You are on page 1of 16

Augmentative and Alternative Communication

ISSN: 0743-4618 (Print) 1477-3848 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iaac20

Vocabulary selection in augmentative and


alternative communication

David Beukelman, Jackie McGinnis & Deanna Morrow

To cite this article: David Beukelman, Jackie McGinnis & Deanna Morrow (1991) Vocabulary
selection in augmentative and alternative communication, Augmentative and Alternative
Communication, 7:3, 171-185

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07434619112331275883

Published online: 12 Jul 2009.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 366

View related articles

Citing articles: 22 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iaac20

Download by: [University of Otago] Date: 01 November 2015, At: 06:26


0743-4618/91/0703-0171$3.00/0; Volume 7, September 1991
AAC Augmentative and Alternative Communication
Copyright © 1991 by ISAAC

Vocabulary Selection in Augmentative and


Alternative Communication
David R. Beukelman, Jackie McGinnis, and Deanna Morrow
Barkley Memonal Center, University of Nebraska-Lincoln and Meyer Rehabilitation Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center-Omaha, USA
(D. R. B.), Augmentative Communication Service, Hugh MacMillan Medical Centre, Toronto, Ontario, Canada (J. M.), and Madonna Rehabilitation
Hospital-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska, USA (D. M.)

The purpose of this article is to review those factors influencing vocabulary selection and
retention for persons who use augmentative and alternative communication systems. The vocab-
ulary needs of literate individuals who are able to spell their messages are discussed separately
from the needs of preliterate or nonliterate individuals who are unable to spell and must be supplied
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

with extensive vocabularies to cover their communication requirements. Research regarding


informants and informant tools in the vocabulary selection process is reviewed.

KEY WORDS: augmentative and alternative communication, vocabulary

“Warning, if you expect to see it, you will probably the consultative support that will make this task positive
find it — whether or not it is there.” and efficient;
(Sign posted in the viewing room of a Radiology 4. that the role of some clients in the selection of their
Department). own vocabulary will vary depending upon several fac-
In vocabulary studies we often find what we are tors such as cognitive limitations (e.g., severe brain
looking for. If we look for patterns of frequently occurring injury), lack of familiarity with a new communicative
words that could be included in an augmentative and situation (e.g., geography class), or lack of understand-
alternative communication (AAC) device to enhance ing and acceptance of their communicative impairment;
communication efficiency, we will surely find them. If we 5. that multiple informants including peers are re-
are sensitive to the uniqueness of the individual, we will quired to ensure appropriate and efficient vocabulary
find words that are infrequently used, but are essential selection;
to the accurate communication of the particular mes- 6. that the size of the vocabulary that is selected must
sage. If we search for whole messages that are com- often be limited by physical access, learning, or techni-
municated repeatedly with precise timing requirements, cal constraints; and
we will find at least a few of them. If we assume that 7. that the selection and retention of vocabulary
vocabulary selection is too difficult for persons who use (words and more complete messages) is a critical ele-
AAC systems to participate actively in the process, we ment of each AAC intervention and cannot be ignored
will indeed observe how difficult vocabulary selection is. because it is difficult or frustrating.
In order to protect ourselves from only “seeing the The general purpose of this article is to review those
expected” as we review issues in the vocabulary selec- factors that influence vocabulary selection for and vo-
tion and use of persons who use AAC systems, we must cabulary use by persons who use AAC systems. We
accept several assumptions: begin with a discussion of the most linguistically capable
1. that vocabulary selection is a dynamic process that individuals, those who are able to spell their messages
attempts to capture the changing experiences, inter- and for whom vocabulary is selected to enhance and
ests, and knowledge of the person who uses an AAC supplement their spelling ability. Next, we discuss those
system ; individuals who are unable to spell and must be supplied
2. that vocabulary items are included in AAC systems with extensive coverage vocabularies if they are to
to serve different purposes depending upon many dif- communicate at all. These individuals may be preliter-
ferent variables, including literacy skills, communicative ate children or individuals who are unable to develop
context, or interactive purpose; literacy skills. This article also contains a review of
3. that persons who use AAC systems should be information about informants and tools used in the
involved extensively in the selection and retention of selection of vocabulary specific to the needs of a given
vocabulary for their system and they should be given individual. Finally, several future research directions are
172 Beukelman, McGinnis, and Morrow

provided. Throughout the article, we attempt to describe reduce fatigue for persons with reduced strength and
some of the tools that have been developed by re- endurance.
searchers to support their vocabulary studies. The vocabulary selection strategies used to select
acceleration/timing vocabularies are quite different than
VOCABULARY SELECTION FOR PERSONS those used to select coverage vocabularies, that is
WHO ARE LITERATE vocabularies that provide an individual “with the ability
to communicate most effectively and about the widest
Goals and Guidelines for Vocabulary Selection range of topics, given the limited word set” (Vander-
heiden & Kelso, 1987, p. 196). For the person who is
During written or spoken communication, words are literate, vocabulary selection is primarily intended to
selected by the natural speaker from a vast vocabulary accelerate communication speed and to improve the
store. During the elementary years, children’s spoken prompt delivery of those messages that must be commu-
vocabularies grow by about 3,000 words per year (Nagy nicated in a timely manner to retain their communicative
& Anderson, 1984). The primary task of the speaker is effectiveness, such as etiquette routines, conversation-
to select those words that best convey intended mean- al control phrases, and emergency requests.
ings while being sensitive to a variety of personal,
listener, and style issues. Considering that an adult Selection of Acceleration/Timing Vocabularies
speaks at a rate of 150 to 250 words per minute during
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

conversation (Goldman-Eisler, 1968) the selection of Clinically, four different vocabulary sources have been
vocabulary is a remarkably efficient process. Only oc- used to select acceleration/timing vocabularies for spe-
casionally does the speaker experience the “tip of the cific individuals: (1) the performance of successful AAC
tongue” phenomenon during which the preferred word system users (Beukelman, Yorkston, Poblete, & Nar-
is momentarily illusive. At those times, most speakers anjo, 1984; Yorkston, Honsinger, Dowden, & Marriner,
are able to circumvent the communication breakdown 1989); (2) the past performance of the specific individual
by drawing on a store of synonyms that, while not for whom vocabulary is being selected (Yorkston, Smith,
preferred, allow the speaker to communicate effectively. & Beukelman, 1990); (3) the performance of natural
For the severely communicatively impaired person speakers (Yorkston, Beukelman, Smith, & Tice, 1990;
who utilizes AAC techniques, the goal of accurate, Yorkston, Smith, & Beukelman, 1990); and (4) the re-
efficient communication is the same as for the natural ports of informants (Carlson, 1981), including peers who
speaker with similar interests, abilities, and constraints. understand the communication requirements of the
For all communicators, vocabulary selection is the pro- communicative setting or context. This combination of
cess of selecting the word options that are appropriate sources provides guidance regarding the “core” vocab-
for a particular circumstance (Richards, 1974). How- ulary that is common to many different communicators,
ever, the vocabulary use patterns of persons who use as well as the “fringe” vocabulary that is unique to an
AAC systems may be very different from those de- individual, context, or setting (Yorkston, Dowden, Hon-
scribed above for the natural speaker. Given their slow singer, Marriner, & Smith, 1988).
communication rate, many persons who use AAC sys-
tems purposely produce telegraphic messages in order Vocabulary Use Patterns
to reduce communication time and fatigue.
Ideally, persons who are able to spell spontaneously People Who Use AAC Systems. Of particular in-
are capable of covering their unique communication terest to researchers and clinicians has been the perfor-
needs through letter-by-letter spelling, provided they mance of those persons who are operationally and
have sufficient time to formulate their messages. While socially competent with their AAC systems (Beukelman
letter-by-letter spelling allows for excellent message & Yorkston, 1982; Beukelman et al., 1984; Yorkston,
coverage and permits the use of unique and personal Beukelman, et al., 1990; Yorkston, Smith, & Beukel-
communication forms, letter-by-letter spelling results in man, 1990). These researchers have collected commu-
a written and face-to-face communication process that nication samples over extended time periods from
usually is very slow and may be very fatiguing. During individuals who use a letter-by-letter AAC approach,
conversation, these individuals often have difficulty and therefore, were able to communicate messages of
meeting the timing demands of group and dyadic inter- their choice using word patterns that reflected their own
action in terms of information sharing and discourse turn preferences.
taking. For these individuals, “acceleration/timing” vo- In an early effort to study the commonality or similarity
cabulary is included in their AAC systems in order to in vocabulary use, Beukelman et al. (1984) collected the
accelerate their communication rate and enhance their communication samples that were produced in letter-
communication timing by reducing the number of acti- by-letter fashion with Canon Communicators¹ over a
vations (keystrokes) required as portions of words, en-
tire words, or complete messages are retrieved with a ¹Canon Communicator. Canon Corporation, USA Inc., 1 Canon
brief code. This reduction in keystrokes also serves to Plaza, Lake Success, NY 10042.
Vocabulary Selection 173

14-day period by five persons. It should be noted that Comparison to “Published” Vocabularies
only the messages that were expressed using the Canon
Communicator were included in the communication In an effort to compare and contrast the vocabularies
samples. Those messages that were communicated of AAC users with other published vocabularies, York-
with gesture or other modes were not collected or ston et al. (1988) compared 11 vocabulary lists from
analyzed. The subjects were all young adults from 21 to various fields of investigation, including vocabulary lists
25 years of age. One lived at home, two in rehabilitation taken from natural speech samples, from AAC devices
centers, one in a nursing home, and one in a residence with extensive vocabularies, from AAC system users,
center for individuals with cerebral palsy. One was a from curricular guides, and from English as a second
student in a junior college while the others were not language resource materials with nine vocabulary lists
actively involved in an educational program. No attempt that were included in the AAC systems of adult subjects.
was made to analyze vocabulary use patterns by setting The authors concluded that when the published vocab-
or context. The frequency of word occurrence was ulary lists were compared to the AAC lists, nearly one-
computed using a custom computer program written by third of the words in the AAC lists were not found in even
Poblete and Naranjo for a mainframe computer. In an the largest of the published vocabulary lists. No effort
effort to calculate the commonality of word occurrence, was made by the authors to consider the frequency of
the presence of words in the lists of the individual word occurrence as they compared the various vocab-
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

subjects was compared to the composite list of the 500 ulary lists. The authors suggested that the composite
most frequently occurring words for all of the subjects lists may serve as a source of “core” vocabulary when
combined. The investigator’s selection of a 500 word selecting vocabulary for an individual. They further sug-
composite list was an arbitrary decision. The common- gested the need to develop composite vocabulary lists
ality or overlap scores are reported in Figure 1. For based on “carefully selected groups of augmented com-
example, when comparing the 50 most frequently oc- municators” (p. 189).
curring words produced by Subject 1 with the core
vocabulary list of 50 words, 76% of the words occurred Individualized versus Published Vocabularies
on both lists. Similar comparisons were made for all
subjects using core lists of three different sizes: 50 In order to compare and contrast the relative benefits
words, 200 words, and 500 words. No attempt was of word lists that are individualized to those derived from
made in this study to compare the commonality of words published word lists, Yorkston, Smith, and Beukelman
used across the individual subjects. In addition, the (1990) compared the performance of 10 persons who
authors did not compare the results of this research to used AAC systems to word lists selected from “published”
the vocabulary lists generated by other sources. vocabulary sources. The individuals studied ranged in age
from 13 to 30 years. All used letter-by-letter spelling to
express their messages (the five persons from the
Beukelman et al., 1984, study were included in this
study). Their communication samples were analyzed
using an IBM XT² computer with Microsoft Word³ (Ver-
sion 3.0) word processing program, which generated a
list of all different words according to frequency of
occurrence. A listing of different words was entered into
the Revelations TM4 database, so that the occurrence
of words could be compared across individual lists and
standard source lists.
The published word lists included the following: F—
500 Frequently Occurring Words (Beukelman et al.,
1984); U—Augmented Communicator Composite List
(Yorkston et al., 1988); V—Vois 140 Word List (VOIS
1405, Phonic Ear); S—Standard Composite List (York-

²IBM XT Computer. IBM Corporation, P.O. Box 1328-C, Boca


Raton, FL 33432.
3
Microsoft Word. 16011 NE 36th Way, Box 97017, Redmond, WA
98073-9717.
4
Revelations TM Data Base. Cosmos, Inc. 19530 Pacific Hwy
Figure 1. Congruence between core and individualized word lists for South, Suite 120, Seattle, WA 98188.
5
5 subjects. Comparisons are made for lists of 50, 200, and 500 words Vois 140 Word List. Phonic Ear, 250 Camino Alto, Mill Valley, CA
(Beukelman, Yorkston, Poblete, & Naranjo, 1984). 94941-1466.
174 Beukelman, McGinnis, and Morrow

ston et al., 1988); W—Words+ List (Words+6 AAC De- Yorkston, Smith, and Beukelman (1990) further re-
vices); M—Minspeak Single Words Strategy List.7 ported that individualized word lists are more efficient
The data from this project are illustrated using two of than composite vocabulary lists, if efficiency is defined
the figures reported in the article. Figure 2 shows the as the largest proportion of the total sample repre-
means and standard deviations of the percent of words sented by word lists of limited size. For example, con-
contained in the lists that were used by the ten subjects. sider the case in which an AAC device such as the
Note that when the word lists are ordered from smallest Minspeak TouchTalker8 or the Vois 140 is selected for
(F) to largest (M), there is a consistent decrease in the a linguistically-intact individual, and that person is al-
proportion of the words from the various standard lists lowed to retrieve the vocabulary items using a code
that were actually used. The communication samples without concern for the individualized use patterns. The
produced during an extended 2-week period by AAC learning costs are quite great considering that over
system users were compared to selected standard lists. 70% of the words included in the Minspeak Words
Sixty percent of the words from the 500 frequently Strategy List or the Vois 140 list did not appear in the
occurring words (Beukelman et al., 1984) occurred in samples produced by the 10 individuals. The authors
the communication samples, and only 27.1% of the concluded that:
words from the Minspeak Words Strategy list were ever
used in the communication samples. This is not to suggest that standard (composite) word lists
should be abandoned. On the contrary, our data also sug-
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

In Figure 3, the percentage of the total communication


sample represented by the various vocabulary lists is gests that they are an excellent source of potential words
presented. The individualized lists were those lists pro- to be included in an AAC application. The inclusion of
duced by the person using an AAC system; these lists standard word lists in the memory of an AAC device is a
contained all words that were produced two or more great time saving for augmented communicators and their
times. Words that occurred in the samples a single time facilitators. However, these standard lists must not be
(unique words) were eliminated. The authors concluded taught without careful consideration. Systematic strategies
that when vocabulary lists for linguistically-intact per- are required to eliminate unnecessary or ‘costly’ words from
sons are individually derived based on analysis of com- the standard vocabulary lists as an AAC device is individu-
munication samples of the given individual, a relatively alized for a given client. In some cases, unnecessary words
short list of words can represent a large proportion of need not actually be eliminated from the system. In other
the total communication sample for that person. Thus, cases, codes for useful words can simply be taught first.
if the individual generates communication samples us- Thus, the sequence of teaching codes is individualized
ing a letter-by-letter spelling approach, it is productive rather than curricular with all students proceeding through
to analyze these samples and suggest acceleration the same learning sequence regardless of their needs. (p.
vocabulary that might be useful to this individual. 223.)

Vocabulary Lists Number of Words

Figure 2. The means and standard deviations of the percentage of Figure 3. The percentage of the total sample plotted against the
words contained in the standard lists as defined in the texts that were number of words in the selected standard lists (Yorkston, Smith, &
used by 10 subjects (Yorkston, Smith, & Beukelman, 1990). Word lists Beukelman, 1990). Word lists are identified in the text.
are identified in the text.

6
Words+ Word Lists. Words+, P.O. Box 1229, Lancaster, CA
93534.
7 8
Minspeak: Words Strategy. Prentke Romich Co., 1022 Heyl Rd., TouchTalker. Prentke Romich Co., 1022 Heyl Rd., Wooster, OH
Wooster, OH 44691. 44691.
Vocabulary Selection 175

Multiple-Word Sequences integrated into a variety of different contexts, the accel-


eration vocabulary needed to enhance communication
In a companion article, Yorkston, Beukelman, et al., speed will also probably change across these contexts.
(1990) studied the same communication samples pro- The vocabulary associated with a specific context
duced by ten linguistically intact individuals who used may be implemented in an AAC system in several ways.
AAC systems, as were investigated in Yorkston, Smith, First, these context specific words can be integrated
and Beukelman (1990), but this time they investigated along with other vocabulary items in a comprehensive
the occurrences of multiple word sequences rather than AAC system. These words are available for retrieval as
the single words in the samples. For this work, the are the remainder of words stored in the system. Sec-
researchers developed a custom software program (Vo- ond, the specific context vocabulary can be included in
cabulary Frequency Analyzer) for the IBM XT computer a comprehensive AAC system, but isolated in such a
to calculate the frequency of word occurrence and order way that it is efficient to recall and retrieve, for example,
the word lists. Several conclusions were drawn from this as a single page in a communication book. Finally, a
study. First, word sequences of three to five words in context specific vocabulary may be the only items dis-
length occurred very infrequently in extended commu- played on an AAC technique, such as a communication
nication samples. Second, when the multi-word se- board specifically designed for a Halloween party.
quences were compared across subjects, there was To date, there is little published information of the
very little overlap between any of the subject pairs (no vocabularies produced in specific communication set-
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

overlap for four- or five-word sequences and 14 over- tings or contexts. Although clinical reports exist that
lapping three-word sequences). Finally, the authors describe the letter-by-letter spelling performance of an
concluded that retrieval of two-word sequences that individual in a specific context, few of these reports have
were statistically derived holds promise of increasing found their way into the published literature. For exam-
communication efficiency, because a number of two- ple, Schutz-Muehling and Beukelman (1990) prepared
word sequences occurred quite frequently within the a clinical study in which Schutz-Muehling documented
samples of an individual subject as well as across the several years of use of an augmented writing system to
samples of the ten subjects (115 two-word sequences compensate for her fibrositis, a condition that causes
were produced by at least two different subjects). De- extensive fatigue and pain when she attempts to hand-
spite the findings with respect to the longer multi-word write. During her years as a graduate student and then
sequences, statistically-based multi-word analysis as a professional speech-language pathologist, Schutz-
should not be abandoned altogether. Individuals who Muehling used an abbreviation expansion program
use AAC systems may have need for quick efficient (Productivity Plus9) with a portable IBM compatible
retrieval of messages that have important timing require- computer. In order to guide Schutz-Muehling’s vocabu-
ments. Because these messages are selected to meet lary selection, her written passages (letter-by-letter
the needs of the individual, they are usually suggested spelling) were analyzed using the Vocabulary Frequency
by the AAC user or informants rather than by statistically Analysis software developed by Tice and Beukelman.
derived methods. With this source information and her own knowledge of
In summary, the research that has investigated the the vocabulary requirements of her educational and
vocabulary use of literate individuals who use AAC professional settings, Schutz-Muehling acted as her
systems is still limited. The research that has been own informant. At the time this paper was written, she
completed has focused primarily on a single group of utilized approximately 1900 single and multi-word se-
AAC users who communicated through letter-by-letter quences in her Productivity Plus application. According
spelling. A core of single word and two-word sequences to the keystroke savings data provided by the Productivity
has been identified and appears to be quite commonly Plus system, Schutz-Muehling realizes approximately a
used across AAC users. Few three-, four-, and five-word 40% keystroke savings as compared to letter-by-letter
sequences were used. Neither statistically derived vo- typing. In this case it was possible to individualize her
cabularies used by natural speakers nor vocabularies acceleration vocabulary analysis process. However,
selected for inclusion in AAC devices were highly pre- depending upon the spelling level and the capability of
dictive of the overall vocabulary of AAC users. Appar- the individual, such an analysis may not be possible.
ently, the uniqueness of individual expression requires Written Samples from Students in the Elementary
multiple sources when vocabulary is selected for inclu- Grades. Investigations of the vocabulary use patterns
sion in AAC systems. of nondisabled individuals have been an important source
of information to support the selection of acceleration
Vocabulary Source Lists for Specific Contexts vocabulary. Studies by Hillerich (1978) and Rinsland
(1945) suggested that the written vocabulary of children
AAC Sources. As more and more persons who use in elementary school is very “simple.” Hillerich (1978)
AAC systems are successfully integrated into education-
al, vocational, and recreational settings, the opportunity
9
to study their successful communication performance Productivity Plus. Productivity Software International, Inc., New
will increase. As persons who use AAC systems are York, NY.
176 Beukelman, McGinnis, and Morrow

reported that in the creative writing of children in grades (1989) should be recognized as a preliminary study, as
2 through 6, 100 common words account for 60% of all a limited number of elementary classrooms in one geo-
words used. graphic location participated in this study. In a broad
In an initial effort to document the written vocabularies review of the information known about the vocabulary
used by regular students in elementary classrooms, used by early elementary students, Stemach and Wil-
McGinnis and Beukelman (1989) collected three written liams (1988) published the 2,500 most commonly spo-
assignments (personal letter, science, and language ken words by first grade children. This composite list
arts) from students in 2nd through 6th grade classrooms was compiled from a wide variety of source lists includ-
(68 to 77 students per grade level). The samples for ing spontaneous oral vocabularies of children in primary
each writing task were combined across the students grades (Murphy, 1957); vocabulary for beginning read-
within a grade level and the frequency of occurrence and ing (Johnson, 1971); and vocabulary of first grade chil-
commonality scores were computed. Those words with dren (Hopkins, 1979; Moe, Hopkins, & Rush, 1982).
a frequency of occurrence score of at least 1 per 1,000 In summary, a variety of vocabulary use lists reveals
words of sample were included in a commonality anal- a core group of words (single words and two-word
ysis. An analysis of the communication samples from sequences) that are commonly communicated using
the third, fourth, and fifth grade students, revealed that natural speech and AAC devices. The words in this core
62% of the words occurring in the language arts sample list appear quite frequently and represent a sizeable
also occurred in the letter writing sample. However, only proportion of the total number of words produced during
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

37% of the words in the letter writing sample occurred face to face communication. However, many of the
in the science samples. Although this was a very pre- words used by natural speakers and individuals using
liminary study, variability in vocabulary use across the AAC systems do not appear on these core vocabulary
specific subject areas is suggested. lists. During AAC applications, these “fringe” or unique
Spoken Communication Samples in Elementary words must be predicted by sources other than statisti-
School Classrooms. School talk is very different from cally derived core lists.
home talk. In school, language is not used to meet
immediate needs, as it often is at home, instead children Future Research: Acceleration Vocabularies
talk to communicate with unfamiliar adults and begin to
build a theory of reality and acquire knowledge (Westby, Although the research focusing on the selection of
1985). The differences in vocabulary use between con- acceleration vocabulary for literate individuals is as yet
versational communication and spoken academic com- quite limited, several areas of future research are clear.
munication will be important distinctions as persons who As persons who use AAC systems are more completely
use AAC systems are integrated into regular class- integrated into educational and vocational settings, the
rooms. However, at this time there are no studies re- specific vocabularies used by augmented and natural
ported that directly address this issue. Some work has communicators in these settings will assist in effectively
been completed that addresses the vocabulary used in selecting acceleration vocabularies. Clearly, the vocab-
spoken classroom communication. Trevor and Nelson ulary items produced and the frequency of vocabulary
(1989) gathered and analyzed the vocabulary used by use vary depending upon the setting and context of
students for one entire academic day in regular educa- communication. If AAC systems continue to be biased
tion first, third, and fifth grade classrooms. The voices toward conversational vocabulary, the efficiencies that
of the entire class of students were audio-recorded and could be experienced from message retrieval will not be
transcribed. The results of this study revealed that stu- realized. Preliminary studies suggested that the vocab-
dents in a third grade classroom produced a significantly ulary used in the classroom differs from that used in
greater number of words than did students in the fifth conversation. One would expect these differences to
grade classroom. However, while the total number of become more apparent as an individual progresses
words was greater for third than first grade classrooms, through school. Clearly, the performances of individuals
the difference was not significant. The type-token ratios who are successful students and employees are of
(i.e., the ratio between the number of different and the primary interest. However, study of the natural commu-
number of total words in a sample) were not significantly nicator in these specific settings may also be a valuable
different for the students at the three grade levels. source of vocabulary. Finally, the careful study of the
Vocabulary overlap scores were computed for words vocabulary suggested by expert informants, including
produced by all students in each grade at a frequency the peers and the person using the AAC system, may
of more than 0.5 occurrences per 1,000 words of total contribute to efficient selection of acceleration vocabu-
sample. Forty-nine percent of the words produced by lary in specific communication settings.
the fifth grade students were also produced by the first The differences in vocabulary use by natural commu-
grade students. Forty-six percent of the words produced nicators during written and spoken communication have
by fifth grade students were also produced by the third been documented to some extent. As individuals use
grade students. Forty-six percent of the words produced their AAC systems for social conversation, academic
by the third grade students were also produced by the and vocational face to face communication, and aca-
fifth grade students. This report by Trevor and Nelson demic and vocational written language, the need to
Vocabulary Selection 177

select specific vocabularies for the various communica- nonliterate individual may also be a factor in vocabulary
tion tasks will be clear. AAC devices can now easily selection decisions. Whether or not the individual has
manage multiple vocabularies and the need for re- the time to make up for deficits in reading and writing
search to guide the selection of these vocabularies is ability, given age and educational or vocational status in
apparent. life, may affect whether or not reading and writing vocab-
Preliminary research documents the differences in ulary reflects items to remediate language deficiencies.
communication patterns that occur with age. While the
vocabulary development of children has received con- Clinical Suggestions
siderable research attention, the vocabulary usage of
adolescents and elderly individuals has not. Because of The literature on vocabulary selection for persons
the number of elderly persons who have difficulty com- who are nonliterate contains a strong clinical component
municating through natural speech, the need to under- with a small amount of actual research documentation.
stand more completely the messages that they use and Musselwhite and St. Louis (1988) provided suggestions
the words that they employ is critical in an attempt to for the selection of vocabulary items that promote inter-
meet their communication needs more adequately actions between the person who uses an AAC system
through AAC approaches. and his/her communication partner. They suggested
Although the focus of this article has been on the that initial vocabulary meet the following criteria: (1) be
vocabulary requirements of persons who use AAC sys- of high interest to the individual; (2) have potential for
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

tems, a slightly broader view of vocabulary analysis is frequent use; (3) denote a range of semantic notions or
of interest and that is the identification of the communi- pragmatic functions; (4) reflect “here and now” for ease
cated topics by persons of a variety of ages in a range of learning; (5) have potential for later multi-word use;
of contexts. Often communication interaction is en- and (6) provide ease of production or interpretation.
hanced if the listener is familiar with the topic. This is Additionally, Musselwhite and St. Louis (1988) de-
true for persons who use AAC systems, as well as for scribed “topic setters,” which are generic topics (family,
those with moderate to severe motor speech disorders home, books, etc.) that can promote initiation of com-
or persons with aphasia who continue to communicate, munication interactions. The vocabulary selection pro-
at least in part, through natural speech. cess for the individual with an AAC system should
Finally, as the vocabulary source materials continue include identification of “topic setters” that allow the
to expand, the clinician will benefit from computerized individual to initiate and change topics.
tools to select and manage the vocabularies that are Similarly, a report from the Augmentative Communi-
developed to meet the communication needs of specific cation Service (1984) outlines specific programming
individuals in specific contexts. One effort to develop guidelines for vocabulary selection. This document
such a computerized tool is already underway through states that the guidelines are intended for use with
a joint project including research groups from the Uni- persons who have the potential to be literate but have a
versity of Nebraska, University of Washington, and delayed rate of learning. Three general considerations
Western Michigan University (Beukelman, Yorkston, et are proposed: (1) that functional, motivating vocabulary
al., 1989). address the interests, needs, and wants of the individual
within his/her daily environment; (2) that the character-
VOCABULARY SELECTION FOR PERSONS istics of the AAC system and the effort required by the
WHO ARE NOT LITERATE person to learn new vocabulary be considered; and (3)
that selection of vocabulary enhances the development
The focus of this section is to describe current vocab- of the early concepts of language. For these persons,
ulary selection techniques for persons who are not developmental information can provide access to vocab-
literate. What does it mean to suggest that an individual ulary that promotes language understanding and growth.
is nonliterate? Webster’s Ninth New Collegiate Dictio-
nary (1988) states that the definition of a literate person Review of Current Research
is an educated person, one who can read and write. A
person who is nonliterate then would be one who is not The following review of clinical and research literature
able to read and write. People are nonliterate for any with respect to the vocabulary selection process for per-
number of different reasons that may have a direct sons who are nonliterate focuses on readings that suggest
impact on the vocabulary selection process. For the that developmental information is an important component
person who is nonliterate due to severe cognitive and/or of vocabulary selection, and readings that take a more
language disability, developing literacy may not be a environmental approach. Each of these approaches has
realistic goal. However, persons may be judged to be merit and provides information that is important for per-
nonliterate simply because they have not been given the sons who are nonliterate who may or may not be able to
necessary access to educational opportunities that de- achieve their own individual literacy level.
velop and enhance literacy. In this case, the development Vocabulary selection for nonliterate persons who use
of vocabulary items that support literacy and language AAC systems can be broken down into two strategies.
development may be warranted. Finally, the age of the One relies on developmental information, while the
178 Beukelman, McGinnis, and Morrow

other, a behavioral-remedial approach, focuses on en- could be useful in; or (4) of no value, when designing an
vironmental considerations (Karlan & Lloyd, 1983). initial expressive vocabulary. Results showed none of
While the developmental theorists rely on normal lan- the 84 items to be rated “of no value” and the remaining
guage data, they do not suggest that vocabulary selec- items were rated in the “useful” or “could be useful”
tion should ignore the environments for which category. Social validation of checklists or word lists can
appropriate messages are needed for effective commu- determine the perceived appropriateness of these items
nication. Strict adherents to a behavioral-remedial ap- for different populations of nonspeaking persons.
proach would advocate that developmental information Reichle, Williams, and Ryan (1981) in their criteria
is limiting when used for older children and adults be- for selecting initial signs, suggested that in addition to
cause it can limit their access to communicative oppor- the functional utility of the signs (i.e., value to both
tunities. However, there may be a time when a mesh of speaker and listener, frequent use, reinforcement value,
the two models is needed, given the age and ability to generalizability), the cognitive representational level of
develop literacy for persons using AAC systems. Next, the sign is important for most individuals. Blau (1983)
this paper examines vocabulary selection strategies suggested that to develop an adequate initial lexicon,
from the two different perspectives. clinicians must understand both the components of
language development and, most importantly, the func-
Developmental Perspective tional needs and capabilities of the nonspeaker.
It is important from the developmental perspective to
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

Clinical and Theoretical Issues. Several authors look at the literature on the development of lexical style.
have written about the importance of using a normal Evidence exists to document the lexical styles of chil-
developmental model when choosing the initial vocab- dren (Bloom, Lightbown, & Hood, 1975; Nelson, 1973).
ulary to use with language disordered children who are These styIes a rereferential/hominal and expres-
natural speakers rather than AAC system users. Hol- sive/pronominal. A predominantly referential style con-
land (1975) outlined several contexts for language ther- tains a high proportion of object word use, and a
apy and suggested vocabulary words that could be used predominately expressive style contains a high propor-
as teachable content within these contexts. These con- tion of stereotyped phrases (Nelson, 1973). These dif-
texts for therapy were to: (1) use child language as a ferent learning styles have an obvious impact on the
model; (2) incorporate events that are both rewarding vocabulary selection process for persons who are de-
and important for the child; (3) promote an active, dy- veloping language and literacy skills.
namic communicative exchange; and (4) stress objects Thus, the developmental model for initial vocabulary
and events that are part of the here and now. From these selection uses normal child language acquisition data
contextual premises, the author generated a list of 35 as a framework for choosing which words to teach first.
vocabulary selections that could be taught in a language However, both Holland (1975) and Lahey and Bloom
therapy situation. These words included nouns, adjec- (1977) stressed the functional nature of communication.
tives, verbs, personal pronouns, and polar concepts. Additionally, persons who advocate the use of environ-
Lahey and Bloom (1977) expanded the ideas of Hol- mental inventories suggested that a developmental base
land (1975) by analyzing the 35 vocabulary words ac- for vocabulary selection is appropriate to make the items
cording to form and content. They added the following meaningful for the nonspeaker (Carlson, 1981). It could
two additional stipulations: (1) words that are difficult to be that most models utilize developmental underpin-
demonstrate nonlinguistically should be presented after nings in functional, meaningful, contexts. It need not be
those concepts that can be more readily illustrated; and a question of either/or, when a mesh of the two princi-
(2) frequency of word use determines whether or not the ples can best serve the vocabulary selection needs of
word is functional for the child to learn. nonspeaking persons. All of the authors who use the
Fristoe and Lloyd (1980) incorporated the ideas of developmental model in determining an initial vocabu-
Holland (1975) and Lahey and Bloom (1977) in planning lary pool also stressed the fact that their lists were by no
an initial expressive sign vocabulary for persons with means appropriate for use with every student and that
severe disabilities. Using the work of the previous au- the lists are best used as a guide to planning.
thors, Fristoe and Lloyd developed a list of 70 initial sign Spoken Language of Preschool Children. Infor-
vocabulary selections. To meet the previous criteria, the mation regarding vocabulary use patterns among non-
70 vocabulary items chosen had to reflect data from literate persons is being obtained from the study of
normal child language development, be important to disabled and nondisabled, speaking preschool children
children, stress interactive communication, emphasize in home and school settings. In an ongoing research
the use of objects present in the here and now, be easily effort, M. Fried-Oken (personal communication, July,
understood, be of functional use, and be related to 1990) is examining vocabulary selection for AAC sys-
content categories. tems of young children with cerebral palsy. To develop
Karlan and Lloyd (1983) tested the social validity of vocabulary sources for these children, parents of able-
the initial sign lexicon proposed by Fristoe and Lloyd bodied children and parents and clinicians of children
(1980). Judges were asked to rate the vocabulary items with cerebral palsy are asked to prepare a list of 100
as one of the following: (1) essential to; (2) useful in; (3) words that their children would or do use to communi-
Vocabulary Selection 179

cate effectively and efficiently on a daily basis. Sponta-


neous language samples are also collected from the
speaking children. Preliminary results suggest that a list
of 37 words was common across all sources. This list
fits into the semantic categories first proposed by Lahey
and Bloom (1977) and then used by Fristoe and Lloyd
(1980) in the initial sign lexicon articles. The categories
include: negation, recurrence, existence, actions on
objects, actions locating objects or self, attributes or
descriptions of objects, and substantives (M. Fried-
Oken, personal communication, July, 1990).
An initial study was completed by Beukelman, Jones,
and Rowan (1989) who audio-recorded and transcribed
the spoken communication samples of six nondisabled
preschool students (3 years-10 months to 4 years-9 Composite List Size
months) in three different classrooms. Using a new
generation of Vocabulary Frequency Analyzer Software10 Figure 4. Percentage of total words produced that are accounted for
that was developed for the Macintosh11 computer to ana-
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

by the most frequently used words in the composite sample of


lyze the frequency of occurrence of individual single- preschool children (Beukelman, Jones, & Rowan, 1989).
word and multi-word sequence, these samples were
analyzed in individual and composite form.
Communication samples of approximately 3,000 words
were collected from the six subjects. The number of
different words per sample ranged from 404 to 468
words. Two hundred and fifty words occurred with a
frequency of at least 0.5 occurrence per 1,000 words of
the composite communication sample for the six sub-
jects. The proportion of the total sample represented by
sublists of increasing size is shown in Figure 4. A review
of this figure reveals that 45.1% of the total sample was
represented by the 25 most frequently occurring words,
60% by the 50 most frequently occurring words and 85%
of the total language sample by the 250 most frequently
occurring words. When these results are compared to
the samples generated by other individuals during con- 25 50 75 100 125 150 175 200 225 250
versation and writing, it is apparent that the vocabulary Frequency of Occurrence (Composite List)
used by these young children in the preschool setting
was much more limited than that used by the adults and Figure 5. The commonality scores for composite sublists of fre-
children described by Vanderheiden and Kelso (1987). quently occurring words (Beukelman, Jones, & Rowan, 1989).
The commonality score with which the six subjects
used the 250 most frequently occurring words in the
composite sample is reported in Figure 5. Because six They only studied six children from three classrooms for
a limited period of time. Much more study is required to
subjects were included in the study, a commonality
describe successful communicative participation in the
score of “6” indicates that each of the subjects used the
word with at least a 0.5 occurrence per 1,000 words of preschool classroom.
sample frequency. A commonality score of six occurred
Environmental Perspective
for the 25 most frequently occurring words in the com-
posite sample indicating that all of the subjects used
these twenty-five words in their samples. A review of the In the behavioral-remedial model, the environment
and the child’s activities are the basis for choosing
figure indicates that the most frequently occurring 100
words of the composite sample were used by at least 5 vocabulary items. Strict adherents to this model de-em-
of the 6 children. The research by Beukelman, Jones, phasize normal developmental data, because they are
often misused when applied to populations of older
and Rowan (1989) represents a very preliminary effort.
children, adolescents, and adults. This is especially true
of older adults with acquired severe communication
10
Frequency Analyzer Software. Barkley Memorial Center, Univer-
disorders, such as aphasia. The environmental inven-
sity of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE 68583-0732. tory that is associated with this model can aid in deter-
11
Macintosh Computer. Apple Computer, Inc., 20525 Mariana mining frequency of word occurrence, individual prefer-
Ave., Cupertino, CA 95014. ence, functional utility of words across situations, and
180 Beukelman, McGinnis, and Morrow

basic human needs (Karlan & Lloyd, 1983; Nietupski & 74-year-old man with Broca’s aphasia. One component
Hamre-Nietupski, 1979). of this system was a word dictionary arranged topically
An environmental inventory can be conducted by around Mike’s favorite conversational themes. The first
simply observing and recording the daily events of the step in the vocabulary selection process for this word
severely communicatively impaired person. Several forms dictionary was conducting an exhaustive inventory of
are available for this type of inventory. The person(s) these conversational themes, as well as the specific
who complete this inventory should script the activities words pertaining to each theme. A second step then
of the severely communicatively impaired person in all took place in which Mike edited the themes and words
appropriate environmental settings and specific sub- collected during the inventory phase. Thus, the final
environments. Carlson (1981) stresses that an environ- product was a word dictionary containing messages that
mental inventory must document how the nonspeaker Mike, rather than the informants, felt were important.
both participates in and observes each environmental It is also worth noting that commercially available
activity. The author states, “By discriminating between “word dictionaries” such as the Daily Communicator12
observatory and participatory events, it was possible to have been developed specifically for the adult popula-
gain a better picture of the child’s actual experiences tion. Although the method of selection of the words and
within the area rather than the adult’s perception of the messages in such products are not reported in the
experiences” (p. 42). The special equipment, physical literature, they can and do act as “core” vocabularies for
positioning, other persons, and materials that are pres- adults with aphasia.
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

ent in the environment should be documented. From this


environmental inventory, vocabulary words that would Case Study Research
enhance the communication interaction can be listed.
This pool of vocabulary words can then be reduced to a Case study research focusing specifically on the vo-
smaller list of words that can be managed by an AAC cabulary selection process is severely limited. Yorkston
system. et al. (1989) used a combination of vocabulary selection
Mirenda (1985) described an ecological inventory tools to generate vocabulary for a 36-year-old woman
derived from a special education curriculum to select with physical challenges and limited literacy skills. They
vocabulary for persons who are nonliterate. This vocab- used environmental inventories, communication diction-
ulary selection technique is based on an ecological aries (written documentation of the communicative ex-
inventory strategy for curriculum development (Brown changes between the individual and her communication
et al., 1980) and is broken down into six phases: (1) partners), and a review of four vocabulary lists to estab-
determine the current and subsequent school and com- lish a pool of vocabulary words. These four lists included
munity environments in which a severely handicapped the Karlan and Lloyd (1983) list, the most frequently
student is required to function; (2) conduct detailed, used words of Canon communicators (Beukelman et al.,
on-site analyses of the communication requirements of 1984), the Picsyms Dictionary (Carlson, 1984) and the
nonhandicapped persons during actual activities in those word list from the Vois 140.5
environments; (3) conduct on-site handicapped student From this vocabulary selection process, a list of 240
inventories related to the same activities and environ- messages was developed. At a later date, two additional
ments; (4) conduct discrepancy analyses to determine words were added bringing the list total to 242. Of these
the communication deficits and needs of handicapped 242 items, 218 or 90% were single words and 24 were
students; (5) interview significant others regarding com- whole phrases. When comparisons were made be-
munication needs; and (6) create instructional adapta- tween the subject’s final word list and the four standard
tions (such as picture communication books) to address lists, none of the lists contained all of the words that the
those needs. This vocabulary selection technique en- subject had in her list. If any one of these lists had been
sures that students will have the relevant vocabulary used as the only source for this individual’s word list, the
they need to communicate in a variety of natural, age- vocabulary that she needed to communicate would not
appropriate environments (e.g., with the counter person have been available. However, a larger standard list of
at a fast-food restaurant, friends at the mall, librarian at words may still not contain all of the words that subjects
school, supervisor at work, etc.). Vocabulary is specific would use in their own word lists. In essence, bigger
and individually determined according to the environ- may not necessarily be better when reviewing standard
mental demands of the settings. word lists for vocabulary selection. Their use at this point
An environmental inventory approach has also been should not be ruled out, instead the authors suggest that
used with the adult aphasic population. The individual standard word lists may be a “necessary but not suffi-
with aphasia who is nonliterate will have to rely on cient aspect of vocabulary selection” (Yorkston et al.,
preselected messages. In order to select these items, 1989, p. 106). In this case study, environmental inven-
“a vocabulary/message inventory in multiple environ-
ments becomes crucial” (Beukelman & Garrett, 1988, p.
12
117). Garrett, Beukelman, and Morrow (1989) described Daily Communicator. Interactive Therapeutics, P.O. Box 1805,
a multimodality AAC system developed for Mike, a Stow, OH 44224.
Vocabulary Selection 181

tories and communication diaries were an excellent Persons Who Are Preliterate. First, how can de-
source for unique or specialized vocabulary. velopmental information be integrated into the vocabu-
In conclusion, the number of nonliterate individuals lary selection process? For example, what steps in the
who require AAC intervention is large and the task of vocabulary selection process are needed to ensure that
selecting appropriate vocabulary for them is extensive. words are developmentally appropriate and challenging
However, the empirical research base to guide this as well as functional?
effort is extremely limited. Fortunately, the theoretical Second, how should vocabulary be selected to fit the
structures for this research endeavor appear to be well lexical style of the person who uses an AAC system?
developed. Learners who use a predominantly referential style may
have difficulty with vocabulary that is replete with phrases
Future Research Directions because they prefer to use single words in an attempt
to “label” the object of their communicative message.
Future research directions for helping persons who The reverse is true for the learner who uses a predom-
are nonliterate will be divided into two different sections inantly expressive style and is more interested in social
to reflect the needs of persons who have severe cogni- interaction and people than labeling actual objects.
tive and language delays and those persons who are Clearly, the vocabulary needs will be distinct given the
preliterate. individual’s learning style.
Persons Who Have Severe Cognitive and Language
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

Third, what types of literacy remediation techniques


Disabilities. First, how do we select appropriate vo- are available to enhance the skills of the person who is
cabulary to meet the challenge of educating students developing language? What strategies are most effi-
with severe cognitive and language disabilities in regu- cient to promote spelling, reading, and writing develop-
lar education settings? Professionals, parents, and per- ment? How can computer-assisted writing systems
sons with disabilities are advocating a regular life in the function in the regular academic classroom?
home, school, and community. This has direct implica- Fourth, what words do regular education students
tions for the vocabulary that is included in the AAC now use in academic classrooms? We require informa-
system when the individual is mainstreamed into the tion about both the spoken and written vocabulary that
regular education classroom and participates in commu- regular students use. Documentation of the actual words
nity-referenced activities. Here, the context and goals that these students use throughout the school day is
that are set will drive the vocabulary selection process. crucial to promote research into using lists of frequently
Specifically, the research questions that must be occurring or “core” words.
addressed are related to determining the different per- Fifth, how can the role of the person who uses an AAC
spectives (i.e., developmental, environmental, and sta- system be enhanced in the selection of his/her own
tistically derived lists) and the combinations that can vocabulary? As these individuals are encouraged to
provide the best vocabulary pools. take personal responsibility for their communication
Second, for persons with severe cognitive and lan- systems, certainly vocabulary selection should be fore-
guage disabilities, do the vocabulary selection techni- most in these activities. Mary Ann Romski (personal
ques change with an individual’s age? Is the perspective communication, September, 1989) reported that she
(either developmental or environmental) different be- includes a symbol that means “What’s that?,” so the
cause the individual is an adult and, if so, which tech- individual can request certain types of words. It may also
nique or combination of techniques is better? be helpful to assist the individual to “capture” words by
Third, what vocabulary selection strategies are most adding a symbol that communicates the message “I
effective to choose vocabulary items that promote con- want that word” in order to signal his/her preferences.
versation and interaction? Stuart (1988) suggests that Obviously, these two strategies for “capturing” words
vocabulary items do several things: (1) give the individual and messages require quite different skills on the part
an opportunity to talk to people of different ages, intel- of the AAC user. The first strategy of indicating, “What’s
lects, and interests; (2) reflect personal identity, mem- that?,” requires linguistic capabilities, while the second
bership in subgroups, and opportunities for involvement strategy of indicating, “I want that word” also requires
in many different true communication situations; and (3) meta-linguistic capabilities.
provide special function vocabulary for holidays, jobs, Finally, because persons who are nonliterate (regard-
prayers, reading stories, etc. Specifically, persons who less of the reason for being nonliterate) must still rely on
are nonliterate (including those with severe intellectual informants to select some vocabulary to be placed in
disability) require vocabulary items that promote sponta- their AAC system, research is needed on the use of
neous communicative interactions about every topic im- informants and the selection tools that are required to
aginable and in all of the settings they access. It is then help informants choose appropriate vocabulary. Pion-
necessary to identify those tools (i.e., inventory, diary, eering research in this specific area will be the focus of
dialogue) that best achieve this goal. the next section.
182 Beukelman, McGinnis, and Morrow

INFORMANTS AND VOCABULARY unpublished study by Morrow, Beukelman, and Mirenda


SELECTION METHODS (1989). In this study, the investigators compared vocab-
ulary size, vocabulary characteristics, and informant
Basic Assumptions About Informants reactions associated with three different methods of
vocabulary selection and three different categories of
Returning to the assumptions outlined at the begin- informants. The three methods for selecting vocabulary
ning of this paper, note that three of the six assumptions for children who used AAC systems included (1) a blank
are directly related to the selection and use of informants. sheet task in which informants were instructed to pro-
An informant is anyone involved in making decisions vide a listing of vocabulary words for a specific child, (2)
about what messages to include in an AAC system. One a categorical listing task in which the informants listed
assumption states that persons who use AAC systems specific words under several different categorical head-
themselves should serve as their own informants when- ings that were provided to them, and (3) a checklist task
ever possible. This statement is qualified, however, by (Bristow & Fristoe, 1984) in which informants identified
the next assumption that for a variety of reasons some words from a checklist.
individuals are unable to act as the sole informant. The
third assumption suggests that multiple informants will Effect of Informant Type on Vocabulary
help to ensure appropriateness and efficiency in the Size and Characteristics
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

vocabulary selection process.


The third assumption, the use of multiple informants Morrow et al. (1989) investigated the effect of infor-
with a variety of backgrounds has been promoted widely mant type on vocabulary size, i.e., how many words did
in the literature (Augmentative Communication Service, each informant contribute? The speech-language pa-
1984; Carlson, 1981; Karlan & Lloyd, 1983; McDonald thologist offered more of the total words than the parent
& Schultz, 1973). In fact, there is little else in the or the teacher for four of six subjects. For the other two
literature about informants except the advice to use lots subjects, the parent offered the largest number of total
of them. words.
Carlson (1981) mentioned the use of parents, teachers, Further investigation indicated the effect of informant
speech-language pathologists, siblings, and friends. Other type on vocabulary characteristics (i.e., what kind of
significant others such as group home staff, teacher words did each informant contribute?). Specifically, the
aides, and counselors have been added to this list of characteristic of uniqueness was investigated. Unique
potential informants (Augmentative Communication words were defined as “those words yielded by only one
Service, 1984). Involving persons who use AAC sys- [vocabulary selection] method and one informant” (Mor-
tems themselves, both to make contributions and to edit row et al., 1989). Results indicated that for three of six
the contributions of significant others, is also advocated subjects, the parent contributed most of the unique
(Augmentative Communication Service, 1984; Carlson, words. For the other three subjects, the speech-lan-
1981; McDonald & Schultz, 1973). guage pathologist offered the most unique words. Al-
In addition to the recommendation to use a variety of though teachers did not contribute as many unique
informants, there are some suggestions regarding the words as the other two informants, they were an impor-
procedures that can be used to guide the participation tant resource for school related vocabulary.
of informants. Van Tatenhove (1987) provided five tech-
niques that can be used to instruct communication Informant Reactions to Vocabulary
partners (i.e., caregivers and facilitators) to select vo- Selection Methods
cabulary for the individual. These techniques include:
(1) looking at the student’s environments to inventory; One of the important sub-questions in the Morrow et
(2) writing dialogues for familiar routines; (3) asking al. (1989) study was that of the informants’ reactions to
partners to write down on a blank piece of paper sug- using the vocabulary selection methods. This was in-
gestions for relevant vocabulary; (4) following proce- vestigated in terms of the level of satisfaction, level of
dures similar to number 3, except that the partner and difficulty, and amount of time needed to complete the
the trainer role-play different daily activities; and (5) task. Informants indicated their level of satisfaction on a
using audio- and video-tape to record communicative five-point rating scale. Overall satisfaction level was
exchanges and writing down the relevant vocabulary for relatively constant across informant groups. Thus, no
the specific activity. Each of the methods delineates one group of informants was more satisfied than any
environmental vocabulary that provides words relevant to other group. Across informants, satisfaction level was
the setting and activity, but also promotes interaction rated the same for the blank sheet and categorical
beyond requesting. method, with the checklist being rated as slightly more
satisfactory than the other two methods.
Using Multiple Informants Level of perceived difficulty with each method was
also rated on a five-point rating scale. Parents and
One approach to organizing the efforts of multiple teachers judged the difficulty level of vocabulary selec-
informants has received some initial investigation in an tion tasks similarly, while speech-language pathologists
Vocabulary Selection 183

rated the vocabulary selection tasks as being slightly (Boser, 1988; Goyder, 1982; Heberlein & Baumgartner,
less difficult. Across informants, difficulty level was rated 1978).
the same for the first two techniques, blank sheet and Finally, further investigation into the concept of the
categorical method, with the checklist (the third tech- cumulative approach to using multiple informants and
nique) rated as slightly less difficult than the other two other vocabulary selection strategies is warranted in
methods. light of the goals to increase the quality and efficiency
of the vocabulary selection process.
Future Directions
CLOSING COMMENTS
The trend in the literature to this point has been to
utilize as informants only persons who are familiar with In this article, we have attempted to summarize the
and knowledgeable about the persons who use AAC information that is currently available regarding the se-
systems. This raises an interesting question. Does an lection of vocabulary to be included in AAC systems.
informant have to know the individual for whom vocab- While this information base is expanding, our knowl-
ulary is being selected? Is it perhaps enough for the edge of how to appropriately and efficiently select vo-
informant to be an expert on the communicative de- cabulary is not. In clinical practice, we face a paradox.
mands of a specific environment? One example of this Individuals who use AAC systems have very unique
is the history professor who knows the unique verbal vocabulary needs and a personalized vocabulary should
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

and written communicative requirements of history, and be developed for them. Yet, to do so is a very time
so acts as an expert informant for an unfamiliar student consuming and difficult activity. Therefore, an effort is
who will be enrolled in the class during the following being made to develop sources of vocabulary to be used
semester. as guidelines in the vocabulary selection process. To
One might assume that when the informant is signif- date, our source lists are very general resources that
icantly different from the person who uses an AAC have not been drawn from communication performance
system, whether it be due to age, interests, or other in specific contexts. Clearly, the research in vocabulary
factors, there is a danger of obtaining a vocabulary use and selection must shift to focus on data that is more
actually more suited to the informant than to the in- contextually and demographically specific. Studies of
tended user of the vocabulary. Thus, another issue that the performance of both natural speakers and persons
deserves further consideration is that of how best to use who use AAC systems, who are of different age levels,
“peer” informants. Again, these peers may or may not and in specific settings perhaps will provide a data base
be familiar with the individual, but their participation in that will be useful. Our research is just beginning to
similar activities and environments may give them spe- consider the efficacy of particular vocabulary sources
cial insights into a highly appropriate vocabulary. for the various types of persons who use AAC systems.
More research needs to be completed in order to In addition, information is needed about the ways in
study how the vocabulary selection method affects the which persons who act as informants and vocabulary
vocabulary obtained. Not only do vocabulary size and managers utilize source lists and vocabulary selection
general characteristics need to be investigated, but tools. Obviously, the goal of vocabulary selection is to
more importantly, the impact of selection methods on enhance the communication skills of the individual, not
the specific words and messages that are chosen for just to develop extensive lists of vocabulary efficiently.
individual AAC users should also be investigated. Are The impact of vocabulary source information on clini-
the words yielded by the various vocabulary selection cal/educational practice of those who facilitate vocabu-
methods really appropriate for the individual, or are they lary selection remains an important research question.
biased by the informant and/or the method itself? There Of particular interest is the role that individuals who
is a particular need to validate the appropriateness of use AAC systems can play in the selection and retention
predicted words by comparing them to the words the of vocabulary items for their own systems. In the future,
individual actually uses while communicating. the AAC field will need to develop tools and strategies
The issue of how the vocabulary selection task is that allow individuals to control decisions regarding their
presented to informants is also an important one. For own vocabulary without having to develop extensive
example, in a wide variety of fields, researchers have meta-linguistic skills.
studied the factors affecting response rates to question- Because the size of the vocabulary selected for the
naires. Many factors have been found to have a significant person who uses an AAC system is nearly always
effect on response rates. These include the salience of constrained due to the technical limitations of the AAC
the subject matter to the respondent, which may be an technique or the learning capacity of the individual,
important consideration in the use of expert informants strategies to set priorities for vocabulary to be included
(Goyder, 1982; Heberlein & Baumgartner, 1978). Also, in an AAC system must be considered. A review of this
personalization of questionnaires, number of contacts manuscript reveals little information about the strategies
with the respondent, and sex of the respondent were and guidelines that are used to set priorities for the
significant factors affecting questionnaire responses vocabulary to be included in an AAC system. The lack
184 Beukelman, McGinnis, and Morrow

of information is surprising because the amount of clin- Carlson, F. (1984). Picsyms categorical dictionary. Lawrence, KS:
ical/educational activity related to vocabulary selection Baggeboda Press.
Fristoe, M., & Lloyd, L. L. (1980). Planning an initial expressive sign
is extensive, as decisions are made each year for lexicon for persons with severe communication impairment. Jour-
thousands of individuals. Unfortunately, little of this nal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 45, 170-180.
information is being collected, analyzed, and dissemin- Garrett, K., Beukelman, D., & Morrow, D. (1989). A comprehensive
ated. Individuals who use AAC systems, clinicians, edu- augmentative communication system for an adult with Broca’s
cators, and researchers still have much to do. aphasia. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 5, 55-61.
Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Experiments in sponta-
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS neous speech. New York: Academic Press.
Goyder, J. C. (1982). Further evidence on factors affecting response
rates to mailed questionnaires. American Sociological Review, 47,
This paper is based in part on a presentation at the think 550-553.
tank on State of the Art and Science in the field of Aug- Heberlein, T. A., & Baumgartner, R. (1978). Factors affecting response
mentative and Alternative Communication held at Purdue rates to mailed questionnaires: A quantitative analysis of the pub-
University during September of 1989. The authors wish to lished literature. American Sociological Review, 43, 447-462.
thank Janice Light for her editorial suggestions. Hillerich, R. (1978). A writing vocabulary of elementary children.
Address reprint requests to: David R. Beukelman, Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Holland, A. (1975). Language therapy for children: Some thoughts on
202F Barkley Memorial Center, University of Nebraska- context and content. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 40,
Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68583-0732, USA.
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

514-523.
Hopkins, C. J. (1979). Spontaneous oral vocabulary of children in
grade one. Elementary School Journal, 79, 240-249.
REFERENCES Johnson, D. D. (1971). A basic vocabulary for beginning reading.
Elementary School Journal, 72, 29-34.
Augmentative Communication Service, Hugh MacMillan Medical Cen- Karlan, G., & Lloyd, L. L. (1983). Considerations in the planning of
tre. (1984). Programming Guidelines: Vocabulary Selection. Un- communication intervention: Selecting a lexicon. Journal of the
published manuscript, Hugh MacMillan Medical Centre, Toronto, Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 8, 13-25.
Ontario. Lahey, M., & Bloom, L. (1977). Planning a first lexicon: Which words to
Beukelman, D., & Garrett, K. (1988). Augmentative and alternative teach first. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 42, 340-350.
communication for adults with acquired severe communication dis- McDonald, E. T., & Schultz, A. R. (1973). Communication boards for
orders. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 4, 104-121. cerebral-palsied children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-
Beukelman, D., Jones, R., & Rowan, M. (1989). Frequency of word ders, 38, 73-88.
usage by non-disabled peers in integrated preschool classrooms. McGinnis, J., & Beukelman, D. (1989). Vocabulary requirements for
Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 5, 243-248. writing activities for academically mainstreamed students with disabil-
Beukelman, D., & Yorkston, K. (1982). Communication interaction of ities. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 5, 183-191.
adult communication augmentation system use. Topics in Lan- Mirenda, P. (1985). Designing pictorial communication systems for
guage Disorders, 2(2), 39-54. physically able-bodied students with severe handicaps. Augmen-
Beukelman, D., Yorkston, K., Poblete, M., & Naranjo, C. (1984). tative and Alternative Communication, 1, 58-64.
Frequency of word occurrence in communication samples pro- Moe, A. J., Hopkins, C. J., & Rush, R. T. (1982). The vocabulary of
duced by adult communication users. Journal of Speech and first grade children. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Hearing Disorders, 49, 360-367. Morrow, D. R. (1989). Vocabulary selection for augmentative commu-
Beukelman, D., Yorkston, K., Tice, R., Nelson, N., lacono, T., & nication systems: A comparison of three techniques. Unpublished
Laikko, P. (1989). Vocabulary Selection Toolbox. Master’s thesis, University of Nebraska, Lincoln.
Blau, A. (1983). Vocabulary selection in augmentative communica- Murphy, H. A. (1957). Spontaneous speaking vocabulary of children
tion. Where do we begin ? In H. Winitz (Ed.), Treating language in primary grades. Journal of Education, 140, 3-106.
disorders: For clinicians by clinicians (pp. 205-234). Baltimore: Musselwhite, C., & St. Louis, K. (1988). Communication programming
University Park Press. for persons with severe handicaps: Vocal and augmentative strat-
Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation egies. Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Company.
in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Nagy, W., & Anderson, R. (1984). The number of words in school
Development, 40(Serial No. 160). English. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304-330.
Boser, J. A. (1988). Teacher-education graduate surveys: Variables Nelson, N. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Mono-
related to response rate. Journal of Educational Research, 81, graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(Serial
369-373. No. 149).
Bristow, D., & Fristoe, M. (1984, November). Systematic evaluation Nietupski, J., & Hamre-Nietupski, S. (1979). Teaching auxiliary skills
of the nonspeaking child. Miniseminar at the American Speech, to severely handicapped students. AAESPH, 4, 107-124.
Language, and Hearing Association National Convention, San Reichle, J., Williams, W., & Ryan, S. (1981). Selecting signs for the
Francisco, CA. formulation of an augmentative communicative modality. Journal
Brown, L., Falvey, M., Vincent, L., Kaye, N., Johnson, F., Ferrara-Parrish, of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 6, 48-56.
P., & Gruenewald, L. (1980). Strategies for generating comprehens- Richards, J. C. (1974). Word lists: Problems and prospects. Regional
ive longitudinal and chronological age appropriate individual edu- English Language Center Journal, 5(2), 69-84.
cation programs for adolescent and young adult severely handi- Rinsland, H. (1945). A basic vocabulary of elementary schoolchildren.
capped students. Journal of Special Education, 14, 199-215. New York: Macmillan.
Carlson, F. (1981). A format for selecting vocabulary for the nonspeak- Schutz-Muehling, L., & Beukelman, D. (1990). An augmentative
ing child. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 12, writing system for a college student with fibrositis: A case study.
140-145. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 4, 250-255.
Vocabulary Selection 185

Stemach, G., & Williams, W. (1988). Word express: The first 2,500 Westby, C. (1985). Learning to talk-talking to learn: Oral-literate
words of spoken English. Navato, CA: Academic Therapy Publica- language differences. In C. Simon (Ed.), Communication skills and
tions. classroom success. Boston: Little, Brown, and Company.
Stuart, S. (1988). Vocabulary selection -Augmentative communica- Yorkston, K., Beukelman, D., Smith, K., & Tice, R. (1990). Extended
tion. South Dakota Speech Language Hearing Association Jour- communication samples of augmented communicators II: Analysis
nal, 31, 17-19. of multi-word sequences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-
Trevor, K., & Nelson, N. (1989). Vocabulary use by children in first, ders, 55, 225-230.
third, and fifth grade classrooms. Unpublished research manu- Yorkston, K., Dowden P., Honsinger, M., Marriner, N., & Smith, K.
script, Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo. (1988). A comparison of standard and user vocabulary lists. Aug-
Vanderheiden, G., & Kelso, D. (1987). Comparative analysis of fixed mentative and Alternative Communication, 4, 189-210.
vocabulary communication acceleration techniques. Augmentative Yorkston, K., Honsinger, M., Dowden, P., & Marriner, N. (1989).
and Alternative Communication, 3, 196-206. Vocabulary selection: A case report. Augmentative and Alternative
Van Tatenhove, G. (1987). Training caregivers and facilitators to Communication, 5, 101-108.
select vocabulary: lmplementation strategies for improving the use Yorkston, K., Smith, K., & Beukelman, D. (1990). Extended commu-
of communication aids in schools. Rockville, MD: American nication samples of augmented communicators I: A comparison of
Speech-Language-Hearing Association. individualized versus standard single word vocabularies. Journal
Webster’s Ninth New Collegiate Dictionary. (1988). Springfield, MA: of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 217-224.
Merriam Webster Publishers.
Downloaded by [University of Otago] at 06:26 01 November 2015

ARTICLES TO APPEAR IN FORTHCOMING ISSUES OF AAC


University Students Using Augmentative and Alternative Communication in the U.S.A.: A Demographic Study by Mary Blake
Huer (USA)
Augmentative Communication Training with a Nonspeaking Adult: Lessons from M.H. by Sharon L. Glennen, Maureen A.
Sharp, and Daniel C. Tullos (USA)
Individual Language Learning Styles and Augmented Communicators by Teresa A. lacono (USA)
Comments on Terminology by Catriona Gunn (Australia)
The Further Development of an Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Symbol Taxonomy by Donald R. Fuller
(USA), Lyle L. Lloyd (USA), and Ralf W. Schlosser (FRG)
Effects of Rate and Pitch Variations on the Intelligibility of Synthesized Speech by H.S. Venkatagiri (USA)
Communication Development of Young Children with Tracheostomies by Lauren B. Adamson and Barbara Dunbar (USA)
Children With Severe Speech Impairments: Caregiver Survey of AAC Use by Janet H. Allaire, Risa P. Gressard, James A.
Blackman, and Sharon L. Hostler (USA)
Initial Comparison of the Efficiency of a Variety of Augmentative or Alternative Communication Systems for Ordering Meals
in Fast Food Restaurants by L. Scott Doss, Peggy Ann Locke, Susan S. Johnston, Joe Reichle, Jeff Sigafoos, Paul J.
Charpentier, and Dulce J. Foster (USA)
Performance Is the Prize: Language Competence and Performance among Users of AAC by Nickola Nelson (USA)

ISAAC CORPORATE MEMBERS


ISAAC’s continued growth and development has been due to the efforts of many outstanding individuals and groups.
Among those we would like to acknowledge the following corporate members:

Adaptive Communication Systems, Inc. Fondation Suisse Pour Les Teletheses Unidad de Comunicacion Aumentativa
Pittsburgh, USA Neuchatel, Switzerland Madrid, Spain

AICA (Association Incremento Phonic Ear Inc. United Cerebral Palsy Assoc., Inc.
Comunicazione Alternativa) Mill Valley, USA New York, USA
Milano, Italy
Prentke Romich Company United Cerebral Palsy Assoc., Inc.
CNR-Cansigleottozionale Ricerche Wooster, USA Washington, USA
Pisa, Italy
Rehabilitation Technology Assessment Words+, Inc.
Don Johnston Developmental Equipment Program Lancaster, USA
Wauconda, USA University Hospital Rehabilitation Center
Elizabethtown, USA

August 7-11, 1992


Biennial ISAAC Conference
The Fifth International Conference of the International Society for Augmentative and Alternative Communication (ISAAC)
will be held in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, U.S.A., at the Wyndham Franklin Plaza on August 7-11, 1992. For further
information, contact: Beth Mineo, Ph.D., or Susan Quinlisk-Gill, M.Ed., Conference Chairs, Alfred I. duPont Institute,
1600 Rockland Rd., Wilmington, DE, U.S.A. 19899; Telephone: (302) 651-6830; FAX: (302) 651-4019.

You might also like