You are on page 1of 18

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at:

https://www.researchgate.net/publication/49721225

Gestalt therapy and cognitive therapy


—Contrasts or complementarities?

Article in Psychotherapy Theory Research Practice Training · December 2010


DOI: 10.1037/a0021185 · Source: PubMed

CITATION READS

1 365

4 authors, including:

Jan Tønnesvang
Aarhus University
15 PUBLICATIONS 51 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Available from: Jan Tønnesvang


Retrieved on: 23 July 2016
Psychotherapy Theory, Research, Practice, Training © 2010 American Psychological Association
2010, Vol. 47, No. 4, 586 – 602 0033-3204/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021185

GESTALT THERAPY AND COGNITIVE


THERAPY—CONTRASTS OR COMPLEMENTARITIES?

JAN TØNNESVANG ULLA SOMMER


Aarhus University, and Institute for Private Practice, and Hedebocentret,
Integrative Gestalt Practice, Aarhus, Herning, Denmark
Denmark

JAMES HAMMINK MIKAEL SONNE


Private Practice, Nr. Snede, Denmark Gestalt Training Center, Aarhus, Denmark
The article investigates the relationship a dialogue between the two approaches
between crucial concepts and under- will pave the way for addressing the
standings in gestalt therapy and cogni- connection between fundamental aware-
tive therapy aiming at discussing if and ness work in gestalt therapy and the
how they can be mutually enriching tendency within cognitive therapy
when considered as complementary toward incorporating mindfulness as
parts in a more encompassing integra- a therapeutic tool. In the conclusion of
tive therapeutic approach. It is argued the article, additional complementary
that gestalt therapy, defined as a field- points between the two approaches are
theoretical approach to the study of outlined.
gestalt formation process, can comple-
ment the schema-based understanding Keywords: integrative therapy, gestalt
and practice in cognitive therapy. The formation process, field theory, mem-
clinical benefits from a complementary ory, mindfulness
view of the two approaches will be a
wider scope of awareness toward indi-
Gestalt therapy and cognitive therapy are both
vidual and contextual aspects of thera- anchored in phenomenology and are undogmatic
peutic change processes, toward differ- in their attitude toward integration of elements
ent levels of memory involved in these from other therapeutic and scientific approaches,
processes, and toward the relationship as long as such elements do not violate the phe-
between basic needs, sensation and nomenological principles (Resnick, 1995; Rosen-
berg & Mørch, 2005). In spite of this common-
cognition in therapeutic work. Further, ality, the approaches are different from each other
in several respects concerning their view on hu-
Jan Tønnesvang, Department of Psychology, Aarhus Uni- man nature, mental health, and methodology.
versity, and Institute for Integrative Gestalt Practice, Aarhus, While cognitive therapy has become increasingly
Denmark; Ulla Sommer, Private practice and Hedebocentret, respected and popular in academia, among pro-
Herning, Denmark; James Hammink, Private practice, and fessionals, health institutions and insurance com-
Institute for Integrative Gestalt Practice, Aarhus, Denmark; panies, gestalt therapy is lacking recognition in
and Mikael Sonne, Gestalt Training Center (Gestalt.dk), Aar- these areas. One of the reasons for this might be
hus, Denmark.
the better adaptability of cognitive therapy to
We would like to thank Sophie Madsen for fruitful assis-
tance during the manuscript preparation.
training models for applied therapeutic ap-
Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed proaches compared to experiential models. An-
to Jan Tønnesvang, Department of Psychology, Aarhus Uni- other reason might be that gestalt therapy has
versity, Jens Chr. Skousvej 4, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark. been more of an oral tradition with a compara-
E-mail: jan@psy.au.dk tively sparse production of written material and

586
Gestalt Therapy and Cognitive Therapy

an almost nonproduction of traditional research, ity, growth, etc.) sense. In that respect, the gestalt
while cognitive approaches were born with a approach parallels the organismic-dialectic posi-
metaphor of man as a scientist (Kelly, 1955) and tion in Deci & Ryan⬘s (2000) self determination
since their inception have been inhabited by re- theory in which needs for relatedness, compe-
searchers and writing practitioners. While cogni- tence, and autonomy are considered basic to hu-
tive therapists can celebrate this state of affairs, man nature. When a need presents itself (that is:
we find the situation unfortunate for those clients when something is needed), our phenomenolog-
who would benefit more from gestalt therapy, and ical field polarizes into figure and ground. Energy
for the potential contributions from gestalt theory arises in relationship to the figure, which sharp-
to the scientific knowledge base in psychotherapy ens it and brings it into the foreground of our
and psychology, which will stay in a state of awareness. In this way, the need, in relationship
potentiality unless the gestalt approach is brought with the context, becomes a determinant factor
into research-based dialogues with other thera- in the awareness process and the following cog-
peutic and theoretical approaches. nitive, emotional, and behavioral attempts to sat-
The aim of this article is to contribute to such isfy the need. If successful, the need is satisfied
dialogues by establishing a meeting point be- and the gestalt dissolves.
tween the theoretical structures of gestalt therapy According to Burley (2004), the central part-
and the cognitive approaches as a baseline for processes in the gestalt formation process are:
investigating mutual areas of inspiration. The ar- figure formation, figure sharpening, self-
ticle is part of an ongoing development of a environment scan, resolution, assimilation, and
so-called ‘integrative gestalt practice,’ in which the undifferentiated field. These part-processes
we investigate the potential of gestalt theory serv- will usually follow each other in the presented
ing as an integrative framework for the under- order. If, for example, a therapist is concentrated
standing and practice of psychotherapy, supervi- on working with a client while peripherally reg-
sion, and organizational work (Tønnesvang, istering an unfamiliar sound in the room, we
Hammink & Sonne, 2007; Sommer & Tønnes- would say that the sound exists as an unclear
vang, 2008). After a review of central theoretical figure in the gestalt formation process (figure
foundations of gestalt therapy and cognitive ther- formation). If the sound persists, it becomes
apy, the article will investigate the potential sharpened as a figure as more awareness is fo-
complementarity between the approaches and cused on it (figure sharpening). The therapist
point to some of the clinical implications of a becomes distracted from the contact with the
dialogue between them with respect to comple- client and interested in knowing the source of the
mentary nodal points. Toward the end of the sound and if possible stopping it. The therapist
article a schematic overview of complementari- mentions the distraction to the client, looks
ties and comparisons between the two approaches around the room (self-environment scan), and
is presented. discovers that the new thermos is hissing. At the
moment of this discovery the figure shifts to
resolution (intending, planning, execution, veri-
Basic Concepts in Gestalt fication) of the situation as the therapist adjusts
Gestalt Formation Process as the Basic Unit the top of the thermos and the sound stops. As-
of Analysis in Gestalt similation in the form of registering the changes
in the self-environment field as the result of the
Gestalt therapy was defined by Perls, Heffer- resolution of the gestalt formation process will
line & Goodman as the “science and technique of take place, and for a brief moment the field will
figure/background forming in the organism/ be undifferentiated (equilibrium) before a new
environment field” (1951/1994, p. 250). It is a figure forms and sharpens again, when the ther-
field-theoretical approach to understand and in- apist returns with full attention to the client.
vestigate how we (as organisms) create meaning Figure formation processes including the steps
through processes of forming and dissolving ge- above can vary in length of time stretching from
stalts. It holds the view that such gestalt forma- less than a second till days and even years. Under
tion processes are initiated by organismic needs certain circumstances there can be attempts at
in both a physiological (hunger, thirst, shelter, figure formation that cannot be successfully com-
touch, etc.) and a psychological (interest, curios- pleted. This might be the case with each of the

587
Tønnesvang, Sommer, Hammink, and Sonne

part-processes. The usual progression in the ge- ded. It must be recognized that the field is influ-
stalt formation process can be blocked or turned enced and experienced differently in relation to
off at any point in the sequence. The blockages the client’s position therein (Yontef & Jacobs,
can either be in the service of the organism (use- 2008, p. 12). And it must be recognized that these
ful prioritizing) or they can result in unhealthy conditions are the same for the therapist, who,
‘unfinished business’ in the organism’s self- therefore, cannot have a complete understanding
regulation. When, for instance, a client inhibits an of what is going on in an interventive process or
appropriate expression of criticism in the thera- in the life-space of the client. But the therapist
peutic setting this might result in unhealthy ‘un- can continuously gain more understanding
finished business’. When avoiding being critical through the use of contact.
toward a potential violent person on the street the
same sort of blockages might be functioning as
useful prioritizing. The Concept of Contact
Given that we always find ourselves within a
Field-Theoretical Foundation complex field, which we influence and are influ-
enced by, contact processes become the key to
As a field-theoretical approach, gestalt therapy understanding how such influences take place.
is inspired by Smut’s holism (Perls, 1947/1969, The gestalt concept of contact can be defined as
p. 28) and Lewin’s field theory (Parlett, 1991). As the exchange of information between I-ness and
clarified by Staemmler (2006) there have been otherness.1 Defined this way, contact is not only
discussions as to whether or not the field should something that is going on between one person
be considered only in terms of the psychological and another, or between a person and something
field or in terms of the field as a whole including else in the field (a table, for instance). Contact
(but not limited to) the psychological aspects. refers also to the exchange of information within
While Yontef and Jacobs (2008) propose that we the organism that makes it possible for the ‘I’ to
only consider the psychological field as being a experience a sensation in its own foot or to create
genuine part of the gestalt approach, we find such an experiential relation to an emotion. At the
a proposal as unnecessarily limiting for an inte- definitional level, the gestalt conception of con-
grative gestalt approach. To avoid reductionism tact does not differentiate between contact in
and unnecessary dichotomization between psy- physical, social and psychological spheres. The
chology and other scientific disciplines (for in- benefit from such a definition is that it makes it
stance, neurobiology and sociology) we would possible to work with contact in the therapeutic
argue that the gestalt conception of the field relation (the social sphere) without dichotomiz-
should be in accordance with the Lewinian con- ing the exchange of information in that relation
ceptualization of the ‘field as a whole’ including from the exchange of information in the client’s
both its psychological and nonpsychological as- psychological sphere. For example, if the client
pects (Lewin, 1951, pp. 172–174). provides the therapist with information about the
Working with the whole field as the basic weather and the therapist nods his head, there is
stance doesn’t mean that the therapist must in- contact in the sense of exchange of information.
clude the entire complex, and, in principle, infi- This might be considered a superficial contact with
nite field when working with a client. In psycho- no further therapeutic implications or it might be
therapy as well as in research it is a methodical
necessity to delimit the field focus under investi-
1
gation (Lewin; in Perls, Hefferline, & Goodman, Correspondingly, it is the exchange of information with
1951/1994, p. 277). And the way in which such otherness that creates the possibilities for development and
methodical limitations of the field are realized is creation of meaning for the individual organism. Because the
I-ness of the organism in this way is linked to otherness in the
continuously up to the therapist’s professional
field, it is a logical outcome of gestalt theory that the gestalt
skills, aesthetic judgment and capacity to be therapist works with dialogue, contact and contact disruptions
aware of the fact that this is what is happening at in the therapeutic process. Gestalt theory and gestalt practice
the moment. are based on the same premises and consistently aligned to
To work with a field conception means that an each other, aiming at investigating gestalt formation processes
understanding of the client must include relevant as they take place in contact-relatings in the organism-
aspects of the field in which he or she is embed- environment field.

588
Gestalt Therapy and Cognitive Therapy

considered a first step in establishing a sense of trust as easily and unproblematic as in the aforemen-
between client and therapist. As in all forms of tioned thermos example, or as smooth as in the
contact the therapist is being aware of the contact dialogue above, and, in some situations, it does
patterns of the client. If the more superficial level of not happen at all. Thus, in some cases the out-
contact regarding the weather reflects the client’s come will be unfinished business that continu-
tendency to avoid contact with conflictual themes, ously will absorb energy with the unfulfilled need
and the therapist brings awareness to this avoid- functioning as a tense readiness to seek fulfill-
ance, the client might experience a deepening of ment in different suitable and unsuitable situa-
contact with the therapist (a feeling of being seen). tions. In other cases, it may be a matter of ending
This deepening can pave the way for the client’s an unfulfilled gestalt by recognizing that the need
contact with the origins of the avoidance pattern and cannot be met (in its present form, at this point, in
its gestalt formational character (figure formation, this context, or never). In such cases, ending the
figure sharpening, self-environment scan, resolu- gestalt formation-in-process will lead to an ac-
tion, assimilation). A short example will illustrate ceptance of the fact that the need cannot be ful-
the point: filled. This might be linked to different degrees of
Therapist: “I notice that as I am speaking there are tears in
self-soothing or grieving processes extending
your eyes.” (By focusing awareness, the therapist invites the from the recognition of a lost childhood (where
client to recognize sensation. e.g., figure formation) one, e.g., has to say forever goodbye to the hope
Client: “I can’t stand it.” (It seems like the client gets more that one’s mother would be present as a secure
contact with the feeling. e.g., figure sharpening) base) to the less comprehensive grieving project
in everyday life situations; for instance, when one
T: “Try looking at me and saying, ‘I can’t stand it!’” (The
therapist invites to self-environment scan and to expanding/ has been looking forward to spending some time
deepening contact with therapist.) with a good friend who cancels because of ill-
The client looks at the therapist; crying becomes louder and
ness. The nature and scope of such self-soothing
deeper. and grieving processes are, of course, relative.
For those with an unstable personality structure,
C (through the tears): “When you said that you had to cancel
our next appointment because you have to go to a funeral, I
it is characteristic that - what by others would be
felt as if you stabbed me in the heart. I know it’s crazy but considered a small blow and disappointment of
that’s how it felt. I was just about to run out – I can see that everyday life - can be experienced as a shaking of
is what I usually do.” (The client’s awareness of old patterns foundations (in the terms of Tillich, 1948).
of gestalt formation processes becomes a new figure.) Just as a gestalt formation process can be dis-
T: “Even with people you love and trust?” (The therapist solved in different ways, the appearance of needs
invites to sharpening the new figure.) can have different degrees of ‘legitimacy’ (Bur-
C: “Yes.” (The client seems to experience contact with the ley, 2004). Some needs might be the result of a
new figure.) faulty perception of a bodily state or a faulty
T: “What are you doing now that is different?” (The therapist interpretation of, for instance, body sensations in
invites to sharpening the figure of the new gestalt formation the stomach region as hunger when in fact they
process.) are an expression of thirst or possibly nervous-
C: “I’m still here.” (The client affirms the figure sharpening.) ness. As a consequence, the continued figure for-
mation process will rest on a false premise and
T: “Can you look at me and say that?” (The therapist invites
to stay with the new figure in contact with the therapist.)
result in an impaired organismic self-regulation.
It will not lead to the health and well-being that
C: The client looks at the therapist and says, “I am here.” usually results from the flow of healthy organis-
(Resolution)
mic self-regulation.
T: “How does this feel?” (The client is invited to self-contact.) In a gestalt framework mental unbalances and
C: “Scary but right.” (Assimilation) disturbances are seen as expressions of organis-
mic self-regulatory processes that have been mis-
Health and Pathology in Gestalt Theory trained or misformed such as the example in the
previous paragraph. As a result, the processes
During smooth organismic self-regulation the through which figures are formed and dissolved
gestalt formation process is brought to an end by are either diffused or blocked or rigid, creating
the fulfillment of the need(s) that underlie it’s unsuitable and poor functional fits between the
initiation. This of course does not always happen individual’s administration of needs and the de-

589
Tønnesvang, Sommer, Hammink, and Sonne

mands of the surroundings. When organismic system with the gestalt formation processes be-
self-regulation takes place without too many neath the symptoms.
‘process lumps’, the result will be that the most The central assumption in gestalt process diag-
vital and central need in the situation becomes nosis is that the type of disturbed self-regulation
figure (cf. the description of the first stages in the is a manifestation of specific dysfunctions in the
process of gestalt formation). This requires the gestalt formation process (Burley, 2004, 2005).
individual organism to know and acknowledge Where there is a problem in the gestalt formation
what it senses, feels, thinks, and does here and process, and what that problem is in each of these
now. It further requires that it adapts the fulfill- steps (figure formation, figure sharpening, self-
ment of these needs to the concrete context (in- environment scan, resolution, assimilation re-
terpersonal, material, and cultural) wherein it is spectively) will determine the differences be-
situated. In order for such “creative adjustments” tween specific psychopathologies at a process
to happen, the individual’s ego-boundaries must level. Thus, psychopathologies in which figure
be properly permeable. They must, on the one formation is inhibited from the start are different
hand, make it possible to be in contact with and from psychopathologies in which figures are
say yes to that which facilitates creative adjust- formed, but not held clear long enough to secure
ment and, on the other hand, make it possible to the realization of the need of which it is an
reject that which is psychologically or physically expression. And psychopathologies in which an
invading and disabling (Perls et al., 1951/1994, p. overtly strong figure is formed are different from
230; Yontef & Jacobs, 2008, p. 20). pathologies in which figure formation holds a
Ego-boundaries can be off-balance in many split between two phenomenological worlds that
ways, and to a greater or lesser extent. For some are mutually exclusive. The situation concerning
clients with a personality disorder, we will find splitting is, for instance, characteristic of border-
excessive permeability amounting almost to dis- line disorders where ground as well as figure is
solution when a potentially nourishing contact split in two mutually excluding phenomenologi-
with another activates fear of merging into and/or cal modes that coexist through the whole gestalt
being destroyed by the other. At the same time, formation process. When one of these phenome-
an intense anxiety of being left to ones own nological modes is activated (and dominates) the
immature (mistrained or misformed) self- client’s figure formation process has a positive
regulation and self-support can be activated when valence and s/he might idealize the therapist,
these persons get nearer a potential ego-boundary while when the other mode dominates, the figure
and recognize their separation. Recognizing the formation process has a negative valence and s/he
life-situations at the contact boundary for such might devaluate the therapist. Due to the split
persons gives us a clue about the painful existen-
between the two modes the client will not recog-
tial extremities that basic contact disorders are
nize the inconsistency in the two processes lead-
about.
ing to the oscillating attitudes. The situation in
which we find inhibited figure formation, is char-
acteristic of depression where no figure of inter-
Gestalt Diagnosis est is formed—followed or determined by feel-
Traditionally, gestalt theoreticians and thera- ings of helplessness or hopelessness. When no
pists have had a strained attitude toward diagno- figure is formed there will be no need-based
sis. Many have completely disassociated them- action (resolution) and satisfaction (assimilation).
selves from the use of it. However, with the boost In obsessive– compulsive disorders there will be
of evidence-based therapy during the last de- a clear figure formation, but at the moment (in the
cades, gestalt therapists have come to recognize gestalt formation process) when self-environment
that the nonchalant strategy of rejecting diagnosis scanning takes place, the clear figure is captured
is a residual from the past. It is therefore suitable (and replaced) by other figures in the environ-
that Burley (2005) developed the contours of a ment; the result being that the primary need (the
diagnostic strategy, which on the one hand is original figure) remains unsatisfied and therefore,
anchored in basic gestalt principles and on the there is no resolution or assimilation of that need.
other hand seems to promise a match of the (For further elaboration and examples, see Bur-
symptom descriptions in ICD-10 and the DSM ley, 2005).

590
Gestalt Therapy and Cognitive Therapy

Uncovering Character Structure In relation to the transformational work on gestalt


formation processes, awareness is the means by
Within a gestalt framework, psychological dis-
which the individual comes to know (the prob-
turbances are considered to be expressions of
lems in) the steps of the process through which
dysfunctional gestalt formation processes. As
figures are formed and sharpened, followed by
mentioned, the different types of disturbance are
the result of where in the gestalt formation pro- self-environment scanning, resolution, and as-
cess the organism has a tendency to interrupt, as similation. At the same time, awareness is an end
well as to the typical style or way in which it in the sense that by becoming aware of what
interrupts itself. With reference to Burley and happens and when it happens, smooth organismic
Freier (2004) we can say that the implicit ten- self-regulation most likely will occur.
dency to perform particular gestalt formations It is, tangentially, in close relation to the estab-
expresses the character structure of the individ- lishing of process awareness that we find the ratio-
ual, which will manifest itself in a uniform way nale behind conceiving gestalt therapy as a how-
regardless of the individual’s participation in dif- therapy, in which the therapeutic activity primarily
fering contexts. It will appear in therapeutic sit- focuses on the “how” instead of the “why” or the
uations with a typical ‘phenomeno-logic’ pattern “what” (Perls et al., 1951/1994, p. 232). It is the
which will repeat itself in divergent life situations very process (how, what is happening, happens) that
more or less pronounced. Although different life is the focus of attention. The purpose is to provide
situations call for different experiences, behav- the client with the opportunity to capture and expe-
iors, and manifestations of identity, the way in rience with awareness the gestalt formation pro-
which the individual forms and dissolves gestalts cesses as they are taking place here and now in the
as process and process interruptions will typi- contact between the client and the therapist. The
cally be the same across contexts. For example by point is that the contact between therapist and
repeating a pattern of mistrust in otherwise trust- client nurtures the client’s capacity to be in con-
worthy relationships or by seeking the same en- tact with, to sense, know, and accept the changing
forced mirroring responses in all sorts of different
figures that appear from moment to moment
relationships. Correspondingly, it is the character
(Yontef & Jacobs, 2008, p. 35). And to the extent
structure, expressed in the patterns of the individ-
ual’s gestalt formation processes that are the ba- that the client increases his or her awareness of
sic goal of the transforming therapeutic work these changing figures, the organismic self-
(Burley & Freier, 2004; Yontef & Jacobs, 2008). regulation will improve, thus loosening the rigid-
The pivotal point in gestalt therapeutic work is to ity of the character.
bring awareness to how gestalt formation pro-
cesses typically unfold (as the patterns making up
the character structure) in relation to different Exploration and Techniques in Gestalt Therapy
experiential content and contexts.
The prototypical methods and strategies in ge-
stalt therapy will explore direct experience in
Awareness as Means and End in the contact-relations through phenomenological ex-
Therapeutic Work perimentation (Yontef & Jacobs, 2008, p. 36;
Naranjo, 2000, p. 50). Phenomenological exper-
Awareness refers to the attentiveness that we
can have toward what is going on and when it imentation implies focused attention to the gestalt
happens. It is a sort of knowing as one is doing. formation process and can unfold in any creative
More strictly, awareness can be defined as con- experimental way that the skilled therapist might
tact with difference and movement at a boundary. choose (Zinker, 1978). The two-chair dialogue
Framed this way, the meaning of awareness is has been one of the techniques used for that, and
close to the meaning of core consciousness as since it has been repeatedly used, it has often
defined by Damasio (1999), as a sense of being been mistaken for being equivalent to gestalt
an organism in which something happens due to therapy, which it is not. Gestalt therapy is not a
its relation to an object. To be aware (as a pro- set of techniques but a basic explorative stance
cess) is related to that which the awareness is toward working with facilitation of organismic
about (sensations, feelings, thoughts, acts, etc.). self-regulation through contact-full dialogue, and

591
Tønnesvang, Sommer, Hammink, and Sonne

by means of creating awareness of gestalt forma- themselves as behavioral patterns or as mental


tion processes—as they happen.2 (thought) patterns. Thus, in relation to a schema
such as distrust—which is often activated in ther-
Cognitive Understanding of Pathology apy with neglected youngsters, and which in
and Health Young’s model belongs to the domain of discon-
nection and rejection—the following strategies
The cognitive model explains psychological may be used: (a) an overcompensating coping
disturbances and disorders as dysfunctional style in which one acts according to a mental and
thinking at different levels. The most superficial behavioral logic of using or hurting others before
level is the situational level of automatic thinking getting hurt oneself. Next: (b) a coping style of
and the most fundamental level is the level of surrender in which one, for example, repeatedly
schemas (also termed core beliefs) considered as attaches oneself to friends who abuse and take
personality structure (Beck, Freeman & Davis, advantage, sexually, economically, or in other
2004, p. 27).3 Schemas are organizing principles ways. And finally: (c) the coping style of avoid-
or meaning structures, which—when activated— ance by which one’s distrust makes one avoid
establish a perceptual and experiential filter in the involvement and intimacy with others, including
individual’s relation to the environment. Accord- the therapist whom one also tries to keep at a
ing to Young, Klosko & Weishaar (2003, p. 54) distance in different ways. The paradox in these
schemas originate in the dyadic interaction be- strategies is that all three of them are self-
tween caretaker and child and have affinity with perpetuating because others typically respond to
Bowlby’s working models. They develop from them in ways that confirm their beliefs that social
early (preverbal) childhood through adolescence, contact with others is compromising.
and though they become rather stable when they
have been established, they will continuously be
2
elaborated and adjusted throughout life. The cru- Correspondingly, the gestalt experiment serves to bring
cial difference between psychological distur- into the present contextual aspects including the conditions of
experiencing, the social milieu and the habitual self-
bance and health is the rigidity versus flexibility
regulations of the client, to support awareness of the gestalt
that characterizes different schemas as well as the formation process in the here and now (Sonne, 1998). Even if
relative ease and strength with which they are the therapist can see the behavior and convictions of the client
activated. In personality disorders we generally as limiting and inappropriate, the client may not be able to see
find that the most central schemas are prepotent or understand this. A fundamental aspect of such a problem
(repress other schemas into the background) and might be the client’s way of thinking about the problem. The
hypervalent (low threshold of activation) (Beck experiment is designed to give the client a possibility in a new
et al., 2004, p. 28). Further, there will be rigidity setting to experience the problem—including the organism/
with a tendency to not let information and expe- environment field—with the purpose of heightening aware-
riences that diverge from the activated schema ness of the gestalt formation process involved, the result being
heightened awareness of self-regulations (involving cogni-
make an impression; consequently, there will be
tive, behavioral and somatic aspects) and awareness of new
no modification or adjustment of the activated possibilities, which might seem more satisfying and less lim-
schema. In accordance with Piaget we can say iting than the old ones. The purpose of the experiment is, so
that the adjustments will primarily be assimila- to speak, to unfold the phenomenology of the client, not with
tive and not accommodative. the explicit purpose of changing anything, but, through aware-
ness of what is, to give way to the client’s own choice of
change. In principle, the therapist and client together design
Maintenance of Schemas
the experiment. The purpose of the experiment is to explore
Once schemas have been established, the indi- whether the once-appropriate self-regulations and convictions
vidual will typically try to confirm and maintain of the client are still appropriate.
3
them, also in the face of disconfirming data in the We choose to ignore the concept of modus, which is
proposed to be at a deeper level than schemas by referring to
environment. It happens partly through cognitive a state in which more schemas are activated at the same time
distortions, and partly through the behavioral pat- (Beck, 1996; Rosenberg & Mørch, 2005). The concept of
terns that are developed to cope with schemas. modus is used somewhat differently by, for instance, Beck
Young et al. (2003) presents three such coping and Young. We leave out the discussion of how modus should
strategies: (a) compensation; (b) surrender; (c) be understood and will use schemas as referring to the most
avoidance. Each of these strategies can manifest basic level (together with modus).

592
Gestalt Therapy and Cognitive Therapy

Young’s model consists of 18 early established in the environment are ascribed to oneself; (e)
maladaptive schemas that can be grouped into arbitrary conclusions in which causal explana-
five broad categories termed schema domains. tions are randomly and inconsistently distributed;
These domains are related to the assumption that and finally (f) catastrophic thinking related to
five basic and universal emotional needs (when anxiety.
not being met sufficiently in childhood) provide
the background for the development of maladap- Therapeutic Goal and Treatment Principles
tive schemas in varying degrees. The more mal-
adaptive these schemas influencing and guiding In line with the conceptualization of psycho-
the person are, the more severe the disturbance pathology as dysfunctional cognitions, the goal of
and disorder will be. According to Young et al., cognitive therapy is the adjustment of the client’s
we can outline the basic needs and their related way of thinking and interpreting. In other words,
domains as the following: if the dysfunctional beliefs can be modified in a
more functional and self-supporting direction, it
1. Secure attachment related to the schema will result in less suffering and reduction of
domain of disconnection and rejection. symptoms (Beck et al., 2004, p. 4). Even though
Examples of specific schemas contained cognitive therapists as Young et al. (2003) and
are mistrust, defectiveness, and emotional Beck (2005) are also concerned with behavioral
deprivation. oriented coping strategies, they consider these
2. Competence, autonomy and experience of pathology-maintaining strategies to be dictated
identity related to the schema domain of by schemas, and so the therapeutic aim will still
impaired competence and autonomy with be a modification or healing of the client’s sche-
specific schemas as for instance, vulnera- mas. While modification or healing of schemas is
bility to harm, enmeshment/undeveloped to be expected, it should be noted that this is not
self and failure. the same as a more fundamental restructuring,
which is rarely realistic. As a change at a less
3. Freedom to express needs and feelings re- schema-modifying level, reinterpretation can be a
lated to the schema domain of other- strategy toward establishing more suitable ways
directedness. Examples of schemas con- of living with the already existing schemas. In the
tained are self-sacrifice and subjugation. treatment of, for instance, a histrionic person, this
could be manifested in supporting the person in
4. Spontaneity and play related to the schema finding relevant scenarios and contexts in which
domain of inhibition and overvigilance, it may be possible to realize a desire of being at
where emotional inhibition and unrelent- the center of others’ attention.
ing standards are examples of specific Regarding the therapeutic work with modifica-
schemas. tion of schemas, Young et al. (2003) describes
5. Realistic limits and self-control related to four general change strategies. These are:
the schema domain of impaired limits. Ex-
1. The therapeutic relation with particular
amples of schemas under this domain are
focus on the empathic confrontation and
entitlement/grandiosity and insufficient
the so-called “limited parenting” directed
self-control.
toward those of the client’s needs that
Besides the described coping and behavioral have been thwarted.
strategies, cognitive distortions are a crucial part 2. Cognitive strategies using standard cogni-
of maintaining already established schemas. tive methods. For instance, Socratic ques-
Some of the most common distortions are: (a) tioning and guided discovery to create in-
selective abstractions in which parts of reality are sight in the content of thinking, and in the
ignored while others are accentuated; (b) over- attributional processes and biases that re-
generalization when events in a specific situation inforce nonadaptive belief systems and be-
become general assumptions; (c) dichotomous haviors.
thinking with changing polarities of, for instance,
good-evil, hate-love, self-determination- 3. Experience-oriented Strategies borrowed
subjugation; (d) personalization in which events partly from gestalt therapy.

593
Tønnesvang, Sommer, Hammink, and Sonne

4. Behavioral strategies with the aim of in- tions, as they have been constructed from prior
terrupting the behavior that maintains and experience together with the strategies that have
sustains the existing schemas. been developed in order to cope with them. In
that respect, a clear individual-specific version of
Collaborative Empiricism the general model appears that can shed light
upon the predisposing as well as the maintaining
In cognitive therapy the therapeutic relation- cognitive systems. The method is commonly re-
ship is understood according to the principle of garded as an essential tool in the treatment of
collaborative empiricism forming the basic mind- personality disorders where it also serves a func-
set for using specific cognitive methods and gen- tion in predicting cooperation difficulties in the
eral strategies. Collaborative empiricism refers to therapeutic relationship.
a joint investigation of how the client interprets
and acts in the world, which consequences this
Pedagogical Elements
may have for the client, and how the client iden-
tifies and obtains transformational goals (Holm, Together with strategies for developing the
2001; Padesky, 1993). The mental and behavioral client’s insights into his or her problems, peda-
patterns and the symptoms that are the objects of gogical elements also play a significant role in
the therapy are considered as data, which like cognitive therapy. The two most central elements
other data can be submitted to a closer explora- in that respect are psycho-education, aimed at
tion and be tested for their validity. Implicit in the giving the client knowledge of his or her mental
attitude of collaborative empiricism is an aim that condition; and social skills training, dealing with
the client will learn to evaluate thoughts, behav- the development and training of the communica-
iors, moods, and life circumstances more gener- tive skills and problem solving strategies of the
ally aside from the specific problems that brought client, so that s/he can learn better and more
the client to therapy (Padesky, 1993). Further, the effective methods for communication and prob-
collaborative attitude also implies that the cogni- lem solving, including more suitable ways of
tive therapist seeks to communicate the cognitive coping with symptoms.
framework to the client—in a condensed and
simplified form (of course), adjusted to the indi-
Points of Convergence and Complementarities
vidual client’s level of understanding.
Between Gestalt and Cognitive Therapy

Case Conceptualization In gestalt therapy and cognitive therapy we


find different terminologies, different views on
Related to the collaboration between client and what constitutes individual variability and health,
therapist in cognitive therapy, the individual case and different views on therapeutic methodology.
conceptualization is important in at least two Basically we can say that cognitive therapists,
ways. Partly as a joint working tool for the ther- working in modes of collaborative empiricism,
apist and client, and partly as a support to the are aiming at modifying thoughts and behavior in
therapist’s planning of the treatment: in the pre- order to make these more adaptive and self-
liminary stages as well as in its function as a supporting. Gestalt therapists are aiming at cre-
guideline for the therapeutic sessions and as a ating, expanding, and focusing awareness on the
horizon for the therapy in its entirety (Beck, gestalt formation process as it happens here and
2005). As a common denominator in all the vari- now in a therapeutic contact modus between
ants of case conceptualizations (see, for instance, I-ness and otherness. This is due to the fact that
Young et al., 2003, p. 66; Beck, 2005, chap. 2; awareness of this process must precede any pos-
Mørch, 2005, p. 209), case conceptualization pro- sible basic change. Considered this way, we can
vides an individual-specific version of the general differentiate between a modifying cognitive meth-
cognitive model of psychological difficulties. In odology and a facilitating gestalt therapeutic
the cognitive case formulation, the client’s cur- methodology (Kellogg, 2004).
rent difficulties and symptoms are illuminated In our view these differences are usually con-
through childhood and life experience as remem- sidered more incompatible than necessary given
bered by the client and through identification of that both positions have a common understanding
exactly this client’s central schemas and assump- of the significance of memory in psychological

594
Gestalt Therapy and Cognitive Therapy

dysfunction; that the current dysfunction is the organisms possess about the world. Episodic memory medi-
result of patterns formed through a child’s adap- ates the remembering of personally experienced events.”
(Tulving, 1985, pp. 2)
tation and creative adjustment to conditions in the
years of growing up. In the gestalt approach such According to Tulving, the three systems are hi-
patterns are understood in terms of typical figure erarchically ordered with procedural memory as the
formation tendencies, and in the cognitive ap- most fundamental and inclusive category entailing
proach they are understood as typical tendencies semantic memory as a subcategory, which then
in schema activation. In both positions, memory again entails episodic memory as a subcategory.
is considered central to both the establishing of Each form of memory is also characterized by a
these typical tendencies and to their persistence different kind of consciousness. While episodic
in the face of changing circumstances. Within the memory is related to autonoetic consciousness (self-
cognitive position, inertia against change is seen knowing), semantic memory is related to noetic
as determined by the persistence of schemas, consciousness (knowing), and procedural memory
once they have come into existence and been is related to anoetic consciousness (nonknowing),
related to the maintaining system, with behav- bound to current situations.
ioral strategies, interpersonal contexts, and cop- Procedural memory then, is nondeclarative,
ing patterns having a strong self-perpetuating ef- bodily anchored, and implicit in the activities that
fect. In the gestalt position the inertia against are unfolded. When we ride a bike, tie our shoes or
change is anchored in frozen organismic self- when a combination for a lock simply “lies in our
regulation (rigid character structure patterns) cre- fingers”, we use procedural memory as an automat-
ating contact disturbances in gestalt formation ically activated pattern of movements. If the com-
processes. The schematic processing systems in bination for the lock is demanded from us, we
the cognitive approach and the character structure might not be able to figure it out without doing the
patterns in gestalt are both determined by mem- movement pattern once again. Unlike episodic and
ory. But the aspect of memory that becomes the semantic memory, procedural memory is self-
most central differs in the two approaches with contained in the sense that it does not include any
the cognitive approach paying most attention to reference to nonpresent extraorganismic stimuli and
semantic memory, and the gestalt approach pay- states of the world. It is a memory for doing things
ing particular attention to procedural memory. without reflected thoughts and use of language. It is
Our point, then, is that precisely because they learned through attunement between the organism
focus on different aspects of the memory system, and the situations that it happens to be in and relate
we are given a key for opening a dialogic door- to: procedural memory happens at the moment
way between the positions, by which we can gain when it happens.
a better understanding of the relation between While procedural memory can be understood as
process and content in psychological dysfunction a memory for how to handle situational activities
and the treatment thereof. We shall, therefore, (broadly speaking), episodic memory is a figural
start our considerations of the complementarities memory for the episodes we participate in. Such
between the two approaches by looking at this. episodes become part of autobiographical memory
and the life-narratives we tell about ourselves as
building blocks of our identity (McAdams & Pals,
Procedural Memory and Semantic Memory
2006). Episodic memory develops through accre-
As famously elaborated by Tulving, we can tion with a specific self-reference: It was I who was
assume the existence of three different memory- in that specific situation at that given time in my
systems: procedural, semantic, and episodic life. This auto-noetic consciousness in episodic
(Tulving, 1983). All three systems make possible memory does not belong to semantic memory. It is
the utilization of acquired and retained knowl- with semantic memory that we extract and gener-
edge, but they differ in the ways in which differ- alize information and knowledge about the world
ent kinds of knowledge are acquired or used. As on the basis of situations with a certain resem-
stated by Tulving: blance. Semantic memory develops and changes
through restructuring processes, in which we form
“Procedural memory [. . .] is concerned with how things are
done—with the acquisition, retention, and utilization of per- internal representations (beliefs or schemas) that
ceptual, cognitive, and motor skills. Semantic memory [. . .] can be processed and manipulated at a purely men-
has to do with the symbolically representable knowledge that tal level and at a distance from direct contact to

595
Tønnesvang, Sommer, Hammink, and Sonne

specific situational happenings and without behav- knowledge) makes efforts to be lovable in the eyes
ioral response. When a cognitive therapist uses So- of others and oneself. Procedural patterns win over
cratic questioning and guided discovery to explore semantic knowledge.
the background for and the actual functionality of a In Burley and Freier’s (2004) view, cognitive
belief, the exploration will most typically be at a therapy and gestalt therapy differ from each other in
semantic level with glimpses of episodes to validate their different estimations of memory: cognitive
or invalidate the assumptions and spoken words. If therapy pays primary attention to semantic mem-
procedural memory is to be activated, it will typi- ory, while gestalt therapy is methodologically fo-
cally afford some kind of bodily action or concrete cused on procedural memory. While we fully agree
sensation, which cannot be manipulated solely at a with the latter part of the postulate, we do not fully
mental level but has to be lived out more directly. agree with the former part. Even though there is a
sense of truth in the claim that cognitive therapy
Forms of Memory in Gestalt Therapy and typically is more semantically orientated, it is also
Cognitive Therapy true that procedural level aspects are implied in the
behavioral experimental orientation included in the
According to Burley and Freier (2004), it is pri- cognitive methodology. Cognitive– behavioral ex-
marily at the procedural level that we find the ten- periments are primarily created (a) to test the valid-
dencies for psychological dysfunctionalities to per- ity of thoughts as constructions of world, self, and
sistently repeat themselves, and so the procedural others; (b) to create new/modified living rules and
level should be of primary concern in the therapeu- schemas; and (c) to practice new behavior. We do,
tic process, if we want therapy to promote long- however, agree with Burley & Freier that the fact
term change. At the same time they argue, that this that the explanatory models in the cognitive ap-
is precisely what gestalt therapy is aiming at. proach are primarily anchored at the semantic level
Though there might have been a tendency for those will tend to steer the typical practice of cognitive
looking from the outside to consider gestalt therapy therapy in a specific direction for considering which
as a cathartic approach working with relieving sit- micro processes that are stimulated for investiga-
uations out of episodic memory in the here and tion, and how their influences should be conceived.4
now, this is not what defines the approach. As Thus, we would agree that some of the central
mentioned earlier, the defining element of the ge- instructions of traditional cognitive therapy make it
stalt approach is to bring awareness to the gestalt less likely that certain micro processes are priori-
formation process in order to provide the organism tized and granted the necessary time to create suf-
with the possibility of greater choice (more appro- ficient awareness on them to promote change at the
priate self-regulation) and thereby freeing it from procedural level. When, however, Teasdale et al.
typical patterns dictated by procedural memory. (2002) recently differentiated between metacogni-
This process may include mental processing at the tive knowledge and metacognitive insight, they
semantic level but it is not always necessary or even seemed to attune the cognitive approach to such
desirable. If awareness is not consistently directed processes, and thereby also brought it closer to the
toward the procedural level, the individual will keep awareness work at a procedural level in gestalt
on doing what s/he is doing, regardless of how therapy. The point is that metacognitive knowledge
unsuitable this might be in a specific life situation; deals with semantics by referring to “beliefs about
that is, if the behavior is anchored at the procedural cognitive phenomena stored in memory as propo-
level. An individual may often be capable of rec- sitional facts in much the same way as other facts”
ognizing (at a semantic level) the unsuitable aspect (p. 286), while metacognitive insight refers to the
of a particular behavior pattern. For instance, after
some time in therapy one might see that one’s core 4
belief of oneself as being unlovable says more With the same sort of attention Williams illustrates (from
a cognitive frame of reference) the inertia in procedural mem-
about the family one grew up in than it says about
ory changes when related to the effect of trauma. He reminds
oneself. Nonetheless, one continues to live the pro- us that in therapy we should be attentive towards “not only
cedural pattern as if it was true. This procedural what they remember but the way in which they remember it;
pattern will then be activated every time one finds not only their conscious recollection, but their behavioral
oneself in situations with affinity to one’s former memories that have survived long after the initial event that
relationship with one’s parents, with the conse- precipitated them; not only their retrospective memory but
quence that one (independent of one’s semantic their prospective memory (Williams, 1996, p. 111).

596
Gestalt Therapy and Cognitive Therapy

way mental phenomena are experienced as they her/his body). What s/he attends to right now? Or
arise, which points directly to what gestalt therapists to tell her/him that I (the therapist) notice that
would call awareness of the gestalt formation pro- her/his shoulders are rising a little bit, and that I
cess. Interestingly, Teasdale et al. (2002), indepen- would encourage her/him to lift them even more.
dently use the term metacognitive awareness, when The aim is to investigate whether by focusing
they talk about metacognitive insight as “actually awareness on a present, though unaware (proce-
experiencing thoughts as thoughts . . . in the mo- dural) behavior, and through conscious exagger-
ment they occur” (p. 286). ation of the behavior, there could be a facilitation
of the therapeutic process by creating more
Experiential Processes Versus Attributional awareness at precisely the procedural level.
Processes Though there is, again, in principle, nothing
wrong with such interventions and mirroring in a
From a gestalt perspective, the tight structuring cognitive framework, they are not the most ob-
advocated in traditional cognitive therapy of the vious interventions in cognitive therapy. With
individual sessions and the therapy in general reference to their therapeutic guidelines cognitive
with explicit formulations of goals (preferably therapists will presumably tend to look at such
agreed upon and operationalized early on) will be interventions as less productive errands on their
seen as a restriction on the possibility of investi- way. They might seem to be too much of a
gating the procedural patterns in gestalt forma- diversion from the cognitive case conceptualiza-
tions as they evolve and are experienced by the tion, the negotiated goals for the therapy, and the
client in the present, lived moment. Also, from a explicitly formulated agenda items for an indi-
gestalt perspective, the cognitive focus on cogni- vidual session.
tion will seem to be restricting the scope of In this respect gestalt methodology can obvi-
investigation by excluding sensation and aware- ously contribute to creating more room for the
ness of need-based gestalt formation processes in experiential and sensational moments in cogni-
a broader sense. According to Fodor, the cogni- tive practice. On the other hand, the conceptual
tive stance means that “highlighting the moment apparatus of cognitive therapy can contribute to
of experience is often lost in the talking about being more specific in formulating the cognitive
beliefs” (1996, p. 33). It is characteristic of the aspects of the gestalt formation process than has
cognitive-Socratic investigation of thinking (i.e., traditionally been done in gestalt. As also pointed
what goes through your mind right now? what do out by Burley (2004), gestalt formation processes
you tell yourself?) that it invites the client to are not just about sensations and experiences but
make attributions and assumptions about the now, contain meaning ascriptions and interpretations at
which is lived and experienced. In gestalt therapy a fundamental level. In cognitive terminology,
such assumptions are preferably suspended,5 such ascriptions and interpretational elements
while in a sensory, present, almost meditative are called thinking, and according to Fodor
attitude the therapeutic work is oriented toward (1996) it is a mistake to consider them as
sharpening and focusing awareness of the gestalt dichotomous with sensation. To counterpara-
formation process in order to strengthen the or- phrase one of Perls’ famous quotes, there is no
ganismic self-regulation. In that respect it is in- point in loosing our heads when we turn to our
teresting that Naranjo (2000, p. 24) compares the senses. It will be more profitable to use our
(gestalt) therapist’s frequently used support and heads in sensitive ways and to avoid bringing
encouragement of the client to be aware of the ourselves in a delimiting position in which
stream of consciousness with a verbalized inter- work with awareness is restricted to sensation
personal meditation. and experience.
On the other hand, in relation to the cognitive
classic “what are you telling yourself right 5
To be sure this does not mean that gestalt therapists do not
now?”, there is, in principle, nothing that pre- work with thinking or interpretations. The concept of introjec-
vents the therapist from being silent and letting tion in gestalt therapy is, for instance, rather consistent with
the now-moment unfold in its dynamic experien- the cognitive terminology of dysfunctional and irrational be-
tial structure if that is what is considered most liefs. Likewise, gestalt therapists are usually interested in
conducive in the present situation. Neither is it separating sensations from fantasies and ideas (that is, think-
forbidden to ask the client what s/he senses (in ing).

597
Tønnesvang, Sommer, Hammink, and Sonne

The question then is: Are psycho-functionalities - determined how of the individual⬘s schema-
that in the cognitive approach are explained with the activation.
activation of schemas and underlying assumptions -
roughly the same as those which, in the gestalt The Meaning of the Therapeutic Relationship
approach, are explained with reference to gestalt in Therapy
formation processes? Kellogg (2004) thinks so, and
when we define gestalts as dynamic knowledge In the cognitive literature, discussions of the
structures that organize experience, Fodor would therapeutic relationship are often concerned with
agree (1996, p. 34), and so would we—at least how the collaboration between therapist and cli-
generally as an invitation to continue looking at the ent can be maintained or reestablished after in-
approaches as complementarities rather than con- terruptions in order to reinstate the use of stan-
tradictions. dard cognitive (restructuring) methods (Burns &
Auerbach, 1996; Beck, 2005). The general atti-
Mentality of the Individual Versus Field tude seems to be that for a therapy to be success-
Orientation ful it must be done within an atmosphere of
warmth, genuineness, and empathy. Presumably,
One of the fruits of a complementary position because this is considered obvious, discussions of
is an increased attention toward aspects that the the therapeutic relationship and contact are typi-
approaches in themselves would not be attending cally lacking or at least not prioritized in the
to with the same strength. Concerning the relation cognitive literature. Instead, discussions of the
between schemas and contexts in cognitive ther- subject are concerned with the technical direc-
apy, the field orientation of gestalt would, for tions for maintaining the collaborative relation-
instance, more consistently than the cognitive ship and adjusting the standard methods (for the
descriptions of triggering situations investigate specific symptom disorders) so that they might
the contextual circumstances under which spe- become suitable for working with personality-
cific schemas are activated. In cognitive therapy, disturbed clients (Beck et al., 2004; Beck, 2005).
triggering situations are explicitly investigated in The number of problem identifications and meth-
the initial phases of therapy as part of collecting odological suggestions for solutions, created in
data in order to obtain a systematic analysis of the that respect, are definitely inspirational material.
client’s difficulties in everyday life and to formu- Still, they concern the technical aspects of ther-
late hypotheses about the central rules and sche- apy and not the therapist/client contact and rela-
mas that are underlying the different problem tionship as such.
situations of the client. But when the clarification From the observer of the field, a question could
of the client’s maintaining system through a cog- be that if certain techniques alone are what make
nitive case conceptualization has been made— the difference in therapy, then why not just re-
and due to the fact that cognitive therapists place the psychotherapeutic context with a mix-
through situational analyses often manage to ture of self-help books and computer programs—
make the client’s maintaining thought and behav- provided that the computer technique becomes
ioral patterns appear very vivid and clear—it sufficiently sophisticated (Naranjo, 2000, p. 3).
seems as if no further clarification of the field is Aside from our guess that probably not many
necessary to understand the client’s phenomenol- psychotherapists would think of this as a realistic
ogy and actions. While there might be some possibility, two objections arise based on what
advantages to such focused strategies, the poten- Hougaard (2004, p. 622) calls the anthropological
tial disadvantage is— of course—the risk of treat- dilemma of psychotherapy. The first objection
ing the client’s beliefs (and maybe his or her will adduce that experiences of contact and the
disorder in general) as being uninfluenced by the process between people in the therapeutic room
various contexts in which he or she lives (Fodor, with all its ramified implications of lived pres-
1996). With a field orientation as a basic stance, ence between client and therapist are actually
the therapist will - all things being equal - be healing in and of itself. Thus, the contact (as an
more inclined to dwell on which contextual cir- exchange of information between I-ness and oth-
cumstances support or provoke the activation of a erness) in the therapeutic relationship becomes an
particular thought/schema and will, therefore, get end in itself and not only a means. The second
a more thorough understanding of the field- objection will adduce that no matter how sophis-

598
Gestalt Therapy and Cognitive Therapy

ticated and effective our techniques, it still takes up prototypic (and principal) differences between
two to tango; that is, an interpersonal space and the two approaches. We expect both gestalt ther-
processes therein are what make the techniques apists and cognitive therapists to have their per-
work. Thus, the relational contact becomes a nec- sonally anchored strategies which in the diversity
essary means for the healing potential in the of the therapeutic meeting will to some extent
techniques to work. While the prototypical cog- compensate for those aspects of reality that are
nitive stance will be in accordance with the latter not so well described in their preferred theoretical
understanding, the gestalt approach will basically and practical references. By sharpening the lines
match the view that contact has a definitive func- between the perspectives, the aspects of the hu-
tion in the therapeutic work with change pro- man condition that each approach are particularly
cesses. attentive to, become clearer.
To those who are concerned about the evi-
Conclusion dence of effectiveness of gestalt therapy in com-
parison with the effectiveness of cognitive and
By virtue of a common phenomenological cognitive– behavioral therapy, there are some
stance, a compatible understanding of psycholog- outcome studies that compare the effect of the
ical dysfunctionality as related to memory, and a approaches showing that gestalt therapy is at
reasonably common attitude toward the client in least as promising as the more evidence-
the therapeutic room, gestalt therapy and cogni- investigated cognitive approaches. 6 For in-
tive therapy are so sufficiently alike that it seems stance, Beutler et al. (1991) found that gestalt
reasonable to consider the relationship between therapy outperformed cognitive therapy in
the approaches in integrative terms (not just tech- treating depressed persons. Johnson and Smith
nically or from an eclectic stance, but also theo- (1997) found that in the treatment of a phobia,
retically). Precisely because they are also suffi- the gestalt technique of the empty chair and
ciently different in several respects, they have the cognitive desensitization seemed to work
potential of being complementary in clinical equally well in contrast to the no treatment
practice. Although we have argued that the ge- group, and that the gestalt group in contrast to
stalt formation process and the processes con- the desensitization group indicated a variety of
cerning development and maintenance of sche- benefits beyond the focus of therapy. Watson
mas in essence are to be considered as synonyms
(2006) compared 40 clients classified with ei-
describing the same phenomena, the two ap-
ther good or poor outcome in either cognitive
proaches are at the same time focusing the pro-
behavior therapy or manualized gestalt therapy,
cesses differently. While cognitive therapists are
and showed that good responders of either ther-
mostly concerned about what the machinery
apeutic approach exhibited superior levels of
does, and how it can be modified, gestalt thera-
emotional processing and that clients receiving
pists are mostly concerned about how the ma-
chinery works, and how the working processes gestalt therapy ended up being superior to cog-
can be facilitated. Precisely this attention regard- nitive behavior clients in emotional processing.
ing different aspects of the same process phe- And, finally, to mention the classic study of
nomena makes a mutual dialogue between the emotionally focused couples therapy by John-
approaches on their complementarities a worth- son and Greenberg (1985), it was shown that
while endeavor - a dialogue in which neither of cognitive behavior therapy and gestalt ap-
the positions should be reduced to a derivation of proaches in a general comparison turned out to
the other. be equivalent, and that there is a benefit to
Among the themes that we have been working gestalt therapy that does not appear in cogni-
with in the article, we can sum up the following tive therapies, namely that those who receive
characteristics of the positions in the hope that gestalt therapy continue to improve after the
they may stimulate a continuing dialogue among end of therapy rather than to simply hold the
advocates of gestalt and cognitive therapy regard-
ing the complementarities between the ap- 6
We appreciate the discussion with Todd Burley and the
proaches (Table 1). references provided by him on this subject during our meeting
To avoid a misreading of the content in the in the Aarhus Research Group in Gestalt (ARGG) on Novem-
table, it should be kept in mind that it is summing ber 3, 2009.

599
Tønnesvang, Sommer, Hammink, and Sonne

TABLE 1. Characteristics of Gestalt and Cognitive Therapy change: change happens when we become what
we are, without trying to change who we are
Cognitive therapy Gestalt therapy
(Beisser, 1970). Further, there is a remarkably
Individual orientation Field orientation strong resemblance between the gestalt as-
Schema-activation Gestalt formation process sumption about the paradox of change and the
Types of schemas Gestalt process diagnosis
Cognition Experience and sensation
dialectical view of the relationship between
Explicit goals and tight Awareness on here-and-now accept and change as it is seen in dialectical
structure as guiding as guiding principle and behavior therapy, in which we find an explicit
principle and focus focus adaptation of Zen-philosophy (Swales &
The therapist as expert onThe therapist as expert on Heard, 2007; Kåver & Nilsonne, 2004) (which
truth, including the contact-processes,
pedagogical element including the therapeutic has been part of gestalt since the early years).
license to be creative The client is supposed to both accept who s/he
Modifying modus – working Facilitating modus – working is and undergo change. Through development
with collaborative with phenomenological of self-acceptance—which to a large extent is
empiricism experimentation
transmitted through the therapist’s acceptance
and validation of the client’s here and now
existence—the client becomes capable of self-
progress that they have made. The last result transformation in the next now moment
might be precisely a result of gestalt therapy (Swales & Heard, 2007, p. 187). Gestalt ther-
working at the procedural level. In general, apists and some cognitive therapists now seem
there seems to be no reason for concern regard- to agree that change is to be understood in this
ing the scientific and practical soundness of paradoxical sense. That when we have basi-
either of the two approaches we have discussed cally become reconciled to ourselves and to
in this article. Following Beutler’s (2009) reality as it is, then we have often already
warning against oversimplification of psycho- changed quite a lot.
therapy through methodological one-sidedness,
our ‘article of faith’ (p. 306) will be, that mean- References
ingful disagreements between researchers and
practitioners with advanced capacities for per- BECK, A. T. (1996). Beyond belief: A theory of modes,
spective taking is the main road toward a future personality and psychopathology. In P. M. Salkovskis
integrative psychotherapy that has transcended (Ed.), Frontiers in cognitive therapy (pp. 1–25). New
York: Guilford Press.
sectarianism and included the most valuable BECK, A. T., FREEMAN, A., & DAVIS, D. D. (2004).
parts of our existing knowledge of what really Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. New York:
matters. Guilford Press.
BECK, J. S. (2005). Cognitive therapy for challenging
problems—what to do when the basics don⬘t work. New
A Closing Remark About Some Remarkabilities York: Guilford Press.
BEISSER, A. (1970). The paradoxical theory of change. In
In combination with the earlier mentioned J. Fagan & I. Shepherd (Eds.), Gestalt therapy now:
attention toward metacognitive awareness as Theory, techniques, applications (pp. 77– 80). Palo Alto:
formulated by Teasdale et al. (2002), it is quite Science and Behavior Books.
interesting that meditation and mindfulness BEUTLER, L. E., ENGLE, D., MOHR, D., DALDRUP, R. J.,
BERGAN, J., MERIDITH, K., & MERRY, W. (1991). Dif-
have appeared in - what has been called - a ferential response to cognitive, experiential, and self
third wave or a second revolution within cog- directed psychotherapeutic procedures. Journal of
nitive therapy (Wøllestad, 2007; Holden, Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 333–340.
2008). The aim of the meditative techniques is BEUTLER, L. E. (2009). Making science matter in clinical
practice: Redefining psychotherapy. Clinical Psychol-
partly to cultivate an open and accepting atti- ogy: Science and Practice, 16, 3, 301–317.
tude toward one’s experiences without meeting BURLEY, T. (2004). A phenomenologically based theory
them with judgment or steering them in any of personality. In T. Burley (Ed.), Gatla reader, Sum-
particular direction and partly to cultivate the mer 2004, (pp. 23–38). Santa Monica: Gestalt Associ-
ability to reconcile oneself with reality as it is, ates Training Los Angeles (GATLA).
BURLEY, T. (2005). Explanatory notes for gestalt process
without wanting to change it (Neff, 2003). This diagnosis. Santa Monica: Gestalt Associates Training
attitude is astonishingly similar to the assump- Los Angeles (GATLA).
tion in gestalt therapy about paradoxical BURLEY, T., & FREIER, C. M. (2004). Character structure:

600
Gestalt Therapy and Cognitive Therapy

A gestalt-cognitive theory. Psychotherapy: Theory, Re- dress]. European Congress of Behavioral and Cognitive
search, Practice, Training, 41, 321–331. Therapies, London.
BURNS, D. D., & AUERBACH, A. (1996). Therapeutic PARLETT, M. (1991). Reflections on field theory. The
empathy in cognitive-behavioral therapy: Does it re- British Gestalt Journal, 1, 69 – 81.
ally make a difference? In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), PERLS, F. (1947/1969). Ego, hunger and aggression. New
Frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 135–164). New York: Vintage Books.
York: Wiley. PERLS, F. S., HEFFERLINE, R. F., & GOODMAN, P. (1951/
DAMASIO, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body, 1994). Gestalt therapy—Excitement and growth in the
emotion and the making of consciousness. London: Wil- human personality. New York: Vintage Books.
liam Heinemann. RESNICK, R. W. (1995). Gestalt therapy: Principles, prism
DECI, E. L., & RYAN, R. M. (2000). The “what” and and perspectives. British Gestalt Journal, 4, 3–13.
“why” of goal pursuit: Human needs and the self- ROSENBERG, N., & MØRCH, M. (2005). Kognitiv terapi
determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, [Cognitive therapy]. In Merete M. Mørch & Nicole K.
227–268. Rosenberg (Eds.), Kognitiv terapi, modeller og metoder
FODOR, I. E. (1996). A cognitive perspective for gestalt [Cognitive therapy, models and methods] (pp. 17– 45).
therapy. British Gestalt Journal, 5, 31– 42. Copenhagen: Hans Reitzel.
HOLDEN, B. (2008). Aksept og forpliktelsesterapi: En SOMMER, U., & TØNNESVANG, J. (2008). Gestalt terapi
atferdsanalytisk psykoterapi [Acceptance and com- og kognitiv terapi— en mulighed for gensidig be-
mitment therapy: A behavioral analytic psychother- frugtning. [Gestalt therapy and cognitive therapy—a
apy]. Tidsskrift for Norsk Psykologforening, 9, 1118 – potential fruitful relationship]. Psyke & Logos, 29,
1126. 284 –309.
HOLM, L. (2001). Observation af patientens adfærd [Ob- SONNE, M. (1998, JULY). The role of the experiment in
servation of the patients behaviour]. In I. Oestrich & L. gestalt therapy. Unpublished paper, presented at Gatla,
Holm (Eds.), Kognitiv miljøterapi (pp. 30 –34). Copen- European Residental, Vevey, Schweiz.
hagen: Dansk Psykologisk Forlag. STAEMMLER, F. M. (2006). A babylonian confusion? On
HOUGAARD, E. (2004). Psykoterapi, teori og forskning the uses and meanings of the term “field”. British Ge-
[Psychotherapy, theory and research]. København: stalt Journal, 15, 64 – 83.
Dansk Psykologisk Forlag. SWALES, A. M., & HEARD, L. H. (2007). The therapy
JOHNSON, S., & GREENBERG, L. (1985). Emotionally fo-
relationship in dialectical behavior therapy. In P. Gil-
cused couples therapy: An outcome study. Journal of
bert & R. L. Leahy (Eds.), The therapeutic relationship
Marital and Family Therapy, 11, 313–317.
in the cognitive behavioral psychotherapies (pp. 185–
JOHNSON, W. R., & SMITH, E. W. L. (1997). Gestalt
204) New York: Routledge.
empty-chair dialogue versus systematic desensitization
TEASDALE, J. D., POPE, M., MOORE, R. G., HAYWURST,
in the treatment of a phobia. Gestalt Review, 1, 150 –
H., WILLAMS, S., & SEGAL, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive
162.
awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Em-
KÅVER, A., & NILSONNE, Å. (2004). Grundbog i dialek-
tisk adfærdsterapi [Textbook of dialectical behavioural pirical evidence. Journal of Counseling and Clinical
therapy]. København: Dansk Psykologisk Forlag. Psychology, 70, 275–287.
KELLOGG, S. (2004). Dialogical encounters: Contempo- TILLICH, P. (1948). The shaking of the foundations. New
rary perspectives on “chairwork.” Psychotherapy: The- York: Scribner’s.
ory, Research, Practice, Training, 41, 310 –320. TØNNESVANG, J. (2007). Gestalt in other contexts – be
KELLY, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal con- aware of the basics and the fields [Conference Manu-
structs. London: Routledge. script, PowerPoint presentation]. Gestalt Therapy in
LEWIN. (1951). The field approach: Culture and group life Other Contexts than the Therapeutic. Conference pre-
as quasi-stationary processes. In W. L. French et al. sented at the Department of Psychology, Aarhus Uni-
(Eds.). Organization, development and transformation. versity, Aarhus, Denmark.
Managing effective change (pp. 112). New York: TØNNESVANG, J., HAMMINK, J., & SONNE, M. (2007).
McGraw-Hill. Integrativ gestalt praksis: Et eksperiment over 4 mod-
MCADAMS, D. P., & PALS, J. L. (2006). A new big five: uler. [Integrative gestalt practice: An experiment
Fundamental principles for an integrative science of consisting of 4 modules]. Unpublished course mate-
personality. American Psychologist, 61, 204 –217. rial (PowerPoint presentation), Aarhus: Gestalt
MØRCH, M. M. (2005). Kognitiv Terapi ved person- Training Center.
lighedsforstyrrelser [Cognitive therapy and personality TULVING, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Ox-
disorders]. In M. M. Mørch & N. K. Rosenberg (Eds.), ford: Clarendon Press.
Kognitiv terapi - modeller og metoder (197–224). TULVING, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Cana-
Copenhagen: Hans Reitzel. dian Psychology, 26, 1–12.
NARANJO, C. (2000). Gestalt therapy – The attitude & WATSON, J. (2006). Clients’ emotional processing in
practice of an atheoretical experimentalism. United psychotherapy: A comparison between cognitive-
Kingdom: Crown House Publishing. behavioral and process-experiential therapies. Jour-
NEFF, K. (2003). Self-compassion: An alternative concep- nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 152–
tualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self 159.
and Identity, 2, 85–101. WILLIAMS, M. G. J. (1996). Memory processes in psycho-
PADESKY, C. (1993, SEPTEMBER). Socratic questioning: therapy. In P. M Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers of cognitive
Changing minds or guiding discovery? [Key-note ad- therapy (pp. 97–113). New York: Wiley.

601
Tønnesvang, Sommer, Hammink, and Sonne

WØLLESTAD, J. (2007). Opmerksomt nærvær: Meditative therapies (pp. 328 –367). Belmont: Thomson Brooks/
teknikker som utgangspunkt for psykologisk behan- Cole. Also published at: www.gestalt.org
dling. [Being attentively present: Psychological treat- YOUNG, J. E., KLOSKO, J. S., & WEISHAAR, M. E. (2003).
ment based on meditative techniques]. Tidsskrift for Schema therapy: A practitioner⬘s guide. New York:
Norsk Psykologforening, 7, 860 – 867. Guilford Press.
YONTEF, G., & JACOBS, L. (2008). Gestalt Therapy. In ZINKER, J. (1978). Creative process in gestalt therapy. New
R. J. Corsini & D. Wedding (Eds.). Current Psycho- York: Vintage Books.

602

You might also like