You are on page 1of 13

AJSLP

Review

Anxiety and Stuttering: Continuing


to Explore a Complex Relationship
Lisa Iverach,a Ross G. Menzies,a Sue O'Brian,a
Ann Packman,a and Mark Onslowa

Purpose: The relationship between anxiety and stuttering Results: Despite some remaining ambiguous findings, re-
has been widely studied. However, a review conducted more search published since the Menzies et al. review has provided
than 10 years ago (Menzies, Onslow, & Packman, 1999) far stronger evidence of a relationship between stuttering
identified 5 methodological issues thought to preclude con- and anxiety, and has focused more on social anxiety, expec-
sistent research findings regarding the nature of this relation- tancies of social harm, and fear of negative evaluation.
ship. The purpose of the present review was to determine Conclusion: The aims of future research should be to im-
whether methodological improvements have occurred since prove research design, increase statistical power, employ
the Menzies et al. (1999) review. multidimensional measures of anxiety, and further develop
Method: Literature published since the Menzies et al. review anxiolytic treatment options for people who stutter.
was evaluated with regard to the 5 methodological issues
identified in that review: (a) the construct of anxiety, (b) trait
anxiety measures, (c) participant numbers, (d) treatment Key Words: stuttering, anxiety, review, social phobia,
status of participants, and (e) speaking tasks. fear of negative evaluation

A
nxiety is a complex psychological construct com- Anxiety and Stuttering
posed of verbal-cognitive, behavioral, and physio-
Anxiety is one of the most frequently observed and widely
logical components (Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004; studied psychological concomitants of stuttering (Ingham,
Menzies, Onslow, & Packman, 1999). It is also considered
1984; Peters & Hulstijn, 1984). There are a number of reasons
to include both transient (state) aspects and more general-
this association between anxiety and stuttering has been so
ized and stable (trait) characteristics (Menzies et al., 1999;
widespread. In particular, speech is fundamental to daily
Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983). The
functioning and to the development and maintenance of so-
experience of anxiety can include thoughts and expectan-
cial networks and relationships (Messenger, Onslow, Packman,
cies of negative events, escape or avoidance of feared situ-
& Menzies, 2004). However, many people who stutter face
ations, and physical sensations such as muscle tension and
considerable difficulties when trying to speak (Packman,
heart palpitations (Kraaimaat, Vanryckeghem, & Van Dam-
Menzies, & Onslow, 2000). Added to this, stuttering is asso-
Baggen, 2002; Menzies et al., 1999). Numerous measures
ciated with negative consequences that can adversely affect
have been developed to evaluate anxiety, including uni- social and emotional functioning, relationships, quality of
dimensional measures that assess anxiety as a single or
life, and mental health (Craig, Blumgart, & Tran, 2009). In
global construct, such as the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
fact, the negative consequences of stuttering can begin shortly
(Spielberger et al., 1983), and multidimensional measures after onset during the preschool years (Ezrati-Vinacour,
that regard anxiety as composed of numerous components,
Platzky, & Yairi, 2001; Langevin, Packman, & Onslow, 2009)
such as the Endler Multidimensional Anxiety Scales (Endler,
and can continue across the lifespan to include negative
Edwards, & Vitelli, 1991).
listener reactions and stereotypes, bullying and teasing, social
isolation and rejection, relationship difficulties, educational
a
Australian Stuttering Research Centre, The University
and occupational underachievement, expectancies of so-
cial harm, and fear of speaking in social situations (Blood &
of Sydney, Australia
Blood, 2007; Cream, Onslow, Packman, & Llewellyn, 2003;
Correspondence to Mark Onslow: mark.onslow@sydney.edu.au Davis, Howell, & Cooke, 2002; O’Brian, Jones, Packman,
Editor: Laura Justice Menzies, & Onslow, 2011; Turnbaugh, Guitar, & Hoffman,
Associate Editor: Patrick Finn 1979).
Received October 28, 2010 As a result, anxiety in speaking-related or social situations
Revision received February 23, 2011 can be considered a predictable outcome of the negative
Accepted March 17, 2011 communication consequences experienced across the life-
DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0091) span for people who stutter (Bloodstein, 1995; Ingham, 1984;

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 20 • 221–232 • August 2011 • A American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 221
Miller & Watson, 1992). Therefore, understanding the nature research findings regarding the nature of anxiety and stutter-
of the relationship between anxiety and stuttering is an im- ing. These methodological issues included use of (a) phys-
portant issue (Kraaimaat et al., 2002). However, the nature iological measures of anxiety, rather than self-report or
of this relationship and the mechanisms underlying it have behavioral measures of anxiety (such as situation avoidance),
previously been poorly understood, and past scientific evi- fear of negative evaluation, and expectancy of social harm;
dence has been regarded as ambiguous and difficult to inter- (b) unidimensional measures of trait anxiety rather than
pret (Andrews et al., 1983; Attanasio, 2000; Blood, Blood, multidimensional measures designed specifically to evaluate
Bennett, Simpson, & Susman, 1994; Craig, Hancock, Tran, & social anxiety; (c) small sample sizes with insufficient power
Craig, 2003; Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004; Ingham, 1984; to detect significant differences; (d) participants who had
Kraaimaat et al., 2002; Menzies et al., 1999). The incon- received treatment for stuttering in the past or who were
sistent nature of research findings regarding anxiety and seeking or currently undergoing speech treatment rather than
stuttering has been discussed in past reports (Craig & Tran, participants who had never received treatment; and (e) sin-
2006; Ingham, 1984; Menzies et al., 1999). gle speaking tasks in order to elicit or manipulate anxiety
in people who stutter rather than multiple or individualized
behavioral tasks. A number of these methodological issues
Ingham’s (1984) Review have been highlighted by other researchers (e.g., Craig,
In 1984, Ingham made a significant contribution to the 1990).
field by reviewing the principle theoretical models under- Menzies et al. (1999) concluded that the relationship
pinning associations between anxiety and stuttering. In par- between anxiety and stuttering could not be fully understood
ticular, Ingham reviewed the influence of psychological until the above methodological limitations had been over-
theories on the development of stuttering treatments and come. In a similar manner to Ingham (1984), they also argued
evaluated anxiety-modification techniques used to treat stut- that
tering. Ingham also reviewed research regarding the phys- the inadequate literature on anxiety and stuttering is not
iological factors associated with anxiety and stuttering,
just an academic problem. On the contrary, an ambiguous
including the experience of physical tension during moments
literature on the role that anxiety plays in the condition
of stuttering. He argued that conclusive evidence of such a impairs clinical practice and retards the development of
relationship was not apparent in his review of the research
novel clinical procedures. Until the precise nature of the
literature at the time. In particular, Ingham suggested that
relationship between anxiety and stuttering is understood,
research evidence regarding the causal effect of physiolog-
fully appropriate treatment of adult stuttering cannot be
ical tension associated with stuttering was unclear, and that
offered. (p. 8)
physiological evidence of anxiety could not necessarily be
taken to indicate the actual presence of anxiety. In addition, Hence, Menzies et al. highlighted the need for improve-
Ingham argued that the theoretical foundations underpinning ments in research design in stuttering research, including
some anxiety-reduction approaches to stuttering were ques- the use of larger sample sizes to increase statistical power,
tionable and lacking in objective speech data. and the application of multidimensional measures of anxiety
Ingham (1984) highlighted a number of methodological (Blomgren, Roy, Callister, & Merrill, 2005; Craig et al.,
flaws in the stuttering literature that were thought to explain 2003; Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004; Menzies et al., 1999).
why a clear and systematic relationship between anxiety and They also underscored the need to provide anxiolytic treat-
stuttering had not become apparent. According to Ingham, ments for people who stutter and who report anxiety.
these inadequacies included the use of anecdotal reporting,
the lack of independent measures of state anxiety, and fail-
ure of anxiety treatments to result in stuttering reductions. The Present Review
Ingham also suggested that a linear relationship between Although the Menzies et al. (1999) review shed necessary
stuttering and anxiety should not be assumed for all people light on the “long and tangled” relationship between anxiety
who stutter, and that state anxiety might have a facilitative and stuttering (Attanasio, 2000, p. 89), over a decade has
rather than debilitating effect on stuttering in some cases. passed since its publication. Therefore, it is timely to inves-
Overall, Ingham concluded that “the overall tenor of the find- tigate whether any changes in research design or method-
ings from the studies reviewed is that there is little evidence ological improvements have occurred since publication of
of a clinically significant, or even theoretically palpable, the Menzies et al. review. As a result, the purpose of the
relationship between stuttering and anxiety” (p. 132). present review is to (a) determine whether the five method-
ological issues identified by Menzies et al. are still rele-
vant to research published since their review, (b) establish
Menzies et al.’s (1999) Review whether research evidence regarding the relationship be-
Fifteen years after Ingham’s (1984) review of what he tween anxiety and stuttering has become less ambiguous and
considered to be “relatively unprofitable research” (p. 133), more conclusive since the Menzies et al. review, (c) identify
Menzies et al. (1999) conducted a review of the literature any further methodological issues apparent in the research
to further explore the complex and often poorly understood regarding anxiety and stuttering, and (d) determine whether
relationship between anxiety and stuttering. In their review, any methodological improvements in anxiety research since
Menzies et al. identified five core methodological issues the Menzies et al. review have resulted in changes to clini-
in the literature thought to preclude clear and consistent cal practice and the treatment of anxiety in stuttering.

222 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 20 • 221–232 • August 2011


Methodological Issues A more substantial number of studies have investigated
The Construct of Anxiety the socially evaluative aspects of anxiety in stuttering, in-
cluding use of self-report measures of anxiety and fear of
The first methodological issue highlighted by Menzies negative evaluation such as the Endler Multidimensional
et al. (1999) relates to the construct of anxiety. In particular, Anxiety Scales-Trait (EMAS-T; Endler et al., 1991) and the
Menzies et al. argued that much of the research at the time Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (FNE; Watson & Friend,
of their review had not taken into account the multidimen- 1969). Findings from these studies have highlighted the
sional nature of anxiety. Instead, a sizable proportion of propensity for people who stutter to experience anticipatory
research had focused on the physiological component of anxiety and fear of being penalized by listeners (Cream et al.,
anxiety, which Menzies et al. argued was possibly the least 2003; Plexico, Manning, & Levitt, 2009).
useful indicator of social anxiety. Not only are the results Messenger and colleagues (2004), in particular, explored
of physiological measures of social anxiety thought to vary the relationship between stuttering, social anxiety, and nega-
across individuals, thereby making it difficult to interpret the tive social expectancies. They found that scores on the
findings of such evaluations, but they are also poorly cor- FNE and the Social Evaluation and New/Strange Situations
related with the verbal-cognitive and behavioral components subtests of the EMAS-T were significantly higher for a sam-
of anxiety (Menzies et al.). Ingham (1984) also suggested ple of 34 adults who stuttered than for 34 controls. These
that “physiological evidence of anxiety is not necessarily findings, which confirmed the socially evaluative nature of
evidence that the subject actually experiences anxiety” (p. 129). anxiety in stuttering, have also been supported by further
Therefore, Menzies et al. argued that the use of physiological research. For instance, Blumgart, Tran, and Craig (2010)
measures of anxiety reduces the likelihood of clearly iden- reported that the mean FNE score for a large sample of
tifying anxiety, whereas self-report and behavioral measures 200 adults who stuttered was significantly higher than the
may provide more sound indications of anxiety, at least with mean score for 200 controls. Similarly, Iverach, O’Brian,
research about social anxiety and stuttering. et al. (2009) administered a number of anxiety-related self-
In addition, Menzies et al. (1999) suggested that evalua- report measures, including the FNE and the EMAS-T, to a
tion of expectancies of social harm, fear of negative eval- large sample of 92 stuttering adults. Results revealed that the
uation, and anticipation of stuttering is also of particular adults who stuttered were characterized by increased fear
importance when evaluating the relationship between anxiety of negative evaluation and anxiety in socially evaluative or
and stuttering (Ginsberg, 2000; Ingham, 1984; Menzies new/strange situations when compared with matched con-
et al., 1999). Fear of negative evaluation and expectancies of trols. However, these anxiety levels were not as high as those
harm are central constructs in the experience and assessment reported for clinically anxious or socially phobic samples.
of anxiety (Messenger et al., 2004). Adults who stutter tend Mahr and Torosian (1999) also reported that adults who
to fear speaking situations (Mahr & Torosian, 1999), and stutter may have increased levels of social anxiety and avoid-
negative social evaluation is thought to be one of the most ance compared to nonstuttering controls, but that they may
likely mediators of speech-related anxiety for adults who not experience fear of negative evaluation to the same degree
stutter (Menzies et al., 1999). However, at the time of the as individuals with social phobia. In particular, Mahr and
Menzies et al. review, only a single study (Poulton & Andrews, Torosian compared anxiety and fear of negative evaluation
1994) was identified that evaluated social expectancy and in a sample of 22 adults who stuttered with nonstuttering
subjective anxiety during a speaking task. Therefore, Menzies controls and adults with social phobia. FNE scores for the
et al. recommended the incorporation of self-report and stuttering group did not differ significantly from nonstutter-
behavioral measures of anxiety into future research, includ- ing controls but were significantly lower than scores for
ing measures of expectancies of social harm and negative participants with social phobia. In addition, social anxiety and
evaluation. distress scores for the stuttering group were significantly
Since the Menzies et al. (1999) review, a small number of higher than controls but significantly lower than participants
studies have continued to explore the physiological com- with social phobia. The stuttering group also demonstrated
ponent of anxiety. For instance, Dietrich and Roaman (2001) significantly higher anxiety symptoms than controls and sig-
investigated the relationship between perceptions of speech- nificantly lower social and agoraphobia symptoms than the
related anxiety and physiological arousal in specific speak- social phobia group.
ing situations in a sample of 24 adults who stuttered. They Findings regarding anxiety and fear of negative evalua-
found no correlations between participants’ predictions of tion in stuttering may be relevant for older adults who stutter.
speech-related anxiety in 20 hypothetical speaking situations For instance, Bricker-Katz, Lincoln, and McCabe (2010)
and actual skin conduction responses during enactment of conducted a qualitative study of 11 stuttering adults over the
four speaking situations. In addition, Alm (2004) reviewed age of 55 years; the authors reported that some participants
findings regarding the propensity for adults who stutter to continued to fear negative evaluation in socially evaluative
demonstrate reductions in heart rate in stressful speaking sit- situations. Further, Bricker-Katz, Lincoln, and McCabe (2009)
uations (e.g., Peters & Hulstijn, 1984; Weber & Smith, 1990). reported that scores on the FNE for a small sample of 12 older
Alm suggested that parasympathetic suppression of heart adults who stuttered were in the social phobia range and
rate might be caused by speech-related anticipatory anxiety were significantly higher than scores for a sample of 14 con-
in people who stutter and argued that anticipatory anxiety trols. In addition, scores on the EMAS-T for socially eval-
in speaking situations might be a conditioned response to uative trait anxiety and anxiety related to physical danger
previous stuttering experiences. were also significantly higher for older adults who stuttered

Iverach et al.: Anxiety and Stuttering 223


than for controls, even though scores for both groups fell conversations, meeting new people, being assertive, or speak-
within the average range. These findings correspond largely ing to authority figures) and performance situations (e.g.,
with those of Messenger et al. (2004), indicating that fear public speaking or performing in front of others; American
of negative evaluation and socially evaluative anxiety may be Psychiatric Association, 2000; Orsillo & Hammond, 2001).
apparent for older adults who stutter. Prior to the Menzies et al. (1999) review, Stein et al. (1996)
Anxiety and fear of negative evaluation have also been and Schneier et al. (1997) contributed valuable research
investigated among adolescents who stuttered. Mulcahy, evidence regarding the presence of social anxiety and social
Hennessey, Beilby, and Byrnes (2008) reported that a sample phobia in adults who stutter. This evidence base has grown
of 19 adolescents who stuttered exhibited significantly considerably since the Menzies et al. review with the addi-
higher trait and state anxiety and significantly higher fear of tion of larger scale studies. For instance, Kraaimaat et al.
negative evaluation than 18 nonstuttering controls. This sug- (2002) administered a social anxiety inventory to a sample
gests that increased anxiety and fear of negative evaluation of 89 adults who stuttered and 131 nonstuttering controls. In
for those who stutter have the potential to commence during their study, the stuttering group demonstrated significantly
the “socially difficult adolescent years” (Huber, Packman, higher emotional discomfort in social situations than the con-
Quine, Onslow, & Simpson, 2004, p. 176). However, fur- trols did, and the stuttering group also reported significantly
ther research regarding the development of fear of negative less social response than controls. Further, scores for the
evaluation and social anxiety among children and adolescents stuttering group on emotional discomfort in social situa-
is required. tions were comparable to a normative sample of psychiatric
Overall, the above findings highlight the potential for patients, even though they were lower than scores for psy-
stuttering to be associated with fear of negative evaluation chiatric patients diagnosed with social phobia. These findings
and expectancies of social harm. This corresponds with a correspond somewhat with those previously reported by Mahr
growing body of evidence regarding the presence of social and Torosian (1999), who found that adults who stuttered
anxiety or social phobia among people who stutter (Schneier, demonstrated significantly increased anxiety symptoms and
Wexler, & Liebowitz, 1997; Stein, Baird, & Walker, 1996), social avoidance and distress than nonpatient controls, yet
to be discussed below. when compared to social phobics, they exhibited significantly
lower fear of negative evaluation, social avoidance, and dis-
tress, and fewer social phobia and agoraphobia symptoms.
Trait Anxiety Measures However, more comprehensive evidence regarding social
The second methodological issue identified by Menzies anxiety and stuttering has been obtained in studies that have
et al. (1999) related to the use of trait anxiety measures in used diagnostic assessments of anxiety rather than self-report
stuttering research. Menzies et al. criticized the administra- measures and inventories. It is possible that the use of such
tion of trait anxiety measures in situations characterized by assessments may contribute a less ambiguous picture of
heightened state anxiety, such as making a phone call. A fur- anxiety in stuttering. For instance, in the Menzies et al. (2008)
ther criticism was drawn against the use of unidimensional clinical trial of cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) for the
measures of trait anxiety such as the State-Trait Anxiety treatment of anxiety in stuttering, clinical psychologists con-
Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al., 1983), which regard ducted comprehensive assessments with adults who stuttered
anxiety as a single or global construct. to determine the presence of social phobia prior to speech
More recent conceptualizations of anxiety have high- treatment only versus speech treatment plus CBT. Sixty per-
lighted its multidimensional nature (Endler et al., 1991). cent of the Menzies et al. sample received a social phobia
Consequently, unidimensional measures of anxiety may not diagnosis at pretreatment, but none of the participants who
be sensitive enough to detect or discriminate between the received CBT treatment for anxiety met criteria for social
various facets of speech-related anxiety for people who phobia following treatment.
stutter (Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004; Menzies et al., 1999) Iverach, O’Brian, et al. (2009) also comprehensively eval-
and have subsequently yielded mixed results in the field of uated the presence of anxiety disorders, including social
stuttering (Craig & Tran, 2006). Hence, the previous lack phobia, in a large sample of 92 adults who stuttered, accord-
of research regarding the multidimensional nature of anxiety ing to the diagnostic criteria employed by the Diagnostic
in stuttering may in part explain why “fine tendencies in and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–IV–TR;
the stuttering-anxiety connection have not been detected” American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and the Interna-
(Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004, p. 138). tional Classification of Diseases (ICD-10; World Health Or-
Since the Menzies et al. (1999) review, one of the most ganization, 1993). When compared with matched controls,
significant methodological improvements in stuttering re- the stuttering adults exhibited six- to sevenfold increased
search has been the use of self-report measures and diagnostic odds for any DSM–IV–TR or ICD-10 anxiety disorder and
assessments of anxiety and social phobia. These studies build 16- to 34-fold increased odds for DSM–IV–TR or ICD-10
on the important work of earlier research (e.g., Schneier social phobia. This same sample of adults seeking treatment
et al., 1997; Stein et al., 1996) and contribute to a growing for stuttering were also found to be at increased risk of meet-
body of evidence regarding the presence of social anxiety or ing first-stage screening criteria for anxious personality dis-
social phobia in adults who stutter. Social phobia, in partic- order (Iverach, Jones, et al., 2009b), which is considered to
ular, is characterized by significant, excessive, and persistent be a more severe form of social phobia (Reich, 2000).
fear of humiliation, embarrassment, or negative evaluation In a related manner, Blumgart et al. (2010) investigated
in social interactional situations (e.g., initiating or maintaining social anxiety in a large sample of 200 adults who stuttered

224 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 20 • 221–232 • August 2011


and 200 nonstuttering adults by administering a variety of That is, state anxiety, but not trait anxiety, was found to be
anxiety measures, including the STAI, the FNE, and the associated with stuttering severity, whereby individuals with
Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory (Turner, Beidel, & more severe stuttering were found to exhibit higher state
Dancu, 1996). In addition, the Psychiatric Diagnostic Screen- anxiety. Ezrati-Vinacour and Levin concluded that anxiety
ing Questionnaire (Zimmerman & Mattia, 2001) was used is a personality trait that gradually develops over time for
to screen for social anxiety disorder (also known as social people who stutter.
phobia) and generalized anxiety disorder in a subsample of In addition, Craig et al. (2003) conducted a randomized
50 adults who stuttered and 50 controls. Results revealed that population study of the prevalence of stuttering in 4,689
the adults who stuttered had significantly higher trait and Australian households. Trait anxiety scores for 63 residents
social anxiety than the controls, with moderate to large effect identified as people who stuttered were compared with scores
sizes. Of particular interest, the adults who stuttered had sig- from a sample of 102 matched controls from a previous study
nificantly elevated specific fears about embarrassing them- (Craig, 1990). Although trait anxiety scores for adults in the
selves in situations such as public speaking, eating in public, stuttering group who had not received stuttering treatment
saying stupid things, asking questions in a group, business did not differ from nonstuttering controls, scores for adults
meetings, and social gatherings or parties. Further, in the who had received previous treatment for stuttering did differ
Blumgart et al. study, the stuttering group demonstrated a significantly from nonstuttering controls. However, no sig-
significantly higher risk for social anxiety disorder than con- nificant differences in anxiety were found between stuttering
trols, indicating a spot prevalence of 46%. This estimate cor- adults who had received stuttering treatment and those who
responds with the prevalence rate of 44% reported by Stein had not, or between those with more or less severe stuttering.
et al. (1996). Overall, Craig et al. concluded that assessments of anxiety
These findings highlight the prevalence of anxiety and in adults who stutter may be influenced by whether partici-
social phobia among adults who stutter. However, findings pants have received previous stuttering treatment or whether
by Blumgart et al. (2010) and Iverach, Jones, et al. (2009b) they are currently seeking treatment.
should be viewed with caution on the grounds that they A number of the above-mentioned studies attributed dis-
used screening instruments rather than full diagnostic inter- crepancies and ambiguities in their findings to the complex
views to evaluate the presence of social anxiety disorder and multidimensional nature of anxiety, as highlighted by
(Blumgart et al., 2010) and anxious personality disorder Menzies et al. (1999). That is, by treating anxiety as a single
(Iverach, Jones, et al., 2009b). Also, of course, a caveat to construct, the STAI limits the ability to detect the presence
those findings is that they pertain to clinical cohorts and of specific dimensions of anxiety (Blomgren et al., 2005)
cohorts seeking self-help group support.1 Despite this, ad- and may not be sensitive enough to measure, or differentiate
vances in the methods used to assess anxiety in stuttering between levels of, speech-related anxiety experienced by
allow us to more fully appreciate the nature of this relation- adults who stutter (Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004).
ship. That is, they provide a methodological leap from the Despite this, unidimensional trait anxiety measures have
unidimensional anxiety measures that have been used before. also been used in a small number of studies of anxiety in
However, a number of studies since the Menzies et al. children and adolescents who stutter. According to Craig and
(1999) review have continued to employ unidimensional or Tran (2005), however, very few of these studies have re-
trait measures of anxiety. For instance, Alm and Risberg ported significant differences between stuttering and non-
(2007) investigated trait anxiety and neuromuscular activ- stuttering participants. For instance, Hancock et al. (1998)
ity in a sample of 32 adults who stuttered and 28 controls. investigated the long-term effectiveness of three stuttering
The stuttering group demonstrated significantly higher trait treatments in a large sample of children and adolescents age
anxiety when compared with controls, but trait anxiety was 11 to 18 years, as originally reported by Craig et al. (1996).
not found to be related to sensorimotor activity, as evaluated Seventy-seven participants completed the STAI for chil-
by the startle response. In addition, Blomgren et al. (2005) dren at 12 months posttreatment, and 62 participants completed
investigated the affective functioning of 19 adults who com- the STAI for children between 2 and 6 years posttreatment.
pleted a 3-week intensive stuttering modification treatment No significant differences in state or trait anxiety were found
program. Although significant improvements in psychic and between groups or over time.
somatic anxiety were found up to 6 months posttreatment, Similarly, Davis, Shisca, and Howell (2007) investigated
trait and state anxiety as measured by the STAI was not found state and trait anxiety in a sample of children and adolescents
to decrease significantly over the same posttreatment period. age 10 to 17 years, including 18 participants with persis-
Ezrati-Vinacour and Levin (2004) also compared anxiety tent stuttering, 17 who had recovered from stuttering, and
in 47 adults who stuttered and 47 nonstuttering controls. Trait 19 nonstuttering controls. To evaluate trait anxiety, partici-
anxiety, as measured by the STAI, was higher in the stuttering pants completed the STAI for children. State anxiety was
group when compared with controls, and state anxiety in assessed with a scale consisting of four different speaking-
social communication was higher for severe stutterers when related situations, including asking for help in a shop, talking
compared with mild stutterers and nonstuttering controls. on the phone to a friend, talking with a group of friends,
and answering a question in class. Overall, trait anxiety was
not found to differ significantly between groups, although
1
Methodological issues with the Iverach, Jones, et al. (2009a) report have
the persistent group exhibited higher state anxiety on three
been discussed extensively elsewhere: Manning and Beck (2011) and of the four speaking situations than the recovered and control
Iverach, Jones, et al. (2011). groups.

Iverach et al.: Anxiety and Stuttering 225


Finally, Mulcahy et al. (2008) also used the STAI and the is identified, yet it is of substantial importance in cases where
FNE to assess social anxiety in a sample of 19 adolescents there is insufficient power to detect a significant difference.
who stuttered in comparison with 18 nonstuttering controls. Hence, Menzies et al. suggested that insufficient power in a
In their study, the stuttering group demonstrated significantly large proportion of research studies regarding the presence
higher state, trait, and social anxiety than controls. Further- of anxiety in stuttering may have resulted in the failure to
more, state and trait anxiety for the stuttering group was sig- obtain significant results. Therefore, they advocated the use
nificantly associated with communication difficulties in daily of more rigorous methods and sample sizes based on power
situations. Again, it is possible that the inconsistent nature calculations, with a minimum 80% chance of detecting a
of findings in these studies may, in part, be attributable to the significant effect or difference.
use of unidimensional trait anxiety measures. Other authors have echoed this call (Craig, 1990; Craig
As suggested by Menzies et al. (1999), more consistent & Tran, 2006). For instance, Craig et al. (2003) suggested
results may be obtained with multidimensional measures that small participant numbers in studies investigating anx-
of anxiety among children and adolescents who stutter, such iety and stuttering have potentially increased the risk of
as the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS; committing Type II statistical errors, thereby restricting the
Reynolds & Richmond, 2000). The RCMAS evaluates the ability to detect true differences between stutterers and non-
level and nature of anxiety symptoms in children and ado- stutterers in terms of anxiety levels, as well as failing to
lescents and is composed of four nonoverlapping subscales guarantee a representative sample. In addition, Craig and
regarding physiological anxiety, worry/oversensitivity, social Tran (2006) recently identified 20 studies that investigated
concerns/concentration, and social desirability. Blood and anxiety in people who stuttered in comparison with nonstut-
Blood (2007) utilized the RCMAS in their investigation of tering controls. Of these 20 studies, 13 concluded that adults
anxiety and vulnerability to bullying in 18 children who stut- who stuttered were more anxious than controls, whereas
tered and 18 nonstuttering controls, age 11 to 12 years. In seven studies did not find a significant difference in anxiety
comparison with controls, the stuttering group demonstrated levels between groups. Of particular interest, Craig and
a significantly higher total RCMAS anxiety score and a sig- Tran reported that the seven studies reporting nonsignificant
nificantly higher score on the social concerns scale, indicat- results had a mean of 20 participants per group, whereas the
ing heightened anxiety regarding the expectations of others. 13 studies with significant differences between groups had
Blood, Blood, Maloney, Meyer, and Qualls (2007) also a mean of 50 participants per group. This discovery supports
investigated the relationship between anxiety and self-esteem Menzies et al.’s (1999) claim that insufficient power may
in a sample of 36 adolescents who stuttered and 36 adoles- be one factor responsible for the lack of consistency in find-
cents who did not stutter. Although scores on the RCMAS ings regarding anxiety and stuttering.
were within the normal range for the large majority of partici- However, a number of studies during the last decade have
pants in both the stuttering and control groups, the stuttering employed medium to large sample sizes and have reported
group demonstrated significantly higher levels of anxiety significant findings (e.g., Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004;
when compared with the controls. Overall, further studies are Ginsberg, 2000; Kraaimaat et al., 2002). Other studies have
required regarding the development of anxiety and social also conducted power analyses to determine suitable sam-
phobia in children and adolescents who stutter. This would ple sizes (e.g., DiLollo, Manning, & Neimeyer, 2003). For
include the use of direct measures or diagnostic assessments instance, in evaluating the presence of DSM–IV–TR and
of social anxiety. We concur with Davis et al. (2007) that ICD-10 anxiety disorders in a large sample of 92 adults who
“the work on children who stutter is of great potential impor- stuttered and 920 matched controls, Iverach, O’Brian, et al.
tance as it may provide an indication about the role of anxiety (2009) reported 80% power to detect 2.5 increased odds
in the development of stuttering” (p. 400). with a 5% level of significance. In addition, Blumgart et al.
In sum, the accumulation of research evidence since the (2010) reported higher than 90% power to detect a signifi-
Menzies et al. (1999) review has improved our understand- cant difference in social anxiety between 50 adults who
ing of anxiety and stuttering with the implementation of stuttered and 50 controls.
diagnostic assessments and multidimensional measures of Other studies since the Menzies et al. (1999) review have
anxiety and social phobia. However, mixed results continue reported significant results with moderate to large sample
to emerge when unidimensional trait anxiety measures are sizes, although not all of these have conducted power analy-
employed. Regardless, the current evidence base highlights ses to confirm the ability to detect significant differences
the urgent need for clinical assessment, management, and between groups. For instance, Iverach, Menzies, et al. (2011)
treatment of anxiety and social phobia among adults who investigated the prevalence of social phobia and anxious
stutter (Iverach, O’Brian, et al., 2009). Implications for treat- personality disorder in a sample of 140 adults who stuttered,
ment will be discussed below. and the authors found that roughly one quarter of participants
met criteria for social phobia, and roughly one third of
participants met screening criteria for anxious personality
Participant Numbers disorder. Ezrati-Vinacour and Levin (2004) also found that
The third methodological issue identified by Menzies trait anxiety for a sample of 47 adults who stuttered was
et al. (1999) pertained to the application of small sample higher than 47 nonstuttering controls. Furthermore, Kraaimaat
sizes and insufficient statistical power. In particular, Menzies et al. (2002) also reported significant differences between
et al. stated that the issue of statistical power may not be 89 adults who stuttered and 131 nonstuttering controls in
critical in cases where a significant difference between groups terms of emotional discomfort in social situations.

226 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 20 • 221–232 • August 2011


Significant results have also been reported with small to Changes regarding the treatment status of participants
moderately sized samples of adults and adolescents who since the Menzies et al. (1999) review have been lacking. In
stuttered (e.g., Alm & Risberg, 2007; Blood et al., 2007; particular, the large majority of studies regarding anxiety
Craig et al., 2003; Gabel, Colcord, & Petrosino, 2002; Mahr and stuttering have included samples of adults who had re-
& Torosian, 1999; Menzies et al., 2008; Mulcahy et al., ceived treatment in the past or who were currently seeking
2008). However, a number of other studies have utilized or receiving treatment for stuttering (e.g., Blomgren et al.,
small to moderate samples of people who stuttered and have 2005; Blumgart et al., 2010; Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004;
reported less consistent results. For example, Dietrich and Gabel et al., 2002; Ginsberg, 2000; Iverach, Menzies, et al.,
Roaman (2001) investigated the extent to which perceptions 2011; Iverach, O’Brian, et al., 2009; Kraaimaat et al., 2002;
of speech-related anxiety correlated with physiological Mahr & Torosian, 1999). This has also been the case in
arousal in specific speaking situations in a sample of 24 adults studies of children and adolescents who stutter (e.g., Blood
who stuttered; they found no correlation between predictions et al., 2007; Davis et al., 2007; Mulcahy et al., 2008).
of speech-related anxiety in 20 hypothetical speaking situ- To our knowledge, only one study since the Menzies et al.
ations and skin conduction responses during enactment of (1999) review has divided samples into participants who
four speaking situations. Similarly, Davis et al. (2007) reported have and have not received treatment for stuttering (Craig
no significant differences in trait anxiety between 35 adoles- et al., 2003). As previously mentioned, Craig et al. (2003)
cents and children who stuttered and 19 nonstuttering con- conducted a randomized population study to investigate
trols. However, state anxiety for participants with persistent anxiety in treated and untreated adults who stuttered. Of
stuttering was found to be higher than for participants who 63 stuttering adults who completed the STAI, 33 reported
had recovered from stuttering and nonstuttering controls. no previous treatment for stuttering. Trait anxiety for these
Furthermore, Blomgren et al. (2005) reported that state and 33 participants did not differ significantly from nonstutter-
trait anxiety (as measured by the STAI) was not found to ing controls, yet stutterers who had received previous treat-
change following stuttering treatment with a sample of ment demonstrated significantly higher trait anxiety than
19 adults who stuttered. controls. These findings suggest that there may be differ-
Overall, it appears that use of larger sample sizes in the ences in anxiety levels for adults who received treatment for
stuttering literature since the Menzies et al. (1999) review stuttering versus those who have not received treatment.
has resulted in a less ambiguous picture of the relationship In sum, the above research suggests a difficulty inherent
between anxiety and stuttering. However, this increased in recruiting stuttering samples from those who have never
clarity of evidence may also stem from the use of measures sought treatment for stuttering. Further research is required
of anxiety and social phobia that are multidimensional or to determine whether differences in anxiety exist between
diagnostic in their assessment. Regardless, it is imperative samples of treated and untreated adults who stutter. In addi-
for future research in this area to contain a priori power anal- tion, it may be useful for future research to investigate whether
yses to ensure sufficient statistical power to detect significant adults who are currently seeking treatment are more or less
differences. anxious than adults who have received treatment in the past
but who are not currently seeking treatment. Having said
this, the comorbid presence of anxiety and stuttering may in-
Treatment Status of Participants crease the chances that an individual will seek treatment for
The fourth methodological issue identified by Menzies his or her stuttering (Iverach, O’Brian, et al., 2009). Further-
et al. (1999) pertained to the treatment status of participants. more, it is likely that adults who stutter may have an increased
In particular, it has been suggested that research findings risk of experiencing anxiety regardless of whether they
regarding the link between anxiety and stuttering may be have received treatment for stuttering (Craig et al., 2009).
confounded by the inclusion of both treated and untreated
adults who stutter (Craig, 1990; Craig et al., 2003; Craig &
Tran, 2006). For instance, Craig has argued that speech treat- Speaking Tasks
ment may potentially reduce anxiety and negative expec- The final methodological issue highlighted by Menzies
tancies, which means that the inclusion of adults who have et al. (1999) pertained to the use of single speaking tasks (e.g.,
received treatment for stuttering has the potential to reduce speaking on the telephone) to elicit or manipulate anxiety
the likelihood of identifying anxiety in stuttering samples. in people who stutter. In particular, Menzies et al. argued that
This may also be the case for adults who are attending stut- no single speaking task is capable of eliciting anxiety in all
tering support groups (e.g., Mahr & Torosian, 1999). participants, and the authors suggested that future research
Given the difficulties inherent in obtaining sizable sam- would benefit from the use of individualized behavioral
ples of adults who stutter but have never received treatment, assessment tasks to maximize the chances of eliciting and
Menzies et al. (1999) suggested that future research would detecting anxiety. That is, participants could be assigned
benefit from dividing samples into those who have and have individualized behavioral tasks specifically aimed at maxi-
not received treatment, thereby gauging the potential impact mizing the experience of anxiety, rather than simply assigning
of speech treatment on anxiety levels. At the time of the the same task for all participants.
Menzies et al. review, no study had divided samples based on Since the Menzies et al. (1999) review, only a small num-
treatment status, nor had any study investigated the presence ber of studies have attempted to manipulate anxiety in people
of anxiety in a sample composed only of untreated adults who stutter. Of particular note, Gabel et al. (2002) inves-
who stuttered. tigated differences in self-reported anxiety for 10 adults who

Iverach et al.: Anxiety and Stuttering 227


stuttered and 10 matched controls. In a similar manner to Further Considerations
in vivo cognitive assessments used in cognitive behavioral
In addition to the methodological issues outlined above, a
treatments, all participants rated their anxiety at specific number of further considerations pertain to future research
moments during a diagnostic speech assessment, including
regarding the relationship between anxiety and stuttering.
(a) three baseline reports, (b) four reports during a period
in which they were thinking about their speech (“the thinking
period”), and (c) six reports during speaking tasks (“the The Development of Anxiety Across the Lifespan
speaking period”). Overall, the adults who stuttered reported Since the Menzies et al. (1999) review, a number of studies
significantly more anxiety throughout the diagnostic assess- have contributed to our understanding of anxiety in children
ment than controls and were more likely to report anxiety and adolescents who stutter (e.g., Blood & Blood, 2007;
in speaking situations than in other situations. Davis et al., 2007; Mulcahy et al., 2008). However, we cur-
Similarly, Ezrati-Vinacour and Levin (2004) developed rently know very little about the development of anxiety in
an assessment of task-related anxiety for their sample of children (Furnham & Davis, 2004). Studies regarding the
47 adults who stuttered and 47 controls. Participants were progression of anxiety over the lifespan could be of signifi-
asked to evaluate their anxiety after performing two speech cant benefit to our understanding of the etiology, management,
tasks and two nonspeech tasks: (a) a conversation about and treatment of anxiety (Craig & Tran, 2005). In particular,
personal information, (b) reading a passage aloud, (c) silent Craig and Tran (2005) have suggested the following:
reading of a passage, and (d) listening to a recorded passage.
Participants rated their anxiety in each of these situations What is really needed is an investigation that measures
the progression of anxiety in PWS [people who stutter] in
on a scale ranging from “extremely anxious” to “not at all
the community using randomized and stratified selection
anxious.” Anxiety in speech-related tasks was found to be
procedures. Such a sample is far more likely to provide
higher for adults with more severe stuttering, indicating that
data representative of the population of people who
stuttering severity may play a role in the experience of anx-
stutter. (p. 44)
iety in social communication situations.
These studies provide valuable information about the This could also include investigations regarding differences
assessment of anxiety across multiple tasks. However, the between stuttering people who develop social anxiety and
anxiety assessments used in both studies were not inclusive those who do not (Craig & Tran, 2006).
of a wide range of anxiety-provoking situations for people
who stutter (e.g., ordering food, taking in front of a group,
or talking in a public place), nor were they individually as-
The Relationship Between Anxiety
signed to maximize the experience of anxiety for individual and Stuttering Severity
participants. Further limitations also apply to other studies There are some indications in the literature that a rela-
that have attempted to evaluate anxiety across speaking tionship may exist between anxiety and stuttering severity.
tasks. For instance, although Davis et al. (2007) evaluated However, current research evidence regarding this relation-
participants’ self-perceived state anxiety in four different ship is inconclusive. For instance, Blumgart et al. (2010)
speaking situations, this was done by self-report, not by reported that stuttering severity was not associated with any
actual exposure to these situations. Likewise, a small number anxiety measure or with increased symptoms of social anx-
of studies have also utilized routine and challenging phone iety disorders. Other studies have also reported a null rela-
calls as the basis for calculating stuttering frequency for tionship between stuttering severity and trait anxiety (e.g.,
adults who stuttered in their samples, including ratings of Alm & Risberg, 2007; Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004;
participants’ subjective units of distress following each call Miller & Watson, 1992), between stuttering severity and
to gauge how anxiety-provoking the phone call was (e.g., social phobia (Menzies et al., 2008), and between pretreat-
Iverach, Jones, et al., 2009a; Menzies et al., 2008). However, ment stuttering severity and the presence of mental health
these studies did not include any other individually assigned disorders (Iverach, Jones, et al., 2009a). However, Ezrati-
behavioral tasks specifically aimed at eliciting anxiety. Vinacour and Levin (2004) reported that adults in their sam-
To our knowledge, only one study since the Menzies et al. ple with more severe stuttering exhibited higher state anxiety.
(1999) review has utilized individualized behavioral experi- Craig et al. (2009) have also suggested that stuttering se-
ments as a means of treating anxiety in adults who stutter. verity may increase the risk of poorer emotional functioning.
Helgadottir, Menzies, Onslow, Packman, and O’Brian (2009b) Perhaps these inconsistent findings relate to the different
reported on the use of a web-based CBT program for the strategies used by those affected to cope with stuttering
treatment of anxiety in two stuttering adults diagnosed with (Plexico et al. 2009). Therefore, future research would bene-
social phobia. As part of the computerized program, partic- fit from explorations of the relationship between stuttering
ipants engaged in individually formulated behavioral experi- severity and the experience of anxiety in social situations.
ments to facilitate exposure to feared situations or experiences.
This also included the provision of additional cognitive
restructuring exercises. Therefore, the methodological issues Treatments for Anxiety in Stuttering
regarding speaking tasks used in stuttering research as raised Ingham’s (1984) review mentioned a number of anxiety-
by Menzies et al. still requires attention in future research modification techniques used to treat stuttering, including
and may benefit from applications to treatment of anxiety in reciprocal inhibition, hypnotherapy, drug therapy, and bio-
adults who stutter. feedback. He noted the use of reciprocal inhibition therapy

228 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 20 • 221–232 • August 2011


(Wolpe, 1958) to systematically desensitize and inhibit anxi- web-based CBT program for the treatment of social anxiety
ety in people who stutter, including the application of relax- among adults who stutter. This web-based program was
ation techniques and psychotherapy. Ingham suggested designed to imitate the complex relationship between a client
that, in some cases, the theoretical foundations underpinning and therapist by using individualized formulations based on
these approaches were “shaky” (p. 135) and lacking objective Clark and Wells’s (1995) cognitive model of social phobia.
speech behavior data obtained in “clinically meaningful One of the most significant benefits of this program is that it
speaking situations” (p. 141). Moreover, treatment effects facilitates accessible and cost-effective treatment for social
reported for such approaches were typically not associated phobia for adults who stutter without reliance on adminis-
with reductions in stuttering and were not sufficient to justify tration by clinicians. Preliminary evidence from a Phase I
their primary use. Thus, Ingham concluded, “Against this trial has demonstrated the utility of this online CBT program
backdrop of relatively unprofitable research, it seems unlikely in eliminating social phobia in adults who stutter and sig-
that treatments directed towards anxiety reduction would nificantly improving unhelpful cognitions, fear of negative
prove to have general therapeutic merit” (p. 133). evaluation, behavioral avoidance, quality of life, and low
However, research evidence regarding the relationship mood (Helgadottir et al., 2009b).
between anxiety and stuttering since Ingham’s (1984) review In addition to clinician- or computer-delivered CBT, mea-
has become more robust and convincing, and evidence for sures regarding the cognitions associated with anxiety in
treatment practices such as CBT has grown. Consequently, stuttering have also been developed. For instance, Iverach,
the urgent need for clinical assessment, management, and Menzies, et al. (2011) and St Clare et al. (2009) reported
treatment of anxiety and social phobia among adults who on the validation of the Unhelpful Thoughts and Beliefs
stutter has been highlighted in a number of reviews (e.g., About Stuttering (UTBAS) scales, which were developed to
Menzies et al., 2008). evaluate the frequency of negative thoughts and beliefs as
One of the most compelling reasons why treatment of well as anxiety associated with stuttering. The UTBAS was
anxiety in adults who stutter is of crucial importance is based originally developed via a comprehensive file audit of un-
on findings that mental health disorders, including anxiety helpful cognitions of adults who stuttered involved in a CBT
disorders, have the potential to result in failure to maintain program for anxiety over a 10-year period (St Clare et al.,
the benefits of speech-restructuring treatment in the short 2009). St Clare et al. reported that the UTBAS was capable
and medium term (Iverach, Jones, et al., 2009a). In addition, of discriminating between unhelpful thoughts related to so-
the presence of anxiety disorders has also been associated cial anxiety for the adults who stuttered and control partic-
with increased avoidance of speaking situations following ipants, and was also sensitive to changes in social anxiety
treatment for stuttering (Iverach, Jones, et al., 2009a). Not following CBT treatment.
surprisingly, adults who stutter have also been found to report Following on from this, Iverach, Menzies, et al. (2011)
that stuttering treatment does not adequately address speaking- administered the UTBAS scales to a sample of 140 adults
related fears and concerns (Cream et al., 2003; Yaruss, seeking treatment for stuttering; the authors found that mean
Quesal, & Murphy, 2002), and some adults who stutter may UTBAS scores were significantly higher for participants
still report considerable levels of anxiety following speech who met criteria for social phobia and first-stage screening
treatment (Craig & Tran, 2006). Therefore, the success of criteria for anxious personality disorder. These findings
speech treatment may depend on prior or concurrent manage- support the use of the UTBAS scales in assessing negative
ment of anxiety (Attanasio, 2000). thoughts related to anxiety among adults who stutter. There-
CBT is the most efficacious intervention available for fore, a tutorial has been developed for speech-language pa-
treating social anxiety (Andrews, Crino, Hunt, Lampe, & thologists regarding the use of CBT with adults who stutter,
Page, 2003) and has been evaluated extensively in non- including application of the UTBAS to identify unhelpful
stuttering populations (Heimberg, 2002). Presumably this thoughts and beliefs associated with stuttering (Menzies et al.,
has prompted a number of studies that have reviewed or 2009).
investigated the use of CBT in the treatment of anxiety in On the basis of the above evidence, it is clear that future
adults who stutter (Kawai, 2010; Menzies, Onslow, Packman, collaboration between speech-language therapists, psychia-
& O’Brian, 2009; Neilson, 1999; Stein et al., 1996). In the trists, and clinical psychologists in the assessment and
first randomized controlled trial of CBT treatment, Menzies treatment of stuttering is warranted (Ginsberg, 2000; Iverach,
et al. (2008) reported that CBT for anxiety was associated O’Brian, et al., 2009). It may also be necessary for psycho-
with significant reductions in anxiety and avoidance, as well logical measures and treatment strategies to be integrated
as the elimination of social phobia diagnoses at 12-month into standard speech treatment practices (Craig & Tran, 2006).
follow-up. The CBT treatment package was also associated This could also include more adequate training for speech-
with significant improvements in global functioning even language pathologists in terms of addressing anxiety-related
though there were no significant differences in rates of flu- aspects of the disorder.
ency for the CBT group and the group that received speech
restructuring only. In contrast, 50% of participants who had DSM–IV–TR Social Phobia Exclusion Criteria
received speech restructuring only still met criteria for social
phobia at 12-month follow-up. These findings highlight the for Adults Who Stutter
utility of CBT for social anxiety among adults who stutter. The DSM–IV–TR currently prohibits a diagnosis of social
Helgadottir, Menzies, Onslow, Packman, and O’Brian phobia in cases where social anxiety and avoidance relate to a
(2009a, 2009b) also reported on the development of a general medical condition such as stuttering. This exclusion

Iverach et al.: Anxiety and Stuttering 229


criterion is at odds with growing evidence of clinically signifi- Alm, P. A., & Risberg, J. (2007). Stuttering in adults: The acous-
cant levels of social anxiety among adults who stutter (Iverach, tic startle response, temperamental traits, and biological factors.
O’Brian, et al., 2009; Stein et al., 1996) and also has the Journal of Communication Disorders, 40, 1–41.
potential to limit treatment options for adults who stutter American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and sta-
tistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision).
(Craig & Tran, 2006). In particular, Stein et al. (1996) have
Washington, DC: Author.
argued, “Prohibiting a diagnosis of social phobia is likely Andrews, G., Craig, A., Feyer, A., Hoddinott, S., Howie, P.,
to lead clinicians, under the misconception that social phobia & Neilson, M. (1983). Stuttering: A review of research find-
is an inevitable, expected concomitant of stuttering, to ig- ings and theories circa 1982. Journal of Speech and Hearing
nore clinically relevant social anxiety when it occurs in their Disorders, 48, 226–246.
patients who stutter” (p. 279). Andrews, G., Crino, R., Hunt, C., Lampe, L., & Page, A.
In countries such as Australia, government rebates for (2003). The treatment of anxiety disorders: Clinician guides and
mental health treatment are only available for people with a patient manuals (2nd ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge
mental health diagnosis. This poses a significant limitation University Press.
for stuttering adults who cannot receive funded treatment for Attanasio, J. (2000). A meta-analysis of selected studies in anxiety
and stuttering: Response to Menzies et al., 1999. American
social phobia without an appropriate diagnosis. Therefore, Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 89–91.
changing the DSM–IV–TR exclusion criterion for the release Blomgren, M., Roy, N., Callister, T., & Merrill, R. M. (2005).
of the fifth edition of the DSM in 2012 is critical in terms Intensive stuttering modification therapy: A multidimensional
of increasing anxiety treatment options for adults who stutter, assessment of treatment outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language,
as well as for improving the quality of life of these individ- and Hearing Research, 48, 509–523.
uals (Blumgart et al., 2010). Once this exclusion criterion Blood, G. W., & Blood, I. M. (2007). Preliminary study of self-
has changed, it will be interesting to see the impact this has reported experience of physical aggression and bullying of
on treatment options for stuttering people who report debili- boys who stutter: Relation to increased anxiety. Perceptual
tating levels of social anxiety. and Motor Skills, 104, 1060–1066.
Blood, G. W., Blood, I. M., Bennett, S., Simpson, K. C., &
Susman, E. J. (1994). Subjective anxiety measurements and
Conclusion cortisol responses in adults who stutter. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research, 37, 760–768.
More than 2 decades ago, Ingham (1984) reviewed the Blood, G. W., Blood, I. M., Maloney, K., Meyer, C., & Qualls,
literature regarding anxiety and stuttering, and concluded C. D. (2007). Anxiety levels in adolescents who stutter. Journal
that “the overall tenor of the findings from the studies re- of Communication Disorders, 40, 452–469.
viewed is that there is little evidence of a clinically significant, Bloodstein, O. (1995). A handbook on stuttering (5th ed.). San
or even theoretically palpable, relationship between stuttering Diego, CA: Singular.
and anxiety” (p. 132). Over a decade later, Menzies et al. Blumgart, E., Tran, Y., & Craig, A. (2010). Social anxiety
(1999) conducted a further review to shed more light on the disorder in adults who stutter. Depression and Anxiety, 27,
poorly understood relationship between anxiety and stuttering. 687–692.
Bricker-Katz, G., Lincoln, M., & McCabe, P. (2009). A life-
Menzies et al. highlighted five core methodological issues time of stuttering: How emotional reactions to stuttering impact
in the research and concluded that the relationship between activities and participation in older people. Disability and
anxiety and stuttering could not be fully understood until the Rehabilitation, 31, 1742–1752.
above methodological limitations had been overcome. Bricker-Katz, G., Lincoln, M., & McCabe, P. (2010). Older
The aim of the current review was to review the research people who stutter: Barriers to communication and perceptions
to date with reference to these methodological issues. Over- of treatment. International Journal of Language & Communi-
all, research since the Menzies et al. (1999) review has cation Disorders, 45, 15–30.
produced far stronger evidence for a relationship between Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social
stuttering and social anxiety. However, a number of method- phobia. In R. Heimberg, M. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R.
ological issues and theoretical gaps in the literature persist Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and
treatment (pp. 69–93). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
and require further exploration. Future research efforts should Craig, A. (1990). An investigation into the relationship between
be aimed at improving research design, increasing statisti- anxiety and stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
cal power, employing diagnostic assessments and multi- 55, 290–294.
dimensional measures of anxiety, and developing anxiolytic Craig, A., Blumgart, E., & Tran, Y. (2009). The impact of stut-
treatment options for stuttering people who report anxiety. tering on the quality of life in adults who stutter. Journal of
Fluency Disorders, 34, 61–71.
Craig, A., Hancock, K., Chang, E., McCready, C., Shepley, A.,
Acknowledgment & McCaul, A. (1996). A controlled clinical trial for stuttering
persons aged 9 to 14 years. Journal of Speech and Hearing
This review was supported by Program Grant 633007 from the
Research, 39, 808–826.
National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia.
Craig, A., Hancock, K., Tran, Y., & Craig, M. (2003). Anxiety
levels in people who stutter: A randomized population study.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46,
References 1197–1206.
Alm, P. A. (2004). Stuttering, emotions, and heart rate during Craig, A., & Tran, Y. (2005). The epidemiology of stuttering: The
anticipatory anxiety: A critical review. Journal of Fluency need for reliable estimates of prevalence and anxiety levels over
Disorders, 29, 123–133. the lifespan. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology, 7, 41–46.

230 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 20 • 221–232 • August 2011


Craig, A., & Tran, Y. (2006). Fear of speaking: Chronic anxiety between mental health disorders, stuttering severity and treat-
and stammering. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 12, 63–68. ment outcomes among adults who stutter. Journal of Fluency
Cream, A., Onslow, M., Packman, A., & Llewellyn, G. (2003). Disorders, 34, 29–43.
Protection from harm: The experience of adults after therapy Iverach, L., Jones, M., O’Brian, S., Block, S., Lincoln, M.,
with prolonged-speech. International Journal of Language & Harrison, E., . . . Onslow, M. (2009b). Screening for personal-
Communication Disorders, 38, 379–395. ity disorders among adults seeking speech treatment for stutter-
Davis, S., Howell, P., & Cooke, F. (2002). Sociodynamic rela- ing. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 34, 173–186.
tionships between children who stutter and their non-stuttering Iverach, L., Menzies, R. G., Jones, M., O’Brian, S., Packman,
classmates. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and A., & Onslow, M. (2011). Further development and valida-
Allied Disciplines, 43, 939–947. tion of the Unhelpful Thoughts and Beliefs About Stuttering
Davis, S., Shisca, D., & Howell, P. (2007). Anxiety in speakers (UTBAS) Scales: Relationship to anxiety and social phobia
who persist and recover from stuttering. Journal of Communi- among adults who stutter. International Journal of Language
cation Disorders, 40, 398–417. and Communication Disorders, 46, 286–299. doi:10.3109/
Dietrich, S., & Roaman, M. H. (2001). Physiological arousal and 13682822.2010.495369.
predictions of anxiety by people who stutter. Journal of Fluency Iverach, L., O’Brian, S., Jones, M., Block, S., Lincoln, M.,
Disorders, 26, 207–225. Harrison, E., . . . Onslow, M. (2009). Prevalence of anxiety
DiLollo, A., Manning, W. H., & Neimeyer, R. A. (2003). Cogni- disorders among adults seeking speech therapy for stuttering.
tive anxiety as a function of speaker role for fluent speakers and Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 928–934.
persons who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 28, 167–186. Kawai, N. (2010). An overview of cognitive-behavioral therapy
Endler, N. S., Edwards, J. M., & Vitelli, R. (1991). Endler strategies for stuttering. Japan Journal of Logopedics and
Multidimensional Anxiety Scales ( EMAS): Manual. Los Angeles, Phoniatrics, 51, 269–273.
CA: Western Psychological Services. Kraaimaat, F. W., Vanryckeghem, M., & Van Dam-Baggen, R.
Ezrati-Vinacour, R., & Levin, I. (2004). The relationship be- (2002). Stuttering and social anxiety. Journal of Fluency Dis-
tween anxiety and stuttering: A multidimensional approach. orders, 27, 319–331.
Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29, 135–148. Langevin, M., Packman, A., & Onslow, M. (2009). Peer responses
Ezrati-Vinacour, R., Platzky, R., & Yairi, E. (2001). The young to stuttering in the preschool setting. American Journal of
child’s awareness of stuttering-like disfluency. Journal of Speech, Speech-Language Pathology, 18, 264–276.
Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 368–380. Mahr, G. C., & Torosian, T. (1999). Anxiety and social phobia
Furnham, A., & Davis, S. (2004). Involvement of social factors in in stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 24, 119–126.
stuttering: A review and assessment of current methodology. Manning, W., & Beck, J. G. (2011). Comments concerning
Stammering Research, 1, 112–122. Iverach, et al., Screening for personality disorders among adults
Gabel, R. M., Colcord, R. D., & Petrosino, L. (2002). Self- seeking speech treatment for stuttering. Journal of Fluency
reported anxiety of adults who do and do not stutter. Perceptual Disorders, 36, 61–65.
and Motor Skills, 93, 775–784. Menzies, R. G., O’Brian, S., Onslow, M., Packman, A., St Clare,
Ginsberg, A. P. (2000). Shame, self-consciousness, and locus of T., & Block, S. (2008). An experimental clinical trial of a cog-
control in people who stutter. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, nitive behavior therapy package for chronic stuttering. Journal
161, 389–399. of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1451–1464.
Hancock, K., Craig, A., McCready, C., McCaul, A., Costello, D., Menzies, R. G., Onslow, M., & Packman, A. (1999). Anxiety
Campbell, K., & Gilmore, G. (1998). Two- to six-year and stuttering: Exploring a complex relationship. American
controlled-trial stuttering outcomes for children and adolescents. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 3–10.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, Menzies, R. G., Onslow, M., Packman, A., & O’Brian, S.
1242–1252. (2009). Cognitive behavior therapy for adults who stutter: A
Heimberg, R. G. (2002). Cognitive behavioral therapy for social tutorial for speech-language pathologists. Journal of Fluency
anxiety disorder: Current status and future directions. Biological Disorders, 34, 187–200.
Psychiatry, 51, 101–108. Messenger, M., Onslow, M., Packman, A., & Menzies, R.
Helgadottir, F., Menzies, R., Onslow, M., Packman, A., & (2004). Social anxiety in stuttering: Measuring negative social
O’Brian, S. (2009a). Online CBT I: Bridging the gap between expectancies. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29, 201–212.
Eliza and modern online CBT treatment packages. Behaviour Miller, S., & Watson, B. C. (1992). The relationship between
Change, 26, 245–253. communication attitude, anxiety, and depression in stutterers
Helgadottir, F., Menzies, R., Onslow, M., Packman, A., & and nonstutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
O’Brian, S. (2009b). Online CBT II: A Phase I trial of a stand- 35, 789–798.
alone, online CBT treatment program for social anxiety in Mulcahy, K., Hennessey, N., Beilby, J., & Byrnes, M. (2008).
stuttering. Behaviour Change, 26, 254–270. Social anxiety and the severity and typography of stuttering in
Huber, A., Packman, A., Quine, S., Onslow, M., & Simpson, J. adolescents. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 33, 306–319.
(2004). Improving our clinical interventions for stuttering: Can Neilson, M. D. (1999). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of adults
evidence from qualitative research contribute? Advances in who stutter: The process and the art. In F. Curlee (Ed.), Stutter-
Speech-Language Pathology, 6, 174–181. ing and related disorders of fluency (2nd ed., pp. 181–199).
Ingham, R. (1984). Stuttering and behavior therapy: Current status New York, NY: Thieme.
and experimental foundations. San Diego, CA: College Hill O’Brian, S., Jones, M., Packman, A., Menzies, R., & Onslow, M.
Press. (2011). Stuttering severity and educational attainment. Journal of
Iverach, L., Jones, M., Menzies, R. G., O’Brian, S., Packman, A., Fluency Disorders, 36, 86–92.
& Onslow, M. (2011). Response to Walter Manning and J. Gayle Orsillo, S. M., & Hammond, C. (2001). Social phobia: A brief
Beck: Comments concerning Iverach, Jones et al. (2009a). overview and guide to assessment. In M. M. Antony, S. M.
Journal of Fluency Disorders, 36, 66–71. Orsillo, & E. Roemer (Eds.), Practitioner’s guide to empirically
Iverach, L., Jones, M., O’Brian, S., Block, S., Lincoln, M., based measures of anxiety (pp. 159–164). New York, NY:
Harrison, E., . . . Onslow, M. (2009a). The relationship Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

Iverach et al.: Anxiety and Stuttering 231


Packman, A., Menzies, R. G., & Onslow, M. (2000). Anxiety Stein, M. B., Baird, A., & Walker, J. R. (1996). Social phobia
and the anticipatory struggle hypothesis. American Journal of in adults with stuttering. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 88–89. 278–280.
Peters, H. F. M., & Hulstijn, W. (1984). Stuttering and anxiety. Turnbaugh, K. R., Guitar, B., & Hoffman, P. R. (1979). Speech
Journal of Fluency Disorders, 9, 67–84. clinicians’ attribution of personality traits as a function of stut-
Plexico, L. W., Manning, W. H., & Levitt, H. (2009). Coping tering severity. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22,
responses by adults who stutter: Part I. Protecting the self and 37–45.
others. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 34, 87–107. Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Dancu, C. V. (1996). Social
Poulton, R., & Andrews, G. (1994). Appraisal of danger and Phobia and Anxiety Inventory: Manual. Toronto, Ontario,
proximity in social phobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
32, 639–642. Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative
Reich, J. (2000). The relationship of social phobia to avoidant anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33,
personality disorder: A proposal to reclassify avoidant person- 448–457.
ality disorder based on clinical empirical findings. European Weber, C. M., & Smith, A. (1990). Autonomic correlates of
Psychiatry, 15, 151–159. stuttering and speech assessed in a range of experimental tasks.
Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (2000). Revised Children’s Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 690–706.
Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS): Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford,
Western Psychological Services. CA: Stanford University Press.
Schneier, F. R., Wexler, K. B., & Liebowitz, M. R. (1997). World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 classification
Social phobia and stuttering. American Journal of Psychiatry, of mental and behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for
154, 131. research. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R. E., Vagg, P., Yaruss, J. S., Quesal, R. W., & Murphy, B. (2002). National
& Jacobs, G. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inven- Stuttering Association members’ opinions about stuttering
tory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. treatment. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 27, 227–242.
St Clare, T., Menzies, R. G., Onslow, M., Packman, A., Zimmerman, M., & Mattia, J. I. (2001). A self-report scale to
Thompson, R., & Block, S. (2009). Unhelpful thoughts and help make psychiatric diagnoses: The Psychiatric Diagnostic
beliefs linked to social anxiety in stuttering: Development of a Screening Questionnaire. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58,
measure. International Journal of Language & Communication 787–794.
Disorders, 44, 338–351.

232 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 20 • 221–232 • August 2011


Anxiety and Stuttering: Continuing to Explore a Complex Relationship

Lisa Iverach, Ross G. Menzies, Sue O'Brian, Ann Packman, and Mark Onslow
Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2011;20;221-232; originally published online Apr 8, 2011;
DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0091)

The references for this article include 12 HighWire-hosted articles which you can
access for free at: http://ajslp.asha.org/cgi/content/full/20/3/221#BIBL

This article has been cited by 1 HighWire-hosted article(s) which you can access for
free at:
http://ajslp.asha.org/cgi/content/full/20/3/221#otherarticles

This information is current as of November 13, 2012

This article, along with updated information and services, is


located on the World Wide Web at:
http://ajslp.asha.org/cgi/content/full/20/3/221

You might also like