You are on page 1of 58

Characterisation through

Language:
Perspectives from Stylistics

PIERGIORGIO TREVISAN

Gratitude to:

UNIVERSITÀ
DEGLI STUDI
Dl UDINE

The volume has been published with the financial support


of the Department of Languages and Literature,
Communication, Education and Social Studies (DILL)
of the University of Udine (Italy). BICA
Table of Contents

15 Preface

PART 1: THE STUDY OF CHARACTER

19 What is a character?
24 Mimesis and 'anti-mimesis'
27 The 'dissolution' of character
27 The Morphology of the Folk Tale
29 Structuralism
31 Bringing all together: 'stylistics' models
34 Conclusion
First published in Great Britain in December 2019
by Bica Publishing
bicapublishing@bicagroup.com PART 2: A TOOLKIT FOR THE STUDY OF CHARACTERISATION
© 2019 Bica Publishing 37 Introduction
39 'Top-down' processes
All rights reserved. No part of this book maybe reprinted or reproduced or uti-
lised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical or other means, now known or 39 Prototype theory and Schema theory
hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information
46 Social cognition
storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers.
49 'Inferring'characters
ISBN 978-0-9931724-6-5
51 Bottom-up processes
ISSN 2058- 7929 52 Narratorial control
55 Self and other presentation
Preface
56 Implicit and explicit information about characters
61 Narratorial 'filters'
61 Point of view
65 Mind style
66 Mind style and narratology
69 Mind style and metaphorical patterns
72 Mind style and deictical patterns
75 Mind style and pragmatics
78 Mind style and under/over-lexicalisation
81 Mind style and corpus stylistics
83 Transitivity patterns and characterisation
88 Dialogue and characterisation
88 Politeness and impoliteness
This book is about the 'inhabitants' of fictions: characters. More than ever
92 Expressive identity before, our daily lives are surrounded by them, in a plethora of different
93 Characters as 'inhabitants of possible worlds' ways: we encounter characters in literature, poetry, drama, movies, TV
shows, etc. Sometimes they 'migrate' from one medium to another, or be-
97 Characters and 'intertextuality '
come 'serial' in one single medium for decades. Moreover, characters may
98 Conclusion become so central in people's lives, that identification phenomena may
arise: we have all experienced what it means to 'see the world' through the
eyes of a character we love, to 'feel' that the character of a book we are
PART 3: A CASE-STUDY: 'THE BODY ARTIST'
reading would behave in a certain way in a situation we are experiencing
101 Introduction in our 'real' world. Fiction can have a dramatic impact on our lives, as the
TV show binge-watching phenomenon testifies every day: millions of peo-
102 Chara~terisation through 'fictional world' features
ple spend a great deal of their time following the lives and adventures of
103 The impact of character presentation modes the characters they love, feeling good or bad because of them, sometimes
105 Characterisation through lexis imitating their actions or their speech patterns.
111 Characterisation through 'mind-style' Despite all this, crucial theoretical issues are still hotly debated by crit-
ics: what is a character? how are characters different from human beings?
113 Characterisation through conversation and politeness patterns how shall they be approached, from a critical perspective? Centuries of
115 Transitivity patterns and characterisation fiction writing have not been of great help from this point of view: several
readers know that Don Quixote (1605) was the first character in the his-
118 Characterisation through a newly-expanded narratology: actions
tory of literature and one of the most studied, yet very few of them would
as thoughts, intennental thinking doubly embedded narratives
probably analyse him using different tools from those used to understand
122 Characterisation and intertextuality real people. At most levels, characters continue to be studied as if they
123 Structure of the novel and characterisation were human beings, without considering the fact that they are, first and
124 Conclusion foremost, the product of one writer's imagination and, most importantly,
they are communicated via language, that is to say, with words, so words
127 References are how characters are realised in the 'real world'. When analysing them,
as will be discussed, 'textual' properties also need to be considered, along Ruqaiya Hasan's claim that "in verbal art the role of language is central.
with the 'human-like' attributes. Here language is not as clothing to the body; it IS the body" (1985, 91).
Broadly speaking, these issues have been addressed by two main This book is therefore both about character and about characterisation.
schools of thought: the first one has been referred to as 'humanising', since Though obviously related, the two terms have different meanings: the first
it tends to investigate characters as if they were real people; the second one one is the 'output' of the second, since character is the 'result' of all the
has been called 'de-humanising', since the approach is purely textual and strategies used by an author in order to impress a certain image in the mind
tends to investigate characters as if they were only "patterns of recurrence" of the readers. The first Part, in particular, is more concerned with charac-
(Weinsheimer 1979, 195), "functions" which are constantly dissolved into ter, even if some crucial theoretical aspects are inevitably addressed. The
'textuality' (see Culpeper 2001, 6-7). Both approaches are equally prob- second and third Parts are more concerned with characterisation, therefore
lematic, as they fail to take into account crucial attributes of the category: with all the strategies employed for producing particular characters, with
the textual one in the first case, the 'human-like' in the second. distinct traits and psychological aspects.
By building and significantly expanding the models discussed in a
previous volume of the author (Trevisan 2008), this book aims at finding
a compromise between the two positions, i.e. at corroborating the study
of so-called 'mixed approaches' (Culpeper 200 I, 9), i.e. approaches that
account for both dimensions of characters and, at the same time, for the
readers' cognitive processes.
This book is divided into three main parts. The first one is concerned
with the 'evolution' of characters in the last few centuries: as will be shown,
characters have undergone drastic changes with the passing of time, espe-
cially towards the end of the 191h century. Their 'construction strategies'
have changed a lot throughout the centuries, resulting in what we may call
a divergent 'species' of character. Very interestingly, the analytical theo-
ries have followed the same path: 'humanising' approaches have gradually
been replaced by 'de-humanising' ones, to the point that characters have
been analysed as 'actants', or 'roles' nested in the syntactic patterns of the
narrative.
The second part of the volume is entirely devoted to the construc-
tion of a model for characterisation: starting from existing theories, several
points are addressed for outlining a coherent and comprehensive approach
taking into account both the psychological and the textual dimensions of
characters. Very importantly, the model can be applied not only to nar-
rative, but also to drama and TV shows. In other words, the theoretical
framework is positioned within the tradition of' stylistics': when some ob-
servations are drawn about the psychology of a particular character, for
example, they are always 'bottom-up' in origin, since they originate from
a close scrutiny of the language used in the texts. The chapters, introduced
and discussed with many examples, are to be conceived as a guide for ad-
dressing different aspects of characterisation. As will be explained, not all
the features discussed are always present at the same time, yet they mostly
work in combination with the aim of producing a clear-cut image of char-
acters in readers' minds.
ln the third section, the discussions outlined in Part 2 are further ap-
plied to a case-study: the novel The Body Artist by the American writer
Don DeLillo. The central role of stylistics is reinforced here, in line with
Part 1: The study of character

1.1 WHAT IS A CHARACTER?

In an episode of the British TV series Black Mirror called The Waldo Mo-
ment, the protagonist Jamie Salter is a failed comedian who, for a living,
performs the voice and movements of a cartoon character called Waldo.
Originally designed for children, Waldo suddenly becomes extremely pop-
ular after provocatively addressing politicians by means of screens posi-
tioned on the side of the road: while in a van parked near the screens, James
Salter animates Waldo and 'gives voice' to him when politicians walk by
the screens. What initially seems to be a funny game enjoyed by the politi-
cians themselves, soon turns into a totally unexpected social phenomenon
whereby Waldo is acclaimed as the symbol of a new political era and asked
by the public at large to run for the next presidential elections.
In the space of a TV episode, The Waldo Moment addresses crucial is-
sues regarding the ontology of fictional characters: despite being 'non-ex-
~istent entities', they are largely present in people's lives; sometimes they
have the power to significantly influence real world events 1, or to change
over time and 'migrate' from one medium to another in order to reach
Gider audiences. However, their existence is dependent on some authors
(a writer, a screen producer) providing 'life' for them: from this point of
view, their status is tha emiotic constructs, i.e. entities that consist
of mediatedsigns. Waldo, for example, is first and foremosta 'linguistic'
artefact depending on the decisions of his author, James Salter - himself a
fictional character. Yet, it would be hard and perhaps a bit unfair to think

1
It will be sufficient to think of how many people committed suicide to imitate
fictional characters like Goethe's Werther, or Flaubert's Madame Bovary.
exclusively of characters in terms of 'linguistic signs': as we all know, they character" (1988, 39). A 'double ontology' seems to emerge from Lodge's
are much more than that.
As these preliminary observations make clear, defining the nature of
characters is a crucial starting point for identifying the best analytical tools
T extract: characters are both 'signs on a page', and 'entities' projecting am-
bitions, desires and frustrations very similarly to human beings. Therefore,
they can be approached at least from two points of view: 'textual' and
[ for understanding and analysing them. Authors and critics have been con- _ 'psychological'.
cerned with this for a very long time, and sometimes discussions about More generally, as Eder et al. (2010, 8) have suggested, discussions
the ontology of characters have even been incorporated within the fiction- focusing on characters' ontology can be grouped into four main positions:
al works themselves. The British author David Lodge, for example, has
addressed this issue in the presentation of Robyn, one of the two central 1. Semiotic theories consider characters to be signs or structures of
characters in his novel Nice Work (1989): fictional texts.

According to Robyn (or, more precisely, according to the writers who 2. Cognitive approaches assume that characters are representations
have influenced her thinking on these matters), there is no such thing of imaginary beings in the minds of the audience.
as the 'self' on which capitalism and the classic novel are founded 3. Some philosophers believe that characters are abstract objects be-
- that is to say, a finite, unique soul or essence that constitutes a yond material reality.
persons identity; there is only a subject position in an infinite web of
discourses - the discourses of power. sex, family, science, religion, 4. Other philosophers contend that characters do not exist at all.
poetry, etc. And by the same token, there is no such thing as an
Each position has important implications for the analysis of characters:
author; that is to say, one who originates a work of art ab nihilo.
position 4, in particular, is the most extreme of them since it implies the
Every text is a product of intertextuality, a tissue of allusions to and
investigation of character only in terms of yet another 'textual element'
citations of other texts; and, in the famous words of Jacques Derrida,
like narrative technique, time, point of view, description, etc. According to
(famous to people like Robyn, anyway), 'il n '.Y a pas de hors-texte',
position 4, in other words, characters and real people have nothing in com-
there is nothing outside the text. There are no origins, there is only
!11Qn, a reason why no reference tohwruin beings shoula ever be made in
production, and we produce our 'selves' in language. Not 'you are
t~ygs. P_£,si!!.£!LU.s....s.omehow similar-but not.so extremexcharacters
what you eat', but 'you are what you speak' 01~ rather, 'you are what
ar~rt of the .'..faruic' o~t§_ip which they are embedd~ however
speaks you' is the axiomatic basis of Robyn s philosophy, which she
this does not entail their_nQp_~istence; in fact, it is not uncommon for
would call, if required to give it a name, 'semiotic materialism'. It
characters to 'migrate' from one book to the other, or from a book to a
might seem a bit bleak, a bit inhuman ('anti-humanist, yes; inhuman,
movie, TV series, or live show. Therefore, they seem to have an existence
no'; she would interject), somewhat deterministic ('not at all, the
that goes "beyond purely textual boundaries" (McIntyre 2014, 150):__this
truly determined subject is he who is not aware of the discursive
is in line with position 3 claimino that characters are abstract ob· ects be-
formations that determine him. Or her', she would add scrupulously
YQ.!:d matena rea ity, i.e. "something aveuncL.a.b0-¥e-St~~"
being among other things a feminist). But in practice this doesn't
(Eder et al. 2010, 132), a statement that admits the possibility of creating a
seem to affect her behaviour very noticeably - she seems to have
character before creating a story (Eder et al. 2010, 116). Position 1, finally,
ordinary human feelings, ambitions, desires, to suffer anxieties,
adds to position 3 the account.of.how real reader~ompr.ehenclcharacters:
frustrations, fears, like anyone else in this imperfect world, and to
this is a crucial perspectiYe_,__since personal_r_eading experiences are ofte
have a natural inclination to try and make it a better place. I shall
in conflict with literary criticaLac.counts af characters (see also McIntyre
take the liberty of treating her as a character, not utterly different in
2014, 151 ). Position 2 is also...tb.e_o,n4! one that accommodates-aJ+.al:ld~nce;
kind, though of course belonging to a very different social species,
therefore, it is in tune with 'pragmatic concerns.'..., i.e. concerns that revolve
from Vic Wilcox (Lodge 1989, 40-41).
around.the communicatioJLOLineanmg-s (Culpeper and Fernandez-Quinta-

[
Even though characters are a 'product oflanguage', and 'there is noth- nilla 2017, 94). To be noted, 'imaginary beings' are mentioned for the first
ing outside the text', no 'origin' but only 'production', the author lets us time: this admits for the possibility of characters being not only actual peo-
know that Robyn seems to have ordinary human feelings and anxieties like ple or representations of actual people, but also anthropomorphic animal
everyone else in the world; therefore, he will take the liberty of treating characters like those found in Disney or science fiction.
her as a character, even if in the previous page he had pointed out that Despite these philosophical and semantic perspectives, however, it
"rather awkwardly for me, [she] doesn't herself believe in the concept of could not be denied that, for a very long time, it was taken for granted
that characters represented human beings, that novels were about people, be present in human nature, or, if there is no omission, to what
and that psychological motives sustained plots. According to Henry James, distortion of these elements is it due that such beings as some of
characters had to be considered 'representations' ofreal people: similarly these come to exist? (Bradley 1926, 264).
[to paintings, they needed "a basis in life without being mere copies of
Jexisting people", and authors had to "mediate and interpret human experi- Bradley was not the only critic who approached characters as if they
ence through them" (James 1984, 1322). For this reason, at least until the were real people: in those same decades, following Freud's groundbreak-
1920s, characters were mostly referred to as 'people', both by critics and ',ing contributions to modern psychology, so-called 'psychoanalytic ap-
by authors: E.M. Forster, for instance, used the term 'People' in the title of 1proaches' became so popular that expressions like 'Oedipus complex',
his two renowned chapters focusing on characters in Aspects of the Novel. I 'consequences of sexual repression', 'death wish', to name but a few, were
"Since the actors in a story are usually human", he explained, "it seems commonly used in characters' analyses:
convenient to entitle this aspect People" (Forster 1954 [1927], 30).
, The very first critical approaches to characters, therefore, tended to The underlying theme relates ultimately to the splitting of the mother
image which the infantile unconscious effects into two opposite
j 'humanise' them, a reason why these models have been called 'humanising
approaches' (Culpeper 2001, 6): in Shakespearian Tragedy, for example, pictures: one of a virginal Madonna, an inaccessible saint towards
the critic A.C. Bradley talks about Shakespeare's characters in a manner whom all sensual approaches are unthinkable, and the other of a
that could be very similar to the way in which we could talk about real sensual creature accessible to everyone . . When sexual repression is
people: highly pronounced, as with Hamlet, then both types of women are
felt to be hostile: the pure one out of resentment at her repulses, the
So he comes before us, dark and grand, with a light upon him from sensual one out of the temptation she offers to plunge into guiltiness.
the sun where he was born; but no longer young and now graved, Misogyny, as in the play, is the inevitable result (Jones 1949, 97-8).
self-controlled, steeled by the experience of countless perils,
hardships and vicissitudes, at once simple and stately in bearing and Language, as can be seen, was hardly mentioned in these types of
in speech a great man naturally modest but fully conscious of his analyses, nor were other crucial narrative features like narration, point of
worth, proud of his services to the state, unawed by dignitaries and hiew, speech and thought presentation, etc. 'Humanising' approaches, in
unelated by honours, secure, it would seem, against all dangers from other words, were in line with readers' text experiences, according to the
without and all rebellion from within (Bradley 1926, 189). notion that "what we all do when we read a book, or watch a play or film
iato at©n-pU.oJnl.qpret characters, despite their imaginary status, in large
His chief desire is not by any means to ensure his mothers silent part with knowledge about people acquired through our real life experienc-
acquiescence in his design of revenge; it is to have her soul. And es" (Culpeper 200, 6- 7).
while the' rough work of vengeance is repugnant to him, he is at home After 1920, however, things began to change: critics started to express
in this higher work. [. . .] The truth is that, though Hamlet hates his dissatisfaction with the treatment of characters solely in a 'person-like'
uncle and acknowledges the duty of vengeance, his whole heart is way, therefore new perspectives began to emerge. In Modern Fiction, for
never in this feeling or this task; but his whole heart is in his horror instance, Virginia Woolf pointed out that her characters were all but 'tra-
at his mother 's fall and in his longing to raise her (Bradley 1926, ditionally constructed', thus implicitly implying that fictional entities and
138).
human beings did not have much in common:
Both in Othello's (first extract) and in Hamlet's analysis (second ex-
Examine for a moment an ordinary mind in an ordinary day. The
tract), no reference is made to the linguistic features characterising the
mind receives myriad impressions - trivial, fantastic, evanescent,
texts: the characters are treatecLaS-iL.tbey were real.men, and past and
or engraved with the sharpness of steel. From all sides they come,
future lives are even imagined for them. Things are not dissimilar in the
an incessant shower of innumerable atoms; and as they fall, as they
extract below, when Bradley analyses King Lear's traits starting from a
shape themselves into the life of Monday or Tuesday, the accent falls
series of extra-textual considerations:
differentlyfrom of old[. . .] If a writer were a free man and not a
How can there be such men and women? We ask ourselves. How slave, ifhe could write what he chose, not what he must, ifhe could
comes it that humanity can take such absolutely opposite forms? base his work upon his own feeling and not upon convention, there
And, in particular, to what omissions of elements which should would be no plot, no tragedy, no love interest or catastrophe in the
accepted stvle. and perhaps not a single button sewn on as the Bond care to obscure the persons by such different circumstances, degrees
Street tailors would have it (1921, 189; my emphasis). and colours, that it will be impossible to guess at them with any
degree of certainty; and if it ever happens otherwise, it is only where
Very interestingly, Woolf's extract pointed to two different directions at the the failure characterised is so minute, that it is a foible only which
~
I same time: on the one hand, she expressed her dissatisfaction with.rhe.tra-
diti,QI1al..pmG~s~-oLc.haractro construction - i.e. with what would today
be called 'characterisation:; on the other hand, by clearly differentiating
"ordinary minds" (i.e. 'real' people's minds) from fictional ones, she was
the party himself may laugh at as well as any other (Fielding 1970
[1742], XXII).
After 1885, however, presentation techniques started to change, as
Henry James's production well shows: more traditional characterisation
actually expressing her point of view regarding the distance among the
characters in her novels and the characters in the literature of the past. strategies dominated his novels until The Bostonians (published serially
·n 1885-1886), whereas from The Princess Casamassima (published seri-
Crucially, then, in order to fully understand why additional approach-
ally in those same years) onwards, characters had been introduced "as if
es began to be used along with the more traditional 'humanising' ones,
readers were already very familiar with them" (Genette 1987, 57): for this
it is necessary to reflect upon the changes that occurred to characters at
the beginning of the 20th century. As a matter of fact, a 'different type of ---reason, detailed descriptions of their physical features were not necessary
character' seemed to emerge at the end of the 19th century, and an entirely ~Y more.
'new' one was already in Woolf's mind in 1921, when the extract above These changes became even more evident in the first decades of the
was composed. The 'new' characters and the 'new' theories were not un- 20th century, after Albert Einstein, Henri Bergson, William James and Sig-
related phenomena. mund Freud had heavily affected traditional scientific and philosophical
frameworks. As the novels by WoQlf, Joyce and Proust showed, writers had
~ome increasing4Lc.oncemed__wi.tlllheir charactersminds and their inner
conflicts,-..a reason wh..y_characterjsation_techniques drastically.evolved. As
1.2 MIMESIS AND 'ANTI-MIMESIS' D.H. Lawrence pointed out in 1914, the 'old-fashioned' representation of
characters was not interesting anymore:
The development of representational, mimetic analytical methods like the
ones discussed above was, at least in part, related to the 'types' of charac- Somewhat that which is physic, not human, in humanity, is more
ters produced by traditional literature: throughout the centuries, novelists interesting to me than the old fashioned human element, which
had repeatedly created their fictional 'prototypes' by copying them from causes one to conceive a character in a certain moral scheme and
'the book of nature'; as a consequence, characters and real people had tra- make it consistent. In Turgenev, and in Tolstoy, and in Dostoevskij,
ditionally shared several attributes and behaviours. the moral scheme into which almost all the characters fit - and it
As Gerard Genette (1987, 57-58) made clear some decades later, how- is nearly the same scheme - is, whatever the extraordinary of the
ever, things had begun to change around the 1880s: in carrying out what characters themselves, dull, old, dead. [. . .] You mustn't look in my
he called ironically 'a simple experiment' with the help of a six-year-old novels for the old stable ego o[the characters. There is another ego.
child, he analysed the opening pages of several novels produced before according to whose action the individual is unrecognizable. and
1850, and compared them with later productions. In doing so, he noticed passes through. as it were. allotropic states which it needs a deeper
that a significant change had occurred around 1885: until that moment, sense than any we've been used to exercise. to discover are states of
character presentations had mainly been carried out by means of very de- the same single radically-unchanged element (in Huxley 1932, 198;
tailed descriptions that started from their body parts and culminated in a my emphasis).
careful account of every single element of their face. As a consequence,
Both Virginia Woolf and D.H. Lawrence were therefore concerned
during the eighteenth and the nineteenth centuries, characters had become
with very similar phenomena: the "incessant shower of innumerable at-
so similar to human beings that their authors had often felt they needed to
oms" (Woolf's extract above) or the "allotropic states" (Lawrence's ex-
provide 'precautionaryexplanations to avoid risks ofreader identification:
tract) inherently characterising and influencing people's lives. From the
And here I solemnly protest I have no intention to vilify or asperse point of view of a character's construction, the most immediate conse-
anyone; for though everything is copied from the book of nature, and quence was that accurate body descriptions were now replaced by detailed
,
scarce a character or action produced which I have not taken from accounts of internal thoughts and states of minds, mainly reported by in-
my own observations and experience; yet I have used the utmost novative techniques like free indirect speech and interior monologue (see
.IPart 2 of this volume). Very little was now told about characters by narra-
. tors: most of the information was instead shown by means of inner speech
or thought representations2•
1.3 THE 'DISSOLUTION' OF CHARACTER

Along with the advent of new character types, theories more con- 1.3.1 The Morphology of the Folk Tale
cerned with 'textual' aspects started to emerge. The most significant one
In 1928, the Russian scholar Vladimir Propp formulated an approach to
is perhaps represented by E.M Forster's acclaimed work Aspects of the
narrative structure where characters had a central role. In particular, by
Novel (1927): although the two chapters devoted to characters were called
analysing 115 Russian folk tales, he singled out 31 underlying narrative
'People', the connection with the texts in which they were embedded was
functions performed by 8 character types: as far as characterisation was
central to the work. In particular, besides the renowned distinction between
concerned, they performed the role of 'doers' (e.g. hero, villain, victim,

I
Flat and Round characters', Forster focused on the importance of narrative
etc.) without any reference to the psychological motives that might have
techniques like point of view for the construction of characters, along with
motivated them to carry out the deeds. By way of illustration, the list of
other textual aspects like patterns and rhythms.
functions is given below (Propp 1968 [1928], 21):
Despite its pioneering nature, this model became a recurrent point of
reference for approaches to character analysis in the following decades: 1. One of the members of a family leaves home.
William Harvey in 1965, Baruch Hochman in 1985, and David Fishelov in
1990, for example, all referred back to it during their attempts to single out 5. An interdiction is addressed to the hero.
a 'typological taxonomy' of characters in literature. 6. The interdiction is violated.
After Forster, the past tendencies to imagine previous or future lives
for the characters tended to disappear. Indeed, as the English critic L.C. 7. The villain makes an attempt at reconnaissance.
Knights pointed out in an essay provocatively called How Many Children 8. The villain receives information about his victim.
Had Lady Macbeth?, good criticism could only occur if textual elements
were also taken into account: 9. The villain attempts to deceive his victim in order to take posses-
sion of him or of his belongings.
The main difference between good and bad critics is that the good
critic points to something that is actually contained in the work of 1 O. The victim submits to deception and thereby unwittingly helps his
art, whereas the bad critic points awayfrom the work in question: he enemy.
introduces extraneous elements into his appreciation - smudges the
canvas with his own paint (Knights 1963, 18). 11. The villain causes harm or injury to a member of a family ( defined
as 'villainy').
12. Misfortune or lack is made known; the hero is approached with a
request or command; he is allowed to go or he is despatched.
13. The seeker agrees to or decides upon counteraction.
14. The hero leaves home.
15. The hero is tested, interrogated, attacked, etc., which prepares the
2
Most of what we know about Mrs Dalloway, for example, is not 'told' by the way for his receiving either a magical agent or helper.
narrator, but is the result of a technique whereby her actions and thoughts are 16. The hero reacts to the actions of the future donor.
'shown' as they unfold.
17. The hero acquires the use of a magical agent.
18. The hero is transferred, delivered, or Jed to the whereabouts of an
object of search.
19. The hero and the villain join in direct combat.
20. The hero is branded.
LO

21. The villain is defeated. of characters in both literature and film analyses: Michael Toolan (200 I,
19-20), for instance, applied it to the study of movies and popular narra-
22. The initial misfortune or lack is liquidated. tive", and Ruth Wodak (2009) analysed the political drama television series
23. The hero returns. The West Wing by making reference to the characters roles outlined by
Propp. Moreover, Culpeper and Fernandez-Quintanilla (2017) have recent-
24. The hero is pursued. ly shown how, even if the model tends to 'de-humanise' characters, it can
25. The hero is rescued from pursuit. still be fruitfully used in combination with cognitive theories for analytical
purposes: clearly, the critics explain, there are schematic associations be-
26. The hero, unrecognised, arrives home or in another country. tween fictional character roles and real-life social categories (2007, 101),
27. A false hero presents unfounded claims. therefore whenever a particular character function (in Propp's terms) is
identified, a particular schema containing 'anthropomorphic' traits is re-
28. A difficult task is proposed to the hero. trieved by readers (for the notio]k.Qf~'sc~, see 2.2.1 - Part 2 of this
29. The task is resolved. volume).

30. The hero is recognised. 1.3.2 Structuralism


31. The false hero or villain is exposed.
"Character is the major aspect of the novel to which structuralism has paid
32. The hero is given a new appearance. least attention and has been least successful in treating" (Culler 1975, 230):
Jonathan Culler's statement may perhaps be sufficient for outlining the
33. The villain is punished.
approach to characters during Structuralism, one of the most important
34. The hero gets married and ascends to the throne. intellectual movements of the 201h century. Indeed, this period showed very
little interest in the category, considering it merely as a 'functional' cate-
The number of functions, Propp pointed out, is always the same, and the
gory, or as an 'actant'.
functions of characters "serve as stable, constant elements in a tale, inde-
pendent of how and by whom they are fulfilled" (1968, 21 ). Moreover, As regards the former, the most important model elaborated at the
seven basic character types (also called 'roles') were identified: time was Philippe Hamon's Pour un statut sémiologique du personage,
first published in Littérature in 1972. As_tbe title suggests,..nature..GÎ-GA-ar-
• Villain acter was inherentl.y_:sei:niotic' ,Jherefore analyses had...tQ-be--eftFFi~d-outin
• Donor/provider terms of linguist~signs in S~~re1_tgrri_s (1916): their signifier (prop-
erties, 'marks') ha "fo'"'oerelated..t0-their..signified (their 'sense'), and both
• Hero
• False hero I dimensions needéd to be compared to these same dimensions embodying
the other characters in the text or in other texts. In other words, accord-
ing to this model, ch~acters had a 'semantic.-Malue;-wmGi:i-'.Mas_obtained
• Dispatcher through phenomena of 'accumulation' J:bat started at the beginning of the
• Helper text and enaed at its conclusion. These values, which coincided with char-
acteristics like 'sex', 'job', 'ideology', etc. contributed to the creation of a
• Princess, (+ father) hierarchical classification among characters along specific axes5. By way
A character, Propp made clear (1968, 92), ..c.an-fu-lfil more than 0J1e-.role of illustration, the table below shows how 5 characters could accumulate
(some individuals in the tale could for example be both villain and false their 'semantic value' according to 4 categories (1972, 98):
hero, for instance); by the same token, one role can be fulfilled by sever-
al individuals (several characters could function as helpers or villains). 4
For instance, he used the model to analyse the Star Wars film trilogy.
Whatever the case, it is important to note that in this approach characters 5
The following situation is an example: if a recurrent opposition between a
were considered 'functions': they were subordinated to the progression of king and a young shepherdess was identified, the fundamental semantic axes
the plot, therefore their 'anthropomorphic' traits were backgrounded. would be sex (male character VS female character), age (old character VS
Despite being created in 1928 and first published in Europe not before young character), ideology (noble character VS plebeian character), habitat
1965, the model was used extensively and is still used today for the study ( cultural urban world VS natural, rustic world), etc.
£0

SEMANTIC AXES GEO-GRAPHIC


In Hamon 's and Greimas 's approaches, character dissolution was
SEX IDEOLOGY MONEY 'completed': no reference to any psychological motive driving the charac-
CHARACTERS ORIGIN
Cl + + + + ters' action was ever made, therefore the two models were at the opposing
end to Bradley's approach mentioned above. Very interestingly, the theo-
C2 + + + +
retical 'de-humanisation' of the character found analogies in what was hap-
C3 + 0 0 0
pening within the fictional domain, as Barth's The Literature of Exhaustion
C4 + + 0 0 (1967) makes clear: Borges's language games and 'ironic' use of conven-
cs + + 0 0 tions, Pynchon's mixing of'high' and 'low' literature, Eco's intertextuality,
to cite but a few writers, all pointed towards a very unconventional idea
In the example, characters 1 and 2 would be considered more 'complex' of character, that seemed to have lost all its traditional, 'representational'
than characters 3, 4 and 5, since they are 'informed' by a bigger number potential. This was also in line with the postmodern notion of dispersion
of semantic axes: the notion of 'complexity', it goes without saying, has of a unified, 'solid', 'authentic' self. In Cixous's words (1974, 384), tradi-
nothing to do with the 'psychological complexity' typical of 'humanising' tional representational characters served to "repress the unconscious of the
approaches, yet researchers following the structuralist tradition found it reader, keeping the subject in its limited place by means of identification
very useful to identify similar characters ('galaxies' of characters sharing strategies"; 'contemporary' characters, on the other hand, by being "multi-
the same semantic value) or characters who were in a relation of opposition ple, decentred, differentiated into a trans-subjective effervescence", could
with one another. enhance the "full realization of the reading subject" (1974, 387).
As regards the actant model, it was originally formulated by the French For at least 40 years, then, theories tended to 'de-humanise' charac-
narratologist Algirdas Greimas in 1966, and was strongly dependent on ters, after the 'humanising' approaches of the past. This went hand in hand
Propp's approach developed in Russia forty years earlier. In Greimas's with a general tendency to produce characters that had lost many of their
view, all narratives were underlain by a fixed number of very general cate- long-established attributes. Only in the 1960s, with the emergence of the
gories called actants, themselves structured into three interrelated couples so-called 'stylistic' approaches, a compromise between the two positions
in a relation of opposition (I 966, 38): could finally be reached. A general introduction to this research arena is
provided in 1.4, whereas all Part 2 of this volume is devoted to a model of
Giver Receiver
characterisation that may be termed 'stylistic'.
Subject Object
Helper Opponent
1.4 BRINGING ALL TOGETHER: 'STYLISTICS' MODELS
As remarked by Toolan (2001, 82), the model could well fit many tradi-
tional folk and fairy tales: a young character of lowly origin (subject), for
example, may fall in love with a princess (object) and want to marry her. In the past few decades, new approaches have been developed for the study
In his quest, he may be helped by a friend (helper), but their efforts could of narrative fiction. As a consequence, character analysis has received re-
be frustrated by some opponent (usually a wicked relative of the princess), newed attention, particularly as regards the representation of 'fictional
until a sender (the king, usually, or some individual with magical powers) minds' by means of the so-called 'cognitive stylistic approaches' (See part
might intervene creating a possible happy end. In the same vein, more 2)~ênerally speaking, ffietwo most important research arenas underlying
complex narratives may also be analysed: in Joyce's short story Eveline, stylistics are '!œ_guistics' and 'narra~ogy', two disciplines that enjoyed
for example, the main character embodies both the subject and the receiver a similar developmënrboffiïn Britain and in the United States. Although
role, and her desire for freedom and happiness is the object; her helper evolving from different bases, the two fields have both proved successful
Frank has the power to support her escape from a life she does not want to for the analysis of narratives, either in combination or on their own.
live any more, but her violent opponent father insists on not letting her go. As the linguist Katie Wales made clear back in 2001 (372), the wide
The absence of a superhelper is openly announced by the narrator ("Out of variety of approaches underlying contemporary stylistics is heavily de-
a maze of distress, she prayed to God to direct her, to show her what was pendent on the influences coming from the different branches of linguis-
her duty") (Joyce 1996 [1904], 42), who refers to God as the absent actant, tics and literary criticism. Th~rence to literary criticism is emblematic,
a reason why Eveline cannot obtain the freedom and happiness she desires since the texts analysed by stylisticians are, indeed, mostly 'literary': for,
(her objects). this reason, the theory has often been conceived of as an expansion of the
~u

earlier studies of 'elocutio' in rhetoric (Burke 2014, 1). As Semino and and quite another to concede equal dignity to a linguistic approach
Culpeper pointed out, however, "it would be restrictive to conceive of the to thriving modern and contemporary literature (Miller and Turci
discipline as a phenomenon pertaining exclusively to the realm of litera- 2007, 3).
ture" (1995, 513-520): stylisticians indeed analyse 'language style' in sev-
In the last few decades, stylistics has been the arena where more ef-
~ fields ranging from.literature. to.advertising, from newspaper.reponing
forts have been made to narrow the gap discussed above: by not limiting
to ~ligion, fro_!!} bureaucracy to everyday conversation.
itself to merely describing texts' 'formal features' for their own sake, it
Unlike other research arenas, stylistics has a precise 'date of birth': it has successfully taken into account the functional significance of such fea-
was in fact referred to explicitly for the very first time in 1958 during..the tures, in the light of better interpretations (Wales 2001, 372-73). Stylistic
renowned Indiana Style Conference, its ':futheLbeiJ1g.Ro.m.an ...Iakobeen, models, in other words, are to be considered 'necessary complements' to
one of the most influential linguists of the 201h century. In closing the con- the 'implicit nature' of much literary criticism, their scopes
ference, he stated that "a linguist deaf to the poetic function in language
and a literary scholar indifferent to linguistic problems and un-conversant going far beyond the rhetoric, poetics, formalism, structura/ism
with linguistic methods are equally flagrant anachronisms" (Jakobson and functionalism of the past to embrace corpus, critical, cognitive,
1960, 3 77). This position shed important light on a dispute that had con- pedagogical, pragmatic, gender, multimodal, and most recently,
cerned literary studies for decades, regarding the more useful tools to be neuroscientific approaches (Burke 2017, 2).
used for the analysis of narrative texts. Van Peer's words (1989, 2), formu- Broadly speaking, then, the growth and diffusion of stylistics since
lated almost thirty years later, are crucial for understanding this dispute: the 1960s coincided with a movement away from the merely formalist ap-
Literary scholars frown upon linguists and their methods because proaches of the past, in favour of a closer focus on the relationship between
of their formalism and their stubborn rationality. They feel that language and the pragmatic, social, ideological and cognitive dimensions
somehow the real 'literary' nature of the works studied is not done of literary production and reception. This was also made possible by the
justice to, and they therefore prefer to exclude linguistics (and enormous advances in fields like discourse analysis, socio-linguistics,
linguists) from their field of study altogether. Since literature also computational methods, pragmatics, systemic functional linguistics and
deals with emotions and with the irrational side of human existence, cognitive linguistics, to name but a few. Some of them will be very shortly
they feel that little help is to be expected from an all-too-narrowly introduced below.
defined rational approach. The linguists, on the other hand, find One of the most influential contributions has been Michael Halliday's
fault with the looseness of terms and methods adopted by literary 'systemic model' of language, which provides a 'multi-level' and 'mul-
scholars, their superficial linguistic knowledge and their lack of ti-functional' view of style: the theory explains each linguistic choice by
system, as a result of which they also question the validity of the referring to the system as a whole, and relates the formal features to the
conclusions arrived at in literary scholarship. They also feel that functions they perform inside and outside language (see 2.3.5 Part 2). Hal-
literary language is amenable to linguistic analysis. liday himselfrecognised that different writing styles could foreground dif-
ferent functions (Leech & Short 1981, 31 ), as will be discussed later.
Thirty years after Jakobson's renowned statement, then, the dispute Another research area successfully used in stylistics - and in the anal-
had not yet been resolved: indeed, despite providing useful notions on their ysis of character interaction in particular - is Pragmatics, a field that draws
own, both disciplines seemed to lack some tools that could only be inte- attention to the particular 'contextual configuration' in which communi-
grated by recurring to 'cross-fertilization' practices. Linguists seemed of- cation occurs6: developments in this field have proved very useful for the
ten too 'formal' and 'rational' in their analyses, whereas literary critics did analysis of meaningful exchanges in fictional dialogues, a reason why the
not seem to reflect enough on the 'linguistic texture' of the texts. Eighteen approach has been used widely in the study of characterisation. Addition-
years later, things were not very different: ally, so-called 'literary pragmatics' has looked at the linguistic features of
texts emerging from the 'real' interpersonal relationships between author,
One tends to see the late twentieth century at least as the site of
text and readers (Wales 2001, 238).
an at times impassioned agon for pre-eminence between linguistic
stylistics on the one hand, and, on the other, a myriad variety of lit- Stylistics has also been fruitfully influenced by Foregrounding Theo-
critic angles which contend with, but are basically tolerant of, each ry, a field that investigates the linguistic choices creating reactions of 'de-
other. And this was, and continues to be, the case. After all, it was
one thing to smile on linguistic attention to texts in 'dead' languages, 6
See, in particular, Black (2006).
familiarisation' in the readers, i.e. reactions triggered by patterns of lan- approaches', where characters were analysed as if they had been human
guage use that deviate from what we would normally expect. As regards beings, a series of 'de-humanising' approaches appeared when characters
characterisation, the theory has focused on defamiliarising strategies used began their 'metamorphosis'. In particular, 'textual' approaches like the
by authors to foreground particular characters - or particular 'aspects' like ones described in 1.3 began to look at characters as if they were only 'use-
emotional reactions or speaking styles - and not others. ful functions' for the progression of the plot. In those approaches, no refer-
Recent advances in cognitive science have also proved important for ence to psychological motives was ever carried out, therefore they were in
stylistics, as the large number of publications in this field indicates: Cogni- total opposition with the 'person-like' models of the past.
tive stylistics - or Cognitive Poetics, as this branch is often called (Stock- In Part 2 of this volume a model of characterisation 'bringing all to-
well 2002; Freeman 2017) - investigates the 'cognitive effects' of style gether' will be proposed: starting from recent research on language, the
in readers. Particularly influential for cognitive stylistics are the studies model will take into account both the language underlying the texts - there-
of 'frames' and 'schemas' elaborated initially by cognitive psychologists fore the 'textual' aspect, and the 'mental functioning' of the characters re-
and later used by stylisticians for analysing reader interpretation of literary sulting from cumulative patterns of specific language configurations. A
texts (Cook 1994; Semino 1997; Culpeper 2001; Stockwell 2002; Emmot, series of linguistic approaches very useful for characterisation will be pro-
Alexander and Marszalek 2017). Cognitive Stylistics has also provided posed, together with many examples from contemporary fictions.
important insights for approaching character's 'mental functioning' (Sem-
ino 2006, 2011, 2014; Trevisan 2010).
Finally, in very recent years 'corpus techniques' have been used for
the study of style and of characterisation, as demonstrated by innovative
research published in leading journals or in monographs (Toolan 2008;
Bednarek 2010, 2011; Mahl berg 2012; McIntyre and Walker 2019). In par-
ticular, as will be shown in Part 2, corpus methods provide the possibili-
ty to analyse consistent linguistic patterns typical of particular characters
and not of others, thus projecting particular 'styles' and 'mental function-
ing'. For example, it has been shown that 'neurotypical' characters tend
to use personal 'deictic' patterns starting from the here and now of their
personal experience (Lyons 1977, 63 7), yet this 'deictic centre' (McIntyre,
2006, 2007) might breakdown in characters with limited cognitive abilities
(Bockting 1990, 1995; see also Semino 2011, 423). Intuitively, this may
project peculiar mind styles.

1.5 CONCLUSION

In Part 1 of this volume an overview of the notion of character has been


proposed. Starting from a discussion about his/her ontological status, a
general introduction to his/her 'morphological' evolution in the last three
centuries has been provided.
In particular, it has been noted that a drastic change in character con-
struction occurred at the end of the 191h century, resulting in a decline of
physical descriptions and in a tendency to 'move inwards': as a result, in
the first half of the 20'h century 'traditional' characters seemed to dissolve
into a language that to a great extent only reported their thoughts and their
'streams of consciousness'. Very interestingly, something similar occurred
to theories concerned with their analysis: after centuries of 'humanising
Part 2: A toolkit for the study of characterisation

2.1. INTRODUCTION

In a book that would soon become a classic, the narratologist Rim-


mon-Kenan (1983) joined the debate about literary characters by pointing
out that it is not a mistake to conceive of them both as human beings and as
linguistic signs. This view seemed to reconcile the conflicting perspectives
presented in the first part of this volume, and encompassed both psycho-
logical studies and linguistic ones:
If both approaches [textual and mimetic} end up cancelling the
specificity of fictional characters, though from different standpoints,
should the study of character be abandoned, or should both
approaches be rejected and a different perspective sought? Can such
a perspective reconcile the two opposed positions without destroying
character between them? Is it possible to see characters 'at once as
persons and as parts of a design'? I think it is, provided one realizes
that the two extreme positions can be thought of as relating to
different aspects of narrative fiction. In the text, characters are nodes
in the verbal design; in the story they are - by definition - non (or
pre-) verbal abstractions, constructs (Rimmon-Kenan 1983, 33).
Starting from the assumption that any novel is both 'text' and 'story'
at the same time, 'story' coinciding with the narrated events abstracted
from their disposition in the 'text' (1983, 3), characters could therefore be
conceived of as 'human beings' in the 'story' and as 'textual patterns' in
the 'text'. In other words, characters may be looked at as two equally im-
portant sides of the same coin and analysed accordingly: tools belonging to
the realm of psychology may be used for the story-part, whereas linguistic
,jl)

tools may be used for the 'text' part. Following this perspective, characters narrative technique, point of view, mind style, figurative language, per-
could be defined as "possible individuals made up of words" (Margolin sonal deixis, (im)politeness patterns, power (a)symmetries, to name but a
1990, 453), i.e. as text-embedded patterns not reducible to words, or, fol- few. Both these phenomena need to be investigated in order to outline the
lowing Lubomir Dolezel ( 1989, 236), as "inhabitants of a semiotic mecha- 'psychology' of the character, something that has also been referred to as
nism for the construction of alternate universes". 'mental functioning' (Palmer 2004, 170).
With all this in mind, the second part of this volume aims at outlining In the following pages a model of characterisation will be outlined
an approach to characterisation that mixes cognitive structures and linguis- drawing both on 'top-down' and on 'bottom-up' processes (Culpeper 2001,
tic features: indeed, when readers infer characters from a text, they draw 27), namely on simultaneous operations that together contribute to the
both from their own experiences with real people and with the linguistic characters' impression in readers' minds.
features assembled in the texts by their authors. Therefore, for an adequate
account of characterisation to take place, both cognitive and textual aspects
need to be considered, something which "literary critical comment and
2.2 'TOP-DOWN' PROCESSES
debate have often failed to do" (Culpeper 2002, 251-2).
As an example, let us go back to the novel quoted at the beginning of
Part 1: Nice Work, by David Lodge. The female protagonist, Robyn, is a 2.2.1 Prototype theory and Schema theory
University Professor teaching Victorian literature; as a result of a partner-
ship project between her university and one of the most important indus- The issue of how people remember past information and experiences, and
tries in the area, she is required to spend one day a week with the industry of how such previous knowledge is used to interpret newly acquired infor-
manager Vic Wilcox, who is very successful at his job but knows nothing mation has long been investigated by cognitive psychologists. Very gen-
about literature and the humanities in general. The dynamic of their rela- erally, two types of 'memory stores' have been singled out by researchers:
tionship constitute the core of the novel, which alternates moments of high 'long-term memory' and 'short-term memory'. The first one is likely to be
tension with comical situations that result from the 'inter-character' behav- more permanent, therefore more 'passive', whereas the latter is defined
iour. ln particular, irony tends to involve the male character, who is often as a 'working memory', therefore more active and prone to changes. Our
portrayed while he is wondering about the meaning of words pronounced experience with texts activates mainly the first type of memory, which is
by Robyn while they are driving to some locations or while she is visiting better understood in the light of two areas of study: prototype theory and
the factory he manages: concepts like 'metaphor', 'metonymy', 'semiot- schema theory.
ics', are totally obscure to him, while she uses them regularly to talk about The most important notion for prototypetheory is 'category', which
the elements of the foundry they visit, or about the hidden meanings be- "consists of a typical or ideal exemplar called a prototype and a range of
yond the cigarette brand he smokes. peripheral or less good examples" (Culpeper 2001, 61 ). Prototypes are only
The degree of reader enjoyment during the reading process, howev- formed through experience, and the degree to which a newly experienced
er, depends heavily on their ability to understand that the whole novel is item becomes a member of a particular category depends on the similarity
itself a parody of Victorian literature: the colours in the foundry closely between its features and those of the category's prototype. Categories are
resemble the colours of London as described by Dickens, the books Robyn organised hierarchically, according to the parameter of inclusiveness: a car,
quotes are mainly 191h century novels, their relation echoes the relationship for example, can be included within the category 'means of transport', thus
between characters during the Victorian Age, etc. Only readers with good being subordinate to it. Such hierarchies have three levels: a superordinate
knowledge of Victorian literature will therefore be able to fully appreci- level (such as 'means of transport'), a basic level (car, bike, bus, etc.),
ate Lodge's novel, whereas readers who do not have such knowledge will and a subordinate level (the 'kind' of car). The optimal level for cognitive
still enjoy the story, but at a different level. In other words, readers' own activity is generally considered the basic one, since at this level categories
impression and enjoyment of the characters will result both from the text are more differentiated from one another.
itself, and from their own 'background knowledge' about literature: this is As far as schema theory is concerned, interest for this area of cogni-
mainly the reason why the same text can be experienced so differently by tion took off around the mid 1970s, and stylisticians used it at least from
different readers. the 1990s. Schemata are "mental representations of typical instances"
Characterisation, then, results from the interaction of at least two cru- (Cook 1994, 11): they involve generic knowledge and, in the words of
cial phenomena: readers' prior knowledge ('background knowledge'), and Eysenck and Keane, "may be used to represent events, precepts, situations,
inputs coming from the text - which include a vast array of strategies like relations, and even objects" (Eysenck and Keane 1990, 275). A typical
:>O

example is the schema all of us have for restaurants, which includes slots the text. Differences in comprehension - or in retrieving information - are
about roles (waiters and customers), slots about props (tables, the menu), therefore also to be ascribed to differences in readers' schemata: although
and slots about actions ( entering, ordering, eating, paying the bill). Sche- people belonging to a similar culture are likely to share similar schemata,
mata are generally assumed to be induced from experience, thus they are different personal experiences may result in the creation of different sche-
associated with episodic memory: people's airports schemata, for example, mata, a reason why communicative misunderstandings could also be inter-
are acquired through the similarity between all the actions that need to be preted as 'lack of relevant schemata' for a particular situation. At present,
carried out in all the different airports of the world. Once they have been to the author's knowledge, no research has ever considered the perception
experienced as episodes, these experiences become part of a more general of characters by readers who, by belonging to totally different cultures,
schema, later recalled in similar situations. Crucially, schematic knowl- may have developed totally different schemata for similar situations. For
edge also shapes the way in which we view and make inferences about example, starting from the same text, it could be very stimulating to in-
newly elaborated information: therefore, the notion of schemata is useful vestigate whether the elaboration of a particular character in a novel is
for understanding how what we see is, at least in part, determined by what different for a reader coming from a Western country compared to that of a
we expect to see. reader coming from an Eastern country.
As far as the relation between schemata and the comprehension of Schemata are also very useful for approaching the mental functioning
texts is concerned, the following example by Cook (1994) helps make of fictional characters. As an example, Jet us consider the central charac-
things clear: "imagine", he proposes, "a witness in a trial who has been ter of Ian McEwan 's novel Atonement (2001 ), a young girl called Brion y
asked to tell the court about his movements during a specific morning". Tallis 7. In the third chapter of the novel, she is described while observing
Such a witness could produce the following statement, thus satisfying the a scene from the nursery window of her rich parents' country villa (2002:
needs of the court: 37-40). The protagonists of the scene are her sister Cecilia and the cleaning
lady's son, Robbie Turner. A very precious old family vase has just been
1 woke up at seven forty. 1 made some toast and a cup of tea. 1
broken by them in an attempt to fill it with water at the house's Triton
listened to the news. And I left for work at about eight thirty (Cook
fountain, and Cecilia decides to enter the freezing water to rescue at least
1994, 12).
some of its broken fragments. The scene, which will prove crucial for the
However, this alternative report might also be produced in the same development of the plot, had already been narrated a few pages before
situation: (2002, 27-31 ), but on that occasion the reader could not realize that Briony
was observing it. The extract below is taken from the first of the two de-
1 woke up at seven forty. 1 was in bed. 1 was wearing pyjamas. After
scriptions, and is divided into two parts. The first part reports the dialogue
lying still for a few minutes, I threw back the duvet, got out of bed,
between Cecilia and Robbie before the vase was broken, while the second
walked to the door of the bedroom, opened the door, switched on the
is concerned with the characters' reactions in the moments following the
landing light, walked across the landing, opened the bathroom door,
accident:
went into the bathroom, put the basin plug into the plughole, turned
on the hot tap, ran some hot water into the washbasin, looked in the (1) 'Let me take that, 'he said, stretching out a hand. (2) 'I'll fill it for
mirror . . (Cook 1994, 1 2). you, and you take the flowers. '
Despite being true, the content of the second extract contains far too
much information. If the court wants to know 'the whole truth', however, (3) 'I can manage, thanks.' (4) She was already holding the vase over
the basin.
why should details be omitted? Schema theory explains it by postulating
that the 'default elements' of the schema activated in this particular cir-
cumstance can be taken for granted: when the sender of a message judges (5) But he said, 'Look, I've got it.' (6) And he had, tightly between
that his interlocutor's schema corresponds largely with his/her own, the forefinger and thumb. (7) 'Your cigarette will get wet. (8) Take the
only details worth mentioning are those not contained in the schema, all flowers.'
the other ones being present by default.
Schemata, in other words, provide the possibility to construct an inter- (9) This was a command on which he tried to confer urgent
pretation containing more information than the one actually present in the masculine authority.
text: empirical evidence has indeed shown that it is difficult to distinguish
7
what is supplied by our schematic knowledge from what is supplied by For a detailed analysis of the novel, see Trevisan (2010).
'fU

[.. .} though issuing a command which Cecilia dared not disobey. (12) It
was extraordinary that she could not resist him. (13) At his insistence
(10) 'You idiot! (11) Look what you've done. ' she was removing her clothes, and at such speed. (14) She was out
of her blouse, now she had let her skirt drop to the ground and was
(12) He looked into the water, then he looked back at her; and simply stepping out of it, while he looked on impatiently, hands on hips.
shook his head as he raised a hand to cover his mouth. (13) By this (15) What strange power did he have over ha (16) Blackmail? (17)
gesture he assumed full responsibility, but at that moment, she hated Threats? (18) [. . .] The sequence was illogical- the drowning scene,
him for the inadequacy of the response. (14) He glanced towards the followed by a rescue, should have preceded the marriage proposal.
basin and sighed. (15) For a moment he thought she was about to (McEwan 2002: 38)
step backwards onto the vase, and he raised his hand and pointed,
A significant number of textual cues indicates that the same events are
though he said nothing. (16) Instead he began to unbutton his shirt.
now experienced from a totally different 'deictic centre' (McIntyre 2007,
(17) Immediately she knew what he was about. (18) Intolerable.
124)8: to start with, the use of the modal auxiliary "must" (1 ), makes it clear
(19) He had come to the house and removed his shoes and socks
that the events are presented in the order that Briony herself experiences
- well, she would show him then. (20) She kicked off her sandals,
them, a phenomenon Short calls "psychological sequencing" (1996, 275).
unbuttoned her blouse and removed it, unfastened her skirt and
Such a shift in focalization is even more evident if we consider the use of
stepped out of it and went to the basin wall. (21) He stood with
the prepositional phrase "in the distance at least" (2) and the comparative
hands on his hips and stared as she climbed into the water in her
forms "closer" (3) and "nearer still" (3), which are to be interpreted in rela-
underwear. (22) Denying his help, any possibility of making amends,
was his punishment. (23) The unexpectedly freezing water that
tion to Briony's position: in actual fact they refer to those parts of the park
caused her to gasp was his punishment. (24) [. . .} He did not exist,
Briony can see from the nursery window. In addition, although the extract
he was banished, and this was also the punishment. (McEwan 2002,
is narrated in the third person, the language used is likely to be perceived
as Briony's: "humble" (9), "boldness of ambition" (9), "imperiously" (11 ),
29-30)
"extraordinary" (12), and "strange" (15), are all to be ascribed to her, even
The stylistic features of the extract will be analysed in detail in section though they are reported by the external narrator.
2.3.4.1, as they belong to the 'bottom-up' processes we have referred to Briony's viewpoint becomes more explicit in the sentence "There was
above. For the time being, let us focus on the way Briony makes sense of something rather formal about the way he stood [ ... ]" ( 4), which coincides
the same scene, reported in the extract below: with the moment readers start to penetrate her mind. Such a moment is fur-
She [Briony] had arrived at one of the nurserys wide-open windows ther signalled by the abrupt shift in narrative technique: unlike sentences
and must have seen what lay before her some seconds before she 1 and 2, which are clearly a narrative report, sentence 4 is an example of
registered it. (2) It was a scene that could easily have accommodated, free indirect thought, and such a shift is even more evident in the following
in the distance at least, a medieval castle. (3) [. . .} Closer; within the sentence ("A proposal of marriage"), which has the typical (un)grammat-
boundaries of the balustrade, were the rose gardens and, nearer still, ical construction of colloquial speech (and is therefore more likely to be
the Triton fountain and standing by the basins retaining wall was attributed to Briony than to the author/narrator). Moreover, sentence 5 fea-
her sister, and right before her was Robbie Turner. (4) There was tures a crucial parallelism with Cecilia's utterance "intolerable" in the first
something rather formal about the way he stood, feet apart, head extract analyzed (18): in both cases the content of the free direct thought is
held back. (5) A proposal of marriage. (6) Briony would not have foregrounded by means of a structural strategy which, by rankshifting the
been surprised. (7) She herself had written a tale in which a humble phrase to the level of a sentence, better records the moment of highly emo-
woodcutter saved a princess from drowning and ended by marrying tional tension9. Sentence 5 hence marks the moment when Briony starts to
her. (8) What was presented here fitted well. (9) Robbie Turner, only
son of a humble cleaning lady and of no known father; Robbie who 8
"The location of a speaker in time and space is referred to as their deictic
had been subsidised by Briony s father through school and university, centre. [ ... ] All fictional narration is made up of a number of deictic fields,
had wanted to be a landscape gardener, and now wanted to take up defined by Stockwell (2002, 4 7) as a set of deictic expressions all relating to
medicine, had the boldness of ambition to ask for Cecilia s hand. the same deictic centre" (McIntyre 2007, 123-124).
(JO) It made perfect sense.[. . .] (11) What was less comprehensible, 9
See 2.3. I for an introduction to narrative techniques and, in general, Short
however; was how Robbie imperiously raised his hand now, as and Semino (2004, 42-65)
'+£

elaborate the scene as a marriage proposal. Quite significantly, the external a "proposal of marriage" (5): marriage proposals are indeed prototypical
description of the physical environment characterizing the first three sen- actions in fairy tales and gothic novels, and thus play a crucial role in her
tences in narrative report (NR) is here replaced by a series of sentences in mental schemata.
free direct thought (FDT) which coincide with a mental interpretation of As the text unfolds, however, it seems clear that the TALE schema
the events, such an interpretation being necessarily the result of Briony's is not the only one she applies in order to make sense of the events she is
intentionality and volition. witnessing: the expression "What was less comprehensible" (11) signals
Although the two extracts record the same event, the second is there- the activation of a second schema which makes her process the story world
fore a personal, 'private' re-elaboration of the first one and can thus be in a completely different manner. This happens because she has noted a
referred to as a 'subworld' of the character: in particular, since Briony's behavioural act - ("Robbie imperiously raised his hand now", sentence
interpretation is a blend of 'real' and 'fantastic' elements, her 'subworld' is 11) which comes into conflict with the schematic behaviour of a "humble
a 'mistaken knowledge world' (Ryan 1991, 124) of a very particular type. woodcutter" of her own gothic-romantic tale, as a result of which Rob-
The extract in fact presents both the 'content' of Briony's 'subworld' and bie is suddenly perceived as a powerful, domineering character whose
its 'online creation', i.e. the character's mental processing responsible for will Cecilia "dared not disobey" (11 ). In this (second) part of the text her
it: stated differently, readers are allowed to both observe the scene from a 'worldview' is thus significantly influenced by a mental schema contra-
more 'objective' perspective, provided by the narrator, and to observe the dicting the first schema she applies, a fact which makes her experience
same events while the character is elaborating them. the story world in highly dramatic as opposed to romantic terms. Again,
Schema theory can be usefully applied to investigate Briony's mental this is consistent with the assumption that Briony tends to apply 'fictional
functioning: indeed, in order to make sense of the scene she is observing, world' schemata in order to make sense of the situations she experiences.
Briony searches her memory for similar situations, what cognitive psy- Indeed, as the co-text makes clear (2002, 6- 7; 39-40), she has spent most of
chologists call TOP (Thematic Organization Point, Schank 1982, 113)1°: her childhood reading dramatic stories and romantic tales. However, Bri-
once the situation has been identified, a series of mental schemata related ony seems to realize the contradiction embedded in the activation of two
to that situation are activated. Since Briony's knowledge of the world de- conflicting schemata, as the sentences "What was less comprehensible"
rives mainly from the books she has read, the situations resembling most (11) and "The sequence was illogical - the drowning scene, followed by a
closely those stored in her memory are the ones narrated in fictional sto- rescue, should have preceded the marriage proposal" (18) make clear. Yet,
ries, and in tales unfolding in medieval castles in particular. The expres- she persuades herself that the 'dramatic fiction' schema is the truly valid
sions "fitted well" (8) and "made perfect sense" (10) explicitly refer to the one in order to mentally process Robbie and Cecilia's actions: Robbie's
activation of a particular TALE mental schema which makes her process raised hand, which is attributed no precise purpose in the first extract, is
Robbie, the only son of her family's "humble cleaning lady" (9), in the hence processed by Briony's mind as issuing a command Cecilia cannot
same way es the "humble woodcutter" (7) prototypically asking for the dare disobey; Cecilia's speed in removing her own clothes is interpreted as
hand of a princess who is the protagonist of one of the stories she has writ- a consequence of Robbie's insistence, and Robbie's embarrassed staring at
ten as a young child. The occurrence of the nominal phrase "a proposal of her in the first text is here turned into a sign of his impatient desire to see
marriage" in sentence 5 is therefore part of a free direct representation of her without her clothes. From this point on, this second schema is the only
Briony's thoughts, which sheds light on her particular world view: a par- one she uses in order to make sense of the narrated world, and all the events
ticular context - the rose gardens of her house, the Triton fountain - is con- she witnesses afterwards are therefore interpreted - and reinforced - in
sistent with the typical settings of the Medieval tales she adores, and thus this light11• Such a "schema reinforcement strategy" also sheds significant
it triggers the activation of a schema which turns Cecilia and Robbie into light on her in terms of "traits" - attributes like 'fixity' and 'rigidity' may
'prototypical characters who behave as the canon of such tales would im-
pose. The adjective "formal" (4) is particularly significant in this respect, 11
Starting from Edelman 's assumption (] 992) that people's minds must build
since it seems to pave the way for Briony's interpretation of the scene as new schemata [mental representations of typical instances] and adjust ex-
isting ones to adapt to new experiences, Cook explains that discourse [and,
10 "TOPs are convenient collections of memories involving goals and plans, more generally, world situations] may be divided into three major types:
written in terms of a sufficiently abstract vocabulary to be useful across do- "schema reinforcing", "schema preserving" and "schema refreshing" (Cook
mains. For any reminding experience that crosses contexts, we can expect the 1994, 10). In particular, a schema refreshing situation will deviate from sche-
two experiences to share a goal type, some planning or other conditions, and matic expectations, whereas a schema reinforcing one will tend to maintain
one or more low level identical features" (Schank 1982, 113). and reinforce already existing schemata.
'+'+

be ascribed to her personality - and is closely connected to the dramatic and feminine categories or 'equivalence classes', regardless of
evolution of the plot12• their differences on a variety of dimensions unrelated to gender; for
Briony's massive exposure to the worlds of fiction, in conclusion, has example, spontaneouslyplacing items like 'tender' and 'nightingale'
a great impact on her background knowledge of the real world, and in into a feminine category and items like 'assertive' and 'eagle' into a
tum influences the way in which she makes sense of it: her mind appears masculine category.
to have a tendency to conceive of most aspects of experience either in The activation of social schemata is closely connected with evaluative
dramatic or in romantic terms, a fact which is responsible for most of the beliefs and worldview: people belonging to different groups are likely to
misunderstandings that occur in the novel. ln addition, although she openly develop different schema types for assessing the same social group. Such
declares that she loves romantic stories, she tends to apply 'dramatic fic- attitudes may influence the way the schema is employed in the interpreta-
tion' schemata that tum Robbie into a prototypical aggressor and Cecilia tion and production of social discourse, which in tum influences the devel-
into a victim. Overall, this results in the impression of a creative but also opment of the schema.
simple mind, which shows little ability to evaluate the consequences of its Our perception of others is not based on generalised social schemata
own operations, and is consistent with the young age of the protagonist. only: both semantic and episodic memory also need to be accounted for.
Semantic memory contains abstract generalisations about groups of peo-
2.2.2 Social cognition ple, the central belief of these schemata constituting the stereotype for that
group; episodic memory, on the other hand, contains inferences related to
Schemata and prototype theory are not the only 'cognitive' approaches that particular experiences of individuals or groups, which are themselves influ-
can be used for the study of character. Researchers working on the area of enced by ideology or worldview. People, in other words, can be perceived
social cognition, for example, have generally focused both on the psycho- through two main alternative procedures: category-based impressions - in
logical mechanisms by which people mentally represent themselves and which case the person is perceived on the basis of our general catego-
other people, and on the social nature of perceivers, namely on the way in ries related to the group a particular person belongs to - and person-based
which the social world emerges in the course of social interactions. impressions, in which case great emphasis is placed on the idiosyncratic
Generally speaking, social categories include three main groupings, features of the particular person we are interacting with. Category and per-
which are defined by the kind of information that constitutes them: the first son-based impressions have very different characteristics, categorisation
one comprises personal categories, which encompasses knowledge about implying simplification and lack ofrichness, and person-based impressions
people's preferences, interests, goals, and so on. These are quite idiosyn- implying complexity and personalisation (see also Culpeper 2001, 83).
cratic features, and are related more to an individual's characteristics than Together with prototype and schemata, social cognition research can
to a particular social group. The grouping comprises social role categories, be fruitfully applied to the study of characters: Propp's dramatic roles in-
which are cencerned with people's social functions: kinship roles, occupa- troduced in Part 1 of this volume, for instance, are concerned with 'univer-
tional roles, relational roles, etc. Finally, the third one includes group mem- sal roles' underlying all narratives. If a character in a novel or in a tale is
bership categories, which is related to knowledge about social groups: sex, the author of several good deeds, he will likely be processed as a 'hero' by
race, class, nationality, religion, etc. readers, therefore a whole 'schema network' will be activated in readers'
A particularly relevant notion in social cognition studies regards the minds containing features like 'male', 'not old', 'handsome', 'brave', etc.
role of social schemata, i.e. cognitive structures containing links between Recognising the specific genre of a story, in other words, activates a whole
the social categories referred to above: when a category is activated, a series set of' dramatic expectations' which will later guide readers' perception of
of other categories is automatically activated at the same time, thus creating characters. In Culpeper's words:
something similar to a network. Once an individual has been 'categorised'
in a certain manner, for instance, the schema activated also provides a large [. . .} the attributes and features of a flat character are organised
amount of additional information, as Bern (1983, 604) exemplifies: according to a preformedcategory or schema to form a category-
based impression; the attributes and featuresof a round character
Gender-schematic processing in particular thus involves combine to form a person-based impression. The staticism/change
spontaneouslysorting attributes and behaviours into masculine dimension can be explained: a categorised character implies no
change, while the piecemeal integration of a personalised character
12 Briony's misunderstanding of the scene will prove catastrophic for the lives implies change. And the 'surprise' dimension alsofollows: the
of both her sister Cecilia and her lover Robbie. confirmatory categorisation of a character means being satisfied
'+0

that a current schema adequately accounts for the information you the deaf-mute character having already appeared many times, no one - in-
have about that character. whereas piecemeal integration means that cluding the audience - has activated any type of 'killer' schema in relation
a character will not fit an existing schema and is thus 'surprising' to him, since people with this type of disability are generally perceived
(Culpeper 2001, 94). as more quiet, shy, non-threatening. The audience's decoding activity, in
other words, was manipulated thanks to the combination of information
On the other hand, if readers identify another type of genre, for ex- coming from the movie itself (the fact that Dave came home covered with
ample a biography, they might give more weight to their knowledge about blood) and of schemata types activated both in the viewers and in the
real people, a knowledge that, however, is widely employed by readers in characters of the movie.
all types of genres. This phenomenon can be referred to as dramatic recategorisation
Forster's binomial recalled by Culpeper can also be explained in the (Culpeper 2001, 96), a recurrent strategy whereby writers create a situa-
light of Cook's schema refreshing/schema reinforcing dichotomy: as he tion in which a character is formed according to a particular schema, but
points out ( 1994, 10), readers' schemata can be either disrupted (refreshed) then force the reader to abandon that schema entirely and activate a new
or reinforced by a literary text. In the first case, a clash takes place between one. Several renowned characters are formed exploiting the activation of
the information contained in the text and the schemata present in the read- conflicting schemata, consider Bianca, Katherina's sister in Shakespeare's
ers' mind, such a process probably resulting in a change of schemata; in the The Taming of the Shrew: in the first half of the play, Shakespeare makes
second case the text somehow confirms readers' schemata, thus reinforcing the reader think that she is the truly good daughter. In the latter half of the
them. As far as characterisation is concerned, fl.at characters may be de- play, however, a dramatic change in schema leads readers to fully re-cate-
scribed as a typical schema reinforcing model, due to their stability of traits gorise her, turning her into a truly bad character.
throughout the novel, whereas round characters fall under a typical schema
Social cognition theories, in conclusion, provide crucial insights for ap-
refreshing model, since they tend to change and are likely to surprise and proaching significant aspects of characterisation, largely ignored in the
disrupt readers' expectations. past. Before focusing on the bottom-up processes, the next section will
The schema potential of stories and situations is regularly exploited shortly introduce a further dimension of the top-down ones: how our mind
by writers or film-makers in order to dramatically surprise their readers/ infers i.e. recognises :fictional entities before categorising them.
viewers: in the movie Mystic River, for example, a young woman is found
dead in a field after a night out with some of her female friends. The last
2.2.3 'Inferring' characters
person who saw her in a pub that night, an old friend of her dad's, is sur-
prised by his wife when he comes home very late at night covered with Before attributing traits to characters, readers are required to recognise
blood. When questioned about the blood, he tells his wife that he was them, therefore it is crucial to focus on how and when this process takes
assaulted in a parking Jot on his way home. This same character, called place. ln other words, it is important to single out which top-down pro-
Dave, was kidnapped and raped as a child, a reason why he now tends to cesses make it possible for readers to decide that a particular behaviour
be solitary and emotionally unstable. His wife pretends to believe him, - linguistic or non-linguistic - contributes to the formation of a character
but it is clear to all viewers that she suspects him of having murdered the impression. Broadly speaking, two theories have been used to address this
young girl. Cognitively speaking, both his wife and all viewers are likely question: corresponding inference theory and covariation theory.
to activate social schemata that combine person-based and category based Correspondent inference theory and covariation theory are both
impressions: as regards the former, he clearly gives the impression of an sub-systems of attribution theory, an area of research devoted to under-
emotionally troubled character, who finds it hard to interact with others; standing how real life situations trigger the creation of particular person-
as regards the latter, he is likely to be categorised in the same manner as ality traits for people in social contexts. In particular, the former generally
most people who have endured violence and sexual abuse as children, i.e. tries to single out those traits through a correspondence inference, i.e. a
more fragile, weak, perhaps unpredictable in their behaviour. These sche- process depending on different factors, primarily context13. The latter, on
mata, combined with the information coming from the text (the movie)
are at the base of the sensational mistake characterising the movie: when 13
The other factors are causal ambiguity, according to which the fewer reasons
his wife tells the girl's father that, according to her, Dave is the killer of there are for doing something, the more likely we are to be sure of why a
his daughter, he kidnaps Dave and kills him in revenge. However, very person did it - and thus of making a correspondent inference -, and social
soon some police officers arrest the real murderer, a deaf-mute teenager desirability, according to which behaviours that are unusual tend to be more
who was jealous of his older brother's relationship with the girl. Despite informative about a person. See also Culpeper (2001, 116)
.. 0

the other hand, tries to neatly distinguish those personality traits that are As Douthwaite points out (2000, 177), the theory can be also read
caused by context from those that are more specifically rooted in the per- through the lenses of cognitive psychology: "habituation in perception",
son's singularity, notwithstanding the context. Following Culpeper (2001, he explains, "is indeed a normal phenomenon in human life, one that rou-
127), three possible types of attributions may be identified: a person attri- tinizes life and dulls the senses". Therefore, one way of combating habit-
bution, a stimulus attribution, and a circumstance attribution. If a person uation is that of "experiencing an entity in an innovative way, so that our
sees me laughing, for instance, automatic modes of processing are impeded, slowed down" (2000, 177):
defamilarisation thus takes place, which obliges us to examine a particu-
[. . .] you may decide that it is because I am characteristically a jovial
lar entity from a new perspective. All this can help form a new interpre-
person (a person attribution), or because the.film I am watching is tation of reality; additionally, there seems to be empirical evidence that
very funny (a stimulus attribution), or because I have just consumed foregrounded elements are not only psychologically more striking but may
a bottle of wine (a circumstance attribution) (Culpeper 2001, 128). also be regarded by readers as more important in relation to the overall
Hence, covariation of cause and effect can help decide how an at- interpretation of the text (van Peer 1989, 1986).
tribution should be made. In particular, the integration of correspondent By focusing on unexpected irregularities and on unexpected regular-
inferencetheory and covariation theory gives the possibility of associating ities, the theory takes into account behaviours that deviate from a norm,
a particular behaviour to a particular person's trait or disposition, provided and is therefore, in some sense, analogous to attribution theory. While the
that the behaviour is: latter is only concerned with non-linguistic evidence, the former is strict-
ly linguistic-oriented; therefore, far from being different, the two theories
1. Free from external pressure; may be interpreted as two sides of the same coin: readers construe their
2. Low in ambiguity; own idea of characters both through their non-linguistic behaviour and
through their language. Linguistic strategies for foregrounding will be
3. Unusual - departing from the perceiver's expectations; discussed at various points in the book, and do not constitute a section
on their own. In the next paragraphs, a range of bottom-up processes will
In other words, under these conditions observers should be able to
be discussed.
make some reasonable inferences regarding someone's traits, starting from
their behaviour. This, of course, works differently for characters, compared
to real people, since in the case ofreal people inferences may also take into
account additional factors, like past events involving the person, or contex- 2.3. BOTTOM-UP PROCESSES
tual clues. Characters, on the other hand, do not have a past life or any type
of life outside the texts in which they are embedded; therefore, things tend As anticipated above, readers' or viewers' impressions of characters result
to become ri-iore complicated. both from top-down and from bottom-up processes: although presented
In order to overcome this impasse, Culpeper argues that attribution separately, it must be kept in mind that mental operations (top-down pro-
theory should be combined with foregrounding theory, an analytical frame- cesses) and textual configurations (bottom-up processes) work simultane-
work rooted in Russian Formalism and in the Prague School of the first ously during the act of reading ( or watching), and are equally responsible
decades of the twentieth century, and later developed in Europe in the 1960s for the creation of characters' images in readers' minds.
and 1970s. Very generally speaking, the theory explains how some textual In the rest of Part 2, a wide range of linguistic cues contributing to
elements achieve particular salience (i.e., they are foregrounded) through characterisation will be addressed and carefully explained: as other re-
deviation from the norm, whereas others tend to stay backgrounded, i.e. searchers have done (Rimmon-Kenan 1983, 59- 70; Pfister 1988, 183-95;
not noted by readers. Deviation can take place at different levels; therefore, Culpeper 2001, 163-234; Culpeper and Fernandez-Quantanilla 2017, 93-
particular patterns can be foregrounded from the phonological, syntactic, or 128), a checklist of textual cues will be provided, inevitably overlapping,
semantic point of view. In the sentence "Green is fantastic", for example, at least in part, with those formulated by some other scholars. To start with,
two grammatical rules are broken, thus creating deviancy and foreground- three main cues will be discussed: narratorial control, self or other's orien-
ing: first of all, an adjective (green) cannot fill the subject slot in a clause; tation, explicit versus implicit".
secondly, an adjective cannot function as an attribute of another adjective
realising the function subject (fantastic). Both 'green and 'fantastic' are 14
For a detailed discussion of these cues, see also Culpeper and Fernan-
therefore deviant from the norm, thus foregrounding the sentence content. dez-Quantanilla (20 I 7).
2.3.1 Narratorial control Apart from N, which involves no speech or thought presentation but
just pure narration, all the other options encompass the reporting of char-
Taking for granted that all aspects of character behaviour and talk, includ- acters' words or thoughts, with different degrees of control over them.
ing direct speech, are ultimately under narratorial control, there are some In particular, narrative presence is quite strong in NRSA/NRTA and in
discourse presentation methods that seem freer than others, thus contribut- IS/IT, where narrators tend to be perceived as in total control of what
ing to the impression of characters being more in control of their choices they are reporting. As we move towards the right hand side of the scale,
and decisions. The presentation of characters' speech and thought has been however, control seems to gradually decline, and readers tend to perceive
investigated at least for 40 years, with Leech and Short's model (1981) they are closer to the character's own words or thoughts. In DS/DT and
being one of the most referenced even nowadays 15: in FDS/FDT, in particular, it is canonically assumed that the direct string
Speech Presentation reports exactly the words and thoughts used by the character, without
DS FDS any filter.
N NRSA IS FIS
Different options produce different effects, mainly related to the au-
Thought presentation tonomy of characters and how close readers feel towards them. As an
N NRTA IT FIT DT FDT example, let us consider the opening of Virgina Woolf's Mrs Dal/away,
The model, as the examples below make clear, presents on the left where different presentation options are used in sequence:
a situation in which narrators are in total control of characters' speech or Mrs. Dal/away said she would buy the flowers herself. For Lucy had
thought, and, on the right, an increasing lack of control in favour of char- her work cut out for her. The doors would be taken off their hinges;
acters' freedom: Rumpelmayer '.s men were coming. And then, thought Clarissa
Examples from the two categories Dal loway, what a morning - fresh as if issued to children on a
beach.
N (Narration): She looked at him.
NRSA (Narrative report of speech acts): She promised she would return. What a lark! What a plunge! For so it had always seemed to her;
when, with a little squeak of the hinges, which she could hear now,
NRTA (Narrative report of thought acts): She wondered about her she had burst open the French windows and plunged at Bourton
brother's feelings. into the open air. How fresh, how calm, stiller than this of course,
IS (Indirect speech): She said she would return the following week to the air was in the early morning; like the flap of a wave; the kiss
see them. of a wave; chill and sharp and yet (for a girl of eighteen as she
then was) solemn, feeling as she did, standing there at the open
IT (Indirect thought): She wondered if her brother was still in love window, that something awful was about to happen; looking at
with his wife. the flowers, at the trees with the smoke winding off them and the
FIS (Free indirect speech): She would return there to see them the rooks rising, falling; standing and looking until Peter Walsh said,
following week. "Musing among the vegetables?"- was that it? - "I prefer men
to cauliflowers"- was that it? He must have said it at breakfast
FIT (Free indirect thought): Did her brother still love his wife? one morning when she had gone out on to the terrace - Peter
DS (Direct speech): She said: "I will come back to see you next week". Walsh. He would be back from India one of these days, June or
July, she forgot which, for his letters were aw(ullv dull: it was his
DT (Direct thought): She wondered: "Does my brother still love his
sayings one remembered; his eyes, his pocket-knife. his smile, his
wife?" grumpiness and, when millions of things had utterly vanished -
FDS (Free direct speech): "I will come back to see you next week". how strange it was! - a few sayings like this about cabbages. [my
emphasis}
FDT (Free direct thought): "Does he still love his wife?"
The first sentence is IS, with the narrator indirectly reporting the con-
15 This is not the most recent model for speech and thought presentation. How- tent of Clarissa Dalloway's utterance. However, things are different in
ever, it is very clear and easy to use. For a more detailed model, see Semino the second sentence, where the focus is moved into the character's mind:
and Short (2004). by means of FIT, we have the possibility of feeling closer to Clarissa's
::>L

thoughts and mind, without any narratorial intervention (for example, the 2.3.2 Self and other presentation
author might have decided to use a narratorial tag such as 'she reflected',
Generally speaking, character presentation may occur in at least three dif-
'she thought', etc., but she did not). The fact that the maid is called by
ferent manners: self-presentation, self-presentation in the presence of other
name or that a colloquial expression like "cut out" has been used, prompts
characters, other-presentation. In the first case, a character provides explic-
readers to feel as if they were actually listening to the character's thoughts,
it information about him/herself when he/she is alone; in the second case,
without any narratorial mediation. he/she provides explicit information about him/herself in the presence of
The sentence starting with "And then, thought Clarissa Dalloway" other characters; in the third case, a character provides explicit information
moves back towards a higher narratorial control, even though the phrase about someone else, a process also called 'altero-characterisation' by Eder
"what a morning, fresh as if issued to children on a beach" sounds more et al. (2010, 32). Crucially, altero-characterisation also provides important
character-oriented than narratorial-oriented. The following paragraph, clues about the character, since the statements regarding others implicitly
starting with "What a lark! What a plunge!" is FDT, where readers can reveal the character's own values and worldviews (Eder et al. 2010, 33).
actually hear the thoughts of the character as if they had been plugged A crucial issue here is reliability, since characters may lie for strate-
to her brain. Interestingly, the Westminster morning triggers memories of gic reasons, both when they are talking about themselves and when they
events happened several decades before, when she was still a young wom- are talking about others. As happens with real people, they might distort
an spending time at Bourton, her parents' country house. Therefore, "fresh information about themselves ( or about others) in order to reach particular
as if issued to children on a beach" refers both to her present feelings in aims: self-presentation is inevitably oriented towards others, therefore it is
London, and to the feelings she had experienced at Bourton, while opening rarely undistorted; therefore, readers should be able to "discount aspects
the big French windows. The actual and the metaphorical, time present and of self-presentation that are likely to be motivated by strategic reasons"
times past interweave in her mind, each memory triggering another one. (Culpeper 2001, 168), but doing so is not always easy.
The fact that we have access to her mind by means of FDT is even clearer As regards self-presentation in the absence of other characters, for ex-
if we consider the sentences "Musing among the vegetables?"- Was that ample when characters speak to themselves in a monologue, it mostly tends
it? -"I prefer men to cauliflowers"- Was that it? He must have said it at to happen at crucial turning points of the plot: for this reason, it is generally
breakfast one morning when she had gone out on to the terrace - Peter experienced as true by readers, since there does not seem to be any good
Walsh", in which it is clear that Clarissa cannot trust her memories from reason for a character to lie when he/she is speaking to him/herself. Of
some decades before. course, monologues are not only used for self-presentation purposes, but
'Narratorial control' is a powerful tool that authors use to create their authors may often use them as indicators of characters' inner life: in this
characters' image in the mind of the readers. As noted above, different case, clashes may take place between the public and the private image of
the characters, a very recurrent experience with Shakespearian characters.
choices could be carried out, with totally different effects. To conclude this
section, let us observe how a portion of the same extract would sound, had Other-presentation, finally, takes place when a character is introduced
a different technique been used (see also Lodge 1992, 45): by another character: the kinds of inferences readers can gain from this
choice are not dissimilar from the other ones. In particular, the informa-
*What a lark! What a plunge! For so it had always seemed to mg, tion provided by other characters is not necessarily less important or reli-
when, with a little squeak of the hinges, which I can hear now, l able than the information provided by the particular character in question.
burst open the French windows and plunged at Bourton into the As in the case of self-presentation, however, readers should be aware that
open air. How fresh, how calm, stiller than this of course, the air information coming from other characters may be distorted for strategic
was in the early morning; like the flap of a wave; the kiss of a reasons, thus complicating inference activities: for this reason, "before we
wave; chill and sharp and yet (for a girl of eighteen as l then was) can assess the value of the characterising statement for the target character,
solemn, feeling as l did, standing there at the open window, that we must first discount aspects motivated by the characteriser or the situ-
something awful was about to happen [. . .]. ation" (Culpeper 2001, 171 ). An important factor influencing whether a
particular cue is discounted is the credibility of the character carrying out
Had this option been chosen, the character would of course be the the presentation, since statements coming from unreliable characters are
same, Clarissa Dalloway. However, she would probably sound totally dif- likely to be false. Generally speaking, it is therefore important to analyse
ferent in readers' minds, especially as regards how close they would feel to how different characters speak of a certain character before turning the
her. Would she therefore be the same character? information into a trait.
2.3.3 Implicit and explicit information about characters Conversations make use of lexis: whether a character is portrayed
while speaking to another character, or is presented during a monologue,
In addition to self- or other-presentation, information about characters can the way words are used is a crucial parameter for the readers' impressions
be cued in a range of other ways, including statements from the narrator or of him/her. Broadly speaking, very formal lexis tends to be associated with
character's language behaviour. quite pompous, serious - often boring - characters, whereas a more infor-
Very generally, two broad categories can be singled out for identifying mal use of words is generally perceived as the norm and associated with
useful information about characters: explicit or implicit cues. Starting from more friendly or down to earth characters. There is much more to this, of
the first ones, they coincide straightforwardly with the authorial 's naming course: one way of pinning down the relationship between lexis and char-
of traits, especially in descriptions or introductions. A character may be acter is to look at the predominant etymological origin of the words used
explicitly described as honest, or rich, or a liar by the author, and there by a character, since different stylistic dimensions correlate with lexical
is generally no reason why readers should not attribute those traits to the source. The most common words in English - the words of speech - tend to
character being described; the adverb 'generally' is here used because there have a Germanic origin, whereas words originating from Latin tend to be
are, indeed, examples ofunreliable narrators who deliberately say things in rarer and appear largely in written language: moreover, Germanic originat-
order to deceive their readers, as happens in The Murder of Roger Ackroyd ing words are more often used in informal, private contexts, whereas Latin
by Agatha Christie, or in William Thackeray's VanityFair. originating lexis is more formal and therefore used on public occasions.
Things are very different if we focus on implicit characterisation cues, Concrete things are usually referred to through Germanic terms, whereas
since that information always has to be derived by inference, and contex- more abstract concepts tend to be explained by Latin lexical items. It goes
tual factors also need to be taken into account. Both verbal and non-verbal without saying that a character using lots of Latin originating words is
behaviour all contribute to the formation of inferences, with verbal ones likely to be perceived as more educated and more formal than a character
having more weight. All verbal activity is very informative, and, as point- using merely words of Germanic origin, even if all these features work in
ed out above, interacts with schemata that readers have developed for real combination so it might of course happen that a very uneducated character
people: a character described with a very high pitch of voice, for instance, uses some Latin based words or that a very educated one is portrayed most-
may be perceived as agitated, or bossy; a character whose speech is very ly in informal situations where words of Germanic origin are preferred.
fast may be perceived as lacking self-confidence, or in an unusual emotion- Whatever the case, lexical richness plays a key role: Bradac (1982, 107)
al state, and so on. Culpeper (2001) is probably the most comprehensive and Culpeper (2001, 188) both explain how, by dividing the number of
study dealing with implicit cues so far: in his view, those cues comprehend different words by the total number of words spoken by one character, it
conversational, lexical, syntactical, paralinguistic, non-verbal and contex- is possible to observe the diversity of lexis relative to the size of the total
tual features, accent and dialect. All of them, although not providing di- sample of speech. Nowadays, this is easily managed by means of compu-
rect information about a character, trigger meaningful inferences that may tation corpus techniques. It is commonly accepted that a lower diversity
sometimes prove more important than the direct cues themselves. A short stands for lower communication competences, lower socio-economic sta-
discussion of each of them will be presented below. tus and a higher level of anxiety.
To start with, conversationalfeatures can provide useful insights about Studies on lexis have also focussed on the concept of surge (see for
power relations: by analysing how power is balanced between characters, example Caffi and Janney 1994, 328), a notion that is closely concerned
it may be possible to identify the most and the least powerful ones (see with personal affect and is conveyed by a range of linguistic features that
Sacks et al. 1974; Levinson 1983). One way of doing this is by identifying include evaluative lexis, modal verbs, lexical repetitions and pronouns.
the way in which talk is distributed among participants, in particular the According to Taavitsainen (1999, 219-220):
number of conversational moves for each participant, the length of each
tum, the way in which speakers alternate. This last point is particularly Personal affect is a component of participant relations andfinds
important: do speakers wait for their tum or do they just enter the 'con- outlets in various forms; thus it gives a picture of the persons
versational floor' whenever they feel? Do they interrupt each other? Who behavioural patterns and mental characteristics. It may exhibit more
interrupts whom? Do characters usually stick to a topic or do they tend to permanent qualities of emotion, with a long-term realisation, e.g.
change the topics they are discussing? All these points are related to pow- love, fondness, sadness or dislike, or more transient and volatile
er, as actions like interrupting, changing topic, speaking much more than states of mind like anger, or dislike erupting into telling someone
someone else tend to happen when a character is powerful enough to be off These outbursts of emotion can be called surge features. [. . .}
allowed to do so. According to my earlier studies, outbursts of emotion in surge
;;,u

featuresof personal affect include exclamation, swearing, and thus attitudes towards the narrated world have to be attributed to a speak-
pragmatic particles. ing voice distinct from the main one. Therefore a range of resources exists
for expressing attitudes, amplifying them and attributing them to sources.
A very promising and relatively new field of investigation related to Three types of attitudes have been defined: expressing emotion, judging
the notion of surge is Appraisal Theory, which was first developed in Syd- character and valuing the worth of things. Technically speaking, resources
ney within the broader Systemic Functional Linguistics framework. By fo- used to express emotions are referred to as affect, resources used to judge
cusing on people's evaluation practices, it considers aspects like 'attitudes' character are referred to as judgement, resources used to value the value of
negotiated by people in texts, together with the strength of the feelings things are referred to as appreciation. Clearly, this model can be a useful
involved and the ways in which values are sourced and readers aligned. tool to characterisation, especially when used in combination with the oth-
The resources provided by the appraisal framework are used to negotiate er aspects introduced above.
social relationships, therefore they are considered interpersonal resources.
Further significant means of signalling social information are terms of
Broadly speaking, Appraisal is concerned with attitudes, that is to say address, which include vocatives and pronouns and place characters with-
with how people - or characters - evaluate things and other people from in particular social groups. As Leech explains (1999), the main semantic
the linguistic point of view. Such evaluations can be more or less intense categories related to terms of address are ordered from those that signal
(more or less amplified) and come from different sources. In the following a familiar or very intimate relationship to those signalling a more distant
extract, for instance, different types of attitudes are used: and formal one, including endearments (my darling, love, sweetie, etc.),
He was working in a /.QJ2. security structure. It was the beginning of family terms (mummy, daddy, grandma, granny, etc.),familiarisers (guys,
a beautiful relationship. We even spoke about marriage. A bubblv. mate, folks, etc.),first names (with shortened first names as a subgroup),
vivacious man who beamed out wild energy. Sharply intelligent. titles and surnames and honorifics (sir, madam, etc.). The usage of terms
Even if he was an Englishman, he was popular with all the 'Boer' of address depends on several factors, including the emotive state of the
Afrikaners. And all my girlfriends envied me. Then one day he said speakers, the context, the relationship between the characters.
he was going on a 'trip'. 'We won't see each other again. . maybe Another implicit cue to account for in characterisation is syntax: even
never again'. I was torn to pieces. So was he (Quoted in Martin and though research is not very developed in this area, Scherer (1979, 172), has
Rose 2007, 26, my emphasis). associated simple syntax with more simple-minded characters, and more
complex syntax with more mentally complex ones. However, it is crucial
The speaking voice begins by appreciating the value of her partner's to keep in mind that even more developed characters might decide to use
work and their forthcoming relationship (a top security structure, a beau- simpler syntax, and Jess developed ones might use more complex syntax,
tiful relationship), then turns to the man's character, which she holds in on some occasions: characterisation is a complex process and the combina-
very high esteem (a bubbly vivacious man, wild energy, sharply intelligent, tion of several factors should always be the aim of the analyst.
popular), and finally she describes her girlfriends' emotional response to Accent and dialect are also important implicit cues: standard ac-
their relationship and her own feelings on their separation (envied, torn cents and dialects are stereotypically associated with a higher social sta-
to pieces). Her evaluations can thus be divided into three basic kinds, tus, whereas regional accents or dialects are expected to be used by less
according to what is being appraised: 1) the value of things, 2) people's ambitious and self-confident characters. Two main problems arise here:
character and 3) people's feelings16• Moreover, her evaluations are often which accent really constitutes the standard? What is, again, the role of the
amplified (sharply intelligent, wild energy, tom to pieces), which points context? In some contexts - local pubs, for instance - using the standard
out how attitudes are gradable - their intensity can be increased or de- accent may be considered snobbish, thus even more high-class people may
creased depending on the author's aim. Finally, it is important to consider opt for the local accent in order to feel more 'at home'; on the other hand,
from whom attitudes come: in this case, they all come from the speaking there might be contexts in which characters are expected to use standard
voice (narrator), but we should keep in mind that a common strategy is to accents but this does not happen. Clearly, in written texts indications about
create another voice in the story by using direct or indirect speech. In "I accents tend to come from the narrator, therefore indications of this nature
end with a few lines that my wastes vulture said to me one night: 'I have to may prove particularly significant for characterisation.
live with this hell", for instance, a second evaluating voice is introduced, As far as paralinguistic features are concerned, Giles and Powesland
(1975, 1-4) have pointed out that they might sometimes be more significant
16
This short overview of the Appraisal model owes much to Martin and Rose's with respect to social evaluation than the content of speech itself (they are
own explanation (2007, 25- 72). phonetic features and can be discussed independently of the sequences of
vowels and consonants, of the stress patterns of words, of the stressing of satiric or didactic writers can afford to be exuberantly inventive, or
lexical rather than grammatical words, and of intonation structure). Para- obviously allegorical, in their naming (Thwackum, Pumblechook,
linguistic cues are to do with particular aspects of personality and charac- Pilgrim). Realistic novelists favour mundane names with appropriate
ter, and usually work side by side with non-verbal ones. Culpeper takes the connotations (Emma Woodhouse, Adam Bede). The naming of
following paralinguistic cues into consideration (2001, 215-221 ): characters is always an important part of creating them, involving
many considerations, and hesitations, which I can most conveniently
• Speech tempo and non-fluency features: experimental work has
illustrate from my own experience.
suggested that the absence of hesitation pauses and a generally
faster speech rate usually stand/or extroversion, competence A writer can profit from the meaning potential of a particular name
and likeability. Slow speech, on the other hand, seems to suggest for different purposes, and in each case particular schemata are activated
seriousness and ponderousness. in readers' minds. Such schemata can later be confirmed (schema reinforc-
ing) or disrupted (schema refreshment) by the text, yet very rarely will the
• Pitch range: it is a good indicator of sex, the typical female choice of a particular name sound neutral to readers. It may also happen
range being higher. As far as male speakers are concerned, that characters choose a particular name for themselves, thus capitalising on
higher pitch may suggest extroversion and confidence. If a male intertextuality: indeed, not rarely the names chosen point back to legendary
speakers pitch results similar to a females one, then a high pitch figures of the past, a fact which provides significant information about the
may suggest weakness, nervousness and emotional instability. beliefs and ambitions of the character himself/herself The most famous
• Variation in pitch: generally speaking, a high variability in pitch case is probably Don Quixote de la Mancha, who chose his own name in the
is associated with dynamic and extroverted people, whereas a flat same manner as the heroes of the past used to do: by selecting a new name
voice with limited pitch movement suggests a more masculine, for themselves, in other words, characters are likely to activate particular
sluggish and withdrawn speaker. expectations in readers' minds, a fact which, together with all the other cues
discussed above, strongly contributes to the characterisation process.
Of course, in narrative :fiction this information comes from the narra-
tor, whereas things change significantly in drama. Kinesic features (how 2.3.4 N arratorial 'filters'
someone positions him/herself in relation to other people is correlated with
the degree of social distance, thus of power), appearance features (people As anticipated above, readers' access to :fictional worlds is always ':filtered'
generally tend to attribute more positive qualities to attractive people and by narrators. The degree of :filtering depends on different factors, and high-
negative qualities to unattractive ones) and characters company and set- ly affects character perception (see, for instance, Popova 2016). In this
ting (a person's surrounding can communicate a lot about that person: we section two :filtering strategies are introduced and discussed: point of view
tend to use our, schematic knowledge to 'encapsulate' a particular character and mind style.
on the basis of the people he/she mixes with and the places he/she usually
likes to go to) are other implicit cues that might contribute to the formation 2.3.4.1 Point of view
of a character image in reader's minds.
Finally, another crucial cue (that originates from an author's choice) is To start with, let us go back to the two extracts from McEwan 's novel
the name. As Lodge (1992, 36- 7) remarks: Atonement introduced in 2.2.1:

Our first names are usually given to us with semantic intent, having [1]
for our parents some pleasant or hopeful association. Surnames (I) 'Let me take that,' he said, stretching out a hand. (2) 'I'll.fill it for
however are generally perceived as arbitrary: we don i expect our you, and you take the flowers. '
neighbour Mr Sheperd to look after sheep, or mentally associate
him with that occupation. If he is a character in a novel, however, (3) 'I can manage, thanks.' (4) She was already holding the vase over
pastoral and perhaps biblical associations will inevitably come the basin.
into play. One of the great mysteries of literary history is what
exactly the supremely respectable Henry James meant by calling (5) But he said, 'Look, I've got it.' (6) And he had, tightly between
one of his characters Fanny Assingham. ln a novel names are forefinger and thumb. (7) 'Your cigarette will get wet. (8) Take the
never neutral. They always signify, if it is only ordinariness. Comic, flowers.'
ov

(9) This was a command on which he tried to confer urgent which Cecilia dared not disobey. (12) It was extraordinary that she
masculine authority. [ ... ] could not resist him. (13) At his insistence she was removing her
clothes, and at such speed. (14) She was out of her blouse, now she
(10) 'You idiot! (11) Look what you've done. ' had let her skirt drop to the ground and was stepping out of it, while
he looked on impatiently, hands on hips. (15) What strange power
(12) He looked into the water; then he looked back at her, and simply did he have over her. (16) Blackmail? (17) Threats? (18) [. . .] The
shook his head as he raised a hand to cover his mouth. (13) By this sequence was illogical - the drowning scene, followed by a rescue,
gesture he assumed full responsibility, but at that moment, she hated should have preceded the marriage proposal. (McEwan 2002: 38)
him for the inadequacy of the response. (14) He glanced towards the
As observed above, a significant number of textual cues indicate that
basin and sighed. (15) For a moment he thought she was about to
the same events are experienced from two totally different deictic centres
step backwards onto the vase, and he raised his hand and pointed,
in the two extracts: the first one is described by an external narrator who
though he said nothing. (16) Instead he began to unbutton his shirt.
seems to be endowed with a bird-eye view making it possible to shift from
(17) Immediately she knew what he was about. (18) Intolerable.
(19) He had come to the house and removed his shoes and socks one character to the other without keeping the focalization fixed; narration
- well, she would show him then. (20) She kicked off her sandals, of events, in other words, is perceived as objective, even though it is clear
unbuttoned her blouse and removed it, unfastened her skirt and that some words are likely to belong to the characters ("intolerable", for
stepped out of it and went to the basin wall. (21) He stood with example). Things are very different in the second extract, which is entirely
hands on his hips and stared as she climbed into the water in her focalized on Briony: to start with, the use of the modal auxiliary "must"
underwear. (22) Denying his help, any possibility of making amends, ( 1 ), makes it clear that the events are presented in the order that Brion y
was his punishment. (23) The unexpectedly freezing water that herself experiences them, a phenomenon Short calls "psychological se-
caused her to gasp was his punishment. (24) [. . .] He did not exist, quencing" (1996, 275). The shift in focalization is made even more evident
he was banished, and this was also the punishment. (McEwan 2002, by the prepositional phrase "in the distance at least" (2) and by the compar-
29-30) ative forms "closer" (3) and "nearer still" (3), which are to be interpreted in
relation to Briony's position: in actual fact, they refer to those parts of the
[2] park Briony is able to see from the nursery window. In addition, although
the extract is narrated in the third person, the language used is likely to be
(]) She [Briony] had arrived at one of the nursery's wide-open perceived as Briony's by the readers: "humble" (9), "boldness of ambition"
windows and must have seen what lay before her some seconds (9), "imperiously" (11 ), "extraordinary" (12), and "strange" (15), to quote
beforeshe registered it. (2) It was a scene that could easily have but some of them, are all instances of a negative attitude of the 'Judgement
accommodated, in the distance at least, a medieval castle. (3) type' (Martin and White 2005, 52) that are formulated by the character.
[. . .] Closer, within the boundaries of the balustrade, were the rose Briony's viewpoint becomes even more explicit in the sentence "There
gardens and, nearer still, the Tritonfountain and standing by the was something rather formal about the way he stood [ ... ]" ( 4), which also
basin's retaining wall was her sister; and right beforeher was Robbie coincides witli the moment readers start penetrating her mind. Such a mo-
Turner. (4) There was something rather formal about the way he ment is further signalled by the abrupt shift in narrative technique: unlike
stood, feet apart, head held back. (5) A proposal of marriage. (6) sentences 1 and 2, which are clearly a narrative report17, sentence 4 is an
Briony would not have been surprised. (7) She herself had written a example of free indirect thought, and such a shift becomes more evident in
tale in which a humble woodcutter saved a princess from drowning the following sentence ("A proposal of marriage"), which has the typical
and ended by marrying her. (8) What was presented here fitted (un)grammatical construction of colloquial speech (and is therefore more
well. (9) Robbie Turner; only son of a humble cleaning lady and likely to be attributed to Briony than to the author/narrator). Moreover,
of no known father, Robbie who had been subsidised by Briony 's
father through school and university, had wanted to be a landscape 17
The modal verb "must" (1), together with the conceptual content, establish-
gardener, and now wanted to take up medicine, had the boldness es the first sentence as thought external to Briony; in sentence 2, the modal
of ambition to ask for Cecilia's hand. (1 OJ It madeperfect sense. "could" appears to be the narrator stating that the physical conditions - "in the
[. . .] (11) What was less comprehensible, however, was how Robbie distance", a prepositional phrase significantly upshifted to the level of clause
imperiously raised his hand now, as though issuing a command - objectively warrant the application ofBriony's own mental schemata.
sentence 5 features a crucial parallelism with Cecilia's utterance "intol- earth, on an earth that wore the aspect of an unknown planet" (Conrad
erable" in the first extract (18): in both cases the content of the free di- 1899 [2007], 14). Clearly, the narrator is presenting the point of view of
rect thought is foregrounded by means of a structural strategy which, by Europeans, since the term "prehistoric" brings with it a set of assumptions
rankshifting the phrase to the level of a sentence, better records the mo- about cultural naivety which are connected to the Eurocentric attitude that
ment of highly emotional tension. Sentence 5 hence marks the moment is explored in Conrad's novel (see also Short 1996, 278).
when Briony's brain suddenly decides that the scene in front of her eyes is
to be interpreted as a marriage proposal. Quite significantly, the external 2.3.4.2 Mind style
description of the physical environment characterising the first three sen-
tences in narrative report (NR) is here replaced by a series of sentences in
Mind style is the second narratorialfilter used by authors to develop char-
free direct thought (FDT) which coincide with a mental interpretation of
acterisation. Like point of view, it is a bottom-up process, as it springs out
the events, such an interpretation being necessarily the result of Briony's from the language of the text and interacts with readers' schemata to form
intentionality and volition. the impression of a character in their mind.
Manipulation of viewpoint plays a crucial role in the presentation of
Introduced for the very first time by Fowler in order to capture "any
characters' minds in all novels: in Atonement, in particular, the juxtaposi-
distinctive linguistic representation of an individual mental self' (1977,
tion ofBriony's viewpoint with that of Cecilia and Robbie makes it possi-
103), the notion was later developed by Leech and Short in chapter 6 of
ble for the readers to gain better access to Briony's mind and to the sche-
their seminal work Style in Fiction (1981): a "fictional world", they ar-
mata she uses to make sense of the scene she is observing. If the first of
gued, is "what is apprehended", whereas mind style is concerned primarily
the two extracts was omitted, in other words, it would be very hard for the
with how a fictional world is apprehended and conceptualised (1981, 187).
readers to realise that Briony's interpretation of the fountain scene borders
Therefore, the notion has to do with the way in which characters make
on the absurd due to the inappropriate application of certain schemata. The
sense of the story world in which they are embedded, and can in principle
same narratological strategy is exploited by McEwan in other parts of the
be applied to all texts, even if a cline exists between "mind styles which
novel, as will be observed in the section analysing the use of metaphorical
can easily strike a reader as natural and uncontrived [ ... ], and those which
patterns for the description of fictional mental functioning (see paragraph
clearly impose an unorthodox conception of the fictional world" (1981,
2.3.4.2.2): as a result, readers are positioned closer to the mind of a particu-
188-9). In other words, the focus on how systematic linguistic choices re-
lar character, and have the chance to better understand the way that mind
works 18• flect the workings of individual minds becomes increasingly more obvious
as we move towards the unorthodox end of the cline: "the more normal
Generally speaking, the notion of point of view includes at least three
the choices become, the less force the mind-style concept tends to have"
major perspectives: spatiotemporal, psychological and ideological points
(Short 2006, 250), i.e. mind styles are more interesting (and more easily
of view. McEwan's extracts have highlighted how both spatiotemporal and
detected) when they are idiosyncratic, distinctive19•
psychological points of view are often interrelated, as the former often
gives access to the latter. As regards ideological point of view, on the other Clues about characters' mental functioning can originate from a wide
hand, it has to do with "the set of values, or belief system, communicat- range of different linguistic patterns, including metaphors, deixis, lexis,
ed by the language of a text" (Fowler 1996, 165): therefore, this concept etc., and have been analysed by different stylistic and narratological ap-
captures "those aspects of world views that are social, cultural, religious proaches. Very importantly, all of them contribute to shaping crucial as-
or political in origin" (Semino 2002, 97). Like the other two, ideological pects of characters' inner lives, so a general overview of each of them will
point of view is very important for characterisation, especially when value be offered in the next sections.
judgements are found· in a particular character's speech and/or thoughts
(Culpeper and Fernandez-Quintanilla 2017, 21). In Joseph Conrad's Heart 2.3.4.2.1 Mind style and narratology
of Darkness, for instance, the character-narrator Marlow describes his first
experience of Africa by stating that "We were wanderers on prehistoric One of the main attractions of reading fiction is that it can give us an in-
volving impression of what it is like to be somebody else - to do, feel
18
Importantly, experiencing the point of view of a character does not necessar- and think things that are not part of our own personal experience (Semino
ily grant access to that character's mind. In the opening of Dickens's Great
Expectations, for instance, the point of view is that ofa child (Pip, the protag- 19
In this volume, the expressions mind style, mental functioning and mind func-
onist of the novel) but readers cannot access his mind at that point. tioning are used interchangeably.
2007, 154): in other words, a very important part of suspension of disbelief the future (2004, 183). Embedded narratives, Palmer points out, are "the
is the possibility of accessing directly the minds of characters by means product of the application of the continuous consciousness frame to the
of different narrative techniques. This point has also been highlighted by discourse" (2004, 184): to put it differently, the continuous conscious-
different narratologists: Fludernik (1996, 30), for example, has pointed out ness frame is the means by which we are able to construct fictional minds,
that the reader's narrative experience always implies the protagonist's con- whereas the embedded narrative is the result of such a construction. Three
sciousness and has made clear that the centrality of this notion is closely subframes of the continuous consciousness frame are crucial for capturing
related to the prominence of consciousness representation in the 201h cen- characters' minds: the relationship between thought and action, interrnen-
tury (see part l of this volume). tal or group or shared thinking, doubly embedded narratives.
The representation of characters' minds is central also to Palmer's As regards the first one, it must be kept in mind that any construction
framework, where narrative fiction is considered "in essence, the rep- of a fictional mind is inextricably linked to the presentation of actions: in
resentation offictional mental functioning" (2004, 184). In Fictional Minds other words, direct access to inner speech and states of mind only repre-
(2004), he remarks how, in order to investigate characters' inner lives, nar- sents a small part of the process of making sense of a mind in action; phys-
ratology has traditionally used a speech category approach, namely an ical events are always associated with a mental event, textually cued by the
approach in which the categories traditionally applied to fictional speech verb or an adverb. The sentence "The three statesmen hid themselves", for
have been extended to fictional thought. Such an approach, he points out example, quite clearly represents the description of an action, yet it does
(2004, 53), is concerned primarily with that part of the mind known as in- more than that, since it also captures the related mental processes much
ner speech, but fails to account for other important aspects of mental life, more than, let's say, "They stood behind a curtain", that would leave the
in particular the ones he calls the whole mind (2004, 87-129), i.e. cognitive readers uncertain as to why "they" are standing there (Palmer 2004, 211 ).
activities like thinking and perceiving complemented by dispositions, feel- As regards the second one, intermental thinking is based on the notion
ings, beliefs and emotions; the social mind, (2004, 130- 169), i.e. the minds that mental functioning can be properly applied to social as well as to indi-
of characters captured while they are interacting with other characters; and vidual forms of activity, and is therefore closely connected to the concept
the mind in action (2004, 205-239), i.e. the mind of characters while they of communicative action, i.e. the kind of action happening when more than
are engaged doing something. one character is present. Examples singled out by Palmer include:
In his own approach, probably the most exhaustive model for the
study of characters' minds in narratology, Palmer focuses in particular on 1. Outside his door, two very limp detective sergeants had deserted
the notion of continuous consciousness frame: their posts.

The reader uses existing or prestored knowledge of other minds in 2. When they reached the pits they decided they were hungry. It
the actual world in order to process the emergent knowledge that is seemed too far to climb up the dining tent, so they ate as much
supplied by fictional-mind presentations. The work that we put into - of the mechanic's lunch as Miss Runcible 's cigarette had spared
constructing other real mindsprepares us as readers for the work of (2004, 222).
constructing.fictional minds. Because fictional beings are necessarily In the first example, the joint action of desertion exemplifies inter-
incomplete, frames, scripts, and preference rules are required to mental thinking: the verb deserted, according to him, captures the two
supply the defaults that.fill the gaps in the story-world and provide characters' mental processes better than what a description of a physical
the presuppositions that enable the reader to construct continually movement would do. In other words, even if the text does not make the
conscious mindsfrom the text (2004: 175-6). reasons for the characters' decision to leave explicit, their shared feelings
Clearly, the explanation above encompasses the notion of schemata or emotions can still be inferred from the context. In the second example,
introduced in 2.2.1, and extends it: to understand fictional mental function- a paradigmatic intermental thinking dynamic is presented, since an initial
ing, our consciousness needs to blend with the consciousness of the char- decision shared by both characters is explicitly reported and actualised af-
acters, thus creating a consciousness frame that continuously shifts from ter the adverb 'so' has been used.
our own mind to the one of the character, and vice versa. Once the continu- As far as the third subframe is concerned, doubly embedded narratives
ous consciousness frame is activated, it is possible to access the character's refers to a character whose mind is contained within another character's
embedded narrative, namely the whole of a character's mind, including mind: his/her identity, in other words, does not only encompass his/her em-
perceptual and cognitive viewpoint, ideological worldview, memories of bedded narrative only, but springs from all the doubly embedded narratives
the past and the set of beliefs, desires, intentions, motives and plans for of which he/she is the subject. Something similar happens in real people's
daily lives: they constantly create more or Jess detailed narratives of others their minds. As a consequence, no instances either of doubly embedded
in their minds, while making several efforts to control the narratives that narrative or of intermental thinking can be found, a strategy that might
the other people have of them. According to Palmer (2004, 232), "the so- explain the feeling of objectivity readers usually experience in Grillet's
cial role which we acquire as it is formed within the embedded narrative literature".
of others thus becomes part of our situated identity". The example below In conclusion, the newly expanded narratology of the fictional mind
describes a situation in which a character called Emma is talking to her proposed by Palmer can usefully complement all the other bottom-up ap-
friend Clara about an invitation she has recently received: proaches discussed in this part of the volume; together with the more tra-
ditional speech category models introduced above, and perhaps integrat-
I am very happy Robert has invited me to go to the cinema with
ed by frameworks originally conceived for real minds (psycholinguistics,
him, though I guess there must be some strange motive behind his
psychology and philosophy of the mind, to quote but a few), it can open
decision to invite me.
up new avenues of research for investigating the functioning of characters'
By speculating about the motives of Robert's invitation, Emma is cre- minds. An example will be introduced and discussed in Part 3 of this vol-
ating a doubly embedded narrative, i.e. she is creating the embedded narra- ume.
tive of another person in her own mind. As a consequence, readers have the
possibility to observe the functioning of two different minds at the same 2.3.4.2.2 Mind style and metaphorical patterns
time: Emma's one, captured while thinking about possible explanations for
Robert's invitation, and Robert's one introduced by Emma's speculations. Mind style can also be approached in relation to the use of figurative lan-
As a consequence of this, an ontological question is at issue here: what guage, as work by Fowler (1976) and Leech and Short (I 981) has brilliant-
can be said about a particular character's identity when contradictive ver- ly shown. The development of Cognitive Metaphor theory at least from the
sions of that character are presented through doubly embedded narratives? publication of Lakoff and Johnson's milestone Metaphors We Live By in
To state it with an example, how should we make sense of a character's particular has highlighted the crucial importance of particular metaphori-
personality whose traits are not explicitly defined by the narrator, but are cal patterns for the projection of mind style (see Black 1993; Semino and
defined by the doubly embedded narrative as negative (mean, for exam- Swindlehurst 1996; Semino 2002, 2006, 2014; Demjén, Zs6fia, and Elena
ple) and, by him/herself as generous? If the readers also find the character Semino 2016): the central claim of the theory is that consistent patterns of
mean, "there is an interesting sense in which the ontologically real char- conventional metaphorical expressions in language ( e.g. You are wasting
acter is less real than the epistemological versions" (Palmer 2004, 233). my time; that job cost me an hour) reflect conventional patterns of meta-
According to Margolin (I 996, 114-15), a character can be real within the phorical thought, known as conceptual metaphors ( e.g. TIME IS MON-
ontology of the story-world, and a second version can exist within that EY). Conceptual metaphors involve systematic sets of correspondences
same story-world, Generally speaking, characters are stipulated as real by between a source conceptual domain (e.g. MONEY) and a target concep-
the narrator, and as epistemic versions (I 996, 115) by the doubly embed- tual domain ( e.g. TIME), and typically help speakers to make sense of
ded narratives that contain them. abstract, complex, or poorly delineated experiences (such as time and emo-
Leaving the ontological discussion aside, it seems clear that an inter- tions) in terms of more concrete and well-delineated experiences. As Sem-
mental thinking approach to characters can also be very useful in order to ino and Swindlehurst (1996, 147) have remarked, the use of consistent and
capture the type of affective relationship among them: often, two or more unconventional metaphorical patterns within a particular text reflects the
characters involved in several intermental actions are emotionally very conceptual system of its creator. In the same vein, at an individual level:
close, either in a positive or in a negative way. In the same vein, the total The systematic use of a particular metaphor (or metaphors) reflects
Jack of intennental thoughts in contexts where they could reasonably take an idiosyncratic cognitive habit, a personal way of making sense of
place can often be interpreted as a sign of solitude, loneliness, suffering. and talking about the world: in other words, a particular mind style.
Clearly enough, the phenomena discussed by Palmer are not necessar- (Semino and Swindlehurst, 147)
ily present in all novels: some of them may be more highlighted than others
within the same novel, and different genres of novels may foreground dif- In their analysis of the novel One Flew Over the Cuckoo :S- Nest, for
ferent phenomena. Additionally, there may be novels in which such phe- example, Semino and Swindlehurst show how the language of the first-per-
nomena are not present at all: in the works by the French novelist Alain
Robbe-Grillet, for instance, characters are usually portrayed exclusively 20
Alain Robbe-Grillet is generally considered the father of the so-called Nou-
from the outside, with no reference to the mental processes happening in veau Roman, a literary trend that originated in France during the 1960s.
uo

son narrator Bromden is characterized by the consistent presence of un- hand) as their referent. In addition, it is significant that the character's hand
conventional metaphorical expressions drawn from the source domain of is perceived as, possibly, having a life of its own.
MACHINERY (e.g. I'm running out of steam or I don't want to throw a In cognitive metaphor terms, a series of targets (some parts of her
spanner in the works), that reflect his peculiar conceptualization of reality: body, other people) are explained through only one source (machines), the
society is described as the Combine (an allusion to combine harvesters), conceptual metaphors underlying these linguistic realizations being there-
the mental hospital where he lives as a patient as a factory for the Combine fore THE HUMAN BODY IS A MACHINE/HUMAN BEINGS ARE MA-
[. . .] for fixing up mistakes made in the neighbourhoods and in the schools CHINES. Even the expression "fleshy spider" (2), which does not seem
and in the churches, and the group of patients he belongs to as machines to refer to machines directly, may be processed by readers as a metaphor
with flaws inside that can't be repaired. This, they argue, projects a mind for machines, since science fiction has repeatedly portrayed 'post-human'
style that reflects the character's cognitive habits and limitations, since he bodies equipped with hands that are similar to spiders. Like her hands,
appears to use his knowledge of machinery to compensate for his lack of which do not seem to be "entirely at her command" (2) and may be viewed
understanding of other areas of experience, including other people's minds as a synecdoche for her larger self, even all the other people - "was every-
and emotions ( comprising his own), and society at large (I 996, 156). one else really alive as she was?" (6) - are processed by her as potentially
Cognitive metaphor theory has proved very useful for analysing the lacking the possibility of acting autonomously, a fact which is consistent
mind style of characters with cognitive limitations (Semino and Swindle- with the actantial model (she unwittingly applies) referred to above.
hurst 1996; Semino 2006, 2014); however, it can be applied to all charac- The machine model seems therefore to account for at least a part of
ters who use consistent patterns of metaphorical expressions. Going back her mental functioning, and is also in line with her habit to assign the other
to McEwan 's Atonement, for instance, Trevisan (2010) has shown that the characters stereotypical roles. Her sister's lover Robbie, in particular, is
central character's mind tends to make sense of the fictional world in a constantly conceived of as a potential aggressor, and such a tendency is not
mechanistic way which brings her to perceive everybody else as an actant rarely extended to the other characters, as the metaphorical expressions re-
incapable of developing outside the roles that have been thought of for lated to 'attack' and 'defence' in the following exchange make clear (2002:
them. Not surprisingly, such a cognitive habit is extended to herself: 140):
(1) The silence hissed in her ears and her vision was faintly distorted (1) "Shut up, for goodness' sake! (2) You really are a tiresome little
- her hands in her lap appeared unusually large and at the same time prima donna. (3) The boys had no clean socks so I took some of
remote, as though viewed across an immense distance. (2) She raised yours." - (4) Briony stared at her, amazed. (5) Attacked, betrayed, by
one hand and flexed its fingers and wondered, as she had sometimes the only one she longed to protect.
before, how this thing, this machine for gripping, this fleshy spider
In answering her sister Cecilia's accusation, which is realised by
on the end of her arm, came to be hers, entirely at her command. (3)
means of a metaphorical reference to the world of drama ("tiresome little
Or did it have some little life of its own? (4) [ . .} She brought her
prima donna", in the first sentence), Brion y resorts to a series of linguistic
forefingers closer to her face and stared at it, urging it to move. (5)
expressions ("attacked", "protect", in the second sentence) which reflect
It remained still because she was pretending, she was not entirely
the conceptual metaphor ARGUMENT IS WAR: as Lakoff and Johnson
serious, and because willing it to move, or being about to move it,
demonstrate (1980, 5), we "talk about arguments that way because we con-
was not the same as actually moving it. (6) [ . .} A second thought
ceive of them that way - and we act according to the way we conceive of
always followed the first, one mystery bred another: was everyone
things". Although the conceptual metaphor ARGUMENT IS WAR is quite
else really alive as she was? (7) [. . .} If the answer was no, then
common in people's conceptual system, it is particularly significant in the
Briony was surrounded by machines, intelligent and pleasant enough
case of Briony, since it is part of her more general tendency to conceptu-
on the outside, but lacking the bright and private inside feeling she
alize several events she experiences in terms of war, as is testified by the
had. (McEwan 2002: 35-36).
role aggression plays in her mental life. More generally, the metaphorical
The text, which intersperses the language of the narrator with that of expressions characterising Briony's language are drawn from the source
the character, is replete with non-standard metaphorical images that cast domains of WAR, ATTACK, ASSAULT, a fact that reflects her tendency
crucial light on Briony's distinct conceptualisations of reality: "thing" to cognitively elaborate many of the events in terms of 'aggression' and
(2),"machine for gripping" (2), and "fleshy spider" (2) are all unconven- has catastrophic consequences for the destiny of the characters, including
tional metaphorical expressions that have a part of Briony's body (her herself.
2.3.4.2.3 Mind style and deictical patterns al pronouns, objective pronouns, possessive adjectives. By using corpus
techniques that allow her to compare the distribution of person references
As pointed out by Sanchez-Davies (2017), "readers' capacity to project in the novel to the distribution of the same patterns in the Imaginative Writ-
themselves into different perspectives in the story-world is a major factor ing Section of British National Corpus, she finds that first-person singular
in their understanding of fictional characters and feeling of involvement pronouns are overused in the novel, while first-person plural pronouns are
with the narrative" (see also McIntyre 2006, 92; Jeffries 2010, 147-8). The underused (2011, 430). This, she argues, reflects Christopher's tendency to
area of stylistics focusing on the effects of perspective-taking is deixis, be unusually focused on himself, and, more specifically, his own actions
which concerns issues of distance and proximity in space, time and social and thoughts, which may contribute to the impression that he has an au-
relations, and is rooted on the notion that speakers tend to interpret deictic tistic-spectrum disorder. Additionally, the under-use of plural first-person
terms in relation to where they are positioned (McIntyre 2006, 50). Start- pronouns suggests "some degree of alienation from others" (2011, 431) or,
ing from the assumption that reading is an activity where the author/narra- in Palmer's words introduced in 2.3.4.2.1, a lack of 'interrnental thinking':
tor and the readers inhabit different (temporal) locations, readers need to
undergo a deictic shift to project themselves into the character's/ narrator's Overall, I would argue that, in The Curious Incident, the
deictic centre and make sense of the narrative. combination of overuse of singular first-person pronouns and
The framework called 'Deictic Shift Theory' (DST) was therefore de- underuse of both plural first-person pronouns and second-person
veloped in order to investigate the ways in which linguistic cues (grammat- pronouns contributes to create an impression of the particular kind
ical, lexical, figurative, etc.) may encourage or discourage readers to adopt of egocentricity that characterizes Christophe,'.He is a loner; and
a particular point of view by undergoing a deictic shift (Galbraith 1995; tends to be content in his own company. He also has difficulties
McIntyre 2006): as Galbraith (1995) explains, readers may be pushed from understanding the mental states of others, and seldom feels a sense
the real-world into a fictional world (a phenomenon he calls 'PUSHes') of affinity and commonality with them. This is consistent with the
or popped out of the fictional world (a phenomenon he calls 'POPs'), for attribution to Christopher of a high functioning autistic-spectrum
example when they are disturbed while reading. disorder such as Asperger s syndrome (Semino 2011, 436).
McIntyre (2006) expands Galbraith's explanation by pointing out that
Something similar happens in the short story Flowers for Algernon
it is possible to 'PUSH' and 'POP' in at least two different ways, the first
(1959), which describes a 'neuro-challenge': turning the protagonist Char-
being along the different levels of the discourse structure (author-reader,
lie into a neuro-typical subject, starting from a condition of severe de-
narrator-narrattee, character-character), and the second being along the
velopmental delay. In particular, a surgery already tested successfully on
temporal chronology of the fictional reality ( through flashbacks/ flashfor-
wards, for example). This, he explains, in itself highlights that "the terms Charlie's alter ego Algernon - a mouse who is initially able to beat Charlie
PUSH and POP ,suggest there are two processes involved in moving be- in many intelligence games - is supposed to significantly enhance his IQ,
tween deictic fields when actually there is only one" (McIntyre 2006, 110). which only reaches 68 at the beginning of the story. After what seems to
Deixis, however, is not limited to temporal and spatial reference points be an initial failure, it soon becomes clear that Charlie's transition will
only, but also includes dimensions which are central to the study of charac- not only tum him into a neuro-typical individual, but will rather trans-
ter's mind style: in particular, 'person' deixis, 'empathetic' deixis, 'social' form him into a genius whose mental functioning cannot be compared to
deixis, and 'discourse' deixis (Levinson 1983; McIntyre 2006; Stockwell any of the other characters in the story-world, including the experimenters
2002) can shed important light on characters' mental functioning, as they themselves. As the story unfolds, however, Charlie notices that Algernon's
can provide clues to their degree of egocentricity or openness towards oth- intelligence starts to slowly decrease, and through a series of highly so-
ers and towards the outside world in general. phisticated scientific argumentations he comes to the conclusion that the
Semino (2011), in particular, analyses the deictic patterns used by futuristic treatment he and Algernon have received will not last for long:
Christopher Boone, the first-person narrator and protagonist of the ac- his brain indeed undergoes a fatal deterioration which, by the end of the
claimed novel The Curious Incident of the Dog in the Night-Time pub- story, results in the character being as cognitively delayed as he was at the
lished in 2003. Christopher is described on the book's back cover as being beginning.
autistic, and specifically as being affected by Asperger's syndrome, a form The structure of the story therefore presents two versions of the char-
of high-functioning autism. In addition to phenomena of 'under-lexicali- acter: 'Charlie 1 ', who is severely retarded, and 'Charlie 2', who is a men-
zation' and 'over-lexicalization' characterizing the protagonist's language, tal genius with an incredibly high IQ. 'Charlie l' is present at the beginning
Semino focuses on the use of personal deixis, i.e. on the use of person- and at the end of the novel, whereas Charlie 2 inhabits the central part of
the book. Interestingly, the number of words in the two parts is very simi- "I think it's a good thing.finding out how everybodylaughs at me. I
lar, so they can be statistically compared. think about it a lot. It's because I'm so dumb I don't even know when
In addition to phenomena of under-lexicalisation that characterise I'm doing something dumb. People think it's funny when a dumb
mainly Charlie I, and of over-lexicalization that characterise Charlie 2, person can't do thins the same way they can" (Keyes, 1959: 293).
the analysis of deictical patterns is particularly illuminating in relation to
his mental functioning: after creating two texts (Charlie 1 text and Charlie
2 text), a keyness analysis of words was conducted by means of Wmatrix 2.3.4.2.4 Mind style and pragmatics
online, comparing text 1 and text 2. The analysis revealed that the subject
In addition to narratological, metaphorical and deictical patterns, charac-
pronoun 'I' is overused in text 1, namely in the part in which Charlie is
developmentally delayed: by occurring 485 times (versus 291 in text 2), it ters' mind style can also emerge through the presentation of their conver-
sational behaviour, especially when direct speech is used.
results in a log-likelihood of 19.15, which is a significant result. Second-
ly, a POS (part of speech) analysis was conducted on the two parts using In the last 30 years, pragmatics and conversation analysis have be-
the same tool: the analysis revealed that the most used verbs are mental come central areas oflinguistics and have been increasingly used by stylis-
processes, in Halliday and Mathiessen 's terms (Halliday and Matthiessen, ticians in the analysis of fictional conversations, particularly in plays ( e.g.
2014), namely verbs evoking cognitive and emotional states and changes: Culpeper 2001, Culpeper, Short and Verdonk 2002). As regards novels, it
to think, to mean, to suppose (2"\ 3rct and 41h position respectively). might be argued that salient and systematic patterns in a character's com-
municative behaviour can produce inferences about the peculiar function-
In order to collect more evidence, the use of personal deixis in the
ing of that character's mind, especially in cases where these behaviours are
impaired version of Charlie was also compared to the Imaginative Writing
non-deliberate.
Section of the British National Corpus (BNC) sampler: the keyness anal-
ysis for individual words revealed that the personal pronoun 'I' is the top Research concerned with the analysis of mind style by means of prag-
key-word in the story, with a log-likelihood of 516. 71; the object pronoun matics has mainly approached character's conversational behaviour in the
'me' and the possessive adjective 'my' were also found to be overused, light of Grice's maxims (1975, 44):
with a log-likelihood of 124.13 and 17.39 respectively. Crucially, the plu- Maxims of Quantity:
ral pronoun 'we' was absent both in the comparison for keyness between
text 1 and text 2 (Charlie's two versions), and in the comparison between 1. "Make your contribution as informative as required."
text 1 and the BNC sampler. 2. "Don't make your contribution more informative than is required."
Very interestingly, these findings are in line with Semino 's discussed
above, thus perhaps singling out a common trait in the mental functioning Maxims of Quality: Be truthful.
of characters who are cognitively delayed: on the one hand, the tendency 1. "Don't say what you believe to be false."
to be egocentric, i.e. to make sense of the world almost exclusively from
their own deictic centre, a fact which also accounts for their vulnerability 2. "Don't say what you lack adequate evidence for."
in social relations; on the other hand, the tendency to lack intennental po- Maxim of Relation: "Be relevant."
tential for sharing mental states with others, probably due to the fact that
they have not properly developed abilities to understand the workings of Maxims of Manner: "Be perspicuous."
others' minds (Semino 2011, 434; see also Palmer 2004, 205-240).
I. "Avoid obscurity of expression."
Crucially, by adopting the perspective of a character who is cogni-
tively impaired, the author defamilarises readers' everyday conversation- 2. "Avoid ambiguity."
al interaction, potentially refreshing their schemata (Cook 1994): while 3. "Be brief (avoid unnecessary prolixity)."
reading the story, we indeed have many chances to become more aware
of the potential for misunderstanding that 'normal' people usually avoid 4. "Be orderly."
successfully and automatically. Thanks to the story, in other words, we For example, in the previously mentioned analysis of Christopher in
can experience the complexities of communication we are generally not The Curious Incident of the Dog at Night-Time, Semino (2011) describes
conscious of, and at the same time become aware of the difficulties expe- the character's conversational exchange with a police officer some minutes
rienced by people who would not be classified as neurotypical individuals. after Christopher has found a dead dog in his neighbours' garden. The ex-
In Charlie's words: change is reported in the following extract:
, .
I) Then the police arrived. 2) I like the police. 3) They have [2]
uniforms and numbers and you know what they are meant to be
I tried to avoid all discussions of intellectual concepts and to keep
doing. 4) There was a policewoman and a policeman. 5) The
the conversation on a simple, everyday level, but she just stared at
policewoman had a little hole in her tights on her left ankle and
me blankly and asked what I meant about the mathematical variance
a red scratch in the middle of the hole. 6) The policeman had a
equivalence in Dobermann s Fifth Concerto. [. . .} No matter what
big orange leaf stuck to the bottom of his shoe which was poking
I try to discuss with her, I am unable to communicate. [. . .} I must
out from one side. 7) The policewoman put her arms round Mrs
review Vrostadt '.s equations on Levels o(Semantic Progression
Shears and led her back towards the house. 8) I lifted my head off
(Keyes, 1959: 298, bold mine).
the grass. 9) The policeman squatted down beside me and said,
"Would you like to tell me whats going on here, young man?" I 0) In the first extract, Charlie is reflecting upon a recent exchange he has
I sat up and said, "The dog is dead." 11) "I'd got that far," he had with Dr Strauss, the man who did his surgery and someone he used to
said. 12) 1 said, "J think someone killed the dog. " 13) "How old have great admiration for; in the second extract, on the other hand, he is
are you?" he asked. 14) 1 replied, "] am 15 years and 3 months reporting an interaction with Miss Kinnian, the teacher who helped him
and 2 days. (Haddon 2003, 7) when he was struggling with reading and writing and who is now his only
friend (and someone he is in love with). By saying that during a previous
As she remarks, the character has difficulties providing information interaction with Dr Strauss the latter was not able to discuss mathematics
at the level of detail that would normally be expected from a fifteen-year beyond what he terms "elementary levels", and that he could not speak lan-
old: he describes some features of the police officers' legs that are not guages other than Latin, Greek and Hebrew, Charlie makes it clear that that
required in that particular context, (a hole in the tights, a leaf stuck un- conversation had not been carried out "with reasonable dispatch" (maxim
der a shoe), and that will not turn out to be relevant to any subsequent of 'manner') by him, who was fruitlessly expecting his interlocutor to use
developments in the plot. Moreover, his answers to the policeman's intellectual categories he was not familiar with. In 2), Charlie is clearly
questions are either uninformative ("The dog is dead"; "I think some- trying to convey too complicated concepts to his previous mentor, Miss
one killed the dog") or too informative ("I am 15 years and 3 months Kinnian: despite his decision to "keep the conversation on a simple, every-
and 2 days"). From the pragmatic point of view, then, the character has day level", he still uses concepts like "mathematical variance equivalence"
difficulties with the maxims of quantity and relation introduced above. which embarrass her and produce blank stares. In other words, unlike the
Christopher, however, does not deliberately flout the maxims in order characters sharing the story-world with him, Charlie is now over-lexical-
to obtain some advantage for himself, but this behaviour is nonetheless ized in most of the fields he discusses, a fact that jeopardises his social rela-
very informative for the reader as it provides clues for a better under- tions by producing 'pragmatic failures': indeed, people do not usually talk
standing of the character's mental functioning. Therefore, Christopher's about mathematical variance equivalence in Dobermann's Fifth Concerto,
communicative behaviour can be interpreted as an infringement, i.e. a or Vrostadt's equations on Levels of Semantic Progression, yet he finds it
breaking of the rule which has no intention of deceiving his interlocutor strange that the other characters do not possess his same level of expertise.
(Thomas 1995, 74). Not rarely, Charlie's frustration in social situations is openly verbal-
Charlie's behaviour in Flowers for Algernon is not dissimilar from ised by him: in 2), he indeed complains about not being able to commu-
this: after the neuro-transition that has turned him into a genius, he is nicate properly with the other characters, i.e. to pragmatically fail for not
finally able to hold conversations on many different topics with the other being able to keep conversation at a simple level. The following extract is
characters, however these conversations are pragmatically problematic: emblematic from this point of view:

[1] [3]
1 was shocked to learn that the only ancient languages he could read Dr Nemur appears to be uncomfortable around me. Sometimes when
were Latin, Greek, and Hebrew, and that he knows almost nothing I try to talk to him, he just looks at me strangely and turns away. I was
of mathematics beyond the elementary levels of the calculus of angry at first when Dr Strauss told me 1 was giving Dr Nemur an
variation. I found myself almost annoyed. It was as if he'd hidden inferiority complex. [. . .} I am unable to communicate. [ ... ]I find I
this part of himself in order to deceive me, pretending - as do many don't communicate with people much any more. Thank God for books
people (Keyes, 1959: 297-298, bold mine). and music and things I can think about (Keyes, 1959: 298, bold mine).
Besides jeopardising his social relations Charlie's pragmatic failures what that means but everybody likes me and we have fun. I cant wait
also risk damaging the other characters' positive face (Brown and Levinson to be smart like my best frends Joe Carp and Frank Reilly (Keyes
1987, see also 2.3.6.1 this volume): Dr Nemur is experiencing an inferiority 1959, 290).
complex due to the fact that, after being introduced as over-educated at the
In this part of the story, Charlie is still in a condition of severe mental
beginning of the story, he is now face-threatened by Charlie's cultural su-
retardation, which also explains why he is not able to spell words properly.
periority and by his own inability to properly hold a conversation with him.
Examples in this sense include 'eye-q' for 'IQ', or 'motor-variation' for
Similarly to what was observed for Christopher in The Curious Inci-
'motivation'. According to Fowler (1996, 152), the inability to properly
dent of the Dod in the Night-Time, however, the breaking of Grice's maxim
use the lexical items normally shared among the speakers of a particu-
of manner does not seem to be caused by Charlie's intention to appear
lar language suggests that the individual lacks the relevant concept and is
superior, or to manipulate the people he interacts with: his uncooperative
likely to provide readers with clues on the distinctive way in which his/her
behaviour is rather to be interpreted as a result of his inability to assess his
mind works.
addressees' actual level of cultural competence, and can therefore be inter-
At this point in the plot, underlexicalisation concerns Charlie's lack
preted as an infringement.
of knowledge of scientific language, but there is more to this than that: as
In Thomas's terms (1995, 74), different reasons may impair a com- can be observed in the extract, the grammatical structures he uses tend to
municative performance: these include nervousness, drunkenness, excite- be fairly simple and repetitive ("Dr Strauss said", "He said", "I felt proud
ment, but also cognitive impairment, or simply the constitutionally incapa- when he said"), a fact that contributes to the readers' own perception of
bility of speaking clearly and to the point. In Charlie's case, the breaking the character's particular mental functioning. In addition, Charlie's report
of Grice's maxims provides evidence of his social difficulties, even when in extract 1 makes it clear that he cannot properly elaborate the content of
he is a genius. This, of course, strongly contributes to the projection of a Dr Strauss 's explanation, mainly because he lacks the proper schemata for
peculiar, idiosyncratic mind style. this particular situation: indeed, the Doctor tells him that the mouse's moti-
vation is driven by the cheese the researchers put in his box, but Charlie is
2.3.4.2.5 Mind style and under/over-lexicalisation not familiar with scientific experiments, therefore he bases his assumptions
solely on the comparison between himself and the mouse. Since the outset
The mind style of a character can also be conveyed by the degree of exper- of the story, then, the combination of underlexicalisation and the Jack of
tise he/she has in relation to particular domains. Characters can be over-lex- proper schemata contribute to the projection of a peculiar, non-standard
icalised or under-lexicalised (Fowler 1996, 152) in particular fields due to a mind style for the character.
number of reasons. In both cases, the phenomenon provides important clues In extract 2, misunderstanding with the other characters originates
for approaching the characters' mental functioning, especially when they are from Charlie being underlexicalised in areas people his age are generally
over-lexicalised in fields people are generally not expert in, or vice-versa. familiar with: in particular, as he explicitly states, the expression "Charlie
In Flowers for Algernon, for example, Charlie is initially under-lexi- is a card when hes potted" sounds totally obscure to him, thus preventing
calised in several fields the other characters are very familiar with, as the him from making sense of the situation - i.e. his colleagues making fun of
following two extracts make clear: him. As a consequence of this, he still enjoys the situation and erroneously
[1] believes his co-workers are also his close friends. There is, in other words,
a clear distinction between the actual domain of the story, in which Charlie
Dr Strauss said I had something that was ve,y good. He said I had is made fun of, and his own private subworld, which is characterised by
a good motor-vation. I never even knew I had that. I felt proud when his inability to imagine the contents of the other characters' minds, hence
he said that not every body with an eye-q of 68 had that thing. I dont misunderstanding originates.
know what it is or where I got it but he said Algernon had it too. Very interestingly, the parts in the text in which he is over-Iexicalised
Algernons motor-vation is the cheese they put in his box. But it cant also trigger clues about Charlie's mind style:
be that because I didnt eat any cheese this week (Keyes 1959, 287).
I must not become emotional. The facts and the results of my
[2] experiments are clear; and the more sensational aspects ofmy own
rapid climb cannot obscure the fact that the tripling of intelligence
Everybody faffed and we had a good time and they gave me lots of by the surgical technique developed by Drs. Strauss and Nemur must
drinks and Joe said Charlie is a card when hes potted. I dont know be viewed as having little or no practical applicability (at the present
10

time) to the increase of human intelligence. As I review the records in English, Clegg's conceptual pattern is necessarily idiosyncratic, a fea-
and data on Algernon, I see that although he is still in his physical ture of Clegg's personal mind style.
infancy, he has regressed mentally. Motor activity is impaired; At a certain point, however, the correspondences between butterflies
there is a general reduction of glandular activity; there is an and Miranda become weaker, and the character seems to be lost in his in-
accelerated loss of co-ordination. There are also strong indications terpretation efforts:
of progressive amnesia ], . .] (Keyes 1959, 301)
"Please", she said. Very gently and nicely. It was difficult to resist. It
Terms like "applicability", "motor activity", "glandular activity", was like not having a net and catching a specimen you wanted in your
"progressive amnesia" clearly require a type of expertise that not all people first and second.fingers (I was always very clever at that), coming
have: in Charlie's case, this is even more striking if we consider the lack up slowly behind and you had it, but you had to nip the thorax, and
of very common concepts that characterise his other version. Clearly, this it would be quivering there. It wasn't easy like it was with a kil/ing-
is very indicative of his mental functioning in this part of the story: he is bottle. And it was twice as difficult with her, because I didn't want to
now very smart and he easily acquires knowledge about several scientific kill her, that was the last thing I wanted (Fowles, 1963: 40).
domains (see also extracts I and II section 3.4.2.4).
Miranda has just tried to convince Clegg to free her, promising him
Generally speaking, under and over-lexicalisation are very indicative that she will not report him to police and assuring him she will meet him
of mind style when the character is particularly expert in one domain and whenever he wants. As he always does, Clegg uses his knowledge of but-
tends to make sense of all aspects of the story-world according to that terflies to speak about her metaphorically (a 'specimen') but things are
domain. In John Fowles's The Collector (1963), for instance, the protag- more difficult now, given that as a collector he rarely frees the animals he
onist Clegg kidnaps Miranda, a twenty-year old middle-class woman he collects. The butterfly metaphor, in other words, cannot properly explain
is in love with. Clegg is obsessed with collecting butterflies and seems to the situation Clegg is experiencing, as the sentence "It was twice as dif-
have no other interests in his life apart from this. As a consequence, he is ficult with her, because I didn't want to kill her, that was the last thing I
over-lexicalised in that domain, to the point that he is unable to make sense wanted" makes clear. Clegg's attitude towards Miranda is very different
of the world outside that particular field. As Semino shows (2002, 110), from his attitude towards butterflies, but he continues to deploy the only
Miranda herself is constructed from the beginning of the narrative as if she domain in which he is overlexicalised, even if that domain creates prob-
was a butterfly: lematic correspondences. This is very indicative of his mind style. For a
[I] more comprehensive analysis of this, see Semino (2002).

When I had a free moment from the.files and ledgers I stood by the 2.3.4.2.6 Mind style and corpus stylistics
window and used to look down over the road over the frosting and
sometimes I'd see her ln the evening I marked it in my observations The remarkable growth of corpus linguistics in the last three decades has
diary, at first with X, and then when I knew her name with M provided new exciting opportunities and tools for the linguistic study of
(Fowles, 1963: 9). literature (Bednarek 2010, 2011; McIntyre and Walker 2019), particularly
with respect to the analysis of systematic linguistic patterns that may con-
[II] tribute to the projection of mind style (Hoover 1999; Archer and McIntyre
2005; Semino 2011, 2016; Mahlberg 2012). Corpus-based techniques can
Seeing her always made me feel like I was catching a rarity, going up
be exploited both to test the analyst's intuitions and to identify patterns that
to it very careful, heart-in-mouth, as they say. A Pale Clouded Yellow,
might have otherwise been missed.
for instance. I always thought of her like that, I mean words like
elusive and sporadic, and very refined-not like the other ones, even There are many different software packages available for carrying out
corpus analyses, but in this section I will only focus on one, which was
the pretty ones. More for the real connoisseur (Fowles, 1963: 9).
used for collecting the data presented in 2.3.4.2.3: here, data presented in
The frequency and elaboration of metaphorical expressions drawing that section will be slighty expanded to show the potential of the approach.
from the source domain of butterflies suggest that a systematic set of cor- The software is called 'Wmatrix online' and was developed by Paul Ray-
respondences between the butterfly domain and the Miranda domain is part son, from the University of Lancaster (UK). It allows researchers to carry
ofC!egg's conceptual structure (Semino 2002, 110) and, since the relevant out analyses at the level of word, part of speech, or semantics. Once a
metaphorical expressions are not part of a particular conventional pattern particular corpus (in this case, text 1 and text 2 from Flowers from Alge-
rnon- i.e. 'Charlie as a mentally retarded character' versus 'Charlie as a Conceiving of language as a systemic resource to construe meaning in
cognitive genius') has been uploaded, it needs to be compared to another, everyday interactions (Halliday 1978; Halliday & Matthiessen 2004), sys-
larger corpus in order to investigate keyness. In this case, since the corpus temic functional linguistics (SFL henceforth) sets forth a stratification ar-
was a fictional one, it was decided to compare it to the Imaginative Writ- chitecture leading from context to semantics to lexicogrammar to phonolo-
ing Section of the British National Corpus (BNC) sampler, which contains gy through ever more delicate choices. Contextual information is proposed
about 231000 words belonging to several works of fiction. to encompass three key variables, namely: field (the subject matter and
As anticipated in 2.3.4.2.3, the two texts with the two 'versions' of overall setting governing the communicative event), tenor (the relation-
Charlie were compared to each other, revealing that in version 1 there is an ship among physical or implied interlocutors), and mode (the role assigned
overuse of the personal pronoun 'I', this overuse being statistically signif- to language in a particular situation, guided by the interlocutors' expecta-
icant. The keyness analysis investigating the overuse of a particular word tions). These parameters are linguistically manifested in three strands of
or structure in the corpus, compared to the other one, was also conducted meaning, termed ideational ( concerned with the construal of events and
with the BNC sampler as the reference corpus, revealing that the personal entities in the world), interpersonal ( concerned with the representations of
pronoun 'I' is the top key-word in the story, with a very high log-likelihood relationships among people in a situation), and textual (concerned with the
of 516. 71. The object pronoun "me" and the possessive adjective "my" role of the medium of communication and the text's rhetorical function).
were also found to be overused, with a Jog-likelihood of 124.13 and 17.39 Chosen configurations in these semantic dimensions are realized by par-
respectively. Therefore, corpus techniques revealed that the personal pro- ticular lexico-grammatical patterns, themselves selected from a wide range
noun 'I' is overused in Charlie's 'developmentally delayed' version com- of options. Those wordings are then realized as phonological sequences,
pared to Charlie's other version and in comparison to the BNC corpus. The which are in tum realized by phonetic items. With this overall framework
software also provides the possibility of a part of speech analysis, which and numerous specifications therefrom, SFL is capable of accounting for
was conducted on Charlie's two versions: the analysis revealed that the a myriad of linguistic, communicative, and discourse-level phenomena
most used verbs are "think", "mean", "suppose", suggesting a mind style (Hasan 2019; Martin & Zappavigna 2019), beyond the constraints of for-
more inclined to reflecting about things, rather than to acting upon things. mal theories focused on sentential analysis.
As pointed out above, the software also allows the investigation of The only strand of meaning we are currently interested in for the study
under-used linguistic patterns: in this case, it was found that the first per- of transitivity patterns is the 'ideational', which is concerned with how
son plural pronoun "we" was absent both in the comparison for keyness reality is represented in language, and is realised through a series of lexi-
between text 1 and text 2 (Charlie's two versions), and in the comparison co-grammatical choices regarding process types, participants and circum-
between text I and the BNC sampler. This suggested a mind style more in- stances: every speaker, in other words, makes sense of reality by selecting
clined to egocentricity and Jess prone to social relationships with the other from a limited set of process types and participant roles embedded in the
characters, which is also a typical trait of subjects with some developmen- grammar of the clause. In the sentence 'Mark kicked the ball', for example,
tal delays like autism. both 'Mark' and 'the ball' are considered participants ('Mark' is a partic-
Corpus-based methods, it goes without saying, are not the holy grail ipant called 'actor' and 'the ball' is a participant called 'goal'), whereas
for linguistic analysis: without competence, statistical expertise, qualitative 'kicked' is the process. In addition, language also gives us the possibility
knowledge, and intuition they are totally useless. Having said that, their po- to construe a circumstance, i.e. a time, a location or a manner in which the
tential can be incredibly high for many areas of linguistics, not only irt sty- process was carried out: 'Mark kicked the ball in the garden/an hour ago/
listics: critical discourse studies, forensic linguistics, translation studies, film with great energy'.
studies all benefit greatly from the application of corpus techniques. Word In the grammar of English, there are six types of processes: material,
frequency, multi-word frequency, keywords, concordance, are only few of mental, verbal, relational, behavioural, and existential. For each of them,
the tools analysts can use to obtain immediate feedback for their intuitions. participants have different names, as the six examples below make clear:
I. David (Actor) hit (Process: material) Tom (Goal) yesterday
2.3.5 Transitivity patterns and characterisation
evening (Circumstance: time).
Among the bottom-up information contributing to the formation of a char- 2. Mary (Senser) loves (Process: mental) pizza (Phenomenon).
acter image in readers' minds, transitivity patterns play a very central role.
Considering their importance and the technicality of the theory underlying 3. He (Sayer) always asks (Process: verbal) lots of questions (Verbi-
them, a short introduction to its main tenets is offered below. age).
4. Nick (Carrier) is (Process: intensive) a professor (Attribute)/Nick Moreover, approaching characters through transitivity structures can pro-
(Possessor as Carrier) has (Process: intensive) two dogs (Thing vide important clues as to whether they tend to be doers or thinkers, or
possessed as Attribute as to whether they have the possibility to carry out actions independently
and autonomously, or whether they are more dominated by external forces
5. He (Behaver) smiled (Process: behavioural) joyfully (Circum- heavily influencing their actions. In the corpus-based study of Flowers for
stance: manner). Algernon discussed above, for example, a POS analysis has demonstrated
6. There was (Process: existential) a lot of traffic (Existent) in Lon- that the personal pronoun 'I' is almost exclusively accompanied by men-
don (Circumstance: location). tal processes like 'think', 'mean', 'suppose', suggesting that the character
tends to be more inclined to reflect upon things, rather than to act upon
Starting with material processes, they are processes of doing, and them.
can be transitive (when a goal is present) or intransitive (when there is no In a milestone study published in 1971, the father of SFL Michael
goal): for example, the sentence "David (Actor) has been working (Pro- Halliday applied the framework to the analysis of Lok, one of the central
cess: material) all day (Circumstance: time)" is intransitive, as the action is characters in William Golding's The Inheritors (1955). The novel deals
not directed to another participant. Mental processes, on the other hand, are with a prehistoric struggle for survival: the characters of the book are a
processes of feeling, and include cognition (verbs of thinking, knowing, small number of Neanderthal men and women, who call themselves 'the
understanding), affection, (verbs of liking and fearing), perception (verbs people', and a group of invaders whom they call 'others', or 'new people'.
of seeing, hearing, etc.). Unlike material processes, mental ones always The impact between the two tribes will eventually cause the end of the first
need to have at least two participants, one of which must be a conscious group, the 'people'. Most of the book is narrated from the point of view of
human being (Senser). Verbal Processes are processes of verbal action, Lok, who observes the rituals and habits of the invaders from his hiding
typically realised by verbs like 'say', 'tell' and all their synonyms. They place beneath a tree.
usually contain three participants: a sayer, a receiver and a verbiage, the
Halliday argues that Lok's limited experience ofreality around him is
sayer coinciding with whatever can put out a signal (not only conscious
sharply conveyed by the syntactic structures he uses:
beings), and the verbiage coinciding with the nominalised statement of
the verbal process (usually a noun expressing some kind of verbal behav- ln The Inheritors. Golding is offering 'a particular way of looking
iour). Relational processes are processes of characterisation or identifica- at experience', a vision of things which he ascribes to Neanderthal
tion. They cover a very complex and rich area of transitivity (see Halliday man; and he conveys this by syntactic prominence, by the frequency
and Mathiessen 2004, 210-248 for an exhaustive introduction), and mainly with which he selects certain key syntactic options. It is their
describe reality by means of 'being' or 'having'. Behavioural Processes frequency, which establishes the clause types in question as
describe spontaneous or uncontrolled internal or external human activities prominent[ .. .] (Halliday 1981, 343).
and are somehow 'half-way' between mental and material processes. Typ-
ically, they are processes of physiological or psychological behaviour and In particular, Halliday uses the tenets of SFL to approach characterisa-
are realised by verbs like breath, cough, dream, laugh, sniff, stare, smile, tion of Lok in the following extract:
watch, etc. Finally, existential processes represent experience by declaring The bushes twitched again. Lok steadied by the tree and gazed. A
that 'there is' or 'there was' something: they are quite easy to identify since head and a chest faced him, half-hidden. There were white bone
the structure involves the use of 'there' as an existential subject rather than things behind the leaves and hair. The man had white bone things
as a circumstance of location. above his eyes and under the mouth so that his face was longer
With all this in mind, carrying out a transitivity analysis involves de- than a face should be. The man turned sideways in the bushes and
termining the process types, the participants and the circumstances realised looked at Lok along his shoulder. A stick rose upright and there
in each clause: this type of investigation provides very significant clues as was a lump of bone in the middle. Lok peered at the stick and the
to how experiential meanings, i.e. meanings about the world and personal lump of bone and the small eyes in the bone things over the face.
experience, are encoded through language choices by a real speaker or a Suddenly Lok understood that the man was holding the stick out to
character. him but neither he nor Lok could reach across the river. He would
Quite intuitively, the investigation of transitivity patterns can be very have laughed if it were not for the echo of the screaming in his
useful to distinguish, among other things, characters who are more active, head. The stick began to grow shorter at both ends. Then it shot out
more agentive, from those who are more acted-upon, passive, powerless. to full length again.
O'f

The dead tree by Lok s ear acquired a voice. foreground a general feeling of inaction, stillness, paralysis. Moreover, it
is quite significant that half of the actors of the material processes are not
'Clop'. people, but body parts, thus again shedding light on the lack of agency of
characters.
His ears twitched and he turned to the tree. By his face there had The transitivity patterns, in other words, provide important clues for
grown a twig: a twig that smelt of other, and of goose, and of the identifying Lok 's idiosyncratic way of elaborating reality. The case of the
bitter berries that Lok s stomach told him he must not eat. This twig stick is emblematic from this point of view: there is no doubt that the pro-
had a white bone at the end. There were hooks in the bone and sticky cesses in "a stick rose upright" and "the stick began to grow shorter on
brown stuff hung in the crooks. His nose examined this stuff and did both hands" are goal-directed, therefore transitive; quite intuitively, some-
not like it. He smelled along the shaft of the twig The leaves on the one held up a bow and someone drew an arrow. However, in Lok's men-
twig were red feathers and reminded him of goose. He was lost in a tal re-elaboration of the event, the processes are undirected, self-caused:
generalised astonishment and excitement. He shouted at the green therefore, the syntactic foregrounding of intransitives conveys the limita-
drifts across the glittering water and hear Liku crying out in answer tions of actions of 'the people'.
but could not catch the words. They were cut off suddenly as though Most of the other process types have the same pattern: mental pro-
someone had clapped a hand over her mouth. He rushed to the edge cesses tend to be limited to a single participant, and the 'phenomenon' (i.e.
of the water and came back. On either side of the open bank the the content of perception) is either not expressed, or expressed only indi-
bushes grew thickly in the.flood; they waded out until at their farthest rectly ("he smelled along the shaft of the twig"); in addition, the subject
some of the leaves were opening under water; and these bushes of cognition is rarely a human being, but rather a 'sense organ' ("his nose
leaned over. examined this stuff and did not like it"). Quite interestingly, there are no
examples in which Lok's own actions extend beyond himself: similarly to
s
The echo of Liku voice in his head sent him trembling at this the other members of his tribe, he does not seem prepared to open himself
perilous way of bushes towards the island. He dashed at them where to otherness and to 'act upon the world' in a new and more striking way. In
normally they would have been rooted on dry land and his feet Halliday's words (1981, 349):
splashed. He threw himself forward and grabbed at the branches with The entire transitivity structure of language can be summed up by
hands and feet. He shouted: 'I am coming!' (Golding 1955, 106-7) saying that there is no cause and effect. More specifically: in this
The passage, narrated by a third person narrator, is not always easy language, processes are seldom represented as resulting from an
to understand: what does the sentence "the stick began to grow shorter at external cause; in those instances where they are, the 'agent' is
both ends" mean, for example? Would that same concept be communicated seldom a human being; and where it is a human being, it is seldom
differently if the point of view had not been Lok's, but a member of the one of the people. Whatever the type of process, there tends to be
others? By referring to the categories introduced above, Halliday explains only one participant; any other entities are involved only indirectly,
that, out of the 56 clauses composing the extract, 21 of them are material as circumstantial elements (syntactically, through the mediation
processes, 14 are clauses of location (including possession), 16 are mental of a preposition). It is as if doing was as passive as seeing, and
processes, and the others are attributive clauses. The processes are normal- things no more affected by actions than by perceptions: their role
ly expressed by finite verbs in simple past tense ( 46), half of them describ- is as in clauses of mental process, where the object of perception is
ing very simple movements (turn, rise, hold, reach, etc.); the majority of not in any sense 'acted upon' - it is in fact the perceiver that is the
these processes are intransitive, their typical pattern being exemplified by 'affectedparticipant, not the thing perceived- and likewise tends
the first two clauses: "The bushes twitched again" and "Lok steadied by to be expressed circumstantially (e.g. Lok peered at the stick). There
the tree". is no effective relation between persons and objects: people do not
bring about events in which anything other than themselves, or parts
The overall picture construed by this syntactic configuration is one in
of the bodies, are implicated.
which the characters, despite acting, do not seem to act on things: in other
words, their actions do not seem to affect the objects around them. As an In conclusion, the importance of transitivity patterns for the study
example, in the sentence "he grabbed at the branches" a transitive verb of characters has been established for at least 40 years. Despite the tech-
usually accompanied by a direct object in the English language is used nicality and the complexity of its structures, SFL can offer crucial tools
intransitively: recurrent and systematic deviations from the norm like this for approaching the way in which characters make sense of the world
(ideational metafunctions) and the dynamics among them (interperson- force of a speaker's utterance: Culpeper (2001, 239) reports the example
al metafunctions, not discussed here). After Halliday, many stylisticians "I wondered if I could trouble you to do this", which threatens the neg-
have used SFL-based approaches (Hasan 1985; Hoover 1999; Taylor-Tor- ative face of a speaker's interlocutor similarly to "Do this!" but is much
sello 1990; Miller and Turci 2007; Miller 2017), while others have heav- more softened, therefore much more polite. Moreover, the degree of face-
ily criticised it: in 'What is Stylistics and Why are They Saying Such threat of a particular act varies significantly according to three factors:
Terrible Things About It?', for example, Stanley Fish (1979) condemned the social distance between participants, the relative power of the hearer
the use of SFL for the study of literature, by pointing out that the theory above the speaker and the absolute ranking of the imposition involved in
only "provides labels for its constituents. [ ... ] When a text is run through the act. In human relationships, it is essential to "minimize ( other things
Halliday's machine, its parts are first disassembled, then labelled, and fi- being equal) the expression of impolite beliefs" and to "maximize ( other
nally recombined into their original form" (1979, 137). Forty years later, things being equal) the expression of polite beliefs" (Leech 1983, 81 ).
analyses by scholars from all around the world have shown that things are The type of relationship between the interlocutors is central in these re-
somehow different. gards, 'directness' being closely related to the degree of intimacy be-
tween the speakers.
2.3.6 Dialogue and characterisation The power of (im)politeness configurations for characterisation can
be better understood with reference to a well-known scene from the movie
In the last decade, scholars have often used the expression 'interpersonal Scent of a Woman ( 1992), already analysed by other stylisticians (Culpeper
pragmatics' to approach characterisation starting from a character's con- 2001; Culpeper and Fernandez-Quintanila 2017) for its power of exempli-
versational behaviour, and from dialogue in particular (Locher and Sage fication: set in the US, the movie narrates the relationship between Charlie,
2010; Haugh, Kadar and Mills 2013; Bednarek 2018). This section will a student at a prestigious private school supported by student aid, and the
cover two main areas of interest for the study of dialogue: politeness and 'colonel', and old and very educated man who is now blind and occasion-
impoliteness, and expressive identity. ally looked after by Charlie in exchange for some money. The following
extract reports their first encounter:
2.3.6.1 Politeness and impoliteness
CH: Sir? (1)
(lm)politeness frameworks have been used in stylistics mainly for the COL: Don't call me sir. (2)
study of drama (Leech 1992; Simpson 1989, 1993) and, more occasionally,
prose (Martinez-Cabeza Lombardo 1994). More recently, the framework CH: I'm sorry, I mean mister, sir. (3)
has also been used to approach characterisation in TV or film dialogue, COL: Uh-ooh, we've got a moron here, is that it? (4)
(Mandala 2011; Bednarek 2012; Dyne] 2012, 2015;). In their seminal work
on politeness, Brown and Levinson ( 1987) had already pointed out that "an CH: No mister .... I....er .... that is .... er ... .Jieutenant, yes sir .. .lieu - (5)
understanding of the significant dimensions on which interaction varies
COL: Lieutenant-Colonel. 26 years on the line, nobody ever busted
should provide insights into the dimensions on which personality is built,
me four grades before. Get in here, you idiot. [Pause] Come a
as well as social relationships" (1987, 232).
little closer, I want to get a better look at you. How's your skin,
Central to the analysis of politeness is the concept of face, which is son? (6)
often associated to notions such as reputation, prestige and self-esteem.
Face can be positive or negative, positive face coinciding with the "want CH: My skin, sir? (7)
of every member that his wants be desirable to at least some others [ ... ]
COL: Ah, for Christ's sake! (8)
in particular, it includes the desire to be ratified, understood, approved
of, liked or admired" (Brown and Levinson 1987, 61 ). Negative face, on CH: I'm sorry, I don't- (9)
the other hand, is defined as "the want of every competent adult member
that his actions be unimpeded by others" (Brown and Levinson 1987, 62). COL: Just call me Frank. Call me Mr. Slade. Call me Colonel, if you
Any action encroaching upon a person's face is considered a/ace-threat- must. Just don't call me sir. (10)
ening act (FTA): orders, insults and criticisms are typical examples of CH: Alright, Colonel. (11)
it. In order to maintain their face, people constantly use redressive strat-
egies, typical examples being certain linguistic elements that soften the COL: Simms, Charles, a senior. You on student aid, Simms? (12)
CH: Ah, yes I am. (13) Very interestingly, towards the end of the movie the following ex-
change seems to sharply deviate from the norm established by the previous
COL: For student aid read crook. Your father peddles card telephones conversation behaviour:
at a 300 per cent mark-up; your mother works on heavy commis-
sion in a camera store, graduated to it from expresso machines. COL: Get out of here. (I)
Ha, ha! What are you ... dying of some wasting disease? (14)
CH: I'm staying right here. (2)
CH: No ... I'm right here. (15)
COL: Get out of here. (3)
COL: I know exactly where your body is. What I'm looking for is
CH: I'm staying right here. (4)
some indication of a brain. ( 16)
COL: I'll blow your fucking head off. (5)
Considering that the characters are complete strangers and that the
young one is taking care of the older, viewers' schemata regarding inter- CH: Do it. You want to do it, do it. Let's go. (6)
personal relationships in this context are frustrated from the start: indeed,
since the outset the colonel's conversational behaviour 'slaps' Charlie in COL: Get out of here. (7)
the face - negative face. In other words, readers' prior knowledge of the
CH: Look, you fucked up, alright, so what? So everybody does it.
role relationship between Charlie ( carer) and the Colonel ( cared for) and
Get on with your life would you. (8)
its power implications may more easily have given rise to contrary expec-
tations about their behaviour. COL: What life? I got no life. I'm in the dark here, you understand?
In tum 2, for example, the Colonel uses an imperative form to carry I'm in the dark. (9)
out a command, and the same verb tense is repeatedly used throughout the
whole dialogue. The Colonel's turns are brusque and transgressive of social CH: So give up. You want to give up, give up. Because I'm giving
rules: first, he is interested in taboo information (the student's income, for you up too. You say I'm thorough, you're right. I'm through.
instance), then he offends Charlie's parents without any evidence - thus We're both through. It's all over. So let's get on with it, Jet's
breaking Grice's maxim of quality. He also threatens Charlie's positive face fucking do it. Let's fucking ... pull the trigger ... you miserable
by using the term of address "moron" or "you idiot" in turns 4 and 6. De- blind motherfucker. Pull the trigger. ( 10)
spite the Colonel's provocations, Charlie's behaviour is polite and respect- The Colonel is preparing to kill himself with a gun and Charlie has
ful of social rules: he speaks much less, uses deferential terms of address tried to stop him by resorting to a series of politeness strategies, as we
("sir", turns 1 and 7; "Colonel" tum 11 ), apologises and keeps trying to would expect. When he realises he will not be able to get the gun from the
comply with h1s interlocutor's requests to do something. The correspond- Colonel's hands, however, his conversational behaviour changes abruptly:
ent inferences readers are likely to draw from the Colonel's conversational for the first time he defies the Colonel's commands (turns 2 and 4) and uses
behaviour go in the direction of a warped personality, perhaps with misan- taboo words (turns 8 and 10) in order to become impolite (tum 10) and
thropic traits (see also Culpeper 2001, 257). Charlie, on the other hand, is cause maximum face damage (see also Culpeper 2001, 260). By deviating
likely to be perceived as a prototypically shy, a bit passive, and a generally dramatically from his usual pattern of behaviour, he thus triggers a new se-
positive character thanks to his consistent and somehow unexpected polite ries of correspondent inference: traits like determination or bravery might
behaviour, which does not change despite the Colonel's attacks. be now attributed to him. By being now ready to use any means to achieve
As observed above (see, in particular, 2.1 and 2.2), the impression his goal, in other words, he shifts from the status of flat character to the
of characters in readers' minds results from the combination of top-down status of round character in readers' minds, i.e. to the status of a character
and bottom-up information. The former coincides with readers' schemata who can surprise and evolve (see Part 1 of this volume for the distinction
about people and situations, those schemata being reinforced or refreshed between flat and round characters).
by the information coming from the text (bottom-up). Interestingly, in The role of (im)politeness strategies, as can be seen from these illus-
Scent of a Woman readers' inferences tend to be reinforced by both charac- trative examples, may crucially contribute to the construction of charac-
ters' conversational behaviour, which remains the same for almost the total ters: by combining with the many other factors introduced in the previous
duration of the movie. Therefore, inferences are likely to be interpreted as sections, it plays one of the most important roles in the analytic model of
personality traits of the characters. characterisation outlined in this volume.
2.3.6.2 Expressive identity which this deviant behaviour takes place is emotionally foregrounded. For
example, a character called Rory tends not to use strong expletives such
Traditionally, emotion and affect have been investigated by looking at a as 'hell', in line with her expressive identity; similarly, a character called
range of linguistic features that include, among the others, evaluative lexis, Luke would not use an interjection like 'yay', which is instead used largely
hedges, emphatics, modal verbs, lexical repetitions and pronouns, in com- by the other characters. Therefore, the very few instances of 'hell' by Rory,
bination with non-verbal clues (Caffi and Janney 1994). or of 'yay' by Luke are to be interpreted as a departure from a text-internal
More recently, a great amount of research in this area has been con- norm or target-based expectancy set up for the characters, resulting in the
ducted by Bednarek (2010, 2011, 2012, 2018), who has focused mainly construal of strong emotionality.
on characterisation in televisual discourse. Starting from the assumption To be noted, only interjections are taken into consideration here.
that one of the functions of dialogue is "the revelation of character, telling However, corpus techniques provide the possibility of focussing on all
viewers something about characters' mental states and personality" (2010, the linguistic resources expressing emotions listed above. Therefore, a
101 ), she has used the term expressive character identity to refer to identity very large amount of information might be considered in approaching
encompassing both the emotionality and the attitudes/values/ideologies of characterisation from the point of view of emotionality, potentially an
characters. In her study, the term 'expressive' is preferred to the more tradi- almost infinite number of variables. Also of some note is the fact that, as
tional 'affective', or 'evaluative' as it captures different kinds of attitudes, pointed out above, the term 'expressive identity' does not only encom-
including emotions, values and ideologies: therefore, under the same label, pass emotions, but also attitudes/values/ideologies of the character. The
she groups traditionally emotive interjections like "Oh my God!", evalu- discussion of these other resources goes beyond the scope of this volume
ative statements like "that was a good kiss!" and announcements of ide- (if interested, see Bednarek 2010, chapter 8), however in some particular
ological beliefs like "I don't see how anybody can resist eating meat". novels or TV shows they might prove very central for the construal of
Additionally, she also focuses on less explicit resources and multimodal characters.
features that might not be immediately evident.
Broadly speaking, Bednarek provides an overview of selected resourc- 2.3. 7 Characters as 'inhabitants of possible worlds'
es that can be classified as potentially expressive: they include speech acts
like apologising, congratulating, complimenting, complaining, praising, Traditionally, the term 'modality' has been used to refer to the "judgement
boasting, etc.; linguistic resources linked to emotion, affect, evaluation, of belief and obligation that speakers attach to their utterances" (Simpson
stance, appraisal, subjectivity like rhythm, pitch, loudness, evaluative lex- 2005, 313): in other words, the term covers those linguistic expressions
is, the emotion lexicon, connotative meanings of lexis, modal adverbs/ad- that relate to the truth of what is being declared in a given proposition. In
jective/verbs, nouns, hedges, intensifiers, emphatics, exclamations, swear- phrase 'Mary's words could be true', for example, the modal verb could
ing, style markers, metaphor, affective morphology, etc. signals the degree of certainty of the speaker about Mary's words, and is
Additionàlly, she combines linguistic research with corpus methods therefore central for conveying epistemic modality. Other words or expres-
and shows how characters are construed and kept credible also by means of sions that convey epistemic modality are: maybe, perhaps, may, might, I
expressive resources. In particular, she focuses on the American TV show think, I believe, to quote but a few. The term 'modality' in the linguistic
Gilmore Girls and analyses the frequency of interjections in the characters tradition is also used to refer to the degree of obligation or permission con-
with more turns and the frequency of breaking the norm. In the first case, veyed by a certain utterance: in the sentence 'You should read that book',
corpus methods allow her to conclude that some characters in the show are for example, the modal verb 'should' conveys deontic modality, in the
construed as more emotional than others, while others have very similar same manner as alternatives like 'must', 'ought to', 'may', 'it's necessary',
emotional patterns. In addition to frequency, she also uses techniques like 'you're obliged to', and so on.
key-words and n-grams that show how evaluative language is employed: As regards characterisation, modality can quite intuitively be used to
in particular, she investigates which words tend to occur together in char- analyse the interpersonal metafunction of language (see section 2.3.5): it
acters' conversation, which is also indicative of their peculiar idiolect. In can provide important clues for investigating the relationship among char-
the second case, she does not focus on repeated and patterned semiotic be- acters, how certain they are about what they say, how polite they are to
haviour, but in the disruption of such patterns. In other words, she investi- each other in their requests, etc.
gates what happens when a character's one-off use of an interjection breaks The study of modal structures and systems has also been carried out
the norm that has been set up previously in the show (2010, 132): when within a more narratologically-oriented framework, with the emphasis be-
characters use an interjection they normally would not use, the context in ing placed on their capacity to shape narrative worlds and their potential
to produce stories. The pioneering work in this area is Lubomir Dolezel 's domains are present. Heterogeneous world structures, on the other hand,
Heterocosmica. Fiction and possible worlds (1998), where it is argued that are dominated by multiple domains within the same narrative: in tradition-
narrative worlds, also referred to as fictional universes, or fictional worlds al epics, for example, the laws underlying the world of Gods are totally dif-
are themselves organised according to modal systems. As a consequence, ferent from those governing the world of human beings, yet both domains
one of the first aspects to consider for character analysis is the structure of are present at the same time.
the fictional world in which he/she is embedded. The internal furnishing of Once the specific domains characterising a particular fictional world
the worlds of fiction may in fact vary a lot according to the type of narra- have been identified (single or multiple, factual or counterfactual), it is
tive being produced: in some cases the attributes of a fictional universe are necessary to proceed with the analysis of the modal structure underlying
very similar to the ones of our real world, but this is not always the case: that fictional universe: according to Dolezel, a limited set of basic mac-
very different sets of properties characterise, for instance, the narrative ro-constraints underlying the formation of all stories can be identified, such
worlds of so-called Science Fiction, therefore we could expect characters constraints coinciding with one of these four modal systems introduced
to be affected by the particular fictional worlds they inhabit. below:
The internal structure of the worlds of fiction - and the properties
A. Deontic modalities are concerned with the notions of permission,
characters may have in them - has been studied extensively by possible
obligation and necessity: they rule what is permitted or forbidden
world theory, a field developed from 'possible worlds' in logic in the early
in a particular fictional world and are therefore crucial for the ac-
1960s (Ryan 1985, 2004; Pavel 1986): according to those studies, our ac-
tions of characters. In different deontic conditions, the same ac-
tual world is just one of a number of possible worlds, each of which could
tions may indeed acquire totally different values and cause very
be regulated by different sets of possible laws. The same holds true for
different consequences: for example, the same behaviours could
narrative: as Dolezel points out (1988, 475-96), traditional mimetic criti-
be considered neutral in one deontic environment and dangerously
cism has always tied fictions exclusively to the actual world, thus relating
transgressive in another deontic environment.
even the most fantastic narratives to one sole universe of discourse, i.e. our
actual world. Narrative, however, often makes use of alternative conceptu- As a consequence, the deontic dimension of actions is one of the
al systems, a reason why it is necessary to also make use of different sets richest sources of narrative creation: prohibition or obligation
of tools. The actual world usually participates in the formation of fictional produce stories dominated by the prohibition-violation-punish-
worlds by providing models for its structure or by anchoring the fictional ment structures, where characters struggle to carry out prohibited
story to a historical event: yet, the ontology of a fictional world is very actions, or stories dominated by an obligation-fulfilment-reward
different from the ontology of the real world: structure, where characters struggle to fulfil tasks that are required
of them.
Possible-worlds semantics makes us aware that the actual material
has to undergo a substantial transformation at the world boundary: Quite interestingly, the deontic structure of a particular fictional
it has to be converted into non-actual possibles, with all the world may undergo drastic changes during the story unfolding:
ontological, logical and semantic consequences. We have already deontic acquisitions, for instance, generally coincide with the can-
noted this conversion in the special case of fictional individuals; cellation of particular prohibitions or obligations, thus typically
actual world (historical) persons are allowed to enter a.fictional producing stories of personal, national, or racial liberation; deon-
world only if they assume the status of possible alternates (1988, tic losses, on the other hand, generally coincide with the establish-
485). ment of new prohibitions or obligations, thus producing stories
characterised by sudden oppression, lack of freedom, slavery, etc.
In analysing characters, the starting point should therefore be the iden-
tification of the internal structure of the world they inhabit, which may B. Axiological modalities are concerned with notions of goodness,
consist of a single ontological domain, governed by a single set of laws, or badness and indifference and involve people's personal system of
of several ontological domains, governed by separate sets of laws. Single values: because values for one character may be considered dis-
ontological domains are worlds dominated by homogeneity: in traditional values by another one, axiological modalities are among the most
realistic narratives, for example, the only domain underlying the fiction- powerful triggers for characters actions. Indeed, the presence of
al world has very different laws to the world of reality (natural fictional values or disvalues originates desires or repulsions, in tum respon-
world). Similarly, most fairy tales are dominated by a single domain, that sible for characters' decisions to undertake specific actions and not
breaks the laws of our world (supernatural fictional world), but no other others.
::, .
C. Epistemic modalities are concerned with characters knowledge, whose physical and mental abilities differ significantly from those of the
belief or ignorance of particular aspects of the fictional world: other characters: in some fictions, for example, some characters are able to
usually, this may coincide with scientific knowledge, ideology, re- fly, or read other characters' thoughts, while others may suffer from alethic
ligion, cultural mythology, etc., but it is not only that. Epistemic restrictions, i.e. they may not have the same physical or mental abilities as
also refers to characters' awareness of what is going on in the fic- the other ones.
tional world in terms of other characters' lives, actions, thoughts: When used in combination with other analytical tools, possible world
all detective stories, for example, are characterised by the need to theories provide a stimulating and very useful framework for the analysis
acquire the necessary knowledge for solving a particular problem of literary worlds, and of characters in particular. Practical examples will
or better understanding a particular situation happening in that sto- be provided in part 3 of the volume.
ry-world. Similarly, in 'bildungsromans' characters are described
through their diachronic acquisitions or losses of knowledge pat- 2.3.8 Characters and 'intertextuality'
terns, usually from a particular moment in time when there is little
knowledge of the world to a moment in which a proper knowledge Even if readers do not always realise it, several characters are construed
opens up new possibilities for living in different manners. through a net ofreferences to other characters from novels of the past. The
phenomenon is usually referred to as intertextuality, a term which originat-
D. Finally, al ethic modalities are concerned with notions of possibil- ed in the l 960s21. A text can refer to another one in different ways, some
ity, impossibility and necessity: they determine the fundamental examples of which are parody, allusion, direct quotation and structural par-
conditions of the fictional world, starting from spatio-temporal allelism.
parameters, causality, and characters possibility of action. The As far as characters are concerned, intertextuality can function at dif-
manipulation of such conditions determines what may or may not ferent levels, starting from their very first attribute, the name. In David
happen in a particular fictional world: as Dolezel points out (1988, Lodge's Small World, for example, most of the names recall mythical char-
479), when the fictional world shares very similar characteristics acters of the past, therefore they inevitably create some expectations in the
and possibilities with the real world, it can be referred to as a nat- readers, at least in the ones that recognise the allusion: a character called
ural fictional world, which is a realisation of all the physically 'Morgana', for instance, will trigger schematic knowledge about Arthurian
possible worlds. On the other hand, there are many examples of legends and about the traits of the characters belonging to those cycles. The
fictional worlds regulated by alethic possibilities that do not exist same may happen when a character's name is very similar to the one of
in the real world: characters may have supernatural powers and another character: 'Hubert' and 'Hubertine' in Zola, for instance, are likely
therefore being able to fly, travel in time, etc.; animals may have to trigger expectations of similarity in the readers.
anthropomorphic traits and talk like humans, etc. These worlds are
A character may also recall another one by means of allusions to ac-
called supernatural fictional worlds.
tions and settings of the past, a strategy which 'enriches his/her meaning':
According to this model, when something is possible within a nar- Don Quixote, for example, insists on trying to imitate the actions of legend-
rative universe, it is possible with reference to the characters - or at least ary heroes from the past, therefore at least part of his characterisation uses
to some of them. Dolezel has used the term 'alien' to refer to characters parody strategies. Something similar happens with settings: characters are
who differ from their world-mates for one or more modal possibilities: a often described while living or working in places that look very similar to
deontic alien, for example, is a character who decides not to follow the those from other fictional worlds. In Lodge's Nice Work, for instance, Vic
moral prescribed Jaws of that particular fictional world, and therefore acts Wilcox - one of the two protagonists - is often portrayed while fulfilling
according only to his or her own will; an axiological alien is a character his job as a boss in a foundry, a location described in almost identical terms
who deliberately denies the axiological order of the world: he/she is often to those used by Dickens's 191h century novels. In order to fully understand
a nihilist - a person who replaces the values shared by the other characters and enjoy Lodge's character, the reader needs to be familiar with Dickens's
with some specific values of his/her own - or a rebel - a person who re- characters, who are, in some ways, Vic Wilcox's prototypes.
places the axiological code of the world with an opposite, subjective one;
an epistemic alien is a character whose knowledge ofreality is particularly 21
The term was first coined by the critic Julia Kristeva in the 1960s, and since
rich or, on the other hand, particularly poor: some characters have access to then has become very important within literary and cultural studies. It refers
some aspects of reality in much more detail than the others, and thus may to the several connections that link a text to other texts that have preceded it.
have the possibility to control them; finally, an alethic alien is a character A very good introduction to the concept can be found in Allen (2007).
Finally, intertextuality may also regard the use of specific narrative Taken together, the processes introduced in the various sections of
techniques: some novels are in fact emblematic for highlighting a very par- 2.3 can be thought of as a toolkit for the creation of an analytical model
ticular character's type of speech over the others. In Don DeLillo 's The Body for character investigation: analysts could start by looking at the attributes
Artist, for example, the narrative technique foregrounds the consciousness of the characters' story-world (see 2.3.7), before addressing all the other
of Lauren, the female protagonist: almost every aspect of the fictional world points discussed from 2.3.1 to 2.3.8. As will be discussed in Part 3, not all
is filtered through her own mental experience, in a very similar way to re- these points can be addressed in all narratives: in some cases, for exam=
nowned novels of the past like Woolf's Mrs Da!loway or To the Lighthouse. pie, mind style may not be prominent, therefore it will not be analysed.
Again, some readers will identify, in the dense net of intertextual allusions, Similarly, some novels may foreground politeness patterns but disregard
some features belonging to Woolf's characters, which will probably acti- intertextuality, or vice versa; characters' use of language may cue their
vate the prior knowledge they have acquired during their 'encounters' with belonging to a specific social class, but in some other cases this may not
Woolf's characters. In tum, Don DeLillo's Laureen will be elaborated by be the case, etc. All the aspects discussed, however, are all very crucial to
those readers through extra layers of meaning: in other words, she will be, characterisation, and should be addressed if possible.
at least in some ways, a different character for those readers.

2.4 CONCLUSION

In Part 2 of this volume a series of stylistic approaches to characterisa-


tion have been introduced and discussed, with examples drawn from the
author's work or from other scholars'. The models singled out are not the
only ones elaborated for the study of character, but are, in the author's
view, the optimal ones for a systematic and comprehensive approach en-
compassing both the 'person' and the 'part of a design' dimension.
In particular, it has been observed that the formation of a character's
image in readers' minds emerges from at least two sets of processes: the
first ones are called top-down and describe readers' cognitive mechanisms
while they are engaged with reading fiction. As discussed, prior knowl-
edge, short-term rpemory, long-term memory and schemata all contribute
to readers' personal experience of meaning-creation with texts and char-
acters: other things being equal (the text itself), those mechanisms are re-
sponsible for the different types of experiences different readers may have
with texts. Additionally, it has also been pointed out that the same concepts
can be applied to characters' minds: Briony Tallis in Atonement, for exam-
ple, fails to properly make sense of the situation in the story-world for lack
of proper schemata, which are replaced by others which are more aligned
with her prior knowledge. This causes tragic events.
Top-down processes interact with bottom-up ones, i.e. with the lan-
guage used in the texts. The interaction flows continuously during the
progression of the plot, even if for analytical purposes the two operations
are presented separately. Bottom-up processes include all those strategies
used by authors in order to construe characterisation: narrative techniques,
presentation types, implicit/explicit information about the characters, met-
aphorical/deictical/pragmatic patterns that make mind style explicit, tran-
sitivity profiles, politeness or impoliteness behaviours, intertextuality, etc.
Part 3: A Case-Study: 'The Body Artist'

3.1. INTRODUCTION

In the second part of this volume a model of characterisation combining


top-down and bottom-up processes has been proposed, along with exam-
ples taken from narrative extracts. The processes introduced belong to sty-
listics and to narratology domains, both contributing for their part. This
chapter will provide another example of analysis based on a single novel:
Don DeLillo's The Body Artist (2001).
Ideally, as made clear in Part 2, analyses should address all the tech-
niques introduced in that section: characters' 'habitat' m the story-world,
narratorial control, self and other-pr'eS"enumon, 1mphc1t and explicn m-
formation about characters, narratorial filters mcluding point of view and
mind style, transitivity patterns, (im)politeness attems and expressive
identity, intertextuafity. Actct16onal1y, readers' schemata sliou a so e a -
dressed. In practical terms, since not all characters are construed according
to the same criteria, analyses are likely to focus on only some of them,
while others will be omitted. Additionally, some points may be addressed
together for analytical purposes.
r The-novel under.Jnvestigation in this cha.12ter narrates the.Jife-of a
character called Lauren Hartke: slre~works as a~conce12tual artist and con-
tinuously transforms her body on stage in various ways. Lauren lives alone
in a very bigseaside house she had rented.wiifîher.husband'Rey. G:\fu!r .his
deaili';she realises someone else is sharing-the house with h.£!:, a mysterious
childlike man hidden in one of the bedrooms on the first floor. The man,
whom she baptis-~uttle', seems to have-supernatural p.oW'ers.&f ost
of.rhe story narrates the-relationship betweerîfhem.jfocussihg.mostly on
L Lauren's----inner thoughts=ând feelings. For analytical purposes, some ex-
tracts from the novel will be fully reproduced when necessary.
becomes clear that a a1wensin. the.narrative could never be possi,ble
in t e real world. To put it different!):'. and someho.v..wnot'e..technically, a
modification of the afëthic modalities that determine.the conditions of the
fictional w'OTlcrsudcterrty-o-ccurs, thus turning the fictional universe into a
supernatural fictional w..o_.rld,Jhat is a realisation of all the physically pos-
3.2 CHARACTERISATION
FEATURES
THROUGH 'FICTIONAL WORLD'
I s,ibJ..e ~tlds.
Such modality manipulation in the story-world is not without conse-
quences for the characters: as pointed out in 2.3.7, when something be-
As stated in section 2.3.7 above, the attributes of the fictional world inhab- comes possible in a particular fictional world, it primarily affects the in-
ited by characters can help to shed light on their traits. In particular, the habitants of that world, or at least some - or one - of them. In The Body
modal structure of the fictional environment may recall the real world, or Artist, then, Don DeLillo_has-created-an heterogeneous world structure
it can be very different from it; additionally, narrative worlds can be cJ:IBI- regulated by twÜ different ontological domains: the first of them is the
acterised both by single ontological domains and by 'multiple ontological

-
domains' (alethic, epistemic, deontic axiolooi a - ~
In The Bo y Anist (BA henceforth), a natural fictional world is ini-
tially ~at~.d: an American married couple of artists decides to spend six
domain m which.most of the characters act, a domain which displays the
same laws Ôfthe actual world, whereas the second one - the supernatural
domain - is only .inhabited by Lauren and Mr Tuttle. Here, the laws that
hâve been stipulated confer supernatural powers on the characters: they
months in a rented house near the sea in order to find rest and concentra- are able to reproduce perfectly other characters' speeches or they are able
tion. Such a plot does not seem to present anything strikingly different to use their body to reproduce other people's bodies and movements. In
from what could happen in the real world: since the laws underlying the Dolezel's words introduced in 2.3.7, bo.th...Mr Tuttle and Lauren_can be
narrative are of one type only, we can refer to this ontological domain as referred to as 'aliens', 'al ethic aliens' in particular.
'homogeneous'. However, things are very soon doomed to change abrupt-
ly: during a business trip to New York, the male character Rey commits
suicide; although in deep sorrow and very lonely, Lauren decides to keep
living in the isolated house on her own for the remaining months of rent, 3.3. THE IMPACT OF CHARACTER PRESENTATION MODES
but during her isolation she begins to realise that someone else is sharing
the house with her. After some days of great apprehension, she manages to As observed in 2.3 .2 above, some methods of character presentation are
understand what is happening: a "smallish and fine bodied human being" generally considered more reliable than others: for example, r.resentations
(BA, 43), whom she initially considers a child, is found sitting on the edge carried out by a third person omniscient narrator ~e._g~nerally considered
of a bed on the first floor of the house. This character does not seem able to more cred!61e than presentations made by other characters or by a single
speak, and initially she persuades herself that he is a patient escaped from character speakingabout him/herself.
some hospital or psychiatric facility. But suddenly something bewildering Very interestingly, in The Body Artist most of the information about
happens: the person, whom she baptises Mr Tuttle after one of her high Mr Tuttle i e ed thrQYgQ Lauren's lelill§., since narration.is almost.to-
school teachers, begins to speak: in doing so, he manifests his capabilities t _carrieci.ouLthrough.her point of view. The following extract describes
to reproduce the voices of Rey and Lauren. Not only does he speak exactly the exact moment in which she discovers him in one of the rooms of the
as Rey and Lauren used to do when Rey was still alive, reproducing their big house:
pitches and sounds, but he is also able to reproduce some of the conver-
He didn't raise his head. There was something so strange about him
sations the two had had in the house in the previous months. As the story
that she heard her words hang in the room, predictable and trite. She
unfolds, we realise that Mr Tuttle's abilities are not the only unexpected
felt no fear. He had a foundling quality- lost and found- and she
things that take place in the narrative: Lauren is in fact able to totally trans-
was, she guessed, the.finder (BA, 45).
form her body during her artistic exhibitions, giving it either a male or a
female shape scene after scene. Is it possible to legitimately infer that "strangeness" and "a foundling
The introduction of Mr Tuttle's and of Lauren's supernatural abilities quality" are Mr Tuttle's personality traits? In other words, is strangeness an
thus produces a modification in the readers' previously-formed schema- attribute of the character or does it need to be ascribed to someone else's
ta about the modal structure of the novel: the fictional context is in fact conjecture? Does Mr Tuttle exist or is he merely the product of Lauren's

----
no longer regulated by a homogeneous ontological domain, as it suddenly imagination? When Mr Tuttle is introduced for the first time in the text,
for example, readers have already had more than one chance to realise that in indoor settings there tends to be less action than in outdoor on.es, there-
Lauren's perceptions tend to be altered by the grief caused by Rey's death, forelfarrativesTend to coincide with a predominance of inner life report-
and by the long time spent in total isolation in the remote house. Moreover, ing: it is not a coincidence that in The Body Artist, almost entirely narrated
unreliability may also be corroborated by the fact that 'Mr Tuttle' is a name through FDT ( see section 2.3 .1 above) ~ters think rather than act. In
Lauren herself has created, therefore the traits of the character she has just addition, it is not to be ruled out that cumulative narrative parts which take
discovered are unlikely to be totally objective - they may indeed coincide place indoors might generate some feeling of distress or claustrophobia, at
wi!b-so.m-e-idios-yncrnti&-projectiGns-o.fher mind. Mr Tuttle, in other words, least in some readers.
might perhaps display such traits as strangeness and a foundling quality, ln conclusion, character introduction-in.Ihe Body,jrti§f mainly comes
but in order for them to be classified as personality traits of the character fr~mimplici_Lçu~s (see 2.3 .3): the~narrator does not tell readers about them,
they should come from more reliable voices, or emerge from other textual but mainly leaves them to infer their traits from contextual factors and
verbal/non-verbal behaviour. Conversational features, in particular, trigger
_ ···-· -·· -·-o.1. for_sba.racteLptesentation is action: personality.J.ra1ts important inferences: as will be discussed in 3.4, the characters' use of
may be lm lied either by one-time actions or b.y...habitual ones, One-time lexis is highly indicative of their social status; in Rey and Lauren's break-
ctions tend to evo e t e ynamic aspects of the character, whereas ha- fa~ lexicalchoices are often sophisticated and uncommon.
bitual actions are likely to reveal the character's more static aspects, quite
Ll
often combined with a comic effect. Although a one-time action may not
reflect permanent qualities, however, it is no less peculiar to the character
As reg~~-Mi:-T-1:1tt-fe;:'hi~;-paralingu1Stic features (mostly variation in pitch
and pitch range), together with the idiosyncratic use of lexis, suggest a
character who is hard to capture from the identity point of view, someone
(Rimmon-Kenan 1983, 61). Rey's act of shooting himself is emblematic in
who is troubled in some ways, even if it is not clear how. When it becomes
these respects, and readers may infer some particular traits of his personal-
evident that he is able to precisely reproduce other people's speeches, new
ity, e.g. emotional instability, or tendency to depression. Indeed, such traits
traits are likely to be ascribed to him by readers, even ifhe tends to remain
are somehow anticipated by a series of repeated actions reported in the
first part of the book: the first chapter of the book is entirely devoted to the a mysterious creature: it is never clear to what degree his traits are actually
description of Rey and Lauren's breakfast, but it soon becomes clear that his traits, and to what degree they derive from Lauren's own individual
the actions described (reading the newspaper in a particular way, asking elaboration of the situation.
the same questions several times, repeating the same acts more than once)
have the function of conveying crucial information about the emotional
condition of the character. Indeed, the traits readers may infer from those 3.4. CHARACTERISATION THROUGH LEXIS
descriptions are later reinforced by the character's tragic, ultimate one-
time action.
As observed in Part 2, the words charac~ use.in their conversations can
Externai appearance is also Jilsely to con.ve~_the traits of a ch be highly revealing of some of their personality traits: in particular, Cul-
ter. Conversely, the total lack of references to physicality is equally sig- peper)2001, 183) has argued that associating people's choices of words
nificant: in The Body Artist, for example, both Lauren and Mr Tuttle are
with particular personalities is not straightforward, yet it is undeniable that
not described from the physical point of view, and this may be related to
such choices play a pivital role in the general idea readers form of a person
their identity: indeed, both characters are presented as 'troubled' from.the

---
or of a character. The more informative c!!Qi_ceê_ involve etymological ori-
identi point of view, at least in this part of their lives. Lauren feels that
gin of the words,Jexical richness and surge features.
she woul e to issolve", to be someone else, to "disappear in Rey's
smoke" (BA, 36), whereas Mr Tuttle's own words are mostly incoherent, As regards the etymological origin of the words, since The Body Art-
fragmented, and a-semantic - a fact that is linked to identity. ist is largely the recording of a personal, quite intimate experience of a
Environment is also important in the in1!:29.yction ofa_char].Çj_er. ;Yb.se character, lexis tends to be QllÏlejnformal, mainly derived from Qerm~nic.
does the action tend to take place'? Do characters act mostly indoors or out- On the other hand, the onlyJ.):~.o-parts of.the book which do not report the
'oors'? ln 1'hë..B__Që/iAr.tüt, mos.t..oLthe a_çtiQ.11,.take§_place indoors: the first intimate life of.the character - the two journalistic reports, one about Rey's
chapter of the book is entirely devoted to Lauren and Rey's breakfast in the death, the other one about Lauren's work - are much mor_ç fQ_rmal and dis-
kitchen; the rest of the novel mostly describes the time Lauren spends with plaµ_.greater number of words of Latin origin. The inner-life nature of the
Mr Tuttle on the second floor of the house, or in the kitchen; the chapter de- novel, however, makes it hard to focus on the role of formal and informal
scribing her job as a body artist is entirely set in a theatre. Quite intuitively, language, since Lauren is portrayed mostly while speaking either to herself
or to Mr Tuttle, i.e. therefore in contexts which are not particularly inform- As far as surge features are concerned, the appraisal framework initial-
ative of her personal attitude in more formal social relationships. ly elaborated by Martin and White (2005) can be used in order to approach
Lexical richness, on the other hand, can be quite informative even in this aspect, among others. Some examples are singled out below to account

r
nore private contexts: we would not expect a character to use a formal for the affective relationship established among the characters in The Body
vocabulary when he or she speaks to him/herself or to his/her family, yet Artist. The first example uses the first system of appraisal theory, 'Affect':
a higher or lower diversity of usage in these same contexts can be expect-
ed. As pointed out in Part 2, a lower diversity of lexis "results in receiver - "You like everything. You love everything. You 're my happy home.
judgement oflower communicator competence, lower socio-economic sta- Here," he said (BA, 19).
tus, and higher anxiety" (Bradac 1982, 107), whereas a.higher diversity of
lexis generally_stands for higher competence. and social status. - "Then its not gone. Good. I'm happyforyou" (BA, 20).
InJhe BodyArtist, access to Lauren's lexis comes mainly iruwoways: Both turns are uttered during Lauren and Rey's breakfast in the first
direct aridïi'ëe1ndirect diSCOÛp,€. The short text below is an example of chapter of the book. In the first of them, Rey uses two mental processes
direct discourse and is pronounced by Lauren in a very private setting, yet ("like" and "love") to refer to Lauren's attitude towards the house they
it provides useful information about her communicative competence, and have just rented, and he speaks about his wife in metaphorical terms ("You
therefore about her education22: are my happy home"). More realistically, he is here using the system of
You see there is a chest of drawers. Its stored in a room somewhere 'Affect' with an ironic purpose, since the character's suicide will soon
upstairs. Wrapped, I think. Probably wrapped in that padded fabric make it clear that Lauren is anything but a "happy home" for him. The
they use. Maybe you've come across it. Because it was about to be second tum is similar to the first one: it occurs during the same breakfast,
moved, shipped, and then somehow, well, you know how these things while Lauren is trying to communicate with him with no success. Rey
don't always happen when they are supposed to. Its a delicate piece, keeps reading the newspaper while Lauren anxiously asks questions about
in two parts, and fairly old (BA, 72). some noises on the second floor of the house. Their conversation is a sort
of nonsense, and it is quite evident that Rey is getting more and more nerv-
Out of 55 total words pronounced, 38 are different words, and the
ous (He said, "What? You insist you will drag this thing out of me. Lucky
remaining 17 coincide mainly with grammatical words23. Lauren's degree
we don't normally have breakfast together. Because my mornings" BA,
of lexical richness is therefore the result of38/55, namely 0.7, a ratio quite
20). The attributive relational of affect "I'm happy" sounds therefore total-
close to 1. Her language in this extract is therefore to be interpreted as quite
ly unnatural and signals an attitude much closer to an ironic position than
rich and conveys the feeling of a character belonging to a medium-high so-
to real personal involvement. Though very short, the two turns are symbol-
cial class, endowed with a good education level. This fact is not surprising:
ic of Rey's interpersonal attitude throughout the only chapter in which he

i
she has been married to a film director for many years and works regularly
in the most important American theatres. As for Mr Tuttle, his language is interacts with Lauren: in most of his interactions, he uses the grammar of
generally very fragmented and displays a very large number of words or 'Affect' to communicate a fundamental lack of it.
expressions repeated over and over again: the degree of lexical richness is The attitudinal subsystem of Judgement comprises meanings which
thus, not surprisingly, very low, and projects the image of a character with serve to evaluate human behaviour positively and negatively: this subsys-
low communicative competence and socio-economic status. tem thus emerges when a speaker provides an assessment of some human
participant with reference to that participant's acts or dispositions. There-
22
It goes without saying that this is just for the purposes of exemplification. De-
fore, under Judgement behaviour can be assessed as moral or immoral,
tailed analysis of much longer stretches of text would be needed. Generally legal or illegal, socially acceptable or unacceptable, normal or abnormal,
speaking, however, Lauren's language in different parts of the text is fairly and so on. In the three sentences below, for example, Judgement is used to
rich in number of different words. assess the character's dispositions:
23
In order to sort out the words for calculation, see Halliday (1989, 61-75). As
- She has transformed herself shockingly for this event, and although
far as the distinction between lexical items and grammatical items is con-
cerned, it is important to remember that lexical items are 'content words', the brief run is over, she continues to look well, wasted (BA, 105 my
whereas grammatical items are 'function words'. Grammatical items are emphasis).
those that function in closed systems in the language: determiners, pronouns,
most prepositions, conjunctions, some classes of adverbs and finite verbs. - Her hair looks terroristic (BA, 105 my emphasis).
.LUO

- This is what happened, causing walkouts among the less committed mind It was his trick that he knew he could do when he needed it. He
(BA, 106 my emphasis). even made me see him in the chair (BA, 60).
The sentences are pronounced by Mariella Chapman, a character who The character talking on the phone with Lauren is Isabel, Rey's first
appears in one of the last chapters of the book. Her aim is to interview Lauren wife. Some days have passed since Rey's funeral, and the two women are
and create a reliable account of her skills as a body artist: in order to do so, talking to each other for the very first time in their lives. The extract is
she describes her appearance and at the same time provides the reader with a therefore quite significant in revealing interpersonal styles. FQL.lbi.~_plll:=
series of judgements that are entirely hers, subjective. The adjective "shock- pose, we will focus on...the use of.modalisation patterns: a character .using
ingly" in the first sentence evokes a token of subjective judgement, similarly I màm!f"unï;;:odalised clauses is likely to be perceived as leaving littl.LJ:Q.QJ.n
to the last verb in the sentence, "wasted". In terms of characterisation, nei-
7 for doubt-.i.a.Jitrle-room for.altematize positions. On the other hand, a
ther "shockingly" nor "wasted" necessarily represent objective traits of the character who tends to modalise his/her assertions is more lik(jyJo be
character, since they come from another character with a specific purpose: perèeivea as more open to alternative voices and-Positions. In other words,
corroborating the description of Lauren's qualities for the article she is pro- th; distinction1s between a 'monoglossic' or a 'heteroglossic' personal
ducing. Similarly, the adjective "terroristic" in the second sentence has the style. Quite significantly, Isabel's clauses in the extract above are most-
aim of conveying a positive judgement. The third sentence glorifies Lauren's ly unmodalised, with the exception of "can" in the second sentence. The
art through a negative judgement: those who decide to leave her show be- image she projects is one of a very self-confident character, someone who
fore it is finished are described by the journalist as "less committed", which knows what she is talking about and does not leave room for doubts. For
strategically asserts that only the 'most committed', i.e. the most gifted can t explanation's sake, let us re-write the paragraph, changing the different
modalisation patterns:
understand Lauren's art. Although very circumscribed, the three examples
may help understand how the Appraisal system is ultimately connected with
At the funeral we barely talked So you avoided me a little, which I
ideology: the aim of the journalist is to use a language supporting her own
understand it, believe me, and can sympathize. I also accept what
purpose, i.e. writing an article for an important American newspaper, and the
he did because I think I know him forever. But for you its probably
language used to assess the character meets this end.
different. I feel bad we didn't talk. I could see it coming for years.
The second broad semantic domain of Appraisal theory is Source,
This is a thing that was likely to happen. We all thought we knew
which is concerned with the resources for positioning the speaker's voice
this about him. For years it seemed he was going to do this thing.
with respect to the various propositions and proposals conveyed by a text.
It was apparently a thing he carried with him. It was probably his
The focus is here on who is making the evaluations: how is he/she posi-
way out. In my opinion he wasn't a man in despair. This thing was
tioned with respect to what is being said? Is he/she totally certain of it?
perhaps a plan in his mind. It might have been was his trick that he
Are there any significant doubts? Source has been developed in order to
knew he could do when he needed it. He even made me see him in
explore how texts are construed not only to persuade explicitly, but also to the chair.
influence and ultimately 'naturalise' attitudes, beliefs and assumptions in
more indirect and implicit ways. Two terms are usually associated with this By opening up many possibilities to doubt, this second option would
notion: monoglossic and heteroglossic. The first one, as White points out, be an instance of heteroglossic style, with much room left to alternative
is used for a "bare, 'undialogized' assertion" (2002, 2), which admits no standpoints. Th.e italics-uv factsignal.rhe non-objectivity-of what is being
engagement with alternative positions and voices: it is therefore concerned s~I.l..lhYs the.possibility.of different points of. view. The two versions
with what is taken to be shared knowledge or beliefs. In heteroglossic as- of the same conversation contribute to construing two different character
sertions, on the other hand, the speaker/writer leaves room for alternative identities: on the one hand, one who seems more self-confident and less
viewpoints, thus not presenting it as an 'absolute truth': open to alternative positions; on the other hand a character who consist-
ently admits the possibility of being wrong, thus projecting a more open
At the funeral we barely talked So you avoided me a little, which I
disposition to otherness. The interpersonal style recorded in the first extract
understand it, believe me, and can sympathize. I also accept what he
is further reinforced in the last exchange between the two characters:
did because I know him forever. But for you its different. I feel bad
we didn't talk. I could see it coming for years. This is a thing that But I want to say it. I insist to say it. This man hated who he was.
was going to happen. We all knew this about him. For years he was Because how long do I know this man and how long do you know
going to do this thing. It was a thing he carried with him. It was his him? I never left. Did I ever leave? Were we really separated? I knew
way out. He wasn't a man in despair. This thing was a plan in his him in my sleep. And I know exactly how his mind was working.
He said to himself two things. This is a woman I know forever. And ideological positions and styles of interaction with the other characters in
maybe she will not mind the mess (BA, 61-2). the story-world."?" - - -
Again, no room is left for alternative points of view: Isabel is very cat-
egorical in her assertions as the very short unmodalised sentences "I want
to say it"; "I insist to say it" make clear. She also uses rhetorical questions, 3.5. CHARACTERISATION THROUGH 'MIND-STYLE'
the answers to which are quite clear, according to her. Additionally, it is
Linguistic patterns often project a particular world view, i.e. a particu-
not hard to perceive that Isabelle's aim is also to create competition with
lar way of making sense of the textual world: since 'reality' derives from
Lauren: "how long do I know this man and how long do you know him?
Rerce12tual..filld_c_ogpitive processes which vary.from.person to persgn, con-
I never left" significantly positions Isabel interpersonally. No negotiation
ceptualisations of the same experiences may be very different from indi-
is possible between the speaker and her interlocutor, a fact that helps the
vidual to individual. In other words, some representations ofreality can be
creation of character inferences in the readers.
more idiosyncratic than others, thus projecting individual understandings
The third broad semantic domain of Appraisal Theory is Graduation. of the world that are strikingly different from those projected by the other
This domain comprises a set of resources for strengthening and weaken- characters: it is on those deviant, distinctive elaborations of the world that
ing feelings and attitudes. According to Martin and Rose, graduation is stylistics studies have focused the most.
a resource "for adjusting the volume of gradable items" (2003, 41 ). The
As discussed in Part 2, important info.nnati.Pn..abouLcharacters:.minds
amplification or reduction can take different forms, from the use of inten-
comes from the analysis of the metaphors they use.even.though metaphors
sifiers like 'very', 'really', etc. to the use of attitudinal lexis, i.e. lexis that
are not the only cues. Imagine, for example, a character using two dif-
.l:Lttributes different semantic values to the same action: fèi-eni;"ë!istinct languages throughout the novel: a Language A, structured
- "Its such astonishing shit" (BA, I 6) into grammatically correct sentences and appropriate lexical choices, and
a Language B structured in a totally different way, characterised by sever-
- The piece, called Body Time, sneaked into town for three nights al grammar mistakes and inappropriate lexical choices. Though not being
[. . .] (BA, I 06). metaphoric, such a language would provide significant insights regarding
that character's mind, for example indicating a possible split self.
In the first sentence, pronounced by Rey at breakfast time, the ad- In, The Body Artist, the role of language is particularly_im_portant for~
jective "astonishing" works as an intensifier, in that it adjusts the volume approaching the-min.d._of.Mr Tuttle: from his first appearance in the novel,
of an attitude that is already very clear. Indeed, *'It's such a shit' could it soon becomes cl~r that his identity is heavily dependent on the languag£,_
have been strong enough for conveying his point of view on what is be- he uses, which strikingly devrntes from tnewliat readers would normally
ing broadcast on the radio, yet Rey feels the need to amplify it by means expect. Some of the initial exchanges between himself and Lauren are re-
of an adjective. In the second sentence, written by the journalist who has ported below:
interviewed Lauren, the amplification is realised by means of highly con-
notative lexis: the verb "sneaked", which refers to Lauren's show, is in fact 'What do you see?' she said, gesturing towards the boat and the
used in place of a more common one, such as 'perform'. The sentence: advancing cloudline.
*'The piece, called Body Time, was performed in town for three nights',
could have been a more congruent option, yet the journalist uses a less 'The trees are some of them', he said (BA, 46).
neutral verb in order to create a more colourful connotation. Declaring that
'If there is another language you speak', she told him, 'say some
the show sneaked into town somehow provides a value judgement, since it
words'.
is clear that the success of a show which silently sneaked into town, rather
than being advertised, is more well deserved than the success of perfor- 'Say some words'.
mances highly promoted.
The Appraisal System is much more complex than what appears from 'Say some words.Doesn't matter ifl can't understand'.
this short account, yet these examples may be sufficient to understand that,
once it is applied systematically to a character's conversational pattern, it 'Say some words to say some words' (BA, 57).
can significantly strengthen the formation of that character's image in the
minds of the readers. Crucial cues can be formed regarding that character's She whispered, 'What are you doing?'
'I am doing. This yes that. Say some words'. ical patterns or idiosyncratic representations of reality for her. What goes
on in her mind coincides mostly with her narrative programmes, an expres-
'Did you ever? Look at me. Did you ever talk to Rey? The way we sion introduced by Greimas (1966) to refer to what characters intend to do,
are talking now' i.e. their programmes of action:

'We are talking now'. This was her work, to disappear from all her formal venues of aspect
and bearing and to become a blankness, a body slate erased of every
'Yes. Are you saying yes? Say yes. When did you know him? ' past resemblance. [. . .} ln the mirror she wanted to see someone who
is classically unseen, the person you are trained to look through, bled
'I know him where he was' (BA, 64). of familiar effect, a spook in the night static of every public toilet
(BA, 86).
'How could you be living here without my knowing?

'But you know. I am living'{BA, 71).


Apart from the sentences "I know him where he was" and "This yes 3.6. CHARACTERISATION THROUGH CONVERSATION AND
that", all the utterances produced by Mr Tuttle are correct from a grammat- POLITENESS PATTERNS
ical point of view. Most of his sentences in the novel are indeed correct, yet
it is evident that his ~ge 1?!9jects a strQ!igly deviantJl,Dd idiosy.HGFat- Linguistic research addressing real people has demonstrated that the anal-
!.f. . relationship with the world outs.füe. Every act of communication takes ysis of conversational exchanges proves extremely useful in order to un-
place within a particular context, and it is precisely when Mr Tuttle's ex- derstand how power is distributed and balanced among speakers, among
changes are analysed in context that deviancy is more evident: his answers other things. The extract below, recording a typical conversation between
have nothing to do with Lauren's questions, thus breaking Grice's maxim Lauren and Mr Tuttle, will be used to show how characters' conversational
of relation (See 2.3.4.2.4). Metaphorically speaking, the impression he behaviour can be approached, taking total volume of speech, tum alloca-

I may provide is that of a machine programmed for creating grammatically


acceptable sentences that sound like sheer nonsense from the semantic and
the pragmatic point of view. Within this specific context of situation, he
sounds uncooperative and totally deviating from the norm.
tion, topic control and terms of address into account:
(1) She said, 'if you had a name. Just suppose now. Is there anyone
who would know what it is? Where is your mother? When I say
What readers can infer about Mr Tuttle's mental functioning is that his mother; the woman who gives birth to a child, the parent, the female
mind does not .possess the possibility to interact with the other characters. parent, does this word? Tell me. What?'
His mind, to stick to the machine metaphor, is like a hard drive that has the
capacity to store huge quantities of information but cannot communicate He knew what a chair is called and a window and a wall but not the
it properly. That Mr Tuttle.is more similar...t-0--a--maGhine-than...to-a_human tape recorder, although he knew how to turn it off, and not, it seemed,
being is also confirmed by the fact that, as the J.J.QY_eJ unfolds, Lauren re- who his mother was or where she might be found.
alisès thatlië is able to reproduce exactly all the dialogues she had had
with Rey before his death. Furthermore, he can also replicate the tones of (2) 'If there is another language you speak', she told him, 'say some
voices they had used during their exchanges: such an ability is not possible words'.
in our natural world, therefore the modal configuration of the novel is that
of a supernatural fictional world inhabited by a character who is an3lethi (3) 'Say some words '.
alien. Mr Tuttle, in other words, seems to embody a very typical character
àf-postmodemism, the so-called post-human character. As a matter of fact, (4) 'Say some words. Doesn't matter if I can't understand'.
Lauren "amused herself by thinking that he'd come from cyberspace, a
man who'd emerged from her computer screen in the dead of night. He was (5) 'Say some words to say some words'.
from Kotka, in Finland" (BA, 47).
As regards Lauren, although most of her existence is mental (mostly (6) 'All right. Be a Zen master, you little creep. How do you know
narrated through FIT), it is not possible to single out particular metaphor- what I said to my husband? Where were you? Were you here,
somewhere, listening? My voice. It sounded word for word. Tell me not seem interested in satisfying her curiosity. Tums 3 and 5, for instance,
about this'. are entirely inconsistent with Lauren's requests, thus breaking Grice's
maxim of relation. Similarly, in turns 8 and 10 he produces sentences that
When there was a pause in the conversation, the recorder stopped are totally disjointed from Lauren's enquiries. The topic she wishes to in-
hissing. She watched him. She tried to press him on the matter but vestigate throughout the whole extract is therefore repeatedly frustrated,
got nowhere and changed the subject again. even if it may be difficult for the reader to understand why this happens. Is
this the result of Mr Tuttle's incapacity to understand and formulate ade-
(7) 'What did you mean earlier yesterday when you said, when quate answers? Is it to be interpreted as an explicit refusal to cooperate in
you seemed to say what? I don't recall the words exactly. It was the exchange of information? Whatever the reason, the impression is that
yesterday. The day before today. You said I'd still be here, I think, power is not on her side: despite her almost total control of the volume of
when the lease. Do you remember this? When I am supposed to speech and of more than half of the turns, she is not in control of the con-
leave. You said I do not'. versation, therefore power seems rather to be in Mr Tuttle's hands. This
is further confirmed by the mood structure of most of her clauses, where
(8) 'I said this what I said'. the demand for goods and services (Halliday 2004) prevails over all the
other options: she is the one 'in need', not vice versa; the large number of
(9) 'You said this'. That you somehow'. questions she asks (9) cues her jittery need to know, whereas the lack of
relevant answers cues the lack of adequate power to obtain proper answer.
(10) 'Somehow. What is somehow?' All this makes her increasingly more anxious, as the terms of address
she uses in exchange 6 clearly remark: "Zen master" is evidently sarcas-
(I I) 'Shut up. That you somehow but never mind. When the lease tic, whereas "you little creep" is aggressive and impolite. Lauren becomes
ends. Or something else completely' (BA, 57-58). more and more nervous as she realises that she cannot exercise power over
In terms of total volume of speech, the most striking observation is that Mr Tuttle, and this does not seem to have any impact on him. In a ma-
most of the words are uttered by Lauren (about 170 - 90% -, including arti- chine-like manner, he keeps employing the same patterns despite the emo-
cles and preposition, versus about 20). Therefore, at least in this particular tional reactions of his interlocutor.
context, she is the one who dominates the conversation, with a number The conversational strategies used in this exchange are therefore
of words about nine times greater than Mr Tuttle's. Interestingly, this is useful for the formation of readers' mental impressions of the characters:
a recurrent pattern in the novel, with Lauren speaking a lot and asking anxiety, frustration and some aggressiveness may be attributed to Lauren,
questions, and Mr Tuttle being silent or using very short and nonsensical whereas Mr Tuttle appears somehow inscrutable, mysterious and therefore
sentences. Readers might therefore preliminarily conclude that she is the difficult to categorise.
more powerful character among the two, even though the analysis of other
features may contradict this.
The allocation of turns is quite significant from this point of view: out
of 11 turns, 7 (64%) are spoken by Lauren and 4 (36%) are spoken by 3.7 TRANSITIVITY PATTERNS AND CHARACTERISATION
Mr Tuttle. In the light of volume of speech discussed above, however, a
much higher number of turns spoken by her would be expected. Mr Tuttle's As pointed out in 2.3.5, transitivity patterns can be very useful for identi-
speaking space, in other words, has not been inhibited by Lauren: the deci- fying which characters are agentive and which ones tend to be acted-up-
sion to limit the volume of speech and keep turns very short is entirely Mr on, which characters are doers and which ones are thinkers, which forces
Tuttle's, since the space would be there, available for his speech. His speak- dominate the story-world, and so on ( see also Toolan 2001, 111 ). Generally
ing behaviour therefore has the potential to trigger some inferences regard- speaking, identifying a transitivity profile for a character means analysing
ing their relationship: although she is anxiously asking for explanations, he the main verb processes used by him/her or by the narrator, while at the
decides not to provide any. Is she really more powerful than him? Does she same time focusing on the presence or absence of other key notions like
get what she wants from him, as more powerful people normally do? Goals, Phenomena, etc. It goes without saying that the recurrence of a
The analysis of topic control may help find answers: the only topic particular transitivity structure in a novel does not exclude the occurrence
Lauren seems interested in is Mr Tuttle's present identity and his existence of alternative options: what generally happens is that, over the course of a
in the house she used to share with Rey. Mr Tuttle, on the other hand, does narrative, certain choices are much more frequent than others, thus creat-
. ......

ing a sense of transitivity profile (Simpson 2004, 120) which constitutes a preponderance of material processes is usually related to the presence of
stylistic mark of a character. characters who act a lot upon the world around them, yet it is not always
To make things clearer, let us observe how particular transitivity op- so: careful scrutiny of the transitivity patterns soon makes it clear that it
tions are used in an extract from The Body Artist. The following text is would be problematic to conceive of Lauren as an active character, at least
taken from the concluding page of the book: Lauren is again alone in the until the very end of the text. If, on the one hand, she is the main actor in
big house after Rey has died and Mr Tuttle has disappeared. most of the material processes, it is very significant that in some cases the
processes are carried out by different characters or by parts of her body
She stood a while, thinking into this. She stopped at room s edge, (her back, in "her back to the fluted wood, squat-rising[ ... ]"; her face, in "It
facing back into the hall, and felt the emptiness around her. Thats was a look that nearly floated free of her"[ ... ]). Despite the fact that Lauren
when she rocked down to the floor. backed against the doorpost. She is the subject in many processes, the material processes used cannot inter-
went twisting/y down, slowly, almost thoughtfully, and opened her preted as cues for a 'doer' character.
mouth, oh, in a moan that remained unsounded. She sat on the floor Moreover, it is crucial to notice that the vast majority of material pro-
outside her room. Her face still wore a decorative band, a trace cesses are not goal-directed, i.e. they are intransitive': in "She stopped at
across the eyes of the prospect of wonders. It was a look that nearly the room's edge, facing back into the hall" (line 1), or in "That's when she
floatedfree of her so she could puff her cheeks, childlike, and blow it rocked down to the floor, backed against the doorpost (line 2)", to quote
away. but two examples, the actions described by the verbs are not addressed to-
wards any particular object, but involve only the actor carrying out the ac-
She thought she would not bother looking in there. It was pathetic tion and a circumstance oflocation/range. The world around the character,
to look. The roomfaced east and would be roiled in morning light, in other words, is not affected by her, remaining exactly the same after her
in webby sediment and streams of sunlit dust and in the word motes, actions have been carried out. This particular kind of transitivity pattern
which her mother liked to use. strategically conveys an atmosphere of stillness and ineffectual activity, a
text world where nothing seems to really change.
Maybe it was all an erotic reverie. The whole thing was a city built In this part of the text Lauren is therefore construed as a character who
for a dirty thought. She was a sexual hysteric, ha. Not that she tends to act (and think) a lot, but not according to any particular strategy:
believed it. actions and thoughts do not result from a precise plan she has elaborated
for a particular purpose. Other linguistic options operate to reinforce this
She sat there, thinking into the blankness of her decision. Then she general atmosphere of stillness: some processes, which are usually found
worked herself up along the doorpost, slowly, breathing completely, in the continuous form, are here employed in the simple form, therefore
her back to the fluted wood, squat-rising, drawing out the act over an they are marked: "Her face still wore a decorative band (sentence 6)" in-
extended length of time. Her mother died when she was nine. It had stead of *was wearing; "She sat there, thinking into the blankness of her
nothing to do with ha decision (sentence 15)" instead of *was sitting; agency can be attributed
to Lauren in the concluding part of the novel, at least in most of it. The
The room was empty when she looked. No one was there. The light character, though acting, does not act on the world around her, and this
was so vibrant she could see the true colours of the walls andfloors. may project the image of an actor who is 'acted-upon'. The text is also
She'd never seen the walls before. The bed was empty. She'd known permeated by a general atmosphere of solitude, confirmed by the almost
it was empty all along but was only catching up. She looked at the total lack of verbal processes.
sheet and blanket swirled on her side of the bed, which was the only Things seem to change in the last part of the extract, even if this does
side in use. not have the power to affect the previously-formed atmosphere: the two
processes - "She opened it [the window]" and "She threw the window
She walked into the room and went to the window. She opened it. She open" - are goal-directed, a fact that restores at least some agency to the
threw the window open. She didn't know why she did this. Then she transitivity profile of the character. The actor in the last two sentences thus
knew. She wanted to feel the sea tang on her face and the flow of time performs as a doer, a situation that is further reinforced by the mental pro-
in her body, to tell her who she was (BA, 125-6). cess "She wanted to feel", highlighting the character's will. The atmosphere
Most of the processes used in the text are material (24), and there is of stillness dominating the preceding sentences of the text now seems to
a significant number of relational ( 18) and of mental processes (13). The fade away, an impression reinforced by the repetition of the same syntactic
.L.LO

structure ( one-clause sentences of the 'she+material process' type), which a character whom she is trying to make sense of. The verb "stood", in par-
conveys dynamism24. ticular, can be classified as a material process in systemic functional terms,
In conclusion, the foregrounding of intransitive material processes in yet the text does not describe actions, but thoughts. In other words, rather
the first part of the extract, together with other options of verb tenses and than introducing actions as we could expect, the verb "stood" introduces
participant types, are likely to influence the readers' perception of the char- and captures a moment of stillness: the only movement seems to be the
acter: in the first part of the extract (but the impression holds in the other flow of thoughts that permeates her mind while she is looking at Mr Tut-
parts of the book), Lauren is depicted as a character who is almost para- tle. Her thoughts seem to say: "I would like to discover who you are", "I
lysed by the situation she is experiencing. Only in the last sentences of the need you to stop being so mysterious", etc., yet these processes are never
extract does her will emerge, and her actions are somehow concerned with explicitly verbalised. ln the example, an action verb "stood" is therefore
an 'identity quest': similarly to Woolf's Mrs Dalloway, she ardently wants to be interpreted as opening up a series of mental process where nothing
to know who she is, a desire that is in both cases fulfilled by the contact physically really happens.
with the world outside through an open window. The notion of intermental thought is based on the assumption that
mental functioning can be properly applied to social as well as individual
fonns of activity and is closely connected to the notion of communicative
action, the kind of action that takes place when more than one character
3.8 CHARACTERISATION THROUGH A NEWLY-EXPANDED is present. An 'intermental thought approach' to characters can be very
NARRATOLOGY: ACTIONS AS THOUGHTS, INTERMENTAL useful to understand the type of affective relationship between them, since
THINKING DOUBLY EMBEDDED NARRATIVES two - or more - characters involved in several actions are more likely to be
emotionally close. By the same token, the absence of interrnental thought
As made clear in 2.3 .4.2.1, the inner life of characters has traditionally may become equally significant, as the extract below seems to show:
been analysed through a speech category approach, which was based on
the assumption that the categories applied to fictional speech could be ap- It happened thisfinal morning that they were here at the same time,
plied to fictional thought, too. That approach was concerned primarily with in the kitchen, and they shambled past each other to get things out of
that part of the mind known as inner speech, but did not take other aspects cabinets and drawers and then waited one for the other by the sink
of mental life into consideration, in particular the whole mind, the social or fridge, still a little puddled in dream melt, and she ran tap water
mind and the mind in action. A newly-expanded narratology of the fictional over the blueberries bunched in her hand and closed her eyes to
mind revolving around the concept of continuous consciousness frame and breath the savor rising (BA, 9).
its subframes (the relationship between thought and action, interrnental or The extract describes Lauren and Rey's last breakfast before Rey's
shared thinking, doubly embedded narratives) has been proposed by Palm- suicide. In the first part of the extract, narration is in third person plural,
er (2004), some examples of which will be discussed below. To start with, which typically generates interrnental verbs. However, the verbs used in
let us consider the following extract from The Body Artist: the extract ("shambled", "waited") do not possess any intermental fea-
She stood looking at him, two bodies in a room. He seemed to tures: they "shambled past each other" does not indeed describe a joint,
recede under observation, inwardly withdraw, not in discomfort, shared decision to carry out an action, and the fact of being in the kitchen
she thought, but spontaneously, automatically, guided by some law together is to be interpreted as something that simply happened, an event
of his bodys own devising. She put her hands on his shoulders and almost out of the characters' control. As a matter of fact, the sentences
looked into his eyes. She thought, When did people start looking into opening the novel do indeed communicate the feeling of lack of control
each others eyes? This is what she did, searchingly, standing in the over the world outside: "Time seems to pass. The world happens, unroll-
kitchen with Mr Tuttle (BA, 87). ing into moments, and you stop to glance at a spider pressed to its web"
(BA, 9). Additionally, it is not without significance that the first person
The text reports some actions ( or non-actions) carried out by Lauren plural is very soon replaced by the third person singular, as if the char-
while she is struggling in order to understand the personality of Mr Tuttle, acters were sitting in the kitchen on their own: very interestingly, for the
following 18 pages the third person pronoun 'they' is never used again,
24 The first verb process of the text is "stood" (first paragraph), followed by narration coinciding with a very long series of 'she' or 'he' which describe
"sat" (second paragraph). This atmosphere of stillness is symbolically re- the actions or the feelings experienced by the character separately. The to-
solved in the last paragraph, where the verb process used is "walked". tal lack of intermental thought in a context where it could reasonably oc-
.L.LO

cur is therefore symbolic of the emotional distance characters experience, mind that is embedded within Lauren's and not vice versa. It is Lauren, in
and might generate readers' inferences relating to solitude, loneliness and, other words, who is interested in establishing a series of conjectures about
ultimately, suffering. As already remarked, Rey - a very successful busi- her husband's mind, while he does not simply show any interest in it. As
nessman in New York's art context - will commit suicide soon after this Palmer notes, "the lack of doubly embedded narratives demonstrates some
scene, while Lauren will experience a time of confusion and pain until the very solipsistic states of mind" (2004, 234): this, therefore, adds to the al-
very end of the novel. ready noted feeling ofloneliness that may be one of Rey's traits.
The third sub-frame of the continuous consciousness frame, doubly As far as the relationship Lauren-Mr Tuttle and Mr Tuttle-Lauren is
embedded narratives, refers to a character whose mind is contained with- concerned, things go as we would expect them to go: no trace of doubly
in another character's: a character's identity, in other words, consists not embedded narrative is present in Mr Tuttle's mind, whereas Lauren's is
just of his/her own embedded narrative, but is the result of all the doubly filled with conjectures about his potential mental processes. A few illustra-
embedded narratives of which he/she is the subject. Do doubly embedded tive examples are reported below:
narratives play a role for the characters in The Body Artist? If so, which
characters are involved in the phenomenon? Considering the very limited She tried not to press him for information. She found the distance
number of characters interacting in the novel, the following combinations interesting, the halting quality of his speech and actions, the self-
are the only ones of some interest: Lauren-Rey and Rey-Lauren; Lau- taught quality, his seeming unconcern about what would happen
ren-Mr Tuttle and Mr Tuttle-Lauren. to him now. Not apathy or indifference, she thought, but his limited
As far as the relationship between Lauren and Rey is concerned, their ability to consider the implications (BA, 46-7).
relationship is characterised by a strikingly small number of doubly em-
bedded narratives. This is one of them: Maybe this man experiences another kind of reality where he is
here and there, before and after, and he moves from one to the other
Now that he'd remembered what he meant to tell her; he seemed
shatteringly, in a state of collapse, minus an identity, a language, a
to lose interest. She didn't have to see his face to know this. It was
in the air. It was in the pause she trailed from his remark of eight, way to enjoy the savor of the honey-coated toast she watches him eat
ten, twelve seconds ago. Something insignificant. He would take it (BA, 66-7).
as a kind of self-diminishment, bringing up a matter so trivial (BA,
11-12). She watched him, his hair looked chalky today. He seemed barely
here, four feet away from her. He didn't know how to measure himself
Besides reporting Lauren's thoughts, the extract also grants access to to what we call the Now. What is that anyway? It's possible there is no
Rey's mind, since what goes on in the female character's mind is a rep- such thing for those who do not take it as a matter of faith (BA, 68-9).
resentation of Rey's own one. Embedded in her mind, in other words, is
his mind: the clause "he seemed to lose interest", although reported by the He was not skittish under her touch, or only routinely so, and she
narrator, is a sensation Lauren is experiencing, in some part of her mind thought that nothing could seem unusual to him, or startling, or
that does not coincide with inner speech. She senses it somehow, therefore
stirring, measured against the fact, the blur, whatever it was - the
she mentally constructs Rey's own feelings. Something similar happens in
breathless shock of his being here (BA, 71).
the following extract:
He stood shaking the container. He shook it longer than he had to Several more instances of the phenomenon can be found as the novel
because he wasn't paying attention, she thought, and because it was unfolds: Lauren in particular, is very curious about the alethic potential
of Mr Tuttle, and it is for this reason that she constantly tries to interpret
satisfying in some dumb and blameless way, for its own childlike sake,
his actions and his words in the light of what is probably happening in
for the bounce and slosh and cardboard orange aroma (BA, 12).
his mind. Even when no apparent sign of doubly embedded narrative is
Again, by giving access to Lauren's mind, the narrator also shows present, it is easy for the readers to understand that she is always trying
Rey's own thoughts elaborated by Lauren. Quite significantly, however, to decode his mental processes to find out what lies beyond his words and
the rest of the chapter devoted to the description of their breakfast does not actions.
provide any more examples of doubly embedded narrative, since narrative The scrutiny of doubly embedded narrative therefore contributes to re-
coincides with the presentation of the characters' actions rather than of inforce readers' inferences about Lauren being anxious to know, probably
their thoughts. Moreover, it is also very significant that it is always Rey's needy to know in order to better overcome her own loneliness and difficult
.LLV

life-moment. As we would expect, Mr Tuttle never makes any hypothesis in DeLillo's novel may imply that his characters are to be read in a simi-
about Lauren's mind, either because he is not interested in her, or because lar way, namely by focusing on the impact of the real world on the inside
he is not able to carry out any conjectures in general. It goes without saying universe of the characters. The importance of the first line, for instance,
that the lack of doubly embedded narratives still contributes to his charac- bas nothing to do with the obvious assumption that time passes: rather,
terisation. DeLillo is interested in the impact of time passing on the character that is
experiencing it. Time does not simply pass, it seems to pass, which is to be
ascribed to the feeling of the character experiencing an atmosphere of.in-
action and passiveness. Quite interestingly from this point of view, the fol-
3.9 CHARACTERISATION AND INTERTEXTUALITY lowing sentence ("the world happens") describes the world in intransitive
terms, again strikingly conveying the character's feeling of inaction and
ln The Body Artist, the role of intertextuality for the constructi.Qn of the paralysis introduced by the first sentence. The same atmosphere is experi-
character concerns mainly the selection of narratorial modes: most Qf.lh.e enced in the rest of the extract, where only mental or relational processes
noveÏ is characterised by a narrative technique that higfilights the con- are used, but not material ones.
'!ëi~ of Lauren: it is tl!!2ligh 1!er_that almost every aspect of the :fic- Si~y_tq__Woolf's, then, these characters seep to be anything but
fional-world is presented, mediated and filtered. 'd~rs': they tend to experience the world passively and to be acted-upon,
Readers, at least some of them, will not find it hard to recognise simi- rajher.than.acting. They are centres of consciousness: eveœhing_is experi-
larities between DeLillo's character-construction method and some writers enced through them and from their point of view, that is the only gateway
from the past, especially from the first part of the twentieth century: in
other words, together with all the points discussed in the previous sections,
'intertextualallusions' to very renowned narrative techniques elaborated in
-
to the narrated world for the readers.

the past may_als_Qcontribute to the.formation of characters' images in)1!ad~-


ers' minds, at least in some of them. In particular, intertextual allusions of 3.10 STRUCTURE OF THE NOVEL AND CHARACTERISATION
this kind can be traced with reference to Virginia Woolf's two novels Mrs
Dalloway and To the Lighthouse: similarly to hers, DeLillro,_charactei:s Novels are generally structured into parts, chapters, sections, and so on;
are far from being 'traditionally conceived' (Woolf 1921, 189), and coin.- such a strategic arrangement of the narrative material constitutes what we
sige rather with what might be called a 'centre of conscious~a-w.in.d. may call the syntax of the novel, a metaphoric expression that should be
interacti_ng~ith the outside world without any apparent consequences op interpreted with reference to what goes on above the clause. What happens
The following extract is the opening of the novel: above the clause level is very important for character construction: indeed,
somi cbaractersmay appear exclusively in some parts of the nove!, ~y
Time seems to pass. The world happens, unrolling into moments, be assigned unequal space in the general economy of the text: this may con-
and you stop to glance at a spider pressed to its web. There is a
quickness of light and a sense of things outlined precisely and streaks
of running luster on the bay. You know more surely who you are on
--
tribute to sheddinglight on their importance in an ideal textual hierarchy,
The structure of The Body Artist is fairly significant in these respects:
as previously observed, the general atmosphere of the novel is one of sol-.
a strong bright day after a storm when the smallest falling leaf is
itude and melancholy, and such feelings are strongly reinforced by the or-
stabbed with self-awareness. The wind makes a sound in the pines
ganisation of the text. The seven short chapters which structure the novel
and the world comes into being, irreversibly, and the spider rides the
are in fact symmetrically arranged through a construction that powerfully
wind-swayed web (BA, 9).
isolates the first and the last of them, which are the parts of the novel in
Intertextuality is at work here: indeed, Woolf's To the Lighthouse which the protagonists' solitude is stronger and harder to control. In the first
contains a section called "Time Passes" (1996 [ 1927], 187-212), where of them Lauren is described during her last breakfast with Rey: everything
characters' presentation is carried out in exactly the same manner, through in the scene seems unnatural, forced, what emerges being only a strong
FIT. The choice of a very similar opening, also characterised by the same sense of non-communion and non-communication, thus of loneliness and
narrative technique, may perhaps be a coincidence, but even in that case it suffering; in the last chapter of the book, then, Lauren is portrayed while
would arouse a series of inferential mechanisms in some readers. she is living on her own in the big house, after even Mr Tuttle has disap-
Virginia Woolf's ideas about character construction are very well peared: here her solitude is highlighted by her incessant questions about
known (see 1.2), thus it cannot be excluded that the intertextual references her own identity and by her simulated dialogues with imaginary presences.
.LL=>

Such structural isolation of the first and last chapters is made possible book. Though considering only very short stretches of text, the analyses
by the interposition of two voices which do not belong to the main narra- have proved useful to describe the same features of the characters from
tive one: the former - just after the end of the first chapter - is an obituary different points of view: Lauren's loneliness, for instance, has emerged
of Rey after his death, whereas the latter - just before the last chapter of both from thedescription of the novel structure and from the account of its
the novel - is an interview with Lauren before her show in New York. Both transitive patterns. The analysis of doubly embedded narrative and inter-
voices have nothing to do with the main one underlying all the rest of the mental thought has further reinforced such feeling. AsJar as Mr Tuttle is
novel, yet they are emblematic for the structural emergence of a series of concerned, he has been defined as an alethic alien in Dolezel's terms, yet
issues which are further conveyed at the thematic level. his alienation has been confirmed also through the analysis of his languâgé"
The feeling~_haracter are thus echoed and reinforced..by the (the deviancy from what would be the normforegrounds the character, in
St!Uctu~tion of the novel: this is a common strategy and indeed Douthwaite's terms) and of his mind style. Different approaches to char-
not the only one at the author's disposal. Sometimes, for_in~tmce, charac- acterisation therefore often shed the same light, although from different
ters only appear in some chapters of the novel, according to regular pat- angles.
terns whiëFi regulate their presence25: this is likely to create a sense of ex- - The approaches outlined in this book are, of course, a selection among
pectation in the readers, who may associate a particular character only with several other tools that may be equally useful and rewarding: critical dis-
a particular section of the book (the odd chapters, for instance) but not with course analysis, for example, by focusing on inequality in society and the
the others. Clearly, in those cases the main attributes of the characters tend ways in which texts are used to realise power and ideology, may prove
to be associated with the recurrent atmosphere in those parts of the book in useful for investigating the relationships among systems of characters and
which they appear, and not vice versa. the way power is managed; Paul Simpson's studies in Language, Ideology
Choices concerned with the arrangement of the narrative material can and Point of View have already shown that it is possible to investigate a
therefore be very powerful for characterisation: Virginia Woolf's Mrs Dal- point of view through the analysis of linguistic 'modality'; the applica-
loway, for example, is not structured into different sections or into different tion of staîisficarand computational methods made possible by the current
chapters; narration is never interrupted, a choice strongly corroborating rate of progress in information technology may open up a range of further
the general effect created by the narrative style of the novel, which fore- possibilities, including, for example, the study Qf ling-yistic variation in a
gro~ds the several streams of consciousn~of the different characters. character's utterances.
Victorian novels, on the other hand, were normally structured into several Last but not least, new models are required in order to analyse the
chapters, themselves divided into sections and sub-sections: such syntactic significant diffusion of multimodal narratives permeating the last few dec-
choices were dictated by the need to introduce several different characters ades: characters in TV series, for instance, are among the most appreciated
from an external point of view, through an omniscient third-person narrator ones in today's fictional landscape, therefore new tools need to be devel-
who often interrupted narration in order to cope with the different charac- oped to account for the other semiotic modes at play. From this perspec-
ters, settings and times. tive, even though a lot needs to be done, Bednarek has already established
Needless to say, these strategies need to be studied alongside the other herself as the leading scholar (2010, 2011, 2012, 2018).
ones introduced above, and always need to be tested: characterisation is the
result of a large ensemble encompassing many factors and processes, this
being only one among them.

3.11 CONCLUSION

In this part of the book the characters of the American novelist Don De-
Lillo 's The Body Artist have been studied through a model of characteri-
sation that draws on stylistic approaches outlined in the second part of the

25
In some novels, for instance, some characters only appear in the even-num-
bered chapters and some others only in the odd-numbered ones.

You might also like