You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [University of Connecticut]

On: 13 October 2014, At: 14:28


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The Clinical Neuropsychologist


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ntcn20

WAIS-III Matrix Reasoning Test Performance in a


Mixed Clinical Sample
Anthony T. Dugbartey , Phyllis N. Sanchez , J. Gail Rosenbaum , Roderick K. Mahurin , J.
Mark Davis & Brenda D. Townes
Published online: 09 Aug 2010.

To cite this article: Anthony T. Dugbartey , Phyllis N. Sanchez , J. Gail Rosenbaum , Roderick K. Mahurin , J. Mark
Davis & Brenda D. Townes (1999) WAIS-III Matrix Reasoning Test Performance in a Mixed Clinical Sample, The Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 13:4, 396-404

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/1385-4046(199911)13:04;1-Y;FT396

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of
the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied
upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall
not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other
liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
The Clinical Neuropsychologist 1385-4046/99/1304-396$15.00
1999, Vol. 13, No. 4, pp. 396-404 © Swets & Zeitlinger

WAIS-III Matrix Reasoning Test Performance in a Mixed


Clinical Sample*

Anthony T. Dugbartey1, Phyllis N. Sanchez1, J. Gail Rosenbaum1, Roderick K. Mahurin1,2, J.


Mark Davis3, and Brenda D. Townes1,4

1University of Washington, Seattle, 2Battelle Memorial Research Institute, Seattle, 3University of Georgia,
Athens, 4Bogaziçi University, Istanbul, Turkey
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

ABSTRACT

This study examined the relationship between the Matrix Reasoning subtest (MRT) of the WAIS-III and
a selected number of neuropsychological tests in a heterogeneous clinical sample of English-speaking
American (n = 41), and non-English-speaking immigrant (n = 14) adults. A moderate association between
the Halstead Category Test and the MRT (–.58) was found in the English-speaking sample. Multiple re-
gression analysis revealed a significant association between measures of verbal abstract reasoning and
verbal fluency, and performance on the MRT. Among the immigrant sample, the MRT was also found to
be significantly associated with verbal fluency task performance, as well as with the Comprehensive Test
of Nonverbal Intelligence. Correlational analyses therefore suggest a strong verbal mediation element in
the MRT, and that labeling it a nonverbal task may be misleading.

Approximately 60 years after David Wechsler’s however, and perhaps beyond the actual ‘per-
intelligence test was initially introduced (Wechs- ceptual inputs’ (Carpenter, Just, & Shell, 1990)
ler, 1939), the third edition of the Wechsler of the presented content, is an assessment of
Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III; Wechsler, higher level reasoning abilities that has various-
1997a) has, for the first time, incorporated a ly been labeled ‘g’ (Spearman, 1927, 1964),
subtest adapted from the Raven Progressive Ma- ‘fluid’ intelligence (Cattell, 1963), or even a
trices Test (RPM; Raven, 1938). The primary form of working memory capability (Carpenter,
reason for this addition was to increase the test’s Just, & Shell, 1990; Salthouse, 1993) that typi-
utility in evaluating novel, abstract, and nonver- cally is not domain specific (Prabhakaran,
bal reasoning abilities (The Psychological Cor- Smith, Desmond, Glover, & Gabireli, 1997).
poration, 1997). Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test, which was
The matrix reasoning paradigm is only one inspired by Spearman’s concept of intelligence,
subset of the broad spectrum of rule induction is considered by many to be the ‘‘gold stan-
tests. As Alderton and Larson (1990) have not- dard’’ of inductive reasoning tasks (Alderton &
ed, different types of inductive reasoning tests Larson, 1990; Mills, Ablard, & Brody, 1993;
can be devised by simply varying the content of Prabhakaran, Smith, Desmond, Glover, & Ga-
presentation, which may range from figural de- brieli, 1997; van den Broek & Bradshaw, 1994).
signs to numerical or verbal stimuli. Basically, Perhaps because of their versatility, tests of

*
Address correspondence to: Anthony T. Dugbartey, now at Forensic Psychiatric Services Commission,
2840 Nanaimo Street, Victoria, B.C. Canada V8T 4W9.
Accepted for publication: May 14, 1999.
WAIS-III MATRIX REASONING TEST PERFORMANCE 397

matrix reasoning abilities have been included in ably, with the most frequent presenting diagnoses
a number of other cognitive ability measures being closed-head trauma (n = 8), industrial neuro-
still in use today, such as the Stanford-Binet In- toxin exposure (n = 6), unipolar depression (n = 5),
alcoholism (n = 4), asymptomatic HIV disease (n =
telligence Scale-4th Edition (Thorndike, Hagen,
3), cerebral neoplasms (n = 2), and subarachnoid
& Sattler, 1986), and the Kaufman tests (Kauf- hemorrhage (n = 2); and the rest presented with
man & Kaufman, 1983, 1990). The Standard ver- mixed clinical diagnoses including learning dis-
sion of Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RSPM; ability, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder,
Raven, 1976), which is reported to correlate sig- status post high-voltage electrical injury, and
nificantly with the WAIS-III (i.e., .49 with the chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. The mean
Verbal, .79 with the Performance, and .64 with WAIS-III Full Scale IQ (FSIQ) of the participants
was 96.7 (SD = 13.7).
the Full Scale IQs; The Psychological Corpora- Most of the participants (80.5%) were right-
tion, 1997), also has a strong reputation as a cul- handed. With regard to ethno-racial background,
ture-reduced measure of intelligence and ab-
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

85% identified themselves as Caucasian, African


stract reasoning abilities (O’Leary, Rush, & Americans comprised 10%, while the rest were
Guastello, 1991; Powers, Barkan, Jones, 1986; Native Americans (2.5%) and Hispanic Americans
Spreen & Strauss, 1998; but see Owen, 1992). It (2.5%). None of these participants were either first
is, however, not clear whether the Matrix Rea- or second generation immigrants to the United
States.
soning Test of the WAIS-III (MRT), in spite of
its high association with the RSPM, can be of Measures and Procedure
clinical utility in individuals with neurocogni- The WAIS-III (Wechsler, 1997a) was individually
tive deficits. administered to all participants as part of a com-
The present research involves two studies prehensive clinical neuropsychological evaluation,
aimed at examining the extent to which the MRT which also included the Controlled Oral Word As-
sociation Test (COWAT; Spreen & Benton, 1969),
is associated with other measures of abstract
Trail Making Test (Army Individual Test Battery,
reasoning, divergent thinking, and cognitive 1944), Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (Oster-
flexibility. The first study primarily addresses rieth, 1944; Rey, 1941), and Halstead Category
the strength of the relationship between MRT Test (Reitan & Davison, 1974).
and the Halstead Category Test (HCT; Reitan &
Davison, 1974). The nature of the relationship, Statistical Analysis
if any, between the MRT and other selected In order to facilitate intertest comparisons, the raw
neuropsychological data obtained in this study
problem-solving tasks is also examined. The
were converted into normalized T scores with a
second study addresses the contention that the mean of 50 (SD = 10). Statistical analyses (i.e.,
MRT is a ‘culture-fair and language-free’ including two-tailed paired t test comparisons,
(Wechsler, 1997a, p. 17) test by exploring its Pearson product-moment correlations, and simulta-
utility in a diverse, non-native English-speaking neous multiple regressions) were performed using
immigrant population. the SPSS for Windows Version 7.0 statistical
package (SPSS Inc, 1996). The more conservative
alpha level of .01 was selected as the cutoff for
STUDY 1 significance in light of the number of statistical
analyses performed with a rather modest sample
METHOD size.

Participants
A total of 41 adults who had been referred to a RESULTS
University of Washington-affiliated neuropsychol-
ogy clinic for neuropsychological evaluations were Table 1 presents simple correlations for the neu-
included in this study. The sample comprised 22
ropsychological test data. There was no signifi-
men and 19 women. Mean age at the time of evalu-
ation was 38.2 years (SD = 12.10; range = 17 to 60 cant difference between the sexes on the MRT
yrs), with an average education of 12.5 years (SD (t = –.85, p > .05). The paired sample t test indi-
= 2.81). Reasons for referral varied quite consider- cated a significant difference between the Cate-
398
Table 1. Intercorrelations of Test Variables

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

11. CTT –
12. SRF –.22 –
13. SPF –.95** –.09 –
14. PRF –.83** –.29 –.61** –
15. Animals –.15 –.12 –.05 –.26 –
16. COWAT –.28 –.21 –.20 –.37* –.41** –

ANTHONY T. DUGBARTEY ET AL.


WAIS–III
17. FSIQ –.57** –.15 –.53** –.50** –.49** –.51** –
18. VIQ –.47** –.07 –.43** –.40** –.48** –.50** –.91 –
19. PIQ –.57** –.20 –.51** –.50** –.40** –.42** –.90** –.64** –
10. VCI –.42** –.13 –.39* –.35* –.49** –.50** –.79** –.93** –.50** –
11. WMI –.35** –.03 –.33* –.29 –.37* –.35* –.72** –.79** –.50** –.65** –
12. Comp –.55** –.02 –.56** –.41** –.15 –.31 –.71** –.71** –.58** –.60** –.37* –
13. Sim –.37* –.11 –.33* –.34* –.45** –.59** –.66** –.78** –.41** –.86** –.59** –.43** –
14. MRT –.58** –.20 –.55** –.46** –.45** –.52** –.86** –.71** –.83** –.64** –.54** –.57** –.56** –
15. Pic Arr –.42** –.05 –.40** –.36* –.24 –.20 –.70** –.52** –.76** –.44** –.27 –.53** –.31* –.59** –
16. RCFT–Copy –.44** –.23 –.47** –.24 –.27 –.11 –.36* –.26 –.39* –.26 –.27 –.16 –.12 –.31* –.20 –
TMT (sec)
17. Part A –.41** –.19 –.36* –.37* –.43** –.35* –.55** –.44** –.54** –.37* –.44** –.24 –.42** –.67** –.23 –.41** –
18. Part B –.57** –.32* –.52** –.48** –.42** –.29 –.59** –.53** –.51** –.48** –.59** –.29 –.46** –.61** –.27 –.45** –.65** –

Note. CTT= Total number of errors on the Halstead Category Test; SRF= Symbol Recognition Factor; SPF= Spatial Positioning Factor; PRF= Proportional
Reasoning Factor; Animals = Animal Fluency Test of the COWAT; COWAT= Phonemic Fluency Test of the COWAT; Comp = WAIS–III Comprehension test;
Sim = WAIS–III Similarities test; MRT= WAIS–III Matrix Reasoning Test; Pic Arr = WAIS–III Picture Arrangement Test; RCFT = Rey–Osterrieth Complex
Figure Test.
*p < .05; ** p < .01.
WAIS-III MATRIX REASONING TEST PERFORMANCE 399

gory Test total error scores and MRT (t = 8.51, STUDY 2


p < .001). Recent construct validation findings
(Allen, Goldstein, & Mariano, 1999; Johnstone, METHOD
Holland, & Hewett, 1997) have demonstrated
Participants.
the multidimensionality of the Category Test. Fourteen non-native English-speaking, first gener-
Paired sample t tests were therefore used to com- ation immigrant adults (7 males; 7 females) diag-
pare the means of each of the three Category nosed with a variety of medical, neurological, and
Test subtest clusters (as identified by Johnstone psychiatric disabilities were evaluated. These par-
et al., 1997) and the MRT. There was a signifi- ticipants were primarily referred from international
cant difference between the Symbol Recogni- medicine clinics, community advocacy, and outpa-
tient mental health centers. Presenting complaints
tion/Counting Factor (i.e., subtests 1 & 2 of the and diagnoses ranged from short-term memory
Category Test) and the MRT (t = 23.51, p < loss (28.6%), seizure disorder (14.3%), and cardiac
.001). The Spatial Positioning Factor (compris-
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

disease (14.3%) to a number of other disorders


ing subtests 3, 4, & 7) and MRT scores were not (i.e., cerebrovascular accidents, cerebral neo-
significantly different (t = 1.50, p > .05). Simi- plasms, depression, hypertension, schizophrenia,
larly, no significant difference emerged between and chronic renal disease) that each comprised
the MRT and Proportional Reasoning Factor 7.1% of the study sample. In terms of country of
origin, 5 (36%) were native Cambodians and 4
(subtests 5 & 6) of the Category Test (t = .36, (28%) were from Vietnam, whereas 2 (14%) were
p > .05). native born Iranians. There was one participant
Visuospatial perception skills, as measured each (i.e., 7%) from The Ukraine, Mexico, and
by drawing reproduction accuracy on the Rey- Cuba. The participants’ ages ranged from 23 to 77
Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, was signifi- years (M = 55.56; SD = 17.9). Mean duration of
cantly different from performance on the MRT stay in the United States was 14 years (SD = 6.4),
and average length of pre-migration formal educa-
(t = –21.16, p < .001), with a bivariate correla-
tion was 4.5 years (SD = 4.3; range = 0 to 11).
tion coefficient of .31. The mean MRT score
was significantly different from both the TMT Measures and Procedures
Part A (t = –10.86, p < .001), and the TMT Part The Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence
B (t = –12.30, p < .001) times to completion. (CTONI; Hammill, Pearson, & Wiederholt, 1997),
To investigate the relationship between the MRT subtest of the WAIS-III, and Animal Fluency
MRT and measures of verbal processing, a mul- subtest of the COWAT were individually adminis-
tered in the patient’s native language with the as-
tiple regression analysis was conducted with
sistance of a professional, certified bilingual trans-
MRT as the dependent variable and the follow- lator/interpreter as part of a cross-cultural cogni-
ing verbally mediated tests as predictors: phone- tive evaluation.
mic and semantic verbal fluency tests, the
WAIS-III Verbal Comprehension Index, and the
Comprehension subtest from the WAIS-III. The RESULTS
verbally mediated tests were significant predic-
tors of MRT scores (F (4, 36) = 10.52, p < There was no significant association between
.0001) with an Adjusted R2 of .49. Examination MRT scores and length of pre-migration formal
of the predictor variables revealed the WAIS-III education in this sample (r = .09, p = .75). A
Comprehension scores to be a significant unique significantly high correlation between the MRT
predictor of MRT scores (p < .05). and Animal Fluency test performance (r = .76,
p < .001) was found. A multiple regression anal-
ysis was performed with MRT as the dependent
variable and the three CTONI composite scores
(Nonverbal IQ, Pictorial Nonverbal IQ, & Geo-
metric Nonverbal IQ) as predictors. Results
showed that the CTONI composite indices sig-
nificantly predicted performance on the MRT
400 ANTHONY T. DUGBARTEY ET AL.

(F(3, 10) = 4.98, p < .01; Adjusted R2 = .48). A appears that elementary visuoperceptual abilities
significantly high correlation between the MRT may be a necessary, but not sufficient, require-
and CTONI Nonverbal IQ was found (r = .75, p ment for success on reasoning with matrices.
< .01). It is especially interesting to note that the This view, regarding the non-domain-specificity
Animal Fluency test and CTONI Nonverbal IQ of higher level reasoning with matrices, is not
scores each had similar coefficients of determi- new (Carpenter et al., 1990; Prabhakaran et al.,
nation (i.e., 56.3% and 57.8%, respectively) re- 1997). The finding of a significant relationship
lative to the MRT. between the MRT and measures of higher order
verbal conceptualization and intelligence sug-
gests a strong verbal mediation component of
DISCUSSION MRT performance. The robustness of this un-
derlying verbal processing element is supported
This study was designed to explore three major by the high correlation between verbal fluency
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

issues: (1) an examination of the relationship and the MRT in both our American and non-
between the MRT and the Category Test; (2) English-speaking immigrant samples. It must be
determination of whether the MRT is associated noted, however, that there is evidence to the ef-
with performance on measures of verbal abstract fect that verbal associative fluency scores are
conceptualization and verbally mediated diver- themselves significantly influenced by ethnic
gent thinking; and (3) a comparison of the MRT group membership (Johnson-Selfridge, Zalew-
and a nonverbal intelligence test in a group of ski, & Aboudarham, 1998), although exactly
non-English-speaking immigrants to the United why this is so remains unclear. Additional con-
States. Some shortcomings of this investigation firmatory evidence for this general trend of a
must be acknowledged. First, although generali- verbal mediation in MRT scores is demonstrated
zability in clinical neuropsychological research by the .64 correlation coefficient between the
is enhanced by the inclusion of individuals from MRT and WAIS-III Verbal IQ of the original
a wide variety of diagnostic groupings, the ra- WAIS-III standardization sample (The Psycho-
ther small intra-diagnostic sample sizes did not logical Corporation, 1997).
permit an analysis by diagnostic category. Sec- With some exceptions (Berker & Smith, 1988;
ond, the effect, or more precisely, relationship Drebing, Takushi, Tanzy, Murdock, Stewart, &
between the acculturation process (broadly de- Majovski, 1990), there is a compelling body of
fined as learning as a result of culture contact) evidence (Berman & Weinberger, 1990; Haier et
and performance on the MRT was not investi- al., 1988; Kertesz & McCabe, 1975; Luria, 1973;
gated. This, however, is a legitimate area for Piercy & Smyth, 1962; Villardita, 1985; Zaidel,
future study. Zaidel, & Sperry, 1981) emphasizing the strong
Our results suggest that the MRT is closely contribution of the dominant left hemisphere
associated with aspects of the Category Test that toward success on matrix-oriented (abstract rea-
measure complex spatial abstract reasoning and soning and spatial synthesis) tasks. Thus, the
the use of conceptual rules in reasoning with fact that MRT stimuli are visually presented and
ratios and proportions. Although the correlation expressive language skills are not overtly re-
between MRT and Category Test is rather mo- quired does not necessarily imply that subvocal
dest (–.58), it nevertheless suggests some degree verbalization or other linguistically-mediated
of convergent validity as to the underlying task cognitive problem-solving strategies are not es-
requirements of the MRT. Also, the low coeffi- sential to success on this task. The label ‘‘non-
cient of determination between MRT and the verbal’’ is therefore misleading, because a ver-
copy version of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex bal mediation element seems to be present in the
Figure test may be taken as preliminary evi- MRT. Perhaps a more appropriate descriptive
dence of the relatively minimal contribution of term for such tasks may be ‘‘non-enuntiary’’
visuoperceptual and visuospatial construction (from the Latin derivative of enunciative), rather
skills in MRT performance. In other words, it than ‘‘non-verbal,’’ abstract reasoning.
WAIS-III MATRIX REASONING TEST PERFORMANCE 401

An alternative explanation of the results is Our finding of a very low correlation between
that executive functions, rather than verbal me- length of pre-migration formal education and the
diation, may be the underlying mechanism that MRT is, again, with some exceptions (e.g.,
accounts for the observed correlations between Grossi et al., 1993), consistent with similar re-
the MRT and other measures of higher order ports on both the Coloured (Esquivel, 1984) and
conceptual skills. After all, in a broad sense, rule Standard (Llabre, 1984) versions of the RPM
induction tasks (the rubric under which matrix test, although socioeconomic background (Frey
reasoning tests fall) are themselves measures of & Pinelli, 1991) has been documented to exert a
executive skills in much the same way as the strong differential influence on matrix reasoning
Category Test and the other higher order pro- test performance. The mean length of formal
blem-solving measures employed in this study. education in our non-English-speaking sample
But then the executive functions explanation, was rather small, with 14.3% of the participants
however attractive, begs the question as verbal reporting no history of formal education. This
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

self-regulation of behavior is itself a crucial ele- may be of some import, as cognitive processing
ment of the executive functions construct (for a strategies of healthy illiterates are thought to
thorough discussion of verbal self-regulation as differ qualitatively from individuals with a his-
a process underlying rule-governed behavior, tory of formal education (Reis & Castro-Caldas,
see Lyon & Krasnegor, 1996). 1997; Castro-Caldas, Reis, & Guerreiro, 1997),
Whether described in metacognitive (Dennis, regardless of the actual amount of time spent
Barnes, Donnelly, Wilkinson, & Humphreys, receiving formal schooling.
1996; Paris & Winograd, 1990), strategic plan- The high correlation between the MRT and
ning (Shallice, 1982), or working memory CTONI provides some evidence for the validity
(Owen, Downes, Sahakian, Polkey, & Robbins, of the MRT as a potentially useful contributor to
1990; Pennington, 1994) terms, the skillful use a more comprehensive assessment of intellectual
of strategies in solving novel problems (Es- ability. By implication then, contentions that the
linger, 1996) invariably occurs most effectively validity of the WAIS-III is enhanced by the in-
through the use of language. Moreover, propos- corporation of the MRT (The Psychological
ing executive functions as an explanatory con- Corporation, 1997) receives empirical support
struct does not necessarily negate the verbal pro- by our findings with a non-English-speaking
cessing substrate hypothesis. In other words, the sample. However, the extent to which the MRT
verbal processing and executive functions hy- can be considered a culture-fair intellectual abil-
potheses need not be mutually exclusive. Indeed, ity measure, is more difficult to determine. Cul-
there are strong indications (Denckla, 1996; ture-fairness in psychological tests must be con-
Dugbartey, Rosenbaum, Sanchez, & Townes, sidered as relative constructs. The MRT, or for
1999; Levin, Eisenberg, & Benton, 1991; Levin that matter, CTONI, may only be minimally cul-
et al., 1996; Roberts & Pennington, 1996; Stuss ture-fair to the extent that the testee shares simi-
& Benson, 1986; Taylor, Schatschneider, Petrill, lar test-taking attitudes or epistemological value
Barry, & Owens, 1996; Wang, 1987) that execu- systems implicit in the psychological constructs
tive function tasks measure multiple cognitive being measured. The neuropsychological, cross-
functions, and hence are factorially confounded. cultural, and cultural psychological literature is
Unfortunately, most empirical neuropsychologi- replete with admonishments to the effect that
cal investigations of executive skills do not in- cognitive ability tests are culturally relative
volve manipulations that parse the processing (Ardila, 1995; Artiola i Fortuny & Mullaney,
demands of psychometric tests and break them 1997; Board of Ethnic Minority Affairs Task
down into their component features for greater Force on the Delivery of Services to Ethnic Mi-
understanding. Whether the current findings re- nority Populations, 1991; Boesch, 1996; But-
flect either executive function capabilities or cher, 1982; Greenfield, 1997; Helms, 1992; Loe-
verbal processing components (or a combination wenstein, Argüelles, Argüelles, & Linn-Fuentes,
thereof), awaits further corroborative study. 1994; Neisser et al., 1996; Rogoff & Chavajay,
402 ANTHONY T. DUGBARTEY ET AL.

1995). The assumption that cultural variables Berker, E., & Smith, A. (1988). Diaschisis, site, time
can effectively be controlled solely by eliminat- and other factors in Raven performance of adults
with focal cerebral lesions. International Journal
ing the overt verbalization of responses is te-
of Neuroscience, 38, 267-285.
nuous, because ‘... not only verbal, but also non- Berman, K. F., & Weinberger, D. R. (1990). Latera-
verbal tests may be culturally biased’ (Ardila, lisation of cortical function during cognitive tasks:
1995, p. 146. See also, Irvine, 1969; Jacobs et Regional cerebral blood flow studies of normal
al., 1997). The challenge for neuropsychologists individuals and patients with schizophrenia. Jour-
nal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
engaged in research and clinical practice with 53, 150-160.
culturally diverse populations then, is to famil- Board of Ethnic Minority Affairs Task Force on the
iarize themselves with the dominant value sys- Delivery of Services for Ethnic Minority Popula-
tems and worldviews of their clientele in order tions. (1990). Guidelines for providers of psycho-
to be able to make informed inferences about the logical services to ethnic, linguistic, and culturally
diverse populations. Washington, DC: American
integrity of their neurocognitive functioning le-
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

Psychological Association.
vels from test findings (see for example, Artiola Boesch, E. E. (1996). The seven flaws of cross-cul-
i Fortuny & Mullaney, 1998). tural psychology. The story of a conversion. Mind,
In conclusion, and taken together with a num- Culture, and Activity, 3, 2-10.
ber of empirical reports on other matrices tests, Butcher, J. N. (1982). Cross-cultural research methods
in clinical psychology. In P. C. Kendall & J. N.
our results lead us to propose that the MRT is Butcher (Eds.). Handbook of research methods in
not devoid of an underlying verbal-linguistic clinical psychology (pp. 273-308). New York:
process that contributes to success on this induc- Wiley.
tive reasoning test. However, the MRT’s signifi- Castro-Caldas, A., Reis, A., & Guerreiro, M. (1997).
cant correlation with the Category Test, a well- Neuropsychological aspects of illiteracy. Neuro-
psychological Rehabilitation, 7, 327-338.
established neuropsychological tool (Choca, Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized
Laatsch, Wetzel, & Agresti, 1997), and its po- intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Edu-
tential as a culturally sensitive measure, are cational Psychology, 54, 1-22.
quite encouraging. Choca, J. P., Laatsch, L., Wetzel, L., & Agresti, A.
(1997). The Halstead Category Test: A fifty year
perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 7, 61-75.
Denckla, M. B. (1996). Research on executive func-
REFERENCES tion in a neurodevelopmental context: Application
of clinical measures. Developmental Neuropsy-
Alderton, D. L. & Larson, G. E. (1990). Dimensional- chology, 12, 5-15.
ity of Raven’s Progressive Matrices items. Educa- Dennis, M., Barnes, M. A., Donnelly, R. E., Wilkin-
tional and Psychological Measurement, 50, 887- son, M., & Humphreys, R. P. (1996). Appraising
900. and managing knowledge: Metacognitive skills
Allen, D. N., Goldstein, G., & Mariano, E. (1999). Is after childhood head injury. Developmental Neuro-
the Halstead Category Test a multidimensional psychology, 12, 77-103.
instrument? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Drebing, C. E., Takushi, R. Y., Tanzy, K. S., Mur-
Neuropsychology, 21, 237-244. dock, G. A., Stewart, J. C., & Majovski, L. V.
Ardila, A. (1995). Directions of research in cross-cul- (1990). Re-examination of CPM performance in
tural neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical and lateralized brain injury. Cortex, 26, 661-664.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 143-150. Dugbartey, A. T., Rosenbaum, J. G, Sanchez, P. N., &
Army Individual Test Battery. (1944). Manual of di- Townes, B. T. (1999). Neuropsychological assess-
rections and scoring. Washington, DC: War De- ment of executive functions. Seminars in Clinical
partment, Adjutant General’s Office. Neuropsychiatry, 4, 5-12.
Artiola i Fortuny, L. A., & Mullaney, H. A. (1997). Eslinger, P. J. (1996). Conceptualizing, describing,
Neuropsychology with Spanish speakers: Lan- and measuring components of executive function.
guage use and proficiency issues for test develop- In G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention,
ers. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro- memory, and executive function (pp. 367-395).
psychology, 19, 615-622. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Artiola i Fortuny, L. & Mullaney, H. A. (1998). As- Esquivel, G. B. (1984). Coloured Progressive Matri-
sessing patients whose language you do not know: ces. In D. J. Keyser & R. C. Sweetland (Eds.), Test
Can the absurd be ethical? The Clinical Neuropsy- Critiques. Vol 1 (pp. 206-213.). Missouri: Test
chologist, 12, 113-126. Corporation of America.
WAIS-III MATRIX REASONING TEST PERFORMANCE 403

Frey, P. D., & Pinneli, B. Jr. (1991). Visual discrimi- Levin, H. S., Fletcher, J. M., Kufera, J. A., Harward,
nation and visuomotor integration among two H., Lilly, M. A., Mendelsohn, D., Bruce, D., &
classes of Brazilian children. Perceptual and Mo- Eisenberg, H. M. (1996). Dimensions of cognition
tor Skills, 72, 847-850. measured by the Tower of London and other cogni-
Greenfield, P. M. (1997). You can’t take it with you. tive tasks in head-injured children and adolescents.
Why ability assessments don’t cross cultures. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12, 17-34.
American Psychologist, 52, 1115-1124, Llabre, M. M. (1984). Standard Progressive Matrices.
Grossi, D., Correra, G., Calise, C., Ruscitto, M. A., In D. J. Keyser & R. C. Sweetland (Eds.), Test Cri-
Vecchione, V., Vigliardi, M. V., & Nolfe, G. tiques. Vol 1 (pp. 595-602). Missouri: Test Corpo-
(1993). Evaluation of the influence of illiteracy on ration of America.
neuropsychological performance by elderly per- Loewenstein, D. A., Argüelles, T., Argüelles, S., &
sons. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77, 859-866. Linn-Fuentes, P. (1994). Potential cultural bias in
Haier, R. J., Siegel, B. V. Jr., Nuechterlein, K. H., the neuropsychological assessment of the older
Hazlett, E., Wu, J. C., Paek, J., Browning, H. L., & adult. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro-
Buchsbaum, M. S. (1988). Cortical glucose meta- psychology, 16, 623-629.
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

bolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and at- Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. An introduc-
tention studied with positron emission tomography. tion to neuropsychology. London: Penguin.
Intelligence, 12, 199-217. Lyon, G. R., & Krasnegor, N. A. (Eds.). (1996). At-
Hammill, D. D., Pearson, N. A., & Wiederholt, J. L. tention, memory, and executive function. Balti-
(1997). Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelli- more: P. H. Brookes.
gence. Austin, TX: Pro Ed. Mills, C. J., Ablard, K. E., & Brody, L. E. (1993). The
Helms, J. E. (1992). Why is there no study of cultural Raven’s Progressive Matrices: It’s usefulness for
equivalence in standardized cognitive ability test- identifying gifted/talented students. Roeper Re-
ing? American Psychologist, 47, 1083-1101. view, 15, 183-186.
Irvine, S. H. (1969). Factor analyses of African abili- Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J. Jr., Boykin,
ties and attainments: Constructs across cultures. A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F.,
Psychological Bulletin, 71, 20-32. Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., &
Jacobs, D. M., Sano, M., Albert, S., Schofield, P., Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and un-
Dooneief, G., & Stern, Y. (1997). Cross-cultural knowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77-101.
neuropsychological assessment: A comparison of O’Leary, U-M., Rusch, K. M., & Guastello, S. J.
randomly selected, demographically matched co- (1991). Estimating age-stratified WAIS-R IQs
horts of English and Spanish-speaking older adults. from scores on the Raven’s Standard Progressive
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy- Matrices. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 277-
chology, 19, 331-339. 284.
Johnson-Selfridge, M. T., Zalewski, C., & Aboudar- Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure
ham, J-F. (1998). The relationship between ethnic- complex: Contribution a l’étude de la perception et
ity and word fluency. Archives of Clinical Neuro- de la mémoire. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 286-
psychology, 13, 319-325. 356.
Johnstone, B., Holland, D., & Hewett, J. E. (1997). Owen, K. (1990). The suitability of Raven’s Standard
The construct validity of the Category Test: Is it a Progressive Matrices for various groups in South
measure of reasoning or intelligence? Psychologi- Africa. Personality and Individual Differences, 13,
cal Assessment, 9, 28-33. 149-159.
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). K-ABC: Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey,
Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Circle C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1990). Planning and spa-
Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. tial working memory following frontal lobe lesions
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1990). Kaufman in man. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1021-1034.
Brief Intelligence Test Manual. Circle Pines, MN: Pa ris , S. G ., & Winogra d, P. (1990). H ow
American Guidance Services. metacognition can promote academic learning and
Kertesz, A., & McCabe, P. (1975). Intelligence and instruction. In B. J. Jones & L. Idol (Eds.), Dimen-
aphasia: Performance of aphasics on Raven’s sions of thinking and cognitive instruction (pp. 15-
Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM). Brain and 51). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Language, 2, 387-395. Pennington, B. F. (1994). The working memory func-
Laatsch, L., & Choca, J. (1991). Understanding the tion of the prefrontal cortices: Implications for de-
Halstead Category Test by using item analysis. velopmental and individual differences in cogni-
Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consult- tion. In M. M. Haith, J. Benson, R. Roberts, & B.
ing and Clinical Psychology, 3, 701-704. F. Pennington (Eds.), Future oriented processes in
Levin, H. S., Eisenberg, H. M., & Benton, A. L. development (pp. 243-292). Chicago: University of
(Eds.). (1991). Frontal lobe function and dysfunc- Chicago Press.
tion. New York: Oxford University Press. Powers, S., Barkan, J. H., & Jones, P. B. (1986). Reli-
404 ANTHONY T. DUGBARTEY ET AL.

ability of the Standard Progressive Matrices Test Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. New York:
for Hispanic and Anglo-American children. Per- Macmillan.
ceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 348-350. Spearman, C. (1964). Theory of general factor. British
Piercy, M., & Smyth, V. O. G. (1962). Right hemi- Journal of Psychology, 36, 117-131.
sphere dominance for certain non-verbal intellec- Spreen, O., & Benton, A. L. (1969). Neurosensory
tual skills. Brain, 85, 775-790. Center Comprehensive Examination for Aphasia
Prabhakaran, V., Smith, J. A. L., Desmond, J. E., (NCCEA). Victoria: University of Victoria Neuro-
Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, D. E. (1997). Neural psychology Laboratory.
substrates of fluid reasoning: An fMRI study of Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of
neocortical activation during performance of the neuropsychological tests. (2nd ed.). New York:
Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Cognitive Psy- Oxford University Press.
chology, 33, 43-63. Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986). The frontal
Raven, J. C. (1938). Progressive Matrices. London: lobes. New York: Raven.
H. K. Lewis and Co. The Psychological Corporation (1997). The WAIS-III-
Raven, J. C. (1976). Standard Progressive Matrices. WMS-III Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: Au-
Downloaded by [University of Connecticut] at 14:28 13 October 2014

Oxford, England: Oxford Psychologists Press. thor.


Reis, A., & Castro-Caldas, A. (1997). Illiteracy: A Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M.
cause for biased cognitive development. Journal of (1986). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale: Fourth
the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, Edition: Technical Manual. Chicago: Riverside.
444-450. van den Broek, M. D., & Bradshaw, C. M. (1994).
Reitan, R. M., & Davison, L. A. (1974). Clinical neu- Detection of acquired deficits in general intelli-
ropsychology: Current status and applications. gence using the National Adult Reading Test and
Washington: Winston. Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices. British
Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans les cas Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 509-515.
d’encéphalopathie traumatique. Archives de Villardita, C. (1985). Raven’s colored progressive
Psychologie, 28, 286-340. matrices and intellectual impairment in patients
Roberts, R. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). An inter- with focal brain damage. Cortex, 21, 627-634.
active framework for examining prefrontal cogni- Wang, P. L. (1987). Concept formation and frontal
tive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, lobe function: The search for a clinical frontal lobe
12, 105-126. test. In E. Perecman (Ed.), The frontal lobes revis-
Rogoff, B., & Chavajay, P., (1995). What’s become of ited (pp. 189-205). New York: IRBN.
research on the cultural basis of cognitive develop- Wechsler, D. (1939). Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence
ment. American Psychologist, 50, 859-877. Scale. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
SPSS, Inc. (1996). SPSS Base System 7.0 for Windows Wechsler, D. (1997a). Wechsler Adult Intelligence
User’s Guide. Chicago, IL: Author. Scale-Third Edition. Administration & Scoring
Salthouse, T. A. (1993). Influence of working mem- Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Cor-
ory on adult age differences in matrix reasoning. poration.
British Journal of Psychology, 84, 171-199. Zaidel, E., Zaidel, D., & Sperry, R. W. (1981). Left
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. and right intelligence: Case studies of Raven’s Pro-
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of gressive Matrices following brain bisection and
London, 298, 199-209. hemidecortication. Cortex, 17, 167-186.

You might also like