You are on page 1of 14

Research Report

Early Intensive Postural and


Movement Training Advances Head
Control in Very Young Infants
Hui-Min Lee, James Cole Galloway
H-M. Lee, PT, PhD, was a graduate
student in the Department of
Background. Daily experiences are thought to play an important role in motor Physical Therapy, University of
development during infancy. There are limited studies on the effect of postural and Delaware, Newark, Delaware, at
movement experiences on head control. the time this study was con-
ducted. Address all correspon-
Objective. The purpose of this study was to quantify the effects of postural and dence to Dr Lee at: noralee0724@
yahoo.com.
movement experiences on head control through a comprehensive set of measure-
ments beginning when infants were 1 month old. J.C. Galloway, PT, PhD, Infant
Motor Behavior Laboratory, and
Biomechanics and Movement Sci-
Design. This was a prospective, longitudinal, 2-cohort study. ences Graduate Program, Depart-
ments of Physical Therapy and
Methods. Twenty-two full-term infants who were healthy were randomly Psychology, Center for Biomedical
assigned to either a training group or a control group. Infants were observed every Engineering Research, University
other week from 1 to 4 months of age. Head control was assessed using a standard- of Delaware.
ized developmental assessment tool, the Test of Infant Motor Performance (TIMP), as [Lee H-M, Galloway JC. Early
well as behavioral coding and kinematics of infants’ head postures and movements in intensive postural and movement
a supported sitting position. Caregivers performed at least 20 minutes of daily training advances head control in
postural and movement activities (training group), or social interaction (control very young infants. Phys Ther.
2012;92:935–947.]
group) for 4 weeks.
© 2012 American Physical Therapy
Results. The training group had higher TIMP scores on head control–related items Association
during the training period and after training stopped compared with the control Published Ahead of Print:
group. Starting from the during training phase, the training group infants had their March 30, 2012
heads in a vertical and midline position longer compared with the control group Accepted: March 23, 2012
Submitted: June 13, 2011
infants. After training stopped, the training group infants actively moved their heads
forward more often and for larger distances.

Limitations. The experiences outside daily training were not monitored, and the
results may be specific to the experimental setup for infants with typical
development.

Conclusions. Young infants are able to take advantage of postural and movement
experiences to rapidly advance their head control as early as 4 to 6 weeks of postnatal
life. Infant positioning, caregiver handling, and caregiver-infant interactions were
likely contributing factors. This database of comprehensive measures may be useful
in future trials focused on head control in infants with special needs.

Post a Rapid Response to


this article at:
ptjournal.apta.org

July 2012 Volume 92 Number 7 Physical Therapy f 935


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

T
he development of head con- tively weak and poorly controlled. experiences on head control, how-
trol begins during the prenatal Thus, newborns often are provided ever, are rare. The general purpose
period. Head movement is one external support when they are held. of the current study was to address
of the first fetal movements observed By 3 months old of age, infants typ- the need for a longitudinal investiga-
as early as 7 to 8 weeks of postmen- ically maintain their heads in an tion of the effect of postural and
strual age, with increasing move- upright, midline position when held movement experiences on head con-
ment varieties seen by the 11th upright. Clinically, this upright, mid- trol during early infancy.
week.1 Infants display dramatic line position has been considered an
changes in their head control during important basic level of head con- Head control is the first major motor
the first months of their postnatal trol,3,4 which continues to increase milestone on most assessments of
life. Not surprisingly, head control is throughout the first year and early skills.11–14 Head control impair-
critical for a range of early behaviors, beyond.5–9 ments often are cited as an early risk
including those involving vision, oro- factor for future developmental
motor skills, and the trunk and arms. The above description of the “natu- delays.15 For example, head control
For example, a dynamically stable ral” timing of the emergence of head on the Test of Infant Motor Perfor-
head may provide less mechanical control should not be taken as uni- mance (TIMP) in the first months of
perturbation to the trunk and arms versal and immutable. Cross-cultural life is a factor in predicting which
and may support the function of studies on the handling of young children will be diagnosed with
vision.2 Less perturbation and infants by non-Western cultures sug- cerebral palsy by 2 years of age.16
improved use of vision, in turn, may gest that the above descriptions In several studies, infants born pre-
allow for better body control, which, strongly reflect culture norms. One maturely or developing cerebral
in turn, assists infants in learning implication of these studies is that palsy at a later age displayed poorer
more complex behaviors. early head control is influenced by head control compared with
the degree to which infants are pro- infants with typical develop-
Conventional wisdom, based on vided with postural and movement ment.17–22 Delayed head control
studies that typically involve infants opportunities (reviewed in Adolph greatly influences many aspects of
raised in Western cultures, holds that et al10). As outlined below, systemic a child’s life.23–25
a newborn’s neck muscles are rela- investigations of the effect of early
Despite the importance of head con-
trol in early intervention,4 compre-
The Bottom Line hensive studies are limited and are
typically cross-sectional investiga-
tions of infants at term age.17–22,26
What do we already know about this topic? The assessment of head control usu-
ally was conducted as a part of
For infants with typical development, movement experiences play an
larger, more general neuromotor or
important role in motor development. For example, infants provided with
neurobehavioral evaluation.11,13,14
additional advanced postural or movement experiences reach earlier than Therefore, results often were com-
infants provided with additional social experiences. posed of general posture control or
What new information does this study offer?
Available With
This study provides the first longitudinal quantification of the effect of This Article at
postural and movement experiences on the development of head control. ptjournal.apta.org
Very young infants with typical development who were provided with
additional experiences rapidly displayed advanced head control. • eAppendix: Training Activities for
the Training Group (20 Minutes
If you’re a caregiver, what might these findings mean Total per Day)
for you? • eTable: Summary of the Effects
of Postural and Movement
The daily experiences that families provide to their infants during play or Training on Head Control
therapy may influence the emergence of even the earliest skills such as
• Demonstration Video of Infants
head control. Daily play involving a range of positions and movements in the Control Group and the
may influence the motor development of infants with special needs. Training Group During Chair Play

936 f Physical Therapy Volume 92 Number 7 July 2012


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

posture response scores, with few studied, training before the onset of University of Delaware Human Sub-
specifics on head control. Further- a behavior can advance the emer- jects Review Board. Due to schedul-
more, results on specific head con- gence and the quality of the prac- ing conflicts, 2 infants did not con-
trol impairments were not always ticed behavior.27,31,32,37 For example, tinue the study. Twenty-two infants
consistent, such as in the case of infants had earlier onset of crawling were randomly assigned to either a
impairments in pull-to-sit and sitting after receiving 3 weeks of training.31 training group (n⫽11, 6 male and 5
tasks.17–22,26 Six-week-old infants who received 7 female) or a control group (n⫽11, 8
weeks of sitting training displayed male and 3 female). Two infants
The above review points out that longer upright sitting duration imme- were Asian, 2 were African Ameri-
most studies on very young infants diately posttraining.37 We know of can, and the remaining infants were
determined the presence or absence no empirical investigation of the Caucasian.
of milestones, which do not provide training effects on head control in
information on the process by which typical development. Procedures
these infants develop impaired head Infants were seen for testing every
control or what factors lead to In pediatric intervention, postural other week for 3 months, from 1 to 4
impaired head control. Without a and movement training are generally months of age. There was no signif-
detailed understanding of these fac- well-accepted strategies for promot- icant difference in averaged age at
tors, there is little evidence from ing behaviors for young infants.38,39 each testing session between groups
which to build early interventions. Few studies, however, have focused (P⫽.67–.95). There were a total of 9
This is the current situation with on the effect of specific training for testing sessions, including 2 home
respect to treating infants with head head control in young infants, and testing sessions and 7 laboratory test-
control impairments. The current none were comprehensive and lon- ing sessions (see Table for the sum-
study addressed the need for a more gitudinal. As mentioned previously, mary of procedures).
comprehensive and longitudinal cross-culture research on motor
investigation on the development of development supports the use of Home Testing Sessions
head control from 1 to 4 months of physical activities and active han- One experimenter visited infants at
age with multilevel measures. As a dling as a potentially effective inter- their home when they were 1 and
first step, this study focused on build- vention for head control impair- 1.5 months old. During the home
ing a normative database using ments.40 – 44 These studies suggest sessions, a standardized develop-
infants with typical development. that child-rearing practices through mental test (ie, TIMP) was con-
daily formal training (eg, training to ducted. When infants were 2 to 4
If very young infants were receptive sit or walk) or informal handling (eg, months old, the TIMP was con-
to additional postural and movement how infants are carried) affect the ducted in the laboratory (see “Labo-
experiences, this receptiveness onset ages of many motor mile- ratory Testing Sessions” section for
would provide support for the study stones, including the development details), and there were no more
of the effects of these experiences as of head control. The specific pur- home testing sessions.
“training” for infants with poor head pose of the current study, therefore,
control. In general, daily experience was to test the hypothesis that daily The TIMP is a sensitive, predictive,
is critical for the emergence of basic postural and movement experiences and valid tool for detecting the
motor skills in early childhood.7,27,28 would significantly influence the changes and differences in motor
Studies of infants who were develop- development of early head control. performance in young infants.45,46 In
ing typically have shown the effects Results from this study of infants previous studies, the TIMP discrimi-
of postural and movement experi- with typical development could be nated among infants with different
ences on a range of behaviors, useful for future intervention studies degrees of risk for poor motor
including stepping, kicking, reach- of infants at risk for long-term impair- impairments at the time of initial
ing, sitting, crawling, and postural ments in head control. testing,46 – 48 as well as predicted
muscle responses.27,29 –37 These stud- future motor impairments.16,49 –53
ies demonstrated positive effects of Method The TIMP scores also were sensitive
training after as well as before a Participants to detect changes in development in
behavior initially emerged. Twenty-four full-term infants with no infants with typical development
known sensory or motor impair- over a 2-week period.48 In our study,
Training after a behavior is displayed ments were recruited after their par- we used specific TIMP items related
can advance the quality of the prac- ents signed informed parental con- to head control to quantify the devel-
ticed behavior.33,35,36 Although less sent forms approved by the opment of head control in full-term

July 2012 Volume 92 Number 7 Physical Therapy f 937


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

Table.
Summary of Study Proceduresa

Variable Results

Age of infants (mo) 1 1.5 2 2.5 ... 4

Average age in each group (wk) CG: 4.47 CG: 4.57 CG: 8.78 CG: 10.71 CG: 16.73
TG: 4.46 TG: 4.48 TG: 8.69 TG: 10.70 TG: 16.69

Study week 1 1 3 3 5 7 ... 13

Visit number Home 1 Laboratory 1 Home 2 Laboratory 2 Laboratory 3 Laboratory 4 ... Laboratory 7

Location Home Laboratory Home Laboratory Laboratory Laboratory ... Laboratory

Weeks of training 0 (pretraining) 0 (pretraining) 2 2 4 Posttraining Posttraining


(training was
introduced right
after this visit)

Measures Chair play K K K K ... K


B B B B B

Developmental test TIMP TIMP TIMP TIMP ... TIMP


a
CG⫽control group, TG⫽training group, K⫽kinematics, B⫽behavioral coding of head postures and movements, TIMP⫽Test of Infant Motor Performance.

infants who were healthy (see the laboratory testing session. The ponents: chair play and the TIMP.
“Behavioral Coding” section for experimenter checked for proper When infants were 1 and 1.5 months
details). Most of these items have training and answered training- old, only the chair play was per-
been shown to be key characteristics related questions. formed in the laboratory. Starting
for identifying differences between from the session at 2 months of age,
children with and without cerebral Laboratory Testing Sessions the TIMP was added to laboratory
palsy.16 Caregivers brought their infants to procedures.
the Infant Motor Behavior Labora-
During the second home testing ses- tory, Department of Physical Ther- Chair play. Motion capture proce-
sion, caregivers were asked to dem- apy, University of Delaware, every 2 dures (see “Kinematic Data Acquisi-
onstrate training activities intro- weeks while the infants were 1 to 4 tion” section for details) were con-
duced at the end of the first months of age. There were 2 com- ducted during the chair play. Infants
were seated in a customized infant
chair reclined 30 degrees (Fig. 1)
with their trunk secured with a wide
cloth band. Two synchronized video
cameras recorded right front and
bird’s-eye views of the infants for
behavioral coding. Three experimen-
tal conditions were conducted:
no-toy arm-free (NTAF), no-toy arm-
held (NTAH), and toy (TOY). The
NTAF and TOY conditions were
used to test for differences in head
control with and without a midline
toy present. The NTAF and NTAH
conditions were used to test for dif-
ferences in head control during free
and restricted arm movements.
Experimental conditions were ran-
domly ordered across infants and
sessions.
Figure 1.
The setting of chair play. The picture shows the TOY condition.

938 f Physical Therapy Volume 92 Number 7 July 2012


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

There were six 15-second trials for studies27,32,54 using various types of dard low-pass, recursive filter (But-
each condition. For each trial, the training for young infants, including terworth filter) with a cutoff fre-
experimenter held the infant’s head both postural training, requiring quency of 4 Hz reduced high-
in a neutral (midline and upright) increased use of neck, shoulder frequency noise. The position of the
position before the start of a trial and girdle, and trunk muscles, and move- head in 3D space was computed by
then gently released the head right ment training, requiring increased the singular value decomposition
after a trial start. The starting posi- use of arm movements for reaching method.55 A loss of no more than 10
tion of infants’ arms was not (see eAppendix, available at frames consecutively was interpo-
restricted for the NTAF and TOY ptjournal.apta.org, for detailed infor- lated using cubic spline interpola-
conditions. mation on training activities). In our tion. A customized MATLAB pro-
previous studies, infants received the gram (The Mathworks Inc, Natick,
TIMP. The TIMP was conducted 2 types of training separately, either Massachusetts) was used to calculate
and videotaped to quantify the gen- postural or movement training, and the head kinematics. Variables of
eral motor development with spe- began at an older age (8 weeks of age head kinematics were analyzed
cific focus on the development of or older). To provide infants with based on the data concatenated
head control. additional upright experience, care- across trials in each condition.
givers were instructed to carry their
Kinematic Data Acquisition infant for an additional 20 minutes The kinematic dependent variables
A 6-camera (120-Hz) Vicon motion daily with a front carrier (BabyBjörn, were: (1) maximum lateral dis-
capture system (Pulnix TM6701-120 Bredaryd, Sweden) provided to fam- placement (in millimeters), defined
Progressive, Vicon Motion Systems ilies of the training group infants. as the maximum displacement from
Inc, Los Angeles, California) was the midline along the x axis; (2)
used to obtain 3-dimensional (3D) Control Group maximum anterior-posterior dis-
position-time data of the infants’ To control for the increased social placement (in millimeters), defined
head movements. The cameras were interaction that infants in the train- as the maximum displacement along
arranged in a circular fashion, with 2 ing group experienced during train- the y axis; (3) speed of head move-
cameras on each side and 2 in the ing, infants in the control group ment (in millimeters per second),
front and in the back of the infant. received structured, face-to-face defined as the average 3D speed of
Infants were seated within a cali- communication with their caregivers head movement; and (4) variability
brated volume of 160 cm ⫻ 160 for 20 minutes daily for 4 weeks. of head 3D speed, defined as the
cm ⫻ 200 cm. The average range of Caregivers were asked to place their coefficient of variation of head 3D
calibration residuals was no larger infants in a supine position and inter- speed.
than 0.35 mm for the given cali- act with their infants visually and ver-
brated volume. Arrays of three 8-mm- bally without physically contacting Behavioral coding. Videotapes
diameter, retroreflective, nonlinear their infant or presenting toys. were coded for head postures and
markers were placed on the center movements during chair play and
of the forehead. Two single markers Daily activity journals were provided the TIMP. The following behavioral
were placed on the toy in line. The to caregivers in both groups to track coding dependent variables for head
array positions were kept the same the amount of activities performed control behaviors were chosen
between a static trial that defined the each day. The percentage of training based on observations from a pilot
reference position of the arrays and completed for the daily 20-minute study showing that infants fre-
the movement trials. The static trial activities was similar between quently display these behaviors
data were collected with the head groups (training group: X⫽103.2 %, when seated in the infant chair: (1)
and trunk maintained in a neutral SD⫽43.1%; control group: percentage of time in lean, defined
(upright and midline) position. X⫽102.1%, SD⫽10.3%; z⫽⫺0.413, as amount of time infants laterally tilt
P⫽.68). On average, the use of the their heads from the midline more
Training Group front carrier was completed for more than 15 degrees; (2) percentage of
Infants in the training group than 85% of the required time in the time upright, defined as amount of
received postural and movement training group. time infants keep their heads in an
activities provided by their caregiv- upright, midline position and faced
ers for 20 minutes daily for 4 weeks Data Analysis forward; (3) percentage of time in
starting when they were 1 month of Kinematics. Standard kinematic turn, defined as amount of time
age. The specific training activities analysis was conducted for head infants turn their heads to the right
were selected based on our previous movements during chair play. A stan- or left over 15 degrees from the mid-

July 2012 Volume 92 Number 7 Physical Therapy f 939


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

line with their head in an upright, Total Scores of the TIMP


midline position; (4) percentage of Group
time in pop-up, defined as amount of CG
time infants move their head forward 150 * TG

and away from the back of the infant


*
chair with their head in an upright,
midline position; and (5) percentage 125 *
of time with other postures and

Raw Score
movements, defined as amount of
time infants show head postures and 100
movements other than lean, upright,
turn, or pop-up. Each variable was
coded frame by frame (1 frame⫽1/
75
120 second) and normalized with
respect to time for group
comparisons.
50
The total score of the TIMP was the
summed score of all of the 42 items
of the TIMP. The summed score of Baseline During After Training After Training
the head control–related items was Training Phase 1 Phase 2
separated out from the total score to Phase
quantify head control from 1 to 4 Figure 2.
months of age. Selected head control The box plots for the total scores of the Test of Infant Motor Performance (TIMP)
items reflected the infants’ ability to between the control group (CG) and the training group (TG) in each phase. The median
hold their head in midline or an for each group is represented by the single line in the box. The boxes represent the
distribution of data from the 2 middle quartiles. The error bars represent the maximum
upright position without additional and minimum values and the top and bottom quartiles of data distribution. Asterisk
visual or auditory stimulation in dif- signifies a group difference (P⬍.05) based on the Mann-Whitney U test.
ferent body orientations (ie, supine,
prone, and sitting). The following 10
items were selected: item 1 (head in tures and movements coding. Fol- Statistical Analysis
midline in supine); items 15, 16, 17, lowing training, each coder was The focus of the current study was to
and 18 (head control: supported sit- assigned half of the infants to code determine the training effects on the
ting, posterior neck muscle, anterior for head postures and movements. development of head control by test-
neck muscle, lower from sitting); The infants each coder received ing for differences between infants
item 21 (head in midline without were randomly selected from the 2 in the training and control groups.
visual simulation [supine]); item 32 groups. Head control–related items Preliminary analysis did not show
(pull to sit); item 36 (head lift in in the TIMP were scored by a third consistent patterns of significant dif-
prone position); and items 41 and 42 coder for all infants. The interrater ferences among the 3 experimental
(lateral head turning to the right and reliability of TIMP items then was conditions across the 7 experimental
left). tested with the scoring of 2 ran- sessions. That is, certain conditions
domly selected infants coded by a were significantly different for cer-
Reliability of behavioral coding. fourth coder. The reliability tain ages, but there was no clear pat-
The primary experimenter (H-M.L.) ([amount of agreement/(amount of tern of results across conditions and
was not blinded to group assign- agreement ⫹ amount of disagree- sessions. Thus, these 3 conditions
ment. To promote coding score ment)] ⫻ 100) for the coding of head were combined for the final analysis.
reliability, 2 blinded coders were postures and movements was The 7 experimental sessions were
first trained to code with interrater greater than 90%: upright (⬎90%), combined into 3 phases for the final
reliability of greater than 90% for lean (⬎91%), turn (⬎91%), and analysis based on when sessions
variables measuring head postures pop-up (⬎95%). Interrater reliability, occurred relative to the training peri-
and movements using 2 randomly calculated as intraclass correlation od: the baseline phase (1-month-old
selected infants in each group. Two coefficient between 2 coders for the session), the during training phase
random sessions were selected for head control–related items in the (1.5- and 2-month-old sessions when
testing the reliability of head pos- TIMP, was .943. the training was being performed),

940 f Physical Therapy Volume 92 Number 7 July 2012


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

and the after training phase. The TIMP Scores of Head Control–Related Items
after training phase was split into 2 Group
50
phases—after training phase 1 (2.5- CG

and 3-month-old sessions), and after TG


training phase 2 (3.5- and 4-month- *
*
old sessions)—to match the length *
40
of the during training phase. We
combined data from 2 sessions into

Raw Score
phases to decrease the number of
comparisons and ease interpretation 30
in this initial study of head control.
Data from each of the 2 sessions per
phase were averaged. Future studies
20
may build on our results to use non-
combined data.

As the data did not follow a normal 10


distribution and the nonkinematic
data were rank orders or propor- Baseline During After Training After Training
tions, a nonparametric analysis Training Phase 1 Phase 2
(Mann-Whitney U test) was used to Phase
analyze all of the variables for group Figure 3.
differences in each phase. The Mann- The box plots for the summed scores of head control–related items in the Test of Infant
Whitney U test was conducted using Motor Performance (TIMP) in each phase between the control group (CG) and the
SPSS software (SPSS Inc, Chicago, training group (TG). Asterisk signifies a group difference (P⬍.05) based on the Mann-
Illinois) with Pⱕ.05. The effect size Whitney U test.
(r) was calculated using the equa-
tion: Z/(N)1/2, in which Z is the z
score and N is the total number of
group infants in all phases (during 36, and 41: z⫽⫺3.330 to ⫺2.186,
participants.56
training phase: z⫽⫺3.715, P⫽.000, P⫽.001 to .029, r⫽⫺.71 to ⫺.47).
r⫽⫺.79; after training phase 1:
Results z⫽⫺3.909, P⫽.000, r⫽⫺.83; after Behavior Coding of Head
Total Scores of the TIMP
training phase 2: z⫽⫺3.367, Postures and Movements
Results of total scores of the TIMP
P⫽.001, r⫽⫺.72), except for the Results of behavior coding of head
showed that the training group had
1-month-old baseline session. postures and movements showed
higher scores in the during training
that starting from the training phase,
phase (z⫽⫺3.309, P⫽.001, r⫽⫺.71)
In the during training phase (age training group infants had their
and the after training phases (phase
1.5–2 months), the training group heads in a vertical and midline posi-
1: z⫽⫺3.514, P⫽.000, r⫽⫺.75;
infants had significantly higher tion longer compared with control
phase 2: z⫽⫺1.977, P⫽.048,
scores on all 10 individual head group infants. Training group infants
r⫽⫺.42) compared with the control
control–related items except item 1 continued to display advanced head
group (Fig. 2).
(z⫽⫺3.447 to ⫺2.009, P⫽.001 to control in the phases after training
.045, r⫽⫺.73 to ⫺.42) compared stopped. The percentage of the
TIMP Scores of Head Control–
with control group infants. In after types of head postures and move-
Related Items
training phase 1 (age 2.5–3 months), ments infants displayed when they
Results of the TIMP scores of head
the training group had significantly were seated in the infant chair is
control–related items showed that
higher scores in 6 of the 10 items shown in Figure 4A–D.
the training group had higher scores
(items 15–18, 32, and 42: z⫽⫺3.811
during the training period and after
to ⫺2.591, P⫽.000 to .010, r⫽⫺0.81 In the baseline phase (age 1 month,
training stopped compared with the
to ⫺.55). In after training phase 2 Fig. 4A), 3 main behaviors character-
control group. Figure 3 shows that
(age 3.5– 4 months), the training ized both training and control group
training group infants had higher
group had significantly higher scores infants’ head postures and move-
scores on head control–related items
in 5 of the 10 items (items 15, 18, 32, ments in our infant chair: lean,
in the TIMP compared with control

July 2012 Volume 92 Number 7 Physical Therapy f 941


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

A Percentage of Types of Head Postures and Movements B Percentage of Types of Head Postures and Movements
in the Baseline Phase in the During Training Phase
100 100
90 90
80 80 *
70 70
Percentage

Percentage
Lean
60 60
Upright
50 50 Pop-up
40 40 * Turn
Others
30 30
20 20
10 10
0 0
CG TG CG TG
Group Group

C Percentage of Types of Head Postures and Movements D Percentage of Types of Head Postures and Movements
After Training Phase 1 After Training Phase 2
100 100
90 * 90
80 80
70 70

Percentage
Percentage

60 60
50 50
40 40
30 30
*
20 20
*
10 10
0
*
0
CG TG CG TG
Group Group
Figure 4.
The percentage of types of head postures and movements between the control group (CG) and the training group (TG) in the
baseline phase (A), the during training phase (B), and after training phases 1 (C) and 2 (D) when infants were seated in an infant
chair. Asterisk signifies a group difference (P⬍.05) based on the Mann-Whitney U test.

upright, and turn. Infants in both r⫽⫺.44) behaviors compared with In after training phase 2 (age 3.5– 4
groups spent most of the time the control group infants. There months, Fig. 4D), infants in both
(approximately 50%) leaning their were no significant differences groups demonstrated upright, pop-
head in various directions. The per- between groups for turn and pop-up up, and turn as the main behaviors
centage of time infants kept their behaviors during the training phase. characterizing their head postures
heads in upright and turn was the and movements in an infant chair.
second and third highest, respec- In after training phase 1 (age 2.5–3 The training group infants had more
tively. There was no significant months, Fig. 4C), infants in both pop-up (z⫽⫺3.354, P⫽.001,
group difference in any of the coded groups spent the majority of the time r⫽⫺.72) and less turn (z⫽⫺3.243,
head postures and movements dur- with their heads upright, but the P⫽.001, r⫽⫺.69) of their heads in
ing the baseline phase. order of the percentage of time in the chair compared with the control
the other head postures and move- group infants. (See a demonstration
In the during training phase (age ments varied between groups. The video of infants in the control group
1.5–2 months, Fig. 4B), infants in training group infants demonstrated and the training group during chair
both groups spent the majority of less lean (z⫽⫺2.017, P⫽.044, play, available at ptjournal.apta.org.)
the time keeping their heads in r⫽⫺.43) and more pop-up (z⫽⫺ There was no significant difference
upright, lean, and turn, in the order 2.753, P⫽ .006, r⫽⫺.59) of their in lean and upright behaviors
of the highest to lowest percentages, heads in the chair compared with between groups during after training
respectively. The training group the control group infants. There was phase 2.
infants demonstrated less lean no significant difference in upright
(z⫽⫺3.016, P⫽.03, r⫽⫺.64) and and turn behaviors between groups
more upright (z⫽⫺2.043, P⫽.041, during after training phase 1.

942 f Physical Therapy Volume 92 Number 7 July 2012


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

A Maximum Head Displacement Along B Maximum Head Displacement Along


the x Axis the y Axis
Group
250 300
CG
TG
* *
200 * 250

Displacement (mm)
Displacement (mm)

150
200

100
150

50
100

0 Baseline During After After


Training Training Training
Baseline During After After Phase 1 Phase 2
Training Training Training
Phase
Phase 1 Phase 2
Phase
C Average Speed of Head Movements
100

80
*

*
Speed (mm/s)

60

40

20

Baseline During After After


Training Training Training
Phase 1 Phase 2
Phase
Figure 5.
The box plots for head kinematics between the control group (CG) and the training (TG) in each phase: the maximum head
displacement along the x axis (A), the maximum head displacement along the y axis (B), and the average speed of head movements
(C). Asterisk signifies a group difference (P⬍.05) based on the Mann-Whitney U test.

Head Kinematics In the baseline phase (age 1 month), In the during training phase (age
Results showed measurable quantita- the training group infants had 1.5–2 months), there was no signifi-
tive differences in head control abil- smaller maximum head displace- cant difference in any of the mea-
ity between the training and control ment along the x axis (z⫽⫺2.084, sured head kinematics between
groups, especially during the phases P⫽.037, r⫽⫺.44, Fig. 5A) and groups. In after training phase 1 (age
after the training stopped. Head slower average speed of head move- 2.5–3 months), the training group
kinematics with significant differ- ments (z⫽⫺2.097, P⫽.036, r⫽⫺.45, infants had larger maximum head
ences between groups are shown in Fig. 5C) compared with the control displacement along the y axis
Figure 5A–C. group infants. (z⫽⫺2.121, P⫽.034, r⫽⫺.45,

July 2012 Volume 92 Number 7 Physical Therapy f 943


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

Fig. 5B) compared with the control trol. Two major findings from the infants continued to show greater
group infants. In after training phase during training phase are: (1) the head control and more advanced
2 (age 3.5– 4 months), the training speed with which training experi- head behaviors. Interestingly, these
group infants had larger maximum ences led to advances in head con- training effects differed from those
head displacement along the y axis trol and (2) the rapid changes across noted during the training period.
(z⫽⫺2.626, P⫽.009, r⫽⫺.56, multiple measures. The after training phase results sug-
Fig. 5B) and faster average speed of gest that training has a lasting, posi-
head movements (z⫽⫺3.668, First, changes in head control began tive effect on the development of
P⫽.000, r⫽⫺.78, Fig. 5C) compared within weeks of providing infants head control. First, the training
with the control group infants. with additional postural and move- group infants not only achieved a
There was no significant difference ment experiences. Our data showed basic level of head control but also
between groups in the coefficient of that within the first 2 weeks of daily were able to perform basic behaviors
variation of head movement speed in training, the training group infants for longer durations or with larger
all phases. displayed significant differences degrees of movement than the con-
from the control group infants. Spe- trol group infants. For example, in
Discussion cifically, the training group infants after training phase 2, the training
Training Advances Head Control had higher scores for all head group infants had higher scores in 5
in Infants Developing Typically control–related items except item 1, of the 10 head control–related items.
Results from the multiple measures which is an easy item on which most These particular items are of interest,
suggest that enhanced postural and infants in both groups achieved the as a Rasch analysis suggested they
movement experiences (ie, training) highest score during the baseline were more difficult items to per-
advanced the development of head phase.48 Second, training effects form.48 Second, the training group
control. The training group infants were not only rapid but also were infants continued the advanced head
showed more advanced head control seen across multiple measures. Spe- control from the during training
compared with the control group cifically, training altered both the phase and displayed advanced move-
infants during the 4-week training quality and the quantity of infants’ ment strategies while engaging in
period, as well as months after train- performance on the TIMP, as well as activities in the infant chair in the
ing. Moreover, advances displayed how they used their head during weeks and months after training
by training group infants showed an seated play (Fig. 4C and D). Taken stopped. For example, in after train-
interesting additive pattern with together, the training group infants ing phase 1, for the first time, the
advanced behavioral measures in the scored higher on head control items training group infants moved their
during training phase, then in prone, supine, and sitting posi- heads off the back of the chair (eg,
advanced behavioral and spatial tions and during activities that pop-up) longer than the control
kinematics (ie, maximum displace- required active participation. group infants (Fig. 4C). Interestingly,
ment) in after training phase 1, fol- in after training phase 2, although
lowed by advances in behavioral, These changes, combined with the the training group infants had more
spatial, and temporal kinematics (eg, cross-cultural research on caregiver time in pop-up behavior, the control
average speed) in after training handling,10,40 – 44 strongly suggest group infants continued to spend
phase 2. Findings from total TIMP that a young infant’s head and neck more time in turn behavior (Fig. 4D).
scores indicated that the training are neither as muscularly weak nor Pop-up behavior may be an espe-
group infants were more advanced as poorly controlled as traditionally cially interesting measure to follow
in their general motor development thought. Moreover, young infants in future studies, given that it clearly
compared with the control group are able to take advantage of postural shows purposeful intent and
infants. A summary of the results of and movement experiences to requires significant effort. It is impor-
training effects on head control is advance their head control as early tant to note, however, that pop-up
provided in the eTable (available at as 4 to 6 weeks of postnatal life, if behavior may require supported sit-
ptjournal.apta.org). The results of not much earlier. The clinical impli- ting in a reclined seat.
each phase are further discussed in cations will be discussed in a later
the following sections. section. Lastly, kinematic and behavioral
results during the after training
Training Effects in the During Training Effects in After Training phases suggest that the training
Training Phase Phases 1 and 2 group infants continued to gain
Training had an immediate and sig- In the weeks and months after train- strength and advanced control com-
nificant effect on infants’ head con- ing stopped, the training group pared with the control group infants

944 f Physical Therapy Volume 92 Number 7 July 2012


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

in the months after training. For than 20 minutes daily in a prone ing time.” Future studies can now
instance, the training group infants position.60 Based on the training build on these foundation data to
not only held their heads off the back activities in the current study, the develop valid and reliable methods
of the chair for longer durations but training group infants spent an addi- to track the type and duration of
also moved with greater excursions tional 6 minutes in a prone position infant-caregiver interaction both dur-
(Fig. 5B) and moved their heads and 12 minutes in an upright posi- ing and after the daily training
faster than the control group infants tion, which are body positions typi- period.
(Fig. 5C). During the baseline phase, cal of older, more mobile infants.
when infants’ head control was gen- Prone and upright positions would Clinical Implications
erally poor, the faster speed of head be expected to increase the muscle One major principle of early inter-
movements likely resulted from performance and coordination of an vention is to provide activities to
insufficient control to counteract infant’s neck, shoulders, and upper advance behaviors in infants and
gravity or a lack of motivation to trunk; provide a new view of the children at risk for a developmental
maintain an upright position. In after environment; and provide novel ves- delay before the delay is directly
training phase 2, head control in tibular stimulation, all of which have observed. Several recent reviews,
both groups was beyond the basic been associated with increased alert- including a meta-analysis, however,
level such that the training group ness and motor development.61– 68 have not found strong effects of tra-
infants were likely purposefully pro- ditional early intervention strategies
ducing faster movement speeds Caregiver handling. In addition on motor development during
through greater muscle activation to the positions that infants were infancy, preschool age, or school
levels, for example.57 placed in for play activities, the activ- age.69 Authors highlighted the need
ities themselves altered the typical for comprehensive, sensitive mea-
How Early Postural and manner in which caregivers handled sures to quantify both the short-term
Movement Experiences their infants. As stated previously, in training period and longer-term
Advanced Head Control Western cultures, caregivers tend to follow-up period for intervention
Results from the current study pro- hold young infants carefully, sup- programs for functional abilities.38,70
vide empirical evidence that the porting their heads for at least the
emergence of head control can be first months of their life (reviewed in As discussed above, we know of no
advanced through early intensive Adolph et al10). In the current study, longitudinal study with sensitive
postural and movement training. In caregivers of the training group measurements on the effectiveness
addition, these results join those of infants were instructed to perform of interventions focused on head
other studies to strongly suggest that activities that facilitated head control control. The dependent variables
postnatal experience is an important and were encouraged to handle their and training program from the cur-
factor in typical development of infants daily with less passive sup- rent study were specifically chosen
head control.30,35 To better under- port of the head and in a much more to provide a framework for future
stand how our training had rapid and active manner overall. The gains in trials with pediatric populations at
significant effects during the training head control in the current study fit risk for lifelong motor impairments.
period and continued effects months with the cross-cultural research The head control measures in the
after training, 3 training compo- showing active handling advances current study are appropriate for lon-
nents—positioning, handling, and head control. gitudinally tracking the development
infant-caregiver interaction—are dis- and effectiveness of intervention for
cussed next. Infant-caregiver interaction. head control ability during early
Lastly, the training may have influ- infancy. These measures are multi-
Infant positioning. In the during enced infant-caregiver interaction level, which provides a comprehen-
training phase, the training group beyond physical handling. During sive view of infants’ performance of
infants received positioning experi- daily training time, caregivers of the head control, and can be conducted
ences that were advanced for infants training group infants likely encour- on infants as young as 1 month old.
during the first months of their life. aged their infants to actively control Results from the current study sug-
Typically, in Western cultures, their head appropriately. As infants gest these measures successfully
young infants spend much of their developed better head control abil- detected the training effects on head
waking hours in a supine posi- ity, their caregivers likely continued control from general behavioral
tion.58,59 More than 30% of 4-month- to offer additional opportunities to changes to specific kinematic
olds never experienced a prone posi- facilitate advanced head control differences.
tion while awake, and 75% spent less even outside of the prescribed “train-

July 2012 Volume 92 Number 7 Physical Therapy f 945


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

Limitations can build on these findings but will 8 World Health Organization, Multicentre
Growth Reference Study Group. WHO
Given that this is the first compre- need to tailor the methods and Motor Development Study: windows of
hensive, longitudinal study of the hypotheses to the patient popula- achievement for six gross motor develop-
ment milestones. Acta Paediatr Suppl.
training effect on the emergence of tion, age, and level of experience of 2006;450:86 –95.
head control, there are several limi- the cohort of these clinical trials. 9 Woollacott MH, Shumway-Cook A.
tations that need to be considered Changes in postural control across the life
span: a system approach. Phys Ther. 1990;
when generalizing these results and 7:779 – 807.
Both authors provided concept/idea/re-
planning for future studies. First, the search design, writing, and project manage- 10 Adolph KE, Karasik LB, Tamis-LeMonda
experiences outside the daily train- ment. Dr Lee provided data collection and CS. Moving between cultures: cross-
cultural research on motor development.
ing sessions were not monitored. analysis. Dr Galloway provided fund pro- In: Bornstein MH, ed. Handbook of Cross-
Caregivers likely altered the infants’ curement, facilities/equipment, institutional Cultural Development Science. Hillsdale,
liaisons, and consultation (including review NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2009:
daily experiences outside of the for- 61– 88. Domains of Development Across
of the manuscript before submission). The
mal training time, which would be authors thank all of the infants and their Cultures; vol 1.
expected to influence head control. parents for their effort and enthusiasm for 11 Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Tod-
dler Development. 3rd ed. San Antonio,
Such procedural validity will be participation. They also thank many under- TX: Harcourt Assessment Inc; 2006.
important in future studies inter- graduate assistants for their help with data
12 Campbell SK, Girolami GL, Kolobe THA,
collection and processing.
ested in actual intervention dosage. et al. The Test of Infant Motor Perfor-
mance. Chicago, IL: Infant Motor Perfor-
Second, only head behaviors were Preliminary data from this study were pre- mance Scales LLC; 1993.
assessed. The development of head sented at PT 2009: Annual Conference and
13 Folio MR, Fewell RR. Peabody Develop-
Exposition of the American Physical Therapy
control involves not only the head, mental Motor Scales: Examiner’s Manu-
Association; June 10 –13, 2009; Baltimore, al. 2nd ed. Austin, TX: Pro-ED; 2000.
but also the limbs and trunk, as well Maryland. Parts of the manuscript were pre- 14 Piper MC, Darrah J. Motor Assessment of
as sensory-perceptual and cognitive sented at the XVIIth Biennial International the Developing Infant. Philadelphia, PA:
development. It will be necessary to Conference on Infant Studies; March 10 –14, WB Saunders Co; 1994.
investigate the range of related sys- 2010; Baltimore, Maryland; and the North 15 Scherzer AL, Tscharnuter I. Early Diagno-
American Society for the Psychology of Sport sis and Therapy in Cerebral Palsy: A Pre-
tems to fully understand the devel- mier on Infant Developmental Problems.
and Physical Activity (NASPSPA) Annual
opment of head control. Conference; June 10 –14, 2010; Tucson, 2nd ed. New York, NY: Marcel Dekker;
1990.
Arizona.
16 Barbosa VM, Campbell SK, Smith E, Ber-
Third, data were analyzed and baum M. Comparison of Test of Infant
DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20110196
reported as group effects. Individual Motor Performance (TIMP) item
responses among children with cerebral
infants likely responded to the train- palsy, developmental delay, and typical
ing differently. Identifying sub- References development. Am J Occup Ther. 2005;59:
1 Prechtl H. Prenatal and early postnatal 446 – 456.
groups and individual patterns development of human motor behavior. 17 Aylward GP, Hatcher RP, Leavitt LA, et al.
would assist the detection of infants In: Kalverboer A, Gramsbergen A, eds. Factors affecting neurobehavioral
with head control impairments who Handbook of Brain and Behaviour in responses of preterm infants at term con-
Human Development. Norwell, MA: Klu- ceptional age. Child Dev. 1984;55:1155–
often form subgroups based on the wer Academic Publishers; 2001:415– 428. 1165.
distribution and degree of motor 2 Bertenthal B, Von Hofsten C. Eye, head 18 Forslund M, Bjerre I. Neurological assess-
involvement. Fourth, our primary and trunk control: the foundation for man- ment of preterm infants at term concep-
ual development. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. tional age in comparison with normal full-
analysis focused on between-group 1998;22:515–520. term infants. Early Hum Dev. 1983;8:
studies. Secondary analysis of within- 3 Bobath K. A Neurophysiological Basis for 195–208.
group comparisons is equally impor- the Treatment of Cerebral Palsy. 2nd ed. 19 Jeng S, Tsou Yan K, Teng R. Neurobehav-
London, England: William Heinemann ioral development at term in very low
tant. Fifth, the results are specific to Medical Books Ltd; 1980. birthweight infants and normal term
the experimental setup. Typical 4 Kramer JF, Ashton B, Bander R. Training of infants in Taiwan. Early Hum Dev. 1998;
head control in the sitting and semi prone 51:235–245.
head control emerges within multi- positions. Child Care Health Dev. 1992; 20 Palmer PG, Dubowitz LMS, Verghote M,
ple, dynamic environments. Investi- 18:365–376. Dubowitz V. Neurological and neurobe-
gations that include different physi- 5 Gassier J. A Guide to the Psycho-Motor havioural differences between preterm
Development of the Child. New York, NY: infants at term and full-term newborn
cal and social contexts, including a Churchill Livingstone Inc; 1984. infants. Neuropediatrics. 1982;13:183–
range of cultures, are necessary to 6 Gesell A, Thompson H. The Psychology of
189.
fully understand head control. Sixth, Early Growth. New York, NY: The Mac- 21 Mercuri E, Guzzetta A, Laroche S, et al.
millan Company; 1938. Neurologic examination of preterm
this study focused on infants with infants at term age: comparison with term
typical development as a first step. In 7 McMraw MB. The Neuromuscular Matu- infants. J Pediatr. 2003;142:647– 655.
ration of the Human Infant. London,
terms of clinical implications, the United Kingdom: Haffner Publishing; 22 Ricci D, Romeo DM, Haataja L, et al. Neu-
1943. rological examination of preterm infants at
key effects of training will not be term equivalent age. Early Hum Dev.
known until after studying infants 2008;84:751–761.
with special needs. Future studies

946 f Physical Therapy Volume 92 Number 7 July 2012


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Early Intensive Postural and Movement Training in Very Young Infants

23 Leiper CI, Miller A, Lang J, Herman R. Sen- 40 Hopkins B. Culturally determined patterns 55 Soderkvist I, Wedin PA. Determining
sory feedback for head control in cerebral of handling the human infant. Journal of movements of the skeleton using well-
palsy. Phys Ther. 1981;61:512–518. Human Movement Studies. 1976;2:1–27. configured markers. J Biomech. 1993;26:
1473–1477.
24 Mital MA, Belkin SC, Sullivan MA. An 41 Hopkins B, Westra T. Maternal handling
approach to head, neck and trunk stabili- and motor development: an intracultural 56 Rosenthal R. Effect sizes: Pearson’s corre-
zation and control in cerebral palsy by use study. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. lation, its display via the BESD, and alter-
of the Milwaukee brace. Dev Med Child 1988;114:379 – 408. native indices. Am Psychol. 1991;46:
Neurol. 1976;18:198 –203. 1086 –1087.
42 Hopkins B, Westra T. Motor development,
25 Wooldridge CP, Russell G. Head position maternal expectations, and the role of han- 57 Thelen E, Corbetta D, Spencer J. Develop-
training with the cerebral palsied child: an dling. Infant Behav Dev. 1990;13:117– ment of reaching during the first year: role
application of biofeedback techniques. 122. of movement speed. J Exp Psychol Hum
Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1976;57:407– Percept Perform. 1996;22:1059 –1076.
43 Rabain-Jamin J, Wornham WL. Practice
414. and representations of child care and 58 Mildred J, Beard K, Dallwitz A, Unwin J.
26 Cameron EC, Maehle V. Comparison of motor development among West Africans Play position is influenced by knowledge
active motor items in infants born preterm in Paris. Early Development and Parent- of SIDS sleep position recommendations. J
and infants born full term. Pediatr Phys ing. 1993;2:107–119. Paediatr Child Health. 1995;31:499 –502.
Ther. 2006;18:197–203. 44 Super CM. Environmental effects on motor 59 Salls JS, Silverman LN, Gatty CM. The rela-
27 Lobo MA, Galloway JC, Savelsbergh development: the case of “African infant tionship of infant sleep and play position-
GJ. General and task-related experiences precocity.” Dev Med Child Neurol. 1976; ing to motor milestone achievement. Am J
affect early object interaction. Child Dev. 18:561–567. Occup Ther. 2002;56:577–580.
2004;75:1268 –1281. 45 Campbell SK, Kolobe THA. Concurrent 60 Majnemer A, Barr RG. Influence of supine
28 Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott MH. Motor validity of the Test of Infant Motor Perfor- sleep positioning on early motor mile-
Control: Translating Reach Into Clinical mance with the Alberta Infant Motor stone acquisition. Dev Med Child Neurol.
Practice. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippin- Scale. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2000;12:1– 8. 2005;47:370 –376.
cott Williams & Wilkins; 2007. 46 Campbell SK, Kolobe THA, Osten ET, et al. 61 Bly L. The Components of Normal Move-
29 Angulo-Kinzler RM, Ulrich B, Thelen E. Construct validity of the Test of Infant ment During the First Year of Life and
Three-month-old infants can select spe- Motor Performance. Physi Ther. 1995;75: Abnormal Motor Development. Chicago,
cific leg motor solutions. Motor Control. 585–596. IL: Neuro-Developmental Treatment Asso-
2002;6:52– 68. ciation; 1983.
47 Campbell SK, Hedeker D. Validity of the
30 Hadders-Algra M, Brogren E, Forssberg H. Test of Infant Motor Performance for dis- 62 Clark D, Kreutzberg J, Chee F. Vestibular
Training affects the development of pos- criminating among infants with varying stimulation influence on motor develop-
tural adjustments in sitting infants. risk for poor motor outcome. J Pediatr. ment in infants. Science. 1977;196:1228 –
J Physiol. 1996;493:289 –298. 2001;139:546 –551. 1229.
31 Lagerspetz K, Nygard M, Strandvik C. The 48 Campbell SK, Levy P, Zawacki L, Liao P-J. 63 Neal M. Vestibular stimulation and devel-
effects of training in crawling on the Population-based age standards for inter- opmental behavior of the small premature
motor and mental development of infants. preting results on the Test of Infant Motor infant. Nurs Res Rep. 1968;3–5.
Scand J Psychol. 1971;12:192–201. Performance. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2006;18: 64 Fredrickson WT, Brown JV. Posture as a
119 –125.
32 Lobo MA, Galloway JC. Postural and determinant of visual behavior in new-
object-oriented experiences advance early 49 Campbell SK, Kolobe THA, Wright B, Lina- borns. Child Dev. 1975;46:579 –582.
reaching, object exploration, and means- cre JM. Validity of the Test of Infant Motor 65 Gregg CL, Haffner ME, Korner AF. The
end behavior. Child Dev. 2008;79:1869 – Performance for prediction of 6-, 9-, and relative efficacy of vestibular-
1890. 12-month scores on the Alberta Infant proprioceptive stimulation and the
Motor Scale. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;
33 Needham A, Barnett T, Peterman K. A upright position in enhancing visual pur-
44:263–272.
pick-me-up for infant’s exploratory skills: suit in neonates. Child Dev. 1976;47:309 –
early simulated experiences reaching for 50 Barbosa VM, Campbell SK, Sheftel D, et al. 314.
objects using “sticky mittens” enhances Longitudinal performance of infants with 66 Pederson D, TerVrugt D. The influence of
young infants’ object exploration skills. cerebral palsy on the Test of Infant Motor amplitude and frequency of vestibular
Infant Behav Dev. 2002;25:279 –295. Performance and on the Alberta Infant stimulation on the activity of two-month-
Motor Scale. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr.
34 Rovee CK, Rovee DT. Conjugate reinforce- old infants. Child Dev. 1973;44:122–128.
2003;23:7–29.
ment of infant exploratory behavior. J Exp 67 Pin T, Eldridge B, Galea MP. A review of
Child Psychol. 1969;8:33–39. 51 Flegel J, Kolobe THA. Predictive validity of the effects of sleep position, play position,
the Test of Infant Motor Performance as
35 Sveistrup H, Woollacott MH. Practice mod- and equipment use on motor develop-
measured by the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test
ifies the developing automatic postural ment in infants. Dev Med Child Neurol.
of Motor Proficiency at school age. Phys
response. Exp Brain Res. 1997;114:33– 2007;49:858 – 867.
Ther. 2002;82:762–771.
43. 68 TerVrugt D, Pederson D. The effect of ver-
52 Kolobe THA, Bulanda M, Susman L. Pre-
36 Thelen E. 3-month-old infants can learn tical rocking frequencies on the arousal
dicting motor outcome at preschool age
task-specific patterns of interlimb coordi- level in two-month-old infants. Child Dev.
for infants tested at 7, 30, 60, and 90 days
nation. Psychol Sci. 1994;5:280 –285. 1973;44:205–209.
after term age using the Test of Infant
37 Zelazo NA, Zelazo PR, Cohen KM, Zelazo Motor Performance. Phys Ther. 2004;84: 69 Orton J, Spittle A, Doyle L, et al. Do early
PD. Specificity of experience effects on 1144 –1156. intervention programmes improve cogni-
elementary neuromotor patterns. Dev Psy- tive and motor outcomes for preterm
53 Lekskulchai R, Cole J. Effect of a develop-
chol. 1993;29:686 – 691. infants after discharge? A systematic
mental program on motor performance in review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51:
38 Blauw-Hospers CH, Hadders-Algra M. A infants born preterm. Aust J Physiother. 851– 859.
systematic review of the effects of early 2001;47:169 –176.
intervention on motor development. Dev 70 Ketelaar M, Vermeer A, Helders PJM. Func-
54 Heathcock J, Lobo MA, Galloway JC.
Med Child Neurol. 2005;47:421– 432. tional motor abilities of children with cere-
Movement training advances the emer- bral palsy: a systematic literature review of
39 Damiano DL. Rehabilitative therapies in gence of reaching in infants born less than assessment measures. Clin Rehabil. 1998;
cerebral palsy: the good, the not as good, 33 weeks gestational age. Phys Ther. 2008; 12:369 –380.
and the possible. J Child Neurol. 2009;24: 88:310 –322.
1200 –1204.

July 2012 Volume 92 Number 7 Physical Therapy f 947


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by Emma Horton on July 19, 2012
Copyright of Physical Therapy is the property of American Physical Therapy Association and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like