You are on page 1of 20

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Borsa _Istanbul Review


_
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112
http://www.elsevier.com/journals/borsa-istanbul-review/2214-8450

Full Length Article

Investor psychology in the stock market: An empirical study of the impact


of overconfidence on firm valuation
Ruqayya Aljifri
King Abdulaziz University, Department of Economics, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia
Received 17 March 2022; revised 20 September 2022; accepted 20 September 2022
Available online 29 September 2022

Abstract

Behavioral theories suggest that overconfident investors overestimate the quality of their information and underestimate risk. They have a high
demand for risky assets and require a lower risk premium, causing asset prices to rise and leading to overvaluation. We investigate how over-
confidence affects firm valuation in Saudi Arabia's emerging stock market. We used 4004 firm-quarter observations. To ensure that our results are
robust to unobserved firm-specific heterogeneity and endogeneity issues, we used the fixed-effects panel data model and the dynamic panel data
model. The findings show that overconfidence positively and significantly affects firm valuation. Results remain robust when using various
overconfidence proxies and when replacing the different econometric models. This study has important implications for academics, investors, and
regulators. Hopefully, it will make investors more aware of the impact of their psychology on asset pricing; thus, increasing the rationality of their
stock market decision making for improved market efficiency.
Copyright © 2022 Borsa İstanbul Anonim Şirketi. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

JEL classification: C12; C23; D81; G11; G41


Keywords: Behavioral biases; Overconfidence; Firm valuation; Investor psychology; Emerging stock market

1. Introduction decision-making process (Bakar & Yi, 2016; Barber & Odean,
2001; Daniel & Hirshleifer, 2015; Hirshleifer & Luo, 2001;
The behavior of stock markets may deviate from the as- Lambert, Bessière, & N'Goala, 2012; Statman, Thorley, &
sumptions of standard theories, and such theories have failed to Vorkink, 2006; Tekçe & Yılmaz, 2015).
provide plausible explanations for stock market anomalies such Behavioral theories predict that the overconfidence bias of
as mispricing and overvaluation. Stock prices and market investors may lead to overpricing and overvaluation
valuation are affected not only by fundamental company fac- (Adebambo & Yan, 2018). Theories posit that overconfident
tors (Fama & French, 2015, 2021) and macroeconomic fun- investors overestimate the quality and precision of their in-
damentals (Burmeister, Roll, & Ross, 1994) but also by formation while underestimating risk; as a result, they have an
investor psychology, such as overconfidence bias (Adebambo irrationally high demand for risky assets and require a lower
& Yan, 2018). Investor psychology and how investors pro- risk premium, inflating asset prices and eventually leading to
cess available information affect their decision-making process firm overvaluation. In other words, overconfidence among in-
and, therefore, stock prices and market valuation. Over- vestors leads to stock overpricing because overconfident in-
confidence is a key factor in understanding the decision- vestors overestimate the quality of their knowledge while
making process in financial markets (Daniel & Hirshleifer, underestimating the risk in the stock (Trejos, van Deemen,
2015). Overconfidence bias significantly affects investors' Rodríguez, & Gómez, 2019). This simply means that they
deem their assessment and judgment of a situation to be su-
E-mail address: raljifri@kau.edu.sa. perior to what the situation truly is (Pompian, 2011). As a
Peer review under responsibility of Borsa İstanbul Anonim Şirketi.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bir.2022.09.010
2214-8450/Copyright © 2022 Borsa İstanbul Anonim Şirketi. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

result, the required risk premium is lower, and the price paid companies in the Saudi stock market. Fig. 1 shows that the
for the security is higher (Adebambo & Yan, 2018). Over- TASI series seems to have increased rapidly overall. According
confident investors overvalue their own knowledge and skills to behavioral finance, this rapid increase can be explained by
(Trejos et al., 2019) and act on their beliefs. Because they investors’ psychology, which is consistent with the Saudi-
overestimate their skills and abilities in interpreting available related literature that found investors in the Saudi stock mar-
information, they may interpret the same signals differently ket are characterized by overconfidence bias, which has a
than rational investors. They may cause overpricing and significant effect on their investment decisions (Alquraan,
overvaluation because they may trade excessively and buy Alqisie, & Al Shorafa, 2016; Matoussi & Mostafa, 2016).
more shares of a particular company simply because they Overconfidence may cause undesirable effects on stock
believe the stock they are buying will outperform the one they markets, such as mispricing, excessive trading volumes, and
are selling. Excess demand is caused by overconfidence bias, increased market volatility (Kunjal & Peerbhai, 2021). This
which may result in mispricing (Adebambo & Yan, 2018; study demonstrates the significance of overconfidence. It
Brown & Cliff, 2005; Trejos et al., 2019). demonstrates how overconfidence bias can lead to stock mar-
A valuation is an opinion of market investors and is affected ket mispricing and overvaluation. The study raised the concern
by their psychology and the way in which they process in- that stock market investors may overestimate their own
formation. According to Brown and Cliff (2005), investor knowledge and interpret available information in ways that
psychology drove the rapid rise and fall of technology stock lead to overpricing and overvaluation. However, asset price
prices and valuations. They mentioned the need for stock increases are associated with bubbles such as the Mississippi
valuation models to measure deviations from theoretical prices bubble (1719–1720) and the South Sea bubble (1720)
Hong, Scheinkman, and Xiong (2006) confirmed that hetero- (Brunnermeier, 2016).
geneous beliefs due to overconfidence cause asset prices to There has been much research on overconfidence (Barber &
exceed their fundamental value, which may lead to over- Odean, 2000, 2002; Gervais, Heaton, & Odean, 2007; Grinblatt
pricing. Investor psychology is crucial in determining stock & Keloharju, 2009; Odean, 1998). However, previous literature
prices and may be a source of mispricing (Baker & Wurgler, has focused on examining the effect of investors’ biases on their
2007, 2013). The proposition that changes in overconfidence decision-making process, market anomalies and trading volume
affect stock prices is mentioned in studies by Gervais and (Barber & Odean, 2000, 2001; Grinblatt & Keloharju, 2009;
Odean (2001) and Scheinkman and Xiong (2003). Our Odean, 1998; Statman et al., 2006). To the best of our knowl-
assumption is that firm valuation is directly influenced by edge, there have been two studies on its influence on firm and
overconfidence bias; therefore, firms with overconfident in- stock market valuation (Adebambo & Yan, 2018; Bouteska &
vestors are overvalued. Regaieg, 2018), both of which focused solely on the US stock
We note that psychological biases affect investor behavior market. Moreover, Adebambo and Yan (2018) and Bouteska
and, therefore, their investment choices. Focusing on the Saudi and Regaieg (2018) applied cross-sectional and fixed effect
stock market context, Fig. 1 illustrates the evolution of the models, respectively, failing to provide an alternative estimation
Saudi stock market index, TASI, over 11 years. The TASI approach such as a dynamic panel model to control for dynamic
index is the major index tracking the performance of all listed endogeneity and to check the robustness of their findings.

Fig. 1. Evolution of the TASI market index over the 11-year period (2009–2019).

94
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

In Saudi Arabia, no literature examines the impact of these studies, the current study used two econometric models; a
investor bias on the behavior of a stock. Only a few studies fixed effect model and a dynamic panel data model (system
have examined the impact of investor bias on their decision- GMM) to ensure that our findings are robust to unobserved
making process, with the goal of identifying which biases firm-specific heterogeneity and endogeneity issues. As a result,
affect investor decision making (Alnajjar, 2013; Alquraan this study contributes to the development of empirical models
et al., 2016; Alsedrah & Ahmed, 2017; Matoussi & Mostafa, for the relationship between investor overconfidence and firm
2016). However, this literature does not examine the influ- valuation. Third, a challenge any empirical study faces when
ence of investor biases on the behavior of stock markets, such examining the impact of investor overconfidence is finding
as mispricing and market valuation. Unlike previous studies in good proxies of overconfidence. This study adds to the body of
the Saudi context, which have focused solely on the impact of knowledge by introducing new proxies of investor over-
investor bias on investors’ decision-making process, this study confidence (three alternative proxies to measure excessive
examines the impact of investor bias on the behavior of the trading) that are correlated with overconfidence bias and are
stock market and particularly examines the impact of investor based on prior research, as shown in Section 6. We also pro-
overconfidence on asset prices and stock market valuation. vide evidence that our proxies of investor overconfidence do
Behavioral finance studies not only the impact of psychological not simply pick up the previously documented results.
biases on investor behavior but also the subsequent effect on The methodology considered here consists of developing
the behavior of stock markets (Sewell, 2010). panel data models, using 4004 firm-quarter observations
Given investors’ emotional behavior in the stock market, covering 11 years, from 2009 to 2019. We applied this study's
this paper explores the relationship between investor psychol- fixed-effects panel data model for the main analysis. We
ogy and stock market behavior. Particularly, we analyze the employed a panel regression model to analyze our secondary
influence of investor overconfidence on stock prices and firm data because the data collected exhibited cross-sectional and
valuation. We employ two econometrics models of fixed effect time-series dimensions. The panel data model has the advan-
and dynamic system generalized method of moments (GMM) tage of providing cross-sectional and time-series dimensions,
estimations to ensure our results are robust for both unobserved allowing the researcher to control for unobserved heterogene-
firm-specific heterogeneity and endogeneity issues. Moreover, ity. We used a panel fixed-effects model to examine the impact
we experimented with many alternative proxies for investor of overconfidence bias on the valuation of Saudi companies
overconfidence to ensure that our results are not sensitive to the listed on the stock exchange. Generally, pooled panel data
proxy variable. In this study, we present six alternative ways to analysis ignores the heterogeneity that might exist among
measure investor overconfidence, introducing new proxies of companies (Gujarati & Porter, 2003), and it does not distin-
investor overconfidence (three alternative proxies to measure guish between the various companies included in our study.
excessive trading) that are based on prior literature and are not Based on the F-statistic (homogeneity test) and the Hausman
simply picking up previously documented results, as shown in test results (χ 2 test), As discussed in detail in the methodology
Section 6. Our results are robust when using all six over- section, we concluded that the fixed-effects model is the most
confidence proxy variables and replacing the different econo- appropriate and consistent model for our sample. Therefore, we
metric models, which confirms our findings. This study used the fixed-effects model for panel data regression to
considered the aggregate investor behavior of companies listed examine our null hypothesis. In addition, as an alternative
on the Saudi stock market. Chuang and Lee (2006); Gervais analysis to test the robustness of our results, we used dynamic
and Odean (2001) argued that investor behavior should be systems GMM estimation to control for endogeneity issues.
observable in aggregate level data. This is supported by Fama Dynamic endogeneity occurs when the past values of the
(2021a), who stated that for a financial theory to be valid, it dependent variable influence the future values of the indepen-
should explain market behavior rather than that of a specific dent variable, which can lead to incorrect inferences and biased
group of investors. estimates. Despite controlling for unobserved firm-specific
We are making three contributions. First, the Saudi stock heterogeneity, the fixed effect ignores the correlation between
market is a developing market in a collectivist society that is a the lagged dependent variable and the error term, which may
member of both the Middle East and North Africa (MENA) result in biased coefficient estimates. For instance, all else
and the Gulf Cooperation Council (GCC) stock markets. As a being equal, the firm valuation in a previous year may affect
result, this study adds to our understanding of the impact of investor overconfidence in subsequent years. To ensure our
overconfidence on mispricing and firm valuation in emerging results are robust to endogeneity issues such as dynamic
markets with collectivist societies like Turkey, China, India, endogeneity, we use the dynamic system GMM estimation as
and Taiwan, as well as the MENA and GCC stock markets, an alternative estimation approach to ensure our results are
where data are not easily available. To the best of our robust to endogeneity issues.
knowledge, two studies, Adebambo and Yan (2018) and The findings have implications for academic researchers,
Bouteska and Regaieg (2018), have examined the impact of market investors, and market regulators, as well as implications
investor overconfidence on firm valuation; however, they were for investment expenditures, capital allocation, and market
solely focused on the US stock market. Second, Adebambo and efficiency. Our findings may also help explain mispricing and
Yan (2018) and Bouteska and Regaieg (2018) applied cross- overvaluation in emerging markets with collectivist societies
sectional and fixed effect models, respectively. In contrast to and in the MENA and the GCC stock markets, because Saudi
95
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

Arabia's stock market is an emerging market with a collectivist with lower p-values of less than 5% for overconfidence proxies
society that is a member of both MENA and GCC stock compared to the fixed effect approach, implying a stronger
markets. The findings also support behavioral theories that relationship and higher significance levels. The dynamic sys-
predict that investor psychology affects asset pricing and, tems GMM results are consistent with the main results reported
therefore, the market valuation, suggesting that the role of for the analysis of the fixed-effects estimation. Overall,
investor behavior should be considered in asset pricing models. robustness checks confirm our results and suggest that our
Furthermore, the current study should help stock market main findings are robust. In both the main and alternative
investors become more aware of the impact of their own psy- regression analyses, the significant positive relationship be-
chological factors, specifically overconfidence bias, on asset tween the valuation of listed companies in the Saudi stock
pricing and market valuation, thereby increasing the rationality market and all overconfidence proxy variables supports our
of their stock market decision making for improved market hypothesis.
efficiency. Further interventions may be needed to raise in- The rest of this paper is structured as follows. Section 2
vestors' awareness of their behavioral biases, helping them outlines the theoretical background and reviews the related
make better use of information sources when making invest- literature. Section 3 contains the research design, and Section 4
ment decisions. Furthermore, our results show that this explains the methodology. Section 5 reports the empirical
cognitive bias can cause firm value in the Saudi stock market to findings and discussion; Section 6 contains the robustness and
deviate from normal toward overvaluation. However, price sensitivity analysis, and Section 7 concludes this paper.
bubbles arise when asset prices exceed the asset's fundamental
value. Bubbles were associated with asset price increases 2. Theoretical background and related literature
(Brunnermeier, 2016). Famous examples include the Dutch
tulip mania (1634–1637), the Mississippi bubble (1719–1720) Unintentionally, people use shortcuts and heuristics to deal
and the South Sea bubble (1720). Therefore, market regulators with some decisions, avoiding the complexity of estimating
should be aware of the influence that investor behavior can probabilities with each decision, which may save time and
have on market bubbles and economic activity. effort while resulting in good decisions. However, they may
Market valuation is also a key concept in finance, which has also lead to systematic biases causing serious losses, particu-
significant implications for market efficiency, capital alloca- larly in the case of financial decisions (Goodwin & Wright,
tion, and investment expenditures. According to studies by 2014). Contrary to what standard financial theories suggest,
Khanna and Sonti (2004), Zaigham, Wang, and Ali (2019), and investors' psychology influence their decision-making process
Blanchard, Rhee, and Summers (1993), an increase in market to a great extent, and therefore, investors are not fully rational.
valuation results in an increase in a company's capital expen- These psychological factors may lead to biases such as over-
diture activity. As a result, investment expenditures are reduced confidence, and therefore must be taken into consideration
when a company's market value declines (Asker, Farre-Mensa, while making investment decisions (Chandra, 2008). Funda-
& Ljungqvist, 2015; Del Brio, De Miguel, & Pindado, 2003; mental data do not explain the stock market crisis, remarkable
Dessaint, Foucault, Frésard, & Matray, 2019; Kouser, Saba, & price decrease (Shiller, 1987), and high price volatility in 1987
Anjum, 2016). Significant evidence of linkages between mar- (Siegel, 1992); these events are mainly caused by investors'
ket valuation, capital market efficiency and capital allocation biases. Behavioral finance has introduced investors’ behavior
can be found in Baker (2009). The current study investigates as a factor that may influence stock returns, performance and
the impacts of behavioral bias, namely, the overconfidence of valuation (Adebambo & Yan, 2018; Bollerslev, Gibson, &
investors in the valuation of the Saudi stock market companies. Zhou, 2011; Bouteska & Regaieg, 2018) as well as market
Our model's main prediction is that investor overconfidence anomalies (Stracca, 2004).
bias leads to overvaluation. Consistent with our prediction, we There has been a growing need in the relatively recent field
discovered that investor overconfidence has a positive and of behavioral finance, which enables us to explain the empir-
significant impact on company valuations in the Saudi stock ically observed financial phenomena and anomalies that cannot
market. Our results show that overconfidence bias causes the be explained by traditional theories (Barberis & Thaler, 2005,
Saudi stock market valuation to deviate from normal toward pp. 1–76) and proposes alternative theories that consider the
overvaluation. Two main robustness and sensitivity analyses effect of investors' psychology on their decisions and stock
were performed in this study. First, we used six different markets. The field of behavioral finance is a combination of
overconfidence proxies to ensure that our results were not standard finance, psychology, and sociology, attempting to
affected by the variable proxy. All proxies yield statistically explain anomalies (Ricciardi & Simon, 2000). Behavioral
significant results. Our findings remained unchanged when we finance can be defined as the area that explores the influence of
used the alternative overconfidence measures. The results agree investors’ psychology on their financial decision-making pro-
with our main proxy variable of overconfidence. Second, we cess (Shefrin, 2002), and consequently, financial markets.
conducted dynamic system GMM regression analyses as an The reasons for conducting this research in the Saudi stock
alternative estimation approach to ensure our results were market are as follows. First and foremost, the Saudi stock
robust to endogeneity problems. Results show that over- market is by far the largest, most liquid, and most actively
confidence is significant across all models at 1% and 5% levels. traded in the GCC countries and the Arab world as a whole
The dynamic systems GMM model yielded larger coefficients (Abdalla & Suliman, 2012; Mensi, 2019; Rahman,
96
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

Chowdhury, & Sadique, 2015). Second, this type of research as better than that of their peers (Pikulina, Renneboog, &
has been done on the US stock market (Adebambo & Yan, Tobler, 2017). Specifically, stock market investors' exhibit
2018; Bouteska & Regaieg, 2018); however, the Saudi stock overconfidence when they make financial decisions (Baker &
market, an emerging stock market in a collectivist society that Nofsinger, 2002). More than half of investors see their stock
is a member of both the MENA and the GCC stock markets, is selection skills as better than other investors’ (Statman et al.,
considered in this study. Therefore, we use this research to 2006). Chuang and Lee (2006) provided extensive evidence
expand our understanding of the impact of overconfidence on of the presence of overconfidence in financial markets.
firm valuation in emerging markets with collectivist societies Overconfident investors tend to give more privilege to their
such as Turkey, China, India, and Taiwan, as well as the own information than information that is publicly available to
MENA and the GCC stock markets, where data are not easily all other investors as well as their abilities and skills in inter-
available. preting the available information, and consequently will trade
Third, investors in the Saudi stock market are characterized excessively (Barber & Odean, 2000; Korniotis & Kumar,
by overconfidence, which has a significant effect on their in- 2008). They believe that if they follow their own ideas and
vestment decisions (Alquraan et al., 2016; Matoussi & rules, they will be more successful, which satisfies them even if
Mostafa, 2016). Alsabban and Alarfaj (2020) investigated it is not justified. In line with these findings, Daniel,
overconfidence bias in the Saudi stock market and found evi- Hirshleifer, and Subrahmanyam (2005) found that investors
dence that investors exhibit overconfidence bias. The majority overreact to private information signals while underreacting to
of traders in the Saudi stock market are individuals, inexperi- publicly available information signals. Overconfident investors
enced males aged 30–39 years (Alsedrah & Ahmed, 2017). overestimate the precision of their private information in
According to Menkhoff, Schmeling, and Schmidt (2013), in- comparison to publicly available data. According to Trejos
dividual investors are more overconfident than institutional et al. (2019), overconfident investors tend to believe that
investors. According to surveys and experimental studies, men their abilities and the accuracy of their own information are
are more overconfident than women (Barber & Odean, 2001; better than average. They also added that overconfident in-
Lundeberg, Fox, & Punćcohaŕ, 1994; Prince, 1993; Prosad, vestors tend to have a high trading volume.
Kapoor, & Sengupta, 2015). These findings are supported by Overconfident investors are not necessarily incompetent or
Tekçe and Yılmaz (2015), who found that overconfidence is ignorant; rather, they assume that their judgments’ accuracy is
higher among males and young investors than among females higher than what it truly is (Pompian, 2011). Benos (1998)
and older investors. Additionally, overconfidence behavior in examined an extreme form of posterior overconfidence in
emerging markets such as Saudi Arabia has received consid- which some risk-neutral investors overestimate the accuracy of
erable critical attention, as empirical studies have proven that their personal information and compete in market orders with
market anomalies tend to be more dominant in emerging another group of informed traders who have rational expecta-
markets (Abdeldayem & Mahmoud, 2013; Chen, Kim, tions. He confirmed that a higher trading volume and a deeper
Nofsinger, & Rui, 2007; Metwally & Darwish, 2015). In- market result from overconfident investors in the stock market.
vestors in emerging markets with collectivist characteristics, More importantly, he demonstrated that such traders may
such as Turkey, China, India, and Taiwan, are prone to outperform rational traders in terms of expected profits.
behavioral biases compared to their counterparts in developed Overconfident investors may profit more than rational investors
and individualistic societies, such as the USA (Tekçe & because they take on more risk (Bradford, De Long, Summers,
Yılmaz, 2015). Therefore, we may expect the effect of over- & Waldmann, 1991) and may exploit mispricing caused by
confidence bias on the emerging stock market of Saudi Arabia noise trading (Hirshleifer & Luo, 2001). In line with these
with a collectivist society to be significant and strong. There- findings, Kyle and Wang (1997) showed that overconfident
fore, research on this issue in the Saudi stock market is traders may strictly outperform rational traders because an
worthwhile. However, to our knowledge, no studies have overconfident trader is more likely to generate higher expected
investigated the effect of biases from which Saudi investors profit and utility than his rational opponent and higher than if
suffer on the behavior of the stock market. he were also a rational trader. They concluded that over-
This section contains a review of overconfidence bias, in confident investors could persist and survive eventually.
addition to the Saudi-related literature, related studies, and the Overconfidence bias is the source of high levels of trading
research hypothesis. Overconfidence bias is based on a large on financial markets because overconfident investors tend to
body of psychological experiments and survey research. have higher trading volumes than rational investors (Odean,
1998). In line with these results, Barber and Odean (1999)
2.1. Overconfidence bias concluded that overconfidence bias increases trading volume
because overconfident investors overvalue their knowledge and
The overconfidence concept is driven by psychological abilities and, as a result, are too certain about their own
experiments and surveys, and it is the most frequently choices, ignoring other options. They also concluded that
mentioned bias in behavioral finance (Ho, 2011). Most in- investor psychology results in systematic overconfidence bia-
dividuals rank themselves better than average and better than ses, which may influence asset prices. Jain, Jain, and Jain
others rank them (Taylor & Brown, 1988). They frequently (2015) found that stock market investors tend to be over-
overestimate their own performance as better than it is, as well confident and therefore trade too much, driving stock prices to
97
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

behave unexpectedly. In line with these findings, according to a country-specific context. The literature analysis reveals that
Alrabadi, Al-Abdallah, and Aljarayesh (2018), overconfidence this topic requires further investigation.
bias explains the majority of the changes in stock prices on the
Amman stock exchange. Given the influence of the over- 2.2. Review of Saudi-related literature
confidence bias in investors' decisions and stock prices, it is
expected that this bias will ultimately affect stock market Alnajjar (2013) investigated the investors' irrational attitude
valuation. Adebambo and Yan (2018) investigated the effects while investing in the Saudi stock market, using a question-
of investor overconfidence on US stock market firm valuation naire approach collected from 119 respondents who invest in
and discovered a strong positive relationship between investor Tadawul. He used correlational statistics to describe the rela-
overconfidence and firm valuation; firms with more over- tionship between the variables in his study. The findings
confident investors are overvalued. Consistent with these indicate that investors' decision-making processes in Tadawul
findings, Bouteska and Regaieg (2018) found that over- are influenced by their psychology. He tested the correlations
confidence positively impacts Tobin's Q of companies in the between the independent variables, namely information type,
US stock market. information asymmetry and risk propensity on the risk
This part of the literature showed that overconfidence bias perception variable. The information - type variable includes
influences the valuation of companies listed in the Saudi stock different kinds of information: interest rate, improper man-
market, which leads to our hypothesis. agement and variation in stock market policies. Then, subse-
H0: Overconfidence bias positively affects the valuation of quently, he tested how risk perception is correlated with the
companies listed in the Saudi stock market. variables, namely, reinvestment intension, investment perfor-
In conclusion, this review includes pioneering and cutting- mance, return expectations and investment performance. He
edge research on this topic. The aforementioned studies' find- found that the information type is positively correlated with the
ings show that the investors' overconfidence bias has been the investors’ risk perception. This shows that different types of
focus of financial literature studies (Barber & Odean, 2000, information are significantly important for an investment de-
2001; Grinblatt & Keloharju, 2009; Heger & Papageorge, cision. Information asymmetry is also positively correlated
2018; Odean, 1998; Statman et al., 2006; Trejos et al., 2019). with risk perception, whereas the relationship between risk
Previous studies have focused on examining the effects of in- propensity and risk perception was negatively formed in his
vestors' biases on their decision-making process, market study. Moreover, he found that reinvestment intension invest-
anomalies, and trading volume (Barber & Odean, 2000, 2001; ment performance, and investment satisfaction are negatively
Grinblatt & Keloharju, 2009; Odean, 1998; Statman et al., correlated with risk perception. In comparison, there was a
2006). Overconfidence has been scrutinized by a great deal positive correlation between risk perception and return expec-
of research Barber and Odean (2000, 2002); Gervais et al. tations. However, he focused only on risk perception: What
(2007); Gervais and Odean (2001); Grinblatt and Keloharju factors are correlated with risk perception, and, subsequently,
(2009); Odean (1998), yet there have been few studies on its how is risk perception correlated with other variables of rein-
influence on firm and stock market valuation Adebambo and vestment intension investment performance, return expecta-
Yan (2018); Bouteska and Regaieg (2018). These studies, tions, and investment performance? Also, he only used the
however, have concentrated on the stock market in the United correlational stats method to analyze his data.
States. None have been used in Saudi Arabia because previous Alquraan et al. (2016) examined the impact of behavioral
research has ignored the Saudi stock market in favor of finance factors on the investment decisions of investors in the
American data, which is more readily available. The few Saudi stock market using a multiple linear regression test. They
studies examining the impact of investor overconfidence on distributed 140 questionnaires randomly. They found that
firm valuation have not used a dynamic panel model, so they behavioral factors (overconfidence, loss aversion, and risk
do not account for dynamic endogeneity. This study aims to fill perception) significantly affect the stock investment decisions
that gap by controlling for endogeneity issues and focusing on of individual investors in the Saudi stock market, whereas herd
the emerging Saudi stock market. Our study is the first to has an insignificant effect. However, this study only looked at
investigate the impact of overconfidence on the valuation of Saudi investors in 2015. (Fama, 2021b) stated that for a
companies listed on Saudi Arabia's emerging stock market, and financial theory to be valid, it should explain the market rather
it will explain the market rather than a specific group of in- than a specific group of investors. We considered the aggregate
vestors, as Fama (2021b) suggested. investor behavior of companies listed on the Saudi stock
Moreover, the influence may differ depending on the country market. In the same year, Matoussi and Mostafa (2016)
or location of investigation; for example, Zia, Ilyas Sindhu, and investigated the effect of psychological biases on Saudi in-
Haider Hashmi (2017) found that investors in the United States vestors’ decision making using a survey approach and
are more overconfident than investors in South Asian countries. analyzing the data using descriptive statistics and correlation.
Corredor, Ferrer, and Santamaria (2013) studied four European They found that Saudi investors tend to be overconfident
nations with similar financial development and found that in- because they are affected by rumors and herding behavior that
vestors’ sentiments influence stock prices, and country-specific hinder them from making rational decisions. However, they
characteristics influence sentiments. Therefore, it is necessary to used simple descriptive statistics and correlation approaches to
investigate how overconfidence bias influences stock markets in analyze their data.
98
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

Alsedrah and Ahmed (2017) attempted to determine the observations, including 91 of the companies over 11 years. The
behavioral factors that influence investors' speculative behavior. survey analysis might suffer from reliability and validity issues.
The partial least square method results indicate that over- The survey results do not necessarily reflect the investors' real
confidence bias, among other biases, has a significant rela- thoughts. Additionally, the investors' attitudes will be affected
tionship with speculative behavior. In contrast, loss aversion by the investing environment, especially if this environment is
and regret aversion do not play a significant role in investors’ difficult at the time of collection. For example, it is difficult to
speculative behavior. Recently, Alsabban and Alarfaj (2020) ensure that coronavirus (COVID-19) does not affect the in-
used monthly data from 2007 to 2018 to investigate the pres- vestors' attitudes when completing the survey. Therefore, the
ence of overconfidence bias in the Saudi stock market and current research results are more reliable than the conclusions
applied the vector autoregression (VAR) model. They discov- of an analysis based on a survey. Moreover, real trading data
ered that Saudi stock market investors are overconfident. They, accurately reflect the psychological bias tendencies of in-
on the other hand, used data at the market level, whereas we vestors. Ho (2011), Bouteska and Regaieg (2018), Adebambo
used data at the firm level over time. Unlike stock market and Yan (2018), and Bailey, Kumar, and Ng (2011) used
trading volume, each company may have specific information in actual market trading data to substitute for investors’ psycho-
each period that explains a large portion of the trading volume. logical biases.
By reviewing the relevant Saudi literature, there are a few Fifth, Saudi-related literature has empirically proven that
points to be clarified, and we will identify the gap in the Saudi investors exhibit irrational behavior in the Saudi stock market,
studies as follows. First, regarding overconfidence bias results, which confirms that investors do not always follow traditional
according to Statman et al. (2006), if historical stock returns are finance theories, and their irrational behavior must be consid-
able to explain the current trading volume, then it confirms the ered when studying the Saudi stock market. However, none of
presence of overconfidence bias. Alsabban and Alarfaj (2020) the studies reviewed above has considered the influence of
found that historical stock returns are able to explain the cur- investor overconfidence on the Saudi stock market. Saudi-
rent trading volume in the Saudi stock market, and therefore, related studies have focused solely on finding evidence for
they confirmed the existence of overconfidence bias in the the existence of behavioral biases for investors in the Saudi
Saudi stock market. Moreover, overconfidence has a significant stock market in an attempt to identify which biases influence
positive effect on the investor decision (Alquraan et al., 2016), investors' decisions while also ignoring the consequent influ-
and Saudi investors are characterized by overconfidence ence of these biases on the Saudi stock market. In contrast to
(Matoussi & Mostafa, 2016). the previous studies reviewed above, this study is the first to
Second, these studies used correlational stats, ordinary least examine the impact of investor overconfidence on the valuation
squares (OLS) and its extension multiple linear regression, the of companies on the Saudi stock market. According to Sewell
partial least squares and VAR methods to analyze their data. (2010), behavioral finance studies the impact of psychological
Traditional linear regression models can lead to biased esti- biases on investors' behavior and its subsequent effect on the
mators due to unseen individual-specific heterogeneity. Panel stock market behavior. Our study, for the first time, examines
data regression, on the other hand, combines two dimensions the impact of investor overconfidence on the valuation of
that provide more information and data variation than time- companies on the Saudi stock market. This point is particularly
series regression. Panel fixed-effects regression, among other important, given that psychological biases may play a signifi-
panel regression models, provides significant interpretations and cant role in the Saudi stock market; for example, the main cause
can be generalized due to the large sample size (Abdulsamad, of the Saudi stock market crash of 2006 was the result of in-
Yusoff, & Lasyoud, 2018). In this study, we use the fixed- vestors’ behavior (Aljloud, 2016; Malik & Hammoudeh, 2007).
effects panel data model for the main analysis, as well as dy-
namic systems GMM estimation to control for endogeneity is- 3. Research design
sues as an alternative analysis to test the robustness of our
results. As a result, by controlling for unobserved heterogeneity 3.1. Data, sample, and variables measurement
and endogeneity issues, this study adds to the empirical models
used in the relationship between investor overconfidence and The target population for the current study is all nonfinance
the valuation of listed firms in the Saudi stock market. companies listed on the SSE from 2009 to 2019. Following the
Third, few studies exist that have considered the effect of literature, we excluded the financial and insurance firms from
investors' biases on their investment decision in the Saudi the sample for various reasons. We excluded financial com-
Stock Exchange (SSE) Tadawul since its establishment. Hence, panies from our sample due to the significant differences in
the current study remedies the relative dearth of research on their accounting methods and practices, as well as the various
this topic. Fourth, apart from Alsabban and Alarfaj (2020), regulatory restrictions for these companies compared to their
earlier studies conducted in the Saudi stock market context are nonfinance counterparts (Abed, Al-Attar, & Suwaidan, 2012;
questionnaire-based studies and faced small sample problems, Al-Najjar, 2011; Estrin, Hanousek, Kocenda, & Svejnar, 2009;
for example, Alnajjar (2013), Alquraan et al. (2016), Matoussi Jiraporn, Singh, & Lee, 2009), which can make capturing the
and Mostafa (2016), and Alsedrah and Ahmed (2017) distrib- effect of investor biases on firm valuation more difficult.
uted 119, 140, 180, and 130 questionnaires respectively. The Similar to previous works (Adebambo & Yan, 2018; Bouteska
current study uses secondary data with 4004 quarterly & Regaieg, 2018), excluding financial firms contributes to a
99
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

better exploration and analysis of the research questions, and it overconfidence variable proxy. Table 1 presents the source and
helps to avoid distorting the results. The following parameters definition of our variables.
were used to select the study sample of companies: To begin,
the sample includes companies that were listed on the stock 3.2. Measures of investor overconfidence
market at the start of the sample period but excludes companies
that ceased operations or were listed on the exchange during The fact that investor biases are not directly observable is a
the study's sample period. Second, no changes occurred in the challenge for any study of them. Because behavioral biases are
included companies, such as mergers, during the study period. difficult to measure precisely and directly, it is difficult to
Third, the company's annual reports and stock market data obtain a direct proxy for investor overconfidence (Baker &
must be available for all 11 years from 2009 to 2019. After Wurgler, 2013; Malmendier & Tate, 2005). Nevertheless, in
implicating the above criteria, the sample consisted of 91 Section 6, we experiment with many alternative variable
nonfinancial firms listed on the Saudi stock market that proxies for investor overconfidence to ensure our results are not
comprised 4004 firm-quarter observations, covering the period sensitive to the variable proxy and to check the robustness of
2009–2019. For this paper, we consider the quarterly data our findings. In this study, we provide six alternative ways to
period (frequency). The choice of the quarterly frequency is measure investor overconfidence, and we will show that, based
driven by the fact that, for calculating Tobin's Q ratio for firm on prior literature, these variable proxies are correlated with
valuation and other firm-specific attributes (e.g., market capi- overconfidence bias. Investor overconfidence proxies are
talization, return on assets and leverage of all firms in our defined as follows: the change in trading volume (Δtvit), three
sample, and the number of shares outstanding used to calculate alternative proxies to measure excessive trading (etit, etdit &
some overconfidence proxies), a few of these variables are etmit), turnover rate (turnoverit), and the increase in the number
disclosed with the SSE on a quarterly basis. The data for this of shares outstanding (isoit). There are no uncontroversial
study were provided by the SSE, which is a unique indepen- proxies for investor biases; however, the proxies used in this
dent financial data provider in Saudi Arabia. study are motivated by the literature. Although few of these
This study used Tobin's Q ratio (Tobin′sQit) as a dependent researchers have directly explored the company and stock
variable to measure the valuation of firms in the Saudi stock market valuation on which we concentrate, it is crucial to
market. We used Tobin's Q ratio (Tobin′sQit) as a measure of provide evidence that our proxies of investor overconfidence
firm valuation (Brainard & Tobin, 1968; Buchanan, Cao, & are not simply picking up the previously documented results.
Chen, 2018; Daines, 2001; Fang, NOE, & Tice, 2009;
Gompers, Ishii, & Metrick, 2003; Kumar & Singh, 2013; 3.2.1. The change in trading volume (Δtvit)
Yermack, 1996). Moreover, previous literature has identified a Based on this proxy, the overconfidence of investors in the
number of firm characteristics that are related to firm valuation, stock market is measured by the change in trading volume for
such as market capitalization, ROA, and leverage: market each company included in the current study and for each
capitalization (MarketCapit) (Chen & Lee, 2017; Feng & Wu, period. This proxy is motivated by a number of pioneer studies.
2018; Liu & Magnan, 2011; Yung & Jian, 2017), return on De Bondt and Thaler (1985) stated that investor overconfidence
assets (ROAit) (Al-Najjar & Anfimiadou, 2012; El Ghoul, is the key behavior in understanding the trading volume puzzle.
Guedhami, & Kim, 2017; Hassan, 2018; Jiao, 2010; Jo & Odean (1998) developed a theoretical model of over-
Harjoto, 2011; Liu & Magnan, 2011), and leverage (Lever- confidence, suggesting that overconfident investors overvalue
ageit) (Al-Najjar & Anfimiadou, 2012; Chen & Lee, 2017; El their own personal assessment and knowledge of an asset's
Ghoul et al., 2017; Feng & Wu, 2018; Hassan, 2018; Jiao, value, therefore, increasing trading volume. Odean (1998,
2010; Yung & Jian, 2017). We tested H0 by including the 1999) concluded that overconfidence bias is the source of high

Table 1
Description of variables.
Variables Definition Source
Tobin's Q ratio (Tobin'sQ) Ratio between a firm's stock market value and its Database of Saudi Stock Exchange
fixed capital replacement value, which is Company www.tadawul.com.sa
calculated by the source as the market value of
equity plus the book value of assets minus the
book value of equity divided by the book value
of assets.
Market capitalization (MarketCap) Total market value of a firm's outstanding shares,
which is calculated by the source as the product
of share price and the number of shares
outstanding.
Return on assets (ROA) Net profit as a percentage of total assets (%).
Leverage Ratio (Leverage) Ratio between total debt and total assets.
Investor overconfidence (Δtvit) Change in trading volume.

100
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

levels of trading volume on financial markets because over- De Bondt & Thaler, 1985; Odean, 1998; Zaiane, 2013). Similar
confident investors tend to trade more actively and to take results were documented by Barber and Odean (2001, 2002),
greater risk than rational investors. Similar results were docu- who provided evidence that overconfident investors engage in
mented by Barber and Odean (1999), who found that over- excessive stock market trading volumes.
confidence bias increases trading volume because The majority of the overconfidence models suggest a posi-
overconfident investors overvalue their knowledge and abili- tive correlation between trading volume and investor over-
ties; therefore, they are too sure about their own choices, confidence (Glaser & Weber, 2007). More precisely, investors
ignoring other choices. Chuang and Lee (2006) tested the with higher overconfidence tend to trade more excessively,
overconfidence hypothesis, which proposes that overconfident which will lead to higher trading volume (Gervais & Odean,
investors trade more risky securities because they underesti- 2001; Odean, 1998). Trinugroho and Sembel (2011) conduct-
mate risk, and discovered empirical evidence to support this ed experimental research to examine the excessive trading
hypothesis. An experimental study was carried out by Deaves, hypothesis. They found that investors who have higher over-
Lüders, and Luo (2009) provides solid evidence that higher confidence tend to practice the excessive trading strategy. They
investor overconfidence leads to increased trading volume. confirm that highly overconfident investors have higher trading
They confirmed that these findings hold true at both the indi- volumes than less overconfident investors. According to Trejos
vidual and market levels. Investors' overconfidence is linked to et al. (2019), overconfidence has been measured by excessive
stock trading changes (Strahilevitz, Odean, & Barber, 2011). trading. However, the manifestation of excessive trading is
A substantial amount of research has confirmed that investor hazy. As a result, we calculated three alternative proxies based
overconfidence leads to increased trading volume in stock on various thresholds to capture the expression “excessive”
markets, implying a positive relationship between trading trading volume.
volume and overconfidence (Barber & Odean, 2002; Benos, First, we calculated this proxy variable etit by including the
1998; Bradford et al., 1991; Chuang & Lee, 2006; Glaser & change in trading volume only if the change in trading volume
Weber, 2007; Grinblatt & Keloharju, 2009; Hirshleifer & is more than 0; that is, overconfident investors. To capture the
Luo, 2001; Statman et al., 2006). Several studies have used increase in trading volume, we defined overconfident investors
trading volume as a proxy for investor overconfidence using the 0 threshold. This variable is defined as follows:
(Alsabban & Alarfaj, 2020; Asaad, 2020; Barber & Odean,
Δtvit , if Δtvit > 0
1999, 2000; Deaves et al., 2009; Statman et al., 2006; Trejos etit = { (2)
et al., 2019). Therefore, trading volume seems to be a suit- ϕ, if Δtvit ≤ 0
able proxy variable for investor overconfidence. This proxy is
Where etit is the increased trading volume that represents
calculated from the change in trading volume as either an in-
“excessive trading,” and ϕ denotes a null value. Over-
crease or a decrease in trading volume relative to its prior
confidence is defined as a change in trading volume that is
period as follows:
greater than the previous period; otherwise, it is a null value.
tvit − tvit−1 This proxy was calculated for each company and period; each
Δtvit = (1)
tvit−1 company may have certain information in a period, which
would explain a large portion of the increase in trading volume.
Where Δtvit represents the change in trading volume for Second, we calculated this proxy variable etdit by including
company i at time t, tvit is the trading volume in the current the change in trading volume only if the change in trading
period and tvit−1 is the trading volume in the previous period. volume is more than the median μ1/2; that is, overconfident
Unlike stock market trading volume, each company might investors. We defined overconfident investors based on the
possess certain information in each period, which would median following Ho (2011). This variable is defined as
explain a large amount of the trading volume. follows:

3.2.2. Measures of excessive trading (etit, etdit & etmit) Δtvit , if Δtvit > μ1/2i
To more precisely capture the expression of “excessive etdit = { (3)
ϕ, if Δtvit ≤ μ1/2i
trading” we calculated three alternative proxies based on
different thresholds. Based on these proxies, investor over- where μ1/2i is the median of the trading volume change of a
confidence in the stock market is measured by an increase in company i.
trading volume, which is calculated for each company and time Third, we calculated this proxy variable etmit by including
period. Kyle and Wang (1997) stated that overconfident in- the change in trading volume only if the change in trading
vestors trade more excessively because they believe their own volume is more than the mean μik; that is, overconfident in-
information is better than others. Barber and Odean (1999) vestors. We defined overconfident investors based on the mean
proposed that excessive trading is an investor's common level for Δtvit to capture any increase in trading volume above
mistake. Many studies have highlighted that overconfident an average increase. The mean μik is calculated as follows:
investors will trade more excessively in stock markets and that
1 k
overconfidence is the key behavior to explain the observed μik = ∑t Δtvit (4)
excessive trading volume (Benos, 1998; Chuang & Lee, 2006; n

101
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

where μik is the mean for period k of company i, k is a fixed overconfidence that can be used as a robustness check.
window, and n is the number of observations in k. k is shifted Therefore, we provide two arguments to justify using this
forward by n observations to create the next nonoverlapping proxy for investor overconfidence.
window. In this study, k is equal to one year, n is equal to four First, according to Gasteren (2016), overconfidence is
quarters. This variable proxy is defined as follows: caused by culture, individual factors, and social factors. Indi-
vidual factors vary from person to person. However, over-
Δtvit , if Δtvit > μik
etmit = { , ∀t ∈ k (5) confidence is shared by the entire population due to both social
ϕ, if Δtvit ≤ μik and cultural factors. This population component implies that all
The linkage between overconfidence and the increase in individuals must share some degree of overconfidence in their
trading volume is plausible because overconfident investors behavior. As a result, it is reasonable to assume that the cultural
will trade excessively and increase trading volume. and social causes of overconfidence for investors and CEOs are
the same; thus, CEO overconfidence measures are good proxies
3.2.3. Turnover rate (turnoverit) for investor overconfidence from this perspective. This
Based on this proxy, investors’ overconfidence in the stock assumption is supported by relevant studies of Bouteska and
market is measured by the turnover rate for each company Regaieg (2018); Gasteren (2016). Following Bouteska and
included in this study and for each period. Overconfident in- Regaieg (2018), we chose an increase in the number of
vestors trade excessively and, therefore, increase the turnover shares outstanding as a substitute variable for investor over-
rate (Ho, 2011; Odean, 1998; Statman et al., 2006). According confidence. We consider an investor to be overconfident if he
to Barber and Odean (2001), higher turnover implies higher increased his holdings of firm shares during the period.
overconfidence, suggesting that turnover rate and investor Second, firms with more overconfident investors issue more
overconfidence are positively correlated. The turnover rate is equity and make more investments because corporate managers
used as the main proxy for overconfidence by a number of exploit stock market mispricing in their financing and invest-
studies Chuang and Lee (2006); Griffin, Nardari, and Stulz ment decisions (Adebambo & Yan, 2018; Baker & Wurgler,
(2007); Statman et al. (2006); Tekçe and Yılmaz (2015) 2002; Gilchrist, Himmelberg, & Huberman, 2005; Polk &
because the turnover findings are robust theoretically and Sapienza, 2008). Specifically, firms with more overconfident
empirically. investors issue a significantly higher number of shares than
Tekçe and Yılmaz (2015) used an annual turnover to mea- those with less overconfident investors (Adebambo & Yan,
sure overconfidence, which was calculated by multiplying the 2018). This proxy is motivated by the literature findings sug-
average monthly turnover by twelve. However, in our study, gesting that firms with more overconfident investors will issue
we used the quarterly turnover to measure overconfidence. For more shares, increasing the number of outstanding shares.
each period and firm, the turnover rate is defined as the ratio of According to these findings, the number of outstanding
the number of shares traded to the number of shares shares is positively related to investor overconfidence.
outstanding. Each firm may have more information in each Although the increase in shares outstanding is primarily used to
period, explaining a large portion of the turnover rate. There- measure managerial overconfidence, we believe it can also be
fore, the turnover rate seems to be a suitable proxy for investor used to measure investor overconfidence. As a result, the in-
overconfidence, and it is calculated as follows: crease in the number of shares outstanding was used as a
measure of investor overconfidence in this study, which was
tvit
turnoverit = (6) calculated as follows:
soit
Δsoit , if Δsoit > 0
isoit = { (7)
where soit is the number of shares outstanding. ϕ, if Δsoit ≤ 0

3.2.4. The increase in the number of shares outstanding where isoit represents the increase in the number of shares
(isoit) outstanding, and Δsoit is the change in the number of shares
Based on this proxy, the overconfidence of investors in the outstanding. If the number of shares outstanding in this period
stock market is measured by the increase in the number of is more than that in the previous period, it is defined as
outstanding shares, which is calculated for each company and overconfident.
each period. This proxy is motivated by Bouteska and Regaieg
(2018), who used shares outstanding as a proxy for investor 4. Methodology
overconfidence in the US stock market. This variable was
widely used as a proxy for managerial overconfidence We employed a panel regression model to analyze our
Malmendier and Tate (2005); Michailova (2010). Although the secondary data because the data collected exhibited cross-
justification for using proxy variables based on trading volume, sectional and time-series dimensions. The panel data model
excessive trading, and turnover is obvious, the justification for has the advantage of providing cross-sectional and time-series
using the proxy variable of shares outstanding may be less dimensions, allowing the researcher to control for unobserved
obvious. We assume, however, that including this proxy vari- heterogeneity. Using Stata software, we ran a panel fixed-
able will provide an additional alternative proxy for investor effects model StataCorp (2019) to capture the effect of

102
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

overconfidence bias on the valuation of companies listed on time in quarters that covers the period 2009–2019. H0 predicts
the SSE. In general, pooled panel data analysis ignores the that the coefficient of overconfidence variable (β4) is signifi-
heterogeneity that might exist among companies (Gujarati & cant, suggesting that investor overconfidence has an empirical
Porter, 2003), and it does not distinguish between the influence on the valuation of companies listed in the SSE. The
various companies included in our study. However, we model controls a number of other variables found to affect firm
decided the most appropriate model was the fixed-effects valuation in the literature.
model based on the results of the F-statistic (homogeneity
test, Wald test: P(F-statistic) < 0.05) and the Hausman test (χ 2 5. Results and discussion
test). Homogeneity test results provide (F-value = 62.25, p-
value = 0.0000) for our model, suggesting that the pooled This section presents the results and discussion. We discuss
regression model (pooled OLS) is not appropriate for our data descriptive statistics and correlation analysis, and then we
because there is a high heterogeneity among the companies present the results and discussion related to our hypothesis
included in our study. These results were expected as we are (H0).
working with different companies that are likely to have un-
observed differences. 5.1. Descriptive statistics and correlations
We used the Hausman test to determine which of the
random and fixed-effects models provides a consistent and This subsection describes the descriptive statistics and
unbiased estimator. A Hausman test can be used to determine pairwise correlation of the variables included in the current
whether unobserved heterogeneity is related to the explanatory study. Table 2 contains three panels: Panel A provides
variables. If the explanatory variables are correlated with the descriptive statistics for all variables used in this study,
individual effect, the fixed-effects model is the only approach including means, medians, first quartile (Q1), third quartile
that provides consistent estimators. The Hausman test provides (Q3), and standard deviations. Panel B provides pairwise cor-
a chi-square value with a corresponding p-value, which formed relations for variables in this study's main analysis, and Panel C
the basis of accepting or rejecting the null as appropriate. The provides pairwise correlations for all six investor over-
null hypothesis of the Hausman test is that the random effect confidence proxies used in this study. Over an 11-year period,
estimator is preferred and consistent. Based on the Hausman the study included 4004 quarterly observations from 91 com-
test results, χ 2 = 345.56, with a p-value of 0.0000 for our panies (2009–2019). Variables used in this study are Tobin's Q,
model, which rejects the null hypothesis and indicates that the market capitalization (MarketCap), return on assets (ROA),
random effects model is inconsistent and the fixed-effects leverage, and investor overconfidence proxies (Δtv, et, etd,
estimator is consistent and preferred. Based on homogeneity etm, turnover, and iso).
and the Hausman tests, we conclude that the fixed-effects The average Tobin's Q ratio was 2.34, which is consistent
model is the most appropriate and the only consistent model with recent studies that reported an average Tobin's Q ratio of
for our sample. Therefore, for panel data regression, we used approximately 2 (Adebambo & Yan, 2018; Villalonga &
the fixed-effects model to analyze our data. Amit, 2006). The mean market capitalization of the com-
R-squared is typically lower in cross-sectional data than in panies investigated was 7.88 million Saudi Riyal (SAR), with
time-series data due to the heterogeneity of cross-sections. The a median of 7.54 million Saudi Riyal (SAR). The sample firms
cross-sectional dimension in our data is larger than the time- had an average ROA of 3.37%, and the average leverage was
series dimension. Therefore, in panel data analysis, we rely 0.33. Finally, the main variable of overconfidence (Δtv) had a
more on the model's individual significance and overall sig- mean of 0.39 with a median of −0.03 and a standard deviation
nificance instead of R-squared. Also, the within R-squared of 2.48.
value is the main focus because a within-group fixed-effects In Table 2 Panel B, we examined the magnitude and di-
approach examines the deviations in means within a company. rection of linear associations between the variables used in the
Moreover, to address the issue of both heteroskedasticity and current study: the dependent, explanatory, and independent
serial correlation, we applied the “vce” (robust standard errors) variables using pairwise correlation analysis. The results of a
option. The model used to examine the effect of over- correlation analysis indicate whether or not there is a rela-
confidence on the valuation of companies listed on the Saudi tionship between the dependent variable and the independent
stock market (H0) can be written as follows: variables (Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2014). As shown in Panel B,
the main variable of overconfidence (Δtv) is positively corre-
Tobin′ sQit = α0 + β1 MarketCapit + β2 ROAit + β3 Leverageit lated with Tobin's Q, and this correlation is statistically sig-
nificant at the 1% level. These results provide initial evidence
+ β4 Overconfidenceit + 3 it
that investor overconfidence is strongly and positively associ-
(8) ated with market valuation expressed by Tobin's Q ratio. As
expected, the market valuation is positively and highly corre-
where α0 is the intercept term. β1, β2, β3, and β4 are the co- lated with market capitalization and ROA, while it is negatively
efficient of market capitalization, ROA, leverage, and over- correlated with leverage. Finally, the correlation between the
confidence, respectively. 3 it is the error term, i is a company, independent variables is low, indicating that there is no mul-
which sampled 91 companies listed at the SSE, and t represents ticollinearity problem.
103
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

Table 2
Descriptive statistics and correlations.
Panel A: Descriptive statistics
Variable Mean Q1 Median Q3 Std
Tobin′sQ 2.3426 1.22 1.77 2.7 2.3034
MarketCap (SAR Mn) 7.8752 6.7024 7.5459 8.9260 1.5413
ROA (%) 3.3741 0.402 2.7808 6.2468 10.7951
Leverage 0.3285 0.1428 0.3139 0.4865 0.2118
Δtv 0.3923 −0.3799 −0.0337 0.4964 2.4802
et 1.2278 0.2044 0.5327 1.2366 3.3768
etd 1.1756 0.1753 0.4959 1.2031 3.32
etm 1.3755 0.2338 0.6419 1.3899 3.6421
turnover 1.2744 0.1807 0.5217 1.4433 2.0406
iso 0.1345 0.0029 0.0248 0.1436 0.3148
Panel B: Pairwise correlations
Tobin′sQ MarketCap ROA Leverge
MarketCap 0.0512***
ROA 0.0621*** 0.1846***
Leverge −0.1863*** 0.1773*** −0.0342
Δtv 0.1194*** −0.0451*** −0.0133 −0.0568**
Panel C: Correlation of investor overconfidence proxies
Δtv et etd etm turnover
et 1.0000***
etd 1.0000*** 1.0000***
etm 1.0000*** 1.0000*** 1.0000***
turnover 0.1727*** 0.1300*** 0.1344*** 0.1391***
iso 0.5000*** 0.5348*** 0.5352*** 0.5802*** 0.0626***
Abbreviations: Std, standard deviation; Q1, first quartile; Q3, third quartile; Mn, Million.
*** and ** denote statistical significance at the 1% and 5% levels, respectively.

Table 2 Panel C shows the correlations among over- stock market valuation. The purpose of this study is to deter-
confidence proxies (Δtv, et, etd, etm, turnover, and iso). The mine whether investor overconfidence bias has an effect on the
correlation of all variables is positive and highly significant at valuation of companies listed on the Saudi stock market. The
the 1% level of significance, implying that the variables are regression analysis covers the entire 11 years period
good proxies for measuring overconfidence. Hence, we did not (2009Q1–2019Q4) for the 91 companies listed on the Saudi
exclude any proxy from further regression analysis for over- stock market. Table 3 reports the results of the main estimated
confidence robustness checks. The correlation of turnover with regression model used to test H0.
other overconfidence proxies is the lowest; however, it is As shown in Table 3, the F-value (F = 16.25, p-
highly significant at the 1% level. Since et, etd and etm are by value = 0.0000) suggests that this model is overall jointly
definition calculated based on Δtv, these variable proxies are significant at 1% level. (R-squared = 0.38) shows that 38% of
perfectly correlated. However, as mentioned earlier, we intro- the variation in firm valuation (represented by the company's
duced the variable proxies et, etd, and etm as different alter- Tobin's Q) within each firm is captured by our model regres-
native measures that might capture the expression of sion. It suggests that explanatory variables do explain 38% of
“excessive trading” more precisely. changes in valuation within each company over time. Indi-
vidually, overconfidence and market capitalization are statisti-
5.2. Empirical results and discussion related to each cally significant at the 5% and 1% levels, respectively, in
hypothesis explaining Tobin's Q. However, the other variables (leverage
and ROA) are statistically nonsignificant. We applied the “vce”
In this section, we examine our hypothesis H0. We present (robust) option to obtain robust standard errors for hetero-
the results, then discuss our results and compare them with the skedasticity and serial correlation.
findings of other studies. To identify the impact of over- Consistent with our previous prediction, we found that
confidence bias on the valuation of companies listed on the investor overconfidence (measured by the company's trading
Saudi stock market, we use the panel fixed-effects model as volume change Δtv) positively and significantly affects the
clarified in the methodology section. valuation of companies in the Saudi stock market with a co-
Behavioral theory predicts that investor overconfidence will efficient of 0.079 and p-value < 0.05. This value shows that
have a significant and positive effect on the valuation of holding other variables in the model constant, an increase in
companies listed on the Saudi stock market and, therefore, the investor overconfidence variable of one unit is associated
104
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

Table 3
Investor overconfidence and firm valuation.
H0: Overconfidence bias positively affects the valuation of companies listed in the Saudi stock market.
Tobin′sQit = α0 + β1MarketCapit + β2ROAit + β3Leverageit + β4Overconfidenceit + 3 it

Variable Coef SE t
Intercept −13.6974*** 2.3279 −5.88
MarketCap 1.9007*** 0.2876 6.61
ROA −0.0001 0.0006 −0.31
Leverage 0.5954 0.4889 1.22
Overconfidence 0.0790** 0.0363 2.18
F-value 16.25 *** χ2 345.56***
Abbreviations: Coef, coefficient; SE, standard errors; t, t-statistics.
***, **, and * denote statistical significance at the 1%, 5% and 10% levels, respectively.

with an increase in Tobin's Q ratio of 0.079 units. The positive institutional investors. According to survey and experimental
effect shows that investor overconfidence has a positive and studies, men are more overconfident than are women (Barber &
statistically significant effect on the valuation of Saudi com- Odean, 2001; Lundeberg et al., 1994; Prince, 1993; Prosad
panies. Market capitalization is positively and statistically et al., 2015). Overconfidence is higher among males and
significant at a 1% level. This means that if market capitali- young investors than among females and older investors
zation increases by 1%, then Tobin's Q ratio is expected to (Tekçe & Yılmaz, 2015). Therefore, the results of the current
increase by 0.019 units. study are consistent with the researchers’ prior expectations
Our results show that overconfidence bias causes the Saudi that investor overconfidence has a significant effect on the
stock market valuation to deviate from normal toward over- valuation of companies in the Saudi stock market.
valuation. No similar study currently exists in the published
Saudi-related studies. Several studies have found that over- 6. Robustness and sensitivity analysis
confidence bias has a significant and positive impact on in-
vestors' decision making (Chin, 2012; Khawaja, Bhutto, & For robustness and to ensure our results are not sensitive for
Naz, 2013; Qadri & Shabbir, 2014), which is likely to affect the variable proxy, we first experimented with a number of
stock market valuation. Our findings are supported by Brown alternative variable proxies for investor overconfidence. In
and Cliff (2005), who provided evidence that sentiment posi- subsection 6.1, we re-examined the hypothesis H0 using five
tively affects asset valuation. The findings in this study are also alternative investor overconfidence proxies. Second, as pre-
consistent with those of Adebambo and Yan (2018), who sented in subsection 6.2, to ensure our results are robust to
investigated the effect of investor overconfidence on firm endogeneity problems, we applied an alternative estimation of
valuation and found a strong positive relation between investor the dynamic panel data model (system GMM) by Arellano and
overconfidence and the valuation of the US firms. Their find- Bond (1991); Arellano and Bover (1995); Blundell and Bond
ings were also consistent with those of Bouteska and Regaieg (1998). Robustness checks confirmed our results and sug-
(2018), who found that the overconfidence variable positively gested our main findings are robust.
impacts Tobin's Q of US companies.
The results are consistent with our prior predictions that 6.1. Alternative overconfidence proxies
investor overconfidence significantly affects firm valuation in
the Saudi stock market for the following reasons. First, the In this subsection, we re-examined our hypothesis using five
Saudi-related literature suggests that Saudi investors are char- alternative variable proxies for investor overconfidence to
acterized by overconfidence, which has a significant positive ensure our results are not sensitive for the variable proxy and to
effect on investors' decision-making process in the Saudi stock check the robustness of our findings. In addition to the main
market (Alquraan et al., 2016; Matoussi & Mostafa, 2016), and variable proxy (Δtv), we considered five alternative investor
is consequently expected to affect stock market valuation. overconfidence proxies (et, etd, etm, turnover, and iso). As
Moreover, Alsabban and Alarfaj (2020) investigated over- discussed in subsection 3.2, our proxies of investor over-
confidence bias in the Saudi stock market and confirmed the confidence were derived from and supported by prior literature,
existence of overconfidence bias in the Saudi stock market. and, therefore, they are not simply picking up previously
Therefore, the Saudi-related literature supports the current documented results. We also show that the proxies used in this
study's findings that investor overconfidence significantly af- study are plausibly correlated with overconfidence bias.
fects Saudi stock market valuation. We conducted five fixed-effects panel models for which
Second, most traders in the Saudi stock market are in- each alternative proxy was included in the regression analysis
dividuals, mostly inexperienced males between 30 and 39 years one at a time to capture the effect of overconfidence on the
old (Alsedrah & Ahmed, 2017). According to Menkhoff et al. valuation of companies listed on the SSE, ensuring our results
(2013), individual investors are more overconfident than were not sensitive for the variable proxy and checking the
105
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

robustness of our findings. We decided the most appropriate alternative proxy variables are statistically significant. The
model to use was a fixed-effects model based on the results of significant and positive coefficients indicate that investor
the F-statistic (homogeneity test, Wald test: P(F-statistic) < overconfidence has a positive and statistically significant
0.05) and Hausman test (χ 2 test). Homogeneity test results for impact on the valuation of companies listed in the Saudi stock
alternative overconfidence proxies et, etd, etm, turnover, and market. iso is significant at the 1% level, while etm is signifi-
iso, respectively, were F-value = 23.86, p-value = 0.0000; F- cant at the 5% level and et, etd, and turnover are significant at
value = 28.19, p-value = 0.0000; F-value = 19.53, p- the 10% level. We applied the “vce” (robust standard errors)
value = 0.0000; F-value = 56.11, p-value = 0.0000; and F- option to address heteroskedasticity and serial correlation
value = 48.71, p-value = 0.0000. Results of the homogeneity issues.
test suggested that a pooled regression model (pooled OLS) is To summarize our findings on the robustness of alternative
not appropriate for our data because there is a high heteroge- investor overconfidence proxies, results for all proxies are
neity among the companies included in our study. These results statistically significant. Considering alternative proxies for
were expected as we are working with different companies that robustness, proxies for investor overconfidence generated
are likely to have unobserved differences. consistent results when used in our analysis. The findings of
We conducted the Hausman test to determine which model our analysis did not change when we used the alternative
would provide a consistent and unbiased estimator between overconfidence measures. Results are in line with those of the
random and fixed-effects models. The null hypothesis of the change in trading volume (Δtv). Having established robust
Hausman test is that the random effects estimator is preferred measures for investor overconfidence, we can confirm the
and consistent. Hausman test results for alternative over- positive and significant relation between investor over-
confidence proxies et, etd, etm, turnover, and iso, respectively, confidence and the valuation of companies listed on the SSE.
were χ 2 = 202.76, p-value = 0.0000; χ 2 = 235.29, p-
value = 0.0000; χ 2 = 166.84, p-value = 0.0000; χ 2 = 229.14, 6.2. Dynamic system generalized method of moments
p-value = 0.0000; and χ 2 = 316.90, p-value = 0.0000. (GMM) regression
Therefore, we rejected the null hypothesis indicating that the
random effects model is inconsistent and the fixed-effects With panel data, researchers commonly use a fixed-effects
estimator is consistent and preferred. Based on homogeneity estimator to address the concerns of omitted variables (Ding,
and Hausman tests, we concluded that fixed-effects models are Fan, & Lin, 2018; Gooris & Peeters, 2016; Li, Ding, Hu, &
the most appropriate and the only consistent approach for our Wan, 2021). However, some researchers have noted that firm
sample. Therefore, we used fixed-effects panel data regression and market valuation research results are plagued with endo-
to analyze all alternative overconfidence proxies models. geneity problems (Beiner, Drobetz, Schmid, & Zimmermann,
Table 4 displays regression results for overconfidence 2006; Caixe & Krauter, 2014; Chi, 2005; Palia, 2001). Dy-
robustness checks based on et, etd, etm, turnover, and iso, namic endogeneity is one type of endogeneity, and it occurs
controlled for a number of variables that prior research has when the past values of the dependent variables affect the in-
found to explain the valuation of companies. The table sum- dependent variables, violating a strict exogeneity assumption in
marizes results where the five alternative proxies were included fixed-effects estimators (Li et al., 2021; Nickell, 1981). Failing
in the regression analysis one at a time. Values for our five to adjust for endogeneity has consequences such as generating

Table 4
Alternative overconfidence proxies and firm valuation.
Robustness
Tobin′sQit = α0 + β1MarketCapit + β2ROAit + β3Leverageit + β4Overconfidenceit + 3 it

Variable et etd etm turnover iso


Intercept −15.5108*** −14.2262*** −16.4574*** −12.8988*** −13.4767***
(3.2922) (2.469635) (3.6828) (2.3517) (2.4689)
MarketCap 2.1044*** 1.9268*** 2.2090*** 1.7954*** 1.9000***
(0.4113) (0.3018) (0.4606) (0.2887) (0.3101)
ROA −0.0206 −0.0130 −0.0237 0.0002 −0.0003
(0.0215) (0.0194) (0.0257) (0.0007) (0.0006)
Leverage 1.0374* 1.0909** 1.1881* 0.5379 0.5427
(0.5652) (0.5219) (0.6417) (0.4777) (0.5386)
Overconfidence 0.0987* 0.0979* 0.1020** 0.1647* 2.2399***
(0.0506) (0.0505) (0.0501) (0.0939) (0.6054)
F-value 12.77*** 15.56*** 13.37*** 12.91*** 16.24***
χ2 202.76*** 235.29*** 166.84*** 229.14*** 316.90***
R2 0.41 0.45 0.41 0.34 0.40
No. of observations 692 720 593 1479 1280
***, **, and * denote statistical significance at the 1%, 5%, and 10% levels, respectively. Robust standard errors in parenthesis. These results were generated using
et, etd, etm, turnover, and iso in the regression analysis one at a time.

106
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

biased estimates and drawing the wrong inferences that lead to instrument subsets indicate that all instruments are jointly valid
invalid conclusions. In this model, firm valuation (represented and instrument subsets are uncorrelated with error terms,
by the firm's Tobin's Q) is dynamic, depending on its own respectively. The Hansen test produces J-statistics and follows
past realizations. Wilkins (2018) confirms that including the a chi-square distribution.
additional lags yields more accurate coefficient estimates; re- Second, the dynamic systems GMM estimation uses lags as
searchers should include lagged dependent variables as part of instruments, and therefore the Arellano-Bond test for autocor-
a robust estimation strategy, and such strategies are frequently relation, with a null hypothesis of no serial correlation, ascer-
used to eliminate autocorrelation in the residuals. tains whether the model includes sufficient lags to control for
This subsection presents a test of our hypothesis in which possible autocorrelation (Roodman, 2009; Wintoki, Linck, &
we applied a dynamic systems GMM estimation as an alter- Netter, 2012). The null hypothesis of no first-order serial cor-
native analysis to examine the robustness of our results. We relation in first differences (AR(1) test) can be rejected because,
employed the dynamic systems generalized method of mo- by construction, disturbances in first differences AR(1) can be
ments (dynamic systems GMM) estimation (Arellano & Bond, correlated. However, the null hypothesis of no second-order
1991; Arellano & Bover, 1995; Blundell & Bond, 1998) to serial correlation in first differences (AR(2) test) should not
control for the problems of endogeneity; hence, this study be rejected, indicating the validity of the historical firm valu-
provides unbiased estimates. Dynamic panel estimators are ation (Roodman, 2009).
designed for situations with few time periods and many in- Two-step estimation is argued to be asymptotically more
dividuals and are increasingly popular estimators (Roodman, efficient (Arellano & Bond, 1991; Blundell & Bond, 1998).
2009). We applied Stata software and, specifically, the xta- However, according to Windmeijer (2005), estimated asymp-
bond2 package Roodman (2009) to conduct our empirical totic standard errors of the efficient two-step GMM estimator
analysis. can be severely downwardly biased in small samples. In this
Many researchers argue that dynamic systems GMM pro- study, we used the xtabond2 package that applies a finite-
vides efficient estimates because it exploits all the available sample correction to the two-step covariance matrix derived
information in a sample (Arellano & Bond, 1991; Arellano & by Windmeijer (2005), leading to more accurate inference. We
Bover, 1995; Blundell & Bond, 1998). Blundell, Bond, and included time dummies, making no correlation across in-
Windmeijer (2001) theoretically and experimentally dividuals in the idiosyncratic disturbances assumption more
confirmed that the use of the systems GMM estimator greatly likely to hold. We also applied a robust option to correct for
improves precision and reduces finite-sample bias. An essential heteroscedastic and autocorrelation. We retested our hypothesis
property of dynamic systems GMM is internal instruments H0 applying a dynamic systems GMM estimation as an alter-
embedded in the present dataset and available to the re- native analysis to examine the robustness of our results. The
searchers (Schultz, Tan, & Walsh, 2010). Only a few studies dynamic systems GMM used to re-examine the effect of
have examined the impact of investor overconfidence on firm overconfidence on the valuation of companies listed on the
valuation, and those have not applied a dynamic panel model Saudi stock market is stated as follows:
and, therefore, failed to control for dynamic endogeneity.
These studies focused on the US stock market, ignoring ΔTobin′ sQit =α0 + L.ΔTobin′ sQδit
emerging stock markets. This study intends to fill this gap, + β1 ΔMarketCapit + β2 ΔROAit
control for these endogeneity issues, and focus on the emerging (9)
+ β3 ΔLeverageit
market of the Saudi stock market.
+ β4 ΔOverconfidenceit + Δ3 it
6.2.1. Dynamic system GMM results
Equation (9) shows the impact of lag valuation proxy changes
This subsection presents and discusses the results from (L.ΔTobin′sQδit), market capitalization (ΔMarketCapit), ROA
dynamic systems GMM estimation. We will show how fixed- (ΔROAit), leverage (ΔLeverageit), and overconfidence
effects estimator provide different regression results (ΔOverconfidenceit) on changes in firm valuation
compared to GMM estimator. An exogeneity assumption in the (ΔTobin′sQit). Where the L represents a lag operator of the
dynamic systems GMM model suggests that historical firm valuation proxy (Tobin′sQ). Δ denotes the difference.δ is the
valuation and explanatory variables are exogenous concerning lag time coefficient for differenced firm valuation proxy
error terms. This means the instruments must be exogenous for (LΔTobin′sQ), across N observations. The system GMM im-
the GMM model to be valid. To verify this assumption, two proves on the moment conditions using instruments with lag-
fundamental tests should be conducted (Arellano & Bond, ged variations of Tobin's Q and the independent variables.
1991; Blundell & Bond, 1998; Roodman, 2009). The first Thus, the system GMM produces efficient and consistent co-
test, the Hansen test for joint validity of all instruments, as- efficients for the independent variables which are not affected
certains whether or not the model is overidentified, in addition by dynamic endogeneity and unobserved heterogeneity.
to the difference-in-Hansen test for the exogeneity of IV and Table 5 shows the regression results of the dynamic systems
GMM instrument subsets (Roodman, 2009; Schultz et al., GMM equation for overconfidence robustness checks based on
2010). Accepting the null hypothesis, the Hansen test for Δtv, et, etd, etm, turnover, and iso, which controls for a number
joint validity and the difference-in-Hansen test of exogeneity of of variables that prior research has found to explain the

107
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

Table 5
Dynamic system GMM results.
Robustness
ΔTobin′sQit = α0 + L.ΔTobin′sQδit + β1ΔMarketCapit + β2ΔROAit + β3ΔLeverageit + β4ΔOverconfidenceit + Δ3 it
Variable Δtv et etd etm turnover iso
Intercept −0.6849 −0.3891 −0.4254* −0.3692 −1.8668** −0.2574
(0.2457) (0.3499) (0.2340) (0.3295) (0.9362) (0.1687)
L.Tobin′sQ 0.7786*** 0.7523*** 0.7225*** 0.8115*** 0.6331*** 0.7802***
(0.0921) (0.0882) (0.0569) (0.0764) (0.1303) (0.0761)
MarketCap 0.0583* 0.0216 0.0225 0.0033 0.2834** 0.0491
(0.0340) (0.0234) (0.0137) (0.0165) (0.1232) (0.0303)
ROA 0.0008 0.0174 0.0234 0.0089 0.0003 0.0009
(0.0023) (0.0222) (0.0179) (0.0190) (0.0025) (0.0023)
Leverage −0.1562 −0.1491 −0.1268 −0.0529 −0.3987 −0.1780
(0.1922) (0.1606) (0.1082) (0.1243) (0.3134) (0.1624)
Overconfidence 0.1327*** 0.1087** 0.1219*** 0.1102** 0.8431** 3.7856***
(0.0297) (0.0459) (0.0381) (0.0467) (0.4229) (0.2033)
Year dummies Included Included Included Included Included Included
F-value 100.98*** 448.80*** 685.10*** 440.70*** 100.39*** 673.21***
AR(1) test (p-value) 0.037 0.274 0.118 0.078 0.234 0.024
AR(2) test (p-value) 0.296 0.478 0.227 0.433 0.987 0.207
Hansen test of overidentification (p-value) 0.125 0.426 0.773 0.879 0.547 0.230
Diff-in-Hansen tests of exogeneity (p-value) 0.248 0.497 0.754 0.868 0.523 0.206
No. of observations 1474 690 718 590 1474 1275
***, **, and * denote statistical significance at the 1%, 5%, and 10% levels, respectively. Robust standard errors are in parentheses (). Each column displays
regression results of a dynamic systems GMM equation where the six alternative proxies of overconfidence (Δtv, et, etd, etm, turnover, and iso) are included in the
regression analysis one at a time. The Arellano-Bond test statistic, AR(1) and AR(2), follow an asymptotic normal distribution, with null (H0): No autocorrelation in
the differenced errors. The residual values in first differences AR(1) can be correlated by construction; however, there should not be a serial correlation in the second
difference AR (2). The Hanson test of overidentification (J-Statistic) follows a chi-square distribution; with a null (H0) = the instruments as a group are exogenous
and specified correctly, which means that instruments in the dynamic system GMM are valid. Difference-in-Hansen tests of exogeneity have a null
(H0) = instruments in the systems GMM equation are exogenous.

valuation of a company. Each column displays regression re- serial correlation AR(1), but there should be no significant
sults of the dynamic systems GMM equation where each of the serial correlation in the second order AR(2). Therefore, the
six alternative proxies of overconfidence was included in the diagnostic tests support the estimation choices made.
regression analysis one at a time. The F-values across all Overconfidence is the main variable of interest and is sig-
equations indicate that the dynamic systems GMM results are nificant across all models at the 1% and 5% levels. The dy-
statistically significant at 1%. The Hansen tests and the namic systems GMM model yielded larger coefficients with
Arrelano–Bond are standard tests to be sure that estimates are lower p-values of less than 5% level for overconfidence proxies
unbiased and consistent. compared to the fixed-effects approach, implying a stronger
The first is the Hansen test of overidentification with the null relationship and higher significance levels. The dynamic sys-
hypothesis of joint validity of all instruments. As reported in tems GMM results are consistent with the main results reported
the results across all equations, the Hansen–J test yielded a p- for the analysis of the fixed-effects estimation.
value between 0.125 and 0.879 for the Δtv and etm proxies, As previously stated, this study included two major robust-
respectively, suggesting that our instruments are valid at all ness and sensitivity analyses. First, we used five different over-
conventional levels of significance. We also followed the confidence proxies to ensure that our results were not affected by
recommendation of Roodman (2009) about good practices in the variable proxy. Second, we used dynamic systems GMM
applying the difference-in-Hansen test for the exogeneity of regression analyses as an alternative estimation method to ensure
instrument subsets. For all six models, p-values imply that that our results were robust to endogeneity issues. Overall,
instrument subsets are exogenous and valid. robustness checks confirm our results and suggest our main
The second is the Arrelano–Bond test for second-order se- findings are robust. The significant positive relationship between
rial correlation AR(2), with null hypothesis that there is no the valuation of listed companies in the Saudi stock market and
serial correlation. The test results across the six dynamic sys- all overconfidence proxy variables in both the main and alter-
tems GMM equations show that there is no serial correlation, native regression analyses support our hypothesis.
indicating dynamic systems GMM estimates are valid and
models contain sufficient lag instruments to control for auto- 7. Conclusion
correlation. According to prior research (e.g., Areneke and
Kimani (2019); Blundell and Bond (1998); Esho, Kirievsky, Empirical evidence suggests that investors in the stock
Ward, and Zurbruegg (2004); Flannery and Hankins (2013)) market are affected by overconfidence bias, and such a bias
in a GMM estimation, there can exist a significant first-order has been neglected by traditional financial theory, which relies

108
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

on the rationality assumption. Behavioral biases may affect collectivist society that is a member of both MENA and GCC
investor decisions and consequently affect stock markets. In stock markets. The findings also support behavioral theories that
this paper, we examine the impact of investor overconfidence predict that investor psychology affects asset pricing and, there-
on the valuation of companies listed on the SSE. Theories fore, market valuation, suggesting that the role of investor
predict that investor overconfidence leads to firm overvaluation behavior should be considered in asset pricing models.
(Adebambo & Yan, 2018). Based on theories of behavioral Furthermore, the current study should help stock market
finance, overconfident investors expected to have an irratio- investors become more aware of the impact of their own psy-
nally high demand for risky assets because they tend to un- chological factors, specifically overconfidence bias, on asset
derestimate risk and overestimate the quality and precision of pricing and market valuation, thereby increasing the rationality
their own information, requiring a lower risk premium; this of their stock market decision making for improved market
results in a higher price for risky equities, eventually leading to efficiency. Further interventions may be needed to raise in-
overvaluation. vestors' awareness about their behavioral biases, helping them
We tested this prediction using a main variable proxy of make better use of information sources when making invest-
overconfidence (Δtv) and applying a fixed-effects panel model ment decisions. Furthermore, our results show that this
as the main estimation. Additionally, to check the robustness of cognitive bias can cause firm valuation in the Saudi stock
our findings, we performed two robustness and sensitivity an- market to deviate from normal toward overvaluation. However,
alyses in this study. First, we used five alternative proxies to price bubbles arise when asset prices exceed the asset's
measure investor overconfidence and to ensure our results were fundamental value (Werner, 2014). Bubbles are associated with
not sensitive for the variable proxy. Second, we conducted the asset price increases (Brunnermeier, 2016). Famous examples
dynamic systems GMM estimation as an alternative estimation include the Dutch tulip mania (1634–1637), the Mississippi
approach to ensure our results were robust to the problems of bubble (1719–1720) and the South Sea bubble (1720). There-
endogeneity. Consistent with our prediction, we found that fore, market regulators should be aware of the influence that
investor overconfidence positively and significantly affects the investor behavior can have on market bubbles and, conse-
valuation of companies in the Saudi stock market. Robustness quently, economic activity.
checks confirm our results and suggest that our main findings Additionally, market valuation is a key concept in finance,
are robust. Regarding our findings on the robustness of alter- which has significant implications for market efficiency, capital
native investor overconfidence proxies, all proxies were sta- allocation, and investment expenditures. According to studies
tistically significant and generated consistent results when used by Khanna and Sonti (2004), Zaigham et al. (2019), and
in our analysis. Results are in line with those of the main Blanchard et al. (1993), an increase in market valuation results
variable proxy of overconfidence (Δtv). The dynamic systems in an increase in a company's capital expenditure activity. As a
GMM results are consistent with the results reported for the result, investment expenditures are reduced when a company's
main analysis of the fixed-effects estimation. Having estab- market value declines (Asker et al., 2015; Del Brio et al., 2003;
lished robust measures for investor overconfidence using both Dessaint et al., 2019; Kouser et al., 2016). Significant evidence
main and alternative regression analyses, we confirm the pos- of linkages between market valuation, capital market effi-
itive and significant relation between investor overconfidence ciency, and capital allocation can be found in Baker (2009).
and the valuation of companies listed on the SSE. A possible limitation in this study includes data availability.
Given the fact that the Saudi stock market is the largest in terms Due to the data availability and special conditions of Saudi
of market capitalization and the most liquid in the Arab world Arabia's security market, we could not use more proxies of
(Mensi, 2019), the literature analysis revealed that the emerging investor overconfidence. For example, we could not use ana-
stock market of Saudi Arabia requires further investigation. None lyst's earnings forecast deviation proxy because we could not
of the previous studies considered the influence of investor find data for profit (earning) level predicted by the manager or
overconfidence on the valuation of companies in the Saudi stock the financial analyst. This proxy method refers to the com-
market. The Saudi-related literature has focused on finding evi- parison between the profit (earning) level predicted by the
dence for the existence of behavioral biases for investors in the manager or the financial analyst and the actual (earning) profit
Saudi stock market in an attempt to identify which biases influ- level. This kind of cognitive deviation can be used to determine
ence investors’ decisions while also ignoring the consequent in- if a manager or an investor is overconfident. The greater the
fluence of these biases on the Saudi stock market. In contrast with gap between the predicted situation and the actual situation, the
the earlier studies reviewed in this paper, this research is the first higher the degree of managerial overconfidence and investor
to examine the impact of investor overconfidence on the valuation overconfidence (Lin, Hu, & Chen, 2005).
of companies in the Saudi stock market. Investor biases may affect investor decisions and conse-
The findings have implications for academic researchers, quently affect stock markets. Especially that an emerging
market investors, and market regulators, as well as implications markets are more prone to behavioral biases, such as over-
for investment expenditures, capital allocation, and market effi- confidence, generally to a higher extent, compared to the
ciency. Our findings may also help provide an explanation for developed markets (Chen et al., 2007; Tekçe, Yılmaz, &
mispricing and overvaluation in emerging markets with collec- Bildik, 2016). However, few studies have examined the in-
tivist societies and on the MENA and the GCC stock markets fluence of investor overconfidence on firm and stock market
because Saudi Arabia's stock market is an emerging market with a valuation and mainly focus on the United States, ignoring
109
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

emerging stock markets. More research is needed to investigate Arellano, M., & Bond, S. (1991). Some tests of specification for panel data:
the impact of investor biases on their decisions in emerging Monte Carlo evidence and an application to employment equations. The
Review of Economic Studies, 58, 277–297.
markets, particularly the impact of these biases on emerging Arellano, M., & Bover, O. (1995). Another look at the instrumental variable
stock markets such as the Saudi stock market. We focused on estimation of error-components models. Journal of Econometrics, 68, 29–51.
the Saudi stock market in this paper because it is an emerging Areneke, G., & Kimani, D. (2019). Value relevance of multinational direc-
market with a collectivist culture. Future researchers could torship and cross-listing on MNEs national governance disclosure practices
propose a study comparing emerging stock markets with in Sub-Saharan Africa: Evidence from Nigeria. Journal of World Business,
54, 285–306.
collectivist societies versus developed stock markets with Asaad, C. T. (2020). Investor confidence: Are you your own worst enemy?
individualistic societies. They could investigate how over- Financial Planning Review, 3, Article e1092.
confidence bias influences stock markets in a society-specific Asker, J., Farre-Mensa, J., & Ljungqvist, A. (2015). Corporate investment and
context and, therefore, how society-specific characteristics stock market listing: A puzzle? Review of Financial Studies, 28, 342–390.
may affect such a relationship. Bailey, W., Kumar, A., & Ng, D. (2011). Behavioral biases of mutual fund
investors. Journal of Financial Economics, 102, 1–27.
Bakar, S., & Yi, A. N. C. (2016). The impact of psychological factors on in-
Declaration of competing interest: vestors' decision making in Malaysian stock market: A case of Klang valley
and Pahang. Procedia Economics and Finance, 35, 319–328.
None. Baker, M. (2009). Capital market-driven corporate finance. Annu. Rev. Financ.
Econ., 1, 181–205.
Baker, H. K., & Nofsinger, J. R. (2002). Psychological biases of investors.
Acknowledgments
Financial Services Review, 11, 97.
Baker, M., & Wurgler, J. (2002). Market timing and capital structure. The
This research did not receive any specific grant from Journal of Finance, 57, 1–32.
funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit Baker, M., & Wurgler, J. (2007). Investor sentiment in the stock market. The
sectors. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21, 129–152.
Baker, M., & Wurgler, J. (2013). Behavioral corporate finance: An updated
survey. In Handbook of the Economics of Finance (Volume 2, pp.
References 357–424). Elsevier.
Barberis, N., & Thaler, R. (2005). A survey of behavioral finance.
Abdalla, S. Z. S., & Suliman, Z. (2012). Modelling stock returns volatility: Barber, B. M., & Odean, T. (1999). The courage of misguided convictions.
Empirical evidence from saudi stock exchange. International Research Financial Analysts Journal, 55, 41–55.
Journal of Finance and Economics, 85, 166–179. Barber, B. M., & Odean, T. (2000). Trading is hazardous to your wealth: The
Abdeldayem, M. M., & Mahmoud, M. R. (2013). An examination into the impact common stock investment performance of individual investors. The Journal
of trading motives on the dynamic relationship between stock returns and of Finance, 55, 773–806.
trading volume: Evidence from Egypt. Global Advanced Research Journal of Barber, B. M., & Odean, T. (2001). Boys will be boys: Gender, overconfidence, and
Management and Business Studies (GARJMBS) ISSN, 2315–5086. common stock investment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, 261–292.
Abdulsamad, A. O., Yusoff, W. F. W., & Lasyoud, A. A. (2018). The influence Barber, B. M., & Odean, T. (2002). Online investors: Do the slow die first?
of the board of directors' characteristics on firm performance: Evidence Review of Financial Studies, 15, 455–488.
from malaysian public listed companies. Corporate Governance and Sus- Beiner, S., Drobetz, W., Schmid, M. M., & Zimmermann, H. (2006). An in-
tainability Review, 2, 6–13. tegrated framework of corporate governance and firm valuation. European
Abed, S., Al-Attar, A., & Suwaidan, M. (2012). Corporate governance and Financial Management, 12, 249–283.
earnings management: Jordanian evidence. International Business Research, Benos, A. V. (1998). Aggressiveness and survival of overconfident traders.
5, 216. Journal of Financial Markets, 1, 353–383.
Adebambo, B. N., & Yan, X. (2018). Investor overconfidence, firm valuation, Blanchard, O., Rhee, C., & Summers, L. (1993). The stock market, profit, and
and corporate decisions. Management Science, 64, 5349–5369. investment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 108, 115–136.
Al-Najjar, B. (2011). The inter-relationship between capital structure and div- Blundell, R., & Bond, S. (1998). Initial conditions and moment restrictions in
idend policy: Empirical evidence from Jordanian data. International Review dynamic panel data models. Journal of Econometrics, 87, 115–143.
of Applied Economics, 25, 209–224. Blundell, R., Bond, S., & Windmeijer, F. (2001). Estimation in dynamic panel
Al-Najjar, B., & Anfimiadou, A. (2012). Environmental policies and firm data models: Improving on the performance of the standard GMM esti-
value. Business Strategy and the Environment, 21, 49–59. mator. Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
Aljloud, S. A. (2016). The law of market manipulation in Saudi Arabia: A case Bollerslev, T., Gibson, M., & Zhou, H. (2011). Dynamic estimation of vola-
for reform. Ph.D. thesis. Brunel University London. tility risk premia and investor risk aversion from option-implied and real-
Alnajjar, M. I. (2013). Behavioral inferences of Tadawul investor. International ized volatilities. Journal of Econometrics, 160, 235–245.
Journal of Business and Management, 8, 17. Bouteska, A., & Regaieg, B. (2018). Loss aversion, overconfidence of investors
Alquraan, T., Alqisie, A., & Al Shorafa, A. (2016). Do behavioral finance and their impact on market performance evidence from the US stock
factors influence stock investment decisions of individual invest- markets. Journal of Economics, Finance and Administrative Science, 25,
ors?(evidences from Saudi stock market). American International Journal 451–478.
of Contemporary Research, 6, 159–169. Bradford, J., De Long, A. S., Summers, L. H., & Waldmann, R. J. (1991). The
Alrabadi, D. W. H., Al-Abdallah, S. Y., & Aljarayesh, N. I. A. (2018). Behav- survival of noise traders in financial markets.
ioral biases and investment performance: Does gender matter? Evidence from Brainard, W. C., & Tobin, J. (1968). Pitfalls in financial model building. The
Amman stock exchange. Jordan Journal of Economic Sciences, 5, 77–92. American Economic Review, 58, 99–122.
Alsabban, S., & Alarfaj, O. (2020). An empirical analysis of behavioral finance Brown, G. W., & Cliff, M. T. (2005). Investor sentiment and asset valuation.
in the Saudi stock market: Evidence of overconfidence behavior. Interna- Journal of Business, 78, 405–440.
tional Journal of Economics and Financial Issues, 10, 73. Brunnermeier, M. K. (2016). Bubbles. In Banking crises (pp. 28–36). Springer.
Alsedrah, I., & Ahmed, N. (2017). Behavioral finance and speculative behavior Buchanan, B., Cao, C. X., & Chen, C. (2018). Corporate social responsibility,
of investors: Evidence from Saudi stock market. The European Pro- firm value, and influential institutional ownership. Journal of Corporate
ceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences, 44, 604–618. Finance, 52, 73–95.

110
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

Burmeister, E., Roll, R., & Ross, S. A. (1994). A practitioner's guide to arbi- Flannery, M. J., & Hankins, K. W. (2013). Estimating dynamic panel models in
trage pricing theory. Finanzmarkt und Portfolio Management, 8, 312–331. corporate finance. Journal of Corporate Finance, 19, 1–19.
Caixe, D. F., & Krauter, E. (2014). The relation between corporate governance Gasteren, L.v. (2016). The effect of overconfidence on stock market bubbles,
and market value: Mitigating endogeneity problems. Brazilian Business velocity and volatility.
Review, 11, 90–110. Gervais, S., Heaton, J., & Odean, T. (2007). Overconfidence, investment policy,
Chandra, A. (2008). Decision making in the stock market: Incorporating psy- and manager welfare. Fuqua School of Business Working Paper, 1–46.
chology with finance. In National conference on forecasting financial Gervais, S., & Odean, T. (2001). Learning to be overconfident. Review of
markets of India. Financial Studies, 14, 1–27.
Chen, G., Kim, K. A., Nofsinger, J. R., & Rui, O. M. (2007). Trading per- Gilchrist, S., Himmelberg, C. P., & Huberman, G. (2005). Do stock price
formance, disposition effect, overconfidence, representativeness bias, and bubbles influence corporate investment? Journal of Monetary Economics,
experience of emerging market investors. Journal of Behavioral Decision 52, 805–827.
Making, 20, 425–451. Glaser, M., & Weber, M. (2007). Overconfidence and trading volume. The
Chen, R. C., & Lee, C. H. (2017). The influence of CSR on firm value: An Geneva Risk and Insurance Review, 32, 1–36.
application of panel smooth transition regression on Taiwan. Applied Gompers, P., Ishii, J., & Metrick, A. (2003). Corporate governance and equity
Economics, 49, 3422–3434. prices. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 107–156.
Chi, J. (2005). Understanding the endogeneity between firm value and share- Goodwin, P., & Wright, G. (2014). Decision analysis for management judg-
holder rights. Financial Management, 34, 65–76. ment. John Wiley & Sons.
Chin, A. L. L. (2012). Psychological biases and investor behaviour: Survey Gooris, J., & Peeters, C. (2016). Fragmenting global business processes: A
evidence from Malaysian stock market. International Journal on Social protection for proprietary information. Journal of International Business
Science, Economics and Art, 2. Studies, 47, 535–562.
Chuang, W. I., & Lee, B. S. (2006). An empirical evaluation of the over- Griffin, J. M., Nardari, F., & Stulz, R. M. (2007). Do investors trade more when
confidence hypothesis. Journal of Banking & Finance, 30, 2489–2515. stocks have performed well? Evidence from 46 countries. Review of
Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2014). Applied multiple regression/ Financial Studies, 20, 905–951.
correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Psychology Press. Grinblatt, M., & Keloharju, M. (2009). Sensation seeking, overconfidence, and
Corredor, P., Ferrer, E., & Santamaria, R. (2013). Investor sentiment effect in trading activity. The Journal of Finance, 64, 549–578.
stock markets: Stock characteristics or country-specific factors? Interna- Gujarati, D. N., & Porter, D. C. (2003). Panel data regression models. Basic
tional Review of Economics & Finance, 27, 572–591. Econometrics, 4.
Daines, R. (2001). Does Delaware law improve firm value? Journal of Hassan, O. A. (2018). The impact of voluntary environmental disclosure on
Financial Economics, 62, 525–558. firm value: Does organizational visibility play a mediation role? Business
Daniel, K., & Hirshleifer, D. (2015). Overconfident investors, predictable Strategy and the Environment, 27, 1569–1582.
returns, and excessive trading. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 29, Heger, S. A., & Papageorge, N. W. (2018). We should totally open a restaurant:
61–88. How optimism and overconfidence affect beliefs. Journal of Economic
Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D., & Subrahmanyam, A. (2005). Investor psychology Psychology, 67, 177–190.
and security market under-and overreaction. Advances in Behavioral Hirshleifer, D., & Luo, G. Y. (2001). On the survival of overconfident traders in
Finance II, 460–501. a competitive securities market. Journal of Financial Markets, 4, 73–84.
De Bondt, W. F., & Thaler, R. (1985). Does the stock market overreact? The Ho, C. M. (2011). Does overconfidence harm individual investors? An
Journal of Finance, 40, 793–805. empirical analysis of the Taiwanese market. Asia-Pacific Journal of
Deaves, R., Lüders, E., & Luo, G. Y. (2009). An experimental test of the Financial Studies, 40, 658–682.
impact of overconfidence and gender on trading activity. Review of Hong, H., Scheinkman, J., & Xiong, W. (2006). Asset float and speculative
Finance, 13, 555–575. bubbles. The Journal of Finance, 61, 1073–1117.
Del Brio, E., De Miguel, A., & Pindado, J. (2003). Investment and firm value: Jain, R., Jain, P., & Jain, C. (2015). Behavioral biases in the decision making of
An analysis using panel data. Applied Financial Economics, 13, 913–923. individual investors. The IUP Journal of Management Research, 14, 7.
Dessaint, O., Foucault, T., Frésard, L., & Matray, A. (2019). Noisy stock prices Jiao, Y. (2010). Stakeholder welfare and firm value. Journal of Banking &
and corporate investment. Review of Financial Studies, 32, 2625–2672. Finance, 34, 2549–2561.
Ding, H., Fan, H., & Lin, S. (2018). Connect to trade. Journal of International Jiraporn, P., Singh, M., & Lee, C. I. (2009). Ineffective corporate governance:
Economics, 110, 50–62. Director busyness and board committee memberships. Journal of Banking
El Ghoul, S., Guedhami, O., & Kim, Y. (2017). Country-level institutions, firm & Finance, 33, 819–828.
value, and the role of corporate social responsibility initiatives. Journal of Jo, H., & Harjoto, M. A. (2011). Corporate governance and firm value: The impact
International Business Studies, 48, 360–385. of corporate social responsibility. Journal of Business Ethics, 103, 351–383.
Esho, N., Kirievsky, A., Ward, D., & Zurbruegg, R. (2004). Law and the de- Khanna, N., & Sonti, R. (2004). Value creating stock manipulation: Feedback
terminants of property-casualty insurance. Journal of Risk & Insurance, 71, effect of stock prices on firm value. Journal of Financial Markets, 7,
265–283. 237–270.
Estrin, S., Hanousek, J., Kocenda, E., & Svejnar, J. (2009). The effects of Khawaja, H. A., Bhutto, N. A., & Naz, S. (2013). Investors' behavioral biases
privatization and ownership in transition economies. Journal of Economic and the stock market development: An empirical study of the Pakistani
Literature, 47, 699–728. stock market. Journal of Financial Economics, 84, 529–560.
Fama, E. F. (2021a). Efficient capital markets a review of theory and empirical Korniotis, G. M., & Kumar, A. (2008). Superior information or a psycholog-
work (pp. 76–121). The Fama Portfolio. ical bias? A unified framework with cognitive abilities resolves three puz-
Fama, E. F. (2021b). Market efficiency, long-term returns, and behavioral zles. Unpublished Manuscript: University of Texas.
finance. University of Chicago Press. Kouser, R., Saba, I., & Anjum, F. (2016). Impact of asymmetric information on
Fama, E. F., & French, K. R. (2015). A five-factor asset pricing model. Journal the investment sensitivity to stock price and the stock price sensitivity to
of Financial Economics, 116, 1–22. investment. Journal of Accounting and Finance in Emerging Economies, 2,
Fama, E. F., & French, K. R. (2021). The cross-section of expected stock 1–16.
returns. University of Chicago Press. Kumar, N., & Singh, J. (2013). Effect of board size and promoter ownership on
Fang, V. W., Noe, T. H., & Tice, S. (2009). Stock market liquidity and firm firm value: Some empirical findings from India. Corporate Governance:
value. Journal of Financial Economics, 94, 150–169. The International Journal of Business in Society, 13, 88–98.
Feng, Z., & Wu, Z. (2018). Technology investment, firm performance and Kunjal, D., & Peerbhai, F. (2021). Investor overconfidence in the south african
market value: Evidence from banks. In Community banking in the 21st exchange traded fund market. Cogent Economics & Finance, 9, Article
century research and policy conference. 1978190.

111
R. Aljifri _
Borsa Istanbul Review 23-1 (2023) 93–112

Kyle, A. S., & Wang, F. A. (1997). Speculation duopoly with agreement to Roodman, D. (2009). How to do xtabond2: An introduction to difference and
disagree: Can overconfidence survive the market test? The Journal of system GMM in Stata. The Stata Journal, 9, 86–136.
Finance, 52, 2073–2090. Scheinkman, J. A., & Xiong, W. (2003). Overconfidence and speculative
Lambert, J., Bessière, V., & N'Goala, G. (2012). Does expertise influence the bubbles. Journal of Political Economy, 111, 1183–1220.
impact of overconfidence on judgment, valuation and investment decision? Schultz, E. L., Tan, D. T., & Walsh, K. D. (2010). Endogeneity and the
Journal of Economic Psychology, 33, 1115–1128. corporate governance-performance relation. Australian Journal of Man-
Li, J., Ding, H., Hu, Y., & Wan, G. (2021). Dealing with dynamic endogeneity agement, 35, 145–163.
in international business research. Sewell, M. (2010). Behavioural finance (Vol. 13). University of Cambridge.
Lin, Y.h., Hu, S.y., & Chen, M.s. (2005). Managerial optimism and corporate Shefrin, H. (2002). Beyond greed and fear: Understanding behavioral finance
investment: Some empirical evidence from taiwan. Pacific-Basin Finance and the psychology of investing. Oxford University Press on Demand.
Journal, 13, 523–546. Shiller, R. J. (1987). Investor behavior in the October 1987 stock market crash:
Liu, M., & Magnan, M. (2011). Self-dealing regulations, ownership wedge, and Survey evidence.
corporate valuation: International evidence. Corporate Governance: An Siegel, J. J. (1992). Equity risk premia, corporate profit forecasts, and investor
International Review, 19, 99–115. sentiment around the stock crash of October 1987. Journal of Business,
Lundeberg, M. A., Fox, P. W., & Punćcohaŕ, J. (1994). Highly confident but 557–570.
wrong: Gender differences and similarities in confidence judgments. StataCorp. (2019). Stata statistical software: Release 16. College station, TX:
Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 114. StataCrop LLC.
Malik, F., & Hammoudeh, S. (2007). Shock and volatility transmission in the Statman, M., Thorley, S., & Vorkink, K. (2006). Investor overconfidence and
oil, US and Gulf equity markets. International Review of Economics & trading volume. Review of Financial Studies, 19, 1531–1565.
Finance, 16, 357–368. Stracca, L. (2004). Behavioral finance and asset prices: Where do we stand?
Malmendier, U., & Tate, G. (2005). Ceo overconfidence and corporate in- Journal of Economic Psychology, 25, 373–405.
vestment. The Journal of Finance, 60, 2661–2700. Strahilevitz, M. A., Odean, T., & Barber, B. M. (2011). Once burned, twice
Matoussi, H., & Mostafa, M. A. (2016). An investigation of investors bias on shy: How naive learning, counterfactuals, and regret affect the repurch-
Saudi stock exchange. International Research Journal of Finance and ase of stocks previously sold. Journal of Marketing Research, 48,
Economics, 66–81. S102–S120.
Menkhoff, L., Schmeling, M., & Schmidt, U. (2013). Overconfidence, expe- Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psy-
rience, and professionalism: An experimental study. Journal of Economic chological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193.
Behavior & Organization, 86, 92–101. Tekçe, B., & Yılmaz, N. (2015). Are individual stock investors overconfident?
Mensi, W. (2019). Global financial crisis and co-movements between oil prices Evidence from an emerging market. Journal of Behavioral and Experi-
and sector stock markets in Saudi Arabia: A VaR based wavelet. Borsa mental Finance, 5, 35–45.
Istanbul Review, 19, 24–38. Tekçe, B., Yılmaz, N., & Bildik, R. (2016). What factors affect behavioral
Metwally, A. H., & Darwish, O. (2015). Evidence of the overconfidence bias in biases? Evidence from Turkish individual stock investors. Research in
the Egyptian stock market in different market states. International Journal International Business and Finance, 37, 515–526.
of Business and Economic Development (IJBED), 3. Trejos, C., van Deemen, A., Rodríguez, Y. E., & Gómez, J. M. (2019).
Michailova, J. (2010). Overconfidence and bubbles in experimental asset Overconfidence and disposition effect in the stock market: A micro world
markets. based setting. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 21, 61–69.
Nickell, S. (1981). Biases in dynamic models with fixed effects. Econometrica: Trinugroho, I., & Sembel, R. (2011). Overconfidence and excessive trading
Journal of the Econometric Society, 1417–1426. behavior: An experimental study. International Journal of Business and
Odean, T. (1998). Volume, volatility, price, and profit when all traders are Management, 6, 147.
above average. The Journal of Finance, 53, 1887–1934. Villalonga, B., & Amit, R. (2006). How do family ownership, control and man-
Odean, T. (1999). Do investors trade too much? The American Economic agement affect firm value? Journal of Financial Economics, 80, 385–417.
Review, 89, 1279–1298. Werner, J. (2014). Rational asset pricing bubbles and debt constraints. Journal
Palia, D. (2001). The endogeneity of managerial compensation in firm valua- of Mathematical Economics, 53, 145–152.
tion: A solution. Review of Financial Studies, 14, 735–764. Wilkins, A. S. (2018). To lag or not to lag?: Re-evaluating the use of lagged
Pikulina, E., Renneboog, L., & Tobler, P. N. (2017). Overconfidence and invest- dependent variables in regression analysis. Political Science Research and
ment: An experimental approach. Journal of Corporate Finance, 43, 175–192. Methods, 6, 393–411.
Polk, C., & Sapienza, P. (2008). The stock market and corporate investment: A Windmeijer, F. (2005). A finite sample correction for the variance of linear
test of catering theory. Review of Financial Studies, 22, 187–217. efficient two-step GMM estimators. Journal of Econometrics, 126, 25–51.
Pompian, M. M. (2011). Behavioral finance and wealth management: How to Wintoki, M. B., Linck, J. S., & Netter, J. M. (2012). Endogeneity and the
build investment strategies that account for investor biases, ume 667. John dynamics of internal corporate governance. Journal of Financial Eco-
Wiley & Sons. nomics, 105, 581–606.
Prince, M. (1993). Women, men, and money styles. Journal of Economic Yermack, D. (1996). Higher market valuation of companies with a small board
Psychology, 14, 175–182. of directors. Journal of Financial Economics, 40, 185–211.
Prosad, J. M., Kapoor, S., & Sengupta, J. (2015). Behavioral biases of Indian Yung, K., & Jian, Y. (2017). Effects of the shareholder base on firm behavior
investors: A survey of Delhi-NCR region. Qualitative Research in Finan- and firm value in China. International Review of Economics & Finance, 49,
cial Markets. 370–385.
Qadri, S. U., & Shabbir, M. (2014). An empirical study of overconfidence and Zaiane, S. (2013). Overconfidence, trading volume and the disposition effect:
illusion of control biases, impact on investor's decision making: An evi- Evidence from the Shenzhen stock market of China. Journal Issues ISSN,
dence from ISE. European Journal of Business and Management, 6, 2350, 157X.
38–44. Zaigham, G. H. K., Wang, X., & Ali, H. S. (2019). Causal relation between
Rahman, M. A., Chowdhury, S. S. H., & Sadique, M. S. (2015). Herding where stock market performance and firm investment in China: Mediating role of
retail investors dominate trading: The case of Saudi Arabia. The Quarterly information asymmetry (Vol. 9). SAGE Open, Article 2158244019885146.
Review of Economics and Finance, 57, 46–60. Zia, L., Ilyas Sindhu, M., & Haider Hashmi, S. (2017). Testing overconfidence
Ricciardi, V., & Simon, H. K. (2000). What is behavioral finance? Business. bias in Pakistani stock market. Cogent Economics & Finance, 5, Article
Education & Technology Journal, 2, 1–9. 1289656.

112

You might also like