I., Varnum, M., Kitayama, S., Gonzalez, R., & Social Psychology, 93, 814-830.
Nisbett, R. (2010). Cultural differences are not always reducible Thornton, A., & Young-DeMarco, L. (2001). Four decades of trends
to individual differences. Proceedings of the National Academy in attitudes toward family issues in the United States: The 1960s
of Sciences, 107, 6192-6197. through the 1990s. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 1009-1037.
National Institute of Population and Social Security Research. Tonnies, F. (1957). Community and society. New York, NY: Harper
(2009). Syussei doukou kihonchousa [Birth tendency research]. & Row. (Original work published 1887)
Tokyo, Japan: Author. Triandis, H. (1995). Individualism & collectivism. Boulder, CO:
Newson, L., Postmes, T., Lea, S., & Webley, P. (2005). Why are mod- Westview.
ern families small? Toward an evolutionary and cultural explana- Triandis, H., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M., Asai, M., & Lucca, N.
tion for the demographic transition. Personality and Social (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspec-
Psychology Review, 9, 360-375. tives on self-ingroup relationships. Journal of Personality and
Ng, F. F.-Y., Pomerantz, E. M., & Lam, S.-F. (2007). European Social Psychology, 54, 323-338.
American and Chinese parents’ responses to children’s success Trommsdorff, G. (1983). Value change in Japan. International
and failure: Implications for children’s responses. Developmental Journal of Intercultural Relations, 7, 337-360.
Psychology, 43, 1239-1255. Trzesniewski, K., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010). Rethinking “genera-
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: Why we think the tion me”: A study of cohort effects from 1976–2006. Perspec-
way we do. New York, NY: Free Press. tives on Psychological Science, 5, 58-75.
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology Tweed, R., & Lehman, D. (2002). Learning considered within a
of violence in the South. Denver, CO: Westview. cultural context: Confucian and Socratic approaches. American
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture Psychologist, 57, 89-99.
and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psy- Twenge, J. M., Abebe, E. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2010). Fitting in or
chological Review, 108, 291-310. standing out: Trends in American parents’ choices for children’s
Oishi, S. (2010). The psychology of residential mobility: Implica- names, 1880–2007. Social Psychological and Personality
tions for the self, social relationships, and well-being. Perspec- Science, 1, 19-25.
tives on Psychological Science, 5, 5-21. Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Age and birth cohort dif-
Oishi, S., Lun, J., & Sherman, G. (2007). Residential mobility, self- ferences in self-esteem: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Person-
concept, and positive affect in social interactions. Journal of ality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 321-344.
Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 131-141. Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Increases in positive
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking self-views among high school students: Birth cohort changes in
individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assump- anticipated performance, self-satisfaction, self-liking, and self-
tions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3-72. competence. Psychological Science, 19, 1082-1086.
Putnam, R. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American Twenge, J. M., & Im, C. (2007). Changes in the need for social approval,
community. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. 1958–2001. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 171-189.
Roberts, B., & Helson, R. (1997). Changes in culture, changes in per- Twenge, J. M., Konrath, S., Foster, J. D., Campbell, W. K., &
sonality: The influence of individualism in a longitudinal study Bushman, B. J. (2008). Egos inflating over time: A cross-temporal
of women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, meta-analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Journal
641-651. of Personality, 76, 875-902.
Robinson, W. (1950). Ecological correlations and the behavior of Twenge, J. M., Zhang, L., & Im, C. (2004). It’s beyond my control: A cross-
individuals. American Sociological Review, 15, 351-357. temporal meta-analysis of increasing externality in locus of control,
Rozman, G. (1991). The East Asian region in comparative perspec- 1960–2002. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 308-319.
tive. In G. Rozman (Ed.), The East Asian region: Confucian United Nations Statistics Division. (2009). Social indicators. Retrieved from
heritage and its modern adaptation (pp. 3-42). Princeton, NJ: http://unstats.un.org/unsd/demographic/products/socind/default.htm
Princeton University Press. Vandello, J. A., & Cohen, D. (1999). Patterns of individualism and
Sachs, J. (2005). The end of poverty: How can we make it happen in collectivism across the United States. Journal of Personality
our lifetime. London, UK: Penguin. and Social Psychology, 77, 279-292.
Schaller, M., & Murray, D. R. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and Weber, M. (1904/1992). The Protestant ethic and the “spirit” of capi-
culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in talism and other writings. London, UK: Routledge. (Original
sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. work published 1904)
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 212-221. World Bank. (2010). International migration stock. Retrieved from
Schug, J., Yuki, M., & Maddux, W. W. (2010). Relational mobility http://data.worldbank.org/data-catalog
explains between- and within-culture differences in self-disclosure World Values Survey Association. (2009). World Values Survey.
toward close friends. Psychological Science, 21, 1471-1478. Retrieved from http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/
Snibbe, A., & Markus, H. (2005). You can’t always get what you Yamagishi, T. (1999). From assurance based society to trust based
want: educational attainment, agency, and choice. Journal of Per- society: Where is the Japanese system heading [In Japanese].
sonality and Social Psychology, 88, 703-720. Tokyo, Japan: Chuo Koron Shinsha.
24 Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(1)

Yamagishi, T., Hashimoto, H., & Schug, J. (2008). Preferences Yang, K. (1996). Chinese personality and its change. In M. H. Bond
versus strategies as explanations for culture-specific behavior. (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp. 106-170).
Psychological Science, 19, 579-584. Hong Kong, Hong Kong: Oxford University Press.
Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the Zou, X., Tam, K., Morris, M., Lee, L., Lau, I. Y., & Chiu, C. Y.
United States and Japan. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 129-166. (2009). Culture as common sense: Perceived consensus vs. per-
Yang, K. (1988). Will societal modernization eventually eliminate sonal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influence. Journal of
cross-cultural psychological differences. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 579-597.
The cross-cultural challenge to social psychology (pp. 67-85).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

View publication stats

You might also like