You are on page 1of 12

Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Research report

Influence of excessive sucrose consumption on exploratory behaviour in


rats – Possible implications for the brain reward system
Klaudia Modlinska *, Anna Chrzanowska, Katarzyna Goncikowska, Wojciech Pisula
Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Due to the low cost of production and the strong evolutionary preference for sweet taste in humans, sugar is
Exploration added to many food products. This leads to often involuntary overconsumption of high amounts of sugar. Yet,
Reaction to novelty growing evidence indicates that high-sugar diets impact brain function and impair cognitive ability. It may be
Reward brain system
due to physiological changes in specific regions of the brain or/and maladaptive changes in dopamine signalling
Sucrose addiction
similar to those observed in the etiology of addiction. In our study, rats from the experimental group were kept
Hippocampus
Learning on a feeding protocol involving intermittent access to sucrose solution for eight weeks. Then, the animals un­
derwent a spontaneous exploration test in an experimental arena divided into three zones where stationary and
movable objects were placed. Studying the rats’ exploratory behaviour allowed us to assess the impact of the
sucrose diet on a broad spectrum of behaviours related to the general functioning of the organism in its envi­
ronment. Analyses showed differences in reaction to novelty between different diet groups which had been
placed in different experimental setups. Rats from the sugar-fed group responded to change with more pro­
nounced exploratory behaviours directed at the source of the novel stimuli and the surrounding environment.
These results may indicate a lower reward value of novelty resulting from diminished responsiveness of the
reward system in the sugar-diet group. We have not found evidence for memory and/or learning impairments in
rats on the sugar-rich diet.

1. Introduction dopamine signalling similar to those observed in the etiology of addic­


tion (e.g. [4]). Furthermore, evidence suggests that under certain cir­
Added sugars, mainly sucrose and high-fructose corn syrup, have cumstances, it can lead to sugar dependency (e.g. [4,5]).
become major components of the human diet today. Sugar consumption Data from research on rats provide strong evidence that sucrose
is increasing worldwide (e.g. [1]). This creates a major challenge for consumption can lead to deficits in spatial cognition and reward-
social policy and healthcare systems. The obvious effects of sugar oriented behaviour (for a review, see [6]). These deficiencies were
overconsumption, such as diabetes or obesity, are widely recognised, also observed under conditions of high pre- and postnatal sugar con­
while much less attention is paid to cognitive and emotional changes sumption [7,8]. Only a few days of a hypercaloric diet is sufficient to
resulting from a diet based on excessive sugar intake. trigger spatial-specific deficits in rats [2,9]. On the other hand, some
Due to the low cost of production and the strong evolutionary pref­ studies suggest that diet-induced spatial memory deficits are not present
erence for sweet taste in humans, sugar is added to many food products when discrete spatial cues are placed in the arena [9]. Such results
(including sauces, dairy products, sausage, etc.). This leads to often indicate that hypercaloric diets may impact different aspects of learning
involuntary overconsumption of sugar. Yet, growing evidence indicates and memory and that diet-induced behavioural deficits may become
that high-sugar diets (HSDs) profoundly impact brain function and more apparent when task difficulty and cognitive demands are
impair cognitive processes even in the absence of extreme weight gain or increased. Another study showed that diet-induced obesity resulting
excessive energy intake [2,3]. The most important fact is that highly from excess sucrose intake, but not fat intake, impairs spatial learning
palatable foods (e.g. products containing sucrose) are likely to increase and memory in young animals [10]. Therefore, it is hypothesised that
dopamine release in the brain reward centres. Over time, regular con­ the effect of sucrose on the brain is not directly correlated with excessive
sumption of this type of food may lead to maladaptive changes in caloric intake in general.

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: kmodlinska@wp.pl (K. Modlinska).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114085
Received 2 March 2022; Received in revised form 1 August 2022; Accepted 28 August 2022
Available online 31 August 2022
0166-4328/© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

The cognitive changes observed in subjects on high-sucrose diets weather conditions, or ensuring food security). It involves a broad range
may be triggered by effects on specific areas of the brain. For example, of behavioural phenomena such as reaction to novelty, risk assessment,
the hippocampus, which is involved in spatial, contextual, and episodic arousal, locomotor activity, habituation, memory, etc. Moreover, a
memory formation, is particularly vulnerable to neuropathological novel stimulus (even when it lacks rewarding or biologically beneficial
changes following excessive sucrose consumption (e.g., [6,11–16]). In attributes) acts as a positive reinforcer, highlighting its powerful moti­
animal studies, markers of neuroinflammation, such as microglia acti­ vational properties (e.g., [36,37]). The organism’s inherent interest in
vation measured by IBA-1immunoreactivity and increased hippocampal novelty (the main element of exploratory behaviour) is considered to be
levels of pro-inflammatory cytokines such as interleukin(IL)-1β, IL6 and an evolutionary prerequisite for complex learning, and it guides or­
tumour necrosis factor-α (TNF-α), were observed in rats and mice ganisms toward acquiring adaptive behavioural repertoires [38]. On the
exposed to HSDs [2,13,14]. Furthermore, high-caloric diets have been other hand, high scores on the novelty-seeking scale are considered risk
shown to reduce brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) mRNA factors for several neuropsychiatric disorders, such as addiction and
expression in the hippocampus [7,17]. These studies suggest that the bipolar disorder [39]. The free exploration test we proposed [35] could
mechanisms through which HSDs impair hippocampal neuroplasticity allow us to observe how organisms cope with changes in the environ­
may include increases in pro-inflammatory cytokines, as well as de­ ment in a comprehensive manner, ensuring that the animals’ stress level
creases in neurotrophic factor expression, which may ultimately remains low and taking into account behavioural dynamics over time.
contribute to diet-induced cognitive deficits [18]. In rats, HSDs also led Unlike most behavioural tests, which measure a single behavioural
to a decrease in the level of markers of hippocampal neurogenesis in the parameter, the free exploration test is based on the assumption that
dental gyrus [14] and neuroproliferation markers doublecortin and rodents demonstrate their full behavioural spectrum only in a rich
proliferating cell nuclear antigen (PCNA) immunoreactivity [19]. testing environment that enables observation of more complex behav­
Another area of research on the influence of excessive sugar con­ iours. From the viewpoint of the theory of levels of integration [40], the
sumption on behaviour concentrates on addiction-like changes in the study of exploratory behaviour makes it possible to register changes in
brain (mainly in the dopaminergic system - for a review, see [4]; also [5, behaviour occurring at higher levels of integration, without being
20,21]). Dependency based on activation of physio-behavioural systems limited to measuring behaviours or components of behaviour at a lower
controlling such areas as metabolism, foraging, and eating to maintain level of integration/organization. The general hypothesis underlying
energy homeostasis is sometimes called “natural addiction” in opposi­ this approach is as follows:
tion to “drug addiction”, which activates specific system related to their Changes that occur due to sucrose overconsumption may not be fully
pharmacology [21]. This analogy is based on the propensity of sugar to manifested in elementary dimensions of behaviour and highly struc­
evoke bingeing, withdrawal symptoms, and abstinence-induced moti­ tured cognitive tasks; they may not exceed the registration threshold.
vation to gain access to sucrose (e.g., [4]). This motivation occurring However, the presence of these changes in various areas of the organ­
during the deprivation period is manifested in rats, among other things, ism’s functioning may, as a result of their cumulative effects, lead to
by more frequent lever pressing for sugar [22], increased alcohol con­ changes at higher levels of integration specific for higher forms of
sumption in rats with sugar-bingeing [23], and stronger response to functioning, e.g., behavioural styles, behavioural syndrome, etc.
sugar-associated cues [24]. In addition, the psychostimulant properties Therefore, our research will focus on changes in the overall strategy of
of sugar are evidenced by the observed cross-sensitization with coping with challenges posed by the environment and not on selected
amphetamine [21]. From a behavioural perspective, substance depen­ and highly specific aspects of cognitive and emotional functioning (they
dence may be detected by measuring symptoms of addiction, such as will, however, also be controlled throughout the experiment). What is
tolerance (a gradual decrease in responsiveness to the substance more, this paradigm seems particularly useful for assessing the influence
resulting in an increased demand for that substance to produce the same of sucrose overconsumption on reward processing (incentive value of
effect - [25]) or craving (increased efforts to obtain the substance - [4]). novelty), as well as recognition and memory processing (changes in
It is well known that repeated activation of the reward circuit by experimental settings) associated with the brain reward system and the
abusing a specific substance may lead to neuroplastic changes (i.a. hippocampus, respectively.
reduction in the density of dopamine receptors). It should be noted that To increase the accuracy and reliability of the measurement we
the dopamine system is highly engaged in memory and learning (e.g. decided to provide the test subjects with two levels of novelty as regards
[26,27]). It also plays a crucial role in initiating motor actions, as its complexity. It is well known that animals prefer more complex en­
observed in patients with Parkinson’s Disease, and depletion in dopa­ vironments to simplified or impoverished ones [41–44]. This preference
mine in the striatum is associated with disorganization of behaviour [20, for complexity is most clearly demonstrated when the environmental
28]. What is more, there is evidence that dopaminergic mechanisms are change takes the form of an increase in complexity [43]. In the experi­
critical to the process of cognitive shifting and executive control [27]. mental setting, complexity may be introduced in numerous forms. In
Thus, chronic consumption of this type of food may lead to maladaptive addition to placing novel objects in the experimental arena, we can also
changes in dopamine signalling similar to those observed in the etiology provide animals with movable objects. Movability adds a new dimen­
of addiction [4]. This may be mainly due to a reduction in striatal D2/3 sion to the new object and enables animals to control the novel envi­
receptor density repeatedly observed in diverse populations of drug ronment to a certain degree. In addition, this specific type of novelty
users [29,30], which has also been found in obese subjects [31]. generates sensory stimuli by virtue of its movability, but also as a result
Studies conducted to date indicating adverse changes in cognitive of the organism’s own activity. Reaction to this kind of stimuli may
functioning have focused on cognitive tasks measuring a narrow and differ among individuals with various levels of different psychological
out-of-context range of psychological properties. These included: the properties (e.g. [45]). Therefore, it is possible to hypothesise that the
Water Morris Maze (spatial memory and learning), object recognition expected neuronal and behavioural changes triggered by excessive sugar
test (memory and learning), progressive ratio task (reward processing) - consumption may lead to specific reactions to varying levels of
e.g., [11,31–34]; but: [15,32]. Although these studies show a deterio­ complexity of the novel stimuli.
ration in selected cognitive skills in many cases, they cannot show how Considering the above, the general hypothesis underlying our study
these changes translate into the functioning of an individual in a broader is the expectation that high sugar consumption will affect the func­
sense. In our project, we intend to identify changes in the overall tioning of the reward system, leading to changes in reaction to novelty
behavioural characteristics by using a free exploration protocol [35]. and general exploratory behaviour. Excessive sugar consumption will
Exploration can be defined as behaviour directed toward acquiring in­ induce a tonic, inter-situational increased and persistent desire for
formation about the environment. Among other things, it allows in­ events and stimuli that activate the reward system. The indicators of
dividuals to prepare for the future (e.g., escape from predators, harsh these changes will be behaviours focused on novelty seeking, lack of

2
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

resistance to postponed reinforcements, or high variability in the forms was available to the animals. Sucrose is the most commonly consumed
of activity, combined with an increase in the amount of time spent on the sugar-based sweetener (a disaccharide containing 50 % fructose and 50
exploratory activity. % glucose). The animals were given a 10 % sucrose solution, which was
similar to the sugar dose found in commercially available sweet bever­
H1. Sugar consumption will lead to increased exploration after the ages and in diets with hidden sugars [49,50]. The Control group fol­
introduction of novelty. We hypothesise that increased activity lowed the standard feeding protocol with ad libitum access to plain
directed at the sources of sensory stimulation will be a result of water.
decreased sensitivity to the reward value of novelty due to addiction- Bottles with sweetened water were weighed before and after placing
like changes in the dopamine system (cf. [39,46,47]). them in the rats’ home cages. Analysis of mean values of a weekly su­
H2. Sugar consumption will result in a slower rate of habituation to crose solution consumption using Anova indicated that the amount of
novelty due to learning impairment resulting from excessive sugar sugar water drank by rats increased significantly during the 8-week
consumption having a damaging effect on specific areas of the brain feeding procedure (F(7,35) = 16.474; p < 0.001) - Fig. 1. There were
e.g. the hippocampus (cf. [11,31,32]). no differences in body weight between the groups at the start of the
H3. Excessive sugar consumption will have a different impact on behavioural test (approx. 430 g for an animal).
reactions to various levels of complexity of the novel stimuli. Higher
complexity (a movable object) will lead to more intensive explora­
tion by stimulating the reward system to a greater degree than a non- 2.3. Experimental setup and procedure
movable object and/or due to a higher level of the reward value of
stimulation resulting from the animal’s own activity (cf. [48]). The measurement apparatus and methods were similar to those used
in our previous studies [35,43,51–54]. The experimental chamber
To test these hypotheses, we conducted a two-phase study. First, the (Fig. 2) was a box measuring 800 mm × 600 mm × 800 mm. The
animals from the experimental group were maintained on a feeding chamber was divided into three zones: A, B, and C by two walls running
protocol involving intermittent access to sucrose solution for eight perpendicularly to its longer side. The partition walls between the zones
weeks. The control group was kept in standard conditions. Second, all had triangular openings (120 mm × 140 mm) at the bottom, which
subjects underwent a spontaneous exploration test in an experimental enabled free movement between the chamber parts. There was a hole
arena divided into three zones. The test started with a series of habitu­ curved in the back wall of the chamber that served as an entrance for
ation sessions the purpose of which was to familiarise the animals with animals moving from the transporter into the chamber. The front of the
the experimental setting. Subsequently, novel objects were placed in one chamber was made of transparent plexiglass and it could be lifted to
of the zones, and behavioural reactions to the changed environment obtain full access to the experimental arena. The entire chamber was
were measured. What is more, half of the animals encountered a novel covered with a layer of washable varnish. In zones B and C, we placed
stimulus with increased complexity (i.e. a movable object). We believe tunnels (200 mm × 120 mm × 80 mm) made of hardwood covered
that this procedure allowed us to induce symptoms of sugar dependency, with washable paint. In contrast to the most frequently used
making it possible to assess a broad range of behavioural changes two-dimensional experimental settings, these tunnels provided a com­
resulting from the impact of sucrose on its neural substrates. plex three-dimensional environment. The central zone (A) was left
empty.
2. Methods At the start of each trial, a transporter (a small cylindrical cage –
60 mm in diameter with doors 120 mm high and 100 mm wide) with the
2.1. Animals tested animal inside was placed by the entrance to zone A (Fig. 2) behind
the wall of the experimental chamber. The entrance door was then lifted,
The sample consisted of 49 male Lister Hooded rats. The rats were and it was left open until the end of the trial. The animal was free to stay
bred and housed in the vivarium of the Institute of Psychology, Polish in the transporter or leave it to explore the chamber. The first seven
Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland. At the beginning of the study, the trials were habituation trials during which the apparatus was arranged
rats were approx. 30 days old and weighed approx. 140 g. in the same way. The introduction of novelty (addition of tunnels) took
The rats were housed in groups of 3–4 in Tecniplast© Eurostandard place between trials 7 and 8. The three subsequent trials were conducted
Type IV cages (610 mm × 435 mm × 215 mm) with dust-free softwood with the chamber in this new arrangement (Fig. 3). Each trial was 7 min
granules Tierwohl Super© as bedding. They had ad libitum access to long and was conducted for each animal once a day.
water and standard laboratory fodder (Labofeed H, WP Morawski, Two different types of novelty were introduced in trial 8. In the first
Kcynia, Poland). The day/night cycle was set at 12/12 h (lights-on at setting, the novelty was introduced by adding new tunnels to the
8.00 a.m.). The temperature was maintained at a constant 21–23ºC, and experimental arena. The second setting had the same configuration of
humidity at 45–60 %. Prior to the experiment, the cages were cleaned added tunnels, but the top tunnel was built in a way allowing the rats to
once a week. However, in order to ensure that the experimental pro­
cedure was not disturbed, the cages were cleaned just before the start of
the behavioural test and again after the test was completed.
All the rats were housed, bred and taken care of in accordance with
the Regulation of the Polish Minister for Agriculture and Rural Devel­
opment of 14 December 2016 on laboratory animal care. The experi­
mental procedures were approved by the First Local Committee for
Ethics in Animal Experimentation in Warsaw, Poland, permit #1268/
2021.

2.2. Feeding protocol

Prior to the experiment, the animals followed two different diet


protocols for a period of 8 weeks. One group was given access to sucrose
solution for 2 h per day at the beginning of the circadian cycle’s dark Fig. 1. Sucrose solution consumption (in grams per day) in the sugar-diet group
(active) phase. During the access to the sucrose solution, no plain water prior to the behavioural test.

3
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

Fig. 2. Experimental chamber - frontal view through the transparent front wall.

Fig. 3. Arrangement of objects in the experimental chamber in each experimental setting.

move it like a seesaw (Fig. 3). experimental day (trial 8), two supplementary tunnels were placed in
Setting 1 - Addition of a stationary novel object in the experimental zone C (Fig. 3). The tunnel located on top resembled a seesaw – it was
box (Add group). During the habituation sessions, two tunnels the same structure as in the other configurations, but it was placed on
(200 mm × 120 mm × 80 mm) were present in each of zones B and C. four small pegs (which served as pivots) in the middle and at the end of
They were arranged in the same manner (Fig. 2). On the first experi­ the side walls. The pegs allow for movement only in one direction. This
mental day (trial 8), two supplementary tunnels were placed in zone C meant that when one end of the tunnel was touching the ‘roof’ of the
(Fig. 3). The arrangement of the tunnels in zone B remained unchanged. tunnel underneath it, the other end was raised. The arrangement of the
A total of 27 (12 rats from the sugar diet and 15 rats from the standard tunnels in zone B remained unchanged. A total of 35 (12 rats from sugar
diet) rats were used for this condition. diet and 23 rats from standard diet) rats were used for this condition.
Setting 2 - Addition of a movable novel object in the experimental To avoid the deceptive effect of lateralization or visual-auditory
box (MoveAdd group). During the habituation sessions, two tunnels cues, the novelty was introduced into the right zone (as described
(200 mm × 120 mm×80 mm) were present in each of zones B and C. above - zone C) for half of the rats and in the left zone (zone B) for the
They were arranged in the same manner (Fig. 2). On the first remaining half (mirror image of Fig. 3).

4
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

2.4. Data processing and statistical analyses

We used BORIS software [55] to encode the behaviours on the basis


of the recorded material, which made it possible to define selected be­
haviours and to assess their duration and frequency. We scored the be­
haviours the animals engaged in during the entire experimental trial.
Consequently, we were able to assign specific scores to the time of
separate bouts of behaviours, their frequency, and the total time the
animals spent engaging in a given behaviour. The following variables
were measured: (1) Time spent in the transporter (excluding the latency
to leave the transporter); (2) Time spent in the unchanged zone of the
chamber; (3) Time spent in the changed zone of the chamber; (4) Fre­
quency of moving between the zones (left/right/transporter) of the
chamber; (5) Time spent on contact with the tunnels in the unchanged
zone of the chamber; (6) Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the
unchanged zone of the chamber; (7) Time spent on contact with the
tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber; and (8) Frequency of
contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber.
To enhance the legibility of the results and tables, the habituation
phase has been indicated as H, while the test trials have been indicated Fig. 4. Z-scores for the time spent in the transporter in groups encountering
as T1, T2, and T3, respectively. Novelty (i.e. addition of tunnels) was different types of novelty.
introduced in the first test trial (T1). Different experimental settings (a
movable or stationary tunnel as novel objects) are indicated in the Re­ p = 0.034; T3: t = 3.833; p = 0.005).
sults as a “move” factor.
The first step of the analysis was to establish a reference value for the
3.2. Time spent in the changed zone of the chamber
test trials. The mean value for the habituation trials H5 to H7 was
calculated, and the created variable was labelled as ‘habituation phase’
Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been
(phase H). The next step consisted in calculating the Z-scores for phase H
violated (χ2(5) = 17.884, p = 0.003), so the degrees of freedom were
within the four experimental samples separately. Next, all values from
corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.797).
test trials T1 to T3 were converted to Z-scores on the basis of phase H
The analysis showed an interactive effect of trial and diet: F
statistics (Mean, StdDev). These data were analysed using a General
(2.391,107.604)= 5.010, p = 0.005, Eta2 = 0.100. The following main
Linear Model procedure (GLM), with repeated measurements (H, T1, T2,
effects were also detected: a main effect of diet: F(1,45) = 11.293,
T3) as within-subject factors, as well as diet and setting assignments as
p = 0.002, Eta2 = 0.201, and a main effect of trial: F(2.391,107.604)
between-subject factors. This was followed by PostHoc t-tests with
= 60.220, p < 0.001, Eta2 = 0.572. There was no interaction effect of
Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Differences were
trial, group and diet or an interactive effect of move and diet or an
considered significant for p ≤ 0.05.
interactive effect of trial and move or a main effect of the move.
In addition, partial eta squared (Eta2) obtained in the RM ANOVA
A post hoc analysis showed that rats on different diets demonstrated
were analysed using Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA to compare the effect size of
different patterns of reaction to change in the experimental box – Fig. 5.
different factors under study. Despite some limitations, partial eta
Rats maintained on the standard diet increased the time spent in the
squared (Eta2) is regarded as an effect size measure, which makes it
changed zone in the first test trial compared to the habituation phase
possible to compare the results obtained in different subject groups or
(T1: t = 6.191; p < 0.001). They also spent more time in this zone in the
even studies. Therefore, we decided to use this measure in order to
second (T2: t = 4.315; p < 0.001) and third test trials (T3: t = 6.194;
obtain more detailed information about the effects of experimental
p < 0.001) compared to the habituation phase. There were no differ­
factors on rat behaviour in the experimental box. For non-significant
ences in the time between test trials. However, in rats from the sugar-fed
effects, the Eta2 value was set to “0”.

3. Results

3.1. Time spent in the transporter

The analysis showed an interactive effect of trial and move: F


(3135)= 4.429, p = 0.005, Eta2= 0.090; a main effect of trial: F
(3135)= 14.041, p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.238; and a main effect of move: F
(1,45)= 18.637, p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.293. There was no interaction effect
of trial, move and diet or effect of trial and diet or diet and move. There
was no main effect of diet either.
A post hoc analysis showed that rats from different groups followed
different patterns of reaction to change in the experimental box - Fig. 4.
In the setup with added stationary tunnels, there was a decrease in the
time spent in the transporter in the first test trial (T1: t = 6.151;
p < 0.001). Then the time remained low. In the condition with the added
movable tunnel, there was no change in the time spent in the transporter
(p > 0.05). Differences between the groups were also observed in indi­
vidual trials. Rats from the added stationary tunnel setup spent less time
in the transporter than their counterparts from the added movable Fig. 5. Z-scores for the time spent in the changed zone of the chamber in the
tunnel setup in all test trials (T1: t = 3.935; p = 0.003; T2: t = 3.298; sugar-fed and standard-diet groups.

5
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

group, after an increase of the time spent in the changed zone in the first (3135) = 16.342, p < 0.001, Eta2 = 0.266, a main effect of diet: F(1,45)
test trial (T1: t = 11.382; p < 0.001), there was a decline of the time in = 5.123, p = 0.028, Eta2 = 0.102, and a main effect of move: F(1,45)=
the second test trial (T2: t = 5.414; p < 0.001). Then, the time remained 6.032, p = 0.018, Eta2 = 0.015. There was no interaction effect of trial,
constant in the third test trial. There were also differences in the time group and diet and no interactive effect of diet and move.
spent in the changed zone between the diet groups. Rats from the sugar- A post hoc analysis also showed that rats on different diets demon­
fed group spent more time in that zone than rats from the standard-diet strated slightly different patterns of behaviour – Fig. 7. Rats maintained
group in the first test trial (T1: t = 4.737; p < 0.001). on the standard diet moved between the zones less frequently in the first
test trial (T1: t = 4.351; p < 0.001), and less frequently in the third test
3.3. Time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber trial (T3: t = 5.240; p < 0.001) compared to the habituation phase. The
frequency increased in the second test trial (T2: t = 3.240; p = 0.042)
Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been compared to the first test trial, and again decreased in the third test trial
violated (χ2(5) = 21.726, p < 0.001), so the degrees of freedom were (T3: t = 4.130; p = 0.002). In rats from the sugar-fed group, the fre­
corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.807). quency of moving between the zones decreased in the first test trial (T1:
The analysis showed an interactive effect of trial and diet: F t = 4.008; p = 0.003). Then, the frequency increased in the second test
(2.422,108.987) = 3.947, p = 0.016, Eta2 = 0.081, a main effect of diet: trial (T2: t = 5.238; p < 0.001), and then remained the same in the third
F(1,45) = 24.349, p < 0.001, Eta2 = 0.351, a main effect of move: F trial (p > 0.05). There was a difference in the frequency of moving be­
(1,45) = 11.075, p = 0.002, Eta2 = 0.198, and a main effect of trial: F tween the zones in the third test trial between the diet groups. Rats from
(2.422,108.987) = 12.155, p < 0.001, Eta2 = 0.213. There was no the sugar-fed group move between the zones less frequently than rats
interaction effect of trial, group and diet or effect of trial and group. from the standard-diet group (T3: t = 4.735; p < 0.001).
A post hoc analysis showed that rats on different diets demonstrated A post hoc analysis showed that rats from different conditions also
different patterns of reaction to change in the unchanged zone of the demonstrated different patterns of behaviour – Fig. 8. In the setup with
chamber – Fig. 6. Rats maintained on the standard diet spent less time in added stationary tunnels, there was a decrease in the frequency of
the unchanged zone in the first test trial compared to the habituation moving between the zones in the first test trial (T1: t = 5.923;
phase (T1: t = 4.406; p < 0.001). The time remained low in the subse­ p < 0.001), then an increase in the second test trial (T2: t = 6.317;
quent test trials. However, in rats from the sugar-fed group, time spent in p < 0.001), and again a decrease in the third test trial (T3: t = 5.727;
the unchanged zone in the first test trial was the same as in the habit­ p < 0.001). In the setting with the added movable tunnel, there was no
uation phase (T1: p > 0.05). This time then decreased in the third test change in the test trials compared to the habituation phase (T1:
trial compared to the second trial (T3: t = 4.047; p = 0.002). There were p > 0.05). However, a change between the first and third test trials was
also differences between the diet groups in the time spent in the un­ observed (t = 3.560; p = 0.014), with higher frequency in the third test
changed zone. Rats from the sugar-fed group spent more time in the trial. A comparison of the frequency levels in the individual trials indi­
unchanged zone than rats from the standard-diet group in the first test cated that rats from the added movable tunnels setting moved between
trial (T1: t = 3.716; p = 0.008), and in the second test trial (T2: the zones more frequently than rats from the other setup in the third test
t = 4.228; p = 0.001). trial (T3: t = 5.038; p < 0.001).
In addition, there was a difference between the rats encountering
different types of novelty. In general, rats from the added movable
tunnel setup spent more time in the unchanged zone than rats from the 3.5. Time spent on contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the
other setup (t = 3.328; p = 0.002). chamber

3.4. Frequency of moving between the chamber zones (left/right/ Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been
transporter) violated (χ2(5) = 19.001, p = 0.002), so the degrees of freedom were
corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.822).
The analysis showed an interactive effect of trial and diet: F(3135)= The analysis showed an interactive effect of trial, diet and group: F
8.055, p < 0.001, Eta2 = 0.152; an interactive effect of trial and move: F (2.467,111.015)= 3.926, p = 0.016, Eta2= 0.080; an interactive effect
(3135) = 11.212, p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.199; and a main effect of trial: F

Fig. 6. Z-scores for time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber in the Fig. 7. Z-scores for the frequency of moving between the chamber zones in the
sugar-fed and standard-diet groups. sugar-fed and standard-diet groups.

6
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

experiment – Fig. 9. Rats from the group maintained on the standard diet
spent more time exploring the added stationary tunnels in the first test
trial (T1: t = 4.112; p = 0.008), and that amount of time remained high
in consecutive test trials. A similar pattern was observed in the rats
maintained on the standard diet which encountered the added movable
tunnel (T1: t = 5.550; p < 0.001). There were no differences between
these two groups in the individual trials.
Although in rats maintained on the sugar diet there was also an in­
crease in the time spent on contact with the tunnels in the first test trial
in the setting with stationary tunnels (T1: t = 10.600; p < 0.001), as well
as in the setting with movable tunnels (T1: t = 5.479; p < 0.001), there
were differences in the amount of time between individual trials. Rats
from the group confronted with a stationary tunnel spent more time
exploring the tunnels in the changed zone than rats which encountered a
movable tunnel in the first (T1: t = 4.621; p < 0.001), second (T2:
t = 4.745; p < 0.001), and third test trials (T3: t = 4.743; p < 0.001).
In addition, rats from the sugar-fed group spent more time exploring
Fig. 8. Z-scores for the frequency of moving between the chamber zones in the stationary tunnels than rats from the standard-diet group in the first
groups encountering different types of novelty. (T1: t = 5.855; p < 0.001), and in the second test trial (T2: t = 3.759;
p = 0.028). There were no differences between the diet groups in the
of trial and diet: F(2.467,111.015)= 3.894, p = 0.016, Eta2= 0.080; an see-saw-tunnels setup.
interactive effect of trial and move: F(2.467,111.015)= 3.820,
p = 0.018, Eta2= 0.078; an interactive effect of diet and move: F 3.6. Time spent on contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the
(1,45)= 16.795, p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.272; a main effect of diet: F(1,45)= chamber
9.428, p = 0.004, Eta2= 0.173, a main effect of move: F(1,45)= 15.804,
p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.260, and a main effect of trial: F(2.467,111.015)= The analysis showed only a main effect of trial: F(3135)= 6.713,
62.019, p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.580. p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.130, and a main effect of move: F(1,45)= 14.621,
Post hoc analyses showed differences in reaction to change between p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.245.
different diet groups subject to different conditions during the In general, rats from the added movable tunnel setup spent more

Fig. 9. Z-scores for the time spent on contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber in the sugar-fed and standard-diet groups encountering different
types of novelty.

7
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

time on contact with tunnels in the unchanged zone than rats from the group increased their activity in the zone where the change was intro­
other setup (t = 3.824; p < 0.001). duced in the first test trial, but then the activity decreased and remained
at a low level in the third test trial. On the other hand, subjects from the
3.7. Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the standard-diet group increased their activity after the introduction of
chamber novelty, but the activity remained high until the end of the experiment.
All groups of rats increased their contact with the tunnels in the changed
The analysis showed an interactive effect of trial and diet F(3135)= zone after new tunnels had been introduced. However, rats from the
3.539, p = 0.016, Eta2= 0.073, a main effect of trial: F(3135)= 4.072, sugar-fed group spent more time exploring the stationary tunnels than
p = 0.008, Eta2= 0.083, and a main effect of diet: F(1,45)= 16.102, investigating the movable tunnels across all test trials. In addition, the
p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.264, and a main effect of move: F(1,45)= 6.588, sugar-fed group spent more time exploring the stationary tunnels
p = 0.014, Eta2= 0.128. There was no interaction effect of trial, move compared to the standard-diet group (in the first and second test trials).
and diet or effect of trial and move. No interactive effect of move and In addition, the increase in the duration of contact with the tunnels after
diet was found either. the introduction of novelty was much higher in the sugar-fed group than
A post hoc analysis showed differences between the rats on different in the standard-diet group (in the stationary tunnels setting).
diets in terms of their reaction to change in the experimental box. Rats Differences were also observed in the unchanged zone of the box.
maintained on the standard diet interacted with the tunnels in the un­ Animals from the standard-diet group decreased the amount of time
changed zone in the third test trial less frequently than in the habitua­ spent in this zone after the novelty had been introduced in the other
tion phase (T3: t = 3.576; p = 0.014). There were no differences zone, while their activity remained low in the subsequent trials. Animals
between the first and second test trials or between the second and third from the sugar-fed group maintained the same level of activity in the
trials. In rats maintained on the sugar-rich diet, there were no changes in unchanged zone despite the introduction of novelty in the other zone. In
the frequency of interactions with tunnels in the unchanged zone general, rats from the sugar-fed group spent more time in the unchanged
(p > 0.05) throughout the experiment. The diet groups differed in terms zone than the rats from the standard-diet group (in the second and third
of frequency of contact with the tunnels across individual trials. Rats test trials). Moreover, the frequency of contacts with the tunnels in the
from the sugar-fed group demonstrated a higher frequency than rats unchanged zone was higher in rats from the sugar-fed group (in the
from the standard-diet group in the second test trial (T2: t = 3.610; second and third test trials).
p = 0.011) and the third test trial (T3: t = 3.773; p = 0.006). Rats from different diet groups differed slightly in terms of the fre­
Moreover, rats from the group which encountered the movable quency of moving between the chamber zones. Both groups decreased
tunnels had contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone more the rate of moving between the zones in the first test trial and then
frequently than the rats from the added stationary tunnel setup increased it in the second trial, but the animals from the control group
(t = 2.567; p = 0.014). again decreased the frequency in the third trial, while the rats from the
sugar-fed group maintained the same frequency as in the second test
3.8. Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the trial.
chamber Furthermore, rats from the stationary tunnels setting reduced their
frequency of moving between the zones after the introduction of the
The analysis showed only a main effect of the trial: F(3135)= 7.126, novel tunnels then increased the frequency in the second test trial and
p < 0.001, Eta2= 0.137. There was no interaction effect of trial, group decreased it again in the last test trial. Rats from the movable tunnels
and diet or effect of trial and group or effect of trial and diet. No main setting did not change their behaviour in this respect after the intro­
effects of group or diet were found either. duction of novelty, but the frequency in the third trial was higher than in
A post hoc analysis showed a significant increase in the frequency of the first trial.
interaction with the tunnels in the changed zone in the third test trial There was also a difference in the amount of time spent in the
(T3: t = 4.608; p < 0.001) compared to the habituation phase. There transporter between the experimental setups. Rats from the stationary
were no changes between the first, second and third test trials. tunnels group decreased the amount of time spent in the transporter in
the first trial and generally spent less time in the transporter than the rats
3.9. Effect size analysis from the other setup.
The effect size analysis has shown that the experimental factors
Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA showed (H=22.004, df=6, p < 0.001) that directly linked to the test arena arrangement (Trial and Move) play a
the effects associated with the experimental factors ranked differently, major part in explaining the variability in the rats’ behaviour. This
with the Trial factor ranking first, followed by Move and Diet factors. seems in line with current knowledge about environmental control over
Table 1 shows the median of Eta2 for experimental effects. behaviour. The Diet factor was placed in the third position, with the
median Eta2 = 0.137. The Diet factor, together with the interaction of
3.10. Summary of results Diet and Trial factors, explained more than twenty percent of behaviour
variance (Eta2 = 0.217), which should be considered to be a powerful
Rats maintained on the sugar diet differed substantially from rats effect.
maintained on the standard diet in terms of their reaction to change
introduced in the experimental chamber. Subjects from the sugar-fed 4. Discussion

Table 1 Studying intrinsically motivated exploratory behaviour makes it


Median of Eta2 for experimental effects. possible to formulate hypotheses about the effects of experimental
manipulation on reward-mediated neural pathways. This approach is
Group N Median
based on the assumption that neophilic preferences, which are the main
Trial 8 0.225
feature of exploration, reflect the reward value of novelty [37]. The
Move 8 0.158
Diet 8 0.137 mesolimbic dopamine system plays a major role in the processing of the
Trial x diet 8 0.08 reward value of novelty, as it does for drugs of abuse and other forms of
Diet x move 8 0.0 reinforcement [56]. Novel stimuli are known to excite dopamine neu­
Trial x move 8 0.0 rons [57] and boost signalling in brain regions receiving dopaminergic
Trial x move x diet 8 0.0
input [58]. Therefore, the influence of factors affecting the reward

8
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

system may well be detected in the analysis of reactions to novelty (cf. malfunction in sugar-fed rats. It is also likely that this kind of “broad”
[59]). As mentioned in the Introduction, excessive sugar consumption exploration constitutes, to some degree, a source of reward to the ani­
may lead to changes in the dopamine system similar to those observed in mals and that it is influenced by the same changes in the reward system
drug addiction (e.g. [4]). We hypothesised that the influence of a as the reaction to the object’s novelty. Some support for this point of
sugar-rich diet on the brain reward system may be assessed by analysing view is provided by Ricker and colleagues [67], who claim that a
exploratory behaviour. self-directing choice behaviour can lead to the choice itself becoming a
The results of our study showed differences in reactions to novelty reinforcing factor.
between different diet groups in different experimental settings. It is However, these findings hold true only with regard to the stationary
known that the introduction of novelty into a familiar environment tunnels’ setting. Novelty may also involve changes in the complexity of
triggers a range of investigative behaviours directed at the source of the the environment, with more complex objects eliciting a stronger
novel stimuli [37]. In our experiment, the animals maintained on a exploratory reaction [43,54]. The magnitude and scope of this reaction
sugar-rich diet responded to the change in the experimental box by depend on the degree of change in the environment. It is characterised
spending three times more time exploring the novel stimulus than the by the discrepancy between a single exposure to a stimulus and subse­
animals from the standard-diet group. The amount of time spent in the quent exposures. Therefore, an increase in complexity should lead to
changed zone of the experimental arena was also much higher but increased exploration. In our study, the introduction of a movable tunnel
contrary to the standard-diet group, which spent more time in the did not elicit an enhanced exploratory response in the sugar-fed group,
changed zone throughout the consecutive sessions, the rats from the and the rats’ activity was similar to the activity of the standard-diet
sugar-fed group spent less time in that zone in the next test session and animals. It is possible that the movable object (placed on top of the
maintained this lower level in the third test trial. These results may two-level tunnel structure and moving unexpectedly like a seesaw when
indicate a lower reward value of novelty in the sugar-fed group, which a rat interacted with it or climbed on top of it) may have been a source of
needed to spend more time on exploration in order to reach the same stress. Even though the animals could recognise the stimulus as novel,
level of reward as the other group. This argument is supported by the they may have chosen not to approach or investigate it. Neophobia (fear
fact that the diminished responsiveness of the reward system was of novelty) can suppress neophilia if the degree of novelty is too high or
detected in the pre-test period when the rats kept on the sugar-rich diet if some other factors generate high levels of fear, as demonstrated in the
increased their sugar intake in the course of eight weeks. In addiction theoretical analysis by R. Hughes [37]. It seems that this fear reaction
studies, this reaction is considered to be a sign of tolerance to substances was more pronounced in the sugar-diet group, resulting in the reward
consumed in excessive amounts and cravings (e.g. [25], for a review, see value of novelty being reduced. The reasoning about the fear response to
[4]). This strong reaction to novelty may also suggest a a movable novel object is also supported by the differences in time spent
cross-sensitisation effect of sugar, eliciting a stronger response to the in the starting box between the groups encountering different types of
rewarding stimulus (cf. [60,61]). However, sensitisation often involves novelty. The starting box is a safe, familiar environment, and the time
increased motor activity (e.g. [4]), which was not observed in our study spent in it may be considered a measure of anxiety (cf. [68]). In our
as measured by the frequency of moving between the zones of the study, the rats exposed to novelty in the form of a seesaw tunnel spent
experimental box. Both groups demonstrated similar levels as regards more time in the starting box in all test sessions, which may suggest that
this behaviour until the last test trial when the rats from the this change increased the level of neophobia. However, the fear response
standard-diet group reduced the frequency of moving between the zones hypothesis does not support the earlier assumption about increased
of the experimental arena, but the rats from the sugar-fed group impulsivity in the sugar-fed group. Stressful situations should lead to
exhibited a sustained high frequency of this behaviour. This effect, more impulsive behaviours and higher novelty/sensation seeking in this
however, seems to be consistent with the overall higher demand for group (cf. [69]).
stimulation in the sugar-fed group. Another aspect of neural changes resulting from excessive sugar
Enhanced reactions to the introduction of novelty may also be consumption, which can be discussed by analysing exploratory behav­
explained in terms of heightened impulsivity in the sugar-fed group (cf. iour, is memory processing. Numerous studies point out to the hippo­
[62]). Impulsive behaviours in chronic drug abusers have been found to campus as the region significantly affected by sugar-rich diet (e.g., [6,
be associated with reduced dopamine D2/3 receptors in the striatum (e. 11–16]. It has been suggested that relationships between hippocampal
g. [63]), and a low D2/3 receptor availability was correlated with lower memory processes and neotic preferences may specifically involve
latency to come into contact with the novel object, which is consistent novelty detection in which current experiences are compared with
with higher novelty-reactivity and potentially higher impulsivity [64]. encoded information about past events [70–72]. If such comparisons
Again, these insights point to the dopaminergic system being involved in result in a discrepancy between the stored data and the current event,
establishing differences in behaviour between the different diet groups. the detection of novelty would be registered and addressed with specific
Reaction to novelty is not the only element of exploratory behaviour behaviour [73]. Hippocampus impairment may lead to interference with
that can be empirically observed. As proposed by Berlyne in his classical information comparison and thus with the ability to detect or respond to
work [65], exploration involves not only specific exploration directed novelty [74,75]. Another sign of properly functioning memory is the
towards a specific source of stimulation but also diversive exploration relatively quick pace of habituation to change. Our analyses have not
comprising responses directed at a wide range of stimuli. This type of shown any negative changes in memory processing. Habituation to the
behaviour enables an ongoing revision of mental maps and enhanced experimental setup was similar in animals from both diet groups, as
certainty as to the information obtained from the environment, which measured by the reduction in the time spent investigating the novel
seems to be a crucial adaptive function [66]. In our study, rats from the tunnels in the successive test trials. The rapid decrease in time spent in
sugar-fed group reacted to the introduction of novelty with a high rate of the changed zone of the experimental arena seems to support this
object exploration, but at the same time, they did not reduce the time conclusion. It is not necessarily in conflict with previous findings, as the
spent in the unchanged zone of the experimental box and maintained a memory functions of the hippocampus are mainly associated with its
relatively high rate of contacts with the unchanged tunnels in the test dorsal region, in contrast to the regulation of fear or anxiety-like
trials. This may indicate a higher level of diversive exploration in this response associated with the ventral region of the hippocampus [75].
group, suggesting a higher demand for updated information about the It may be speculated that different regions of the hippocampus respond
environment. Although the neural substrates of this distinctive type of differently to excessive sugar consumption (cf. [76]). However, not all
exploration are yet to be identified, it may be hypothesised that the studies on the effect of sucrose consumption have detected impairments
hippocampal regions involved in memory processing are employed. in all memory functions [77,78]. What is more, some findings indicate
Based on our findings, it is unclear if this behaviour relates to memory that sucrose overconsumption may lead to improved learning in the

9
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

operant bar-pressing task [79], which may be associated with a higher However, it should be noted that by proposing hypotheses about
reward value in sugar-fed subjects. changes in neural functioning we built on the previous extensive liter­
To sum up, the results of our study seem to corroborate previous ature on the subject. We are aware that this approach does not exhaust
findings about the significant influence of intermittent sucrose con­ the subject and requires further research into neural processes and
sumption on the brain reward system. Despite a subtle manipulation of changes involved in excessive sugar consumption.
the feeding protocol, which can be roughly compared to humans eating
a few sweets every day, the observed behavioural changes in the CRediT authorship contribution statement
experimental group seem very significant. As illustrated by effect size
analyses, the diet factor is responsible for over 20 % of the behavioural KM - Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Methodology; Project
variance. All animals were kept in very similar conditions. There were administration; Resources, Supervision; Validation; Writing – original
no differences in the animals’ weight and overall characteristics. It draft; Writing – review & editing; WP - Conceptualization; Data cura­
looks, therefore, that even in the test situation, where the proximate/ tion; Formal analysis; Methodology; Validation; Visualization; Writing –
situational factors play major roles, subjects following the sugar-rich original draft; Writing – review & editing; AC and KG - Conceptualiza­
diet behaved significantly differently compared to their counterparts tion; Investigation. All authors reviewed and accepted the manuscript.
maintained on the standard rat diet. On the other hand, we have not
found any evidence for memory and/or learning impairments in rats on
a sugar-rich diet. It may be suggested that the dosage of sucrose pro­ Declaration of Competing Interest
posed in our feeding scheme was too low or that sucrose consumption
has a limited damaging effect on the hippocampus. Nonetheless, The authors declare no competing interests.
studying exploratory behaviour allows us to assess the impact of a
sucrose-rich diet on a broad spectrum of behaviours related to the Data availability
general functioning of the organism in its environment. Our study has
contributed to the existing body of knowledge about the influence of diet Data will be made available on request.
on cognitive and emotional development.
Yet, the presented above analyses of results have some important Acknowledgments
limitations. Our study aimed to pinpoint the relation between sugar
overconsumption and its behavioural correlates. Further, we attempted This research project was supported by the National Science Centre,
to interpret this result and suggest possible neural mechanisms. Poland – grant No. 2015/19/D/HS6/00781.

Appendix 1. Descriptive data for analysed variables

Trials

H T1 T2 T3

Diet Exp. Condition Mean Stdev Mean Stdev Mean Stdev Mean Stdev

Frequency of moving between the chamber zones


Standard ADD 20.400 2.187 17.733 2.890 20.733 2.865 17.200 3.707
MoveAdded 21.536 3.374 18.565 3.553 18.87 3.152 20.000 3.931
Sugar ADD 19.750 1.545 17.083 1.832 19.917 1.832 18.917 1.443
MopveAdded 18.556 2.185 17.667 2.309 19.75 2.527 20.833 2.406
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber
Standard ADD 7.178 1.068 8.600 2.131 8.267 2.052 8.600 1.724
MoveAdded 7.449 1.569 9.435 3.188 8.957 2.931 9.087 3.356
Sugar ADD 6.917 0.944 7.833 2.552 7.667 1.435 8.750 1.712
MoveAdded 7.444 2.066 8.250 1.658 7.667 2.934 9.750 3.223
Time spent on contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber
Standard ADD 64.795 13.608 140.413 32.392 125.827 22.969 156.588 33.762
MoveAdded 59.337 11.622 154.358 42.258 126.236 42.180 121.450 32.069
Sugar ADD 62.019 5.870 144.61 48.740 121.804 44.846 126.015 23.954
MoveAdded 62.307 9.412 130.749 26.083 92.479 24.701 99.255 26.859
Time spent in the changed zone of the chamber
Standard ADD 111.766 20.659 187.21 35.122 167.045 33.808 197.975 39.986
MoveAdded 102.868 20.377 194.264 41.981 163.389 34.944 163.793 38.668
Sugar ADD 104.241 11.622 182.926 50.334 160.503 26.949 179.519 36.307
MoveAdded 107.185 8.466 174.568 33.82 131.589 29.525 148.020 36.577
Time spent in the transporter
Standard ADD 73.150 6.676 55.018 27.64 54.177 25.573 52.716 24.202
MoveAdded 63.836 19.604 49.943 25.379 43.53 24.185 53.683 27.678
Sugar ADD 82.032 9.782 40.899 10.346 51.839 17.098 56.514 20.581
MoveAdded 80.382 16.921 52.996 20.152 56.511 13.592 63.932 20.596
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber
Standard ADD 7.511 0.958 6.267 1.387 6.733 1.280 5.600 1.682
MoveAdded 8.101 3.300 6.435 2.041 7.000 1.977 7.261 2.200
Sugar ADD 6.389 1.213 6.333 1.614 6.833 2.290 6.583 1.443
MoveAdded 6.694 1.291 6.667 1.670 8.333 1.723 6.917 1.621
Time spent on contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber
Standard ADD 60.774 8.279 49.524 15.589 57.449 17.947 50.298 14.215
MoveAdded 67.623 17.945 45.216 13.505 62.753 19.929 61.409 21.762
Sugar ADD 64.304 5.562 59.125 17.794 59.767 10.726 52.178 9.942
(continued on next page)

10
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

(continued )
Trials

H T1 T2 T3

Diet Exp. Condition Mean Stdev Mean Stdev Mean Stdev Mean Stdev

MoveAdded 59.649 9.541 62.519 17.162 73.202 17.971 53.658 14.851


Time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber
Standard ADD 105.452 7.018 81.414 17.742 94.524 25.874 78.136 14.166
MoveAdded 118.819 25.327 79.374 17.279 100.191 24.397 103.125 29.011
Sugar ADD 103.887 12.496 99.215 30.240 112.452 30.020 90.758 19.918
MoveAdded 99.685 13.345 98.27 26.794 125.517 29.801 93.716 25.249
Time spent in the central zone of the chamber
Standard ADD 133.02 11.135 98.601 25.95 103.692 26.823 88.886 34.661
MoveAdded 131.969 23.068 98.644 42.319 112.854 42.065 100.497 28.440
Sugar ADD 130.427 14.864 95.896 31.172 90.914 22.605 100.066 20.096
MoveAdded 130.826 20.128 90.48 28.395 104.578 37.489 116.273 26.472

References plasticity, and learning, Neuroscience 112 (2002) 803–814, https://doi.org/


10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00123-9.
[18] J.E. Beilharz, N.O. Kaakoush, J. Maniam, M.J. Morris, The effect of short-term
[1] O. Bavyko, M. Bondarchuk, Trends in the development of the world food market,
exposure to energy-matched diets enriched in fat or sugar on memory, gut
Mod. věda 5 (2019) 5–13.
microbiota and markers of brain inflammation and plasticity, Brain, Behav.
[2] J.E. Beilharz, J. Maniam, M.J. Morris, Short exposure to a diet rich in both fat and
Immun. 57 (2016) 304–313, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2016.07.151.
sugar or sugar alone impairs place, but not object recognition memory in rats,
[19] A.C. Reichelt, M.J. Morris, R.F. Westbrook, Daily access to sucrose impairs aspects
Brain, Behav. Immun. 37 (2014) 134–141, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
of spatial memory tasks reliant on pattern separation and neural proliferation in
bbi.2013.11.016.
rats, Learn. Mem. 23 (2016) 386–390, https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.042416.116.
[3] A.C. Reichelt, S. Killcross, L.D. Hambly, M.J. Morris, R.F. Westbrook, Impact of
[20] O. Arias-Carrion, N. Freundlieb, W. Oertel, G. Hoglinger, Adult neurogenesis and
adolescent sucrose access on cognitive control, recognition memory, and
Parkinsons disease, CNSNDDT 6 (2007) 326–335, https://doi.org/10.2174/
parvalbumin immunoreactivity, Learn. Mem. 22 (2015) 215–224, https://doi.org/
187152707783220875.
10.1101/lm.038000.114.
[21] B.G. Hoebel, N.M. Avena, M.E. Bocarsly, P. Rada, Natural addiction: a behavioral
[4] N.M. Avena, P. Rada, B.G. Hoebel, Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and
and circuit model based on sugar addiction in rats, J. Addict. Med. 3 (2009) 33–41,
neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake, Neurosci. Biobehav.
https://doi.org/10.1097/ADM.0b013e31819aa621.
Rev. 32 (2008) 20–39, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.019.
[22] N.M. Avena, K.A. Long, B.G. Hoebel, Sugar-dependent rats show enhanced
[5] A. Jacques, N. Chaaya, K. Beecher, S.A. Ali, A. Belmer, S. Bartlett, The impact of
responding for sugar after abstinence: evidence of a sugar deprivation effect,
sugar consumption on stress driven, emotional and addictive behaviors, Neurosci.
Physiol. Behav. 84 (2005) 359–362, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Biobehav. Rev. 103 (2019) 178–199.
physbeh.2004.12.016.
[6] M.D. Kendig, R.A. Boakes, K.B. Rooney, L.H. Corbit, Chronic restricted access to
[23] N.M. Avena, C.A. Carrillo, L. Needham, S.F. Leibowitz, B.G. Hoebel, Sugar-
10% sucrose solution in adolescent and young adult rats impairs spatial memory
dependent rats show enhanced intake of unsweetened ethanol, Alcohol 34 (2004)
and alters sensitivity to outcome devaluation, Physiol. Behav. 120 (2013) 164–172,
203–209, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.alcohol.2004.09.006.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.012.
[24] J.W. Grimm, A.M. Fyall, D.P. Osincup, Incubation of sucrose craving: effects of
[7] K.W. Lange, J. Hauser, I. Kaunzinger, Y. Nakamura, A. Reissmann, E. Stollberg,
reduced training and sucrose pre-loading, Physiol. Behav. 84 (2005) 73–79,
J. Guo, S. Li, Chronic increase in sugar consumption and visual attention in Wistar
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.10.011.
rats, JFB 3 (2018) 161–167, https://doi.org/10.31665/JFB.2018.3159.
[25] K. McSweeney, E.S. Murphy, B.P. Kowal, Regulation of drug taking by sensitization
[8] E.E. Noble, T.M. Hsu, J. Liang, S.E. Kanoski, Early-life sugar consumption has long-
and habituation, Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 13 (2005) 163–184, https://doi.org/
term negative effects on memory function in male rats, Nutr. Neurosci. 22 (2019)
10.1037/1064-1297.13.3.163.
273–283, https://doi.org/10.1080/1028415X.2017.1378851.
[26] A. Dickinson, J. Smith, J. Mirenowicz, Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental
[9] D.M.D. Tran, R.F. Westbrook, Rats fed a diet rich in fats and sugars are impaired in
incentive learning under dopamine antagonists, Behav. Neurosci. 114 (2000)
the use of spatial geometry, Psychol. Sci. 26 (2015) 1947–1957, https://doi.org/
468–483, https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.468.
10.1177/0956797615608240.
[27] F.H. Previc, Dopamine and the Origins of Human Intelligence, Brain Cogn. 41
[10] N. Jurdak, A.H. Lichtenstein, R.B. Kanarek, Diet-induced obesity and spatial
(1999) 299–350, https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1999.1129.
cognition in young male rats, Nutr. Neurosci. 11 (2008) 48–54, https://doi.org/
[28] R. Cools, Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for l-DOPA
10.1179/147683008×301333.
treatment in Parkinson’s disease, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30 (2006) 1–23,
[11] R. Agrawal, F. Gomez-Pinilla, ‘Metabolic syndrome’ in the brain: deficiency in
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.024.
omega-3 fatty acid exacerbates dysfunctions in insulin receptor signalling and
[29] M. Winterdahl, O. Noer, D. Orlowski, A.C. Schacht, S. Jakobsen, A.K. Alstrup, A.
cognition: Metabolic syndrome and brain, J. Physiol. 590 (2012) 2485–2499,
M. Landau, Sucrose intake lowers μ-opioid and dopamine D2/3 receptor
https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2012.230078.
availability in porcine brain, Sci. Rep. 9 (2019) 1–11, https://doi.org/10.1038/
[12] J.A. Davis, J.R. Paul, L.J. McMeekin, S.R. Nason, J.P. Antipenko, S.D. Yates, R.
s41598-019-53430-9.
M. Cowell, K.M. Habegger, K.L. Gamble, High-fat and high-sucrose diets impair
[30] N.D. Volkow, J.S. Fowler, G.-J. Wang, J.M. Swanson, Dopamine in drug abuse and
time-of-day differences in spatial working memory of male mice, Obesity 28 (2020)
addiction: results from imaging studies and treatment implications, Mol.
2347–2356, https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.22983.
Psychiatry 9 (2004) 557–569, https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001507.
[13] T.M. Hsu, V.R. Konanur, L. Taing, R. Usui, B.D. Kayser, M.I. Goran, S.E. Kanoski,
[31] P.J. Kenny, G. Voren, P.M. Johnson, Dopamine D2 receptors and striatopallidal
Effects of sucrose and high fructose corn syrup consumption on spatial memory
transmission in addiction and obesity, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23 (2013) 535–538,
function and hippocampal neuroinflammation in adolescent rats: Adolescent Sugar
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.04.012.
Intake Impairs Spatial Memory, Hippocampus 25 (2015) 227–239, https://doi.org/
[32] D. Cao, H. Lu, T.L. Lewis, L. Li, Intake of sucrose-sweetened water induces insulin
10.1002/hipo.22368.
resistance and exacerbates memory deficits and amyloidosis in a transgenic mouse
[14] K. van der Borght, R. Köhnke, N. Göransson, T. Deierborg, P. Brundin, C. Erlanson-
model of Alzheimer disease, J. Biol. Chem. 282 (2007) 36275–36282, https://doi.
Albertsson, A. Lindqvist, Reduced neurogenesis in the rat hippocampus following
org/10.1074/jbc.M703561200.
high fructose consumption, Regul. Pept. 167 (2011) 26–30, https://doi.org/
[33] P. Cottone, V. Sabino, L. Steardo, E.P. Zorrilla, Intermittent access to preferred food
10.1016/j.regpep.2010.11.002.
reduces the reinforcing efficacy of chow in rats, Am. J. Physiol. -Regul. Integr.
[15] E. Soares, R.D. Prediger, S. Nunes, A.A. Castro, S.D. Viana, C. Lemos, C.M. De
Comp. Physiol. 295 (2008) R1066–R1076, https://doi.org/10.1152/
Souza, P. Agostinho, R.A. Cunha, E. Carvalho, C.A. Fontes Ribeiro, F. Reis, F.
ajpregu.90309.2008.
C. Pereira, Spatial memory impairments in a prediabetic rat model, Neuroscience
[34] N. Jurdak, R.B. Kanarek, Sucrose-induced obesity impairs novel object recognition
250 (2013) 565–577, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.07.055.
learning in young rats, Physiol. Behav. 96 (2009) 1–5, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
[16] T.J. Xu, A.C. Reichelt, Sucrose or sucrose and caffeine differentially impact
physbeh.2008.07.023.
memory and anxiety-like behaviours, and alter hippocampal parvalbumin and
[35] W. Pisula, K. Modlinska, Protocol for measuring free (Low-stress) exploration in
doublecortin, Neuropharmacology 137 (2018) 24–32, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
rats, BIO-Protoc. 10 (2020), https://doi.org/10.21769/BioProtoc.3485.
neuropharm.2018.04.012.
[36] W. Pisula, Curiosity and Information Seeking in Animal and Human Behavior: a
[17] R. Molteni, R.J. Barnard, Z. Ying, C.K. Roberts, F. Gómez-Pinilla, A high-fat, refined
Review the Literature and Data in Comparative Psychology, Animal Cognition,
sugar diet reduces hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor, neuronal
Ethology, Ontogenesis, and Elements of Cognitive Neuroscience as They Relate to
Animal Inquisitiveness, Brown Walker Press, USA, 2020.

11
K. Modlinska et al. Behavioural Brain Research 436 (2023) 114085

[37] R.N. Hughes, Neotic preferences in laboratory rodents: Issues, assessment and [59] V.D. Costa, V.L. Tran, J. Turchi, B.B. Averbeck, Dopamine modulates novelty
substrates, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31 (2007) 441–464, https://doi.org/10.1016/ seeking behavior during decision making, Behav. Neurosci. 128 (2014) 556–566,
j.neubiorev.2006.11.004. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037128.
[38] Z.Z. Farahbakhsh, C.A. Siciliano, Neurobiology of novelty seeking, Science 372 [60] O. Blomqvist, M. Ericson, D.H. Johnson, J.A. Engel, B. Söderpalm, Voluntary
(2021) 684–685, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abi7270. ethanol intake in the rat: effects of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor blockade or
[39] M. Zuckerman, Behavioral Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking, subchronic nicotine treatment, Eur. J. Pharmacol. 314 (1996) 257–267.
Cambridge University Press, 1994. [61] C.C. Carr, C.R. Ferrario, T.E. Robinson, Intermittent access cocaine self-
[40] W. Pisula, J. Matysiak, Matysiak, Stimulus-seeking behavior, in: G. Greenberg, M. administration produces psychomotor sensitization: effects of withdrawal, sex and
M. Haraway (Eds.), Comparative Psychology: A Handbook, Routledge, USA, 1998, cross-sensitization, Psychopharmacology 237 (2020) 1795–1812.
pp. 198–202. [62] J.W. Dalley, B.J. Everitt, T.W. Robbins, Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down
[41] H.W. Nissen, A study of exploratory behavior in the white rat by means of the cognitive control, Neuron 69 (2011) 680–694, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
obstruction method, Pedagog. Semin. J. Genet. Psychol. 37 (1930) 361–376, neuron.2011.01.020.
https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1930.9944162. [63] D. Morgan, K.A. Grant, H.D. Gage, R.H. Mach, J.R. Kaplan, S.H. Nader, M.A. Nader,
[42] W. Pisula, P.M. Ostaszewski, J. Matysiak, Effects of physical environement and Social dominance in monkeys: dopamine D2 receptors and cocaine self-
social experience on stimulus seeking behaviour and emotionality in rats (Rattus administration, in: J.T. Cacioppo, G.G. Berntson (Eds.), Social Neuroscience,
norvegius), Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 5 (1992) 124–137. Psychology Press, New York, 2013, pp. 243–252.
[43] W. Pisula, K. Modlinska, A. Chrzanowska, K. Goncikowska, Response to novelty [64] P.W. Czoty, H.D. Gage, M.A. Nader, Differences in D2 dopamine receptor
induced by change in size and complexity of familiar objects in Lister-Hooded rats, availability and reaction to novelty in socially housed male monkeys during
a follow-up of 2019 study, Sci. Rep. 11 (2021) 10281, https://doi.org/10.1038/ abstinence from cocaine, Psychopharmacology 208 (2010) 585–592, https://doi.
s41598-021-89289-y. org/10.1007/s00213-009-1756-4.
[44] G.P. Sackett, Response to stimulus novelty and complexity as a function of rats’ [65] D.E. Berlyne, Complexity and incongruity variables as determinants of exploratory
early rearing experiences, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 63 (1967) 369–375, https:// choice and evaluative ratings, Can. J. Psychol. 17 (1963) 274, https://doi.org/
doi.org/10.1037/h0024618. 10.1037/h0092883.
[45] S.M. Reader, Causes of individual differences in animal exploration and search, [66] R.N. Hughes, Intrinsic exploration in animals: motives and measurement, Behav.
Top. Cogn. Sci. 7 (2015) 451–468, https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12148. Process. 41 (1997) 213–226.
[46] S. Cortese, M. Angriman, C. Maffeis, P. Isnard, E. Konofal, M. Lecendreux, [67] J.M. Ricker, J.D. Hatch, D.D. Powers, H.C. Cromwell, Fractionating choice: a study
D. Purper-Ouakil, B. Vincenzi, B.D. Bernardina, M.-C. Mouren, Attention-Deficit/ on reward discrimination, preference, and relative valuation in the rat (Rattus
Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and obesity: a systematic review of the literature, norvegicus), J. Comp. Psychol. 130 (2016) 174–186, https://doi.org/10.1037/
Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 48 (2008) 524–537, https://doi.org/10.1080/ com0000034.
10408390701540124. [68] R.J. Rodgers, A. Dalvi, Anxiety, defence and the elevated plus-maze, Neurosci.
[47] J.T. Nigg, J.M. Johnstone, E.D. Musser, H.G. Long, M.T. Willoughby, J. Shannon, Biobehav. Rev. 21 (1997) 801–810, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(96)
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and being overweight/obesity: 00058-9.
new data and meta-analysis, Clin. Psychol. Rev. 43 (2016) 67–79, https://doi.org/ [69] L. Nower, J.L. Derevensky, R. Gupta, The relationship of impulsivity, sensation
10.1016/j.cpr.2015.11.005. seeking, coping, and substance use in youth gamblers, Psychol. Addict. Behav. 18
[48] F.C. Kidd, B.Y. Hayden, The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity, Neuron 88 (2004) 49–55, https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.18.1.49.
(2015) 449–460, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010. [70] L.L. Johnson, G.P. Moberg, Adrenocortical response to novelty stress in rats with
[49] P. Varsamis, R.N. Larsen, D.W. Dunstan, G.L. Jennings, N. Owen, B.A. Kingwell, dentate gyrus lesions, Neuroendocrinology 30 (1980) 187–192, https://doi.org/
The sugar content of soft drinks in Australia, Europe and the United States, Med. J. 10.1159/000122998.
Aust. 206 (10) (2017) 454–455. [71] R.T. Knight, T. Nakada, A review of EEG and blood flow data, Rev. Neurosci. 9
[50] J. Boulton, K.M. Hashem, K.H. Jenner, F. Lloyd-Williams, H. Bromley, S. Capewell, (1998) 57–70, https://doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.1998.9.1.57.
How much sugar is hidden in drinks marketed to children? A survey of fruit juices, [72] D.G. Mumby, S. Gaskin, M.J. Glenn, T.E. Schramek, H. Lehmann, Hippocampal
juice drinks and smoothies, BMJ Open 6 (3) (2016), e01033. damage and exploratory preferences in rats: memory for objects, places, and
[51] W. Pisula, The Roman high- and low-avoidance rats respond differently to novelty contexts, Learn. Mem. 9 (2002) 49–57, http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/
in a familiarized environment, Behav. Process. 63 (2003) 63–72, https://doi.org/ 10.1101/lm.41302.
10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00032-9. [73] J.E. Lisman, N.A. Otmakhova, Storage, recall, and novelty detection of sequences
[52] W. Pisula, J. Siegel, Exploratory behavior as a function of environmental novelty by the hippocampus: elaborating on the SOCRATIC model to account for normal
and complexity in male and female rats, Psychol. Rep. 97 (2005) 631–638, https:// and aberrant effects of dopamine, Hippocampus 11 (2001) 551–568, https://doi.
doi.org/10.2466/pr0.97.2.631-638. org/10.1002/hipo.1071.
[53] Ł. Tanaś, W. Pisula, Response to novel object in Wistar and wild-type (WWCPS) [74] R.C. Honey, A. Watt, M. Good, Hippocampal lesions disrupt an associative
rats, Behav. Process. 86 (2011) 279–283, https://doi.org/10.1016/j. mismatch process, J. Neurosci. 18 (1998) 2226–2230.
beproc.2010.12.018. [75] E. Save, B. Poucet, N. Foreman, M.C. Buhot, Object exploration and reactions to
[54] W. Pisula, K. Modlinska, A. Chrzanowska, Behavioural response to the spatial and nonspatial changes in hooded rats following damage to parietal cortex
environmental changes of various types in Lister-Hooded male rats, Sci. Rep. 9 or hippocampal formation, Behav. Neurosci. 106 (1992) 447, https://doi.org/
(2019) 1–10, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-42924-1. 10.1037/0735-7044.106.3.447.
[55] O. Friard, M. Gamba, BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software [76] D.M. Bannerman, J.N.P. Rawlins, S.B. McHugh, R.M.J. Deacon, B.K. Yee, T. Bast,
for video/audio coding and live observations, Methods Ecol. Evol. 7 (2016) J. Feldon, Regional dissociations within the hippocampus-memory and anxiety,
1325–1330, https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12584. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28 (2004) 273–283, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
[56] M.T. Bardo, J.L. Neisewander, R.C. Pierce, Novelty-induced place preference neubiorev.2004.03.004.
behavior in rats: effects of opiate and dopaminergic drugs, Pharmacol. Biochem. [77] L.M. Chepulis, N.J. Starkey, J.R. Waas, P.C. Molan, The effects of long-term honey,
Behav. 32 (1989) 683–689, https://doi.org/10.1016/0091-3057(89)90018-X. sucrose or sugar-free diets on memory and anxiety in rats, Physiol. Behav. 97
[57] E.S. Bromberg-Martin, M. Matsumoto, O. Hikosaka, Dopamine in motivational (2009) 359–368, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.001.
control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting, Neuron 68 (2010) 815–834, https://doi. [78] M.D. Kendig, C.S. Lin, J.E. Beilharz, K.B. Rooney, R.A. Boakes, Maltodextrin can
org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.022. produce similar metabolic and cognitive effects to those of sucrose in the rat,
[58] N. Bunzeck, P. Dayan, R.J. Dolan, E. Duzel, A common mechanism for adaptive Appetite 77 (2014) 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.011.
scaling of reward and novelty, Hum. Brain Mapp. 31 (2010) 1380–1394, https:// [79] C. Messier, K. Whately, J. Liang, L. Du, D. Puissant, The effects of a high-fat, high-
doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20939. fructose, and combination diet on learning, weight, and glucose regulation in
C57BL/6 mice, Behav. Brain Res. 178 (2007) 139–145, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
bbr.2006.12.011.

12

You might also like