You are on page 1of 5

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232538868

Partial knowledge in vocabulary acquisition: General constraints and


specific detail.

Article in Journal of Educational Psychology · June 1990


DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.2.315

CITATIONS READS

40 387

2 authors, including:

Wendelyn Shore
Pacific Lutheran University
18 PUBLICATIONS 436 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Wendelyn Shore on 11 August 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Educational Psychology Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1990, Vol. 82, No. 2,315-318 0022-O663/90/S00.75

Partial Knowledge in Vocabulary Acquisition:


General Constraints and Specific Detail
Wendelyn J. Shore and Francis T. Durso
University of Oklahoma

The authors investigated the relationship of partial word knowledge and instruction by asking
subjects to choose, from a pair of sentences, the correct use of a target word that they had or had
not looked up in a dictionary. When the correct sentence of the pair could be chosen on the
basis of general semantic constraints, subjects performed well even for words they had dented
were part of their language, and instruction had no effect. Studying dictionary definitions did,
however, increase the amount of specific, detailed information available in memory.

Researchers have speculated on the role of partial word Levels of word knowledge have received some empirical
knowledge in the area of reading comprehension and vocab- attention. While investigating incidental learning from con-
ulary acquisition and on the exact characteristics of known text, Jenkins, Stein, and Wysocki (1984) manipulated sub-
words (e.g., Beck, McKeown, & Omanson, 1987; Dale, 1965; jects' prior experiences with the meanings of the words to be
Loewenthal, 1971; Loewenthal & Gibbs, 1974; Trembly, learned. They speculated that many of the target words used
1966). Three general levels of word knowledge can be identi- in vocabulary acquisition studies may be frontier words rather
fied: One may know nothing at all about the word's meaning; than unknown words as has been assumed. Subjects per-
one may have complete knowledge of the word's meaning; or formed significantly better on tests of vocabulary knowledge
knowledge may fall between these two extremes. Different after context learning of preexposed words than they did after
researchers have operationalized these levels of word knowl- context learning of unknown words.
edge in different ways, especially the intermediate level of When others measured familiarity using an assessment of
familiarity where some, but not complete, knowledge is prior experience rather than a laboratory manipulation, the
assumed. results disagreed with Jenkins et al. (1984). Nagy, Herman,
Like Loewenthal (1971), we identified words about which and Anderson (1985) assessed the subjects' preexperimental
the reader has no knowledge as "unknown*' words. According knowledge of the words. Subjects then read a passage that
to Dale (1965), these are words to which the reader responds, contained the target words. Their results indicated that sub-
"I never saw the word before.* At the other extreme, words jects benefited more from context for unknown words than
that constitute the expressive vocabulary were classified as for words about which they had partial knowledge
"known" words (Dale, 1965; Loewenthal, 1971). Whether or not instruction has an impact on familiar words
In addition to these two extremes of word knowledge, there may depend on the extent to which that instruction augments
is a somewhat nebulous intermediate level that has been the information that already exists in the lexicon. Instruction
defined in a variety of ways. One of the clearest conceptuali- used by Nagy et al. (1985) may have had little impact on
zations of this level is given by Dale (1965) and again by familiar words because the context instruction duplicated the
Trembly (1966). Dale characterized this level by the state- preexperimental experiences that placed the word on the
ment, "I know there is such a word, but I don't know what it frontier. Frontier words are familiar because of prior experi-
means." Such words have been encountered in some form, ence in context, and additional training in context may not
most likely in the context of reading or listening. Trembly supply the type of information essential to make the word
labeled this type as frontier words because they exist at the part of the subject's active vocabulary. Words at different
"frontier" of the learner's knowledge. We have adopted Trem- levels may differ in a qualitative rather than a quantitative
bly's nomenclature in our discussion of this intermediate level way; thus, effective instruction may require the introduction
of word knowledge. Hence, our study focuses on three levels of information other than that normally constituting partial
of word knowledge: unknown, frontier, and known. knowledge.
This difference between levels of word knowledge may be
one of implicit versus explicit knowledge (e.g., Schacter, 1987)
This project was part of the first author's master thesis under the of the word's meaning. In fact, Loewenthal (1971) found that
direction of the second author. subjects performed surprisingly accurately on items that used
Support from the National Science Foundation Grant IST- unknown words, and she hypothesized that "it may be that
8418853 to the second author is gratefully acknowledged.
We thank Larry E. Toothaker, Richard Reardon, Joe Rodgers, and subjects have some knowledge of the word without being
Tom Dayton for their help with the manuscript, and Baron Van aware of it" (p. 227). Readers may have implicit memories
Burkleo and Jon Wright for their help with data collection. for the meanings of their frontier words and perhaps also for
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to their unknown words, whereas words in levels beyond the
Francis T. Durso, Department of Psychology, University of Okla- frontier are represented by more explicit memories. Further,
homa, Norman, Oklahoma 73019, or Bitnet: AM1067@UOKMVSA. it is possible that the information that makes meaning explicit

315
316 WENDELYN J. SHORE AND FRANCIS T. DURSO

is qualitatively different from the meaning that is present in Materials


an implicit memory.
A reasonable initial hypothesis is that implicit memories Word pool. We compiled a pool of 158 words, Some were thought
to be in the vocabulary of the typical subject and some not. To
are those that contain constraint information about a word,
confirm this supposition, a group of students from the same pool as
whereas explicit memories contain more specific, detailed our subjects participated in a norming study. They were asked to
information about the meaning of a word. To test this hy- check all of the words with which they were familiar or had heard
pothesis, we developed two types of sentences. One type before, and then to define all of the words they had checked or to
required subjects to know only the most general constraints write a sentence using the word if they could not define it. Of the 158
concerning a word's meaning, such as whether it is a "good" words, 60 words spanning the range of word knowledge were retained
word or a "bad" word, or whether it refers to a person or to for this experiment.
an action. The second sentence type required knowledge of Precheck. A precheck was used to determine levels of word
more than just general constraints. Rather, it was necessary knowledge for each subject. Classification of each word for a partic-
for the subject to know more details specific to the meaning ular subject depended on that subject's response to the word on the
of the particular word. precheck. In addition to the 60 words, 15 plausible nonwords were
included (e.g., phisteron). These nonwords were developed by the
To address the question of how instruction interacts with experimenters and followed the rules of English orthography. The
partial knowledge, we presented half our subjects with stan- nonwords permitted a purer measure of the unknown level of word
dard dictionary definitions of the target words. Does looking knowledge. Order of the 75 items on the precheck was randomized
up a word in a dictionary tend to aid words at one level of across subjects.
knowledge more than another? Dictionary definitions have Sentences. Two sentence pairs were created for each of the 60
proved to be a relatively weak method of increasing vocabu- words. One sentence used the word correctly; this sentence was always
lary knowledge (e.g., Beck et al., 1987). However, the impo- used as the correct sentence member of the sentence pair (e.g., The
tence of dictionary definitions may not necessarily hold for dowager has her deceased husband to thank for her great wealth).
ail levels of word knowledge. In addition, dictionary def- The incorrect sentence used defined the constraint-only and specific,
initions may supply only one type of knowledge about a detailed sentence groups.
In the constraint-only sentence group, the incorrect sentence con-
word, either constraint information or specific, detailed
tained an anomalous use of the target word (e.g., The circus performer
information. could juggle the dowager and two bowling pins). These sentences
Finally, our experiment attempted to assess levels of word were designed so that knowledge of only the most general constraints
knowledge more precisely than has been the case in the past of the target words was sufficient to choose the correct sentence. The
(see Curtis, 1987). For instance, Loewenthal (1971) had sub- incorrect sentences in the detail sentence group required a greater
jects rate the target words according to their familiarity; they understanding of the target word's meaning than did the incorrect
were never asked to define a word or to use it correctly in a sentences in the constraint-only sentence group (e.g., The dowager
sentence. Similarly, Nagy et al. (1985) simply required sub- and her husband recently celebrated their fiftieth wedding anniver-
jects to indicate whether or not the word's meaning was sary)- In the detail sentence group, the incorrect sentences were
constructed so that knowledge of only the general constraints of the
known. Again, subjects were not required to supply a defini-
target word was insufficient to distinguish the correct and incorrect
tion for the word or to use the word correctly in a sentence. versions of the sentence.
In the following study, levels of word knowledge were assessed Order of the 60 pairs of sentences was randomized across subjects
by filtering each word for each subject through a series of for each sentence group. For half of the words, the incorrect sentence
questions that required progressively less understanding of appeared above the correct sentence, whereas for the other half the
the target word. incorrect sentence was presented below.

Procedure
Method
Precheck. Upon entering the laboratory, subjects were asked to
Design complete the precheck. Responses were subject paced. On their first
pass through the precheck, they were told to define the words whose
This experiment was a 2 (sentence type: constraint only or detail) definitions they knew by writing a short definition or by giving a
x 2 (study: studied or not studied) x 3 (word level: unknown, frontier, synonym of the word. Subjects were told that it was acceptable to
or known) mixed factorial, with repeated measures on word level. leave a word undefined if they were not sure of its meaning.
Sentence type and study condition were manipulated between
After completing the definition phase, subjects were asked to go
subjects.
through the precheck again and attempt to use each of the remaining
words in a sentence. It was acceptable to leave a word blank if the
subject was not confident enough of his or her knowledge of its
Subjects meaning to use it in a sentence. Words for which the subject gave a
One hundred thirty-two volunteers from the introductory psychol- correct definition or wrote a sentence that used the word correctly
ogy subject pool at the University of Oklahoma served as subjects. formed the set of known words for that subject.
They were tested in pairs randomly assigned to study condition and Upon completion of the sentence phase, subjects were instructed
sentence type. All subjects were native English speakers and were to make another pass through the precheck. For any words they could
comparable on a measure of preexperimental verbal ability: Overall not define or use in a sentence, subjects were asked to place a check
mean on the vocabulary subscale of the Shipley-Hartford Intelligence by the word if it was familiar to them. They were told that this would
Test was 72%. include any word that they had heard or seen before, even if they had
PARTIAL WORD KNOWLEDGE 317

no real idea what it meant. Words that were checked, but for which Table 1
no attempt at a definition or sentence was made, formed the set of Means and Standard Deviations of Proportion Correct on the
frontier words for that subject. Sentence Decision Task
Finally, subjects made one more pass through the precheck. On
this final pass, they were told that if there was anything left on the Word level
list that they thought was a word, they should circle it. Subjects were Sentence type Unknown Frontier Known
informed that not all of the words on the precheck were valid English
words. They were instructed that if they did not circle a letter string, Constraint
it meant they thought the letter string was not a word in the English Nonstudied
language. The words left uncircled formed the set of unknown words M .70 .76 .98
for that subject. SD .24 .20 .04
Definition presentation. Following the precheck, subjects ran- Studied
domly assigned to the study condition were shown definitions of the M .71 .75 .95
60 English words used in the precheck. Each definition was presented SD .23 .22 .08
only once to approximate a reader's exposure to a definition when it
is simply looked up in a dictionary. Definitions were typed on 3 X 5 Detail
index cards; the target word was centered above its definition. Defi- Nonstudied
nitions from the Oxford English Dictionary (1971) or Webster's New M .49 .60 .93
World Dictionary, Second College Edition (Guralnik, 1982) were SD .18 .18 .06
used. Subjects were told that they would have 10 seconds to study Studied
each of the definitions. Subjects turned to the next card when they M .72 .72 .91
heard a tone on a cassette tape. Order of the 60 cards was randomized SD .27 .19 .09
across subjects.
In lieu of studying, subjects randomly assigned to the no-definition
condition completed the Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT; .030, p < .001. Second, overall, studying the definition helped,
Oltman, Raskin, &. Witkin, 1971) as a distractor task. Standard F(l, 115) = 7.22, MSe = .030, p < .01. Third, the more
administration of the GEFT was followed.
familiar the word, the better subjects performed, F(2, 114) =
Sentence decision task. Following study of the words or comple-
tion of the distractor task, subjects were randomly assigned to either 3.77, MSe = .033, p < .05. However, these factors were
the constraint-only sentence group or the detail sentence group. They involved in three interactions: Sentence Type x Study, F{ 1,
were instructed to read each pair of sentences and to mark the 115) = 9.68, MS, = .030, p < .01; Word Level x Study, F(2,
sentence that used the underlined word appropriately. Subjects were 114) = 6.53, MSe = -033, p < .01; and the triple interaction
told that there would always be one sentence that used the word of Sentence Type x Study x Word Level, F(2, 114) = 3.57,
correctly, and that they should guess if they were not sure which MSe = .033, p < .05. The Study x Word Level interaction
sentence contained the correct usage. Subjects were given as much suggests that studying dictionary definitions produced the
time as needed to complete this sentence decision task. greatest benefit for unknown words and no benefit for known
Shipley-Hartford Intelligence Test. Upon completion of the sen- words; frontier words benefited an intermediate amount. The
tence decision task, subjects were administered the vocabulary section Study x Sentence Type interaction suggests that studying
of the Shipley-Hartford Intelligence Test (Sines, 1958). This test was dictionary definitions aids the learner only by supplying spe-
used to measure subjects' preexperimental vocabulary knowledge and
was included as a covariate in the statistical analyses. None of the
cific information.
target words were included on the Shipley-Hartford Intelligence Test. These two conclusions were also apparent in the triple
interaction. When the advantage of studying (study perform-
ance minus nonstudy performance) is considered as a function
Results of word level, it is clear that reading a dictionary definition
supplies almost no constraint information beyond what the
Frequencies of unknown, frontier, and known words were subject already has; even if the subject denied the letter string
computed for each subject. Data of 12 of the subjects were was an English word, studying provided only a 1 percentage
deleted because they did not have words in all of these point advantage (.71 vs. .70) in the constraint condition. On
categories. The proportion correct within each of the cate- the other hand, in the specific sentence condition, studying
gories was computed for each subject, and these means appear adds considerable specific information to unknown words
in Table 1. Planned comparisons indicated that performance (+.23), some to frontier words (+.12), and, of course, nothing
in all conditions was reliably above chance (.50) except when to known words (-.02).
subjects made judgments that required detailed knowledge of
unknown words they had not studied. The data were submit- Discussion
ted to a Sentence Type (2) x Study (2) x Word Level (3)
multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA). A MANCOVA It appears that dictionary definitions impart specific infor-
was used to account for violations of sphericity (Winer, 1971) mation about a word rather than general constraint informa-
that may occur across the repeated measure. tion. Accuracy on the constraint sentences was not improved
The MANCOVA revealed three significant main effects. First, by presentation of the dictionary definition. In fact, subjects
distinguishing between correct and incorrect sentences in the entered the experiment with quite a bit of information about
constraint-only sentence condition was generally easier than the constraints of words, even words they denied were part of
in the detail sentence condition, F(\, 115) = 16.34, MSK = their language. In keeping with Loewenthal (1971), we found
318 WENDELYN J. SHORE AND FRANCIS T. DURSO

that subjects were surprisingly accurate on sentences contain- References


ing their unknown words in the constraint sentence condition.
This provides preliminary support for the hypothesis that Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Omanson, R. C. (1987). The effects
and uses of diverse vocabulary instructional techniques, fn M. G.
subjects have implicit memories for the meanings of words,
McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acqui-
even for words they do not identify as an English word. This sition (pp. 147-163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
implicit knowledge is knowledge of general constraints of a Black, J. B., & Moran, T. P. (1981). Learning and remembering
word rather than specific, detailed information about the command names. In Proceedings of tke Association for Computing
word's meaning. Machinery {pp. 8-i 1). New York: ACM.
Our results support Nagy et al.'s (1985) findings that sub- Curtis, M. E. (1987). Vocabulary testing and vocabulary instruction.
jects improved the most on their unknown words. However, In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.). The nature of vocabulary
our work shows that, for dictionary definitions, this was only acquisition (pp. 37-51). Hillsdale, NJ; Eribaum.
true in the specific, detailed sentence condition. When the Dale, E. (1965). Vocabulary measurement: Techniques and major
letter string is so unfamiliar as to be outside the reader's findings. Elementary English, 42, 395-401.
language, direct instruction of the meaning can be beneficial: Guralnik, D. B. (Ed.). (1982). Webster's new world dictionary (2nd
ed.). New York: Simon & Schuster.
Looking the word up in the dictionary will supply specific
Jenkins, J. RM Stein, M. L., & Wysocki, K. (1984). Learning vocab-
information, but little in the way of the constraints necessary ulary through reading. American Educational Research Journal,
to use the word properly. However, it would seem these 21, 767-787.
constraints are already available in memory, at least for Loewenthal, K. (1971). A study of imperfectly acquired vocabulary.
several words, in some implicit form. This is consistent with British Journal ofPsychology, 62, 225-233.
Miller and Gildea's (1987) observation that children acquire Loewenthal, K., & Gibbs, G. (1974). Word familiarity and retention.
a word's meaning in two stages: a rapid stage during which Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26, 15-25.
children assign new words to categories, and a slower stage Miller, G. A., & Gildea, P. M. (1987). How children learn words.
during which distinctions are made between words within a Scientific American, 257, 94-99.
category. Because the constraint sentences required basic cat- Nagy, W, E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, R. C (1985). Learning
egory knowledge, dictionary definitions added little informa- words from context. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 233-253.
tion that subjects did not already have. Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., & Witkin, H. A. (1971). Group Embedded
Figures Test. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
When knowledge is assessed with a multiple-choice format Oxford English Dictionary, Compact Edition. (1971). Oxford: Oxford
of the type used here, dictionary definitions can be a good University Press.
way to acquire detailed vocabulary knowledge, especially for Schacter, D. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status.
the most unfamiliar words. Such definintions could be valu- Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cog-
able in learning technical vocabulary where the constraints nition, 13, 501-518.
are imposed by the situation and learning environment, and Sines, L. K. (1958). Intelligence test correlates of Shipley-Hartford
where the specific, detailed information is of critical impor- performance. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 14, 399-404.
Trembly, D. (1966). Laws of learning general and specialized voca-
tance (e.g.. Black & Moran, 1981). However, most educators
bularies. In Proceedings of the Meeting of the American Psycholog-
should be reluctant to embrace dictionary definitions, not ical Association (pp. 229-230). Washington, DC: American Psy-
because they are ineffective generally, but because this type chological Association.
of definition provides only very specific information and Winer, B. J. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design (2nd
nothing about the appropriate constraints. ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
In conclusion, our work has shown that it is possible to
tease apart methodologically the type of partial word knowl-
edge that the learner has. In addition, it is possible to assess Received March 13, 1989
not only whether the intervention in question is effective, but Revision received September 29, 1989
also the type of information that the intervention provides. Accepted October 10, 1989

View publication stats

You might also like