You are on page 1of 13

University of Groningen

The art of entrainment


Roenneberg, T; Daan, S; Merrow, M

Published in:
Journal of Biological Rhythms

DOI:
10.1177/0748730403253393
10.1177/0748730403018003001

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to
cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version
Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date:
2003

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):


Roenneberg, T., Daan, S., & Merrow, M. (2003). The art of entrainment. Journal of Biological Rhythms,
18(3), 183-194. DOI: 10.1177/0748730403253393, 10.1177/0748730403018003001

Copyright
Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the
author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy
If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately
and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the
number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

Download date: 22-10-2017


10.1177/0748730403253393
JOURNAL OF
Roenneberg et BIOLOGICAL
al. / THE ART
ARTICLE
RHYTHMS
OF ENTRAINMENT
/ June 2003

BASICS

The Art of Entrainment


Till Roenneberg*,1, Serge Daan†, and Martha Merrow*
*Institute for Medical Psychology, University of Munich, Germany,

Zoological Laboratory, University of Groningen, the Netherlands

Abstract The circadian system actively synchronizes the temporal sequence of


biological functions with the environment. The oscillatory behavior of the sys-
tem ensures that entrainment is not passive or driven and therefore allows for
great plasticity and adaptive potential. With the tools at hand, we now can con-
centrate on the most important circadian question: How is the complex task of
entrainment achieved by anatomical, cellular, and molecular components?
Understanding entrainment is equal to understanding the circadian system. The
results of this basic research will help us to understand temporal ecology and will
allow us to improve conditions for humans in industrialized societies.

Key words circadian, oscillator theory, clock genes, clock proteins, feedback loops,
photoperiodism, PRC

WHAT’S THE PROBLEM? appreciate the function of the circadian system, we


have to understand how clocks entrain to the
The ability to oscillate with a self-sustained ampli- environment.
tude in constant conditions is regarded as the circa- The issue of entrainment was very much at the
dian clock’s central quality, yet in nature, the opportu- heart of early circadian research (see many contribu-
nity for free-running rhythmicity (which occurs in the tions to the proceedings of the Cold Spring Harbor
absence of environmental cues) is surely rare. Rather, Symposium, “Biological Clocks,” 1960), whereas only
the entrained oscillation prevails. That circadian peri- certain aspects of entrainment have been part of the
ods in constant conditions can substantially deviate molecular genetic approach to understanding the
from 24 h led to the often-repeated reasoning that clock. These are mainly related to entrainment by
entrainment is necessary because clocks have to be cor- light: (1) light regulation of “clock molecules” (e.g.,
rected. This viewpoint may be misleading: The circa- Crosthwaite et al., 1995; Hunter-Ensor et al., 1996) or
dian clock is not entrained because its free-running light induction of immediate early genes (Rusak et al.,
period deviates from 24 h, but it is able to free run 1990; Meijer and Schwartz, 2003 [this issue]), (2) the
because of how it evolved to work optimally when close association of the molecular clock mechanism
entrained. This is supported by the fact that there is no with light input pathways (Crosthwaite et al., 1997),
exact circadian period because it depends on the and (3) specialized photoreceptors involved in light
nature of the constant conditions (e.g., constant light entrainment (Berson et al., 2002; Freedman et al.,
vs. constant darkness; Aschoff, 1951). Thus, the correct 1999). Yet how light entrains the circadian clock at the
statement should be the following: The circadian clock molecular level remains an open question.
has evolved to fine tune biological functions to spe- So what is entrainment and how does it differ from
cific times within the day or night, and, when put into simple synchronization? Any system could be syn-
constant conditions, it free runs close to 24 h! To fully chronized to daily environmental changes simply by

1. To whom all correspondence should be addressed: Institute for Medical Psychology, University of Munich, Goethestr. 31, D-
80336 Munich, Germany; e-mail: till.roenneberg@imp.med.uni-muenchen.de.
JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS, Vol. 18 No. 3, June 2003 183-194
DOI: 10.1177/0748730403253393
© 2003 Sage Publications
183

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


184 JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS / June 2003

passively responding to changes in light and/or tem- ent from those that control the circadian system
perature (e.g., by using a timer that is triggered each (Mrosovsky, 2001).
dawn). In contrast, the process of entrainment refers to The basic, formal rules of entrainment are as impor-
a system that is itself an oscillator (hence, the potential tant for molecular circadian research as they were for
to free run) and that responds discriminately to those the initial understanding of the system. Genetic and
external stimuli that specifically can reset the circa- molecular components of the circadian system have to
dian clock (Zeitgeber, “time giver” in German). The be investigated for their role in entrainment. Which
clock’s responses are formally predictable (see below) components receive the zeitgeber stimulus first (e.g.,
and are quite distinct from passive ones. For example, are light induced or destroyed by light) and how do
oscillators may take several cycles until they reach they shift the phase of other clock components? Are
synchrony with the zeitgeber, and once they do, they there components that contribute more to advancing
establish a predictable phase relationship to it. The the oscillator and others more to delays? Do some
term phase relates to times within an oscillation. The respond preferentially to dawn and others to dusk?
phase of a cycle can be defined by any reference point And finally, entrainment and masking have to be dis-
of an observable rhythm. Within zeitgeber cycles, tinguished on the molecular level, as they are on the
dawn or dusk are frequently used as external refer- systemic level. A synopsis of the formal properties is,
ence points, while the onset of activity or the maxi- therefore, summarized in this introductory article of
mum of a rhythmic protein could serve as internal ref- the special issue on entrainment.
erence points. The phase of entrainment relates internal
and external phase and thus defines the relative tim-
ing of a given circadian event (e.g., activity onset) LIKE MECHANICAL OSCILLATORS
within the external 24-h day. The phase of entrainment
is not fixed but shows plasticity, which depends on Any oscillator, including a circadian system, can, to
several parameters: the period of the endogenous some extent, be compared to either a pendulum or a
oscillator (τ, the amount of time it takes for a full cycle swing. Consider a swing that swings 12 h from left to
in constant conditions), the period of the zeitgeber right, for example, from dawn to dusk (Fig. 1A) and
cycle (T), its proportion of light and darkness 12 h back (from dusk to dawn). If the swing is pushed
(photoperiod), as well as the zeitgeber strength or (for a negligibly short time), its speed transiently
amplitude. Due to the predictability of responses by changes, and depending on when the push is given,
circadian clocks to zeitgeber signals, the pioneers of different phase shifts result. The response of any oscil-
circadian research compared circadian clocks to phys- lator to a perturbation depends on phase. These sys-
ical oscillators (Aschoff, 1959) and used the underly- tematic responses can be graphed as a phase response
ing mathematics to explain results of entrainment and curve (PRC) (Fig. 1B).
resetting experiments. A PRC is a graph depicting phase shifts that move a
Although most organisms synchronize to their system forward, backward, or not at all in response to
environment by entrainment, not all of their rhythmic a stimulus. Pushing the swing when it is moving fast,
features are synchronized via clock control: Direct in the middle position (Fig. 1A), mainly changes the
effects of light (or other stimuli) can mask the expres- amplitude of its oscillation and has little effect on
sion of the clock. Many animals become active or inac- phase. Pushes given when the swing stops at the
tive just because lights go on or off. Their apparent extreme positions will cause phase advances when
phase of entrainment then coincides with one of the directed toward the center (by convention positive
zeitgeber transitions. To distinguish between masking values) and phase delays when directed away from
and clock control, entrained organisms are commonly the center (negative values). Pushes given at phase 6,
released to constant conditions, and the onset of free- when the swing has little speed, advance its phase
running activity is extrapolated back to the last day of (they are directed toward the center). When the swing
entrainment. When it does not coincide with the picks up speed, the advancing effects decrease until
apparent phase of entrainment, circadian control was they have no effect around phase 12. This so-called
masked by direct light effects. Although masking fea- dead zone of the PRC exists only at phase 12 in a sim-
tures aspects of oscillatory behavior (Aschoff and ple mechanical oscillator but can be much larger in
Goetz, 1988), it appears to involve mechanisms differ- PRCs of circadian oscillators (compare with Figs. 2A

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


Roenneberg et al. / THE ART OF ENTRAINMENT 185

Figure 1. Phase response curves (PRCs) and principles of entrainment explained with the help of a swing oscillator. (A) Perturbations can
be introduced by pushing the swing in one direction with a given force. (B) The phase shifts resulting from these perturbations are a func-
tion of the oscillator’s phase and can be summarized in a PRC. When the pushes are strong enough, so they even can reverse the swing (e.g.,
with 1.5 times the swing’s maximum velocity), the PRC reaches maximum phase shifts, that is, half a cycle, when hit at phase 24 (thick and
stippled line). Weaker pushes (e.g., with 50% maximal velocity) result in a different PRC (thin line) (C) Alternatively, the perturbations can
be drawn as a phase transition curve (PTC), where the new phase, resumed by the swing after the perturbation, is graphed as a function of
the old phase at which it received the push. Strong resetting results in a curve with an average slope of 0 (type-0 resetting), while weaker
pushes lead to new phases that lie, on average, around a line with a slope of 1 (type-1 resetting). (D) Entrainment of a swing with a 24-h
period by repetitive, weak perturbations (white area) applied every 23 h (left panel) or 25 h (right panel). In these phase plots, the back
swing of the pendulum is drawn as bars. The days on which stable entrainment is reached are indicated by arrows.

and B). When the swing decelerates again, a push will they decrease the swing’s amplitude and make it turn
give it extra energy, increasing its amplitude and caus- sooner (at phase 06). If the push has just the right
ing a delay. When the swing has turned (at phase 18), strength to stop the swing exactly at phase 0/24, a har-
pushes will delay its phase even more. monic oscillator will stop dead (at its “singularity”;
The PRC for perturbations that are strong enough Winfree, 1970). A push harder than the singular force
to reverse the swing’s direction when traveling at full at phase 0/24 inverts the direction of the swing and
speed toward the pusher (thick line in Fig. 1B) is differ- will, therefore, shift its phase over half a cycle. In this
ent from the PRC for weaker pushes (thin line). The case, a discrimination between advances and delays is
largest differences between strong and weak pushes not possible; the PRC for strong pulses can therefore
occur when the swing travels toward the pusher. For also be drawn as delays only (see stippled continua-
weak pushes, the delaying effects decrease again with tion in Fig. 1B).
increasing speed until they become ineffective at PRCs for mechanical oscillators as well as for circa-
phase 24. After that, weaker pushes advance because dian systems display phase shifts as a function of

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


186 JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS / June 2003

Figure 2. Phase response curves (PRCs), phase planes, and limit cycles. (A) Type-0 PRC of the pupal eclosion rhythm of Drosophila
pseudoobscura (from Pittendrigh and Minis, 1964). (B) Type-1 PRC of the hamster activity rhythm (Daan and Pittendrigh, 1976a). Both
PRCs (A and B) are plotted as a function of circadian time (CT) (top abscissa) and internal time (InT) (bottom abscissa). Unlike a swing, cir-
cadian oscillators do not damp (compare thin and thick lines in C-E). When the velocity, ν, of the oscillator (C) is drawn as a function of its
position (D), it is represented in a phase plane (E). Self-sustained oscillators form a limit cycle to which the system returns (thick line in E),
while harmonic oscillators are damped and gradually spiral inward with decreasing velocity and amplitude. Note that while the position
of a harmonic oscillator changes with decreasing amplitude (D; it does not swing as far), the phase (e.g., when it reaches a maximum) does
not.

phase (Fig. 1B). Phase shifts can also be drawn as a rately predicted on the basis of PRCs (Daan and
phase transition curve (PTC), where the new phase Pittendrigh, 1976b; Pittendrigh and Minis, 1964). This
that the oscillator adopts after the perturbation is is true for entrainment of circadian systems by
drawn as a function of the old phase at which it was zeitgebers (e.g., a light:dark cycle) and also holds for
perturbed (Fig. 1C). PTCs for strong and for weak mutual entrainment (“coupling”) of oscillators such
resetting have distinct slopes. In the former case, the as 2 SCN neurons. That oscillators establish different
average slope is 0 (thick curve), while in the latter case, phases of entrainment depending on their period (τ)
phase shifts undulate around a diagonal line (slope = and that of the zeitgeber cycle (T) is predicted from the
1). Based on the slopes of their PTCs, strong resetting PRC. In Figure 1D, the repetitive perturbations are
has also been called “type-0” and weak resetting drawn as white areas (exemplifying a daily light
“type-1” (Winfree, 1970). pulse), and each second half of the oscillation is drawn
While PRCs show the effects of a single perturba- as a bar (e.g., representing activity of a nocturnal ani-
tion, entrainment results from regularly repeating mal). The oscillator has an endogenous period of 24 h
perturbations. Entrainment of oscillators can be accu- (τ = 24), and the period of the zeitgeber is either shorter

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


Roenneberg et al. / THE ART OF ENTRAINMENT 187

or longer (T = 23, left panel in Fig. 1D, or T = 25, right (InT) 0 to 24 (Daan et al., 2002). Similarly, a zeitgeber
panel in Fig. 1D). This is unnatural, but it makes the cycle (regardless of its length, which can be varied in
math simpler. Stable entrainment is reached when the experiments) is divided into 24 h of external time
phase shift (∆φ) corrects for the difference between τ (ExT), starting from midnight. In symmetrical LD
and T, ∆φ = τ – T = 24 -–23 = +1 or ∆φ = τ – T = 24 – 25 = –1 cycles only (e.g., LD 12:12), ExT equals ZT + 6 and InT
(arrows in Fig. 1D). The examples in Figure 1D are is CT + 6 (compare lower and upper abscissa in Fig. 2
based on the type-1 PRC (thin line) shown in Fig- A,B). ZT and CT are traditional and widely used con-
ure 1B, with the first pulse arbitrarily given at ventions, both starting to count with the onset of light.
phase 23. The new convention was proposed because ExT 0
When the perturbations recur every 23 h (τ > T; left always coincides with the middle of darkness and
panel in Fig. 1D), they gradually “walk” to the left, ExT 12 with the middle of light, irrespective of the
through the delay portion and the flat zone of the PRC length of light (photoperiod) or darkness
into its advance portion until they reach (after 21 (scotoperiod).
cycles) the phase of the PRC, where ∆φ = +1 h (shortly
after the end of activity). Only in this configuration
will pulses recurring every 23 h hit the system at NOT LIKE MECHANICAL OSCILLATORS
exactly the same (stable) phase. When the perturba-
tions reoccur every 25 h (τ < T; right panel in Fig. 1D), In spite of the similarities between the PRCs of sim-
they gradually “walk” to the right through the ple oscillators and the PRCs of circadian clocks, obvi-
advance portion and the flat zone of the PRC until they ously there are profound differences. While few vari-
reach ∆φ = –1 h on day 29 (shortly before the onset of ables contribute to the behavior of a simple oscillator
activity). Note the huge difference in the phase of (e.g., the length of the pendulum), the circadian sys-
entrainment that is derived from the same PRC but tem is highly complex, involving many components at
using a zeitgeber with a different period. the anatomical and at the molecular level. Further-
In the given examples, T is varied and τ remains more, any mechanical oscillator will damp out as it
constant, while in real life, T remains constant and τ loses energy due to friction. Damping affects both
can vary. The phase of entrainment is similarly pre- velocity (ν, thin line in Fig. 2C) and amplitude (thin
dictable from the τ/T relationship: The shorter τ, the line in Fig. 2D) but does not affect period. In mechani-
earlier its phase of entrainment (e.g., the time of the cal clocks, damping is prevented by an escapement
body’s daily temperature minimum or the melatonin that transfers energy to the pendulum (e.g., from a
peak relative to dawn). This has been demonstrated weight), maintaining velocity and amplitude (thick
for dozens of organisms, including humans: Those lines in Fig. 2 C,D). When the velocity of the oscillator
who like to go to sleep and get up early (morning is drawn as a function of position in a phase plane
types) tend to have a shorter free-running period than (Fig. 2E), self-sustained oscillations trace stable limit
those who prefer to sleep later (evening types; Duffy cycles (thick line) while damped oscillations gradu-
et al., 2001). ally spiral inward (thin line). Thus, self-sustained bio-
The PRCs measured for circadian systems of all logical clocks, such as in the SCN, are stable limit cycle
phyla (see examples in Fig. 2) closely resemble those oscillators that spontaneously revert to a constant
derived for the swing example (Fig. 1B). Both type-1 amplitude when perturbed or even after being
and type-0 PRCs can be found depending on the stopped. This is in contrast to damped oscillators and
organism and the strength of the pulse. Because free- apparently also to the damped circadian rhythms
running periods may deviate from 24 h, the progres- found in peripheral organs (Yamazaki et al., 2000).
sion of the endogenous circadian rhythm must be Perturbations of a limit cycle oscillator transiently
described with its own time base. The half of the change its velocity and/or position, but the system
endogenous cycle (in constant conditions) that coin- quickly returns (is attracted) to its inherent limit cycle,
cides with daytime under entrained conditions is progressing through time (see the numbers indicating
called “subjective day,” and the other half is called different phases in Fig. 2E). During transients, the sys-
“subjective night.” Just as the positions of the swing tem will respond according to a different PRC, but
(Fig. 1A) have been labeled phases 0 to 24, the phases once back on its limit cycle, the PRC is identical to the
of the endogenous circadian cycle (regardless of its one it had before the perturbation. Systems that
endogenous period, τ) are designated internal time always respond with the same PRC are called “phase-

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


188 JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS / June 2003

only systems.” Thus, the stronger the attraction to the Phase-only models also do not predict circadian
limit cycle, the faster the system returns and the more behavior under extreme photoperiods. When dawn
it behaves like a phase-only system. Predicting circa- and dusk are presented as only 2 light pulses (“skele-
dian entrainment on the basis of PRCs alone (as in ton” photoperiods), activity (α) occurs only within 1 of
Fig. 1D) implies such a phase-only system. Using 2 the alternative intervals. When this interval is system-
consecutive light pulses (double pulse experiments), atically reduced, the subjective night of woodmice
the assumption of rapid return has been found valid (Peromyscus) is compressed more than is predicted
for the Drosophila pseudoobscura eclosion rhythm from the PRC (Pittendrigh and Daan, 1976b), again
(Chandrashekaran, 1967). For short light pulses recur- calling for more complex paradigms. For example,
ring once every 24 h, one can readily and precisely pre- onset and end of activity may be controlled by sepa-
dict details of entrainment on the basis of a phase-only rate oscillators within the circadian system (evening
model (e.g., Pittendrigh, 1981). [E] and morning [M]; Pittendrigh and Daan, 1976c).
However, circadian systems do not always behave More generally, predicting entrainment from the
like phase-only systems. Although Winfree (1973) PRC (phase-only model) assumes that both the PRC
confirmed the observations for D. pseudoobscura and τ are constant, intrinsic properties of the system.
eclosion, he observed that the return is much slower This is probably rarely true since both the PRC and τ
after pushing the system further away from the limit are affected by entrainment, which can be observed
cycle either by prolonged continuous illumination when the organism is released to constant conditions
(LL) or by a brief light pulse tuned to push the system (after effects). For instance, when mice are released
onto the point of singularity. Phase-only models are from long or short zeitgeber cycles to constant dark-
insufficient to predict circadian entrainment for long ness (DD), τ is changed for up to 100 days (Pittendrigh
perturbations (e.g., 12 h of light) because different and Daan, 1976a). Inversely, constant conditions can
light levels cause different velocities of the circadian have major effects on the PRC, for example, in ham-
oscillator (parametric effects; Daan and Pittendrigh, sters kept in DD for a long time (Pittendrigh, 1981). In
1976b). While the phase-only model is based on single summary, the light (and/or dark) history of an organ-
short pulses and considers only the nonparametric ism has pronounced effects on the detailed patterns of
influences, entrainment in the real world must be entrainment and helps to tune the system to its envi-
based on both nonparametric and parametric effects ronment (Beersma et al., 1999).
(Aschoff, 1963) or even primarily on parametric effects
(e.g., by tracking changes of light intensity over the
course of the day; Hut et al., 1999). ENTRAINMENT: SYNCHRONIZATION
The phase-only model also fails to predict entrain- WITH MANY DEGREES OF FREEDOM
ment by frequent perturbations. If, for example, the
swing was pushed every hour at phase 19 back to The active process of entrainment, in contrast to a
phase 18 (∆φ = –1; see Fig. 1A), it would get stuck passive response, creates flexibility in temporal orga-
between these 2 phases. However, in reality, circadian nization and thus has adaptive potential. A change in
systems continue to oscillate close to their endoge- the phase of entrainment could be achieved, for exam-
nous period (e.g., Aschoff, 1999; Eriksson and Veen, ple, by simply speeding up or slowing down the pro-
1980). Entrainment has to correct for the difference τ – T gression of the endogenous rhythm because the phase
on a daily basis. When a zeitgeber cycle is too short or of entrainment depends on τ/T (see above). Songbirds
too long, this difference may be larger than the maxi- considerably shift the onset of activity forward each
mum delays or advances of the PRC, so that a circa- spring, related to their dawn chorus in reproduction
dian clock cannot establish a stable phase of entrain- (important for territorial advertisement and mate
ment. Outside of their range of entrainment, clocks attraction early in the morning). Whether this shift is
tend to freerun with regular interactions with the accomplished by changes in the circadian pacemaker
zeitgeber (relative coordination; Holst, 1939). Even or in the outputs controlling behavior remains to be
zeitgeber cycles with T = τ/2 are outside the circadian established (Daan and Aschoff, 1975).
range of entrainment, so that circadian clocks typi- The phase of entrainment is not only affected by τ
cally demultiply the frequency. In this case, 2 consecu- (see Fig. 1D) but also by the strength of the zeitgeber,
tive zeitgeber perturbations add up to correct the for example, by the amplitude in day-night light
endogenous period to 2 × T. intensity differences. The effect of zeitgeber strength

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


Roenneberg et al. / THE ART OF ENTRAINMENT 189

on phase of entrainment again depends on the indi-


vidual’s free-running period. With decreasing
zeitgeber strength, the clock will move forward to an
earlier time if τ < 24. For τ > 24, typical for most
humans, the clock will, conversely, move sleep and
activity to a later phase. Thus, the distribution of
chronotypes becomes broader with decreasing
zeitgeber strength. In some humans, the phase of
entrainment affects normal integration into everyday Figure 3. The circadian pathway. The circadian system can be
depicted as a pathway with the mechanism that generates the cir-
life (Advanced or Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome).
cadian rhythmicity at its center, receiving entraining signals via a
Individuals suffering from these syndromes regularly specific receptor and transduction pathway and controlling out-
wake up as early as 4 AM or, respectively, cannot fall put rhythms by sending signals down another transduction path-
asleep until 3 AM (e.g., Ebisawa et al., 2001; Toh et al., way. Besides the molecular feedback loops that form the basis of
generating a circa 24-h rhythmicity, other feedback loops may be
2001). Such extreme entrained phases may be exacer-
part of the transduction pathways or provide feedback from the
bated by decreasing zeitgeber strength in industrial- rhythm generator to the input pathways.
ized societies (Roenneberg et al., 2003).
Thus, the phase of entrainment can vary for differ-
ent reasons, each related to different parts of the circa- nitrate mutually interact (for references, see
dian system: (1) the zeitgeber signals may be received Roenneberg et al., 1998). Modification of entrainment
or transduced with different efficiency, for example, with light has also been shown for the nonphotic
due to genetic differences in the receptor or the effects of activity on entrainment (Mrosovsky, 1991) or
transduction cascade; (2) clocks may (genetically) for entrainment by periodic food intake (Stephan,
have or (adaptively) adopt different free-running 1986).
periods; or (3) outputs may be differently coupled to Light is most effective in circadian resetting during
the circadian clock. the subjective night. This can be predicted from the
PRC since the most sensitive portion of the PRC, that
is, the portion yielding the largest phase shifts, always
FROM TIME GIVERS shifts the oscillator away from the time of day when
TO CLOCK MOLECULES the signal is most prominent. Because this is a pace-
maker property, it concerns both diurnal and noctur-
Light probably represents most reliably the pro- nal organisms and holds for many different stimuli
gression of day and night in most environments since (see the PRC atlas, Johnson, 1990): dark or tempera-
the timing of many other rhythmic cues depends on ture-down pulses are more effective during the sub-
sunlight (temperature, most weather and climatic fac- jective day, while temperature-up pulses elicit larger
tors, or food sources). Yet zeitgebers other than light phase shifts during the subjective night. It even holds
play an important role in entraining circadian clocks for signals that become zeitgebers only by exposure;
and can interact with the zeitgeber light. These inter- nitrate is more effective during the subjective day for
actions add another level of complexity to the art of Gonyaulax when the cells normally are found in the
entrainment. Light and temperature cycles, for exam- nitrate-poor upper layers of the ocean.
ple, mutually interact in the entrainment of Neurospora Light is generally less effective in phase shifting
(Liu et al., 1998; Roenneberg and Merrow, 2001). during the subjective day than would be predicted for
Even environmental factors that are not zeitgebers a simple oscillator, as manifest in the larger dead zone
in a strict sense because they do not oscillate in nature of circadian PRCs (compare Fig. 1B with Figs. 2 A,B).
may contribute to entrainment. This is the case when This may be due to the many feedback loops within
exposure and/or uptake of environmental factors are the circadian pathway beyond those that generate cir-
under circadian control. The unicell, Gonyaulax cadian rhythmicity (Fig. 3). For instance, circadian
polyedra, is exposed to higher nitrate concentrations systems often control their own inputs with the conse-
only during the night, when the cells sink to lower quence that signals are received and transduced with
depths during their daily vertical migration. Under different sensitivity or efficacy at different circadian
constant conditions, rhythmic nitrate exposure phases. In plants, for example, expression of light
entrains the Gonyaulax clock, and both light and receptors is controlled by the clock (see Millar, 2003

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


190 JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS / June 2003

[this issue]), and in Neurospora, clock and light input glutamate and PACAP as principal neurotransmitters
pathway components are inseparable (see Liu, 2003 and indirectly via the IGL and the midbrain using
[this issue]). Thus, rhythm generator and input path- GABA, neuro-peptide Y, and serotonin as transmitters
ways are not easily distinguishable in such a network: (for references, see Reppert and Weaver, 2001). The
While the inputs change the qualities of the clock, the intracellular transduction pathways appear to involve
clock changes the properties of the input. In fact, the Ca 2+ -mediated phosphorylation of CREB, which
observed phenotypes of clock mutants (arhythmicity, binds to cAMP-responsive elements of the promoters
altered period length, or loss of temperature compen- of Per1 and Per2. All 3 light-regulated clock genes are
sation) can also be modeled when the clock compo- induced (with very distinct kinetics) at specific circa-
nents function as part of an input pathway under cir- dian times: Dec1 throughout the subjective night, Per1
cadian control (Roenneberg and Merrow, 1998). both at the beginning and the end of the subjective
Unlike in most other animals, light reaches the cir- night, and Per2 only in the early subjective night.
cadian system in mammals exclusively through the These differences suggest that the 3 light-inducible
eyes (Nelson and Zucker, 1981). Yet light entrainment genes of the mammalian circadian system perform
persists in mice without rods and cones (Berson et al., different functions and may even receive zeitgeber
2002; Freedman et al., 1999). The race to identify the information via different transduction pathways, pos-
responsible light receptor(s) has reached the final laps, sibly involving different light receptors. In addition,
indicating melanopsin as one likely circadian they may be spatially differently distributed over sub-
photopigment (see Rollag et al., 2003 [this issue]). areas of the SCN (LeSauter and Silver, 1999). An
However, light-dependent entrainment and pupillary important open question is how individual input
restriction still persist, although reduced, in genes fine-tune entrainment; they may be responsible
melanopsin knock-out mice. Only combinations of for different light responses (e.g., delays and
genetic deficiencies of the known mammalian light advances), or the measurement of day length, by sepa-
receptors will answer the question of how they inter- rately responding to dawn and dusk (Daan et al.,
act and whether additional, nonvisual photo pig- 2001).
ments still remain to be discovered. In contrast to
recent statements (News and Editorial Staffs of Sci-
ence, 2002), the issue of how light is received and UNDERSTANDING ENTRAINMENT =
reaches the molecular clocks in the mammalian SCN UNDERSTANDING THE CLOCK
cannot be “put to rest.”
Conceptualizing the circadian system as a pathway It is remarkable that both the clock mechanism and
(Fig. 3) works on several levels. At the systemic level the light input pathway to the circadian clock appear
in mammals, the receptor resides in the retina and the difficult to destroy. While single mutations or knock-
pacemaker in the SCN, while receptor(s) and rhythms outs of clock genes appeared to abolish self-sustained
generator(s) are inherent to the cell for cellular clocks. rhythmicity, further experiments have shown that
For the system, the zeitgeber is exogenous (e.g., light), many circadian qualities remain intact (e.g., Merrow
while the entraining signals for cellular clocks are et al., 1999; Yoshii et al., 2002) or reappear when tested
endogenous (transmitters, hormones, etc.). In the case in different conditions (Spoelstra et al., 2002;
of the liver clock, both signals from the SCN and cues Steinlechner et al., 2002) or when additional clock
from feeding and metabolism contribute to entrain- mutations are introduced (Oster et al., 2002). Similarly,
ment (see Schibler et al., 2003 [this issue]). Hence, the inactivation of light input components does not lead
nature of entrainment is distinct for different tissues. to complete “blindness” of the clock (see many of the
This could serve an adaptive function, for adjusting to contributions in this issue). The circadian pathway
different timing of food sources, to changing shown in Figure 3 is more complex than the original
photoperiod and seasons. input-output scheme but is still insufficient to explain
In mice, light appears to affect the circadian molec- all the experimental results. The clock consists of a net-
ular network via the induction of 3 genes (Per1, Per2, work of feedbacks (Roenneberg and Merrow, 2003)
and Dec1) in the SCN (Honma et al., 2002; Shigeyoshi connected to multiple receptors and input pathways,
et al., 1997; Zylka et al., 1998). Light received via retinal and it may be helpful to create a taxonomy of possibili-
photoreceptors is transmitted to the SCN via 2 path- ties concerning both the anatomical and the molecular
ways: directly via the retino-hypothalamic tract using level (Fig. 4, the complexity of the input increases from

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


Roenneberg et al. / THE ART OF ENTRAINMENT 191

Figure 4. Possibilities of how the input is connected to the clock. The matrix formally shows how different numbers of receptors (and
input pathways) can be connected to a clock mechanism containing one to several feedback loops. The number of inputs increases from top
to bottom (I-V), the number of feedbacks from left to right (A-C). In column D, the complexity is increased by light inputs connecting sepa-
rately to different feedbacks.

top to bottom and that of clock mechanisms from left The complexity of circadian systems is likely to
to right). The 2 feedbacks in column B could represent have increased over the course of evolution. Except for
a retinal clock and the SCN, SCN core and shell, or the cyanobacteria (Schmitz et al., 2000), all circadian
FRQ- and the FRQ-less oscillator in Neurospora model systems appear to be comprised of more than
(Iwasaki and Dunlap, 2000). one (coupled) feedback loop, excluding them from
Diagram A-I in Figure 4 represents the simplest column A, and appear to receive light via more than
possibility: a single input connected to a single feed- one input, excluding them from row I. This is true for
back. Although row II represents systems with multi- Gonyaulax (Roenneberg and Hastings, 1988;
ple receptors, the clock receives a single (fused) signal, Roenneberg and Morse, 1993), higher plants,
for example, inducing a clock gene (the receptors act Neurospora (Dragovic et al., 2002), insects, and
as an antenna collecting photons over a wider spectral mammals.
range). In row III, the signals received via multiple A formal analysis of these possibilities (Fig. 4) pro-
receptors interact before they fuse and could, there- vides a framework for experimental results and facili-
fore, detect spectral qualities (rudimentary color tates their interpretation. In mammals, for example, at
reception). In rows IV and V, the multiple inputs stay least 3 separate molecular feedbacks (Per1, Per2, and
separate (e.g., interacting with different light-respon- Dec1; for a description of circadian networks of feed-
sive elements of a clock gene promoter). Regardless of back loops, see Roenneberg and Merrow, 2003) are
the level of complexity, input signals affect only one regulated by light, possibly via different inputs (possi-
feedback of the molecular network in columns A to C, bilities D-II to D-V). Using the taxonomy, 2 questions
while different feedbacks are directly affected in col- have to be answered: (1) Do the inputs interact before
umn D. they reach the clock components? and (2) Are the

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


192 JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS / June 2003

genes induced via separate input pathways? Experi- different photoperiods (Suri et al., 2000). Interpreta-
ments investigating the effects of single and combined tions based on simple release experiment from
receptor/input mutants on the response of the entire darkness to light or from light to darkness are only
system would, for example, address question 1, and marginally useful in understanding entrainment on
those investigating the induction and entrainment the molecular level (Collett et al., 2002). In addition,
kinetics of the individual genes address question 2. special emphasis should be given to the role of pro-
Existing results indicate that (1) image-forming and teins (Nuesslein-Hildesheim et al., 2000) because nei-
non-image-forming light inputs interact (Russell Fos- ther entrainment nor the mechanisms underlying
ter, personal communication March 3, 2003), (2) differ- photoperiodism can be explained based on RNA pro-
ent clock genes are entrained with different transients files alone (Yanovsky and Kay, 2002). Another impor-
(Reddy et al., 2002), and (3) entrainment of the Per1 tant focus should examine the behavior of different
rhythm is instantaneous while activity rhythms show clock components relative to each other, for example,
transients for many cycles (Yamazaki et al., 2000). by using protocols that elicit typical circadian behav-
Thus, an initial analysis would place the mammalian ior under extreme entraining conditions. If, for exam-
system into category D-V. ple, a circadian output shows frequency demultiplica-
But how can the modern tools of molecular biology tion in an LD 6:6 cycle (entraining to 2 × T = 24), some
and genetics be used in conjunction with the components may follow the light cycle, others the out-
formalisms described here to decipher the mecha- put rhythm. When different phases of entrainment are
nisms involved in entrainment? Entrainment (in con- achieved by different zeitgeber strengths, the
trast to passive synchronization) relies on the exis- response of some components may directly reflect, for
tence of an oscillator with a period somewhere close to example, different light intensities while others will
the zeitgeber cycle. Many of the molecular features, reflect the changes in phase. Which of the clock com-
necessary to generate the circadian oscillation, have ponents progressively change their kinetics and rela-
been described based on mutants. To understand the tionship to other components in prolonged DD, so
role of different circadian components in entrainment, that they could be used to explain the changes of the
we must determine how these mutants affect different PRC under these conditions? Which components con-
characteristics of entrainment. Many clock mutants, tribute to the “temporal memory” of the system that
for example, have different free-running periods and becomes evident in after effects (Brandstätter et al.,
should, therefore, show different phases of entrain- 2001)?
ment. Although in some cases, different phases are
suggested by available data, too few systematic exper-
iments have been performed on mutants in clock and/ ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
or input components. In this context, it is important to
distinguish between “masking” and clock control of This article is dedicated to the memory of Art
the locomotor activity. This distinction is also relevant Winfree (1942-2002) who taught us to think logically
at the molecular level: Within a molecular network, about entrainment. We thank Zdravko Dragovic, Jim
some components may respond acutely to light (i.e., Merrow, and Russell Foster for helpful comments on
appear to be driven) despite the fact that they play an the manuscript. Our work is supported by the
important role in maintaining a self-sustained rhythm Eppendorf Company, Hamburg; by the Deutsche
in DD. The mammalian Per1 gene, for example, is F o r s c h u n g s g e m e i n s c h a f t , t h e D r. - M e y e r-
strongly light induced, so that a luciferase gene under Struckmann-Stiftung; by BrainTime from the Euro-
control of the Per1 promoter may report acute light pean Commission; and by the Netherlands Founda-
responses rather than entrainment of the entire net- tion for Scientific Research (NWO-ALW).
work (Yamazaki et al., 2000). Interactions between
molecular clock components have often been con-
cluded from their behavior in DD (e.g., the lag REFERENCES
between RNA and protein). Many of these conclu-
sions (for both wild type and mutants) have to be reex- Aschoff J (1951) Die 24-Stunden-Periodik der Maus unter
amined in time series collected under different konstanten Umgebungsbedingungen. Naturwiss 38:506-
entraining conditions, using different T-cycles and/or 507.

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


Roenneberg et al. / THE ART OF ENTRAINMENT 193

Aschoff J (1959) Zeitliche Strukturen biologischer Vorgänge. eveningness, usual wake time, and circadian phase.
Nova Acta Leopoldina 21:147-177. Behav Neurosci 115:895-899.
Aschoff J (1963) Comparative physiology: Diurnal rhythms. Ebisawa T, Uchiyama M, Kajimura N, Mishima K, Kamei Y,
Ann Rev Physiol 25:581-600. Katoh M, Watanabe T, Sekimoto M, Shibui K, Kim K, et al.
Aschoff J (1999) Masking and parametric effects of high-fre- (2001) Association of structural polymorphisms in the
quency light-dark cycles. Japanese J Physiol 49:11-18. human period3 gene with delayed sleep phase syndrome.
Aschoff J and Goetz CV (1988) Masking of circadian activity EMBO Rep 2:342-346.
rhythms in hamsters by darkness. J Comp Physiol A Eriksson L-O and Veen TV (1980) Circadian rhythms in the
162:559-562. brown bullhead, Ictalurus nebulo-sus (Teleostei). Evidence
Beersma DG, Daan S, and Hut RA (1999) Accuracy of circa- for an endogenous rhythm in feeding, locomotor, and
dian entrainment under fluctuating light conditions: reaction time behaviour. Canad J Zool 58:1899-1907.
Contributions of phase and period responses. J Biol Freedman MS, Lucas RJ, Soni B, von Schantz M, Munoz M,
Rhythms 14:320-329. David-Gray ZK, and Foster RG (1999) Non-rod, non-cone
Be r so n D M, D un n FA, a n d Ta k a o M (20 02 ) ocular photoreceptors regulate the mammalian circadian
Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the behaviour. Science 284:502-504.
circadian clock. Science 295:1070-1073. Holst Ev (1939) Die relative Koordination als Phänomen
Biological clocks (1960) Long Island Biological Association, und als Methode zentralnervöser Funktionsanalyse.
Cold Spring Harbor, New York. Ergebn Physiol 42:228-306.
Brandstätter R, Kumar V, Van’t Hof TJ, and Gwinner E (2001) Honma S, Kawamoto T, Takagi Y, Fujimoto K, Sato F,
Seasonal variations of in vivo and in vitro melatonin pro- Noshiro M, Kato Y, and Honma K-I (2002) Dec1 and Dec2
duction in a passeriform bird, the house sparrow (Passer are regulators of the mammalian clock. Nature 419:841-
domesticus). J Pineal Res 31:120-126. 844.
Chandrashekaran MK (1967) Studies on phase-shifts in Hunter-Ensor M, Ousley A, and Sehgal A (1996) Regulation
endogenous rhythms. I. Effects of light pulses on the of the Drosophila protein Timeless suggests a mechanism
eclosion rhythms in Drosophila pseudoobscura. J Comp for resetting the circadian clock by light. Cell 84:677-685.
Physiol A 56:154-162. Hut RA, van Oort BE, and Daan S (1999) Natural entrain-
Collett MA, Garceau N, Dunlap JC, and Loros JJ (2002) Light ment without dawn and dusk: The case of the European
and clock expression of the Neurospora clock gene fre- ground squirrel (Spermophilus citellus). J Biol Rhythms
quency is differentially driven by but dependent on 14:290-299.
WHITE COLLAR-2. Genetics. 160:149-158. Iwasaki H and Dunlap JC (2000) Microbial circadian oscilla-
Crosthwaite SK, Dunlap JC, and Loros JJ (1997) Neurospora tory systems in Neurospora and Synechococcus: Models for
wc-1 and wc-2: Transcription, photoresponses, and the cellular clocks. Curr Opin Microbiol 3:189-196.
origin of circadian rhythmicity. Science 276:763-769. Johnson CH (1990) PRC-Atlas. Vanderbilt University, Nash-
Crosthwaite SK, Loros JJ, and Dunlap JC (1995) Light- ville, Tennessee.
induced resetting of a circadian clock is mediated by a LeSauter J and Silver R (1999) Localization of a
rapid increase in frequency transcript. Cell 81:1003-1012. suprachiasmatic nucleus subregion regulating locomo-
Daan S, Albrecht U, van der Horst GTJ, Illnerova H, tor rhythmicity. J Neurosci 19:5574-5585.
Roenneberg T, Schwartz WJ, and Wehr TA (2001) Assem- Liu Y (2003) Molecular mechanisms of entrainment in the
bling a clock for all seasons: Are M and E oscillators in the Neurospora circadian clock. J Biol Rhythms 18:195-205.
genes? J Biol Rhythms 16:105-116. Liu Y, Merrow M, Loros JL, and Dunlap JC (1998) How tem-
Daan S and Aschoff J (1975) Circadian rhythms of locomotor perature changes reset a circadian oscillator. Science
activity in captive birds and mammals: Their variations 281:825-829.
with season and latitude. Oecologia 18:269-316. Meijer JH and Schwartz WR (2003) In search of the pathways
Daan S, Merrow M, and Roenneberg T (2002) External time- fo r l i gh t -in duce d p a c e ma k e r re se t t i n g i n t h e
internal time. J Biol Rhythms 17:107-109. suprachiasmatic nucleus. J Biol Rhythms 18:235-249.
Daan S and Pittendrigh CS (1976a) A functional analysis of Merrow M, Brunner M, and Roenneberg T (1999) Assign-
circadian pacemakers in noctural todents: II. The vari- ment of circadian function for the Neurospora clock gene
ability of phase response curves. J Comp Physiol A frequency. Nature 399:584-586.
106:253-266. Millar AJ (2003) A suite of photoreceptors entrains the plant
Daan S and Pittendrigh CS (1976b) A functional analysis of circadian clock. J Biol Rhythms 18:217-226.
circadian pacemakers in nocturnal rodents. III. Heavy Mrosovsky N (1991) Double-pulse experiments with
water and constant light: Homeostasis of frequency? J nonphotic and photic phase-shifting stimuli. J Biol
Comp Physiol A 106:267-290. Rhythms 6:167-180.
Dragovic Z, Tan Y, Görl M, Roenneberg T, and Merrow M Mrosovsky N (2001) Further characterization of the pheno-
(2002) Light reception and circadian behavior in “blind” type of mCry1/mCry2-deficient mice. Chronobiol Int
and “clock-less” mutants of Neurospora crassa. EMBO J 18:13-25.
21:3643-3651. Nelson RJ and Zucker I (1981) Absence of extraocular photo-
Duffy JF, Rimmer DW, and Czeisler CA (2001) Association of reception in diurnal and nocturnal rodents exposed to
in t r i n sic ci rc a di a n pe r i o d wi t h mo r n i n g n e ss- direct sunlight. Comp Biochem Physiol 69A:145-148.

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009


194 JOURNAL OF BIOLOGICAL RHYTHMS / June 2003

News and Editorial Staffs of Science (2002) Breakthrough of Rollag MD, Berson DM, and Provencio I (2003) Melanopsin,
the year: The runners-up. Science 298:2297-2303. ganglion-cell photoreceptors, and mammalian photo-
Nuesslein-Hildesheim B, O’Brien JA, Ebling FJP, Maywood entrainment. J Biol Rhythms 18:227-234.
ES, and Hastings MH (2000) The circadian cycle of mPER Rusak B, Robertson HA, Wisden W, and Hunt SP (1990)
clock gene products in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of Light pulses that shift rhythms induce gene expression in
the Siberian hamster encodes both daily and seasonal the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Science 248:1237-1240.
time. Eur J Neurosci 12:2856-2864. Schibler U, Ripperger J, and Brown SA (2003) Peripheral cir-
Oster H, Yasui A, van der Horst GTJ, and Albrecht U (2002) cadian oscillators in mammals: Time and food. J Biol
Disruption of mCry2 restores circadian rhythmicity in Rhythms 18:250-260.
mPer2 mutant mice. Genes Dev 16:2633-2638. Schmitz O, Katayama M, Williams SB, Kondo T, and Golden
Pittendrigh CS (1981) Circadian systems: Entrainment. In SS (2000) CikA, a bacteriophytochrome that resets the
Biological Rhythms, J Aschoff, eds, pp 95-124, Plenum, cyanobacterial circadian clock. Science 289:765-768.
New York. Shigeyoshi Y, Taguchi K, Yamamoto S, Takekida S, Yan L, Tei
Pittendrigh CS and Daan S (1976a) A functional analysis of H, Moriya T, Shibata S, Loros J, Dunlap JC, et al. (1997)
circadian pacemakers in noctural rodents: I. The stability Light-induced resetting of a mammalian clock is associ-
and lability of circadian frequency. J Comp Physiol A ated with rapid induction of the mPer1 transcript. Cell
106:223-252. 91:1043-1053.
Pittendrigh CS and Daan S (1976b) A functional analysis of Spoelstra K, Okeljewicz M, and Daan S (2002) Restoration of
circadian pacemakers in nocturnal rodents. IV. Entrain- self-sustained rhythmicity by the mutant clock allele in
ment: Pacemaker as clock. J Comp Physiol A106:291-331. mice in constant illumination. J Biol Rhythms 17:520-525.
Pittendrigh CS and Daan S (1976c) A functional analysis of Steinlechner S, Jacobmeier B, Scherbarth F, Dernbach H,
circadian pacemakers in nocturnal rodents: V. Pacemaker Kruse F, and Albrecht U (2002) Robust circadian
structure: A clock for all seasons. J Comp Physiol A rhythmicity of Per1 and Per2 mutant mice in constant
106:333-355. light and dynamics of Per1 and Per2 gene expression
Pittendrigh CS and Minis DH (1964) The entrainment of cir- under long and short photoperiods. J Biol Rhythms
cadian oscillations by light and their role as photoperiod 17:202-209.
clocks. Am Natural 98:261-294.
Stephan FK (1986) Interaction between light- and feeding-
Reddy AB, Fields MD, Maywood ES, and Hastings MH
entrainable circadian rhythms in the rat. Physiol Behav
(2002) Differential resynchronisation of circadian clock
38:127-133.
gene expression within the suprachiasmatic nuclei of
Suri V, Hall JC, and Rosbash M (2000) Two novel doubletime
mice subjected to experimental jet lag. J Neurosci
mutants alter circadian properties and eliminate the
22:7326-7330.
delay between RNA and protein in Drosophila. J Neurosci
Reppert SM and Weaver DR (2001) Molecular analysis of
20:7547-7555.
mammalian circadian rhythms. Annu Rev Physiol
63:647-676. Toh KL, Jones CR, He Y, Eide EJ, Hinz WA, Virshup DM,
Roenneberg T and Hastings JW (1988) Two photoreceptors Ptacek LJ, and Fu YH (2001) An hPer2 phosphorylation
influence the circadian clock of a unicellular alga. site mutation in familial advanced sleep phase syn-
Naturwissenschaften. 75:206-207. drome. Science 291:1040-1043.
Roenneberg T and Merrow M (1998) Molecular circadian Winfree AT (1970) An integrated view of the resetting of a
oscillators—An alternative hypothesis. J Biol Rhythms circadian clock. J Theor Biol 28:327-374.
13:167-179. Winfree AT (1973) Resetting the amplitude of Drosophila’s
Roenneberg T and Merrow M (2001) The role of feedbacks in circadian chronometer. J Comp Physiol A 85:105-140.
circadian systems. In Zeitgebers, Entrainment and Masking Yamazaki S, Numano R, Abe M, Hida A, Takahashi R-I,
of the Circadian System, K Honma and S Honma, eds, pp Ueda M, Block GD, Sakaki Y, Menaker M, and Tei H
113-129, Hokkaido University Press, Sapporo, Japan. (2000) Resetting central and peripheral circadian oscilla-
Roenneberg T and Merrow M (2003) The network of time: tors in transgenic rats. Science 288:682-685.
Understanding the molecular circadian system. Curr Yanovsky MJ and Kay SA (2002) Molecular basis of seasonal
Biol. Forthcoming. time measurement in Arabidopsis. Nature 419:308-312.
Roenneberg T, Merrow M, and Eisensamer B (1998) Cellular Yoshii T, Sakamoto M, and Tomioka K (2002) A temperature-
mechanisms of circadian systems. Zoology. 100:273-286. dependent timing mechanism is involved in the circa-
Roenneberg T and Morse D (1993) Two circadian oscillators dian system that drives locomotor rhythms in the fruit fly
in one cell. Nature. 362:362-364. Drosophila melanogaster. Zoolog Sci 19:841-850.
Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, and Merrow M (2003) Life Zylka MJ, Shearman LP, Weaver DR, and Reppert SM (1998)
between clocks—Daily temporal patterns of human Three period homologs in mammals: Differential light
chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms 18:80-90. responses in the suprachiasmatic circadian clock and
oscillating transcripts outside of brain. Neuron 20:1103-
1110.

Downloaded from http://jbr.sagepub.com at University of Groningen on July 31, 2009

You might also like