You are on page 1of 6

14698749, 2005, 9, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01207.x, Wiley Online Library on [02/02/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.

com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Infant developmental Acquisition of motor skills has numerous implications for
other aspects of development. The ability to respond to the
environment appears to have an impact on all aspects of devel-
milestones: a 31-year opment, and each new accomplishment brings with it an
increasing degree of independence. These changes affect emo-
follow-up tional and social development, communication, and the
understanding of distance and space. During the first months
of life, development is very fast. By the end of infancy, at about
18 months, most children can walk, run, climb, communicate
Anja Taanila* PhD, Professor, Department of Public Health in speech and gestures, and use two hands in complex coordi-
Science and General Practice, University of Oulu, Finland. nated actions (Goldfield and Wolff 2002). Psychomotor devel-
Graham K Murray MD MRCPsych, Clinical Lecturer, opment of children during the first year of life has been studied
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, UK. in many countries and ethnic groups (Hindley 1968, Touwen
Jari Jokelainen, Statistician, Unit of General Practice, 1976, von Wendt et al. 1984). Previous research has concentrat-
University Hospital of Oulu; ed on individuals with developmental delays (e.g. von Wendt et
Matti Isohanni PhD MD, Professor, Department of Psychiatry, al. 1984, Tenovuo et al. 1988, Larroque et al. 2001, Hollo et al.
University of Oulu; 2002, Yliherva 2002), but studies of the long-term sequelae of
Paula Rantakallio PhD MD, Emeritus Professor, Department subtle developmental differences within the so-called ‘normal’
of Public Health Science and General Practice, University of range have received comparatively little attention.
Oulu, Oulu, Finland. Werner et al. (1968) found that those who developed faster
during their first year of life had a higher educational capacity
*Correspondence to first author at University of Oulu, than those who did not. Rantakallio et al. (1985) found simi-
Department of Public Health Science and General Practice, lar associations when children in the Northern Finland 1966
PO Box 5000, 90014 Oulu, Finland. Birth Cohort were 14 years old. Members of the Northern
E-mail: anja.taanila@oulu.fi Finland 1966 Birth Cohort (Rantakallio 1969, 1988) are now
over 30 years of age. This large study population provides a
good opportunity to investigate early developmental mile-
stones in relation to later educational performance. The pre-
sent study aimed to examine whether age at achievement of
This study examined the association between infant developmental milestones assessed at 1 year (standing and
developmental milestones and educational level at 31 years of walking without support, speaking, and continence) were
age in the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort (n=12 058). associated with educational level in adolescence and early
Developmental data (age at standing, walking, speaking, and adulthood.
measures of bowel and bladder control) were gathered from
children’s welfare centres. Information on type of schooling Method
at 14 years of age was reported by children and parents. Data were collected from the ethnically homogeneous popula-
School achievement at 16 years of age and educational level at tion of northern Finland (the provinces of Oulu and Lapland;
31 years were obtained from national registers. Those who Rantakallio 1969, Hovatta et al. 1999) and included all women
reached infant developmental milestones sooner in their first (n=12 068) with an expected date of delivery in 1966 as well as
year of life had significantly better (p<0.05) mean scores in their live born children (n=12 058). The study covered 96% of
teacher ratings at 16 years, and at 31 years they were more all births in the region in that year (Rantakallio 1988). Life expe-
likely to have achieved a better educational level than slower riences, development, health, and education of these children
developers. The adjusted odds ratios for individuals who were followed up from gestation through to birth and at the
developed more slowly to remain at a basic educational level ages of 1, 14, and 31 years. The 31-year follow-up study was
(7 to 16y) ranged significantly from 1.1 to 1.3. The possibility conducted between 1997 and 1998. Those cohort members
of advancing from secondary to tertiary level was 1.4 times who were known to be living in Finland (n=10 631) participat-
greater in faster developers than in slow developers. In ed in the study. Cohort members gave written consent for ear-
conclusion, those who develop faster during their first year of lier and current data to be used. Permission for gathering
life tend to attain higher levels of education in adolescence register data was obtained from the Finnish Ministry of Social
and adulthood. Welfare and Health Affairs in 1993.

DEVELOPMENTAL DATA AT 1 YEAR OF AGE


Developmental data in infancy were gathered from children’s
welfare centres (children visited the centres 10 times on aver-
age, during their first year). Data were supplemented with
information obtained in a special examination performed by
public health nurses for research purposes. At 1 year of age,
98.4% (n=11 870) of the children were alive. Developmental
information was obtained for 91% of the children, which was
collected at an age of at least 11.5 months in 95% of cases.
When calculating cumulative indices for passing developmen-
tal levels at age 11 or 12 months, those children whose health

Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2005, 47: 581–586 581


14698749, 2005, 9, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01207.x, Wiley Online Library on [02/02/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
records had been filled in before this age and who had not school health nurses and school social workers so that the
reached the actual level of development by the age of com- final response rate was 97.6%. All the available information (IQ
pleting the health records, were assumed to develop similar- scores, records from child guidance centres, hospitals, and
ly to those who had been observed until the age of 12 months institutions for children with learning disabilities*) was used to
(von Wendt et al. 1984, Isohanni et al. 2001). calculate the number of children with learning disability
The following developmental milestones were studied at (n=140) and below average intelligence (n=161). Information
the 1-year examination. (1) Age at standing and walking with- was obtained from all but 14 children who had emigrated. Of
out support: parents were asked the age (in months) at the whole cohort (n=11 766), 94% of the children (n=11 064)
which the child acquired these skills. The following cate- attended school at a level appropriate for their age (normal
gories were used for analyses: standing (before nine, at 10, at class or above) and 3.8% (n=449) attended a class below their
11, over 12mo) and walking (before 10, at 11, over 12mo). age-appropriate level. A total of 2.2% (n=253) of the children
(2) Speaking: parents were asked how many words the child had been exempted from compulsory education or they
spoke at 1 year of age: no words, one to two words, three or attended a school for children with learning disabilities, motor
more words. (3) Potty training (bowel control): parents were disabilities, hearing deficits, or behavioural problems (Ranta-
asked whether the child defecated into a potty (mostly or kallio and von Wendt 1986).
always, sometimes, never). (4) Day/night-time wetting (blad- Information on children’s school marks (teacher ratings)
der control): parents were asked whether the child was mostly was obtained from the national application register for upper
dry, always dry, or wet every day/night. The variable was dichot- secondary education, including vocational schools and upper
omized: sometimes dry versus always wet. secondary schools, until 16 years of age. Compulsory schooling
in Finland ends when children are 16 years old and after that
SCHOOL PERFORMANCE IN ADOLESCENCE all adolescents are advised to apply to enter upper secondary
In 1980, when the children were 14 years of age, data on cog- education. All applications and preferences for upper secondary
nitive and physical development, as well as school perfor- education are stored in the national register, including data
mance, were gathered using a questionnaire completed by on school marks as well as mean scores of those school marks.
the children and their parents. The response rate was 97%. These data were extracted until 1985 (Isohanni 2000).
In the remaining cases, data were completed by interviewing
*US usage: mental retardation.

Table I: Number (%) of cohort members in different milestone variables at 1 year of age stratified by type of school at 14 years of
age in Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort (Rantakallio and von Wendt 1986)

Developmental milestones School type at 14 years of age


Exempted from compulsory Training Special Normal Normal Normal
education due classb classc class, below class class, above
to severe n (%) n (%) appropriate n (%) appropriate
disabilitya age level age level
n (%) n (%) n (%)

Age at standing without support (mo)


≤9 – 9 (0.42) 1 (0.05) 55 (2.54) 2055 (95.0) 43 (1.99)
10 3 (0.13) 16 (0.68) 5 (0.21) 73 (3.12) 2215 (94.6) 30 (1.28)
11 4 (0.15) 25 (0.93) 4 (0.15) 89 (3.32) 2523 (94.2) 34 (1.27)
≥12 65 (2.01) 67 (2.07) 17 (0.53) 154 (4.77) 2902 (89.8) 25 (0.77)
Age at walking without support (mo)
≤10 1 (0.05) 6 (0.29) 1 (0.05) 52 (2.55) 1941 (95.3) 36 (1.77)
11 1 (0.04) 16 (0.64) 4 (0.16) 75 (2.99) 2365 (94.4) 45 (1.80)
≥12 69 (1.18) 96 (1.64) 22 (0.38) 246 (4.20) 5367 (91.7) 52 (0.89)
Speaking at 1 year of age
3 or more words 13 (0.22) 46 (0.79) 12 (0.21) 191 (3.29) 5462 (93.9) 90 (1.55)
1–2 words 13 (0.58) 20 (0.89) 2 (0.09) 79 (3.50) 2117 (93.9) 23 (1.02)
No words 45 (1.87) 52 (2.16) 13 (0.54) 102 (4.24) 2175 (90.4) 20 (0.83)
Defecated in a potty
Mostly or always 13 (0.33) 24 (0.61) 8 (0.20) 111 (2.83) 3712 (94.5) 58 (1.48)
Sometimes 13 (0.39) 31 (0.92) 10 (0.30) 126 (3.74) 3145 (93.4) 44 (1.31)
Never 44 (1.39) 63 (1.98) 9 (0.28) 128 (4.03) 2899 (91.3) 33 (1.04)
Daytime wetting
Sometimes dry 7 (0.36) 14 (0.72) 1 (0.05) 55 (2.81) 1847 (94.4) 33 (1.69)
Always wet 63 (0.74) 105 (1.23) 26 (0.30) 311 (3.65) 7925 (92.9) 101 (1.18)
Night-time wetting
Sometimes dry 14 (0.36) 38 (0.97) 9 (0.23) 132 (3.38) 3667 (93.8) 50 (1.28)
Always wet 56 (0.85) 80 (1.22) 18 (0.27) 235 (3.59) 6079 (92.8) 83 (1.27)
aClass for children with severe learning disability; bclass for children with moderate and mild learning disability;cclass for children with motor

or hearing impairments or behavioural problems. Total number of participants varies because data were missing for some variables. p<0.05 for
all variables was considered significant.

582 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2005, 47: 581–586


14698749, 2005, 9, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01207.x, Wiley Online Library on [02/02/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
HIGHEST EDUCATION LEVEL AT AGE 31 YEARS The model was adjusted for gestational age, birthweight, sex,
Data were gathered from the National Educational Register mother’s marital status, and father’s social class. Social class
of Statistics on the highest completed education level of was recorded and assessed in 1966 by father’s occupation
cohort members in 1997 at the age of 31 years. The classifica- and prestige (Mäkikyrö 1998). Analyses were performed
tion of education (basic, secondary, tertiary) describes the using SAS statistical software (version 8.0). Statistical signifi-
system of regular school and university education which is cance was set at p<0.05.
usually pursued on a full-time basis and with a view to attain-
ing a certificate, diploma, degree, or qualification for an occu- Results
pation. The classification principle is essentially consistent MILESTONE VARIABLES AND SCHOOLING AT 14 YEARS OF AGE
with the Unesco International Standard of Classification of At 1 year of age psychomotor development (age of standing
Education (ISCED 199, Isohanni 2000). and walking without support, speaking, defecating in a potty,
day/night-time wetting) of the children who at the age of 14
STATISTICAL METHODS years attended a school for children with learning disabili-
All available information from each milestone variable was ties, a special school, or a normal school in a class below
used in the statistical analysis. The total numbers of cohort their appropriate age, was more likely to be slower than in
members varies, because data were missing for some vari- those attending a normal school in a class at or above their
ables. Mean values and standard deviations for total scores of appropriate age level (Table I). Most children who were able to
all school subjects were calculated for each variable category, stand at 9 months of age, walk without support by 10 months
stratified by sex. Statistical comparison of the means of the of age, or speak two or more words at 1 year were, at least, in
total school scores of all school subjects was made by analysis an age-appropriate class at 14 years of age. Females scored
of variance (ANOVA), adjusted for month of birth, gestational higher than males in all developmental milestones assessed
age, birthweight, sex, mother’s marital status, and father’s in the present study (not shown in Table I).
social class. Levene’s test was used to test for equality of vari- Table II shows that mean scores at 16 years of age for females
ance. χ2 was used for categorical variables. The continuation were significantly associated with developmental measures
ratio model of a polytomous logistic regression was applied across all domains of development, but for males the associa-
(Agresti 1990). This method partitions the analysis of the tion was significant only with the milestones of standing and
original outcome variable (educational level) into two differ- walking without support, bowel control, and daytime wetting.
ent logit models (logistic regression): basic education versus However, in the model which contains all covariates, the total
other education, and secondary versus tertiary education. contribution to overall variance of school marks varied for

Table II: Mean (SD) scores of all school subjects by milestone variables of all participants attending normal school (class below,
normal class, and class above) of Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort

Developmental milestones Mean scoresa for all children at 16 years of age Analysis of varianceb
Males Females All Males Females Allc
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) F(df1,df2), p value F(df1,df2), p value F(df1,df2), p value

Age at standing without support (mo)


≤9 7.57 (0.95) 8.22 (0.88) 7.91 (0.97) 3.7(3,4602), 0.011 7.5(3,4552), <0.001 8.4(3,9174), <0.001
10 7.48 (0.96) 8.23 (0.89) 7.85 (1.00)
11 7.46 (0.98) 8.15 (0.92) 7.81 (1.01)
≥12 7.48 (0.98) 8.07 (0.92) 7.77 (1.00)
Age of walking without support (mo)
≤10 7.57 (0.93) 8.21 (0.90) 7.90 (0.97) 5.1(2,4596), 0.006 7.8(2,4537), <0.001 11.0(2,9152), <0.001
11 7.49 (0.97) 8.23 (0.87) 7.86 (0.99)
≥12 7.47 (0.98) 8.11 (0.92) 7.79 (1.00)
Speaking at 1 year of age
3 or more words 7.51 (0.97) 8.19 (0.92) 7.88 (1.00) 0.7(2,4648), 0.522 6.0(2,4589), 0.003 5.0(2,9254), 0.007
1–2 words 7.50 (0.96) 8.12 (0.89) 7.81 (0.98)
No words 7.45 (0.97) 8.09 (0.88) 7.72 (0.98)
Defecated in a potty at 1 year of age
Mostly or always 7.57 (1.00) 8.22 (0.91) 7.94 (1.00) 10.3(2,4646), <0.001 16.8(2,4588), < 0.001 24.6(2,9253), <0.001
Sometimes 7.42 (0.96) 8.14 (0.92) 7.76 (1.00)
Never 7.37 (0.97) 8.00 (0.90) 7.65 (0.99)
Daytime wetting at 1 year of age
Sometimes dry 7.57 (0.99) 8.22 (0.88) 7.96 (0.98) 7.9(1,4659), 0.005 8.3(1,4592), 0.004 15.7(1,9269), <0.001
Always wet 7.44 (0.97) 8.11 (0.92) 7.75 (1.01)
Night-time wetting at 1 year of age
Sometimes dry 7.47 (0.98) 8.17 (0.92) 7.85 (1.00) 0.6(1,4645), 0.434 6.2(1,4580), 0.013 5.5(1,9243), 0.019
Always wet 7.45 (0.98) 8.11 (0.91) 7.76 (1.01)
aIn Finland, school marks range from 4 to 10, defined as: 4, rejected; 5–6, poor; 7–8, satisfactory; 9–10, excellent. bAdjusted for month of birth,

gestational age, birthweight, mother’s marital status, and father’s social class in 1966. cAdjusted as for ‘b’ and for sex.

Developmental Milestones and Educational Attainment Anja Taanila et al. 583


14698749, 2005, 9, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01207.x, Wiley Online Library on [02/02/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
different milestones, from 6% to 6.5% among males and from between milestones variables contributed significantly to
5.6% to 6.2% in females (not shown in Table II). overall variance in school marks (Table II). In other words, the
earlier the development in infancy, the better the educational
EDUCATIONAL LEVEL AT 31 YEARS OF AGE outcome in adolescence and adulthood. This result is also sup-
The trends seen at ages 14 and 16 years were strengthened at the ported by Werner et al. (1968) and Rantakallio et al. (1985).
31 year follow-up (Table III). Of those who had been able to The research showed that in some domains of develop-
stand or walk by 1 year of age, or could speak three or more ment, such as standing without support and language (Table
words, or had attained continence, two-thirds had achieved III), cohort members with three or more words at 1 year of
a secondary level education and over a quarter attained ter- age fared better in adulthood than cohort members with one
tiary level education at 31 years of age. There was a statistically or two words. Cohort members with no words had the poor-
significant probability (p<0.05) of remaining at a basic level of est outcomes. Similarly, those who were able to stand after 1
education for those who could not stand without support at year of age had a 1.3-fold statistically significant probability
9 months and had never defecated in a potty by the age of 12 of remaining at basic educational level in adulthood. Those
months. The possibilities for the slower developers to advance who had never defecated in a potty by the age of 1 year had a
from secondary to tertiary level education were nearly 12 times 1.2 times statistically significant risk of remaining at the basic
less than faster developers (OR 1.42, 95% CI 1.26–1.62). level of education and were less likely to progress to tertiary
level education compared with those who had done so by 1
Discussion year of age.
Faster infant development was associated with better educa- The influence of fast infant development was maintained
tional outcomes in adolescence and adulthood in this large after adjusting for factors that have previously been indicated
population sample, although speed of development only to have an effect on infant psychomotor development and on
accounted for a modest percentage of the variance in out- later educational achievements (gestational age, birthweight,
come. This finding is not simply because individuals with sex, mothers’ marital status, and fathers’ social class). Rant-
developmental delay have poorer educational outcomes, as akallio and von Wendt (1985), for instance, indicated that 6% of
the associations between developmental milestones and later children in the total series had educational problems and,
outcomes held true across the ‘normal’ range of develop- among them, children with low birthweight were over-repre-
ment. Although the final fully-adjusted model accounted for sented. In the study by Hille et al. (1994), 19% of children
only a small percentage of variance in school marks (7% for with very low birthweight or gestational age less than 32
males, 6% for females), it was found that the difference weeks were in special education at 9 years of age. According

Table III: Number (%) of cohort members in different milestone variables at 1 year of age stratified by attained educational level
at 31 years of age and odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) in Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort

Developmental milestones Educational level at 31 years of age Basic Secondary


Basic Secondary Tertiary (vs secondary and (vs tertiary
education education education tertiary education) education)
n (%) n (%) n (%) OR (95% CI)a OR (95% CI)a

Age of standing without support (mo)


≤9 213 (10.9) 1211 (62.0) 530 (27.1)
10 268 (12.6) 1287 (60.4) 577 (27.1) 1.18 (0.97–1.44) 1.00 (0.86–1.15)
11 309 (12.6) 1485 (60.5) 660 (26.9) 1.17 (0.96–1.41) 0.98 (0.85–1.13)
≥12 400 (13.6) 1863 (63.5) 671 (22.9) 1.25 (1.04–1.50) 1.20 (1.05–1.38)
Age of walking without support (mo)
≤10 223 (12.1) 1119 (60.8) 499 (27.1)
11 264 (11.5) 1408 (61.3) 625 (27.2) 0.93 (0.76–1.13) 1.01 (0.87–1.17)
≥12 702 (13.2) 3302 (62.2) 1305 (24.6) 1.07 (0.90–1.26) 1.10 (0.97–1.25)
Speaking at 1 year of age
3 or more words 627 (11.8) 3265 (61.5) 1419 (26.7)
1–2 words 275 (13.4) 1264 (61.4) 518 (25.2) 1.10 (0.94–1.29) 1.05 (0.93–1.19)
No words 295 (13.5) 1361 (62.4) 524 (24.0) 1.06 (0.91–1.24) 1.11 (0.98–1.25)
Defecated in a potty at 1 year of age
Mostly or always 407 (11.3) 2122 (59.2) 1057 (29.5)
Sometimes 351 (11.4) 1909 (62.2) 809 (26.4) 0.93 (0.80–1.09) 1.13 (1.01–1.26)
Never 442 (15.3) 1852 (64.1) 597 (20.7) 1.23 (1.06–1.43) 1.42 (1.26–1.62)
Daytime wetting at 1 year of age
Sometimes dry 195 (10.9) 1077 (60.4) 512 (28.7)
Always wet 1003 (12.9) 4819 (62.0) 1955 (25.1) 1.06 (0.90–1.26) 1.13 (1.00–1.28)
Night-time wetting at the age of 1 year
Sometimes dry 430 (12.0) 2206 (61.7) 938 (26.2)
Always wet 768 (12.9) 3671 (61.6) 1523 (25.5) 1.02 (0.90–1.16) 1.04 (0.94–1.15)
aAdjusted for gestational age, birthweight, sex, mothers’ marital status, and fathers’ social class in 1966. Reference value odds ratio (OR)=1.0.

584 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2005, 47: 581–586


14698749, 2005, 9, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01207.x, Wiley Online Library on [02/02/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
to the study by Hollo et al. (2002), 25% of children who were adolescence and adulthood. Although individuals’ subtle varia-
small for gestational age were less successful at school com- tions in speed of development are not influential enough to
pared with 14% of children who were an appropriate size for predict later educational outcomes, at the population level
gestational age when followed up at 10 years of age. Cui et al. these factors may assume much greater importance. Those
(2001) suggested that parents’ knowledge of caring for who develop faster during their first year of life have an incr-
infants and higher educational level were beneficial to the eased chance of attaining higher education in adulthood and
development of infants. Many other studies have shown the higher educational attainment is linked with better health out-
effects of sex, mothers’ marital status, and fathers’ social class comes. In addition, even though most children who are mildly
on children’s development and educational achievements delayed in developmental milestones will function normally in
(e.g. Astone and McLanahan 1994, Jonsson and Gahler 1997, their adult life, this research raises the question of whether
Riala et al. 2003). interventions in infancy that result in faster attainment of neu-
This study has a number of strengths due to its population- rodevelopmental milestones may also lead to long-lasting
based and longitudinal design. Previous studies of the sequelae educational and other benefits later in life.
of variations in development have tended to be drawn from
specific populations such as groups with low birthweight, low DOI: 10.1017/S0012162205001155
IQ, or physical disabilities (Tenovuo et al. 1988, Larroque et al.
2001, Hollo et al. 2002, Yliherva 2002), whereas the current Accepted for publication 15th September 2004.
sample is drawn from the general population. Being drawn
from northern Finland, the sample shows ethnic homogeneity
(Rantakallio 1969, Hovatta et al. 1999) and socioeconomic References
Agresti A. (1990) Categorical Data Analysis. New York: John Wiley &
homogeneity due to the more egalitarian Finnish class struc- Sons.
ture. In Finland the educational system at basic, secondary, and Astone NM, McLanahan SS. (1994) Family structure, residential
tertiary levels is uniform throughout the country and is almost mobility, and school dropout: a research note. Demography
entirely financed from public funds, suggesting that there is 31: 575–584.
equal access to education. The reliability of this research is also Cannon M, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Taylor A, Murray RM,
Poulton R. (2002) Evidence for early childhood, pan-
confirmed by the developmental data which were gathered developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder:
from children’s welfare centres and supplemented with infor- results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry
mation obtained in special examinations performed by public 59: 449–456.
health nurses, thereby reducing recall bias. Only the mile- Cui H, Hou J, Ma G. (2001) Influences of rearing style on the
intellectual development of infants. Wei Sheng Yen Chiu/Journal
stones of speaking and bowel and bladder control were report- of Hygiene Research 30: 362–364.
ed by mothers. It is accepted practice in Finland to teach a child Goldfield EC, Wolff PH. (2002) Motor development in infancy. In:
to sit on a potty at 1 year of age. In 1967, when these children Slater A, Lewis M, editors. Introduction to Infant Development.
were 1 year of age, if the mothers reported that the child had sat Oxford: Oxford University Press. p 61–82.
on a potty the report was considered to be reliable. Moreover, Eliot L. (2001) Early Intelligence: How the Brain and Mind Develop
in the First Years of Life. London: Penguin Books.
previous studies of the same sample using the same develop- Hille E, Ouden AL, Bauer L, Oudenrijn C, Brand R, Verloove-Vanhorick
mental measures to investigate neurodevelopmental seque- SP. (1994) School performance at nine years of age in very
lae of infant development (e.g. Von Wendt et al. 1984, Isohanni premature and very low birth weight infants: perinatal risk factors
et al. 2001, 2004) have been extremely consistent with results and predictors at five years of age. J Pediatr 125: 426–434.
Hindley CB. (1968) Growing up in five countries: a comparison of
from other longitudinal birth cohorts, providing increased data on weaning, elimination training, age of walking, and IQ in
face validity for the developmental measures used (Jones et relation to social class from European longitudinal studies. Dev
al. 1994, Cannon et al. 2002). Med Child Neurol 10: 715–724.
There are few, if any, previous population-based studies Hollo O, Rautava P, Korhonen T, Helenius H, Kero P, Sillanpää M.
that have addressed the links between developmental mile- (2002) Academic achievement of small-for-gestational-age
children at age 10. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 156: 179–187.
stones and adult educational outcomes. Previous authors Hovatta I, Varilo T, Suvisaari J, Terwilliger JD, Ollikainen V, Arajärvi R,
have speculated that variations in development within the Juvonen H, Kokko-Sahin M-L, Väisänen L, Mannila H, Lönnqvist J,
so-called ‘normal’ range have little or no consequence as Peltonen L. (1999) A Genomewide screen for schizophrenia
regards adult outcomes in terms of intellectual performance genes in an isolated Finnish subpopulation, suggesting multiple
susceptibility loci. Am J Hum Genet 65: 1114–1124.
(e.g. Eliot 2001). Although in the present study adult intel- International Standard of Classification of Education, ISCED.
lectual performance was not directly measured, previous (1999) Unesco International Classification of Education 1997.
research indicates that educational attainment is correlated Finnish Implementation Manual. Helsinki: Statistics Finland.
with adult IQ, with a correlation coefficient of approximately Isohanni I. (2000) Education and mental disorders. A 31-year
0.6 (Mackintosh 1998). Our findings suggest that there is an follow-up in the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort.
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Public Health
association between developmental milestones and adult Science and General Practice, University of Oulu. Acta
intellectual capacity, and it may be that the speed of attaining Universitatis Ouluensis D 617.
developmental milestones reflects some underlying advan- Isohanni M, Jones PB, Moilanen K, Rantakallio P, Veijola J, Oja H,
tageous biological factors (e.g. efficiencies in certain neural Koiranen M, Jokelainen J, Croudace T, Järvelin M-R. (2001) Early
developmental milestones in adult schizophrenia and other
systems) that also confer benefits on intellectual or educa- psychoses. A 31-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966
tional performance. Birth Cohort. Schizophr Res 52: 1–19.
Isohanni M, Murray G, Jokelainen J, Croudace T, Jones PB. (2004) The
Conclusion persistence of developmental markers in childhood and
To conclude, our research has shown that faster infant adolescence and risk for schizophrenic psychoses in adult life.
A 34-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth cohort.
development is related to better educational outcomes in Schizophr Res 70: 213–225.

Developmental Milestones and Educational Attainment Anja Taanila et al. 585


14698749, 2005, 9, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01207.x, Wiley Online Library on [02/02/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Jones PB, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M. (1994) Child
developmental risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British
1946 birth cohort. Lancet 344: 1398–1402.
Jonsson J, Gahler M. (1997) Family dissolution, family
reconstitution, and children’s educational careers: recent
evidence for Sweden. Demography 34: 277–293.
Larroque B, Bertrais S, Czernichow P, Leger J. (2001) School
difficulties in 20-year-olds who were born small for gestational
age at term in a regional cohort study. Pediatrics 108: 111–115.
Mackintosh NJ. (1998) IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
E
Mäkikyrö T. (1998) Schizophrenia. Diagnoses, early social factors SAV September 14 - 17, 2005
THE
and comorbidity. Department of Psychiatry, University of Oulu.

ES
Acta Universitatis Ouluensis D 484.
59th Annual Meeting
DAT
Rantakallio P. (1969) Groups at risk in low birth weight infants and
perinatal mortality. A prospective study of the biological
characteristics and socioeconomic circumstances of mothers in Omni Orlando Spa at
12,000 deliveries in North Finland 1966. A discriminant function
analysis. Department of Pediatrics, University of Oulu. Acta ChampionsGate
Universitatis Ouluensis.
Rantakallio P. (1988) The longitudinal study of the Northern Finland Orlando, Florida
Birth Cohort of 1966. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2: 59–88.
Rantakallio P, von Wendt L. (1985) Prognosis for low-birthweight
infants up to the age of 14: a population study. Dev Med Child 52 Papers 32 Instructional Courses
Neurol 27: 655–663. 12 Breakfasts with the Experts
Rantakallio P, von Wendt L, Mäkinen H. (1985) Influence of social Posters and Exhibits
background on psychomotor development in the first year of life
and its correlation with later intellectual capacity: a prospective
cohort study. Early Hum Dev 11: 141–148. Symposia Topics:
Rantakallio P, von Wendt L. (1986) Mental retardation and
subnormality in a birth cohort of 12,000 children in Northern Structural & Functional Changes
Finland. Am J Ment Defic 90: 380–387. in Muscle Due to Cerebral Palsy
Riala K, Isohanni I, Jokelainen J, Jones P, Isohanni M. (2003) The
relationship between childhood family background and educational
ICF Classification Myelomeningocele
performance, with special reference to single-parent families: a Feeding Disorders Spasticity
longitudinal study. Social Psychology of Education 31: 1–16. Botulinum Toxin A
Tenovuo A, Kero P, Korvenranta H, Piekkala P, Sillanpää M, Erkkola R.
(1988) Developmental outcome of 519 small-for-gestational age
children at the age of two years. Neuropediatrics 19: 41–45.
This is THE meeting to attend if you see
Touwen B. (1976) Neurological Development in Infancy. Clinics in children with developmental disabilities,
Developmental Medicine No 58. London: Spastics International whether you are an orthopedic surgeon,
Medical Publishers (Mac Keith Press). p 13–112.
Werner EE, Honzik MP, Smith RS. (1968) Prediction of intelligence neurologist, pediatrician, or physical therapist.
and achievement at ten years from twenty months’ pediatric and
psychologic examinations. Child Dev 39: 1063–1075.
von Wendt L, Mäkinen H, Rantakallio P. (1984) Psychomotor Quotes from previous meetings’ attendees:
development in the first year and mental retardation – a
prospective study. J Ment Defic Res 28: 219–225. “Excellent. All presentations well done.
Yliherva A. (2002) Ennenaikaisina ja pienipainoisina syntyneiden Truly motivating and thought-provoking.”
lasten puheen- ja kielenkehityksen taso kahdeksan vuoden iässä. “Very nice, large osteoprospective controlled
Pohjois-Suomen syntymäkohortti 1985–86. (Speech and language
agilities of preterm and low birthweight children at 8 years of age study. This Academy has certainly grown in its
in the northern Finland 1-year birth cohort for 1985–86). scientific sophistication….”
Department of Finnish, Saami and Logopedics and Department
of Public Health Science and General Practice, University of
“Very well done! Nice synopsis and
Oulu. Acta Universitatis Ouluensis B 44. (Conclusion in Finnish, all presenters had different styles of
articles in English.) presenting in a very effective manner.”
“Course was so educative. This course has
equipped me with new knowledge….”

6300 N. River Road, Suite 727


Rosemont, IL 60018-4226
(847)698-1635
E-mail: raymond@aaos.org
http://www.aacpdm.org

586 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2005, 47: 581–586

You might also like