You are on page 1of 10

International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction


journal homepage: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/ijdrr

Personality, trait EI and coping with COVID 19 measures


Catherine Prentice a, *, Susan Zeidan b, Xuequn Wang c
a
Griffith Business School, Griffith Institute for Tourism Studies, Griffith University, Brisbane, Australia
b
College of Business, Zayed University, Dubai, United Arab Emirates
c
School of Engineering and Information TechnologyMurdoch University Perth, Perth, WA, 6150, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: The study views the preventive measures undertaken by government to combat COVID 19 as stressor for in­
COVID 19 dividuals, and examines how individuals’ personal traits including emotional intelligence and personality factors
Preventive measures influence their coping strategies. The concept of trait EI is used in this study to understand its relationship with
Personality
personality factors and their respective effects on the opted outcomes. Coping strategies in this study are cat­
Emotional intelligence
Stress
egorised into task, emotion and avoidance-oriented coping. The results show that emotional intelligence is
Coping strategies significantly related to all coping strategies whereas only certain personality factors make unique variances.
When both emotional intelligence and personality are in the same equation, with the latter being controlled, the
former shows incremental variance and the influence of personality factors is reduced. Detailed discussion of
these findings and implications for policy makers and researchers are highlighted and conclude the paper.

1. Introduction behaviours [1]. Numerous studies have examined how individuals cope
with health-related stress (e.g., Carr and Umberson, 2013; Folkman,
Covid 19 is a respiratory infection and recurrent disease that has 2011; Krueger and Chang, 2008) and what measures may be effective to
been determined as a pandemic in March 2020. Every country takes prevent the health risk. Government interventions are imperative pre­
similar curative and preventive measures to prevent the spread of Covid ventive measures to control the spread of the virus. These measures are
19 infections and protect lives. The former includes developing vacci­ widely and intensively broadcasted in the media with the intention to
nations and other medical means. The latter primarily refers to gov­ reinforce the importance of behavioural compliance by individuals [2].
ernment interventions including imposing lockdown, curfew, and social However, such extension and intensity also become the major source of
distancing. The preventive measures are crucial to reduce the chances of stressors leading to anxiety and exorbitant distress for individuals
infection and the spreading of diseases. This pandemic has a very taxing (Wheaton et al., 2012). How well individuals cope with such stress has a
detrimental impact on people in particular and society at large, affecting significant impact on their mental health and wellbeing [3]. Very
almost every aspect for individuals, businesses, industries, and limited research has attended to understand how these preventive
countries. measures are perceived as stressors and how individuals cope and
The pandemic not only endangers human life but also encumbers respond to these stressors rather than the pandemic per se.
their daily life. The severity of a pandemic and its associated uncertainty Coping denotes efforts to either ease the individual emotional anxi­
become a stressor that affects individuals’ mental health (Fiorillo and ety provoked by the stressor or modify the source of environmental
Gorwood, 2020; Holmes et al., 2020; Pfefferbaum and North, 2020). anxiety (Duangdao and Roesch, 2008). Personality traits affect how one
When dealing with stressors from the external environment such as perceives external stressors, including health crises, thereby contrib­
disease outbreaks or natural disasters, each individual undertakes uting to the diverse responses to outbreaks (Hill, Turiano, Hurd, Mroc­
different coping strategies and behavioural response (Taha et al., 2014). zek, and Roberts, 2011). Personality traits determine the individual
Protection Motivation Theory (PMT) provides a helpful context for differences in cognitive, emotional, and behavioural performance (e.g.
apprehending how individuals will react to health hazards such as Ref. [4–6], and hence can influence personal responses to health-related
pandemics and suggests that individuals’ drive to defend themselves stress (e.g., pandemics). Previous research such as Jokela et al. (2013),
from a health risk by assessing the risk and undertaking protective Kern and Friedman (2011), and Turiano, Chapman, and Mroczek (2015)

* Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: cathyjournalarticles@gmail.com (C. Prentice), susanzeidan@hotmail.com (S. Zeidan), xuequnwang1600@gmail.com (X. Wang).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2020.101789
Received 8 July 2020; Received in revised form 24 July 2020; Accepted 26 July 2020
Available online 11 August 2020
2212-4209/© 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

indicates that individual personality traits are associated with health the challenge [14]. Folkman and Lazarus [15] and Lazarus and Folkman
status, and are antecedents of health behaviours. Gaygısız. et al. (2012) [11] differentiated among two types of coping: problem-focused and
reveal that general activity, interest levels, and impulsiveness can result emotion-focused coping. The former is focused on solving a problem or
in behavioural changes regarding the later stages in the A/H1N1 virus changing the cause of the stress, e.g., problem solving, information
outbreak. Impulsiveness might be linked to both positive and negative searching, and problem-focused assistance. The latter is exemplified by
behaviours’ as impulsive people are less likely to plan their actions. Also, individuals’ attempting to normalize negative emotions accompanied
tourists who are resistant to external crises such as terrorism and with a stressor by participating in certain activities such as seeking
influenza pandemics score significantly higher extraversion (Hajibaba social-emotional support, self-blame, denial, and wishful thinking.
et al., 2015). Nevertheless, a research void is evident that no studies In an effort to classify the different coping strategies, other re­
have examined how personality may influence individuals’ coping searchers have used terms such as ‘approach’ and ‘avoidant’ strategies
strategies for government interventions. (e.g., Ref. [16]. Approach strategies are strategies that tackle a problem
Since coping is an emotion-driven effort to manage stress, in­ by handling the stressor (e.g., seeking advice and social support)’
dividuals’ emotional abilities manifested in emotional intelligence (EI) whereas, avoidant strategies try to diminish the undesirable emotions by
may shape the coping outcome. EI refers to an individual’s ability to avoiding the stressor (e.g., withdrawal, wishful thinking). According to
perceive, understand, use, and manage emotions of oneself and others Smith et al. [17] and Cohan, Jang, and Stein [18]; of the several coping
and has been reported to influence stress coping (e.g., Ref. [7]. Since its strategies exploited to process and manage stress, three have been highly
official incept into the psychology domain in the early 1990s, EI has researched, including task-oriented coping, emotion-oriented coping,
been extensively discussed and debated in the relevant literature over and avoidance-oriented coping. This study opted for this classification.
the last three decades. The debate is centred on whether this construct In the case of COVID 19, the virus itself as a health stressor entails
should be conceptualised as a personality trait or an intelligence [8–10]. people to take up medical and non-medical measures (e.g., washing
Petrides and his colleagues (2001; 2007; 2016) proposed a via media hands, wearing masks) as individual mandatory tasks to minimise
approach and introduced trait and ability EI concepts based on how this infection. Whilst they can be effective preventive measures, the gov­
construct is measured to boost sporadic pessimism in EI research. ernment interventions impose limitations and restrictions on people’s
Prentice [9] has provided a thorough review of this approach and any­ daily lives (e.g., shopping, exercising, socialising, entertaining), which
thing relating to EI, including, inter alia, conceptualisation, modelling, may elicit individuals’ different types of coping. Some may perceive
measurement, and construct validities. In sum, trait EI is related to these measures as indications of uncertainty towards length and scale of
personality, and measured by self-reporting, whereas ability EI is sub­ such restrictions, as interruptions to their daily routines, which may
sumed into the intelligence domain and assessed by objective mea­ result in emotion-oriented coping. Others may view these measures as
surement. For the purpose of this study, trait EI is opted for discussion. an opportunity for them to spend more time with family and on them­
Consistent with the foregoing discussion, this research examines how selves, hence opt for more task-oriented coping. The option of a coping
individuals’ personality and EI are related to coping strategies for strategy is largely dependent on personal traits and individuals’
COVID19-related preventive measures. The study views these measures emotional abilities (aka, EI) [7,19].
as a stressor and understands how individuals cope with this stressor and
how their personality and emotional ability affect their coping strate­ 2.2. Personality and coping strategies
gies. This examination provides insights into stress management and the
effectiveness of government intervention. The following section reviews Personality traits have been widely recognized as determinants of
the relevant literature and presents hypotheses for the proposed re­ individual differences in cognitive, emotional, and behavioural perfor­
lationships. The methodology of testing these hypotheses will be out­ mance (e.g., Ref. [4–6]. The Five-factor model of personality, often
lined, followed by presenting data analysis and results. The paper is dubbed as the Big Five [20], namely, extraversion, agreeableness,
concluded with discussion and implications of the research findings. conscientiousness, openness to experience, and neuroticism, can be used
to express the most prominent features of personality, and has been
2. Literature review validated and used across different cultures and different settings for
decades [21]. Research shows that some personality characteristics are
2.1. Coping strategies associated with the proneness to undergo undesirable and pessimistic
emotions as a reaction to distresses [22]. For example, Zanini and Forns
Lazarus and Folkman [11] defined coping strategies as continuously [23] showed a strong relationship between avoidance coping and anx­
varying cognitive and behavioural attempts to handle certain stresses iety trait while examining the impact coping on personality and
that go past the current resources of an individual. This definition im­ behavioural problems of 558 adolescents from Spain. Personality traits
plies coping strategies comprise efforts required to handle new situa­ can also be a noteworthy predictor of coping style and post-traumatic
tions and occurrences that are likely to be demanding and threatening. stress disorder signs [24–26]. Although the big-five model has become
Lazarus [12] reasoned that stress contains several processes, including widely accepted worldwide, a comparatively limited number of re­
the process of perceiving a threat, thinking of a potential response to the searchers have explored the function of these five personality traits in
threat, and coping by executing that response. This conceptual analysis coping, especially in coping with pandemics.
of coping and stress formed a starting point for this research. Consistent Openness to Experience (O). People high on O are viewed as
with this conceptualisation, this study views government interventions curious, original, intellectual, imaginative, artistic, creative, innovative,
manifested in preventive measures (e.g., lockdown, social distancing) as and flexible (curious, intellectual, imaginative, creative, innovative, and
the stressors that entail deliberate effort to manage (coping) in order to flexible). McCrae and Costa [27] established that individuals high on O
minimise the health-associated risk and cope with living a restricted life. are more likely to employ persistence, positive thinking, retraction and
According to Morales-Rodriguez and Perez-Marmol (2019, p. 2), escaping, and emotional articulation as a way of coping with stressors.
coping strategies are “efforts to regulate emotions, behaviours, cogni­ Hooker et al. [28]; on the other hand, found no relationship between O
tions, psychophysiology, and environmental aspects in response to the and coping. Hooker et al.‘s study explored the impact of O on coping for
stress of everyday events”. As such, the extent to which a specific individuals giving care to their spouses that have dementia. O’Brien and
strategy is effective or not depends on whether an individual identifies Delongis [29] argue that this could have been due to the fact that var­
the situation as threatening [13]. People who are more positive tend to iances between people high on O and those low on O are not influenced
utilize more effective strategies; yet, in unmanageable circumstances, by such a confined group of stressors. Folkman et al. [30] argue that
they are inclined to employ ineffective strategies such as submission to since people high on O are highly inventive and resourceful, they might

2
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

be projected to be better copers. This is in line with Williams et al.‘s 2.3. EI and coping strategies
(2009) study findings, in which they investigated the relationship be­
tween O and stress exposure and recovery. They observed that in­ EI denotes the ‘ability to monitor one’s own and other’s feelings and
dividuals that were high on O had lower blood pressure reactivity. They emotions, to discriminate among them, and to use this information to
further established that life stress was linked to reduced sleep levels for guide one’s thinking and actions’ [44]; 189). Salovey and Mayer [44]
individuals low on O, but not for those high on O. identify four main dimensions of EI: the ability to appraise and express
Conscientiousness (C). Individuals high on C have been are of one’s own emotion (self-emotional appraisal); the ability to under­
considered to have the propensity to be careful, responsible and stand and recognise others’ emotions (others’ emotional appraisal); the
dependable, organized, efficient, hard-working, and achievement- ability to regulate one’s own emotion which enables quick recovery
oriented [21,31]. Hooker et al. [28] observed that individuals who from psychological distress (regulation of emotion); and, the ability to
were high on this trait utilize more problem-focused coping strategies as make use of emotions to improve performance (use of emotion) [45].
opposed to emotion-focused strategies. Best and Both [32] confirm that The four domains are related and function hierarchically [9].
support-seeking coping was expected from individuals high on C. Car­ Research at large has established that persons with high EI are more
valho, Pianowski, and Gonçalvez [33] also found conscientiousness to competent of achieving progress on a variety of results such as higher
be linked with engagement with handwashing and social distancing. engagement, higher organizational commitment, job and service per­
Extraversion (E). Extraverts need stimulation. Researchers state formance, venture and organizational performance, and academic per­
that extroverts are very energetic, ambitious, talkative, optimistic, formance, lower levels of burnout and cynicism, lower turnover
confident, outgoing, and reward-seeking (e.g., Ref. [22,34]. Carvalho, intentions, and improved psychological health (e.g., Ref. [46,47];
Pianowski, and Gonçalvez [33] research findings indicate that extro­ Prentice and King, 2010; [48–51]. EI is specifically relevant to the
version affects a person’s engagement with advised Covid-19 quarantine context of health-related crises, which often lead to very emotionally
procedures. Studies have shown that individuals higher on E are more aggravating issues relevant to affected families [52,53].
prone to using problem-focused coping than individuals who are lower Previous research has suggested links between EI and coping.
on E [27,28]. They are also inclined to engage in less avoidance; rather, Matthew and Zeidner [54] advocate that effective handling of stressful
they are more likely to employ support seeking and positive thinking. situations is fundamental to EI. Taylor [55] argued that individuals who
Agreeableness (A). People high on this trait are helpful, good- have higher levels of EI are better capable of controlling their emotions
natured, courteous, cooperative, sympathetic, trusting, and forgiving. and dealing with challenges they face more successfully. Bar-On [56]
A study by Hooker et al. [28] investigated the role of A on coping and found positive correlations between EI and task-oriented coping. Austin
found those high on this trait usually cope through pursuing support et al. [57] also showed that both emotion and task-focused coping are
from others, instead of adopting other forms of emotion-focused coping associated with EI. Researchers revealed that people with higher EI
strategies such as self-blame and wishful thinking. Best and Both [32] utilize approach-oriented coping strategies, and individuals low on EI
examined the relationship between personality and coping styles and employ avoidance oriented coping strategies [58].
collected data from 271 females and 96 males and found that individuals Given that EI has four ability domains. Each may have a different
low on A used support-seeking coping. Yildiz et al. [35] also examined effect on how individuals cope with stress. Research has shown that self
the relationship between personality traits and coping strategies of pa­ and other-emotion appraisal is significantly related to proactive coping
tients with Hashimoto’s Thyroids and found higher agreeableness to be [59,60]. Other researchers argue that coping is more related to the
predictive of emotion-focused coping styles. regulation of emotions (Compas et al., 2014; [61]. Ong and Thompson
Neuroticism (N). Individuals who are high on N are typically [62] observed that an individual’s ability to regulate his or her emotion
neurotic, nervous, insecure, fearful, and anxious [21,36]. Eichel et al. by reevaluating the situation is a useful method of coping with cir­
[37] investigated the impact of personality traits on psychological af­ cumstances that are considered stressors. Use of emotion is also believed
termaths of COVID-19 using a sample of 23,137 people from six Euro­ to affect coping strategies used. In a study directed by Lazarus and
pean countries. They found that N was strongly linked to stress and Folkman [11]; Pelletier [63] observed that family members of organ
isolation, affirming the need for necessary interferences for individuals donors felt several different emotions and utilized different coping
high in N by offering them appropriate coping methods. Kroencke et al. strategies. Ke and Barlas [64] investigated the relationship between trait
[38]; using a German sample of 1609, reported that those high in N emotional intelligence, early maladaptive schemas, and coping styles
underwent greater negative influence and distress in their daily lives and they observed that lower levels of trait emotional intelligence were
throughout the Covid-19 pandemic. Fauerbach, Lawrence, and Scmidt linked with a higher possibility for maladaptive coping in response to
[39] found that disaster survivors with neurotic personalities had more early maladaptive schemas. Consistent with the above discussion, the
negative emotions following the disaster. A study of a Dutch sample by following hypothesis is offered:
Murakami and Murakami [40] established a substantial negative rela­
tionship between N and job-related pressures and stresses. Several re­ H2 EI is significantly related to coping strategies, and each EI
searchers maintained that individuals that are high on N have a lower dimension (self-emotion appraisal, other-emotion appraisal,
probability of taking part in problem-focused coping, and are more regulation of emotion, use of emotion) has a different effect on
likely to engage in emotion-focused comping like avoidance and coping strategies.
blaming themselves [28,41]. In agreement, Boyes and French [42] also
found participants of their study that were high on N to be more 2.4. Personality, EI and coping strategies
involved in emotion-focused coping. Moreover, Pearson et al.‘s [43]
results also imply individuals high on N that experience psychological EI has been extensively discussed in the literature. This concept has
distress experienced as a consequence of a racist incident is correlated been conceptualised as an intelligence (Salovey and Mayer, 1997), a
with engaging in avoidant and self-critical coping strategies. Consistent personality trait, or a mix of the two (Bar-On, 1998; Goleman, 1995;
with the foregoing discussion, the following hypotheses are offered: 1998). The three models are predominant in the relevant literature and
also cause confusion as what EI exactly is. This confusion leads to the
H1 The five-factor personality is significantly related to coping birth of ability EI and trait EI suggested by Petrides and Furnham [65].
strategies, and each personality factor (O, C, E, A, N) has a Furnham et al. (2016) proposed that EI through self-report measure
different effect on coping strategies. should be operationalised as a personality trait, embedded within the
personality framework, referred to as trait EI, while the measurement
through performance tests as a cognitive ability, referred to as ability EI.

3
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

They further stated that trait and ability EI should be distinguished on “I always set goals for myself and then try my best to achieve them” are
the assessment methods, not on the elements of the conceptual model. included in the UOE dimension. The dimension ROE items include “I am
For instance, the emotional intelligence scale (EIS) developed by Schutte able to control my temper so that I can handle difficulties rationally.”
and her colleagues in 1998 is a self-report measure, hence classified as Items were measured on a five-point Likert scale, with 1 indicating
trait EI, albeit based on the ability model developed by Salovey and strongly disagree, and 5 strongly agree. The reliabilities were 0.93 for
Mayer [44]. Moreover, Furnham et al. (2016) found Trait EI to be SEA, 0.93 for OEA, 0.94 for UOE, and 0.95 for ROE.
generally significantly related to personality factors and proposed that Coping strategies were measured by adapting Carver, Scherier, and
individual disparities in trait EI are a constant forecaster of an in­ Weintraub’s [13] multidimensional scale to understand how re­
dividual’s behaviour through the life span. The authors also stressed on spondents cope with the government’s preventive measures for COVID
the importance of trait EI training as it has several benefits including 19. Specific coping strategies include active coping (e.g., I take addi­
improved mental well-being, enhanced life satisfaction, more happiness, tional action to try to get rid of the problem), planning (e.g., I try to come
an improved quality of life, a reduction in psychological problems, up with a strategy about what to do), seeking social support for instru­
better physical health and is even likely to enrich the quality of both mental reasons (e.g., I try to get advice from someone about what to do),
marital and social relationships. seeking social support for emotional reasons (e.g., I talk to someone
Subsumed under the personality domain, trait EI should not only about how I feel), positive reinterpretation & growth (e.g., I look for
explain variance in outcome variables that have been accounted for by something good in what is happening), behavioural disengagement (e.
personality, but also demonstrate incremental validity in these out­ g., I give up the attempt to get what I want), mental disengagement (e.g.,
comes (see Brackett and Mayer, 2003; [7]. Studies have shown that trait I turn to work or other substitute activities to take my mind off things),
EI indeed explained additional variance over and above the personality and alcohol-drug disengagement (e.g., I drink alcohol, in order to think
measure in some criterion variables such as support, leadership (Van about it less). Consistent with the suggestion by Ref. [17] and Cohan,
Dan Zee et al., 2004), and attitudes towards organizational change Jang and Stein [18]; we grouped these coping strategies into three
(Vakola, Tsaousis, and Nikolau, 2003), service performance [8,10], categories: task-oriented coping (i.e., active coping, and planning),
loneliness and social stress (Chapman and Hayslip, 2005), as well as emotion-oriented coping (i.e., seeking social support for instrumental
coping [66,67]; Ke and Barlas, 2020). For example, O’Connor et al. [67] reasons, seeking social support for emotional reasons, and positive
examined the validity of the short form trait EI questionnaire in reinterpretation and growth), and avoidance-oriented coping (i.e.,
task-stimulated stress setting with a sample of 225 volunteers and found behavioural disengagement, mental disengagement, and alcohol drug
that the trait EI projected low negative affect and elevated task perfor­ disengagement). Items were measured on a five-point Likert scale, with
mance using more emotion focused coping. They further stated that trait 1 indicating strongly disagree, and 5 strongly agree. Please note that we
EI improves performance in circumstances and conditions that are have also collected individuals’ coping strategies with the pandemic.
perceived as stressful by regulating and controlling negative emotions. Their measures also had good validity, and the results of hypotheses
These results are consistent with Siegling et al.‘s [68] findings, which testing for coping strategies with the pandemic were quite similar to
showed that trait EI had consistent incremental effects beyond the Big those for coping strategies with the government’s preventive measures .
Five and coping strategies. Consistent with these studies, the study offers Personality was measured by ten items from Gosling et al. (2003) (i.
the following hypothesis: e., two items for each of the five dimensions of personality). Items
including “is outgoing, sociable” and “is reserved” (reversed) for extra­
H3 EI has a significant incremental variance in coping strategies over version, “is generally trusting” and “tends to find fault with others”
and above personality factors. (reversed) for agreeableness, “does a thorough job” and “tends to be
lazy” (reversed) for conscientiousness, “has an active imagination” and
3. Methods “has few artistic interests” (reversed) for openness, and “gets nervous
easily” and “is relaxed, handles stress well” (reversed) for neuroticism.
3.1. Sample Items were measured on a five-point Likert scale, with 1 indicating
strongly disagree, and 5 strongly agree.
Since the COVID 19 was originated in China where government in­
terventions (lockdown and social distancing) were enforced to control 3.3. Data collection procedure
the spread of the virus. This was our pilot study to understand in­
dividuals’ coping strategies and their influencing factors. The data were A pilot study with 30 Chinese residents was undertaken prior to
collected with Chinese residents who were aged 18 and lived in different distributing the formal survey. The pilot study was to assess the timing,
provinces, and who understood the impact of COVID 19 and the gov­ appropriateness, and clarity of the questionnaires. The formal survey
ernment interventions. The survey was posted on the most popular was conducted online through WeChat. WeChat has more than one
Chinese social media platform -WeChat. A snowball sampling was billion users monthly in China for messaging, one-stop shopping, and
deployed to reach beyond the researchers’ social network with an playing games. This platform can reach the target population in various
intention to access a larger sample. regions and locations. In total, 468 complete responses were received
and deemed to be sufficient sample size to conduct meaningful statistical
3.2. Instruments analysis for the research hypotheses. Participants’ demographic infor­
mation is shown in Table 1. Participants had different levels of age and
EI was measured by Law, Wong, and Song’s (2004) self-report EI education. The majority of them were single, and about three-fourths of
scale (WEIS) that is based on the ability EI model (see Brackett and them had annual incomes below 80,000 RMB.
Mayer, 2003). This scale has been widely used and cited in the literature
with great reliability and validity. The original WEIS contains 16 items 3.4. Common method variance
(statements), and four dimensions. These four dimensions are labelled as
self-emotion appraisal (SEA), other-emotion appraisal (OEA), use of Both ex-ante and ex-post remedies were implemented to minimise
emotion (UOE), and regulation of emotion (ROE). Each dimension has response errors and examine common method variance (Lindell and
four items. The dimension SEA includes items, “I have a good sense of Whitney, 2001; Podsakoff et al., 2003). First, validated scales were used
why I have certain feelings most of the time” and “I have a good un­ and thoroughly checked with the relevant researchers and experts in the
derstanding of my own emotions”. The dimension OEA items include, “I field. Second, participants were informed that there were no right or
always know my friends’ emotions from their behaviour”. Items such as wrong answers. Third, to minimise response bias, items measuring

4
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

Table 1 constructs on the average scores of their first-order constructs. The


Participants’ demographic information. model fit was χ2 = 167.86, d.f. = 17; CFI = 0.94; TLI = 0.90; RMSEA =
Category Percentage (N = 468) 0.08. All the path coefficients were significant at the 0.05 level. These
results support modelling three coping strategies as second-order con­
Gender
structs. Loading, Cronbach’s alpha, composite reliability, and average
Female 53.7% variance extracted (AVE) are shown in Table 2. The correlations among
Male 46.1%
Age
the proposed variables are present in Table 3.
18 - 25 40.7%
26 - 35 19.6% 4.2. Hypotheses testing
36 - 45 14.3%
46 - 55 11.3%
Previous research shows that some demographic variables (i.e., age,
56 or above 13.9%
Education gender, and education) are related to individuals’ coping strategies
Elementary school 30.9% [69–73]. In this study, these variables are controlled to understand how
High school 24.5% personality and EI influence the outcome variables. We first assessed
Some college 27.1%
how coping strategies differ across these demographic variables. To
Bachelor degree 17.3%
Marital status
facilitate analysis, we first code participants into different groups. For
Single 52.7% gender, male was coded as 0, and female was coded as 1. For age, we
Married without children 7.2% coded those between 18 and 35 as 0 (the young group), those between
Married with children 39.9% 36 and 55 as 1 (the middle group), and those above 56 as 2 (the old
Annual income (RMB)
group). For education, university education was coded as 1, while the
Less than 80,000 75.5%
80,000–150,000 17.5% rest were coded as 0. One-way ANOVA and t-test were conducted. The
150,000–300,000 4.9% results in Table 4 show that the overall coping strategies and the three
More than 300,000 1.9% coping strategies were scored higher for older group. However, the male
counterparts were more inclined to take avoidance-oriented coping than
the female. Individuals with university education were less likely to
certain constructs were distributed in different sections to avoid iden­
engage in avoidance-oriented coping. The results show that people at
tical responses. Harman’s single factor test was performed first to assess
different demographic groups rated coping strategies differently. It is
common method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2003). The results showed that
plausible to control their potential variance in the proposed outcome
eight factors were present, and the first factor explains 38.37% of the
variables (coping strategies).
variance. Second, confirmation factor analysis (CFA) procedure was
H1 proposes that the five-factor personality is significantly related to
conducted to check the model fit for one-factor and multiple-factor
coping strategies, and each personality factor (O, C, E, A, N) has a
models (Iverson and Maguire, 2000). The one-factor model (χ2 (989)
different effect on coping strategies. The results (Table 5) show that
= 15841.16, p < .001, CFI = 0.43; TLI = 0.40; RMSEA = 0.18) has a very
openness had a significant positive effect on task-oriented and emotion-
poor model fit compared to the 12 factors model (χ2 (923) = 2714.05, p
oriented coping while negatively related to avoidance-oriented coping.
< .001, CFI = 0.93; TLI = 0.92; RMSEA = 0.06). The last procedure was
Conscientiousness is positively related to emotion-oriented coping and
to control for the effect of an unmeasured latent factor and to compare
negatively to avoidance-oriented coping. On the other hand, neuroti­
the item loadings with and without adding an unmeasured latent
cism is positively related to avoidance-oriented coping.
methods factor (Podsakoff et al., 2003). No great differences between
H2 proposes that EI is significantly related to coping strategies, and
two sets of loadings (all less than 0.01) were identified.
each EI dimension (self-emotion appraisal, other-emotion appraisal,
regulation of emotion, use of emotion) has a different effect on coping
4. Data analysis and results
strategies. The results show that overall, EI has a positive effect on task-
oriented coping and emotion-oriented coping (Table 6), surprisingly,
4.1. Confirmatory factor analysis
also on avoidance-oriented coping. When testing the relationship be­
tween each EI dimension and coping strategies, self, other-emotion
EI has been viewed as a first-order [9] as well as a second-order
appraisal, and use of emotion are significantly related to task-oriented
factor (Law et al., 2004). For this study, we opted for both for hypoth­
and emotion-oriented coping strategies. Surprisingly, other-emotion
esis testing with the first-order factor for understanding the relationship
appraisal is positively related to avoidance-oriented coping. Neverthe­
between each dimension and coping strategies, and with second-order
less, regulation of emotion is not related to any of coping strategies,
factor for testing incremental variance testing [10]. Both factor struc­
contradicting to what has been reported in previous studies.
tures in this study have acceptable model fit indices. The first-order
H3 proposes EI explains incremental variance over and above per­
model has good model fit: χ2 = 319.23, d.f. = 98; CFI = 0.97; TLI =
sonality factors. Consistent with the approach to testing the incremental
0.97; RMSEA = 0.07. The path coefficients between the indicators and
validity of EI in Prentice and King [10]; both personality factors and EI
their respective first-order factors were significant at the 0.05 level. We
were entered in the regression equation in a hierarchical order with
also examined the second-order factor structure by conducting a
personality being the first. For comparison, the results for this hypoth­
one-factor CFA on the average scores of the four first-order constructs.
esis testing are also present in Table 5. The testing shows that EI did
The model fit was χ2 = 36.17, d.f. = 2; CFI = 0.97; TLI = 0.91; RMSEA =
explain significant incremental variance manifested in significant R
0.08. All the path coefficients were significant at the 0.05 level. These
square change values. Interestingly, Openness and Conscientiousness
results show that EI has good construct validity.
were no longer significantly related to task and emotion-oriented coping
We then assessed the validity of coping strategies with government
strategies when EI was in the equation. Neuroticism was still negatively
interventions. The second-order CFA, including 3 s-order constructs, has
related to avoidance-oriented coping. H3 was supported.
acceptable model fit: χ2 = 1865.50, d.f. = 397; CFI = 0.91; TLI = 0.90;
RMSEA = 0.08. The path coefficients between the indicators and their
5. Discussion
respective first-order factors were significant at the 0.05 level. In addi­
tion, all the path coefficients between the second-order construct and its
This study views government interventions manifested in mandatory
three dimensions were significant at the 0.05 level. We also examined
preventive measures for controlling the spread of COVID 19 as a stressor
the second-order factor structure by conducting a CFA, including three
and examines whether individuals’ personality and EI influence their

5
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

Table 2 Table 2 (continued )


Confirmatory factor analyses results. Items Loading Alpha CR AVE
Items Loading Alpha CR AVE
Task-oriented coping
Task-oriented coping
I am sensitive to the feelings and emotions of .87
Active coping .89 .90 .68 others.
AC1: I take additional action to try to get rid of .80 I have good understanding of the emotions of .87
the problem. people around me.
AC2: I concentrate my efforts on doing .84 Use of emotion .94 .94 .80
something about it. I always set goals for myself and then try my .88
AC3: I do what has be done, one step at a time. .79 best to achieve them.
AC4: I take direct action to get around the .87 I always tell myself I am a competent person. .86
problem. I am a self-motivated person. .93
Planning .95 .95 .82 I would always encourage myself to try my best. .91
P1: I try to come up with a strategy about what .89 Regulation of emotion .95 .95 .84
to do. I am able to control my temper and handle .93
P2: I make a plan of action. .90 difficulties rationally.
P3: I think hard about what steps to take. .94 I am quite capable of controlling my own .95
P4: I think about how I might best handle the .89 emotions.
problem. I can always calm down quickly when I am very .87
Emotion-oriented coping angry.
Seeking social support for instrumental .93 .93 .82 I have good control of my own emotions. .92
reasons
SSI1: I try to get advice from someone about .90
what to do. coping strategies for these measures, and whether EI provides incre­
SSI2: I talk to someone to find out more about .94 mental variance in these strategies over and above personality factors.
the situation.
Demographic factors, including gender, age and education, are included
SSI3: I talk to someone who could do something .88
concrete about the problem. as control variables in the proposed relationships. This study classifies
Seeking social support for emotional reasons .93 .94 .84 coping strategies into task, emotion, and avoidance-oriented coping.
SSE1: I talk to someone about how I feel. .85 Age, gender, and education exert various effects on the coping strate­
SSE2: I try to get emotional support from friends .95 gies. The results show that EI is significantly related to different coping
or relatives. I discuss my feelings with
someone.
strategies, whereas only certain EI dimensions and personality factors
SSE3: I get sympathy and understanding from .94 made unique variances in coping strategies. EI did provide incremental
someone. variance in the outcome variables. Detailed discussions of these findings
Positive reinterpretation & growth .96 .96 .87 are as follows.
PR1: I look for something good in what is .88
happening.
PR2: I try to see it in a different light, to make it .92
seem more positive.
5.1. Personality, EI and coping strategies
PR3: I learn something from the experience. .97
PR4: I try to grow as a person as a result of the .95 To understand the influence of personality and EI on coping strate­
experience. gies, age, gender, and education were controlled in the analysis. How­
Avoidance-oriented coping
ever, mean comparisons did show that older people tend to opt for
Behavioural disengagement .94 .94 .80
BD1: I give up the attempt to get what I want. .85 proactive coping such as task and emotion-oriented strategies. Both
BD2: I just give up trying to reach my goal. .91 males and females can be task oriented. However, females are more
BD3: I admit to myself that I can’t deal with it, .90 inclined to seek emotional support, and males are more avoidant prone
and quit trying.
or engaging in drinking and hedonistic behaviours. These findings are
BD4: I reduce the amount of effort I’m putting .92
into solving the problem
not consistent with those in prior research reporting the senior and fe­
Mental disengagement .87 .88 .64 male are more likely to take precautious and protective behaviours.
MD1: I turn to work or other substitute .73 Older people may see them have more responsibilities to protect their
activities to take my mind off things. families. Similarly, females are perceived to be more nurturing and
MD2: I go to movies or watch TV, to think about .86
caring, and naturally would take a more positive approach to deal with
it less.
MD3: I daydream about things other than this .84 preventive measures. Whilst education does not make different in the
MD4: I sleep more than usual. .76 task and emotion-oriented coping, less educated people tend to take
Alcohol-drug disengagement .98 .98 .94 avoidance approach, and engage in drinking and other hedonistic be­
AD1: I drink alcohol, in order to think about it .94 haviours to cope with the lockdown and other preventive measures, as
less.
AD2: I drink more alcohol, in order to think .95
shown in this study.
about it less. Among the five personality factors, only openness was significantly
AD3: I smoke in order to think about it less. .99 related to three categories of coping strategies. Openness is reflective of
AD4: I smoke more in order to think about it .99 being creative, adventurous, and innovative. Its significant variance in
less.
proactive coping is plausible. The pandemic status quo and its preven­
EI
Self-emotion appraisal .93 .94 .78 tive measures may be unique. People with open mind may view these as
I have a good sense of why I have certain .81 new challenges and proactively seek remedies (e.g., new hobbies,
feelings most of the time. beneficial activities) to enrich their life experience. These people tend to
I have good understanding of my own emotions. .91 be less likely to take avoidance approach by engaging in alcohol and
I really understand what I feel. .94
I always know whether or not I am happy. .87
hedonistic behaviours as this study shows that openness is negatively
Others’ emotion appraisal .93 .93 .78 related to avoidance-oriented coping. This finding is consistent with that
I always know my friends’ emotions from their .87 in Williams et al. [74]. Conscientiousness in this study is positively
behaviours. related to emotion-oriented coping but negative to avoidance coping.
I am a good observer of others’ emotions. .91
Since conscientiousness is reflective of traits associated with being
hardworking and achievement oriented, people with a high level of

6
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

Table 3
Correlations and squared root of AVEs.
AC P SSI SSE PR BD MD AD SEA OEA UOE ROE O C E A N

AC .82
P .74** .91
SSI .62** .74** .91
SSE .60** .70** .79** .92
PR .59** .78** .72** .69** .93
BD .35** .31** .31** .31** .25** .89
MD .40** .46** .49** .47** .46** .60** .80
AD .19** .11* .10* .15** -.04 .52** .38** .97
SEA .41** .54** .50** .56** .63** .26** .37** .07 .88
OEA .41** .53** .52** .57** .59** .25** .37** .13** .74** .88
UOE .43** .53** .49** .53** .62** .24** .28** .05 .71** .64** .89
ROE .36** .44** .39** .44** .50** .19** .25** .07 .68** .61** .73** .92
O .17** .20** .13** .13** .24** -.10* -.08 -.16** .25** .20** .23** .20** –
C .15** .18** .12* .14** .23** -.15** -.15** -.24** .29** .19** .35** .33** .32** –
E .05 .04 -.01 -.02 -.01 -.02 -.03 .10* .03 .10* .08 .06 .26** .12** –
A -.12* -.14** -.09* -.06 -.11* -.06 -.03 -.07 -.11* -.20** -.15** -.12** -.33** -.14** -.36** –
N .08 .03 .06 .07 .02 .08 .13** .07 .02 .08 -.00 -.09 .02 .07 -.17** -.07 –

Note: AC = Active coping, P = Planning, SSI = Seeking social support for instrumental reasons, SSE = Seeking social support for emotional reasons, PR = Positive
reinterpretation & growth, BD = Behavioural disengagement, MD = Mental disengagement, AD = Alcohol-drug disengagement, SEA = Self-emotion appraisal, OEA =
Others’ emotion appraisal, UOE = Use of emotion, ROE = Regulation of emotion, O = Openness, C = Conscientiousness, E = Extroversion, A = Agreeableness, N =
Neuroticism.
*p < .05, **p < .01.

conscientiousness appreciate the uncertainty and stress relating to this


Table 4
pandemic and the compulsory preventive measures. Rather than being
The effect of demographic variables on coping strategies.
passively reacting or indulging in binge drinking, they stay active and
Task-oriented Emotion-oriented Avoidance-oriented engage in learning and growth during the lockdown and seek support
coping coping coping

Age
Table 6
Young: 3.76 3.85 3.08
18–35
EI and coping strategies with government interventions.
Middle: 3.93 3.90 3.13 Task-oriented Emotion-oriented Avoidance-oriented
36–55 coping coping coping
Old: 56 4.05 3.99 3.32
above EI .55*** .66*** .27***
ANOVA F = 5.26, p < .01, F = 1.31, p < .05, F = 2.23, p > .05, Control variable
ƞ2 = .02 ƞ2 = .01 ƞ2 = .01 Gender -.00 .09* -.09
Gender Age .01 .00 .05
Male 3.85 3.82 3.21 Education -.02 .02 -.08
Female 3.84 3.94 3.05 R2 .32 .45 .09
T-test t = .18, p > .05, t = − 2.05, p < .05, t = 2.24, p < .05, EI dimensions
Cohen’s d = .01 Cohen’s d = .18 Cohen’s d = .20 SEA .17* .24*** .13
Education OEA .23*** .29*** .21***
Below 3.85 3.85 3.20 UOE .25*** .28*** .01
Bachelor ROE -.02 -.06 -.04
Bachelor 3.83 3.92 3.03 Control variable
above Gender -.01 .08* -.09*
T-test t = .30, p > .05, t = -1.15, p > .05, t = 2.10, p < .05, Age .02 .01 .05
Cohen’s d = .03 Cohen’s d = .11 Cohen’s d = .20 Education -.02 .01 -.08
2
R .33 .47 .11

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 5
Personality and coping strategies with government measures.
Task-oriented coping Emotion-oriented coping Avoidance-oriented coping

Personality

Openness .05 .14** .07 .13* .04 -.12* -.17***


Conscientiousness -.04 .09 -.06 .14** -.06 -.26*** -.38***
Extroversion -.01 -.02 -.01 -.07 -.05 .06 .07
Agreeableness -.10 -.08 -.02 -.06 .01 -.10* -.06
Neuroticism .09 .05 .06 .04 .06 .13** .14**
EI .55*** .67*** .41***
Control variable
Gender -.01 .00 -.01 .09* .08* -.09* -.10*
Age .13** .06 .02 .06 .00 .07 .04
Education -.00 .01 -.01 .05 .02 -.05 -.07
R2 .04 .07 .33 .07 .45 .13 .27
Δ R2 .25*** .38*** .15**

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

7
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

from family and relatives and. This finding is contrasting to those in confirmed the concept of trait EI that is more related to personality traits
Hooker et al. [28]; who reported that conscientious individuals are less though based on an ability model (e.g. Ref. [65,66,75]). The significant
prone to engage in emotion-oriented coping. People with a high level of incremental variance explained by trait EI conforms to what has been
neuroticism tend to be nervous and anxious. The level of being neurotic reported in Ref. [8,10] albeit the criterion variable is different.
can be exasperated by external stress, such as the pandemic. Since
vaccination has not been successfully developed, there is no time limit 6. Implications
for implementation of the preventive measures, which are perceived as
restrictions to people’s daily life. Neurotic individuals can be pessimistic The study is intended to understand how people cope with preven­
about the development of the situation. The best approach to dealing tive measures undertaken to combat the pandemic COVID 19 and
with such uncertainty for them is to avoid and indulge in hedonistic examine whether their EI and personality may influence their coping
behaviours reflected in avoidance coping. strategies. The preventive measures in this study indicate government
EI is significantly related to all three coping strategies. EI in this interventions, including lockdown and social distancing, for controlling
study was measured by four dimensions, namely, self-emotion appraisal the spread of the virus. This study integrates government policing, crisis
(SEA), other-emotion appraisal (OEA), utilisation of emotions (UOE), management, and psychology and hence contributes to these disciplines.
and regulation of emotions (ROE) based on the ability model. When Whilst these government preventive measures are intended for man­
analysing how each dimension made unique variance in coping strate­ aging the virus, these restrictions affect people’s daily lives and their
gies, the results show that the first three dimensions were significantly mental wellbeing. How they cope and what they do to cope with these
related to task and emotion-oriented coping. Task-oriented coping refers restrictions have implications for public policy research and crisis
to how individuals’ proactively plan and seek to engage in positive management literature.
conducts. Emotion-oriented coping is reflective of seeking support from The study shows that personal factors are important in their coping
family and friends. The preventive measures undertaken in this strategies and behaviours. In particular, EI has a most significant impact
pandemic may be unprecedented for most people, especially if there is on individuals’ positive and proactive coping strategies (i.e., task and
no definite time of easing lockdown measure. Such uncertainty can emotion-oriented coping). Only certain personality factors are signifi­
cause fear and anxiety. Those with a high level of SEA and OEA which cantly related to coping strategies. These findings provide a fresh
indicate perceiving emotions of oneself and others tend to appreciate perspective on how personality and individual traits can be incorporated
this situation and identify suitable tasks to cope with the lockdown, as into shape public policy and crisis management research. Managing a
shown in this study. Lockdown and social distancing also limit social severe crisis like a pandemic may not be attributed to government policy
activities. Family and relatives can be a good source of dealing with such and regulation per se. Individuals play a significant role in the effec­
restrictions as reflected in emotion-oriented coping. UOE is also tiveness of implementing these policies. On the other hand, prior
important as this dimension indicates that individuals with a high level research on EI and personality primarily focus on their impact on indi­
of UOE are more goal-oriented and confident in what they opt to do. vidual performance, success, and wellbeing. Although some studies have
They may view these preventive measures as an opportunity to engage examined their relationship with coping (e.g., Montes-Bergres and
in hobbies that they were unable to in normal situations and spending Augusto, 2007; [7,76], the contexts were mostly limited to the work
more time with family. context. This study extends to crisis management literature, and reveals
In the case of avoidance coping, only OEA explains a significant how EI and personality can shape individuals’ coping strategies towards
variance. Avoidance coping is reflective of a passive approach to a government policies.
stressful situation, and avoiding the stress by engaging in drinking. In­ The findings that personality factors have little influence on coping
dividuals with a high level of OEA manifest in understanding the emo­ strategies when entering in the same regression equation with EI
tions of others who may express distress and other negative emotions confirm the trait EI concept proposed by Ref. [65,66]. The EI measure
towards these preventive measures. Emotions are contagious. The opted for this study was based on an ability model, nevertheless tested
appraiser may be affected by such emotions and mimic others’ attitudes by self-reporting. Hence, the self-assessed ability measure is more
and behaviours such as passive coping and drinking. Interestingly ROE is related to personality traits and are highly correlated to personality
not significantly related to any coping strategies. This dimension in­ factors. Given the coping strategies are emotionally connotated, EI is
dicates the ability of managing one’s own emotions. Coping strategies deemed to account for more of these strategies and associated behav­
refer to actions taken to cope with pandemic combating measures. This iours. The values of significant R square change indicate that EI explains
finding may indicate that managing emotions is simply reflective of incremental variance than personality factors. This result confirms that
maintaining internal peace without engaging in any activities. EI is different from personality and provides incremental validity as an
independent psychological construct. This finding is consistent with that
5.2. Incremental variance in Petrides, Perez-Gonzales, and Furnham [66]; and Prentice and King
[8,10].
When EI and personality variables were entered in the same The study also has several practical implications for policymakers
regression, surprisingly, openness and conscientiousness are not signif­ and relevant practitioners. Since personality factors and demographic
icantly related to task and emotion-oriented coping strategies. This variables are significantly related to coping with the pandemic in­
finding can be interpreted as that the two personality factors are highly terventions, the government and authorities should segment citizens
correlated with EI. This interpretation was supported as the correlation based on these characteristics and implement these interventions pro­
values between these variables in Table 3 were significant. The measure gressively to minimise side effects and potential damages to the public.
that was opted for this study to assess EI is reflective of individuals’ As numerous reports show that these measures affect people’s mental
personality traits rather than emotional abilities. EI is classified as in­ wellbeing, and mental health can affect how they behave, for instance,
telligence that should be distinguished from personality traits and engaging in abnormal behaviours or criminal conduct, it is imperative
measured by objective indicators. However, the EI studies are inundated for the policymakers and the relevant authorities to identify appropriate
with self-reporting EI in the relevant literature. This conundrum has means to enhance the public’s mental wellbeing. Since EI has a signifi­
been resolved by induction of trait and ability EI proposed by Petrides cant impact on positive coping strategies and is proved to be trainable,
and his colleagues (e.g., 2001; 2016). The key difference between the training can be provided to residents who have difficulty coping with
two is that the former is more related to personality traits, which can be these restrictions and interventions to prevent further damage to
self-reported, whereas the latter is associated with the intelligence themselves or the local communities. On the other hand, age, gender,
domain, and can only be assessed objectively. The finding in this study and education play important roles in their coping. The relevant

8
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

authorities should reinforce the influence of role models and find [17] M.M. Smith, D.H. Saklofske, K.V. Keefer, P.F. Tremblay, Coping strategies and
psychological outcomes: the Moderating effects of personal resiliency, J. Psychol.
appropriate means to reward those who play significant roles in
150 (3) (2016) 318–332.
enhancing the safety of local communities. [18] S. Cohan, K. Jang, M. Stein, Confirmatory factor analysis of a short form of the
coping inventory for stressful situations, J. Clin. Psychol. 62 (3) (2006) 273–283.
7. Limitations and future research [19] C.S. Carver, J. Connor-Smith, Personality and coping, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61
(2010) 679–704.
[20] L.R. Goldberg, An alternative “description of personality”: the Big-Five factor
A few limitations must be acknowledged in this research. First, structure, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59 (1990) 1216–1229.
although popular, self-reporting EI is never ideal or well-recognized in [21] R.R. McCrae, O.P. John, An introduction to the five-factor model and its
applications, J. Pers. 2 (1992) 175–215.
EI research. We understood this limitation yet opted for such a measure [22] S. Taylor, The Psychology of Pandemics: Preparing for the Next Global Outbreak of
as no funding was generated for this research. Ability EI measure can be Infectious Disease, Cambridge Scholar Publishing, UK, 2019.
costly with a large sample cohort. This assessment is also lengthy and [23] D.S. Zanini, M. Forns, Does coping mediate personality and behavioural problems
relationship? Psychology 5 (2014) 1111–1119, https://doi.org/10.4236/
might generate a low response rate. Such consideration determined the psych.2014.59123.
use of a self-reporting measure. The scale used in this study may not [24] E. Mattson, L. James, B. Engdahl, Personality factors and their impact on PTSD and
represent the trait EI domain. The findings should be interpreted with post-traumatic growth is mediated by coping style among OIF/OEF veterans, in:
Military Medicine, 00, 0/0. Published by Oxford University Press on Behalf of the
cautions. Second, the personality measure opted for this study is a short Association of Military Surgeons of the United States 2018, 2018.
version of the original scale. This option may limit the finding of indi­ [25] Y.-h. Xiang, X. Chi, Y.-q. Jiang, R.-f. Wang, L. Mo, Psychosomatic status,
vidual differences on coping strategies. Third, the data was only personality traits, and coping styles of bereaved and non bereaved survivors of the
2008 wenchuan earthquake, China, Front. Psychiatr. 7 (2016) 17, https://doi.org/
collected in China. The coping mechanism for Chinese may be different
10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00017.
from other races and ethnicities. The findings cannot be generalised [26] Y. Zhang, F.C. Kong, L. Wang, H. Chen, X. Gao, Mental health and coping styles of
without caution. Comparative studies from different countries and cul­ children and adolescent survivors one year after the 2008 Chinese earthquake,
tures would provide more insights. Finally, snowball sampling was Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 32 (2010) 1403–1409, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
childyouth.2010.06.009.
deployed. This method has certain merits but also some limitations on [27] R.R. McCrae, P.T. Costa Jr., Personality, coping, and coping effectiveness in an
generating a sample cohort that is reflected in the researchers’ social adult sample, J. Pers. 54 (1986) 385–405.
network. Random sampling would provide a better understanding of the [28] K. Hooker, L.D. Frazier, D.J. Monahan, Personality and coping among caregivers of
spouses with dementia, Gerontol. 34 (1994) 386–392.
proposed relationships in this study. [29] T.B. O’Brien, A. DeLongis, The interactional context of problem-emotion-, and
relationship-focused coping: the role of the big five personality factors, J. Pers. 64
Declaration of competing interest (4) (1996) 775–813.
[30] S. Folkman, R.S. Lazarus, C. Dunkel-Schetter, A. DeLongis, R.J. Gruen, Dynamics of
a stressful encounter: cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes, J. Pers.
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial Soc. Psychol. 50 (1986) 992–1003.
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence [31] R.R. McCrae, P.T. Costa Jr., Validation of the five-factor model of personality
across instruments and observers, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54 (1987) 81–90.
the work reported in this paper. [32] L.A. Best, L.E. Both, Coping Style as Predicted by Personality and Attachment.
European Congress on Psychology, Millan, Italy, 2015.
References [33] L.F. Carvalho, G. Pianowski, A.P. Gonçalvez, Personality differences and COVID-
19: are extroversion and conscientiousness personality traits associated with
engagement with containment measures? Trends Psychiat. Psychother (2020)
[1] J.E. Maddux, R.W. Rogers, Protection motivation theory and self-efflcacy: a revised
https://doi.org/10.1590/2237-6089-20200029, 2020;00(0):000-000.
theory of fear appeals and attitude change, J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 19 (1983)
[34] P.T. Costa Jr., R.R. McCrae, NEO-PI-R: Professional Manual, Psychological
469–479.
Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL, 1992.
[2] S.A. Abdulkareem, E.-W. Augustijn, T. Filatova, K. Musial, Y.T. Mustafa, Risk
[35] M. Yıldız, Ş. Şahin, S. Batmaz, E. Songur, F. Kutlutürk, The relationship between
perception and behavioral change during epidemics: comparing models of
depression, anxiety, personality traits and coping strategies of patients with
individual and collective learning, PloS One 15 (1) (2020), e0226483, https://doi.
euthyroid Hashimoto’s Thyroiditis, Eur. J. Psychiatr. 31 (3) (2017) 113–118,
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226483.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpsy.2017.06.001.
[3] R. Hewett, A. Liefooghe, G. Visockaite, S. Roongrerngsuke, Bullying at work:
[36] R.R. McCrae, P.T. Costa Jr., The five-factor theory of personality, in: O.P. John, R.
cognitive appraisal of negative acts, coping, wellbeing, and performance, J. Occup.
W. Robins, L.A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, third
Health Psychol. 23 (1) (2018) 71.
ed., Guilford Press, New York, NY, 2008, pp. 159–180.
[4] P.T. Costa Jr., A. Terracciano, R.R. McCrae, Gender differences in personality traits
[37] K. Eichel, F. Turk, S. Chrona, A. Lieberoth, D. Pankowski, Stress and loneliness
across cultures: robust and surprising findings, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81 (2) (2001)
during the COVID-19 pandemic: the role of personality traits, Soc. Psychol.
322–331, https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322.
Personal. Sci. (2020).
[5] R.R. McCrae, P.T. Costa Jr., Personality trait structure as a human universal, Am.
[38] L. Kroencke, K. Geukes, T. Utesch, N. Kuner, M. Back, Neuroticism and emotional
Psychol. 52 (5) (1997) 509–516, https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.5.509.
risk during the Covid-19 pandemic, Preprint (2020), https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.
[6] R.R. McCrae, A. Terracciano, 78 Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures
io/8c6nh.
Project, Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective:
[39] J.A. Fauerbach, J.W. Lawrence, C.W. Schmidt, Personality predictors of injury
data from 50 cultures, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88 (3) (2005) 547–561, https://doi.
related posttraumatic stress disorder, J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 188 (2000) 510–517,
org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-200008000-00006.
[7] M. Zeidner, G. Matthews, R.D. Roberts, EI, coping with stress, and adaptation, EI
[40] Y. Murakami, C. Murakami, The standardization of a Big Five personality inventory
Everyday Life (2006) 100–125.
for separate generations, Jpn. J. Pers. 8 (1999) 32–42.
[8] C. Prentice, B.E. King, Impacts of personality, emotional intelligence and
[41] D.J. Terry, Determinants of coping: the role of stable and situational factors,
adaptiveness on service performance of casino hosts: a hierarchical approach,
J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66 (1994) 895–910.
J. Bus. Res. 66 (9) (2013) 1637–1643.
[42] M.E. Boyes, D.J. French, Having a Cyberball: using a ball-throwing game as an
[9] C. Prentice, Managing service encounters with emotional intelligence, J. Retailing
experimental social stressor to examine the relationship between neuroticism and
Consum. Serv. 51 (2019) 344–351.
coping, Pers. Indiv. Differ. 47 (5) (2009) 396–401, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
[10] C. Prentice, B. King, The influence of emotional intelligence on the service
paid.2009.04.005.
performance of casino frontline employees, Tourism Hospit. Res. 11 (1) (2011)
[43] M.R. Pearson, V.J. Derlega, J.M. Henson, K.Y. Holmes, R.A. Ferrer, S.B. Harriso,
49–66.
Role of neuroticism and coping strategies in psychological reactions to a racist
[11] R. Lazarus, S. Folkman, Stress, Appraisal, & Coping, Springer Publishing Company,
incident among african American university student, J. Black Psychol. 40 (1)
New York, NY, 1984.
(2014) 81–111, https://doi.org/10.1177/009579841247168.
[12] R.S. Lazarus, Psychological Stress and the Coping Process, McGraw-Hill, New York,
[44] P. Salovey, J.D. Mayer, ‘EI,’ imagination, Cognit. Personal. 9 (1990) 185–211.
1966.
[45] C.-S. Wong, K.S. Law, The effects of leader and follower EI on performance and
[13] C.S. Carver, M.F. Scheier, J.K. Weintraub, Assessing coping strategies: a
attitude: an exploratory study, Leader. Q. 13 (2002) 243–274, https://doi.org/
theoretically based approach, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56 (2) (1989) 267.
10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00099-1.
[14] M.F. Scheier, J.K. Weintraub, C.S. Carver, Coping with stress: divergent strategies
[46] Y. Brunetto, S.T.T. Teo, K. Shacklock, R. Farr-Wharton, EI, job satisfaction, well-
of optimists and pessimists, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51 (1986) 1257–1264.
being and engagement: explaining organisational commitment and turnover
[15] S. Folkman, R.S. Lazarus, An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community
intentions in policing, Hum. Resour. Manag. J. 22 (2) (2012) 428–441.
sample, J. Health Soc. Behav. 21 (3) (1980) 219–239.
[47] L. Karimi, S.G. Leggat, L. Donohue, G. Farrell, G.E. Couper, Emotional rescue: the
[16] M. Rosario, S. Salzinger, R.S. Feldman, D.S. Ng-Mak, Community violence exposure
role of EI and emotional labour on well-being and job-stress among community
and delinquent behaviors among youth: the moderating role of coping,
nurses, J. Adv. Nurs. 70 (1) (2014) 176–186.
J. Community Psychol. 31 (2003) 489–512.

9
C. Prentice et al. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 51 (2020) 101789

[48] H.N. Perera, The role of trait emotional intelligence in academic performance: [64] T. Ke, J. Barlas, Thinking about feeling: using trait emotional intelligence in
theoretical overview and empirical update, J. Psychol. 150 (2) (2015) 229–251, understanding the associations between early maladaptive schemas and coping
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1079161. styles, Psychol. Psychother. Theor. Res. Pract. (2018), https://doi.org/10.1111/
[49] A. Ingram, W.O. Peake, W. Stewart, W. Watson, EI and venture performance, papt.12202.
J. Small Bus. Manag. 57 (3) (2019) 780–800. [65] K.V. Petrides, A. Furnham, Trait EI: psychometric investigation with reference to
[50] C. MacCann, Y. Jiang, L.E.R. Brown, K.S. Double, M. Bucich, A. Minbashian, EI established trait taxonomies, Eur. J. Pers. 15 (6) (2001) 425–448.
predicts academic performance: a meta-analysis, Psychol. Bull. 146 (2) (2020) [66] K.V. Petrides, J.C. Pérez-González, A. Furnham, On the criterion and incremental
150–186. validity of trait EI, Cognit. Emot. 21 (1) (2007) 26–55.
[51] S. Zeidan, Empirical investigation of the relationship between EI, organizational [67] P. O’Connor, J. Nguyen, J. Anglim, Effectively coping with task stress: a study of
commitment, and turnover intentions, J. Inf. Knowl. Manag. 19 (2) (2020) 1–13. the validity of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire–short form
[52] E. Engleberg, L. Sjoberg, EI, affect intensity and Social adjustment, Pers. Indiv. (TEIQue–SF), J. Pers. Assess. (2016), https://doi.org/10.1080/
Differ. 37 (2003) 533–542. 00223891.2016.1226175.
[53] N.S. Schutte, J.M. Malouff, E.B. Thorsteinsson, N. Bhullar, S. Rooke, A meta- [68] A.B. Siegling, Ashley K. Vesely, K.V. Petrides, Donald H. Saklofske, Incremental
analytic investigation of the relationship between EI and health, Pers. Indiv. Differ. validity of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire–short form (TEIQue–SF),
42 (2007) 921–933. J. Pers. Assess. 97 (5) (2015) 525–535, https://doi.org/10.1080/
[54] G. Matthew, M. Zeidner, EI, adaptation to stressful encounters & health outcomes, 00223891.2015.1013219.
in: R. Bar-On, J.D.A. Parker (Eds.), The Handbook of EI, Jossey-Bass/Pfeiffer, San [69] M. Barr, B. Raphael, M. Taylor, G. Stevens, L. Jorm, M. Giffin, S. Lujic, Pandemic
Francisco, CA, 2001. influenza in Australia: using telephone surveys to measure perceptions of threat
[55] G.J. Taylor, Low EI and mental health, in: J. Ciarrochi, J. Forgas, J. Mayer (Eds.), and willingness to comply, BMC Infect. Dis. 8 (2008) 117, https://doi.org/
EI in Everyday Life: A Scientific Inquiry, Psychology Press, Philadelphia, 2001, 10.1186/1471-2334-8-117.
pp. 67–81. [70] H. Marshall, P. Ryan, D. Roberton, J. Street, M. Watson, Pandemic influenza and
[56] R. Bar-On, EQ-i Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory, Multi-Health Systems, community preparedness, Am. J. Publ. Health 99 (2009) S365–S371.
Toronto, 1997. [71] R.J. Blendon, L.M. Koonin, J.M. Benson, M.S. Cetron, W.E. Pollard, E.W. Mitchell,
[57] E.J. Austin, D.H. Saklofske, S.M. Mastoras, EI, coping and exam-related stress in M.J. Herrmann, Public response to community mitigation measures for pandemic
Canadian undergraduate students, Aust. J. Psychol. 62 (2010) 42–50. influenza, Emerg. Infect. Dis. 14 (5) (2008) 778–786.
[58] M. Perković Kovačević, I. Požgain, P. Filaković, I. Grujčić, Relationship between [72] H. Seale, J. Leask, K. Po, C.R. MacIntyre, “Will they just pack up and leave?” –
coping strategies and EI among patients with schizophrenia, Psychiatr. Danub. 30 attitudes and intended behaviour of hospital health care workers during an
(3) (2018) 299–304, https://doi.org/10.24869/psyd.2018.299. influenza pandemic, BMC Health Serv. Res. 9 (2009) 30, https://doi.org/10.1186/
[59] A.C. Baum, M.A. King, Creating a climate of self-awareness in early childhood 1472-6963-9-30.
teacher preparation programs, Early Child. Educ. J. 33 (2006) 217–222, https:// [73] M. Singh, P. Sarkhel, G. Kang, et al., Impact of demographic disparities in social
doi.org/10.1007/s10643-005-0050-2. distancing and vaccination on influenza epidemics in urban and rural regions of the
[60] S. Nizielski, S. Hallum, A. Schütz, P.N. Lopes, A Note on Emotion appraisal and United States, BMC Infect. Dis. 19 (2019) 221, https://doi.org/10.1186/s12879-
burnout: the mediating role of antecedent-focused coping strategies, J. Occup. 019-3703-2.
Health Psychol. 18 (3) (2013) 363–369. [74] P.G. Williams, H.K. Rau, M.R. Cribbet, H.E. Gunn, Openness to Experience and
[61] E.A. Skinner, M.J. Zimmer-Gembeck, The development of coping, Annu. Rev. stress regulation, J. Res. Pers. 43 (5) (2009) 777–784, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Psychol. 58 (2007) 119–144. jrp.2009.06.003.
[62] E. Ong, C. Thompson, The Importance of Coping and Emotion Regulation in the [75] K.V. Petrides, M. Mikolajczak, S. Mavroveli, M.-J. Sanchez-Ruiz, A. Furnham, J.-
Occurrence of Suicidal Behavior, Psychological Reports, 2018, 003329411878185. C. Pérez-González, Developments in trait emotional intelligence research, Emot.
[63] Maryse Pelletier, Emotions Experienced and Coping Strategies Used by Family Rev. 8 (4) (2016) 335–341, https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916650493.
Members of Organ Donors, Canadian Journal of Nursing Research Archive, 1993 [76] C.H. Reynolds, L.M. O’Dwyer, Examining the relationships among EI, coping
[S.l.], ISSN 1705-7051. mechanisms for stress, and leadership effectiveness for middle school principals,
J. Sch. Leader. 18 (5) (2008) 472–500.

10

You might also like